H J! J \ {\ ‘I H H W f \ UH’ I I' _' I U) AN EXPLORATORY WHEY 0F MASCULINHY AND THE ATTRIIUFEON OF DOMENANCE AND LOVE TO PARENTS “was $09 Wu Dogma 05 pk ,D. HECHHMN SMW WW?“ deh R. Swan 1965 mzsxs ‘ t . .. ' “’M' J LIBRAR 2’ Michigan- State University This is to certify that the ‘ thesis entitled A'N EXPLORATORY STUDY OF MASCULINITY AND THE ATTRIBUTION OF DOMINANCE AND LOVE TO PARENTS presented by Ralph R . Swarr has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for __P_h_._D_.__ degree him Major professor Date \/\j \LC‘ \‘\ A? Q Ul AA» is 0-169 ROOM USE ONLY 33— W7 ABSTRACT AN EXPLORATORY STUDY OF MASCULINITY AND THE ATTRIBUTION OF DOMINANCE AND LOVE TO PARENTS p( \pfl’ ‘ by Ralph RT Swarr The present study was designed to investigate some of the phenomena subsumed under the concept of identifi- cation and to test empirically hypotheses suggested by identification process theories. A major assumption of the research was that an individual's perception of his parents as reflected by the interpersonal behavior traits he attributes to them will function as a measure of his degree of identification withthe parent who has been more influential in this process. The general prediction was that the degree of adoption of masculine behavior patterns in adult males will be positively related to their attribu— tion to their fathers of interpersonal behavior traits of dominance and love and negatively related to their attribu- tion of interpersonal traits of dominance and love to their mothers. Ninety—two male undergraduate university students. served as volunteer, paid subjects. The students used the Interpersonal Check List once to describe their mother and Ralph R. Swarr once to describe their father. This Check List includes 128 words and phrases which describe various kinds of inter- personal behaviors. Responses to this instrument were analyzed so that summary scores representing the behaviors of dominance and love could be derived. The subjects also completed the first 420 items of the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory. The Masculinity—Femininity scale scores of the Inventory were the independent variables of this study. Total scores from this scale were assumed to be a measure of masculine-feminine role behavior and interest preference. The final task of the subjects involved the completion of a Family Information Questionnaire which pro— vided information for excluding the protocols of subjects whose parents were deceased or divorced. The data were computer analyzed to obtain multi— correlation coefficients between the measures of masculinity— femininity, mother—dominance, mother-love, father-dominance, and father—love. It was found that no significant positive relationship existed between the subjects' preference for masculine behavior and their attribution of dominant and loving behavior to their fathers as was hypothesized. The prediction that there would be a significant negative re- lationship between the preference for masculine behavior and the attribution of dominant and loving behavior to mothers was also not supported by the data. Instead a Ralph R. Swarr significant positive relationship between degree of mascu- linity and attributed mother-dominance was found. Also, a significant negative relationship was found between the two dependent variables, parent-dominance and parent-love, as they were attributed to a given parent; and this relation- ship was found with respect to the subjects' attributions to both parents. The results were discussed in terms of behavior rein— forcement, personality theory and related to some findings of sociological theorists. It was concluded: (1) that the basic assumption of the research was not tenable in the light of the findings, (2) that the results represented a challenge to rather than support of the central proposition of identification theories which stresses the importance of the paternal figure as a model and crucial influence on the development of sex-typed interests and behaviors in males, and (5) that the basic learning mechanisms postu— lated by learning and role theorists as well as reinforce— ment, personality theorists about the development of masculine sex-role preference and behaviors may be the most pertinent interpretation of the findings. Q/VVL/LVL ‘Uiélll / j A/ a f \jfié( (/1ka V L/L7M C/W MW?) [965 AN EXPLORATORY STUDY OF MASCULINITY AND THE ATTRIBUTION OF DOMINANCE AND LOVE TO PARENTS BY Ralph R? Swarr A THESIS Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Psychology 1965 Dedicated with love to JANE ii ACKNOWLEDGMENTS Gratitude is expressed to Dr. Bill Kell, the thesis committee chairman, for his significant contribution to the author's understanding of human behavior and for the directed freedom he allowed the writer with reSpect to the planning, execution, and reporting of the present research. Thanks are also given to the other committee members, Drs. Norman Abeles, Harry Grater, and Bertram Karon, for their constructive comments and suggestions. The author also wishes to thank Mr. James Clark for serving as a consultant with respect to the use of the com- puter programs and facilities. The use of the program written by Mr. Joel Stutz was also greatly appreciated. Gratitude is expressed to the students who volunteered to serve as subjects for this research. Without their co— operation the study would not have been completed according to the planned schedule. Finally, appreciation is expressed to the author's wife, Jane. She was able to understand the demands of this work upon her husband, and she gave of her time for the typing of the manuscript. iii TABLE OF CONTENTS I. INTRODUCTION . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . II. REVIEW OF THE LITERATURE . . . . . . . . . . . The Concepts of Identification and Sex- Typing . . .‘. . . . . . . . . . . . Theories of Identification. . . . . . . . Relevant Research . . . . . . . . . . . . The Problem . . . . . . . . . . . . . . III. THE INSTRUMENTS OF INVESTIGATION . . . . . . Interpersonal Check List. . . . . . . . . Masculinity-Femininity Scale of the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory. . . . . . . . . . . . . . IV. HYPOTHESES . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . V. METHOD . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . The Sample. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Procedure . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Scoring of the Instruments. . . . . . . . VI. RESULTS. . . . . . . . . ... . . . . . . . . . Comparisons Between the Sample Groups . . Relation of the Results to the Hypotheses Relation of the MF Scale to the Ego Strength Scale . . . . . . . . . . . VII. DISCUSSION . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . The Findings. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Theoretical Implications. . . . . . . . . Implications for Future Research. . . . . VIII. SUMMARY. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . BIBLIOGRAPHY. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . APPENDICES. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . iv Page 12 15 17 17 20 25 24 24 26 27 50 50 32 57 58 58 59 48 50 55 56 TABLE II. III. IV. LIST OF TABLES Page Criteria for and number of eliminations of subject protocols from the analyses of the sample Groups . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 25 Results of the t—test for the significance of differences between the mean scores of the Introductory Psychology and the Personality Theory subjects with reSpect to the variables Masculinity-Femininity (MFT, dominance, love and Ego Strength. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 51 Intercorrelation matrices for the Masculinity- Femininity (MMPI) and the dominance and love (ICL) scores of the Introductory Psychology (N = 26) and the Personality Theory (N = 66) groups. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 55 Intercorrelation matrix for the Masculinity- Femininity (MMPI) and the dominance and love (ICL) scores of the combined Introductory Psychology and Personality Theory groups (N = 92). . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 54 Correlations between MMPI Masculinity—Feminin— ity (MF) and Ego Strength scores of the Introductory Psychology and Personality Theory groups and both groups combined . . . . . . . . 57 APPENDIX A. LIST OF APPENDICES General Instructions . . . . . . . . . . The Interpersonal Check List — Form IV . Family Information Questionnaire . . . . -True—false Items of the’Masculinitye Femininity Scale of the Minnesota Multi— phasic Personality Inventory . . . . . vi Page 57 59 61 65 I. INTRODUCTION The process of identification in children and adolescents has been the subject of a considerable amount of theorizing and empirical research. Stemming from this research and associated with it has been an increasing number of theoretical speculations about the nature of the process of identification and some of its antecedents, concomitants, and consequences in terms of parent—child interpersonal behavior. Also, it has generally been conceived that one of the goals or consequences of the identification process is sex—typing, i.e., the acquisi- tion of the behavior and personality characteristics of one's own sex. The implication of these propositions is apparent for investigations of the interpersonal behavioral character- istics of subjects and their parents. Since Freud first introduced the term identification and later more extensively discussed it (Freud, 1924) as a mechanism for the resolution of the Oedipus complex, the concept has taken on a variety of meanings depending on the context in which it is used. Tolman (1945), Stoke (1950). Sanford (1955), and others have noted the many meanings attached to the term. Sanford has stated, "Why not agree that identification is not an explanatory concept... (p. 107)," and in his evaluation of the usefulness of the term, he re—' defined its boundaries. The increasing diffusion of the meaning of identifi— cation has increased the implications of the concept beyond those of Freud's hypothesis to include those of others who have given their own meanings to the term. The positive result of these varied implications has been a broadened base of research about identification theory along with some provo- cative new theoretical speculations about the familial inter— personal antecedents and consequences of the identification process. Kagan (1958), McCandless (1961), and Lynn (1962) are among those who have attempted to abstract some of the phenomena of identification from empirical findings.. It is generally conceded that one of the consequences or goals of identification is adoption of the appropriate sex-role. Another proposition frequently advanced is that important antecedents of identification are found in the nature and quality of familial interpersonal behaviors. The identifi- cation process is also generally believed to vary along such dimensions as degree of adoption of the appropriate sex-role and amount of parental and cultural influence. The present study is designed to investigate some of the phenomena subsumed under the concept of identification and to test empirically hypotheses suggested by identification process theories. The purpose is to study the relationship between the sex—role preference of a group of male subjects and the interpersonal behavior traits they attribute to their parents. II. REVIEW OF THE LITERATURE The Concepts of Identification and Sex-Typing Stoke (1950) and others have credited Freud for the introduction of the term identification into the psychological literature. Freud postulated two aspects of identification-— primary identification referred to the infant's initial, undifferentiated perception in which an external object is perceived as part of the self, and secondary identification was viewed as only beginning after the child had learned to discriminate a world of objects separate from himself. Secondary identification is the concept most relevant to the context and purposes of this review. Stoke (1950) made the following statement about Freud's use of the term identifi- cation: From the wealth of context in which the term is used it is usually implied that a child gives its emotional allegiance to one of its parents and attempts to duplicate in its own life the ideas, attitudes, and behavior of the parent with whom it is identifying. There are occasional other uses of identification in Freudian literature aside from the above, but this is the chief usage and the one with which we shall be concerned (p. 165). Freud, in essence, referred to identification as a process in which the individual takes into himself the psychological attributes of other people. Also relevant is Bronfenbrenner's (1958) effort to emphasize that Freud referred to identification 5 as a tendency for a child to "take on" the total pattern of a model and not merely its discrete aSpects, and he noted the marked emotional intensity by which Freud thought this pattern is acquired. Sanford (1955) noted the many meanings that have be— come attached to the concept identification. In an attempt to clarify the meaning of the term, he made a distinction between "identification proper” and “real identification." He defined the former as a more or less unconsciously determined maladaptive attempt to handle parental conflicts N by attempting to behave exactly like the object (i.e., to be identical with another) and the latter as "essentially an integration of parental standards and modes of behavior in a stable ego... (p. 110)" because they serve useful, adaptive purposes. Real identification represents the use of the term most pertinent to the purposes of the present study. Sears, Maccoby, and Levin (1957) define identification or sex-typing as the development of complex social and emo- tional behavior which is appropriate to the individual's sex. They view sex—typing as a result of covert or fantasy role practice on the part of the child in which all of the feelings, attitudes, values, and actions of a model are taken on at least momentarily. Finally, Mussen and Distler (1959) make the following statement: By identifying with his parents... the child begins to acquire his parent's characteristics, behavior, values, motivations, and attitudes. Two of the major consequences of the process of identification are the development of the super ego, or conscience, and the acquisition of behavior and personality characteristics appropriate to his own sex, i.e., sex-typing (p. 550). Theories of Identification The present review includes the following theories which represent three viewpoints about the identification process for males: The Defensive Theory of Identification, The Learn— ing Theory of Identification, and The Role Theory of Identi— fication. The Defensive Theory of Identification Psychoanalytic theory includes the concept "primary identification" which refers to the initial undifferentiated perception of the infant boy in which an external object is perceived as part of the self. As soon as the infant is able to perceive his environment, he will attempt to relate to his surroundings through the manipulation of objects and the incorporation into his mouth of objects with which he has con— tact. Munroe (1955) defined Freud's usage of the concept as follows: The yearning for release from unpleasant stimulation and from desire is, of course, very closely associated with the mother. At first the process takes the form of primary identification, that is, the infant does not distinguish clearly between the mother's breast (then the face and gradually the person of the mother) and his own self (p. 181). In addition to the incorporative aspect of primary identification, the infant begins a process of imitation of vhat is perceived and manipulated as a primitive attempt at the mastery of the intense stimuli which surround him. Fenichel (1945) views incorporation and imitation of objects as behaviors which are the beginning of object relations, and in elaborating this developmental phenomenon, he stated: The concept of primary identification denotes that actually "putting into the mouth“ and "imitation for perception's sake" are one and the same and represent the very first relation to objects. In this primary identification, instinctual behavior and ego behavior are not differentiated from each other (p. 57). ". 7‘” ’1 Secondary identification follows primary identification at a later stage in the child's development. At this period he is better able to cope with his biological needs, and his attention is more focused on the discovery of himself as a social being. His interaction with "significant others," both parents and peers, has much emotional meaning for him; and he has learned to know the seemingly powerful parental figures on whom his sense of well—being depends. He has also become aware of the nuances of their relationship with him that may signify affective acceptance or rejection. This is the period and the milieu in which Freud (1949) postulated that the Oedipus complex occurs. He wrote suc— cinctly about this as follows: The boy enters the Oedipus phase; he begins to manipu— late his penis, and simultaneously has phantasies of carrying out some sort of activity with it in relation to his mother; but at last, owing to the combined effect of a threat of castration and the spectacle of women's lack of a penis, he experiences the greatest trauma of his life, and this introduces the period of latency with its attendant consequences (Pp. 29—50). The boy thus experiences ambivalent feelings of love and hate for his father who is also his rival for his mother's affection and the person from whom he fears imaginary castration. In order to defend against this great "trauma of his life“ and the intense anxiety associated with it, the boy represses his incestuous desires for his mother and identifies with his father. Anna Freud (1957) contributed to the meaning of this identification process in her discussion of "identification with the aggressor." She wrote the following: By impersonating the aggressor, assuming his attri— butes or imitating his aggression, the child trans— formed himself from the person threatened into the person who makes the threat (p. 121). Thus, in the face of his anxiety, the young boy discovers that if he allies himself with the aggressive, punitive father, the threat is reduced; and he approaches an oneness or ”identity" with the powerful parent. He does not, however, incorporate the father but rather the attitudes, values, and/or behavior of this figure. While Freud postulated that the successful resolution of the Oedipus complex constitutes a vital influence on the development of masculinity in the boy's character, he also wrote that the child's views of his parents change as he grows older and that "identifications take place with these later editions of the parents as well, and regularly provide important contributions to the formation of character (1955, p. 92)." Learning Theory of Identification Mowrer (1950) is one of several theorists who have postulated views of the identification process in the context of learning theory. He made a distinction between what he calls "developmental“ and "defensive" identification processes. The former is accomplished via imitation and is motivated by the love and admiration of the parents. The latter is an attempt to reduce anxiety caused by fears of punishment and is synonymous with Anna Freud's (1957) idea of "identification with the aggressor." Developmental identification is essentially a result of a conditioning process in which rewards play a crucial role. The infant senses (i.e., sees, feels, hears, and smells) aspects of the person who is the satisfier of his basic needs, and via a conditioning process these stimuli acquire secondary reinforcement value. The child's first basic skills of Speech, walking, and other activities are learned because he earns parental rewards for his initial efforts in these activities. Mowrer believes that identification with the father stems from a positive and affectionate relationship between the father and the son. The characteristics and responses of a father who is also an important source of affection and nurtur— ance will acquire secondary reward properties, and consequently the boy will imitate the father's behavior in order to “reproduce bits of the beloved and longed-for parent (1950, p. 615)." Mowrer hypothesizes that defensive identification begins at about age two or three. At this time the parents begin to punish the child for some of his behavior instead of generally only meeting his wants, and the child no longer views his parents as all ”good" but also as partly "bad." This combi— nation of attitudes on the part of the child precipitates marked anxiety, conflict, and ambivalence at the height of which he discovers that he can reduce his internal conflicts and also satisfy his parents by accepting as his own their standards of behavior and social values. Once he has intro- jected the parental standards for himself, he tends to experience self punishment or guilt when he violates these standards. Mowrer believes that initially both sexes identify with the mother as the source of the most care and affection, and that later males shift their loyalties and affections to a nurturant and rewarding father and learn to behave "along lines prescribed as correct and proper for members of their particular sex (1950, p. 579)." Role Theory of Identification Role theorists view identification or sex—typing as the development of social and emotional behavior patterns appro— priate to the child's sex. They believe that sex-roles are learned under basic conditions of motivation, performance, 10 and reinforcement. A major motive for such learning is the child's desire to reproduce pleasant experiences, and he will enjoy playing the parental role in fantasy if the parental behavior has been supporting, accepting, and nurturant (Sears, Maccoby, and Levin, 1957). Emphasis is also placed on the dominance and involvement of parental models for the assimi— lation of sex—role behavior. It is maintained that identifi— cation, or role playing, depends on the power of the identi- ficand —— a combination of his reward value and his threat or punishment potential. Brim (1958) made the following more specific statement: Given two... persons with whom one interacts and who differ in power over the actor (the identifier), i.e., differ in the degree to which they control rewards and punishments for the actor, one would predict that the actor would adopt many of the characteristics of the powerful as compared to the less powerful other person. This follows from the fact that it is more important to the actor to predict the behavior of the powerful figure, that he is motivated more strongly to take his role (i.e., to identify), that rewards and punishments are more impressive and the learning consequently more impressive (p. 5). Role theorists generally believe that the boy initially identifies with his mother because she is typically the source of his greatest satisfaction during his early experience. However, very soon sex—role behavior becomes important in the thinking of the parents, and he is strongly encouraged to behave according to masculine role standards. Thus, Sarbin (1954) states: Early in life, a child is taught acts which are dif— ferentiated for example according to sex. Little boys are rewarded or punished for certain kinds of actions; 11 little girls are rewarded or punished for other kinds of actions. Thus begins the acquisition of actions for the later performance of one's generalized sex—role (p. 226). Thus, the boy receives rewards from both parents for masculine behavior. However, role theorists emphasize that a strong motivation for the boy to practice his father's role also occurs when there is considerable interaction and involvement with a father who is viewed as a powerful administrator of rewards and punishments. When this occurs, the boy obtains valuable father—role practice and consequently begins to adopt more of the father's characteristics including those associated with sex—role behavior (Sarbin, 1954). To summarize, it can be stated that each of the reviewed theories have placed some importance on the behavior of both parents and/or the child's perception of that behavior in the development of identification. The defensive identification point of View emphasizes the child's ambivalent and fearful feelings toward his father which are resolved by aligning himself with his father and his behavior patterns and values in order to reduce his anxiety. Learning theorists postulate that identification occurs with a model who is both aggressive as well as rewarding, but they believe that the male tends to adopt the values and behavior patterns of a paternal figure with whom they have experienced warm and affectionate relation— ships. Role theorists note the reward value of both parents, but they emphasize that the dominance and involvement of a 12 paternal figure who is both rewarding and punitive is crucial for the boy's masculine identification and subsequent adoption of male-role behavior patterns. Relevant Research A brief review of studies involving the examination of the relationships between parent-child behavioral phenomena and the degree of male—role adoption is reported below. While there has been very little such research testing the defensive identification theory, a number of researchers have reported findings which appear to support the learning and role theories of identification process leading to male sex—role behaviors. Sears (1955) studied the relationship between child— rearing factors and sex-typed role playing in children of kindergarten age. Antecedent factors were obtained from rat— ings of interviews with the mothers of the subjects. She found that boys who tend to play the father—role in doll—play activi— ties had fathers who were warm and permissive in their child— rearing behavior according to the mothers' reports. To the extent that doll—play behavior is an index of male—role preference, it may be inferred that warm and permissive paternal behavior influenced these boys toward the preference for masculine behavior. Sears, Pintler, and Sears (1946) reported that pre—school aged boys displayed a delay in acquisition of appropriate male—sex behavior, as measured by projective doll—play re— sponses, when the father was absent from the home. However, 15 they also found that the fathers' presence in the home did not necessarily result in the boys' adoption of masculine behavior, but was dependent on the extent to which the father was involved and/or a dominant influence in meeting the affectional and other needs of the boy. Using male college students as subjects, Azimi (1964) studied the relationship between the preference for specific roles and behavior patterns (as measured by the MMPI MF Scale) and the perception of parents as projected in stories to TAT pictures. He reported that men who preferred culturally accepted masculine roles and behavior patterns told stories involving more warm and salient father—son relationships than those subjects who preferred more feminine roles and behavior. The latter group of men told stories of mother—son relation— ships which were rated as more warm and salient than stories told by the former group of subjects. It was concluded that both warmth and saliency of parent—son relationships were significant factors for the development of masculine or feminine interest patterns in adult males. Mussen and Distler (1959) administered the IT Scale for Children (a projective test including 56 cards) as a measure of sex—typing to a group of kindergarten boys. Structured doll—play behavior and responses to incomplete family situ- ation stories were rated in order to measure the amount of nurturance, punishment, and power (a combination of nurturance and punishment scores) attributed to each of the parents 14 individually and as units. They found that young boys who were strongly aligned with the male role perceived their fathers as more rewarding and nurturant than their more weakly identified peers did. They interpreted this finding as sup— port for the developmental theory as formulated by Mowrer. They also reported that strongly masculine sex—typed boys perceive their fathers as more punitive than those boys who were less aligned with the male role. This finding would seem to lend support to the defensive identification hypothesis. Finally, they found that boys who were high in masculinity perceived their fathers as more rewarding and nurturant and more punitive (as indicated by the power scores) than their less masculine peers perceived their fathers. They interpreted this finding as indicating support for role—theory which maintains that the child is most likely to assimilate the role behavior of an individual with whom he has intensive inter— actions and whom he perceives as a dominant and powerful figure. Mussen and Distler (1960), in a follow—up study with kindergarten boys, examined the relationship between masculine sex-typing (as measured by the IT Scale) and their parents' child rearing practices (as judged from interviews with the mothers). They reported that the variables of father—son relationships are more directly associated with appropriate sex—typing than are those pertinent to mother—son relationships. Also, according to the mothers' reports, the fathers of the 15 highly masculine group had strong affectional bonds, and acted more affectionately toward their sons, than the fathers of boys low in masculinity. These findings were interpreted as supportive to the developmental and role—playing hypotheses, although the defensive hypothesis was not supported. The Problem Each of these researchers has considered masculine sex—typed behavior of males as a consequence of successful identification with the same-sexed parent, although they have generally recognized the influence of the cross—sex parent 'w as well as non—familial persons or events on the development of sex—role behavior patterns. While it would seem likely that in adults much of culturally accepted masculine behavior would be learned by the male from peers and others besides his father, it also is plausible that the inclination or amenability for learning and adopting Such behavior would have antecedents in the familial milieu and interaction. The reported findings tend to support the following contentions: (1) the adoption of appropriate sex—role be— havior in males is influenced by the degree of dominance, strength, and involvement of a father who may at times be either rewarding or punitive and by the degree of love, warmth, and affection which a father displays in his relation— ship with his son; (2) the adoption of sex—typed behavior is not an either-or proposition, but can vary along such di— mensions as degree of adoption and parental influence; and 16 (5) the dominance as well as love and warmth of a mother in her relationships with her son has an important influence on his sex—typed behavior. A central proposition suggested by identification theory and the research findings is that adult behavioral patterns have antecedents related to the identification pro- cess. In other words, an individual's adoption of sex-role behavioral patterns may be assumed to reflect the strength of his earlier identification with a same—sex or a cross- sex parent. Q In this study an exploratory evaluation of this propo- sition is made by investigating the relationships between the sex—typed behavior patterns of a group of male subjects and the interpersonal behavior traits they attribute to their parents. It is assumed that an individual's perception of his parents as reflected by the interpersonal behavior traits he attributes to them will function as a measure of the degree of his identification with the parent who has been more in— fluential in this process. The following general hypothem sis is tested in the present study: The degree of adoption 9£_masculine behavioral patterns in adult males lg positively related to their attribution to their fathers of interpersonal behavioral traits of dominance and love and negatively related to their attribution of interpersonal traits of dominance and love to their mothers. ——___——._——_— III. THE INSTRUMENTS OF INVESTIGATION Interpersonal Check List The Interpersonal Check List (ICL, see Appendix B) was developed by LaForge and Suczek (1955) as a device for measur— ing a subject's self—perception as well as his perception of other individuals. The rationale underlying the instrument is that the subject's responses represent structured communi- cations about himself and others which he is willing to convey to the examiner in writing. The ICL (Form IV) comprises 128 items consisting of words or phrases which describe different kinds of inter— personal behaviors, and there are eight items for each of six- teen interpersonal behavior variables. An intensity dimension has been built into the check list such that each of the sixteen interpersonal variables is represented by a four—point scale. For each variable there is one item for intensity level 1 which reflects "a mild or necessary amount of the trait." Intensity level 2 has three items which refer to ”a moderate or appropriate amount of the trait." Three items are used to reflect intensity level 5, ”a marked or inappro- priate amount of the trait”; and one item expresses intensity 4, an "extreme amount of the trait." A detailed description of the development of the check list was reported by LaForge and Suczek (1955), and a recent 17 18 report on its research uses was made by LaForge (1965). In the earlier report, the authors indicated that the sixteen interpersonal variables could be conceptualized as forming related sectors of a single—plane circumplex. They reported that adjacent variables on the circular continuum are more closely related than non—adjacent ones, and the relationship between two variables is a monotonic decreasing function of their separation. With statistical support for the conception of the ICL content as being represented by the interpersonal variables as sectors of a circumplex as described above, the authors were further able to divide the circumplex into eight sectors of interpersonal behavior patterns. They also divided the circle by a horizontal axis representing a love—hate (Lov) continuum and a vertical axis representing a dominance— submission (Dom) continuum so that the interpersonal behavior variables could be represented by a circular continuum along which the sixteen interpersonal categories lie, arrayed about the two perpendicular axes of Dom and Lov. The two dimensions dominance—submission and love—hate can be extracted by a weighted sum of each set of reSponses to the 128 items. The sixteen interpersonal categories are alphabetized, and the following formulae for calculating Dom and Lov summary scores have been formulated: Dom = A — I + .924 (B+P—H—J) + .707(C+O—G—K) + .585 (D+N-F—L) Lov = M - E + .924(N+L-D—F) + .707(O+K—C-G) + .585 (P+J—B—H) 19 In the initial presentation of the ICL (LaForge and Suczek, 1955), the authors reported test-retest correlations ranging between .64 and .77. LaForge (1965) reported that a direct measure of internal consistency can be obtained from the communalities of each of the 16 raw score measures when their principal components are obtained in factor ana— lytic studies. The communalities of these variables for the "self descriptions" of male University of Illinois student groups ranged from a low of .51 to a high of .86. Considering that the sixteenths are personality scales of only eight items, LaForge believed that the communalities were encourag— ing. The summary scores Dom and Lov obtained from the same sample have communalities above .90. On the basis of the factor analysis of the Illinois male students‘ responses, LaForge concluded that ”the summary scores Dom and Lov con- tain the important variance of the ICL ... and therefore, best summarize the information carried by the list (1965, p. 25).” On the basis of the above information, the ICL was believed to be an appropriate instrument for obtaining and evaluating the attribution of interpersonal traits of sub- jects to their parents. That is, when the subjects reSponded to the test items as descriptive of their parents, they may be said to have attributed to them their own perception of the parents' interpersonal behavioral traits. Their responses were then available for appropriate scoring and analysis with respect to the hypotheses to be tested. 20 The Masculinity—Femininity Scale (MF) 9: the Minneapolis Multiphasic Personality Inventory (MMPI) The MF scale is composed of sixty heterogeneous, true- false items (see Appendix D) ranging over interests in social activities, interpersonal relationships, vocational and avo— cational interests, religious preferences, personal sensitivi— ties, and candid sexual material. Hathaway (1956) described the derivation of the MF scale. Pre—college and college stu— dents as well as other age groups provided data for construct- ing and validating the measure. Hathaway and McKinley (1951), who developed the scale, indicated that it measures subjects' tendencies toward masculinity or femininity of interest pat— terns. Separate T—scores are provided for the two sexes so that "... in either case, a high score indicates a deviation of the basic interest pattern in the direction of the opposite sex. Males with very high MF scores have frequently been found to be either overt or repressed sexual inverts (1951, p. 20).“ Dahlstrom and Welsh (1960) list the available research findings on the test—retest reliability of the MF scale including college student subjects, and concluded that the test is quite stable over time. Research reviewed by Drake and Oetting (1959) indicates that the scale has discriminatory power in separating masculinity and femininity of interest and behavior patterns, especially with male subjects. Thus, assuming that the test responses were a reflection of the 21 subject's interest and behavior patterns, the instrument was believed to be appropriate for the purposes of the present investigation. As an attempt to evaluate possible additional meaning associated with the MF measures, it was decided to correlate the MF scores with those of Barron's (1955) experimental MMPI scale for Ego Strength. The difficulty with this pro- cedure is the fact that eighteen items of the sixty—eight in the Ego Strength scale were not included in the 420 item Hastings Short Form of the MMPIl which was to be administered to the subjects. However, since only two of the items in the MF and Ego Strength scales overlap, it was believed that such an analysis might reveal additional meaning associated with the MF measures. Barron developed the Ego Strength scale for the purpose of predicting reSponse to psychotherapy. His item analysis identified the pretreatment attributes that bore some relation— ship to the degree of improvement the subjects showed after individual psychotherapy. Inspection of the item content and the personality and intelligence test correlates of the scale led Barron to assume the items to be indicative of dif— ferences in strength of ego structure. Among the character— istics which he collectively referred to as ego strength are 1Olson (1954) has reported research comparing this form with the complete MMPI, and Dahlstrom and Welsh (1960) have discussed..its appropriateness for research and clinical purposes. 22 physiological stability and good health, a strong sense of reality, feelings of personal adequacy and vitality, per— missive morality, lack of ethnic prejudice, emotional out— goingness and spontaneity, and intelligence. Dahlstrom and Welsh (1960) have reviewed a number of researches which have indicated that the Ego Strength scale is a useful predictor of improved outcome in psychotherapy and as an instrument that can differentiate psychiatric patients from non— hospitalized subjects. Barron (1955) reported a correlation of —.45 between Ego Strength and MF scale scores of a group of male graduate students; however, the correlation between the same measures for a group of male clinic patients was -.04. On the basis of these findings and the assumption that the reduced number of Ego Strength scale items given to the subjects of the present study does not invalidate the use of the scale, it was expected that Ego Strength and MF measures would be negatively correlated in the present sample of college males. If such a correlation was found, additional interpretations could be made relative to the meaning of what the MF scale measures . Iv. HYPOTHESES The general hypothesis, as previously stated, is as follows: the degree of adoption of masculine behavioral patterns in adult males is positively related to their attribution to their fathers of interpersonal behavioral traits of dominance and love and negatively related to their attribution of interpersonal traits of dominance and love to their mothers. From this general hypothesis, hypotheses were formulated: 1. There is a significant positive correlation between the degree of preference for masculine behavior patterns as measured by the MF scale and the amount of attribution to fathers of dominant interpersonal behavior traits as measured by the ICL. There is a significant positive correlation between the degree of preference for masculine behavior patterns as measured by the MF scale and the amount of attribution to fathers of loving interpersonal behavior traits as measured by the ICL. There is a significant negative correlation between the degree of preference for masculine behavior patterns as measured by the MF scale and the amount of attribution to mothers of dominant interpersonal behavior traits as measured by the ICL. There is a significant'negative‘correlation between the degree of preference for masculine behavior patterns as measured by the MF scale and the amount of attribution to mothers of loving interpersonal behavior traits as measured by the ICL. 25 the following operational V. METHOD The Sample The sample consisted of male college students enrolled in undergraduate psychology courses at the Michigan State University during the spring and summer terms of 1965. Recruitment announcements were made to students enrolled in the classes on Personality Theory during the spring and summer terms. In order to increase the size of the sample, announcements were also made in the Introductory Psychology classes during the summer term. The students were asked to volunteer as subjects in a research project attempting to relate personality characteristics and attitudes toward parents. They were told that their involvement as anonymous subjects would include Spending about one hour and fifteen minutes taking paper and pencil tests in a group situation. They were further told: that their names would not be used on the test response sheets; that they would be paid two dollars ($2.00) for their time; and that they would receive a printed summary of the findings when the study was com— pleted. A total of 114 male students were tested. Eighty—four subjects were enrolled in the Personality Theory (Pers Theory) courses and 50 in the Introductory Psychology (Intro Psy) courses. Students in the former had already taken the 24 25 Introductory course and in some cases one or more other psychology courses while students in the latter had never before enrolled in a psychology course. Both groups of subjects included predominantly Sophomores and Juniors with a few Seniors and very few Freshmen. Their ages ranged between 18 and 22. There were no significant age or college- class differences between the two groups. Of the 114 proto— cols obtained, eighteen were eliminated from the Pers Theory group and four from the Intro Psy group. The criteria for elimination are listed in Table I as well as the number of 1’ 1 protocols deleted from each sample group. Two of the in— complete tests occurred because the subjects felt they could not continue responding to the MMPI for personal reasons. Table I. Criteria for and number of eliminations of subject protocols from the analyses of the sample groups. Sample Group Pers Theory Intro Psy Either or both parents deceased 10 0 Parents divorced 5 2 Failure to complete the tests 2 1 Not born in the United States 1 1 The third incomplete test involved a student's failure to respond to the items on the second page of the ICL. A check of the MMPI validity scales revealed no extreme scores which 26 might have indicated that the MMPI responses were not valid. The number of protocols used in the final analyses was 66 for the Pers Theory group and 26 for the Intro Psy group. The sample may be considered to be representative of male students enrolled at that time in either Personality Theory or Introductory Psychology classes of Michigan State University who were willing to volunteer as research subjects under the conditions stated above. Procedure The instruments were administered in class or lecture rooms to groups ranging from 10 to 18 students. Each student chose one of three possible testing times which were scheduled so as not to conflict with class attendance. The subjects were first informed that their partici— pation in the research would involve completion of two tests (the ICL and the MMPI) and a questionnaire (Family Information Questionnaire) all of which would be given to them at the beginning of the period. They were told first to read care— fully the sheet of General Instructions (see Appendix A) clipped on top of the material to be given to them. The stu— dents were informed that there was no time limit for any of the tests and that the General Instructions would explain how and in what order they were to proceed from one test to the other until they had completed the three parts. They were allowed to ask questions if they did not understand the General Instructions. The only questions raised were 27 from subjects whose parents were divorced or not living. These subjects asked how to complete the Interpersonal Check List (see Appendix B), and they were told to describe their divorced or deceased parents if they still knew or remembered them or else to omit the relevant descriptions. As noted above in the section describing the sample, such subjects' protocols were not included in the final analyses. A copy of the Family Information Questionnaire may be found in Appendix C. Most of the students required between one hour and fifteen minutes and one hour and forty minutes to complete 1 the tests. Observation by the examiners revealed no evidence that the subjects were not responding to each test item or not following the required order for completion of the instru— ments. Scoring of the Instruments Templates were prepared for machine scoring of each MMPI scale. Separate output data cards were obtained for each scale, and these cards contain punched identifying information and the total score for the scale. All of the MMPI validity and clinical scales were scored according to the instructions given by Hathaway and McKinley (1951), and the MMPI Ego-Strength scale was scored by summing the number of answered true—false items listed by Barron (1955) as being included in the scale. Only the MF and Ego Strength measures were included in the final analyses. For the MF scale, each true—false item (see Appendix D) answered in the 28 direction of the interest pattern of the opposite sex was counted as one point. Thus, for male subjects, low scores indicate a preference for responding to "masculine" items and high scores indicate a preference for responding to "feminine“ items. The ICL consists of 128 items (see Appendix B), but six additional alternate items have been included in the list for future experimental item analysis. LaForge (1965) has sug— gested this procedure as a means of obtaining information relative to possible future revisions of the Check List. Responses to the six alternate items were not scored for the present study. The test sheets were machine scored yielding output cards with punched information relative to identifi— cation and the nature of each item response. The answers were scored and analyzed according to procedures outlined by LaForge (1965). A program was written for computer analysis of the individual item reSponses recorded on the initial output cards.l For each of the sixteen interpersonal categories, the number of items marked "True” (of the eight items in each category) were summed. These category totals were then used to compute the summary scores Dom and Lov as described on page 18. This analytic procedure was followed separately for both sets of responses which the subjects er. Joel Stutz, from the Department of Psychology, Michigan State University, wrote the program for the analy— sis of the ICL item response data. 29 made with respect to their mother and to their father. Thus, separate output cards containing the results of this ICL analysis were obtained for each parent, and these were the datum cards employed in the final analysis of the experi— mental hypotheses. IV . RESULTS Comparisons Between the Sample Groups To facilitate comparisons between the data of the two samples, between the various variables within the samples, and an understanding of the data in general, the data cards were duplicated and computer analyzed by the Michigan State University CDC 5600 computer via two separate programs. The DAP 2 Program1 yielded scattergram analyses of each of the variables and their relationships with each of the other variables, and the CORE ROUTINE2 resulted in the multicorrelations to be presented below. An early aspect of the data analysis involved a com— parison of the several variable scores of the two sample groups of subjects in order to determine whether there were any significant differences between the measures of the two groups. Inspection of the data gave no evidence to suggest that the data were not normally distributed, and thus it was believed likely that normal parametric statis— tics might be apprOpriate for tests of significant differences lThe DAP 2 Program was written by Mr. James Clark, of the Department of Psychology, Michigan State University. Mr. Clark also served as a consultant on the use of the com— puter programs and the other facilities of the Michigan State University Computer Laboratory. 2Available in the library of the Michigan State Uni— versity Computer Laboratory. 50 51 between the variable scores of the two sample groups. To further evaluate the assumptions underlying parametric statistics, an F-test for the homogeneity of two variables was used for all the variables of the two groups. None of the variances differed significantly except those of the mother—dominance scores (.05 level). Thus, normal t-test comparisons were made between the means of all the variable scores except the mother-dominance measures, and the latter means were compared with a t—test which does not require the assumption of homogeneity of variance. The results of these comparisons are presented in Table II. Table II. Results of the t-test for the significance of differences between the mean scores of the Introductory Psychology and the Personality Theory subjects with reSpect to the variables Masculinity—Femininity (MF), dominance, love and Ego Strength. ‘ Variable Groups N M S t P MF Intro Psy 26 28.77 4.42 O 71 NS (MMPI) Pers Theory 66 27.94 5.25 ' Moth—Dom Intro Psy 26 4.40 9.04 0 47 NS (ICL) Pers Theory 66 5.55 6.60 Moth-Lov Intro Psy 26 5.55 11.07 1 04 NS (ICL) Pers Theory 66 2.85 11.18 ' Fath—Dom Intro Psy 26 8.07 8.55 0 08 NS (ICL) Pers Theory 66 7.81 7.64 ' Fath—Lov Intro Psy 26 —5.52 12.75 -0 50 NS (ICL) Pers Theory 66 —2.14 11.46 ' Ego Strength Intro Psy 26 55.81 4.72 2 O7 025 (MMPI) Pers Theory 66 57.95 4.40 ' *- All the t—tests were two—tailed tests. 52 The values in Table II show that there were no signifi— cant differences between the means of the variables relevant to the hypotheses. However, the means between the two groups for the Ego Strength scores were significantly different (.025 level). While the ego strength variable is not salient to the hypotheses of the study, it was thought that since the samples differed to this extent, it would be useful to present separate analyses for the two samples as well as an analysis including the combined scores of the two groups. This procedure was followed for subsequent analyses presented in this chapter. In this section the hypotheses are restated and the results are presented in Tables III and IV. The intercorre— lation matrices for the Masculinity—Femininity (MMPI) scores and the dominance and love (ICL) scores of the Introductory Psychology and the Personality Theory groups are presented in Table III. A similar intercorrelation matrix for the combined scores of the two groups is found in Table IV. All of the correlations were tested for significant differ— ences from a zero correlation. The letters following some of the correlation values indicate various levels at which they were significantly different from zero correlations. Also, correSponding correlations of the Introductory Psy— chology and the Personality Theory groups were tested for significant differences, and none of these correlations of 55 Table III. Intercorrelation matrices for the Masculinity— Femininity (MMPI) and the dominance and love (ICL) scores of the Introductory Psychology (N = 26) and the Personality Theory (N = 66) groups.a MF Moth-Dom Moth—Lov Fath—Dom Fath—Lov MFb a g Moth—Dom - . 52 69C U) A Moth—Lov —.1569 —.5208f o E Fath-Dom -.oeo7 —.oo15 -.0182 H Fath—Lov -.0274 .1567 —.1070 —.2867 MP >‘ Moth—Dom —.2846d $4 8 Moth—Lov -.o114 -.29ose .C‘. E Fath-Dom —.1001 .2465 .1719 U) § Fath—Lov —.1586 .1151 —.0459 -.5265f aNo significant differences were found between corresponding correlations of the Introductory Psychology and Personality bTheory groups. Correlation Significant Significant Significant Significant s with no letter after them are not significant. at the .05 level. at the .025 level. at the .01 level. at the .005 level. the two groups were found to differ significantly. Finally, the scattergrams of each of the variables as they relate to every other variable were inspected, and no evidence was found toindicate that curvilinear relationships existed. It should be remembered when reading these tables that low MF 54 Table IV. Intercorrelation matrix for the Masculinity— Femininity (MMPI) and the dominance and love (ICL) scores of the combined Introductory Psychology and Personality Theory Groups (N = 92). MF Moth—Dom Moth—Lov Fath—Dom Fath-Lov. MFa b Moth—Dom —.2955 Moth-Lov —.0555 —.5685C Fath—Dom —.0876 .1527 .1159 Fath-Lov —.1264 .1512 —.oe79 —.5158b :Correlations with no letter after them are not significant. cSignificant at the .005 level. Significant at the .0005 level. scores indicate a preference for more masculine behavior and high values indicate a preference for less masculine behavior. Hypothesis 1. There is a significant positive' correlation between the degree of preference for masculine behavior patterns as measured by the MF scale and the amount of attribution to fathers of dominant interpersonal behavior traits as measured by the ICL. This hypothesis is not supported. While the corre— lations between masculinity and the attribution of dominant behavior to fathers are in the predicted direction for both groups, the values are so small that they permit the in- ference of only a negligible or nonexistent relationship. 55 Hypothesis 2. There is a significant positive correlation between the degree of preference for masculine behavior patterns as measured by the MF scale and the amount of attribution to fathers of loving interpersonal behavior traits as measured by the ICL. The results do not support the above hypotheSis” Although in the predicted direction, the correlations are not significant and do not allow any inference of a relation- ship between maSculinity and attribution of loving behaviors » to fathers. )1 Hypothesis 5. There is a significant negative correlation between the degree of preference for masculine behavior patterns as measured by the MF scale and the amount of attribution to mothers of dominant interpersonal behavior traits as measured by the ICL. This hypothesis also is not supported. However, for both groups of subjects as well as the combined groups, the correlations are significant in the direction opposite from that predicted. Thus, the more masculine the subjects were, the more they tended to see their mothers as behaving in dominant ways. Hypothesis 4. There is a significant negative correlation between the degree of preference for masculine behavior as measured by the MF scale and the amount of attribution to mothers of loving interpersonal behavior traits as measured by the ICL. 56 The results do not bear out this prediction. It is interesting to note that the correlation values, while small and nonsignificant, are in the direction opposite from that which was hypothesized. Also, the correlation for the Introductory Psychology students is slightly higher than that of the Personality Theory group, although as noted above in the previous section, these correlations do not differ significantly. Neither of the correlations are large enough to infer any relationships between masculinity and attributed loving behavior to mothers. Further examination of Tables III and IV shows that for both groups of subjects, significant negative correlations are found when the attribution of dominance and the attribu- tion of love to the same parent were compared. The only exception was the comparison between father-dominance and father-love in the Introductory Psychology group. However, this negative correlation is relatively high and not signifi- cantly different from the similar comparison in the Personality Theory group. When the data of the two groups were combined, the comparisons between dominance and love for the mother yielded a negative correlation significant at the .0005 level, and the negative correlation of similar variables for the fathers is significant at the .005 level. Thus, when the subjects described a given parent as dominant, they also saw the same parent as significantly less loving, and when a parent was viewed as being loving he was also seen as behav- ing in Significantly less dominant ways. 57 When the attribution of dominance to mothers and the attribution of dominance to fathers were compared, none of the correlations were significant. However, for the Person— ality Theory group the correlation of .2465 provides some basis for inferring that there may be a positive relationship for these subjects between seeing one parent as dominant when the other is seen as dominant. Comparisons between mother— love and father—love scores yielded no evidence to infer a relationship between these variables. Ego Strength Scale The results of the comparisons between the MF and the Ego Strength scores are presented in Table V. The corre- lation of these variables is significant for the larger Personality Theory group. When the groups are combined, the correlation is also significant so that a positive relationship between these variables may be inferred from the results. Table V. Correlations between MMPI Masculinity—Femininity (MF) and Ego Strength scores of the Introductory Psychology and Personality Theory groups and both groups combined. Groups Variables Intro Psy Pers Theory Combined Groups (N = 26) (N = 66) (N = 92) a b b MF and Ego Strength -.2958 -.4224 —.5951 aNot significant. Significant at the .0005 level. VII. DISCUSSION The Findings It was found that no significant positive relationship existed between the subjects' preference for masculine behavior and their attribution of dominant and loving be- havior to their fathers as predicted in Hypotheses 1 ehg.2. Thus, neither of these hypotheses were sustained. The pre— diction of Hypothesis 5 that there would be a significant 3 negative relationship between the preference for masculine H behavior and the attribution of dominant behavior to mothers was also not supported by the data. Instead, a significant positive relationship between masculinity and attributed mother—dominance was found for both groups of subjects. There was also no evidence to support the inference of a significant relationship between degree of masculinity and attribution of loving behavior to the mothers as predicted in Hypothesis 1- A significant negative relationship was found between the two dependent variables, dominance and love, as they were attributed to a given parent. In other words, subjects who viewed a parent as more dominant were also likely to see the same parent as significantly less loving, and subjects who viewed a parent as more loving also tended to see the same parent as behaving in significantly less dominant ways. This relationship was found with respect to 58 59 the subjects' attributions to both of their parents. No significant relationships were found between the attribution of the same kind of behavior (i.e., either dominance or love) to both parents. Finally, a significant positive relation— ship was found between the MMPI scale variables of masculinity and ego strength. Theoretical Implications The findings reported above clearly do not support the exploratory hypotheses of the Study. These hypotheses were derived from a central proposition suggested by identifi— ...—~— ...—Ah- cation theory and research. The proposition is that adult behavior patterns have antecedents related to the identifi- cation process, i.e., an individual's preference for sex—role behavior patterns may be assumed to reflect the degree of his earlier identification with a same—sex or a cross—sex parent. The major assumption of the present research was that an individual's perception of his parents as reflected by the interpersonal behavior traits he attributes to them will function as a measure of the degree of his identification with the parent who has been more influential in this process. In the light of the findings, this basic assumption does not seem tenable. Therefore, the research findings represent a challenge to rather than support of the central proposition suggested by identification theory and research. An interesting question which might seem to be implicit in this research is whether or not an instrument (the ICL) 40 tapping conscious communication would yield results which are comparable to those reported in the literature in which projective tests were used? In other words, will findings about parental behavior obtained from the conscious level of response be comparable to those obtained from the un- conscious level of response as reported by previous researchers? It would seem, upon initial consideration of the findings, that the question has been answered in the negative. However, the study was not designed to answer only this question or to answer it in any direct way. The research reported in the second chapter involved the use of projective instruments to obtain unconscious or fantasy responses related specifically to father—son and mother—son relationships. The present study made no attempt to elicit reports of specific parent-son interpersonal behavior. Rather, the subjects, when responding to the ICL, were asked to use their own opinion in describing their parents as they viewed them at the present time. It was assumed that their responses would be conscious, perhaps defended, but probably not objective. Thus, the different question of whether responses obtained in this way about less Specifically prescribed interactions of parents would yield findings con— sistent with identification theory and research using other approaches seems to be answered in the negative. The above discussion and the nature of the results indicate that the present findings cannot appropriately be 41 interpreted in terms of identification theory which stresses the importance of the paternal figure as a model and as a major influence in the development of masculine interests and behaviors in males. The most significant finding is the positive relationship between masculinity and the attribu— tion of dominant interpersonal behavior to mothers. A related finding is the significant negative relationship between mother—dominance and mother—love. Thus, when mothers were seen as dominant they were also viewed as aggressive and hostile, since the ICL love measure is calculated by subtract— -..p—u—vv ing aggression—hostility scores from love scores. Freud (1924, 1955, 1949) postulated that the boy resolves the threat of the Oedipal Situation by allying himself with the attitudes, values, and behavior of the aggressive, puni— tive, and powerful father. He believed that resolution of the Oedipus complex constituted a vital influence in the development of masculinity in the boy's character, although he allowed that identifications take place with the child's later views of the parents as well. The present findings cannot be directly related to the boy's Oedipal view of his father, however they indicate that college—age men who have adopted masculine behaviors and interests tend to view their mothers as dominant and aggressive while no such relationship was found with respect to their views of their fathers. A likely interpretation is that the attributes of parental behavior as well as the nature of the familial milieu may be different in this culture as compared to the one Freud 42 experienced and understood as he developed his theory. Further, if it is assumed that a boy's views of his father are different from his later adult ones in some way that is characteristic for the Oedipal period, then the assumption must also be made that his views of his mother's behavior are also different during that period from those he expresses when he is a young adult. A more likely interpretation is that a mother who is seen as dominant and aggressive by the adult male expressed these same behaviors during the male's childhood. It can be speculated that such boys were aware of and responsive to their mothers' dominant (rather than passive and more loving—seductive) behaviors, and that this awareness and responsiveness functioned to quiet the Oedipal fears assumed to be related to the fathers. Thus, Freud's theory may be inappropriate for explaining the mechanisms involved in the development of masculine role acceptance and behavior in this culture. In terms of the present findings, the emphasis of learning and role theorists on the idea that a father who is aggressive, dominant, and loving is a crucial influence on the boy's acceptance and adoption of masculine behaviors is clearly not supported. However, their explanation of the learning mechanisms involved in the development of masculine interests and behavior may represent a more useful interpre— tation of the present findings. Mowrer (1950) describes developmental identification as basically a result of a 45 conditioning process in which parental rewards have a crucial influence, and his interpretation of defensive identification also involves a learning process. The role theorists (Sarbin, 1954; Sears eh el., 1957; and Brim, 1958) also believe that sex-roles are learned under basic learning conditions of motivation, performance, and reinforcement. Both groups of theorists thus interpret the development of appropriate sex— typed behavior as involving a learning mechanism in which both parents have reward value. However, their view of the father's predominant influence, at least in the later stages of sex—role learning in males, is not supported by the results of the present study. Sanford (1955) may have introduced some needed clarity with reSpect to the theories of how sex—role behaviors develop. He concluded that such "development can be largely explained, without benefit of either identification or introjection, on the basis of common forms of learning. A child learns which of his actions please, and which dis— please, his parents, which win him love, which disapproval; he learns what reactions are effective in inhibiting those impulses which if allowed free rein would lead to catastrophe; he learns how to regard himself from the way others regard him; and in building his ego system and his self conception he learns what to keep and what to discard (1955, p. 117)." In the light of Sanford's conclusion and drawing from some of the more recent ideas of behaviorally oriented, 44 reinforcement theorists of personality and psychotherapy as represented by Lundin (1961) and Wolpe eh el. (1964) respectively, an interpretation of the present findings may be formulated. Briefly, most individuals who grow up in this culture experience more interpersonal contact with their mothers than their fathers. Thus, the mother is the most frequent agent of reinforcement with respect to the boy's behavior. Since the home is frequently the ”mother's domain," she tends to have more opportunities for dominant behavior in the home than the father who may be quite aggressive and dominant in other situations. Given these considerations, if the mother is dominant and aggressive and can tolerate these behaviors in herself without undue anxiety, it would seem likely that she would also be able to tolerate such behaviors in her son. Her selective reinforcement of the boy's dominant, aggressive and other masculine behaviors and interests would help to shape such behaviors so that they would gradually conform to socially acceptable standards for males. It seems likely that such males would consciously attribute dominance and aggressiveness to their mothers. On the other hand, a mother who is unable to be dominant and aggressive may be unable to tolerate such behaviors in others including her son and perhaps her husband. The learn— ing environment for such a boy would be quite different from that where the mother could tolerate and accept appropriate expression of dominance and aggression as discussed above. 45 Such behaviors would receive different kinds of reinforce— ments from the latter kind of mother who may discourage them, who may become anxious when they are expressed, or who may elicit guilt feelings in her son after he has ex— pressed them. Thus, he may learn to avoid them, he may experience undue anxiety or guilt when he expresses them, and he may fail to learn appropriate ways of expressing them. It seems likely that such a boy might fail to learn and accept some of the important aspects of masculinity in this culture, and he may attribute little dominance and aggressive— ness to his mother. The above interpretation of the results also seems to be closely aligned with sociological theory and findings reported by Warner eh _l. (1960). For example, these authors found a relationship between the level of achievement and upward social mobility of lower—class background males and the dominant and aggressive characteristics of their mothers. These achieving men also had many of the behavior character— istics such as outgoingness, independence, aggressiveness, etc. associated with masculinity in this culture. This reported relationship between dominant mothers and the adoption of certain masculine behaviors by sons, at least in the lower— classes, seems to be consistent with the results of the present study. The findings of Warner eh l. with respect to lower— class individuals raise some important questions with respect to the possible effects which social class variables 46 may have had in the present research. Thus, might there have been a predominant number of subjects from lower—class backgrounds in the present samples which may have effected the nature of the results? Also, if social class background factors had been controlled, would the reactions of subjects from the several social class levels have differed signifi— cantly? Similar kinds of questions might be asked with respect to possible differences of responses of subjects who came from rural environments as compared with those from urban backgrounds. For example, might subjects from rural backgrounds where the father may play a more visably dominant and aggressive role and the mother a more submissive one in the familial interactions differ in their responses as com— pared with males from urban environments where the familial roles and interactions may be qualitatively different or somewhat opposite? These questions cannot be answered in the context of findings resulting from the present research design. The finding that the attribution of dominance to mothers is related negatively to the attribution of love may support a Speculation with respect to the college-age subjects in the present samples. It would be expected that at this age young men would have relatively strong needs for independence from their mothers coupled with strivings to be clearly dif— ferent from them. In effect, they may have needs to reject the source of strength which has aided them in learning to 47 develop masculine interests and behaviors, and therefore they might tend to see their mothers as less loving while they may be unable to reject the view that they are dominant. An assumption underlying this study was that the preference for sex—role behavior and interest patterns in adults as measured by the MF scale is a measure of the success and direction of one's earlier "identification," or in keeping with this discussion, progress toward adoption of appropriate sex—role behaviors. This assumption is still tenable. This measure was used successfully by Azimi (1964), and the present findings provide no evidence for interpreting the assumption as untenable. The significant and expected relationship found between the MMPI and Ego Strength scores closely parallels the findings reported by Barron (1955). Thus, the acceptance of cultural standards of masculinity is related to characteristics subsumed (by Barron) under ego strength such as physiological stability and good health, lack of ethnic prejudice, a strong sense of reality, feelings of personal adequacy and vitality, and emotional outgoing- ness and spontaneity. This finding clearly supports the interpretation that degree of masculinity and good emotional and physical health tend to be related. This positive relationship between masculine sex—role behaviors and good health coupled with the finding that males who have such characteristics attribute dominance and aggres— siveness to their mothers present a challenge to some of the 48 views about the influence of "dominant mothers" expressed in the popular literature. It has been suggested that the boys in our culture experience so much exposure to females who dominate and control their activities (both in the home as well as in the early years of school) that there may be a real danger that males in this culture will begin to loose some of their masculine characteristics. Sometimes the sug- gestion is also made that the increasing dominance of females coupled with the seemingly decreasing exposure of boys to their fathers and other males will have detrimental effects not only on their masculinity but on their mental health as well. The present findings provide evidence that these Speculations may be unfounded. Implications for Future Research The interpretations given to the present findings have included a number of interesting theoretical questions which have implications with respect to future research. For example, a useful compliment to the present study would be a replication of the design with the modification that social class and rural—urban background variables also be evaluated. If different age groups were used as subjects, such a replication would also answer questions with reSpect to the influence of age on the reSponses of the subjects. Perhaps the most important implication resulting from the findings and the tentative explanations given to them is that future researchers might consider the relevance of 49 both learning and sociological theory as well as personality theory when they attempt to answer some of the questions relating to the development and acceptance of sex—typed behavior. Definitive answers may depend upon such consider— ations. VIII . SUMMARY The present study was designed to investigate some of the phenomena subsumed under the concept of identification and to test empirically hypotheses suggested by identification process theories. A major assumption of the research was that an individual's perception of his parents as reflected by the interpersonal behavior traits he attributes to them will function as a measure of his degree of identification with the parent who has been more influential in this process. The general prediction was that the degree of adoption of masculine behavior patterns in adult males will be positively related to their attribution to their fathers of inter— personal behavior traits of dominance and love and negatively related to their attribution of interpersonal traits of dominance and love to their mothers. Ninety—two male undergraduate university students served as volunteer, paid subjects. The students used the Interpersonal Check List once to describe their mother and once to describe their father. This Check List includes 128 words and phrases which describe various kinds of inter— personal behaviors. Responses to this instrument were analyzed so that summary scores representing the behaviors of dominance and love could be derived. The subjects also completed the first 420 items of the Minnesota Multiphasic 50 51 Personality Inventory. The Masculinity—Femininity scale scores of the Inventory were the independent variables of this study. Total scores from this scale were assumed to be a measure of masculine—feminine role behavior and interest preference. The final task of the subjects involved the completion of a Family Information Questionnaire which provided information for excluding the protocols of subjects whose parents were deceased or divorced. The data were computer analyzed to obtain multi— correlation coefficients between the measures of masculinity— femininity, mother—dominance, mother-love, father—dominance, and father—love. It was found that no significant positive relationship existed between the subjects' preference for masculine behavior and their attribution of dominant and loving behavior to their fathers as was hypothesized. The prediction that there would be a significant negative relation— ship between the preference for masculine behavior and the attribution of dominant and loving behavior to mothers was also not supported by the data. Instead, a significant positive relationship between degree of masculinity and attributed mother-dominance was found. Also, a significant negative relationship was found between the two dependent variables, parent—dominance and parent—love, as they were attributed to a given parent; and this relationship was found with respect to the subjects' attributions to both parents. 52 The results were discussed in terms of behavior rein— forcement, personality theory and related to some findings of sociological theorists. It was concluded: (1) that the basic assumption of the research was not tenable in the light of the findings, (2) that the results represented a challenge to rather than support of the central proposition of identi- fication theories which stresses the importance of the paternal figure as a model and crucial influence on the development of sex-typed interests and behaviors in males, and (5) that the basic learning mechanisms postulated by learning and role theorists as well as reinforcement, per— sonality theorists about the development of masculine sex— role preference and behaviors may be the most pertinent interpretation of the findings. BIBLIOGRAPHY Azimi, C. Masculinity, femininity, and perception e; warmth and saliency 1h parent-son relationships. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Michigan State University, 1964. Barron, F. An ego strength scale which predicts response to psychotherapy. g. consult. Psychol., 1955, 11, 527-555. Brim, O. G., Jr. Family structure and sex—role learning by children: A further analysis of Helen Koch's data. Sociometry, 1958, 21, 1—16. Bronfenbrenner, U. The study of identification through interpersonal perception. In R. Tagiuri and L. Petrullo (Eds.), Person perception ehd interpersonal relationships. California: Stanford University Press, 1958, Pp. 110—150. Dahlstrom, W. G. and Welsh, G. S. Ln MMPI handbook: A guide he use 1h clinical practice and research. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press, _1960. Drake, L. E. and Oetting, E. R. Ln MMPI codebook for counselors. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press, 1959. Fenichel, O. The psychoanalytic theory of neoursis. New York: W. W. Norton and Company, 1945. Freud, Anna. The ego and the mechanisms of defense. London: Hogarth Press Ltd. 1957. Freud, S. The passing of the Oedipus complex. In Collected papers. Vol. 1;. London: Hogarth Press Ltd., 1924, Pp. 269—276. Freud, S. New introductory lectures 1h psychoanalysis. New York: W. W. Norton and Company, 1955. Freud, S. Ln outline of psychoanalysis. New York: W. W. Norton —and Company, 1949. 55 54 Hathaway, S. R. Scales 5 (masculinity—femininity), 6 (paranoia), and 8 (schizophrenia). In G. S. Welsh and W. G. Dahlstrom (Eds.), Basic readings eh phe MMPI 1h psychology ehg medicine. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press, 1956. Hathaway, S. R. and McKinley, J. C. The Minnesota multi— phasic personality inventory manual, revised. New York: The Psychological Corporation, 1951. Kagan, J. The concept of identification. Psychol. Rev., 1958, §5, 296—505. LaForge, R. Research use of the ICL. Oregon Research Institute Technical Report, 1965, 5, No. LaForge, R. and Suczek, R. F. The interpersonal dimension of personality III: An interpersonal checklist. 5. Pers., 1955, eg, 94—112. Lundin, R. W. Personality, eh experimental approach. New York: Macmillan Company, 1961. Lynn, D. B. Sex-role and parental identification. Child Develpm., 1962, 55, 555-564. McCandless, B. R. Children and adolescents: Behavior and development. New York: Holt, Rinehart, and Winston, 1961. Mowrer, O. H. Learning theory and personality dynamics. New York: Ronald Press, 1950. Munroe, Ruth L. Schools 95 psychoanalytic though . New York: Henry Holt and Company, 1955. Mussen, P. and Distler, L. Masculinity, identification and father—son relationships. 5. abnorm. soc. Psychol., 1959, 52, 550—556. Mussen, P. and Distler, L. Child—rearing antecedents of masculine identification in kindergarten boys. Childhood Develpm., 1960, 51, 89—100. Olson, G. W. The Hastings short form of the group MMPI. 5. clin. Psychol., 1954, 19, 586—588. Sanford, N. The dynamics of identification. Psychol. Rev., 1955, 55, No. 2, 106—118. Sarbin, T. Role theory. In G. Lindsey (Ed.), Handbook 5: social psychology. Vol. 1, Cambridge: Addison Wesley Publications, 1954, Pp. 225—258. 55 Sears, Pauline. Child rearing factors related to playing of sex—typed roles. Amer. Psychol., 1955, 5, 451. Sears, R., Maccoby, Eleanor, and Levin, H. Patterns 5: child rearing. Evanston: Row Peterson and Company, 1957. Sears, R., Pintler, M., and Sears, Pauline. Effect of father separation on pre—school children's doll play aggression. Child. Develpm. 1946, 11, 217-245. Stoke, S. An inquiry into the concept of identification. 5. gen. Psychol., 1950, 76, 165-189. Tolman, E. C. Identification and the post—war world. 5. abnorm. soc. Psychol., 1945, 55, 141—148.' Warner, W. L., Meeker, Marchia, and Eells, L. Social class 1h_America. New York: Harper and Row, 1960. Wolpe, J., Salter, A., and Reyna, L. J. (Eds.). The conditionihg therapies: The challenge 1h psychotherapy. New York: Holt, Rinehart, and Winston, 1964. APPENDICES 56 APPENDIX A GENERAL INSTRUCTIONS Please read carefully before beginning to respond to the research materials. The questions that you will answer to not represent achievement test items in the sense that there are right and wrong answers. Rather they are a means of obtaining responses from you as an anonymous subject in a research project. A summary of the research findings will be available next September in the Psychology Department Office, 109 Olds Hall, for each of the participants of the study. Each subject's answer sheets have a randomly assigned number which will permit identification of his separate 5 answer Sheets as belonging to one person. Do not write your name on any of the answer sheets. gee phe blue scoring pencil given to you he mark 311 your answers. The materials given to you, the general instructions for each questionnaire, and the order in which you are to respond to them are as follows: 1. The Interpersonal Check List (ICL) and two separate IBM answer sheets marked MOTHER and FATHER which are inserted between the two pages of the ICL. Carefully read both the "Instructions” for the ICL and the "Directions" on the answer sheets. Then begin and mark your answers on the separate answer sheets as instructed. 2. A Booklet for the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory (MMPI) which has a separate IBM answer sheet marked MMPI inserted inside the cover of the booklet. Read the instructions on the cover of the MMPI booklet. Then begin the test and mark your answers on the separate answer sheet. Note that you are to answer only phe first gag items 1h phe MMPI Booklet. The 421st answer space on the answer sheet is crossed out and STOP is written above the 421st space to remind you that you are finished with this test when you have answered the first 420 items. 57 58 5. A Family Information Questionnaire. There is no answer sheet for this part, so mark or fill in the answers directly on this questionnaire as instructed. When you have completed this questionnaire, you are finished with your part of the research. There is no time limit for any of the tests, so when you complete one section, go on to the next until you are finished. Before leaving your seat, carefully check to determine that you have all of the materials which were given to you. Clip these materials together, return them to the examiner, and collect your payment. APPENDIX B THE INTERPERSONAL CHECK LIST - FORM IV This is a list of words and phrases which describe the way people behave in relation to one another. First, on the separate answer sheet marked MOTHER, mark the item true if the statement is descriptive of your mother, in your opinion, at the present time. Mark it false if it is not descriptive of her. When you have answered e11 the items for your mother, begin with the separate answer sheet marked FATHER and describe him by the same procedure. This will be your opinion of your father. Your first impression is generally the best, so go through the list as quickly as you can. However, be certain to consider and answer each item. Able to give orders . Appreciative . Apologetic Able to take care of self Able to doubt others 27. Complaining 28. Can be indifferent to others 29. Critical of others 50. Can be obedient 51. Cruel and unkind Affectionate and understanding 52. Dependent Acts important 1 2 5 4. 5. Accepts advice readily 6 7 8 9 Able to criticize self 10. Admires and imitates others 11. Agrees with everyone 12. Always ashamed of self 55. Dictatorial 54. Distrusts everybody 55. Dominating 56. Easily embarrassed 57. Eager to get along with 15. Very anxious to be approved of others 14. Always giving advice 15. Bitter 16. Bighearted and unselfish 17. Boastful 18. Businesslike 19. Bossy 20. Can be frank and honest 21. Clinging vine 22. Can be strict if necessary 25. Considerate 24. Cold and unfeeling 25. Can complain if necessary 26. COOperative 58. Easily fooled 59. Egotistical and conceited 40. Easily led 41. Encouraging to others 42. Enjoys taking care of others 45. Expects everyone to admire him *44. Faithful follower 45. Frequently disappointed 46. Firm but just 47. Fond of everyone 48. Forceful * Alternate items not scored for 59 the present study (see page 28). 6O Friendly Forgives anything Frequently angry Friendly all the time Generous to a fault Gives freely of self Good leader Grateful Hard—boiled when necessary Helpful Hard—hearted Hard to convince Hot-tempered Hard to impress Impatient with others' mistakes Independent Irritable Jealous Kind and reassuring Likes reSponsibility Lacks self—confidence Likes to compete with others Lets others make decisions Likes everyone Likes to be taken care of Loves everybody . Makes a good impression . Manages others . Meek . Modest Hardly ever talks back Often admired Obeys too willingly Often gloomy Outspoken Overprotective of others Often unfriendly Oversympathetic Often helped by others Passive and unaggressive Proud and self-satisfied . Always pleasant and agreeable o Resentful Respected by others Rebels against everything Resents being bossed Self-reliant and assertive Sarcastic Self—punishing Self—confident Selfeseeking 100. 101. 102. *105. 104. 105. 106. 107. 108. 109. 110. 111. 112. 115? *114. 115. 116. 117. 118. 119. 120. 121. 122. 125. 124. 125. 126. 127. 128. 129. 150. 151. 152. 155. *154. Shrewd and calculating Self-respecting Shy Sincere and devoted to friend Selfish Skeptical Sociable and neighborly Slow to forgive a wrong Somewhat snobbish Spineless Stern but fair Spoils people with kind- ness Straightforward and direct Stubborn SuSpicious Too easily influenced by friends Thinks only of self Tender and soft—hearted Timid Too lenient with others Touchy and easily hurt Too willing to give to others Tries to be too success- ful Trusting and eager to please Tries to comfort everyone Usually gives in Very respectful to authority Wants everyone's love Well thought of Wants to be led Will confide in anyone Warm Wants everyone to like him Will believe anyone Well-behaved APPENDIX C FAMILY INFORMATION QUESTIONNAIRE Please place a vertical mark above the short line following each answer which is most applicable to you. 1. Are your parents both living? Yes — No — 2. If the answer is "Nd," please check one of the following items: a. Did one of your parents die when you were a child? Yes - No - If so, which one? Father - Mother — b. Did one of your parents die when you were an adolescent? ' Yes — No — If so, which one? Father — Mother — 5. Are your parents presently living together? Yes — No - 4. If the answer is ”No," check one of the following items: a. Did your parents divorce each other when you were a child? Yes — No — If so, with whom did you live? Father — Mother — b. Did your parents divorce each other when you were an adolescent? Yes — No — If so, with whom did you live? Father — Mother - c. If you did not live with one of your parents, please specify with whom you lived: When you were a child When you were an adolescent 5. Are you an only child? Yes — No — 6. If the answer is “No," specify the following: a. The order of your birth 61 62 b. The number of male siblings: Older than you Younger than you c. The number of female siblings: Older than you Younger than you 7. Please Specify: Your age , your place of birth , your class , and your college major After having completed the two tests and this questionnaire, what do you think the researcher is attempting to assess in this study? True- APPENDIX D false Items of the Masculinity—Femininity Scale of the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory* True 140. 205. 204. 217. 226. 251. 259. 261. 278. 282. 295. 297. 299. think I would like the work of a librarian. would like to be a singer. am very strongly attracted by members of my own sex. used to like drop—the—handerchief. have often wished I were a girl. (Or if you are a girl) have never been sorry that I am a girl. enjoy reading love stories. like poetry. would like to be a florist. would like to be a nurse. like dramatics. like collecting flowers or growing house plants. At times my thoughts have raced ahead faster than I could Speak them. I like to cook. I used to keep a diary. I am worried about sex matters. HHHHHHHHHHHH . My hands have not become clumsy or awkward. If I were a reporter I would very much like to report news of the theater. I would like to be a journalist. I frequently find myself worrying about something. Some of my family have habits that bother and annoy me very much. I like to talk about sex. I have been disappointed in love. If I were an artist I would like to draw flowers. I have often felt that strangers were looking at me critically. Once in a while I feel hate toward members of my family whom I usually love. I like "Alice in Wonderland" by Lewis Carroll. I wish I were not bothered by thoughts about sex. I think that I feel more intensely than most people do. 36 Adapted from the Booklet for the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory by Hathaway and McKinley, 1945. 65 64 False 1. I like mechanics magazines. 19. When I take a new job, I like to be tipped off on who should be gotten next to. 26. I feel that it is certainly best to keep my mouth shut when I'm in trouble. 28. When someone does me a wrong I feel I should pay him back if I can, just for the principle of the thing. 79. My feelings are not easily hurt. 80. I sometimes tease animals. 81. I think I would like the kind of work a forest ranger does. 89. It takes a lot of argument to convince most people of the truth. 99. I like to go to parties and other affairs where there is lots of loud fun. 112. I frequently find it necessary to stand up for what I think is right. 115. I believe in a life hereafter. 116. I enjoy a race or game better when I bet on it. 117. Most people are honest chiefly through fear of being caught. 120. My table manners are not quite as good at home as when I am out in company. 155. I have never indulged in any unusual sex practices. 144. I would like to be a soldier. 176. I do not have a great fear of snakes. 198. I daydream very little. 215. In walking I am very careful to step over sidewalk cracks. 214. I have never had any breaking out on my skin that has worried me. 219. I think I would like the work of a building contractor. 221. I like science. 225. I very much like hunting. 229. I should like to belong to several clubs or lodges. 249. I believe there is a Devil and a Hell in afterlife. 254. I like to be with a crowd who play jokes on one another. 260. I was a slow learner in school. 262. It does not bother me that I am not better looking. 264. I am entirely self—confident. 280. Most people make friends because friends are likely to be useful to them. 285. If I were a reporter I would very much like to report sporting news. 500. There never was a time in my life when I liked to play with dolls. M'élll'lllfiilfltfllfllflifllflffliflilflfllflfllll“