E’SYCHQGENIC ARTICULATION DISORDERS RELATED TO VERBAL SKILLS AND INTELLIGENCE AS MEASURED BY THE WECHSLER INTELLIGENCE SCALE FOR CHILDREN Then!- for the Degree of PhD. MICHIGAN STATE UNIVERSITY Katharine Gotten Bum: 1967 -.W._ .‘4 ,. ' r7. . -a. ~. . 3'“. j r in n he? I . d - ‘ 1.. 4 a) 4.1. A” 2?.- )j’ "I Michigan State University .Hh‘" This is to certify that the thesis entitled { i Psychogenic Articulation Disorders Related To Verbal Skills And Intelligence As Measured By The Wechsler Intelligence Scale For Children presented by Katharine Gorrell Butler has been accepted towards fulfillment f of the requirements for ‘ J Ph.D. degree inAudiologz J and Speech Sciences 2% can. Major 'ofessog Date June 8, 1967 0-169 l I” U ,3. 'Y! q! 1'“-.- . .. “u .r m In '1' i (b (> cf an O O ., ’5 UV i.‘ ‘ c"4 n ."I u‘ fer. 0"..- .hov l, O.._ ‘ de.°ns ‘ 95» h ., ‘3‘ U n 0710 y‘e 5 fl .0. I: ”flea fl \. V 4 ‘z::'0r ‘ ‘ ‘8 SZNy H ‘r- ‘5 P’gia I ‘ ‘- u e \ fieahac «5“, I , '~ I. is ‘VIOI‘ s' l I, ‘ L“7D \- Cm ““ H ‘9 ‘ ~h Erk s». Jo‘s! a ‘43? :- “in ‘. J as . “18 a ABSTRACT PSYCHOGENIC ARTICULATION DISORDERS ‘RELATED TO VERBAL SKILLS AND INTELLIGENCE AS MEASURED BY THE ‘WECHSLER INTELLIGENCE SCALE FOR CHILDREN by Katharine G. Butler While there has been a considerable amount of research in the area of speech and language in the technical. aspects of identifying and remediating speech and language disorders, comparatively little research has been conducted regarding the relationship of functional articulatory disorders and verbal skills. Furthermore, even less research has drawn upon a standardized measurement which yields both quantitative and qualitative data on verbal and performance levels of functioning. 'The purpose of this study is to evaluate the functioning of children exhibiting severe articulatory defects in terms of their verbal skills based upon their performance on the 11 subtests of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children. The normative group of the standard- ization sample serves as controls. The experimental group is rigidly controlled for sex, age, degree of speech defect, I.Q. range, peripheral auditory competency, visual- motor skills, monolingualism, and socio—economic status. Children exhibiting brain injury and known central nervous system dysfunction were excluded from the experimental sample, as were gifted and mentally retarded children. The functional articulatory defect was characterized by a t s. I I ("t ~ IA D .lo' ‘1’”, ‘.'S .F‘ “‘~=,. a *“V A--t~..~ corax‘Ih'c Hunt-\.,.~ . Alan u...e encnc VOWEQ‘ Y , V.UF “‘ ‘0‘. A CT. gt... 4 n .3 ”Abby-1“ bins". Cl 1 h n‘l'ed Do, ‘3'- V. C l 5 — Katharine Gorrell Butler a minimum of 5 or more misarticulated phonemes, consistently occurring in at least 2 of the 3 consonant positions in single word utterances. Each of the 50 subjects, ranging in age from 6-0 to 12-0, with a mean age of 8-0, were administered the'Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children under standardized testing conditions. The examiner recorded all verbal and motor responses on the protocol forms. Subjects' responses were tabulated and the raw data for analyses consisted of 542 subtest scaled scores and 150 I.Q. scaled scores. These 692 scores were subjected to a series of tests of significance for the difference between the means of the groups. It was found that there were significant differences between the two populations in regard to both Verbal I.Q. and several of the subtests. A On the basis of the results, the following con- clusions seem warranted: 1. Children with psychogenic speech defects and those with normal speech reveal no differences in performance in "general intelligence" on the Full Scale I.Q. as deter- mined by the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children. 2. Children with psychogenic speech defects reveal I.Q. scores that are significantly lower in the area of verbal skills than do children with normal speech. FW—F‘u—u—u—v _J (a! 0 .Fl.‘.“ v. .I- COCO-db e. h-, rimfizF,.“ - te-.-\,_“'-J a v4 ' '- b~.«r-\-&-C..’ 'nl‘ n A 5-.--21‘53 h'.‘ I P. u v y ‘ “--".Elc k'. ‘4 r9“.\ w‘“: S'C-c- l» '1 I“ “ Aug .0' u a ." ‘er-Cv~- -.. t. A ‘Jr nnl‘ . V..- a“; \ .~ b—~. 0.: t}: \ A. - " C3C'Q H‘~ :- v Katharine Gorrell Butler 3. Verbal subtests of the WISC, as achieved by children with psychogenic speech defects, namely, Informa- tion, Vocabulary, Arithmetic and Digit Span, are sig- nificantly lower than those achieved by children with normal speech. However, children with psychogenic speech problems reveal significantly better performance on Picture Completion, a subtest of the Performance Scale, than do children with normal speech who are of average intelligence. 4. There are no significant differences between children with psychogenic speech defects and children with normal speech for the subtests which measure Picture Arrangement, Block Design, Object Assembly, and Coding on the Performance Scale of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children. Recommendations for further research were made on the basis of these findings. PSYCHOGENIC ARTICULATION DISORDERS RELATED TO VERBAL SKILLS AND INTELLIGENCE AS MEASURED BY THE WECHSLER INTELLIGENCE SCALE FOR CHILDREN by ‘x i. v a ( ( Katharine G? Butler A THESIS Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Speech 1967 (c) Copyright by KATHARINE GORRELL BUTLER 1968 ACKNOWLEDGMENTS My debt to many is very great: To Dr. Charles Van Riper, who placed my feet upon the path, To Dr. Herbert Oyer, who lengthened the path into infinity, To Dr. Marion McPherson, who, on one occasion, branched the path, And to my husband, who has let me share yet another path, and surely . . . the most important. K. G. B. Ur Ova-I1 .. U“ § . Y":‘r 5'- H-U A V. "" ' .‘-~ H Tye-nun.— - any _. ._ HI '1 f;’ i-t '7) tel-T. if! F'“ '7 I I (4, (D i I) (1' (‘1 (’J ( t) (I (1' ”9 f1 " TABLE OF CONTENTS Page LISTOFTABLES................. v LIST OF ILLUSTRATIONS. . . . . . . . . . . . . . vi CHAPTER I. INTRODUCTION. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 Purpose of the Study Importance of the Study Definitions Organization of the Report II. REVIEW OF BACKGROUND LITERATURE. . . . . . 27 DevelOpment of Articulatory Skills Psychogenic Aspects of Articulatory Skills Intelligence Testing Variability as Related to Articulation and Language Skills Dyslalia and Dyslexia as Related Language Functions The Influence of Parent-Child Interaction upon Functional Articulatory Disorders Summary III. SUBJECTS, TEST INSTRUMENT AND PROCEDURES. 91 Subjects Test Instrument Standardization Sample Procedures Scaled Scores Experimental Group Procedures IV. RESULTS AND DISCUSSION. . . . . . . . . .105 Results Discussion 111 V 11.: -""1-‘v ““3" . TABLE OF CONTENTS (Continued) V. SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS. . . . . . . . . . Summary Conclusions Implications for Further Research BIBLIOGRAPHY. APPENDIX A. APPENDIX.B. APPENDIX C. APPENDIX D. APPENDIX E. SUMMARY TABLE OF WISC SCORES FOR THE EXPERIMENTAL GROUP . . . . . . SUMMARY TABLES OF MEAN VERBAL AND PERFORMANCE SUBSCALE SCORES AND S.D.'S FOR ALL SUBJECTS BY AGE GRWPS . O O C C O O O O O O O O 0 SUMMARY TABLE OF INDIVIDUAL VERBAL AND PERFORMANCE I.Q. SCALE SCORES-—DIRECTIONALITY OF MEANS. . RANKING OF THE WISC SUBTEST SCORES BY DEVIATION FROM AVERAGE TEST AGE SCORES FOR THE SIX AGE GROUPS, EXPERIMENTAL SAMPLE. . . . . . . . TEST AGE EQUIVALENTS ON SUBTESTS OF THE WISC BY MONTHS FOR DEFECTIVE SPEAmS O O O O O O O O O C O O 0 iv 151 160 173 175 180 182 185 i‘ TABLE I. II. III. IV. VI. VII. VIII. IX. LIST OF TABLES Page Mean Full Scale Intelligence Quotients for Speech Defective and Normal Children. . . 109 Mean Verbal Scale Intelligence Quotients for Speech Defective and Normal Children. . . 110 Mean Performance Scale Intelligence Quotients for Speech Defective and Normal Children . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 111 Mean Verbal Subtest Scores on the W130 for Speech Defective and Normative Samples. . 113 Mean Performance Subtest Scores on the W180 for Speech Defective and Normative Samples. . 114 Vocabulary Subtest Scores and Mean Verbal Test Scores of the Experimental Group . . . . 115 Analysis of Directionality of Verbal and Performance Scale I.Q.'s for the Experimental Group. . . . . . . . . . . . . . 122 Ranking of WISC Subtest Scores by Deviation from Average Total Scores for the Experi- mental Group. . . . . . . . . . . . . . .‘. . 124 Intra-Test Scale Score Ranking for Combined Age Groups. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 126 Ranked Means of Groups of Gifted, Average, Mentally Retarded and Speech Defective Children on the High and Low Subtests of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children. . .n. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 145 LIST OF ILLUSTRATIONS Figure I 1. Mean verbal and Performance Subtest Scale Scores for Normal and Defective Speakers, Ages 6-0 to 7‘0 0 o o o o o 2. Mean verbal and Performance Subtest Scale Scores for Normal and Defective Speakers, Ages 7-0 to 8-0 . . . . . . 3. Mean Verbal and Performance Subtest Scale Scores for Normal and Defective Speakers, Ages 8-0 to 9-0 . . . . . . A. Mean verbal and Performance Subtest Scale Scores for Normal and Defective Speakers, Ages 9-0 to 10-0. . . . . . 5. Mean verbal and Performance Subtest Scale Scores for Normal and Defective Speakers, Ages 10-0 to 11-0 . . . . . 6. Mean verbal and Performance Subtest Scale Scores for Normal and Defective Speakers, Ages 11-0 to 12-0 . . . . . vi Page 116 117 118 119 120 121 CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION Speech and language are learned processes, but the manner and degree to which they are learned is of primary importance to those interested in verbal communication. Difficulty in verbal communication based upon faulty learning of articulatory skills has long been considered an area of concern in the field of speech pathology and audiology. Speech disorders may be classified in gross fashion as either functional or organic, with the presum- ably functional, or psychogenic, disorders far outweighing those of organic origin in frequency of occurrence. How- ever, comparatively little is known regarding psychogenic factors in functional articulatory defects, although the study of the acoustical manifestations of articulation deviancy has been underway for three decades. In addition, little research has been attempted in the specific realm of articulatory deficiencies and verbal skills as measured by a standardized instrument which assesses several aspects of verbal performance. Developmentally, the importance of articulatory adeptness is reflected in the other speech and language 1 measures. As Schreiber points out throughout her text, successful skills in one area increase the probability of success in other areas.1 The magnitude of failure of many children to achieve success is indicated by Hall's comprehensive review of functional disorders of articula- tion. She indicates that articulation problems have long been recognized as the most prevalent of all the disorders of speech. Because this is true and since only a small fraction of articulation cases are organic- ally based, functional articulation problems constitute a highly significant group of dis- orders in the total field of speech pathology. They merit serious study and much greater scientific investigation than they have yet received, not only because they are so common but also because they are by no means so simply explaineg and treated as many people have assumed. In addition, she indicates that "probably more than any other type of speech disorder, ... . functional articula- tion cases are intimately associated with all dimensions of . . . the individual's growth."3 These dimensions in- clude physical, intellectual, emotional and social growth patterns and their intra—relationships as well as the modifi- cation of these patterns as a function of the environment. Thus, it can be seen that functional articulatory defects 1Flora R. Schrieber, Your Child's Speech (New York: G. P. Putnam's Sons, 1956), pp. ii-391. 2Margaret Hall Powers, "Functional Disorders of Articulation——Symptomatology and Etiology," Handbook of Speech Pathology, ed. Lee Edward Travis (New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, Inc., 1957), p; 707. 3Ibid., p. 709. represent only the diagnostic label for a comprehensive disorder. The American Speech and Hearing Association has indicated its concern regarding the etiological signifi- cance of articulatory disorders by appointing a committee to study the research needs specifically related to this problem. The committee concluded that research in the area of articulatory problems not only must be related to other dimensions of language which may be affected, but also must building upon normative data involving the development of articulation from birth through adulthood. These data and the conditions from which they arise must provide the starting point for investigation of deviant behavior. Unfortunately, information on this subject is not highly reliable. The conventional phonemic concept used by most research workers and clinicians in defining the articulatory disorder were questioned by the committee. Creation of a more satisfactory concept is dependent in part upon the availability of more infiormation about the development of articulation. The committee also indicated that there was a need for research in relation to linguistic factors such as vocabu- lary, sentence structure, sentence length, and articulation development in relation to social, psychological, physical and intellectual factors, among many others.5 The interweaving of articulatory ability in the ”Jesse Villarreal et al., ”I. Report of Sub- Committee on Articulation Problems,” Journal of Speech and Hearing_Disorders Monograph: Research Needs in Speech Pathglggy_and Audiology, Supplement VISeptember, 1959), Del. 5Ibid., p. 15. ‘o" fabric of speech, language and personality is not a recent concept. For example, Terman and Merrill pointed out in 1937 that language, essentially, is the shorthand of the higher thought processes, and the level at which this shorthand functions is one of the most import- ant determigants of the level of the processes themselves. Therefore, it can be seen that the cognitive aspects of language have long been evaluated as essentially determining the extent of the growth of the cognitive processes. Language and cognition appear to develop together. While Terman and Merrill attempted to measure this growth through a considerable number of verbally-oriented sub-tests on the Stanford-Binet, David Wechsler later moved to a more global concept of verbal performance and its intellectual connotations. He reported that clinical experience and research in the past two decades have shown that it is not possible to identify or equate general intelligence with intellectual ability, however defined. Actually any and every test of intelligence measures something more, often a good deal more, than sheer intellectual ability-—or any aspect of it, verbal, abstract, numerical or even 'g'. Thus, in designing a test of intelligence, Wechsler concluded A AA 6Lewis M. Terman and Maud A. Merrill, Measurin Intelligence (New York: Houghton Mifflin Company, 1937;, p. 5. 7David Wechsler, WISC Manual: Wechsler Intelli- gence Scale for Children (New York: The Psychological Corporation, 19E§7, pp. 4-5. ,._ ‘1 -1’_-_-_ __ - -._..__.... .. that while intellectual capacity (or any facet of it) may be a unitary trait or ability, general intell- igence is not. In brief, intelligence is a pgrt of a larger whole, namely personality itself. Wechsler noted that the 12 tests which comprise the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children are divided into two subgroups identified as verbal and Performance, and indicated that most of the verbal tests correlate better with each other than with tests of the performance group. Concomitantly, the performance tests correlate more adequately with each other than with the verbal sub- tests. However, he takes care to emphasize that both sub- groups of tests tap other factors, among them non-intellective ones, which cut across the groups to produce other classifications or catagories that are equally important to consider in evaluating the individual's performance.9 It may be concluded, therefore, that language, cognition, intellectual capacity, general intelligence, and personality cannot be truly dichotomized along any series of continuums, but rather, reflect a reciprocal interweaving of processes and developmental patterns. In addition, recent research in oral language performance indicates a close relationship between psycholinguistic 81b1dc’ p0 5. 9Ibid., p. 6. t . u - . . . o n a. . o . . . . e . O C . , \ O _ .1 . . e o O . . . o, . O o . . h I . o, u . v I I e y t . , . . o . '< . . x . O . . r‘ O . I 1 I P I functions, as detailed above, and articulatory competence. While defective articulation and disturbance of psycho- linguistic function may be differentiated, DeHirsch, Jansky and Langford report that the two are closely related. Difficulties with formulation of conceptual material belonging to the psycholinguistic aspect of language often leads directly to a breakdown in articulation because the organizational load becomes too heavy.10 They report that articulatory competence depends upon the accurate perception and recall of "auditory-verbal gestalten; on adequate central integration of fine movement patterning of the peripheral speech mechanism; and according to Hardy, . . . on feedback."11 The complexity of functional, or psychogenic, articulatory disorders is thus confirmed. Purpose of the Study; The present study deals with the performance of speech defective children who exhibit a functional articu- lation disorder as measured by a standardized intelligence test. While there are literally hundreds of published intelligence tests, of the standardized tests available, the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children constitutes the most reliable and valid instrument which provides for t w — WH~H ”'u 10Katrina DeHirsch, Jeannette Jefferson Jansky and William S. Langford, "The Oral Language Performance of Premature Children and Controls " Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, XXIX, No. 1 (February, 1964), p. 64. 11Ibid., p. 63. both verbal and non-verbal measurement, as well as permit- ting analysis of several verbal scales related to speech and language functions. The purpose of the study is to evaluate the functioning of children exhibiting severe articulatory defects and the "normal child" as portrayed by the mean performance of the standardization groups utilized in the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children sample. The following null hypotheses were formulated for this study: 1. There are no significant differences in functioning between speech defective children, as defined in this study, and normal children on the Full Scale Intelligence Quotients of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children. 2. There are no significant differences in functioning between speech defective children and normal children on the Verbal Scale of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children. 3. There are no significant differences between speech defective children and normal children on the Pere formance Scale of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children. 4. There are no significant differences between speech defective children and normal children on the sub-test scaled scores, and, in particular, the vecabulary subtest of the Verbal Scale. Importance of the Study, As was recently indicated by the U. S. Department of Health, Education and Welfare, impairments of articula- tion are among the treatment problems of over 15% of the entire population of the United States, ages birth to 21.12 In addition, surveys over the past thirty years have indicat- ed that children and adults suffering from some type of speech disorder outnumber all other handicapped groups.13’ 14 Evidence of the current interest in the speech handicapped is reflected by the level of current federal support in this area. Research support for the year 1965 was supplied by a number of agencies, including the Vocation- al Rehabilitation Administration, Division of Handicapped Children and Youth, and the Division of Educational Research of the U. S. Office of Education, Children's Bureau of the 12v. s. Department of Health, Education, and Wel- fare, Public Health Service, Research Profile, Summer of Pro am in Hearing, Language,_and Speech Disorders, Prof le E, PHS Pub. No. 1156, Rev., 1965, p. 2} 13A. W. Mills and H. Streit, "Report of a Speech Survey, Holyoke Mass.", Journal of Speech Disorders, VII, No. 2 (June, l9fi2), pp. 161- 7. lJ‘LCharles van Riper, Speech Correction: Principles and Methods (New York: Prentice-Hall, Inc., 1954), pp.'33-36. Welfare Administration, Division of Chronic Diseases of the Public Health Service, National Institute of Neurological Diseases and Blindness, and the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development. These agencies contributed a grand total of approximately $18,575,639.00 for speech and hearing in the year 1965, $8,798,743.00 of it for research. One agency, the Office of Education, reports that among the objectives of its Children's Bureau are two of importance to this study: (1) To assist in the early detection of children with conditions resulting in communicative handicaps and (2) the provision of multi-disciplinary and comprehensive diagnostic evaluations of speech, hearing and language dis- orders.15 So important is the need for specific identification of children suffering from a variety of speech disorders (the most common of which are articulatory defects) that the U. S. Office of Enucation, Division of Handicapped Children and Youth, is funding a multi-million dollar project which is to be designed as a prevalence study of speech and hearing disorders among school children in the United States. The primary objective is to obtain a reliable estimate of the prevalence of speech and hearing disorders in school children 15American Speech and Hearing Association, "Federal Support of the Profession of Speech and Hearing," ASHA, VIII, No. 5 (may. 1966). 9. 197-99. ' 10 by means of a sampling technique and screening procedures. Data will be gathered from approximately 200,000 children 16 The current focus via from the 48 continental states. funding on speech and hearing problems and thus upon articu— latory disorders which make up the numerical majority of these problems reinforces the need for knowledge regarding the child who possesses this most common syndrome, the functional articulatory defect. As the American Speech and Hearing Association Com- mittee on Articulation Disorders indicated in 1965, there is a need to develop a more satisfactory definition of "articulation disorder" as well as a need to define both the factors which promote and the factors which impede the normal development of speech sound articulation.17 In addition, while research devoted to the etiology of function- al articulation problems has been fairly extensive, this research has failed to indicate specific factors as having causal significance. Winitz and Lawrence, in a review of the major studies in this area, report that present research knowledge of functional articulation cases has failed to demonstrate systematic deficiencies for any of the factors 16American Speech and Hearing Association, "In- stitutional News and Announcements," ASHA, VII, No. 11 (November, 1965), p. 480. 17v111arrea1, op. cit., pp. 14-17. 11 studied. They state that since organic factors have never been clearly shown to operate in the "usual" functional articulation case, it may be assumed that certain unidenti- fiable learning factors have operated in the past to account for the differences in articulatory functioning of young 18 children. Their study led them to conclude that there was no difference between children with good and poor articula- tion in rate or level of learning. In summary, it would seem that differences in articulation ability may be due to some rather complex reinforcement contingencies that have operated in the past or still Operate, for when learning conditions are made similar, as in this study, differences between children with good and poor articulation are not apparent in rate or level of learning. Since little is known about the factors that account for language learning although much information is available on the stages of language growth, it is not as yet cledg what the reinforcement contingencies might be. The "yet-unindentifiable learning factors" influencing articu- latory skills, to which Winitz and Lawrence allude, may be among those which Andreas has referred as "language habits." He indicates that language represents learning of long standing in the temporal sense, and he reports that divergence 18Harris Winitz and Martha Lawrence, "Children's Articulation and Sound Learning Ability," Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, Iv, No. 3 (September, 19617} pp. 259-68. 19Ib1d., p. 266. 12 of past language experience will be reflected in verbal responses.20 In past attempts to understand articulatory problems, little emphasis has been placed upon the psychological factors as well as the learning factors. For example, in her welleknown discussion of functional disorders of articulation, Margaret Hall Powers is able to provide reference information on only 5 studies related either directly or indirectly to the functional aspect of such defects, although lfi§_references are included in the chapter regarding the symptomatology and etiology of articulatory disorders.21 In another review of research on articulatory (isorders and personality, Spriestersbach indicates that studies geared to assess the impact of functional articulation disorders have been largely ignored and points out that lack of interest cannot be due to lack of material since artic- ulatory disorders constitute the bulk of the speech pathologist‘s clinical load.22 He stresses the psychogenic nature of a non—organic articulatory disorder by stating that there is a "personal backwash" in speech or communication. 2OJBurton G. Andreas, Experimental Psychology, (New York: John Wiley and Sons, Inc., 1960), p. 5. 21Powers, op. cit., pp. 707-68. 2'2Duane C. Spriestersbach, "Research in Articula- tion Disorders and Personality," Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, XXI, No. 3 (September, 1956), pp. 329-35. 13 He adds that this logic, pursued to its ultimate conclusions, would appear to say that a speech disorder which does not have a psychological impact on the speaker is not a disorder of any consequence since communication is apparently proceeding without difficulty. . . . An articulation disorder may cause such psychological repercussions within the speaker thaB maladjustments will be the probable conclusions 3 Spriestersbach's review included every appropriate study that had been abstracted or published in Speech Monographs, the Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders and Psychological Abstracts during the period 1950 to 1956. He found 9 studies in all and, after evaluating each in terms of research design and statistical analysis, reported that with a five to four count in favor of maladjust- ment it would appear that no conclusive data exist on which to base any kind of a general- ization, one way or the other. However, one is struck by the wide range of severity of the articulatory defects frequently found in the groups studied. It is to be questioned whether many of the mild articulatory defects that are worked with clinically reagly represent serious barriers to communication. The writer's review of research in this area would indicate that the above criticism of past research design has not as yet been remedied. There is little or no similarity between the various samples of "mild", "moderate" or "severe" artic- ulatory defects as described in current research, even on 23Ibid., p. 330. zunid 0’ p 0 333-31} 0 14 such a gross measure as the number of misarticulated phonemes. As Spriestersbach pointed out in his summary . . . it would be surprising to find that measurable maladjustments are related to this rather vaguely defined group of problems. The study is yet to be done which attempts to measure the adjustment problems of a group of speakers with severe articulatory problems. The multiplicity of causes in so great, the effects of the disorder are so varied, and the inherent error within the testing instruments used is so great that it would be surprising, indeed, to find conclusive data on this question from the study of the usual 'representative' samgge of individuals with articulatory problems. He further feels that test measures of personality are, at best amorphous. Such measures are likely to be invalid. He states one is forced to conclude that the contribution of research to an understanding of the relation- ship between articulatory defects and personality is largely negative. The data do Bgt justify a statement about the relationships. Spriestersbach concludes that "utilization of appropriate instruments and the construction of relevant hypotheses are long overdue."27 A possible method of approach to this problem of the effect of functional, or psychogenic, speech problems upon the verbal communicative behavior of children may be through the use of the more adequately standardized and validated diagnostic instrument, the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for 25 26- '55 1d. '5' i 27 E 15 Children. The speech defective child's performance on the Verbal Scale is of particular interest since it is a matter of clinical observation that school adjustment is related to verbal skills. The Vocabulary subtest holds special interest as well since performance in Vocabulary appears to be sensitive to early parent-child relationships. Finally, achievement, as reflected by both oral and visual skills, is measured throughout both the verbal and Performance Scales of the Wechsler. Everhart states still more explicitly that speech and reading are inextricably associated in the process of language and adds that "any limitation or facility in one is directly reflected to some degree in the other. Articula- tory defects are considered to affect reading skills."28 For at least the past two decades, those interested in written language disorders and in reading difficulties have been utilizing the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children in an attempt to establish a "poor reader profile." Sampling difficulties have also been encountered in this area, and most of the data reported have been collected on a small number of subjects. Just as there tends to be little or no differentiation made between milld, moderate or severe articulation disorders, there also tends to be little 28Rodney W. Everhart, "Literature Survey of Growth and Development Factors in Articulatory Maturation," Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, XXV, No. 1 (February, 1965), p. 62. 16 differentiation made between a mild, moderate or severe reading disorder. Thus, it is not surprising to find that Everhart reports that an investigation of research reports indicates an absence of complete agreement as to the re- lationship of reading defects to articulation aberrations. . . . Any limitation in real or vicarious experiences, with their concomitant limitations in word meaning and articulatory fluency could pegadventure have a bearing upon reading ability. Whatever the sampling difficulties encountered, it has been reported by psychologists involved in clinical work that research on 14 well-known and standardized tests, including the Binet and the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children "supported" at least one hypothesis which is per- tinent to the present study, and that is that "each subtest item of standardized intelligence tests can tap wide per- sonality dimensions."30 Since the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children subtests scaled scores may reveal typical performance on the part of articulatory defective children, it is of importance to note that while the numerical measures of scatter in themselves are not regarded as very helpful in clinical 29Ibid., p. 60. 30Erika Fromm, Lenore Hartmann and Marian Marschak, "Children's Intelligence Tests as a Measure of Dynamic Per- sonality Functioning,“ American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, XXVII, No. 1 (January, 1957}, pp. 134iufi; . ... u. m . .a— a: a: ... r” 5 .5. ad I. F: F? C» nu 9U l. . \ Av 7. S “A a . a» S u e a. E a. .n.» W .G .6... S a. so -vu tge ht! 0“,. I‘ll—I ”Ike ‘ in! 17 diagnosis, the extent and scatter and the qualitative analysis of the test performance are extremely valuable in determining the extent of maladjustment. Scatter is thought of dynamically as the interrelationship of functions underlying the individual's achievement on the various subtests and represents the intra-individ- ual configuration or pattern."31 It is hoped that this study may make a contribution in the specific area of analysis of the relationships be- tween functional articulation disorders in children and the subtest scatter on the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children. Definitions Several terms appear in the literature dealing with the various aspects of this study. These terms and their definitions follow. Articulation. As defined by Powers, it is the production of speech sounds by the stopping or constricting of the vocalized or non-vocalized breath stream by move- ment of the lips, tongue, velum or pharynx.32 Disorders of Articulation. These refer to the faulty placement, timing, direction, pressure, speed, or integration of these movements, resulting in absent or incorrect speech sounds, as defined by Powers.33 31Erika Fromm and Lenore Hartmann, Intelligence, a Dynamic Approach, (Garden City, New York: Doubleday and Company, Inc., 1955), pp. 45-46. 32 Powers, 0p. cit., p. 707 33Ibid. 18 Functional Disorders of Articulation. Powers states . . . A functional articulation disorder can be defined as an inability to produce correctly all of the stand- ard speech sounds of the language, an inability for which there is no appreciable structural, physiological, or neurological basis in the speech mechanism or its supporting structures, but which can be accounted for by normal variations in thfi organism or by environmental or psychological factors.3 For the purposes of this study, a functional or psychogenic articulation disorder is operationally defined as misartic- ulations or omissions of standard English phonemes by an individual who reveals no structural, physiological or known neurological basis for these deviations. Classification of Functional Articulation Defects. Considerable confusion exists in the literature regarding classification of articulatory defects. With children under eight, maturational effects of phonemic growth must be taken into account.35 Children below this age may misarticulate some phonemes and still possess "normal speech." Templin and Darley state that accurate articulation is "assumed completed for most children by the age of eight."36 3uIbid., p. 708. 35Mildred Templin, "Norms on 3 Screening Test of Articulation for Ages Three through Eight," Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, XVIII, No. 4 (December, l953),pp.32}31. 36Mildred Templin, The Templin-Darley:l§sts of Artic- ulation, (Iowa City, Iowa: Bureau of Educational Research and Service Extension Division, State University of Iowa, 1960), p. 8. 19 Templin's research untilized as a criterion of phon~ emic age the passage of test items by 90% or more of her sample. Age scaling procedures are common, as exemplified by the Laradon Scale37 but do not measure levels of severity. In addition, recent research indicates that the age scales themselves may be inaccurate. A recent study of 15,255 children revealed that by the end of the first grade, no phoneme was misarticulated by 10% of the children. The authors concluded that all phonemes are fully developed by at least the age of seven.38 Severity judgments are difficult for yet another' reason. As Morrison points out, "Indices based solely on the frequency of error may be inadequate for some clinical and experimental purposes.39 Misarticulations must be eval- uated in terms of the degree of misarticulation, the consist- ency of misarticulation, and the intelligibility level of communicative speech as well as the "raw" number of errors. There appears to be no satisfactory classification system. Even crude counting becomes complex since each W37William Edmonston, Laradon Articulation Scale Man- ual, (Beverly Hills, California: Western Psychological Serv- ices, 1963), p. 2. 38Kathleen Pendergast, et. a1., "An Articulation Study of 15,255 Seattle First Grade Children with and without Kindergarten," Exceptional Children, XXXII, No. 8 (April, 1966), pp. 541-50. 39Sheila Morrison, "Measuring the Severity of Artic- ulation Defectiveness," Journal of Speech and Hearing Dis- orders, XX, No. 4 (December, 1955): p. 348. 20 position in which it may occur in a word, making a total max- imum count of 96, as in one study,40 or by defining misartic- ulated phonemes as ones which are deviant in one or more positions, as in another study.41 A severe articulatory disorder has been variously portrayed as a numerical entity of 3,)+2 or has been noted as 3 to 4,43 as a minimum of 4,M a maximum of 5,45 or even as 6 errors.46 A few studies uOCarl R. Weaver, Catherine Furbee, and Rodney W. Everhart, "Paternal Occupational Class and Articulation Defects in Children " Journal of Spggch and Hearing Dis- orders, xxv, No. 2 (May, 1960), pp. 171-75. 41Rodney Everhart, "The Relationship between Artic— ulation and Other Developmental Factors in Children," Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, XVIII, No. 4 (December, 1953): 9903322350 qudna Jenkins and Frances E. Lohr, "Severe Artic- ulation Disorders and Motor Ability," Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, XXIX, No. 3 (August, 1964): pp. 286e92. 43Louis Lerea and Bruce Ward, "Speech Avoidance Among Children with Oral-Communication Defects," Journal of Psychology, LX, 2nd half (July, 1965), p. 266. qurnest L. Kronvall and Charles Diehl, "The Re- lationship of Auditory Discrimination to Articulation De- fects of Children with No Known Organic Impairment," Journal of S eech and Hearinngisorders, XIX, No. 3 (September, 195 9 pp. 335-380 uSErnest L. Kronvall, "An Investigation of Some of the Factors Frequently Suggested as Causes of Functional Articulation Disorders," Dissertation Abstracts, Vol. XXVI, No. 8, p. 4810. 46Ronald K. Sommers et. al., "Effects of Speech Therapy and Speech Improvement upon Articulation and Read- ing," Journal of Speech and Hearinngisorders, XXVI, No. 1 (February, 1961), pp. 27-38} 21 47’ 48 While the above-cited provide no phonemic criteria. studies vary considerably in the age range of subjects, most utilize six-year—olds whose "normal" speech may well incorpor- ate 2, 3 or more errors. For the purpose of this study, subjects have been chosen who exhibit a minimum of 5 misarticulated phonemes, which occur consistenply in at least two of the 3 positions (initial, medial, and final) in single word utterances, and whose intelligibility is reduced in running speech. Since the mean age of the children in this study is 8-0, this would indicate on an a priori basis, that a severe articulatory defect exists. This is a more stringent application of defectiveness than has been heretofore applied. Application of the term functional or ppyphogenic for this study incorporates the Power's definition quoted early in this section. Each subject was evaluated for structural, physiological, or neurological disorders based upon case history, school records, medical reports, and an oral peripheral examination, as well as gross and fine motor evaluation. Children with known organic, neurological or brain-injured syndromes were rejected from the sample. 47Dorothy K. Marge, "The Social Status of Speech- Handicapped Children," Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, IX, No. 2 (June, 1966), pp. 165-781 48Genivieve Arnold, "The Illinois Test of Psycho- Linguistic Ability and Severe Articulatory Problems," ASHA Convention Abstracts, v, No. 10 (October, 1966), p. 789. 22 pyslalia. This term refers to defective articula- tion due to faulty learning or to abnormality of the extern- al speech organs and is not due to lesions of the central nervous system.49 In this study, dyslalia is referred to solely as defective articulation resulting from faulty learn- ing. stlexia. This term has been variously defined as partial inability to read characterized by associative learning difficulty and as a form of dysphasia by Wood,50 or as a symptom of congenital language disability by Arnold,51 or as a specific reading disability known as specific dyslexia, implying an idiopathic condition, by Kessler.52 For the purpose of this study, dyslexia is defined as a partial in- ability to read, a disorder of unknown etiology, and specific dyslexia is a symptom of general language disability. Test Variabiligy. .According to Wechsler, test variability defines two types of erraticism in performance, i.e. 49Kenneth S. Wood, "Terminology and Nomenclature," Handbook of Speech Pathology, ed. Lee Edward Travis (New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, Inc., 1957), p. 54. 5oIbid. 51Godfrey E. Arnold, "I. Present Concepts of Etiologic Factors," Studies in Tachyphemia (New York: Speech Rehabilitation Institute), p. 11. 52Jane W. Kessler, Psychopathology_of Childhood (Englewood Cliffs, New Jersey: Prentice-Hall, Inc.), p. 147. 23 inter-test and intra-test variability.53 Inter-test varia- bility is often referred to as "scatter“ or the uneven- ness in "the level of achievement on different subtest groups or tests. One can observe scatter in the distribution of passed and failed tests."54 Scatter is also defined as the intra-individual configuration or pattern, or the "inter- relationship of functions underlying the individual's achievements on various subtests."55 For the purpose of this study, inter-test varia- bility, scatter, and patterning will all refer to the variation in performance by the subject on the sub-tests of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children. Anxiety. Among selected definitions of anxiety appear the following: (1) it is a feeling, or affect, of a particularly unpleasant nature which may limit the individual's freedom of action;56 (2) it is a disruptive state which re- flects itself in fluctuations in attention and inability to concentrate.57 Wechsler states that some of the W180 sub- 53David Wechsler, The Measurement and Appraisal of Adult I telli ence (Baltimore, Maryland: The Williams and Wilkins Co., 1958), p. 162. . 4 5 Erica Fromm and Lenore Hartmann, Intelligence, A D amic A roach (Garden City, New York: DouEleday and 00., Inc., 1955 , p. 45. 55Ibid. 56 Kessler, op. cit., p. 44. 57Wechsler, op. cit., pp. 175-75. 24 tests are more sensitive to anxiety, such as Arithmetic, Digit Span, Digit Symbol, and Coding.58 For the purpose of this study, anxiety is defined as a disruptive state which may be reflected by the de- pression of certain sub-test scores. Intellectual Dysfunction. Intellectual dysfunction may be of two types. General intellectual dysfunction is referred to as intellectual inhibition effecting observable behavior, while learning disabilities are simply intellect- ual dysfunctioning limited to scholastic endeavors.59 The latter definition is reflected in Wechsler's statement that intellectual functioning may be thought of as the "ability to learn."60 Thus, intellectual dysfunction is the inability to learn. For the purpose of this study, intellectual dys- function is defined in a very restrictive fashion, 1. e., the depression of the sub-test scores indicating disability in learning. Organization of the Report Chapter I has introduced the concept of functional disorders of articulation and its importance to general speech and language function, as well as its relationship to verbal 58Wechsler, op. cit. 59Fromm and Hartmann, op. cit., p. 25. 6OWechsler, op. cit., pp. 4-5. 25 performance as measured by standardized tests of intelligence. The problem of this paper was presented, y;§,, the evaluation of the verbal and non-verbal functioning level of children exhibiting severe articulatory defects as compared with nor- mal children on the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children. Several terms encountered in the current study were defined and discussed. Chapter II presents a comprehensive overview of the literature related to past research in the areas of artic- ulation, psychogenic disorders, intelligence testing, ling- uistic functioning, intellectual patterning, and parent-child relationships. These areas will be considered in the follow- ing order: (1) development of articulatory skills, (2) Psycho- genic aspects of articulatory skills, (3) intelligence testing variability as related to articulation and language skills, (4) dyslalia and dyslexia as related linguistic functions, and (5) the influence of parent-child interaction upon functional articulatory disorders. Chapter III presents the procedures related to this study, including selection of subjects and test administra- tion. Chapter Iv details the results of the statistical analyses. The results of this study are discussed with regard to the hypotheses set forth in Chapter I. The find- ings of this study are related to previous research. 26 Chapter V presents a summary of the present study. Conclusions are drawn on the basis of the analysis, and recommendations for further research are made. CHAPTER II REVIEW OF THE BACKGROUND LITERATURE There has been considerable research over the past years which indicates that the development of articulatory skills is dependent upon much more than a normal physiolog- ical substructure. Everhart reviewed the growth and devel-8 opmental factors in articulatory maturation as identified in the literature during the past 15 years reported that whether or not the child develops acceptable patterns of articulation depends upon numerous complex and’ multi-dimensional elements. In the final analysis, it is not practicable to relegate articulatory maturation to any one single variable of growth and development. Actually, competency in articula- tion seems to focus upon the extent to which all developmental propensities contribute to the eventuation of speech out of the psycho-physical systems inherent in the human organism. The maturation of articulation in many children does not proceed in an orderly cycle, but is subjected to various disturbances imposed by individual deviations in sequence, Kate and pattern of growth and development.6 In a somewhat more simplified statement, Myklebust indicated that three "integrities" are necessary for the acquisition of language: (1) the integrity of the peripheral nervous system, (2) the integrity of the central nervous 61Everhart, "Literature Survey of Growht and Develop- ment Factors in Articulation Maturation," op. cit., p. 59. 27 28 system, and (3) the integrity of the emotions.62 Milisen further defines such a triumvirate in his statement that conditions which precipitate and maintain artic- ulation defects after the child has begun to speak are only an extension of the conditions which limited the production and differentiation of sounds and which interferred with the develop- ment of a communication attitude before he began to speak. . . . This concept that misarticulation is a substitute response precipitated by a break- down in the normal learning process should simplify articulation therapy because one does not need to create distinct types of therapy for all of the 'organic' and 'non-organic' groups. He will instead deal with each case on its own merits which will be determined primarily by behavior and speech per- formance, not by appearance and inheritance. While others might not agree with the therapeutic approach suggested above, there would be considerable agree- ment among research workers in the area of articulatory dis- orders that articulation defects are related to environmental conditions and communication attitudes.64’ 65’ 66 62Helmer Myklebust, "Language Disorders in Children," Exceptional Children, XXII, No. 4 (January, 1956), pp. 163-64. 63Robert Milisen, et. al., "The Disorder of Artic- ulation: A Systematic Clinical and Experimental Approach, " Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, Monograph Supple- ment No. 4 (December, 1954), p. 8. 64Ernest Henrickson, "Psychological Aspects of the Development of Speech and Language, "Archives of Physical Medicine Rehabilitation, XXXXI, No. 3 (March, 1960), pp. 95- ~102' 65Everhart, "The Relationship between Articulation and Other Developmental Factors in Children,"o op.cit., Do 332- 66 Powers, op. cit., p. 711. 29 Many speech pathologists feel that it is the communication attitude of the speech defective himself which contributes heavily to the etiology of articulatory defects.67’ 68’ 69 Others have found little or no correlation between disturbed communication attitudes and articulatory skills.70’ 71 Some point an accusing finger at the mother-child relationships as the crux of both the articulatory disorder and the child's personality difficulties. McCarthy summarizes much of this research by saying When the child whose speech is not developing normally is examined, it is usually found that there have been anomalies of language develop- ment present in one form or another throughout 67Lerea and Ward, op. cit., pp. 265-70. 68Arthur L. Solomon, "Emotional and Behavior Problems of First Grade School Children with Functional Defects of Articulation," ASHA, II, No. 10 (October, 1960). p. 378. 69Arthur L. Solomon, "Personality and Behavior Patterns of Children with Functional Defects of Artic- ulation," Child Development, XXXII, No. 4 (December, 1961), pp 0 731-370 700. w. Nelson, "An Investigation of Certain Factors Relating to the Nature of Children with Functional Defects of Articulation," Journal of Educational Research, XXXXVII, No. 3 (November, 1953), pp. 211-16. 71Leonard Goodstein, "Functional Speech Disorders and Personality: A Survey of the Research," Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, XXIII, No. 4 (December, 1958), pp. 359-75. 30 his life. These language disorders tend to appear in children who manifest certain types of personalities.72 There have been a number of conflicting studies in this general area. For example, Andersland reports that a relationship exists between children's articulation skills and maternal scores on certain personality tests which purport to measure hostility and rejection.73 Lerea, however, reported that no relationship existed between the rated severity of children's articulation disorders and the personality of the mother.74 Mowrer's current theory of speech acquisition, however, tends to stress the role of vocalization by the mother and the necessity for verbal and physical interaction between the mother and Child.75 McCarthy's research indicates that children who enjoy only a minimum of individualized adult contact in early Childhood have poorer speech and language. She indicates that since the mother is normally 72Dorothea McCarthy, "Language Disorders and Parent-Child Relationships," Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, XXX, No. 4 (December, 1954), pp. 514-23 73Phyllis Andersland, "Maternal and Environmental Factors Related to Success in Speech Improvement Training," Journal of Speech and Hearing Researpp, IV, No. 4 (December, 1961). pp- 79-90. 71+Louis Lerea, "Assessing Language Development," Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, I, No. 1 (March, 1958). pp. 75485. 75O. H. Mowrer, "Speech Development in the Young Child: 1. The Autism Theory of Speech Development and Some Clinical Applications," Journal of_Speech and Hearing Disorders, XVII, No. 3 (Septemberjfl952), pp. 263-68} 31 the Child's first language teacher in our culture, "it stands to reason . . . that the kind of nurture the child receives during this important formative period will have much to do with determining the facility with which he acquires speech."76 Goodstein cites poorly controlled studies util- izing a very small number of subjects as factors which make reliable conclusions regarding the relationship between personality factors and articulatory disorders as unable to be demonstrated. He reviewed the literature in the Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, Psycholog- ical Abstracts and Speech Monogrephs over a 25 year period, ending with December, 1957, issues. He reported that only two systematic investigations of the relationship between parental adjustment and the presence of functional artic- ulatory defects in children had been conducted during the past 25 year period.77 (However, when the author of this study reviewed the two systematic investigations, it was found that both utilized subjects with organic speech de- fects, not subjects with functional articulatory problems.) To continue, Goodstein then summarized the studies on the personality and adjustment of children with functional 76McCarthy, op. cit., p. 515. 77Leonard Goodstein, "Functional Speech Disorders and Personality: A Survey of the Research," op. cit., pp. 359-73- 32 articulatory disorders by pointing out that five studies' reported a positive relationship between functional artic- ulatory disorders and personality, with all five reporting emotional disturbances in the speech-defective child. The four studies which reported no relationship between per- sonality and articulatory disorders used the Children's form of the California Test of Personality, which Goodstein felt was not an instrument appropriate for the task.78 Goodstein's survey of the literature does not cover any research which has occurred during the past decade, although his summary statements are still reflected in many, if not most, of the medical, educational, psycho- logical and speech pathology references on this subject. Kessler, for example, points out in her comprehensive compendium of child psychopathology that it is "common professional practice for articulation disorders to be treated by speech therapists alone. For this reason, plus the fact that articulation disorders usually do not have an overwhelming effect on the total personality, " she indi— cates that psychologists primarily concern themselves with problems of delayed Speech and stuttering.79 781bid. 79Jane W. Kessler, op. cit., p. 130. 33 Pediatricians also tend to feel that unless speech intelli- gibility is severely reduced, articulatory disorders may be considered as inconsequential, representing only a matura— tional lag which time will heal. Medical journals often recommend that speech defects "as evidenced by stuttering, cluttering, stammering, or dyslalia should be noted" and included in Clinical work-ups, however.80 Recent research within the field of speech pathology indicates that speech defective children, particularly those with articulation defects, are somewhat less popular with their peer groups than are normal— speaking Children, when measured in school settings by 81 In addition, there appears to sociometric techniques. be a relationship between paternal occupational class and articulatory defectiveness. Paternal status is said to be significantly related to both early speech maturation for upper occupational groups and to severe articulatory disturbances in children for the two lowest occupational classes.82 Such references to occupational class and social status appear to be confirmed by current research projects conducted by speech pathologists functioning in' 8OKeith Hammond and Hans G. Keitel, "Childhood Academic Underachievement," Medical Science, (December, 1964), p. 63. 81Marge, loc. cit. 82Weaver, Furbee and Everhart. OELHElE-: p. 174‘ 34 Head Start programs. Irwin recently reported on such an experimental program, indicating that 94% of the children in the program (ages 4-5 to 6-0) had articulatory errors, and 71% had moderate to severe articulatory disorders. In addition, Irwin pointed to a language deficit which encompassed a depressed vocabulary as well as numerous misarticulations.83 A relationship between articulatory ability and language ability has long been postulated. Over a decade ago, Schneiderman reported that these language variables showed an increase with growth in both mental age and 84 Among disadvantaged children, chronological age. such language factors are reportedly delayed. Head Start therapists reported that since verbal behavior was not sufficiently reinforced in the home, verbal responses tended to be diminished in both quantity and quality upon school entrance. The stimulus deprivation found in the marginal circumstances of individuals in low- income, socio-disadvantaged areas appears to be related to language deficiencies. The environmental milieu 83Ruth Becky Irwin, "A Study of Certain Linguistic Skills of Children in Project Head Start of the South- Western School District," Paper read before the American Speech and Hearing Association's 42nd Annual Convention, Washington, D. C., November 12, 1966. 81*Norma Schneiderman, "A Study of the Relationship between Articulatory Ability and Language Ability," Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, XX, No. 4 (December, 1955), pp. 359-64- 35 provided in such circumstances may also serve to reduce the attentivity and auditory perceptual skills of such pre~school Children. Allen indicates that when a child learns to be inattentive prior to school attendance, this lack of attentivity reduces even further the perception of incoming auditory stimuli.85 Verbal language recognition requires that the individual "be able to organize a complexity of acoustic events into multitudinous patterns by certain rules of probability.86 Both attentional patterns and inter- pretive ability are thus seen as significant factors in the development of articulatory skills in children. The phonetics Limits Test, devised by Young, is designed to measure both auditory perception and interpretive ability. It provides for phonetic analysis and synthesis relative to a whole word construct without benefit of the total configuration. According to Young, even 3 and 4 year olds who showed little evidence of conscious analysis were able to respond appropriately, thus suggesting that such auditory tasks are perceived as a sudden Closure of 85Evelyn Y. Allen, "A speech and Language Develop— ment Program for Children in Operation Head Start," Paper read before the American Speech and Hearing Association's 42nd Annual Convention, Washington, D. 0., November 12, 1966. 86Norton E. Young, "Phonetic Limits of Word Recog- nition in Children," Paper read before the American Speech and Hearing Association's 42nd Annual Convention, Washington, D. C., November 13, 1966, p. l. 36 auditory Gestalten. Children with articulatory disorders exhibited a poorer performance on this listening task than did normal-speaking 4 and 5 year olds. It was postulated that there may be a relationship between the child's auditory perception and integration of certain phonemes and his own specific articulation errors.87 Allied studies report a variety of auditory- perceptual performances by speech-defective children which are less adequate than performances by normal Children. Kronvall reports that delay in discriminative auditory skills may be due to a slow maturation of this function in children with defective articulation.88 Masland and Case state that auditory behavior is a "most important aspect of language development, when factors of mental retardation, impairment of hearing acuity, and emotional n89 illness have been ruled out. They considered four facets of auditory memory in their study: (1) Memory span, which included not only duration of auditory atten- tion, but also the number of bits of auditory information which can be recalled in relation to the rate of occurrence, (2) sequence, the order in which auditory events are 87Ibid., pp. 8-9. 88 Kronvall, loC. cit. 89Mary Wooton Masland and Linda W. Case, "Limita- tion of Auditory Memory as a Factor in Delayed Language DevelOpment," De Therapia Vocis et Loquelae, et Phoniatriae, XIII Congressus Vendobonae Anno MCMLXV Acta, p. 81. 37 recalled, (3) petterning of stress, inflection and rhythm, and (4) patterning of phonetic detail. All of these are presumably related to the process of serial order temporal integration, which is the basis for the language process itself. The authors concluded that children with artic- ulatory difficulties reveal aberrations of auditory memory, as defined in their study.90 A well-designed and controlled study, although with a limited N, indicates that children whose verbal communi- cation difficulties are related to maturational lags have both short auditory memory spans and undifferentiated auditory discrimination. Auditory-perceptual Gestalten is reportedly poor and language reception is diffuse. Among other symptoms, these children reveal defective articulation, immature syntax, primitive sentence con- struction and awkward formulations?l A considerable number of studies also reveal that not only is auditory memory decreased in severely artic- ulatory—defective Children, but that selected motor skills 2 are also depressed.9 In a recent unpublished report, 90Ibid., p. 82. 91Katrina De Hirsch, Jeannette Jefferson Jansky and William S. Langford, Predicting Reading Failure: A Prelim- inary Study, (New York: Harper & Row, 1966), p. 88. 92T. David Prins, "Motor and Auditory Abilities in Different Groups of Children with Articulatory Deviations," Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, V, No. 2 (June, 1962), pp. 161-68. 38 Luper and Price found that children with articulation problems "were differentiated from Children with normal speech on tests of auditory perception and auditory motor I perception.93 There is not unanimity among the studies, however. Sandy states, based on a study involving an N of 132, that even though past studies suggest positive relation- ships, this study indicates that those with many articulation errors compared with those with few do not display (1) more difficulty with auditory discrimination tasks, (2) more incoordination in performance of movements necessary for successful production of speech sounds, (3) no abnormal degree of feelings, or (4) lower IQ'S a§4measured by the Pintner-Cunningham Primary Test. Winitz and Lawrence in a study of 96 kindergarten children with good and poor articulation found them to be equally facile in learning to perform a sound task consisting of sounds not present in the English language, and concluded that their findings contraindicate the presence of any factors, either physical or psychological, that inhibit or decelerate "sound learning ability."95 The area of articulatory skills as related to gross and fine motor abilities has also been explored. Jenkins and Lohr evaluated 80 subjects at the first grade 93Letter from Harold L. Luper, Ph. D., Head of the Department of Audiology and Speech Pathology, The University of Tennessee, Knoxville, Tennessee, December 8, 1966. 9"Don Sandy, "Auditory discrimination and Articu- lation Proficiency of Kindergarten Children," Dissertation Abstracts, XXVI, No. 8, p. 4891. 95Winitiz and Lawrence, loc. cit. 39 level, with no known emotional or physical disabilities, utilizing the Oseretsky Test of Motor Proficiency. They reported that the children with severe articulatory defects had more difficulty in motor proficiency than did Children without severe articulation disorders.96 Difficulty with fine motor tasks, such as that measured by the W180 digit-symbol sub-test, was also found in a study of 18 children with a median age of 9 years who had severe articulatory disturbances. While this study may be viewed as simply another visual-motor task, and the results interpreted as another example of motoric deficiency on the part of severely articulatory-defective children, the authors chose this particular sub-test of the W130 as a meas- ure of anxiety. They based this upon the assumption, held widely in clinical circles, that children with functional articulatory defects frequently suffer from excessive anxiety. They hypothesized that if the severity of the speech disorder was a function of anxiety level, different- ial performances should be obtained between mild and severe- ly articulatory-deficient Children. Using three groups of matched pairs of 18 Children each (normal-speaking Children, mild articulatory-defective children, and severe articulatory- defective children), They report that the "severe" group did 96Jenkins and Lohr, loc. cit. 40 significantly less well than did the "mild group or the 97 The psychogenic aspects of articulatory "normal" group. disorders will be further explored in the next section of this chapter. A discussion of the development of articulation skills would be incomplete without reference to its importance as a primary symptom and a major component of any number of so-called learning disabilities, whose etiological factors are currently being investigated. There is considerable confusion and nosological overlapping among such terms as "general language disability", "specific language disability", "minimal brain damage", "congenital language disability", and others. Clark defines general language disability, when it occurs "in older children", as an articulation disorder "other than a lisp" and refers to it as a condition based upon constitutional and inheritable neurological factors.98 Arnold defines specific language disability (known as SLD) as related to congenital language disability and a familial, hereditary and idiopathic syndrome is hypothesized. He defines articulation disorders as encompassing infantile 97E. Philip Trapp and Janet Evans, "Functional Articulatory Defects and Performance on a Nonverbal Task," Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, XXV, No. 2 (May, 1960), pp. 176480. 98Ruth Clark, "General Language Disability: Use of Psychological Tests in Diagnosis, ' Studies in Tachyphemia, (New York: Speech Rehabilitation Institute, 1965), pp. 87-91. 41 dyslalia, residual dyslalia, and pararthric speech. Linked with psycho-motor disabilities known as motor infantilism, habitual clumsiness, developmental awkward- ness and delayed motor maturation, the articulatory 99 disturbance is transformed into SLD. Other authors may classify the symptomatology differently, referring simply to "learning disorders," the primary symptoms of which may be dyslexia and articulatory errors, frequently with "known brain damage" excluded.100 " as a term, may refer "Central language disorders, to subjects without central nervous system dysfunction, although the utilization of the term "central" would appear to imply such an organic component. For example, children operationally defined as those "who exhibit specific disabilities in language and communicative skills" are seen as having a central language disorder, but are "not retarded."101 Another term in current use is that of "the inter- jacent Child". This is a child who may exhibit marginal 99Godfrey E. Arnold, "I. Present Concepts of EtiOIOEical Factors," Studies in Tachyphemia (New York: Speech Rehabilitation Institute, 1965), pp. 6-7. 100James C. Coleman and Malathi Sandhu, "Intellect- ual Level and Background Factors in Learning Disorders," Psychological Reports, XVII, (August, 1965), pp. 69-70. 101Matilda McIntire, John Wiley and William Wolski, "Central Language Disorders as Seen in a Mental Retardation Clinic," The Journal-Lancet, LXXXVI, No. 7 (July, 1966), pp. 374-75. 1+2 or weak "but not altogether deficient" aptitudes or modality functioning. These Children also reveal articulation dis- orders and "propositional weakness," 102 as well as poor syntact- ical formulation and vocabulary. The interjacent child would also appear to suffer from a central nervous system disorder, since Doll points out that "his CNS impairment has been called 'neurophrenic,‘ (and this) . . . implies that his abnormal behavior is related to neuropathology. The organic bases as to site, structure or function have not yet been Clearly catalogued. Some pediatric neurologists have reported that present techniques in this field do not permit precise evaluation of the constitutional foundations but rather these must be inferred from behavior symptoms."103 Behavioral descriptions abound throughout the clini- cal and research literature. The language behavior of the types of children described above almost always encompasses an articulation disorder. In congenital language disability, as in many of the other disorders described, it is character- istic to find the verbal IQ significantly lower than the performance IQ.1O" 102Edgar A. Doll, "Education and the Interjacent Child," Paper read before 3 Vanguard School audience at Roberts Hall, Haverford College, Haverford, Pennsylvania, January 30, 1965, p. l. 103Ibidc, ppo 3-H. lo"Godfrey E. Arnold, "The LLMM Theory of Language Disability," A paper read before the Northern Section of the California Speech and Hearing Association, San Francisco, California, April 4, 1964, p.1. 43 There is, as yet, no agreement as to whether or not general or specific language disabilities, central language disorders, or even dyslexic children suffer from an organic- ally-based, but presently non-measurable disorder. There is consensus, however, that Children with severe misarticula- tions who are presently Classified as "functional articula- tion problems" are much in evidence among the children suffering from these disabilities. Psychogenic Aspects of Articulatory Skills Myklebust, in referring to the child's need for emotional integrity prior to the development of normal language, states that unless the child continues to identify with the talking human, unless he finds language enjoyable instead of threatening and anxiety-producing, he might reject the world of talking. This means that disturbed or lack of language development may occur on a psychological basis as a result of emotional disturbance.10" It is almost universally assumed that this process of identification and the consequent influence of "mothering" upon young children is an important factor not only in articulation disorders, but in many other types of speech disorders.105: 106, 107: 108. 109 105Myklebust, op. cit., p. 164. 106Henrikson, loc. cit. 107McCarthy, op. cit., p. 515. 108Mowrer, loc. cit. 109Milisen, op. cit., p. 16. 42+ However, very few of the studies attempting to measure maternal-child interaction and its influence on subsequent articulatory disorders have been eminently successful. Andersland reported that only by analyzing the extremes of the personality test scores given to mothers of articulatory-defective children was she able to find a relationship which indicated a positive correlation. When the mothers were so poorly adjusted as to need psycho- therapy, she reported that their attitudes "may have a detrimental effect on their children which might be evidenced by functional articulatory errors. The mothers thus identified were found to have needs for achievement outside the home, to lack motivation for care of others and to show aggressiveness in interpersonal relationships.110 M011 and Darley, utilizing a similar research design and identical measurement instruments, found that mothers of children with impaired articulation but no retardation in other language areas, scored higher on "breaking the will" and "taking the natural meanness out of the child." These mothers also expressed disapproval of children's aCtivities more than did mothers of normal children or of delayed-speech children. While trends were noted, no significant differ- ences were found. The authors explain it in this manner: A more likely explanation of the lack of discrimin- ation is that the generally low reliabilities of these attitude scales make them relatively insensitive to 110Andersland, op. cit., p. 89. 45 subtle differences in maternal attitudes. Results of previous research indicate that differences which exist between thIII three populations are probably fairly small. Speech pathologists and psychologists with wide clinical backgrounds often report that the child's speech behavior is indirectly related to the parental behavior under dis- cussion. For example, Nichols reports that some children may be willing to pay the price of continuous speech surveillance but that "others may be resentful that their need for love is being exploited to serve the demands of society for clear articulation. Still others may be fighting two battles, one against a perceptual or physical disability "112 and another against parental domination. DeHirsh, with twenty years of Clinical and assessment experience, points out that dyslexic and dyslalic children reveal the follow- ing kinds of behavior: We were struck by the diffuseness of these children and by their difficulty in mobilizing energy in the service of a goal. It is possible, of course, that maternal anxiety played a part here. . . . Such maternal anxiety may flow over to the children and inhibit Epeir freedom to function in a variety of ways.1 The maternal personality structure found in studies which 111M011 and Darley, op. cit., p. 384. 112Alan C. Nichols, "Allocation of Time in the Articulation Program: Applications of Research," ASHA, VI, No. 1, (January, 1964), p. 9. ““— 113DeHirsch, Jansky and Langford, Predicting Read- ing_fa11ure,op. cit., p. 68. utilized children with known organic defects, such as cleft palate, however, was found to be undifferentiated from the maternal personality structure of the mothers of 4 l normal-speaking Children.11 ’1 5 When considering the speech-defective child, him- self, there is considerable agreement among the authorities that speech is not "merely verbal" and speech disorders are n116 Language: it has been pointed out, not "mere symptoms. has been viewed by behavioral scientists as a centrally important variable in understanding the behavior of human beings. Cognitive, conceptual and adaptive behaviors have been thought of as depending to a large extent on functional language. The heavy weighting of verbal items on most measures of intelligence bears witness to this fact. The dependence of language development on early sensory and motor experience has been highlighted by the7 work of such psychologists as Hebb and Piaget. Dokecki concludes that a related and complementary point of view is that language dictates the way in which an indi- vidual interacts with his environment. . . . The important point to note here is that the modern psychological zeitgeist does not view language as 114Lerea, loc. cit. 115Goodstein, loc. Cit. 116Wendell Johnson, "Are Speech Disorders 'Super- ficial' or 'Basic'?” ASHA, III, No. 8 (August, 1961), pp. 233-36. -. 117Pau1 R. Dokecki, "verbalism and the Blind: A Critical Review of the Concept and the Literature," ’ Exceptional Children, XXXII, No. 8 (April, 1966), pp..525-32o 47 an ancillary function or one that merely exists alongside others. Rather, language is seen as be- ing an important variable affecting other psycho- logical processes, as well as being affected by these processes. To put it another way, experience is important in determining language, but language is also impppéant in directing the course of experience. There seems to be some evidence that personality correlates which exist in conjunction with articulation defects may also be effected by age differentials. Templin reported several years ago that the more severe the artic- ulatory defect in the college-age adult, the more aggressive he became. However, the level of aggression was initially very low. In a study of 49 normal speakers, 37 articulatory- defective speakers, 15 defective voice speakers and 19 stutterers, the average aggressiveness of the articulatory defective group was the lowest of the 3 groups of speech defectives and significantly lower than that of the normal group. She concluded that when the degree of the articula- tion defect was considered, however, the subject tended to become more aggressive as his defect became more severe.119 Children with functional articulatory disorders are reported to be socially and emotionally maladjusted, and, 118113163. 11 9Mildred Templin, "A Study of Aggressiveness in Normal and Defective Speaking College Students," Journal of Speech Disorders, III, No. 1 (March, 1938), pp. 43-49. an : I.-- -‘ 1 .[ .umnv we. . . C _ n 3. o J. 3. .~. 3 E .1 .1 Z .3 0 f 0 .. 1:9. C .7. -C 3 T C .f. .c T. I. C. It i. C .5 an E E S ..u c. c. C. .e .v a... It rv a: .0 and .u c. C» h. E e a ,. e C. 0 .. a nu nu D. a u a m r E a: S It 5 a ti Qe(\ 48 in particular, to have feelings of inadequacy. Children suffering from severe functional articulatory problems are reported to possess a high level of anxiety,121 and to be withdrawn and constricted in their social relationships.122 In a study of 36 Children with functional articulation problems in Grades Four, Five and Six, with matched controls, Greenberg concluded that there was a relationship between personality and articulatory errors although the nature of the relationship could not be determined. He noted tendencies on the part of the speech defective group to be more dependent upon others, to Show more defensiveness, to maximize frustration situations, to be less well-adjusted in home and school and to be more insecure generally.123 12oC. Hill, "Problems Connected with the Intelligence Assessment of Children with Defective Speech," New Zealand Speech Therapists' Jflurnal, IX (September, 1954), pp. 7-10. 121Trapp and Evans, 100. cit. 122B. A. Deming, "A Study of the Emotional Adjustment of Functional Articulation Cases as Indicated by the Bender-Gestalt Test" (unpublished Master's thesis, University of Oklahoma, 1952), cited by Spriestersbach, loc. cit. 1‘3K. R. Greenberg, "A Study of the Relationship between Articulatory Disorders and Personality in the Inter- mediate Grades" (unpublished Master's Thesis, Ohio State University, 1952), cited by Spriestersbach, loc. cit. 49 Solomon, in a study of 49 speech defective Children with articulation disorders and a matched group of tlormal speaking children in terms of age, sex, grade placement and intelligence reported that . . . the hypothesis that functional defects of articulation are not isolated phenomena but appear as a part of the total adjustment pattern was supported. . . . Infantile and nonassertive behavior could very well serve as anxiety-redpfiing devices to meet the environmental pressures. In another study, Solomon reported that children with functional disorders of articulation tended to be passive children who internalized their responses and were characterized by submissiveness, timidity, and a need for approval. He hypothesized that these personality traits might represent a refusal to acquire socially acceptable functions because of unfavorable stress or environmental pressures.125 Lerea and Ward also found children who had been diagnosed as severe functional articulation defectives as expressing a greater number of avoidant responses than did children with only a few misarticulations. They concluded that the severity of a speech disorder seemed to be related to speech avoidance. They stated 121‘LSolomon, "Personality and Behavior Patterns of Children with Functional Defects of Articulation," 10c. cit. 125Solomon, "Emotional and Behavior Problems of First Grade School Children with Functional Defects of Articulation," loc. cit. 50 that anxiety, manifested by expressed emotions and avoidant responses in speaking situations may Einfifiifii‘éi Z‘Sfi‘ifuié‘ii‘éfiiéiii2.353658%“ In a study of children who exhibited poor language achievement at the fourth grade level, Wait reported that the anxiety level of these children was such that it not only may have inhibited language achievement, but expression of verbal defense mechanisms as well.127 In a study of children with learning disorders, Coleman and Sandhu reported that of 364 subjects, ages 7-0 to 15-9, seen at a University Remedial Clinic, 25% of the total group exhibited speech problems. They con- cluded that the intelligence of those subjects tested varied significantly only with the incidence of nail- biting and speech problems. They found that 13% of those children with I.Q.'s above 110 exhibited speech problems; 27% with I.Q.'s between 90 and 109 revealed speech abnorm- alities; and finally, 30% of those with I.Q.'s of less 28 than 89 also exhibited speech problems.1 In a 126 2 1 7Mary E. Wait, "Language Development, Anxiety and Early Socialization Processes," Dissertation Abstracts, XXVI, No. 9, p. 5255. 128 Lerea and Ward, 0p. cit., p. 269. Coleman and Sandhu, op. cit., pp. 69-70. 51 separate study, Tjossen, Hansen and Ripley reported that 58% of the underachievers they evaluated and 66% of the children exhibiting enuresis also had Speech problems. Speech was reported as "immature” and characterized by substitutions and omissions of speech sounds.129 In a review of schizophrenic speech behavior, Burk and Saxman indicated that objective information about the speech characteristics of schizophrenics is limited and inconclusive. Although much attention has been given to the language and thought patterns of psychotics and to their verbal behavior, the authors indicate that the emphasis has been primarily on the content of speech.130 They do, however, cite a study by Weiss which indicates that there were sharp differences in disturbances of articulation among various groups of schizophrenics.131 They also report that Green surveyed the speech of 1,891 psychatric patients and found a mean of 8.45 129T. D. Tjossen, T. J. Hansen, and H. J. Ripley, "An Investigation of Reading Difficulty in Young Children," American Journal of Psychiatny, CXVIII, No. 12 (June, 1962), pp. 1104-1113. 130K. W. Burk and J. H. Saxman, Acoustic Analysis of Schizophrenic Speech Behavior, Sponsored by the U.S. Department of Health, Education and Welfare, Public Health Service, National Institutes of Health, National Institute of Mental Health, Grant No. NH 07112, Purdue University (Lafayette, Indiana: By the authors, December, 1965), p. 10. 131D. A. Weiss, "Logopedic Observations in a Mental Hospital," Folia Phoniatrica, XVI, pp. 130-138, cited by Burk_and Saxman, ibid., p. 11. 52 articulatory errors.132 In a study of the communication patterns of schizophrenic Children, focusing clincically on speech and voice production, and sub-grouped as "reactive behavior disorders" and "Childhood schizophrenia," it was found that in articulation, specific sound distortions are common to both the schizophrenic and be- havior disorder groups. Complete omission of normally acquired sounds or substitutions is more frequent, howevig3 among the 12 schizophrenic Children. The author of the present study evaluated the speech and language of 60 emotionally-disturbed, hospital- ized children in Kalamazoo, Michigan. Of the 60 children examined, 28 displayed significant articulatory difficult- ies. In a longitudinal study of this group, it was dis- covered that 66% of the group had received from one to 7 years of public school speech therapy for "functional articulatory disorders" prior to commitment to the hospital for the insane. When these children were com- pared with other institutionalized children who did not possess such speech defects, they revealed significantly 132Antje E. Green, "Speech of Psychiatric Patients: A Hospital Survey," (unpublished Master's Thesis, Purdue University, 1962), cited by Burk and Saxman, 0p. cit., pp. 9-10. 133w1111am Goldfarb, Patricia Braunstein, and Irving Lorge, "Childhood Schizophrenia Symposium, 1955: 5. A Study of Speech Patterns in a Group of Schizo— phrenic Children," Journal of Orthopsychiaggy, XXVI, No. 3 (July. 1956), p. 548. 53 greater self—concept difficulty as measured by a drawing task. It was concluded that inadequate articulatory skills and inadequate self-concept, as well as a high degree to resistance to standard speech therapy tech- niques, were related.134 In another study of speech defective Children enrolled in public schools and attending a university speech and hearing center, projective testing revealed that 77% of the children revealed significant inter- personal difficulties. Of the 160 subjects in the study, 15% exhibited a high level of fantasy and unmet depend- ency needs, while approximately 30% were considered to be aggressive and action-prone. Children exhibiting articulation and delayed speech produced more primitive drawings that did children with voice and stuttering problems, indicating that the latter two categories may be reflecting an emotional etiology, rather than a 135 perceptual-motor dysfunction. Spriestersbach reports on the variability 134Katharine Butler, "Self-Concept as a Psycho- logical Correlate to the Development of Oral Language Skills," Paper read before the American Speech and Hearing Association's 40th Annual Convention, San Francisco, Cal- ifornia, November 21, 1964, pp. 7-8. 135Katharine Butler, "Psychological Correlates of Speech Defects as Revealed through Projective Tech- niques," Paper read before the American Speech and Hearing Association's 4lst Annual Convention, Chicago, Illinois, November 1, 1965, pp. 1-5. 54 of functioning of speech defective Children in his re- view of articulatory disorders and current research, and states one must also reckon with the possibility that the adjustment problems of this group of speakers may be highly specific to their speech. Perhaps the frustrations of faulty communication have not become generalized maladjustments. If so, testing instruments designed to measure general maladjustment and anxiety states can hardly be expected to identify the differenigg between these speakers and normal speakers. In a study of 30 children, mean age 7-1, who exhibited moderate or severe articulatory disorders and who had attended two years of intensive individual speech therapy at a university speech and hearing center, it was found that children who improved their articulatory skills also improved along other dimensions. These child- ren increased not only in articulatory ability, but in vocabulary, syntax, parts—of—speech, visuo-motor skills, and motor proficiency as well. The number of emotional indicators on the Bender-Gestalt decreased as the oral skills increased. Conversely, those children who failed to make significant gains in speech therapy also showed significant increases in the emotional component of their disorder as measured by the Koppitaz scoring of the Bender- 136Spriestersbach, op. Cit-1 Do 33"- 55 Gestalt. In addition, a trend was noted among the group who made little or no articulatory gains during the two year period toward increased withdrawal from Contact with the environment, as measured by 3 projective tests and the Brenner Developmental Gestalt Test of School Readiness.137 In conclusion, the review of the literature re- vealed 3 studies which failed to find significant differ; ences between speech-handicapped children and their normal counterparts. Marge, in a study of social status, found no differences in social position of speech defect- ive and normal speakers and reported that it was a surprisng finding since it was expected that the difference would be significantly great in light of the traditional view of the adverse effects of speech handicaps. It was piggdnggafiftifighgzg;srdgéngs corroborated the While Marge indicates that 6 categories of speech difficul- ty were part of the research design, results are not re- lated to categories of speech defects. Nelson studied 35 children with functional artic— ulatory difficulties and reported that his subjects were not more poorly adjusted than normal speakers of the same 137Katharine Butler, "Indicators of Emotional Disturbance in School-Age Children with Articulation and Delayed Speech Problems," Paper read before the American Speech and Hearing Association's 42nd Annual Convention, Washington, D. C., November 20, 1966, pp. 1~3. 138Marge, op. cit., p. 175. 56 age. However, the level of articulatory competency was not noted.139 Sandy, who utilized 132 children in his study but failed to define either the type or degree of artic- latory defectiveness, found that such Children had "no "140 The measurement instru- abnormal degree of feelings. ment upon which this conclusion was based is not Cited. To summarize, the great majority of studies in- dicate that children with articulatory disorders of sufficient severity do differ significantly from children with normal or only mildly disturbed articulation skills. Intelligence Testing Variability as Related to Articulation and Language Skills Intelligence and language skills are measured in a multitude of ways, both generally and specifically. Attempts to correlate any one of the many aspects of these two global concepts result in diversified patterns. For example, Matthews reports that in studies which have related 1.0. and onset of speech, or I.Q. and Speech proficiency, low correlations have been reported. Abt, Adler and Bartelme (1929) studied 1,000 children excluding those with I.Q.'s below 70 and those who did not begin to talk until after five years. 139Nelson, 10C. cit. 14o Sandy, loc. Cit. 57 The correlation between age of onset of speech and Binet I. g. was —-.41, indicating that the earlier the onset of speech is, the more intelli- gent is the child. Bangs (1942) found that when chronological age was held constant there was a correlation of .39 between speech proficiency and mental age. In a study of twelve birth-injured children with defective speech, Doll (1932) re- ported a correlation of .02 between I.Q. and severity of speech defect. Schlanger (1953C) found a correlation of .37 between mental age and articulation proficiency. Although all of the correlations are low, they do point to a relationship between intelligence and degree of speech involvement.1 1 Milisen, in reporting on the incidence of artic— ulation disorders points out that other factors such as intelligence, influence the incidence of articulation defects; Loutitt and Halls (1936) found 2.5 times as many child- ren in classes for subnormals with articulation defects as in the total school population. Wallin (1926) stated that defects of articulation were 'distinctly more prevalent among mental defect- ives.’ However, as Van Riper (1954) pointed out, the speech problem may contribute to the apparent subnormality. Lima (1927) reported that Binet tests of 402 children with speech defects (type unspecified) in the St. Paul, Minn., schools showed a median I.Q. of 97.7, well within the normal range. This sample, however, must not be considered random, since it was a school popula- tion from flpich the 'uneducable' would have been excluded.1 Powers discusses the intellectual variables of 1"1Jack Matthews, "Speech Problems of the Mentally Retarded," Handbook of Speech Pathology, ed. Lee Edward Travis (New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, Inc., 1957). 99- 538-39. 1"2Robert Milisen, "The Incidence of Speech Disorders," Handbook of Speech Pathology, ed. Lee Edward Travis (New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, Inc., 1957), pp. 253-54. 58 articulatory disorders thoroughly, and reports that "the solution of functional articulatory disorders has been sought by many speech pathologists in differences in general intellectual endowment. . ."143 However, follow— ing an exhaustive review of 11 studies, she concludes that the relationship of intelligence to articulatory deficienc- ies has certainly not been shown to be so close that it has much predictive value except within broad limits. At the same time, results of research are consistent in showing a gross relationship, particularly for the low end of the intelligence range. Except for the greater incidence of articulatory deficiency among mentally retarded individuals, intelligence appears to be relative- 1y unimportant as a determining factor in articu- latory disorders, at least above the age range during which most speech learning takes place. In short, during infancy and the preschool years intelligence appears to be an important factor in articulation growth. Above that level intelligence bears only a general relationship to articulatory proficiency except when intelligence is below normal limits whep it unquestionably affects speech adequacy.l 4 Powers, in a later review of clinical and educa- tional procedures, indicates her bias regarding the interp- retation of 1.0. testing. With children handicapped in speech it is necessary to be cautious in interpreting intelligence test results. Even when an artic- ulation problem is not severe enough to inter- 1"3Powers, op. cit., p. 748. 144Ibid., p. 750. 59 fere with intelligibility, it may still inhibit the child sufficiently so that he fails to make a maximum effort in giving test responses. The intelligence of speech-handicapped children is frequently underestimated on standardized tests. On the other hand, we cannot assume that a child's intelligence is higher than the obtained test result merely because he has a speech problem. It is always advisable to check the results of a 'verbal' type of test, like the Stanford-Binet, with a 'non-verbal' or 'performance' test. A test such as the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (Wechsler, 1949) has the advantage of including both a verbal and a performance”L scale, each yielding an independent rating. 5 Just such a study is reported by Vandemark and Mann. They studied oral language development and achievement in 100 matched subjects, 50 of whom had defective articula- tion and 50 with normal speech. Language scores were derived from language samples in terms of mean length of response, structural complexity, mean of 5 longest responses, number of different words, type-token ratio, and standard deviation of the response length. Only structural complexity scores provided significant dif- ferences between the groups. In addition, the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children was administered and the performance of the experimental and control groups com- pared by analysis of covariance. The Performance Scale of the W180 was chosen as the covariant because it was felt to be less dependent upon verbalization and therefona 1"5Margaret Hall Powers, "Clinical and Educa- tional Procedures in Functional Disorders of Articulation,’ Handbook of Speech Pathology, ed. Lee Edward Travis (New York: Appleton—Century-Crofts, Inc., 1957), p. 771. 60 should have been an intelligence measure that was less influenced by differences in oral language skill. The authors concluded that children with defective articulation are not in— hibited in terms of the amount of verbal output, but they do perform less well in the areas of grammatical gompleteness and complexity of responses.14 Arnold, in a recent study of children with severe articulatory defects, utilizing the Illinois Test of Psycholinguistic Abilities, reported that most Children with articulation problems have significantly low scores in one or more basic linguistic functions, as measured by the ITPA. Generally, these are in the areas of automatic— sequential abilities which underlie memory for patterning and in automatic use of grammatical structure of languagi4 which is basic to fluent expression of ideas. 7 Thus it can be seen that grammatical structure as well as verbal expressive skills of children with articulatory dis- turbances may be depressed when measured by standardized tests. Braen and Healing attempted to identify the most commonly used intelligence tests by psychologists in measur- ing the performance of special groups of children, such as 146Ann Ahlstrand vandemark and Mary Bachmann Mann, "Oral Language Skills of Children with Defective Articulation," Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, VIII, No. 4 (December, 1965), p. #12. 147Arnold. "The ITPA and Severe Articulation n 02. Cito,.po 7890 Problems, 61 the speech defective, deaf, blind, etc. They questioned 125 psychologists in public schools, child guidance clinics and centers for the handicapped and found that 48% utilized the standard form of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, while an additional 25% utilized the WISC with modifications for speech defective children. The Stanford-Binet, Grace-Arthur, and Wechsler~Bellevue were also used, but to a lesser extent. They expressed the opinion that from the results of this survey, it is apparent that a good proportion of the respnndents prefer to compare the score a handicapped child achdiyed with the norms available on normal Children. Darley, in referring specifically to children with com- munication disorders, points out that the verbal Scale of the WISC is based upon tasks which require verbal express- ion, but that the Performance Scale, while not requiring verbal expression, dpep "require an understanding of language."149 Goodstein chose the WISC to measure the in- tellectual performance of children who exhibited artic- ulation defects as a result of an organic problem. He 148Bernard Braen and Joseph M. Masling, "In- telligence Tests Used with Special Groups of Children," Exceptional Children, XXVI, No. 1 (September, 1959), p. 45. 149Frederick Darley, Diagnosis and Appraisal of Communication Disorders (Englewood Cliffs, New Jersey: Prentice-Hall, 1964), pp. 106-107. 62 concluded that it is Clearly evident . . . that the intellectual impairment of these children with cleft lips and p:%:§i:c%3ado:£iddpsEggtia1 in the area of verbal As indicated earlier, children from culturally disadvantaged environments reflect not only greater artic- ulatory immaturity, but also reveal difficulty in audi— tory discrimination. In addition, there are low but r significant relationships between individual measures of intelligence, achievement and information, as well as vocabulary, as measured by the WISC.151 In an attempt to measure the intellectual patterns of gifted, average, and retarded children on the WISC, Gallagher and Lucito found that mentally re- tarded children, who are known to have a high percentage of articulatory disorders, also did the most poorly on the Vocabulary and Information sub-tests of the WISC. On the other hand, the gifted Children tended to score highest in tests involving verbal comprehension, includ— ing Information, Similarities, Comprehension, and Vocab- ulary. The average child in both the Gallagher and Lucito 150 Leonard Goodstein, "Intellectual Impairment in Children withCleft Palates," Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, Iv, No. 3 (September, 1961), p. 292. 151Doris LOper, "Auditory Discrimination, Intelli- gence, Achievement and Background of Experience and Infor— mation in a Culturally Disadvantaged First Grade Population,’ Dissertation Abstracts, XXVI, No. 10, p. 5873. 63 sample and in the original standardization sample as re- ported by the Psychological Corporation, differs from both the gifted and retarded child. While there was no relationship found between the average and retarded samples, there was a negative relationship between the patterns of the gifted sample and the average group. The authors point out that only the gifted sample appears to excel in verbal skills.152 While such differences as noted above were found in general population samples, efforts to utilize the WISC performance to provide a basis for differentiating non- defective brain-damaged children from emotionally dis- turbed children present certain difficulties, according to Rowley.153 While he found no differences between groups on Verbal and Performance I. Q., in another study Beck and Lam report that "organics" tend to do more poorly on WISC Performance and Full Scale scores than on the Verbal Scale. The possibility of organicity increases considerably as the I. Q. drops to between 70 and 80 on 4* 152James J. Gallagher and Leonard J. Lucito, "Intellectual Patterns of Gifted Compared with Average and Retarded," Exceptionalehildren, XXVII, No. 9 (May, 1961), pp. 479-82. 153v. N. Rowley, "Analysis of the WISC Performance of Brain Damaged and Emotionally Disturbed Children," Journal of Consulting Psychology, XXV, No. 6 (December, 1961), p. 5531 64 the WISC.15" Other authors have reported that such discrepancies between Verbal and Performance sub-scale scores may be related to either personality155 or to specific traits, such as "withdrawal,""restlessness," "enuresis," etc.156 In a comprehensive review of Wechsler's work, Guertin, Rabin and Ladd point out that a Verbal-Performance I. Q. discrepancy of 13 or more points is not likely to be spurious in the sense of a statistical measurement error, but that such "real" differences are not unusual in the general population until they reach the magnitude of 25 I. Q. points or more. They indicate that while Wechsler himself felt that 15 or more I. Q. points differential between verbal and Performance was diagnostically signif- icant and two or more scaled score units was a convenient cut-off point, their work indicates that subtests must 15in. H. Beck and R. L. Lam, "The Use of the WISC in Predicting Organicity," Journal of Clinical Psychology, XI, No. 2 (April, 1955), pp. 154-58. 155Fujiro Shinagawa, "Studies of the Relationship between Intelligence Structure and Personality Traits: An Analysis of WISC Discrepancy," Japanese Psychological Research, v, No. 2, pp. 55-62, cited in Psychological Abstracts, XXXVIII, No. 4 (August, 1964), p. 651. 156Fujiro Shinagawa, "A Statistical Study of Dis- crepancy between verbal IQ and Performance IQ on WISC," Japanese Journal of Child Psychiatgy, I, pp. 403-411, cited in Paychological Abstracts, XXXVI, No. 2 (April, 1962), p. 3 1. 65 deviate by at least 5.75 weighted score points from the mean of the remaining subtests in order to be significant at the .05 level.157 They conclude, however, that the frequent occurrence of positive studies may be regarded as evidence that analysis of patterns can be meaningful and that something other than the tool might account for the failure of research tg provide consistent and definitive answers.15 They also point out that inferring other personality var- iables from intellectual functioning is really an import- ant avenue to diagnosis, although additional work on "scatter" profiles and patterns has not yet led to more solid "diagnostic ground".159 Lessing and Leasing provide yet another view on the analysis of the WISC aubtest variability. They point out that clinical inference has held that the WISC under- estimates mental ability when the pattern of subtest IQ scores is markedly uneven. In a study in which 188 sub- jects' WISC subtest scores were reviewed, they concluded that subtest variability does not assist in determining if the child's potential and obtained WISC IQ underestimates 157w. H. Guertin, A. Rabin and C. Ladd, "Research with the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Adults: 1955-1960," Psycholpgical Bulletin, LIX, No. 1 (January, 1962), pp. 1-25. 1581bid., p. 19. 159Ib1d., pp. 2-21. 66 that potential. The authors report that the obtained IQ can be considered as the measure of the child's potential ability level.160 For the most part, however, those interested in subtest pattern variability report that potential and obtained WISC IQ's are possible and reflect both intell- ectual and personality variables.161’ 162’ 163’ 16" The most clinically oriented maintain that even each subtest item on standardized intelligence tests can tap wide per- sonality dimensions. Fromm, Hartmann, and Marschak, for example, report that such tests tap reality awareness, reality mastery, sensory perception, integrative behavior, the ego ideal, anxiety (either as an affect or as an ego defense) and ego defenses.165 In an empirical review of the Wechsler, Frank points out that inconclusive and 1603. E. Lessing and J. C. Lessing,"WISC Subtest Variability and Validity of the WISC IQ," Journal of Clinical Psychology, XIX, No. 1 (January, 1963), pp. 92—5. 161J. 0. Field, "Two Types of Tables for Use with Wechsler's Intelligence Scales," Journalnof Clinical Psycho- logy, XVI, No. 1 (January, 1960), pp. 3-7. 162Kenneth D. Hopkins and William B. Michael, "The Diagnostic Use of WISC Subtest Patterns," California Journal of Educational Research, XII, No. 3 (May, 1961), pp. 116-30. 1530. H. Frank, "Empirical Critique of Research with the Wechsler-Bellevue in Differential Psychodiagnosis," Journal of Clinical Psychology, XI, No. 3 (July, 1955), pp. 291495. 16"From, Hartmann, and Marschak, op. cit., p. 142-44. 165Ibid. 57 inconsistentresults from utilization of such a test as the Wechsler-Bellevue is not the "fault" of the instrument, but rather the selection of subjeCts by such criterion measures as psychiatric diagnosis.166 In addition, Hopkins and Michael indicate that those researchers who generalize findings from group comparisons to the individual case, without making adequate provisions for the great increase in differential required for significance with a sample of one become vulnerable to commission of a Type I error. They conclude, "Obviously, before offering diagnostic interpretations, one should be reasonable sure that there is something other than Chance variation to interpret.167 In summary, the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, with its Verbal and Performance Scales and its numerous subtests, provides an opportunity for measuring the articulatory-defective child in both language and non~ language areas. It has been indicated that Children who possess a number of misarticulations also possess some language deficits, and that language deficits may be reflected in complex learning and concept formation difficulties. As Mussen points out Language functions primarily as a means of communi- cation at the outset, but gradually becomes the most 166Frank, op. cit., p. 130. 167Hopk1ns and Michael, op. cit., p. 130. 68 important mediator and regulator of behavior. . . . From the age of five-ang-a-half, almost all new learning involves language.10 In stressing the importance of language, he adds According to the Russian research reports, behavior that is learned with the use of language is acquired quickly, is highly stable, and generalizes widely, whereas reactions learned without verbal participa- tion are relatively unstable, depend on constant reinforcement, and are easily forgotten. Children over five years of age function and control their behavior primarily by means of verbal stimulation; that is, . 6 . mediated generalization or verbal mediation.1 9 In considering language skills and the measurement of intelligence, Mussen concludes that "all useful, valid intelligence tests are highly correlated with, and probably depend on, facility in language."170 What, then, of the child who lacks this "facility in language", this measure of intelligence, at a chronological age level where such facility is taken for granted? Dyslalia and Dyslexia as Related Linguistic Functions Children exhibiting dyslalia, i. e. defective articulation due to faulty learning, also reveal many attributes of the dyslexic child as well as the child 168Paul H. Mussen, The Psychological Development of the Child, (Englewood Cliffs, New Jersey: Prentice-Hall, Inc.), (1963), pp. 45-60 1691bid., p. 46. 1701bid. 69 with specific learning disorders as noted in Chapter I. Weaver, Furbee and Everhart state that speech and reading are closely associated in the linguistic process involving symbolic formulation, evaluation and expression.171 In a study of 638 children at the first grade level, they report that there was a significant and continuous drop in reading readiness as the number of articulatory errors increased. Therconcluded that reading readiness and acquisition of adequate speech are to some extent related, although the proportion of variance common to reading readiness measures and articulation measures is small.172 In another study of first-graders, selected from 54 first grades due to their articulatory disorders, Sommers reports that speech improvement was found to sig- nificantly affect reading skills, as expressed in reading factor scores. However, reading comprehension scores for children with misarticulations and for children with normal articulation were not significantly Changed by speech correction procedures. They indicate that there was one exception. Twenty-five children who exhibited severe art- iculatory defects, defined as 6 or more errors in the 10 consonant sounds used in the study, did show significant changes in both reading comprehension and reading skills, 1710. H. Weaver, Catherine Furbee and Rodney W. Everhart, "Articulatory Competency and Reading Readiness," Journal of Speech and Hearing_Research, III, No. 3 (June, 1960), pp. 174-80. 172Ibid. 70 as well as improved articulatory skills.173 Yedinack reported considerable overlap between children with articulatory and reading disabilities, ind- icating that 40% of a group of second grade "poor readers" also had articulatory defects and 38% of a group of cases identified as having articulatory defects also suffered from reading disabilities.17" Cass found that children with reading problems were deficient in sound-blending, as defined by the 1932 Monroe test, more familiarly known to speech pathologists as synthesis and analysis of phon- emes. While these children revealed no known defects in auditory or visual acuity, they were unable to identify a series of separated sounds which form a word. This deficiency was at the .001 level of significance.175 As in the discussion of articulatory skills, the current research in the area of dyslexia also has a number of proponents who feel that poor or non-readers have a subtle central nervous system disorder. Penn reviews 173Rona1d K. Sommers, et. a1., "Effects of Speech Therapy and Speech Improvement upon Articulation and Reading", Op. cit., pp. 27-38. 174J. G. Yedinack, "A Study of Linguistic Func- tioning of Children with Articulation and Reading Dis- abilities," Journal of Genetic Psychology, LXXIV, lst half, (March. 1949). pp. 23-59. l7SCorrine Kass, "Psycholinguistic Disabilities of Children with Reading Problems," Exceptional Children, XXXII, No. 8 (April, 1966), pp. 533-39. 71 specific dyslexia in medical literature, genetic studies of reading disability, and concludes that the evidence speaks strongly in favor of a majority of Cases . . . (approaching 75 percent) being caused by neurologiclzafélD impairment or neurological maturat- ional delay. In a well-designed study of the test performance of 50 brain-damaged Children ranging in age from 10 to 14 years of age, and matched in pairs with 50 normally functioning children on the variable of age, Reed, Reitan and Hallgrim found that the brain-damaged Children per- formed significantly less well than the controls on all tests, with differences between the two groups occurring more frequently on tests of language function than on other testing procedures.177 Of particular importance was the Wechsler performance of the experimental group. Results indicate that Verbal and Performance total scores, the Total I.Q. score, as well as Information, Comprehension, Digit Span, Arithmetic, Similarities, Block Design and Digit Symbol all were significantly depressed (at the .001 level) for the brain-damaged children. At the .01 level 176Julia M. Penn, "Reading Disability: A Neurological Deficit?" Exceptional Children, XXXIII, No. 4 (December, 1966), pp. 243150. l77Homer B. C. Reed, Ralph M. Reitan, and Klnve Hallgrim, "Influence of Cerebral Lesions on Psychologic- al Test Performance of Older Children," Journal of Con— sulting Psycholqu, XXXX, No. 3 (June, 1965), pp. 247—51. 72 ‘was Picture Arrangement and at the .02 level was Picture Completion.178 Thus, while all of the verbal scale was significantly related to cerebral lesions, some portions of the performance scale were not significantly effected at the .02 or greater level. Proponents of learning disabilities as expressions of inner conflicts point out that "a great many learning disabilities are closely connected with intellectual dys- functioning" and add that learning disabilities are diff- erentiated from general intellectual dysfunctioning only in the fact that learning disabilities reflect intellect- ual inhibition which is limited to scholastic situations only.179 Fromm and Hartman conclude that there is a difference between intellectual lack and intellectual dysfunctioning. The latter is often taken for the former, but should not be. Learning disabilities may be due to a native lack of intelligence. But many learning disabilities are not due to a lack of intelligence; they are a specific form of neurotic dysfunction, a symptom of a total personality disturbance. Their background may be either traumatic, or it may reveal chronically disturbing experiences, preventing ngrmal, anxiety- free development of the individual.1 0 Support for this view is set forth by Blatt, Allison and Baker who report that when Wechsler originally develOped the intelligence 178Ibid., p. 249. 179Fromm and Hartman, op. cit., p. 24. 180Ibid., p. 25. 73 scales, he saw their primary function as a valid and reliable evaluation of global intelligence and only secondarily as a technique for personality assessment. With the development of ego psychology, however, intelligence has been more clearly concept- ualized as an integral part of the total personality structure. One of the basic assumptions in psycho- analytic ego psychology is that cognitive processes, as expressed in a variety of problem-solving situa— tions, reflect important dimensions of personality organization. The Wechsler scales present relatively neutral an highly structure situations, and the scales assess the relative integration and balance of a variety of ego functions such as memory, judgment, anticipation, planning, visual-motor integration, concept formation, attention, and concentration. U In a study of anxiety related to Object Assembly on the Wechsler, the above authors reported that those patients showing anxiety in terms of bodily concern also "scored significantly lower on Object Assembly than on any other Performance item of the Wechsler.182 Griffiths hypothesized that the Digit Span sub— test would be adversely affected by the presence of anxiety, and found that both Digit Span and Information were significantly inferior under the influence of anxiety. Having induced anxiety in 60 college Freshmen by experi- mental means, he reported that the Digit Span interference was interpreted as occurring primarily during perception 181Sidney J. Blatt, Joel Allison, and Bruce L. Baker, "The Wechsler Object Assembly Subtest and Bodily Concerns," Journal of Consulting_Psychology, XXIX, No. 3 (June, 1965):'pp. 223. 182Ibid., pp. 223—30. 74 arui retention, rather than during recall. The nature of tflue interference was found to be intrusion of inapprop- :riate thought, loss of interest, Change of attitude and/or EH1 active psychological withdrawal from the testing sit- nation.183 In a study of the Children's Manifest Anxiety Scale and Performance on the WISC, Hafner, Pollie and ‘Wapner report that correlations between the CMAS and WISC scales and subtests were all negative except for the Picture Completion subtest. They add, however, that only the corr- elations between the CMAS and the Block Design and Coding reached significance at the .05 level, pointing out that these two performance tests are most frequently interpreted Clinically as being susceptible to the influence of anxiety.18+ A study by Matarazzo, with adult subjects, utilizing the Taylor Manifest Anxiety Scale and the Wechsler-Bellevue, failed to reveal any significant relationships, a result which he reports as surprising in view of the long and widely held belief among clinicians that Digit Span especially, and perhaps Object Assembly to a lesser extent, are 183J. s. Griffiths, "The Effect of Experimentally Induced Anxiety on Certain Subtests of the Wechsler-Bellevue," Dissertation Abstracts, XVIII, 1958, pp. 655-66. 182+A. J. Hafner, D. M Pollie, I. Wapner, "The Relationship between CMAS and WISC Functioning," Journal pf _Qlinical Psychology, XVI, No. 3 (July, 1960), pp. 322-23. 75 vulnerable to anxiety.185 Perhaps it is the age differential which significantly «affected these two studies, since there seems to be some (evidence that Children with speech and reading difficulties <30 exhibit significant variability of performance on the idechsler Intelligence Scale for Children. In a study of children ranging in age from 7-0 to 12-9 with no known organic problems, Tjossen, Hansen and Ripley report that "low scores on the Digit Span and (Zoding subtests of the WISC appear as possible indicators of reading difficulty."180 In analyzing the frequency of developmental problems and behavioral signs of these 24 subject who read at "fourteen grades below normal grade placement," they reported the following: Motor awkwardness 10 Nervous Habits 14 Speech problems 14* Allergic Reaction 9 Developmental delay 5— Headaches and stomach aches l3 Accidents 9 *Speech is characterized by substitution and 8 ~omissions of speech sounds and as "immature."1 7 185R. G. Matarazzo, "The Relationship of Manifest Anxiety to Wechsler-Bellevue Subtest Performance " Journal of ConsultingpPsychology, XIX, No. 3 (June, 1955), p. 218. 186Tjossen, Hansen and Ripley, op. cit., p. 1112. lailbldl. p. 1109. 76 It can be seen that 58% of the subjects in the above study revealed articulatory disorders and that the prevalence of the speech disorder was the most commonly occurring devel- opmental problem. Research concerning patterns of WISC subtest scores for poor or retarded readers has specific meaning .for this study, since defective speech and defective read- ing are related factors in the communication or language process. Too, considerable research has been done in the area of reading retardation as related to standardized intellectual measures, whereas little or no literature is as yet available regarding dyslalic children and their sub- test scatter on such instruments. In an exhaustive review (1945-1965) of research concerning patterns of retarded readers on the WISC, Deal reports that in many of these studies the results and conclusions' are difficult to interpret and evaluate. In most of the studies, the inferences have been based on data collected on a small number of subjects. Ways of determining retardation in reading have been varied. Some of the ways used in determining the amount of retardation were: from the expected grade placement level, from the expected level as determined by the results of the WISC, from the level as determined by mental age, and percentage below expecteg grade level as determined by standardized tests.1 8 In summarizing the twelve best studies, she reports that the subtests of Arithmetic, Vocabulary and Digit Span 188Margaret Deal, "A Summary of Research Concern- ing Patterns of WISC Subtest Scores of Retarded Readers," Reading Specialist, (May, 1965), pp. 107. 77 (zontribute heavily to reading success, as do Comprehension, (Object Assembly and Coding. However, low Arithmetic scores ‘were found to be indicative of retarded readers, as were Ilow scores on Information, Coding and Vocabulary. Other subtests were variously reported, with Picture Completion ‘being,both "high" and "low" for retarded readers. Three of the 12 studies reported no significant differences 'between Verbal and Performance IQ, with three others reporting such a difference as existing, in favor of the Performance over the verbal Scale.189 Hirst reports that in comparing "mildly retarded" and "severely retarded readers," the latter were signifi- cantly lower than the former on Digit Span, Vocabulary and Similarities.190 Sawyer reported that while it was possible to distinguish between mildly disabled and sev- erely disabled readers on the basis of WISC subtests, such differentiation was more effective at younger ages.19l While subtest patterns differed, Altus reported that no significant difference was found between Verbal and Performance IQ in a study of 25 children reading two 189Ibid., pp. 101-11. l90Lynn Shellberg Hirst, "The Usefulness of a Two- Way Analysis of WISC Subtests in the Diagnosis of Remedial Readin Problems," Journal of Experimental Education, XXIX, No. 2 December, 1960), pp. 153-60. 191Rita Sawyer, "A Study of Discrimination by the Subtests of the WISC between Mildly Disabled and Severely Disabled Readers Diagnosed at the Syracuse Reading Center, September, 1958 to June, 1963," Dissertation Abstracts, XXVI, No. 5, p. 2594. 78 ;years or more below expected level. However, there were sigmificant subtest differences, with Coding and Arithmet- ic being the lowest.192 DiNello found no reliable differ— ences in WISC verbal and Performance total scales when fibomuma tem Hmsboz pom monoow mamom bmmunsm cosmenoupmm tem Hmnhm> smmzuu.H mmbwmm mummppsm moanEpophmm muwmpnfim Hmnhm> > K6 \0 v0 as Vd 047 .910 A S v 0 I/ 0 so. at TI... 0.... .01. o I. J o u 0. SC. 80 do mo 88 o w t. m J I. £8 to . d at. e . o. d o u . 0 8X. 3 G. u. . or 4 u . . w R V \\ a A 1 \ \ \ 1s m S 0 \ \ \ \ _ a \ \ \ 1.1.... S 9 \ \ 1m m. R \ \ 1m w k L . . [OH 1:” $85on 338qu H. mswxmoam Hmesoz HE 117 .onm on 0:5 mama .msmxmoom mbauomuoo can amssoz pom mmwoom madam unopndm mocmELOMAmm new Hmnpw> cmmznu.m mchHm as w as s s ‘ 1 H. m \ m m “ MN m... x, \ \ \ a a e a a a : a a a \ s s \ mg memam Hmanz H § 118 .01m 0» onm momg .wpmxmmam m>fipomema was Hmsaoz sou moboom canon ummphSm mocmsgoesmm ram Hansmb cmczuu.m mmbon s s e s . s w \ \ .im \ s s s a. a wsmxmoom Hmsgoz H .S 119 .ouma oh man ,. .r n F: .t.’ (L .K.‘. 9.. I1 ) .t . .5; .1 . .C i .. . at I. . .... |. a... , J. . \ .14.). . I . \1 . x a . ,. O .t . . _. mu: all. .v . ,. 0.. v I! . 'O' O ,n 5.. ... 1...- .1 .4 disk .1 o (.... 17m x Lt. H $.5C3 .sc :1. .0 CR0 or. n. H a D.,... .1. or u. 21C .90: six? Manon ash ... H fuss... fr CZ. i r. . 5H... 88.1003 SIC-{OS 1189311113 . ...... s\ ww . s. H. a a a m HM . a a a -3 e \ a \ -. a s \ \ 1. a \ a W H... a. a a “A... ,4 mammal mam H wagon H § i .onHH on onoH mmm< .mpmxmmam m>fieomumm ram awesoz sou mopoom meom unopnsm cosmEsomsoi ram Hmngc> 2mm: us.m mmDon mpmmbhsm cosmssoupom mpmcpnsm Hmngm> '> > 120 m p m“... a. mm m. H searching H s V was V .x V \ Veer \\\ \ \\ \\\i r. a a. a a \_ a a : .oan o» ouHH mmw¢ .mhcxmoaw mbfiuomuom can amenoz sou monoom mamom uncunsm mocmEEOMbmm rem ampsm> and: ua.© mmbon nonsensm anosc> |l> i? monounsm cosmssomucm 121 \\\\\\\\\\\\.\\\V \\ \\ &. ‘\\. V V \ \ a a. msmxmmam. m>fipoouonHI name—moan. HmEsozug \\\\\\\\\\.‘ ‘SSSsc \\ ‘FSSS. ‘FSSS. \\ a “5§§SS§§§§§§E \, a a ‘fi a \\ m / \ 1 V0 MR Vd 0d 80 A S V O I sq T. at. or. d... 0 I. J o u Sn... so do mo 88 o m t. m I. A8 1.0 d UT. 8 . 1. d 0 W fix. . 4 o. u. . H N mm. c. is m 3 m3. 8 mm ..L We “run mo 7% mm HO HH CU H 122 It can be seen that the speech defective group's vocabulary level is significantly depressed below the mean of the experimental group's other Verbal tests when Vocabulary is excluded from the computations. If a significant difference had been found when Vocabulary was included in the statistical treatment of the signifi- cance of the difference between the means, Vocabulary alone would then serve as a predictor of verbal perform- ance rather than the Verbal Scale in its entirety. In view of the significant difference in means of the Verbal Scale of the speech—defective and normative samples, analysis of the difference in means of the verbal and Performance scales of the experimental group alone was attempted, not only for the examination of such a differ- ence, but also in order to derive the directionality of the mean differences and the error of the differences. The results of this analysis are presented in Table VII. TABLE VII ANALYSIS OF DIRECTIONALITY OF VERBAL AND PERFORMANCE SCALE I.Q.‘S FOR THE EXPERIMENTAL GROUP Direction of Proportion Mean Diff.E t Difference of N Differences - Verbar>Performance 13 4.23 6.98 4.8818 Verba1Mean2 and also the possibility of MeanI humasnmoo> cwfimcm xooflm. .aaoo .poam coamccnmhaaoo .osoo manpofim .spg manpOfim hsmasnmoo> . .n.m muncunsm ..<.o ..sfim ..<.m swam pfimflm Homegm pfimfio coapmssomcH ummnwfim omega coapcaasoo .poam hansommd pomwno oHpcsanp¢ mmfiufismafisam mma on m .«.o Ha on s .¢.o Ha on N .<.o ad on N .«.o cm a z mmuz mom a z m: u 2 we“ mama hoapsm Adana pcamcomzv Assam gmamnoozv Amuse poamnoczv nxsmm snowman noccam scenafico .m .2 sosoafino cwmsc>¢ cohoaflno cappuo unopnsm are so mammemsm god nza mean was 20 zmmmqamo msHeommmm mommmm has amassemm squeezes .moammsa .nmemHo mo minced mo mzamz nmszam "I zmmqumo mom fiddom mozmeAAMBZH mmqmmomz N mamVB 146 for dyslexic groups of children. Certain similarities between dyslexic and dyslalic groups now seem to be apparent. Flanary, for example, reported that retarded readers reveal a meager vocabulary, poor planning, and poor conceptual and memory functions.296 Burks and Bruce report that poor readers have significantly low scores on Information, Arithmetic and Coding subtests.297 Altus reports significantly low Digit Span, Vocabulary and -Similarities scores, while Hirst reports significantly 298. 299 low Arithmetic and Coding. There would appear to be some areas of agreement, such as the lowered Information, Arithmetic and Vocabulary subscale mean scores, which, of course, are identical withthose reported in this study of dyslalic children. However, since the studies of retarded readers are so diverse in design and, in general, reflect small N's and simple ranking procedures, interpretation of the results must be cautiously attempted. It is probable our. z- 1'.» 7')- . r"). 296Flanary, 02¢ Cito’ p. 1045‘ 29"Burks and Bruce, op. cit., PP- “88‘93° 298 Altus, loc. cit. 299Hirst, op. cit., pp. 153-60. ii— 147 that there is a small positive relationship between both dyslalia and dyslexia, as reported by Tjossen, Hansen and 300 and that the consistently positive results Ripley, found in terms of Vocabulary depression on the part of dyslexic groups and retarded readers may also be found at a highly significant level with severely disordered speech-defective children. The_results of the present study would tend to support such a hypothesis, if it were restricted to children meeting the criteria of severity of this study. Intratest variability and Personality Functioning. While there has been considerable speculation in the fields of psychology and speech pathology regarding the personali- ty functioning of both the speech defective child and his parents, the utilization of a standardized intelligence test is useful only insofar as the extent and scatter of the test and the qualitative analysis of the protocols provides interpretive data rendered by'a skilled examiner. It will be recalled that scatter was defined as the "inter- relationship of functions underlying the individual's achievement on the various subtests."301 Wechsler himself cautions the use of the test, pointing out that such a scatter, or "configuration" is "both arbitrary and 300Tjossen, Hansen and Ripley, op. cit., PPo 1109-12- 301Fromm and Hartmann, op. cit., pp. 45-6. 148 ephemeral. It should be noted that in test profiling it is essentially the magnitude of the test score rather than their configuration with which the investigator is ostensibly concerned."302 Qualitative interpretation is therefore usually limited to group measures, in order to “m yield statistically significant data. Trapp and Evans utilized the Digit-Symbol (Goding) subtest as a measure of anxiety with 18 children who exhibited severe articulatory disturbances as compared is with children with mild articulation problems, and re- ported that the "severe" group did significantly less well on Coding, and therefore, presumably were more anxietous.303 It will be noted by examining Table v that Coding was not found to be a significantly depressed subtest when means of the experimental and normative groups were examined, nor does Coding appear among the 3 lowest subtests in Table IX, when all subtests were examined for significance of difference between the means on an observed and theoretical probability basis. On the other hand, Coding was found to be the tenth of 11 subtests when ranked by deviation from average total scores, as in Table VIII. Thus, the observed differences 302Wechsler, The Measurement and Appraisal of Adult Intelligence, op. cit., p. 165. 303 Trapp and Evans, op. cit., pp. 176-80. 149 in relative placement may well be a function of the level of statistical complexity applied to the task. Simple ranking would appear to give significantly different results, which may account for the disagreement between the above-described study and the present research. The personality functioning of speech defective children with articulatory disorders was discussed in Chapter II, and it was indicated that a great majority of the studies cited reported that these children differed significantly from children with normal or only mildly disturbed articulatory skills. It may now be said, how- ever, that the statistical bases for these studies was of limited scope and that few studies utilizing the WISC as the instrument are available, other than in the area of Coding and its presumed relationship to anxiety. The results of this study, as seen in Tables IV and V, would appear to indicate that insofar as de- pressed verbal skills may be related to aberrant personali- ty functioning, to that extent this study provides support for a significantly different level of functioning between speech—defective and normal children. The relationship is perhaps more clearly drawn between the significant depression of Vocabulary, Information, Arithmetic, and Digit Span and inadequate school functioning. 150 Since non-verbal skills, as revealed by the Per- formance subtests, are not significantly different from the normative population, it may be assumed that the speech- defective population is not significantly different from the comparable normative group in the areas of "social E“? intelligence" (Picture Arrangement), non-verbal intelligence, E (Block Design), visual acuity, motor coordination and speed (Digit Symbol-Coding), or perception of visual J Gestalts (Object Assembly).3o" 7* If it may be assumed that the speech defective group functions adequately in the above areas, considerable personality strengths may be hypothesized based upon these essentially non-verbal performances. It may be that studies which report that speech-defective children do not reveal personality dysfunction may be tapping these areas, at least in part, while those studies which report deviant personality functioning may be tapping verbal areas of weakness. The results of this study, when compared with the clinical research reviewed in Chapter II, would not appear to reject the null hypothesis of no difference be- tween speech-defective and normal children in personality areas as indirectly measured the Full Scale I.Q. of the WISC. 30"Wechsler, Measurement and Appraisal 0f AdUIt Intelligence, op. cit., pp. 79-83. CHAPTER V SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS While there has been a considerable amount of research over the years in the area of speech and language, and in the technical aspects of identifying and remediat- ing disordered speech and language, comparatively little is known regarding the relationship of functional articu- latory disorders and verbal skills as measured by a standardized instrument which assesses not only several aspects of verbal performance, but some aspects of non- verbal performance as well. Speech pathologists are well aware of the acoustic manifestations of articulatory deviancy, but perhaps much less aware of their meaning within the large, more inclusive Gestalt of language functioning and verbal skills. A review of the research indicates that general intelligence is not a unitary trait but is part of a larger whole, namely, personality. In addition, intellect- ual functioning, language, cognition, and personality are felt to represent a reciprocal interweaving of processes and developmental patterns which may well in- clude specific aspects of speech performance. 151 152 Researchers have been particularly active in the following areas: (1) measurement of the development of articulatory skills, (2) the psychological aspect of articulatory development, (3) intelligence testing varia- bility as related to articulation and language, (4) deviant verbal and written skills, as exemplified by dyslalia and dyslexia, and (5) the influence of parent-child relation- ships upon functional articulatory disorders. However, the research in many of the above areas has been based upon non-standardized tests, inadequate experimental samples, and little or no differentiation of the speech- defective child based upon the degree of severity of the articulation disorder. It was found that considerable confusion reigned regarding the meaning of the simple term, "severe functional articulation disorder." In the present study there has been an attempt made to provide considerably more rigid controls in terms of sample selection, identification of the variables.to be measured, as well as the utilization of a test measure- ment device which was not only standardized, but reliable and valid as well. The purpose of this study was to evaluate the functioning of children exhibiting severe articulatory defects in terms of their verbal skills, based upon the utilization of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, 153 as compared to the performance of non-defective children as represented by the mean performances of the standard- ization groups on the above tests. The following hypotheses were proposed, stated in null form: Would there be significant differences in functioning between speech-defective children and normal children on the Full Scale, Verbal Scale and Performance Scale I.Q. of the WISC? Would there be significant differ- ences in functioning between speech defective children and .2! normal children on the subtest scaled scores? Of particu- lar interest was the vocabulary subtest, since this test would appear to be the subtest most likely correlated with articulatory disorders. Fifty children, ranging in age from 6-0 to 12-0, served as subjects for this study. Subjects' mean age was 8—0; median age was 7-11. All subjects were at grade level and exhibited no known auditory, visual, organic, or central nervous system problems. Children with known "soft" neurological signs were specifically excluded, as were mentally-retarded and gifted children. I.Q. range of the subjects was restricted to 80 to 126 I.Q. Mean Full Scale I.Q. for all subjects was 97, well within the average range of 90-110 I.Q. Subjects were monolingual and from middle-class families. Subjects were diagnosed as poSsessing a functional articulation defect of severe 154 degree, based upon a complete oral-peripheral examination, articulation testing, audiometric screening, and gross and fine motor testing. Speech production was character— ized by a minimum of 5 or more misarticulated phonemes, occurring consistently in at least two of the 3 consonant positions in single word utterances, and by decreased intelligibility. The Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children was administered to the 50 subjects, with 11 of the 12 sub- tests being utilized, Mazes being omitted. Each subject was tested in an examining room 6' by 10', outfitted with appropriately—sized tables and chairs. All distracting stimuli were removed. The examiner recorded all verbal responses of the subjects and noted all motor responses on the WISC protocols. Subjects' responses were tabulated and the raw data for analysis consisted of the sub- scale and total scale raw scores which were converted into standard subscale and full scale scores, following the procedure provided in the Wechsler manual. Appropriate conversions were made when 6, rather than 5, subtests were utilized in the verbal Performance Scale. The raw data consisted of 542 subtest scaled scores and 150 I.Q. scaled scores, which were subjected to a series of tests of significance for the difference between the means of the groups. It was found that there were significant differences between the two populations in re- 155 gard to both verbal I.Q. and several of the subtests, including one subtest of the Performance Scale. Conclusions Within the limitations of the present study, the following conclusions seem warranted: 1. Children with psychogenic speech defects and those with normal speech reveal no differences in perform- ance in "general intelligence" on the Full Scale I.Q. as determined by the Wechlser Intelligence Scale for Children. 2. Children with psychogenic speech defects reveal I.Q. scores that are significantly lower in the area of verbal skills than do children with normal speech. 3. Verbal subtests of the WISC, as achieved by children with psychogenic speech defects, namely, Informa- tion, Vocabulary, Arithmetic and Digit Span, are sig- nificantly lower than those achieved by children with normal speech. However, children with psychogenic speech problems reveal significantly better performance on Picture Completion, a subtest of the Performance Scale, than do children with normal speech who are of average intelligence. 4. There are no significant differences between children with psychogenic speech defects and children with normal speech for the subtests which measure Picture Arrange- ment, Block Design, Object Assembly, and Coding on the Performance Scale of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children. 156 Implications for Future Research While considerable research utilizing the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children has been conducted by those interested in evaluating reading disorders, very little use of this instrument has yet been made in evalua- ting speech disorders. Since reading and speech are aspects of a common modality, the need for such exploration would appear to be evident. In addition, the WISC permits the examiner to explore 10 to 12 areas of functioning within a relatively limited time, usually one hour or less. Specifically, the following areas are suggested for future study: 1. General intellectual functioning of specific sub-groups of speech-defective. s: to - p - - 86 8 2 3 a ‘3 115 .32 .31: 8"; at. a 2" 80’ :0” Sexaaaasaaaamssaaasagusa 1-M 8 9 9 11 7 10 11 11 8 8 8 94 94 93 2-F 9 3 12 9 3 10 14 6 13 6 6 5 93 88 3-F 6 8 10 6 7 11 13 11 8 - 84 106 93 6—0 4-M 13 9 14 8 4 - 15 12 14 12 10 97 118 108 5-M 9 15 6 4 16 6 17 10 5 12 10 96 106 101 to 6-M 10 13 8 12 6 6 l4 9 11 12 7 95 104 99 7—F 10 11 9 11 9 14 9 14 8 11 104 97 101 7—0 8-M 15 12 11 10 7 1 12 18 13 14 13 110 128 120 9-M 1g 16 11 12 14 10 14 16 17 14 18 119 129 126 lO-F 13 9 12 9 9 10 91 90 90 ll-M 11 10 13 11 10 11 11 12 8 10 8 106 99 103 l2-M 14 10 16 11 9 13 13 9 16 9 109 114 112 13-F 11 6 10 6 7 - 7 11 13 12 7 87 100 93 14-F 7 6 11 6 5 14 14 - 12 15 12 89 122 105 15-M 9 11 10 13 10 9 11 9 12 9 10 103 101 102 l6-F g 10 10 a 8 l1 6 g 6 8 12 2 5 8 8—0 17-M 10 9 13 8 5 9 2 4 l8-M 6 11 11 8 11 - 13 12 15 11 15 96 122 109 to l9—M 7 11 10 6 8 8 9 11 12 13 90 106 97 20-M 7 10 8 11 1o 13 8 8 10 7 14 99 96 97 9-0 21-M 13 11 12 10 13 12 14 11 14 12 10 111 115 115 22-F 10 12 8 9 11 8 8 11 9 8 97 93 95 23-M 8 12 12 8 6 12 11 8 9 11 11 97 100 99 24-F 10 11 12 16 6 11 11 10 13 5 10 106 99 103 25-F 10 6 10 lo 10 11 12 12 10 8 9 97 101 99 26—F 5 10 8 9 6 4 6 10 9 8 11 81 92 85 27-M 12 9 7 6 9 7 10 5 90 86 87 8 28—F 7 11 6 12 7 - 10 ll 8 11 10 91 100 95 ‘0 29-F 7 8 8 8 9 13 10 11 12 13 92 106 99 t 30—F ; 10 6 8 8 14 13 10 11 15 92 110 101 0 31-M 13 11 9 10 7 11 9 10 9 97 97 97 32—11 5 6 ,5 7 9 9 7 9 10 9 10 80 93 85 9-0 3 —M g 10 9 10 11 7 l3 l3 8 4 94 94 93 3 -M 10 7 14 ll 13 12 9 7 12 10 104 100 102 35—M 10 13 12 16 13 7 14 14 11 15 - 111 125 120 36—M 5 10 5 9 7 6 9 7 5 5 12 81 83 80 9—0 37-M 11 9 10 14 7 6 10 13 13 15 13 97 120 109 38-M 12 13 10 14 11 12 14 12 12 11 8 113 110 112 t0 39-M 6 7 8 9 10 6 11 7 10 10 9 85 96 89 40-M 7 9 5 8 7 5 14 7 7 8 8 80 92 84 10—0 41-M 6 6 6 10 7 6 Z a 7 9 12 80 90 8 42—M 9 11 7 12 9 6 1 10 13 6 94 99 9 43-M 6 7 9 6 5 8 10 9 11 9 9 80 97 87 10-0 44-M 6 4 11 12 8 11 9 8 10 15 11 l 104 9 to 45—M 7 5 7 10 5 12 10 8 9 10 11 5 97 9 11-0 46-M 5 10 10 7 g 6 9 11 8 7 4 85 8 83 47-11 9 7 13 9 8 13 13 8 12 7 95 10 99 11-0 48—M 6 9 7 7 9 - 7 7 8 7 8 85 82 82 to 49-M 4 g 8 10 g - 11 10 9 g 11 80 100 88 12—0 50-M 10 12 11 1 8 15 10 13 1 15 103 124 114 APPENDIX.B SUMMARY TABLES OF MEAN VERBAL AND PERFORMANCE SUBSCALE SCORES AND S.D.‘S FOR ALL SUBJECTS BY AGE GROUPS 176 .05036 m>ap 105302 0:» mpcmmmpaoa m 0:030 «0:036 m>3uommon 300000 on» mpcmmmaomh d nachos 0.33 3.00 0.0 0.0 0.0 0.03 0.0 0.0 0.0 0.03 0.0 0.03 0.0 0.0 0 0-0 , on 0.0 0.03 0.0 0.0 0.0 0.0 0.0 0.03 0.0 0.0 033 0.03 0.3 0.» 0 0-0 0.03 0.00 0.0 0.0 0.0 3.03 0.0 0.0 0.0 3.03 0.0 0.03 0.0 0.03 m 0-0 on 3.0 00.03 0.0 0.03 3.0 0.0 0.0 0.0 0.3 3.03 0.3 3.03 0.0 3.0 0 0-0 0.03 00.00 0.0 0.0 0.0 3.03 0.0 0.0 0.0 3.03 0.0 0.03 0.0 0.03 0 0-0 on 0.0 30.03 0.0 0.0 0.0 0.0 3.0 0.0 0.0 0.03 3.0 3.03 0.0 0.03 0 0-0 0.0 082 .00 :00: .90 :00: 33.0 one: 0.0 0002 0.0 one: 0.0 000: w W m 0 03000950 .0 .Q .oo> .830 .np3h¢ .0800 .omcH * 3nbpm> abnoonsm 3mnpo>_ IIIILIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIII||||| mmbomc m¢< Mm wmqmzdm M>Hedzmoz 924 m>HBomme Mummmm Mme 2H maomwmbm AQ¢ mom m..m.m 924 mmmoom mq¢owmbm Q Z¢m2 mo Mm30 105302 0:» 0020003003 m 0:030 «0:030 0>3000m0n £00000 0n» 0030003003 0 05030* :.NH 3.0m m.m 0.03 m.m 0.0H m.m 0.03 m.m o.oH N.m N.oH o.m m.m m omma b.» m.H: 0&0 o.m m.m o.m m.H m.m N.m o.m m. o.m :.N 0.0 < OIHH w.ma m.om m.m 0.03 H.m H.03 o.m 0.0H H.m N.oH H.m H.0H m.m m.m m omwa m.: w.mm m.m o.m ®.H w.© H.N m.® o.N 0.0H H.N 0.0 :.H 0.0 4 ouoa m.HH 0.0m m.w m.m m.m 0.0H o.m 0.0H o.m H.0H w.m H.OH m.m m.m m ome H.m o.m: m.m 0.0 ®.H m.m ©.m m.OH m.a m.b H.m m.m N.N o.m ¢ 01m 83.0 :00: .30 :00: .90 000: 33.0 :00: .30 :00: .90 :00: .3320~ 000: mac W m m .0 .0 .8> .230 .0330 .250 .803 ... 03000930 30930> 09009950 H0n30> E.II.IIIIll-IIIJIIIIIIIIIIIIIIII.IllIllillll.l|lu|ll..|||l meomwmbm Had mom m..Q.m Qz< mmmoom mq¢ommbm A¢mmm> Z¢m2 m0 Mm¢SZDw ('1 ill mmbomc mw< wm mmqm£H802mOz Qz< N>Heommmm NOMMmm was 2H .05030 0>30083oz 09» 0000003003 M 03030 «00030 0>3900009 900000 099 0900003003 0 050300 2.0H H.0m H.m 0.0H m.m m.m o.m H.0H m.N 0.0H m.N H.0H m 01m . 09 0.0 m.om «.0 0.03 m.m 0.0 m. 0.0 0.0 0.03 0.0 «.03 ¢ 010 m.m m.om H.m H.0H o.m m.m m.m H.0H m.m H.0H m.m 0.0H m 01w 0» o.w m.om ©.N m.oH o.m m.m m.m ©.OH m.H ©.m ©.N m.OH 4 01b m.m 0.0m m.m H.0H H.m 0.0H b.m 0.0H m.m m.m m.m 0.0H m 01h op m.m m.:m :.H o.m m.m m.OH :.m m.OH m.m m.HH o.m O.NH < 010 .D.m €002 .Q.m 3300—: .Q.m £002 .Q.m 3.300: .Q.m c002 .Q.m 0502 % 0W 0 m .0 .COO .¢.O .Q.m .d.m .o.m * 03000 00008300300 09009930 00003300300 mmbomu mc< Mm mqm£HBOMmMQ mommmm NEE 2H mBthmDm AA¢ mom m..m.m Qz< mmmOom ma¢ommbm moz 905305 099 0900003003 0 03030 “03030 0>3900300 900000 029 0930003 03 0 030300 3.33 0.0: 3.m 0.0 0.m 0.03 3.m m.m 3.m 0.03 m.m 3.03 m 01m3 09 0.33 0.3m 0.m m.33 9.3 0.03 3.m 0.03 9.3 0.0 m.m 0.33 ¢ 0133 m.03 0.0m 3.m 0.03 0.0 0.03 0.m 3.03 3.m 0.0 0.m 0.03 m 0133 09 3.m «.0: ®.m :.m h.m 0.03 m.3 «.0 0.3 m.m m.3 N.03 d 0103 m.33 0.0m 0.0 0.03 0.m 3.03 m.m 0.03 0.m 0.0 0.m 0.0 m 0103 09 m.m 0.00 m.m 0.0 N.m 3.03 ®.N 3.0, m.m 0.0 0.0 0.33 3 Cum . Q. m. Cams 5. m. €002 . Q. m .9002 .Q. m Cam: .9. m. Cam: .Q. m Emmi. 9 V J 3 o o o o o o o o o m % 03000 000 4 0 Q m < m 0 n3 3...... 00005303300 09009930 00305303300 llfillllllllllllllllllrLll mmbomc m0¢ Mm quzdm m>HB¢£mOz 924 MPHaommwQ mOMMmm mus zH WHOMhmDm 33¢ mom m..m.m 92¢ mmmoom m3 mfiflwfi CH 03w wwofiflhmkhfig Cam: CS“ wmQOOm ohhmm CG“ Hwnhw> pr09 "@802 181 30 303 m03 om ; 3 303 30 mm 00 003 00 m: 3 mm 003 30 m 00 mm 03 m 003 30 mm m 303 mm 33 3 mm 30 mm 0 mm mm 03 : m33 333 30 03 30 mm m: m 00 mm 00 M3 {303 30 33 03 003 00 03 33 30 00 m: 00 003 00 03 m 00 30 m: 3 mm 00 33 03 00 00 33 3 mm mm 03 03 mm 00 03 0 303 m03 m3 33 00 mm mm mm 003 00 33 m 033 m33 0m m 003 30 m3 mm 003 3m 3m 0 333 003 m3 0 mm 30 mm m m03 003 33 :3 003 333 mm 3 00 30 03 a 003 303 :m 03 003 033 m 0 :0 :0 mm 03 003 033 0 M3 mm 00 mm 3 30 303 3 0 30 3m 3m 0 303 mm 0 03 033 mm om 03 003 00 m :3 003 mm mm 30 033 30 0 m 003 30 00 mm 003 30 m 3 00 00 30 0 mm mm m 0 mm 30 00 0 am :0 3 0a> 0y> 0A> .0.3 .0.3 0000 0n> 0y> mn> .0.3 .0.3 0000 .830330m Hmnhm> .EhOhhmm Hmnhm> 3 mbomc 3.333.232.3ng 333.3. mom 910.3 mozflzmommmm 32¢ 333mg; .330 EH3 "09:0 .008 3.033 n.3300 T008 0.30 n.30> 333:0 .008 0.00n.0300 3.008 0.00n.30> u¢ezn .008 m.m0u.0300 0008 0.33 I30> “38:0 03: 0000 93030 .33 0 n 003000 00030 .33 0 n 003000 30030 .03 3 - 0390809334 .03 m n 003000 .0 3 - 00390830303 .0 m - 0300000333 .0 00-03 m - 003000 .0 00-0 m - 00390830303 .3 m - 0000 93030 .3 3 - .33¢ .9030 .0 09 o 030309000> .0 09 0 330309000> .m 0 .0800 .9030 .m m + 33080000 .090 .3 0-0 3 + 03980000 900390 .3 0:0 3 + 0030000030800 .m m + 003933033830 .0 03+ .0800 .9030 .0 3 + .33¢ .9030 .0 03+ 0039330333830 .3 m + 0030000030800 .3 0 n .330 .9030 .33 0 n 003000 .33 m - 3303090000 .03 0 n 003933033830 .03 m - 33000003 .300 .0 m - 330300000> .0 3 - 003930033030 .m 00-0 3 - 33050003 000300 .m 00-3 3 - 00390830303 . o 0000 93030 . 3 - 0000 93030 .0 09 0 039080933¢ .0 09 0 .0800 .9030 .m 0 .0800 .9030 .m m + 003000 30030 .3 0-3 m + 00390830303 .3 0:0 3 + 0030000030800 .0 m + 003000 #0033 .m 3 + 0390809330 .0 0 + 0030000030800 .0 0 + 003000 .3 0 + .330 .9030 .3 009002 30 03000 0000 000 00900: 30 03000 300m 000 30909 .>3 8030 .>00 9009000 30908 .>< 8030 .>00 9009000 m30z¢0 3¢Bzm£Hmm0NM 0000000 30¢ KHm mmB mom 030000 30¢ Emma mu¢mm>¢ 20mm ZOHB¢H>33 N3 030000 8038390 00H3 mma mo OZHMZ¢K 184 .008 m.mm3n.330m 3.008 0.0m3u.30> 3 30020 00- 0000 93030 .33 03- 0030000030000 .33 om- 00390830303 .03 m3- 003000 .03 33- .33< .9033 .m m - 00390830303 .0 m - 0030000030800 .W 30-03 3 - 330300000> .m 00:33 3 - 0390009330 . 3 - 003000 00030 .3 3 - 003000 00030 .0 09 3 - .330 .9033 .0 09 0 33000003 .300 .0 m + 0000 93030 .0 0 330300000> .3 0-33 m + 003933033830 .3 0-03 m + 003933033030 .0 0 + .0000 .9030 .m 0 + .0000 .9030 .0 03+ 33000000 .390 .0 .03+ 003000 .3 00+ 0390809330 .3 00900: 30 03000 0000 00¢ 009002 30 03000 0000 . 000 30909 .30 8033 .>00 9009000 30909 .30 8033 .300 9009080 'l" 'll‘ll' itiii'iiil‘l’ m00z¢m 0< 20mm ZOHB¢H>M0 Mm wmmoom Emmembm omH3 fire 00 wZHMzHDGm mo< Emma A A # om :HH :mH ooH mmfl mad moH :oH om E moH mm moa Hod Hm A mmH mm mm m» mm mm oHH mm om :m mm 0» mm om o-m mmH oHH om ooH om HoH m» om mm :m mm m» mm ma :ma ooH mma mm :HH NHH an :m op am mm on mm ma op mm mm m» we moH mu on mm mm mm om mp p» H 00H :5 mm on mm m» mu am mm mm mm mu m» ma 0.» mm om ooH mp om mm m» mm mm mm om m» mm ma oofi mmfi :OH I: oma oHH mad mm mm mm mm on :5 :H :mm spofi opofi omHfi omHH mmoa o:m omm mmm mmm whoa mzoa mmofi..poa 0» aw mm mm mm m» In mm mm mm mm mm mm ma m» mma mu mm mod ooH on o» om moa mm moH om ma a» mm mm am om om om :m om :m mm mm om Ha on om mm :5 o» H» mm om mm 0» mm o» :5 ca m» oHH mza :HH :HH mHH m» «OH om gm :HH moH mm m o-~ ooH :HH moH me om moH oHH mm m» mm om moH mm m zm mm 0» mm mm m» mHH on mm on mm mu mm 5 mm mm :5 op mp :5 mm mm mm mm om c» on m o» z» mm mm mm mzfi mm mm moH mm mm mm o» m» m :5 mm moH :m :HH Hm nu mm mm :m mm em N» z .I mm 2. mm mm E. mm mm mm i mm mm mm m 0.0 mm mm :m mm mm mu on mm me :u mm on 50 m mm mm mm m» mm mm mm mm mm mm 0» mm mm H w wm mm mm mm. Wm «m M m m w m. 3 mm mm 0.. Sn... 80 J0; m0 as 83 O m .L. m J as mo. 08 Tk rue 7&0 . 1. Aunq 1. u.& E . 4+ .0 0 4+ OK; n u . 0 WM. n . n ...... O. U. . . 89 d. S o, J a . . do 8 3 a a... mocmspompmm awnam> fl lIIIIIJIrIIIIllIHIIIIIII 187 mmm mmm omm mmoH 3mm Hmm mmm omm mom on mom :mp mpm .uoe nu «ma omH mam mmH :ma mp «NH mmH :HH mmH ooH mHH mm mom omH mp mm :HH HoH :mH moH mmH mm mm mm pod mm mm mm mm mma mmH :oH mp mam om moH mom mm mm mm oum mm mm moH mm mp mm om no op op mp op mp mm mm mm mm mm moH mm mp mm om moH :HH mp mm am on omH nHH moH mma om :HH opH om mm mm mm mm moH om :HH mma :HH moH om mom omH mm mm mm om mm mm mm oum mm :HH mm «NH :HH poH .1 mp mmH mm mom mm mm mm mm mm mm mp om am mp om mm mm om mom mm pm mmmH mafia mmma mmoa momH zomH mpoa :Hoa mmoH mama mmHH mmoa omofl. .poa no op mp mm :p om mm mm mp op mm mm mm mm mp op. mm mm om :m om mm mm mm mm mm om mm o-m mm mm :HH :m om mm om mm mmH mm om mm mm mm 0» mm moa mm mm moH mm mHH mm op mom mop mm- om mm -p mm mp mm oofl mp mm mp- map mm. mm moH mm, Hm mm o m mm mm mm mm Wm mm m. m m m .m mm mm. mm P So} Snu Tao may «as 913 O m .L m .J an: m.H o.a Tr pus 7&0 . 1. .an+ 1. u.& a . 1. d o “4 05} n U . 0 W]. n . n n+ q U. . . 38 d 3 1. J J . . JO 8 9 a a... mocpromgmm Hmnhm> mmmx¢Mmm m>H90mmMQ mom mmezoz Mm omH3 mme m0 mammambm zo mezmA¢>HD®m mu< BmMB u HmpoB 188 «NNH mNHm 3mm: ommm mHmm mmmm ommm «mm: Hmmm omm: mom: om.z momma om: mHH mHH mpm mm: mpm N omm opm mmm mHm mpm .poe ooH ooH omH mmH omH mmH omH :mH HHH :mH mmH om o-«H «HH mNH HNH mmH «:H om mmH mHH moH «m moH m: on mHH NoH «HH ooH «oH «NH NoH «oH mNH «oH pmH m: ouHH ram omw «mm 0:0 m:m oom mom 0mm «0: :m: pmm .uos :oH mmH moH :pH mmH :«H :HH mmH mm mHH pHH p: moH mNH :HH :oH mNH moH om mNH mNH mm «oH m: o-HH mop mNH :HH moH mNH Nm mNH NoH mp NoH oHH m: on :mH omH mNH :oH :HH oHH omH mmH «m :m mHH a: onoH mHH mHH omH :HH mNH mp «m :HH mm mm mm m: :pp #:m mzp oom :pm mop mam 0mm mmp 0mm mop .909 «m NoH mm moH mp om :HH mm «m mp mm «H NmH mmH :HH ooH mmH oHH mmH mm mNH moH poH H: o-oH mm mm mm «m mmH om om Nm moH mm mm oz moH mHH :HH No mNH mHH NoH «oH om mmp mm mm on mo mNH oHH mmH mmH «NH omH :HH NHH mNH HmH mm «mH NmH mmH «mH :HH om omH HHH moH mHH moH pm oum mHH mm «m «m NoH om NoH «m moH mp mm_ mm 0 V0 Him Vd 9d A cu V nu I VL NS V m. m m m m. m m. mu m m. m m m. m m mm. mm m. wuw Trm. . m..1mM w. . 1. .d 0 0+ 05} n Ha n+ .w J a . m. . . an“ .M a a Q mOCmBeHOhhwm Hmflhm> mmmxamm m>HBommmm mom mmezoz Mm omH3 MPH. m0 memmEumSm 20 mezmq<>HD®m mad Emma. "an N“ All! m"