AN INVESTIGATIGN 0F ROLE- GRIENTATIUN AND REFERENCE-GROUP IGENTIFICATIUN IN SYSTEMS DF BROADCAST COMMUNICATION Thesis for the Degree of Ph. D. MICHIGAN STATE UNIVERSITY - 9mm 0. BECKWITH 1968 TIM-Ila 0.169 LIBRAR 1» Michigan State nivcrsity This is to certify that the thesis entitled AN INVESTIGATION OF ROLE-ORIENTATION AND REFERENCE-GROUP IDENTIFICATION IN SYSTEMS OF BROADCAST COMMUNICATION Date presented by Gerald C. Beckwith has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for Ph . D . degree in Communication Major professor r‘K Julx 26, 1968 9’ amDING BY 1 l‘IUAG 81' rSIlNS' II IQUEAEII‘ AIMEE _ _ h .. . ‘.,_.LCHISA!_ ABSTRACT AN INVESTIGATION OF ROLE-ORIENTATION AND REFERENCE-GROUP IDENTIFICATION IN SYSTEMS OF BROADCAST COMMUNICATION by Gerald C. Beckwith The present study was devised in an effort to develop theoretically sound procedures for investigating certain critical aspects of role in systems of mass communication. The investigation focused upon the elements of role—orienta- tion and reference-group identification in systems of broad- cast communication. The theory devised for the study was abstracted from recent studies of behavior and attitude in formal organizations and was structured in terms of certain aSPeCts of the theory of action described principally in the work of Talcott Parsons. Data for analysis was obtained from a national sample of c ' - - ommerCial broadcasters, including station managers, sales mana - - gers, program directors and news directors, both radio and tel ' ' eV1Sion. The sample Was developed by processes of near- rand - om selection. Data was obtained by means of a self— adm' ' . . lnlstered mail questionnaire. A total of 247 useable return S Were tabulated, representing approximately 69 percent of - the questionnaires distributed. Gerald C. Beckwith Two measures were devised for inclusion in the questionnaire. The first was a Role-Orientation Measure consisting of 38 forced-choice items, each of which was substantively related to one or another of five significant "objects of orientation" identified with the broadcaster's working environment. The items were also designed to re- flect one or another of the so—called pattern-variables, or "dilemmas of choice," which had previously been described by Parsons. The second measure devised for the study was the Reference—Group Identification Measure. This consisted of Ia set of 16 scales, each requiring the subject to respond to a named reference-group in terms of the group's relative importance as a source of influence on the subject's own job performance. Thirteen variables were examined in relation to the results obtained from use of the measures identified. These Were classified in three sets, as background variables, sys- tem variables and test or behavior variables, and included such factors as system position, role—function, education, age and experience; station type and location, system size and type of ownership; job loyalty, system tenure, member— ship status, attendance at broadcast-related organization aneetings, and activeness in organization affairs. Three study propositions governed the focus of the study in its empirical aspects. These were (1) the system pcmition proposition, which held that the individual's Gerald C. Beckwith role—orientation is, in part, a function of his position in the system, (2) the system size proposition, which held that the individual's role-orientation is, in part, a function of system size, and (3) the reference-group identification pro- position, which held that the individual's role-orientation is, in part, a function of his reference-group identifications. Related to each of these propositions were a set of five principal hypotheses and five alternative hypotheses, each of which was constructed in terms of one or another of the five pattern-variables. The Role-Orientation Measure and the Reference-Group Identification Measure were both subjected to factor analysis as a means of operationalizing types of role-orientation and reference-group identifications for testing purposes, and as a means of judging the relative independence of the theorized pattern-variables. Seven modes, or types, of role-orienta- tion were identified by this process, each of which was rep- resented by a particular factor. Each identified orientation factor was taken to be suggestive of a certain point of view or manner of relating to the situation maintained by differ- ing numbers of broadcasters. The seven obtained orientation factors were named as follows: Factor I was identified with utilitarianism and was assumed to reflect a "goal—centered" role-orientation focus; Factor II was identified with inter-personalism and was assumed to reflect a people—centered" role-orientation focus; Factor III was identified with instrumentalism and Gerald C. Beckwith was assumed to reflect a "job-centered" role-orientation focus; Factor IV was identified with altruism and was assumed to reflect a "service-centered" role—orientation focus; Factor V was identified with aestheticism and was assumed to reflect a "product-centered" role-orientation focus; Factor VI was identified with individualism and was assumed to reflect a "self—centered" role-orientation focus; Factor VII was identified with parochialism and was assumed to re— flect a "situation-centered" role—orientation focus. Four primary reference factors, and one incidental reference factor, were clearly identified in the process of factoring the Reference-Group Identification Measure. Ref- erence Factor I was named a Career-Set factor; Reference Factor II was named a Client-Set factor; Reference Factor III was named a Rule-Set or Power-Set factor, and Reference Factor IV was named a Primary—Group factor; Reference Factor V, which was minor in importance, was named a Value-Set factor. For testing purposes, following the identification of the obtained factors, scores were derived for each sub- ject on each named factor. Subjects were then classified as "high," "medium" or "low" scorers. Chi-square procedures were used as a means of testing for significant differences between groups. The independent variables employed for this purpose were the previously described background, sys- tem and behavior variables. The dependent variables were Illivl -‘v-‘w Gerald C. Beckwith subject factor scores for each of the identified role- orientation factors and reference-group factors. The ref- erence-group factors were also employed as independent variables in relation to the several role-orientation factors. The results of the study were interpreted in terms of two primary considerations, one theoretic and the other empirical. From the point of View of theory, the study clearly revealed that the concept of role-orientation, as operationalized, had distinct value. It was demonstrated that groups of broadcasters could reasonably be differen- tiated in terms of modes, or types, of role-orientation. It was also demonstrated that types of reference-group identifications could be derived from subject responses that could be meaningfully interpreted. The value of the pattern- variable, as described in the theory of action, however, was not found capable of demonstration. It was observed, rather, that the pattern-variables, as operationalized, did not main— tain the theoretic level of independence required for use empirically. Only two of the obtained factors, Orientation Factor II and Orientation Factor IV, could clearly be identi- fied in pattern-variable terms. These results, however, were not viewed as conclusive evidence of a general lack of validity for the pattern-variable formulation. From an empirical point of view the following results were obtained. It was found that the broadcaster's position in the system was not fully controlling on his role-orientation. Gerald C. Beckwith Broadcasters who maintained economic-related role-functions (station managers and sales managers) and those who main- tained service—related role—functions (program directors and news directors) could not be differentiated in terms of either role-orientation or reference-group identifica- tions. It was, however, found that position in system, taken without regard to role-function, exercises a signifi- cant influence on the broadcaster's role-orientation in certain fundamental respects, relative to instrumentalism (p = <.02) and individualism (p = <.05) particularly. The results obtained with reference to the system size proposition were generally supportive. It was found that broadcasters located in different size systems tended to maintain different role—orientations. Large system broadcasters were found to score higher in affectivity (p = <.05) and to be more collectivity-oriented (p = <.Ol) than broadcasters located in small systems, as determined by relative scores on Orientation Factors II and IV. Small system broadcasters, on the other hand, were found to be more job-centered (p = <.02) and less goal—centered (p = <.02) than broadcasters located in large systems, as determined by relative scores on Orientation Factors III and I. The reference—group identification proposition was not generally supported by the study results. Broadcasters who scored high on given types of reference factors did not differ in role—orientation from broadcasters who scored low i Gerald C. Beckwith on the same reference factors. The one principal exception to this rule occurred in regard to the Client-Set factor. Broadcasters who scored high on this factor tended also to score high on affectivity (p = <.02) and parochialism (p = <.02) and relatively low on instrumentalism (p = <.01). Other results obtained from the study indicate that broadcasters located in stations that were group-owned or in stations owned by newspapers or other press-related organizations tended to be more collectivity-oriented, or service-centered, in their orientations to role (p = <.Ol) than were broadcasters located in stations that were inde- pendently owned. It was also found that television broad- casters tended to be more collectivity—oriented than radio broadcasters generally (p = <.Ol) and more affectivity- oriented, or people-centered (p = <.01). Television broad- casters, in addition, appeared to be somewhat more goal- centered and less job-centered (p = <.05) than radio broadcasters, tending toward greater utilitarianism and less instrumentalism. It was also observed that broadcasters who occupied different positions within the system differed in their orientations to role depending upon their location in either television or radio stations. Differences of this order were also discovered, with regard to one or another of the orientation factors, when the education and experience levels of broadcasters were tested. To a degree, too, Gerald C. Beckwith differences in station location resulted in certain observed differences in role-orientation. Finally, it was found that observed differences in the role-orientations of broadcasters were significantly related to certain differences in behavior, including sys- tem loyalty, system tenure, the broadcaster's membership status in broadcast-related organizations, his attendance at meetings and his activeness in association or organiza- tion affairs. The relationship between role-orientation and role-behavior, to this extent, therefore, was supported. djiwfloea .O P' neieiiib eisiiib I ed: sis: 193 T h ‘ I o y' - : a ’W’WW’W‘I '- ,. I . ' 1 L " 3'1, v‘t .U . t ‘1‘)”; TAT-lg; a ’. 1.? {R |o .HI I ~ ’ ‘ ,. A a l " ' -“| y ' 4 'D a v | , ‘ | F “‘!-L I Q ~ " ~ - ‘_ s, , . . . '.-a , 9 v . I v ‘ - - lu‘ . ‘ I ,' r u I O .Wsmranou or ROLE-ORIENTATION ma» . ’ _- gg‘ffx- " hammer—snow IDENTIFICATION IN I SYSTEMS or BROADCAST COMMUNICATION BY .ggora' ' Gerald CV Beckwith \ A THESIS Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements ’ for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Communication 1968 . I . y‘ ' V.. 1.2”. a} _ .‘v 1 I I .' ‘IN " #1: ‘ " T ‘ . : II urn. ‘ ' I ' I"~‘ 2W! k. MI” :3" I I " I ‘ II ‘ ‘ ‘ SEAS} I I . ' LII}I " ' I I“ l' I ‘ ‘l V. I ' 'fn . . 3'3”?" ibzrfifi‘| . ."D '73:". II \c. LII . | , .’ .1 ' . 1 -' ‘,I In} ‘ I \II I 'v' 6‘ H ‘33“? I ‘1 . r “l I‘ . . ."‘ -, A. . . 0' .1: ' I ’ I.» to»; “I QCopyright by: Gerald Clifford Beckwith 1969 ii ‘V I ' F':‘ w— , 4;}. .~ . f5. ‘ ‘ 1‘ . a 'I . h “Pm I) _ \"1Iw 1 u‘ I »- -‘ . ' - y 5 -0! . . IN I.’ 1 VJ. I‘ W , I'. . ,4 In».- M-.. .3th . In.» I; -. w .‘ ,- ‘rv' “I‘M. ’ “ " ’ , , ’ U‘v‘s. . .fl .. . '3'. .., n" 2' , j . ytt‘uI 4-} L . "It. '0, It.“ . \ "VS TI .._ _s x " ‘- '.' I. ’ “Q. V.‘ ‘.:}L '1‘ ‘r ‘P K-I ‘ v -' . ‘ l. ?«a- .4” i I e 7* “If ' Q. v: I Fifi-:I‘Ifi' ‘ \ t we}: 1'>: S. A II ‘4 t '3gceptsd by the- faculty of the Department- of Com- . :‘ii r a 3 fimfi College of Communication_Arts, Michigan State .‘ If; ' ’ ' igyi in partial fulfillment of the requirements for; , '" tor of Philosophy degree. mix Director of Thesis ”a -. a. shoe Committee: ' , Chairman , V -‘ " PREFACE The present study had its beginnings in a paper pre— pared while the writer was a student in a seminar in mass communication theory conducted by Dr. Malcolm S. MacLean, Jr., during the Winter of 1960. That paper represented an attempt to devise a conceptual model of the mass communica- tion process. In its preparation, however, it soon became apparent that one of the key elements in this process could not adequately be described. This was the role of the mass communicator. Little was known concerning the general role of the mass communicator; still less was known concerning the role of the broadcaster as a representative type of mass communicator. The idea of making some effort to fill this obvious gap in the literature on mass communication grew over time and resulted in the present study. Without Dr. MacLean's encouragement and early assistance, then, this study might never have been developed. Many other persons have, of course, contributed sub- stantially to the study, both directly and indirectly. As thesis advisor, Dr. Hideya Kumata offered the writer the benefit of his considerable insight and understanding of iv the complex problems involved in any attempt to construct a theoretically sound procedure for studying the role of the American broadcaster. Professor Leo Martin's keen awareness of the issues involved also contributed greatly to the final product. The writer also wishes to acknowledge the assistance of Dr. David Berlo, who, as chairman of the department, pro- vided the research project with some much needed support as well as the means of conducting the computer analysis re- quired. Dr. Charles P. Loomis, in addition, is to be thanked for providing necessary encouragement in the utilization of the theoretic formulation of the pattern-variable, and Dr. Bruce L. Smith, for his many helpful suggestions on the preparation of the manuscript. The writer also wishes to acknowledge the valuable assistance of A1 Talbott, Jeff Katzer and Anita Immele of the Communication Research Center, who provided much needed statistical and programming support. Finally, and most particularly, the writer wishes to extend a kind word of thanks to the many professional broad- casters throughout the country who participated in this study. Theirs was a contribution truly beyond measure. TABLE OF CONTENTS Page PREFACE o I o O o l o o o o o o I o o o I o I o c o iV LIST OF TABLES . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . ix LIST OF FIGURES . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . xi LIST OF APPENDICES . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . xii CHAPTER I. INTRODUCTION . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 Statement of the Problem . . . . . . . . 1 Media as Organization. . . . . . . . . . 4 Media as Social Systems . . . . . . . . 5 The Concept of Role. . . . . . . . . . . 6 Role-Behavior Variance . . . . . . . . . 9 The Concept of Orientation . . . . . . . 15 Summary . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 22 II. DEVELOPING THE THEORETIC RATIONALE . . . . 24 Focusing the Study . . . . . . . . . . . 24 Theoretic Rationale . . . . . . . . . . 26 The Theory of Action . . . . . . . . . 28 Concept of the Pattern Variable . . . 29 Orientation Defined Conceptually . . 34 Orientation Defined Operationally . . 36 Modes of Orientation . . . . . . 38 Developing Typological Systems . . . 40 Developing a System of Modes of Orientation . . . . . . . . . . . 45 Primary Study Propositions . . . . . . . 49 System Position Proposition . . . . . 50 System Size Proposition . . . . . . . 54 Reference-Group Identification Proposition . . . . . . . . . . . 58 vi T CHAPTER Page Secondary Study Propositions . . . . . . 62 Personal Factors . . . . . . . . . . 63 System Factors . . . . . . . . . . . 63 Role-Behavior Hypotheses . . . . . . . . 64 Study Limitations . . . . . . . . . . . 66 Summary . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 68 III. INSTRUMENT DEVELOPMENT . . . . . . . . . . 70 Background . . . . Item Construction . . . . Scoring Procedure . . . . . . . . . . 76 Item Selection . . . . . . . . . . . 78 Pre- -Testing the Instrument . . . . . 81 Content Validation . . . . . . . . . . . 84 Instrument Reliability . . . . . . . . . 87 Item Distribution . . . . . . . . . . . 88 Summary . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 90 IV. SAMPLE DESCRIPTION AND STUDY PROCEDURES . . 91 Population Description . . . . . . . . . 91 Sampling Procedures . . . . . . . . . . 92 Sample Description . . . . . . . . . . . 94 Study Procedures, Problems and Limitations . . . . . . 107 Description of the "Non-Respondent" . . 111 Summary and Conclusion . . . . . . . . . 114 V. DATA ANALYSIS, PART I . . . . . . . . . . . 116 Correlation Analysis . . . . . . . . . . 116 Factor Analysis . . . . . . . . . . . . 124 Reference-Group Measure . . . . . . .- 125 Orientation Measure . . . . . . . . . 130 Factor Scores . . . . . . . . . . . . . 143 VI. DATA ANALYSIS, PART II . . . . . . . . . . 146 Testing the Primary Study Propositions . 148 System Position Results . . . . . . . 149 System Size Results . . . . . . . . . 154 Reference-Group Results . . . . . . . 158 Testing the Secondary Study Propositions . . . . . . . . . . . 161 Role- Behavior Test Results . . . . . . . 174 CHAPTER Page Controlled Variable Test Results . . . . 178 Controlling System Size . . . . . . . 178 Controlling System Position . . . . . 184 The Influence of Education Level . . . . 188 VI I . CONCLUS IONS O O O O I I C I D I O C O I O O 19 4 Theoretic Considerations . . . . . . . . 194 Empirical Considerations . . . . . 197 Recommendations for Further Study . . . 208 APPENDICES O O I O I I O O O O D I I I I I O I I O 211 BIBLIOGRAPHY I c o o o c o I O o I I o o c o O c o 311 -o—i- - viii Table 3.1 3.2 3.3 4.1 LIST OF TABLES Sample responses to forced choice item . Combinations of objects of orientation . Distribution of test items by number . . Distribution of stations in the population by geographical regions . . . . . . . Distribution of stations sampled by geo- graphical regions . . . . . . . . . . Composition of the sample, by position, station type and geographical region, indicating the number of requests for study participation in each category and the number of favorable returns received . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Composition of the sample, by position, station type and geographical region, based upon the total number of ques- tionnaires returned . . . . . . . . . Composition of the final study sample, by position, station type and geo- graphical region . . . . . . . . . . A comparison of the primary sample and the control sample returns . . . . . A comparison of certain personal charac- teristics of respondents . . . . . . A comparison of certain general system characteristics . . . . . . . . . . . Field modification of the primary sample ix Page 84 86 89 96 97 98 100 102 104 106 108 110 Table Page 5.1 Background and behavior variables: inter-item correlations . . . . . . . 118 5.2 System variables: inter—item correlations . . . . . . . . . . . . 118 5.3 Reference factor solution . . . . . . . 127 5.4 Role—orientation factor solution . . . . 131 5.5 Summary identification of orientation factors 0 O I O O O O O I I O O O O O 143 6.1 Chi-square results relating background variables and orientation factors . . 150 6.2 Chi—square results relating system variables and orientation factors . . 155 6.3 Chi-square results relating reference- factors and orientation factors . . . 159 6.4 Chi—square results relating orientation factors and system behaviors . . . . 175 6.5 Controlled variable chi-square results with size of system controlled . . . 182 6.6 Controlled variable chi-square results with position in system controlled . 186 6.7 Controlled variable chi—square results with station type and experience controlled and level of education varied . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 190 6.8 Controlled variable chi-square results with education controlled and role- function varied . . . . . . . . . . . 193 LIST OF FIGURES Figure Page 1. Unit of Social Action . . . . . . . . . . 28 2. System Orientation Patterns . . . . . . . 46 xi Appendix A. B. C. D. LIST OF APPENDICES Study Participation Request Forms . Rules of Order for Sample Selection Study Questionnaire . . . . . . . . Correlations of Background, System and Test Variables . . . . . . . Reference Measure: Inter—Item Correlations . . . . . . . . . . Orientation Measure: Inter—Item Correlations . . . . . . . . . . Reference Measure: Factor Loading Matrix Orientation Measure: Factor Loading Matrix . . . . . . . . . . . . . Reference Measure: Rotated Factor Loadings . . . . . . . . . . . . Orientation Measure: Rotated Factor Loadings . . . . . . . . . . . . Orientation Measure: Item Means and Standard Deviations . . . . . . . Orientation Measure: Item Analysis . Reference Measure: Factor Score Coefficients . . . . . . . . . . Orientation Measure: Factor Score Coefficients . . . . . . . . . . Reference Measure: Roster of Factor Scores . . . . . . . . . . . . . Orientation Measure: Roster of Factor SCOreS I I I I I I I I I I I I . Chi-Square Results: Reference Factors Shown as Dependent Variables . . Page 212 220 224 237 244 247 253 256 265 268 272 276 284 286 289 299 309 CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION Statement of the Problem For a period of years the study of communication has followed the pattern suggested by Harold Lasswell in his now famous formulation, "who says what in which channel to whom with what effect?"1 Of the various elements con— tained in this formulation, however, the first element, the "who," has received less than its full measure of attention, at least so far as its application to the study of mass communication has been concerned. With the exception of a wide range of biographical studies, personal remembrances and fan magazine "profiles," few attempts have been made to investigate the mass commu- nicator in his natural setting. Rosten's early studies of the Washington correspondent and the Hollywood movie maker are somewhat unique in this regard,2 as is the later work lHarold Lasswell, "Communication Functions," in Lyman Bryson, ed., The Communication of Ideas (New York: Harper and Brothers, 19487, p. 37. 2Leo Rosten, The Washington Correspondents (New York: Harcourt, Brace and Co., 1937). Leo Rosten, Holly— wood: The Movie Colony and the Movie Makers (New York: Har- court, Brace and Co., 1941). '12. in. - of Hortense Powdermaker,3 Charles Swanson,4 and Warren Breed.5 Other works of a closely related nature include White's famous study of the "gatekeeper,"6 Gieber's inves— tigation of the editing practices of a group of wire edi- tors,7 Pool and Shulman's analysis of possible bias among a group of newsmen,8 and the work of Donahew,9 Gieber,10 3Hortense Powdermaker, Hollywood: The Dream Fac- tory (New York: Little, Brown and Co., 1950). 4Charles Swanson, "Midcity Daily: The News Staff and Its Relation to Control," Journalism Quarterly, Vol. 26 (Winter, 1949), pp. 20-28. 5Warren Breed, "Social Control in the News Room," in W. Schramm, ed., Mass Communications (Urbana, Ill: Univ. of Illinois Press, 1960), pp. 178—195. 6David White, "The 'Gatekeeper': A Case Study in the Selection of News," Journalism Quarterly, Vol. 27 (Fall, 1950): PP. 383-390. 7Walter Gieber, “Across the Desk: A Study of 16 Telegraph Editors," Journalism Quarterly, Vol. 33 (Fall, 1956). pp. 423-432. 8Ithiel de Sola Pool and Irwin Shulman, "Newsmen's Fantasies, Audiences, and Newswriting," Public Opinion Quarterly, Vol. 23 (Summer, 1959), pp. 145-158. 9Lewis Donahew, "Publishers and Their 'Influence' Groups," Journalism Quarterly, Vol. 42 (Winter, 1965), pp. 112—113. loWalter Gieber, "The Communicator of the News: A Study of the Roles of Sources and Reporters,“ Social Forces, Vol. 39 (October, 1960), pp. 76-83. 12 Judd,l3 Gieber and Johnson,11 Gray and Edward, and Win- ick,14 among others. A review of the studies cited reveals a number of significant gaps in present knowledge of the working role of the mass communicator. The lack of knowledge in this respect is particularly evident in the case of the profes— sional broadcaster. As pointed out by Leo Bogart, some years ago: "Much is known about the audiences for various types of television programs, but remarkably little is known about the broadcasters themselves."15 Bogart's statement is further echoed by Riley and Riley. In their study of mass communication and the so— cial system, the Rileys conclude that a great need exists llWalter Gieber and Walter Johnson, "The City Hall 'Beat': A Study of Reporter and Source Roles," Journalism Quarterly, Vol. 38 (Summer, 1961), pp. 289—297. 12David L. Grey and Gerald J. Edward, "How the Newsman Lives: A Self-Image Study," paper presented at A.E.J. convention, Austin, Texas (August, 1964). 13Robert Judd, "The Newspaper Reporter in a Subur- ban City," Journalsim Quarterly, Vol. 38 (Winter, 1961), pp. 35-42. 14Charles Winick, "The Television Station Manager," édvanced Management Journal, Vol. 31 (January, 1966), pp. 3—60. 15Leo Bogart, The Age of Television (New York: F. Ungar Publishing Co., 1956), p. 348. for more concentrated investigation in this important area "if the social role of the mass communicator is to be thor- oughly understood.“16 Media as Organization The "who" in the Lasswell formulation quoted ear- lier is, in Wilbur Schramm's words, "a communication or— 17 Since the ganization or an institutionalized person." mass communicator functions within the context of such an organized system, an understanding of his working role might best be secured through an examination of the nature of the formal, or the complex, organization. Systems of this type have, of course, been studied in some detail by 18 20 March and Simon, Amitai Etzioni,19 Victor Thompson, 16John Riley and Matilda Riley, "Mass Communication and the Social System," in Merton, Broom and Cottrell, eds., Sociology Today (New York: Basic Books, Inc., 1959), p. 568. l7Wilbur Schramm, "How Communication Works," in W. Schramm, ed., The Process and Effects of Mass Communica— tion (Urbana, 111.: University of Ill. Press, 1955 , p. 8. 18James G. March and Herbert A. Simon, Organiza- tions (New York: John Wiley & Sons, Inc., 1958). 19Amitai Etzioni, A Comparative Analysis of Complex Or anizations (New York: The Free Press of Glencoe, Inc., 1961). 20Victor A. Thompson, Modern Organization: A Gen— eral Theory (New York: Alfred A. Knopf, 1961). Robert Golembiewski,21 Blau and Scott,22 and others. The results of these investigations appear to indicate that all formally constituted organizations have certain char- acteristics in common. Formal organizations are (1) sys— tems of cooperative relationships, (2) containing any number of persons, (3) usually differentiated by function and arranged in some form of hierarchy, (4) for the explicit purpose of gaining some stated goa1(s) or for the produc- tion of certain output(s), (5) the attainment of which will result in the return of given forms of satisfaction, ma- terial or otherwise, to the members. Media as Social Systems Any formal organization consisting of "systems of cooperative relationships" may be described as a social system. Social systems have been variously defined by a number of writers. The definition employed by Parsons and Shils, however, seems most applicable to present considera— tions. This reads as follows:23 21Robert Golembiewski, Behavior and Organization (New York: Rand McNally, 1962). 22Peter M. Blau and W. Richard Scott, Formal Or- ganizations: A Comparative Approach (San Francisco: Chandler Publishing Co., 1962). 23Talcott Parsons and Edward Shils, eds., Toward a General Theory of Action (Boston: Harvard University Press, 1951). p. 197. W A social system is a system of the actions of individ- uals, the principal units of which are roles and con- stellations of roles. It is a system of differentiated actions, organized into a system of differentiated roles. Mass communication systems, as formal organizations, may, then, be described as social systems and studied ac- cordingly. Conceptually, as Parsons and Shils have pointed out, an analysis of this type may best be conducted in terms of a study of roles. The Concept of Role An examination of the main body of sociological literature reveals that few concepts have been so widely accepted or so generally applied as that of the social role.24 It has been pointed out, however, that although the concept has been widely used, there nonetheless exists a lack of agreement concerning certain aspects of its use. Although role has been variously used, it most fre— quently has been defined in terms of the expected or ap- propriate modes of behavior which are assigned to occupants or given positions within set social systems. Newcomb has 24Lionel J. Neiman and James W. Hughes, "The Prob- lem of the Concept of Role—-A Re-survey of the Literature," Social Forces, Vol. 30 (December, 1951), pp. 141-149. 25Neal Gross, Ward Mason and Alexander McEachern, gxplorations in Role Analysis (New York: John Wiley & Sons, 958), pp. 11-47. 25 z written, for example, that "the ways of behaving which are expected of any individual who occupies a certain position constitute the £212 . . . associated with that position."26 The development of the concept of role in terms of "what others expect of the person," gives rise to the com- plimentary concept of role-expectations, which are said to be approved standards of behavior having many of the attri- butes of norms. "While a norm is a directive to action," Turner has written, "a role is a set of norms, with the additional normative element that the individual is ex- pected to be consistent."27 In addition to defining what is expected of the person under given circumstances, the concept of role fre— quently takes into account the characteristic mode of be- havior actually exhibited by persons occupying the desig- nated social positions. Newcomb has, in this regard, developed the related concept of role—behavior.28 The concept has also been described as "role-enactment"29 and 26Theodore M. Newcomb, Social Psycholggy (New York: Holt and Co., Inc., 1950), p. 280. 27Ralph H. Turner, "Role-Taking, Role Standpoint and Reference—Group Behavior," American Journal of Socio— logy, Vol. 51 (January, 1956), pp. 316-328. 28 Newcomb, op. cit., p. 330. 29Theodore R. Sarbin, "Role Theory," in G. Lindzey, ed., Handbook of Social Psycholo , Vol. I (Reading, Mass.: Addison-Wesley Publishing Co., 1954), pp. 223-258. as "role-performance."30 Regardless of the specific term employed to express concept, however, the view of the matter is that differences may exist, and most frequently do exist, between how a person is expected to behave in a given social context and how he actually does behave. The differentiation between the expectational as— pect of role and the performance aspect has been accorded wide acceptance both theoretically and empirically. This has been due in no small measure to the work of Gross, Mason and McEachern who focused on the general problem of what they chose to call the "postulate of consensus" in their study of the school superintendency role.31 This postulate takes the form of a belief that there exists basic agreement among the various members of a social sys- tem with reference to what is or is not "appropriate beha- vior" for the position occupant. Agreement in this context, of course, may be taken to imply normativeness. The postulate of consensus also reflects the assump- tion that different position occupants will respond to the demands of the role in quite similar fashions. Such an assumption, however, is often difficult to reconcile with experience as Gross, Mason and McEachern point out. "The 3OTurner, op. cit., p. 317. 31Gross, Mason and McEachern, op. cit., pp. 21-47. assumption that there is consensus on role definition," they write, "on the basis of which socialization takes place is untenable for the occupational position we studied."32 Rple-Behavior Variance As Gross, Mason and McEachern have also observed, "one of the tasks role theorists set themselves is to ac- count for the variability of the behavior of incumbents of the same position."33 This is a task which has been ap— proached from several points of view. Gordon Allport's approach to the problem is that of personality theorist. According to Allport the concept of role has four aspects: (1) role-expectations, which are defined in terms of "what the culture or subculture prescribes,’ (2) role-conception, defined as the manner in which the individual defines his role, (3) role-acceptance, which is said to be the degree to which the individual is willing to accept or act out the role expectations pre- scribed for the position, and (4) role-performance, which is defined as the manner in which the individual actually acts out his role in the social system.34 "The 32Ibid., p. 321. 33Ibid., p. 4. 34Gordon W. Allport, Pattern and Growth in Person- ality (New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston, 1961), p. 184. 10 expectations," Allport writes, "are uniform and prescribed, but the variation that occurs through differing concep- tions, degrees of acceptance, and all attendant features and traits of personality, modifies greatly the eventual role-performance."35 Newcomb's point of view regarding variation in role behavior differs somewhat from Allport's although it, too, is psychologically based. Variations in role behavior, Newcomb suggests, are tied simply to differences in the individual's "predispositions to take roles," depending upon differences in role perceptions and differences in motive patterns. "Because role—behaviors are personally motivated," he writes, "and because they are in part de- termined by self-perceptions which are never fully shared, no two individuals ever take the same roles in identical ways."36 Other factors which, according to Newcomb, contri— bute to the "commonplace observation“ that no two persons ever take the same role in exactly the same way are (1) the existence of certain constitutional elements which are related to the fact that individuals are born different and develop different physical attributes, (2) the unique conditions under which given roles are learned by different 35Ibid. 36Newcomb, op. cit., p. 333. 11 individuals, and (3) the particular sets of attitudes con- cerning the nature of the self and the nature of others which are maintained by different individuals.37 The process of differential socialization has also been employed to account for differences in role—behavior. Socialization, as defined by Child, "Is . . . a broad term for the whole process by which an individual, born with behavioral potentialities of enormously wide range, is led to develop actual behavior which is confined within a much narrower range--the range of what is customary and accep- table for him according to the standards of his group."38 In effect, socialization refers to a system of learning practiced in a given social group and directed toward. training the child to take on himself appropriately pre- scribed roles. Because of differences in technique, in degrees of effectiveness, and in a number of related fac- tors, however, the process of socialization usually results in behavior patterns that differ somewhat in practice. All .children are socialized, it seems, but not always to quite the same degree. 37Newcomb, op. cit., pp. 298—334. 38Irvin L. Child, "Socialization," in G. Lindzey, ed., Handbook of Social Psychology, Vol. II (Reading, Mass.: Addison-Wesley Publishing Co., Inc., 1954), p. 655. ‘ 12 The concept of socialization has, in addition, been extended to include considerations of other than childhood experiences. David Gottlieb, for example, employs the 39 It is Gottlieb's view concept of "adult socialization." that socialization can be described as gay development "which entails the modification of self through the acqui- sition of personality characteristics through contact with 'significant others.'"40 Used in this manner, the concept may refer to the general process by means of which the individual takes on himself any unfamiliar role. This is also the sense of the term employed by Brim and Wheeler.41 The process of "taking on the role of the other" has been well described in the work of George Herbert Mead.42 This process is itself basic to the process of socialization. To take the role of the other implies the development of an individual self and the development within the self of those attitudes of identification which 39David Gottlieb, "Adult Socialization: A Discus- sion of the Sociological Literature," Unpublished paper, Department of Sociology, Michigan State University (East Lansing: 1960). 4oIbid., p. 3. 41Orville G. Brim, Jr., and Stanton Wheeler, So- cialization After Childhood: Two Essays (New York: John Wiley & Sons, Inc., 1966). 42George Herbert Mead, Mind, Self and Society (Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1934). 13 are approved by the social group. "I have pointed out," Mead writes, "that there are two general stages in the full development of the self."43 At the first of these stages, the individual's self is constituted simply by an organization of the particular attitudes of other individuals toward himself and toward one another in the specific social acts in which he participates with them. But at the second stage in the full development of the individual's self that self is constituted not only by an organization of these particular attitudes, but also by an organization of the social attitudes of the generalized other of the social group as a whole to which he belongs. At another point in his writings, Mead states: "What goes to make up the organized self is the organization of the attitudes which are common to the group. A person is a personality because he belongs to a community, because he takes over the institutions of that community into his own conduct."44 The process of socialization and the process of taking the role of the other, as described by Mead, are joined by one common understanding, that of the importance of the role-model, whether in the form of the "significant other" or in the form of the "generalized other," persons or groups who tend to influence directly or indirectly the 43Ibid., p. 235. 44Mead, op. cit., p. 239. l4 role—taking (socialization) process. In a more general body of literature such persons or groups have been termed reference objects or reference groups.45 It has been suggested by a number of writers that the group, or groups, to which one is oriented, by member- ship or identification, tend to serve systematically as a measure of the individual's social perceptions and as a primary source of attitudes, values and norms for behavior. As Sherif and Sherif have pointed out, the group thus forms a general "frame of reference" for the individual by pro— viding him with a meaningful basis for relating to his environment.46 There are, of course, many different types of groups which can be characterized as reference groups in this sense. Merton, for example, has distinguished between the membership and the non-membership group, the positive reference group and the negative reference group and groups serving single reference functions and multiple reference functions. "Reference groups are, in principle," he 45Robert K. Merton and A. Kitt, "Contributions to the Theory of Reference Group Behavior," in R. K. Merton and P. F. Lazarsfeld, eds., Studies in the Scope and Method of the American Soldier (Glencoe, 111.: the Free Press, 1950), pp. 42-53. 46Muzafer Sherif and Carolyn W. Sherif, An Outline of Social Psycholpgy, Rev. Edition (New York: Harper & Brothers, 1956), p. 80. 15 writes, "almost innumerable. . . ."47 Any group, in brief, has the capability of becoming a point of reference "for shaping one's attitudes, evaluations, and behavior."48 The multiplicity of possible reference groups, each one of which may for the individual be quite determining in matters relating to the process of taking the role of the other, makes the variability of role behavior more understandable. In the above consideration several well defined perspectives have been described, each one of which has from time to time been employed as the basis for at least a partial explanation of why the behavior of various per- sons performing similar roles might tend to differ in light of presumed "agreement" in role-expectations. Attention will next be focused on yet another possible explanation for the same phenomenon, that related to the concept of orientation. The Concept of Orientation Max Weber, the German sociologist, was perhaps the first to employ the concept of orientation in a description of social action. According to Weber, "social action, like 47Robert K. Merton, Social Theory and Social Struc- ture (Glencoe, I11.: The Free Press, 1957), p. 233. 48 Ibid. 16 other forms of action, may be classified in the following ,four types according to its mode of orientation: (1) in terms of rational orientation to a system of discrete indi- vidual ends . . . (2) in terms of rational orientation to an absolute value . . . (3) in terms of affectual orienta— tion, especially emotional, determined by the specific affects and states of feeling of the actor; (4) tradition- ally oriented, through the habituation of long practice."49 Today it is possible to identify several additional uses of the orientation concept. The most widely recog- nized of which is as an element in the theory of action.50 The actor's system of orientations is constituted by a great number of specific orientations. Each of these 'orientations of action' is a 'conception' (explicit or implicit, conscious or unconscious) which the actor has of the situation in terms of what he wants (his ends), what he sees (how the situation looks to him), and how he intends to get from the objects he sees the things he wants (his explicit or implicit, normatively regulated 'plan' of action). While the Weberian notion of orientation is clearly recognized in this statement, much has been added. The individual's mode of orientation is, in this context, con- ceptualized as a gystem of orientations. This implies that 49Max Weber, The Theory of Social and Economic Organization, Edited by Talcott Parsons (New York: The Free Press, 1964), p. 115. 50Talcott Parsons and Edward A. Shils, "Categories of the Orientation and Organization of Action," in Parsons and Shils, eds., Toward a General Theogyyof Action (Boston: Harvard University Press, 1951), p. 54. 17 the individual is not strictly limited to a single orienta- tion but that he maintains a general orientation to types of situations and certain specific orientations to objects located within these situations. As places in which actions occur, situations con- sist of "objects of orientation," while the orientation of the actor consists of "the set of cognitions, cathexes, plans and relevant standards which relates the actor to 51 the situation." The concept of orientation, then, as employed by Parsons and Shils, refers first to the actor's definition of the situation and secondly to what the actor thinks about or feels toward objects contained within the situation. The concept of orientation defined by the theory of action is also described in terms of a combination of elements. Of most direct interest in these regards are those of motivation and evaluation.52 The orientation of the actor to the situation may be broken down into a set of analytic elements. These elements are not separate within the orientation pro— cess; they might be conceived as different aspects or different ingredients of that process. They may be divided into two analytically independent categories: a category of elements of motivational orientation (appearances, wants, plans), and a category of elements of value-orientation (cognitive standards, aesthetic standards, moral standards). 51Parsons and Shils, op. cit., p. 56. 52Ibid., p. 58. F" 18 Parsons and Shils further define motivation to include drives, "a set of tendencies on the part of the organism to acquire certain goal objects," and need-dispositions, "tendencies to orient and act with respect to objects in certain manners and to expect certain consequences."53 Value—orientations are said to refer "to those aspects of the actor's orientation which commit him to the observance of certain norms, standards, criteria of selection, when- ever he is in a contingent situation which allows (and requires) him to make a choice."54 A second use of the concept of orientation, one which is also suggested in the work of Parsons, is as an internalized system of values. In this sense one speaks of an individual's value-orientation. Clyde Kluckhohn is most identified with the concept when used in this sense. A value-orientation as employed by Kluckhohn, “may be de- fined as a generalized and organized conception, influenc— ing behavior, of nature, of man's place in it, of man's relation to man, and of the desirable and nondesirable as they may relate to man-environment and interhuman rela- tions."55 53Ibid., pp. 110-114. 54Ibid., p. 59. 55 Clyde Kluckhohn, "Values and Value—Orientations in the Theory of Action," in Talcott Parsons and Edward A. Shils, eds., Toward a General Theory of Action, op. cit., p. 411. 19 As Kluckhohn views the matter, a value is not a simple preference nor is a person's value-orientation sim- ply an organized system of preferences. Values go beyond statements of preferences and involve the abstract sense of the desirable as opposed to an image of that which may actually be desired. "The desirable," Kluckhohn writes, "is what it is felt or thought proper to want. It is what an actor or group of actors desire--and believe they 'ought' or 'should' desire--for the individual or a plurality of "56 Values, then, are standards of what should individuals. be desired. In the abstract these are enduring and not directly limited to any particular situation or set of circumstances. Another use of the concept of orientation is re- flected in the work of Theodore Newcomb, where it is em- ployed in the sense of a generalizing tendency or predis- position. To an extent Newcomb's use of the concept implies the more basic term attitude. "Attitudes," he writes, "represent persistent, general orientations of the 57 individual toward his environment." Elsewhere Newcomb states that "the concept of attitude is a shorthand way of 56Ibid., p. 396. 57Newcomb, op. cit., p. 119. 20 saying that people learn as a result of experience to orient themselves towards objects and symbols."58 As Newcomb views the matter, then, attitudes are representative of general orientations to action: "A per- son's attitude toward something refers not so much to his learned forms of response to it, which may vary, as to his learned direction-set toward it."59 A fourth, closely related, use of the concept of orientation--one frequently employed by Merton, for exam- ple—-is in the sense of a simple social or psychological relationship, as in the phrase "the child's peer group orientation." What is implied here is that the individual maintains a certain characteristic outlook or perspective with reference to others. "Others" in this sense can be defined as other persons or as other ideas, systems of ideas, values, or objects, as in reference group orienta- tion, career orientation, political orientation, economic orientation or religious orientation. A somewhat different approach to the concept of orientation is reflected in the work of Etzioni, where it is used as a form of involvement or commitment to a social 60 group or to a complex organization. For Etzioni persons 58Ibid., p. 118. 59Ibid. 60Amitai Etzioni, op. cit., pp. 1-16. 21 tend to relate to organizations in terms of their relative acceptance or rejection of the system of power which is characteristic of the organization. This can be seen from the following:61 The orientation of the subordinated actor can be char- acterized as positive (commitment) or negative (aliena- tion). It is determined in part by the degree to which the power applied is considered legitimate by the sub- ordinated actor and, in part, by its congruence with the line of action he would desire. We refer to this orientation, whether positive or negative, as involve- ment in the organization. In this respect the actor's orientation may be defined as a system of beliefs and values, including a set related to conceptions of power and authority. As suggested in the above discussion, the concept of orientation has had a variety of interpretations and uses. Principally, however, it has been employed to refer to the relationship between individuals and certain types of others in given situations and to refer to those elements, both cognitive and affective, which are most characteristic of the individual's "point of view" and which tend thereby to define relationships for the indi- vidual. 61lbid., p. 4. r.__ _- .1. 22 Summary At the beginning of this chapter it was pointed out that little is known concerning the role of the mass communicator. The lack of information concerning the role of the broadcaster was particularly stressed. It was also suggested that in order to increase the level of under- standing of this role, for the specific benefit of students of mass media and society, a systematic study of the role would need to be undertaken. In a consideration of the more general aspects of the problem it was recognized (1) that the so-called mass communicator occupies a position within a formal organiza- tion, (2) that the formal organization has all of the at- tributes of a social system, (3) that studies of social systems frequently involve a utilization of the concept of role, and (4) that the concept of role introduces certain theoretically related problems, namely the problems of consensus and role-behavior variability. A brief review of the literature related to the concept of role and role-behavior was undertaken. Several explanations of role—behavior variability were considered. These included explanations based upon personality differ- ences, perceptual and predispositional differences, dif- ferential socialization and differences in orientation. The concept of orientation was given special attention and 23 detailed review because it appeared to offer a reasonable solution to the problems of role-consensus and role-behavior variability and a basis for interrelating other primary study concepts in a meaningful manner. P’——-‘- CHAPTER II DEVELOPING THE THEORETIC RATIONALE It is the purpose of this chapter to (1) provide a specific focus for the study to be undertaken, (2) develop a theoretic rationale based upon a consideration of the theory of action, (3) operationalize the principal concepts which will be employed in the development of the study, and (4) indicate the primary and secondary study proposi— tions and related hypotheses which will be tested. Focusing the Study The preceding chapter concluded with the view that little attention has been given in the research literature to the general role of the mass communicator. It is evident too, from a review of the literature that no specific in- vestigation has yet been attempted of the role of the American broadcaster as a representative type of mass communicator. The general purpose of the present study is, then, to develop an awareness of certain aspects of the role of the mass communicator, namely his role—orientation and his reference-group identifications. 24 25 To sharpen the focus of the study still further, its purpose is to deve10p an awareness of the role-orien- tation and reference-group identifications of broadcast communicators, that is, persons occupying positions of responsibility in systems of broadcast communication as defined by certain standards of sample selection. Not all persons engaged occupationally in systems of mass communication can technically be defined as mass communicators of course. All persons engaged occupationally in systems of mass communication do, nonetheless, perform roles. The role of the mass communicator, however, is distinct. The mass communicator is a message producer, one who is engaged directly with the process of determining message content and form. Broadcast engineers, regardless of their positions in the system hierarchy, can not be considered mass communicators. Broadcast newsmen, however, at least those who write the news or make decisions regard- ing the reporting of events, may be viewed as mass commu— nicators in this sense of the term. No attempt need be made at this time to define more specifically the character of the mass communication sys- tems or the special positions within such systems that fit the above qualifications and which will be sampled for study purposes. These particular concerns will be consid- ered in Chapter IV, the chapter on procedures and method— ology. 26 Theoretic Rationale The theoretic rationale developed for the present study can be expressed simply. In brief, it is assumed (1) that all broadcast stations exhibit the attributes generally associated in the literature of the social sciences with the concept of the formal organization, (2) that the concept of the formal organization implies a sys- tem of social action, (3) that systems of social action are composed of units of social action, (4) that basic to any single unit of social action is the concept of a posi- tion, (5) that individuals occupying positions in the sys- tem enact roles, (6) that roles are best defined in terms of expectations, (7) that role-expectations are implicitly or explicitly communicated to position occupants, (8) that role-expectations so communicated are subjected to proces- ses of individual interpretation, (9) that the processes of interpretation are mediated by the individual's role- orientation, (10) that the mediated responses of the posi- tion occupant constitute his role-behavior, and (11) that the output of the system is in large measure determined by the types of role—behaviors performed. The elements of this system center on the position occupant, his role-orientation and role-behavior. These elements, conceptualized in terms of a single focal posi- tion, are represented in Figure 1. Here "system others" are assumed to be persons who occupy positions in the 27 system having reciprocal relations with the occupant of the focal position and who develop certain expectations regarding the ideal behavior of the focal position occu- pant. These expectations, termed role-expectations, are transmitted to the person occupying the focal position. Communications containing role-expectations are termed the "sent role," following the terminology employed by Kahn, Wolfe, Quinn, 2E. al,, in their study of organiza- tional stress.1 The messages comprising the "sent role" are received by the occupant of the focal position and mediated by the occupant's "role-orientation." The "med- iated responses" of the position occupant, which are assumed to be internalized processes, result, in part, in certain patterns of role-behavior.2 Role-behaviors as the product of the process de- scribed, may take one of two forms. They may be covert, in the form of attitudes or predispositions to respond, or overt, in the form of actions. System output, as an observable phenomenon, is assumed to be the consequence of role behaviors performed as action only, however. 1Robert L. Kahn, Donald M. Wolfe, Robert P. Quinn, et. 81-: 0p. cit., p. 15. 2Charles E. Osgood, Method and Theory in Experi- mental Ps cholo (New York: Oxford University Press, 95 ’ pp. 3 - 2o 28 J Role-Orientation of Focal Position Occupant Mediated Response J System ’ Role-Behavior . System Others of OccupantAy Output Role-ExpectationsJ Figure 1: Unit of Social Action The Theory of Action In the preceding chapter the concept of orientation was reviewed in terms of its various uses by different theorists. The concept as develoPed by Parsons and Shils was stressed particularly. This conceptualization of orientation has been employed as an essential element in an involved theoretic formulation described by Parsons and Shils as the theory of action. Basically, the theory of action "is a conceptual scheme for the analysis of the behavior of living organisms."3 As defined, any behavior,' “mm m"- of whatever form, can be termed an action. _A§inDS_Lake—— mm- a... fim'wul" um "#‘ MM.—_,W place in situations”whichmconsistwoflobjacts,~social, "w”,111._n—— ‘ h....n..._-—"v--U‘ 3Parsons and Shils, op. cit., p. 53. 29 physical or cultural. "Each actor has a system of rela- tions-to-objects; this is called his 'system of orienta- tions.”4 Objects of all types are defined by the actor. in terms of their relative significance. They also are cathected, that is, viewed as either wanted or not wanted. Objects so regarded "become organized into the actor's system of orientation."5 The actor's system of orientation is constituted by a great number of specific orientations. Each of these "orientations of action" is a "conception" (explicit or implicit, conscious or unconscious) which the actor has of the situation in terms of what he wants (his ends), what he sees (how the situation looks to him), and how he intends to get from the objects he sees the things he wants (his explicit, normatively regulated "plan" of action). The theory of action, then, simply stated, "involves ac- tors, a situation of action, and the orientation of the actor to that situation."6 Concept of the Pattern Variable The consideration of how actors relate to situa- tions is conceptualized in the theory of action in terms of Talcott Parsons' well-known formulation of the "pattern variable." A pattern variable is defined as "a dichotomy, 4Parsons and Shils, 0p. cit., p. 54. 5Ibid., p. 54. 61bid., p. 56. 30 one side of which must be chosen by an actor before the meaning of a situation is determined for him, and thus before he can act with respect to that situation."7 The manner in which the concept of the pattern variable is joined with the concept of orientation in the theory of action is described as follows:8 At the outset we must mention two general features which characterize and perhaps define all such orien- tations, but which are of such a general nature that they are not termed as separate modes of orientation. These are (l) the choice aspect and (2) the expectancy aspect of the orientation. The first implise that every orientation is explicitly or implicitly an orien- tation of alternatives; the orientation involves a scanning of several possible courses of action and a choice from them. The second implies that every orien- tation is an "expectancy" in the sense that it is an orientation to the future state of the situation as well as the present. The actor's orientation to the situation is, then, deter- mined by the pattern of choices among possible action al- ternatives which he is required to make with regard to certain relevant objects within the particular situation. These may be discrete choices for action, habits of choice as determined by personality factors, aspects of role- definition, and aspects of value standards. Basically, too, pattern variables are, in most applications, "the categories for the description of value- orientations . . . A given value-orientation or some 71bid., p. 77. 81bido, PI 68. 31 particular aspect of it may be interpreted as imposing a preference or giving a primacy to one alternative over the other in a particular type of situation."9 The aspect of the pattern variable concept which will be Operationalized in the present study relates to the definition of role. In the theory of action, "for most purposes the conceptual unit of the social systems is the £212, The role is a sector of the individual actor's total system of action. It is the point of contact between the system of action of the individual and the social system."10 "The primary ingredient of the role," moreover, "is the role-expectation. Role-expectations are patterns of evaluation; their primary constituents are analytically derivable from the pattern-variable combinations and from derivatives of the pattern variables when these are com- bined with the specific types of situations."11 There are in the theory of action a total of five pattern variables which, in Parsons' view, constitute an exhaustive listing. These include, (1) affectivity vs. affective-neutrality, (2) self-orientation vs. collectivity- orientation, (3) universalism vs. particularism, (4) 9Parsons and Shils, 0p. cit., p. 78. loIbid., p. 190. llIbid. 32 ascription (quality considerations) vs. achievement (per- formance standards), and (5) specificity vs. diffuseness. Each of the five pattern variables identified in the Parsonian system will be described below, relative to its direct application in the definition of roles. Related to each is one of the five so-called "dilemmas of choice," or areas of relevance, which Parsons employs to focus his theory. These are the five specific choices which the actor must make "before the situation has a determinate 12 meaning for him." 1. The dilemma of gratification of impulse versus discipline:' a. Affectivity: the role-expectation that the incumbent of the role may freely express cer- tain affective reactions to objects in the situation and need not attempt to control them in the interest of discipline. b. Affective neutrality: the role-expectation that the incumbent of the role in question should restrain any impulses to certain affec- tive expressions and subordinate them to con- siderations of discipline. In both cases the affect may be positive or negative, and the discipline (or permissiveness) may apply only to certain qualitative types of affective ex- pression (e.g., sexual). 2. The dilemma of private versus collective interests, or the distribution between private permissiveness and collective obligation. a. Self-orientation: the role-expectation by the relevant actors that it is permissible for the incumbent of the role in question to give pri— ority in the given situation to his own private 12Ibid., pp. 76-91. The 33 interests, whatever their motivational content or quality, independently of their bearing on the interests or values of a given collectivity of which he is a member, or the interests of other actors. Collectivity-orientation: the role-expectation by the relevant actors that the actor is obliged, as an incumbent of the role in question, to take directly into account the values and interests of the collectivity of which, in this role, he is a member. When there is a potential conflict with his private interests, he is expected in the particular choice to give priority to the collective interest. This also applies to his action in representative roles on behalf of the collectivity. dilemma of transcendence versus immanence. In confronting any situation, the actor faces the dilemma whether to treat the objects in the situa- tion in accordance with a general norm covering 311 objects in that class or whether to treat them in accordance with their standing in some particular relationship to him or his collectivity, indepen- dently of the object's subsumibility under a general norm. a. The Universalism: the role—expectation that, in qualifications for memberships and decisions for differential treatment, priority will be given to standards defined in completely gen- eralized terms, independent of the particular relationship of the actor's own statuses (quali- ties or performances, classificatory or rela- tional) to those of the object. Particularism: the role-expectation that, in qualifications for memberships and decisions for differential treatment, priority will be given to standards which assert the primary of the values attached to objects by their partic- ular relations to the actor's properties (qualities or performances, classificatory or relational) as over against their general uni- versally applicable class prOperties. dilemma of object modalities. When confronting an object in a situation, the actor faces the dilemma of deciding how to treat it. Is he to treat it in the light of what it is in itself 34 or in the light of what it does or what might flow from its actions? a. Ascri tion: the role—expectation that the role incumbent, in orienting himself to social ob- jects in the relevant choice situation, will accord priority to the objects' given attributes (whether universalistically or particularisti- cally defined) over their actual or potential performances. b. Achievement: the role-expectation that the role incumbent, in orienting to social objects in the relevant choice situation, will give priority to the objects' actual or expected performances, and to their attributes only as directly rele- vant to these performances, over attributes which are essentially independent of the specific performances in question. 5. The dilemma of the scope of significance of the ob'ect. PIn confronting an object, an actor must choose among the various possible ranges in which he will respond to the object. a. Diffuseness: the role—expectation that the role incumbent, at the relevant choice point, will accept any potential significance of a social object, including obligation to it, which is compatible with his other interests and obliga-l tions, and that he will give priority to this expectation over any disposition to confine the role-orientation to a specific range of signifi- cance of the object. b. Specificity: the role-expectation that the role incumbent, at the relevant choice point, will be oriented to a social object only within a speci— fic range of its relevance as a cathectic object or as an instrumental means or condition and that he will give priority to this expectation over any disposition to include potential aspects of significance of the object not specifically defined in the expectation pattern. Orientation Defined Conceptually Orientations may be conceived both generally and specifically. As employed in the present study, a general 35 orientation may be said to provide direction or motivation for the actor in any of a number of situations. In this form it consists of a set of generalized standards for be- havior, conceptions of what one should do or should not do under given circumstances, particularly with reference to certain approved goals and plans for action. A general orientation may also be thought of as a formula for relating one's self to a wide range of life situations. It is cognitively structured and effectively charged and is experienced by the actor covertly in terms of patterns of right and wrong, good and bad, and so forth. In this regard general orientations are the cognitive and moral products of general cultural conditioning processes and of specialized patterns of socialization, invoked by the demands of the society and by the sanctions of primary groups within it. A general orientation tends to govern the actor's relations in most situations, whether social, political, economic or religious. The general orientation serves thus to structure an actor's attitudes and behaviors within the situation; to assign meaning to the social and material ob- jects within the situation; and to arbitrate among his in— dividual preferences within any system of alternative possibilities. The general orientation, therefore, acts as a standard for the individual's judgments and values. De- fined in this manner, it corresponds with Parsons and Shils' 36 conceptualization of a "system of orientation," and with Kluckhohn's concept of "value-orientation." A specific orientation, as distinguished from a general orientation, is one which is situationally bound, defining situation in terms of the theory of action. As employed in the present study, the specific orientation may be said to refer to that which relates the actor to a single situation or type of situation and to sets of objects or symbols anchored in such situations. Under the terms of these definitions, the actor may be oriented specifically to pig place of business, pig church or political party and oriented generally to certain typeg of business, religious or political values, or attitudes. The basic distinction between the general and the specific orientation, therefore, is the difference between the ab— stract value or the value system and its particular mode of expression or application within the situation. A situa- tional orientation is, then, logically derived from, or is a product of, a general orientation specifically determined. Orientation Defined Operationally Kerlinger defines an operational definition as one which "assigns meaning to a construct or a variable by Specifying the activities or 'operations' necessary to mea- 13 sure the construct or variable." Such activities or k 13Fred N. Kerlinger, Foundations of Behayioral Re- search, (New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston, Inc., 1965), po 3:. 37 operations, as applied to the concept of orientation, will, for the purposes of the present study, take the following form. An instrument will first be designed, composed of a variety of related items. The responses to these items will be said to constitute the respondent's orientation to the defined situation. This instrument will be constructed theoretically in terms of the pattern variables described earlier, and Operationally in terms of the following study definitions. 1.0 The pattern of choices made by the respondent on a devised questionnaire, the items of which will be constructed in terms of pattern variable formula- tions, will be said to constitute the respondent's general orientation, as expressed within the con- text of the specific situation. 1.1 Objects about which the respondent is required to "pattern" his choices on such a question- naire will be referred to as oriented objects or as objects of orientation. 1.2 Oriented objects may include social objects, material objects or symbolic objects. 2.0 The objects of orientation described in item form on the devised questionnaire will be said to Constitute elements of the respondent's specific orientation. 2.1 The sum of the scores assigned to the responses made on the devised pattern variable question- naire will be said to constitute the respondent's situational orientation. 2.2 Any number of respondents in the study sample who exhibit high levels of similarity, or con- sensus, with reference to patterns of choice on items contained on the questionnaire, and thus who may be said to "share" particular situational orientations, will be said to be re- lated according to given modes of orientation. 38 The special instrument which was devised for the purpose of this study will be described in detail in the next chapter. This instrument was devised according to the following specifications, each of which can be related to the theory of action previously described and to more general considerations of role and role-orientation. 1. Each item devised for the questionnaire must des- cribe a particular object of orientation. 2. The object of orientation must be characteristic of the specific situation defined. That is to say that such objects must relate to the broadcast station and be relevant in terms of the broadcaster role. 3. Each item referring to a specific object of orien— tation must be described in terms of one or another of the "dilemmas of choice" indicated in the for- mulation of the pattern variables. Modes of Orientation The theory of action, the concept of role, and the concept of orientation, are each tied to the theoretic frame of reference selected as model and guide in the development of the present study. The final element which must be fitted to the system is the concept of modes of orientation. By definition, any set of respondents who exhibit high levels of similarity with reference to certain patterns of choice and who thus share particular situational orienta- tions can be described in terms of the same mode of orientation. 39 Defined in this manner the modes of orientation concept implies a common pattern of relating to the situa- tion, one shared by numbers of occupants of similar system positions. Of necessity, then, the modes of orientation concept also implies an element of consensus, although not to the same degree complained of by Gross, Mason and McEachern.l4 The theory of action also postulates certain views concerning consensus. This is reflected in the use of the concepts of role-expectation and value-orientation, both of which are defined in consensual terms, and in the basic con— ceptualization of the social system.15 A social system is a system of action which has the fol- lowing Characteristics: (1) It involves a process of interaction between two or more actors; the interaction process as such is a focus of the observer's attention. (2) The situation toward which the actors are oriented includes other actors. These other actors (alters) are objects of cathexis. Alter's actions are taken cogni- tively into account as data. Alter's various orienta- tions may be either goals to be pursued or means for the accomplishment of goals. Alter's orientations may thus be objects for evaluative judgment. (3) There is (in a social system) interdependent and, in part, con- certed action in which the concert is a function of collective goal orientation or common values, and of a consensus of normative and cognitive expectations. The consensual basis of modes of orientation in the sense implied in the theory of action can be taken for 14Gross, Mason and McEachern, pp, cit., pp. 21-47. 15Parsons and Shils, 9p. cit., p. 54. 40 granted. However, this is not to be taken as an indication that all persons occupying similar positions in the system will develop similar orientations or structure role-behavior in like manners. As pointed out in Chapter I, observation supports the view that no two individuals pattern behavior in exactly the same manner when occupying a given position on similar positions in the system. Definitions of role based upon over estimates of role consensus, therefore, tend to depart from reality. "Conceptual schemes for role analysis," Gross, Mason and McEachern have written, "that preclude the investigation of the basic question of role consensus are distinctly limited."16 This may be taken to imply that unless the theory devised for the analysis of system roles allows for or takes into account variations in role-behavior, the re- sulting analyses are likely to be weakened. The modes of orientation concept is believed to satisfy this requirement of role theory. Developing Typological Systems17 That behavioral differences exist among role-performers is generally recognized and hardly surprising. Of greater 16Gross, Mason and McEachern, op. cit., p. 43. 17The concept of the "pure type" was well developed, in a methodological sense, by the German sociologist Max Weber. Weber's thesis is that from a study of individual cases it is possible to abstract certain unifying elements 41 interest is the observation that such differences appear to be patterned. In the majority of cases studied it was pos- sible for investigators to classify by type the particular modes of behaviors exhibited by occupants of similar posi- tions in similar social systems. In an early investigation of bureaucratic behavior Leonard Reissman identified several types of bureaucrats.l8 The "functional bureaucrat," as described by Reissman, is one "who is oriented towards and seeks his recognition from a given professional group outside of rather than within the bureaucracy." The "job bureaucrat," on the other hand, is one who "is immersed entirely within the structure" of the bureaucracy itself. Other types described by Reissman are the "specialist" and the "service" bureaucrats, types that fall between these two points. Peter Blau also typed the subjects of his investiga- tions into bureaucratic systems, in terms (1) of the indi- vidual's relative commitment to the system rules, and (2) the that on application will form a system of empirical possi- bilities. The four "types of social action" which Weber identified in his study of bureaucracy form one example of this process. These "types" are also linked historically and logically to the system of pattern variables employed by Parsons in the theory of action. As Parsons has stated: "It (the type) does not describe an individual course of action but a 'typical' one--it is a generalized rubric within which an indefinite number of particular cases may be classi- fied." (Max Weber, The Theopy of Social and Economic Organi- zation, op. cit., pp. 8-29.) 18Leonard Reissman, "A Study of Role Conceptions in Bureaucracy," Social Forces, Vol. 27 (March, 1949), pp. 305-310. 42 individual's relative concern for the best interests of the client, independently of the system rules; these were de- fined respectively as a ritualistic (or legalistic) type and a particularistic type.19 Thompson also developed a system of bureaucratic 20 Those types in his analysis of the modern organization. members of the system who emphasize "the objective, exter- nalized goals of the organization" were identified as one type, while those members of the system "most closely associated with personal goals in our bureaucratic structures," those who stress rights and authority, were identified as another type. The former were termed "specialists" by Thompson, and the latter were termed "bureaucrats." Those persons termed specialists by Thompson were labeled "professionals" by Blau and Scott in their investi- gation of organizational behavior.21 Professionals as viewed by Blau and Scott, are persons governed in the situa- tion by certain universalistic standards (norms), by feelings of affective neutrality, by the specificity of professional expertness, by achieved status (performance standards), and by a lack of self-interest motivations. Blau and Scott also 19Peter M. Blau, The Dynamics of Bureaucracy, (Chi— cago: Univ. of Chicago Press, 1955). 20Thompson, op. cit. 21Blau and Scott, op. cit., p. 60. 43 characterize the standard bureaucrat as a person distin- guished by his emphasis upon motivations of self-interest and organizational attachment. In a study of engineering groups, Barnes discovered and identified three types of role—behavior, or role- orientation;22 these were exhibited (1) by persons who tended to identify with science-professional reference groups or values, (2) by those who tended to identify with management, with the organization proper or with business values in general, and (3) by persons who tended to identify mainly with socio-familial-religious reference groups. These groups were termed, respectively, as professionals, organizationals and socials. Presthus' work was related primarily to methods of system accommodation, for the purposes of which he was able to identify three system types, the upward-mobile, the in— 23 Persons identified as different and the ambivalent. upward-mobiles were characterized by their strong identifi- cation with the system, their commitment to the larger purposes of the organization, and their general involvement with organization affairs. The so-called indifferents were 22Louis B. Barnes, Organizational Systems and Engi— neering Groups, (Boston: Harvard University, Division of Research, Graduate School of Business Administration, 1960). 23Robert Presthus, The Organizational Society, (New York: Alfred A. Knopf, 1962). 44 persons who had "come to terms with their work environment by withdrawal and by a redirection of their interest toward off-the-job satisfactions." The third type, termed ambiva- lents, "ordinarily play a specialist, 'cosmopolitan' role." As such they most frequently identify with extra-organiza- tional groups and values, while remaining relatively com- mitted to the status-reward system honored by the organization. In each of the several studies cited, the tendency of writers to form typologies is quite evident. The typo- logical systems formed, moreover, appear to have certain elements in common. Each is based upon a pattern of self- other or self-system identification which assumes that the position-occupant accepts to some extent the organization's value system or develops an identification with certain non- organization related professional groups or value-systems. In summary, it appears that a middle position may thus be taken, theoretically, between the extreme View that roles exhibit consensually validated patterns of behavior exe pected of Eli occupants of given social positions,amdfixaequally extreme view that no two persons ever pattern role-behavior in exactly the same manner, because of qualities of indivi- dual differences. It also seems in keeping with the evidence, there- fore, to suggest the possibility of designing simple typo- logies of role-behavior for given positions in systems of mass to be exhibited by individuals occupying similar 45 positions in the systems under study. This implies that role-behavior in any given situation is not infinitely variable but tends to follow certain patterned forms of similarity and dissimilarity. Developing a System of Modes of Orientation By cross-tabulating the fiVe pattern variables postulated by Parsons and Shils, a typological system of possible orientations may be devised. Logically such a combination of variables presents thirty—two possibilities, as indicated in Figure 2. Each of the cells shown, result- ing from a particular combination of pattern variables, represents a logical action possibility, or, in keeping with present terminology, a particular mode of orientation. Not all possible combinations of pattern variables are equally likely to occur, however, and for this reason it is not necessary to conceptualize the entire system of logical possibilities. The present study focuses on indi- viduals performing roles in a certain type of situation, the formal organization, moreover, it focuses on a certain type of formal organization, that of the typical broadcasting station. In America, broadcasting stations are usually de- fined as economic organizations. Such organizations have been termed "utilitarian" by Etzioni.24 The principal characteristics of utilitarian organizations, according to 24Etzioni, op. cit., p. 31. 46 mcumuumm cofiumucwauo vamhm "N cummwm chmpcma owumuosmousm anemones Hmcoflmmmmoum NOI suawm wuomvzxmsuo 20H wszHD éZHDDBHBBd .0 n N... an on 33 3.2 Z mm mm m... a 1. 2 HH mam H K n HmcofluMNecumuo t. m d T. a p. 1 I J I. o m 3.. mm as G om a: 3 S m a a O m 3 3: Z 2 NH 3 8: m n .4 0 .A u T. A a 1 d S a E I T. .3 m. a: a p m a m m E m m P u o a UMHODMOHDQ Hmflowmmwuohnm unwowmwommm mmmcmmsumwo auaowmwommm mumsmmsmmwo mmmcmmsmmwo huwowmeoomm mmmcmmsmwwo muwowmwommm suaamuusmz m>auoomu¢ sua>auommu< sua>auommuc suaamuuamz m>auommm¢ coaumucoauouumaom coaumuaoauounmua>auooaaoo 47 Etzioni, are (l) the development and maintenance of economic goals, (2) the production of commodities or services which may be supplied to outsiders, (3) a stress on calculative involvement, and (4) a system of renumerative power. A description of the broadcasting station as an utilitarian system, after Etzioni, cannot be altogether accurate, however. The broadcasting station is not exclu— sively an economically-oriented organization; it is also licensed formally by the federal government for the purpose of providing certain classes of needed services to the com- munity. To this extent, then, the broadcasting station can also be defined as a public-service organization, one simi- lar to those termed "normative organizations" by Etzioni.25 As an organizational type the broadcasting station appears to be a mixed system, containing elements of both the utilitarian and the normative forms. This being the case, it is logical to assume that certain position-occupants within the system will be oriented more to one then to the other of these separate functions, either the economic-related function or the service-related function. In part, system- roles so defined tend to reflect the patterns of self-other or self-system identifications (orientations) described in the studies previously discussed. In the Parsonian system of.pattern variables, this dichotomy is represented clearly 25Etzioni, op. cit., p. 40. 48 in terms of the collectivity-orientation versus self- orientation distinction. In their study of organizational behavior Blau and Scott identified two primary system types, as previously noted; these were termed, respectively, professionals and bureaucrats.26 Professionals were described as persons governed in the situation by certain universalistic stan- dards (norms), by feelings of affective neutrality, by the specificity of professional expertness, by concern for achieved status (performance standards), and by low levels of self-interest motivation., In contrast, bureaucrats appeared to be characterized mainly by their emphasis upon motivations of self-interest and by organizational attach- ment and system loyalty. A bi-polarization of the characteristics identified with the two system types described by Blau and Scott would result in a location of the professional (as a mode of orientation) in cell number one of Figure 2 and the so- called bureaucrat in cell number eight, in the opposite corner. This follows from the observation that each of the distinguishing characteristics described is directly linked to one or the other of the Parsonian pattern vari- ables. Assuming that the contrasting patterns of orienta- tion identified by Blau and Scott may be generalized to ¥ 26Blau and Scott, op. cit., pp. 60-74. 49 types of organization other than the type studied by these writers, and assuming further that the system of possibili- ties identified within the context of the present study can be validated empirically, it should be possible to identify and describe certain characteristic types of broadcasters as suggested. It may be anticipated, then, that the forms and variety of system-types discovered will conform to the pat- tern of possibilities described; that broadcasters will tend to differ in terms of orientation to self-interest and orientation to the interest of the collectivity; in degrees of attachment to the situation; in the degrees of specific- ity with which they tend to define their roles; in the de- grees to which they tend to apply general rules or principles of behavior; in the degrees to which they accept and apply standards of performance, relative to significant objects in the situation. Whether broadcasters do in fact differ in terms of these factors is here considered a matter for empirical testing. A further object of interest is the factors which act upon the broadcasters in the situation to produce the differences in orientation described. PrimaryiStudyfPropositions One of the underlying assumptions in the present Study is that broadcasting stations, as representative 50 types of mass communication systems, may be likened to other types of bureaucratic structures in modern society. It is also assumed that because of the unique system of organization and control in broadcasting in America, that is, a system of private ownership operating within a frame- work of government regulation, that broadcasting stations represent a mixing of two organizational types, the utili- tarian and the normative, having both economic-related systems of goals and service-related systems of goals. System Position Proposition In each of the organization studies cited, indivi- duals occupying given positions in the system were differ- entiated in terms of modes of orientation, and each system of types devised to account for such differences was predi- cated to a greater or lesser degree on certain distinctions of self-other or self—system identification.' The first prOposition proposed for investigation in the present study is derived from the elements of theory and research discussed in the preceding paragraphs and is expressed in the following terms. Pl: That an individual's role—orientation is, in part, a function of his position in the system. It is assumed, then, that occupants of certain positions within the system, are more oriented then others toward one or the other of the systems of goals identified with the "mixed" organizational type. In other words, it 51 is assumed that occupants of certain positions in broadcast- ing stations will be more identified with economic-related system-goals and occupants of certain other positions will be more identified with service-related system-goals. Uti- lizing the pattern-variable concept of collectivity-orienta- tion versus self—orientation, it is possible to specify the following hypotheses with regard to the system—position proposition. H1: Broadcasters with service-related role-functions may be expected to be more collectivity-oriented than broadcasters with economic-related role- functions. H2: Broadcasters with economic-related role-functions may be expected to be more self-oriented than broadcasters with service-related role-functions. Here, for purposes of test, economic-related role-functions are assumed to be associated predominantly with the posi- tions of station manager and sales manager and service—related role-functions are assumed to be associated predominantly with the positions of program director and news director. By demonstrating that a person's position within the system is likely to reflect either a self-orientation pattern or a collectivity-orientation pattern, it should also be possible to demonstrate that different degrees of personal or situational detachment or commitment are func- tions of given positions. Logically, it is assumed, if a person's relation to the system is expressed primarily in terms of the self he will be more likely to develop and maintain a sense of detachment, or lack of involvement 52 (commitment), than if he is primarily oriented to the col- lectivity. Therefore: H3: Broadcasters with service-related role-functions may be expected to demonstrate less personal and situational detachment than broadcasters with economic-related role-functions. H4: Broadcasters with economic-related role-functions may be expected to demonstrate more personal and situational detachment than broadcasters with service-related role-functions. The concept of personal and situational detachment is here related to the Parsonian pattern variable termed "affective- neutrality" and attachment or commitment (involvement) to the variable termed "affectivity." It is further assumed that a high degree of commit- ment, or attachment, expressed in terms of role-affectivity, implies an increased probability of normativeness as a value-orientation. Usually, the more a person is committed to a situation, or to an object, the more he has tended to assimilate the values or norms generally associated with it. Commitment, then, logically implies normativeness, which can be taken as an emphasis upon certain general standards of behavior or rules of conduct which may be exhibited by the person in the defined situation or in relation to the speci- fied object. If, as has been suggested, persons occupying service—related positions in the system show evidence of higher levels of commitment, or demonstrate greater role- related affectivity, the following should also hold. 53 H5: Broadcasters with service-related role-functions may be expected to demonstrate greater normative— ness in the situation than broadcasters with economic-related role-functions. H6: Broadcasters with economic-related role-functions may be expected to demonstrate greater expediency in the situation than broadcasters with service- related role-functions. Normativeness may here be expressed in terms of the pattern variable termed "universalism" and expediency in terms of the associated concept of "particularism." Emphasis on expediency, defined in particularistic terms, tends to be linked with detachment (affective neu- trality) and concern for the self, in ways suggestive of the classic definition of "economic man," a type of ration- alistic person who is, by definition, profit-oriented, concerned with the results of the activity, and relatively unconcerned with the means employed to achieve the results sought. This suggests a relationship among high valuations of expediency, detachment and achievement. Therefore: H7: Broadcasters with economic-related role-functions may be expected to be more oriented to standards of performance for significant objects in the situa- tion than are broadcasters with service-related role-functions. H8: Broadcasters with service-related role—functions may be expected to be more oriented to the quali- tative aspects of significant objects in the situation than are broadcasters with economic- related role-functions. Here the pattern variable "ascription (quality) versus achievement (performance)” is considered in relation to the hypotheses stated. $5.816 54 A final aspect of the orientation system delineated in theory needs now to be considered, relative to the system- position proposition. This refers to the scope of signifi- cance of the object, the View that the actor must choose among the various possible ranges in which he will respond to the object. Logically, if concerns for self take prece- dence over concerns for the collectivity in the orientations of broadcasters with economic-related role-functions and if such broadcasters tend also to be more oriented to the sit- uation in terms of expediency, detachment and achievement, they should also show more of a tendency to relate to the situation in terms of its specifics. Those aspects of ob- jects relevant to the situation which tend to affect the productivity and efficiency of the system should be of more concern to broadcasters so oriented. Therefore: H9: Broadcasters with economic-related role-functions may be expected to be more oriented to narrowly- defined expectations of role performance, that is to say the specifics of behavior, than broadcasters with service-related role-functions. H10: Broadcasters with service-related role—functions may be expected to be more oriented to broadly- defined expectations of role performance, that is to say to diffuseness of behaviors, than broad- casters with economic-related role-functions. System Size PrOposition The second proposition proposed for investigation in the present study is derived from a body of literature closely related to and partially integrated with organization theory, Tums 55 at least with those theories which are concerned with the natures and functions of bureaucracies.27 Of special in— terest here is the work of Ferdinand Tonnies, who intro— duced the now familiar Gemeinshaft und Gesellschaft 28 dichotomy. The concept of community and society defined by T5nnies is based essentially upon the proposition that social systems vary in certain fundamental respects as a function of size. Theoretically, the larger the system, the more complex is its pattern of organization, and the more structured are the social relationships engaged in by persons within the system. Basically, large-system behavior (society) differs from small-system behavior (community) as follows: (1) It tends to be defined in terms of specific rules of order, or contractually determined rights and ob- ligations; that is, system rules tend to be specific rather that diffuse. (2) The larger system tends to allocate status by achievement criteria rather than by ascription criteria, that is, to define the meanings of objects in terms of the performances of objects rather than in terms of "inherent" qualities of the objects. (3) It tends to be governed by general norms, or universalistic standards, more 27Robert K. Merton, Ailsa P. Gray, Barbara Hockey and Hanan C. Selvin (eds.), Reader in Bureaucracy, (New York: The Free Press, 1952). 28Ferdinand Tonnies, Community and Society, trans. and edited by Charles P. Loomis, (East Lansing: Michigan State University, 1957). 56 than by particularistic standards. (4) It tends to be gov- erned by elements of self-discipline, or affective neutra- lity, more than by elements of personal gratification, or affectivity, and (5) individuals tend to be ruled by self- interest rather than collectivity-interests, in most situations, at least within the limits of system-universal collective norms. The relationship of the Gemeinshaft und Gesellschaft conceptualization to the pattern-variable formulations of Parsons and Shils is very clear, as is its relationship to the work of Blau and Scott and others. This, then, provides the basis for the second major study proposition, which takes the following form. P2: That an; individual's role-orientation is, in part, a function of system-siie. The hypotheses subsumed under this general proposition fol— low the pattern previously employed relative to the system- position proposition. There are five primary hypotheses and five alternative hypotheses. Each of the hypotheses is based upon an assumed relationship of system—size with one or another of the five pattern variables. Without benefit of any intervening descriptions these are presented below. H11: Broadcasters located in large systems may be ex- pected to be more collectivity-oriented than broadcasters located in small systems. 57 H12: Broadcasters located in small systems may be ex- pected to be more self-orfignted than broadcasters located in large systems. H13: Broadcasters located in large systems may be ex- pected to demonstrate more personal and situational detachment than broadcasters located in small systems. H14: Broadcasters located in small systems may be ex- pected to demonstrate more personal and situational attachment (commitment) than broadcasters located in large systems. H15: Broadcasters located in large systems may be ex- pected to demonstrate greater normativeness in the situation than broadcasters located in small systems. H16: Broadcasters located in small systems may be ex- pected to demonstrate greater expediency in the situation than broadcasters located in large systems. H17: Broadcasters located in large systems may be ex- pected to be more oriented to standards of perfor- mance for significant objects in the situation than broadcasters located in small systems. H18: Broadcasters located in small systems may be ex- pected to be more oriented to the qualitative aspects of significant objects in the situation than broadcasters located in large systems. 29Hypotheses were developed in the form indicated in seeming counterdiction to the theory expressed, which stated that largeness of size is more related to orientations of self-interest, due (1) to the develOpment of professionalism in large systems such as that defined--here recognizing, as did Blau and Scott, that professionalism builds into the normative system an orientation to the collectivity, and (2) to the fact that collectivity-orientation tends to corre— late with level of education and that persons with high education tend to migrate to larger systems and away from smaller systems. In this regard the factor of collectivity— interest is defined in terms of the interest of a broad community or an entire society rather than the small group, the definition implicit in the T6nnies study. 58 H19: Broadcasters located in large systems may be ex- pected to be more oriented to narrowly defined expectations of role performance, that is to say the specifics of behavior, than broadcasters lo— cated in small systems. H20: Broadcasters located in small systems may be ex- pected to be more oriented to broadly defined expectations of role performance, that is to say the diffuseness of behaviors, than broadcasters located in large systems. Reference-Group Identification Proposition The third proposition proposed for investigation in the present study grows out of the on-going work in reference- group theory, some of which has been previously described. Assuming that various groups are used as points of reference for shaping the individual's attitudes, values, and behavior, the types of identifications with groups maintained by a position-occupant in the system may be presumed to influence his orientation to the situation and, more particularly, his role-orientation. b The influence on the individual of his identifica- tions with particular groups has been demonstrated in many of the studies cited. Reissman's description of the "func- tional bureaucrat" as "one who is oriented towards and seeks his recognition from a given professional group outside of rather than within the bureaucracy" is a case in point, as is his description of the "job bureaucrat."30 30Reissman, op. cit., pp. 305-310. Tum: 59 The works of Blau, Thompson, Barnes, and Presthus, and others, also can be used to support the view that the types of group-identifications maintained by the individual located in an organizational setting have strong influences on the individual's orientation to the situation and to his system behavior. March and Simon reinforce this view in their analysis of motivation and intra-organizational decisions, particularly in their description of the functions of the individual's goals in the system.31 "Humans, in contrast to machines," they write, "evaluate their own positions in relation to the value of others and come to accept others' goals as their own. In addition, individual members of an organization come to it with a prior structure of prefer— ences .-. . on the basis of which they make decisions while in the organization."32 March and Simon also identify "four principal targets for identification" for persons functioning within the or- ganizational setting. These are:33 (l) organizations external to the focal organization (i.e., extra-organization identification), (2) the focal organization itself (organizational identification), (3) the work activities involved in the job (task identifi- cation), and (4) subgroups within the focal organization (sub-group identification). 31March and Simon, op. cit., pp. 65-81. 32Ibid., p. 65. 33Ibid. 60 On the basis of these considerations, then, it is possible to deve10p a third study-prOposition, termed the reference- group identification proposition. This can be expressed in the following manner. P3: That an individual's role-orientation is, in part, a function of his reference-group identifications. In this regard reference-groups are defined as "persons or groups that tend to set those standards of [job] performance with which [the broadcaster is] most in agreement."34 Subsumed under this preposition are five hypotheses, and alternate hypotheses, each of which is expressed in terms of one or another of the five pattern variables. It is assumed that different types of reference-groups will in- fluence the individual's role—orientations along these particular dimensions: H21: Broadcasters with primary "out-of—system" reference- group identifications may be expected to be more collectivity-oriented than broadcasters with primary "within-system" reference-group identifications. H22: Broadcasters with primary "within-system" reference- group identifications may be expected to be more self-oriented than broadcasters with primary "out- of-system" reference-group identifications. H23: Broadcasters with primary "out-of—system" reference- group identifications may be expected to demonstrate more personal and situational detachment than broad- casters with primary "within-system" reference-group identifications. H24: Broadcasters with primary "within-system" reference- group identifications may be expected to demonstrate more personal and situational attachment (commitment) 34See Study Questionnaire, Part II, Item number 22. 61 than broadcasters with primary "out—of—system" reference-group identifications. H25: Broadcasters with primary "out-of—system" reference— group identifications may be expected to demonstrate greater normativeness in the situation than broad- casters with primary "within-system" reference-group identifications. H26: Broadcasters with primary "within-system" reference- group identifications may be expected to demonstrate greater expediency in the situation than broadcasters with primary "out-of-system" reference-group identifications. H27: Broadcasters with primary "out-of—system" reference- group identifications may be expected to be more oriented to standards of performance for significant objects in the situation than broadcasters with pri- mary "within-system" reference-group identifications. H28: Broadcasters with primary "within-system" reference- group identifications may be expected to be more oriented to the qualitative aspects of significant objects in the situation than broadcasters with pri- mary "out-of-system" reference-group identifications. H29: Broadcasters with primary "out-of—system" reference- group identifications may be expected to be more oriented to narrowly defined expectations of role performance, that is to say the specifics of behav- ior, than broadcasters with primary "within—system" reference-group identifications. H30: Broadcasters with primary "within-system" reference- group identifications may be expected to be more oriented to broadly defined expectations of role performance, that is to say the diffuseness of be- haviors, than broadcasters with primary "out—of- system" reference-group identifications. "Out-of—system" reference-groups are assumed to be groups related positively to those groups described in the previously cited studies on role-orientation and organiza- tional behavior as "professional groups," and to other extra-organizational groups that appear supportive of a 62 professional frame of reference. The professional broad- caster, then, in this sense, is here assumed to identify positively with out—of—system professional types of groups, associations or organizations. The definition of the pro- fessional used here is the same as that used by Blau and Scott in their investigation of the formal organizations.35 It is also assumed that "within-system" reference- groups are the opposites of those identified above. Broad— casters who relate positively to groups of this type, therefore, can be thought of as "organizationals." The professional and the organizational, then, are presumed to fall at Opposite ends of the pattern-variable spectrum, and the hypotheses presented are drawn accordingly. SecondarypStudprropositions It is possible next to consider certain secondary propositions and related hypotheses relative to the role- orientations of broadcasters. There are two classes of secondary propositions, those relating to personal factors that might be presumed to influence the orientation process and those relating to certain situational factors, or system- related factors, which may also be presumed to influence this process. 35Blau and Scott, op. cit., p. 60. 63 Personal Factors In the development of the present study a number of personal variables were proposed for investigation to deter— mine the degree, if any, of their independent influence on the role-orientation of the broadcaster. Of major interest in this regard are the factors of education, experience, age and sex. An analysis of the literature provides justification for the view that education, for example, is significantly related to that type of orientation described as profes- sional. In this regard it has been suggested that persons with high education are more likely to develop perspectives or points of view that are collectivity—centered, to define relations in specific rather than in general terms, to re- main affectively neutral in the performances of job-related functions, to develop and apply objective and impersonal standards of performance, and to be normatively oriented. Experience, age and sex may be expected to relate positively with similar elements of orientation. System Factors One of the primary study propositions proposed for investigation in the present study states that role- orientation is, in part, a function of system size. The implication of this proposition, broadly considered, is that certain aspects of the situation, certain physical 64 characteristics of the system or controlling environmental factors, have a positive influence on the individual's role-orientation. Several system factors which may be pre- sumed to have such an influence are therefore proposed for investigation. These include factors of station type, station affiliation, type of ownership, and station loca- tion.. It is assumed that each factor will relate positively to the professional role-orientation identified to the ex- tent that it is actually supportive of the primary study prepositions named. What distinguishes the secondary study propositions from the primary study pr0positions is in most cases the lack of a clearly based theoretic rationale for the types of statements made. In effect the propositions preposed for investigation in these regards were derived on an E priori basis and are included for their suggestive value. Data concerning these particular variables, then, should have descriptive value in terms of the present study if not direct theoretic relevance. Role-Behavior Hypotheses One of the assumptions made in the theoretic rationale for the present study is that role-behavior is in large mea- sure a function of the individual's role-orientation. This assumption is, of course, Open to empirical test, given sets of differing role—orientations and certain measurable role- behaviors. 65 Considering the detailed nature of the considera- tions of the present study, relative to the specification of modes of orientation and aspects of influence, it was determined that a complete investigation of role-behavior would not be possible at this time. Nonetheless, in order to develOp a feel for the question, certain role-behavior variables were built into the study. These include the factors of aspiration-level, system—tenure, system-loyalty, mobility, membership in broadcast-related organizations or associations, attendance at conventions or broadcast con- ferences, and degree of activeness in association affairs. Although the role-behaviors named are not of a functional or operational nature with respect to system output, their relationship to the professional orientation described in the preceding sections may be presumed to have theoretic implications. In addition, the acquisition of supportive data, however tentative, for the proposition that role-behavior is a function of the individual's role- orientation can be expected to open up this aspect of the overall problem for further detailed investigation. The hypotheses developed with regard to considera- tions of role-behavior are in accord with those already stated. It is assumed, for example, that each of the role- behaviors named, when expressed in variable form, will to some degree relate to various aspects of role—orientation, again defining role-orientation in terms of the pattern- variable formulations. 66 In particular, it is hypothesized that system-tenure and system-loyalty will be positively related with the orga- nizational side of the pattern-variable spectrum, that is, with situational attachment or commitment, expediency, the qualitative aspects of significant objects, the diffuseness of behaviors, and self-orientation, while the same role- behaviors will be negatively related with the professional side of the pattern-variable spectrum. It is also hypothe- sized that the individual's membership status will be posi- tively related with the professional side of the pattern- variable spectrum, as will his attendance at meetings and the degree of his activeness. Study_Limitations No attempt will be made in this study to describe the organization or structure of the broadcasting industry as a system of mass communication nor will any attempt be made to define the limits of the social system under in- vestigation by describing the pattern of organization of the typical broadcasting station which is taken as the focal situation within which the role-behavior of the broadcaster takes place. In the development of the study the presence of given "system others" is also taken for granted. Therefore no attempt will be made to describe the relationships among the occupants of various positions or to describe the 67 operations performed by each. Similarly, no attempt will be made to define or structure, objectively, the nature or range of the role-expectations which are assumed to be de— velOped and projected by system others. There will in other words be no attempt made to describe the patterns of system- communication engaged in with relation to the encodement and transmission of the "sent role." The interrelationship of the reference-group and the role—orientation, i.e., the process of identification and the process of taking the role of the other, will be des- cribed in brief, but will not specifically be the concern of the study; nor will the process of mediating role-behavior, the manner of interpreting the "sent-role," or the objective consequences of certain types of role-behavior. In effect, then, the feedback circuit will be ignored for the present. Finally, the specifics of the actor's role-behavior, as determined by his role-orientation, will not receive primary attention in this study, although these will be considered to some extent; nor will any attempt be made to link role-behavior with system output. In brief, then, the present study will be concerned principally with (1) developing a means for describing the role—orientation of the broadcaster, as mass communicator, (2) defining the role-orientations or modes of orientation, i.e., the characteristics patterns of relating to the situa— tion,of representative broadcasters, and (3) identifying the 68 factors or processes which might be presumed to influence the orientations identified. SummaEy In these first chapters an attempt was made to in- terrelate various theoretic perspectives which have been used to focus on the nature of roles in an organizational setting. From these statements a theoretic rationale was developed, centering on the individual's role orientation, which appeared to offer certain posSibilities for explain- ing role-behavior in systems of mass communication. For this purpose the theory of action developed by.Parsons and Shils was found to be particularly useful. The concept of the pattern variable was also selected as a means of structuring primary study considerations. A series of pro- positions based on the available literature concerning role- orientations and types of role-behavior in organizational settings was devised and a number of hypotheses were formu- 1ated as a means of testing certain aspects of theory as applied to the role of the broadcaster. The remainder of the study will treat the steps followed in testing the propositions and hypotheses presented. Chapter III will consider the development of the instrument used to delineate the role-orientations of subject broadcasters. Chapter IV will describe the procedures employed in selecting x. the sample and the various problems and limitations associated 69 with the sampling techniques employed. Chapters V and VI will describe the data analysis and Chapter VII, the study conclusions, interpretations and recommendations for further study. CHAPTER III INSTRUMENT DEVELOPMENT The principal instrument which was devised for the present study was the Role—Orientation Measure. The Role- Orientation Measure consists of thirty-eight forced-choice items, each of which is linked in theory to the pattern— variable scheme devised by Talcott Parsons. The procedures employed in the develOpment of this instrument will be des- cribed in this chapter. Background Underlying the pattern-variable theory, as was pre- viously pointed out, is the view that there exists a finite number of determinate choices which a person occupying a ,position within a given social system is required to make lbefore the situation can be said to have meaning. The pattern-variable formulation also holds that eaach of the choices made will be made with reference to sipecific objects, social or material, that are typical of tdne situation. Thus, in the case of the broadcaster, the (flioices made would be expected to relate to objects which tYpically characterize the broadcaster's professional 7O 71 environment, that is, the location in which he carries out his regular functions as a broadcaster. The problem faced in the development of the instru- ment, then, was twofold: (l) to devise a convenient and operable system for relating the pattern-variable scheme to the selected set of objects of orientation; and (2) to de— vise a meaningful and discriminating set of items for inclu- sion on the Role-Orientation Measure. Item Construction A pattern-variable, by definition, is represented by "a dichotomy, one side of which must be chosen by an actor before the meaning of a situation is determinate for him." The theory deve10ped for the present study, therefore, based as it was on the pattern-variable formulation, seemed to re- quire some form of forced—choice procedures.l Although the selection of the forced-choice method seemed most logical, it was recognized that certain limita- tions, or dangers, were associated with its use. Kerlinger has emphasized, for example, the technical and psychological difficulties involved, "among which the most important seem to be the lack of independence of items, the perhaps too 1"The essence of the forced-choice method," Ker- linger writes, "is that the subject must choose among alternatives." '(Kerlinger, 0p cit., p. 496.) 72 complex nature of some items . . . and the resistance of subjects to difficult choices."2 Because of this limitation, it was thought necessary to modify the forced-choice procedure. Fortunately this was not entirely without precedent. For instance, Stouffer 3 and Toby, in their study "Role Conflict and Personality," made use of Guttman type scales when testing certain appli- cations of pattern—variable theory. The results of their investigation, moreover, suggested the existence of an under- lying structure, such as that initially described by Lazars- feld,4 which bridged the poles of the pattern-variable cxonsidered, producing what could be termed a pattern-variable continuum. The idea of a continuous, as Opposed to a discrete, puattern-variable appeared worthy of consideration. It was knelieved that an instrument constructed along such lines might avoid some of the problems associated with the more cxnrventional forced-choice procedures. A scale of this type 2Kerlinger, Op cit., p. 499. 38. A. Stouffer and J. Toby, "Role Conflict and Per- sonality," Toward a General Thegy of Acti_o_r_1_, edited by Parsons and Shils, op. cit., pp. 481-496- 4P. F. Larzarsfeld, "The Logical and Mathematical Foundation of Latent-Structure Analysis," Chapter 10, Mea- sgrement and PredictiOll, Vol. IV, Studies in Social Psy- C—.°\Ogy in World War II, Science Editions, (New York: John Wiley & Sons, Inc., 1966), pp. 362-412. 73 was in fact suggested by John C. McKinney, in a paper titled "The Typological Tradition."5 In his paper, McKinney presented a review of work originally conducted by Loomis and others in a particular social system. In this system, roles were described by means of pattern-variable constructs. The procedure involved hav- ing a group of judges chart a profile of each role in ques- tion by responding to a series of pattern-variable scales, of the type presented below. Each scale contained one side of a single pattern-variable on either end of a nine interval continuum, much like that of a Semantic Differential Scale. A profile for the role in question was secured by drawing connecting lines through the points which represented the mean responses of the experts on each of the scales. Pattern-Variable Scale Affective- . . neutrality .__.__.__.__.__.__.__.__.__. Affect1V1ty Universalism : : : : : : : : : : Particularism The work of Milton Rokeach6 also influenced to some extent the final form of the Role—Orientation Measure. IROkeach made extensive use of a forced-choice procedure in ¥ 5John C. McKinney, "The Typological Tradition, in IFerdinand TOnnies, Community and Society, Trans. and Edited 13y Charles P. Loomis, (East Lansing: Michigan State Univer- sity Press, 1957) . 6Milton Rokeach, The Open and Closed Mind, (New York: Basic Books, 1960) . 74 his investigations of Open and closed belief systems. The Rokeach Dogmatism Scale, for example, requires that a sub- ject respond to each item in terms of relative levels of agreement or disagreement, as follows:7 +1: I AGREE A LITTLE -l: I DISAGREE A LITTLE +2: I AGREE ON THE WHOLE -2: I DISAGREE ON THE WHOLE +3: I AGREE VERY MUCH -3: I DISAGREE VERY MUCH It is not only important for the subject to respond to each item on the Rokeach scale but also to respond by voicing either agreement or disagreement with the item in question. The Rokeach scale does not provide for "neutral," "undecided," or "no response" categories. These, then, were the three sources which influenced the form and style of the Role-Orientation Measure. The idea of testing subject responses to a series of situational prob- lems, each one of which might be constructed to reflect a pattern-variable dilemma,8 was taken from Stouffer and Toby. The technique of using a continuum was borrowed from Loomis and his associates. This was done in an attempt to avoid ssome of the pitfalls of the simple dichotomous choice. Fi- IIally, the idea of employing relative values of forced-choice ‘vas drawn from Rokeach. The requirement of non—neutrality —.¥ 7R0keaCh' 9p. Cit. ’ pp. 72-73. 8For the purposes of the present study it was found more effective to make use of contrasting statements, or Points of view, which could be defined as paired Opposites. also was taken from Rokeach. Each element cited appeared in keeping with the requirements of the pattern-variable sys- tem and therefore applicable to the Role-Orientation Measure . What follows is an example of the form and style adOpted for the Role-Orientation Measure, a form which was believed in keeping with the pattern-variable concept and which seemed to take into account the experiences of other researchers. Statement A In a well organized and effi- ciently run station, every person on the staff is ex- pected to have specifically defined duties and responsi- bilities which may be regu- lated on the basis of quite generally recognized and understood rules and regula— tions. 4'. O. (3) (2)“ (l) 4 Statement B Broadcasting is not a busi— ness which lends itself well to structured bureaucratic type operations. Staff re- lations and activities, there- fore, tend to be more diffuse than specific and based more generally upon subjective type considerations. -(l) (2) (3) The two statements above are assumed to reflect one side or the other of the pattern-variable identified as "specificity versus diffuseness." The subject is required to select the statement that appears best to characterize his view of the situation. His selection is also relative, to a degree, in that he is asked to assign a particular weight to his choice. The assigned weights are as follows: 76 g.- ll Tend to Agree 2 Agree Very Much 3 = Agree Absolutely Under this procedure the subject is required to select both the statement he prefers and the relative strength of his preference. At no time, however, is he asked specifically to reject a given statement. Failure to select, then, must not be taken to signify disagreement in the usual sense. Scoring Procedure The scoring procedure employed by the Role-Orientation Measure follows Rokeach, with a single difference. While Rokeach assumed a positive and a negative side to the re- sponses, it is assumed in the present instance that both sides of the scale are positive. However, like Rokeach, an assignment is made of scores ranging from one to seven on each item. The principal scoring rule is as follows. The extreme scale position "3" is scored as a seven if, and only if, that statement selected by the subject can be said to re- flect a given side of the specified pattern-variable. Other— wise, the extreme scale position "3" is scored as a gag. Certain of the statements, then, were pre-selected anddesignated as "approved" statements, not in the sense of being more valued, but in the sense of their reflecting cer- tain theoretically assumed relationships. In the list of 77 pattern-variables which follows, those elements which are listed in Column A are taken to be "approved" while those listed in Column B are taken to be "non-approved." Speci— fication of approved status for the pattern-variables listed reflects the point of view of the "professional," as described by Blau and Scott.9 A. B. Affective- 4/ \_ . . Neutrality .\ 7, Affect1V1ty Universalism 4< ); Particularism Achievement j, \_ Ascription (Performance) ‘T 7' (Quality) Specificity :( r )= Diffuseness Collectivity- \_ . . Orientation \ ,7 Self Orientation If reference is made to the example given on page six, Statement "A" will be found to be the "approved" state- ment in that it reflects the left side of the specificity- diffuseness pattern-variable. Therefore a subject's score on this particular item would be read in the following manner: Statement A Statement B Score: : 7 : 6 : 5 : : 3 : 2 : l : Response: (3) (2) (1) (l) (2) (3) Another important feature in the scoring system is that a subject may receive a score of "4," the hypothetical 9Blau and Scott, op. cit., pp. 60-62. 78 center of the scale, only in cases of missing data or when it is otherwise impossible to determine which of the two statements the subject is registering agreement with; that is, which side of the pattern-variable he favors. Item Selection The development of items for the Role-Orientation Measure involved much intuition and critical judgment. An attempt was made whenever possible, however, to select real statements, drawn either from the available literature or from observation, which were both relevant and suggestive. The standard of relevance required that a statement relate meaningfully and critically to the situation under study. The standard of suggestiveness required that the statement reflect a pattern-variable consideration. The following statement meets both standards adequately. It is drawn from David Ogilvy's Confessions of an Advertising Man.10 The executive is inevitably a father figure. To be a good father, whether it is to his children or to his associates, requires that he be understanding, that he be considerate, and that he be human enough to be affectionate. This statement involves (l) a personal judgement (2) made with reference to a’relationship that is said to exist (3) between an executive and his associates, or staff. As des- cribed by Ogilvy, this relationship is affectively charged, 10David Ogilvy, Confessions of an Advertising Man, (New York: Dell Publishing, 1963), p. 19. 79 as Opposed, it may be assumed, to a system of relations built upon a general rule of affective neutrality. The student of formal organization is aware that one of the more critical areas of concern in organization life is that involving staff—management relations, as re— flected in administrative style, interpersonal behavior, and so forth. It seemed quite reasonable, therefore, to consider this same area of concern in the present study. The Ogilvy statement quoted above, appropriately modified, seemed to have the making of a significant item. Items of similar nature were also drawn from books and articles on broadcasting, quotations of broadcasters which appeared in various publications, and other standard sources. In addition, oral comments made by broadcasters, both public and private, were found to be valuable sources of useable items. A statement made by CBS programming Vice President Michael Dann, which suggested that he never based network programming decisions upon his own personal taste and interests, typlified a form of detachment or affective :neutrality, and thus formed the basis for yet another item.11 .A.statement made by a sales manager of a large market tele- ‘vision station to a college audience, implying that a program sshonld be viewed chiefly as "a hooker," a device used to ' ‘ 11Time, October, 1966. 80 attract a large number of viewers to the sponsor's message, was cast in the form of a collectivity-self-orientation item.12 Given a useful and revealing statement, the next task involved the reformulation of the thought in terms of its opposite. For example, the source statement might read as follows, The broadcaster may be expected or required ligitimately to carry out in action only responsibilities which have been specifically defined by law or ordered by the pub- lished rules of the FCC. This statement clearly assigns the aspect of specificity to the broadcaster-commission relationship. However, in its present form it suggests only one side of this dilemma of choice. Therefore it would be necessary to recast the statement in the form of its opposite for use as a pattern- variable item. The FCC has the right to require any behavior from the broadcaster which it deems to be pr0per and in keeping with "the public interest," with or without the appli- cation of a specific rule or section of law. The statement as now phrased clearly tends to reflect the aspect of diffuseness. For the sake of discussion (subject to test) it may be assumed that the two statements listed above are "true" cpposites and that the tendency of the broadcaster to agree with either the one or the other will reflect his view of the relationship in question as either 12Observation of the researcher, Fall, 1966. 81 specific or diffuse. It was in this manner, and by these means, that the items were devised for the Role-Orientation Measure 0 Pre-Testing_the Instrument The Role-Orientation Measure passed through a series of revisions and was subjected to two major pre-tests before it was considered ready for use in the field. Form A of the instrument, the form used in the initial pre-test, consisted of a set of twenty-five paired statements designed according to the specifications discussed above. Each of the five pat- tern variables was paired with each of five objects of ori- entation in the form of a 5 x 5 matrix of items. The items were arranged on the instrument in random order with the positions of "A" statements and "B" statement periodically reversed to avoid "response-set" problems. The instrument in this form was subjected to test on two small groups during the Summer of 1966. S1 (N = 19) was composed of a group of graduate students in broadcasting education at Michigan State University, many of whom had had broadcasting experience. S (N = 18) was drawn from a popu- 2 lation of active broadcasters located in the area of central Texas. Because of problems of time and convenience neither sample was drawn at random from its respective population. Form B of the instrument, the form used in the second pre-test, consisted of a set of seventy-two paired statements. 82 This form of the instrument was subjected to test with a group of thirty-eight advanced undergraduate and graduate students (S3) in broadcast station management at Michigan State during the Fall of 1966. The initial pre-test was intended to serve two func- tions: (1) as a check of content validity and reliability, and (2) as a check on the clarity and ease of application of the study procedures. The second pre-test was concerned with more theoretic considerations. It was devised chiefly as a check on the relative independence of the pattern- variable dimensions which, for test purposes, were defined operationally by certain combinations of items.13 In addition to these functions, both pre-tests were intended to serve as the basis for selecting items of high discrimination value for possible inclusion on Form C of the Role-Orientation Measure, the form which was to be used in the final study. For this purpose an arbitrary criterion was set, making use of the mean score of the item. If the l3This consideration was tested by means of factor analysis. Space does not permit a detailed description of the results of analysis here, however. At this point we need only say that those items were selected for the final study which seemed "best" to support pattern-variable con— siderations; that is, those items which tended to cluster in some form of stable relationship and which also seemed to reflect given pattern variables. While the results of the analysis werermnuperfect--certain variables tended to collapse--the "fit" appeared sufficient to justify further investigations in terms of the original study design. 83 mean of an item fell within a score range of from three to five, the item was classified as potentially "useful"; that is, it was said to discriminate sufficiently to be of pos- sible use on the final form of the instrument.14 An example of an item which appeared to discriminate and which thus proved to be useful for the purposes of the study is shown below. The pattern-variable designation of the item is that of Performance versus Quality. The oriented object is presumed to be the program. Statement A Statement B The broadcaster's primary con- The broadcaster's primary cern with any particular pro- concern with any particular gram must be in terms of program must be in terms of whether or not the program its inherent merit, value or serves its intended purpose, quality, independent of any in the delivery of audience, purpose it may otherwise the sale of products, or serve. whatever. The mean response of S1 subjects to the item in this form was 4.2 and the mean response of 52 subjects was 4.8. The usefulness of this particular item may also be estimated on the basis of both S and S4 responses. These means along 3 with their respective standard deviations are presented in Table 3.1. 4A mean four would, quite obviously, indicate a perfect distribution of subject responses. 84 Table 3.l.--Sample responses to forced choice item. S1 82 83 S4 Item Mean 4.20 4.80 3.86 3.43 Item S.D. 1.57 1.78 1.62 1.81 U) I 1 Student Sample, First Pre-Test, Summer, 1966. (D I 2 Broadcaster Sample, First Pre-Test, Summer, 1966. U) l 3 Student Sample, Second Pre-Test, Fall, 1966. (D l 4 Broadcaster Sample, National Study, Spring, 1967. Content Validation Two types of content validation were employed in the develOpment of the Role-Orientation Measure. The first con- cerned the selection of a set of relevant objects with refer- ence to the broadcaster's environment. The second concerned treatment and construction, that is, the method of relating each object to the pattern-variable scheme. A relevant Ob- ject was defined as any object which might logically evoke certain pattern-variable considerations. Items on the Role-Orientation Measure were validated with regard to both concerns, first, on the basis of private judgement and observation, and, second, by means of the Opinion Of a panel of qualified judges. 85 A thorough examination of published works on broad- casting revealed a number of objects which appeared to be critically related to the broadcaster role. TheSe included the client, the audience, the program, staff, manager-owner, government, network, community, and other job-related con- cerns. Confirmation of the relative importance of these and other objects was obtained by means of a series of interviews with broadcasters in central Texas during the Spring and Summer of 1965. Selection of the objects to be used for test purposes was made on the basis of which of the objects identi- fied seemed to be most generally applicable; that is, which seemed to have high relevance to broadcasters in different situations, considering size of market, station type, and so forth. Selection was also made on the basis of a presumed balance, the objects being viewed as neither too critical (and thus too highly controversial) nor too neutral, objects which a person might take too much for granted. Out of regard for these criteria, objects such as the following were passed over: Networks, Unions, Congress, The FCC, the NAB, Rating Services, Community Leadership, and others equally critical. It goes without saying, then, that any re-development of the Role-Orientation Measure in terms of "completeness" Of role considerations would need to take into account such objects as these in addition to the ones contained on Form C of the questionnaire. 86 The five objects of orientation selected for study are combined with the five pattern variables in Table 3.2. Table 3.2.--Combinations of objects of orientation, m 1 Pattern Variables Objects . Manager/ Client Program Audience Staff Owner Collectivity-Self _ _ Orientation +/- +/- +/- +/ +/ Affective- Neutrality- +/- +/- +/- +/- +/— Affectivity Universalism- Particularism Achievement— Ascription _ _ _ _ _ (Performance- +/ +/ +/ +/ +/ Quality) Specificity- _ _ _ _ _ Diffuseness +/ +/ +/ +/ +/ Following the selection and validation of the objects, items making use of the objects were constructed and patterned after the examples described earlier.6 The items were then submitted to a panel of professional broadcasting educators at Michigan State University, each of whom was asked to re- view the items critically in terms of content, construction and presumed relevance to the concerns of the study. In addition, the pre-test procedure employed called for a simple validation check. Sample Group 1, the student 87 sample, was asked to write comments and critically review the instrument following its completion. Each member of Sample Group 2, the broadcaster sample, also agreed to an interview concerning the nature of the instrument, its con— tent and treatment. In each instance the original formula- tion of the study received support. The greatest measure of criticism received, both from the written comments and from the interviews, related to the forced-choice procedure which was employed in the instrument design. As one broad- caster reported: At a place or two I got the feeling that you were ask- ing me if I had quit beating my wife. (I asked her, and she said: "Tell them you never startedl") But that doesn't fit under Statement A or B. Instrument Reliability An important consideration in the develOpment of the Role-Orientation Measure concerned the matter Of reliability. Here reliability is referred to in terms of Kerlinger's first definition; that is, "in stability, dependability, predict- ability terms."15 The method employed in determining the reliability of the instrument is that most commonly referred to as a "split-half" estimate of reliability. The various subjects in the pre-test samples were numbered and assigned to odd- even groups. Pairs of subjects were then drawn and correlation 15Kerlinger, op. cit., p. 430. 88 coefficients computed among items. The entire set of twenty-five items was checked in the first pre-test and a random selection of fifteen items was checked in the second pre-test. The mean of the separate sets of matched-pair correlations was used as the final estimate of reliability for the split-half. Split-half reliability was then sub- jected to correction by means of the Spearman-Brown formula,16 providing an estimate of reliability for the instrument as a whole. The resulting estimates are reproduced below. tt S1 .82 .90 52 .94 .97 S3 .82 .90 Item Distribution Having checked for content validity and reliability, as well as for ease of application and discrimination, a final selection of items was made for the study. The items selected were distributed in terms of a cross-tabulation of pattern-variable constructs and oriented objects. In several instances, however, due to the substantive nature of some of the items or to existing questions regarding the power of the items, more than a single item was selected to fill a 16J. P. Guilford, Fundamental Statistics in Psychology and Education, (New York: McGraw-Hill, 1956), pp. 452-453} 89 particular cell. For these reasons the Role-Orientation Measure as finally constructed was composed of a total of thirty-eight items rather than twenty-five as might have been expected. The thirty-eight items were arranged as shown below.17 The items were, of course, randomly distri— buted with the positions of "A" statements and "B" statements reversed periodically to avoid "response-set" problems. Table 3.3.--Distribution of test items by number. Manager/ Client Audience Program Staff Owner Collectivity-Self . l8 Orientation 30 3 37 27 15 Universalism- 6 10 2 29 l Particularism 24 20 35 11 Performance- 12 25 23 21 8 Quality 19 Affective— Neutrality- 9 28 14 22 17 Affectivity 34 33 26 31 Specificity- 4 13 32 7 5 Diffuseness 16 38 17 Form C of the instrument, that employed in the study, is reproduced in the Appendix. The item numbers used in the distribution presented are the same as those employed in Form C. 90 Summary In this chapter a description of the processes em— ployed in the develOpment of the Role-Orientation Measure, the principal study instrument, was presented. This included such concerns as item selection, treatment and construction, validation and measures of reliability. In the chapter that follows the process of sample selection, the procedures em- ployed in the development of the study, and certain related problems, will be considered. CHAPTER IV SAMPLE DESCRIPTION AND STUDY PROCEDURES The procedures employed in the national study will be considered in the present chapter. A description of the sampling techniques used and a general discussion of the characteristics of the sample will also be presented. In addition, a special section will be devoted to a considera— tion of questions of methodology with particular reference' to the use of the self-administered mail questionnaire as a primary source of data. Population Description For the purpose of this study the assumed pOpulation was defined as all persons regularly employed on a full-time basis in licensed commercial broadcasting stations within the territorial limits of the United States who, at the time of the study, occupied positions of managerial responsibility in other than technical fields. These were classified by title or function and included General Managers, Station Managers, Program Directors, News Directors and Sales Man— agers. Not included were network personnel, officers and agents of group owners not directly involved in station 91 92 operation, performers, producers, writers, directors, or technicians of any class. Defined in this manner, the population corresponds to the middle level of functional responsibility described by Talcott Parsons in his analysis of the formal organiza- tion.1 Parsons' three levels of responsibility include the "technical" level, a form of suborganization composed of in- dividuals primarily concerned with the performance of certain Operational functions, the "managerial" level, a higher order suborganization charged chiefly with administrative functions, and the "institutional" level, that suborganization mainly responsible for policy-making functions and for articulating system-needs with regard to the broader social order of com- munity or society. Sampling Procedures Because of the character of the population, it was believed that one or both of two study techniques would need to be employed to yield the data required for a proper con- sideration of the propositions and hypotheses described earlier. The first, and preferred, method was that of the field survey, based upon random sampling procedures and con- ducted by means of the structured or focused interview. Un- fortunately this procedure involved factors of cost and time lTalcott Parsons, Structureyand Process in Modern Societies, (Glencoe, Illinois: The Free Press, 1960), pp. 16-96. 93 that were beyond the means of the researcher. The alterna- tive method was that Of the mail questionnaire, a procedure which, in spite of its many limitations, has been defended by Roy Carter in a consideration of field methods in communi- cation research.2 A variation of the standard mail questionnaire design was devised for the current study, in an effort to maximize questionnaire returns and to minimize costs. Instead of in- cluding the study questionnaire in the participation request, a letter was first sent inviting the subject to participate in the project. Only those subjects who expressed a "willing- ness" to participate in the study, then, actually received the study questionnaire. In addition to the use of the participation request procedure, an effort was made to stimulate questionnaire returns by the use of printed materials, by personally ad- dressing all correpondence, by stressing the importance of the study, by emphasizing the confidential nature of each individual return, by labeling the study with a "quality" identification—-The American Broadcaster Study Project--and by making use of a private post office box in East Lansing for study purposes. 2Roy E. Carter, Jr., "Field Methods in Communication Research," IntroduCtion to Mass Communication Research, edited by RaIph O. Nafziger and David W. White, (Baton Rouge, Louisiana: Louisiana State University Press, 1958), pp. 78-127. 94 Finally, an attempt was made to avoid bias in the selection of the primary sample3 and to insure its repre- sentativeness by applying certain rules of order in the selection process. These rules, in complete form, are ap- pended. By this means a primary sample of 542 broadcasters, representing as many different radio and television stations, was drawn in near random order from the published listings contained in the 1967 edition of the Broadcasting Yearbook.4 Each of the broadcasters selected was sent a "per- sonal" mailing inviting participation in the study. A business reply postcard was enclosed in each request letter for the convenience of the respondent. A total of 320 post- cards were returned. A study questionnaire was subsequently mailed to each Of these "willing" respondents, following a check of the approximate representativeness of the returns. This subsample of 320 broadcasters can be viewed, then, as the actual study sample in that only these broadcasters re- ceived study questionnaires. Sample Description The national sample employed in the present study was compared with certain known characteristics of the assumed 3The primary sample is composed of those persons initially requested to participate in the study project. 4Broadcasting Yearbook, (Washington, D. C.: Broad- casting Publications, 1967). 95 population, in an effort to determine its relative repre- sentativeness. The first characteristic considered was that of the regional distribution of respondents' stations, by type. Information relating to this consideration is pre- sented in the following tables. Table 4.1 provides a break- down, by number and percentage, Of the "on air" radio and television stations in each of four geographical regions in the United States, the North Central,5 the Eastern,6 the Southern,7 and the Western.8 The figures presented here are known to differ somewhat, for the reasons given, from the "true" population but are nonetheless taken to represent the population sufficiently for the purposes of this study. Therefore, unless otherwise noted, these are the base line figures employed in computing all sample frequencies. Making use Of the near-random sampling procedures devised for the present study, a total of 542 broadcasters were selected from the defined population. These broadcasters 5Illinois, Indiana, Iowa, Kansas, Michigan, Minne- sota, Missouri, Nebraska, North Dakota, Ohio, South Dakota, and Wisconsin. 6Connecticut, Delaware, District of Columbia, Maine, Maryland, Massachusetts, New Hampshire, New Jersey, New York, Pennsylvania, Rhode Island, Vermont, and West Virginia. 7Alabama, Arkansas, Florida, Georgia, Kentucky, Louisiana, Mississippi, North Carolina, Oklahoma, South Carolina, Tennessee, Texas, and Virginia. 8Alaska, Arizona, California, Colorado, Hawaii, Idaho, Montana, Nevada, New Mexico, Oregon, Utah, Washington, and wyoming. 96 Table 4.1.--Distribution of stations in population by geo- graphical regions.* M E = Television Perct. Radio Perct. Total Perct. North Central States 157 29% 913 24% 1070 25% Eastern States 90 17 657 17 747 17 Southern States 171 31 1462 39 1633 38 Western States 127 23 756 20 883 20 Totals 545 100% 3788 100% 4333 100% *Frequency count of broadcasting stations is not a true and accurate count of stations actually on the air at the time of the study. The figures presented reflect certain defini- tions which were devised for sampling purposes. (See Sampling Rules of Order, Appendix B.) The 1967 Broadcasting Yearbook, for example, reports that as of November 10, 1966 there were a total of 617 commercial TV stations on the air (including UHF stations) and 4,093 AM radio stations on the air. made up the primary sample. The distribution of the stations involved in the primary sample is indicated in Table 4.2. The numbers and frequencies, by station type and geographical region, compare quite favorably with the true population pro- portions. One of the problems observed is the use of a mail questionnaire is that no matter how much care is exercised in the selection of the primary sample, the failure of numerous subjects to respond creates a considerable risk that the final sample--the actual study sample—-will not match the population pattern described. It is not surprising, then, 97 Table 4.2.--Distribution of sampled stations by geographical regions.* Television Perct. Radio Perct. Total Perct. North Central States 55 28% 94 27% 149 27% Eastern States 42 21 56 16 98 18 Southern States 56 28 130 37 186 34 Western States 45 23 64 19 109 20 Totals 198 100% 344 99% 542 99% *Differences in total percentages due to rounding error. to discover that certain discrepancies do appear in some of the tables that follow, particularly those which relate specifically to the nature and composition, first, of the sample of so-called "willing" subjects (those who indicated a willingness to participate in the study), and, second, those who actually completed and returned study questionnaires. In spite of the problem of attrition, an analysis Of Table 4.3 reveals that the total returns on cards, considered in terms of station type and region, do not vary as much as might be feared. The average difference, in fact, is no more than three or four percentage points, with the exception of the returns received from Southern radio stations, which were high by seven points. A close examination of the composition Of the returns, as shown in the table, indicates, moreover, Lil 98 .mmmH no cusumm womN wmm u mumosgmm mo mmmucmo numm mm cusumm ONO u magnumm Hmuo9 mvm u mummsvmm Hmuoa Nmm NOO NON NON NmO NON OOH NNN NHN NNN "mmmuamonmm cucumm OOH HmH mm ON *OO Nm mm ON Om NO .umm HON OOH «O OH ONH mm mm NO ON mm .Omm "2309 {ON mm 9O OH {NO NO *NO NO mH OO .umm “muouomnHo O9 NO NH NH mN OH OH OH NN NH .Omm mgmz mm OH NH 9O HN HH NH OO OH NH .umm "muouomNHo ON OH OH HH OH NH NH OH HN OH .Omm smumoum *ON OH «HO mO POH NO OO OH HH NH .umm "mummmcmz NO NO OH OH ON mH OH HH OH OH .wmm mmHmm HO *mN NH *OO . NN IOO *NO *mO OH OH .uwm “mummmcmz OOH Om HN NH OH OH OH HH Nm mH .Omm :oHumum oHOmm >9 oHOmm >9 oHOmm >9 oHOmm >9 oHOmm >9 mamuoa cumummz cumcusom cumummm Hmnucmo nunoz ,.Om>HmomH mcusumn manmuo>mm mo Hones: map can whommumo zoom :H coaummwowpumm wpsum How mummsvmu mo Hones: on» mchmOHUcH .coflmmu Hmowcmmnmomm 0cm mama coflumum .GOHUHmom ma .OHQEOm mo cowuwmomEOO|l.m.v OHQOB 99 that while the total percentage of return was not greatly out of line (when compared to the composition of the pri- mary sample) there was above average variation in the re- turns received from persons occupying different positions. Most notable is the fact that returns received from station managers averaged fifty percent or less in four out of the eight station categories while those received from sales managers, program directors and news directors averaged well above fifty percent in most cases. Small discrepancies--variations from category per— centages set in the primary sample--may also be noted in both Table 4.4 and Table 4.5, which reveal the composition of the study sample itself, based, first, on the number of completed questionnaires, not excluding a certain percentage of non- useable returns or returns that arrived too late for analy- sis, and, second, on the final study sample, which includes several questionnaires circulated for control purposes. While small discrepancies may be Observed, they appear not to be of sufficient magnitude to markedly affect the basic representativeness of the study sample, at least along the dimensions indicated. The final table in this particular series represents the composition of the study sample which is made up of those respondents whose complete and useable returns form the basis for the analysis contained in the chapters which follow. This sample is composed of 247 respondents. Of this 100 wow u mmHHOGOOHummsO mo ommucmoumm wmv u mmmucmoumm mHmEmm unmEHHm Hem n H8909 Ocmuw NOOH NOOH NNH NON Nmm NON NON NNN NHN Nmm "mmmucmoumm va OHH mm om Ow mm mm mm HO mm mHmuOB "mHOHOOHHQ hm om mo no mo mo mo 9o OH 9o m3mz “mHOHOOHHO vm mm we we mH mo mo mo _ mo mo Emumoum “mummmcmz mm Hm mo Ho mo 9o mo mo mo HH mmHmm "mummmcmz mv Hm mo mo 9H mo we we mH mo OOHumum OHUmm >9 Owcmm >9 OHUmm >9 OHUmm >9 OHUmm >9 mHmu09 cumumoz cuwsusom cumummm Hmuucmo anoz .Uocusumu mmHHmccoHpmmsw mo Hmnfisc Hmuou on» coma comma .conmH HOOHnmmumomm can mmmu coHumum .GOHuHmom an .mHmEmm mo coHuHmomEOO|l.v.w mHnm9 101 number 232 are a carry over from the primary sample of 542 subjects who were initially requested to participate in the study. In conventional terms this represents a 43 per- cent useable return. If only the number of questionnaires circulated is considered, however, this represents a return of about 73 percent. To this number of completed questionnaires we add those which were returned by members of a select "control sample" of 40 broadcasters. This sample was devised for two reasons; first, as a check on the usefulness, methodolo— gically speaking, of the business reply participation request procedure and, second, for the purpose of achieving a better balance of returns from station managers. To gain these objectives a random sample of 32 television station managers and eight radio station managers were drawn from the 1967 Broadcasting Yearbook. These particular managers received a COpy of the study questionnaire directly with a simple re- quest to complete and return. NO follow-up requests were made. A total of 15 useable returns were received from this sample. These are included, in the appropriate categories, in Table 4.5. A comparison of Table 4.5 with Table 4.4 suggests that the addition of fifteen additional station managers did tend to balance the representation of respondents by position so far as the total composition of the study sample is con- cerned. This may be demonstrated by dividing the totals of 102 .mocmumcH comm OH 90990 mcHocsou an 909 omucsooom mum mHmuou OH coHumHHm> .mHmEmm mHmEHHm mo .mnommumo an .ommucmoumm cam mHmEmm nosum HOGHM mo mHmuou mo mmmucmoumm cmmzumn mocmHOMMHQN NH: wml Nvl NH: Nv+ Nm+ Nv+ wm+ NumocmHOMMHQ NooH NooH NmHl Nom Nmm Nnm mom Nmm NHm Nom "unmoumm HMH mHH mm mm mv Hm mm nm ow mm umHmHO9 "mnouomHHQ om om mo no mo mo mo mo mo no m3mz “muouomHHo mm mm mo mo MH mo mo mo mo mo Emumonm "mummmcmz mm mm mo mo mo no mo oH mo oH mOHmm “mummmcmz ow mm oH mo mH mo mo mo «H oH cOHHmum OHomm >9 OHomm >9 OHomm >9 OHUmm >9 OHUmm >9 mHmu09 cumummz cumcusom cumummm Hmnucmo nuuoz H.mHmEmm Houucoo map 8099 UO>HOOOH mcusumu mHnmmms mosHocH ocm mcusumu OHnmmmslcoc HHm mmmH mum omucmmmnm mmusmHm oc9v .GOHUOH HOOHcm Imnmoom paw waxy GOHumum .GOHuHmom ma .mHmEmm wosum Hmch mo GOHuHmomEOOII.m.O mHam9 103 each column by the number of positions, which is equal to four in each case. The resulting figure is the average number of respondents which might be expected for each cell in any particular column. The difference between the ob- served frequency and the expected frequency is the cell dis- crepancy. By summing the total number of cell discrepancies, without regard to sign, and by dividing by the number of cells--which is equal to 32--the average cell discrepancy acquired is 1.8, a relatively low figure. It is concluded, therefore, that the addition of the control sample aided in achieving a reasonably well balanced study sample, consider— ing position, type of station and geographical region. As explained above, however, one of the primary reasons for adding a secondary control sample was methodologi- cal. There was an interest in determining whether or not the study procedure of requesting subject participation in ad- vance was a useful device. Table 4.6 which follows, provides a partial answer to this particular question. On the basis of the returns reported, it must be concluded that it did not. NO real differences were observed in the proportion of returns received from the primary sample (the request sample so-called) and the control sample. Although only station managers were involved in the selection of a control sample, it is assumed that were a similar check made of per- sons in other positions the same results would likely hold. 104 Table 4.6.--A comparison of primary sample and control sample returns.* Card N Return % Q-Return % Sample % Primary Sample Returns Television Sample 198 131 66% 104 79% 53% Radio Sample 344 189 55 128 68 37 Total 542 320 59 232 73 43 Control Sample Returns (Managers Only) Television Sample 32 --— --- 12 38 38 Radio Sample 08 --- --- 03 38 38 Total 40 --- --- 15 38 38 Primary Sample Returns (Managers Only) Television Sample 54 25 46 21 84 37 Radio Sample 108 61 57 44 72 41 Total 162 86 51 65 76 4o *Only useable returns reported. The question regarding the general representative- ness of the study sample involves several considerations in addition to those described. Two areas of concern appear especially pertinent. The first relates to certain personal considerations; the second, to certain system concerns. A prOper analysis of each will involve a comparison of the ob- served sample characteristic with certain known or assumed population dimensions. 105 The immediate problem which arises with regard to the first consideration is the fact that no actual census of broadcast station personnel exists. Thus, for purposes of comparison, it was necessary to rely upon certain published descriptions of other samples which were drawn from the same or similar pOpulations. Here it was possible to utilize two studies which made use of management level personnel in broadcasting. The first of these is the PeOple In Broadcast- ing study conducted some years ago by the Association for Professional Broadcasting Education and the National Asso- ciation of Broadcasters.9 The second is that conducted by Charles Winick on the occupational role of the television 10 Both studies provide some basic insight station manager. with reference to certain relevant characteristics of their respective samples. A match of these characteristics is made, for comparison purposes, against the present study sample in Table 4.7. Allowing for the fact that some nine years separates the studies reported,11 it is interesting to note that few differences can be discerned between the three sample profiles presented. This would seem to indicate, 9N.A.B., People in Broadcastin , (Washington, D. C.: National Association of Broadcasters, l 62). 10Charles E. Winick, "The Television Station Manager, Advanced Management Journal, 31:1 (January, 1966), pp. 53-60. llThe APBE-NAB data was actually gathered in a field study initiated in 1958. 106 Table 4.7.--A comparison of certain personal characteristics of respondents. APBE-NAB Survey Winick Study Present Study (N=366) (N=287) (N=247) Characteristics TV Radio TV Managers TV and Radio Present Age of Re- n n spondents: 43.8 40.7 Eacly 405 40.7 Years Experience in Broadcasting: 16.2 14.0 15 16.5 Years of System Tenure: 8.8 7.5 7 to 8 9.5 Years of Position ____ ____ 5 5 9 Tenure: ' Previous Experience by Type and Per- centage: Sales Experience: 44.7% 40.8% 40% 42.9% Pr°9r9mmlng Ex" 25 1 26 3 40 31.2 perience: ' ° Engineering Ex- 10 3 8 5 10 9 7 perience: ' ° ° Other Type Ex- perience: 3.1 3.6 10 9.7 Education Level, by Type and Percentage: Attended High School 99.4% 98.5% 100.0% Graduated from H.S. 95.2 93.0 -- 97.9 Attended Vocational or Technical 15.0 29.9 -- 14.9 School Attended College: 86.2 72.1 75% 79.8 Graduated from College: 52.7 41.8 -- 54.3 Attended Graduate or Graduate-Pro- 14.7 12.0 17 22.3 fessional School: Sex of Respondents, by Percentage: Male: 99.0 99.0 -- 98.8 Female: 01.0 01.0 -- 01.2 Tum 107 at the very least, that each of the samples reported has been drawn from the same pOpulation, a. fact which tends to increase confidence in the representativeness of the present sample. This confidence is balanced, however, by the rela- tive non-representativeness of the sample which becomes evident when certain known system characteristics are con- sidered. An analysis of Table 4.8, for example, reveals that the respondents in the present sample tend dispropor- tionately to represent stations affiliated with the National Association of Broadcasters, particularly radio stations, and stations which are identified as Radio and/or Television Code subscribers. In addition, respondents associated speci- fically with radio stations tend also, in proportions not representative of the pOpulation in general, to be identified with stations which maintain affiliation with one or the other of the various national networks. Small radio stations and large television stations seem also to be somewhat over- represented in the sample. Study Procedures, Problems and Limitations In the development of the present study some thought was given to methodological considerations. Special emphasis was given to the problems and limitations associated with the use of the self-administered mail questionnaire. 108 Table 4.8.--A comparison of certain general system characteristics. POpulation Propositioni Sample Proportion TV Stations AM Radio TV Stations2 System AM Radio Characteristics (N=587) (N=4116) (N=123) (N=124) Station Affiliation NAB Membership3 88.6% 53.5% 95.1% 79.8% State Association 5 84.8 75.1 93.5 87.9 Radio and/or TV Code 67.5 43.9 90.2 69.4 National Network6 -- 33.6 96.8 51.6 Station Size7 1 - 10 Employees 5.9% 49.0% 0.1% 56.5% 11 — 25 Employees 17.0 42.9 0.5 30.7 26 - 49 Employees 31.6 5.5 17.1 10.5 50 - 74 Employees 19.6 1.5 19.5 0.2 75 - 99 Employees 11.3 0.6 26.0 0.1 100 ‘ Empl°yees °r 14.9 0.5 31.7 0.0 More lFCC Report, May, 1967. 2Radio-TV Station combinations added to arrive at percent— age listing. 3NAB Report, August, 1967, (TV Stations, N=520; AM Radio Stations, N=2201). 4NAB Report, January, 1966, (TV Stations, N=498; AM Radio Stations, N=3089). 5NAB Report, August, 1967, (TV Stations, N=396; AM-FM Radio Stations, N=2521). (Note: NAB does not maintain separate membership categories for AM and FM Radio Stations in this regard. Percentage for radio based on combined AM FM Station population of 5746, as per FCC Report, dated May, 1967.) 6Broadcastinngearbook, 1967. (Note: Figures listedwk>not reflect station affiliation with any of the 53 regional networks identified in the 1967 Yearbook but only station affiliation with one or the other of the recognized national networks.) 7Adapted from data published in Broadcasting Yearbook, 1967, p. A-l70. (Note: Study questionnaire employed category of "less than 12 employees," in the first instance and "12 - 24" in the second instance. With this exception the data published in the 1967 Yearbook and that derived from questionnaire data gen- erally match. 109 Although a great deal of care was taken initially to draw a random sample from the defined population, it was evident that randomness as a process gave very little assur- ance of representativeness so far as the study returns were concerned. The matter of subject participation was, for the most part, beyond the control of the researcher. Regardless of the effort made to increase participation, by means of the request procedure and other devices, the conclusion was reached that there exists in the field a "return-no-return" response set which exerts an influence on subject response patterns, one which is difficult to identify and even more difficult to evaluate. An additional problem Observed in the use of the mail questionnaire concerns the field modification of the primary sample. The results of this process are presented in Table 4.9. The analysis here reveals that sample modification in the field resulted principally from two causes, the factor of personnel turnover and the factor of management style. When one considers that the present study was initi- ated within weeks of the January, 1967 publication of the Broadcasting Yearbook--the primary reference guide in the field and the source employed in develOping the present sample--it may seem surprising that a modification such as that could have occurred. However, a study prepared by the National Association Of Broadcasters suggests that the aver- age turnover for radio station personnel in 1964 was 33.8 110 wo.mH mv mm.HN ow mH.m mo mHm>O9 .mGOHuocsm ucmHOMMHO mcHEHomumm udn HO>OH msumum mEmm co m.o mo m.o Ho m.o Ho mcomEom .Ommmoupom GOmHmm can» Hmnuo mmmum co conumm EOHM Um>HmomH mHmmH mmmchsm .mOOHuocsm HOHHEHm mcHEHomumm can name O.H NO H.N NO N.O HO uuummmn 888m 9H OmumooH mac Iofiom .mumcHouonsm HOOnnsm Eoum OO>HOOOH nHmmH mmmchsm .Hmmmcme GOHHMHm N.m NH N.O NH O.O OO >HHmsms .uomH>ummsm pommnsm EOHM ©m>HmomH memH mmmchDm .mucmEOOMHmmH OOHuHmom Scum hHHmsms .Ummmmupom o.m mm «.mH mm O.O mo mmocu can» Hozuo mcomnmm E099 OO>HOOOH mHmmH mmOCHmsm unmoumm Hmnfidz usmoumm HOQEOZ ucmonmm Honssz Hommuzv HmmHuzv HHmanv Hmu09 mHmEmm mHmEmm OHUmm mHmEmm conH>mHm9 .mHmamm numsHum as» No coHNNOHNHoos OHmHNuu.N.O mHnm9 111 percent while the average for personnel in television was 28.5 percent.12 The Observed turnover rate, then, as pre- sented in Table 4.9 is much below the industry rate as a whole. This, of course, is explained in large part by the shorter time span and by the fact that the present study treats management level personnel only. The second factor which accounts for a measure of the field modification is that identified as management style. This factor explains 6.3 percent of the modification in radio and was found to Operate most frequently in the smaller stations, stations in which most "out of system" contacts appear to be maintained by the station manager alone, either as a matter Of policy or Of Operational convenience. Description of the "Non-Respondent" In his discussion of the mail questionnaire, Roy Carter makes the following Observation.13 Unfortunately, most journalism researchers who have used mail questionnaires have dutifully reported the response rate but have failed to indicate whether any data were available concerning the crucial question, how (and how much) did the no-answer folk differ from those who did answer? 12N.A.B., Survey of Emplgyee Turnover in Radio and Television Stations, 1964, YWaShington, D. C.: National Association of Broadcasters, 1965). 13Carter, Op. cit., p. 114. 112 Out of regard for Carter's criticism, this section will be devoted to a brief description of the so-called "non- respondent." It will be concerned, then, not with the nature of the study sample--which has already been described in some detail--but with the character of the non-sample, made up of persons known to be represented in the population at large but who are not adequately represented in the sample studied. The first and most Obvious characteristic to be con- sidered in this regard is that of sex. Whereas some 5 per- cent of the primary sample (the "request sample“) was composed of females, only 1.3 percent (N=3) of the completed returns received were from female respondents. Although 42 percent of the male contacts in the primary sample completed and re- turned study questionnaires, only 11.1 percent of the female contacts in the sample did so. The conclusion, therefore, is Obvious. The non-respondent is more likely to be female than male. A second characteristic of the non-respondent which can be noted is the fact that he or she is more likely to be associated with a non-affiliated radio or television sta- tion, something which is brought out clearly in Table 4.8. Third, it is significant to note that only 4.9 per- cent of the study respondents reported station profitability as "quite poor," a comparatively low figure considering the fact that more than one-third of the radio stations on the 113 air are known to Operate near or at a profit loss. It must be suggested, then, that the non-respondent is more likely to be associated with a marginal station Operation than is the respondent. Fourth, it can be reported that the non—respondent is more likely to be located at some distance from the study center. The greater the distance in fact, the less likely the response. This conclusion may be supported by a close analysis of Table 4.3. Fifth, the fact that completed questionnaires were returned by only 39 percent of the persons located in the primary radio sample while 56 percent of those located in the primary television sample returned questionnaires, leads to the conclusion that the non-respondent also is more likely to be associated with a radio station than with a television station. Sixth, because of the very high percentage Of re- spondents who report a college education, in relation to the pOpulation in general, both in the present sample and in others (Table 4.7), it seems possible to suggest that the non-respondent is likely to possess less education on the average than the respondent. Finally, it may be possible to suggest that the non- respondent is more "locally oriented" than the respondent, has more distrust of out of system contacts and has less of a sense of obligation to his profession, factors which tend, 114 on the average, to correlate highly with the level of a per- son's education. Summary and Conclusion On the basis of the above description of the non- respondent, in addition to the factors discussed in the description of the sample, and on the basis of the limita- tions related to the self-administered mail questionnaire prOper, it is concluded that some caution should be exer- cised in generalizing the results obtained from the present study. Although the sample appears to be reasonably well balanced in most significant respects, it should not be taken to totally reflect the population. This caution seems justified in principle in view of the methodological weakness of the mail questionnaire itself, particularly as a means for studying problems of the type involving certain socio-psychological considera- tions. For matters of this nature it would seem more appro— priate to make use of the structured personal interview. This latter procedure offers the advantage of allowing the researcher to probe certain sensitive subject areas when necessary. In addition, it provides the researcher with the Opportunity to clarify, for the benefit of the respondent, his procedures and intentions. The present chapter provided a detailed description of the national study sample, the procedures used in its 115 selection and some of the problems and limitations associated with the application of these procedures. Particular atten- tion was given to the nature and use of the self-administered mail questionnaire as a primary data source. In the chapters that follow an analysis will be made of the data obtained by these means. CHAPTER V DATA ANALYSIS, PART I It is the purpose of the present chapter to describe the principal methods employed in the analysis of the data obtained from the broadcaster role-orientation study and to present the results obtained from this analysis. The data obtained from the national sample was sub— jected to three levels of analysis: (1) correlation analysis, (2) factor analysis, and (3) analysis of contingency tables. Each level of analysis will be discussed independently in the pages that follow even though these forms of analyses were sequentially ordered and intended for the purpose Of testing the specific theoretic propositions and hypotheses discussed in Chapter II. Correlation Analysis Data derived from the national questionnaire was subjected to correlation analysis to develop an understand- ing of the inter-relationship of the specified study vari- ables, those variables that were assumed to have an effect upon the role-orientations of broadcasters but whose inde- pendence could not otherwise be demonstrated. 116 117 The study questionnaire generated data on a total of thirty-eight useable variables independently of the thirty-eight-item Role-Orientation Measure and the sixteen- item Reference-Group Inventory. These were formed into three classes. The first consisted of a series of back- ground variables, which included such standard considerations as subject age, sex, education, system position, role-func- tion, and experience. The second class consisted of a series of so-called system variables and involved a consideration Of questions on type of station, station size and location, market size and type of station ownership among others. The third class consisted Of a series of behavior variables, factors which might be presumed to be acted upon or be in a dependent relation to subject role-orientation. Included in this class were questions relating to job loyalty, career commitment, system tenure, membership status in broadcast- related organizations or associations, degree of activeness in broadcast-related organizations and associations, and similar factors. The inter-item correlation matrix constructed from the three classes of variables identified is reproduced in Appendix D for reference purposes. The values of r shown in this table are significant at a level Of .05 when r = .14 and at a level of .01 when r = .19, as determined by the size of sample and number Of degrees of freedom.1 1Guilford, Op. cit., pp. 538-39. 118 Table 5.l.--Background and behavior variables: correlations. inter-item 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 Position 1 -— College Education 2 '04 Age 3 -.23 .03 -- Experience 4 -.29 .03 .52 -- System Position Mobility 7 .14 .08 -.41 -.31 -.50 -.45 -- JOb Loyalty 8 -011 .05 .33 .14 .08 .16 _022 -- Career Com- mitment 9 -.04 -.02 .05 -.01 -.13 -.04 .02 .28 -- ASPlrat1°n 10 -.03 -.10 -.23 .16 .24 -.06 .09 -.19 -.20 -- Level Table 5.2.--System Variables: inter-item correlation. l 2 3 4 5 6 7 Station Type 1 -- Station Size 2 .77 -- Market Size 3 .47 .74 -- Network 4 .45 .40 .14 -- NOAOBO 5 _022 -029 -021 -019 -- Ownership 6 .34 .51 .45 .21 -.25 -- Location 7 -.10 -.16 -.15 .03 .07 -.12 -- 119 An analysis of intra-item correlations revealed a number of significant relations. These were clustered principally around a relatively small core of primary vari- ables, chief among which were type of station, system posi- tion, education, age, experience, size of station and size of market. Of special interest was the degree to which these particular variables were themselves interrelated. Station type, for example, correlated positively with station size (r = .77) and with size of market (r = .47), system position with market size (r = .74), age with experience (r = .52), and the presence of absence of a college education with both market size (r = .27) and station size (r = .26). The degree to which the total complex of variables were interrelated considerably simplified the study process and resulted in a reduction of the total number of variables planned for test (1) through the transtrmation of certain related variables, and (2) through the elimination Of cer- tain duplicate variables, i.e., variables that appeared to be measuring essentially the same underlying structure. As noted, certain variables, operationally, were combined for test purposes. The so-called attendance vari- ables (variables 29-32) were in this class. Due to the high order of intercorrelation Observed among these items it was judged best to construct a new variable for the final test. This was done by summing across the four items. Subjects 120 were then coded on the newly constructed variable as "Regu- lar Attenders" (attended 3-4 different meetings annually), "Non-Regular attenders" (attended 1-2 different meetings annually), and as "Non-Attenders" (did not attend any meet- ings within the preceding year).2 The education variable subjected to test was selected on a similar basis. It will be recalled that a total Of five education variables were incorporated in the study question- naire. It was not intended, however, that more than a single education variable would be employed for test purposes. Fol- lowing an analysis of the returns, a judgement was made to utilize a reconstituted form of the college education item. The new variable consisted of three categories, "Non-College Attenders" (subjects lacking a college education in any form), "College Attenders" (subjects with less than four years of college), and "College Graduates" (subjects with four or more years of college indicated.) Certain other variables contained on the study ques- tionnaire were also found to be so highly correlated that their continued use independently appeared Open to question. Market size and station size (r = .74) were two such vari- ables, for example. For test purposes, therefore, it was 2Attendance at National N.A.B. Conventions, Regional N.A.B. Conventions, State Broadcasting Association Confer— ences, and National and/or Regional conferences or conven- tions of "other" broadcasting related organizations or associations. 121 determined that only the market size variable would be em- ployed. This particular variable, then, was theoretically redefined as the coded system size variable and subjected to test accordingly. Tenure in system and tenure in position (r = .55) and tenure in system and mobility (r = -.50) were similarly treated. Tenure in system alone was subjected to eventual test. Job loyalty and career commitment (r = .28), although showing a somewhat weaker relationship, were also paired. Job loyalty is therefore reported and career commitment is not considered further. Subject age and experience (r = .52) were provided similar treatment. The project simplification process was further ex- tended by dropping out certain types of variables. Such variables were not carried to the final testing stage either because they appeared to be relatively non-productive in their existing Operational form or because of other compli- cations. Sex, for example, was dropped from consideration because of the smallness of the sub-sample size. A similar decision was made with regard to the entrepreneurship vari- able.3 Subject aspiration was also drOpped due to compli— cations in coding. In addition, variables relating to subject evaluation of station share of market, level of competition, 3Entrepreneurship was operationally defined in terms of whether or not the subject concerned held an identifiable ownership interest in the station with which he was associated. 122 relative profitability and level of community service were dropped from further consideration because they appeared relatively non-productive, as evidenced by the mean and standard deviation in each instance. Network affiliation was drOpped on the basis of its high correlation with sta- tion type (r = .45) and size of station (r = .40). Questions of employer support of subject membership in professional associations and employer support of subject attendance at association gatherings, while of interest, did not appear to be of sufficient value with regard to the propositions under study to justify further consideration. The result of the simplification process described was that a total of thirteen variables were carried over for final test, out of the more than forty on which data was initially gathered. In other words, these thirteen variables were scheduled for use, either in an independent or a dependent relation to the orientation factors derived from the thirty-eight-item Role-Orientation Measure4 for the purpose of determining questions of influence with ref- erence to the primary study propositions. The results of the correlation analysis described made possible certain observations which, while not directly related to the primary purposes of the study were, nonethe- less, sufficiently suggestive to warrant comment. It should 4To be discussed in detail in the section following. 123 be of interest to the broadcast educator, for example, to discover that although a college education is positively related to factors of station size and market size it is somewhat negatively related to levels of aspiration (r = -.10). Subject aspiration, in fact, appears to be more highly related to technical or vocational education (r = .28) and to "other" forms of education (r = .25). It may not seem surprising, however, to discover that aspiration is negatively related, at a significant level, with high school education (r = -.39) and very positively related to entrepreneurship (r = .40). That network affiliation appears to be a function of station type (r = .45) is not at all surprising and is generally supported by direct observation, as is the rela- tionship between network affiliation and size of system. However, it is somewhat surprising to find that network affiliation is more or less negatively related to other types of system affiliation, such as membership in the NAB (r = -.l9), membership in the state association of broad- casters (r = -.10) and membership in the radio and/or TV code (r = -.l4). Of direct interest to those persons concerned with the development of professionalism in broadcasting is the finding that the variable which correlated highest with the individual's membership in professional (defined as broad— cast-related) organizations or trade-related associations T 124 was employer support of membership and that the relationship was negative (r = -.52). On the other hand, with regard to the degree of activeness of the broadcaster in such organiza- tions or associations, the highest related variable was em- ployer re-imbursement of expenses (r = .46). In other words, the degree of support by employers of the broadcaster's mem- bership in a professional or trade-related association is not a direct consideration with regard to the membership itself, but such support is a determining influence on subject attendance at meetings and subject participation in associa- tion related activities. Factor Analysis The second level of data analysis engaged in was the process of factor analysis. This procedure was employed as a method of operationalizing subject reference-group identi- fication and subject role-orientation, and as a check on a primary aspect of the theoretic rationale, the use‘of pattern-variable concepts in the grouping of items on the Role-Orientation Measure. Two factor problems were run, making use of the Michigan State University Control Data 3600 computer system. The items factored in both cases were subjected to varimax rotation, applying the Kiel-Wrigley Option, which was set at three, with unities inserted in the diagonals. The first problem run consisted of data derived from subject responses 125 to Item 22, Part III of the study questionnaire, which reads as follows: As a broadcaster, to whom do you personally look for some indication as to how your particular job should be performed--that is, persons or groups that tend to set those standards of performance with which you are most in agreement? Fifteen groups were identified on the questionnaire. In each instance subjects were asked to indicate whether the group identified was considered "Very Important," "Somewhat Impor- tant," or "Relatively Unimportant." Subject responses were scored "3," "2," and "1" respectively. Space was also pro— vided for the inclusion of additional groups at the discretion of the subject. The second problem run for factor analysis purposes consisted of data derived from subject responses to Part II of the study questionnaire, the so-called Role—Orientation Measure . Reference—Group Factor Analysis The concept of the reference group was introduced in Chapter I. At that time it was suggested that orienta- tion to role was, in part at least, a function Of the in- dividual's reference-group identification. Item 22, Part II, Of the study questionnaire was included as a means of testing this assumption so far as the role of the broadcaster is concerned. The procedures employed for this purpose were as follows. The fifteen reference-group items were 126 intercorrelated (Appendix E) and subjected to factor analy- sis for the purpose of isolating certain general types of reference groups. A factor score was then computed on each of the identified factors. Subjects were next grouped on the basis of obtained scores on the identified factors. Devised groups of high, medium and low scorers were employed finally as control variables for chi-square test purposes. The procedures described, made it possible to clearly identify four primary reference factors and one in- cidental reference factor. Reference Factor I, consisting of a set of six items, was identified as a Career—Set factor. It-was found that broadcasters who score high on this parti- cular factor tend to assign high, positive, value to media critics, trade publications, other broadcasters, network personnel and to professional groups in consideration of how their jobs should be performed. It may also be noted, as a matter of incidental interest, that the reference groups identified with the Career-Set factor are "out—of—system" or "other-system" type groups, i.e., groups that are located at some distance, physical or psychological, from the broad- caster's own situation but with whom the broadcaster tends to identify nonetheless. Reference Factor II, consisting of a set of three items, was identified as a Client-Set factor. Broadcasters who score high on this factor, it was found, tend to assign high, negative, value to listeners and viewers, advertisers 127 and sponsors and to influential community leaders in con- sideration of how their jobs should be performed. Each of the reference groups identified with this factor, moreover, while not directly "within-system," is in a position none- theless to directly apply sanctions in one form or another against the broadcaster should his actual role performance fail to satisfy their collective expectations. Factor II reference groups, then, tend to be more or less controlling or at least somewhat demanding, which may account, in part, for the negative signing of the items. Table 5.3.—-Reference factor solution. Factor Number Item Number Factor Loadings Communalities I 3 .674 .462 7 .582 .442 10 .644 .486 12 .631 .562 13 .630 .598 15 .436 .532 II 2 -.739 .614 6 -.376 .294 11 -.710 ' .617 III 1 -.631 .542 4 -.722 .587 14 -.429 .459 IV 5 .686 .568 8 .759 .679 9 .656 .614 V 16 .885 .799 128 The groups associated with Reference Factor III are also Of a controlling or demanding type although located "out-of—system" for the most part. These include groups associated with government, the FCC and congress, the NAB leadership and the broadcast unions. Reference Factor III has been identified logically, then as a Rule-Set or Power- Set factor. Broadcasters who score high on this factor' tend to assign high, negative, value to role defining influ- ences received from these sources. Again, the negative signing of the factor loadings on Reference Factor III may well follow from the broadcaster's reaction to the firm, sometimes mandatory, direction given in role definition by the groups identified as well as the authority implied in the relationship. Reference Factor IV was identified as a Primary- Grogp factor in that it consists of items which describe groups more directly related to the broadcaster on a day- to-day basis. In some respects, too, this factor might also be thought of as a situation-related factor. Broad- casters who score high on this factor tend to assign high, positive, value to station colleagues, to station management and/or station ownership and to members of the broadcaster's own family in consideration of how their jobs should be performed. Basically, too, Reference Factor IV appears to represent "within-system" type groups; that is, groups that are locally situated and with whom the broadcaster is closely I‘ll) I‘A 129 associated on a personal basis. In addition, Reference Factor IV can easily be viewed as having social rather than strictly occupational implications. Reference Factor V, in comparison with the other factors identified, is a distinctly minor factor. It con- sists of but a single item, that listed on the study ques- tionnaire as "other groups." The intent of the item was to provide subjects with the opportunity of identifying certain groups not otherwise listed. Few subjects actually elected to employ the item, however (N = 12). Nonetheless, the re- sponses that were Obtained tended to follow a set pattern and for this reason the identification of the factor may likely serve some purpose. It was observed, for example, that of those broadcasters who elected to identify "other" important groups for job definition purposes, most tended to assign high, positive, value to personal beliefs, past experience, religious values or to personal insight. It seemed possible, therefore, to at least tentatively identify Reference Factor V as a Value-Set factor and to assign to it the sum of those unique influences, hard to define but none- theless real, which give substance to character and result in decisions of a distinctly personal nature. The results Obtained through the factoring process resulted in several conclusions, theoretic and procedural. It was first observed that the results tended to support the general view that the individual tends to pattern his 130 reference groups into meaningful reference types, forming, as it were, a personal gestalt which can be employed for identification purposes. Second, the reference types so identified tended to confirm previous research, particularly with regard to the contrast between "within-system" and "out- Of-system" identifications, a contrast which has frequently been employed to designate so-called localite and cosmopolite orientations. The results Obtained from the factoring process also made possible a testing of the assumed relationship between the individual's reference-group identificationsaufl.his role- orientation. For this purpose the reference factors identi- fied were treated as control variables and the factors obtained from the Role-Orientation Measure, which will be discussed in the section following, as dependent variables.5 Orientation Measure Factor Analysis The concept of role-orientation has previously been discussed as has the instrument which was designed to Opera- tionalize this concept for study purposes. It will be re- called that the primary purpose Of the orientation measure 5As an independent exercise, non-related to the pri- mary concerns of this study, each of the reference factors identified were subjected to contingency analysis as a de- pendent variable. The independent, or control, variables in each instance were the background, system and test or behavior variables previously identified. The results of this analysis are appended for reference purposes (Appendix Q). 131 was to provide a practical means for testing certain funda- mental propositions regarding the variables that were pre- sumed to act upon (or be themselves acted upon) the individual's role-orientation. The use of factor analysis was intended to further this Objective in two ways: (1) by testing the relative independence of the theoretically applied pattern-variables and the assignment of items to such variables, and (2) by providing a method for more directly testing the relation- ship between the variables described and other theoretically assigned factors. As indicated in the preceding section, chi-square procedures were employed to test relationships between variables. Before proceeding to a discussion of these procedures and the results Obtained from their application, some space will be devoted to a consideration of the factors obtained from the analysis of the thirty-eight-item Role- Orientation Measure. The Role-Orientation Measure was subjected to factor analysis, by means of a varamax rotation method with an applied Kiel-Wrigley Option (set at three) and with unities inserted in the diagonals; this yielded a seven-factor solu- tion which offered a reasonable basis for interpretation, although not totally within the frame of the theory employed. A complete listing of item loadings and communalities for this solution is appended (Appendix J). Table 5.4, which follows, Table 5.4.--Role-orientation factor solution. 132 Factor Number Item Number Factor Loadings Communalities I 2 -.581 .393 8 .433 .344 10 -.596 .370 15 -.599 .506 19 -.523 .568 23 .357 .389 27 -.443 .316 II 7 .422 .385 17 .493 .286 22 .395 .338 26 .459 .552 28 .612 .418 31 .674 .486 33 .507 .469 38 .591 .597 III 1 -.542 .390 4 -.441 .324 16 -.677 .488 21 -.301 .249 IV 11 .399 .339 18 .434 .403 20 .505 .304 24 .344 .294 29 .507 .441 30 .538 .383 34 -.478 .400 37 .422 .416 V 6 -.416 .460 9 .611 .470 12 .394 .418 14 .403 .380 25 .519 .387 VI 5 .397 .176 13 .456 .441 32 .557 .349 35 .497 .449 VII 3 -.401 .286 36 .596 .418 133 lists the obtained principal factors resulting from the assignment of items in terms of their highest loadings. On investigation it was found that the factor solution arrived at however was at some variance with the theoretically defined pattern-variables. The exceptions to this rule that were observed related only to Factor II, which in the main tended to correspond to the basic nature of the affective-neutrality vs affectivity variable, and Factor IV, which tended to reflect a collectivity-orientation. The finding of non-correspondence reduced the direct utility value of the pattern-variable concept as applied to the present study. This is not to suggest, however, that the theory itself had no relevance. Quite the contrary. The concept of the pattern-variable was found to have great merit in the interpretation of the revealed factors. The expectation of devising broadcaster profiles based directly upon pattern-variable classifications, however, had to be discarded. In addition, several hypotheses which were re- lated to the anticipated develOpment of such a classifica- tion system also were discarded. For test purposes, then, the derived factors were assigned as the primary dependent variables, replacing the designated pattern-variables in this important regard. Operationally speaking, therefore, each obtained factor was taken as a separate and distinct role-orientation and chi- square tests were constructed accordingly. 134 The seven factors obtained from the analysis of the thirty-eight-item Role-Orientation Measure were tentatively identified by means of the following procedure. The item mean was taken as an indication of which side of the "forced- choice" item was generally "preferred." If the item asso- ciated with a particular factor was signed positive, the preferred side of the item was taken as a direct indicator of factor content. If the item associated with a factor was signed negative, the preferred side of the forced-choice was taken to be rejected for purposes of factor interpreta- tion. (This was not, however, taken to imply that the non- preferred side of the item in such an instance was accepted.) The results of this process are described in the pages that follow. Orientation Factor I was assumed to be related to the resolution of certain types of value choices which the broadcaster might be required to make in the determination of his role. The types of choices made were found to be closely related to Parsons' universalism vs particularism dichotomy but were not limited to such considerations. For purposes of interpretation this factor was tentatively identified with the value of utilitarianism, based upon the types and qualities of decisions made by the broadcasters sampled in response to the items loaded on the factor. By definition, the utilitarian person is one who tends to favor "a system that holds usefulness to be the end and criterion of action." 135 The broadcaster who scored high on Orientation Factor I might be said to assign high, more or less negative, value to certain normative standards of behavior and to assign posi- tive value to others. In the main, however, the orientation is negative. In addition, the choices of action observed ap- pear to be more pragmatic than idealistic, more to the case than to the rule. A broadcaster so oriented tends for example, not to assign to any one time period or program a degree of importance either lesser or greater than that ‘assigned to any other time period or program. He also tends to reject the view that all segments of his audience are equally important and therefore "deserving Of equal atten- tion and consideration." Such a broadcaster affirms or at least fails to specifically reject, the principle of profit- ability as central in station operations and to define his own role as broadcaster primarily in light of the concept of the marketplace. Although motivated seemingly by profit considerations, a Factor I oriented broadcaster nonetheless tends to measure management performance on the basis of a station's relative standing in the community and other "qualitative" considerations, believing, perhaps, that the station that stands well in the public eye is likely also to be the most profitable. Such a broadcaster also tends to judge programs not on the basis Of results alone, as might be expected, but in terms of considerations of merit, value or quality. In brief, then, a Factor I oriented 136 broadcaster tends not to make a primary distinction between matters of service and profit. The two goals appear, on face, to complement each other in the orientation factor identified. The utilitarian view of the matter, then, might be said to hold that the one is a necessary pre—condition to the other, which is to say, that a balanced perspective is preferred to a singleness of purpose. To this extent at least, the Factor I oriented broadcaster can be said to be goal-centered. Orientation Factor II was viewed as a people-centered factor. The primary focus of this factor was found to be on the type, form and quality of the broadcaster's interpersonal relations, particularly relations with staff. The types of alternative behaviors represented by Orientation Factor II appear to be quite similar, in fact, to those described by Parsons' affective-neutrality vs affectivity pattern variable. Clearly the main concern is whether or not the broadcaster is expected to maintain close, friendly and personal relations with his associates or to develOp and maintain a sense of personal reserve or detachment. Orientation Factor II broadcasters might be said to assign high, positive, value to open expressions of affecti- vity and to inter-personal relations that are warm, close and relatively informal. The image Of the broadcaster, as defined by this factor, is that of a father figure, one who takes personal pride and satisfaction in his staff and who relates 137 to his fellow workers as a professional colleague. The image rejected is that of a rather reserved, formal, and impersonal administrator, an individual who is task cen— tered and who evidences concern for his staff only in terms of "getting the job done." In brief, then, the broadcaster who scores high on Orientation Factor II appears to define the broadcaster role affectively rather than neutrally, a quality more frequently identified with a professional point of view in the literature on this subject. Orientation Factor III, treats generally of that pattern variable which is principally concerned with "the sc0pe of significance of the object," asking whether the actor should respond to the several aspects of the object, including its various unique qualities, or only to a clearly specified and limited range of aspects. The quality or value of objectivism appears, in this regard, to be the chief element associated with Orientation Factor III. Broad— casters who score high on this factor might, then, be said to assign high, negative, value to the diffuseness of func- tion concept and to certain other qualities of subjectivism in the definition of role. Characteristically broadcasters of this type tend not to look much beyond matters of job description or assignment or the specifics of the contract. Neither do they appear to focus on the character of the person with whom they do business or to stress any of the qualitative aspects of the relationship implied. The 138 expression "what a person is, as a person, is often more important to a station than what he does or seems capable Of doing on the job," is one which is quite generally re- jected by broadcasters so oriented. Factor III oriented broadcasters tend also to reject the element Of personalism in staff relations, to avoid considerations of the personal life and character of the individual (except as related to job performance) and to stress strict measures of technical proficiency and more narrow, job-centered, assessments of professional performance. This factor has therefore been associated with the value of instrumentalism and is said to be job-centered in terms of its role-orientation focus. The question of whether or not the broadcaster should give primacy to the needs and interests of the gen- eral community or to the short term goals and special in- terests Of the self and of the station is the principal focus of Orientation Factor IV. This factor was, then, defined as service-centered. Tentatively, too, it was identified with the special value of altruism, the "self- less devotion to the welfare of others." Broadcasters who score high on Orientation Factor IV tend to assign high, positive value to a sense of public service responsibility. In most essential respects, the factor may be characterized by such socially approved terms as fairness, public interest, community service, and social responsibility. The emphasis is on the personal commitment 139 of the broadcaster to the ideal of serving the needs of the community first and the needs of the advertiser second. The concept of "fair employment," which tends to be treated negatively (a judgment based upon the mean sample response), is a curious exception to the otherwise clearly identified 6 sense of the factor. Orientation Factor V, while not limited to consid- erations of the order of performance vs quality, is nonethe- less closely related to the pattern-variable so identified. The basic dilemma of choice appears to reflect the question of whether the actor should respond to the object in the light of what it is, of and in itself, or to what it does, i.e., to its actions or the consequences of its actions. Tentatively, Orientation Factor V has been identi- fied with concerns of quality, reflecting the broadcaster's primary emphasis upon the nature of the product or the service. Broadcasters who score high on this factor might be said to assign high, generally positive, value to the creative act, either in the production of "exciting and original" commercials or in the development of special programs. Characteristically Factor V oriented broadcasters tend to favor a personal involvement in the programming 6One may speculate that the sense Of responsibility identified is a public sense only and does not carry over into the interna Operations of the station, which might otherwise be perceived as a matter of rivate concern. This explanation, however, does not appear suffiCient in all respects. 140 effort and a strong, personal, attachment or commitment to the station output. In a manner of speaking, broadcasters of this type tend to view the program itself almost as an extension of self, in the manner of the artist. They ap- pear, too, to take great satisfaction in appealing to a select audience and in receiving some degree of critical acclaim for their efforts. Concern for results is of a secondary order for such broadcasters as is an interest in and a regard for the client role in broadcasting. Program decisions are viewed as personal decisions. Client deci- sions, on the other hand, are made without regard to such personal considerations. Orientation Factor V, then, tends to emphasize the quality of the product rather than the nature of the service rendered. For this reason it is classified with the value of aestheticism and is said to be product-centered. Orientation Factor VI appears to be similar in nature to Orientation Factor III in that it too treats of "the sCOpe of significance of the object," with whether obligations owed or owing are of a specific order or more diffuse in character, whether fixed and rule-centered or more flexible and situation centered. Unlike Factor III, however, this particular factor is expressed in positive terms and is concerned with points of view maintained rather than those rejected. 141 Tentatively, Orientation Factor VI has been identi- fied with the tendency toward generality or diffuseness in the definition of the broadcaster's role. Broadcasters who score high on this particular factor might, then, be said to assign high, positive, value to treatment by the case rather than treatment by the rule and to person-centered authority as Opposed to office-centered authority. A Fac— tor VI oriented broadcaster, characteristically tends not to be fixed or rigid in his relations to staff. He might tend, rather, to individualize or even personalize decision making, to assign tasks, make demands and to evaluate per- formance largely on the basis of certain subjective con- siderations, without regard to objective criteria or rules of order. Characteristically, too, the broadcaster who scores high on Factor VI tends to be quite flexible in de- fining his own role vis a vis the general public, performing the role more in the manner of a politician or a public official than as a private person. The broadcaster role, then, viewed from such a perspective tends to be non-struc- tured and generally undefined in terms of specifics and otherwise dependent upon matters of individual interpreta- tion. Such an orientation can be considered relatively self-centered and individualistic in terms of value. The Final factor identified, Orientation Factor VII, appears to reflect a combination of pattern-variables, uni- versalism vs particularism and collectivity-orientation vs 142 self-orientation. Tentatively the factor has been identi- fied with the value of parochialism and is said to be situation-centered. Broadcasters who score high on this factor might be said to assign high value to a certain narrowness of view, to define the concept of community in limited terms (those which reflect the "paricular views, values and interests" of the local service area), and to assign primacy to the interests of the family over those of the company or station. Factor VII oriented broad- casters, then, tend to be classed as localites rather than as cosmopolites insofar as their conception or image of the role is concerned. The results obtained from the factoring Of the thirty-eight-item Role-Orientation Measure tended to mini- mize the basic utility value of the pattern-variable con- cept so far as the present study is concerned. The five pattern-variables identified by Parsons were, however, of direct value in interpreting the factors obtained from the measure. The suggestiveness of the results in this regard, then, appear to justify further investigation of the theory potential in studies of the type developed. While the factor results tended to vary from the theoretically constructed variables, it was nonetheless possible to proceed with the testing Of the major proposi- tions and many of the related hypotheses initially described. For this purpose the identified factors were substituted for the pattern-variables which were defined in theory. Each 143 identified factor, then, for test purposes, was said to represent a specific role-orientation. Table 5.5 presents in summary form the seven orientation factors described. Table 5.5.--Summary identification of orientation factors. Factor Number Identification Role-Orientation Focus I Utilitarianism Goal-Centered II Inter-Personalism People-Centered III Instrumentalism Job-Centered IV Altruism Service-Centered V Aetheticism Product-Centered VI Individualism Self-Centered VII Parochialism Situation-Centered Factor Scores The analytic procedures employed in factoring the reference group items (Part III, Item 22) and the Role- Orientation Measure (Part II, Items 1-38) produced a set of factors which could be said to represent a grouping of subjects around a common pattern of scoring items. In effect, each obtained factor could be taken to represent a particular type of broadcaster, or, more specifically, a particular type of reference identification or a parti- cular role-orientation. The next task was to identify the L / 144 position of each subject relative to the Obtained factors. This was accomplished by means of derived factor scores. Factor scores were obtained for each subject by the following procedure. The pattern of scoring of items on the Obtained factors was first estimated. This was done by assigning weights to each item response; the higher a subject's loading on the factor, the greater the weight assigned. The factor score coefficients, or factor weights, for each item and each obtained factor are appended, both for the sixteen-item Reference Group Index (Appendix M) and the thirty-eight-item Role-Orientation Measure (Appendix N). An item array of weighted responses for obtained factors was then produced by summing the assigned weights across each item separately. The weighted sum for each subject was then produced and converted to a z-score. The resulting array of z-scores, ordered by subject and by factor (Appendix 0 and Appendix P), was taken to be a dis— tribution of factor scores and used as the basis for assign- ing subjects to reference—group types and role—orientation types, or categories, for further test purposes. Taking the array of Obtained factor scores described, a judgement was made to classify subjects into three cate— gories on the basis of individual scores on each identified factor. For this purpose it was decided that a score of z = .35, plus or minus from the mean, would be used as the basis for making an assignment of subjects to categories. 145 If a subject's factor score on any particular factor was above 2 = .35 the subject was assigned to the category of "high scorers" for test purposes. If, on the other hand, a subject's factor score was below that of z = -.35 the sub- ject was assigned to the category of "low scorers." Subjects who scored between these points were assigned to the category of "medium scorers." CHAPTER VI DATA ANALYSIS, PART II The third level of data analysis consisted of an analysis of contingency tables. This procedure was employed for the purpose of testing the principal propositions and hypotheses specified in Chapter II. This was accomplished by determining the differences and the significance of the differences between certain observed patterns of role- orientation among the broadcasters surveyed and certain expected patterns of role-orientation. Thirteen variables were subjected to chi-square test in relation to the seven identified orientation factors. The variables selected for this purpose are classified and listed below. Background Variables System Position Subject Role-Function Level of Education Level of Experience (Age) System Variables Station Type Station Location Station Ownership System Size 146 147 Test Variables Tenure in System Job Loyalty Membership Status Meeting Attendance Activeness Each of the background and system variables and each of the previously identified reference factors were desig- nated as control variables for chi-square test purposes. These variables can also be referred to as independent vari- ables. The so-called spread variables, or dependent variables, in each instance were the seven orientation factors as repre- sented by subject factor scores. The seven orientation factors were also employed as control variables and tested in relation to the dependent test or behavior variables described, including tenure in system, system loyalty and membership status. The results of these various test runs are shown in the following tables. Table 6.1 represents the chi-square results and levels Of significance of the principal back- ground variables when taken in relation to the orientation factors. Table 6.2 represents the results Obtained when similar tests were applied to the four system variables and Table 6.3 shows the results of the reference factor tests. The final table in this series, Table 6.4, depicts the re- sults Obtained when the orientation factors were employed as control variables in relation to the dependent test variables. 148 Testing the Primary Studngropositions An analysis of the various tables reveals certain recognizable patterns of significance or non-significance which were taken as preliminary support or lack of sup- port Of the propositions and hypotheses specified in Chap— ter II. Only those hypotheses which could be clearly tested, however, are discussed in these regards.1 In ad- dition to the specified prOpositions, therefore, only those hypotheses were tested which related to the elements of collectivity and affectivity. To Operationalize the pattern variables identified it was necessary to employ only two Of the seven orienta- tion factors described. Orientation Factor II, the inter- personal factor, was employed as the Operational form of affectivity and Orientation Factor IV, the altruistic factor, was employed as the operational form Of collectivity. The hypotheses tested with reference to these factors were those subsumed by the principal study prOpositions as speci- fied below. Pl: That an individual's role-orientation is, in part, a function of his position in the system: H1: Broadcasters with service-related role-functions may be expected to be more collectivity-oriented 1Once again attention is called to the earlier dis- cussion in which it was indicated that because several of the pattern variables constructed for the study did not hold unidimensionality when subjected to factor analysis, several of the hypotheses initially develOped for test were discarded. 149 than broadcasters with economic-related role- functions. H2: Broadcasters with service—related role-functions may be expected to demonstrate less personal and situational detachment than broadcasters with economic-related role—functions. P2: That an individual's role-orientation is, in part, a function of system size: H3: Broadcasters located in large systems may be expected to be more collectivity-oriented than broadcasters located in small systems. H4: Broadcasters located in large systems may be expected to demonstrate more personal and situational detachment than broadcasters lo- cated in small systems. P3: That an individual's role-orientation is, in part, a function Of his reference-group identification: H5: Broadcasters with primary "out-of-system" ref- erence identifications may be expected to be more collectivity—oriented than broadcasters with primary "within-system" reference identi- fications. H6: Broadcasters with primary "out-of-system" ref- erence identifications may be expected to demonstrate more personal and situational de- tachment than broadcasters with primary "within- systems" reference identifications. System Position Results The hypotheses subsumed by the first study proposi- tion (Pl) were tested, as indicated, in terms of types of role-functions. In these regards program directors and news directors were said to have implied service-related role-functions and station managers and sales managers were said to have implied economic-related role-functions. 150 Ho.v H m««% No.v H m%« NO.v n ON HNO NO.N NOO.OH OO.O NNNHO.OH Nm.N NNmH.NH Nm.m Hm>mH mocmHummxm HOV mN.v mm.o mm.m mm.m Namo.MH Hm.H %««¢m.¢H Hm>mq GOHUMODUM Avv mm.m mm.v mm.m mn.n mm.m mm.H MH.m GOHHOnzmImHom Hwy mm.H «om.vH «H.m mv.m «Nom.mH oo.NH ov.m Gofluflmom Emumhm MU ollmo mllmo mIIhO wilmo Mllmo NIImO Hllho mmHQMHHm> .0 .m muouomm coHumucmHHo .muouomm COHumucmHno 0cm mmHQMHHm> ocuoumxomn mcHHMHmH mqummH mumsvm|H£UIl.H.o mHnm9 151 The chi-square results Obtained by relating role- function, as defined, with both Orientation Factor II and Orientation Factor IV, applying four degrees of freedom, did not permit a rejection of the null hypothesis. In addition no significant relations were Observed between role-function and the five remaining orientation factors presented in Table 6.1 for which hypotheses were not speci- fically formed. It was concluded, then, that role-function, as defined, does not materially affect role-orientation. The one exception to the rule stated was demon- strated in regard to Orientation Factor III, the so-called instrumentalism factor, when the element of role-function was "purified." By eliminating the category of "combined role-function"2 and conducting a chi-square test in terms only Of "pure" service-related and economic-related role— functions, the following results were Obtained. Orientation Factor III Lo Med Hi Totals Econ. 33 62 43 138 2 Role- (x Function Serv. 38 28 30 96 (P II II A O o N V Totals 71 90 73 234 2The category in question (N=13) included sub- jects who combined in a single role the service and economic functions defined. A station manager who also acted as a program director would fit this classification as would a news director who also acted as a sales manager. 152 When two degrees of freedom were applied this dis- tribution proved highly significant, a result which lends partial support to the assumption underlying proposition ' P1. In no other case was significance attained by the "purification" process described. While the hypotheses subsumed by the first study proposition (Pl) were not supported by the results, the general proposition that subject position in the system would tend to influence role-orientation was not rejected. Significant results were Obtained when the total range of subject positions were taken in relation to the tested orientation factors without regard to function. Specifi- cally, subject position in system was found to be signifi- cantly related to Orientation Factor III (p = <.02) and Orientation Factor VI (p = <.05). In effect, then, the theoretic rationale developed for the study tended not to be totally refuted so far as the influence of subject position in system is concerned although it was quite gen— erally rejected with regard to the specific combination of subject positions identified with service-related and economic-related role-functions. Further evidence in support of proposition P1 was also obtained through a partial analysis of the chi-square results when taken in relation to the positions of sales managers and program directors alone.3 When the occupants 3By definition the two positions identified differ in matter of role-function. , 1i, 1, I... . mm: 153 of these positions were singled out and compared directly, significant differences were Observed in Orientation Factor II, the interpersonal factor, and Orientation Factor V, the so-called aesthetic factor. These results are reproduced below. Orientation Factor II Lo Med Hi Totals 815. 11 I 16 I 22 | 49 . 2 Subject (x = 8.67) Position Pgm. 22 l 21 I 10 ' 53 (p = <.02) Totals 33 37 32 102 Orientation Factor V LO Med Hi Totals 815. 21 14 14 I 49 . 2 Subject (x = 6.70) Position Pgm. ll 26 16 l 53 (p = <.05) Totals 32 40 30 102 The obtained results appear to indicate a tendency on the part of sales managers to be more affectivity-oriented in inter—personal relations than program directors and less aesthetically-oriented to the broadcast product or service. In terms of role-orientation focus, the sales manager ap- pears more people-centered and less product-centered than the program director. m m 154 System Size Results The second proposition which was investigated (P2) concerned the relationship of system size to subject role- orientation. In this regard market size was taken as an index of system size. The hypotheses subsumed by the second proposition (P2) were expressed in terms similar to those employed in reference to prOposition P1, as indicated above. The fac- tors tested were Orientation Factor II, the inter-personal factor, and Orientation Factor IV, the altruistic factor. The chi-square results of these tests are reproduced below. Orientation Factor II Lo Med Hi Totals Small 36 22 16 74 2 . . (x = 10.19) System Size Medium 25 31 30 86 (p = <.05) Large 24 31 32 87 Totals 85 84 78 247 Contrary to expectation, broadcasters located in large systems appear to be somewhat more affectivity-oriented than broadcasters located in small systems. The hypothesis (H4) stated that broadcasters located in large systems "may be expected to demonstrate more personal and situational detachment . . . ." This hypothesis was not, then, sup- ported, although the null hypotheses was in this instance rejected. mm Ho.v n m««« 155 No.v u m*« mo.v H m* Ase On.N HN.m mm.N NNNOO.ON «NNN.NH NNH.OH Nn.N mNHm Smum>m Ase NO.N OO.N mm.m NNNHN.NN HH.N NO.O ON.N ms>9 mHsmsmszo HOV NH.NH NNNHN.ON m0.0H ns.N N9.HH mO.NH ON.N mmsN HNOHsmmsmomw HOV ON.O OO.H mm.s NNNON.HN ns.m NNNNN.OH mN.n soHumum mm>9 No nnumo O-umo m-umo Ouumo muumo Nuumo Huumo mmHanNm> mamum>m muouomm coHumucano .muouomm cOHumucmHuo cam mmHanHm> Emumwm mCHumHmH mpHsmmH mumswmlHSOIu.m.m OHQO9 156 Orientation Factor IV LO Med Hi Totals Small 38 22 14 74 2 . . (x = 20.69) System Size Medium 27 28 21 86 (p = <.01) Large 18 28 41 87 Totals 83 78 86 247 The results of the Orientation Factor IV test proved to be significant and in the direction indicated by the stated hypothesis (H3). The hypothesis stated that broadcasters in large systems could be expected to be "more collectivity-oriented than broadcasters located in small systems," and this was clearly demonstrated by the results. Therefore the null hypothesis was rejected and the hypothesis as expressed supported. The proposition (P2) stating that role-orientation is, in part, a function of the size of the system was sup- ported even though, in the one instance, the results were contrary to those expected. This prOposition is further supported with reference to Orientation Factor III, the in- strumentalism factor, as indicated on the following page. 157 Orientation Factor III LO Med Hi Totals Small 18 25 31 74 2 . . (x = 12.22) System Size Medium 22 34 30 85 (p = <.02) Large 34 37 16 87 Totals 74 96 77 247 An examination of the corner cells in the chi-square table reproduced above is particularly interesting. The results suggest that broadcasters located in small markets are more instrumentally oriented than are broadcasters located in large markets. That small market broadcasters should more significantly reject the diffusion of function concept is a highly interesting and suggestive finding, one which could not altogether have been anticipated in advance on the basis of theory. If small market broadcasters, as indicated, appear more job-centered in terms of Orientation Factor III scores, they are nonetheless less goal—centered in terms of their scores on Orientation Factor I. The chi-square re- sults when applied to Orientation Factor I were not signif- icant, as revealed in Table 6.2. However, when, as shown below, only the largest and smallest markets were contrasted, broadcasters in the larger markets were found to differ significantly in this respect from their small market count— erparts. These results appear even more impressive when only the corner cells are compared. rum. 158 Orientation Factor I LO Med Hi Totals Small 32 I 22I 20 74 (x = 8.57) (p = <.02) System Size Large I19 I 33 I 35 87 Totals 51 55 55 161 Applying two degrees of freedom to the above dis- tribution, the results again demonstrated support of the general prOposition regarding the influence of system size on role—orientation. In this instance the evidence appears to point to a more develOped pragmatic sense in the role- orientation of broadcasters located in large markets as compared to those located in small markets. Reference-Group Results The third study proposition (P3) specified that role-orientation is, in part, a function of reference- group identification. The primary test results, as shown in Table 5.6, are not supportive of the proposition, how— ever. The influence of reference-group identification, as Operationalized, was clearly evident in only four of the thirty-five tests run, and in only one instance was a level of significance Obtained with regard to either Orientation Factor II or Orientation Factor IV. In this instance Reference Factor II, which was identified as a client-centered factor, negatively signed, was found to 159 Ho.v m«*% No.v QNN mOJ 9.. Hwy vm.m on.m mm.v mm.m mo.~ Ho.N mw.m I mm Avv *«Nmm.mm vm.m Hm.N mm.m mm.n mo.N Hm.H I mm Avv Hm.H mN.N mm.m mm.N Hm.H mm.H mm.m I mm Avv «Nmo.MH nm.m nN.H no.0 «*4om.mH *m¢.HH mm.¢ I mm Avv mm.m mm.m an.o wm.m mH.N mo.m mm.N I mm MU nIImO mIIhO mIImO ellho mIImO NIImO HIImO mHOuomm mocmummmm .mHOHUMH COH#M#GOHHO UGM mHOuhumm GOGGHGMQH mfifluvMHOH mflHDmmH GHMSUmIHEUllomsm GHQMH. 160 relate significantly with affectivity in role-orientation. Broadcasters who scored high on Reference Factor II also scored high on Orientation Factor II. Orientation Factor II LO Med Hi Totals Low 36 30 23 89 2 Reference . (x = 11.48) Factor II Medium 37 31 26 94 (p = <.05) High 12 23 29 64 Totals 85 84 78 247 Broadcasters who scored high on Reference Factor II also tended to score low on Orientation Factor III (p = <.01) and high on Orientation Factor VII (p = <.02). The general influence, comparatively speaking, of this par- ticular reference factor combined with the relative insig- nificance of the career-centered reference factor (RFl) and the primary-group reference factor (RF4) seems to imply that how a broadcaster relates to listeners and viewers, advertisers and sponsors and to community leaders, is apt to be more important than any other single reference factor in determining how he is likely to relate to his general role as broadcaster. In summary, there appears overall to be insufficient evidence in support of proposition P3 and the hypotheses subsumed thereunder to permit a rejection of the null hypo- thesis in these regards. 161 Testing the Secondary Study Propositions In the develOpment of the study a number of secondary propositions were postulated relating role-orientation to such factors as subject age and experience, education, sta— tion type and location, and type of station ownership. Sec— ondary propositions were, in most instances, distinguished from the primary study prOpositions described earlier by their lack of clear determination by theory. Hypotheses subsumed by such propositions were largely arrived at by a priori processes. The secondary propositions investigated and, where applicable, the hypotheses subsumed thereunder are repro— duced below. P4: That an individual's role-orientation is, in part, a function of experience and age. H7: Older and more experienced broadcasters may be expected to be more collectivity-oriented than broadcasters who are younger and comparatively less experienced. H8: Older and more experienced broadcasters may be expected to demonstrate greater personal and situational detachment than broadcasters who are younger and comparatively less experienced. P5: That an individual's role-orientation is, in part, a function of level of education. H9: Broadcasters with higher levels of education may be expected to be more collectivity-oriented than broadcasters with lower levels of education. H10: Broadcasters with higher levels of education may be expected to demonstrate greater personal and situational detachment than broadcasters with lower levels of education. 162 The above two propositions and the hypotheses sub- sumed thereunder were tested in the manner described earlier, utilizing Orientation Factor II and Orientation Factor IV as the operational equivalents of affectivity and collectivity respectively. The results of these tests will be described before proceeding with a discussion of the other secondary propositions indicated. The results presented, however, must be interpreted in light of the following considerations. Initially subject experience and age were postu- lated as distinctly different variables. The degree of correlation demonstrated by analysis, however, (r = .52) argued that experience and age were directly and positively related. Any test of subject experience in relation to role-orientation, therefore, tended also to become a test of subject age, the reverse also being true. For this reason proposition P4 considers experience and age as re— lated variables. Therefore only the results of the test of experience are detailed below. The results of the test of age, however, are included in Table 5.4. A positive and significant correlation was also demonstrated between college education and station size (r = .26) and market size (r = .27). It could be argued, therefore, that any demonstrated influence of education on role-orientation was an influence of system size. It could also be argued that the previously demonstrated 163 influence of system Size was itself the consequence of variance in education. Questions of this order were taken into account in the interpretation of the test results re- lating to the influence of education on role-orientation.4 The results relating to the study consideration of proposition P4, the influence of experience and age on role-orientation, are presented in Table 6.1. These results confirm the relative influence of experience with regard to Orientation Factor II. The influence of experience on role- orientation is, in addition, demonstrated in relation to Orientation Factor VI. The direction of the influence of experience on Orientation Factor II and Orientation Factor IV may be determined from the crossbreaks shown below. Orientation Factor II LO Med Hi Totals Low 23 15 24 62 2 . . (x = 12.15) Experience Medium 38 42 45 125 (p = <.02) High 24 27 9 60 Totals 85 84 78 247 A close analysis of this crossbreak indicates that broadcasters with the highest levels of experience (twenty- One years or more) also score comparatively low on 4A full consideration of this topic will be presented in the pages that follow. 164 affectivity while broadcasters with the lowest levels of experience (ten years or less) score comparatively high on affectivity, as measured by Orientation Factor II. These results, while not altogether as clear as one might hope, tend to support the hypothesis (H8) and necessitate a rejection of the null hypothesis in this regard. Similar support is provided for hypothesis (H7) concerning the ques- tion of collectivity-orientation and experience in broadcasting. Orientation Factor IV Lo Med Hi Totals Low 30 18 14 62 2 . . (x = 14.01) Experience Medium 40 43 42 125 (p = <.01) High l3 17 30 60 Totals 83 78 86 247 An analysis of the crossbreak on experience and collectivity—orientation, as measured by Orientation Factor IV, lends support to the hypothesis (H7) as expressed. Broadcasters with high levels of experience do tend to score higher on the dimension of collectivity than broad- casters with low levels of experience. Study proposition P5, relating to the influence of education on role-orientation, was only partially supported (Table 5.4) on the basis of the results obtained and only in relation to Orientation Factor I, the utilitarian factor (x2 = 14.339; p = <.01), and Orientation Factor III, the 165 instrumentalism factor (x2 = 13.059; p = <.02). The two factors employed to test the hypotheses subsumed by prOpo— sition P5, Orientation Factor II and Orientation Factor IV, did not produce significant results. The null hypothesis, then, was not rejected in either instance.5 However, in consideration of Orientation Factor IV, it appeared reasonable to re-analyze the results Obtained by means of a second test run, applying only two degrees of freedom. For the purposes of this particular test, High Orientation Factor IV LO Med Hi Totals Low 49 31 33 113 2 . . (x = 8.89) Education (p = <.02) High 34 47 53 134 Totals 83 78 86 247 Education was defined as four or more years of college and Low Education as less than four years of college or no college education at all. The first and second categories of education were combined and contrasted with the totals obtained in the third category, reproduced here as High Education. It was reasoned that the non-college graduate was less like the college graduate than like subjects who had never attended college. The results appear to support 5 . . For a more complete analySis on education as a significant variable compare the section on controlled variable tests which follows. 166 this assumption. Broadcasters with High Education, as de- fined, scored higher on the dimension Of collectivity than broadcasters with Low Education, as re-defined. It may also be reported that although broadcasters with High Education scored significantly higher on the di- mension of utilitarianism (Orientation Factor I) than broadcasters with low education they scored low, compara- tively speaking, on instrumentalism, as Operationalized by Orientation Factor III. PrOposition six, concerning the relation of station type (a system variable) took the following form. P6: That an individual's role-orientation is, in part, a function of system or station type. Initially two hypotheses were subsumed under this proposi- tion. The first hypothesis (H11) stated that broadcasters working in television stations could be expected to be more collectivity-oriented than broadcasters working in radio stations. The second hypothesis (H12) stated that broad- casters working in television stations could be expected to demonstrate greater personal and situational detachment than broadcasters working in radio stations. The two hypo- theses were Operationalized, as before, in terms of Orientation Factor II and Orientation Factor IV, identified in terms of affectivity and collectivity respectively. The chi-square results Obtained in support of each of the above hypotheses are reproduced below. 167 Orientation Factor II LO Med Hi Totals Radio 56 38 30 124 2 16.572) <.01) (x Station Type TV 14 30 24 68 (p Radio/TV 15 16 24 55 Totals 85 84 78 247 The obtained results appear substantially to sup- port the view that broadcasters working for radio stations are less oriented to affectivity, as scored by Orientation Factor II, or are less people-centered than broadcasters working for television stations or for combination radio and television stations. These results fail to confirm the hypothesis as expressed (H12). The null hypothesis is therefore rejected, although the hypothesis is not supported as phrased. Orientation Factor IV Lo Med Hi Totals Radio 56 39 29 124 2 . TV 16 18 34 , 68 (x = 21.263) Station Type . (p = (.01) Radio/TV ll 21 23 55 Totals 83 78 86 247 Applying four degrees of freedom to the above prob— lem the level of significance obtained was sufficient to confirm the hypothesis relative to the dimension of 168 collectivity (H11) and allow a rejection of the null hypo- thesis. Clearly television broadcasters tend to score higher on Orientation Factor IV than radio broadcasters. In failing to reject the null hypothesis in either instance, some confidence is generated in support of study proposition P6, which holds that role—orientation is, in part, a function of system type. There is some doubt, however, that the results can be expressed clearly in this form. Attention is called to the results which were des- cribed earlier in a discussion of one of the three primary study prOpositions, that which postulated a positive rela— tion between system size and role-orientation (P2). It will be recalled that system size was related to levels of affectivity and collectivity as defined by the instruments. The present results are clearly supportive if one considers that a relationship exists between system size and station type. As indicated in the correlation tables appended, station size and station type are related at a level of r = .77 while market size and station type are related at a level of r = .47. These high correlations taken in rela- tion to the distribution of subjects in the chi-square tables shown on size and station type seem to point to a strong need for additional study. At least such study would appear to be needed in advance of stating with con- viction that system size and system type can or should be treated and reported separately. 169 Proposition seven, concerning the relation of sta— tion location (an additional system variable) to role- orientation was expressed in the manner indicated. P7: That an individual's role-orientation is, in part, a function of station location. In this regard station location was considered in terms of four geographical areas, the Middle-West, the East, the South, and the West. How these sections were defined is described in an earlier chapter. It was anticipated that subjects in certain sections of the country would tend to develOp differing role-orientations. No distinct hypotheses were formulated along these lines, however, although it was assumed that subjects located in stations in the South would evidence greater affectivity and proportionately less col— lectivity than subjects located in stations in the North or East. Employing a distribution of subjects in terms of the four geographical areas identified, and applying six degrees of freedom to the obtained chi-square results, it was found that station location had a significant effect upon Orientation Factor VI only. The results obtained from this distribution while significant, nonetheless failed to provide any measure of insight which might clearly be interpreted in support of prOposition P7. The results are simply not that distinct with regard to any particular region of the country. 170 Orientation Factor VI LO Med Hi Totals North 15 14 23 52 Station EaSt 25 2° 30 75 (x2 = 26.308) L°°at1°n South 16 44 14 74 (P = ('01) West 19 13 14 46 Totals 75 91 81 247 An attempt was made, therefore, to devise a second set of chi-square tests that would result in patterns of difference which could be more easily interpreted. For this purpose station location was recast in terms of two categories, North-East and South-West. In these terms, applying two degrees of freedom, relatively clear differ- ences were Obtained in relation to Orientation Factors II, III and VII, in addition to Orientation Factor VI. An examination of the re-constituted chi-square table on Orientation Factor II, the affectivity factor re— veals that contrary to expectation, broadcasters located in stations in the North and East produced higher scores on this factor than did broadcasters located in the South and West. Broadcasters located in the North and East were also found substantially to be less instrumentally-oriented, as Operationalized by Orientation Factor III. Station North-East Location South-West Totals Station North-East Location South-West Totals 171 Orientation Factor II Lo Med Hi Totals 36 I 40 SlI 127 (x2 = 9.33) 49 I 44 27 120 (P = ('01) 85 ‘ 84 78 247 Orientation Factor III Lo Med Hi Totals 48 47 I32 127 (x2 = 8.57) 26 49I 45 120 (P = ('02) 74 96 77 257 Surprisingly enough, too, it was found that broadcasters located in the South and West were less situation-centered, as determined by their relative scores on Orientation Factor VII, then were broadcasters in the North and East. Station North-East Location South-West Totals Orientation Factor VII Lo Med Hi Totals 34 52 41 127 (x2 = 6.97) 51 36 33 120 (P = ('05) 85 88 74 247 By re-constituting the basic chi-square tables re- 1ating to the geographical location of subjects, a number of significant relations became evident. It is possible, then, to conclude that the proposition concerning the rela- tive influence of station location (P7) was partially 172 supported. However, some caution must be exercised in freely interpreting these results in such terms. Although the correlations between station and market size and geo- graphical location Of stations is relatively weak and nega- tive, (r = -.16 and -.15 respectively, U. S. census figures indicate that more than sixty percent of the American urban population is located in those sections of the country identified in the re-constituted chi-square tables as North and East.7 It is possible, then, that the reported signif- icant differences are to a degree related to population density, or indirectly, to size of markets. Proposition eight, which concerned the relation of type of station ownership (also a system variable) to role- orientation has been expressed as follows. P8: That an individual's role-orientation is, in part, a function of type Of system ownership. Data on three types of system ownership were derived from the study questionnaire and subjected to test. The three system types were (1) independent ownership, (2) group ownership, and (3) press ownership, including group owned stations with press identifications. Again, although no specific hypotheses were constructed in relation to proposi- tion P8 it was generally anticipated that a collectivity- orientation would be more closely associated with position 7An urban area is defined as at least one city of 50,000 inhabitants (central city) plus contiguous, closely settled areas (urban fringe). Department of Commerce, Bureau of the Census, 1960. 173 in a group-owned station or a press-related station and that similar results would prevail with regard to the question of personal and situational detachment. An analysis of Table 6.2 shows that a significant relationship between system ownership and role-orientation occurs in one instance only. As anticipated, this result is obtained with reference to Orientation Factor IV, the operational form of collectivity-orientation. These results are shown below. Orientation Factor IV LO Med Hi Totals Independent 53 29 25 107 2 System _ (x = 22.807) Ownership Group Owned 18 29 37 84 (p = (.01) Press-Owned ll 19 23 53 Totals 82 77 85 244 The results Of the chi-square test relating system ownership to the question of personal and situational detachment (Orientation Factor II) gave evidence of no sig- nificance, as did the results of the tests applied to each of the other orientation factors. Only in relation to col- lectivity-oriented broadcasters was the matter of system ownership important.8 8Compare section on controlled variable test results for additional insight on tOpic considered. 174 Role—Behavior Test Results As indicated in an earlier chapter, one of the pri- mary assumptions made relative to the present study was that role-behavior could be expected to vary essentially as a function Of role-orientation. To test this assumption on a tentative basis several data types were gathered on role- behavior. Included was information on system tenure, job loyalty, membership status in broadcast-related organiza— tions, attendance at organization meetings or conventions, and activeness in organization affairs. The five behavioral variables, termed test variables in the preceding sections, were dependently related to the seven orientation factors and subjected to chi4square analysis to determine the level of relative significance of any obtained differences. The chi-square results in these regards are reported in Table 5.7. These results indicate that five of the seven orientation factors reached a level of significance equal to or greater than p = .05 in rela- tion to at least one of the five behaviors named. Although the variables tested were rather loosely defined and the test applied somewhat crude for the pur- poses intended, the results obtained nonetheless are suf- ficient to permit at least a tentative rejection of the null hypothesis. This conclusion, however, is one which should be subjected to further test in view of the fact 175 HO.v n 9444 N0.v H m«« I m0.v H m« Amy Aev Amy Amy A00 m0 0v.H HN.N #Hm4n mm4H 00.H n I 90 vm.H NNHN.MH mH.m 0n40 nm.m 0 I 90 v¢.N 00.m mm.H «Hm.0 atmn.0H m I mo mm.v tttv0.na «yam.n mm.H 00.0H v I no tmm.m nN4N 0m4H mm40 n0.m m I 90 mH4H hm4H v0.H 00.0 H040H N I ho mH.0 nm.H , mm.m 004N M040 H I 90 mmmcm>Huo¢ monvcmuud mswwuw MuHmmOH 095:09 muouomm coHumucwHHo mHsmanamz Emumhm Emummm .mHOH>m£OQ Emummm ocm muouomm cOHumucmHno mcHumHmH manmmH mumsvaHcOII.w.o OHQOB 176 that the relation between role-orientation and role-behavior was not itself the primary focus of the present study. Certain of the relations noted are of special in- terest, however, and are therefore reported below. It was observed, for example, that product—centered broadcasters, those scoring highest on Orientation Factor V, were somewhat less disposed to system loyalty than broadcasters who ap- peared less product-centered. System Loyalty Non- loyal Loyal Totals Low 21 59 80 . 2 Orientation . (x = 6.913) Factor V Medium 29 61 90 (p = <.05) High 34 40 74 Totals 84 160 244 Here system loyalty is defined in terms of whether the broadcaster was considering a move to a different job or anticipated making such a move within the near future. A result of similar interest was produced with reference to the relation of Orientation Factor IV, the altruistic factor, and the membership status of broadcasters. Broadcasters with a.high collectivity orientation were found more likely to maintain membership in broadcast-related organizations or associations than broadcasters with a low collectivity orientation. 177 Membership Status Non- Member Member Totals Low 35 48 83 . . 2 Orientation . (x = 7.990) Factor IV Medium 43 33 76 (p = <.02) High 54 31 85 Totals 132 112 244 In addition, it was found that a collectivity-orienta- tion, as measured by Orientation Factor IV, had a significant effect on the broadcaster's habit of attending or not attend- ing broadcast-related meetings or conventions. Broadcasters who were defined as regular convention goers (those who re- ported attending at least three meetings within the year) were found to score significantly higher on Orientation Factor IV than the non-attending broadcasters. Attendance at Meetings ‘ Non- Non- Regular Regular Attend Totals Low 16 33 34 83 . . 2 . Orientation . (x = 17.835) Factor IV Medium 26 37 15 78 (p = (.01) High 33 39 14 86 Totals 75 109 63 247 The results obtained in these regards appear sufficient to support the view that role-orientation does have an effect upon role-behavior. 178 Controlled Variable Testing In the preceding discussion it was pointed out that system size was significantly related to role-orientation, particularly with reference to Orientation Factors II and IV. In this section the results of certain controlled variable tests will be presented for discussion. By a con- trolled variable test is meant a three-dimension chi—square test. This is a procedure whereby a primary variable is held constant in one of its aspects while a secondary vari- able is manipulated in relation to a third, dependent, variable. Controlling System Size By means of controlled variable testing an attempt was made to demonstrate the pervasiveness as well as the relative independence Of the influence of system size on role-orientation. This was to be accomplished by testing certain of the basic background variables, such as educa- tion and eXperience, and certain other system variables, such as station type, geographical location, and so forth, against the orientation factors which were tested previously. This time, however, size of system was controlled. That is, subjects located in small markets were tested independently, as were subjects located in medium sized markets and sub- jects located in large, metro, markets. 179 An analysis of the contingency tables resulting from this series of tests, with system size controlled, revealed that most of the secondary variables tested in relation to the orientation factors selected for this purpose achieved significance with a remarkable degree of consistency. The significance achieved, with few exceptions, was usually ob- served with regard tO subjects located in small markets and subjects located in large, metro markets. The results of the analysis developed by the means described are presented in Table 6.5. It may be recalled that both system size and station type, for example, tended to relate significantly to Orien- tation Factors II and IV. When station type was varied by market size, that is, when market size was controlled, the level of significance achieved by variance in station type, with four degrees of freedom applied, exceeded p = .01 and remained constant, at least in small and large markets. However, when the same procedure was employed with type station controlled and system size varied, radio stations tested against size of system, and television stations tested against size of system, no significant differences were Observed. Contrary to expectation, type of station appeared to have a much more profound influence on role- orientation than the size of the system itself. The re— sults previously described, then, appear not as important as initially thought. 180 Size of system (i.e., market size) is fundamentally important but this importance is reflected in terms of sta- tion type. Attention is drawn however to the relation be- tween station type and station size (Television stations, on the average, are larger organizational types.) and the additional relation between station type and size of market. (Television stations, on the average, are located in larger markets.) The results of the controlled variable test procedure relating system size and station type to role-orientation appears also to imply that radio and television broadcasters differ significantly in orientation to role when located in small systems and when located in large systems but do not differ when located in medium sized systems. Radio broad— casters alone, however, did not differ significantly when located in different sized systems, nor did television broadcasters. It is concluded, therefore, that radio broadcasters in small markets are more similar in role- orientation to radio broadcasters located in medium and large systems than to television broadcasters located in systems of similar size. Additional insights of equal interest were gained from the controlled variable tests. Geographical location, for example, (Table 6.2) was generally a non-significant variable when tested in relation to role-orientation,auzleast as originally constituted. However, when system size was 181 controlled it was discovered that geographical location makes a difference in orientation to role, again in both small and large systems, although the degree of the dif- ference Observed was much more evident in small markets than in large markets. Education resulted in similar differences in role- orientation when considered in terms of system size. The level of education was found to be significantly related to differences in orientation to role in systems of small size and in systems of large size but not in systems of medium size. Experience, like education, also tended to be related significantly to variations in role-orientation in small and large systems. The same held true for system ownership. Whether a station was independently owned, group owned or owned by a press-related company appeared to relate significantly to differences in role-orientation. It was suggested in the introduction to this section that an attempt was made to demonstrate the pervasiveness as well as the relative independence of the influence of system size on role-orientation. In summary it is suggested that while size of system can be shown to have a more or less pervasive influence on subject role-orientation, at least in small systems and large systems, its complete independence could not be demonstrated. There is reason to believe, in fact, that a complex of variables, each to a degree related to the matter of system size, may be at work producing that Table 6.5.--Controlled variable chi-square results with 182 system size controlled. Category Control Spread x2 df Significance Small Market Sta. Type OF-l 53.59 4 .01 OF-2 55.83 .01 OF-3 52.40 .01 OF-4 55.95 .01 Medium Market Sta. Type OF-l 6.87 4 n/s OF-3 7.83 n/s OF-4 4.41 n/s Large Market Sta. Type OF-l 27.01 4 .01 OF-2 29.65 .01 OF-3 27.68 .01 OF-4 25.11 .01 Small Market Geog. Area OF-l 12.83 6 .05 OF-2 16.99 .01 OF-3 14.40 .05 OF-4 20.10 .01 Medium Market Geog. Area OF-l 7.92 6 n/s OF-3 3.99 n/s Large Market Geog. Area OF-l 12.50 6 n/s OF-2 12.12 n/s OF-4 14.21 .05 Small Market Education OF-l 17.03 4 .01 OF-2 22.85 .01 OF—3 18.11 .01 OF-4 25.17 .01 Medium Market Education OF-l 6.30 4 n/s OF-2 2.71 n/s OF-4 1.22 n/s Table 6.5 (Continued) 183 Category Control Spread x2 df Significance Large Market Education OF-l 10.42 4 .05 OF-Z 11.66 .05 OF-3 15.10 .01 OF-4 15.54 .01 Small Market Experience OF-l 13.95 4 .01 OF-2 15.92 .01 OF-3 17.99 .01 OF-4 22.52 .01 Medium Market Experience OF-l 6.36 4 n/s OF-2 1.25 n/s OF-3 7.55 n/s OF-4 4.12 n/s Large Market Experience OF-l 15.59 4 .01 OF-2 10.42 .05 OF-3 15.29 .01 Small Market Ownership OF-l 43.38 4 .01 OF-2 41.59 .01 OF-3 39.06 .01 OF-4 43.80 .01 Medium Market Ownership OF—l 11.21 4 .05 OF-2 3.76 n/s OF-3 4.35 n/s OF-4 5.84 n/s Large Market Ownership OF-l 24.58 4 .01 OF-3 24.13 .01 OF-4 24.63 .01 184 relationship with role-orientation which has been observed. How these different variables themselves interrelate and act upon orientation is not at all clear at this point, however. Controlling System Position As revealed earlier, the influence of system posi- tion on role-orientation is relatively clear. This is suggested at least by observed response patterns to items loaded on Orientation Factors III and VI. The influence of system position, however, was not discernable in terms of role-function as was originally believed except in rather specific instances, such as the Observed differences between sales managers and program directors when considered inde- pendently in relation to Orientation Factors II and V. The question to be considered in this section is the same as that reviewed in the preceding section; that is, to what extent would certain related variables influence role- orientation should position in system be held constant? In response to this expressed concern it was arranged to test three related variables against the four basic position categories of station manager, sales manager, program director and news director. The three variables tested were station type, education and experience. The orientation factors taken as dependent variables for such tests were factors II, III and VI. The procedures employed THE." 185 for these tests were the same as those described in the preceding section and identified as a controlled variable chi-square test. The results obtained are indicated in Table 6.6, next page. An analysis of the results shown in Table 6.6 in- dicates that with system position held constant, education, experience and station type have varying degrees of influ- ence on the individual's role-orientation. The most direct influence was the factor of station type, whether the broad- caster was associated with a radio station or with a tele- vision station. The type stations broadcasters were associated with produced, in most instances, variations in role-orientation, as measured by scores on Orientation Factors II, III, and VI. Radio broadcasters differed from television broadcasters without regard to position along the various dimensions in- dicated with the following exceptions: Radio and television station managers did not differ significantly in terms Of relative scores on Orientation Factor VI which indicates that station managers cannot be differentiated by degrees of individualism. Radio television sales managers also did not significantly differ in terms Of relative scores on Orientation Factor III, the factor of instrumentalism, which may indicate the radio sales managers, on the whole, are neither more or less job-centered than sales managers. Finally, radio and television program directors did not 186 Table 6.6.--Controlled variable chi-square results with system position controlled. Category Control Spread x2 df Significance Station Managers Sta. Type OF-2 13.77 .01 OF-6 9.47 n/s Sales Managers Sta. Type OF-2 13.58 .01 OF-3 5.66 n/s OF-6 14.49 .01 Prog. Directors Sta. Type OF-2 13.55 .01 OF-3 15.12 .01 OF-6 8.44 n/s News Directors Sta. Type OF-2 13.81 .01 OF-3 10.95 .05 OF-6 13.25 .02 Station Managers Education OF-2 11.34 .05 OF-3 9.07 n/s OF-6 5.81 n/s Sales Managers Education OF-2 16.55 .01 OF-6 16.25 .01 Prog. Directors Education OF-2 8.60 n/s OF-3 6.08 n/s OF-6 4.92 n/s News Directors Education OF-2 7.50 n/s OF-3 6.23 n/s OF-6 7.23 n/s Station Managers Experience OF-2 14.87 .01 OF-3 22.08 .01 OF-6 17.48 .01 Sales Managers Experience OF-Z 18.10 .01 OF-3 12.73 .02 OF-6 16.50 .01 Prog. Directors Experience OF-2 19.65 .01 OF-3 6.40 n/s OF-6 5.00 n/s News Directors Experience OF-2 2.72 n/s OF-3 6.63 n/s OF-6 11.11 .05 187 materially differ in terms of relative scores on Orientation Factor VI, the individualism factor. With these few exceptions, it was demonstrated that type of station had a decided influence on role-orientation across all positions in the system with regard to the three orientation factors identified. This finding taken together with those described in the preceding section strongly sug- gests the need for additional research on the relationship between orngaization type and structure and role-orientation. The influence of education on role—orientation, taken in regard to system position, in a controlled variable chi- square test, was demonstrated clearly in association with the sales manager position only. This implies the relative dominance of system position over education in the develop- ment of the broadcaster's orientation to role. Education as an influence on role-orientation cannot be treated so simply, however, in spite of the results presented. It may be recalled, for example, that differences in education were related to differences in role-orientation in both small market and large market stations. Similar differences were also Observed when education was considered in relation to types of stations and to varying levels of experience. The third and final variable related to system posi- tion in a controlled variable chi—square test was that of experience. Here, as shown in Table 6.6, it was found that differences in levels of experience among broadcasters were mm: 188 related to variations in role-orientation, considering Orientation Factors II, III, and VI only. These results, however, were Observed primarily with regard to station managers and sales managers. Experience differences among program directors resulted in significant differences in role-orientation only in the area of affectivity, as opera- tionalized by Orientation Factor II. Similarly, differences in level of experience among news directors resulted in differences in role—orientation only with reference to Orientation Factor VI, the individualism factor. In summary, then, it was found that with subject position held constant, the principal influence on role- orientation among the variables tested was that of station type, which,when varied, resulted in significant differences across each position category studied. Conversely, differ- ences in level of eudcation generally resulted in significant differences in the role-orientation of sales managers only, and experience, when varied, influenced clearly only the orientations of station managers and sales managers except as otherwise indicated. The Influence of Education Level To return to the question raised concerning the relative influence of education, it was found that this particular variable significantly affected role-orientation only under certain conditions. In the initial test Of the 189 education variable, significant results were Obtained in relation to both Orientation Factor I and Orientation Factor III and, when redefined in "high" and "low" categories only, in relation to Orientation Factor IV. These results, more- over, were substantially supported in the controlled vari- able chi-square test when system size was held constant, at least so far as broadcasters located in small systems and large systems were concerned. When subject positions was held constant, however, Orientation Factor III alone, of those factors tested, was found to be significantly related to education and then only in relation to the position of the sales manager. Education was also varied in terms Of station type and in terms Of eXperience to provide greater understand- ing of the relative influence of this particular variable on role-orientation. The results of the controlled variable chi-square tests of education are shown in Table 6.7. Attention is focused first on the relationship of education to role-orientation with type of station con- trolled. Here the influence was clearly evident in relation to Orientation Factor IV, the service-centered factor. Dif— ferent levels of education resulted in significant differ- ences in subject scores on Orientation Factor IV in radio stations, television stations and in combination radio and television stations. The influence was general regardless of station type so far as this particular factor was THE.” 190 Table 6.7.--Chi-square results with station type and experi- ence controlled and education varied. Category Control Spread x2 df Significance Station Type Radio Education OF—l 8.35 4 n/s Stations OF-4 12.27 .02 OF-7 7.20 n/s Television Education OF-l 6.07 4 n/s OF-7 10.18 .05 Radio/TV Education OF-l 12.79 4 .02 Stations OF-4 11.71 .02 OF-7 8.02 n/s Experience 10 yrs. or less Education OF-l 13.11 4 .02 exp. OF-2 13.89 .01 0F—4 14.98 .01 OF-6 14.54 .01 11—20 yrs. eXp. Education OF-l 8.97 4 n/s OF-2 7.79 n/s OF-4 6.82 n/s OF-6 7.67 n/s 21 yrs. or more Education OF-l 12.66 4 .02 exp. OF-2 15.54 .01 OF-4 13.13 .02 OF-6 7.99 n/s 'l-IED‘I 191 concerned. Education level otherwise resulted in difference in subject scores on Orientation Factor I only in combination radio and television stations and on Orientation Factor VII in television stations. The influence of education level on role-orientation with experience in broadcasting controlled was somewhat less evident, being limited to broadcasters with the least experi- ence and the most experience primarily. NO significant differences appeared to result when education level was considered among broadcasters having medium levels of ex- perience, that is, more than ten but less than twenty years experience. Inasmuch as several of the orientation factors tested against the education variable with experience con- trolled were also tested against this variable with system size, subject position and type station controlled, a com- parative analysis of these results can be shown. Considering Orientation Factor IV independently, it was found that education generally influenced subject scores on this factor under the following conditions; In small and large systems (but not in medium sized systems), in all types of stations, and among the least and most experienced broad- casters (but not among those broadcasters with medium levels of experience). Considering Orientation Factor I, it was found that education tended to influence subject scores on this factor 192 in both small and large systems and among the least and most experienced broadcasters but not in either radio or televi— sion stations considered independently. Level Of education tended to influence subject scores on Orientation Factor II also, again in both small and large systems, among station managers and sales managers (but not program directors or news directors), and among the most experienced and the least experienced broadcasters. Level of education tended, in addition, to influence subject scores on Orientation Factor III, but to a much less extent, being evident in both small and large systems and otherwise limited to sales managers. One result of the controlled variable chi-square test which is of special interest concerns the question of role-function. It will be recalled that when role-function, defined in terms of economic and service-related positions, was tested independently no significant differences in role- orientation scores were observed. However, when role- function was tested against Orientation Factors I, III, and IV--the factors most generally influenced by education-- with level of education controlled, a number of significant differences became apparent. These results are shown in Table 6.8. When groups of broadcasters were taken with regard to similarity in levels of education, role-function dif— ferences resulted in distinct differences in role-orientation. 193 Table 6.8.--Controlled variable chi-square results with education controlled and role function varied. Category Control Spread x2 df Significance Non-College Function OF-l 13.91 4 .01 Education OF-3 5.09 n/s OF-4 13.04 .02 Less Than 4- ‘ Function OF-l 11.98 4 .02 Years College OF-3 15.78 .01 Education OF-4 10.64 .05 4 or More Years Function OF-l 12.33 4 .02 College OF—3 9.45 n/s Education OF-4 11.59 .05 It is evident, then, given these results, that broadcasters having different role-functions, whether service-related or economic-related, may develop different role-orientations and that these differences may be significant under certain conditions of test. In summary it may be suggested that level of educa- tion tends to influence role-orientation, particularly with regard to Orientation Factors I and IV, and that it does so under certain conditions and in relation to certain other intervening variables, such as size of system, station type, experience and position in system. It can be suggested further than education level relates to the level of influ- ence of role-function on role-orientation. Much additional research, however, will be required before a pattern Of in- fluence can be clearly determined for level Of education in these regards. rum: CHAPTER VII CONCLUSIONS The results described in the preceding chapters provide a basis for developing certain conclusions con- cerning (1) the value of the theoretic rationale employed in the study and (2) the propositions and hypotheses tested. These conclusions will constitute the main part of this final chapter. A consideration of their applicability to future studies of a similar type will also be presented. Theoretic Considerations Two aspects of this study stand out from a theoretic point of view. The first concerns the concept of orienta- tion itself and its placement at the center Of a conceptual model of social action; the second concerns the utility value of the pattern-variable formulation. When Operationalized by meanscfiffactor analysis, the concept of role—orientation was found to have distinct emh pirical value. Seven modes of role-orientation were identi- fied in the study, each one of which was represented by a particular factor. Each factor was suggestive Of a certain point of view, or manner of relating to the situation, main- tained by certain numbers of broadcasters. 194 195 'The study also identified a number of significant influences on the role-orientations of broadcasters, chief among which were those Of position, education, experience, system-size, station-type and station-location. Finally, and of primary importance to theory, the study demonstrated the general tendency of the role- orientations identified to influence the role-behaviors of the broadcaster, at least within the limits described. The second concern relating to theory construction which grew out of the present study involved a considera- tion of the utility of the pattern-variable formulation. A lack of empirical validity for this formulation had to be assumed to some extent as a result of the failure of the orientation items to factor clearly in terms of the five pattern-variables identified. Although the items used may be faulted to some extent, in form or content, there is sufficient evidence to suggest nonetheless that these vari- ables do not fully maintain independence when treated empirically. On the basis of the results obtained in the present investigation it can be suggested that the universalism- particularism pattern-variable may easily be confused with the collectivity—self—orientation variable and that specifi- city-diffuseness and performance-quality are interrelated. There is also reason to believe that these same variables may fuse with the affectivity-affective-neutrality variable, THIS! 196 depending upon the specific content of the items con- cerned. Over time, given a sufficient number of trials, it might be possible to obtain pure factors based upon the pattern-variables described. This would require much greater refinement of procedural detail than was permitted in the present study and a much larger universe of items. However, such factors, if Obtained, probably would tend to be of a highly abstract nature, having somewhat limited value when applied to specific problems of role in the real world. It had been hoped, Of course, that the use of the pattern-variable formulation would make possible the devel- opment of a convenient and reasonably practical procedure for making needed comparisons among roles and role-incum- bents located within given systems of mass communication or those located in different systems. It was hoped, in other words, that progress could be made in the development of a standard instrument for measuring important aspects of role in general. It now appears that further work will be required before this can be done. The results of this study, naturally, need not be taken as conclusive in this important regard. The pattern- variable formulation still is quite suggestive as applied to role theory and may yet prove to be an extremely valuable tool for this type of research. It is possible that all 197 that all that is really needed is a sufficient amount of refinement and additional testing. Empirical Considerations Three primary study propositions and several sec— ondary prOpositions were devised for the study of the broadcasters' role-orientations. Subsumed under each were five hypotheses and alternative hypotheses, each one of which was related to one or the other of the five pattern- variables. Because of the failure of the pattern-variables to factor clearly, the number of the hypotheses was reduced from five to two in each instance, but this had no corres- ponding effect on the study propositions themselves. The principal substitution in the study was the acceptance of the factor results as operational definitions of various role-orientations in place of the theorized pattern-variables. The conclusions developed from the study, then, reflect the specified orientation factors and not the original pattern-variable formulation. This is true even though the pattern-variables themselves were suggestively employed in the identification and interpretation of the designated factors. The primary study propositions investigated were (1) the system-position prOposition, (2) the system-size proposition, and (3) the reference—group identifications proposition. The major conclusions drawn from the Obtained 198 test results are related to these considerations, although, in certain instances, they have been mediated by other fac- tors, particularly education, experience, station type or station location. It must be concluded, first, that system position is an important influence on the broadcaster's role-orienta- tion but not in the ways originally thought. It was found that broadcasters who primarily maintained economic-related role-functions (station managers and sales managers) and those who primarily maintained service-related role-functions (program directors and news directors) could not generally be differentiated in terms of orientation, except in terms of relative instrumentalism. Broadcasters who maintained economic-related role-functions tended, in effect, to be more job-centered than broadcasters who maintained service- related role-functions. When system position was not defined in terms of role-function, however, but simply in terms of location, it was demonstrated to have considerably greater influence on role—orientation. The degrees of differences observed, moreover, were particularly noticeable when sales managers and program directors were compared and when station man- agers and news directors were compared. Sales managers and news directors, in fact, had much more in common than could have been expected. 199 The observed similarity between sales managers and news directors may be related to their locations within the system. Both can be said to occupy "boundary positions," in that each is required to maintain extensive out-of- system contacts. This suggestion grows out of the research on organizational stress conducted by Robert L. Kahn and his associates at the University of Michigan. Kahn states that "persons in boundary positions are more likely than others to reduce the intensity of their relationships with role-senders within their organization and seek other iden- tifications outside the organization. . . ."1 Within the present study this view is supported by evidence that sales managers and news directors, on the average, are more likely to belong to broadcast-related organizations or associations, other than trade-associa- tions, than are station managers or program directors. The tendency of sales managers to orient themselves to the interpersonal aspects of the role and to relate to others affectively rather than in terms of strict affective- neutrailty, the tendency, in other words, of sales managers to be people-centered, may also be interpreted in this same manner. "Lacking formal authority over his outside con- tacts," Kahn writes, "a person at the company boundary 1Robert L. Kahn, D. M. Wolfe, R. P. Quinn, 33. 31., gp, cit., p. 106. 200 must rely heavily on personal sources of power over these role senders; the trust, respect, and friendship of his outside contacts is less a reward than a necessity."2 The system-size proposition and the hypotheses associated with it also produced certain unexpected re- sults. Contrary to expectation broadcasters located in large systems tended to score as more affectively oriented, or peOple-centered, than broadcasters located in small systems. Large system-broadcasters also appeared to be less instrumentally oriented, or job-centered, than small- system broadcasters. There are several explanations which may account for these findings. The first is that broadcasting is a unique Operation, generally unlike the standard industrial- business system most frequently studied by social scien- tists. The production and distribution of messages for mass audiences might be said to differ fundamentally from the simple production of goods and services. The qualities in broadcasting which produce uniqueness in this respect, moreover, may be more in evidence in large systems than in small systems. It is also possible that broadcasters located in large systems are more beset with problems and worries relative to their task-responsibilities and have, therefore, 21bid., p. 109. 201 a greater sense of insecurity than broadcasters located in small systems. For example, if the large-system broad- caster experiences more role-conflict or has a more ambig- uous type of role to perform than the small system broad- caster, it is possible that he will seek mainly to develop ties with "system others" based more on qualities of friend- ship, trust and mutual confidence than on systems of rights and obligations. A third possibility is that broadcasting itself is relatively more unstable, from a system point of view, than some other forms of organizations, that it is more dependent upon the capriciousness of the advertiser, the transience of public taste, the uncertainties of the marketplace and the will of government, all factors which may be more sa- lient to actors in large systems than to actors in small systems. Other factors related to system-size which were clearly in evidence in the present study, and which tended to support the system-size proposition, were (1) that broadcasters located in large systems were more collec- tivity-oriented, or service-centered, than broadcasters located in small systems, and (2) that broadcasters located in large systems were more utilitarian, or goal-centered, than broadcasters located in small systems. 202 What is of special interest to students of the mass media is the relation of these results to certain well known aphorisms in broadcasting. Public policy, for exam- ple, has long been predicated on the belief that the closer the broadcaster is to the community he "serves," the more responsive he is likely to be to the needs and interests of the peOple. It has also been generally assumed that the independent station, particularly if locally owned, is more responsive to the community than stations which are group- owned. Bigness as represented by multiple-station owner- ship or by simple size has been viewed as a frightening thing, to be guarded against or firmly controlled in the "public interest." Virtue has traditionally been thought to be on the side of smallness of size and of independent ownership. If the results of the present study are indicative, it is possible that these assumptions are Open to question. Contrary to pOpular belief, large system broadcasters show- ed evidence of being more collectivity-oriented than small system broadcasters. In addition, broadcasters located in stations that were group owned or in stations owned by newspapers or by press-related organizations tended to evi- dence higher levels of collectivity orientation than broad- casters located in stations that were independently owned. 203 Corresponding to the above considerations of system size are certain conclusions concerning the factor of sta- tion type. A review of the relation of station type to system size was presented in the preceding chapter. What alone needs to be emphasized here is the finding that re- gardless of the size of the system, excluding medium sized stations, television broadcasters exhibited marked differ- ences from radio broadcasters in role-orientation, and that these differences were supportive of the results ob- tained from the study of system size. Television broadcasters were more collectivity- oriented, or service-centered, than radio broadcasters and generally more affectivity-oriented, or peOple-centered. Television broadcasters also appeared to be more goal- centered and less job-centered than radio broadcasters, tending toward greater utilitarianism and less instrumen- talism. Broadcasters occupying different positions within the system also showed different orientations to role de- pending upon their locations. Television station managers, sales managers, program directors and news directors, on the average, scored higher on affectivity than their coun- terparts in radio. With the exception of sales managers, television broadcasters also tended to be less job—centered than radio broadcasters. In addition, television sales managers and television news directors tended to be less 204 individualistic, or self-centered, than radio sales managers and radio news directors. It is interesting to speculate why television broadcasters differ from radio broadcasters in role-orien- tations to the extent indicated. As suggested earlier, television stations for the most part are located in larger markets and are larger in size than radio stations. Tel- evision station personnel are, in addition, frequently more experienced and tend to have higher levels of education. The simple combination of these factors, therefore, might have been sufficient to produce the level of differences noted between the role-orientations of television and those of radio broadcasters. Although the interaction of these variables may account for station type influence on role-orientation, a great deal of additional research will be required before anything along these lines can be firmly established. At present it can only be suggested that type of station has a significant and frequently determining influence on role- orientation, often beyond that assigned to system size independently. The third study prOposition, identified as the reference-group identification proposition, was generally not supported by the data. Broadcasters who related to one or another of several reference-group types did not exhibit, as a consequence of their relationship, the 205 predicted role-orientations. This is not to suggest, how- ever, that the concept of the reference-group lacks meaning when applied to the study of role in systems of mass com- munication. The patterns of identification obtained by factoring the various influence-groups scaled in Part III of the study questionnaire are too clearly in support of earlier research to allow so summary a judgment to be made at this time. The view that broadcasters perceive various groups as meaningful reference types was credibly demonstrated. The reference types defined, moreover, are consistent with findings of previous research concerning the contrast be- tween "within-system" and "out-of—system" identifications. The results of the present investigation, then, appear quite valid in this important request. What could not be demonstrated was the influence that identifications of this order have on the broadcaster's role-orientation. The contrast between the Career-Set factor, an ob- viously cosmopolite reference-group identification, and the Primary-Group factor, which is more in keeping with the localite concept, is particularly interesting and deserving of further investigation. The significant relationship between level of education and the Career-Set factor is also quite suggestive, as is the relationship between sys- tem tenure and the Primary-Group factor. Of equal interest, too, is the clear relation between station type and exper- ience and the Rule-Set (or Power-Set) factor. Tum 206 The fact that the N.A.B. leadership, a group most commonly viewed as having a professionalizing influence in broadcasting, factored as a Rule-Set item rather than as a Career-Set item is deserving of special note. The latter, logically, is more in keeping with the norms of profession- alism as traditionally defined. On the basis of these results it seems in order, perhaps, to suggest a reassess- ment of the potential of the N.A.B. to "professionalize" broadcasting. A number of other incidental results should be men- tioned in conclusion. Many of these items have greater descriptive value than basic theoretic relevance, but this does not detract from their importance. Of primary in- terest is the influence of education on the role-orienta— tion of the broadcaster. Although the results of the tests on education, taken on the whole, are not altogether clear, it was conclusively demonstrated that education is signif- icantly and positively related to the factor of altruism. If altruism is one of the distinguishing character- istics of professionalism, as is often implied, then the emphasis upon higher education for broadcasters is quite important. A trend in the direction of increased levels of education, over time, might in fact be interpreted as a trend toward increased professionalism in broadcasting. THC. 207 The concept of professionalism, of course, was not directly an element in the development of the broadcaster role-orientation study. However, several of the results obtained from the study relate to this important subject. To an extent, this consideration was inevitable, due in part to the dichotomous nature of the pattern-variable formulation and the tendency of various researchers--Blau and Scott, for example--to associate one side or the other of the "dilemmas of choice" with professional behavior in an organizational setting. The two pattern-variables which could logically be related to the orientation factors identified in the study were affective-neutrality versus affectivity and collec- tivity-orientation versus self-orientation. Both of these variables, moreover, can be tied directly to the general concept of professionalism. It is interesting to observe that while the direc- tion of collectivity-orientation (Orientation Factor IV) was quite in keeping with what could be expected, the di- rection of affectivity (Orientation Factor II) was counter to what could be expected. In effect, the present study supports the view that broadcasting is a mixed system so far as these defined professional values are concerned. Affectivity in orientation to role, rather than affective- neutrality, tends to vary inversely with a collectivity- orientation in broadcasting. What this implies is not 208 clear at this point, but it can be suggested that unless, or until, these two factors are more nearly in agreement, professionalism as a general pattern in broadcasting is likely to remain a moot issue. Recommendations for Further Study The results obtained from the role-orientation study of the American broadcaster are, perhaps, more sug- gestive than conclusive. Although the study offers some new insight into the role-orientation of mass communica- tors, its greatest value may be in the implications it has for further research. Although it appeared to confirm that there are relations between role-orientations and role-behaviors, the study suggests the need (1) to delin- eate further the matter of role-orientation itself through work with additional objects of orientation and the refine- ment of procedures and instruments, and (2) to focus di- rectly upon the central elements of role-behavior per gs, particularly with reference to those behavior patterns that have a clearly defined relationship with system output. It may also be possible in future studies of this type to relate the individual's role-orientations to fac- tors of managerial style. Several patterns of behavior, for example, have, over time, become identified with dif- ferent styles of management. It has also been suggested that each style of management results in certain forms of 209 accommodation to the system on the parts of actors located in the system. The inter-play between managerial style and various forms of accommodation, moreover, can be said to produce determinable consequences in the form of differing levels of productivity, morale, motivation and personnel turnover. An investigation of the extent to which these factors are operable in broadcasting, or in other systems of mass communication, and the degree to which they may be linked to role-orientations might be of great value. There is also some need to define in specific terms the contents and sources of the so-called "sent role," those messages received by occupants of designated system- positions which incorporate role-expectations. Future studies should focus upon this aspect of the problem as well. Another area of possible investigation relates to the concept of the reference-group. Additional study should be directed to this specific concern for the purpose of answering the questions raised by the factor analysis of the designated reference-groups in the present study. There appears also to be a need to define clearly the nature of the feedback links in the social-action model conceptualized. Attention should be directed, for example, to the consequences of action, not only in terms of system- output, but also in terms of certain internal concerns. What impact, for instance, do the behaviors resulting from L... 210 the broadcasters' role-orientations have upon the develop- ing content of the "sent roles?" What, in addition, is the likely effect on the individual broadcaster of forced com- pliance with modes of behavior which are required by sig— nificant "system others" but which may be in conflict with the broadcaster's own role-orientation? Studies should be undertaken to supply the answers to these and other related questions. Such studies, should also be undertaken with a regard for the general aspects of behavioral theory and with a view to constructing systematic approaches to the study of the role of the mass communicator. Attempts should also be made to gather data by means of the focused interview in addition to the use of the mail questionnaire. Advantages and limitations are associated with each approach. For this reason both meth- ods should be employed in concert and efforts should be made to interrelate the results. TH“ APPENDICES 211 APPENDIX A STUDY PARTICIPATION REQUEST FORMS 212 THE! MICHIGAN STATE UNIVERSITY msr LANSING - MICHIGAN 43323 COLLEGE OF COMMUNICATION ARTS - DEPARTMENT OF COMMUNICATION 0 CABLE: COMLIDBPT March 2M, 1967 The Department of Communication at Michigan State University, in conjunc- tion with the Department of Television and Radio, is currently seeking to gather information on the occupational role of the American broadcaster. In view of the recognized importance of the television and radio media in today's changing world, such information is deemed to be of special significance. Because of the great expense involved in gathering information of the type required by normal interview methods, it has been decided to employ a mail ques- tionnaire for the purpose of this study. As you no doubt know from your own experience, however, information gathered by this means is likely to be quite misleading unless some special care is taken to insure the overall representa- tiveness of the sample. For this reason, then, we are endeavoring in advance to secure your personal cooperation in this undertaking. We are hopeful that you will be able to participate in the study with us. The time required for you to complete the questionnaire will be less than thirty minutes. The infor- mation received from you, moreover, will be treated as completely confidential. Please indicate on the enclosed card your willingness to assist us in this project. When received by us we will forward to you the actual study form for completion at your convenience. I repeat, in order to insure the reliability of the information gathered by this means, we need very much to secure your cooperation, since you are a member of the select sample group of some 300 broadcasters chosen especially to participate in this study. Sincerely yours, Gerald C. Beckwith, Research Associate GCB/lj THE.- FIRST CLASS Permit No. 178 East Lansing. Mich. BUSINESS REPLY MAIL HoPoatagaStanplmnl-fladhuutadlmu Postage Will Be Paid By THE AMERICAN BROADCASTER STUDY PROJECT P. O. Box 652 East Lansing, Michiqan 40823 Att: G. C. Beckwith Research Assnciatn AMERICAN BROADCASTER STUDY PROJECT Department of Communication Michigan State University Gentlemen : In reply to your recent request: I (am) (am not) willing and able to assist you in your current study of the occupational role of the American broadcaster. (Signed) Code No (Station) MICHIGAN STATE UNIVERSITY EAST LANSING - MICHIGAN 48323 COLLEGE OP COMMUNICATION ARTS 0 DEPARTMENT OF COMMUNICATION - CABLE: COMB/[DEPT Dear Broadcaster: Recently you received an invitation from us to participate in a sig- nificant new study of the occupational role of the American broadcaster. Since we have not yet heard from you regarding your participation in this study, we are taking this opportunity to once again call your attention to the importance of the undertaking and to your possible role in it. As we pointed out before, your name was selected by means of certain controlled sampling procedures from a listing of all known broadcasters in positions similar to your own, a fact that makes your personal participa— tion highly desirable. In addition, this particular study, for the first time, attempts to provide a complete understanding of the real role of the American broadcaster, a fact that will likely have wide implications for future programs of professional broadcasting education in this country. While you are certainly under no direct obligation to assist us in this project, we think nonetheless that you will want to do so in order to insure the overall completeness and accuracy of the resulting data. As manager, newsman, programmer or salesman, your assistance is very much needed. So may we invite you once again to complete the enclosed reply card, indicating your willingness to act as a professional subject in the American Broadcaster Study Project, and return it to us at once. Thank you. Sincerely yours, Dr. Hideya Kumata, Project Director, Department of Communication AMERICAN BROADCASTER STUDY PROJECT Michigan State University Prof. Leo Martin, P.O. Box 652 Project Advisor, East Lansing, Michigan h8823 Department of Tele— vision and Radio encl- Mr. G. c. Beckwith, Research Associate THE. AMERICAN BROADCASTER STUDY PROJECT Department of Communication Michigan State University Dear Broadcaster: Thank you for your willingness to participate in the study of the American broadcaster now being conducted by this department. I am certain that your assistance will help insure the overall reliability of the information gained from our investigation. The study questionnaire is attached. It is self- explanatory and should present no difficulty. When you have completed the questionnaire, place it in the enclosed envelope and return it at once to the American Broadcaster Study Project, Box 652, East Lansing, Michigan. No postage will be required. If you have any comments or questions concerning the project or the interpretation of the results, please let me know. Once again, thank you for your cooperation. Sincerely yours, Gerald C. Beckwith, Research Associate GCB/ll Enclosure MICHIGAN STATE UNIVERSITY BASI' LANSING . MICHIGAN 48323 COLLEGE OF COMMUNICATION ARTS - DEPARTMENT OF COMMUNICATION 0 CABLE: COMIIDEPT May S. 196? Dear Broadcaster: As a participant in the American Broadcaster Study Project you were recently sent a confidential questionnaire which was to serve as the basis for a preliminary analysis of certain aspects of the broadcaster's occupational role. Inasmuch as we have not yet received your completed questionnaire, it is possible that our mailing to you was either misdirected or delayed. We are therefore enclosing a second copy of the questionnaire for your use if necessary. Please understand that it is quite important that we hear from you soon, if at all possible. In order for us to make full use of the univer- sity data processing facilities, it will be necessary to submit all of our available returns to coding and analysis within the near future. Thank you once again for your consideration and helpful cooperation. Without the aid of many broadcasters such as yourself this study could not have reached its present stage of near completion. Sincerely yours, Gerald C. Beckwith, Research Associate American Broadcaster Study Project P.0. Box 652 East Lansing, Michigan h8823 encl. MICHIGAN STATE UNIVERSITY EAST LANSING - MICHIGAN 48325 COLLEGE OF COMMUNICATION ARTS 0 DEPARTMENT OF COMMUNICATION - CABLE: COMMDEPT May 25, 1967 Dear Broadcaster: This is to let you know that the American Broadcaster Study Project is now entering its final stages. It is expected in fact that we will be able to submit all available data for computer analysis within the next ten days or so, at which time all of the questionnaires which.‘we have on hand will be destroyed, along with all other records of identifica- tion of project participants. However, there is still time for you to complete your questionnaire, if you have not already done so, and count yourself among those broadcasters who have seen this project through to its conclusion. May we look forward to receiving your completed questionnaire within the next few days? Incidentally, if you wish to receive a copy of the study project's final report, please let us know when you return your questionnaire. We will be more than happy to comply with your request. Sincerely yours, Gerald C. Beckwith, Research Associate American Broadcaster Study Project P.O. BOX 652 East Lansing, Michigan h8823 GCB/ll MICHIGAN STATE UNIVERSITY EAST LANSING . MICHIGAN 48323 COLLEGE OF COMMUNICATION ARTS 0 DEPARTMENT OF COMMUNICATION 0 CABLE: COMJIDEPT May 8, 1967 Dear Broadcaster: As we recently brought to light in a survey of the professional litera- ture, comparatively little is known today regarding the occupational role of the American broadcaster. This is true despite the very great importance of the radio and television media in our fast changing society. In order to correct this oversight and provide the modern student of professional broadcasting with a more realistic and factually accurate picture of the broadcaster than has here before been available, a national study is now being conducted by this department, in association with the Department of Television and Radio. You are now being asked to assist us in this important undertaking. While you are certainly under no direct obligation to do so, we think that you will nonetheless want to do so in order to insure the overall completeness and accuracy of the resulting data. In fact, as manager, newsman, programmer or salesman your personal participating in this project is very much needed for this very reason. The actual time required for you to complete the attached questionnaire will be something less than thirty minutes. Your personal responses will, of course, remain quite confidential. When you have completed the questionnaire, place it in the enclosed enve- lOpe and return it at once to the American Broadcaster Study Project, Box 652, East Lansing, Michigan. No postage will be required. If you have any comments or questions concerning the project itself or the results, please let us know. We will endeavor to provide you with what— ever information you request, including a copy of the final report when com- pleted. Thank you very much for your assistance. Sincerely yours, Gerald C. Beckwith, Research Associate ‘mMflican 13roadcaster Study Project P° 0- Box 652 EastlanSjLng, Michigan h8823 (mcl, APPENDIX B RULES OF ORDER FOR SAMPLE SELECTION 220 221 RULES OF ORDER FOR SAMPLE SELECTION Employing the following procedures, a sample of American broadcasters will be drawn from listing published in the most recent BROADCASTING YEARBOOK (1967). I. Having determined the actual number of AM radio sta- tions on the air, as indicated in the BROADCASTING YEARBOOK, enter a table of random numbers, by random process. The number "hit" will serve as a beginning point in the listing of radio stations contained in the yearbook. 00 - 09 Begin with first station listed 10 - l9 Begin with second station listed 20 - 29 Begin with third station listed 30 - 39 Begin with fourth station listed 40 - 49 Begin with fifth station listed 50 - 59 Begin with sixth station listed 60 - 69 Begin with seventh station listed 70 - 79 Begin with eighth station listed 80 - 89 Begin with ninth station listed 90 — 99 Begin with tenth station listed From the beginning point in the Yearbook listings, select every nth. station (n = 10). Having selected a given radio station as one to be contained in the sample of radio stations by this process, select in rotation order,the named person holding the position to be sampled. Position A: Station Manager Position B: Program Director Position C: Sales Manager Position D: News Director 222 Selection of named person or station may be varied accord- ing to the following rules of order: 1. In cases of duplication, e.g., when person listed as station manager, is listed as station manager for more than one station--as may happen with closely connected chain Operations--move to the next station in order and repeat process. In cases in which a "non-commercial" station is hit, move to the next station in order and repeat process. In cases in which a commercial station has ac- tually been on the air less than one year, move to the next station in order and repeat process. Avoid all count of FM radio stations, even if listed separately. In cases in which both a General Manager and a Manager is listed, select one or the other in alternate order. In cases in which "corporate officers" are listed, rather than conventional station management titles, select most apprOpriate name. If selection cannot be made on this basis, proceed to next station in order and repeat process. In cases in which both corporate officers and sta- tion management titles are listed, select from list of management titles. In those cases in which desired position is not listed (e.g., News Director), proceed to next sta- tion in order and repeat process. In cases such as those indicated in #8 above, check to make certain that function itself is not covered by other title, such as substitution of Production Manager for Program Director, or Pro- gram Manager for Program Director, Public Affairs Director for News Director, etc. Remember the title is not being sampled, only the function nor- mally associated with the title. II. 223 With reference to television: Having determined the actual number of TV stations on the air (commercial only), enter a table of random numbers, by random process, and repeat procedures listed above with ref- erence to AM radio stations, with the following addi- tions and/or revisions. 1. From the beginning point in the Yearbook listings of TV stations, select every nth. station. (n = 3) 2. Avoid selections of UHF television stations in so-called "mixed markets." 3. Always favor TV station Manager, or similarly Specified roles, over General Manager role, when such relates to both Radio and TV properties. 4. In the event a combination station is "hit," that is, a station listing General Manager over both Operations without separate listing of TV and Radio Managers, use General Manager as selected position. 5. Avoid selection Of Television Station if such is "sister station" of previously selected radio station. APPENDIX C STUDY QUESTIONNAIRE 224 IIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIII IIIIIIIIIIIII AMERICAN BROADCASTER CONFIDENTIAL MANAGEMENT SURVEY IIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIII Code NO. I We are interested in learning more about the occupational role of the American broadcaster. As a Ienber of the national sample you are asked to help in this important undertaking by completing the fern and questionnaire attached. Your responses will be employed only for the particular purposes of this study and will otherwise remain absolutely confidential. No identification is expected to be made of yourself or of the station you represent, either in the final report or in any publication based upon it. PART I. Index Items 1. What type station are you presently associated with? a. Radio Station b. Television Station c. Radio and Television 2. Please indicate your present position in broadcasting. If you position involves duties normally associated with more than one of the following, please rank titles in order of importance (1-2-3) with reference to the actual duties performed. a. General Manager d. Sales Manager b. Station Manager e. News Director c. Program Director f. Other (Please Specify) 3. Please check the type schools which you have attended and circle the actual number of years in attendance in each instance. a. _High School, 1 2 3 u b. _____Iechnical or Vocational School, 1 2 3 u c. _____pollege or University, 1 2 3 u d. _____Graduate or Professional School, 1 2 3 u e. _____pther (Please Specify“ , l 2 3 u a. What is your present age? 5. Sex? Male Female PART II Instructions A number of statements appear on the pages that follow which are said to characterize various aspects of the broadcaster's occupational role. In each case two opposing statements have been listed on either end of a scale of relative measure. You are asked to indicate in each instance which of the statements presented seems to best Characterize how the situation described is actually viewed by the broadcaster. EXAMPLE Statement A Statement B Because Of the great amount of pressure involved in the broadcaster's job, it is expected that anyone occupying such a position be relatively young. Because of the great amount of responsibility involved in the broadcaster's job, it is expected that anyone occupying such a position be an older and more mature person. (3) (2) (l) (l) (2) (3) PART II A. If you agree with Statement A, indicate your agreement by checking the most appropriate place on the left side of the scale. Place "3" implies that you "agree absolutely" with the statement as presented. Place "2" implies that you "agree very much" with the statement and place "1" that you simply "tend to agree." 8. If you find yourself in agreement with Statement 8, indicate your agreement (and the strength of agreement) by checking the most appropriate place on the right side of the scale. As above, place "3" implies that you "agree absolutely" with the statement as presented, place "2" that you "agree very much" and place "1" that you simply "tend to agree" with the statement. C. Please respond to each item in the list following even if you feel that neither of the statements paired in any given case is particularly appropriate to the situation. This is important. D. If you feel that both statements presented in any given case are appropriate to the situation please make a choice anyway by indicating which of the statements paired seems to be most appropriate. E. Do not check any scale more than once. It will be impossible to score any scale so marked. Statement A 1. In hiring or setting employment policy, the broadcaster is expected to stipulate that only the individual's capacity to perform his or her duties properly, according to station requirements, is a legitimate basis for giving or withholding employment. In a word, technical proficiency is the only important criteria for employment in broadcasting. Statement 8 In hiring or setting employment policy, the broadcaster is expected to look beyond the individual's apparent capacity or skill. He is expected also to be concerned with how the person is likely to fit into the community, with whether he is or is not a good family man, with his attitudes, values and beliefs, his credit rating and a hundred and one other personal considerations. (3) (2) (l) 2. Not excluding the special case of the sus- taining program, it is expected that the broadcaster give as much time and attention to the development and promotion of one program or time segment as another. (1) (2) (3) Not excluding the special case of the sus- taining program, it is expected that certain programs or time segments on the broadcaster's schedule will be of greater importance than others and that the broadcaster will plan his time and allot his resources accordingly. (3) (2) (l) 3. The broadcaster's definition of the concept community is expected to be broadly conceived, even to the point of involving the most general views of the greater society and the national interest. (1) (2) (3) The broadcaster's definition of the concept community is expected to be more or less limited to a consideration of the particular views, values and interests of his special service area. (3) (2) (l) u. The only real obligation a broadcaster owes his client is to do his very best to satisfy that client's specific advertising needs, as defined by contract or agreement. (1) (2) (3) In general the broadcaster owes those Obligations to his client, personal or otherwise, which may, within limits, be required to insure a continuation of their business relationship. (3) C2) (1) 5. In broadcasting, authority is vested more generally in the person than in the office. This means that station activities tend not to be defined specifically in terms of recognized and accepted rules of order so much as to reflect the particular will a and character of management. (1) (2) (3) In broadcasting, authority is vested more in the office than in the person. Station activities, therefore, are usually defined in terms of rather Specific duties and respon- sibilities, rights and Obligations, and rules of order which are not subject to change on the basis of the personal whims of management. (3) (2) (1) (l) (2) (3) PART II 10. 11. Statement A In all dealings with his client, the broadcaster is expected to maintain a policy of full disclosure; that is, he is expected to be completely open with the client when informing him of the relative advantages and limitations of his station as an advertising medium for the client's particular products or services. Statement 3 The broadcaster's aim is expected always to be to sell the prospective client on the unique merits of his station as an effective adverb- tising medium. His role is not that of an advisor or counselor. If making an important sale requires that he be not too specific with regard to certain types of information, a sound practice would be to avoid the discussion. (3) (2) (l) The broadcaster is expected to hire highly adaptable and interchangeable personnel, persons with general skills who have the capacity of fitting into almost any situ- ation and performing in a creditable, if not always a distinguished, manner. Persons of this type are called generalists. (l) (2) (3) The broadcaster is expected to hire whenever- possible, highly trained and specialised personnel, persons who possess rather special. skills and knowledge which qualify them uniquely to perform the specific functions to which they may be assigned but which may tend to limit their general utility value. Persons of this type are called specialists. (3) (2) (1) Management performance over time can best be evaluated in terms of profit and loss statements, ratings and similar Objective considerations. (1) (2) (3) Management performance over time can best be evaluated in terms of a station's relative standing in the community, its status of leadership, public approval and similar qualitative considerations. (3) (2) (l) (l) (2) (3) The broadcaster is expected to maintain The broadcaster is expected not to allow his business relations only with those personal feelings with regard to the client or- clients for whom he has high respect the client's product or service to affect his and personal esteem. judgments one way or another. (3) (2) (l) (l) (2) (3) It is expected that all segments of It is expected that certain segments of the the broadcasting audience will be of broadcasting audience will always be of greater- equal importance to the broadcaster importance to the broadcaster than others, fin! and therefore deserving of equal one reason or another, and therefore more attention and consideration from his deserving of his attention and consideration. point of view. (SO far as the planning and development of program sources is concerned.) (3) (2) (l) (l) (2) (3) In making purchasing agreements, the broadcaster is expected to deal only with those concerns which are able to supply the highest quality goods or services at the lowest prices consistent with station requirements, regardless of source or location or such personal considerations as friendship with or relationship to the seller. In making purchasing agreements, the broad- caster is expected to use his buying power to attract new business to the station or me a means of continuing old business. He is there- fore expected to buy locally when at all possible and to buy primarily from station advertisers, at a slight disadvantage, if necessary. (3) (2) (l) (l) (2) (3) PART II 12. 13. 1Q. 15. 16. 17. 10. Statement A It is expected that the broadcaster will be primarily interested in the production of exciting and original local advertising: that is, in producing commercial spots that will attract attention and sustained interest as well as critical acclaim. Statement 8 It is expected that the broadcaster concentrate all his efforts on the production of local advertising that will increase client sales and profits, without any special regard for Spot originality or other strictly qualitative measures. Performance is all that counts. (3) (2) (1) Although there are many private, or personal, aspects to his job, the broadcaster is expected to be most concerned with its public character. His relations with the public, in fact, are expected to be of a type quite similar to those of a politician or of a public official. (1) (2) (3) Although there are many public aspects to his job, the broadcaster is expected to act as if his is essentially a private role like that of any other businessman. His "performance", therefore, is usually guided more by the specific demands of the situation than by any overriding concern with his "public image." (3) (2) (1) It is expected that the broadcaster be strongly committed to his own programing efforts. He would not be honest with him- self or with his audience if he were to allow anything on the air that he would object to personally. (I) (2) (3) It is expected that the broadcaster make his decisions on programming without regard to any measure of personal concern or interest. He is expected instead to remain personally detached and non-comitted when it comes to determining what is to go on the air. (3) ‘ (2) (1) In performing his job the broadcaster usually expects to be evaluated in terms of his relative contribution to the good and well-being of the general community. His job takes its primary meaning from the concept of the public interest. (1) (2) (3) In performing his job the broadcaster fully expects to be evaluated in terms of his relative contribution to the well-being of the industry and the good of the economy. His job is one that takes its primary meaning from the general concept of the free enterprise system and the open marketplace. (3) (2) (l) The personal life of a station employee is expected not to be of concern to a broadcaster so long as it does not directly reflect discredit on the station or otherwise interfere with the individual's ability to perform his assigned duties in an acceptable manner. (1) (2) (3) As a staff member of a broadcasting station, the individual is believed at all times to be "in the public eye," and cannot avoid identifying the station with his activities as a result. Therefore it is expected that the personal life of the station employee will bs_the proper concern of the broadcaster. (3) (2) (l) The broadcaster who is most apt to do the best job in his conduct of station relations may be described as rather cool, formal and somewhat impersonal. Such a man will, for the most part, hold himself aloof from his staff both on the job and off. (1) (2) (3) The broadcaster who is most apt to do the best job in his conduct of station relations may be described as warm, friendly and personable. Such a man will usually maintain rather close relations with his staff both on the job and off. (3) (2) (1) In the selection of individual programs or in the selection of station formats, the broad- caster's primary concern is expected always to be in terms of the program or format's capacity to attract advertiser interest. (1) (2) (3) In the selection of individual programs or in the selection of station formats, the broad- caster's primary concern is expected always to be in terms of the program or format's capacity to actually satisfy the interest of the audience. (3) (2) (l) (l) (2) (3) PART II Statement A 19. The broadcaster's primary need is for a sense of accomplishment, something which can best be drawn from the satisfaction of knowing that he is doing the best job possible under any given circumstance. Technical competency is its own reward so far as the broadcaster is concerned. (3) (2) (l) 20. The broadcaster is expected always to select program themes on the basis of general value, merit and importance, themes which deal with some of the real, human problems of the day, even if controversial or unpopular. Statement 8 Most broadcasters have an inherent need for feeling important and wanted and their work allows them unique Opportunities for gratifying these particular needs. The satisfaction of having done a good job is usually not enough for such a man. He must also experience the warmth of personal acceptance and the feeling that people recognise and appreciate his work and 'alue his special worth as a person. (1) (2) (3) The broadcaster is expected to select program themes with reference to the particular values of the community actually being served, even if this should mean going easy on certain controversial issues or unpopular topics. (3) (2) (l) 21. Because of the rather unique, personal nature of broadcasting, management- staff relations must often be based upon somewhat subjective considerations, or at least upon factors other than strict performance. What a person is, as a person, is often more important to a station than what he does or seems capable of doing an the job. (1) (2) (3) In broadcasting, as in most other businesses, management is expected to base all decisions affecting personnel entirely on conditions of performance, with reference to some objective standard of behavior, for example, or some other criteria of productivity or efficiency. that a person is, as a pencil, is an interim]. consideration. (3) (2) (l) 22. The broadcaster is expected to relate to each member of his staff as a pro- fessional colleague, one with whom he shares certain unique perspectives, ideals and special compentencies. Such a relationship is also likely to stress shared authority and joint responsibility. (1) (2) (3) The broadcaster is expected to relate to each member of his staff in the formal manner of an employer-employee or boss-worker association. His is the position of decision maker, while the subordinate's position is more that of implementation or execution of the decisions made. (3) (2) (l) 23. The broadcaster's primary concern for any particular program on his schedule must always be in terms of its inherent merit, value or quality, as a program, independ- ently of any special purpose which it may otherwise serve. (l) (2) (3) The only concern which the broadcaster is obliged to have for any particular program on his schedule is whether or not it achieves its intended purpose, in the delivery of audience, the sale of products or what have you. (3) (2) - (1) 2n. From a business point of view, the broad- caster is expected generally to provide some special consideration to certain of his clients; for example, those who advertise favored products, who maintain larger than average accounts or who require less direct service. (1) (2) (3) As a matter of principle, the broadcaster is expected always to treat all of his clients with equal degrees of consideration, regardless of the special characteristics of the client, service or product or the otherwise closeness of the client-broadcaster relationship. (3) (2) (l) (l) (2) (3) _. ,.,——- q.- M PART II 25. 25. 27. 2.. 29. 30. Statement A The broadcaster's measure of an audience for any particular program is expected always to be in terms of certain general qualities, such as whether or not his listeners or viewers are of a particular desired type. He is expected to take satisfaction from the knowledge that certain kinds of persons tuned in, regardless of the actual numbers involved. M1 The broadcaster's measure of an audience for any particular program is expected always to be in terms of how many persons did or did not tune-in. His satisfaction will come from the knowledge that a particular program outdrew its competition for the time period involved, regardless of the nature or composition of the audience in general. (3) (2) (l) The broadcaster is expected to take a great deal of pride and personal satisfaction in his staff, to feel strongly about each man's unique contribution to the station and to demonstrate his feelings and regard openly whenever possible. (1) (2) (3) The broadcaster is not expected to openly acknowledge any special feeling for or dependence upon any individual member of his staff, no matter how important, and only indirectly his regard for the staff as a whole. He is expected to maintain his reserve. (3) (2) (1) It is expected that the broadcaster will assume personal responsibility for selling each member of his staff on the idea of profitability, with the end in view of having this basic and central concept order their thought and action on a day to day basis. (1) (2) (3) While the broadcaster is expected not to sell the idea of profitability short, he is nonethe- less expected to place the concept in a proper balance with other, more basic, principles and motives, such as public service, program originality, and so forth. (3) (2) (l) The broadcaster is expected to maintain a special kind of affection for the audience he serves, a feeling close to that of love. At least he is required to have a sense of being quite close to the audience and of being dependent upon its good favor. (l) (2) (3) So far as his relationship with the audience is concerned, the broadcaster is expected to carry out his responsibilities with as much of a sense of detachment as possible. A sense of objectivity or of affective neutrality, is an essential characteristic of the broadcaster role. (3) (2) (1) In this day and age it is expected that the broadcaster take a leadership role in developing and maintaining basic standards of fair employment. Prospective employees have the right to expect to be judged exclusively on the basis of set performance criteria and without regard for such factors as race, religion, creed, sex or what have you. (3) (2) (l) The broadcaster is expected to recognize that the advertiser-client has but a single purpose, that of maximizing sales and profits, and that his purchase of broadcasting time is intended as a means of achieving this goal. Therefore, the broadcaster is expected to make such ad- justments in his programming commitments as may be required to satisfy the main objective of the advertiser-client. (l) (2) (3) While recognising the value of the principle of "fair employment," it is nonetheless expected that the broadcaster take a somewhat broader view of the situation. The broadcaster's objective is the development of a smooth working organiza- tional team, something which can only be achieved by hiring those persons who will be most apt to fit in with the existing character of the station. (1) (2) (3) The broadcaster is expected to take a hard line with his various advertiser-clients, at least relatively so, in apposition to the view that his medium is simply an extension of the adver- tiser's marketing plan. He is expected to advocate the view that the client share in the broadcaster's own public service commitments, by, in part at least, supporting the programming goals designed for the station. (3) (2) (l) (l) (2) (3) mu II 7 Statement A Statement 3 31. A good broadcaster is inevitably a father A broadcaster is required to be a good employer figure to his staff. To be a good father, only. His understanding and considerateness whether to his children or to his associates, may or may not be needed, depending upon the it is expected that he be understanding and situation. As for relating warmly to his staff considerate, but above all that he relate . . . this is simply an irrelevant consideration. warmly to other people. e 0 e as 0 e e e e e as e s e (3) (2) (l) (1) (2) (3) 32. Today's most successful broadcasters are Today's most successful broadcasters tend to usually men who do not consider a station's develop programs having rather specialized public service obligations apart from their purposes and appeal. Such broadcasters do regular programming efforts. Such broad- not usually mix programs by form or type. casters attempt rather to integrate elements Programs of a public service nature, for example, of this nature in all their programming. A may be created and given fair placement on the program characterized as entertainment, for schedule, as will programs intended to entertain example, may be designed to satisfy certain and to sell, but the one type is not intended to public service commitments as well. satisfy the purpose of the other. (3) (2) (l) (l) (2) (3) 33. The broadcaster most generally expects to be Hhile the broadcaster generally expects that loved, admired and respected on a personal his work will be accepted and appreciated by basis by the public for his work as a the public, from a professional point of view broadcaster. he does not expect, need or want personal admiration or attention. (3) (2) (l) (l) (2) (3) 3“- It is expected that the broadcaster It is not expected that the broadcaster PCP- terminate any account on his station if and sonally judge or 9V31U8t° the prOducts or when he loses personal confidence in the services offered t° th° public by th' GdVOP' worth of the product or service being tiser. His responsibility in this regard extends advertised only to the point of making the public aware of ' ’ the offer. (3) (2) (l) (l) (2) (3) 35. The broadcaster is expected to treat each The broadcaster is expected to make a practice member of his staff with complete impartiality, of being quite flexible when dealing with various no matter what his professional status or members of his staff, treating each according personal relationship to the broadcaster. to his particular needs, qualities or expectations. (3) (2) (l) (l) (2) (3) 36. In his approach to his job, it is expected In his approach to his job, it is expected and that the broadcaster place company interests understood that the broadcaster will place his and job-related responsibilities first above responsibility to his family above the interests all else, including the personal needs and of the company. A dedicated family man, in fact, interests of his family. A sense of complete is more apt to fulfill the demands and require- dedication and loyalty to the job is an ments of the broadcaster role than any other. absolute requirement of the broadcaster role. (3) (2) (l) (l) (2) (3) 37. It is generally expected that the broadcaster It is generally expected that the broadcaster will define programming, whether network or will define programming, whether network or local, local, as simply the over-all attraction OP in terms of potential service values only. The hooker for the station . - - something to greater the program's appeal, or attraction, the pull an audience of sufficient size or greater the station's potential service to the composition to be appealing to prospective audience, by definition. time buyers. (3) (2) (l) (l) (2) (3) PART II I Statement A Statement 3 38. In relating himself to his staff it is In relating himself to his staff it is expected expected that the broadcaster will think that the broadcaster will think primarily in primarily in terms of the overall human terms of how each person might be used most factor, of developing high levels of effectively to satisfy the special demands of staff morale and personally satisfying his own job. His concern for the individual, relationships. His concern for the then, is basically of an instrumental order. individual, then, is not unlike a father's concern for the welfare of one of his children. (3) (2) (l) (1) (2) (3) PART III To better enable us to properly place your particular responses to the scale items presented in Part II of the questionnaire in perspective, it is necessary that we also tabulate the following items of information. Your further cooperation in completing this part of the questionnaire, then, will be greatly appreciated . Thank you. l. Hhat position did you occupy immediately before assuming your present job? 2. In terms of your long range career objectives in broadcasting, what position do you aspire to? 3. How many years experience have you actually had in broadcasting? u. How long have you been with your present employer? 5. How many years have you been in your present position? 6. How many times have you changed locations (or situations) within the past ten years? 7. Do you at present consider making a change in location (or situation) or do you think that you may do so within the next two or three years? Yes No 7A. If your answer to question "7" is "Yes", please indicate your reasons for considering a change in the space below. 8. If you were offered a slightly better paying position outside of broadcasting at the present time, do you think that you would be willing to make such a move? Yes No .A. Without considering salary, what factors might cause you to leave broadcasting for other fiedds? PART III 9. Do you at present maintain in your own name any type memberships in organizations relating to broadcasting, whether local, regional or national? Yes No 9A. If your answer to question "9" is "Yes", please list the memberships actually held below. QB. Are your membership fees to the above named organizations paid for in whole or in part by your employer? Yes No 10. Do you regularly attend meetings of your state broadcasting association? Yes No 10A. Regional NAB meetings? Yes No 108. National NAB meetings? Yes No ll. Have you attended the meetings of any other national, regional or state association relating to broadcasting within the past two years? If so, please specify below. llA. were you, in whole or in part, reimbursed for your expenses on such occasions by your employer? Yes No 12. Have you ever held office in or served on a committee of any of the above named organisations? Yes No 12A. If "Yes", please provide particulars below. 13. What is the size of the station in which you are presently working? Please indicate your answer by specifying the approximate number of full time employees working for the station. Less than 12 full time employees 12 - 2n full time employees 25 - us full time employees 50 - 7n full time employees 75 - 99 full time employees 100 or more full time empoyees lu. What is the size of the market in which your station is located? Less than 25,000 population 25,000 - 100,000 population 100,000 - 250,000 population 250,000 - 500,000 population 500,000 - 1,000,000 population One million or more population r PART III 10 15. 16. 17. II. 19. 20. 21. 22. Does your station maintain NAB membership? Yes No Does your station maintain membership in your state association of broadcasting? Yes No Is your station a Radio and/or TV Code Subscriber? Yes No On the average, how would you rate your station's share of the market? Excellent Quite Good About Average Quite Poor On the average, how would you rate your station's competition? Very Strong Strong Moderate Heak From your own point of view, how would you rate your station's relative profitability? Excellent Quite Good About Average Quite Poor From your own point of view, how would you rate your station's willingness and capacity to satisfy community needs and interests? Excellent Quite Good About Average Quite Poor As a broadcaster, to whom do you personally look for some indication as to how your particular job should be performed -- that is, persons or groups that tend to set those standards of performance with which you are most in agreement? Considered Considered Considered Very Somewhat Relatively Important Important Unimportant a. NAB Leadership b. Listeners and/or Viewers c. Hedia Critics and other Professional Observers d. Government, including the FCC and Congress e. Station Colleagues (Men with whom you work) f. Advertisers, Sponsors and/or their Agents g. Trade Publications, Editors and Publishers PART III Station Management (and/or Station Ownership) Members of your own family Other Broadcasters Influential community Leaders, Business or Professional Professional Groups and Associations (RTNDA, IRTS, BPA, etc.) Friends and Associates, in or out of Broadcasting Broadcast Unions (APL-CIO) Network Personnel Others (Please Specify) Considered Very Igpgrtant Please Return To AMERICAN BROADCASTER STUDY PROJECT P. O. Box 652 East Lansing, Michigan ”8823 (Att: G. C. Beckwith) Considered Somewhat Igpgrtant Considhred Relatimny \ l APPENDIX D CORRELATION OF BACKGROUND, SYSTEM AND TEST VARIABLES 237 238 00.H m0.0 mm.0l m0.0l HH.0I mN.0I 0m.0 0m.0| 0H.0 NH.0 00.0! 00.0I NH 00.H m0.0 hH.0 m0.0 vH.0I 00.0I H0.0I Nm.0l m~.0l m0.0| 0H.0 HH 00.H HH.0I 0H.0I 0H.0 NN.0 hH.0 HH.0I vH.0I 00.0 No.0 0H 00.H 0H.0 00.0I 50.0 00.0 ma.0l m0.0 00.01 m0.0 m 00.H hm.0l 0H.0I m0.0| «H.0I v0.0 va.0l hH.0 m 00.H hN.0I hN.0 0H.0 m0.0l mm.0 m0.0| h 00.H 0N.0I 00.0 no.0 N0.0I m0.0l 0 00.H mH.0I 0H.0I No.0 00.0I m 00.H no.0 No.0 H0.0I e 00.H 0H.0I 5H.0 m 00.H N0.0I N 00.H H ea ma NH dd 0H m 0 h 0 m v m N H wZOHadqmmmOU mddem¢> Emma 92¢ .Zmamwm .QZDomUMU ummu can .Emumhm .ossoumxomm 240 0H.0 00.0 HH.0I 00.0 no.0 H0.0 0H.0 00.0I 00.0 00.0I 0H.0I no.0I 00.0 0o.o mm 00.0 00.0I 00.0I 00.0 00.0I no.0 0N.0 00.0I 00.0 No.0 o0.0| 0H.0 NH.oI om.0 nm 00.0I N0.0I H0.0 H0.0I 00.0 N0.0I 00.0I 00.0I N0.0I H0.oI oo.0 00.0 No.0 No.0 00 no.0! 0H.0I HH.0 o0.0 0H.0 00.0I 00.0I H0.0I 00.0 00.0 H0.0 0H.0I 00Lo NH.0I 00 00.0I 0H.0I 00.0 00.0I H0.0I H0.o 00.0I 00.0 o0.0 o0.ol o0.o H0.0I N0.0I HH.0I om H0.0I No.0 00.0 N0.0I 00.0 oH.0 00.0 00.0 o0.o H0.0I H0.0I o0.ol 00.0I H0.0I mm oo.0l 0H.0 00.0 00.0 00.0 00.0 00.0I o0.0l no.0! NH.0 00.0I nH.0I n0.0I HN.0I Nm o0.ol 00.0I N0.0I oo.0l 00.0 oo.0I HH.0I N0.0I 00.0I 00.0 00.0 0H.0I 0H.0 HH.0I Hm oH.0I o0.0 N0.0I HH.0I 00.0 00.0 nH.0I o0.o 00.0I 00.0 00.0 oH.0I no.0 NN.0I om HN.0 oo.o oH.0I 0N.0 00.0I no.0 nN.o mH.oI 00.0 0H.0I 00.0I 0N.0 0H.0I no.0 0N NN.0 no.0 NH.0I NN.0 00.0I 00.0 0N.0 no.0l 00.0 MH.0I 00.0I NN.0 0H.0I nn.0 0N o0.0l 00.0 00.0I 00.0I NH.o H0.0I 00.0I 00.0 00.0I N0.0I 00.0I 0H.0 0H.0I o0.0I nN 00.0 HH.0 00.0I 00.0! 00.0 HH.0 00.0 No.0 o0.0l no.0 H0.o oo.o mH.0I 00.0 0N oH 0H NH HH 0H 0 0 n 0 0 o m N H .AomscHumouo mooHumHmnuoo mHQMHHm> umou can .Emumnm .ocsonmxomm 241 oN.01 00.0 00.01 00.0 0H.0 oH.0 00.01 00.01 0H.o H0.0 No.0 00.01 H0.01 00.01 N0 0H.01 0H.01 oH.01 0H.01 00.0 00.0 oH.0 oo.01 0H.0 00.01 00.0 00.0 o0.0 00.01 H0 0N.01 0H.01 0H.01 no.01 oN.0 oN.0 00.01 no.01 nH.0 No.01 00.01 oH.0 00.01 NH.01 00 on.0 NH.0 o0.0 00.01 00.01 H0.0 0H.0 0o.0 HN.01 00.0 00.0 00.01 00.01 0H.0 0N oo.H oo.0 0H.o 00.0 NH.01 00.0 00.0 0H.0 0N.01 00.0 00.0 0N.01 H0.0 00.0 0N oo.H 0o.0 0H.0 0H.o 0H.0 0H.0 nH.0 No.01 00.0 HH.01 nH.0 NH.01 00.01 nN oo.H 00.0 00.0 00.0 no.0 n0.0 HN.0I oH.01 NH.01 o0.0 oo.0 oH.0 0N oo.H oH.0 0N.0 00.0 00.0 HH.01 No.0 o0.0 00.01 H0.01 00.0 0N oo.H o0.0 No.0 HH.0 00.0 0H.01 00.01 o0.0 00.01 H0.01 oN oo.H oo.0 00.0 00.0 NH.01 No.01 no.0 00.01 00.01 0N oo.H 00.0 0H.0 No.01 00.01 No.0 No.01 00.0 NN oo.H N0.0I 00.01 No.0 o0.0 00.0 00.0 HN oo.H H0.0 H0.01 oo.01 H0.01 no.01 0N oo.H 0N.0 No.0 oo.01 0H.01 0H oo.H NN.01 0H.0 00.0 0H oo.H 0o.01 00.01 nH oo.H 00.0 0H oo.H 0H 0N nN 0N 0N oN 0N NN HN 0N 0H 0H nH 0H 0H AooooHuooov mcoHHMHoumoo wHQMHmm> pmmu one .EoumNm .onooumxomm 242 oo.H HN.o 00.0 oo.01 00.0 no.0 oH.0: 0H.01 0H.01 oH.0 mm oo.H 00.01 0H.01 oo.01 oo.0 HN.01 0H.01 0N.01 0o.0 n0 oo.H 0H.o 00.01 nH.0 0H.o 00.0 00.0 no.01 0m oo.H 00.01 oo.0 mH.o 1no.o 0N.0 oH.01 mm oo.H 00.01 00.01 oo.01 00.0 0N.0: o0 oo.H no.0 oo.01 HH.0 0N.0 mm oo.H 0H.o om.o 0H.01 Nm oo.H N0.o 0H.01 Hm oo.H HN.ou om oo.H mm 00 nm 00 00 om mm mm Hm om 0N oo.0 00.01 no.0 No.01 0H.01 0H.01 00.0 0H.o NH.01 00.0 oo.0 NH.01 oo.0 oH.0 mm H0.0 oo.0 0H.o No.0 oo.0 No.01 0H.o oH.0 0H.01 0o.0 No.01 no.01 00.01 oH.0 n0 0H.01 00.01 oo.01 00.01 no.0 00.0 00.0 no.01 No.01 0H.01 nH.01 No.0 oo.01 no.0 0m 0N.0: oo.0 No.01 0H.01 00.0 00.0 oo.0 no.01 H0.0 no.01 00.01 00.0 oo.01 00.01 mm HN.01 HH.01 No.o1 00.01 oo.o1 NH.01 NH.01 H0.01 00.0 00.0 00.01 00.0 00.01 00.01 om No.01 oo.0 oo.01 0H.01 oo.0 0H.o 00.0 No.0 No.01 no.0 oo.0 nH.0 NH.01 0H.01 mm mm nN 0N 0N oN 0N NN HN 0N 0H 0H nH 0H 0H AooscHuoouo mGOHumHmHHoo mHanmmb umou can .EmumNm .ocdommxomm Background Variables Variable Number System Variables Variable Number 1 2 21 26 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 Test Variables Variable Number 13 15 16 17 18 19 20 22 23 24 25 27 243 KEY Variable Identification System Position Role Function Entreprenurship High School Education Technical-Vocational Education College Education Graduate or Professional Education Other Education Age Sex Experience Variable Identification Station Type Geographical Location Employer Support of Memberships Employer Support of Activity Station Size Market Size Station NAB Membership Station State Association Membership Station Radio/TV Code Subscriber Station "Share of Market" Station "Competition" Station "Profitability" Station "Service to Community" Station Ownership Network Affiliation Variable Identification Aspiration Level Tenure in System Tenure in Position Mobility Loyalty Career Commitment Membership Status Attend NAB National Convention Attend NAB Regional Convention Attend State Association Convention Attend "Other" Association Meetings Activeness in Association Affairs APPENDIX E REFERENCE MEASURE: INTER-ITEM CORRELATIONS 244 245 oo.H 00.0 m0.0l No.0 h0.0l H0.0I v0.0l v0.0 N0.0I m0.0l v0.0! NH.0I 00.0I Hobo! >0.0I nH.0! 0H oo.H V¢.0 NN.0 Nm.0 NH.0 0m.0 0H.0 HN.0 0H.0 HH.0 NN.0 wH.0 0N.0 v0.0 NN.0 ma oo.H vN.0 nn.0 ma.0 NN.0 0H.0 0H.0 wN.0 0H.0 mN.0 NN.0 NN.0 oH.0 NN.0 vH 00.H 0m.0 mm.0 mm.0 mv.0 00.0 ma.0 NH.0 HN.0 m0.0 Hm.0 v0.0 MH.0 MH oo.H Nm.0 mm.0 HH.0 HH.0 0m.0 no.0 «N.0 HN.0 Nm.0 50.0 mm.0 NH oo.H 0N.0 NN.0 00.0 ¢0.0 0N.0 Nm.0 0H.0 NH.0 0N.0 NH.0 Ha 00.H 0N.0 NH.0 NN.0 NH.0 HN.0 H0.0 vm.0 m0.0 NH.0 0H oo.H 0N.0 50.0 VN.0 vm.0 m0.0 NH.0 @0.0 00.0 m oo.H 00.0 NH.0 mm.0 MH.0 50.0 m0.0| mH.0 0 oo.H vH.0 00.0 m0.0 hN.0 00.0I 0N.0 h oo.H NH.0 00.0 00.0 vH.0 00.0 0 00.H NH.0 00.0 NH.0 00.0 m 00.H oH.0 mH.0 Hm.0 v 00.H N0.0I nH.0 m oo.H No.0 N 00.H a ma ma va ma NH AH 0H m 0 h m m w m N H mZOHB cmmHm m 15 m m w m N H muouomm meadz UZHQGOH moauflh “MMDm¢m2 mozmmmmmm 255 i»...- ia ‘Il mm.o oo.0 NH.o no.ou oo.0 no.0- Ho.o OH.o o~.o- mH Ho.H mo.o- mo.o- mo.o vv.o- HH.o No.o om.o- NN.0 mH mo.H oo.0 mo.o- mH.o- mv.o mH.o- oo.o mo.o m~.o HH oo.H km.o oH.o- HH.0 mo.o H~.o HH.o- no.0- mm.o MH no.H oH.0- mm.o mm.o mH.o nH.o 0H.o nH.0 H0.0 NH mH.H nH.0- Hm.o- vH.o- Ho.o- No.0 Hm.o Ho.o mo.o- HH om.H HH.0 no.0 no.0 No.0- om.o- ~o.o- h~.o- Hm.o- OH v~.H ~m.o- HH.0 mo.o- so.o- mH.o- HH.0- no.0- mm.o a om.H Ho.o- mH.o- mH.o o~.o m~.o OH.o- nH.0- nn.0- m mm.H no.0- o~.o- «O.O NH.o- oo.0- m~.o- HH.0 NH.o- n HH.H mo.o mH.o NH.o oo.0 no.0 m~.o mH.o- oo.0- m mm.H 0N.0 om.o HN.0- nH.0- mc.o- o~.o HH.0 mo.ou m mG.H mo.o mo.o- oH.0 NH.0- mm.o- mo.o no.0 No.o- H Hm.H mo.o- mo.o HN.0- vo.o- NN.0 mo.o mo.o- pm.o- m Ho.m mo.o no.0 oo.0 Ho.o HH.o nH.0- no.0 HN.0- m mH.H oo.o HH.0 mm.ou No.0 mo.o mH.o- Hm.o- mo.o H mmsHm> :mmHm SH mH «H mH NH HH OH m muouomm Aflmfidflflfiouv XHHHME UGHUMOH HOHUMM "whammmfi OOGOHOMOM APPENDIX H ORIENTATION MEASURE: FACTOR LOADING MATRIX 256 257 Nm.o 50.0: m0.0| NH.0 Nm.01 00.0 mN.o| 00.0: HH.0 Hm.o on.0: mH mm.o m0.0 mH.o| mm.o NN.0 vH.0 HN.0 0N.0 NH.0 NH.0 «v.01 MH .Nm.o 00.0 HH.0 HN.0 00.0I oH.0: 00.01 mN.0 NH.0 5N.0 mM.o 5H oN.0 0N.0 HH.0 H0.0 NH.0 no.0 mH.0| mm.0| 0N.0: 0N.0 oH.0 0H H0.H HH.0 H0.0: NN.0: no.0 0N.0: HH.0 5H.0: mv.o mo.0 mv.ou mH mo.H 00.0 HN.0 HM.0 oo.0 vH.0I 5H.0 oH.0: 0H.0| NN.0 v0.0 «H oo.H no.0 MN.0 vH.o mo.0 5v.0 MN.0I NH.0I mN.0| MN.0 00.0 MH 50.H NN.0: vH.on no.0 0N.0 NN.0 NN.0 50.0: 00.0 mo.0 mv.0 NH MH.H H0.0: 50.0: 0N.0: no.0: mo.0| oo.0 No.0: mm.o| vv.o oH.0: HH 0N.H vN.0 50.0: MH.0| 00.0: 00.0: NH.0: NN.0: mm.o nH.0 mm.o: 0H HN.H mN.0 No.0 0N.0 NN.0: HN.0 5v.o NN.0: MH.0 mo.o| H0.0: m om.H nH.0 NN.0: vH.0 00.0 00.0: 50.0 oH.0: mm.ou oo.0 NH.0 m 0M.H oo.0: 0H.o NN.0: v0.0: no.0 0N.0 vN.o oo.0: om.o No.0: 5 Hv.H 50.0 No.0: oo.0 mN.o 50.0 Nm.o| H0.0 HH.0 No.0 mm.o| m Nm.H 0N.0 mm.0| MN.0 mN.0 H0.0: v0.0 mH.o MH.o NH.0 mo.ou m mm.H NN.0: mo.ou 0N.0: 00.0 NH.0I mm.0I H0.0: H0.0: vm.o 00.0 e «m.H 50.0: MH.o mm.0| v0.0: 50.0I 0H.o 0H.0 mN.0I 50.0 oH.0: m v0.m 0H.01 v0.0: oH.0 0H.01 00.0 mo.on on.0: Hm.o mo.o mm.ou N mH.v MH.0| mm.o mo.o NH.0 «N.01 oH.0: 5m.o| MN.o| mH.0 HN.0 H mmsHm> 0H m m 5 m m v m N H muouomm cmmHm mefidz UZHQdOH m080€m "MMDmflmz ZOHBNBZmHmO 258 0N.0 MH.0! NH.0 M0.0 HH.0I MH.0I MN.0 M0.0I vN.0 «v.0 0m.0 MM NM.0 00.0I MH.0 0N.0 MH.0 0H.0 vH.0 vH.0 MH.0 NH.0 mm.0l 5M MM.0 MN.0I M0.0 HH.0 5M.0 Mv.0l M0.0I M0.0I MH.0 MH.0 MH.0 0M MM.0 5H.0 MM.0I N0.0I Hv.0 MH.0 H0.0I 0H.0I NH.0 0¢.0 M0.0I MM Hv.0 NH.0 No.0 0N.0 H0.0I 00.0I vH.0I 0M.0| Hv.0 M0.0I 0N.0 VM v¢.0 HH.01 MH.0I M0.0 0N.0I 00.0 0H.0I 0N.0 o0.0 MM.0 HH.0I MM mv.0 50.0 MM.0 0H.0 vH.0 HM.0 M0.0I No.0 vH.0 MH.0 HH.0 NM mv.0 0N.0I H0.0 N0.0I MH.0I H0.0 N0.0I MN.0 0N.0 0¢.0 NM.0 HM 0m.0 VH.0I MH.0I vN.0 HN.0 M0.0I MH.0 0N.0 HN.0I 00.0 0v.0l 0M NM.0 MH.0 H0.0 HN.0I 00.0I 0N.0I 0N.0 0H.0I NM.0I MM.0 HN.0I MN vm.0 MM.0 MH.0 00.0I M0.0I NN.0I 00.0 MM.0 00.0 vv.0 NN.0 MN 5m.0 50.0! 0N.0 50.0! oH.0! H0.0 NN.0 00.0I vN.0 M0.0 M¢.0I 5N H0.0 HN.0 00.0I vH.0 «H.0 5N.0I 5N.0I 00.0 0N.0 0N.0 HM.0 0N MM.0 No.0 MN.0I vH.0I H0.0 MH.0 NM.0 5H.0! «M.0 MH.0I HM.0 MN 05.0 M0.0 0N.0I 0H.0I 0N.0 N0.0I 00.0 00.0 oH.0 0N.0 MM.0I «N H5.0 N0.0I 00.0I HN.0I MH.0 00.0 ¢H.0 M0.0 ¢H.0 5H.0I ¢m.0 MN V5.0 H0.0I HH.0: 00.0I MH.0: MH.0 HH.0! MH.0 0H.0 oH.0 vw.0 NN 05.0 0N.0 MN.0I N0.0 00.0 NH.0 00.0 50.0I «H.0I MM.0 0N.0 HN 00.0 HN.0I 00.0I H0.0 00.0I vH.0I MH.0 M0.0I MN.0I MN.0 MM.0: 0N mmDHm> 0H m M 5 m m w M N H mHO#Omm :mmHm AconcHucouv xHuumE mcHoMOH Houomm “whommmfi cOHumucmHHo 259 NM.0 MH.0 No.0 NH.0I 00.0I N0.0I HH.01 H0.0I 00.0 0M.0 00.0 0H MM.0 M0.0I 5H.0I 00.0I 0H.0 No.0 ¢N.0I HH.0I 50.0I MH.0 oH.0 0H NM.0 MH.0 NH.0 MH.0 HH.0 0N.0 vN.0I 5H.0! 0H.0 MH.0I 0N.0 5H 00.0 NH.0I M0.0I v0.0 vH.0 ¢0.0I 5H.0I v0.0 N0.0I 00.0 MH.0! 0H H0.H 0H.0I M0.0 00.0 00.0I MH.0 00.0I NH.0I MH.0 0N.0! H0.0I MH M0.H H0.0 MH.0: No.0 MH.0 NM.0I 5H.0 0H.0I HH.0 HN.0 00.0I vH 00.H 00.0 H0.0 M0.0 5N.0 v0.0 HH.0 MN.0I 00.0I NH.0I 0N.0I MH 50.H 00.0I HM.0! 00.0 NH.0 0N.0 M0.0 MH.0I 00.0I 0N.0I MH.0I NH MH.H M0.0I vN.0I 00.0 M0.0I MH.0 NN.0I MN.0 vM.0 HH.0 00.0I HH 0N.H 0N.0 MN.0I M0.0 N0.0I M0.0I HM.0 MN.0I No.0 HH.0I 50.0 0H «N.H HM.0 0N.0 N0.0I H0.0I 00.0 HH.0I M0.0 v0.0l 0H.0I 0N.0I m 0M.H 0N.0I 00.0I NH.0I N0.0I 00.0: 00.0 MH.0I MN.0! H0.0I MN.0 M 0M.H 0H.0I 0N.0 N0.0I 00.0I NM.0 MH.0 MH.0: oH.0! «0.0! 0N.0I 5 Hv.H NH.0I 50.0 M0.0I HM.0 00.0 MH.0 0H.0I HH.0! HH.0 NH.0 m Nm.H HH.0I M0.0 5H.0: 50.0 0M.0! MN.0! N0.0I NN.0I 00.0: 00.0 m mm.H HH.0: 00.0 NH.0 0H.0 00.0 oH.0 vN.0I 00.0 M0.0I 0H.0 v vm.H 0H.0I 0M.0: 0H.01 00.0 vH.0 50.0 MM.0 MN.0I 0N.0I M0.0 M v0.M 50.0I H0.0 00.0I HM.0I vH.0I 50.0 00.0 MM.0I vH.0I 00.0I N 0H.v 00.0 H0.0I 5N.0 0N.0I 00.0 00.0: MH.0 MH.0I NH.0 vH.0 H mmsHm> 0N 0H 0H 5H 0H MH vH MH NH HH muouomm GmmHm AUchHucOUV xHHumE MGHGMOH Houomm ”whammmfi GOHHMHGOHHO 260 NN.0 00.0I H0.0 N0.0I oo.0 no.0 00.0I NH.0 N0.0I HH.0I oH.0 mm NM.0 50.0: 5N.0I v0.0: NN.0: M0.0I 00.0 00.0: M0.0 N0.0I mH.0 5M MM.0 NN.0 M0.0 MH.0: 00.0I v0.0 MH.0 00.0 0N.0I No.0: HH.0| 0M MM.0 v0.0: 00.0 HH.0: N0.0I MH.0 50.0 MH.0 0N.0 MM.0: No.0: mm -Hv.0 mM.oI M0.0: M0.0 v0.0 H0.0: No.0: MH.0 wo.o mo.0 MH.0: HM vv.0 vo.0| H0.01 HH.0 HH.0: M0.0I MH.0 50.0 mH.0| NN.0 HN.0: MM mv.0 «0.0 M0.0 00.0I NN.0: m0.o NN.0 MM.0 HN.0 H0.0 NN.0 NM NH.0 N0.0I 50.0: 0N.0I MH.0 oH.0: 0N.0: H0.0: H0.0: No.0 0H.01 HM Nm.0 NH.0: HN.0 5N.0 NH.0: HN.0: H0.0: 50.0 v0.0 MH.0: HH.0: om NM.0 MH.0 HH.0: HH.01 0H.01 MH.0: 00.0I 0N.0I HH.0 0H.01 5H.0 mN vm.0 5H.0: no.0 NH.0 no.01 N0.0I HM.0 No.01 00.0 NN.0 MH.0: MN 5m.0 HH.0: No.0 eN.0 HN.0 oo.0 H0.0: NH.0 0m.0| 5H.0I 5N.0 5N H0.0 m0.0 M0.0: NN.0 HH.0 0H.0| M0.0 «0.0 no.0 HN.0: 50.0: 0N MM.0 00.0: v0.0 M0.0: vH.0 «0.0: HN.0 HH.0 oH.0 5N.0 HH.0 MN oo.0 mN.o H0.0: MN.0 ¢0.0 M0.0 vN.0I HH.0 5N.0I 0N.0 MH.0: «N H5.0 oH.0 MN.0: MN.0 M0.0: 50.0 HH.0 00.0 NN.01 oo.0 HH.0 MN V5.0 m0.0 50.0: NH.0 5H.0| MH.0: oo.0: v0.0 oo.0 5N.0I H0.0: NN 05.0 50.0 mN.o M0.0I mH.0| no.0 «0.0: No.01 0N.0: 00.0 Nv.o HN 00.0 0H.0 HH.0 no.0 MM.0 HN.0: 0N.0 Hv.0 H0.0 VH.0| vH.0 0N mmsHm> 0N NH 0H 5H 0H mH «H MH NH HH muouomm cmMHm HomscHucoov xHHumE mcHUmoH HOHUMM u QHDmMOE COHHMHGOflHO 261 NM.0 H0.0 mo.o 50.0 MH.0: mo.o 00.0I 0H.o 0H.01 No.0 No.0 mH NM.0 MH.0: NH.0I No.0: MH.0: 0H.0u M0.0: MH.0 MN.0 50.0 00.0 MH NN.0 N0.0I no.0 No.0: NH.0 mH.0 5H.0 vH.o mo.0| .mo.o NN.0: 5H oN.0 0H.0| no.0: No.0: oo.0: NN.0: oH.0 No.0 No.0: N0.0I ov.0| mH H0.H no.0 m0.o| HH.0: NH.0I N0.0I NH.0I no.0 HH.0 mN.o m0.0| mH mo.H mo.o No.0: v0.0 oo.0 H0.0 MH.0: M0.0I vN.0| oH.0 v0.0 vH oo.H HH.0 00.0: 0H.0I oo.0 oH.0 H0.0 no.0: HM.0 No.0: NH.0 MH 50.H MH.0: 0N.0 mo.o 00.0 00.0: oH.0: 5H.0: MH.0: oo.0 No.0: NH mH.H H0.0 NH.0: oH.0 wo.o| MH.0 50.0: H0.0: No.0 5H.0: oo.0: HH 0N.H mo.on No.0: m0.o MH.0: M0.0 om.o 0N.0: 00.0: «0.0 0H.o 0H vN.H MH.0 H0.0: v0.0 5H.0: mo.o| oo.0: oH.0 M0.0I oo.0: v0.0 m om.H HN.0 No.0 50.0 HN.0: 0N.0 NN.0 mo.o NH.0: No.0 mo.ou m mm.H HH.0: mo.0| mo.o No.0 50.0 00.0 HH.0 H0.0: MN.0: HH.0 5 H¢.H 00.0: No.0 0M.0 No.0 oH.0 HM.0: «N.o MH.0: NH.0I 0H.01 m Nm.H mo.o H0.0 HH.0: m0.0 ¢0.0| H0.0 NH.0 MH.0: 0H.0 oH.0 m mm.H No.0 «0.0: HM.0: H0.0: H0.0: HH.0: 0H.o HH.0: No.0 mH.o v vm.H mo.ou H0.0 0N.0: H0.0 No.0 HN.0: v0.0 M0.0: 00.0 00.0 .m vo.m no.0: v0.0: HH.0 5N.0 NH.0 50.0: Ho.0| MN.0 NH.0 0N.0: N 0H.v NH.0 NH.0 No.0 50.0: N0.0I 5H.0: H0.0: HH.0 HH.0 00.0 H mosHm> om mN 0N 5N 0N mN wN MN NN HN mnouomm cmmHm HomscHucouv xHuumE mcHUMOH Houomm “muommma COHumucmHHo 262 MN.0 M0.0I MN.0I MH.0 0H.0 N0.0I M0.0 «0.0I H0.0 HN.0 v0.0 MM NM.0 MM.0 H0.0I M0.0I MH.0 v0.0l M0.0 H0.0! 00.0I 5N.0I H0.0I 5M MM.0 M0.0I 50.0I MH40I MH.0I M0.0 00.0I 50.0 00.0 MN.0I M0.0I MM MM.0 HN.0 M0.0 M0.0 50.0 0H.0I M0.0I M0.0 MH.0I M0.0 No.0 MM H¢.0 MH.0I M0.0 M0.0 0H.0 M0.0 NH.0 HH.0 H0.0I MN.0I 5M.0 «M vv.0 M0.0 NH.0 v0.0 M0.0 NM.0I HN.0 vH.0 H0.0I 50.0 0H.0I MM M¢.0 5H.0I M0.0 M0.0I vH.0I vH.0 M0.0 N0.0I M0.0I MH.0 H0.0! NM M¢.0 0H.0 MH.0 MH.0 MH.0I HH.0 N0.0I 5H.0I HN.0 M0.0 v0.0 HM NM.0 HH.0I 50.0 M0.0 MH.0I 0H.0 H0.0 M0.0 M0.0 HH.0 M0.0 0M NM.0 5H.0I VM.0 M0.0 M0.0 v0.0 v0.0 MN.0 vH.0 HH.0I 00.0 MN M¢.0 00.0I M0.0 0H.0I H0.0I M0.0 M0.0I HH.0: v0.0 M0.0 HH.0I MN 5M.0 M0.0 0H.0 M0.0 HN.0: 50.0: No.0 vH.0I 50.0I MN.0I HH.0I 5N HM.0 50.0 MH.0 v0.0 00.0I M0.0I MH.0: No.0 0H.0 v0.0! NH.0 MN MM.0 50.0 MH.0 MH.0! H0.0 MH.0 v0.0l M0.0 MN.0 «H.0I MH.0! MN MM.0 M0.0I No.0 MH.0: MH.0 0M.0 50.0 5H.0I 0N.0! H0.0I M0.0 «N H5.0 M0.0 MH.0I M0.0 H0.0 H0.0I 50.0 H¢.0 M0.0 M0.0 M0.0 MN v5.0 M0.0I MH.0I M0.0I. vH.0I ¢0.0 5H.0! M0.0 M0.0I MN.0I NN.0! NN M5.0 50.0! M0.0I v0.0 M0.0 MH.0: 5H.0I MN.0I MH.0 MH.0I NH.0 HN 0M.0 M0.0I M0.0I H0.0 NH.0 v0.0a MH.0 N0.0I HH.0 v0.0 00.0 ON mmaHm> 0M MN MN 5N MN MN wN MN NN HN muouomm cOMHm ApmssHucoov xHHumE MCHUmOH Houomm "mnnmmwfi GOHumucmHHo 263 NM.0 M0.0 0H.0I M0.0 MM.0I M0.0 M0.0 5H.0 50.0 MH MM.0 M0.0 0H.0I HH.0 M0.0 M0.0I MH.0 NH.0I M0.0I MH NM.0 M0.0I M0.0 M0.0 0H.0 M0.0 0H.0 5H.0 0H.0 5H MM.0 v0.0l v0.0! N0.0I M0.0I MH.0I NN.0I M0.0 H0.01 MH H0.H M0.0I MN.0 H0.0I MH.0I NH.0 M0.0I 0H.0I HH.0 MH M0.H MH.0I 50.0 MH.0: H0.0 v0.0! MH.0 No.0 MH.0 vH M0.H M0.0 00.0I HH.0: 50.0I MH.0 No.0 M0.0I «0.0 MH 50.H M0.0 50.0 MH.0 50.0: HH.0 No.0 M0.0 v0.0l NH MH.H M0.0 HH.0 N0.0I NH.0 No.0 MH.0 vH.0 MH.0I HH 0N.H H0.0 H0.0I 50.0 5H.0 M0.0I M0.0I NH.0 NH.0 0H @N.H No.0 M0.0 MH.0 NH.0 H0.0I M0.0I No.0 MH.0I M 0M.H M0.0 M0.0 M0.0 M0.0 N0.0I 50.0 NH.0I v0.0: M MM.H H0.0 H0.0 M0.0 50.0! MN.0I H0.0 H0.0I MN.0 5 Hv.H v0.0 M0.0 M0.0 NH.0 M0.0 5H.0! M0.0I H0.0I M NM.H 50.0 M0.0 M0.0 M0.0 N0.0 0H.0 5H.0 v0.0! M MM.H M0.0I M0.0I H0.0! MH.0 NH.0I v0.0l H0.0 5H.0 v vM.H 00.0I H0.0 M0.0I M0.0 H0.0I 50.0I 0H.0 MH.0 M «0.M v0.0: v0.01 H0.0! M0.0I 0H.0I MH.0 50.0 M0.0I N MH.v No.0 M0.0I MH.0 HH.0 No.0 No.0 50.0! 0N.0 H mmDHm> MM 5M MM MM MM MM NM HM muouomm GOMHM mescHucouv xHHumE MGHUmoH Houomm umHHHmMOE COHn—anCOHHO 264 MN.0 MN.0 HH.0: NH.0I M0.0 M0.0 MH.0: M0.0: H0.0 MM NM.0 50.0 M0.0 M0.0 M0.0I M0.0I MN.0: H0.0 50.0: 5M MM.0 H0.0: M0.0 50.0: M0.0 oH.0 No.0 M0.0 50.01 MM 0M.0 NH.0I MN.0: N0.0I M0.0 M0.0 M0.0 v0.01 M0.0 MM HM.0 M0.0I M0.0 M0.0 M0.0 MH.0 00.0I v0.0: N0.0I «M vv.0 v0.0 5H.0 NH.0I MH.0 HH.0 M0.0 HH.0: M0.0 MM MM.0 «0.0: HH.0 M0.0I N0.0I 0H.0I H0.0: oH.0: HH.0: NM Mv.0 MN.0: M0.0 No.0 v0.0 HH.0: 5H.0: N0.0I v0.0 HM NM.0 M0.0 No.0: 0H.0I v0.0 M0.0 5H.0: MN.0 H0.0 0M NM.0 No.0 M0.0I M0.0I H0.0: M0.0 M0.0: vH.0I M0.0: MN vM.o v0.0: MH.0: MH.0 H0.01 vH.0 M0.0I H0.0 HM.0: MN 5M.0 v0.01 M0.0I 5H.0: ¢0.0| 50.0 NH.0 H0.0 M0.0: 5N HM.0 MH.0 M0.0 50.0: oH.0: MN.0I HH.0 M0.0 NH.01 MN MM.0 NH.0 N0.0I No.0: M0.0 No.0 H0.01 NH.0 MN.0 MN MM.0 No.0 00.0I M0.0: No.0: M0.0I M0.0I MH.0: 00.0 vN H5.0 5H.0: No.0: 50.0: MH.0: No.0 0H.0I HH.0 50.0: MN v5.0 H0.0: 00.0 M0.0 H0.0: M0.0 N0.0I MH.0I HH.0 NN M5.0 50.0: MH.0 M0.0: No.0: M0.0 50.0: 0H.o H0.0: HN 0M.0 M0.0I oH.0 vN.0 MH.0: No.0 No.0 N0.0I M0.0I 0N mmsHm> MM 5M MM MM «M MM NM HM muouomm cmmHm AcmscHucouv xHHumE MCHMMOH Houomm "whammme GOHumucmHHo APPENDIX I REFERENCE MEASURE: ROTATED FACTOR LOADINGS 265 266 HH.0 oH.o- oo.o MH.0- -.o .wm.o 5 mm.o mH.o- mm.o oo.0 «mm.o- HH.o m nm.o HH.0- .mm.o 5H.o- Hm.o- OH.o m mm.o mo.o mo.o .mh.o- m~.o- 5o.o- e ov.o mo.o mo.o- oo.0- No.0 .5G.o m Hm.o oo.ou no.o- HN.0- «V5.o- HH.ou N Hm.o MN.0- oo.0 .mm.o- No.0 om.o H mmHuHHmcsasoo > >H HHH HH H muouomm mm.o m5.o «5.0- v5.o- hm.o mmmwmmmm no.o NH.o OH.o OH.o MH.0 wmmmwmmwowm mozHo<0H moaomm omamaom “mmammmz mozmmmmmm 267 .mchmoH smHm. om.o «mm.o 5o.oa mo.o «0.0 vo.on GH mm.o mm.o mm.o mm.ou Ho.o .H¢.o MH m¢.o 0N.0 HN.0 .me.on mo.ou H¢.o «H om.o OH.o HN.0 MH.0 mm.ou .mm.o mH MM.0 mo.on «0.0 mm.ou 5H.0- .mm.o NH NM.0 No.0 MH.o HH.o- .H5.ou MN.0 HH me.o No.0- HN.0 mo.o MH.0: .em.o OH Hm.o NH.0 «mo.o MN.0 H~.ou MN.0 m mm.o Ho.o .m5.o mm.o- HN.o Ho.on m mmHuHHmcsesoo > >H HHH HH mnouomm AcoscHucouv mmcHMmoH Houomm Umumuon “QHDmMOE OOCOHOMOM APPENDIX J ORIENTATION MEASURE: ROTATED FACTOR LOADINGS 268 269 N¢.o mo.o nn.0 .ov.o NH.0- 5o.ou 50.0 mm.o NH em.o 0H.o- mo.o OH.ou .oe.o MH.0- HH.0 «0.0- HH nn.0 mo.o no.0 mo.o- oo.0 «O.O- Ho.o- .oo.o- OH nH.0 mm.o- Ho.ou .Hm.o H0.0- oo.o, HH.0- mH.o- a mm.o mo.o 5o.o- HH.o «o.ou 5m.o- mo.o .me.o m mm.o mo.o- HH.o HH.o He.o mo.ou .me.o mc.o 5 me.o ho.o NN.0 .me.ou mH.o mo.o HN.0- MH.0- m mH.o “O.O .oe.o oo.o mo.o Ho.o oo.0 mo.o- m HH.0 mH.o- mo.o no.0 mo.o- .He.o- HH.0 on.0- v m~.o .ov.ou HN.0- 5H.o- oH.0 Ho.o- HH.o oo.0- m mm.o mo.o- mo.o HH.0 40.0- mo.o- oH.0- .mm.o- m mm.o 5N.0 mo.o- mo.on mo.cu .Hm.o- No.0- HH.o H mmHuHHmasaeoo .HH> H> > >H HHH HH H umnssz EmuH oo.0 mm.o HM.0 em.o MM.0- 5M.0 oo.0- mmmwmmmm «O.O vo.o mo.o no.0 mo.o 5o.o moaMHum> mo 50.0 mcoHuuomoum MOZHQNOH moeudm QMB H> > >H HHH HH H Hmnasz smuH HomscHucoov mmchmoH Houomm Mmpmuou "whammmfi GOHumucmHHO 271 .mcHOmoq anm. 0M.0 MH.0 00.0I Hv.0 HH.0I MH.0I «MM.0 M0.0 MM Nv.0 N0.0 NN.0 50.0I «Nv.0 MN.0 0H.0I MM.0I 5M NM.0 «0M.0 M0.0I M0.0I H0.0 HH.01 0N.0 H0.0 MM MM.0 MN.0 ¥0M.0 N0.0I MH.0 0N.0I 5H.0 MH.0! MM 0M.0 MN.0 N0.0 MH.0 «Mv.0l 5H.0I M0.0 MN.0I VM 5M.0 NN.0I 5H.0 MH.0I 0N.0 HH.0! *HM.0 NN.0I MM MM.0 NH.0I «MM.0 50.0 HH.0I 00.0I M0.0 N0.0 NM MM.0 H0.0 MH.0 M0.0 M0.0I 00.0I *5M.0 N0.0I HM MM.0 H0.0 M0.0 MH.0I «vM.0 MH.0 MH.0! v0.0l 0M wv.0 v0.0l 5H.0I MH.0 NHM.0 MM.0! 50.0 50.0I MN Nv.0 NH.0 M0.0I M0.0I MH.0 M0.0 «HM.0 M0.0 MN mmHuHHmcsaeoo HH> H> > >H HHH HH H Hmnesz smuH HomscHucoov mmchmoH Houomm Mmumuou "augmmme coHumucmHuo APPENDIX K ORIENTATION MEASURE: ITEM MEANS AND STANDARD DEVIATIONS 272 273 ORIENTATION MEASURE: ITEM MEANS AND STANDARD DEVIATIONS A f Item No. Sample Mean S.D. 1 Student Sample 2.81 1.65 Broadcaster Sample 1.98 1.29 2 Student Sample 2.50 1.32 Broadcaster Sample 2.60 1.90 3 Student Sample 3.78 1.97 Broadcaster Sample 4.28 2.10 4 Student Sample 3.83 1.95 Broadcaster Sample 3.50 2.15 5 Student Sample 3.50 1.85 Broadcaster Sample 3.81 1.99 6 Studen Sample 4.19 1.94 Broadcaster Sample 5.28 1.71 7 Student Sample 4.31 1.97 Broadcaster Sample 4.15 1.89 8 Student Sample 3.36 1.84 Broadcaster Sample 3.02 2.02 9 Student Sample 3.89 2.01 Broadcaster Sample 5.67 1.41 10 Student Sample 3.50 1.76 Broadcaster Sample 4.17 2.11 11 Student Sample 3.44 1.69 Broadcaster Sample 3.77 1.99 12 Student Sample 3.61 2.00 Broadcaster Sample 3.22 1.73 13 Student Sample 2.75 1.64 Broadcaster Sample 2.89 1.79 14 Student Sample 4.00 1.90 Broadcaster Sample 3.94 2.04 15 Student Sample 4.19 1.84 Broadcaster Sample 4.96 1.87 Orientation measure: 274 item means and standard deviation (Continued) Item No. Sample Mean S.D. 16 Student Sample 4.28 1.94 Broadcaster Sample 3.58 2.13 17 Student Sample 2.42 1.77 Broadcaster Sample 2.45 1.37 18 Student Sample 4.92 1.77 Broadcaster Sample 5.92 1.19 19 Student Sample 3.97 1.77 Broadcaster Sample 3.94 1.96 20 Student Sample 3.86 1.83 Broadcaster Sample 5.04 1.76 21 Student Sample 4.78 1.72 Broadcaster Sample 3.57 1.48 22 Student Sample 2.61 1.59 Broadcaster Sample 2.78 1.64 23 Student Sample 3.86 1.62 Broadcaster Sample 3.43 1.81 24 Student Sample 4.36 1.84 Broadcaster Sample 5.28 1.61 25 Student Sample 3.81 2.01 Broadcaster Sample 3.51 1.74 26 Student Sample 2.50 1.57 Broadcaster Sample 2.10 1.31 27 Student Sample 4.94 1.72 Broadcaster Sample 5.58 1.57 28 Student Sample 3.53 1.85 Broadcaster Sample 3.53 1.83 29 Student Sample 3.78 2.00 Broadcaster's Sample 3.94 2.23 Orientation measure: 275 item means and standard deviation (Continued) Item No. Sample Mean S.D. 30 Student Sample 3.75 1.69 Broadcaster Sample 5.02 1.57 31 Student Sample 2.94 1.65 Broadcaster Sample 3.20 1.65 32 Student Sample 3.28 1.84 Broadcaster Sample 3.14 1.87 33 Student Sample 4.64 1.78 Broadcaster Sample 4.64 1.77 34 Student Sample 3.00 1.75 Broadcaster Sample 2.92 1.74 35 Student Sample 3.00 1.60 Broadcaster Sample 3.04 1.86 36 Student Sample 2.58 1.59 Broadcaster Sample 3.29 1.78 37 Student Sample 3.58 1.67 Broadcaster Sample 5.12 1.56 38 Student Sample 3.81 1.82 Broadcaster Sample 3.35 1.62 Student Sample (N=38) consists of advanced under- graduate and graduate students in courses in broadcast management at Michigan State University who were included in second pre-test of instrument in the fall of 1966. Broadcaster Sample (N=247) consists of respondents to national study in the Spring of 1967. APPENDIX L ORIENTATION MEASURE: 276 ITEM ANALYSIS 1i 277 ORIENTATION MEASURE: KEY 1 ITEM ANALYSIS Code Variable Identification Category Identification S/P ED EXP M/S S/T System Position Level of Education Level of Experience Market Size Station Type 0) 1) 2) 3) 4) 1) 2) 3) "Other" Position2 Manager or Gen'l Manager Sales Manager Program Director News Director Less than College Education Less than Four Years of College Four or More Years of College 10 Years or Less 11 - 20 Years 21 Years or More Less than 100,000 Population 100,000 - 500,000 Population More than 500,000 Population Radio Station Television Station Radio-TV Station 1 differences are reported. 2 Owners and related positions. Only those variables and items showing significant "Other" positions include Corporate Officers, Station 278 F-Statisticl Item No. Variable Categories Mean S.D. Sig. 1 S/P 0 2.29 2.14 3.74 .006 1 1.62 .79 2 2.29 1.43 3 2.00 1.21 4 2.36 1.74 M/S 1 1.70 1.14 7.48 .001 2 1.80 .94 3 2.40 1.60 2 M/S 1 3.22 2.19 6.36 .002 2 2.49 1.88 3 2.18 1.52 S/T 1 3.17 2.17 12.57 <.0005 2 1.90 1.31 3 2.18 1.45 3 S/T l 3.82 2.04 6.98 .001 2 4.54 2.15 3 5.00 2.00 4 S/P 0 4.00 2.38 3.35 .010 1 2.98 1.96 2 3.47 2.23 3 3.68 2.21 4 4.34 2.18 5 NO SIGNIFICANT DIFFERENCES OBSERVED 6 M/S 1 5.38 1.78 3.84 .023 2 5.58 1.63 3 4.89 1.68 7 S/P 0 4.29 1.70 2.98 .020 1 3.99 1.85 2 4.65 1.82 3 3.57 1.76 4 4.59 2.11 M/S l 3.08 1.73 19.19 <.0005 2 4.53 1.90 3 4.67 1.67 S/T l 3.58 1.87 12.53 <.0005 2 4.57 1.80 3 4.89 1.72 279 S.D. F.Statistic1 Item No. Variable Categories Mean Sig. 8 ED 1 2.56 1.74 3.64 .028 2 2.71 2.08 3 3.33 2.05 EXP l 3.16 2.16 4.47 .012 2 3.26 2.04 3 2.35 1.70 9 NO SIGNIFICANT DIFFERENCES OBSERVED 10 ED 1 4.76 2.12 4.21 .016 2 4.41 2.15 3 3.83 2.05 EXP l 4.50 2.19 4.69 .010 2 3.77 2.11 3 4.65 1.92 M/S 1 4.64 2.16 4.61 .011 2 4.28 2.08 3 3.66 2.03 11 M/S 1 2.85 1.64 10.84 <.0005 2 3.60 2.07 3 4.71 1.78 S/T 1 3.01 1.67 22.78 <.0005 2 4.78 2.01 3 4.24 1.96 12 ED 1 2.70 1.61 3.20 .043 2 3.49 1.80 3 3.29 1.72 13 NO SIGNIFICANT DIFFERENCES OBSERVED 14 NO SIGNIFICANT DIFFERENCES OBSERVED 15 S/P 0 3.71 1.98 2.40 .050 1 4.78 1.92 2 4.94 1.98 3 4.91 1.89 4 5.61 1.48 16 ED 1 3.20 2.23 3.14 .045 2 3.22 2.12 3 3.89 2.06 280 Item No. Variable Categories Mean S.D. F-Statistic1 Sig. M/S l 3.04 2.15 4.70 .010 2 3.56 2.20 3 4.06 1.94 17 NO SIGNIFICANT DIFFERENCES OBSERVED 18 NO SIGNIFICANT DIFFERENCES OBSERVED 19 NO SIGNIFICANT DIFFERENCES OBSERVED 20 S/P 0 5.00 1.53 2.78 .030 l 4.83 1.77 2 4.82 1.68 3 4.98 1.92 4 5.82 1.50 S/T l 4.71 1.80 4.53 .012 2 5.37 1.77 3 5.38 1.53 21 EXP 1 3.55 1.44 3.10 .047 2 3.76 1.51 3 3.18 1.43 M/S l 3.12 1.43 4.93 .008 2 3.72 1.48 3 3.79 1.47 22 EXP 1 2.69 1.58 3.32 .038 2 3.02 1.74 3 2.37 1.43 23 S/P 0 4.29 2.06 3.14 .020 1 3.28 1.87 2 3.41 1.62 3 4.06 1.90 4 2.91 1.60 24 NO SIGNIFICANT DIFFERENCE OBSERVED 25 ED 1 3.92 2.01 3.23 .041 2 3.71 1.75 3 3.26 1.60 EXP 1 3.44 1.80 3.34 .037 2 3.76 1.78 3 3.07 1.54 281 F-Statisticl Item No. Variable Categories Mean S.D. Sig. 26 S/P O 3.00 1.41 3.41 .010 1 1.88 1.12 2 2.55 1.70 3 1.89 1.03 4 2.18 1.39 EXP l 2.35 1.37 3.93 .021 2 2.16 1.47 3 1.72 .74 27 EXP 1 5.60 1.75 2.99 .050 2 5.38 1.66 3 5.98 1.07 28 S/P 0 3.14 1.86 2.96 .020 1 3.38 1.78 2 3.98 2.02 3 3.02 1.60 4 4.05 1.82 M/S 1 3.04 1.77 4.02 .019 2 3.80 1.81 3 3.69 1.84 S/T 1 3.04 1.75 9.89 <.0005 2 4.12 1.81 3 3.93 1.76 29 EXP l 3.37 2.19 3.68 .027 2 3.98 2.24 3 4.45 2.16 M/S l 3.36 2.19 5.95 .003 2 3.83 2.25 3 4.54 2.12 S/T 1 3.52 2.20 4.47 .012 2 4.31 2.22 3 4.42 2.18 30 S/P 0 3.71 1.89 4.37 .002 1 5.41 1.45 2 4.53 1.65 3 5.13 1:45 4 4.82 1.59 282 Item No. Variable Categories Mean S.D. F. Statistic1 Sig. ED 1 4.54 1.91 3.26 .040 2 5.25 1.34 3 5.10 1.50 EXP 1 4.74 1.65 3.82 .023 2 4.94 1.57 3 5.48 1.40 31 EXP 1 3.35 1.52 3.21 .042 2 3.34 1.76 3 2.73 1.46 32 NO SIGNIFICANT DIFFERENCES OBSERVED 33 M/S 1 4.14 1.97 4.40 .013 2 4.84 1.65 3 4.87 1.64 S/T 1 4.25 1.84 6.27 .002 2 5.01 1.59 3 5.05 1.67 34 M/S 1 3.36 1.95 3.58 .029 2 2.80 1.73 3 2.67 1.52 35 S/P 0 3.71 2.29 2.49 .040 1 2.98 1.78 2 3.69 2.15 3 2.74 1.65 4 2.73 1.73 S/T 1 3.52 2.20 4.47 .012 2 4.31 2.22 3 4.42 2.18 30 S/P 0 3.71 1.89 4.37 .002 1 5.41 1.45 2 4.53 1.65 3 5.13 1.45 4 4.82 1.59 ED 1 4.54 1.91 3.26 .040 2 5.25 1.34 3 5.10 1.50 283 Item No. Variable Categories Mean S.D. F—Statistic1 Sig. EXP 1 4.74 1.65 3.82 .023 2 4.94 1.57 3 5.48 1.40 31 EXP 1 3.35 1.52 3.21 .042 2 3.34 1.76 3 2.73 1.46 32 NO SIGNIFICANT DIFFERENCES OBSERVED 33 M/S l 4.14 1.97 4.40 .013 2 4.84 1.65 3 4.87 1.64 S/T 1 4.25 1.84 6.27 .002 2 5.01 1.59 3 5.05 1.67 34 M/S l 3.36 1.95 3.58 .029 2 2.80 1.73 3 2.67 1.52 35 S/P 0 3.71 2.29 2.49 .040 1 2.98 1.78 2 3.69 2.15 3 2.74 1.65 4 2.73 1.73 36 NO SIGNIFICANT DIFFERENCES OBSERVED 37 EXP l 4.71 1.61 4.97 .008 2 5.10 1.57 3 5.58 1.36 S/T 1 5.35 1.51 2.99 .050 2 4.84 1.49 3 4.93 1.69 38 NO SIGNIFICANT DIFFERENCES OBSERVED 1Analysis of variance. APPENDIX M REFERENCE MEASURE: FACTOR SCORE COEFFICIENTS 284 285 MM.0 MM.0 MM.0I M5.0I MM.0 55m 0M.0 M0.0I H0.0! N0.0 M0.0I MH MH.0 M0.0 MH.0! M0.0 HH.0 MH 50.0 M0.0 MH.0! H0.0 M0.0 vH N0.0 M0.0 MH.0 MH.0I vN.0 MH N0.0I M0.0I NH.0I M0.0I NN.0 NH H0.0 H0.0 H0.0 5M.0! v0.0 HH N0.0I N0.0 M0.0 N0.0I 0N.0 0H «0.0 0M.0 HN.0 50.0! v0.0 M N0.0 MM.0 MH.0I HN.0 NH.0I M M0.0I v0.0! v0.0! NH.0 MH.0 5 M0.0I M0.0 M0.0 0H.0I H0.0I M «0.0! MN.0 v0.0l M0.0I M0.0I M v0.0 00.0I MM.0! M0.0I NH.0I w H0.0 50.0I N0.0 M0.0 NN.0 M N0.0 50.0I 0H.01 Nv.0l MH.0I N 50.0I M0.0I MN.0I M0.0 50.0 H > >H HHH HH H Houomm MBZmHUHmmmOU mmoum m090 H> > >H HHH HH H uouomm mazmHonmmoo mmoom moaoOm ”mammamz oneOazmHmo 288 HO.O ON.H OO.O O5.O OO.H- 5H.H OO.H- sum 5O.O OO.O- HN.O OO.O- OO.O- NN.O OO.O- OO OO.O OH.O HO.O HH.O OH.O HO.O- OO.O- 5O ON.O OO.O- OO.O- OO.O OO.O- OO.O OO.O- OO OH.O ON.O HO.O- OO.O OO.O: HO.O NO.O- Om OO.O HO.O OO.O 5H.O- 5O.O- HO.O- OH.O- Om NH.O- OO.O OO.O- OO.O HO.O- OH.O OOOO- Om OO.O- ON.O NO.O OO.O- NO.O OO.O- HO.O NO OO.O- OO.O OO.O OO.O- OO.O NN.O OO.O- HO OO.O OO.O OO.O- 5H.O OO.O OO.O- OO.O OO OO.O- HH.O- OO.O OH.O OH.O- HO.O OO.O ON OO.O OO.O- OO.O- 5O.O OO.O OH.O OO.O ON NO.O- NO.O- OO.O OO.O OO.O OO.O- HH.O- 5N HN.O NO.O- NH.O- OH.O- HO.O- OH.O NO.O- ON OO.O 5O.O ON.O 5O.O- OO.O NO.O- OO.O- ON OO.O 5O.O HO.O OO.O NO.O OO.O OO.O- ON OO.O OO.O OO.O 5O.O- OO.O HO.O OO.O ON OO.O- OO.O OO.O- OO.O- NO.O OH.O NO.O NN OO.O- 5O.O OO.O OO.O 5O.O- OO.O OO.O HN HO.O OO.O- HO.O OH.O OO.O- OO.O OO.O- ON HH> H> > >H HHH HH H Houomm HomscHucouv mucmHOHmwwoo OHOQW Honomm u OHHHMMOE COHHMUPHOHHHO APPENDIX 0 REFERENCE MEASURE: ROSTER OF FACTOR SCORES 289 290 REFERENCE MEASURE: ROSTER OF FACTOR SCORES Factor Subject Number I II III IV V 001 -0.12 0.96 0.41 0.48 -0.24 002 -1.35 -0.40 —0.18 -0.91 -O.13 003 1.41 1.37 0.46 0.94 -0.54 004 0.23 -1.18 1.23 -0.94 -0.21 005 -1.48 -0.25 0.00 0.22 -0.07 006 -0.53 -0.13 -0.12 0.52 -0.02 007 0.52 -0.85 -0.25 0.78 -0.38 008 -0.60 -0.87 1.33 0.79 -0.23 009 -0.07 —0.15 0.61 -0.20 —0.38 010 0.13 —0.49 -0.58 -1.41 -0.45 011 -1.32 0.09 -0.06 -1.29 —0.12 012 -0.14 -0.25 1.13 0.26 -0.24 013 -0.09 -0.35 0.89 0.05 -0.29 014 -0.49 0.90 -0.65 0.25 -0.54 015 -0.37 -1.08 1.04 1.08 -0.17 016 -0.27 0.76 —0.84 0.02 -0.33 017 0.91 -0.55 -0.98 0.23 -0.19 018 1.83 0.66 -0.06 0.87 -0.06 019 0.05 —0.47 -0.42 0.08 -0.23 020 —0.22 0.13 1.26 -0.03 -0.06 021 -1.18 0.38 0.34 0.22 -0.18 022 0.27 -0.54 0.09 0.39 0.19 023 0.04 —0.41 -0.34 0.09 —0.17 024 0.73 0.73 0.54 0.39 -0.22 025 0.29 —0.32 1.63 0.63 -0.17 026 -0.97 1.70 0.14 0.13 -0.28 027 0.43 -0.98 0.28 -0.93 -0.44 028 1.63 0.89 0.66 -0.61 -0.20 291 Reference measure: roster of factor scores (Continued) Factor Subject Number I II III IV V 029 -0.27 -1.00 1.71 1.21 -0.21 030 0.38 0.05 -0.72 -0.69 -0.28 031 0.91 0.37 -0.55 -0.84 -0.40 032 -0.62 0.41 0.72 -0.47 -0.14 033 1.02 0.22 0.94 0.47 -0.02 034 -0.35 -0.33 0.91 0.46 -0.04 035 0.25 -0.82 -0.35 0.76 -0.36 036 -0.74 -0.13 -0.28 -0.03 -0.34 037 1.09 -0.12 -1.14 0.30 0.00 038 -0.75 0.34 0.10 -1.45 -0.26 039 -0.06 —0.74 0.23 0.53 -0.38 040 1.19 -0.70 -0.17 0.36 —0.02 041 -1.03 —0.40 -0.25 0.17 —0.42 042 —0.09 0.13 —0.24 0.07 -0.49 043 1.75 20.56 -1.09 0.26 —0.11 044 -0.54 -0.26 0.33 -1.47 -0.16 045 0.71 0.85 -0.41 0.36 -0.07 046 -0.58 0.26 —0.23 -l.58 -0.35 047 0.17 0.07 -0.77 -0.52 -0.14 048 -1.15 0.23 0.05 -0.02 -0.13 049 0.79 -0.64 -0.71 0.03 -0.22 050 -0.79 0.25 0.18 -1.33 -0.30 051 -2.99 1.07 0.29 -0.74 -0.22 052 1.88 -0.70 -1.15 0.43 0.17 053 -1.05 -0.03 0.11 0.54 -0.27 054 -1.13 0.25 -1.21 -0.03 -0.42 055 -0.01 -0.63 -0.50 -0.79 -0.34 056 -O.59 0.28 0.82 -0.99 -0.27 057 —0.69 -0.22 0.10 0.45 -0.23 292 Reference measure: roster of factor scores (Continued) Factor Subject Number I II III IV V 058 —0.48 -0.79 0.33 0.09 0.02 059 -0.29 —0.81 0.67 1.04 -0.31 062 0.06 0.12 0.78 0.28 -0.16 063 0.02 0.02 —1.23 0.62 0.32 064 -0.43 0.40 1.02 -0.78 -0.18 065 -1.28 -0.39 -l.08 —0.42 -0.24 066 -0.03 0.79 0.10 0.38 0.17 067 -0.18 —0.59 -0.65 0.17 -0.19 068 -0.63 -0.25 0.87 -1.24 -0.18 069 0.64 -0.52 -1.08 0.21 -0.17 070 -0.79 -0.27 0.41 -0.32 -0.29 071 -0.32 -0.91 0.03 1.09 0.08 072 -1.13 -0.43 0.26 0.26 -0.04 073 -1.32 -0.00 0.01 0.51 -0.24 074 0.58 -1.05 1.13 0.94 0.05 075 0.40 -1.29 1.01 -0.49 -0.20 076 —1.09 -1.72 -0.15 -3.48 —0.92 077 -0.15 -0.67 -0.17 0.42 -0.25 078 0.10 1.08 0.16 0.18 -0.49 079 0.99 1.14 0.96 -0.12 -0.33 080 -1.28 0.78 -0.59 0.25 -0.26 081 1.24 -0.87 0.37 1.04 0.06 082 -0.24 0.87 -1.09 0.15 -0.31 083 1.13 0.56 0.19 -0.87 0.03 084 -0.42 1.02 0.88 0.48 -0.12 085 -0.26 -0.17 -0.36 0.40 -0.29 086 0.31 1.29 0.39 -0.48 -0.43 087 -0.74 0.49 -0.13 0.26 -0.16 Reference measure: 293 roster of factor scores (Continued) Factor Subject Number I II III IV V 088 0.62 -0.48 0.68 0.15 -0.18 089 -0.42 -0.72 —0.79 0.16 -0.02 090 -0.92 0.42 -0.24 -1.21 -0.23 091 0.57 -0.79 0.26 -1.53 -0.34 092 0.26 1.89 -0.18 -0.26 -0.29 093 0.46 -0.70 -0.10 -0.10 -0.33 094 -0.11 —0.08 -0.58 0.47 -0.07 095 -0.64 0.39 -0.13 -0.18 -0.26 096 —1.05 0.27 -l.05 -0.53 -0.23 097 1.45 0.11 -0.32 0.31 -0.05 098 0.79 -0.70 0.45 -0.01 -0.44 099 0.31 0.11 0.57 -0.03 -0.02 100 —0.34 0.47 -0.96 -1.06 -0.28 101 0.76 0.28 -1.29 0.30 -0.09 102 0.96 -O.55 -0.85 0.11 0.24 103 0.68 -0.34 0.33 0.59 0.01 104 -0.38 0.07 -1.35 0.21 -0.06 105 1.25 0.98 -0.82 -0.25 -0.13 106 -0.20 0.16 -0.62 0.33 —0.16 107 1.60 -0.56 -0.07 -0.76 -0.11 108 0.70 1.22 0.35 0.21 2.94 109 0.75 -0.08 0.40 0.94 0.15 110 0.75 0.05 —l.39 0.26 -0.03 111 0.37 0.28 0.60 0.20 -0.34 112 —0.13 -0.41 0.41 0.96 0.06 113 0.97 1.91 0.16 0.14 -0.22 114 -0.02 1.37 -0.06 -0.21 -0.07 115 0.12 0.31 -0.71 -0.66 -0.12 116 0.34 0.19 -0.09 -0.31 2.92 294 Factor Subject Number I II III IV V 117 -0.15 -1.04 -0.46 1.13 0.18 118 -0.69 0.95 0.03 -0.14 -0.25 119 0.82 -0.49 -1.66 0.63 0.33 120 —0.12 -0.17 0.38 0.06 -0.12 121 0.10 —0.68 -1.04 0.39 0.21 122 -0.97 0.63 -0.71 -0.15 0.31 123 1.10 -1.07 -0.60 1.24 0.57 124 -0.16 -0.65 -O.82 0.00 0.01 125 -0.64 -0.23 -0.48 -0.73 0.06 126 0.65 -0.25 0.52 0.06 -0.24 127 0.35 —1.19 0.13 -0.53 -0.09 128 -0.07 0.01 -0.67 -0.15 -0.31 129 1.00 -0.55 -0.51 0.64 -0.10 130 0.93 0.00 0.31 -1.20 -0.19 131 0.42 0.10 0.09 0.02 -0.08 132 -0.48 0.20 -0.37 -0.11 -0.21 133 0.16 -0.63 0.52 -0.60 -0.28 134 -O.l9 0.80 -0.25 0.46 0.05 135 -0.03 1.41 -1.12 -0.48 -0.12 136 —1.81 -1.63 1.86 1.33 -0.15 137 —0.36 0.39 1.18 -1.07 4.46 138 0.06 1.26 1.74 1.52 -0.05 139 -0.59 —0.04 -0.41 —O.38 -0.34 140 1.07 -1.22 -0.24 0.99 0.18 141 0.19 -O.14 0.24 0.42 -0.38 142 —0.47 0.18 1.08 0.51 -0.37 143 0.36 -0.14 -0.79 0.55 0.04 144 -0.29 -1.00 -0.66 0.09 -0.31 145 0.45 0.35 0.87 0.00 4.46 146 -0.21 0.17 -0.35 0.47 -0.16 295 Reference measure: roster of factor scores (Continued) Factor Subject Number I II III IV V 147 -1.92 1.73 1.32 -0.73 -0.82 148 0.67 1.64 0.10 -1.18 —0.23 149 -0.90 -0.10 0.29 0.23 -0.26 150 -0.59 —0.99 1.38 -0.44 -0.15 151 -0.39 -0.32 0.18 —1.28 0.00 152 0.12 0.17 -0.75 -0.34 0.30 153 -0.21 -0.24 -0.40 0.32 -0.11 154 0.81 -O.22 1.17 1.24 0.46 155 0.24 -0.80 -0.12 0.53 —0.09 156 —0.17 -0.69 0.07 0.69 0.04 157 0.07 0.77 1.23 0.77 -0.00 158 -0.69 -0.56 0.09 0.32 -0.03 159 -0.51 0.20 -0.77 0.17 0.09 160 0.21 0.48 —1.26 -0.07 -0.14 161 0.16 1.46 -0.50 -0.05 0.03 162 0.75 0.74 0.04 0.81 -0.34 163 -0.94 0.01 -0.65 0.04 -0.24 164 -0.87 0.74 —0.26 0.86 0.22 165 0.85 -0.97 0.10 0.84 -0.09 166 0.34 1.03 -0.11 -0.26 -0.14 167 -0.79 0.10 -0.87 0.11 -0.03 168 —0.27 0.26 -0.48 -0.33 -0.19 169 0.64 -0.02 -0.76 0.46 0.07 170 -0.24 0.41 -0.88 -0.73 -0.24 171 -0.19 0.13 -0.05 -0.32 0.02 172 -0.24 0.20 -0.29 -0.02 -0.45 173 0.23 —0.34 0.21 0.37 -0.10 174 -1.18 -0.59 0.45 0.56 -0.59 175 0.58 —0.81 0.63 0.86 -0.34 Reference measure: 296 roster of factor scores (Continued) Factor Subject Number I II III IV V 176 0.48 1.14 -0.30 0.10 -0.26 177 -2.99 -0.31 2.47 1.25 -0.44 178 -0.71 0.12 —0.27 -0.45 -0.27 179 0.07 -0.87 -0.17 0.41 -0.20 180 -0.02 -O.86 0.05 1.13 -0.06 181 -0.20 -0.11 0.19 -0.50 -0.40 182 -0.62 0.27 -0.48 1.10 -0.17 183 —0.69 -0.52 -1.15 -0.52 -0.30 184 1.52 -0.68 0.60 0.61 -O.37 185 1.47 -0.84 -0.80 0.60 0.25 186 0.55 -0.04 0.46 0.75 -0.42 187 0.27 0.05 0.49 0.58 -0.31 188 0.14 1.03 0.69 —0.08 4.59 189 1.26 -0.39 2.07 -0.14 -0.16 190 0.22 -0.81 -0.66 0.49 -0.14 191 0.44 -0.36 -0.18 -1.64 -0.19 192 1.04 0.26 -0.84 0.47 0.13 193 -0.73 0.09 -0.80 0.16 -0.08 194 0.93 -0.37 -0.27 -0.50 0.07 195 1.60 -0.13 -0.10 -0.87 0.12 196 0.83 -0.34 -0.34 0.56 —0.31 197 -0.10 —0.62 -0.92 -0.20 -0.29 198 -0.30 -0.92 0.38 —1.38 -0.20 199 0.46 —0.46 0.69 1.09 0.03 200 1.21 -0.37 1.26 -0.90 -0.17 201 0.64 1.71 0.]6 0.18 -0.45 202 -0.21 0.14 -0.30 -0.02 -0.47 203 —0.23 -0.26 0.59 0.26 0.05 204 0.01 0.30 1.45 -1.00 -0.23 Reference measure: 297 roster of factor scores (Continued) Factor Subject Number I II III IV V 205 0.64 1.57 -0.34 0.51 -0.43 206 -0.68 0.13 -0.89 -0.37 -o.02 207 -0.87 2.27 0.96 0.45 -o.33 208 0.49 0.86 1.16 1.15 -o.17 209 -0.52 -0.54 -1.16 0.59 0.03 210 0.22 0.38 -0.30 -0.80 4.45 211 -0.17 1.98 -0.31 -0.63 -0.32 212 —1.58 0.15 -0.90 0.44 0.11 213 -1.39 0.56 -0.60 -0.75 —0.16 214 0.73 -0.83 0.81 0.13 0.16 215 -0.65 -0.44 1.15 0.69 -0.07 216 -o.39 -0.71 -0.04 0.02 -0.03 217 —0.68 -0.16 -0.48 0.18 -0.32 218 -1.26 0.50 0.33 1.12 -0.07 219 0.18 -0.40 -0.40 0.34 -0.25 220 1.99 1.07 0.72 -1.53 -0.04 221 0.12 0.07 —0.58 -0.64 -0.06 222 0.63 -o.51 -0.81 0.34 -0.16 223 -2.15 0.45 2.47 -4.68 -0.37 224 -1.31 0.01 -0.66 0.72 -0.18 225 0.25 -0.74 -1.19 0.58 0.37 226 0.53 —0.69 -0.35 0.31 -0.06 227 -0.74 -0.42 1.46 -0.19 4.41 228 2.00 -0.30 -1.76 0.38 0.20 229 -0.21 0.12 —0.06 -0.18 -0.14 230 0.07 -0.12 0.99 0.51 -0.32 231 1.21 -0.18 0.35 0.91 0.14 232 -0.21 -0.71 -0.35 0.41 -0.27 Reference measure: 298 roster of factor scores (Continued) Factor Subject Number I II III IV V 233 -0.71 -0.94 -0.43 0.64 -0.30 234 0.55 2.22 0.04 -0.25 -0.14 235 1.30 -1.00 0.26 1.01 0.22 236 0.24 1.44 0.35 0.82 -0.11 237 -1.19 -0.31 -0.90 -0.11 4.88 238 0.27 -0.46 0.76 -0.92 —0.33 239 0.26 —0.27 0.04 -0.74 -0.04 240 1.33 0.68 -0.68 0.58 -0.03 241 -0.89 -0.39 0.23 0.32 4.44 242 -0.69 1.01 0.29 0.08 -0.18 243 0.18 0.90 -1.14 0.14 -0.25 244 -0.24 -0.13 -0.67 -0.72 -0.28 245 -0.44 -0.34 -0.56 0.09 -0.10 246 -0.66 -0.46 —1.42 0.04 -0.15 247 -0.66 -0.46 -1.42 0.04 -0.15 APPENDIX P ORIENTATION MEASURE: ROSTER OF FACTOR SCORES 299 ORIENTATION MEASURE: 300 ROSTER OF FACTOR SCORES Factor Subject Number I II III IV V VI VII 001 0.40 -0.68 1.04 -0.58 0.77 0.58 -0.16 002 -1.32 0.61 -1.30 2.05 0.44 0.83 0.88 003 -0.10 -1.50 1.23 0.90 0.56 -0.37 0.74 004 -0.28 -0.29 -0.08 0.37 -0.65 -0.21 -0.41 005 1.03 0.27 -0.45 —0.98 -0.56 -0.74 1.96 006 -0.13 0.32 -0.55 -0.27 0.32 -0.86 -0.54 007 -1.07 0.47 0.28 0.25 -0.83 -0.46 0.11 008 0.15 -0.60 1.11 -0.40 -0.43 -1.23 0.34 009 1.66 -1.20 -0.26 0.61 -0.56 1.03 —0.08 010 ~0.52 -0.13 1.07 -0.45 -0.13 0.21 -0.43 011 -0.75 -0.33 -0.72 -1.09 0.91 -0.12 0.87 012 -0.09 -0.32 0.33 -0.67 0.72 -0.27 -0.48 013 0.30 -0.66 0.43 0.07 -0.37 -0.47 0.56 014 -0.19 -0.30 0.26 -0.34 0.49 -0.51 0.81 015 —0.13 -0.37 1.50 -0.08 -0.47 1.06 -0.55 016 0.26 -0.53 0.41 0.16 -0.06 1.23 -0.34 017 -0.72 —1.47 0.27 0.33 0.14 2.05 -0.17 018 0.06 0.17 -0.98 -1.30 -0.74 -0.30 -1.12 019 1.24 0.39 0.23 —0.82 1.45 1.38 0.21 020 0.87 0.32 -2.19 0.58 -0.51 0.54 -0.07 021 -1.57 -0.02 -1.80 -1.18 -0.99 0.62 0.29 022 -0.53 1.08 -0.14 1.10 -0.82 -0.18 -0.01 023 -1.34 —0.79 1.09 0.33 0.78 -0.95 -0.06 024 -0.87 -0.20 0.72 0.69 -0.37 0.48 0.84 025 —1.47 0.76 -0.25 -0.72 -0.84 -0.25 -0.03 026 0.29 -0.64 —0.21 0.67 0.61 -0.29 0.85 027 0.94 -0.38 0.93 -0.68 0.11 1.13 0.21 028 0.42 0.36 -1.49 0.46 -l.64 -0.58 0.64 029 -1.00 —0.70 1.49 -0.18 -0.50 0.23 -0.61 301 Orientation measure: roster of factor scores (Continued) Factor Subject Number I II III IV V VI VII 030 0.08 1.04 -0.47 0.91 0.25 0.40 0.79 031 -0.33 1.14 -0.08 0.46 -0.30 0.65 —0.20 032 0.92 -0.69 -0.36 -0.09 -0.98 -0.25 0.01 033 0.78 -0.15 -1.01 0.28 -0.61 -0.30 -0.65 034 -0.18 1.24 0.69 0.14 -0.05 -0.90 -0.38 035 -0.00 -1.50 0.40 -0.72 0.38 -0.21 1.62 036 -1.15 -0.38 0.21 0.19 -0.61 0.27 -0.44 037 —1.15 -0.82 0.75 -0.96 -0.94 -0.13 -0.29 038 -0.44 -0.38 -0.28 -0.61 -0.31 -0.89 -0.02 039 -0.20 -0.31 -1.00 -1.15 0.69 —1.06 -0.12 040 0.24 -1.37 -0.48 0.93 1.69 0.16 -0.29 041 0.48 -1.11 0.83 -0.82 -0.56 0.53 0.53 042 0.72 -0.32 0.79 -0.67 -0.11 1.00 0.45 043 -2.29 -0.83 0.74 -0.17 -0.11 1.73 -0.98 044 -1.58 0.24 -1.54 1.05 —0.09 -0.33 0.21 045 0.65 -0.17 -1.14 -0.18 0.04 0.05 0.12 046 0.04 -0.46 0.15 -0.11 0.17 —0.12 0.98 047 0.17 -0.42 -0.55 -0.47 -0.15 -0.18 0.80 048 -0.49 1.39 0.43 1.08 0.24 -0.86 -0.31 049 -0.45 -0.23 -0.29 0.62 -0.38 0.21 —0.08 050 -1.99 —1.16 1.42 -O.15 —0.77 -0.45 -0.49 051 -0.87 -0.10 1.29 0.38 -0.31 -0.22 0.87 052 -1.69 0.85 -0.49 -1.20 0.09 -0.92 -1.05 053 0.59 -0.52 —0.90 -0.55 -0.00 -0.53 0.30 054 -1.44 -1.06 0.75 -0.41 0.54 -0.15 0.04 055 0.81 0.32 -0.01 0.01 -0.34 0.49 -0.06 056 1.29 -0.12 0.74 -0.41 —0.92 0.19 0.28 057 -0.22 1.18 0.37 0.74 -1.29 0.70 0.41 058 0.29 -0.65 0.54 -1.48 0.27 -0.36 0.09 302 Orientation measure: roster of factor scores (Continued) Factor Subject Number I II III IV V VI VII 059 1.13 0.11 -0.07 0.09 0.31 -0.13 -0.55 060 -1.01 0.52 -0.49 -0.21 -0.02 0.51 -0.52 061 -0.36 0.37 0.26 0.40 0.95 -0.87 0.69 062 -0.02 0.14 -0.20 -1.13 0.29 0.80 -0.35 063 -1.54 0.65 0.26 1.34 0.48 -0.89 -0.90 064 0.67 0.51 0.15 -0.90 -0.58 0.42 0.18 065 0.75 -0.92 0.69 0.86 0.83 -0.58 -0.08 066 -0.80 0.12 0.16 -0.37 -0.12 0.45 -0.36 067 0.07 0.29 1.38 1.21 1.13 1.36 1.09 068 -0.02 -0.78 -0.27 0.11 -0.22 0.39 0.87 069 0.95 -1.09 0.45 —0.32 —0.07 -0.31 -0.23 070 -0.64 1.08 -0.30 -0.51 0.17 -0.87 1.14 071 -0.88 -1.13 1.33 -0.43 0.36 0.26 -0.66 072 -1.17 0.47 0.85 -0.62 -0.03 -0.20 0.24 073 1.03 0.61 -0.23 0.79 -0.45 -0.02 -1.19 074 -1.24 -0.10 0.56 -0.05 0.95 -0.13 -1.04 075 -0.54 -1.71 -0.09 1.40 -0.45 0.04 -0.09 076 -1.00 0.44 0.31 0.17 -0.21 0.15 0.12 077 —0.15 -0.86 0.26 -0.50 -0.18 -0.23 -0.19 078 0.64 -0.44 0.87 -0.25 -l.69 0.79 0.82 079 -1.13 0.35 0.32 0.11 —1.08 -0.63 0.89 080 1.21 -0.52 0.06 0.04 0.00 —0.70 -1.06 081 0.28 -1.08 0.17 0.54 -0.61 0.26 —0.44 082 0.07 0.82 0.54 0.17 0.49 -0.26 -1.06 083 0.66 0.71 -0.45 -0.06 0.02 0.76 0.34 084 -0.15 -0.10 0.29 -0.59 -0.51 0.30 -0.67 085 0.84 -0.73 —0.40 -0.72 0.05 -0.47 -0.10 086 0.48 0.95 -0.84 —0.27 -0.05 -0.82 0.09 087 -0.35 —0.24 0.28 0.78 -2.29 0.50 1.13 303 Orientation measure: roster of factor scores (Continued) Factor Subject Number I II III IV V VI VII 088 1.32 -0.88 -0.36 —1.17 0.62 -0.15 -0.21 089 -1.08 -0.77 -0.02 -0.38 0.32 0.09 0.33 090 0.95 -0.20 0.14 -1.05 0.72 0.21 -0.43 091 0.43 -0.22 1.64 0.06 -1.21 0.30 -1.24 092 1.84 -0.50 -0.55 -0.84 1.70 0.79 -0.46 093 -0.32 1.45 -0.54 -0.56 0.08 0.37 0.34 094 -0.69 0.01 -0.25 -0.15 -0.19 0.44 -0.38 095 -0.78 -0.25 -0.30 -0.69 -0.31 0.03 0.53 096 -0.68 0.70 -0.48 0.52 0.49 -0.60 -0.10 097 0.11 0.85 -0.06 -0.44 -0.37 0.10 -0.63 098 0.11 0.96 -0.19 71.40 -1.05 -0.93 -0.09 099 0.28 -0.05 0.91 0.66 0.10 0.54 0.38 100 0.09 0.76 -2.46 -0.21 -0.09 0.47 1.24 101 -0.23 1.62 1.44 0.04 0.59 -0.20 -0.93 102 -0.23 1.23 0.25 -0.17 0.37 -0.80 1.06 103 0.48 -0.91 1.05 0.02 0.25 —0.73 0.44 104 —0.44 -1.10 0.23 0.90 0.07 0.18 -0.50 105 -0.21 -0.26 0.18 1.24 -0.35 -0.38 -0.25 106 —0.20 -0.01 0.14 1.08 0.37 -0.88 -0.98 107 0.06 0.96 -0.28 -0.62 0.57 0.11 -0.06 108 0.52 1.24 -1.01 0.60 0.43 0.52 -0.08 109 0.95 -0.97 -2.82 -0.96 0.62 -0.00 —0.49 110 -0.59 -O.15 0.39 0.68 0.82 -0.81 0.55 111 0.34 -0.30 1.49 0.78 0.43 -1.29 -0.22 112 0.61 -0.10 -0.22 0.03 -0.02 -1.22 -0.89 113 —0.63 —0.07 —0.42 0.97 -1.08 0.24 0.33 114 0.69 1.32 0.17 0.59 1.18 -0.10 -1.34 115 0.44 0.41 -1.00 -0.02 -0.45 -0.76 -0.32 116 -1.71 1.52 —O.58 0.52 0.60 0.75 0.93 304 Orientation measure: roster of factor scores (Continued) Factor Subject Number I II III IV V VI VII 117 -0.33 0.52 -0.54 0.58 0.44 -1.12 -0.60 118 0.97 -0.68 -1.61 -O.17 2.23 -1.78 1.23 119 0.45 -1.20 -0.28 0.90 0.14 -0.89 -0.73 120 0.85 0.69 1.72 0.42 1.01 -0.79 -0.83 121 -1.74 -0.77 0.55 0.23 -0.53 -0.79 -l.20 122 —0.31 1.12 0.71 -2.63 0.29 -1.77 1.02 123 0.22 -O.25 -0.34 0.52 0.43 0.21 0.93 124 0.25 -0.20 1.01 0.72 -0.15 -1.52 0.02 125 -O.30 0.86 -0.64 0.79 0.83 -0.51 1.32 126 0.84 2.14 0.16 —0.95 0.47 —0.24 0.50 127 -0.06 -0.12 0.95 1.93 -0.47 -0.10 —0.35 128 0.76 -0.57 0.04 -0.72 -0.65 0.88 0.87 129 0.84 0.73 0.32 0.67 2.02 0.26 -0.43 130 -0.43 0.33 -0.23 -0.30 0.07 0.46 -0.55 131 0.68 0.10 0.12 -0.24 0.34 -0.04 0.27 132 0.10 -0.30 0.16 0.16 0.16 0.92 -1.56 133 0.42 -0.13 -0.47 0.21 -0.14 0.23 -0.18 134 0.93 1.97 -0.12 0.64 1.52 1.66 -0.84 135 1.35 0.33 -1.59 -0.85 0.07 -1.54 -0.03 136 0.58 0.80 0.11 0.97 -l.33 0.38 -1.67 137 0.23 0.23 -0.10 0.39 -0.51 —0.45 0.12 138 -0.12 0.78 -0.55 -0.67 -0.98 -0.34 —1.00 139 -1.46 1.41 -0.23 -0.27 -l.02 0.63 2.32 140 0.37 0.81 -O.15 1.33 1.53 0.88 -0.15 141 0.34 -1.48 0.78 -0.85 0.10 0.07 -0.44 142 1.60 1.25 0.44 0.36 -0.02 0.90 -0.76 143 -1.04 0.12 -0.47 —0.27 0.13 0.68 -0.38 144 -0.13 0.46 -0.93 -0.30 -0.81 -0.32 0.47 145 0.13 0.16 0.05 0.74 -0.73 0.40 0.86 305 Orientation measure: roster of factor scores (Continued) Factor Subject Number I II III IV V VI VII 146 0.57 0.81 0.86 -2.12 -0.52 -0.80 1.00 147 -0.07 0.31 0.06 -0.42 -0.95 -0.06 0.40 148 1.56 0.93 0.32 -0.60 0.43 0.38 0.88 149 0.12 1.71 -0.18 0.28 0.69 0.34 -0.44 150 —0.20 -1.80 -0.08 1.77 -0.30 -0.17 0.97 151 0.00 -0.17 0.15 0.18 -0.95 0.13 0.27 152 -1.21 0.12 -0.21 1.64 -0.12 0.10 -0.03 153 0.65 -1.04 —0.04 0.36 -0.62 -0.91 0.86 154 0.31 -0.57 0.06 0.50 -0.26 -0.59 0.40 155 0.25 —0.56 -0.67 -0.05 -1.45 0.38 -0.64 156 -0.39 0.17 0.03 0.38 -0.35 -0.24 -0.06 157 0.74 -0.19 -0.18 0.88 0.55 0.29 -0.38 158 0.90 -0.98 -0.41 0.57 -0.57 —0.59 -l.l7 159 0.66 —0.23 -0.16 0.56 0.03 -0.72 -0.38 160 0.40 -0.01 -1.03 0.84 0.25 0.62 -0.11 161 0.53 0.44 -0.18 -0.17 0.00 0.72 0.67 162 —0.38 0.36 0.56 -0.93 0.23 1.00 1.03 163 -0.56 -0.41 -0.36 0.72 -0.98 -0.93 0.53 164 -0.66 0.70 -0.71 -0.15 -0.23 -0.02 -1.50 165 0.16 -0.10 0.43 0.09 -1.00 0.44 -0.18 166 1.14 1.45 0.58 -O.96 0.27 1.30 0.44 167 0.23 0.06 '-0.52 -0.83 0.59 -0.06 -0.60 168 0.88 -0.79 -0.63 -0.96 0.35 0.82 -0.40 169 0.91 1.25 0.31 —0.28 -0.35 -l.62 -0.69 170 -0.74 -0.18 -0.60 -0.48 0.54 0.39 0.64 171 -0.50 -0.14 -0.05 -0.12 0.19 0.86 -1.14 172 0.54 -0.59 -0.95 -0.26 -0.17 -0.69 1.18 173 -0.53 -0.85 -0.41 10.5 -0.30 0.34 -0.01 174 -0.74 0.23 -0.73 -0.24 0.10 0.47 0.12 306 Orientation measure: roster of factor scores (Continued) Factor Subject Number I II III IV V VI VII 175 -1.43 -1.60 0.55 -0.38 0.28 -0.76 -0.29 176 0.53 1.03 0.18 -0.20 0.31 0.11 0.15 177 0.15 -0.65 -0.06 0.63 0.67 0.02 -1.09 178 1.52 0.03 -0.29 -0.44 1.25 -0.52 -0.39 179 1.27 -0.10 1.85 -1.42 —1.00 0.52 -0.63 180 -1.16 -1.07 1.37 -0.80 0.36 0.20 -0.92 181 1.01 0.01 -0.42 0.18 0.41 -0.06 -0.29 182 0.71 —0.33 1.15 0.47 1.24 -0.26 0.62 183 -0.28 2.46 1.63 -2.81 1.53 0.18 1.31 184 0.06 -0.88 0.54 0.73 0.09 -1.01 -0.06 185 1.27 -0.68 1.05 -0.47 -0.51 -0.63 -0.99 186 0.03 0.91 0.85 -0.22 0.05 0.60 1.30 187 0.69 0.40 0.54 0.75 -1.04 -1.21 -0.48 188 0.61 0.25 -1.49 -0.56 -2.72 0.46 -1.34 189 -0.58 1.20 0.57 2.26 1.82 0.85 0.56 190 -1.50 0.34 0.70 -0.47 -0.38 -0.13 -0.67 191 -0.71 -0.49 0.51 1.06 0.19 -0.78 -0.57 192 0.26 1.03 -0.78 -0.32 -0.59 0.52 —0.96 193 0.52 0.74 0.11 0.40 -1.85 1.37 -1.66 194 0.24 -0.31 0.36 0.03 -0.08 0.10 0.34 195 -0.35 -1.19 -1.25 -2.15 0.49 -0.53 0.71 196 -1.24 -0.38 0.37 0.77 0.02 0.49 -0.50 197 -0.12 -0.05 -0.42 0.82 -1.59 1.12 -0.33 198 0.35 -0.32 0.08 0.07 -0.08 0.67 —0.16 199 -0.56 -0.18 -0.79 0.87 -1.02 0.61 0.13 200 0.11 2.98 0.16 -0.80 0.03 0.57 0.60 201 0.91 0.60 -0.53 0.22 -0.20 0.42 1.37 202 0.88 -0.22 0.02 -0.51 0.94 -0.80 -0.15 203 0.08 0.77 -0.49 0.20 -1.50 -0.05 -0.26 307 Orientation measure: roster of factor scores (Continued) Factor Subject Number I II III IV V VI VII 204 -0.46 0.03 0.10 -0.59 0.20 -1.08 0.01 205 0.29 -0.36 0.33 0.83 0.41 1.88 0.40 206 -1.09 -0.37 -0.73 0.21 0.86 1.87 0.04 207 1.12 2.24 0.07 —0.95 0.76 1.17 2.40 208 0.16 0.88 -0.18 -0.27 0.28 0.44 -1.34 209 -0.47 -0.12 1.06 -0.42 -0.11 0.03 0.11 210 0.14 0.48 0.68 0.11 -0.80 0.04 0.10 211 —1.34 0.48 -1.41 0.71 1.59 -0.44 0.47 212 -0.24 -1.10 -0.21 -0.50 0.28 -0.03 -0.13 213 0.48 -0.62 —1.16 —1.90 1.35 —1.34 -0.49 214 0.92 -0.61 0.38 0.36 0.18 -l.06 0.27 215 0.50 0.37 -0.46 -1.12 -0.10 -0.08 -0.35 216 -1.52 -0.30 0.75 0.08 0.09 -0.11 -0.68 217 -0.11 -0.41 -0.00 -1.02 0.50 -0.73 0.12 218 0.23 0.87 -1.12 -0.78 —0.67 0.35 0.49 219 -0.56 0.26 0.54 0.13 1.03 -0.08 40.19 220 -0.99 0.17 -0.78 -0.24 -1.43 -0.44 -0.09 221 -0.02 -1.02 0.55 -0.87 0.23 -0.39 -0.65 222 0.23 -0.70 0.53 0.64 —0.36 0.45 -0.29 223 -1.00 -0.92 0.21 -1.36 0.57 -0.50 0.54 224 -1.03 -0.28 -1.08 1.66 -0.42 -0.14. -0.53 225 -0.05 -0.37 -0.32 0.35 -0.75 -0.22 “0.23 226 -0.11 -0.59 1.49 1.17 —0.48 0.10 2222 227 0.96 0.38 -0.30 0.02 1.11 -0.08 -0.37 228 0.12 0.32 -0.36 0.68 -0.51 0.50 0.26 229 1.03 —0.84 0.34 0.30 0.95 -0.57 0.42 230 1.27 -0.54 0.44 0.48 —0.74 -0.89 0.30 231 0.69 —0.80 -0.77 -0.59 0.36 0.68 -0.32 232 -0.83 -0.43 0.50 0.20 0.28 -0.26 —O.98 308 Orientation measure: roster of factor scores (Continued) Factor Subject Number I II III IV V VI VII 233 1.13 0.02 0.45 -0.87 -0.99 0.59 1.50 234 -0.55 2.16 -0.58 1.26 0.93 -0.77 0.59 235 1.19 -0.19 0.43 0.14 -0.41 —0.98 -0.23 236 1.31 0.89 —0.35 -0.34 0.29 0.01 -1.01 237 1.06 -0.64 -0.39 0.13 0.47 -0.09 -0.50 238 0.38 0.07 -0.25 -1.03 0.67 -0.85 0.42 239 —0.26 -2.33 -l.02 —0.51 1.67 1.95 0.46 240 -0.27 -0.20 -0.29 0.42 0.35 -0.96 0.47 241 -0.28 —1.05 1.51 -0.72 0.05 0.08 0.80 242 -0.44 -0.17 0.27 0.47 0.01 0.83 0.87 243 -1.05 0.47 0.38 0.43 -0.13 0.18 -0.76 244 -0.72 -0.72 -1.14« 1.62 -0.63 0.12 -0.18 245 0.45 -0.65 0.17 1.69 -0.49 -0.68 0.98 246 -1.92 0.79 -0.98 -0.29 1.63 1.96 -0.99 247 -0.60 0.12 -0.98 0.78 -0.05 -0.05 -0.20 APPENDIX Q CHI-SQUARE RESULTS:: REFERENCE FACTORS SHOWN AS DEPENDENT VARIABLES 309 H0.v u m n ««« 310 N0.v H m u «« M0.v u m n « "50M OOGMOHMHGMHM ANO ...5OO.HH OOO.H OOO.H HOO.H ONH.O mumsvmuHao mnumum mHnmumnsmz 1N0 5OO.O H5O.H OOO.O OOO.H OON.O mumsvmano OuHmmoH non LOO HHO.O «NOO.NH ONO.O OOO.N OOO.O mumavmuHso muscme amumam mOHQMHHm> HOH>msmm LOO NO5.O OO5.O OOO.O OOO.N HOO.H. mumswmuHao mHOmumczo aoHumuO LOO ONH.O OOO.O OOO.N NON.O OO0.0 mumsvmuHso mOHm 06x06: LOO O50.0 OO5.0 5OO.O OH0.0 500.H mumswmano aoHuOooH coHuOuO LOO .OHO.HH OOO.5 .OOOO.NH OO0.0 OON.5 mumsvmano 0055 coHumpm mmemHHm> Emummm LOO HO5.O NOO.N «.HO5.NH O5O.H OOO.O mumsvmuHOo Hm>mH moquumdxm LOO .O5O.OH NOO.N O50.0 OOH.O «ONO.O mumswmano H0>0H coHumonOm LOO OOOOH OOO.N OOO.O ON5.O NOO.O mumswmano coHuoasmumHom AOO «.OOO.OH ON5.O HO5.O 500.0 OOO.HH mumsvmquo aoHuHOoN awpmmm mmHQMHum> Masonmxomm OO gummumsHO>= =ummudsouo gummumHsmg .umm =00O muouumm IMHMEHHm: lucmHHU: IHOOHMU: mocmummmm mmadem¢> Bzmazmmma mfl 230mm mfiOBUfim mUmemmmm ”MBHDmmm mm