MSU BLTURNING MATERIALS: Place in book drop to Lin/nuns remove this checkout from “—- your record. FINES wm be charged if book is returned after the date stamped be10w. ANALYSIS OF ULTRASONIC COMMUNICATION DURING SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN DEER MICE (PEROMYSCUS MANICULATUS BAIRDI) By Steven M. Pomerantz A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Psychology and Neuroscience Program 1981 ABSTRACT ANALYSIS OF ULTRASONIC COMMUNICATION DURING SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN DEER MICE (PEROMYSCUS MANICULATUS BAIRDI) By Steven M. Pomerantz The present study investigated the role of ultrasonic communication by deer mice during sexual behavior. In sexual behavior tests with sexually receptive females, males that copulated always produced 35-kHz ultrasonic vocalizations, whereas males failing to copulate seldom produced ultrasonic vocalizations. There was no evidence to indicate that females produced ultrasounds. Hormonal status of the female influenced male ultrasound production. Males vocalized with sexually receptive females, but rarely vocalized with ovariectomized females. Moreover, sexually receptive females that were anesthetized elicited male ultrasonic calling. A courtship role for precOpulatory ultrasounds was indicated by the finding that males always produced ultrasounds prior to the onset of copulation. Furthermore, during the period when males were producing precopulatory ultrasounds, increases in male solicitation rate, female locomotor activity, and in the proximity maintained between the male and female were observed. Thus, prec0pulatory Steven M. Pomerantz ultrasounds were associated with both elevated levels of male sexual behavior and female proceptive behavior (i.e., female behavior that facilitates cepulation). Male ultrasounds do not appear to play an important role during copulation, since vocalization rate declined during this period, while other measures of male and female sexual behavior remained high. Following ejaculation, male ultrasonic calling resumed despite substantial decreases in other male sexual behaviors. Also, females were very active during this period, indicating that postejaculatory vocalizations may serve to sustain proceptive behavior of the female during a time when the male is in a withdrawn state. Gonadal hormones influenced male ultrasonic calling. Testos- terone, as well as two of its major metabolites, dihydrotestosterone, and estradiol, restored ultrasound production in long-term castrated males. Combined treatment with subthreshold dosages of both . dihydrotestosterone and estradiol activated male ultrasonic calling and male copulatory behavior. This observation supports the hypothesis that testosterone may stimulate all aspects of male sexual responding in deer mice by being metabolized to both its reduced metabolite, dihydrotestosterone, and its aromatized metabolite, estradiol. In conclusion, these experiments suggest that ultrasonic vocalizations by male deer mice are an integral component of their sexual behavior repertoire. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS I offer’many thanks to Dr. Lynwood Clemens for his support and good-nature during my graduate career. I also wish to thank Dr. Lauren Harris, Dr. John Johnson, Jr., and Dr. John King for their helpful comments and suggestions regarding my graduate work. I also gratefully acknowledge the expert technological assis- assistance of Evan Fox, Richard Mark, and Michael Vandenberg. Insightful editorial comments were made by Dr. Gary Dohanich and Dr. Anthony Nunez. My thanks go out to all the members of Dr. Clemens' laboratory. I also recognize the secretarial contribution made by Ms. Elizabeth Keith. Finally, I wish to affectionately recognize my parents and friends for their encouragement and support of my academic ambitions. TABLE OF CONTENTS Page LIST OF FIGURES O O O O O O O O O 0 O O O O O O O O O O O O O O —. iv LIST OF TABLES O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O I O O O O O O O O C v INTRODUCTION 0 I O O O O O O I O O O O O O I O O O O O O O O O O 1 Communication During Rodent Reproductive Behavior . . . . . 2 Stimuli Capable of Eliciting Ultrasounds . . . . . . . . . . 8 Behavior of the Ultrasound Producer and Endogenous Factors Influencing Ultrasound Production . . . . . . . . . . . . 14 Behavioral Responses to Ultrasonic Vocalizations . . . . . 27 Objectives of the Present Study . . . . . . . . . . . . . 34 GENERAL METHODS O o o o o o e e ‘ o o o o o o o o o o o o o o o o 36 EXPERIMENT 1 O O O O O I O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O 39 EXPER IMMT 2 O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O 0 O O O O O O O O O 45 EXPERIMENT 3 O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O 49 EHERIMENT 4 O 0 O O O O I O O O O O O O O O O O O I O O O O 0 6o EXPERIMENT 5 O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O I 74 SUWRY AND CONC IOUSIONS O 0 O O I O O O O O O O O O O I O O O O 85 REFERENCE NOTES 0 O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O 87 LIST OF REFERENCES 0 O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O % iv LIST OF FIGURES Figure Page 1 Mean (iSEM) vocalization frequency and vocalization rate of male deer mice before copulation (PVIL), during copulation (EL), and after ejaculation (PEI) . . . . . . 51 2 Mean (:SEM) solicitation frequency, solicitation rate, and % locomotor activity by vocalizing and copulating male deer mice during different temporal periods of the sexual behavior test . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 56 3 Percentage of castrated male deer mice exhibiting ultrasonic vocalizations and mounting behavior following treatment with 1 ug EB/day, 2 ug EB/day, 3 ug EB/day, or OIL . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 63 4 Percentage of castrated male deer mice exhibiting ultrasonic vocalizations, mounting, intromission, and ejaculation following treatment with 50 ug DHTP/day, 100 ug DHTP/day, or 200 ug DHTP/day . . . . . . . . . . . 65 5 Percentage of castrated male deer’mice exhibiting ultrasonic vocalizations, mounting, intromission, and ejaculation following treatment with 1 ug EB/day, 50 ug DHTP/day, 1 ug EB + 50 ug DHTP/day, or 200 ug TP/day . . 68 6 Mean GtSEM) solicitation frequency, solicitation rate, 1 locomotor activity, and % proximity observed during different temporal periods of the sexual behavior test in sexually receptive female deer mice tested with vocalizing and copulating males . . . . . . . . . . . . . 78 LIST OF TABLES Table Page 1 Rodent Ultrasonic Calls During Heterosexual Encounters . . 5 2 Relationship Between Male Copulation and Ultrasonic vocalizat ions 0 O O O O I O O O O O O I 0 O O O O O O O O 41 3 Comparison of Male Solicitation Frequency, Solicitation Rate, and Locomotor Activity in 3 Groups of Male Deer Nice 0 O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O I I O O O O 54 4 Vocalization Performance of Castrated Male Deer Mice Given Various Daily Hormone Treatments . . . . . . . . . 70 5 Influence of Male Behavior on Female Proceptive Behavior in Deer nice 0 O O I O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O 76 INTRODUCTION Coordination of male and female sexual behavior is a key requirement for successful reproduction. At appropriate times, both the male and female must be sufficiently motivated and able to perform sex-specific and species-specific sexual behaviors. Obviously, a mating pair does not achieve the synchrony observed in their reproductive behavior by accident or good fortune, instead coordinated patterning of sexual activity is facilitated by communication between the sexual partners. Individuals of both sexes transmit various signals that advertise their species identity, sexual identity, sexual arousal, sexual and social status, location, and other information important to a prospective mate. The receiver must accurately perceive and integrate the incoming messages so that an apprOpriate and effective response can be made and the communication process continued. Thus, social signalling occurring in a reproductive context means that both members of a mating pair are assuming and exchanging the roles of sender and receiver of messages. In this fashion, natural selection would be expected to operate on both sexes of the communicating dyad to yield "co-evolved, bidirectional signal production and signal reception" (Green & Marler, 1979)- Communication is not restricted to one modality. Depending on the species and the ecological constraints on that species, various social signals may be appropriate. As noted by Kelly (1980), ”Species diversity in signalling modalities may be viewed as adaptations for effective communication in different habitats” (p.111). Certain signals are better suited for communication in certain situations than others. For example, acoustic and chemical signals are generally able to be transmitted and received over longer distances than visual or tactual signals (Marler, 1976). Communication During Rodent Reproductive Behavior Various modes of communication are important for rodent reproductive behavior (Note 1). Although tactile and olfactory signalling will be briefly covered in this discussion, the major emphasis of this review will be on the role of ultrasonic communication (>20 kHz) in rodent sexual behavior. Somatosensory stimulation provided by both partners is extremely important for reproduction. Tactile stimulation of the female's flanks, rump, and tailbase by the male has been found to be both necessary and sufficient for eliciting the lordosis reflex (Pfaff, 1980). Lordosis, in turn, promotes genital stimulation of both sexes; thereby, faciltating vaginal entry of the male's penis, sperm transport following ejaculation, and subsequent fertilization of the ova (Adler, 1978). A Tactile stimulation is obviously essential for directing copulatory mounts by the male. However, prior to the initiation of copulation, messages must be sent which serve to bring the sexes together. Whether by glandular secretion, marking, or urination, males and females of all rodent species studied send chemical signals. There is good evidence to suggest that these chemosignals function to attract and arouse conspecific mates (Bronson & Caroom, 1971; Caroom & Bronson, 1971; Doty, 1972; Johnston, 1974; 1975; 1979; Murphy, 1973)- Moreover, the possibility that the hormonal status of the animal is being communicated by these chemosignals seems likely, since both the chemical composition of the odors and the behaviors necessary for odor emission have been found to be regulated by gonadal hormones (Bronson, 1971; Johnston, 1977; 1979; Thiessen, Friend & Lindzey, 1968). The importance of olfactory stimulation was further demonstrated by studies in which olfactory impairment was produced experimentally. Complete bilateral olfactory bulbectomy of males in many different rodent species resulted in severe disruption or elimination of mating (reviewed by Murphy, 1976). In females, preventing olfactory stimulation apparently does not interfere with performance of lordosis, but deficits in precopulatory or courtship behavior have been noted (reviewed by Murphy, 1976). The ability of muroid rodents to produce and hear sounds above 20 kHz is well-established (Brown & Pye, 1976; Nyby & Whitney, 1978). Ultrasonic vocalizations occur in a variety of social settings. For example, studies demonstrated that ultrasonic communication is important during mother-infant interactions (Allin & Banks, 1971; 1972; Bell, 1974; Bell, Nitschke, Gorry, & Zachman, 1971; Bell, Nitschke, Zachman, 1972; Colvin, 1973; Smotherman, Bell, Starzec, Elias, & Zachman, 1974). Separation from the home nest, cold stress, rough handling by the dam, and presence of novel conspecific odors are all stimuli which elicit ultrasound production from infant rodents. These ultrasonic calls, in turn, signal the mother to modify her maternal behavior (reviewed by Bell, 1974; 1979; Noirot, 1972; Sales & Smith, 1978). Among adult rodents evidence suggests that ultrasonic signalling infuences animal aggressive behavior, territoriality, alarm behavior, and reproductive behavior (reviewed in Nyby & Whitney, 1978). The present discussion will concentrate on the importance of ultrasonic communication occurring within the context of reproductive behavior. Occassionally, it will be necessary to discuss and compare the role of adult ultrasounds in this social context with that found in other social contexts. Table 1 summarizes the heterosexual situations in which ultrasounds have been detected in 13 rodent species, the sex or sexes' producing vocalizations, and the physical characteristics of the vocalizations. Depending on the species, ultrasounds have been detected during one or all of the following time periods of a mating sequence: (1) precopulatory or courtship - time from introduction of mating pair until first mount; (2) copulatory - time from first mount until ejaculation; and (3) postejaculatory - time from ejaculation until the next mount of the following copulatory species (Nyby a Whitney, 1978). Species differences with respect to the sex of the animal producing ultrasounds have also been noted. In species such as golden hamsters and collared lemmings, both sexes participate in ultrasound production (Brooks & Banks, 1973; Floody, Pfaff, & Lewis, 1977), while in species such as house mice, rats, and Mongolian muoumasmou msoaum>amw Amnmfiv modem mucumasmoomum N mwlmn calm maamvom< mucumasmou msofiuanmo Amnmfiv moamm mucumasmoumum w colon oomlood mNEou< Ammmfiv umav< w xuoucfiaouz umsm .oxmfifi< Amnmgv umhmo a vamamumm aneumasomnm Aaasnv cache scoueasumfiwuua mama Nu ooomaooon Aoma~v . vamamumm a smouaHoz Amaamv vamwmumm w umamu uncumasmou . macawm>uos Awhmnv mmsam sconeflsaoouua mama cones comical nausea Admmgv somHaH a couxocum mmauficz mucumaamoo msanumaa Amsmnv modem snouufisaouuua mama om cannon ms: m '5 12 3.0 2' s 2.0 4 1.0 0 0 Before During After Cop. Cop. Ejac. Figure 1. Mean (:SEM) vocalization frequency and vocalization rate of male deer mice before copulation (PVIL), during copulation (EL), and after ejaculation (PEI). Vocalizations/Min 52 (F[1,26]-4.95, p<.05), and that both of these time periods had a significantly higher VF than that found during copulation (F[1,26]-27.0, p<.01). VR also varied during the three time periods (one-way ANOVA, F[2,26]-17.0, p<.01). Again, VR before c0pulation and after ejaculation was significantly higher than VR during copulation (F[1,26]=33.8, p<.01). Moreover, precopulatory and postejaculatory VR did not differ; indicating that the differences observed in VF during these two time periods was simply a result of the postejaculatory time span being longer than the prec0pulatory time period. It should also be noted that ultrasonic calls monitored during the copulatory period were detected during the intervals between mounts and intromissions and, occasionally, were synchronous with ejaculation, but were never detected during a mount or an intromission. Relationships between measures of male copulatory behavior and ultrasonic vocalizations were evaluated by performing Pearson product-moment correlations. Independent correlation coefficients were computed using data from the first and second test in which both sex behavior and ultrasounds were exhibited. A significant negative correlation existed between VR before copulation and the length of the PVIL for both the first (ne25, r--.4775, p<.01) and second test (n-22, r'-.3661, p<.05). VL was not found to be related to precopulatory VR or to any measures of sex behavior performance. In the males' first test with ejaculation, VR during c0pulation was not correlated with measures of male sexual performance (i.e. MF, IF, EL, and MIII). However, in the males' second test with ejaculation, there was a highly positive correlation between VR during c0pulation and the length of the MIII (n=20; r=.6694, p<.01), but no relation with other 53 measures of sexual performance. Finally, VR after ejaculation was significantly and positively correlated with the length of the PEI in the males' first test (n=8, r-.6653, p<.05). but not in their second test. Part B On the basis of the sex behavior tests, males could be classified into 3 independent groups: (1) males exhibiting ultrasounds and copulatory behavior (N-15); (2) males exhibiting ultrasounds but no ' copulatory behavior (N-16); and (3) males not exhibiting either ultrasounds or c0pulatory behavior (N-16). Of the two groups of males producing ultrasonic vocalizations, copulating males had a significantly higher VR than non-copulating males (t(29)-3.67, p<.01). More importantly, precopulatory VR of copulating males was significantly higher than the overall VR of vocalizing, non-copulating males (t(29)-9.76, p<.01), with the means (iSEM) being 3.3: .4 and 1.3 t .2, respectively. Solicitation frequency, solicitation rate, and locomotor activity of the 3 groups of males is presented in Table 3. Solicitation frequency and solicitation rate varied across the 3 groups such that the two groups of non-copulating males did not differ, but both groups had a significantly lower solicitation frequency and rate than vocalizing and copulating males. The 3 male groups did not differ in locomotor activity. Solicitation rate and locomotor activity during the VL in both groups of vocalizing males were similar to the overall solicitation rate and locomotor activity observed in non-vocalizing and 54 musnmooum mammM1amsszIu=onsum an vaOHHom <>oz¢ amalmco .mona magmaamoo> a wcfiumanmou m> mo.v a Azmm HQ some mouammma Ham Hows as mean ea made I noweém egngn .n.b + e.sm H.e.+ «can s.« + e.em N. mmn.s e. m m.m ... N. H ~.n e. H.~.s be + e s as + H a a. + o.m «.4 H153 no H198 1. n.14 ~.e e.n14 A.e am.m + e.s~ n~.~ + n.8s o.n + ~.ne mms a quNHseoo> NUHZ mama mqHBU< a A> umumm A> weapon hufi>fiuo< youOSOUOA N A> Hmumm A> wefimso mama coaumufiofiaom A> momma A> weapon zoomscmum moaumuaoaaom omoH><=mm was: MOHQZOUOA 02¢ .mfiumuefi muoumasomnmumoanHmm new ahosmuma coaumaaumfimlqm exoemuma scammfiaouuew coaumnaamoo>umomnAH>m .hoemuma souumsaflmoo>lg> .muoz .ummu uow>mson assume can no museums HmuomEou unmuomm«v magmas mews Homo mama wefiumdsmoo new wcwnqamoo> an zufi>auom HouoEOUOH N new .oumu eofiumufiofiHom .kommsvmum coaumuqowaom ASMm+V emu: umnmdws. 4._._>..8< 88.2000... NEE 29.5.2013 QHGHE ZOFSUDOm .N munwwm 56 .Hm>umuefi encumdeomfimumomlem new .hosmuma doaumasumnmlgm .xoemuma scammfiaouuea coaumNHHmoo>umonngH>m .hosmuma coaumuwamoo>la> .ouoz .ummu pow>mnma Hmsxmm msu mo museums Hmuonamu uemummufiv messes mafia ammo mama wsfiumasmoo new wewuwfimoo> up zuq>euoc Houoaoooa N was .mumu ecwumuauuaom .zoemsvoum cowumufioaaom Axum+v sum: wmawduz J BIOMW ISNOIIVIIOI'IOS V OOIHBd 3W”. :IO 96 SNOIlVlIDI‘lOS JO HBBWON >t>.._.Q< 5.520004 mks“. Shaw-59.5w >UZH5HUE ZQEhQJOm .N muewwm 57 of events which, if performed appropriately, led to reproduction. Male behavior among c0pulating males did not differ from that exhibited by non-copulating males until the production of the first ultrasound. Once they began vocalizing, males that copulated increased both their rate of soliciting females and their level of locomotor activity. In contrast, non-copulating males (both vocalizing and non-vocalizing) maintained a constant low rate of solicitations and low level of locomotor activity throughout the testing session. Additionally, the VR of vocalizing, non-copulating males was much lower than the precopulatory VR observed in c0pulating males. In sum, the results of this experiment indicated that the initiation of capulation in deer mice is associated with high levels of male courtship ultrasonic vocalizations, solicitations, and male locomotor activity. The temporal patterning of ultrasounds in male deer mice was such that VR was highest befbre the onset of copulation and after ejaculation. In comparison to these two time periods a dramatic reduction in VB was observed during c0pulation. Sales (1972) also reported ultrasonic emission during both prec0pulatory and c0pulatory sequences, but did not measure VR during these two time periods. To further elucidate the relation between ultrasonic vocalizations and cOpulatory behavior, correlations based on .performance of these two behaviors were considered. Several factors could account for the inverse relationship found between precopulatory VR and the length of the PVIL. First, precopulatory VR may indicate the male's readiness to mate. Second, precopulatory ultrasonic calls may serve a courtship function. High VR by the male could then 58 facilitate the onset of c0pulation by promoting increased levels of female proceptive and receptive behaviors (Beach at al, 1976; Floody and Pfaff, 1977b; Geyer et al., 1978b; McIntosh et al., Following the onset of c0puletion the VR declined. This decline seen in VR while the animals were copulating might indicate that during this period, vocalizations were less critical to the maintenance of female proceptive and receptive behaviors (Beach at al., 1976; Floody & Pfaff, 1977b). Additionally, along with the males' copulatory behavior, their high solicitation rate and high level of locomotor activity most likely served to sustain female behavior. It should also be noted that a positive correlation between VR during c0puletion and the length of the MIII was found in one of the two tests in Part A. Since this relationship was not found in both tests it may be spurious, reflecting the number of correlations that were computed. However, McIntosh and Barfield (1980) reported a similar positive correlation between VR during copulation and MIII in rats. Possibly, in males exhibiting long intervals between 'intromissions, it is necessary to make use of the same strategies during c0puletion, such as high rate of ultrasonic vocalizations, that were employed during the courtship sequence before c0puletion. Male deer'mice returned to high rates of vocalization following ejaculation. Also, after ejaculation, males exhibited a reduction in solicitation rate and locomotor activity similar to that found in ' laboratory rats (Adler & Anisko, 1979: Dewsbury, 1957)- Thus, the high rate of postejaculatory calling may serve to maintain the proximity and sexual activity of the female (Barfield & Geyer, 1975). so that additional c0pulatory series and ejaculations can be achieved. 59 Female deer mice were found to require more than one ejaculation for maximal pregnancy initiation (Dewsbury, 1979); therefore, it would clearly be adaptive for males to perform behaviors which maintain the copulatory readiness of females. In rats, the 22-kHz postejaculatory call of males may inhibit potentially aggressive behavior of conspecifics (Anisko et al., 1978; Barfield & Geyer, 1975). Such a function for postejaculatory vocalizations is not incompatible with the one being proposed for deer'mice. 60 EDERIMENT 4 The previous experiments established that ultrasonic vocalizations by male deer mice are intimately related to other aspects of male sexual behavior. .Since endogenous factors such as gonadal hormones have a major controlling influence on male deer mice copulatory behavior (Clemens & Pomerantz, 1981), it would be expected that the same sex hormones controlling c0pulation would also exert a similar influence on male ultrasonic calling. For cells within the central nervous system and genital tissues, it has been demonstrated that metabolism of testosterone (T) by aromatase enzymes (aromatization) leads to the production of estradiol (E) (Callard, Petro, Ryan, 1978; Lieberburg & McEwen, 1975; Naftolin, Ryan, & Petro, 1972) and T metabolism by Sa-reductase enzymes (So-reduction) results in the production of Sa-dihydrotestosterone (DHT) and other Sa-reduced androgens (Bruchovsky & Wilson, 1968; Massa, Justo, & Martini, 1975: Whalen & Rezek, 1972). Both Scsreduction and aromatization of T were found to be obligatory for T to reliably stimulate male c0pulatory behavior in castrated male deer mice (Clemens & Pomerantz, 1982). The present experiment was designed to test the extent to which T, and two of its major metabolites, E and DHT, influence male ultrasonic vocalizations. 61 Method Subjects and Procedure Hale deer mice that exhibited ultrasounds and copulatory behavior in previous experiments were selected for use in the present experiment. Following their successful sexual behavior test, 72 males were castrated under Metofane anesthesia. Six weeks after castration, males were pretested for sexual behavior with a receptive female. Hales were matched according to whether they exhibited ultrasounds in this pretest and were assigned to one of several different daiLy hormone treatment groups. Hormone treatment began the day after the pretest and continued for two weeks. In Part A, the daily treatments used were: 1 ug estradiol benzoate (EB, n-e); 2 ug EB (N'9); 3 ug EB (n-7); and sesame oil (OIL, fl-8). In Part B, the daily treatments were: 50 ug dihydrotestosterone prOpionate (DHTP, N=8); 100 ug DHTP (n-s); and 200 ug DHTP (N-B). In Part C, the daily treatments were: 1 ug EB + 50 ug DHTP (n-s) and 200 ug TP (n-a). Hormone treatments were dissolved in .02 ml sesame oil and injected subcutaneously. Hales received two sexual behavior tests with a receptive female, one per week, beginning one week after the start of hormone treatment. During the course of the experiment, one male receiving 50 ug DHTP/day and one male receiving 200 ug DHTP/day died. Measures All measures were the same as in the General Methods, with the exexception that overall VR was defined as the number of 35-kHz vocalizations/min starting with the production of the first ultrasound. This redifined measure was used so that VR in 62 non-copulating males could be compared to the precopulatory VR of copulating males. Results Part A Percentages of castrated males exhibiting ultrasonic vocalizations and mounting are represented in Figure 3. On the first week, 1 ug EB EB/day activated male ultrasound production to a greater extent than any other treatment (x2(1)-4.85, p<.05). By the second week, calling was stimulated to a greater extent in castrated males receiving 1 ug EB/day or 2 ug EB/day than in males receiving either 3 ug EB/day or OIL (x2(1)-13.69, p<.01). It should be noted that males receiving 3 ug/day were noticeably lethargic during testing and non-testing periods. Regarding measures of ultrasound performance, vocalization latencies of males receiving 1 ug EB/day and 2 ug EB/day were similar, with a mean.(tSEH) of 416::95 sec and 5981:106 sec, respectively. Similarly, vocalization rates of the two groups did not differ, with the mean (iSEH) of the 1 ug EB/day group being 1.01:.4 and the mean (iSEH) of the 2 ug EB/day group being 1.11:.5. Measures of ultrasound performance were not analyzed in males receiving 3 ug EB/day or OIL, since differences from the other groups in performance would only reflect differences in the percentage of males vocalizing. Although a few EB-treated animals exhibited mounting behavior (most notably males receiving 2 ug EB/day), none of the EB treatment Figure 3. Week I ‘0- °/. VOCALIZING 20- 40' % MOUNTING 20- llllllfll OI HORMONE 63 20- Week 2 IpqEB m-e) a ZngBm-e) I MEBm-‘n [j OIL m-e) W 80‘ 40' 20‘ .\\\ / 0. TR EATMENT Percentage of castrated male deer mice exhibiting ultrasonic vocalizations and mounting behavior following treatment with 1 ug EB/day, 2 ug EB/day, 3 ug EB/day, or OIL. 64 groups were significantly different from OIL controls (Figure 3). Also, no males exhibited intromission or ejaculation. Part B Figure 4 illustrates the effects of DHTP treatment on male ultrasonic calling and male copulatory behavior. Doses of 100 ug DHTP/day and 200 ug DHTP/day restored ultrasounds in all males. The 200 ug DHTP/day dosage activated ultrasounds after only one week of treatment, whereas two weeks were necessary for the 100 ug DHTP/day dosage to be completely effective. DHTP also activated male copulatory behavior in a dose-dependent manner (Chi-square test for number of males mounting, x2(2)-7.14 p<.05). Chi-square tests for the number of males in different groups exhibiting intromission and ejaculation were not significant; however, intromission frequency of males receiving 200 ug DHTP/day was significantly higher than males receiving 100 ug DHTP/day (Mannewhitney U test, U89, p<.05). Comparison of measures of ultrasound performance revealed that males receiving 200 ug DHTP/day had a significantly shorter VL (U-B. p<.OO1) and higher VR (U-Z, p<.OO1) than males receiving 100 ug DHTP/day. Mean (iSEM) VL of the two groups was 57::16 sec and 414i:91 sec, respectively; and mean CtSEM) VR of the two groups was 4.41:.2 and 1.51;.3, respectively. 65 .eee\ae=n we com no .sme\mema we cos .zmwxmyma w: on nuwa ucmauoouu wawzoaaou soaumasumfio was .aowmmfiaouunfi .wcuucsoa .msoaumufiamoo> cacommuuas wcfiufinfinxo wows your mama wououummu we owwucoouom .e ouswfim hzmzh vasomouuas wcfiuanasxe mafia Home mama vmumuummo mo mmeuseoumm .m shaman hzuzhoz< mmslmco .mo.v ma Ame +v sews nonsense Ham Home 2 S 2 nonnaos mo z as m 92 News m ~.m~ 5 noses I as H m.» RN H ca .5 noise BEN + 8: 1m + «.2 ca. + 9: reasons N nos m an: oNé m «.3 .5 none... I m; H Rem cg H ES .5 mafia. n~.e + m.mm o.s + e.m~ m.m + s.sw sna>saoo nonoEoooa N 8. m .3. S. m S. .5 not... I no. H an. 2. H 3. .5 wage n8. + 2. 2. + 3. 8. + 3. Son 833338. mom; .HI n.m mom; M ~.m .5 none... I To M So is M ed .5 means. om; + ~.~ Ts + Em m; + Rm sonoaeonu 833328 osoH>HHnmuomn masses mmaIzoz a quNHs a quNHs monHHazoo mDADEHHw mq<=mm m>HHmmuoxm mq<2mm zo MOH>