..... PERIPHERAL SEXUAL MORPHOLOGY, SENSITIVITY AND ANDROGENIZATION= THEIR EFFECTS ON MALE SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN RATS AND HAMSTERS Dissertation for the Degree 'of Ph. D. MICHIGAN STATE UNIVERSITY ARCHIE J. VOMACHKA 1976 fins“? 3’) L113I21‘? RY This is to certify that the thesis entitled Peripheral Sexual Morphology, Sensitivity and Androgenization: There Effects on Male Sexual Behavior, In Rats and Hamsters presented by Archie J. Vomachka has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for PH .D. degree in ZOOLOGY Major professor June 7, 1976 I)ate 0-7639 ABSTRACT PERIPHERAL SEXUAL MORPHOLOGY, SENSITIVITY AND ANDROGENIZATION: THEIR EFFECTS ON MALE SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN RATS AND HAMSTERS. By Archie J. Vomachka Bilateral section of the dorsal nerve of the penis (DNP) in male rats increased mOunt frequency but eliminated intromission and ejacu- lation. Unilateral section of the DNP caused a transient decrease in intromission and ejaculation frequency. Treatment with fluoxymesterone (FM) in castrated unsectioned males maintained mounting, intromission and ejaculation, but FM‘was not as effective as testosterone propionate (TP. The effects of fluoxymesterone on mounting were—sabolished by sec- tioning thelnfl’suggesting that fluoxymesterone exerts its faciliatory effect upon mounting via an influence upon genital morphology. In a second experiment, male and female hamsters castrated on day 1 (day of birth considered day l) were treated neonatally with fluoxy- mesterone, testosterone or the hormone vehicle. At adulthood, mounting behavior and lordosis behavior were assessed. 'While the degree of penile or clitoral virilization did not differ between testosterone and fluoxymesterone treated animals, behavioral differences occurred. Neonatal treatment with fluoxymesterone failed to "masculinize" or "defeminize" behavior. Neonatal treatment with testosterone induced mounting and intromission. It was concluded from these results and those of others that fluoxymesterone has little or no central neural effects. Archie J. Vomachka A third experiment employed male hamsters castrated on the day of birth and treated with TP or estradiol benzoate (EB) on days 2, 3 and 4 of life. Neonatal treatment with TP but not EB induced penile viri- 1ization as adults. TP induced sexual behavior in these animals was assessed prior to and following sectioning of the DNP. Penile desensi- tization had little effect upon hamster sexual behavior regardless of the degree of penile virilization. Replacement with either fluoxy- mesterone or sesame oil caused a decline in sexual behavior but no statistical differences were observed among sectioned or sham animals or among neonatal treatment groups. Fluoxymesterone treated animals in all neonatal groups tended to mount longer than sesame oil treated animals but this trend was nonsignificant. Penile size or sensitivity in hamsters appears to have little effect upon the maintenance of sexual behavior. PERIPHERAL SEXUAL MORPHOLOGY, SENSITIVITY AND ANDROGENIZATION: THEIR EFFECTS ON MALE SEXUAL BEHAVIOR IN RATS AND HAMSTERS By ,__ 3,\ Archie J? Vomachka A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Zoology I976 ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS I wish to thank the members of my guidance committee, Dr. John A. King, Dr. John R. Shaver and Dr. Harold D. Hafs for their valuable assistance throughout various portions of my graduate training. A special word of thanks must go to "The Chief", Dr. Lynwood G. Clemens, for having faith in my abilities. Thank you for smoothing the rough spots and developing my academic interest. I could not have done it without your guidance and understanding. 7 I wish to acknowledge the love and able assistance of my wife, Mary, who provided the occasional boost_and continued devotion throughout the last few years. To my fellow graduate students, past and present, the technicians in the Hormones and Behavior Lab and the cheerful, helpful secretaries, thank you one and all for the help and good times. ii TABLE OF CONTENTS List of Tables. . . . . . . . . List of Figures . . . . . . . . . . . . GENERAL INTRODUCTION. . . . . . . . . EXPERIMENT 1. . . . . . Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . EXPERIMENT 1, PART 1. . . . . . . . . . Methods and Materials. . . . . . . Results. . . . . . . . . . . . . Ejaculations. . . . . . . . . Intromissions .'. . . . . Mounts. . . . . . . . . . . Latency Measures. . . . . . . Histology . . . . . . . . . . EXPERIMENT1,PART2.......... MEthods and Materials. . . . . . . Results. . . . . . . . . . . . . . Ejaculations. . . . . . . . Intromissions . . . . . . . . munts I O I O O O O O O O 0 Discussion . . . . . . . . . . . EXPERIMENT 2. . . . . . . . . . . . . . Introduction . . . . . . . . . . Methods and Materials. . . . . . Subjects. . . . . . . . . . . Treatment Groups. . . . . . . Test Procedures . . . . . . . Morphological Measures. . . . Statistica1.Analysis. . . . . Results. . . . . . . . . . . . . Mounting Behavior of Males. . 'Mounting Behavior of Females. Lordosis Behavior of Males. . Lordosis Behavior of Females. Morphological Measures. . . . iii Page . v vii l . 2 . 4 . 4 . 8 . 8 . 10 12 14 . 17 18 . 18 . 19 O 19 . 19 . 19 . 21 . 27 . 27 . 30 . 30 . 31 . 32 . 33 . 34 . 34 . 34 O 38 . 38 . 38 . 44 Table of Contents cont'd. Ovarian Histology. . . . . . . . . . . . . . Discussion. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . EHERIMNT 3 O O O O I O O O O O 0 O O O O O O O O O I Introduction. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Methods and Materials . . . . . . . . . . . . . Subjects . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Treatments and Behavior Testing. . . . . . Morphological Measures . . . . . . . . . . . Statistical Analysis . . . . . . . . . . . . Re8u1t8 I O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O 0 Effects of Neonatal Treatments (Tests 1-5) Effects of Penile Desensitization (Tests 6-8). Effects of Hormone Manipulation (Tests 9- 16) Morphology . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Discussion. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Conclusion. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . APPENDIX 0 o o o o o o e o o o o o o o o o o o 9 g o . BIBLIOGRAPHY . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . iv Table 1. 2. A2. Al. 0 Mean Latency Measures for Male Rats Prior to and Following LIST OF TABLES Section of the Dorsal Nerve of the Penis. Effects of Hormone Manipulation on Mean Latency Measures (seconds) of Male Rats With or Without Section of the Dorsal Nerve of the Penis . Mean mount latency for male hamsters castrated on the day of birth and treated neonatally with fluoxymesterone, testosterone or sesame oil (in seconds i s.e.). . . . . . . Mean amount latency for female hamsters treated neonatally with fluoxymesterone, testosterone or sesame oil (in Q seconds i s.e.) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Morphological measures of male hamsters castrated on the day of birth and treated neonatally with fluoxymesterone, testosterone or sesame oil. Morphological measures of female hamsters treated neonatally ‘with fluoxymesterone, testosterone or sesame oil. . . . . . Mean anogenital distance, penile bone plus cartilage lengths (in mm) and penile condition in male hamsters treated neonatally with TPlOO, TPZS or EB 25. . . . . . . . Mean latency to first mount (in seconds) for castrated male hamsters treated neonatally with TP or EB and given various adult treatments. Mean head and side mount frequency in castrated male hamsters treated neonatally with T? or EB and given adult treatments . Mean rear mount frequency in castrated male hamsters treated neonatally with TP or EB and given various adult treatments. Mean intromission frequency in castrated male hamsters treated neonatally with TP or EB and given various adult treatments. V O Page 15 16 37 41 45 45 60 67 68 69 7O Table Page A5. A6. A7. Percentage of male hamsters showing head and side mount, rear mount or intromission for behavioral tests. Animals were castrated on day 1, given TPlOO neonatally, and received various adult treatments. . . . . . . . . . . . . . 71 Percentage of male hamsters showing parameters of male mating behavior. Animals were castrated on day 1, given TP25 neonatally and received various adult treatments. . . . 72 Percentage of male hamsters showing parameters of male mating behavior. Animals were castrated on day 1, given EBZS neonatally and received various adult treatments. . . . 73 vi LIST OF FIGURES Figure l. 2. 9. 10. 11. 12. Effects of penile desensitization and hormone replacement on ejaculation in male rats. . . . Effects on penile desensitization and hormone replacement on intromissions per minute active in male rats. . . . . . . Effects on penile desensitization and hormone replacement on mounts without intromission per minute active in male rats. Effects of neonatal administration testosterone or sesame oil on mean in castrated male hamsters . . . . Effects of neonatal administration testosterone or sesame oil on mean castrated male hamsters. . . . . . Effects of neonatal administration testosterone or sesame oil on mean frequencies in male hamsters . - - Effects of neonatal administration testosterone or sesame oil on mean female hamsters. . . . . a a . . . Paremeters of lordosis behavior in of fluoxymesterone, head and side frequencies of fluoxymesterone, rear mount frequencies in o o, o o g o o g o o .10 o of fluoxymesterone, head and side mount of fluoxymesterone, rear mount frequencies in castrated male hamsters treated neonatally with fluoxymesterone, testosterone or sesame oil . . . . . . . . . . . . Parameters of lordosis behavior in female hamsters treated neonatally with fluoxymesterone, testosterone or sesame oil. Rear mount frequency and intromission frequency in day 1 castrated male hamsters treated with TPlOO neonatally and given various adult treatments . . Rear mount frequency and intromission frequency in day 1 castrated male hamsters treated with TP25 neonatally and given various adult treatments . . vii Page 11 13 35 36 39 40 .42 43 54 55 Figure Page 13. Rear mount frequency and intromission frequency in day 1 castrated male hamsters treated with EB25 neonatally and given various adult treatments. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 56 viii GENERAL INTRODUCTION While the importance of gonadal hormones for the control of male 'mating behavior is recognized, where gonadal hormones act to control sexual behavior has been debated extensively. While there is evidence to indicate that the gonadal steroids stimulate behavior by action on the central nervous system, there is also reason to suspect gonadal hormones acting upon genital (Peripheral) structures, exert control over sexual behavior. The problemthas been summed up most effectively: Tell me, where is fancy bred, In the heart or in the head? In the glands (of which the're legions) Or is it in the pelvic regions? (Kohn, 1971) The following series of experiments examine the role of penile size and sensitivity in the sexual behavior of rats and hamsters. Experiments were designed to test hypotheses related to the peripheral control of sexual behavior. EXPERIMENT 1 Introduction Experimental evidence suggests that there are two modes by which gonadal hormones may facilitate male sexual behavior in the rat; 1) by direct action upon the central nervous system, 2) by action upon genital morphology. In support of a central action, it was found that testosterone implanted into the preoptic area restored mating behavior in castrated rats when given in quantities far below the amount re- quired for stimulation of seminal vesicles (Davidson, 1966; Lisk, 1967). Electrical stimulation of the posterior hypothalamus or lateral pre- optic area lead to stimulus bound copulation or activation of sexual behavior (Caggiula & Hoeble, 1966; Caggiula, 1970; Madlafousek, Freund & Grofova, 1970, Caggiula & Szechtman, 1972). By the same token, discrete lesions in the medial forebrain area (Hitt, Hendricks, Gins- berg & Lewis, 1970, Hitt, Byron & Mbdianos, 1973) and medial preoptic area (Giantbnio, Lund and Gerall, 1970; Singer, 1968) abolished male sexual behavior in the presence of exogenous androgens. Experimental decreases in genital sensory feedback or penile structure in rats, cats, and monkeys reduced or eliminated intromission and ejaculation behavior but did not cause immediate decreases in mounting (Beach & Holz, 1946; Carlsson & Larsson, 1964; Adler & Bermant, 1966; Aronson & Cooper, 1966 & 1968 & 1969; Whalen, 1968; Herbert, 1972; Larsson and Sodersten, 1973). Evidence for hormonal effects upon sexual behavior via genital morphology was suggested by Beach 5 Levinson (1950) 2 who correlated the decreased size of penile spines following castration with a decrease in ejaculation and the "tendency to copulate." Nadler (1969) gave cyproterone acetate, an anti—androgen, pre— natally to rats and attributed the lack of male copulatory behavior in the offspring at adulthood to deficient penile development. In addition, neonatal castration of male rats resulted in a reduction in penile size and weight. Limited androgen administration neonatally and/or in adult- hood did not overcome this effect of castration on genital size (Beach & Holz, 1946; Grady, Phoenix & Young, 1965; Whalen, 1968). Likewise, neonatal treatment of male rats with gonadotropin antiserum produced animals with penes of a reduced size and a smaller number of penile spines than normal control animals (Goldman, Quadagno, Shryne & Gorski, 1972). These neonatal manipulations with antiserum resulted in animals which mounted a receptive female but failed to intromit or ejaculate. While these experiments suggest the importance of penile structure in male sexual behavior, the treatments used to achieve a reduction in penile development may also have resulted in a reduced central neural androgenization or virilization. Consequently, we cannot establish the relative contribution of central and peripheral factors to male sexual behavior. In an attempt to circumvent some of these problems, Hart (1972) gave fluoxymesterone or TP to day 4 castrated male rats and tested these animals for sexual behavior as adults in response to testosterone treat- ment. Fluoxymesterone is known to have strong virilizing effects on genital morphology. While animals treated neonatally with T? show high levels of mounting, intromission and ejaculation, fluoxymesterone treated animals showed a marked reduction in ejaculation. However, the penes of the fluoxymesterone treated animals were not visibly different from those of the T? treated animals. Hart therefore suggested, that "the behavioral difference between the groups could not be attributed to peripheral factors" (p. 845). In a study of adult rats, Beach & Westbrook (1968) gave fluoxymesterone to non-copulating, castrated males and although mating behavior was not reinstated, the penile spines and papillae as well as seminal vesicle morphology were restored to the level of testosterone treated animals. The data from these experiments suggest that fluoxymesterone may not have strong effects upon the central nervous system even though it is a powerful androgen for penile tissues. In the present study we provide experiments to determine whether penile sensory feedback may facilitate mounting, even though it may not be necessary for this behavior. We surgically sectioned the dorsal nerve of the penis (DNP) in male rats and tested them for copulatory behavior. These animals with desensitized penes were placed on various hormone regimes.‘ Testosterone propionate was used as an androgen with central neural activity as well as an agent to maintain penile morphology. Fluoxymesterone was used to provide androgenic stimulation of peripheral genital structures,presumably in the absence of strong central effects upon male sexual behavior. EXPERIMENT 1, PART 1 Methods and Materials The subjects for this study were 47 male rats of the Long-Evans strain. These animals were 90-100 days old at the beginning of the experiment and were housed in groups of 3-5 in stainless steel cages with food and water available ad_libitum. Lights in the colony were off from 1100 hr. to 2100 hr.; testing began 1-3 hours after the lights had gone off. Temperature and humidity were maintained at a constant level. All rats were castrated under ether anesthesia 2 weeks prior to the beginning of testing, and were placed on daily injections of testosterone propionate (TP): SOO'ug in 0.1 cc sesame oil. At 13-18 days post-castration, animals were selected for the ex- periment on the basis of sexual performance in a selection test in which each animal was placed with a sexually receptive stimulus female. Stimulus females were ovariectomized and brought into sexual receptivity with exogenous estrogen and progesterone. Animals which failed to mount the female in 10 minutes or failed to ejaculate within 20 minutes were excluded from the study. In order to establish a baseline of sexual activity, two pretests of 20 minutes each (tests 1 and 2) were given to the experimental animals 4 and 8 days after the selection test. In each pretest, a sexually receptive female was placed into a 2'x2' plexiglass arena with the experimental male after the male had been allowed to adapt to the arena for at least 5 udnutes. All subsequent testing followed the same procedure. At the completion of the second pretest, animals were randomly divided into 2 groups. In one group, a 3-5 mm section of the dorsal nerve of the penis (DNP) ‘was removed bilaterally from.each animal. Animals in the remaining group received a sham operation. The opera- tions were performed under ether anesthesia. The skin was incised and the penis retracted. The fascia was removed to expose the DNP, which was then lifted and a section removed. For the sham operated animals, the DNP was similarly exposed, however, it was neither lifted nor sectioned. Animals were allowed a recuperation period of 14-17 days before re- ceiving 3 postoperative tests (tests 3-5) at 4 day intervals. Upon com- pletion of the third postoperative test, the 2 groups were subdivided into a total oftigroups and assigned to the following hormone treatments: 1) 2) 3) 4) 5) 6) TP Cut, n=8. These animals with DNP section continued to receive 500 ug TP daily. TP Sham, n=8. These were Sham Operated animals which received 500 ug TP daily. Fluoxymesterone Cut, n=8. These animals with DNP section as well as all of the following groups, received no further TP administration. Replacement in this group was 500 ug of fluoxymesterone (Halotestin. Upjohn) daily, suspended in 0.1cc sesame oil. Fluoxymesterone Sham, n=8. These Sham operated animals re- ceived 500 pg fluoxymesterone daily in 0.1cc sesame oil. 011 Cut, n=7. These animals with DNP section received 0.1cc sesame oil (the hormone vehicle used in this study) daily. Oil Sham, n=8. These Sham operated animals received 0.1cc sesame oil daily. Following the onset of these group assignments, animals were tested on day 4, 8, 12, 17, 21, 28, 35, 42, 49 and 56 (tests 6-15). For each 20 minute test, all mounts, intromissions and ejaculations were recorded on an Esterline-Angus Event Recorder. This provided a temporal sequence of behaviors and the following measures were derived from the record: 1) 2) 3) 4) 5) 6) 7) 8) 9) Mount Latency. The time in seconds from introduction of the stimulus female at the beginning of the test until the first mount by the male, up to 1200 seconds for an animal which failed to mount. Intromission Latency. The time in seconds from the beginning of the test until the first intromission response by the male, or 1200 seconds for animals which did not intromit. Ejaculation Latency. The time in seconds from the first intro- mission to ejaculation, or in subsequent ejaculations, time from first intromission in a series to ejaculation. In animals which failed to ejaculate, ejaculation latency was taken as 1200 seconds minus the intromission latency. Mount Frequency. Number of mounts without intromission. Intromission Frequency. Number of mounts with intromission. Ejaculation Frequency. Total number of ejaculations in a 20 minute test. Post-ejaculatory Interval. Time in seconds from ejaculation to the reinitiation of copulatory activity. Mounts Per Minute. Mount frequency divided by total test time (20 minutes) minus total time in post-ejaculatory interval. Intromissions Per Minute. Intromission frequency divided by total test time minus total time in post-ejaculatory interval. Following ejaculation, the male rat enters a post-ejaculatory interval (PEI) lasting 200-300 seconds. During this period, he shows no copulatory behavior. Since DNP cut animals rarely ejaculated, they seldon entered a PEI. Animals with no ejaculations often mounted regularly throughout the entire 20 minute test, while animals with multiple ejaculations were sexually active for a much shorter amount of time. In such a situation, elevated mount frequencies for cut groups per 20 minute test would reflect a lack of the PEI normally seen in ejaculating males. To correct for this, comparisons of mount frequencies and intromission frequencies were made on the basis of frequencies per minute active. Thus, for animals achieving ejaculation, the PEI was subtracted from the 20 minute tests, and dividing mount frequency or intromission frequency by this number gives behaviors per minute active. At the completion of testing, all animals were sacrificed and penes were removed. Penes were fixed in Bouin's solution, divided into glans and shaft portions and embedded in parafin. The tissue was sectioned at 10 u and representative sections were mounted on glass slides and stained for histological examination. Results Ejaculations. ‘The mean ejaculation frequencies for each group across all tests are shown in Figure 1. Comparison of ejaculation frequency scores from the 2 pretests with the ejaculation frequency scores from the postoperative tests 3-5 showed that sectioning the DNP nearly abolished ejaculation (Sign Test, p<0.001). In tests 3-5, sham animals showed a statistically significant increase in ejaculation frequency over the pretest scores (tests 1 & 2) (Sign Test, p=0.002). N H o 25 P a c u R T E TP SHAM o c " 2 20 R H .9.- a A ~ 5 IA ‘3 8 ‘.‘ :MP #\ k s 15 ~“y. O - I.“ ‘ P \a a5 ,3 FM SHAM m' A I I pit SHAM 1.0 0 IN E O‘ o 0.5 ‘-, ’0..- 17).". .. [A ,~ _ \ FMqCUT . ‘ \‘h ' .‘ ’.’ ~~ “:' '*-'d«:’ “_-O ‘.‘ '0. .TIE‘CUT ' ~ 01L _cur'\ \ 0 "out-:0 h-- a." w H_q ‘ ‘ ‘“w 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 TEST Figure 1. Effects of penile desensitization and hormone replacement on ejaculation in male rats. 10 The effect on ejaculation frequency of replacing TP with either fluoxymesterone or oil in cut and sham groups is also shown in Figure 1, tests 6—15. In all DNP section groups, ejaculation frequency re- mained low throughout the experiment. The TP Sham group showed no significant change in ejaculation frequency between tests 6 and 15, whereas there was a significant decrease in ejaculation frequency be- tween tests 6 and 15 (Wilcoxon Matched Pairs, Signed - ranks Test, p<0.01 and p<0.02 respectively) for both the Fluoxymesterone Sham and Oil Sham groups. At test 6, the scores for the sham groups were not significantly different, however, by test 15, a statistical difference was obtained among groups (Kruskal-Wallis, 1 way AOV by ranks, p<0.01). Ejaculation frequency in the TP Sham group was significantly higher than in the Fluoxymesterone Sham group (Mann-Whitney U-test, p<0.01); likewise, the Fluoxymesterone Sham scores were significantly greater than were those of the Oil Sham group (MWU, p<0.01). Intromissions. Mean intromission scores per non-PEI minute for each group are shown in Figure 2. DNP section resulted in a significant decrease in intromission scores in tests 3-5 as compared with tests 1 and 2 (Sign Test, p<0.001). For the sham group, intromission scores did not differ significantly between pre- and post—operative tests. Comparison of test 6 scores with test 15 scores showed that both Fluoxymesterone Sham and Oil Sham animals had significant decreases in intromission scores over time (Wilcoxon, p<0.002), while TP Sham animals showed no change in intromission frequency. At test 15, TP, Fluoxymesterone, and Oil Sham animals were significantly different on intromission measures (K-W, p<0.01), whereas, in test 6, these groups 11 H D m A III 2 c 8 t3 1’ E < c I; 8 2 Z S g 2 H a III E a. O o S E R A 2 T 0 g N IL 0 g ',.o"'s H 34 5 6 7 8 9101112131415 Figure 2. Effects of penile desensitization and hormone replacement on intromissions per minute active in male rats. 12 were not different from each other. Onttest 15, TP Sham animals achieved intromission at a higher rate than the Fluoxymesterone Sham animals (MWU, p<0.05) and likewise, Fluoxymesterone Sham animals had a significantly higher intromissions-per-minute score than did the Oil Sham animals (MWU, p=0.002). Intromission scores for the cut groups remained low throughout tests 6—15. Mounts. Mean mount frequency per non-PEI minute for each group is shown in Figure 3. Mount scores in the DNP section groups showed a significant increase between pre- and post-operative tests (Sign Test, p-0.034), however, sham groups showed no change when comparing tests 1 and 2 with tests 3-5. In Fluoxymesterone Sham and Fluoxymesterone Cut groups, mount scores decreased significantly between tests 6 and 15 (Wilcoxon, p<0.05 and p<0.002, respectively), as did Oil Sham and Oil Cut groups (Wilcoxon, p<0.002 and p=0.002, respectively). All TP animals remained unchanged over time. Although the 6 groups did not differ significantly at test 6, a statistically significant difference in mount scores was seen among the 6 groups in test 15 (K-W, p<0.01). In test 15, TP Sham animals had higher mount scores than did the Fluoxymesterone Sham animals (MWU, p<0.05) and Fluoxymesterone Sham animals had significantly higher mount scores than the Oil Sham group (MWU, p<0.02). In the cut groups, TP Cut animals achieved significantly higher mount scores at test 15 than did the Fluoxymesterone Cut animals (MWU, p<0.002), however, Fluoxymesterone Cut animals were not different from Oil Cut animals. 13 an I R a. 8 Lu ~ . 2 I' P 3’ "s, 5‘ 5 C I" "' 0 < l1! N " E IL" 0 C 3 *4: R H g 15 . " a N P 1" G 1.0 h 1M E (I) p. Z 8 05 b 2 5 O l J L A l g 1 2 3 4 TEST ,, OIL SHAM I x on. cur-g 5M. ’ o I- ‘w "am-"W”— . J L I J l j I l L 6 789101112131415 TEST Figure 3. Effects of penile desensitization and hormone replacement on mounts without intromission per minute active in male rats. l4 Latengy Measures. Results of dorsal nerve section on latency measures can be seen in Table l. Sectioning the DNP caused a signifi- cant increase in ejaculation latency (Sign Test, p=0.002) with no change in mount latency or intromission latency for pre- and post- operative scores. In sham operated animals, a statistically significant decrease in ejaculation latency was observed (Sign test, p<0.034) with no changes in mount latency or intromission latency between these same tests. Latency scores for tests 6 and 15 can be seen in Table 2. Following the change in hormones in test 6, mount latency and intromission latency were not significantly different among the 6 groups, although ejaculation latency did differ among the groups (Kruskal-Wallis, p<0.02). By test 15 a significant difference for all measures was noted among groups (KPW; EL, IL, & ML p<0.001). Comparison of test 6 latency scores with test 15 scores within groups gives an indication of changes over time. TP Sham animals showed no change in mount latency or intromission latency measures but did exhibit significant increases in ejaculation latency (Wilcoxon, p<0.05): TP Cut animals showed no change in mount latency or ejaculation latency but did increase in intromission latency (W., p<0.05) in a comparison between the 6th and final test. Fluoxymesterone Cut and Sham animals as well as Oil Cut and Sham animals all exhibited statistically significant increases in mount latency, intromission latency and ejaculation latency between tests 6 and 15 (W., all p values<0.02). 15 Table 1. Mean Latency Measures for Male Rats Prior to and Following Section of the Dorsal Nerve of the Penis. Test 2 Test 5 Mount Cut M 18.3 9.5 Latency (sec.) SE i 3.7 i 1.7 Sham M 10.1 6.5 SE i 2.3 i 0.6 Intromission Cut M 56.0 187.9 Latency (sec.) SE $17.7 $73.4 Sham M 34.1 29.8 SE :12.3 i 9.4 Ejaculation Cut M 518. 0 1047 . O Patency (sec.) SE $71.6 $53.5 Sham M 460.9 356.8 SE $75.3 $70.2 16 .mwoom onu mo o>uoz Hanson mnu mo aowuoom uponuwz no sues mood can: mo Amoaououv sensuous unsound nous so soauoaonfiomz oaoahom mo uuomwwm m.~m o.mm n.5nH e.coa o.mo m.H um H.5HHH e.HHH o.~coa H.Hm0H H.HHH o.w 2 use ado o.o o.o m.HmH e.oma c.a m.m um o.oo~a o.oo~H o.nen H.Hme <.¢N e.m 2 Lemon ado ~.m ~.H¢H o.nma e.aNH w.mm e.H mm m.HmHH m.mmoa w.mam .m.Hom 0.50 a.» x uso 2m n.qHH m.mqa n.oeH w.m¢ H.m w.o mm m.oHo o.oo< m.mma e.HH~ o.mH m.m : seem an ¢.hm n.5n m.q m.ne N.MH e.~ Mm o.m~oa o.N~H m.HH H.aoHH w.me o.oa z uoo my m.mm ~.nu ~.m n.~w m.m H.H mm m.oaq m.mq «.ma o.mn~ o.a~ m.o z swam ma monsoon unsound hummus; unsound zoomuma Monsoon coauoasoonu doammaaouuoH uwooz aoaumflsomfim scammwaouuoH ammo: n.— uooa o omen. .N manofi 17 Between group comparisons of test 15, latency scores revealed that although Fluoxymesterone Cut animals were not different from Oil Cut or Sham animals in ejaculation latency, intromission latency or mount latency, Fluoxymesterone Sham animals showed significantly shorter latencies in all measures than did Fluoxymesterone Cut, Oil Cut or Oil Sham.animals (Mann Whitney U, p<0.02 in all comparisons). Scores of Fluoxymesterone Sham animals compared to the TP Sham animals at test 15 showed that the TP group had a significantly shorter mount latency (MWU, p<0.05) and intromission latency (MWU, p<0.05) but that TP and Fluoxy- mesterone Sham animals did not differ with respect to ejaculation latency. On test 15, TP Cut animals had a shorter mount latency than the Fluoxy- mesterone Sham animals (MWU, p<0.05) while intromission latency did not differ between these groups. Fluoxymesterone Sham animals had a signifi- cantly shorter ejaculation latency than did the TP Cut animals on test 15 (MWU, p<0.05). Histology. Microscopic examination of cross sections of the glans penis confirmed earlier observations (Beach and Levinson, 1950; Beach and Westbrook, 1968) in that penes of animals treated with TP and Fluoxymesterone were indistinguishable, while those animals treated with daily sesame oil showed a marked regression of papillae and a nearly complete lack of penile spines. Within hormone treatments, no difference could be seen between DNP Cut and Sham animals. Although a large difference in behavior was seen between cut and sham animals, no histo- logical differences were evident upon examination of the shaft. The following study was run to assess the effects of partial penile desensitization on male sexual behavior in rats. 18 EXPERIMENT 1, PART 2 Methods and Materials Subjects for this experiment were 19 male rats of the Long-Evans strain, approximately 120 days old at the beginning of the study. They were housed under the same conditions as were those in Experiment 1, Part 1. All animals were castrated as adults and given TP replacement of 500 ug daily. Behavioral measures and conditions were identical to those in Experiment 1, Part 1. A selection test was given two weeks after castration. At 4 and 8 days after the selection test, animals were given a 20 minute pretest with a sexually receptive female to establish a baseline of copulatory behavior. Following a second pretest 1 day later, animals were randomly assigned to a cut or sham group. DNP sectioned animals were divided into 2 groups of 6 animals each. One group received a surgical section of the left dorsal nerve of the penis; in the second group, the right dorsal nerve of the penis was sectioned. Following a 2 week recuperation period, all animals were tested 3 times at 4 day intervals (Tests 3-5). After test 5, surgery was again performed to section the remaining nerve. Following a two week recovery period, the males were given the final 3 tests at 4 day intervals (Tests 6-8). Sham animals received 2 sham operations and were tested similarly. Since unilateral sectioning of the left and right produced scores which were not different, the data were pooled for statistical analysis. 19 Results Ejaculations. The effects of cutting one of the DNP and then the other on ejaculation frequency are shown in Figure 4. Surgical section of one nerve caused a decrease in ejaculation frequency in test 3, when compared to pretest levels. However, this decrease was followed by a recovery in test 5. Ejaculation frequency for DNP section animals and sham animals was not significantly different in test 1 and test 5. However, recOvery of ejaculation frequency was not seen following sec- tioning 0f the second nerve. Test 8 scores for ejaculation frequency were significantly lower in the sectioned animals than in the sham operated animals (MWU, p<0.002). Within groups, comparing test 1 with test 8, sectioned animals showed a significant decrease in ejaculation frequency (Wilcoxon, p<0.002), while scores for the sham animals showed no change between tests 1 and 8. Intromissions- Intromission scores on tests 1-8 are shown in Figure 4. Unilateral sectioning caused a decrease in intromission scores, but intromission frequency returned to the level of the Sham group by test 5. Bilateral sectioning caused a sustained decrease in intromissions-per-minute, as seen in Experiment 1. Sectioned animals had significantly lower intromission-per-minute scores than did the sham animals on test 8 (MWU, p<0.002). Sectioned animals showed a significant decrease in intromissions—per-minute on test 8 as compared to the pretest score on test 1 (Wilcoxon, p<0.002). Sham animals did not change in intromissions-per-minute between tests 1 and 8. Mounts. Mount scores for all tests are given in Figure 4. Sectioning of one of the dorsal nerves had no effect on mount scores 20 CU'T’.’ IEEUURURUUUUUU. SHAM ‘ I I IUUUEUURUURU...‘ SECOND DNP CUT OR SHAM EUUEUUUURUU.UUUO CUTI_, .4 w .s h I.I. I.l.l Ii. .I.l.l .t . .Io IHHIIIHIIHHH ”memmwwtnmtunfimfim y. . o. 1 .0 - - - I- . n - n u n - - I-Ir - n n u m. m m o m m w m. a o m m m whDZ=2 mun. 9.2302 3.32.5. mum wszmgomhz. mZO_._.<._DU<—.u TEST Effects of partial and complete penile desensitization on mounting, intromissions and ejaculations in the male rat. Figure 4. 21 when compared to sham animals on test 5. However, following the second nerve section, a significant increase in mounts-per-minute was noted on test 8 (MWU, p<0.01). Sectioned animals showed a significant in- crease in mounts-per-minute between tests 1 and 8 (Wilcoxon, p<0.002), but sham animals had scores on tests 1 and 8 which did not differ. Discussion Androgenic steroids and their metabolites vregulate'male sexual behavior by action at various sites. In the adult male rat, evidence indicates that the central nervous system, particularly the medial preoptic area (MPOA) and its connections represent a major site of androgen action. Intracerebral implants of testosterone into the MPOA will reinstate mating behavior in castrated animals (Davidson, 1966; Lisk, 1967; Christensen & Clemens, 1974). However, minute quantities of testosterone in the brain enter the peripheral circulation in amounts high enough to cause stimulation of penile tissue and growth of penile spines and papillae (Kierniesky & Gerall, 1973). While this penile stimulation might be regarded as only a side effect of the presence of central androgen, it renders conclusions based on these findings diffi- cult to interpret in terms of strict central control of male behavior. Intracerebral implants of estrogen, the aromatized metabolite of testosterone, are also very effective in restoring male behavior in male rats, however, no penile virilization is seen with estrogen implants (Christensen & Clemens, 1974). Stimulation of behavior without concom- mitent penile stimulation, ejaculatory maintenance by systematic injections of EB (Sgdersten, 1973), studies of estrogen uptake (Whalen & Littge, 1970) and in_vitro studies of brain tissue 22 metabolism.of steroids (Naftolin, Ryan & Petro, 1971 a,b) have been noted. These findings have led to the hypothesis that estrogenic metabolites of androgens may be the centrally active compounds respon- sible for male mating behavior. It has been suggested that testosterone's action may be achieved by its metabolic products; So reduced androgens appear necessary for morpho- logical maintenance of penile and sexual accessory tissues, while estro- genic metabolites or their precursors function centrally (Parrott, 1974). That androgens may influence sexual behavior at a peripheral locus has been suggested by the finding that the administration of presumably peri- pherally active non aromatizable androgens such as dihydro—testosterone (DHT) presents or slows down the loss of ejaculatory performance in castrated male rats (Parrott, 1974 a,b). Although androgen metabolites such as DHT will stimulate penile and sexual accessory tissues, DHT, even in large doses will not reinstate male mating behavior (McDonald et a1. 1970; Whalen & Luttge, 1971). Brain implants of DHT are also in- effective (Johnston & Davidson, 1972). Recent evidence has shown that administration of estrogen, in com- bination with DHT or fluoxymesterone, to castrated male rats will induce the full complement of male sexual behavior (Baum.& Vreeburg, 1973; Larsson, Sgdersten & Beyer, l9733,b; Feder, Naftolin & Ryan, 1974; Johnson & Tieffer, 1974). Presumably, such treatment provides a peri- pherally active androgen for appropriate stimulation and androgen main- tenance of the genitalia, coupled with activation of central neural tissues which mediate sexual behavior. Studies of male sexual behavior utilizing estroge, DHT combination indicate that the administration of EB + DHT to castrated male 23 rats with the DNP sectioned yielded results similar to those of intact rats (Clemens & Vomachka, unpublished observations). However, upon withdrawl of estrogen treatment, animals with desensitized penes still receiving DHT showed a more rapid decline in mounting behavior than intact animals receiving only DHT. The more rapid decline in rats with desensitized penes suggests an important behavioral role of penile tissues. In the present study, fluoxymesterone administration after TP withdrawl produced 3 effect similar to that seen with EB + DHT following desensitization. Animals with desensitized penes showed a more marked decline in behaviors than animals with intact penile afferent nerves. While these experiments suggest a synergistic influence of brain and genital mechanisms, it is apparent that genital influences are neither necessary nor sufficient for mounting behavior. Likewise, treatment with testosterone propionate maintained mount frequencies at a high level even after section of the DNP. These findings with TP and fluoxymesterone suggest that sensory feedback from the penis may have a facilitative influence upon behavior, but in the presence of centrally active steroids, this influence is masked. Studies of the influence of other sensory processes on sexual behavior have suggested that sensory effects are additive in terms of their influence on sexual behavior and that like penile sensitivity, the other sensory channels are not individually essential for the initiation of mating (Beach, 1942, Adler & Bermant, 1966; Carlsson & Larsson, 1964). For example, the effects of deprivation of different sensory modalities, including blinding, anaemia and deafening, on male 24 sexual behavior were explored in a classic study by Beach (1942). Loss of each modality individually does not eliminate copulation in sexually vigorous males. However, elimination of two or more of these did cause a reduction in copulation. Given the additive nature of sensory stimu- lation, it might be predicted that loss of penile sensations combined with loss of other sensory modalities would result in decreased sexual behavior. The question remains as to how penile afferent sensations, sup- ported by androgenic agents, mdght exert an effect on sexual arousal. An analogy may be drawn to the effects of intromissions on ejaculation. It has been suggested that the input of information from each.intro- mission adds to the general excitatory state of the animal's central nervous system or maintains the excitatory state until the ejaculatory ‘mechanism and ejaculation occurs (Beach, 1956: Sachs & Barfield, 1974). An animals readiness to mount a receptive female may also be influenced by penile stimulation in a similar manner. Day to day stimuli from the fluoxymesterone treated penis, whether from contact with the penile sheath, grooming, copulation, or non-specific tactile input, may in- crease a central excitatory state. This state then becomes manifest in the form of mounting when the proper stimulus condition is met. In the normal animal with a high androgen titer in the CNS, these penile contributions to sexual arousal, seen as shorter latencyand higher frequency measures, are masked and only act to augment a highly stimulated motivational mechanism. In the present preparation, with reduced central androgenization but with selective androgenic support of penile tissue, a possible contribution of penile stimuli to behavior- al maintenance becomes evident. 25 While fluoxymesterone treatment maintains penile morphology and facilitates mounting, the effects of FM on penile sensitivity are not known. Beach and Levinson (1950) postulated a sensory role for penile spines and papillae. However, in a series of neurophysiological ex- periments, Cooper and Aronson (1974) found that genital sensory fields and sensory thresholds were not different between intact and castrated cats. Assuming that this finding in another species applies to rats as well, where might androgen act to facilitate penile sensory processes? One site of androgen action could be at the level of the spinal cord. Possibly fluoxymesterone maintained penile input to the CNS through spinal pathways to a greater degree than that seen in oil treated animals or animals with the DNP severed. Hart (1967, 1968, 1973) has investigated the decrease in sexual reflexes in spinal male rats fol- lowing castration. Such decreases may reflect changes in the neural functioning of the spinal cord involved in the transfer of information from the genitalia to the CNS. Cooper and Aronson (1974) report that castration alters central processing of genital sensory input, in that stimulation of penile afferent nerves of castrates and non castrates resulted in differences in various measures of neocortical activity. The "thalamus or cortex or other sensory relay or integrative centers" were proposed as possible areas involved in the observed differences between castrates and non-castrates. In this light, the spinal path- ways, devoid of androgen stimulation could conceivably alter penile afferent sensations and thereby alter behavior. Of course, the possibility remains that fluoxymesterone is actually acting at other central neural sites to maintain sexual 26 behavior. Although this seems unlikely, the following experiment was designed to test the central neural androgenicity of fluoxymesterone in the developing animal. EXPERIMENT 2 Introduction Androgenic steroids which stimulate growth of genital and sexual accessory tissues do not always stimulate male sexual behavior. Di— hydrotestosterone (DHT) is one such peripherally active compound. This steroid has been shown to have little influence upon behavior but is a powerful virilizing agent peripherally (Feder, 1971; Johnston and Davidson, 1972; Parrott, 1974 a,b, and c, 1975; Christensen, Coniglio, Paup, and Clemens, 1973; Feder, Naftolin and Ryan, 1974). Another such compound is fluoxymesterone which was originally administered clinically to humans (Reilly and Gordon, 1961). When given to stimulate growth in boys of short stature, a concomitant phallic enlargement was noted without "evidence of stimulation of libido or masturbation". In rats fluoxymesterone would not restore the loss of mating in long term castrated males but did stimulate the normal size of seminal vesicles (Beach and westbrook, 1968). Likewise, the penile epithelium, which generates and becomes smooth following castration, was maintained with papillae and cornified penile spines by fluoxymesterone. Johnson and Tiefer (1974) also found that fluoxymesterone, given to castrated male rats, did not maintain mounting, intromission or ejaculation any better than did sesame oil. However, combination of estradiol benzoate (EB) treatment with fluoxymesterone was highly 27 28 effective in maintaining sexual behavior at a level above that achieved with either EB or fluoxymesterone alone. Similar findings with DHT in combination with EB (Baum and Vreeburg, 1973; Larsson, Sgdersten and Beyer, 1973 a and b; Feder, Naftolin and Ryan, 1974) have strength- ened the hypothesis that sexual behavior is the result of both central neural activation of behavior (by estrogenic steroids) and maintenance of genital tissues (by peripherally active steriods). In an experiment designed to determine the extent to which penile development is influential in the development of male sexual behavior, Hart (1972) administered either testosterone propionate (TP) or fluoxymesterone to neonatal male rats and tested their sexual behavior and sexual reflexes as adults. Comparison between the sexual behavior of animals which had been castrated on day 4 and received TP on days 5, 7, 9, and 11 and those which were castrated and received fluoxy- mesterone showed that fluoxymesterone animals achieved fewer ejacu- lations. While the fluoxymesterone treated animals did mount and intromit, vehicle control animals achieved significantly fewer intro- missions. Since gross penile development and reflexes were not different between the androgen treated groups, Hart could not attribute the behavioral differences observed to peripheral factors. The results support the concept that ejaculation behavior requires the influence of androgen during neonatal development (Hart, 1972). That Hart could not find differences between testosterone and fluoxymesterone treatment neonatally on mounting and intromission does not mean that these behaviors develop independent of steroid influences. Lack of differences may have been the result of the nature of the 29 animal with which he was working. In the rat, testicular differentiation occurs on about day 13 (post coital age), while behavioral sexual differ- entiation occurs between days 18 and 27 (see Clemens, 1973). When castrated on day 4 of postnatal life (assuming that birth occurs on day 21 post coital) neonatal male rats have already been exposed to their own endogenous androgens for at least 7 days. Conclusions at to the "organization" of mounting and intromission based on post natal hormone administration to rats are tenuous at best. Since the hamster has a relatively short gestation period (16 days) it offers a better model for studying androgenic control of behavioral development. While testicular differentiation occurs at about 11.5 days post coital, behavioral differentiation appears to be entirely post natal, beginning at 16 days post coital age (Nucci and Beach, 1971; Clemens, 1973). The male hamster castrated on the day of birth (day 1) will not show mounting, intromission, or ejaculation as an adult even when given large doses of androgen as an adult. On the other hand, the female or Day 1 castrated male hamster which receives gonadal hormones during the first few days of life will show mounting and sup- pressed lordosis behavior at adulthood (Carter, Clemens and Hoekema, 1972; Paup, Coniglio, and Clemens, 1972, 1974; Coniglio, Paup, and Clemens, 1973; Coniglio, 1973). Consequently, the estent to which neural or genital virilization influence behavioral sexual differen- tiation and later behavior may be more easily examined in the hamster. The following experiment: 1. tests the virilizing effects of fluoxymesterone upon behavior and genital tissue when administered to day 1 castrate male or 30 female hamsters. 2. tests two alternate hypotheses regarding the mechanism of androgen influence upon development and the control of mating: a) Does treatment neonatally with fluoxymesterone induce a behavioral differentiation similar to that seen with testos- terone, and b) is male sexual behavior in the hamster programmed prior to the neonatal organization of peripheral tissues. 3. provides comparative data on the development of sexual behavior in rodents. The timing of behavioral sexual differentiation in the hamster makes it a more suitable subject than the rat to study the androgenicity of certain compounds (meachka, Paup, Coniglio, McManus and Clemens, 1974), the control of sexual differentiation (Clemens, 1973) or the extent to which sexual behavior is controlled by central or peripheral stimuli (see Experiment 3). Methods and Materials Subjects The subjects of this experiment were 27 male and 36 female hamsters (Mesocricetus auratus) born and raised in the Hormones and Behavior Laboratory at Michigan State University. All animals were given food and water §g_libitum and were maintained on a reversed day-night 31 light cycle of 14 hours light, 10 hours dark with lights out at 11:00 hours. All animals were weaned at 21 days of age and housed in unisexual groups of 2-6 animals. Cages used were plexiglass with removable stainless steel tops with dimensions of 26X20X15 cm or 35X3OX17 cm depending upon the size of the group. Treatment Groups 0n the day of birth (day 1), males were castrated through a mid- ventral incision using hypothermal anesthesia. The incisions were sutured and sealed with flexible collodion and the pups were returned to their mothers. On days 2, 3, and 4 of postnatal life, all animals in each litter received a subcutaneous injection of one of the follow- ing substances: fluoxymesterone (lOOug/day) suspended in sesame oil, testosterone (lOOug/day) free alcohol form, or the hormone vehicle, sesame oil (0.03 cc/day). Subcutaneous injections were administered under the skin of the back with needle entry in the posterior back region and the bolus of hormone being deposited under the skin of the neck. Upon withdrawl of the needle, puncture sites were sealed with flexible collodion and animals were inspected for leadage of hormone before being returned to their mothers. At 60 days of age, all animals were earpunched for identification and females were ovariect- omized under ether anesthesia. Ovaries were fixed in Bouin's solution, embedded in paraffin, sectioned and stained with hematoxylon-eosin for histological examination. 32 Test Procedures MOunting Behavior: At approximately 75 days of age, all animals began weekly testing for mounting behavior. Testing (1300-1700 hr.) was conducted in an air conditiOned annex to the animal quarters which were dimmly illumi- nated. Observation arenas were 10 gal. aquaria with Sani-cellr (ground corn cobs) covering the floor. All mounting behaviors during a 10 minute test were recorded on an Esterline-Angus even recorder. Tests followed a 3 minute adaptation period during which the experi- mental animal was placed alone in the observation arena. After the adaptation period, a sexually receptive stimulus female was introduced into the arena and behaviors were recorded. Receptivity was induced by administration of 12ug EB for 3 days and 0.5 mg progesterone to ovarectomized females on the morning of testing. Stimulus females were proven receptive prior to experimental use by placing themwwith vigorous nonexperimental males. Male and female experimental hamsters were tested weekly for 5 con- secutive weeks. Following the first test for mounting (pretest) daily administration of 300ug TP was used to induce mounting. 'All mounts and intromissions were recorded and mounts without intromission were further divided into catagories as to whether they were directed to the head or side, or to the rear of the receptive stimulus female. A mount ‘was scored as such only if it was accompanied by a clasp and pelvic thrusting. In addition, the length of time to the first mount and/or intro- mission was measured (mount latency and intromission latency). If no sexual behavior was observed during a particular test, a latency 33 measure of 600 seconds (the length of the test) was assigned for that test. Following the final test for mounting, TP administration ceased. _ Lordosis Behavior: At approximately 30 and 44 days following the fifth test for mounting behavior, 10 minute tests for lordosis behavior were given to all experimental animals. Lordosis tests were preceded by daily admini- stration of 6 ug EB for 3 days and an injection of 0.5 mg progesterone on day 4, 4-6 hours prior to testing. Testing occurred between 1300 and 1700 hours. Measurement of lordosis behavior entailed placing the experimental animal into an observation arena (10 gal. aquarium) with a sexually vigorous indicator male and recording the lordosis behavior of the experimental animal. Indicator males which did not mount were replaced. Measures derived from the Esterline-Angus recorder record included the frequency of lordosis exhibited, the total amount of time spent in lordosis and the duration of the longest lordosis re- sponse during each 10 minute test. A mean for each of these measures was taken for each animal. Morphological Measures Approximately one month after the final lordosis test, all animals were sacrificed and inspected for the presence of testicular or ovarian tissue. If gonadal tissue remained, the data from that particular animal were eliminated from the study. Anogenital distance was measured in all animals. Penile or clitoral bone and cartilage was dissected out and the total length of these was measured. 34 Statistical Analysis The data were evaluated using one way analysis of variance and further comparisons were made using the Student-Newman-Keuls test for multiple comparisons among means (Sokal and Rohlf, 1969). Results MountingZBehavior of Males Twenty eight days of TP administration induced high levels of mounting in males treated neonatally with testosterone (Fig. 5 and 6, Table 3) but not in animals treated neonatally with fluoxymesterone or sesame oil. By test 4 (28 days) analysis of variance revealed a significant difference among groups for mount latency, head and side mount frequency, and rear mount frequency. While animals treated neo- natally with T showed significantly higher head and side mount scores and rear mount scores than the other groups (p<0.05), animals treated neonatally with fluoxymesterone or sesame oil did not differ statistic- ally (F-6.58, df=2,24, p=0.005; F=ll.9l, df=2,24, p<0.01; and F=3.64, df-2,24, p<0.05 respectively). Similarly, fluoxymesterone and sesame oil treated animals showed significantly longer latencies to the first mount than did the neonatal testosterone group (p<0.05). Fluoxymesterone and sesame oil groups did not differ in mount latency. The only group to show any intromission patterns was the neonatal testosterone group (mean intromission frequency - 1.8 on test 4). 35 TEST s m o nuanbm anOW aaIs cm (was NVSW Figure 5. Effects of neonatal administration of fluoxymesterone, testosterone or sesame oil on mean head and side frequencies in castrated male hamsters. 36 2 TEST Pro ADNIODJUi .lNflOW 8V3! NVIW Figure 6. Effects of neonatal administration of fluoxymesterone, testosterone or sesame oil on mean rear mount frequencies in castrated male hamsters. 37 Table 3. Mean mount latency for male hamsters castrated on the day of birth and treated neonatally with fluoxymesterone, testosterone or sesame oil (in seconds i s.e.). Neonatal Treatment Weekly Tests (d2-3-4) Pretest 1 . ' 2 3 4 Fluoxymesterone 600 600 600 573 458 (100 ug/day) i0 i0 i0 :20 £73 Testosterone 600 389 p 310 261 106 (100 113/ day) :0 :80 :85 :86 :50 Sesame Oil 600 600 454 389 373 (0.03 cc/day) i0 i0 . :74 :84 £92 38 Mounting Behavior of Females At 28 days of daily TP treatment, females treated neonatally with testosterone showed higher head and side mount and rear mount fre- quencies than fluoxymesterone or sesame oil treated females (p<0.05). Fluoxymesterone and sesame oil treated females were not different from each other in mount frequencies (Fig. 7&8). Fluoxymesterone and sesame 1011 groups did not differ from each other in latency to the first mount (Table 4). However, both groups took a longer time to the first mount than did the testosterone treated females, (H&SMF, Fb8.83, df-2,33, p<0.01; RMF, Fb15.49, df-2,33, p<0.001; and ML, Fb7.05, df-2,33, p< 0.005). Intromission patterns were not seen in any of the treatment groups. Lordosis Behavior of Males Testosterone treated animals showed more lordoses than did fluoxymesterone treated animals (F-4.24, df-2,23, p<0.05) but the treated animals were not different from sesame oil animals (Figure 9). Likewise, fluoxymesterone and sesame oil animals were not different from each other in lordosis frequency. Lordosis Behavior of Females Females treated neonatally with testosterone showed more lordoses than sesame oil or fluoxymesterone treated animals (Figure 10) (L.F. F-5.20, df-2,33, p<0.025, and Longest L F-4.53, df-2,33, p<0.025) while sesame oil and fluoxymesterone animals were not significantly different. In contrast to lordosis frequency, the mean longest lordosis for testosterone 39 ‘- M 5 N Ill .— F O b O. 2 ‘0 ° ADNJOOSIH lNflOW 301$ ONV Oval-l NVSW Figure 7. Effects of neonatal administration of fluoxymesterone, testosterone or sesame oil on mean head and side mount frequencies in female hamsters. 4O 0— —O—O—1Q ~ 9. 2 IL . -I” .— . .Nm .— . 1|— 2 I '1; I J I l l l 1 ~O V N O ADNinOBUi .0400“! I“!!! NVBW Figure 8. Effects of neonatal administration of fluoxymesterone, testosterone or sesame oil on mean rear mount frequencies in female hamsters. 41 Table 4. Mean mount latency for female hamsters treated neonatally with fluoxymesterone, testosterone or sesame oil (in seconds:ts.e.) Neonatal Treatment Weekly Tests (d_2-3-4) Pretest 1 2 3 4 Fluoxymesterone 600 600 571 ‘ 523 445 (100 ug/day) i0 i0 , :29 .t49 :60 Testosterone 461 170 123 49 132 (100 ug/day) 161 :66 :55 :10 :53 Sesame 011 550 600 535 473 457 (0.03 cc/day) :50 i0 :76 :76 £75 42 sanoaas NI (canvas) SIsoaaot Isaoum nvaw auv nouvana 5500301 111101 uvaw § 3 § .3. In .— I: In E ‘( m .‘L‘ if .- ‘1 z: 8 + 92 l:— "' + E “1 fl' 0: c1 " ADNJDOJUJ 51500801 NVBW Figure 9. Parameters of lordosis behavior in castrated male hamsters treated neonatally with fluoxymesterone, testosterone or sesame oil. 43 sonooas NI (OiOVHS) SIsoozIO'I 1539N01 nvaw an Mouvana SIsoaIIOI 1v101 nvaw C) C) C) c) CD CD CD 0 n v 8 l' T NEONATAL TREATMENT In Q ('5 N '- ADNiflOBIH SISOOUO'I NVJW Figure 10. Parameters of lordosis behavior in female hamsters treated neonatally with fluoxymesterone, testosterone or sesame oil. 44 treated females was shorter than that of either fluoxymesterone or sesame oil treated females. Morphological Measures Males: While anogenital distance did not vary significantly among the three groups, analysis of variance did reveal a significant difference among treatments in length of penile bone and cartilage (F=58.63, df-2,24, p<0.01). Neonatal fluoxymesterone and testosterone treatment caused a significant increase in penile length when compared to sesame oil treated animals (Table 5) (P<0.05). Fluoxymesterone and testos- terone treated males did not differ from each other significantly. Females: Neonatal treatment with fluoxymesterone on testosterone resulted in a longer anogenital distance than did sesame oil treatement (Fh69.5, df=2,33, p<0.001); fluoxymesterone and testosterone treated animals were not different from each other (Table 6). Fluoxymesterone and testosterone treated females, while not different from each other, showed greater clitoral virilization than did sesame oil treated females (F=58.4l, df=2,30, p<0.001). Ovarian Histology None of the neonatal treatments, fluoxymesterone, testosterone, or sesame oil disrupted ovarian function. Ovaries possessed.corpora- lutea and developing follicles, and appeared normal. 45 Table 5. Morphological measures of male hamsters castrated on the day of birth and treated neonatally with fluoxymesterone, testosterone or sesame oil. Neonatal treatment (d 2-3-4) AeG distance mean length in mm Penile bone and cartilage mean (i s.e.) length in mm ($s.e.) Fluoxymesterone 16.0 4.6 (100 Ug/day) $0.4 $0.0 Testosterone 15.2 4.5 ;(100 ug/day) $0.4 $0.0 Sesame Oil 14.7 3.4 '(0.03 cc/day) $0.2 $0.0 Table 6. ‘Morphological measures of female hamsters treated neonatally with fluoxymesterone, testosterone or sesame oil. Neonatal treatment . (d 2-3-4) A-G distance mean length in mm Clitoral bone and cartilage mean ($s.e.) length in mm ($s.e.) Fluoxymesterone 10.2 4.0 1 (100 ug/day) $0.2 $0.0 Testosterone , 10.3 3.9 (100 ug/day) $0.2 $0.0 Sesame Oil 7.2 3.0 (0.03 cc/day) $0.2 $0.0 46 Discussion While neonatal administration of fluoxymesterone or testosterone to castrated male and female hamsters induced similar patterns of penile and clitoral growth in response to androgen at adulthood, only testosterone achieved significant masculinizing influence upon behavior. Fluoxymesterone neither "masculinized" nor "defeminized" behavior. These findings as well as those of others (Reilly and Gordon, 1961; Beach and westbrook, 1968; Johnson and Tiefer, 1974; Hart, 1972) strengthen further the idea that fluoxymesterone is a peripherally acting androgen with little or no central neural actions. The absence of mounting and intromission in fluoxymesterone treated animals in spite of marked genital virilization agrees with other work in which a dissociation of sexual behavior and genital virilization was found (Coniglio, 1973; Paup et a1., 1974). The fact that the present results with hamsters do not agree entirely with those Hart obtained with the rat may be attributable to species or dosage differences. Indeed, male rats castrated neonatally normally show male sexual be- havior (Larsson, 1966) when given androgen as adults, while hamsters do not. Although the neonatal dosages of hormone given did not induce ejaculation in any group, mounting and intromission were induced by testosterone given neonatally. This suggests that in the hamster, mounting and intromission are not organized independent of androgen as has been speculated for the rat (Beach and Holz, 1946; Whalen, 1968). 47 Whalen (1968) argued that in the rat, the reduced incidence of intro- mission in males castrated neonatally reflects inadequate penile development. Similar conclusions have been reached by Nadler (1969) and Goldman et a1., (1972). Loss of intromission after various mani- pulations including removal of sections of penile bone (Beach and Holz, 1946), the glans penis (Whalen, 1961) anesthetization of the penis (Carlsson and Larsson, 1964; Adler and Bermant, 1966) and removal of the dorsal nerves of the penis (Larsson and Sgdersten, 1973) lend credence to the hypothesis that successful intromission is a function of an adequate phallus. Although the phalli of the testosterone and fluoxymesterone groups in this as well as in Hart's study (1972) were not different, intromission behavior always occurred with greater frequency in the testosterone treated groups. It is proposed that testosterone, prenatally in the rat, and postnatally in the hamster programs the CNS for mounting. FM on the other hand lacks this effect, but shares with T the ability to virilize the genital morphology. The differences in T & FM in intromission scores reflect the differential effect of these hormones on mounting behavior. Since intromissions cannot occur without a mount, an androgen which does not program the CNS for mounting cannot induce intromissions. In the present study, higher doses of testosterone neonatally or more prolonged administration might have induced adult ejaculations. It is doubtful that increased fluoxymesterone would have, since ejaculation was not induced in rats with much higher doses of fluoxy- mesterone . EXPERIMENT 3 Introduction Evidence that sensory feedback from the penis is necessary for intromission and ejaculation has accumulated for several species (Adler and Bermant, 1966; Whalen, 1968; Aronson and Cooper, 1966, 1968; Herbert, 1973; Larsson and Sgdersten, 1973). However, manipu- lations which result in changes in penile sensitivity, size, and structure of the penis do not have as profound an effect on mounting as they do on intromission (Beach and Holz, 1946; Adler and Bermont, 1966; Whalen, 1968; Nadler, 1969; Goldman et a1., 1972). The fact that penile sensitivity has little influence on mounting .is not entirely clear. Beach and Levinson (1950) postulated that the loss of mounting behavior subsequent to castration in rats was correlated with a loss in penile sensitivity. Even though electro- physiological research suggests that castration does not cause a decrease in penile sensitivity in cats (Cooper and Aronson, 1974), it has been shown that decreases in peripheral sensitivity produced by local anesthesia or nerve section can lead to decreases in mounting over time (Carlsson and Larsson, 1964; Aronson and Cooper, 1968; Herbert, 1973). Experiment 1 of this thesis showed that penile sensory feedback may have a facilitatory effect upon mounting as well as intromission and ejaculation in rats. In order to extend the work of Experiment 1 and to entend the comparative basis for theories of 48 49 the peripheral influence on male mating behavior, the hamster was chosen for further work. The questions asked in this experiment are: 1) Does penile sensory feedback facilitate male mating behavior in the hamster? 2) Does sexual behavior vary as a function of penile size in the hamster? Sexual differentiation of male mating behavior in hamsters is presumably under the control of gonadal steroids present during some "critical period" in development (Clemens, 1973; Eaton, 1970; Coniglio, 1973). The presence of estrogens or androgens which can be aromatized to estrogens during the period of behavioral sexual differentiation leads to an animal which will show mounting as an adult. In the. absence of such androgen or estrogen the female behavior pattern of receptivity (lordosis) but not mounting behavior are evident at adulthood. Gonadal steroids also affect the growth potential of the genital tissue of the male. The presence of androgens during develop- ment results in a phallus which will respond with growth to pubertal increases or exogenous administration of androgens at adulthood (Bruner and Witschi, 1946; Beach, Noble, and Orndoff, 1969; Coniglio, 1973). However, estrogens, which will cause behavioral masculinization do not increase the potential for genital virilization (Coniglio, 1973, Paup, Coniglio and Clemens, 1974). This dissociation of behavioral and morphological development with estrogens and the concomitant 50 behavioral and morphological virilization with androgens can be used to create an experimental model to study the influence of penile sensory feedback on behavior. Neonatally androgenized and estrogenized animals have a similar potential for mounting but a different potential for phallic growth when given T? as adults. It was presumed that sensory feedback from the penis in neonatally androgenized hamsters would be greater than the sensory input from the unvirilized neonatal estrogenized animal., The present experiment attempted to.reveal differential feedback effects on the maintenance of mounting behavior. Fluoxymesterone maintains penile size and integrity but not sexual behavior while section: (DNP) 0f the dorsal nerve of the penis provided a presumably complete reduction in penile sensations. Methods and Materials Subjects The subjects of this study were 107 male golden hamsters (Meso- cricetus auratus) born and raised in the Hormones and Behavior Laboratory at Michigan State University. Animals were maintained as outlined in Experiment 2. Treatments and Behavior Testing_ All males were castrated on the day of birth (day 1) using the procedures described in Experiment 2. On days 2, 3, and 4 of life all animals received subcutaneous injections of one of the following hormones: testosterone propionate, either lOOug or 25ug per day (TP 100 or TP25) or estradiol benzoate 25 ug/day (E825). Injections were administered under the same procedural paradigm as Experiment 2. 51 At 60 days of age all animals were earpunched for identification. When the animals were 75 days of age they entered a weekly testing schedule for mounting behavior with test procedures and measures identical to those in Experiment 2. The first 5 weekly tests were the same as those in Experiment 2, namely, a 10 minute pretest prior to T? treatment, daily injections of 300ug TP and 10 minute tests at 7 day intervals for 28 days. Following the 5th test for mounting behavior, animals from each of the neonatal treatment groups were randomly assigned to one of two groups; to be called Cut or Sham. Males in the Cut groups were sub- jected to bilateral removal of a 2-4 mm section of the dorsal nerve of the penis (DNP). Procedures used were the same as in Experiment 1. Animals were allowed a recuperation period of 10 days with daily TP injections before receiving 3 weekly postoperative tests (tests 6-8). Following the third postoperative test, TP injections were terminated and animals from cut and sham groups were assigned to one of the fol- lowing two hormone conditions. Condition 1 consisted of daily admini- stration of 300ug fluoxymesterone. Condition 2 was no exogenous hormone at all but daily injections of the sesame oil vehicle (0.05cc). All animals were then tested for mounting at weekly intervals for 8 weeks (tests 9-16). Behavioral measures were the same as those taken for male behavior in Experiment 2. The final groups for data analysis are as follows: TP IOOEg neonatal 1) FM CUT: DNP section; fluoxymesterone for the final 8 tests. n-9 52 2) FM SHAM: sham operated males; fluoxymesterone for the final 8 tests. n=11 3) OIL CUT: DNP section; sesame oil for the final 8 tests. n=10 4) OIL SHAM} sham operated; oil vehicle for the final 8 tests. n=lO . TP 25 pg neonatal 1) FM CUT n=8 2) FM SHAM n=6 3) OIL CUT n-IO 4) OIL SHAM n=10 EB 25 uggneonatal 1) FM CUT n=9 2) FM SHAM n=9 3) OIL CUT n=8 4) OIL SHAM n=7 These 12 final groups were sacrificed 1-2 days after the completion of test 16. Morphological Measures At sacrifice all animals were inspected for completion of gonadect- omy. Any evidence of remaining testicular tissue eliminated the animals from.the experiment. Ana-genital distances were measured in all animals. Penile bone and cartilage were removed as a unit and measured in all animals. Portions of the glans penis‘were fixed in Bouin's solution for histological examination. Penile glans tissues were embedded in paraffin, sectioned at 10 u, stained with hematoxylin- eosin and examined for the presence of spines and/or papillae. 53 Statistical Analysis Data.were evaluated using one way analysis of variance. Further comparisons were made using the Student-Newman—Keuls test for multiple comparisons among means (Sokol and Rohlf, 1969) or Mann-Whitney U-test. Although not listed in results section,S-N-K and MPW-U p<0.05. Beagles. Results of neonatal treatment (Tests 1-5), penile desensitization (Tests'6—8) and hormone manipulation (Tests 9-16) on parameters of male mating behavior will be considered separately. Appendix Tables Al-A4 give means $ standard errors for various copulatory behaviors. (Tables A1, mean latency to first mount; Table A2, mean head and size mount frequency. Table A3, mean rear mount frequency, and Table A4 mean intromission frequency. Tables A5-A7 list the percentage of animals exhibiting either head and side mounts, rear mounts or intro- missions for each neonatal treatment group.) Effects of Neonatal Treatments (Tests 1-5) Twenty eight days of TP administration in adulthood caused an in- crease in mating behaviors in all groups (Table A l and Figures 11-13, tests 1-5). Analysis of variance of scores from test 5 revealed no differences within any of the neonatal treatment groups for mean rear mount frequency, latency to mount or mean intromission frequency. Comparison of test 5 scores among the 3 neonatal treatment groups revealed no differences in rear mount frequency, however TP 100 and TP 25 animals achieved significantly more intromissions than did neo- natally EB 25 treated animals (f-4.730, df-2,33, p<0.025; S-N-K p<0.05). 54 10} FM CUT CW. 0". CUT a-v-u /‘ FM SHAM 0—0 W“ on SHAM ._. MEAN INTROMISSION FREQ. on YT'UIV D I IST )- 2 § Io . E 'w 5 .W. 2 s \M f 3 2 ) j l l l l J l 1 _H 67891011‘23T4'5T6 TEST _. up up #- “I' Figure 11. Rear mount frequency and intromission frequency in day l castrated male hamsters treated with TPlOO neonatally and given various adult treatments. 55 1 FM cur ............. o to on cur ........... “ on SHAM ._.. 3 .._... g s . ./ \ o . E. z D g o P M. M: I". ‘ Isr ,- U 2 III E 10 I- E .- z 3 o i 5. g I B ' , 2 P a < . . g o ‘/ Km A I I I I I I I I I I I I 78910111213141516 ... ”I a. ub- u.- 0 TEST Figure 12. Rear mount frequency and intromission frequency in day l castrated male hamsters treated with TP25 neonatally and given various adult treatments. 56 i3 --2 1.: III! \ / I... i X 55%;. . /><,/ \ 115 \d. . o I? no .- 4 C / ill, ‘C ‘ \g\. are X4“ 3""; T—Lg f‘fl“? '03!!! NOISSIWOUINI ADNiflOEHi lNflOW 8V3! NVSW Figure 13. Rear mount frequency and intromission frequency in day l castrated male hamsters treated with EB25 neonatally and given various adult treatments. 57 Ejaculations were infrequent in all neonatal treatment groups. Only 7.5% of TP 100 animals, 18% of T? 25 animals and 0% of EB 25 animals achieved ejaculation on test 5. Effects of Penile Desensitization (Tests 6-8) For the 4 groups treated neonatally with TP 100, fluoxymesterone sectioned animals had significantly more mounts on test 8 than the other 3 groups (p<0.05) while those groups did not differ from each other (Figure 11). Analysis of latency measures showed that the TP 100 sectioned groups had shorter latencies to mount (Table A1) than sham animals (RMF f=4.752, df=3,36, p<0.01 and ML F=5.346, df=3,37, p<0.005). Mean intromission frequency on test 8 did not differ statistically among the 4 groups of TP 100 animals. Animals treated neonatally with T? 25 (Figure 12) did not differ among groups on test 8 in rear mount frequency. While fluoxymesterone sham and oil sham groups did not differ in intromissions, fluoxymesterone sham animals did intromit more than cut groups (F=3.509, df=3,35, p<0.05). The 4 groups of animals treated neonatally with EB 25 were not different from each other in either mount latency, rear mount frequency or intromission frequency on test 8. As can be seen in appendix Tables A5-A7, a considerable variation in the number of animals mounting occurred on each test. To compensate for animals which did not mount on test 8, analysis of mount positive animal scores was run: Mount + rear mount frequency = RMF X 1 Z mounting. When only animals which mounted were considered, no differences on 58 test 8 rear mount scores were observed for animals receiving neonatal TP 100, TP 25 or EB 25. Effects of Hormone Manipulation_(Tests 9-16) Following withdrawal of daily TP treatment and substitution of either daily fluoxymesterone or sesame oil, a marked decline in mounting and intromission was observed in all 3 neonatal treatment groups (Table A1, Figure 11-13, Tests 9-16). Analysis of test 9 scores for rear mount frequency or intromission frequency gave results identi- cal to those of test 8 without consistent differences among treatment conditions in each neonatal treatment group. By test 16, mounts and intromissions were at a very low level in all groups in each neonatal treatment. Analysis of variance revealed no differences in rear mount or intromission frequency within neonatal treatment groups. Likewise, analysis among neonatal treatment groups revealed no differences. How- ever, there was a trend that within each neonatal treatment group, fluoxymesterone treated animals showed more mounts and intromissions than oil treated animals but this was not statistically significant. By test 16, no oil treated animal in any group exhibited intromissions. The only oil treated animals to show rear mounting on test 16 were one animal in TP 100 oil cut group and one animal in TP 100 oil sham. Morphology Measures for anogenital (AG) distances and penile bone plus cartilage were analyzed for differences both within, each neonatal treatment group and among neonatal treatments. Results of these comparisons will be considered seperately. 59 Within Neonatal Treatments: NEONATAL TP 100 GROUP. (Table 7) For AG distance, oil cut, oil sham and fluoxymesterone cut animals did not differ statistically. Fluoxymesterone sham animals exhibited significantly longer AG distances than any other group (AG F=8.792, df=3,35 p<0.001). Mean bone and cartilage lengths for oil out and oil sham animals did not differ from each other. Oil cut animals had significantly smaller penile bone and cartilage lengths than did fluoxymesterone treated animals (B & C F53.437, df=3,36, p<0.05). Oil sham, fluoxymesterone cut and sham groups did not differ from each other. NEONATAL TP 25 GROUP. (Table 7) Within the neonatal TP 25 group oil sham.and oil cut animals did not differ in AC distance, however both groups had significantly shorter AG distances than did the fluo- xymesterone cut group (F=l4.531, df=3,30, p<0.001). Fluoxymesterone cut animals had significantly shorter AG distances than did the fluo- xymesterone sham group (p<0.05). Fluoxymesterone sham animals showed greater penile virilization than did any of the other groups (F-6.350, df=3,30, p<0.005) which did not differ from.each other. NEONATAL EB 25 GROUP. (Table 7) For the neonatal EB 25 treatment group, bone and cartilage lengths were uniformly small for the 4 treatment conditions. Analysis of the AG distances revealed that the oil sham group had significantly shorter AG distances than the other 3 groups (Fh5.016, df-3,29, p<0.01) which did not differ from eadh other. - ‘(llllll 60 Table 7. Mean anogenital distance, penile bone plus cartilage lengths (in mm) and penile condition in male hamsters treated neo- natally with TPlOO, TP25 or EB 25. AG distance Bone + Cartilage Penile spines and $ s.e. length $ s.e. papillae at sacrifice ‘TP100 FM Cut 15.9 0.6 5.5 0.1 + Oil Cut 15.0 0.3 5.2 0.0 0 FM Sham 18.2 0.6 5.6 0.0 + 011 Sham 15.7 0.4 5.4 0.0 0 TP25 FM Cut 18.0 0.6 5.4 0.0 + 011 Cut 16.3 0.7 ’5.3 0.1 0 FM Sham 19.5 0.5 5.8 0.0 0 Oil Sham 5.4 0.3 5.5 0.0 O (EB25 FM Cut 15.5 0.7 3.5 0.2 + Oil Cut 14.7 0.3 3.3 0.1 0 FM Sham. 14.8 0.5 3.7 0.0 + Oil Sham 12.8 0.2 3.4 0.0 O 1 I__‘ .IIIIII I II III lull l Il'lll I." I I‘ll: 61 Among Neonatal Treatments: Comparing neonatal TP 100, TP 25 and EB 25 animals revealed that EB 25 animals, whether from fluoxymesterone or sesame 011 groups, had significantly shorter AG distances than either of the TP groups (fluoxymesterone Fe16.760, df=2,50, p<0.001) (sesame oil F=9.902, dfs2,50, p<0.001). TP 100 and TP 25 animals did not differ from each other. Mean length of penile bone and cartilage across neonatal treat- ment groups also revealed highly significant differences whether the final hormone condition had been fluoxymesterone (F5199.966, df-2,51, p<0.001) or sesame oil (F=230.337, df=2,48, p<0.001). Neonatal EB 25 treated animals showed significantly less penile virilization than either neonatal TP treated group regardless of final hormone condition .(p<0.05). TPlOO and TP 25 induced the same amount of penile virili- zation. Penile Spines and Papillae: Because of the procedures used for removal of penile bone and cartilage, glans penis tissue was too damaged for quantitative measure- ment of penile spines and papillae. However, histological examination of portions of the glans revealed that all fluoxymesterone treated animals possessed penile spines and papillae while sesame oil treated groups had none (see Table 7). Microscopic examination did reveal subtle differences among the neonatal treatment groups after adult fluoxymesterone administration. Neonatal TP groups, whether TP 100 or TP 25, exhibited many long spines and deep, regular papillae. Neonatal EB 25 animals had spines and papillae which were more irregular and 62 misshapen than those of neonatal TP animals. The general impression is that fluoxymesterone treatment of neonatal EB animals induced fewer and smaller spines. Discussion The present study demonstrates that penile desensitization yields no clear evidence of an effect of genital sensations on male sexual behavior in hamsters. Sectioning the DNP caused no consistent loss of intromission as has been reported for rats (Experiment 1; Larsson and Sgdersten, 1973), cats (Aronson and Cooper, 1968) and monkeys (Herbert, 1973). Thus for the hamster although the 3 neonatal hormone treatments used in the present study induced different degrees of penile viriliza- tion, few behavioral differences were observed. This was contrary to the prediction based upon data on penile feedback effects in rats. TP administration in adulthood induced similar mounting levels in neo- natally TP or EB treated animals but intromissions were fewer in the neo- natal EB group. This dissociation of behavioral and genital virilization has been previously reported (Coniglio, 1973; Paup et a1., 1974). The general decline in mating behaviors for all groups when TP treatment was withdrawn and sesame oil or fluoxymesterone was sub- stituted is consistent with results reported by other workers. (Christensen, Coniglio, Paup & Clemens, 1973). Although fluoxymesterone has not been used previously in hamsters the decline in sexual response observed was similar to that seen in male rats (Johnson and Tiefer, 1975; Experiment 1). The trend for animals treated with peripheral androgens to continue mounting longer than non-treated castrated animals has been previously reported in rats (Parrott, 1974b). 63 However, fluoxymesterone does not have dramatic behavioral effects on castrated male hamsters suggesting that penile integrity is not as important for maintenance of sexual behavior in hamsters as it is in rats. The general decline following the hormone change was not cor- related with differences in penile size. Fluoxymesterone did maintain penile integrity in the form of penile spines and papillae. Since the role of penile sensations in the control of male mating behavior seems to be different for rats and hamsters, a consideration of possible reasons for this apparent difference is in order. That section of the DNP in hamsters was less effective than DNP section of rats in blocking intromissions may reflect anatomical species dif- ferences. The DNP of rats branches extensively as it approaches the distal end of the penis (Calaresu, 1970). The position of the nerve cut along the penile shaft was made as close to the proximal end of the penis as possible to avoid this branching. While the branching pat- tern of the hamster's DNP has not been studied, it is possible that the hamster has more extensive branching at the proximal end of the penile shaft. This would result in incomplete desensitization and would account for the persistence of intromission in nerve cut groups. Likewise, if both cut and sham groups continued to receive penile sensations through undamaged portions of the nerves, the similar behavioral decline which was seen would not be surprising. Experi- ment 1 with rats and work by Herbert (1973) in monkeys have shown that partial desensitization does not eliminate intromission. Further anatomical work would help us understand this problem. 64 Differences in behavioral decline that were seen with the rats in Experiment 1 and not in the present experiment may have been related to the level of masculinization in these hamsters. Male mating be- havior in rodents might be viewed as a continuum with sexual arousal at one end and ejaculation at the other. An animal must be sexually aroused in order to mount a receptive female. Intromission cannot occur without a mount and intromission must preceed ejaculation. In the present study the experimental model used was at the lower end of the continuum. The specific neonatal treatments used in this experiment resulted in males which were sexually aroused, mounted, intromitted occasionally and seldom ejaculated. Experiment 1 employed experimental animals which encompassed the whole range of the continuum. If the hamsters in the present study had also shown the whole range of behav- iors, more consistent differences among groups may have resulted. Another possible explanation for the discrepancy seen between rats and hamsters is the difference in mating pattern. In the rat, a basic unit of sexual behavior is the mount bout. The mount bout is defined as a sequence of mounts with or without intromission, uninterrupted by any behavior other than genital autogrooming. A series of mount bouts leads to ejaculation. Reducing genital sensations with local anesthetic in rats increases mount frequency, mount bout length and the number of mounts per mount bout (Sachs and Barfield, 1970). In Experiment 1, continued copulation in the fluoxymesterone treated animals may have been related to this phenomenon instead of genital feedback. The male hamster shows a somewhat different pattern with mount bouts interrupted only by ejaculation. Our inability to find differences between cut and 65 sham animals may be related to this pattern since these hamsters were not programmed for ejaculation. The discrepancy seen between the results of Experiments 1 and 3 with rats and hamsters emphasizes the need for comparative work. Pre- dictions based upon findings with the rat did not hold for the hamster. Likewise behavior seen in these hamsters following nerve section ap- peared to be different from work with cats and monekys. Further com- parisons between species will add more to our knowledge of the control male sexual behavior. Conclusion In the rat, penile sensations are involved in the expression of intromission and ejaculation. However, previous experiments have shown little effect of these sensations on mounting behavior. In Experiment 1 we were able to demonstrate for the first time that sensory feedback from the penis is involved to a degree in the maintenance of mounting. Other investigations have tried to show that penile virilization at some critical period in development is necessary for later male behavior. Contrary to this position, Experiment 2 showed that fluoxy- mesterone treated hamsters which had the capacity for penile growth at adulthood exhibited no capacity for male behavior. These results con- firmed and extended those of others in establishing that penile virili- zation during development is not sufficient to bring about adult sexual behavior. Penile sensations in adult hamsters seem to be less important for male mating behavior than in rats. In Experiment 3 reduction of penile 66 sensory feedback in male hamsters caused little interruption of intro- mission behavior. The decline in behaviors seen with fluoxymesterone was not related to genital size or sensitivity. Clearly, penile sensations via the dorsal nerve of the penis are not among the most important sensory mediators of male mating behavior. Whether sensations from deep pressure receptors, or other perigenital nerve endings are involved to a greater degree is not known. Further work along these lines may lead to other conclusions. The old central vs. peripheral controversy is little more than academic. That central neural stimulation and maintenance of behavior has far more import than peripheral sensation has been shown many times. The present series of experiments which was designed to explore the genital feedback effects on behavior lends little support to the peripheral side of the central- peripheral debate. APPENDIX 67 HN NNH HN NNH HN NN NHH NNH NN NN HN HN NN NNH. «N o NN« NH« N«« .N«N, NNN NNNII NNN NNH NNH N«H N«H NHH NNH NNN NNN NNN swam HHo NNH NNH NN HN NN NN HN NN NN NN NN oN NN HN HN o NNN NNN NNN NNH, NNN NHH. NNH. No NNH NNH N«H NNNIINNH NNH NNN NNN swam 2N NN NN NN NN NN NN om NN NNH oN NN NN NN NN NN NN NNN H«N NN« NN« «««. NNN, NNN NNN NHN oNN NNN .NNN NHN NNN «N« NNN use HHo NN NN NN NN NN NN NN HN NN «N NN NN NN NN NN o NN« NNN NN« No« «NN NHN HH« NNN N«N NNN HNH NNH NNN NNH NNN NNN one am N« HN NN NN «N NN NN NN «N NN NN NN NN NN «N on NNN N«« HN« NNN N«N. NN «N Na NNH «NH NN N«H N«H «NH NNN H«N swam HHo oN NN NN NN NN NN NN NN NN NH NH «N NN NN NN NN NN« HNN NNN HHN «NN NHN NNH, N«H NNH «N NN NNH HNH «NH NNN N«N aNNN ea c o o o o NHH No «N HHH NNH NHH HNH NN NNN NN a CNN NNN NNN NNN NONI. NH« .NN« NNH NNN NNN HNN «NN NNN NNN NN« NNN use HHo HN NN NN NN NN NN HN NN NN NN NN NN NN N NNH «N NNN NNN NNN NHN‘ NNN NNN N«H NNH NNH NNH NNH NNH «NH NN HNN NHN use 2N NN «N o HN NN NN NN NN NN NN NN NN «N NN ea c NN« NNN NNN «N« omet HN« NNN NN« NNN NNN «N« NNN NN« NNN NNN NNN swam HHo NN NN «N NN «N NN NN NN NN NN NN Hm NN NN NN NN N«N, NN« «N« H«NI, NN« NN« NN« NNN N«« «NN NNN NNN ««N NNN NNN NN« aNNN za NN NN «N NN NN HN NN NN NN NN NN HN NN NN NN « NNN NN« NNN. No« «HN HNN «NN NNN NNN NNH NNH NNN N«H NNH NNH NNN .uao HHo NN NN NN HN NN NN NN NN NN NN HN NN NH NH «N «N HNN NNN NNN NNN NNN NHH oN NNH NN NNH NNH N«H «N NN NNN NHN use an NH NH «H NH NH HH oH N N N N N « N N H guano NNNN .mudguwwuu HHH—flu 30.?“ri fl0>fiw van mm HO 9H Sag hHHflufldOOd vmuwwuu anon—mam..— OHQE vmufluummu Rom Amvfioowm adv Ugo:— umHHH OH hugumfi so: .H< Quads EB 25 TP25 TP 100 Mean head and side mount frequency in castrated male hamsters treated neonatally with TP or EB and given various adult treatments. Table A2. 68 so moorxoonnmoomo‘ PTMHHOOHHONOQ H .oooooro o at. o o o o so to o coo filcawIwIc>c>~Ic>rICPc>c>c>c>c>c>oIoIc>c>¢>0IFIFI I I ‘ . .n cuconncouncncucn~o~o INInNIc>c>c>c>c>eINIc>c>c>c>NIc>c>c>c>c>vsoaktc: -o curqcaxor4\o c>ux OJGDGDQ’Flh-Q'OIW)C>FTF- '7 oININIc>FIc>c>c>dINIc>c>91c>c>c>r4c:c>c>o~<-F1c> «I c>o~mIOIotoI<-oaoso- ~o¢n~suncnoIFIc>c>FIrIoINIc>c>c>c>rIc>cqr4c3c3uncu~o-o o; FTC>V1F~GDU\O\O\P1C> o~n~oam><~wsdsoaosm>qu> '1 01F!OIFIC>C>OJF401F1C>C>FIC>V1F4F16301F1h-F1¢)V\ .4 [\Cfi-¢~¢€>FTF1CDOJF4C>OIC>F~F4V16JV16121V1h-d’ c: «sto~c>L>FI<'c>u\oIanIc>NIoIr~FIFIc>r4~¢cucnvd~¢unoqur~t5<:cocoINcotzOIFI<-NIoINIoIc>u>FI oaoszg3u>¢>oI h. cocusoc>F4o3c>a>o>oIosoI In.4\o::owaaos ‘0 c>usuac>0Iawo~o~¢>o~a>c>a>NIUIrIwIPIwINIusoIFIc>::oonoI u‘ «JCDOJOJF~F~F1«Db-61¢)O\h-€)<'F-C)F)¢>C)¢>C>0‘“) manIOIFIFIc>oIc>::«Iv\FImIPIh-FIh~oINIF153oso\oI ~o coco¢D~0cnso~o¢3~ocnoI~IoI 0000000000000... 0000.. O o \ocococucnqudIqxocwcnraxo.4Invatxcncn.4:3¢>U)O\FlV1C>dDU161F1UIVIO\hIOJOIO\O\<’C)§’V1 .0 COCO. .00 00 00 O 000.. 000 O €)61O‘BJOIFIOIF122F163FI«Dhlh-FIO\F)F1C>UNOIO\U) cw \D¢Dwtoac>olFI¢>oI¢IoIo5FIoINIexuded—Ih~o1w5<- <- F. o10~fl5c¢r~~¢ coin FIFI cues 0.000. I. O. .0 7. Hooooooooooooooooooooooo a u a 5 u g m u E 5 u a o .5 u 5 .o u a o .5 o. o (J .o co 5 (J .o to a (J .c to g ‘3 F4 on NI c) PI 0: PI L) F4 o: rt u -H ~H -H -H :5 EoEoEoEanES TP 100 TP 25 EB 25 Mean rear mount frequency in castrated male hamsters treated neonatally with TP or EB and given various adult treatments. Table A3. 69 TEST ll 12 13 14 15 16 10 Group 4.4 1.7 2.0 1.4 3.8 3.1 1.6 4.6 6.2 10.7 7.6 3.4 7.6 13.6 2.4 2.2 2.8 0.6 3.1 7.1 4.6 4.8 0.4 1.8 3.6 1.4 1.4 FM Cut 2.3 1.8 U. 2.3 3.1 2.0 UoJ A 0.4 U78: 4.0 0.6 1.6 6.4 6.] 5.2' 6}U 6.2 2.47 410T’2.0 3T2 0 Oil Cut 0.3 0.3 0.5 1.9 3.1 1.9 2.4 2.2 1.5 0.8 1.3 0.7 1.2 2.6 2:4’ 1.0 1.7 2.0 1.2 1.0 ”1.8 1.2 NH 0.7» 0.2 0.5 0.2 1.27’TI.4 0.8 .2 1.7 4.0 2.3 3.5 1.5 1.1 1.2 0.6 3.0 518 1.7 2.9 7.6 5.9 3.4 12.9 0.7 0.6 '1.2 0.9 0 0 FM Sham Oil Sham 0 U 1.3 2.9 4213 0.6 1.5 1.6 0.4 1.1 1.9 3.7 1.4 0.4 0.7 1.3 1.7 0.1 2.8 2.1 3.5 4.8 0.1 4.4 1.6 1.5 1.7 O 1.4 2.5 1.1 1.5 1.2 0.9 0.9 4.8 6.5 5.5 9.4 3.2 2.0 3.1 3.5 5.7 4.2 PM Cut 1.5 2.5 1.2 0.7 6.0, 0.3 2.5 4.8 2.1 3.3 0.7 1.7 3.0 3.4 0.5 0.5 0.8 1.1 0 Oil Sham 091 1.1 2.8 4.6 5.2 O 0.2 0 Oil Cut 2.4 0.3 8.4 11.5 2.2 2.6 11.7 14.1 2.9 3.1 3.3 0.1 1.3 0.1 0.1 0.1 0.1 2.2 2.1 4.0 0.7 1.3 5.8 2.2 9.8 7.8 9.1 0 0 FM Sham 1.2 1.1 0.6 1.6 1.8 1.5 2.2 0.3 0.2 2.6 1.4 5.8 1.6 5.8 9.7 8.7 3.4 3.8 1.1 2.7 2.7 0.1 0.6 1.0 1.2 1.7 1.9 2.2 3.1 2.1 1.2 3.1 1.7 1.4 0.8 2.7 7.1 5.9 4.2 4.2 3.1 3.6 4.4 1.6 1.8 2.1 1.2 5.6 4.7 5.1 O 0 FM Cut 101 1.8 0.8 1.6 2.6 2.1 0.2 2.0 0.9 3.6 1.9 3.4 4.2 1.0 1.7 0.7 5.0 3.1 2.7 2.9 4.4 1.4 1.3 2.2 011 Cut 2.1 0.2 1.5 0.6 1.4 1.0 2.5 1.6 1.4 0.9 3.8 1.5 1.1 1.5 2.5 0.8 0.6 5.1 4.0 5.3 5.1 0 1.9 4.3 5.9 6.6 0 Oil Sham 0 FM Sham 1.9 1.9 1.4 7.0 12.0 3.2 8.0 1.6 1.3 1.8 1.1 2.7 2.3 2.5 5.4 3.4 0.6 2.8 3.2 7.0 5.4 8.4 6.9 8.4 1.0 3.8 5.7 5.7 0.4 0.8 3.0 2.4 5.0 0.8 1.7 2.7 1.7 0 Mean intromission frequency in castrated male hamsters treated Table A4. neonatally with T? or EB and given various adult treatments. 70 :3 «nus axes «am: c>c>t>c>c>c>c>c>c>c>c>c>c>c>c>c>c>c>c>c>crc>c>c> In «sex «am: Ho. 00 c>c>c>c>c>c>c>c>c>c>c>c>c>c>c>c>c>c>c>c>c>c>c>c> :3 ¢D\O I\I\ a>c> «saw ~¢~¢ 00 o. 0. o. .0 C>C>C>C>C>C>C>C>F4F4CDCDCDCDCDCDC>C>C>C>C>C>C>C> :3 ¢n~o fawn pxo~ , Fara c>c>c>c>c>c>:>c>:>c>:>c>F>c>c>c>c>c>c>c>c>c>c>c> :3 a~a> rsc> o\oa Psos c>c>c>c>c>c>c>c>rasqc>c>raoac>c>c>c>F>c>c>c>c>c> :1 filedrd pnun yac> r~<3+ocn ro~sxnC>C>C>C>C>F1F1C>C>U501C>C>C>C>C>C>C>C>C>C> E: a>c> uwuwcvcvmn-c $101046? Papchoa F¢Fac>c>c>cacaoJc>c>c>c>a~o:<-nac>c>c>c>c>c>c>c> 0‘ Fiasn:uwa>aac>c>c>c>asoJu>oac>c>k>c>c>c>rdp4 a: «Dhlh-F1U1USCQF4\OIn «sw4c>u>awawauoaoqup4h- 0000...... 0.0.0.000... ~¢¢v~o~¢c3c>awawc>c>c>c>53rwm>oac>c>c>c>c>c>wic> '\ r4\Oa~oiwdrqawcqu965c>c>rwoac>c>c>c>::oauso:c>c>c>c>c>c>rapq \o oqwqh-C>Fapqvao: d)r40\d> ~c~¢ c>use»c>c>c>c>c>c>c>c>o~oa<-Fic>c>c>c>c>c>c>c> u‘ unq-msc>c~oxr4c>cwa3uwCDcnrqcntn~¢cuco~o~o~¢cncu \DCV(VCV<3C>C>C>C>C>C>C> ~c IdCD~¢\O~¢~3c>oaolvapac>c><-o:«sw+c>c>wcw4~1c>c>c> a, c>oaa>c> «sevens: €>¢JO\C><'Flh-c>nioir4c>c>c>wic>wipcc>c>c>c>c>c>c>c> 0‘ Ftaaospd F«F4 vwasoan+ \OIn\O\D . .000 00 0... 0... F4c>c>c>c>c>c>c>c>c>c>c>c>c>c>c>c>c>c>c>c>c>c>c> Fir! 0. F4 c>c>c>c>c>c>c>c>c>c>c>c>c>c>c>c>c>c>c>c>c>c>c>c: a E u a m u a S u a a u a m .n u a .c u : E3 g. a (J .2 co 9 (J .c to a c: E5 0 7’ vi an F4 L, vi a) rd c, F! .4 H -H -H -H -H -H sEoEoEoEoEo-Es TP 100 TP 25 EB 25 71 O OH ON OH ON Om O H.O n.ne N.- s.eo n.OO OH O OH OH OH OH Om O H.O m.m~ H.HH m.mm e.e¢ nH O O O OH ON on O «.mH «.mm ~.- e.¢¢ e.ce «H O OH ON O OH OH O «.mH n.m¢ m.mm O.mn s.eo OH O ON on O on On O m.n~ O.mO ~.N~ O.nm c.e< NH O O< Om OH ON Om H.O m.em n.9m H.HH 0.0w O.mm HH ON ow Om OH on on O m.en m. om>Hmumu can .hHkumaoma OOHMH am>Hw .H kmv do vmumuummu mama «HwaHa< .mumou Hau0H>mnoo now :onmHaouucH no unaoa.umou .uanoavmva com com: wcHsonm muoumams uHma mo ammunmunmm .m< mHomH. 72 O O OH OH ON ON O O O O m.NH ON OH O OH on O OH ON O O O O ON n.Nm OH O O on OH ON ON O O O Om on On OH O ON OO OH ON Om O O O O.NH ON ON OH O OO OO Om OO OO O O O mN m.NO Om NH Om Om OOH OO OO OO O m.mm m.mm ON m.NO ON HH on em Om OO OO OO O N.OH Om ON Om m.NO OH OO Om Om ON Om Om O N.OO OOH m.NH m.NO m.NO m Om ON Om ON ON .OO O m.mm N.OO ON OOH ON O ON Om Om Om OOH OOH O on N.OO O O.NO m.Nw N OO OOH Om Om Om OOH O on N.OO O n.NO OOH O O.NN O.NN m.OO ON Om Om m.mm m.mm m.mm H.Nm N.OO m.mw O OO ON Om OO Om OOH N.OH Om N.OO Om mN O.NO q H.Nm H.Nm OOH om OO Om O O Om m.Nm mN OOH O OH On On OH ON ON .O N.OH m.mm m.NH Om Om N O OH O O O OH O O O O m.NH mN H H 2m Swan H 2% ZOOM H 2m Swan H xx imam umoa OH»: aosm HHO OHIc swam 2h Ona uao HHO Oua uso ah .muawaumouu uHavm m50Hua> Ow>Hmumu can NHHmuaaom: mth amwa .H Now do vuumuummu who: mHmaH:< .u0H>m:wn waHuma mHma mo mumumamuwm wafisonm mumumao: mHma mo ownusouumm .O< «Hams 73 O O 0.0N O 0.00 OO O O H.HH O m.OO m.OO OH O 0.0N 0.0N O O.NO O.NO O O H.HH O N.NN N.NN OH O 0.0N m.Ne O.NH O.NO O.NO O O m.OO O 0.00 N.NN OH O O.NO ¢.HN O.NH O.NO ON O O m.OO O N.NN 0.00 OH m.OH. m.NO N.OO O O.NO ON O m.OO m.OO O m.OO 0.00 NH m.OH H.NO N.OO O.NO ON O.NO O m.OO O.¢O O N.NN N.OO HH m.OO N.OO N.OO O.NH OO ON O m.OO N.OO O N.NN O.NN OH 0.0N H.NO O.HN O.NH O.NO O.NO H.HH N.OO N.OO H.HH 0.00 O.NN m 0.0N H.NO N.OO N.OH m.OO m.OO N.NN 0.00 0.00 H.HH 0.00 N.OO O 0.0N N.OO N.OO ON O.NO OOH N.NN N.NN 0.00 H.HH N.OO m.OO N O.NO O.HN N.OO O ON O.NO H.HH 0.00 N.OO O O.NN 0.00 O 0.0N N.OO N.OO O.NO ON O.NO N.NN 0.00 0.00 m.OO O.NN O.NN O m.OH ¢.HN N.OO OO ON O.NO e.ee O.e< 0.00 H.HH O.ee O.NN O m.OO O.HN N.OO m.NO H.NO O.NO N.NN N.NN 0.00 N.NN O.NN O.NN n O 0.0N H.NO O.NH O.NO ON H.HH m.OO 0.00 O N.OH 0.00 N O O O O O O O H.HH H.HH O O O H H an «6.3 H a zmwm H. 2% Swan H. 5 Swan “Emu. Nla swam HHO Ola swam 2m man usu HHO mIa use an .muamaumouu uHflOQ n=OHHu> O0>Huuou Odd AHHnuwuoua ONOm aw>HO .H Nae so voumuunuu uhmbvamlan .uou>u:mO.OnHuna uHma.mo muuuuauuma mafiaonm muoumads «Hal mo amuuawuuom .dN «Hana BIBLIOGRAPHY BIBLIOGRAPHY Adler, N. & G. Bermant, 1966. Sexual behavior of male rats: Effects of reduced sensory feedback. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 61: 240-243. Aronson, L.R. & M.L. Cooper, 1966. Seasonal variation in mating be- havior in cats after desensitization of glans penis. Science 152:226-230. Aronson, L.R. & M.L. Cooper, 1968. Desensitization of the glans penis and sexual behavior in cats. in M. Diamond (ed.), Perspectives in Reproduction and Sexual Behavior, Indiana University Press, Bloomington, Indiana, pp. 51-82. Aronson, L.R. & M.L. Cooper, 1969. Mating behavior in sexually in- experienced cats after desensitization of the glans penis. Animal Behavior 17:208-212. Baum,‘M.J. & J.T.M. Vreeburg, 1973. Copulation in castrated male rats following combined treatment with estradiol and dihydrotestos- terone. Science 182:283-285. Beach, F.A., 1942. Analysis of the stimuli adequate to elicit mating behavior in the sexually inexperienced male rat. J. Comp. Psychol. 33:163-207. Beach, F.A., 1956. Characteristics of masculine "sex drive." In M. Jones (ed.) Nebraska Symposium on Motivation University of Nebraska Press, Lincoln, Nebraska. Beach, F.A. & A.M. Holz, 1946. Mating behavior in male rats castrated at various ages and injected with androgen. J. Exptl. Zool. 101:91-142. Beach, F.A. & G. Levinson, 1950. Effects of androgen on the glans penis and mating behavior of castrated male rats. J. Exptl. 2001. 114:159-168. Beach, F.A., Noble, R.G. and R.K. Orndoff, 1969. Effects of perinatal androgen treatment on responses of male rats to gonadal hormones in adulthood. J. Comp. Physiol. Pyschol. 68:490-497. Beach, F.A. & W.H. Westbrook, 1968. Dissociation of androgenic effects on sexual morphology and behavior in male rats. Endocrinology 83:395-398. 74 75 Bruner, J.A. and E. Witschi, 1946. Testosterone-induced modifications of sex development in Semole hamsters. Am. J. Anat. 79:293-320. Caggiula, A.R., 1970. Analysis of the copulation-reward properties of posterior hypothalamic stimulation in male rats. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 70:399-412. Caggiula, A.R. & B.G. Hoebel, 1966 "Copulation-reward site" in the posterior hypothalamus. Science 153:1284-1285. Caggiula, A.R. & H. Szechtman, 1972 Hypothalamic stimulation: A bi- phasic influence on copulation of the male rat. Behav. Biol. 7:591-598. Calaresu, F.R., 1970. Experimental studies on the dorsal nerve of the penis of the rat. Am. J. Anat. 127:415-422. Carlsson, S.G. & K. Larsson, 1964. Mating in male rats after local anesthetization of the glans penis. Z. Tierpsychol. 21:854-856. Carter, C.S., Clemens, L.G. & D.J. Hoekema, 1972 Neonatal androgen and adult sexual behavior in the golden hamster. Physiol Behav. 9:89-95. Christensen, L.W. & L.G. Clemens, 1974 Intrahypothalamic implants of testosterone or estradiol and resumption of masculine sexual behavior in long-term castrated male rats. Endocrinology 95:984-990. Christensen, L.W., Coniglio, L.P., Paup, D.C., and L.G. Clemens, 1973 Sexual behavior of male golden hamsters receiving diverse androgen treatments. Horm.Behav. 4:223-229. Clemens, L.G., 1973 Development and Behavior, in D.A. Dewsbury & D.A. Rethlingshafer (ed.), Comparative Psychology: A Modern Survey, McGraw-Hill Book Company, New York, pp. 238-270. Coniglio, L.P., 1973 Hormonal determinants in the development of sexual behavior in the golden hamster (Mesocricetus auratus). Thesis: Michigan State University. Coniglio, L.P., Paup, D.C. & L.G. Clemens, 1973 Hormonal specificity in the suppression of sexual receptivity of the female golden hamster. J. Endocrinol. 57:55-61. Cooper, K.K. & L.R. Aronson, 1974 Effects of castration on neural afferent responses from the penis of the domestic cat. Physiol. Behav. 12:93-107. Davidson, J.M., 1966 Activation of the male rat's sexual behavior by intracerebral implantation of androgen. Endocrinolggy 79: 783-794. 76 Eaton, G., 1970 Effect of a single prepuberal injection of testos- terone propionate on adult bisexual behavior of male hamsters castrated at birth. Endocrinology 87:934-940. Feder, H.H., 1971 The comparative actions of testosterone propionate and 50 - androstan-l7B-ol-3-one propionate on the reproductive behavior, physiology and morphology of male rats. J. Endocrinol. 51:241-252. Feder, H.H., Naftolin, F., & K.J. Ryan, 1974 Male and female sexual responses in male rats given estradiol benzoate and Sa-androstan- 17B-Ol-3-one propiOnate. Endocrinology 94:136-141. Giantonio, G.W., Lund, N.L. & A.A. Gerall, 1970 Effect of dienceph— alic and rhinencephalic lesions on the male rats' sexual behavior. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 73:38-46. Goldman, B.D., Quadagno, D.M., Shryne, J. & R.A. Gorski, 1972. Modification of phallus development and sexual behavior in rats treated with gonodotropin antiserum neonatally. Endocrinology 90:1025-1031. Grady, K.L., Phoenix, C.H., & W.C. Young, 1965 Role of the developing rat testiszhxdifferentiation of the neural tissues mediating mating behavior. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 59:176-182. Hart, B.L., 1967 Testosterone regulation of sexual reflexes in spinal male rats. Science 155:1283-1284. Hart, B.L., 1968 Sexual reflexes and mating behavior in the male rat. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 65:453-460. Hart, B.L., 1972 Manipulation of neonatal androgen: Effects on sexual responses and penile development in male rats. Physiol. Behav. 8:841-845. Hart, B.L., 1973 Effects of testosterone propionate and dihydro- testosterone on penile morphology and sexual reflexes of spinal male rats. HormijBehav. 4:239-246. Herbert, J., 1973 The role of the dorsal nerves of the penis in the sexual behavior of the male rhesus monkey. Physiol. Behav. 10:293-300. Hitt, J.C., Bryon, D.M. & D.T. Mbdianos, 1973 Effects of rostral medial forebrain bundle and olfactory tubercle lesions upon sexual behavior of male rats. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 82:30-36. 77 Hitt, J.C., Hendricks, S.E., Ginsberg, S.I. & J.H. Lewis, 1970 Disruption of male, but not female, sexual behavior in rats by medial forebrain bundle lesions.’ J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 73:377-384. Johnson, W.A. & L. Tiefer, 1974 Mating in castrated male rats during combined treatment with estradiol benzoate/and fluoxymesterone. Endocrinology,96:912-915. Johnston, P. & J.M. Davidson, 1972 Intracerebral androgens and sexual behavior in the male rat. Horm. Behav. 3:345-357. Kierniesky, N.C. & A.A. Gerall, 1973 Effects of testosterone pro- pionate implants in the brain on the sexual behavior and peripheral tissue of the male rat. Physiol. Behav. 11:633-640. Kohn, L.A., 1971 Answers to the bard. Perspect. Biol. Med. 14:338. Larsson, K., 1966 Effects of neonatal castration upon the development of the mating behavior of the male rat. 2. Tierpsychol. 23: 867-873. Larsson, K. & P. Sgdersten, 1973 Mating in male rats after section of the dorsal penile nerve. Physiol. Behav. 10:567-571. Larsson, K., Sgdersten, P. 5 C. Beyer, 1973a Induction of male sexual behavior by oestradiol benzoate in combination with dihydrotestosterone. J. Endocrinol. 57:563-564. Larsson, K., Sgdersten, P., & C. Beyer, 1973b Sexual behavior in male rats treated with estradiol in combination with dihydro- testosterone. Horm. Behav. 4:289. Lisk, R.D., 1967 Neural localization for androgen activation of copulatory behavior in the male rat. Endocrinology 80:754-761. Madlafousek, J., Freund, K. & I. Grofova, 1970 Variables determining the effect of electrostimulation in the lateral preoptic area on the sexual behavior of male rats. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 72:28-44. McDonald, P., Beyer, C., Newton, F., Brien, G., Baker, R., Tan, H.S., Sampson, C., Kitching, P., Greenhill, R. & D. Prichard, 1970 Failure of 5a-dihydrotestosterone to initiate sexual behaviour in the castrated male rat. Nature, Lond. 227:964-965. Nadler, R.D., 1969 Differentiation of the capacity for male sexual behavior in the rat. Horm. Behav. 1:53-63. Naftolin, F., Ryan, K.J. & 2. Petra, 1971a Aromatization of androstenedione by limbic system tissue from human fetuses. J. Endocrinol. 51:795-796. 78 Naftolin, F., Ryan, K.J. 8 Z. Petro, 1971b Aromatization of androstenedione by the diencephalon. J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab. 33:368-370. Nucci, L.P. 8 F.A. Beach, 1971 Effects of prenatal androgen treat- ment on mating behavior in female hamsters. Endocrinology_ 88:1514-1515. Parrott, R.F., 1974a Effects of l7B-hydroxy-4-androsten-19—o1-3- one (l9-hydroxytestosterone) and Sa-androston-l78—01-3-one (dihydrotestosterone) on aspects of sexual behavior in castrated male rats. J. Endocrinol. 61:105-115. Parrott, R.F., 1974b Studies on the maintenance of sexual behaviour in castrated rats treated with various androgens. J. Endocrinol. 61:xix. Parrott, R.F., 1974c The effects of various androgens on peripheral structures in the castrated male rat. J. Reprod. Fert. 38:49-57. Parrott, R.F., 1975 Aromatizable and Sa-reduced androgens: differentiation between central and peripheral effects on male rat sexual behavior. Horm. Behav. 6:99-108. Paup, D.C., Coniglio, L.P. 8 L.G. Clemens, 1972 Masculinization of the female golden hamster by neonatal treatment with androgen or estrogen. Horm. Behav. 3:123-131. Paup, D.C., Coniglio, L.P. 8 L.G. Clemens, 1974 Hormonal determinants in the development of masculine and feminine behavior in the female hamster. Behav. Biol. 10:353-363. Reilly, W.A. 8 G.S. Gordon, 1961 Dissociation of growth-stimulating and skeleton-maturing actions of the synthetic androgen- fluoxymesterone. J. Pediat. 59:188. Sachs, B.D. 8 R.J. Barfield, 1970 Temporal patterning of sexual behavior in the male rat. J. Comp, Physiol. Psychol. 73:359-364. Sachs, B.D. 8 R.J. Barfield, 1974 Copulatory behavior of male rats given intermittent electric shocks: Theoretical implications. J. Comp. Phygiol. Psyghol. 86:607-615. Singer, J.J., 1968 Hypothalamic control of male and female sexual behavior in female rats. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 66:738-742. Sgdersten, P., 1973 Estrogen-activated sexual behavior in male rats. Horm. Behav. 4:247-256. Sokol, R.R. 8 F.J. Rohlf, 1969 Biometry: The principles and practice of statistics in biological research. W.H. Freeman 8 Co., San.Francisco, California. 79 VOmachka, A.J., Paup, D.C., Coniglio, L.P., McManus, MQJ. 8 L.G. Clemens, 1974 Progestin-induced pseudo-hermophroditism.in the female hamster. J. Reprod. Fert. 37:269-276. Whalen, R.E., 1968 Differentiation of the neural mechanisms which control gonadotropin secretion and sexual behavior. in M. Diamond (ed.), Perspectives in Reproduction and Sexual Behavior Indiana University Press, Bloomington, Ind. Whalen, R.E. 8 W.G. Luttge, 1970 Long-term retention of tritiated estradiol in brain and peripheral tissues of male and female rats. Neuroendocrinology 6:255-263. Whalen, R.E. 8 W.G. Luttge, 1971 Testosterone, androstenedione and dihydrotestosterone: Effects on mating behaviour of male rats. Horm. Behav. 2:117-125. 77 mEMZ 293 03 '4' I" H l l T” H l u H H “ 31