COGNITEVE STYLES AND CLIENT - THERAP!ST RELAT!ONSH!P EFFECTIVENESS WITH HEROIN AND NON - HEROIN USERS Dissertation for the Degree of Ph. D. MICHIGAN STARE UNIVERSITY THOMAS F. UPDME, Jr. 1974 "we? ‘ 7." 7) 11 . g L LJ Pk 4’). ‘\ ’1 1‘ .‘l-..’ l‘ a th4(i1i{:1r1 3!.“ /, i I, University ' .raleg rfi’fln‘o‘” I. “O J .5 This is to certify that the thesis entitled COGNITIVE STYLES AND CLIENT-THERAPIST RELATIONSHIP EFFECTIVENESS WITH HEROIN AND NON-HEROIN USERS presented by Thomas F. Updike, Jr. has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for Ph.D. f degee in Counseling, Personnel l. Services & Educational Psycholoqy Date Feb. 20, 1974 0-7639 ABSTRACT COGNITIVE STYLES AND CLIENT-THERAPIST RELATIONSHIP EFFECTIVENESS WITH HEROIN AND NON-HEROIN USERS BY Thomas F. Updike, Jr. Statement of the Problem With the increased incidence of drug abuse in the United States, it is increasingly desirable to more clearly understand the individual we are attempting to motivate for treatment. This study was an attempt to understand the drug user from the point of view of his cognitive style and secondly to examine the therapeutic relationship as one factor which might contribute to motivating a client to remain in a program. This particular study consisted of two parts. The first was an experimental study which matched the client, according to his cognitive style, as closely as possible with a therapists cognitive style. The quality of the re- sulting relationship and the change in behavior of the client were examined. The notion was that the greater the similarity between client and therapist with respect to their cognitive styles, the better the resulting Thomas F. Updike, Jr. relationship and thus the greater the probability of in- creasing the client's motivation for treatment. The second portion of the study was a comparative study which examined the cognitive styles (which is com- posed of three factors; symbols, cultural determinants of the meaning of symbols and modalities of inference) of heroin users and non-heroin users. This facet of the study provided insights into the manner in which a drug user ac- quires knowledge and derives meaning from life. Methodology The Experimental Study The sample for the experimental study were male in- mates incarcerated at the Ingham County Jail, Mason, Michi- gan who were found to have drug related problems. The sample had the following characteristics: 1. incarcerated male inmates 2. no psychosis present 3. heroin users 4. drug abuse of more than three months but less than three years 5. limited exposure to previous therapy programs. Sixty incarcerated inmates with drug problems who volunteered to participate were randomly assigned to one of two treatment groups. One was the experimental treatment group where the client was matched, according to cognitive Thomas F. Updike, Jr. styles, as closely as possible with a therapist. The second was the treatment control group where clients were randomly assigned to a therapist. Both groups, each containing thirty clients, received six hours of therapy, followed by the post-test measures. Therapy for both the treatment and control groups was provided by three therapists. Each therapist had his Master's degree and a minimum of one year experience; each joined the Drug Abuse Treatment Program at approximately the same time; all were males; and each had similar philOSOphi- cal approaches to treatment, however, their cognitive styles were different. The post-test measures were the Fiedler 75 Item Relationship Instrument which was used to measure the quality of the therapy relationship and the Behavior Rating Forms which were deve10ped by the researcher to report act- ual observed behaviors of the client. The Comparative Study The sample for the comparative study was composed of drug abusing persons from the tri-county area of Lansing, Michigan, who had presented themselves for treatment in a community based program. Thirty heroin abusers were ran- domly selected from the total case load of the community program and given the cognitive style instrument. Another group of thirty non-heroin drug users were randomly selected Thomas F. Updike, Jr. and given the cognitive style instrument. The sample had the following characteristics: 1. non-incarcerated males from the Lansing, Michi- gan, area 2. no psychosis present 3. drug abuse of more than three months but less than three years. Analysis of the content of the cognitive styles of both groups was done. Similarities and differences between heroin and non-heroin users were evaluated. Results and Implications The Experimental Study The results indicated that clients who were matched according to their cognitive style had a significantly better quality relationship with their therapist than the non- matched control group. Behaviors of the client which were observed by the teachers and turnkeys were significantly better for the experimental group than for the control group. These data appear to support the notion that the therapy relationship is important and the client appears to respond more rapidly to treatment where a good therapy relationship exists. The implications of this work suggest that it may be feasible to attempt to match, as closely as possible, client and counselor in order to effect more rapid progress in treatment. Further, through providing counselors who Thomas F. Updike, Jr. are similar in cognitive style to the prospective client, there appears to be a greater probability of a more effec- tive relationship. It must be noted, however, that these data have demonstrated the efficacy of matching clients and counselors for the short run, but the data does not project the long run results of such matching techniques. The Comparative Study From the comparative study it was found that the cognitive styles of the heroin and non-heroin drug users do differ, that the heroin user has fewer major elements in his cognitive style map and that the heroin group has fewer ways of deriving meaning and acquiring knowledge from life than the non-heroin group. The cognitive style of heroin users was found to be highly similar on several elements, likewise the non-heroin users had a number of common ele- ments which suggests intra-group similarities. As an example of the differences between the two groups, the heroin group was found to derive meaning through the cognitive style element (I) Individual. This suggests he derives meaning from independence in making his own de- cisions. He possesses innate knowledge that his way is best in addition to an ability and willingness to direct his own behavior. The heroin user does not derive meaning from his friends or peer group, i.e., the cognitive style Thomas F. Updike, Jr. element (A) Associates. This means that in the process of making a decision, the heroin user relies on himself, not his peer group, to arrive at what he perceives as an appro- priate behavior for a given situation. The non-heroin group differs significantly from the heroin group in that the non-heroin abuser does not derive meaning from the element (I) Individual. Instead the ele- ment (A) Associates does provide him with a source of mean- ing. Thus, in the decision making process, he relies on in- put from the peer group in arriving at what he perceives as apprOpriate behavioral response. Perhaps the heroin user is attracted to the "sub- culture" more out of a necessity to obtain the drug than to derive psychological support. The non-heroin user ap- parently derives more than just the drug from the "sub-cul- ture" but apparently also derives psychological support from the group. These data suggest there are differences in the cognitive styles of the two groups, thus, consideration must be given to providing differential treatment programs. Finally, the cognitive style map should be viewed as a devise which can provide the counselor with a window, through which he can look into the life space of his client. The instrument indicates where an individual is now in terms of his personal and unique method of deriving mean- ing and knowledge from life. The major elements in the Thomas F. Updike, Jr. client's map represent those things which have been meaning- ful to him. This is but one tool which may contribute to increased understanding and communications with the client. In order to change attitude-behaviors, we must first be able to communicate in meaningful ways with the individual we are seeking to have an impact on. If we understand the client's cognitive style and are sensitive to him as a unique human being, then perhaps we can estab- lish an effective communications system which will further enhance the therapy relationship. Hopefully, the result will be the client's attitude- behavior change, his resocialization and his ultimate re- integration into society as a productive citizen. COGNITIVE STYLES AND CLIENT-THERAPIST RELATIONSHIP EFFECTIVENESS WITH HEROIN AND NON-HEROIN USERS BY 'm MFG Thomas F. Updike, Jr. A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Counseling, Personnel Services and Educational Psychology 1974 Copyright by THOMAS F. UPDIKE, JR. 1974 DEDICATION To Jeanne, Eric and Ian ii ACKNOWLEDGMENTS There are many persons to whom I am deeply grateful to who have assisted me in the conceptualization of this research project. I am especially grateful to Dr. Richard Johnson, Chairman of my advisory committee, for his encour- agement and assistance and especially for allowing me to experience what it means to do "my own research." I am extremely grateful to Dr. John E. Jordan who constantly provided inSpiration and encouragement to me during this project. His insights into social psychology research have had a profound impact on me and my profes- sional career. I am indebted to Dr. Andrew Porter who helped make statistics a meaningful tool for me. I am also grateful to Geoffery Yager who served as statistical consultant and prepared the data for computer analysis. I am grateful to Dr. Ron Richards who introduced me to systematic program evaluation. Also, Dr. Ron Jordan and others in the Office of Medical Education, Research and Development for sharing ideas for developing evaluation methodologies for drug programs. iii All my committee members which include Dr. Richard Johnson, Dr. John Jordan, Dr. Andrew Porter and Dr. Ron Richards, assisted me in understanding the difference be- tween data which are interesting and data which are mean- ingful. I owe particular thanks and gratitude to Dr. Joseph E. Hill who devoted so much of his time in training me in the theory of cognitive style and who so willingly shared with me the fruits of over sixteen years of toil in the deveIOpment of this theory. I owe so much to Dr. Virginia Svga who shared the cognitive style instruments with me and who provided guid- ance in the interpretation. A special thanks goes to the many persons whose thesis contributed to the body of knowledge regarding cognitive styles. Particular thanks to Dr. Laurence Wasser for the clear concise way he was able to explain cognitive style theory in his paper. I wish to thank Sheriff Kenneth L. Preadmore with- out whose help the drug program would not have materialized and this prject would not have been possible. Thanks is also extended to the teachers and turnkeys at the Ingham County Jail for their c00peration. I am indebted to Jerry Gallagher, Bill Nicholson, Keith Johnson, Dave Riddle, and Jim Kaple who helped collect the data. iv A particular word of thanks to John B. Aycock for helping to develop the grant which made the drug program possible. He continually encouraged me in this work and provided insights into the entire notion of cognitive style. He helped me develop as a therapist and researcher for which I am profoundly grateful. My deepest appreciation and love goes to my parents, Mr. and Mrs. Thomas F. Updike, Sr., for their steadfast confidence and support given me during my student days. Their love for Jeanne and me has been ever present and a source of comfort for us. My greatest thanks goes to my family who have endured so much while this project was developed. My wife Jeanne was steadfast in her love and encouragement during all my days as a student. She con- tinuously supported me and reassured me as we worked toward the doctorate. TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES . . . . . . . . . . . . LIST OF FIGURES . . . . . . . . . . . LIST OF APPENDICES . . . . . . . . . . Chapter I. RATIONALE . . . . . . . . . . . Introduction . . . . . . . . Purpose of the Study . . . . . . Theory, Supportive Research and Justifi- cation of the Project . . . . . . Justification of the Project . . . Relationship Research . . . . . . The Theory of Cognitive Style . . . . An Overview . . . . . Comprehensive Study of Hill's Cognitive Style Theory . . . . . . . . A Conceptual Framework of Education . Cognitive Style as an Educational Science . . . . . Symbologosics--the Educational Science of Symbols . . . . . . . . The Theoretical Symbol . . . . . The Qualitative Symbol . . . . . Sensory Stimuli . . . . . . . . Programmatic Effects . . . . . . Codes . . . . . . . . . . . Review . . . . . . . . Determantics--The Educational Science of Cultural Determinants of the Meanings of Symbols . . . . . . . . . The Family Determinant . . . . . . The Associates Determinant . . . . . The Individuality Determinant . . . . Review . . . . . . Inferensics--The Educational Science of Modalities of Inference . . . vi Page ix xii xiii PAH 0000.50.90») Hi4 26 26 27 30 3O 34 35 39 39 44 46 47 48 41 50 50 Chapter II. III. Inductive Processes . . . . . . Magnitude . . . . . . . . . Difference . . . . . . . . . Relationship . . . . . . . . Appraisal . . . . . . . . . Deductive Processes . . . . . . Review . . . . . . . . Principles for Determining Major and Minor Orientations . . . . . Cognitive Style . . . . . . . . METHODOLOGY . . . . . . . . . . Overview . . . . . . . . . . Population . . . . . . . . . Sample . . . . . . . . . . . Therapists . . . . . . . . . Selection . . . . . . . . . Subject Assignment . . . . . . . Materials . . . . . . . . . . Pre-Experimental . . . . . . . Experimental Materials . . . . . Measures . . . . . . . The Fiedler' s (1950,1953) 75-item Q- Sort . . . . . . . Behavior Rating Form . . . . . Hill Cognitive Style . . . . . Treatment Procedures . . . . . . Pre-Experimental . . . . . . . Experimental . . . . . . . . Post Experiment . . . . . . . Control Group . . . . . . . . Comparison Group . . . . . . . Hypothesis . . . . . . . . Experimental Design . . . . . . Analysis of Data . . . . . . . RESULTS . . . . . . . . . . . Treatment Effects . . . . . . . Block Effect . . . . . . . . . Counselor Effect . . . . . . . Interaction Effect . . . . . . . Comparative Study . . . . . . . Hoyt's Reliability of Behavior Rating Scales . . . . . . . . . . Status of Research Hypothesis . . . vii Page 51 53 54 55 55 57 57 59 62 67 67 69 69 76 76 77 79 79 8O 80 80 81 82 82 82 83 85 85 85 86 88 90 91 91 100 101 101 101 105 133 Chapter Page IV. DISCUSSION . . . . . . . . . . . 134 Overview . . . . . . . . . . . 134 Conclusions and Implications . . . . 135 Relationship Study . . . . . . . 135 Limitations . . . . . . . . . 140 Comparative Study . . . . . . . 142 Limitations . . . . . . . . . 172 Summary . . . . . . . . . . . 173 APPENDICES . . . . . . . . . . . . . 176 REFERENCES . . . . . . . . . . . . . 217 viii 10. 11. 12. 13. LIST OF TABLES Demographic Data--Jail Population Type of Drug . . . . . . . . . . . . Demographic Data--Jail Population . . . Demographic Data—-Jail Population Age . Demographic Data--Street Population--Type, Amount and Time on Drugs . . . . . Demographic Data--Street Population Age . Mean Scores on Relationship Measure for Experimental and Control Groups . . Mean Scores on Turnkey Behavior Rating Scale for Experimental and Control Groups by Blocks . . . . . . . . . . . . Mean Scores on Teacher's Behavior Rating Scale for Experimental and Control Group by Blocks . . . . . . . . . . . Mean Score on Relationship Measure for Ex- perimental and Control Groups by Counselor Mean Score on Teacher's Behavior Rating Scale for Experimental and Control Group by Counselor . . . . . . . . . Mean Score on Truckey's Behavior Rating Scale for Experimental and Control Groups by Counselors . . . . . . . . Formulas Necessary for Calculating Degrees of Freedom, Quasi F, and F Statistics--3 way ANOVA . . . . . . . . . . . Summary of the Relationship Scale Analysis of Variance Table . . . . ix Page 70 7O 71 74 75 92 92 93 94 94 94 95 96 Table 14. 15. 16. 17. 18. 19. 20. 21. 22. 23. 24. 25. 26. 27. 28. 29. 30. Page Summary of the Teacher's Behavior Rating Scale Analysis of Variance Table . . . . 97 Summary of the Turnkey's Behavior Rating Scale Analysis of Variance Table . . . . 98 Behavior-Relationship Outcome (Raw Score) from High Quality Match to Low Quality Match . . . . . . . . . . . . . 102 Frequency and Percentage of Major and Minor by Element . . . . . . . . . . . 104 Hoyt's Analysis of Variance Estimate of Internal Consistency Reliability for Teacher's Behavior Ratings . . . . . . 106 Hoyt's Analysis of Variance Estimate of Internal Consistency Reliability for Turnkey's Behavior Ratings . . . . . . 106 Chi Square Comparisons Between Addiction Levels (in Street Population)on Cognitive Style Elements: Theoretical Auditory Linguistic - T(AL). . . . . . . . . 107 Chi Square Compariosn Between Addiction Levels (in Street Population) on Cogni- tive Style Elements: Theoretical Auditory Quantitative T(AQ) . . . . . 108 Theoretical Visual Linguistic — T(VL) . . 109 Theoretical Visual Quantitative T(VQ) . . 110 Qualitative Auditory - Q(A) . . . . . 111 Qualitative Olfactory - Q(O) . . . . . 112 Qualitative Survey - Q(S) . . . . . . 113 Qualitative Tacticle - Q(T) . . . . . 114 Qualitative Visual - Q(V) . . . . . . 115 Qualitative PrOprioceptive — Q(P) . . . 116 Qualitative Code Empathetic - Q(CEM) . . 117 Table Page 31. Qualitative Code Estuetic - Q(CES) . . . 118 32. Qualitative Code Ethic - Q(CET) . . . . 119 33. Qualitative Code Histrionic - Q(CH) . . . 120 34. Qualitative Code Kinesica - Q(CK) . . . . 121 35. Qualitative Code Kinestantics - Q(CKH) . . 122 36. Qualitative Code Proximics - Q(CP) . . . 123 37. Qualitative Code Synnoetics - Q(CS) . . . 124 38. Qualitative Code Trousactional — Q(CT) . . 125 39. Associates - A . . . . . . . . . . 126 40. Family - F . . . . . . . . . . . 127 41. Individuality - I . . . . . . . . . 128 42. Difference - D . . . . . . . . . . 129 43. Appraisal - L . . . . . . . . . . 130 44. Magnitude - M . . . . . . . . . . 131 45. Relationships — R . . . . . . . . . 132 xi Figure 1. LIST OF FIGURES Page Research Design--Subject and Block Assignments . . . . . . . . . . . 78 Pictorial Representation of Experimental Design . . . . . . . . . . . . 89 xii Appendix A. B. C. LIST OF APPENDICES Set Theory Form . . . . . Cognitive Style Instrument Cognitive Style Mapping Tally Sheet Cognitive Style Score Sheet . Cognitive Style Map . . . . Fiedler's 75 Item Relationship Instrument . . . . . . . Behavioral Rating Form Turnkey Behavior Rating Form--Teachers xiii Page 177 181 203 205 207 209 213 215 CHAPTER I RATIONALE Introduction The counseling relationship is stressed in much of counseling theory. A variety of variables has been examined which may contribute to the formations of such a relation— ship. The importance of this counseling relationship with regard to therapy outcome has been emphasized. Counselor and client have been equated, matched, and assumed to be similar on numerous factors thought to relate to the process and outcome of counseling. One concept which has not been examined is that of cognitive style as defined by Hill (1967). The present experiment examined cognitive style matching and its effectiveness toward enhansing the therapy relationship and therapy outcome. Through this experiment it was hoped that a method could be derived which would stimulate the progress of therapy with persons who are drug abusers. The experiment also sought to study the relationship between the cognitive styles of heroin vs. non heroin users. Purpose of the Study The purpose of this study was twofold: (1) to deter- mine what differences exist between cognitive styles of hard 1 drug users (physiologically addicted) vs. soft drug users (non-physiologically addicted), (2) to determine whether the formation of a quality therapeutic relationship will be enhanced by matching clients and counselors who have similar cognitive styles. The therapeutic relationship is seen by many re- searchers as an essential ingredient required to expedite counseling outcome. This becomes particularly crucial in certain counseling settings where time is a limiting factor. In these settings (e.g., county jails, college counseling centers, vocational rehabilitation offices) it may be important to move the client as rapidly as possible to some desired counseling outcome. In order to do this, a counseling relationship must be promptly established. As an illustration consider the short term jail sentences received by the drug abusers sent to county jails. It is imperative that the therapist, as expeditiously as possible, establish a therapeutic relationship with the client, and thus increase the probability of positive therapy outcome prior to the inmate's release back into the community. The major question in this study was: would it be possible to use Hill's Cognitive Style Map for matching therapists and clients and thereby enhance the therapy relationship and the probability of positive therapy outcome? By matching clients and counselors and by employing short intensive therapy, an individual may be moved more rapidly to deal effectively and realistically with his drug problem while incarcerated and after release, to continue in a treatment program. Theory, Supportive Research and Justification of the Project Justification of the Project Because of the large number of immates cycling through the Ingham County Jail with drug related problems, it has become imperative to offer these individuals some type of treatment that will have a direct effect upon their recidivism rate, employment, family, and drug abusing behavior. The primary difficulty in providing treatment is the relatively short period of incarceration received by inmates at the Ingham County Jail (the average stay is approximately 4 months). It is, therefore necessary to exert as much influence on the inmate as possible if his behavior is to be modified. As a result of a recent Federal Grant providing services at the Ingham County Jail, and because of the comprehensive nature of the drug treatment program for the Lansing, Michigan Community, it seems that all the elements are present which may assist the inmate in his ultimate objective of a well adjusted and drug free life. We therefore seek methods which will enable the inmates to receive maximum benefit from the drug program. According to the research reviewed below, the more rapidly a quality therapeutic relationship can be estab- lished between client and counselor, the greater the proba- bility of producing a positive therapy outcome. Thus it seems logical that the more rapidly a therapeutic relation- ship is established, the more rapidly the inmate will begin to deal seriously with his drug related problem. Once motivated for treatment the inmate would be more likely to participate in the aftercare program upon his release from jail. Otherwise during the short incarceration period, if the therapist cannot establish a relationship rapidly, probably the inmate will be released before therapy has an impact on him. The result would be an individual who has a greater probability of returning to jail or failing in the aftercare program. RelationshipyResearch Much has been written regarding the importance of the relationship between the client and his therapist. As Snyder, (1961) notes with regard to relationship, "The importance of the relationship is constantly mentioned in the literature, although different authors mean quite different things by the same term" (page 271). The terms relationship, interaction, transaction, transference- countertransference appear to describe segments of the constellation of dynamics between counselor and client. However this constellation of dynamics is defined, (i.e., relationship, interaction, transaction, and transference) it is important to therapy outcome. Snyder, (1961); Grinker, (1961); Jackson, (1961); Libo, (1957), (1959); Wolberg, (1954); Rodgers, (1961); Murry, (1963); Shoben, (1963); Wolpe, (1968); Lazarus, (1961); Rachman, (1959); Goldstein, et. a1., (1966) all basically agree that the key to the influence of psycho- therapy on the patient is the relationship with the therapist. As Bordin, 1959 states: . . . whenever psychotherapy is accepted as a signifi- cant enterprise, this statement is so widely subscribed to as to become trite. Virtually all efforts to theorize about psychotherapy are intended to describe and explained what attributes of the interactions between the therapist and the patient will account for whatever behavior change results (p. 235). Goldstein et a1., 1961 states that: . . . with regard to outcome investigations of rela- tionships . . . a number of studies have examined the widely . . . held belief that the 'favorable' or 'good quality' therapist-patient relationship is essential for a positive therapeutic outcome . . . research evidence is generally unequivocal in its demonstration of a positive influence of the therapy relationship upon treatment outcome (pp. 75 & 77). Studies by Bown, (1954); Parloff, (1961); Hunt, Ewing, LaForge, and Gilbert, (1959); all indicate that the better the patient- psychotherapist relationship, the greater the symptomatic relief experienced by the patient. Other individuals study- ing therapist-patient relationship include, Gendlin, Jenny and Shlien, (1960); Holden and Uborasky, (1952); Sapolsky, (1965); Truax, 0961);and VanDerVeen, 0961). Research evidence here is generally unequivocal in it's demonstration of positive influence of the therapy relationship on treatment outcome. Strupp (1962) speaks of the same issue: . . . from the beginning of any psychotherapy, the therapist must succeed in sparking strivings in the patient-sometimes called 'the will to recovery,‘ 'motivation for therapy,‘ the striving for 'self— realization,‘ and the like which enable him to cooperate with the therapist and to oppose the neurotic forces within himself. Undoubtedly, no single combination of attributes will yield the answer. Much would depend, one suspects, on the patient's capacity to identify with the therapist as a 'good parent.‘ As yet, we know little about the dynamics of this process in patients or in children, but it may well turn out to be the fulcrum upon which effective therapy turns (p. 583). He further stated that: . . . every neurotic patient is unconsciously committed to maintain the status quo, and psychotherapy, par- ticularly if aimed at confronting the patient with his inner conflicts, proceeds against the obstacle of powerful unconscious resistances. Therefore, unless there is a strong conscious desire to be helped and to collaborate with the therapist, the odds against a favorable outcome may be insuperable (p. 6). Goldstein et a1. (1966) states the following: . . . psychotherapeutic efficiency may be markedly enhanced by increasing the degree of therapist influence over the patient. Manipulation of a major component of the therapy relationship, that is, patient attraction to the therapist, is offered as the primary means for increasing the level of therapist influence . . . by heightening the favorableness of patient attraction toward his therapist, to that degree does the patient become more receptive to therapist influence attempts (p. 81). Specific research studies are now presented which represent a composite and recapitulation of relationship investigations. Bown (1954) in one of the earliest studies of therapist-patient relationship and its relevance to posi- tive therapy outcome focused on the relationship developed in twenty sessions of nondirective therapy with six patients. During the treatment process, Q-sort rations of the quality of the relationship were obtained from each patient- therapist pair. For therapy characterized as successful, in terms of independent outcome measures, Bown reported that "the quality of the actual relationship as perceived by both therapist and client was substantially different from the quality of the relationship in unsuccessful therapy" (p. 43). It was also reported that the client's perception of the relationship more accurately distinguished between the successful and unsuccessful cases than did those of the therapist. Also the greater the agreement between therapist and patient regarding the nature of their rela- tionship, the greater the degree of rated improvement. Generalization of the above findings is difficult because of the small sample size and the rudimentary nature of Bown's measures. However, the research did stimulate additional studies which provided additional supporting evidence of Bown's notions. Parloff's (1961) research operationally defined relationship in terms of Fiedler's (1950, 1953) 75-item Q-sort deck for the ideal therapeutic relationship. The study sought to determine whether an association exists between the therapeutic relationship and outcome of treatment in a group therapy setting. Three criteria of improvement were used: Comfort, Effectiveness, and Objectivity. These criteria were measured by a total of fourteen scales. Additionally, a study was made of the therapist-patient relationship established with patients who terminated therapy prematurely. Twenty-one patients participated in the experiment. The Fiedler sortings were done by observers who sat in on the meetings of the three participating groups for each of the subjects as pairs with his therapist. The sortings were correlated against the Fiedlerideal therapeutic rela- tionship standard. The better the correlation with the standard, the better the relationship between therapist and client was assured to be. The other scales measuring comfort, effectiveness and objectivity were administered to each patient, to other members of each patients group, and to the research staff before and after the twenty session course. The results indicate a significant degree of association between the quality of the therapeutic relationships and certain of the change criteria. Parloff (1961) notes: The data indicate that the better the patient- psychotherapist relationship, the greater the symptomatic relief experienced by the patient, the more likely it was that fellow group members would describe the patient as having become more dominant (leader), and the greater the increase in objectivity attributed to the patient by the research staff (p. 35). He also noted that: Premature termination of therapy by a patient appears to be related to his perception of the 'goodness' of the relationship he has established with his therapist relative to the general level of patient-therapist relationship within his group. Individuals having the poorer relationships in a group tended to drop out of therapy. . . . Patients who established better relationships with this therapist tended to show greater improvement than those whose relationships with the same therapist were not as good (p. 37). Fiedler (1950, 1953), in a variety of studies, examined the therapeutic relationship ratings by therapist of different schools and with different degrees of training and reputed skill. The therapists included masters degree psychologists, doctoral psychologists, social workers, nurses, and medical doctors ranging in experience from one year to twenty—six years. The schools of psychotherapy represented included psychoanalytic, Washington School, Chicago, Nondirective, Adlerious, and Eclectic. The studies indicated that "theoretical allegiance to one system of psychotherapy does not change the thera- pists' goal with respect to the relationship which he strives to create with his patient" (p. 244). Quinn (1950) and Heine (1950) support Fiedler's and Parloff's findings. Quinn's research suggests that the therapist, not the patient, is the one who plays the major role in determining the nature of their consequent rela- tionship. Heine requested patients treated by therapist of differing orientation to describe any changes they 10 experienced as a result of therapy and the aspects of the treatment which they attributed the changes to. The result indicated that patients of therapist from different schools describe factors leading to their change in terms of different treatment methods and also in terms of the same treatment relationship. Other research dealing with the psychotherapeutic relationship has examined another variable--interpersonal attraction, interpersonal influence. The major contention of many writers is that psychotherapeutic efficiency may be enhanced by increasing the degree of therapist influence over the patient. Manipulation of a major component of the therapy relationship, that is, patient attraction to the therapist is offered as a means of increasing therapist influence. Libo (1957, 1959) studied relationship in terms of interpersonal attraction. Utilizing Libo's Picture Impressions Test, a projective test consisting of four cards illustrating therapy like situations, client's were requested to respond in a manner analogous to TAT adminis- tration. The test was administered to a series of patients immediately following their initial psychiatherapy interview. The resulting score is expressed in terms of attraction to the therapist and the therapy process as judged from the protocols. As hypothetically the attraction-relationship score predicted whether or not a patient would return for 11 his second interview. The more attracted a patient was the more likely he was to return. Helber and Goldstein's (1961) in replicating Libo's work provided additional supportive data of the notion that interpersonal attraction (as a measure of relationship) does contribute to clients "willingness" to continue therapy (i.e., through increased interpersonal attraction interpersonal influence appears to increase). Wogon (1970) did a study of the effect of therapist- patient personality variables on therapeutic outcome. The study included eighty-two subjects (male and female) on the psychiatric ward of a medical tending hospital. Each patient was seen by one of twelve psychiatric residents. Both therapists and patients were given the MMPI. These results were used to derive personality measures. Factor scores for each therapist and patient were computed for each of the factor scales (anxiety, depression, withdrawal, subtlety, suppressed anger and somatization), and a matrix of scores generated. In addition to the therapist's and patient's scores, the matrix included the cross-product for each therapist-patient pair on each of the factor scales. The cross-products were used as indexes of therapist-patient similarity. Outcome measures were the Patient Rating Scale and the Therapist Rating Scale. These scales asked for ratings of the degree to which each felt he could communicate with the other and the degree of 12 liking for the other. Therapy outcomes were found to be positively related to therapist's level of anxiety and negatively related to therapist's level of repressiveness. The findings suggest that patients both liked their therapist and showed more progress in therapy if the therapist was able to acknowledge some forms of unpleasant experience in himself (anxiety) and tended not to deny symptoms in himself (repression). Therapist who denied symptoms was found to be less effective in terms of out- come behaviors demonstrated by the patient. Bock (1951), Burdich and Burnes (1958), Goldstein et a1. (1966) through their various experimental research support the notion that interpersonal attraction increases receptivity to interpersonal influence. McClelland (1965) in his therapy of motive acquisition suggests: 1. The more reasons an individual has . . . to believe that he can, will, or should develop a motive, the more . . . attempts designed to develop that motive are likely to succeed. 2. The more an individual perceives that developing a motive is consistent with the demands of reality (and reason), the more . . . attempts designed to develop that motive are likely to succeed (PP-324'325lo Thus it seems reasonable to speculate that an individual can be encouraged toward motive acquisition more readily if the therapist is successful in his interpersonal influence of the patient. Another study by Strong and Schmidt (1970) suggests that unconscious motivations on the part of the counselor 13 do affect counseling outcome. In the study which attempted to examine trustworthiness and influence in counseling, the researchers found a positive effect on the relationship. Most interesting, however, was that counselors who were playing the "role" of untrustworthy were never the less seen by the patients as being "somewhat" helpful in the therapy situation. This was not the intention of the researcher, however, because of methodological weakness, the above was observed (comments by Strong & Schmidt, pp. 202-203). The points of the above two research studies are two fold: (1) Patient client similarity can be measured on several different variables; (2) unconscious motivations on the part of the therapist is often expressed in therapy. More will be said of this later. In the study by Kunce and Anderson (1970) it was hypothesized that in a free-choice situation a counselor would tend to have clients referred to him on the basis of subtle similarities between his personality character- istics and those of the clients. The MMPI was utilized in the study with a sample of sixty-three. Evidence was found to support the hypothesis. In summary thus far, it appears that therapist— patient similarity measured through a variety of variables, does positively effect therapy relationship and outcome. Further it seems evident from the above studies that relationship does indeed appear to be a most central and l4 powerful psychotherapeutic variables. It is also evident from reviewing the literature (Tyler, 1961; Goldstein, 33 31;, 1966) that the counselor's own feelings are believed to be important in this transaction. Tyler states that a counselor "should be an accepting person who finds it easy to be interested in diverse kinds of individuals and to wish them well." This is not a blanket attitude that covers all with equal warmth. Inevitably he will like some clients better than others. Because the counselor is a sensitive human being, he will react on an immediate unconscious level to subtle indications of hostility toward the client. Tyler further suggests that in such a situation the client should be referred immediately to another counselor. Other writers such as Freud, Ellis, Rogers, Horney, Kelly, and Adler, indicate that only certain types of clients are amenable to their particular therapy, therefore, clients should be care- fully selected prior to extended treatment. As noted earlier Fiedler pointed out that "all psychotherapists have as their effective core, the interpersonal relationship rather than specific methods of treatment and that the relationship is created by the therapist who must convey feelings to the client rather than concentrate on method." Relationship then appears to be as important as the method in terms of therapy outcome. Before considering the therapy of cognitive style, an additional comment is required regarding the concept of cognitive dissonance. 15 Goldstein, et a1. (1966) comment: The notion that A's interpersonal attraction toward B increases A's receptivity to influence by B is . . . a significant point of convergence among four major contempory cognitive theories (p. 85). The specific theories were the "balance" theories of Osgood and Tannenboum (1955), Festinger (1957), Heider (1958), and Newcomb (1943, 1956). Osgood, et al. report that "attitude change for a given object of judgment in the direction of the assertion is an approximately linear function of the favorableness of the original attitude toward the other object of judgment with which it is associated" (p. 54). Relationship has been described by Festinger in terms of cognitive dissonance reduction; Heider in terms of change toward balance states; and Newcomb viewed the attraction influence alignment as a "strain toward symmetry." The implied common theoretical thread is that for A to be attracted to B while remaining nonreceptive to B's attempts to influence results in imbalance, asymmetry, or cognitive dissonance more readily than had A accepted B's efforts to influence. Bock (1951) and Burdich and Burnes (1958) supplied experimental research evidence supporting the above lmation. These theories suggest that interpersonal attraction and interpersonal influence might be diminished as a result of "imbalance," "asymmetry," or "cognitive dissonance" between therapist and client. Festinger's (1957) cognitive dissonance theory speaks about conditions that arouse dissonance in an 16 -individual and the various ways in which dissonance reduction takes place. The focus of the theory is on cognitive ele- ments and the relationship between them. Cognitive elements are items of information, knowledge, opinion, as belief about one-self, one's behavior or one's environment. Two cognitives are said to be consonant if they are mutually consistent, that is, if one follows from, implies, or is compatible with the other. Dissonance is said to exist when two cognitive elements, occurring together, are mutually inconsistent. The existence of dissonance, being psychologically uncomfortable, will motivate the person to attempt reduction of the dissonance and achieve consonance. When dissonance is present, in addition to trying to reduce it, the person will actively avoid situations and information which would likely increase the dissonance. Dissonance then is the existence of nonfitting relations among cognitious and is a motivating factor. The notion is presented that if the therapy relation- ship is less than favorable, for whatever reason, cognitive dissonance might be the result. Hill (1967), Blanzy (1970) DeLoach (1970) have demonstrated that when students are taught employing methods centered around the students cognitive style, as defined by Hill (1967), the student's performance in class is improved. It has also been reported that students with teachers of similar cognitive style tend 17 to perform in school more successfully than those students with dissimilar cognitive styles. It has been suggested, DeLoach (1969, 1970) that cognitive style disparity is an antecedent of cognitive dissonance. The literature seems to support the following notions: that it is imperative a good working relationship be established between the client and the therapist regard- less of the method of psychotherapy itself; that it is not possible for the counselor to get along equally well with all prospective clients and he must be able to recognize his limitations; that in order for therapy to be truly effective the therapist-client relationship must be positive; and finally that positive therapy outcome is enhanced when there is a good quality counselor-client relationship; the dynamics between therapist and client are important in forming a therapeutic relationship; interpersonal attraction is related to interpersonal influence;‘if the relationship is not harmonious then imbalance, asymmetry or cognitive dissonance is said to occur; therapist effectiveness is reduced by a poor relationship and motive acquisition and positive observed behavior is enhanced through a positive therapeutic relationship. This study introduces the notion that through cognitive style matching, the formation of a therapy rela- tionship might be enhanced, and that counselors can be 18 matched with clients with whom they will be effective. Through matching clients and counselors utilizing cognitive style mapping, it is postulated that patient interpersonal attractions to the therapist will be enhanced thus increasing interpersonal influence resulting in accelerated motive acquisition and positive observed behavior. The Theory of Cognitive Style An Overview This section consists of two parts: the first is a overview of cognitive style, the second is for those readers who might be interested in an indepth analysis of the therapeutical and mathematical deviations of Hill's Cognitive Style concept. Joseph Hill (1968) has developed a cognitive style in- strument which identified the client's methods of receiving meaning from his own existence. Hill has defined the educational process as a seeking of knowledge and meaning. This can further be expanded to include the concept of counseling in that counseling is essentially a means of enhancing the client's ability to seek meaning from his own life. Hill has further demonstrated that it is possible to match the cognitive style of the teacher and student and thereby enhance the student's probability of success in school. A mis-matched student/teacher results in a higher 19 probability of the student either being dissatisfied or failing in school. This research attempts to match cognitive styles of the counselor with that of the client in hopes of enhancing a quality therapeutic relationship thereby increasing the probability of a positive therapy outcome. Before discussing this aspect of the research further, the theory of cognitive style itself must be examined. The construct of cognitive style, according to Hill, has been developed as one of the educational sciences and is therefore, different from those defined and described in the field of psychology. Hill employed a modified form of Guttman's meta theory of facets as a model. The concept of cognitive style is expressed as, what mathematicians call, a Cartesian product of sets. In this context, cognitive style can be considered somewhat related to Guilford's dimensions of intelligence (Guilford, 1967)- Guilford's model is a Cartesian product of three sets that represent intellect, content and "things," whereas Hill's Cartesian product is designed to represent cognitive style composed of the following four sets: 1. Symbols and meanings 2. Cultured determinants of the meanings of symbols 3. Modalities of inference 4. Neurological, electrical chemical and biochemical aspects of memory storage and learning 20 As Hill pointed out, because of the highly speculative nature of the fourth set, his present Cartesian product representing cognitive style is limited to the first three sets. Cognitive style, then, is a vehicle which can be employed in diagnosing individuals and prescribing activities that provide the highest probability of accomplishing successfully the educational or behavioral task confronting him. It is important to note that an analysis of the student as well as the substance of the educational or counseling task requires a similar analysis. According to Hill, the counselor or teacher should be skilled in the use of information pertaining to symbols and meanings, perceptions, differential patterns, biochemistry of memory, cognitive styles, and systematic analysis and decision making so he can adequately evaluate, prescribe and then make adjustments to correct inefficiencies in the individual sub-systems. The cognitive style map is composed of three factors: Symbols; Cultural Determinants of the Meanings of Symbols; Modalities of Inference. Fragale, Svagr, Zussman (1971) have summarized the conceptual framework as follows: I. SYMBOLS Man is a creature that searches for meaning through the use of symbols. These symbols are theoretical (words, numbers, and graphic symbols) or qualitative (sensory 21 and code data). It is impossible to communicate with peOple without using one or both of these forms of symbols. The theoretical symbol forms a means for communicating experiences or generalizing about the past, present, and future and may be either auditory (spoken) or visual (written). In turn, each of these types can be identified as linguistic or quantitative. These forms are: l. The Theoretical Auditory Linguistic-T(AL)-the sound of a word or graphic symbol. 2. The Theoretical Auditory Quantitative-T(AQ)-the sound of a number or a mathematical symbol. 3. The Theoretical Visual Linguistic-T(VL)-the written word or graphic symbol. 4. The Theoretical Visual Quantitative-T(VQ)-the written number or mathematical symbol. ~ The qualitative symbols derive their meanings from: (1) sensory stimuli, (2) humanly constructed formalisms (codes or games), and (3) the programmatic effects of phenomena which convey an impression of a definite series of images, events, or operations. Identified qualitative symbols are: l. The Qualitative Auditory-Q(A)-symbol is the per- ception of meaning from non-verbal sounds. 2. The Qualitative Olfactory-Q(O)—symbol is the obtain- ing of meaning from odors, smells, or aromas, etc. 22 The Qualitative Savory-Q(S)-symbol is the mediation of meaning through taste. The Qualitative Tactile-Q(T)-symbol is the gaining of meaning through the sense of touch. The Qualitative Visual-Q(V)-symbol is the ability to derive meaning from what one sees. The Qualitative Proprioceptive-Q(P)-symbol is the ability to combine or coordinate imputs from muscular functions into a specific response or operation which is monitored by sensory imput, e.g., as in running to and catching a baseball or typing from written material. Organizing the qualitative symbolic codes in like manner: 1. Qualitative Code Empathetic-Q(CEM)—is the ability to put yourself in another's place, e.g., to know how it feels when someone hits his thumb with a hammer. Qualitative Code Esthetic-Q(CES)—is the ability to enjoy the beauty of an object or an idea. Qualitative Code Ethic-Q(CET)-is commitment to specific values or duties. Qualitative Code Histrionic-Q(CH)-is the ability to deliberately stage behavior to produce a desired effect. II. 23 5. Qualitative Code Kinesics-Q(CK)-is the ability to communicate through bodily movements or peripheral vascular reactions. 6. Qualitative Code Kinesthetics-Q(CKH)-willingness and interest in acquiring motor skill abilities. 7. Qualitative Code Proximics-Q(CP)-is the ability 'to judge the appropriate physical and social distance between oneself and another as defined by the other person, e.g., being able to recognize if you may put your arm around that girl or call the boss by his first name. 8. Qualitative Code Synnoetics-Q(CS)-is knowledge of one's abilities, i.e., being able to establish realistic goals for oneself. 9. Qualitative Code Transactional-Q(CT)—is the ability to establish with others a positive communication system which influences their actions or goals, e.g., to convince someone that your way is best. CULTURAL DETERMINANTS OF THE MEANINGS OF SYMBOLS The meaning brought to man by symbols is influenced by the person's culture and experiences. A person interprets the theoretical and qualitative symbols as an individual in a role that has specific expecta- tions. These expectations are imposed by societal norms, peers or associates, or the family and exert III. 24 an influence over the person throughout his life. What the person perceives as the meaning of symbols is greatly determined by: 1. his associates -A- or peers-this is represented by the various groups with whom the person has the greatest contact and changes throughout life. 2. his family -F- this is either immediate or extended or surrogate changing throughout life. 3. his individuality -I- this is the person's innate knowledge that his is the best way along with his ability and willingness to direct his behavior accordingly. MODALITIES OF INFERENCE The meanings of symbols are influenced also by the modes of inference or reasoning that the individual has a tendency to employ in an effort to formulate hypothesis. There are two classifications of modes of inference. First, are the inductive processes which yields probability conclusions. Second is the deductive process which yields a logically necessary conclusion derived from the given information. There are four inductive inference processes, which are classified as: l. Magnitude -M- is categorical thinking, i.e., using rules, definitions, and/or classifications. 25 2. Differences -D- is the emphasis of one—to-one contrasts of selected characteristics or traits. 3. Relationships -R- is the comparison of two or more selected characteristics or traits through similarities. 4. Appraisal -L- is the process involving the appli- cation of Magnitudes, Differences and Relationships in reaching a probability conclusion. Circle K -(K)- is the deductive inferential process. This inferential process is utilized most frequently in logical proofs, e.g., in mathematics and in symbolic logic. Hill, et a1. (1967, 1968, 1969, 1970, 1971) have demonstrated a significant improvement in outcome performance when a student has been assigned educational tasks according to his cognitive style as compared with the regular educa- tional methods. When student and teacher are matched, according to cognitive style, the teacher is more efficient and the student learns readily. Cognitive style matching has therefore been demonstrated to be an effective method of improving "teaching." This work seeks to test this same notion as applied to the field of Counseling. If, as the research indicated, the therapeutic relationship is important, then it appears logical to match therapist-client cognitive style as a means of 26 enhancing this relationship and thereby speeding therapy outcome. Comprehensive Study of Hill's Cognitive Style Theory The following represents a synthesis of Hill's work including forty doctoral dissertations dealing with the concept of cognitive style (Wasser, 1971; Hill, et a1., 1967, 1968, 1970). A Conceptual Framework of Education "If education is to be regarded as a relatively rigorous applied field of information, similar to those of medicine, engineering, and pharmacy, its conceptual framework must reflect a scientific orientation " (Hill, 1968, p. 1). Various kinds of factoral descriptions, con- cepts, generalizations, laws and theories could comprise a scientific conceptual framework for education. It is, therefore, necessary to recognize that the conceptual framework which will probably best serve the purposes of education will be composed of a set of disciplines which one might call the educational sciences. Presuming this notion is assumed, then the framework can be mapped in terms of the sciences considered to be fundamental to the educative process. Hill has identified seven strata to construct a conceptual framework unique to the applied field of 27 education. These strata are: (a) symbols and their meanings, (b) cultural determinants of the meaning of symbols, (c) modalities of inference, (d) neurological, electrochemical, and biochemical aspects of memory functions, (e) cognitive styles of individuals, (f) teaching, adminis- trative, and counseling style, and (g) systemic analysis and decision making. Cognitive Style as an Educational Science History has demonstrated psychologists interests in studying the consistency and predictability of personality. Allport suggests the concept of "style," which essentially he defined as the consistency and pattern of expressive behaviors that individuals manifest in performing various types of activities. In the context of Allport's descrip- tive definition of style, the term is highly similar to its common use in such expressions as: an individual's way (style) of living, a style of speaking, a style of writing, or a style of dress. This orientation permits the use of the term "style" to denote an entire pattern of responses, a way of life, and also a class of events. Traditionally approached investigations of cogni- tive behavior have dealt mainly with concept formation. Studies of this nature have been limited to considerations such as: what are concepts?, how are they obtained?, or how are they learned? Essentially these investigations 28 have studies various ramifications of what could be termed "concept learning." These types of studies however, have not re-examined cognitive functions in the context of personality. During the past 15 years Hill and others have studied the concept of cognitive style in the context of cognition as a facet of personality. Witkin has advanced the notion that the phenomena described as cognitive style is a type of personality structure expressed in the inter- action between perceptual (cognitive) response systems and antecedent conditions in the life history of the person. In this context then, Hill was interested in such phenomenon as the quality of mother-child relationships as antecedents to certain types of cognitive styles. According to Hill: certain contemporary studies of cognition have been designed to consider the phenomena as a particular individual's mode of response to a given set of stimuli (variables). Other modern efforts have sought antecedents, or correlates, between cognitive style and: (1) such personality variables as: dependency, anxiety, and passivity; (2) such cognitive variables as: intelligence, problem solving, and reasoning; and (3) such psychological processes as: learning and preception (Hill, 1968, p. 2). Essentially contemporary studies of cognitive style involve the investigation of cognitive procesSes in the context of personality and defined social variables. Investigators such as Hill and other psychologists (e.g., Broverman, Gardner, Kagan, Moss, Sigel, and Witkin) recognize that cognitive 29 behaviors form a fundamental part of the socio-personal matrix, and that the employment of certain classes of behavior called "cognitive" have consistent qualities which justify their being defined as stylistic. An important and central point of cognitive style theory is that individuals are unique in that they search for meaning in many remarkable and diverse ways. Each person possesses his own "style" for making sense out of his world and discovering answers to the question. "What's happening and why?" The educational science of cognitive style considers individual modes of meaning-seeking behavior in a manner that allows the social scientist (i.e. educator, psycholo- gist, social worker, rehab counselor) to communicate accurately and precisely, without misappropriate labels and concepts, about each individual (client, student) unique search for meaning. The educational science of cognitive style is comprised of four sets of distinct but highly correlated sciences: (l) symbologosics--the educational science of symbols and their meanings; (2) determanitics--the educational science of cultural determinants of the meaning of symbols; (3) inferensics--the educational science of modalities of inference; and (4) the educational science of biochemical and electrophysiological aspects of memory. However, 30 since the body of information pertaining to the fourth set is highly speculative at this stage of its development, cognitive style is at present defined by the first three sets. Symbologosics--the Educational Science of Symbols A distinguishing characteristic of human activity is the creation, translation and transformation of symbols to the end of producing meaning. As fundamental elements of intellectual activity, symbols are requisite to man's search for and realization of meaning. Man is capable of acquiring and mediating meaning through the creation and utilization of two types of symbols: (1) theoretical symbols (e.g., words and numbers) and (2) qualitative symbols (e.g., sense data). The Theoretical Symbol The theoretical symbol can best be characterized as an arbitrarily selected reference which signifies an object. The most familiar forms of these symbols are words and numbers. Definition: the Theoretical symbol is that symbol which represents to the "awareness" or nervous system of the individual something different from that which the symbol itself is. Theoretical symbols include percepts, private interpretations of sensations, and concepts, shared and agreed upon interpretations of sensations which are capable of being generalized. Thus, words and numbers 31 are the most common forms of theoretical symbols. This definition conforms to the usual or standard definition of a symbol as being representative of something other than what it itself is. Theoretical symbols comprise a class which can be divided into two sub-classes: (1) visual symbols and (2) auditory symbols. Each of these categories consists of linguistic and quantitative elements. In this context, linguistic refers to words and quantitative indicates numbers. The theoretical visual linguistic symbol.--Theort- ical visual linguistic symbols are written or printed words, such as the expressions on this page, which bring to the "awareness" of the reader imageries different from the printed arrangement of the letters and words involved. That is, configurations of these letters stand for some- thing other than randomly assigned marks on paper. They help stimulate sensations and imageries in the nervous system of the reader which convey meanings that can be both interpreted individually (percepts) and generalized (concepts). The theoretical auditory linguistic symbol.-- Theoretical auditory linguistic symbols are sounds of words which bring to the "awareness" of the listener imageries different from the arrangement of the sounds involved. Configurations of sounds help stimulate 32 sensations and imageries in the nervous system of the listener which transmit meanings that can be generalized (concepts) in addition to being interpreted individually (percepts). Since the sound of the letters "b," "o," and "y" for example, do not bring to the individual's "awareness" an imagery that differs from the sound of the letter themselves, they are not a theoretical auditory linguistic symbol. If, however, we rearrange the letters "0," "y," "b" to form the word "boy" (representing a concept), then their sounds would comprise a theoretical auditory linguistic symbol since the sound would bring to an individual's nervous system an image (percept) different from the sounds of the letters themselves. The theoretical visual quantitative symbol.--The theoretical visual quantitative symbol is the written or printed number which can be interpreted individually and generalized. In the theoretical visual symbolic form it may be difficult to determine whether the printed or written expression of a number, e.g., "five," is con- sidered to be a number [quantitative] or a word [linguis- tic] by the individual involved. Under these circumstances, should the printed numeral "5" (representing a concept) bring to the nervous system of the individual an imagery (percept), e.g., a 33 picture (qualitative visual symbol) of five persons, dif- ferent from the printed expression of the numberal itself, then "5" would be an example of a theoretical visual quantitative symbol. The theoretical auditory quantitative symbol.-- The theoretical auditory quantitative symbol is the sound of a number which produces percepts and concepts by presenting to the nervous system of the individual some- thing different from that which it itself is. For example, the sound of the number "three" (representing a concept) brings to the "awareness" of the individual an imagery (percept), e.g., a qualitative visual picture of three grapes, different from the sound itself. (Although in theoretical auditory symbolic form it is not easy to specify whether the sound "three" should be considered as a number [quantitative] or a word [linguistic] by the individual involved, in the interest of clarity, the theoretical auditory quantitative symbol is arbitrarily defined and said to exist). In its complex form, this symbol is the set of sounds selected by the individual's nervous system from a series of algebraic (in its generic sense) terms forming some type of mathematical expression to form a set of images different from the sounds of the terms themselves. For example, in considering the following problem, 34 presented orally: "What is the value of X if 2X + 6 = 12?" both the auditory linguistic and the auditory quantitative theoretical symbols involved bring to the individual's "awareness" a set of images which are possibly a picture (qualitative visual symbol) or an image of the problem different from the sounds of the question formed by the words and numbers comprising it. The Qualitative Symbol Meaning is usually associated with the theory of signs and symbolic logic. In this context, meaning is defined in terms of the lexical and grammatical aspects of linguistics and the formal and functional analyses of logic. This approach limits meaning to the realm of the theoretical symbol. Meaning in the context of the educational sciences is associated not only with the realm of the theoretical symbol but with that of the qualitative symbol as well. This association is based on the assumption that man is capable of mediating each of these two related but different types of symbols into meaning. Definition: the Qualitative symbol is that symbol which presents and then represents to the "awareness" of the individual that which the symbol itself is to that individual. These symbols, for example, are employed by infants in the process of acquiring and mediating meaning before 35 they have attained any theoretical symbolic capabilities. Babies, for instance, can "feel" (qualitative tactile) cold temperatures before they have the ability to say, "I am cold" (theoretical auditory linguistic symbol). In addition, children begin at an early age to acquire quali- tative symbolic code systems, e.g., esthetics, ethics, kinesthetics, which, with modifications, they tend to retain throughout their lives. Under circumstances such as theSe, qualitative symbols derive their meanings from three sources: (1) sensory stimuli, (2) humanly constructed formalisms such as codes or "games" and (3) programmatic effects of objects or phenomena which convey an impression of a definite series of images, scenes, events or operations. Sensory Stimuli Since qualitative symbols are those symbols that present and then represent to the nervous system of the individual that which they (the symbols) themselves are to that individual, they are associated with the five perceptive senses of man: (1) visual, (2) auditory, (3) tactile, (4) savory and (5) olfactory. Qualitative sensory symbols, consequently, become forms of immediately attained individualized meanings or perceptions. l. The qualitative visual symbol presents and then represents to the individual visual entities, such as size, shape and color, whose meanings are attained 36 immediately but which cannot be generalized. (Contrast this to theoretical linguistic symbols, i.e., the spoken or written word, which can be both interpreted individually and generalized.) For example, the shade of red of a particular object presents to the "awareness" of the individual, and then represents to that person, that which it (the shade of red) itself is to him. In order for two or more individuals to approximately share knowledge of that shade of red exhibited by the object at a given time, it is necessary that these persons observe the object at that point of time. In addition, the quality of "redness" cannot be "known" by someone who has never seen this symbol. The sense stimuli residual remaining in the "awareness" of the individual as the result of his observation of the shade of red is, in essence, the "stored" qualitative symbol. 2. The qualitative auditory symbol presents and then represents to the individual perceived sounds, such as music, hammering and drilling, whose immediately attained meanings cannot be generalized. A strain of music, for instance, produced by an instrument presents and then represents to the nervous system of the individual that which it (the strain of music) itself is to him. This symbol can be only approximately shared by persons who hear it at the same point of time. It cannot be shared with a person who has never heard the symbol (the strain 37 of music). The sense stimuli residual "recorded" in the individual's "awareness" as a result of his hearing the music is the "stored" qualitative symbol. 3. The qualitative tactile symbol presents and then represents to the individual various tactile sensations, such as skin sensitivity to temperature changes or the ability to determine the composition of various objects by touch, whose meanings are attained immediately but which cannot be generalized. The feel of a particular swatch of valvet, for example, presents and then represents to the "awareness" of the individual touching it that which it (the feel) itself is to that person. Knowledge of this symbol cannot be attained by an individual who has never felt this piece of velvet. The residual of the sense stimuli "registered" in the nervous system of the person as the result of his feeling the velvet is, in substance, the "stored" qualitative symbol. 4. The qualitative savory symbol presents and then represents to the individual perceived tastes, such as the flavor of wine, whose immediately attained meanings cannot be generalized. For instance, the taste of a particular type of dessert presents and then represents to the nervous system of the individual that which it (the taste) itself is to that individual. This symbol cannot be shared with someone who has never tasted the dessert. The sense stimuli residual remaining in the nervous system of the person as 38 the result of his tasting that particular type of dessert is essentially the "stored" qualitative symbol. 5. The qualitative olfactory symbol presents and then represents to the individual a variety of olfactory sensations, such as the scent of flowers, whose meanings are attained immediately but which cannot be generalized. The aroma of frying bacon and coffee in the morning at a camp site, for example, presents and then represents to the "awareness" of the individual that which it (the aroma) itself is to him. Knowledge of this scent cannot be appre- hended by a person who has never experienced the aroma presented and represented by this symbol. The olfactory sense stimuli residual "recorded" in the nervous system of the individual as the result of his smelling the aroma in question is the "stored" qualitative symbol. The qualitative symbol is composed of the sense stimuli residual associated with the object of the symbol that remains or is "stored" in the nervous system (brain, spinal cord, sub-system of nerves) of the individual. In essence, qualitative sensory symbols are "what they are" to each perceiving individual--figurative, as opposed to literal, expressions that convey and express meaning through the senses of sight, hearing, touch, smell and taste. 39 Programmatic Effects The ability to synthesize stimuli produced within the body into a manifest intelligent behavior is termed "proprioceptive." This ability is exhibited in the per- formance of many tasks (e.g., typing, which may require the synthesis of an individual's qualitative visual and tactile skills with his qualitative kinesthetic capabili- ties). Other complex activities, such as heading a neuro— surgical team, orchestra-conducting, playing a musical instrument while reading music or any other seeming "automatic" activity, may require the synthesis of addi— tional qualitative intellectual competencies. Such func- tional Operations and events are programmatic and, in effect, are sources of meaning for qualitative symbols. Under these circumstances, qualitative proprioceptive, the qualitative symbol which is the device for transmitting and expressing meanings associated with programmatic effects, becomes, as does each qualitative sensory symbol, a form of individualized meaning. Codes Qualitative symbolic codes derive their meanings from humanly constructed formalisms, such as "games." As symbols, they are vehicles by which meanings are expressed in mosaic or figurative patterns which tend to produce immediate insights into the "worlds" or realms under 40 consideration. Whereas generalization is the concern of theoretical symbolic languages, qualitative symbolic codes are oriented toward forms of subcultural individualism, i.e., they comprise unified patterns of subjective meanings as distinguished from the individualized meanings inherent in qualitative sensory and proprioceptive symbols. To clarify the nature of the nine qualitative symbolic codes, they are defined as follows: 1. Qualitative code empathetic--the ability of an individual to identify with, or have a vicarious experience of, another person's (or persons') feelings, ideas or volitions. This ability, for example, is demonstrated by the "empathizer" executing gestures, gesticulations and bodily movements supplemented by facial expressions that are sympathetic with the moods and/or bodily movements of the other person. 2. Qualitative code esthetic--the ability of a person to View with enjoyment the "beauty" and "pureness" of a resulting product, situation or idea. For instance, students who appreciate paintings, geometrical theorems, music, historical analogies and scientific generalizations exhibit this capability. 3. Qualitative code ethic--a commitment to a set of values, a group of moral principles, obligations and/or duties. This ability would be exemplified by a student who feels it necessary and thus is determined to complete 41 all of his homework assignments or a bank robber who insists on finishing the job while under fire. 4. Qualitative code histrionic--artificial behavior, staged conduct or a deliberate exhibition of emotion or temperament to produce some particular effect in, or evoke responses from, other persons. Actors and actresses are especially noted for their possession of this type of ability. Not to be outdone, of course, are teachers and trial lawyers. 5. Qualitative code kinesics--the ability of an individual to communicate by means of nonlinguistic functions and motions of the body, such as blushes, shrugs, gesticu- lations and gestures. Perhaps, up to 93% of human communi- cation is comprised of "body language." Revealing moments occur, for instance, when a person smiling and speaking with a gentle voice, keeps his fists tightly clenched. 6. Qualitative code kinesthetics--comprised of motor skill abilities and bodily reactions, such as threshold of awareness (limen) of different weights, athletic per- formances, ballet and finger dexterity. Excellent typists, dancers and athletes are examples of groups of people who enjoy this capability. 7. Qualitative code proxemics-—the ability of a person to judge and effect "critical" physical and social distances (e.g., closeness, estrangement) between himself and others in the act of communicating, such as a culturally 42 determined "permissible" physical or social distance people maintain between them during conversations. This ability is shown by the individual who knows when not to slap you on the back or the girl who is aware of how close or how far she should stand from a male acquaintance. 8. Qualitative code synnoetics--the possession of personal knowledge of oneself in all qualitative and theo- retical symbolic forms which constitute the totality of the individual in relation to his environment. The person who knows himself well enough so that he is able to select realistic goals is an example of an individual who ranks high in this capability. 9. Qualitative code transactional--the ability of an individual to maintain a positive communicative inter- action which significantly influences the goals of persons involved in that interaction. This ability was exemplified by such political leaders as Roosevelt and Hitler and is frequently exhibited by informal group leaders. Where theoretical symbols are used in ordinary languages to communicate ideas in a connected, consecutive manner, according to the principles of common logic, the qualitative symbols are used to convey feelings, commitments, values and to provide particular types of insights into the domain of "self." The main function of the qualitative symbol is figurative expression, not literal statement. 43 Theoretical symbolic languages are oriented toward gener- alization, qualitative symbolic codes toward subcultural individualism. Theoretical symbolic languages present meanings in linear sequential patterns. Qualitative symbolic codes transmit meanings in the form of mosaic patterns which tend to produce direct or immediate insight. Meaning is derived from ordinary (theoretical symbolic) languages at the termination of a presentation either implicitly or explicitly, while the figurative (mosaic) expressions of qualitative symbols in either sensory, code or programmatic form present meaning in an immediate sense. Qualitative symbolic codes have a unique logic, involving distinctive patterns (mosaics), characteristic orders and relationships. As forms of meaning they are not random, disconnected entities, but neither does their organization tend to follow the necessary sequential patterns of theoretical symbolic rationality. In this context, qualitative symbols are sometimes mistakenly thought of as irrational expressions of experience such as emotions. Actually, qualitative symbols are employed extensively by man to solve problems and interpret the various worlds that comprise a variety of human situations. Under these circumstances, the essential distinction between ordinary languages and qualitative symbolic codes is not found in their applications, but in differences between the classes 44 of symbols in which they are expressed, i.e., between the theoretical symbols used in ordinary languages and the qualitative symbolic forms comprising the codes. Expres- sions composed of qualitative symbols are more apprOpriate than theoretical symbolic statements when the purposes of communication are best served by direct presentation of forms instead of sequential elements that ultimately yield con- clusions. Qualitative symbols, whether they be in sensory, code or programmatic form, are individual (and sometimes subcultural) objectifications of subjective states of meaning. In this context, qualitative codes are unified patterns of subjective meanings, while the qualitative symbols associated with the five senses and programmatic effects become forms of individualized meanings. Review For purposes of review and clarification, the major classifications identified in the discussion of symbologosics are restated below in outline form with the addition of abbreviations for each element. Capital letters express major orientations, small letters minor orientations--the determination of which is explianed later in this chapter. 45 l. Symbologosics (S) A. Theoretical symbols (T) Theoretical Visual linguistic (Tvl’ tvl) Theoretical auditory linguistic (Tal’ tal) Theoretical Visual quantitative (T , t ) vq vq Theoretical auditory quantitative (Ta t ) q’ as B. Qualitative symbols (Q) 1. Sensory stimuli a. Visual (Qv’ qv) b. Auditory (Qa, qa) c. Tactile (Qt’ qt) d. Savory (Qs' qs) e. Olfactory (Q0, qo) Programmatic effects a. Proprioceptive (Qp. qp) Codes a. Empathetic (Q ) cem' qcem b. Esthetic (chs’ qces) c. Ethic (Q ) cet' qcet d. Histrionic (Qch’ qch) e. Kine51cs (Qck’ qck) f. K1nesthet1cs (Qckh’ qckh) p I qcp) h. Synnoetics (QCS, qcs) g. Proxemics (QC 1. Transactional (Oct, qct) 46 Determantics--The Educational Science of Cultural Determinants of the Meaning of Symbols Man derives meaning for symbols not as a totally unique person, but as an individual cast in a social role which has definitions and expectations imposed upon it by societal norms, other individuals and groups with which the individual interacts. Therefore, an individual's symbolic mediations are influenced greatly by his own roles in various groups, roles of other group members and the interactions of these roles. It is in this context that the groups of family and associates, together with a person's individuality, are considered to be the main cultural influences at work on the individual throughout his life. Consequently, those groups which exert the main influence on him determine, in great part, his perceptions of life. Each determinant is, in effect, a synthesis of selected aspects of the concepts: "cultural perceptions," "norms" and "roles." These syntheses can be used to define major and minor classifications to describe the influence of the determinants on the theoretical and qualitative symbolic mediations of the individual. Although each of these determinants influences the individual's interpretations 47 of theoretical and qualitative symbolic information per- taining to a given educational task, the relative strength of its influence tends to vary with the age of the person and the level and conditions of the task. Since these determinants seem to be almost omnipresent in a person's theoretical and qualitative symbolic mediations and influ- ence greatly the meanings he ascribes to them, the determi- nants are fundamental considerations of any effort to educate. The Family Determinant It is reasonable to assume that the meanings of symbols for the individual during the early stages of his life are affected greatly by the group of persons he con- siders to be his family. Between the ages of four and seven the child is developing a set of code systems, and in the process is also beginning to develop what Parsons terms his "individuality." However, the main influence on the meaning of the symbols being acquired by him during this stage of his life is usually the family. Thus, most children participating in pre-school projects or nursery school programs, and those enrolled in early elementary school education (grades K-3) will be influenced mainly by the family and individuality cultural determinants. The family determinant in the early stages of a person's life is mainly composed of those societal norms 48 and role expectations which the family as a group tends to support and prOpagate. Since the individual is a member of a family, his symbolic mediations tend to have some influence on the norms and roles that family members in primary roles (e.g., father, mother) expect him to accept and internalize. In this manner, the individuality of a person begins to take form and develop to a greater or lesser degree. The Associates Determinant Although the meaning of symbols for a young child is usually affected considerably by his family, the rapidity with which he frequently learns from other children indi- cates that the effect of the associates determinant is at work early in the life of the individual. As the individual grows older and his sphere of acquaintances (associates) expands, the influence of the associates determinant tends to increase. The conflict resulting from the difference between the norms and role expectations held for him by his associates and those which his family wish to have him accept and internalize influences the development of his individuality. The associates determinant begins to "show" its influence on most elementary school-age children, especially during the time they are in grades four to six. This influence tends to increase for most persons until they 49 attain the age of 17 to 19. After that point, but depend- ing greatly upon the situation (e.g., joining a fraternity or a gang), the influence tends to decline in favor of the individuality or the family determinant (for example, when an individual marries and starts a "new" family). If a person, however, becomes a member of a profession, a labor union or a strong social organization that empha- sizes brotherhood, the opportunity for the associates determinant to become a major influence exists, and his symbolic mediations may be affected accordingly. The Individuality Determinant The individuality determinant is composed of the physiological, neurological, biochemical and electrochemical "structures" of an individual's body, modified, in part, by the societal norms and role expectations, brought to bear upon his "awareness" (nervous system) by his family, associates and physical environment. The individuality of a person begins to develop at the earliest stages of life and continues a form of developmental change through- out his lifetime. This determinant manifests itself in the form of the willingness of the person, based upon his individuality, to bring his own influence (definition) to his symbolic mediations. The so-called "self-taught" person will usually reflect a high degree of individuality in solving problems 50 or explaining phenomena. This particular type (self- taught) individual might mispronounce certain words (although he knows the meaning of them) and will tend to use them in an unusual sense (although the definitions of the words might be interpreted in the sense in which he has employed them). Persons found in leadership roles usually reflect either a major or minor individuality influence on their symbolic mediations. Review To review the discussion of the main points consid- ered in determantics, they are summarized in the outline below. It must be noted that the family, associates and individuality determinants are not exclusive in the sense that an individual can possess only one of them. (Major determinants are written with capital letters, minor determinants with small letters. II. Determantics (E) A. Family (F, f) B. Associates (A, a) C. Individuality (I, i) Inferensics--The Educational Science of Modalitiés of Inference One quality distinguishing man from other animal species is the uniqueness of the inference processes em- ployed by him in his search for meaning. The educational science of modalities of inference, inferensics, deals with the diverse methods of mediating into meaning theoretical and qualitative symbols whose meanings are influenced by 51 family, associates and/or individuality determinants. Since, generally speaking, there are two principal methods of sym- bolic mediation, deductive and inductive, inferensics con- sists of both deductive and inductive inference processes. Inference is of a dual nature: First, it is the process used to derive a conclusion and, second, it is the conclusion which is derived from the data. A modality of inference can be classified as either an inductive or deductive process, depending upon the manner in which the conclusion it produces is derived. An inductive logical process yields a probability conclusion. A deductive logical process produces a conclusion which is a logical nec- essary consequence resulting from that particular chain of reasoning. The inductive process produces an experienced- based probability conclusion of the type that man continu- ously makes. From previous experience, for example, we might conclude that it is safe to cross the street because the nearest moving vehicle is over one block away, or that tomorrow will be a day possess of bounteous, beautiful weather signaled by the bright-red sunset today. Perhaps an individual has an appearance highly similar to that of an- other person whom we consider to be honest, hence we might conclude that that individual is probably honest, also. Or a man is tall and appears to be muscular. From past experience, we might conclude that he, too, is probably strong. Inductive Processes The advancing of an hypothesis is the first step of an inductive inference process. Considered in this light, 52 the hypothesis plays an unusual role in the process of inquiry or deriving a conclusion. It is an antecedant to the extent that it provides direction for and imposes limits upon the process, and it is a consequence of the inquiry in that it terminates the procedures by being accepted or rejected. Using the process of statistical inference as a model to construct a body of information composed of factual descriptions, concepts, generalizations and principles which refers to the inductive modalities of inference of an individual, the following six-step process is considered to be the generalized form of these types of modalities: l. Advancing the hypothesis for testing; 2. Determining the acceptable amount of risk for making either (a) the error of rejecting a true hypothesis or (b) accepting a false hypothesis; 3. Calling upon a universe of samples of experience that seem to pertain to the "world" referred to in the advanced hypothesis; 4. Defining the critical regions of the universes of experience being employed by establishing limits beyond which, if the information yielded by the observations comprising the samples occurs, the hypothesis must be rejected; 5. Calculating the values of the variables involved from the information provided by the observations comprising the samples being employed; 53 6. Making the decision to accept or reject the hypothesis. Following the prescriptions of the model of sta- tistical inference, it is assumed that man can advance only three different types of hypotheses and is limited to four inductive inference processes for drawing probability conclusions. The three hypotheses are classified as: (1) magnitude, (2) difference, and (3) relationship. The four inference processes are: (l) magnitude, (2) difference, (3) relationship, and (4) appraisal, or evaluation. Magnitude The inference process called "magnitude" deals with deriving a probability conclusion to accept or reject the advanced hypothesis which is one dealing with magnitudes consisting of norms, categorical classifications and atti- tudes accepted as true by the individual. Magnitude inferential patterns, for example, are exemplified by teachers who grade solely on test scores and dutifully fail those students who fail to meet their standards, by corporation presidents who constantly demand that their employees always follow the company chain of command and by those persons who insist on following the letter of the law. A magnitude modality of inference is neither good nor bad. Basically, it consists of the valuing, forming and advancing of hypotheses composed of norms, attitudes 54 and categories considered true by the individual in the process of producing probability conclusions. For instance, the statement, "Schools are better today than ever," implies a value of the present-day educational system which is worthy of use as a norm or standard of past and contempo— rary society. The magnitude inference process, then, is concerned with deriving a probability conclusion to reject or accept the advanced magnitude hypothesis, such as that stated above. Magnitude inferential patterns are prominent in rule-oriented enterprises, such as games, mathematics and the devising of line and staff organization Charts. Difference Inferences of difference, for instance, are exhibited by teachers who compare students on a one-to-one basis. For example, "Bill received an A in English while Phil got a C," or "Bill does more homework than Mark." Difference inductive inference processes consider hypotheses of dif— ference such as linear or mosaic one-to-one comparisons of selected characteristics, measurements or traits of the phenomena in question. For example, the hypothesis of difference concerning the differentiation of the physical appearance of one individual from another can be carried out by means of comparing: (1) eye color, (2) hair color, (3) height, (4) weight, (5) length of arms and any other physical feature which can be considered on a one-to-one basis--a linear differential comparison. 55 Relationship The relationship inference process tests a hypothesis consisting of a relationship of at least two, and usually more than two, characteristics, measurements or traits of the phenomena under consideration. Characteristic of this pattern is the statement: "Bill is a hard worker, speaks well and is a class leader; that's why he gets high marks and scholarship awards." An hypothesis of relationship is one which employs comparison by analogy in multivariate terms. It may be hypothesized, for instance, that height and weight are generally directly related and therefore people who are "tall" will tend to have larger skeletal frames, in various dimensions, than will those of "medium" height. Thus, we are considering correlations of variables. Appraisal The inferences of magnitude, difference and rela- tionship are effected by submitting hypotheses of magnitude, difference and relationship, respectively, to test by the six-step inference process. The inference of appraisal is produced by assessing the results of the other three inference processes in terms of determining the degree to which a priori objectives of the entity under consider- ation have been attained. For example, a person may evaluate, or appraise, a particular brand of automobile tire by considering: (1) information concerning the degree 56 to which it showed wear for, say, 25,000 miles (measured in terms of a magnitude inference); (2) its demonstration of superiority when compared with a competing tire, i.e., linear differential comparison of various features of the respective tires (determined by an inference of difference); and (3) its performance capabilities in relationship to the type of driving, the type of automobile, loads to be carried and other similar types of characteristics (judged by a relationship inference). Consequently, appraisal inferential processes are unique in that they are comprised of magnitude, difference and relationship hypotheses in deriving probability con- clusions of evaluation concerning the phenomena under investigation. An appraisal inferential oriented teacher, for instance, might appraise a student's performance by contemplating: (l) the pupil's achievement on standardized tests (assessed by an inference of magnitude); (2) the student when compared on specific behaviors with each student in his class (measured in terms of a difference inference); and (3) the pupil's performance in relation to type of work assigned, his home environment and physical health (determined by an inference of relationship). The inference of appraisal would be effected by evaluating the results of the other three inference processes in terms of judging or grading the student. 57 Deductive Processes In addition to the four inductive inference processes, individuals must also employ certain deductive inferential processes when dealing with information in selected aspects of the natural sciences, the life sciences and in almost all realms of mathematics and symbolic logic. Although the use of these types of inference processes is necessary when a person is confronted with solving certain kinds of edu- cational tasks, such as proving a geometrical theorem, the assumption is generally accepted that these processes are required, and/or employed, relatively infrequently in every- day living situations. Less emphasis has been placed, therefore, upon the study of deductive inference processes than upon those efforts designed to yield information about inductive inference processes. Review The major categories comprising cymbologosics are outlined below with their designated abbreviations. (Capital letters indicate major modalities of inference, small letters represent minor inference modalities. III. Inferensics (H) A. Inductive processes 1. Magnitude (M, m) 2. Difference (D, d) 3. Relationship (R, r) 4. Appraisal (L) B. Deductive processes (GO) I. II. 58 SUMMARY CHART OF COGNITIVE STYLE MAP ELEMENTS Symbologosics (S) A. Theoretical Symbols (T) 1. Theoretical Visual Linguistic (Tvl, tvl) 2. Theoretical Auditory Linguistic (Tal' tal) 3. Theoretical Visual Quantitative (Tvq' t 4 . Theoretical Auditory Quantitative (Taq' Qualitative Symbols (Q) l. Senory Stimuli a. Visual (Qv' qv) b. Auditory (Qa, qa) c. Tactile (Qt' qt) d. Savory (QS, qS) e. Olfactors (QO, qO) 2. Programmatic Effects a. Propriocaptive (Qp, q ) P 3. Codes a. Empathetic (chm' qcem) b. Esthetic (chs' qces) c. Ethic (cht' qcet) d. Histrionic (Qch' qch) e. Kinesics (Qck' qck) f. Kinesthetics (Qckh’ qckh) g. Proxemics (Qcp' qcp) h. Synnoetics (QCS, qcs) i. Transactional (Qct’ qct) Determantics (E) A. B. C. Family (F, f) Associates (A, a) Individuality (I, i) vq 59 SUMMARY CHART OF COGNITIVE STYLE (continued) III. Inferensics (H) A. Inductive Processes . Magnitude (M, m) . Difference (D, d) . Relationship (R, r) wal-J . Appraisal (L) B. Deductive Processes (QQ) Principles for Determining Major and Minor Orientations Major and minor orientations are relative classi- fications estimated by means of a systematic procedure for determining an individual's capabilities in the realm of each of the twenty-seven cognitive style elements. With the understanding that the basic purpose of determining major and minor orientations of the individual is to reflect his relative strengths in skills, knowledge and aptitudes in the cognitive style domains at a given level of development, the following principles are employed: Principle I. If the percentile rank of an individ- ual's score in a given theoretical or qualitative symbolic domain occurs in the array of values ranging from the fiftieth through the ninety-ninth percentile (inclusively) of a population of these scores, then the individual is accorded a major orientation in that domain, written as a capital letter (T or Q). 60 Principle II. If the percentile rank of a person's score in a particular theoretical or qualitative symbolic domain occurs in the array of values ranging from the twenty-fifth through the forty-ninth percentile (inclu- sively) of a population of these scores, then the individual is assigned a minor orientation in that domain, expressed as a small letter (t or q). Principle III. If the percentile rank of an individual's score in a given theoretical or qualitative symbolic domain occurs in the array of values ranging from the zero percentile through the twenty-fourth percentile of a population of these scores, then the person is accorded neither a major nor a minor orientation in that domain. Principle IV. If the percentile rank of a person's score in a particular cultural determinant occurs in the array of values ranging from the fiftieth through the ninety-ninth percentile (inclusively) of a population of these scores, then the individual is assigned a major determinant, expressed as a capital letter (F, A or I). Principle V. If the percentile rank of an individ- ual's score in a given cultural determinant occurs in the array of values ranging from the twenty-fifth through the forty-ninth percentile (inclusively) of a population of these scores, then the individual is accorded a positive minor determinant, written as a small letter (f(+), a( or +) i (+) ' 61 Principle VI. If the percentile rank of an individ- ual's score in a particular cultural determinant occurs in the array of values ranging from the zero percentile through the twenty-fourth percentile of a population of these scores, then the person is assigned neither a major nor a minor determinant. Principle VII. If the percentile rank of an individ- ual's score in a given modality of inference occurs in the array of values ranging from the fiftieth through the ninety-ninth percentile (inclusively) of a population of these scores, then the person is accorded a major inferential mode, written as a capital letter (M, D or R). Principle VIII. If the percentile rank of a person's score in a particular modality of inference occurs in the array of values ranging from the twenty-fifth through the forty-ninth percentile (inclusively) of a population of these scores, then the individual is assigned a minor inferential mode, expressed as a small letter (m, d or r). Principle IX. If the percentile rank of an individ- ual's score in a given modality of inference occurs in the array of values ranging from the zero through the twenty- fourth percentile of a population of these scores, then the person is accorded neither a major nor a minor inferen- tial mode. Principle X. Since the inference of appraisal is, by definition, composed of the following three major elements: (1) magnitude, (2) difference, and (3) relationship, if the 62 percentile ranks of a person's score in all three inference processes occur in the array of values ranging from the fiftieth through the ninety-ninth percentile (inclusively) of a population of these scores, then the individual is assigned a major appraisal inferential mode, written as a capital letter (L). Principle XI. If the percentile rank of an individ- ual's score in a particular deductive inference process occurs in the array of values ranging from the fiftieth through the ninety-ninth percentile (inclusively) of a population of these scores, then the person is accorded a major deductive mode, written as K . Thus on the basis of the test scores and through applying the above principles, one is able to determine whenever an individual has a major or minor orientation for each of the twenty-seven cognitive style elements. Cognitive Style The educational science of cognitive style is some- what different from those defined and described in the discipline of psychology. Employing a modified form of Guttman's methatheory of facets as a model,* cognitive style *The term "facet" was formally proposed by Louis Guttman in, "An Outline of Some New Methodology for Social Research," Public Opinion Quarterly, 18 (Winter, 1954-55), pp. 393-404. Itgis interesting to note that although this concept had been employed extensively throughout the social and behavioral sciences, as well as in mathematics, it had not been standardized prior to Guttman's proposal. 63 is expressed as, what mathematicians call, a Cartesian product of sets. In this context, cognitive style can be considered somewhat related to Guilford's "dimensions of intellect." Where Guilford's model is a Cartesian product of three sets representing products, contents and operations, the educational science of cognitive style is defined as a Cartesian product, G, comprised of three sets, S, E and H: G - S x E x H, where S indicates the set of elements defining symbologosics, E represents the set of determantics and H denotes the set of elements comprising inferensics.* Therefore, G is defined as the universal set of all possible three-element profiles over the Cartesian product of sets S, E and H. In functional notation form, this universal set is expressed as G = O (S, E, H), where O (phi) indicates a function in the form of a Cartesian product. In set theory form, the expression becomes: *A Cartesian product is a particular type of space or set whose elements may be combined into profiles defined over that space. The "x" sign does not denote any algebraic or numerical operations but indicates that elements from each of the three sets, S, E and H, must be combined to determine the exact reference points of each three-element profile in space. Thus, a Cartesian product is a display of elements which, when combined, comprises an individual's various cognitive style profiles. Refer to Appendix for a discussion of Set Theory Form for the three Sets S, E and H. 64 §. (TX-0y), (TX—qy)r (tx—qy)’ (Qy'tx); E. I, A, F: (I-a(z))r (A-i(2))' (F-i(z))l (I-f(z)), (A-f(z)), (F-a(z)). E . 5(M-d), (D-m), (R-m). (:).( ((M-r), (D-r), (R-d). ( Set S consists of a maximum number of 240 elements, 15 are included in set E, and set H is composed of 11 elements. Since G - S x E x H, the universal set G, the Cartesian product of sets 8, E and H, includes: 240 x 15 x 11 = 39,600 different possible cognitive style profiles. A person, however, could at best attain only a maximum number of: 60 x 4 x 7 = 1,680 three-element profiles or modes of searching for meaning. The explanation for this limitation lies in the fact that should an individual possess various "style" elements, he cannot by definition hold others. For example, if a person is characterized by a major theo- retical visual linguistic (Tvl) orientation, he cannot at the same time be identified by a minor theoretical visual linguistic (tvl) capability, for the minor symbol is sub- sumed in the major element. Conversely, should an individual 65 possess a minor orientation, he could not at the same moment command a major capability. The cognitive style of an individual is defined as a set 9, i.e., a Cartesian product composed of sub-sets of elements drawn from sets S, E and H of G. Therefore, g = H (s, e, h), or g - {§} x {g} x {h}, where s, e and h represent sub-sets of elements drawn from those comprising universal sets S, E and H, respectively. The cognitive style of an individual is a relative concept, and depends not only upon the age, educational level and cultural background of the person involved, but upon the level and conditions (e.g., symbolic, determantic, infernsic) of the educational or behavioral task to be accomplished. When "style" and task are both considered, we realize that there is no one standard method for achiev- ing our educational or behavioral goals. In this context, the derivation of an appropriate "style" for an individual demands that the diagnostician analyze the client as well as the substance of the behavioral task to be considered. Under these circumstances, cognitive style provides a means of analyzing, interpreting and evaluating behavioral endeavors in a manner relatively different from those usually employed. The educational science of cognitive style, there- fore, is not a fixed or static entity. It is not rockbound and sterile, devoid of concepts and possessed of empty statements. Rather it is a value-free, dynamic, neutral 66 science reflecting the ever-changing status of human growth and development. Although individuals' cognitive styles are constantly changing (a person is not stuck with a life "style"), they can be employed for diagnostic purposes. This procedure leads to the formulation of strategies for the matching and augmentation of "styles" to increase the probability of a client's successful performance in the myriad of educational and behavioral tasks he faces daily. Cognitive style not only expresses the idiosyncratic nature of our clients as persons engaged in meaning-seeking behavior but allows us, social scientistics, to communicate this uniqueness precisely and accurately. CHAPTER II METHODOLOGY Overview Sixty incarcerated inmates who were found to have drug related problems were invited to participate in the Drug Abuse Treatment Program. For the purposes of this experiment the clients were seen for six hours of therapy followed by the post-test measures. Inmates who volunteered to participate were randomly assigned to one of two treat- ment groups. One was the experimental group where the client was matched, according to cognitive styles, as closely as possible with a therapist. The second was the treatment control group where clients were randomly assigned to therapists. Both groups received six hours of therapy. Cognitive style therapy was developed by Hill who utilized it in matching students and teachers. The researcher expanded this concept to include matching of clients and therapists to facilitate the therapeutic relationship and behavior outcome. Therapy for both the treatment and control groups was provided by three therapists. Each therapist had his masters degree and a minimum of one year experience; each 67 ..‘lllll]l]ll‘lll{(‘l‘|. \[Ell.[l_l.lf[[iv!l}[l{(ll'. 68 joined the Drug Abuse Treatment Program at approximately the same time; all were male; and each had similar philo- sophical approaches to treatment. However, their cognitive styles were different. The Fiedler 75 item Relationship instrument was used to measure the quality of the therapy relationship. A behavior Rating Form was developed by the researcher which sought to report actual observable behaviors. Following treatment, the Behavior Rating Form was administered to two teachers and two turnkeys all of whom had daily contact with the client. Note Tables 16 and 17 for interrate reliability between teachers; turnkeys; and teachers-turnkeys. The client was not aware of these measures. He was asked, however, to complete the Fiedler 75-item Relationship instrument which provided a dependent measure of therapy relationship quality. Analysis of variance was performed to test for group main effect. The second portion of this study examined cognitive style differences between hard drug users (physically addictive) and soft drug users (non-physically addictive). Sixty persons who sought treatment for their drug problem were invited to participate in the study. Thirty were sought from a drug program which serves primarily "soft-drug" users, thirty additional were sought from another program which primarily serves hard drug users. Only persons who .411! )IIVIIIEEII". IE. EJIAIELIIIv f".lv 69 habitually used drugs were used in the study. All were given the Hill Cognitive Style instrument. A comparative study was done between these two groups regarding differences in cognitive styles. Demo- graphic variables of age, sex, amount, type and length of time using a particular class of drugs were reported. Population The population for the experimental study were inmates incarcerated at Ingham County Jail, Mason, Michigan. Generalization will be made toward this population, however specific demographic data and sampling procedure will be carefully delineated, allowing the reader to judge how it compares with some population to which he might wish to generalize (Cornfield, Tukey's argument). Regarding the comparative study, the population were drug abusing persons from the tri-county area of Lansing, Michigan, who presented themselves for treatment. Again generalization is directed toward this population and Cornfield, Tukey's argument is once again employed. Sample The sample for the experimental study have the following characteristics: 1. Incarcerated male inmates at Ingham County Jail. 2. No psychosis present. 3. Heroin or barbiturate addicted (physically addicted). 7O 4. Drug Abuse of more than 3 months but less than 3 years. 5. Limited exposure to previous therapy programs. (Note: 99% of the drug abusing inmates fall into the above category.) TABLE l.—-Demographic Data--Jail Population Type of Drug- Cumulative Cumulative Drug Frequency Percent Number Percent Heroin 47 78.33 47 78.33 Combination Hard Drugs 4 6.67 51 85.00 Hard and Soft Drugs 5 8.33 56 93.33 Heroin and Cocaine l 1.67 57 95.00 Heroin and Grass 3 5.00 60 100.00 TABLE 2.--Demographic Data-~Jail Population. Cumulative Cumulative Frequency Percent No. Cases Percent Length of Time on Drugs 6 mo.- 1 5 8.33 5 8.33 l - 2 years 44 73.33 49 81.67 2 - 3 years 11 18.33 60 100.00 Amount of Money Required to Support Drug Habit* $25-50/day 45 75.00 45 75.00 $50—100/day 15 25.00 60 100.00 *Data based on client's self report. 71 TABLE 3.--Demographic Data--Jail Population Age. Cumulative Cumulative Age Frequency Percent No. Cases ' Percent 19 13 21.67 13 21.67 20 8 13.33 21 35.00 21 6 10.00 27 45.00 22 7 11.67 34 56.67 23 5 8.33 39 65.00 24 5 8.33 44 73.33 25 2 3.33 46 76.67 36 3 5.00 49 81.67 27 3 5.00 52 86.67 28 3 5.00 55 91.67 29 2 3.33 57 95.00 30 l 1.67 58 96.67 33 1 1.67 59 98.33 34 l 1.67 60 100.00 Mean = 22.83 Standard deviation = 3.72 A sample of 60 subjects was obtained employing the following procedure: 1. All inmates at the Ingham County Jail were screened to determine evidence of drug abuse. This was accomplished through the following means. A. Identifying inmates that have been arrested on drug related charges. B. Inmates who have been brought in previously on drug related charges. C. Clients who have been identified by the physician as having drug related problems. D. Clients who indicated they have drug related problems, and who sought treatment although they were not arrested for drug offenses. made in The the 72 E. Clients referred to the drug rehabilitation programthrough other professionals on the jail rehabilitation staff. F. Physical observation of this client: 1. Withdrawal symptoms 2. Tracks 3. Overt behavior changes All clients from the above group were further screened by the professional staff of the Drug Abuse Treatment Program where the following data was obtained: A. Is there an indication of psychosis? If so these persons were not included in this study. B. Type of drug and level of addiction, in— cluding the extent and amount of drug use. From the pool of clients who met the above criteria, 60 clients were randomly selected. The pool consisted of 120 clients during a two month period. determination of the amount of drug use was following manner: Client's self report. Observed physiological symptoms and extent of withdrawal, evidence of tracks or flashbacks. Report from the attending physician. Interview by the professional drug rehabili- tation staff. 73 5. Reports of known pushers who have had the inmate as "clientele." 6. Previous records. Clients for this study were invited to participate in the drug program and each demonstrated the basic chara- cteristics as listed above. A second sample of sixty was drawn from two addi- tional treatment programs which were based in the com— munity. The criteria of acceptance was as follows: 1. Drug Abuse of more than three months but less than three years. 2. Clearly on either physically or non-physically addictive drugs--not a compensation. 3. Screened by staffs of the respective programs regarding the type and extent of drug use. Thirty of the above sixty were randomly selected from a crisis center program which deals primarily with persons on non-physically addictive drugs. The other thirty were randomly drawn from a methadone program which serves only persons who are addicted to heroin. Of the total persons coming into the two programs, thirty from each were randomly selected and invited to participate. This method was followed until thirty persons from each program had been received. i 15“... i 74 00.000 00 00.0 N 00000 0 I 0 00.00 00 00.00 0 00000 N I 0 00.00 00 00.00 00 new» 0 - .05 0 .s00 00.00 00 00.00 00 000005 0 - 0 .. 00.00 0 00.00 0 000005 0 - 0 00.000 00 00.0 0 0003\000 - 000 00.00 00 00.00 0 0003\000 . 000 000s.. o o . x ,. CP< 00 00 00 00 00 00 0003\000 . 000 00.00 0 00.00 0 0003\000 . 0 00.000 00 00.00 00 0000 00000000500 00.00 00 0.0 0 mnnwn .:.0 00.00 .-0 00.00 .0 0 100 :. 00.00 0 00.00 0 am; 00.00 0 00.00 0 00000 Amanuo uwomv muomu CH0002ucoz 00.000 00 00.00 0 00000 0 - 00000 0 00.00 00 00.00 00 00000 0 . 0000 0 00.00 00 00.00 00 new» 0 . .05 0 0200 00.00 0 00.00 0 mrncoe 0 u 0 0 0 0 0 000005 0 - 0 00.000 00 00.0 0 0005 no 0000 00.00 00 00.00 0 000\0000 - 000 .20000 00.00 00 00.00 00 smex000 . 000 s . . 00.00 0 00.00 0 000\000 . 0 00.000 0 00.00 0 000: 000 0000 oo.om am mm.m0 v pumm co0ums0nECU mmwb 00.00 00 00.00 00 000000 muons cfiouwm unmoumm quEDZ unwound mocmscwum m>fiUMHDESU O>flfiwHDEDU .mmsuo co mEHB 0cm DCSOE< .mmzellcofluchdom Dmmuumllmumo 00nmmnm06wont.v m0mo amm.m mm.m No.m mm.m mm.m mo.m mv.m oo.m mm.m mv.m mh.m NE «mn.m nm.m nv.m mw.m mm.m ma.m mm.m om.m hh.m mm.m 00.0 09 new: pamnw 00m 0m 0m 0m 0m mm 0m 0m mm 0m Houucoo pcm Hmucmsflummxm How mamom mcflumm Hoe>mcom .mRUOHm an mmsouo wmxcuce co mmnoom cmmzll.n m0m¢9 100. M as mxoon n oamuam .pcsow mmocmnmmwflp DEMOHMHcmHme @5000 Honucou N NE msoum HmECGEAHmmxm n HE oa.v u we can HE new: Hamum>o «wo.m no.m mm.m mo.v no.w oa.m mw.m hm.m m>.m mn.m mm.v NB «mw.v mo.w >~.v n¢.v hm.v mv.v on.w mm.v om.v m0.m mm.m as new: venue 00m 0m 0m 0m 0m mm 0m 0m mm 0m .mxooam an mmsouo Houucou paw Hmucmefiummxm How whamwmz mflnmcowumawm co mmnoom :mmzln.0 mqmde 93 A00. M E mxooam u oamlam .cc50m mmocmanMHp unm00m0cmflms dsoum Houucou N NE @5000 HmucmEHHmmxm n 0% mm.m n we can 09 ans: 00m0m>o «vm.m ov.m mm.m mo.w om.m nm.~ oo.m om.m m0.m mm.m mo.w me 400.v m0.m mm.m 00.0 00.0 m0.v >0.v m0.v 0m.m ov.v mm.v 09 new: ocmuo 00m 0m 0m 0m 0m mm 0m mm mm 0m .mxoon %Q @5000 Houucoo can HmucmEHmmxm How mamom mafiumm Hofl>mnmm m.um£omme so mmuoom cmmzll.m m0moz< >83 mnumoflumfiumum mmoumwo maeumHsonu 00m Nummmmomz mchEMOMII.NH mumme 96 TABLE 13.--Summary of the Relationship Scale Analysis of Variance Table. ifdggigrzgm df Mean Square F* (df) Grand Mean 103833.600 1 103833.600 Treatment (T) 1440.600 1 1440.600 @ 21.408** 1:15 B (T) 898.467 18 49.915 @ 3.54 (A) 18:36 Counselor (C) 260.800 2 130.400 7.23 (B) 2;2 CXT 36.400 2 18.200 1.06 (C) 2;36 CB (T) 634.133 36 17.615 TOTAL 3270.40 59 55.431 *Because the 3d-dependent variables are seen as repeated tests, a has been portitioned such that a = .01. . . . . > **QuaSi-F Significant when F - 8.68. = F significant when F 3 2.55. l \O \O O O O F significant when F > I U"! N O = F significant when F > «some» ll Significant F. 97 TABLE l4.--Summary of the Teacher's Behavior Rating Scale Analysis of Variance Table. Reduced Sum of Squares df Mean Square F* (df) Grand Mean 87669.037 1 87669.037 Treatment (T) 495.937 1 495.938 @ 12.515** 1:11 B (T) 661.108 18 36.728 @ 3.42A 18:36 Counselor (C) 47.775 2 23.888 6.34B 2:2 CXT 7.525 2 3.763 3.27C 2:36 CB (T) 388.367 36 10.788 TOTAL 1600.713 59 27.131 *Because the three dependent variables are seen as repeated tests, a has been portitioned such that a = .01. u 0 ' ' > **QuaSi F Significant when F - 9.65. A = F significant when F 3 2.55. B = F significant when F 3 2.55 C = F significant when F 3 99.00 @ = Significant F. 98 TABLE 15.--Summary of the Turnkey's Behavior Rating Scale Analysis of Variance Table. Eidggigrizm df Mean Square F* (df) Grand Mean 77041.667 1 77041.667 Treatment (T) 236.017 1 236.017 @ 11.266** 1:19 B (T) 376.650 18 20.925 1.99A 18:36 Counselor (C) 60.558 2 30.279 2.88B 2:2 CXT 1.908 2 0.954 0.09C 2:36 CB (T) 377.200 36 10.478 TOTAL 1052.333 59 17.836 *Because the three dependent variables are seen as repeated tests, a has been partitioned such that a = .01. **Quasi F significant when F 3 8.18. A = F significant when F 3 2.55. B = F significant when F 3 99.00. C = F significant when F 3 5.29. @ = Significant F. 99 Because the three dependent variables are seen as repeated tests, the alpha level has been partitioned such that a = .01. A Quasi F was necessary to determine signi— ficance of treatment main effects. Significance was found when the Quasi F was equal to or exceded 8.68 for the Analysis of Variance dealing with the Fiedler relationship measures; 8.18 for the Turnkey's Behavior Rating Scale; and 9.65 for the Teacher's Behavior Rating Scale. Therefore, the experimental group did better than the control group on all measures for treatment main effects. Specifically, this implied the rejection of the following null hypothesis: H03: Following therapy, counselors and clients with high quality similar cognitive style matches, as defined by Hill, will not have a better quality relationship as measured by Fiedler's 75-item instrument than the control group. H05: Following therapy, counselors and clients with high quality similar cognitive style matches, as defined by Hill, will not have a better therapy outcome than clients and counselors in the control group. The following alternate hypothesis results: HA3: Following therapy, counselors and clients with high quality similar cognitive style matches, as defined by Hill, will have a better quality relationship as measured by Fielder's 75-item instrument than the control group. HA5: Following therapy, counselors and clients with high quality similar cognitive style matches, as defined by Hill, will have a better therapy outcome than clients and counselors in the control group. 100 Block Effect The F test statistic for determining block (row) ef- fect is significant at or beyond a value of 2.55 for each of the three dependent variables. On two of the measures (the Relationship and Teacher Instruments), the tests showed significant differences between experimental and control groups. The blocks represent a decreasing similarity of cognitive style match between counselor and clients. It must be noted that utilizing the Turnkey Behavior Rating Scale, the test failed to reject the null hypothesis. The two remaining dependent variables, however, did reject the null hypothesis. Specifically, this implied the rejection of the following null hypothesis: H02: Following therapy, counselors and clients With high quality Similar cognitive style matches, as defined by Hill, will not have a better quality relationship as measured by Fiedler's 75-item instrument than clients and counselors with low quality cognitive style matches. H 4: Following therapy: counselors and clients with high quality similar cognitive style matches, as defined by Hill, will not have a better therapy outcome than clients and counselors with low quality cognitive style matches. The following alternate hypothesis results: HA2: Following therapy, counselors and clients with high quality similar cognitive style matches, as defined by Hill, will have a better quality relationship as measured by Fiedler's 75-item instrument than clients and counselors with low quality cognitive style matches. 101 Following therapy, counselors and clients with high quality similar cognitive style matches, as defined by Hill, will have a better therapy outcome than clients and counselors with low quality cognitive style matches. A4: Counselor Effect An F test was done to determine counselor effect on the treatment groups. In order to be significant, the F statistic must equal or exceed 99.00 at an a level of .01. The F statistic on the three dependent variables were found to be less than 99. Although not in the original hypothesis to be tested, the above suggests that the treatment main effects and block effects were not due to counselor differ— ence . Interaction Effect An F test was done to determine interaction effect between counselor and treatment. In order to be significant, the F statistic must equal or exceed 5.29 at an a level of .01. On examining each of the dependent variables, the F test failed to reject the null hypothesis that there was no interaction effect. It would appear from the above that the differences found were not related to either counselor or interaction of counselor and treatment effect (see Table 16)- Comparative Study The chi square comparisons between non-physically addictive drug users and physically addictive drug users TABLE l6.-Behavior-Relationship Outcome (Raw Score) from High Quality Match to Low Quality Match. Behavior Group Behavior Relation- Ship % Match Counselor Number Behavior Behavior Group Relation— ship Counselor Number II II Match 3.8 3.6 3.9 3.6 4.4 4.1 1 l 5.8 4.9 4.7 4.4 4.5 5.6 5.2 5.2 93 3.7 4.1 (‘1 4.0 4.2 4.6 4.1 93 3.9 4.2 3.9 4.0 4.3 3.9 4.9 4.6 4.1 92 3.7 4.0 4.4 4.8 4.2 90 (V 3.9 3.4 3.6 3.9 4.0 3.6 3.9 3.9 4.2 3.6 3.7 4.0 4.2 3.7 4.2 O\ 89 88 3.3 4.1 7.0 4.1 3.9 4.3 4.2 4.1 L0 3.9 3.6 4.2 88 3.7 3.9 4.0 3.2 3.2 3.8 3.8 4.0 4.1 4.1 4.0 87 3.4 3.6 3.4 3.1 4.0 3.6 3.6 3.8 3.7 3.6 3.9 4.1 4.1 3.9 85 3.0 3.0 3.7 3.2 3.1 3.9 4.1 84 3.6 3.3 3.8 3.9 3.4 4.1 82 102 3.8 3.2 3.9 3.9 4.4 4.1 4.8 82 3.0 3.1 3.0 2 2 4.2 3.8 4.2 3.6 3.9 4.0 4.5 4.2 4.0 4.3 4.0 81 4.3 81 3.0 3.0 3.0 3.2 3.9 3.9 3.4 3.4 3.9 3.8 81 3.8 3.9 P! "O (V) 4.6 4.6 80 80 3.0 (‘4 ('3 L!) .(‘J P4 'if' r4 If) C\ (\J l (‘1 ("I 2.9 3.1 3.9 2.8 3.1 3.0 2.8 2.9 2.9 3.2 ('1 P1 (‘1 3.0 3.0 3.2 P" 4. (‘1 3.2 3.2 3.4 3.8 3.9 3.1 3.1 3.7 3.9 4.0 4.0 78 78 77 (‘3 1 4.2 3.0 2.8 3.2 3.0 3.1 3.8 3.6 4.0 4.1 3.9 4.3 4.3 3.2 3.7 4.0 4.2 3.1 3.0 2.8 2.9 2 4.0 4.0 4.1 76 75 75 (‘J 3.3 3.1 3.7 3.9 3.7 4.0 3.9 4.1 2.6 (‘J 1 3.7 4.5 3.9 4.0 4.3 3.8 4.3 103 are presented in Tables 20-45. It Should be pointed out that although the group is defined as "users of physically addictive drugs" the population was herion users. A summary chart is presented which illustrates each cognitive style element, Significance and the direction (see Table 17). It was found that the heroin and non—heroin differed on the following elements: theoretical visual linguistic; theoretical Visual quantitative; qualitative auditory; qualitative olfactory; qualitative tactile; qualitative proprioceptive; qualitative code empathic; qualitative code kinesics; qualitative code synnoetics; associates; indivi— duality; differences; magnitude and relationships. Qualita- tive code esthetics and qualitative code proximics are Significant when the data were pooled according to Tables 31 and 36. In considering differences it is important to determine whether the element in question is a major one or not. The data as analyzed looked at three areas: major, minor and negligible. It was more important to determine whether the element was either major or minor, therefore, by considering the categories minor and negiligible as one, the data demonstrated Significant differences between physically addictive drug abusers and non-physically addic- tive drug abusers on the elements qualitative code esthetics and qualitative code promimics. This is discussed in Chapter IV. 104 .8. M m .mumsvm ago so mmocmu000fi© acmofl0flcmflm mmumoHUCHt 00.05 0v mm.mm «a 00 0H 0v NH 50.05 mm mm.mm 5 M .5m 50.0w 0N mm.mm .mm 00 0H 0v NH 50.0w 0 mm.M5 mm «m .0m mm.mm mm 50.0w mm 50.0w Va mm.mm 0H 50.05 mm mm.mm 5 42 .mm 50.0m mm mm.m0 mm mm.mm 0H 50.00 0N 50.0w 0 mm.M5 mm A .00 50.0m mm mm.m0 mm 50.00 0m mm.mm 0H 50.0m Ha mm.m0 0H 40 .mm mm.m m 50.Hm mm 0 0 00.00H 0m mm.mm 0H 50.00 0m «H .mm mm.mm mm 50.H¢ mm 50.0w 0H mm.mm 0H 50.0m Ha mm.m0 0H m .Hm 00.00H 00 0 0 00.00H 0m 0 0 mm.mm 5 50.05 mm «a .om 00.00 0m 00.00 vm mm.m0 0H 50.0m Ha mm.mm 0H 50.00 «a A9000 .ma 50.Hm Hm mm.mv mm 00.0w NH 00.00 0H 0 0 00.00H 0m «Amova .ma vm.m0 av 50.Hm 0H 50.05 mm mm.mm 5 mm.mm 0H 50.0w 0H tAmUVO .5H 50.0w 0m mm.mm mm mm.mv ma 50.00 5H mm.mm 0H 50.00 0m AmMUVO .0H 00.05 0v 00.00 ma 00.00 vm 00.0w 0 50.0m 0 mm.M5 mm «AMUVO .ma mm.m5 vv 50.0w 0H mm.m0 mm 50.0H m 00.00 0m 00.0w 0 A0000 .vH 50.Hv mm mm.mm mm 00.0m 0 00.05 am 50.0w 0 mm.m5 mm ABMUVO .ma 00.0w vm 00.00 0m 50.0w 0 mm.m5 mm 50.0 m mm.m0 mm «Ammuva .NH 00.0w 5m 00.00 mm 50.0m Ha mm.m0 0H mm.m a 50.00 mm «Azmuva .HH 50.00 0v mm.mm 0m 00.00 «N .00.0m 0 50.0H m mm.mm mm «Amvo .oa mm.ma Ha 50.H0 av 00.0m 0 00.05 am 50.0H m mm.m0 mm A>VO .0 mm.wa Ha 50.Hm 0v 00.00 0 00.00 vm mm.m H 5Q.0m 0m «A900 .0 mm.ma Ha 50.Hm mv 50.0 m mm.mm mm 00.0H m 00.00 5m Amva .5 mm.m0 mm 50.0m mm 00.05 am 00.0m 0 mm.ma v 50.00 0m «A000 .0 00.0w 5m 00.00 mm 50.0m Ha mm.m0 0H mm.m a 50.00 mm «A000 .m mm.mm om 50.0H 0H 00.00 5m 00.0H m 50.00 5H mm.m¢ ma tAa>VB .v 00.mm am 00.00 mm mm.mm 0H 50.0w 0H 00.0w 0 00.00 «m «AA>VB .m vm.m5 5v 50.Hm ma mm.mm mm 50.0H m vm.m5 mm 50.0w 0 A0009 .m 50.H0 5m mm.mm mm mm.mv ma 50.00 5H 50.0w va mm.mm 0H Aqmve .H w m w .m w .m w .m w m w .m Mona: Hoflmz Hoses H0502 ROCHE H0502 :Houwm afloumm afloummucoz Hflmn ummuum ucmEmHm .ucmfimam ma MOSHE 0cm Honmz mo wmmucmoumm 0cm mocmsvmumll.5a mqmoo pumUCTum 00.H mm.a nv.q cmox 0m. 56.H 00H 00H 00M c300 utuoumm N0.H Mm.mw mm nnOuoc ULrHHGr7NH~ m w am neurcmmxd wanufism n~16 ..3.w 1 a (so mud . ; in.» 6 mm.nm 6:.nw - . A - 1 , urmtnyi team an.,, an 0mmrfl. it .Dwut1)uvi U ”01 4.33:5 meund 4 >,. . : H0. 0. . , . : c4 mummu maoumm 5 a a 50 00 mm mu ucmmumm . - . Icoz mo oomucoouom cfloumzlcoz 0 00.0H 00.0H xocmsvmum macep I m Caoumm and 9:.00H ma we N am c300 omU 0 35 080 C . . >8 . m ucoosmm £3 a L mmocu 033 Tmoce mo mwmuumoumm 0>0 6mm mmouoa a a L 5m. 5w.H mm.m 00.00 53.:m 32:,00 CA mummm :aoumm dart n. ) A 00 vzmucmommm mom: afloumm 4 NH 5H 5ozwswoum coflumfl>wo cmmz manaoflammz uocflz Mona: UumUcmum . . .AAnoufl054 maxum T>Hufic000 co coflumasmom ummuum :Hv mHm>wq cofluoflpvé cmm3uom HMOHuOHOTLB ”mucwEmHm MCOmHummEOU mumsqm «no II.ON mamdfi 108 .Ho. v m .pcsom mocmummwflv MCMUHMHcmflm 02 N u Eoommum mo mmmummo 000. u 008000 aro mm. am. an. 00.0 >60 600 00.0 00.H 00.0 00.0 :60: 00.000 00.000 00.00H 00.000 00.0 0300 son 00.00H 00.0 00.00 50.HN 00.0 nnouod eon em. 00.H 00.00 00.0 00.00 00.00 00.0 0006:0000 H6000 00.00 00.00 00.00 em.a0 00.0 dzoa Boa anono 00.00H 50.0 50.00 00.00 00.0 mnouod Boa eaonmm mm. 00.0 00.00 00.0 00.00 00.0 00.0 adamsvmnm unoz m 30m 00.00 00.00 00.00 00.00 00.0 c300 eon macaw 00.000 00.0H 00.00 00.0H 00.0 mnouod eon aaoumm mm. 00.H 00.00 00.0 00.00 00.0 00.0 mormsvmnm . H 36m cowUMH>TQ wanflm cnmvcmum cams Hmuoa (Hammz MOSHE Hoflmz .Aodva m>aumuaudmso muouflcsm HMOHumHomze ”mucmfimam mamum m>auwcmoo co Acoflumasmom ummuum :00 mam>mn coauoavwm cwm3umm GOmHHmmEOU mmmsvm H£U|:.HN mqm¢e A00. A 00 .pcsom TOGMOHchmflm H u Eowmmum mo mmmumoo 550.5 I 008900 00o 109 00. 00. 00.0 >00 000 50.0 00.0 00.0 0062 00.000 00.000 00.000 00.0 0360 Boa 00.000 50.00 00.00 00.0 mnono0 sue me. 50.0 00.00 00.00 00.00 00.0 socdsvdna 08005 00.00 50.50 00.00 00.0 :300 Boa dsono 00.000 00.00 00.00 00.0 nnouoa Boa daonmr 00. 00.0 00.00 00.0 00.00 00.0 moemswmnm Icoz 0 300 00.00 05.05 00.00 00.0 dzoo eon 00.000 00.00 50.00 00.0 mnono0 Goa dsouo 00. 00.0 00.00 00.00 00.00 00.0 5060:0000 aaonmm H 300 coflu00>mn n UHMUCMflw C002 HMfiOB MOSHE H0. ME .AA>VB I oaumwsmcflq HMSMH> HMOHumHomaaII.NN mqmde 110 100. M 00 .00000 000000000000 N u Eowmmum mo mwmnmma 000.00 0 000500 0:0 00.0 00. 00. 00.0 >00 0pm 00.0 Nv.H 00.0 00.0 cam: 00.000 00.000 00.000 00.000 00.0 c300 900 00.000 50.0 50.00 50.0N 00.0 mmouofi Hum 00. 00.0 00.00 00.0 00.00 00.00 00.0 mocmswmum 00009 00.00 00.0 00.00 00.00 00.0 0300 sum 00000 00.000 00.0 50.00 00.00 00.0 mmouo¢ Bum :00000 00. 50.0 00.0m 00.0 00.50 oo.mH 00.0 xocmsvmum Icoz N 30m 00.00 00.000 00.50 05.00 00.0 c300 90m @9000 00.00H 00.00 50.05 00.00 00.0 mmouod 900 C00000 00. mo.N 00.00 00.0 00.0N 00.0 00.0 hocmswmum . a 30m coHuMH>ma manflm cumvcmum cams Hmuoa Iflammz 00:02 00mm: .AO>VB m>0umufiucmso H05m0> HMUHuwHOTQBII.mN mqmde 111 A00. 00 .00000 00000000000 00000000000 0 N 8000000 00 0000000 500.00 0 000000 000 00. 00. 00.0 >00 000 00.0 00.0 00.0 0002 00.000 00.000 00.000 00.0 0300 000 00.000 00.00 00.00 00.0 000000 000 00. 00.0 00.00 00.00 00.00 00.0 000000000 00000 00.00 00.0 00.00 00.0 0300 000 00000 00.000 00.0 50.00 00.0 mmouo< Bum 000000 00. 00.0 00.00 00.0 00.00 00.0 000000000 I002 0 300 00.00 50.00 00.00 00.0 0300 000 00.000 50.00 00.00 00.0 000000 000 00000 50. 50.0 00.00 00.00 00.00 00.0 000000000 000000 0 300 000000300 n 00000000 :00: 00009 Hoc0z 00.02 .0000 I 0000005< 03000000090II.VN mqm00 00m oo.0 00.0 00.0 oo.o 000: 00.000 oo.000 00.000 00.000 00.0 0300 900 oo.000 00.0 00.00 mm.mm oo.o 000000 900 mm. mv.0 00.00 00.0 oo.vm oo.mm oo.o 000050000 00009 oq.om 00.0 00.00 mm.vn 00.0 00300 900 0:000 00.000 00.0 mm.m0 00.0m oo.o 000000 900 :00000 mm. m0.0 oo.om 00.0 00.0 oo.m~ oo.o 000000000 I002 m 300 oo.om 00.000 mm.mm 00.mm 00.0 0300 000 05000 00.000 mm.m 00.00 oo.om 00.0 000000 000 000000 mm. mn.0 oo.om 00.0 oo.om oo.m oo.o 000050000 . 0 300 000000>0o 00000 0000:00m 0002 00009 IHHm0z 00:02 00002 .0000 I 000000000 03000000000uu.0~ 00000 113 Aao. A mv .wcsom mmocmHmMMHU unmoflwflcmflm oz Eowmmum mo mmmummo man. u mumswm Hnu mm. om. oo.o >mo cum om.a mq.a 00.0 cmmz oo.ooa oo.ooa 00.00H oo.o czoo sum 00.00H mm.m no.am oo.o mmouoa Bum mm. mo.a oo.oo oo.m oo.mm 00.0 xocmswmum Hmpoa oo.om oo.oo mo.mv oo.o axon Bum msouo 00.00H oo.oa oo.om oo.o mmouo< Bum afloumm Hm. OH.H oo.om oo.m oo.- oo.o mocmswwnm uaoz m 30m oo.om oo.o¢ Hm.om 00.0 czoo Bum msouw 00.00H no.m mm.mm oo.o mmouo« Bum aaonmm mm. no.H oo.om oo.~ oo.m~ oo.o mocmswmum . H 30m GOHuMH>mQ a m n Unawamum m 2 Hmuoa Hocflz Ho.m2 .Amvo u mm>nsm m>HumuHHmsoun.m~ mqmmo cum ¢H.H mm.H oo.o cmmz oo.ooa oo.ooa oo.ooa oo.o CBOQ Bum oo.ooa hm.HH oo.mm oo.o mmouofi Bum mm. NH.H oo.om 00.5 oo.mm oo.o mocmsvmum Hmuoe oo.om mm.¢a ~>.¢m oo.o :3oa Bum msouo 00.00H mm.m nm.mm oo.o mmouom Bum afloumm ma. mo.a oo.om oo.a oo.mm oo.o wocmsvmnm ucoz m 30m oo.om H5.mm mm.m¢ oo.o c300 Bum msoum oo.ooa oo.om oo.om oo.o mmonod Bum caoumm av. om.H oo.om 00.0 oo.vm 00.0 wocmswmum . a 30m coflpmfl>ma cam m 0 Hon n wumvcwpm z a u 9 H2 Ho.m2 .ABVO I macauome m>flumuflam50Iu.nm mamaa Aao. v my .mmocwummmHU ucmoHMHcmHm 02 a u Eowmmum mo mmmnmmo Hm¢.a n mumsvm Hnu 115 om. om. oo.o >mo gum mm.a qm.H 00.0 cmmz oo.ooa oo.ooa 00.00H oo.o caoo Bum oo.ooa mm.m~ hm.mn 00.0 mmouom Bum mv. mm.H 00.00 oo.¢a oo.ov oo.o mocmsvmum Hmuoa oo.om Hp.mm mm.qm oo.o c3oo Bum msouo oo.ooa no.wa mm.mm oo.o mmouo< Bum afloumm mm. 5H.H oo.om oo.m oo.mm oo.o wocmsvmum ucoz m 30m oo.om mm.vm mm.mv oo.o c3oo Bum msouu oo.ooH oo.om 00.05 oo.o mmouo¢ Bum caoumm 5v. om.H oo.om oo.m oo.am oo.o mocmswmum . a 30m coaumw>ma saw a n @Hmccmum 2 Hmuoa no as Ho.mz .A>vo . Hmsmfl> w>fiumuflamsouu.m~ mummy 116 Aao. my .mcsOw mmogmummmaw ucmoHMHcmHm Eocwmum mo mmmummo Hmv.¢~ u mumsvm ago 00.0 ow. ov. oo.o >mo cum oo.a ma.a Hm.a oo.o cam: oo.ooa oo.ooa oo.ooa 00.00H oo.o GSOQ 90m oo.ooa oo.m mm.mv hm.am oo.o mmono< Bum om. mm.a oo.oo oo.m oo.wm oo.Hm oo.o mocmswmnm Hmpos oo.om oo.o m~.ma mm.om 00.0 :300 Bum msouo oo.ooa oo.o nm.ma mm.mm oo.o mm0H04 Bum afloumm mm. pH.H oo.om oo.o oo.m oo.mm oo.o mocmsvmum Icoz m 30m oo.om oo.ooa nn.om mm.ma oo.o czon Bum msouw oo.ooa oo.oa 00.05 oo.om oo.o mmono< Bum caoumm mm. om.H oo.om oo.m oo.a~ oo.o oo.o mocmnvmum . a 30m cofluMfl>mo manflm cumccmum cmwz Hmuoa Iflamoz HOCHS Henmz .Amvo I m>Hummoowumoum w>flumuHHMDOII.mN mqmda 117 Ado. A mv .UCSOM mmocmHmMMHc unmoHMflcmHm Eovmwum mo mmmumwa nav.oa u mumswm Hsu mm. mv. oo.o >mo cum mo.H om.a oo.o :mmz oo.ooa oo.ooa oo.ooH oo.o c300 Bum oo.ooa oo.om oo.om oo.o mmouofl Bum ov. om.a oo.om oo.ma oo.mv oo.o mocmswmum Hmuoa oo.om mm.m Nw.om oo.o c300 Bum msonw oo.ooa mm.m mm.mm oo.o mmonofi Bum afloumm ma. mo.H oo.om oo.H oo.mm oo.o mocmsvmum Icoz N 30m oo.om nm.am mm.mm oo.o CSOQ Bum msouw oo.ooa hm.mm mm.mw oo.o mmouom Bum caoumm mv. nm.a oo.om oo.HH oo.ma oo.o wocmsvmum . a 30m coauMH>wo cam: Hmuoa Hocaz Honmz Unmocmum . . .AZmovo u uaumnquEm mcoo m>flumuflamsouu.om mam¢e 118 “do. A m0 .Umaoom manflmflamm: cam HOCHE cm£3 UGDOM mocmofimwcmflm Eowmmnm mo mmmumma mm¢.¢ u mumswm ago 00.0 vv. on. 00.0 >ma Gum 00.H NN.H 0m.a 00.0 cme 00.00H 00.00H 00.00H 00.00H 00.0 :300 Bum 00.00H no.a 00.mH mm.m0 00.0 mmouod Bum m¢. mH.H 00.00 oo.a oo.m oo.om 00.0 mocmswmum Hmuoe 00.0m 00.0 NN.~N 00.0m 00.0 HH309 Bum msono 00.00H 00.0 50.0 mm.mm 00.0 mmouofi Bum afloumm mm. 00.H 00.0m 00.0 00.~ 00.0w 00.0 wocmswmum Icoz m 30m 00.0w 00.00H 00.50 00.00 00.0 :3oa Bum msonw 00.00H mm.m mm.m~ mm.mn 00.0 mmonofi Bum caoumm mm. 0m.a 00.0m 00.H 00.5 00.~m 00.0 wocmswmum . H 30m coauMH>mQ wanflm Unmocmum cmmz Hmuoa Iflammz HOGHZ Hommz .Ammovo n ofiumsumm mcou m>HuMUflHMDOII.Hm wands 119 Aao. v m0 .mmocmummwflw unmoHMHcmHm oz H u Eowmmum mo mmmummo «mo. u mumswm ago am. am. 00.0 >m0 Uum >¢.H Hm.H 00.0 cmmz 00.00H 00.00H 00.00H 00.0 c300 Bum 00.00H mm.mm no.an 00.0 mmouom Bum mv. mm.H 00.00 oo.na oo.m¢ 00.0 muamswmnm Hmuoe oo.om 00.nv ma.am oo.o c300 sum macaw 00.00H 50.0m mm.mn 00.0 mmono< Bum afloumm m0. >~.H 00.0m 00.0 00.~m 00.0 mocmskum scoz N 30m 00.0m vm.mm 00.0v 00.0 c300 Bum msouw 00.00H 00.0m 00.0w 00.0 mmouo¢ Bum caoumm “0. 0m.H 00.0m 00.0 00.Hm 00.0 mocmswwum . a 30m coauMH>mo cmm n cuwccmum Z Hmuoa HOGHZ uo.mz .ABmUVO I Danum 0600 m>flumuflHMDOII.Nm mqm<9 120 HHO. v my .mmucmumMMHU UCMUHMHcmHm oz m u Eocwmum mo mmmumma vmv. u wumsum Hnu 00. H0. mm. 00.0 >ma Uum mm.H 00.H mm.H 00.0 cmmz 00.00H 00.00H 00.00H 00.00H 00.0 c300 sum 00.00H 00.0 50.05 mm.mH 00.0 mmouud sum 50. 50.H 00.00 00.m 00.00 00.HH 00.0 mucmswmum Hmuoe oo.om mm.mm oo.om mm.¢m 00.0 c300 sum msouu 00.00H mm.m 50.05 00.0w 00.0 mmouum Bum aflonmm 0s. mm.H oo.om oo.H oo.mm 00.0 00.0 mucmnwmmm Icoz m 30m 00.00 50.00 00.00 00.00 00.0 c300 Bum msouu 00.00H 50.0 50.05 50.0H 00.0 mmouud Bum caoumm 00. 00.H 00.0m 00.m 00.mm 00.0 00.0 mucmsvmum . H 30m GOHuMH>wQ mHQHm unmvcmum cams Hmuoe IHHmmz ROCHE Hunmz . AmUvO I. UHCOHHUWHE QUOU m>HUMHrHHMSOIIoMM man—”QR. 121 HHo. A m0 .chom mucmummmsv unmUHMHcmHm Eocmmnm mo mmmummo Hov.5H n mumsum snu 00.0 00. N0. 00.0 >mo cum 00.H 0N.H m5.H 00.0 cams 00.00H 00.00H 00.00H 00.00H 00.0 GSOQ Bum 00.00H 50.H 50.Hm 50.00 00.0 mmouofl Bum mm. mm.H 00.00 00.H 00.Hm 00.0N 00.0 mucwswmum Hmuoe oo.om 00.0 Hm.mm 5m.ms 00.0 axon sum msouu 00.00H 00.0 50.0N mm.05 00.0 mmouod Bum afloumm ms. 5N.H oo.om oo.o 00.0 oo.mm 00.0 mucmsuwum Icoz N 30m 00.00 00.00H 0H.¢5 mv.HN 00.0 G309 Bum macaw 00.00H mm.m 50.05 00.0N 00.0 mmouum Bum :aonmm 0v. mm.H 00.0m 00.H 00.mN 00.0 00.0 wucmskum . H 30m GOHuMH>wQ anHm oumccmum cams Hmuoa IHHmmz HOGHE Mamas .Axuvo I musmwch mcou m>HumuHHmsonu.¢m mqm0s 122 AHO. vsmv .wcsom mocmuHMHcmHm oz Eocwmuh mo mmmnmwo mms.H u mumsum Hnu 00.0 H0. H0. 00.0 >ua cum 00.H 0¢.H vm.H 00.0 cmmz 00.00H 00.00H 00.00H 00.00H 00.0 c300 Bum 00.00H 50.H 50.0m 50.H0 00.0 mmouom Bum mm. 00.H 00.00 00.H 00.NN 00.5m 00.0 wocmsvmum Hmuoe 00.00 00.0 00.00 00.00 00.0 c300 Bum msouw 00.00H 00.0 mm.mm 50.00 00.0 mmouud Bum csoumm 00. mm.H 00.0m 00.0 00.0H 00.0N 00.0 mucmswmnh Icoz N 30m 00.00 00.00H 00.00 00.00 00.0 2300 Hum msouu 00.00H mm.m 00.00 50.00 00.0 mmouua Bum caoumm 50. 5v.H 00.0m 00.H 00.NH 00.5H 00.0 mocwsvmum . H 30m :oHums>ma mHnHm vumccmum :mmz Hmuoa IHHmmz Hons: Hohmz .Hmmqu I mosucmummcwm $000 m>Hum¢HHmsall.mm mqmde 123 .00Hoom mHQHmHHmm: 0cm HOGHE 0H 0:500 mucmusmscmsm N n Eo0mmum mo mmmsmmo Hmm.v u mumswm snu 00.0 cm. ms. 00.0 >mm cum oo.H mv.H 50.H oo.o cams 00.00H oo.OOH 00.00H 00.00H oo.o c300 sum 00.00H 50.H mm.m0 oo.mm 00.0 mmouua sum Hm. 50.H 00.00 oo.H oo.mm oo.Hm 00.0 mocmswmum Hmuos oo.om 00.0 HH.~¢ no.00 00.0 c300 sum msomu 00.00H oo.o mm.mm 50.0¢ oo.o mmouum sum :Hommm Hm. mm.H oo.om 00.0 00.0H oo.¢H oo.o mucmuummm Icoz m 30m oo.om 00.00H mm.5m mm.mm oo.o axon sum msouu oo.OOH mm.m mm.m5 mm.mm oo.o mmouud sum amoumm ms. om.H oo.om oo.H oo.mm 00.5 oo.o mocmsvmum . H 30m cospmH>mQ mHnHm 0Hm0cmum 0002 Hmuoa IHHmmz Hons: Honmz .Hmovo I mOHEHxOHm 0000 0>HumuHHMDOII.0m mqmde 124 “Ho. A m0 .ucsom mmocmmmMMHu ucmoHMHcmHm H u Eo000um mo m00um0o ooo.mH n mumswm Hnu oo.o m0. 00.0 >00 cum oo.H m0.H 00.0 cmmz 00.00H 00.00H 00.00H oo.o c300 sum 00.00H oo.o~ 00.00 00.0 mmouum sum ov. om.H 00.00 oo.~H oo.ms 00.0 mocmsvmmm Hmuos oo.om oo.o om.m0 oo.o c300 sum msonu oo.OOH oo.o oo.o0H oo.o mmouum sum :Houmm oo.o oo.H oo.om 00.0 oo.om 00.0 mucmsvmum Icoz m 30m oo.om 00.00H om.5m 00.0 2300 sum msouu 00.00H oo.ov 00.00 oo.o mmouum sum cHoumm om. o¢.H oo.om oo.NH oo.mH 00.0 muamswmum . H 30m 00H00H>0Q 000 n UHMUCMUm 2 HMUOH. Hogflz .HO . m2 .Amovo I 00Hu0occmm 0000 0>HumuHHmsOII.5m mqmda 125 HHO. v m0 .00000 m0ucu0me0 0:00HMHcmHm oz E0000um mo m00um00 05¢.H u mumswm Hnu 00.0 H0. H0. 00.0 >00 0pm 00.H 50.H 00.H 00.0 000: 00.00H 00.00H 00.00H 00.00H 00.0 0300 B00 00.00H 50.H 50.00 50.H0 00.0 mmouum 80m mm. 00.H 00.00 00.H 00.0m 00.0N 00.0 >000000Hm H0008 00.00 00.0 00.50 oo.0m oo.o c300 sum msouu 00.00H 00.0 mm.mm 50.00 00.0 mmouud 90m :Hon0m H0. m0.H 00.0m 00.0 00.0H 00.0H 00.0 muc0sv0um Icoz N 30m 00.00 00.00H 00.Nm 00.00 00.0 0300 B00 msomw 00.00H mm.m 00.00 50.0m 00.0 mmouufi Bum cHou0m mm. 50.H oo.om 00.H oo.mH oo.HH 00.0 mucmswmnm . H 30m 00H00H>00 0H0Hm 0H00c0um C002 H0009 IHHm0z HOGHS H0002 .HBUVO I H000Huo0mnoua 0000 0>Hu0UHH000II.0m mqmda 126 HHO. A m0 .00000 000000000H0 0C00H0HcmHm N u Eo0000m 00 0000000 mNH.0m u 000000 H00 00.0 N0. 00.0 00.0 >00 00m 00.H NN.H 00.N 00.0 000: 00.00H 00.00H 00.00H 00.00H 00.0 0300 900 00.00H mm.0 mm.mm mm.0m 00.0 000000 900 N0. 05.H 00.00 00.0 00.Nm 00.0N 00.0 >000000um H0009 oo.om oo.o mm.HN 00.00H oo.o axon sum msonu 00.00H 00.0 mm.mN 50.05 00.0 00000< B00 0H000m mv. 0N.H 00.0m 00.0 00.5 00.0N 00.0 >000000Hm Icoz N 30m 00.00 00.00H mH.05 00.0 00.0 0300 B00 @0000 00.00H 50.0H mm.mm 00.0 00.0 000000 900 qaou0m 0m. 5H.N 00.0m 00.0 00.0N 00.0 00.0 >000000um . H 30m 00H00H>00 0H0Hm 00000000 0002 H0008 IHHm0z HOGHE Hofl02 .0 I 0000H00000II.mm mqm¢a 127 AHo. v m0 .mmoa0000000 0cmo000cmHm oz 8000000 00 0000000 500.0 N 000000 H00 00.0 H0. H0. 00.0 >00 000 00.H 00.H 00.H 00.0 000: 00.00H 00.00H 00.00H 00.00H 00.0 0300 900 00.00H mm.0 mm.mm mm.0m 00.0 000000 900 m0. om.H oo.00 oo.m oo.om oo.mm 00.0 wucmswmum H0009 00.00 00.0 00.00 0N.vm 00.0 0300 900 00000 00.00H 00.0 50.0m mm.m0 00.0 000004 900 0H0000 mv. 5m.H 00.0m 00.0 00.HH 00.0H 00.0 000000000 I002 N 300 00.00 00.00H 00.00 H5.mv 00.0 0300 900 00000 00.00H 50.0H 00.0m mm.mm 00.0 000000 900 0Ho000 05. m0.H oo.om oo.m oo.m 00.0H 00.0 muamsvmum . H 300 00H00H>00 0H0H0 00000000 0002 H0009 IHH00z 000H2 0on0: .0 I 0H0000II.0¢ 0000s 128 HH0. 00 .00000 00000000000 00000000000 8000000 00 0000000 ooo.~H n 000900 Hnu 00.0 m0. oo.o >00 000 00.N 00.H 00.0 :00: 00.00H 00.00H 00.00H 00.0 c300 sum 00.00H 50.0H mm.mm oo.o mmo0u< sum mm. 0H.H oo.00 oo.OH oo.om oo.o 00:0:0000 Hmuos oo.om 00.00H 00.00 00.0 czoo sum mso0u 00.00H mm.mm 50.00 00.0 mmo0u0 sum 2000mm m0. mm.H oo.om oo.OH oo.om 00.0 00:050000 Icoz m 300 oo.om 00.0 00.00 00.0 asoo sum 0:000 00.00H 00.0 00.00H 00.0 mmo0u< sum cao0mm 00.0 00.H oo.om oo.o oo.om oo.o 000050000 . H 300 000000>00 000 0 0 0 0 00000000 2 H 0 9 00 HS 00.02 I >00H000H>000HII.H0 00009 129 A00. A 00 .000000 000000H000 000 00008 00 00000 00000000000 00000000000 8000000 00 0000000 000.0 u 000000 000 00.0 00. 00. oo.o >00 000 00.0 0m.0 00.0 00.0 0002 oo.ooa 00.000 oo.oo0 oo.o 0300 000 oo.oo0 00.0 00.00 mm.00 00.0 000000 sum 00. mm.0 00.00 00.0 00.00 oo.mm oo.o 000000000 0000s 00.00 00.0 00.00 00.00 00.0 0300 sum m0000 oo.ooa 00.0 00.00 mm.mm oo.o 000000 sum 000000 00. 0m.a oo.om 00.0 00.00 00.00 oo.o 000000000 I002 m 300 00.00 00.000 00.00 00.00 00.0 0300 sum m0000 oo.oo0 mm.m mm.m0 mm.mm 00.0 000000 sum 000000 mm. 00.0 oo.om 00.0 00.00 00.00 oo.o 000000000 . 0 300 000000>00 00000 000000um 0002 H0009 :0H00z 00002 00n02 .D I 008000MMHQII.N¢ mam¢8 130 A00. v 00 .00000 00000000000 00000000000 oz 0 n 8000000 00 0000000 000. u 000000 000 00. 00. 00.0 >00 000 00.0 «0.0 00.0 0002 00.000 00.000 00.000 00.0 0300 sum 00.000 00.00 00.00 00.0 000000 sum 00. 00.0 00.00 00.00 00.00 00.0 000000000 0000s 00.00 00.00 00.00 00.0 0300 sum 0:000 00.000 00.00 00.00 00.0 000000 sum 000000 00. 00.0 oo.om 00.0 00.NN 00.0 000000000 1002 m 300 00.00 00.00 «0.00 00.0 0300 sum m0000 00.000 00.00 00.00 00.0 000000 sum 000000 00. 00.0 00.00 00.00 00.00 00.0 000000000 . 0 300 000000>00 00000000 0002 00000 00002 00002 000000m00un.mv 0000s 131 200. A 0V .00000 00000000000 00000000000 N n 8000000 00 0000000 000.0 u 000000 00u 00.0 00. 00. 00.0 >00 000 00.0 00.0 om.0 00.0 0002 00.000 00.000 00.000 00.000 00.0 0300 900 oo.oo0 00.0 00.00 mm.mm 00.0 000000 000 mm. mm.0 00.00 00.0 oo.0m oo.mm oo.o 000000000 00009 00.00 00.0 00.00 00.00 00.0 0300 sum 00000 00.000 00.0 00.00 mm.mm 00.0 000000 900 000000 m0. 00.0 oo.om oo.o oo.mm 00.5 00.0 K000000000 I002 m 300 oo.om 00.000 00.0m nm.mm 00.0 0300 900 00000 00.000 mm.m oo.m0 mm.mm 00.0 000000 900 000000 hm. om.0 oo.om 00.0 oo.m0 00.00 oo.o 000000000 . 0 300 000000>00 000 00000000 0002 00008 I0000z 00002 00002 .3 000000002I|.00 00009 132 Aao. A mv .UCSOm mmocmumMMfln pamoHMHcmHm H Eonmmum mo mmmumwa hmh.m u mumswm Hno 5v. mv. oo.o >mo wum Hm.H mm.a oo.o cam: oo.ooa oo.ooa 00.00H oo.o c309 Bum oo.ooa mm.mv no.mm oo.o mmouom 80m om. mw.a oo.om oo.mm oo.vm oo.o moamswmnm Hmuoe oo.om nm.om H>.vm 00.0 :300 Bum msouw 00.00H hm.m~ mm.m> oo.o mmouofi Bum cHoumm nm.a oo.om oo.m 00.NN oo.o mucmsvmum Icoz m 30m oo.om m~.mm mm.mm oo.o c30o Bum msouw oo.ooa oo.om oo.o¢ oo.o mmouofl Bum :Honmm om. om.a oo.om oo.mH oo.~H oo.o mocmsvmum . H 30% cofiumw>ma cam m o H CH n cumccmum 2 a p B o .2 Mo.mz .m I mmflanOHumemll.mv mqmde 133 Status of Research Hypotheses As a result of the analysis, the null hypotheses were rejected thus supporting the following alternative hypothesis: H Al: A2: A3: A4: A5: There will be differences in cognitive styles, as defined by Hill, between individuals abusing physically addictive drugs and those individuals using non-physically addictive drugs. Following therapy, counselors and clients with high quality similar cognitive style matches, as defined by Hill, will have a better quality relationship as measured by Fiedler's 75-item instrument than clients and counselors with low quality cognitive style matches. Following therapy, counselors and clients with high quality similar cognitive style matches, as defined by Hill, will have a better quality relationship as measured by Fiedler's 75-item instrument than the control group. Following therapy, counselors and clients with high quality similar cognitive style matches, as defined by Hill, will have a better therapy outcome than clients and counselors with low quality cognitive style matches. Following therapy, counselors and clients with high quality similar cognitive style matches, as defined by Hill, will have a better therapy outcome than clients and counselors in the control group. CHAPTER IV DISCUSSION Overview This experiment examined several major questions: (1) Are there differences in cognitive styles between "hard" and "soft" drug users? (2) Is the quality of the therapeutic relationship enhanced by matching, as closely as possible, client and counselor according to their cog- nitive styles? To seek these answers, two separate samples of sixty clients each were selected. The first sample was selected from an incarcerated population residing at the Ingham County, Michigan jail. This sample comprised the experimental group used in the matching-relationship portion of the research. The second sample, being selected from individuals residing in the tri-county area of Lansing, Michigan who presented themselves voluntarily for treatment, comprised the comparative group which was used to respond to the first question above. 134 135 Conclusions and Implications Relationship Study Sixty inmates who were found to have drug related problems were invited to participate in the Drug Treatment Program. Thirty of the individuals were randomly assigned to the experimental treatment group; the remaining thirty being assigned to the treatment group. Clients in the Experimental group were assigned counselors according to the similarity of cognitive styles. Six hours of coun- seling was provided, followed by the post-test measures. Clients in the control group were randomly assigned to one of three counselors. Likewise they received six hours of counseling followed by the post-test measures. As a result of the above experiment (refer to Chapter III for the data), one may conclude that the quality of the therapeutic relationship between therapist and client does improve when their cognitive styles are matched as closely as possible. Further, Table 16 suggests the higher the quality of match between client and therapist, the better the outcome. When one defines counseling as essentially a means of enhansing a client's ability to derive meaning and knowledge from life, which is also Hill's definition of a teacher, then these results are not surpris- ing. Hill and his associates have repeatedly demonstrated 136 the desirability of matching teacher and student as a means of improving student performance as well as teacher effec- tiveness. These data suggest the same is true for the client-counselor. This research, as well as others like Zussman (1971) and Hoogasian (1971), has again demonstrated the deasibility of cognitive style matching. Additional implications are that therapy outcome does seem to improve as a result of matching clients and counselors. From the review of the literature in Chapter I, it was found that a number of variables can be used to match client and counselor with a resulting improvement in the therapy relationship. Cognitive style matching appears to be yet another variable which can be employed to improve the therapy relationship. In addition to improved therapy relationship, the behavior of the client was seen to improve which is supportive of the findings of researchers like Wegan (1970), Strong and Schmidt (1970), Edwards and Edgerly (1970) and Heller and Goldstein (1961). Thus a higher qual- ity match will result in a more favorable therapeutic rela- tionship as well as improved behavior outcome than lower quality matches. Another interesting aspect of this research is in relation to Fessinger's theory of Cognitive Dissonance. Fessinger (1957) states that the existence of dissonance, being psychologically uncomfortable, will motivate the 137 person to try to reduce the dissonance and achieve conso- nance. Perhaps the dynamics which are occurring in the Therapist-client relationship are those of an active attempt by both parties to reduce dissonance. There is a greater probability of dissonance resolution if the cognitive styles of the two parties are similar. The notion is presented that as a result of dissimilar cognitive styles, a dissonant situation is created resulting in increased distance between client and counselor. Because this dissonance is psycho- logically uncomfortable, both seek ways of ameliorating the situation. The relationship is therefore more difficult to establish, trust more difficult to built, and the results are poorer outcomes in terms of observed client behavior. Conversely, where the styles of cognition (Fessinger defines cognition as any knowledge, opinion or belief about environ- ment, about oneself, or about one's behavior) are similar in that both parties perceive reality from a similar point or loci of points of view, dissonance is minimized. As a re- sult anxiety centered around the counseling situation is more easily controlled; resulting in consonance, improved therapy relationship, and improved behavior outcome for the client. This suggests that a client who has been matched with his counselor may be motived to remain in the relation- ship as dissonance is minimized in the interaction between the two individuals. Dissonance may also motivate the client to leave a treatment program. 138 Because of the ambivalent state of many drug users, one may encounter a set of circumstances which may create a "positive" dissonance, thus directing the client toward treatment. Conversely another set of Opposing circumstances may set up a "counter-dissonance" situation which outweighs the clients "positive dissonance" motivation for entering the program, thus he leaves. If the client is met in the program with a counselor who reduces the dissonance in the relationship, he may be able to exert sufficient influence on the client to reinforce the "positive dissonance" which motivated the client to seek treatment. At the same time the counselor would be dealing with the "counter dissonance" which may otherwise motivate the client to leave treatment. The balance of the scales may be swung in favor of the client remaining in the program if matching were done. Ad- ditional investigation is required to further develop these notions. In conclusion it appears the prOpositions listed in Chapter II are supported by this research. Briefly stated they are: that relationship appears to be a most central and powerful variable to therapy outcome; that all psychotherapists have as their effective core the inter- personal relationship rather than the specific methods of treatment; that therapists will not be able to work equally well with all clients; that counseling is essentially 139 seeking of knowledge and meaning for the client; that each individual has his own cognitive style or his own way of seeking meaning; that when students/clients and teachers/ counselors are matched according to cognitive style, there is substantial improvement in the student's performance. One can further state that following therapy, coun- selors and clients with high quality similar cognitive style matches do have a better quality relationship than clients and counselors with low quality cognitive style matches; that following therapy, counselors and clients with high quality similar cognitive style matches do have a better quality relationship than a control group; that following therapy, counselors and clients with high quality similar cognitive style matches do have a better therapy outcome than clients and counselors with low quality cog- nitive style matches; and that following therapy, counselors and clients with high quality similar cognitive style matches do have a better therapy outcome than clients and counselors in the control group. This portion of the research suggests that if one desires to deve10p a more effective treatment delivery system for drug abusers, serious consideration should be given to the notion of matching clients and counselors both as a means of improving outcome, but also as a method of holding clients in treatment programs. 140 As the data later in this chapter indicate, there are a number of elements on the cognitive style map which suggests similarity between different types of drug abusers. There appears to be a marked similarity between heroin users cognitive styles. As a result, drug program directors could utilize this similarity by providing prOperly trained ex-addict counselors at the entry point of a program. Pre- sumably the ex-addict's cognitive style is already similar to the prospective client, thus one would expect the rela- tionship to develop rapidly. "Positive dissonance" could be reinforced hOpefully resulting in a better "holding" power for the program and a greater probability of chang- ing the client's behavior. Limitations Because a jail population was used for the experi- ment, several cautions must be presented. As a result of incarceration, the client's observable behavioral reposi- tory is somewhat limited. When designing the Behavior Rat- ing Instrument, it was apparent the teachers and turnkeys were suggesting those limited behaviors required for an indi- vidual to make a satisfactory adjustment to the jail. As the client's selection of behavior was limited, it is natural to center counseling around those limited behaviors which represent "appropriate" adjustment by the client to a jail 141 setting. For example, the counselor was not able to deal with specific drug taking behavior as this behavior is con- trolled (by the jail environment) regardless of the therapy employed. The desired behaviors, then, were such things as reporting to class on time, doing homework, interested in counseling, behaving in class, lack of fighting in the cell. Should an individual fight or cause disturbance, he could receive solitary confinement, therefore, he was less likely to fight. The position taken in this paper is that because of the limited behavioral Opportunities and because of the con- trols placed on the client, most of the counseling was centered around those very specific behaviors required in the jail. Where there was a good match between client and counselor, the client appeared more willing to do those things required of him by the counselor. Through this positive relationship, perhaps the client gained self- confidence through his interaction with the counselor and was therefore willing to try new behaviors i.e., going to school. It should be noted that factors and/or conditions which exist on the outside would preclude the clients from attempting certain new behaviors, however, these conditions are controlled in the jail resulting in a less hostile en- vironment for the client to attempt new behaviors. 142 It is not known whether this positive relationship and resulting behavior will continue over time when the client is released from jail. It is possible the positive behavior resulted from the client's desire to do "anything" rather than remain confined to his cell. Also some clients may feel that participation in the program may result in reduced sentence or probation. Although the above are factors which are difficult to control, this experiment has demonstrated the possibility of matching counselor and client cognitive styles with the resulting improvement in the therapy relationship and improve- ment of appropriate behaviors. Comparative Study A sample of sixty persons were randomly selected from persons volunteering for drug treatment in a local drug treatment program. Thirty were heroin users and thirty were non-heroin users. (Reader is referred to Chapter II for a more complete description.) The cognitive style instrument was administered to both groups. The results indicated differences between the two groups on various elements. The following is a discussion of each element on the cognitive style map in relation to the two groups. Regarding the element Theoretical Auditory Linguis- tic T(AL), in the non-heroin street pOpulation it was found 143 that 53.33% of the population were considered "Major" in this element; 46.67% were considered "Minor" or "negligible." For the heroin-street sample, 56.67% were major and 43.33% minor. The heroin-jail sample, 38.33% major and 61.67% mi— nor. Chi square analysis suggest no difference between the street heroin-nonheroin samples for this element. It is in- teresting to note a slightly higher percentage of the jail heroin sample falling into the minor catagory. Overall an- alysis of the full street sample and the jail sample did not find differences on this element. These data suggest that approximately half of the drug abusers derive meaning from the sound of a word or graphic symbol; the remaining do not utilize T(AL) as their primary source of deriving mean- ing and knowledge. Theoretical Auditory Quantitative (T(AQ) was found to be minor in all groups of drug abusers tested both from the jail and street sample. The non-heroin street sample 16.67% major and 83.33% minor; the heroin jail sample con- tained 21.67% major and 78.34% minor for the element T(AQ). This implies that drug abusers, either heroin or non-heroin do not derive, as a major emphasis, meaning or knowledge from the sound of a number or a mathematical symbol. It should be noted that this finding was constant across all groups. The Chi square analysis failed to reject the null hypothesis for this element. 144 Theoretical Visual Linguistic T(VL) was found to be significantly different between the heroin versus non-heroin street population. Eighty percent of the non-heroin street sample were major while twenty percent were minor for this element. The street heroin group contained 46.67% majors and 53.33% minors for T(VL). This suggests that for a gen- eral street population the majority of "soft" drug users rely on the written word or graphic symbol as a major modal- ity for deriving meaning. This is not surprising in View of the fact that most "soft" drug abusers in this sample were college students or "higher class" educationally speak- ing, These persons learned‘that in order to be successful in school it is necessary to read from books in order to derive meaning. Our educational system tells us that in order to be successful in school, one must be able to de- rive meaning (learn) from textbooks. The general educational socioeconomic level of the heroin sample suggests an indi- vidual who has not been as successful in school. There- fore this person has not deve10ped T(VL) as a way of deriv- ing knowledge. For this individual the educational system has not responded to his needs nor has he responded to the needs of the educational system. The heroin jail sample, however, was found to be 65% majors and 35% minors for this element. It is interest- ing to note the emphasis in the jail program is education 145 through classes in adult basis education, general educational development exam preparation, high school completion, or college preparatory. Further, there is more small group and/ or individual emphasis. Perhaps as a result of this positive emphasis and experience in education, the clients are "learn- ing" how to derive meaning utilizing reading or skills re- quired in T(VL). This may account for why the jail heroin group contains a larger number of "major" than the street Heroin group. Since cognitive styles can change, perhaps this is what these data reflect. Possibly this can be seen as a positive reflection of the program. However, this must be confirmed in later research. These data found a significant difference between non-heroin and heroin street samples. These data also found differences between the heroin-jail and heroin-street pOpu- lations. The above suggests possible reasons for these differences. Theoretical Visual Quantitative T(VQ) element re- sults found 43.33% of the non-heroin street sample being majors and 56.67% were minors. The heroin-street sample, however, found only 10% majors and 90.00% minors for the T(VQ) element. The Chi square analysis found significant differences between these two groups. The heroin jail population was found to be similar to the heroin street population in that 16.67% were majors and 83.33% were 146 minors. These data suggest that the heroin user (both jail and street) do not derive meaning and knowledge through the written number or mathematical symbol. Approximately half the street non-heroin user, however, does seem to derive meaning through T(VQ). The heroin user definitely tends to be minor for this element. An overall review of the theoretical elements finds the heroin user minor in both the Theoretical Quantitative elements, and the non-heroin street sample minor in only one i.e., T(AQ). Neither the written nor the sound of numbers or mathematical symbols provide meaning for the heroin user. Also for the street heroin sample, apparently they do not utilize T(VL) which would assist their success in school. This implies that from a treatment point-of-view, the pro- gram should not expect to deal with the heroin user in ab- stract numerical terms. They should not be expected to immediately succeed in a typical class room situation. In- stead the system should be adjusted such that the client is given an Opportunity to develop these other elements as pro— ductive ways of deriving meaning and knowledge. This data suggest that by utilizing the strong ele- ments i.e., Auditory Linquistic, the client will possibly be better able to respond. For example, straight drug educa- tion i.e., reading books, pamphlets may not be a productive way Of "getting-to-the-clients." Because his strength rest 147 in his ability to derive meaning through hearing the Spoken word rather than reading, auditory methods should be em- ployed as tools which may have an impact on the client. As expressed in the vernacular, you will be coming out Of the same "bag" as the client. One would be communicating by utilizing a modality which is meaningful to that client. This could justify rap sessions Often seen as a part of drug programs. The non-heroin addict seems to derive meaning from the written word, therefore, this can be seen as an addi— tional tool for the counselor to work with in attempting to assist the client in his search for meaning and knowledge in life. Future research will have to determine whether the elements which are not major in an individual's cognitive style represents a possible direction Of emphasis for treat- ment. For example, will the client's behavior improve if the number of major elements of his cognitive style can be expanded such that the client has increased avenues to travel in his search for meaning? The next series of elements in the cognitive style map are the qualitative symbols which derive their meanings from: (1) sensory stimuli; (2) humanly constructed formal- isms (codes Or games); and (3) the programmatic effects of phenomena which convey an impression of a definite series Of images, events, or Operations. 148 The first sensory qualitative symbol is Qualitative Auditory [Q(A)]. The non-heroin street sample contained 96.67% majors and 3.33% minors on this element. The heroin- street sample contained 63.33% major and 36.67% minor. The Chi square analysis found significant differences between these two samples. The jail heroin sample was similar to the street heroin group in that 55.00% were major and 45.00% were minors. The results suggest that non-heroin drug abusers perceive meaning from non-verbal sounds, i.e., music. Although the majority of heroin abusers derive mean- ing from Q(A), a substantial number do not use this element. Future research will have to determine why fewer heroin users as a group seem to utilize this element when compared tO non-heroin users. The Qualitative Olfactory [Q(O)] symbol for the non- heroin street sample contained 86.67% major, l3.33% minor; the heroin street sample was 30.00% major, and 70.00% minor; the heroin jail sample contained 36.67% major and 63.33% minor for this element. The Chi square analysis found sig- nificant differences between the heroin and non-heroin groups. The majority of the non-heroin users were majors while the majority of the heroin users were minor. These data suggest that the non-heroin drug abuser derives mean- ing from odors, smells or aromas, however, the heroin user does not seem to derive as much meaning through the olfac- tories. Future research will have to determine whether 149 this is related to a physiological difference as a result of damage done to the (nares) from inhaling heroin. Since there is the absence of a major in Q(O), this reiterates the position that a large number of drug abusers have a deficit of sensory awareness. The Qualitative Savory [Q(S)] symbol for the non- heroin street sample was found to contain 90% major and 10% minor; the heroin-street sample contained 93.33% major and 6.67% minor; the heroin jail sample being 81.67% major and 18.33% minor. The Chi square analysis found no signifi- cant differences between these groups. These data suggest that both groups derive meaning through tasts. The Qualitative Tactile [Q(T)] symbol for the non- heroin street sample contained 96.67% major and 3.33% minor; the heroin sample contained 80.00% major and 20.00% minor; the heroin jail sample contained 81.67% major and 18.33% minor for this element. The Chi square analysis found significant differences between the non-heroin and heroin samples. The non-heroin sample tended to utilize Q(T) more frequently as a major than did the heroin users. These data suggest the non-heroin user gains meaning through the sense of touch. It is interesting to note that from ob- servations made during the past years, a large number of "soft" drug users seem to employ drugs for reasons such as religious feelings, mind expanding, expansion of visual, 150 Olfactory and tactile sensory expansion, whereas heroin users do not seem to want these kinds of experiences. For example Often a person on "soft" drugs may "groove" on be- ing touched or rubbed during the drug experience whereas the heroin user during his high does not appear to be seek- ing that type of experience. The "soft" user seeks contact with others while the heroin user tends to be alone in his experience. These are generalizations and observations which must be researched later. The Qualitative Visual [Q(V)] symbol for the non- heroin street sample contained 83.33% major and 16.67% minor; the heroin sample was 70.00% major, 30% minor and for the jail heroin sample 81.67% major and 18.33% minor for this element. The Chi square analysis did not find significant differences between these groups. All groups were majors in this element suggesting drug users possesses the ability to derive meaning from what he sees, ie.e. strapes. The last element in this series of sensory stimuli is Qualitative Proprioceptive [Q(P)]. The Chi square an- alysis found significant differences between the heroin and non-heroin drug abuser. The non-heroin street sample con- tained 83.33% majors and 16.67% minors; the street heroin sample contained 20.00% majors and 80.00% minors; the heroin jail sample was 33.33% major and 66.67% minors. These data suggest the non-heroin drug abuser derives major 151 meaning through Q(P) whereas the heroin group does not. The non-heroin abuser seems to have the ability to combine or coordinate input from muscular functions into a specific response or Operation which is monitored by sensory imput, e.g., as in running to and catching a baseball or typing from written material. Apparently the heroin user does not have the ability to sense or predict consequences in the way soft drug users can. From clinical observations, it appears the heroin user has fewer ways to derive knowledge and meaning from life. The "soft" drug user, as a group, appear tO have a larger number of major elements in their cognitive styles. All of the above data suggest the non-heroin abuser does in fact have a larger (repository) of material of cog- nitive style elements which he can draw upon in his seeking of meaning. The following series of elements are those dealing with codes or games. The first qualitative symbolic code is Qualitative Code Empathetic [Q(CEP)]. The non-heroin street sample contained 96.67% majors and 3.33% minors; the heroin sample contained 63.33% majors and 36.67% minors; the jail heroin sample contained 55.00% majors and 45.00% minors. The Chi square analysis reveals significant differences between the heroin and non-heroin drug abusers. The non- heroin abuser seems to have the ability to put himself into 152 another's place, i.e., he is sensitive to other’s feelings. A significant number of heroin users do not have the ability to place himself in another's position. These data do not suggest why these differences should exist, however, from experience it appears the heroin user is more of an indi- vidual (this is confirmed later in this paper) whereas the "soft" drug user is more interested in peer group rela- tions. It is postulated that as a result, the heroin user is not as concerned for others as the non-heroin. Again this must be researched separately. The implications of the above suggest to workers in drug programs that when working with heroin users, one may not expect "empathic" type of therapy to be as meaningful as it would perhaps to non-heroin drug abusers. One may question whether the "Rogerian" type of treatment would be as effective with heroin users. It must be noted that some heroin users do have majors in this element. Perhaps this is suggesting a "weakness" in the heroin user, therefore, therapy should be centered around develOping his ability to relate to significant others. As previously stated, heroin users have difficulty in communicating with significant (Others. These data seem to support this notion. Qualitative Code Esthetic [Q(CES)] element found 93.33% of the street non-heroin population falling in the major category; 6.67% were in the minor catagory. The heroin users sample consisted of 73.33% major and 26.67% 153 in the minor category. The jail population heroin user was found to contain 60% in the major area and 40% in the minor area. These data were not found to suggest significant dif— ferences between the heroin and non-heroin groups if one considered the elements to contain three levels, (i.e., major, minor and negligible) however, when one considers that minor and negligible are essentially the same group and combine the two, significance is found. Thus in the chi square analysis minor and negligible scores were pooled resulting in significant differences between groups. All groups tended to be major, however, the non- heroin group had a larger percentage of persons in the major category than either the street heroin or jail heroin popu- lation. Thus heroin users do derive meaning through the ability to enjoy the beauty of an Object or an idea. The non-heroin user group contained a larger percent of indi- viduals who do derive meaning through Q[CES]. This is ex- pected as a significant number of the soft users use LSD or other hallucinogens. They want the psychodelic experience whereas heroin users are not after this type of experience. Originally this writer expected the non-heroin group to be major for this element and the heroin group to be minor. Thus the data was surprising in that a larger per- centage of heroin users do derive meaning from esthetic things. In retrospect, when considering heroin users this 154 writer has worked with, the majority enjoyed flashy, color- ful clothing, posters and decorations for their rooms. As for counseling, these data suggest the heroin user has considerable strength in the area of esthetics than some persons may have originally considered. As a result, the counselor could focus on this strength area in terms Of assisting the client in "turning on to life" in non-drug terms. Through increased emphasis on improving the client's environment so it is more attractive, visually as well as physically, this possibly could aid in the over- all rehabilitation of the heroin user. Q(CES) also is the ability to enjoy an idea. This should be remembered by the counselor in working with the heroin user. He can respond to "temporal" as well as physical stimuli. NO significant differences were found between the heroin and non-heroin groups on the Qualitative Code Ethic [Q(CET)]. 73.33% of the non-heroin group were major; 26.67% were minor; 70% of the heroin group were major; 70% of the heroin group were major and 30% were minor. For the jail heroin populations the figures were 58.33% major and 41.67% minor. ' An interesting point to consider regarding this ele- ment is one may have predicted that because drug users oper- ate outside of the law, they may not be committed to a set ethics or principles. These data suggest that the majority 155 Of drug users (heroin and non-heroin groups) are committed to specific values or duties. The question remains, how- ever, are they committed to values and duties as interpreted by the "middle class society" or do the drug abusers have a different set Of values and duties that they are committed to. With regard to rehabilitation, the notions that drug abusers are committed to specific values and duties suggests the client may have strengths which before may have been as- sumed to be missing from his behavioral repository. NO significant differences were found between the groups on Qualitative Code Histrionic [Q(CH)] which is the ability to deliberately stage behavior to produce a desired effect. In the non-heroin group 20.00% were major; 80.00% minor; the heroin group were 16.67% major, 83.33% minor; the heroin jail population were 26.67% major and 73.33% minor. The majority of all groups tested were found to be minor in this area. This suggests drug users do not derive meaning from the ability to deliberately stage behavior to produce the desired result. These results are quite sur- prising when one considers how much manipulative behavior is Observed in a drug treatment program. Significant differences were found in Qualitative Code Kinesics between the two groups. The non—heroin group tended to be major in this area (73.33%) while the heroin 156 group tended to be minor (80.00%). The heroin jail popula- tion were also minor (75%). These data suggest that soft drug users derive mean— ing from the ability to communicate through bodily movements. This would be in keeping with this pOpulation's apparent in- terest in acid rock. One would expect the heroin group to derive meaning in this same fashion, however, the data sug- gest otherwise. Perhaps one reason for these differences center around the drug of choice. The non-heroin group tended to be individuals on "uppers" while heroin is a "downer." It is possible as a result of the "down" or seda- tive effect of the heroin, this group presently do not de- rive meaning through this method. These data raise the question as tO whether the heroin user has always been the way he is (from a cognitive style point of View) or has his style changed as a result of the drug? Also did his cogni- tive style suggest his drug of choice? NO significant differences were found for the ele- ment Qualitative Code Kinestheitcs Q(CKH). All groups were major with the non-heroin group at 66.67%; the heroin group at 56.67% and the jail group at 53.33%. These data suggest drug abusers tend to derive meaning through the willingness and interest in acquiring motor skill abilities. Regarding the rehabilitation of the client, these data suggest the client has an interest in develOping motor 157 skills which may be taught as a meaningful Option to drug behavior. Possibly the client can "turn on" to sports and athletic events instead of drugs. Further, these data sug- gest the importance of an apprOpriate recreational component in a complete treatment program. Significant differences were found between the non- heroin and heroin group for the element Qualitative Code Prosimics [Q(CP)]. This was true when the minor and negli- gible categories were pooled. As previously discussed, the major research question is whether the element is major or not. 46.67% of the non-heroin street group were major; 53.33% were minor; 23.33% of the street heroin group were major; 76.67% were minor; 31.67% of the jail heroin group were major and 68.34% minor. These data suggest that approximately half the non- heroin users derive meaning in the ability to judge appro- priate physical and social distance between oneself and another as defined by the other person, e.g., being able to recognize if you may put your arm around that girl or call the boss by his first name. The heroin group data suggest that 76.67% are not able to measure appropriate social dis- tance. From observations of the two populations these data might be expected. The soft drug users tend to be more "socially skilled" and more accustomed to judging a wider 158 variety of social situations. This does not suggest the heroin users are not able to measure social distance, in- stead the magnitude or variety of social situations may be more limited. The data seem to support other clinical ob- servations including the notion that heroin users have an inability to form meaningful interpersonal relationships or to communicate effectively. Therapy should be centered around increasing the client's social articulation regarding his relationships and communicating formations. Significant differences were found between the groups for the element Qualitative Code Synnoetics [Q(CS)]. Both groups of drug abusers were major for this element, however, 100% Of the non-heroin users were major while only 60% Of the heroin group were majors, and 51.67% of jail heroin popula- tion were minor. These data are surprising when one consid- ers that qualitative code synnoetics represents one's knowl- edge Of one's abilities, i.e., being able to establish realistic goals for oneself. Clinical observations suggest drug clients generally are not able to establish realistic goals, however, these data suggest otherwise. It is inter- esting that 100% Of the non-heroin (soft drug users) were major in this element. Soft drug users perhaps "feel" or "believe" their drug taking behaviors are realistic goals for them. Again the data do not speak to the issue of "ap- propriate" goals, only realistic goals for that individual. 159 What may be realistic goals for one group (i.e., taking drugs) may not be considered realistic for another pOpula- tion. The other interesting point is perhaps that soft- drug users "plan" to take drugs and establish "drugs" and the effects thereof as a goal for themselves whereas the heroin group "evolve" into drugs not as much as a matter Of choice as it is chance. These opinions must be further researched. Another consideration is that drug clients in fact are able to establish goals for themselves, thus in treat- ment one must utilize this element as a strength. The element Qualitative Code Transactional [Q(CT)] was not found to differ significantly between the test groups. 46.67% Of the non-heroin group were majors; 53.33% minors; the heroin group contained 36.67% majors; 63.33% minors; the heroin jail group contained 40.00% majors and 60.00% minors. Q(CT) is the ability to establish with others a positive communication system which influences their actions or goals. It is interesting to note that the data for Q(CT) and Q(CP) are very similar. This suggests that emphasis should be placed on assisting the clients in increasing their ability to judge the apprOpriate physical and social distance between oneself and another (i.e., Q(CP) as well as increasing the clients ability to establish a positive commu- nication system with others. 160 The next three cognitive style elements are the Cultural Determinants of the meanings of symbols. Significant differences were found between the test groups on the element Associates (-A-). 76.67 percent of the non-heroin group are majors, while 23.33 percent were minors for this category. 100 percent of the heroin (both street and jail groups) were minor. The implication of these data is that soft drug users tend to derive meaning from his relationship and in- teraction with his peers while the heroin group does not receive meaning in this manner. Clinical observations tend to support this notion. Most drug abusers are social in nature, enjoying group dynamics while the heroin users do not appear to be as group oriented. Further, the heroin user appears to require the group as a means of securing his drugs but not necessarily as a means of psychological sup- port. On the other hand, the soft drug abuser appears to enjoy the group as a means of psychological support. It was extremely significant that in all samples of heroin abusers tested, none derived meaning through their peer group whereas over three quarters of the soft drug users did derive meaning from the associate peer relation- ship. As a result of these data one must pose the follow- ing questions: Is it appropriate for drug treatment and re- habilitation programs to utilize the group therapy technique 161 as its primary mode for providing services for heroin abusers? DO these data suggest that because the heroin abuser previ— ously has not derived meaning from his associate or peer group that this is a point Of weakness within the individual client and therefore, considerable effort should be placed in group therapy centered around the idea of resocialization, increasing intercommunication skills, and assisting the client in develOping apprOpriate social interaction skills? These data appear to be in conflict with the widely held con- cept that all heroin abusers are essentially group oriented and as a result of this orientation, fall into heroin abuse because Of this group dependency. These data raise serious questions with that hypothesis and suggest that considerable research is required to evaluate this Opinion. With regard to the soft drug abuser, the data suggest that although group therapy may be meaningful to these individuals, per- haps emphasis should be placed on the decision-making process so that the individual is able to make his own decision based upon input from the group, but not a decision which is dictated by the group. There were no significant differences found in the Family (F) element between the two groups. The non heroin (soft drug user) group consisted of 63.33 percent in the major category and 36.67 percent in the minor category. For the street heroin population, 53.33 percent were in the major 162 category and 46.67 percent were in the minor category. The heroin jail population consisted of 41.67 percent in the major category and 58.33 percent in the minor category. These data suggest that the non-heroin abuser de- rives meaning from his family relationship. Additionally, the heroin abuser, both the street population and jail population, are approximately split in the category suggest- ing that a large number of heroin abusers do in fact derive considerable meaning from their family or surrogate family relationship. Regarding the implications for rehabilitation, these data suggest tht drug users generally do derive meaning from family relationships and experiences, thus the family should be involved in the entire rehabilitation dynamic if we are to be truly successful in resocializing and reintegrating the client back into the community. It appears that this is a strength within drug abus- ers which perhaps has not been used in the past as extensively as it should have been. As there were no sufficient differ- ences between groups, these assumptions appear to hold for the majority of drug abusers. Thus, in designing a truly comprehensive treatment and rehabilitation program, it is necessary to involve the family in the overall treatment process. This could include family counseling with the client. 163 For the element Individual—I-, significant differen- ces were found between the groups. 66.67 percent of the non- heroin street group were in the major category while 33.33 percent were minor. The street heroin population contained 100 percent in the major category. The jail population of heroin abusers contained 91.67 percent in the major category and 8.33 percent in the minor category. These data suggest that the majority of all drug abusers are major in the indi- vidual category, but interestingly enough, a much larger percentage of heroin abusers are major. Heroin abusers derive meaning from individual inter- action with others but not group interaction. This suggests that the person has innate knowledge that his way is best, along with an ability and willingness to direct his own be- havior accordingly. It further indicates independence in decision making. Thus it appears that the heroin abuser has an ability and a willingness to direct his behavior either toward or away from drug abuse. It is interesting to note, from a clinical point of view, that the majority of indi- viduals in programs will verbalize the fact that they regret having used heroin, but when challenged they admit they en- joy using heroin and if at all possible, they would like to continue doing so. One third of the non-heroin group did not derive meaning from the cognitive style element Indi— vidual. 164 There are several implications for treatment. First, a number of persons assume heroin abusers do not possess sufficient internal strength to direct their own behavior. Many assume the heroin user abuses drugs to "go along with the group." These data refute the notion and further suggest the individual has substantial internal strength which can be utilized in directing his behavior in another direction. In treatment, more emphasis should be placed on individual "behavior contracts" with the client instead of all group contracts. One should not assume that group experience alone be sufficient and that individual work be nonproductive for heroin users. On the cognitive style element Difference-D-, sig- nificant differences were found if the negligible and minor categories are combined. Again, the important issue is whether or not the individual is major in an area. As a result Of pooling these categories, it was found that 63.33 percent of the non-heroin street users were major, 36.67 percent were minor; the street heroin population contained 33.33 percent major and 66.67 percent minor. When comparing the jail population to the non-heroin street population, there are no significant differences in that 63.33 percent of the heroin jail population were major in this category, and 36.67 percent were minor. These data do not suggest reasons for this apparent difference between the jail pOpu- lation and street population. 165 For persons with a major -D-, this indicates that in his reasoning process the client places emphasis on a one- to-One contrast of selected characteristics or traits. Thus in the decision making process, the data suggests the non- heroin abuser, in order to formulate hypothesis, makes a differential one-to—One comparison before he makes a decision regarding a situation. Apparently the street heroin user does not utilize -D- in this fashion, however, the jail heroin user does. Another way of understanding this element is that a major -D- person has a tendency to think in terms of Oppo- sites. For example racial extremists would likely show a -D- exclusive of a major -R- (relationship). An integration- ist would be more likely to have a major R as he tends to look for similarities rather than differences in peOple. Thus, the element -D- must be examined in light of element -R-. It is interesting to speculate that a person with -D- would form stereotypic attitudes. The counseling approach for major -D- should be highly pragmatic straight forward. Abstract philoSOphical approaches would seem unreal and not useful to the client. NO significant differences between the groups were found on the element appraisal -L-. It was found that 73.33% Of the non-heroin abusers were in the major category, and 26.67% were in the minor category. The street heroin pOpu- lation contained 66.67% majors and 33.33% minors. The 166 heroin jail pOpulation contained 63.33% majors and 36.67% minors. The majority of drug abusing individuals use appraisal as a means of formulating hypothesis in the decision making process. Appraisal is the process involving the application of Magnitudes, Difference and Relationships in reaching a probability conclusion. These data are surprising in that one would not expect drug abusers to be appraisers. From clinical experience, a large number of drug abusers appear to make a decision on the basis of a snap judgment. Per- haps these data suggest that the client has the potential for appraisal, but from clinical experience he does not necessarily use it. This suggests a deficiency in the de- cision making process. The counselor would have to examine the dynamics of the client's past decision making behaviors and determine which factors influenced the decision. By utilizing the client's appraisal ability, the counselor can evaluate other Options which were Open to the client in the past but were not taken. In this way the client can learn to use his appraisal abilities. Significant differences were found between the groups on the cognitive style element magnitude -M—. 23.33% of the non-heroin street group were majors, 76.67% were minors. The jail heroin pOpulation consisted of 46.67% major and 53.33% minors. 167 The majority of non heroin soft drug abusers do not derive meaning through categorical thinking, i.e., using rules, definitions, and/or classifications in formulating hypothesis. Approximately half of the heroin abusers, both street and jail groups, do use categorical thinking. These differences are not surprising in that the majority of soft drug users appear to be alienated by rules, definitions, laws and civic requirements for acceptable behavior. They select behaviors not on the basis of what a rule might say, but instead on what they feel is appropriate. Interestingly enough, half of the heroin population do use rules in this process. This could reflect the notion that a large number of heroin abusers come from cultures which stress the use of rules. These rules were dictated by the "middle class" for the "lower class." thus for survival, the culture has learned it must obey the rules established by others. Mag- nitude as a major represents an elementary method of decision making and requires considerable structure in a person's life. Also the individual uses external instead of internal forces to make a decision. The heroin abuser requires more structure in his life than the non-heroin abuser according to the data. As for treatment, this suggests that a highly structured treat- ment program will be more meaningful to the clients who are abusing heroin, but at the same time there is the danger of 168 reinforcing the client's categorical thinking and perhaps, stimulating dependency on the program. From clinical ex- perience, an alarming number of clients within the thera- peutic community have learned a highly structured way of life where rules and regulations are well known and as a result, the client has a great deal of difficulty in making the transition from the therapeutic community to the commu- nity at large. If we are to reintegrate the client into the larger community, therapeutic communities, as well as other treatment modalities, must first provide apprOpriate struc- ture in order for the client to deal with his drug abusing behavior, but as the client progresses through treatment, it is necessary to begin a full range of services which will wean the client from the therapeutic structured protected environment to the broader community to which the client must eventually return. It is interesting that the majority of the therapeutic communities deal with heroin abusers while most non-heroin abusers seek treatment from rap houses and outpatient facilities. This is in keeping with the soft drug users dislike for structure. Significant differences were found in the Relation- ship -R- category in that 73.33% of non-heroin abusers were majors, while 26.67% were minors. The heroin street popula- tion contained 40% majors and 60% minors. The jail pOpula- tion consisted of 53.33% majors and 46.67% minors. 169 The non-heroin soft drug user derives meaning by a comparison of two or more selected characteristics or traits through similarities. In other words, in the decision mak- ing process the client will look for likeness in things, com- pare characteristics and examine relationships between these characteristics before a decision is made. Such a client would reSpond well to a counselor who uses examples and similes to enhance explanations. Approximately half the heroin users derive meaning from -R-. NO significant differences were found between the groups on the last element in the cognitive style map, circle K -(K)-. 23.33% of the non-heroin abusers were major in this category, 73.67% were minor. For the street population 43.33% were major, 56.67% were minor. The jail population contained 23.33% majors and 76.66% minors. Thus the majority of drug abusers, either heroin or soft, do not derive meaning from the deductive inferential process. This inferential process is utilized most frequently in logical proofs, e.g., in mathematics and in symbolic logic. With regard to treatment, the majority of the clients will not utilize deductive reasoning in arriving at a de- cision, but instead utilize inductive reasoning as found either in magnitude, difference, relationships, or the ap- praisal categories. In therapy a deductive presentation on the ill effects of drug abuse will not be particularly mean- ingful to the client. The counselor can not deal with the 170 "universe" drug abuse and then reduce this down to the in- dividual. In other words, from given premises centered around a "universal" truth, one can not expect the drug user to reduce this to a necessary conclusion. Instead, if the counselor deals with particulars, the client is more likely to generalize to a broader universe of behaviors. The comparative study found that non-heroin abusers do differ from heroin abusers on a number of cognitive style elements and that these differences suggest specific types of activities which should occur in a comprehensive treat- ment program. Further, there needs to be a clear delinea- tion between the types of services provided in treatment programs. The concept is that by clearly defining the treatment process, and by knowing the cognitive styles of the client, it is possible to perscribe the treatment expe- rience most likely meaningful to the client. This is similar to what Hill has been able to do in the field of education. For example, the heroin abuser requires substantial group resocialization, improved interpersonal relationship and communication skills, whereas, the soft drug user re- quires emphasis on increasing individual internal strength. If our rehabilitation goal is the resocialization and behavioral reorganization of the client, the treatment programs must be sensitive to the fact that each person is a unique individual with his own way of deriving meaning 171 from life. The cognitive style map provides us with insights required in order to see the uniqueness of the client. Also it helps to understand the client in terms of how, in the past, he has derived meaning and how in the future he might be expected to derive meaning from life. TABLE 46.--Summary Composit Cognitive Style Map of Heroin and Non—Heroin Drug Users. Non-Heroin Soft Element Heroin User Drug User T(AL) Major T(AQ) Minor T(VL)* Major T(VQ)* Minor Q(A)* Major Major Q(O)* Major Minor Q(S) Major Major Q(T)* Major Major Q(V) Major Major Q(P)* Major Minor Q(CEM)* Major Minor Q(CES)* Major Major Q(CET) Major Major Q(CH) Minor Minor Q(CK)* Major Minor Q(CKH) Major Q(CP)* Minor Q(CS)* Major Q(CT) Major A* Major Minor F Major 1* Major 13* L Major Major M* Major R* Major X *Indicates significant differences. (P 3 .01) Blocks which are blank indicate element was not clearly either major or minor. 172 Table 46 represents a summary composit of the cogni- tive style maps of the heroin and non-heroin drug user. For those blocks which are blank, this indicates the potential Of either a major or minor. Where a major or minor is indi- cated, this was where the element was very clearly one or the other. Limitations As was previously stated, the population has been clearly defined and generalizations from these data would be appropriate to the described pOpulation. It is the reader's reSponsibility to determine whether or not the population, as described here, is similar to the population to which he wishes to generalize. The information in this research should provide in- dicators to program planners regarding areas to be consid- ered in developing a comprehensive human services delivery system. It is important that these data be seen as prelimi- nary and that additional confirmation work is required. It should km: pointed out, however, this study has been success— fully duplicated in Virginia. The following considerations are presented. To fully understand a client it is necessary to examine his in- dividual cognitive style map and to examine the elements in relationship to each other. Caution must be taken not to stereotype all drug users in a category, instead this work 173 must be viewed as defining parameters in which the majority of drug" abusers deem to go, but within these parameters there is much room for individuality. A client's cognitive style can change as he progresses through therapy. Presently, it is not known exactly what ef- fect treatment has on cognitive style or exactly what effect COgnitive style has on treatment. We do not know whether it is "good" or "bad" for a person to be a major or minor in an element. For example, if a person is a minor in Associates, does this mean we should not place him in a group experience as he does not derive meaning from the group, or does it mean we should place him in a group so he can "learn" to de- rive meaning from peers? Does the minor represent a "strength" or a "weakness?" HOpefully this research will stimulate additional studies designed to resolve some of these issues. Caution is advised regarding the possibility of random differences being reported as significant differ- ences when large numbers of chi squares are performed. Summary From the experimental study it was learned that the higher the quality of match between client and counselor, the better the therapeutic relationship. Outcome behaviors also improved. From the comparative study it was found that the cognitive styles of heroin and non—heroin drug users do 174 differ, that the heroin user has fewer major elements in his cognitive map and that the heroin group has fewer ways of deriving meaning and knowledge from life than the non- heroin drug user. The cognitive style of heroin users was found to be highly similar on several elements, likewise the soft drug users had a number of common elements which sug- gests intra-group similarities and inter-group differences. As a result of the study the following recommenda- tions are made. Clients and counselors should be matched as closely as possible to facilitate the therapeutic rela- tionship. Because of intra group similarities of heroin users, qualified trained ex-addicts already have highly similar cognitive styles to the clients, thus the rela- tionship should form quickly, hOpefully resulting in better holding power of the program. Further, because the cognitive styles of the heroin user is different from the soft drug user, consideration must be given to providing differential treatment programs. It was found the drug abusing client enters the therapy relationship with a considerable number of strengths which the counselor can use as building blocks in the rehabili- tation process. Finally, the cognitive style map should be viewed as a devise which can provide the counselor with a window, through which he can look into the life of his client. The instrument indicates where an individual is now in terms of his personal development and growth. The major elements in 175 the client's map represent those things which, in the past, have been meaningful to him. It serves as an indicator of activities which may be meaningful in the future. This is but one tool which may contribute to increased understand- ing and communications with the client. In order to change attitude-behaviors, (Jordan, g£_§l., 1972) we must first be able to communicate in mean- ingful ways with the individual we are seeking to have and impact on. If we understand the client's cognitive style and are sensitive to him as a unique human being, then per- haps we can establish an effective communications system which will further enchance the relationship. Hopefully the result will be the client's attitude- behavior change, his resocialization and his ultimate rein- tegration into society as a productive citizen. APPENDICES 176 APPENDIX A Set Theory Form 177 SET THEORY FORM Symbologosics can be considered as a set, "S," comprised of 240 possible binomial combinations involving major and minor theoretical and qualitative symbolic orien- tations. Expressed in the form of set notation, we have: ‘Tx‘Qy" (TX-qy>. (tx‘qy): (Qy-tx)° where "T" denotes a major theoretical symbolic orientation of an individual; "Q: indicates a major qualitative symbolic orientation; "t" a minor theoretical symbolic orientation; "a" a minor qualitative symbolic orientation; the subscript "x" is a place-holder for one of the following notations: "al"-auditory linguistic, "aq"-auditory quantitative, "v1"- visual linguistic and "vq"-visual quantitative; and the sub- script "y" holds a place for one of the fifteen following qualitative notations: "v"-visual, "a"-auditory, "t"-tac- tile, "s"-savory, "o"-olfactory, "p"-prOprioceptive, "cem"- code empathetic, "ces"-code esthetic, "cet"-code ethic, "ch"-code histrionic, "ck"-code kinesics, "ckh"-code kin- esthetics, "cp"-code proxemics, "cs"-code synnoetics and "ct"-code transactional. Since there are 60 combinations of 178 179 the form (Tx-Qy), 60 Of the type (TX-qy), 60 of the group (Qy-tx) and 60 of the "double minor" category (TX-qy); "S" is a finite set consisting of 2 x 60 = 240 elements. Determantics is a set, "E," composed of fifteen elements, twelve of which are binomial combinations and three Of which are of monomial form. Stating the set of cultural determinants of the meaning of symbols in set theory form, we consider the following: I! A! F! (I-a (A-i (F-i (I-f (A-f (F (2)” <2)" “(2)" where "I" indicates a major individuality determinant of the meaning of symbols, "A" denotes a major associates determinant, F a major family determinant, "i" a minor in- dividuality determinant, "a" a minor associates determinant, "f" a minor family determinant and the subscript "2" holds a place for either a positive sign (+) or a negative sign (-), depending upon the type of influence the minor deter- minant involved tends to exert on the individual when he is in the process of determining meanings of symbols (e.g., words, sentences, pictures, stories, tastes). The inferensics set, "H," consists of five monomial and six binomial elements. Expressed in set notation, the eleven elements appear as follows: 180 s M, D, R, L, (M‘d) I (D‘m) I (R-m) I ®I H = ( (M-r) I (1)-'1.) I (R-d) o S hwere "K" indicates a deductive inference process, "M" de- notes a major magnitude modality of inference, "D" repre- sents a major difference inferential mode, "R" indicates a major modality Of relationship, L represents the major in— ference of appraisal, "M" denotes a minor magnitude infer- ence of appraisal, "m" denotes a minor magnitude inferential mode, "d" indicates a minor modality of difference and "r" represents a minor relationship mode of inference. APPENDIX B Cognitive Style Instrument 181 25H 1. 16C 2. 25D 3. 16B 4. 15E 5. 27C 6. 1C 7. 18B 8. 6B 9. 15C 10. 182 In evaluating the performance of others, I find it important to determine the standards which were set for him. A. Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually Learning to swing a bat the right way is important. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually I prefer working in situations where standards and rules are stated explicitly. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually I am well-coordinated. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually Walking with a spring in your step gives the impression that you are happy. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually I find the reasoning patterns required in statistics rewarding to use. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually I understand the daily news better if I hear it on the radio rather than reading about it in the newspaper. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually I know my capabilities. A.Rare1y B.Sometimes C.Usually I believe that the customary smell of a store influences its sales volume. A.Rare1y B.Sometimes C.Usually I shrug my shoulders when saying "I don't know. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually 10B 20E 20F 27E 27G 26C 11G 21B 21F 26F 16E 11. 12. 13. 14. 15. 16. 17. 18. 19. 20. 21. 183 My partners tell me I am a good dancer. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually Before taking a new job, I discuss it with my friends. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually I make personal decisions after discussing them with my friends. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually I find it easier to win an argument when I state premise (Blank is true) and give a conclusion to the premise which is unescapable: (Therefore Blank must be true). A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually I understand geometric theorems. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually I tend to see all parts of the world as being interconnected. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually I can tolerate the inability to concentrate which characterizes those who are newly "in love." A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually The family that prays together stays together. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually I understand events better after I have discussed them with my family. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually I try to understand why people break rules. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually To become a good typist, I would practice correct finger movements. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually 16F 17B 17B 17G 21H 22F 22D 1H 2G 3B 11H 22. 22. 23. 24. 25. 26. 27. 28. 29. 30. 31. 184 When I play golf or other sports I take several practice swings before I start to play. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually I prefer to ask favors of close friends and associates rather than from work supervisors. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually If I attempted to kiss someone, I would not be slapped. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually If I bump against another person in a store, I apologize. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually I make it a point not to let my work interfere with family plans. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually I have little need for others to help me make decisions. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually When given a problem to solve, I can come to the best solution by myself. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually I prefer to be in lecture type classes. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually I discuss the "sale" prices before I go shopping. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually My written explanations are better than my spoken ones. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually I laugh with the person who laughs when he stubs his toe because I know it hurts. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually 24C 13A 13C 13E 13H 14A 19H 7D 18H 19B 14F 32. 33. 34. 35. 36. 37. 38. 39. 40. 41. 42. 185 I don't understand how people can appreciate a problem until they know as much about it as possible. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually The values of our society are just. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually I would give up monetary gain to avoid a compromise of principles. A.Rare1y B.Sometimes C.Usually I do not permit personal affairs to interfere with completing an assignment. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually I would stop for a S.T.O.P. sign at 3:00 a.m. even if there were no other person in sight. A.Rare1y B.Sometimes C.Usually I can imitate someone else before a group. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually Sales people always find the merchandise that I'm asking for. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually Blindfolded, I can Taste the difference between chicken and beef. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually I know my anxiety threshold. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually In group discussions, I am the catalyst for reaching decisions. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually I can act hurt and depressed in order to acquire favors. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually 23C 14C 18F 5G 8A 88 9A 6G 14B 10D 2A 43. 44. 45. 46. 47. 48. 49. 50. 51. 52. 53. 186 In my choice of clothing, I usually wear contrasting colors. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually I can give the impression that I am happy and comfortable even though I am angry and uncomfortable. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually I know what my physical responses will be to a particular task. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually Random sounds interfere with my ability to con- centrate. A.Rare1y B.Sometimes C.Usually I can feel the difference between wool worsted and double knit. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually I can get dressed in the dark. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually I prefer to read articles which are accompanied by pictures or drawings. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually Paint smell in a room is disturbing to me. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually I am a good actor. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually I can write legibly while another person dictates to me. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually I find it easy to add spoken or dictated numbers mentally. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually 7E 17F 10G 15G 8D 23A 20G 10H ZB 9B 17H 54. 55. 56. 57. 58. 59. 60. 61. 62. 63. 64. 187 I return to a restaurant because the food there tasted good. A.Rare1y B.Sometimes C.Usually I feel uncomfortable when children call me by my first name. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually I can pitch horseshoes or lawn darts quite well. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually I interpret a person's mood by the way they sit or pose. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually I prefer furniture that "feels" good when I run my hand over the upholstery. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually I understand a topic better if I examine it to learn how it differs from other topics. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually I value my friends' political opinions. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually I am considered to be a "good" amateur athelete. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually I quote statistics to others in order to prove my point in an argument. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually I choose clothes for the way they look. A.Rare1y B.Sometimes C.Usually I do not pat strangers on the back if I have an occasion to congratulate them. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually 9D 11F 9E 27H 10A 20H 9F 2C 9C 19A 11B 65. 66. 67. 68. 69. 70. 71. 72. 73. 74. 75. 188 I understand a lecturer better if I can see him while he talks. A.Rare1y B.Sometimes C.Usually When someone is frightened, I can be patient and calm rather than get angry. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually A story is easier to understand in a movie than in a book. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually I avoid probability statements in solving problems. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually I can run and catch a ball that has been struck or thrown. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually I would join a particular religious group because my friends belong to it. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually I "think" in pictures and graphic models instead of words and phrases. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually Oral mathematics tests are easier than written mathematics tests. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually I enjoy art exhibits. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually Children find me easy to get along with. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually I understand the emotions of others. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually 25B 22G 9G 2D 16A 11B 25C 22E 9H 2E 16H 76. 77. 78. 79. 80. 81. 82. 83. 84. 85. 86. 189 I have no sympathy for people who break the law. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually My "best" decisions are made alone. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually When I tune a radio, I pay close attention to the numbers on the dial. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually When taking classes in mathematics, I find it easy to "talk in formulas" with my classmates and teacher. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually I can repair Objects with small parts without watching my hands. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually I am able to offer criticism without offending. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually Life is simple if you go by the rules. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually I regard my personal goals as more important than the goals of others. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually I feel better acquainted with someone after seeing pictures of him rather than reading about him. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually It is easy for me to remember the numbers and formulas I hear during a conversation. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually I have practiced handwriting skills until I write legibly. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually 15H 25G 21E 5A 2F 17C 11A 26D 21G 87. 88. 89. 90. 91. 92. 93. 94. 96. 97. 190 Eye movements are important supplements to my conversations. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually I don't find sufficient reason to change my mind on a subject once I identify the rule which applies. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually I find it important to consult my family in planning vacations. - A.Rare1y B.Sometimes C.Usually I can tell if something is wrong with an engine by listening to it. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually I can remember a telephone number once I hear it. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually I am seldom "brushed off." A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually My friends tell me that I am understanding. A.Rare1y B.Sometimes C.Usually When looking at something constructed by someone else (e.g., a painting, a building, a piece of furniture) I like to figure out why the person created it as he did. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually I consult with my immediate family before making important decisions. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually The tone or inflection of a speaker's voice gives additional meaning to what he says. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually} 1E 17D 11D 27A 22A 5D 1F 19F 15F 23B 22B 98. 99. 100. 101. 102. 103. 104. 105. 106. 107. 108. 191 I prefer verbal directions rather than trying to follow a map. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually Unless spoken to first, I do not speak to a supervisor. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually I avoid saying things which hurt the feelings of others. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually I enjoy games or puzzles in which the solution is deduced from information contained in the rules. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually I make my own political choices. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually I can identify musical notes well enough to recognize a "tune" the next time I hear it. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually After I write a letter, I ask someone to read it to me so that I know that it sounds right. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually Peers involve me in resolving problems. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually When I shake hands with someone, the handshake tells me something about them. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually I "play the devil's advocate" with people to force them to look at other points of view. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually Religion is a purely personal thing. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually 5E 4H 19G 14D 23F 22C 5F 4G 19C 14B 109. 110. 111. 112. 113. 114. 115. 116. 117. 118. 192 I am able to tell which groups of instruments are playing at various times during a concert. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually If I were buying a car, I would ask the salesman to write out the engine specifications. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually At parties, I am able to verbally stop arguements involving others before they go too far. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually I am able to "play a role" if I agree to. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually Holidays are different from other days of the year. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually I would rather do things my way even if they don't conform to the expectations of my family or friends. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually I can tell the difference between two closely related sounds. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually When I go shopping, I read the prices of my purchases and add them in my head. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually I am able to presuade people in disagreement to strive for agreement. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually I can act "cultured" when the situation demands such formalized behavior. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually 23G 20C 5H 2H 19D 15A 10C 23H 21A 6A 1D 119. 120. 121. 122. 123. 124. 125. 126. 127. 128. 129. 193 I choose music to fit my mood. A.Rare1y B.Sometimes C.Usually When shopping for clothes, I like to have a friend along to help me make choices. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually I tune the radio by sound not by looking at the dial. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually If I were buying a car, I would discuss the engine specifications with the salesman. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually I can convince others to do the things that I would like them to do. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually I blush. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually When I type, I keep my eyes on the copy. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually Characteristics for successful people are not the same as those for unsuccessful people. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually Before voting in an election, I review choices With my family. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually I can tell "what's for dinner" by the smell. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually I prefer to communicate with friends and colleagues by telephone rather than writing notes to them. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually 19E 15B 24A 20D GB 1A 18A 15D 24H 21C 6C 130. 131. 132. 133. 134. 135. 136. 137. 138. 139 O 140. 194 I am able to put peOple at ease in tense situations. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually I use facial expressions in showing emotion. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually I often have to make a decision before I know enough about the situation. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually I like to share ideas with friends and associates. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually I can distinguish fresh bread from stale bread by the smell. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually I can make more sense out of what a person means when they speak to me rather than when they write to me. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually I can predict my prospects for success in most situations. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually I "talk with my hands." A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually There are as many facets to a problem as there are on a well cut diamond. A.Rare1y B.Sometimes C.Usually I enjoy outdoor activities more if my family is with me. A.Rare1y B.Sometimes C.Usually I can distinguish between several varieties of flowers by smelling them. A.Rare1y B.Sometimes C.Usually 1B 18C 13F 24D 21D 6D 3A 18D 13G 20A 7F 141. 141. 142. 143. 144. 145. 146. 147. 148. 149. 150. 195 I do better on a test if it is about information I heard in a lecture. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually I set goals consistent with my needs and abilities. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually I believe that a promise should be kept. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually Information should be analyzed in a number of ways before a conclusion is reached. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually I talk with my family before doing anything that might effect them. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually The aromas in a room determine for me whether it is pleasant or unpleasant. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually After I dictate a letter, I have to read it to be certain it is correct. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually I can tell if I will be able to get my work done. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually The quality of one's work does not vary when the supervisor is away. A.Rare1y B.Sometimes C.Usually I learn a subject better when I can discuss it with other students. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually In selecting a beverage, my choice is based on taste appeal. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually 3D 18B 12F 24F 20B 7G 4E 18G 13B 24G 22H 151. 152. 153. 154. 155. 156. 157. 158. 159. 160. 161. 196 I score high on achievement tests which emphasize reading comprehension. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually I can anticipate accurately how well I will do in a new situation. A.Rare1y B.Sometimes C.Usually I enjoy the beauty of the stars. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually A person can never know enough about life. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually I enjoy activity more when my friends participate in it with me. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually -I enjoy trying new foods in order to find new tastes that are pleasing to me. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually I use a written budget in order to manage money for which I am responsible. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually I can program myself to handle boring tasks. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually I live according to moral values. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually The more I know about a problem, the more I want to know about it. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually When given a job to do, I prefer to work on it myself. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually 8C 1G 16G 12A 23D 8B 4C 16D 12G 26G 8F 162. 163. 164. 165. 166. 167. 168. 169. 170. 171. 172. 197 I use my fingers to determine the quality of the finish on wood. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually People say I speak better than I write. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually I enjoy acquiring good motor skills so that I can compete successfully in sports. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually I enjoy the beauty of people dancing. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually In evaluating the performances of others, I find it helpful to determine how this performance differed from another performance. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually I Pick up and feel vegetables and fruits in the store before buying them. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually I keep good written records in my check book. A.Rare1y B.Sometimes C.Usually I enjoy practicing dance steps until I can do them perfectly. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually Poetry is beautiful because of its concepts as well as its words and structure. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually There's always a reason for a person's behavior. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually I decids that my hair needs washing by the way it feels. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually 198 4D 173. When I am in a group Of peOple trying to solve a written problem involving numbers, I am among the first to reach the solution. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually 17A 174. I would wait to be introduced to a "big name" rather than introduce myself. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually 12H 175. Utility and efficiency are important, but they should not be emphasized to the exclusion of beauty. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually 26H 176. Problem-sloving involves related variables. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually 26B 177. I like to figure out how parts of a whole fit together. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually 12E 178. I would go out of my way to see beautiful scenery. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually 4B 179. I score high on written mathematics tests. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually BB 180. I prefer to write with a pen that "feels" good to my fingers. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually 25E 181. In play as well as work and life in general, I find it essential to "play by the rules." A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually 12D 182. I enjoy listening to good music for quality of its sound. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually 12D 4F 8G 3G 12B 23B 3H 10E 11C 12C 182. 183. 184. 185. 186. 187. 188. 189. 190. 191. 199 I enjoy listening to good music for quality of its sound. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually I find it necessary to write down a telephone number as soon as I hear it or I cannot remember it. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually I can distinguish a nickel from a dime in my pocket without looking at it. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually I prefer to read directions rather than have someone read them to me. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually I require beauty in my surroundings. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually I use jokes or humorous remarks to change the focus in different situations. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually I understand more easily by reading than by hearing. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually When I drive a car, I look ahead and in other directions outside of the car. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually I understand how a person feels when being punished. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually I enjoy concerts. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually 200 26A 192. I would find it interesting to discover how people behave by evaluating things which make people tick (e.g. physiological, sociological, and psychological). A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually 6H 193. When there are gas fumes in the car or the house, I notice them. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually 3E 194. I have no difficulty in following a map. (I prefer maps to verbal directions.) A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually 14G 195. I shout and act tough in order to frighten others when I am frightened myself. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually 5C 196. I can recognize who is on the phone just by listening for a few moments. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually 27D 197. I find the type of reasoning demanded by the rules of mathematics suits my style of thinking. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually 7A 198. I can tell whether milk is sour by tasting it. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually 27B 199. I find reasoning like this statement helps me to clarify my thoughts: "All men are mortal; Socrates is a man; Socrates is mortal." A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually 10F 200. When I tune a musical instrument, I use the piano or another instrument for the correct pitch. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually 7B 201. When cooking, I use various spices until the dish tastes "right." A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually 14H 6F 4A 25F 25A 26B 3C 7H 24B 13D 7C 202. 203. 204. 205. 206. 207. 208. 209. 210. 211. 212. 201 I can act attentive and interested even though bored when listening to a teacher or supervisor. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually The "smell" is an important part of the pleasure connected with a new car. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually I solve written mathematical problems rapidly. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually When shopping for clothes, if I find the article I had in mind, I buy it without further comparison. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually I work best in a structured situation. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually I have no difficulty in understanding how to put puzzles together. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually I prefer classes which rely heavily on testbooks. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually My "suffering" in the dentist's chair is alleviated if he does not use unpleasant tasting materials in my mouth. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually When I attack a problem, I approach it from as many angles as possible. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually I would give up an immediate goal rather than sacrifice a principle. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually The taste of food is more important than its appearance. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually 202 3F 213. I prefer to read a paper myself rather than have someone read it aloud to me. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually 27F 214. Knowledge flows logically form given premises. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually 24E 215. I take longer than others in coming to a conclusion because I want to know more about an issue than others do. A.Rarely B.Sometimes C.Usually Age: Sex: M F Type of Drug Use: Grass LSD Speed Barbs Heroin Other Length of Time used: (check one) 1-3 months Amount: 3—6 months 6 mo.--l year 1 year to 2 years ___ 2 years to 3 years More 0-$25 per day $25-$50 per day More 1 to 3 joints per week 4 to 6 joints per week 7 to 10 joints per week 10 to 20 joints per week More APPENDIX C Cognitive Style Mapping Tally Sheet 203 204 COGNITIVE STYLE MAPPING TALLY SHEET NAME DATE PLACE NOS ITEMS RARELY SOMETIMES USUALLY 1 T(AL) 2 T(AQ) 3 T(VL) 4 T(VQ) 5 Q(A) 6 Q(O) 7 Q(S) 8 Q(T) 9 Q(V) 10 Q(P) 11 Q(CEM) 12 Q(CES) 13 Q(CET) 14 Q(CH) 15 Q(CK) l6 Q(CKH) 17 Q(CP) 18 Q(CS) 19 Q(CT) 20 A 21 F 22 I 23 D 24 L 25 M 26 R 27 (K) OAKLAND COMMUNITY COLLEGE, Diagnostic Center, Sept., 1971 APPENDIX D Cognitive Style Score Sheet 205 2()6 .u I .mexn,t- - .;._ _IH. ; o x em .1 mm m 2 mm A 4 am a o mm H mm s E MN m an. :N U :73...) :1, .. ”my; ., .t. A My; J r“. r _ .MLU . A...m..JV 1. «Ta TWUOO Ina U lemoco Am a Amuovo ms m 4- o Azuovo .2 Aaeo OH C50 m 3 Amvo n I AOVO Q U.” A “did m. Ao>ve q . T D A172: 9. r a Aomie m a m 77.; .H. H .Omz mosz monaz ameoe II n m x II II n m x II. n a xll mfiuh .mOz mu oom SqqHm>m mmEmHQ mm . o m .HHMm Tap DOOQO MCHMHQEOO m: .m .co>m:m cowHO mH m: .h o .mpsme OucH mumm m: . \DKDKOKDKD mmmmm VWQ‘Q‘Q‘ mmmmm NNNNN r—lt—ir—ir—ir-I .mmumECH nguo Os“ LDHB HHOB mcon mumm mm .m o m w m m H .EHOO\EOOHmmmHO map CH mEmHQOHm momsmo mm .v o m w m m H .Emumonm .bmnwm can cH UmpmmumucH mammm mm .m o m w m m H .mmmHO MOM HHOO ch mo poo OQHEOO ODTH mH mm .m o m s m N H .AchEoonm .mmzuoao cmmHOv wocmummmmm CH poms mH mm .H mmEHu p.cmmoo m>83H¢ mHHmst ImEOm EOOHmm Hm>mz m m w m . m H Auwmcommmh OHMHHQOHQQO OQH OHOHHUV .HOH>OQOQ mcHOHmmOH OHQHmmOQ mm mHummcos mm mCOHpmmsw mcHBOHHOm wnw Hmzmcm mmmmHm meszB EmOh DZHB¢mmm APPENDIX H Behavior Rating Form--Teachers 215 216 (IIIILni-i II-.I-I.rrl|! (Ill; II.II'|’\.III\ ’1‘ s I I VHS-1.. k” . .. ._ {1.57 ... . 4 .J c 4.4. I 1.1... hr 0 1 . 1.. I 7.). .f... I}. 1;. A, .- s > a 1 ,. a 4 o a l. 1.. .1 1.1.. .3 .44‘. . .4 o A... . . + . 4‘. I. ,-:_E. u ._ 21.-.7 L: . x u r _ . . u . KL H l (‘ ~J r——1 u 1 .74 L4 / \ L. 4 5‘ M :J I.) 2) 'f) C. o q —-( 4 x L! O.) A 3.. 1 o . q , 4 1H TOOOH:-co l . . .. e , x )J . T (u o I __ .. I .8 \Lfr .v HIV rhfiuorrr. C .y. ’ OJ; . . . . \- 1 O I 24 ‘4 .4 . J o a. M , 1 m; _ A w.... LHJH . . , . r _ 4- .. 45. a . ,. . v. . I. J. u o. . HH . . l— 1 u z -. b 4 J n H. o .4 I ,4- .I HF. U :1 . _ __l _. I . . I L . I. 4 . v . W; _ t I .4 . . 1 .2 - . I I . a. . .1,- .(.m . .L. .1 x) .. . I o 1 .. J 4 x.) . 1v 4- ) ./ao}1 I I 4 I. n1 4 1. Th- -H u. H} 1...: U r! ,r. , .L 3.4: . H 14 -. v V I2. .I A . .Momuo :DHB HHHJ wsoHa mu. LO: LO Esau manO wzu ca ,mu.aH Hun; IBMU-THJ .mvutgvn H , -f .4 u . n .x I... .\ .. . Iu.L . V vchicxw n1u:.t O: .w x a 3 Id :4. l .11. J 2.1. ~ 4 I. 4 \I 4 . I \ C c m m n . .,r3 5;. ... u. -F-H-Lc3 .Yraoj ,;1 m H 7 H 1 1 A n.. nlndailj AMI . 4 1 J. 1 l 1.... I. . 1 K r f. .4 .4 .\. V.) J 1 a 1 . ,-_ -. J-x ::. ;L L..- pH . .w o r w 4 - «\JWHH ».I,..Im- \ .H.\» H) I J ) ..4v .1 \ L HI b. r . . r (\p. a s x . I p.» w:\ .mdo... J1... 4., (IN-j. 4.4 u I . . .11 I r-I. r.._1»r . . (A c r P r v 4 In??? VI... u V D.:mTOQ m>d5Hd hHHzfim: @ m I 5 A (f) E O ,_'4 I) f. L4 2 .S- v m m H Anvmcommou ODOHMLOHLAO OED OHOHHQV .HOH>OLOQ mcHchmgn OHLHmmCQ mo >Humwcoz mm mcoHumwsw mcHBOHHOm mzu Mw3mcn mmmva mmAJUduE .UCL UZHde ZCH>GZQQ REFERENCES 217 REFERENCES Abrams, A., Gagnon, J. H., Levin, J. J. Psychosocial aspects of addiction. American Journal of Public Health., November, 1968, 58, 2142—2155. Adam, K. S. and Lohrenzed, J. G. Drug abuse, self abuse, and the abuse of authority. Journal of the Canadian Psychiatric Association, 1970, 15, 79-81. Armore, Sidney J. Introduction to Statistical Analysis and Inference for Psychology and Education. New York: John Wiley and Sons, Inc., 1966. Bock, K. W. Influence through social communications. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1951, 46, 9-23. Barber, Bernard. Drugs and Society. New York: Russell Sage Foundation, 1967. Berg, I. Measures before and after therapy. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 1952, 1, 46-50. Berger, F. M. and Porterfield, J. Drug Abuse and Society. In Drugs and Youth, Proceedings of the RAPCUS Symposium on Drug Abuse, Whittenborn (ed.), Spring- field, Charles C. Thomas, 1969, 39. Birdwhistle, Raymond L. Introduction to Kinesics. Louis- ville: University of Lousiville Press, 1952. Blanzy, James. Cognitive style as an input to Mathematics curriculum systems and exploratory studies in the educational sciences. Unpublished doctoral disser- tation, Wayne State University, 1971. Block, J. and Thomas H. Is satisfaction with self a measure of adjustment? Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1955, 51, 254-259. Blosser, Charles R. A pilot study to explore the relation- ships between cognitive style, need achievement, and academic achievement motivation. Unpublished doc- toral dissertation, Wayne State University, 1971. 218 219 Blum, Richard et. al. Drugs I: Society and Drugs. San Francisco: Jossey—Bass Publication, 1969. Bordin, E. S. Inside the therapeutic hour. In Research in Psychotherapy: E. A. Rubiustein and M. B. Par- loff (eds.), Washington: American Psychological Association, 1959, 235-246. Bown, O. H. An investigation of therapeutic relationships in client-centered psychotherapy. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Chicago, 1954. Brock, T. C. Cognitive restructuring and attitude change. Journal Of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1962, 64, 264-271. Brock, T. C. and Blackwood, J. E. Dissonance reduction, social comparisons, and modification of others' Opinions. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1963, 65, 319-324. Buckman, John. Psychology of drug abuse. Medical College of Virginia Quarterly 1 (3), 98-102, 1971. Burdick, H. A. and Burnes, A. J. A test of "strain toward symmetry" theories. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1958, 57, 367-370. Campbell, D. T. and Stanley, J. C. Experimental and Quasi- Experimental Designes for Research. Chicago: Rand McNally and Company, 1969. Carson, R. C. and Heine, R. W. Similarity and success in therapeutic dyads. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 1962, 26, 38-43. Champlin, Nathaniel L. Controls in qualitative thought. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Columbia Uni- versity, 1952. Chein, I. et. al. The Road to H. New York: Basic Books, 1964. Cohen, Sidney. The Beyond Within: The LSD Story. New York Antheneum, 1964. Cohen, Sidney. The Drug Dilemma. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1968. 220 Cotter, Jude Thaddeus. The affects of educational science of cultural determinants of the meanings of symbols on curricular choice. Unpublished doctoral disser- tation, Wayne State University, 1970. Cox, D. R. Planning of Experiments. New York: John Wiley and Sons, Inc., 1966. DeLoach, Joseph F. Cognitive Styles and Cognitive Dissonance. Bloomfield Hills, Michigan: Oakland Community Col- lege Press, 1970. DeLoach, Joseph E. An analysis of cognitive style diSparity as an antecedent of coqnitive dissonance in instruc- tional evaluation: an exploratory study in the edu- cational sciences. Unpublished doctoral disserta- tion, Wayne State University, 1969. Dehnke, Ronald E. An exploration of the possible isomorphism of cognitive style and successful teaching of second- ary school english. Unpublished doctoral disserta— tion, Wayne State University, 1966. Dewey, John. In On Experience, Nature, and Freedom, Richard J. Bernstein, (ed.), "Qualitative thought," 1960, New York: Boffs—Merrill Company, Inc. Dinkmeyer, D. DevelOpmental Group Counseling. Chicago: De Paul University Press, 1968. Dworkin, Leo. A systems theory approach toward the re- conceptualization of curriculum. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Michigan State University, 1969. Ebin, David (ed.). The Drug Experience. New York: Grove Press, 1965. Edwards, B. C. and Edgerly, J. W. Effects of counselor- client cognitive congruence on counseling outcome in brief counseling. Journal of Counseling Psy- chology, 1970, 17, 4, 313-318. Festinger, Leon. A Theory of Cognitive Dissonance. Stan- ford, California: Stanford University Press, 1957. Festinger, L. and Carlsmith, J. M. Cognitive consequences of forced compliance. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1959, 58, 203-210. 221 Festinger, L., Schachter, S., and Back, K. Social Pressures in Informal Groups. New York: Harper, 1950. Fiedler, F. E. Factors Operative in shaping the concept of the ideal therapeutic relationship. Paper presented at the Midwest Psychological Association, Chicago, April, 1949. Fiedler, F. E. In Psychotherapy Theory and Research. 0. H. Mowrer (ed.). "Quantitative studies on the role of therapists' feelings toward their patients." New York: Ronald Press, 1953, 296-315. Fiedler, Fred E. The concept of an ideal therapeutic rela- tionship. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 1950, p. 14, 239-245. Flanagan, John C. General considerations in the selection of test items and a short method of estimating the product-moment from the data at the tails of the distribution. Journal of Educational Psychology, December, 1939, 32. Fragale, Marvin. A pilot study of cognitive styles of selected faculty members and students in a commu- nity college setting. Unpublished doctoral disser- tation, Wayne State University, 1969. Fragale, Marvin J. Pretty—Much Alike but Somewhat Differ- ent. Bloomfield Hills, Michigan: Oakland Commu- nity College Press, 1971. Fragale, Marvin, Svagr, Virginia, Zussman, Steven. Person- alizing Human Interactions Utilizing Cognitive Style Mapping. Detroit: GMS Associates, Inc., 1971. Frank, J. D., Gliedman, L. H., Imber, S. D., Nash, E. H., and Stone, A. R. Why patients leave psychotherapy. Archives of Neurology and Psychiatry, 1957, 77, 283-299. Freud, Anna. Adolescence. Psychoanalytic Study of Children, 1958, 30, 255. Freud, Anna. The Ego and The Mechanisms of Defense. New York: International Universities Press, 1966, 149-150. Gardner, G. G. The psychotherapeutic relationship. Psy- chological Bulletin, 1964, 61, 426-437. 222 Gendlin, E. T., Jenny, R. H., and Shlien, J. Counselor ratings of process and outcome in client-centered therapy. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 1960, 16, 210—213. Goldstein, A. P., Heller, K.,Sechrest,lu B. Psychotherapy and The Psychology of Behavior Change. New York: John Wiley and Sons, 1966. Greyson, Mark E. A comparison Of counseling using the cognitive style map of the educational sciences and the traditional approach. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Michigan, 1971. Grinker, R. R. In Contemporary Psychotherapies, M. I. Stein (ed.). "A transactional model for psychotherapy," New York: Free Press, 1961, 190—213. Guilford, J. F. The Nature of Human Intelligence. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1967. Guttman, Lewis. An outline of some new methodology for social research. Public Opinion Quarterly, Winter, 1954-55, 18. Hall, Edward T. The Silent Language. Garden City, N. J.: Doubleday and Company, 1959. Hays, William L. Statistics. New York: Holt, Rinehard and Winston, 1963. Heider, J. The Psychology of Interpersonal Relations. New York: Wiley, 1958. Heine, R. W. An investigation of the relationship between changes and reSponsible factors as seen by clients following treatment by psychotherapists of the psychoanalytic, adlerion, and non—directive schools. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. Heller, K., and Goldstein, A. P. Client dependency and therapist expectancy as relationship maintaining variables in psychotherapy. Journal of Consult- ing Psychology, 1961, 25, 371-375. Hill, Joseph E. Appendis A, A synOpsiS of the educational sciences. Unpublished paper, Wayne State Univer- sity, 1967. 223 Hill, Joseph E. Cognitive Style as An Educational Science. Bloomfield Hills, Michigan: Oakland Community College Press, 1968. Hill, J. E. Natural, Life, Behavioral and Social Sciences. Bloomfield Hills, Michigan: Oakland Community College Press, 1971. Hill, Joseph E. Selected Aspects of Measurement and 'Map- ping'. Bloomfield Hills, Michigan: Oakland Commu- nity College Press, 1968. Hill, Joseph E. Symbols and Their Meanings. Bloomfield Hills, Michigan: Oakland Community College Press, 1969. Hill, Joseph E. The educational sciences. Unpublished paper, Wayne State University, 1968. Hill, J. E., and Kerber, August. Models, Methods, and Analytical Procedures in Educational Research. Detroit: Wayne State University Press, 1967. Holt, R. R. and Luborsky, L. Research in the selection of psychiatrists. Bullentin-Menninger Clinic, 1952, 16, 125-135. Homans, George C. The Human Group. New York: Harcourt, Brace and Company, Inc., 1950. Hoogasian, Vaughn. An Examination of Cognitive Style Profiles as Indicators of Performance Associated with a Selected Discipline. Bloomfield Hills, Michigan: Oakland Community College Press, 1971. Hoyt, C. Test reliability obtained by analysis of variance. Psychometrika, 1941, 6, 153-160. Hunt, J. McV., Ewing, T. N., LaForge, R., and Gilbert, W. M. An integrated approach to research on thera- peutic counseling with samples of results. Journal Of Conseling Psychology, 1959, 6, 46-54. Jackson, D. D. In Contemporary Psychotherapies, M. I. Stein (ed.). New York: Free Press, 1961, 256-271. Jordan, J. E., Kaple, J., Maclean, C., and Nicholson, W. A facet theory model for studying attitudes toward addiction dependency: some cross-culture findings. Abstracts. 2nd International Symposium on Drug Abuse, Jerusalem, May 28-31, 1972. 224 Kelly, G. A. The Psychology of Personal Constructs. New York: Norton, 1955. Korzybski, Alfred. Science and Sanity: An Introduction to Non-Aristotelian Systems and General Semantics. 3rd ed. Lakeville, Connecticut: The International Non- Aristotelian Library Publishing Company, 1949. Kunce, Joseph and Anderson, Wayne. Counselor-client Simi- larity and referral bias. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 1970, 2, 17, 102—106. Lazarus, A. Group therapy of phobic disorders by systematic desensitization. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology: 1961, 63, 504-510. Libo, L. Picture Impressions: A Projective Technique for Investigating the Patient-Therapist Relationship. Baltimore: University of Maryland Medical School, 1959. Libo, L. The projective expression of patient-therapist attraction. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 1957, 13, 33-36. Lichtenstein, E. Personality similarity and therapeutic success: a failure to replicate. Journal of Con- sulting Psychology, 1966, 30, 282. Liebert, Robert S. Drug use: symptom, disease, or adoles- cent experimentation--the task of therapy. Journal of American College Health Association, 1967, 16, 25-29. Lindesmith, A. R. The Addict and The Law. Bloomington, Indiana: Indiana University Press, 1965. Loevinger, J. and Ossorio, A. Evaluation of therapy by self-report: a paradox. Journal of Abnormal and social Psychology, 1959, 58, 392-394. Lorr, M., McNair, D. M., Michaus, W. W. and Raskin, A. Frequency of treatment and change in psychotherapy. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1962, 64, 281-292. Maslow, A. H. Synanon and Eupsychia. Journal of Human— istic Psychology, 1967, Z (1), 28-35. Mason, Percy. Clinical and psychological observations on narcotic drug addiction. Israel Medical Journal, 1960, 19 (1-2), 19—26. 225 Masserman, Jules H. Part IV: drugs and addiction. Current Psychiatric Therapies, 1967, 1, 118-156. McAdam, Glenn F. Personalizing instruction through the edu- cational sciences of cognitive style and teaching style. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Wayne State University, 1971. McClelland, D. C. Toward a theory of motive acquisition. American Psychologist, May, 1965, 321-333. Mendelsohn, G. A. and Geller, M. H. Effects of counselor- client Similarity on the outcome of counseling. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 1963, 10, 71-75. Miller, J. G. Objective methods of evaluating process and outcome in psychotherapy. American Journal of Psy- chiatry, 1951, 108, 258—263. Murray, E. J. Learning theory and psychotherapy: biotrOpic versus sacrotrOpic approaches. Journal of Counsel- ing Psychology. Newcomb, T. M. Personality and Social Change: Attitude Formation in a Student Community. New York: Dry- den, 1943. Newcomb, T. M. The prediction of interpersonal attraction. American Psychologist, 1956, 11, 575-586. Newcomb, Theodore, Turner, Ralph, and Converse, Philip. Social Psychology. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston, Inc., 1965. Nishioka, Richard T. An evaluation of a systems analysis Operation in a community college. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Wayne State University, 1970. Ort, Barbara. An examination of relationships between the measurable cognitive characteristics of a French I teacher and the student's success in that course. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Michigan State University, 1971. Osgood, C. E., Suci, G. J., and Tannenbaum, P. H. The Measurement of Meaning. Urbana: University of Illinois Press, 1957. Parloff, M. B. An Analysis of Therapeutic Relationships in a Group Therapy Setting. Case Western Reserve University, 1953. 226 Parloff, Morris B. Therapist-patient relationships and outcome of psychotherapy. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 1961, 25, 1, 29—38. Patterson, C. H. The place of values in counseling and psychotherapy. Journal of Conseling Psychology, 1958, 5, 216-223. Quinn, R. D. Psychotherapists expressions as an index to the quality Of early therapeutic relationships established by representatives of the non-directive, adlerian and psychoanalytic schools. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Chicago, 1950. Rackman, S. The treatment of anxiety and phobic reactions by systematic desensitization psychotherapy. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1959, 58, 259-263. Rankin, Stuart C. A theory of an isomorphism-model-hypothe- sis method of thought. Unpublished doctoral disser- tation, Wayne State University, 1964. RapOport, Anatol. What is semantics? The American Scien- tist, 49 (1), January, 1952. Rasof, Elvin L. Approach to Optimal decision-making in selected areas of education. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Wayne State University, 1965. Robinson, Richard L. A descriptive study of specific achievements and aptitudes of the 'high risk' stu- dents in Oakland University's higher opportunities program in education. Unpublished doctoral disser- tation, Wayne State University, 1971. Rogers, C. R. In Contemporary Psychotherapies, M. I. Stein (ed.), "The characteristics of a helping relation- ship." New York: Free Press, 1961, 92-115. Sabatu, Gerald. Some trends in the treatment and epidemiol- ogy of drug addiction: psychotherapy and synanon. Psychotherapy: Therapy, Research and Practice, 1967, 4 (2), 92-96. Salowich, LeRoy. Some biochemical products associated with memory in the cognitive style Of an individual: an exploratory study in the educational sciences. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Wayne State Uni- versity, 1971. 227 Sapolsky, A. Relationship between patient-doctor compata- bility, mutual perception, and outcome of treatment. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 1965, 70, 70-76. Schroeder, A. V. Cognitive Styles: 'The Key to Student Placement'. Bloomfield Hills, Michigan: Oakland Community College Press, 1971. Schroeder, Arlen V. A study of the relationship between student and teacher cognitive styles and student- derived teacher evaluations. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Wayne State University, 1970. Sharoff, R. L. Character problems and their relationship to drug abuse. American Journal of Psychoanalysis, 1969, 29, 186-193. Shoben, E. J., Jr. The therapeutic Object: men or machines? Journal of Consulting Psychology, 1963, 10, 264-268. Shuert, Keith L. A study to determine whether a selected type of cognitive style predisposes one to do well in mathematics. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Wayne State University, 1970. Snyder, W. U. The Psychotherapy Relationship. New York: MacMillan, 1961. Spitler, Gail. An investigation of various cognitive styles and the implications for mathematics education. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Wayne State Uni— versity, 1970. Strong, S. R. Counseling: an interpersonal influence proc- ess. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 1968, 15, 215-224. Strong, Stanley R. and Schmidt, L. D. Trustworthiness and influence in counseling. Journal of Counseling Psyghology, 1970, 17, 3, 197—204. Strupp, H. H. In Experimental Foundations Of Clinical Psy- chology, A. J. Bachrach (ed.). "Patient-doctor relationships," 1962, 576-615. Summers, Irvin B. Exploratory study: comparison of cogni- tive styles to employee evaluation and empooyee satisfaction. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Wayne State University, 1971. 228 Svagr, Virginia, Fragale, Marvin, Zussman, Steven. Teacher's Rating Scale. Detroit: G.M.S. Associates, Inc., 1971. Tannenbaum, P. H. and Osgood, C. E. The principle of con- gruity in the prediction of attitude change. Psy- chology Review, 1955, 62, 42-55. Taylor, J. W. Relationship of success and length in psy- chotherapy. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 1956, 20, 332. Truax, C. B. Effective ingredients in psychotherapy. Jour- nal of Counseling Psychology, 1961, 10, 256-263. Truax, C. B. The process of group psychotherapy. Psychology Monographs, 1961, 75, Whole NO. 511. Truckey, George Russel. A study directed toward develOpment of a procedure for analyzing and improving communi- cation processes within school districts.- Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Wayne State University, 1963. Tuma, A. H. and Gustad, J. W. The effects of client and counselor personality characteristics on client learning in counseling. Journal of Counseling Psy- chology, 1957, p, 136-141. Tyler, L. E. The Work of the Counselor.- New York: Apple- ton-Century-Crafts, Inc., 1961. Van der Veen, F. Effects of the therapist and the patient on each other's therapeutic behavior. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 1965, 29, 19-26. Van der Veen, F. The perception by clients and by judges of the conditions Offered by the therapist in the therapy relationship. University of Wisconsin Psyé chiatric Institute Bulletin, 1961, NO. 10, 1. Warner, James L. An analysis of the cognitive styles of community college freshmen enrolled in a life science course. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Wayne State University, 1970. Warner, J. L. An Analysis of The Cognitive Styles of _ Students Utilizing Different Modes Of_Instruction. Bloomfield Hills, Michigan: Oakland Community ' College Press, 1971. III! 229 Wasser, Laurence. Are Marks Larks? Bloomfield Hills, Michigan: Oakland Community College Press, 1971. Wasser, Laurence. The Educational Science Of Cognitive Style. Detroit: Oakland Community College Press, 1971. Wasser, Laurence. An investigation into cognitive style as a facet of teacher's systems Of student appraisal. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Michigan, 1969. Waters, Thomas F. A report Of an internal evaluation sys- tem for an experimental educational institution in an urban area. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Wayne State University, 1970. Wagan, Michael. Effect of therapist-patient personality variables on therapeutic Outcome. Journal of Con- sulting and Clinical PSYChOlOgYI 1970, 32 (3), 356- 361. Wolber, L. R. The Technique of Psychotherapy. New York: Grune and Stratton, 1954. Wolpe, J. Psychotherapy By Reciprocal Inhibition. London: Oxford University, 1958. Wyett, J. L. A pilot study to analyze cognitive style and teaching style with reference to selected strata of the defined educational sciences. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Wayne State University, 1967. Yasaitis, V. D. The Underachievement Syndrome. Bloomfield Hills, Michigan: Oakland Community College Press, 1971. Zussman, Steven. Cognitive Style and Administrative Style. Bloomfield Hills, Michigan: Oakland Community College Press, 1971. Zussman, Steven. A pilot study exploration of cognitive style and administrative style as defined in the educational sciences. Unpublished doctoral disser- tation, Wayne State University, 1968.