ERCREASENG EMPATHSC MPABleES {if EWGATIONAL CHANGE AGENTS: EVALUATiON 0F THE EFFECT OF A mama-Fame mmnom 3mm - f, . 5 ; ‘ i; iiissertation for the. Degree of Ph‘. 53. {WCHlGAN S?ATE ENEVERSEW ARTHUR i... SAVAGE. ER.- 3973 , .‘q..~.....~_....~v.r--.' mwfi rflJBRARY } Michigan Stan: University This is to certify that the thesis entitled "Increasing Empathic Capabilities of Educational Change Agents: Evaluation of the Effect of a Three-phase Instructional Strategy" presented by Arthur L. Savage, Jr. has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for the Pho Do degree in J“ ”Cé "7,: £40 as: {I 0 H N Curr/cu /o :7; Secondary Education and Curriculum ’23"th ' t “‘42! d 9—K ( fiajor pro(e$:0r7 Erling S. Jbrgensen Date 20,19 73 J 0-7639 lemmas av‘ ‘ Hols"! an8' 880K MUERY IN? . LIBRARY BINDER r :mmrcn new ; e~ # ‘ I Mlmorr" J H ‘1 Ln fiv a ABSTRACT INCREASING EMPATHIC CAPABILITIES OF EDUCATIONAL CHANGE AGENTS: EVALUATION OF THE EFFECT OF A THREE-PHASE INSTRUCTIONAL STRATEGY BY Arthur L. Savage, Jr. The present research was an attempt to adapt an exist- ing instructional strategy to the training of instructional developers relative to the improvement of their interpersonal communication skills with emphasis on their empathic capa- lxilities. The adaptation was evaluated as to its effective- ness in increasing the affective sensitivity and co-orienta- tional accuracy of the subjects. The experimental treatment was administered in three gfiuases: (l) a filmed lecture-demonstration dealing with the abiliiy'of the subjects as interviewers with emphasis on the identification of interviewer responses as exploratory or non—exploratory, listening or non-listening, honest labeling or distorting, and cognitive or affective. The lecture was followed by a practice session in response discrimination with feedback. (2) Videotape Recall of Affect Simulation (VRAS) during which the subjects viewed and responded to six stimulus films illustrative of various kinds of remarks or Arthur L. Savage, Jr. comments typical of communication between the instructional developer and his client. The subjects were videotaped dur- ing the viewing of and responding to the vignettes; later they were assisted by a trained recall worker to analyze and understand their cognitive and affective reactions to the films while viewing a playback of the videotape. (3) Inter- personal Process Recall (IPR) in which one subject in the role of an instructional developer conducted a simulated interview with another subject in the role of an instructor- client. This activity was also videotaped and the same re- call process described in Phase II (above) was conducted. In the recall process, the recall worker encourages the subject to stop the playback frequently and comment upon any thought or emotional feeling which the subject experienced during the original activity. Though the recall worker urges the subject to critique his own behavior, at no time does he offer evaluative comments on the subject's responses. The subjects (N = 13) were doctoral candidates in In- structional Development and Technology in the Department of Secondary Education and Curriculum at Michigan State Univer- sity. They were also Fellows in two special media institutes funded by the United States Office of Education. The sub- jects were not randomly drawn from a defined population, but were selected for the study upon the basis of the Fellowships held in the two institutes. The research was begun in the Arthur L. Savage, Jr. Winter Term and concluded in the Spring Term of the 1972-73 academic year. The subjects were given two pre-tests and two-post- tests with the experimental treatment as an intervening variable. The pre-tests were measures of affective sensi- tivity and co-orientational accuracy. The findings permitted the rejection of the null hypothesis relative to affective sensitivity, but would not permit the rejection of the hy- pothesis relative to co-orientational accuracy beyond the .05 level of confidence. It was inferred that the experimen- tal treatment increased the subjects' ability to assess the affective state of another. It was not determined whether the failure to reject the null hypothesis relative to co- orientational accuracy was due to inadequacies in the experi- mental treatment, inadequacies in the measuring instruments, or to limitations inherent in the design of the study. Two conclusions were made from the findings summarized above. 1. The subjects' ability to accurately assess the affec- tive state of another did increase from pre-test to post-test. The precise cause of the increase is not known. 2. The subjects' ability to predict the Opinions and belief system of another (co-orientational accuracy) did not increase from pre-test to post-test. INCREASING EMPATHIC CAPABILITIES OF EDUCATIONAL CHANGE AGENTS: EVALUATION OF THE EFFECT OF A THREE-PHASE INSTRUCTIONAL STRATEGY BY Arthur L. Savage, Jr. A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Secondary Education and Curriculum 1973 ‘ DEDICATION MOTHER AND DAD Most of my early formative years were spent during the Depression of the 19305. Many in our small Mississippi town lost much in the way of material possessions. This proved to be rigorous support for their contention that education, once attained, was something that could never be lost. Though she did not live to see this symbol of ultimate attainment, I strongly feel that she knows that one of her greatest ambitions for me has been realized. and BONNIE Climbing with another may be prompted by a will to reach peaks that a person cannot reach alone. --Walter Kaufmann ii ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS So many participated in this study and made such self- less contributions, that the researcher has taken great care to see that no one who made a contribution to its achievement is omitted. First, the Guidance Committee: ...Dr. Erling S. Jorgensen, the major professor, whose sage counsel and advice and ready wit made the project not only a scholarly work ... but a pleasure to do; ...Dr. Kent L. Gustafson whose dissertation served as a model for this one and who provided the original idea; who as teacher and friend helped shape much of my academic career; ...Dr. Ronald G. Rex who before this project was under- taken had become a close friend, and who I hope will remain such long after it has been put away; ...Dr. Norman Kagan who initially developed the experi- mental treatments herein utilized, and whose scholarly back- ground in interpersonal relations provided a major source of knowledge and in-put for this study; ...and Dr. Thomas F. Baldwin, the cognate advisor, who has served as a model for me from the outset of my academic career, and who epitomizes for me scholarly inquirer, iii dedicated researcher, outstanding teacher, and dear friend; ... to these gentlemen belong the major credit for whatever appears herein. The researcher was merely a medium through which they all expressed themselves. Deep appreciation goes to the faculty members who served as actors for the stimulus films. Mr. Barry D. Bratton, Dr. Sarah Boling, Dr. Nicholas J. Fiel, Dr. Ronald G. Rex, Mr. Richard E. Snoke, Jr., and Dr. Robert C. Ward endured incredible problems and complications ... and turned in most professional performances. With reference to the making of the films, most sincere thanks goes, also, to Mr. James R. Respress, without whose expert technical knowl- edge of both film and electronic recording technique, the films would not have been possible. Grateful thanks goes also to those Michigan State Univer- sity faculty members who served as interviewees for the pre- tests and post-tests: Dr. Robert Schlater, Dr. J. Colby Lewis, Jr., Dr. J. David Lewis, Professor Arthur Weld, Dr. John D. Abel, Dr. Robert M. Babe, Dr. Kenward Atkin, Dr. Bradley S. Greenberg, Dr. Lawrence Sarbaugh, Dr. David Ralph, Professor Donald Cushman, Professor Yvonne Waskin, and Dr. Thomas F. Baldwin were all most generous with their time. The subjects in the study probably endured as many hard- ships and inconveniences as any involved in the project. In Spite of heavy demands on their time by classes, projects, iv field trips, and their own research commitments, they always found time to do what was asked of them ... and most always on time. They were Sister Mary Ann Brady, Mr. David Broski, .Mr. Charles Brown, Mr. Larry Donahue, Mr. David Gillmore, .Mr. Joel Fleming, Mr. Randy Gross, Mr. Adelbert Jones, Mr. Oswell Person, Mr. Robert Price, Mr. Lawrence Redd, Mr. John Stamper, and Mr. Paul Woodworth. They were, indeed, the very heart of the study, and much gratitude is due them. The recall workers volunteered their time and consider- able talents, and the researcher is deeply grateful to Dr. Michael J. Doyle, M.D., Mr. John Casbergue, Mr. Sam Ramtu, and Mr. Harold J. Spooner. All those noted above played essential roles in the study itself, but there were those who made major contribu- tions to the study without being directly involved in it. Specific reference is made to Dr. Paul W. F. Witt for whom the researcher worked as graduate administrative assistant during the course of the research, and who always saw to it that duties and responsibilities never interferred with the research; to Virginia B. Foster who has pulled more thanbone doctoral candidate through his program with ready words of encouragement and genuine concern; and especially to the late Professor Leo A. Martin who served as friend, teacher, and sagacious advisor for more than a quarter of a century, and without whose assistance and support this project would never have become a reality. Finally, to Katie and the late George Smith who gave me she who means more to me than life itself ... and who probably paid more dearly than anyone for the attainment of this goal. vi TABLE OF CONTENTS CHAPTER Page I. INTRODUCTION AND THEORY OF STUDY. . . . . . . . 1 Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 Importance of the Study. . . . . . . . . . . 2 Definitions. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 6 Theory Underlying the Study. . . . . . . . . 8 Elements of Interpersonal Communication . 8 Change Agent-Client Relationship. . . . . 10 Hypotheses . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 13 Summary. . . . . . . . .’. . . . . . . . . . 14 II 0 REVIEW OF THE LITERATURE O O O O O O O O O O O O 15 Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 15 Scales and Measurements. . . . . . . . .‘. . 16 Empathy Training . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 22 Related Studies in Empathy . . . . . . . . . 25 III 0 DESIGN OF THE STUDY 0 O O O O O O O O O O O O O 34 The Sample . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 34 The Treatment. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 36 Phase I . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 36 Phase II. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 37 Phase III . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 41 The Recall Workers . . . . . . . . . . . . . 44 Instruction. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 46 Affective Sensitivity . . . . . . . . . . 46 Co-orientational Accuracy . . . . . . . . 48 The Experimental Design. . . . . . . . . . . 52 Statistical Hypotheses . . . . . . . . . . . 53 Analysis of the Data . . . . . . . . . . . . 54 Limitations of the Study . . . . . . . . . . 57 Summary. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 60 IV. FINDINGS O I O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O 62 Findings . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 62 Discussion of the Findings . . . . . . . . . 67 vii TABLE OF CONTENTS--Continued CHAPTER V. SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS. . . . . . . . . . Summary . . . . . . . . . . . . . Conclusions . . . . . . . . . . . Implications for Fu ure Research. Implications for Education. . . . O O O O O BIBLIWRAPHY. O O O O O O O O O I O O O Q 0 O O O APPENDICES A. SCRIPTS FOR THE FILMS. . . . . . . . . . . B. VIDEOTAPE RECORDER, MONITOR AND EQUIPMENT ARRANGEMENT. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . C. DIAGRAM OF EQUIPMENT ARRANGEMENT . . . . . D. SAMPLES OF TEST ITEMS. . . . . . . . . . . E. FACULTY INSTRUMENT OF ATTITUDE . . . . . . F. FACULTY INSTRUMENT OF ATTITUDE . . . . . . viii Page 72 72 81 84 90 92 96 99 100 101 113 117 LIST OF TABLES AND FIGURE TABLE Page 1. Comparison of Pre-test and Post-test Means and Mean Difference of the Affective Sensitivity Scale. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 63 2. Comparison of Pre-test and Post-test correla- tion Means, Including Comparison of Partial Correlation. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 66 3. Comparison of Pre-test and Post-test Mean and Mean Difference of Measure of Co-orientation . 67 FIGURE 1. A General Model of Co-orientation. . . . . . . 48 ix CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION AND THEORY OF STUDY Introduction The research reported in this report was conducted at Michigan State University during the winter and spring quarters of the 1972-73 academic year. The primary purpose of the study was to develop an instructional strategy that would increase the interpersonal communication skills of doctoral candidates in Instructional Development and Tech- nology within the Department of Secondary Education and Curriculum in the College of Education. An additional pur- pose of the study was to evaluate the proposed instructional strategy as to its capability to assist the subjects to enter into a more empathic, or accurate, relationship with another. The experimental experiences included a filmed lecture and demonstration dealing with the identification and classi- fication of various types of responses characteristic of interpersonal communication between two individuals. In a second experience, the subjects viewed and reacted to special- ly designed and produced stimulus films which simulated specific affects on the part of faculty members. During the viewing sessions, the subject was videotaped to assist him in later recalling his feelings and emotions while viewing and responding to the films. A third experience involved the simulation of a two-person interview during which the subject attempted to identify and react to the cognitive and affective dimensions of the comments of the interviewee. This process was also videotaped to assist the subject in appraising his reactions to the interviewee during subsequent playback of the videotape in the presence of a recall worker. If the experimental strategies employed in this study enable practitioners of instructional development and tech- nology to enter into more empathic relationships with their clients and to establish more open and facilitative channels of communication, the instructional developer's mission to assist in the solution of instructional problems and to pro- vide alternative modalities of instruction will be consider- ably enhanced. Importance of the Study Students who enroll in the study of Instructional DeveloPment and Technology at Michigan State University come to the academic program from varied backgrounds of education- al and professional experience. Some come directly from master's degree programs in education or other disciplines with no practical experience as classroom teachers; some come as former classroom teachers or administrators in elementary and secondary school systems; and some have been librarians or audio-visual co-ordinators. Their previous experiences as students or educators are investigated and documented prior to their acceptance into the doctoral pro- gram. Proposed programs of study have as a referencial base the candidate's cognitive and psychomotor skills derived from previous educational and/or practical experiences. Upon these bases, the candidates' academic programs are designed and formulated to bridge the perceived gaps between entry behavior and competencies and desired terminal behavior and competencies. The constituent experiences of each student's academic program are intended to expand cognitive skills and competencies, and, to somewhat a lesser degree, psychomotor skills and competencies. There are, however, no provisions made in the academic programs, as they are presently consti- tuted, to deal with the students' deficiencies in empathic capabilities, especially as such deficiencies are related to interpersonal communication skills. By and large, the student leaves the academic program with essentially the same inter- personal communication skills, or lack of skills, that he brought to the program. He receives no instructional experi- ences which are specifically designed to assist him to func- tion successfully as a change agent within an educational system, which is what many perceive a large portion of his role to be. Thiagarajan (1973) indicates that the need for highly- developed interpersonal communication skills by instructional developers is critical in forming productive relationships with those who come to him for assistance. Further, he indi- cates that lack of training in interpersonal communication skills is common. With specific reference to his cooperative endeavors with subject matter experts, he says: Ever since my early days in the programmed instruction business, I have been trained to function as an instruc- tional developer (ID) along with a subject matter expert (SME). The advantages of such team work have been repeatedly pointed out to me. But nobody warned me about the problems of working with all those insecure and paranoid teammates. None of the programming courses, workshops, or other textbooks seem to have as an objective the improvement of interpersonal communica- tion skills so vital for an SME and an ID to work to- gether (p. 1). It is not intended that the previous citation reflect the divers negative psychic conditions of subject matter experts, but rather to point up the critical need for highly-developed interpersonal communication skills by instructional devel- Opers. Further support for this position is contributed by Davies (1973). He notes that the field of instructional develOpment is a design form which involves systematic inter- vention into an on-going operational system, and comments as follows regarding the importance of an open and facilitative relationship between the instructional developer and his client organization: Instructional developers and evaluators, therefore, need to develop a special relationship with their client organization--whether it be a complete school, college within that school, division, department or small team of instructors responsible for teaching a specific course. This relationship, if it is overlook- ed or undervalued, can threaten, undermine, and even nullify the very accomplishment of the task itself. Creating and nurturing healthy, helpful and enriching relationships between developers/evaluators and their clients does take a great deal of time and effort, but the investment yields a very high return indeed in the form of goodwill, cooperation and general feelings of satisfaction (p. l). The successess of alumni of the Instructional Develop- znent and Technology emphasis at Michigan State University bear strong witness to the general excellence of the academic program as it has been constituted for the past several years. These persons, as students, did not have the benefit of training in interpersonal communication. One wonders what might have been accomplished in the intervening years had such experiences been available to them as students as they passed through the program. The experimental training techniques utilized in this study have had a measure of success in training personnel counselors, psychologists, and physicians in establishing facilitative rapport with their clients. If these techniques can be adapted to the setting of professional educators, and instructional developers in particular, then the current academic program in Instructional DeveloPment and Technology can be improved by academic experiences that will train instructional develoPers to function in their perceived roles. That is the main potential of this particular piece of re- search. The techniques which were utilized are simple enough to be adaptable to any academic program which has as its pur- pose the training of instructional develOpers. Definitions A number of terms used in the subsequent discussion require Specific definition. Such terms as "empathy" and "affect" are more commonly found in the literature of psycho- logical inquiry; terms such as "change agent" are more applicable to the literature in the field of communication theory. Inasmuch as these terms may not be commonly employed in the standard vocabulary of the educator, it is deemed advisable to establish a common referent. Affect The feeling, emotions, or mental state of an individual, especially during interpersonal communication. Affective Sensitivity The quantifiable ability of one individual to assess and personally identify with the mental state of another. The term is usual;y synonymous with other such terms as "empathy," "co-orientation,” "co-orientational accuracy," and "'real"accuracy." Recall Worker Kagan's term for a specially trained individual to aid subjects in recalling feelings and emotions during video- tape playback of simulations. Simulation Use of film and projector to create situations of pre— determined and demonstrated affect to subjects; also refers to the assumption of specified roles in dyadic communications. Simulator Small room containing two chairs, film projector, film, screen and partially concealed television camera, monitor, videotape recorder, and microphone. Accuracy Term employed by Wackman referring to a comparison of individual A's prediction of individual B's rating of any given Object (O). Represented: ApBrO. Agreement Term employed by Wackman referring to the comparison of individual A's rating of any given Object (O), and indi- vidual B's rating of the same object. Represented as ArO and BrO respectively. -~\ 5% Congruence Term employed by Wackman referring to the comparison of individual A's prediction of individual B's rating of any given Object (0), represented ApBrO, and A's own rating of the Object (0). Theory Underlying the Study In order to establish an adequate theoretical base for the study, it is necessary to identify and more closely examine those elements involved in interpersonal communica- tion. The theoretical position is that an instructional developer functions, in part, as a change agent, and that his ability to bring about change, including the adoption of innovation, is a direct function of his ability to enter into a facilitative relationship with his client. This may be an individual instructor or the collective members of an instruc- tional organization, viz, an academic department or college. The balance of this section is devoted to a discussion of the role of the instructional developer and the dimensions of his relationship with his clients. Elements of Interpersonal Communication Even the most intuitive kind of observation would indi- cate that there are many elements involved in successful interpersonal communication. One of the most obvious is a commonality of language. Unless dyads in interpersonal rela- tions speak the same language, the likelihood of them estab- lishing an effective relationship through interpersonal communication is remote. The same kind of observation might reasonably be made with regard to writing skills. The more effective communication will result if the initiator of a communication in written form is capable of putting his notions on paper in a lucid and cogent style. Commonality of language and fluency of writing are per- haps the more basic elements involved in effective inter- personal communications. There are, however, other sophisticated elements about which one should be concerned. The concepts extroversion and affiliation were determined by Lorr and McNair (1965) to be main dimensions of interpersonal interaction. Titus and Hollander (1959) studied the dominr ance-submission relationship in dyads and found that comple- mentarity between these two factors enhanced communication. Argyle (1969) writes extensively of what he refers to as "social competence." It is his contention that it is comprised of cognitive skills which people can be trained to perform or to utilize in interpersonal transactions. He states, for example, that social anxiety is incapacitating, and that techniques have been devised to modify it. Training is available, also, to heighten awareness of both verbal and non-verbal elements of interaction. He further indicates that 10 the "poised and self-confident performer" should be able to present himself clearly to others without concealment, exaggeration, or embarrassment; that in so doing, his inter- personal relationships will be of a very positive nature. Change Agent-Client Relationship It has been previously noted that Davies suggests that the instructional develOper systematically intervenes in an on-going operational system. He states that the purpose of such intervention is to increase the effectiveness of the instructional system. The inference is clear that his inter- vention is accompanied by the introduction of change. Miller (1971) and Davis et 2l° (1972) support the notion that the introduction of innovation into instructional systems is a prime function of the instructional developer. Nord (1973) states: "It means the instructibnal developer must act both in the capacity of product developer and change agent in a people world." (p. 8ff) (Italics supplied). In further consideration of the instructional developer as change agent, Rogers and Shoemaker (1971) supply the following definition: A change agent is a professional who influences innova- tion-decisions in a direction deemed desirable by a change agency. In most cases, he seeks to secure the adoption of new ideas.... (p. 227). It is the present contention that the interpersonal communication between the instructional developer, functioning 11 in the role of change agent, and his client is dependent on identifiable variables; that the relationship between the instructional developer and his client has a direct bearing on the regard that the client will hold for any proposed innovation or change. Newcomb (1953), in introducing his now widely-noted A-B-X theory of co-orientation, holds that the communication between individuals is not separable from their orientation toward the object of their communication. He said: ... communication among human beings performs the essen— tial function of enabling two or more individuals to maintain simultaneous orientation toward one another as communicators and toward objects of communication (p. 393). Rogers and Shoemaker (1971) also note that the change agent and his innovation are perceived in essentially the same light. It is their belief that unless the change agent is able to effect a facilitative relationship, the elements of which are notable, his chances of securing adoption of his proposed innovation are diminished. Their treatment of this relationship is specifically: Once a need for change is created, a change agent must develop rapport with his clients. He may enhance his relationship with his clients by creating an impression of credibility, trustworthiness, and empathy with their needs and problems. Clients must accept the change agent before they accept the innovations he promotes (p. 229). It is perceived, therefore, that empathy plays a most highly contributory role in the establishment of a facilitative 12 interpersonal relationship between the change agent and his client. It seems reasonable to assume that change agents can anticipate a greater degree of success in their efforts to effect change if they are capable of establishing an empathic relationship with their clients. Two other factors play important roles in productive interpersonal communication: homophily and heterophily. Again, Rogers and Shoemaker provide an apt definition: "Homophily is the degree to which pairs of individuals who interact are similar in certain attributes, such as beliefs, values, education, social status, and the like" (p. 240). Heterophily is noted to be an angnymn for homophily. They also provide a comment as to how homophily bears on communi- cation: More effective communication occurs when source and receiver are homophilous. When they share common mean- ings, a mutual subculture language, and are alike in personal and social characteristics, the communication of ideas is likely to have a greater effect in terms of knowledge gain, attitude formation and change, and overt behavior change. When homophily is present in communication, therefore, interaction is likely to be more rewarding to both source and receiver (p. 15). The implication is plain that homophily-heterophily are naturally-occurring states. One might readily suppose that a state of artificial homophily might be created for the express purpose of facilitating a more productive relation- ship, but this is not likely to produce a lasting relation- ship of increasing productivity over the long haul. In fact, 13 if the artificiality of the relationship is discovered by the client, dysfunctionalism is likely to occur. The implications for the instructional deve10per as re- gards homophily are apparent. If he and his client are in a state of homophily, his expectations of success are enhanced; if the state of the relationship is characterized by heterophily, the instructional developer must exert greater effort in establishing a productive relationship with his client. The fact that homophily-heterOphily are uncontrol- lable variables in interpersonal communication processes makes the empathic capabilities of the change agent that much more critical to the adoption of innovation. Hypotheses The preceding discussion generates the following hypothe- ses for testing. Identical hypotheses, stated in stastically testable form will be found in Chapter III. H1: Subject candidates in Instructional Development and Technology receiving the experimental treatment will have a significantly higher mean score on a measure of affective sensitivity from pretest to posttest. H : Subject candidates in Instructional Development and Technology receiving the experimental treatment will have a significantly higher mean score on a measure of co-orientational accuracy from pretest to posttest. 14 Summary The foregoing sections outlining the theory underlying the study have attempted to present a framework for the study. Briefly, the researcher has attempted to indicate the importance of interpersonal communication skills in the professional activities of the instructional developer as he functions in the role of change agent within an educa- tional system. Some elements which have contributory capa- bilities toward successful interpersonal relationships be- tween the instructional developer and his clients have been noted. These were empathy, accuracy, and homophily. The noted absence of training for future instructional developers in the improvement of interpersonal communication skills, and the importance of having these skills developed to a productive, functional level establishes a need for the development of an instructional strategy in this area. The design and testing of such a strategy should be apprOpriate objects of fruitful research. CHAPTER II REVIEW OF THE LITERATURE Introduction In the previous chapter, it was pointed out that one who functions as an instructional developer is often per- ceived as an innovator or change agent. As such, the prob- abilities of his success are directly tied to his ability to establish a facilitative communication relationship with his clients. It was also pointed out that his ability to establish an empathic relationship with those with whom he is professionally involved is a positive ingredient in facilitative communication. At the core of the instructional system herein prOposed and tested is a treatment, the purpose of which is to heighten the subject's capabilities of enter- ing into an empathic relationship with another--to increase his affective sensitivity. Affective sensitivity is only one aspect of the more global skill, empathy, which is character- ized by Kagan 25 31. (1967) as the "ability to detect and describe the immediate affective state of another, or in terms of communication theory, the ability to receive and decode affective communication” (p. 463). This review of the liter- ature will deal with studies in empathy as they relate to 15 16 measurements and scales designed to detect the amount and presence of empathic capability. It will also deal with systems and devices designed to heighten empathic capability. Scales and Measurements One of the earlier and more notable attempts to devise a scale to measure empathy was undertaken by Dymond (1949). She set forth her concept of empathy as "the imaginative tranSposing of oneself into the thinking, feeling, and acting of another, and so structuring the world as he does. (p. 127). A six-item, five-point rating scale was constructed. Different groups were given problemponiented tasks to solve and were asked, during the problem-solving sessions, to note the structure and function of the group. Person A was asked to rate himself on the scale, to rate person B on the scale, and to indicate how he thought person B would rate him, A, on the scale. She found on test and re-test that empathy could be isolated and measured. Chapman (1966) used a videotape recording technique to playback scenes of personnel counselors and their clients in which identifiable emotions were displayed. Subjects noted the presence or absence of empathic reaction, and responded to a three-choice multiple item instrument dealing with counselor effectiveness with regard to each emotional display. It should be noted that most of the work done in the development of empathic scales and training has been 17 accomplished in the area of personnel counselor training. It was in this area that Campbell (1971) developed and vali- dated a scale to measure affective sensitivity in personnel counselors. The videotape recording technique was again utilized employing actual counselor-client interviews. The subjects in the study were shown the recorded interviews and were asked to respond to multiple choice items consist— ing of one correct answer and two distractors dealing with the client's feelings about himself and his feelings about the counselor. The conclusion was reached that affective sensitivity is a measurable trait and that it can be de- veloped through training. It has been noted that the presence of empathy in dyadic communication is a facilitative ingredient, but it is neces- sary that the empathy offered by one member of the dyad be perceived by the other. This was the subject of a study by Caracena and Vicory (1969) in which they attempted to dis- tinguish between empathy objectively offered and empathy achieved or successfully communicated. Utilizing VTRs of twenty-two counselor-client interviews, subjects were asked to rate the interviews as to empathy offered, considering such factors as verbal patterns in interchanges and verbal domi- nance. Other subjects rated the same interviews as to sub- jective behavior. Findings indicated that many factors enter into the perceptive realization of the empathic relationship, 18 and that the presence of empathy can be explicitly indicated, e.g., decreasing verbosity is associated with the presence of empathy. As the counselor-client relationship has been the set- ting for studies of empathic relationships, the physician- patient setting has also been studied. When the patient presents himself at a clinic or a physician's office for treatment, he must be able to communicate to the physician the nature of his malady and other important data which the physician requires to arrive at an accurate diagnosis and sub- sequent treatment. It is important to note, however, that the burden of establishing a communicative relationship be- tween the physician and the patient is most frequently on the physician rather than on the patient. The physician-patient communication system was the sub- ject of a study by Hawes (1971) in which interview styles between physician and patient were able to determine criterion behavior, and to establish whether interview styles had in- fluence over the process of interviewing. Four physicians each interviewed four patients to obtain personal medical histories and to make preliminary diagnoses of possible dis- orders. Results of the interviews were later correlated with personal histories of the patients as they recorded them on self-administered questionnaires. Diagnoses were later corre- lated with actual clinical findings. The study concluded 19 that those physicians who conducted the interviews in a facilitative manner, i.e., one in which empathy was exhibited, obtained a significant correlation between both the personal medical history questionnaire completed by the patient and by the clinical findings of physical disorder. Among communication researchers, predictive tests for empathic capability (co-orientational accuracy) have been the most popular in the literature. In such tests one person is asked to make predictive statements as to another's feelings about himself or a third party or object (see Dymond, 1949, above). Astin (1967) devised a situational test to assess empathy. The subjects estimated the value structure of others on the Allport-Vernon-Lindzey Study of Values. Other subjects responded to recorded statements by others. The two procedures were then compared by trained psychologists for ranking in terms of the amount of empathy represented in the A-V-L Study of Values and the responses to the recorded state- ments° It was found that the latter ranked higher in the quantity of empathy present. The importance of empathy as an element in successful communication has been previously noted. One might naturally wonder if there are conditions, situations, or feelings which might inhibit the ability of a member of a dyad to exhibit empathy to the other. Zimmer and Anderson (1968) selected the construct of positive regard as a possible deterent to the 20 exhibition of empathy, and attempted to investigate a pos- sible connection between them. Specifically, they sought to determine whether positive regard and empathy as constructs are influenced by isolated factors, and to discover whether positive regard and empathy can be described as one con- struct on a single dimension. A VTR counseling session with a client was rated by one set of judges for positive regard and by another set of judges for empathy-understanding. It was determined that positive regard and empathy are ortho- gonally—related factors, and that when they are looked at in terms of multiple factors are definable and public rather than undefinable and private. The implication is that where there is an absence of high positive regard, the likelihood of establishing an empathic relationship is diminished. The multi-dimensional concept of empathy was studied by Greenberg 25 11. (1969) in an attempt to isolate individual factors, or dimensions, of empathic judgment. Thirty-one professional clinicians viewed eleven counselor-client sessions on VTR. Each rated the client's exhibited feelings on a twenty-six-item bi-polar scale of opposite adjectives which were descriptive of connotative or emotional qualities. Three empathic dimensions emerged from factor analysis: Dependency, Anger-Hostility, and Avoidance; these appeared in over fifty per cent of the ratings. Several different methodologies for detecting the presence of empathy and for attempting to quantify it have been 21 produced, as will be noted. One of the more widely noted of these is the Bennet-Lennard Relationship Inventory. It was designed in 1962 as a measure of empathic understanding and unconditionality of regard. The reliability of the scale was established by Walker and Little (1969), through factor analysis. Subjects were divided among and inter- viewed in a therapeutic setting by sixteen counselors. The subjects then rated the counselors on a seven-point Likert- type instrument of sixty-four items selected from the B-L Inventory. The responses to the items were then intercor- related using principle-component analysis with unity in the diagonals. Fifteen items were retained and rotated in the Equimax method. It was found that the unconditionality dimension of the B-L Inventory and the regard dimension define separate factors; that empathy and congruence are explained by a single factor, and that this is consistent with correla- tion data obtained by Bennet-Lennard. Low and non-significant correlation between unconditionality and level of regard supported the theoretical and operational separation of these two variables. High correlation between empathic understand- ing and congruence was found. The findings tended to support Dymond's (see above) findings of positive relationship between self insight and ability to understand others. Perhaps the most notable of the empathy scales was de- vised by Truax in 1961 to be used in the training of personnel 22 counselors and is known as the Truax Accurate Empathy Scale. Its initial usage was to assist students in counseling to assess their own empathic capabilities in order that they might be able to enter into a more productive relationship with their clients. Truax comments on the importance of empathy in the counselor-client setting: Accurate empathy involves more than just the ability of the therapist to sense the client's or patientfs "private world" as if it were his own. It also in- volves his ability to know what the patient means. Accurate empathy involves both the therapist's sen sitivity to current feelings and his verbal facIIIty to communicate this understanding in a language attun- ed to the client's feelings (p. 15). A scale for the measurement of empathy has recently been developed by Kagan e£_al. (1971). Their instrument is called the Affective Sensitivity Scale. It provides that the subject watch videotaped sessions between counselor and client. At the conclusion of each exchange of comment be- tween the two, the subject selects from among three choices a statement which most accurately summarizes the client's feelings with regard to himself. He then selects from among three choices a statement which most accurately summarizes the client's feelings with regard to the counselor. It is this instrument which was used in this study. Details of its application will be found in Chapter III Empathy Training Those involved in the design of various instructional systems and devices usually hold opinions as to the value of 23 feedback during or after the instructional sequence is concluded. This topic and its relationship to empathy train- ing was investigated by Reddy (1969). The students involved were engaged in empathy training as a part of a personnel counseling program. The specific purpose of the study was to determine what effects were observable and differentiated when feedback was either immediate or delayed in teaching empathic capability. As a pre-test, the Truax Accurate Empathy Scale was administered. The subjects were divided into two groups. Each group viewed and verbally responded to a simulated psychotherapy training film. One group was given immediate feedback from a trained personnel counselor as to the quality of the response to each vignette. The other group saw the same training film, but no feedback was given until the entire array of vignettes had been viewed. Both groups were again given the Truax Accurate Empathy Scale as a post-test. It was reported that the group which received the immediate feedback showed significant gains over the group receiving delayed feedback. Two different types of instructional settings for em- pathy training were reported by Anthony and Wain (1971). Applicants for training as Medical Corpsmen in the United States Army were randomly divided into two groups. The first group received six hours of classroom lecture and training films dealing with empathic understanding. The second group received ten hours of role-playing in which they portrayed 24 specific roles dealing with emotional problems. Each group received pre- and post-tests on a standardized empathy scale. The group involved in the role-playing exercises scored sig- nificantly higher on the post-test than did the classroom group. The researchers concluded from the study that empathy has a cognitive component, and that it is best taught in a setting that actively involves trainees in the process rather than treating them as passive listeners or mere observers. The effects of various kinds of supervision during empathy training was the subject of a study by Blane (1968). The study was made to determine the effect of positive, neg- ative, or no supervision experiences on measuring empathic understanding on counselor trainees. The subjects received the Carkhuff Empathy Understanding Scale II prior to testing. They were divided into three groups, and each subject inter- viewed two "clients" (stooges). Each interview was video- taped and played back in the presence of a trained psycholo- gisto During the playback, one group got positive super- vision of its interviewing technique, one group got negative supervision, and the third group got no supervision. The pre-teSts showed no significant difference between groups; however, the post-test showed that all groups registered gains from pre-test to post-test. The greatest gains were exhibited by the group receiving the positive supervision, and the least gains were exhibited by the group which had received no supervision. 25 Another situational training setting is reported by Kagan and Schauble (1969) called Affect Simulation. In this treatment, the subject views brief filmed vignettes in which actors display specific affects (emotions). At the conclu- sion of each film vignette, the subject delivers a verbal response to the comment presented by the actor on the film. Usually six to eight vignettes are presented. The procedings are videotaped by means of a special effects generator so that both the film and the subject's face are presented on a split-screen playback. After all vignettes have been seen and responded to, the tape is replayed to the subject and a recall worker. It is the reSponsibility of the recall worker to assist the subject in re-creating his own thoughts and emotions as he sees them on the Split screen. The subject is encouraged to analyze his reactions as to his own feelings when confronted with the simulated affect. A similar process was utilized in this study, and more detail as to its employ- ment is reported in Chapter III. Related Studies in Empathy It has been noted that empathy in dyads enhances the overall communication capabilities, and Shapiro 32 El. (1969) investigated this phenomenon. The study was undertaken to determine whether psychotherapists who were high in genuiness, empathy, and warmth elicited greater self-exploration from 26 clients. The tests were conducted outside the therapeutic setting. The subjects were three groups of socially and professionally unrelated individuals. Each group was randomly divided into two sub-groups and clinical therapists inter- viewed each group. With one sub—group the therapist exhibited a warm genuine interest during the interview, while a de- tached, mechanistic interest was exhibited with the other sub- group. Each session was videotaped and rated by other therapists during playback for the amount of self-disclosure displayed in each sub-group. It was found that the subjects disclosed themselves more deeply to those to whom they per- ceived as offering high therapeutic conditions, and that they disclosed themselves differentially in a manner related to the amount of empathy, warmth, and genuineness they perceived in the interviewer. It is possible to conclude that the amount and degree of open communication which exists can be controlled by the amount of empathy each member of a dyad perceives in the other. Another facet of this area of interpersonal perception was reported in studies by Crow and Hammond (1957). Actually, two studies were undertaken. The first was to investigate the generality of interpersonal perceptiveness when the influ- ence of response sets was controlled. The second study in- vestigated the generality of accuracy of interpersonal perceptions over time. These studies would seem to have a t 1'- (In If fill 27 particular bearing on the relationship between a change agent and his client with regard to the initial contact and the establishment of an on-going relationship. In the first Study, the subjects (medical students) were asked to view videotaped interviews of patients and to indicate reticence, vocabulary levels, and personality dimensions. These data were correlated with data received from tests over the same topic areas administered to the patients themselves. Correlations were not significant, indicating that generaliz- ability of interpersonal perceptiveness is not existant. In the second study, a group of patients received the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Scale three different times over a Six-month period. Medical students involved with clinical treatment of the subjects were asked to estimate the patients' ratings of themselves. The patients' accuracy scores over time were consistent as was the medical students' appraisal of the ratings. Conclusions were that response set is more consistent over time than was differential accuracy, indicating that interpersonal perceptiveness improves as the relationship increases in tenure. Interpersonal perceptiveness was also tested by Cronbach and Gleser (1953). This study investigated the ability of teachers to predict test scores of specific students as op- posed to scores of students in general. Prior to adminis- tering tests to their classes, eight elementary school teachers were asked to estimate the mean score for the class 28 and also to estimate the score which would be achieved by specific students randomly selected from the class. Corre- lations for all teachers were high in the prediction of the class mean score, but no teacher exhibited the ability to predict the score ranges of individual students. Some teachers were able to predict the score range for some individuals on certain areas of the tests, but no correla- tions were significant for the overall test scores. The conclusion was that similarity is not a general quality; specific dimensions must be stated. This study should indi- cate to the instructional developer that the making of broad general assumptions about clients and/or prospective clients based on general similarities can produce a dysfunctional communication relationship. Each interpersonal relationship must be entered into in full cognizance of individual per- sonality differences and abilities of interpersonal percep- tiveness. It is noted that various other psychological variables can have a bearing on the establishment of an empathic rela- tionship in dyads. Tosi and Carlson (1970) investigated the relationship between levels of dogmatism in clients and per— ceptions of the counselor's empathy, level of regard, con- gruency, unconditionality, and positive regard. The subjects were college students seeking vocational education counseling, and each was given a test for dogmatism using the Rokeach Dogmatism Scale as a part of a battery of pre-interview tests. ‘nu Vb. 7L 9" 29 After the interviewing and counseling sessions were completed, the subjects were given a Likert-type test asking for their ratings of the counselors which had attended them. Dogmatism scores and negative scores of counselors on the portion of the post-test related to empathy and positive regard achieved quite high correlations. The study supported Allen's previous claim that "high dogs" initially generate barriers to effective counselor relationships and affective involve- ment in facilitative interpersonal relationships. It was mentioned in Chapter I that the innovator and the innovation for which he is attempting to attain adoption are often perceived in the same light, e.g., if the innovator is well received and regarded, his innovations are likely to be well received, also. Moore and Mizuba (1969), reported a classic account of the failure of the introduction of public and personal health practices in the rural closed societies of Peru. They note that the suggestion made to the rural Peruvians to boil water before drinking it or cooking with it did not achieve adoption because the change agent who pro- posed the idea was an "outsider," and mistrusted by the locals. This same phenomenon was investigated by Berger (1969), who undertook to determine the relationship between the evalu- ation of the communicator and the degree of attitude change, and between the evaluation of the communicator and the reten- tion of an attitude message. The subjects were randomly ‘1) 30 divided into two groups, and were shown filmed messages which depicted labor unions in both favorable and unfavorable lights. The communicator in the filmed messages was . described to the subjects prior to their viewing the film as articulate, intelligent, expert in the field, etc. The other group saw the same messages but without any prior information regarding the communicator. Pre- and post-tests to obtain attitudes of the subjects toward labor unions were adminis- tered. After seeing the filmed messages, both groups exhib- ited attitude change, but the group to whom the communicator had been represented in very positive terms showed much greater attitude change. After a time lapse of six weeks, the same post-test was re-administered to both groups. There was virtually no attitude change in the first group (the one to whom the communicator had been introduced), but signifi- cant change was noted in the second group. It was concluded that the positive evaluation of the communicator by the receiver of the message influences the retention of the mes- sage. This is particularly significant in attempting to alter attitudes. Under certain circumstances, it is quite possible that the instructional developer will find himself in the role of a leader in an instructional design project. Some perceive this to be one of his main functions. The relationship be- tween leadership and empathy was investigated by Bell and Hall (1954). Subjects in the study were randomly divided 31 into eighteen groups of five members each. A leader was not appointed or otherwise indicated. Prior to the study, each subject was given the Dymond Empathy Scale and the Guilford Leadership Scale. At the first meeting of each group, it was assigned a problem to be solved. At the conclusion of the first meeting, each group was asked to meet subsequently, prepared to designate one of their number the group leader. Correlations were obtained between the empathy scores and the leadership scores. It was determined that most of the groups picked as their leader the individual who had the highest positive correlation between leadership score and empathy score in the group. The findings supported the "need theory" of leadership selection, i.e., a group, left to its own devices, will select as its leader that individual which best meets the needs of the group. It should be obvious that the instructional developer who must operate as the leader of an instructional design team will function more effectively as such if he is capable of establishing empathic relationr ships with members of the team. There is a widespread belief that empathy is a psycho- logical phenomenon related more to the affective domain of learning than to the cognitive. In fact, Katz (1963) refers to empathy as "psychological understanding" (p. 40). Efforts have been made to investigate the possible relationship be- tween empathy and other psychological characteristics, the 32 notable study being one done by Passons and Olsen (1969). Again, the study was done in the area of personnel counselor effectiveness, and investigated the ability to predict counselor effectiveness via other psychological character- istics of counselor trainees. A group of such trainees was given the Empathic Sensitivity Scale. Subsequently, the trainees were tested for characteristics deemed to be essen- tial in effective counseling in interpersonal relations, viz: openmindedness, cognitive flexibility, ability to sense feelings, willingness to communicate feelings, and positive self-concept. Typical of the psychological tests used in the various areas was the Rokeach Dogmatism Scale for openminded- ness. Each score on the five tests was correlated with the Empathic Sensitivity Scale scores. The correlations between these scores indicated a positive relationship between em- pathic sensitivity and the five variables. In summary, it Should be noted that most of the studies and experimentation designed to develop scales for the' measurement of empathy and the instructional systems for teaching it have been developed in the field of personnel counseling. The literature does not indicate that much has been done to heighten empathic capabilities of individuals professionally involved in fields other than medicine and the social sciences. This researcher did not discover any liter- ature which identified instructional systems designed and 33 operationalized for the purpose of increasing the affective sensitivity of instructional developers, or others in the field of professional education. Those who function as instructional developers and change agents within educational systems might indicate great need for this type of educa- tional experience. With regard to interpersonal communica- tion Skills and education, Harris (1973) notes: Thomas Aquinas, who knew more about education and per- suasion than almost anybody who ever lived, once said that when you want to convert someone to your point of view, you go over to where he is standing, take him by the hand (mentally Speaking), and guide him to where you want to go. You don't stand across the room and shout at him. You don't call him a dummy. You don't order him to come over to where you are. You start where he is, and work from that position. That's the only way to get him to budge (p. 15). CHAPTER III DESIGN OF THE STUDY The purpose of this study was to design and evaluate an instructional strategy which would increase the interper- sonal communication skills of doctoral candidates in Instruc- tional Development and Technology. The study concentrated on devising methods of increasing the subjects' ability to enter into empathic relationships with others, and to accur- ately determine the affective state of others. Chapter III presents a description of the three-phase experimental treatment used in the study. Also reported are the data collection procedures, the data collection instruments, and the type of data analysis. In addition, several limitations of the present research are considered. The Sample The subjects who participated in the study were first— and second-year doctoral candidates in Instructional Develop- ment and Technology in the Michigan State University Depart- ment of Secondary Education and Curriculum. They were Fellows in two Specially-funded institutes under the auspices of the United States Office of Education. The Media Specialist 34 35 Institute consisted of seven first-year doctoral candidates and one second-year candidate. The Research Utilization Project, the second Institute, consisted of five first-year candidates, making a total of thirteen subjects. The subjects were not randomly drawn, but were selected as members of the two media specialist groups because such procedure pmesentad certain advantages to the researcher: the total number of subjects was administratively manageable in light of the limited physical facilities for administer- ing the treatments; the subjects were all assigned study space and desks in the same office building as the researcher and thus a maximum of communication between the researcher and the subjects was possible with a minimum of effort; the participation of the subjects in the study was sanctioned by the two faculty directors of the institutes and was included as a part of the subjects' academic program and for which they received partial academic credit; and it was hoped that because the subjects were holders of special fellow- ships, all subjects would complete the experimental treatment. Additionally, it was inferred by the researcher that the subjects would be typical of doctoral candidates in Instruc- tional Development and Technology. It Should be noted, however, that no claim of typicality of all such doctoral candidates is made, and no attempt at generalization beyond the parameters of the sample will be attempted. (I) "t1 () 'r.1 ‘I (I) A, U ‘1. 36 The Treatment The three-phase experimental treatment used in the study was adapted from a course designed by Kagan (1971) and has been widely used in the training of personnel counselors, physicians, and others involved in the studies of social sciences and interpersonal relations. The treatment was modified by the researcher for application in the training of ' instructional developers. Phase I In the first phase, the subjects were assembled in a conference room where, in addition to chairs, there was a portable videotape recorder and a television set. The sub- jects viewed a videotape lecture and demonstration by Dr. Norman Kagan. The videotape was Unit I of his instructional package Influencipg_Human Interaction. The purpose of the unit is to improve the ability of the subjects as interview- ers, to teach them to listen more closely, to become more deeply involved, and to respond to others in such a way as to encourage the others to go further, to explore deeper, to cooperate, and to change. The subjects are taught to dis- criminate between different kinds of Specific responses on the part of interviewers, viz: exploratory vs. non-explora- tory; affective vs. cognitive; listening vs. non-listening; and honest labeling vs. distorting. The demonstrations of 37 these kinds of responses are provided during the videotape by personnel counselors working with actors. Provisions are also made for the subjects to practice listening and respond- ing to remarks made by the actors. At the conclusion of Unit I, the subjects were shown a portion of Unit III of Influencing Human Interaction, which is a videotape of an actual counseling session with a counsel- or and a client. At the conclusion of each response made by the counselor to his client, the videotape was stopped, and the subjects were asked to identify the nature of the response according to each of the four dichotomous categories detailed above. Each subject's identifications were made in writing. There were a total of thirteen counselor responses which were identified by the subjects. At the conclusion of the interview, the tape was re- wound, and the subjects saw a repeat of the same interview. During this viewing, each response made by the counselor was discussed with the subjects by a trained personnel counselor, who provided the subjects with feedback on their initial classification of the responses. The entire process from the beginning of the lecture to the conclusion of the feedback session took approximately two hours. Phase II The second phase was devised by Kagan 25.31. (1967) and is called Videotape Recall of Affect Simulation (VRAS). 7 l () ‘1 '5: 'I . i ‘5. .- '_.. I“ it v. 5. 8‘ '1 38 It is a technique wherein clients are confronted with films which encourage them to engage in Simulated interpersonal relations. Both the client and film are videotaped while the client watches the film. The videotape is then played back to the client while his recall worker helps him examine his reactions to the film. In some cases, an actor directs affec- tion or rejection toward the viewer; in others the actor reacts as if he had been rejected or seduced (Kagan g£_gl., 1969). The films used in the VRAS were jointly developed through the efforts of the researcher and Dr. Kent L. Gus- tafson. In order that the filmed confrontations be as real- istic as possible, actors were not used. Inasmuch as instructional developers will be dealing, in the main, with faculty members and/or colleagues, these films were made in color using members of the faculty at Michigan State Univer- sity. The scripts for each filmed vignette were written by the researcher and approved and edited for affect and content by Dr. Norman Kagan. Four of the six films used in this por— tion of the treatment were filmed on a small sound stage, and two were filmed on location in the respective faculty members' offices on the campus. (Scripts for each of the films appear as Appendix A.) The films contained both male and female instructors. The affects presented ranged from warmth and Open reception to open hostility and personal threat. All were judged to be 39 representative of kinds of communication situations an in- structional deve10per might be expected to encounter in his transactions with faculty-clients. The facility for administering this phase of the exper- imental treatment was originally designed by Kagan (1967). It originally consisted of a room in which was situated a motion picture projector, viewing screen, two television cameras, a television monitor, two comfortable chairs, and a small coffee table. The television cameras were partially concealed behind plywood panels perforated in a symmetrical pattern. Sound was picked up by a partially-concealed micro- phone near the subject. The videotape recorder and split- screen generator were located in a nearby room. Due to the non-availability of the originally-developed facility at the time this treatment was to be administered, alternative arrangements and some slight modifications in equipment and layout of the facility were necessary. A small sound stage was selected and equipped with the motion picture projector and viewing screen.- The subject was seated so that the projector was behind and above him, pro- jecting the picture over his head onto the screen. Only one television camera was used. It was located beneath the pro- jection screen and slightly behind it, focused on the face of the subject. The micrOphone was suSpended out of Sight above the subject's head. When the projector was running, neither the camera nor the microphone were visible to the subject. h. We 40 It should be noted that no effort was made to conceal the television camera, the microphone, or the motion picture projector. The subjects knew in advance they were being videotaped. It was deemed advisable that the camera, micro- phone, and other gear be as unobtrusive as possible so as to minimize any possible self-consciousness on the part of the subjects while being videotaped. The videotape recorder and the monitor were in an adja- cent room along with two chairs. It was deemed advisable, also, to have this equipment out of Sight of the subject to avoid any unnecessary anxiety that its presence might have generated. It was from this adjacent room, which was to the rear of the subject, that the recall worker viewed the films along with the subject. (A diagram appears as Appendix B.) It was not possible to obtain a split screen generator for this portion of the treatment, so that much of the treat- ment as detailed above was eliminated. Instead, the sound track of the motion picture film was recorded on the same videotape which recorded the subject as he watched the film. After the subject had viewed and reSponded to the six vignettes, he went into the adjacent room where the videotape recorder and monitor were located, and went through the recall process with the recall worker. In this process, the subject and the recall worker view the tape recording together. The subject is encouraged to stop the playback whenever he re- called a particular thought, feeling, or emotion which he had 41 experienced during either the viewing of the vignette or dur- ing his response to it. If the subject permitted the record- ing to run for a long while without stopping it, the worker would ask him to stop it and attempt to recall what he had thought or felt at that particular instant. If the subject passed over any notable physical or verbal reaction to the film, the worker asked him to stop the tape and discuss it. The subject was also asked to evaluate his handling of each situation based on his thoughts and feelings. If he expressed dissatisfaction with his reactive behavior, the subject was asked to suggest possible alternative methods of handling the situation. In no case did the recall worker make any type of comment that would tend to place an evaluation on the sub- ject% reactive behavior. He did ask the subject frequently if he thought that his reactions would have tended to estab- 1ish a facilitative, workable communication relationship with a client who exhibited each particular affect. Phase III The third phase of the treatment was also developed by Kagan gt El° (1967), and is called Interpersonal Process Recall (IPR). It is a technique using stimulated recall of videotape to accelerate insight and change. The facilities and equipment used in IPR are similar to that used in the VRAS technique, i.e., a television camera, a videotape recorder, 42 and a television monitor. No motion picture projectors or screens are used, nor is a split-screen generator required. Two comfortable chairs are arranged at forty-five degree angles to each other, and the television camera is arranged so that the faces and upper torsos of the subjects are in simultaneous view. The micrOphone was suspended above and between the subjects, and the videotape recorder was placed to one side out of sight of the subjects. (A diagram of the equipment arrangement appears as Appendix C.) This phase of the treatment was administered in two stages. In the first stage, one subject was designated as the "Instructional Developer" and the other subject his "Client" (faculty member). Prior to the interview, the "Client" was told by the researcher to demonstrate, to the best of his ability, a specific affect. These included and were limited to "anxiety," "elation," "threat," "depression," "open cooperation," and "hostility." These affects were written on index cards and were arranged in random order. They were then assigned to each "Client" in serial rotation so that if any "Client" drew the same affect more than once, it would be due to pure chance. (In an interview with the researcher, Dr. Kagan stated that the "role-playing" in- volved in having one of the subjects assume and display a specific role might be beneficial in assisting the subject to become more familiar with certain dimensions of that affect.) 43 The "Instructional Developer" was not advised as to what affect he might confront in his interview with his "Client." A setting for the interview was provided in order that it might have some cognitive meaning and focus for the partici- pants. Each was told that the instructor had experienced an instructional problem, and had requested assistance from the campus "Learning Service." The "Instructional Developer" had come to the instructor's office in response to that re- quest, and that this interview would be the first encounter between the two. With these instructions, the tape recorder was started, and the participants began the interview. During the course of the interview, the recall worker monitored the progress and content of the interview from an adjacent room, as did the researcher. When the interview had gone on for approximately six to eight minutes, the partici- pants were told to stop. The subject who had played the role of the "Client" was excused, and the "Instructional Developer" and the recall worker sat down and viewed a playback of the interview. Again, the same recall process was employed by the recall worker that was described in the previous section dealing with the VRAS technique. At the conclusion of the recall session, the subject who had previously played the role of the "Client" was called back into the room, and the entire process was repeated with the same two subjects in reversed roles. A different affect was assigned to the new "Client." Again the IPR process was 44 repeated with the pairs in reversed roles, and the new "Instructional Developer" was conducted through a recall ses- sion at the conclusion of the interviewer. The second stage of this phase of the treatment was essentially the same process as the first, but with two differences. Care was taken so that the same pairs were not involved in the second round of interviewing. Also, during the recall process, the subject in the role of the "Client" was not excused as before, but stayed in the room with the "Instructional Developer" and the recall worker and partici- pated in simultaneous recall. Again, during these recall sessions, the videotape playback was stopped and started by either the recall worker or the subject(s). During these sessions, the recall worker attempted to get the subjects to interrogate themselves and gain insight into their own be- havior. Gustafson (1969) notes: He encourages the subject to deal with basic or recur- ring fears and aspirations rather than linger over one or two responses or gestures which he may initially have chosen to discuss. The recall worker constantly seeks to cultivate the subject's own awareness of his behavior, and through this awareness to understand the motivation and belief system underlying his behavior (p. 55). The Recall Workers The recall workers used the VRAS,-and IPR treatments in- cluded the researcher and four Fellows from the Office of Medical Education Research and Development at Michigan State University. The five workers had been the recipents of 45 Special training in recall counseling during the fall term of 1972. The Fellows were prepared to work in the course in Doctor-Patient Relationships in the College of Human Medicine at Michigan State University, and the researcher was prepared for this study. The training was conducted by Drs. Norman Kagan, John Schneider, and Arnold Werner. There was a "refresher session" for the workers conducted in the winter quarter by Dr. Kagan. The training sessions for the workers included a review of the rationale, functions, and technique of the recall process. Next the workers viewed a series of videotapes of recall sessions conducted by trained recall workers, during which they were asked to identify spots in the interview in which the subject might be encouraged to stop. They were asked to indicate why that Spot was chosen and to frame ques- tions which might be asked of a subject at that time. The recall trainee was taught to identify both verbal and non- verbal cues which are indicative of various affects, viz: changes in voice tone, posture, and facial expression. The workers then engaged in interviews with each other and sub- sequent recall sessions under the supervision of Skilled re- call workers. 46 ' Instrumentation Affective Sensitivity Affective sensitivity was measured twice during the experiment, in a pre-test and a post-test. The pre-test was administered to the assembled subjects prior to their receiv- ing Phase I, and the post-test was administered to the assembled subjects subsequent to the completion of the second stage of Phase III. Both pre-test and post-test employed the Affective Sensitivity Scale (see Appendix D for samples of test items). The Affective Sensitivity Scale (ASS) is reported by Kagan st 31. (1971). It is a situational test involving clients and personnel counselors; both male and female clients and counselors are involved. The subjects view videotaped scenes taken from actual counseling sessions, representative of typical sessions; they vary in emotional depth and content of client concern. Each showing of a videotaped sequence is followed by the subject's answering several multiple-choice items to describe the affective states which the client may be experiencing. A subject must choose from among three sentences the one which most clearly defines what he, the sub- ject, thinks the client feels about the content of client communication. He also chooses from among three other sen- tences which describe the client's feelings about the counselor. 47 A two-way assessment of the reliability of the scale has been made. Internal consistency reliability coefficients range between .58 and .77, with the majority of the co- efficients in the .705. A test-retest coefficient of corre- lation was .75 over a two-week period (Campbell, 1967; Kagan gt_gl., 1967). Several studies have assessed the validity of the scale, notably a validity study using two one-year-long National Defense Education Act Institute groups. Findings indicate that these groups did increase their affective sensitivity, and that the increases were not caused by the practice effect of the pre-testing and post-testing by a study using a place- bo group (Kagan 25 31., 1967). Form B of the ASS, which was used in this study, con- tains sixty-six items, and required approximately two-hours for its administration. The subjects were assembled in a small conference room containing, in addition to a table and chairs, a videotape recorder and monitor. The purpose of the test as well as its mechanics were explained to the sub- jects, and they were lead through the provided sample item. The videotape containing the ASS was started and stopped after each verbal transaction between client and counselor, and the subjects were given ample time to record their selection from the multiple items. At the conclusion of the pre-test, the answer Sheets were collected and scored by three independent blind scorers. No discrepancies in scoring were noted, and 48 each subject's test score was recorded by the researcher. The scorers were the researcher and two doctoral students not involved in the study. Co-orientational Accuragy Co-orientational accuracy (empathy) was measured twice during the experiment, in a pre—test and a post-test. The pre-test was administered to the subjects prior to their receiving Phase I, and the post-test was administered subse- quen to Phase III. A situational test was devised by the researcher after a procedure suggested by Wackman (1969). His procedure is based on a general model of co-orientation originally de- vised by Newcomb (1953), and revised by Chaffee 23 a1. (1969). See Figure l. X = ApBrO Z ArO Y = BrO Figure l. A General Model of Co-orientation. A and B are two persons, both orienting to an object O and to each other. The object is a general concept, referring 49 to any object of cognition. The ratings of the object, ArO and BrO, may involve any kind of cognition-~attitudina1 or attributive kinds of items. The three co-orientation concepts in the model involve comparisons among the sets of ratings, taken two at a time: 1) accuracy is the comparison between X and Y, A'S pre- diction of B's rating of object O, and B's rating of O. 2) agreement is the comparison between Z and Y, A's rating of O and B's rating of O. 3) congruency is the comparison between X and Z, A's prediction of B's rating of O and A's own rating of O (Wackman, 1969, p. 2). Several things should be noted about the general co- orientation model. First, the object O is a general concept, which may be gay object of cognition, such as a political object, a book, a person (as in person perception research), etc° Second, the rating of the object, ArO and BrO, may in- volve any kind of cognition. Thus, the items may be attitu- dinal kinds of items, measuring A's attitude toward the object, or they may be attributive kinds of items, measuring A's beliefs concerning the object (Wackman, 1969). Two Likert-type instruments of twenty-five items each were generated for measures of attitude. The Likert-type, or summated rating scale, was chosen because of its adaptability to behavioral research and its ease of development and because p 0 lite 50 it "... yields about the same results as the more laboriously constructed, equal-appearing interval scale" (Kerlinger, 1964, p. 487). Each item in each instrument was a five-point anchored statement ranging from "strongly disagree" to "strongly agree." The tOpics for the two instruments were "accountability in higher education," and "use of behavioral objectives in teaching." (Samples of the instruments appear as Appendices E and F respectively.) The two topic areas were selected because they represent areas of interest to most professional educators, and because they would likely repre- sent areas which would generate Specific notions during the interviews. Thirteen Michigan State University faculty members were selected to serve as interviewees, one for each subject. Care was taken to select instructors in whose classes the sub- jects were not enrolled as students. Specific appointments were made by the researcher for each of the subjects with each faculty member, with the understanding that the subject would appear at the faculty member's office at the appointed hour. Prior to the subject's appointment with the faculty member-interviewee, the interviewee was sent a c0py of one of the attitudinal instruments, and was told that the tOpic covered in the instrument would be the area of the subject's interview with him. The interviewee was told to restrict 51 the interview to twenty minutes, and to answer the subject's questions in the tOpic area as fully, completely, and honest- ly as possible. Additionally, the interviewee was sent two extra c0pies of the same attitudinal instrument along with a set of instructions to be given to the subject at the con- clusion of the interview. The subject was advised as to the name of his interviewee, time and place of the appointment, and the designated topic area of the interview. He was fur- ther instructed that he could ask any question he so desired in the tOpic area, and that the interviewee would attempt to supply as comprehensive answers as possible. At the conclusion of the interview, the interviewee passed on to the subject the two extra questionnaires and the instructions as to their completion. Each subject was told to complete one instrument reflecting his own views and atti- tudes in the tOpic area. He was told to complete the second instrument reflecting his predictions as to how the inter- viewee would complete the same instrument. He was then told, as was the interviewee, to return the instruments to the researcher. Prior to the utilization of either of the instruments, each item of each instrument was rated for polarity by three blind raters. Raters included the researcher, and two other doctoral candidates not involved in the study. There was con- census on all items on both instruments with one exception on ‘Oo‘l \. I \‘gg' .UbOl ‘: a mu 3‘ (D Q.“ 52 each. In these instances, two of the three raters agreed on polarity and one disagreed. It was decided to accept the polarity rating of the two raters who agreed. In constructing the instruments, all items for each instrument were written on index cards and shuffled. Each item was drawn from the pile of cards in order to establish a random order of polarity and to off-set the possibility of response set being a factor in the completion of the instru- ments. In the post-test, the identical procedure was followed as in the pre-test. In the administration of the post-test, care was taken to insure that each subject did not interview the same instructor that he had seen on the pre-test, and that he did not diScuss the same tOpic area he used in the pre-test. All instruments were again scored by three blind scorers, and no discrepancies were noted in the scoring. The scorers were the researcher and two doctoral candidates not involved in the study. The Experimental Design The experimental design for the present research incorpo- rates one experimental group (N = 13). A pre-test-post-test design was employed to collect data on affective sensitivity and co-orientational accuracy. The subjects received an e €139? a G». -. . u l a: on o n‘u «an a q4u .. . NJ .. a . Y; A v A: «1 .6 set . . .— . C. 1 ~ «4 QKV . .< ‘3 53 experimental treatment in three phases spread over a period of approximately eighteen weeks. The first phase was admin- istered to the subjects as a group. The second phase was administered individually to each subject. The third phase was administered to the subjects singly and in pairs. Statistical Hypotheses To evaluate the effectiveness of the three experimental treatments and the underlying theoretical positions relating to affective sensitivity and co-orientational accuracy as teachable skills, the following statistical hypotheses were generated and tested. Each null hypothesis tested is pre- Sented first, followed by an accompanying alternate hypothe- sis. Null Hypothesisl: No difference will be found between the group mean scores between the pre-test and the post-test as measured by the Affective Sensitivity Scale. Symbolically: Ti =‘T2 Alternate Hypothesisl: Subjects, after receiving the experimental treatment, will have a higher group mean score on the post-test for affective sensitivity than they did on the pre- test. Symbolically: Ti f T2 ‘ MU sq! ‘ i e .14 resea 54 Null Hypothesisz: No difference will be found between the group mean scores between the pre-test and the post-test as measured in terms of co-orientational accuracy. Symbolically: T1 = T2 Alternate Hypothesisz: Subjects, after receiving the experimental treatment, will have a higher group mean score on the post-test for co-orientational accuracy than they did on the pre- test. Symbolically: ‘fl ye T2 Analysis of the Data For each of the statistical hypotheses presented above, the test of Significance is based on analysis of variance. Since the experimental group is a single group involved in a pre-test-post—test design, the test for Significance will be a matched-pair t test. Kerlinger (1964) states: The benefits to be derived from matching are sometimes considerable. It is imperative to use a matching de- sign of some sort when objects in the environment are naturally matched. To have matched objects in a re- search experiment and not to take advantage of the variance due to the matching is a statistical and de- sign blunder (p. 308). It should be noted that pairing should be made on some 3 priori basis, and, since the pre-test and the post-test are made om.the same subjects, this condition is deemed by the researcher to have been met. the It ~\~ «Am I.“ 0 o} t u iRVQl )5 V “I :r 55 Further comment on the desirability of using the match- ed-pairs design is offered by Senter (1969): The paired-observation technique is easier computation— ally, and it reduces the effects of some sources of random sampling variation that can obscure "real" dif- ferences between sets of data (lead to Beta error). The net result of the paired-observation technique is that it will produce lar er (more Significant) t ratios with a given mean dif erence if there is any positive relationship between the pairs of scores (p. 202). The computational procedure for matched-pairs will also be found in Senter (p. l97ff). The use of the matched-pairs computational technique in the test for co-orientational accuracy requires comment. It was previously noted in this chapter that Wackman (1969) has devised a new measure of co-orientational accuracy which was employed in this study. Wackman's method is referred to as the Profile Similarity (PS) method. Generally, any relational concept measured by comparing two sets of responses is typically measured by the difference, or D2, score method (Kerlinger, 1964). Typically, A's accuracy in predicting B's ratings on an object is scored by taking the difference between A's prediction of B's rating and B's actual rating on each item of a set of items referring to object O. The total D score of accuracy would be the sum of the absolute differences on each item. A major problem with the D score is that it is very complex mathematically, involving a number of components. The varied components and 56 the mathematical complexities are well illustrated by Cronbach (1953, 1955), in his analysis of the D and D2 scores. The PS method for measuring these relational concepts, in this case accuracy, would be scored by correlating A's prediction of B's responses and B's actual responses over the set of items referring to O. The two methods tend to emphasize two different aspects of the similarity of two sets of responses. The D score method tends to emphasize Similarity between absolute levels of responses, while the PS method tends to emphasize relative similarity of responses over a set of items. A high accur- acy score here would indicate that A is accurately predicting B's relative pattern of responses (Wackman, 1969). Wackman further notes that the D score method for de- termining accuracy does not allow for the contamination in- jected into the measure by projection and anti-projection, i.e., A rates object O as he thinks B rates object 0 because of A's perceived Similarity between himself and B--he pro- jects his perceived personality similarities into the ratings. In the case of anti-projection, A perceives himself differ- ent from B, so rates object O oppositely from what he per- ceives B's rating of O to be. Wackman's method, using a partial correlation technique, partials out chance accuracy and yields what he has called "real" accuracy. It is the 57 partial correlation technique across all items for all sub— jects that was used in this study. The partial correlations for all instruments and items were converted to Fisher's Z (McNemar, 1969), and pre-test and post-test comparison for Significance was made using the matched-pairs t test as previously indicated. An alpha level of .05 was selected for rejecting the null hypothesis. But since the present research is explor- ing the application of established instructional techniques to novel Situations, we shall report and discuss probability levels between .05 and .10 as indicating potential levels of future research. Chapter IV reports the results of all computations with their accompanying Significance tests. Limitations of the Study Possibly the most serious limitation of the study is found in the experimental design. Campbell and Stanley (1963) indicate that the one-group pre-test post-test design can be confounded by several extraneous variables; among them are: history--intervening events between pre-test and post-test; maturation--intervening biological or psychological processes affecting the subjects; testing--the effect of the pre-test itself; instrument decay-~autonomous changes in the measuring instrument; and statistical regression-~the phenomenon which 58 finds subjects scoring toward the extremes in a pre-test regressing toward the mean on a post-test. The authors state that these factors are possible sources of comtamination in this particular study design. In the present study, it is questionable whether the pre-test or instrument decay, for example, affected the gain Scores. In neither of the pre- test situations were the subjects given any feedback re- garding their scores on either instrument. In the Situational interview pre-test, care was taken that no subject saw the same interviewee in the post-test that he saw in the pre- test, nor did he use the same topic area as a subject of the interview. As to instrument decay, changes in the instrument, it is not believed that such was probable, though it was possible. Statistical regression is a non-controllable, and this certainly could have been a source of contamination. A second limitation is found in the size of the sample (N 13). The small sample Size means any difference between the pre-test and post-test scores has to be larger than is needed for a larger sample in order to obtain significant differences between them. The small N was necessary for two reasons: (1) the facilities for administering the VRAS and IPR stages of the experimental treatments were limited both in size and availability to the researcher; it was not pos— sible to set up the equipment needed for the treatment and leave it in position for use over periods of several days at 59 a time; and (2) the thirteen subjects represented nearly all of the full-time, on-campus doctoral candidates in Instruc- tional Development and Technology at the time the research was undertaken. A third limitation is the fact that the subjects did not represent a "sample" of a population; they were, in fact, a population. Inferences which can be drawn from the find- ings of the study cannot be generalized beyond the subjects themselves. As a pragmatic consideration, it is a fact that the subjects are reasonably representative of students who have enrolled for graduate work in the Instructional DevelOp- ment and Technology emphasis area. One might make the purely pragmatic (but not statistical) inference that the findings generated by this research are applicable to those students who follow these subjects through the academic program. A fourth limitation of the present study was the in- ability to control for the Hawthorne effect. There is no question that the subjects knew that they were participating in experimental research--a process to which the few remain- ing full-time and part-time doctoral candidates were not sub- jected. Considering the effect that research participation has been known to have on subjects, it is probable that the present research subjects responded positively to the treat- ment over and above any improvement due to the treatment. 60 Summary The experimental treatment was adapted from processes previously shown to be effective in training personnel counselors, medical students and others. The three phases of the treatment consisted of (l) a lecture-demonstration by videotape followed by a training exercise with feedback; (2) the use of Videotape Recall of Affect Simulation in which the subjects viewed and responded to six filmed vignettes, and were subsequently aided in their recall of their emotion- al and cognitive reactions by a trained recall worker; and (3) the use of Interpersonal Process Recall in which each subject alternately played the role of instructional develop- er and client in an interview setting followed by aided recall with videotape and a trained recall worker who assisted the subject in an analysis of his emotional and cog- nitive reactions to various elements of the interview. The subjects were first- and second-year doctoral candi- dates in Instructional DevelOpment and Technology at Michigan State University who were Fellows in two media Specialist institutes funded by the United States Office of Education. Two instruments were employed to collect data for the research. Each instrument was administered as pre-test and post-test on the same group of subjects. The first instru- ment was the Affective Sensitivity Scale, the reliability and validity of which have been established. The second 61 instrument was a twenty-five item Likert-type attitudinal instrument designed to measure the degree of co-orientational accuracy between the subjects who were paired with faculty members in an interview setting in which the subject and interviewee discussed previously-assigned topics. The recall workers used in the present research had been previously trained for Similar activities in instruc- tional procedures in other disciplines. Data were analyzed in a matched-pairs procedure between pre-test and post—test, with a matched-pairs t test determin- ing significance. Two statistical hypotheses were generated and tested using the above procedure. There are several limitations to the study. The research design itself using one group in a pre-test and post-test setting incurred possible contamination from such extraneous variables as history, maturation, instrument decay, and statistical regression. The fact that the subjects represented a population rather than a sample, limits general- ization beyond the parameters of the population. The Hawthorne effect probably was a source of limitation, as was the small size of the sample necessitated by limited physical facilities for administering the treatments and the small total number of available subjects. CHAPTER IV FINDINGS Findings A compilation of the findings of the study are reported in this chapter. The effect of the three experimental treat- ments on affective sensitivity and co-orientational accuracy are individually discussed. Although the research findings will be discussed in detail, conclusions based on the find- ings will be reported in Chapter V. It should be noted that there was no mortality among the subjects which permits the matched-pairs observations described in Chapter III. The first hypothesis tested in the present study was: H 1: No difference will be found between the group mean scores between the pre-test and the post- test as measured by the Affective Sensitivity Scale. The first step in the analysis was to compute a corre- lation coefficient between the pre-test and post-test. The correlation coefficient was .87, which is somewhat higher than those obtained during the developmental processes of the Affective Sensitivity Scale. No analysis of variance was conducted on the pre-test data since the matched-pairs technique offers a substantial 62 63 degree of control over chance variability (sampling varia- tion). In this regard, Senter (1969) states: It should be relatively clear that if observations are made on the same individual twice (or on individuals closely related) the amount 6f chance variability expected to occur among scores Is considerably less than it would be if these scores were to be randOmly selected from totally unrelated individuals (p. 196). The mean score on the pre-test for all subjects was 32.5 and the mean score on the post-test was 38.2. Standard deviations were 7.12 on the pre-test, and 8.76 on the post— test. These data are reported for informational purposes only Since they are not involved in the matched-pairs compar- ison. The score distribution on which the significance test is based is the mean difference of all scores, and this was computed at 5.62 from preétest to post-test. The critical value for a mean difference of 5.62 was computed at 4.496. The critical value of t (two-tailed) at .05 Alpha is 2.179. These data are reported in Table 1. Table 1. Comparison of Pre-test and Post-test Means and Mean Difference of the Affective Sensitivity Scale Pre-test 2" PoStetest M gain M d df‘ t M. SD M 4SD 32.5 7.12 38.2 8.76 5.7 5.62 12 4.496 t = 2.179 at Alpha .05 64 This analysis indicates that the mean difference of 5.62 observed between the pre-test and the post-test is too large to be reasonably attributed to sampling variation. Assuming that all factors other than the treatment effect were either eliminated from the experiment or equated by the matching technique, it can be inferred that the increase from pre-test to post-test was significantly influenced by the intervening treatment. The null hypothesis can be re- jected, and support can be inferred for the alternate hypothe- sis of a higher mean score on the post-test than on the pre- test for affective sensitivity. Since rejection of the null hypothesis does not indicate the direction of the difference between pre-test and post-test scores, the mean of each test must be examined for direction. The post-test mean of 38.2 is greater than the pre-test mean of 32.5 supporting the directional hypothesis presented in Chapter III that the experimental treatment does increase the subject's affective sensitivity. The second hypothesis tested was: H 2: No difference will be found between the group 0 mean scores between the pre-test and the post— test as measured in terms of co-orientational accuracy. Data on co-orientational accuracy was collected by pro- viding Specific topics of discussion for interviews between the subjects and faculty members at Michigan State University. 65 At the conclusion of each interview, both pre-test and post- test, the faculty member (interviewee) completed a twenty- five item Likert-type instrument registering his views and opinions of the assigned discussion topic. Each of the sub- jects was given two:copies of the identical instrument by the interviewee at the conclusion of the interview. The subject was asked to complete one of the instruments so that it reflected his own views and opinions of the topic. He was asked to complete the second instrument so that it re- flected the interviewee's views and opinions. The attitudinal instruments were constructed on a five-point anchored scale ranging from "strongly disagree" to "strongly agree." The reader will recall from the co-orientation model presented in Chapter III, that person A's prediction about person B's ratings of object()(ApBrO) is represented by X, that B's ratings of object O (BrO) are represented by Y, and that A's ratings of object O (ArO) are represented by Z. After scoring each instrument, simple correlations were obtained between each item on each instrument scored by each subject and his interviewee; thus, the following correlations were computed: rxy' rxz' and ryz' The first correlation, therefore, was between each item of the subject's perceptions of the interviewee's ratings and the interviewee's ratings, ApBrO. The second correlation was between the subject's perceptions of the interviewee's ratings and his own ratings, 66 ApBrO and ArO. The third correlation was between the sub- ject's ratings and the interviewee's rating ArO and BrO. The three correlations were summed and a mean obtained for each. Again, these data were derived for informational purposes of the reader who may be interested in computing data by the D method. The mean for each correlation was used in computing a partial correlation, partialing for z, in both pre-test and post-test. These data are reported in Table 2. Table 2. Comparison of Pre-test and Post-test correlation Means, Including Comparison of Partial Correlation Pre-test Post—test Gain r .202 .316 .114 XY r .449 0460 .011 xz r .306 .192 -.114 yz r .076 .262 .186 xy-z For the purposes of testing for Significance, the simple cxxrrelations across the three instruments were used in com- suiting partial correlations for z for each subject on both pre-test andpost-otest. Each partial correlation was trans- formed to Fisher's Z and summed. Means for both pre-test and 67 post-test were computed and compared using the matched-pair technique previously described. Table 3 reports these data. Table 3. Comparison of Pre-test and Post-test Mean and Mean Difference of Measure of Co-orientation Mean of 23 Gain M d df t Pre-test Post-test .09104 .30129 .21115 .1838 12 1.356 t = 2.179 at Alpha .05 (two-tailed). The resulting value of 1.356 is less than the critical value of 2.179 indicating that the experimental treatment was not a source of significant difference between the pre- test and the post-test; in fact, it indicates that—no sig- nificant difference was observed. The null hypothesis was not rejected. Thus, there is no support for the hypothesis that there would be an increase in mean scores between pre- test and post-test on the measure of co-orientational accuracy. Discussion of the Findings A comparison of the pre-test and post-test scores to produce a mean difference failed to reject the null hypothesis of no difference between pre-test and post-test scores on the measure of co-orientational accuracy. 68 There are several probable explanations for the failure to produce significant difference. Several of the subjects reported of their own volition to the researcher that the twenty minutes allotted for the interviews was not long enough for them to obtain the information they felt they needed in order to assess the attitudes and beliefs of the interviewee with regard to the topic areas. After both the pre-test and post-test experiences, some subjects reported that most of the twenty minutes was spent in discussing extraneous matters. Some reported that the interviewees expended most of the time interrogating the subjects as to the nature of the research project and seeking information as to how the attitudinal instruments were to figure in the testing procedure. Other subjects reported that the inter“ viewees expended the time asking them questions relating to their personal backgrounds and doctoral programs. The sub- jects who were involved in these kinds of situations reported that it was extremely difficult to keep the interview oriented toward the topic area, and consequently they had little or no time to interogate the interviewee regarding more germane matters. The selection of tOpicS of discussion seems to have posed more of a problem than was anticipated. The researcher attempted to select tOpics about which professional educa- tors would have a reasonably broad base of information and 69 interest. One topic, "accountability in higher education," seemed to pose no problem, but the tOpic "use of behavioral objectives in teaching" caused difficulty. Well over half of the subjects indicated that they had to spend at least half of the allotted interview time explaining what behavioral objectives were to the interviewee in spite of the fact that a definition was provided at the tOp of the instrument in an attempt to forestall just such an occurrence. After the pre- test, three of the subjects reported back to the researcher that their interviewees had extremely negative, nearly hostile, attitudes about behavioral objectives. Five of the subjects reported the same information after the post-test. Observations of the attitudinal instruments completed by these interviewees did not appear, however, to reflect what might be termed an extremely negative attitude toward this topic. There can be little doubt that the instruments re- ported negative attitudes in all cases, but the respondents did not utilize nearly as many "strongly disagree" and "strongly agree" responses as the subjects involved seemed to indicate on the ApBrO instrument. One of two things obviously happened: either the interviewees' ratings on the instruments did not.ref1ect their true feelings, or the subjects read more into the interviewees' diatribes than existed. It is also interesting to note that most of the subjects reflected ‘very positive attitudes toward the use of behavioral objec- tives. 70 Another cause of the failure to achieve significance might be attributed to personality variables among the sub- jects. While the subjects never received any standardized personality inventories in connection with the research, the researcher was able to personally observe the subjects at close range over the academic year. As a result, it was possible to make some kinds of intuitive personality assessments with regard to introversion-extroversion. Those subjects who exhibited either neutral or extroverted orien- tations toward their environment appeared to exhibit the greatest individual gain scores on the post-test measures of co-orientational accuracy. Those who were introverted con- fessed higher anxiety levels in the simulations and exhibited lesser gains in the post-teSt. It is notable that all the partial correlations for individual subjects did not move in the same direction. Of the thirteen subjects, ten showed an increase from pre-test to post-test when partialing for 2; three showed decreases. AS it is not possible to accurately account for the increases, neither can the decreases be accounted for. It will be re- membered that during the post-testing, more subjects reported the behavioral objectives topic to be more of a controversial issue than a topic of inquiry and discussion than they did on the pre-test. It is possible that this situation accounted for the post-test decreases on the partial correlations. This conjecture may tend to be supported by the fact that 71 the ryz correlation mean decreased from .306 to .192 pre- test to post-test. It will be recalled that the ryz corre- lation is a comparison of the interviewees' ratings with the subjects' ratings. One might also conjecture that these data indicate that there was less Similarity between the views of the subjects and their interviewees as they were paired in the post-test. This factor would appear to vitiate the intended positive effect of the experimental treatment. The notion of personality variables previously noted could have also been an unforeseen source of vitiation of the treatment. If those subjects who exhibited tendencies toward introversion were paired with interviewees of decided extroversion characteristics and found the interview situa- tion to be an uncomfortable one, then the decrease in the ryz correlation may be accounted for. It is interesting to note that the critical value of t for the rejection of the null hypothesis at the .05 level of confidence was 2.179. As noted the critical value of the mean difference 1.356 would not permit the rejection of the null hypothesis at that level of confidence. Because the experimental treatment was an adaptation of an existing and previously validated treatment, an examination of a possible rejection of the null hypothesis at the .10 Alpha was made. The critical value of t at this level is 1.782, meaning that the null hypothesis could not have been rejected at .10 Alpha either. CHAPTER V SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS Summary The present research studied the effect of a three- phase experimental treatment on the abilities of doctoral candidates in Instructional DevelOpment and Technology to enter into empathic relationships with others. This capa- bility was measured in two dimensions: (1) the ability of the subjects to accurately assess the affective state of others, and (2) the ability of the subjects to accurately predict the opinions and belief systems of others with re- gard to specific tOpics. The treatment's first phase consisted of a filmed lecture-demonstration which dealt with the ability to discrim- inate between various kinds of responses made by an inter- viewer in a dyadic setting. The subjects were shown various responses by means of videotape recording, and classified them as either exploratory or non-exploratory, affective or cognitive, listening or non-listening, and labeling or dis- torting. The second phase of the treatment required the subjects to view six stimulus films in which faculty or staff members 72 73 at Michigan State University, serving as actors, portrayed specific affective states. The subjects were videotaped as they watched the stimulus films and as they verbally responded to the films. In responding, the subjects were instructed to deal with either the cognitive or affective content of the films, or both if they felt such was possible. After viewing all Six of the filmed vignettes, the subjects watched a playback of the videotape made of them. They were aided by trained recall workers in analyzing the affective and cognitive elements in their reactions and responses to the vignettes. The subjects were encouraged to critique their own performances and to suggest more appropriate altern- ative ways of responding in those cases where they felt such was needed. Subjects were given access to the start-stop switch of the videotape recorder, and were instructed to stop the playback whenever they felt the need to comment on their performance or at the recall of a particular thought or feel- ing stimulated by the playback. The recall workers also had access to the start-stOp switch, and stopped the playback ‘whenever they felt that comment or interrogation relative to the subjects' responses was required. The third phase of the treatment required the subjects to engage in.a Simulated interview with each other. One sub— ject played the role of an instructional developer, and another subject played the role of a faculty member who was 74 seeking a solution to an instructional problem. The subject in the role of the faculty member was instructed prior to the interview to do his best to display a Specific state. These included "anxiety," "elation," "threat," "depression," "open cooperation," and "hostility." The subject in the role of the instructional developer was not advised as to what affective state he would encounter in the interview. He was instructed to deal with whatever affective state was displayed by the other member of the dyad in a manner which he judged would best lead to an on-going facilitative rela— tionship; one that would more likely lead to the ready adoption of any innovative instructional methodologies which might be proposed at a later date as a solution to the instructional problem. Each interview was recorded, with most of them running approximately six to eight minutes in length. At the con- clusion of each interview, the subject in the role of the faculty member was temporarily excused and the other subject was joined by a recall worker. The videotape of the inter- view was played back, and the subject and the recall worker went through the critique process described above. As before, the subject was asked to stop the playback frequently and to comment on his handling of the interview with regard to its cognitive and affective content. At the conclusion of the recall process, the subject who had taken the role of the faculty member was called back into the room. The process of 75 simulated interview between instructional developer and faculty member was repeated with the two subjects in reversed roles. Again the faculty member was given an affective state to display during the interview. Recall was done with the second subject in the manner previously described. The simulated interviews were repeated a second time with different subjects in the dyads. The second round of interviews was identical to the first with one exception. At the conclusion of the interview, the subject in the role of the faculty member was not excused for the recall session. He stayed in the room and participated in what is referred to as "simultaneous recall." In this process, both the interviewer and the interviewee participate in the recall process with the recall worker. Both subjects are encouraged to stop the playback and comment on the behavior of either subject. Two measurement techniques of empathic capability were employed as both pre-test and post-test. The first technique is a measurement of the ability of the subject to accurately describe and assess the affective state of another, and is called the Affective Sensitivity Scale. The subjects watched a videotape of actual counseling sessions between a client and a professional personnel counselor. At specific points during the verbal exchanges between the client and the coun- selor, the videotape was stopped. The subjects were asked to select from among three statements the one that most 76 accurately described the inner feelings of the client with regard to either himself or the state of the interview. The subjects also selected one statement from three choices the one which most accurately described the client's thoughts and/or feelings regarding the counselor. The Affective Sensitivity Scale contains a total of sixty-six items. The instruments were scored for the number of correct responses by three blind scorers. There were no discrepancies noted in the scoring of either the pre-test or the post-test. The second technique for measuring empathic capability was a situational test of co-orientational accuracy. Each subject was paired with a Michigan State University faculty member for the purpose of an interview in the faculty member's office. The dyads were assigned one of two tOpics for dis— cussion during the interview: "accountability in higher edu- cation," or "the use of behavioral objectiyes in teaching." The tOpics were assigned several days in advance of the appointed time of each interview. The subjects were told at the time the topics were assigned that they could pose any questions in the topic area that they wished. They were also told that the faculty member with whom they were to talk had agreed to answer all questions as comprehensively and con— scientiously as possible. The interviews were limited to twenty minutes in length. At the conclusion of the interview, each faculty member completed a twenty-five item Likert-type instrument which 77 called for an expression of his opinions in the topic area on a five-point anchored scale ranging from "strongly dis- agree" to "strongly agree." The subject completed two c0pies of the same instrument. On one, he was instructed to reflect his own personal views in the tOpiC area; on the second copy he was instructed to attempt to predict the way his inter- viewee would respond, based on whatever information he had been able to gather during the interview. All respondents were asked to return the instruments to the researcher. In no case from pre-test to post-test, did a subject interview the same faculty member, nor did a subject have the same tOpic of discussion in both interviews. Polarity of the items in each instrument was determined by three blind raters prior to the assemblying of the instru- ments. Items on each instrument were randomly arranged in order to obviate the possibility of response set. After the administration of the pre-test and the post-test, each instrument was scored by three blind raters; no discrepancies in scoring were found. Correlations among the items of each interview instrument were computed, and partial correlations of all instruments were computed holding constant the instru- ments which reflected the subjects' ratings of the tOpics. Each of the two results of the pre-test and post-test was compared by the matched pairs technique for mean differ- ence, and two-tailed t tests for significance were conducted. 78 The pre-test was administered early in January, 1973, and the post-test toward the end of May, 1973: the time lapse from pre-test to post-test was approximately eighteen weeks. The following two hypotheses were examined using the matched pairs analysis of variance: 1. No difference will be found between the group mean scores between pre-test and post-test as measured by the Affective Sensitivity Scale. 2. No difference will be found between the group mean scores between the pre-test and the post-test as measured in terms of co-orientational accuracy. An initial purpose of this study was to test an adapta- tion of an existing instructional methodology for increasing empathic capabilities of personnel counselors and other social scientists to application in the training of profes- sional educators. The core of the experimental treatment has been used with notable success by Kagan (1972) and Rowe (1972). It was hoped that with certain changes in methodol- ogy and instructional materials, the treatment would be effective in training instructional developers. One change involved the stimulus films used in Phase II (VRAS) of this study. The stimulus films used in the original version of the treatment involve settings and content more applicable to the training of personnel counselors than instructional developers. It was decided to produce a new series of stimu- lus films in color which would have a higher degree of typicality to the types of interpersonal communication 79 situations encountered by those involved in instructional development. It was the making of the new films which may have provided what could be a source of vitiation of the experimental treatment. It has been noted that the pre- and post-test were separated by approximately eighteen weeks. When the study was initially planned, it was intended that the treatment be completed within a ten week period, or less. Unantici— pated delays were encountered in the making of the new stimulus films. The filming equipment, which was leased from an out-of-town supplier was not available when promised. After the equipment was obtained and the vignettes shot, the film processing house experienced unaccountable delays in developing the film and conforming the optical track. A second source of delay was the unexpected nonavail— ability of the original VRAS simulator. An alternate facil- ity was necessary, and it required some additional time to locate a suitable facility and arrange to borrow the neces- sary equipment from different sources. The nonavailability cf the original VRAS simulator may have caused a more serious problem than loss of time. The original facility was a small intimate room draped and carpeted in a most attractive manner. It would seem to be quite suitable for and conducive to the kind of unselfcon- scious state of mind Subjects require to view and react to the stimulus films qut beneficially. 80 The alternative arrangement used for the VRAS was a rather large sound stage furnished only with that equipment required for the Simulation. This facility did not have a Split-screen generator available; the sound track of the stimulus films was recorded instead for use as a referant in the recall process. It was thought at the time this decision was made that not having a simultaneous recording of the subjects' physical reactions to the film and the film's visuals would not be a factor as far as the VRAS process is concerned. Subsequent occurrences during the VRAS recall process proved this assumption to be erroneous. It was noted during the recall that some subjects were having difficulty in bringing to mind an accurate mental picture of the actor in the film. They also mentioned that hearing the sound track of the film without seeing the actor's face and facial expressions was a deterant to their being able to recall some of their own emotional reactions to the film. The avail- ability of a Split-screen generator would have eliminated this problem. In addition, it was noted by the researcher and other recall workers that some of the subjects seemed distracted by having to frame and deliver a response to what was obvious- ly an empty projection screen hanging some distance away in an almost empty room. Some remarked that they "felt foolish" and were self-conscious in this situation. 81 The findings permitted the rejection of the first null hypothesis beyond the .05 level of confidence, but not the second. The rejection of the null hypothesis infers support for the hypothesis that the experimental treatment increased the affective sensitivity of the subjects. It should be noted, however, that no definite decision regarding the validity of this finding can be made without additional re- search to confirm or contradict the present finding. Rejection of the null hypothesis relative to co-orienta- tional accuracy was not possible. The failure to reject the null hypothesis may have been due to the failure of the experimental treatment to increase the subjects' ability to accurately predict and assess the interviewees' beliefs and attitudes, to the instrument's incapability to accurately. measure precise degrees or co-orientational accuracy, or to the various limitations in the study design detailed in Chapter III. Conclusions Some conclusions are made from the findings summarized above. 1. It appears that the experimental treatment played a significant role in increasing the subjects' capabilities to correctly assess and identify the affective state of others. The original treatment from which the experimental treatment 82 used in this study was adapted normally exposes the subject to thirty hours of instruction. Some versions of the orig— inal treatment have exposed the subjects to fifty hours of instruction. In the present research each subject received approximately fiyg_hours of instruction. Nevertheless, as a result, group mean scores increased from 32.5 on the pre— test to 38.2 on the post-test, a gain of 5.7. Rowe (1972), in repeated administrations of the ASS using one—group de— signs with essentially this same type of treatment interven- ing between pre- and post-test, realized gain scores of slightly less than 5.7 in all instances. Her application of the experimental treatment exposed the subjects to fifty hours of instruction. These data would tend to support the original validation studies conducted during the development of the ASS (see Chapter III). It will be recalled that the mean difference between pre-test and post-test reported in Chapter IV was 5.62 with a critical value of 4.496. This was Significant beyond the .05 level of confidence which was established for the rejection of the null hypothesis. It is interesting to note'that the critical value of 4.496 would also have permitted rejection of the null hypothesis beyond the .01 level of confidence (t = 3.055 at .01 Alpha). Again, it should be emphasized that this result was attained with five hours of instruction per subject in place of the usual thirty to fifty hours of instruction. 83 It is not intended that the limitations inherent in the study design be completely ignored. It would appear, however, that the sizeable gain score from pre-test to post-test was affected more by the experimental treatment than by the design-affiliated intervening variables previously noted, and certainly more than could be attributed to pure chance or sample variation. 2. The capabilities of the subjects to enter into a . state of co-orientational accuracy with others did not in- crease as a result of the experimental treatment, or for any other reason according to a comparison of the attitudinal instruments. The failure to reject the null hypothesis rela- tive to co-orientational accuracy may have several probable explanations. First, it is possible that the attitudinal instruments did not accurately measure the subjects' capa- bilities in the area of co—orientational accuracy. Second, the utilization of a rather controversial topic for the test interviews may have inhibited the subjects' ability to gather information as to the interviewees' beliefs and value sys- tems to the extent needed to accurately respond to the test instruments. Third, the experimental treatment did not in fact have any instructional ability as far as co—orienta- tional accuracy is concerned. It will be noted that the two test instruments actually measured two different dimensions of empathic capability: the ability to identify and assess the psychological state of another; and the ability to predict 84 the responses of another in both cognitive and affective domains. It was intended that the gpg_three-phase experimen- tal treatment increase subjects' capabilities in 332 areas. Based on the findings, one can only conclude that the treat- ment was effective in improving one capability but not effective in improving the other. Fourth, the original in- structional model calls for a rather compressed time period for the treatment. The fact that the administration of the three phases was spread over eighteen weeks instead of ten weeks as initially planned might have had a negative effect on the potential cumulative effect of the treatment; what exact effect the uncontrollable delay might have had is unknown. Implications for Future Research Since the present research explores the modification and adaption of proven techniques of instruction to a novel application, the study should be replicated to provide a second independent set of findings. Any replication Should utilize a study design which incorporates experimental and control groups randomly drawn from a well-defined population. The one-group pre-test/post-test design represents the most serious flaw in the present study. This would be eliminated by having available for comparisons findings produced by a two-group design. The fact that the present study was made 'f If; (n hi 0-4 85 using what amounted to a population is a hindrance to mean- ingful generalizations beyond the parameters of the popula— tion. Any replication undertaken should utilize the Simulator originally designed by Kagan SE 31. (1967) which contains the split-screen generator and more intimate physical surround- ings. As previously noted, the recording of only the voice track of the stimulus films did not provide a satisfactory referent for the recall process in Phase II of the treatment. The more intimate surroundings and closer quarters of the original simulator would tend to eliminate much of the self— consciousness exhibited by the subjects in the present research. In planning a replication of this study, attention should be given to selecting topic areas for the interview portion of the tests for co-orientational accuracy. There is probably no way to completely eliminate controversy in the topic areas, but care should be taken in the selection of topic areas to ensure that tOpics which have obvious under- tones of controversy are not used. In the present research, it was apparent from the subjects' comments that the be— ‘havioral objectives t0pic was one of a controversial nature. They reported that some interviewees had extremely negative attitudes about behavioral objectives, and that they expended most of the interview time in what amounted to a monologue. 86 The subjects felt that they did not get sufficient time to put questions to the interviewee that would have given them the necessary background information to make a more accurate prediction of the interviewees' Opinions across the wide range of items used in the test instrument. An increase in the interview time from twenty minutes to thirty minutes or more might be considered. All subjects reported that some of the allotted time was expended in various social amenities and extraneous conversation. While a facilitative atmosphere for the interview can be estab- lished by observing certain social conventions inherent in the first meeting of two persons who are not previously acquainted, these activities consume time which could be more profitably spent as far as information gathering is con- cerned. It would appear that extending the time of the interview by ten or fifteen minutes would not be burdensome on either interviewee or interviewer. It is recommended that any replication of the present research be done over less than eighteen weeks elapsed time. As has been noted, it was intended that this study be com- pleted in ten weeks or less; unanticipated delays extended the total time required for the study to nearly double that which was initially intended. It is certainly not known what the end result of the extended time frame was on the post- testing. It has been observed that the end result of the treatment had no positive effect on the post-test for 87 co-orientational accuracy. Phase I of the treatment, it will be recalled, was a lecture-demonstration on interviewing techniques, and it is of interest to speculate what effect this portion of the treatment might have had on the post-test interviews for co-orientational accuracy had the post-tests been more closely related to Phase I in time. Other delays between pre-test and post-test can result from the difficulty in scheduling subjects' participation in the various phases of the treatment. The researcher found it necessary to administer the three phases of the treatment at the convenience of the subjects' schedules. On occasion, this caused time to be lost. It is not deemed to have been of significance in this study, but it should be a concern of any who attempt a replication of this study. Any replication of the study should be done with a greater N than was used in this study. Although there was no subject mortality in the present group, the small sample (N = 13) required much larger gain scores to produce signifi- cance than would have been required with a larger number of subjects with regard to the measure of co-orientational accuracy. Before undertaking future research, consideration should be given to the fact that the null hypothesis relative to co-orientational accuracy was not rejected in the present study. As has been noted, the present study was designed to attempt to increase empathic capability in two areas with one 88 treatment. The fact that the null hypothesis relative to the ASS was rejected beyond the .05 level of confidence permits the inference that the treatment was effective in the attempt to increase the subjects' empathic capabilities relative to assessment of the affective state of others. The inference is even stronger when one recalls that the gain scores were of such magnitude to have permitted rejec- tion of the null hypothesis beyond the .01 level of confidence. However, it would appear that the treatment as presently constituted does not have an effect on the sub- jects' ability to enter into a state of co-orientational accuracy with others. In addition to the compression of time and the elimina- tion Of controversial t0pics, the following two recommenda- tions are made to the future researcher in an effort to increase the treatment's effectiveness as it relates to co- orientational accuracy: (1) Kagan's original instructional model (Influencing Human Interaction, 1972), on which the experimental treatment in the present research was based, contains a unit dealing with Inquirer Training. Kagan states the purpose of the Inquirer Training to be: "... to give you an opportunity to learn about and experiment with assertive but non-judgemental, non-hostile, interviewer leads" (p. IV-l). It is felt that this additional training as an interviewer would enhance the over-all treatment and enable the subject to become more adept at information gathering as a 89 facilitative interviewer. (2) During the IPR interviews which constitute Phase III of the present research, the inter- viewer was instructed to deal with either the cognitive or affective content of the statements made by the interviewee. The only admonition was to conduct the interview in such a manner that an on-going, functional communication link would be established. A change in that procedure is recommended for future replication of this study. It is recommended that in future interviews, the interviewee be assigned not only an affective state to portray, as was done in this study, but the core of a belief system relative to a tOpical area. The topics could include such areas of instructional interest as "behavioral objectives," "modular instruction," "mediated instruction," "behavior alteration," or "mastery methodology." The interviewer would not only have to deal with the assigned affective state encountered in the interviewee, but would have to conduct the interview in such a way that he elicited accurate information relative to the topic area. During the recall process, which would be simultaneous recall during both of the interviews, the recall worker would assist the interviewer to deal with his feelings and emotions as is usually done; and would assist the interviewer in a compari- son Of the information he had gathered during the interview with the assumed belief system of the interviewee. It is not known what implications for future research might be held in racial differences. In the present study, £01 par be rac 90 four of the subjects were black males; the rest of the sample was composed of whites. All of the faculty members who participated in the test interviews and in the stimulus films as actors were white. It is recommended that future research be done with faculty members as interviewees having a multi- racial composition if the sample is multi-racial. Another point of concern to future researchers using multi-racial samples is the fact that one of the actors in the stimulus films was an attractive white female. A trained psychologist who viewed the vignette in which she appeared noted that there was an obvious sensual undertone to her portrayal. It was noted that the reSponses of some of the black male sub- jects to her differed markedly in both cognitive and affec- tive content from other's responses. It is conceivable that matters of racial concern could have been a factor that militated against any positive instructional benefit intended in the VRAS and against some subject's ability to gather in- formation which would have been helpful in their making accurate predictions of belief during the test interviews. Implications for Education There can be little doubt that the experimental treat- ment used in the present research has been successful in training persons whose professional achievement is geared to their ability to communicate with others, e.g., Rowe (1972). 91 It was the purpose of this research to determine if this in- structional strategy could be adapted to the training of instructional developers/change agents. The findings suggest that this has been done. It is regrettable that the research reported here cannot claim positive results in both of the dimensions Of empathic capability. However, it has been demonstrated to a reasonable degree that instructional development trainees can be taught to more accurately assess the affective state of others and have gained insight into the many facets Of affective sensitivity. It is deemed ad- visable that academic programs which have as their goal the training of instructional develOperS/change agents institute this type of instructional strategy if such is presently not available. It is believed that this treatment with the modi- fications suggested above will also prove effective in~ enabling trainees to enter into a state of co-orientational accuracy with those who will become their clients. There can be little doubt that the ability to do so will enhance their probabilities of success. The lack of inferrable data in the present research indicating a failure of the experimental treatment to improve capabilities of co-orientational accuracy should not deter or discourage future research into the development of this prOposed instructional strategy for appli— cation in the training of educational change agents. BIBLIOGRAPHY BIBLIOGRAPHY Anthony, W. and Wain, H. Two methods of selecting prospec- tive helpers. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 18: 155-156 (1971). ’ Argyle, Michael. Social Interaction. New York: Atherton Press, 1969. Astin, H. Assessment of empathic ability by means of a situational test. Journal of Counseling Psyphology, 14: 57-60 (1967). Bell, G. and Hall, H. The relationship between leadership and empathy. Journal of Abnormal Social ngchology, 49: 165-167 (1§31). * Berger, W. "Some Correlates of Attitude Change: Retention of the Attitude Message and Evaluation of the Communi- cator." Unpublished Ph. D. dissertation, Michigan State University, 1969. Blane, S. Immediate effect of supervisory experiences in counselor candidates. Counselor Education and Super- vision, 8: 39-44 (1968). I7 Campbell, R. "Development and Validation of a Multi-choice Scale to Measure Affective Sensitivity." Unpublished Ph. D. dissertation, Michigan State University, 1967. Campbell, D. and Stanley, J. Ex erimental and Quasi-Experi- mental Designs for Researc . Chicago: Rand McNally, 1963. I Caracena, P. and Vicory, J. Correlates of phenomological and judged empathy. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 16: 510-515 (1969). Chaffee, S. g£_gl, "A General Model of Coorientation." Paper presented to the Theory and Methodology Division, Association for Education in Journalism, Berkley, California (1969). 92 93 Chapman, J. "Development and validation of a scale to measure empathy." Journal of Counseling Psychology, 18: 281-282 (1971). Cronbach, L. Processes affecting scores of "understanding others" and "assumed similarity." Psyghological Bulletin, 52: 174-194 (1955). Cronbach, L. and Gleser, G. Assessing similarity between profiles. Psychological Bulletin, 50: 456-474 (1953). Crow, W. and Hammond, K. The generality of accuracy and response sets in interpersonal perception. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 54: 384-390 (1957). Danish, S. and Kagan, N. Measure of affective sensitivity: toward a valid measure of interpersonal perception. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 18: 51-54 (1971). Davies, I. "The Management of an Instructional Developer- Client, Evaluator-Client Relationship: Some Aspects of a Theory of Advice." Paper presented to the Associa- tion for Educational Communication and Technology, Las Vegas, Nevada, 1973. (By special permission of the author.) Davis, IL. et 21. Educational DeveIOpment at Michigan State UniverSity. East Lansing, 1971. Dymond, R. A scale for the measurement of empathic ability. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 13: 127-133 (1949). Greenberg, B. 25.31. Dimensions of empathic judgement of clients by counselors. Journal of Counselipg Psychol- ogy, 16: 303-308 (1969). Gustafson, K. "Simulation of Anxiety Situations and Its Re- sultant Effect on Anxiety and Classroom Interaction of Student Teachers." Unpublished Ph. D. dissertation, Michigan State University, 1969. Harris, S. "Shouting's Worst Way to Conduct Argument." Detroit (Michigan) Free Press, May 3, 1973. Hawes, L. "An Empirical Definition and Analysis of Physician- Patient Communication System." Unpublished Ph. D. dissertation, Ohio State University, 1971. 94 [Kagan, N. Influencing Human Interaction. East Lansing, Michigan State University, 1972} Kagan, N. 32.31."StudieS'in‘Human'InteraCtiOn. 1(Final . Report, Grant No. OE7-32-0410-270). Education Publi- cation Services, Michigan State University, East Lansing, 1967. Kagan, N. and Schauble, P. Affect simulation in interpersonal process recall. Journal of Counselipg Psychology, 16: 309-313 (1969).. 77 Katz, R. Empathy, Its Nature and Uses. New York: The Free Press, 1963. Kerlinger, F. Foundations of Behavioral Research. Holt, Rinehart, and Winston, 1964. Lorir, M. and McNair, D. Expansion of the interpersonal behavior cycle. Journal Of Personal Social PsychOlOgy, 64: 259-309 (1965). New York: MCNemnar, Q. Psyghological Statistics. New York: John Wiley and Sons, 1969. Miller, E. "Directions for Instructional Development." ‘Unpublished paper delivered to ID & T Workshop, Michigan ‘State University, East Lansing, 1971. Innovation diffusion: a study in Moore, S. and Mizuba, K. 33: 181-185 (1969). credibility. Education Forum, NeWcomb, T. An approach to the study of communicative arts. Psycholggical Review, 60: 393-404 (1953). chrtia' J. "Instructional Development: A Second Look." Unpublished paper. Michigan State University, East .Lansing, 1973. (By special permission of the author.) P ass"Ons, W. and Olsen, L. Relationship of counselor charac- ‘teristics and empathic sensitivity. Journal of Counsel- ggng Psychology, 16: 440-445 (1969). RE: ddy, W. Effects of imediate and delayed feedback on the Ilearning of empathy. Journal of Counseling PsychOlOgy, 16: 59-62 (1969). Eh: gets , E. and Shoemaker, F. Communication of Innovations. IVew York: The Free Press, 1971. 95 "A 50-Hour Intensified IPR Training Program for Rowe, K. Unpublished Ph. D. dissertation, Michigan Counselors." State University, 1972. Senter, R. Analysis of Data. Glenview, Illinois: Scott, Foresman and Co., 1969. Shapiro, J. et a1. Therapeutic conditions and disclosures beyond the therapeutic encounter. Journal of Counsel- ing Psychology, 16: 290-294 (1969){—_ "SME+ID (Choose One:) Frustration/Confusion/ National Society for Programmed In- 1 (1973). Thiagarajan, 8. Collaboration." struction Newsletter, XII, The California F scale in Titans, H. and Hollander, E. Psychological Bulle- psychological research 1950-1955. tin, 54: 47-65 (1957). Client dogmatism and perceived Tosi, D. and Carlson, W. Personnel and Guidance Journal, counselor attitudes. 48: 657-660 (1970). Toward a tentative measurement of the central Truax, c. Fayetteville, University of therapeutic ingredients. Arkansas, 1967. walJ