" VESUAL ACUITY AS AFFECTED BY ADJACER‘F EORDERS N A TARGET Thesis for the Degree of DH. D. MECBIGEN STATE UNEVERSITY J. Yves Lorrie 1965 THESES LIBRARY—L1 Michigan State University This is to certify that the thesis entitled Visual Acuity as Affected by Adjacent Borders in a Target presented by Jean Ives Lortie has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for PhoDo degree in PSXCh010gy WJWSZ‘ 0’“ ’fi L . K Major professor \ 17d? Date MM 3 0-169 ABSTRACT VISUAL ACUITY AS AFFECTED BY ADJACENT BORDERS IN A TARGET by J. Yves Lortie This investigation examines the effect of adjacent borders upon detection of a fine line. A review of the literature on contour processes indi- cates that borders near one another exert a mutually depressive influence. This influence was shown to extend to distances as far as four degrees on the retina. If in a set-up traditionally used for studying visual acuity two dark bars are introduced at different distances from a fine line to be detected, one would expect visual acuity to be impaired when the bars are close to the line. As the dis- tance is increased, the depressing effect of the bars should progressively decrease. Moreover, such a depressive effect, if present, can be better understood by performing temporal manipulations of borders. Thus, varying the order of presentation of the bars and line should reveal signifi- cant interactions between contours. It should also throw some light on the relationship between contour processes and visual acuity. J. Yves Lortie It was the purpose of this author to examine those points, or more generally to analyze some of the impli- cations of relating visual acuity and contour processes. Two trained observers participated in the experiment. Six situations were investigated in which several spatial and temporal manipulations of borders were performed, such as length of bars and line, distance of bars from the line, order of presentation of bars and line, and shifting of bars. The results indicate that visual acuity is affected by the presence of borders in the vicinity: (1) Distance be- tween the bars turned out to be a significant factor. When these bars were near the line, more time was required for its detection than when they were far out. At an inter- mediary distance, however, they had a facilitatory effect upon visual acuity. (2) The length of bars, when near the line, counterbalanced the facilitatory effect of an increase in length of this one. (3) Removal of the bars at the same time as the line was projected had a strong depressive effect upon visual acuity. (4) A shifting of the bars away from the line also required more time for its detection. (5) A shifting of the bars toward the line was facilitatory in the case of one observer, and inhibitory in the case of the second one. (6) Equally, for one observer simultaneous presentation of the bars and line had a facilitatory J. Yves Lortie influence upon visual acuity while the effect was inhibitory for the other. The results were discussed in terms of contour forma— tion. They suggest that any factor which impairs visual acuity does so by interfering with this neural process. Among the factors found to play a significant role are the presence of other contours in vicinity, their order of formation and their destruction. Approved: gfiQNA—BS;)~wQL CommitteeSChaifman Date: YMM:21gcwb2f VISUAL ACUITY AS AFFECTED BY ADJACENT BORDERS IN A TARGET BY .— ‘t' ' f 1 J3 Yves Lortie A THESIS Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Psychology 1965 ACKNOWLEDGMENT S The author is indebted to Dr. S. Howard Bartley for his advice and supervision. His many helpful sug- gestions led to the realization of the present research. He is also grateful to Dr. H. Richard for his encourage- ment and for the provision of facilities in the Depart- ment of Psychology of Laval University, Quebec. ii TABLE OF CONTENTS Page INTRODUCTION . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 METHOD . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 25 Subjects . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 25 Apparatus and Material . . . . . . . . . . 26 Preparation of Subjects. . . . . . . . . . 31 Procedure. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 55 RESULTS . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 59 DISCUSSION . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 65 SUMMARY . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 82 BIBLIOGRAPHY . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 85 iii LIST OF TABLES TABLE Page 1. Raw data on threshold duration obtained in some of the situations—-for the two ob- servers. Photic intensity .055 c/ft2 . . . 59 2. Raw data found to be less stable. Photic intensity .055 c/ft2. Observer J.V. . . . 40 iv LIST OF FIGURES FIGURE 1. 2. 10. 11. 12. Targets 1 F and 1 E used in fore and ex- posure periods respectively. . . . . . . . The six experimental situations examined under three different levels of photic in- tensity. . . . Visual acuity as dependent upon the dis- tance of adjacent borders. . . . . . . . . Effects of diverses manipulations of ad- jacent borders upon detection of the line. Data for J.Y.L. . . The influence of diverse manipulations of adjacent borders upon visual acuity. Data for J.V. . . . . . . Comparisons of sets of data collected at a six month interval . . . Visual acuity as affected by a shifting of position of adjacent borders in fore and exposure periods . . . . . . . . . . . . . The relation between bars length and visual acuity for two different distances of the bars. . . . . . Visual acuity as dependent upon bar length and distance between the bars. . . . . . . Comparison of duration thresholds for five of the experimental situations. Data for J.Y.L. O O O O O O O O O I O O O I O O C 0 Mean detection thresholds for five of the situations, with J.V. as the observer. . . Gradient-wise projection of a dark bar upon the retina. . . . . . . Page 29 34 41 44 45 48 51 54 56 59 62 69 INTRODUCTION Visual acuity is a spatial discrimination involving a simultaneous comparison of different areas of a target. It is defined as the reciprocal of the minimum visual angle subtended by some relevant element of the target, measured in minutes of arc. In order for visual acuity to take place, there must be a detectable disparity or lack of uniformity in the different portions of the target; in other words, borders must exist in the target itself. A recent review (Westheimer, 1965) indicates that many different data have been collected concerning visual acuity. Equally, various explanations have been offered: the most important of these have been critically reviewed by Senders (1948) and Falk (1956). The discussion has been brought up to date by Boynton (1962)" However, the detection of a fine line, a grating, or any other figure, implies the formation of contours (Bartley, J. 1941, p. 354). This neural mechanism has not been given all the attention it deserved in psychophysical as well J'Because he is dealing with three different types of phenomena: physical, neurophysiological, and perceptual, the author, following Bartley (1958 a, p. 150), differen— tiates between borders, contours, and edges. The abrupt changes in illumination of the target are called borders, the corresponding neural processes are named contour forma— tion or contour processes and the perceptual end-results are the edges. 1 as in neurophysiological researches. For example, one of the major findings of the studies devoted to it is that borders or abrupt changes of luminance in a target, at certain distances one from the other, interact in the formation of their reSpective contours, thus changing the perceptual outcome (Fry & Bartley, 1955). Any condition affecting this process, as for example, the depressing in- fluence of borders in near vicinity, should change visual acuity. Very few studies, however, have approached the problem in this way. It was the purpose of the present author to examine the evidence offered about this process, to study its relation to visual acuity, and to perform further manipulations of borders in order to observe their effects upon detection of a fine line. In this chapter some of the studies on contour forma- tion are reviewed and generalizations to which they led are examined. A review of these generalizations in relation to visual acuity follows. Finally, Specific questions are formulated which it is the purpose of the author to study. Contour Processes Definition When a target with abrupt borders is projected upon the retina, the information which reaches the cortex through the optic tract can be conceived of as being of two types: (1) a longitudinal propagation of activity along the dif- ferent channels of the pathway; (2) a lateral or cross- sectional type of activity which takes place in the retina and across the channels (Nelson, Bartley & Wise, 1963). This lateral type of discharge has been called contour forma— tion (Bartley, 1941, p. 229) or contour processes (Bartley, 1958 a, p. 150). Thus, contours are neural processes under— lying Spatial discriminations in which edges are involved. Contour processes have been posited in order to deal with these facts: (1) the retinal image of a sharp border is blurred; (2) brightness discrimination is affected by the distance between borders in a target, and by the timing of successive presentations of borders; (3) the perceptual or phenomenological properties of a target differ depending on the degree of illumination used. Blur of the Retinal Image In prOper conditions, abrupt borders in any portion of a target lead to the perception of steep, sharp edges. However, the corresponding image formed on the retina, besides being upside down, is more or less blurred, never as clear-cut as the borders themselves. In other words, the image on the retina is not an exact replicate of the physical object impinging on it. As the edge is seen as clear-cut, some neural mechanism, conceived in terms of enhancement and depression of lateral activity, is needed to resharpen what has become blurred. The gradient-wise distribution of retinal illumination results mainly from diffraction effects and different aber- rations of the refractive power of the eye. These two types of defects vary with the size of the pupil. Thus as the pupillary diameter decreases, diffraction effects become larger (Westheimer, 1963). With large pupils diffraction becomes negligible but, on the other hand, chromatic aberra- tion comes in: it depends on different wavelengths being refracted by different amounts, and this creates indistinct color effects along the edges of the perceived image. Another type of aberration found in optical systems is spherical aberration: the rays refracted through the outer portion of the lens are brought to a focus nearer that lens than those from the center. This normally would pro- duce a blurred and distorted image. This factor, however, is negligible in the eye: it is compensated to a high degree by the peripheral flattening of the cornea (Bartley, 1960, p. 202; Westheimer, 1963), and also by the size of the pupillary aperature and the structure and curvature of the lens (Bartley, 1960, p. 202). Other characteristics of the eye also affect the amount of blur of the retinal image: one of these is the accommo— dative power of the eye. Poor accommodation increases the amount of blur by bringing the image out of focus (Westheimer & Campbell, 1962). Moreover, it has been found that, during fixation, accommodation is constantly changing (Campbell & Westheimer, 1960; Arnulf & Dupuy, 1961). Finally, physiological nystagmus would be assumed to decrease the steepness of the gradient: the rapid oscilla- tions of fixation successively present adjacent areas of the target to the same retinal receptors and this should enhance the blur of the formed image. The author, though, did not find any direct evidence supporting this view. Distance Between Borders in Brightness Discrimination Suppose a small disk is surrounded by a larger ring and that the luminance of the disk is increased till the ob- server perceives a difference in brightness between the disk and its annular surround. It has been found (Blachowski, 1915) that the threshold for the detection of the disk decreases as the area of the surround is increased. Blackowski explained his results in terms of Spatial summation. Fry & Bartley (1935), however, have demonstrated that the decrease in thres- hold detection when the area is enlarged is due to the fact that in these conditions the outer borders of the ring are farther away from the borders of the disk and they interfere less with the formation of the appropriate contour. In their experiment they used a disk with a diameter of 5/8 inch and a large 6 inch ring surrounding it. Also a dark circular band, the diameter of which could be increased or decreased, was superimposed on the ring. By decreasing the diameter of the dark band, the distance between its inner border and the border of the disk was decreased, and the opposite was observed when the experimenter was increasing the diameter of the band. The area of the ring, however, remained constant. It was found that when the distance between the borders of the disk and the inner border of the band was 4 degrees or less the threshold of detection of the disk was higher; it increased in proportion as the distance between those borders was decreased. The authors concluded that already formed contours exert a depressive influence upon the formation of other contours in their immediate sur- round. Other manipulations by the Same authors allowed them to reach a second conclusion, that is: contours parallel to the activating contour are depressed while those perpen— dicular or at right angles to it tend to be facilitated. Thirdly, existing contours prevent activity from Spreading in the visual system; a third contour interposed between two activating contours will block the activity between these two. Timing of Successive Presentations of Borders The work of Fry & Bartley, reviewed above, has indicated the effect of Spatial distribution of events upon contour formation. Timing of events is a second important variable. Helson & Fehrer (1952) presented targets of different shapes tachistoscopically. With a very brief exposure, these were seen as "dim patches of light." As the exposure was length— ened, definite forms with sharp edges were recognized. In an extensive experiment, Werner (1955) used many different targets and manipulated Spatial as well as temporal variables. For example, in a part of the experiment he had a small illuminated disk alternate with an illuminated ring the inner border of which coincided with the outer border of the disk. When the disk was briefly presented first and was followed after about 150 msec. by the ring, the disk was not seen. The outcome was the same, when the figures were dark and the ground illuminated. With a slower rate of succession, the disk was seen and then the ring. Werner concluded that with a fast rate of succession, contours do not have time to form and consequently the figure is not perceived as such. Too short a duration of presentation depresses or destroys the formation of contours and it affects the appreciation of the brightness that a surface would otherwise have. Other shapes, where the space between two wide dark bars was occupied by several thin lines parallel or perpentidular to these bars yielded results sensibly Similar to those of Fry & Bartley for the parallel lines: these wereystrongly affected by the previous presentation of the wide bars. The lines perpendicular to the bars were depressed also, but to a lesser degree. Fry & Bartley had found that borders at right angles tended to facilitate contour formation. Fry & Bartley, however, were manipulating Space or distance be- tween borders, while Werner manipulated duration of presenta- tion in that part of his experiment. Role of Luminance Upon the Edge Properties of a Target The intensity of illumination of the different portions of a target is another variable which has been shown to con- siderably alter the formation of contours. Suppose (Bartley, 1941, p. 6) a stimulus consisting of a small illuminated disk (spot of illumination) in a dark field. When the intensity is very low, the observer sees an indefinite Spot of light, not well differentiated from the surround, which seems to wander about and may even disappear for brief moments. Phenomenologically the target is not seen as a disk, but as an indifferentiated Spot. If the intensity of illumination is increased sufficiently, then the result is quite different: what emerges is a disk with Sharp edges. An eXperiment which demonstrates the role of illumi- nation upon the edge properties of a disk has been performed by Bartley (1956). He used a target arrangement consisting of a disk surrounded by a ring the inner border of which coincided with the outer border of the disk. The disk portion of the target was alternately light or dark at each of several Slow rates, while the ring portion was kept constant in illumination. Physically the duration of the light and dark phases of the disk was the same; however, the intensity of illumination of the ring determined the perceptual outlook: when the intensity of the ring was kept above the mean value of the two phases of the disk, the dark phase of the disk became predominant. When the intensity of the ring was re- duced to a value below the mean, it was the light phase which precominated. The predominant phase possessed sharp edges and seemed to occupy most of the cycle, while the diminished phase had no definite edge but was seen as a mere shadow. Contours in Relation ot Other Phenomena Contour processes are involved not only in brightness discrimination, but also in many other phenomena: Bartley (1941) presents some evidence to the fact that contour formation has a decisive role in Fechner's paradox, in after- images and in visual acuity. Osgood (1955, p. 252) considers contour formation as being fundamental to all perceptual activity. The relation between contour processes and visual acuity will be examined after the following section on neuro- physiological researches. Neurophysiological Studies on Contours Although several psychophysical researches Show the legitimacy and importance of contour processes, these have 10 not been much investigated as such by neurophysiological techniques. This may be due to the complexity of the processes themselves: for example, it seems that in order to study them adequately large areas of the retina or many different channels of the optic tract would have to be covered simultaneously. In recent years microelectrode studies have tended to explore in a piecemeal fashion very restricted regions of the pathway, while macroelectrode techniques did not systematically investigate those processes. However, there has been some interesting work on what can be considered as part-effects of contour formation, that is on inhibitory mechanisms. For example, Hartline and collaborators (Ratliff, 1961), working with the lateral eye of the horseshoe crab, Limulus, and using diffuse illumination as a stimulus, reached the conclusion that the detection and enhancement of edges is explainable in terms of lateral in- hibitory interaction among retinal elements. While Hartline and collaborators.were working at the retinal level, Jung and co-workers (1961) investigated the visual cortex, recording neuronal discharges in the primary visual area of the cat. The stimulus used was diffuse illumi— nation. For binocular stimuli, they proposed to classify the response patterns of the visual cortex into five neuronal types, named A to E, thus expanding the classification of the retinal elements which comprises on, off, and on—off neurons. As no physical borders were used, not much could 11 be reported related to perception of edges. But, in 1958. Baumgartner, in the same laboratory, began to use what he calls "patterned light with white-black contrast," or targets with abrupt borders (a grid of light and dark bars). In one of their experiments, he and Hakas (Jung, 1961) exposed one of the light bars, subtending a visual angle of 5 degrees 41 minutes, and moved it by steps across the receptive fields of the cortical neurons. As expected, the response of these neurons was different from what is observed when there is no borders (diffuse illumination). When the illumination is on, a maximum of discharges is observed in the cortical B neurons if their receptive field is stimulated by the portion of the light bar at the margin of the dark bar (at the border). When the receptive field of these same neurons is presented with the dark ground, there is a minimum of discharges at light-on and also a reversal of their responses to onset the termination of illumination, that is when the illumination is on, the discharge of the B neurons is inhibited; it is activated when the illumination is off. The D neurons behave in the opposite way; they are activated when-the illumination is on, and depressed when it is off, if their receptive fields are presented with the dark part of the target. When the light part is used, the off-reSponse of these D neurons is maximal. To explain this reciprocal activation and inhibition of these two types of antagonistic cortical neurons and also the increased frequency of discharges at the border between 12 the light and dark bars, they utilized the reciprocal and lateral inhibition schema of Jung, that is there is a recipro- cal inhibition of antagonistic neurons (B and D) in the same receptive field, and a lateral inhibition of synergic neurons in the surrounding regions. Another interesting finding of Baumgartner is that the neuronal discharges corresponding to the borders between light and dark bars vary with the width of the bars, a fact in agreement with psychophysical observations to be reviewed later. From what has been done till now, one can easily con— clude that physiological studies are still far from providing the information necessary to clarify the mechanism of contour formation. The conclusions reached by Hartline when applied to the human or even the vertebrate retina at a lower level (the cat, for example) are very limited, at least for the reason that the compound eye of the Limulus is structurally and functionally very different from the vertebrate eye. Hartline and co-workers agree that in the vertebrate retina interaction would be more complex and would comprise excita- tory as well as inhibitory influences (Ratliff, 1961, p. 200). Also it is to be recalled that Hartline was recording neural impulses in one active nerve fiber at a time, thus reaching conclusions very limited in scope. Baumgartner worked with a vertebrate organ, the cat eye, and also used borders in his target. Second, he took I 15 care of comparing his physiological findings (neuronal dis- charges in cats) with corresponding visual perceptions in man, thus coming closer to bringing the elements which would ‘ allow a thorough understanding of the process of contour formation. However, here also one finds serious limitations: for example, his conclusions are valid only when eye move- ments are excluded. Optokinetic nystagmus seems to be an important factor in the blurring of the retinal image as it was mentioned previously. Besides, here again the work done was very analytical, in a piecemeal fashion, the authors recording from one receptive field at a time, while contour formation seems to work over very large areas of the retina, as the psychophysical studies of Bartley have Shown. In short, much more researches are needed before one can trans- late into clear physiological terms the processes of contour formation. Therefore, it is not the purpose of this author to work out such a mechanism. Attempts have been made else- where (Osgood, 1955, p. 229; Milner, 1958), but it is the author's Opinion that they are premature. For the time being, more work should be performed on the psychophysical as well as on the physiological levels: physiological researches will offer direct evidence on contour processes, while psycho- physical ones will bring in new facts and data which eventually one should be able to explain if he wants the processes to be of more than very restricted validity. 14 Contours Processes and Visual Acuity While in studies involving brightness discrimination one was working with extended areas, in visual acuity the area becomes minimal, at least along one dimension, and borders are much nearer one another. This effect of borders upon one another Should, therefore, become more prominent. Manipulations of Borders in Visual Acuity The distance between borders, in a visual acuity target, can be looked upon in several different ways: for example, in a target made up of a Single line there is the distance between the two longitudinal and parallel borders of the line, the distance between the borders at the two extremities of the line, and also the distance between the line and the borders formed by the frame of the whole target. When the target consists of two parallel lines, there is, besides the factors just mentioned, the interspace between the two lines, In a grating, there are Several interSpaces. With a Landolt ring, the relation between borders is more complex, while with letters the complexity of the relations is very variable, All of these shapes, and others, have been used in studying visual acuity. For example, disks (Ogle, 1961) as well as fine wires or lines (Fry & Cobb, 1955; Hecht & Mintz, 1959) have been used for studying the minimum visible. For the minimum separable the following configurations have been utilized: pairs of parallel bars (Fry & Cobb, 1955; Wilcox, 15 1952), Landolt ring (Shlaer, 1957), grating (Shlaer, 1957; Graham & Cook, 1957), two points (Oliva & Aguilar, 1957), and vernier adjustments.(Baker, 1949; Leibowitz, 1955). Among these, a very few are relevant to the problem with which the present author is concerned, those where borders in simple relation to one another are implied. Single line. The minimum width for a single dark line to be visible was Shown by Hecht & Mintz (1959) to be nearly 0.5 second at the highest illumination they used (50 milli- lamberts) and with binocular vision. The target was made up of a circular ground, an opal glass illuminated from behind and measuring 2 feet in diameter. The lines were made up of wires varying in thickness. The subject, in a chair on rollers, moved toward the target till he could ascertain the position of the line in the target. Hecht & Mintz explained its detection by the fact that the illumination on one row of cones is just perceptibly less than on the rows on either side of it. So, even if the retinal image of a fine line is fuzzy, it is not perceived as such but it is seen as a clear line because it would stimulate one row of cones less than the others adjacent to it. They maintained that no central mechanism of contour formation is necessary to convert the gradual distribution of illumination on the retina into a Sharp line at the perceptual level. Their eXplanation runs into serious difficulties. These have been covered by Senders (1948),and Falk (1956). Let‘s 16 mention only that according to Hecht & Mintz visual acuity would depend only on intensity of illumination. As will be seen, other factors are also implied, for example the length of the line. In the previous experiment, Hecht & Mintz always used wires of the same length. In another experiment, Hecht, Ross & Mueller (1947) varied the length of the fine wires. Again they found 0.45 sec. as the minimum width of a line to be seen against a bright sky. They found, however, that in order to get this result, the minimum length had to sub- tend an angle of about one degree at the eye. Below that value the threshold raised rapidly. The facilitating effect of length of line was confirmed by Ogilvie & Taylor later on (1959). The effect of the width of a light bar on visual acuity has been investigated by Fry & Cobb (1955). Using bars 50 minutes long they determined the threshold values for a number of different widths, beginning with a bar whose width was 5 minutes 12 seconds and then using bars of lesser width till they obtained one in which width and intensity were reciprocal. They were able to demonstrate that the retinal distribution of illumination is Gaussian in character; therefore, it is the intensity at the center of the retinal image of the bars which determines visual acuity. With a very narrow bar the intensity at the center may be subthres— hold. Increasing the width of the bar increases the intensity 17 at the center, so that the line may then become visible. However, one reaches a point where this relationship of width and intensity does not hold any longer; for example, any bar wider than 4 minutes 2 seconds, in the conditions of the experiment, did not reduce the threshold of detectability. It is to be noted that in this direct method of estimating the blur of the retinal image, nystagmus or the rapid oscilla— tory movements of the eye as well as diffraction and the chromatic and Spherical aberrations were taken into account. That the retinal gradient of illumunation is Gaussian or approximately so has been confirmed by Westheimer and Campbell (1962). In their experiment they used a streak light filament. They estimated the Spread of the corresponding retinal illumination by determining the distribution of in- tensity in the aerial image formed by reflection of the filament at the fundus and subsequent reverse passage through the eye. Parallel lines. When one is using two parallel bars visual acuity has been shown to depend not only on the inter— Space between these bars but to a certain point on the width of the bars themselves. For example, Fry & Cobb (1955) in the same series of experiments as described above, used two pairs of light bars measuring 55 minutes 20 seconds in length. One set of bars were 16 minutes 40 seconds in width while the second set, the narrow bars, were 2 minutes 48 seconds. Their results indicated that with an increase in the 18 luminosity of the bars from 0 to 5 foot-candles visual acuity. in the case of the wide bars, always increased, rapidly at first and then more slowly. In the case of the narrow bars, visual acuity improved markedly till the luminosity reached slightly more than one foot-candle, and then it deteriorated. To explain their findings the authors utilized the principle established by Fry & Bartley, namely that borders near one another interfere in contour formation. With narrow bars, parallel borders are near one another, the contour processes corresponding to each one of these borders interfere and depress each other thus elevating the threshold for detection of the interspace. With wider bars, the borders are farther apart and their influence is much less marked. The importance of bar width in determining visual acuity was further studied by Kravkov (1958). Wilcox (1952), studying the effect of illumination upon visual acuity, had found that with dark bars on a light ground detection of the interSpace between the two bars increased with an in- crease in retinal illumination, but that with light bars on a dark ground, visual acuity first improved and then de- teriorated. Furthermore, he observed that dark bars tended to be subjectively widened at low intensity, and that this apparent enlargement was decreasing with an increase in intensity, the bars subjectively appearing to Shrink. Kravkov showed that the conclusions of Wilcox are valid for narrow bars only, not for wide bars. He explained this 19 phenomenon of shrinking, called negative irradiation by Wilcox, in terms of the findings of Fry & Bartley on the depressing influence of borders parallel to one another on contour formation. In brief, with narrow bars two factors are at work, one photic intensity, favoring visual acuity, the other, borders near one another, impoverishing it by creating a shifting in contour processes toward one another. With an,increase in photic intensity, the increased steepness of the blur gradient would overcome the depressing effect of the other factor and an improved visual acuity would result. With broad bars, photic intensity becomes the main factor and no shifting is observed. The Present Problem The studies reviewed above, especially those by Kravkov, Fry & Cobb, indicate that borders near one another affect each other in determining visual acuity. Moreover, the work of Fry & Bartley on brightness detection had shown that this depressing influence may extend to distances as far as 4 de- grees on the retina. If a whole target subtends a visual angle of only a degree or so, which is very often the case in experimental set-ups where visual acuity is investigated, then the borders formed by the frame of the target should exert some influence on the perception of the line or lines. For example, the background field in Wilcox's study subtended an angle of 1 degree 10 minutes in width and 20 minutes in 20 height. According to the generalizations of Fry & Bartley, these borders of the field which are parallel to the two lines should affect their detection. Second, those borders perpendicular to the lines should exert a facilitatory in- fluence. As the lines are relatively short, 20 minutes in length, this influence should be serious. But these vari- ables have not been dealt with in Wilcox's study. The same problem is encountered in any set—up wherein relatively small background fields are used. It seems then that systematically examining the effects of bars presented at varying distances from a fine line would be justified: besides throwing new light on the pro— cesses investigated, such a study would allow one to test the validity of using a background field of restricted di— mensions and at the same time ignoring its effects when theorizing about visual acuity. In fact, one would expect visual acuity to be impaired if these borders are in close vicinity of the line. As the distance is increased, the depressing effect of the bars should progressively decrease. As simple as this may seem, the present author does not know of any study trying to investigate it. One study came close, though: Flom and co— workers (Flom, Weymouth & Kahneman, 1965) examined the effect of four dark bars placed tangential to a Landolt ring and at varying distances from it, upon detection of the position of the gap in the ring. The maximum interaction was found at 21 approximately five times the gap width. However, in that study the relation between borders is very complex; it is far from being the ideal situation for unraveling the func- tioning of contour processes. Another aSpect of the problem refers to temporal manipulations of borders. As contour processes take time to complete themselves, it appears that varying the order of presentation of different portions of the target would be essential for one to better understand the relationship between contour formation and visual acuity. For example, the bar borders: the bars borders may be presented before the line to be detected, or Simultaneously with it, or they may be removed at the exact moment the line is projected, etc.: all these manipulations should reveal significant interactions between borders. To the author‘s knowledge, such manipulations have not been tried. Traditionally all the elements of a target have been presented at the same time. In a typical experiment the target is presented for unlimited time and the observer indicates if he can detect or not the relevant portion (Hecht & Mintz, 1959). Or, while the observer is looking at the target, the distance between bars or lines may be varied by the experimenter till these are viewed as separate (Wilcox, 1952). In other experiments, the observer has a limited time for looking at the target, but again all the relevant portions of the target are pre- sented together for a certain duration (Niven & Brown, 1944). 22 Rare are the exceptions to that procedure: for example, Granit & Harper (1950) used the c f f method, but their purpose was to study the relation between spatial summation and visual acuity. Equally, Bartley and collaborators (Bartley, Nelson & Soules, 1965) utilized intermittent illumination; this was done, however, in order to investigate the effect of brightness enhancement upon visual acuity. Thus, in these two types of studies temporal manipulations of different portions of a target were effected but they were not of the kind suggested above. Third, another generalization of Fry & Bartley Should lead to interesting studies in visual acuity. They have shown that already formed contours prevent activity from Spreading in the whole retina. Therefore, in specific con- ditions to be determined, already formed contours should improve visual acuity. As far as the present author knows, this generalization has not been investigated. The purpose of the present study was to examine those factors, or more generally to analyze some of the impli- cations of relating visual acuity and contour processes. If distance and timing of borders can be shown to affect visual acuity, a new type of evidence will be brought forth for the processes involved. In order to determine whether this is so, the effects of diverse Spatial and temporal manipulations of borders were analyzed: (1) (2) 25 manipulations of length of borders and distance between them; temporal manipulations of different portions of the target. Within this general framework, Six Specific questions were studied: (1) (2) (5) (4) (5) Would a decrease in the distance between two dark bars retard the detection of a fine line appear- ing in the interSpace between those bars? Similarly, would a dark foreperiod followed by a simultaneous presentation of the bars and line, hinder visual acuity? To what extent would a lighted foreperiod without any border in the target and followed by a simul- taneous projection of the bars and line, affect the detection of this same line? Would a foreperiod with two bars which vanish when the line appears, create much interference upon seeing the line? Would an instantaneous shifting of the positions occupied by the bars while the line is projected, increase the duration required for detecting the line? If so, would the effect be different when the shifting is toward the line than when it is away from it? 24 (6) Finally, would longer bars cancel out the facili- tatory influence of an increase in the length of the line upon visual acuity? The general plan of this research involved testing two trained observers for visual acuity under three different levels of photic intensity, in situations wherein Spatial and temporal manipulations of targets were performed. METHOD Subjects Two subjects were used throughout the whole experi- ment: J.V., a graduate student in Education (male, 55 years old) and the author himself (male, 55 years old). J.Y.L. had his vision corrected for my0pia. As for J.V., he declared, at the beginning of the experiment that he had normal vision. However, a few months after the second part of the experiment was completed, that is in April 1965, he had his eyes examined and a Slight degree of hypermetropia (+1.50 diopter) was found. The implications of this re- fraction defect are discussed in the prOper section of this report. J.V. had been trained thoroughly in May 1964: he then spent more than fifty hours as experimenter or subject in a first attempt at studying contour formation. The data col— lected at that time had to be rejected when the whole pro— cedure was modified later on. However, the experience acquired by both J.V. and J.Y.L. led to more reliable data in the second attempt. AS Wilcox (1952) mentions, visual acuity for fine lines varies with practice. J.V. though well practiced, was not aware of the hypotheses to be tested and he knew vaguely about the process of contour formation. 25 26 Nelson, Bartley and De Hardt (1960) have shown that data obtained from well-trained subjects are alike in pattern and that they are close to the mean found with more naive subjects. In order to check that point, the author, at first. expected to have two naive subjects besides the two trained ones. It was not possible, at this period of the year, to find students who would spend a month or so in the laboratory. Only one subject was found (D.V., male 55 years old, a rela- tive of J.V.). He was trained during two days and then data were collected. However, heirepeatedly failed the test tar- gets (to be described later) and his data had to be rejected entirely. Apparatus and Material The apparatus was the Gerbrands Tachistoscope, with . two illuminated G backs and the associated Dual Timer. Because the illumination of the G backs was not uniform ex- cept for a very small area, the apparatus was slightly modi— fied in the following ways the two neon bulbs, in each G back, instead of being about one inch behind the milk glass, as in the original set-up, have been moved approximately six inches away from the glass; a wooden frame, painted white inside, completed the arrangement. As a result the illumie nation became uniform to an acceptable degree over the entire area allowed by the tachistoscope (7%" x 7%"). Second, the small rubber head-set on the Tachistosc0pe was replaced by a 14" X 20" wooden black board with three 27 sides protruding 5" in order to block any photic radiation coming from other sources than the one to look at. An ad- justable chin-head rest (American Optical Company) was used which was much more reliable and comfortable than the original set-up besides allowing one to wear glasses. When properly installed, the observer has his right eye 24" from the source and viewed the different targets through a 7/8" peephole located 1%" from his eyel: this set-up allowed an angle of vision of 55 degrees. A piece of black paper pro- truding 1" was put to‘the left of the peephole in order to impede any illumination from reaching the left eye. With the right eye closed, only with full illumination on was it possible for the observer's left eye to detect a certain amount of brightness, and it was so low it was assumed not to interfere with the behavior of the right eye during the experiment prOper. The illumination was varied by means of Cinemoid Grey Filters (No. 60) introduced immediately behind the peephole. Calibration of the Dual Timer was accomplished with a K M 1 Short Timer (which could measure from 0.005 msec. to 10 sec. with an accuracy of 2%). Afterwards, the main source 1No artificial pupil was used for fear that it would not be in line with the natural one. As the purpose of the author was not to measure the highest visual acuity attain- able, in which condition the pupil Size would become import— ant, but to study the effetts of borders upon contour forma— tion in visual acuity, the pupil diameter becomes a less important variable. 28 of error resulted from trying to duplicate the different settings: a very Slight variation in the successive settings for a specific value yielded results which differed by several milliseconds. The material consisted of 8%" x 15" transparent (Cellu- loid) cardboards introduced in the G backs. These cardboards supported the fine line to be detected and the dark bars assumed to exert an influence upon its detection. Thirteen targets were prepared for the fore period and twelve for the exposure period. Two more targets, used in exposure period, were test targets. The first five targets were identical as for the size of the bars and of the line, the only factor varied being the distance separating the bars from the line. In target 1 F (fore period), the two bars, which measured 'i" x'é" (56‘ x 10 12') each, were separated 6" (140 14') on the horizontal plane, and occupied the middle of the target on the vertical plane. At the center, two small red disks of approximately 1/20" in diameter and separated by 5/16" served as fixation points. Target 1E (exposure) was identi— cal as for the dark bars, but a fine line made up of copper wire-é" (1o 12') long and .006" (52") thick occupied the center of the target and was parallel to the bars. (This line was cleaned and treated with HgClg in order to become "gray" and have the least reflection possible, even though the illumination was coming from behind.) These two targets are outlined in Figure 1. With this arrangement, the only 29 1 F 1.2 Fig. l. Targets 1 F and l E used in fore and eXposure periods respec- tively. In fore period two dark bars, which measured-fi“ x'é“ (1° 12' x 36'), were 6" (14° 14') apart. Two small "red“ fixation points helped maintain proper fixation. In eXposure period target 1 F vanished, being almost instantaneously replaced by target 1 E in such a way that every- thing seemed unchanged except for the fine line appearing in the middle of the space previously occupied by the two fixation points. (Not drawn t0 80310). 50 change observed when one was switching from the fore period to the exposure period was the vanishing of the red disks and the appearance of the line in the middle of the Space separating those two disks. The dark bars for both periods had to coincide in order to avoid any perceived movement. Accordingly, great care was exercised during the preparation of the targets: measurements of the bars and of the length of the wire were taken by means of a rule graduated in 64th of an inch, and a template and magnifying lens were used to facilitate the work. The thickness of the wire was measured with a Cenco Ratchet Micrometer. In targets 2 F and 2 E the distance between the bars was 2" (4° 46') . One inch (20 25'), s" (10 12') and 5/64" (11') separated the bars in targets 5 (F and E), 4 (F and E), and 5 (F and E) respectively. In target 5 F the dark bars occupied the place where the red disks would normally be, in which case the bars themselves served as fixation points. In the next group of targets the distance between the bars was maintained at 5/64" (11'), but this time the length of the bars was varied. In targets 6 F and 6 E the bars were 4" long (90 51'); in targets 7 F and 7 E they were 2“ long (40 46'), while in targets 8 F and 8 E they were only i“ long (56'). The length of the line in the exposure targets (6 E, 7 E, and 8 E) coincided with that of the bars. In the third group of targets the same lengths were utilized as in the second group, except that this time the 51 distance between the bars was kept at 6" (140 14'). Thus, targets 9 F and 9 E contained bars measuring 4" in length (90 51'), but these were 6" (140 14') one from the other. Targets 10 F and 10 E were 2" long (40 46'), while targets 11 F and 11 E were-&" long (56'). Target 12 F was a black cardboard with two small fix- ation points: two small holes were made in the cardboard and a piece of "red "filter was put behind these fixation points. Target 15 F was tranSparent and contained only the two fixation disks. Target 14 E had no bars, but only the fine line in the middle. This line was é“ long. Two more targets were test targets. One was a piece of transparent celluloid without any line nor bars on it. The other was identical to target 5 E, except that it had no line but only the two bars. In all the targets the area was kept constant in order to rule out any explanation of the results in terms of spatial summation. Preparation of Subjects Generalgpreparation. Because the two observers served as experimenters also, it was essential that J.V. as well as the author be familiar with the psychophysical method used. As said before, J.V. participated in a first attempt made in May 1964. At that time the author explained to him 52 the nature and shortcoming of the method to be used and readings were assigned to him on the topic. As a subject he was asked to use as consistent a criterion as possible and he was warned that test targets, where the line is absent, might be used during the experi- ment. The author as a subject also was aware that test trials would be used once in a while when he would be the observer. No formal written instructions were given the ob- server, but he was cautioned again and again to look at the pair of fixation points, and to indicate in the test period whether or not a line was visible in the direction of the fixation points. It is always to be recognized that attention may vary somewhat and that this may be a factor in the production of variability in the results. In the absence of any marked variability in the results, it is also possible to account, in part, for the results as the target is varied, by saying that attention is different for each target condition. This is another way of saying that the central nervous system organization is varied by the targets and that not all of the systematic differences often attributed solely to retinal patterns of stimulation and/or to neuroretinal activity, is rightly so attributed. Immediategpreparation. At the beginning of each session the subject dark adapted for 50 minutes; for the first 15 or 20 minutes he wore dark adaptor goggles (Picker 55 Panoramic Goggle) and then went in the dark room for the remaining 10 or 15 minutes. Procedure The experiment took place in a 16' x 12‘ dark room. However, two 7.5 watt "red" bulbs were used by the experi- menter to manipulate the targets, operate the timer, and collect data. The observer sat comfortably in front of the apparatus and observed the targets. Before each presentation a signal was given to him. Between trials, he was informed not to look directly at the target, especially when the illumination was high. Experimental situations. The experiment comprised two parts. In Part One, which took place in July and August 1964, six Situations were investigated under three levels of photic intensity (see Figure 2 for an illustration of these situations): (1) The distance between the two dark bars and the line was decreased. The first five sets of targets were used so that the distance of the bars respective to one another was varied in five steps from 140 14' to 11'. (2) The fore period was dark with only two small fix- ation points visible, and the line and bars appeared to— gether in exposure period. Target 12 F as well as targets 1 E and 5 B were utilized. (5) The fore period was lighted but the target did not contain the vertical bars. During the exposure period the SITUATIONS ‘ FP sp FP EP 1 Eli 32 i 3 I l 13 3 .. Ed 5 I E 13F 53 13F 1E E! l i H 1 5r 143 1r us I"H WE Ii IllE 3r 5E 5F 33 III 8F 8 E Fig. 2. The six eXperimental situations examined under three different levels of photic intensity. Not all the conditions investigated in the first and sixth situations are included. I“? is for fore period, E? for OXposure period. 1 F, l‘E, etc, correspond to the different targets described previously in the text. (Not drawn to scale). 55 two bars and the line appeared simultaneously. Target 15 F was used in fore period. The exposure period was identical with the one in the second situation. (4) The two vertical bars were present in fore period, but they vanished and the line appeared alone in exposure period. Targets 1 F and 5 F as well as target 14 E were used. (5) The two bars did not occupy the same position in fore and exposure periods. In one of the conditions targets 5 F and 5 B were used so that a shifting of borders toward the center resulted. In the other condition the use of targets 5 F and 5 E created a shifting of borders toward the periphery. (6) The length of the line and of the two bars was in- creased. Targets 6 F and 6 B through targets 11 F and 11 B were utilized. The length was varied from 90 51' to 56' in three steps. Two distances between the bars (140 14', and 11') were used. Part Two was performed as a check on the reliability of data in those Situations where the two observers dis- agreed. The situations re—examined were: Situations 2, 5 and 5. This part took place approximately six months after the first one, that is in February 1965. The psychophysical method used was the method of limits. At first it was decided that the descending as well as the ascending orders would be used. After a few days of 56 experimentation it was found that the threshold as de- termined by the descending order was consistently higher than it was with the ascending order if the illumination was low and the task difficult. Then the author checked the literature and found that often the ascending order is used exclusively. For example, Wilcox used this order only, arguing that it has "great reliability and consistency" (Wilcox, 1956). Furthermore, there are other arguments which can be used against the descending order; for example, because of habituation one can say he saw the line when in fact he did not see it, but in the ascending order when he sees the line for the first time, he is much less influenced by what he has seen before. Thirdly, the problem of seeing the after-image is much less critical with the ascending order. A minor change in this method was effected: a doubtful answer was considered as a "no" answer. Ten threshold determinations were effected in every case and the threshold of resolution was computed by averag— ing all the ten readings. Order of presentation of targets: in Part One, situ- ations 1, 2, 5, 4, and 5, were tested all together during a same session at three predetermined levels of illumination. The purpose, here, was to avoid day-to-day variations affecting the seeing of the different targets in different ways. As the order of investigation of each situation might 57 affect the performance of the subject, the order of these situations 1, 2, 5, 4, and 5, was determined at random, before each session, by means of a table of random numbers. Also, for any situation, the order of presentation of each target size was predetermined in the same manner. For each target two stimulus threshold determinations was taken in a row and written down. In some instances where J.V. acted as subject, three or four determinations were taken in a row. Also on the first two sessions one or two trials were effected before recording the threshold values. When all the situations were covered at the lowest level of illumination, the experi- menter passed to the next level under a different predeter-i mined order of presentation. The test targets, without any line appearing in the exposure period, were used approximately once every two sessions. A session lasted 5 or 4 hours and it always took place in the afternoon. The last situation (Situation 6) was tested alone at a different session because testing it with all the others would have required too much time (approximately 5 hours). In these circumstances one situation had to be discarded. Situation 6 was chosen because it used targets different from those utilized in the other Situations. As a result, the data obtained for that Situation are not directly comparable with the others. ' In Part Two of the experiment the same procedure was followed, except that only three situations were examined. 58 As a result a session required about half the time needed by one in the First Part. The length of the sessions in Part One is somewhat un- usual. But it is to be remembered that the subject was in- structed to ask for a temporary halt any time he felt he needed one. For this reason the time spent on the task was sensibly less than the one indicated above. Moreover, a comparison of the results in Part One and Part Two, this one being much shorter, does not seem to indicate any detrimental effect due to fatigue. RESULTS For any condition and unless otherwise specified, the results are the averages of ten threshold determinations of which no more than two were taken on the same day. In general the data were stable and consistent. Table 1 presents in raw form typical data from J.Y.L. and J.V. on threshold duration for low photic intensity. At medium and high intensities, the stability of data is even better. Table 1. Raw Data on Threshold Duration Obtained in Some of the SituationS-—for the Two Observers--Photic Intensity .055 c/ft2. Readings in Situation Condition Observer milliseconds Means 2, Dark fore period Distance: 14014' J.Y.L. 55,65,45,55,45 35,55,55,55,55 so J.V. 75,95,65,55,55 25,45,45,35,45 54 1, Distance varied Distance: 4046' J.Y.L. 45,45,55,55,55 45,85,85,55,45 53 J.V. 55,55,55,25,15 45,25,55,55,55 54 In some conditions J.V.‘s data were less stable and the task seemed more difficult for him. As this does not Show in the data chosen at random and presented in Table 1 59 40 above, the two conditions found, upon inspection, to be less stable than the others are reported in Table 2. Table 2. Raw Data Found to Be Less Stable, Photic Intensity .055 c/ft2. Observer J.V. Readings in Situation Condition milliseconds Means 2, Dark fore period Distance: 11' 205,155,565,265,95 65,245,505,175,85 196 5, Shifting of Distancg in 285,175,245,225,85 borders fore: 2 25' 85,145,125,85,65 152 First Situation. Effect on visual acuity of a decrease in the distance between borders.--Figure 5 illustrates the effect of a decrease in the distance between the two dark bars upon detection of the line. The results of J.Y.L. are shown in Figure 5A. First, it can be seen that for any dis- tance examined, the time required for seeing the line de- creased as the illumination was increased from .055 c/ft.2 to 10.97 c/ft.2. Second, the effect of the distance between the bars upon the threshold of detectability is clearly apparent at all the three levels of intensity used. A Kendall coefficient of concordance (W= .85) is significant at P < .02, which means that for this observer the target (distance) that required more time under low illumination also tended to require more time at medium and high photic intensity. Mindanaivwvfiwm“ H._.H F...- 7— H wt A~.H~dm_u\n1~v-~ur HA ed H . H. On.-.~.u.~A H as dwuva.“ MEAN DETECTION THRESHOIDS IN MILLISECOIDS 41 80,, , l B i DISTANCE BETWEEN BARS 70-1 4‘ \ '— _ "‘ “ 11' i \ e— — -- -I 1012' 1 : ‘ .____g 2023' g , \ Ar— — — at 4°46. .0; I \ ~———- 14% i l 3 50-3 i 40a 304 20. J.Y.L. J.V. .oéa .19 15197 ' .053 .33) 173.97 marques (c/rr.2) Fig. 3. Visual acuity as dependent upon the distance of adjacent borders. The distance between the inner borders of the two vertical bars was varied in five steps from 14° 14' of visual angle to 11'. In part A, JeYeLe V88 the observer, and JeVe in part.B. 42 However, the duration required for detectability of the line did not necessarily increase proportionately with a decrease in distance from 140 14‘ to 11'. If one takes the curve obtained for the condition where the distance between the bars subtended 140 14' as the criterion for comparing all the other curves it can be seen that the fine line was perceived more rapidly at 10 12' and 20 25' than at 140 14', the condition where the bars were almost coincident with the frame and as such were assumed not to exert any influence on the detectability of the line. This effect is maintained at all three levels of photic intensity, thus Showing some facilitatory influence of the bars upon visual acuity. When the bars were very close together (11'), however, it took much more time to detect the line, as can be seen by inspecting the upper curve of Figure 5A, which indicates some inhibitory effects of the bars upon visibility of the line. Also, it can be seen that while the effects noticed for all the other distances examined tended to diminish when the photic intensity was increased to 10.97 c/ft.2, the inhibitory effect created by the bars when they were close to one another tended to be comparatively larger at the same intensity. Results of J.V. are shown in Figure 5B. It can be seen that data for this observer are very similar in trend to those of J.Y.L. A Kendall W test (w= .79) was significant at p < .05. 45 As compared to the standard condition, the inhibitory influence of the bars when they were close together is even more apparent here. Yet, the facilitatory influence noticed above is much less evident in Figure 53. For example, at 10 12‘ duration is increased as compared to the standard condition, thus indicating that some inhibitory influence began to appear when the bars were slightly more than 10 apart. At a greater distance (20 25') and with the lowest illumination used there is a slight indication of the facilitatory effect noticed before. The curve corresponding to a distance of 40 46' is almost identical with the criterion, thus showing some sup— port to the assumption that at a distance of 40 the influence of borders upon one another is no longer effective. For this observer also, while the effects of borders at 10 12', 2O 25', and 40 46' seemed to be less marked when the illumination was increased (the curves are very close together at 10.97 c/ft.2), the inhibitory effect of borders at 11' was maintained even at the highest illumination used. Situation 2. Effects of a darkened fore period.-- Besides distances between borders, other manipulations were effected. For example, in one of the Situations the fore period was dark, with only two small fixation points visible and the line and bars appeared together in exposure period. Figure 4A presents the results obtained by J.Y.L. while those of J.V. are shown in Figure 5A. MEAN DETECTION THRESHOLDS IN ldILLISECOICDS 44 60 a 11.0..1 r A _, c 1.0 - 120 j DISTANCE BETWEEN BARS \ 1 -( \ e-— -— ——e 11' 20 . 1004 \ 11.011.‘ .4 - J.Y.L. 0 V 1 fi 80 -1 so -, 40-1 20 1 J.Y.L. J.Y.L. o . , 11 o , fi . .053 .69 10.97 .053 .65 . 10.97 mmnscs (c/r'r.2) Fig. 4. Effects of diverse manipulations of adjacent borders upon detection of the line. Data for J.Y.L. In part A the fore period was unilluminated; in part B it was lighted but contained no vertical bars, while in part 0 the bars vanished in eXposure period. In. dhuA ne— A w\.‘v4.—... .. 1‘ —|..-.. N 1nd w.— H aha—KNAVu— Q.“ .nuv—u THU whAU H. rv‘Vu.~.Huu.HnL—. z < Inn .N 46 By examining Figure 4A one can see that at low illumi— nation it took less time for the observer to detect the line when the bars were close together (11') than when they were far apart (140 14'), suggesting that in some conditions borders near one another exert a facilitatory influence upon visual acuity. At medium intensity no difference was ob— served between the two curves, thus indicating that the distance between the bars did not interfere with visual acuity, while at high intensity there seemed to be a tendency for both curves to raise again, this tendency being slight for the curve representing a distance of 11' but more accentuated in the case of the wide distance. Data for J.V. show a very different pattern depending on the distance of bars (Figure 5A). Thus, if at medium intensity the time required for detection of the line was about the same be the bars close to one another or far apart, one cannot conclude the same thing when low or high illumi— nation were used. At low intensity much more time was required for detecting the line when the bars were near one another. This is the opposite of what was found with J.Y.L. as the observer. At high intensity the trend is the same as the one observed in J.Y.L.'S data, except that the tendency is much more pronounced here; thus, it became much harder for J.V. to detect the line when the bars were far apart. It seems as though in the present situation, borders in near the vicinity of the line improved visual acuity by sub- stantially decreasing the glow affect produced under some 47 conditions when the bars are far apart. This will be further discussed in the prOper section. Because the data of the two observers did not agree, another session took place six months later (February, 1965) in order to check their reliability. In Figure 6A it can be seen that the first observer was fairly consistent over a period of six months. The same tendency is observed. Only for the lowest intensity is there any increase in duration noticed, Specially in the condition where the bars were close together. Equally, the second observer (J.V.) was consistent over the same period. By inSpecting Figure 6C one can notice that the trend remains the same, except that the difference be- tween the two curves representing wide and Short distances respectively were less marked at low intensity than on the first occasion. Situation 5. Effects of a lighted fore period without any borders.--In the present Situation the fore period was lighted but the target did not contain the vertical bars. During the exposure period the two bars and the line appeared Simultaneously. In one condition, the bars were 140 14' apart, and in the other they were close together (11'), as in the previous situation. The curves obtained can be seen in Figures 4B for J.Y.L. and 5B for J.V. In Figure 4B one can again notice the tendency for visual acuity to require less time with an increase in illumination. However, it does g‘ «.4 nnfldéflvuwfhun H. u..~. H414 wk H Hung ~.~.fiJ——MU«V~U——u.fih ZAJH.<.HN.~.H.JHAN 7N < 4. .mm‘ MEAN DETEETION THRESHOLDS IN MILLISECONDS 100 1 D 80 T 60 . DISTAhOE BETWEEN BARS .. } 140 14! 1.0 1 o ___________ o} 11 J 20 - \\ - \ J.Y.L. \ 0 . Q \ I“‘ \ .“ \ 80 l '. \ \ -< X. \ 60 4 \ J \ .“ \ 4o 4 ‘ \ 20- .0 J.Y.L. J.V. J.V. 0 I T I O r j —1 0053 .69 10.97 .053 .053 .69 10.97 LUMINANCE (c/rr.2) Fig. 6. Comparisons of sets of data collected at a six month interval. Portions A and C represent, for J.Y.L. and J.V. respectively, the situation where the fore period was dark, and portions B and D stand for the one where the fore pe- riod was illuminated but contained no bars. The solid lines represent the con- dition where the bars were far apart, and the broken ones where they were close together. The’closed circles represent data gathered in July 1964, and the open circles data for the same conditions collected six months later. 49 not seem that the presence or absence of borders near the line differently affected visual acuity when these borders were presented at the same time as the line. A Sign Test for paired replicates was not significant at p= .05. If one looks at Figure 5B, he can observe a clear dif- ference between the curve representing data when bars were close together and the one for a distance of 14C 14': thus, it took J.V. much more time to see the line when the bars were close to it than when far apart. This is not in agree- ment with results for J.Y.L. Because the two observers did not agree when tested on the conditions depicted here, a retest was effected six months later. The results obtained on the second occasion appear in Figures 6B and 6D. Both observers were consistent over that period, the only difference worth noting being for J.V. a certain improvement in detecting the line when the bars were close to it, and for J.YLL. a slight but con- sistent increase in time for the three levels of photic intensity, as one can see by comparing the curves drawn with open circles with those drawn with closed circles. Situation 4. Effects of disappearipg_borders upon detection of the line.-_Another situation was one in which the vertical bars, which were present in fore period, vanished and the line appeared alone in exposure period. The results for J.Y.L. are shown in Figure 4C, and those for J.V. in Figure 5C. Here the two observers Show a very similar pattern 50 of data. For the two of them there was an improvement in visual acuity when the intensity was increased from .055 c/ft.2 to 10.97 c/ft.2. For the two observers also, detection of the line required more time when the borders of the bars were near one another, thus suggesting a strong inhibitory influence in such a condition. Situation 5. Effects of a shift in the position of the bars in fore and exposure periods.--In the present Situ— ation, the dark bars occupied a predetermined position in fore period and a different one in exposure period. As the switching from fore to exposure was almost instantaneous, there was a shifting of borders from one position to another. In one of the two conditions examined the bars were set at 20 25' from one another in fore period and at 11' in ex- posure period. In the other, the reverse was done. The results obtained with these manipulations are shown in Figure 7. In portion A the curve represented by closed triangles has been obtained when the borders were 20 25' apart in pre—exposure and 11' in exposure periods. The closed circles represent the curve obtained when the borders were 11' apart in pre—exposure and 20 25' in exposure. A comparison of those two curves indicates that when borders were Shifting away from the line, this one required much more time to be detected than when they were shifting toward it. A replication of Situation 5 Six months later gave the two curves represented by open circles and open mu.~.~.ew.w.:nh.—.~HT- 2H qu~.~A::n.&:—:.F .nn,.r.rr..h....n;> 2...;i.‘ MEAN DETECTION THRESHOLDS IN MILLISECONDS 160-1L 9 140 d .e‘ 1204 \ ‘ 100— \ \ 201 i .053 Fige 70 l .69 51 j 10.97 —l — (DISTANCE bsrwzsn BARS IN EXP.FERIOD) 1...“. _ '_""”"" } 2° 23! o- ---------- -e J.V. .653 .65 10397 LUMINANCE (C/FT.2) Visual acuity as affected by a shifting of position of adja- cent borders in fore and exposure periods. Portion A represents data for JeYeLe and portion B data for JeVe shifting of bars toward the line, while the broken lines represent a shifting away from the line. The Open triangles and circles are for the retest, made six months later, of the conditions represented by closed triangles and circles. The crosses display data showing poor stability (see text for eXplanation). The solid lines represent a 52 triangles. Again the stability of the results is well main— tained, the tendency is the same, the only difference being a higher threshold for the two conditions where photic intensity was .055 c/ft.2. For medium and high intensities there is an overlapping of curves obtained over the period of six months. Data for the second observer are illustrated in part B of Figure 7. The results yielded by the condition where the shifting was toward the line (identified by crosses) and the condition where it was away from it (identified by closed circles) were about the same- At that time (July 1964) an inspection of the data indicated a noticeable tendency toward a decrease in duration as the experiment went on (Table 2, p. 40). To check that point the A. took several more data for this situation as well as for some others. While the tendency remained the same for the other situations, it definitively changed in the condition where shifting was toward the line. The curve identified by closed triangles shows other results based on eight threshold determinations for low and medium photic intensities, and six determinations for the high level of intensity. A valid explanation of this marked decrease in duration is not easy: the targets have been carefully inSpected and there was nothing wrong with them. One possibility is that the observer did not fixate properly at first; he may have been distracted by the apparent movement of the two bars and may have learned, after several trials, to look only at the fixation points, 55 not the bars. To check that assumption, J.Y.L. tried, later on, to bear his attention on the bars and he found out that apparent movement became more objectionable and that visual acuity was much impoverished. A replication of the whole situation conducted six months later showed essentially the same trend for the con- dition where the bars were set at 20 25' in exposure period (curves with closed and open circles). When the bars were close together in exposure period the data were almost identical with those obtained as a check and based on eight determinations (curves represented by open and closed tri- angles in part B of Figure 7). With this taken into consider— ation it can be seen that the two observers agreed in their tendency to require more time for seeing the line when the borders of the bars shifted away from it. Finally, an overall inspection of Figure 7 shows that whatever process was acting at the time, there was a marked decrease in duration required for detection of the line as photic intensity was increased. Situation 6. The effect of length of the line and bars upon visual acuity.--In the present situation three different lengths of bars and line were used. Portions A and B of Figure 8 present the results for the first observer (J.Y.L.) and portions C and D present those for J.V. In part A the distance was kept at 140 14'. The length of the line here became a significant factor in visual acuity, MEAN DETECTIOI‘J THRESHOLDS IN MILLISECOIDS 54 LENIfl1CW'BARS 60~ ‘ . A . x, B x—————at9°31' 40 - “.\ .. - K ‘ _ \ 20 d .. Distance 14° 14' l Distance 11' J.Y.L. J.Y.L. O I l I l I I 1 60 ‘1 - x .1 ‘\ 40 -4 “x .. \ «r "x \ ‘ ‘ - \ ‘ Distance 140 14' “ Distance 11' J.V. J.V. .653 .5 16197 .353 .89 £9.97 MIINANCE (c/FT.2) Fig. 8. The relation between bars length and visual acuity for two different distances of the here. In parts A and G the two vertical bars are kept at a distance of 11.0 14', while in portions B and D they are at 11' of visual angle. Data for J .Y .L. are displayed in parts A and B and those of J .V. in parts 0 and D. 55 eSpecially at low intensity where the required duration decreased With an increase in length from 56' to 90 31'. This is in agreement with results of Hecht, Ross & Mueller (1947), and Ogilvie & Taylor (1959). It can be seen that when the length does not subtend at least 10 the threshold of detection raises sharply with a decrease in illumination, as Hecht and collaborators have found. In part B, the same bars and line were used, but the distance between the bars was kept at 11‘. A comparison of the three lengths used does not show any differential effect due to the length of the line and bars. It seems as though there was a slight reduction in required duration when the line and bars were very short, but this is about all. The six conditions (three lengths at two different distances) have been submitted to the Kendall coefficient of concord— ance (W) and this one has been found to be significant at p < .05. So, the rank of each condition tended to remain the same over the three levels of photic intensity used. Figure 9A, B, C, is a different illustration of the phenomenon. Here, instead of comparing the effects of a difference in length upon detection of the line, the A. compared the effects of distance. Hence, part A of Figure 9 represents the influence of borders of long bars (90 51') upon visual acuity first when these bars were near one another (11'), and second, when they were far apart (140 14‘). Part B illustrates the same phenomenon for bars of medium ECTION THRESHOLDS IN MILLISECOIJB MEAN DET 56 65., q 60‘ DISTANCE 51;?me ems ’ "~ \ ‘ \. x\ F--—-——x ll' \\\ .4 \\ F—‘X 140 14c .. \ \\ X\ 40- \ q \ \\ A \ D . .K‘ \\ \~ “ \ 20~ ‘x e d) J'YSI" ‘ J.V. Length 9° 31' Length 90 31: o . , fl . . fi 60- _ x l \ - \\ B d E \ \ Xf— _____ —- x 40- \\ - \\\ q ‘\\x\ ‘ ‘\ “~x 20' - J.Y.L. ‘ J.V. Length 4° 46' Length 4° 46' o _ l A ' I ‘1 v v ~—j 60-, . F 40‘ - 20~ a ‘ ‘ J.V. Length 36' Length 36' 0 j l I ~——l .053 .89 10.97 .053 .69 10.97 matures ( C/FT.2 ) tance between F1 . 9 Visual acuity as dependent upon bar length and dis ths bars. In parts A and D the length of the bars and line subtends 9° 31', in parts B and E 4° 46', and in portions C and F 36'. In each case the broken line represents the narrowly spaced bars (11'), the solid line, the widely spaced ones (14° 14'). 57 length (40 46'), and portion C for short bars (36'). An overall inspection of the three graphs shows that when the bars were close to the line, whatever the length of the line, this one required a fairly long time in order to be detected. When the bars were far apart, however, the duration required decreased as the line extended. Finally, the facilitating effect of an increase in luminance is clearly apparent in this situation also. Data obtained when J.V. was the observer are in general similar to those of the former observer. A Kendall coefficient of concordance is significant at p < .01. Figure 8C shows the effects of increasing the length of the line when the bars were very far apart (and considered to be nonexistent). At low illumination eSpecially the time was reduced when the length was increased. Figure 8D resembles Figure 8B for medium and high intensities. At low illumination, however, duration was increased for seeing the long line as compared to the other two lengths. Parts D, E, F show the same trend notices for J.Y.L., that is when the bars were far apart, the length of the line seemed to be a decisive factor for its visibility. When the bars were close to it, time was comparatively in- creased. While for J.Y.L. this effect was manifested at all three levels of photic intensity, it was more pronounced at medium intensity in the case of the second observer. 58 To this point, the results yielded by the six experi- mental situations have been reviewed separately. Figures 10 and 11 present an overall comparison of all the situations, with the exception of Situation 6 which, using different lengths of line and bars is not directly comparable. Thus in Figure 10, which represents data for J.Y.L., a comparison of Situations 1 and 2, when the bars wereofar apart (first and second blocks of columns from the left) indicates that a darkened fore period shortened the time required for visual acuity at medium intensity only: at this level it took the observer 5 milliseconds to detect the line when the fore period was unilluminated as compared to 22 milliseconds in the reference situation (Situation 1). When the distance between the bars subtended ll' (fifth and sixth blocks from the left), it became much easier to detect the line when the pre-exposure was darkened than when it was illuminated and contained the two bars. This facilitatory effect was maintained over the three levels of intensity used. There was not much difference, except at low illumi- nation, between Situations 1 and 5 when the distance between the bars was wide (first and third blocks from the left), but when it subtended only 11' (fifth and seventh blocks) a lighted fore period without any borders required less time for detection of the line as compared to a fore period with the presence of borders. These results suggest that borders 59 .A.w.h you open .pOHsoQ ohdmOQXo ca pom: one one ma poosoacca conspmfip one .m soapsSpflm mom .mcoapsspwm anacoaauuexo say we o>am you mvHonmousp cowpsnso mo comwpsdsoo .OH .mah m z o H e d D a H m w H e m as m SGNOOESITTIW NI SGHOHSHHHL NOILOHESH NVSW mb ZDHQHE WWW Om. . mon flHQ “OH whHmszZH r A‘l \ r 1W \ r 1‘ L ONH MN 0N .HH .VH OQH «ma mm