ABSTRACT THE USE OF DEVELOPMENTAL TASKS FOR TRAINING COUNSELORS USING INTERPRRSONAL PROCESS RECALL By James David Spivack The purpose of this study was to determine the effects of two different counselor training experiences on subse- quent counseling skills of counselor candidates. The first training procedure was an adaptation of the Interpersonal Process Recall (IPR) technique within a developmental task framework. The second procedure was a traditional class- room counselor training experience using lectures, demon- strations, and discussions. The experimental approach was based on two assumptions. The first was that counselors face a series of developmental tasks during counselor preparation around which training should be deve10ped. Four such tasks were defined: (a) to become aware of and sensitive to one's own feelings during the counseling process, (b) to become sensitive to, aware of, and understanding of client communication, (c) to become aware of the elements of effective communication behaviors in counseling, and (d) to become sensitive to, aware of, and understanding of the bilateral nature of the counseling relationship and the mutual impact between counselor and client. The second assumption was that if trainees were James David Spivack exposed to stimulated videotape recall and the use of simu- lation materials, their sensitivity to interpersonal commun- ication would be ehightened and their ability to communicate effectively with clients would be increased. The traditional method of classroom teaching consisted of lectures, discussions, and demonstrations by a professor in the Department of Counseling, Personnel Services, and Educational Psychology at Michigan State University and by guest lecturers. It was anticipated that the questions "Could some of the newer IPR techniques deve10ped in a laboratory setting be incorporated into the present training model?", and "Could the training model be applied practically within the time and financial limitations of an ongoing masters level pro- gram?" would be answered in this study. The trainees used in this study were 20 graduate students at Michigan State University. (None of the students had yet had a counseling practicum.) The subjects were ran- domly assigned to one of two group treatment sequences (10 students per group), with group 1 first receiving the IPR training and group 2 first receiving the traditional class- room method of teaching. At the midpoint of the course, the treatments were reversed, with group 1 now receiving the traditional method and group 2 receiving the IPR. All IPR tasks were supervised by advanced graduate students, and no faculty members were involved in the IPR training procedures. James David Spivack During the first stage of IPR training, the Affect Simulation Films were shown, with the purpose of having trainees gain an awareness of their own feelings to situa- tions depicted on film. The Affect Simulation Films were shown, trainees were videotaped watching these films, and a stimulated recall of this videotape was conducted. In the second stage, a brief lecture on verbal and nonberbal ways of communicating was presented; each individual then had the Opportunity to role play counselor and client, which was audiotaped. A stimulated recall was conducted for each audiotape, with the purpose Of having trainees gain an awareness of client communications. In the third stage, a lecture was presented on the elements of effective communica- tion behaviors in counseling. Training audio and videotapes were played and trainees practiced rating these tapes on the dimensions discussed. Trainees also had the opportunity to role play and rate their own interview behaviors. During the final stage, trainees role played counselor anc client and were videotaped, "Counselor Recall" and "Mutual Recall" sessions were conducted, with the purpose of having trainees begin to understand the bilateral nature of the counseling relationship. In order to evaluate the effectiveness of the two treatments, a pre-mid-post design was used, using the pre, James David Spivack mid and post sessions to sample the trainees' behavior on the Affective Sensitivity Scale and under two interview con- ditions (coached client and role play). Judges used the Counselor Verbal Response Scale (CVRS) and Empathic Under- standing Scale (EUS) for rating audiotaped interviews of each counselor under each interview condition for each of three interview sessions (pre, mid, post). An analysis of variance for repeated measures was per- formed on the mean gain score changes from pre to mid to post on the criterion measures (ASS, and on the CVRS and EUS for the interview conditions), to ascertain whether there were differences between groups 1 and 2 based on the treat- ments they received. Signiticant differences (p(.05) were found between groups 1 and 2 on the understanding, specific, and explora- tory subscales of the CVRS under the coached client condi- tion and between groups 1 and 2 on the affective, understand- ing, specific, and exploratory subscales of the CVRS under the role play condition when the IPR method was directly compared to the traditional method. No significant differ- ences were found between the two groups in the level of effectiveness for accurate empathy (EUS) and on the ASS (although there was significant within group growth for the group receiving the IPR training). The means increased in the hypothesized direction on all dimensions. James David Spivack The analysis of the data suggests that the IPR techni- ques which were developed in a laboratory setting were successfully incorporated into the present training design. The results take on added significance because this study was a replication of another study conducted under laboratory conditions, and the present program was effectively applied within the time and financial limitations of an on-going masters level program using advanced graduate students as supervisors and trainers. THE USE OF DEVELOPMENTAL TASKS FOR TRAINING COUNSELORS USING INTERPERSONAL PROCESS RECALL By James David Spivack A THESIS Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Counseling, Personnel Services and Educational Psychology 1970 ,/’7 \"‘5 wa‘ DEDICATION PEACE ii ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS A number of individuals have contributed their inspira- tion, support, encouragement and time. Without them, this project would never have been possible: To Norm Kagan, who always seemed to be there in time of need, and who, through his guidance, perseveration, creativity, and dedication, taught me the true meaning of the word "Professional". To Cecil Williams, who gave of himself freely and un- selfishly, who always had time, and who enabled me to grow and become a fuller, richer person. To Bill Kell and Greg Miller, who showed patience, understanding and support during the most trying of times. To fellow colleagues, students, and friends Mary Heiserman, Karen Rowe, Cathy Scharf, Bob Dende, Al Grzegorek, Dan Munoz, Paul Schauble, Tom Spierling, and Bob Van Noord, who offered their services freely generously, and unselfishly, and to Sharyon Morgan who typed my dissertation. Lastly, but most importantly, to my wife Barb, who proved to be a source of comfort and inspiration and who continually validated me and my purpose. iii Dedication. TABLE OF CONTENTS Acknowledgements. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . List Of Tab List Of Fig List Of App CHAPTER les. 0 O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O ures O O O I O O O O O O O O O O O O O O C 0 end ices. O O C O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O I STATEMENT OF THE PROBLEM. . . . . . . . . . . . Purpose Of The Study . . . . . . . . . . . . Need For The Study . . . . . . . . . . . . . Theoretical Framework. . . Delimitations Of The Study . . . . . . . . . Definition Of Terms. . . . . . . . . . . . . Assumptions. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Hypotheses . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Overview . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . II REVIEW OF LITERATURE. . . . . . . . . . . . . . III DES Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Traditional Approaches To Supervision And Training. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Technological (And Innovative) Approaches To Supervision And Training . . . . . . . Application Of Specific Techniques To Train- ing And Supervision . . . . . . . . . . . Conclusions And Implications . . . . . . . . IGN AND m mommy I O C O O O O O O O O O 0 Description Of Experimental Procedures . . . Description of Experimental Treatment . . Description Of Control Treatment. . . . . Coached Clients . . . . . . . . . . . . . Training Facility . . . . . . . . . . . . Supervisors . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Subjects. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Instrumentation. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Affective Sensitivity Scale . . . . . . . Counselor Verbal Response Scale . . . . . Empathic Understanding Scale. . . . . . . Scoring Of Criterion Measures. . . . . . . . Analysis of Data . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Hypotheses . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Summary. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . iv PAGE ii iii vi viii ix 35 36 45 56 75 78 79 87 93 95 97 99 100 102 103 106 109 112 114 114 116 CHAPTER IV ANALYSIS OF THE DATA . . . . . . . . . . Affective Sensitivity Scale . . . . . Counselor Verbal ReSponse Scale And Empathic Understanding Scale . . . Coached Client Condition . . . . . Role Play Condition. . . . . . . . Intercorrelations Of Judges' Ratings. Summary . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . V SUMMARY AND IMPLICATIONS . . . . . . . . Summary . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Conclusions An Interpretations. . Implications. . . . . . . . . . . . . Clinical Observations. . . . . . . Implications For Counselor Training. Implications For Future Research. . . BIBLIOGRAPHY C O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O APPENDICES O O O O O O O O I O O O O O O C O O 0 PAGE 118 121 126 127 134 141 142 145 145 152 163 163 173 178 181 195 TABLE 3.1 3.2 3.3 3.4 3.5 3.6 4.2 4.3 4.4 4.5 LIST OF TABLES Schematic Representation Of Over-All Exper- imental Design . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Description Of Experimental Design. . . . . . . Comparison Of Subjects On The Demographic Var- iables Of Sex, Age, And Educational Level In Groups 1 And 2 O O O O O O C O O O O O 0 Data Resulting From The Administration Of Form B Of The ASS On A Pretest-Posttest Basis To Sample Groups Of Counselor Educa- tion Students. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Comparison Of Mean Scores Of PhD And MA Coun- selor Candidates On The Dimensions of The CVRS O O O O O O O O O O I I O O I O O 0 Findings On The Therapeutic Effectiveness Of Accurate Empathy . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Results Of The Analysis Of Variance Comparing The Mean Gain Scores On The ASS Over Time Intervals 1 And 2 Between Groups 1 And 2 . . Comparison Of Group Raw Score And Gain Score Means For Pre To Mid To Post Treatment Measurements Over The Sequence Of Treat- ments On The ASS Between Groups 1 And 2. . . Results Of The Analysis Of Variance For Bet- ween Group Differences In Mean Gain Scores On The CVRS and EDS Over Time Under Coached Client Condition . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Scheffe Post Hoc Analysis Between Groups 1 And 2 On Each Of The Subscales For The Mid-Pre Time Interval Under The Coached Client Condition . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Scheffe Post Hoc Analysis Between Groups 1 And 2 On Each Of The Subscales For The Post- Pre Time Interval Under The Coached Client Condition. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . vi PAGE 80 81 102 105 109 111 122 123 128 127 129 TABLE PAGE 4.6 Comparison Of Group Raw Score Means On The Five Subscales Under The Coached Client Condition Between Groups 1 And 2 Over Time . 131 4.7 Results Of The Analysis Of Variance For Bet- ween Group Differences In Mean Gain Scores On The CVRS and EUS Over Time Under The Role Play Condition. . . . . . . . . . . . . 135 4.8 Scheffe Post Hoc Analysis Between Groups 1 And 2 On Each Of The Subscales For The Mid- Pre Time Interval Under The Role Play Con- dition . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 136 4.9 Scheffe Post Hoc Analysis Between Groups 1 And 2 On Each Of The Subscales For the Post- Pre Time Interval Under The Role Play Con- dition . . . . . . ... . . . . . . . . . . . 136 4.10 Comparison Of Group Raw Score Means On The Five Subscales Under The Role-Play Condi- tion Between Groups 1 And 2 Over Time. . . . 138 4.11 Intercorrelations Of Rater 1 And Rater 2 For CVRS and EUS Measures Made At Each Of Three Times For Role Play Interviews . . . . . . . 141 4.12 Intercorrelations Of Rater 1 And Rater 2 For CVRS And EUS Measures Made At Each Of Three Times For Coached Client Interviews. . 142 4.13 Summary Of Hypotheses Related To Between Group Differences On The Criterion Measures ove r Time 0 O O O O O O O O O O O I O O O O o 144 5.1 Comparison Of Mid-Pre And Post-Pre Treatment Mean Gains On The ASS For Groups 1 And 2 . . 154 5.2 Descriptive Comparison Of Counselors At Var- ious Levels Of Training On Each Of The Dimensions Of The CVRS . . . . . . . . . . . 156 vii LIST OF FIGURES FIGURE PAGE 4.1 Mean Gain Scores For Groups 1 And 2 Over Three Measures During Training . . . . . . . 124 4.2 Scattergram Of The Individual Gain Score Means Obtained On The ASS Over Time (Mid-Pre) Between Groups 1 And 2 During Training . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 125 4.3 Graphic Representation Of Group Raw Score Means On The Five Subscales Under Coached Client Condition Between Groups 1 And 2 Over Time. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 132-3 4.4 Graphic Representation Of Group Raw Score Means On The Five Subscales Under Role- Play Condition Between Groups 1 And 2 Over Time. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 139-40 viii APPENDIX LIST OF APPENDICES IPR Counselor Verbal Response Scale And Manual For Training Judges In Use Of Counselor Verbal Response Scale Empathic Understanding In Interpersonal Processes Scale Description Of Role, Function, And Train- ing Of Interrogators. Sample Typescript IPR Simulation Films . Description Of Experimental Group ”Curri- culum . . . . Training Schedules And Syllibus For Pro- cedures In Counseling Course. . . . . . Rationale And Training For Coached Clients Intercorrelations Of Gain Scores On The ASS For Groups 1 And 2 Over Time Inter- vals 1 And 2. PAGE 196 212 217 223 226 243 247 254 CHAPTER I STATEMENT OF THE PROBLEM Because of the complexity of our society, and the changing nature of social, economic, technological and polit- ical variables, the field of counseling is continually pres- sured to modify and upgrade its services to remain relevant and helpful to the needs of the constituency it serves. Individuals may be employed to perform counseling functions without having attained a level of proficiency sufficiently high to help effect personal growth in clients. The efficacy of counseling and therapy was always in question. Eysenck (1952) documented the lack of evidence in support of coun- seling and therapy services, and other researchers (Arbuckle, 1968; Bergin, 1963, 1967; Cartwright and Vogel, 1960; Fair- weather g5, a1,, 1960; Truax, 1963) have supported Eysenck's premise that, rather than counseling and therapy doing at worst no harm, apparently some counselors and therapists produced negative changes in clients. Truax (1963) also pointed out: "Since we know that counseling on the average is not more effective than no counseling, that some counselors and in- deed some clinics, counseling centers and other units are therapeutic, while other counselors, clinics and counsel- ing centers are in fact, on the average, harmful, the field of counseling itself has an ethical responsibility in the identification of participants and in- gredients in therapeutic versus psychono- xious counseling." (p. 213) Truax's statement suggests the following formulation of the problem: "How can counseling and therapy services become more relevant and facilitative in the product it dispenses?" If the responsibility for providing facilitative, help- ing services falls on the practicing counselors, it must also rest on those institutions preparing these professional coun- selors. However, institutions preparing counselors have failed to demonstrate their ability to produce effective, helping counselors. Berenson (1967, p. 7) stated "...there are no professional training programs which demonstrated their efficacy in terms of translation to constructive behavioral gains in clients." Task groups (Whiteley, 1966), concerned with the need to refocus research and training in counseling and counselor edu- cation, have suggested that in order for training programs to become more relevant to counselor education, and successful in training effective counselors, a balance between existing knowledge of affective and cognitive components in counseling must be adequately achieved in training programs, using coun- seling developmental tasks as an important vehicle for counselor education. The literature on counselor education contains suggestions that counselor education programs must address themselves to the development of effective training programs, the problems inherent in training and supervision in counselor education, and objective assessments of student progress through train- ing programs. In view of the need to improve results of coun- seling, the question of effectiveness in counseling must be considered, and from it a program of training and supervision for producing successful versus unsuccessful counselors needs to be developed and evaluated. Purpose of the Study The purpose of the present study is to evaluate the effectiveness of a model of counselor education in which affective and cognitive elements of counseling are combined in a developmental sequence. More Specifically, this study will evaluate the effectiveness of a counselor education "course” in which neophyte counselors1 experience develop- mental tasks in discrimination and communication skills, designed to make them better counselors. The developmental tasks in discrimination focus on becoming more sensitive to and aware of their own dynamics, client dynamics, and the relationship established between the counselor-trainees and their "clients". Tasks in the development of communication skills are designed to attune trainees to different ways of responding, taking the discriminations they have made and translating them into helpful counseling behaviors through concrete communications. 1 No one had as yet had a formal counseling practicum and all had only a few courses in counseling. The effects of two types of counselor education exper- iences on the subsequent behavior of the trainees will be tested. One approach, the typical classroom approach to counselor education is the traditional method of conducting the ”course", while the use of a developmental series of tasks using the Interpersonal Process Recall (IPR) techniques is innovative and has only recently been developed. The question constituting the major focus of the present study is whether there is any difference in the counseling behaviors of neophyte trainees exposed to traditional classroom proce- dures versus a program using developmental tasks in discrimin- ation and communication (implemented by IPR). Need for the Study, Of late, a growing interest has been shown in the nature of supervised counseling experiences during training. Although the need for supervised training experiences has been noted since the time of Freud (Jones, 1959), there has been little agreement as to the basic method, content and structure of this experience (Roeber, 1962)., For example, efforts have been made by counselor educators to develop training programs based on a distinct counseling vieWpoint, emphasizing cogni- tive-didactic input, theoretical, empirical, and pragmatic elements gr affective-experiential, intuitive, self-discovery and self-exploratory elements. For instance, Levy (1968) indicated that effective counselor education emphasizes imagination, cognitive flexibility, and avoidance of "pre- mature closure in the realm of ideas”, but Gibbs (1968) indicated that an effective program of counselor education stresses a quasi-therapeutic program built on on-going T-group and related self-discovery experiences. In addition to this lack of agreement, there have been relatively few objective assessments of students' progress through training programs (Carkhuff, 1966; Cash and Munger, 1966; Moss, Riegel, and Schaeffer, 1962; Schmidt and Pepinsky, 1965). In view of the stress placed on counselor education pro- grams to find a successful blending of ingredients, to meet the demands for bigger and better programs (KlOpf and Cohen, 1962; Munger, Brown, and Needham, 1964; Ross, 1963), it is important to continually investigate and evaluate both traditional and newer training procedures in an attempt to find effective approaches for educating counselor candidates. Theoretical Framework Because the purpose of the present study is to develop and evaluate a model for training counselors, it is necessary to identify first those elements which should be included in the model (to "put the horse before the cart", it is first important to find out what skills you want candidates to develop); from this knowledge, an attempt to devise a training program for the acquistion of these skills can be made. The first portion of this section establishes guidelines for the development of the counselor education model. These guidelines will be followed in the development of the theory and the identification of the components of the theory. In the second portion of this section an attempt is made to identify the elements which will be included in the train- ing model presented in this study. Two models of interper- sonal facilitation-counseling and individual counselor supervision-are examined to identify counselor and supervisor skills which lead to successful outcome in each respective process. Once these skills and conditions are identified, a training model can be developed to include these findings, attempting to teach counselor candidates to develop the skills which successful counselors and supervisors possess. In the third portion of this section an attempt is made to construct a comprehensive training model for counselor education. This model is based on the assumption that effec- tive counseling behaviors involve discrimination skills and communication skills. The program is implemented by struc- turing a series of developmental tasks in discrimination and communication which demonstrates a logical learning sequence for the acquisition of the skills for becoming an effective counselor. The final portion of this section is a discussion of how to implement the program of developmental tasks most effectively. The use of "machines" such as videotape and audiotape recorders, films and television are considered, and the relevance of the Interpersonal Process Recall technique is discussed. Guidelines for the Counselor Education Model The following concepts serve as the guidelines for the counselor education model: 1. One guideline is that the formulation should re- present a broad spectrum to include whatever "truths" can be gleaned from a wide range of philosophies and vieWpoints; it should not be un- necessarily limited by selecting only "truths" from the orientation of one particular school of counseling, to the exclusion of all others. 2. Another guideline is that the formulation should consist of teachable concepts. 3. Another guideline is that the formulation should clearly demonstrate a logical learning sequence for the student which progresses from where a student is likely to be at the start of training (in terms of counseling skills) to the levels and skills he will need in order to be an effective counselor. 4. Another guideline is that the formulation should clarify the distinction between what is counseling and what is not counseling. 5. Another guideline is that the formulation should be clearly defined so that replication is possible. 6. Another guideline is that the formulation should permit the addition of new elements as new data or observations are obtained. The theoretical framework for the present development of the program for training counselors will attempt to follow these guidelines to synthesize a number of concepts from research, inference, experience, and knowledge in the areas of coun- seling and counselor training. An Examination of Two Interpersonal Processes 1. Model of Growth in Counseling The relationship established between the counselor and client i3 counseling can expedite 9£_inhibit client growth and change. Counseling can be conceptualized as an inter- personal process between two (or more) persons, one indiv- idual presenting problems in development, adjustment, and/or coping in whom growth and change is desired (client), the other being a professionally trained individual who offers the conditions of helping to facilitate growth, self-explor- ation, and change (counselor). The importance of this rela- tionship between client and counselor is documented in the research literature (Foreman, 1967; Hobbs, 1962; Kell and Mueller, 1966; Krumboltz, 1966; Rogers, 1967; Sullivan, 1953; Ullman and Krasner, 1964; Whiteley, 1966). According to Hobbs (1962), counseling which leads to client gain (growth and change) is characterized by the extent that a facilita- tive relationship is established between counselor and client. A counselor who is facilitative can work to establish a rela- tionship with the client which can enable the client to work towards realizing increased self-understanding and self- experiencing; this exploration and increased experimentation with alternative ways of functioning can lead to more satis- factory ways of behaving. Hobbs further asserts that the counselor, by helping to create an atmosphere in which the client can explore and discover, provides an opportunity for the client to experience closeness to another human being without getting hurt, to relieve anxiety from those symbols associated with negative and traumatic experiences, to learn not to need neurotic distortions, to practice being respon- sible for himself, and to clarify an old or learn a new way of coping in his world (Hobbs, 1962). Further, Patterson (1966), in reviewing a number of theories of counseling (ra- tional, psychoanalytic, learning theory, perceptual-phenomeno- logical, existentialist psychotherapy) concluded: "Thus all approaches seem to deal with clients who are in need of help, recognize this need, believe they can change, believe that the counselor can help them change, and engage in some activity in the attempt to change...a11 approaches, then, appear to involve a relationship between a coun- selor and client, each of whom contributes to the relationship certain characteris- tics which lead to client change...evidence seems to be accumulating that the effec- tive element in counseling is the nature of the relationship established by the counselor." (p. 500) It can be seen that the counselor can have a profound impact on the amount of client growth and change by the relationship 10 he establishes with the client. The type gf_relationship the counselor establishes with 9f_elements. A core of facilitative conditions has been identified which seem to be common elements to all helping relationships. These are elements which the counselor must possess to establish a helpful relationship with the client, which enhances client exploration, discovery, and change (Berenson and Carkhuff, 1967; Carkhuff, 1967; Pierce, Cark- huff, and Berenson, 1967; Rogers, 1951; Truax and Carkhuff, 1967). These conditions are: 1. the facilitators must communicate an accurately empathic understanding of the deeper as well as surface feelings of the client (abbreviated as HEN) . 2. the facilitators must communicate a very deep respect for the other persons' worth as an individ- ual ("R"). 3. the facilitators must be freely and deeply them— selves in a non-exploitative relationship (abbre- viated "G”). 4. the facilitators must be helpful in guiding the discussion of personally relevant feelings and experiences in specific and concrete terms ("C"). Results from a series of studies of individuals under- going counseling and therapy indicated that clients whose therapists offered a high level of unconditional positive regard, self-congruence or genuineness, and accurate emphathic understanding showed significant positive personality and behavioral change. Whitehorn and Betz (1954), in attempting 11 to account for the difference between successful and unsuc- cessful counselors and therapists, found that differences in counseling outcome were related to the counselors' attitudinal approach to the counseling situation. Success- ful counselors were seen as warm, and as attempting to understand the clients in personal, immediate and idio- syncratic ways. Less successful counselors tended to relate to clients in a more impersonal manner, focusing on pathology I and more external kinds of 'understanding". Halkides (1958) and Barret-Lenard (1962) investigated the importance of non-possessive warmth, the therapists maturity, genuiness, sensitivity and accuracy in understanding a clients' inner experiences, and concluded that these traits were relevant conditions for success in counseling. Significant client growth seemed to be a product of the depth to which clients engaged in self-exploration. Rogers (1962) reported that the therapist conditions of accurate empathy, unconditional positive regard, and thera- pist genuineness were related to the depth of client self- exploration, which was related to client growth. Truax and Carkhuff (1967) also tested the experimental question of whether client depth of self-exploration, which was determined by the level of conditions offered by the therapist (they varied accurate empathy and unconditional positive regard). They varied the pre- 12 presentation of these conditions over parts of the interviews and found that when the therapists successfully lowered the level of conditions of accurate empathy and unconditional positive regard, there was a corresponding drop in the depth of client self-exploration; when the conditions were raised, there was a return to high self-exploration. These results were taken to suggest a causal relationship between the levels of conditions offered by the therapist and the depth to which self-exploration and discovery occurs in clients. Ih§_dimensions gf_warmth and respect, accurate empathy, genuineness, and concreteness ig_the counselor have been related §2_the establishment 9f_facilitative relationships tive client gain when these conditions are present and deterioration in_clients when absent for individual (Rogers, 1962; Truax, 1963; Truax and Carkhuff, 1964) a§d_ggggp_ therapy (Truax, 1961; Truax, Carkhuff and Kodman, 1966). Not only must the facilitative counselor accurately understand, empathize, and care about the client (in a sense, be able to discriminate and differentiate stimuli), the facilitative counselor must also be able to let the client know that he accurately understands and empathizes with him, cares for him, and genuinely is concerned about him (in a sense, to 13 communicate in a concrete way). As such, the conditions of accurate empathy, genuineness, regard, and concreteness are traits which seem to be related to skills which enable the counselor to discriminate and communicate in facilitative ways. Discrimination skills involve perceptual behaviors and communication skills involve overt, interactional behav- iors. Discrimination skills involve recognition, understand- ing, and an accurate empathy of the surface as well as deeper meanings of behaviors, including being sensitive to and aware of the cognitive and affective frames of reference of oneself and of clients. Communication skills involve responding along certain behavioral dimensions which can lead to growth or retarda- tion in relationships. Facilitative counselor communication entails translating the discriminations of client behaviors (along verbal, postural, tonal, and gestural dimensions) into concrete and specific terms so the client concerns and behaviors can more clearly and easily be understood in light of the client's problems. The "knowledge" of what consti- tutes facilitative communication has been derived from research evidence, inference, deducation, and experience, l A description of these behavioral dimensions can be found be referring to the Counselor Verbal Response Scale in Appendix A, and by referring to the Empathic Understanding Scale in Appendix B. 14 and this knowledge and skill can be cognitively taught (Carkhuff, 1966, 1968; Martin and Carkhuff, 1968; Pierce, Carkhuff and Berenson, 1967; Truax, Carkhuff, and Douds, 1964). In retrospect, a counselor who can establish a relation- ship which is facilitative for clients is able to differen- tiate and discriminate what the client's concern(s) is 32d is able to translate these discriminations into effective and helpful counseling behaviors by communicating with clients in a concrete and clear manner. 2. Model of Growth in Counselor Supervision Counseling and psychotherapy apg_their training pgp: cedures can pg_looked upon as instances p£_all interpersonal processes. Those instances which can be constructive or destructive in human relationships have the same effect in counselor and therapy, and can be shown to have the same types of effects in counselor training. It is proposed that in counselor training, the relationship between the trainee and his supervisor can expedite or inhibit the trainee from developing those intrapersonal and interpersonal skills required for effective counseling, just as the quality of the relationship in counseling can influence the development of intrapersonal and interpersonal skills in clients leading to growth and change. Counselor education and supervision of counselor trainees 15 share many commonalities with the process pf counseling. Supervision and training of counselors are instances of inter- personal relationships sharing many similarities with the counseling relationship. If the counselor offers high levels of interpersonal facilitation encompassing the condi- tions of accurate empathy, regard, genuineness, and con- creteness, a relationship can be established in which the client may feel free to explore and discover new ways of perceiving and responding without fear of reprisal. The effective supervisor can also create an atmosphere in which the trainee is not threatened, but is accepted, understood, and respected, so he is free to analyze and explore his re- lationships with clients. According to Kell and Mueller (1966); "The good supervisor, we believe, combines in his own person those qualities which cause others to believe that he can control, if necessary, and also that he is emotion- ally supportive of exploration, experimen- tation, and learning for its own sake. Further, the good supervisor does not or- dinarily experience these qualities in himself as being emotionally competitive or antithetical." (p. 99) The conditions which appear to be conducive for the establishment of a facilitative relationship and client gain in counseling appear to be the conditions of super- vision which achieve desired results in trainee growth. The presence of a core of facilitative conditions in the supervisor can free the trainee to engage in self-explora- 16 tion, experimentation, and discovery of ways he can behave constructively and helpfully. Recently, Carkhuff (1969) reviewed 16 studies dealing with mean levels of overall functioning1 of beginning and advanced trainees and their trainers, and concluded that: "...trainer level of functioning appears most critical with trainees moving in their functioning in the direction of their trainers. Trainees tend to gain the most with high level trainers and lose the most or terminate with low level trainers when the discrepancies between trainer and trainee level are greatest. Those programs proving most effective were those concentrating systematically and behaviorally upon the facilitative and action oriented dimensions." (p. 238) The level of functioning of the counselor trainer and the quality of the relationship established by him (based on the presence or absence of the core of facilitative conditions) can expedite or inhibit trainee growth as a person and counselor. If significant client growth is due to the depth of self-exploration engaged in by the client, it should also follow that the trainee will experience a deeper understand- ing of his relationships with clients if he has a supervisor Level of functioning was defined according to the de- gree of presence or absence of the core of conditions of empathy, regard, genuineness, and concreteness. 17 who can enable him to explore and discover who he is, what effects he has in his relationships with clients and other people, and how his perceptions influence his thoughts, feel- ings, and behaviors. The deeper the trainee can explore, the more fully he can (a) come to understand his own needs, (b) understand client needs, (c) differentiate his own dy- namics from those of others, and (d) achieve a more profound understanding of the bilateral nature of his relationships with his clients. In sum, the supervisors who offer high levels of the core of facilitative conditions can enable trainees to SE? plppg and discover new ways of behaving and also didactically Eggpp_trainees gppp£_effective ways of behaving, and can provide a role mpdgl of a person who can live effectively. The process of the development of counseling skills in trainees can therefore be shown to involve experiential and cognitive learning. Experiential learning emphasizes exploration and dis- covery, where the trainees internalize, experience, and under- stand the meaning of the ”feeling" qualities of their rela- tionships; growth as a person and as a counselor can be realized from this new-found understanding (Altucher, 1967; Gysbers, 1964; Hansen and Barker, 1964; Kell and Mueller, 1966; Lister, 1966; Orton, 1965; Peters and Hansen, 1963). Experiential learning focuses on examining certain perceptual 18 and attitudinal conditions in trainees based on the belief that trainee growth will result from discovery and exploring of these experiences. The trainees can come to recognize themselves as beings with needs, emotions, and conflicts unique to themselves and different from others. At the same time, they can become more aware of, sensitive to and under- standing of the needs, emotions, and conflicts of others and can come to affectively understand why facial expressions, tonal quality, body attitudes and posture, and verbal and non-verbal behaviors are reflections of underlying feelings and emotions. In generalizing from trainees' own personal experiences, the flow is inductive. Because these learnings involve recognition, understanding, and an accurate empathy of the surface as well as deeper meanings of behaviors, exper- iential learning can be viewed as a process used to acquire discrimination skills. Cognitive learning refers in this discussion to cogni- tive input, teaching, guiding, and modeling behaviors, by which trainees can learn objectively derived and defined behavioral correlates of what is considered effective, facili- tative counseling practice (Bandura and Walters, 1966; Fleming, 1964; Korner and Brown, 1952; Moore, 1963). The trainees can be instructed in how to communicate with clients along certain dimensions, how to encourage clients to communicate with them, what facial expressions, tonal quality, body l9 attitudes and posture, and verbal and non-verbal behaviors mean in light of behaviors of oneself and clients, and whether these behaviors conflict and/or support one another. This approach entails a "passing down" of accumulated know- ledge to trainees; they incorporate these "established premises", from which they may deduce that there are a number of ways of behaving. Cognitive learning can be in- volved in the acquisiton of discrimination and communication skills. Discrimination skills can be sharpened by learning to identify certain types of behaviors which might have under- lying heightened emotionality. Communication skills can be sharpened by learning that there are certain dimensions and ways of responding which can communicate in a concrete way and certain ways of responding which are vague, non-specific and do not communicate clearly with clients. It has been demonstrated (Carkhuff, 1966, 1968; Martin and Carkhuff, 1968; Pierce, Carkhuff, and Berenson, 1967; Pierce and Schauble, 1969) that counselor candidates can be trained to effect significant constructive gains in clients following a training program emphasizing discrimina- tion and communication of those interpersonal skills related to individual growth and change. Trainees in counselor educa- tion can learn to develop discrimination and communication skills in themselves (with the assistance of "teachers” who can provide the conditions conducive to self-exploration and 20 the development of cognitive flexibility), which can lead to their becoming more facilitative counselors. Model of Counselor Education and Training The development of a comprehensive model of counselor education begins with the identification of the components which will be used to construct the program. It is postulated that counselor education should look to the processes of counseling and supervision, and attempt to teach trainees to develop those skills that have been found to lead to growth and change in clients and counselor candidates. The previous two portions of this section dealt with identifying certain skills which were possessed by both facilitating counselors and supervisors. In the portion dealing with a model of growth in counseling, the relationship established by the counselor, shown to directly effect the amount of growth a client may realize, was affected by the degree of the pre- sence or absence of discrimination and communication skills in the counselor. Similarly, in the portion dealing with a model of growth in individual counselor supervision, it was shown that the relationship established by the supervisor with the trainee, shown to be directly related to the develop- ment of counseling skills in the trainee, was also affected by the degree of the presence or absence of discrimination and communication skills in the supervisor. Because a core of conditions and skills has been identified 21 (involving skills in discrimination and communication) as components which are needed to establish facilitative rela- tionships, and since studies have shown that these conditions and skills can be taught and developed, it is posited that coun- selor candidates should be exposed to a series of experiences of different types which are designed to help them develop skills to discriminate and communicate so they in turn can establish facilitative relationships with their clients. The implementation of the counselor education model can be accomplished through the use of counseling develop- mental tasks. The concept of developmental tasks can pre- sently be used for constructing a logical learning sequence for the acquisition of discrimination and communication skills. This can be particularly relevant for training coun- selors because the students can progress from where they are likely to be at the outset of training in terms of skills possessed to the level of skill needed in order to be a successful counselor. The following tasks in the development of these skills face the trainees as they learn to be facilitative counselors: Discrimination Skills 1. The counselor-trainee must be aware of and sensitive 1This model is a modification of a design for counselor supervision developed by Goldberg (1967). 22 pp his owp feelings and communications during the inter- personal encounter. Basic to this premise is the belief that unless the counselor is keenly, confidently, and com- fortably aware of who pp_i§_and i§_becoming, he will not provide clients with the most effective experience. Appel (1963) states: "The most significant resource a counselor brings to a helping relationship is him- self. It is difficult to understand how a counselor unaware of his own emotional needs, of his expectations of himself as well as of others, of his rights and pri- vileges in relationships can be sensitive enough to such factors in his counselees." The beginning of a helpful counseling experience, there- fore, starts with the counselor or therapist. The thoughts and feelings the counselor has about himself are reflected in his thoughts and feelings about others; conversely, how he thinks and feels about others will reflect how he thinks and feels toward himself (Berenson and Carkhuff, 1967). In order to have a truly therapeutic effect on another indiv- idual, the counselor must have a profound depth of under- standing of his own fully experienced personal needs and conflicts so they do not intrude in a deleterious manner; above all else, the counselor must learn to trust his own motives. Carkhuff (1967, in Berenson and Carkhuff, 1967) feels that: 23 "The counselor must trust his own exper- ience, for in the end all that he has to offer the client is 100% of his own exper- ience...trusting his own motives and exper- ience will enable the counselor to be open to the feedback that he gets from his client. Trusting the feedback that he gets from his client will enable him to be shaped by what is effective for the client...the beginnings of potent practice, therefore, involves a self-experiencing and self-knowing counselor who is shaped by what is facilitative for his client.” (p. 5-6) To achieve this awareness and sentitivity, it is theorized that the trainee must remain open to his own experiences so as to understand his own emotional needs, expectations, and conflicts; this can make him more aware of the affective messages which emanate from clients. By knowing that it is permissible to talk about and have feelings, and knowing how profoundly they influence his own behavior, the trainee can come to respect others for their feelings and concerns, can more accurately empathize with them because he too has had related experiences, and can more clearly conceptualize client concerns in light of his own previous experiences. Rogers (1951) indicated that personal growth of the counselor-trainee should be given high priority in counselor education, and Landsman (1965) noted that existentialist focus on the "self" of the trainee and his enrichment in training shows great promise for coun- selor education. The model for counselor education should, 24 therefore, include experiences designed to enable the trainees to become as fully-functioning and experiencing individuals as possible. 2. The counselor-trainee must become aware pf.apd_ sensitive pp client feelings and communications. The coun- selor-trainee must become sensitive to, aware of, and under- standing of the affective themes and connotive implications, as well as the cognitive aspects of client concerns which permeate clients' behaviors. This involves the development and refinement of a critical perceptive ability in trainees of the ability to recognize the more subtle as well as blatant affective messages and their meanings emanating from clients, and an understanding of what these behaviors mean in terms of the clients' behaviors. It is further posited that the counselor-trainees must be able to identify clients' verbal and non-verbal cues and commentary (verbal, postural, gestural, tonal) during coun- seling. The role of "Interrogator" was deve10ped (Kagan, 1., 1969) to facilitate client self-analysis of under- pp. lying thoughts, feelings, images, expectations and general pattern of reacting; training in this skill is relevant to the development of counselor skills in identifying poten- tially important areas of concern for clients. Training 1Complete description of the training procedure and role Of interrogator can be found in Appendix D. 25 involves developing the trainees' sensitivity to specific types of cues which are of assistance in recognizing client communications which may be indicative of heightened under- lying emotionality (Some examples are seen in abrupt shifts in theme during interviews, change in voice level, tone or pace, use of vocabulary, shifts in body posture, change in visual focus and eye contact, and innappropriate affect). If one considers an accurate empathy and understanding of the surface as well as deeper meanings of client concerns to be important factors in the outcome of counseling and therapy, then this task is relevant to attempting to attune trainees to recognize these cues in client communications, and understand what they mean in the light of client concerns. 3. The counselor-gpainee must become sensitive pp_§pg_ aware p£_the bilateral nature pf_the counseling interaction and the mutual impact between counselor and client. The counselor-trainee can become a more potent agent of change for clients by recognizing and differentiating his own feel- ings and conflicts from those of the client, and by under- standing that he establishes a relationship with clients which has a reciprocal impact on his behavior and the behavior of the clients. According to Goldberg (1967): 26 "...it becomes necessary for the trainee to be aware of his impact and influence and to understand the meaning of his interaction with the client. Perhaps, even more important, however, the trainee needs practice at examining his rela- tionship with the client using the coun- seling session itself and the client/ counselor interaction as both subject and object of the counseling process.” (p- 19) It is posited that because the trainee has certain reactions, feelings and thoughts, and clients have certain reactions, feelings and thoughts, there is more than likely going to be an interaction between some of these elements. The more fully the trainee examines his relation- ships with clients, the more fully can be come to understand that he begins to hold a Special meaning for the clients, in a sense, a model and social reinforcer for the client. Further, the client also holds a meaning for the counselor, and the counselor will react to various client behaviors based on his own previous experiences. Often these reactions will be facilitating, but they can also serve to inhibit clients from exploring further. It is this dynamic inter- action which can be examined, and the counselor-trainee can learn those behaviors which will be effective for his clients. Communication Skills The counselor-trainee mpst become increasingly aware pf_ elements p£_effective communication ip_counseling. Truax, 27 Carkhuff and Douds (1964) suggested that trainees be made aware of the relevant dimensions of counseling by presenting the trainees with a model rated high in the therapist Offered core of conditions. By presenting trainees with a model of communication skills which involve responding behaviorally along concrete dimensions (facilitating versus non-facilitating), the trainees can learn what constitutes an effective, helpful response; the trainees can practice making and rating their own responses and interactions (and those of others) along the identified dimensions and under various circumstances. Certain types of responses and dimensions of reSponding can facilitate client growth, inhibit or negate growth, or result in impasses in counseling. Research by Kagan pp, p1,, (1967) led to the identification of certain dimensions of responses which led to the development of the IPR Counselor Verbal Response Scale (C.V.R.S.),1 which was an attempt to describe counselor statements and responses to client com- munications on four dichotomized dimensions: 1. affective-cognitive 2. understanding-nonunderstanding 3. specific-nonspecific 4. exploratory-nonexploratory 1The C.V.R.S. is described fully in Appendix A. 28 These dimensions deal with types of responses which can be made by counselors, but not with the quality of the re- sponses. The effectiveness of counselor responses depends also on the level at which they were communicated by the counselor. Research by Carkhuff and Berenson (1967) and Truax and Carkhuff (1967) has led to the identification of five levels of reSponse at which counselor communications can be made; the level of the response directly influences the effectiveness of The five levels Level 1 Level 2 Level 3 Level 4 Level 5 the counselor-client interaction. 1 are: The verbal and behavioral expressions of the counselor either do not attend to or detract significantly from those of the second person. The counselors response subtracts noticeable affect from the client communications. The counselors responses are essen- tially interchangeable with the clients'. The counselors responses add noticeably to client expressions and communications. The counselors responses add signifi- cantly to client expressions and com- munications. Implementation of a Model for Counselor Education and Training The use of "machines," such as audio and video-tape re- corders, films and film projectors, and closed circuit tele- vision has begun to demonstrate its applicability to the complex processes of counseling and counselor preparation. l The E.U.S. is described fully in Appendix B. 29 The Interpersonal Process Recall (IPR) technique, deve10ped by Kagan pg, 31. (1967) using videotaping and simulation film materials seems to have relevance as a means of imple- menting training procedures. Videotaping the counseling session, using films to simulate personal encounters, and replaying the video-recording of these encounters for trainees is intended to capture the maximum number of cues (verbal and non-verbal) for trainees to examine, explore, and understand. The verbal and non- verbal data is available for immediate playback and recall, under the guidance of a clinical interrogator especially trained to focus on the salient features of the on-going relationship, dynamics in the interview, and personal re- actions to the simulated materials, reproduced on videotape. The IPR techniques seem to have the following applica- tions for counselor educations: 1. Use of simulation materials affords the oppor- tunity to create situations in a laboratory setting, designed to make trainees more aware of and sensi- tive to their own dynamics and ways of reacting, and to the dynamics and ways of reacting of other individuals, is specific instances and circumstances. 2. Use of videotape recall affords the opportunity for the trainee to see himself and clients in a new light, and to explore attitudes which influence and effect counseling behavior; this maximizes learning by providing immediate feedback to the trainees. 3. Use of IPR techniques can afford the opportunity to focus on essential components of counselor-client communication. 30 Use of the role of interrogator, training in the role of interrogator, and serving as an interro- gator builds skills which have direct application in counseling. The IPR techniques therefore seem to be an appropriate vehicle for the development of discrimination and communication skills in counselor-trainees. Delimitations of the Study Generalizations of the results of the present study are delimited by the following: 1. There is no attempt to obtain a geographic or historic sampling of students. The nature of the group used in the present study may not be representative of groups in counselor education at another point in time at the same institution, or of a group in counselor education at another institution. No attempt is made to seek a stratified sample or to in other ways control all possible relevant characteristics of the student counselor sample. The sample of individuals comprising the group may vary along dimensions not accounted for in the present study: control for sex of student differences in personality types differential rates and ways of learning different experiences and background different reasons for taking the program differential level of involvement and com- mitment to the treatment procedures. "dl‘dUOtfiP The level of skill and ability to function effectively as a trainer, supervisor, and interrogator may vary among different individuals, deSpite attempts to control for consistency. Coached clients are used instead of "real” clients. The coached clients may be functioning in a dif- ferent and inconsistent fashion over trainees, 31 despite training and practice aimed at achieving some consistency Of performance. 5. Ability to be effective with the coached clients is not identical to ability to function with real clients. Definition of Terms Special terms, used in this study, are defined as follows: Interpersonal Process Recall (IPR): The term used to describe the technique of video-recording of counselor trainees' personal interactions (to simulation materials) and interpersonal interactions (e.g. counseling interviews) and then playing back the videotape to enable the parti- cipants to examine their intrapersonal and interpersonal dynamics in the original experiences. Stimulated Recall: The phase of IPR where videotape is replayed to trainees(s) and a clinical "interrogator" helps to stimulate trainee(s) examination of the underlying thoughts and feelings during the recorded session. Interrogator: The label given to a third person (coun- selor, therapist) whose function is to facilitate trainee(s) self-analysis of thoughts, feelings and reactions to filmed simulation materials and interpersonal encounters in train- ing; the term "interrogator" was selected because this per- sons clinical function is limited to actively probing the trainees' immediate reactions. 32 Affect Simulation: The technique of using films designed to simulate various kinds and degrees of intensity of emotional states. The films are designed to confront individuals viewing them with various interpersonal stress situations. The reactions to these situations can be examined in order to develop understanding and awareness of typical ways of responding to these stressful situations, and for identifying new and possibly more satisfying ways of responding to the interpersonal situations depicted on the films. Facilitative: "...that which frees the individual to attain higher and more personally rewarding levels of intra- personal and interpersonal functioning" (Berenson and Cark- huff, 1967). (also helping, effective, successful) Core of Facilitating Conditions: "...to denote those counselor dimensions of attitude and sensitivity that create a therapeutic atmosphere enabling the client to relate constructively to the counselor and to himself within the therapeutic encounter” (Berenson and Carkhuff, 1967). In this study, the conditions of accurate empathy, genuineness, regard, and concreteness are components of this core of conditions. Cognitive: The term used to refer to didactic (intell- ectual, reasoning and thinking) behaviors which involve in- formation gathering and evaluative processes. 33 Affective: The term used to refer to experiential (emotional) behaviors involving exploration, discovery, and understanding of "feelings". Assumptions 1. Changes in counselor trainees' affective sensitivity can be measured and changes in affective sensitivity can be determined. 2. A counselor trainee's within-interview behavior can be measured and changes in interview behavior determined. 3. The rating of 20 consecutive counselor responses, taken from the middle segments of an interview will be a re- presentative sample of counselor trainees' performance throughout the interview. Hypotheses An experimental research design was formulated which would permit the testing of the following general hypothesis: A program using specific developmental tasks in interpersonal discrimination and communication, employing stimulated video- taped-recall sessions and affective simu- lation materials (IPR) will be more effec- tive in bringing about changes in trainees' interview behavior on discrimination and communication dimensions than will trainees receiving a more traditional classroom approach to counselor education. More specifically: l. Trainees exposed to a program of specific develop- mental tasks in counselor education, implemented by using IPR techniques, will develop greater ability to discriminate (and differentiate) affec- tive stimuli and conditions than trainees exposed 34 to a more traditional classroom approach to counselor education. 2. Trainees exposed to a program of specific develop- mental tasks in counselor education, implemented by using IPR techniques, will develop greater ability to communicate in a facilitative way than trainees exposed to a more traditional classroom approach to counselor education. 91w There is a need for counselor education programs to provide relevant experiences to the process of becoming a facilitative counselor. In the present study, an attempt is made to delineate a series of developmental tasks in dis- crimination and communication skills using IPR techniques as a vehicle for the training model. Chapter II will con- tain a review of the literature focusing on traditional and more innovative, technological approaches to counselor supervision and training (including research using IPR). Chapter III offers a detailed paradigm for a model program designed to train individuals to be facilitative counselors. Chapter IV will contain an analysis of the data generated from the study. The relevance of the outcomes of this approach to counselor education is presented in Chapter V. CHAPTER 11 REVIEW OF LITERATURE Introduction Included in the first chapter was the identification of a number of skills which training programs in counselor educa- tion should seek to develop in counselor candidates. The review of literature in this chapter will not retrace related literature about this material, but will focus on both traditional and some of the more innovative approaches for teaching trainees to develop counseling skills. This chapter contains reviews of: (a) traditional approaches to supervision and training, including the role of didactic input, role playing, the counseling practicum and adjunct group counseling experiences, and an integration of didactic and experiential elements in training, (b) technological- innovative approaches to supervision and training using electronic devices, beginning with the use of audio-tapes and progressing to more recent innovative devices such as videotapes and simulation materials, and (c) the application of three Specific approaches to training and supervision (Microcounseling, Video-Simulation, and Interpersonal Pro- cess Recall). 35 36 Traditiongl Approaches To Sppervision and Trainipg Overview It has long been believed (Foreman, 1967; Gysbers, 1964; Hansen and Barker, 1964; Lister, 1966; Orton, 1965; Patterson, 1964; Peters and Hansen, 1963; Truax, Carkhuff, and Douds, 1964) that self-awareness and self-understanding are critical components of the skills which counselor candi- dates should develop. Others who share this belief (Appel, 1963; Arbuckle and Wicas, 1957; Arnold, 1962; ACES Standards, 1967; Lister, 1964; McKinnon, 1969) indicated that this understanding can help trainees find a fuller acceptance of their clients. While this importance of self-understanding for trainees is accepted by many counselor educators, how this understanding should be achieved is not agreed upon with nearly as much unanimity (Bonney and Gazda, 1966). A number of people have noted that while programs provide both didactic and experiential training experiences, there is a growing concern that possibly this integration is not accomplished often enough or successfully enough (Chennault, 1964; Landsman, 1963; Lister, 1964; Perronne and Sanborn, 1966). There, therefore, appears considerable support in the literature for the premise that for a program in supervision to accomplish its goals, it should provide a successful blend of didactic and experiential tasks which could lead 37 to the development of whatever skills one wishes trainees to possess. The following is a review of a number of components which are usually included in a counselor training program: A. Didactic Input Although many counselor educators would agree that knowledge of counseling theory is a necessary part of achieving awareness and becoming a counselor (Brammer, 1966), there appears to be a question as to its place in training. Brammer's (1966) point of view regarding didactic (cognitive) input seems to represent the position which advocates the necessity of the didactic facet of training. He noted that exploration and understanding of various theories and approaches to counseling can serve to help the counselor conceptualize what he is doing so he can examine his behaviors critically in light of his assumptions about clients' behaviors and needs. This didactic input can help trainees cognitively understand their behaviors, and develop a frame of reference for thinking of their behaviors, client behaviors, and the way each person in the relationship can interact and influence each other. A possible criticism of Brammer's point is that he seemed to emphasize the role of theoretical material almost to the exclusion of practical and applied (discovery) types of experiences. 38 B. Role Playing, A technique relating theory with practice has been the use of role playing (Sperle, 1933) to introduce trainees to interviewing procedures, to develop a "feel" for the nature of the counseling relationship (Hoppock, 1949; Rogers, 1951; Wrenn, 1962), to illustrate and stimulate student discussion of the counseling process (Bridgewater and Cookston, 1952), as a means of increasing students' awareness of non-verbal components in counseling (Thompson and Bradway, 1950), and to increase counselor skills by helping students grasp the understanding of the relationship between their internal and external behaviors (Lister, 1966). According to its advocates, using role playing procedures enabled trainees to analyze and understand many facets of the problems they might be faced with in counseling and their reactions to these problems without having to assume respon- sibility for "damaging” a real client. The argument continues that trainees can also experiment with new and different ways of responding in the relative safety of a laboratory setting. Although role playing seems to many to be a useful method for simulating counseling experiences, limitations were also noted. Roark (1969), in an attempt to study the influence of training on counselor responses in actual and role playing interviews, concluded that there was a difference 39 in response patterns between (typescripts of) role playing and actual interviews. One possible explanation he gave was that role playing situations are somewhat different from actual counseling encounters since they closely approxi- mate but do not actually duplicate the true counseling experience. In any event, while differences were found, he did not consider them of the type or magnitude to contra- indicate the use of role playing in research and training. However, Roark did note that generalizations from role playing to actual counseling situations should be made with caution. C. The CounselingiPracticum and Adjunct Group Experiences Actual client contact is considered by a number of counselor educators to be the most effective and appropriate way of realistically training counselors (Landy, 1953; Black, 1953; Kirk, 1955). According to Haseley and Peters (1966), the counseling practicum where trainees are assigned real clients and fieldwork experiences, seemed to provide opportunities for studying the counseling process as it occurred with trainees directly involved in the process. Because the (on-campus and off-campus) practicum involves counseling with real clients, it has been referred to as the "heart of counselor preparation" (Hansen and Moore, 1966; Haseley and Peters, 1966). 40 The use of group counseling as an adjunct experience to the practicum has recently received much attention (Bonney and Gazda, 1966; Dreikurs and Sonstegard, 1966; Foreman, Poppen, and Frost, 1967; Lister, 1966; Meek and Parker, 1966), founded on the assumption that the group experience can help trainees achieve the self-understanding needed to be a successful counselor. Gazda and Ohlsen (1961) indicated the following benefits which a group counseling experience could provide for trainees: 1. to discover that Others have problems like themselves, which can be helped by counseling 2. to extend their knowledge of human behavior and understand the behavior of others 3. to observe a qualified counselor at work who can serve as a model for effective practice with clients 4. to practice the role of "co-therapist" to help others. Although group counseling has received support as an adjunct practicum experience, Betz (1963) reviewed the literature and concluded that there have been few controlled studies favorably evaluating the impact of group counseling on trainees and that little experimental evidence existed to demonstrate the value of group techniques in counselor training. As a result of this review, he undertook to evaluate the use of group procedures in training. Spec- ifically, he took 30 NDEA counselors and attempted to 41 ascertain whether an affectively oriented or cognitively oriented group counseling approach would have a greater impact on these counselor trainees. The affective group focused on feeling tone and emotional material produced by the group; the cognitive group focused on cognitive material and informational needs of the group. Betz found that after 14 hours of group experience, trainees exposed to the affective group experience gained in ability to respond to clients' affect when they counselled individually and inde- pendently of the group; trainees in the cognitive group did not. Betz concluded from this study that an affective group counseling experience can have value in counselor training since trainees became more affective in their approach to clients as they became more aware of their own feelings. However, despite Betz's findings, there has been a lack of consistently significant results using a group procedure as an adjunct to the practicum (McKinnon, 1969; Wirt, Betz, and Engle, 1969). These findings can mean that group counseling does not always favorably effect outcome measures. In attempting to explain these findings, certain consider- ations might be kept in mind. For example, these incon- sistencies may not necessarily mean that group counseling is not an effective adjunct experience. Betz (1963) did achieve significant results in an affectively oriented group experience which covered a fairly long period of time. This might 42 imply that an adjunct group approach in training may require a long enough period of time to allow for the impact of these experiences to become integrated and translated into behavioral changes. Further, it should be pointed out that a number of studies did achieve positive results. These studies may need to be replicated, controlling for consis- tency within studies (using large samples, tight designs, and longer periods of time) and across studies (length of time and type of treatment) to improve the credibility of results (Campbell and Stanley, 1963). Finally, the level of functioning of the supervisor, which has been shown (in Chapter I) to be directly and consistently related to the amount of growth in clients and trainees, may have to be more closely measured and controlled to finally conclude that supervisors can be effective agents of change. D. Integrated Didactic and Experiential Elements in Training Carkhuff (1966) has noted that while a number of counselor training programs advocate the direct experiential involvement of trainees in supervision (Arbuckle, 1963; Patterson, 1964; University of Minnesota Counselor Education Staff, 1960), none have researched the process or outcomes of their programs. As a result of this lack of empirical validation, he found that there has been a trend away from programs of supervision advocating only experiential exper- iences toward more balanced, "eclectic", integrative programs, 43 which focus on three sources of learning which operate in counselor training: (1) the didactic, or direct shaping of behavior; (2) the experiential base of learning; and (3) the role model for effective counseling established by the trainer (Carkhuff, 1966). Truax, Carkhuff and Douds (1964) suggested that approaches to supervision should com- bine didactic and experiential elements into one model, which entailed shaping trainees' behaviors with quasi-therapeutic activities. They found the benefits of this combination to be threefold: l. trainees could experience an analog of therapy 2. trainees could receive a role model of effective counseling from supervisors 3. trainees had Opportunity for self-exploration. Using an integrated approach to supervision, they found that trainees could be brought to levels of interpersonal func- tioning comparable to experiences professionals, and clients of these trainees evidenced greater constructive behavioral change than a comparable control group after 100 hours of training. However, attempts to replicate these findings in shorter periods of time (20 hours; Berenson, Carkhuff, and Myrus, 1966) have resulted in failure to reach the same level of functioning achieved in the longer programs. The inconsistent results obtained in the studies re- viewed so far give rise to a number of questions and invite speculation concerning the length and depth of these experiences. 44 One might be led to postulate that a reason for the inability to replicate these results in shorter periods of time, and for the inconsistent support for group counseling experiences might be the depth at which learning occurred. Longer exposures to treatment techniques may have the advantage of allowing for the integration of new learning, building upon this learning, proceeding to deeper and more intense levels of experiencing and understanding. Shorter exposures could result in integration of experiences at a more superficial level of understanding. Thus, based on particular approaches to training, a longer period of time may be necessary to realize the development of counseling skills in trainees using particular teaching-learning models. On the other hand, the length of time may not be as important a variable as the intensity of the treatment pro- cedure. Traditional means of implementing and operationalizing training programs may lack the potency and impact to acceler- ate the learning process. Longer periods of time may be necessary simply because a particular techniques does not allow for the process of discovery and integration of learning to occur more rapidly. This points up the need for studies seeking to identify those types of training proce- dures which can result in the quickest and most effective learning, based on the task at hand. One study is relevant. Reddy (1968) attempted to identify which types of situations 45 can produce the most effective learning. He examined the differential effects of immediate and delayed feedback of knowledge of results in the learning of empathy in counselor training, and found that trainees learned most effectively and efficiently when immediate knowledge of results was given. He concluded by noting the importance of the need to consider and evaluate different methods of teaching and learning in an attempt to discover the most relevant methods for training. Technological (and Innovative),Approaches To Supervision and Training The use of technological devices has begun to be applied to the training of counselors. Onder (1970) noted that the recent willingness of the professions of Psychiatry, Psychotherapy, and Counseling to take advantage of technology reflects a general shift in the phiIOSOphy of the process of therapy from the notion that the therapist/client relation- ship could not be disturbed by another person or device without jeopardizing client benefits to the willingness to use "extra-personal" devices. Onder further asserted that the development and use of electronic devices in this profession and its training procedures heralds a greater acceptance of techno- logical devices. A. Audiotapes In the 1940's, the use of audiotapes became an integral part of counselor education (Covner, 1942; Rogers, 46 1942). Early use of audiotapes for stimulated recall was conducted by Gaier (1951). He audiotaped classroom dis- cussions and replayed the tapes to each Of 11 subjects in an attempt to gain understanding of the students' covert behaviors and experiences during a learning situation. During replay, the tape was stopped at pre-selected points and stimulated recall of students' conscious thoughts at the time of the original experience was conducted. Gaier noted the implications of this technique of inquiry into obtaining data for reporting inner thoughts and feelings. The use of audiotapes, and stimulated recall has since been adopted for supervisory activities in counselor training (Beiser, 1966; Holmes, 1964; Kubie, 1958); those advocating this use of audiotapes found that by using this approach in supervision trainees were able to achieve a greater self- awareness with audiotapes than without their use. According to many, the addition of audiotapes represented an improvement in supervision, and has become an integral part of the supervisory process. However, a number of limitations were noted. Some deleterious effects have been reported, such as potentially constricting the naturalness of the counseling situation (Eckstein and Wallerstein, 1958), increasing counselor anxiety (Covner, 1942), and being unre- presentative of the total process which could result in distortion in supervision of what actually occurred (Heimann, 47 1964). Ryan (1969) noted that while the naturalness and anxiety dimensions could be overcome as the counselor and client became more comfortable in the situation, certain other weaknesses could not be overcome. According to him, audiotapes did not appear to capture non-verbal behaviors and a full representation of the on-going process. In 1., (1967) reported that when addition to this, Kagan pg. verbal and non-verbal behaviors were recalled, they were subject to the personal distortions of the trainees. For example, they noted that when audiotapes of previously transcribed counseling sessions were replayed in supervision, trainees recalled their interviews in retrospect, often forgetting the way verbal and non-verbal behaviors actually occurred in the original situation. The supervisor often interpreted client behaviors to trainees, albeit without a complete record of data from the interview. Kagan and his associates concluded that while supervisors dealt with impasses trainees experienced in counseling, their recall was somewhat removed from their original occurrences. B. Videotapes The use of videotape has recently begun to be used in the training of counselors. Gruenberg, Liston, and Wayse (1969) indicated that vidoetaping improved the procedure of supervision for the following reasons: 48 1. The supervisor can feel a sense of encounter with the client which he could not feel before using videotape. 2. The supervisor was able to see the client for himself and did not have to rely on the subjective reports of the trainee; this allowed the super- visor to form his own impressions and conclusions. They claim the supervisor can function more effectively when videotape is used, and this transfers to improved supervision of trainees (this ultimately transfers into subsequent client growth as a result of improved trainee skill). 3. Videotaping provides a complete and comprehensive record of what transpired in the counseling ses- Sion. 4. The use of videotaping seemed to increase the amount of therapeutic activity during therapy. They in- dicated that "...when one is under intense scrutiny, whether it be in doing psychotherapy or surgery, it is reasonable to expect a more intense invest- ment of attention to the problem at hand (p. 104)." Trainees, being aware that their behaviors in counseling were to be closely reviewed, showed a tendency to more actively participate and invest themselves in the process. A number of studies have been conducted using videotape procedures for counselor training and supervision.1 Landsman and Lane (1963) and Walz and Johnston (1963) explored the effects of videotaping a counseling session and its subsequent replay on a counselor's attitudes toward himself and his coun- seling behaviors. Landsman and Lane had trainees role play interviews and videotaped the interaction. These interviews 1 All studies relating to the use of the Interpersonal Process Recall (IPR) technique in counselor training and supervision will be discussed in a later section of this chapter. 49 were later replayed for the trainees to watch. The authors concluded that a greater understanding of their own behaviors was possible for trainees watching a replay of videotaped role playing than was possible using audiotapes. Walz and Johnston attempted to assess the effects of watching a videotape replay of trainees' counseling behaviors (using coached clients) on trainees' attitudes toward their coun- seling behaviors. An Interview Check List was administered to measure changes in counselors' self-perceptions as a result of viewing a video-replay of their in-counseling behaviors. The authors concluded that counselor candidates exhibited a greater agreement with the supervisors' view of their interview behaviors after video-viewing than before watching the replay. This result was supported in a later study by Poling (1964). Other studies have demonstrated the possible benefits of the application of videotape for counselor supervision. Videotape was a useful tool for increasing trainees' under- standing of interview dynamics of the counselor and client, including non-verbal communications which added material for the supervisory session (Schiff and Reivich, 1964; SueSS, 1966), for improving the development of empathic responses of coun- l., 1965), to critique counseling selors (Buchheimer, 33. sessions and achieve a greater understanding of the process of counseling (Poling, 1968), and to develop an effective 50 repertoire of counseling response leads (Delaney, 1969).1 In sum, the use of videotaping in counselor training (and in client/patient growth) seems to add dimensions not previously available. By viewing a total picture of the counseling interaction between the trainee and client, cues of a non-verbal nature can be identified and discussed, and the counseling dyad can be scrutinized in more objective terms. The use of videotaping shows promise as a means of increasing self-awareness and accelerating the discovery process (as indicated in the studies by Landsman and Lane, 1963; Poling, 1964, 1968; Schiff and Reivich, 1964; Suess, 1966; Walz and Johnston, 1963). However, a possible criticism of most of the studies reviewed so far can be that the train- ing models and exercises presented have attempted to assess only the changes in self-understanding achieved by trainees using paper and pencil attitude scales (e.g. Interpersonal Checklist) and supervisor ratings. No attempts were made to help trainees translate this understanding into behaviors. With the exception of a few studies (Delaney, 1969; Eisenberg and Delaney, 1970; Goldberg, 1967; Ivey pg. 1., 1968), there l The use of videotape and videotape replay has also been reported related to changes in self-understanding as perceived by patients in conjoint marital therapy (Alger and Hogan, 1967), psychiatric in-patients in a V.A. Hospital (Boyd, Vernon, and Susney, 1967), and in patients in psychodrama at Langley-Porter Institute (Goldfried and Levy, 1968). 51 has been a dearth of attempts to use videotape for developing behavioral skills in counselor candidates. A number of researchers (Bordin, 1965; Carkhuff, 1966) take the position that studies are needed which evaluate the application of this understanding to developing counseling behaviors and skills. Limitations in the use of videotape have been noted in the literature. One area in particular was the amount of threat caused by viewing one-self behaving.1 Poling (1964) studied the impact of videotaping counselor trainees in counseling interviews in three different physical environments. He reported that the first video-recorded interviews and their subsequent critique resulted in higher defenses, hostility, and resistance in trainees. Along similar lines, Goldberg (1967) found that videotape, by capturing an abundance of material, can flood the trainee with an overwhelming amount of data which is too difficult to sift through and interpret. He suggested that when videotape is used in supervision, the supervisor should have had experience with videotape and stimulated recall techniques; also, trainees should be grad- ually introduced to this wealth of material by a supervisor who can provide support for the trainee in the early stages This was noted eSpecially with hOSpitalized psychiatric patients (Moore, pp, 11., 1965), and with a psychiatric out- patient (Geertsma and Reivich, 1965). 52 of exposure. He went on to say that the supervisor who is knowledgeable of the potential effects of the videotape experience can function in a way that enables the trainees to incorporate the material into their behavior repertoires without becoming overwhelmed by the myriad of data. Goldberg (1967) further asserted that while the results of studies show the potential advantages of videotape usage in super- vision, the undifferentiated use of videotape does not necessarily lead to changes in counselors' behaviors. He concluded that for videotape to be used most effectively, it must of necessity be incorporated into an integrated series of supervisory experiences,1 with the purposes and goals of each experience being clearly defined for trainees showing a functional relationship between these tasks. He noted that if this was not accomplished trainees may be left to establish their own goals, which might be quite divergent from those they were expected to work toward. This conten- tion was supported by the findings of Griffin (1966), who found that when the goals of counseling were clearly delineated by focusing counselors on learning Specific dimensions of behavior using videotape procedures, pre-post changes in 1 This is in basic agreement with Truax, Carkhuff and Douds (1964), who advocated the use of an integrated series of training experiences combining didactic and experiential components in training. 53 counselors' interview behavior on the Counselor Verbal Re- sponse Scale was found; no change was found in a comparable group where a similar focus was not provided. Griffin noted that if the goals of training were to increase the incidence and quality of counselors' behaviors, it was important to recognize the need to focus on the development of these desired behaviors. C. Simulation Simulation techniques have been used (e.g., pilot and astronaut training, industry, and armed forces) to pro- vide training Opportunities which recreate conditions closely approximating actual situations, while minimizing the chances for personal harm and/or mechanical damage (Kersh, 1965). The appeal of simulation techniques has been discussed by Greenlaw, Huron, and Raudon (1962); they described the benefits occuring from the use of simulation materials as including opportunities for trainees to learn to understand process and decision making behaviors, to practice desired behaviors, and to examine their various thoughts and responses in the simulated situation free from the responsibility of producing deleterious effects. The use of simulation materials has been shown to be applicable to counselor training. Delaney (1969) indicates that research using a simulation approach to counselor education is justified for the following reasons: 54 l. simulation is beneficial as an instructional technique 2. the use of a television monitor for stimulus presentations is appropriate 3. realism is not a primary requirement for trans- fer of learning 4. simulation positively effects actual performance 5. simulation provides economy of time and reduces long term expense 6. the application of simulation techniques to counselor education has shown to be feasible and effective (p. 18). The importance of the use of different applications of mechanical media such as simulation films and tapes in guidance and counselor education has also been noted by Kagan (1970). He indicated that one of the important con- tributions technology can provide seems to be its ability to extend the level of creativity, imagination, and potential of educators and students: "...it provides exciting new Opportunities for implementing a guidance phiIOSOphy. Enactment of the various stress situations on film, video, or audiotape can serve not only to recreate the situation, but in the format of media reproduces the potentially threatening situation within the relative safety of an electron tube or film projector. Situations can thus be created which, in life, are too threa- tening to be effectively dealt with, but in simulation can be safely examined and alternative responses considered and even practiced." 55 Although the application of simulation techniques to training counselors has obvious implications, it has little empirical validation. A study by Beird and Standish (1964) did attempt to provide research evidence by using a simulated environment to train counseling practicum students to dis- criminate between cognitive and affective client responses and then using a counselor response lead to facilitate more affective behavior in clients. This study used audio-re- cordings as simulation stimuli in a programmed learning format. The results indicated that the experimental groups demonstrated a significant performance gain over the control group, as well as differences in post-training interview performance. Schauble (1970) raised the point that if positive results can be obtained using an audio-Simulation approach, one wonders what the application of film or video techniques could add. Along this line, research with the Interpersonal Process Recall project has led to the development and use of Affect Simulation Films (Kagan and Schauble, 1969; Schauble, 1968). These films were developed to simulate a number of emotional situations which portrayed areas of conflictual feelings found to exist in most interpersonal relationships. The authors found that by confronting clients with a number of these films, and viewing a video-replay of this tape with simulated recall, client growth could be accelerated. 56 Because the creation and application of Affect Simulation Films was deve10ped from research in IPR, more thorough review of this technique for accelerating growth will be conducted in the section under IPR later in this chapter. Application of Specific Techniques to Training and Supervision Of the many studies incorporating technological de- vices in the training of counselors, three approaches will presently be considered and evaluated (Ivey, 25, al,, 1968, Microcounseling; Eisenberg and Delaney, 1970, Video Simulation; Kagan, pp. gl,, 1967, IPR project). These have been chosen because of their attempts to increase the incidence and quality of counselor behaviors, using technological devices as a vehicle for training. A. Microcounseling and Attending Behavior t. al., (1968) conducted a series of three Ivey, studies in which it was postulated that brief training, focused on specific skills of counseling, could be useful in a counseling pre-practicum. This brief method of training, called Microcounseling, was "a video method for training counselors in basic skills of counseling within a short period of time (p. 1)," and was based on the research of Allen and his associates (1967), who have used similar concepts of microteaching in teacher training. The idea of training quickly and effectively stemmed from the works of 57 Carkhuff and Truax (1965), who indicated that trainees can function at levels of effective therapy commensurate with experienced therapists in less than 100 hours of training. Microcounseling was described as a scaled-down sample of counseling in which neophyte counselors talked with volunteer "clients" for five minute sessions which were videorecorded. These sessions focused on Specific counseling skills or behaviors; the principle aim was to provide a bridge between classroom theory and actual practice at the pre-practicum level of training. Three separate studies were conducted, each involving the use of microcounseling training procedures in one of three areas: Study 1 - "attending behavior" Study 2 - reflection of feeling Study 3 - summarization of feeling These target skills, considered reinforcing counselor behaviors, indicated that the counselor was paying attention to the client, understanding the client by accurately sensing the feelings which were being expressed, and meaningfully integrating the responses of the person. In all, a total of 59 counselors were used in the three studies (38 in Study 1; 11 in Study 2; 10 in Study 3); the same number of paid volunteers served as ”clients”, proportioned with the number of counselors per study. Briefly, the Microcounseling model included tasks involving: 58 l. videotaping an initial five minute interview with a volunteer "client" for analysis 2. reading special manuals designed to illustrate and discuss basic components of the target behaviors 3. watched a videotape of effective and noneffective examples of target behaviors 4. engaged in three minute role playing interviews which were videotaped for analysis. Sessions (1,3,4) using videotape were videoviewed to note instances of attending behavior, accurate reflection, and summarization of feeling, and instances where accuracy might be improved. The entire procedure for each trainee was completed within a two-hour time block. The criterion measures for the three studies consisted of: (a) trainees' videotaped interviews (rated by two external judges on a rating scale adapted from Truax and Carkhuff (1967) for presence or absense of the target behavior for that task), (b) client ratings of his counselor on a semantic differ- ential scale, the Counselor Effectiveness Scale, and also on a relationship questionnaire adapted from Truax and Carkhuff (1967). An increase in the amount of eye contact, verbal following behavior (.05 level), in ratings of accurate reflection of feeling (.001 level), and in ratings of accurate summarization of feeling (.001 level), was found in judges' ratings from first to later interviews. Client ratings of counselor effectiveness and counselor ability to 59 establish and maintain a relationship increased significantly over trials for all target behaviors. The authors concluded that these three skills could be successfully taught to neophyte counselors using Microcounseling, and felt that "direct viewing of the videotapes of the counselors provided dramatic and convincing evidence of the validity and power of the Microcounseling procedure"(p. 10), for teaching these skills. They took the position that one of the most difficult tasks in training neophyte counselors is to get them to pay attention to the client. They noted that a benefit of this approach was that Microcounseling helped trainees learn to pay more attention to clients and their communications, and helped supervisors more rapidly intro- duce trainees to basic developmental skills of counseling. The authors also pointed out some limitations of these studies. Self-ratings and ratings of "clients” were used throughout as an indicator of support for the Microcounseling paradigm. While they reported that the trainees seemed enthusiastic about their behaviors, and expressed more con- fidence in their abilities as future counselors, they cautioned that there was little or no evidence that this would translate into actual counseling behaviors. Also, improvement in "clients'” ratings was possibly accounted for by a halo effect simply because the two individuals become better acquainted and felt more comfortable with one another. 60 Microcounseling may be effective in identifying basic skills for neophyte counselors to develop. However, a possible criticism of these studies may be the lack of a competing control group. The control group in these studies consisted of people who simply sat around drinking a coke or some such activity. A more acceptable control group may have been one in which trainees received a competitive treatment, such as having trainees read about the behaviors they should develop. The only difference between the con- trol and experimental procedures would then be the effect of the experiential involvement (for the experimental group) in developing these skills directly with videotape replay and supervision. In conclusion, these studies seem to directly focus on teaching trainees to develop desired skills for counselors to possess by using videotape as a medium for increasing trainee learning. No attempts were made to help trainees understand the process of counseling in terms of the influence the counselor could have in the encounter; no attempts were made to enable trainees to understand the ramifications of the mutual impact which occurs between the counselor and client in their relationship. As posited earlier (in Chapter II), self-understanding and self-awareness were seen t.al., by many as one goal of training. The studies by Ivey, appeared to deprive trainees of self-discovery and self- 61 exploring experiences. The Microcounseling technique focused on modeling procedures and cognitive input, designed to help trainees to attent to clients' behaviors; no attempts were made to help trainees attend to an understand their own reactions and behaviors. B. Video Simulation of Counseling_for Training Counselors A unique approach for training counselors using simulation techniques was proposed by Delaney (1969). It was designed to help trainees develop an effective repertoire of counseling response leads. He chose to use videotape and based his theory of training on a simulation strategy. This approach was chosen out of his belief that simulation can bring maximum control of those situations and behaviors considered necessary to be dealt with. By presenting a videotape containing a series of clients describing various problems to counselor trainees, Delaney felt that it would be possible to ”hit” many potential areas of importance for trainees to experience. Trainees could learn to develop be- haviors of good counseling in Specific areas and under varying conditions. According to this paradigm counselors would be given the opportunity to respond to videotaped clients in the hope that this would help to make the situation authentic and appear that the counselor was actively involved in a counseling session. Based on this strategy, Delaney proposed a training package consisting of an integrated 62 series of videoLapes, each designed to train counselors to use a well-defined counseling response lead. As an offshoot from this study, Eisenberg and Delaney (1970) attempted to construct a training "package" using video simulation of counseling for training counselors. Using principles of modeling and systematic manipulation of important reinforcers, the authors attempted to train coun- selors to respond in a way (referred to as counselor tacting response leads CTRL) which would elicit a client verbal response which would either describe a particular abstract concept in more operational or behavioral terms, or would give specific examples of whatever it was the client was attempting to communicate (referred to as client tacting response). A videotape of 40 individual client statements followed by a 15 second period for counselors to reSpond to each one was constructed. For the first 20 statements, one group Saw an "expert" counselor model CTRL; another group was encouraged to respond to the client, but without the benefit of the model expert; a third group simply responded to all client statements without exposure to the model or reinforce- ment. The last 20 clients statements were to be responded to by all the trainees with no model or reinforcement. A situation was also included in an attempt to assess the transfer of learning from the laboratory to actual "client” 63 contact. (Undergraduate students volunteered to talk to trainees for approximately 1/2 hour with the conversation being audiorecorded for later analysis.) Two raters were trained to score the audiotapes made during the videotape situation and transfer situation. (Training consisted of a verbal explanation of CTRL response class and rating several practice tapes.) Audiotapes from each condition were randomly assigned to each rater; in scoring these tapes, the raters listened to and scored trainees' responses to each of the last 20 videotape clients as to whether or not it was a CTRL. The authors found that systematic exposure to modeling procedures of effective counselors significantly increased the quality of trainees' responses to clients on videotape (.01 level), but this did not carry over to "live" clients. They concluded that one possible way trainees learned about effective counseling was by emulating their teachers. In explaining the failure to show transfer effects of this learning from simulation experiences to live experiences, they postulated that the types of statements they attempted to elicit from trainees may have been out of step (i.e., may belong to the goal-setting stage rather than the initial interview Stage). Another possible explanation they offered was that because trainees were responding in a laboratory situation removed from an actual counseling encounter, the 64 situation may have had a ring of nonauthenticity and may not have simulated the real experience as closely as the authors had hoped. In attempting to explain the failure to demonstrate transfer effects in the Eisenberg and Delaney (1970) study, a number of considerations might be noted. While trainees were taught (using modeling techniques) what counselor behaviors may be desirable under various circumstances in counseling, they were not able to transfer this knowledge from the simulated experience into an experience with a ”live" client (i.e., trainees may have learned cognitively what counselor tacting response to make, but knowing what to say in a training exercise and being able to say it in an applied situation may have been a problem for trainees). Advocates of the need for self-discovery and awareness experiences in training might posit that exploratory experiences can help trainees become more aware of some of the reasons why they might not be able to make a helping response at selected times, even though they might "know” intuitively what to say; from this recognition, trainees may begin to work through a number of blocks to effective communication. Along similar lines, the level at which the counselor learns to respond to the client, found by Truax and Carkhuff (1967) to be directly related to client growth, may be positively or negatively influenced by various training procedures. Those who advocate 65 the application of self-discovery experiences in training may interpret this as further validation of the need for experiences in which trainees can examine some of their behaviors and come to recognize and understand the impact and intensity of these experiences. This awareness can then be used to help the counselor more accurately recognize the impact of client experiences on clients' behaviors, so the counselor can respond to the client on at least the same level of intensity as the client is experiencing, which can possibly take the client a step further in the growth process. C. Interpersonal Process Recall (IPR) An important application of electronic devices to the field of counseling was the development of the IPR technique begun by Kagan, Krathwohl, and Miller (1963). This technique, using videotaping procedures, was started out of the perceived need for counselors to more accurately ascertain what their clients were thinking and why they behaved as they did, in an attempt to accelerate client growth and increase counselor effectiveness. The problem was formulated: "If we could capture the relevant stimuli in the situation in such a way that we could recreate it at a later time, we could better help the person to relive the original situation." (Kagan, pg, 31., 1967) 66 A procedure using videotaping of the counseling session and replaying it immediately afterwards with the client was adopted. The authors felt that this approach provided the possibility of capturing the maximum number of cues (verbal and non-verbal) for the individual which would be available for immediate playback and recall. Initial results with this process indicated that observing one's self on videotape in interpersonal situations enabled clients to identify, label, and discuss at length and in depth the subtle and blatant meanings of their behaviors (Kagan, Krathwohl, and Miller, 1963; Kagan, g5, p1,, 1967, 1969; Resnikoff, Kagan, and Schauble, 1969; Schauble, 1970). Although originally developed to accelerate client growth, this technique has also been applied to the training and supervision of coun- selors (Goldberg, 1967). The entire IPR process includes a number of techniques, including the use of videotape and stimulated recall of the videotape playback which focuses on the underlying dynamics of the interaction on tape, the use of simulation materials designed to expose individuals to various kinds and degrees of emotional situations, and the use of a clinical interrogator who is specifically trained to use IPR techniques. The following is a brief discussion of these techniques: 67 1. Stimulated Videotape Recall Stimulated videotape recall is the IPR technique developed for viewing a videotaped replay of a previously held personal interaction. The IPR session begins when two participants enter the studio and begin the interview. Typically, the interview is observed by the person who will serve as the recall worker (Interrogator); the interview is videotaped and when the interview is concluded, the tape is rewound for immediate replay. At this point, one or the other of the participants, but usually it is the counselor, leaves the room and the recall worker takes his place. The recall session involves a video-replay of the original session over the television monitor in the room now begins. A remote control switch permits either the subject or recall worker to start and stop the video-machine from the recall room. Either the recall worker or the other individual may stop the tape. Discussions are related to feelings in specific instances or situations (Kagan, pp, 31., 1969). A number of individuals who have conducted research with IPR have reported that the approach seems to change interpersonal behaviors by focusing participants on underlying dynamics of feeling and thought as they relate to interactions with others (Kagan and Krathwohl, 1967; Kagan, Krathwohl, and Miller, 1963; Woody, Kagan, Krathwohl, and Farquahr, 1965). 68 2. Affect Simulation Materials Simulation materials have been developed (Kagan, pp, g1,, 1967, Kagan and Schauble, 1969; Schauble, 1968) which allow for the re-creation of an interpersonal situation which is based on reactions to filmed vignettes of individuals por- traying aggression and seduction, and fear of the rejection attached to these dimensions.1 The films were based on the assumption that ”...if the client were exposed to various kinds and degrees of emotional situations, if his reactions to these situations were videotaped, and if after each such exposure he were given the opportunity to view his behavior a counselor via IPR, could client movement be accelerated?" A series of planned behaviors portrayed by actors was developed. These actors were instructed to portray basic affective states often manifest in counseling dyads. These dimensions fell along an approach-avoidance continum, deal- ing with client feelings that (a) the counselor might hurt or reject him, (b) the counselor might make an affectionate with 9 dependent or seductive approach toward him, (c) the client's own hostile impulses toward the counselor might emerge, and (d) the client's own affectionate, dependent, or seductive impulses toward the counselor might emerge (Kagan and Schauble, 1969). 1A sample typescript of the Affect Simulation Films is found in Appendix D. 69 Stimulated recall sessions were held using the video- taped replay of the individual viewing the simulated films and reacting to them. The rationale behind this technique was that if the client was confronted with these videotaped reactions to planned threatening behaviors, this experience might serve as a microscope of the type of interpersonal behaviors so stimulated. The interrogator focused on the reactions the individual had to the films-~his feelings about the filmed actor, his feelings about himself, his feelings about the situation--the personal confrontation being re- lated to increased self-exploration and growth (Danish and Kagan, 1969; Kagan, pp, 31., 1967; Kagan and Schauble, 1969). Preliminary results (Schauble, 1970) have demonstrated that significant client growth was found in controlled studies based on this application of IPR. 3. Clinical Interrggator The role of clinical interrogator was deve10ped, through IPR research, to facilitate client self-analysis by teaching clients to develop self-explorative behaviors. This was accomplished by using a videotaped playback of clients' actual behaviors and focusing on the dynamics of the original relationship on tape. The counselor and client meet for a counseling session which is videotaped. The counselor then leaves the room and the interrogator enters in his place. The interrogator's 70 purpose is not to establish another counseling relationship with the client. Instead, he and the client view the videotaped replay of the session just completed and a recall which is conducted focuses on the on-going dynamics of the original relationship. The interrogator's function is "to facilitate the subject's self-analysis of his underlying thoughts, feelings, images, expectations, and general pattern of interaction with his counselor" (Kagan, 1969). The focus for the client is on his feelings and thoughts throughout the interview, his self-perceptions, how he would like to see himself, how he believes and wants the counselor to see him, and the possible alternative behaviors which are Open for him. One study to come out of the IPR project, using a number of these techniques, was that of Goldberg (1967). He used a sequential series of supervisory experiences implemented by IPR as a means of educating counselors during practicum. He postulated that exposure to stimulated recall using videotape would increase a counselor's sensitivity to inter- personal communication, as he would be stimulated to recall his feelings, expectations, and perceptions in the counsel- ing interview. He also posited that counselors face a series of developmental tasks in practicum: (a) to become aware of the elements of good counseling; (b) to become sensitive to and understand a greater amount of client 71 communication; (c) to become aware of and sensitive to one's own feelings during the counseling session; and (d) to become sensitive to the bilateral nature of the counseling inter- action. A total of 36 Masters level graduate students in Coun- seling, Personnel and Educational Psychology at Michigan State University were included in this study. Two groups were formed, with half the students receiving the individual IPR supervision (experimental group) and half receiving traditional individual supervision (control group). The IPR treatment consisted of four integrated stages designed to meet the trainees' developmental needs. In Stage 1, the dimensions of the C.V.R.S. served as'a reference point for discussion a videotaped counseling session, focusing on the elements of good counselor/client communication. In Stage 2, the trainee held a 30 minute counseling session with a client. This was followed by a supervisor-client video- recall (lasting 15-20 minutes) and a subsequent supervisor- trainee video-recall (lasting approximately 45 minutes). In Stage 3, each trainee served as interrogator, conducting a video-recall session with a partner's client. In Stage 4, trainee and client conducted a counseling interview; this was immediately followed by a video-replay and recall of this session with trainee, client, and supervisor present, the supervisor encouraging the trainee and client to share 72 with each other the feelings experienced during this inter- view. Using a 5 test for paired samples to determine whether significant differences existed within groups over time and between groups after treatment, Goldberg found that while both groups of counselors were rated as more affective, understanding, exploratory, and effective (.005) level), significantly greater change was Observed within the IPR group. He concluded that the sequential series of super- visory experiences using IPR was an effective means of educating counselors during practicum. The model used by Goldberg seems to meet a number of criteria established by other researchers, and may be seen to contain advantages over the other studies which have been reviewed. By incorporating a number of developmental tasks into an integrated series of experiences, he noted that his model adhered to the criteria established by Carkhuff (1966) and Truax, Carkhuff, and Douds (1964). According to Higgins, Ivey, and Uhlemann (1970), a major weakness of the developmental task concept usually arises when the focus is placed on the task instead of the behavior the task is designed to teach. The approach used by Goldberg appeared to avoid this pitfall by emphasizing the behavioral aspects of each particular task. Goldberg found he was able to accelerate the development of counseling skills over and 73 above the amount achieved using more traditional methods as a result of functionally relating each task to all others. The studies by Ivey, $5. $1,, (1968) and Eisenberg and Delaney (1970) were concerned with using modeling procedures to teach trainees Specific counseling skills. Goldberg used modeling procedures in a training session during which trainees viewed a videotaped counseling interview and discussed the dimensions of concrete counselor/client commun- ication with a supervisor; this was also accomplished by the supervisor (in the role of interrogator) who modeled behaviors of how to relate to a client and help the client to clarify his concerns. However, Goldberg's training paradigm appeared to go beyond the sole use of modeling techniques. The "client" and "mutual" recall phases provided trainees with direct and immediate feedback concerning the client's reactions to the trainee's counseling behaviors. AS a result of this Goldberg found that trainees were helped to realize a greater awareness of the underlying implications of a client's verbal and non-verbal behaviors. He also found that by drawing on the trainee's feelings, fears, and expectations during the ”counselor" recall phase that while similar material was evident in traditional supervision, it was not dealt with until a later time (i.e. the early expression of feelings in the IPR group seemed to help trainees explore their discomforts earlier in supervision). 74 He further noted that the exploration of "self" enabled the trainees to overcome their preoccupation with themselves during the counseling interview, and this had the effect of opening them to receive a greater amount of client communica- tion. This also helped trainees realize that their perceptions were often surprisingly accurate and could often be used to monitor client feelings and perceptions. A possible modification in Goldberg's study might be the incorporation of the Affect Simulation Films as a vehicle for enabling trainees to become more sensitive to and aware of their own feelings and reactions to a variety of situa- tions. Trainees can be videotaped while watching the films, and a stimulated recall of this videotape can be performed by a recall worker, in much the same way as a recall of a counseling session or interview. A methodological note with the Goldberg Study might be raised. He began training with a didactic presentation of the elements of good counseling. Instead of beginning this manner, it is suggested that another way to begin might be by having trainees look at some of their own feelings and reactions. Danish and Kagan (1969) found that when the Affect Simulation Films were used at the beginning of counseling, they served to orient clients to the fact that talking about feelings is "fair game". These researchers go on to indicate that the use of films "legitimize, stimulate, 75 and define the counselor's willingness to deal with affect in depth". The use of these films in training may set the "tone" by establishing that his own feelings are important for counselors to recognize. By first making trainees aware of how feelings motivate and influence their own behaviors, perhaps they can use this awareness to understand how feelings might also motivate and influence a client's behaviors. This may be accomplished by having trainees use their own self-exploratory and discovery experiences as a model of how they might go about helping clients explore their concerns and feelings. Conclusions and Implications A body of evidence seems to be accumulating which supports the use of an integrated, multi-dimensional approach to training. One important aspect seems to be the need for focusing trainees on developing desired behaviors if one's goal is to increase the incidence and quality of such behaviors (Griffin, 1966). These considerations can take on added significance when the use of technological devices is introduced into supervision and training. The application of technology to training procedures seems to have potential as a means of implementing and accelerating the process of counselor preparation, provided the undifferentiated use of these techniques is controlled. 76 Studies using IPR have indicated that growth can be accelerated (in clients in counseling and trainees in supervision) over and above the amount achieved using traditional approaches. The use of stimulated videotape replay and simulation films has a number of implications for future research and train- ing. The use of these films suggests the possibility of creating laboratory training situations portraying a number of encounters which trainees were more than likely to be confronted with in their counseling experiences. For example, trainees are more than likely to experience impasses in counseling revolving around their fears of: (a) appearing inadequate, (b) being rejects, (c) being depended upon, (d) being seduced, and (e) directing these impulses toward others (Kagan and Schauble, 1969; Schauble, 1968). With the use of videotape, it now becomes possible for trainees to explore these reactions and situations without having to experiment and learn at a clients' expense. The IPR approach can be especially relevant when used with neophyte coun- selor candidates. Training procedures, including role playing, use of coached clients, use of simulated exper- iences, and use of technological equipment can be applied to laboratory settings to help beginning trainees develop a "feel" for the multidimensionality of the counseling situation without the danger of their performing for "better or worse" with clients. 77 Few of the studies reviewed attempted to determine the impact of training on actual counseling behavior. While some focused on goals of self-awareness and recognition of non- verbal behavior, others looked at changes in trainees' attitudes or responses to paper and pencil instruments. Most of the studies which attempted to evaluate counselor behavior relied solely on modeling and behavior procedures and made no attempts at helping trainees explore their own reactions and perceptions in the counseling situation. Of those studies which did attempt to evaluate counselor be- havior as a function of type of training, only those by Goldberg (1967), Carkhuff and Truax (1965), and Truax, Carkhuff and Douds (1964) did not restrict training to the singular application of modeling or self-awareness techniques. In summary, because most studies reviewed in this chapter appeared to demonstrate a lack of comprehensiveness in their training paradigms, this can be taken as support for the assumption that there is a need for studies which incorporate training experiences into an integrated series of developmental tasks, using a multi-dimensionsl approach incorportating discovery-experiential and behavioral- didactic experiences. These studies may be implemented by the use of technological devices to accelerate the learning process. CHAPTER III DESIGN AND METHODOLOGY OF STUDY In order to test the hypotheses posited in Chapter I, the design of the present study had to permit comparison of the trainees on measures of discrimination and communication skills in counseling, based on the exposure to the sequence of treatments. Using a pre, middle, and post measurement de- sign as the framework for answering the main research ques- tions, the subjects were divided into 2 groups of 10; the course, which lasted 10 weeks in length, was divided into 2, 4~week treatments. Measurements on the criterion measures were obtained during the first, sixth, and eleventh weeks. The division of the course into two equal halves enabled all trainees to receive the experimental and control treatments, and allows for reversal of treatments to ascertain the rela- tive strengths of each type of sequential program. The group receiving the Experimental treatment (Develop- mental tasks using I.P.R.) the first half of the course (weeks 2-5) and receiving the Control treatment (Traditional classroom lectures) the second half of the course (weeks 7~10) is designated Group 1. The group receiving the Control treat- ment the first half of the course (weeks 2-5) and receiving the Experimental treatment the second half of the course 78 79 (weeks 7-10) is designated Group 2. A schematic representa- tion of this design is presented in Table 3.1 on page 80. A pre, middle, and post measurement design allowed for a baseline measure to be obtained, which also served as a check on the power of the treatment sequence. To insure that a change in trainees' interview and counseling behavior could be determined, and relative growth through the term measured, the groups were compared in terms of growth during the first half of the course, second half of the course, and from pre- post changes over the full term, thus comparing the two groups on the strength of the treatments gpg_the treatment sequences. Description of Experimental Procedures A detailed summary of the experimental procedures within the framework of the over-all design is presented in Table 3.2, (page 81) followed by a discussion of the experimental procedures. Session 1 The initial class meeting was devoted to introducing the course and presenting an overview of the goals of the course to the trainees, placing them in either Group 1 or Group 2, LA complete summary Of the purpose of each developmental task, a description of each task, and its implementation appears in Appendix E. 80 mucmsummue um>o mammamc< , L mmH mmxmma sesame: Ousuooq mmmHo .III. Ousmmmz-umom Hmucosaoam>en wave“: Hmaowuwpmua enammuz-mum N msouw Oucmsvmm unmaummuy uuuuuuuuuuuuuuuuuuuuuuuuuuuuuuuuuuuuuuuuuuuuuuuuuuuuu --- ------------------------------- coesumm mmm>ama< enduomq mmmHu menace: mmH mmxmmH rll- mHSmmez-umom amdofiuwpmua eavaz Headmamoae>en Ouammmz-Oum H msouu Ha xomz oH-n mxooz o xooz m-N mxmmz H x003 mxmm3\muaeaumeua Aoauzv mmaouw summon Hauamafiumaxm HH<-ue>o mo coaumucOmoummm OHumaenom H.m mummH 81 .OHsuenom HancH>Hch Some you m prcemm¢ xoono OmmOHm mesHu paw waHHspOnum nuH3 mOHuHao -HHHHO Ou one OOchHu some wow mOHum> waHzmHa-OHOH mam uaOHHo-pwnomoo Ho 30H>ueucH mo pepuo meek peucmmeum mmHmHuaHua Oumuumaoswp ou pom: mommuOHp5< Ausemmua “OuosuumcH udewwuv umusuomH ummso :wcHHmmcsoo cH mHmou mam memoausm-zuomsH wcHHmmcsoo: "maenH musom m - eBHu wcHumOE msouu ":oHumHuomOQ GOHumucmmoua aooummmHo HmSOHqumuH "Omommmm N conmOm zmHamu mmmuompH>nusom H .mEHHw wchoum3 usom w musom MH - OeHu wcHuOOE HmSOH>Hch “GOHOQHuomOQ mmaHHemm G30 mo mmmamumsm Honuusm aHmw ou "omomusm mm cOHmmOm mEHHm GOHumHSEHm uommm< no em: musom wH - QEHu - wcHummE msouw ":0Humwuumea mmcHHOmm s30 Ho mmeamumzm dwmw ou "emomunm 4N conmmm N Ems: Ammusawa omv mHmum ALH>Huumamm m>uuumuua HOV Ameuaafia omV waHmmHa-OHOH .N AmmusaHa mHv ucmHHo venomoo .H kmea mHou\ucOHHu venomoo new? 30H>uousH Anv mmusaHa om - cowuospouucH AmV "mmmchuu HHm co pmchuno mwesmmmE-Oum paw .cm>Hw mmHSOO wo 3OH>HO>O Hmuecmu - H GOHmmOm H mama N mbomw H mbomu :mHmen HauaeEHueaxm mo GOHuaHuumOQ N.m mHde 82 muao: MH mwdHuwu msouw paw manuoepH> zuH3 muHma :H wcHhmHQ-OHOH meson wH mama-ompH> mo wcHumm musom m - eEHu - waHueuE macaw “=0HuaHuomOQ wcHHmmasoo :qu moaeHumaxO OQMuopr> .wcHHOmaaoo w>Huowmme mo muawEOHO cummH umnuusm ou «Mmmmmmm mm aOHmmOm mummucoo mumuumsHHH ou vmzmHm maneuoHpsd nouonuumaH umstou he doHumuumaoaep paw muauooH muse: wH :aoHuMEHOHdH Hmaowummsooo mama-OHpam mo waHumu meson wH mo Honomoa new ..uomanam. Hmuama - mam .mm>u mo GOHHMudOmOuQ -aouH>cm .uamuHSmaoo m< HOHOmasoo 05H: «mammH musom m - eSHu waHueefi msouu "cOHuaHuomOQ musom m - maHu waHuOOE asouo uaOHunHuommm wdHHomnaoo GOHUMuammmum aboummmHo HmeHqumuH mmmmmmmm m>Huommmm mo muaeEmHe aumeH ou mummmmmm s GOHmmmm ms dOHmmOm IanII S x003 HomosuumaH umstou %n OHSuOOH muso: wN-wdemu owpnm-waHmmHa :mOHuHDOHHHHQ OHaepm04 :H muaep OHom meuaaHE mH AwaHumOHaaa -:um waHmHmm - uaOaO>OHso< Hoonom mam -soo Ho mama ouv :oHuospouudH uaeoaoo-mHmm OHBOpmo< .uaeocoo-mHmm: "mamas musom m - maHu mcHuomE adouo ":OHuaHuommn musom m - OaHu wsHqua asouo ”aOHuQHuomOn maOHumuHGDEEoo mam mwnH GOHumudmmOum EooummmHo HmcowuwpmuH mmmmmmmm -Hemm ucoHHo mo mmeamumzm :Hmw ou "mmmmmmm n QOHmmom . m aOHmmmm m x003 N mbomo H macaw 83 .N macaw you mmum> mOH> cam Auamsumeuu HouuGOUv meHumucemoum aboummeo HmdoHqumuH muew so: Beau mo HHm: umuHm How uaeaummuu HeuaeEHuomxm pO>HOomu nana H adouu - muceaummuu :H ucHoq mHsu um neuHsm usonm pHou mmsouu Amossaua omV "mcoHuosuumGH AOV Ammusaua omv onom muu>uuumsom O>Huommu< an AmmunaHE omv wcHhmHa OHou .N AmmuaaHE mHv ucmHHo penumoo .H zmHm OHou\ucmHHo pmnomoo :uH3 3eH>uOuaH Amv ”mOOaHmuu HHm co pOcHMuno meusmmea evaHz - o conmem mumeucoo OumuumsHHH Ou HomnmmHm mmmuoepH> use OHSuOOH HHm: vacuum - uOuosuumcH umeso HHm: umHHm - HouoduumaH umHawum o sums HoHOmcaoo mmHm moechuu neon measuoepH> Ome - moonHmuu neon mo HHmo umu Hanusa OusaHa om zmHQ-OHOH oudawa oH - HHmowH Havana uoHOmcsoo mmHa moeaHmuu neon HHmOOu uoHOmasoo OusaHa Om saquOpH> :uHs mmHm-OHOH OusaHa oH-HHmoou AaonmOm wv :EmHu0H>mnomz uoHOmasou muse: m - uaHu AcOHmmOm wv :waHHomaaoo HMGOHumoo>z "mmaosa Homeowou ms-uHmm mooaHmuu N "aoHunHuumen mason m - OaHu wdHumma asouw ”amfiwmfiuwmmn QHanOHumHOn wcHHOmasoo sOHOmuaOmoum BooummmHo Hm:OHqumuH “Omomuam Ho menus: HmuOumHHn pneumuepas ou "emomusm m aOHmmOm m GOHmmmm m xeoz N moomo H macaw H macaw you me GOHmmOm mm seam ”aOHumHuomOo H uncuo you me :onmwm mm mamm "Omomusm mm GOHmmOm H asouo wow <¢ aOHmmOm mm mamm "cOHumHuomon N asouo How q GOHmmOm mm mamm "GOHumHuomoo H asouo you <¢ GOHmmem we seem "Omommmm N aneuo How s conmom mm seam “Omomusm <¢ :onmOm m GOHmmOm m xwmz A. H msouo How m SOHmmOm mm mamm "aOHumHuoqu N maouo new m GOHmmom we seem "=0HumHuumOQ a. H abouo mom m GOmeom mm seem "Omomuam N anouo now m conmmm mm 08mm “omommmm m conmOm w GOHmmOm w x o3 H msouo How mN GOHmmmm we seem "coHumHuomOQ H uncuo How mN aOHmmem mm seem "Omomusm mm conmom H msouu mom uuumamm o>uuoouu< Hay mmHa mHou\uceHHo penance nuHa SOH>HOuGH Adv no mam H meHmmmm :H ecow szsoH>mua mm .moochuu HHm do pmsHmuno mounmmma umom - HH GOHmmom HH some H asouo you n conwmm we seem "GOHumHuomen N msouo How m conmOm we seem "GOHumHuomon H msouu How m aonmOm mm mamm “Omomusm N msouu pom m aonmom mm QEMm "Omomusm OH eonmam 0H acummom oH xoez Hmaao N nHDOMU 86 and obtaining pre-measures on the criteria used in this study. All participants were assured that each would receive identical treatment, but in different order. The subjects were in- formed that there would not be a treatment versus no-treat- ment (or placebo) condition, only differences in sequence. During the instruction phase of this session, the trainees were told that the purpose of this course was to help develop skills in discrimination and communication, which would help them to be more effective helpers and coun- Se lors. The supervisor discussed the importance for trainees tO be aware of and sensitive to their own feelings and com- mLlnications, the feelings and communications of clients, and how each individual in the counseling process had an impact on the bilateral relationship formed between the counselor and client. The trainees were also told that there were ef fective versus non-effective ways to communicate with clients, Wh ich would be discussed during the course. After the introduction was given, the trainees were assigned to either Group 1 or Group 2; each individual was given a curriculum sheet containing the Schedule they would fOllow for criterion measurement and for the remainder of the Coursel (this included weekly schedules and specific individual g l A sample copy of an individual trainee's schedule is presented in Append ix F. 87 schedules for obtaining interviews of coached clients and role-playing). The trainees now followed their individual schedules for performing the tasks required to obtain measures on the criterion; the trainees were given instruc- tions on how to structure their interviews with coached clients and role-playing partners, and also were administered the Affective Sensitivity Scale. Description of Experimental Treatment: Developmental Tasks Using IPR As outlined in Table 3.2, the developmental tasks using :[19Ii involved a logical learning sequence, beginning by focus- iilngg trainees on their feelings to (a) sensitize them to tléiilking about and looking at feelings in themselves and others (1)) help them to recognize that feelings can influence be- hElviors and (c) give them a better appreciation of how feel- ings can influence behaviors by using their experiences as e—Xamples (Sessions 2, 2A, 7, 7A). The next task involved training in recognition of client feelings to make trainees mOre (a) sensitive to client feelings, to (b) help them to IVEcognize that feelings influence client behaviors and (c) to \Inderstand how clients' behaviors are influenced by their ifeelings and how this is communicated (Sessions 3, 8). Once these discriminations were made, the trainees were presumed ready to learn how to translate these (by communicating with 88 clients in certain ways) into helpful behaviors. Elements of effective counseling behaviors were presented to the group, and they received practice at rating these dimensions using an audio-tape (Sessions 4A, 9A). The next phase of this task involved rating a videotape of an "expert" counselor, to provide trainees with a tape previously rated high in the ealements of communication trainees were learning. Trainees £1180 had an opportunity to role-play and were videotaped to (as) give them experience in a counseling situation, (b) to "€Lase" them into being videotaped while counseling, and (c) t2<> rate these videotaped role playing sessions (Sessions 48, 5913). Finally, the last task involved understanding the bi- lateral nature of the counseling relationship, where trainees Eltmsd "clients” (role-played) were videotaped counseling. C3<>Ianselor and client then viewed a replay of these inter- aC:tions during which they told each other their recalled tlhoughts and feelings, thus focusing on the mutual impact between counselor and client in the encounter: (Sessions 5, 1()). De ve lopmental Task #1 The purpose of this task was to make trainees aware of Eind sensitive to their own feglings during interpersonal Communication. This was done in two separate phases: Phase 1: .During this part, the group met together and was shown vignettes from the Affect Simulation Films. After each vignette, the supervisor 89 involved the group in a discussion of how they felt toward the actor on the screen, focusing on their feelings, thoughts, and reactions to the situation. Each vignette lasted from between 1-2 minutes, and the discussion after each vignette ranged from short to involved discussion, depending on the amount of data the group supplied (Sessions 2A, 7A). Phase 2: During this part each trainee individually sup- plied a time block (approximately 1% hours) and met with a supervisor at the I.P.R° Suite A (See Training Facility, page 97), to view the rest of the Affect Simulation Films that remain- ed from the group meeting. The trainee viewed the films for % hour and was videotaped while watching the films. Immediately after the films, the supervisor (interrogator) entered Suite A and conducted a 1 hour video-recall session (of the trainee's reactions to the films) with the trainee. The supervisor attempted to focus the trainee on the feelings he had watch- ing the films, on the meaning of these feelings, how he typically reacts to these situations, drawing on the trainees reactions to the filmed encounters (Sessions 2B, 7B). Developmental Task #2 The purpose of this task was to make the trainee's more aware of and sensitive to, and understanding of a greater 90 amount of client communication. The trainees met as a group and were introduced to the task with a mini-lecture designed to increase trainees' awareness of client behavior by "teaching" them what cues (verbal and nonverbal communica- tions) to look for during the interview. After this, two trainees paired up and role played a counselor/client dyad, vehich was discussed by the group, focusing on the identifi- <:ation of client communications and their affective meanings; tflne group was encouraged to share their impressions of the twale-playing client. (All trainees get the Opportunity to Itc>le-p1ay the client) and how they arrived at these impres- 8 ions (Sessions 3, 8). I)€2velopmental Task #3 The purpose of this task was to make trainees increas- Iitlgly aware of elements p£_effective ppmmunication behaviors fizz; counseling: (a) to have trainees practice rating a tape 1ligh in elements of effective communication; (b) to give tirainees experience at being videotaped interacting with another person; (c) to become involved first-hand in a coun- Seding experience; and (d) to give trainees more practice iit rating trainees' counseling behaviors. This was accom- Plished in two phases: Phase 1: The supervisor passed out copies of the Coun- selor Verbal Response Scale (CVRS) and Empathic Understanding Scale (EUS) and the group dis- cussed each of the elements and dimensions of I. Phase 2: 91 the scales; these scales were characterized by specific ways of responding which have been defined as important behaviors for trainees to learn in order to be able to communicate effectively with clients. This provided each trainee with a model which served as a frame- work to evaluate counseling behaviors; these behaviors stressed what seemed to be essential to effective communication, regardless of theoretical orientation or position. After this conceptual framework was discussed, trainees were presented with a training audio- tape made by Carkhuff, which served as a stimulus for rating counselor-client inter- actions. The supervisor oversaw the group dis- cussion, and helped facilitate rating the tape (Sessions 4A, 9A). The supervisor played a videotape of a coun- seling session which was characterized by effective counseling behaviors. Trainees rated the tape after each 3-minute segment, based on the previous discussion of the CVRS and EUS scales, to further practice conceptualizing elements of effective communication. 92 After practicing ratings, two trainees paired up to role-play a counselor/client dyad, which was videotaped and replayed for the group. The supervisor helped focus the group on rating the encounters and why certain ratings were assigned. This was intended to enable trainees to experience involvement in the counseling procedure, to see themselves involved inter- acting with another person on videotape, and to practice rating counseling behaviors using a more "real-life" experience than provided by audio-tape (Sessions 4B, 9B). I)evelopmental Task #4 The purpose of this task was to enable trainees to become ssensitive to and aware of the bilateral nature pf_ppp_counseling Irelationship and the mutual impact between counselor and client. CDwo trainees paired up on the basis of compatible schedules, for a 3-hour time period. The session was divided into two equal parts: counselor recall only, and mutual recall with counselor and client. 1. Counselor Recall: During the counselor recall phase, each trainee took a turn at role playing counselor and client. Approximately 10 minutes was allowed to elapse, and was videotaped. The supervisor (inter- rogator) then conducted a recall with the counselor 93 only, with the client leaving the room (recall lasts approximately 40 minutes). After one role-played counselor, the roles were reversed, to give each trainee an opportunity to be counselor and be "re called" alone. The supervisor (interrogator) focused the trainee on the underlying affect and meaning which was per- ceived and communicated in the interview between counselor and client, to help the counselor identify the meaning of his reactions to the client, and to encourage the counselor to explore his relationship with the client. 2. Mutual Recall: During mutual recall, a videotape was made of each trainee taking a turn at role-playing counselOr and client. The recall sessions were con- ducted with both the counselor apg_client present. The supervisor (interrogator) focused the recall on underlying feelings present in the session between counselor and client, encouraging the trainees to share their feelings and perceptions about each other and the relationship they established. (Sessions 5, 10). Description of Control Treatment: Traditional Classroom Procedures The course, Procedures ip Counseling has traditionally been taught (at Michigan State University) as a didactic lecture course, in which issues in counseling, different 94 theoretical approaches and schools of thought, and various techniques are discussed. These may be implemented by the use of films, audio tapes, and guest lecturers. The tradi- tional classroom procedures have therefore been installed £18 the control treatment for the present study, so as to (zompare the innovative approach to the traditional approach for this course. The regular lecturer for the traditional classroom pro- cedures was a Full Professor in the Department of Counseling, Personnel, and Educational Psychology at Michigan State IJrriversity, who has been at MSU for 9 years, with 2 years' exPerience at another university and 9 years' experience in Public schools as a teacher and counselor. Guest lecturers Ilticluded an advanced Ph.D. candidate in Counseling at Michigan State University, and a regular faculty member who held the rank of Assistant Professor. The themes of the classroom presentations included "Counseling Theory - Purposes and Goals in Counseling" (taught 13y a guest lecturer who used audiotapes to demonstrate the principles presented), "Self-concept, academic self-concept, and school achievement-helping students in academic difficul- ties" (regular instructor), "The Counselor as Consultant, ‘Environmental 'Engineer', and Teacher of Occupational Informa- tion" (regular instructor, using audiotapes), "Vocational 95 Counseling” (regular instructor), and ”Behaviorism" (guest lecturer, using videotape). Coached Clients Description Because of the limitations of time, Space, and avail- zataility all counselor-trainees were unable to interview the szime client. In order to obtain a sample of interview be- lieivior for each trainee, 5 coached clients were used, with each one being assigned 4 trainees to interview them; this enabled all 20 trainees to interview coached clients in Elle: time and Spaces available. The counselor-trainees inter- V:Lewed the same coached client on the pre, middle, and post measurements; this served to measure consistency over the Saume individual, and compare trainee growth in the same stimulus situation. The coached clients were selected because of their cIlosely related educational backgrounds and related personal eEXperiences. The coached clients each had the following (tharacteristics: I. married female, age 25, Masters degree-Student Personnel husband-working on Ph.D. dissertation in Counseling Psychology 2. married female, age 25, Masters degree-Rehabilita- tion Counseling husband-working on Ph.D. disser- tation in Counseling Psychology 3. married female, age 24, Masters degree-Rehabilita- tion Counseling (degree expected confirmed March, 1970), husband-working on Ph.D. in Chemistry 96 married female, age 25, Masters degree-Student Personnel, husband-working on Ph.D. in Biophysics married female, age 25, Bachelors degree-Com- munication husband-working on Ph.D. dissertation in Counseling Psychology Training The coached clients received a total of between 3-4 hours The first portion of training ()f tutoring and training. iJncluded an introduction to the purpose of their task of 'rcale playing and discussion of what areas they were expected t<3 include in their role playing. The second part of train- itlg;involved practicing of role playing, followed by dis- (ZLISSIOD of how the role was authentic and credible, and/or flcnn the roles needed improvement or change; opportunity V9618 available to further practice roles after discussion of time quality of the role playing. The third part of training ?ilnvolved a recapitulation of the purpose of their task, dis- <>ussion of their role-playing behaviors, and how these roles Uuay be refined through practice. Again, the training was Ein attempt to have each coached client functioning as similiar- ly and closely as possible to each other coached client to Ininimize the biases built in by using different individuals, and this was stressed to the coached individuals. l The rationale and training for the coached clients can be found in Appendix G. 97 Training Facility The IPR training facility was designed with three rooms zadjacent to each other. Figure l is a schematic diagram of tJIe studio which the treatment was administered and recall nuns conducted (Studio A), the studio in which other recall seassions could be held simultaneously (Studio B), and an (apaerating room containing the hardware (Studio C). FIGURE 3.1 Schematic Diagram Of IPR Suite - 'lll/l/I/l/I/IL/l. Studio C 98 Studio A contained two chairs facing each other, in which the trainees sat and viewed the simulation films, and counseling interviews and stimulated videotape recall was conducted. A Hartzfield microphone was visibly placed on the coffee-table between the two chairs, and transmits the audio signal. A television monitor was used, over which the videotaped materials were replayed; the visual image appeared on the screen and the audio portion emanted from the speaker in the TV monitor. A remote control switch was placed on the coffee table, and the trainee used this switch to stop the videotape replay on the monitor an/or start the replay at any time. A telephone on the coffee table permitted the Staff to interrupt the recall proceedings at any time to inform the recall worker of salient features of the trainee's I‘e<:all, or to call into the tape room for directions regard- ing taping and viewing procedures. Studio B contained two chairs and a television monitor and was used for viewing the videotaped replay, and for con- clucting another recall of the videotape independent of the recall in Studio A. NO remote control was available for this room. Studio C contained two cameras situated behind thermo- pane glass separating Studio A from Studio C. One camera was focused on one of the two chairs occupied by the trainee 99 and the other was focused on the chair in which the "client" sits, or where the cardboard screen was placed to Show the simulation films. Although no attempts were made to conceal the cameras from view, placing them in another room in an unobtrusive fashion served to prevent them from being a concern to trainees. Further, the one-way mirror could be used to isolate the room and prevent viewing out from Studio A into Studio C. An Ampex 5100 Video Tape Recorder (VTR) was used to video-record the interactions in Studio A. A SPecial Effects Generator (SEC) was used which allowed for the blending of the images from the two cameras so they aPpeared side-by-side on a single picture. The video Signal from the cameras in Studio C and the audio signal from the microphone in Studio A was cabled into the Special Effects Generator, and this was recorded on the videotape llsing the video-tape recorder. Supervisors Supervision of all trainees receiving the experimental treatment tasks was conducted by various members of the I.P.R. 100 staff. All staff members were currently enrolled in the pro- gram leading to the degree of Ph.D. in Counseling, in the Department of Counselor Education, Michigan State University, had completed at least two terms of Practicum in Counseling at the Counseling Center at Michigan State University, and were selected to be Interns at the Counseling Center the next academic year or were currently Interns. All super- visors had had previous experience with I.P.R., and were trained to use the various I.P.R. techniques,1 (including the role of clinical interrogator for use with simulation materials and stimulated videotape recall), and therefore had a common frame of reference regarding the approach to take in the tasks they were to perform. Equality of supervisor skill, background and training with I.P.R., and level of effectiveness was assured for each particular deveIOpmental task. The supervisors had the same amount of duty and re- sponsibility for both experimental groups. M Twenty students enrolled in the Masters level course in Procedures ip.Counseling during the Winter term, 1970, at A manual for training supervisors to function as clinical interrogators for the I.P.R. technique can be found in Appendix A. 101 Michigan State University, served as subjects (the course, Education 816D was offered through the College of Educa- tion, M.S.U.). The subjects were assigned to one of two groups: Group 1: this group received the experimental treatment (IPR) for the first half of the term, and received the control (the traditional classroom) treatment the second half of the term. Group 2: this group received the control (the traditional classroom) treatment for the first half of the term, and received the experimental treatment (IPR) for the second half of the term. Demographic variables were obtained on the sample com- prising each group. Table 3.3, page 102, synthesizes the data according to groups. The trainees participating in the present study as subjects were at varying stages of training, education, age, and experience. Further it was evident that not all trainees, even at the same stage of training could perform with the same degree of counseling skills. No attempt was made to equate the two groups on the variables of sex, age, and experience, but none had as yet taken a counseling prac- ticum. Because trainees were randomly assigned to one group or another, it was postulated that any difference would distribute themselves evenly over the two groups. 102 TABLE 3.3 Comparison Of Subjects On The Demographic Variables Of Sex, Age, And Education Level In Groups 1 and 2 GROUP 1 GROUP 2 Sex: # Males 3 5 # Females 7 5 Age Range In Years: Males 25-28, ave. age 27 22-34, ave. age 27 Females 22-32, ave. age 25 22-45, ave. age 28 Education: Highest Degree Attained #Bachelors 8 8 #Masters 2 2 Instrumentation Three instruments were used to measure trainee effective- ness in counseling (discrimination and communication skills). The Affect Sensitivity Scale (ASS), Counselor Verbal Response Scale (CVRS), and Carkhuff's Empathic Understanding Scale (EUS) served as criterion measures. The Affect Sensitivity Scale served to measure the ability of the trainees to dif- ferentiate and discriminate client states; the Counselor Verbal Response Scale and Empathic Understanding Scale served as measures of the extent to which the trainees were able to communicate with clients along a facilitative-inhibitive con- 103 tinuum. Affect SensitivitypScale (ASS) Affect sensitivity refers to a persons ability to detect and describe the immediate affective state of another indiv- idual. The Affect Sensitivity Scale was developed by Camp- bell (1967) and Kagan pp, El, (1967), to measure an individuals' reactions to experiences designed to focus on sensitivity to interpersonal situations and dynamics. The instrument con- sists of 89 videotaped excerpts from actual counseling inter- views. Persons taking the test are asked to try to identify what feelings the client on the screen felt toward themselves and toward the counselor they were working with at the end of each excerpt; a test booklet with alternative choices is provided and answer sheets are provided on which answers to the choices were marked, and scored by I.B.M. Results were obtained on reliability and validity studies conducted with Form B of the Affect Sensitivity Scale. The following conclusions were drawn: 1. For most somewhat heterogeneous groups (such as practicing school counselors and most groups of individuals entering counselor education programs), the reliability of Form B was above .70.1 l A Kuder-Richardson Formula Reliability Check (Formula 20) was .69 for the pretest and .74 for the posttest, and the pre-post test correlation was .76 (Kagan, g5, g1,, 1967, p. 200). 104 Some indication of the content validity of the scale was gleaned from results of various item analysis and other internal analysis data, from the procedures used in deve10ping the scale. The scales' concurrent validity was demonstrated in a low but positive relationship between scale scores and the usually more subjective measures of coun- selor effectiveness. The average correlation obtained across all studies was +326, with a high of +342 and a low of +.l6. A somewhat more sub- stantial relationship was found to exist between scale scores and subjective measures of affective sensitivity. The average correlation obtained across all studies was +.38, with a high +.64 and a low of -.10. From B measured change in individuals affective sensitivity resulting from counselor training pro- grams and was unaffected by the practice effect of pretesting and subsequent posttesting on the same instrument. A construct validity study was done to determine if individuals' Form B scale scores would increase during a counselor education pro- gram. A small but significant amount of growth occurred between pre and post measurements. Table 3.4 presents the data resulting from the administra- tion of Form B to counselor education students. 105 TABLE 3.4 Data Resulting From The Administration Of Form B On A Pretest And Posttest Basis To Sample Groups Of Counselor Education Students (Kagan, et. al., 1967) Sample N Pretest Posttest Mean .E Group Mean Mean Difference Value J 26 51.35 54.08 2.73 2.21* K 30 54.26 57.03 2.77 2.80** *Significant at .025 level, one-tail test (df=25, £_value of 2.06 = .025 level) **Significant at .005 level, one-tail test (df=29, p value of 2.76 = .005 level) 5. Counselors rated as effective by peers tended to score high on the scale; those rated as ineffective tended to score either high or low. High scores on the ASS may be a necessary but not a sufficient condition for counselor effectiveness. While some questions were raised concerning the reliability and validity of the ASS, the authors of this scale concluded:1 "In summary, given our present state of knowledge in deve10ping procedures to measure interpersonal sensitivity and given 1Form C of the Affective Sensitivity Scale was used for the present study. This form contained the 67 items which have held up "best" (on item analyses) throughout the years. The authors of this scale indicated that reducing the number of items did not reduce the reliability and validity of the in- strument . 106 that the face validity of the procedures used in this research are accepted, then it can be generally stated that scale Form B is presently the 'best' means available for measuring the psychological trait of affective sensitivity" (Kagan, et. al., 1967, p. 191). Counselor Verbal Response Scale CXRS. The CVRS consists of 5 forced choice, dichotomous dimen- sions measuring the extent to which counselors' reSponses in counseling are characterized by the following: affective- cognitive responses (whether a counselors response refers to any affective component of a client's communication or con- cerns itself primarily with the cognitive components of that communication); understanding-non-understanding responses (whether a counselors reSponse communicates to the client that the counselor understands or is seeking to understand the client's basic communication); Specific-nonspecific reSponses (whether the counselors response delineates the client's problems and is central to the client's communica- tions or whether the reSponse does not specify the client's concerns; in essence, it describes whether the counselor deals with the client's communication in a general, vague, or peripheral manner, or "zeroes in” on the core of the client's communication); exploratory-pppexploratory reSpon- ses (whether a counselors response permits or encourages the client to explore his cognitive or affective concerns, or whether the response limits a client's exploration of these concerns); and the effectiveness-poneffectiveness of each 107 response (ratings on this dimension may be made indepently of ratings on the other four dimensions of the scale; this rating is based solely upon the judges' professional opinion and impression of the appropriateness of the counselors re- sponse-how adequately does the counselors response deal with the client's verbal and nonverbal communication). The CVRS focuses on a series of individual client/coun- selor units (client statement-counselor reSponse) during the course of the interview, rather than on global ratings of entire interviews or of longer interview segments. This was based on a format developed by DeRoo and Rank (1965). Typically, the judge is required to rate 20 consecutive counselor responses.to client statements drawn from the interview, on each of the five dimensions of the scale. After 20 counselor reSponses have been dichotomized on each dimen- sion, totals are obtained. Thus, a maximum score of 20 and a minimum of O is possible for each dimension.1 The development of the CVRS came as a result of research generated through use of the IPR technique. Reliability and validity studies were conducted on this instrument, to ascer- tain the efficacy of its use for evaluating counselor effective- ness. The following is a synOpsis of some relevant findings concerning the CVRS: 1The entire CVRS, a manual for training judges, and scoring sheet is found in Appendix A. 108 Religbility Using Hoyts analysis of variance technique in deter- mining interjudge reliability of two sets of judges who had rated sessions of counselors interviewing coached clients, coefficients were obtained by Griffin (1966), of average tape interjudge reliabilities of .84 (affect- cognitive), .80 (understanding-nonunderstanding), .79 (specific-nonspecific), .68 (exploratory-nonexploratory), and .79 (level of effectiveness-noneffectiveness), on the dimensions of the scale. Validit Data collected supported the validity of the dimen- Sions of the scale for characterizing counselors at different levels of ability. On each dimension of the scale, significant differences were found at the .01 level between reSponses of Ph.D. candidates and M,A. candidates with the Ph.D. candidates having more re- sponses rated as affective, understanding, specific, exploratory, and effective. Table 3.5 below summarizes the comparisons made between Ph.D. and M.A. level coun- selors on the dimensions of the CVRS (Kagan, 9;. al., 1967, p. 89): 109 TABLE 3.5 Comparison Of Mean Scores Of The Ph.D. And M.A. Counselor Candidates On The Five Dimensions Of The CVRS Dimension N Mean N Mean 5. p Ph.D. 'jLéLL Affective 8 10.04 45 4.33 3.401 .005 Understanding 8 14.08 45 9.66 2.164 .025 Specific 8 10.04 45 5.50 2.51 .01 Exploratory 8 14.17 45 8.56 3.07 .005 Effective 8 12.42 45 6.99 2.646 .01 Empathic Understanding In Interpersonal Processes II Scale (EUS) According to Truax and Carkhuff: "Accurate empathy involves more than just the ability of the therapist to sense the client or patient's 'private world' as if it were his own. It also involves more than just his ability to know what the patient means. Ac- curate empathy involves both the therapist sensitivity £p_current feelings and his ygr: bal facility pp_commupigate this understand- ipg.in a language attuned to the clients current feelings".1 (Truax and Carkhuff, 1967) 1The underlined passages are authors' italicizing. 110 The Empathic Understanding Scale has been derived in part from "A Scale for the measurement of accurate empathy" by Truax (Truax and Carkhuff, 1967) and in part from an earlier Version of this scale (Carkhuff and Berenson, 1967). These Scales have been validated in extensive research on counseling ade psychotherapy. The scale used in the present study1 is cOmprised of five levels of communication. Level 1.indi- <2ates that the verbal and behavioral expressions of the first person either Qp_not attend pp.or detract significantly from the verbal and behavioral expressions of the second person(s). Leyel_2_indicates that while the first person responds to the expressed feelings of the second person(s), he does so in such a way that he subtracts noticeable affect from the com- munications of the second person(s). Level §_indicates that the expressions of the first person in reSponse to the ex- pressed feelings of the second person(s) are essentially interchangeable with those of the second person(s). Level 4_ indicates that the reSponses of the first person add noticeably to the expressions of the second person(s). Level §_indicates that the first persons responses add significantly to the 2 feeling and meaning of the expressions of the second person(s). 1A complete COpy of this scale is found in Appendix B. 2For comparative"purposes, Level 1 of the EUS is approxi- mately equal to Stage 1 of the Truax scale. The remaining levels are approximately correSpondent: Level 2 and Stages 2 and 3 of the earlier version; Level 3 and Stages 4 and 5; Level 4 and Stages 6 and 7; Level 5 and Stages 8 and 9. 111 Table 3.6 below is a summary of the findings of Truax and Carkhuff (1967) on the therapeutic effectiveness of accurate empathy, as gleaned from research on hospital, de- linquent, and outpatient populations. TABLE 3.6 Findings On The Therapeutic Effectiveness Of Accurate Empathy 'um '00) a: 33 S3 2 '33- “53%" o g d 'g m-Hoa “H 0) HI!) TypeOf S ogwg eggs)“ Treatment ”4 :: gag : gggg 0 cu Hu cu u- HUOO HUOOV c3 §“$% “WERE z <3<5:n:= <§4.41 Comparisons of pre to mid to post changes in group raw score means and in gain score means on the ASS between groups 1 and 2 over time are found in Table 4.2 on page 123, During the first half of the course (Mid-Pre), group 1 gained an average 3.40 points (38.20 to 41.60 with IPR training), while group 2 lost an average of -.10 points (43.70 to 43.60 with traditional training). Over the full sequence of train- ing treatments (Post-Pre), group I gained an average of 3.80 points (38.20 to 42.00) and group 2 gained an average of 2.80 points. 123 ’ TABLE 4.2 Comparison Of Group Raw Score And Gain Score Means For Pre To Mid To Post Treatment Measurements Over The Sequence Of Treatments On The ASS Between Groups 1 And 2 Pre Mid Mid- Post Post- Treat- Train- Treat Pre Trains Treat- Pre ent ing ment Mean ing ment Mean easure Model Measure Gain Model Mbasure Gein Group 1 N = 10 38.2 IPR 41.6 3.40 Trad 42.0 3.80 Group 2 N = 10 43.7 Trad 43.6 - .10 IPR 46.5 2.80 Figure 4.1 is a graphic representation of the mean gain scores on the ASS for groups 1 and 2 over pre, mid, and post measures during training and is shown on page 124. The data shows that although more gain took place on the ASS during the IPR training phase than the Traditional training phase within groups 1 and 2 (3.40 vs .40 for group 1; 2.90 vs -.10 for group 2), and between groups 1 and 2 over the two time sequences (3.40, group 1 vs -.10 group 2, 2.90, group 2 vs .40 group 1, post-mid) no significant differences were found between the two groups from pre to mid to post. In order to further explain the failure to achieve significance between groups on the A88, a scattergram is presented in Figure 4.2, page 125, to Show the dispersion of mean gain scores for each individual in groups 1 and 2 over time intervals 1 and 2. 124 FIGURE 4.1 Mean Gain Scores For Groups 1 And 2 Over Three Measures During Training 4 3 ASS Mean 2 Gain Scores 1 0 -l T I 1 Pre Mid Post Time Legend G1 = Mid-Pre-IPR Post-Mid-Traditional G2 """"""""" Mid-Pre-Traditional Post-Mid-IPR 125 FIGURE 4.2 Scattergram Of The Individual Gain Score Means Obtained On The ASS Over Time (Mid-Pre) Between Groups 1 (IPR) and 2 (Traditional) During Training 12 10 Legend X = IPR ' IV 1.65 2 4 6 8 10 12 = Traditional Time Interval = Mid-Pre; Group Mean = 1.65 126 Individuals in Group 1 (+) were expected to Show the most gain during the Mid-Pre time interval, in which time they received IPR training. Consequently, most of the gains achieved by individuals in group 1 were expected to fall somewhere in quadrant II. Individuals in group 2 (0') were not expected to show much change during the Mid-Pre time interval, in which time they received Traditional training. Consequently, most of the changes by individuals in group 2 were expected to fall somewhere in quadrant III. AS indicated in the scattergram in Figure 4.2, while most changes were in the posited direction, a wide variability existed in the amount of mean gain for each individual within each of the groups (e. g. in group 1, one individual gained 12 points, another lost 7 points; in group 2, one individual gained 6 points, another lost 7 points), which may have been a possible cause of the failure to achieve significance between groups. Counselor Verbal Resppnse Scale (CVRS) And Empathic Under- standing Scale (EUS) An analysis of variance of the mean gain scores between groups 1 and 2 on the subscales of the CVRS and EUS over the two time intervals (Mid-Pre, Post-Pre) was conducted under each of the two interview conditions (coached client, role play). 127 A. Coached Client Condition The results of the analysis of variance for re- peated measures on the mean gain scores on the CVRS and EUS obtained by groups 1 and 2 over time under the coached client condition are presented in Table 4.3 on page 128. For a one tail test of significance with l, 18 degrees of freedom, an F ratio of 4.41 was necessary in order to reject the null hypotheses of no differences between the various effects beting tested. Significant differences were found on the main effect for subscales and on the groups* intervals* sub- scales interaction. A Scheffe post hoc analysis was conducted and the results are presented in Tables 4.4 and 4.5. TABLE 4.4 Scheffe Post Hoc Analysis Between Groups 1 And 2 On Each Of The Subscales For The Mid-Pre Time Interval Under The Coached Client Condition A Group 1 Group 2 Totgl \N’ p Affective 2.950 2.150 2.550 .800 ns Understanding 5.400 3.500 4.450 1.900 p (.05 Specific 5.450 3.500 4.475 1.950 p (.05 Exploratory 5.200 3.250 4.225 1.950 p4(.05 EUS Level 0.420 0.324 0.372 0.096 ns (Q - 1.660) (“'9 <(Q+ 1.660) 128 TABLE 4.3 Results Of The Analysis Of Variance For Between Group Differences In Mean Gain Scores On The CVRS And EUS Over Time Under Coached Client Condition Sources of Variance Sum Sq. df Mean Sq, F-ratio A_p Main Effects Groups Intervals Subscales Interactions Groups* Intervals Groups* 13.745 1.056 527.503 173.632 3.667 l—" 13.744 1.056 131.875 173.631 0.916 0.153 0.018 16.375 2.708 0.114 HS IlS p (.05 ns IlS Subscales Intervals* Subscales Groups* Intervals* Subscales 2.841 1 0.716 0.206 ns 70.043 1 15.226 4.608 p (.05 Error Terms Subjects (Groups) Intervals* 1612.879 18 89.064 1155.085 18 64.171 Subjects (Groups) Subscales* Subjects (Groups) Intervals* Subscales* Subjects (Groups) Total 581.976 245.320 4388.116 18 18 199 8.083 3.304 at 1, 18 df, p<.05 if F74.41 129 Tables 4.4 and 4.5 contain comparisons of groups 1 and 2 on each of the subscales for the two time intervals (Mid- Pre, Post-Pre) under the coached client, interview condition, using a Scheffe post hoc analysis. Under.the coached client condition for Interval 1 (Mid-Pre), group 1 (IPR) gained significantly more (.05 level) on the understanding, specific, and exploratory subscales than group 2 (Traditional); no significant differences were found (.05 level) between the two groups in mean number of responses gained on the affective subscale, and on the level of the effectiveness for accurate empathy (EUS) subscale. TABLE 4.5 Scheffe Post Hoc Analysis Between Groups 1 And 2 On Each Of The Subscales For The Post-Pre Time Interval Under The Coached Client Condition Total Q P Group 1 Group 2 Affective 1.300 2.750 2.025 -1.450 n5 Understanding 2.650 5.950 4.300 -3.330 p 41.05 Specific 2.650 5.950 4.300 -3.330 p (,.05 Exploratory 2.650 6.250 4.450 -3.600 p <~.05 EUS Level 0.126 0.415 0.270 -0.289 ns (‘1’ -1.660) «‘1’ < (Q+1.660) 130 Under the coached client condition for Interval 2 (Post-Pre), group 2 (Traditional - IPR sequence) gained Significantly more (.05 level) on the understanding, specific, and exploratory subscales than group 1 (IPR - Traditional sequence); no significant differences were found (.05 level) between the two groups in mean number of responses gained on the affective subscale and on the level of effectiveness for accurate empathy subscale. Table 4.6, on page 131, contains a comparison of the group raw score means on the four subscales of the CVRS and the subscale of level of effectiveness for accurate empathy (EUS) under the coached client condition, from pre to mid to post time intervals between groups 1 and 2. \ Figure 4.3, on page 132 and 133, is a graphic represen- tation of the group raw score means (which were contained in Table 4.6) on the four subscales of the CVRS and the subscale of level of effectiveness for accurate empathy (EUS) under the coached client condition, from pre to mid to post time intervals between groups 1 and 2. As seen in Figure 4.3, group 2 began training at a slightly higher level on each of the subscales for the coached client interview than group 1. During the mid-pre interval, in which time group 1 received IPR training and group 2 received traditional train- ing, both groups increased on all subscales, but group 1 gained significantly more on the understanding, specific, and 131 om.H Hm.H mH.H mN.HH mN.w oo.m oo.oH m¢.w mm.¢ om.oH ms.w mm.¢ om.m mm.N om. N msomo mmH muse mmH some mmH emuH mmH papa mmH ewes RN.H mm.H «H.H mm.m om.w om.m ow.m oo.w mH.m ow.m mm.m mH.m mw.H om.m mm. H macaw ewes MmH ewes mmH mesa MmH mesa mmH cosy MmH umom eHz mum umom eHz mum umom pH: mum umom sz mum umom UHZ mum mam mmodo>Huoommm Ho Hm>oH zuoumuoHaxm OHHHooam mchdmOmuopas O>Huoomw< oEHH uo>o N pd< H mazouo cmozuom COHqucoo ucoHHo pocomoo 629 Home: monomnsm O>Hm O58 so memo: o.s mHmHHummm< mEHH uo>o N pc< H masoHo som3umm aOHqucoo udeHo posomoo pops: monomndm o>Hm ozH do mcmoz muoom 3mm anode mo coHumUCommuamM onmmHu m.¢ MMDUHM 133 95g. OEHH. ”Jon NH: mam swam EH: wum H N s muse: ouoom 0 3mm m 0H NH Hm>mH mDm wm0H§QHnHXm 134 exploratory subscales than group 2. At the midpoint, the groups switched treatments, with group 2 now receiving the IPR training and group 1 now receiving the Traditional training. Group 2 continued to increase on all subscales, while group I dropped on all the subscales. As a result, Group 2 gained significantly more on the understanding, specific, and explora- tory subscales from pre to post than group 1. B. Role Play Condition The results of the analysis of variance for repeated measures on the mean gain scores on the CVRS and EUS obtained by groups 1 and 2 over time under the role play conditions are presented in Table 4.7 on page 135. For a one tail test of significance with l, 18 degrees of freedom, an F-ratio of 4.41 was necessary in order to reject the null hypotheses of no differences between the various effects being tested. Significant differences were found on the groups* intervals and groups* intervals* subscales interactions. A Scheffe post hoc analysis was conducted, and the results are presented in Tables 4.8 and 4.9. Tables 4.8 and 4.9 contain comparisons of groups 1 and 2 on each of the subscales for the two time intervals (Mid-Pre, Post-Pre) under the role play interview condition, using a Scheffe post hoc analysis. Under the role play interview condition for Interval l (Mid-Pre), group 1 (IPR) gained significantly more (.05 level) 135 TABLE 4.7 Results Of The Analysis Of Variance For Between Group Differences In Mean Gain Scores On The CVRS And EUS Over Time Under The Role Play Condition Sources of Variance Sum Sq. df Mean qu7 F-ratio p Main Effects Groups Intervals Subscales Interactions Groups* Intervals Groups* Subscales Intervals* Subscales Groups* Intervals* Subscales Error Terms Subjects (Groups) Intervals* Subjects (Groups) Subscales* Subjects (Groups) Intervals* Subscales* Subjects (Groups) 131.771 105.968 35.088 303.861 30.883 18.590 60.576 2133.802 533.846 661.197 186.059 H l8 18 18 18 131.771 105.968 8.772 303.861 7.721 4.648 15.144 118.544 29.658 9.183 2.258 1.028 3.577 0.967 10.263 0.846 1.840 5.868 HS HS ns p (.05 118 RS p (c05 Total 4201.640 199 at 1, 18 df, p(.05 if F74.4l 136 TABLE 4.8 Scheffe Post Hoe Analysis Between Groups 1 And 2 On Each Of The Subscales For The Mid-Pre Time Interval Under The Role Play Condition Group 1 Groupygi Total \i’ p Affective 3.150 -2.500 .325 5.650 p (.05 Understanding 2.550 -2.100 .225 4.650 p (.05 Specific 2.900 -2.100 .400 5.000 p <.05 Exploratory 2.450 -2.250 .100 4.700 p‘<.05 EUS Level 0.195 -0.248 -.026 0.443 ns (47- 1.660) ( KP ((Q+ 1.660) on the affective, understanding, specific, and exploratory subscales than group 2 (Trad.); no significant differences were found (.05 level) between the two groups on the level of effectiveness for accurate empathy (EUS) subscale. Under the TABLE 4.9 Scheffe Post Hoc Analysis Between Groups 1 And 2 On Each Of The Subscales For the Post-Pre Time Interval Under The Role Play Condition Group 1 Group 2 Total d;’ p Affective 1.950 2.700 -.375 -0.750 ns Understanding 1.550 2.800 -.125 -1.250 ns Specific 1.400 2.100 -.350 -0.700 ns Exploratory 1.050 2.450 -.700 -l.400 ns EUS Level 0.248 0.357 -.055 -0.109 ns (‘35- 1.66O)<\\’ «(9+ 1.660) 137 role play interview condition for Interval 2 (Post-Pre), there were no significant differences (.05 level) between the two groups in mean number of responses gained on any of the subscales. Table 4.10, page 138, contains a comparison of the group raw score means on the four subscales of the CVRS and the subscale of level of effectiveness for accurate empathy (EUS) under the role play interview condition 1 from pre to mid to post time intervals between groups 1 and 2. Figure 4.4, pages 139 and 140, is a graphic representa- tion of the group raw score means which were contained in Table 4.10) on the four subscales of the CVRS and the sub- scale of level of effectiveness for accurate empathy (EUS) under the role play condition, from pre to mid to post time intervals between groups 1 and 2. As seen in Figure 4.4, group 2 began training at a slightly higher level on each of the subscales for the role play interview than group 1. During the Mid-Pre interval, in which time group 1 received IPR training and group 2 received traditional training, group 1 increased on all the subscales and group 2 dropped on all the subscales, with group 1 gaining significantly more on the affective, under- standing, specific, and exploratory subscales than group 2. At the midpoint, the groups switched treatments, with group 2 now receiving the IPR training and group 1 now receiving the traditional training. Group 2 reversed its downward 138 NH.N Hm.H ok.a om.oH om.m mk.w ow.m os.m oh.k om.HH om.m oo.m oa.w om.m oo.6 N emcee mmH ease and anus an sane nnH sane mmH sane Na.u k6.H as.H om.m om.m mm.s om.m oo.k cu.s oH.o OH.“ mm.s ow.s oa.m mm.~ H gnome papa MmH pduH mmH pduH mmH omHH mmH uduH mmH anon was. was umom pHZ mum unom use was anon SH: 6am anon as: mum mam mmoco>Huoowwm mo Ho>oH muoumuonxm uuuuomnm waHpamumuopcD m>unnnuu< oEHH uo>o N mam H mdsouo som3uom coHqucoo mem-OHom sea Home: monOmHSm o>Hm may do memo: ouoom 3mm aaouw mo COmHummfiou OH. e MHmo N pc< H mdsouu amoBuOm GOHUdeoo mmHm wHom Home: mmHmomnsm o>Hm 0:9 do masoz ouoom 3mm asouu mo GOHumuaOmouaom oHnamuu q.¢ mmDon 140 omom mafia use mum N U s so ease \ 7‘ .s s C x x \ Hm>oH mDm anon use and mess (\l .\ wMOH¢MOmem N s memos ouoom 0 3mm m 0H NH 141 Slope, and gained on all the subscales, and group 1 dropped on all the subscales. As a result, there were no pre to post differences between the two groups in the mean gain on any of the subscales. Intercorrelations Of Judges' Ratings Intercorrelations between judges' ratings for coached- client and role-play interview tapes over pre, middle and post measurement sessions were obtained on the dimensions of the CVRS (affective, understanding, specific, exploratory) and the EUS (level of effectiveness for accurate empathy), and are presented in Tables 4.11 and 4.12. TABLE 4.11 Intercorrelations Of Rater l and Rater 2 For CVRS And EUS MeaSures Made At Each Of Three Times For Role Play Interviews PRE MID POST CVRS A .972 .962 .992 U .977 .979 .997 S .977 .981 .971 E .985 .968 .992 EUS level .901 .961 .993 142 TABLE 4.12 Intercorrelations Of Rater 1 And Rater 2 For CVRS And EUS Measures Made At Each Of Three Times For Coached Client Interviews PRE MID POST CVRS A .940 .979 .950 U .939 .971 .943 S .939 .974 .943 E .935 .979 .930 EUS level .901 .928 .911 Because the CVRS and EUS require judges to describe counselor responses, inter-correlations of judges' ratings across responses was computed. Comparisons between the groups of counselors were made in terms of the gain in each counselors' modal interview response pattern (i.e., - the average number of responses rated affective, understanding, Specific, exploratory, and the average level of effective- ness for accurate empathy) between pre- middle- post measure- ment sessions. Summary The hypotheses postulated in this study were tested within an analysis of variance for repeated measures frame- wOrkon the gain scores obtained on the ASS, and on the four Subscales of the CVRS and the level of effectiveness for accllrate empathy (EUS) subscale over coached-client and role- pla); interview conditions. 143 The hypotheses related to differences between groups 1 and 2 on the ASS, and on each of the subscales under the two interview conditions over time are summarized in Table 4.13, page 144. For intervals 1 and 2 (Mid-Pre, Post-Pre), there were no differences between groups 1 and 2 on the amount of mean gain on the ASS. For interval 1 (Mid-Pre), the IPR trained group gained a Significantly greater number of understanding, specific, and exploratory responses than the traditionally trained group for the coached client condi- tion, and significantly more affective, understanding, specific, and exploratory responses than the traditionally trained group for the role-play condition. For interval 2 (Post-Pre), the group receiving the Traditional-IPR training sequence gained a significantly greater number of understanding, specific, and exploratory responses than the group receiving the IPR-Traditional training sequence for the coached client condition; there was no difference between the two training sequences in number of responses gained on any of the subscales for the role-play condition. Summary Of Hypotheses Related To Between Group Differences 144 TABLE 4.13 On The Criterion Measures Over Time Hypotheses p Ho a ASS, Mid- Pre, Group l-Group 2 b ASS, Post-Pre, Group l-Group 2 Ho a C.C. condition, Mid-Pre, Group l-Group 2 A scale ns b U scale p ( .05 c S scale p (.05 d E scale p (.05 e L scale ns f C. C. condition, Post-Pre, Group l-Group 2 A scale ns g U scale p (.05 h 8 scale p (.05 i E scale p (:05 j L scale ns k R.P. condition, Mid-Pre, Group l-Group 2 A scale p (305 1 U scale p (.05 m S scale p (.05 n E scale p<.05 o L scale ns p R.P. condition, Post-Pre, Group l-Group 2 A scale ns q U Scale ns r S scale ns S E scale ns t L scale ns CHAPTER V SUMMARY AND IMPLICATIONS Sew-311 The purpose of this Study was to determine the effects of two different counselor training experiences on the sub- sequent discrimination and communication counseling behaviors of counselor candidates. The first training procedure was an adaptation of the Interpersonal Process Recall technique (IPR) developed by Kagan e3, g1,, (1967), within a develop- mental task framework modified from a study by Goldberg (1967). The second procedure was a traditional classroom counselor training experience using lectures, demonstrations, and dis- cussions. In previous research using IPR to supervise counselors, Kagan e3, 31, combined stimulated videotape recall procedures with a clinical interrogator to heighten a client's ability to recall his covert feelings during the counseling session. Later, the counselor listened to an audiotape of the client recall session, viewed a videotape replay of the session, and was supervised by the interrogator. Judges' ratings of the counseling effectiveness of counselors receiving video IPR, audio IPR, or traditional supervision indicated no significant differences in the effectiveness of counselors 145 146 supervised by any of the three procedures. Goldberg, in reviewing this Study and related literature, posited that there was a need for a more developmental approach to prac- ticum supervision than the approach used in the Kagan study. In Goldberg's study basic recall techniques were expanded to include both client and counselor recall and were integrated into a sequential series of training experiences based on a counselor developmental theory of supervision. Goldberg found that analysis of pre to post changes in coun- selor interview behavior within each group and between each group indicated significant differences in counselor per- formance on each dimension of the CVRS at the .001 level. It was anticipated that the questions "Could some of the newer techniques developed in a laboratory setting (by Goldberg, Kagan, pp, g1, using IPR) be incorporated into the present training model?", and "Could the training model be applied practically within the tflme and financial limitations of an ongoing masters level program?" would be answered in this study. The sequential program of training experiences used by Goldberg was modified and incorporated into the overall design of the present study. The use of this approach to training stemmed from the assumption that there were a series of developmental tasks which faced the counselor trainees as they became counselors: (a) the need to become sensitive to and aware of own feelings, (b) the 147 nned to become sensitive to, aware of, and understanding of client communications, (c) the need to become sensitive to and aware of elements of effective communication in coun- seling, and (d) the need to become sensitive to and aware of the bilateral nature of the counseling relationship. The IPR treatment consisted of four integrated states which were de- signed to meet these developmental needs. The recall tech- niques which Goldberg used were also used and included client, counselor, and mutual recalls (both counselor and client present). Afect Simulation Films were added, and a number of training exercises were done in a group rather than on an individual basis. The trainees used in this study were 20 graduate students enrolled in the masters level course Procedures ip_Counseling offered at Michigan State University prior to any practicum experience. The subjects were randomly assigned to one of two group treatment sequences (10 students per group), with group 1 first receiving the IPR training and group 2 first receiving the traditional classroom method of teaching. At the midpoint of the course, the treatments were reversed, with group 1 now receiving the traditional method and group 2 receiving the IPR. The traditional method of classroom teaching consisted of lectures, discussions, and demonstrations by a professor in the Department of Counseling, Personnel Services, and Educational 148 Psychology at Michigan State University and by two guest lecturers in the following areas: "Counseling Theory - PurpOSes and Goals in Counseling", "Helping Students in Academic Difficulties”, "The Counselor as Consultant, Environmental Engineer, and Teacher of Occupational Infor- mation,” "Vocational Counseling", and ”Behaviorism." During the first stage of IPR training-becoming sensi- tive to and aware of own feelings - the group first met as a whole. The Affect Simulation Films were shown with the purpose of having trainees gain an awareness of their own feelings to situations depicted on film. This was followed by an individual meeting with each trainee for the purpose of having each gain more awareness of his own feelings. The Affect Simulation Films were shown, trainees were videotaped watching these films, and a stimulated recall of this video- tape was conducted to help trainees further identify feelings to these films and situations. In the second stage- becoming sensitive to,. aware of, and understanding of client communications--the group met as a whole. A brief lecture on verbal and non-verbal ways of communicating was presented; each individual in the group then had the opportunity to role play counselor and client, which was audiotaped. A stimulated recall of the audiotapes of the role-playing sessions was conducted in the group. In the third stage-- becoming aware of elements of effective communication in counseling-~the group again met as a whole. A lecture was 149 presented on the elements of effective communication behaviors; training audiotapes were played and trainees practiced rat- ing these tapes on the dimensions discussed. During the second phase of this stage, another group meeting was held for the purpose of having trainees further learn the elements of effective counseling. This was done by pairing trainees up, having them role-play, conducting a stimulated videotape recall of these role playing exercises in the group, and rating the interactions on the dimensions of effective commun- ication previously discussed. During the final stage, two individuals were paired up for the purpose of helping them begin to understand the bilateral nature of the counseling relationship. The first phase of this session was designated "counselor recall". One trainee role played counselor and the other trainee role played the client (for approximately 15 minutes). A stimulated videotape recall was then con- ducted for the trainee playing the counselor role, focusing on the dynamic interaction between the counselor and client in the role-playing session just conducted (lasting approx- imately 30-40 minutes). After this, the two trainees switch- ed roles, role-played (for approximately 15 minutes), and a stimulated videotape recall was then conducted for the trainee who played the counselor role this time. The second phase of this session was designated ”mutual recall". Each 150 trainee again alternated role playing counselor and client. A stimulated videotape recall was conducted this time for the two trainees together, focusing on the dynamic inter- action between the two of them as counselor and client in the role playing session just conducted. The interrogator encouraged the two trainees to share the thoughts and feel- ings each had about their relationship with each other. In order to test the research hypotheses, a pre-mid- post design was used, using the pre, mid, and post training sessions to sample the trainees' behavior on the ASS, and under two interview conditions (coached client and role play). The mid measure session was also the point in the course at which time the training sequences were reversed for the two groups. The basis hypotheses of this study were: Hypothesis I Trainees exposed to a program of specific developmental tasks in counselor education, implemented by using IPR techniques will dev- elop greater ability to discri- minate (and differentiate)affective stimuli and conditions than trainees exposed to a more traditional approach to counselor education. Hypothesis II Trainees exposed to a program of specific developmental tasks in counselor education, implemented by IPR techniques, will develop greater ability to communicate in a facilitative way than trainees exposed to a more traditional classroom approach to counselor education. 151 An analysis of variance for repeated measures was per- formed on the mean gain score changes from pre to mid to post on the criterion measures (ASS and on the CVRS and EUS under coached client and role play conditions), to ascertain whether there were differences (p<.05) between groups 1 and 2 based on the treatments they received. There were no significant differences (p (.05) between groups 1 and 2 on the ASS over the two time intervals (Mid-Pre and Post- Pre), with both groups showing significant gains over the Post-Pre time interval.1 On the coached client interview condition for the first half of the course (Mid-Pre) the IPR trained group gained significantly more understanding, specific, and exploratory responses (p(.05) than the traditionally trained group. On the role play interview condition, for the first half of the course, the IPR trained group gained significantly more affective, under- standing, specific, and exploratory responses than the traditionally trained group. Over the full training sequence (Post-Pre) under the coached client interview condi- tion, the group receiving the Traditional-IPR sequence gained significantly more understanding, specific, and explor- atory responses than the group receiving the IPR-Traditional There do appears to have been differences within each of the groups. See section on conclusions and interpreta- tions in this chapter. 152 sequence. Under the role play interview condition, there were no differences between the two training sequences (IPR first, Traditional second, or vice versa), on any of the subscales. Conclusions and Interpretations In sum, significant differences were found between groups 1 and 2 on the understanding, specific, and explora- tory subscales of the CVRS under the coached client interview condition, and significant differences were found between groups 1 and 2 on the affective, understanding, specific, and exploratory subscales of the CVRS under the role play interview condition when the IPR method was directly com- pared to the traditional method. No significant differences were found between the two groups on the amount of increase on the ASS and in the level of effectiveness for accurate empathy (EUS). However, despite the evidence of differences between groups and the lack of differences between groups on a number of measures, it might also be desirable to discuss the observed behavioral changes. The question of what constitutes a "meaningful" change on the ASS can be answered by referring to previous research which used the ASS as a criterion measure. In a study by Kagan g5, El: (1967, p. 180), a comparison of gain on the 153 l ASS was made on a pretest-posttest basis for each of two sample groups consisting of individuals attending masters degree full year NDEA Counseling and Guidance institutes at large midwestern universities . Mean differences pre to post for each of the groups (2.73 and 2.77) were found to be significant over a full year time period. Comparisons of the groups used in the present study with the groups used in the Kagan study can be warranted because both samples involved students who were at the masters level of training. Over a full year time period the NDEA groups gained an average of 2.73 and 2.77 points (Form B). Over the two four-week periods of IPR training in the present study, students gained an average of 3.40 and 2.90 points; over the full 10 week term, the groups gained an average of 3.80 and 2.80 points respectively (Form C). More gain was achieved for the groups in the present study, using a shorter time period, on a shorter form of the scale than for the groups in the Kagan study. Although no significant differences were found between groups 1 and 2 in this study in the amount of mean gain on the ASS over time intervals 1 (Mid-Pre) and 2 (Post-Pre), 1Form B, containing 89 items was used, more gain is needed to achieve significance using Form B than Form C (67 items). 2Refer to Table 3.4, page 105. 154 it might also be important to discuss the data in terms of within group changes on this scale. For the present study, a g-test of differences between means was performed to assess within group changes in mean number of points gained on the ASS. The results of this analysis are summarized in Table 5.1. Comparison Of Mid-Pre And Post-Pre Treatment Mean Gains On The ASS For Groups 1 And 2 TABLE 5.1 N Mean Gain t p Group 1 Mid-Pre 10 +3.40 8.21 .001 Post-Pre lO +3.80 4.46 .001 Group 2 Mid-Pre 10 -0.10 .02 ns Post-Pre 10 +2.80 2.00 .05 necessary: g .10 1.833 for 9 degrees of freedom A £_va1ue of 1.833 for a one tail test of significance with 9 degrees of freedom was necessary before chance differences within treatment groups could be rejected at the .10 level of confidence. Inspection of Table 5.1 reveals g values greater than 1.833 for each time interval except the Mid-Pre interval for group 2. training were effective in bringing about changes on the ASS It seems clear that both sequences of ‘within each group, most of which might be accounted for by 155 gains made during the IPR training period. However, despite this post hoc analysis, in which clear trends were noted, the hypotheses as stated about the ASS in this study were refuted. It might also be helpful to compare the interview behaviors of the trainees in this study with interview be- haviors of other counselor candidates at known levels of training on the four dimensions of the CVRS. Two sources of data were available for such a comparison: (1) the ratings of audiotape segments of 45 M.A. counselors involved in a study by Kagan gt. al., (1967, p. 89), and (2) the pre- treatment interview ratings of counselors participating in the Goldberg (1967) study. Since the interview situations were not directly comparable (the interview segments from the Kagan study represented interviews with coached clients while interview segments in the Goldberg study represented inter- views with high school students) no statistical comparisons were made. Finally, the small fl_involved in some of the possible comparisons dictated a descriptive comparison. A comparison was made of the pre-treatment interview txahavior of the 11 counselors in the Goldberg study (who had already completed practicum), the 25 counselors in the Goldberg study (who had not yet taken practicum), the interview be- havior of the 45 M.A. counselors (who had also completed practicum), with the two groups of 10 counselor trainees in the present study (who had not yet taken practicum) under eatfli of two interview conditions. 156 TABLE 5.2 Descriptive Comparison Of Counselors At Various Levels Of Training On Each Of The Four Dimensions Of The CVRS DIMENSIONS N Affective Understanding Specific Exploratory Practicum: 45 4.33 9.66 5.50 8.56 Practicum b 11 5.76 11.82 5.67 8.88 Non-Practicum 25 2.09 3.67 2.25 4.33 Group 1: 10 0.55 3.15 3.15 3.30 Group 1 10 1.85 5.80 5.80 5.95 Group 2: 10 0.80 4.95 4.95 5.00 Group 2 10 3.50 10.90 10.90 11.25 Group 1: 10 2.55 4.55 4.10 4.55 Group 1 10 4.50 6.10 5.50 5.50 Group 2: 10 6.00 8.00 7.70 8.75 Group 2 10 8.70 11.30 9.80 10.20 acounselors in study by Kagan, gt, a}, (1967) bcounselors in study by Goldberg (1967) pre-treatment c . . . . counselors in present study - coached client condition, pre treatment counselors in present study - coached client condition post- treatment e counselors in present study - role-play condition pre-treatment counselors in present study - role-play condition post-treatment 157 As indicated in Table 5.2 there were distinct similarities in the pattern of interview behaviors exhibited by counselors who had already completed a practicum experience. There were also clear differences between the interview behaviors of these trainees who had not had a practicum experience and the pre- treatment interview behaviors for groups 1 and 2 as compared to those counselors who did have a practicum (with the excep- tion of group 2 under the role-play condition). Comparisons of post treatment interviews of counselors in the present study under the two interview conditions with interview behavior of non-practicum trainees and of the M.A. candidates who had completed practicum yield the following observations: (1) The post training interview behaviors for both groups 1 and 2 in the present study under the coached client condition were described as more understanding, specific and exploratory than the non-practicum group in the Goldberg study.i Group 2 was described as being as affective, understanding, specific, and exploratory as the practicum groups in the Kagan and Goldberg studies. (2) The post training interview behaviors for both groups 1 and 2 in the present study under the role play con- ciition were described as more affective, understanding, specific, and exploratory than the non-practicum group in the (3oldberg study. Group 1 was described as being as affective arud specific as the practicum groups in the Kagan and Goldberg 158 studies. Although group 2 was described as slightly more affective, specific and exploratory than either practicum group on the pre-measure, they increased their scores on these dimensions after training and ended up more affective, under- standing, Specific, and exploratory than any of the other groups. There are several questions relative to the research design and methodology which must be considered in drawing conclusions and interpretations. One critical question can be raised regarding the extent to which possible supervision- teacher bias may have favorably influenced the outcome of the study. The possibility of this bias cannot be entirely ruled out, and there still exists the chance that the supervisor/ teacher's enthusiasm or preference for the IPR training was conveyed to the trainees, thereby affecting their motivation. However, two of the five lecture presentations were conducted by faculty members who did not know the hypotheses of the present study, and all those teaching the traditional classroom presentations were discussing areas of their own particular interests, using audiotapes and videotapes to augment their presentations and stimulate discussions. Further, the records of participation gave no evidence of greater interest and/or motivation between groups. Trainees in both groups regularly kept appointments even under adverse weather conditons, and all returned to the university after the course was over for 159 post-training assessment session. There was also written and verbal evidence from trainees that their experience had been positive and they had noticed changes both in their knowledge of counseling theory and their ability to counsel. While these reports are subject to a halo effect, they tend to suggest that supervisors and teachers both communicated their enthusiasm and interest to trainees regardless of the type of training offered. Another possible bias effect may be an increase in trainee motivation due to the novelty of the IPR treatment. An attempt was made in the design of the study to minimize this effect by reversing the training sequences so both groups received both of the treatments, but in different order. As a result of this, there were no observable reactions of feeling ”cheated”. Finally, since neither group knew what to expect from either of the treatments and had no expectations concerning the nature of the class, they could not gauge whether or not they were being deprived of a particular training experience. Another question can be raised concerning the impact of a "captured”, heterogeneous sample. All the trainees were enrolled in the course Procedures in Counseling for credit, came from varying fields within psychology and education, and were at varying stages of education, experience, and ability. Further, there may have been a number of individuals who were in the class simply to fulfill a departmental requirement for 160 a particular degree, and were unmotivated and uncommitted to the training tasks. The notion of the "captured" sample may explain the variability of scores and could possibly account for the variance in the amount of gain between the trainees (as seen, for example on the ASS), where the training may have had less impact on some of the less-motivated students. While this could undoubtedly affect their performance on the criterion measures, it was felt that the random assignment of individuals to one group or another served to distribute these differences evenly over the two groups. A number of questions may be raised about the procedures used to measure pre, mid, and post treatment interview be- haviors. One question centers around the adequacy of judges' ratings of a single portion of an interview for making judg- ments about a counselor's behavior. While there is no de- finite answer to this question, Hart (1961) found that a four- minute interview segment is as reliable a sample unit for rating a counselor's behavior as any other time unit. In the present study, a twenty response segment of the interview was used, which represented from 8-10 minutes of a 15 minute interview. The results of the validity study of the CVRS in which four minute interview segments were rated indicated that differences in counselor's interview behavior were observable within time segments shorter than those used to obtain the current results. 161 Another question concerns the use of coached clients and a role play interview condition for obtaining samples of trainee interview behaviors. It is quite possible that a counselor could ”look good” with an extremely verbal, moti- vated sensitive client and might experience difficulty with a more reluctant client. In the present study, each coached client was interviewed by two trainees from each group with clients being randomly assigned to trainees, to offset this possibility. Further, the trainees interviewed the same client on each of three occasions (pre, mid, post), so they would be compared on the same stimulus. While these questions also hold true for the role-play condition, trainees "took turns” counseling each other during each of the three assess- ment sessions, and it was expected that the problem of client "difficulty” or "cooperation” was randomly Spread out over the entire class. Another question might be raised regarding the general- izability of results obtained on coached client and role play interviews to interviews with real clients. It should be pointed out that none of the counselor candidates in the pre- sent study had as yet taken the formal practicum experience and it could not be assumed that some of them had a supervised experience elsewhere comparable to that offered at Michigan State University. Because of this, and because it might be considered unethical to have clients interviewed by a counselor 162 who might very well have a deleterious effect on the client, the use of real clients was ruled out. Since neophyte coun- selors do not or ethically should not be interviewing real clients at that level of training, there may be little justi- fication in obtaining their behavior on a real client. How- ever, since an in-interview sample was needed to assess trainee growth and development, it was concluded that the two interview situations used in the present study constituted a viable alternative to the use of real clients. In looking at the growth curves in Figures 4.3 and 4.4, one may be led to postulate that the decrease on the four subscales of the CVRS for group 1 (Mid to Pre) after IPR training may be due to forgetting or a lack of permanence of the behavior changes. It should be pointed out that group 2 also decreased on the four subscales of the CVRS during the time they received the traditional classroom training (Pre to Mid on the role play condition), leading to the speculation that impersonal nature of the classroom situation (compared with the experimental treatment) may have actually detracted from the trainees' learning to respond facilitatively in inter- personal situations. Indeed, it would be interesting to speculate about the possible effect of going from IPR train- ing directly into a supervised practicum. Finally, a question may be raised about having trainees "practice" counseling behaviors which the criterion instruments are designed to measure. It is important to point out that the 163 criterion measures used in the present study are not static, narrow and specific but are dynamic and complex in nature and cannot be learned by rote. Growth on these scales involves a dynamic integration of perceptual and behavioral components so that changes on these scales reflect not only changes in perceptiveness but also changes in basic interview style. In summary, then, the differences observed in the behaviors of counselors in the two treatment groups (signifi- cant at .05 level) strongly suggest that the sequential pattern of developmental tasks using IPR in this study is a viable and effective means of training counselors. Implications A discussion of some of the clinical observations of trainees during the treatment tasks may help not only to ex- plain the data (these clinical observations may help explain why the video procedures were more effective than the tradi- tional classroom training approach in changing a counselor's interview behavior) but may also serve to develop the implica- tions of this study for counselor education and future research. Clinical Observations In most instances, counselor educators, when devising a curriculum for training counselors have usually insisted that counselor candidates build a strong theoretical framework for understanding the counseling process before they are 164 allowed to have practicum, fieldwork, and internship experiences. It is not until trainees reach the practicum that they must make the transition from theory to practice. In a sense, then, the IPR curriculum devised for the present study differs from the traditional curriculum in that the developmental tasks using IPR begin to provide this bridge between theory and practice before the practicum. (The class in this study consisted exclusively of students who had not yet had a formal counseling practicum). In order to bridge this gap, it was decided that trainees should be presented with an experience designed to begin to teach them to recognize, understand, and have a "working” knowledge of the various elements which com- prise the counseling process, imbedded in an experiential context to give them a first-hand encounter with the process. For all trainees, the initial session (during which time they were assessed on the ASS, interviewed a coached client, and role-played) served to create an overall "attitude" that this course would involve actual interview and counseling situations as well as separate traditional theoretical presen- tations and discussions. After the initial coached client and role-play interviews a number of trainees were moved to indi- cate that they felt inadequate in the interview situation and did not know how to approach the clients. In a sense, then, this initial session served to ”prime" the trainees for the IPR experience. 165 In the first stage, trainees were exposed to situations which were designed to focus them on their own thoughts, feelings, and reactions to planned threatening situations (Affect Simulation Films).l This task was first for a number of reasons, and the assumptions underlying these reasons seemed to be borne out as their training progressed. It was posited that by focusing trainees on their own thoughts, feelings, and behaviors, they would be encouraged to express and deal with the feelings, fears, and expectations they experienced while watching the filmed interpersonal encounters. Trainee after trainee began to describe his own discomforts in interpersonal situations. The initial thoughts, anxieties, and fears trainees had about interview situations were also brought into awareness. They were frequently concerned about the impression they would make on a client, if they would appear adequate, and could they really be of help to clients. The expression of underlying thoughts and feelings seemed to help the trainees identify and explore the locus of some of their discomforts early in the training process. Further, by helping trainees deal with their concerns at the outset, it was possible to begin early to build on these experiences, At the beginning of this phase, a number of supervisors sat in while the group saw the Affect Simulation Films. A number of group members indicated that this made them feel un- comfortable; subsequently, no "extra" supervisors (outside of the group leaders) were present during training procedures. 166 helping trainees learn to use their thoughts and feelings to establish and maintain relationships with clients rather than be constricted by them. Finally, by helping trainees draw on their own experiences, they began to more accurately understand how clients might be reacting, carrying over to the next stage of training. In the second stage of training, counselors began to recognize and understand client communications. While they began to use some of their own experiences and resources (thoughts, feelings and reactions to the role play clients), and increased their ability to understand client communications, they still seemed a bit reluctant (or unable) to share their perceptions with clients. In stage three, the trainees were taught about specific dimensions of effective counselor/client communication and were shown a videotaped interview of what was expected of them in terms of these effective communication behaviors in counseling. This presentation seemed to tie the previous two experiences together by providing trainees with a focal point around which they could organize the experiences they had during the first two stages. In the final stage, trainees were again placed in interview situations which were videotaped. This time, stimu- lated videotape recalls (counselor and mutual) were conducted, with the trainees watching their interview behaviors and dis- cussing these behaviors and their reactions to them. During the counselor recall, the supervisor attempted to focus the 167 trainees on identifying the meanings of their own behaviors and their effects on the client and on the underlying meanings and implications of client behaviors. By drawing on the trainees' own thoughts and feelings on viewing the videotape replay, the supervisor encouraged the trainees to express and deal with their feelings, fears, and expectations during the counseling interviews. For example, counselors frequently noted their preoccupation with themselves, rather than with their clients. It seemed possible that this exploration in the interpersonal context helped the trainees overcome this preoccupation, "opening" them to receive a greater amount of client communi- cation. This further enabled the trainees to pay attention and thus learn more about clients - to achieve a greater awareness and understanding of the underlying implications of the clients verbal and non-verbal behaviors. Frequently, counselors commented during recall that "I backed off from that subject because it was touchy and I didn't want to turn him (client) away,” and "I didn't want to hurt him (client) because he might reject me." At times, trainees would recall that "I didn't know what he (client) was reacting to", and "I didn't know what to do in that situation", while almost in the same breath displaying considerable insight into the nature of the client's concerns. It often seemed that the trainees were afraid to act on their own perceptions for fear of taking a 168 risk and being proven wrong or inaccurate. During the mutual recall phase, both the client and counselor were present during the stimulated videotape recall, and they were encouraged to share their thoughts and feelings with each other (as they had previously done in counselor recall with the supervisor), permitting them to learn how to relate to each other about the ongoing impact each had on the other. During this phase, the trainees seemed able, often for the first time, to look at their relationship, to become aware of the mutual impact each had on the other, and came to recognize and to talk to each other about some of the difficulties they had in their "here and now” interpersonal communication. An answer to the question ”What caused trainees to change?" can now be speculated. As training progressed, and as counselors became involved in the interviews and recall sessions, it seemed that they began to realize that their perceptions could be in fact surprisingly accurate. When trainees saw that clients wondered how much they might dis- close without meeting rejection or misunderstanding from the counselor, the trainees may have themselves begun to under- stand their own fears of rejection, and they could interact with their clients without meeting. They also seemed to learn that clients were not as fragile as the trainees had expected them to be. 169 Viewing the recall sessions seemed to increase the trainees' awareness of the entire communication process during the interview. At the beginning, trainees focused almost entirely on the most overt, surface verbal and nonverbal dimen- sions of behavior. At the end of training, counselors were starting to recognize the deeper meanings and implications of both the clients and their own verbal and nonverbal behaviors. They noted instances of the looks of boredom as a client rambled on, their physically pulling back or moving forward when certain areas were discussed, and instances of discom- fort (fidgeting) during the interviews. These observations suggest that the focus on trainee dynamics, client dynamics, and the bilateral nature of the counseling relationship (coun- selor and mutual recall sessions) increased the trainees' awareness of a greater amount of client communication, began to sensitize trainees to their own dynamics during the coun- seling sessions, and began to make them more aware of the mutual impact and two way communication between counselor and client which occurred in counseling. Finally, while some of the trainees may have been aware of counseling theory previous to this course most had little previous coursework and they often had little or no knowledge of actual counseling practice. The elements of counseling discussed in stage three seemed to be basic dimensions of the counseling process, and helped give trainees a model of effective 170 interpersonal communication, making them more aware of the behavioral correlates of effective practice. An analysis of clinical observations may also help in understanding why some criteria measures failed to show dif- ferences between the groups or seemed inconclusive. There was a lack of significant change between the groups in the level of effectiveness for accurate empathy (EUS) over every train- ing segment and over the full sequence of treatments. There were inconclusive results on the affective subscale of the CVRS (in the coached client condition, there was no difference between the two groups on this subscale; in the role play con- dition, there was a significant difference), and on the ASS (although significant gains were made within the group re- ceiving the IPR training, there were no differences between the groups). During the IPR training, counselors seemed to realize that their perceptions could be accurate and often could be used to help them identify what clients were concerned about and experiencing. Further, trainees appeared to more accur- ately identify both verbal and nonverbal behaviors of both themselves and clients. Trainees were able to make more affective, understanding, specific, and exploratory "kinds" of responses after training, and there appeared to be a tendency for IPR training to increase the trainees' ability to recognize verbal and nonverbal communications; however, there was little 171 or no change noted in the level of effectiveness for accurate empathy (EUS) which was still far below the so- called "minimally facilitative" level (level 3). This leads to the speculation, based on observation, that while trainees may have been able to gain in ability to recognize and dis- criminate affective states in clients, they were not yet able to use these discriminations to communicate with clients at at least the same level that clients were experiencing the feelings. Trainees seemed to have learned to help clients understand, specify, and explore the cognitive but not necessarily the affective components of their concerns (i.e. - trainees may have sharpened their ability to commun- icate cognitively but not necessarily affectively with clients). This was borne out by the many trainee comments which re- flected that they were often able to "feel" what the client was feeling and experiencing, but were as yet unable to communicate and share this understanding with clients. There- fore, while there may be little question that the training procedures increased the incidence of affective, understanding, specific and exploratory responses in trainees' interview behavior, four weeks (approximately 20 hours) is not enough to significantly increase the level of a counselor candidates ability to communicate in an accurately empathic, facilitative way with clients. These neophyte counselors seemed to have many fears and questions concerning their adequacy as a counselor 172 and felt quite uncomfortable approaching anxiety situations. Because these beginning counselors often were preoccupied with their own feelings and thoughts, they may have been quite closed to receiving client communications, and it may take considerably more than four weeks to enable them to fully understand and resolve some of their fears and anxieties. Until they can become more aware of the meanings and impli- cations of their own behaviors, they may not be "freed-up" enough to respond in an accurately empathic, facilitative way with clients. This lends further credence to the belief (Chapters I and II) that self-awareness and understanding are important ingredients for the developing counselor to achieve. In essence, clinical observations tend to support the assumption that the developmental tasks using IPR have a positive impact on subsequent (ne0phyte) counselor behavior. The impact of the use of simulation materials, videotaped playback and recall, and discussion of elements of effective communication behaviors in counseling seemed to create an atmosphere in which trainees were able to change. The series of structured developmental tasks seemed to provide experiential learning situations in which trainees were able to develop a number of basic counseling skills. The treatment was clearly more effective than the traditional classroom methods for training counselors; however, certainly 173 more is needed than a four week experience to reach effec- tive levels of functioning. o o s or a' i In view of the changes in counselor behavior as a result of IPR training and the significant differences between these changes and the changes observed as a result of tradi- tional classroom learning, it is assumed that the IPR pro- cedures are a potentially important tool for use in programs of counselor training. The data presented in Chapter IV, in addition to being statistically significant, are meaningful when the interview behaviors are compared with that of counselors at various levels of training and experience. Finally, these changes should be considered in light of the brevity of the training sequences (two four-week training pro- grams). The results of this study seem to thus have implications for counselor education. Many programs of counselor education rest on the assumption that a body of counseling theory exists which the counselor candidates must learn prior to and counseling prac- ticum work. The possibility of placing IPR procedures early in graduate training programs is suggested by the findings of 174 this study. The data presented suggest that the learning of effective interpersonal communication skills may not depend on first learning counseling theory per se. During IPR training, both groups gained in mean number of responses on each of the subscales of the CVRS under both interview conditions; during traditional training, the groups did not. It can be argued that if the goal of counselor education is to train counselors to be able to respond facilitatively in interpersonal situations, it should follow that training procedures should be used which are most directly relevant and similar to the task itself -- namely practice in inter- personal situations. In the present study, the IPR proce- dures increased interpersonal skills in trainees while the traditional classroom techniques for educating counselors seemed to contribute little to this goal. The possibility exists that if trainees are exposed to an IPR program offered in conjunction with courses in theory, a bridge between theory and practice can be made at an early stage in training. It may also be argued that the longer trainees receive practical exposure and adequate supervision, the more likely they are to develop into competent counselors. The IPR procedures used in the study are practical (it was run by graduate students) and safe (real clients were not used). It seemed that much anxiety was generated because trainees lacked a model of effective counseling behaviors. 175 One possible way of dealing with this might be to incorporate videoviewing and rating experiences into the training pro- cedures which serve to prepare counselors for actual client contact. Trainees might be presented with tapes of coun- selors rated high, middle, and low on the dimensions of counselor/client communication. They might also tape their own interviews and rate them on these dimensions to pro- vide them with a means of assessing the changes in their own behaviors. Another implication for counselor training is suggested by the data from the CVRS. It is quite possible that the counselor candidates' patterns of behavior modify and change as they go through a counselor education program (especially during the practicum experience). While trainees may first begin by communicating some degree of understanding, and might be able to encourage client exploration, it appears that it is more difficult to respond to the affective components of a client's communication. If this is so, it seems that experiences which help trainees become more aware of and understanding of the affective themes which permeate be- havior, and which enable trainees to more adequately and successfully deal with affect in counseling should be in- cluded early in counselor preparation. If trainees have difficulty responding to affect, they may also be helped by involvement in a group or individual counseling exper- ience and/or individual supervision, in conjunction with 176 the other counselor education experiences. As implemented in this study, the IPR training experiences did not include any of the group and individual procedures common in prac- ticum training, nor did it allow for extensive feedback from colleagues. If self-understanding is one goal of these experiences, it seems possible that the design used in this study could be expanded, using more videotaped confronta- tions and physiological feedback as a stimulus for personal exploration. In retrospect, the questions posed at the beginning of this chapter regarding the possibility of incorporating newer techniques into a training model within the financial and time limitations of an on-going masters level program can now be answered affirmatively. The IPR training pro- cedures were run exclusively by advanced graduate students, and pg faculty time was required. It should also be noted that in many ways, the results of the present study are more meaningful than those obtained by Goldberg (1967) and Kagan gt, al., (1967) since this study was a replication of their studies and the results of this study were obtained in an on-going course offered through the university, and not in a laboratory setting. It is apparent that although a number of behavioral changes occurred in the present study over a fairly short period of time, the brevity of the training program had a number of inherent disadvantages. For example, the present 177 design might be expanded to include "interrogation" training and having counselors practice the role of interrogator with other trainees. This can help trainees learn to communicate in a more concrete and specific fashion (which is assumed to help clients differentiate their concerns). Also, in order to cover all the areas outlined in the present design in the short period of time allotted, it was necessary to "water- down" the treatment procedures by limiting the number of exposures to each task in the sequence. This may have had the effect of weakening the full impact of each develop- mental task. In longer periods of time, it would be possible to build on each of the tasks, progressing to deeper and more intense levels of discrimination and communication. This might also make it possible for trainees to develop at their own particular rates in each particular area. For example, if a trainee was able to communicate affective- ly, but had difficulty differentiating client concerns, he might spend time in exercises which would help him increase this level of skill. On the other hand, if a trainee was able to differentiate client concerns, but was unable to communicate this understanding to a client, a number of exercises might be undertaken to help the trainee develop communication skills. Finally, it is possible that the purposes of each particular developmental task -- being aware of own feelings and communications, being aware and understanding of the 178 client's feelings and communication, being aware of and understanding of the bilateral nature of the counseling pro- cess, being aware of the elements of effective communication behaviors in counseling; and being able to communicate facilitatively -- could be expanded and considered as general goals to be achieved during counselor preparation. Implications For Futgre Research While the results of this study indicate the effective- ness of IPR as a counselor training technique, a number of implications for further investigation emerge. 1. The limitations of time, scheduling and availability of supervisors resulted in difficulties with train- ing all the students at the same time, and in the same sequence. It was necessary to split the class in half, so that all students would have the Oppor- tunity to receive the IPR treatment. This had the effect of dividing the potency of the IPR treat- ment in half, resulting in the exclusion of a number of training exercises (e.g. interrogator training, more counselor and mutual video recall exposures, more time spent on the elements of communication in counseling, and the introduction of physiological feedback techniques) which might strengthen the design further. Any replication 179 of this study should consider including these modifications. With an expanded training model (to include interrogation training), it may be possible to select those trainees who made significant gains in their own training to become supervisors for subsequent training programs -- in a sense, select- ing the more advanced and competent students who had received IPR training to serve as "faculty" members and run the course. While trainee progress was accelerated during IPR training, there was little evidence of whether such progress resulted in significantly different behavior in subsequent interviews. A follow-up study should be undertaken to determine the per- manence of behavior changes over time. The ques- tions "Did the curves in Figures 4.3 and 4.4 re- flect a reaction to classroom experience or would such curves result over time alone?" and "Would such trainees now do better if placed directly into a 'regular' practicum.‘ need to be answered. Because interviews with real clients are more mean- ingful than interviews under either coached-client or role play conditions, future research should 180 consider the possibilities of using actual client contacts. This may help answer the question "Will clients improve from contact with counselors trained in this manner?" The same basic approach should be used with a different set of supervisors, who should be trained to use IPR procedures. This would help determine whether the results were due to the experimental treatments or to the skill of the supervisors using the IPR techniques. The question ”Would faculty members running IPR get the same results as graduate students running IPR did?" may need to be answered. If observations about the efficacy of the sequence of developmental tasks in training is correct, it is necessary to determine the relative importance of each stage of the training procedure on trainee behavior. Thus far, the choice of sequence for the develop- mental tasks has been made on the basis of clinical intuition. Future research might compare various sequences to ascertain whether there is a difference in the order of the tasks and whether it is impor- tant to delete, expand or add new tasks. B IBLIOGRAPHY BIBLIOGRAPHY ACES Standards for the preparation of secondary school counselors-~1967. Personnel and Guidance Journal, 1967, 1, pp. 96-106. Alger, I, and Hogan, P. Use of videotape recordings in conjoint marital therapy. American Journal 2: Psychia- try, 1967, 123, pp. 1425-1430. Allen, D. W. (Ed.) Micro-Teaching: .A Description, Stanford, California: Stanford Teacher Education Program, 1967. Altucker, N. Constructive use of the supervisory relation- ship. Journal 2i CounseliggfiPsycholggy, 1967, 14, 2, pp. 165-170. Appel , M. L. Self understanding for the guidance counselor. P & .9 Journal, 1963, 42, pp. 143—148. Arnold, D. L. Counselor education as responsible self- development. Counselor Education and Sppervision, 1962, 1, pp. 185-192. Arbuckle, D. S. The learning of counseling: Process not product. Journal 2: Counselipg Psychology, 1963, 10, pp. 163-168. Arbuckle, D., and Wicas, E. The development of a counseling perception instrument. Journal 9f Counseling Psychology, 1958, 4, pp. 304-312. Bandura, A., and Walters, R. H. Social Learning and Person- ality Development, Holt, Rinehart and Winston, Inc., 1963. Barrett-Lennard, G. T. Dimensions of therapist response as causal factors in therapeutic change. Genetic Psycho- logical Monographs, 1962, 76, 43 (whole #562). Beiser, H. R. Self—listening during supervision of psycho- therapy. Archives 2: General Psyghiatry, 1966, 15, pp. 135-139. Berenson, B. G., and Carkhuff, R. R. Sources 2; Gain_i§ Counseling and Psychotherapy, Holt, Rinehart and Winston, Inc., 1967. Berenson, B. G., Carkhuff, R. R., and Myrus, P. The inter— personal functioning and training of college students. Journal g£_Counseling ngcholggy, 1966, 13, pp. 441-446. 182 183 Berenson, B. G., Mitchell, K. M., and Moravec, J. A. Level of therapist functioning, patient depth of self exploration and type of confrontation. Journal gf Counseling Psychology, 1968, 15, 2, pp. 136—139. Bergin, A. E. The effects of psychotherapy: Negative results revisited. Journal 9£.Counseligg Psychology, 1963, 10, pp. 244-255. Bergin, A. and Soloman, S. Personality and performance correlates of empathic understanding in psychotherapy. Paper read at American Psychological Association, Philadelphia, September, 1963. Betz, R. L. Effects of group counseling as an adjunctive practicum experience. Journal g£_Counseling Psychology, 1969, 16, 6, pp. 528-533. Bierd, J. H., and Standish, J. T. Audiosimulation in counselor training. Final report, NDEA Title VII, No. 1245, Oregon System of Higher Education, Monmouth, Oregon, 1964. Black, F. A. Counseling laboratory experience. Personnel and Guidance Journal, 1953, 32, pp. 36-38. Bonney, C. W., and Gazda, C. Group counseling experiences: Reactions by counselor candidates. Counselor Education and Sgpervision, 1966, 5, 4, pp. 205—212. Bordin, E. S. Editorial: Narrowness or breadth? Journal 2f Counselinginychology, 1962, 10, p. 2. Bordin, E. Simplification as a strategy for research in psychotherapy. Journal 2f Consulting Psycholggy, 1965, 29, pp. 493-503. Boyd, R., Vernon, S., and Sisney, V. Immediate self-image confrontation and changes in self-concept. Journal gf Consulting ngchology, 1967, 31, pp. 291-294. Brammer, L. Teaching counseling theory: Some issues and points of view. Counselor Education and Sppervision, 1966, 5, 3, pp. 120-131. Bridgewater, R. B., and Crookston, B. B. An experiment in counselor training on the master's level. School and Society, 1952, 76, pp. 24-26. 184 Buchheimer, A., Goodman, J., and Sircus, G. Videotapes and kinescope recordings as situational test and laboratory exercises in empathy for the training of counselors: Technical report to U. S. Office of Education, NDEA Title VII, research project #7-42-0550-1670. Hunter College of the City University of New York, 1965. Campbell, R. J. The development and validation of a multiple-choice scale to measure affective sensitivity. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Michigan State University, 1967. Campbell, D. T. , and Stanley, J. C. Experimental and Quasi- experimental Designs for Research, Rand McNally and Co., Chicago, 1963. Carkhuff, R. R. Toward failure or fulfillment: Training and practice in counseling and psychotherapy. TEE Counselors Contribution £p_Facilitative Processes, Urbana, 111., R. W. Parkinson, 1966, pp. 1-18. Carkhuff, R. R. Training in the counseling and therapeutic practices: Requiem or reveille? Journal pf Counselipg Psychology, 1966, 13, pp. 360-367. Carkhuff, R. R. Toward a comprehensive model of facilitative interpersonal processes. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 1967, 14,1, pp. 67-72— Carkhuff, R. R. Differential functioning of lay and professional help. Journal pf_Counseliqg Psychology, 1968, 15, 2, pp. 117-126. Carkhuff, R. R. Critical variables in effective counselor training. Journal pf Counselipg Psychology, 1969, 16, 3, pp. 238-245. Carkhuff, R. R., and Berenson, B. G. Beyond Counseling and Therapy, Holt, Rinehart and Winston, Inc., 1967. Carkhuff, R. R., Kratochviel, D., and Friel, T. Effects of professional training: Communication and discrimina- tion of facilitative conditions. Journal pf Counseling Psychology, 1968, 15, 1, pp. 68-74. Carkhuff, R. R., and Truax, C. B. Lay mental health counsel— ing: The effects of lay group counseling. Journal pf ConsultipgfiPsyphology, 1965, 29, pp. 426-431(a). Carkhuff, R. R. and Truax, C. B. Training in counseling and psychotherapy: An evaluation of an integrated didactic and experimental approach. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 1965,29, pp. 331-336(b). 185 Cartwright, R. D. and Vogel, J. L. A comparison of changes in patients during matched periods of therapy and no— therapy. Journal pf Consulting Ppychology, 1960, 24, pp. 121-127. Cash, W. L. and Munger, P. F. Counselors and their prepara- tion. Review pf Educational Research, 1966, 36, pp. 256—263. Chennault, J. The education of the phony counselor. Phi Delta Kappan, 1964, 45, pp. 450-452. Covner, B. J., Studies in phonographic recordings of verbal material: The use of phonographic records in counsel- ing practice and research. Journa1_pf Consulting Psychology, 1942, 6, pp. 105-113. Danish, S. and Kagan, N. Emotional simulation in counseling and psychotherapy. Psychotherapy: Theogy, Research and Practice, 1969, 6, 4, pp. 261-263. Delaney, D. Simulation techniques in counselor education: Proposal of a unique approach. Counselor Education and Supgrvision, 1969, 8, 3, pp. 183-188. DeRoo, W. M. and Rank, R. C. Counselor response system of the behavior interaction description system. (Paper read at the APGA convention, Minneapolis, Minnesota, 1965) Dreikurs, R. and Sonstegard, M. A specific approach to practicum supervision. Counselor Education and Supervision, 1966, 6, 1, pp. 18-27. Ebel, R. L. Estimation of the reliability of ratings. Psychometrika, 1951, 16, pp. 407-424. Eckstein, R. and Wallerstein, R. S. The Teaching and Learning pf_Psychotherapy, New York: Basic Books, Inc., 1958. Eisenberg, S. and Delaney, D. Using video simulation of counseling for training counselors. Journal pf Counseling Psychology, 1970, 17, 1, pp. 15—19. Gysenck, H. J. The effects of psychotherapy: An evaluation. Journal pf Consulting Psychology, 1952, 16, pp. 319-324. Fairweather, G., et.a1. Relative effectiveness of psycho- therapeutic programs. Ppychological Monographs, 1960, 74, 5, (whole No. 492). 186 Foreman, M. T-groups: Their implications for counselor supervision and preparation. Counselor Education and Supervision, 1967, 7, pp. 48-53. Foreman, M., P0ppen, W. and Frost, J. Case groups: An in- Service education technique. Personnel and Guidance Journal, 1967, 46, 4, pp. 388-392. Gaier, E. L. The use of stimulated recall in revealing the relationship between selected personality variables and the learning process. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Chicago, 1951. Gazda, G. and Ohlsen, M. The effects of short-term group counseling on prospective counselors. Personnel and Guidance Journal, 1961, 39, pp. 634-638. Geertsma, R. H. and Reivich, R. S. Repetitive self-observa- tion by videotape playback. Journal pf Nervous and Mental Disease, 1965, 141, pp. 29-41. Goldberg, A. D. A sequential program for supervising counselors using the Interpersonal Process Recall technique. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Michigan State University, 1967. Goldfried, M. D. and Levy, R. The use of television video- tape to enhance the therapeutic value of psychodrama. American Journal p£_P§ychiatry, 1968, 125, pp. 690-692. Gibbs, J. R. in Counseling Theories and Counselor Education, edited by Clyde A. Parker, Boston: Houghton Mifflin Co., 1968. Griffin, G. G. An evaluation of the objectivity of an Audio— Visual Counselor Scale which compares the behaviors of M.A. candidate counselors with the behaviors of Ph.D. candidate counselors. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Michigan State University, 1966.. Gruenberg, P., Liston, E. and Wayse, G. Intensive supervision of psychotherapy with videotape recordings. American Journal pf Psychotherapy, 1969, 23, pp. 95-105. Gysbers, N. C. Strategies for practicum supervision. Counselor Education and Supervision, 1964, 3, pp. 149-152. Halkides, C. An investigation of therapeutic success as a function of four variables. Unpublished doctoral disser- tation, University of Chicago, 1958. 187 Hansen, J. C. and Barker, E. N. Experiencing and the supervi- sory relationship. Journal pf Counseling Psychology, 1964, 11, pp. 107-111. Hansen, J. and Moore, G. The off-campus practicum. Counselor Education and Supervision, 1966, 6, 1, pp. 32-40. Haseley, L. and Peters, H. Practicum: On-campus and off— campus. Counselor Education and Supervision, 1966, 5, 3, pp. 141-148. Heimann, R. A. and Whittemore, R. G. Electronic aids to practicum supervision. Counselor Education and Supervi— sion, 1964, 3, pp. 104-107. Higgins, W., Ivey, A. and Uhlemann, M. Media therapy: A programmed approach to teaching behavioral skills. Unpublished manuscript, 1970. Hobbs, N. Sources of gain in psychotherapy. American Psychologist, 1962, 17, pp. 18-34. Holmes, J. E. Counselor listening: Another dimension of the counseling process. Counselor Education and Supervision, 1964, 3, pp. 153-157. Hoppock, R. Two methods of training counselors for group guidance. Occupations, 1949, 27, PP. 523—526. Ivey, A. E., Normington, C, Miller, C. D., Morrill, W. and Haase, R. Microcounseling and Attending Behavior--an approach to pre-practicum counselor training. Journal pf Counseling_Psycholpgy, 1968 -- Monograph, 15, 5, Part 2. Jones, E. Biography pf Sigmund Freud, Vol. II, New York: Basic Books, 1959. Kagan, N. Multi-media and its application in guidance and counseling. Unpublished manuscript, Michigan State University, 1970. Kagan, N.,Krathwohl, D. and Farquhar, W. Interpersonal Process Recall: Stimulated recall by videotape, East Lansing, Michigan: Michigan State University, NDEA Title VII, Grant #OE7-32-0410-216, 1965. Kagan, N., Krathwohl, D. and Farquhar, W. Using IPR to accelerate client progress in counseling. Educational Research Series, No. 29, March, 1965, Michigan State University. A report submitted to the U. 8. Office of Education, NDEA Grant #7-32-0410-216. 188 Kagan, N., Krathwohl, D. and Miller, R. Stimulated recall in therapy using videotape: A case study. Journa1_pf Counseling Psychology, 1963, 10, pp. 237-243. Kagan, N., Krathwohl, D. R., et. al. Studies iu_Human Interaction (final report, Grant No. OE7-32-0410-270) Educational Publication Services, Midhigan State University, East Lansing, 1967. Kagan, N. and Schauble, P. Affect Simulation in Interpersonal Process Recall. Journal pf Counseling Psychology, 1969, 16, 4, pp. 309-313. Kagan, N., Schauble, P., Resnikoff, A., Danish, S. and Krathwohl, D. Interpersonal Process Recall, Journal p£_Nervous and Mental Diseases, 1969, 148, 4, pp. 365— 374. Kell, B. L. and Mueller, W. J. lupact and Change: lu_Study .9; Counseling Relationships, New York: Appleton— Century—Crofts, 1966. Kersh, B. Y. Classroom simulation: Further studies on dimensions of realism. Teaching Research Division: Title VII, NDEA, Project No. 5-0848, 1965. Kirk, B. A. Techniques of in-service training. Personnel and Guidance Journal, 1955, 34, pp. 204-207. .Klopf, G. and Cohen, N. The impact of NDEA Counseling and Guidance Institutes on the professional education of school counselors. Counselor Education and Supervision, 1962, 1, pp. 151-161. Korner, I. N. and Brown, W. H. The mechanical third ear. Journal pf Consulting Psychology, 1952, 16, pp. 81-84. Landsman, T. Humanistic training for a mechanistic society. Counselor Education and Supervision, 1963, 2, pp. 113-120. Landsman, T. Existentialism in counseling: The scientific view. Personnel and Guidance Journal, 1965, 6, pp. 568-573. Landsman, T. and Lane, D. A. V. media, yes, depersonalization, no." Audio Visual Instruction, 1963, 8, pp. 24—28. Landy, E. Counselor training through practice. Personnel and Guidance Journal, 1953, 31, pp. 310-314. Levy, L. H. in Counseling Theories and Counselor Education, edited by Clyde A. Parker, Boston: Houghton Mifflin Co., 1968. 189 Lister, J. The counselors personal theory. Counselor Education and Supervision, 1964, 4, pp. 207-213. Lister, J. Counselor experiencing: It's implications for supervision. Counselor Education and Supervision, 1966, 5, 2, pp. 55-61. Magoon, T. Innovations in counseling. Journal pf Counseling Psychology, 1964, 11, pp. 342-347. Martin, J. and Carkhuff, R. R. Changes in personality and interpersonal functioning of counselors-in-training. Journal pf Clinical Psyehology, 1968, l, 24, pp. 109—111. McKinnon, D. W. Group counseling with student counselors. Counselor Education and Supervision, 1969, 8, 3, pp. 195-201. Meek, C. R. and Parker, A. W. Introductory counseling course: Use of practicum students. Counselor Education and Supervision, 1966, 5, 3, pp. 154-159. Moore, F. J., Chernell, E. and West, M. J. Television as a therapeutic tool. Archives pf General Psychiatry, 1965, 12, pp. 217-220. Moss, C. S., Riegel, L. M. and Schaeffer, R. W. Objective assessment of the psychology intern experience. Journal .pf Counseling_Psychology, 1962, 9, 2, pp. 158-163. ‘Munger, P. F., Brown, D. F. and Needham, J. T. NDEA Institute participants two years later. Personnel and Guidance Journal, 1964, 12, pp. 887-900. Onder, J. The Manual pf_Psyuhiatric Television: TheoEy, Practice, Imagination, The American University Department of Communication, Maynard House Publishers, Ann Arbor, 1970. Orton, J. W. Areas of focus in supervising counseling practicum students in groups. Personnel and Guidance Journal, 1965, 44, pp. 167-170. Patterson, C. H. Supervising students in the counseling practicum. Journal pf Counseling_Psyeholpgy, 1964, 11, pp. 47-530 Patterson, C. H. Theories pf Counseling and Psychotherapy, Harper and Row Publishers, New York, 1966. Patterson, C. H. The counseling practicum-~An ethical issue. Counselor Education and Supervision, 1968, 7, pp. 322-324. 190 Perronne, P. A. and Sanborn, M. P. Early observation: An apprenticeship approach to counselor education. Counselor Education and Supervision, 1966, 6, 1, pp. 63-69. Peters, H. and Hansen, J. Counseling practicum: Bases for supervision. Counselor Education and Supervision, 1963, 2, pp. 82-85. Pierce, R., Carkhuff, R. R. and Berenson, B. G. The differential effects of high and low functioning counselors upon counselors-in-training. Journal_pf Clinical Psyuhology, 1967, 23, 2, pp. 212-216. Pierce, R. M. and Schauble, P. G. Graduate training of facilitative counselors: The effects of individual supervision. Journal pf Counseliug Paychology, 1970, 17, 3, pp. 210-215. Poling, E. G. Videotape recordings in counseling practicum, Title VII, Project 1235, NDEA of 1958, Grant No. 7-51- 0140-246, University of South Dakota, 1964. Poling, E. G. Videotape recordings in counseling practicum: II-Critique considerations. Counselor Education and Supervision, 1968, 8, 1, pp. 33-39. Reddy, W. B. Effects of immediate and delayed feedback on the learning of empathy. Journal_pf Counseling Psychology, 1968, 16, 1, pp. 59-62. Resnikoff, A., Kagan, N. and Schauble, P. Acceleration of psychotherapy through stimulated videotape recall. American Journal p£_Psychotherupy, 1970, 24, 1, pp. 102—111. Roark, A. The influence of training on counselor responses in actual and role playing interviews. Counselor Education and Supervision, 1969, 8, 4, pp. 289-296. Roeber, E. Position paper: Practicum and internship, in American Personnel and Guidance Association. Counselor Education: .5 Progress Report up Standards, Washington: American Personnel and Guidance Association, 1962. Rogers, C. R. The use of electrically recorded interviews in improving psychotherapy technique. American Journal ‘pf Orthopsychiatry, 1942, 12, pp. 429-434. Rogers, C. R. Cligut Centered Therapy, Boston: Houghton Mifflin Co., 1951. 191 Rogers, C. R. Training individuals to engage in the therapeutic process, in Strother C. R., editor, Psychology and Mental Health, Washington, D. C.: American Psychological Association, 1957. Rogers, C. R. The interpersonal relationship: The core of guidance. Harvard Educational Review, 1962, 32, pp. 416-429. Ross, 8. Report on conference on an ideal training program for psychotherapists. American Psychologist, 1963, 18, pp. 677-679. Ryan, C. Video aids in practicum supervision. Counselor Education and Supervision, 1969, 8, 2, pp. 125-129. Schauble, P. G. Emotional simulation in personal counseling: An application of research innovations in counseling to accelerate client movement. Paper read at APGA meeting, Detroit, 1968. Schauble, P. G. The acceleration of client progress in counsel- ing and psychotherapy through Interpersonal Process Recall. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Michigan State University, 1970. Schiff, S. B. and Reivich, R. Use of television as aid to psychotherapy supervision. Archives prQeneral Psychi- atr , 1964, 10, pp. 84-88. Schmidt, L. D. and Pepinsky, H. B. Counseling research in 1963. Journal pf Counseling Psychology, 1965, 12, pp. 418-427. Sperle, H. The case method technique in professional train- ing. Teachers College Regard, 1933, 35, pp. 235-237. Suess, J. F. Teaching clinical psychiatry with closed-circuit television and videotape. Journal p§_Medical Education, 1966, 41, p. 483. Sullivan, H. S. The Interpersonal_gheory pf_Psychiatry. Norton Press, New York, 1953. Thompson, C. and Bradway, K. The teaching of psychotherapy through content-free interviews. Journal prConsulting Psychology, 1950, 14, pp. 321-323. Truax, C. B. The process of group psychotherapy--Relationship between hypothesized therapeutic conditions and intra- personal exploration. Psychological Monographs, 1961, 75, 7 (whole No. 511). 192 Truax, C. B. Effective ingredients in psychotherapy: An approach to unraveling the patient-therapist interaction. Journal p£_Counseling Psychology, 1963, 10, pp. 256-263. Truax, C. B. Discussion of Krumboltz's "Future directions for counseling research," in Whitely, John. Research in Counseling: Education and Refocus, Merrill Publish- ing Co., 1967, pp. 208-214. Truax, C. B. and Carkhuff, R. R. Concreteness: A neglected variable in research in psychotherapy. Journal p§_ Clinical Psychology, 1964, 20, pp. 264-267. Truax, C. B., Carkhuff, R. R. and Douds, J. Toward an integration of the didactic and experiential approaches to training in counseling and psychotherapy. Journal pf Counseling Psychology, 1964, 11, pp. 240-247. Truax, C. B., Carkhuff, R. R. and Kodman, F. The relation- ships between therapist-offered conditions and patient change in group psychotherapy. Journal p£_Clinical Psycholggy, 1966. Ullman, L. and Krasner, L. (Eds.) Case Studies in_Behavior Modification. Holt, Rinehart, and Winston, 1965. Walz, G. E. and Johnston, J. A. Counselors look at them- selves on videotape. Journal gf_§ounseling Psychology, 1963, 10, pp. 232-236. Whitehorn, J. C. and Betz, B. J. A study of psychotherapeutic relationships between physicians and schizophrenic patients. American Journal pf,Psychiatry, 1954, 3, pp. 321-331. Whiteley, J. Research in_gounseling: Education and Refocus, Merrill Publishing Co., Columbus, Ohio, 1967. Whiteley, J. M. and Jakubowski, P. A. The coached client as a research and training resource in counseling. Counselor Education and Supervision, 1969, 9, pp. 19-29. Winer, B. J. Statistical Principles ip.Experimental Design. McGraw Hill Co., New York, 1962. Wirt, M., Betz, R. L., Engle, K. The effects of group counseling on the self-concepts of counselor candidates. Counselor Education and Supervision, 1969, 8, 3, pp. 189-195. 193 Truax, C. B. Discussion of Krumboltz's "Future directions for counseling research," in Whitely, John. Research iu_Counseling: Education and Refocus, Merrill Publish- ing Co., 1967, pp. 208-214. Truax, C. B. and Carkhuff, R. R. Concreteness: A neglected variable in research in psychotherapy. Journal pf Clinical Paychology, 1964, 20, pp. 264-267. Truax, C. B., Carkhuff, R. R. and Douds, J. Toward an integration of the didactic and experiential approaches to training in counseling and psychotherapy. Journal pf_Counseling-Psycholugy, 1964, 11, pp. 240-247. Truax, C. B., Carkhuff, R. R. and Kodman, F. The relation- ships between therapist-offered conditions and patient change in group psychotherapy. Journal pf Clinical Psychology, 1966. Walz, G. E. and Johnston, J. A. Counselors look at them- selves on videotape. Journal pf Counseling Paychology, 1963, 10, pp. 232—236. Whitehorn, J. C. and Betz, B. J. A study of psychotherapeutic relationships between physicians and schizophrenic patients. American Journal of Psychiatry, 1954, 3, pp. 321-331. ‘— Whiteley, J. Research iu Counseling: Education and Refocus, Merrill Publishing Co., Columbus, Ohio, 1967. Whiteley, J. M. and Jakubowski, P. A. The coached client as a research and training resource in counseling. Counselor Education and Supervision, 1969, 9, pp. 19-29. Wirt, M., Betz, R. L., Engle, K. The effects of group counseling on the self-concepts of counselor candidates. Counselor Education and Supervision, 1969, 8, 3, pp. 189- 195. Woody, R. H., Krathwohl, D., Kagan, N. and Farquhar, W. Stimulated recall in psychotherapy using hypnosis and videotape. American Journal pf Clinical Hypnosis, 1965, 7, 3, pp. 234-241. Wrenn, C. The Counselor in 3 Changing World, Washington: American Personnel and Guidance Association, 1962. 194 Woody, R. H., Krathwohl, D., Kagan, N. and Farquhar, W. Stimulated recall in psychotherapy using hypnosis and videotape. American Journal p§_glinical Hypnosis, 1965, 7, 3, pp. 234-241. Wrenn, C. The Counselor ip_§_ghanging World, Washington: American Personnel and Guidance Association, 1962. APPENDICES . APPENDIX A IPR COUNSELOR VERBAL RESPONSE SCALE AND MANUAL FOR TRAINING JUDGES IN USE OF COUNSELOR VERBAL RESPONSE SCALE The scale was developed as a part of a project supported by a grant from the U.S. Department of Health, Education and Welfare, Office of Ed- ucation, "Exploration of the Potential Value of Interpersonal Process Recall Technique (IPR) for the Study of Selected Educational Problems" (Project Nos. 7-32-0410-216 and 7-32-0410-270). IPR Counselor Verbal gesponse Scale The Counselor Verbal Response Scale is an attempt to describe a counselor's response to client communication in terms of four dichotomized dimensions: (a) affect-cognitive; (b) understanding--nonunderstanding; (c) specific--non- specific; (d) exploratory--nonexploratory. These dimensions have been selected because they seem to represent aspects of counselor behavior which seem to make theoretical sense and contribute to client progress. A fifth dimension-- effect-noneffective-~provides a global rating of the adequacy of each response which is made independently of the four descriptive ratings. The unit for analysis is the verbal interaction between counselor and client represented by a client statement and counselor response. A counselor response is rated on each of the five dimensions of the rating scale, with every client- counselor interaction being judged independently of preceding units. In judging an individual response the primary focus is on describing how the counselor responded to the verbal and nonverbal elements of the client's communication. Description of Rating Dimensions I. Affect--Cognitive Dimension The affective-cognitive dimension indicates whether a counselor's reSponse refers to any affective component of a client's communication or concerns itself primarily with the 197 198 cognitive component of that communication. A. Affective Responses--Affective responses generally make reference to emotions, feelings, fears, etc. The judge's rating is solely by the content and/or intent of the coun- selor's response, regardless of whether it be reflection, clarification, interpretation. These responses attempt to maintain the focus on the affective component of a client's communication. Thus they may: (1) Refer directly to an explicit or implicit reference to affect (either verbal or nonverbal) on the part of the client. Example: "It sounds like you were really angry at him." (2) Encourage an expression of affect on the part of the client. Example: "How does it make you feel when your parents argue?" (3) Approve of an expression of affect on the part of the client. Example: "It doesn't hurt to let your feelings out once in a while, does it?" (4) Presents a model for the use of affect by the client. Example: "If somebody treated me like that I'd really be mad." Special care must be taken in rating responses which use the word "feel." For example, in the statement "Do you fepl_that your student teaching experience is helping you get the idea of teaching?", the phrase "Do you feel that" really means "do you think that." Similarly the expression "How are you feel- ing?" is often used in a matter-of-fact, conversation manner. Thus, although the verb "to feel" is used in both these examples, 2.." A] _‘ 199 these statements do pp£_represent responses which would be judged "affective." B. Cognitive Responses-~Cognitive responses deal primarily with the cognitive element of a client's commun- ication. Frequently such responses seek information of a factual nature. They generally maintain the interaction on the cognitive level. Such responses may: (1) Refer directly to the cognitive component of the client's statement. Example: "So then you're thinking about switching your major to chemistry?" (2) Seeks further information of a factual nature from the client. Example: "What were your grades last term?" (3) Encourage the client to continue to respond at the cognitive level. Example: "How did you get interested in art?" II. Understanding--Nonunderstanding Dimension The understanding-nonunderstanding dimension indicates whether a counselor's response communicates to the client that the counselor understands or is seeking pp understand the client's basic communication, thereby encouraging the client to continue to gain insight into the nature of his concerns. A. Understanding Responses--Understanding responses communicate to the client that the counselor understands the client's communication--the counselor makes appropriate reference to what the client is expressing or trying to ex- press both verbally and nonverbally--or the counselor is 200 clearly seeking enough information of either a cognitive or affective nature to gain such understanding. Such responses: (1) Directly communicate an understanding of the client's communication. Example: "In other words, you really want to be treated like a man.” (2) Seek further infonmation from the client in such a way as to facilitate both the counselor's and the client's understanding of the basic problems. Example: ”What does being a man mean to you?" (3) Reinforce or give approval of client communications which exhibit understanding. Example: CL: "I guess then when peOple criticize me, I'm afraid they'll leave me." CO: "I see you're beginning to make some connection between your behavior and your feelings." B. Nonunderstanding Responses--Nonunderstanding responses are those in which the counselor fails to understand the client's basic communication or makes no attempt to obtain appropriate information from the client. In essence, non- understanding implies misunderstanding. Such responses: (1) Communicate misunderstanding of the client's basic concern. Example: CL: "When he said that, I just turned red and clenched my fists.” CO: "Some people don't say nice things." (2) Seek information which may be irrelevant to the client's communication. Example: CL: "1 seem to have a hard time getting along with my brothers.” C0: "Do all your brothers live at home with you?" 201 (3) Squelch client understanding or move the focus to another irrelevant area. Example: CL: "I guess I'ms really afraid that other people will laugh at me." CO: "We're the butt of other people's jokes sometimes." Example: CL: "Sometimes I really hate my aunt." CO: "Will things be better when you go to college?" III. Specific-Nonspecific Dimension The specific-nonspecific dimension indicates whether the counselor's reSponse delineates the client's problems and is central to the client's communication or whether the response does not specify the client's concern. In essence, it describes whether the counselor deals with the client's communication in a general, vague, or peripheral manner, or "zeros in" on the core of the client's communication. NB: A response judged to be nonunderstanding must also be non- specific since it would, by definition, misunderstand the client's communication and not help the client to delineate his concerns. ReSponses judged understanding might be either specific (core) or nonspecific (peripheral) i.e. they would be peripheral if the counselor conveys only a vague idea that a problem exists or "flirts" with the idea rather than helping the client delineate some of the dimensions of his concerns. A. Specificygesponses--Specific responses focus on the core concerns being presented either explicitly or implicitly, 202 verbally or nonverbally, by the client. Such responses: (1) Delineate more closely the client's basic concerns. Example: "This vague feeling you have when you get in tense situations--is it anger or fear?" (2) Encourage the client to discriminate among stimuli affecting him. Example: "Do you feel in all your classes or only in some classrooms?" (3) Reward the client for being specific. Example: CL: "I guess I feel this way most often with someone who reminds me of my father." CO: "So as you put what others say in per- spective, the whole world doesn't seem so bad, it's only when someone you value, like Father, doesn't pay any attention that you feel hurt." B. Nonspecific Responses--Nonspecific responses indi- cate that the counselor is not focusing on the basic concerns of the client or is not yet able to help the client differ- entiate among various stimuli. Such responses either miss the problem area completely (such responses are also non- understanding) or occur when the counselor is seeking to understand the client's communication and has been presented with only vague bits of information about the client's con- cerns. Thus such reSponses: (l) Fail to delineate the client's concern and cannot bring them into sharper focus. Exapple: "It seems your problem isn't very clear--can you tell me more about it?” (2) Completely miss the basic concerns being presented by the client even though the counselor may ask for specific details. 203 Example: CL: "I've gotten all A's this year and I still feel lousy." CO: "What were your grades before then?" (3) Discourage the client from bringing his concerns into sharper focus. Example: "You and your sister argue all the time. What do other people think of your sister?" IV. Exploratory--Nonexploratory Dimension The exploratory--nonexploratory dimension indicates whether a counselor's response permits or encourages the client to explore his cognitive or affective concerns, or whether the response limits a client's exploration of these concerns. A. Exploratory Responses--Exploratory responses encourage and permit the client latitude and involvement in his response. They may focus on relevant aspects of the client's affective or cognitive concerns but clearly attempt to encourage further exploration by the client. Such responses are often Open-ended and/or are delivered in a manner per- mitting the client freedom and flexibility in response. These responses: (1) Encourage the client to explore his own concerns. Example: Cognitive--"You're not sure what you want to major in, is that it?" Affective--"Maybe some of these times you're getting mad at yourself, what do you think?" (2) Assist the client to explore by providing him with possible alternatives designed to increase his range of responses. ‘ 204 Example: Cognitive--"What are some of the other alter- natives that you have to history as a major?" Affective--"In these situations do you feel angry, mad, helpless, or what?" (3) Reward the client for exploratory behavior. Example: Cognitive-~"It seems that you've considered a number of alternatives for a major, that's good." Affective--"So you're beginning to wonder if you always want to be treated like a man." B. Nonexploratory Responses--Nonexploratory responses either indicate no understanding of the client's basic communication, or so structure and limit the client's re- sponses that they inhibit the exploratory process. These responses give the client little opportunity to explore, ex- pand, or express himself freely. Such responses: (1) Discourage further exploration on the part of the client. Example: Cognitive--"You want to change your major to history." Affective--”You really resent your parents treating you like a child." V. Effective--Noneffective Dimension Ratings on the effective--noneffective dimension may be made independently of ratings on the other four dimensions of the scale. This rating is based solely upon the judge's professional impression of the appropriateness of the coun- selor's reSponses, that is how adequately does the coun- selor's response deal with the client's verbal and nonverbal communication. This rating is not dependent on whether the !F" 205 response has been judged affective--cognitive, etc. A rating of 4 indicates that the judge considers this response among the most appropriate possible in the given situation while a 3 indicates that the response is appro- priate but not among the best. A rating of 2 indicates a neutral response which neither measurably affects client progress nor inhibits it, while a rating of 1 indicates a response which not only lacks basic understanding of the client's concerns but which in effect may be detrimental to the specified goals of client growth. 206 Manual For Training Judges Ip.Use Q£_Counselor Verbal Response Scale This manual is intended to standarize procedures for use of the IPR Counselor Verbal Response Scale in the judging of either video or audio tapes and to provide some guide- lines in the training of judges in the use of the scale. I. What is a scorable response? Scorable responses include words, phrases, or sentences used by the counselor which interrupt the flow and momentum of client communication or which are in direct response to a client's statement. Although such responses normally occur at the conclusion of a client's communication, there are times when a counselor will deliberately interrupt a client's statement. In either case, such counselor statements are treated as scorable responses. Responses which are part of a counselor's unconscious mannerisms, and do not interrupt the flow and momentum of client communication, e.g., "Umm," "Okay," "Hmm," ”Yes," and "I see" are not scored. Responses such as ”continue" and "go on" are scorable reSponses. In essence, they urge and encourage the client to continue with his flow of communica- tion and reinforce the client's pattern of communication. There are times when counselors make two distinct re- sponses to a single client communication, these responses normally being separated by a pause: 207 Example: CL: ”It's good to get rounded especially if I decided to teach which you really can't decide until you get into college, I guess." CO: "Kind of hard to figure things out, isn't it?" "Do you think student teaching is helping you get the feel of teaching?" In this case, there are clearly two reSponses differing in nature which must be scored separately to accurately evaluate the counselor. There are other instances in which there is a clear shift in the content of the counselor's response without a distinct pause. In such cases, as in the pro- ceeding example, counselor statements are treated as two separate responses and are scored individually. N.B.: Each scorable counselor response must be rated on each of the five dimensions of the scale. 11. How pany reSppnses are scored? For purposes of accurate evaluation, twenty consecu- tive counselor responses are scored from each counseling session rated. These responses should be drawn from the middle portions of a counseling session, avoiding both the beginning and the terminating segments of the interview. Judges should, however, be given an opportunity ot listen to a few responses prior to the start of judging so that they may become acclimated to the voices and pace of both client and counselor, and may gain some familiarity with the general tone of the interview. 208 III. When is ratinggdone? Each response is rated at the conclusion of the coun- selor's statement (word or phrase). The tape (audio or video) should be stOpped after each scorable counselor re- sponse and scoring should be completed by all judges prior to the playing of the next unit. As judges become more comfortable with the use of the scale, they should require no more than thirty seconds for the scoring of each response. As in all judging procedures, it is desirable that communi- cation among judges be minimized during the rating session. Thus, judges should be so placed that, while they have adequate view of the video monitor or can clearly hear the audio tape, there is a minimum of contact among judges. IV. How are judges trained? It is important that adequate time be given to the training of judges. The success of any evaluation of this sort depends upon the agreement reached by the judges in defining the dimensions of the scale. Training must involve actual rating of practice tapes. The number of tapes used will, of course, depend on the needs of the judges. However, the tapes used should represent a variety of counseling interviews, i.e., experienced, inexperienced or beginning, etc. During training, discussion should follow the rating of each counselor response (obviously in the later stages of training, this is not as crucial and a group of responses may be rated before discussion occurs) until agreement about 209 interpretation of the dimensions of the scale is reached. Prior to the actual rating, judges should again go over the definitions of the five dimensions to insure complete under- standing. V. How much timeidoes rpginggrequire? It has been found that at least one hour is required for the rating of two audio tapes. It is clear that more time would be involved in the rating of video tapes due to the time required for changing tape. These time allowances should be noted prior to establishing a rating session.* It has also been our experience that approximately three hours is necessary for adequate training of judges and discussion of the definitions of the five dimensions. General Questions: The most frequent question which occurs centers about whether responses can be judged independently of prior content in the interview. The intent of this scale is to focus primarily on a single client communication and coun- selor reSponse. It is obvious, however, that many counselor responses take into account material which has been elicited in prior portions of the interview. Most interviews also *Care should be taken in scheduling rating sessions so that judges are not required to rate too many tapes at any one session. If this is allowed to occur, judges may acquire a "set" which will affect later ratings. 210 present a general theme within which individual interactions occur. The judge must clearly be aware of this larger frame- work in making his rating. However, the emphasis still remains on the individual response to a client communication. In this context, it is important to note that ratings of responses take into account the appropriateness of the response at a given moment in time. For instance, while a response may be specific in the early portions of the inter- view, the same reSponse coming later in the interview may not only be nonspecific, but also inappropriate in moving the client to a further understanding of his own concerns. To this extent, the unfolding theme and the appropriate timing of responses must be considered by the judge in making his rating. n c m N H .mmmm mQHmmZDOo mo Am>mA HA MQHMWZDOU mmH 211 APPENDIX B EMPATHIC UNDERSTANDING IN INTERPERSONAL PROCESSES SCALE Empathic Understanding in,1nte;persona1 Processes A_Scale For Measurement Robert R. Carkhuff State University of New York at Buffalo Level 1 The verbal and behavioral expressions of the first person either pp_not attend £p_or detract significantly from the verbal and behavioral expressions of the second person(s) in that they communicate significantly less of the second person's feel- ings than the second person has communicated himself. Examples: The first person communicates no awareness of even the most obvious, expressed surface feelings of the second person. The first person may be bored or disinterested or simply operating from a pre- conceived frame of reference which totally ex- cludes that of the other person(s). In summary, the first person does everything but express that he is listening, understanding or being sensitive to even feelings of the other person in such a way as to detract significantly from the communications of the second person. Level 2 While the first person responds to the expressed feelings of the second person(s), he does so in such a way that he pup; tracts noticeable affect from the communications of the second person. 213 214 Examples: The first person may communicate some awareness of obvious surface feelings of the second person but his communications drain off a level of the affect and distort the level of meaning. The first person may communicate his own ideas of what may be going on but these are not congruent with the expressions of the second person. In summary, the first person tends to respond to other than what the second person is expressing or indicating. Level 3 The expressions of the first person in response to the ex- pressed feelings of the second person(s) are essentially interchangeable with those of the second person in that they express essentially the same affect and meaning. Example: The first person reSponds with accurate under- standing of the surface feelings of the second person but may not respond to or may misinterpret the deeper feelings. The summary, the first person is responding so as to neither subtract from nor add to the expressions of the second person; but he does not respond accurately to how that person really feels beneath the surface feelings. Level 3 constitutes the minimal level of facilitative interpersonal functioning. 215 Level 4 The responses of the first person add noticeably to the expressions of the second person(s) in such a way as to express feelings a level deeper than the second person was able to express himself. Example: The facilitator communicates his understanding of the second person at a level deeper than they were expressed, and thus enables the second person to experience and/or express feelings which he was unable to express previously. In summary, the facilitator's responses add deeper feeling and meaning to the expressions of the second person. Level 5 The first person's responses ugg_significantly to the feeling and meaning of the expressions of the second person(s) in such a way as to (l) accurately express feelings levels be- low what the person himself was able to express or (2) in the event of ongoing deep self-exploration on the second person's part to be fully with him in his deepest moments. Examples: The facilitator responds with accuracy to all of the person's deeper as well as surface feelings. He is "together" with the second person or "tuned in" on his wavelength. The facilitator and the other person might proceed together to explore previously unexplored areas of human existence. 216 In summary, the facilitator is responding with a full aware- ness of who the other person is and a comprehensive and accurate empathic understanding of his most deep feelings. APPENDIX C DESCRIPTION OF ROLE, FUNCTION AND TRAINING OF INTERROGATION Interrogation We believe that basic to obtaining significant data and learning from video recall is the introduction of a third person into the counselor - client relationship who conducts the videotape recall sessions for either or both of the participants. Role and Functgg; The third person's function is to facilitate a self- analysis by the client of his underlying thoughts, feelings, images, expectations and his general pattern of interaction with the counselor--it is not to establish another relation- ship like the one being reviewed. The interrogator avoids establishing another counselor-client focused on the feelings or the content of the original relationship. He helps the client relive the original experience and talks only about what transpired then. He reminds the subject of the task to keep him from straying and especially to keep him from focusing his attention on the interrogator. The subject needs to be encouraged to pay attention to the T.V. monitor as much as possible and to the third person as little as possible. The questions the interrogator asks are very brief to keep the client focused on the T.V. monitor. Although clinical skill is needed by the interrogator to help the client re- cognize his underlying feelings, the third person needs to 218 219 structure carefully his relationship and act more like a clinical interrogator than like a counselor or a therapist-- in fact, because this third person in the counseling dyad must delimit his clinical function to actively probing the immediate past, we chose to name "interrogator" to describe his role. The interrogator is not as concerned as the counselor with the ppppl_dynamics of the client but rather with teach- ing the client how to interrogate himself and how to gain insight through the "self-confronting" experience afforded by the videotape. No attempt is made to relate the recalled thoughts and feelings to the client's life in general. Only the relationship on videotape is studied, although major elements of the dynamics within that relationship p£e_sought (thus the interrogator usually encourages the client to deal with basic recurring or persistent fears, aspirations etc., rather than spend much of the interrogation session focusing in on one or two responses or gestures about which the client might choose to be extremely verbal). Whereas the counselor may allow the client more freedom to ”set the pace" in the investigation of areas of client concern, the interrogator tends actively to "push" the client for greater clarity in describing and understanding specific behaviors. Since the interrogator has the videotaped behavior, he may choose to examine any client response by 220 asking the client to stop the playback and to elaborate on the meaning of a piece of behavior the interrogator con- siders important. Interrogation can be a learning situation where the client comes to be intensely aware of his own behavior and personal idiosyncracies. The more he examines himself in interaction with the counselor, the more he may consciously choose to alter or redirect his behavior with the counselor and with others. It is the interrogator's function to create within the client this "intense awareness of his own behavior" in one relationship, and it is the counseling relationship within the IPR process that uses that ”awareness" to help the client enter a new relationship with the counselor and to promote client growth in his total life situation. Client involvement in the interrogation process develops around: 1. The client's feelings: their origin and develop- ment within the interview. 2. The client's thoughts: their origin and deve10p- ment within the interview. 3. The way the client sees himself. The things he likes, dislikes and fears about himself. 4. The way the client would like the counselor to see him. 5. The way the client believes the counselor actually does see him. Thus the interaction between interrogator and client is structured to enable the client to become aware of his behavior in relating to the counselor. 221 Training Interrogators Generally, prospective interrogators are recruited from among competent clinicians. They are given the following experiences: First, after the rationale, function and tech- niques of interrogation are explained, various videotaped interviews are played. The interrogator is asked to identify places in the interview where he might encourage a subject to stop (assuming that the subject did not stop at those points by himself). He is asked to explain why he chose to stop at that point and what he might ask the subject at that point. Efforts are made to develop the trainee's sensitivity to Specific cues which assist the interrogator in recognizing verbal and nonverbal communication which might be effectively used in the interrogation session. They are taught that: abrupt shifts in theme during the interview; shifts in body posture; changes in voice level, tone or pace; use of vocabulary which describes intense affect; changes in visual focus (espeically glances at the counselor after the client has made a statement); instances in which either person clearly misinterpreted the other or appeared to not hear the other; possible use of metaphoric communication (e.g., "my counselor at school gets me angry"); inappropriate affect, such as a laugh following a serious comment; and similar cues may be indicative of heightened underlying emotionality and so are often productive times to stop the playback. 222 Next, each interrogator-trainee is shown films and video- tapes demonstrating various interrogation sessions. He is encouraged to critique the tapes. He is then videotaped in a counseling session with a client of his own and is inter- rogated by one of the IPR staff. After another counseling session, he is given the Opportunity to watch a member of the project staff conduct an interrogation with his client. Finally, the interrogator-trainee is paired with a second trainee and acts as an interrogator for this trainee's counseling sessions. They then discuss the interrogation sessions with one of the project staff. Throughout the training process, attention is called to the underlying assumptions of interrogation discussed above. APPENDIX D SAMPLE TYPESCRIPT IPR SIMULATION FILMS IPR Simulation Films Film Series Title: Rejection A (Cat. No. 1) Scene 1 Non-verbal behavior: Actor presents somewhat aloof, contempla- tive attitude. Sits back in chair, appear- ing somewhat condescending. Actor: I don't know that ... well, yeah, I don't suppose we'd be too unhappy about having you around here, if you decide you would like to stay with us. Ah, we could probably fit you in ... I imagine. I don't know that ... No, I don't suppose that would be very much of an objection. I mean I think we have decided found out pretty much what you are; and we would not be altogether unhappy with that. You seem to be the sort of person we could get along with, so I think you would be pretty welcome with us. Scene 2 Non-verbal behavior: Actor still aloof, more condescending, pompous. He appears to be lecturing, or "talking down" to the viewer. Actor: I really don't think that's true. I mean ... I think that probably you are very wrong in that. I suppose that you can go right on believing that, but... Certainly no one is going to tell you not to think what you honestly believe, but I think you should examine some of the things that you believe, because most of us couldn't bring ourselves to feel that way. How about it, humm? Well, you think about it ... You think about it. Scene 3 Non-verbal behavior: Actor sits back in chair, is extermely condescending, patronizing. Actor: You know, I don't very often say this to someone right across the table, you know. But I am afraid I don't like you very much! I don't know why it is 224 Scene 4 225 but I just don't like to be around you ... So I, I wish that in the future you would go see some- one else. I don't think I can help you at all. I have such a strange negative reaction to you. I am sorry! I don't like to feel like this about anyone. It's just that it upsets me to be around you, and I don't see any call to be upset. I am sorry, but there it is. Non-verbal behavior: Actor is very confrontive, hostile. Actor: Scene 5 Bites off words very deliberately. Well, I've listened; and I don't like your ideas one damn bit. As a matter of fact, they just about make me sick. I think it's you -- and people like you -- that cause most of the stink and the misery that we have got around us, and I'm just not fond of it! I hate to see it! And if it weren't for some of the proprieties of life, I think I'd like to climb right over this table and punch you right in the mouth. Now I h0pe that's quite clear ... (pause) ... As far as I'm concerned you can just pick up your ideas and your notions and get right the hell out of here! Non-verbal behavior: Actor expresses much anger in facial Actor: expressions; threatens, shakes fist at viewer, pushes chair away from table as if preparing to climb over it at viewer, pounds table with fists. Well, you son of a bitch! Somebody ought to just kick your face right in! Honest to God, I'd just like to ... Will you get away from me before I come over there and just clobber the shit out of you! Now get away! Go on! APPENDIX E DESCRIPTION OF EXPERIMENTAL GROUP "CURRICULUM" Description Of Tasks: Experimental Group "Curriculum" 11221;; Session 1 (1) Introduction (30 minutes) The initial meeting of the class was a group meeting, in which a general overview of the course was given and pre-measures are obtained on all trainees. The whole group was given the following introduction: ”The purpose of the present course is to attempt to provide experiences which combines counseling theory and practical applications of this theory. Experiences will be offered in both theory of counseling and actual laboratory participation with counseling and interviewing, in which each of you will become familiar with the cognitive and experiential aSpects of counseling and the counseling relationship, and learn how to trans- late these experiences into effective counseling behaviors. The experiences you will be provided with have been used at the doctoral level, and include tasks which are used to train PhD candi- dates in counseling. The process of counseling includes counselor behavior, client behavior, and the dynamic inter- action that takes place between the two of them. In order for you as a counselor to be able to help another person, you must first be able to indentify what concerns the client; after these discriminations are made clear to you the counselor, you must then translate these per- ceptions and discriminations into behaviors which will help clients to work through these concerns. The structure of the course entails first pre- senting situations in which you as the trainee can identify and become sensitive to some of your own feelings and communications which occur in your everyday interacting with people. The purpose of this exercise is to help you 227 228 to be able to begin to recognize that feelings do influence the behaviors of clients we will see. We will also be looking at client communi- cations and feelings closely so as to sharpen your skills in being able to more clearly and accurately understand the clients and their concerns. Because of the two-way communication that exists between counselor and client, the counselor must be albe to sort out and recognize his own feelings, conflicts, and needs from those of the client; this entails becoming sensitive to your own feelings, sensitive to client feel- ings, and aware of the bilateral nature of the relationship established between you and your clients. After you become more sensitive to and aware of the problem presented by the client, you must be able to translate these perceptions and discriminations into effective counseling and helping behaviors. Experiences focusing on dimensions of effective/noneffective com- munication behaviors will be presented and Opportunities to practice counseling behaviors will be afforded, using videotaped feedback for instant analysis of your in-counseling behaviors. A supervisor will be available to guide you through an exploration and evalu- ation of your performances. Presently, we will now ask you all to become involved in some tasks designed to help us identify the points in time during the course which seem to indicate where the most learning has taken place. Schedules for the remainder of the term will now be handed out, with your individual assignments for this evening and for the remainder of the term."1 (2) Pre-Measures (A) Audio tape with Coached Client (15 minutes) The trainees were instructed that they would be asked to interview someone three times during the course of the term (pre, middle, and post) for the purpose of obtaining a measure of their interviewing and counseling behaviors, and to check on the changes that may occur throughout the term. The whole group was given the following instructions: "You will be asked to Spend 15 minutes interviewing a client we have supplied for (B) 229 you. Because this is a course in counseling and interviewing procedures, we are asking you to do this task so we can monitor any changes in your interviewing behaviors at various points in the term. Your assignment for this task is to get to know this client. While it is important to know how they think about them- selves and different things, we feel it is more important to know how they feel about what they are thinking. Therefore, you should try as best as you can to understand how the person is feeling. This will help you to get to know this person better." An audio tape was made of these interviews for later analysis. Audio tape Role Playing_- with fellow trainee (30 minutes) Two trainees were paired up and assigned to pre-determined rooms. The rooms contained two chairs and an audio-tape recorder. The 30 minutes was divided into two 15 minute parts, with one trainee instructed to be counselor and the other client for 15 minutes; after this amount of time elapses, they reversed roles for the second 15 minutes, with the trainee who was counselor becoming client and the trainee who was client now became counselor. They were given the follow- ing instructions: (1) Trainees who are counselors are given the same instructions for interviewing the coached client. (2) Trainees who are clients are told that: "You will be asked to talk with the counselor and you will try to let him get to know you; you can talk about something that may be important to you now, previously, or that is important to someone else. Try to let the counselor know how you feel as well as think about certain things that are important to you." An audio tape was made for later analysis. 230 (C) Affective Sensitivity Scale (60-90 minutes) The A. S. S. was given to the class as a whole group, and required between 60-90 minutes to administer. The group was given the test booklet on which the inn structions for the scale were printed, I.B.M. answer sheets were to be marked, and a #2 pencil. The class was divided into two groups of 10. Those trainees in Group 1 were to receive the Experimental treatment (the develOpmental tasks in discrimination and communication skills using the I.P.R. technique) for the first half of the term; Group 2 trainees were to receive the Control treatment (tradi- tional classroom lectures and presentations) for the first half of the term. After the mid-point of the course is reached (Week 6, where middle criterion measures are obtained), the two groups switched treatments. Group 1 now received the traditional classroom procedures, and Group 2 now received the developmental tasks in discrimination and communication skills using the I.P.R. technique. Week 2 Session 2A Purpose: The purpose of this session was to make trainees aware of and sensitive to their own feelings during their interpersonal communication. Description of Task (2A): (1% Hours) The trainees met as a group and were shown a film designed to simulate varying degrees of stressful interpersonal situations 231 (Affect Simulation Films). After each vignette (lasting for approximately 1-2 minutes), the film was stOpped and the train- ees discussed the feelings and reactions they had to the vignette. The supervisor gave the group the following intro- duction to the task: (1) "We know that the mind works faster than the voice. (2) As we talk with peOple and interact with others, we think of things which are quite different from the things which we are talking about; everyone does this, and there is no need to feel embarrassed or to hesi- tate to 'own up to it' when it does occur. (3) We know as we interact with peOple, there are times when we are annoyed with what they say, and there are times we like what they say; there are times when we think they really understand and there are times when we feel they have missed the point of what we were saying or really don't under- stand what we were feeling or how strongly we were feeling something. (4) There are also times when we are concerned about what the other person is thinking about us. Sometimes we want the other person to think about us in ways he may not be then thinking about us." The group was now shown a series of vignettes in which an actor portrayed a particular emotional state (hostility, re- jection, seduction). The trainees were asked to face the projection screen and were further by the supervisor: (5) "The following vignettes include individual stimulus expressions-expressions by another individual of feeling and content in dif- ferent areas. You may conceive of this 232 as simply a person who is sitting across from you and has something to say to you and you alone. We would like you to imagine how you would react, and how you might feel if this person was talking to you across the table or room." After each individual vignette was shown, the supervisor (acting here as interrogator) stOpped the film and structured the discussion to involve the trainees to focus on: (1) What are you thinking about this person? What does this person think about you? How and what would you like this person to think of you? Have you ever thought like this before? (2) What are you feeling about this situation and person? What does this person feel about you? How and what would you like this person to feel about you? Have you ever felt like this before? (3) What would you like to do in this situation? What should you do in this situation? What will you do in this situation? What have you typically done is this situation? Session ZB Purpose: The purpose of this session was to make trainees more aware of and sensitive to their own feelings during interpersonal communication. Description of Task (2B): (1% Hours) Each trainee spent a 1% hour time block viewing the Affect Simulation Films individually, and was videotaped as he viewed the films and reacted to them. Viewing of the vignettes task approximately 3 hour, with recall of the video- tape taking approximately 1 hour. 233 The supervisor (in the role of interrogator) structured the session and recall as he did in the previous group meeting for Session 2A, and added the following: (6) "If we ask you pug, how you were thinking or feeling toward the filmed actor, it might be very difficult for you to remem- ber. With the T. V. playback of your interaction, you will find it possible to recall these thoughts and feelings in detail. StOp and start the playback by means of the switch as often as you remember your thoughts and feelings. The recorder is on remote control, so you are not troubling anyone no matter how often you stop and start the playback. As you remember thoughts and feelings, stop the tape and tell me what they were." The trainee was again instructed to focus on feelings, thoughts, and possible actions, with interrogation taking place as the trainee and interrogator viewed the videotaped replay. Week 3 Session 3 Purpose: The purpose of this session was to make the trainees more aware of, sensitive to, and understanding of a greater amount of client communication. This was done in groups. Description of Task: (180 minutes) The supervisor began by addressing the group about dif- ferent ways clients can communicate withthe counselor: "There are different ways that one person can communicate with another person. We can talk to each other and hear verbally what the other is saying. However, some- times what they are saying may not be what they are feeling, so we can become confused 234 by what they really mean. Therefore, we can judge the other persons communications more accurately if we can more fully understand the different way their thoughts, feelings, and behaviors are interacting. For example, what they say may indicate a very strong feeling, but the tone of their voice may indicate not reflect any underlying emotion- ality. They may appear to be very relaxed postually, and may not appear to be as agi- tated as they say they feel. Conversely, a person can talk about strong feelings and this can be reflected in their tone of voice, gestures, posture, use of vocabulary, and the way they generally are responding. There are, therefore, different dimensions of communications along verbal and nonverbal dimensions (posture, gesture, tonal quality of voice, and how congruous and appropriate the communications appear to be). These are ways in which you as a counselor can monitor what the client is trying to communicate to you. As we become sensitive to identifying and under- standing client communications, and role-play throughout the remainder of the course, try to keep in mind these dimensions as you learn to recognize the communications of others." Two trainees were now asked to role-play (one to play the counselor and the other to play the client). The client was given the instructions to talk about themselves, their feelings or a concern they might have (could be positive or negative). Because the focus of this task was to become aware of and under- standing of client communications, the counselor was told to try to understand the clients concern and feelings. Role playing was carried on for approximately 5 minutes and was audio-recorded. After each dyad of role playing was stopped, the audiotape was rewound and replayed. The group was involved in 235 the recall of the interaction. The tape was stopped as usual at key points for discussion. A group discussion was then held under the lead of the supervisor who guided the discussion of relevant material as the interrogator would by focusing on feelings, thoughts, and possible actions. The group members were encouraged to talk about what they thought and felt the client was communicating or attempting to communicate to the counselor. The counselor was asked what he thought the client was feeling and how he arrived at these impressions; the group was encouraged to share their impressions. Finally, the client was asked to verify or refute the impressions of the rest of the group, by telling them what he was thinking and feeling. The focus of this session was solely on the clietn, his feelings, thoughts, and reactions. The session ended when all trainees had a chance to role-play the client. Week 4 Session 4A Purpose: The purpose of this session was to make the trainees increasingly aware of the elements of effective communication behaviors in counseling. Description of Task (4A): (180 minutes) Each member of the group was given a copy of the Counselor Verbal Response Scale (CVRS) developed by Kagan, pg. 31., (1967), covering the affective/cognitive, understanding/nonunderstanding, Specific/nonopecific, and exploratory/nonexploratory dimensions of counselor communications (effective/noneffective dimension not included), and a cOpy of the Empathic Understanding Scale (EUS) in Interpersonal Processes developed by Carkhuff (1967). 236 The supervisor and studnnts read each dimension of the CVRS, one dimension at a time, and discussed how each dimen- sion could facilitate or inhibit effective communication be- tween client and counselor. After discussing the dimensions of the CVRS, the supervisor introduced the 5 levels of Empathic Understanding Scale (EUS). The CVRS dimensions, it was pointed out, were ways of determining whether a counselor reSponse was of a particular type, but did not attempt to measure the quality of these reSponses. The EUS was introduced to demonstrate how each counselor response was of a particular type, but did not attempt to measure the quality of these responses. The EUS was introduced to demonstrate how each counselor response can add to or subtract qualitatively from the clients affect, thus Opening up or closing down channels of communication between counselor and client. After the group was given a 15 minute break, the trainees listened to a pre-recorded training tape made by Cakhuff, in which client statements were followed by 4 or 5 possible counselor reSponses. The trainees first rated each counselor response according to whether it added to (+), subtracted from (-), or did not add to nore subtract from the clients statement of affect (0). After practicing this procedure of rating for 3 or 4 counselor responses, the trainees rated the subsequent counselor responses according to the 5 levels of the EUS, with group discussions as to why each individual rated as he did. 237 Session 43 Purpose: The purpose of this session was to help trainees further understand the elements of facilitative communication behaviors in counseling. The purpose was four-fold--first, to have trainees practice rating a model of effective behaviors in counseling, second, to give trainees experiences at being video- taped; third, to be able to have firsthand experience at becoming involved in a counseling relationship; and fourth, to have the group rate each trainees' performance to further crystallize and conceptualize the counseling process in terms of levels of facili- tation. Desripgion of Task (4B): (180 minutes) The first portion of this session, (lasting 1% hours) was devoted to rating segments of a videotaped counseling session which had been previously conducted by a PhD Counseling Psycholo- gist rated high on level of facilitation. (This characterized effective counseling and served as a model for trainees; the trainees learned to rate the levels of facilitation and what constituted a helpful reSponse). This counseling session lasted approximately 20 minutes in length; the supervisor stopped the tape after each 3 minute segment. The trainees in the group were asked individually to rate that particular segment of the inter- view on the EUS°levels 1-5, and to indicate how such a rating was arrived at. This task continued until the videotaped counseling session was completed and rated. After a 15 minute break by the group, the next portion of this session began (lasting approximately 1% hours). This was devoted to having trainees practice establishing relationships in role playing sitautions using videotape and group discussions. 238 Trainees were paired up and were asked to role-play counselor- client as previously done during the course of the term. They were allowed approximately 5 minutes to role play, and this interaction was videotaped. After this, the tape was rewound and replayed for the whole group to rate and feedback impressions of the session to the trainees who role played that particular session; the tape was stopped after 1/3, 2/3 and all of the session had passed. This role-playing, group rating and experience with videotape concluded when all trainees had played the role of counselor. Week 5 Session 5 Purpose: The purpose of this session was to enable trainees to become more sensitive to and aware of the bi- lateral nature of the counseling relationship and the mutual impact between counselor and client. Description of Task 5: (180 minutes) Two trainees were assigned per session and were paired up for a 3-hour time block. Two trainees were asked to role-play the counselor twice and the client twice during the course of the session. The three-hour block was divided into two parts, as follows: Part 1 (90 minutes) Counselor Recall Trainee #1 wearasked-to-role-play the client and trainee #2 was asked to role-play the counselor. They were instructed to enter I.PzR. Suite A and role-play a concern that was real. The client was told to talk about something he would like help with: 239 ”You have come to this counselor because you would like to talk about an area of concern to you. You are asking for assis- tance." The counselor was instructed: "The client has come to you to talk of an area of concern to him; we want you to respond to this individual who has come to you seeking assistance, in helping him to understand his feelings and work out his concerns." The role playing session ran approximately 10 minutes and was videotaped. After this, the videotape was rewound and made ready for counselor recall. The interrogator entered Suite A, the client left the room, and the interrogator struc- tured the recall (by giving the same instructions as were given in Session 2). Because the purpose of this portion of the session was counselor recall, the counselor was focused on be- coming aware of his influence in the interaction and communica- tion which has taken place. The client was instructed to simply observe the videotape replay on the T. V. monitor in the other room. The counselor recall lasted for approximately % hour, after which the two trainees reversed roles (i.e. the trainee who role played the counselor was nowiinstructed to role play the client for the next 10 minutes, and vice versa for the other trainee) to enable both trainees to be recalled as a counselor. The time allotted for Part 1 of the session is broken down as follows: 240 Part 1 (1% Hours) 1 - first 40 minutes (a) 10 minute role play (b) 30 minute counselor recall trainee #1 . counselor trainee #2 = client 2 - second 40 minutes (a) 10 minute role play (b) 30 minute counselor recall trainee #1 - client trainee #2 - counselor Part 2 (90 minutes) Mutual Recall The second part of the session focused on a mutual recall of both counselor and client of a videotaped interview just conducted between the same two trainees. The two trainees were assigned to role-play counselor/client as previously done in Part 1 of the same session, and this was videotaped. The struc- ture of this portion of the task was the same as Part 1, the only difference being the substitution of mutual recall for counselor recall. The role playing sessions ran approximately 10 minutes and were videotaped. After the role playing was completed, the videotape was rewound and made ready for recall. The inter- rogator entered I.P.R. Suite A with the client remaining in the room the interrogator structured the recall session as previously done, focusing on the reciprocal interaction of client and counselor behaviors as it unfolded in the counseling session tape. The interrogator involved the counselor and client to- gether, encouraging the two trainees to interact with each 241 other to share the feelings they had as they role played. This was done by focusing on areas such as: l-What was the client feeling? 2-What was the counselor feeling? 3-What would the counselor like to be for the client? 4-What does the client want the counselor to be for him? S-How does the counselor see the client? 6-How does the client want the counselor to see him? 7-Does the client feel comfortable with the counselor? Why? The time allotted for Part 2 of the session is broken down as follows: Part g_ (1% Hours) 1 - first 40 minutes (a) 10 minute role play (b) 30 minute mutual recall trainee #1 = counselor trainee #2 = client 2 - second 40 minutes (a) 10 minute role play (b) 30 minute mutual recall trainee #1 = client trainee #2 = counselor Week 6 Session 6 Purpose: The purpose of this session was to obtain middle measures on each individual on the same criteria, and using the same procedure, as Session 1. Weeks 7-10 Experimental Group Sessions 7-10: Purposes: Those individuals who were receiving the Con- trol treatment for Weeks 2-5 (Sessions 2-5) 242 of the traditional classroom lectures now received the developmental tasks which comprised the Experi- mental treatment for the first half of the course. Description of Tasks: (Sessions 2-5, Experimental Treatment) The description of tasks for this portion of the course follows the format established for the Experimental Group during Sessions 2-5. Week 11 Session 11 Purpose: The pUrpose of this session was to obtain post- measures on each individual on the same criteria, and using the same procedures as Session 1. APPENDIX F TRAINEE SCHEDULES AND SYLLIBUS FOR PROCEDURES IN COUNSELING COURSE TRAINEE: (1-10) Group (1) WEEK 1 Introduction.Schedules, Measurements Tuesday evening 7-10 P.M. Group Room, Erickson Hall, Room 250 WEEK 2 Becoming Sensitive To Own Feelings (1) Tuesday Evening 7-10 P.M. Group Room, Erickson Hall, Room 250 (2) Two hour-block individually Erickson Hall, Room 217 WEEK 3 Becoming Sensitive To Client Feelings Tuesday evening 7-10 P.M, Group Room, Erickson Hall, Room 250 WEEK 4 (1) Learning Elements Of Effective Counseling Tuesday evening, 7-10 P.M. Group Room, Erickson Hall, Room 250 (2) Practice Elements Of Effective Counseling Group Two hour block Group Room, Erickson Hall, Room 250 WEEK 5 Counselor and Mutual Recall (Bilateral) Counseling Relationship Pair for Three hour block Erickson Hall, Room 217 WEEK 6 Measurements Tuesday evening, 7-10 P.M. Group Room, Erickson Hall, Room 250 WEEKS 7-10 Classroom Discussion Themes Tuesday evening, 7-10 PJH. Seminar Room, Bessey Hall, Room 108 WEEK 11 Measurements Tuesday evening, 7-10 P.M. Group Room, Erickson Hall, Room 250 244 TRAINEE: WEEK 1 WEEKS 2-5 WEEK 6 WEEK 7 WEEK 8 WEEK 9 WEEK 10 WEEK 11 245 (ll-20) Group (2) Introduction Schedules, Measurements Tuesday evening, 7-10 P;M. Group Room, Erickson Hall, Room 250 Classroom Discussion Themes Tuesday evening, 7-10 P.M. Seminar Room, Bessey Hall, Room 108 Measurements Tuesday evening, 7-10 P.M. Group Room, Erickson Hall, Room 250 Becoming Sensitive To Own Feelings (1) Tuesday Evening 7-10 P.M. Group Room, Erickson Hall, Room 250 (2) Two hour-block individually Erickson Hall, Room 217 Becoming Sensitive To Client Feelings Tuesday evening, 7-10 P.M. Group Room, Erickson Hall, Room 250 (1) Learning Elements Of Effective Counseling Tuesday evening, 7-10 P.M. Group Room, Erickson Hall, Room 250 (2) Practice Elements Of Effective Counseling Group Two hour block Group Room, Erickson Hall, Room 250 Counselor and Mutual Recall (Bilateral) Counseling Relationship Pair for Three hour block Erickson Hall, Room 217 Measurements TUesday evening, 7-10 P.M. Group Room, Erickson Hall, Room 250 246 Classrouu_giscussiup.Thempp Goals of Counseling - Conversation with a Purpose Theories of Counseling Vocational Counseling Test Interpretation Listening to and Critiquing Tapes Guest Lecturers Group Behavioral Counseling (Socioeconomic) Class Differences in Counseling 8:30-8:45 8:45-10:00 Weeks 1, 6, 11 WEEK. SCHEDQEE Introduction Counsel with client Room 1 Break Role playing, Rm. 6 8:00-8:15 counselor 8:15-8:30 client Break Group scales Sample Schedule For Critericn Measures WEEK SCHEDULE Introduction Counsel with client Room 2 Break Role playing, Rm. 6 8:00-8:15 client 8:15-8:30 counselor Break Group scales APPENDIX C RATIONALE AND TRAINING FOR COACHED CLIENTS Coached Clients A. Rationale When comparing measures of counselor in-interview be- haviors over a number of counselors, client differences can be an important source of variance which must be accounted for. Ideally, counselors should be compared on the way they counsel the same individual so that only differences in counselor skill becomes manifest. In practice, this is un- feasable and unrealistic. In order to reduce the variance caused by different clients, measures should be taken on a standardized stimulus (or as close to this standard stimulus as possible), so as to minimize the differences caused by different clients. This approaches the ideal of having each counselor measured on the same stimulus. The use of coached clients in counselor education and measurement of counselor in-interview behavior is a viable and important resource for simulating counseling experiences and coming close to standard- izing the client variability. The coached clients can be taught to behave in a variety of ways which simulated those experiences the practicing counselor will face. According to Whiteley and Jakubowski (1969): "If the coached clients' role is carefully constructed, it can facilitate the necessary shift in counselor education and evalua- tion from what the counselor ip,supposed £p_pe_in terms of some static trait model toyfigpfiipepand Mpeisggbehave in the counseling interview. The counselor who participates in a coached-client inter- view is required to demonstrate behavior- 248 249 ally what he would gp_and how he would act in a counseling situation." (p. 19). In a very real sense, then, it may be possible that the simulated experience with the coached client can still main- tain the aura of an authentic counseling session, where the counselor is required to behave as facilitatively as possible to the situation and client concerns. In addition to providing a simulated counseling situa- tion, the use of coached clients can help to standardize the counseling situations. When using coached clients for research purposes, it is important to have client consis- tency in terms of behaviors demonstrated in the interviews. Coached clients should be consistent in the way they each present themselves over a number of interviews with the same and different trainees, and for each coached client to be as similar as possible to each and every other coached client, to insure that trainee in-interview behaviors are as comparative as possible. Because the purpose of the present study was designed to examine a program of counselor education based on a series of developmental tasks in discrimination and communication skills in counseling, one criteria for counselor growth was the degree to which the counselors were able to display effective communication behaviors in the interview session. In view of the purposes of the present study, the role of the coached client was developed so that clients would 250 receive practice in talking about a variety of personally relevant areas so that if-the counselor moved into an area the client would be willing and able to talk about that area of her life. It was decided that specific, pre-deter- mined client statements may not appear spontaneous and/or genuine, may appear awkward for certain individuals, or may be poorly times in delivery. The coached clients were given the set to continue to explore themselves in certain pre- determined areas if_they were encouraged to do so by a counselor who facilitated exploration (the coached clients were encouraged, to include as many of the areas that were rehearsed as possible). B. Training Part i, - Introduction to purpose and discussion of desired behaviors in coached clients: The coached clients met as a group, and were given the following introduction to the purpose of their task: ”The purpose of the present study is to attempt to teach counselors in training to be more effective counselors. Coun- selors can become more effective if they can work towards understanding clients' thoughts and feelings and how these feel- ings they have can influence behaviors, and if they can learn how to communicate with clients in such a way that explora- tion of these thoughts and feelings is made easier and clearer. The purpose of your role is to serve as a coached client for these trainees. 251 These trainees will interview you three times (for the pre, middle, and post measure), and will be given the instructions to get to know you, by not only understanding how you think, but especially how you feel about yourself and different things. They will be told to help you explore some of these feelings you have. Your task is to explore thoughts and feelings you have, in certain pre-determined ways, if the counselor en- courages you to do so." If the coached clients had any particular concerns, re- garding the study and how their role fits into the overall scheme, a recapitulation of the model and the purposes of the study were in order. After the coached clients were introduced to the pur- pose of their task, a discussion was held as to the desired ways they were to respond. Because the coached clients were either students or the wives of students (and had re- cently graduated themselves), a number of areas of concern were evident which they could all personally relate to and talk about. Three such areas were chosen: Clients' attitudes toward and their relationships with their husbands as graduate students; clients' attitudes toward school and/or work; clients' attitudes toward campus unrest. The coached clients were given the following instructions: "There are different ways that you can com- municate with others. You can talk about your thoughts and you can talk about your feelings regarding these and other thoughts. While we are attempting to train counselor candidates to be able to help you talk about what you are thinking, the main pur- pose is to get them to help you explore 252 your feelings. Therefore we want you to be able to talk to the counselors about 'feel- ings' you might have about personally rele- vant materials. Certain pre-determined areas of conversation have been selected for you to talk about. Rather than have you memorize a pre-determined sentence or dialog, you will be asked to talk in three areas: 1. your attitudes toward and your relation- ships with your husbands as graduate students; 2. your attitudes toward school and/or work; 3. your attitudes toward campus unrest. You will also be asked to be able to talk about these areas incorporating various types of response cues into your conversation. These will reflect various degrees of emotion, and include: 1. tentativeness-statements which reflect con- fusion which attempt to communicate to the counselor that you are unclear about your thoughts and feelings, ranging from mildly unsure (e.g. "I'm not sure how I feel...", "Perhaps" "I think") to confusion with possible anxiety correlates (e.g. "I'm scared, and don't know what's happening", "I'm not sure and this has me worried"). 2. negative affect-statements designed to re- flect negative feelings that you might have, ranging from a mildly negative de- gree (e.g. "I don't really like that", "Not really") to strong negative affect (e.g. "I dislike intensely", "I don't give a damn", "I'm very unhappy with that"). 3. positive affect-statements designed to reflect positive feelings you might have ranging from a mildly positive amount of joy and elation (e.g. "That's O.K.", "I like ", "It pleases me ___j§ to strong positive affect (e.g. ”I'm very happy, satisfied ___V, "I really like "Very nice "). 253 Pap; 2_- Practicing the role of coached client. This part of training involved practicing the role of coached client. Two coached clients paired up and one role played the client, the other the counselor, for approximately five minutes. The coached client worked on being able to talk about the pre-determined concerns. Discussion followed each role playing session, focusing on how authentic and spontaneous it appeared, and how the coached client might involve more certain types and degrees of affective state- ments into their response repertoire. After this, the two were told to try a few more minutes more, incorporating some of the comments into their role. They were also encouraged to practice on their own, rehearsing (vocally and subvocally) dialogs and making the desired kinds of responses. This portion continued until all had role played the client and had gotten feedback on their portrayal. Pa££_3_- Recapitualtion and Summary of coached client roles. This part of training focused on "tying up loose ends” in the form of answering questions concerning how the role related to the task of interviewing, and how the coached clients might continue to practice the roles and types of Statements which were rehearsed. APPENDIX H Intercorrelations Of Gain Scores On The Affective Sensitivity Scale For Groups 1 And 2 Over 'Intervals 1 And 2 Pearson Product Moment Intercorrelations of measures involved in formation of gain scores (rpre,mid rpre,post) for each of the treatment groups on the ASS from mid-pre and post-pre time intervals. r1 r2 rpre, mid rpre,post Gro 1up -851 .952 G o Overall .828 .879 255 111111111 llllllllll 6 5 7 7 4| 3 0 3 9 2 1 3 111111 will