AN EXPLORATORY STUDY EMPLOYING THE EDUCATIONAL SCIENCE OF COGNITIVE STYLE AS A PREDICTOR 4 OF GROUP LEADERSHIP WITHIN I ‘ 7 AN ORIENTATION PROGRAM . Thesis for the Degree 0f Ph. D. EfiECHIEAI‘I STATE UNIVERSITY VIEWER? GUSTAVE SIGREN 2973 L [B R [It I: 1’ Michigan $72.12*: I I Uni-w {3.3.17 (i This is to certify that the thesis entitled AN EXPLORATORY STUDY EMPLOYING THE EDUCATIONAL SCIENCE OF COGNITIVE STYLE AS A PREDICTOR OF GROUP LEADERSHIP WITHIN AN ORIENTATION PROGRAM presented by Vincent Gustave Sigren has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for Ph. D;degreein Education / gt» /? W [ll .42 #41:!- tum—m. r Msjorprofeér/ Date April 12, 1973 0-7639 I BINBING‘BY IIIJAB & SONS’ . 800K BINDERY INL‘. LIBRARY amoms srnmerom. llcmu ABSTRACT AN EXPLORATORY STUDY EMPLOYING THE EDUCATIONAL SCIENCE OF COGNITIVE STYLE AS A PREDICTOR OF GROUP LEADERSHIP WITHIN AN ORIENTATION PROGRAM BY Vincent Gustave Sigren Pur 2086 This exploratory study was undertaken to deter— mine whether the educational science of cognitive style as developed by Hill and associates1 can be used as a predictor of group leadership and to compare the effectiveness of directive and nondirective leadership styles in "assigned" and "build" groups within a college orientation program. Thus, the focus of the study is directed to the problems created by the use of small group techniques; leader selection, style of leadership and the manner of group construction. Collective cog- nitive styles were determined for the thirtyvtwo leaders by a technique described by Flanagan and comparisons were drawn between the most and least effective leaders. Vincent Gustave Sigren The effect of leadership style and group construction was evaluated by their effect upon new student satisfaction, attendance and first-semester attrition rate. Summary The student sample employed in this study includes almost all the new students of the entering 1972 fall class of Olivet College. The leaders for the small group orientation program were selected from approximately 100 upperclass students, who had applied for the position of student advisor. The measures used were: (1) Leadership profile for leader selection, (2) Cognitive style testing, (3) Orientation group evaluation, (4) Leader self- evaluation, (5) Observation of leadership behavior and (6) Composite interview reaction. Six general questions were posed in order to con- duct the exploratory study effort. Where these questions generated a hypothesis, the hypothesis was tested, e.g., when there is a difference in leadership style or group construction, there will be a difference in (1) student satisfaction and (2) attendance or attrition rate. The Komolgorov-Smirnov statistical test was used to test the null hypothesis form of the operational hypothesis at alpha = .05 level of significance with the appropriate degrees of freedom. ings: Vincent Gustave Sigren Findings The analysis of data supports the following find- The science of cognitive style can differentiate between the most effective and least effective leaders. Thus, cognitive style can be used as a basis for leader selection after an appropriate collective cognitive style base has been estab— lished, and also can be used to prescribe certain programs to increase the probability of effective leadership. The Collective Cognitive style of the most and least effective leaders showed differences in all three sets of cognitive style. In the set, symbols and their meanings, the significant dif— ference occurred in the "most effective leaders" group having a greater number of qualitative sym- bolic orientations. The cultural determinants set indicated that the family had a greater influence on meanings for most effective leaders while this element had less influence for least effective leaders. The most effective leaders group had the additional major elements of (D) Difference and (L) Appraisal in the modalities of inference set. These inference patterns Vincent Gustave Sigren refer to the ability to distinguish differences in arriving at decisions and also to utilize all the inferences in the process of appraising a situation. 3. There was no preference expressed by new students for leadership style as measured by new student satisfaction. There was, however, a tendency to greater satisfaction in the "build" groups. The latter conclusion is based on the finding of < "placing the hypothesis in doubt" i.e., .lO _ p f .05. 4. There was a preference for nondirective leadership at a highly statistically significant level (p = .01) among the leaders. 5. Leadership and group construction did not affect the attrition rate. 6. There was no significant difference in attendance rates between the directive and nondirective groups. Attendance had been taken and emphasized in the directive groups and had not been visibly recorded or emphasized in the nondirective groups. During the leader interviews, special group prob- lems became evident which directly and indirectly influenced attendance. These problems include the Vincent Gustave Sigren negative effect of upperclass students on new students, the unique needs of transfer, commuter and football groups and the length of the program. Lastly, student leaders indicated that they bene- fited greatly from the leadership education and experience. 1Joseph E. Hill, Pamphlet on the Educational Sciences, Oakland Community College, February, 1971, p. l. AN EXPLORATORY STUDY EMPLOYING THE EDUCATIONAL SCIENCE OF COGNITIVE STYLE AS A PREDICTOR OF GROUP LEADERSHIP WITHIN AN ORIENTATION PROGRAM BY Vincent Gustave Sigren A THESIS Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Administration and Higher Education 1973 i Dedicated , "A to Linda, Eric, Krista, Beth, Lief and Mrs. Sigrid Forberg ii ACKNOWLEDGMENTS Sincere appreciation and acknowledgment of assistance are extended to Dr. Walter F. Johnson, chairman of the committee, whose guidance and counsel have been instrumental throughout this exploratory study and to Dr. Joseph E. Hill for his assistance and advice on the educational sciences. Their contributions have been sub— stantial and always given in a most gracious manner. The author is appreciative of the help and encouragement given by Dr. Wilbur B. Brookover, Dr. Vandel Johnson and Dr. Eldon Nonnamaker who along with Dr. Walter Johnson served as members of the committee. Special thanks are extended to Mrs. Doris Harring— ton who assisted in data collection and typing and to the student leaders of Olivet College for their willing— ness to share in the adventure of behavioral patterns of leadership. There are, of course, others who have assisted in their special ways and to them I will always be grateful. iii TABLE OF CONTENTS Chapter Page I. INTRODUCTION . . . . . . . . . . . l The Problem . . . . . . . . . 1 Background of Study. . . . . . . . 1 Educational Sciences . . . . . . 5 Leadership Styles . . . . . . . 7 Olivet College Study . . . . . . ll Purpose of Study. . . . . . . . l4 Significance of the Study. . . . . . 14 General Questions To Be Explored . . . 16 Underlying Assumptions. . . . . . . 17 Definitions of Key Terms . . . . . . 17 Educational Sciences . . . . . . l8 Cognitive Style Mapping . . . . . l8 Cartesian Product of Three Sets. . . 19 Theoretical Symbols (e.g. Words and Numbers) . . . . . . . . . 20 Qualitative Symbols. . . . . . . 20 Four Theoretical Symbols . . . . . 20 Five Qualitative Symbols . . . . . 21 Ten Qualitative Codes . . . . . . 22 Cultural Determinants . . . . . . 23 Modalities of Inference . . . . . 24 Major . . . . . . . . . . . 25 Minors . . . . . . . . . . 25 Collective Cognitive Style . . . . 26 Orientation Terms . . . . . . . 26 Overview of the Study . . . . . . . 28 II. REVIEW OF RELATED LITERATURE. . . . . . 29 Educational Science of Cognitive Style . 29 Small Group Leadership . . . . . . 39 Small Group Orientation . . . . . . 54 Summary. . . . . . . . . . . . 62 iv Chapter III. DESIGN OF STUDY . . . . . . . . . Source of Data . . . . . . Samples Employed by Study. . . . . Sample of Leaders . . . . . . Representativeness of Sample of the Defined Populations. . . . . Adequacy of Sample Size . . . . Data Collection . . . . . . . . Instrumentalities . . . . . . Procedures . . . . . . . . Summary of Design of Study . . . . IV. ANALYSIS OF DATA AND FINDINGS . . . . Analysis of Data. . . . . . Analytic Techniques Employed. . . . Testable Hypothesis. . . . . . F ind ings Q C I O O O Q O O V. SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS . . . . . . Findings and Conclusions . . . . . Theoretical Symbols. . . . Qualitative Symbols. . . . Cultural Determinants . . . Modalities of Inference . . Male-Female Leadership. . . O O O 0 O O O O l O Discussion and Implications . . . . Recommendations . . . . . . . . BIBLIOGRAPHY . . . . . . . . . . . . APPENDICES Appendix A. Composite Observations of Leadership Behaviors . . . . . . . . . . B. Student Adviser Leader Selection Profile. Page 64 64 65 67 69 69 69 70 74 78 81 81 81 84 86 102 104 105 106 108 109 110 113 117 120 127 128 Appendix C. D. Individual-Orientation Group Evaluation Leader Self-Evaluation Rating Form Leadership Behavior. Cognitive Mapping Card Sort Questions. Interview Session-~W0rksheet Composite Interview Reactions vi Page 129 131 133 135 136 L I ST OF TABLES Table Page 1. First Semester Withdrawals, January 1971 (Pilot Study) . . . . . . . . . . . 13 2. Two Sample Komolgorov—Smirnov Statistical Techniques . . . . . . . . . . . . 83 3. Differences in Leadership Style and Group Con- struction of Most Effective and Least Effec— tive Leaders . . . . . . . . . . . 89 4. Differences Between Most Effective and Least Effective Leaders According to Sex . . . . 90 5. Difference in Preference of New Students for Leadership Style . . . . . . . . . . 91 6. Differences in Preference of New Students for Group Construction . . . . . . . . . 92 7. Frequency Distribution of Leadership Style and Group Construction Preference of Leaders . . 93 8. Leadership Style Preference of Leaders . . . 93 9. Leader-Group Construction Preference of Leaders. 94 10. Differences Between Leadership and Group Con— struction Relative to First Semester Attrition O O C O O O O O O O O O 95 11. Frequency Distribution of Attendance Within Categories of Leadership Style and Group Construction . . . . . . . . . . . 96 12. Differences Between Directive and Nondirective Leadership in Terms of Attendance . . . . 97 13. Composite Interview Results . . . . . . . 98 vii LIST OF E IGURES Figure Page 1.. Cognitive Map . . . . . . . . . . . 19 2. Major Elements of Student Leaders . . . . 37 23. Design Model for Measuring Effectiveness of Leaders . . . . . . . . . . . . 79 44. Most Effective Leader Collective Cognitive Sty1e O I I O O O O O I O I O 8 6 £5. Least Effective Leader Collective Cognitive Sty1€ O O I O O O O O I O O I 87 (5. Differences in Cognitive Styles of Most Effective and Least Effective Leaders . . 88 '7. Differential Influence Patterns in the Cultural Determinant Set of Most and Least Effective Leaders. . . . . . . 109 viii CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION The Problem Periodically in society there is a need to select potentially effective leaders for certain endeavors. Once selected there is a need for the chosen leader to lead in such a manner that he or she enables the group to actualize its potential and receive optimum benefits. Each summer or fall, colleges across the nation face this problem when staffing their orientation programs. One Of the orientation formats gaining wide usage utilizes small group techniques which, in turn, creates the PrOblems of leader selection, style of leadership to be employed and the manner of group construction. The focus Of this study is directed toward these problems. Background of Study Effective leader selection methods have often been the object of search by military, government, indus— trial and educational groups. This search has been halt\pered by a very marked difference of opinion between Subscribers to the trait approach to leadership and those who :felt that the situational aspect of leadership was the: tended to view leadership in terms of personality traits, the situation and the interaction between the two. In aan early study Jennings4 concluded from her investi- gation of girls in correctional institution that both :iscilation and leadership were found to be products of interpersonal interaction and not of attributes residing within persons. An exploratory study by John K. Hemphill5 indicated that a view of leadership which stresses the situational nature of the leader's behavior gives a sound behavioral foundation for practical programs in the selection and training of those who are to direct group activities. If sufficient knowledge about the relation of leadership to dimensions of the group can be obtained, selection of leaders can be made with reference to the <flemnands of the situation in which they are to lead. A stuniy by Martin, Gross and Darley6 produced findings which led the investigators to conclude: The paucity of dif— feI‘ences found between leaders and nonleaders tends to negate the trait approach to leadership and suggests \ 4H. H. Jennings, Leadership and Isolation (New York: Longmans, Green & Co., 1943), p. 24. E 5John Hemphill, "Situational Factors in Leadership,‘ V\dlfi3gtiona1 ResearchoMonographs, No. 32, Ohio State Uni- eI‘SIty, pp. v and v1. “ 6W. F. Martin, N. Gross, and J. G. Darley, Leaders, Followers, and Isolates in Small Groups," mal of Abnormal Social Psychology, XLVII (1952), 842. the litilization of other frames of reference in the study of leadership phenomena. Thomas Gordon,7 however, points out that in their haste to drop the trait theory, some social scientists rnaaf have swung too far in the direction of emphasizing the situation. Conceivably, the situationists may be overlooking the possibility that at least some of the traits predispose their possessors to positions of leadership, or at least increase the chances of their becoming leaders in most situations. Gouldner had also iruiicated this position when he stated that, "by and large, the former school, characterized as 'situationlist,‘ have won the day. . . . Uneasy rests the head that wears the crown of science." Gouldner further suggests: Suppose, however, it were demonstrated that all human groups contained some elements in common, and that these could be spelled out. It should, there- fore, be expected that there would be some leader- ship traits manifested commonly by all leaders. In short, there is no reason why leadership traits should constitute adaptions only to the diversities of groups; they should, too, involve adaptions to the similarities of groups. Thus, some leadership traits, should be unique, specific to concrete groups and situations, while some could be common to all leaders.8 7Thomas Gordon, Group Centered Leadership (Boston: Houghton Mifflin Co., The Riverside Press Cambridge, 955), p. 49. 8A. W. Gouldner, ed., Studies in Leadership (New Yerk: Harper Bros., 1940), p. 35. Gordon9 points out that this position, if sound, paves the way for an integration of the previously perceived disrergent theories about leadership. Such an integration wcnlld retain the important contribution of the situa- tionists--their emphasis on the demands of the group and needs of the members, yet it would not close the door on tlle: possibility of discovering some traits or character- istics of importance to leaders in most group situations. This is hinted at by a survey of the use of student coun- selors in 128 senior colleges and universities undertaken 1J1 11963 by William F. Brown and Vernon Zunkerlo which. iruiicated that the five main basis for selection included previous leadership experience, dormitory directors‘ evaluations, college grade average, faculty members evaluation and peer acceptance ratings. Other items used less frequently included scholastic ability test Scores, study habits, survey scores, screening inter- View and Dean's evaluation. ngfiiétional Sciences Although the above main basis of selection are rather general, greater specificity could be ascribed to \ 9Gordon, gp. cit., p. 51. 10William F. Brown and Vernon Zunker, "Student Ch?unselor Utilization at Four Year Institutions of lgher Learning," Journal of College Student Personnel, II, No. 1 (January, 1966), 41-46. themn. One way would be through the utilization of the Edruzational Science of Cognitive Style as developed by Joseph Hill and Associates. Hillll indicates that the Educational Sciences were created as a conceptual frame- work for education (defined as the process of searching for meaning), a system within which inquiry of significance ftDI: the fundamental aspects of the applied field of edu— cation can be conducted. In this context, then the Edu- cational Sciences provide a conceptual framework and laruiverse of discourse for the applied field of education. The seven "sciences" are: (1) Symbols and their meanings; (2) Cultural determinants of the meaning of symbols; (3) Modalities of inference; (4) Biochemical and electrophysiological aspects of memory-concern; (5) Cognitive style; (6) Teaching style, administrative style and coun- seling style; and (7) Systemic analysis decision-making. 'Tkua following assumptions are essential to these bodies of information: 11Hill, 92. cit., p. 1. 1. Man is a social creature with a unique capacity for deriving meaning from his environment and personal experiences through the creation and use of symbols. 2. Not content with biological satisfactions alone, man continually seeks meaning. 3. Education is the process of searching for meaning. 4. Thought is different from language. The Educational Sciences are continually evolving in both theory and refinement. The fourth science relating to 'Hnennory-concern" for example, is still in its early developmental stages . Eadership Styles Orientation supervisors are also faced with the Problem of determining the method of leadership style tC> be employed by the selected leaders. Although, tIKEre are many nomenclatures to leadership, they can be difirided into the general categories of directive and ncundirective leadership. Directive leadership connotes haSicly that the authority is leader centered while non— directive leadership connotes the authority as being gtubup centered. A continuum illustrative of leadership ““3uld range from autocratic leadership on the directive extremity to laissez faire on the nondirective end. 'rhere are advocates of directive leadership who indicate that new students for the most part are conditioned to directive leadership from previous societal influences, j. -e., family, school, church, and need someone who can literally take charge in this new experience. Advocates of nondirective leadership point out that the student is ultimately responsible for his own destiny and is looking toward college as being an experience different from high school. This expectation usually includes increased freedom in making personal choices. There are others who feel that institutional objectives should be the prime consideration. One might easily visualize directive leadership in the U.S. Marine Corps and nondirective leadership in an institution attempting to maximize democratic participation. Some advocates would recommend that the leader should lead in a style which best fits his or her personality, While others would recommend that, as the leadership Situation changed, the leadership behavior should also c31'1-iz'iulge. This latter category would undoubtedly start d'jLJZ‘ectively and as the group acclimated itself to the new environment, become more nondirective. Regardless of the method selected, the orientation supervisor must make a choice or allow the decision to go by default. In one of the classic pioneering experiments by Lewin, white and Lippittl2 groups of lO-year-old children were \ n] lzKurt Lewin, Ralph White and Ronald Lippitt, atterns of Aggressive Behavior in Experimentally subjected to autocratic, democratic and laissez faire leadership. Hostility was either greater in the auto- cratic groups or they evidenced extremely nonaggressive apathetic patterns and the perceived satisfactions of leadership were greatest for the groups led democrati- <::£ally and secondly for the laissez faire groups. Alex Bavela313 in an experiment with W.P.A. workers showed that it was possible to create fundamental changes in leadership philosophy and leadership techniques within three weeks. The experiment involved a change from autocratic leadership to democratic leadership and resulted in improved morale, greater productivity and increased enrollment in programs. Both the control and experimental samples were selected from previously mediocre workers. Shaw and Blum14 using Fiedler‘s con- tingency model and evaluating leadership under different task situations found that the results of the experiment Showed clearly that directive leadership is more effective than nondirective when the task is highly structured; \ CJi‘eated Social Climates," Journal of Social Psychology, X ( l 939), 271-79. L 13Alex Bavelas, "Morale and the Training of eaders," in Civilian Morale, ed. by G. Watson (Boston: Q1-‘lghton Mifflin, 1942), pp. 143- 65. Q l4Marvin E. Shaw and J. Michael Blum, "Effects :5 Leadership Style Upon Group Performance as a Function 2E Task Structure," Journal of Personality and Social W, III, No. 2 (February, 1966), 241. 10 'that is, when there is only one solution and one way (or (Jnly a few ways) of obtaining this solution. . . . How- ever, in tasks that require varied information and .aalgproaches, nondirective leadership is clearly more 6 :ffective . Recent literature has also emphasized the rirrnportance of both the leaders and their respective groups. The Hazen Report points out the fact that the most effective teachers are the students and that 1:;lne peer group influence of friendship groups is so <:>13vious that educators must be able to integrate it :i.11to the educational experience. Yet, the knowledge of how these friendship groups can contribute positively 1:;<3 the educational process is still meagre. The report §3r<3es on to state: We know that the trauma of leaving home for the first time and entering the relatively impersonal milieu of the college can be severe for young people. But far from attempting to facilitate the transition from home to college, we generally act so as to reinforce the freshman trauma. 5 JQ¥3=71:hur Chickeringl6 states that a student's most important t-'-‘E=Eicher is another student. Friend and reference groups jESimilter and modulate the messages from the larger student \ 15The Hazen Report, The Student in Higher Edu— ‘E-~d;3iign, Report of the Committee on the Student in Higher G-"-—1o::ation, The Hazen Foundation, p. 12. 16Arthur Chickering, Education and Identity (San F b . Ein01sco: Jossey-Bass Inc., 1969), p. 253. 11 culture. They amplify or alternate the force of the curriculum, faculty, parental rules and institutional regulations. They can trump the best teacher's ace and stalemate the most thoughtful or agile dean. Thus, relationships with close friends and peer groups, or subcultures, are primary forces influencing student develOpment in college. Both the Hazen Report and Chickering l7 emphasize the importance of friendship or reference groups and the role of the student as a teacher (leader). Usually, the first teacher a new student has in college is the Orientation Group (O-Group) leader and it should follow that the leadership situation, the leader's personal traits, the style of leadership and the manner in which the new group is formed could have great impact for the new student. The manner of group formation is an extension of the directive or non— d irective style of leadership. Directive leadership would be enhanced by assigned groups while nondirective leadership would allow some self—determination of the re Spective groups . 9fliet College Study A pilot study to determine the effect of leader— eh - . . . 1p within the Or1entat1on Program was conducted at Q - :L :Lvet College in 1971. New students were randomly \ l7Ibid. 12 assigned according to confirmation dates to four leader- ship categories: directive assigned, directive build, nondirective assigned and nondirective build. Build is a term for group construction in which one student chooses a partner, they in turn choose another pair and they then proceed geometrically to the established group size. Student leaders were randomly assigned to one of the four groups and were given general instructions in directive or nondirective techniques. A weakness in the study was that although they were to use their assigned leadership style, they were not given specific behavioral actions to carry out. Members of the student orientation groups were asked to differentiate the leaders on a five- point scale (1, most directive to 5 least directive). The following mean scores were noted: Directive Assigned 2.26, Nondirective Assigned 2.33, Directive Build 2.44, Nondirective Build 3.33. The tendency toward Central continuum scoring was probably due to the above noted weakness. Students, however, were able to dif— ferentiate between groups with some discrimination. The purpose of the pilot study was to see if the leadership styles might have an effect on the first SexT‘lester attrition rate and the perceived satisfaction (>13 Students within the orientation process. There was 1% S-s attrition in the nondirective build category. Stu—- 6Q hts indicated on a scale of l excellent--5 poor, that 13 ‘they perceived greater satisfaction in the directive gyroups; directive build 2.6, directive assigned 2.8, nondirective build 3.08 and nondirective assigned 3.56. {Iihe largest number of evaluation returns, 49 of 56, were turned in by the nondirective groups. Thus, a conflict- ing pattern was emerging, one in which students were per- ceiving greater satisfaction in the directive groups and gireat attrition rate and evaluation returns favored the ercondirective build groups. The next logical step is to .Eaxttempt to explain these tendencies through a study based IJIIPOD concepts that permit greater explanation of behavior <51Lifferentiation than was possible in previous efforts. {IPIHe emphasis on behavior differentiation returns one to 1tzlne earlier question as to whether or not effective, (ilsirective or nondirective, leadership can be predicted eaerd thus sets the purpose of the present study. TIEZKJBLE 1.--First semester withdrawals, January 1971 (Pilot Study) Leadership and Group Number Percentage Construction \ Directive Assigned 8 - 80 10 Nondirective Assigned 9 - 56 16 Directive Build 7 — 56 12.5 Nondirective Build 2 - 56 3.6 Total 26 — 248 10.5 l4 Purpose of Study The purpose of this exploratory study is two-fold. {The first purpose is to determine whether the educational science of cognitive style can be used as a predictor (of group leadership. The second purpose is to compare the effectiveness of directive and nondirective leader- :ship styles in "assigned" and "build" small groups wwithin an orientation program measured in terms of a :seven-point scale over leadership categories. The jLnstrument used to measure "effectiveness" will be JLocally constructed. Significance of the Study The demise of the small college was predicted in the 1960's and again in the 1970's. These predictions .Ilave been viewed with alarm by educators who feel that “izlua strength of higher education lies in its very «ciaiversity. The advent of the Community College movev Jnflfiant, demographic changes in population growth, lessen- :iJIMg of Vietnam pressures and the questioning of the Worth of higher education has made this latter pre- (ii-jitztion especially threatening to the small colleges. 1:5“:3Inission prOSpects have dwindled, costs have increased ‘Ea‘lrhd young people are seeking avenues other than college £E“=Ei an alternate route to their eventual life style. This sequence of events has made it imperative 1tlctlat.the small college be able to minimize the cultural 15 shock felt by the incoming new students and the accompany- ing attrition rate while at the same time attempting to increase the holding power at all levels. The orien- tation program offers the first opportunity for the college to address itself intelligently to the problem. It is generally conceded that the impact and import of first impressions have a significance for most people Ipeople far beyond the significance of their actual (occurrence. As a result of optimum orientation programs, a greater number of students could benefit by an increased probability of success and the accompanying psychological and economic regards. The institution benefits via tangible financial benefits and intangible institutional goodwill. The Orientation group leaders themselves are receiving an inservice education in the art of leader- Ship. The initial selection process via cognitive mapping could point out differences to be investigated and possibly initiate individual change. By the same token, administrators may be able to distinguish dis- tinctive cognitive styles which could lead to identifi— Qation of potentially effective leadership in a variety Q 1‘? situations. The data collected could have implications l-:egarding leadership and cognitive style within the 16 education sciences. Information collected via inter- views should be helpful for future planning by orien— tation directors and finally the study should serve as laction research to improve an ongoing orientation pro- gram at Olivet College. Generalyguestions To Be Explored The purpose of the proposed study will be :realized by seeking the answers to the following c>rts. A later study by Ivey increase the academic and intellectual emphaSis of the 5 found that efforts to orlentation programs did not appear to Win recognition by freshman students. The three-year study showed that freshmen responded best to social or informational A series of meetings with residence hall activities. counselors appeared to be the best received feature of each Freshman week program. Regardless of emphasis, the utilization of small groups seem to be increasing in orientation programs. The study of Hoffman and Plutchuck76 of orientation c‘:>‘--‘-I.J:‘s.es at other colleges as well as their own exper— iences have convinced them that the purposes of orien- t'a-‘C.i._<3n are best realized through a small group approach with teachers employing a group-centered method of leadership. Ninety-eight per cent of the students agreed or strongly e Valuated in a study by Pappas a greed that small group orientation seSSions \ "A Three Year Evaluation of a 75 l hlege Freshman Week Program," Journal of College Stu— \Personnel, V, No. 2 (December, 1963), 113-18. 76Hoffman and Plutchuck, up. Clt., p X1. "Student Reaction to a Small— 77 John G. Pappas, "119 Orientation Approach, " College and University, it Q II 84- 87. II ‘ 59 (eaight-twelve students) are more desirable than orien— ta tion meetings where large numbers of students are in It is suggested in a similar study by Miller attendance. 78 and Ivey that because of the consistent favorable rxzsslponse of students to small group meetings and indi— vidual sessions, regardless of the type of program, that perhaps shortened programs emphasizing small groups would be the ideal precollege orientation. As previously stated, college authorities insti- ttitzee: orientation programs in an effort to decrease Research in this area, however, is generally a. 1: tr ition. One positive finding is reported by no t promising . 79 Freshman students who met voluntarily in ESIrijLitiln. Small group sessions throughout their first semester kjL51C3- <3n1y an 8 per cent withdrawal rate at the end of the Seruester, as compared to a 24 per cent rate for a control group and 31 per cent for experimental group members who However, Kopecek80 "Structure (Eift21CLGEztjded fewer than three meetings. _‘____________ IQ‘EE 78C. Dean Miller and Allen E. Ivey, SQ Spcnnse to Three Types of Orientation Programs," Per- ““-353£ELS§1 and Guidance Journal, XLV (June, 1967), 1,025-29. 79Smith, "Higher Education Programs," 1963, cited lle, Vincent Harris, and Carolyn Dragger, "Orien- in tatEarl Ko 1Nr<:> i3~<>n Programs," Review of Educational Research, XXXVI, ‘ 2 (April, 1966f, 243. "Freshman Orientation Pro- 80Robert J. Kopecek, A Comparison," Journal of College Student Per- :61“ (:3’Jr: £3: \\“““‘~35£Sal, VIII, No. 5 (September, 1967), 51-52. 60 found that voluntary withdrawal and academic dismissal are not affected by orientation programs. This study utilized three different approaches to orientation: small-group nondirective and nonauthoritarian, authority centered and mailed material. The program covered 180 randomly selected students at a resident two-year tech- nical college in rural New York State. A similar result I in a community ‘3‘» . obtained by Rothman and Leonard was During the summer of 1965 they constructed a col 1 ege. "good" semester-long orientation program to meet the needs as indicated by a previous questionnaire. Three small group meetings and one large presentation meeting Were held each month. Twelve sessions were held and groups were randomly selected and assigned. The results of the study showed that control and experimental groups not differ significantly in grade point average d id during either the first or second semester. Similarly, there was no difference found between the two groups in In his study focusing a“t‘t-thition rate for either semester. found that or) academics rather than attrition, Pappas t 11:) Se students who completed either the "directive- fa Q"ilial" approach or the "small—group" approach of " E 81Leslie K. Rothman and Donald G. Leonard, ffectiveness of Freshman Orientation," Journal of Q Q 3 Qllege Student Personnel, VIII, No. 5 (September, 1967), ‘04. 82Pappas, 22' cit., pp. 84-87. t 61 additional college orientation, demonstrated significantly higher academic achievement than those who participated only in the pre-college program. There was no significant difference, however, between the "factual" and "small- gro up" approach. The role of the student as leader, adviser or Counselor, has increased as the size of Freshman classes have become larger each year and as orientation has become impersonal in its approach. Grier supports k this increased emphasis as he comments on the accepted technique of using upper-class students as counselors: ZIf the emphasis is to be on life adjustment, the Iase of upperclassmen makes some sense, since they eare closer to the new student than the faculty or iadministrator. However, we must have considerable areservation about how well in-service training Iprograms for student counselors really work. At loest, they may be more useful to the student coun— sselor than to the new student.33 84 Brown and Zunker in a study cited previously found that the main basis for selection of student counselors included previous leadership eXperience, dormitory directory evaluation, college grade average, faculty 1In‘a‘113ers' evaluation and peer acceptance ratings. They a 1 so found that 84 per cent of the respondents felt that 81: 13*d-ents made an effective and positive contribution to \ 83Grier, 92. cit., pp. 37-41. 84Brown and Zunker, EE- cit., pp. 41-46. 62 the total guidance program of their institutions. The information on small-group orientation is best summed up by Patty,85 who observes that there is very little written on the training of freshman advisors, and for that matter, on the use of experienced graduate students as freshman advisors. His concern voices the concern that there is absolutely nothing in the literature on the evaluation of existing orientation programs at indi- vidu a1 institutions and on research that explores whether or not a particular institution is compatible with an individual freshman. This statement along with the pre- sented evidence of conflicting findings re-emphasizes the need for additional research on small-group orien- ta 1:. ion programming . Summary This chapter has presented research on the J'3‘e-3—a.ted areas of the Educational Science of Cognitive StYl e, Small Group Leadership and Small Group Orientation. IE: has emphasized the concepts and supporting theory 11an which the educational science of cognitive style is ba- sed, presented the controversy regarding leadership thecry and provided both research and recommendations E Q 1|: small-group orientation programs. In the next 85Austin H. Patty, "Freshman Orientation: Q'b Q"Wing Concern," Improving Collgge and University '3? ‘53: W, XIV (Summer, 1966), 184-88. A 63 (:Imapter, the design of the study is considered in respect 't<> the general questions raised in Chapter I and also ‘tliee related research presented in Chapter II. CHAPTER III DESIGN OF STUDY Source of Data This study involved the new students of the entering 1972 Fall class of Olivet College and the upper-class orientation leaders. Olivet College is located in Olivet, a city of approximately 1,000 inhabitants in south central Michigan, thirty-five miles southwest of Lansing,'the state's Cap ital. The College is a four-year co-educational liberal arts institution of approximately 800 students. The faculty student ratio at the time of the study was 1 3 = IL. Students represented thirty states and fifteen fereign countries. The northeastern United States was e S p ecial 1y well represented . Olivet was founded in 1844 and is a member of the American Council of Education, Association of Mexican Colleges, Association of Independent Colleges and Universities of Michigan, Council for Higher Edu- Qation of the United Church of Christ, Michigan Academy Q28 EScience, Arts and letters, Michigan Association of 64 h 65 Colleges and Universities, Michigan Colleges Foundation and Michigan Intercollegiate Athletic Association. It is accredited by the North Central Association of Colleges and Secondary Schools. The Teacher Preparation Program is approved by both the Michigan State Department of Education and the National Council for the Accredi- ta tion of Teacher Education. Olivet has a relationship to the Congregational Church and the United Church of Cl‘irist, although its organization is that of an indepen— dent, private college directed by a Board of Trustees. OIL ivet College has a faculty of approximately sixty-five metnbers of which approximately 30 per cent possess their doctorate. Samples Emplpyed by Study The population of Students for the study include a1 1 new students of the fall term, 1972, who had been con firmed for admission prior to the deadline date for College entry. This universe includes freshman, transfer, commuter and residential students. The only Q'a-":egory not included would be that of students return— ing to Olivet after a period of absence. The original :6 igure for group division purposes of the defined popu- a“e“‘tion was 256 students. However, as a number of stu- a‘Ql'lts had confirmed at several colleges, the actual e altlple size for the study was somewhat smaller. h 66 Students forming the sample for the study were randomly assigned to their respective groups. After the deadline date, late entering students were arbitrarily assigned to disjunct groups. The sample of students included in the measures of the study involving most effective and least effective groupings comprised 50 per cent, i.e. 128 students, of 'the above group. The sample group showed characteristics liighly similar to those of the defined population, i.e. c>f the total class. Characteristics of the 1972 incoming class86 kaased on percents include the following: (1) 52.5 per cent are male and 47.5 per cent are female; (2) 92 per cent are included in l8-l9-year—old bracket; (3) 95.6 per cent are White/Caucasian; (4) 50 per cent of their fathers had attended college; (5) 40 per cent of their mothers had attended college; (6) 64.4 per cent came from small or moderate sized towns; 86American Council of Education, Summary of Data EE}_JEntering Students, Olivet College, FalI, 1972. (7) (8) (9) (10) (ll) (12) (l3) (14) 67 78 per cent graduated from classes of less than 500 students; 57 per cent are Protestant and 13 per cent are Catholic; 53.8 per cent ranked at the tOp quarter of their high school class; 96 per cent attended public schools; 40 per cent came from distances of over 100 miles; 30 per cent expressed interest in fine arts as a major, 11 per cent in education, 11 per cent in social sciences; 58 per cent classify themselves as middle of the road in current political preference; 22 per cent are undecided as to possible career occupations. Sample of Leaders The Leaders for the small group orientation Program were selected from approximately 100 upperclass students, who had applied for the position of student adviser . Each spring semester, an attempt is made to encourage as many students to apply for the position of adViser as possible. The 100 applicants represent approximately 25 per cent of the upperclass students 68 living on campus and include students who represent the academic, vocational and social aspects of the campus. There are approximately twenty-seven actual positions available, but all candidates completing the program are rank ordered to provide for both a selection and an alternate selection list. Additional orientation group leaders are selected from the alternate list. During the evaluation process, potential advisers Jreceived a comprehensive generalized leadership training Iprogram. This included a job description review, par- t:icipatory decision-making in regulations, human relation- :ship training, drug information sessions, small group vnork on case problems and individual work on potential ruesidence hall situations. The program lasted for ten weeks and served to act as a screening device for those applicants who were not highly motivated. The final evaluation was accomplished through the use of a leader- Ship profile (see Appendix B) which includes ratings on aSJ€3~class-experience, academic standing, attendance, Eiéi\fisors' ratings, interview and testing. After selection, J-eiéidems were randomly assigned to the various categories <:’IE leadership for the exploratory study and were given £31F>€ecific behavioral instructions in their category of l e adership . Thirty—two advisers and alternates were selected f0): orientation leadership positions (sixteen male and 69 sixteen female). Data on sixteen or 50 per cent of the student leaders was utilized in the portion of the study dealing with.most effective and least effective leadership. Representativeness of Sample of the Defined POpulations With the exception of a few students, the entire ‘population comprises the sample for data collected for certain of the exploratory general questions and is *therefore highly representative of the population. {Those students who enrolled after the deadline date .and necessarily missed the majority of orientation Iprogramming were not included in the data. The sample used for the most and least effective lxeaders involved approximately 50 per cent of the leaders and students. Thus, this sample also is highly repre— sentative of their respective defined pOpulations. Adequacy of Sample Size The study was designed to examine the data at till£a exploratory study level of consideration. Therefore, Small sample theory could be employed. The number, .r1 _§ 30, of leaders is adequate under the terms of small £3iaumple theory and for exploratory study efforts. Data Collection The instrumentalities and the procedures used for ‘tille collection of the data employed in the study are as 1E(allows: 70 Instrumentalities The instruments used to collect the data in the study were (1) Leadership profile scores; (2) Cognitive Style testing; (3) Orientation group evaluation; (4) Leader self-evaluation; (5) Observation of leadership behavior and (6) Composite interview reaction sheet. The Leadership Profile provided a composite score for leadership potential and was utilized for selection of both advisers and alternates. The basis for measure- ment included age-class-experience, academic standing, attendance, advisor ratings, interview and testing. All measurements were converted to a four-point scale. In the age-class-experience category, applications were reviewed and two of the four points were ascribed to Exrevious counseling experience and two points were ascribed to current chronological age and class standing. The academic standing refers to the cumulative grade point average to date. Attendance at selection meetings Was converted to the four-point scale. In the adviser ratings, a list of all applicants was submitted to the then current student advisers. They were asked to simply cfleck yes or no or no knowledge as to whether a candi- f a.cognitive style map for an individual. Thus the process of "mapping" a student‘s cognitive style not 1? these results into elements found in the "map." This instrument employed is in effect a card S5<>rt composed at Oakland Community College covering the tzllltee sets of cognitive style: symbols and their mean— jLI‘sys, cultural determinants and modalities of inference. A total of 216 responses were secured to gather data iEJEWDm which the cognitive style map of three sets of Li‘I‘dfonmation was produced. (The card sort was adminis-. taxed at Olivet College and the results sent to Oakland QQImmunity College for scoring and processing.) Cognitive and Cer in CO IE (I) 72 style testing utilizing the regular battery, and also the card sort techniques, have been given to approxi- mately 35,000 students at Oakland Community College. Reliability and validity indices on these instruments and procedures are available from the Diagnostic Testing Center of the College. Sample questions are illustrated in the Appendix F. The Orientation Group Evaluation was a locally <:onstructed instrument designed to glean information :regarding leader behavior and student reactions to the Lleadership behaviors and orientation programming for this astudy. It was especially designed to provide a cross <1heck on specific leader behavior and to provide data vwhich were not available in the pilot study made in 1971. Students were also asked to indicate their satisfaction regarding leadership, group construction, program effec- tiveness and attendance on a seven-point continuum (see Appendix C). The Leader Self-evaluation was also constructed JL<3<3ally. It served the purpose of specifying the various ID‘EIJaviors desired in the leadership patterns as a leadership training device and also recorded the J“Qader's observations regarding himself and his satis— Zifiamction with his leadership assignments, the method of gt‘(Jup construction and his evaluation of the program's a‘Sszistance to new students. In addition, the leader 73 reported the type of leadership role and group con- struction which he felt would be best for his or herself (see Appendix D). The Observation of Leadership Behavior form paralleled the two previously mentioned forms and served as a cross check also for verification of leader behavior. Its use was confined to the professional observers. The Student Counselor, Associate Dean of Students and Dean of Students served as the observors. Each evaluated the groups independently and on more than one occasion. Specific behaviors were recorded, as well as a desig- 1nation made of a leadership rating based on a seven-point continuum (see Appendix E) . The Composite Interview Reaction Sheet was con— sstructed after completion of thirty-minute individual .iJlterviews with leaders. They were conducted upon the <3c>nclusion of the orientation program. Comments and £31mggestions that had some frequency were categorized and resubmitted to the student leaders. They were asked to agree or disagree with the written statements and to make any further suggestions desired. The Composite j~lirterview reaction is a variation of the technique of " brainstorming." The purpose of the interviews was to 9a in information regarding leader feelings and per- Q'eptions. The leader self-evaluation was used as an :i4r1itiating tool for the interview and the student leaders 74 were encouraged to make evaluative judgments, as well as suggestions. A special emphasis was made to determine preference for leadership style and group construction. A complete list of the responses is available in Appendix G. The locally constructed instruments were designed to complement each other and to arrive at data specifi- cally for this study. The procedures in implementing these instruments follow in the next section. .Procedures The main procedures of data collection employed :in the study are as follows: 1. Applications were taken and processed for leader positions. This included campus-wide notification of the coming selection process and securing nominations from the current advisory staff. The nominees were then invited to join the selection process. Every attempt was made to secure the largest possible number of applicants. 2. A general inservice training program was con- ducted for all candidates. This included case studies, human relations training, Specialty topics, job description review and sample testing. The program was conducted by the student personnel staff members under the direction of the Dean of 75 Students. Make-up sessions were available for students who missed meetings due to the schedule conflicts or illness. Upon completion of the inservice training pro- gram, students were assigned to adviser or alter- nate positions by their composite profile score. The professional staff had the opportunity to change any rankings so desired. For the second successive year, no rankings were changed. Assignments were completed prior to the end of the college year (June, 1972). During the fall inservice training program, the student leaders were informed of the design of the leadership study to be conducted within the orientation program. They were appraised of the inconclusive results of the previous study and the importance of carrying out the behavioral characteristics of their particular leadership category. Part of this appraisal included a legitimizing of each of the methods to be used in the leadership study. They then took part in the random selection of leadership assignments. Specific behavioral training of directive and nondirective leaders was given separately. The leadership behaviors listed on the leader 76 evaluation sheet were emphasized and examples and demonstrations used where necessary. New students were randomly assigned to their respective groups. The build groups were assigned to a general pool while the assigned groups were assigned to either a directive assigned or nondirective assigned group at random. The general pool was necessary to allow for the "building" sequence which was to follow. Stu- dents receiving assigned groups received an orientation letter to report to the gymnasium while students in the build classification received notice to report to the Collegiate Center. Upon arrival at their respective assign— ments the students were encouraged to socialize with each other via a socializing technique based on the occult sciences. This allowed for the arrival of late comers and the usual last- minute mix-ups. Parents at this same time were being hosted by administrative officers at a program in the Mott Auditorium. After the socializing period of approximately thirty minutes, the students in the gym were brought together and briefly welcomed. They were then told that the orientation program was a require— ment for all new students and turned over to 77 their respective student leader. For the duration of the program, attendance was emphasized in the directive groups and no further mention was made‘ in the nondirective groups.v The groups assigned to the Collegiate Center followed the same socializing format. However, after being brought together and the same attendance announcement made, they participated in "building" their own groups and selecting their own leaders. Both groups followed the same pro- gram format which consisted of both small and large group activities. Initiation of the Orientation Program began with a concentrated three and one—half days (morning, afternoon, evening) sessions and once each week for seven weeks thereafter to coincide with the mid-term marking period. Observations were made independently of the leadership styles by three professional observers (Counselor, Associate Dean of Students and Dean of Students). The Counselor and the Associate Dean of Students were new to the campus. Leaders were cognitively mapped using the Oakland Community College Card Sort and the computer facilities of Oakland Community College. 78 10. During the seventh week of the program, the Orientation Group Evaluation sheet was administered. 11. During the eighth week, the leader self-evaluation sheet was administered and the interview conducted. 12. During the ninth week, the Composite interview sheet was constructed and distributed to the student leaders. The responses were then col— lected and tabulated. Summary of Design of Study This is an exploratory small sample study search— ing for indications and implications of the posited general questions. One of the main purposes is to determine whether the educational science of cognitive style can indicate differences in most effective and least effec— tive leadership and if so, what might these differences be. Thus, collective cognitive style for most effective and least effective leaders will be ascertained by sur- veying each of the cognitive maps of the individual leaders. Predominant elements within maps served to form a collective cognitive style and these styles were then compared for differentiating elements. The next step was to pose some possible alternatives in the use of cognitive style in predicting effectiveness of leadership or probable style of leadership. 79 The effect of directive and nondirective leader- ship, as well as method of group constructions is of importance and this was measured by the Komolgorov- Smirnov statistical technique. Non— Non- Directive Directive Directive Directive Assigned Assigned Build Build MEsnl (X) LESn2 (X) Maximum Deviation Fnl (x) - Sn2(x) Figure 3.--Design model for measuring effectiveness of leaders Likewise, the leadership style and method of group instruction will be compared with the attrition rate and the significance of required attendance investigated. The responses to the Composite Interviews were tabled and a general summary statement composed from the findings. This should be of assistance for the continuing research on the Olivet Orientation Program. The design of the study is such that it is to provide information for intelligent adjustments to an ongoing program and to provide some investigative 80 possibilities as well as lend support to existing theory. A model of the statistical technique used and the find— ings follow in the next chapter. CHAPTER IV ANALYSIS OF DATA AND FINDINGS Analysis of Data The data yielded by the study were those of (l) the descriptive, qualitative type statements; (2) the nominal scale of measurement; (3) the ordinal scale of measurement; and (4) the interval scale of measurement. Descriptive-qualitative data are of primary importance in answering the first two general questions of the study. The remaining data which deal with ratings of leaders' personal preferences, attrition rate and attendance were distributed over the nominal, ordinal and interval scales of measurement. Cognizant that the majority of the data collected is of the nominal and ordinal level of measure- ment, nonparametric tests have been employed. Analytic Techniques Employed The statistical measurements utilized in this study were primarily directed at the most effective and least effective leaders. This is a modified technique 81 82 consistent with the report of Flanagan87 concerning satisfactory approximation to the biserial coefficient through the use of upper and lower groups. The chart utilized in the procedure was based on the findings of Kelly88 that upper and lower groups containing 27 per cent of the cases were optimum for certain related esti- mations. Individual cognitive maps were surveyed to deter- mine a collective cognitive style for both the most effective and least effective leader-categories. After determining collective cognitive maps for both cate- gories, they in turn were surveyed for differences which might attribute to leader effectiveness. Testing the styles of leadership in terms of most and least effec- tiveness was accomplished by the use of the Komolgorov- Smirnov two-sample test, which compares the distribution of relative frequencies over the stated categories. It is a test of whether two independent samples have been drawn from the same population (or from pOpulations with the same distribution). This process is described by Siegel: 87Flanagan, gp. cit., pp. 674-80. 88T. L. Kelly, "The Selection of Upper and Lower Groups for the Validation of Test Items," Journal of Educational Psychology: XXX (1939), 17-24. 83 . . . The two-tailed test is sensitive to any kind of difference in the distributions from which the two samples were drawn——difference in location (central tendency), in dispersion, in skewness, etc. . . . If the two samples have in fact been drawn from the same population distribution, then the cumulative distributions of both samples may be expected to be fairly close to each other inasmuch as they both should show only random deviations from the population distribution. . . . When com- pared to the t-test, the Komolgorov-Smirnov test has high power efficiency (about 96%) for small samples. . . . seems to be more powerful in all 89 cases than either the X2 test or the median test. The difference in the one sample test is that it compares the distribution with the theoretical relative distri— bution rather than that of another sample. The following table illustrates the method used in the study: TABLE 2.--Two sample KomolgorOV-Smirnov statistical tech- niques . . . . Non- Non- gggicfiége Dlgfiiiéve Directive Directive g Assigned Build Most . l 2 7 8 Effective —§—- .§_. _§_. .5. S (x) 81 Least Effective —§— —§— —§— —%— S (x) 8 8 8 82 DeMi‘fit’Efi‘fi 4 .3... 1 _0_ V 8“ 8 '§" 0 Snl(x)-Sn2(x) 89 Sidney Siegel, Nonparametric Statistics for Behavioral Sciences (New York: McGraw Hill Book Co., 1956). pp. 127-36. 84 Testable Hypothesis The statistical inference process employed was as follows: 1. Null hypothesis: HO:RF = RF2 i.e.; in 1 statement form--the expected relative frequencies of the most effective leaders nl are equal to the expected rela- tive frequencies of the least effective leaders n2 for the k categories covering the range of leadership styles. The statistical alternative hypothesis is H :RFl # RF 1 2’ i.e. the expected relative frequencies of the most effective leaders is not equal to the expected relative frequencies of the least effective leaders. 2. Statistical Test: Two small independent samples of equal sizes (n =n =8) are to be compared on a l 2 categorical basis, hence the Komolgorov-Smirnov two— sample test is an appropriate one. 3. Level of Significance: (a) The level of significance is .05. 4. Sampling Distribution: A complete table of 90 critical values is listed by Siegel. 5. Critical Region: Since the null hypothesis HO and the statistical alternative hypothesis H1 do not 90 Ibid., p. 278. 85 predict direction, a two-tailed test is used. The null hypothesis is rejected if the value for KD for the maximum deviation is of such magnitude (equal to or larger than the appropriate KD in the table) that the probability associated with its occurrence, if no is supposedly true, is equal to or greater than the critical values found for D if a = .05. 6. Compute the value of the statistic: The value of KD with which this form of the Komolgorov- 'Smirnov test is concerned is the numerator of the maximum deviation D which appears in the bottom row of each indi- vidual table (in the sample form it is 4 from D = —%—u 7. Decision concerning H0: Since the critical value of KD for n = n = 8 under the two-tailed test 1 2 employed at the a = .05 level of significance is: KD = 6 and the value of KD yielded by the sample data is KD = 4, the null hypothesis cannot be rejected. Additional data deemed to have import for the study have been reported in descriptive forms and in tables of nominative data which are presented in the response to the general questions of the study. The following findings are listed in order of the posed questions. Findings The findings regarding the first two general questions are presented together as they are related to each other. 1. How can the educational science of cogpitive 86 These questions are: style be used in the process of predicting effective leadership? 2. How do the most effective leaders differ from the least effective leaders? The collective cognitive style for the most effec- tive leaders constructed from the eight most effective leaders using a modified technique described by Flanagan91 is as follows: (AL) (V) (T) (S) (0) (CT) (CS) (CP) (CK) (CH) (CET) (CES) (CEM) (A) oooooooooooooe' .. .1 Figure 4.-—Most effective leader collective cognitive style 91 - AI 1! J ml Flanagan, gp. cit., pp. 674-80. D ml J 87 The collective cognitive style for the least effective leaders selected in the same fashion is as follows: T '(u)u.) '1" (AQ) T (VL) '1" we) r J F T (S) (0) I R (CT) (GS) X F' x D' (cx) (CET) A' M. (CES) (CEM) (CKH) _ _. L. J ._ J Figure 5.-—Least effective leader collective cognitive style LQ ll 00000000006 The differences within the first set of symbols and their meanings occur in both the theoretical and qualitative symbols.' The least effective leaders have either majors or minors in all four theoretical symbols while the most effective category has a minor in T' AL. The most effective category has the following qualitative symbols not evidenced in the least effective category--Q (V), Q (CP), Q (CH), Q (A). The least effec- tive category has a Q (CKH) not evidenced in the most effective category. In the second set of cultural determinants the difference lies in the I rotating from the major position 88 in the least effective group to the weakest minor position in the most effective group. I FA F' A| A' I' Least Effective Most Effective In the third set of modalities of inferences the difference lies in the fact that the most effective group has a major D and an L while the least effective group has a minor D. Thus, the differences are noted below with the least effective group distinguished by underlining: ' 1 _. - r - T' (A0) T (VL) T' (VQ) , Q (V) F _I_ Q (CP) x A' g; x D L Q (CH) 0 (A) (QCKH) 1' a; D! L. .J _ o _._- J Figure 6.--Differences in cognitive styles of most effective and least effective leaders In addition to the comparison of collective cog- nitive styles the most effective and least effective leader samples were compared over the randomly assigned categories of leadership. The following statistical results are presented using the Komolgorov-Smirnov two sample statistical test. 89 In each of the tables the null hypothesis states that there is no difference between most effective and least effective leader categories. TABLE 3.--Differences in leadership style and group con- struction of most effective and least effective leaders Directive Directive Non- Non- Assi ned Build Directive Directive g Assigned Build Most Effective _Z_ 3 6 8 581(X) Least Effective _§_. 4 5 8 532(X) Maximum . . l 1 l 0 DeViation —§—- —§— _§_ _§_ Snl(x)-Sn2(x) The numerator of the largest deviation is l. The Table of Critical Values of KD in the Komolgorov-Smirnov two-sample test indicates a value of 6 is necessary at the a level of .05. Therefore, the null hypothesis cannot be rejected. With regard to the differences between male and female leadership, the following table is presented: 90 TABLE 4.--Differences between most effective and least effective leaders according to sex Male Female Most Effective —1— —§— 8 8 831(X) Least 0 8 Effective —§— —§—- 532(X) Maximum 7* 0 Deviation 8 —§— Sn1(x)’5n2(x) The numerator of the largest deviation is 7. The Table of Critical Values indicates that this value is highly significant at the a level of .01 in a two- tailed test. The null hypothesis is rejected. The third question asks: Do new students show a preference for leadership style and group con- struction? Leadership style was rated on a seven-point con— tinuum with seven, very satisfactory; five, satisfactory; three, somewhat satisfactory; one, unsatisfactory. The sample of thirty—two groups, sixteen in each category represent all of the new students in the study. The following table presents the mean satisfaction of groups by leadership style: 91 TABLE 5.--Difference in preference of new students for leadership style New Student Satisfaction Leadership Style 6.5 6.0 5.5 5.0 4.5 Non-Directive l 6 10 14 16 5161 (X) l6 16 I6 I6 16 Directive 2 6 ll 15 16 5162 (X) 16 16 l6 16 16 Maximum Deviation l 0 l l 0 Sn1 (X) Sn2 (X) 16 16 16 16 16 The numerator of the largest fraction in the Maximum Deviation column is l and the Table of Critical Values calls for a value of 8 when n = 16, a = .05, two- tailed. Thus, the HO: Snl = Sn2 cannot be rejected. Group construction satisfaction was rated on a similar continuum of seven, very satisfactory to one, unsatisfactory and also includes all groups. The table shown on the following page presents the mean satis- faction of groups with regard to group construction. The numerator of the largest fraction in the Maximum Deviation column is four and the Table of Critical Values establishes a value of eight when n = 16, a = .05 two-tailed. Thus, the HO: S = S cannot nl ——-——— n2 be rejected. The fourth question is: Do leaders show a preference of leadership style and group construction? 92 TABLE 6.--Differences in preference of new students for group construction New Student Satisfaction Group Construction 6.0 5.5 5.0 4.5 4.0 3.5 3.0 2.5 Assigned 2 5 l9. 14 14 15 15 15 8151 (X) 16 16 l6 l6 l6 l6 16 16 Build 4 9 13 15 16. 16 16 16 5152 (X) 16 l6 l6 l6 16 16 l6 16 Maximum Deviation 2 4 3 l 2 l l 0 Sn1 (X) - Sn2 (X) l6 16 16 16 16 16 16 16 The responses indicate the preference of leaders for leadership and group construction according to their randomly selected categories. The findings are presented in Table 7 and include all leaders. Using the Komolgorov-Smirnov one-sample test, the following statistic (Table 8) is computed for leader- ship preference, N = 32. The Maximum Deviation is 9/32 which is .281. Table E92 shows that for N = 32, D Z .281 has an associated probability under Ho of p = .01. Thus, the HO ig rejected at the a = .01 level. Using the one—sample test, the following statistic (Table 9) is computed for Group Construction Preference, N = 29. 92Siegel, gp. cit., p. 251. 93 TABLE 7.—-Frequency distribution of leadership style and group construction preference of leaders Leadership Preference Group Construction Preference Directive Non- No Leader- Directive Assigned Build Prefer- ship Leadership ence Directive Assigned 3 5 3 3 2 Non- Directive Assigned 2 6 4 3 l Directive Build 3 5 0 8 Non- Directive Build 1 7 l 7 Totals 9 23 ~ 8. 21 3 TABLE 8.--Leadership style preference of leaders Directive Non-Directive Leadership Leadership 9 23 F (X) 1_8 .32 ° 32 32 S (X) 9 32 32 3'2‘ 32 F (X) - s (X) _g* 0 ° 32 32 3'2 94 TABLE 9.--Leader-group construction preference of leaders Assigned Build 8 21 F0 (X) 14.5 14.5 29 29 s (X) 8 29 29 '2'9 ‘29" F (X) - s (x) 6.5 0 ° 29 29" 2'9 The Maximum Deviation is 6.5/29 which is .234. Table E shows that for N = 29, a = .05, the value called for is circa .24. At the a = .05 level the Ho cannot be rejected although it could be rejected at the a = .10 level. This situation of .10 5 5 .05 places the hypothesis in doubt. Question Number Five: Does leadership and group construction methods affect the first semester attrition rate? The Komolgorov-Smirnov one—sample test is used to measure the attrition rate over the categories of leadership and group construction. The reported numbers are indicated in the following table. 95 TABLE lO.—-Differences between leadership and group con— struction relative to first semester attrition Leadership and Group Construction Directive Non- Directive Non- Assi ned Directive Build Directive g Assigned Build Attrition Frequency 8 5 4 7 Frequency _2 12 £9 £3 Under H0 24 24 24 24 S24 (X) _3. L2 _l_7_ a 24 24 24 24 Maximum Deviation _g _L l 0 FO(X) - 824 (X) 24 24 5;- fl— The Table of Critical Values93 of D in the Komolgonov-Smirnov one-sample test establishes the values for N = 24, a = .05 to be circa .27. The computed value of the Maximum Deviation is 2/24 or .085. Thus the H : DB = NDB = DB = NDB cannot be rejected. Question Number six: Does the use of directive or nondirective leadership affect the attendance rate of programs? Attendance was rated on a seven-point continuum with seven representing all orientation meetings; five, 93Ibid., p. 251. 96 most; three, some and one, none or very few. The table reveals attendance within the categories of leadership style and group construction. TABLE ll.--Frequency distribution of attendance within categories of leadership style and group construction Group Attendance Means 6.5 6.0 5.5 5.0 4.5 4.0 3.5 3.0 Directive Assigned 0 2 l l 2 2 0 0 Directive Build 1 0 2 4 l 0 0 0 Non-Directive Assigned 0 1 3 3 1 0 0 0 Non-Directive Build 0 0 3 l 2 l O l A comparison of directive leadership in which attendance was visible and emphasized with nondirective leadership in which attendance was not emphasized and not visible is reflected in Table 12. The numerator of the largest fraction in the Maximum Deviation is two. The Table of Critical Values calls for a value of eight for N = 16 a = .05. Thus, the null hypothesis cannot be rejected. Complete results of the composite interview responses are included in the appendix. In Table 13 are listed those responses relating directly to the study. 97 TABLE 12.--Differences between directive and nondirective leadership in terms of attendance Group Attendance Means 6.5 6.0 5.5 5.0 4.5 4.0 3.5 3.0 Directive _1_ _3. _6. 1.1. 1.4. 16 1.6. 3.9 Sn16 (X)l 16 16 16 16 16 l6 l6 l6 Non-Directive _0 _1 '41 ll 14 15 15_ 16. Sn16 (X)2 16 16 l6 l6 16 16 16 16 Maximum Deviation _l _2 _l _0 0 _l. _1‘ _Q 16 16 16 16 1—6 16 l6 l6 Snl(X) - Sn2(x) 98 TABLE l3.--Composite interview results Responses Statements Agree Disagree 1. The Leader should size up the group needs and go according to their needs always looking for the group to take over. 30 2 2. Start Directive in leadership and end Non-Directive (Situational Leadership) 25 3 3. Directive is best as that's what they are used to. 5 25 4. Since students are eventually going to be on their own, they should have the experience of non— directive groups. 20 13 5. I was successful in going according to the specific behavioral objec- tives. 18 ll 6. I feel the inservice training given prior to the O-Group experience was adequate. 24 7 Groupronstruction 7. Students would prefer to be members of build groups. 27 10 8. I feel eight plus a leader is an ideal size group. 25 6 9. There is less confusion in assign— ing the groups. 23 9 10. There should be one football player, one commuter, and one transfer student in each group. 5 26 ll. Equalize the sexes in the groups. 25 6 12. Have interaction between O—Groups. 32 0 99 TABLE l3.--Continued Responses Statements Agree Disagree Attendance 13. Stress the Attendance-—more reluc— tance, but they enjoy it when they are there. 16 ll 14. Required attendance is the only way to go. 5 22 15. Students have a feeling, "unless it goes on my record, it's unimportant." l4 l7 l6. "Optional and good" is the secret - to programming. 22 5 Length of Program 17. There is value in having the program go until midterm. l7 14 18. The program is too long. Have it twice a week for three weeks. 18 13 19. Meet fewer times but have some sub- stance to the meeting. 22 10 20. The meetings should be bi—weekly, alternating with the Convocation Program. 19 ll 21. Set up a special night and time for O-Group meeting to eliminate hassle of deciding the meeting dates. 22 9 22. Start the Orientation Program early for those who come early. 13 17 23. Initially have four days rather than three-~spread out activities with more free time. 26 4 24. Go three or four weeks and then have a "reunion" at midterm. 25 7 25. Get the program to end on a climactic note, rather than downhill. 31 l 100 TABLE l3.--Continued Responses Statements Agree Disagree Special Group Problems 26. Coming early by Football and Music students affects their attitude nega- tively toward the Program. 21 9 27. Upperclassmen affect new students' attitudes toward orientation nega— tively. 25 7 28. There should be a separate program for commuters. 26 6 29. There should be separate programs for music students. 13 19 30. There should be separate programs for football players. 20 ll 31. Separating groups out will destroy a sense of community. ‘ 12 19 32. There should be separate programs for transfer students. 30 l 33. Orient transfer students to a dif- ferent school rather than to college. 25 7 34. Program should be shorter for transfer students. 27 4 Leader Benefits 35. I learned a lot about myself as a result of O—Group experience. 27 2 36. The experience has given me new confidence. 23 7 37. I have become more sensitive in my relationship with peOple. 29 3 38. I now have several close, freshman friends. 26 4 39. I felt that all of a sudden I was supposed to act five years older. 9 22 101 As was noted in Chapter I, specific behavioral actions for the leadership styles had not been specified in the pilot study. This was remedied in this study and a chart appears in Appendix A indicating the evalua- tive scoring of observors, leaders and orientation groups on specifically designated leadership behaviors. Observors' scores, of necessity, had to be taken in the first three days of the program when directive leadership was more likely to occur. In cases of dis- crepancy of leadership style, each specific behavioral action was rechecked in the interview. The conclusion is that student leaders are able to lead groups in styles which can be clearly differentiated and did so in this study. Chapter V which follows presentsthe summary and conclusions to the data presented in this chapter. CHAPTER V SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS Leader selection and leadership methods have been the object of intensive study, particularly since the end of WOrld War II. The controversy between the "traitists," "situationalists" and "behaviorists" continues unabated and the search for effective methods of leadership pre- diction is still a priority item for managers and deans alike. One of the purposes of this study was to deter- mine whether the educational science of cognitive style could be used to predict effective leadership. A second purpose, still focusing on effectiveness, was to compare the directive and nondirective leadership styles in "assigned" and "build" small groups within an orientation program. This study involved almost all of the new stu— dents of the entering 1972 Fall class of Olivet College and the upperclass orientation leaders. The new students (n = 256) consisted of resident, commuter and transfer students. The upperclass leaders (n = 32) were chosen from approximately 100 student adviser candidates. 102 103 The measures used in the study were (1) Leader- ship profile for leader selections; (2) Cognitive style testing; (3) Orientation group evaluation; (4) Leader self-evaluation; (5) Observation of leadership behavior and (6) Composite interview reaction. All instruments, except the cognitive style testing battery for this study, were designed by the investigator. The cognitive style testing battery was composed at Oakland Community College and the instrument used was in effect a card sort. The results were sent to Oakland Community College for scoring and processing. Six general questions were posed for exploration and they are listed under the rubric of findings and conclusions. Where these questions generated a hypothe- sis, the hypothesis was tested: e.g., when there is a difference in leadership style or group construction, there will be a difference in (1) student satisfactiOn and (2) attendance or attrition rate. The Kmolgorov- Smirnov statistical test was used to test the null hypothesis form of this operational hypothesis a = .05 level of significance with the appropriate degrees of freedom. The emphasis on leadership and group construction has been a part of an action research program at Olivet College for several years. The objectives of this program are to chart and explore new ideas for improving 104 an existing orientation program and to eliminate alterna- tives which seemingly have little chance of increasing the effectiveness of the program. The focus on the exploratory nature of the study will be evident in the findings, conclusions, discussion, implications and recommendations. The specific findings of the study are stated immediately after each question and are presented together with the inferred conclusions for ease of reference and continuity. Findings and Conclusions Question 1: How can the educational science of cog- nitive style be used in the process of predicting .effective leadership? This study has shown, through the use of the technique described by Flanagan that the science of cog- nitive style can differentiate between the most effective and the least effective leaders. These differentiations are indicated and discussed in question number two. Thus, two distinct possibilities are readily apparent for the use of the educational science of cog- nitive style: 1. Cognitive Style can be used as a basis for selection after an appropriate collective cog- nitive base had been established. 105 2. Cognitive Style can be used to prescribe certain programs to increase the probability of effective leadership, i.e., an individual lacking the element Q (CEM) might be assigned to a human relations unit prior to assuming the role of leadership. By utilizing the collective cognitive style to represent the elements needed for effective leadership in a particular situation, it becomes apparent that a “blend" has occurred between the theories advocated by the "traitists," "situationalists” and "behaviorists." Question 2: How do the most effective student leaders differ from the least effective leaders? The elements differentiated within the collective cognitive style of the most effective and least effective leaders occur in all three sets of cognitive style: symbolic orientations, cultural determinants and modali— ties of inference. Within the symbolic orientation of the collective cognitive styles of least effective leaders, a greater number of theoretical orientations appear, while in the most effective leaders a greater number of qualitative orientations occur. Theoretical Symbols While both categories of leadership exhibited a T' (AL), Minor Theoretical Auditory Linguistic, ability 106 to acquire meaning through spoken words, the least effec- tive leadership category also exhibited a T' (AQ), Minor Theoretical Auditory Quantitative, ability to find mean- ing in terms of quantities (numerical symbols and measurements you hear), and T (VL), Major Theoretical Visual Linguistic, ability to find meaning from words you see, and a T' (VQ), Minor Theoretical Visual Quanti- tative, ability to find meaning in terms of quantities (numerical symbols and measurements you hear). The conclusion drawn here is that these extra theoretical elements were not necessary to effective leadership in this situation. Two of these elements are quantitative in nature. Qualitative Symbols The most effective leaders' collective cognitive styles contained four qualitative symbols not evidenced in the least effective leaders‘ collective cognitive style. They include: Q (A) Qualitative Auditory, which is the ability to perceive meaning through the sense of hearing, par- ticularly as it might apply to nondiscussive symbolic forms of sound. This would indicate that the most effective leader is more alert to these sounds and can interpret them in a meaningful way for the benefit of the group members. 107 Q (V) Qualitative Visual, which is the ability to perceive meaning through the sense of sight. Thus, the effective leader has the ability to immediately visualize the group processes in such a manner that he can recall and piece together at a future time, past events which have meaning for the group at a particular point in time. Q (CP) Qualitative Code Proxemics, which is the ability to judge "critical" physical and social distances between himself and others in the act of communication. The effective leader does not alienate his group by misjudging social distance. He can uniquely judge the "comfort" quotient of both the group and himself. Q (CH) Qualitative Code Histrionics, which is the ability to deliberately exhibit a dramatic flair of emotion or temperament to produce some particular effect on or evoke responses from other persons. In short, the most effective leader is a good actor, able to give to the leadership role that quality which is often described as charisma. The qualitative symbol evidenced in the least effective leader's collective cognitive style and not listed in the most effective leader‘s style is: Q (CKH) Qualitative Code Kinesthetics, which relates particularly to motor skills and muscular coordi- nation in which following form is considered of prime importance. 108 Thus, a person with Q (CKH) as a major element is apt to be conscious of emulating a leadership form, without an awareness of its effect upon the group. The emphasis on the process of emulating can lead to a lack of congruence upon the part of the leader and this is quickly sensed by the group. The difference is between "being the leader" and "playing the role" of the leader. Too often, the leader playing the role is perceived by the group as being very mechanical. Cultural Determinants The comparison of the collective cognitive styles shows a marked difference between the most effective and least effective leaders in the cultural determinants set. This difference indicates that the family is the major influence on meaning for the most effective leaders while the individual himself is the major influence for the least effective leader. These positions are reversed in the least effective category of leadership. This influence is a fluid state and it shifts as roles change throughout life and at certain times a par- ticular determinant may play a greater or lesser part in the influence brought to bear on one‘s perceptions and subsequent meanings to symbols. At this point in time, however, the influence of the cultural determinants of the most effective and least effective leaders in this 109 situation differ in the respective amounts of influence brought to bear by the cultural determinants. This can best be illustrated by the following figure: Family Most Effective Least Effective Figure 7.--Differential influence patterns in the cultural determinant set of most and least effective leaders The reversal of the relative importance of the cultural determinants of family and individuality is the significant difference. The most effective leader is much more apt to treat his group as an extension of his familial orientation while the least effective leader is more strongly dominated by a sense of individuality. This strong individual orientation could lead toward satisfying leader needs rather than group needs, thus serving as a barrier to group satisfaction. Modalities of Inference The most effective leaders have two additional elements in how they reach a decision through inductive reasoning. 110 (D) Difference, which suggests a tendency to think in terms of one-to-one contrasts or comparisons of selected comparisons or measurements. Thus, the most effective leader is one who is able to differentiate the members of his group, treating them in one—to-one relationships and considering their individual needs. (L) Appraisal, which indicates that the most effective leader tends to analyze, question or appraise an issue carefully before making a decision. This type of thinking leads to effective decision— making and increased confidence by the group in the leader. The general conclusion is that there are marked differences in each of the sets of cognitive style between the most effective and least effective leaders. These differences lend promise to future research in predicting leadership through the use of cognitive style. Male-Female Leadership One additional difference noted between the most and least effective leaders is that males are signifi— cantly more effective as leaders than are females. This difference is highly significant (p = .01). This finding will have to be interpreted care- fully in the light of the differences in cognitive styles noted above. The differences of cognitive style may have some relationship to the male-female element 111 as there is only one crossover of sex in the most and least effective leader groups. Question 3: Do new students show a_preference for leadership style and group construction? There was no preference exhibited for leadership style as measured by new student satisfaction. There was, however, a tendency to greater satisfaction in the "build" group, but it was not statistically significant. Question 4: Do leaders show a preference for leadership style and group construction? There was a preference for nondirective leadership at a high level of statistical significance (p = .01) among the leaders. The leaders also showed a preference for "build" groups at a lower level of statistical sig- nificance (p = .10). Thirty of thirty-two leaders in the composite interview felt that the leader should size up the group needs and go according to their needs, always looking for the group to take over. A vast majority (90%) felt that the optimum way would be starting the group direc- tively and ending up nondirectively. The leaders felt that there was less confusion in assigning groups, but that new students generally preferred "build" groups. 112 Question 5: Does leadership and group construction affect the first semester attrition rate? Leadership and group construction did not affect the attrition rate. In fact, a tendency toward less attrition in the nondirective build category, which was in evidence in the pilot study was not in evidence in this study. Question 6: Does the use of directive or nondirective leadership_affect the attendance rate? There was no significant difference in attendance between the directive and nondirective groups. During the orientation program, attendance in the directive groups was emphasized and visibly recorded. In the nondirective groups it was not emphasized nor was it recorded. Leaders in the latter group did, however, covertly keep a record of attendance. During the leader interviews, special group problems became evident which directly and indirectly affected attendance. One of these problems was the general feeling that upper class students had a negative effect upon new students concerning orientation. Other special problems included groupings for transfer and com- muter students as well as for football players to increase attendance probability. This support, however, 113 did not hold true for music students reporting to the campus for early rehearsals. Student leaders also do not feel that required attendance is the only way to go, yet more than half of them feel that they would stress attendance despite the negative overtones it might create. The length of the program has some bearing on the attendance and leaders concluded that increased earlier programming and some modification of length might be beneficial. The leaders also felt that the orientation program for transfer students should be modified to orient them to a different college rather than to college, generally. Lastly, student leaders indicated that they benefited greatly from the leadership education and experience. Discussion and Implications The marked difference between the cognitive styles of the most effective and least effective leaders would imply that the educational science of cognitive style could be a useful tool in predicting effective leadership. Care would have to be exercised in order to be certain that the method of collective cognitive style measurement is isomorphic to a predictive leadership pattern. For example, the problem of weighting elements could be a 114 crucial factor, as well as the relative significance of each of the three sets and eventually the fourth set of memory—concern. In addition to predicting effective leadership in a given situation, it would be possible through the matching of an individual's cognitive style to the collective cognitive style, to determine areas of potential leadership weakness. Leader education programs could therefore tailor their instruction to these specific areas to insure greater probability of effective leader- ship. The differences in the collective cognitive styles of the most effective and least effective leaders definitely emphasized the qualitative aspect of effective leadership. The increased number of qualitative symbols, the familial cultural determinant and the onus on dif- ferences as well as appraisal processes in the inductive approaches to decision-making all indicate that "con- sideration" of group members appears to be a unifying thread throughout the pattern of differences. The analysis of data shows that whether the group is led directively or nondirectively is unimportant, but that the leader's feeling of true concern is all-important. Evidently, the new student has sufficient motivation and the flexibility to adapt to either a directive or nondirective process. 115 The first and foremost implication is that the purposes and objectives of the orientation program must be well planned, delineated and rank ordered according to priority. If the effectiveness of the program is the prime consideration, then initial selection is vital and the results of this study would indicate greater use of male leaders. If the orientation process is also con- ceived of as an educational venture seeking to present equality of opportunity and self-improvement of leaders, then assessment and education for leadership would be of all importance. Normally, an orientation program is established to decrease the attrition rate. However, if this cannot be statistically substantiated and a certain amount of information must be assimilated by new students, then another possibility which is a little more progressive would be to utilize the program as an educational device for those deficient in leadership skills, i.e., in this study, greater opportunity for women. This study indicates that it is immaterial whether a leader leads directively or nondirectively so the stu- dent should be allowed to lead in the manner that best fits his own personality. However, the leadership edu- cation program should present the facets of both directive and nondirective leadership so that the stu- dents are aware of the dimensions of each style, as well 116 as the elements which contribute to effective leadership. The program should definitely attempt to eliminate areas of potential leadership weakness. The strong support by leaders of a situational type of leadership in which they start directively and end nondirectively should be explored. Although not indicated to be statistically sig- nificant, there was a definite trend toward greater satisfaction in the "build" groups. This also was evi- denced in the composite interviews of leaders. Fifty per cent of the new students were not members of "build" groups and therefore did not share the experience. Stu- dents expect their college experience to be different; they expect to participate in decision-making and build groups contain both elements. The tendency toward greater satisfaction in the "build" groups would imply additional experimentation with build groups in terms of size and composition. Kronovet's94 study had indicated that 80 per cent of the institutions required attendance at orientation programs. In essence, this study would favor the abolition of emphasized attendance in favor of an initial statement of "expectancy of attendance as a student seeking a college education." At this initial meeting, the purposes and objectives of the orientation program 94Kronovet, pp. cit. 117 would be clearly defined and the benefits from the program enumerated. The program as designed would have to be meaningful to the student and it is hypothesized that his motivation at this particular time plus a meaningful program structured within small groups would insure satisfactory attendance. The data in this study support this hypothesis. Implication for future research would include ascertaining collective cognitive styles for a variety of situations in which selection is a necessity, and then testing the predictive effectiveness of the collective cognitive style with the individual candidate. Another study could ascertain the cognitive styles of individuals and match them with a previously defined collective cog- nitive style. Then, using control and experimental groupings, a leadership education program could be administered with a subsequent remapping of the cognitive style to determine changes. Differences in effectiveness could then be measured between the groups. A specialized study could be one to investigate the backgrounds of the most and least effective leaders in respect to F (family) and I (individual) majors in the cultural determinants set. Recommendations This has been an exploratory study in the use of the educational science of cognitive style as a predictor 118 of group leadership within an orientation program. As such, it has indicated that effective leadership has a strong relationship to the qualitative elements that represent a leader's cognitive style. In view of the results of this study, the following recommendations are made: 1. A related study using a similar population base should be undertaken to test these findings in a predictive situation. A leadership education program should be created based upon the evaluation and subsequent re— evaluation of an individual's cognitive style toward increased effectiveness in leadership. Special leadership training and experience for female students should be made available. The emphasis for effective leadership in an orientation program should focus on consideration and concern, i.e., human relations. The idea of starting directively, when the initial impact is overwhelming to a new student and gradually becoming nondirective as he becomes more secure has the ingredients of a situational leadership style which is the method most preferred by student leaders and could serve as the starting point of a leadership education program. 8. 119 The emphasis on required attendance should be modified, a simple statement indicating that students are expected to assume responsibility for their attendance should suffice as long as the purposes and objective of the orientation program are clearly presented and small group techniques used. Special programs should be instituted for special groups, i.e., commuters, athletes, and transfer students. This latter group should be oriented to a different college rather than to college as are other new students and this process should be of shorter duration. A recommendation which stems from the literature is that a faculty member should be included in the structure of the small group. This could be initiated on an optional basis and inservice training in small group leadership provided for the faculty member. A day, if needed, should be added at the beginning of the program and the bulk of the orientation process completed prior to the arrival of upper class students. BIBLIOGRAPHY BIBLIOGRAPHY Allport, Gordon. Personality and Social Encounter. Boston: Beacon Press, 1960. Argyris, Chris. "Essay Review." Harvard Educational Review, VIII, No. 4 (1971). Bavelas, Alex. "Morale and the Training of Leaders." Civilian Morale. Edited by G. Watson. Boston: Houghton Mifflin, 1942. . "Communications in Task Oriented Groups." The Police Sciences. Edited by D. Lerner and Lt. D. Lasswell. Stanford, Calif.: Stanford University Press, 1951. Bellows, Roger. Creative Leadership. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice Hall, 1969. . Psychology of Personnel in Business apg Industr . 2nd ed. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice Hall, 1954. Bird, Charles. Social Psychology. New York: D. Appleton Century Company, 1940. Bloom, Benjamin, ed. Taxonomy of Educational Objectives. New York: David McKay Company, Inc., 1956. Bales, R. F., et al. "Channels of Communication in Small Groups." American Sociological Review, XVI (1951). Brameld, Theodore. Education as Power. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston, Inc., 1965. Brown, William F., and Zunker, Vernon. "Student Counselor Utilization at Four Year Institutions of Higher Learning." Journal of College Student Personnel, VII, No. 1 (January, 1966). 120 121 Cartwright, D., and Zander, A. Group Dynamics. Evanston, 111.: Row Peterson & Company, 1953. Chickering, Arthur. Education and Identity. San Fran- cisco: Jossey-Bass, Inc., Publishers, 1969. Cowley, W. H. "The Traits of Face to Face Leaders." Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology (1931-32). DeCecco, John P. Regeneration of the School: Decision Making in a Democracy. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston, Inc., 1972. DeLoach, Joseph F. Cognitive Style and Cognitive Dissonance. Bloomfield Hills, Mich.: Oakland Community College Press, 1970. Dugger, June Armistead. "A Study of Measurable Personal Factors of Leaders and Non-Leaders Among Uni- versity Freshman women: The Florida State Uni- versity 1969." Dissertation Abstracts Inter- national, XXX, Nos. 4;6 (1969). Eagley, Alice H. "Leadership Style and Role Differen- tiation as Determinants of Group Effectiveness." Journal of Personalipy, XXXVIII, 509. Eaton, Joseph W. "Is Scientific Leadership Selection Possible?" Studies in Leadership. Edited by Alvin Gouldner. New York: Harper & Bros. Pub- lishers, 1950. Fielder, Fred B. A Theopy of Leader Effectiveness. New York: McGraw-Hill Book Company, 1967. Fitzgerald, Laurine D., and Busch, Shirley A. "Orien- tation Programs: Foundation and Framework." College and Universipy Business, XXXVIII (April, 1963). Flanagan, John C. "General Considerations in the Selection of Test Items and a Short Method of Estimating the Product-Moment from the Data at the Tail of the Distribution." Journal of Educational Psychology, XXXII (December, 1939). Gagne, Robert M. The Conditions of Learning. 2nd ed. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston, Inc., 1970. Gardiner, John W. Excellence: Can We Be Equal and Excellent Too? New York: Harper & Rowe Pub— lishers, Harper Colophon Books, 1961. 122 Gardner, Riley W. "Cognitive Styles in Categorizing Behavior." Journal of Personality, XXII (1953). Getzels, J. W., and Guba, E. B. "Social Behavior and the Administrative Process." School Review, LXV (Winter, 1957). Gibbs, Cecil. "The Principles and Traits of Leadership." Journal of Abnormal Social Psychology (1947). Gibbs, Jack R. Dynamics of Leadership, Defensive and Emergent. Chicago, 111.: 22nd National Con- vention on Higher Education, March 7, 1967. Goldsen, Rose K. "Recent Research in the American College Student." Orientation to CollegeiLearning. Edited by Nicholas Brown. washington, D.C.: American Council of Education, 1961. Gordon, Thomas. Group Centered Leadership. Boston: Houghton Mifflin Company, The Riverside Press Cambridge, 1955. Gouldner, A. W., ed. Studies in Leadership. New York: Harper Brothers, 1950. Grier, Daniel J. "Orientation--Tradition or Reality?" Journal of National Association of Student Person- nel AdminIStratIOn, III, No. 3 (January, 1966). Guilford, J. P. "Dimensions of Intellect." International Colloquium on Factor Analysis, Paris, 1955. . "The Structure of Human Intellect." Paper presented to the National Academy of Sciences, Pasadena, California, November, 1955. Guttman, Louis. "An Outline of Some New Methodology for Social Research." Public Opinion Quarterly, XVIII (Winter, 1954-55). Halpin, Andrew W., and Winer, B. James. "A Factorial Study of the Leader Behavior Descriptions." Leader Behavior: Its Description and Measurement. Edited by Ralph.M. Stogdill and AIVin E. Coons. Columbus: Ohio State University, 1957. Hemphill, John K. Situational Factors in Leadership. Monograph No. 32‘TColumbus: Ohio State University Studies, Bureau of Educational Research, Ohio State University, 1949), p. 45. 123 Hemphill, John K., and Coons, Alvin E. "Development of the Leader Behavior Description Questionnaire." Leader Behavior: Its Description and Measurement. Edited by Ralph M. Stogdill and Alvin E. Coons. Columbus: Ohio State University, 1957. Hill, Joseph E. The Educational Sciences. Bloomfield Hills, Mich.: Oakland Community College Press, 1971. Hofmann, Randall, and Plutchuck. Small Groups in Orien- tation and Teaching. New York: G. P. Putnam's, 1959. Homans, George C. The Human Group. New York: Harcourt, 1950. Hoogasian, Vaughn. "An Examination of Cognitive Style Profiles as Indicators of Performance Associated with a Selected Discipline." Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, Wayne State University, 1970. Ivey, Allen E. "A Three Year Evaluation of a College Freshman Week Program." Journal of College Student Personnel, III (October, 1961), 25-32. Jenkins, William 0. "Review of Leadership Studies with Particular Reference to Military Problems." Psycholggical Bulletin, January, 1947. . "Review of Leadership Studies with Particular Reference to Military Problems." Psychological Bulletin, 1947, pp. 44, 54-79. Jennings, H. H. Leadership and Isolation. New York: Longmans, Green & Co., 1943. Kelly, Earl C. Education for What I§ Real. New York: Harper & Row Publishers, 1947. Kemp, Clarence Gratton. Perspectives on the Group Pro- cess. Boston: Houghton Mifflin Company, 1964. Kimura, Doreen. "The Assymetry of the Human Brain." Scientific American, CCVIII, No. 3 (1973), 70. Kopecek, Robert J. "Freshman Orientation Programs: A Comparison." Journal of College Student Perspn— nel, VIII, No. 5 (September, 1967), 51-52. 124 Kronovet, Esther. "Current Practices in Freshman Orien— tation." Improving College and UniversitygTeach- ing, XVII (May, 1969), 204-05. Lange, Chrystal Marie. "A Study of the Effects on Learn- ing of Matching the Cognitive Styles of Students and Instructors in Nursing Education." Unpub- lished Ph.D. dissertation, Michigan State Uni- versity, 1972. Lewin, Kurt; White, Ralph; and Lippitt, Ronald. "Patterns of Aggressive Behavior in Experimentally Created Social Climates." Journal of Social Psychology, X (1939), 271-79. . "The Consequences of an Authoritarian and Democratic Leadership." Studyion Leadership. Edited by Alvin A. Gouldner. New York: Harper. Martin, W. F.; Gross, N.; and Darley, J. G. "Leaders, Followers, and Isolates in Small Groups." Journal of Abnormal Social Psychology, XLVII (1952), 842. McGrath, J. E. A Summary of Small Group Research Studies. ‘ HSR/TN-62/3-GN. Cited in Fred E. Fiedler. Arlington, Va.: Human Sciences Research, Inc., 1962. Mueller, Kate H. Student Personnel Work in Higher Edu- cation. Boston: Houghton Mifflin Company, 1961. Miller, C. Dean; and Ivey, Allen B. "Structure Response to Three Types of Orientation Programs." Personnel and Guidance Journal, XLV (June, 1967), 1,025-29. Munn, R. D. "A Review of the Relationships Between Per- sonality and Performance in Small Groups." Psychological Bulletin, LVI (1959), 241-70. New York State Regents Advisory Committee on Educational Leadership. Leadership for Education, 1967. Ort, Barbara. "An Examination of Relationships Between the Measurable Cognitive Characteristics of a French I Teacher and the Student Success in That Course." Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, Michigan State University, 1971. 125 Pappas, John G. "Student Reaction to a Small Group Orien- tation Approach." College and University, XLIII, 84—87. Patty, Austin H. "Freshman Orientation: A Growing Con- cern." Improving College and University Teachipg, Peltz, P. C. "Leadership Within an Hierarchal Organi— zation." Journal of Social Issues, VII, 49-55. Pigors, Paul. Leadership or Domination. Cambridge, Mass.: The Rivergate Press, 1935. Rothman, Leslie K., and Leonard, Donald G. "Effectiveness of Freshman Orientation." Journal of College Student Personnel, VIII, No. 5 (September, Sanford, Nevitt. "Recent Research in the American College Student Orientation to College Learning." American Council in Education. Edited by Nicholas C. Brown. Washington, D.C.: 1961. Schroeder, Arlen V. "A Study of the Relationship Between Student and Teacher Cognitive Styles and Student Derived Teacher Evaluation." Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, Wayne State University, 1969. Selznick, Phillip. Leadership in Administration. New York: Harper & Row Publishers, 1957. "Personal Factors Associated With Leadership; A Survey of the Literature." Journal of Psy— chology, XXV (1948), 35-71. Shaw, Marvin E., and Blum, J. Michael. “Effects of Leadership Style Upon Group Performance as a Function of Task Structure." Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, III, No. 2 (February, 1966), 241. Siegel, Sidney. Nonparametric Statistics for Behavioral Sciences. New York: McGraw Hill Book Co., 1956. Smith. Cited in Earl Kolle, Vincent Harris and Carolyn Dragger. "Orientation Programs." Review of Educational Research, XXXVI, No. 2 (April, 1966), 243. 126 Snodowsky, Alvin M. "Group Effectiveness as a Function of Communication Networks, Task Complexity and Leadership Type." Dissertation Abstracts International. The City University of New York, 1969. Stodgill, Ralph M. "Personal Factors Associated With Leadership: A Survey of the Literature." The Journal of Psychology, XXV (1948). Tannenbaum, R.; Weschler, I.; and Massarik, F. Leader- ship and Orggnization. New York: McGraw Hill, 1961. Tautfest, Patricia B. "An Evaluation Technique for Orien- tation Programs." Journal of College Student Personnel, XXX (October, 1961), 25-32. The Hazen Report. The Student in Higher Education. Report of the Committee on the Student in Higher Education, The Hazen Foundation. Wharton, John F. "The Making of Leaders." Saturday Review, April 13, 1968. White, Ralph, and Lippitt, Ronald. "Leader Behavior and Member Reactions in Three Social Climates." Group DynamicsLiResearch and Theory. Edited by Darwin Cartwright and Alvin Zandeffi Evanston, 111.: Row, Peterson & Co., 1953. Williams, Stanley 3., and Leavitt, Harold. Journal of Consulting Psychology, No. 11 (1947), 283-91} Witken, H. A. "Differences in Ease of Perception of Embedded Figures." Journal of Personalipy, XVIIII, 1-15. Wyett, Jerry L. "A Pilot Study to Analyze Cognitive Style with Reference to Selected Strata of the Defined Educational Sciences." Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, wayne State University, 1967. Zussman, Steven. Cognitive Style and Administratigp_ St 1e. Bloomfield Hills, Mich.: Oakland Com- munity College Press, 1971. APPENDICES APPENDIX A COMPOSITE OBSERVATIONS OF LEADERSHIP BEHAVIORS APPENDIX A COMPOSITE OBSERVATIONS OF LEADERSHIP BEHAVIORS Leadership Observations Scoring is derived from assigning values of 5, l, 3 to specific r— behaviors listed on evaluation sheets. Directive Behavior Nondirective Behavior 1 34 27 20 11 4 11:5 38 -_ II 37 ‘g; 32 U1 48 It: mwmobmt’ int-(Um b m 3) It": rs: .5 w \oooqcxmc-www “I“! \00 IA \0 h U1 229 32 9 35 30 8 9 H N b \D k U1 34 H U" «hub 00 u U1 H \J N ox ti {3 UZ N O N U1 I: SM: SI SI N .b w m N mwmcz’mr 29 33 27' 31 49 36 29 l: :11 Legend: X Mean Score, three observors of Leadership Behaviors Leader self-evaluation of behavior XI X Mean Score of group members of Leadership Behaviors 1427 APPENDIX B STUDENT ADVISER LEADER SELECTION PROFILE APPENDIX B To: From: Dean Sigren Re: Student Adviser Application A committee made up of Head Residents and the Director of Housing have evaluated your application and as a result you have been assigned to the following classification. ( ) Student Adviser--pending semester grades ( ) Alternate status--pending openings and rankings of alternates. Here is your profile. Age--C1ass-- Experience Academic Stand- ing Attendance-- Selection Meeting Adviser Ratings Head Resident Ratings--Inter- view Testing 4.0 3.5 3.0 2.5 2.0 1.57 1.0 Comp051te Remarks: Thank you for your participation in the selection program. We hope you have gained some understanding of both your- self and the program. If you are an underclassman, we invite you to apply again next year. 128 APPENDIX C INDIVIDUAL-ORIENTATION GROUP EVALUATION APPENDIX D LEADER SELF-EVALUATION Your Name Circle the number of the phrases which best represents your behavior as the O-Group Leader. 1) 2) 3) 4) 5) 6) 18) referred to group as "my" group referred to group as “our" group not observable assigned the meeting place group decided meeting place not observable sat at the head of the group blended into the group not observable took attendance did not take attendance not observable' leader announced schedule group members announced schedule not observable group meetings followed a definite pattern group meetings were very informal not observable 19) 20) 21) 22) 23) 24) 25) 26) 27) 28) 29) 30) 31) 32) 33) leaders settled conflicts in group allowed group to settle conflicts not observable leader assigned responsi— bilities group decided on assign— ments not observable leader made decisions group made decisions not observable encouraged members to participate allowed members to par- ticipate as they desired not observable Made frequent suggestions and provided information made suggestions and pro- vided information about the same as other group members not observable Circle the X on the following continuum which best repre- sents your position as the O-Group leader. X X Personally took Leader, charge of group X the group 131 but more as a member of X X Became just The group a regular actually member of took charge the group 130 Circle the X on the following continuum which best represents your feeling about the manner in which your group was led. X X X X X X X Very SatiSfactory Satisfactory Somewhat Unsatis— Satisfactory factory O-Groups were either assigned or self-selected. Circle the X on the following continuum which best represents your feeling about how your O—Group was formed. X X X X X X X Very Satisfactory Satisfactory Somewhat Unsatis- Satisfactory factory Circle the X on the continuum which best represents how you feel the O-Group Program has assisted you in starting your first year on the Olivet Campus. X X X X X X X Very Satisfactory Satisfactory Somewhat Unsatis- Satisfactory factory Circle the X on the following continuum which best represents your attendance in the O-Group sessions. X X X X X X X All Most Some None or very few WOuld you recommend the O—Group experience for new students next fall? ( ) yes ( ) no Please add any comments you might have for additions or improvements to the O-Group program, as well as suggestions for the leader and manner of leading. Use reverse side. OVER APPENDIX E RATING FORMS LEADERSHIP BEHAVIOR APPENDIX E RATING FORM LEADERSHIP BEHAVIOR Observor Date O-Group Leader Time Location O-Group # Circle Number which applies or N.O. (Not Observable) Meeting Place N.O. l. Assigned place of meeting 2. Group decision of meeting place Seating N.O. 1. Sat at the head of the group 2. Blended in with group Initiation of Action Within the Group N.O. l. Visibly recorded attendance 2. No visible recording of attendance N.O. l. Referred to group as "my" group 2. Referred to group as "our" group N.O. 1. Leader announced schedule 2. Group announced schedule N.O. 1. Group meetings followed a definite pattern 2. Group meetings very informal N.O. 1. Leader settled conflicts in the group 2. Allowed group to settle conflicts N.O. 1. Leader assigned responsibilities 2. Group decided on assignments N.O. 1. Leader made decisions 2. Group made decisions N.O. l. Encouraged group to participate 2. Allowed members to contribute as they desired N.O. 1. Made frequent suggestions and volunteered infor- mation 2. Made suggestions and volunteered information only as a regular member of the group and after others had an opportunity 133 134 Circle the X on the following continuum which best represents the above Group Leader. X X X X X X X Personally took Leader, but more Became just The Group charge of group as a member of a regular actually the group member of took charge the group APPENDIX F COGNITIVE MAPPING CARD SORT QUESTIONS APPENDIX F COGNITIVE MAPPING CARD SORT QUESTIONS Oakland Community College The following questions are marked usually, sometimes, or rarely. There are eight questions in each of twenty-seven categories and a total of two hundred and sixteen questions. Samples of questions Theoretical Symbols T (AL) - I find it easy to add spoken or dictated numbers easily. T (VL) I score high on achievement tests which emphasize reading comprehension. T (AQ) - I find it necessary to write down a telephone number as soon as I hear it or I cannot remember it. Cultural Determinants I - When given a job to do, I prefer to work on it myself. A - I like to share ideas with friends and associates. Qualitative Codes Q (CS) — I can anticipate accurately how well I will do in a new situation. Q (CH) — I can act attentive and interested even though bored when listening to a teacher or supervisor. Q (CET) — I would give up a monetary gain to avoid a com- promise of principles. Q (CT) - I am able to persuade people in disagreement to strive for agreement. Modalities of Inference R - I have no difficulty in understanding how to put puzzles together. K - I avoid probability statements in solving problems. L - I take longer than others in coming to a conclusion, because I want to know more about an issue than others do. 135 APPENDIX G INTERVIEW SESSION--WORKSHEET COMPOSITE INTERVIEW REACTIONS Eh £4 APPENDIX G INTERVIEW SESSION--WORKSHEET--COMPOSITE REACTIONS O-Group Leaders, December 1972 "How Can The Orientation Program Be Improved" Please place an (A) for agree next to those items which you feel would enhance the Orientation Program. Place a (D) for disagree next to the statements which would not be beneficial to the program. General A D 21 §_ The program is great at the beginning, but becomes a hassle later on. 31.19 Students would prefer to be members of build groups. 7 20 i3 Since students are eventually going to be on their own, they should have the experience of nondirective groups. _5 £5 Directive is best as that's what they are used to. £3 2_ There is less confusion in assigning the groups. ££.§_ "Optional and good" is the secret to programming. '_§.g2 Required attendance is the only way to go. _fl_21_The program pace was too fast in the beginning. 39 2_ The Leader should size up the group needs and go according to their needs always looking for the group to take over. ighiZ_Students have a feeling, "unless it goes on my record, it's unimportant." 2£_Z__The Painting Exercise—-Project Alpha was excellent for getting students out of their shells. i1_ii There is value in having the program go until midterm. 2i_9_ Coming early by Football and Music students affects their attitude negatively toward the program. 25 l_ Upperclassmen affect new students attitudes toward orientation, negatively. 136 137 _2_i__Separating groups out will destroy a sense of com- munity. ‘2_‘3_ Start Directive in leadership and end Nondirective (Situational Leadership). Brainstorming allows for hitchhiking on other ideas. Please add any suggestions or comments. Personal A D 31 g_ I learned a lot about myself as a result of the O-Group experience. I now have several close, freshman friends. I—‘ N m ox re A [—1 I was successful in going according to the specific behavioral objectives when leading. 35 6_ I feel eight plus a leader is an ideal size group. £§.Z_ The experience has given me new confidence. 29';_ I have become more sensitive in my relationship with people. 9 22 I felt that all of a sudden I was supposed to act five years older. 24 Z_ I feel the inservice training given prior to the O-Group experience was adequate. Please indicate any ways in which the O-Group Opportunity did or did not add to your leadership skills. Suggestions A D 30 2_ An individual sheet for each member on activities so they can read and discuss rather than the O-Group leader read off. £§.£_ Drop the summer reading On Becoming an Educated Person. 20 ii Structure work sheets so that students can work at their own speed. Have just a final turn—in date. 24 §__Tie the initial program in with the Convocation Program. I)“ \O N O l-‘ (.3 IF 6- \o H O to I" \1 la N ON (a H m 1» H b [-4 DJ N '00 g...- 0 Ix] N l‘” |°‘ u N lo 138 The meetings should be bi-weekly, alternating with the Convocation Program. There should be a separate program for football players. There should be separate programs for transfer students. There should be separate programs for music students. Eat together for the first dinner and breakfast only. Set up a different library sheet for each member of the group. Thus, they can work on their own or as part of the group. The comp test could be separated into parts and made more specific. A new format is needed for Sunday. Perhaps sports activities, games, recreation, etc. There should be a separate program for commuters. There should be one football player, one commuter, and one transfer student in each group. Have a program on the History of Olivet College. Stress the attendances—more reluctance, but they enjoy it when they are there. The program is too long. Have it twice a week for three weeks. Sunday brunch was a bomb—-drop it. Meet fewer times, but make more substance to the meeting. Go three or four weeks and then have a "reunion" at midterm. Have inservice academic advising for O—Group leaders. Set up individual conferences for O—Group academic advising. Have interaction between O—Groups. N N w H loo N H- m \J H ox (a .5 I" (o (JJ 1" N H IH O l-‘kO w NN U1\l lb lxo IH l—‘N KO bl" l“ I“ 139 Initially have four days rather than three-—spread out activities with more free time. Have a tournament of O-Groups on Sunday-~Consolation brackets, prizes, etc. Have a scavenger hunt, like getting the inscription off Father Shipherd's grave, the name of the printing press, etc. Have a treasure hunt. Equalize the sexes in the groups. Start the Orientation Program early for those who come early. Get the program to end on a climactic note, rather than downhill. Set up a crossword puzzle to get across pertinent information. Set up a Special night and time for O-Group meeting to eliminate hassle of deciding the meeting dates. The hassle is what it's all about. Have Orientation assigned as part of the scheduling process and switch leaders around accordingly. Program should be shorter for transfer students. Orient transfer students to a different school rather than to college. Your additional ideas.