rt. .V’fiq... . ‘ 6 . hhfluéfadfifisfliniéi Liirfiafiflg mt $thfiL $.21, 2;? Hart :9. {3.124 17:1". at"... «“31. n: {I ; . 2 $33.? . . 1...?“ , V $3 $23. "2.5.;me om all 5 .zrhquwmvflw ihh.‘..l$ v5.1.3. amwg . I! .{1 n. K“: 1.5! In,“ ., I: n x . ...I¢uau..o.1( .9 1: Ltt :5... a a ($.33? ginflv . . pf . diving..." L. .. . .i i Mn. an. :3 I}: r5935... .. away... 1 v 9 I 59 3...? , t: « . . ‘ 5:155:3132 J I: 1...: I .3 31:: i. Wilt: $3152.41! 2. 51-11 . av t .. £11 £5. .. nanny-1...... lam . i n l .4"- d . kg? 9 .3 t.” 3‘”? .1. at (13551115 rll...r. Am?” LIBRARY 2 Michigan State 2 " r5 I University This is to certify that the dissertation entitled APPLICATION OF THE CONVERSION AND TRACKING MODELS IN MEASURING THE EFFECTIVENESS OF TRAVEL MICHIGAN’S 2003 TRAVEL ADVERTISING CAMPAIGN presented by KUDZAYI MAUMBE has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for the DOCTORAL degree in PARKS, RECREATION AND TOURISM RESOURCES ‘Major Professor's Signature 97/174 9/ $50 63 Date MSU is an Affinnative Action/Equal Opportunity Institution _ - -.-.—.------.-.- -t- _ _ . a _ _.-,-.-.-.-._- - ‘ o‘ffi' PLACE IN RETURN BOX to remove this checkout from your record. TO AVOID FINES return on or before date due. MAY BE RECALLED with earlier due date if requested. DATE DUE DATE DUE DATE DUE 2/05 p:/C|RC/DaleDue.indd-p.1 APPLICATION OF THE CONVERSION AND TRACKING MODELS IN MEASURING THE EFFECTIVENESS OF TRAVEL MICHIGAN’S 2003 TRAVEL ADVERTISING CAMPAIGN By Kudzayi Maumbe A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University In partial fulfillment of the requirements Of the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Parks, Recreation and Tourism Resources 2006 ABSTRACT APPLICATION OF THE CONVERSION AND TRACKING MODELS IN MEASURING THE EFFECTIVENESS OF TRAVEL MICHIGAN ’S 2003 TRAVEL ADVERTISING CAMPAIGN By Kudzayi Maumbe This study’s objective was to determine the effectiveness of the Travel Michigan’s 2003 travel advertising campaign by application of the conversion and tracking models. Seven stages of data analysis were conducted in this study. The first stage was to determine whether significant differences existed in pre and post campaign advertising awareness. In the second stage, T-tests were conducted to identify significant differences in Destination Awareness (DA), Intention to Visit (IV) and Actual Visitation (AV) between those aware and those not aware of advertising. The third stage involved principal components analysis to reduce the captured 22 attitude variables into a more manageable data set that was used in subsequent analyses. In the fourth stage, correlation analysis was conducted to determine the correlation of the image components with Advertising Awareness (AA), Destination Awareness (DA) and Intention to Visit (IV). In the fifth stage, two models (Unaided Advertising Awareness (UAA) and Destination Awareness (DA)) were evaluated using logistic regression. In the sixth stage, the conversion model was applied to the data to calculate conversion ratios using cross- tabulations. Four different approaches named routes (0-3) were utilized to calculate the four alternative conversion measures. The routes were derived from the conversion and tracking models given by Siegel and Ziff-Levine (1990). Route zero was the direct route onl} infll as d mor trac con Sev ICIL adv we] aw; CIIC imr EX} W e SCI’ rou yiel Ovc only considering those visitors to the state who indicated their decision to visit was influenced by the advertisements without necessarily having gone through all the stages as depicted by the conversion and tracking models. Route one was from the tracking model, and it included the destination awareness stage, route two was also from the tracking model, but it excluded the destination awareness stage. Route three was from the conversion model which included all the stages plus the “inquiry-fulfillment” stage. Seventh, the conversion ratios from the preceding stage were utilized to calculate the return on advertising investment. The results at each stage consistently show the effectiveness of Travel Michigan’s advertising. Significant differences in advertising awareness were found between phases 1 and 2, implying the advertisements managed to gain the audience’s attention. There were also significant differences in DA, IV and AV between those aware and those not aware. Results showed Michigan is viewed mainly as a family destination and as a close enough destination for short trips. Logistic regression results showed TV is the most important in increasing one’s advertising awareness, followed by gender and experience. Experience is the most important in increasing one’s destination awareness followed by Web-use, intention to visit and exposure to advertising. Results from the sixth and seventh stages of analysis show positive conversion ratios and ROI. Route zero yielded a 11.2% conversion ratio and a $10.47 ROI for every dollar spent on advertising; route one yielded a 5.2% conversion ratio and a $4.86 ROI for every dollar spent on advertising; route two resulted in a 8.5% conversion ratio and $7.94 ROI and finally route three yielded a 2.2% conversion ratio with $2.06 ROI for every dollar spent on advertising. Overall, Travel Michigan achieved a positive return on advertising investment. DEDICATION To my great parents Eliah and Emmah Chitiyo. iv thi: m) Do this my all IOL up. pro (35;). faci for 1 line my. me; Chili ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS There are people to whom I need to express my sincere gratitude, without whom this degree and dissertation would not have been possible. My deepest gratitude goes to my dissertation advisor and also the chair of my graduate academic committee Professor Donald Holecek for his guidance and concise support throughout my study and writing of this dissertation. I would also like to express my sincere gratitude to the other members of my graduate committee Professors Christine Vogt and Bonnie Knutson for their guidance and input towards the success of this study. I also feel very indebted to Mr. Mavuso and all the library staff of the University of Fort Hare Library, South Africa for allowing me to use their library and computer resources during the last stages of my dissertation write up. I thank you all very much. I am eternally grateful to the PEO and International Peace Scholarship (IPS) for providing part of the funding for the first two years of my doctoral program. I am especially grateful to Carol Goeman for being my required U. S. contact person and facilitator for the IPS without which this degree would have just remained a pipe dream for me. I would also like to thank my friends who were there cheering me up to the finish line. My greatest thanks go to my dearest friend Sue Tims and her husband Ted, literally my other parents in the U. S. for their unconditional love and support and being there for me all the time as I struggled to balance the demands of being both a parent of young children and a doctoral student. I owe you. Thanks also go to all the members of the University United Methodist Church community for their moral, emotional and spiritual support during my stay in East Lansing. I would also like to offer my deepest gratitude to my family. To my parents, I thank you for building a strong academic foundation for me and instilling the hard work ethic in me. Special thanks goes to my elder brother Gwinyai and elder sister Moud for their contribution in laying the foundation to my education and to my eldest brother Onai, younger sister Tendayi and brothers in-law Munya and Antony for their unconditional love and all round support during my stay and academic struggle in the U. S. Last, but not least I would like to thank my dear husband Blessing for his encouragement and great support; putting his career on hold to help me realize my dreams. Deepest gratitude goes to my lovely children Samantha, Mike and Victoria, for the patience and cheer. Now you can play with the computer and bang the keyboard. vi TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES ......................................................................................................... viii LIST OF FIGURES ......................................................................................................... ix LIST OF APPENDICESKEY TO ABBREVIATIONS ................................................ x KEY TO ABBREVIATIONS ......................................................................................... xi Chapter 1 ............................................................................................................................. 1 1.0 Introduction and Problem Statement ...................................................................... 1 1 .1 Objectives ........................................................................................................... 4 1.2 Hypotheses .......................................................................................................... 5 Chapter 2 ............................................................................................................................. 8 2.0 Literature Review .................................................................................................... 8 2.1 Theoretical Background ...................................................................................... 8 2.1.1 Persuasion Theories .................................................................................... 8 2.1.2 Selective Processes Theories (Cognitive Dissonance Theory) ................. 15 2.1.3 Communication theories ........................................................................... 15 2.2 Does Advertising Really Work? ....................................................................... 18 2.2 Travel Advertising Effectiveness Methods Review ......................................... 23 Chapter 3 ........................................................................................................................... 28 3.0 Methods ................................................................................................................. 28 3.1 Study Design ..................................................................................................... 28 3.2 Data collection .................................................................................................. 28 3.3 Data analysis techniques used ........................................................................... 33 Chapter 4 ........................................................................................................................... 36 4.0 Results and Discussion ......................................................................................... 36 4. 1 Descriptives ....................................................................................................... 36 4.2 Pre and Post Advertisement Comparisons ........................................................ 38 4.2.1 Advertising Awareness ..................................................................................... 38 4.3 Hypothesis Testing .................................................................................................. 39 4.3.1 Model Estimation ......................................................................................... 55 4.3.1.1 Advertising Awareness .............................................................................. 55 4.3.1.2 Destination Awareness .............................................................................. 60 4.4 Conversion Rates .............................................................................................. 63 4.4.1 Advertising Conversion Model ................................................................. 63 4.4.2 The Advertising Tracking Model ..................................................................... 73 4.5 Return on Advertising Investment (ROI) ......................................................... 76 Chapter 5 ........................................................................................................................... 80 5.0 Summary and Conclusion ........................................................................................... 80 5.1 Study Limitations .............................................................................................. 90 References ......................................................................................................................... 92 vii Tat Tat T31 T31 Ta T2 LIST OF TABLES Table 1: Advertising, Related Behavioral Dimensions and Advertising Research Related to the model ..................................................................... 22 Table 2: Variables measured and their levels of measurement ................................ 31 Table 3: Gender * Number of respondents by their residence DMA.. . . . . . . . . . . . . . ........38 Table 4: T-tests for Equality of means for DA, IV and AV between those aware and those not aware of adverts ............................................................ 41 Table 5: T-tests for attitude towards Michigan as a tourism destination. . . . . . . . .. ...42 Table 6: Principle components analysis results showing loading of each variable on the components ................................................................................ 45 Table 7: Principle components results showing eigenvalues for the components and % of variation explained by each component ........................................... 47 Table 8: Correlation results between attitude factors and AA, DA and IV .................. 51 Table 9: Correlation results for Annual Income, DMA location and AA, DA, and IV ..... 53 Table 10: Variables in the Unaided Advertising Awareness (UAA) Model. . . . . . ........56 Table 11: Goodness of fit tests for the Unaided Advertising Awareness (UAA) Model ..................................................................................... 57 Table 12: Variables included in the Destination Awareness (DA) Mode] at each step ..... 61 Table 13: Goodness of fit tests for the Destination Awareness (DA) Model ............... 61 Table 14: Cross-tab results for AA and DA with the three image factors ................... 66 Table 15: Conversion rates from Image stage to Inquiry Fulfillment stage ................. 69 Table 16: Conversion rates from the Inquiry to the Motivation stage ........................ 70 Table 17: Conversion rates for the “Conversion Behavior” stage ............................ 72 Table 18: R01 figures for the 2003 Travel Michigan advertising campaign ............... 78 viii LIST OF FIGURES Figure 1: H-J-K instrumental theory of communication and persuasion model. . . . . . ..9 Figure 2: An integrative framework of advertising persuasion ................................ 14 Figure 3: (A) Conversion and (B) Tracking Models ........................................... 35 Figure 4: Distribution of respondents among the three DMAs ................................. 37 Figure 5: The conversion model for Travel Michigan’s 2003 travel advertising. . . . . .......65 Figure 6: Advertising tracking model showing the different routes (0-3) used in calculating the conversion rates and ROI ........................................................ 74 Figure 7: Advertising tracking model for Travel Michigan ................................... 75 ix AP: AP: AP: LIST OF APPENDICES Appendix 1: Sample of Questionnaire used in the study ...................................... 97 Appendix 2: Description of the advertisements by channel and DMA ................... 105 Appendix 3: Detailed Descriptive Statistics by DMA ....................................... 107 Appendix 4: Test for appropriateness of data set for factor analysis ..................... 109 ax CR DA D.\ 1v: Mi 0C PD: PEI PFI R01 TTI UA. AV: CR: DA: DMA: MI: OCR: PDI: PEI: PFI: ROI: TTRRC : UAA: KEY TO ABBREVIATIONS Advertising Awareness Actual Visitation Conversion Rate Destination Awareness Designated Marketing Area Intention to Visit Michigan Overall Conversion Rate Positive Distance Image Positive Entertainment Image Positive Family Image Return On Investment Travel, Tourism and Recreation Resource Center Unaided Advertising Awareness xi Th do 1‘31 CC Chapter 1 1.0 Introduction and Problem Statement The economic importance of tourism to states, regions and the world has been well documented and can’t be disputed. Within the United States alone, travel and tourism is a US$13 trillion industry, is the leading employer in states such as Florida and Hawaii and ranks among the top three employers in 29 of the 50 states (TIA 2005). Recognizing this economic importance of tourism to the economy, states have agencies that promote inbound tourism with budgets ranging from about $1.7m in Delaware to over $70m in Hawaii (TIA, 2003). With such big tourism budgets coupled with tight economic conditions, states’ tourism agencies are faced with increasing pressure to justify their promotion budgets and be accountable for the planned impacts. Conversion studies have been commonly used in most of the advertising effectiveness research. These studies, however, have been criticized for having methodological and design shortcomings that limit their usefulness and application (Messmer and Johnson, 1993). Some of these limitations include: overstating of the conversion ratio as a result of including those individuals who had already decided to visit before being exposed to advertisements, high non-response bias and inability to provide quantitative figures on return to advertising investment. Advertising tracking studies have also been frequently used to determine advertising effectiveness. Advertising tracking studies seek to generate awareness among the target audience, create awareness of the destination as a place to visit, create a positive image of the destination, motivate consumers to travel to the dc tr: 3‘ in it It (I) destination and influence travel behavior (Siegel and Ziff-Levine, 1990). The advertising tracking model, like the conversion model, because it is most suitable for generating awareness and creating positive destination image, can’t be expected to produce an all inclusive estimate of return on investment. Many communication theories have been used as the basis for advertising research including the Hierarchy of Effects model (Palda, 1966) and the AIDA model: Attention, Interest, Desire and Action (Haley, 1985). The Hierarchy of Effects model constitutes six steps, namely Attention-Interest-Comprehension-Impact-Attitude and Sale. Howard and Sheth (1969) developed a theory of buyer behavior that supported the Hierarchy of Effects model of consumer behavior. In this model, Howard and Sheth (1969) employed the following five output variables: attention, brand comprehension, attitude, intention and purchase. Regardless of which model of communication and/or consumer behavior being referred to, the “attention” and ‘interest/attitude’ stages both always precede the action stage. This implies that getting the consumer’s attention, generating interest and changing their otherwise negative, neutral or non-existent attitudes are prerequisites to getting to the action stage. Advertising effectiveness, therefore, can be measured in more ways than just counting the individuals that act. Research has shown that 76% of “definites,” 31% of “probables” and 27% of “mights” actually purchase the advertised brand (Gruber, 1970). The statement of the problem was to determine the effectiveness of travel advertising on us: jus dil fo' peoples’ destination awareness, attitudes towards MI as a tourism destination, motivation (intention) to visit the state and actual visitation. The goal is to determine how effective the advertisements are in moving individuals from a state of ‘not aware of MI as a tourist destination’ to ‘mights,’ ‘probables’ and ‘definites.’ The study also seeks to investigate and analyze the relationship between a number of different variables including advertising awareness, destination awareness, attitude, intention to visit and other demographic variables such as income and education, to mention a few. While this study uses the same kind of procedure as in the conversion and tracking studies, it goes beyond just determining the effectiveness of advertising. It also investigates the relationship of different important variables as they pertain to advertising and destination awareness. The following two main dependent variables will be evaluated in this study: 0 Advertising Awareness (AA) 0 Destination Awareness (DA Several predictor variables will be used including; 0 Exposure to advertisements 0 Experience with leisure travel in the state 0 Advertising channel 0 Income 0 Gender 0 Web use, searching for travel information o Toll-free phone inquiry 0 Distance of DMA from Michigan - Attitude towards or Image/Perception of Michigan as a tourist destination 1.1 Th 1.1 Objectives The objectives of this study are to: 1. 2. Determine consumer awareness of Travel Michigan’s advertising. Establish whether or not Travel Michigan’s advertising program is effective in generating destination awareness. Determine advertising effectiveness in changing consumers’ attitudes of Michigan as a tourism destination. Analyze the relationship between annual income, location of DMA and advertising awareness (AA), destination awareness (DA) and intention to visit (IV) the state of Michigan. Determine advertising effectiveness of the Travel Michigan’s 2003 advertising campaign by computing conversion ratios for the three targeted DMAs including Chicago, Cleveland and Indianapolis. Calculate the return to advertising investment (ROI) for Travel Michigan’ 5 2003 travel advertising campaign. 1.2 Hypotheses The following hypotheses were addressed in this study: 1. Individuals exposed to Travel Michigan’s advertising will have greater awareness of the state as a tourism destination than those who have not been exposed. Creating awareness is the first in a series of stages through which advertising works to change people’s behaviors according to the Hierarchy of Effects Model of communication. Advertising first creates awareness, develops knowledge about the product, generates a liking and preference for the product leading to one’s conviction to purchase the product resulting in the actual purchase of the product (Lavidge and Steiner, 1961). Simply raising top-of-mind awareness of a brand enables it to be evoked into more choice sets, thus increasing the probability of it being selected (Nedungadi, 1990). 2. Travel Michigan’s advertising will change people’s attitudes towards the state as a tourism destination. Advertising theory represents advertising as indirectly influencing behavior through its more direct effect on consumer attitudes (Butterfield et. al., 1998) 3. Those individuals aware of Travel Michigan’s advertising will have greater motivation/intention to visit the state on a leisure trip, than those who are not aware. Development of motivation/intention to purchase is considered one of the intermediate results of advertising. After being exposed to advertising individuals develop motivation to purchase before they exhibit some conversion behavior (Siegel and Ziff-Levine, 1990). Intentions are valid predictors of one’s consumption of a brand (Wansik and Ray, 2000). . Travel Michigan’s advertising is effective in generating visits to the state. Advertising is understood to have an effect on people’s behavior or actions through a hierarchical process which involves getting people’s attention, generating their interest and desire resulting in some action (AIDA) (Sandage and Fryburger, 1963). In this case, the action is taking a pleasure trip to the state of Michigan. . Attitudes towards Michigan as a tourism destination will be positively correlated to Advertising Awareness (AA), Destination Awareness (DA) and Intention to Visit the state (IV). According to the Hovland, Janis and Kelly (H-J-K) (1953) communication persuasion model, audience factors such as attitudes, self esteem, intelligence and others play an important role in effective persuasion communication and affect the way by which the stimuli (message) is perceived and received (Tan, 1985). Attitude and belief changes are a prerequisite to and precede sales, and there exists a positive relationship between changes in recall and attitudes (Haskins, 1964). . Distance of a DMA from Michigan will be negatively correlated with AA, DA, and IV. Travel behavior has changed. People are now taking more short getaways today than they did ten years ago (TIA, 2003). 7. Annual household income will be positively correlated with AA, DA, and IV. Resources are not used to develop judgments, beliefs or plans of action unless some motive exists (Feldman and Lynch, 1988). Because low income people have no financial resources to take pleasure trips, it is assumed that they have no motivation to pay attention to travel advertising hence, have no intention to take pleasure trips. 8. Having taken a pleasure trip to Michigan before (Experience) will have a positive effect on advertising awareness. This hypothesis is based on the selective processes or cognitive dissonance theory which states that individuals selectively attend to messages in an effort to reduce dissonance. Post purchase doubts about the wisdom of choosing a product could lead one to attend to that product’s advertising in order to allay such doubts (Rotzol, 1964). Ehrenberg (2000) concluded that advertising’s role is to reinforce feelings of satisfaction with brands already bought. 9. Advertising channel has a significant effect on advertising awareness. The way a message is conveyed to an audience (pictorially or verbally) has been argued to have a significant effect on the processing of the information contained in the advertisement. These processing differences have been seen to have an effect on the audiences’ attitudes towards the brand and purchase intentions. Pictorial advertising stimuli can yield different results from verbal advertising stimuli, and pictorial stimuli generally are better recalled (Edel and Staelin, 1983). Chapter 2 2.0 Literature Review This chapter covers the literature review on the subject of advertising effectiveness. The theoretical background is given first, which includes a detailed discussion on persuasion, selective processes, and connnunication theories. The chapter is concluded by a discussion on research methods utilized in previous advertising effectiveness studies. 2.1 Theoretical Background 2.1.1 Persuasion Theories The subject of advertising awareness is closely linked and studied using communication and persuasion theories. Many theories have been developed over the years to try to explain the behavior of humans when they receive messages during the communication process. Since one of the purposes of advertising is to persuade and convince the audience to buy the product, persuasion theories become important in advertising studies. Hovland, Janis and Kelly (I-I-J-K) (1953) developed an instrumental theory of communication and persuasion that defines persuasive communication as the process by which an individual (the communicator) transmits stimuli to modify the behavior of other individuals (the audience). Persuasive communication leads to attitude change through changing related opinions. Opinions, according to Hovland et. al. (1953), can be changed by exposure to persuasive communication that contains information and arguments why a new opinion should be accepted. Bayou, Mohamed, Panitz and Eric (1993) defined persuasion as a communication strategy designed to satisfy the parties involved by ac? p61 int Opi det IOV eff, influencing attitudes and beliefs therefore behavior, which is what marketers hope to achieve through their advertisements. The instrumental theory of communication and persuasion as illustrated in Figure 1 divides the communication and persuasion process into three sections namely stimuli, intervening process and response. Stimuli Intervening Processes Response Sources factors ——> Opinion change Expertise, Trustworthiness likability \ Perception change Message factors Type Of appeal Comprehension [ \ Affect change Audience Factors Initial Position A Intelligence ‘ Acceptance I: Action change Self esteem 7V Personality etc Figure 1: : H-J-K Instrumental theory of communication and persuasion model (Fan, 1985) This theory has several important aspects relating to this study. First, according to the H- J -K theory, communication success is not only measured in action change, but also in opinion, perception and affect change. Since, one of the objectives of this study is to determine the effectiveness of specific advertisements in changing people’s attitudes towards Michigan as a tourist destination, according to this theory, the advert is said to be effective if it results in a significant positive change in people’s attitudes and perceptions 10 st: in ad the Th. ind adx hell wh this Var Ho bel ext. lde. towards Michigan as a vacation destination. Second, this theory includes attention as a stage of the intervening processes. Advertising awareness, a variable also being measured in this study, does not occur without the target audience paying some attention to the advertisement. Third, the theory places importance on the effect of audience factors such as initial position, intelligence, personality and self-esteem on the results. This supports the hypothesis that individuals who have visited Michigan before, because of their knowledge and experience, will have more positive attitudes towards Michigan, as well as greater awareness of Michigan advertising. The H-J-K theory also forms the basis for hypotheses 2 and 3 which state that: 1) individuals will rank Michigan higher as a tourist destination after exposure to advertising and: 2) that individuals who are more aware of the advertisements have greater motivation to visit Michigan. The H-J-K theory of persuasive communication helps us understand why individuals change or don’t change their attitudes and/or actions when exposed to an advertisement. This theory is important, especially considering that this study involves some measure of the effectiveness of advertising by comparing variations in some important variables before and after exposure to advertisements. However, the H-J-K theory of persuasive communication, like the other learning and belief-based theories of persuasion, is based on the notion that persuasion depends on the extent to which message recipients learn from, form beliefs about, as well as retain the ideas conveyed by the message (Hovland, Janis and Kelly, 1953). 10 The information-processing perspective, which includes the co gnitive-response model, directly criticize learning belief-based theories such as the H-J-K theory (Meyers- Levy and Malaviya, 1999). The Cognitive-Response Model of persuasion states that persuasion occurs as a result of people’s reflections on, as well as cognitive responses to, the contents of a message (Wright, 1980). Cognitive responses are defined as the thoughts that arise during the process of elaboration when people relate the contents of the message to other messages or to their pre-existing knowledge and views stored in memory. Persuasion in this case is a reflection of the net favorableness of the cognitive responses that are evoked in people’s minds as they elaborate on the message, and a decision is based also on the net favorableness of the information available in memory at the time of judgment (Kisielius and Stemthal, 1986; Keller, 1987; Meyer-Levy and Malaviya, 1999; Weilbacher, 2002). Cognitive-response theory, however, has been criticized mainly for its failure to explain situations in which people are persuaded even when there exists enough evidence to show that they did not elaborate on the contents of the message. Dual process persuasion models were prompted by the failure of the cognitive-response models to address those situations in which individuals are persuaded even though they don’t seem to have elaborated much on the contents of the message. The dual process models such as the Elaboration Likelihood Model (ELM) of persuasion suggest the existence of a systematic (central) and a heuristic (peripheral) route to persuasion (Petty and Cacioppo, 1986). The systematic route produces judgments that are based on critical and extensive elaboration of messages while the heuristic route produces judgments that 11 are based on simple and intuitive inferences involving scanty elaboration (Meyers-Levy and Malaviya, 1999). Which of these two routes mediates persuasion at any given time is affected by a number of factors influencing the amount of cognitive resources that an individual devotes to the message elaborative process. These factors include, but are not limited to, available time, message relevance, experience and prior knowledge. That experience and prior knowledge affect persuasion becomes important to this study when it comes to the differences in advertising awareness, destination awareness and attitude towards Michigan among those individuals that have visited the state and those that haven’t. It is also important in understanding the differences among those who have been exposed to the advertisements more as compared to those who have been exposed less. Individuals with prior knowledge and/or more experience with the state are likely to be more aware of the state’s advertising than those that have neither the experience nor prior knowledge. Another theory, the experiential based theory of persuasion, has been suggested as a third and alternative basis for judgment and persuasion. This theory, unlike the systematic and the heuristic approaches which emphasize the cognitive process of persuasion, states that judgments are mediated by the sensations and/or feelings that are triggered by the act of engaging in an elaboration process, (Strack, 1992). Meyers—levy and Malaviya (1999) developed an integrative framework of advertising persuasion shown in Figure 2 that incorporates all the other theories and goes even further to include a judgment correction phase during which individuals reconsider their decisions and make adjustments in an effort to eliminate biases that might have occurred in the initial elaboration process. This 12 fran in \x 01h: TCIT CUS inte framework was developed as a result of the earlier theories’ failure to explain situations in which individuals change their decisions after having been persuaded and convinced otherwise. This theory highlights to marketers the importance of continual advertising to remind customers of products and services because even if they are initially persuaded, customers always change their judgments and decisions with the passage of time. The integrative framework shown in Figure 2 is divided into two main stages: judgment formation and judgment correction. 13 ll PERSUASION VARIABLES I MessagerecrprentContext I Resource Allocation for . . Message Processing_;~ Minimal / ‘ Modest \Substantial ’E’i‘pcfimfiammmmg Hemficméfising " 3 iii-Systematic mine Strategy Strategy 1 _ , Shategy .. . JUDGMENT FORMATION Attributions abo Item-Specific. f Elaboration Relational V Elaboration v / EMP LOY ' DEFAULT VALUE AS INITIAL JUDGMENT PRODUCEWIUDGMENT; ; .5: ..I “// JUDGMENT CORRECTION STAGE m , , . . . FORM FINAL JUDGMENT Figure 2: An Integrative Framework of Advertising Persuasion (Meyers-Levy and Malaviya, 1999) 14 2.1. An( sele ind: thei ana and sele affo. with 2.1.3 Studi Most Simil; a nutr earlier explai 110110 as adi mam) ”2383 i 2. 1.2 Selective Processes Theories (Cognitive Dissonance Theory) Another important theory relating to this study is that of selective processes including selective attention, selective perception and selective retention (Haley, 1985). This theory agrees with Festinger’s (1957) theory of cognitive dissonance, which states that individuals deliberately attend to or avoid messages and situations that do not conform to their beliefs, lifestyles and needs (Severin, 1979). One objective of this study is to analyze the relationship of some demographic factors such as income with advertising and destination awareness. Some of the variation here may be explained by the theory of selective attention and retention. For example, individuals with low income and can not afford vacations may not necessarily pay attention to travel advertisements while those with higher income would be more likely to tune into travel advertisements. 2.1.3 Communication theories Studies to evaluate the effectiveness of advertising on sales date as far back as the 19305. Most of these studies focused on consumer products, generally single or a small group of similar products (Butterfield et al., 1998). Advertising has been known to affect people in a number of different ways as hypothesized by different researchers over the years. The earlier black box theories of consumer behavior were some of the first ones to try to explain the impact of advertising on peoples’ behavior. These models were based on the notion that individuals show some kind of response when exposed to a set of stimuli such as advertisements. They also assumed advertising operates in a straight-forward one way manner in which the advertiser decides on the message, encodes it and delivers it through mass media to a basically passive or receptive audience (Haley, 1985). 15 One of the popular earlier models is the AIDA (attention, interest, desire, action) model, which assumes that when individuals are exposed to advertising communication, they go through a series of stages namely Attention, Interest, Desire, and Action. Accordingly, the purpose of advertising therefore is to catch people’s attention, leading them to develop some interest and desire in the product resulting in some action (purchase of product) (Haley, 1985). The Hierarchy of Effects model was developed as an upgrade of the AIDA model. It is a straight forward model consisting of six stages namely Attention, Interest, Comprehension, Impact, Attitude and Sale (Lavidge and Steiner, 1961). The main weakness of these straight-forward models lies in their failure to account for situations in which these steps are skipped and/or reversed as evidence has shown that frequently occurs (Ray, 1973; Aaker and Day, 1974). New models were developed which sought to address the weakneses of the previous models. The new models, such as the Richardson-Haley model (1980), have their roots in the two-way communication process first suggested by Bauer (1964) based on the understanding that consumers are not inert passive targets for messages that can be manipulated by advertisements. According to Bauer (1964), message recipients are active participants in the communication process, screening, distorting, adding and subtracting contents of the messages that they are being exposed to. This theory agrees with the selective processes theories that, when individuals are exposed to a message including advertising, they receive what they want to receive and believe what they want to believe (Haley, 1985). The Richardson-Haley model emphasizes that the message recipients when exposed to advertising are not in a blank state, but bring their own experiences, 16 values, interests, personality, lifestyle, moods and habits. This is especially important to advertisers because it highlights advertising has different effects on different people and audiences, there really isn’t a ‘one size fit all’ approach when it comes to advertising. This theory is also important in relation to this study as it makes it worthwhile to investigate the importance of each of the different individual characteristics such as attitude and prior knowledge to advertising effectiveness. Recent theories of how advertising works have their roots in two-way models of communication based on the belief consumers are not inert, passive recipients of advertising. What’s important when individuals make decisions is not the last advert they were exposed to, but also what is remembered from previous advertising, past experience, and a lot of other information about the brand that the customer holds from other non-advertising communications out of the marketer’s control (Weilbacher, 2003). According to Weilbacher (2003), the human brain is complex enough to process a wide variety of incoming information, and, at any given moment, an individual perceives a limited amount of information that has been selected by the brain from all the incoming information fi'om the external environment. Exactly what an individual perceives depends on what the mind remembers from the past, as well as the relevance of this information. Weilbacher’s argument is in line with the theories of selective processes in that individuals choose what they do or do not attend to. These new developments in communication theory make advertising an even more complicated task for marketers. Marketers will find it more challenging to reach their customers, especially considering today’s consumer is bombarded with thousands of advertisements per day. 17 2.2 Does Advertising Really Work? A lot of studies have been conducted on advertising effectiveness using different methodologies. Most studies have been focused on consumer goods in general rather than on tourism and or destination marketing. The questions that most advertising and/or marketing departments and agencies have to answer are; does their expenditure on advertising result in more sales or in increased visits in case of tourism? Does an increase in the advertising budget, or an increase in the frequency and size of advertisements, result in more sales or visits? Responding to these questions has become even more important for government agencies that rely on public funds for their advertising budgets in these times of budget crunches. Simon and Amdt (1980) in their study on the shape of the advertising response function, concluded that there are no increasing returns to advertising and that diminishing returns characterize the shape of the advertising response function. Their study involved a review of over a hundred studies examining advertising effectiveness. The advertising response function they used referred to the quantitative relationship between advertising inputs such as monetary expenditure, size and fi'equency of advertisements and outputs such as sales, ad recall, attitudes and intention to buy. They proposed the advertising response function is a concave-downward function. In the concave firnction, there are diminishing returns based mainly in the micro-economic law of diminishing returns. Their argument for the concave shape is that, for an individual consumer, a given message conveys less and less information with each additional exposure. This finding 18 agrees with Krugrnan’s (1972) argument for the need for two or three exposures. He states the first exposure creates curiosity, the second one brings recognition and the third one clinches a decision while any extra ones have little value. This led to the theory that there are decreasing returns to frequency of advertising. In the S—shaped function, however, there are increasing returns first and then diminishing returns after an inflexion point is reached. Other researchers such as Chamberlin (1962) and Rao (1970) advocate for the S—shaped advertising response firnction. Their arguments for the S-shaped response function are based on the notion there exists a threshold effect. In support of the S-shaped advertising function, Chamberlin (1962) argued a consumer’s consciousness has to be gained first, and, while it is being gained, additional expenditure in advertising yields increasing returns. While both these functions show there are some returns to advertising, they are more related to consumer products, and their applicability to tourism advertising is not clear. Tourism is unique in that once a person has visited a destination, especially long haul destinations, they are not likely to visit the same place again, since long haul travel is typically not a repeat visit to a familiar location, but an exploration of new and unfamiliar parts of the world and experiences (Tourism New Zealand, 2005). Additional advertising in this case might not result in any positive effects. With consumer goods, however, when consumers are impressed alter the first purchase, they are likely to include that product on their short list of preferred brands and are also more likely to repurchase the same brand or product the next time they need it or if they run out of it (Wansink and Ray, 19 2000. the cc Advc inclu< (1972 such : Irelan effect effect Ehren rather alread 2000). In this case, repetitive exposure to advertising might serve a purpose of reminding the consumer of the brand. Advertising has been found to affect market share and firm sales in particular industries including the beer and cigarette industries (Palda, 1964; Peles, 1971). Barry and O’Hagan (1972) used log linear models to examine the effect of advertising and other variables such as income and price on the number of visits and expenditures by British tourists in Ireland. The advertising variable was found to be significant, implying advertising had an effect. Other studies such as the one by Uysal and Crompton (1984) have also shown the effectiveness of increased promotional expenditures on tourist arrivals and expenditures. Ehrenberg (2000), however, does not support this assumed power of advertising, but rather argues advertising’s role is mainly to reinforce feelings of satisfaction with brands already bought. This argument is consistent with the theory of cognitive dissonance which states that individuals selectively attend to advertisements of products they have already purchased in search of information that reassures them that they made the best decision hence reduce post-purchase dissonance. Ehrenberg (2000) argues advertising, though effective, is not as powerful as often thought and that there is no evidence that it works by any strong form of persuasion or manipulation. According to Ehrenberg (2000), advertising works in an Awareness-Tnal-Reinforcement (ATR) sequence. Consumers first gain awareness and then they make a trial purchase and finally a repeat buying habit may develop and be reinforced if the consumer is satisfied with the product. Even though advertising has a role in each stage of the purchase decision process, he argues repeat 20 buying is the main determinant of sales volume and advertising’s role is to reinforce rather than persuade. Ehrenberg also criticizes the traditional Awareness-Attitude- Behavior thinking of how advertising works. He argues peoples’ attitudes can’t be readily changed, and even if they change, there is no evidence to support that attitude change precedes desired purchase behavior. These arguments contrast with other researchers who have found attitude change is an intermediate measure of advertising effectiveness and predictor of end behavior (Lavidge and Steiner, 1961; F ishbein and Ajzen, 1975; Tan, 1985; Butterfield, Deal and Kubursi, 1998, Wansik and Ray, 2000). Other researchers (Schmalensee, 1972; Comanor and Wilson , 1974) joined Ehrenberg (2000) in expressing their skepticism about the effectiveness of advertising. They argue the strong correlation between advertising and sales could be a result of the effects of sales on advertising rather than advertising on sales as widely believed. Both Schmalensee (1972) and Comanor and Wilson (1974) conducted studies using simultaneous equations in which they included both effects, and they found no conclusive evidence that supported advertising had an effect on sales. Whether or not advertising is effective depends on one’s objectives. The important point to note is that it is vital that one considers the initial goal of running an advert when evaluating whether it has been effective or not. This not only ensures that the right conclusions are drawn, but it also enables accurate selection of proper methods to use in evaluating the effectiveness of the advert. For example, it is not fair to evaluate the effectiveness of an advert meant only to generate awareness on the basis of increased 21 sales or visits. The advert might not have resulted in increased sales but might have generated awareness of the product, brand or destination. In this case, if an appropriate method to measure change in awareness is not utilized, then the advert might be considered inefi’ective. Lavidge and Steiner (1961) gave examples as shown in Table l of appropriate research approaches to evaluate different kinds of advertising goals. Table 1: Advertising, Related Behavioral Dimensions and Advertising Research related to the Model Related Behavioral Movement Toward Examples of types of Examples of research Dimensions Purchase promotion/advertising approaches related to relevant to various steps steps of greatest applicability Pur hase Point of purchase Market or sales tests CONATIVE Retail store advertisements Split run tests -the realm of motives Deals Intention to purchase Advertisements ‘Last chance’ offers Projective techniques stimulate or direct sales Price appeals Conviction Testimonials Preference Rank order of preference A Competitive for brands AFFECTIVE advertisements Rating scales -the realm of emotions. Argumentative copy Image measurements Advertisements change including check attitudes and feelings Image advertisements lists and semantic Status, glamour appeals differentials Liking Projective techniques Knowledge Announcements Information questions COGNITIVE Descriptive copy Play-back analyses -the realm of thoughts Classified advertisements Brand awareness surveys Advertisements provide Slogans, Jingles Aided recall information and facts Sky writing Awareness Teaser campaigns Lavidge and Steiner (1961) According to the above model, there are different methods that can be used to evaluate the effectiveness of advertisements depending on the goal. For example, to create 22 awareness of a product, an advertiser can run a descriptive advert and then evaluate its effectiveness through aided recall research. It is, therefore, not appropriate to conduct market or sales tests meant to evaluate point of sale advertisements to determine the effectiveness of a descriptive advert whose objective was to create awareness. Each stage, according to Lavidge and Steiner, is important and relevant as it is supposed to progressively lead to the final point which is ‘purchase.’ It is important, therefore, to clearly state the objectives of advertising as well as to identify a suitable method to evaluate the effectiveness of the campaign prior to launching the advertising campaign. Doing so not only simplifies the evaluation process but also insures that the right conclusions concerning the campaign are drawn. 2.2 Travel Advertising Effectiveness Methods Review Many studies have been conducted on advertising effectiveness in general. However, not many have been conducted that deal specifically with travel advertising. Several methods have been utilized by different researchers to determine the effectiveness of travel advertising, not without limitations, however. Conversion and tracking studies have been the most popular approaches that have been used to determine the effectiveness of travel advertising (Woodside and Reid, 1974; Ballman et. al., 1984; Butterfield et. al.,1998; Zhou, 2000). Advertising is meant to achieve a wide range of different goals including but not limited to: creating awareness, reminding, changing attitudes and increasing sales and/or visits. Different methods and approaches can be utilized in addition to the tracking and 23 conversion models to determine if travel advertising is effective. The advertising tracking model evaluates advertising effectiveness through a series of stages from exposure to advertising to conversion behavior. The advertising tracking model assumes consumers may be converted by advertising solely on the basis of awareness and image building. The conversion model on the other hand, measures advertising effectiveness through a ‘funnel’ process through which consumers are assumed to go from exposure to the final goal of conversion. The conversion model process is linear, sequential,l and the group of consumers gets smaller as they get closer to conversion. Even though conversion and tracking studies have been the popular methods of evaluating travel advertising effectiveness, they have been criticized for a number of reasons. First, conversion models do not provide a quantitative figure on return on travel advertising investment (ROI). They can provide diagnostic information to decision makers. Therefore, the use of conversion models to produce an estimate of R01 is both misleading and misguided (Burke and Gitelson, 1990). Second, conversion studies suffer fi'om non-response bias, especially in mail studies (Woodside and Ronkainen, 1984; Ballman et. al., 1987). Third, the conversion model puts emphasis on inquiry fulfillment. According to the conversion model, inquiry is a necessary step in selling a destination and must occur prior to visitation (Siegel and Ziff-Levine, 1990). The model, therefore, assumes all visitors exposed to travel advertising must inquire for travel information before visiting. This is a major shortcoming of the model for which it has been criticized because not all advertising driven visitors inquire before visiting (Siegel and Ziff-Levine, 1990). Also, by making inquiry a requirement, the model underestimates the effects of 24 advertising. By making inquiry a necessary step, the conversion model also downplays the effects of advertising on creating destination awareness and creating positive destination image, the two stages that precede the inquiry fulfilhnent stage in the model. This model, therefore, is most useful in cases where the study’s objective is to measure the effect of travel advertising in generating inquiries and to provide diagnostic information on advertising effectiveness. The tracking model on the other hand does not require all advertising generated visitors fulfill the inquiry requirement. The tracking model assumes that advertising generated visitations do not depend on inquiry fulfillment. Visitors may or may not inquire before visitation. The belief is that consumers can be converted by advertising as a result of awareness and positive image (Siegel and Ziff-Levine, 1990). However, the tracking model is also not suitable when an ROI estimate is sought. The tracking model is suitable when the goal is to measure the effect of advertising on; generating advertising awareness, generating destination awareness, creating positive image, motivating consumers to visit the destination and influencing travel behavior (Siegel and Ziff- Levine, 1990). Linkage advertising has been suggested as one of the most effective when it comes to measuring the effects of advertising on sales. Linkage advertising makes it easy to evaluate the effects of advertising on sales because it links the up-front advertising to the sale (Rapp and Collins, 1987). Rapp and Collins (1987) argued too often advertising leaves the prospective consumer dangling without any idea what to do next, where to buy 25 or how to obtain additional information. Therefore, marketers should seek to bridge this gap between advertising and sale by providing more information through linkage advertising. While linkage advertising might work well for other industries its applicability to tourism and destination marketing is complicated. State and provincial destination marketers’ hands are tied because, as much as they are well aware of the main attractions in their regions, they are often forbidden to mention them due to political restrictions on highlighting individual cities and attractions. State tourism agencies, therefore, in their efforts to lure visitors find their advertising ludicrously vague about ‘why’ (Garfield, 1994). As much as linkage advertising is important in bridging the gap between advertising and purchase and is certainly suitable for evaluating the effect of advertising on sales, it has a shortcoming in that it fails to establish causal relationships between advertising and purchase (Woodside, Trappey and Macdonald, 1997). To address this problem, the use of true experiments is recommended. For an experiment to be considered true, it has to meet certain criteria including, presence of a control group and random assignment of subjects to the control and experimental groups (Trochim, 2001). Other researchers, such,as Raymond (1974), Caples (1974) and Woodside (1990), have advocated for the use of true experiments to determine the causal relationship between advertising and sales. The use of experiments to determine travel advertising effectiveness is, however, not very practical and is further complicated by the expenses associated with meeting the requirements of true experiments (Messmer and Johnson, 1993; Woodside et al., 1997). 26 Quasi-experiments have been used as an alternative to true experiments. Quasi- experiments are tests of the effects of changing levels of outcome variables caused by treatment variables when random assignment has not been used to create equivalent comparison groups (Woodside et al., 1997). Woodside et. al. (1997) used a quasi- experiment to measure the effectiveness of linkage destination advertising on tourist behavior and expenditures at Prince Edward Island, Canada. They chose the post-test only non-equivalent groups design with higher order interactions to determine with higher accuracy the cause-effect relationship between the advertisements (printed visitors guide) and the outcome behavior (expenditure). The results of their study showed linkage advertising had an indirect effect on expenditures through participation in more activities at the destination. Those individuals who had acquired the guide participated in more activities and spent more than those who had not acquired the guide. Use of the higher order interactions made it possible to establish the cause-effect interpretation. The results showed individuals with knowledge and experience participated in more activities at the destination. Regardless of the type of method utilized it is important to note all approaches have weaknesses and/or limitations. The problem with the supposedly better and more accurate methods is that their applicability to the travel and tourism field is complicated and costly to apply. It is, therefore, of particular importance that the objective of the campaign be clearly stated before initiating an assessment of advertisements so that an appropriate evaluation method can be employed. It is important also to recognize there is no single faultless method to evaluate advertising effectiveness. 27 Chapter 3 3.0 Methods This chapter discusses the methods that were used in this study. The study design is discussed first, then the data collection methods. A discussion on the techniques of data analysis concludes the chapter. 3.1 Study Design The study was a pre and post-test with no control group as depicted below: R 01 X 02 Where: R = Random selection of subjects 01 = Observation 1 (phase 1) 02 = Observation 2 (phase 2) X = Treatment (exposure to advertisements) The study was cross-sectional not panel, implying two different groups of respondents were randomly sampled in each wave. Since the study was measuring awareness a panel study would have been inappropriate, as the subjects would become sensitized. 3.2 Data collection Data for this study were collected through telephone interviews in 2003 from three Designated Marketing Areas (DMAs) including Chicago, Cleveland, and Indianapolis. The sampling frame included all residents of the three DMAs with a land-line telephone. The respondents’ phone numbers, which in this case also represented the study sample, 28 were purchased from Survey Sampling International, Inc. which drew random-digit—dial phone samples from the three DMAs. The data were collected in two waves. The first (pre-campaign) wave was conducted between April 8 and May 1, 2003 and the second (post-campaign) wave was conducted between July 7 and July 26 for Cleveland, between July 27 and August 26 for Indianapolis and between July 27 and September 2 for Chicago. The first wave was conducted simultaneously in the three DMAs, but the second wave was conducted at different times in each DMA depending on the time the advertisements were placed in the media. Even though the initial target of 1,800 completed surveys (300 per DMA per wave) was not reached, a total of 1,117 surveys were completed, enough to conduct reasonable analysis. The 900 target in the first wave was not reached mainly due to administrative reasons. The final contract was agreed upon and signed too late to have allowed more pre-campaign surveys to be completed. The time between the beginning of the survey and the launching of the campaigns in the three DMAs was not long enough to allow more surveys to be completed. However, the complete second wave target of 900 was met. The survey instrument was a questionnaire consisting of 37 questions that were identical across the three DMAs except for the questions that identified and described the different specific advertisements that were run in each DMA (refer to Appendix 1 for the full questionnaire). A complete interview lasted approximately 10 minutes and there were no incentives offered to the respondents for participating in the study. 29 Three different communication channels were utilized for the 2003 travel advertising campaign including Television, radio and magazine. The specific content of the advertisements varied among the three DMAs and the channel of communication. Appendix 3 gives a detailed description of the advertisements by channel and DMA. Advertising Awareness was measured at two levels by measuring unaided and aided recall of Michigan advertising. For unaided awareness respondents were asked to name the states for which they recalled seeing or hearing any vacation or travel advertising within the past six months. The following question was asked to prompt unaided recall, ‘For which states or provinces do you recall seeing or hearing any vacation or travel advertising in the past six months?’ If the respondent mentioned seeing or hearing any Michigan advertising, they were then asked to describe in general what the advertising said and what message they thought it was conveying. A nominal measure was used to measure unaided recall coded as; 1 if they recalled seeing or hearing Michigan advertising and 0 if not. To measure ‘aided recall’ the interviewer would describe a specific advert and then ask whether or not the respondent recalled seeing or hearing that particular advert in the past six months. The following question was asked to prompt unaided recall, ‘ Do you recall seeing or hearing any vacation or travel advertising for. . .in the past six months?’ respondents were also asked to state whether or not they had heard and/or seen specific television, radio and magazine advertisements that the interviewer read and described for them. A nominal scale was also used to measure aided recall coded as; 1 if they recalled seeing or hearing Michigan advertising and 0 if not. 30 Destination Awareness was also measured nominally, 1 if Michigan was mentioned as one of the states that come to mind when thinking of taking a pleasure trip and 0 if not. The following two questions were asked to determine respondents’ awareness of Michigan as a tourism destination; 1. ‘When you are thinking of taking a pleasure trip in the US. or Canada what cities, states or provinces come to mind?’ 2. ‘Now thinking just about a pleasure trip in the Mid-west what cities or states come to mind? There were several other variables that were measured at different levels using different scales as shown in Table 2. 31 basin ”25A seal :85 “8885 $8 .8 c.0803 0:80TN 0:8"— 1.880 Z 8088005 0800 anm 808 UN anmefivm n_ vam n 8 v no 3:on 008 8 8080008 ”—0880 80809088 0885 v.0» H_ 3808 2 «men 05 8 o: Ho 0% D03 8288 3050 P033 08 8053 m8 080 8: 8 08055 ”—8802 8080385 08 9.0? m0» H_ 888888 #028 80% m0.» no 0868 3 08— 0:28 00b :8 90003 08 wow: 08 “on 8 08055 ”—8.882 80808005 885 “echo 088“me 3880 2.200% 88 23M .>.H 88208 20880 05 .8 8a 8988 0808080038 :2 85 awn—85 we .8 0550"— w880: 8 8000 0:308 808338 a «o: .5 8050:? 3 808308 ”108802 8080305 #0880 w8m80>v< m0>H_ 0noeno 08809 88 08808 a no :2 00:?» m8 080 8: no 05055 ”38802 8080908: 00:0t0axm :03 boo—=8 088 Mm £808 fi 808 808308 Cr: 08% 05> 308608 53 H_ 05 8 88 08.80— m co 88m 05 28> 2 £808 080 8: co 805055 ”—88.5 0% 808039: 05 88> 3 82808— 858080 +_ 86.8260 0co=no 888080 H2 88: no 8000 m8 080 0088 .8 8388 08 .3 808808 noted 80809005 8 088%.”.— 3203800 00& 80808808 me $32 805 98 808.808 83.88; ”m 03a. 32 3.3 Data analysis techniques used Data analysis was conducted in stages to fiJlly address each hypothesis and meet the objectives of the study. The first stage of data analysis included descriptive analysis where the general characteristics of the data were described. Demographic information for respondents was presented in tables and charts at this stage. The second stage of analysis included some pre and post advertisement (phases 1 and 2) comparisons to determine if there were any significant differences in advertising awareness before and after exposure to advertising. The Mann-Whitney U test was employed at this stage to detect any significant differences in advertising awareness between phases 1 and 2. The Mann-Whitney U test was used as it is more appropriate for nominal variables than the T-test. This was an important step in the analysis because it was the first test of the effectiveness of the advertising campaign. If no significant differences were found in advertising awareness before and after exposure, then any further analysis would not be meaningful. This step, therefore, determined if Travel Michigan’s travel advertising was successful in breaking through all the clutter and getting the audience’s attention. The third stage of analysis included testing the effectiveness of Travel Michigan’s advertising on; creating destination awareness, improving people’s attitude towards Michigan as a tourism destination, creating some motivation and intention to visit the state, and increasing visitation to the state. A T-test was employed in this stage of analysis. After having managed to capture the audience’s attention, this stage of analysis 33 sought to determine if there were any significant differences in the above mentioned variables between those aware and those not aware of advertising. Next, factor analysis was conducted to reduce the 22 attitude towards Mi as a tourism destination factors to a smaller more manageable set to be used in subsequent analyses. Principal components analysis instead of common factor analysis was used as the extraction method because the focus was on both data reduction and total variance not just common variance. The next stage of analysis involved correlation analysis to determine the kind of relationship between attitudes towards Michigan as a tourism destination, distance of the DMA from Michigan, annual income and advertising awareness, destination awareness and intention to visit the state. The Spearman’s rho correlation test was utilized at this stage because most of the variables tested were either nominal or ordinal. The attitude factors retained from the prior principal components analysis were utilized at this stage. The sixth stage in the analytical framework that was employed was the model estimation stage in which two models were estimated. The dependent variables for these two models were; 1) Advertising Awareness (AA) and 2) Destination Awareness (DA). Logistic regression was used for the AA and DA models since the variables were binary, bound by O and 1. The forward greatest reduction in Log Likelihood value (-2LL value) method was used to enter variables into the logistic model. The independent variables hypothesized for the AA model included: exposure, TV, radio, magazine, attitudes, 34 experience, location, income, intention to visit (IV), age and gender. Independent variables used in the DA model included: IV, exposure, location, web-use, experience, and AA. In the next stage of analysis, the real conversion ratios for the 2003 Travel Michigan advertising campaign were calculated by use of the conversion and tracking models by Siegel and Ziff-Levine (1990) which were presented in figure 3. The final stage of analysis for this study involved the calculation of the return on advertising investment for the 2003 Travel Michigan’s advertising campaign. This stage utilized the conversion figures derived from the previous stage of analysis, 2003 advertising expenditure figures from Travel Michigan, and tourist expenditure figures from the household survey conducted by the Michigan Travel, Tourism and Recreation Resources Center of the Department of Parks, Recreation and Tourism Resources at Michigan State University. Advertesing . _ . Destination Target audience Advertismg Awareness Awareness of advertising f Ar Motivation . . ' Positive , Destination awareness Advertising image of x" + Awareness destination ,v’ Positive lmagepf destination x’ Inquiry fulfillment , * Inquiry C . . . Fulfillment onverSlon Motivation ------ p behavior Conversion behavior (A) (B) Figure 3: (A) Conversion Model (B) Tracking model (Adapted from Siegel and Ziff-Levine, 1990) 35 Chapter 4 4.0 Results and Discussion This chapter covers the findings of the study. The descriptives are discussed first followed by the pre and post campaign comparison results. Hypotheses testing results are discussed next, followed by the conversion estimation results. The chapter is concluded by a discussion of the results on the estimation of actual return on advertising investment. 4.] Descriptives There were a total of 1117 respondents for this study of which 209 were interviewed in the pre-advertisement phase (phase 1) and 908 were interviewed in the post advertisement phase (phase 2). The reason for this major discrepancy is mainly technical. The contract for the project was not approved until very late in phase 1 and real data collection did not start until there were only three weeks before the advertisements started running in the three DMAs. As a result the target of at least 300 interviews per DMA per phase originally set was not met. Since the two pre- and post campaign samples were cross-sectional and random no adj ustments were made during the analyses to reduce the post campaign sample size to match the pre-campaign one. Despite this limitation, the sample was large enough to enable reasonable analyses and comparisons to be conducted. The study was cross-sectional not panel implying the pre and post groups were different but each was randomly selected. Since the study was measuring awareness a panel would not have been appropriate, as the subjects would become sensitized. 36 There were more female respondents than there were male. Of the 1117 respondents, 62% were female and 38 % were male. Of the 209 phase 1 respondents, 43% were male and 57% were female. For phase 2, 37% were male and 63% were female. The average age of the respondents was 46 years. Distribution of respondents in phases 1 and 2 combined among the three DMAs was almost evenly distributed with each DMA representing about a third of the respondents as can be seen in figure 4. Indianapolis 34% Chicago 32% — Cleveland 34% Figure 4: Distribution of Respondents among the three DMAs A comparison of gender among the three DMAs showed there were more female respondents than there were male in each DMA, and the proportion of females to males was exactly the same for Chicago and Indianapolis and slightly different for Cleveland 37 (see Table 3). Other respondent characteristics and demographics such as income, household size and ethnicity varied across the three DMAs (Refer to appendix 3). Table 3: Gender * Number of respondents by their residence DMA DMA of Residence Total Chicago Cleveland Indianapolis Gender Male 134 146 146 426 Female 222 229 238 689 Total 356 375 384 1115 Male % 38% 39% 38% 38% Female % 62% 61% 62% 62% Total % 100% 100% 100% 100% *Gender was not recorded for two Chicago DMA respondents 4.2 Pre and Post Advertisement Comparisons 4.2.1 Advertising Awareness The first stage of analysis was meant to determine whether Travel Michigan’s advertising drew the attention of its intended audience, by testing whether there was any significant difference in Advertising Awareness before and afier the advertisements were placed. Without people being aware of the advertising in the first place, any further tests to determine the effectiveness of the advertising would not have been meaningful. The Mann-Whitney U non-parametric test was conducted to determine if there were any significant differences in Advertising Awareness (AA) between phases 1 and 2 of the advertising campaign. A non-parametric test was used because it is a more appropriate method when working with nominal data than is the T-test. Since AA was measured nominally (0: not aware; I: aware) a non-parametric test was appropriate. The results showed a significant difference in Advertising Awareness (U: 72830, p< .01) between phases 1 and 2. What this result shows is that people were aware of Travel Michigan’s advertising after the advertisements were run. The advertising had the initial effect of 38 catching people’s attention. This is a good starting point in the evaluation of the effectiveness of advertising. As illustrated in the models of communication in the preceding chapter, the initial goal of effective advertising is to grab the attention of consumers. The AIDA (Attention, Interest, Desire and Action), Hierarchy of Effects and the Richardson-Haley models indicate getting the attention of the consumer, getting them to even pay attention, is the first and foremost important step in effective communication, let alone persuasion. Getting the attention of the consumer is a necessary though not sufficient condition for effective advertising. Significant differences were also found in Destination Awareness (DA) (U=48229; p < .01) between phases 1 and 2. 4.3 Hypothesis Testing This study’s objective of determining the effectiveness of Travel Michigan’s advertising follows the conversion and tracking models suggested by Siegel and Ziff-Levine (1990) as illustrated in Figure 3 that was presented earlier. For this study, advertising effectiveness was measured in several ways including Advertising Awareness (AA), Destination Awareness (DA), Attitudes, Intention to Visit the state (IV) and Actual Visitation (AV). This stage of analysis focuses on determining the effectiveness of Travel Michigan’s advertising on creating awareness of Michigan as a tourism destination, improving people’s attitudes/image of the state as a tourism destination, developing motivation to visit the state among the targeted advertisement audiences and stimulating change in consumptive behavior once motivation is developed. This stage of analysis therefore addresses the following hypotheses: 39 Ha]: Individuals exposed to Travel Michigan advertising will have greater awareness of the state as a tourism destination, than those who have not been exposed. Hazz Travel Michigan advertising will have a positive effect on changing people’s attitudes towards the state as a tourism destination. Ha3: Individuals exposed to Travel MI advertising will have greater motivation/ intention to visit the state on a leisure trip, than those who have not been exposed. Ha4: Travel Michigan’s advertising is effective in generating visits for the state. After determining the effectiveness of the advertisements on grabing the attention of the consumer, the next step involved testing whether the advertisements had any effect as measured by the four variables (DA, IV, AV and Attitudes) described above. Independent samples T—tests were conducted to test whether there were any significant differences in the four measures of advertising effectiveness between those aware and those not aware of advertising. Intention to Visit (IV), Destination Awareness (DA) and Actual Visitation (AV) were tested first because they were each measured by one variable. Destination Awareness (DA) was measured nominally (O=unaware; 1=aware). Those considered as being aware of Michigan as a tourism destination were the respondents who indicated that Michigan comes to mind when they are thinking of taking a pleasure trip in the United States and Canada. Intention to Visit (IV) was measured through a 5-point ordinal scale (I: definitely visit; 5=very unlikely visit) and Actual Visitation (AV) was measured on a ratio scale by the number of visits an individual has made to the state. Attitude on the other hand was measured by a number of variables that were meant to capture the 40 different attitudes/perceptions that consumers have towards Michigan’s different tourism industry sectors. Each variable was measured on a five-point ordinal scale (1=disagree completely; 5=agree completely). The results presented in Table 4 show that there are significant differences in DA, IV and AV between those who were aware of advertising and those who were not. Table 4: T-tests for equality of means for DA, IV and AV between those aware and those not aware of advertisements AA N Mean Mean difference Std. Deviation t Sig. DA Not aware 377 .17 .15 .376 5.332 .000 Aware 727 .32 .466 IV Not aware 365 3.30 .41 1.661 3.909 .000 Aware 712 2.89 1.635 AV Not aware 376 .69 .61 1.512 3.713 .000 Aware 726 1.30 3.002 DA: 0 not aware, 1 aware; IV: 1 definitely visit, 5 very unlikely visit; AV number of times one has visited the state within the past 12 months. What the results imply is that Travel Michigan’s advertising had a significant effect on generating awareness of the state as a tourism destination (DA) (t = 5.332; p < .001), generating motivation to visit the state (IV) (t = 3.909; p < .001) as well generating more visits to the state (AV) (t = 3.713; p < .001). Advertising was also found to have a significant effect on changing people’s attitudes towards Michigan as a tourism destination. All the attitude variables were individually tested to see if there were any significant differences between those individuals that were aware of Michigan advertising and those that were not, and significant differences were found as can be seen in Table 5. The results clearly show that there are significant differences in attitudes/ perceptions of people between those aware and those not aware of Travel Michigan’s advertising. People consistently rank Michigan better afier being 41 exposed to the advertisements, than when not. Changing consumer attitudes is an important step in advertising because advertising is understood to influence behavior indirectly through its direct effect on customers’ attitudes (Butterfield, Deal and Kubursi, 1998). Table 5: T-tests results for Attitudes towards Michigan as a tourism destination Mean Diff. Std. AA N Mean“ A-Un Dev t. Sig. MI has great restaurants Unaware 299 3.49 .34 1.160 4.694 .000 Aware 618 3.83 M1 has lovely small towns Unaware 302 3.66 .42 1.117 6.038 .000 Aware 652 4.08 MI has great shopping Unaware 275 3.31 .32 1.187 3.998 .000 opportunities Aware 576 3.63 MI °ffers 3“ excellent mam" value Unaware 209 3.52 .34 1.209 4.031 .000 for the money Aware 538 3.86 MI has many interesting historic sites Unaware 293 3.41 .41 1.177 5.392 .000 Aware 626 3 .81 MI is a good place for family vacation Unaware 319 3.65 .50 1.177 7.113 .000 Aware 686 4.15 MI offers exciting nightlife and Unaware 261 3.15 .27 1.213 3.108 .002 entertainment Aware 526 3.41 MI is a good place to meet friendly Unaware 312 3.49 .31 1.151 4.321 .000 people Aware 658 3.80 MI is great for winter outdoor Unaware 291 3.47 .38 1.332 4.398 .000 recreation activities Aware 598 3.84 MI offers much scenic appeal Unaware 326 3.69 .44 1.195 6.387 .000 Aware 692 4.13 MI has a lot of high quality lodging Unaware 263 3.28 .44 1.150 5.610 .000 Aware 543 3.72 42 Table 5 continued MI is an exciting place to visit MI is great for summer outdoor recreation activities MI is close enough for a weekend getaway There is plenty for me to see and do on a MI getaway tn'p MI is a great place to relax and unwind Vacationing in M1 is a great value for my money MI is great for enjoying quality time with family or friends MI has great beaches MI is great for short getaway trips MI is a great place to escape from my daily routine MI is a great vacation destination for the whole family Unaware Aware Unaware Aware Unaware Aware Unaware Aware Unaware Aware Unaware Aware Unaware Aware Unaware Aware Unaware Aware Unaware Aware Unaware Aware 323 684 324 687 344 709 324 688 327 696 284 615 328 696 264 576 345 708 339 706 330 700 3.30 3.69 3.70 4.19 3.88 4.25 3.49 4.00 3.49 3.96 3.42 3.84 3.57 4.05 3.21 3.70 3.61 4.14 3.23 3.73 3.53 4.10 .39 .49 .37 .51 .47 .41 .49 .49 .53 .50 .58 1.243 1.173 1.212 1.248 1.177 1.144 1.192 1.345 1.253 1.268 1.193 5.107 7.310 5.218 6.902 6.556 5.310 6.767 5.369 7.264 6.340 8.080 .000 .000 .000 .000 .000 .000 .000 .000 .000 .000 .000 *Attitude was measured via a 5 point scale with 5 equal to ‘agree completely’ and 1 equal to ‘disagree completely. ’ The next step in the analysis framework included factor analysis of the attitude variables in an effort to reduce the data as well as determine if there were any similarities among the many attitude variables. The Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin (KMO) measure of sampling adequacy and the Bartlett’s test for sphericity were conducted to test the suitability of the 43 data set for factor analysis. The KMO value was 0.946 greater than the required minimum Of 0.6 and the Bartlett’s test was significant at the .01 level indicating that the data set was suitable for factor analysis (refer to Appendix 4). The R-type factor analysis was utilized since the relationship being studied included variables not cases. The extraction method used was principal components analysis because the objectives were data reduction as well as determination of total variance not only common variance. Varimax orthogonal rotation was used to make interpretation of the results easier as well as to redistribute the variance from the earlier components in-order to produce a more meaningful component pattern. Varimax rotation was selected mainly because it is the best choice when the goal is to reduce the number of the original variables to a smaller set of uncorrelated variables to be used in subsequent analyses. The factors created from this stage of the analysis were to be used later for regression analysis, therefore making Varimax rotation the more suitable choice among orthogonal rotation methods. The results gave a total of 22 components with different variables loading differently on each of the 22 components as illustrated in Table 6. The initial extraction and rotated results varied, with the variation more evenly distributed with rotation than without as shown in Tables 6 and 7. 44 Table 6: Principal components anal sis results showingloading of each variable on the components Un-rotated Component Rotated Component Communality Matrix Matrix Variable /Components 1 2 3 1 2 3 MI is a great vacation destinanon for .812 ”297 .076 199 .202 .296 .753 the whole fanuly MI is a great place to escape from my .769 -. 144 .024 $3 .309 .278 .612 daily routine MI is a great for short getaway trips .681 .115 .492 .373 .224 ,_7_§ .719 MI has great beaches .684 -.257 —.131 £22 .257 .077 .552 MI is great for enjoying quality time .767 -.l79 -.002 £88 .297 .245 .621 with family or friends Vacatiomng in MI 15 a great value for .777 -.O79 .188 £01 .282 .443 .645 my money MI is a great place to relax and .835 -.093 .049 fl .369 .340 .708 unwind There is plenty for me to see and do .817 -.001 .005 £11 .444 .320 .668 on a MI getaway trip MI is close enough for a weekend .566 .308 .632 .144 .229 L1 .814 getaway MI ‘5 3m" f0? 3.9mm“ ”mm” .798 -.188 .118 E .251 .359 .686 recreation act1v1t1es MI is an exciting place to visit .804 .103 -.085 .538 $1 .267 .665 M1 has a lot of high quality lodging .747 .193 -.234 .454 .6_52_ .138 .650 Ml offers much scenic appeal .819 —.211 -.032 fig .316 .230 .716 M1 is great for winter outdoor .592 -.129 -.231 £51 .342 -.009 .420 recreation activities MI is a good place to meet friendly .746 .078 -.045 E .483 .271 .564 people MI offers exciting nightlife and .635 .562 -.138 .115 ._§O_2 .284 .738 entertainment MI is a good place for family .843 -.232 .004 ,7_78_ .298 .264 .764 vacation MI has many interesting historic sites .775 .084 -.215 .544 J; .135 .654 Ml offers an excellent vacation value .803 .067 .125 ,_5_3_Z .428 .439 .665 for the money MI has great shopping opportunities .749 .343 -. 190 .350 £31 .221 .714 Ml has lovely small towns .775 -. 103 —.O99 £31 .398 .184 .620 M1 has great restaurants .776 .268 -.202 .422 ._'Z_(_)_§ .200 .715 45 The latent root criterion was used to select the components to retain. With the latent root criterion, only those components with eigenvalues greater than one are retained. Only three components were retained in this case. The eigenvalues were 12.598, 1.062 and 1.001 for components 1, 2 and 3 respectively. The rest of the components had eigenvalues of less than one (see Table 7), therefore were not retained for further analysis. 46 Table 7: Principal Components Analysis results showing eigenvalues for the components and % of variation explained by each component Compo Initial Eigenvalues Un-rotated matrix Loadings Rotated matrix Loadings “em E.value % of Cumu E.value % of Cumu E.value % of Cumu Variance lative% Variance lative% Variance lative% 1 12.598 57.265 57.265 12.598 57.265 57.265 7.414 33.702 33.702 2 1.062 4.828 62.093 1.062 4.828 62.093 4.501 20.461 54.163 3 1.001 4.552 66.645 1.001 4.552 66.645 2.746 12.482 66.645 4 .765 3.479 70.124 5 .672 3.056 73.180 6 .595 2.707 75.887 7 .532 2.420 78.307 8 .520 2.362 80.669 9 .457 2.077 82.746 10 .434 1.972 84.718 11 .381 1.733 86.451 12 .376 1.710 88.161 13 .354 1.608 89.769 14 .325 1.479 91.249 15 .302 1.373 92.621 16 .288 1.307 93.929 17 .260 1.183 95.112 18 .258 1.171 96.283 19 .238 1.083 97.366 20 .223 1.015 98.381 21 .192 .872 99.253 22 .164 .747 100.000 Extraction Method: Principal Component Analysis. In the initial un-rotated extraction, component I explained about 57% of the total variation of all the variables, and components 2 and 3 accounted for only 4.8% and 4.5% 47 each respectively. Together the three components accounted for 66.6% of the total variation as shown in Table 7. Examination of the individual variables and the components shows that all but one (MI is great for winter outdoor recreation) variable had a communality value of less than the critical value of .5. The rest of the variables have more than 50% of their variation being explained by the 3 components. The results also clearly show that in the un-rotated solution all the variables load significantly on the first component, and the first component accounts for 57% of the 66.6 % total variation with the other two components accounting for the small remainder. The rotated results, however, show an even distribution of the variance. The total variance explained remains 66.6% but is evenly distributed. Components 1, 2 and 3 each account for 33.7%, 20.5% and 12.5% of the variation respectively. The variables also load heavily on only one of the three components making it a lot easier to interpret the results. For naming the variables only those variables with factor loadings equal to or greater than .5 were considered under each factor. In cases where one variable had two or more components on which it loaded greater than .5, only the greater one was considered. The third component was the easiest to name as it had only two variables with greater than .5 factor loadings; these were ‘MI is close enough for a weekend getaway,’ and ‘MI is great for short getaway trips,’ with loadings of .861 and .728 respectively. This component appears to represent perceptions of people regarding how close Michigan is for short getaways or weekend trips. This component was named; ‘MI is close enough for short getaways.’ The second component had six variables significantly loading on it, which 48 appear to revolve around aspects of entertainment. The variable that had the highest loading on this component was; ‘MI offers exciting nightlife and entertainment,’ with a factor loading of .802 (refer to Table 6). The component was named; ‘MI offers great entertainment.’ The first and most important component had sixteen variables with factor loadings greater than .5; however, two were excluded from this component because they loaded even higher on the second component (see Table 6). Component 1 appears to revolve around perceptions of people towards Michigan as a fatnily destination with family oriented activities and entertainment. This component is the most important of the three as it accounts for over half of the total 66.6% variation. The results suggest that people view Michigan as a good tourism and vacation destination for the family. The variable that loaded most heavily on this component with a .79 factor loading was; ‘MI is a great vacation destination for the whole family.’ While both components 1 and 2 have entertainment and activities variables significantly loading on each of them, the kinds of activities and entertainment differ. Component 2 represents mainly the kinds of activities for the single adults or couples without children such as night entertainment, shopping and dining while component 1 represents the activities and entertainment for adults with children such as outdoor recreational activities. The value variables, ‘MI offers an excellent vacation value for the money,’ and ‘Vacationing in MI is a great value for my money,’ both loaded on this component. Price sensitivity is a trait more associated with families with children than with adults traveling without children. Families with children most often have less discretionary income, thus look for ways to stretch their dollars to accommodate the 49 needs of the entire family on their limited travel budgets. This component therefore, was named; ‘MI is a great family destination.’ The next stage of analysis involved correlation analysis to address the following hypotheses: Has: Attitudes towards Michigan as a tourism destination will be positively correlated to Advertising Awareness (AA), Destination Awareness (DA) and Intention to Visit (IV). Ha6: Distance of DMA to Michigan will be negatively correlated to AA, DA and IV. Ha7: Annual income will be positively correlated to AA, DA and IV. These hypotheses are based mainly on the persuasion theories such as the H—J-K theory of communication persuasion which state that the success of communication and persuasion also depend on several audience factors. The initial position of the audience affects the extent to which members comprehend the message they are being exposed to. The initial position includes such factors as personality, socio-economic status, intelligence and others as depicted earlier in Figure one (Tan, 1985). According to cognitive dissonance theory and selective processes theory, people attend to incoming messages selectively, depending on their backgrounds. People with low income or those who reside further from Michigan might selectively avoid Travel Michigan’s advertising. These hypotheses, therefore, intend to examine the relationship that these audience factors have with advertising awareness (AA), destination awareness (DA), and intention to visit (IV). 50 The non-parametric Spearman’s rho correlation test was used since most of the variables being tested were either nominal or ordinal. The parametric tests such as the Pearson correlation are more suited for interval and/or ratio data. The three factors retained from the previous factor analysis procedure were used at this stage to represent the attitudes variables. The results showed that attitudes/perceptions towards Michigan as a tourism destination are significantly positively correlated with both Advertising Awareness and Intention to Visit the state as shown in Table 8. Correlation between one of the attitude variables (MI offers great entertainment) and Destination Awareness (DA), however, was not significant. Table 8: Correlation Results between Attitude Factors and AA, DA and IV (Spearman’s rho correlation) Attitude factor Advertising Awareness Destination Awareness Intention to Visit N ariable Coefficient Sig. (2- Coefficient Sig. (2- Coefficient Sig. (2- tailed tailed) tailed) MI is a great family destination .266“ .000 .204" .000 .225" .000 Ml offers great entertainment .101 * .039 .096 .050 .210" .000 MI is close enough .l49"“'I .002 .133" .006 .157" .000 for short getaways ** Correlation is significant at the 0.011evel (2-tailed); * Correlation is significant at the 0.05 level (2- tailed). The results show the attitudes people have towards Michigan as a tourism destination are positively related to their awareness of advertising, destination and intention to visit the state. The attitudes towards Michigan as a family destination are more highly correlated 51 with all three variables (i.e., AA, DA and IV), than are the other two attitude factors. This indicates advertising awareness increases as people’s attitudes towards Michigan as a family destination improve and vice versa. This implies that for Travel Michigan’s advertising to get more attention, it should most importantly focus on improving its image as a family destination. Both Destination Awareness and Intention to travel are also positively correlated with attitudes towards Michigan as a suitable family tourism destination. As people’s attitudes about Michigan as a family destination improve, so does their awareness of Michigan as a destination and intention to visit the state. Attitude towards Michigan as a close destination for short trips was the next most highly correlated variable with AA, DA and IV with correlation coefficients of .149, .133, and .157 respectively. People attend more to and become more aware of advertising as they view Michigan as a suitable destination for short or weekend getaways. This result is consistent with other research that has been conducted which reveals that Americans now take more weekend and short getaways, than they did ten years ago (TIA, 2003). This may also be attributable to current and most recent world events such as 9/11 that resulted in a reduction in long haul trips as people would rather stay closer to home and visit destinations to which they can drive instead of flying. Therefore, destination marketers such as Travel Michigan need to be positioned to attract those markets closer to home. Attitudes towards Michigan as a great destination for adults without children were the least positively correlated with all three: Advertising Awareness (AA), Destination 52 Awareness (DA) and Intention to Visit (IV). In-fact, the correlation between Destination Awareness (DA) and ‘MI offers great adult entertainment’ was not significant as shown in Table 8. The results show the relationship between people’s attitudes towards Michigan as having great adult entertainment and both Destination Awareness and Intention to Visit, though positive is not as strong as the relationship between Advertising Awareness and Destination Awareness with the family and distance attitude factors. The results imply that in crafting its advertising messages Travel Michigan needs to incorporate the suitability of the state as a destination for both the family and short getaways in-order to increase the audience’s attention, destination awareness and motivation to visit the state. Some of the relationships hypothesized in Ha, and Ha7 were supported by results from the analyses conducted, while others were not. Table 9: Correlation results for Annual Income, DMA location and AA, DA, and IV Advertising Awareness Destination Awareness Intention to Visit Spearman Coefficient Sig. (2- Coefficient Sig. (2- Coefficient Sig. (2- Correlation tailed) tailed) tailed) Annual .088“ .009 .045 .184 .131" .000 Income DMA -.056 .064 -.64* .033 -.083** .006 location ** Correlation is significant at the 0.011evel; *Correlation is significant at the 0.05 level. The results presented in Table 9 support the hypothesized relationship that income is positively correlated with advertising awareness, indicating that as people’s incomes increase they tend to be more aware of vacation or travel advertising. This agrees with the theories of cognitive dissonance and selective processes that individuals actively select 53 messages that they have the financial means to pursue and avoid those messages for products they can’t afford to purchase. Individuals with low income who can’t afford to take vacations, might consciously avoid travel advertising messages. On the other hand, those with higher incomes, because they can afford to take vacations and leisure trips might consciously attend to travel advertising in search of information to use in planning their trips. Annual income also has a significant positive relationship with Intention to Visit the state as was hypothesized. This relationship, however, is more obvious than not, as people’s incomes increase so do their intentions to visit the state on a vacation or leisure trip. It follows Engel’s law which states that as people’s incomes increase the proportion they spend on basic commodities such as food and shelter decreases and the proportion they spent on luxuries increases (Asimakopulos, 1978). Results also indicate that the relationships between DMA location and both Destination Awareness and Intention to Visit were as hypothesized. Results presented in Table 9 show that Intention to travel to the state of Michigan for leisure decreases as the distance from Michigan increases. This again, corroborates the fact that people are more inclined to take shorter than longer trips. People’s intentions to visit the state of Michigan decrease as we move from nearby Cleveland to Chicago to Indianapolis. The further away people reside from Michigan the less likely they are to take pleasure trips to Michigan. However, no relationship was found between DMA location and awareness of Travel Michigan’s advertising (see Table 9). Even though the hypothesis has been disproved, this is, however, a good result for Travel Michigan because it means that its advertising was consistently and equally received across the three DMAs. 54 4.3.1 Model Estimation The next stage of analysis involved estimation of two models including AA and DA through logistic regression analysis. AA and DA were estimated through logistic regression, as they are both dichotomous variables bound between 0 and 1. This stage of analysis sought to address the following hypotheses: Hag: Having taken a pleasure trip to Michigan before (Experience) will have an effect on advertising awareness. Hag: Advertising channel has a significant effect on advertising awareness. 4.3.1.1 Advertising Awareness The following Unaided Advertising Awareness (UAA) model was hypothesized: UAA = f(X1,....,X,,) where: UAA = Unaided Advertising Awareness (X1...Xn) = (Exposure, TV, Radio, Magazine, Attitudes, Experience, Location, Annual Income, IV, Age and Gender) The forward greatest reduction in Log Likelihood value (-2LL value) method was used to enter variables into the logistic regression model. The variables were entered in six steps with Exposure, TV, Radio, Age, Experience and Attitudes entered at steps 1, 2, 3, 4, 5, and 6, respectively. The p—value for entering into the model was set at .05. Results are presented in Table 10 for the variables that met the minimum criterion established to enter the model. The variables that didn’t meet this criterion were: location, IV, annual income, magazine and the three attitude factors which included: 1) MI as a great family 55 destination, 2). MI as having great adult entertainment and 3) MI close enough for short getaways. Table 10: Variables in the Unaided AdvertisiniAwareness (UAA) Model Step Variable B S.E. Wald“ Sig. Exp (B) Step 1 Exposure .066 .006 127.727 .000 1.068 Constant 5.214 .568 84.115 .000 183.756 Step 2 Exposure .066 .006 124.026 .000 1.068 TV 1.304 .236 30.556 .000 3.682 Constant 4.660 .573 66.094 .000 105.607 Step 3 Age .030 .007 17.336 .000 1.030 Exposure .067 .006 126.528 .000 1.069 TV 1.308 .242 29.317 .000 3.698 Constant 3.348 .646 26.832 .000 28.451 Step 4 Age .030 .007 17.490 .000 1.031 Exposure .067 .006 127.448 .000 1.070 Experience .745 .245 9.267 .002 2.106 TV 1.227 .245 25.083 .000 3.410 Constant 3.096 .654 22.397 .000 22.107 Step 5 Age .029 .007 15.342 .000 1.029 Gender .764 .268 8.1 1 1 .004 2.147 Exposure .068 .006 128.731 .000 1.070 Experience .759 .247 9.419 .002 2.135 TV 1.217 .248 24.174 .000 3.379 Constant 1.974 .761 6.732 .009 7.198 Step 6 Age .031 .008 17.348 .000 1.032 Gender .761 .270 7.925 .005 2.141 Exposure .067 .006 125 .307 .000 1.069 Experience .738 .249 8.764 .003 2.093 Radio .693 .274 6.418 .01 1 1.999 TV 1.041 .258 16.248 .000 2.831 Constant 1.632 .777 4.406 .036 5.113 *Assesses the significance of each variable entered in the model The final model is as follows: UAA = 5.11 + 2.83TV + 2.14 Gender + 2.09Experience + 2.00Radio + 1.07Exposure + 1.03Age 56 According to all the Goodness of fit tests run including the R2 values, Hosmer and Lemeshow Test and step Chi-square tests presented in Table 11 the model is a good fit. The Nagelkerke R2 value for the model is .81, implying that the final model explains an estimated 81% of the variation in Unaided Advertising Awareness. The Hosmer and Lemeshow test measures the overall model fit, and the statistic indicates that there was no statistically significant difference between the observed and predicted outcomes (Hair et. al., 1998). The higher the p-value the better the model fit since a higher p-value implies that there are no significant differences between the predicted and observed model. The Omnibus test shown in Table 11 measures the significance of model coefficients at each step, and they are all significant for this model. The Wald statistic assesses the significance of each variable entered in the model, and each one of the six variables entered is significant at the .01 level except for the constant. All the goodness of fit measures support the six-variable model stated above. Table 11: Goodness of fit Tests for the Unaided AdvertisingAwareness (UAA) Model Model Summary Hosmer and Lemeshow Omnibus Test of Test“ Model Coefficients Step -2 Log likelihood Nagelkerke R Square Chi-square Sig. Chi-square Sig._ 1 505.289 .770 1.069 .785 873.102S .000 873.102M .000 2 474.543 .787 2.220 .695 30.746S .000 903.848M .000 3 456.516 .797 14.305 .074 18.027S .000 921.876M .000 4 447.257 .802 8.559 .381 9.258S .002 931.134M .000 5 438.669 .806 10.270 .247 8.589s .003 939.722M .000 6 432.363 .810 11.629 .169 6.306s .012 946.028M .000 8: Step; M: Model * Measures overall model fitness. The higher the p-value the better since we fail to reject the hypothesis that ‘there is no difference between predicted and observed values.’ 57 The results show that advertising channel has a significant effect on advertising awareness. If a person saw the travel advertising on TV instead of the other channels increases the odds of one recalling the advertising. TV ([3 = 2.831; p = .000) is the most important predictor of unaided awareness of travel advertising. Being exposed to radio advertising also significantly increases the odds of top-of-mind recall of advertising. Radio is certainly more important than magazine advertising but not as important as Television advertising. This result supports hypothesis 9, which states that advertising channel has an effect on advertising awareness. This result agrees with the findings presented by Kisielius (1982) who stated that information presented pictorially stimulates more cognitive elaboration therefore resulting in the development of more storage locations and pathways in memory, which in turn also increases the likelihood of the information being retrieved when it is needed for later tasks. The regression model results also showed that experience has a significant effect on advertising awareness (B = 2.093; p = .003), implying that having traveled in the state on a leisure trip prior to being exposed to advertising increases the odds of one’s awareness of the state’s travel advertising. Those who had experience with leisure travel in the state of Michigan were more likely to be aware of the state’s travel advertising than those who didn’t have any experience. This result is also consistent with the theory of Cognitive dissonance discussed in the preceding chapters which says that people selectively attend to advertisements of products they have purchased to reinforce their decision and reduce post-purchase dissonance (Haley, 1985). People who have traveled to the state before might be more aware of the state’s travel advertising because the advertisements revive 58 the memories of the experience they had during their visit, or convince them that their choice of a destination was a good one. Exposure to advertising, as measured by the number of times an individual was exposed to advertising was also significant ([3 = 1.069; p < .001). The more people were exposed to travel advertising the higher the odds of them recalling the advertisements. The study also shows that gender has a significant effect on advertising awareness ([3 = 2.141; p = .005). Being female increases the probability of top-of-mind awareness of Travel Michigan advertising. According to Meyers-Levy’s 1989 selectivity hypothesis, adult males often don’t comprehensively process available information. Instead they tend to simplify the processing task, and they focus on a single often self-related cue from the message. Adult females on the other hand, use a comprehensive strategy to process information. Females tend to assimilate all available cues and engage in more detailed elaboration of message content than males (Meyers-Levy, 1989). The finding is also consistent with the household travel decision-making dynamics. Studies have shown that most of the travel decision-making is done by females. A study by Mottiar and Quinn (2004) showed vacation discussion is initiated mostly by females (58%) as compared to males (25%). Females are also most likely to choose the travel agent (34%) than males (16%) and also make the bookings (54%) compared to 22% for males. However, there are some decisions that are made jointly such as how much to spend, which accommodation to use and when to travel. 59 4.3.1.2 Destination Awareness The following Destination Awareness (DA) model was hypothesized: DA = f ( X1 ....... X“), where: DA = Destination Awareness (X1. ...Xn) = ( Intention to Visit (IV), Exposure, Location, Web-use, Experience, Advertising Awareness (AA)) Logistic regression analysis that was performed produced a four-variable model with four of the six variables included. The cut off p-value for inclusion into the model was .05. The AA and location variables were excluded from the final model because they failed to meet this criterion. Details are provided in Table 12. The model that resulted was: DA = .077 + 2.06 Experience + 1.90 Web-use + 1.49 IV + 1.01 Exposure The entered variables were all significant at the .01 level and each variable’s effect on reduction of Log Likelihood Value (—2LL) was also significant at the .01 level. 60 Table 12: Variables included in the Destination Awareness (DA) Model at each step. Step/Var. B S.E. Wald" Sig. Exp( B) Model Log L Chznlg: in Sig. of Change 1 IV .523 .048 118.641 .000 1.687 -622.114 136.855 .000 Const -2.737 .188 210.881 .000 .065 2 IV .411 .053 59.970 .000 1.509 -573.253 63.212 .000 Expe. .804 .164 23.999 .000 2.235 -553.687 24.080 .000 Const -2.753 .191 207.292 .000 .064 3 Expo. .005 .002 10.162 .001 1.005 -541.647 10.365 .001 IV .407 .054 57.811 .000 1.502 -566.928 60.927 .000 Expe. .796 .165 23.338 .000 2.216 -548.172 23.414 .000 Const -2.549 .200 161.805 .000 .078 4 Expo. .005 .002 9.319 .002 1.005 -S37.625 9.489 .002 Web .642 .240 7.187 .007 1.901 -536.465 7.167 .007 IV .399 .054 55.042 .000 1.490 -561.758 57.755 .000 Expe. .722 .168 18.525 .000 2.058 -542.140 18.519 .000 Const -2.561 .201 162.012 .000 .077 S. E: Standard Error; *Assesses the significance of each variable in the model The R 2 value for the model is .223 and the goodness of fit tests presented in Table 13 show that the model is a good fit. Table 13: Goodness of fit tests for the Destination Awareness Model Step Model Summary Hosmer and Lemeshow Test" -2 Log Nagelkerke R Chi-square df Sig. likelihood Square 1 1107.373 .174 6.565 3 .087 2 1083.294 .203 10.696 5 .058 3 1072.929 .215 11.806 8 .160 4 1065.762 .223 13.841 8 .086 * Measures overall model fitness. The higher the p-value the better, we fail to reject the hypothesis that ‘there is no difference between predicted and observed values.’ 61 Experience with travel in the state was the most important predictor of destination awareness (B = 2.058; p = .000). Web use was the next most important predictor of awareness of Michigan as a tourism destination (8 = 1.901; p = .000). Having visited the state’s tourism website increased the odds of one being aware of the state as a tourism destination. Destination awareness was measured by asking respondents to name destination states or cities that come to mind when they think of taking a pleasure trip in the United State and Canada. This implies that, Michigan comes to mind when those people who visit the state’s tourism website are thinking of taking a pleasure trip more often than those who don’t visit Michigan’s website. This means that the state’s tourism web site is an important tool in generating awareness of the state as a tourism destination. Intention to visit (IV) the state, though not as important as web use and experience, also increases the odds of one’s awareness of the state as a tourism destination ([3 = 1.490; p < .001). Those intending to visit the state actively search for information, therefore, increasing their awareness of the state as a tourism destination. Exposure to advertising also has a significant effect on destination awareness ([3 = 1.001; p = .002). The coefficient for Exposure is very close to one though meaning that its importance in increasing the odds of one being aware of the state as a tourism destination is very minimal as compared to the other three independent variables in the model. 62 4.4 Conversion Rates 4.4.1 Advertising Conversion Model The last stage of analysis involved calculation of actual conversion ratios to determine the effectiveness of Travel Michigan’s advertising. Effectiveness of Travel Michigan advertising was determined following the conversion and tracking models suggested by Siegel and Ziff-Levine (1990). An overview of the analysis performed in calculating conversion ratios is presented in Figure 5. Out of the 908 people that were potentially exposed to Travel MI advertising in the post campaign phase, 769 were found to be aware of the advertising, an 85% conversion rate at that stage. To estimate the conversion rate for the proceeding stage, cross tabs were used to determine the proportion of the people who were aware of advertising, and who also had destination awareness. The analysis showed that 225 (29%) of the peOple who were aware of Travel Michigan’s advertising were also aware of the state as a tourism destination. 63 Advertising Travel MI 1—1 Advertising Target Audience * 903 Aware of Advertising * 769; 85% CR Unaided Awareness of 8’ Destination 7 225: 29% CR Positive Image of ppl p51 PD] destma‘lon _, 214: 95% CR 149: 67% CR 205: 91% CR Inquiry Fulfillment ' 18: 8.4% CR 10: 6.7% CR 16: 7.8% CR M t' t' ‘ 0 rva ‘0" ' 16: 89% CR 9: 90% CR 15: 94% CR Conver§i0n > 8: 50% CR 4: 44% CR 8: 54% CR Behavror Overall Conversion Rate: 2.2% (20/908" 100) PFY: Positive Family Image; PEI: Positive Entertainment Image; PDI: Positive Distance Image; CR; Conversion Rate Figure 5: The Conversion Model for Travel Michigan’s 2003 Travel Advertising (Adapted from Siegel and Ziff-Levine, 1990) To determine the proportion of the target group that hold a positive image of the state as a tourism destination, three attitude variables were used to represent the three attitude factors. The three variables used were those that had the highest loading values on each component in the factor analysis. These attitude variables were: 1. Michigan is a great vacation destination for the whole family 2. Michigan offers great nightlife and entertainment 3. MI is close enough for short getaways To perform this analysis, the three variables were each re-coded. Ranks 1-2 were recoded 0 representing negative attitude and ranks 3-5 were re-coded 1 representing positive 64 attitude towards MI as a tourism destination. A new variable was also created to represent the 225 individuals who had both advertising and destination awareness. Out of the 225 respondents who had both advertising and destination awareness, 214 (95%) had a positive image of the state as a suitable tourism destination for the whole family, 149 (67%) had a positive image of the state as a destination suitable for adults seeking exciting nightlife and 205 (91%) had a positive image of the state as being a close enough destination for short get aways as shown in Table 14. The high conversion rates at this stage showed that, once people are aware of the destination, then their image or attitudes of the state as a tourism destination improve substantially. Combining the three image factors gives an average conversion rate of 84%. The challenge, however, is to improve the content of advertisements to highlight the important attributes of the state’s tourism destinations because of the very low conversion rate (29%) from the Advertising Awareness stage to the Destination Awareness stage. Michigan is not high on the list of destinations that people consider when they are thinking of taking a pleasure trip. While the advertisements reach a lot of people, they are not as effective in informing people what the state has to offer to potential tourists. 65 owns: 3:68 .5: 033qu 556: was 0:3 2333.6:— 4 mNN mON 3 N mmm 3; ON om mNN EN 5 «4 o~m3< owns: owns: owns: owns: owns: owns: 30,—. airmen gamma Z Became—25... =38. 398m o>cmwoz 3:63.83... .83. o>£mom o>umwoz Became—0:3... owes: 8:890 owns: EoEEetoam owes: nouaeumon 388m 386$ owes: web: 2: firs .:0_2 8:03:02 .0365 .085: 00:80.5 03:60: .563 .088: 3:80: 05:00: .<: .5. .8388 2:: :6 EW :20 :88 :0 AEQV 80:: :.:05 055 2:00: :0 5.58:2 0W8m .:0:>0:0m 8:88:00. 05 :0: 00:0: 888500 ”2 030:. 72 By combining all the individuals who took a pleasure trip to the state of Michigan whose trip was influenced by the state’s advertising gives an overall conversion rate of 2.2% ((8 + 4 + 8/ 908)* 100). The overall conversion rate including those individuals who were not captured by the conversion model comes to 11.2%. 4.4.2 The Advertising Tracking Model For two reasons, the tracking model conversion rates are higher and a lot more realistic than those calculated using the conversion model. First, the tracking model does not consider inquiry as a necessary step to conversion, and second the tracking model assumes that one can develop a positive image of a destination just by being exposed to advertising which can produce motivation to visit without necessarily going through the Destination Awareness stage. There are three routes (1-3) through which individuals can end up exhibiting conversion behavior through the tracking model as compared to the conversion model. An additional route (0) was added to capture those individuals who didn’t go through either the conversion model or tracking model stages, but indicated that their decision to travel was influenced by the advertising. This is illustrated in figure 6. 73 Unaided 1 Awareness of Destination \ ' Motivation \ To visit . . . . 2 " . ' Advertlsmg Posmve Image / 3 Advertismg —1> Awareness —I> of Destination / 1 & 2 Inquiry Fulfillment . ~- 3 _- Conversmn Route 0 — Behavior The dotted line to and from the inquiry fulfillment stage means that the stage is not required for conversion to occur. Siegel and Ziff-Levine (1990) Figure 6: Advertising Tracking Model showing the different routes (0-3) used to calculate the conversion rates and ROI. Travel Michigan’s advertising effectiveness was also calculated using the tracking model. Only routes 1 and 2 above were used to calculate the conversion rates at this stage. Recall that only the third route requiring inquiry fulfillment was used in the conversion model. The results from the tracking model illustrated in Figure 7 show that route 1 has an overall conversion rate of 5.2%. This route, though not as conservative as the conversion model, also requires that one goes through the Unaided Destination Awareness stage before developing motivation to visit. Route 2, however, gave the highest conversion rate simply because the route excludes both the Destination Awareness and Inquiry Fulfillment stages. The route presumes that one can develop motivation to travel and eventually visit the state only after having developed a positive image of the destination from exposure to advertisements. For this route, the three image variables were used and 74 three different conversion percentages were obtained. The analysis resulted in a different tracking model for the state’s travel advertising as shown in Figure 7 below. Advertising audience 908 / Advertising Awareness 769: 85% CR 414: 61% CR V Destination Awareness 225: 29% CR I78: 79% CR PFI ’ 683: 89%CR \ 279: 63% CR ‘ V Route 0 Conversion behavior 102: 11.2% CR CR; Conversion Ratio; OCR Overall Conversion Ratio Route 1: Includes ‘Destination Awareness’ stage Route 2: Excludes ‘Destination Awareness’ stage PFI: Positive Family Image PEI: Positive Entertainment Image PDI: Positive Distance Image PEI 440: 57% CR \. 40 . 61% CR Motivation To visit Route 1 PDI Conversion behavior . o 671. 237/. CR 47. 26% CR k 5.2% OCR v ‘ PFI Conversion PEI Conversion PDI Conversion Behavior Behavior Behavior 86: 21%CR 61: 15%CR 85:21%CR 9.5% OCR 6.7% OCR 9.4;/o OCR Route 2 Average OCR 8.5% Figure 7: Advertising Tracking Model for Travel M1(Siege| and Ziff-Levine, 1990) The results show the conversion rates for Travel Michigan advertising range from 2.2% using the conservative conversion model to 8.2% through route 2 of the tracking model and 11.2% through the straightforward route (0). While these results show that the percentage of people who were brought to the state as a result of the state’s travel advertising, they do not say anything about the return on investment (ROI) of the 75 advertising campaign since neither accounts for sales in dollars or advertising costs in this analysis at this juncture. Determination of the ROI is the subject of the next stage of the analysis. 4.5 Return on Advertising Investment (ROI) Return on advertising investment (ROI) was calculated for all the four routes illustrated earlier in Figures 5 and 6. ROI in this study refers only to government tax revenues captured from tourist activity. Calculating these figures was made possible by making use of several assumptions given below: It was assumed that the visitors from the three DMAs spend the same amount of money once they cross the MI borders into the state. The researcher was interested in the visitors’ expenditures once they are in the state not before. Data on visitor expenditures and travel party size were obtained from the Household Surveys conducted by Michigan TTRRC. It was assumed that the advertising target population is the same as the population of the greater metropolitan areas of the three DMAs since 97% of all US households have cable and access to television, and an even bigger percentage have access to some form of media, be it radio magazine or newspaper (N eilsen Media Research, 2002). Only that proportion of the population over the age of 18 was used since it was the targeted population group. Population data were based on the 2000 census. 76 o The state gets approximately 8% of revenue through sales tax, lodging room tax and income taxes from tourism businesses and their employees. According to the research department of The Travel Industry Association of America (TIA) (2004) the state collects 5 cents out of every traveler spending dollar in taxes. Carr and Holecek (2003) estimated the state collects 10% of tourist expenditure in taxes. This study used 8%, an average of these two figures (5% and 10%), which translates to 6% from sales and room taxes and 2% from tourism businesses and their employees. The 8% figure was assumed to be an appropriate estimate of state tax collections in the absence of any better data. The population figures for the greater metropolitan Chicago, Indianapolis and Cleveland are 9,650,137; 1,607,486 and 2,900,000 respectively. Since only those individuals above the age of 18 were considered only 7,044,600 (73%) of the Chicago population, 1,194,362 (74.3%) of the Indianapolis population and 2,073,500 (71.5%) of the Cleveland population belonged to the defined age category. The sum of the populations of the three DMAs was used for the calculation of the ROI. The calculations and results of the ROI analysis are presented in Table 18. 77 Table 18: ROI figures for the 2003 Travel Michigan Advertising Campaign (4 routes used) Item Route 0 Route 1 Route 2 Route 3 POPUIaIiOH 10,312,462 10,312,462 10,312,462 10,312,462 Travel Party size 3.5 3.5 3.5 3.5 Ave. Expenditures/Party US$947 US$947 US$947 US$947 Ave. Expend/individual (US$947/3.5) US$271 US$271 US$271 US$271 Conversion Ratio (%) 11.2 5.2 8.5 2.2 Number of people convened (cwpopulafion) 1,154,996 536,248 876,559 226,874 Revenue USS (90868“ 313,003,847 145,323,215 237,547,562 61,482,898 people*Expend/md1v1dual) 0 State Revenue USS (8 /" °f 25,040,308 11,625,857 19,003,805 4,918,632 Revenue) Tm"? MI advfmsmg ”“5 2,392,062 2,392,062 2,392,062 2,392,062 for th1s campaign (US$) ROI 1: 10.47 1: 4.86 1: 7.94 1: 2.06 Route 0: Straightforward route; Route 1: Tracking model route including ‘Destination Awareness’ stage; Route 2: Tracking model route excluding ‘Destination Awareness’ stage; Route 3: Conversion model route including ‘Inquiry Fulfillment’ stage. ’ These results show a positive return on advertising investment even for the conservative conversion model (route 3) which includes the inquiry fulfillment requirement. Through this route, Travel Michigan more than breaks even getting about US$2.06 for every dollar spent on advertising. Route 2 is the one that assumes that individuals can develop motivation to visit and visit after being exposed to the advert without necessarily having gone through the destination and inquiry fiilfillment stages. This route gives an ROI value of 7.94 implying that there is a $7.94 return for every dollar spent on advertising. The ROI values range from 1: 2.06 for the most conservative route 3 to 1: 10.47 for the most liberal one (route 0). Either way, the state is getting more from advertising than it is spending, meaning that travel advertising is a worthwhile investment. It is also important to note that these analyses only include direct expenditures and net revenues, factoring in the multiplier effect or considering gross revenues would certainly give even higher conversion and ROI ratios. Such further analyses, however, are beyond the scope of this study. These results tend to justify spending of tax dollars on travel advertising and the 78 results are consistent with what other researchers have found. Mok (1991) found gross tourism advertising returns to range from 1:10 to 1:56 and concluded that there is considerable justification for using public resources in tourism advertising. 79 Chapter 5 5.0 Summary and Conclusion The main object of this study was to determine the effectiveness of Travel Michigan’s travel advertising campaign for the 2003 season in three DMAs: Chicago, Indianapolis and Cleveland. The study utilized different analyses in an effort to establish the effectiveness of the travel advertising campaign. First, comparisons were made between the pre and post campaign phases to note if there were any differences in advertising awareness between those interviewed before and after the campaign. The significant difference in advertising awareness found between these two groups of respondents was a good starting point for the study. It meant that the campaign had made that initial breakthrough of getting the attention of the audience that is required for advertising to be effective. Further analyses to determine the effectiveness of the campaign would not have been necessary had there been no differences in advertising awareness between respondents interviewed pre and post campaign. Getting attention and creating awareness are the first and certainly important stages in most of the communication models including both the old and newer models of communication. Today’s consumers are exposed to not less than 1000 advertisements daily (Kotler, 1997), therefore, breaking through such clutter to gain the consumer’s attention is a challenge for any advertiser. Just getting the consumer’s attention is, however, not sufficient by itself. It is hoped that this stage will result in some more positive responses including, but not limited to: opinion change, attitude change, perception change, affect change and/or action change (Tan 1985). 80 Second, having established that the campaign was effective in getting the intended audience’s attention, the analyses that followed were intended to determine if the campaign had any further effects beyond just getting attention. Effectiveness in this study was measured in several ways including: generating destination awareness, creating positive attitude toward the state as a tourism destination, generating motivation to visit the state and generating visits to the state. These measures were used based on the assumption that success in communication is shown in many ways as illustrated by the H- J -K Communication Persuasion Model introduced in Figure 1. It is especially important in tourism because it is a unique industry which relies much on word of mouth (Opperman, 2000); therefore, by generating destination awareness, positive perceptions and attitude towards a destination, an advert can be said to be effective since such people are likely to recommend the destination to others even if they have not visited it themselves. Furthermore, having positive attitudes and product awareness have been shown to be predecessors of action (Lavidge and Steiner, 1961; Haskins, 1964; Fishbein and Azjen, 1974; Tan, 1985; and Butterfield et. al., 1998). Those who are aware of advertising have greater levels of destination awareness, greater intention to visit the state, better attitudes towards the state as a tourism destination and are more likely to take a pleasure trip to the state than those who are not aware of advertising. Significant differences were found in visits to the state among those who were aware of advertising and those who were not. 81 While proponents of experiments and quasi-experiments might argue that this does not establish a causal relationship between advertising and results, it is important to note that the results drawn from the analyses reported herein were all highly and consistently significant at the p < .001 level. Also, Starch (1961) argues that when divided into those that recall and those who do not recall an advert, the differences between these two groups can be attributed to advertising. He also argues that typically those who recall advertisements buy more than those who do not. This argument, however, is not meant to undermine the importance of experiments in establishing true causal relationships, rather it is meant to offer support for the findings of this study. Those individuals who were aware of Michigan’s travel advertising campaign consistently ranked Michigan higher as a good tourism destination than those who were not aware of the advertising. Similarly, those who were aware of the state’s advertising had greater intention/motivation to visit the state and actually visited the state more than those who were not aware. Even though this study does not meet the requirements of an experiment, the consistency of the results can only imply that some credit belongs to the Travel Michigan advertising campaign. Next, principal components analysis was conducted to reduce the attitude data as well as examine the grouping pattern of the different attitude variables. Three components resulted which represented three attitudes towards Michigan as a tourism destination. The first and most important attitude component was the Family Image factor which accounts for 34% of the variance in the rotated matrix. Next most important was the Entertainment Image factor representing 20% of the variance, followed by the Distance 82 Image factor which represented 12% of the variance. This result means that Michigan is viewed by consumers most importantly as a family destination and also as a close enough destination for short get away trips. This information is of particular importance to the state’s destination marketers because it gives them a good idea about what the visitors are looking for in the destination, and therefore, helps them to craft their advertising messages to communicate those messages as well as develop destination attributes to meet the customers’ needs. Correlation analysis showed a positive relationship between attitudes of people towards the state as a tourism destination and advertising awareness, destination awareness and intention to visit the state. These results also show Michigan is most importantly viewed as a family tourism destination. Attitudes towards Michigan as a family destination were found to be highly and positively correlated to advertising awareness, destination awareness and intention to visit the state. As people’s ‘family destination’ attitudes towards Michigan improve, so does their intention to visit the state. Also, as people’s distance attitudes towards the state improve, so do their advertising awareness and intention to visit. Also, the relationship between DMA location and both destination awareness and intention to visit was negative. This means that people’s awareness of Michigan as a tourism destination as well as their intention to visit the state both decrease as the DMA’s distance from Michigan increases. Again, this highlights that the state of Michigan needs to also focus its attention on markets closer to home as people’s preferences for short holidays increase. Recent world events such as 911 and the war have left people more inclined to take short trips to which they can drive rather than fly 83 and also which they feel they are secure. Travel Michigan, therefore, needs to take advantage of the situation and get short haul travelers to its destinations. The fourth stage of analysis involved use of logistic regression to determine the factors affecting unaided advertising awareness (UAA) and destination awareness (DA). The results showed exposure to television advertising was the single most important predictor of unaided advertising awareness, followed in descending order by gender, experience, radio, level of exposure and age. Being female increased the probability of one recalling the advertisements, implying that females were more likely to recall the advertisements than their male counterparts. This is an important finding for destination marketers as it tells them that in designing their advertisements they need to include material that appeals to females since they are more likely to recall the advertisements when it comes to making decisions on which destinations to visit. Results from the previous stages of analysis show Michigan is a state that is ranked very highly as a family destination, so in its effort to attract two adult (male, female) families to the state Travel Michigan should mostly consider including advertisements that appeal to females. Women play a dominant role in the vacation destination selection process in the family (Mottiar and Quinn, 2004). Having traveled to the state on a pleasure trip before was also a significant factor affecting advertising awareness. This finding collaborates Ehrenberg’s (2000) claims that advertising’s main role is to reinforce feelings of satisfaction for brands already purchased and that consumers mostly ignore advertising for brands they are not already using. This also agrees with the theory of cognitive dissonance which states that 84 individuals consciously avoid or attend to messages in an effort to reduce dissonance. By attending to advertisements of brands and/or products they have already purchased, consumers reinforce their decision and convince themselves that their decision was the right one, thereby reducing post-purchase dissonance. Having been exposed to radio advertisements, even though not as important as TV, also increased the probability of an individual recalling the advertisements. Also, the more an individual was exposed to advertising the more likely they were to recall the advertisements. Experience was the single most important factor affecting destination awareness in the DA model, followed by web use, intention to visit and finally exposure to advertisements in that order. Having traveled in the state on a pleasure trip before increased the probability of an individual being aware of the state as a tourism destination. This is an important result for the state DMO because it also suggests that repeat visitors are important to the state’s tourism industry. DA was measured by asking the respondents to name states and/or provinces that come to mind when they are thinking of taking a pleasure trip in the U. S. and Canada. Individuals who have experience traveling in the state on pleasure trips keep considering it for their future vacations, illustrates the importance of repeat business to the state’s tourism industry. Michigan leisure and tourism providers, therefore, need to pay attention to their repeat visitors to the point of developing relationships and loyalty because repeat visitors are an especially important target market. 85 Web use was also an important factor affecting destination awareness. Those individuals who had visited the state’s official travel and tourism web site were more aware of the state as a tourism destination than those who had not. This implies the state’s travel and tourism web site is an important resource in generating awareness of the state as a tourism destination among potential visitors. Intention to visit the state was also significant, maybe because those with the intention to visit the state on a pleasure trip actively seek information resulting in them being more aware than those with no intention to take a pleasure trip to the state. Exposure to advertisements was also a significant factor affecting destination awareness. The more individuals were exposed to advertisements the more they were aware of the state as a tourism destination. Assuming destination awareness was the goal of the advertising campaign, this result show there are positive returns to advertising. This study, however, does not show at what point this ceases to be true (i.e. when does increasing exposure stop increasing destination awareness?) The fifth stage of analysis included calculations of real conversion ratios using four different approaches involving named routes (0-3) developed in this study. The first route was from the tracking model which includes the destination awareness stage. The second route was also from the tracking model, but excluded the destination awareness stage. The third route was from the conversion model which included the inquiry fulfillment stage, and the fourth route was the ‘straightforward’ route called route 0. Under the straightforward route (route 0) all respondents who indicated that they had visited the 86 state and that their visit was influenced by advertising were considered. These individuals needed not to have gone through all the other steps as depicted by the tracking and conversion models. The results ranged from a conversion ratio of 2.2% from the most conservative conversion model to 8.5% for the liberal tracking model route that excludes the destination awareness stage and finally 11.2% for the straightforward route. The straightforward route conversion rate was arrived at by simply considering those individuals who had traveled to the state on a pleasure trip and indicated that advertising influenced their decision to travel without necessarily having gone through the stages given by both the tracking and conversion models. The important result from the sixth and final ROI stage of analysis is that there is a positive return on advertising investment taking all the routes into consideration. The straightforward route yielded a liberal $10.47 return in state tax revenue collections on every dollar spent on advertising, route 1 from the tracking model including the destination awareness stage yielded a $4.86 return on every dollar spent on advertising, route 2 yielded a $7.94 ROI and finally route 3 from the conservative conversion model yielded a $2.06 ROI. It is important to note these figures are actually underestimates as they include only estimated tax revenue collections associated with projected direct expenditures. In summary, the findings of this study reveal the positive effect of travel advertising on a number of important factors. The main weakness of the study that is most likely to be raised by the proponents of experiments is that, it fails to establish and ascertain causal relationships. However, the consistency of results given in the different stages of analysis conducted in this study stand to support that the advertising campaign had substantial 87 positive impacts. Results clearly showed that there were significant differences in destination awareness, intention to visit and actual visitation between those who were aware and those not aware of advertising. There were also significant differences among those aware and those not aware of advertising on the attitudes towards the state as a tourism destination. Those aware of advertising consistently ranked Michigan more highly as a desirable tourism destination than those not aware of advertising. Considering attitude change and destination awareness as prerequisites to visitation, Travel Michigan’s advertising can be considered highly effective. The DA logistic regression model also shows that exposure to advertising is important in increasing one’s chances of being aware of Michigan as a tourism destination. Finally, the conversion ratios and the ROI figures all show the effectiveness of the advertising campaign. So, even though the study is not an experiment, the consistency of the results clearly illustrate the importance of the travel advertising campaign. Woodside (1990) advocates the use of experiments such as A-B or A-B-C splits in which two groups get a different treatment each and the other becomes a control group; and then compare the results from these two or three groups. As much as these are capable of producing the causal relationships among different variables, they are easier to contemplate than conduct, especially in our field of travel and tourism that relies heavily on word of mouth. For instance, how possible is it to run different television travel promotional programs targeting different groups in the same population without the possibility of treatment spillover from one treatment group to the next and even to the control (no treatment) group? This might be easy in a laboratory setting, but it is not so 88 simple to implement in real life situations. Quasi-experiments on the other hand, that have been advocated for and used by a number of researchers (Mok, 1990; Woodside et. al., 1997), though they are more practical than true experiments, have their own shortcomings. Quasi-experiments do not utilize random assignment of subjects between the treatment and control groups implying that the results can’t entirely be accurately attributed to advertising alone. The resulting differences between the groups might result from initial differences before the treatment was administered. Given these shortcomings associated with the supposedly better methods, the important question remains: Is it really worth it to invest in more expensive and laborious methods when there is no guarantee that the outcome is any better than those obtained from cheaper and more practical methods? The answer partly lies in Messmer’s and Johnson’s (1993) conclusion that more testing of both conversion and alternative approaches is needed before conversion studies can justifiably be abandoned. Future research, therefore, should focus more on comparing the results from the different methods including conversion studies, experiments and quasi-experiments to see if there are any great differences that warrant the complete abandonment of one method for another. Also, more work needs to be done in refining experiments and quasi- experiments to make them more practical and applicable to the travel and tourism field. This is important because for most DMOs, the problem is not about which is the best method to utilize, but is about whether or not there are enough funds to conduct any accountability research at all, especially considering the tight budget circumstances under which most of them operate. Investing in more expensive impractical methods with no 89 guaranteed clean results is not an option for them when there are other cheaper more practical alternatives. Unless other cheaper and more practical approaches than the current experiments are developed and tested, conversion studies remain that best viable alternative available. 5.1 Study Limitations The main limitation of the study is the short lag time between the campaign and the post campaign phase. Pre-campaign data collection ended on the 1St of May 2003 and post- campaign data collection started on July 7. There was only two months in between the two phases during which the campaign was run. The period might not have been long enough for exposed individuals to respond to the advertisements. In the future, such studies should allow more time for full campaign effects to be recorded. The campaign effects delivered in this study, therefore, might be under-estimates of actual impacts. Underestimates are also probable since neither the possibility of the advertisements generating positive word of mouth or repeat visits over multiple years were included in the projections. Another limitation of this study is that it did not filter out the effects of other independent campaigns that might have been running simultaneously with the state’s campaign. Even though caution was taken to ensure that no other major campaigns were running in the three DMAS during that time, other Michigan tourism providers were not prohibited from running advertisements to promote their businesses, thereby creating an element of ‘noise’ for this particular study. There were no major advertising events taking place 90 during the time for this study in the three DMAs. Future studies of this sort should, therefore, account for and filter out the impact of other advertising. Future studies should also account for cumulative effect of advertising exposure. The use of secondary data was also a limitation to this study. There are things that could have been done differently, had it been a primary research study. Placement of advertisement campaigns in the media would have been delayed to allow more pre- campaign interviews to be completed. Also, the timing of post campaign interviews would have been different. Consumers would have been allowed more reaction time, between exposure to advertisements and post campaign interviews. 91 References Aaker, D. A. and Day, G. S. 1974. A Dynamic Model of Relationships Among Advertising, Consumer Awareness, Attitudes and Behavior. J oumal of Applied Psychology, 59: 281-286. Asimakopulos, A._1978. An Introduction to Economic Theory: Microeconomics. Oxford. Oxford University Press. Ballman, G., J. Burke, U. Blank, and D. Korte. 1984. Toward Higher Quality Conversion Studies: Refining the Numbers Game. Journal of Travel research, 22 (4): 28-33. Barry, K. and J. O’Hagan. 1972. An Econometric Study of British Tourist Expenditure in Ireland. Economic and Social Review, 3(2): 143-161. Bauer. 1964. The obstinate audience: The influence process from the point of view of social communication. American Psychologist, 19 (5): 319-328. Bayou, Mahomed, E., Panitz and Eric. 1993. Definition and Content of Persuasion in Accounting. Journal of Applied Business Research, 9 (3): 44-51. Buterfield, D. W., K. R. Deal and A. A. Kubursi. 1998. Measuring the Returns to Tourism Advertising. Journal of Travel Resea_rgh, 37 (August): 12-20. Burke, J. F. and R. Gitelson. 1990. Conversion Studies: Assumptions, Applications, Accuracy and Abuse. Journal of Travel Reseaih, 28 (3), 46-51. Caples, J. 1974. Tested Advertising Methods. Engelwood Cliffs, NJ. Prentice-Hall. Chamberlin, E. H. 1962. The Theory of Mongolistic Competition. Cambridge. Harvard University Press. Comanor, W., and T. Wilson. 1974. Advertising and Market Power. Cambridge, MA. Harvard University Press. Ehrenberg, A. S. C. 2000. Repetitive Advertising and the Consumer. Journal of AdvertisingResearch, (November): 39-48. Edell, J. A. and A. Staelin. 1983. The Information Processing of Pictures in Print Advertisments. Journirl of Consumer Research. 10 (June): 45-61. F estinger, L. 1957. A Theory of Cognitive Dissonance. Stanford, CA. Stanford University Press. Fishbein, M. and Azjen, I. 1975. Belief, Attitude, Intention and Behavior. Reading, MA. Addison-Wesley. 92 Garfield, B. June 27 1994. There’s Catch to States’ ‘Come Hither’ Approach. Advertising Aga, 65 (27): p. 32. Gruber, A. 1970. Purchase Intent and Purchase Probability. Journal of Advertising Research 10 (1), 23-27. Haley, R. I. 1985. Developing Effective Communication Strategy: A Benefit Segmentaaion Approach New York. John Wiley and Sons. Haskins, J. B. 1964. Factual Recall as a Measure of Advertising Effectiveness. Journal of Advertising Research, 4 (March): 2-8. Feldman, J. M. and J. G. Lynch. 1998. Self-Generated Validity and Other Effects of Measurement on Belief, Attitude, Intention, and Behavior. Journal of Applied Psychology, 73 (3): 421-35. Hovland, C. I., I. L. Janis and H. H. Kelly. 1953. Cited in: Tan, A. S. 1985. Mass Communication Theories and Research. New York. Wiley. Howard, J. A. and J. N. Sheth. 1969. The Theory of Buyer Behavior. NY. John Wiley and Sons. Keller, K. L. 1987. Memory Factors in Advertising: The Effect of Advertising Retrieval Cues on Brand Evaluations. Journal of Consumer Research, 14 (December), 316- 33. Keller, K. L. 1996. Brand Equity and Integrated Communication. In: Thorson, E. and J. Moore (eds) Integrated Communication: Synergy of Persuasive Voices. Mahwah, NJ. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. Kisielis, J. and B. Stemthal. 1986. Examining the Vividness Controversy: An Availability-Valence Intepretation. Journal of Consumer Research 12: 48-431. Kisielis. 1982. Detecting and Explaining Vividness Effects in Attitudinal Judgement. Unpublished Doctoral Dissertation. Northwestern University. Evanston, IL. Kotler, P. 1997. Marketing Management: Analysis, Flaming, Implementation, and Control. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. Krugrnan, H. E. 1972. Why Three Exposures May Be Enough. J ournal_of Advertising Research, 12 (6): 11-14. Lavidge, R. C. and Steiner, G. A. 1961. A Model for Predictive Measurements of Advertising Effectiveness. Journal of Marketing, 25(October): 59-62. 93 Messmer, J. D. and Johnson, R. R. 1993. Inquiry Conversion and Travel Advertising Effectiveness. Journal of Travel Research, 31(Spring): 14-21 Meyers-Levy, J. 1989. Gender differences in information processing: A selectivity interpretation. In: P. Cafferata and A. Tybout (eds). Cognitive and affective responses to advertising. Lexington, MA. Lexington. Meyers-Levy, J. and Malaviya P. 1999. Consumers’ Processing of Persuasive Advertisements: An Integrative Framework of Persuasion Theories. Journal of Marketing, 64 (special issue), 45-60. Mottiar, Z and Quinn, D. 2004. Couple Dynamics in Household Tourism Decision Making: Women as the Gatekeepers? Journal of Vacation Marketing, 10 (2): 149- 160. Nedungadi, P. 1990. Recall and Consumer Consideration sets: Influencing Choice Without Altering Brand Evaluations. Journal of Consumer Reseach, 17 (4): 263- 76. Opperman, M. 2000. Tourism Destination Loyalty. Journal of Travel Research 39 (1): 78-84. Palda, K. 1964. The Measurement of Cumulative Advertising Effects. Engelwood Cliffs, NJ. Prentice Hall. Palda, S. K. 1966. The Hypothesis of a Hierarchy of Effects: A Partial Evaluation. Journal of MarketingResearcl'i, 3(February): 13-24. Peles, Y. 1971. Economics of Scale in Advertising Beer and Cigarettes. Journal of Business 44 (January): 32-37. Petty, R. E. and J. T. Cacioppo 1986. Communication and Persuasion: Central and Perjrherafl Routes to Attitude Change. New York. Springer. Rao, A. G. 1970. Quantitative Theories in Advertising. New York. Wiley Rapp, S., and T. Collins. 1987. Maximarketing. New York. McGraw-Hill. Ray, M. L. 1973. Marketing Communications and the Hierarchy of Effects: In Clarke, P. (ed.). New Models for Mass Communication Research, vol. 11. Sage Annual Review of Communication Research. Beverly Hills. Sage Publications. Raymond, C. 1974. The Art of Using Science in Marfiketing. New York. Harper and Row. Rotzol, K. B. The Starch and Ted Bates Correlative Measures of Advertising 94 Effectiveness. Journal of Advertising Research, (March): 22-24. Sandage, C. H. and V. Fryburger. 1963. Advertising'l’heory and Practice. Homewood, IL. Richard D. Irwin. Severin, W. J ., and J. W. Tankard Jr. 1979. Communication Theories: Origins, Methods, Uses. New York. Hastings House. Siegel, W., and W. Ziff-Levine. 1990. Evaluating Tourism Advertising Campaigns: Conversion vs. Advertising Tracking Studies. Journal of Travel Research, 28 (3): 51-55. Simon, J. L. and J. Amdt. 1980. The Shape of the Advertising Response Function. Journal of AdvertisingResearch, 20 (4): 11-28. Starch, D. 1961. M_e_asuring Product Sales Made by Advertising. Mamaroneck, N. Y. Starch and Staff. Strack, R. 1992. The Different Routes to Social Judgments: Experiential Versus Informational Strategies. In Martin, L. L., and Abraham Tesser, (eds). m Construction of Social Judgments. Hillsdale, NJ. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Schmalensee, R. 1972. The Economics of Advertising. Amsterdam, Netherlands. North Holland. Tan, A. S. 1985. Mass Communication Theories and Research. New York. Wiley. Tourism New Zealand, 2005. Understanding the Japanese Long Haul Market. Unpublished Manuscript. Travel Industry Association of America (TIA), 2003. 2002-2003 Survey of US. Stag and Territory Tourism Office Budgets. Trochim, W. M. K. 2001. The Research Methods Knowledge Basa. 2nd Edition. Atomic Dog Publishing. Uysal, M. and J. Crompton. 1984. Determinants of Demand for International Tourist Flows to Turkey. Tourism Management, 5 (4): 288-97. Wansik, B. and M. L. Ray. 2000. Estimating an Advertising’s Impact on One’s Consumption of a Brand. Journal of AdvertisLng Research, (November): 106-13. Weilbacher, W. M. 2003. How Advertising Works. Journal of Advertising Research, 43(2): 230-234. Woodside, A. G. and D. M. Reid. 1974. Tourism Profiles Versus Audience Profiles: Are 95 Upscale Magazines Really Upscale? Journal of Travel Research. 12 (Spring):17- 23. Woodside, A. G. and I. A. Ronkainen. 1989. How Serious is Non-response Bias in Advertising Conversion Research, J oumal of Travel Research, 26 (Spring): 34-37. Woodside, A. G. 1990. Measuring Advertising Effectiveness In Destination Marketing Strategies. Journal of gavel Research, 29 (Fall): 3-8. Woodside, A. G., R. J. Trappey III, and R. MacDonald. 1997. Measuring Linkage- Advertising Effects on Customer Behavior and Net Revenue: Using Quasi- Experiments of Advertising Treatments with Novice and Experienced Product- Service Users. Revue Canadienne des Sciences de I’Administration, 14(2): 214- 228. Wright, P. 1980. Message-Evoked Thoughts: Persuasion Research Using Thought Verbalization. Journal of Consumer Research, 7 (September), 151-75. Zhou, Z. 2000. The Impact of Memory on Expenditures Recall in Tourism Conversion Studies. Journal of Travel Research. 38 (February): 304-308. 96 Appendix 1: Sample Questionnaire Date: Survey Number: 03-CL—2- Time: Interviewer: Travel Michigan Market Survey CLEVELAND DMA Awareness Study (Do not read the list) 1. When you are thinking about taking a pleasure trip in the UNITED STATES or CANADA what cities, states or provinces come to mind? Any others? First mention Others [ [ California (Los Angeles, San Francisco, San Diego) Florida (Orlando, Tampa, Jacksonville, Miami) Hawaii Illinois (Chicago) Indiana (Indianapolis) l ] l l l ] l l ] l ] ] Michigan (Detroit, Traverse City, Mackinac) I l l l ] l I l l l ] l ] 1 Minnesota (Minneapolis/ St. Paul, Bloomington [Mall of America]) [ New York [ Ohio (Cleveland, Columbus, Cincinnati, Sandusky [Cedar Point]) [ [ Ontario (Toronto) [ [ Wisconsin (Milwaukee, Green Bay, The Dells, Madison) [ [ Other: , 3 (Do not read the list) 2. Now thinking just about a pleasure trip in the MIDWEST, what cities or states come to mind? Any others? (Do not read the list. Record answers in the first column below) First mention Others [I [l I] ] l ] Illinois (Chicago) Indiana (Indianapolis) Michigan (Detroit, Traverse City, Mackinac) f Minnesota (Minneapolis/St. Paul, Bloomington [Mall of America]) Ohio (Cleveland, Columbus, Cincinnati, Sandusky [Cedar Point]) Wisconsin (Milwaukee, Green Bay, The Dells, Madison) Other: , . 97 [ ] 3. For which states or provinces do you recall seeing or hearing any vacation or travel advertising in the past 6 months? (Read each state NOT checked in the first column. Record responses in the columns on the right column) 4. Do you recall seeing or hearing any vacation or travel advertising for in the past 6 months? Do not read the states Read the remaining states Illinois [ ] Yes No Indiana [ ] Yes No Michigan 8 [ 1 Yes N6 Minnesota [ ] Yes No Ohio [ ] Yes No Ontario [ ] Yes No Wisconsin [ ] Yes No Other (specify) [ ] Do not ask “other” None [ ] If a respondent DID NOT MENTION any MICHIGAN advertising in question 3 or 4, GO TO QUESTION 10. 5. You mentioned that you saw or heard vacation or travel advertising for the State of Michigan in the past 6 months. Please describe that advertising to me. What did it say or show? [ ] —(-99) (Do not read) Don’t know 6. In your opinion, what ideas or messages was Michigan trying to communicate in this advertising? [ ] —(-99) (Do not read) Don’t know 98 7. Approximately, how many times have you seen or heard Michigan travel advertising in the past 6 months? # times [ ] —(-99) (Do not read) Don’t know [If“zero” go to question 10. ] (Read the list) Did the information you saw or heard on Michigan have .°° [ ] —1 a primary influence on your decision to travel to Michigan ] ——2 a partial influence on your decision ] -3 no influence on your decision ] —4 (Do not read) Did not visit Michigan ] -(-99) (Do not read) Don’t know Hf—1f—17—1 (Read the list) Compared with other states and provinces would you say you have seen ] —1 Much more advertising for Michigan ] —2 Somewhat more advertising for Michigan ] —3 About the same ] —4 Somewhat less advertising for Michigan ] —5 Much less advertising for Michigan? ] —(-99) (Do not read) Don’t know f—‘Il—If—1HI—1f—W \o C 10. What do you think is the Internet address for the state of Michigan’s official tourism web site? [ ] —(-99) Don’t know 11. Do you have access to the Internet? [ ] —1 Yes [ ] —2 No => [Go to question 17.] 12. Have you used the Internet for travel planning purposes in the past 12 months? [ ] —1 Yes I l -2 No => [Go to question 16.] 13. Have you made a travel-related purchase over the Internet in the past 12 months? [l—lYes []—2N0 99 14. In the past 12 months, how often, if at all, have you visited the state of Michigan’s official tourism web site to obtain Michigan travel information? # times [ ] —(-99) Don’t know => I Go to question 16.] [If “zero” => Go to question 16.] (Do not read the list, unless respondent is unable to give a specific answer). How did you access it? [ ] -1 Typed in the web site address [ ] —2 Used a search engine (search program) to find it [ ] —3 Used the bookmark from previous use(s) [ ] —4 Clicked on the link on another web site [ ] —(-99) Don’t know (Read the list) 16. In the next 12 months, how likely is it that you will visit the state of Michigan’s official tourism web site to obtain Michigan travel information? [ ]—1 Very likely [ ] —2 Somewhat likely [ ] —3 Somewhat unlikely [ ] —4 Very unlikely [ ] —(-99) (Do not read) Don’t know 17. Have you used a State of Michigan toll-free phone number to inquire about travel information? [ ]—1 Yes [ ]—2 No [ ]—(-99) Don’t know ADVERTISING BLOCK Now I’d like to ask you some specific questions about vacation or travel advertising for the State of Michigan that you MAY or MAY NOT have seen or heard in the past 6 months. For each description I read, please tell me whether or not you have seen or heard that advertising. (Read ALL the descriptions). TV Advertising 100 18. Have you seen advertising on TELEVISION for the State of Michigan in the past 6 months that... .? showed a girl talking about her vacation trip to Detroit and attractions she visited there. The ad ended with the “Great Lakes. Great Times.” logo, and gave the Don’t Michiganorg web address for more information. Yes No Know (Read ALL the descriptions). Magazine Advertising 19. Have you seen similar advertising for the State of Michigan in the past 6 months in a Don’t MAGAZINE? Yes No Know (Read ALL the descriptions). Radio Advertising 2 1 . Have you heard a similar ad on RADIO? Don’t Yes No Know Have you heard RADIO advertising for the State of Michigan in the past 6 months that... .? ... had a boy describing his vacation in Frankenmuth swimming in an indoor pool, visiting a Christmas land and having his tooth come out. The ad ended with the “Great Don’t Lakes. Great Times.” logo, and gave the Michigan web site for more information. Yes No Know had a boy talking about his fun family vacation in Michigan, swimming, fishing, going on a glass-bottom boat. The ad ended with the “Great Lakes. Great Times.” Don’t _logo, and gave the Michigflweb site for more information. Yes No Know had a child during a show-and-tell presentation for his classmates showing memorabilia that his parents brought back from Michigan. The ad ended with the “Great Lakes. Great Times.” logo, and gave the Michigan web site for more Don’t information. Yes No Know 22. Direct Mail Advertising. 23. Internet Advertising. (Read the list) 101 ‘61” 24. Using a 5-point scale where means you “disagree completely”, and “5” means you “agree completely”, please tell me how well, you think, each statement describes MICHIGAN as a vacation destination. Michigan is a great place to escape from my daily routine Michigan is great for short getaway trips Michigan has great beaches Michigan is great for enjoying quality-time with family or friends Michigan is a great place to relax and unwind There is plenty for me to see and do on a Michigan getaway trip Is close enough for a weekend getaway Is great for summer outdoor recreation activities Is an exciting place, to visit Has a lot of high quality lodging Offers much scenic appeal Is great for winter outdoor recreation activities 15 a good place to meet friendly people Offers exciting nightlife and entertainment Is a great place for a family vacation Has many interesting historic sites Offers an excellent vacation value for the money Has great shopping opportunities Has lovely small towns Has great restaurants 25. In the past 12 months, how many times, if any, have you traveled in Michigan on a pleasure trip? # times 0 — None 26. Do you have specific plans to take a pleasure trip in Michigan in the next 12 months? [ ] —1 Yes => ]Go to question 28.] [ ] —2 No (Read the list) 27. How likely is it that you will take a pleasure trip in Michigan during the next 12 months? [ ]—1 Very likely [ ]-—2 Somewhat likely [ ]—3 Somewhat unlikely [ ] —4 Very unlikely [ ] —(-99) (Do not read) Don’t know 102 To conclude, we'd like to ask just a few questions to help us classify your answers. 28. What is your age? [ ] —(-55) Refused 29. (DO NOT ask) Gender: [ ] -—1 Male [ ] —2 Female (Do not read the list) 30. What is the highest level of school that you have completed? ] —-1 Elementary or less ] —2 Some high school ] —3 High school graduate ] —4 Some college ] —5 College graduate ] —6 Graduate / professional ] —7 Vocational or technical school ] —(-55) Refused ] —(-99) Don’t know f—1f—1Hf_|f—‘f—If—If—!f—1 31. Including yourself, how many people live in your household? # 32. How many children under the age of 18 live in your household? # (Do not read the list. Enter exactly what the respondent says.) 33. What is your current occupation? 1=Professional/technical 2=Managerial 3=Sales 4=Clerical 5=Construction 6=Craftsman 7=Operative 8=Transportation perative 9=Labor 10=Personal service 1 1=Retired 12=Unemployed -55=Refused ~99=DK 103 34. What is your zip code? [ l-(-55) Refused [ ]—(-99) Don’t know 35. What racial or ethnic group do you belong to? i I 4'55) Refiised [ ] —(-99) Don’t know 36. The median household income is $42,000. Would you say your total household income before taxes in 2001 was above or below the median? [ ] —1 Above the median [ ] —2 Below or at the median => ]Go to question 38.] I [ ] —(-55) Refused => ] Go to question 38.] ] —(-99) Don’t know => I Go to question 38] 37. Was your total household income above $75,000? [ l [ ] [ ]—(-55) Refused [ ]—(-99) Don’t know 38. Thank you for your time and have a great day / evening. 104 00:88:08: 0:08 :0: 0:0 :03 00:50:: 0:: 02:» 05 .006. .0808. .020 .30.: 86:0: 2:: 0:8 06000 0.. 2:. .08.: 2:: =6 .5: :05: :6 0::N0w02 0w0020 500500: 0 80:0 ”80:0: 50:30:: 5:07.: 0005.002 0: 0.005 8:0: 0:: :0 0.2:» 0.5: 5030:: ....0 80:08:08: 0:08 :0: 0:0 :03 83:03.: 83:08:55 05 80» 5:0 6:0: ...882. 080 .83 00:0: 0:: 5:3 505:0 50 0:.:. .800585: 5:0 8:00.: 0:502 owaoEU 5:0 0:050:03 0:0: 8565: 055.88: :0::D 0.00:5:05: 8 :05000> 0:: 80:0 wax—0: >0: 0 50: ....m 80:08:88 0:08 :0: 0:5: :03 88:02): 0:: 0>0w 5:0 83:08:55 .0w0_ ...88:. :00:0 .000—0.: :80: 0:: 5:3 505:0 50 0:2. 0:55.: 5:0 85:05: 5:0 .80: 0 08: 85:80.: 0:502 OWNOEU .05: 5:00 05 :305 888:: .885 :00m w:::00_m 05:0 8888 80:0 $5050: :0: 0 50: .. :5 80:08:08: 0:08 :0: 0:0 :03 :0u::0=>: 83:08:55 0:: 02:» 5:0 .0u0_ 8.882. :080 .000—0.— 80:08 0:: 5:3 505:0 50 0:.—. 8000: 00:30:): 0 :0 0::N0w02 .>:. 0w0QEU 850:: :0: 5:3 5:0: 0:: 0: "8.0.: 5:0 0580 5:00 0 ”85:0: 80:0 85:0: Eu 0 5030:: ....m 80:08:88 0:08 :0: 0:0 :03 88:22 05 80: 5:0 .0w0_ .2885. :00:0 80:0: :00:0: 05 0:50: 008:5 5:3 0608 0: 60.: .80: 60886 0:8 .600 :5: .83 2:: 8 00%: a 32:6 08:8 :0 ... 00:. .. .0 00:08:08: 0:08 :0: 08.550 :03 3 05 80: 83:08:55 5:0 6:0: ...88::. :00:0 80:5 :00:0.. 05 5:3 505:0 50 0::. 00:05: 0:: 555 5:00 0:: 8 was: 5:0 QEwawz .>.H ommoEU 8:000: 5:0 8:0— .8805:w= 8088:): 8 :0::000> 3:80: :0: 0:: 80:0 85:0: :0: 0 5030:: .— .EE an; wflUfl—OwEQrfia on: aflosommuho>fia :0:—:3 :w:0::: .0::0:0 0:: :0 <29 0058885 ::080m:::0>5< <55 5:0 .000—0:0 : 3008005850 0:: :0 :0:: :88: ”N 0:500 < 105 6003088: 08:08.: 58.0 >:00:.:. 088896;: .80: 880:0": .> 5:0 .:: .0 .0m05:0m 80:88:08: 0:08 :0: 0::: :03 00:80:): 0:: 08» 5:0 .80: 0.885. 80:0 .884 80:03 0:: :::3 505:0 50 0::. .80: 80::0:-::0_u 0 850M: 5:0—95:0 :0 :80: .883: 888853: 80080:: 8 00:800.» 3:80: 8:: ::: 80:0 005:0: m0: 0 50: ...N: 80:88:08: 0:08 :0: 0::: :03 008:0:2 0:: 030: 5:0 .000: 0.882. 80:0 :0:—0‘: 80:03 0:: :::3 505:0 50 0::. .80 0800 :80: :8 «0:8: 5:0 5:0: :08:::::0 350M: 58:0>0:U 0 M8383 .80.: :0058 :0 0: ”888:3: 50880088.: 8 00:800.» :8 :0::8085 :0: 0 50: ...: 80:88:08: 588.820 ”0800902 .>.:. 5:0:0>0:U 0:08 :0: 0::: :03 00:80:): 0:: 08: 5:0 0:0: :.:08:.:. 80:0 :0:—0.: 80:03 0:: :::3 505:0 50 80:08:05 6:00: 2:808:08 2.: .22.: 02:... 2.: 2.2.252. .2: 0.2.0: 3 ...: 8.2.2; :2. 2.2.0 05...: ...: a ..2. ...... 5:0—26:0 80:88:08: 0:08 :0: 0::: :03 00:80:: 0:: 08» 5:0 0:0: ...:08:.:. 83:08:55 80:0 80:01—80:03 0:: :::3 505:0 50 0: .H 80:80:): 80:: 0:00: 8000:: 800:0.— ::: 8:: 850M ow0oEU 0::::0:0808 0830:: 888880 ::: :0: 8:880:08 :0:-5:0..30:: 0 ”8:05 5:80 0 50: ....0 80:88:08: 0:08 :0: 0::: :03 00:80:: 0:: 08» 5:0 0:2 0.883. 80:0 :0:—0‘: 80:0: 0:: :::3 505:0 50 0::. .885 0::N0w02 ow00£U :00: «8.005 80:0 800:: 0 5008808 5:0 :000: 0:: :0 8:30: 8 9:0: 03: 5030:: ....0 00:88:08: 0:08 :0: 0::: :03 88:00): 0:: 0.30» 5:0 0:0: :.:08:.:. 80:0 80:0.— 80:0.. 0:: :::3 505:0 50 250% ow00£U 0: :. .:0:::: :8 :::3 880: «85:: 5:0 ::0:0: 00:80:): 0 :0 088:: 80:0 ”80:0: :0: 0 50: .55 50.8:800 N 85:08:. 106 Appendix 3: Descriptive Statistics by DMA Variable Chicago Cleveland Indianapolis Total Number % Number % Number % Age 45 - 47 - 47 - 46 Gender: Female 222 32.2 229 33.2 238 34.5 689 Male 134 31.5 146 34.3 146 34.3 426 Education Elementary 2 4O 2 40 1 20 5 Some High School 6 42.9 2 14.3 6 42.9 14 High School Graduate 66 24.7 99 37.1 102 38.2 267 Some college 77 32 83 34.4 81 33.6 241 College Graduate 127 36.2 111 31.6 113 32.2 351 Graduate/Professional 69 34.7 64 3 2.2 66 33 .2 199 Technical school 4 18.2 10 45.5 8 36.4 22 Number of people in H. Hold 1 57 32.9 57 32.9 59 34.1 173 2 118 29.8 136 34.4 141 35.7 395 3 57 28.7 69 34.8 72 36.4 198 4 69 36.9 61 32.6 57 30.5 187 5 40 37.7 33 31.1 33 31.1 106 6 4 16 10 4O 11 44 25 7 2 22.2 3 33.3 4 44.4 9 8 l 50 0 O 1 50 2 9 0 0 l 50 l 50 2 107 Appendix 3 contd. 10 l 100 0 0 0 0 1 Number of children under 18 0 229 33.3 225 32.8 233 33.9 687 1 53 30.5 58 33.3 63 36.2 174 2 48 31.6 55 36.2 49 32.2 152 3 18 28.1 21 32.8 25 39.1 64 4 3 23.1 6 46.2 4 30.8 13 5 0 0 0 O 4 100 4 6 0 0 1 50 1 50 2 Annual H. Hold income $45K but < $75K 172 32.3 101 33.2 184 34.5 533 > $75K 124 38.8 325 30.3 99 30.9 320 Racial/ethnic group Asian/Pacific Islander 11 78.6 1 7.1 2 14.3 14 Black/A. American 23 33.8 23 33.8 22 32.4 68 Hispanic 15 83.3 3 16.7 0 0 18 White/Caucasian 278 29.8 321 34.4 333 35.7 932 Multi-racial 3 33.3 3 33.3 3 33.3 9 Other 5 25 6 30 9 45 20 108 Appendix 4: Test for Appropriateness of data set for factor analysis KMO and Bartlett's Test Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin Measure of Sampling Adequacy. .968 Bartlett's Test of Sphericity Approx. Chi-Square 7020.503 df 23 1 Sig. .000 109 CHIGA STAYE UNIV SJTV LIBRAR S 1 111111111 1 llWill/Hill!Hill/(111111111711! 3 02845 4068