MEASURING BOUNDARY SPANNING SUCCESS IN ENTERPRISE SOCIAL MEDIA By Ya-Wen Yu A THESIS Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Media and Information Ñ Master of Arts 2015 ABSTRACT MEASURING BOUNDARY SPANNING SUCCESS IN ENTERPRISE SOCIAL MEDIA By Ya-Wen Yu The study aims to explore empirical proxies of boundary spanning success by developing measurement for hierarchical alignment, goal alignment, and time lag for three boundary spanning activities occurring in enterprise social media. The study employs Generalized Linear Models (GLMs) and Linear Models (LMs) to test the effect of distinct boundary - spanning types and tie strength on each of the success measures. The findings show that strongly tied individuals who engage in information search activity can achieve boundary spanning success, as it is much easier for them to get the response in a short time compared to more weakly tied individuals. Trust as measured by symmetric (strong) tie thus can achieve boundary spanning success. Impli cations for theory and practice are also discussed. !!"""!ACKNOWLEDGMENTS I completed my master thesis under the guidance from professor Wietske van Osch. IÕm so honored to have her full support and time. In addition, I appreciate the other two committee members, Dr. Charles Steinfield and Dr. Constantinos K. Coursaris, who provide me with the valuable suggestions to refine the thesis. The whole process of writing a thesis made me pursue the PhD study in the incoming semester. I spent two years s tudying in the Department of Media and Information at Michigan State University. During the period, I have met knowledgeable professors, who enlightened the start in my research road. Moreover, I appreciate all the friends I have in East Lansing. Without t hem, I could not have felt the warmth. Furthermore, there is no much for feeling thankful to my family. Without my parents, I would not have started the adventure of my American dream. !!"#!TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES .......................................................................................................................... v LIST OF FIGURES ....................................................................................................................... vi KEY TO SYMBOLS .................................................................................................................... vii CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION ..................................................................................................... 1 CHAPTER II LITERATURE REVIEW AND HYPOTHESES .................................................... 6 Boundary Spanning Type and Hierarchical Alignment .............................................................. 6 Boundary Spanning Type and Goal Alignment .......................................................................... 7 Tie Strength and Goal Alignment ............................................................................................... 8 Tie Strength and Time Lag ......................................................................................................... 9 CHAPTER III RESEARCH DESIGN .......................................................................................... 11 Case Organization ..................................................................................................................... 11 Data Collection ......................................................................................................................... 12 Operationalization ..................................................................................................................... 12 Dependent Variables ............................................................................................................. 12 Individual -level Variable ...................................................................................................... 13 Dyadic -level Variable ........................................................................................................... 14 Control Variable ................................................................................................................... 14 CHAPTER IV RESULTS ............................................................................................................. 16 CHAPTER V DISCUSSION ........................................................................................................ 19 CHAPTER VI IMPLICATIONS FOR RESEARCH AND PRACTICE ..................................... 21 CHAPTER VII LIMITATIONS AND FUTURE RESEARCH ................................................... 23 REFERENCES ............................................................................................................................. 24 !!#!LIST OF TABLES Table 1 The Variables of Research Model ÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉ........................................... 14 Table 2 The Summary of R esults ÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉ.......................... 18 !!#"!LIST OF FIGURES Figure 1 The Network Predictors of Boundary Spanning S uccess ............................................... 10 !!#""!KEY TO SYMBOLS ESMÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉ ÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉÉEnterprise Social Media !!$!CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION !The study aims to investigate how trust and distinct boundary spanning activities influence boundary spanning success in Enterprise Social Media (hereafter as ESM). Such success refers to the targeted audiences reached, the effectiveness, and the efficiency for boundary spanning activity enacted by a poster and reciprocated by a responder. T he study tests hierarchical alignment, goal alignment, and time lag as the proxies of boundary spanning success. Moreover, the study looks at tie strength Ña network metric Ñas a proxy of trust. Accordingly, the study would like to explore how tie strength and the type of boundary spanning activity influence hierar chical alignment, goal alignment, and time lag to measure the boundary spanning success. Groups of ESM serve as open innovation communities. Such communities highly depend on social capital built by trust as the communities Ñsimilar to communities of practi ceÑexist independent of formal organizational norms and structures despite ESM being a corporate tool. Additionally, cooperation and coordination between individuals and groups inside the organizations at different levels and ways (Jones and George, 1998; Noteboom and Six, 2003; Stephenson, 2005). The various collaborative and coordination activities between distinct groups inside ESM could be described as intra -organizational or team boundary spanning ( van Osch, Steinfield and Zhao, 2015) . Studies on ESM s uggested that such information communication technologies encourage boundary spanning through their affordances by rendering the interactions between individuals as well as the information and content shared more visible (Steinfield et al., 2009; van Osch and Steinfield, 2015a). Team boundary spanning refers to an activity that encourages effective organizational collaboration through maintaining and developing the relationship with external linkages beyond the team boundary (Ancona, 1990; Ancona and Caldwe ll, 1992, Tushman, 1977). Additionally, boundary spanners serve as !!%!Òguardians who redirect crucial informationÓ from the external linkages ( Tushman and Scanlon, 1981; Fleming and Waguespack, 2007, p.166) . As above mentioned, trust is an important antecede nce to effective collaboration and coordination. Gulati (1994) has proposed tie strength as a proxy for trust. At the dyadic level, tie strength Ñbroadly defined as the distinction between strong versus weak ties Ñrefers to the frequency of interaction for a relationship between a knowledge seeker and source (Granovetter, 1973; Hansen, 1999). ESM helps create opportunities for new social capital developed by enabling repeated social interactions, even between distinct member inside the organization, and thus may contribute to the development of strong ties which in turn is likely to increase individualsÕ willingness to contribute to ongoing boundary -spanning activities on the system through enabling access to expertise and resources (Steinfield et al., 2009) . Accordingly, trust Ñas measured through tie strength based on the frequency of interaction between a poster and responder Ñmay influence the likelihood and promptness of the boundary spanning interaction. Team boundary spanning, i.e., the management of exte rnal linkages and interactions in co-located and virtual teams of ESM, aims to pro cure resources such as Òmonetary, informational, or social Ñas well as coordination and reputational benefitsÓ (van Osch & Steinfield, 2015 ). Additionally, such team boundary spanning distinguishes such group -level activities, namely representation, coordination, and information search ( Ancona and Caldwell, 1992). Of the boundary spanning activities, representation activities in ESM refer to group membersÕ strategic actions for impression management, which aims to draw seniors amangersÕ attention. In other words, the targeted audiences for representation boundary -spanning activities are the members with more influence and power in the groups or organization (Ancona and Caldwell, 1992; van Osch, Steinfield, & Balogh, 2015). Accordingly, the posters tend to content !!&!aimed to reach the high -level targeted audiences in an attempt to gain support, legitimization or access to important monetary team resources. Hence, part of the success of representational activities should be determined by the extent to which the posting is read and reciprocated by users that display a higher hierarchical level than the original poster. In other words, the hierarchical alignment in the interaction betwe en a poster and responder In addition, to the important of the hierarchical level of the responder, the extent to which the strategic goal of the original boundary spanning activity (i.e., posting) is reciprocated by a response of the same nature is anot her measurement of success, which refers to goal alignment. Such goal alignment serves as a predictor of effectiveness for boundary spanning success. In the case of representational activities, the attempts by the poster to lobby up the corporate hierarchy need to yield responses by senior managers that indicate legitimization or the commitment of additional resources in order to be considered effective and thus successful. Given the open and distributed nature of ESM, questions asked by posters and subsequ ent respondersÕ answers constitute an information exchange between a large amounts of distributed individuals (Kearsley 1976; Ram 1991 ; Wasko and Faraj 2005). Beyond the importance of theses metrics, this study further explores how trust measured by indivi dualsÕ tie strength in dyadic relationships influences the hierarchical and goal alignment of postings for each of three boundary spanning activities Ñrepresentation, information search, and coordination Ñrespectively. For the second boundary -spanning activ ity, referred as information search, van Osch and Steinfield (2015) proposed in their study of the interaction between the postersÕ hierarchical positions and post type that general group members tend to engage in information search activities whereas the higher level leaders or managers are more likely to create representational postings. Additionally, information search and coordination boundary spanning activities in ESM !!'!aim to gain access to relevant information or knowledge and engage in task coordinat ion (Ancona and Caldwell, 1992). These two horizontal boundaries spanning activities Ñas opposed to representational activities Ñrequire hierarchical alignment, as general members in the same level of hierarchical positions would be more likely to engage in the activities mentioned above rather than senior managers . Such hierarchical alignment refers to the alignment for the hierarchical position between poster and responder. However, hierarchical alignment is not important for representation boundary spannin g activities while the information search and coordination boundary spanning activities require higher hierarchical alignment. The study thus would like to explore how the type of boundary spanning activity influences hierarchical and goal alignment respec tively in ESM in terms of their engagement in the representation, information search, and coordination boundary spanning activities. In addition to the targeted audience reached (i.e. hierarchical alignment) and effectiveness (i.e., goal alignment) for boundary spanning success, time lag is crucial as well. Time lag refers to the time between the creation of the original response and th e comment by a responder to the original posting. Although hierarchical and goal alignment are proxies of the effectiveness, time lag could be viewed as a proxy for the efficiency of boundary spanning activity. Subsequently, the efficiency of response may be more critical depending on the urgency of the original posting though overall effectiveness is more likely to be important for boundary spanning success. Existing literature shows that strong ties result in faster information exchange due to the existen ce of a common ground characterized by a shared language (Chiu et al., 2006) and mutual relationship heuristics (Uzzi, 1997). Accordingly, individuals could get quick response from the strong ties they connect with. Therefore, the study wants to investigat e how tie strength influences time lag as one of predictors for boundary spanning success. The study !!(!leverages individualsÕ tie strength of dyadic relationships with others as a proxy for trust to measure the boundary spanning success through hierarchical and goal alignment, and time lag in the postings in terms of three distinct types of boundary spanning activities. !!)!CHAPTER II LITERATURE REVIEW AND HYPOTHESES !The study examines boundary -spanning success in ESM, measured through the hierarchical alignment, goal alignment, and time lag, based on a series of network predictors . The network predictors are the type of boundary spanning activity and tie strength (See the Table 1). Boundary Spanning Type and Hierarchical Alignment !Informat ion search and coordination boundary spanning activities in ESM aim to gain access to relevant information or knowledge and engage in task coordination (Ancona and Caldwell, 1992). These two horizontal boundaries spanning activities require hierarchical alignment, as general members in the same level of hierarchical positions would be more likely to engage in the activities mentioned above rather than senior managers . Such hierarchical alignment refers to the alignment of the hierarchical position between p oster and responder. However, hierarchical alignment is not important for representation boundary spanning activities. Representation activities in ESM refer to group membersÕ strategic actions for impression management, which aims to draw the attention of those higher up the corporate ladder. Although, an acknowledgement by peers may be a intrinsic reward for the person engaging in a representational activity, it could never satisfy the goal of representation boundary spanning activities that aim for the l egitimization or monetary support. So the poster expects the response given by someone who is at least one level higher. Therefore, this leads to the following hypotheses: H1: A posterÕs type of boundary spanning activities in ESM influences the alignment for hierarchical position between a poster and responder. !!*!H1a: A posterÕs representational boundary spanning activity in ESM negatively influences the alignment for hierarchical position between a poster and responder. H1b: A posterÕs coordination boundary spanning activities in ESM positively influences the alignment for hierarchical position between a poster and responder. H1c: A posterÕs information search boundary spanning activities in ESM positively influences the alignment for hierarchical position b etween a poster and responder. Boundary Spanning Type and Goal Alignment !Van Osch and Steinfield (2015) investigate the interactions between the hierarchical positions of posters and boundary spanning activities that they engage in ESM by looking at the post type. The finding revealed that original post and the response had distinct objectives; for instance, an information search posting is met with acknowledgement or legitimization rather than an answer to the original question. Accordingly, this study will look at the goal alignment of postings provided between the parent posting and child response across the entire ESM in terms of their origi nal objective defined by representation, information search, or coordination. For example, a poster publishes a post looking for a coordination opportunity but he or she gets a response in which the responder tries to brag about his or her achievements. Su ch representational response would show a lack of alignment with the original posterÕs goal understanding the effectiveness of the boundary -spanning activity in terms of goal alignment. On the other hand, if a poster publishes a posting looking for a repr esentation or promotion opportunity, he or she receives a response from the responder who does not respond to his or her achievement and rather randomly gives an irrelevant information instead, in which two parties does not attain goal alignment. Specifica lly, when posters ÒtailerÓ or ÒframeÓ their words in the postings to represent themselves, they tend to use ambiguous meaning of words to !!+!cover their aims. For instance, they would not use ÒachieveÓ but rather ÒshareÓ. However, such strategy of equivocalit y does not lead to the desired outcome, as the equivocality suggests an implication for an attempt to deceive in nature. In other words, equivocality would result in the conflict between an aim and a outcome. Unlike the words used in the information or coo rdination boundary spanning activities, the meaning of words is much more obvious and easy for a responder to interpret. Under high equivocality, the two parties would extensively exchange information to clarify the ambiguities till everything reaches the agreement ( Daft and Lengel, 1986). Therefore, this leads to the following hypothesis: H2: A posterÕs type of boundary spanning activities that an individual engages in ESM influences whether or not the type of boundary spanning activity in which a poster o r responder engages is identical. Tie Strength and Goal Alignment !If the goal of responder in the child posting matches the one of poster in the parent posting, the goal alignment between these two individuals would be achieved. In other words, such goal alignment implies the quality of response is positive. On the contrary, if the goals of a poster and responder do not match, the quality of response is negative in terms of achievement of his or her boundary spanning goal. From the perspective of BlauÕs so cial exchange theory (1964), individuals are motivated to gain more expected benefits than exert efforts by contributing in the social interaction in ESM, such as information or knowledge sharing. In dyadic level, tie strength, strong versus weak tie, refe rs to the frequency of interaction for a relationship between a knowledge seeker and source (Granovetter, 1973; Hansen, 1999). Individuals are more likely to gain trust that can safeguard against uncertainty with their s trongly tied others (Krackhardt, 1992). Individuals thus are more willing to engage in effective as well as efficient !!,!communication with strongly tied others due to the obligation of reciprocity (Hung , Chen, and Lin, 2009), which is crucial for coordination boundary spanning activities. Ther efore, this leads to the following hypotheses: H3: The tie strength between a poster and responder in ESM influences whether or not the type of boundary spanning activity in which a poster or responder engages is identical. H3a: Strong tie strength between a poster and responder in ESM positively influences whether or not the type of boundary spanning activity in which a poster or responder engages is identical for the coordination boundary spanning activities . On the other hand, individuals are more likel y to gain access to the sources of novel information with their weakly tied others (Granovetter, 1973), which is beneficial for engaging in information search boundary spanning activities . Therefore, this leads to the following hypothesis: H3b: Weak tie between a poster and responder in ESM positively influences whether or not the type of boundary spanning activity in which a poster or responder engages is identical for information search boundary spanning activities . Tie Strength and Time L ag !Granovetter (1973) proposes that weak ties are the ideal resources for obtaining novel information. However, when it comes to the efficiency of information seeking via the questions posted on ESM, as measured through the time lag between post and respo nse, is crucial for information exchange as well, in addition to the effectiveness of boundary spanning as measure through goal alignment. Specifically, the literature shows that strong relationship are more likely to result in faster responses due to the existence of a common ground characterized by a shared language (Chiu et al., 2006), which would facilitate faster information exchange. Additionally, !!$-!strong emotionally tied dyads are more motivated to help each other because of the relationship heuristic s (Uzzi, 1997), which leads to trust. Accordingly, the study would like to explore the interaction between tie strength and response speed of answer as time lag. So this leads to the following hypothesis: H4: Tie strength between a post and responder in ES M positively influences time lag, the duration of time when a responder to post the comment for a poster Figure 1 The Network Predictors of Boundary Spanning S uccess ./01!01"234536 !7/8390:;! <=03 3"32!6;=5 !>"5! <6:532 6?!@"5:0:A?"A01!01"234536 !>"45 !102 !!!$$!CHAPTER III RESEARCH DESIGN !The study leverages individualsÕ tie strength of dyadic relationships with others as a proxy for trust to measure boundary spanning success via the network predictors, hierarchical alignment, goal alignment, and time lag in terms of three distinct types of boundary spanning activities. The case, data collection, operationalization, and data analysis are discussed in the following sections. Case Organization !This research is conducted in a worldwide provider (hereafter referred to as The Company) of technological products, furnishings, and research and consulting services to workplace offices. The Company has over 11,000 employees around the world. Its headquarters is located in the US with offices and divisio ns in nearly 40 countries including Americas, Europe, Asia, Africa, Oceania, and the Middle East. The Company launched an enterprise social media system (hereafter: the System) built based on the Jive platform in March, 2012. Jive is one of the providers o f corporate social technologies for supporting employeesÕ communication and collaboration activities as well as their business connections by providing a built -in platform with the functionalities for group chat, blogging, and social bookmarking. The adopt ion of the System has grown rapidly due to strong involvement by the executives of company. The usage of System is voluntary and the groups of System serve as open forums in which there are no explicit written policies or hard guidelines. Although the Syst em is not normally treated as a project management tool since the Company has an official project management tool in place, it has been used as the platform for informal communication and social interactions within and between the groups of employees, whic h is beneficial for the early stages of collaboration as well as the awareness of the organization for distributed collaboration. Moreover, the shared content !!$%!enables individuals to engage in inter -unit and inter -group connections by blog postings (referre d as a posting). Data C ollection The study examines the success of boundary spanning activities, measured through the hierarchical alignment, goal alignment, and time lag as explained in the theory section, based on the network predictors, the type of bou ndary spanning activity and tie strength. Data for the analysis was collected from the system log data in which individual usersÕ group interactions within all group types (i.e. open, members only, private, and secret group) and outside of the groups are c aptured. Since the complete population of groups and individual group members is used, no sample approach is used. Operationalization !The study explores trust in terms of individualsÕ tie strength to measure hierarchical alignment, goal alignment, and time lag for three boundary spanning activities, as the proxies of boundary spanning success. Data to test all dependent and independent v ariables are collected using the system log data of the System. Dependent V ariables !The study tests the hierarchical alignment, goal alignment, and time lag as the dependent variables. All three variables are operationalized at the dyadic level. The hierarchical alignment refers to the alignment of hierarchical position between a poster an d responder. If the hierarchical level of the poster is not equal to the responder but lower, the alignment would be measured as 0. If the hierarchical level of the poster and responder is identical, the alignment !!$&!would be measured as 1. If the hierarchica l level of poster is higher than the responder, the alignment would be measured as 2. Goal alignment refers to the score for whether or not the type of boundary spanning activity in which a poster or responder engages is identical. If the type of boundar y spanning activity in which the poster or responder engages is identical Ñe.g., a representational post that is met with recognition Ñthe alignment would be measured as 1. If the type of boundary spanning activity in which a poster or responder engages is n ot identical, the alignment would be measured as 0. Finally, time lag refers to the duration of time that passed between the original publishing of posterÕs boundary -spanning activity and the comment by a responder. Time lag is measured as a continuous va lue in minute units . Individual -level V ariable !The study examines the type of boundary spanning activity as one of the independent variables. The type of activity refers to the type of boundary spanning activity that an individual engages through parent o r child posting. If type of boundary spanning activity that an individual engage in parent or child posting is representation, the type would be measured as 0. If type of boundary spanning activity that an individual engages in parent or child posting is c oordination, the type would be measured as 1. If type of boundary spanning activity that an individual engages in parent or child posting is information search, the type would be measured as 2. The boundary spanning type was determined using a machine lear ning approached as is documented in Van Osch, Steinfield and Zhao (2015). !!$'!Dyadic -level V ariable !Tie strength was measured as a dyadic -level variable. The tie strength refers to the strength of the dyadic tie between poster and responder. If there is no t ie between the poster and responder, the tie would be measured as 0. If there is an indirect (i.ei., weak) tie, the tie would be measured as 1. If there is a no -reciprocal/asymmetric tie, the tie would be measured as 2. If there is a reciprocal/symmetric ( i.e., strong) tie, the tie would be measured as 3. It is important to note that the nature of tie is only one dimension of tie strength and that other dimensions Ñsuch as the frequency of personal interactions or the emotional nature of support Ñare other ch aracteristics of tie strength. However, given the nature of our date set, measuring the directionality of the tie was the most objective metric of tie strength and has been supported by previous literature (Gilbert and Karahalios, 2009) Control V ariable !In addition to testing the effects of boundary spanning type, tie strength, the study employed a group -level variable, group type, as the control variable. The group type refers to the type of group that the poster or responder belongs to, such as open or s ecret group. If the group type is open, the group would be measured as 0. If the group type is open, the group would be measured as 0. If the group type is secret, the group would be measured as 1. Such scale thus represents increasing levels of privacy of the group and its content and interactions, where 0 is visible and 1 is invisible. Table 1 The Variables of Research Model Variable Type Definition Level Measurement/Operation Type of boundary spanning activity IV, DV The type of boundary spanning activity engaged in parent or child posting Individual 0=representation 1=coordination 2=information search !!$(!Table 1 (contÕd) Tie strength IV The strength of dyadic tie between poster and responder Dyadic 0=no tie 1=weak tie (indirect) 2=asymmetric tie 3=symmetric tie Hierarchical alignment DV The alignment for hierarchical position between a poster and responder Dyadic 0=poster!=(not equal to) responder 1=poster=responder Goal alignment DV The score for whether or not the type of boundary spanning ac tivity in which a poster or responder engages is identical Dyadic 0=non-aligned 1=aligned Time lag DV The duration of time that passed between the original publishing of posterÕs boundary -spanning activity and the comment by a responder. Dyadic Continuous value by the unite of minute Group type Control The type of group that the poster or responder belongs to Group 0=open 1=secret !!!!!!!!!!!!$)!CHAPTER IV RESULTS !The study employs Generalized Linear Models (GLMs) and Linear Models (LMs) for testing the hypotheses regarding the effects of boundary spanning type and tie strength on hierarchical alignment, goal alignment, and time lag to measure the boundary spanning success, summarized in Table 2 and discussed as follows. GLMs are best used for the analysis in which dependent variables are categorical and binomial while LMs are best used for the analysis in which dependent variables are continuous, which is only used for testing time lag in this study. With respect to the effect of boundary spanning type on hierarchical alignment, the study found that the type of boundary spanning activity significantly affects hierarchical alignment (! =0.180, p=0.023; where ! is the GLMsÕ coefficient, and p is p -values for this coefficient), which supports H1. Additionally, the study expected a negative significant effect of representational boundary spanning activity on hierarchical alignment ( ! =-.18, p=0.023), which supports H1a. Mo reover, the study expected the significant effect of coordination boundary spanning activity on hierarchical alignment but found the effect to be non -significant ( !=0.245, p=0.219), which disapprove H1b. Furthermore, the results found that information sear ch boundary spanning activity significantly enacts hierarchical alignment (!=0.392, p=0.031), which supports H1c. With the respect to the effect of boundary spanning type on goal alignment, the study also discovered that coordination boundary spanning activity significantly affects goal alignment (!2=0.658, p2=0.030). Similarly, information search boundary spanning activities significantly predict goal alignment ( !3=1.881, p3=0.012). However, as predicted, representational boundary spanning activity doe s not significantly predict goal alignment ( !1=0, p1=0). Therefore, the !!$*!study found that the boundary spanning type affects goal alignment for only information search and coordination activities, which supports H2. With respect to the effect of tie streng th on goal alignment, the study found that the results show that weak tie significantly enacts the strongest goal alignment ( ! 1=1.498, p1=0.051), and asymmetric tie also performs strong ( ! 2=1.402, p2=0.069) effect. However, symmetric (strong) ties signific antly enacts less strong on goal alignment ( ! 3=1.278, p3=0.092) compared to weak tie does. Moreover, the results show that no tie has the smallest effect on goal alignment (! 0=0, p0=0). Thus, suggesting a curvilinear effect where asymmetric and weak ties h ave the strongest effect on goal alignment, with no tie having the weakest effect and strong ties a small effect. With respect to the interaction effect of boundary spanning type and tie strength on goal alignment, symmetric (strong) tie does not signific antly affect coordination boundary spanning activity (! 5=12.901; p5=0.962), which disapproves H3a. In addition to symmetric tie, the other two ties, weak ( ! 1 =13.286; p1=0.960) and asymmetric ( ! 3=14.600; p3=0.957), both do not significantly enact coordinat ion boundary spanning activity . Therefor, tie strength does not significantly enact coordination boundary spanning activity coordination boundary spanning activity. The results also reveal that weak ( ! 2=2.987; p2=0.088) and asymmetric ties (similar to weak tie; ! 4=3.459; p4=0.067) show a more significantly positive effect on information search boundary spanning activity than symmetric (strong) tie (! 6=2.648; p6=0.100) does, which supports H3b. Accordingly, tie strength significantly impacts on information s earch boundary spanning activity but not on representational and coordination boundary spanning activities. Moreover, the results show that no tie type reveals a negative significant effect on goal alignment (!0=0, p0=0). !!$+! With respect to the effect of tie strength on time lag, we found that the results also show that symmetric (i.e. strong) and asymmetric tie significantly enacts the least time lag ( ! 2=-4622; p2=0.05) and ( ! 3=-4534; p3=0.05 ) effect, with stronger ties slightly outperforming asymmetric ties. Weak tie still perform better than no tie, but do incur a longer response time ( ! 1=-42912; p1=0.000). Moreover, the results show that no tie reveals a negative significant effect on time lag (! 0=-.429, p=0.000). Therefore, tie strength significantly affe cts time lag, supporting H4. Table 2 The Summary of R esults Hypothesis Poisson Regr. Coefficient/p -value/Std. Err. Supported H1 ! =0.180; p=0.023; Std. Err.=0.079 Supported H1a (rep.) ! =0; p=0; Std. Err.=0 Supported H1b (coor.) ! =0.245; p=0.219; Std. Err .=0.199 Not supported H1c (info.) ! =0.392; p=0.031; Std. Err.=0.182 Supported H2 ! 1(rep.)=0; p1=0; Std. Err.=0 ! 2 (coor.)=0.658; p2=0.030; Std. Err.=0.304 ! 3 (info.)=1.881; p3=0.012; Std. Err.=0.281 Supported H3 ! 0 (no tie)=0; p0=0; Std. Err.=0 ! 1 (weak)=1.498; p1=0.051; Std. Err.=0.769 ! 2 (asym.)=1.402; p2=0.069; Std. Err.=0.770 ! 3 (sym.)=1.278; p3=0.092 ; Std. Err.=0.759 Supported H3a (sym.*coor .) ! 0 (no tie)=0; p0=0; Std. Err.=0 ! 1 (weak*coor.)=13.286; p1=0.960; Std. Err.=267.709 ! 2 (weak*info .)=2.987; p2=0.088; Std. Err.=1.750 ! 3 (asym.*coor.)=14.600; p3=0.957 ; Std. Err.=267.710 ! 4 (asym.*info.)=3.459; p4=0.067 ; Std. Err.=1.888 ! 5 (sym.*coor.)=12.901; p5=0.962 ; Std. Err.=0.759 ! 6 (sym.*info.)=2.648; p6=0.100 ; Std. Err.=1.608 Not supported H3b (weak*inf o.) ! 0 (no tie)=0; p0=0; Std. Err.=0 ! 1 (weak*coor.)=13.286; p1=0.960; Std. Err.=267.709 ! 2 (weak*info.)=2.987; p2=0.088; Std. Err.=1.750 ! 3 (asym.*coor.)=14.600; p3=0.957 ; Std. Err.=267.710 ! 4 (asym.*info.)=3.4590; p4=0.067 ; Std. Err.=1.888 ! 5 (sym.*coor.)=12.901; p5=0.092 ; Std. Err.=0.759 ! 6 (sym.*info.)=2.648; p6=0.1 ; Std. Err.=1.608 Supported H4 ! 0 (no tie)=0; p0=0; Std. Err.=0 ! 1 (weak)= -42912; p1=0; Std. Err.=12045 ! 2 (asym.)= -4622; p2=0; Std. Err.=12092 ! 3 (sym.)= -4534; p3=0; Std. Err.=117 57 Highly supported !!$,!CHAPTER V DISCUSSION !With respect to hierarchical alignment, individualsÕ information search boundary spanning activities are best able to reach the targeted audiences. Little is known about whether or not an individualÕs coordination boundary spanning activity is able to reac h the audience. Such situation might result from the limited number of such activities, occurring in ESM. ESM is not popular for individuals to complete tasks or projects compared to email, Skype, instant messaging, or any task -oriented communicative tools . As for representational boundary spanning activities in ESM, the study found that individuals tend not to seek hierarchically aligned targeted audiences. With respect to goal alignment, individualsÕ coordination and information search boundary spanning activity respectively result in goal alignment, which leads to greater effectiveness of boundary spanning, whereas representational boundary spanning activity is unable to achieve goal alignment, which refers to lower effectiveness of boundary spanning. Th e messages in coordination and information boundary spanning activities may be much more clear and easy for responders to be interpreted than the ones in representation boundary spanning activity. Individuals who engage in representation boundary spanning activity tend to ÒtailorÓ the words in subtle way but they are thus not easily understood and interpreted. Hence, future research explore the role of message ambiguity in obtaining goal alignment, where it appears that representational postings Ñdue to thei r ÒbraggingÓ nature Ñmay be more ambiguous in nature than information and coordination postings. Moreover, weakly and asymmetrically (similar to weakly) tied individuals who engage in information search boundary spanning activity are the most likely to ach ieve goal alignment, though symmetrically (strongly) tied ones also positively affect goal alignment but revealing a !!%-!smaller relationship. Accordingly, overall, individualsÕ information search boundary spanning activity can lead to greater effectiveness an d the reach of targeted audiences to achieve boundary spanning success. Last, with respect to time lag, the stronger the tie between the dyadic individuals, less time between the responder comment and the original posting created on the posting created by poster. Therefore, strongly tied individuals who engage in information search boundary spanning activities can achieve boundary spanning success, as it is much easier for them to get the response in a short time compared to more weakly tied individual who engage in information search boundary spanning activity. Trust operated by symmetric (strong) tie thus can achieve boundary spanning success. !!%$!CHAPTER VI IMPLICATIONS FOR RESEARCH AND PRACTICE !Representational boundary spanning is a dominant activity due to visibility (Treem and Leonardi, 2012) afforded by ESM but was found to be ineffective because of the lack of goal alignment. Additionally, individuals engaging in representational boundary spanning tend not to seek hierarchically aligned au diences, th ough the visibility afforded by ESM seems promising for individuals to engage in these type of interactions. Moreover, coordination boundary spanning activity is not affected by tie strength and was insignificant in terms of triggering hierarchi cal alignment, though it leads to goal alignment, which creates the effectiveness of boundary spanning success. Furthermore, information search boundary spanning activity is able to reach the targeted audiences because of hierarchical alignment. Informatio n search boundary spanning activity is also effective because of goal alignment when a poster and responder have a weak or asymmetrical (similar with weak tie) tie. Additionally, tie strength is important for information search boundary spanning activity. Although strong tie leads to effective information search boundary spanning activity due to its goal alignment, weak and asymmetrical ties outperform the symmetric (strong) tie, which responders to GranovettorÕs the strength of weak tie. However, tie stren gth is not important for representational and coordination boundary spanning activities. Moreover, strongly tied individuals tend to respond to the question faster than weakly tied ones, which further validates the importance of common ground with shared language (Chiu et al., 2006) and relationship heuristics (Uzzi, 1997) possessed in the pair of individuals with strong relationship. These finding imply that different strategic goals require different types of relationships. For instance, if one needs a hi gh-quality answer, weak ties provide more ÒeffectiveÓ responses. However, if one requires a fast response Ñmaybe in an urgent situation Ñstrong ties offer better support. !!%%! Yet the main theoretical contribution of this study lies in offering a perspective of the dyadic interaction between a poster and a responder and using the dyadic perspective to delineate boundary spanning success. This approach is novel as the existing literature has only focused on the boundary spanning activities as enacted by the indivi dual without considering the reciprocating actions of the responding party which is crucial in determining the success of such activities. In addition for advancing theories of boundary spanning, the study offers practical contribution. The results can pr ovide the managers or higher executive levels individuals with the improvement for information exchange by employing the effect of tie strength on the attainment of quality and quick responses for information search boundary spanning activity in ESM. In ot her words, the managers should encourage individuals who would like to get the best quality of response to ask the questions in open group, as the potential audiences would be much more diverse than the ones in closed group. On the other hand, if an indivi dual who wants to get quick response, he or she could post the questions in the groups where people with whom he or she has ever worked with, as there are more strongly tied individuals there. !!%&!CHAPTER VII LIMITATIONS AND FUTURE RESEARCH !The study aims to explore trust as measured through an individualsÕ tie strength to explore its effects on hierarchical alignment, goal alignment, and time lag Ñas the proxies of boundary spanning success Ñfor the three boundary spanning activities. The stud y does not rule out other predictors for trust and only considers tie strength as a proxy of trust from a network perspective. Other antecedences involve individualÕs geographical diversity, groupÕs maturity, and organizational structure. Similarly, the st udy only looks at boundary spanning success via activities. The study only concludes hierarchical alignment, goal alignment, and time lag for boundary spanning success. Future research could explore external outcome metrics unrelated to ESM use per se, suc h as overall team performance and innovativeness as well as job satisfaction. Additionally, future research could broaden the scope to individual -level and group -level factors, such as individualÕs experience and history of using social media as well as gr oup leadership. !!%'! REFERENCES !!%(!REFERENCES Ancona, D.G. (1990). Outward bound: Strategies for team survival in an organization. Academy of Management Journal 33: 334 -336. Ancona, D. G., & Caldwell, D. F. (1992). Bridging the boundary: External activity and performance in organizational teams. Administrative science quarterly (37). pp. 634-665. Allen, T. (1977). Managing the Flow of Technology. MIT Press . Cambridge, MA. Arrow, K. J. (1974). Limited knowledge and economic analysis. American Economic Review , 64(1), 1-10. Blau, P. M. (1964). Exchange and Power in Social Life, New York, USA: Wiley. Brzozowski, M. J. (2009). Watercooler: exploring an organization through enterprise social Media. GROUP Õ09: Proceedings of the 2009 international conference on supporting group work, pp. 219Ð228. Bulgurcu, B., van Osch, W., & Kane, G. (2015). The Rise of the Promoters: Understanding a New Class of Users and their Participation in Enterprise Online Communities. Unpublished manuscr ipt. Burt, R. (2005). Brokerage and closure: An introduction to social capital. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Chiu, M., Hsu, H., and Wang, T. (2006). Understanding Knowledge Sharing in Virtual Communities, Decision Support Systems, 2006, 42:3, pp. 1872-1888. Daft, R. L., & Lengel, R. H. (1986). Organizational information requirements, media richness and structural design. Management science , 32(5), 554-571. Davies, E. (2003). IETF problem statement. http://www.ietf.org/Internet -drafts/draft -ietf-problem -issue -statement -02.txt . Fleming, L., & Waguespack, D. M. (2007). Brokerage, boundary spanning, and leadershi p in open innovation communities. Organization science , 18(2), 165-180. Fulk, J. and Yuan, Y. C. (2013). Location, motivation, and social capitalization via enterprise social networking. Journal of Computer -Mediated Communication, (19:1) , pp. 20Ð37. Gibb s, J. L., Eisenberg, J., Rozaidi, N. A., & Gryaznova, A. (2014). The ÒmegapozitivÓ role of enterprise social media in enabling cross -boundary communication in a distributed Russian organization. American Behavioral Scientist , 0002764214540511. !!%)! Gilbert, E. , & Karahalios, K. (2009). Predicting Tie Strength with Social Media. In Proceedings of the 27th International Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems, pp. 211-220. Granovetter, M. (1973). The Strength of Weak Ties. American Journal of Sociology , 78:1360 -1380. Hansen, M. T. (1999). The search -transfer problem: The role of weak ties in sharing knowledge across organization subunits. Administrative science quarterly , 44(1), 82-111. Hung, S. -W., Chen, C. -J., and Lin, M. -J. J. (2009). Fostering the Determinants of Knowledge Sharing in Professional Virtual Communities. Computers in Human Behavior, 25:4, pp. 929-939. Jones, G.R. & George, J.M. (1998). ÔThe Experience and Evolution of Trust: Implications for Cooperation and TeamworkÕ. Academy of Management Review. 23(3) . pp. 531Ð546 Kearsley, G. P. (1976). Questions and Question Asking in Verbal Discourse: A Cross - Disciplinary Review, Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 5:4, pp. 355-375. Kogut, B., & Zander, U. (1992). Knowledge of the firm, combinative capabilities, and the replication of technology. Organization science , 3(3), 383-397. Krackhardt, D. (1992). The strength of strong ties: The importance of philos in organizations. Networks and organizations: Structure, form, and action , 216, 239. Lawler, E. J., and Thye, S. R. (1999). Bringing Emotions into Social Exchange Theory. Annual Review of Sociology, 25 , pp. 217-244 Majchrzak, A., Faraj, S., Kane, G. C., & Azad, B. (2013). The contradictory influence of social media affordances on online communal knowledge sharing. Journal of Computer - Mediated Communication, (19: 1) , pp. 38Ð55. Molm, L. D. (1994). Dependence and Risk: Transforming and Structure of Social Exchange. Social Psychology Quarterly , 57: 163 -176. Noteboom , B. & Six, F. (2003). The Trust Process in Organizations. Edward Elgar Publishing . Northhampton, MA. Ram, A., ÒA Theory of Questions and Question Asking,Ó Journal of the Learning Sciences, 1991, 1:3, pp. 273 -318. Steinfield, C., DiMicco, J.M., Ellison, N.B., and Lampe, C. (2009). Bowling online: social networking and social capital within the organization. Proceedings of the fourth international conference on Communities and technologies (C&T '09), ACM , New York, !!%*!NY, USA, pp. 245 -254. Stephenson Jr, M. (2005). Making humanitarian relief networks more effective: operational coordination, trust and sense making. Disasters , 29(4), 337-350. Treem, J. W., & Leonardi, P. M. (2012). Social media use in organizations: Exploring the affordances of visibility, ed itablity, persistance, and association. Communication Yearbook (36). Tushman, M. (1977). Special boundary roles the innovation process. Admin. Sci. Quart. 22 587-605. Tushman, M. L., & Scanlon, T. J. (1981). Boundary spanning individuals: Their role in information transfer and their antecedents. Academy of Management Journal. 24(2) . pp. 289-305. Uzzi, B. (1997). Social structure and competition in interfirm networks: The paradox of em-beddedness. Administrative Science Quarterly, 42. 35-67. Van Osch , W, & Steinfield, C. W. (2013). Boundary Spanning through Enterprise Social Software: An External Stakeholder Perspective. Proceeding of 2013 International Conference on Information Systems (ICIS 2013) . Van Osch, W., Steinfield, C. W., & Balogh, B. A. (2015). Enterprise Social Media: Challenges and Opportunities for Organizational Communication and Collaboration. In System Sciences (HICSS), 2015 48th Hawaii International Conference on (pp. 763-772). IEEE. Van Osch, W., & Steinfield, C. W. (2015). Intra -Organizational Boundary Spanning: Strategic Implications for the Implementation and Use of Enterprise Social Media. Unpublished manuscript. Van Osch, W., Steinfield, C. W., and Zhao (2015). Team Boundary Spanning through Enterprise Social Media: Explorin g the Effects of Group -Level Diversity Using a Data Science Approach. Unpublished manuscript. Wasko, M. M., Teigland, R., and Faraj, S. (2009). The provision of Online Public Goods: Examining Social Structure in an Electronic Network of Practice, Decision Support Systems, 47:3, pp. 254-265. !