THESIS WM“- ' ‘ This is to certify that the thesis entitled A DEVELOPMENTAL ANALYSIS OF HAND AND VISUAL HEMIFIELD DIFFERENCES FOR LETTER IDENTIFICATION: EFFECTS OF MODALITY OF PRESENTATION AND LETTER TYPEFACE CHARACTERISTICS presented by Nancy M. Wagner has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY degree in Psychology Lajor professor Date 6 July 198]. 0-7639 OVERDUE FINES: 25¢ per day per item RETURNING LIBRARY MATERIALS: Place in book return to remove charge from circulat‘lon records A DEVELOPMENTAL ANALYSIS OF HAND AND VISUAL HEMIFIELD DIFFERENCES FOR LETTER IDENTIFICATION: EFFECTS OF MODALITY OF PRESENTATION AND LETTER TYPEFACE CHARACTERISTICS By Nancy M. Wagner A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University . in partial fquiTTment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Psychoiogy 1981 \eofi : U ABSTRACT A DEVELOPMENTAL ANALYSIS OF HAND AND VISUAL HEMIFIELD DIFFERENCES FOR LETTER IDENTIFICATION: EFFECTS OF MODALITY 0F PRESENTATION AND LETTER TYPEFACE CHARACTERISTICS By Nancy M. Nagner This study investigated how relative participation of the two cerebral hemispheres in letter recognition varied with typeface com- plexity, presentation modality (visual or tactual), and age. Bryden and Allard (l976) suggested that complex typefaces require preliminary analysis of spatial configuration by the right hemisphere. Also, more spatial integration is needed with tactual vs. visual presentation. Thus, it was hypothesized that right hemisphere participation (rela- tive performance for left visual field and left hand) would increase with: (1) increasing typeface complexity, and (2) tactual vs. visual presentation. Subjects were 48 fourth- and ninth-graders and college under- graduates--l6 of each grade. All were right-handed males. They orally identified single capital letters in eight typefaces of varying com- plexity (defined by ratings on scriptlikeness, confusability, and difficulty). In the visual condition, letters were tachistosc0pically projected about 2 degrees to either side of a central fixation digit. Nancy H. Wagner In the tactual condition, subjects felt raised letters with two fingers of either hand. Presentation time was adjusted independently for each visual field (hand) to maintain 50% accuracy on each side. The depen- dent measure was the difference between median presentation times for the two sides for each typeface. With visual presentation, Hypothesis 1 was supported for all grades. A right field advantage was found for the simplest typeface and a left field advantage for two complex typefaces. However, overall degree of right hemisphere participation was significantly greater for undergraduates than for fourth- and ninth-graders. No hand differ- ence was found for any tactually-presented typeface, thereby failing to support Hypothesis 2. Another grade difference appeared in regression analyses relating complexity characteristics to average visual field differences for the typefaces. Scriptlikeness and confusability were predictors of field differences for undergraduates, but no combination of predictors was significant for fourth- and ninth-graders. In children, then, the balance of processing was shifted more toward left—hemisphere analy- sis and responded to different stimulus characteristics. These findings suggest that both for children and adults the processes used to recognize visually-presented complex letters are different from those used for printlike letters. Tactually-presented letters might require processing in both hemispheres regardless of complexity. Nancy M. Wagner REFERENCE Bryden, M. P., & Allard, F. Visual hemifield differences depend on typeface. Brain and Language, l976, g, 19l-200. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS My sincere thanks to the following people: Dr. Lauren Harris, my committee Chairperson, for his stimulating ideas and his constant support and friendship. Dr. Les Hyman, who served as substitute chairperson during the year I collected data, for his ideas and his help with the problems that arose. Dr. Neal Schmitt and Dr. John McKinney, my committee members, for all their help and encouragement. Steve Schacht, a student in methodology at the University of Chicago, for his valuable advice on the data analysis. My wonderful family, for their love and support. Without them this dissertation would not have been possible. ii TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES . LIST OF FIGURES LIST OF APPENDICES INTRODUCTION Background . Description of the Main Braille Learning Study Discussion of the Results of the Braille Learning Study . . Main Question to be Addressed in This Dissertation Presentation Modality . . . Modality versus Non-modality Approach to Form Perception . . . Relationship of Modality Differences and Hemisphere Specialization . . . . . . . . . Stimulus Characteristics . . Effects of Letter and Typeface Characteristics on the Letter Identification Process . . . Relationship of Stimulus Characteristics and Hemi- sphere Specialization Summary of Hypotheses Modality of Presentation . Stimulus Characteristics . Grade METHOD: PART I--TYPEFACE SCALING Scaling for "Difficulty" Subjects . Apparatus Materials . Procedure. Scaling for Scriptlikeness, Confusability, Unfamiliar- ity, and Superfluity . . . . . . . Subjects . 1'11 Page vi ix Materials . Procedure . METHOD: PART II--MEASUREMENT 0F FIELD DIFFERENCES . Visual and Tactual Conditions . Subjects . Apparatus Materials . Procedure . . . Computation of Laterality Coefficients and Their Stability/Effectiveness RESULTS: PART I--SCALE VALUES AND SELECTION OF TYPEFACES Ratings, Dispersion Measures, and Reliability Initial Group of Typefaces . . . Selected Typefaces . . RESULTS--PART II--FACTORS AFFECTING LATERALITY COEFFICIENTS IN THE VISUAL AND TACTUAL MODALITIES . . Main Analysis: Effects of General Typeface Factor, Grade, and Modality Order . . . . . . Three Repeated-Measures ANOVAs . . . . . . . Specific Typeface Characteristics: Average Threshold, Scriptlikeness, Confusability, and Difficulty . Average Threshold . . Correlations of Visual LCs, ATs, and Part I Characteristics . Prediction of Visual LCs from Typeface Character- istics Using Multiple Regression Stimulus and Procedural Factors: Typeface Position, Letter Order, and Specific Letters . . . Typeface Position Letter Order . Specific Letters . . . . . . . . . Individual Difference Factors: Individual LC Distribu- tion and Relation to AT, LC in Other Modality, and Familial Sinistrality. . LC Distribution . . . Relationship of Individual LCs and Combined Thres- holds, Corresponding LCs in Other Modality, and FS . . . . . iv Page 94 94 94 107 109 113 117 122 122 126 129 137 138 142 Page DISCUSSION . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 146 Presentation Modality . . 146 Reasons for Lack of Asymmetry in the TactuaI Condi- tion . . . . 147 Modality Differences in Processing . . . . . . 160 Stimulus Characteristics . . . . . l63 Implications for Letter Processing in Adults . . . 166 Implications for Letter Processing in Children . . l75 ENDNOTES . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 182 APPENDICES . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 187 REFERENCES . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 205 Table 10. 11. 12. LIST OF TABLES Stability of threshold measure by sensory field, modal- ity condition, typeface, and grade . . . . Percent of correct identification after cut-off by sensory field, typeface, and grade for each modality condition, and modality order . Total number of missed fixation points with correct and incorrect letter identification for each typeface and visual field for the grades separately and averaged across grades . . . . . . . . . . All 22 typefaces Correlations of five characteristics for all 22 type- faces and eight selected typefaces . . . . Selected eight typefaces Average laterality coefficients, standard deviations, and significance of field differences according to grade, typeface, modality condition, and modality order . . . . . . . . . . . . . Summary of analysis of variance on laterality coeffi- cients with modality factor included . . Summary of analysis of variance on visual laterality coefficients Summary of analysis of variance on tactual laterality coefficients Average combined thresholds and standard deviations according to grade, typeface, modality condition, and modality order . . . . . Correlations between Part I ratings on difficulty, scriptlikeness, and confusability and average combined threshold for each grade--(a) with all typefaces included, and (b) with T3 eliminated . . vi Page 80 81 83 87 90 91 98 100 104 108 111 114 Table Page 13. Correlations of laterality coefficients with Part I ratings on difficulty, scriptlikeness, and con- fusability and with average combined threshold for each grade--(a) with all typefaces included, and (b) with T3 eliminated . . . . . . . . . 114 14. Regression coefficients and statistics for analysis including subject and grade variables and for separate analyses by grade with four predictors of laterality coefficients . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 118 15. Average laterality coefficients for each letter order group at each grade for T5 and T8, and relative percent correct for the first three letters in each set . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 128 16. For T1, T2, and T7 in each modality condition, indi- vidual letters listed in order from highest to lowest average percent correct identifications . . . . . 131 17. Correlations between typefaces in average percent correct responses for individual letters . . . . . 132 18. Number of subjects per grade with a left or right field advantage for each typeface and modality condition, and modality order . . . . . . . . 139 19. Cross-modal correlations of laterality coefficients for each of the three typefaces presented in both modality conditions . . . . . . . . . . . . 145 A-1. Summary of analysis of variancecn1visual laterality coefficients obtained by applying the transformation ALCI + .5 x (:1), where the sign of the multiplier is the same as that of the original LC . . . . . 189 8-1. Summary of analysis of variance on visual laterality coefficients without the modality order variable, and Newman-Keuls multiple comparison tests for the main effects of Grade and Typeface . . . . . . . 191 C-1. Summary of analysis of variance on visual combined thresholds, and Newman-Keuls multiple comparison tests for the main effects of Grade and Typeface . . . . 193 0-1. Summary of analysis of variance on tactual combined thresholds, Newman-Keuls multiple comparison tests, and tests for the simple main effects of Grade for each typeface . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 195 vii Table E.1. G-1. H-1. Tabulation of F-ratios from separate ANOVAs for each typeface in each modality condition to test the effects of: (1) Typeface position, and (2) Letter order, and their interactions with grade . . . Correlations of individual laterality coefficients and CTs by grade, typeface, and modality condition Average laterality coefficients for FS+ and FS- Fourth- and Ninth-graders for all typefaces in each modality condition . . . . . . . . . viii Page 197 202 204 Figure F.1. LIST OF FIGURES Hypothesized relation between typeface characteristics and hemisphere specialization for the visual and tactual modalities . Average laterality coefficients for the three type- faces in the visual and tactual conditions for each grade separately and all grades combined Average laterality coefficients for the eight typefaces presented in the visual condition for each grade Average tactual laterality coefficients for each typeface according to grade and modality order Average LCs for each typeface when presented in the first half of the visual condition (N=24) compared to the average LCs when presented in the second half (N=24), with typefaces ordered according to degree of right hemisphere participation . Visual condition Tactual condition . ix Page 95 101 105 124 134 199 LIST OF APPENDICES Appendix A. Summary of ANOVA on Transformed Visual LCs B. Summary of ANOVA on Visual LCs with Modality Orders Combined . . . . . . . . . . . . C. Summary of ANOVA on Visual CTs 0. Summary of ANOVA on Tactual CTs . E. Summary of F-Ratios for Typeface Position and Letter Order . . . . F. Accuracy Per Hand for Individual Letters in the Tactual Condition G. Correlations of Individual LCs and CTs H. Average LCs for FS+ and FS- Groups . Page 188 190 192 194 196 198 201 203 INTRODUCTION Background The ability to recognize letters is generally regarded as a fun- damental prerequisite for reading (Gibson, Gibson, Pick, & Osser, 1962; Vernon, 1957). Yet, despite the importance of understanding the process of letter identification (considered here to include all the separate phases, such as perception and encoding, that might be involved in the total process) and the great volume of research in the area, the process is still poorly understood. Even the general nature of letter identification cannot be agreed upon. Some research- ers stress feature extraction and analysis as a critical step in the process (e.g., Gibson, 1969; Estes, 1975), while others find that it can be explained as a template matching process (e.g., Holbrook, 1975). In their attempt to better understand letter identification, researchers have analyzed the process with respect to a wide variety of factors. One is modality of letter presentation. In addition to studies of visual and auditory letter presentations, there have been studies of tactile presentations, including braille, embossed, and vibrotactile letters (for review, see Kirman, 1973). A second factor is letter qualities, such as letter typeface (Corcoran & Rouse, 1970) and symmetry (Fudin, Garcia, & Solomon, 1975). Researchers also have investigated certain task variables, such as whether subjects are asked to identify one letter or to categorize two letters as the same 1 or different (e.g., Cohen, 1972), or whether letters are presented alone or in groups (Estes, 1975). In addition, some studies have investigated the letter identification process with respect to the factor of cerebral hemisphere specialization (for review, see White, 1969, 1972) in conjunction with several factors mentioned. In a broad sense, the purpose of this dissertation is to further examine the letter identification process by determining whether and how it is affected by variation of some of these factors alone and in combination with each other. In a narrower sense, the purpose is to answer specific questions that arose from a series of studies of hand differences in braille letter learning (Wagner, 1976; Harris, Wagner, & Wilkinson, l976a, l976b; Harris, Wagner, Wilkinson, & Feinberg, l976c). Therefore, this section on background material for the ideas in this disserta- tion will consist of a brief description of the main braille learning study (Wagner, 1976) and the analysis of results that led to the questions currently being considered. Description of the Main Braille Learning Study The purpose of the main braille learning study was to explore possible cerebral hemisphere specialization for braille analysis. Assuming stronger contralateral than ipsilateral connections between hand and cerebral cortex, hemisphere advantage was inferred from hand advantage in learning braille letters. Braille letters are groups of one to five raised dots arranged in designated combinations of the six possible positions in a three by two cell. 0n the one hand, it was supposed that these letters could be analyzed and recalled as symbols of verbal material, like Roman letters for sighted individuals. Consequently, for right-handers, a right hand (left hemisphere) superiority for braille letter learning would be expected, paralleling the right visual hemifield (RVF) advantage for recognition of tachistoscopically projected Roman letters (e.g., Bryden, 1965). Contrarily, it was supposed that braille letters might be pro- cessed primarily spatially as complex patterns distinguished from one another by the number, location, and spatial configuration of their dots. In this case, a left-hand advantage would be expected, as has been found for the tactile perception of line orientation (Benton, Levin, & Varney, 1973; Varney & Benton, 1975) and irregular, presum- ably non-labelable shapes (Witelson, 1974, 1976). Left-hand superi- ority also would be consistent with the left visual hemifield (LVF) superiority found for dot enumeration (Kimura, 1966; Young & Bion, 1979) and dot localization (Kimura, 1969). To examine hand differences in braille letter discrimination, sighted, right-handed children and adults learned a different set of braille-like letters with each hand. Hand testing order and braille letter set assigned to hand were counterbalanced within each age by sex group. The letters were presented repeatedly in a semi-random order. Subjects were told the names of the letters while they first felt each one, and then had to guess the names on subsequent presen- tations lasting several second each. Subjects could see neither their hands nor the braille cards. As measured by the percentage of correct letter identifications with each hand, the group scores revealed a significantly better left-hand than right-hand performance. Although it is this overall result that is important for the purpose of this background section, it should be noted that the extent, and even direction, of hand dif- ference was related to hand testing order and the age and sex of the subject. These variables will be considered later. The overall left-hand advantage was consistent with earlier studies of blind adults and children (e.g., Hermelin & O'Connor, 1971), sighted adults (Smith, 1929, 1934; Harriman & Castell, 1979), and sighted older children (Rudel, Denckla, & Spalten, 1974). Hermelin and O'Connor (1971) suggested an explanation of the hand differences in terms of cortical specialization. They proposed that braille configurations are treated as spatial stimuli and thus are analyzed more efficiently by the right hemisphere before or during verbal identification and naming by the left hemisphere, resulting in an advantage for the left hand. Discussion of the Results of the Braille Learning Study What is still unclear is why the spatial requirements should overshadow the linguistic nature of the stimuli in the case of braille, while the reverse has been found in the case of our Roman letters presented visually. One reason may be that braille letters are more difficult to discriminate than Roman ones, inasmuch as the braille dots require differentiation of minute differences in orientation and spacing of dots separated by a distance just beyond the minimum two-point threshold. The possibility that the superiority of right hemisphere pro- cessing for braille learning stems from the greater difficulty of discriminating braille compared to Roman letters is related to the findings of Bryden and Allard (1976). They showed college students tachistosc0pically-projected single letters in ten different type- faces. Subjects had to orally identify the letter shown. The result was a typeface by visual field interaction. The majority of type- faces showed a RVF (left hemisphere) advantage, but two typefaces, which were more "scriptlike" and more difficult to recognize than the others, showed a LVF (right hemisphere) advantage. Bryden and Allard explained these results in terms of hemisphere processing differences. They supposed that the right hemisphere is more efficient at certain global preprocessing Operations carried out prior to letter naming, while the left hemiSphere is better at the more analytical identifi- cation and naming stages. The more scriptlike typefaces probably required more initial preprocessing “to normalize the stimulus and to focus attention on the relevant characteristics of the target" (p. 198). The greater global preprocessing capacity of the right hemisphere therefore became critical, leading to a right hemisphere advantage. Presumably, then, braille letters, like the more script- like Roman alphabet letters, could require much "preprocessing" by the right hemisphere prior to letter identification and naming by the left hemisphere. According to this account, right hand (left hemisphere) superi- ority would be expected for the tactual discrimination of Roman alphabet letters printed in a simple block form. However, studies of tactual discrimination of Roman letters have suggested greater right hemisphere involvement than generally has been found in visual presentation studies. 0f eight studies reviewed, two suggested a left hand (right hemisphere) advantage (Gardner, 1942; Cioffi & Kandel, 1979, for boys), two found a marginal right-hand (left hemisphere) advantage (Witelson, 1977a, for boys; Oscar-Berman, Rehbein, Porfert, & Goodglass, 1978), and four found no hand differ- ence (Witelson, 1974; LaBreche, Manning, Goble, & Markman, 1977; Klein & Rosenfield, 1980; Manning, 1980). From these findings another possibility is suggested-~that left hand superiority on the braille task is related to the special nature of the tactual modality. Perhaps the way most tactile information is detected or processed necessitates a greater degree of integration and, therefore, greater right hemisphere involvement than the same information presented visually. Main Question to be Addressed in This Dissertation The basic hypothesis of this dissertation is that hemisphere Specialization for letter identification (as reflected in hand and visual field asymmetries) will be influenced systematically by both stimulus characteristics (such as discrimination difficulty and scriptlikeness) and presentation modality. When letters varying in general complexity are presented both visually and tactually, it is expected that left hemisphere advantage (right hand or RVF advantage), if any, will decrease and right hemisphere advantage (left hand or LVF advantage) will gradually appear as the degree of complexity is increased (see Figure 1). However, the point along the continuum of complexity at which a left hemisphere advantage will change to a right hemisphere advantage is expected to differ for the two modali- ties because of their different natures. Left-hand advantage is expected to begin at a more moderate degree of complexity than that required for a LVF advantage. One main question to be addressed in this dissertation is whether age of subject and concomitant variation in familiarity with cursive and manuscript letters will modify the hypothesized effects of modal- ity and letter typeface on hemisphere specialization for letter stimuli. Students about to enter fourth and ninth grades and college students will be tested. The fourth-graders should have just begun to learn cursive writing within the past two years. Therefore, although the fourth-graders should be able to recognize cursive letters, they will be relatively inexperienced with them. Ninth- graders will represent an intermediate degree of familiarity with cursive letters and with linguistic stimuli in general, whereas college students should be highly practiced with this type of mate- rial. In summary, the general purpose of this dissertation is to clarify further the process of letter identification. Specifically, it is Right (R) sensory field (F) adv. LF=RF Left (L) sensory field (F) adv. AVERAGE LATERALITY COEFFICIENTS Visual I——-l Tactual 13— —u Simplest TYPEFACE Most Complex Fig. l. Hypothesized relation between typeface characteristics and hemisphere specialization for the visual and tactual modalities. hypothesized that hemisphere specialization for letter identification depends on the interaction of modality of presentation and letter typeface characteristics, and that this relationship varies with the subject's age. The rest of the introduction is divided into two main sections: first, a discussion of presentation modality; second, a discussion of stimulus characteristics. Each section will begin with a consideration of some apparently important aspects of the factor, followed by evidence for the effects of these aspects on hemisphere specialization for letter identification. Finally, specific predic- tions about the interaction of the two factors will be summarized. Presentation Modality Modality versus Non-modality Approach to Form Perception The main question about modality to be addressed in this disser- tation is whether it critically affects the accuracy and process of letter identification. The first step in dealing with this question will be to address the issue of the influence of modality on form perception in general. This more global issue has been considered by Goodnow (1971a). She assumed that an object could be explored in many different ways, and with different modalities, and that on any two inspections a person might have difficulty deciding whether he was dealing with one and the same object (Goodnow, 1971a, p. 4). Goodnow supposed, however, that the difficulty could not be attributed to presentation of the object in different perceptual modalities. Instead, she suggested an approach emphasizing differences in 10 exploratory behavior, or in memory or encoding processes, that would cut across modalities. Comparison of scanning patterns. This non-modality approach is reflected in other research that has stressed the nature of scanning and exploratory processes, some of which Goodnow cited. Vurpillot (1976), Gibson (1966), Piaget (1956), and many of the Soviet researchers in the area of learning and perception in children (see review by Pick, 1963; Zaporozhets, 1965) have related children's increasingly accurate perceptual judgments to increasingly thorough exploratory movements of eye and hand. In general, for Vurpillot, and for the Soviet researchers (Zaporozhets, l965),thorough explora- tory movements enable the observertxiform an accurate model, or image, of the object being explored. From Gibson's point of view, the emphasis should be more on the importance of the movements for facili- tating the abstraction of critical stimulus features. Piaget (1956, 1961) emphasized the importance of the movements for creating "decen- tration" or more uniform distribution of attention across the object. Despite slightly differentideasmabout the function of the explora- tory movements, all of these researchers have noted the similarity of the development of these movements in the tactual and visual modalities. In both modalities, development proceeds through stages in which the movements become progressively more systematic and more consistent with the stimulus outline. Although the emergence of sys- tematic movements occurs earlier in the visual modality, movements in both perceptual modalities reach a fairly mature stage by at least 11 nine years of age (Vurpillot, 1976; Zaporozhets, 1965). This work therefore emphasizes fundamental similarities in the character of the two modalities, thereby supporting the non-modality approach advo- cated by Goodnow. Comparison of relative saliency of different properties. Other evidence Goodnow related to the non-modality approach is not so strongly suggestive of inter-modal similarities, and might even be reinterpreted to support a strict modality approach. One type of evidence consists of the comparison of stimulus characteristics most salient to the hand and to the eye. To make this comparison, Goodnow (1969) tested children on a match-to-sample task in which the stimuli were examined either tactually or visually. On each trial, the child was given a standard and two comparison stimuli, which would vary from the standard in size, orientation, curvature of line, or number of lines. In general, a stimulus that varied in curvature was most often judged different from the standard when the stimuli were looked at, while a stimulus that varied in orientation resulted in the strong- est sense of difference from the standard when the stimuli were felt. According to Goodnow (1971a), part of the difficulty in judging whether an object that is felt is the same as one that is seen thus can be explained in terms of differential sampling of stimulus proper- ties rather than in terms of modalities per se. Further support for this explanation comes from studies (Pick & Pick, 1966; Gibson et al., 1962) in which children were presented with letter-like standard forms followed by a series of transformations of these forms and at least 12 one duplicate of the standard. The children then were asked to select the forms that were the same as the standard. With visual presenta- tion, Gibson et al. found that the line to curve transformations and orientation transformations were of about equal difficulty. In con- trast, Pick and Pick found orientation transformations to be very easy tactual discriminations, compared with line to curve and other transformations. Therefore, whether the measure is disciminability or preferred basis of similarity, orientation seems to be relatively more salient for the tactual modality, while curvature is relatively more salient for the visual modality. The question that follows from Goodnow's explanation in terms of sampling method is, which factor--modality or sampling method--should be considered more fundamental? If it is sampling method, this implies that a given modality could be easily trained in the stimulus sampling procedures characteristic of another modality. In this case, Good- now's use of the evidence pertaining to the relative salience of stimulus properties to support the non-modality approach seems justi- fied. However, if sampling method is inherent in or a natural product of the special nature of the modality, then the critical factor is still modality. The difference in sampling procedure might even be considered to enhance the difference between modalities. In any case, it seems just as reasonable to suppose that the nature of the modality gives rise to its typical sampling method as to suppose that sampling differences override fundamental modality differences. 13 Comparison of recall accuracy. A second type of evidence used by Goodnow to support the non-modality approach involves research on the role of memory in cross-modal matching. To test for the effects of memory demand, Goodnow (1971b) likened the effects of increasing the time delay between presentation of standard and comparison objects to increasing the number of comparison objects. Both manipu- lations would make it harder to remember the object presented first. Goodnow presented adults with one, three, or five comparison objects after a standard first inspected by eye or hand. In general, errors increased with the number of comparison objects when inspection of the standard was by hand, but errors remained constant when inspection of the standard was visual. These results are consistent with those of a previous study (Posner, 1967) which found that memory for kines- thetically perceived distances decayed more during an unfilled time interval than did memory for visually perceived distances. A greater decrease in recall for conditions involving a tactual component com- pared to those with only visual components also has been found for children by increasing the number of comparison items (Davidson, Cambardella, Stenerson, & Carney, 1974a), and for adults by increas- ing the retention intervals (Abravanel, 1973). The general conclusion from these findings is that increased memory demand had a different effect on stimulus recall, depending on whether the stimulus was perceived by hand or by eye. This conclusion assumes that information acquisition was equal in the two modalities or, at least, that the accuracy of information acquisition did not 14 affect the retention curves. Again, Goodnow (1971a) rejected an explanation in terms of intrinsic modality differences and, instead, attributed the weakness of memory for tactually perceived stimuli to the inexperience of normally sighted subjects at gathering informa- tion by that means. She cited evidence from work with blind subjects to support her point of view. Subsequent research on tactual scanning strategies (Davidson, Barnes, & Mullen, 1974b; see Davidson, 1976, for review) has reinforced Goodnow's view by showing that individuals with extensive tactual experience (e.g., blind people) are not so seriously affected by memory demand as less experienced persons, at least partly because of the experienced individuals' more efficient ways of exploring the stimulus. While this research suggests that memory for tactually per- ceived stimuli can be improved through experience, this mainly holds for extensive long-term experience, like that received by a congeni- tally blind individual. There is, moreover, no evidence that the level of retention of tactual information achieved is equivalent to the retention of the analogous visual information by sighted persons. Thus, for each modality, there might be a range of retention accuracy according to the degree of experience at information gathering through that channel. These ranges might overlap slightly at the extremes, but the fundamental memory difference between modalities will still apply. Comparison of memory encoding_processes. In addition to con- sidering recall accuracy, it is important to consider whether the 15 actual encoding process differs for stimuli presented in the two modalities. Millar (1975) has argued that both blind and sighted children show both tactual and verbal encoding of tactually pre- sented common objects. It also has been shown that adults possess a short-term tactual storage for non-verbal materials (Bliss, Crane, Mansfield, & Townsend, 1966). This short-term tactual store pre- sumably means that the characteristic features of the way an object feels can be stored in memory beyond brief iconic persistence and can be used to aid recall even a minute or two after presentation. Thus, tactual information apparently can survive perception and can affect recall, just as visual information can (Millar, 1972). While both modalities have their own characteristic memory store, the storage of tactually perceived spatial or form information seems to be more flexible than the storage of visual information, and, in many cases, can even involve visual encoding (Attneave & Benson, 1969; Freides, 1974; Pick, 1970). For example, visual imagery seems to play a large role in cross-modal matching studies, even in the intra-modal (tactual-tactual matching) control condition (Cairns & C011, 1977; Jackson, 1973). Researchers who have hypothesized visual storage of tactual information have generally stressed the quali- tatively different facilities for data handling possessed by differ- ent modalities and the tendency for sensory information to be trans- ferred to the modality most adept at processing and storing it. Therefore, the range and type of strategies available for memory storage appear to be different for the two modalities, even if the 16 modality difference in recall accuracy does diminish with extensive practice at gathering information tactually. Comparison of the need for spatial integration in the visual and tactual modalities. After considering some other possible inter- pretations of Goodnow's arguments for a non-modality approach to form perception, it seems that care must be taken not to underemphasize the importance of modality. Though it is possible to treat stimulus sampling method and practice at encoding a stimulus in memory as alternatives to modality explanations and as reasons for modality differences, these factors also can be dealt with as consequences of a difference in the basic natures of the visual and tactual modali- ties. A comparison of these basic natures must be based in large part on speculations and observations, because empirical questions about this problem are difficult to formulate. The main idea of many of the theorists who have compared the two modalities is that tactual perception necessitates more integration from separate points in time and space. For instance, Revesz (1950) has stated that tactual perceptions are more sums of separate parts than are their visual equivalents. Similarly, Vurpillot (1976) observed that "the visual receptor system permits the simultaneous experience of multiple data, while the tactile system only provides limited data in succession" (p. 273). Even though Gibson (1962) emphasized the similarity between visual and tactual information gathering, he also observed that visual perception is based on "the figure-ground phenomenon, the simultaneous registering of the whole 17 contour, whereas the unity of the tactual perception has to be based on either cutaneously separate impressions or on successive impres- sions" (p. 488). Another way of viewing this extra demand on the tactual modality is to consider that the tactual modality may be especially suited for the analysis of spatiotemporal displays. These spatiotemporal dis- plays require the integration of points separated in time and space. Transforming a spatialcfisplayinto a spatiotemporal one by the addi- tion of movement generally impedes perception of qualities by the eye, yet facilitates perception by hand (Kirman, 1973). One example mentioned by Kirman (1973, p. 66) deals with the perception of moving letters by hand and eye. Visually, the moving print is experienced as a blur at relatively low rates of movement. Yet such movement is exactly what is needed to aid the hand in reading braille or embossed Roman letters. Kirman's view is consistent with Kasajima's (1974) comparison of the function of pauses and movements in visual and braille read- ing. According to Kasajima, “movements of the eye are so rapid that no perception of words or letters occurs during them; but in braille reading, perception occurs only during movement" (p. 54). Thus, the integration of successively perceived points is both a requirement and a special ability of the tactual modality. Comparison of informationegathering tempo in the visual and tactual modalities. In general, the hand is slower than the eye at gathering information. The limited tempo of tactual 18 information gathering in combination with the serial nature of tactual exploration determine to a large degree the significantly poorer performance in the tactual-tactual matching condition compared to the visual-visual matching condition in most cross-modal studies of form perception (e.g., Abravanel, 1973, with adults; Jackson, 1973; Cronin, 1973; Davidson et al., 1974a). In such studies, there have been attempts to control for the slower information gathering capa- bilities of the tactual modality by using a longer presentation period for the tactual stimuli than for the visual stimuli. Davidson, Abbott, and Gershenfield (1974c) found that the accuracy of form matching in the visual and tactual intra-modal conditions did not differ significantly with l6-sec. exposure times for tactual stimuli against four-sec. times for visual, although the tactual scores were still lower. Other attempts have not been successful and have found a signifi- cant difference between intramodal conditions even when the tactual stimuli were explored for 30 sec. compared to five sec. for the visual stimuli (Cairns & C011, 1977; Butter & Bjorklund, 1973). This difference became non-significant in one of the studies (Butter & Bjorklund, 1973) only when visual exploration time was reduced to two sec. Jackson (1973) attempted to control for the modality dif- ference in information gathering tempo and in the serial nature of exploration by having children in the visual condition follow a dot of light as it moved around the contour of the form. Errors were still significantly greater in the tactual intramodal condition. The 19 results of these attempts to control for exploration pace and manner demonstrate the much longer exploration time needed in the tactual modality to attain parity in accuracy to that in the visual modality. The findings also indicate that even with controls, visual performance surpasses tactual performance, suggesting that other fundamental dif- ferences between modalities remain. Comparison of the spatial resolving power of the tactual and visual modalities. Another possible critical difference between vision and touch with respect to the question at hand is in their spatial resolv- ing power. Evidence for the poorer spatial resolving power of the skin has come mainly from studies of simultaneous masking (see Kirman, 1973, for review). For instance, Loomis and Apkarian-Stielau (l976) looked at the effects of simultaneous lateral masking on tactile and blurred visual letter recognition. They attempted to control for the differ- ent spatial resolving powers of the skin and eye by changing the spac- ing and size of their visual and vibrotactile arrays. Even with this control, tactile recognition was worse in all masking conditions. Also, when the accuracy scores for each letter perceived visually were corre- lated with the accuracy scores for letters perceived by touch, the correlation was only .68 for the no-mask condition. Thus, the spatial resolving power of the skin is poorer than that of the eye, but even when this factor is controlled, other fundamental modality differences affect the accuracy and quality of performance. In conclusion, in some instances, especially when developmental progression is of main concern, the nature and function of exploratory 20 processes are similar in the visual and tactual modalities, and, in many ways, are more critical than modality considerations. However, the importance of considering the sensory modality in which a stimulus is perceived should not be overlooked. Differences between the visual and tactual modalities in relative saliency of various stimulus dimen- sions and in memory strength and encoding processes can be thought of as reflections of a fundamental difference in the natures of the two modalities. These natures are determined by such factors as spatial resolving power, information gathering tempo, and the need for spatial and temporal integration. While most previous studies of modality differences have been concerned with form perception in general, there is limited evidence to suggest that letter identification may also vary systematically with modality of presentation. Relationship of Modality Differences and Hemisphere Specialization Assuming fundamental differences between the visual and tactual modalities, the next step in developing the hypotheses of this dis- sertation is to examine whether these differences are related to hemisphere specialization. First, consideration will be given to possible reasons for expecting an association between hemisphere and modality on the basis of previously discussed modality differences. Then, evidence from past research on visual field asymmetries will be compared to evidence pertaining to hand differences on similar tasks. If there is a modality difference in direction or degree of asymmetry, then a relationship between modality and cerebral 21 hemisphere might be expected. For the purposes of this dissertation, the emphasis will be on letter and word identification tasks. Finally, several studies that have examined the correlations between left-right asymmetry on a dichotic listening task and a visual field test will be considered. While these studies do not directly com- pare tactile and visual asymmetry, they do address the general issue of the effect of modality of presentation on hemisphere specialization. Expectations based ongpreviously discussed modality differences. One modality difference previously discussed is the difference in relative saliency of curvature (greater for vision) and orientation (greater for touch). An association of modality with cerebral hemi- sphere would be indicated if it could be shown that each hemisphere is specialized for the judgment of one of these characteristics, or if hemisphere specialization is found for one of the characteristics or one of the presentation modalities, but not the other. In general, for both visual and tactual presentations, the right hemisphere seems to be specialized for the analysis of line orientation, as long as the difference between lines is small (Umilta, Rizzolatti, Marzi, Zamboni, Franzini, Camarada, & Berlucchi, 1974, for vision; Varney & Benton, 1975; Benton, Levin, & Varney, 1973; Benton, Varney, & Hamsher, 1978, for touch). With a difference between lines of 45 degress or more, a right visual field (RVF), or left hemisphere, advantage has been found (Umilta et al., 1974; White, 1971). However, it is possi- ble that this left hemisphere advantage indicates only that the easier discriminations are more available to linguistic analysis. 22 A tendency toward right hemisphere specialization for the judg- ment of arc curvature also has been indicated (Longden, Ellis, & Iversen, 1976, for vision; Nebes, 1971a, for touch). Longden et al. found a left visual field advantage (right hemisphere) in reaction time for judgments of whether two arcs differed in curvature. Using commissurotomized patients, Nebes showed that the right hemisphere was superior to the left in matching tactually perceived arcs to their corresponding circles. With normal subjects, a right hemisphere advantage on a modified arc-circle matching test was found by Fagan- Dubin (1978), although no hand difference has been observed by others (Nebes, 1971b; Kutas, McCarthy, & Donchin, 1975). If the per- ception of the orientation and curvature of forms can be related to the perception of simple line orientation and arc curvature, then the modality difference in relative saliency of these two character- istics does not seem to be a reason for expecting an association between modality and cerebral hemisphere. A second modality difference is that for sighted subjects, the image of a tactually perceived object, compared to the image of a visually perceived object, is more susceptible to temporal decay and more likely to be stored at least partly in another modality (vision). There is no clear evidence that memory strength is related to cerebral hemisphere. A LVF superiority for the recognition of complex, random shapes has been found to increase with the length of recall interval, becoming significant at the 10 and 20 sec. intervals (Dee & Fontenot, 1973). This finding might suggest an association of greater memory 23 strength with the right hemisphere, but it is more likely that short- term memory demands merely enhance the existing factors (perceptual and others) affecting hemisphere specialization. Then, greater memory demands would also be expected to increase RVF superiority for verbal stimuli. Rosen, Curcio, MacKavey, and Hebert (1975) investigated this hypothesis by presenting subjects with bilateral columns of four letters and asking them to report only one column in each trial. Based on their finding that the largest visual field asymmetry occurred for letters in the fourth position, they concluded that short-term memory demands contributed to the observed asymmetry. There is also no direct evidence available to associate with either hemisphere the transfer of tactual information to a visual image. However, the left parietal region has been associated with tactile-visual matching (Butters, Barton, & Brody, 1970), which might suggest a left hemisphere advantage for the transfer from a tactile to visual image. Overall, evidence about the association of memory factors with hemisphere differences is weak and requires many infer— ences. No general association of modality and cerebral hemisphere is apparent. A third difference between the hand and the eye is concerned with the degree to which information from successively perceived points must be integrated during stimulus exploration. Since the hand must integrate information from separate points to a much greater degree than the eye, it might be expected that the tactual modality would be associated with the serial processing attributed to the left hemi- sphere (Cohen, 1973). 24 However, more critically, it seems that the tactual integration of separate points requires active construction and manipulation of increasingly more complete images. This active process of construc- tion involves thinking in a "spatial" way, which has been categor- ized as a right hemisphere process (e.g., Kimura, 1973). The integration of separate points required by the tactual modality also seems to depend on what Broadbent (1974) has described as "a stored representationcnithe world that retains its features in parallel for quite long periods of time" (p. 40). In other words, recently per- ceived stimulus points can be integrated with less recently perceived points only if the previously gathered information is retained. These processes that involve sustaining the continuing representations of the environment are characterized by Broadbent (1974, p. 40) as right hemisphere processes. Contrarily, left hemisphere processes would involve categorizing changes in the environment. Thus, intuitive consideration of the fundamental nature of the modalities might lead to an association between right hemisphere processing and the tactual modality. Expectations based on past research: previous studies of visual field asymmetry. In general, past studies of visual field asymmetry have shown a RVF advantage for the recognition of verbal stimuli (see reviews by White, 1969, 1972). A RVF superiority in adults has been found for: (l) recognition of single letters pre- sented along the:horizontal meridian(Bryden, 1965, 1966, 1973; Bryden & Rainey, 1963; Worall & Coles, 1976; Fennell, Bowers, & 25 Satz, 1977; Schmuller, 1979) and, especially, the encoding phase of letter identification (Cohen, 1976); (2) time taken to judge whether two letters have the same name (Cohen, 1972; Geffen, Bradshaw, & Nettleton, 1972); (3) recognition of single letters presented in groups (Bryden, 1966; Kimura, 1966); and (4) word recognition (Fontenot, 1973). These studies that have found a RVF advantage typically have employed a successive unilateral presentation technique with binocu- lar viewing and a verbal identification response. The two major problems with this technique are the possible confounding of the hemisphere specialization effect with trace scanning tendencies and with response factors (White, 1969, 1972). However, since at least some of the studies reporting a RVF advantage used methods or stimuli that minimized the effects of these possible confounding factors, we can conclude that a hemisphere difference in processing is at least one important reason for the RVF advantage. First, trace scanning refers to the scanning of the iconic image that persists briefly following stimulus presentation. While the general tendency to scan the post-exposural stimulus trace in a left to right direction would lead to a RVF superiority for unilaterally presented stimuli, this trace scanning should not have been a critical factor when only single letters were presented. Second, although the RVF advantage in some of the studies might have been partly the result of left hemis- phere control of the identification response, this should not have been the case in other stUdies,especially those requiring a rapid 26 "same” or "different" response. In summary, the RVF advantage for verbal stimuli can be attributed largely to an asymmetry in hemi- spheric processing. Effects of age on visual field asymmetry for the regggnition of verbal stimuli. While a RVF advantage for the recognition of verbal stimuli generally has been found for seven- to l3-year-old children who are normal readers, the findings are not as clear as those for adults. Some developmental studies of visual field asymme- try for word identification have found a significant RVF advantage for the one age group tested (McKeever & Huling, 1970; Marcel, Katz, & Smith, 1974; Marcel & Rajan, 1975) or a constant RVF advantage across all ages tested (Olson, 1973). Other similar studies have found a significant RVF advantage for children older than 10 or 12 years of age, but not for the youngest age groups tested (Forgays, 1953; Miller & Turner, 1973; Turner & Miller, 1975, only under certain conditions of word length and post-exposure field; Reitsma, 1975, cited by Witelson, 1977b; Carmon, Nachshon, & Starinsky, 1976; Tomlinson-Keasey, Kelly, & Burton, 1978, for same-different judgments of sequentially presented words). Since all these studies involved unilateral presentation of horizontally arranged words, one question is whether the effects are due to hemisphere specialization or to left to right scanning of the post-exposural stimulus trace. This question was addressed in two studies that included conditions for which the two factors would predict opposite results. Olson (1973) used a bilateral presentation 27 condition. According to trace scanning theory, scanning should begin at the left most point of the image, and thus the left field word should be reported more accurately. Carmon et a1. (1976) used Hebrew words, which are scanned right to left and therefore should elicit a LVF advantage when presented unilaterally. Since a RVF (left hemisphere) advantage was found in both instances,hemisphere specialization is most likely the dominant factor. It should be cautioned, however, that while the weight of the evidence supports the conclusion of left hemisphere specialization in children for visually presented linguistic stimuli, not all studies are in agreement. Yeni-Komshian, Isenberg, and Goldberg (1975), for example, found no field asymmetry in 10- to 13-year olds for verti- cally arranged digit names. In addition, following the predictions of trace scanning theory, Daves and Werzberger (1971) found a LVF advantage for children in grades one to seven for the identification of strings of six letters presented bilaterally. A second question about the developmental results is whether the pattern of asymmetry changes significantly with age. Of the studies of word identification cited above, over half failed to find a RVF advantage for the youngest age groups tested. While this age trend should be considered tentative because of the lack of controls for changing task difficulty across age groups, further support may be found in several studies of asymmetry for letter discrimination. Reynolds and Jeeves (1978) measured choice reaction time to single lateralized letters. There were four possible letters. Subjects 28 were instructed to press one button if either of two specified letters appeared and to press the other button if either of the other two letters appeared. A RVF advantage was found for 13- and l4-year— olds and adults, but not for seven- and eight-year-olds. Using a similar task with lateralized letter pairs as stimuli, Broman (1978) found a slight RVF advantage for 13-year-olds and adults, a slight LVF advantage for lO-year-olds, and a significant LVF advantage for seven-year-olds. An identical age pattern was found by Carmon et al. (1976) for the identification of single lateralized Hebrew letters. There was no significant field asymmetry at any age except for a LVF advantage for seven-year-olds. Explanations for this increasing RVF advantage with age focused on the ideas of either increasing specialization of the left hemi- sphere for processing the printed word (Tomlinson-Keasey et al., 1978) or, as is more likely the case, a change in strategy with age. For instance, Carmon et a1. (1976) suggested that sequential (left hemisphere) processing of verbal materials becomes more accentuated with age. Similarly Broman (1978) argued that the "configurational? approach may be the more primitive and the "naming" approach the more advanced manner of perceiving alphabetical material. In summary, children, as well as adults, have shown a RVF (left hemisphere) advantage for the processing of linguistic stimuli. If there is a change with age in the relative participation of the two hemispheres in the recognition of printed material, the evidence suggests that it is in the direction of increasing left hemisphere participation with increasing age. Therefore, it there is an age 29 difference in asymmetry in this dissertation, it can be predicted that the effect will be a smaller RVF advantage for fourth-graders compared to ninth-graders and college students. Expectations based (n1 past research: previous studies of hand asymmetry. The eight studies of hand asymmetry for Roman letters that were reviewed can be grouped according to two procedural vari- ables: (1) whether the presentation was dichhaptic (different letters presented simultaneously to the two hands) or unilateral (letters presented to one hand at a time); and (2) whether exploration was active (subjects moved their fingertips over the letter) or passive (subjects held their hands stationary while letters were drawn on them). Results were inconsistent, even within groups of studies using similar procedures. The procedure used by five of the studies involved dichhaptic presentation with active exploration. Witelson (1974, 1977a & c) conducted two studies with six- to l4-year-old boys. Her stimuli were twenty styrofoam upper-case letters, excluding five of the 1 On a total of nine laterally symmetrical letters of the alphabet. given trial, two pairs of letters were presented for two seconds each, and the subject's task was to report orally the names of the four letters he had just felt. No hand asymmetry was found in the first study, while a marginally significant (p < .10) right hand (left hemisphere) advantage was found for boys2 in the second. How- ever, since the verbal identification response probably necessitated left hemisphere involvement, one possible interpretation of the 1.. I .2. li— 30 findings is that right hemisphere superiority in processing was masked by the left hemisphere's control of the response mode. To avoid this confounding of response and processing modes, LaBreche et a1. (1977) used a left hand and a right hand fingerspell- ing letter identification response as well as a written response. While hemisphere specialization for fingerspelling has not been clearly established, the evidence suggests either a bilateral repre- sentation of manuallinguisth:stimuli or a tendency toward right hemisphere dependency.3 Therefore, use of the fingerspelling response might, if anything, tend to enhance any right hemisphere superiority in processing. Despite this change in response method, LaBreche et al. (1977) found no hand differences for deaf and hearing adoles- cents. Klein and Rosenfield (1980) used still a different, supposedly "neutral" response method with a similar dichhaptic procedure. They had third-graders point to their response choices using a "double- handed" pointer in the shape of a divining rod. Again, no hand dif- ference was found. In the fifth related study, Cioffi and Kandel (1979) also used a pointing response, but presented pairs of two consonants (consonant bigrams) rather than single letters. They found a left hand (right hemisphere) advantage for six- to 14-year-old boys. One question raised by this study, though, is whether letters or short sequences of letters are adequate linguistic stimuli. This question was suggested by the results of another condition in which two letter 31 words were presented. A right hand advantage was found for these pre- sumably more adequate linguistic stimuli. One problem with these findings is that, if anything, sequences of two letters should be closer to word stimuli than single letters. Therefore, if right hand advantage increased as the linguistic characteristics of the stimulus increased, consonant bigrams would be more likely than single letters to result in no hand advantage or a trend toward right hand advantage. There was only one study that combined a dichhaptic technique with a passive procedure, where the experimenter controlled delivery of the stimuli (Oscar-Berman et al., 1978). Following the presenta- tion of one pair of single capital consonants, the adult subjects' task was to report the letter names orally in the order then desig- nated by the experimenter. A marginal (p_< .06) right hand (left hemisphere) advantage was found, not only for the hand reported second. According to the authors, asymmetry was enhanced for the hand reported second because a greater memory demand was involved. Finally, stimulus presentation was unilateral in two studies, one with a passive procedure and one with active exploration. Both studies used strings of letters and an oral identification response, and both tested adult subjects. Manning (1980) had subjects hold their hands stationary while the experimenter drew sequences of six capital letters on the palm. There was no hand asymmetry in the number of letters remembered. Using a different exploration technique, Gardner (1942) had 30 students read nonsense syllables with the fingers in both a left I151 wfli.§ 32 to right and a right to left direction. The syllables consisted of laterally symmetrical letters made of cord stitched upon card- board. Performance in both reading directions was faster and more accurate for the left hand than for the right. Summed across the two reading directions, the time difference between the hands was about 5.5 seconds (with 67% of the sample faster with the left hand), and the difference in number of errors was about .7. No statistical tests were performed, but at the least, a trend toward left hand superiority for the recognition of Roman letters was demonstrated. If these findings can be interpreted in terms of cerebral asymmetry, then even tasks requiring the linguistic processing involved in read— ing, when presented through the tactual system, seem to require some degree of spatial, "right-hemisphere" analysis. In summary, of the eight studies of hand asymmetry for Roman letter recognition, only four found a significant hand advantage, two in favor of the right hand and two in favor of the left. One of the studies that showed a right hand advantage used a dichhaptic, active presentation technique and the other used a dichhaptic, passive procedure with a moderate memory demand. However, the three other studies using a dichhaptic, active procedure failed to find a hand asymmetry, as did another study using a passive procedure combined with an even greater memory demand. Also, in both studies that showed a right hand advantage, the letters were named orally. Possibly, then, these results were related mainly to a left hemisphere advantage for the spoken response and not to a hemispheric asymmetry for letter perception and processing. 33 Although the asymmetry was stronger in the two studies that found a left hand advantage, there again were some unanswered ques- tions. No statistical tests were performed in one study, and the left hand advantage switched to a right hand advantage in the other study when two-letter words as opposed to consonant bigrams were used as sitmuli. All in all, the results show that it is difficult to elicit an asymmetry for the tactual perception of Roman letters, possibly indicating that both hemispheres contribute importantly to the letter processing. The findings of a left hand advantage for reading nonsense syllables and bigrams are consistent with some of the more recent reports of hand asymmetry for braille reading and letter recognition mentioned in the first section. With blind adult subjects, Hermelin and O'Connor (1971) found a left hand advantage in the accuracy with which vertical columns of braille letters were read. With blind children, they found that sentences were read faster and more accurately with the left hand than with the right. Thus, even when a tactile alphabet becomes so familiar a linguistic system as to be almost second-nature, the very fact that it is perceived tactually might induce a certain degree of right hemisphere analysis. Research with sighted subjects has supported the conclusion that this left hand advantage at least partly represents a hemi- sphere processing difference and not just a bias resulting from dif- ferent amounts and types of experience with braille reading for the two hands. As stated in the first section, an overall left hand 34 advantage for braille letter identification also has been found for sighted subjects. This pattern of left hand advantage, however, was influenced by the order in which the hands were tested. The main effect of hand testing order was an enhancement of left hand performance when it followed the right. Rudel et al. (1974) tested children with a paired associate learning method simi- lar to the procedure of the braille learning study described in the first section (Wagner, 1976). They found that girls did about as well with their right hand whether it was tested first or second, but their left hand scores were much better than the right when tested second, and worse when tested first. To explain this order effect, Rudel et a1. suggested that prior training with the right hand helps or "prepares" the left, possibly because right hand use activates verbal strategies tended to be relied on by young girls (Harris, 1977). Wagner (1976) also found that left hand performance was enhanced when it followed the right. Although scores were generally higher for the second hand tested, the difference between hand scores was greater when the left hand was tested second than when the right hand was second. These findings could be explained in terms of an interaction of the factors of practice and hemisphere specialization, but they also might indicate that part of the left hand superiority depends on prior preparation through left hemisphere strategies. The latter interpretation is consistent with Kinsbourne's (1973) discussion of lateral asymmetries in terms of "attentional 35 sets" induced by the nature of the task or response method. When the task or response method favors the processing in one hemisphere, that hemisphere is then activated, turning attention toward the oppo- site side, and, at the same time, inhibiting activation of the other hemisphere. Thus, in a braille learning task, it might be easier initially to establish a left-hemisphere, verbal "set," but once the verbal aspects of the task are mastered, a right-hemisphere, spatial "set" would become effective. Hence, the right-hand-first testing order would be the most advantageous. One way to eliminate the effects of hand testing order is to present letters to the left and right hands in simple alternation, with a different group of letters being presented to each hand. This alternating hand method has been used by Feinberg and by Harris and Wagner (both studies described in Harris et al., 1976c), and signifi- cant left hand advantage was found for right-handed adults. Thus, while the order effect may be important, it is not crucial for the emergence of a left hand advantage. To obtain clearer results, an alternating hand method will be proposed for this dissertation. Effects of age on hand asymmetry for recognition of verbal stimuli. In general, the age of the subject has not been shown to influence the pattern of hand asymmetry for the recognition of Roman letters. In all three studies that included more than one age group, no significant effect of age on hand asymmetry was found in six- to l4-year-olds. In addition, both for the two studies that found a left hand advantage and for the two that found a right hand advantage, 36 the subjects were children in one case and adults in the other. Therefore, both within and between studies, there was no association of age and degree or direction of asymmetry. It should be noted that while sex differences in asymmetry were reported in two instances (Witelson, 1977a; Cioffi & Kandel, 1979), there was no sex by age interaction. Contrarily, the age and sex of subject have been found to be related to the pattern of hand advantage for braille letter learn- ing shown by sighted subjects. The general findings were a later emergence of left hand advantage in females than in males, and a tendency toward right hand superiority in younger children. In the develOpmental study of braille letter learning described in the first section, a definite left hand advantage was found by age nine for males, but not until college age for females (Wagner, 1976). The nine-, ll-, and 14-year-old girls tested by Wagner showed no hand difference. Using a similar paired associate letter learning method with children seven to 14 years of age, Rudel et a1. (1974) found a left hand advantage by age 11 for boys, but only by age 13 for girls. In this case, though, the younger girls showed a right hand advantage. In addition, Rudel, Denckla, and Hirsch (1977) tested children and adults on a task requiring same-different judgments of two braille letters examined sequentially by one hand. Left hand superiority was found by the same ages for boys and girls as in the previous study, but a right hand advantage was found for both nine-year-old girls and boys. 37 These findings could be interpreted to mean that young girls, and maybe even young boys, processed the braille letters as verbal stimuli, and they did so even when the letters did not have to be identified verbally or even have to be known as letters. Thus, when the task involves both spatial and linguistic components, as is thought to be the case for braille letter discrimination, the verbal aspects of the task may be more critical for children below the age of 11 years. Expectations based on past research: comparison of previous studies on visual field and hand asymmetry for verbal material. The findings of visual field differences in the recognition of verbal stimuli seem to contrast sharply with the findings pertaining to hand differences in the recognition of similar material. Among the visual field studies reviewed by White (1972), none reported a LVF superior- ity for the recognition of single letters, and nine of 12 studies reported a RVF superiority. Among the studies of hand differences in the recognition of Roman and braille letters summarized in the last section, nearly all reported a left hand superiority or no hand asymmetry. The question now is whether this different pattern of results for the two modalities is evidence for a systematic rela- tionship between modality and hemisphere specialization. One possible interpretation of the findings is that they do not warrant the assumption that, given similar stimuli, tactual pre- sentation is more likely than visual presentation to induce a right hemisphere advantage. The different patterns of asymmetry for the 38 two modalities instead might be attributed to simple task variables. For instance, it may be more difficult to show an asymmetry in either direction in the tactual modality, so that more complex stimuli would have to be used. This idea is related to LaBreche et a1.'s sugges- tion that their failure to find a hand difference for the recognition of Roman letters indicates that "the task demands associated with each of the letters conditions were not sufficiently complex to tax the spatial and linguistic resources of each hemisphere" (p. 193). This lack of sufficient complexity in the tactual stimuli, then, would explain why four out of eight studies of hand differences, but only three out of 12 comparable visual field studies, failed to find an asymmetry for Roman letter recognition. The "lack of com- plexity" hypothesis might also explain the fragile nature of the left hand advantage for braille letter learning, reflected in the fluctuation of results with such variables as age, sex, and hand testing order, and the fact that a left visual field, but not a left hand, superiority was found for dot enumeration (Myers, 1976). While this interpretation in terms of task variables is possible, there is no apparent explanation as to why tactually presented stimuli should need to be more complex for an asymmetry to be shown. Another possible interpretation of the different pattern of results for the two modalities is that, given similar stimuli, a right hemisphere advantage would be more likely to occur with a tactual presentation method than with a visual presentation method. This hypothesized association between modality and cerebral hemisphere, 39 at least for verbal stimuli, was implicit in Witelson's (1974) explanation for her failure to find a hand difference in the recogni- tion of Roman letters. She suggested that "within the tactual sys- tem, linguistic information is analyzed first in a spatial code and then translated into a linguistic code, with spatial analysis more readily processed in the right hemisphere and linguistic analysis in the left hemisphere" (p. 14). Thus, presentation of letters in the tactual modality increases the spatial nature of the identification task, and therefore induces right hemisphere processing. Witelson's description of letter identification in the tactual system is consis- tent with Hermelin and O'Connor's (1971) description of braille letter recognition as a two-stage coding process. It is difficult to talk about modality differences in the case of braille, however, since no evidence is available on hemisphere specialization for the recognition of braille letters presented visually. An even stronger association between right hemisphere processing and a tactual presentation method seems to be advocated by Rudel et a1. (1977). They suggest that one of the main reasons for the left hand advantage in braille letter recognition is the fact that naive sighted subjects and blind subjects cannot visualize the braille letters. Similarly, they argue that much of the evidence for right hemisphere superiority comes from experiments where vision is excluded. One bit of evidence from the clinical literature might also support this idea of a strong association between the tactual modality and right hemisphere processing. 8011 (1974) administered tests of 40 tactile-perceptual ability, including a form recognition task, to patients with right or left hemisphere brain damage. The results showed that the performance of patients with right hemisphere damage was more impaired on the ipsilateral and contralateral sides of the body than for patients with left hemisphere damage. These findings were interpreted as demonstrating the "pre-eminence" of the right hemi- sphere in subserving tactile perception. Whether or not this almost exclusive association of the tactual modality with right hemisphere superiority is warranted is not clear. However, there does appear to be some tendency for a systematic increase in the need for right hemisphere analysis when the presen- tation modality is tactual instead of visual. Expectations based on cross-modal reliability of laterality, ,tests. A third basis for forming expectations about possible asso- ciations between modality and cerebral hemisphere is evidence con- cerning the correlation of left-right asymmetries found in different modalities. If the asymmetries found in different modalities are highly correlated, it might be predicted that the cerebral hemispheres develop specialization for certain functions independent of the modality in which they are performed. On the other hand, if the asymmetries in different modalities are not even moderately corre- lated, or are negatively correlated, a dissociation of laterality effects in different modalities might be predicted, and a systematic variation in laterality effects across modalities would be unlikely. 41 No data on the correlation of asymmetries in the tactual and visual modalities are available, but several studies have looked at the correlation between asymmetries in the visual and auditory modal- ities. At least four did not find even a moderate positive relation- ship between modalities. Bryden (1965) and Zurif and Bryden (1969) found only weak positive correlations between asymmetries revealed in a series of dichotic listening and visual field tests. Using a measure of concordance for direction of laterality effect in a dichotic listening test and a visual field test for letter recogni- tion, Bryden (1973) found a relationship the reverse of what he had expected. Those students who showed better left ear performance were more likely to show a RVF superiority. Fennell et a1. (1977) also found negative or weak positive correlations between ear and visual field scores within and across testing sessions. In contrast to those studies, at least two studies have suggested a significant moderate correlation between asymmetries in the two modalities for right handers. Hines and Satz (1974) improved upon the method used in some of the previous studies by computing separ— ate correlations for left and right handers, and by using a visual field testing method that minimized the influence of direction-of- reading. They found significant positive correlations between asymmetries in the two modalities for the right-handed subjects (average uncorrected r = .37). In addition, they found that about 63% of the right handers showed concordance for direction of asymmetry in the two modalities. When Fennell et a1. (1977) measured directional concordance rates, instead of correlations between dichotic listening #141114 42 and visual field scores, they found a moderate agreement between the early testing sessions, and a strong (81.4%) agreement between the third and fourth sessions. In light of these two studies, it seems that a moderate correlation between asymmetries might be expected, and therefore, systematic modality differences in asymmetry would be possible. One additional issue to be considered when discussing cross- modal reliability is the type of stimuli presented in the different modalities. First, it seems unreasonable to expect that hemisphere specialization for all types of verbal material is a unitary phenome- non (consider Bryden, 1966). Rather, it seems that during development the left hemisphere would come to be specialized for increasingly complex verbal processing "routines,f and that each of these routines might be lateralized to a different degree. Therefore, since some of the studies mentioned used different types of stimuli for the visual and auditory tests (Fennell et al., 1977; Bryden, 1973), the basis for their expectation of a strong correlation between asymme- tries in the two modalities is unclear. Second, even in the cases where the stimuli presented in the two modalities were similar (Hines & Satz, 1974), there was no indication that the amount and type of information perceived by each modality were subjectively equal. In conclusion, then, failure to find a strong correlation between asymmetries in the visual and auditory modalities might be partly the result of lack of control over the objective and subjective similarity of the stimuli presented in the 43 two modalities. The importance of stimulus factors will be considered in the next section. Stimulus Characteristics Effects of Letter and Typeface Characteristics on the Letter Identification Process The question of main concern in this section is whether or not systematic variation of letters or their typefaces along selected dimensions would be expected to lead to differences in performance on a letter identification test. Since this dissertation is mainly con- cerned with hemisphere differences in letter identification, the three dimensions to be discussed are dimensions that Bryden and Allard (1976) have shown to be related to visual field differences. These dimensions are scriptlikeness, difficulty (naming latency), and internal confusability--the extent to which one item in a typeface would be confused with other items in the same typeface. Typeface selection in the current study will be determined by ratings on these three main characteristics. In addition, ratings will be obtained on two dimensions to be mentioned only briefly in this section-~unfamiliarity and superfluity. Unfamiliarity was chosen because Bryden and Allard found it to be moderately, though non-significantly, correlated with visual field differences. Superfluity was chosen because it shared some of the characterisics of scriptlikeness, but might be more appropriate for the type of lettering used both in Bryden and Allard's study and in the current study. 44 Scriptlikeness. Bryden and Allard evidently used the term "scriptlike" to mean "like handwriting" and that is the way it will be used in the current study. Evidence that this definition of scriptlikeness may be associated with differences in the letter identification process comes from a study by Corcoran and Rouse (1970). These authors suggested that the perceptual system is organized into two "subroutines," one for printing (typing) and one for handwriting. They designed lists of words that were copied in four different ways: (1) lower-case typed letters (TL); (2) upper-case typed letters (TU); (3) handwritten by person 1 (H1); and (4) handwritten by person 2 (H2). These words were then assigned to sets that were either unmixed (all from one list) or mixed (half from one list and half from another). Subjects were shown tachistosc0pically projected words from a given set and asked to orally identify them. The data showed that when the mixed set contained H1 and TL words, the mean probability of recognizing the words on the first presentation was significantly lower for the mixed set than for the respective ummixed sets. How- ever, when the mixed set contained either H1 and H2 words, or TL and TU words, there was no difference between the mixed and respective unmixed sets in the mean probability of correct word identification. Thus, Corcoran and Rouse hypothesized that the lower accuracy for sets that contained both typed and handwritten words was caused by the subject's inability to switch to the appropriate processing sub- routine before the stimulus was presented. Given this evidence, then, it would be nice to equate the more scriptlike typefaces used by Bryden and Allard with cursive 45 handwriting. However, it should be noted that, technically and his- torically, cursive script differs in many respects from the more formal script letters used by Bryden and Allard. The term "script" technically means any handwritten letter or character. Formal script is the polished, calligraphic type of writing system that can be traced back at least as far as the ideograms and phonetic symbols used in ancient Sumeria around 3100 B.C. Contrarily, cursive script can be defined as signs and styles of writing changed in form by everyday use and quickly executed construction (Fairbank, 1970). Two styles of writing apparent in ancient Egypt can be related to the formal and cursive scripts (Fairbank, 1970). One style, hieratic writing, derived from Egyptian hieroglyphics, which were characters that represented formalized pictures, and also words, syllables, and consonants. The hieratic style was used by priests during the first dynasty (around 2900 B.C.). A second style, demotic writing, was characterized by a rapid, fluent quality which departed greatly from the pictorial quality of the hieroglyphs. This demotic style has not been traced as existing earlier than the seventh cen— tury B.C., and probably developed because of the need to write with speed, and consequently to simplify, when writing informally. This contrast between the formal and cursive styles was also apparent in Roman times (Delpire & Monory, 1961). The bold, polished Roman rustic and unical lettering styles developed at the same time that cursive writing was becoming more popular, possibly because the pen was able to slide on the surface of papyrus. Delpire and Monory 46 noted that "as beautiful as they were legible, the Roman capitals gratify the mind and the eye" (p. 74). Then, in an aside, they noted that "one might assume that capitals in cursive script fatigue the eye" (p. 77). The contrast between formal and cursive scripts was also evident in their observation that "while someone who knows Latin can without much study successfully grapple with rustic or unical inscriptions, he risks being baffled for a long time, if not for good, with the miniscule (lower-case) cursive" (p. 77). In light of these historical and technical observations, if Bryden and Allard's term "scriptlike" means "like cursive writing," the characteristics of the scriptlike typefaces ought to include: (1) a rapid, fluent QUality; (2) possibly more loops and cirlicues; (3) extensions of the letters at the beginning and end, as if to be 4 and (4) an irregular, even sloppy, qual- joined with other letters; ity reflecting the distinctiveness (Gordon & Mock, 1960) and sometimes sloppiness of adult hands. The typefaces used by Bryden and Allard instead are in a precise, formal style. Therefore, variance along a dimension of cursive handwiritng might not be meaningful. One suggestion for a related dimension that might be more appro- priate for Byden and Allard's distinct, precise letters is superfluity, or the degree to which the letter varies from some impression of the "ideal" letter. This dimension could be operationally defined in terms of the number of extra lines, loops, and curlicues added to the basic form of the letter and in terms of the tilt and thickness of the line. To assess the importance of this related dimension, the 47 typefaces in the current study will be rated on both scriptlikeness and superfluity. Difficulty. The dimension of difficulty also may be important. If this dimension is measured by naming latency (as in Bryden and Allard'sstudy), it probably would be highly related to the dimension of deviation from an ideal. The greater the deviation, the more time it would take to match the perceived letter to the image in memory. When considering the factors that make a letter difficult to recog- nize, both the characteristics of the letter itself and the character- istics of the typeface appear to be significant. Evidence for a marked variation in the difficulty of perceiving particular letters presented alone was found by Budohoska, Grabowska, and Jablonowska (1975). They asked adults to identify printed Roman alphabet letters presented at a central fixation point for 17 msec. each. The percent of errors ranged from about 60% for T, I, and L, to less than 10% for D and 0. A large variation in the difficulty of recognizing the same letter typed in different scripts also has been shown. Bryden and Allard found that mean recognition times for their different typefaces ranged from 671 msec. to 1183 msec. Reasons for the greater diffi- culty of some typefaces might be simple explanations based on the clarity of the script or the amount of black and white contrast pro- vided, or they might be more complex explanations based on factors such as amount of deviation from an ideal. In addition, it might be expected that letter and typeface variables would interact with each other to determine difficulty. Some letters, then, would be more 48 difficult to recognize in certain typefaces than in others. For instance, if a given letter was changed in one script by the addition of a loop to the letter's upper part, the most crucial part for letter recognition (Fairbank, 1970, p. 76), the letter might be relatively more difficult to recognize in that typeface than in another where it was changed only slightly or only in the lower part. Since sensory modality is a primary variable in this disserta- tion, the possible interaction of modality with these dimensions of letter and typeface difficulty should also be noted. Moderate corre- lations between visual and tactual presentations have been found for recognition accuracies of letters (Loomis & Apkarian-Stielau, 1976; Craig, 1979) and forms (Birch & Lefford, 1963). Thus it could be expected that relative letter difficulties would be similar, though not identical, in the two modalities. Since there is evidence for a modality difference in the relative ease of line to curve and orientation transformations, typefaces emphasizing orientation changes from the basic form should be relatively easier with tactual presenta- tion, and those emphasizing curvature changes should be relatively easier with visual presentation. Again, the factors of letter and typeface difficulty may interact. For instance, while Goodnow (1969) found that orientation changes yielded a strong sense of difference from most tactually presented standards, this was not true for all tactually presented standards. This finding was presumably related to whether or not the focal point of a particular standard was changed by an orientation transformation. Thus, the modality of presentation, 49 the difficulty of recognizing a given letter, the difficulty of the typeface, and the two- and three-way interactions among these factors are important to consider. Confusability. Finally, the letter identification process might be influenced by the degree to which the letters in a given typeface are likely to be confused with each other. Again, this dimension is highly related to the others, and especially to difficulty, since confusability is likely to be a main contributor to increased iden- tification times. It seems that a certain degree of confusability is inherent in the nature of typefaces (or scripts). As Fairbank (1947) stated, "family relationships and homogeneity give harmony and read- ableness to the scripts" (p. 17). Letters in any typeface tend to fit into regular family groups, such as those that wholly enclose space within them (A,B,D), and others that have spaces only partly enclosed (C,F,U,V), or those that are either all curved orafll straight. Gibson et a1. (1962) have shown that letterlike forms with some of these family characteristics, such as symmetry, possession of a closed loop, and straightness, are less likely to be confused with forms that are close variants of them than are letter-like forms with the oppo- site family characteristics. In addition, confusability within each family group should be greater than confusability between groups. Thus, it can be predicted that the most confusable typefaces should be those with fewer family groups, more homogeneity within groups and less variance between groups, and a relatively large number of members of the less discriminable groups. 50 Several formalized methods of measuring interletter similarity were compared by Holbrook (1975). These methods included: (1) The Luce-Choice-model Similarity Measure--a model computed from a tachis- toscopic confusion matrix; (2) A Subjective Rating Measure--based on subject's judgments of similarity of letter pairs on a lO-point scale; (3) The Distinctive Feature Measure--a model that considers the number of distinctive features shared by two letters; and (4) The Optimal Distinctive Features Measure--similar to (3) except that each feature is weighted to give an optimal least-squares fit in a regression analysis. The predictions generated by (l) and (2) tended to be very close, and they both were similar to the prediction of a mechanical measure based on the physical overlap of each pair. The distinctive feature measures only moderately correlated with (l). The strength of the correlations improved somewhat when the optimal weights were added, but they still were poorer than the correlations among the other measures. In conclusion, this study provides evi- dence that a simple subjective rating measure might be a fairly reliable method of determining confusability for a given typeface. In summary, these three dimensions could all reasonably be expected to have an influence on the letter identification process. Scriptlikeness may be particularly important because there is evi- dence to show that handwriting and printed material may be processed by different "sub-routines." 51 Relationship of Stimulus Character- istics and Hemisphere Specializa- tion The most direct evidence for a relationship between letter type- face characteristics and hemisphere specialization is provided by Bryden and Allard's study of the effect of typeface on visual hemi- field differences for letter identification. As described in the first section, a typeface by visual field interaction was found. 0f the 10 typefaces, five were recognized better in the RVF (three, significantly better), two were recognized about equally well in both visual fields, and three were recognized better in the LVF (two, significantly better). To verify the stability of the LVF advantage for certain typefaces, Bryden and Allard carried out a second experi- ment similar to the first, except that individual subjects were shown just one typeface. Only the two typefaces that had yielded a signifi- cant LVF advantage in the first experiment were used. Again, these typefaces were recognized more accurately in the LVF, although the visual field difference was not significant in one case. Thus, the LVF advantage for these typefaces was shown to be stable and intrin- sic to the lettering style. In a third experiment, Bryden and Allard investigated the rela- tionship between different typeface characteristics and the visual field asymmetries from Experiment 1. They asked seven additional subjects to rank order the 10 different typefaces on the dimensions of familiarity, scriptlike vs. printlike, and internal confusability. To obtain a measure of the relative difficulty of naming letters in 52 the different typefaces, two of the seven subjects and four additional ones were presented with the stimulus cards from Experiment 1 and were asked to name each letter as quickly as possible. The average naming latency and average ranks on the other three dimensions were recorded for each typeface. Rank-order correlations and product-moment correlations between the visual field asymmetries and each of these dimensions, except familiarity, were significant. A LVF advantage was associated with greater scriptlikeness, internal confusability, and difficulty and, moderately, with less familiarity. Thus, in general, these dimensions do seem to have an effect on the letter identification process, as measured globally by relative hemisphere advantage for letter recog- nition. Since most of the dimensions were significantly correlated with visual field asymmetry, and, it appears, with each other, it is difficult to estimate which particular characteristic or combination of characteristics contributed most to the difference in hemisphere advantage. The rank-order correlation was slightly greater for naming latency than for other dimensions, while the product-moment correlation was highest for the scriptlikeness dimension. Multiple correlation procedures revealed that little was added to the predictability of laterality scores by supplementing the scriptlikeness measure, but, again, this could be due to the fact that the dimensions were highly correlated with each other in these particular typefaces. It should be noted that the rank-order and product-moment corre- lations are in the direction predicted by studies of visual field and hand asymmetry for discrimination of shapes and line orientations. 53 Simple geometrical forms (Bryden & Rainey, 1963) and lines differing in orientation by at least 45 degrees (White, 1971) have been found to be discriminated more accurately in the RVF. It would seem that printed letters, also better recognized in the RVF, could be broken down into these simple lines and shapes. Contrarily, more complex forms (Witelson, 1974) and lines close in orientation (Umilta et al., 1974) are better recognized when presented to the left hand or LVF. Scriptlike letters, also better recognized in the LVF, may be thought to be composed of complex shapes, with many loops, and lines differ- ing slightly from the vertical or horizontal. In addition, a LVF advantage for the perception of depth has been shown (Durnford & Kimura, 1970), and some scriptlike typefaces are designed to appear three-dimensional. Thus, the three-dimensionality of the typeface may be another characteristic to consider. The correlations between typeface dimensions and visual field asymmetries also are consistent with the results of Faglioni, Scotti, and Spinnler's (1969) tests of letter recognition ability in patients with unilateral hemispheric damage. The patients were given four letter identification subtests in which they were shown a letter and had to find it among a series of alternative letters printed with a different type. For all four subtests, the multiple choice letters were printed in a convential form, either block-printed capitals or simple italic small letters. The test letters were printed in a conventional form for Subtests A and B, were incomplete for Subtest C, and were partially hidden, or crossed out, for Subtest D. 54 0f the posterior brain-damaged patients, those with left hemi- sphere damage were specifically impaired on Subtests A and B, and those with right hemisphere damage were specifically impaired on Subtests C and 0, when the letters were presented in a perceptually difficult way. Faglioni et al., concluded that the contribution of the right hemisphere to the letter identification process becomes critical when the letters are presented in a perceptually complicated form. These perceptually complicated forms are comparable to the more scriptlike, internally confusable, and difficult typefaces used by Bryden and Allard. The partially crossed-out letters (Subtest D) are especially analogous to Bryden and Allard's very scriptlike letters, which typically had several extra loops or lines. Finally, given that the relative contribution of the two hemi- spheres to the letter identification process changes when typeface characteristics change, it should be asked how this finding increases understanding of hemisphere differences and of letter identification. In regard to hemisphere differences, the finding that letters are not always more accurately identified in the RVF is inconsistent with a characterization of the hemispheres according to the general type of material they are specialized for dealing with (e.g., verbal vs. spatial). Instead, this finding suggests a characterization of the hemispheres in terms of types of processing (Bryden & Allard). Thus, even verbal material can be better recognized in the LVF if it is complex enough to need much of the preprocessing which can be done more efficiently by the parallel processing (Cohen, 1973), global 55 analysis (Levy, 1969), or diffuse organization (Semmes, 1968) of the right hemisphere. This dissertation will investigate the specific typeface char- acteristics that are associated with greater right hemisphere partici- pation. One purpose of this investigation is a further refinement of the characterization of the processing for which each hemisphere is specialized. This dissertation also will investigate whether and to what extent modality of presentation affects the balance of hemi- spheric processing as typeface complexity varies. It is expected that, contrary to the assumption that hemispheric advantages are independent of modality, the range of processing that can be employed may be limited by the modality in which the information is received. The finding that visual field asymmetries are associated with typeface characteristics also has implications for our understanding of the letter identification process. It suggests that when the letter cannot be immediately matched to the template or distinctive features stored as part of the letter and word identification system, then a process that is partly separate and partly integrated with this system can be employed. This process acts to break down (or normalize) the letter so that its distinctive characteristics can be attended to easily. This suggestion that letter identification involves both left and right hemisphere processes is consistent with the conclusions of Kershner (1975) and Pirozzolo and Rayner (1977). In a review of reading and laterality, Kershner (1975) concluded that the perception 56 of single letters is better when both hemispheres are involved. To investigate the relative contribution of the hemispheres to the word recognition process, Pirozzolo and Rayner (1977) presented four- 1etter words either bilaterally or unilaterally and had the subjects select their response from four choices shown on a response card. More errors occurred for words presented in the LVF, but this effect was especially strong when the errors were visually similar words. Thus, Pirozzolo and Rayner concluded that, in addition to single letter perception, the reading of whole words is also a multi-stage process, with visual feature analysis carried out by the right hemi- sphere and identification and naming by the left hemisphere. A comparison of different age groups in this dissertation may have implications for assessing developmental changes in the relative importance of these two processes for letter identification. 0n the one hand, since children below the age of 11 years have shown a right hand advantage for braille letter learning, and since their RVF scores seem to be more critical for determining reading ability, it might be that they will not employ the right hemisphere normalization process as often as an adult, but will concentrate instead on the verbal aspects of the task. On the other hand, as hypothesized, since even the moderately scriptlike typefaces will be relatively more complex for children than for adults, children might need to employ a greater degree of right hemisphere preprocessing than adults for those type- faces. In summary, Bryden and Allard found that the dimensions of type- face scriptlikeness, difficulty (naming latency). and confusability (:1. -1. 1 GI 57 were strongly related to visual field asymmetries for letter iden- tification. The relationship between these variables is in the direction predicted by studies of hand and visual field differences for the discrimination of shapes or line orientation and by studies of patients with unilateral hemispheric damage. Bryden and Allard's finding has many implications for the nature of the difference between the hemispheres and for the letter identification process. The purpose of this dissertation is to further understanding of these mechanisms by examining the variables of modality of presentation and age and their interaction with typeface characteristics. Summary of Hypotheses Modality of Presentation Difference in direction of asymmetry. Based on the comparison of previous findings pertaining to hand and visual field asymmetries for letter identification, and on the intuitive idea that tactual perception involves more "spatial integration," the tactual modality seems to have a stronger association with right hemisphere processing than does the visual modality. Therefore, it is hypothesized that a greater right hemisphere (left field) advantage will be shown in the tactual modality than in the visual modality (see Figure l). The direction of this modality difference is expected to be constant across levels of typeface complexity. The magnitude of the difference, however, might be smaller for the more complex typefaces because the degree of right hemisphere processing required for those typefaces might be large in both modality conditions. 58 Cross-modal correlations. It is hypothesized that, for a given typeface, the correlation between individual asymmetries in the visual and tactual conditions will be positive and (at least) moder— ately strong. This prediction is based on the assumptions that: (1) an individual will tend to use a strategy involving more or less right hemisphere processing compared to other individuals regardless of stimulus modality, and (2) hemisphere specialization is based on type of processing, regardless of sensory modality. Stimulus Characteristics Interaction of typeface with hand and visual field. Based in part on Bryden and Allard's findings, an interaction of typeface with sensory field is expected. Specifically, LVF and left hand scores should increase relative to RVF and right hand scores as overall com- plexity of the typeface is increased. In the visual condition, it is hypothesized that there will be a significant RVF advantage for the simplest, printlike typefaces and a significant LVF advantage for the more complex, scriptlike type- faces (as was found by Bryden and Allard). In the tactual condition, it is hypothesized that there will be little or no right hand advan- tage for the recognition of plain, block-printed letters (as was found by Witelson, 1974; LaBreche et al., 1977). A significant left hand advantage is expected for the recognition of the more complex, scriptlike letters. This prediction is consistent with the left hand superiority found for learning complex braille letters (e.g., Wagner, 1976). 59 Relative contribution of specific typeface characteristics. If it is possible to select typefaces for which the characteristics of scriptlikeness, difficulty, and confusability are not highly corre- lated, it is hypothesized that each of these characteristics will be significantly correlated with visual field and hand asymmetries and that their effect will be additive. In addition, it is predicted that scriptlikeness will be the strongest predictor of visual field asymmetries, as was tentatively concluded by Bryden and Allard. Gregg Greater right hemisphere participation in the tactual condition than in the visual condition is predicted for all grades. Increasing right hemisphere participation with typeface complexity also is pre- dicted for all grades, but the general level of right hemisphere participation is expected to differ across grades. Specifically, the younger grades should show evidence of more right hemisphere partici- pation than the college students, particularly for the moderately complex typefaces. This prediction of a grade difference is supported by two points. First, developmental studies of visual field asymmetries for verbal material (in simple print) have found either a constant degree of RVF advantage across age (e.g., Olson, 1973; Marcel & Rajan, 1975) or no significant RVF advantage for the youngest ages tested (e.g., For- gays, 1953; Tomlinson—Keasey et al., 1978). Second, right hemisphere participation has been found to increase with increasing typeface difficulty and unfamiliarity. Since the non-printlike typefaces 60 (especially the moderately complex ones) should be relatively more difficult and unfamiliar for the younger grades, greater right hemi- sphere participation is predicted for those grades. METHOD: PART I--TYPEFACE SCALING Scaling for "Difficulty" Subjects The subjects were 20 college students--10 males and 10 females-- with no reported vision problems. They participated for credit in their Introductory Psychology classes at Michigan State University. Apparatus A 3-channel Scientific Prototype Auto-tachistosc0pe (Model G8) with a split-beam binocular eyepiece was used. Each field was back- lit through the diffusion screen by two standard fluorescent bulbs (GE-FT4-5, with onset time reportedly much less than 1 msec.). Only two fields were used--one for the letter stimuli and one for the fixation point. The fields were set at the same moderate brightness level, and both were modified to obtain more equal left-right half field brightness by inserting a matte white strip of cardboard behind each bulb pair. For the stimulus field, 35mm slides were automati- cally fed from lOO-slide rototrays. For the fixation field, a stationary slot held one slide. The timers for both fields were set at 1.5 msec. The tachistoscope was wired so that an on-off switch used by the experimenter triggered the onset of the fixation field. Fixation field offset triggered the onset of the stimulus field, and stimulus 61 62 field offset then triggered the advancement of the rototray. The same switch simultaneously started a reaction timer (Klockcounter), which was stopped by a voice activated relay (VAR) when the subject's response was spoken into a microphone. The "delay" button on the VAR was set to 12 seconds to allow the experimenter to record response time and the response before the relay could be activated again. Materials A sample of 22 different typefaces was selected from those avail- able in 60-point Chartpak Velvet Touch transfer lettering (black). The goal of the selection was to obtain as broad a sample of letter- ing styles as possible, based on the judgment of the experimenter. Selection was made with regard to such characteristics as letter slant and thickness, presence of serifs, and amount tyf detail, as well as the five general characteristics on which the typefaces were to be scaled. For each typeface, the following eight letters were used--C, G, 0, Q, B, R, F, P. They were chosen for two main reasons. First, the letters could be divided into two groups on the basis of physical similarity. The first four letters were circular or nearly circular. In contrast, the second four letters had a vertical line on the left border, with a curved or straight line extending to the right at its midpoint.5 Since it was hypothesized that letter characteristics might affect field differences, the selection of letters that could be grouped according to certain primary characteristics was designed to aid in determining which general characteristics were important. 63 Second, all the letters also were used by Bryden and Allard. There- fore, if the results of the current study differed from Bryden and Allard's findings, the discrepancy could not be attributed to spe- cific letter differences. Slides of the letter stimuli were made as follows. Each letter was transferred onto the center of a 7.6 cm. square piece of white poster board with matte finish. The typefaces and letters varied slightly in size. Heights ranged from about 1.6 cm. to 2.3 cm. and widths ranged from about 1.1 cm. to 2.1 cm. The small squares then were placed one at a time in the center of a large piece of the same poster board and photographed using Kodak Ektachrome film (ASA 160). Standard 35mm slides were developed and mounted commercially. Each slide was examined to check that the letter was centered on the slide, and if not centered to the nearest .5 mm., the slide was remounted by hand. Each slide thus contained a single black letter in the center surrounded by a thin black outline of a square. In terms of visual angle subtended when viewed through the tachistoscope, letter heights ranged from .31 to .50 degrees and widths ranged from .21 to .42 degrees. A fixation slide was made under the same conditions as the letter slides. There was a black dot in the center of the small square of poster board and a small black bracket in each of the four corners of the background poster board. When viewed through the tachistoscope, the diameter of the dot subtended an angle of .18 degrees. The area within the brackets represented the unoccluded 64 part of any stimulus field and permitted the subject to make minor self-centering head adjustments before each trial. This viewing area had an "apparent" viewing distance of 76.2 cm. and projected a visual angle of about 2.9 (height) by 5.25 degrees. Procedure Subjects were tested individually in a small laboratory room, where they were seated facing the tachistoscope with their backs to the experimenter. The sequence for each trial was described to them. They were told that upon hearing the click of the experimenter's switch, they should look into the tachistosc0pe directly at the center dot, while making any minor head adjustments necessary so that all four reference brackets could be seen. After about 1.5 secs., the dot would be replaced by a single capital letter in one of various styles, which they then should identify as quickly and accurately as possible. Before the experiment began, the room lights were turned off, and subjects were given a brief period to adapt to the brightness level of the fixation field. They also were asked to adjust the interocular separation of the eyepieces so that they had a clear view of the fixation field with both eyes, and they practiced saying letter names loudly and clearly into the microphone. There were 176 trials in all, with a rest period of about five minutes after half the trials. Stimulus order was random with the following constraints: (1) no letter or typeface was presented on two consecutive trials, and (2) within a group of 44 trials, each 65 typeface was presented twice and each of the eight letters was pre- sented at least five times. The 176 slides were divided into two rototrays of 88 slides each. The order of the rototrays was switched for half of the subjects of each sex. For each subject, a difficulty rating for a given typeface was determined by taking the median naming latency of the eight letters presented in that typeface. A trial was excluded from the computation of the median (and not replaced) if the response was incorrect (x = 7.3%, o - 40.6%), exceeded 1.5 secs. (x = 0.7%, O-5.6%), or could not be measured due to equipment problems (I = 0.4%, O-l.9%).6 If the typeface with the highest error rate was not included, incorrect responses averaged 5.7% (ranging from O - 14.4%), and responses over 1.5 secs. averaged 0.5% (ranging from O - 2.5%). Scaling for Scriptlikeness, Confusability, Unfamiliarity, and Superfluity Subjects The subjects were 42 college students who participated for credit in their Introductory Psychology classes. There were 35 females and eight males. Materials The eight small squares of poster board containing the letters of each typeface were bunched together to make two rows of four over- lapping cards (with the letters in the same order for every typeface). Five photocopies were made for each typeface. These sheets were then cut and mounted on poster board to serve as cards for the subjects to sort into piles. 66 Signs were made to label the endpoints of the four scales. They were as follows: 1 . . . . . . . . . . . . 10 most familiar least familiar (least typically seen or thought of) 1 . . . . . . . . . . . 10 most printlike most scriptlike (like handwriting) l . . . . . . . . . . . . 10 letters not easily letters most con- confusable fusable l . . . . . . . . . . . . 10 most basic form of the letter farthest removed from (no extra lines or curves) basic form The highest value on each scale was assigned to the end of the con- tinuum expected to be associated with the highest degree of right hemisphere participation. Procedure Subjects were tested in small groups of up to five peOple. They sat at desks widely spaced around a large room and facing the wall. On each desk was a pile of the 22 typeface cards in a random order, the numbers one to ten placed in order, and the labels for the first scale for that group placed above the numbers one and ten. Subjects were instructed to sort the cards into ten ordered and equally spaced groups to represent levels of the characteristic being measured. There was no requirement pertaining to the distribution of the cards in the ten groups, and subjects were allowed to change their place- ments at any time during the sorting. 67 Sorting was individually paced. As soon as any subject finished sorting for one scale (usually after 10 to 15 min.) his responses were recorded, the cards reshuffled, and the next characteristic explained to him. A different randomly selected order of the char- acteristics was used for each small group tested. For each char- acteristic, the median group placement for a given typeface was used as the rating for that typeface. METHOD: PART II--MEASUREMENT OF FIELD DIFFERENCES Visual and Tactual Conditions Subjects The subjects were 48 fourth- and ninth-graders and college students--16 in each grade group. All were male, right-handed,7 and had no reported uncorrected vision problems. The mean ages for the groups, in order, were as follows: 9 yrs., 5 mos. (8 yrs., 11 mos. to 9 yrs., 11 mos.); 14 yrs., 5 mos. (13 yrs., 10 mos. to 15 yrs.); and 20 yrs., 4 mos. (18 yrs., 8 mos. to 27 yrs., 2 mos.). Since the children were tested in the summer or early fall, they were selected on the basis of the grade they were going to enter or had just entered, not on the basis of age or grade just completed. Most fourth- and ninth-grade subjects were students in a pre- dominantly middle class, suburban community.8 They were recruited through their schools in the spring and early fall and through a recreational program during the summer. Letters describing the study were sent home with them, and parents were asked to return a postcard giving their name and phone number if they and their child were inter- ested in the study. The children were paid $3 for their partici- pation. 68 69 College students again were Introductory Psychology students who received course credit for their participation. Handedness for fourth-graders was determined by noting the hand used when the children were asked to perform five common actions (Annett, 1970a). A child was considered right-handed if he performed at least four actions with the right hand. For ninth-graders and college students, handedness was deter- mined by responses to a 12-item handedness questionnaire developed by Annett (1970b) and modified (following Briggs & Nebes, 1975) to include five response categories to distinguish between whether a hand was "always" or "usually" used. To be considered right-handed, it was necessary to "always" write with the right hand and to perform three of the five other primary tasks at least "usually" with the right hand.9 Numerical scores. were computed by assigning number values to response categories, ranging from + 2 for "always" right to - 2 for "always" left. The scores for all subjects were positive. The mean score for college students (I a 17.2, s.d. = 5.5) was not significantly different from the mean for ninth-graders (I = 19.7,s.d. = 3.4; t = -l.51, d.f. = 30, N.S.). The presence of familial sinistrality (FS+) was assessed roughly by asking the subjects (for fourth-graders, the subjects' parents) whether a biological parent or sibling was left-handed (i.e., wrote with the left hand). Seven fourth-graders, eight ninth-graders, and one college student were classified as FS+. 70 Apparatus Visual condition. The tachistosc0pe apparatus used in this part of the experiment was the same as that described for "difficulty" scaling, except for four changes. First, the time setting for the stimulus field was variable. Second, the tachistoscope was triggered by a momentary push-button switch operated by the subject. Third, the offset of the stimulus field triggered the onset of the blank third field (for 1.5 secs.), the offset of which then initiated the advancement of the rototray. This additional step was needed to give the subjects time to respond before the noisy rototray movement. Finally, response time was not measured. Tactual condition. The subject and experimenter sat opposite each other at a small table. Between them was a large cardboard box, with the side facing the subject cut out. There were two holes in the bottom of the box through which the subject placed his hands. A card- board shelf was placed over the holes on the subject's side to ensure that the subject could see neither his hands nor the letters. A block of wood containing two metal card-holders about .5 cm. apart was used to present the stimuli. The card-holders (as well as the holes in the box) were placed close together to reduce the possi- bility that attention directed to the left or right of body midline would influence hand differences. Materials Visual condition. For the visual condition, 144 stimulus slides were used--l6 for each of eight test typefaces and one practice 71 typeface selected in Part I. For every typeface, each of the eight letters occurred once iri the LVF and once in the RVF. Photographs were taken under the same general conditions as those used for "diffi- culty" scaling. The small squares of poster board with letters in the center were placed one at a time either to the left or right of the center point of the background board. Positioned at the center point was a number from one to nine (excluding seven),10 also on a 7.6 cm. square of poster board. For the eight test typefaces, each number was associated with a particular letter twice, once with the letter in each visual field. The numbers were randomly assigned to letters for the practice typeface. All slides were checked to see that the number was in the center of the slide; if not centered to within l.5mm., the slide was remounted. As viewed through the tachistosc0pe, each stimulus slide con- tained a black number (in simple print) in the center outlined by a thin square, with a single letter to the left or right also outlined by a thin square. The center of each lateralized letter was 2.23 degrees from the center of the number. The maximum and minimum visual angles projected by the lateralized letters were nearly equal to those of the letters used for "difficulty" scaling (averaging about .01 degrees less).11 The fixation slide was the same as that used for "difficulty" scaling. Tactual condition. For the tactual condition, 32 letters were used--eight for each of three test typefaces and one practice typeface selected in Part I. The letters were raised surfaces of zinc plates 72 made from photoc0pies of the letters by a process used commercially to make rubber stamps. All black parts of the letters were raised 1 mm. from the plate, and the edges were beveled. The range of letter sizes was about the same as the range for the original trans- fer lettering. The plates (about 3.7 cm. square) for individual letters were glued onto the center of ordinary plastic playing cards, which could be slipped into the card-holders. Procedure Visual condition. Subjects (tested individually) were seated facing the tachistosc0pe, with the experimenter sitting to their right, opposite the control panel. A large board was placed between the subject and experimenter to block out direct light from the table lamp used by the experimenter. Subjects were told that the general purpose of the experiment was to see how well people could recognize letters of different styles presented for brief times. The instruc- tions were as follows: When you press the button, you first will see a slide with a dot in the middle and a bracket in each of the four corners. You should look directly at the dot and position your head to make sure you can see the four brackets out of the corners of your eyes. After about a second the slide will change. Then you will see a number (from 1 to 9) in the middle (about where the dot was) and a letter to either the right or left side of the number. It is important that you keep looking straight ahead so you will be looking right at the number when it appears. Your response should be the number and then the letter. For example . . . Subjects were encouraged to guess at both the number and letter if they were in doubt, but were told to say just the number if they were 73 unable even to guess the identity of the letter. The experimenter emphasized the importance of looking straight at the fixation dot and getting the number correct. The experimenter gave four additional clarifications of the stimuli and task. The first three were given for the first modality condition, whether it was visual or tactual, and the fourth was given for the second modality condition. The experimenter told the sub- jects that: (1) because of the nature of a threshold experiment, mistakes were expected (This clarification was needed to reduce frus- tration and anxiety about doing badly on the tesk.); (2) all letters would be capitals and about the same size; (3) a capital cursive Q could resemble the number two, and a cpaital cursive G could be shaped like the lower case letter, but larger in size. (Pilot testing indi- cated that these letters sometimes caused confusion. As an added precaution, fourth-graders were shown a photocopy of all eight letters in the practice style and asked to name them. After being reminded about the Q and G, every fourth-grader named all letters correctly.); (4) at least some of the lettering styles had been presented in the first modality condition. There were 288 trials in all--32 trials in each of eight test typefaces and one practice typeface. The l6 stimulus slides for each typeface were presented in a row, and then the rototray was reset and the 16 slides presented again in the same order. The practice typeface was presented first to provide practice and to establish a presentation time that served as a starting point for the 12 test typefaces. Subjects always were told when the typeface would 74 change. There was a rest period (5 to 7 min.) after the fourth test typeface, and subjects were told to ask for a brief rest (about l min.) after a typeface was completed if they were tired. These extra rest periods were noted on the data sheets. Since the difficulty of the task varied greatly and, according to pilot testing, nonuniformly across typefaces and also across grades, it was not possible to simply determine an appropriate exposure dura- tion for all test typefaces. Therefore, it was decided to obtain an approximate threshold presentation duration for each field and to use that as the dependent measure. Thresholds were obtained by the random double staircase-method (Cornsweet, 1962). This method was chosen because it is highly efficient (relatively few trials need be presented) and it conceals the contingencies involved in varying the presentation time. The staircase procedure consisted of decreasing the presentation duration by a predetermined step following a correct response and increasing the duration following an incorrect response. Separate staircases were formed for each field, with trials being randomly alternated between the two. The staircases for both fields were started at the point determined during practice. The steps (in msecs.) were l80, l50, 130, 110, 90, 70, so, 30, 20, 15, 10.13 For presentation time to be decreased, both the letter and number had to be identified correctly. If the number was incorrect, but the letter correct, the trial was eliminated and the presentation duration remained the same for the following trial in that field. 75 It was necessary to determine the orders of three factors-- modality conditions, typefaces, and letters. First, the order of modality conditions was varied so that half the subjects of each grade received the visual condition first (VT) and half received the tactual first (TV). Testing always was done in one session lasting about one hour and 45 minutes, with about a five-minute break between modality conditions. Second, eight typeface orders were determined. Each order was used twice in each grade, once for each modality-order group. The orders were random, with the constraints that each typeface would occur once in every position and, to control for the possibility that the immediately preceding typeface established a set, no typeface would follow another more than twice. Finally, the order of the l6 stimulus slides in each typeface was determined separately for every typeface. The orders were random, with the following constraints: (l) no more than three trials in a row to one field, (2) no letter or number twice in a row, and (3) the number of trials on which the field switched equal to the number of trials on which it remained the same. These primary letter orders (A) were assigned to the typefaces in four of the typeface orders. The arrangementtrfletters assigned to the RVF was designated Set l, and the arrangement assigned to the LVF was designated Set 2. For the other four typeface orders, Set l was assigned to the LVF and Set 2 to the RVF (B). 76 Tactual condition. The subjects were seated Opposite the experi- menter facing a large box. They were shown one of the tactual practice letters to give them an idea of the letter size and of the fact that the zinc plate was not part of the letter. The instructions were as follows: Place your hands through the two holes in the box and rest them on these card-holders. Hold them in the same position in-between letter presentations. I will slide a card under either your right or left hand (in random order). When I say "go" and push your fingers down, start to feel the letter with your index and middle fingers. You will have varying amounts of time to feel each letter (l to l5 secs.). When the time is up, I will remove the card and say "answer," and you should make the best guess you can. There were l04 trials in all--eight trials in the practice type- face and 32 trials in each of the three test typefaces. The practice letters were presented first for 12 seconds each. Four letters were presented to each hand, with the letters assigned to hand switched for half of the subjects of each grade. Fewer trials were used in the tactual condition than in the visual condition because of time limi- tations and because it was less important to determine a starting point for the test typefaces. Following practice, the three test typefaces were presented, with brief rest periods (1 to 2 min.) in—between. A staircase procedure was used to determine a rough threshold presentation duration for each hand. llmeprocedure differed from that described for the visual condition only in that presentation times were longer and fewer steps were used. The steps (in secs.) were l6, l2, 8, 4, 2, l. For all subjects, the starting point for each hand was l2 seconds for every typeface. 77 As in the visual condition, eight typeface orders were deter- mined. They were the six possible order51fiithe three typefaces, with two of them (randomly selected) repeated. Each order was used twice in each grade, once for each modality-order group, and always was paired with the same visual typeface order. The letter order for each typeface was the same as the letter order used for that type- face in the visual condition. Computation of Laterality Coeffi- cients and’their Stability/ Effectiveness For each field in each typeface, a threshold measure was compu- ted in two steps. First, to lessen the effects of the starting point, the first three runs were eliminated from the measure. A run was defined as a series of increasing or decreasing times including the reversal point between it and the preceding run, but not the reversal point following it. Second, the median of the remaining points was computed without interpolation and was used as the threshold measure. All points were included in the measure whether they represented cor- rect or incorrect letter identifications, except those representing correct identifications for which the fixation number was incorrect.14 Interpolation was not considered justified because of the rank order nature of the data. Thus, if a median occurred directly on a time step, that step was taken as the median, regardless of the number of points that occurred on that step. If a median occurred between two steps, it was defined as the average of those steps. 78 Laterality coefficients (LCs) to be used as the dependent vari- able in the main analyses were computed by subtracting the right field threshold measure from the left field measure and then dividing that difference by the average of the two thresholds (combined threshold or CT). Thus, the greater the score in the positive direction, the greater the right sensory field advantage, and the greater the score in the negative direction, the greater the left sensory field advan- tage. Use of a ratio score was meant to reduce the effects of the subjective inequality of the two ends of the time scale. A main effect of the inequality was that a given time difference between thresholds was more easily perceptible and more meaningful for lower thresholds than for higher thresholds. Minimizing this effect was especially important because threshold level was predicted to be at least moderately related to the direction and degree of field differ- ences. Several questions may be raised about the stability/effectiveness of the threshold measures: (l) whether the measures were reliable, especially given the relatively small number of trials; (2) whether the measures were successful in establishing a roughly 50% accuracy level; and (3) whether missed fixation numbers significantly affected the visual measures. Finally, an underlying factor in all these con- cerns was their variation across grade, typeface, and modality condi- tion. To investigate the first question, a rough stability estimate for each field was computed by taking the median of each run after the third, 79 then averaging the first and last pair of runs and correlating those averages.15 The correlations are shown in Table l. In the visual condition, most of the correlations exceeded .7, indicating high stability. The main exceptions were the correlations for Typefaces l and 3 (Tl and T3) for fourth-graders. For Tl, the overall corre- lation was only moderately positive (r = .38), while for T3, the correlation averaged across fields was slightly negative (r = -.04). In the tactual condition, the average correlation was lower than in the visual condition (.63 compared to .78), and there were several instances of moderate to low positive correlations. The general level of stability was still acceptable, though. With the exception of the two cases noted in the visual condition, the correlations did not vary systematically across typeface or field. To check the second question, the percent correct identifications out of the total number of points included in each threshold estimate served as a measure of accuracy level. These percents are listed for all grades, modality conditions, and (for the tactual condition oniy)16 modality orders in Table 2. In the visual condition, accuracy level was markedly lower for T3 than for the other typefaces l (averaging about .24 compared to .42 for the next lowest), and slightly lower for T2 relative to the others (.42 compared to .47). Except for these two typefaces, all the percents ranged from .4 to .6 and were highly consistent across grades and visual fields. A 3 x 2 x 8 ANOVA was used to test the effects of grade, visual field, and type- face on accuracy level, and a 3 x 2 x 7 ANOVA tested those effects 80 A.oeeewemo »e_pweeem ease cw eeeapeew use: eueweeau ee>wm e cw epeww ee>wm e Lew mess eeew umeep we saw: mueeneem emegu xpcov .mees we swag umep we emege>e ecu wweiuae geuwe meet we swan umcww we mcewees we emege>e we cewuepegceu u haw—weeemn :3"; No. mm. ms. mm. ee. om. mm. mm. p~< Ge 3: $5 3: :5 3c 375 Nm. mm. Nm. oe. Nw. me. am. em. Ppeu so as so :5 so as 375 me. mm. mm. mm. me. Nu. Nw. PF. sum $5 3; as so so $5 875 cm. No. mu. om. No. om. em. mw. gee weapon» Sen; ww. om. Nm. mm. mm. NN. pm. Pm. ww. mm. mw. mm. we. Nu. om. um. oN. Pu. pp< A8 A2 35 so as as as 3: 35 as Sc 85 as as A8 A2 37.5 em. mm. mm. No. m5 mm. mm. mm. Nm. em. pm. ~m. o5 No. mm. «N. am. 5 p—eu :E as so so :5 Ge 35 to :5 so :3 2: :5 :5 3: as $75 em. em. F5 N5 pm. nm. em. mm. m5 em. em. Na. mm. mm. mm. Nm. om. om. sum so so so Sc 35 so :5 :5 So so Sc to so as Sc 25 $7; No. mm. mm. em. ow. Nw. pm. m. mm. mm. Nm. FR. 7m oN. Nm. mm. on. Ne. gee Peem_> um we mm we mm we um me am a; mm we um we mm “4 um we Fp< m e . aewpeeez meeweexw A. eueewume zepppewum some :. eeeepucp eeeem Lee mueeaeem we geese: es» mw memegucecee :HV .eeegm ecu .euewmexu. cowepeceu xuwpeees. epeww heemeem an esemeee epesmeezu we exuw—weeumii. F u4me w> >w e> _P< me. 8. n 2 22:3 Sena me. we. Nm. om. om. em. Ne. om. mm. Nm. mm. Pm. em. NN. oe. me. we. me. __e Ae_uze me. we. we. Pm. me. om. ee. _m. mm. em. mm. mm. mm. mm. em. oe. ee. Ne. Peoe Amenzv ee. he. em. mm. om. em. om. mm. me. we. we. om. mN. ,N. Ne. we. me. we. see Ampuzv me. me. we. ee. me. em. me. we. em. me. em. em. PN. 0N. Fe. Ne. mm. mm. see _e=m_> em e3 e>e e>e e>e e>e e>e e>e a): e>e e>¢ e>e e>e e>e e>e e>e e>e e>e Pee e m e eo_e_eeoe oeeee seeeeeo: eeeweezw Levee xuwpeees Azece ceeuweeee .eeueeu ecu gewv ecu .eewuwecee xueeeeee some Lew eeegm ece .eeeweezu .epeww xuemcem an wweiuee Loewe eewueeewwueeew pouncee we eceegeeii.N u4mwecw cm me eemmws eNer Neemw> Lee mueNee ceNNNwa ewes one .memegueesee cNe NNenzN one No ANN N.N Nev e.N ANN ON 3 ON Nev N.N 8 ON 3 Ne Lev N.eN 78 N2 “meeegm o.N N.N N.N o.N o.N N.N o.e N.N N.N N.e o.N N.N N.N o.N N.N N.N NooeeooeN mmoeoe N.N N.N N.m N.e N.N N.N N.e o.N N.N N.e N.N N.o N.N o.N o.N o.N Nooeeoo eeoev N.N N.N N N N oN N N N N N N N N N e m NN NooeeooeN NNanN N o N N o o N N e N N o N e NN NN Nooeeoo NNoe N N e N N N e N N N o o o N N e NooeeooeN NNanN N.N N.N N N N m N N N N N N N o N N N NN Nooeeeo NNN N N N N N N N N N N N N N N e e NooeeooeN NeanN N.N N.N o N e N N N N N N N o N N N N N Nooeeoo eNe m>m “>4 m>m m>4 u>m w>4 u>¢ m>4 m>z m>4 u>m u>4 m>m m>4 u>m m>4 m>m u>4 NN+eNNNe N N N N e N N N NN eoNNoN oeeee eeeweexw meeeem mmeeee eemeee>e ecu NNeueNeeem meeegu en» New eNer Nezmw> ecu eeeweexu seem New :eNuNerNuceeN NeepeN ueeggeuce eee uueeeee new: Amuueneem mmeeue nuggemv NNcNee eewuexww eemmNE we geese: Neuewii.m m4mNw ce mchNec we Neccmees cecheemNe ece .mceee .mecep< .meeewecza NN NN NNe N N 0 0 e N N N ceNuNeceu eeuco eeuweexw xuNNuee: ceece NNNNueeE Axpce Nueueuuv ecu .ceNeNecee xuNNueeE .euuweexu .eeucm e» checoeeu Neececewwwe eNer we eucuewwwcmwm ecu .Nmemecucecue cNV Nceeuue>ee ecuecuum .NuceNuNwweee NNNNuceuuN emuce><--.N Nem ecu cw empcemece meeuweczu NcmNe ecu New mucewewwweee ANNNuNeNuN emucm>< .m .mN m 702 103 A 3 x 2 x 8 ANOVA (grade by modality order by typeface) with repeated measures on the typeface factor was performed on the visual LCs. The summary is shown in Table 9.20 Both the main effects of typeface and grade were significant (F = 5.97, d.f. = 7,294, p_< .01; F = 4.99; d.f. = 2,42, 2 < .05, respectively). Newman-Keuls multiple comparison procedures (shown in Appendix B) were used to examine the pattern of differences across typeface and grade. T6 and T7, those for which right hemisphere participation was greatest, significantly differed from all other typefaces, but not from each other. None of the other differences between typefaces was significant. The college students showed significantly greater right hemisphere participation overall than did the fourth- or ninth-graders, who did not differ from each other. Tactual condition. Fig. 4 shows the average tactual LCs as a function of typeface for each grade by modality order. (The average LCs for each modality order with grades combined are provided in Table 7.) While no general trend across grade or typeface was found, there was a clear difference between modality orders that would account for the nearly significant order by modality interaction in the ANOVA with modality included. For every grade by typeface com- bination except T1 for college students, relatively more right hemisphere participation was shown when the visual condition came before the tactual condition (VT) rather than after (TV). This find- ing is contrary to the hypothesis that, if anything, prior visual exposure to the stimuli would make right hemisphere processing less critical. 104 TABLE 9.--Summary of analysis of variance on visual laterality coefficients Source 0. F. MS F (.2 Between Subjects Mean (Visual field) 1 .013 .03 Grade 2 1.902 4.99* .03 Order 1 .020 .05 Grade x Order 2 .010 .03 Sub. w. groups 42 .381 Within Subjects Typeface 7 1.234 5.97** .07 Grade x Typeface 14 .239 1.16 Order x Typeface 7 .153 .74 Grade x Order x Typeface 14 .039 .19 T x sub. w. groups 294 .207 Fmax = 4.15, d.f. = 7,47, 9 < .05 Test of no association in the correlation matrix: 2 X ==34.04, d.f. = 28, N.S. 105 Fig. 4. Average tactual laterality coefficients for each typeface according to grade and modality order. AVERAGE LATERALITY COEFFICIENTS 106 Condition 11 U. 41h ----* ""51! Grade 9th --—I ——[j COIL—O —o F Simplest TYPE FACE 1k l N Most complex 107 The effect of modality order on the pattern of LCs across type- face was not consistent across grades. With grades combined, the predicted increase in right hemisphere participation with increasing typeface complexity was found for the VT condition, but not for the TV condition or both combined. Even in the VT condition, though, right hemisphere participation was not systematically greater in the tactual compared to the visual modality. A 3 x 2 x 3 (grade by modality order by typeface) repeated meas- ures ANOVA on the tactual LCs revealed only a significant main effect of modality order (F = 4.44, d.f. = 1,42, p_< .05). The ANOVA summary is provided in Table 10. Specific Typeface Characteristics: Average Threshold, Scriptlikeness, Confusability, and Difficulty This section pertains to the relation between visual LCs and specific typeface characteristics. The relation was examined by three measures: (1) first-order or simple correlations between LCs and the characteristics, (2) regression analysis including the following as predictors of LCs: the characteristics, dummy-coded variables for subjects, grade, and the interaction of grade with the characteristics, and (3) separate regression analyses for each grade including only the characteristics as predictors of average LCs. The characteristics considered were those on which the typefaces were rated in Part I, scriptlikeness, confusability, and naming latency (NL), and a fourth characteristic, the average of the individual combined thresholds for each typeface (average threshold or AT). The major finding with all 108 TABLE lO.--Summary of analysis of variance on tactual laterality coefficients Source [1. F. MS F 02 Between Subjects Mean (Hand) 1 .364 .92 Grade 2 .199 .50 Order 1 1.763 4.44* .03 Grade x Order 2 .179 .45 Sub. w. groups 42 .397 Within Subjects Typeface 2 .104 .35 Grade x Typeface 4 .345 1.18 Order x Typeface 2 .269 .92 Grade x Order x Typeface 4 .173 .59 T x subj. w. groups 84 .293 Fmax = 1.03, d.f. - 2,47, N.S. Test of no association in the correlation matrix: x2 = 1.25, d.f. - 3, N.S. 109 three measures of the relation was a grade difference. For fourth- and ninth-graders, NL was the best predictor, while scriptlikeness was a poor predictor, and no combination of characteristics signifi- cantly predicted LCs. In contrast, for college students, script- likeness and confusability were significant predictors. Average Threshold Visual condition. Average threshold (AT) was used as a predictor for LCs in addition to the three characteristics used to select the typefaces in Part I. Even though AT reflected typeface difficulty, which to some extent already was measured by the naming latencies (NL) from Part I, it was included because it shared the same measurement factors with the LCs. If the results showed a high correlation of LCs with AT, but not with NL, an explanation of the change in right hemisphere processing across typeface in terms of peripheral measure- ment factors could not be eliminated. In other words, raising the general threshold level for the more complex typefaces could have changed the task in some way or failed to completely equalize task difficulty across typeface. In contrast, NL estimates were obtained with a method independent of LC measurement. The NL estimates involved different subjects and centralized rather than lateralized letter presentation. A high correlation between LCs and ML would suggest an explanation of the change in right hemisphere processing across typeface in terms of difficulty in the higher-order analysis of images of letters all clearly perceived. 110 Table 11 shows ATs (in msecs.) for the eight typefaces in the visual condition for grades separated and combined. In general, ATs varied widely across typeface, covering nearly the entire scale. There was also a general increase in AT with increasing grade. Except for T1, which most subjects found very easy, and T3 for fourth-graders,2] the standard deviations were large (averaging about 37 msecs.). These large standard deviations are one indication that the use of individually determined presentation durations was justified. A 3 x 8 ANOVA to test the effects of grade and typeface on visual ATs revealed significant main effects of both grade and typeface. According to Newman-Keuls tests, all differences between typefaces were significant except those between T5 and T7 and between T6 and T8. Also, fourth-graders were significantly different from ninth- graders and college students, who did not differ from each other. The ANOVA summary and Newman-Keuls results are provided in Appendix C.22 Tactual condition. Since there were only three typefaces used in the tactual condition and since tactual LCs did not vary signifi- cantly across typeface, ATs and the three characteristics from Part I were not examined as predictors of tactual LCs. The ATS were tabulated, though, to look at the general pattern of scores according to type- face, grade, and modality order. These ATs (in secs.) are shown in Table 11. Three observations about the pattern of scores can be made. First, it was of interest to see whether there was a modality order difference in AT that would correspond to the significant order 111 N NeNecmeccw + NeNecmeccw n quv eNecmech eecNeEee ececze » Nuzv 0.N 0.0 N.NN N.N 000 ManzN N.N e.0 N.0N N.N NNe N0euzv 0.N N.0N 0.N NNe N.eN NN.NN N0.NN NONuzN e.0 N.N 0.0 0.N NN00 N0.NN NN.0N N0.NN N0NuzN 0.0 0.N 0.0N N.N 000 N0.NN 0.NN N0.NN N0NuzN N.N N.NN 0.NN N.N eNe Neoeey Pezuueh N0e-zN 0.e0N N.N0 N.e0N N.N0 N.Ne N.00N 0.00 N.NN NNe NNON NONN NNeN NNeN NNNN NONN Nva N0 0 N0Nuzv N.NN N.0N 0.NN 0.N0N N.N0 0.0N N.0eN N.N0 0.NN NN00 NNNN NONN N0eN NNNN NONN NNNN NNNN NNNN N0anv N.eN N.N0 0.N0 0.00 0.00 N.0e 0.N0N 0.00 N.eN 000 NNNN NNNN NeNN N0eN N000 N0 0 NNeN NONN N0NuzN Nmoamev e.00 N.0NN 0.0N 0.eNN 0.NN N.N0 N.NNN N.NNN 0.0N gee N000N> NNe 0 N 0 0 e N N N eoNNNeeoe eeucu . . meuweexh xuepueex ceece Neweueee Aapce Nuauuuuv ecu .ceNNeecee Nuepueee .eeuweeau .eeucm eu checeeeu “memecucecue cwv mcewuuN>ee ecuecuum ecu Amh0.1 N 000-000 1. .11 N N N 000-000 0 N-000-000 e 0N N0. N0. 00. eN.N N0. NN. 0e.- NN. NN00 00. N0. N0. 0N.N N0. 0N. NN. 0e. :00 00. N0. N0. NN.N e0. NN. NN. NN. eNe _u0eN m mceupep NNu mceupep m NNN _ueeu m mceuuee NNu mceuuep m umN m ewe a Noecceu a iueecceu u m 000 a uueecceu a iuoecceu m N0N.0N.eN.NN.0.0.00N0 00000000 «NNN.NN.0N.NeNae.N.NN, 00000000 N 000-000.1 N 000-000.- 11 N N N 000-000 0 N 000-000 0 0N New ceuuee ceeco ceeeee eeucu New ceue cw mceeeee eeccu echw ecu cow uoeccee uceecee e>weuNec ecu .mw ecu 0w cow eeucm coue Nu oeecm ceece ceNNeN cuue cew NNceNewwweee NNNNuceNuN emuce><1-.mN memu ecu e0 eecucEeo NN cow 00 cecmwc 0 Nu eechee Nu: cceeeuc ecu .coNNNecee Nueeeue ecN ce0 .Neeuwecxu ceew «NuN ecu oN eecueeeo Necec eeumwN muN Neeuweeze ceew emcww ecu cow 0: emuce>u cecmwc u mu eechee Nu: cceeuue NNcu .ceNNNecee N000N> ecu co...2r .eeuweexe uucu cow Nceee Noceoeecw ecu eecw eeNNNse ecez eeuwecxu ce>N0 u cow eceN we uceNerweee NNNNuceNuN u cewz meeenceme NeN-zc NN m m 0N m m m oN N e e m NN¢ N0NuzN N m N N m e m e m N N N N NNeu N0N-zc m m N N e N m N e N N N N :00 N0N-zc m N N N m N N N m e N N m cue NueueuN >eu >eu >eu >eu >eu >eu >eu >eu >eu >eu >eu >eu N.NN NN :0 :0 :0 :0 :0 :c :0 :0 :0 00 :0 :0 00.0 00 N0-zN N0-zv N0-zv N0-zv NN-zN N0uzc cuwx meeeneam we cecEez >N N> >N N> >N N) N N N NNe-zc 0 mm NN wN mN o NN mN 0N oN ON NN MN NN eN o NN< N0N-20 oN m NN m m 0 m e m m oN m m m N m N NNeu N0N-00 «N m NN m m m m oN N o e m m m N m N cNm NON-zc mN m oN 0 ON m m m N N e e e m 0 ON m cue Nuemw> .o.N.m.mNuc >eu >eu >eu >eu >eu >eu >eu >eu >eu >eu >eu >eu >eu >eu >eu >eu . . . 0>0 0>c 0>0 0>0 0>0 0>c 0>0 0>0 0>0 0>c 0>0 0): 0>0 0>c 0>0 0>c A N v m NN uc eeecu coNNNecou cow: Nueeneem we cease: e N m m N e m N NNNNeee: NceweeewoNNcuc ececcmNaec 0cmwc chNuececN we ceece ch Nu<000>w -' . . 4‘IH.!'MI..-Wlufl.u fink.“ .usfirnr.7 “IE 7 “:5... 3.“.Eii'.“ Hus “Rh-1.“.‘ulnrfl .0 . r ”.“1II W ”§.§ came: emuce>u ecu Ne New eeuweexu mmeceu cceeeue ecu ceN: NceNNNNceu ecez muceNerweee NNNNuceNuN emocx mueehcem we cece:c ecu mzocm caeNeu NNuN ecN .ceece NNNNueoE NNNce Nueeeuuv ecu .ceNNNecee NNNNueee ecu eouweezu ceue cow oemuecu>eu eNer acoNc_200weN u cuwz eeucm cee Nuueneem we cecEez-.NN 000