THE QUANTIFICATION OF DRIVE H. TWO METHODS OF FOOD PRIVATION Thesis for the Degree of Ph. D,‘ MICHIGAN STATE COLLEGE Orville Auverne Smii‘h, Jr. 1953 0-163 This In to certify that the thesis entitled The Quantification of Drive II. Two Methods 01‘ 1'00? Privation presented by Orville A. Smith, Jr. has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for %& Major @{ofemr mew V n........-.u.~.ouu-. ..... . . THE QUANTIFICATION OF DRIVE II. TWO METHODS OF FOOD PRIVATION BY Orville Auverne Smith, Jr. A THESIS Submitted to the School of Graduate Studies of Michigan State College of Agriculture and Applied Science in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Psychology Year 1953 ACKNOWLEDGEMENT Grateful acknowledgement is made to Doctor M. Ray Denny for his advice and assistance which made possible the Asuccessful completion of this research. INTRODUCTION . . STATEMENT OF‘THE SUBJECTS e e e e APPARATUS. . e e PROCEDURE. 0 e e PREDICTIONS. e 0 RESULTS O O O O O DISCUSSION e e 0 TABLE PROBLEM e SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS. 0 REFERENCES e e e OF CONTENTS Page 12 13 11L 18 27 66 85 88 LIST OF TABLES Table Page 1 Analysis of variance of latency scores as a function of hours deprivation. . . . . . . . . 31 2 "F" ratios of latency variances for all % Fed SUbgPOUpSe e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e 32 3 Analysis of variance of amplitude scores as a function of hours deprivation . . . . . . 36 h Analysis of variance of amplitude scores - as a function of % Fed (LW subgroup) . . . . . 39 5 Analysis of variance of amplitude scores as a function of % Fed (SW subgroup) . . . . . no 6 Analysis of variance of amplitude scores as a function of % Fed (LD subgroup) . . . . . Al 7 Analysis of variance of activity scores as a function of hours deprivation. . . . . . . . 43 8 Analysis of variance of activity scores as a function of % Fed (Combined SW, LD and SD SUbgPOUPS) e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e 0 us 9 Analysis of variance of activity scores as a function of % Fed.(tW subgroup). . . . . . . . A6 10 Chi-square test of linearity for amplitude data of hours deprivation group. . . . . . . . 50 ll Chi-square test of linearity for amplitude data of LW subgroup. . . . . . . . . . . . . . 51 12 Chi-square test of linearity for activity data of the SD, LD, and SW subgroups combined. 52 13 Correlation coefficient between latencies of hours deprivation group and SD subgroup equated as to activity with a fluctuation Of 2 p01nt8. e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e S“ 14 Correlation coefficient between latencies of hours deprivation group and SD subgroup equated as to activity with a fluctuation Of 20 pOlntS e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e 55 LIST OF TABLES Continued Table Page 15 Correlation coefficient between latencies of hours deprivation group and SD subgroup equated as to activity with a fluctuation Of 5 pOlDUSe e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e 57 16 Correlation coefficient between amplitude scores of hours deprivation group and LD subgroup equated as to activity with a fluctuation of 5 points. . . . . . . . . . . . . 58 17 "F" test on variances of the number of trials to extinction for high and low drive SUbJOOtB Of % Fed GPOUPe e e e e e e e e e 60 18 't' test of the number of trials to extinction of the LW subgroup vs. the s SD subgroup. e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e 61 19 Correlation coefficients between the deprivation levels of both hours deprivation group and % Fed group with EMP11DUd6 SOOPOS e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e 62 20 Standard deviations from comparable points of amplitude scores of the hours deprivation and % Fed groups . . . . . . . . . . 6h 21 Correlation coefficients utilizing deprivation level to predict amplitude compared to activity to predict amplitude. . . . 65 LIST OF FIGURES Figure Page 1 Apparatus e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e 15 2 Mean activity scores for two groups of animals on 13 consecutive days. . . . . . . . . . . . 28 3 Latency as a function of number of hours fOOd deprivation. e e e e e e e e e e e e e e 29 h Latency as a function of deprivation level with four different reward situations . . . . 33 5 Standard deviation as a function of depri- vation level with four reward situations. . . 3h 6 AMplitude of response in degrees turn of a wheel as a function of hours food deprivation e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e 35 7 Amplitude of a response in degrees turn of a wheel as a function of deprivation level and three types of reward . . . . . . . 38 8 Activity level as a function of the number or hours fOOd deprivation e e e e e e e e e e #2 9 Activity'as a function of deprivation level and four types of reward. . . . . . . . an 10 Mean activity scores of three combined groups (SW, LW, SD) as a function of deprivation level e e e e e e e e e e e e e e AB 11 Activity, latency and amplitude as a function of the number of hours food deprivation e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e #9 INTRODUCTION ”Among the more elusive variables in the behavior of the intact organism, drive stands preeminent.” This quo- tation from Skinner (17) is probably an understatement, principally because there remains the question of whether drive is actually a primary behavioral variable or whether the behavioral concomitants attributed to it may be deriv- able in terms of other principles. Behaviorists, in reaction to the instinct theories, brought forth the term 'primary drive' with the connotation of "greatly diminished reliance upon hypothetical innate sensoriemotor connections . . .' (8). However, the behav- iorists have had little success in agreeing as to Just what drive is and.what may be attributed to it. Referring to drive or motivation, Holt (9) has said, "In short, all of the appetite drives are persistent afferent impulses coming from organs situated within the body, and producing . . . at the outset, that is, previously to trial and error learning, merely random movements of general restlessness.I This is the only way he uses the term 'drive' - which in essence is as an internal stimulus and nothing more. Holt's usage does not in the least admit of any 'directive preperties' of motivation current in present day psychology. {5 I. r. N Guthrie (7) holds a similar viewpoint. Drive or motivation for him is the action of 'maintaining stimuli'. The maintaning stimuli may come from within, e.g.. hunger, thirst, or may come from.without, e.g., electric shock,. intense auditory stimuli. These stimuli 'drive' the organimm. Tolman.(l9) removes the concept from the equation with raw stimulus and gives it status as a physiological 'state', saying, ”The ultimate motivators of all behavior . . . are, we assume, certain innately provided appetites and aversions. These consist in ultimate demands to get to physiological quiescences or from final physiological disturbance.” The demands in this case are 'internal initiating physiological states' which emerge as one of his four independent causes of behavior. This conceptualization obviously retains instinctual and teleological connotations. Skinner's system (17) contains a flat rejection of drive as a stimulus or as having any teleological impli- cations. He says, "In.measuring the strength of a drive we are in reality only measuring strength of behavior. A complete account of the latter is to be obtained from an examination of the Operations that are found to affect it. The 'drive' is a hypothetical state interpolated between Operation.and behavior and is not actually required in a descriptive system.” And elsewhere he states, "The con- ception of drive as a state rather than as a stimulus is valuable in avoiding arguments about purpose." In these writings are found one of the first formal considerations of 'drive' as an unobservable theoretical construct. Keller and Schoenfeld (13) more recently have again supported the Skinnerian outlook by saying that drive is ‘322 stimulus, 222 a reaponse, and 222 a physiological concept. For them drive is the name for the fact that certain oper- ations can be performed on an organism.that have an effect upon behavior which is different from other operations. In the above, drive has been reduced to a set of operations which have an effect on behavior. This treatment gives drive the essential requisites of a theoretical con- struct. However, even these conceptualizations have not resulted in the strict theoretical formulation employed by Bull, whose theory is founded on the concepts of drive mud drive reduction. Hull's formulation is the most explicit available at the present time and, consequently, the most testable. The Hullian Concept of Drive Hull (10, 11) has attempted to formalize drive by naming OXplloit the experimentally manipulated antecedent conditions and the associated observable behavioral conse- quences. In order for a concept to obtain status as a construct, it must meet several requirements. /V The requirements for a construct as put forth by iHaatsch and Behan (1h) are: 1. Operational definition which contains “a description of manipulations which are performed in the labor- atory, and a description of the effects of these manipulations upon the behavior of the experimental subjects." 2. The concept "must bear explicitly stated relation- ships to other constructs in the theory of which it is a part." 3. It must "vary unidimensionally and continuously, and must affect at least one abstracted aspect of behavior such that the behavior will vary unidimen- sionally and continuously." Hull (10) operationally defines drive (D) by indicating the manipulations in the eXperimental situation (antecedent conditions) which is the number of hours elapsed since the last satiation period for the particular primary drive in question. For the hunger drive, the conditions would be the number of hours since the last food intake. The conse- quent event or behavioral concomitant is "the amount of energy which will be expended by the organism." If energy is expended by an organism and we ignore the magnitude of energy expenditure as a result of neural activity alone without concomitant muscular activity; or that involved in ‘maintaining muscle tonus, then the energy transformation from potential into kinetic will result chiefly from gross movements of the organism's musculature. In other words, the organism will be more active, 1.6., the random move- ments of the organism will increase. We assume from this that with an increase in the number of hours of deprivation there would occur an increase in the activity of the organism. Hull (10) uses the now classical work of Richter (l6),‘Wada (20), and others to illustrate Just this point. As to the second requirement, Hull (11) relates D to the other constructs in his system by the formula sEr: erXV XJxKXD where sEr is reaction potential, er is habit strength, V is stimulus intensity dynamism, J is the delay in rein- forcement and K is the amount of reward. sEr or performance is measured in terms of latency of response, amplitude of response, number of trials to extinction and the probability of reaction evocation. Regarding the 3rd requirement, Hull's theory must say that with all other factors held constant, any variation in D will result in variation of performance (sEr) as measired by above methods, and the variation in performance will occur for 21351 variation in drive. According to Hull (10), ". . . when a condition arises for which action on the part of the organism is a prerequisite to Optimum probability of survival of either the individual or the species, a state of need is said to exist." A need, then, produces a primary drive and every individual drive summates physiologically with every other drive to produce 3 generalized M 9.39.152. in the organism. It is this generalized drive which is formulated as the construct (D). The Scientific Usefulness of Drive (D) The problems involved in using this concept in a strict mathematical theory of behavior are not overlooked by Bull. He recognizes that there must be two equations: one express- ing the degree of drive as a function of the amount of deprivation, another expressing the vigor of organismic action as a function of the degree of drive. One of the major problems is that of finding a measurement parameter with a specified extensive unit with which to express the degree of drive on the behavioral side. If (D) in conjunc- tion with the other factors V, K, J, and er, bears a constant relationship to reaction potential regardless g; £23 dominant typg'gf privation and if this fundamental relationship can be quantitatively determined, then Drive may be used as a genuine theoreticalconstruct. If, on the other hand, a set of equations must be derived for each primary drive constituting the major component of general- ized Drive, there will be no reason to consider generalized Drive as a useful scientific construct. Inasmuch as Drive (D) is one of the most fundamental concepts in Hull's theory validating the concept is essential.. ”-1 The Problem of Quantification A curve representing drive as a function of hours of deprivation may be derived easily by using latency, ampli- tude, or trials to extinction as'Yamaguchi (21) has done. However, as Hull (10) points out, this ignores the whole problem of getting at the intervening variable of Drive itself. Hull specifically mentions the necessity of defin- ing a unit independent of the performance measurements with which to represent Drive. Inasmuch as the activity level of the organism is assumed to be directly associated with the drive level of the animal and yet is not considered to be a function of habit, it may be that the answer to the problem can be found in the measurement of activity. It seems reasonable that if Drive determines the energy output of the organism that possibly the activity level of the organism.may be taken as the measurement parameter and provide the unit of measurement for Drive. In other words, Drive may be quantifiable in terms of activity level. If activity level is to serve as the unit of measurement, then a situation.must be devised in which amount of deprivation may be functionally related to activity level, and activity level may be functionally related to latency, amplitude and number of trials to extinction. With this situation the experimenter could manipulate drive conditions in terms of type of deprivation as well as m] OB amount. If activity level can be used as a measure of (D) then regardless of type of deprivation, activity levels could be equated and the comparisons for the two conditions of drive should result in very similar measurements of latency, amplitude and trialstn extinction, 6.3., a corre- lation coefficient between latencies obtained from different animals under different types of deprivation but equated as to activity level should prove to be very highly significant. The use of a correlation coefficient is a much weaker require- ment than demanding equal latencies for equal activity levels, but it may be that different types of deprivation may result in different absolute magnitudes of response and the use of the correlation technique would allow for this differ- ence. Again, then, if different types of privation. or different methods of inducing drive with but one type of privation, result in different absolute values of sEr, still the shapes of the curves should be the same and a factor should be ascertainable which would enable one to convert the scores of the first condition into the scores of the other condition. Program of Validation A complete investigation into the possibility of quan- tifying drive in terms of activity level would involve two major investigations: 1. One investigation would have to be made involving I- I. a cross comparison of different types of deprivation. For example, a group of animals trained under thirst deprivation would be compared to a group under hunger conditions. Activity levels for the two groups would be equated and latency and amplitude would be compared. This would serve to indicate whether or not activity level could be used as a measurement parameter for various 21333 of deprivation. 2. The second investigation would involve using a different method of setting up drive conditions but with the scum type of deprivation, In other words a different set of Operations which should result in the same physiological effect for the animal should be performed. The consequences based upon equating of activity levels could then be compared to the results derived from the usual 'hours deprivation' method. Problems Involved in Carrying Out Program Before this program could be carried out there would necessarily be a great deal of preliminary investigation not only in the line of practical difficulties but also regarding the rationale for size of reward in the water and food cross comparison (1 above). Specifically, what size food pellet equals what size water reward as an equal reinforcing value? This work has been carried out by Davis (A). The actual work on problem 1 remains to be completed. The experiment to be reported.here it) is concerned with the second investigation mentioned. The alternative method of setting up drive with the sump type deprivation (in this case, food) was chosen with the object not only of finding a method which would yield a change in physiological state similar changes brought about in the hours deprivation.method, but also to determine whether or not a different method might yield more homo- geneous results, or, better said, yield more precise results. The {percentage fed' method was chosen for this purpose. With this method the amount of food normally consumed by the animal which results in satiation is measured over a period of time. When the day to day amount consumed becomes asymptotic the average amount on the asymptote-days is taken as the basal amount to satiation. In varying the drive level for the various organisms, a specified percentage of the basal amount is fed to the animal a determined time before testing. This method should result in a differential physiological state for the organism just as the hours deprivation method does. ‘We assume that this method will consequently alter the drive of the organism in a uniform fashion. This specific technique has to the writer's knowledge, never been used, but it is similar to methods used by Skinner (l7) and Bruce (2). Skinner fed his animals 0, 2, h, or 6 grams of food before measuring the rate of response in the bar-pushing apparatus. The resulting curves, however, l1 ,2...» H were not clear out; there was considerable overlap, but the general result was more rapid responding with the lesser amounts Of food consumed. The results of Bruce (2) seemed to show an effect on performance brought about by prefeeding or prewatering immediately before testing. This may possibly be avoided by increasing the time between feeding and testing. A complete study of the major drive variables could be made in this situation by altering reward size and reward type as well as deprivation level. fl‘ ‘lh I -._ 6.! STATEMENT OF THE PROBLEM The problem here undertaken is to determine whether or not activity level may be used as a means of quantifying the Hullian construct of Drive (D) when Drive is induced by two different methods of food deprivation. The study will also include variations in size and type of reward in order to gain information as to possible interaction effects between Drive (D) and the variable incentive reinforcement (K). i 3 SUBJECTS The subjects used in the present experiment were h8 experimentally naive male albino rats from the colony maintained by the Department of Psychology of Michigan State College. The ages ranged from 90 to 120 days. {as :4:- Apparatus In order to investigate the present problem an apparatus was constructed in which it was possible to measure activity level, response latency, response ampli- tude or force, and the number of responses to extinction. It consisted of a rectangular box constructed of %" plywood with overall dimensions of 20" x 16" x 11". Figure 1 presents a cross section of the apparatus. At the bottom of the activity chamber there was a false floor which was supported by 3 springs and a rubber ball at its exact center. At the four corners of this false floor, small attached, rubber balls served as stOps, pre- venting the floor from tipping any more than &' in any direction. A guillotine door at one end of the activity chamber, when raised, gave access to a hinged, h" x 2" panel. This panel was constructed of a thin rectangular piece Of wood, 1/16" diameter; at the upper end of the panel was a small piece of %" plywood 2%" long, which formed a base for the hinge and brass strips (See Figures 1, 2, and h). The flat gray interior was illuminated by a 7-watt bulb covered by a piece of Opal-flashed glass situated at the end of the box Opposite the guillotine door. Entrance to the box from the tOp was gained through a hinged door, in the center of which was placed a large clear- glass observation window. 15 338a H8... soars owe» ma confluence usueaeon; .a easwam Hench nee-5 3 condo; flee—5 caved—E ewes.— Saved—".30 s\\\\\\\\\\x\~\§\\\\\\\\\§\K\\\\N\\\ \\.\\\\\\\\\\\\\\N\\\\\\\\\\\\\\\\\\\\\. m sea?» bio! I “ one?" no _$S55§§SS\\\\\\\\§\\\§\“\\ gamma» — \\\\\\\\\\\\.m “ OOH-h ....... \ “ a a w m r". m \ A .1. m “ w \ \ m \ “ m .33 \ a 583...... ll.“ 16 Activity level was measured by a device consisting of a GE 2-36KRL mercury switch, suspended vertically beneath the false floor and connected in series to a Gorrell and Gorrell 115 volt electric counter. The mercury switch was situated beneath the floor in such a manner that movement by the animal tilted the floor, causing the liquid in the tube to move, momentarily making and breaking the circuit in accordance with the strength and number of movements (See Figure 1). A thin metal rod, hinged at the tOp of the panel was twisted so as to extend to the back of the panel in one direction and to the tOp of the box in the other. The rod was so designed that the lower half of it "rode" back on the panel as it was pushed Open, and the upper half came .forward toward the activity chamber. By means of this rod, the force applied to the door was transmitted to a slender stick of wood which was attached to a light, plastic wheel, mounted on a plastic axle. The force of the response, which was applied to the door, was thus transmitted into the movement of the wheel. The distance which this wheel moved was measured in degrees. Because the wheel Offered very little resistance to the metal rod, the initial movement of the push-panel invariable caused it to turn out of range of further movements of the metal rod. That is, the degrees which the wheel was displaced depended upon the £9522 with which the door was figgt struck, and not merely upon the distance through which the door was moved. .- I») Response latency was measured by a Standard Electric Timer, connected in series through two switches. The first switch consisted of two brass strips, one being placed along the tOp Of the push-panel, and the other being attached to the upper push-panel jamb, directly above the strip on the push-panel. A second switch was attached to the tOp of the guillotine door. When the guillotine door was raised, this switch was closed. Thus, when the push-panel was closed and the guillotine door Opened, the circuit was closed, starting the Timer. When the panel was Opened by approximately 1/32", the switch was Opened, stOpping the Timer. Thus the timer started.when the guillotine door was raised and stepped as the panel was being pushed Open by an animal. Single reward pellets were placed on a tray which was located approximately fi” below the lower panel jamb. Metal walls were built up on either side of this tray to dis— courage exploratory behavior. The corners of these walls were bent towards the door, forming stOps to prevent the animals from forcing the door and breaking it. This also reduced exploratory behavior. On the tray itself a small wall of solder was constructed to hold the food pellets in place. PROCEDURE The portion of the procedure which was common to all groups will be presented first, and the differences among the groups will be specified subsequently. Habituation All animals were handled for a period of 20 minutes on each of three days previous to being placed in the apparatus. The handling consisted of stroking the animals, picking them up, setting them down and allowing them to run freely on a table tOp on which several boxes were placed. On the fourth day the S's were placed in the appar- atus for a period of six.minutes during which.time activity level was recorded. These six minute habituation trials in the apparatus continued for five days or up to the 9th day of the experimental series. Training On the 9th day the_animals were given the regular six minute habituation trial and at the end of this trial the guillotine door was raised, allowing the animal access to the hinged panel. The panel door was Open to its fullest extent (approximately two inches) making the reward pellet on the food receptable easily accessible. After the animal had taken the reward the panel door was slowly closed and the guillotine door was carefully lowered. As soon as the animals had finished eating, indicated by cessation of chewing movements, the guillotine door was raised again, presenting the S with the Opened panel and the reward pellet on the receptacle. This procedure continued for 8 trials. On the 9th trial the panel was Opened only about } inch or about i'of the full Open position. This forced the animal to nose the panel slightly in order to obtain the pellet. Another trial was given in the same manner, making a total of ten reinforced responses for the day. On the next day the animals received two trials with the door in the full Open position and eight trials with the door in the % Open position. The following day the panel was presented in the % Open position for two trials and was fully closed for the remaining eight trials. The animals, then, received 10 trials per day with the door fully closed for a period of five more days. At the end of training each animal had received 80 rewarded trials in the apparatus. The six minute activity period.before the first trial of each day was recorded. Latency and amplitude measures for each trial were recorded on the last three days of training. Testing On the 17th day of the series the test trials began. m C: The animals were randomly assigned to the particular deprivation level under which they would be tested on that day. The apprOpriate deprivation manipulations were carried out (details to be specified below) and the animals were introduced into the apparatus. The six minute activity levels were recorded and the Sis were given four rewarded trials in exactly the same manner as during the last five training days. Latency and amplitude were recorded for each Of the four test trials. If an S refused to respond for a period of six minutes it was removed from the apparatus and a 'NO Response' was recorded. Each animal was tested at each deprivation level under consideration, however the order Of levels was systematically randomized for each S. There were six deprivation levels studied; hence, there were six testing days, bringing the experimental series to a total Of 22 days. Extinction On the 23rd day Of the series the animals were ran- domly assigned to a deprivation level. They were again given the six minute activity level period in the apparatus and the panel pushing response was then extinguished to a three minute no response criterion. The number of responses to criterion, latency and amplitude were recorded. A summary Of the procedure common to all groups is as follows: Days 1 - 3 Handling - 20 minutes per day. Days A - 8 Habituation in apparatus - 6 minutes - activity recorded. Days 9 - 16 Training - 10 trials per day - activity recorded on all days, latency and amplitude recorded on days lh, 15, and 16 only. Days 16 - 22 Testing - h trials per day under appro- priate drive condition - activity recorded - latency, amplitude measured on each trial. Day 23 Extinction - to a three minute no response criterion - activity recorded - latency and amplitude recorded on all trials. All animals were weighed at the beginning and end Of the 23 day series and at least one other time during the series. ' The aspects of the procedure which differentiated the groups were as follows: Hours Deprivation Group - These animals were fed ad lLE until day 8 Of the series when they were put on a 2k hour depriva- tion schedule. They continued on this schedule during the training days 9-16. 22% hours after their last feeding the animals were placed in the apparatus for the activity level period and the 10 training trials. The reward pellet was approximately 0.08 grams Of dry Calf Manna. After the conclusion of the trials the animals were placed in a feeding cage for a period of at least six minutes, after which time they received the same type pellets used as reward pellets for a period Of 15 minutes. They then received a wet mash composed Of a ho% Purina dog chow and 60% water combination for a period Of 25 minutes. After this time they were removed tO the home cages until the following day. The procedure during the testing days involved feeding the animals with the above mentioned satiation procedure a specified number Of hours before they were to be run. The periods were 1, 2, 6, 12, 2h and 48 hours measured from the time the animals were removed from the feeding cage and replaced in the home cage. ‘Water was available in both ‘home and feeding cages. Large'Wet Mash, % Fed Group - hereafter to be designated.Lw. The S's in this category were handled in the same manner as those above with the exceptions that: 1. They were fed wet mash only. 2. The amount of mash consumed by each animal was recorded on the basis of four hours eating time the first day, l B/h hours the second day, 1 hour the third day, i hour the remaining days. 3. They were on a 2h hour deprivation schedule from the beginning of the habituation days (Day h). h. During testing they were fed 0, 10, 25, SO, 75 or 90 percent of the average amount Of mash they had eaten on the four days previous to the test days. 5. All animals were run 3 hours after they had consumed the above mentioned percentage. 6. The reward pellet was 0.2 gm of wet mash. 7. After testing they were fed enough mash, after a six minute delay in the feeding cage, to equal 100% Of the previously determined amount consumed to produce satiation. Small Dry Pellet, % Fed Group - hereafter to be designated SD. These S's were handled as the Lw group with the following p ‘.—v—-.-—_—_-—.A b—v—_-~‘-’—--—‘————n ' v i U C 0‘. . ~ I '. ___._._..- _ ._.__- m- ._.__i exceptions: N) C»: 1. They were fed 10 minutes on dry pellets (Calf Manna) and 30 minutes on wet mash: the total volume Of wet food consumed was computed each day. 2. Reward pellet was 0.08 gm of dry pellet. 3. Fed only mash before running - fed pellets and mash after testing to equal 100% of satiation amount. Large Dry Pellet, % Fed Group - hereafter to be designated.LD. The only difference between this group and the SD group was the reward pellet size, which was 0.2 gm for this group. Small Wet Mash, % Fed Group - hereafter to be designated SW. This group differed from the LW group regarding size Of reward which was 0.08 gm Of wet mash and time fed during training trials, which was hO minutes. A summary Of the critical differences among groups is as follows: Hours Deprivation N’= ll LW-%Fed N=1L|. sw-%Fed N’= 6 LD-%Fed N=7 SD - "ha '121 o n. S's satiated a specified number Of hours before running. Reward was 0.08 gm dry Calf Manna. S's run 3 hours after receiving specified percent; Of amount required to produce satiation. Reward was 0.2 gm wet mash. S's run 3 hours after receiving Specified percent of amount required to produce satiation. Reward was 0.08 gm wet mash. S's run 3 hours after receiving Specified percent Of amount required to produce satiation. Reward was 0.2 gm dry Calf Manna. S's run 3 hours after receiving specified percent Of amount required to produce satiation. Reward was 0.08 gm dry calf Manna. PREDICTIONS In order to facilitate the presentation Of the results most Of the specific predictions to be tested by the present study are given below. Predictions 1 through 12 are deductions from Hullian theory and the assumptions seemingly implicit in his system with particular regard to the assumption that activity level will measure Drive (D) directly. 1. From the fourth to the ninth day Of the series (corresponding to the first 5 days on Fig. 2) the average activity of all animals will gradually decrease because Of adaptation or what may be termed a decrease in 'eXploratory drive'. 2. The decrease in activity level will be greater for the animals satiated during this period than for those under 22% hours or food deprivation. 3. When the satiated animals are put under 22% hours deprivation activity level will rise abruptly to the level Of the regularly deprived group and remain constant at that level as long as the deprivation schedule is maintained. h. There will be differences between deprivation levels regarding activity and amplitude which will vary positively with deprivation level and latency which will vary negatively with deprivation level. 5. The function for amplitude will be a straight line. 6. The curve for reciprocals Of latency will be negatively accelerated. 7. The curve for activity should approximate Yama- guchi's (21) curve which represents Drive (D) as a function Of hours deprivation. Yamaguchi's curve is described as, ". . . from h = 0 to about 3 hours drive rises in an approximately linear manner until the function abruptly shifts to a near horizontal, then to a concave-upward course, gradually changing to a convex-upward curve reaching a maximum Of 12.30- at about h = 59, after which it grad- ually falls to the reaction threshold (er) at around h.= 100." 8. If animals from the Hours Deprivation Group are matched with animals from the % Fed Group on the basis of activity level, the correlation coefficient between their corresponding latencies and amplitudes Of response will be highly significant and positive. 9. Groups receiving large reward will perform better than those receiving small reward, i.e., lower latencies, higher amplitudes. 10. There will be no significant differences in activity level as a function Of size or type of reward. 11. There will be more trials to extinction for animals {\T' (3') extinguiShed under higher deprivation conditions than those extinguished under low deprivation conditions. 12. There will be differences between groups in extinction based upon size Of reward, i.e., the larger the reward, the more trials to extinction. 13. If 9% Fed method delimits level of deprivation better than the number Of hours since satiation, then a higher correlation.will result from deprivation levels Of the % Fed Group with latency and amplitude than with levels Of the Hours Deprivation Group and latency and amplitude. 1h. Also, if % Fed method delimits level Of depri- vation better than the number of hours since satiation, the standard deviation at each level for % Fed animals will be less than for approximately corresponding points for the Hours Deprivation Group. 15. If activity measures Drive more reliably than hours deprivation, then there should be a higher corre- lation coefficient between activity and latency, and activity and amplitude than for hours deprivation corre- lated with latency and amplitude. RESULTS Figure 2 shows fluctuations in activity level Of two groups of animals measured over a six minute period fOr thirteen consecutive days. The activity levels of the animals decreased during the first five days in the box (habituation period) as predicted. Prediction 1 was con- firmed. The animals in the Deprived Group were under 22% hour hunger conditions while the animals in the Satiated Group had food available at all times. Despite this difference in the groups there were no differences in activity level. Inspection reveals complete overlap Of the two curves. Prediction 2 was not confirmed. Upon putting animals of the Satiated Group under 22% hours food deprivation on the sixth day, there was no sudden increase in activity level, and no separation Of the two curves. The increase in activity level was gradual and was shown by both groups of animals from the sixth to the thirteenth days, 1.6., during the training period in which time food was presented to the animals via panel pushing just subsequent to the six minute activity period. Prediction 3 was not confirmed. Latency as a function Of deprivation level in the Hours Deprivation Group is shown in Fig. 3. Inasmuch as the variances for these animals proved to be homogeneous whee cipfioomdoo ma do manage mo 9 N, 2 O, , O omZmdwo \\\ \\\\ owvffim \\ I I I I ~ I I ~71! K II s IL . a s I s a a z ~ ~- -u---‘ I (- -- ~- ~- ---- -e- ~-- Q-.. CO, OON con soapmbasoop poo.“ mason .HO pong: .HO £0323.“ m we hodopwq .m enswam 20752530 930: fiuu N. m N. a _ q _ _ Q" '5335 NI ID AONBiV'l/Ol an analysis Of variance was performed on the data, which is given in Table 1. From the insignificant "F" it is Obvious that the differences in means of the various levels could have occurred by chance. Figure h shows latency as a function Of deprivation for the % Fed Groups, representing the four curves on a single graph to indicate the differ- ences due to size and type of reward. Table 2 presents the results of ”F" tests performed on the variances of the % Fed Group to determine whether or not the homogeneity of variance requirement was fulfilled. The "F" test indicates that the variances within the groups; that is, between drive levels, are so divergent as to preclude the possibility Of performing any valid analysis of variance or “t” test. An attempt to normalize the data by eliminating all scores beyond Liq- of the mean failed to make the 'F's' non-signifi- cant. Regarding latency data prediction number n is not con- firmed by the results Of the Hours Deprivation Group and no statement can be made concerning the results Of the % Fed Group. Figure 5 shows the changes in standard deviations Of latencies with changes in deprivation level. Figure 6 presents the changes in amplitude Of response as a result Of change in deprivation for the Hours Depriva- tion Group. Table 3 presents the analysis Of variance 3 1 TABLE 1 ANALYSIS OF VARIANCE OF LATENCY SCORES AS A FUNCTION OF HOURS DEPRIVATION Source Of variation Sum of squares d.f. Mean square Levels 137.85 5 27.5? Subjects 172.10 10 17.21 Interaction 606.hS uh. 13.785 No significant differences TABLE 2 "F" RATIOS OF LATENCY VARIANCES FOR ALL 96 FED SUBGROUPS 0 10 25 50 75 90 m 0.2 .0h2 .030 .096 .067 .259 .267 25 - + ‘+ 75 - 90 LD 0.2 .096 .255 .109 .21.? 1.795 10.038 0 - - - + + 10 - - + + 25 - + + 50 + + 75 + 90 SD 0.?- .056 .188 .118 .373 1.932 5.76 0 + - + + + 10 .. . .. + + 25 + + + 75 + 90 sw 0.2 .0001; .0021 .0096 .011 .036 .616 0 + + - + + 10 - + + + 25 - - + 50 II + 75 + 90 + indicates significance at 5% level or less. msogmdpflm panama pdmaowgo .50.“ £33 HpbOH doflpmuflpmop mo mogoqdm m we hummus 3: 09mg om... .\. o 0. mm on on cm T w J 1 d \h h J‘ Anna #03 Hamam Tlll mama no: omnmq Ill... . III erased use Hausa IIIII wedded mac omamH .nu nfim“ m.N .n SONOOBS page .3 men»... asoa pom amped deapmaaaaeo mo aoapoasa m we 32$me neapmaaop campampm .m Oadwah on... ex. 0 0. mm on ma om T W at q . q ‘ ‘\ h, e e I e O I O I e l e ’I’ .II‘ ‘ One 1 I I, e I, I‘ ‘e I'll I e l e no ' I. ’ I l' e I . II I I ’ I I 10.. I III I . I i m _ I I I I // .1 O.N I I I ,i mesa no: :55 T1 “N name no: emamH Illil HOHHOQ mac Hanam Illl wedded haw OmadH CM .0.» VWOIS Goapmbaadop mm poem mason a0 soapoada m as Hoes: a mo dawn meoawop Ga Omaommoa no opspaaa5< .o mammam zo_._.<>_mdwo mmaoz mv «N N. m N. «N 1 “D N $338930 1 00 (V 3%; TABLE 3 ANALYSIS OF VARIANCE OF AMPLITUDE SCORES AS A FUNCTION OF HOURS DEPRIVATION Source Of variation Sum of squares d.f. Mean square Levels 612.512 5 122.50** Subjects 737.199 10 73.72 Interaction 606.705 h3 lu.109 **Significant at 1% level Of confidence > .o—. C53) performed on these data. The results support the respect- ive part Of prediction h, namely that amplitude varies directly with increase in deprivation level. In Fig. 7 the changes in amplitude as a function Of deprivation level are given for the LW, SW, and.LD sub- groups Of the % Fed Group. Apparatus failure prevented Obtaining accurate measurement for the entire SD Group, hence the dataare not included. Also there was a slight difference in the setting Of the amplitude measuring instrument for the LW Group, making comparisons with this group somewhat suspect. However, a mean curve for the three groups was determined and plotted. This mean curve is also represented in Fig. 7 and seems to lend support to prediction 5 concerning the linearity Of the amplitude function. Tables h, 5, and 6 present the results Of analyses Of variance for the LW, SW and LD Groups respectively on the amplitude scores. Using the % Fed method prediction h is again supported. The increase in activity with increase in deprivation for the Hours Deprivation Group is represented in Fig. 8 and the analysis Of variance indicating significant differ- ences is found in Table 7. The related graph for the % Fed Groups is Fig. 9 and the analyses Of variance are shown in Tables 8 and 9. It was found that the Lw Group differed significantly from the other three groups regarding both vamzea no woman mean» one Hoboa soapsbaaaop .ao moaned?“ a we Hoonz e no Gaza moeawov a.“ ounOdmoa m HO ovfipaaaad .w sad—Mam 38 own # O. D N 00 m# 00 n d “N 10 d .J [on at! . \\\ Low 3.. N801 $338930 3- 9 TABLE h ANALYSIS OF VARIANCE OF AMPLITUDE SCORES AS A FUNCTION OF % FED (Lw SUBGROUP) Source Of variation Sum Of squares d.f. Mean square Levels 2,713 5 5h2.6 Subjects 8,587 13 660.5 Interaction 11,300 65 173.85 F = 3.12 - significant at 3% level of confidence I. TABLE 5 ANALYSIS OF VARIANCE OF AMPLITUDE SCORES AS A FUNCTION OF % FED (Sw SUBGROUP) r .—.-: Source Of variation Sum Of squares d.f. Mean square Levels 2226.2 5 hh5.2h** Subjects 2373.99 5 h7ho79 Interaction 15h8.35 25 61.93 **Significant at 1% level of confidence 4.- 3. TABLE 6 ANALYSIS OF VARIANCE 0F AMPLITUDE SCORES AS A FUNCTION OF % FED (LD SUBGROUP) — - Source of variation Sum of squares d.f. , Mean square Levels 3h5.7 5 69.1h Subjects 2hhl.97 6 uOé.99 Interaction 98h.67 30 32.82 F = 2.11 - less than 10% level of confidence 42- noapmaaaaop pooa mason mo areas: on» no doaponsa a me HO>oH haa>apo< zo.._.<>_mn_wo manor Axum _ .m oasmam N . . , is. OON loom CYV 100' A ILAIJ. Icon 1000 43 TABLE 7 ANALYSIS OF VARIANCE OF ACTIVITY SCORES AS A FUNCTION OF HOURS DEPRIVATION Source Of variation Sum Of squares d.f. Mean square Levels 1,096,9h9 5 219.390** Subjects 68h,369 10 68,h37 Interaction 912,536 50 -18,251 **Significant at 1% level of confidence .pamama a0 momma asoa new amped seapmaaammp mo qoapodsm m we mpabapo< .m madmam out. fl 6. nu on 2. on a a A d n nhN can nmv l 0.00 1 as. n venom haw omamn I 9032” mam Hiram IIIII name no: HHSMI.‘ L000 gems no? omaQH AilAllOV TABLE 8 ANALYSIS OF VARIANCE OF ACTIVITY SCORES AS A FUNCTION OF % FED (COMBINED SW, LD AND SD SUBGROUPS) Source Of variation Sum Of squares d.f. Mean square %% Levels 395.666 5 79.133 Subjects 1,302,h62 22 59,203 Interaction 927,908 110 8,h36 ** Significant at 1% level Of confidence ,. v» -— -. _. _ 7--- n .,,-,. a~ -.— . -- ~-.7- _.- _--~_‘._7. -_—-.——.—- 46 TABLE 9 ANALYSIS OF VARIANCE 0F ACTIVITY SCORES As A FUNCTION OF % FED (LW SUBGROUP) Source of variation Sum of squares d.f. Mean square Levels 720,u99 5 lhh,100** Subjects 2.258.717 13 173.7u7 Interaction 1,257,629 65 19,3h8 **Significant at 1% level of confidence ,fl 11 means and variances so that a comparison with the other three groups could not be made; however, the LD, SD, and SW subgroups were found to have comparable means and variances, hence were combined into a single group (Table 8). Figure 10 shows the mean curve for these three sub- groups. All the results using the activity measure clearly support prediction h. Curves representing amplitude, latency, and activity as functions of hours of deprivation are combined on Fig. 11 tO indicate the similarity Of shapes, particularly regarding the drOp in all curves at the two-hour level. The Chi square test for linearity as given in Table 10 shows that the deviations from linearity for the amplitude data in the Hours Deprivation Group is not significant, indicating the rectilinearity is the best assumption for the relationship. Using the amplitude data from the LN subgroup of the % Fed Group, which appeared when plotted to be somewhat curvilinear, it was found that there was still no evidence to indicate any significant deviations from rectilinearity. This Chi square test is found in Table 11. Due to the extreme variability Of the latency data, nothing can be said concerning the true shape of the curve. The activity data for the SD, SN, and LD subgroups combined were analyzed for linearity in Table 12 and the {9 1‘ Hm>ma doapwbaaemp a0 cOapocsa m we AQm.2H.3mV mmdoam pecanEOo moanp mo mmaOom mpa>apom dmoz .OH madmaw Guam» c. on on he on — d a _ a on Is. no... 1 ohm [10¢ 1 out ALIAIAOV coausaaaaop pooa macros .HO aoofiza on» .HO soapoga m we epspaadfiw was honour.” .mpaaapos. zo.h<>_mdmo manor 0? «Sum N. m Awm3wQZMP:>_._.0< Ill. \ \ \ \ \ \ mu- \\ O, 0 [III \ ....I. x A ’74 \ I, \ II x I]! \ III \ II \ 0 [III \ N”. ”’O\ .aa enemas N . BEDSVBH BSNOdSBCI 50 TABLE 10 CHI-SQUARE TEST OF LINEARITY FOR AMPLITUDE DATA OF HOURS DEPRIVATION GROUP Pearson 'r' .557 Eta coefficient .527 Chi-square 2.h96 - not significant TABLE 11 CHI-SQUARE TEST OF LINEARITY FOR AMPLITUDE DATA OF LW SUBGROUP Q“ 1 Pearson 'r' .3h6 Eta coefficient .365 Chi-square 1.22 - not significant CE It TABLE 12 CHI-SQUARE TEST OF LINEARITY FOR ACTIVITY DATA OF THE SD, LD, AND SW SUBGROUPS COMBINED Pearson 'r' .351 Eta coefficient .388 Chi-square 4.36 - not significant A t 03.- deviations were found not to be significant; hence, activity as a function Of deprivation level in the % Fed animals is most safely considered as rectilinear. However, the analysis of activity in the Hours Deprivation Group resulted in a Chi square of 9.3. A Chi square of 9.49 is needed for significance at the 5% level Of confidence with h d.f.; however, one would still not reject the possibility that the data are curvilinear. InSpection Of these data indicates that negative acceleration would probably be the best assumption if the drOp from 1 hour to 2 hours is ignored. However, neither rectilinearity nor negative acceleration could be considered similar to the function found by Yama- guohi (21). Therefore no support was found for prediction 7. With reSpect to prediction 8 which is concerned with the possibility Of using activity as a measurement para- meter Of the construct Drive, three different analyses were made. Each analysis used a different amount Of variation in the matching of the activity levels. The first Pearson 'r' (Table 13), using subgroup SD Of the % Fed Group to equate for size and type of reward with the Hours Depri- vation Group, was computed allowing a plus or minus functu- ation of 2 activity points. The N was limited to 16 with this procedure and the derived 'r' Of 0.29 between latencies was not significant. Allowing a 20 point deviation (Table 1h) an N of 23 was Obtained from the same groups as above. In this comparison the last 3 training days were used as a En gs TABLE 13 CORRELATION COEFFICIENT BETWEEN LATENCIES OF HOURS DEPRIVATION GROUP AND SD SUBGROUP EQUATED AS TO ACTIVITY WITH A FLUCTUATION OF 2 POINTS 35.75 2X = 1111.92 ZY 2x2 = 271.7722 2Y2 200.1505 ZXY = 138.671; '1" = .29 + not significant CI 1 0'“ TABLE 1H CORRELATION COEFFICIENT BETWEEN LATENCIES OF HOURS DEPRIVATION GROUP AND SD SUBGROUP EQUATED AS TO ACTIVITY WITH A FLUCTUATION OF 20 POINTS T‘T '— i A d 32.9u EX = 14-5017 ZY - 130.3515 2Y2 = 85.2280 NI >4 m I 'r' = .211 - not significant ‘3". source instead Of the testing days. The resulting 'r' Of 0.2h was not significant. Table 15 shows the 'r' which was based on a 5 point deviation using the testing days Of the SD Group as‘a source. This increased the N to 3h, but the Obtained 'r' Of 0.26 was still not significant. Because of the previously mentioned apparatus failure there was no accurate amplitude data for the SD subgroup; however, the LD subgroup had the same range Of amplitude score as the Hours Deprivation Group and was selected for the amplitude analysis. The correlation between amplitude scores of the LD subgroup and the Hours Deprivation Group, equated as tO activity plus or minus 5 points was found to be -O.27 (Table 16). Prediction 8 clearly receives no support. Prediction 9, concerning size Of reward, was definitely not confirmed at least as far as latency data are concerned. This is Obvious by inSpection Of Fig. 9. The small reward groups performed better than the large reward which is Opposite to the prediction; therefore, no statistical test was employed. Regarding the amplitude data very little can be said because of the change in setting of the measuring device for the LW Group and the confounding Of reward type in the other two groups. The crossing Of the SW and LW curves (Fig. 6) seems to indicate, however, that at least all differences were not in favor Of the large reward group. It may be concluded that in this eXperiment size Of reward 5:}? TABLE 15 CORRELATION COETTTCIENT BETWEEN LATENCIES 0F HOURS DEPRIVATION GROUP AND SD SUBGROUP EQUATED AS TO ACTIVITY WITH A FLUCTUATION OF 5 POINTS EX = 65.5 215 = 126.11 2 2 EXY = 311.7.77 'r' = .26 - not significant 58 TABLE 16 CORRELATION COEFFICIENT BETWEEN AMPLITUDE SCORES OF HOURS DEPRIVATION GROUP AND LD SUBGROUP EQUATED AS TO ACTIVITY WITH A FLUCTUATION 0F 5 POINTS 2x = 799 EY == 656 2X2 = 28.955 2Y2 18,668 ZXY = 21,1187 'r' = ~.27 - not significant with the values employed was not positively correlated with performance. Reference to Fig. 7 indicates that there were no differences among the SW, LD, and SD subgroups regarding activity which would tend to support prediction 10. It was found that the LW Group had lost an average Of 17 grams body weight from the beginning Of the experiment while on the average all other groups gained weight. Inasmuch as there were no differences between LD, SD, and SW subgroups, the indications are that the increased activity Of the LW Group was due to the loss of body weight instead of to size or type Of reward. With reSpeOt to the extinction data there is support for prediction 11 as to absolute numbers. However, as indicated in Table 17, the variances are much too disparate to allow any valid 't' test to be performed. The only valid analysis which could be performed was between the LW and SD subgroups as given in Table 18. The resulting significant difference, however, gives no indication of the particular variable bringing about the difference because of the con- founding Of size and type of reward variables. It may possibly indicate an interaction effect between the two. Consequently no statement is made concerning prediction 12 which posited an influence of size Of reward on the number Of trials to extinction. Table 19 gives the correlation coefficients between TABLE 17 "F" TEST 0N VARIANCES OF THE NUMBER OF TRIALS T0 EXTINCTION FOR HIGH AND LOW DRIVE SUBJECTS 0F % FED GROUP High Low 0-2 676. 82. Mean2 811e2 9Se66 F = 8.h9 - significant at 1% level of confidence TABLE 18 6 1 't' TEST OF THE NUMBER OF TRIALS TO EXTINCTION OF THE LW SUBGROUP VS. THE SD SUBGROUP M LW SD 0’ 9.3 8.h8 I 20.6 10.3 N 9. 10. 01h 3.29 2.82 0711:. = “‘33 t -.-. M. .-.- 2.38 - significant at the 5% level of “.33 confidence TABLE 19 CORRELATION COEFFICIENTS BETWEEN THE DEPRIVATION LEVELS OF BOTH HOURS DEPRIVATION GROUP AND % FED GROUP WITH AMPLITUDE SCORES Group Correlation '1" Hours Deprivation .53 -- significant at 1% level % Fed .27 -- not significant on or deprivation level and amplitude for both % Fed and Hours Deprivation Groups. The 'r' Of 0.53 for the Hours Depri- vation Group as compared to the 'r' Of 0.27 for the % Fed Group (utilizing the data Of the LD subgroup as it is the most comparable group to the Hours Deprivation Group) indicates a trend toward the superiority Of the Hours Deprivation Group because Of the significance Of the 0.53 correlation and lack Of significance in the 'r' Of 0.27. The difference between the coefficients was not statistically significant. These results do not support prediction 13. Prediction 1h receives no support from the data given in Table 20. The standard deviations for the % Fed Group are consistently larger than for the Hours Deprivation Group, indicating that the procedure used for the latter group served to delimit deprivation more precisely. The slight superiority of deprivation level in pre- dicting amplitude over activity predicting amplitude is not of sufficient magnitude to either support or reject prediction 15. The assumption of no difference is the safest. Data are given in Table 21. ($4 TABLE 20 STANDARD DEVIATIONS FROM COMPARABLE POINTS OF AMPLITUDE SCORES OF THE HOURS DEPRIVATION AND % FED GROUPS r GrOUP At 0% or At 90% or 22% hours 2 hours deprivation deprivation Hours Deprivation 5.31 3.09 % Fed -- LN 17.61 9.25 LD 11-9h 5.31 SN lu.1u 6.58 TABLE 21 CORRELATION COEFFICIENTS UTILIZING DEPRIVATION LEVEL TO PREDICT AMPLITUDE COMPARED TO ACTIVITY TO PREDICT AMPLITUDE CH Deprivation level correlated with amplitude Hours Deprivation Group 'r' = .53 % Fed Group 'r' = .3h Activity correlated with amplitude Hours Deprivation Group 'r' 2 .50 % Fed Group 'r' = .23 68 DISCUSSION As is the case in many experiments, the peripheral data, analyzed simply because they are available, have pro- duced illuminating implications. The first indication that activity level does not reflect Drive, EEEHEE’ is found in the gradual decrease in activity Of animals under 22% hours deprivation (Fig. 2). The decline for these animals actually exceeded in some degree the decline in activity Of the satiated animals. The lack Of any difference between the groups presents evidence contrary to the implications assumed by Hull (10) to be present in the work of Wade (20), Richter (l6) and others; namely, that the general activity Of the organism is a function Of deprivation level. The present data indicate that with the gradual decline in activity an asymptote is reached and after introduction of food into the apparatus there is an increase in activity which shows the common negative acceleration of a 'normal learning curve'. This apparently represents activity as a function of learning rather than Of Drive or deprivation. If this be the case, then the consequent side Of Hull's Operational definition Of Drive has been removed and Drive has been left hanging by its antecedent conditions. As further support for the thesis that the bulk Of the activity measure does not reflect Drive but Habit, is the lack of f. correlation between the performance (sEr) measures of the two groups (Hours Deprivation and % Fed) when theyvvere equated as to activity level (prediction 8). Besides showing that activity cannot be used as a basic measurement parameter for the quantification Of Drive, the lack Of correlation indicates that activity is peculiar to the situation and to the past experience Of the organism rather than tO a 'state' of the organism which would be common to all members Of the Species when they were put under the same deprivation conditions. The question.may then arise as to why activity varies positively with deprivation level as was indicated by the analyses Of variance and the correlation coefficients. And even more disturbing is the question Of why amplitude varies with deprivation level if neither Drive nor habit is invoked to explain it. The answers to these questions must necessarily involve some reformulation of current Hullian theory. It is believed that utilizing our knowledge Of physiology, in conjunction with a strict mechanistic approach to the study of the overt behavior Of the organism, we may arrive at a plausible answer to the question, 'Is organismic activity a function Of generalized Drive or is it a function Of learning?‘ In answering this question.we must first recognize that a certain amount Of activity is due to the structural 68 connections between afferent and efferent nerve fibers. Modern neurOphysiOlogy provides a comparatively thorough list of the various innate responses Of the organism to external and internal stimuli and also lists the intercon- nections from sense organs to the various parts of the nervous system which could result in innate responses. These reflexes are not limited to minute pupillary dilations or knee jerks, but may involve complete bodily orientation. The ramifications and interconnections of the Olfactory system are particularly important and peculiarly ignored by the comparative psychologists. As an indication of the numerous possibilities for reflex action originating with Olfactory stimuli we find the Olfactory bulb connected to the cortex in the anterior perforated Space, the lamina terminalis and the gyrus subcallosus; In the hypothalamus it connects with the mamillary bodiesaand the ventro- medial hypothalamic neucleus. The habenula Of the epi- thalamus is particularly important in that it is an Olfacto- somatic correlation center. Connections also go to the dorsal tegmental nucleus, the interpeduncular nucleus, the nuclei of 5, 7, the superior and inferior salivatory nuclei, the dorsal efferent nucleus and nucleus ambiguous. Most psychologists are aware of the autonomic connections with the Olfactory bulb, but unaware Of the connections with the skeletal muscle system via the habenula. This latter fact will prove more important when we consider the amplitude data. Besides the routing Of the neural pathways we also have some knowledge as to nerve fiber action in conjunction with spread Of effect. We know, for instance, that the greater the afferent stimulation the greater the efferent resultant - especially concerning innate responses, for example it is obvious that an auditory stimulus Of above threshold intensity may result in the reflex action of neck and head orientatiO; in the direction of the stimulus, whereas increased intensity will result in increased bodily involvement, culminating in the well known startle response. It should also be acknowledged that not all efferent result- ants are measureable or even observable as they may be visceral as well as skeletal. . It is here being suggested that the major activity component, however, is due to learning, and not a resuhsant, at least entirely, Of innate receptor-effector connections qualified by the action Of "generalized Drive". Using an elicitation theory of reinforcement” which asserts that S-R connectiOns are set up by contigual association Of a new stimulus with an inherent S-R bond or reflex, it is readily visible that if we can assert an innate tendency for the animal to approach food, that approach would involve locomotion or activity. It also follows that with Optimal bodily conditions the approach response would *Current Michigan State College theory. Papers soon to be published on it by Dr. M. Ray Denny and Jack L. Maatsch. {I 70 be enhanced and a Simple conditioning pattern appears as follows: Bodily conditions -- Food -- approach or locomotion smell The conditioning pattern above says that bodily con- ditions will bring about activity, but the same principle holds in this situation as holds in any conditioning sit- uation, that is the stimulus complex must remain constant, any change in stimulus conditions will result in decreased response strength. Also, before a locomotion reSponse will occur in a particular situation there must be a series of reinforced trials in which the constant stimuli of environ- mental surroundings are associated with the innate Food --- locomotion bond. This then presents a complex stimulus situation in.which bodily conditions are merely a small portion of the total complex, hence its effects are minimal and may be superseded by almost any additional extraneous stimulus, until learning has occurred in.the situation. Following well substantiated conditioning theory we know that if the organism is put under given deprivation con- ditions so as to keep the internal stimuli constant and is placed in a given apparatus, and a reSponse is learned under these conditions, then any deviation from these con- ditions results in generalization. It is asserted here that the reSponse learned in the present situation was not only the panel pushing response but that the locomotion reSponse (not new to the animal, but new to the animal in on H the particudar'situation) is also learned. From this typeAanalyses it follows that in a new situation the organism must learn to eat or make the associ- ation between a stimulus complex and the response to food. It is seen then that the maximal activity Of the animal will occur when bodily conditions are Optimal and the reSponse of eating in a particular situation is asymptotic. If either of the above conditions does not hold, activity declines. The decrease in activity of the animals in all groups away from the original 22% hour training deprivation level is then asserted to be a generalization effect instead of a decreased Drive effect. The Obvious deduction from this approach would be that if the animal were fed in a new situation under given bodily conditions (1.0., depri- vation level) that shifting the animal to a different deprivation level would result in generalization from the learned reSponse and a consequent decrease in activity. Now, it does not seem reasonable that if an animal were trained under 10 hours deprivation that when it was put under 2h hours deprivation that a decrease in activity would occur. Yet, this result is what this analysis demands; however, it is very possible that there is an interaction effect between the innate reflexes and the learned reSponses such that at low levels the innate reflexes might predominate, so that if an actual decrease from 10 to 24 hours did not occur, the increase would be very slight, not at all /§ s It comparable to the increase between those approximate levels in the present study. Actually in the present study there is some evidence for this stimulus generalization effect in that there was a decline in activity from the 22% hour level to the AB hour level shown in Fig. 8. The reduction in activity is definitely not significant, but even if it is considered to be a straight line, this still does not compare with the curve derived by Yamaguchi (21) who stated that Drive is maximal at 59 hours deprivation. In fact. the function Yamaguchi derived shows the increase between 22% and AB hours to be very steep. That the absence Of increase is not due to the approach to the upper limit Of the apparatus in recording activity is seen in the fact that at times several of the animals scored over 1000 on the activity scale and the mean for the 22% hours group equals only u25, which leaves ample room for increase as deprivation increased. Theoretically, to a certain extent amplitude and activity are correlated. In the free situation in which the organism responds directly to a bit Of food the locomotion of the organism will be highly correlated with the amplitude Of the reSponse; in fact, they may be identical.‘ In the panel pushing device, the difference between the two is increased principally by forcing the animal to make a pushing reSponse instead of a simple approach reSponse which would be entirely determined by locomotion. The difference between .3 C!) the two measures allows the possibility for Operation of a new factor in the amplitude measures. The additional factor is the Olfactory stimulus. If we can assume that the deviation of the LW Group (Fig. 9) from the others is due to loss of body weight, it may be readily assumed that the activity levels do not deviate as a function of type or amount of reward. In the Hours Deprivation Group (Fig. 8) the range is between 150 and 500 which compares closely to the ranges of the % Fed animals (Fig. 9). However, in making the available amplitude comparisons we can see that the animals who were rewarded with wet mash were definitely superior at the high drive levels; whereas, the groups were essentially the same at the low levels. Figure 6 shows this quite clearly regarding the LD Group; although the data for the SD Group is not plotted, the highest mean amplitude recorded for this group was 36 which coincides with the high point for the LD Group. Also, if four points are added to the amplitude scores Of the Hours Deprivation Group to make the low point coincide with the low point mean Of the % Fed Groups, we find that the maximum amplitude score attained is about 36. These three groups just men- tioned were rewarded with the dry pellets. A glance at Fig. 6 again shows that the high points for the wet mash rewarded groups were above AB in both cases. The differences between the groups cannot be validly tested, due to differences «3 543:: in settings of the instrumentemd size of reward but the weight of the evidence indicates that wet mash rewarded groups reSpond more forcibly at the high deprivation levels than do the dry pellet rewarded groups. These data probably tie in with the food preference studies carried out by YOung (23); however, if one continues to adhere to the strict S-R outlook, one would be forced into asserting the Operation of an additional stimulus to account for the preference. In this case, it is believed to be the Operation of an Olfactory stimulus. To the human receptors, at any rate, there is a decided difference in the intensity Of the Olfactory sensation between the wet mash and the dry pellet. It does not seem to be overly anthrOpomorphic to suggest that there is definitely a discriminable difference between the two types of reward for the rat. It may be noticed that there is a definite increase from 22% hours to A8 hours in the amplitude data Of the Hours Deprivation Group (Fig. 6). Although the increase is not statistically significant, it still indicates a trend away from the generalization gradient from the 22% hour training conditions asserted to hold for activity. This may be an interaction effect between the drive stimuli and the olfactory stimuli. We know from Tinbergen's results (18) that the internal conditions of an organism must be Optimal before the innate reSponse to a stimulus will be Optimal. The maximal amplitude measure may reflect the existence Of re ’Q U‘. the Optimal internal conditions prerequisite for elicitation of approach response to food by the external Olfactory stimulus. Also, from Tinbergen (18) we know that the ex- ternal stimulus which elicits a reSponse must also be Of Optimal quality and intensity. It is suggested that the wet mash more closely approximates the Optimal conditions than does the dry pellet. No measurements were taken beyond the training level for the % Fed Groups; hence, evidence for this is not available. The preceding analysis asserts that the amplitude Of reSponse is a function largely of the eliciting Olfactory stimulus; in other words, if no Olfactory cue were avail- able, there should be very little change in amplitude as a function of change in internal conditions (i.e., deprivation level). This, then would remove amplitude as a possible means Of quantifying "Drive". In fact, if activity is a function Of learning and amplitude is a function of the eliciting stimulus, there is little basis for asserting the action Of the construct Drive unless a case for "Drive" may be built upon the fluctuations of latency with fluctu- ations in deprivation level. With regard to latency the present data could be dis- missed as apparently confounded by the action Of unknown variables, changing, so tO Speak, the pOpulations from which the various samples were drawn. From the statistical view- point, this might be the thing to do, but it is the author's 76 contention that the effect of variables on the variance Of groups has been totally ignored by most investigators and that the variances may be the source Of important information unattainable by reference to group means or correlation coefficients. Reference to Fig. 5 will Show the increase of the standard deviations Of the various groups as a function of decrease in deprivation level. Comparison of this figure to Fig. A shows the high degree of correlation between the latency measures and the variances Of the groups. One fact is particularly outstanding. At the training level, under zero % Fed conditions, the latencies, and the standard deviations for that matter, are very similar. In other words, the animals were apparently reSponding with approxi- mately the same latencies, and it was not until the stimulus conditions (internal stimuli) were changed that the means and variances began tO become diSparate. In all ththe % Fed Groups the 'F's' were significant but the variances were also very highly Significant. In the Hours Deprivation Group however, the variances were homogeneous and the means were not Significantly different from one another. This presumably indicates that the differences in means is only a reflection Of the difference in variances which shows that in this case the effect of varying deprivation level is to increase variability Of reSponse, not to affect the Speed Of the reSponse directly. When considering the differences between subgroups, the type of reward rather than Size Of reward seems to be the meortant variable. The means and variances Of the wet mash group are consistently below those of the dry pellet reward group. This represents an interaction effect between type of reward and drive level. This could be an indication of different gradients Of generalization as a function Of different types Of reward. This again may be explained by referring to the interaction between internal conditions and external stimuli emphasized by Tinbergen. It may be that an external stimulus Of lesser intensity will exert progressively less effect as bodily conditions are altered than will a stimulus Of stronger intensity. The indications are that latency does not reflect the same principle as either amplitude or activity. There are indications from the work Of Cotton (3) that the major portion of latency is due to the intervention of competing reSponses. Gotten found that after eliminating trials during which reSponses incompatible with performing the learned response occurred, the difference in latencies between deprivation levels from 0 to 22 hours was only 0.h of a second, measured in an eight foot long runway. This component is so small that the effects of deprivation level on latency Of response can be considered negligible as far as representing a relevant behavioral variable. Cotten's findings are supported in some degree by the results Of the present study, when the range of latency scores within a group for all levels of deprivation are con- sidered. The smallest range was 0.2 second occurring in the LW subgroup, the greatest was h.7 seconds in the LD subgroup. The other ranges were 2.h seconds for the SD subgroup and 0.9 second for the SW subgroup. The greatest range was more than 23 times that of the smallest range. The difference appears to be a function of the type of reward interacting with varying internal conditions. With near Optimal reward, in this case large wet mash, bodily conditions are for all practical purposes behaviorally in- consequential with reSpect to latency. The type of reward seems to be a much more important variable in determining mean latency than deprivation level. The charge that latency Of reSponse is not a reflection Of sEr is particularly important in view of the fact that sEr was quantified by Hull and his associates on the basis of the latency Of a bar pressing reSponse (5, 6, 12, 22). Drive was held constant at 23 hours deprivation and the number Of reinforcements were varied. The construct sEr was then quantified on the basis Of the derived latencies. In the body Of one article (6) it is explicitly stated that the validity of the quantification is dependent upon the equation sEr = er X D and the appeal to the validity of the Drive construct was '79 in reference to Perin's data (15), in which reinforcements were held constant at 16 and the deprivation level was varied, in a situation similar to the one employed in the sEr quantification series. Perin pointed out in this study that the latency data were not amenable to any sort of analysis, so he presented most Of his results in terms of the number of trials to extinction. It was the extinction data which Hull used as support for his sEr = er X D equation. In addition when Yamaguchi (21) quantified Drive more fully he also used the number of trials to extinction as his measure. It would seem that if latency yields data adequate for quantifying sEr it would also yield data adequate for quantifying Drive which supposedly is reflected in sEr. Why then, was the utilization of two different measures necessary? The results Of the present study seem to indicate that the reason for this is that variation Of internal bodily stimuli introduces extreme variability in latency scores. If latency is considered a function Of the number Of competing reSponses in action at the time, this would mean that latency would probably not be a measure Of momentary effective reaction potential and therefore not a valid basis for quantifying sEr. It should also be pointed out that the standard deviations for latencies at the various reinforcement levels were not publialed in the quantification series, making it impossible to determine whether or not even those variances were homogeneous. 8'0 The extinction data in the present study were of small value because Of the small N in each group and the extreme variability of the data. There was an increase in the mean number Of trials to extinction with increase in depri- vation in every group. However even this measure cannot be used as evidence for greater strength Of reaction potential mainly because Of the lack of any Operant level data and the possibility that much of the panel pushing after the first few extinction trials may be merely a concomitant of activity. Such possibilities may be easily tested by allowing untrained animals access to the panel after an initial activity recording period and comparing the number Of panel activations with the recorded activity. In the Hours Deprivation Group there is one phenomenon which is particularly interesting. This is the dip in every curve at the two hour level. Every measure showed a decrease in reSponse strength from one to two hours. None of the decreases reached the 5% level of significance. However, the coincidence of the drOp in all three curves is has hardly attributable to chance, particularly since there were no consistent reversals Of the % Fed Group curves. The dip, probably more than anything, reflects the period Of time required for complete assimilation of food into the blood stream. This supports the work Of individuals who claim that the neural basis for hunger is not localized 8’1 solely in the stomach, but is more essentially dependent upon central neural centers which reSpond to changes in blood chemistry, i.e., chemo-receptors. The present data indicate that there is probably a constant decrease in the organism's reSponsiveness from complete satiation to a point somewhere near two hours after satiation. Reference to minimal effective internal conditions should probably be in terms of this two hour level instead Of zero hours Of deprivation from time Of satiation. This observation is consistent with the foregoing drive stimulus analysis and does not support the "Drive" construct formulation. The drive stimulus point of view would postulate that any decreased firing of receptors would, innately, reduce the magnitude Of certain aspects of behavior. AlsO, decreased receptor firing would result in internal stimulus conditions further out on the gradient of generalization from the originally conditioned stimulus complex and would serve to reduce performance of the learned aspects Of behavior. The two hour dip may be one Of the causes for the curvi- linearity of the activity data for the Hours Deprivation Group, which is in contrast to the same data for the % Fed Group which was rather definitely a rectilinear function. Another possible difference was the fact that the Hours Deprivation Group was tested at A8 hours which is past the original training point, and the decrease rather than a con- tinued increase would tend to make the function appear to be 822 curved instead of rectilinear. The % Fed Group was never extended beyond the point of original training hence only one side of the gradient was involved. Although the results of this study very definitely show that activity cannot be used as a means Of quantifying Drive and that the whole concept of a generalized Drive is extremely tenuous it does not present exactly incontrovert- ible evidence against Drive. The Drive theorist might say that every time a reference in this paper was made to internal conditions or internal stimuli or drive stimuli that the word 'Drive' could have been substituted. However, this would at any rate point up the confusion existing between the concepts of Drive and drive stimulus. If the Drive theorist did not make this assertion then it should be clear that the behavioral results attributed to Drive may be explained more parsimoniously with more primary physiological and behavioral concepts. The value of doing this lies chiefly in the fact that modern research methods are enabling the physiologist and ethologist to investigate more thoroughly the various aSpects of stimuli and their interaction with an organism. We have shown in the present paper that Drive cannot be tied down on the consequent side Of the Operational definition by measuring energy expendi- ture in the form of activity. This means that Drive, then, must be accepted as a matter of faith and not scientifically testable. It must be acknowkadged that the present analysis 00 Q5: consists in part of 22 222 explanations; however the point is that the explanations were made by appealing to well known or highly substantiated ethological and physiological data which lead to deductions which may be experimentally investigated. Obviously if one denies the concept of Drive, then something must be said concerning an even.more important concept - that Of reinforcement, which for the Hullians is drive reduction. If one accepts the notion Of reduction Of drive stimulation instead of "Drive" reduction then the essence Of the principle may possibly be retained. However, then again, of what use in this connection is the concept of generalized "Drive"? Although various individuals have been interpreting drive reduction as reduction of drive stimulation, even this tack becomes difficult in the light Of some evidence from the present study. Three of four activity curves and one of three Of the amplitude curves in the % Fed Groups showed an elevation of the 10% Fed level over the 0% which might indicate that feeding a slight amount of food would actually increase the intensity of the stimulation instead of acting to reduce it. This ties in with the results from the experiments on pro-feeding (1, 2) which show that pro-feeding seems to aid performance, increases activity, etc. If this is the case that pre- feeding increases activity (shown in three of the four groups Of the present study), then a bit of food.would neither QC a: reduce drive stimuli nor generalized "Drive" and thusly the possibility Of using these principles as explanations of reinforcement is effectively eliminated. The value Of the present study lies not so much in the absolute evidence which it p'esents for or against a given theory but in the Opening up of new methodological approaches to clearing up several pertinent problems in modern learning theory. The technique utilized in this study measures practically every aSpect Of measurable behavior I Of the organism, enabling the author to make inter-behavioral comparisons which heretofore could not be made. The failure to do this in the past shows up OSpecially in the formu- lation of what were supposed to be reflections of reaction potential (sEr). As this discussion has attempted to point out, these measurements may be only slightly related. The major portions Of each seem to be a result of the Operation of very different principles, with particular reference to habit, innate reflexes and competing reSponses. oo- Q)". SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS The present investigation is concerned with the possibility of utilizing the activity level of an organism as a direct reflection Of Drive when two different methods Of inducing hunger conditions are employed. The results of such a study should present evidence as to whether or not activity level may be used as a measurement parameter which would make possible the quantification of the construct Drive independent of the commonly used reaction potential (sEr) measures, e.g., latency, amplitude, trials to extinction. Further, the size and type of reward are varied to give an indication Of the effect of these variables on the behavior of organisms while performing under varying drive levels. A combined activity box and panel pushing device was employed in the study. The panel pushing device was so arranged as to provide measurements of latency of reSponse, amplitude Of reSponse and the number Of trials to extinction. Forty-eight male albino rats were used in the study, and were divided into two major groups: 1. Hours Deprivation Group N = 11 2. % Fed Group N = 37 The % Fed Group was divided into four subgroups on the basis Of size and type Of reward as follows: (1 85 l. LW subgroup (Large wet mash - 0.2 gm) N 2. SW subgroup (Small wet mash - 0.08 gm) N 3. LD subgroup (Large dry pellet - 0.2 gm) N M. SD subgroup (Small dry pellet - 0.08 gm) N 1% 10 All animals were given habituation in the apparatus for a period of five days, and received training on the panel pushing device 10 trials per day for the next 8 consecutive days. All animals were under 22% hours food deprivation. 0n the testing days the animals in the Hours Deprivation Group were satiated and then given four trials in the appar- atus either 1, 2, 6, 12, 2A, or A8 hours later. The % Fed animals were fed either 0, 10, 25, 50, 75, or 90 percent Of the total amount of food required to produce satiation, computed on the basis of the amount of food each animal had eaten to satiation on the last four days Of the training procedure. They were then given four trials in the apparatus three hours after they had consumed the stipulated percentage. All animals performed under each deprivation level in their reSpective groups, being assigned to each level in a ran- domized order. Activity level, latency and amplitude were recorded on each trial. At the conclusion of the testing days all animals were extinguished under various deprivation levels. The findings were as follows: 1. Activity is not a reflection of Drive pg; 32, but is dependent upon learning in a given situation. 2. Activity cannot be used aS a measurement parameter for Drive, inasmuch as the correlations between reSponse ,fi L7". measure Of different animals equated as to activity Show no significance. 3. There is definite interaction effect between type of reward and deprivation level. A. Latency as a measure Of reaction potential is extremely suSpect. 5. The results do not support the notion Of general- ized Drive as a hypothetical construct. The possibility that the various measures Of reaction potential were essentially measures Of very different factors was discussed and evidence was Offered for this point of view. l. 2. 3. A. 5. 6. 7. 8. 9. 10. ll. 12. 86" REFERENCES Anderson, E. E., The externalization of drive IV. The effect of prefeeding on the maze performance Of hungry non-rewarded rats. ‘g. comp. Psychol., l9hl, 31s 3M9 ' 352e Bruce, R. H., The effect of lessening the drive upon performance bg white rats in a maze. ‘g. comp. O Psychol., 193 25, 225 - 2H8. Cotton, J. W., The relationship between running time in a straight runway and length Of the period of food deprivation. To be published in 1. exp. Psychol. Davis, R. H., The quantification of drive I. Incentive values Of food and water. Ph.D. thesis on file in the Michigan State College Library, 1953. Felsinger, J. M., Gladstone, A. I., Yamaguchi, H. G., and Hull, C. L., Reaction latency as a function of the number of reinforcements (N). g, exB. Psychol., 19h7. 37. 21h - 228. Gladstone, A. 1., Yamaguchi, H. G., Hull, C. L., and Felsinger, J. M., Some functional relationships of reaction potential (sEr) and related phenomena. ie 3.2. PSzChOIe, 19(47, 37, 510 II 526. Guthrie, E. H., The s cholo Of learni . New York: Harper and BrOEEers FuBIISEerST I935. Helson, H. (Ed.), Theoretical foundations g; psycholqu. New York: Van Nostrand, 1951. Holt, E. B., Animal drive and the lea nin process. New York: Henry HOIE and Company, . Hull, C. L., Principles 2f behavior. New York: D. Appleton-Century 00., Inc., I9u3. , Behavior postulates and corollaries - lguge FEEOhOI. ROVee 19509 57’ 173 ' 1800 , Felsinger, J. M., Gladstone, A. I., and Yamaguchi, H. G., A prOposed quantification of habit strength. Psychol. Rev., 19h7, SH, 237 - 25h. Q m.— on... r O I a - ~— . _ - k, u I O I I e .l. »m I “P O O ' . h. .— fl 6 1 ( a e i Q l ' t I O ‘ . I t I O I . t C 0 ' {f 0 O t . a... '- . - Q 0 C ' ‘o c. ' ' i s 0 ' e . e \ r O 1 H.,—M.“ C O h \ Q e .K - . (: C 0 ~—.-—_ .—.——_I...— --..__. f. l . . r . . __ -_.‘..____._l.. ._..._. 13. 1k. 15. 16. 17. 18. 19. 20. 21. 22. 23. Keller, F. W. and Schoenfeld, w. N., Princi les g£ psycholoE%. New York: Appleton-CenEury-Crofts, rice, 0 Maatsch, J. L. and Behan, R. A., A more rigorous theoretical language. Psychol. Rev., 1953, 60, 189 - 196. Perin, T. 0., Behavior potentiality as a joint function of the amount of training and the degree of hunger at thz time of extinction. l. exE. Psychol., 19k2, 30. 9 . Richter, C. P., Animal behavior and internal drives. guar. Egg. Biology, 1927, 2, 307 - 343. Skinner, B. F., The behavior 2: organisms. New York: D. Appleton-Century Eompany, 1938. Tinbergen, N., The stud 22 instinct. Oxford: Clarendon Press, IGSI. Tolman, E. C., Purposive behavior in animals and men. New York: The Gentury Co., 1932. Wada, T., An eXperimental study of hunger in its relation to activity. Arch. Psychol., 1922, No. 57. Yamaguchi, H. G., Drive (D) as a function of hours of hunger (h). ‘g. 6X2. Psychol.,1951, u2, 108 - 117. , Hull, C. L., Felsinger, J. M., and Gladstone, K.—I., Characteristics of dispersions ' based on the pooled momentary reaction potentials (sEr) of a group. Psychol. 531., 19u8, SS, 216- 238. Youngé P. T., Motivation of behavior. New York: Wiley, 193 . '—' py <——.— I, K O MUM—+962 a WW 9 H8 Illa] Ill-0 S I." R “‘79 E H V I1