"u an (r-rvxi a .31.. V} 5.. ‘24 3.... ,.... . .C . . . ‘ . ...‘.u.......n. . I .5 if . x. .3; :5} . . \.:I 3 .. 1 A .v ..:«.v .1. ..... .r.r iv. a" a 'LO v-Lw A. W‘Iflflvlfi‘l I I .4 («up Me: wad: a ‘ . o‘otow'rvO-r.0‘0 . P v. 1.: .. ... This is to certify that the dissertation entitled Americans’ Intentions to Vacation in East Asia: An Empirical Investigation of the Relationships among Information Source Usage, Destination Image, Perceived Risks, and Intention to VISit presented by Jeonghee Noh has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for the Park, Recreation, and Tourism Resources Ph.D degree in WM Major Professor’s Signature Date MSU is an Al‘finnativo ActiorVEquaI Opportunity Institution iiiii —> 7 v LIBRARY Michigan State University a.-.-n-Con-O.-.-o--a-o-o--aO0-:-n-o-o---o-c-n-n---o--‘--.-n----uno-v-o-o-u-s-u-u-u-u-o-u-o- PLACE IN RETURN Box to remove this checkout from your record. To AVOID FINES return on or before date due. MAY BE RECALLED with earlier due date if requested. DATE DUE DATE DUE DATE DUE 2/05 p:/C|RC/Dale0ue.indd-p.1 AMERICANS’ INTENTIONS TO VACATION IN EAST ASIA: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION OF THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG INFORMATION SOURCE USAGE, DESTINATION IMAGE, PERCEIVED RISKS, AND INTENTION TO VISIT By Jeonghee Noh A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University In partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Park, Recreation, and Tourism Resources 2006 ABSTRACT AMERICANS’ INTENTIONS TO VACATION IN EAST ASIA: AN EMPIRICAL INVESTIGATION OF THE RELATIONSHIPS AMONG INFORMATION SOURCE USAGE, DESTINATION IMAGE, PERCEIVED RISKS, AND INTENTION TO VISIT By Jeonghee Noh In 2004, 22.3 million Americans vacationed overseas, increasing by 11 percent since 2003 (TIA, 2005). However, only 10 percent of these tourists visited China, Japan, or South Korea. To attract more American tourists to these destinations, East-Asia-based tourism marketers must better understand which factors contribute to Americans’ decisions to vacation for the first time in China, Japan, and South Korea. The aim of this research was to deveIOp, and to empirically test, a model explaining Americans’ intentions to vacation for the first time in China, Japan, and South Korea. Based on a review of the relevant literature, information source usage, cognitive and affective destination image, and perceived risks were identified as key factors influencing travelers’ intentions to visit East Asia. Data were collected using a self-administered, on-site survey. Only American citizens, 18 years of age or older, who had never visited‘China, Japan, or South Korea, were included in the participant sample. Structural equation modeling (SEM) was used to test the proposed conceptual model relative to these data. The results of the SEM analyses showed that respondents reporting both a high (emotionally neutral) cognitive destination image and a high (emotionally positive) affective destination image of China, Japan, and South Korea, as well as low perceived risks of vacationing in these three countries, had greater intention to visit them. Similarly, respondents reporting a high cognitive image of China, Japan, and South Korea and low perceived risks of vacationing in these countries also reported having high affective image of these destinations. Additionally, respondents who frequently obtained information on China, Japan, and South Korea were found to have a high cognitive image of these three countries and low perceived risks of vacationing there. However, information source usage was not found to directly influence affective image. SEM analysis showed that information source usage indirectly influenced affective image through cognitive image and perceived risks. Cultural experiences was the information source most strongly correlated with Americans’ intentions to visit China, Japan, and South Korea. Similarly, for all three countries the strongest cognitive image items were interesting historical attractions, many things to see and do, and unique cultural resources. Despite these similarities in participants’ responses for China, Japan, and South Korea, some differences were identified. One key difference was that, depending on the country, a different factor was shown to be most strongly correlated with intention to visit; for China, it was affective image; for Japan, it was cognitive image; and, for South Korea, it was perceived risks. These findings suggest that tourism marketers should jointly sponsor cultural events in the US. to promote vacations in China, Japan, and South Korea. Joint trips, in which Americans would visit similar attractions in all three countries could also be marketed. Not only were cultural experiences identified as an information source to be studied in firture research, but study results also demonstrated the value of studying cognitive and affective destination image as being distinct but interrelated. A conceptual model is presented that might be tested in the future regarding other underperforrning international tourism regions. Copyright by J eonghee Noh 2006 First and foremost, this dissertation is dedicated to my parents, my father MyungSun Noh and my mother ChoonJa Lim, for their unconditional love and support, which has given me the confidence to face the world. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS My journey to completing my doctoral program has been long and arduous. Without the considerable support of the people mentioned below, it would not have been possible. In this space, I express my sincere appreciation. I wish to thank the members of my dissertation committee for their constant encouragement in helping me to complete this dissertation. I am extremely grateful for the invaluable support that I have received from my dissertation director and committee chair, Dr. Donald Holecek. Providing the right balance of challenge and support as I drafted my dissertation, Dr. Holecek was a true mentor. His wisdom will continue to guide me long after I have left Michigan State University. Dr. Carl Brochegrevink, a professor in Hospitality Business, served as my academic advisor throughout my Master’s and doctoral programs, as well as one of my dissertation committee members. Dr. Brochegrevink has always been there to listen, to counsel, and to inspire. I have greatly appreciated his advice on navigating the Ph.D. and academic life beyond. Dr. Christine Vogt generously shared her insights on the research methods employed in this study. Dr. Vogt has been my role model as a researcher, as a teacher, and as a woman. Dr. Sarah Nicholls also offered useful advice, especially during the writing process. My friends and colleagues also deserve my sincere thanks for their support. ‘ Soyeon Ahn, Jaemin Cha, Sejung Choi, KaiLonnie Dunsmore, Chang Huh, Hyunsoon Park, SungHee Park, MiKyung Kim, Miran Kim, SeungHyun Kim, Eunsil Lee, Michael vi Morris, Lori Langone, Dottie Schmidt, Charles Jinyen Shih, Kyungoh Song, and Mark VerNooy. I will never forget the happy moments that we have shared together and the tough times that I would not have overcome without them. I am especially grateful to J inyoung Choi for her endless encouragement, smile, and warmth. Together, we have celebrated many milestones in the United States. Whenever I needed true understanding from a friend, J inyoung was always there. My special thanks also go to Ann Lawrence, a friend and mentor throughout all of the dissertation process, whose help enabled me to finish this hard process. I cannot articulate sufficiently how much I thank J inyoung Choi and Ann Lawrence. Additionally, I would like to thank Hyunjoo Choi for assisting me with collecting data for the dissertation study. Without Hyunjoo’s enormous charisma, the sample size for this study would no doubt have been much smaller and the ultimate findings, less interesting. Most importantly, I would like to dedicate this dissertation to my parents, MyungSun Noh and ChoonJ a Lim, and to my brother Kookjin and sisters Yoonhee and Heejung. I thank them for loving, supporting, and praying for me. Despite traveling a great distance, their love has always reached me. To my family, I owe all that I have become. vii TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES .......................................................................................................... xi LIST OF FIGURES ........................................................................................................ xiii CHAPTER 1 INTRODUCTION .......................................................................................................... 1 Purpose of the study ................................................................................................ 4 Organization of the Dissertation ............................................................................. 5 CHAPTER 2 LITERATURE REVIEW ............................................................................................... 6 Destination Image ................................................................................................... 6 Components of Destination Image .................................................................. 11 Destination Image Formation Processes and Information Sources ................ 12 Information source usage ...................................................................... 15 Direct experiences with the destination ................................................ 17 Proximity to the destination .................................................................. 19 Influence of Destination Image on Travelers’ Visit Intentions ...................... 20 Perceived Risks of Vacationing at a Destination .................................................... 21 Types of Risks Associated with the Destination and Their Influences on Travel Behavior .............................................................................................. 22 Role of Information Sources in Perceived Risks ............................................ 25 Proposed Conceptual Model ................................................................................... 27 Hypothesized Relationships ............................................................................ 28 CHAPTER 3 RESEARCH DESIGN AND METHODOLOGY .......................................................... 29 Data Collection and Analysis Procedures ............................................................... 29 Data Collection ............................................................................................... 29 Sub-sample Size .............................................................................................. 30 Profile of Respondents .................................................................................... 31 Survey Instrument and Scales ................................................................................. 33 Destination Image ........................................................................................... 33 Perceived Risks ............................................................................................... 34 Information Source Usage .............................................................................. 34 Intention to Visit ............................................................................................. 34 viii Data Analysis .......................................................................................................... 35 CHAPTER 4 RESULTS ....................................................................................................................... 40 Preliminary Analyses .............................................................................................. 41 Data Screening ................................................................................................ 41 Description of the Variables ........................................................................... 43 Analysis of Mean Differences among the Three Countries ............................ 51 Testing the Measurement Model ............................................................................ 55 The China Model ............................................................................................ 58 Assessment of overall model fit ............................................................ 58 Assessment of reliability and validity ................................................... 59 The Japan Model ..................................................... . ....................................... 61 Assessment of overall model fit ............................................................ 61 Assessment of reliability and validity ................................................... 62 The South Korea Model .................................................................................. 64 Assessment of overall model fit ............................................................ 64 Assessment of reliability and validity ................................................... 65 Comparison of the Measurement Model for the Three Countries. ................. 67 Testing the Structural Equation Model ................................................................... 67 The China Model ............................................................................................ 69 The Japan Model ............................................................................................. 71 The South Korea Model .................................................................................. 73 Comparison of the Structural Equation Model for the Three Countries ......... 75 Hypotheses Testing ................................................................................................. 75 The China Model ............................................................................................ 76 Direct influence related to study hypotheses ........................................ 77 Multiple squared correlation ................................................................. 79 Indirect and total influences of the factors on visit intention ................ 79 The Japan Model ............................................................................................. 80 Direct influence related to study hypotheses ........................................ 82 Multiple squared correlation ....................................................... 84 Indirect and total influences of the factors on visit intention ................ 84 The South Korea Model .................................................................................. 86 Direct influence related to study hypotheses ........................................ 87 Multiple squared correlation ................................................................. 89 ix Indirect and total influences of the factors on visit intention ................ 89 Comparison of the results of hypotheses testing for the three East Asian countries .......................................................................................................... 90 Additional Analysis: The Comparative Influence of Different Types of Information Source on Destination Image and Perceived Risks .......................................................... 93 CHAPTER 5 DISCUSSION, IMPLICATIONS, AND CONCLUSIONS ........................................... 95 Summary and Discussion of the Findings .............................................................. 95 Mean Differences of the Variables among Three East Asian Countries ........ 96 Results of Hypotheses Testing ........................................................................ 99 Overall Pattern of Relationships among the Factors across the Three Countries ......................................................................................................... 104 Relationships between Types of Information Source Used and Destination Image and Perceived Risks .......................................................... 105 Implications ............................................................................................................. 106 Theoretical Implications ................................................................................. 106 Practical Implications ...................................................................................... 108 Limitations of this Study and Suggestions for Future Research ............................. 111 Conclusions ............................................................................................................. 1 12 APPENDIX: Survey Questions ...................................................................................... 114 REFERENCES ............................................................................................................... 1 19 LIST OF TABLES Table 1. Overview of previous tourism research on destination image ........................... 7 Table 2. Distribution of respondents who have and have not visited China, Japan, ....... 31 Table 3. Characteristics of respondents who have never visited China, Japan, and South Korea. ...................................................................................................... 32 Table 4. Constructs and observed variables in the proposed model. ............................... 37 Table 5. Normalitya test results for variables included in the proposed model ............... 42 Table 6. Mean and SD of intention to visit the three countries. ...................................... 43 Table 7. Mean and SD of cognitive destination image of the three countries. ................ 44 Table 8. Mean and SD of affective image of the three countries. ................................... 46 Table 9. Mean and SD of perceived risks of visiting the three countries. ....................... 48 Table 10. Mean and SD of information source usage (type and frequency) for the three countries. ................................................................................................. 50 Table 11. Results of Repeated Measure ANOVA. .......................................................... 52 Table 12. The recommended goodness-of-fit indices ...................................................... 57 Table 13. Comparison of the proposed and modified full measurement models for China. ............................................................................................................... 59 Table 14. Standardized factor loadings for the modified measurement model for China. ............................................................................................................... 60 Table 15. Correlation analysis of factors for China. ........................................................ 61 Table 16. Comparison of the proposed and modified full measurement models for Japan. ............................................................................................................... 62 Table 17. Standardized factor loadings for the modified measurement model for Japan. ............................................................................................................... 63 Table 18. Correlation analysis of factors for Japan. ........................................................ 64 Table 19. Comparison of the initial and modified full measurement models for South Korea ....... 65 Table 20. Standardized factor loadings for the modified measurement model for South Korea. .................................................................................................... 66 Table 21. Correlation analysis among factors for South Korea. ...................................... 67 Table 22. Comparison of the initial and modified structural models for China. ............. 71 Table 23. Comparison of the initial and modified structural models for Japan. .............. 73 Table 24. Comparison of the initial and modified structural models for South Korea... 75 Table 25. Results of hypotheses testing for China ........................................................... 76 Table 26. Standardized estimates for direct, indirect, and total influences for China. 80 xi Table 27. Results of hypotheses testing for Japan. .......................................................... 81 Table 28. Standardized estimates for direct, indirect, and total influences for Japan ...... 84 Table 29. Results of hypotheses testing for South Korea. ............................................... 86 Table 30. Standardized estimates for direct, indirect, and total influences for South Korea. ............................................................................................................... 89 Table 31. Results of hypotheses testing across three East Asian countries. .................... 91 Table 32. Comparison of total influence .......................................................................... 92 Table 33. Correlation between types of information source used and cognitive image and perceived risk. ........................................................................................... 93 xii LIST OF FIGURES Figure l. A model of a tourist’s image formation process ............................................... 14 Figure 2. Theoretical Model of Intention to Visit Three East Asia countries .................. 27 Figure 3. The proposed measurement model for the three countries. .............................. 56 Figure 4. The pr0posed structural equation model. ......................................................... 68 Figure 5. Modified SEM for China. ................................................................................. 70 Figure 6. Results of testing the proposed structural model with standardized path coefficients and squared multiple correlations (R2 ) for China. ....................... 77 Figure 7. Results of testing the proposed structural model with standardized path coefficients and squared multiple correlations (R2) for Japan. ........................ 82 Figure 8. Results of testing the proposed structural model with standardized path coefficients and squared multiple correlations (R2) for South Korea. ............. 87 Figure 9. Proposed conceptual model .............................................................................. 99 xiii CHAPTER 1 INTRODUCTION International leisure travel is an important market for the international tourism industry in terms of its size and economic importance. According to the World Tourism Organization’s Tourism 2020 Vision (2006), international tourist arrivals are expected to reach over 1.6 billion in the year 2020. This is more than twice the number in 2000. The number of American overseas leisure travelers has also increased from 2003 to 2004. Approximately 22.3 million American visited overseas destinations in 2004: an increase of 11.2 percent since 2003 (Travel Industry Association of America, 2005). Of the 7.6 percent of Americans who vacationed abroad in 2004, 17 percent visited Asian countries, while only 9.8 percent visited China, Japan, or South Korea. A much larger portion of American travelers vacationed in other destinations, such as Europe (43 percent of the total) and the Caribbean (20 percent of the total). These American travelers are a potential market for East-Asia-based tourism marketers. Despite the promise of the East Asian tourism market, most tourism research has been conducted on the largest international tourism markets (America and Europe) by primarily American and European tourism researchers. To capitalize on Americans’ interest in vacationing in East Asian countries, East Asian tourism marketers must understand why relatively few Americans have decided to vacation in China, Japan, and South Korea, and how to attract larger numbers of potential American tourists to East Asian countries. However, tourism researchers must first provide tourism marketers with this information. This study, conducted by a South Korean tourism researcher, was an attempt to identify the factors influencing Americans’ intentions to vacation in China, Japan, and South Korea for both international tourism researchers and East-Asia-based tourism marketers. For several decades, research in travel and tourism has demonstrated that destination image is a critical factor influencing travelers’ decision-making behavior (Court & Lupton, 1997; Milman & Pizam, 1995; Rittichainuwat, Qu, & Brown, 2001; Ross, 1993; Urn & Crompton, 1992; Woodside & Lysonski, 1989). Positive images of a destination encourage travelers to choose it over other possible destinations. For this reason, destination marketers have competed progressively more intensely to produce positive destination images in travelers’ minds, and have tried to develop positions that differentiate their destinations from other attractive sites (Baloglu & McCleary, 1999b). Based on research reporting the strengths and weaknesses of their destinations, tourism marketers have developed marketing programs to create and enhance favorable images, or correct negative images, of their destination. For East-Asia-based tourism marketers to design programs that entice prospective American tourists to China, Japan, and South Korea, a thorough understanding of Americans’ images of these three countries is crucial. Numerous tourism researchers have examined prospective and actual tourists’ destination images of cities, regions, states, and countries in order to help tourism marketers design effective positioning strategies. Destination image studies have compared travelers’ evaluations of tourism-related attributes across particular destinations (Goodrich, 1978; Hunt, 1975; Javalgi, Thomas, & Rao, 1992) and identified which attributes are strong or weak, given the destination (Ahmed, 1991; Chou, 1991; Court & Lupton, 1997; Gartner & Hunt, 1987). However, past tourism research has tended to center on cognitive destination image (knowledge about a place), largely overlooking affective destination image (feelings about a place). Contrary to this trend, recent studies (Baloglu & Brinberg, 1997; Gartner, 1993) have proposed that both travelers’ cognitive and affective destination images deserve consideration. The present study attempted to extend this recent body of knowledge on destination image by employing a two-dimensional framework (cognitive and affective) to assess Americans’ images of China, Japan, and South Korea. Tourism research has also shown that travelers consider their perceived risks of vacationing in a destination, as well as their image of the destination in general, before making travel decisions (Coshall, 2003; Lam & Hsu, 2006). Even though travelers may perceive a destination as having many attractive features (e. g., interesting cultural resources and good night life), if they consider vacationing in this destination to be a risk, they may decide not to visit the destination (Crompton, Fakeye, & Lue, 1992). Perceived risks of vacationing in a destination have become especially influential on travelers’ decision-making behavior in recent years, given international awareness of tragic events occurring around the globe (Coshall, 2003; Faulkner, 2001; Floyd, Gibson, Pennington-Gray, & Thapa, 2003; Mazzocchi & Montini, 2001 ). For example, travel in the Asia-Pacific region has been devastated by Severe Acute Respiratory Syndrome (SARS), bird flu, and the tsunami that struck Southeastern Asia. Because of the considerable influence of tourists’ perceived risks of vacationing in a destination on their travel decisions, East-Asia-based tourism marketers must understand American travelers’ reasons for avoiding China, Japan, and South Korea, as well as their reasons for visiting these countries. Different types of perceived risk affect tourists’ travel decisions (Roehl & Fesenmaier, 1992). However, previous tourism studies have isolated travelers’ perceived risks (e. g., terrorism, natural disasters, and disease) in considering their effects on travel behavior (Coshall, 2003; Faulkner, 2001; Floyd et a1., 2003; Gartner & Shen, 1992; Mazzocchi & Montini, 2001; Sonmez & Graefe, 1998a, 1998b). These studies have not considered the combined effect of these individual perceived risks on intention to visit. Such comprehensive information would be useful to East-Asia—based tourism marketers seeking to attract first-time American tourists to China, Japan, and South Korea. Travelers’ image of a destination in general and perceived risks of vacationing in a destination are determined by the types of information source travelers consult on a destination (Baloglu, 1999; Beerli & Martin, 2004b; F akeye & Crompton, 1991; Gunn, 1972). Although several tourism studies have compared the differential effects on travel behavior of direct experience and indirect information source usage, no study before the present dissertation has investigated the influence of type and frequency of indirect information source usage on destination image and perceived risks regarding China, Japan, and South Korea, findings which would greatly benefit East-Asia—based tourism marketers. Purpose of the Study Given these gaps in previous tourism research, the purpose of the present study was to develop and empirically test a conceptual model based on the findings of previous tourism studies that would explain prospective American tourists’ intentions to visit China, Japan, and South Korea for the first time. To this end, the present study sought to determine the interrelationships among information source usage (type and frequency), cognitive and affective destination image, perceived risks, and visit intention, in order to offer both theoretical and practical recommendations to international tourism researchers and East-Asia—based tourism marketers. Organization of the Dissertation A general background for the present study and discussion of its purpose and significance are provided in Chapter One. Relevant literature is reviewed in Chapter Two. The methodological specifications of this study, including procedures relating to study design, data collection, and data analysis are detailed in Chapter Three. The findings of this study are presented in Chapter Four. Finally, theoretical and practical implications derived from these findings are outlined in Chapter Five. CHAPTER 2 LITERATURE REVIEW Previous studies were reviewed in order to develop a conceptual model that explains American tourists’ intentions to visit China, Japan, and South Korea. The results of these studies are summarized in the three sections of this chapter. In the first section, previous research on destination image—its components, its formation processes as they relate to information source usage on a destination, and its influence on tourists’ intentions to visit a destination——is presented. In the second section, previous studies on travelers’ perceived risks of vacationing in a destination are discussed; specifically, types of perceived risk, the relationship between perceived risks and information source usage, and the influence of perceived risks on visit intention. In the final section, previous research is synthesized to create a conceptual model that would potentially clarify the interrelationships among destination image, perceived risks, and information source usage, and explain American tourists’ intentions to visit China, Japan, and South Korea. Destination Image In tourism research, destination image has been considered as an important concept in explaining travelers’ decision-making processes and tourism behavior. Assessing destination image helps destination marketers to design effective positioning strategies, which create, correct, or enhance target travelers’ destination image (Pike & Ryan, 2004). Researchers in the tourism field began to conceptualize destination image in the 19703. Although a number of researchers have attempted to conceptualize the construct, a precise definition of this term is problematic. Generally, destination image refers to the sum of beliefs, ideas, and impressions that people have about a destination (Baloglu & Brinberg, 1997; Crompton, 1979). The image-related literature has focused primarily on the following issues: (1) the components of destination image, (2) destination image formation processes in relation to information sources, and (3) the influence of destination image on travelers’ behavior. Highlights from destination image studies conducted since 1975 on American tourists are grouped according to destination and presented in Table 1. Table 1. Overview of previous tourism research on destination image. 1) In the US. Author(s) Destination Destination Image(s) 1m! age(s)l Hunt (1975) Colorado, Montana, Utah, Disparities between the summer and Cognitive and Wyoming winter climates at home and the destination caused respondents to have a negative image of the destination. Goodrich California, Florida, and California was perceived to possess Cognitive (1978) Hawaii suitable accommodations, entertainment, and cuisine. Florida and Hawaii were perceived to possess water sports, golfing, and suitable accommodations. Fridgen (1987) Michigan Respondents familiar with the Cognitive destination had a more favorable image of the destination. Gartner & Hunt Utah Respondents living closer to the Cognitive (1987) destination were more impressed by the state than respondents living farther away. Those who had previously visited the destination had a generally more favorable image of the state than those who had never visited it. Table 1: Overview of previous tourism research on destination image (cont’d). Respondents living in nearby states answered that the destination was more friendly and less unique as a vacation destination, and that it had many more places of interest to visit, than did respondents living farther away. 1) In the US. Author(s) Destination Destination Image(s) Ilmage(s)l Reilly (1990) Montana and Montana Respondents living closer to Cognitive ski area destination had a stronger and more positive image of the state. Potential visitors from distant areas lacked a ___W _ _ W ,, vivid image of the state. .- _______-__... Ahmed (1991) Utah Perceptions were generally more cognitive favorable for those who had , .. W WW - W previously visited destination. m... -__ W - Chon, Weaver Norfolk, Virginia The three most satisfying features Cognitive & Kim (1991) were suitable accommodations, easy access, and the variety and quality . - _ _W __W WW _ W of attractions. -_.- __ _._____ . W Fakeye & Lower Rio Grande Valley Repeat visitors are well aware of the Cognitive Crompton in Texas destination’ 3 attributes. (1991) The more frequent the visit to the destination, the more recognizable and appreciated its social __ _, ______,...-_._ ,_ . opportunities and attractions. _ , _W_ W __ Milman & Central Florida Respondents familiar with the Cognitive Pizam (1995) destination had a more positive image of it and were more interested in the destination and more likely to _. ____ WW -- revisit. , . , .__ Court & Lupton New Mexico A positive relationShip existed Cognitive (1997) between prior experiences with the destination and intention to visit: ___-.._._... _ __ W ._ . W Climate was not influential. -_.._ W_____WW Vogt & Arizona Whereas cognitive image improved Cognitive Anderecek during a trip, affective Image, when And (2003) positive before a trip, remained Affective ___ _ ___ . . stable. ,. _...__ -_- Hsu, Wolfe & Kansas Visitors had a more positive image Cognitive Kang (2004) of the destination than non-visitors. Table 1: Overview of previous tourism research on destination image (cont’d). 2) Near the US. Author(s) Destination Destination Image(s) Image(s) Assessed Goodrich (1978) Mexico and five West Indian Islands (Bahamas, Barbados, Jamaica, Puerto Rico, and Virgin Islands) Five West Indian Islands were perceived as being similar. These areas were perceived as having rest, relaxation, and water sports. Mexico was perceived as being an historical and cultural interesting site. Cognitive Crompton ( l 979) Mexico There were significant differences between ideal images and actual images of the destination as a tourism site. Respondents living farther away from the destination had a more favorable image of it. The two most positive attributes were climate and low cost. Cognitive Joppe, Martin &Waalen (2001) Toronto, Canada American visitors were concerned about personal safety, value for money, and the hospitality of local people. The three most satisfying features were attractions, transportation, and shopping facilities. Cognitive 3) In Eurom Author(s) Destination Destination Image(s) Image(s) Assessed Phelps (1986) Minorca First-time visitors were more concerned about violence and poverty, and had a vague impression of the destination. Retum visitors had a detailed impression of the destination and were more interested in natural environment and traditional cultural sites. Cognitive Pizam(l991) U.S.S.R. Post- trip respondents showed more positive images than did pre-trip respondents. Cognitive and Affective Javalgi, Thomsa & Rao ( 1 992) British Isles, Central Europe, Scandinavian countries, and Southern Europe Better-educated, employed and retired travelers with a higher annual income chose this destination. Younger travelers (aged 24 and under) were more attracted to Southern Europe than to the other three regions. Cognitive Table 1: Overview of previous tourism research on destination image (cont’d) 3) In Europe . . . . Image(s) Author(s) Destlnatlon Dest1nat10n Image(s) Assessed Baloglu & Four Mediterranean Most cognitive images were Cognitive and McCleary destinations (Egypt, similar between visitors and Affective (1999b) Greece, Italy, Turkey,) non-visitors. Egypt was perceived less positively than Turkey, Italy, and Greece. Baloglu (2001) Turkey Age and education positively Cognitive and correlated with familiarity and Affective familiarity influenced destination image. Respondents more familiar with the destination had a more positive image of the destination. 4) Asia . . . . Image(s) Author(s) Destlnation 1 Destmatlon Image(s) Assessed Chon (1991) South Korea Y Post trip respondents showed Cognitive more positive images than pre- trip respondents. Gartner & Shen China Afier the Tiananmen Square Cognitive (1992) conflict, tourists showed more negative images of China .. _- chanaIedto before the event. Choi, Chan & Hong Kong The destination was considered Cognitive Wu (1999) as a “shopping paradise” but also as a crowded, busy, and stressful urban environment. Rittichainuwat Thailand The more positive and the fewer Cognitive (2001) negative images of a destination, the more likely travelers were to return to that destination. Suh & Seoul, South Korea Business travelers’ were more Cognitive and Gartner (2004) likely to focus on security, Affective opportunities to increase knowledge, and overall atmosphere; pleasure travelers were more likely to focus on opportunities for adventure and overall cost. Kim & South Korea Respondents having either a Cognitive Morrsion negative image or no image of (2005) the destination had a positive image after watching matches on TV hosted by the destination. 10 Componenm of Destination Image The most recent studies (Baloglu & Brinberg, 1997; Gartner, 1993) tend to agree that destination image is composed of two components: cognitive and affective. Cognitive image includes beliefs and knowledge about destination attributes, whereas affective image comprises feelings, moods, and emotions toward a destination. Cognitive destination image is, thus, emotionally neutral, while affective destination image is either emotionally positive (e. g., arousing, pleasant, exciting, and relaxing) or negative (e.g., sleepy, unpleasant, gloomy, and distressing). As shown in Table 1, the earliest destination image studies focused only on cognitive destination image (Ahmed, 1991; Chon, Weaver, & Kim, 1991; Gartner & Hunt, 1987; Goodrich, 1978; Hunt, 1975; Phelps, 1986; Reilly, 1990). Some of this research compared travelers’ evaluations of cognitive destination attributes across particular destinations (Goodrich, 1978; Hunt, 1975; Javalgi et al., 1992) in order to design positioning strategies for these destinations. For example, in their study of American tourists’ images of European destinations (the British Isles, Central Europe, Scandinavia, and Southern Europe), Javalgi, Thomas, and Rao (1992) found that travelers had different knowledge of each destination. On the other hand, some tourism researchers sought to identify which cognitive image attributes were strong and weak for a target destination in order to develop more effective tourism-marketing strategies (Ahmed, 1991; Chon et al., 1991; Court & Lupton, 1997; Gartner & Hunt, 1987). Affective destination image has traditionally been overlooked in tourism research. Reviewing the 142 studies on destination image from 1973 to 2000, Pike (2002) found 11 that only six studies examined affective image attributes. Of the studies that focused on Americans’ destination images, only 4.2 percent examined their affective component. Recently, some researchers have argued that total destination image cannot be understood without considering both its cognitive and affective dimensions (Baloglu & Brinberg, 1997; Gartner, 1993). Baloglu and Brinberg (1997) noted that cognitive destination image is insufficient in explaining travelers’ complex of impressions about a place. Using Russell, Ward, and Pratt’s (1981) binary framework (e.g., unpleasant/pleasant, and sleepy/arousing) to measure affective destination image, Baloglu and Brinberg (1997) sought to develop a more complete understanding of travelers’ destination image. Their study provided empirical evidence supporting the applicability of this framework to tourism contexts. Baloglu and McCleary (1999a) later developed a path analytic model of Americans’ destination image formation regarding four Mediterranean countries, a model which addressed both its cognitive and affective components, employing the term overall image to encompass these two destination image components. Baloglu and McCleary found that destination image formation proceeded sequentially: While cognitive and affective image directly influenced travelers’ overall image, cognitive image also influenced affective image. Baloglu and McCleary’s study demonstrated that cognitive and affective image components are distinct but interrelated. Destination Image Formation Processes and Information Source Usage The existing literature suggests that a tourist’s image of a destination is formed through a multi-stage process. According to Gunn (1972), the initial image formation is largely a 12 function of information source usage before the first visit to the destination. Gunn noted that destination image formation proceeds in two stages: organic image followed by induced image. Gunn asserts that organic image is formed by non-touristic and non- commercial information sources, such as popular culture, education, news reports in print and on TV, word of mouth from family and friends, and other non-tourism-specific information sources. However, Gunn states that induced image is a function of destination marketing. Adapting Gunn’s model, F akeye and Crompton (1991) added complex image to their model of a tourist’s destination image formation process. In their model, destination image formation proceeds in three stages: (1) organic image, (2) induced image, and (3) complex image. Figure 1 shows a model of the image formation process developed by Fakeye and Crompton. (Italicized portions have been added for clarification.) l3 Exposure to non-touristic information sources p——_————-—-———- Organic Image Active information search and processing of commercial materials ‘mm-m-------------- Evaluation of alternative destinations’ benefits and images Induced Image Destination selection and visitation Complex Image Figure 1. A model of a tourist’s image formation process. Modified from Fakeye and Crompton (1991). According to Fakeye and Crompton’s model, first, organic image is developed in travel customers through exposure to non-touristic and non-commercial information sources. This information familiarizes potential tourists, to some extent, with a destination and provides them with some knowledge of a destination. Second, induced image is produced when travel customers, having decided to take a trip, seek out promotional materials from tourism suppliers. This information is often called persuasive promotion and is most effective once prospective tourists have already developed an organic image 14 of a destination. Third, travel customers achieve a complex image of a destination once they have visited this place and had the opportunity to integrate their own impressions of the destination with their previous organic and induced images of the place. Complex image may be reactivated through promotional materials, once travel customers have returned home, and positive complex image has been shown to increase repeat visits and favorable word of mouth. A precursor to Fakeye and Crompton (1991), Phelps (1986) divided destination image into primary and secondary destination image: primary, being the image formed before the first visit to the destination; and secondary, being the modified image resulting from personal experience with the destination. Phelps’s and F akeye and Crompton’s models suggest that travelers’ image of a destination may change after visitation. Thus, it would seem that both information sources about a destination and direct experiences with a destination are critical factors in shaping travelers’ destination images. Information source usage. Information sources refer to all of the resources from which prospective tourists gather impressions of a destination before their (next) actual visit. Information sources can be categorized as being either internal or external. Internal information sources include personal experiences, either with a specific destination or with a similar destination, and the knowledge accumulated through an ongoing information search (Fodness & Murray, 1997; Fodness & Murray, 1998; Gursoy, 2003; Vogt & F esenmaier, 1998). External information sources include the following four resources: (1) family and friends (i.e., word of mouth advice from friends and relatives), (2) destination-specific literature, (3) media, and (4) travel consultants (Snepenger & 15 Snepenger, 1993). Additionally, many tourism researchers consider the Internet as a fifth external information source (Lang, 2000). It is important to understand the role of information source usage in shaping tourists’ destination image because destination image has been shown to be a critical factor in tourists’ intentions to visit a destination. Several studies have demonstrated how information source usage influences travelers’ destination image, which, in turn, affects their visit intention (Baloglu, 1999; Beerli & Martin, 2004a; F akeye & Crompton, 1991; Gunn, 1972; Hanlan & Kelly, 2005; Litvin & Ling, 2001). Information source usage influences travelers’ destination-choice behavior because knowledge about a destination reduces prospective travelers’ uncertainty about vacationing there. New information about a destination can create new positive images in prospective travelers’ minds or correct previous negative images. According to Gartner (1993), not only the quantity of information prospective tourists obtain on a destination, but also the type of information sources consulted, influences cognitive destination image. Similarly, Stern and Krakover (1993) studied the effects of the type and amount of information tourists had on the Israeli city Beer-Sheva on their overall image of the place. Stern and Krakover categorized types of information source as either direct (firsthand) experience with the destination or indirect (secondhand) experience via print or digital media, and word of mouth. However, Stern and Krakover used an imprecise instrument to measure participants’ amount of information on Beer- Sheva: Respondents (residents of Haifa, Jerusalem, and Tel-Aviv) were asked to rate their familiarity with “public institutions and services” in Beer-Sheva (p.142) on a Likert scale ranging from 0 to 10; however, their responses were inconclusive in that Stern and 16 Krakover, in reporting their results, did not indicate the exact amounts of information to which these rating numbers referred. Similarly, Baloglu (2001) found that both the type and amount of information on a destination influenced travelers’ visit intentions indirectly through their cognitive image of the destination. In their study of American non-visitors’ images of Turkey, Baloglu identified four types of information source (nine total sources): professional advice (tour operators, travel agents, and airlines), word of mouth (friends, relatives, and social clubs), advertisements (print or broadcast media), and non-tourism sources (books, movies, and news). However, as in Stern and Krakover’s (1993) study, Baloglu (2001) imprecisely defined “amount” of information on a destination: Respondents were asked merely to check whether they had received information on Turkey from the nine information sources; the exact amount of information was not specified by participants, who effectively gave “yes-or-no” responses. On the whole, previous research suggests that both qualitative (type) and quantitative (amount) measures of information on a destination are important given the influence of information source usage on cognitive destination image and, through it, affective destination image and visit intention. However, limitations in previous studies, regarding the specification of “amount” of information on a destination, suggest that “frequency” of information source usage may be a more accurate quantitative measure. Direct experiences with the destination. A number of previous studies, as presented in Table 1, found that direct experience with the destination seems to positively affect Americans’ destination image. Generally, respondents who have previously visited a given destination have a more positive image of this destination than do non-visitors 17 (Ahmed, 1991; Baloglu & McCleary, 1999b; Chon, 1991; Court & Lupton, 1997; Hsu, Wolfe, & Kang, 2004; Milman & Pizam, 1995; Pizam, Jafari, & Milman, 1991). In his study of tourists’ image of Utah, Ahmed (1991) found that perceptions were generally more favorable for those who had previously visited the state. Similarly, in their study of Americans’ images of the USSR, Pizam et a1. (1991) confirmed that post-trip respondents had more positive images than pre-trip respondents. Moreover, previous research suggests that American repeat visitors, in general, have a more positive image of destinations in the US. than do first-time visitors. Milman and Pizam (1995) compared the images of Central Florida held by those whohad visited the state and by those who were merely aware of it. Milman and Pizam found that repeat visitors had a more positive image, and more accurate knowledge, of Central Florida than did respondents who were only vaguely familiar with the region. Fakeye and Crompton (1991) extended Milman and Pizam’s findings. In their study of differences in destination image of prospective first-time and repeat visitors to the Lower Rio Grande Valley in Texas, Fakeye and Crompton discovered that respondents who had not previously visited the destination had a more negative image of the region than did those who had visited it. They also noted that the more frequently a tourist visited the destination, the more social opportunities and attractions he or she recognized and appreciated. Similarly, regarding Americans’ image of Asian destinations, Chon (1991) discovered that post-trip respondents had a more favorable image of South Korea than did pre-trip respondents. This review of previous studies suggests that repeated direct experience with a destination improves Americans’ image of the place. 18 However, visitors’ image of a destination is not always more positive than non- visitors’ image. Baloglu and McCleary (1999b) observed that visitors’ and non-visitors’ images of the Mediterranean countries Egypt, Greece, Italy, and Turkey were largely the same. American tourists may already have a positive image of a destination before their first visit, and this image may remain static, despite actual experiences in the destination (V ogt & Andereck, 2003). Given that Baloglu and McCleary were focusing on understudied American tourist destinations, it would seem that differences in destination image between visitors and non-visitors may depend on the destination in question. On the whole, previous tourism research suggests that repeat visitors have a more positive image of a destination than do first-time visitors, who, in turn, have a more positive image of a destination than do non-visitors. It would seem, then, that destination marketers should prepare different advertising strategies for visitors and non-visitors. This study examines only the destination images of non-visitors because this population represents a larger potential market for East Asian tourism marketers. Proximity to the destination. Another pattern emerging in Table 1, which relates to the first (direct experience), is that proximity to the destination seems to positively affect Americans’ destination image. Generally, visitors living near a destination have a more positive image of the place than do those living farther away (Crompton, 1979; Hunt, 1975; Hsu, Wolfe & Kang, 2004). In his study of 1,225 out-of-state Americans’ images of Montana, Hunt (1975) observed that proximity to a region enhanced destination image. Respondents living in nearby states, compared to those living further away, more often answered that Montana was friendly and familiar as a vacation destination and that it had many places of interest to visit. Reilly (1990) confirmed Hunt’s 19 findings, establishing that respondents living closer to Montana had a stronger and more positive image of the state than did those living farther away. Hsu, Wolfe and Kang (2004) supported both Hunt’s and Reilly’s findings, in their study of visitors’ and residents’ images of Kansas. However, in his study of American tourists’ images of Mexico, Crompton (1979) found a different result: Respondents living closer to the destination had more negative images of the place than did those living farther away. Crompton explained that Americans living near the border with Mexico associated the country with crowding, poverty, and political unrest. These studies suggest that proximity to the destination reinforces American’s destination image, whether positive or negative. Influence of Destination Image on Travelers ’ Visit Intentions Intention to visit refers to travelers’ perceived likelihood of visiting a specific destination within a specific time period (Woodside & Lysonski, 1989). Intention to visit is important as an outcome variable because it has been shown to be significantly and substantially correlated with travel behavior (Um & Crompton, 1992; Woodside & Lysonski, 1989). Generally, visitors who hold a positive image of a destination are likely to visit that destination (Court & Lupton, 1997; Milman & Pizam, 1995; Rittichainuwat et al., 2001; Ross, 1993). In their image study of Central Florida, Milman and Pizam (1995) found that respondents who were familiar with the destination had a more positive image of it and as a result, they were more interested in revisiting the destination. Court and Lupton (1997) confirmed Milman and Pizam’s finding that a positive relationship existed between destination image and intention to visit. Similarly, in her study of Americans’ 20 images of Thailand, Rittichainuwat (2001) confirmed that the more positive the image (or the less negative the image) of a destination, the more likely it was that travelers would plan to revisit the destination. However, Crompton, et a1. (1992) found that, even if visitors had a positive image of a destination, they might not have any intention to visit the place, that perceived risks might outweigh positive destination image. In sum, this review of previous research on destination image indicates that both cognitive and affective destination image should be studied as distinct factors in order to better understand their interrelationship and their independent effects on other factors. Additionally, as illustrated by Table 1, much of the previous research on Americans’ destination image has focused on images of specific states and regions in the US, of countries neighboring the US, or of other Western countries. Few studies have examined Americans’ destination images of East Asian countries. This trend signals the need for destination image research on China, Japan, and South Korea. The present dissertation study examined both Americans’ cognitive and affective destination images of these three countries in an effort to explain prospective tourists’ intentions to visit East Asia. Perceived Risks of Vacationing at a Destination Perceived risk is another factor possibly explaining Americans’ intentions to visit, or not to visit, a destination. Surveying 500 international tourists, Sonmez and Graefe (1998a, 1998b) found that perceived risks were a stronger predictor of tourists’ avoiding a particular region than of their planning to visit it. In this section, the types of risks 21 associated with tourism, the influence of perceived risks on travel behavior, and, the role of information source usage in the development of tourists’ perceived risks are discussed. Types of Risks Associated with the Destination and Their Influence on Travel Behavior In 1992, the tourism researchers Roehl and Fesenmaier borrowed consumer behavior researchers’ definition of perceived risks in an attempt to investigate tourism behavior. The seven types of perceived risks included in their study were: (1) equipment risk (the possibility of mechanical problems while on vacation), (2) financial risk (the possibility that a vacation will not provide value for the money spent), (3) physical risk (the possibility of physical danger, injury, or sickness while on vacation), (4) psychological risk (the possibility that a trip to the destination will not reflect one’s personality or self-image), (5) satisfaction risk (the possibility that the vacation will not provide personal satisfaction), (6) social risk (the possibility that the vacation will affect others’ opinions of oneself), and (7) time risk (the possibility that the vacation will take too much time or be a waste of time). In 1998, three more specific risks were added by Sonmez and Graefe: (8) health risk (the possibility of becoming sick while traveling to, or while staying at, the destination), (9) terrorism risk (the possibility of being involved in a terrorist actwhile on vacation), and (10) political instability (the possibility of becoming involved in the political turmoil of the host country while on vacation). In recent years, natural disaster (1 1) has also been studied (Faulkner, 2001; Mazzocchi & Montini, 2001). Dolnicar (2005) noted that a number of previous studies had attempted to establish a complete range of possible perceived risks exhibited by tourists. In contrast, she attempted to 22 determine which travel-related fears were most salient to tourists living under the global market conditions of 2004. Using open-ended questions, Dolnicar identified five risk categories: (1) political risk, such as terrorism and political instability, (2) environmental risk, such as natural disasters, (3) health risk, such as lack of access to clean food and water, (4) planning risk, such as an unreliable airline, and (5) property risk, such as theft and loss of luggage. Among all of the perceived risks identified above, terrorism and political turmoil have been identified as having the most negative effect on visit intention (Coshall, 2003; Gartner & Shen, 1992; Sonmez & Graefe, 1998a, 1998b). In their study of Americans’ reaction to worldwide coverage of the 1989 Tiananmen Square conflict, Gartner and Shen (1992) observed that China’s newly developing tourism industry suffered: Hotel occupancy in Beijing fell below 30 percent; 300 groups (about 11,500 individuals) canceled travel plans; and tourism earnings declined by $430 million in 1989. As did Gartner and Shen (1992) and Sonmez and Graefe (1998a, 1998b), Coshall (2003) found that people perceiving terrorism as a risk were more likely to avoid the Middle East. Coshall demonstrated that the Gulf War had a significant impact on British air travel to almost all destinations. Similarly, British air trips to the Near East (Israel, Jordan, Lebanon, and Syria) and the Middle East (Iran, Iraq, Kuwait, the Persian Gulf states, North and South Yemen, Saudi Arabia, and the United Arab Emirates) began to fall in 1990, due to the Iraqi invasion of Kuwait and the seizure of Kuwaiti oil fields. The number of British air passengers declined significantly below expected levels: by 15.4 percent to the Near East and by 9.9 percent to the Middle East. Regarding terrorism, Floyd et a1. (2003) investigated how perceived risks impacted New Yorkers’ intentions to 23 travel during the period of aftershock following September 11, 2001. Floyd et al. found four statistically significant effects on intention to take a pleasure trip in the next 12 months. Two of these effects were shown to be attributed to perceived risks: physical risks and social risks. Natural disaster is another formidable barrier to international tourism (Mazzocchi & Montini, 2001). In their study of the influence of natural disasters on tourism demand, Mazzocchi and Montini (2001) observed that tourist flows decreased after the earthquake that hit Umbria in Central Italy on September 26, 1997. Tourist arrivals fell drastically the month following the main shock, compared to the same month of the previous year. The total loss estimate for the period immediately following the earthquake (October 1997 to June 1998) was about $71 million or a 40 percent reduction in estimated expenditure for the same period under normal conditions. Recently, health risk has become a major factor in choosing a vacation destination (Dolnicar, 2005; KTO, 2004; Richter, 2003). International travel to the Asia- Pacific region has recently been devastated by tragic health-related events, such as Severe Acute Respiratory Syndrome (SARS) and bird flu. According to a Korean Tourism Organization (KTO) report, in 2004, entry tourist numbers decreased noticeably, from 5.3 million in 2002 to 4.8 million in 2003: a 10 percent decrease, attributed mostly to three causes, SARS (even though, no SARS patients were ultimately recorded in South Korea), the Iraq war, and the North Korean nuclear controversy. Richter (2003) supported KTO’s report in her study of West Nile fever in New York and Mad Cow Disease in Europe, which also had a significant negative impact on travel flows to these areas. Dolnicar confirmed both KTO’s and Richter’s findings. In her study comparing 373 Australian 24 students’ patterns of perceived risks regarding domestic and international travel, Dolnicar (2005) found that health risk emerged much more frequently in the context of international travel than in the context of planned domestic travel. Language risk is also a factor possibly influencing destination choice (Basala & Klenosky, 2001). In their study of travel preferences, Balsa and Klenosky (2001) discovered that tourists’ lack of fluency in the native language(s) of the destination was a barrier to international travel to that destination. It seems that language difficulty is a twelfth perceived risk. However, Bales and Klenosky suggest that language risk is the least studied by tourism researchers. Language risk should be explored in the future. Few studies in the field have been conducted on the precise impact of language difficulty on destination choice, even though communication risk has been shown to affect international travel. Given Dolnicar’s (2005) identification of tourists’ most salient travel-related fears, and given recent political instability, terrorism, natural disaster, and disease, as well as the ongoing language barrier for Americans, in East Asia, the present dissertation study selected the following perceived risks to compose the perceived risks factor in the proposed conceptual model: physical risk, political instability risk, terrorism risk, environmental risk, health risk, and language risk. Role of Information Source Usage in Perceived Risks Several researchers (Fodness & Murray, 1999; Maser & Weiermair, 1998; Sonmez & Graefe, 1998b) have indicated that travelers’ search for information on a destination can reduce their perceived risks of vacationing there and increase their visit 25 intention. For example, Gemunden (1985) found that, when people decide on complex products, such as travel holidays, information sources are one of several risk-reducing factors that facilitate their decision making. Similarly, Roehl and Fesenmaier (1992) explored the relationship between risk perceptions and leisure travel, finding that the more uncertain or dissatisfied prospective tourists became about the consequences of a destination choice, the more perceived risks they had regarding this destination. This study identified information search behavior as a common risk-reduction strategy. However, despite the important role of information source usage in reducing travelers’ perceived risks of vacationing in a destination, no research, before the present dissertation study, has been conducted on the comparative influence of different types of information source, and different frequencies of I information source usage, on travelers’ perceived risks of vacationing in China, Japan, and South Korea. In sum, the desire for risk-free travel is a key factor influencing tourists’ intentions to visit a destination. Despite the importance of perceived risks in travelers’ decision- making processes, there has been little research on perceived risks, before the present study. Some destination image studies have included safety as a cognitive image attribute; however, other important risks were excluded. Additionally, these studies did not adequately distinguish prospective tourists’ perceived risks of vacationing in a destination from their general image of a destination. The present study compared destination image and perceived risks as two possible explanations for American tourists’ intentions to visit China, Japan, and South Korea. 26 Proposed Conceptual Model This literature review suggests a conceptual model of the relationships between intention to visit and destination image, perceived risks, and information source usage, as presented in Figure 2. The present dissertation study tested the appropriateness of this model regarding Americans’ intentions to vacation for the first time in three East Asian countries (China, Japan, and South Korea). Cognitive Image Perceived Risks H6 Information Intention Source - Affective H7 : To. Usage Image Vrsrt H3 ‘. H 5 H8 Figure 2. Theoretical model of intention to visit three East Asian countries. 27 Hypothesized Relationships Derived from the literature review, eight hypotheses were tested to achieve the purposes of this study. H1: H2: H3: H4: H5: H6: H7: H8: Increased usage of information sources increases Americans’ (emotionally neutral) cognitive image of the destination in general. Increased usage of information sources increases Americans’ (emotionally positive) affective image of the destination in general. Increased usage of information sources decreases Americans’ perceived risks of vacationing in the destination. Increased (emotionally neutral) cognitive destination image increases Americans’ (emotionally positive) affective destination image. Increased perceived risks of vacationing in the destination decreases Americans’ (emotionally positive) affective image of the destination in general. Increased (emotionally neutral) cognitive destination image increases Americans’ intention to visit the destination. Increased (emotionally positive) affective destination image increases Americans’ intention to visit the destination. Increased perceived risks of vacationing in a destination decreases American’s intention to visit the destination. The literature review presented in Chapter Two informed the eight hypotheses listed here and the resulting conceptual model, as well as the study design presented in Chapter Three. 28 CHAPTER 3 RESEARCH DESIGN AND METHODOLOGY In Chapter Three, the design and research methodology of this study are detailed. In the first section, the theoretical model suggested by the literature review and the related hypotheses are discussed. In the second section, the sampling and survey methods employed in this study are described. In the third section, the measures and scaling utilized in data collection are discussed. Finally, in the fourth section, the statistical methods used in data analyses are presented. Data Collection and Analysis Procedures Data Collection A self-administered survey was used to collect the data for this study. Respondents were chosen based on a convenience sampling method, given the scope of the present dissertation. Bookstore and coffeehouse frequenters were targeted, as the sociodemographics of this group match that of the population targeted in this study. With no particular pattern in mind during the time of data collection, potential respondents were approached about participating in the study by two colleagues and me in bookstores and coffeehouses at three Michigan-area shopping malls and in downtown Chicago. Once respondents agreed to participate in the survey, they were screened. Only American citizens, 18 years of age or older, were invited to complete the questionnaire. The purpose of this survey was explained before a copy was distributed for completion. 29 The questionnaire required approximately 15 minutes to complete, and about one third of those approached refused to participate. Questionnaires were collected on site, but some respondents could not complete the survey, due to lack of time; in this case, a pre-paid return envelope was provided. Seventy-two such envelopes were distributed; only 23 questionnaires were returned by mail. Out of 895 collected surveys, 73 were either partially, or not at all, completed. Data collection was completed between May 20 and June 20, 2006 (210-240 hours in total). Sub-Sample Size One purpose of this study was to test a model explaining prospective American tourists’ intentions to visit China, Japan, and South Korea for the first time. Therefore, previous visitation to one of these countries was a criterion employed to exclude respondents. Based on this criterion, a sub-sample was drawn from the entire sample of 822 respondents. The distribution of respondents in terms of China, Japan, and South Korea is provided in Table 2. Of the 822 completed surveys, 61 were completed by respondents who had visited China in the past 10 years. These 61 were dropped from the analysis because the study’s intention was to examine the perceptions of potential, not repeat, travelers. Similarly, 70 completed surveys were dropped regarding Japan, and 41 regarding South Korea. A total of 709 respondents had not previously visited any of the three East Asian countries of focus in this study; therefore, the final number of subjects used for SEM analysis was n = 709. 30 Table 2. Distribution of respondents who had, or had not, visited China, Japan, or South Korea before the survey. Destinations VlSlted Total Yes No China 61(7.4%) 761(92.6%) 822 Japan 70(8.5°/o) 752(91 5%) 822 South Korea 41 (5 .0°/o) 78 l (95 .0%) 822 Had visited any of the 3 countries ll3(14%) - 709(860%)W 822 Profile of Respondents The majority of the participants were male (60.1%), with an average age of 38. The majority of participants (80.0 %) were European—American. The largest percentage of participants had obtained a Bachelor’s degree (48.0%) as their highest level of education, followed by those with a high-school degree (23.0%), those with a Master’s degree (20.0%), and those with a doctorate (5.9%). A majority of respondents were married (50.8%), while 39.5% were single (never married). Of those who answered the income question (90.7%), 20.4% reported an annual household income in excess of $100,000; 66%, between $40,000 and $59,999; 13.7%, between $60,000 and $79,999; and 10.1% between $80,000 and $99,999. 31 Table 3. Characteristics of respondents who had never visited China, Japan, or South Korea. Category % n Gender Male 60. 1% Female , 39.9 % 691‘ Age 18-24 years 25.1% Mean age: 25-34 years 23.3% 38 years 35-44 years 19.3% 45-54 years 16.4% 55-64 years 9.7% 65-74 years 4.9% 75 years or older 1.3% 678‘ Education Middle/Junior High School 0.6% High School 23.0% College(Undergraduate) 48.0% Master’s Studies 20.0% Doctoral Studies 5.9% Post-doctoral Studies 2.5% 690‘ Income Less than $20,000 19.3% $20,000 - $39,999 19.9% $40,000 - $59,999 16.6% $60,000 - $79,999 13.7% $80,000 - $99,999 10.1% $100,000 - $119,999 9.2% $120,000 - $139,999 3.1% $140,000 or more 8.1% 643‘ Ethnic African American/Black 9.5% Asian American 3.1% Native American/Native Alaskan 2.5% European American/White 80.0% Latino/Latina 4. 1 % Native Hawaiian or Other Pacific 0.3% Islander Other 0.4% 681‘ Marital Status Married/Committed relationship 50.8% Widowed/Widower/ . 1.9% Never remarrred Separated 1.3% Divorced 6.5% Single, never married 39.5% 679. I . o Note: The value of “n” vanes, due to questions not answered. 32 Survey Instrument and Scales An on-site survey was used to collect the data. The survey instrument consisted of four printed pages, each divided into five sections. The first section evaluated participants’ general image of each country (China, Japan, and South Korea). The second section targeted participants’ perceived risks of vacationing in each country. The third section registered the information sources on which participants had based their impressions of each country. The fourth section recorded participants’ intention to visit each country within the next five years. Finally, the fifth section ascertained selected demographic characteristics of the participants. Destination Image Cognitive destination image attributes were derived from previous destination image studies (Crompton, 1979; Echtner & Ritchie, 1993; Fakeye & Crompton, 1991; Goodrich, 1978; Hunt, 1975). Respondents were asked to indicate their level of agreement with these attributes, regarding each country, on a 5-point Likert scale, ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). Affective destination image attributes were adapted from Russell’s (1981) bipolar scales. Those used in this study were Amusing-Sleepy, Pleasant-Unpleasant, Exciting- Gloomy, and Relaxing-Distressing. A 5-point scale was used for all four bipolar scales in which the positive end of each spectrum was assigned the highest value. 33 Perceived Risks The construct perceived risks comprised seven items modified from previous studies (Floyd et al., 2003; Lepp & Gibson, 2003; Roehl & Fesenmaier, 1992; Sonmez & Graefe, 1998b), and was measured on a five-point Likert scale (ranging from 1 = strongly disagree to 5 = strongly agree). However, this study replicated Yamamoto and Gill’s (1999) technique by adding language risk items, such as “I will not be able to understand social expectations or read body language during my vacation here,” a modification of Yamamoto and Gill’s (1999) item “It is important that the people I encounter on a vacation trip speak my language.” All perceived risk items were reverse coded. Information Source Usage Data on information sources were collected by asking survey respondents to answer, on a 5-point Likert scale, ranging from 1 (“never”) to 5 (“frequently”), how often they obtained information on each country from seven information sources: TV/radio, word of mouth (family, friends and native people), books and movies, the Internet, cultural experiences (in art museums, theaters, concert halls, restaurants, courses, etc.), newspapers/magazines, and professional advice (travel consultants, brochures). These seven sources had been identified in previous studies (Chen, 2000; Lo, Cheung, & Law, 2004; Money & Crotts, 2003). Intention to Visit To ensure construct validity, or the survey’s alignment with the proposed conceptual model, several intention to visit items were developed. Intention to visit was 34 defined as interest in, and likelihood of, visiting each country within the next five years. Interest was measured using a 5-point Likert scale (1 = completely uninterested, 3 = neither interested nor uninterested, 5 = strongly interested), and likelihood of visiting was measured on a similar scale (1 = very unlikely, 3 = no opinion, 5 = very likely). These items had been successfully used by previous researchers (Court & Lupton, 1997; Milman & Pizam, 1995; Rittichainuwat, 2001). Data Analysis Survey data were analyzed in three steps. First, preliminary statistics were obtained using the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS). Descriptive statistics were obtained to determine the distributional characteristics of each variable including the means, standard deviation, skewness, and kurtosis. Second, a one-way repeated measure ANOVA was used to examine the mean differences in intention to visit, cognitive destination image, affective destination image, perceived risks, and information sources across the three countries. After the null hypothesis was rejected, a post hoc test was performed to determine where the differences lay (Hair, Anderson, Tatham, & Black, 1998). The third step in the analysis was to engage in structural equation modeling, using the Analysis of Moment Structures (AMOS) to test the adequacy of the hypothesized model. The structural equation model enabled testing of the eight hypotheses, which specified direct and indirect relationships among the latent, as well as the observed, variables (Kline, 1998). In general, structural equation models consist of two sub-models: (1) a measurement model and (2) a structural model. The measurement model defines 35 relations between the variables as indicators of underlying constructs (the observed variables) and the underlying constructs that the observed variables were designed to measure (unobserved latent variables). The structural model represents relations among the latent variables. The constructs, observed variables, and measurement scales in the proposed model are presented in Table 4. 36 Table 4. Constructs and observed variables in the proposed model. Observed Constructs . Survey questions Scale varrables Infoa Infol TV/Radio 1: never to Info2 Word of mouth (Family, Friends, Native 5: frequently peeple) Info3 Books and Movies Info4 Internet Inf05 Cultural experiences (Art Museums, Theaters, Restaurants, Concert Halls, Classes, etc.) Info6 Newspapers/Magazines Info7 Professional advice (Travel consultants, Brochures) Cog“ Cogl Many things to see and do 1: strongly disagree Cog2 Natural scenic beauty to Cog3 Easy to navigate 5: strongly agree Cog4 Plenty of quality accommodations Cog5 Friendly local people Cog6 Appealing local food and drink Cog7 Interesting historical attractions Cog8 Unique cultural resources Affec Affel Unpleasant vs. Pleasant 1: negative emotion Affe2 Sleepy vs. Arousing to Affe3 Gloomy vs. Exciting 5: positive emotion Affe4 Distressing vs. Relaxing PRd PR1 I will get sick from eating food or 1: strongly disagree drinking water here to PR2 Political instability will keep me from 5: strongly agree vacationing here PR3 I will be concerned about potential health problems here PR4 I will worry about terrorism while vacationing here PR5 I will be vulnerable to violence if I vacation here PR6 I will encounter a natural disaster if I vacation here PR7 I will not be able to understand social expectations or read body language during my vacation here Note: a Information Source Usage regarding the destination; 5 Cognitive Destination Image based on knowledge gathered about the place; ° Affective Destination Image based on emotional evaluations of the place; d Perceived Risks of vacationing at the destination; ° Interest and perceived likelihood of visiting the destination within the next five years. 37 Table 4. Constructs and observed variables in the proposed model @ont’d). IV° IVl How interested are you in visiting the 1: completely following vacation destinations in the next uninterested 5 years? 5: strongly interested 1V2 How likely is it that you will visit the 1: very unlikely following vacation destinations in the next 5: very likely 5 years? Note: “ Information Source Usage regarding the destination; b Cognitive Destination Image based on knowledge gathered about the place; ° Affective Destination Image based on emotional evaluations of the place; d Perceived Risks of vacationing in the destination; ‘ Interest and perceived likelihood of visiting the destination within the next five years. Generally, two-step modeling is used to test the structural equation model (Kline, 1998). The first step is to find an acceptable measurement model. A model is first specified as a confirmatory analysis (CF A) measurement. The CFA model is then analyzed to determine how well it fits the data. Overall fit indices are then reported. Chi- square statistics are commonly used to evaluate model fit, but these statistics are known to be too sensitive to sample size (Kline, 1998). Thus, a number of fit indices were considered in this study to evaluate the measurement model for each country, including chi-square statistics adjusted by degrees of freedom (xZ/df), the Tucker-Lewis index (TLI), the Comparative Fit Index (CPI), the Incremental Fit Index (IFI), and the Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA). If the ratio of x2 to degrees of freedom (CMH‘I/DF) is less than 5, an acceptable fit is indicated; a CMIN/DF of less than 2 or 3 also denotes a good fit (Garson, 2006; Tabachnick & Fidell., 2001). An RMSEA value of less than .05 similarly indicates a good fit, though an RMSEA value of less than .08 signifies an acceptable fit (Brown, Mowen, Donavan, & Licata, 2002; Byme, 2001). TLI, CPI, and IF I values of more than .90 are generally considered as indicating an acceptable model fit (Kline, 1998). In this study, if the initial measurement model suggested 38 inadequate goodness of fit, the model was re-specified, based both on theories presented in the literature review and residual matrixes. After modification, differences in xz/df statistics were assessed to determine the significance of any improvement to the modified models. If the overall fit of the CFA model was deemed acceptable, the second stage of two-step modeling was initiated: the testing of the structural model. Testing a structural model includes testing the structural paths between the latent variables as well as testing the overall fit of the hypothesized model. In this study, because the initial model suggested an inadequate overall fit, the initial model was modified, based both on theories presented in the literature review and residual matrixes. After modification, differences in xz/df statistics were assessed to determine the significance of any improvement to the modified models. Chapter Three explained the data collection procedures used in the present dissertation study, including details on survey design and research methodology. Data collection from the on-site survey resulted in 709 usable responses from Americans, 18 years of age or older, who had never before visited China, Japan, or South Korea. This chapter also discussed model fit assessments for both the measurement and structural equation models developed for this study. Chapter Four presents the results of these model fit analyses and of hypotheses testing. 39 CHAPTER 4 RESULTS Findings on the relationships between Americans’ intentions to vacation for the first time in East Asia (China, Japan, and South Korea) and their general destination image of these countries (both affective and cognitive), their perceived risks of vacationing in these countries, and their information source usage regarding these countries are presented in Chapter Four. The results of preliminary analyses, including data screening, description of the variables, and analyses of the mean differences among the variables across China, Japan, and South Korea are discussed in the first section. The results of tests conducted on the measurement models for each of these three East Asian countries, including assessments of overall model fit, reliability, and validity are reported in the second section. The results of tests conducted on the structural equation models (SEM) for China, Japan, and South Korea are presented in the third section. The results of hypotheses testing, including evaluations of the direct, indirect, and total influences among the factors identified in the models, are discussed in the fourth section. Finally, additional analyses of the influence of information source usage on cognitive destination image and perceived risks, from which implications for tourism researchers and marketers will be derived in Chapter Five, are provided in Chapter Four. 40 Preliminary Analyses In this section, data screening procedures, descriptive statistics for the variables included in this study, the mean differences among the variables across the three countries, and results from tests identifying statistically significant mean differences among the variables across the three countries, are described. Data Screening The means and ranges of all of the variables of interest were inspected first for signs of data entry errors and outliers. Then, normality (i.e., the assumption that each variable should be normally distributed) was assessed by evaluating the skewness and kurtosis of each variable in the study. These tests indicated that all values for univariate skewness and kurtosis were inside the acceptable range (-3 to 3 for skewness and -8 to 8 for kurtosis), as established by Kline (1998). Thus, the results of the normality test showed no extreme departure fiom normality. These results are presented in Table 5. 41 Table 5. Normality a test results for variables included in the proposed model. Variable Skewnessb Kurtosisc : Skewnessh Kurtosisc Skewnessb Kurtosisc Constructs a - Names ; H __m_t T China ...__._--.,____J3123!!.- _ South Korea Information Info] .12 g -.57 ‘ .14 l -.51 .68 l -.16 source Info2 .38 -.45 .44 ‘ -.51 f 1.98 1 .53 usage lnfo3 —.00 I -.32 ' .08 -.38 3 .65 -.06 lnfo4 .57 -.52 ' .53 1 -.66 .95 .07 Inf05 .16 g -.62 .22 . -.51 i .81 .04 lnfo6 .24 -.32 : .24 —.33 Q .66 -.14 Info? :52. __ _A “:67, . - .47. 82 87 2.; Cognitive Cogl -1.22 3 1.34 ’ -1.38 1.86 -.26 -.49 destination Cog2 -1.04 I .70 -.95 .45 l -.33 -.35 image Cog3 -.02 3 .60 -.()2 .39 -.01 .88 Cog4 -.68 g .143 -.63 -.09 j -.26 -.12 CogS -120 1.26 . -102 .63 -.37 -.43 Cog6 -.76 .31 5 -.69 . .29 -.35 ’ -.16 Cog7 -.78 .31 -.82 z .42 -.51 1 -.18 C083 ..._..__’-_79 . ...._-46.fl _ ‘35 L _.-7.l. 48......9.5.. Affective Affel -.35 5 .85 -.12 -.66 T -.44 - 54 destination Affe2 ; -.22 ‘ -.98 -.02 -1.17 -.55 -.53 image Affe3 -.28 - -1.12 -.42 -1.01 L -.43 g -.78 Affe4 51.. s18; .50. .54 80 _-__._-_2.§_ Perceived PR1 i .78 -.03 ,3 .94 . .44 .53 -.33 risks PR2 .04 -.75 ; .25 -.62 .04 -.47 PR3 -.05 -.71 i_ .45 -44 -.08 -.63 PR4 ; .10 i -.70 , .49 -.55 .04 -.48 PR5 .30 -' -.72 .36 -.64 ‘ .16 -.71 PR6 -.10 . -27 -.06 -27 ' -.11 -.06 PR7 - .13 . . . -.63 _ .15 . 5.78” .03___ _‘ --...._:-.5._‘L Intention INTI , -.35 g -1.11 -.49 -.98 ' .10 —1.10 to visit INT2 1 .95 -.56_ : .79 -.79 ; 1.12 g .59 Note: aNormality was examined in terms offlsk‘ewness andmkurtosis. b Skewness refers to the symmetry of the distribution. Skewness with a value above 3 is conventionally considered as extremely skewed. ° Kurtosis indicates a relative excess of cases in the tails of a distribution relative to a normal distribution. A kurtosis value of 10 is a conventional criterion indicating normality distribution in terms of its peakedness. A value above 10 is considered extremely peaked. Description of the Variables Americans ’ intentions to visit three East Asian countries. Visit intentions were measured by two items: (a) interest in visiting and (2) likelihood of visiting. The descriptive statistics for the variables measuring intention to visit are presented in Table 6. The mean scores can be considered as study participants’ average evaluations of intention to visit the three East Asian countries. Table 6. Mean and SD of intention to visit the three countries. . China Japan South Korea Variable Mean (SD c) Mean (SD c) Mean (SD c) Interest in visiting a 3.10(.81) 3.30(.83) 2.61(.75) Likelihood of visiting b 1.95(.66) 2.08(.72) 1.72(.51) Note: a All items were on 5-point likert scales (1 = completely uninterested and 5 = strongly interested), b All items were on 5-point likert scales (1 = very unlikely and 5 = very likely). c the number in the brackets is standard deviation. While the mean scores of interest in visiting the three East Asian countries ranged from 2.61 to 3.30, the mean scores of likelihood of visiting China, Japan, and South Korea were low, ranging from 1.72 to 2.08. Study participants’ interest in visiting the countries was higher than their likelihood of visiting. Among the three countries, Japan had the highest mean scores for both interest in visiting and likelihood of visiting, while South Korea had the lowest for both visit intention items. ' Americans ’ cognitive images of three East Asian countries. The means and standard deviations of items measuring cognitive image of the three East Asian countries are reported in Table 7. The mean scores can be considered as study participants’ average evaluations of their cognitive image of China, Japan, and South Korea. 43 Table 7. Mean and SD of cognitive destination image of the three countries. . a China Japan South Korea Vanable Mean (so I’) Mean (so I)) Mean (SD b) Many things to see and do 4.25(.48) 4.31(.46) 3.48(.59) Natural scenic beauty 4.17(.51) 4.13(.53) 3.65(.43) Easy to navigate 2.46(.51) 2.67(.57) 2.51(.49) Quality accommodations 3.17(.48) 3.59(.41) 3.01(.42) Friendly local people 3.29(.42) 3.37(.44) 3.21(.43) Appealing local food and drink 3.68(.51) 3.58(.56) 3.20(.53) Interesting historical attractions 4.28(.46) 4.20(.58) 3.70(.52) Unique cultural resources 4.09(.48) 4.13(.55) 3.79(.49) Note: a All items were on 5-point likert scales (1 = strongly disagree and 5 = strongly agree), b The number in the brackets is standard deviation The mean scores for China ranged widely from 2.46 to 4.28. The cognitive image attributes with mean scores of greater than 4.00 were many things to see and do, natural scenic beauty, interesting historical attractions, and unique cultural resources, indicating that China’s major strengths were its vacation attractions. (Study participants gave the highest mean ratings to China’s interesting historical attractions.) The three attributes, quality accommodations, fiiendly local people, and appealing local food and drink, were, on average, given rather high mean scores, ranging from 3.17 to 3.68. Only one cognitive image attribute was, on average, given a low mean score. This attribute was easy to navigate, with a mean score of 2.46. The mean scores for Japan widely ranged from 2.67 to 4.31. The cognitive image attribute with the highest mean score was many things to see and do. Three more cognitive image attributes, natural scenic beauty, interesting historical attractions, and unique cultural resources, had mean scores of greater than 4.00, indicating that Japan’s major strengths were its vacation attractions. The three attributes, fiiendly local people, appealing local food and drink, and quality of accommodations were on average 44 perceived to be positive, though not strongly so, with mean scores ranging from 3.37 to 3.59. As in the case of China, only one cognitive image attribute for Japan was, on average, perceived to be negative. This attribute was easy to navigate, with a mean score of 2.67. The mean scores for South Korea ranged from 2.51 to 3.79. Most of the cognitive image attributes had mean scores of more than 3.00, indicating that they were, on average, perceived to be rather positive. While cognitive image attributes for South Korea were not strongly positive, two cognitive image items, unique cultural resources and interesting historical attractions, seemed to be South Korea’s strength as a vacation destination, with mean scores of 3.79 and 3.70, respectively. As for both China and Japan, only one cognitive image attribute for South Korea, easy to navigate, was perceived to be negative, with a mean score of 2.51. Examination of cognitive image attributes for the three East Asian countries shows that historical and cultural attractions seemed to be the strength of all three, even though the mean scores for these attributes differed across China, Japan, and South Korea. Easy to navigate seemed to be the weakness of all three countries, suggesting that tourism marketers working in China, Japan, and South Korea must improve this cognitive image attribute if they are to attract more American vacationers. When the mean scores of cognitive image attributes are compared across the three East Asian countries, Americans’ cognitive images of China and Japan were found to be more positive than those of South Korea. China had higher mean ratings on natural scenic beauty, appealing local food, and interesting historical attractions than did Japan, while Japan had higher 45 scores for fiiendly local people, quality accommodations, many things to see and do, and unique cultural resources than did China. Americans ’ affective images of three East Asian countries. The means and standard deviations of items measuring study participants’ affective image of the three East Asian countries are reported in Table 8. The mean scores can be considered as study participants’ average evaluations of their affective image of China, Japan, and South Korea. Table 8. Mean and SD of affective image of the three countries. China Japan South Korea Variableal Mean (so I’) Mean (SD b) Mean (SD 1’) Pleasant 2.94(.69) 3.37(.64) 2.61(.64) Arousing 2.77(.77) 3 .01 (.84) 2.56(.67) Exciting 3.37(.79) 3.51(.77) 2.77(.69) Relaxing 2.43(.73) 2.51(.66) 2.35(.53) Note: a All items were on 5-point Likert scales(l = negative emotion and 5 = positive emotion), b The number in the brackets is the standard deviation. The mean scores for China ranged from 2.43 to 3.37. While the mean score for exciting was 3.37, the mean scores for other affective image items, pleasant, arousing, and relaxing, were less than 3.00, the midpoint of the scale. Study participants gave the lowest mean rating to relaxing. Whereas the mean score for only one affective image item for China was greater than 3.00, the mean scores for three affective image items for Japan were all greater than 3.00. This indicates that study participants had a more favorable affective image of Japan than of China. Study participants gave the highest mean rating to Japan for its being an exciting vacation destination and the second highest mean rating for its being a pleasant vacation 46 destination. As in the case of China, study participants gave the lowest mean rating to Japan for its being a relaxing destination. The mean scores for South Korea ranged from 2.35 to 2.77, indicating that study participants held relatively unfavorable affective image of this country. Study participants gave the highest mean rating to South Korea for its being an exciting vacation destination, but this mean score was much lower than it was for China and Japan. As for China and Japan, South Korea received its lowest mean score for being a relaxing vacation destination. Examination of the mean scores for affective image of the three East Asian countries shows similarities and differences among China, Japan, and South Korea. Americans perceived all three of these countries as being relatively exciting, though not relaxing, vacation destinations. However, study participants held a more favorable affective image of Japan than of China and South Korea. Americans ’ perceived risks of vacationing in three East Asian countries. Study participants were asked to rate their level of agreement with seven statements regarding their perceived risks of vacationing in China, Japan, and South Korea. The means and standard deviations of items measuring these perceived risks are presented in Table 9. 47 Table 9. Mean and SD of perceived risks of visiting the three countries. China Japan South Korea Variable a Mean (so I)) Mean (sob) Mean (SD b) Likelihood of illness 2.60(.57) 2.37(.52) 2.64(.54) Political instability 2.92(.67) 2.27(.57) 2.94(.67) Health problems 2.39(.80) 2.15(.78) 2.40(.79) Terrorism 2.38(.59) 2.31(.57) 2.59(.65) Violence 2.46(.55) 2.31(.51) 2.59(.57) Natural disaster 2.08(.44) 2.14(.48) 2.12(.47) Language barrier 3.14(.56) 3.08(.59) 3.13(.56) Note: Higher mean scores indicated higher perceived risks. a All items were evaluated on a 5-point Likert scale (1 = strongly disagree and 5 = strongly agree), b The number in the brackets is the standard deviation. The mean scores for perceived risks of vacationing in China ranged from 2.08 to 3.14. Higher mean scores indicate that study participants perceived higher risks. Study participants were most likely to agree that they would have a language barrier while vacationing in China: a mean score of 3.14. The item that scored the second highest mean was political instability (mean score of 2.92). Study participants were least likely to agree that they would encounter a natural disaster while vacationing there (mean score of 2.08). The mean scores for perceived risks of vacationing in Japan ranged from 2.14 to 3.08. As in the case of China, study participants were most likely to agree that they would experience a language barrier while vacationing in Japan (mean score 3.08). Also similar to responses for China, the items that had the lowest mean scores were natural disaster and potential health problems (mean scores of 2. 14 and 2.15, respectively). However, though six of seven items measuring Americans’ perceived risks of vacationing in Japan had the lowest mean scores among the three countries, natural disaster registered the highest mean score. 48 The mean scores for perceived risks of vacationing in South Korea ranged from 2.12 to 3.13. As in the cases of China and Japan, study participants were most likely to agree that they would experience a language barrier while vacationing in South Korea, with a mean score of 3.13. The item with the second highest mean score was political instability (mean score of 2.94). Study participants were least likely to agree that they would encounter a natural disaster while vacationing in South Korea. The mean scores for two items, terrorism and violence, were highest for South Korea of the three East Asian countries. Examination of mean scores for Americans’ perceived risks of vacationing in China, Japan, and South Korea shows similarities and differences among the three countries. While study participants were most likely to agree that they would experience a language barrier while vacationing in China, Japan, and South Korea, they were least likely to agree that they would encounter a natural disaster there. Among the three East Asian countries, Japan appeared to evoke the weakest perceived risks, and South Korea appeared to evoke the strongest perceived risks. Americans ’ information usage regarding the three East Asian countries. Study participants were asked to rate how often they obtained information on China, Japan, and South Korea, using a five-point Likert scale (1 = never, 5 = frequently). Seven different information sources were included: TV/radio, word of mouth, books and movies, Internet, cultural experiences, newspapers/magazines and professional advice. The means and standard deviations for these items are reported in Table 10. 49 Table 10. Mean and SD of information source usage (type and frequency a) for the three countries. China Japan South Korea Variable Mean (sob) Mean (so I)) Mean (sot) TV/Radio 2.64(.60) 2.65(.62) 2.25(.54) Word of mouth 2.33(.53) 2.37(.59) 1.97(.57) Books and Movies 2.71(.49) 2.70(.54) 2.12(.51) Internet 2.24(.63) 2.29(.66) 1.95(.56) Cultural experiences 2.62(.58) 2.58(.58) 2.06(.54) Newspapers/Magazines 2.56( .54) 2.57(.55) 2.16(.55) Professional advice 2.07(.67) 2.10(.65) 1.90(.62) Note: a Frequency for all items was measured on a five-point Likert scale (1= never and 5 = frequently), b The number in the brackets is the standard deviation. Regarding China, the mean scores for information sources ranged from 2.07 to 2.71, indicating that study participants appeared to lack information on China. Among the seven information sources identified in this study, books and movies were used most often by study participants, with a mean score of 2.71, followed by TV/radio and cultural experiences, with mean scores of 2.64 and 2.62, respectively. The least used information source was professional advice, with a mean score of 2.07. Regarding Japan, the mean scores for information sources ranged from 2.10 to 2.70, indicating that study participants also appeared to lack information on Japan. Books and movies on Japan were most frequently consulted by study participants (mean score of 2.70). TV/radio followed, with a mean score of 2.65. As in the case of China, the least used information source for Japan was professional advice, with a mean score of 2.10. The mean scores obtained for South Korea ranged from 1.90 to 2.25. When compared to China’s and Japan’s mean scores for information source usage, South Korea’s are much lower. Unlike for China and Japan, study participants most frequently 50 consulted TV/radio for information on South Korea (a mean score of 2.25), then newspapers/ magazines (a mean score of 2.16). However, as in the cases of China and Japan, the least used information source regarding South Korea was professional advice, with a mean score of 1.90. The mean scores for information source usage regarding all three East Asian countries were less than 3.00, showing that study participants did not have enough information about the countries. While the mean scores for books and movies were highest for China and Japan, the mean score for TV/radio was highest for South Korea. The mean scores for each of the seven information sources were lowest for South Korea. Analysis of Mean Differences among the Three Countries As mentioned above, across China, Japan, and South Korea, there were differences in mean scores for visit intention variables, cognitive image variables, affective image variables, perceived risks variables, and information source usage variables. In order to determine the statistical significance of these differences, Repeated Measure Analysis of Variance (ANOVA) was employed. As shown in Table 11, results of Repeated Measure ANOVA indicated statistically significant differences across the three countries for most variables. 51 com a 5 33ch .850 88m avenge mm 9on 533 @982 3 wow: on :8 $3 0338 < .3558 025 05 38% Boat? :03 E coconut? 508 Emu—mafia a 33065 3.8 cc: fiom Qmm >83? . dea 1. £9on ... a $32 $04 :86 men: a? m a .4 a: 86382 eases 8%: 0540 toad 2.. am can one ”we 288% serene assess $540 .486 m2: 8.». ”2 88 are 2a use was. wanna/s. umon :86 Ram 8m tum as 6.8% :32 been; $04 :86 mm. E :2 a? a a .m 22388888 £20 magma :86 :2 EN SN Sn sense: 3 5m $540 :85 $.42 as 2+ 2.4 beans sees Baez swan: Mm .94 .53 8.28 was 5. m3. 8 e5 as e ass as: 6%:an v5. .04 .23 o3 mom EN m3 weave V504 :80 as: in BM Em magnum V504 :86 3% e3 5.... EN 3862 ass 5.04 :03 as: 38. RM 38 Bass 6386? V504 tag :8 N: was ac means a6 82:85 am; V504 :85 8.92 Ga com o; wages a 2225 2 assess vaeeex .. ash mega 2.33 58m 5 5%.. 6%an 6525» seen com “men 6325» as can: .< >0 Z< 0.832: 830%.. .«o $33M .2 2an 52 .58 a 5 3:on .850 :85 “accommu mm 95% :03? @552 8 com: on :8 68 8558a < .8558 085 05 $88 035...? 5.3 5 oocoeobmo :38 Heme—mama a 58855 68 8: 58m Dmm woo—db. .8.on .1. .modvq _. a $32 MonA :80 emu: 8; 88 SN 823 massage V546 .3? cm. _ 2 2 .N EN 62 85882 €88...an V540 toad 93.8 88 was. med 88885 .833 M84 :86 ~33 was an an 5585 V546 teed mesa NZ EN EN use: Be seen swam: v7.4.0 toes 8.8 8.2 EN m2 589 as 263 85% van? :85 £82 an men 43 see: 8:283 anTo e3 91. m _ .m 3m 2 .m sense eweaefi on Mm. a .86 :6 NZ 3 .N a: sesame 3:32 709m .52 ea. _ m an an eves. 85.65 7on :85 2% an :56. mom Sweeney 7% .o 2.86 3. E can a _ .N as asezea exam 7% .u :86 $82 an Rd 8:6. 353% 823 3.5 7mm .0 2.8.0 3% 88 SN 88 was: as 865$: 852$ A 88 pumoh. said/AH 03.0 >1 m Vmnwuvzom M AC Swamp. aUngU oBmv—dxr uoaomm 83 Ken ezeea> as see: A5288 < >0 Z< 0.332: 50803.“ mo 338% ”2033. 53 As displayed in Table 11, significant differences were found in mean scores for all variables, except one: the perceived risks variable language barrier. This finding is not surprising, given that none of the three countries claims English as a national language. Further findings are discussed below. First, significant differences were found across the three countries regarding study participants’ responses to the two items representing their intentions to visit China, Japan, and South Korea. As shown in Table 11, the mean scores of visit intention items were highest for Japan and lowest for South Korea. Second, significant differences were found across the three countries regarding study participants’ responses to the eight items representing cognitive image. As shown in Table 11, Japan was ranked first, in terms of mean score, as being easy to navigate, as having friendly local people, and unique cultural resources, and as offering many things to see and do and quality accommodations. However, Japan ranked second for natural scenic beauty, appealing local food and drink, and interesting historical attractions, all for which China ranked first. South Korea ranked third for all of the cognitive image items. Third, significant differences were found among the three East Asian countries regarding study participants’ responses to the four items representing affective image. As shown in Table 11, the mean scores for affective image items were consistently highest for Japan and lowest for South Korea. Fourth, across the three countries, significant differences were found in six of the seven perceived risks items. (The mean differences for language barrier were insignificant.) The mean scores for terrorism and violence were highest for South Korea, while the mean scores of likelihood of illness, political instability, and potential health 54 problems were highest for China. The mean score for natural disaster was highest for Japan, while the mean scores for other perceived risk items regarding Japan were equal to, or lower than, those regarding China and South Korea. Finally, significant differences were observed across the three countries regarding information source usage items. The mean scores for each of the seven information sources identified in this study were lowest for South Korea. The mean scores for TV/radio, word of mouth, and Internet were highest for Japan, while the mean scores for books and movies and cultural experiences were highest for China. In sum, results indicated that study participants’ responses regarding the variables for the three East Asian countries were different. For this reason, it was appropriate to isolate responses for China, Japan, and South Korea in order to test the hypothesized model. Testing the Measurement Model The measurement model defines the relations between observed variables and the underlying constructs that the observed variables are presumed to measure. A confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) was conducted to confirm the hypothesized relations between observed variables and their underlying constructs to assess the degree to which the data fit the proposed measurement model. The measurement model was estimated using the maximum likelihood method. As shown in Figure 3, the proposed measurement model consisted of five constructs and 28 observed variables. The five constructs were Information Source Usage, Affective Image, Cognitive Image, Perceived Risks, and 55 Intention to Visit. Each construct (e. g., Affective Image) was measured by a number of observed variables (e.g., Afl‘el, Affe2, Affe3, and Affe4). 3'3eesss loom cooscooo oo Cognitive Image @ ® 5’ M o Imm 6’ 1m 6 . 6‘ lnb3 lnfmtion ® Intention 0 Affective Aim @ To Source I mage A (3‘ \mo 9 @0639 £553. Perceived Risks l PR1 PR4 PR6 PR7 dggédéé Figure 3. The proposed measurement model for the three countries. Note: Affel: Pleasant, Affe2: Arousing, Affe3: Exciting, Affe4: Relaxing; Cogl: Many things to see and do, Cog2: Natural scenic beauty, Cog3: Easy to navigate, Cog4: Plenty of quality accommodations, CogS: Friendly local people, Cog6: Appealing local food and drink, Cog7: Interesting historical attractions, Cog 8: Unique cultural resources; Infol: TV/Radio, Info2: Word of mouth, Info3: Books and Movies, Info4: Internet, Inf05: Cultural experiences, Info6: Newspapers/Magazines, Info7: Professional advice; 1V1: Interest, IV 2: Likelihood; PR1: Likelihood of illness, PR2: Political instability, PR3: Health problems, PR4: Terrorism, PR5: Violence, PR6: Natural disaster, PR7: Language barrier Error Terms: e1-e27, rl-r3. 56 The proposed measurement model was first examined for overall model fit using AMOS (Analysis of Moment Structures) to determine the degree to which the model fits the data. A number of commonly used goodness-of-fit indices were utilized to assess the model fit. The goodness-of-fit indices are generally categorized into three types: (1) absolute fit indices, (2) parsimonious fit indices, and (3) incremental fit measures (Hair et al, 1998). For this study, six goodness-of-fit indices from three categories were chosen. In Table 12, the goodness-of-fit indices chosen for this study and recommended range of good fit are presented. Table 12. The recommended goodness-of-fit indices Category Goodness-fit indices Recommended Range Used for the Study of Good Model Fit Absolute Fit Indices o Chi-Square Statistics (x2 ) Insignificant p-value (p>.01) 0 Root Mean Square Error of < 05 Approximation (RMSEA) ' Parsirnonious Fit Indices 0 Chi-Square/Degree of < 3 Freedom (x2 /df) Incremental Fit Indices 0 Comparative Fit Index (CPI) > .90 0 Tucker -Lewis Index (TLl) > .90 O Incremental Fit Index (IFI) > .90 Sources: Byme, 2001; Garson, 2006; Kline, 1998; Tabachnick & Fidell, 2001. After evaluating the overall fit of the model, its validity and reliability were assessed. A set of confirmatory factor analyses (CFAs) were conducted for China, Japan, and South Korea, the results of which are separately reported below. 57 The China Model Assessment of overall model fit. Results of confirmatory factor analyses for the China model suggested that the proposed measurement model was not acceptable ()8 (340) = 1054.26, p = .01, CMIN/DF = 3.10, CFI = .88, TLI =.87, IFI = .88, RMSEA = .05) as displayed in Table 13. While xz/dfand RMSEA results were within the recommended range for a good model, other fit indices indicated a poor fit to the data. Thus, it was decided to modify the proposed model for all three countries, beginning with China. AMOS provides the modification indices for identifying which variables contributed to this poor fit. In order to modify the proposed model, significance tests of parameters were first examined. Inspection of the standardized parameter estimates showed that all observed variables loaded significantly on their respective constructs, indicating that the observed variables were good measures of the underlying constructs. Then, modification indices were examined. Inspection of modification indices indicated that the overall model fit could be improved by removing three items (easy to navigate, relaxing, and potential health problems). After modifying the proposed model, another CFA was conducted for China. The modified model had a better fit to the data. The x2 was equal to 712.68, with 265 degrees of freedom and a p-value of less than .01. While an insignificant value of x2 is preferable, since 12 may be significant if the sample size is large (Kline, 1998), other fit indices were examined to determine the overall model fit of the modified measurement model. As shown in Table 13, xz/df, CFI, TLI, IFI and RMSEA results were within the recommended ranges of a good model fit. Thus, it was concluded that the modified model was acceptable. The x2 difference test, comparing the proposed and the modified 58 measurement models, showed that the )8 difference was 341.58 and statistically significant (p < .01). This means that the proposed model was significantly improved through modification. Table 13. Comparison of the proposed and modified full measurement models for China. Model x2 Df xz/df CFI TLI IFI RMSEA sz Adf Initial Model 1054.26 340 3.10 .88 .87 .88 .05 MOdifiid 712.68 265 2.69 .92 .90 .92 .04 341.58’ 75 Model Note: CF I = Comparative Fit Index, TLI = Tucker-Lewis Index, IFI = Incremental Fit Index, RMSEA = Root Mean Square Error of Approximation. Criteria to accept model: xz/df should be smaller than 3; CFI, TLI, and IFI should be greater than .90; RMSEA should be smaller than .05. significant at p < 0.01 aModified model = Initial model — (Cog3 (Easy to navigate), Affe4 (Relaxing), and PR3 (Potential health problems» Assessment of reliability and validity. The modified measurement model provided support for both validity and reliability. As displayed in Table 14, all factor loadings were statistically significant with t-values ranging from 10.15 to 17.05 (p < .01) and standardized factor loadings ranging from .44 to .83, both of which provide evidence supporting convergent validity. As can be seen in Table 15, estimated correlations between the constructs were not excessively high (less than .85, according to Kline (1998)) and thus offered evidence supporting discriminant validity. Reliability analysis using Cronbach’s alpha (or) indicates the internal consistency of the observed variables measuring each factor. As shown in Table 15, all five factors had composite reliability scores of greater or equal to .70. Since Cronbach’s or exceeded 0.60, it was deemed acceptable (Burnkrant & Thomas, 1982), indicating that the observed variables reliably measured their respective constructs. Given the results of these tests of model fit, validity, 59 and reliability, it was concluded that the modified measurement model was acceptable as the final measurement model for China. Table 14. Standardized factor loadings for the modified measurement model for China. Standardized Factors Paths . t-value Estimates Information Source Usage Info] .58 " 13.39 Inf02 .44 " 10.15 Inf03 .66 ” 13.88 Info4 .63 " 13.82 Inf05 .64 " 13.95 Info6 .69 " 14.77 lnfo7 .56 " 12.27 Cognitive Image Cogl .76 " 16.57 Cog2 .67 " 15.06 Cog4 .46 " 10.75 Cog5 .49 ” 11.40 Cog6 .52 " 12.11 Cog7 .76 " 17.05 Cog8 .66“ 15.12 Affective Image Affel .78 " 14.96 Affe2 .62 " 14.26 Affe3 ,3 .71 ” 14.73 Perceived Risks PR1 .55 " 11.28 PR2 .62 " 12.03 PR3 .67 ” 13.33 PR4 .76 " 14.03 PR5 .58 ” 11.80 PR6 .52 " 10.92 Intention to Visit V11 .83 " 14.43 V12 .62 " 12.95 Note: "Factor loadings are all significant at p < .01. 60 Table 15. Correlation analysis of factors for China. N of Cronbach's 1 2 3 4 5 Variables a 1. Information Source 1 7 .82 Usage 2. Cognitive Image .18M 1 7 .75 3. Affective Image .22M .54" l 3 .81 4 .Perceived Risks - .24M -.39" -.46M 1 6 .71 5. Intention to Visit .33M .42" .49M -.45M 1 2 .78 Note: ”Correlations were all significant at p < .01. The Japan Model Assessment of overall model fit. Results of confirmatory factor analyses for the Japan model suggested that the proposed measurement model was not acceptable (x2 (340) = 931.11, p = .01, CMIN/DF = 2.74, CFI = .90, TLI = .89, IFI = .91, RMSEA = .05) as displayed in Table 16. While xz/df and RMSEA results were within the recommended range for a model with good fit to the data, other fit indices indicated a poor fit of the proposed model to the data. Thus, it was decided to modify the proposed model. AMOS provides the modification indices for identifying which variables contributed to this poor fit. In order to modify the proposed model, significance tests of parameters were examined first. Inspection of the standardized parameter estimates showed that all observed variables loaded significantly on their respective constructs, indicating that the observed variables were good measures of the underlying constructs. Afier modifying the proposed model, another CFA was run for Japan. The modified model had a better fit to the data. The x2 was equal to 649.07, with 265 degrees of freedom and a p-value of less than .01. While an insignificant value of x2 is preferable, since )8 may be significant if the sample size is large (Kline, 1998), other fit indices were 61 examined to determine the overall model fit of the modified measurement model. As shown in Table 16, xz/df, CF I, TLI, and RMSEA were within the recommended range for a model with good fit to the data. Thus, given these positive model fit test results, it was concluded that the modified model was acceptable. The 12 difference test, comparing the pr0posed and the modified measurement models, showed that the x2 difference was 282.15 and statistically significant (p < .01 ). This means that the proposed model was significantly improved through modification. Table 16. Comparison of the proposed and modified full measurement models for Japan. Model x2 Df {my CFI TLI IFI RMSEA 8x2 Adf Initial Model 931.22 340 2.74 .90 .89 .91 .05 MOdififd 649.07 265 2.44 .93 .92 .93 .04 282.15‘ 75 Model Note: CFI = Comparative Fit Index, TLI = Tucker-Lewis Index, IFI = Incremental Fit Index, RMSEA = Root Mean Square Error of Approximation. Criteria to accept model: ledf should be smaller than 3; CFI, TLI, and IFI should be greater than .90; RMSEA should be smaller than .05. significant at p < 0.01 aModified model = Initial model — (Cog3 (Easy to navigate), Affe4 (Relaxing), and PR3 (Health problems)) Assessment of reliability and validity. The modified measurement model provided support for both validity and reliability. As displayed in Table 17, all factor loadings were statistically significant with t-value ranging from 10.21 to .1614 (p < .01) and standardized factor loadings ranging from .49 to .82, both of which provided evidence supporting convergent validity. As shown in Table 18, estimated correlations between the constructs were not excessively high (less than .85, according to Kline (1998)) and thus offered evidence supporting discriminant validity. Reliability analysis using Cronbach’s or indicated the internal consistency of the observed variables measuring each factor. As shown in Table 18, all five factors had composite reliability scores greater than or equal 62 to .70. If Cronbach’s or exceeds 0.60, it is deemed acceptable (Bumkrant & Thomas, 1982). Thus, the reliability test showed that observed variables reliably measured their respective constructs. Given the results of these tests of model fit, validity, and reliability, it was concluded that the modified measurement model was acceptable as the final measurement model for Japan. Table 17. Standardized factor loadings for the modified measurement model for Japan. Factors Paths Standardized t—value ..- Estimates Information Source Usage Info] .57" 13.60 Inf02 .50" 11.53 Info3 .67" 14.90 Info4 .65“ 14.48 Inf05 .70” 15.60 Info6 .80” 16.22 Info7 __,___W___ .69" 15.12 Cognitive Image Cogl .75" 16.14 Cog2 .68” 15.01 Cog4 .50" 11.67 CogS .49” 1 1.40 Cog6 .51 " 11.65 Cog7 .72” 15.82 cogs ., .65” 14.77 Affective Image Affel .82" 15.84 Affe2 .62” 14.46 Affe3 .81" 15.63 Perceived Risks PR1 .59" 11.80 PR2 .60” 11.69 PR3 .72” 13.73 PR4 .74" 13.89 PR5 .58” 11.27 PR6W________W W .48” W 10.21 Intention to Visit V11 .80" 14.34 V12 .65" 13.01 Note: "Factor loadings are all significant at p < .01. 63 Table 18. Correlation analysis of factors for J eman. N of Cronbach's l 2 3 4 5 Variables a l. Inforrnatron 1 7 .79 Source Usage 2. Cognitive Image .27” 1 7 .75 3. Affective Image .25M .54” l 3 .81 4 .Perceived Risks -22" -38" -.39** 1 6 .73 5. Intention to Visit .37" .47" .43" -38" 1 2 .78 Note: ”Correlations were all significant at p < .01. The South Korea Model Assessment of the overall model fit. Results of confirmatory analyses for the South Korea model suggested that the proposed measurement model was not acceptable ()8 (340) = 1144.51, p = .01, CMlN/DF = 3.37, CFI = .90, TLI = .88, IFI = .90, RMSEA = .06) as displayed in Table 19. While xZ/df and RMSEA results were within the recommended range for a model with good fit to the data, other fit indices indicate a poor fit of the proposed to the data. Thus, it was decided to modify the proposed model. AMOS provides the modification indices for identifying which variables contributed to a poor fit of the proposed model to the data. In order to modify the proposed model, significance tests of parameters were examined first. Inspection of the standardized parameter estimates showed that all observed variables loaded significantly on their respective constructs, showing that the observed variables were good measures of the underlying constructs. After modifying the proposed model, another CF A was run for South Korea. The modified model had a better fit to the data. The x2 was equal to 750.30, with 265 degrees of freedom and a p-value of less than .01. While the insignificant value of x2 is preferable, since 752 may be significant if the sample size is large (Kline, 1998), other fit indices were 64 examined to determine the overall model fit of the modified measurement model. As shown in Table 19, xz/df, CF I, TLI, and RMSEA were within the recommended ranges for a model with good fit to the data. Thus, given these positive model fit test results, it was concluded that the modified model was acceptable. The x2 difference test, comparing the proposed and the modified measurement models, showed that the )8 difference was 394.21 and statistically significant (p < .01). This means that the proposed model was significantly improved through modification. Table 19. Comparison of the initial and modified full measurement models for South Korea. Model x2 df xz/df CFI TLI IFI RMSEA sz Adf initial Model 1144.51 340 3.37 .90 .88 .90 .06 M°dififd 750.30 265 2.83 .93 .92 .93 .04 394.21' 75 Model Note: CFI = Comparative Fit Index, TLI = Tucker-Lewis Index, IFI = Incremental Fit Index, RMSEA = Root Mean Square Error of Approximation. Criteria to accept model: xz/df should be smaller than 3; CF I, TLI, and IFI should be greater than .90; RMSEA should be smaller than .05. significant at p < 0.01 aModified model = Initial model — (Cog3 (Easy to navigate), Affe4 (Relaxing), and PR3 (Health problems)) Assessment of reliability and validity. The modified measurement model provided support for both validity and reliability. As displayed in Table 20, all factor loadings were statistically significant with t-values ranging from 9.91 to 17.07 (p < .01) and standardized factor loadings ranging from .44 to .83, both of which provided evidence supporting convergent validity. As can be seen in Table 21, estimated correlations between the constructs were not excessively high (less than .85, according to Kline (1998)), and thus offered evidence supporting discriminant validity. Reliability analysis using Cronbach’s or indicates the internal consistency of the observed variables measuring 65 each factor. As shown in Table 21, all five factors had composite reliability scores of greater than or equal to .70. If Cronbach’s a exceeds 0.60, it is deemed acceptable (Bumkrant & Thomas, 1982). Thus, the reliability test shows that observed variables reliably measured their respective constructs. Given the results of these tests of model fit, validity, and reliability, it was concluded that the modified measurement model was acceptable as the final measurement model for South Korea. Table 20. Standardized factor loadings for the modified measurement model for S. Korea. Factors Paths Standardlzed t-value EstImates Information Source Usage Infol .64" 13.49 Info2 .44" 9.91 Inf03 .69" 13.69 Info4 .70” 13.40 Inf05 .75" 13.73 Info6 .77” 13.99 . Info7 .57” 13.11 Cognitive Image Cogl .77" 16.58 Cog2 .74” 15.07 Cog4 .56" 10.75 Cog5 .59“ 11.40 Cog6 .76” 12.12 Cog7 .68" 17.07 Cog8 ., .66” 16.89 Affective Image Affel .78" 14.95 Affe2 .61" 13.26 Affe3 .71" 14.45 Perceived Risks PR1 .63" 11.28 PR2 .71" 12.03 PR3 .62” ' 13.33 PR4 .71" 14.04 PR5 .59” 10.80 PR6 .79” 14.97 Intention to Visit V11 .83" 15.01 V12 .62” 12.87 Note: "F actor loadings are all significant at p < .01. 66 Table 21. Correlation analysis among factors for South Korea. N of Cronbach's 1 2 3 4 5 Variables a 1. Information 1 .7 .80 Source Usage 2. Cognitive Image .25" 1 7 .75 3. Affective Image .18M .55" 1 3 .81 4 .Perceived Risks -.21** -45“ -.55** 1 6 .74 5. Intention to Visit .32“ .44" .41“ -48“ 1 2 .78 Note: ”Correlations were all significant at p < .01. Comparison of the Measurement Model for the Three Countries. The proposed measurement model was tested for China, Japan, and South Korea. The overall model fit of the proposed model was poor across all three countries. The proposed model was modified for each of the three countries: Inspection of modification indices indicated that the overall model fit could be improved by removing three items (easy to navigate, relaxing, and potential health problems). This modified model was used for China, Japan, and South Korea. Since the modified model had an adequate fit to the data across all three countries, it was accepted as the final measurement model for testing the structural equation model for China, Japan, and South Korea. Testing the Structural Equation Model Since the final measurement model fit was satisfactory, the structural equation model (SEM), combining both the final measurement model and the structural model, was tested. Testing the structural equation model involves assessment of both the measurement model and the hypothesized relationships among the factors identified in this model. The proposed structural equation model is presented in Figure 4. Employing 67 AMOS, a set of SEM analyses was conducted for the three countries, the results of which are separately reported below. 9‘ Infol 92 mm °3 Inf03 °‘ lnfo4 Inf05 66 Inf06 07 Info7 ea 1 6i @‘1 6? l cool cooz coo4 coos (3007 C003 Cognitive Image rl ‘2 Affel 1 Ingmon Affective Affez e1 Usage Image Mk3 1 r3 Perceived Risks p15 PR4 PR5 PR6 PR7 arid $88 lVl IV2 Intention '4 To Visit Figure 4. The proposed structural equation model. Note: Affel: Pleasant, Affe2: Arousing, Affe3: Exciting; Cogl: Many things to see and do, Cog2: Natural scenic beauty, Cog4: Plenty of quality accommodations, Cog5: Friendly local people, Cog6: Appealing local food and drink, Cog7: Interesting historical attractions, Cog 8: Unique cultural resources; Infol: TV/radio, Inf02: Word of mouth, Info3: Books and movies, Info4: Internet, Inf05: Cultural experiences, Info6: Newspapers/magazines, Info7: Professional advice; 1V1: Interest, 1V2: Likelihood; PR1: Likelihood of illness, PR2: Political instability, PR4: Terrorism, PR5: Violence, PR6: Natural disaster, PR7: Language barrier Error Terms: el-e27, rl-r3 68 The China Model The goodness-of-fit indices used to assess the measurement model were also utilized to determine whether the proposed structural equation model fit the data for China. As displayed in Table 22, the )6 statistic was equal to 746.03, with 245 degrees of fi'eedom and a p-value of less than .01. When the )8 statistic is insignificant, the model is deemed acceptable. However, since chi-square statistics based on a large sample size may result in a significant x2 statistic, other fit indices were utilized as alternatives to determine the adequacy of the model fit. CFI, IFI, and TL] values of over .90, RMSEA values of less than .05, and xz/df values of less than three indicate an acceptable fit. Based on these criteria, the results of the fit indices displayed in Table 22 indicated a poor fit of the proposed structural equation model to the data, suggesting that the proposed model needed to be modified. Inspection of modification indices (MIs) showed that the MI value for the error variable parameter, bearing on the association between the error term of cognitive images (r1) and the error term of perceived risks (r3), was relatively high, with a value of 59.81. This suggested that a better fit could be achieved by correlating the errors of the factors. In order to link the error terms, it must be supported by a substantive and/or empirical rationale (Byme, 2001). Several authors have suggested that there is an association between cognitive image of a destination and perceived risks of vacationing there (Suh and Gartner, 2004). When travelers perceive very low risks of vacationing in a destination, their evaluations of the destination tend to be very high. Also, travelers’ negative evaluations may be influenced by perceptions of high risks of vacationing in the destination. Since these suppositions are plausible, it was decided to link the error terms, a decision which resulted in the model for China depicted in Figure 5. 69 “9863896? ICOGI C002 C004 005 006 007 008 Cognitive Image ‘ Affective Perceived Risks eoooooo EEEEEEE I???" PR4 PR5 PR6 added. Figure 5. Modified SEM for China. Note: Affel: Pleasant, Affe2: Arousing, Affe3: Exciting; Cogl: Many things to see and do, Cog2: Natural scenic beauty, Cog4: Plenty of quality accommodations, Cog5: Friendly local people, Cog6: Appealing local food and drink, Cog7: Interesting historical attractions, Cog 8: Unique cultural resources; Infol: TV/radio, InfoZ: Word of mouth, Info3: Books and movies, Info4: Internet, Inf05: Cultural experiences, Info6: Newspapers/magazines, Info7: Professional advice; IVl: Interest, 1V2: Likelihood; PR1: Likelihood of illness, PR2: Political instability, PR4: Terrorism, PR5: Violence, PR6: Natural disaster, PR7: Language barrier Error Terms: el-eZ7, rl-r3 70 The modified model had a better fit to the data. The x2 statistic was equal to 681.01, with 244 degrees of freedom and a p-value of less than .01. While an insignificant value of x2 is preferable, since 752 may be significant if the sample size is large (Kline, 1998), other fit indices were examined to determine the overall model fit of the modified measurement model. As shown in Table 22, xz/df, CFI, TLI, and RMSEA were within the recommended ranges for a model with good fit to the data. Thus, it was concluded that the modified structural equation model was acceptable. The x2 difference test, comparing the proposed and the modified measurement models, showed that the x2 difference was 65.02 and statistically significant (p < .01). This means that the proposed model was significantly improved through modification. Table 22. Comparison of the initial and modified structural models for China. Model 1 df ledf CFI TLI IFI RMSEA sz Adi Initial Model 746.03 245 3.04 .90 .89 .90 .05 Modified Model 681.01 244 2.79 .91 .90 .91 .04 65.02‘ 1 Note: CFI = Comparative Fit Index, TLI = Tucker-Lewis Index, IFI = Incremental Fit Index, RMSEA = Root Mean Square Error of Approximation. Criteria to accept model: xz/df should be smaller than 3; CFl, TLI, and IFI should be greater than .90; RMSEA should be smaller than .05. ~‘significant at p < 0.01 The Japan Model The goodness-of-fit indices used to assess the measurement model were also utilized to determine whether the proposed structural equation model fit the data for Japan. As displayed in Table 23, the x2 statistic was equal to 687.63, with 245 degrees of freedom and a p—value of less than .01. When the )8 statistics is insignificant, the model is accepted. However, since chi-square statistics based on a large sample size may result in a significant )8 statistic, other fit indices were utilized as alternatives to determine the 71 adequacy of the model fit. CFI, IFI, and TLI values of over .90, RMSEA values of less than .05, and ledf values of less than three indicate an acceptable fit. Based on these criteria, the results of the fit indices displayed in Table 23 indicated a poor fit of the proposed structural equation model to the data, suggesting that the proposed model needed to be modified. Inspection of modification indices (Mls) showed that the MI value for the error variable parameter, bearing on the association between the error term of cognitive image (r1) and the error term of perceived risks (r3), was relatively high, with a value of 61 .41. This suggested that a better fit could be achieved by correlating the errors of the factors. In order to link the error terms, it must be supported by a substantive and/or empirical rationale (Byme, 2001). Several authors have suggested that there is an association between cognitive image of a destination and perceived risks of vacationing there (Suh & Gartner, 2004). When travelers perceive very low risks of vacationing in a destination, their evaluations of the destination tend to be very high. Also, travelers’ negative evaluations may be influenced by their perceptions of high risks of vacationing in the destination. Since these suppositions are plausible, it was decided to link the error terms. The modified structural equation model for Japan was the same as that for China as shown in Figure 5. The modified model had a better fit to the data. The x2 was equal to 620.64, with 244 degrees of freedom and a p—value of less than .01. While an insignificant value of x2 is preferable, since x2 may be significant if the sample size is large (Kline, 1998), other fit indices were examined to determine the overall model fit of the modified measurement model. As shown in Table 23, xz/df, CFI, TLI, and RMSEA were within the recommended ranges for a model with good fit to the data. Thus, it was concluded that 72 the modified structural equation model was acceptable. The )6 difference test, comparing the proposed and the modified measurement models, showed that the )8 difference was 66.99 and statistically significant (p < .01). This means that the proposed model was significantly improved through modification. Table 23. Comparison of the initial and modified structural models for Japan. Model x2 df flay CFI TLI IFI RMSEA Ax2 Adi urinal Model 687.63 245 2.81 .92 .89 .92 .05 Modified Model 620.64 244 2.54 .93 .92 .93 .04 66.99‘ 1 Note: CFI = Comparative Fit Index, TLI = Tucker-Lewis Index, IF I = Incremental Fit Index, RMSEA = Root Mean Square Error of Approximation. Criteria to accept model: xz/df should be smaller than 3; CF I, TLI, and IFI should be greater than .90; RMSEA should be smaller than .05. ‘significant at p < 0.01 The South Korea Model The goodness-of-fit indices used to assess the measurement model were also utilized to determine whether the proposed structural equation model fit the data for South Korea. As displayed in Table 24, the x2 statistic was equal to 837.97, with 245 degrees of freedom and a p-value of less than .01. When the )8 statistic is insignificant, the model is accepted. However, since chi-square statistics based on a large sample size may result in a significant )8 statistic, other fit indices were utilized as alternatives to determine the adequacy of the model fit. CFI, IFI, and TLI values of over .90, RMSEA values of less than .05, and xz/df values of less than three indicate an acceptable fit. Based on these criteria, the results of the fit indices displayed in Table 24 indicated a poor fit of the proposed structural equation model to the data, suggesting that the proposed model needed to be modified. 73 Inspection of modification indices (MIs) showed that the MI value for the error variable parameter, bearing on the association between the error term of cognitive image (r1) and the error term of perceived risks (r3), was relatively high, with a value of 95.01. This suggested that a better fit could be achieved by correlating the errors of the factors. In order to link the error terms, it must be supported by a substantive and/or empirical rationale (Byme, 2001). Several authors have suggested that there is an association between cognitive image of a destination and perceived risks of vacationing there (Suh and Gartner, 2004). When travelers perceive very low risks of vacationing in a destination, their evaluations of the destination tend to be very high. Also, travelers’ negative evaluations may be influenced by perceptions of high risks of vacationing in the destination. Since these suppositions are plausible, it was decided to link the error terms. The modified structural equation model for South Korea was the same as that for China and Japan, as shown in Figure 5. The modified model had a better fit to the data. The x2 was equal to 733.13, with 244 degrees of freedom and a p-value of less than .01. While an insignificant value of x2 is preferable, since x2 may be significant if the sample size is large (Kline, 1998), other fit indices were examined to determine the overall model fit of the modified measurement model. As shown in Table 24, xz/df, CF I, TLI, and RMSEA were within the recommended ranges of a model with good fit to the data. Thus, it was concluded that the modified structural equation model was acceptable. The x2 difference test, comparing the proposed and the modified measurement models, showed that the 12 difference was 104.84 and statistically significant (p < .01). This means that the proposed model was significantly improved through modification. 74 Table 24. Comparison of the initial and modified structural models for South Korea. Model x2 df xz/df CFI TLI IFI RMSEA Ax’ Adi Initial Model 837.97 245 3.42 .91 .90 .91 .06 Modified Model 733.13 244 2.98 .93 .92 .93 .05 104.84‘ 1 Note: CFI = Comparative Fit Index, TLI = Tucker-Lewis Index, IF I = Incremental Fit Index, RMSEA = Root Mean Square Error of Approximation. Criteria to accept model: x2/df should be smaller than}; CFI, TLI, and IFI should be greater than .90; RMSEA should be smaller than .05. significant at p < 0.01 Comparison of the Structural Equation Model for the Three Countries The proposed structural equation model, combining both the measurement and structural models, was tested for China, Japan, and South Korea. The overall model fit of the proposed structural equation model was poor across all three countries. The proposed model was modified for each of the three countries, and this modified model was used for China, Japan, and South Korea. Since the modified structural equation model had an adequate fit to the data across all three countries, it was accepted as the final measurement model for examining the hypothesized relationships among the factors. In the next section, the results of these hypothesis tests are presented. Hypotheses Testing The eight hypotheses regarding the relationships among the factors were tested in the structural equation model. The results of these hypothesis tests are separately reported for China, Japan, and South Korea. In this section, findings regarding the direct influence of one factor on another, as they relate to the hypotheses of this study, are reported first. Then, squared multiple correlations are reported to show the total variance in the factor (endogenous variables: dependent variables) as explained by the direct influences of the factor(s) (exogenous variables: independent variables or predictors). One advantage of 75 structural equation modeling is that it allows one to test not only direct influence, but also indirect and total influence between factors (Joreskog & Sorbom, 1993). Findings regarding the indirect and the total influence of one factor on another, as they relate to the hypotheses of this study, are presented at the end of this section. The China Model The results of hypotheses testing are presented in Table 25, including standardized path coefficients, critical ratio, and p values. A path diagram is also presented in Figure 6. The path diagram shows standardized path coefficients, representing the direction and strength of the direct influence of one factor on another, and squared multiple correlations indicating the total variance in a factor explained by the factor(s). Table 25. Results of hypotheses testing for China. Paths Cofffrtgient Critical Ratio P-value Supporta “$2.3 x $213523; sage 9 .04 .98 0.32 Y afrigaofigfismggsage 9 -.26 -5.19 0.00 x gligetlttilt‘l: 13:5: (:4) -56 11.36 0.00 x Arise?) x filflldhetrlrnihift (:16) -19 3.00 0.00 x 31:23:21: tbm;§;t()H7) -41 5.38 0.00 X firetre‘hii‘ofldt?\s’li(ssit_()H8) ~25 -4.86 0.00 x Note: 3X = Supported hypothesis, Y = Did not support hypothesis 76 R-square=.l 1 Cognitive Image .56 .19 ‘ R-square=.53 Affective .41 Intentlon Image * To Visit R-square=.56 -.28 -.25 Perceived Risks — significant R-square=.07 ........ non signifi cant Figure 6. Results of testing the proposed structural model with standardized path coefficients and squared multiple correlations (R2) for China. Note: Squared multiple correlations indicate the total variance explained by the direct influence of one factor on another. Direct influence related to study hypotheses. Regarding China, seven of eight hypotheses were supported, as shown in Figure 6. Information source usage on China had significant direct influences on cognitive image of China and perceived risks of vacationing in China (support for H1 and H3). The direct influence of information source usage on cognitive image ([3 = .33, p < .01) was larger than that on perceived risks ([3 = - .26, p < .01). The results of the hypotheses testing indicate that respondents who had more information on China appeared to have more positive cognitive image of the country and lower perceived risks of vacationing there. Only the hypothesis on the 77 relationship between information source usage and affective image (H2) was unsupported. No significant direct influence of information source usage on affective image was found (B = .04, p > .01). Cognitive image of China and perceived risks of vacationing in China had a significant direct influence on affective image of China (support for H4 and H5). This indicated that respondents who had a more positive cognitive image, and lower perceived risks, of China appeared to have a more positive affective image of China. The direct influence of cognitive image on affective image was very large (B = .56, p < .01), which supported previous studies showing the strong association between cognitive and affective image. Perceived risks of vacationing in China had a significant and negative influence on affective image of China (B = - .28, p < .01). Respondents who perceived higher risks of vacationing in China had a more negative affective image of the country. Cognitive image of China, affective image of China, and perceived risks of vacationing in China had significant direct influences on respondents’ intentions to visit China (support for H6, H7, and H8). This indicated that respondents who had more positive cognitive and affective images of China and lower perceived risks of vacationing in China appeared to have stronger intentions to visit China. Among the three factors influencing Americans’ intentions to visit China, affective image of China had the strongest direct influence on visit intention (B = .41 , p < .01), while perceived risks of vacationing in China had the second strongest direct influence on Visit intention (B = -.25, p < .01). Respondents who had more positive cognitive images of China showed greater intention to visit China ((B = .19, p < .01), even though the influence of cognitive image on Visit intention was weaker than those of affective image and perceived risks. 78 Multiple squared correlations. As shown in Figure 6, 53 percent of variance in intention to visit China was explained by the direct influences of cognitive image, affective image, and perceived risks. Information sources explained 11 percent of variance in cognitive image of China and 7 percent in perceived risks of vacationing in China. The large portion of unexplained variance in cognitive image and perceived risks suggests the possibility of including other factors which may influence cognitive image ' and perceived risks. Fifty-six percent of variance in affective image was explained by cognitive image and perceived risks. Indirect and total influences of the factors on visit intention. The standardized indirect influences of the factors on intention to visit China are presented in Table 26. Information source usage on China had a positive indirect influence on intention to visit China through perceived risks and cognitive image (B = .25). In other words, information source usage on China had a positive direct influence on cognitive image, which, in turn, had a positive direct influence on visit intention. Cognitive image and perceived risks also had indirect influences on visit intention through affective image. While the indirect influence of cognitive image on visit intention (B = .23) was larger than the direct influence (B = .19), the indirect influence of perceived risks on Visit intention (B = - .12) was smaller than the direct influence (B = - .25). Examination of the standardized total influence combining both direct and indirect influences, as presented in Table 26, showed that cognitive image had the largest standardized total influence (B = .42) and affective image had the second largest (B = .41). The total influence of perceived risks (B = .37) was smaller than that of the two destination image factors. 79 Table 26. Standardized estimates for direct, indirect, and total influences for China. Standardized Standardized Standardized Exogenous Endogenous Direct Indirect Total variables variables . . . 1nfluence Influence Influence Affective Image .04 .26 .30 Information Cognitive Image .33 --- .33 30”“ Perceived Risks -.26 -.26 Usage Intention to Visit --- .25 .25 Affective Image Intention to Visit .41 --- .41 Cognitive Image Affective Image .56 --- .56 Intention to Visit ._ .19 .23 .42 Perceived risks Affective Image -.28 --- -.28 Intention to Visit -.25 -.12 -.37 The Japan Model The results of hypotheses testing are presented in Table 27, including standardized path coefficients, critical ratio, and p values. A path diagram is also presented in Figure 7. The path diagram shows standardized path coefficients, representing the direction and strength of the direct influences of one factor on another, and squared multiple correlations indicating the total variance explained by direct influences between factors. 80 Table 27. Results of hypotheses testing for Japan. Paths Coffafitfien t Critical Ratio P-value Supporta g$3;0?mi°gf:§gsage ‘) .38 7.85 0.00 x $233201?“ Egrffiztisage 9 .06 1.41 0.16 Y gigggofii 51555516335835 9 —.29 -5.98 0.00 X gig??? 11:21:53“ .57 1 1.15 0.00 x 5.555551513152551 "’2 ‘4'” 0'00 x giggfletin‘l/aliiet (:16) '34 5' 16 000 X 855 55555317) 28 “5 0'00 X 5152:5555? 1815515318) "20 5'89 0'00 x Note: 5X = Supported hypothesis, Y = Did not support hypothesis 81 R-square=. 14 Cognitive Image .57 .34 ‘ R-Square=.49 Affeh .28 Intention Image To Visit R-square=.52 -.22 -.20 Perceived Risks — significant R-square=.08 ........ non Significant Figure 7. Results of testing the proposed structural model with standardized path coefficients and squared multiple correlations (R2) for Japan. Note: Squared multiple correlations indicate the total variance explained by the direct influence of one factor on another. Direct influences related to study hypotheses. Regarding Japan, seven of eight hypotheses were supported, as shown in Figure 7. Information source usage had significant direct influences on cognitive image of Japan and perceived risks of vacationing in Japan (support for H1 and H3). This finding indicates that respondents who obtained more information on Japan had a more positive cognitive image of Japan and lower perceived risks of vacationing in Japan. The direct influence of information source usage on cognitive image (B = .3 8, p < .01) was larger than this factor’s influence on perceived risks (B = -.29, p < .01). Only the hypothesis regarding the relationship 82 between information source usage and affective image (H2) was unsupported. No significant direct influence of information source usage on affective image was found (B = .06, p > .01). Cognitive image of Japan and perceived risks of vacationing in Japan had a significant direct influence on affective image of Japan (support for H4 and H5). This finding indicates that respondents who had a positive cognitive image of Japan and lower perceived risks of vacationing in the country appeared to have a more positive affective image of Japan. The direct influence of cognitive image on affective image was very strong (B = .57, p < .01), which supported previous studies showing the strong association between cognitive and affective image (Baloglu and McCleary, 1999). Perceived risks of vacationing in Japan had a significant and negative direct influence on affective image (B = - 22, p < .01), but this direct influence of perceived risks was smaller than that of cognitive image on affective image. Cognitive and affective image of Japan and perceived risks of vacationing in Japan had significant direct influences on respondents’ intentions to visit Japan (support for H6, H7, and H8). This result indicates that respondents who had more positive cognitive and affective images and lower perceived risks regarding Japan appeared to have greater intention to visit Japan. Among the three factors influencing Visit intention to Japan, cognitive image had the strongest direct influence on visit intention (B = .34, p < .01); affective image had the second strongest (B = .28, p < .01). The direct influence of perceived risks of vacationing in Japan on intention to Visit (B = -.20, p < .01) was smaller than that of the two destinations’ image factors. 83 Multiple squared correlations. As shown in Figure 7, 49 percent of variance in intention to visit Japan was explained by the direct influences of cognitive image, affective image, and perceived risks. Information source usage explained 14 percent of variance in cognitive image of Japan, but only 8 percent of variance in Americans’ perceived risks of vacationing in Japan. The large portion of unexplained variance in cognitive image and perceived risks suggests the possibility of including other factors which may influence cognitive image and perceived risks in future research. Fifty-two percent of variance in affective image was explained by cognitive image and perceived risks. Indirect and total influences of the factors on visit intention. The standardized indirect influences of the factors on intention to visit Japan are presented in Table 28. Table 28. Standardized estimates for direct, indirect, and total influences for Japan. Standardized Standardized Standardized Exogenous Endogenous Direct Indirect Total variables variables . . . Influence influence . Influence Affective Image .06 .28 .34 Information Cognitive Image .38 --- .38 Source Perceived risks -.29 --- -.29 Usage Intention to visit --- .28 .27 Affective Image Intention to visit "28 _...,._ --- .28 Cognitive Image Affective Image .57 --- .57 Intention to visit 34 .16 .50 Perceived risks Affective Image -.22 --- -.22 Intention to visit -.20 -.06 -.26 Information source usage on Japan had a positive indirect influence on intention to visit Japan through perceived risks and cognitive image (B = .27). In other words, information source usage on Japan had a positive direct influence on cognitive image, 84 which, in turn, had a positive direct influence on Visit intention. Cognitive image of Japan and perceived risks also had indirect influences on visit intention through affective image. The indirect influence of cognitive image on intention to visit (B = .16) was larger than that of perceived risks (B = - .06). Examination of the standardized total influence combining both direct and indirect standardized influences, as presented in Table 28, showed that cognitive image had the largest standardized total influence (B = .50). Affective image had the second largest, but the standardized total influence of affective image (B = .28) was much smaller than that of cognitive image. Perceived risks of vacationing in Japan had the smallest standardized total influence on intention to visit (B = -.26). 85 The South Korea Model The results of hypotheses testing are presented in Table 29, including standardized path coefficients, critical ratio, and p values. A path diagram is also presented in Figure 8. The path diagram shows standardized path coefficients, representing the direction and strength of the direct influence of one factor on another, and squared multiple correlations indicating the total variance explained by direct influences between factors. Table 29. Results of hypotheses testing for South Korea. Paths Coggfiem Critical Ratio P-value Supporta 5.5555555552553555 5 '33 7'31 0'00 X 5555555555555555 5 '0‘ '23 0'81 Y 55255550515555153555 5 “22 '4'96 0'00 X 3m?) X 5555555 15115:: (:15) "28 "6'15 0'00 X gm?) X 35:31::56515553H7) '18 2'96 0'00 X 11555115515515558318) "8 2'96 0'00 X Note: 1’X = Supported hypothesis, Y = Did not support hypothesis 86 R-square=.l 1 Cognitive Image .54 .28 R-square=.45 ‘ NEE . 1 8 Intention Image v To / Visit R-square=.51 -.28 -.33 Perceived Risks — significant R-square=.05 -------- non significant Figure 8. Results of testing the proposed structural model with standardized path coefficients and squared multiple correlations (R2) for South Korea. Note: Squared multiple correlations indicate the total variance explained by the direct influence of one factor on another. Direct influences related study hypotheses. Regarding South Korea, seven of eight hypotheses were supported, as shown in Figure 8. Information source usage had significant direct influences on cognitive image of South Korea and on perceived risks of vacationing in South Korea (support for H1 and H3). This finding indicates that respondents who used more information appeared to have more positive cognitive image of the country and lower perceived risks of vacationing there. The direct influence of information source usage on cognitive image (B = .33, p < .01) was larger than this 87 factor’s influence on perceived risks (B = -.22, p < .01). Only the hypothesis regarding the relationship between information source usage and affective image (H2) was rejected (B = .01, p > .01). Cognitive image of South Korea and perceived risks of vacationing in South Korea had a significant and positive direct influence on affective image of South Korea (support for H4 and H5). This finding indicates that respondents who had a more positive cognitive image of the country and lower perceived risks of vacationing there appeared to have a more positive affective image of South Korea. The direct influence of cognitive image on affective image was very large (B = .54, p < .01), which supported previous studies showing the strong association between cognitive and affective image. Perceived risks of vacationing in South Korea had a significant and negative direct influence on affective image of the country (B = - .28, p < .01) but this direct influence of perceived risks was smaller than that of cognitive image. Cognitive and affective image of South Korea and perceived risks of vacationing in South Korea had significant direct influences on respondents’ intentions to visit South Korea (support for H6, H7, and H8). This indicated that respondents who had a more positive cognitive image of the country and lower perceived risks of vacationing there appeared to have greater intention to visit South Korea. Among the three factors influencing visit intention to South Korea, perceived risks had the strongest direct influence on visit intention (B = -.33, p < .01); cognitive image had the second strongest direct influence (B = .28, p < .01). The direct influence of affective image on intention to visit (B = .18, p < .01) was smaller than that of cognitive image and perceived risks. 88 Multiple squared correlations. As shown in Figure 8, 45 percent of the variance in intention to visit South Korea was explained by the direct influences of cognitive image, affective image, and perceived risks. Information source use explained 11 percent of variance in cognitive image of South Korea, but only 5 percent in variance for perceived risks. The large portion of the unexplained variance in cognitive image and perceived risks suggests the possibility of including other factors which may influence cognitive image and perceived risks in future studies. Fifty-one percent of variance in affective image was explained by cognitive image and perceived risks. Indirect and total influences of the factors on visit intention. The standardized indirect influences of the factors on Americans’ intention to visit South Korea are presented in Table 30. Table 30. Standardized estimates for direct, indirect, and total influences for South Korea. Standardized Standardized Standardized Exogenous Endogenous Direct Indirect Total vanables varlables . . . Influence Influence Influence Affective Image .01 .24 .25 Information Cognitive Image .33 --- .33 Source Perceived risks -.22 --- -.22 Usage Intention to visit --- .21 .21 Affective Image Intentiqp_!.9_Xi.§i__t.__~_.._-_-_wW ,V -13 .18 Cognitive Image Affective Image .54 --- .54 Intentlon to VISIt 28W. .09 .37 Perceived risks Affective Image -.28 --- -.28 Intention to visit -.33 -.O6 -.39 Information source usage had a positive indirect influence on intention to visit South Korea through perceived risks and cognitive image (B = 0.21). In other words, information source usage on South Korea had a positive direct influence on cognitive 89 image, which, in turn, had a positive direct influence on visit intention. Cognitive image of South Korea and perceived risks of vacationing there also had indirect influences on Visit intention through affective image. The indirect influence of cognitive image and perceived risks on intention to visit South Korea was small (B = .09 for cognitive image and B = - .06 for perceived risks). Examination of the standardized total influence combining both direct and indirect standardized influences, as presented in Table 30, showed that perceived risks had the largest standardized total influence on intention to Visit South Korea (B = -.39); cognitive image had the second largest influence (B = .3 7). The total influence of affective image (B = .18) was much smaller than that of cognitive image and perceived risks. Comparison of the results of hypotheses testing for the three East Asian countries. Overall, seven of eight hypotheses were supported in the three models. These results are summarized in Table 31. 90 Table 31. Results of hypotheses testing across three East Asian countries. Std. Path Coefficients i Paths China Japan South Support Korea Information Source Usage 9 33,” 38** 33" X Cognitive Image (H1) ' ° ' Information Source Usage 9 Affective Image (H2) '04 '06 '01 Y Information Source Usage 9 _ H _ H _ H Perceived Risks (H3) '26 '29 '22 X Cognitive Image 9 Affective Image (H4) .56” .57“ .54" X Perceived Risks 9 Affective Image (H5) -.28** -.22"‘* -.28** X Cognitive Image 9 Intention to Visit (H6) .19" .34" .28" X Affective Image 9 Intention to Visit (H7) .41" .28” .18” X Perceived Risks 9 Intention to Visit (H8) -.26** -.20** -.33** X Note: significant at ** p < .01, a X = Supported hypothesis, Y = Did not support hypothesis As shown in Table 31, the finding that information usage had an insignificant direct influence on affective image was the same across the three countries. All other hypotheses were supported for China, Japan, and South Korea. This similarity indicates that the same model can be applied to all three countries in order to explain Americans’ intentions to visit them. Similarly, across China, Japan, and South Korea, cognitive image had a very strong direct influence on affective image, with a standardized direct influence greater than .50. This finding indicates that cognitive image was the strongest factor influencing affective image. A third finding across all three countries is that the direct influence of information sources on cognitive image was greater than this factor’s influence on perceived risks, a finding which indicates that increasing information usage played a more important role in improving respondents’ cognitive image of these countries than did decreasing perceived risks of vacationing there. As well as similarities, there were differences in the results of hypotheses testing across the three models. One such difference is the factor exhibiting the greatest direct 91 influence on intention to visit. For China, this factor was affective image; for Japan, cognitive image; for South Korea, perceived risks. Interestingly, perceived risks had the least direct influence on intention to Visit for China and Japan, while affective image had the least direct influence on intention to visit for South Korea, findings which suggest South Korea’s present inability to instill prospective American tourists with knowledge and good feeling about South Korea, in comparison to China and Japan. It remains to be explained why cognitive image of Japan exerted more influence on Americans’ intentions to visit this country, while affective image did so for China. Another difference in the results of the hypotheses testing was the strength of the total influence combining both the direct and indirect influences on intention to visit, as displayed in Table 32. In both the China and Japan models, cognitive image had the greatest total influence on intention to visit. In the South Korea model, perceived risks had the greatest total influence on intention to visit South Korea. These findings suggest that the information prospective American tourists obtained on China and Japan produced in them more knowledge and good feeling about these countries than did the information obtained on South Korea perhaps because prospective American tourists obtained information on China and Japan more frequently than they did on South Korea. Table 32. Comparison of total influence. Exogenous variables Endogenous Standardized total influence variables China Japan South Korea Informatlon Source Intention to visit .25 .28 .21 Usage Affective Image Intention to visit .41 .28 .18 Cognitive Image Intention to visit .42 .50 .37 Perceived Risks Intention to visit -.37 -.26 -.39 92 Additional Analysis: The Comparative Influence of Different Types of Information Source on Destination Image and Perceived Risks As shown in the results of the above SEM analyses, cognitive image, affective image, and perceived risks were all important factors in promoting American vacationers’ intention to visit China, Japan, and South Korea, even though the strength of their influence varied across the three countries. Given the importance of these three factors in influencing Visit intention, the next task was to determine which type of information source most directly influenced cognitive image and perceived risks and, through these two factors, intention to visit. Affective image was excluded from this assessment, given that information source usage had been shown to have no direct influence on affective image. These correlations are reported in Table 33. Table 33. Correlation between types of information source used and cognitive image and perceived risk. Country Infol Inf02 Info3 Info4 Inf05 lnfo6 Info7 Cogn- China .12M .15** .2011tb .19" .28W .18" .01 itive Japan .14" .21M .23“b .18" 29*“ .20" .02 Image 3. Kor. .15" .21M b .19" .16" .27W 21"" .11* Perce- China -.04 -.11** -.13** -.15**" -.17**“ -.10** -12" ived Japan -.12** -.17** -.19="=tb -.20**° -.18** -.l6" -.04 Risks s. Kor. -.1 l” _21" a -.12** -.16** -.20Mb -.11** -.12** Significant at * p< 0.05, ** < 0.01 Note: a Most correlated; bSecond most correlated. . Infol=TV/radio, Inf02=Word of mouth, Info3=Books and movies, Info4=lntemet, Inf05=Cu1turaI experiences (in art museums, restaurants, concert halls, courses), Info6=Newspapers/magazines, Info7=Professional advice As shown in Table 33, cultural experiences in, for example, art museums, restaurants, concert halls, and courses had the highest correlation with respondents’ cognitive image of all three countries. The type of information source having the second 93 highest correlation with cognitive image was books and movies for China and Japan, and word of mouth and newspapers/magazines for South Korea. However, the type of information source having the highest correlation with perceived risks varied across the three countries. Cultural experience had the highest correlation with perceived risks for China, followed by the Internet. The Internet had the highest correlation with perceived risks for Japan, followed by cultural experiences and books and movies. Finally, word of mouth had the highest correlation with perceived risks for South Korea, followed by the cultural experiences. These findings suggest that, in general, Americans’ perceptions of the risks of vacationing in China, Japan, or South Korea depend most heavily on the type of information source from which they have obtained the most information on each of these three countries. If this primary information source produces knowledge and good feeling about China, Japan, and South Korea, it would seem that this information source lowers prospective American tourists’ sense of potential travel-related risk in these East Asian countries. The results of these correlation analyses will be further discussed in terms of their implications for both tourism researchers and tourism marketers in Chapter Five. "Sun .1” 94 CHAPTER 5 DISCUSSION, IMPLICATIONS, AND CONCLUSIONS The present dissertation study examined the influences of destination image, perceived risks, and information source usage on Americans’ intentions to visit China, Japan, and South Korea in order to identify which factors most influenced Americans’ intentions to visit these East Asian countries. Chapter Five has four sections. In the first section, a summary and discussion of the findings are provided. In the second section, both theoretical and practical implications of the findings are discussed. In the third section, limitations of this research and suggestions for future research are presented. The final section provides conclusions. Summary and Discussion of the Findings The purpose of this study was to develop and empirically test a model to explain Americans’ intentions to visit three East Asian countries (China, Japan, and South Korea). It was proposed that Americans’ visit intentions could be explained by the interrelations among cognitive and affective destination image of these countries in general, perceived risks of vacationing in these countries, and information source usage regarding these countries. Before testing the proposed model against the data collected, it was necessary to determine if statistically significant differences existed among the data on China, Japan, and South Korea. If so, then the proposed model would have to be tested separately against data for each country. If not, data could be pooled for testing. Statistical analyses of the mean differences across the variables for each of the three countries indicated 95 significant differences. Thus, data on China, Japan, and South Korea were separated in order to test the proposed conceptual model. This section consists of three subsections. In the first subsection, findings about mean differences across the variables for China, Japan, and South Korea are summarized. In the second subsection, results of hypotheses testing are summarized and discussed. In the final subsection, findings about relationships between information source usage and destination image and perceived risks are summarized and discussed. Mean Differences across the Variables for Three East Asian Countries Significant mean differences were found across the variables (destination image, perceived risks, information source usage, and visit intention) for China, Japan, and South Korea. First, significant mean differences in cognitive image were found across the three countries. A comparison revealed that study participants’ had a more positive cognitive image of China and Japan than they did of South Korea. South Korea was ranked last for all cognitive image variables. Japan was ranked first as offering many things to see and do, easy to navigate, quality accommodation, friendly local people, and unique cultural resources, while China was regarded as the best East Asian country in terms of natural scenic beauty, appealing local food and drink, and interesting historical attractions. While mean differences in cognitive image were observed across the three countries, the strengths of each were similar. Japan and China had the highest mean scores for many things to do and see, and interesting historical attractions, and South Korea had the highest mean scores for interesting historical attractions and unique cultural resources, findings which suggest that these East Asian countries cultural and historical resources might distinguish them from other destinations. 96 Second, significant differences in affective image were found across China, Japan, and South Korea. The mean scores for affective image items for Japan were the highest, while the mean scores for affective image items for South Korea were the lowest. However, when compared to the mean scores for cognitive image items, the mean scores for affective image items were found to be smaller, suggesting that not all knowledge about a place (cognitive destination image) produces feeling about a place (affective destination image). Third, significant differences were observed in six of the seven perceived risk items. The mean differences for language barrier across the three countries were insignificant; however, regarding all three countries, this perceived risk item received the highest mean score. This indicates that respondents perceived that they would have problems in communicating with people in the three East Asian countries. The mean scores for terrorism and violence were highest for South Korea, while the mean scores for likelihood of illness, political instability, and potential health problems were highest for China. The mean score for natural disaster was highest for Japan, while the mean scores of other perceived risk items for Japan were lower, or equal to, those for China and South Korea. Overall, South Korea and China were perceived as being riskier countries than Japan. Fourth, significant differences across China, Japan, and South Korea were found regarding information source usage. The mean scores for all types of information source usage were lowest for South Korea. The mean scores for TV/radio, word of mouth, the Internet, and professional advice were highest for Japan, while the mean scores for books/movies, and cultural experiences were highest for China. This indicates that respondents were more unfamiliar with South Korea than with China and Japan. While 97 there were mean differences in information source usage across the three countries, the mean scores for all three countries were not high. This finding indicates a lack of information on all three countries, possibly due to a limited search for such information. Finally, statistically significant differences in visit intention across China, Japan, and South Korea were found. Japan had the highest scores for intention to visit, followed by China and South Korea. This result makes sense when considering the mean scores for cognitive and affective destination image, perceived risks, and information source usage. It can be inferred that Japan was the first-choice destination of study participants, since Japan had the highest mean scores for most destination image variables, including all affective image variables, and the lowest mean scores for most perceived risk variables. Similarly, it can be assumed that China was their second-choice destination, since its positive destination image variables were somewhat offset by its perceived risk variables. South Korea, it seems, was the last-choice destination, since South Korea received the lowest mean scores for all destination image variables and the highest mean scores for most perceived risk variables. 98 Results of Hypotheses Testing As presented in Figure 8, the conceptual model this study proposed to explain Americans’ intentions to visit China, Japan, and South Korea included eight hypotheses. Cognitive Image H4 H6 Information Intention Source - Affective H7 W To. Usage Image VISIt H3 H 5 H8 Perceived Risks Figure 9. Proposed conceptual model The results of Structural Equation Modeling analyses showed similarities and differences across participants’ responses regarding China, Japan, and South Korea. For all three countries, seven of the eight hypotheses were supported. For these seven hypotheses, the sign/direction of the relationships was found to be consistent across all three countries; however, the magnitude of these relationships differed. Results of hypotheses testing are summarized below. 99 Hypotheses 1 & 2: Increased usage of information sources increases Americans ’ (emotionally neutral) cognitive image of the destination in general, and increased usage of information sources increases Americans ’ (emotionally positive) aflective image of the destination in general. Across all three countries, the hypothesis regarding the direct influence of information source usage on cognitive image was supported (H1); however, the hypothesis regarding the direct influence of information source usage on affective image was not supported (H2). These findings provide 5 empirical evidence in support of previous studies that noted the influence of information source usage on cognitive destination image (Baloglu, 1999; Gartner, 1993). These findings also indicate that respondents having greater access to information on each of the Ii: three countries had a higher cognitive image of them. The strength of the direct influence of information source usage on cognitive image was similar across the three countries (B = .33 for China, B = .38 for Japan, and B = .33 for South Korea), a finding consistent with Baloglu’s (2001) study, in which he found that the more varied the information sources consulted on a destination, the higher the cognitive image of that destination. Hypothesis 3: Increased usage of information sources decreases Americans ’ perceived risks of vacationing in the destination. Hypothesis 3 was supported for all three countries. Information source usage had a significant direct influence on perceived risks. This finding indicates that respondents who frequently accessed various types of information sources had lower perceived risks of vacationing in China, Japan, and South Korea. The strength of the direct influence of information source usage on perceived risks was similar across the three countries (B = -.26 for China, B =.-.29 for Japan, and B = -.22 for South Korea). The direct influence of information source usage on perceived risks was 100 slightly weaker when compared to that of cognitive image. This finding also provides empirical evidence in support of research showing that the more information travelers have on a destination, the lower their perceived risks of vacationing in that destination (Fodness & Murray, 1999; Gemunden, 1985; Maser & Weiermair, 1998; Sonmez & Graefe, 1998b). Hypotheses 4 & 5: Increased (emotionally neutral) cognitive destination image increases Americans ’ (emotionally positive) affective destination image, and increased perceived risks of vacationing in the destination decreases Americans’ (emotionally positive) affective image of the destination in general. Hypotheses 4 and 5 were supported across China, Japan, and South Korea. While cognitive image had a significant and positive direct influence on affective image for each country, perceived risks had a significant and negative direct influence on affective image for each country. This finding indicates that respondents who had a higher cognitive image of China, Japan, and South Korea and lower perceived risks of vacationing in these countries exhibited more positive affective images. The strength of the direct influences of cognitive image on affective image was similar across the three countries. Cognitive image had a very strong direct influence on affective image, with a standardized direct influence of greater than .5 across China, Japan, and South Korea (B = .56 for China, B = .57 for Japan, and B = .54 for South Korea). The strength of the direct influences of perceived risks on affective image was also similar across the three countries. Perceived risks moderately influenced affective image across the three countries (B = -.28 for China, B =.-.22 for Japan, and B = - .28 for South Korea). This finding regarding the significant influence of cognitive image on affective image is consistent with Baloglu and McCleary’s (1999a) study, which 101 asserted that travelers’ intention to visit a destination was indirectly influenced by cognitive image of the destination. However, the present study is perhaps the first to test and confirm the widely held assumption that Americans’ perceived risks of vacationing in a destination influences their affective image of that destination. Hypotheses 6 & 7: Increased (emotionally neutral) cognitive destination image increases Americans ’ intention to visit the destination, and increased (emotionally positive) affective destination image increases Americans ’ intention to visit the destination. Hypotheses 6 and 7 were supported across all three countries. Both cognitive and affective image had significant and positive direct influences on Americans’ intentions to visit each country. The higher Americans’ cognitive and affective images of a destination, the greater their intention to visit the destination. This finding suggests that both destination image components contributed to travelers’ intentions to visit China, Japan, and South Korea, and thus confirms the findings of previous tourism studies (Baloglu & McCleary, 1999a; Baloglu, 2000; Pizam, 1991; Vogt & Andereck, 2003). While the sign/direction of the influence between the two destination image components and Americans’ intentions to visit was consistent across the three countries, the strength of the relationship differed across China, Japan, and South Korea. Affective image had the strongest direct influence on Americans’ intention to visit China (B = .41 for affective image and B = .19 for cognitive image), while cognitive image had the strongest direct influence on Americans’ intention to visit Japan (B = .28 for affective image and B = .34 for cognitive image). The direct influence of destination image on Americans’ visit intentions was less strong for South Korea (B = .18 for affective image, and B = .28 for cognitive image) than it was for China and Japan. 102 Only one study has compared the influences of both affective and cognitive image on travelers’ intentions to visit a destination (four Mediterranean countries) (Baloglu, I999). Baloglu (1999) found that, before the first visit, cognitive image was more influential on visit intention than was affective image. Findings for the Japan and South Korea models support Baloglu’s study; however, the findings from the China model do not, suggesting that the relative influences of affective and cognitive image may differ depending on the destination. Hypothesis 8: Increased perceived risks of vacationing in a destination decreases American ’3 intention to visit the destination. Hypothesis 8 was supported for all three countries. Perceived risks were found to have significant and negative direct influences on Americans’ intentions to Visit each country. This finding indicates that respondents who perceived higher risks of vacationing in the three countries had diminished intentions to visit them. This result supports the findings of previous tourism studies (Bramwell & Rawding, 1996; Coshall, 2003; Dann, 1996; Sonmez & Graefe, 1998a, 1998b). However, the strength of perceived risks’ direct influence on intention to visit differed across the three countries. It was stronger for South Korea (B = -.33) than it was for China and Japan (B = -.26 for China, and B = -.20 for Japan). In comparison to the direct influence exerted by destination image on visit intention, perceived risks had a stronger direct influence for South Korea, but destination image (either cognitive or affective) had a stronger direct influence for China and Japan. 103 Overall Pattern of Relationships among the Factors across the Three Countries The findings derived from the structural equation model reveal that information source usage, cognitive and affective destination image, and perceived risks significantly influenced American tourists’ intentions to visit China, Japan, and South Korea in both direct and indirect ways: (1) information source usage influenced visit intention indirectly through cognitive image and perceived risks; (2) cognitive image and perceived risks influenced visit intention both directly and indirectly through affective image; and (3) affective image influenced visit intention directly. These results were found to be consistent for all three East Asian countries. However, the total influence, which combines both the direct and the indirect influences of each factor on visit intention, varied across the three countries. The factors which had the strongest total influence were affective and cognitive destination image for China, cognitive destination image for Japan, and perceived risks for South Korea. This result makes sense in terms of the social and political instability in South Korea at the time when respondents were surveyed. (Between May 20 and June 20, 2006, the US. regarded North Korea’s attainment of nuclear capability as a threat to national security: President George W. Bush named North Korea one of three “axes of evil.” Confusion of North Korea with South Korea possibly had a negative effect on study participants’ responses.) American respondents had a less developed cognitive image of South Korea, as shown in the previous section, which seems to have allowed perceived risks to dominate their affective image of the country, and, in turn, their visit intentions. This was not the case for China and Japan, countries about which Americans had more varied information and thus more positive feelings. 104 Relationships between Information Source Usage and Destination Image and Perceived Risks As shown above, cognitive image, affective image, and perceived risks were all important factors in promoting Americans’ intentions to Visit China, Japan, and South Korea, though the strength of influence for each of these factors varied across the three countries. Thus, it was important to examine which types of information source most influenced these three factors. In this study, information sources were found to have significant direct influences on cognitive image and perceived risks, and, through them, an indirect influence on affective image. In other words, information sources had direct influences on both cognitive image and perceived risks, while they had indirect influence on affective image through cognitive image and perceived risks. Thus, it was important to identify what types of information source used were more influential in improving Americans’ cognitive images and in reducing their perceived risks. It was found that cultural experiences (in art museums, restaurants, concert halls, and courses) had the highest correlation with cognitive image across the three countries. Books and movies were the most influential information source on cognitive image for China and Japan, and word of mouth and newspapers/magazines were the most influential information sources on cognitive image for South Korea. These same information sources influenced perceived risks for the three countries: for China, cultural experiences, followed by the Internet, was the most influential; for Japan, the Internet, followed by cultural experiences and books and movies, was the most influential; and, for South Korea, word of mouth, followed by cultural experiences, was the most influential. 105 Implications The findings of this study have both theoretical and practical implications. This section presents, first, the theoretical contributions of this study to existing tourism literature, then its practical implications for East-Asia-based tourism marketers. Theoretical Implications From a theoretical perspective, this study contributes to current understanding of American tourists’ decision-making processes regarding China, Japan, and South Korea. One important implication of this study is the explanatory model it provides. Specifically, this model clarifies the mutual influence of general cognitive image of a destination and perceived risks of vacationing there on affective image. Both of these factors are directly influenced by information source usage, particularly cultural experiences, and all three of these factors directly influence American tourists’ intentions to Visit China, Japan, and South Korea for the first time. Thus, this study provides a model for tourism researchers that may be tested in relation to other tourist destinations. A second theoretical implication of this study emerges from its operationalization of the destination image construct. Recent studies have suggested that destination image includes two distinct but interrelated components: cognitive destination image and affective destination image (Baloglu and McCleary, 1999b; Gartner, 1993; Reilly, 1990). However, most studies of destination image have focused on cognitive destination image. The present study operationalized destination image as a construct comprising both cognitive and affective destination image, and empirically proved the distinctiveness of these two image components, using confirmatory factor analysis. This study also demonstrated the interrelatedness of these two image components, specifically showing 106 that cognitive image can either create affective image (as its antecedent) or modify existing affective image. Thus, the findings of this study suggest that, instead of using either cognitive or affective image as measures of destination image, tourism researchers should use both image components. This two-fold analysis reveals the complexity of destination image to tourism researchers. A third theoretical implication of this study is its contribution to existing literature on destination image formation. Several destination image studies have emphasized the importance of information sources in a prospective tourist’s development of a destination image (Baloglu, 1999; Beerli & Martin, 2004a; Fakeye & Crompton, 1991; Gunn, 1972; Hanlan & Kelly, 2005). For example, Baloglu (1999, 2001) found that the variety of the information sources consulted contributed positively to travelers’ cognitive image of a destination. Baloglu also demonstrated that different types of information sources have varying degrees of effect on tourists’ cognitive images of four Mediterranean countries. The present study confirms Baloglu’s findings, demonstrating that a variety of information source usage significantly influences both prospective American tourists’ cognitive destination image of China, Japan, and South Korea and their perceived risks of vacationing in these countries. This study also extends Baloglu’s findings in demonstrating that different types of information sources are more influential depending on the destination. For example, books and movies were more highly related to perceived risks of vacationing in Japan than they were for China and South Korea. A fourth theoretical implication of this study is that the importance of factors directly influencing visit intention (i.e., cognitive image, affective image, and perceived risks) varied from destination to destination. For China, affective image was the most influential; for Japan, cognitive image; and, for South Korea, perceived risks. Future 107 ‘II research regarding other tourism destinations might also conclude that limited information on a destination allows perceived risks to dominate affective image and thus to influence American tourists’ intentions to visit the destination most heavily. Practical Implications Additionally, the present study provides important information for East-Asia- based tourism marketers. One important practical implication of this study emerges from comparing prospective American tourists’ responses to South Korea to their responses to China and Japan. Because study participants lacked information on South Korea, their perceived risks of vacationing there dominated their affective image of the country in general and ultimately weakened their intention to visit South Korea. The opposite was true for China and Japan. Based on this finding, South-Korea-based tourism marketers should focus on increasing Americans’ cognitive image of their country, particularly by promoting cultural experiences, an information source which was found to have a positive influence on cognitive image for all three countries, but more so for China and Japan. South Korean tourism marketers should invest in museum exhibits, concerts, study abroad programs, and South Korean cuisine festivals in the U. S. Chinese and Japanese tourism marketers should increase their efforts in this domain as well to attract even more American tourists. A second practical implication of this study emerges from comparison of the three countries. It would seem that increased investment in U.S.-based cultural experiences promoting China, Japan, and South Korea might improve prospective American tourists’ cognitive images of these three countries. Not only should East Asian tourism marketers increase their promotional efforts in the US, but they should also place special emphasis 108 on distinguishing their country’s attractions from those of other East Asian countries. (Regarding South Korea, respondents perceived South Korea as having interesting historical attractions and unique cultural resources: two strengths of this country. However, China and Japan were also perceived as having these strengths.) Thus, it would seem that East-Asia-based tourism marketers must also focus on presenting their destinations as being unique. One promotional strategy would be to provide prospective American tourists with more detailed information about China, Japan, and South Korea, helping them to recognize both that each country has cultural and historical resources different from the other two, and that each country’s culture and history are closely related to those of the other two, thereby warranting joint trips to all three destinations. For example, American tourists could visit different Buddhist temples in all three countries, admiring their variation. Cultural experiences orchestrated in the US. might also serve this purpose. For example, a traveling museum exhibit on the tea ceremony in East Asia might be jointly funded by tourism marketers in China, Japan, and South Korea. Americans could, then, sample black tea from China, green tea from Japan, and ginseng tea from South Korea. For the sports-oriented crowd, a similar traveling exhibit, this time targeting sports arenas, could showcase similarities and differences in East Asian martial arts traditions. Americans could watch shows of kungfu from China, karate from Japan, and tae kwon do from South Korea. Although cultural experiences would be created by these traveling exhibits, so would word of mouth, an information source on which South Korea relies. To promote books and movies, East-Asia-based tourism marketers could sponsor public readings and book signings from Chinese, Japanese, and South Korean authors 109 who treat similar themes in their work; or a film festival in which movies from China, Japan, and South Korea would compete for a prestigious award. Competitive, “Iron Chef”-style food tastings at which dishes would be prepared by celebrity chefs from China, Japan, and South Korea might also be arranged to promote cookbooks or cooking- related videos. Increasing prospective American tourists’ cognitive images of these three East Asian countries is crucial in that, as this study demonstrates, lack of information results in the overshadowing of positive affective image by perceived risks, an effect which weakens visit intentions. Tourism marketers in China, Japan, and South Korea have to work particularly hard to counterbalance market-damaging phenomena, such as SARS, bird flu, natural disasters, and terrorism. Sonmez and Graefe (1998a, 1998b) found that perceived risks was a critical predictor of American tourists’ avoiding a particular destination. This finding, combined with the results of the present dissertation study, suggest that East-Asia-based tourism marketers should design promotional tools that address each of the perceived risks identified in this study, as well as positive cultural experiences. Finally, this study offers East-Asia-based tourism marketers information on how to invest more effectively in marketing programs that target prospective American tourists. For example, tourism marketers based in China, Japan, and South Korea should invest in programs that increase cognitive image and decrease perceived risks; however, such investments would especially benefit the South Korean tourism industry. As the Internet was a key source of information concerning perceived risks for the respondents in this study, East-Asia-based tourism marketers should also consider designing Web sites that highlight the safety and the ease of navigation offered by these three countries. All 110 marketing strategies should emphasize the “English-friendliness” of China, Japan, and South Korea by presenting cultural events in English and by providing bilingual resources on promotional Web sites, including on-site translation services. Although tourism marketers in China, Japan, and South Korea face similar challenges, they should not forget to highlight for prospective American tourists their countries’ unique attractions. Limitations of this Study and Suggestions for Future Research One limitation of this study is associated with the sampling frame; only residents of Michigan and Illinois were selected for inclusion. If residents of other states had participated in the study, responses might have differed. Thus, the generalizablity of the findings is limited to similar populations. Future research would need to sample a wider diversity of Americans in order to confirm or deny claims made in this study about Americans’ perceptions of East Asian countries. A second limitation of this study relates to the information source usage items in the survey. Although these items measured the frequency with which each type of information source was consulted, it did not assess the exact content of each information source. (The scale used in the present dissertation was relative, not absolute.) Thus, future research would need to capture the content of each information source in order to advance understanding of how information sources influence destination image and perceived risks regarding China, Japan, and South Korea. A third limitation concerns the scope of this study, which sampled only Americans who had not previously visited China, Japan, and South Korea. As discussed in the literature review, visitors might have responded differently regarding destination image, perceived risks, and intention to visit. If a sample of Visitors were surveyed, an 111 altered model would likely be needed to explain the relationships assessed in this study. However, this possibility remains to be investigated in future research. A fourth limitation of this study also concerns scope: Other factors remain to be incorporated into the model. As indicated in previous research, socio-demographics, such as age, educational background, and gender, affect prospective tourists’ intentions to Visit a destination, due to their potential interactions with destination image (Baloglu and McCleary, I999b; Fakeye and Crompton, 1991; Goodrich, 1978; Gunn, 1989; Javalgi, Thomas, and Rao, 1992). Additionally, previous research has shown that travelers may have different motives for international tourism, and these motives influence their visit intention; for example, travelers who seek either familiarity or novelty, associate different levels of risk with intentional tourism (Lepp & Gibson, 2003). Given the as yet unexplained variances in Americans’ intentions to visit the three East Asian countries, especially regarding South Korea, future research would need to consider adding other potential factors to the conceptual model developed in this study, such as perceived behavioral control, or the perceived expenditure of time and money a vacation to East Asia would require. Conclusions The findings from this study demonstrate that American tourists’ intentions to visit China, Japan, and South Korea are influenced by the interrelationships among information source usage, cognitive and affective destination image, and perceived risks. They also show that, while all of these factors have significant influences on Americans’ intentions to vacation in all three countries, different factors have a more direct influence 112 on visit intention for each country (i.e., affective destination image for China, cognitive destination image for Japan, and perceived risks for South Korea). The results of this study have both theoretical and practical value in that they fill gaps in previous tourism research on visit intention, destination image, perceived risks, and information source usage, and they address an understudied American tourist region (East Asia). This study also provides important information for East-Asia-based tourism marketers, information suggesting how these practitioners might invest more effectively in marketing programs to attract first-time American tourists to their countries. Future research, based on this study, should (I) extend this model by incorporating other possible factors that may influence prospective American tourists’ intentions to Visit China, Japan, and South Korea, (2) replicate this study with a probabilistic survey sample, and (3) test this model on other potential destinations for American tourists. 113 APPENDIX 114 Appendix A: Survey Questions Americans’ Reasons for Vacationing in East Asia Dear Sir/ Madam: I am a doctoral student in Michigan State University’s tourism program. The purpose of my study is to develop and test a model explaining Americans’ reasons for vacationing in East Asia. My related survey will provide the empirical basis of my dissertation. Your participation in this survey will take approximately 15 minutes. This study is for research purposes only. Your responses will not be associated with you in any way and will remain strictly confidential. Your identity will not be linked to the data you provide. There are no anticipated risks associated with participation. You consent to voluntarily participate in this study by completing this survey and you may choose not to participate at all or you may refuse to answer certain questions. Please direct any questions about this study to J eonghee Noh at nohieong@msu.edu., phone (517) 333-3662, fax (517) 432-2296. Jeonghee Noh PhD. Candidate Department of Community, Agriculture, Recreation and Resource Studies 172 Natural Resources Building Michigan State University East Lansing, MI 48824-1222 MICHIGAN STATE UNIVERSITY 1.Have you ever visited the countries listed below? If yes, how many times? Please check the appropriate box(es) for each of the countries listed below. o o 6) Country Havilyéou vrsrted ........... Y. 6.8 ..... Number of visits CHINA D ’? timet's) . JAPAN . 1:1 [I ___—time(s) SOUTH KOREA 1:1 115 ., .A_,ti.r_u9(§) 2. Please rate your image of China, Japan, and South Korea according to the following features. Please circle only one number for each country. 1 2 3 4 5 Strongly Somewhat Neither Agree Nor Somewhat Strongly 3. Please express your feelings about each country as a vacation destination. Please circle only one number for each set of opposite adjectives to indicate how you feel. I) CHINA Extremely Svtrflet- Neither 83%? Extremely Unpleasant 1 2 3 4 5 Pleasant Depressing l 2 3 4 5 Em Boring 1 2 3 4 5 Fascinating .msgessing 1 2 3 4 5 ML... 2 JAPAN S S Extremely walling- Neither gflet- Extremely Unpleasant 1 2 3 4 5 Pleasant _ . Depressing 1 2 3 4 5 Energizing Boring 1 2 3 4 5 Fascinating .DigLressing 1 2 3 4 5 Relaxmg' . 3)South Korea Extremely 5‘3]??? Neither S3113;- Extremely __Uppleasant 1 2 3 4 5 Pleasant Depressing 1 2 3 4 5 Energizing. ”Boring 1 2 3 4 5 Fascinating Distressing l 2 3 4 5 Relaxipg 116 4. Please read each statement and indicate your level of agreement or disagreement, for each country using the scale below. Please circle only one number for each country. 1 2 3 4 5 Strongly Somewhat Neither Agree Nor Somewhat Strongly CHINA JAPAN 1234 234512345 34 2345 234512345 3 4 5 W3”? 3 4 withothers 1 2 3 4 5 5. How ofien do you obtain information on the following countries from the sources listed below? Please circle only one response for each statement. 1 2 3 4 5 Never Rarer Sometimes Often Frguently China Japan South Korea TV/ Radio 1 2 3 4 5 l 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 Word of mouth Farnil , Friends, assassin; , ,4 5 .1 3,. .3__._:__3.3,:_3 Books and Movies 1 2 34 ’5 _l 2 3 4 5 l 2 3 4 5 Professional advice Altamlsgnalltentssr Broshltres) 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 Internet 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 Cultural experiences (Art Museums, j Theaters Restaurants, Concert Halls, 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 l 2 3 4 5 WCl;as_s_es, etc.) . I c r r ’” u ' l 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 l 2 3 4 5 6. How interested are you in visiting the following vacation destinations in the next 5 years? Please circle only one number for each country. Completely Somewhat Neither Somewha Strongly Unintereste Unintereste Interested t Interested CHINA 1 2 3 4 5 SOUTH 1 2 3 4 117 7. How likely is it that you will visit the following vacation destinations in the next 5 years? Please circle only one number for each country. Very Somewhat No Somewhat Ve UnlikelL Unlikely Opinion Likely Lik'el'y CHINA 1 2 -3 4 5 1 JAPAN '1 2 ,-. . W. 3 _ .._._.4 5 SOUTH l 2 3 4 5 ABOUT YOU: (These data will be kept in the strictest confidence and used for statistical purpose only). 1. Are you: [3 Female [3 Male 2. What is your age: 3. What was the FINAL level of school completed? (please check one) D Middle/Junior High School [:1 High School El College (Undergraduate) D Master’s Studies E] Doctoral Studies a Post-doctoral Studies 4. What was your approximate annual household income before taxes in 2005? (please check one) [3 Less than $ 20,000 C] $ 60,000 - $ 79,999 D $ 120,000 - $ 139,999 a $ 20,000 - $ 39,999 E] $ 80,000 - $ 99,999 D $ 140,000 or more D $ 40,000 - $ 59,999 D $ 100,000 - $ 1 19,999 5. What is the zip code of your primary residence? 6. Which one of the following best describes you? (please check one) :1 African American/ Black [:1 European American/ White CI Asian American C] Latino/ Latina 1:] Native American! Native Alaskan [:1 Native Hawaiian or Other Pacific Islander :1 Other (Please Specify) 7. Marital Status (Please check one) a Married/Committed relationship 13 Divorced E] Widowed/Widower/Never remarried [:1 Single, never married CI Separated , 8. Please write the number of children (aged 18 & under) living in your home: Thank you so much for your gracious effort in completing this questionnaire 118 REFERENCES I: "_IL-w-' -' .- 119 REFERENCES Ahmed, Z. U. (1991). The influence of the components of a state's tourist image on product positioning strategy. Tourism Management, 12(4), 331-340. Baloglu, S. (1999). A path analytic model of visitation intention involving information sources, socio-psychological motivations, and destination image. Journal of Travel & Tourism Marketing, 8(3), 81-90. Baloglu, S. (2001). Image variations of Turkey by familiarity index: informational and experiential dimensions. Tourism Management, 22(2), 127-133. Baloglu, S., & Brinberg, D. (1997). Affective images of tourism destinations. Journal of Travel Research, 35(4), 11-15. Baloglu, S., & McCleary, K. W. (1999a). A model of destination image formation. Annals of Tourism Research, 26(4), 868-897. Baloglu, S., & McCleary, K. W. (1999b). U.S. international pleasure travelers' images of four mediterranean destinations: A comparison of visitors and nonvisitors. Journal of Travel Research, 38(2), 144-152. Basala, S. L., & Klenosky, D. B. (2001). Travel-style preferences for visiting a novel destination: A conjoint investigation across the novelty-familiarity continuum. Journal of Travel Research, 40(2), 172-182. Beerli, A., & Martin, J. D. (2004a). Factors influencing destination image. Annals of Tourism Research, 31(3), 657-681. Beerli, A., & Martin, J. D. (2004b). Tourists' characteristics and the perceived image of tourist destinations: a quantitative analysis - a case study of Lanzarote, Spain. Tourism Management, 25(5), 623-636. Bramwell, B., & Rawding, L. (1996). Tourism marketing images of industrial cities. Annals of Tourism Research, 23(1), 201-221. Brown, T. J ., Mowen, J. C., Donavan, D. T., & Licata, J. W. (2002). The customer orientation of service workers: Personality trait effects on self and supervisor performance ratings. Journal of Marketing Research, 39(1), 110-119. Burnkrant, R., & Thomas, P. (1982). An examination of the convergent, discriminant, and predictive validity of Fishbein's behavioral intention model. Journal of Marketing Research, 19(4), 550-561. 120 Byme, B. M. (2001). Structural Equation Modeling with AMOS: Basic Concepts, Applications, and Programming. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Chen, J. S. (2000). A comparison of information usage between business and leisure travelers. Journal of Hospitality & Leisure Marketing, 7(2), 65-76. Choi, W. M., Chan, A., & Wu, J. (1999). A qualitative and quantitative assessment of Hong Kong's image as a tourist destination. Tourism Management, 20(3), 361-365. Chon, K.-S. (1991). Tourism destination image modification process: Marketing implications. Tourism Management, 12(1), 68-72. Chon, K.-S., & Olsen, M. D. (1991). Functional and symbolic congruity approaches to '- consumer satisfaction/dissatisfaction in tourism. The Journal of the International r Academy of Hospitality Research, 3, 2-23. Chon, K. S., Weaver, P. A., & Kim, C. Y. (1991). Marketing your community: Image .- analysis in Norfolk. Cornell Hotel and Restaurant Administration Quarterly, '. s 31(4), 31. Coshall, J. T. (2003). The threat of terrorism as an intervention in international travel flows. Journal of Travel Research, 42(1), 4-12. Court, B., & Lupton, R. A. (1997). Customer portfolio development: Modeling destination adopters, inactives, and rejecters. Journal of Travel Research, 36(1), 35-43. Crompton, J. L. (1979). An assessment of the image of Mexico as a vacation destination and the influence of geographical location upon that image. Journal of Travel Research, 1 7(Spring), 18-23. Crompton, J. L., Fakeye, P. C., & Lue, C.-C. (1992). Positioning: The example of the we lower Rio Grande Valley in the winter long stay destination market. Journal of Travel Research, 31(2), 20-26. Dann, G. M. S. (1996). Tourists images of a destination: An alternative analysis. Journal ' ___ of Travel & Tourism Marketing, 5(1/2), 41-55. Dolnicar, S. (2005). Understanding barriers to leisure travel: Tourist fears as a marketing basis. Journal of Vacation Marketing, 11(3), 197-208. Echtner, C. M., & Ritchie, J. R. B. (1993). The Measurement of Destination Image: An Empirical Assessment. Journal of Travel Research, 3 1 (Spring), 3-13. F akeye, P. C., & Crompton, J. L. (1991). Image differences between prospective, first- time, and repeat visitors to the lower Rio Grande Valley. Journal of Travel Research, 30(2), 10-16. 121 Faulkner, B. (2001). Towards a framework for tourism disaster management. Tourism Management, 22(2), 13 5-147. Floyd, M. F., Gibson, H., Pennington-Gray, L., & Thapa, B. (2003). The effect of risk perceptions on intentions to travel in the aftermath of September 11, 2001. Journal of Travel & Tourism Marketing, 15(2/3), 19-38. Fodness, D., & Murray, B. (1997). Tourist information search. Annals of Tourism Research, 24(3), 503-523. Fodness, D., & Murray, B. (1998). A typology of tourist information search strategies. Journal of Travel Research, 37(2), 108-119. Fodness, D., & Murray, B. (1999). A model of tourist information search behavior. Journal of Travel Research, 3 7(3), 220-230. Fridgen, J. D. (1987). Use of cognitive maps to determine perceived tourism regions. Leisure Sciences, 9, 101-117. Garson, G. D. (2006). Structural Equation Modeling Example Using WinA MOS, 2006.08.01. Retrieved from : http://www2.chass.ncsu.edu/garson/PA765/semAMOSl .htm Garter, W. C. (1993). Image formation process. Journal of Travel & Tourism Marketing, 2(2/3), 191-215. Gartner, W. C., & Hunt, J. D. (1987). An Analysis of State Image Change Over 3 Twelve- Year Period (1971-1983). Journal of Travel Research, 26(2), 15-19. Gartner, W. C., & Shen, H. (1992). The impact of Tiananmen Square on China's tourism image. Journal of Travel Research, 30(4), 47-52. Gemuden, H. G. (1985). Perceived risk and information search: A systematic meta- analysis of empirical evidence. International Journal of Research in Marketing, 2, 79 -100. Goodrich, J. N. (1978). A new approach to image analysis through multidimensional scaling. Journal of Travel Research, 16(3), 3-7. Gunn, C. A. (1972). Vacationscape; designing tourist regions. Austin: Bureau of Business Research, University of Texas at Austin. Gursoy, D. (2003). Prior product knowledge and its influence on the traveler's information search behavior. Journal of Hospitality and Leisure Marketing, 10(3/4), 113-131. 122 Hair, J. F., Anderson, R. E., Tatham, R. L., & Black, W. C. (1998). Multivariate data analysis (Fiflh ed.). New Jersey: Prentice Hall. Hanlan, J ., & Kelly, S. (2005). Image formation, information sources and an iconic Australian tourist destination. Journal of Vacation Marketing, 11(2), 163-177. Hsu, C. H. C., Wolfe, K., & Kang, S. K. (2004). Image assessment for a destination with limited comparative advantages. Tourism Management, 25(1), 121-126. Hunt, J. D. (1975). Image as a factor in tourism development. Journal of Travel Research, 13, 1-7. Javalgi, R. G., Thomas, E. G., & Rao, S. R. (1992). US pleasure travelers' perceptions of selected European destinations. European Journal of Marketing, 26(7), 45-64. Joppe, M., Martin, D. W., & Waalen, J. (2001). Toronto's image as a destination: A comparative importance-satisfaction analysis by origin of visitor. Journal of Travel Research, 39(3), 252-260. Joreskog, K. G., & Sorbom, D. (1993). LIRSEL 8: Structural equation modeling with the SIMPLIS command language. Chicago: Scientific Software International. Kim, S. S., & Morrsion, A. M. (2005). Change of images of South Korea among foreign tourists afier the 2002 F IFA World Cup. Tourism Management, 26(2), 233-247. Kline, R. B. (1998). Principles and Practice of Structural Equation Modeling. New York: Guilford. Korea Tourism Organization (KTO) (2004). Annual Report 2003, Seoul, KTO. Lam, T., & Hsu, C. H. C. (2006). Predicting behavioral intention of choosing a travel destination. Tourism Management, 27(4), 589-599. h Lang, T. C. (2000). The effect of the Internet on travel consumer purchasing behavior implications for travel agents. Journal of Vacation Marketing, 6(4), 368-385. Lepp, A., & Gibson, H. (2003). Tourist roles, perceived risk and international tourism. I Annals of Tourism Research, 30(3), 606-624. Litvin, S. W., & Ling, S. N. S. (2001). The destination attribute management model: an empirical application to Bintan, Indonesia. Tourism Management, 22(5), 481-492. Lo, A., Cheung, C., & Law, R. (2004). Information search behavior of Mainland Chinese air travelers to Hong Kong. Journal of Travel & Tourism Marketing, 16(1), 43-51 . 123 Maser, B., & Weiermair, K. (1998). Travel decision-making: From the vantage point of perceived risk and information preferences. Journal of Travel & Tourism Marketing, 7(4), 107-121. Mazzocchi, M., & Montini, A. (2001). Earthquake effects on tourism in central Italy. Annals of Tourism Research, 28(4), 1031-1046. Mihnan, A., & Pizam, A. (1995). The role of awareness and familiarity with destination: The Central Florida case. Journal of Travel Research, 33(3), 21-27. Money, R. B., & Crotts, J. C. (2003). The effect of uncertainty avoidance on information search, planning, and purchases of international travel vacations. Tourism Management, 24(2), 191-202. Phelps, A. (1986). Holiday destination image-the problem of assessment: An example developed in Menorca. Tourism Management, 7(3), 168-180. Pike, S. (2002). Destination image analysis - A review of 142 papers from 1973 to 2000. Tourism Management, 23(5), 541-549. Pike, 8., & Ryan, C. (2004). Destination positioning analysis through a comparison of cognitive, affective, and conative perceptions. Journal of Travel Research, 42(4), 333-342. Pizam, A., Jafari, J ., & Milman, A. (1991). Influence of tourism on attitudes: US students visiting USSR. Tourism Management, 12(1), 47-54. Reilly, M. D. (1990). Free elicitation of descriptive adjectives for tourism image assessment. Journal of Travel Research, 28(4), 21-26. Richter, L. K. (2003). International tourism and its global public health consequences. Journal of Travel Research, 41(4), 340-347. Rittichainuwat, B. N., Qu, H., & Brown, T. J. (2001). Thailand's international travel image. Cornell Hotel and Restaurant Administration Quarterly, 42(22), 82-95. Roehl, W. S., & Fesenmaier, D. R. (1992). Risk perception and pleasure travel: An exploratory study. Journal of Travel Research, 30(4), 17-26. Ross, G. F. (1993). Destination evaluation and vacation preferences. Annals of Tourism Research, 20, 477-489. Russell, J.A., Ward, L.M., & Pratt, G. (1981). Affective quality attributed to environments: A factor analytic study. Environment and Behavior, 13(3), 259-288. 124 Snepenger, D. J ., & Snepenger, M. (1993). Information search by pleasure travelers. In Mahmood A. Khan, M. D. Olsen & T. Var (Eds), VNR’s encyclopedia of Hospitality and Tourism (pp. 830-835). New York: Van Nostrand Reinhold. Sonmez, S. F ., & Graefe, A. R. (1998a). Determining future travel behavior from past travel experience and perceptions of risk and safety. Journal of Travel Research, 37(2), 171-177. Sonmez, S. F., & Graefe, A. R. (1998b). Influence of terrorism risk on foreign tourism decisions. Annals of Tourism Research, 25(1), 112-144. Stern, E., & Krakover, S. (1993). The formation of a composite urban image. Geographical Analysis, 25(2), 130- l 46. Suh, Y. K., & Gartner, W. C. (2004). Perceptions in international urban tourism: An analysis of travelers to Seoul, Korea. Journal of Travel Research, 43(1), 39-45. Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell., L. S. (2001). Using multivariate statistics (4th ed.). Boston: Allyan and Bacon. Travel Industry Association of America (TIA) (2005). 2005 International Outlook for Travel and Tourism. Washington, DC: TIA. Um, S., & Crompton, J. L. (1992). The roles of perceived inhibitors and facilitators in pleasure travel destination decisions. Journal of Travel Research, 30(3), 18-25. Vogt, C. A., & Fesenmaier, D. R. (1998). Expanding the functional information search model. Annals of Tourism Research. 25(3). 551-578. Vogt, C. A., Andereck, KL. (2003). Destination perceptions across a vacation. Journal of Travel Research, 41, 348-354. Woodside, A. G., & Lysonski, S. (1989). A general model of traveler destination choice. Journal of Travel Research, 27(4), 8-14. World Tourism Organization (WTO) (2006). Tourism 2020 Vision. Retrieved from: http: //www. world- tourism. org/facts/menu. html. Yamamoto, D., & Gill, A. M. (1999). Emerging trends in Japanese package tourism. Journal of Travel Research, 38(2), 134-143. 125 G 11111111 93 8 1111111