smcHASI'r‘Ic stunt-Es or Afl ‘ MD? M " " g"; . IN" :MUIITIZGELLULAR skews; ‘ : - _ _.r- _ - MICE! "AN STA-TE 'uMwER-«ITY ,. ' = , _‘|:RA DAVID SKURMCM r ' f * ~ . i .r. . . "gar -‘ ’ : ~.. ~ ‘. ”(Ma-ww— -‘ ' _~-__,, ,_ Our-‘o. A Ouw fl. . A ' MW I 9- 2W. flu; ,_ I WVJ4m§-N H’ . --... n- , -44w—fi :— ‘M ‘ "v- a --~ou-q~. w; 0 “ “fi‘ -. I _.u.‘ “w.“ “M m-v-r— . D'AI‘ ... a... . I ' I hm "‘"H-NM— a»: . . "w... ”ma...“ ~rrzr-1crmau - z: , , . . 4 .9-.." 9317- n u... v ' d’nv-Lmvdov- ,z'lg" - - J; 4v .- HZ”... . nwrr.- m...“ ,. ‘ vow-1:“. MW 5'“: "" -..'. - ; ... "41;“ W.» @334. deA. -.z‘):: \f'H'“ 14. .‘-> —- -4 _ "Mfg , z. 3.4 ‘ . m w, _~ . ... .m...u.,-. ,. ; - -~ - - .1 w .,_ -_,‘H ...,... ' ' ‘::‘ :a;~~- .—.....”~'— » , u -....:_,._.w-..;. kg... 1’. ‘ , ~m~ ”lufhf. ‘ f; _ " _, _‘;'_ ~"4~k.-u< a...“ . _ ' ”warm -4. ,nm. 7‘ f‘t“ - , ,7 , _ In ._. w. 2'. f“.. 'm. ..-~..-: 4- a. .- u-u o- «wm-I -. av J-“rr .o_ . - _ ., ,_ - A. _.. .._.. mm“. . . ....k “W...“ ‘ -.‘A- .. .fl—m- can...” 3: ‘Jr”‘ _ LIPP* MiCifigtm ‘5': ;.: .. University ‘ , This is to certify that the .32. % thesis entitled sioCHASTIc STUDIES OF AGING AND MORTALITY .i‘ IN MULTICELLULAR ORGANISMS presented by IRA DAVID SKURNICK has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for _EH_._D_._degree in _—BIOPHYSICS ' / fi/CQJHK rig-”N 9‘44 // Major professor ' / Date OCTOBER 1z 1%; 0-7639 ABSTRACT STOCHASTIC STUDIES OF AGING AND MORTALITY IN MULTICELLULAR ORGANISMS By Ira David Skurnick This study is concerned with the development of a quantitative phenomenological theory of aging. Such a theory has two immediate objectives: (1) To mathematically delineate the kinetics of mortality starting from fundamental time-independent considerations, and (2) To explain the temperature dependence of the mortality rate in poikilo- therms. Probabilistic clocks are studied and it is shown that only a very small number of steps likely function as rate—limiting factors in senescence. This is summarized in the derivation and discussion of the Irk‘law. If the number of rate-limiting steps, k, is small, the relative spread in age at death will be broad. This is consis- tent with empirical observation. The asymptotic development of order theory (a branch of the theory of probability) is sketched, and its application to the de— scription of a molecular theory of aging is discussed. It is shown, in particular, that the two simplest empirically determined expres- sions for the mortality rate (Gompertz law and the power law) derive from a common root in order theory. The significance of this Ira David Skurnick observation is four-fold: (1) It becomes clear that observed kinetic results have their fundamental explanation in general probabilistic considerations and are not intrinsically biological in character; (2) The kinetic results do not reflect, in any simple way, the under- lying molecular mechanisms responsible for senescence; (3) To the ex- tent that these simple empirical results provide an adequate kinetic description, no rate-limiting molecular process can be uniquely cho— sen on the basis of kinetic studies alone; and (A) The mathematical character of the rate-limiting events can differ sharply from both Gompertz law and the power law. Theorems are presented which enable one to determine if a proposed molecular process could be consistent with the simple empirical results. Lastly, two specific models are pr0posed which treat kinetic and thermodynamic considerations in a more demanding fashion. Both models contain elementary steps consistent in character with the thermal de— naturation of proteins. Both are in agreement with kinetic studies conducted at constant temperature. The dramatic life shortening ef- fect of elevated temperature on poikilotherms appears in a natural way. Theoretical predictions compare favorably with experimental re- sults obtained at a constant temperature, and provoke re-examination of the question of temperature-memory effects in senescence. Physio- logical vitality is shown to decline linearly with time and the mag- nitude of this decline is consistent with empirical findings. STOCHASTIC STUDIES OF AGING AND MORTALITY IN MULTICELLULAR ORGANISMS By Ira David Skurnick A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of BiOphysics IQTA DEDICATION This dissertation is dedicated to my parents Jack and Nettie Skurnick and to my wife Miriam Carol Skurnick Their encouragement has made this work possible and their enthusiasm has made it a joyous undertaking. ii ACKNOWLEDGMENTS Several individuals and institutions provided assistance through- out the duration of the work presented in this dissertation and deserve recognition. Professors Gabor Kemeny, Barnett Rosenberg, Alfred Haug, and Subhendra Mahanti each read the manuscript and provided thoughtful questions and constructive comments. Professor Edward Eisenstein is appreciated for his early support and many kindnesses. Professor Barnett Rosenberg, with his uncommon appreciation of the role of theory in biology, has been a steady source of encouragement and a fruitful source of guidance. Any expression of appreciation to Professor Gabor Kemeny, my major professor, would be inadequate despite its sincerity. His imprint is felt throughout this dissertation; yet the true extent of his influence can best be measured by the yardstick of time. I consider him both my Teacher and my friend. I am indebted to the Adult DevelOpment and Aging Branch of the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, and particu- larly to Dr. Lester Smith, for the Opportunity to be an active partici- pant in the Fifth Annual Biology of Aging "Summer Course" held at the University of Minnesota/Duluth in August 1973. The meeting served to improve my perspective and helped to sharpen some of the views expressed iii in this dissertation; views which I originally presented in somewhat less polished form at that time. I am grateful to Mrs. Mary Bandurski and Mrs. Bea Rabin for intro- ducing me, in a practical sense, to Drosophila melanogaster, and for the many hours both spent in the careful accumulation of empirical data. Particular thanks are due Miss Peggy McKelvey and Mrs. Marie Betterly for undertaking the very difficult task of transcribing my handwritten notes, and for doing so with care and good humor. Lastly, I am.appreciative of support received from the National Institutes of Health, through Grant GM—Olh22, and for funds provided by the College of Human Medicine and the College of Osteopathic Med- icine at Mighigan State University. iv TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . vi LIST OF FIGURES . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . vii Chapter 1 :mmmmmeN ... ... ... ... ... ... .. l 2 EMPIRICAL BACKGROUND . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 5 3 PROBABILISTIC CLOCKS . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 30 h ASYMPTOTIC DISTRIBUTIONS OF ORDER THEORY . . . . . . 39 5 DOMAINS OF ATTRACTION . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 5h 6 THE CHAIN MODEL . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 56 7 THE SEQUENTIAL MODEL . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 62 8 THE COMPETITIVE MODEL . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 79 9 DECLINE IN VITALITY . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 87 10 CONCLUSIONS . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 92 APPENDIX . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 95 LIST OF REFERENCES . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . l3l Table LIST OF TABLES Page Comparison of compensation law constants Tc and b . . 9 Computer simulation for competitive model k6 = c6, T = 25°C 0 o o o o o o o o o o o o o o o o o o 99 Computer simulation for competitive model k6 = c6, T = 39°C 0 o o o o o o o a o o o o o o o o o o o o 107 Computer simulation for competitive model k7 = c , T = 25°C 0 O O O O O I O O O O O O O O O O O O '0? I O 115 Computer simulation for competitive model k7 = c7, T = 39°C . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 123 vi Figure 10 ll l2 13 1h 15 LIST OF FIGURES A plot of the activation entropy, [38% , versus the activation enthalpy, [RBI , for thermal killing of various organisms . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . A plot of the activation entrOpy, A 8* , versus the activation enthalpy, A;H , for protein denaturation. Characteristic survival curves for closed pOpulations of multicellular organisms . . . . . . . . . . . . . Plot of logarithm of mortality rate versus time (Gompertz plot) for sample populations of DrOSOphila melanogaster at 25°C . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Plot of double logarithm of reciprocal of the sur- viving fraction versus lOgarithm of time (power law plot) for Drosophila melanogaster at temperatures of (l) 33°C, (2) 31°C, (3) 27°C, and (u) 25°C . . . . Gompertz plots of human mortality data . . . . . . . Power law plots of human mortality data . . . . Arrhenius plot of the values of A for Drosophila melanogaster . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Decline in human functional capacity as a function Of age 0 O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O 0 Power law plotzai(k)n._9 1%éf2) . . . . . . . . . . Power law plot:§(k)n .9 V063) . . . . . . . . . Gompertz plot: S§(k)n _4, 7(2) . . . . . . . . . . . General power law plot . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Schematic diagram of the sequential process . . . . . Schematic diagram of the competitive process . . . . vii 11 1h 17 19 21 23 26 28 A8 50 52 6h 67 81 CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION Brian Goodwin once remarked, facetiously, that theoretical bio- logists had "nothing to start from and nothing to prove." All there is in biology, he wrote, Is a set of concepts such as organization, adaptation, regulation, competence, homeostasis, etc., which must carry an enormous burden of more or less intuitive understanding and experience about the essential principles of biological structure and function.1 To justify the biologists' belief in the fundamental importance of macromolecules, reductionists would require that there be deveIOped, ". . . from [a molecular] basis, a superstructure of derived theory which will give . . . some insight into the basic formal organization of the . . . forms of life which we commonly encounter."2 Statistical mechanics is one example of a successful reductionist theory in physics, in which phenomena at one level are explained in terms of dy- namical events at a lower level of organization. Theoretical biologists frequently face difficulties in satisfying the demands of reductionists because biology often lacks adequate laws to describe molecular activ- ities in cells, global prOperties of cells and organisms and, neces- sarily then, the rules which relate such microsc0pic and macrOSCOpic descriptions. This rather general dilemma faces one again in attempts to an- swer two fundamental questions which stand out in sharp relief. The first asks how life emerges from aggregations of non-living atoms and l molecules. The second treats the inverse problem, begs for a physico- chemical explanation of senescence, and is the focus of the present study. It has been stated, unequivocally, that researchers know of no inherent factor(s) which automatically produce senescence in all lines of animals and plants.3 Contrary to Weissman's view that aging is a general property of all multicellular organisms,’4 it now appears that there may be multicellular organisms which do not undergo senescence, and unicellular organisms which do. We can provide an operational definition for senescence; we can define criteria which age—related changes must meet to be considered a part of any basic aging process; and we can describe some of the bio- chemical and physiological correlates of aging.3 In spite of such con- siderable efforts, we remain at a primitive stage in our understanding of senescence,and we are obliged to admit that further general princi- ples have been notoriously difficult to articulate. We do not appreciate the relationship between stochastic and genetically programmed processes in senescence; we do not understand the effects of localized molecular lesions on the performance of in- teracting biological control systems; and.we are not at all clear about the relationship between processes which determine the life Span of organisms and\those which are responsible for the physio- logical decline common in older members of a specie. To this day no preponderance of experimental evidence yet favors any one of the varied hypotheses which postulate primary molec— ular mechanisms for senescence. Even granting that such molecular events occur, no evidence supports, for any of them, a causal association with aging. Moreover, the effects of events occurring on the molecular level must be amplified to higher levels of biological organization before an organism ultimately perishes. Which step(s) in this overall process are slowest, and thus serve to define the charac- ter of a Specie's survival curve and determine its lifespan, are largely unknown. There is no evidence to suggest that those events which trigger senescence are synonymous with rate—limiting step(s) in aging. One should not be too quick to accept the pessimism im- plicit in the view that primary mechanisms must be understood and brought under control before the rate of aging can be slowed down and lifespan significantly lengthened. It might be better to start an inquiry from the Opposite per— Spective and attempt to deduce a quantitative phenomenological theory of aging. The importance of mortality in the develOpment of a general theory of senescence has been recognized by biologists,5 and this pro- vides a natural starting point for the present work. Survival or life tables are available for a broad variety of both poikilothermic and homeothermic organisms under a number Of external influences, and many seemingly dissimilar species exhibit survival patterns which appear to Share a common character. Thus, in Spite of the possibility that spe- cific rate-limiting mechanisms might differ from Specie to specie, it is plausible to believe that one might deduce valid general principles from such a study. A theory that focuses its efforts on understanding something of the character of rate—limiting steps in senescence may or may not shed much light on the nature of primary molecular processes. But it never- theless becomes a yardstick against which such latter hypotheses must eventually be judged. In this respect such a theory is like a dress— maker's mannequin. If garments are the right Size the fit may be sat- isfactory, but this in no way proves that other combinations would not be as flattering. When they are the wrong Size, however, the fit is poor and Obvious to all. It is recognized that certain environmental influences produce 6 more dramatic effects on lifespan than others; it is simple to appre- ciate that the former command attention here. It would be prudent as well in the early develOpment of such a phenomenological theory to draw inferences from facts, and avoid the pitfalls provided by nature to those who draw inferences from inferences. Lastly, one should also be keenly aware that the mathematical analysis of biological phenomena in- volves a compromise between a desire to represent these phenomena real- istically, and limitations imposed by one's mathematical artistry. CHAPTER 2 EMPIRICAL BACKGROUND Strehler has pointed out that individual organisms may cease to exist in four ways: They can give rise to two or more organisms by fission (bacteria); the protoplasm of individuals may coalesce (slime molds); they may engage in wholesale replacement of their constituent parts (sea anemones); or they age and die.3 Only the last alternative is of interest here. The mortality rate, )A(t), which describes the decline in a closed population with age, is defined by the relation DB3 = -\_\/Nu—\] 3; ML) (1) N(t) represents the number of surviving members Of a population at time t. In general fl(t) depends on factors both intrinsic and extrinsic to the organism. At prohibitive temperatures, viruses, yeasts, bacteria, and mam- malian cells in culture commonly decline in number exponentially with time.7 The mortality rate is thus independent of time although it de— pends strongly on environmental temperature. For such organisms the logarithm of the mortality rate constant is found to decline linearly with increasing reciprocal absolute temperature. Such an Arrhenius re- lationship suggests that the rate—limiting events underlying thermal death occur on the molecular level, and may be treated by the absolute rate-theory equation ax? (Ag/R) 2x? (- AHVRT) (2) (KT A”) = “7:“ \C, the transmission coefficient, is approximately equal to unity. kB, h, and R are the Boltzmann, Planck, and gas constants respectively. _T is the absolute temperature. The activation enthalpy for thermal death, .AHI’, can be determined from the Slope of the Arrhenius plot and, when combined with empirically determined values for )A(t), yield the corres- ponding activation entropy, ASA- . Significantly, ASAP and AHA: appear to be linearly related over the range 25 kcal/mole 4 Mi é 200 kcal/mole. This linear relation, 2% \ =\= AS '-“ "1T: A“ 4- b (3) C. is a manifestation of an extra-thermodynamic relationship known as the "compensation law." Tc, is the compensation temperature, is approximate— ly 325°K and b is reasonably approximated as -66 cal/mole-°K (Figure l). The thermal denaturation of virtually all proteins,8 obey similar ex- ponential kinetics, satisfy the compensation law, and yield values for the constants TC and b closely identical to those appr0priate for the thermal death of the unicellular "organisms" described above (Table l and Figure 2). This has led to the provocative hypothesis that the thermal denaturation of proteins may serve as the rate-limiting step in the thermal death of unicellular organisms.T Indeed, Rahn has sug- gested that the exponential decline in pOpulations of unicellular or- ganisms may trace to the destruction of only a single protein molecule per cell.9 Figure l. A plot of the activation entrOpy, 138:,B , versus the activation enthalpy, A H , for thermal killing of various organisms. Yeasts are denoted by ('); bacteria by O ; and viruses byA. For a fuller discussion of the data see Reference 7. 500 ' 400 (N O O 200 AS“ col/mole deg IOO 1 l 600 r x I w u . . / , "GOO ' 5O 1 IOO I50 AkaCOI/mqle Figure l 200 _ jiv...‘ TABLE I Comparison Of compensation law constants, Tc and b. from: A8* = aAH*-+ b ; Tc = 1/3 TC(°K) b(calJmole XIX) Proteins 329 -64.9 Virus 330 -64 Yeasts 325 -64.5 Bacteria 331 -65 10 Figure 2. A plot of the activation entrOpy, AS+ , versus the activation enthalpy, AHT , for protein denaturation. Data taken from Reference 8 and discussion at length in Reference 7. The regression line is a least squares fit to the data. The constants for the line are: T0 = 329°K and b = -6h.9 cal/mole-°K. ' 400 300 IOO AS1F col/mole Cdeg ll N O O . 5O IOO I50 I AH“ kcol/mole Figure 2 12 As early as 1912 Eikjmann reported studies in which the mor- tality rate for thermal killing of bacteria was found to be time de- pendent.10 This is a common finding for multicellular organisms in- cluding man.ll If /A(t) is an increasing function of time, the specie is said to age. For multicellular organisms the characteristic popu- lation survival curve is shown in Figure 3(a). An initial lag phase is followed by a decline which tails Off for longer lived members of the cohort. The number of members of the cohort dying at any given age is shown in Figure 3(b). It is important to notice that the spread in age at death is broad when compared to the mean value, and that the decline about the mean is a continuous but not symmetric func- tion of time. Historically, kinetic studies received their principal impetus from actuaries concerned with obtaining precise numerical expressions for survival data appearing in human life tables. NO compelling effort was made to understand the nature of the physico-chemical processes in— herent in senescence. Gompertz originally proposed that the mortality rate increased exponentially with time11 _ M: (1605 ‘ Be (A) This was modified first by Makeham, who added a constant term to Gompertz' relation,l2 mm = A we“ <5) 13 Figure 3. Characteristic survival curves for closed popula- tions of multicellular organisms. Surviving fraction of population is plotted versus time in curve (a). Proportion of population dying at a specific age is exhibited by curve (b). Note that this latter distri- bution is continuous, relatively broad ( A~0.3), and asymmetric. The power law constants for the curves are: C*.= S and 1: = 70 time units. 114 “o; 8.2 km :32 o m Ohsmflm v AH.» . no no; 15 and subsequently by Perks, who added a fourth constant13 Be)t I+De‘ Presumably, one could obtain an arbitrarily good fit to the empirical data by imaginatively incorporating additional constants. Doing so makes it progressively more difficult to understand where these con— stants originate, and what they truly represent. We are also sobered by Einstein's rebuke that with three constants, one could fit an elephant! It is understandable that most early theoretical efforts set the derivation of Gompertz' expression as a major objective. This seemed reasonable since the relation appeared at least moderately suc- cessful when applied to human mortality. Recently, it was Shown that the mortality rate of a number of poikilothermic and homeothermic organisms could be fairly represented 6 by a simple two—constant "power" law of the form CK Mt): At (7) In Figures h and 5, the Gompertz law and the power law are contrasted using empirical data obtained from survival studies of Drosophila melanogaster at different temperatures. A similar comparison is made in Figures 6 and 7 for data taken from life tables apprOpriate for in In the white males, provided by the U.S. Bureau of Vital Statistics. latter case, it would seem that the power law representation is super- ior, particularly when non-aging related causes of death are subtracted from the total number of deaths in each age category and redistributed 16 Figure A. Plot of logarithm Of mortality rate versus time (Gompertz plot) for sample pOpulations of Drosophila melanogaster at 25°C. Note that data points cannot be fit by a Single straight line. See Reference 27 for discussion of this experiment which compared survivorship and mortality rates of flies reared and housed under sterile conditions with those living in cultures containing the yeasts which normally accompany Drosophila and which serve as one of their immediate food sources. LOG MORTALITY RATE 0 I OI OOI EFFECT OF STERILE CONDITIONS ON MORTALITY RATE 17 TOTA LtMALE + FE MALE) AGE IN DAYS Figure A I I I I I I I I I 0 000630 . o 0 «x9 Ifflb ° Q g.» o o ....e ‘09 °' ' c) a. o ‘r q;n o. ‘0 ° 0 o dx> o 0 @CD ' o ' (9 cc) (b ($9 0 o o " 0 oo 0 Central IIthS'I “ o N-3.740 . 0° 0 Storm "IQHZS‘T N-Lozo o O .0 o o 1 l 1 1 1 l I l s :2 IS 24 30 36 42 48 54 18 Figure 5. Plot of double logarithm of reciprocal of the sur- viving fraction versus logarithm of time (power law plot) for Drosophila melanogaster at temperatures of (l) 33°C, (2) 31°C, (3) 27°C, andf(h7 25°C. According to the power law }A(t) = At from which it follows that anm: A A+o<9me By comparison 9w (LEW/Nah: 9n (f;\ + (om 9M7: l9 2 [I'M]! A m III [[0 o 3 {oil-loo"!!- IIOI'I’ ’0 0 0.7 I If Om, I. o O o ll/lolo/ o In Ollie / I03 IO' I03 ’ no” 2 w «:2: >¢.>¢3m A95 9°“? Figure 5 20 Figure 6. Gompertz plots of human mortality data. Data are for white males dying in the United States in 1967. Curve (1) con- siders all causes of death. Curve (2), by comparison, treats only "age-related" causes of death. See Reference 6 for discussion. 21 10° - IO 80 60 4O 20 h I 2 ..o 4 m I -,_Soo.oo_\m12m9 LEE C3450: AGE (YEARS) , _ Figure 6 22 Figure 7. Power law plot of human mortality data. Data is the same as that used in construction of Figure 6. Curve (1) con- siders all causes of death. Curve (2) treats only "age—related" causes of death. See Reference 6 for further discussion. MORTALITY RATE (DEATHS/IO0,000) IO" - I0”?- I- IOTA 23 2. 1 l l J 20 40 so so IOO _AOE(YEAB§lI_ Figure 7 2h proportionately. The power-law representation is particularly useful with respect to analysis of Drosophila data for another reason: it readily permits thermodynamic analysis. Since }L(t) has dimensions of reciprocal time, A can be written as (8) Betweeen 25°C and 33°C 0K: h.5 i 0.3. While oLiS effectively un- changed, the time constant ’Z’changes by a factor of h.53 (Figure 8). That is, only one constant is markedly temperature dependent over the interval cited. Indeed, A satisfies an Arrhenius relationship too, and the calculated value for AHT is of the order of 200 kcal/mole.6 This large value for the activation enthalpy is consistent with that expected for the thermal denaturation of proteins, and suggests that the rate—limiting step(s) in the senescence Of multicellular poikilotherms may be just such a process. Earlier theorists demanded more than just the derivation of the Gompertz equation. They required also that some variable, to be iden— tified ultimately with physiological vitality, decline linearly with time. This condition was imposed since neural conduction velocity, basic metabolic rate, standard renal plasma flow, and maximum breathing capacity--to name just a few parameters of gross vitality-—were all known to behave roughly in this fashion (see Figure 9). There is no need to discuss earlier theoretical efforts here since they have been critically reviewed elsewhere.3 Generally speak— ing, however, the earlier approaches are fairly described as not funda- mental in the sense of deducing time-dependent behavior from time-in- dependent considerations. Neither were they successful in deriving both 25 Figure 8. Arrhenius plot of the values of A for Drosophila melanogaster. A values are computed from intercept points Of Fig— ure 5. Activation enthalpy, AHT, as computed from the SIOpe of this straight line is 190 kcal/mole. 26 ARRHENIUS PLOT OF A VALUES FROM POWER LAW IO'5 N) VALUES I TITIWT] d=k which can be rewritten in integral form as “ FOB I2 yea v\— a3 t: -— ”(WE3 k ( It) 0 1 <1 7L) C” (20) Here too, it has been assumed that the individual events are independ— ently and identically distributed in time. In order to calculate means and variances the corresponding probability density function C?(k)n(t) is needed. (21) kg fir“ 1“ A (96098: iémgf‘zkh) F “QUIT—i 21%) The mean value for the time at which the kEE event occurs is then given by 3h 00 O The variance is likewise cm 3 l (tack ERA mGft): 071%“) I It” (23) To facilitate integration in Equations 22 and 23 it is convenient to rewrite k?(k)n(t) as -Iz (Joana: %E( t.) Th“) (14:03)“ iiidl (21+) where the term in brackets in the last equation is recognized as the binomial distribution for the probability of k successes in n independ— ent trials. The probability of success in a given trial is just F(t). The binomial distribution, abbreviated as b[k;n,F(t)], can be expanded about that value of k for which the function is a maximum, that is, about k = nF(t). Letting k = nF(t) + 2, it follows that 9%? (—- fiianFL-b) (\~F 0*)» 32m Papa—F(afi (25) This approximation is quite good as n grows large provided that k O as n—%><>0. F(t) can be expanded in a Taylor series about t = t0 where F(to) = k/n. Thus WI = View itFRIPeritifi-ACIIRAIP' (26> t’fia (“7% For n large only the first order correction term need be kept, thus 35 FL‘t\ "J k/fl 3‘ SOC] (27) where 59‘) z 0‘ (Etc) (28) and <1 = i POW 0“: (29) into th The mean value for the time of occurrence of the k——-event thus becomes I—k/n (,MPL-‘kiwv—T SIAM 9°) ° FAIL KI: _ - Eh +8} lQMG': +SXI 7‘ 5) .49!“ (30) The larger the value of n, the smaller 5 mmst be in order that contri- butions to the integrand are non-negligible. For example, if n = 100 and k/n = 1/2, the integral is fi 10'5 for 5 Z/O.2. The expression for the mean can thus be accurately approximated by 00 It \(ts _9‘87’ Ek("=\= 20")? _oe 0+; 9 d8 (31) where w k : Rf ._ ZL ) n) AQW“< (A <1 Va) (32) and 36 P1: “/9~(%—)(I-%\ (33) Similarly, the variance can be expressed as 00 z _ 1 (t:+a%+%:_) Q,F 8d8~ E:(‘Q (3h) IR Van. dfl = _Z-Zn—ég) The ratio of the standard deviation to the mean thus reduces to Mk)" "a: 7:1“ (RAIUJR) (35) where the product (atO) is some function of k/n. For k some fixed '~oD fraction of n it follows that A (k) varies as l/‘ik. A Simple example might serve to illustrate the point. Let F (ii) = (CL-L)“ (36) then It (are) 2' “77 (37) and \ T-"'—_I Am: ”AT, ' (‘/°()A‘"T\ (38) Once again the relative accuracy of the probabilistic clock is seen to vary as the inverse square-root of the number Of independent events counted. In practice, the clock could count a large number of events. The a[R‘law requires that the subset which occurs most slowly, 37 and thus determines the time scale on which the clock functions, must be small in number in order to explain the Observed broad spread in age at death. It is important, as well, to understand what the KRTlaw does not mean. It does not mean that procesSes involving large numbers of molecules or large numbers Of cells can be summarily disregarded. Consider the following example. Suppose that a large number Of mole— cules, n, have to be synthesized before an organism can perish. Let synthesis be by means of an autocatalytic reaction initiated by k molecules in which the mean number Of molecules synthesized grows exponentially in time. That is «— I: VI = h eL (39) There will be some distribution in the number of molecules synthesized by any given time, and Delbrfick has shown that the ratio of the stand— ard deviation to the mean varies as cm“) /E (“I N VAT; (no) for n ‘)f> k, Thus a relatively broad spread in the number of mole- cules synthesized is seen to be due to the small number of molecules needed to initiate the autocatalytic process. Equivalently there will be some distribution in the time at which a critical level of molecules, n*, is formed. The peak in the distribution function occurs at t* given by Y\ (I ZWQ’WM (III) 38 The standard deviation about t*, in units of l/c, is easily Shown to be 6° N VAT: (12) which approximation becomes more accurate as k increases. CHAPTER A ASYMPTOTIC DISTRIBUTIONS OF ORDER THEORY The results of the previous chapter Show that probabilistic aging clocks can function in a manner markedly different from that of their deterministic counterparts. Aging clocks have been discussed elsewhere16 in the context of programmed aging where it was implicitly assumed that the aging of the individual had some positive adaptive value for the Specie. Such a clock could function even if the mortal- ity of the individual derived, in a proximate sense, from a breakdown in the integrity of biological processes. A discussion of such ques- tions is beyond the scope of the present paper. The point to be em- phasized is that if a prObabilistic clock depends on some small number of events, then it may provide for the relatively broad spread in age at death. This, of course, does not mean that any process involving small numbers Of events will be realistic. For example, the amplification process discussed by Delbrfick in his study of fluctuations provides for the broad Spread desired. However, the distribution function giving the time at which the critical number of molecules is synthesized is skewed in the direction opposite to that found in practice for mortal- ity as shown in Figure 3(b). In fact as the number of molecules initi- ating the amplification process is increased the distribution function becomes a (symmetric) Gaussian. A second example is provided by l Szilard in his study of the nature of the aging process. 7 Szilard 39 IIO assumes a typical cohort to be heterogeneous in the number of genetic faults each member inherits. Subsequent "hits" render genes ineffec- tive and death occurs when the number of functional cells falls below some critical fraction. The inherited faults are distributed in the pOpulation at random, and the mean number of faults inherited per per— son must be between 2 and h to account for the relative spread in age at death. Szilard's approach has been faulted by Maynard-Smith18 and the latter's criticisms will not be repeated here. One flaw, however, was appreciated by Szilard himself. In the crude form of the theory, members of a cohort would die only in certain years with no deaths in the intervening period. This prediction contrasts sharply with reality. Apparently then, survival or mortality curves must be derived. Of the counting mechanisms discussed in Chapter 3 for probabilistic aging clocks, the one which assumes that rate-limiting events may occur in random order is of particular interest for three reasons: First, it focuses attention directly on the character of the rate-limiting events. The assumption implicit in this approach is that amplification occurs on a time scale much Shorter than that required for the occurrence Of the initiating events. Second, it permits considerable further analy- sis without the need to specify the cumulative distribution function, F(t). In this respect the conclusions to be drawn are relatively model- independent. And lastly, this approach yields kinetic results which are striking in their agreement with empirical findings. Let the events be labeled as E E l’ 2, . . . En for purposes of identification. The time at which event E Occurs is designated by t, J jn' Since the events occur at random, one could well have hl IA is“ 5 Laws inn '4' Tina (A3) It makes more sense in the present context to label events by the order in which they occur in time. The time at which the jig-temporally— ordered events occurs is t(j)n' Thus Am“ 5 (“(2)-As E thM (MI) Clearly, t(j)n is some function of both n and the set of unordered var- iables Itjni' By definition then it”. “ W‘s, it.) in,” AMI (A5) The set of ordered variables §t(j)nflgis referred to frequently as the variational series of the unordered random variables. The behavior of these "ordered" random variables is described by "order theory." The probability that at least j events occur by time t is just ADA—k6)“ {AK 7 @Lj)‘\£\ (1&6) Consistent with the original assumption that at least k events must occur for an organism to perish, it follows from the law Of large num- berslg that the decline in a pOpulation with time Should be given by N(t)/Nb) ‘2 A- 233‘, EA) k\‘* (W) N(t) is the Size of the pOpulation at time t, and it is implicit in this last expression that no new members are allowed to enter the cohort. A2 Thus a population survival curve can reflect the probability of failure of individual organisms, and the problem Of describing the ki- netics of mortality reduces, in principle, to the determination of the distribution functions §(k)n(t)° The latter should depend upon k, n, and the precise functions which describe the occurrence Of the under- lying events themselves. For the sake of simplicity it can be assumed that the events occur independently of one another, and that they can be described by a common probability density function. This is likely an oversimpli- fication of the problem. These restrictions can be relaxed, but they are used here because they lead to quite powerful results in their own right. Let f(t) be the probability density that an individual event occurs at time t. The probability that the event occurs by time t is then just “t Ht} = It“) An (AB) The probability that the event has not yet occurred is just l—F(t). By any instant only two outcomes are possible with respect to a given event; it either has or has not yet occurred. The n independ— ent events thus constitute a set of Bernoulli trials. The prObability that exactly k of n events occur by time t is then given by the bino- l9 mial distribution ' N(t) [WAIT—h (1:9) \ L(IEV“IFQ£I)':' \C:(hrk3\ A3 The probability that at least k events occur by time t is TL icky: Z (A) FEED—F(tflfl - H (19) which can be written in integral form as T \‘ Fm I: T ‘2 '£)=' 1-1) I A IRAQ I _, (g 3% M (20) (Rafi IR). 6 This last equivalence can be verified by carrying through the indicated integration "by parts." As n-? ob , i. (t) may approach a stable limiting form (k)n gfi(k)(t). If so, the two distributions can differ, at most, by a linear transformation in their arguments. That is, Team») -«~-> abs» (so) as n—57cx) for some choice of the constants an and bn. This is referred to as the stability criterion,20’21 In general, for arbitrary F(t), such stable limit distributions do not exist. The set of functions §F(t)~g which possess a common asymptotic distribution, 36%)“), de— fine the latter's "domain of attraction." If k/n-? 0 or 1 as n-=7‘><> the resultant stable distributions are called extreme-value functions. If, on the other hand, k/n—Tv' A (O 4 7\ 4 l) as n-—?OO , the limiting functions are referred to as asymptotic distributions of the central terms. The latters' respective domains of attraction are termed "norm? al" if the asymptotic distributions are independent of the rate at which k/n’?) . There are only three extreme-value distributions.21 They are: IIII Qt) WAG?) :2 o 3 iéo ‘ if: (51) ...—...— ——?L Ira—I (Ia—OAS Q [X d“ ') ‘l:>o,<=(7o a» f .. I _. - Va 1 (It) '” m) g Q%1 (AK) Wli4°0 (52) and _xx 0%) I -I O z ‘ ) 7t >0 (53) There are four asymptotic distribution' for the central terms with normal domains of attraction.21 They are: o( Q) \ C“: “1.11 ”‘6: (fl: PIE-dg 0' 61 d9: 3 A320) [)0 “‘ (5h) 1' O ) t 40 ~6Ifld . $62) ‘ QIfifxd i4 £> : b— ' 0 6 " 0(6) (ARE 9L ) ’ --00 (55) :2 ”I ') 35>O o( a It $5) ,3... .I \_ fl, - (XOR: I355; 0“ d8 ) A40)L\>o ....ao d, gjt l 7. (56) I I- ”axa '—" Z 4“ Eli-II Q2 7“, "1170) £170 AS and u C? Tm) dc} 3 “I: é: ——I II l~ 6L 3 «1 47h §,I "-3 A 3 9C>| On should already suspect that the distributions for the central terms will prove unsatisfactory (see discussion Of the lSRTlaw in Chapter 3). @051), 935:2), and §3(h)are discontinuous and in view of Equations A7 and 50 can be rejected immediately. In fact, the discontinuity arises in the vicinity of where ETN(t) is itself a maxi- mum. There is no corresponding discontinuity in the empirical data. 51. (3) - . . _ dt EEOL (t) 18 continuous, but only If cl - c2. It would fol- low that.%EN(t) should be symmetric about its maximum, but this is con— tradicted by empirical findings (Figure 3(b)). The extreme value distributions are more interesting. In fact, for k = l at (I) := 1- . 960, (7(2) 6 ) '11 70) 000 (58) From Equation A7 and the definition of the mortality rate, Equation 1, it follows that c&~I MAM (2 0LT: (59) I «A: Similarly t: e A) “' °/\ (H '—’ 1,. 6 (6o) II6 from which the analogous mortality rate can be shown to be )A (*3) 2 2+ (61) A“) Thus, apart from time—scale factors, both the power law and Gompertz law have been recovered. The distributions dpiF)(t) are not of value in the present context. For k small, but no longer equal to unity, departures from lin- earity appear in the characteristic power law and Gompertz plots (see Figures 10-12). When k grows large the very character of the asymptotic distributions are modified. For example, when k = n ..L-‘h °‘ 3;“ “q: Q ) 3 £50,4>0 :: A -’ £70 ) 0‘70 (62) and Q—‘t 0‘) .. ‘ . -004i< co 7: (t -— Q \ > (63) In the first instance the corresponding mortality rate becomes d-\ -.(-i)d -L- t)“ )th)\ Swift) 2 /‘*Q 3 tea (61+) W at In the latter case _ Q-t -1; ..e. - co = a 4-6 ‘ ”“4“ RUN m P / ’ (65) A Neither of these results are satisfactory. 1&7 . <2) , Figure 10. Power law plot: §?(k)(£):7 Eb UT The mortality n A rate is given by Qd-I F (34‘) \ “((1, : 0‘ (3*) \+ (34:50: Only the asymptotes are shown. 1&8 VNG—I —"'> ‘—" V «I Za-I ln_(3t) Figure 10 h9 Figure ll. Power law plot: @(kfiH Vuég), The mortality d. rate is given by 3d“ :4 Mm. -= W. /LwMJ o( Only the asymptotes are shown. In); 50 V'f‘uvc,'-'l I 4—— V~30-I In (31) Figure ll 51 2 Figure 12. Gompertz plot: éé(k) (£)s,zicg. The mortality rate is given by n 2521: “MW = ge Luefl Only the asymptotes are shown. In)! 52 ...... V~2 91 Figure 12 53 Thus both of the simple empirically determined expressions for the mortality rate derive from a common origin in the asymptotic dis- tributions of order theory. In fact, both are deduced for the special case where k = l. Apparently then the survival curves apprOpriate for both homeothermic and poikilothermic multicellular organisms have their fundamental explanation in general probabilistic considerations, and are not intrinsically biological in character after all. CHAPTER 5 DOMAINS OF ATTRACTION Since for each asymptotic distribution there exists a domain of attraction, that is a set of functions RF,(t)?‘all of which yield to the same limit distribution, it is. immediately clear that kinetic studies alone cannot uniquely specify the underlying molecular rate-limiting Processes. It would be of considerable value, nevertheless, to be able to state quickly and confidently whether a proposed mechanism could be Consistent with observed kinetic data. This would be particularly ad- VaJttrtsaigeous since for most arbitrarily chosen functions, F(t), no stable asymptotic distributions exist at all. Several authors have established criteria by which the limit distribution, appropriate to F(t), can be determined.2l’22’23’2h In- deed, Smirnov has proven that if F(t) belongs to the domain of attrac- tiOn of any one of the proper limit laws described in Chapter ’4 for some particular value of k, then it belongs to the same domain of at- t'raction for all values of k.21 For F(t), suitably standardized (i.e., for some choice of the constants a and bn), to belong to the domain of the limit distribu- tion >001(k)(t) it is both necessary and sufficient21 that: (a) There exist some tO such that F(tO) = O and F(tO + 2,) > O for each E> O, and (b) For each ’t’)O 5h 9m flieflfl. a ,1,“ (66) —);—~>o+ F(to+t) Likewise, for F(t) to belong to the domain of attraction of the limit distribution X(k)(t), it is necessary and sufficient21 that Slim n F(adr +\o.n\ -_—. Qt <67) «V900 for each t. The constant bn is defined to be the smallest value of t for which the inequalities \ F _ é / i F +0 U: o) r\ (i: ) (68) hold. The constant an is defined as the smallest value of t '> 0 such ‘5 (Milli—”01) é ‘/ne g F( at jib—0]) (69) Gnedenko has shown that for F(t) to belong to the domain of >\(k)(t) it is fully equivalent to require that22 mm ——\wF ( Mt) : MOK fig 00 1- ? (Keg mi) for allm>0and oLrako. (70) Additional and equivalent sets of criteria have been described by Von Mises23 and Uzgorenflu CHAPTER 6 THE CHAIN MODEL As an abstraction one could represent a biological organism by a chain which consists of a very large number of links. Let the num— ber of links be denoted by n. The results of Chapter h suggest that the chain be assumed to break (and the organism to perish) when the first link breaks, irrespective of which link breaks first. In pre- vious discussions no specific functional forms were chosen for F(t). This is equivalent to not assuming a specific process for the breakage of individual links. This was done to preserve the model-independent character of earlier conclusions. It is worth assuming a specific failure mechanism here, if only to illustrate application of some of the theorems governing domains of attraction for asymptotic limit distributions. Let failure of a link be described by a sequential process. (See Chapter 3 for a discussion of sequential processes in connection with the sk‘law.) Transformation of a link from one state to the next is equivalent to deterioration. After :3," W3 ows . l F(‘Q: (ed— \3\ ZS: F(d-x-SH] (‘83 (86) 4:: fit)“ 01‘. (87) Clearly F(O) = O and F(E'.) > O for each 5 > 0. Likewise ' 6O 9km F(‘rgfl ‘ ’Z’DL _‘ 89% Y (m ‘88) for all ”TV 0. Thus there exist constants an and bn such that F(ant + bn) be- longs to the domain of attraction of the limit distribution 1/0610“). Under such conditions the kinetics of chain breakage (and thus the de— cline in a cohort) would be described by the power law. On the other hand, for (9t) large, the incomplete gamma function may be expanded in decreasing powers of ( 9t): OH 3}, 31—} F(flJ‘Qfl— (gt) a: 2 £531“:— + 19(\5’7L\.M)] W0 09*)“ (89) Where M: \,a,3,. (90) ~17? ék((t). The kinetics of chain breakage, under these conditions, would obey Gompertz law. Not only does it appear then that the simplest empirical find- ings have their root in a common mathematical principle, but both can apparently be derived from a common schematic model. By now it should be clear that while failure mechanisms may lead to unique asymptotic limit distributions, the converse is not true. Thus there is no way, resting on kinetic results alone, to show that the above schematic model is uniquely valid. Furthermore, it should be apparent as well that if biological considerations are to enter the kinetic equations for mortality, they must do so through the constants that appear in those equations. CHAPTER 7 THE SEQUENTIAL MODEL The use of order theory in conjunction with a simple sequential failure mechanism has come very close to describing observed kinetic re- sults. There are, however, at least three limitations to this approach: First, it may be unrealistic to assume that n—€><>0 in practice. Second, the rate at whichwfigkk)n(t) approaches its asymptotic form depends markedly on F(t) itself. In fact, for certain distributions, F(t), the error involved in using the corresponding limit distribution is neglig- ible for n as small as 10. For others the error would still be signifi— 6 cant when n /~’lO . Lastly, the qualitative character ofH§§(k)n(t) can change dramatically as n increases. Certain distributions which tend to )\(k)(t) as n-€?:\ “(H (101) 3: Similarly, the probability that the chain is still intact is Shh) = q: “(fl : <2: 395 (15)“ (102) Consequently the age-Specific failure rate, the mortality rate, is AW (“/i’aX'i‘imt) ”03’ The instantaneous lepe of the power-law plot, a sensitive indicator of ‘ the character of survival curve, is given by v(t) = 3:39;: Swim: if; mum (at Experimental evidence suggests that v(t) is generally not an integral number. This is in contrast with results obtained when n -->oo . It was shown (see Chapter 6) that the sequential model de- seribed above satisfied Smirnov's criteria and could yield to the k . asmptotic distribution 0(L )(t). For k = 1 it has already been Shown that #(fi) 1' mick) (105) Thus ”Um .... a” (106) 70 More generally, for finite n, v(t) can be written as site) 5941+) “(it)”? A’s—...... d‘t d rv N 2‘; +6) Ht) which clearly does not depend upon n. For any given:F(t), increasing n (107) only serves to bring S(t) to zero more rapidly. Thus a plot of the mortality rate versus time stops at progressively smaller values of time as n is increased. Increasing n then effectively serves to mask the intrinsic character of the mortality curve. This can be demon— strated for the sequential model rather easily. Letting cl = c = . . . = c = g , it follows from Equation 2 Ci 87 that ”Twae1— Eff ()0 (108) for (gt) small. In this regime —( 3111 W9) : "“9 Qfl. (109) (0‘4)! and 9: z __ ‘k air-3. (gt) 91 £11 (110 ) (oz-2.) \. Therefore om ( glad/(01-x)! vat=t)——-i . =«4 i: )- ggfl (111) 01‘. Thus the result originally obtained when n—+><30 is recovered as (gt) -—> 0. However, when (3t) >> 1, 71 -\ —— i: ”Hm as as) (SPOOL (ct-0‘. (112) Thus _— N 5’ iii) L— (~— (gt) +(o4 003%) ( (ck-Q (113) and 33:6»ng g1 (Sgt) 26* 083-19 We”) 1*) “(32 11h) From which it follows that 5-1- - r‘v 1' ELL dkxwy+m\ (it + 9* (115) [ gem) LO“ Mam] 1‘ 9* (it) («‘11) (116) 81: and dz ‘ _ .. 311.16%) 350*) _ 3) Expanding the denominator in a power series, multiplying through, and keeping terms only as far as (1/ 9t)2 gives A?» od-l 8: L4“); (117) Thus N 01" (f(gg ~ 34: (118) 72 Thus v(gt) ...», O as (gt) 4700 . But this is the saturation effect. To what extent it is seen in practice clearly depends on n. It makes good sense that each of the 0( rate constants 1C3?) should be of the same order of magnitude. If any of the stages in the process went to completion appreciably faster than the others it should not appear as a component in the rate-limiting sequence. Conversely, if one rate constant were significantly smaller than the others it should be the only term seen in practice. Let C1: C2 = . . . = cd-l= (1% c“. It follows from Equat ion 99 that 1% (S) = C34 (‘/s+c3 (119) for,j=l,2,...,o(-l,and {(fi = :4 (y5+c)°1"( x/sa-Cu) (120) for j = o( . Taking inverse transforms gives —dc '. . (9503: e (at)A WM) (121) forj —l, 2, . . ., ol—l, and oL-\ €_C t C a K‘ffid _H) < V; : C“ C 122 for j = 0( . F is the incomplete gamma function described earlier. Thus 0(-\ (123) 73 d.-\ 5+d- I A, (“i at) c:;___2- 6‘ * (ab-[(C Codi—l A’VQA 4% Q (J \.)\ + (c: QA‘ army“) (12h) N(ZE «48410:: 2:”? (CQW (2:21) [(6 CA1] (125) for small (ct). Letting co(‘54 c gives 46%"; e d (126) as should be expected. Clarke and Maynard—Smith have suggested that there are two components to the senescence process.25 One part is temperature- independent and is associated with aging. The dying "phase" is re- garded as temperature-dependent. This view is supported by Hollings- worth.26 Put into the context of the sequential model, only 9x would be markedly temperature sensitive. Since, from the theory of rate- processes, one can write _Aui/m C': C 5 '3 e (127) this is equivalent to asserting that only AtHj is of considerable value. If this view is correct, then i:(t) and consequently S(t) should approach exponential form as the temperature is lowered. It should be possible to estimate the temperature decrease required to produce this effect and then compare the results with experiment. The Av r» ratio of 4-(t) to its exponential counterpart ( i‘exp) is 7h diiffz «A r Mid/”Ia? 2: (is); 9 (C CM Ef/ 10, (f-Fexp)n é 1+ X 10-5. It is reasonable then to restrict attention to the range 9% t a; 1. For d25andco4/c «'10-2 1.00 S 4(19/xwé ‘15” (129) The lower limit for the inequality corresponds to fix t = O and the upper limit to qg‘t = 1. Thus within the range of practical interest /\/ +03} 4 ‘04 2):”? (130) From Equation 127 it is clear that Q“ ( 91.33:” A%:(_\._—._)_) C0403) E '1“ T2, (131) 2 If coL(Tl) NC and co((T2) ~10- c ..L _ _L.. :3 __ A”) 1.3.... T. '11 Mr: (132) Let Tl be 29°C, the middle of a representative temperature a range commonly encountered in practice. If [&H°< rxa 2OO kcal/mole, then by T2 = 2h.8°C the characteristic survival curve should be expo- . “—t o . nential. For [XHOL /\J 100 kcal/mole, T should be 20.h C. Surv1val 2 curves for wild-type Drosophila are not observed to become exponential for temperatures as low as 17.5°C. In fact the general analytical form 75 of the survival curve remains that described by the power law, and the exponent,c¥ , is held roughly constant over a wide range in temperature. Thus A H: 4 100 kcal/mole. Yet Rosenberg, et al report measured activation enthalpies of approximately 190 heal/mole;6 this then must somehow be shared out among the several steps in the sequential process. Clarke and Maynard—Smith's division of senescence into the two phases described earlier implies an absence of temperature-memory ef- fects in aging. Thus if flies are exposed to a high temperature for some period of time and, surviving this, are shifted to a second lower temperature, they should be expected to die off as if they had been ex— posed to only the lower temperature all along. In the sequential model all ci rate constants are of comparable value and this relationship must be maintained as the temperature is changed if the SIOpe of the power 6 It follows that allc>L rate constants law plot is to remain constant. should be temperature dependent with comparable activation enthalpies. Thus one should expect to see some manifestation of temperature memory. One way to detect the presence of temperature memory would be as follows: Divide a cohort of wildetype Drosophila melanogaster into 5 groups upon emergence of the imago. The entire population should be collected in some short period, say within 2h hours. Let 3 of these groups serve as controls, one at each of three temperatures. Temperae tures of 31°C, 3h°C, and 37°C would be reasonable. Set one of the re- maining test groups at 3h°C and the other test group at 37°C. In prin~ ciple all 5 groups should be started at the same time. After some period of time, sufficient to allow for a small decline (£820%) in the test pOpulation at the highest temperature, shift the test populations at 3h°C and 37°C to the incubator housing the 31°C control group. If 76 Clarke and Maynard-Smith are correct, those flies surviving exposure to the elevated temperatures should die off as if they had been in the in- cubator at 31°C all along. Such an experiment has been performed with individually housed flies and preliminary results suggest that rather than mirroring the decline exhibited by the 31°C control group, the fur— ther expectation of life of the test group originally at 37°C is ap- proximately 1/2 that of the test group originally at 3h°C. This clearly hints at the possible presence of temperature-memory. 'One has to be careful though. Differences in adult lifespan on the order of a factor of two can be explained by differences in the temperature of the pre-imaginal environment or by differences in larval density. Flies used in the preliminary experiments were distributed among the 5 pop- ulation subgroups at random, thus minimizing the effects of differ- ences in larval crowding. Pre-imaginal environmental temperature was the same for all flies. Strehler has considered the effects of high temperature shocks of comparatively short duration (1/2 hour - 3 hours) on the subsequent 27 mortality of adult Drosophila melanogaster. His results suggest the 'presence of temperature memory as reflected by elevated mortality rates as late as 25 days after exposure to thermal shock. Similarly, Lints and Lints have explored the effects of pre-imaginal temperature on adult lifespan of the same specie, and report that adult lifespan is increased with decreasing pre-imaginal temperature.28 The three obser- vations, taken together, suggest that the question of the presence of temperature memory is still open. If the C1 rate constants are of comparable magnitude a further interesting observation can be made. In the limit where ( gt) grows 77 small Equations 103 and 108 combine to give the following expression for the mortality rate. %A(9£)== (\§<9£f:>/EK”D(<1_.Q§§?\ (133) 2: ’Y\ Y (SD—Lyd—VQX—W] \. (131+) for (9t) AA at ! which is reasonable for large n. Thus W/A (97g 2 o< Qw 8 + ,va [Org—(@0031 (135) ‘9 is the only temperature—dependent quantity on the right-hand side of Equation 135. This can be re-written as (my 3% e \<— (a Alf/g) :11: (136) where use has been made of Equation 127 replacing c with S). K 1' QWEQQN afield/(“-011 (137) The quantity (c{ZXH# ) is now an "effective" activation enthalpy and should be compared with that determined from experimental measurement. Although it gives the appearance of representing a single activated step it really is the accumulation of’cx activated processes each with an enthalpy of 11H°F. The fact that the activation enthalpies of the individual steps sum in this manner is not really the consequence of any physical or chemical characteristic of the underlying molecular processes. It is, instead, a consequence of the statistics. If the 78 apprOpriate time scale were (Ot) >7 1, use of Equations 93 and 103 would give #(it) 2 f (138) Thus the effective activation enthalpy would be smaller by a factor of 04 than that observed when (gt) is very small. The point to be ap- preciated is that very large values of effective activation enthalpy may be observed in practice and still be consistent with our general knowledge of molecular reactions. Rosenberg, et al have found that (d AH") ) ’V 190 kcal/mole over the temperature range from 25°C to 33°C, and that at - l ’”’h.5 i 6.6% over the same interval.6 Thus at AH N Bets kaA/Maa (139) from which it follows that a drOp in environmental temperature from 33°C to 25°C should produce an increase in lifespan, calculated from the equation MT23/416“) : W):— QWEZ?) 1):“1563] (1110) by a factor of approximately el'Sl' or h.53. This is fully consistent with the experimental data Rosenberg and his colleagues present. t(T2)/t(Tl) is the ratio of the time required for the population to fall to any prescribed fraction at the temperatures T2 and T1 respec- tively. So long as there are no critical temperatures for the individual steps, the plot of QM» vs l/T should be a single unbroken line with a slope of (- dAH-‘h /R). CHAPTER 8 THE COMPETITIVE MODEL There are two limitations with the simple sequential model. One is its inability to reproduce the non—linearity seen in the mor- tality rate curves at small ((gt). The other is that it does not ex- plicitly permit a link to fail at various levels of wear. If movement from one state to the next were analogous to deterioration, as would be reasonable, failure of the link should be more likely at each suc- ceeding state. In a sense this is implicit in the sequential model since movement from state j to j + 1 brings the link one step closer to failure. Both weaknesses are removed in the competitive model which is outlined schematically in Figure 15. Once again the deterioration rate constant Cj represents trans— formation of a link from state 3 to state j + l. The set of depletion rate constants% kjkare appropriate to breakage of the link at the cor- responding levels of wear. It is assumed explicitly that the rate con- stants refer to reactions on the molecular level. The ratio (kJ/cj) is assumed to increase as J increases. This is consistent with the as- sumption that breakage is more likely at more advanced stages of deter- ioration. Assuming a Markov process, as was done in Chapter 7, the set of differential-difference equations can now be written as a . \ r57; 710:) = C11 (95.169 - ((1821) (W (12.1) 79 80 Figure 15. Schematic diagram of the competitive process. Chain has n links. Allowable states are shown for a single link. The set of rate constants c. refer to deterioration. The set of rate constants {k 1 refer tonreakage of the link at the correspond- ing levels of weal. 82 subject to the same restrictions as were expressed earlier (Equations 97 and 98). Taking Laplace transforms of both sides, and solving for the general term gives £6) 2 C‘CZ'HCA-l 0/5+MM'/5+MJ“'(‘/5+MD (1112) where m = c + k . For the general case, where all the mj's are 3 J J unequal, 3 ”NA: 77 e t3<('é-*A) ls (1’45) 0 emrt ( i“ - ——-—— LC; ~tts (30C) “ e @a)‘. KAXLQ ) (1116) from which it follows that the probability of the link beingsatill unbroken is (43) (11+?) 195219391: e” A 83 where 4A 4. 4—90 — - t MU (A ‘(1 6 J m‘ (1148) Thus . ~11 "Kim C _(‘L‘IQQ ‘) :H‘ «‘t M “£92 A J (1119) M 1 from which it is clear that the slope is independent of n and goes to zero as (t A)‘9’°D . That is, the saturation effect is recovered. The behavior of the slope for small (til) depends on the value of c/ml. For c/ml < l v(tM -—-—> o (150) as (t A) -> O. This is the non-linearity for small (tA) referred to earlier. If c/ml = l - - (41m 9*") “RM : 9”“ t1. (151) btbh‘? C) 6i§sn> Q’ —\ :: Qua)»- ‘ (14:15 (152) amvo = ) (153) c/ml cannot be greater than unity by definition. Thus for kl )7 O the character of the general mortality curve is recreated. The lepe starts out at some small value, rises to a maximum, and then decreases toward zero once again. The instantaneous value of the slope is generally 811 not equal to an integral number; and this, too, is consistent with ex- perimental observation. A related example of some interest would be where successive kj's (depletion rate constants) differ by a constant multiplicative factor. This would correspond to successive activation enthalpies differing by equal increments. Unfortunately, this does not yield a closed-form solution. A computer program, capable of treating arbi— trary values for the respective rate constants has been prepared, and is presented in the Appendix with sample results. Once again, non- linearities appear for both small and large values of reduced time. The curvature and length of the approximately linear segment clearly change as the rate constants are varied. It is explicitly assumed in the computer analysis that kl < k2 < . . .(kj < . . ., and that cl = c2 = . . . = cj = . . . = c. Of coumse, wholly arbitrary values could be chosen too. If the set of deterioration rate contstants % chare assumed to be temperature independent the model would be analogous to the scheme proposed by Clarke and Maynard-Smith. The main contribution to deple- tion would come at that state for which kJ :: c3. As the temperature is increased, the ration kj/Cj increases as well; thus the principal contribution to depletion would occur at progressively earlier states. Likewise, the slope on the power law plot should decrease. As the temperature is increased further a point should quickly be reached where kl '>1j7 cl, and the survival curve should reduce to exponential form. However, no substantial change in the character of the survival curve is seen between l7.5°C and 39°C as determined by experimental measurement. Since deterioration rate constants have been assumed to 85 be temperature independent over this range, their corresponding activa- tion enthalpies must be negligibly small. Thus the measured activation enthalpy would reflect contributions coming primarily from the deple- + )k tion constants. But (AH4: )kj—l > (AH . Thus the measured activa- J tion enthalpy should increase as the temperature is increased. In the same context, one should not expect to see many conse- quences of temperature memory. Clearly the degree of wear experienced would be independent of temperature. Starting at some high temperature the prOportion of deaths by any given level of deterioration would be greater than that at a lower temperature. If the temperature is shifted to a lower value after some interval of time, the surviving population would be at the same average state of wear as a control group run for an equal period of time at the lower temperature. Thus the further ex- pectation of life of the two groups should be the same. If the deterioration rate constants are temperature dependent, the situation is different. In the computer analysis it was arbitrar- ily assumed that kj/kj l 2: 10 at 25°C. Since hi / ‘21-) = any BAT)“;- MAKER—.12) /fT (1511) the incremental difference in activation enthalpy for the depletion constants is approximately 1h.2 kcal/mole. Raising the temperature from 25°C to 39°C reduces the ratio kj/kj to approximately 3.3 with l the consequence that the number of states contributing to depletion is broadened. For the simulation presented in the Appendix, two results become apparent. First, the maximum possible lepe changes by an incre— ment of only about —1.7. This is well within the range observed exper- imentally. Second, although the number of states contributing to 86 depletion increases, the states making the predominant contribution do not change over a broad range in reduced—time. Thus an Arrhenius plot should remain essentially linear over this temperature range. Since the deterioration rate constants are temperature dependent, it neces- sarily follows that the system will reflect the existence of tempera— ture memory. The life-shortening effect of elevated temperatures is achieved primarily by the increased values for the deterioration con- stants, and secondarily by corresponding changes in the depletion constants. It is possible to find a sharp break in the Arrhenius plots with higher values of AHI' at higher temperatures and lower values of AHI‘ at lower temperatures. Consider the following situation. At some tem- perature Tl assume that only states j and j-+ l contribute significantly to depletion. If, as the temperature is increased to T the system 2, passes through the compensation temperature apprOpriate to the pro- tein molecules postulated to be involved in these reactions, then k j—l suddenly becomes larger than kj' How much larger it becomes depends on the activation enthalpies appropriate to k and k . The greater J-1 3 this difference, the sharper the break one would expect to see. The important point to recognize is that the change in measured activation enthalpy would, at least qualitatively, be in the direction suggested by Hollingsworth's experiments.29 Similar results have been obtained experimentally by Hettinger, and Rosenberg, et a1.30 CHAPTER 9 DECLINE IN VITALITY In the previous sections the concept of wear was introduced with respect to individual links. But a chain consists of several links and each can undergo deterioration before the chain breaks. The net deterioration of the chain must reflect the wear of its constituent links. There is some maximum deterioration the chain could likely sus— tain before failure would follow with virtual certainty. This would be the case, for example, if each link was in its last state prior to breakage. At the other extreme it is possible for the chain to break because one link failed although the remaining links had suffered no wear. It is natural to relate the ratio of the deterioration of the chain at some instant to the maximum possible wear it could suffer, with the fractional loss in vitality experienced by a biological organ- ism as it undergoes senescence. In practice, there really is no single measure of vitality. Instead, there are several gross parameters of physiological and bio- chemical function, and as illustrated in Figure 9, all seem to decline linearly with advancing age. By the time a human cohort has been re- duced to between 5% and 10% of its original number, the decline in these various indices is between 20% and 60%. As described earlier, a unit of damage is assumed to be sus- tained when a link undergoes a transformation from one state to the succeeding state. The molecular events dealt with then are the 87 88 rate-limiting events in loss of vitality as well as mortality. Thus both aspects of aging systems are treated here on equal footing. The extent of deterioration can be calculated by the use of generating functions.19 Let p(j;t) be the probability that a given link has sus- tained j units of damage but is still unbroken. Thus WW) ‘1 (3511650 (155) If, on the average, a link can sustain Ci.units of damage before it breaks, then the probability that the link is still unbroken by time t is simply cX-\ I“) I 1+ H8340 (156) j=c> The generating function for the damage sustained by an unbroken link is then just iii 4—( GAS): Z (3(5310§ Z (”(110 jFo =0 (157) where the subscript 1 identifies the ith link in the chain. The gener- ating function for the entire chain, G(s), is given by V\ 7\ G(S\= )\ 61(3)? [61(5)] (158) where the n links are treated as identical. The average deterioration, E[D(t)] sustained by the chain is then (5)3653]: I“ 33% (159) S): 89 and the variance in the deterioration is simply Z _. 2. V1161]: .4- G(s\+_cL . .. 1.4 6. ( As." ) 1360 85 C) (l ) sex sfl 5:( The mean fractional damage sustained by the chain is 55st (31%)] /1. (om) (1611 which reduces simply to 0(\ a1 I 8m(10=(‘,/°U) Z?) 53W) 21 199*) (162) 3.0 =0 and is explicitly independent of n. §h(t) is the negative of the frac- tional change in vitality. The length of the chain only comes into consideration in determining the time by which the cohort has been re- duced to some fraction of its original number. For the simple sequential model discussed in Chapter 7, it fol- lows that . s '1. 10(38): a {Ca/5" (163) where it has been explicitly assumed that deterioration rate constant c = c for all j. Thus J J-1 2 66/J .2 deg MA) (”3; J " ;()°°‘) (1611) is)/( jzo Clearly the second factor on the right hand side, B(t), is a fraction equal to or less than unity and must decline as (ct) increases. If n = 750 the cohort declines to between 5% and 10% of its 90 orignal number by the time ct = 2. If n = 50 the same is true by (ct) = h. Letting CK = 8, a large but not unreasonable value, it is possible to calculate the change in B(t) over a time range of practical interest. Between ct = O and ct = h, B(t) changes from 1.0 to 0.9h, a decline of only 6%. Thus for all practical purposes the fractional change in vitality, given by - gn(t), is just lieu : _— ds/a—\ (165) Thus the vitality declines linearly. The mean decline calculated from Equation 165, is -28.5% for n = 750 and -57.1% for n = 50. This com— pares with corresponding values of -28.h% and -53.6% obtained by use of the more exact expression, Equation 16h. For an intermediate value, n = 250, the mean change is -h2%. The standard deviation can be calcu- lated by use of Equation 160 and that is generally quite small (e.g., 4Eé50 : -0.h2 i .012). Thus the decline in vitality is linear over the range of interest and for reasonable values of n, yields percent declines within the range of experimental observation. In the competitive model the exact results depend, of course, on the values chosen for the respective rate constants. To show that a linear result can be obtained, and that the calculated changes in vitality fall within reasonable limits one need only refer to the com- puter simulation. For the case where 07 = k7, kj/kj—l = 10, and Ci: 7, the ratio of the damage (deterioration) sustained increases by a factor of approximately 2.81 between ct = 1 and ct = 3. The depar- ture from strict linearity is thus approximately 6.3%. Since for n j; 52, Fn(3)f§ .05, it is safe to say that over a reasonable range both in n and ct, the decline in vitality is linear. By ct = 3 the 91 decline is approximately h6.6%, a reasonable value. Using this approach, the decline in vitality and the kinetic results for mortality are treated on equal footing. One does not have to be assumed for the other to be derived as has been the case in 3 earlier quantitative treatments. CHAPTER 10 CONCLUSIONS Examination of probabilistic molecular clocks suggest that they may be used to account for the kinetics of mortality of multicellular organisms, and permit the effects of temperature to be introduced in a natural manner. Three mechanisms are proposed by which such an aging clock could conceivably function. While this does not rule out more complex processes, the simple mechanisms serve to suggest that only a very small number of events likely serve as rate—limiting factor(s) in senescence. This is summarized by the {galaw which suggests that if the number of such steps is small the spread in age at death will be broad. This is consistent with empirical findings. The rate-limiting step(s) thus determine the characteristic lifespan of biological organ- isms and may serve to initiate amplification processes which lead to the proximate causes of death. The time scale for amplification is likely much more rapid than that appropriate for the occurrence of the initiating events themselves. Comments on the character of amplifica- tion processes are beyond the sc0pe of this paper, but interesting re— marks have been made by Orgel both for the senescence of whole organ- isms as well as for the demise of cells grown in culture.31 It is im- jportant to recognize, however, that even when large fractions of cells or organ system constituents are required to malfunction certain tem- jporal aspects of the organism's response are governed by the small num~ 'ber of events which initiate amplification. Care should also be taken 92 93 to distinguish steps which initiate amplification from primary events whose occurrence may serve to make senescence inevitable. One specific mechanism upon which a probabilistic aging clock could function is as follows: From some large set of posSible events occurring in random order, some small number are required to occur without preference shown to any specific subset. If only one such event is needed two interesting limit distributions are obtained. The distributions are interesting specifically because they yield Gompertz law and the power law, the two simple kinetic expressions previously de— rived from empirical studies. The virtue of such an analysis is that it is essentially model-independent, and it reveals that Observed kinetic results have their fundamental explanation in rather general probabilistic considerations. The results are not intrinsically bio- logical in character. An earlier mathematical treatment by Sacher and 32 Trucco which lead to Gompertz law is thus seen to be just one of many plausible mechanisms when judged solely against kinetic data. ReCOgnizing these limitations two specific models are prOposed which treat kinetic and thermodynamic considerations in a more demand- ing fashion. The sequential and competitive models contain component steps consistent in character with the thermal denaturation of proteins. Both models are consistent with kinetic studies conducted at constant temperature and provide for the dramatic life shortening effect ele— vated temperature has on poikilotherms. The presence or absence of temperature memory is determined, respectively, by whether or not the deterioration rate constants are themselves temperature dependent. Ex— perimental resolution of this point would be of considerable interest. In the absence of temperature memory the effect of decreasing core body 9h temperature would be predominantly to extend that portion of life most apprOpriate to senility. The competitive model reproduces non-linear- ities seen in the mortality rate curve both for small and large values of time. The sequential model only provides for the latter result. The competitive model appears more realistic in its assumptions and is thus more appealing. It also has the virtue of being able to account for the existence of critical temperatures. High values of activation enthalpy would be apparent at high temperatures, and low values of activation would be effective at low temperatures. Such a phenomena is beyond the SCOpe of the sequential model as described herein. Both the sequential and competitive models provide for linear decline in physiological vi- tality and treat this variable on an equal footing with mortality. APPENDIX APPENDIX In the discussion of the competitive model (see Chapter 8) it was remarked that the equation “Q”! C}: mvt it“): C‘C2"'CA-) Z 3 (1)13) 3311’ (“u-"‘8 =1 =%v could not generally be expressed in closed—form. pj(t) is understood r:\ to be the probability (density) that a given link is in state j at time t. m = c + k.. cj is the deterioration rate constant corres— J J J ponding to transformation of the link from state j to state j + 1. k3, on the other hand, is the depletion rate constant appropriate for breakage of the link at the corresponding level of wear. The probabil- ity that a given link is unbroken by time t is given by summing pJ(t) over the allowed states and is denoted by 17(t) in the text. A computer program has been written, based on Equation 1&3, to evaluate pJ(t) at arbitrary values of time for arbitrary sets of rate constantsqacjls and )mjk. The values of these rate constants are read by the computer and are denoted in the program by the symbols C(J) and M(J), respectively. The number of allowed states for a given link is denoted by K and is read by the computer along with the values of time, T, at which the dependent variable is to be evaluated. The latter ap— pears in the program as P(J). DP(J) and DDP(J) correspond, respectively, to the first and second derivatives of pj(t) with respect to time. The 95 96 sum of terms P(J) over the allowed states j = l, 2, . . . K is denoted in the computer program by F and is evaluated at each selected value of time. DF and DDF correspond respectively to the first and second deriv- atives of :F(t) with respect to time. Lastly, the lepe v(t) is calcur lated according to Equation 107 and is designated by the symbol SLP. The computer program appears on the following page. Sample results, obtained on a CDC 6500 computer, are presented in Tables 2 through 5. For comparative purposes the deterioration rate constants are adjusted to unity. It is arbitrarily assumed that at 25°C the ratio kj/k _ : 10 for all allowed values of j. Assuming j 1 Tc == 325°K, it follows from Equation 15h that consecutively numbered depletion rate constants have activation enthalpies which differ by 1h.2 kcal/mole. At 39°C the ratio kj/kj-l becomes 3.3. In Tables 2 and 3, depletion and deterioration rate constants become equal at state 6. In Tables 9 and 5 these same constants do not become equal until state 7. Tables 2 and h refer to computer simulations at 25°C while Tables 3 and 5 refer to computer simulation at 39°C. Results are discussed in the main text. 97 --'-:—'r .w:»— s FonMAT(I3) . PRINT 6 , '6 FORMAT(* *) e PRE(1)=1 r - 00 100 J=1.K ‘ * READ 25. C(J)9M(J) ‘ as F09MAT(010.4.010.4) ... pRE(J+1)=PRE(J)*C(J) f‘i'fh'f‘_. 100 CONTINUE - ~ }fi.1 PRINT 35 .I’f?¥ififa K 35 FOQMAT(* J *.8X.*P(J)*o16X9*DP(J)*.14x,§oop(J)§,, J“ a 010x.«C(J)*.8x.*M(J)*) is 10] READ 75. T 5’ i 75 FODMAT(D9.3) a, 9'2», : IF(T)5000.30.30 - v 30 00 2000 J=1.K ' SUM=0 5 05uw=0 DDSUM=O P0 00 1000 L=19J .‘ ML=M(L) . PPOD=1 20 00 202 1:1.J DIFF= M(I)~ML IF(01FF)201.202.201 201 PPOD=PpoD*DIFF .- 202 CONTINUE -. 1: Q = -(ML*T) c1 0N=DEXP(O) ~ 1. ” TEQM(L)=DN/PROD ' ~ -. DTEPM(L)=TERM(L)*(-ML) <1:f.:§ra;:1 DDTERM(L)=TERM(L)*(ML**2) , SUM=SUM+TEPM(L) ’ DSUM=DSUM+DTERM(L) DDGUM=DDSUM+DDTERM(L) 1000 CONTINUE PRINT 6 P(J)=PRE(J)*SUM DP(J)=PRE(J)*DSUM DDD(J)=PRE(J)*DDSUM PRINT 859 J9 P(J). DP(J). DDP(J)- C(J), 89 F0RMAT(I39 5X9 010.30 IOX9 010.39 10X9 €011.59 2X9 011.5) 2000 CONTINUE PRINT 6.‘ _ , A iPRINT-4§“_" , h ,1 , 0 98 3 4R FOQMAT(4X0*T*o14Xo*F*914X9*OF*0l?X9*DDF*912X9*SLP*) 1 F=0 " 0F=0 ;‘ 00F=0 DO 3000 J:19K F=F+D(J) OF=0F+OP(J) DOF=ODF+OOP(J) 3000 CONTINUE SLP=T*(DDF/DF-DF/F) DPTNT 959 T9F9DFQDDFQSLp 9% FODMAT(010.395X9010.305X9 010.305X-DIO.395X9010.3) GO TO 101 5000 CONTINUE " Fm 99 I ,—..-. ..~ ‘- _o+cc~o0wu ~c+c~cco~o ~o+occooa. mo+c~ccc~. ¢o+oonoono .rc+ooc—o~o W h , 1.. mo+eoco-. ~o+cocccmo ~o+ooco-. ~o+occ~c_. ~¢+oc~oo~. ~c+o~coc~. ~o+ccocc—. .fivz . I wg.¢ccoc~r ~o+ceoco~. ~o+coooo~. ~o0Cocoo QJm _o+cCcoc~. _o+Ccooc~. ~o+¢ccco~. ~c+Coooo_. ~o+Ccooo~. ~o+coooo~. _o+cCcoc_. ~o+cOooo~. ~o.¢ccoc~. ~0+Ccooo~. 2J0 om+cmm~. ~o+cm>~.| oo+cmoo. ¢ciommool Lac o~3:_m¢. mmlo¢m¢o N~Icox©o ooucmo¢. chCdc~o ¢oncmo¢. mouecao. oo+ao>oo ~c+c>o~.u oo+cooo. ZJQQC DOWN " _H_ «00 '0 Houaocm. co+c¢~a. cc+omoo. ¢o|coo~.| no mmlommc. mfitcmmh. ¢_tcm—~. mnlcocm. Gotcafid. cclcmcn. ¢CIQm0¢o molcmaoo co+Como. co+oooo.n .fivac mo-cmm¢w Holcmoo. oc+cmco. _o+c¢od. u mmnc¢m©. Hmocmoo. N~OCNNH. 3H-cmrfl. mfiic¢mmo ooucm_¢. {alcmcfio coucmo¢. moICooo. oo+Coaoo Afiva Hmdoa 0>Hpfipwmaoo Mom nofipwddafim AmpsmEoo detaccfi. b o~ a .de38 mo.¢co~o_. _o+cccoc~u .-, -mo-ooo¢. coucorwJ V . ‘ - “ensemo. q .i: -, 100 no+ooc~o—. uoncmdm. ~c+oooo-. ~c+coeoofiw.p mauraorcummx_. . .4. .iedbmmcwwu , .; .moneomo. N _¢+oocco~. Ho.cqm00mp mouommm. - monopmfi. mouc¢qd. ~o+ooco-. _o.ccooo.. “ouc~fim. Houooofi. Noncomg. ~o+occ_o_. _o.Ccooo_. oo+o¢c~. Houofimc. “ouccm~. fio.co~oo~. _o.cccoom. _o-com~. oo+o~um. Aoucmms. ._o+o_ooo_. Ho.ccooo_. oo.oo~o.u oo+emom. oo.cmom. ”Ho.0¢coc~. _o.c°ooo_. oo+o>oc. oo.c~oc. oo+c~co. W oo.ooom. mouoflmm.- ¢c-c¢¢m.u ~o+cco~. y an use .5 ame+daooo~. Ho+co¢co~¢ aguoooaa oyuapmm. mfiucwmo. _¢o+oo~oo~. _o.ccocc~. ofiuooom. m~-ooam. ¢~-c¢mo. _ro+ooofio_. _o.cococg. sc-omom. ooneooc. -oc¢¢c. Wmc+cooo~_. _o.coooofi. moucmom. so-o~«c. .mo-amo~. wfio+acoocm. ~o+¢oooo~. moneomfi. mcucficm. so-co¢~. H~o+acccfi_. ~o+¢oooo~. ~o-¢-¢. mo-o>¢fi. moucorm. a _c.coooo~. Nonc~m¢. moucfimfi. A.u.pgoov m magma 101 mo+.o~coc~. H¢o+oo~oo~. m@.0oc~o_. No+ocoo-. ~o+ccooo~. ~o+occcc~. ~o+Cocoo~. ~o+cooco~. ~O¢coooc~. —o+coooo~. ~o+Ccooo~. ~c+cccoo~o ~o+ccooo~. ~o+oocoo_. meoommm. aJm Ho+coocc~. Ho+cooco—. - coo“. hcnooom. #cnoooa. moueqom. gotoo¢N. delecMN. “clamoo. oo+o¢m~.l oo+omcm.l oo+omcm. Nelooom.l nae mglcom¢o «elabmco mfiuramo. melcmmc. melo¢mo. moICOFN. monommo. ~o|0>¢¢. oo+omm~. oo+c¢a~. «enamom. oo+cmcm.l Molc¢nool ac mfilc¢mco oolo¢mco Nfiloocm. 4“ mcuooofi. moucfiofi. m mo-coe~. NJ mo-o>mm. c? “oncom~. mm floucfifio. ¢ oo+c¢m~. m1 oo+ccom. m oo+cmcm. H _o+coo~. oo+¢ccm. u » ¢~ncm~o. o: odncm_o. ofl A.U.pnoov N mHDdB =~o+ooco-._ Hoocccoomn fifio+coc~c~. . 102 _o+oc~oo~. Ho’oflooo_. Howcoooo~. c mowo~ooo_. ¢o.oo_oo_4 .wo+ooc~c~. .m¢+accc~_. ~c+oococm. ~o+eooo-. .OJoocfiofi. .o+oo~oo~. ~c+o~ocouo 1". .04Ccobcas moocooooy. ~o+Cococ~. ~o+ccooc—. ~o+cccooHo ~o+ccooofi. ~o+om~m. aJm ~o.¢ccoo~. ~o.:¢ooc~. fiowcocoofi. ne.coooc_. ~o+c°coo_. ~o.¢oooofi. ~o+¢oooo~. ~o+coooo~. HO¢coooo~. Holcccuol ~otcc¢m.l ~o|c~¢h.l ~o|0m¢mol Helcxoao ~olcao¢. “clomhh.l use ”fluorcfi. honored. ¢c-cmc~. moucwcfi. fio-com~. mouaocm.u Monomoo.u oo+cmm~.- eclooauo Motom~©.I ~OIC>¢¢.u [oncomm.u douommfi. Houcccm. Honommd. fiolom~hol moo “filohmaol NOICNN~.I ¢OIc>maol melc~m~.l «ano¢__. mc-om_~. Ho-omo~. mouooxm.u ficuomm¢.- ~¢-omm>.u gonooem.u Houomm¢.u oo+co¢~ol kc #Huomom. ucnomor. ¢ouewcr. welco~ro -Icw¢h. hotcmch. ¢3I30¢No Notabmh. ~oIC¢¢m. oo+co>~. oo+cmo~. 00§o©¢~o ~oucmm>c Molommao oo+Co~¢o u fialoomm. bctcomm. concomm. Notcmrm. Ho+ooom._ A.U.pqoov N wands 10h ‘~o+cofioc~. ~o+c~oo¢~. ~o+0ccco~. mo+e~coo~. ¢c+Oo~oo~. mo+aoo~c—. Ne+ooco~ao ~c.:oqad;. "c.c°ooo~. _o+c°ooo~. ~o+com~. ugm po+cCooo~. ~o+coooo~. ~o+Coooc~. ~n«cdooo~6 ~o+Oooco—. ~o+Coooo~. —o+coccc~. ~o+coooo~o no+oocoo~o ~o+cooooao ~a+omm_. ugm mclco¢~ mouQmom. ¢0Io¢mco donocmm! kCo -Ic¢¢~.l houc¢¢uoa ¢OUQ¢¢~ol Nolcm¢~ol ~otQ¢¢~ol ~OIombaou mono¢wm. Honccmmo ”clamom. Nelc¢no. ~OIom~N.I moo Nonc~¢H.I molcmo¢ol ¢0Io¢mcol oo+cmm~.l uo N~Icmmh.l molomm>ol monommbol mOICthol Nolcooo.l Honom¢m.l ~CIemmmou Helomomol Hancocmot melo¢hc.t oo.ommfiol no awn NJ .wwmcfa . nnmw. mouo¢m¢. &.%mm aouc¢m¢. .xwx oo+om_m. Ho.coom. u k m --c~o~. o“ poucficfi. o aoncmcs. m moucmcs. s ”cucumm. c oo+cmm~. m oo.cmm_. ¢ fiouo~¢m. m flouCNmm. m, mcocqso. _ oo.cmo~. “o.ooo¢. u .h I .. A.o.pnoov m wands 105 rmo+occ_c~. mo+cocc-. Ao+¢ocoom. Fc+cccc-. ~c+Ccc~o~. ~c+c¢~oo~. ~o+c~ooc~. ~o+occco~. mc+o~coo~. ¢o+oo~cc~. rc+ecc_o~. mc+cocc_~. _o+oocccm. ~o+cooo-. fic+ooc~cHo ~o+coccc~. ~o+cccoo~.v fic‘ccccoH. ~o+cococ~. _o+5ccoc~. —c+Cocoo~. ~o+cccoo~o ~c¢cccoofi. cc+ccmm. aqm ~o+¢cooo~. ~c+coccc.. _o+cococ~. ~O¢coooc~. ~o+cccoc~. ~0+Coooc_. ~o+coooc_. (oncoOH. :ou:o0~. mcuc>ofi. moucccm. nonc¢o~. ¢cvc¢mm. metabom. couscom. ~cl0~¢~. use N~Ic¢om. molc¢omo methom. mouccom. Noncomm. moicmmm. monom¢mo coacmcm.n ¢oucocm.l moucsbm.n Moucxur.u mcncmm~.u qouchom.n melommq.l conccom.u Houcorm.u no mnucqcc.l melc¢ccol mclcmcc.l melcmocol mctemrc.0 Noncowo.n mcuc-m.l Concmim. :oIC(cm. moucc~m. moucmbq. rolcmcfi. qoucm¢m. moncmm¢. colccom. molo¢hm. m fifluoqod. wolc¢c~. qonc¢c~. Noucacfl. yc-c0c~. Hoocmwa. nocccmn. mo+Ccc~. b o~ A.v.pcoov m mange 106 oc+cmmm. mcuocfio. mo-cmmm.u mo-ooofi. mo+cem~. a4m use kc u » hmo+cficco~. fio.cococ~.‘ mfiuocofl. m~-ax¢m.- mdncmcm. o” .¢c+co~oc~o ~o+cocoo~o OOIGOOM. Gelcmwm.l Gotcmfim. 0 I1 A.u.pqoov m manwe 107 Ho+ce~mo~. ~c+e~moo~. ~c+c¢mocpo mo+ocmmm_. NC+caco~m. «cracc~m_. Hc+cComm¢. ~c+ococom. Ho+oocom~. fic.occoo~. ~c+ec~mc~. ~o+o~¢oo~. ~o+c¢moc~. .fiv: ~o+¢cooo~. _o+ccooo~. ~c+¢ccco_. ficucmmm. a4m _o.coooo~. Fo.ccooo~. #o.cOcoo#. ~o+¢ococ_y .o+cocco_. ~o+cCooo~. .o.coooofi. ~o+cCcco~. _o+cCooc_. .o+:ocoo_. ._,VU oo+cmmn. floucmwm. molcmmq. m Hc+crpd.r co+cm~a. ~o-c¢co. m cO¢cooo. oc+2>cool co+cmco. ~ . mcuocmm.u moccoqm.u ~6+C¢o~. chcocfi. mac me u k w~tcom~. omucmom. mmIcCrm. om «_ucwmfi. nfluc¢q~. omncmmm. c m~-o~bm. ¢_-c¢«~. .--cmoa. m ocuc>o¢. mfilcnwm. aduCNMH. h cotc¢c~. ,0cIoH~¢. mmacmmr. c ¢cnc~o¢. oclcqcfi. Gotcm~¢. m Nonficmo. .qouomoq. (oncmwfi. q oo+coho. mcncmdo. doncmc¢. m _c+cmo~.| oc+¢oa0w menacoo. m oc+cmoo. oo+emoc.l oo+coco. H A:.aoc Awyac 0 Any; 7 CA x comm n B .00 u ox Hmdoa m>fipwpmmaoo h0% COflpdeeflm Ampdeoo .m mange 108 .uhw ,Hmsfi 3r.. . ...A.»,~..... % u» ”S wwwag.._qm~. . .. . .a .5.. KP. «- . . “c.9qeoo.. . mo+oommmfi. mo+ooao>m. mc+ooo~N~. ~c+ooomm¢. ~o+ooooo~. ~o+oooom~. “c.0ooocfi. #01...»- ml ¢2L$¥.. n ?...z , X03. .a.x4..‘.t...r \ v c9aaocpa.._ ..c.eococ~. ( 5 wa.re C ~c+cocccuo ~o+ccccc~.,w 4 go.ooocc~. oo+cmmm. QAm ~o+¢oooo~. Hoocoooon. ~o+Cccocmo ~0+Ccooo~. ~o+¢oooc~. ~o+¢oooo~. ~o+coooo_. 5%. w. ...; 1. gal ommeoJ goqwnmmflq °o¢d-o.nvn cc.oooot Nolo¢~hoi use -no>¢m. oolc¢moo souomom. moncomm. mouoamfi. match~¢. yoncofim. , . monomofi. .gcnom~o. cq.omMm. oc.c~om. cooOfioc.l moucson. no m_uo¢—m. “_uoflmo. oouccoo. sonemmo. monoomm. mouom¢~. NCJOWM+~0 mc-ofim~. , m douc¢m~. ¢# Ho-cmm~. m oo.omom. m oo.oooc. H ~o.coo~. oc+ecc~. L k mfincmmo. o“ m~ucmm~. o educmqfi. a motcmpd. h souccmn. c moac¢~m. m“ mo-cOmH. ¢m A6130"; m edema 5/// 109 ~°.oceom~. .°.cooo°~. ._o.oopm°~. ~m.b~moo~. ao.aamoc~. _ . _mo+oommm~. _mc.oooo>m. A. ’. .l. I Ac+0ococuo E w. .c...~u¥ _owceoccfi. ~°+o°°oo~. .o.ooooo~. _o.c°ooo~. yo.cccoo_. _c.cooco~. _c.Co¢oo.. ~o+om—~. QJw ~o+coooo~. no+cocoo~. gm¢ol wane ~°-awmmr motomooo ...}.MYI . monoesmo L malocmno aouOFNN. gotcmmo. flcloomm. oo+ocmfi.l oo+c¢cm.h oo+ooom9 _olo¢fiacl uco melommn. coIC—mo. Noncomfl. mo|c~o~. ¢clom0h. Novc¢mm. meloamo. Houo-¢. oo+o-—. oo+oo~_. molomo_.l oo+omcm.l Notcghhol uc Oclcmrp. scIQONO. pameMm ? do.o~oo u welcmom. coacmqm. ¢ouc¢(~. moucmhmo meic¢¢m. fio-c~¢~. flouc¢om. oc+cmm~. oc+cccmo co+c>cmo oo+emoo. u ofilcmom. onCbcm. N w." oc+ooomo k cm 0. !h1r A .c . paoov m 93.3. llO po+occocm. .~o+Ccccr~o Ho.occoo~. ~o+ccsmc~. fic.o~¢oo.. ~o+o¢moc~. ro+ocmmm~. mo+cOcowm. mo+ccc~N—o —c+00cmm¢. ~o+occocm. fio+ocoom~. ~c+CcoOo—. ~c+Co>Ncno ~o+o~moo~. _o+ccccc~. ~c+CccocH. ~o+cccoo~. ~o+¢cooo~. ~o+cccoo~. yo.ooooo~. ~c+ohcmo agm ~c+Ccooo~. ~c+Ccooo~. —o+ccooo~. ~c+Coooo—. —c+Cocoo~. ~o+ccooo—. ~o+coooc~o _o+cOooo~. —o+cooco~. Hancocm.u ~cucco¢.| Honcwnc.a “oucmmm.u ~9thom. ~0ICNJ¢o ~OIocmmou urzb noucomn. metabmo. mocccdm. Nonc0£m. _oncfiofi. ~oucm-ou doncmom.u oo+cmm~oi Noncm¢~. Q Honcflmm. ficnomrm. moncmrc. fioncc¢h. ficncxmo. floucmoq. AoICCNmol uc bolcmmoo «onommfi. mouo¢>¢. Notebcc. ficncoom. HonCHQF. ~CIc¢o>. mcucomm.L cc+cmr~.u ~91Corm. oo+c~wfi. oo+c¢cm. oo+cmfiw. oo+c~qa. ~ch¢U¢o co+Co¢o. woncdqm. molccmh. motesxm. moucmom. Houcmmm. ~c-¢m¢~. cc+coc~. oc+c¢©mo oo+cmcmo ~c+Cccmo » CH A.u.pq00v m magma lll rc.oommm_. pmo.occo~m. mo.ccc_m~. _c+cccmm¢. do.eccccm. Ho.occcm_. #o.oo¢oc~. _o.oc»mo~. _o.c~mco~. ~o+c¢moc—o mo+Ccmmm~o mc+occ0bm. .mo+2co~m_. gac+accmm¢w Ho.ecmg. 04m ~c+Coooo~. ~o+coooo_. _c+cccoo_. ~o+¢ocoo_. ~o+Cccoo_. po+cococ~. ~o+cococ.. ~o+¢coco~. ~o+cococ_. ~c+Ccooc.. ~c+oo>~. a;m ~o+ccooo~. ~o+coocc~o ~0+Cocoo_. ~o+coooo~. mouo¢¢q.u ucA. solemmm.u «enamo~.u mou:mom.u moncfimdoi ~3IC¢ON.I gouc~¢m.| doscnm~.u ficncmo~o Nonc¢cm. doucmmd. ~cto>9mol LDC soucwhm.u moncqn¢.| mcncmo~.u Nonchhmol oc+coofl.n uc scuzcaq. mcncoom. mot:m_m. Mcncm~m. mo|c¢cq. dcnco~m.l Hothadol ficscq~>.u fionomqm.l ficncmmfi.l oc+:¢©~.l no eonsm — H . ¢OICOd~o motcmqm. mcso¢¢c. =o+c~m¢. u celcqcm. ¢oucomm. moncqnfi. Holcmma. ~o|cmmc. co+cfi¢~. oo+cm-. oo+corfi. flouc-~. Houcfiqd. co+cm~mo m conc>nd. colcofimo moucq¢¢. ~o’cccH. ~c.¢co¢. c~ O ~c+Cccmo b ca A.u.paoov m mange 112 x~c+cCcOc—. ~c+0c#mc—. macwc~aoc~. ,Hc.c¢moc_. ro+ccmmm_. mc+2oco~m. mo+Cco~m~. ~o+Cccmmq. H¢.cococu. wo+¢¢ccm~. ~c+oco0cmo a~c+ec~mcfio fio.o~moc~. ~c+o¢moc~o pl». hrl'f .I v ~o+cccoc_. _o+Cococ.. po.5coco~. ~o.:ccco~. ~c+ohc~o agm ~o+Cccoo—. ~o+¢ocoo~. goococoo—. ~c+Cooco_. ~o+Cccoo~. ~o.:coco~. ~c.Cccoc~. —o+Coooc~. Po+c:ooc—. ~c+ccocc~o mouccam. melcmm~. NCICOJM. doucm¢¢o ~cno~¢mo use honcmwm.u mouc¢o~.u moucmem.l mcuccom.i ~oucro~.| moucmco.u «o'coms. ~ouammm. _onooom. Noncoco. monoco¢.n mon2m¢~.u monsoom.u ¢oncqq¢.n oo+co-.I u: noncmcn.u mcucorr.: Mclcmr~.t .Nolcca~.I HoncO_~.u doucqnm.u achNHm.u floucmq¢.| poncqcm.n molc~£¢.I metabum. molcfiom. molcfimq. ¢oucm¢¢. co+C>Mqo u concmnm. concmmm. monCNAH. ~otcmmH. _ouca¢o. co+c~_a. oc+Con~. Houc~o~. floucmmm. molcocoo Hc+Cccm. h CH A.o.pcoov m magma ll3 . w . . ., mo.cCc~m~. fio+cccmm<. ~o+£ccccm. m0.¢¢ccm_. #c.cCcoc~. ~c+oc>mc~o 4c.cgmoc_. Hc+o¢mcc—o mc+acrmm~o uo.coco>m. mmc.cCc_m_. ~c+3ccmm¢. Hc+cCccom. 2.5.2.2.. ~o+¢ccoo~. ~0¢Ccooc~. ~c.ccooo_. _o+Ccocc~. ~o+cocoo~. ~o+Ccooo~. ~o¢coooc~. ~oocooocfi. ococmq¢o QJU ~o+Cccoc~. _o.ccooc~. _o+Ccocc~. Ho+Cccco~. ~o+Ccooo_. _c+cccoo~. mo-cwwm. ¢cuc~om. mcucmofi. mc5c¢m~. ¢onc>mn. .oncmflm. moucmsm. eclccow. ficlco-. ucc hoICO-. melccmfi. ¢oucoom. molcmom. moncmmm. metacmm. melcficm.l ¢cucxar.u monoomH.I rotamsfi.l ¢cuc~co.u qcuofimm.n mcucqom.n eolccomot Holcmkaol uc Noncmem.u mc13#mm.l ¢cucoro.u Noncoo~.l mono-¢.u «CICOGm.I moncmc¢. qotCocm. moncxmfi. molc>_m. molCOcH. qolccqm. molCJAJ. coucmom. Ho52mmm. u Noncmrm. molfNoto moICNUH. mQICqsfl. NoICC¢o. melcdrxo A.o.pcoov m magma nc+CcoH. k CH oc.cmom. monocsm._ mo-oqh¢.u QJM uoo no ~o.:oooo_4 ocucocm. oo-oom>.n .fic.ccoco~. No-9.mo. Foncofio.u hr .. .Pnll L. v F”? 1'11“ tlx r III-I'll melcmom. NC¢ComH. u » Gotcfibo. c co-co-. .o. A .P as": m 033. 115 ..51, ao.ccooo_. Ho.om>~.- oc.o¢fiw. -.- chmwmmJ.. H.“ wo.ooooo_.. oo.nmoo. . oo.omoo.u oc.omoo..uw;nmmm‘ _ouooco. ...:wCuommw.u 5. mouooofi.- _c.cao~. _c coed. a4m use no n P _ . .o.ccocofi. mmgcmmo. mmuempg. mm-corm.- fia ¢croo~mc~. Ho.ccoco~. mfiuom¢o. m_-o~c~. o_-c.¢~.- on mc.¢o¢~o~. _c.cccao~. «H-cflm_. e.-o~c_. o~-cc_m. o mo.oooc~_. ~o.coooo~. mfinomdm. ¢H-o~cm. cgccomm. av Ho.ocooom. “o.coooo~. oo-oc~¢. m~uooofi. mducmo~. p .o.ocoo-. flo.aocacfi. ooncch. acuoHH¢. m~uccam. on .fio.eoc~o~. ~o.coooo_. .qo-omo¢. ecncmefi. ooucm~¢. m. .Hc.mo.oo_.. ~c.cooco~. moucoqo. ¢ouomoc. co-cmc~. «W “owpdooog. ~o.¢oooo~. oo.co~o. mouommo. co-cmo¢. m— .fiqwacooo_. ~o.¢ooco~. Ho.e~o~.- oo.oomo. No-oooo. mm _mc.ccoco~. ~o.¢oooo~. co.oooo. oo.oooo. ooycooo. an , .3.: .w.o vaaoo .w.aa .nva A L. ! .. ..-.l - . .. 5.. -.5 - I - ...r.. F p comm n B . o h. M Mecca m>HpHpomaoo no.“ GOHHmHszm .Hmpzmaoo. .: wands 116 H ppLoMoe.- » . &o¢~.u ...n no Q 4 afiwomwo. ....zhyh. .. .fiuooyw. .»¢nomms. mewooem. mc-o~¢H. aolooom. Holcmmho oo+cmom. m oc.o~oo. fl ~o.¢oo~. oc.ooo~. u k owlcwmwo fig c~1Qm¢m. o~ mfinosoo. om ofiuCFmd. a. mouc¢mfi. n. scucmmh. @— moucssm. m‘ monofiwg. ¢L moucmm¢. m. A.u.pqoov : wagua 117 _qo.mchofi. Wmo+cCcH¢_. Wmo.occc-. .AC.¢¢cocm. fiovgccodfi. “c.0cc~o~. ~o.ac~co~. Ho+c~ooc~. ,yc.cccoo~. ~c+Coooo_. .m¢+:~ccc~. «c.oc~oo~. .mo.eoc~o~. xmo+cooo_~. ,yc.ccoocm. 5:31. ~o+Cocoo~. ~o+cccco~o ~o+Cocoo~o _owccooon. ~o+ceooo_. ~o+cooco~. —o+cCcoo~. fioococccH. ~oococoo_. —o+cocoo~. ~o+oocmo QJm ~o+cccoo~. ~0+Coooo~. ~c+coooo~. Moocccoofi. ~c¢ccooo—o . molec¢m. molcm¢m. molcmcm. Noncmhw. fionc>mm. Aolcmch. _OICNHO. oo+o¢m~.l oo+c¢¢m.n co+cqcm. melo~dmol uCC m~IChcm. OOICNcm. concmhm. ¢oICHNc. monoc-. cacuamrfl.. molcmrfio monomdfi. Nonem~m. AGIConfi. “ouomwc. oc+ommfi. oo+ccmfi. meiemomo oc+omcmo qolcmmh.l no ¢~Ioficm. OHIQhem. NOICMPm. welcome. monommdo ooucmmms colcqmm. ¢o|co>m. noncor¢. Notcficm. Holcmm~. fioncm—o. oo+c¢ma. co+cmcm. oo+cm¢m. _o+:oo~. u J#ICC¢¢. Mdlcdcq. aol:>¢¢. oclcomm. ¢oIcmm~. ,o— oc+ocom. - CH A.w_paouv J mapwe 118 . o u ~o+cococ~. Uo+c~ooo~. «o.oo~oo_. mc+coc~o~. mo+occo-. _o.cCccom. _o.occc-. _c.ooc_o~. fio.oc_oc~. _o.o~ooo~. ~c+¢ccoc~o ~c+coooc~o mc+o~ooo~. ’IR I ..l. .Ir.ua.I-l.|. ~0+Coooo~1 ~o+o¢cm. agm ~o+Cccoc~. _o+coooo~. _c+cCooo~. _o+c:ooo~. —o+Cooco~. _o+cOooo~. ~0+Coooo_o ~o+¢ccoo~. ~o+cocoo~. _o+Cocoo~. ~0+Coccc~. ~c+c~m¢. nqm ~o+Coooo~. _ouemo¢.. ~le~¢m.l mac -ucmc~. soucmofi. «oucmcfi. Noncmc~. ~oucoxd. fioaaomm. moucmom.l moucmoo.u oo+cmm~.l moucoqfi. co+cmm~. melocqmol uCC malcc¢mo ~cucmo¢.l #cucmfim.u no -Iommfi. houcmra. «ousmmfi. moucmrfi. flouomcfi. ~olcofiw. ~cnocca. Holcfioo. ¢leoo—.I oc+cmr~.l oc+cmr~ol mcncsm~.u u: . m~lcmmH. fielcmo¢o oo+c~oo. m mfiuccpo. menacnc. molcmwco Moucmoo. monsoco. fioncm¢m. flocoaom. oo+com~. oo+c-m. oo+CH~w. oo+cmm~. ~o+coo~o u malcmwm. A.u.pcoov : magma H ~c+ccom. h ~H oH ~c+0cc~. » fig ll9 ~o.¢¢cgofi. ~o+oq~aa4. :fio+e~coo—v RAC§rooc~. .mo.oocccuz To 3 _i. at” 37.1. $232. N¢.ca~¢o_. mm»noo.~o~. m@.oboc-. wqwmcooom. wwroooo~_. c¢boo_o~. 5:9ogcofi. gm.ofiaoo~. .‘V ~o.o¢¢oom. “as mo.coboo~. .~o+cccoo~4. . M~o+cocoo~. ~c.coooo~.. ..fi ..—o+¢~¢mo aam ~o.noooo_r #¢.coooc_. ~o.¢oooofi. ~o.cocoe~. H¢.ccocc~. floocoooo~. ~o.¢ocoo~. fioycocoofi. _o.ecooo~. “o.coopp». {.5 «I ..l‘ ‘.II‘II ’ .x v :Hew ceum.n..,& m ‘ . . ..A a.~.©\.omm~o.x * #5.. W... dewmwomuw awrommfi. ~ono¢¢o.u nae m—Io-m. malo#~m. melccgm. molcmom. NOIQNMM. McIcmmH.I doIQomm.I HOIco¢hol “oIoo¢N.I no. cmo¢. 1 . 5...... .. pr...» .... .5 ‘l. 5 I . Sgt. Mol omm¢ct .... :Jr . we omm¢. ~OI omwh. douoocm.u Helomm~ol Monommh. no -Ichmmo Nelohmm. :clohmm. welcocm. welonomo "Cleo—co “clommm. ¢OICNNQ.I fielofi¢h.I acloooooI oo.omog oo.a~¢~. "cICMFN. HOIQmmH. oo+cm¢o. u Mfiucmfim. NOICNBN. :ouompm. moucmpm. .doucmrm. yoncmmo. oo.c~o~. oo.c¢«m. oo.c¢mm. oo+coq~. m0... x3..\.W . m fl ~c.oocm. h “A o“ o m ed m.“ ¢ m. .m, A.v.paoov : wands 120 mc+aoa¢dd. Horocccam. “mo.occo-. r u .. . ”fia}ooofla~. .flwnoofioofi. n¢.ogooo~. ”Ho.occco~. fi ”flotocooofi. m ;. I.. .0.,-_ .. km «.282. ¢pwoo~oo~. . n. ... mornoc~b~. mowndoo__. ~o+oo¢ccmc aall.:znnwfl] mo.cccamw. fl,_o+cpoocwk .Hoocoooow. —o+ccoco~. ~o+cooco~. no+coooo~o ”coaccocy. #O¢cocoo—. ~oococoo~. ~OICcooo—. - ~¢IQ9*~I. aJm ”cocococ—. ~o+cccoc~. ~o+ooooo~o Hooccooo~o ~0¢cocoo_o wnpbphbv.-th.ooooc~. 1&7; rLf. #o-cmmfl.- Noncbmucl ~0Iomrm.l “OIocNMLI ~OIQNON.I moIo¢®mo ~OICOMN. HOICNoN- Nelc¢ho- “clomflmol use finlomo~ol helcmo~ol .cclomo_.I mclco-.I Helcnm~.l acnccbh.I--. moccmom. coocfimo. .molcmON. NoIcomo.. NOICNHB. ~0ICOCh. mcuommm.4 oo.ooofi. ficncfimm.I oo+c¢-. "onoucm.I oo+Coa~. Houomom.u ~o:cm¢m. Houoosm.u fioIcsmm. mouc¢po.u _monc¢~o. oc+ocm~.l . ooooomm. no u gunamnm. gaucmmm. NOIcmmm. woocmmm. ¢ouomam. concomm. mcuo¢mm. moucmrm. floucmrm. floucocc. HcIomrm. oo+cm¢~. vllll ~cocco¢o h . ~= ofi A.u.pnoov : manna 121 ,mo+c~coo~. ¢C+Dcmco—o mcocco—C—o Nc+ccoo-. HOIDccccmo ~o+occc-o ~O+Ccc~c—o ,~o+:o~cc—o ~o+e~ooo~o ,HO+Ccccc~o ~o+cccco~o mo+cnccoao r . geowocflwcw. oo+cm~>o aqm ~o.Cococ—w ~o+cccoo—. ~0¢Cccoopo ~o+coooo~o ~0+Cccoo~. ~0+Ccooo~o ~C¢Coooc~. go+cccocgo «Coaccoo_o ~0§Cccoo—o ~o+cccoo~. ~o+Co>~o agm ~c+Coooo—o Fowcoooofi. ficlo_mm. uc: m—Icomm. moIeomm. moICCMM. MOIcrmm. moucwcm. moIcomo. welshom. NOIc~¢m. moIc~¢~. MOIcrcm. ¢OICJmca dolommfi.l uCC -I00m~.l honacm~.- 1 ~cIC~ocol uc NHIc¢mo.I melc¢mo.I mctcmmo.l moIofiqo.I ficIooo~.I ~oIc¢¢~.I ~oI:-#.I Nclcmmm.I moIcma~.I MOIQOQ¢.I ¢OIC¢m¢oI OOIOQN~.I no mfilcmCm. melcmox. ~OIC¢rxo u u A~I0>o~. hoocso~. :oIc>o~. mochofi. flcncacm. ~0IC-m. Holcqu. moICnmN. molc¢m¢o ¢OIC¢m¢. oo+Ccho HfiIComco helcpqc. A.u.pcoov : magma mo+cccfi. h - c~ Hoocccm. h - V cw 122 me #o +Q~coo~. +oo~oo~. _vFCIQCo.c~. d Na +£ooo—m. .~o+oocccmo m~o+ccoo_~. ~c+ooc~c~. ~c+Cc~oc~. ~o+o~ooc~. ~o ~o +ooooo_. +coooo~. III!- ‘Ihabl ,I oo+egm¢. a4m _o+Coooc~. ~o+Ccooc~. ~o.coooo~. —o+ccooo~. ~o+Ccocc~. ~o+Cocoo_. ~o+¢cooc~. ~o+ccooo~. _o+cQooc~. HoIcooQOM. _o+oooood. melodc_o nae m_uco¢¢. ooucc¢¢. couccqa. «ou:m¢¢. moucncq. mouccco. MOImsmm. moucsog. ¢cucmmm. melcwom. celccom. NeIChdmol uc M~IC¢~N.I 0cIo¢~>.I ocIcm—~.I doI5~H>.I McIO¢dh.I moIcmc~.I melcomcol nono~m~.I ¢OIcmcm.I mclcmm¢.l Celooom.I melocmm. mfiIchM. moucxcfi. molcmcd. moucmofi. NoI:m_~. malacma. McIC¢mo. moIcfisfi. ¢ouc¢¢m. moIoom¢. CoICOOM. Nc+comfi. HH 0“ A.u.pcoov J magma Q11" 61... .1. . I . 123 .¢;woflcoooag. . flmwom>~.u oo+o¢~a. ,mmucmcm. . IIINJ . “o.ccooogvm.a.a.o@+ooooq oo+omco.u cc+cmco.. ... _ouommm. .:mm¢ro¢sfi.u nonco¢~.- #o.coo~. "cuoooqu .qu moo me L p , go.coooo~. ONICMON. mmncmom. emucmmm. H" r .mmocmymr ~o+Cco°o_. >~-o¢m~. omncomm. NNICNoA. om #mo+occHw_. "c.coooo_. ¢~-c¢m~. NHIQNoH. omucmmm. o ”_o+oocmm¢. Ho.coooc~. Nfluacdm. «#IQOFH. pfiuooofi. x 185235. ~o.¢ccoo.. ocuco_¢. mfinemmm. 4~-c~m~. p .~o+oocom.. Ho.cGooc~. ocucqc_. couc-¢. m~-o¢mm. c ,wc.ooowQ~. “o‘ooooofi. ¢o-cmo¢. oOIchfi. oo-cm~¢. m ._cwoopmc~. Ho.ccooo~. moucomo. ¢ouomo¢. co-cmo~L ¢. _Ho+o~mocp. .c.¢oocc~. co.co>o. NCIQmao. ¢oncmo¢. «w ~o+ammoc_. ~c.¢ococfi. ~o+c~o_.u oo+oomo. NOICOoo. m. "~o+o~ooo~. ~o.¢cooo~. oo+c~oo. oo+o~oo.- oc.¢ooo. ~ .a.z .w.u Ravage Aw.ao .s.a a ._ . .M - . . IE I- III?! «..l., I- . . . . 3w». comm n 9 .Po n PM Hmuoa v>fipwpmm500 Mom qofipwasaflm hmpsmfioo .m wands 12h 1*" ~o—"~ -———u-—. —.—.—.—— flowocccmm. ~o+QooOo—o ~o+oo~mo~. ~c+o~woo~. ~c+ocmcc—. gooohooc—o rc+0ommm~. mc+occo>m. mc+ooo~NHI ~o+cccmm¢. .~c+ooocomo ~o+oo¢om~. _o+oocoo~. ~c+oo~mo.. ~c+o~moc~. ‘11,. l \‘i’l 1‘ u j _;~o»amq@m~. ~o+Coooo—. ~o+coooo~. ~o+coooc~. ~o+ccooo.. ~o+cocoo~. co+cmmm. adv _c+cooco~. ~o+Coooc~. ~o+ccmoa~6 doocoooe~. ~o+coooc~. ~o+cocoo~. ~o¢Coooo~. _o+cOooo~. ~o+ocooc~. .H.NOI \ gcccoom. cc+cm©~. ~oloo¢h. oo+co~o.l oc+C~ooo moIQm~moI Lac mfiucmom. -Icmco. ooIcfimo. nelo¢no. moucmmm. MCICN¢~. NOIcfimq. fiouc¢~w. oo+ewm~. é... NOImMMMI Mo-c0o~. ficuommc. co+c~mm. cc.omom. oo+ohooo Monacmo.I be mfiucm¢c. mfiugfimfi. onlcc¢—o mOIO©~—. NOIQ¢mh. welco¢M6 moICNq~. NOIScho HCIcomm. moucmmdu moaccm~. dolccma. ~OICNWN. oo+cmcm. oo+ccoo. ~o+coodo m -Ic~m>. qfiuc¢mfi. mflucoqfl. ofincocfi. oncmnfi. noIco¢~. . moIcowm. moucfima. Nolcmm¢o oc+ccc~. h “a CH A.U.pqoov m mHQdB 125 '3‘! wawocoosm. Nc+ooQ~N~. ~o+occmmao _~o+ccoccm. ~c+0ccom~. ~c+occoc~. ~0+Cosmopo ~o+o~mcc~. ~c+o¢mocmo ~o+Q~ooo—. mc+eommm—. NooCGOONM. mcooocgmwo .orocommc. wfio.occocm. .n. \ ...)“ I .¢o+cccoo_. ~o+coooc~. ~o+Ccooc~. ~o+Cccoo_. ~o+Coooo~o _o+ocooc~. ~o+coooc~. ~o+Ccooc~. ~o+cocoo~. ~o+coooo~o ~o+cm~—o aJm ~c+Coooo_. ~o+Coooo~. HoIocooo_. _o4cOoooH. ~o+cocoomo moIcawA. delcmmc. moIcOo~o NCICO_~. ~CIcmomo flolc¢mn. ~OICdmm. oo+cmm~.I oc+c~om.l oo+cm©m. mcIQNMmol ...Ec OCICONO. NGICMOO. mCIO¢N¢o Jelc¢ooo molecgfio onoxmm. ¢oIeom~. moIcmrm. moIomom. ~cIcm-. ~cIc¢qco oc+c_mfi. cc+cma~. molcomm. oc+smcro Nclcmmm.l u: Galahwm. mclc¢mh. ooICCNm. molc¢mmo mouoomfi. ncIcnmm. moIccrm. «olccm¢. «oICcnc. moIcch. ~oIcoqfi. HoIcnoo. oo+erfi. oo+c~¢m. oo+cmcm. co+cooc. m HfiIcmmm. OoIoumm. NOIcmwm. ecIcocwo ¢OICO~Ho oc+Cocm. flu ca A.w.pcoov m magma 126 ... OIacmnmfi. Newcoeopm. mo+o°¢~m~. .~c+ocomm¢. _«oIooooom. H¢+oeoom~. mROIQcoocH. _w+oo>mo_. .wo.owmeo~. HpIQINOCH. w_ero~coc.. T. amo+ocmmm~. I ...MF. II'. I II-II-I'I I’IIIIIIII »_.a.1w-1mw~w.Qmo¢. M.-.. HmIo~o¢uw do.c¢cm. "oIoHo¢.Lm..u. floIcIQm.I mam . use . ,M. ue ._o+cccoo~. FOIoemm. . NOIemo¢. ~o+coooo~. mOIamms. moIomom. ~o+ccocc_. mOIoopm. MoIomofi. _o.ocooo~. NOIoo¢m. moIohom. uo.cooco~. floIoomfi. ~oIocmH. ~o+ccoec~a “CIcmmm. HoIoo¢¢. "c.9ocoofi. NOIo~m¢.I HoIocmm. ~o+coooc_. .OIomoo.I “cIomcm. Ho.cocoo~. oo.o¢m~.I moIo~o~.I ~o.¢oooo~. ma-e~m¢. oo+omm_.I ~o.¢ocoo_. oo+omm~. oo+omm~.u ~o.oo-. doIom¢H.I moIomos.I QJm mac kc _o.ccocc.. scIcmmm. moIoomm. - II I -IIII - .. .I fioIm~o$qwquw. .~ .» a... .. 8.53. 25.3%,, k h 3A G poIcfipfl. fim moIommm. p «oncmom. oi . NOIommH. m NoIc~mm. s _ #OIQ¢~m. o. _ ,k _oICme. m. oc+c»-. e oo+cocm. . m oo+oc~m. m 00.9mm~. _ oo+c~oo. “o.ooo~. u » ooIcm~¢. Ha I IIL A u. pqoov m «Hams 127 c _ .. . ..arul ~m+oococ_.: _~c+oc~mcfi. Fc.c~¢co_. ~c+0¢mcc~o ~c+c~ooo~o rc+ocmmm~. No+oocohmo mo+oco~mfi. ~o+oocmm¢. ~o+Ccooom. ~o+occom~. ~o+oecoo—. ~o+coch—I ~c+c~moo~. ~c+o¢moc_. ~c+¢ccco_. ~o+cococ~. fioocoooo—o ~o+coooc~I ~o+coocc~. ~o+ccmm. a4m _o+ccooc~. ~oICocoo_. _o+cOcoo~. ~o+ccooc_. ~o+coooo~. ~o+coooo~. ~o+Ccooc~. ~o+ooooc.. _o+coooo~. ~o+Coooo~. ~oICo¢¢.I ~0IC¢NNII ~OICQ®~I Melcccm. ~OIcwm~I ficIQom©.I ucc NOIcowm. mOIONMM. MOICCN~I NCICQHH. NOIcmoH. ~0Icom~.I HoIo¢¢m.I ~OIocm~.I HoIc~¢m.I ~OIQOO¢I NOIco¢¢II ~oIcqx¢.I ~cIC>m>.I «oncsqm.I AoIcmm~.I ficIcqqo.I uc onocoo. qu:m_~. monommc. moICorm. ~oIc_cm. HoIemoq. Housedc. NcIQNoN.I ~OION¢NII ~OIcmoool oo+cnm~I oo+cmqa. oo+c¢q~. Hochmw. ~3ICma~. .oo+co~o. holcomm. quCNCA. mOIco~¢. moIccqm. HoIcomw. _oIchr. oo+ccw~. oo+cm~m. oo+cmmNI oo+co¢~. ~9+Cocm. "a Cu A.@.pcoov m mHDwB 128 MmmrQCCWMH. ”fic.ocomm¢. afioo0ococm. ~o+ccoomgo _: aonccooc2. mdc.co>mc_. «~c+c~moc_. qmo+c¢moo—I .wc.o~ooc_. .rc+eemmm_. “NCIQQOoFm. mNCICCcfimfi. W~o+occmmq. ~o+CoccoNI Ho+cccom~. .Ao+ccooc~. —o+Ccooo~. ~c+cococ—o ~o+Cocoo~I ~o+Ccooo~I —o+ccooo~. ~o+Coooo~. _o+ccoco~. ~o+Cccco~I ~c+ocm—I aam ~o+Cccoc~I —coCcooc—. ~0+Ccooc~o ~0+Cocoo~. ~o+Cccoo~. ~c+Cococ~I moIeohm.I NOIcrmm.I ~0IC¢4~.I doIc¢:m.I HOIcoc~.I NOIcmmo. ~OIcmmm. AoIoficm. NOIcmhc. ~0IQ¢~¢II moo NOIemcm.I molcmmdol MOICI>~II NCIC>mm.I ~OIobNHII ~oIcomm.I moIo¢q~. NcIacqw. moIcfiom. NOIcgom.I flcIcmqm.I McIomaw.I HoIooo¢.I ~0IcmcmrI molcmhc.l oo+cmmHIl u: scIcoqa. ¢0IQ¢C~I moIefior. mouommc. ~oI9m>~. Holeoam. moIcmyfi. ~OIcama. Holcfifio. oo+cmn~. oo+cmm~. OOIC¢K~I HoICNAm. ~oIc¢mm. moIcfiho. oo+cymbw L concepfi. ¢olcmmm. moncfimr. “cloned. doncmcm. OOICOFA. Lf‘ fly c~ A.c.pnoov m wands 129 2.2.1.... .I‘ III-’- .0 - ..Ifi.’ ..x.. m.cmo .I...:.1 Hmub2.o2. 2c42mom.I 2oLcm2o. OIOOO222 a2m 2am uc.. 2 2.22 2o.ccooo2. thc¢m2. FOIoomm.I poIeocc. 2m 2o.oooco2. mcIcmm2. mOIo¢o¢. mcIa22m. o2 2o.c°coc2. coIomsm. moIomm2.I. monomon. _ o 2o.2ooco2. moICmoc. NoIom22.I moIcmmm. m 2o.cocoe2. moucoom. mcIooos. 2oIcmm2. s 2o.oooon2. 2o.cccco2. moIcmmm. 20Io222.I 2oIooo2. o. _2fimmococ2. 2o.coooc2. moIoe2m. No-02no. 2OIcmq2. m. 2omoc-c2. 2o+ooeoo2. AOImcmm. NOIcmq¢.I moqumo. «U 2o.owmo22. 2o.aoooo2. No-9»m2. moIoo>2.I moIco2m. m” 2.8 o.o¢~oo2. 2o.coooo2. mOIcomm. moIcmo¢.I moIosac. N ‘m chooc2. 2o.coooc2. .20I02mc. ¢eIo2m¢.I quc2mc. .ww m 2o.com2. moIom2m.I oo.ocp2.I @o.cm2c. 2c.ooom. , w a2m uac no u 2 2 m fimo.conm~2. 2o.eocoo2. NOIcmom.I soIawmm. ooIchm. 22 2 chmewwImnI 2onroooo2. moIcomn.w .I. -.Imeooom. I IoIcoom. .IIImH A.v.p:OUV m manna irawemmm”. . ..W... cacaoobm. . O ccvcm@mo me ~o.cc¢oa~. _o.c°aoc.. _o.coocc~. ~¢¢cccoqmm Hc+ccocc~. ~o.ccooc~. Ho.c°oco~« ~o+cooca_. ~o.¢cacc_. ~0§cooco~o NotoN-o Lac mo-on¢~. co-om-. mo-cmmo. «o-co¢~. mcuoohm. moucmmm. mouem¢m. ¢oncomo. gono¢cm. molcoom. Noncom~.l no couch—m.n oouosom.a monomoo. mo-o¢~_.u mc-o»~¢.n noncomm.s mouqomm.u mouanm~.- «onomqm.u mango—«.u .:IQMcm.I .Ifl. Nonemam. mc.ccmwa. Fa . moue¢~m. _t coucmmm. o ¢o-omq‘. oL .mo-c¢¢~. .u m_ mo-o¢om. s mcucmph. c moLomq¢. m, mo:co¢a. ¢_ eoucmmm. mg. monom¢¢. w coucmom. n ..l...-r.rrkn. A.u_paoov m mapwa LIST OF REFERENCES 10. ll. l2. 13. 1h. 15. 16. 17. LIST OF REFERENCES B C. Goodwin, Temporal Organization in Cells, Academic Press, London and New York, 1963, p. 2. C. H. Waddington, Foreword, in B. C. Goodwin, Temgoral Organiza- tion in Cells, Academic Press, London and New York, 1963. B. L. Strehler, Time, Cells, and Aging, Academic Press, New York and London, 1962. A. Weissman, Essays Upon Heredity and Kindred Biological Prob- lems, Oxford University Press, London and New York, 1891. A. Comfort, Ageing, The Biology of Senescence, Rev. Ed., Routledge and K. Paul, London, 196k. B. Rosenberg, G. Kemeny, L. Smith, I. D. Skurnick, and M. Bandurski, Mech. Age Dev., 2 (1973) 275. B. Rosenberg, G. Kemeny, R. C. Switzer, and T. C. Hamilton, Nature, 232 (1971) h71. M. Joly, A Physico-Chemical Approach to Denaturation of Pro- teins, Academic Press, New York and London, 1965. O. Rahn, Bact. Rev., 8 (l9hh) 1. C. Eikjmann, Folia Microbiol., 1 (1912) 359. B. Gompertz, Phil. Trans. Roy. Soc. (London), Series A, 115 (1825) 513. W. M. Makeham, J. Inst. Actu., 13 (1867) 325. w. Perks, J. Inst. Actu., 63 (1932) 12. Vital Statistics of the United States, 1967, Vol. 2, Mortality (Part A), U. s. Govt. Ptg. Office, 1969. M. Delbrnck, J. Chem. Phys., 8 (19uo) 12o. Z. A. Medvedev, Exp. Geront., T (1972) 227. L. Szilard, Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci. (U. s.), h5 (1959) 30. 131 18. 19. 20. 21. 22. 23. 2h. 25. 26. 27. 28. 29. 30. 31. 32. 132 J. Maynard-Smith, Nature, 18h (1959) 956. W. Feller, An Introduction to Probability Theory and Its Applications, Vol. 1, 6rd Ed.) John Wiley and Sons, New York, 1968. R. A. Fisher and L. H. C. Tippett, Proc. Camb. Phil. Soc., 2h (1928) 180. N. V. Smirnov. American Mathematical Society Translation Number 67, 1952. P. B. Gnedenko, Ann. of Math., 11 (19h3) M23 (in French). R. de Mises, in Selected Papers of Richard von Mises, Vol. 2, American Mathematical Society, Providence, l96h, 271 (in French). N. T. Uzgaren, in Studies in Mathematics and Mechanics, Pre- sented to Richard von Mises, Academic Press, New York and London, 195M, 3h6. J. M. Clarke and J. MaynardrSmith, J. Exp. Biol., 38 (1961) 679. M. J. Hollingsworth, Exp. Geront., l (1966) 259. B. L. Strehler, J. Gerontol., 17 (1962) 3A7. F. A. Lints and C. V. Lints, Exp. Geront., 6 (1971) h17. M. J. Hollingsworth, Exp. Geront., h (1969) h9. B. Rosenberg, personal communication. L. Orgel, Nature, 2h3 (1973) hhl. G. A. Sacher and E. Trucco, Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci., 9h (1962) 985.