m...“ L... 2007 This is to certify that the thesis entitled EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN UNCERTAINTY, FACE, AND COMMITMENT TALK IN COLLEGE DATING RELATIONSHIPS presented by ROSALIE BETH SHEMANSKI has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for the MA. degree in Communication KW MW Majo(.F)bfessor’s Signature I o , l 3 . o (0 Date MSU is an Affirmative Action/Equal Opportunity Institution LIBRARY Michigan State University PLACE IN RETURN BOX to remove this checkout from your record. To AVOID FINES return on or before date due. MAY BE RECALLED with earlier due date if requested. DATE DUE DATE DUE DATE DUE 2/05 p:lClRC/DateDue.indd-pi1 EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN UNCERTAINTY, FACE, AND COMMITMENT TALK IN COLLEGE DATING RELATIONSHIPS By Rosalie Beth Shemanski A THESIS Submitted to Michigan State University In partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of MASTER OF ARTS Department of Communication 2006 ABSTRACT EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN UNCERTAINTY, FACE, AND COMMITMENT TALK IN COLLEGE DATING RELATIONSHIPS By Rosalie Beth Shemanski It is important to understand why partners choose to discuss or not discuss commitment in order to fully understand how commitment talk influences level of commitment, duration of relationship, and certainty of relationship and self. Participants (N=190) were asked to recall and write down their most memorable commitment conversation with their current or most recent romantic partner. Participants were then asked to discuss what triggered the commitment discussions and what each partner did in order to protect the face of their partner and themselves. Likert type scales were also used to determine self and partner uncertainty, concern for self and partner’s face, commitment level, and level of commitment demonstrated. The results suggest that both face and uncertainty play a role in determining if and how commitment will be discussed between romantic partners. DEDICATION I want to dedicate this to all my friends in the Communication Department at Michigan State University. I never would have known where to start or what to do without you. I would also like to dedicate this to my advisor Dr. Kelly Morrison and Stephen Aldrich for their guidance and support. iii ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS I would like to thank the following people for their advice, support. and encouragement throughout my entire Master’s experience. First, thank you to my advisor Dr. Kelly Morrison for challenging me and helping to me to achieve my goals. Also, thanks to Dr. Minda Orina and Dr. M. Brent Donnellen for their thoughtful comments, advice, and feedback. I would like to thank Catherine Newman for all of her time and effort in helping to analyze the data. iv TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES ............................................. vi INTRODUCTION .............................................. 1 CHAPTER 1 LITERATURE REVIEW ........................................ . 7 Commitment Talk ........................................ 7 Uncertainty ............................................. 9 Face ................................................. 12 CHAPTER 2 METHODOLOGY ............................................ 17 Participants ............................................ 17 Instrumentation ......................................... 1 8 Coding of Open-ended Responses .......................... 19 Categories from the Open-ended Response ................... 20 Closed-ended Measures ................................. 25 CHAPTER 3 RESULTS .................................................. 27 Descriptive Statistics .................................... 27 Scale Correlations ...................................... 27 Discussion of Commitment ................................ 28 Uncertainty ............................................ 30 Face ................................................. 31 Post Hoc Analysis ...................................... 32 CHAPTER 4 DISCUSSION ............................................... 34 CHAPTER 5 CONCONCLUSION .......................................... 40 Notes ................................................ 40 REFERENCES ............................................... 41 Table 1. Table 2. Table 3. Table 4. Table 5. Table 6. Table 7. Table 8. Table 9. LIST OF TABLES Examples of Positive and Negative Face Threats ............... 16 Topics of Memorable Commitment Conversations .............. 21 Triggers of Commitment Conversations ...................... 23 Avoid Embarrassment to Self or Partner ..................... 24 Reasons for Not Discussing Commitment .................... 25 Descriptive Statistics for Closed-Ended Scales ................ 27 Scale Correlations ....................................... 28 Correlations Between Uncertainty and Topics of Commitment Talk. 31 Correlations Between Face and Topics of Commitment Talk ...... 31 vi Exploring the relationship between uncertainty, face, and commitment talk in college dating relationships. One goal of studying communication between romantic partners is to understand how communication affects relationship satisfaction and longevity. Commitment and how relational partners communicate about it is a factor influencing satisfaction and longevity. Commitment represents a long-tenn orientation toward a partner with the desire to maintain a relationship. The concept of commitment also entails strong feelings of attachment (Rusbult & Buunk, 1993). Commitment long has been recognized as a significant factor in the development and continued stability of close personal relationships (Adams & Jones, 1997), and scholars agree that commitment to a relationship is strongly related to whether the relationship lasts or dissolves (Arriaga & Agnew, 2001). Commitment talk is the content messages that reference the state of the relationship (Emmers & Canary, 1996). Engaging in commitment talk allows a dyad to discuss and address where their current relationship stands and future plans for the relationship. Yet, according to Knapp and Taylor (1994), direct face-to-face discussion of commitment between partners does not often occur. Little information exists regarding the extent to which partners, and college students in particular, discuss commitment, how they discuss it, or why they avoid discussion of it. Given that many college dating relationships may lead to marriage, it is vital to explore this aspect of relational communication. One potential explanation for this lack of face-to-face commitment talk is that romantic partners may feel uncertain about their relationship. This uncertainty may be enhanced by a desire to avoid embarrassment of themselves or their partners by broaching the topic of commitment. In other words, people may be uncertain about commitment talk because they are worried about threatening their own or their partner’s face. Baxter and Wilmot (1985) suggested that commitment talk can potentially threaten the face of both partners. Thus, in order to improve our understanding of commitment talk the concepts of uncertainty and face must be explored. The goal of this research is to examine how face and uncertainty influence commitment talk in college dating relationships. I begin by reviewing the literature on commitment and commitment talk. Conceptualizations of Commitment Commitment in romantic relationships has been studied in a number of different ways and several scholars have forwarded theories, models, and typologies of commitment, including: Kelley and Thibaut’s interdependence theory (1978), Rusbult’s investment model (1983), and Johnson’s (1973) types of commitment. In addition, the psychological processes related to commitment and the levels of commitment also have been explored (Johnson & Rusbult, 1989; Martz et al., 1998; Miller, 1997; Murray & Holmes, 1993). Theories and Models of Commitment One of the first approaches used to study commitment has come from interdependence theory (Kelley & Thibaut, 1978). Interdependence theory is built on two constructs, (a) comparison level and (b) comparison level of alternatives. The comparison level (CL) is defined as the standard against which individuals judge what they are currently receiving from a relationship. Thus, CL can be thought of as an individual’s expectations of profit from the relationship as well as an indicator of satisfaction with the relationship. Interdependence theory suggests that comparison of the current relationship with the CL determines if an individual is satisfied with their relationship. For example, if current outcomes in the relationship exceed CL, the individual should be satisfied with the relationship. If current outcomes fall below CL an individual should not be satisfied. The second construct is comparison level of alternatives (CLaIt), defined as the goodness of outcomes available in alternatives to the relationship. CLalt is the standard an individual uses to decide whether to remain in the relationship or dissolve the relationship. For example, if an individual believes that her current relationship provides more rewards than those that would be available in an alternative, then the individual will remain in the relationship. If current outcomes fall below CLalt then the individual may decide to terminate it. Relationship stability is distinguished from relationship satisfaction in the interdependence theory. This theory posits that an individual may be satisfied with a relationship but not dependent on it. Therefore, CLaIt, not relationship satisfaction itself, determines relationship stability. For example, an unsatisfied individual may remain in the relationship because the individual does not anticipate receiving greater outcomes from alternatives. The theory also suggests that a satisfied individual may leave a relationship if CLalt is higher than the current relationship. The basic assumption of interdependence theory is that one stays in a relationship only as long as one’s outcomes exceed the outcomes in the best available alternative to the relationship (Dindia & Canary, 1993). Rusbult’s (1983) investment model builds on interdependence theory and posited that people will feel committed to their relationships to the extent that they feel satisfied, believe they have few good alternatives to the relationship, and have invested important resources in the relationship. The investment model supports the idea that an individual’s decision to maintain the relationship is a direct function of the individual’s commitment to the relationship. Satisfaction level is determined by comparing the current relationship with an individual’s CL (what an individual thinks he/she deserves or can reasonably expect). Investments may include emotional energy, time, money, or anything else the individual places value on. The more investments attached to a relationship the more difficult it becomes to dissolve it because the costs become too high. The quality of alternative partners is determined by judging the current relationship against other possible available partners (CLaIt). Support for the investment model can be found in a study linking commitment and willingness to sacrifice in ongoing close relationships (Van Lange, Agnew, Harinck, & Steemers, 1997). Results were consistent with the investment model as the findings indicated that high levels of satisfaction, perceived low levels of relationship alternatives, and large investment levels (equaling high levels of commitment) were associated with greater willingness to sacnfice. Rusbult’s (1983) investment model was again supported when commitment and stability were tested in a study showing the model successfully predicted relationship duration over a 15-year period (Bui, Peplau, & Hill, 1996). Research using the investment model has shown that one’s commitment level is positively correlated with one’s satisfaction with the relationship and the amount invested into the relationship, and negatively correlated with the perceived quality of one’s alternatives (Rusbult & Buunk, 1993). Typologies of Commitment Johnson (1999) described commitment by delineating three types; structural, moral, and personal. Johnson theorized that the three types of commitment combine together to influence an individual’s decision to continue a relationship. Structural commitment is any structural barrier to leaving the relationship, including financial dependence, or harsh divorce laws that may keep an individual in a relationship. In order to survive one feels they “Have To” remain in their current relationship. Moral commitment stems from a sense of obligation and may include religious beliefs, family pressure/tradition, or the culture in which one lives. This is considered the “Ought To” reason to remain in the relationship. Personal commitment is the “Want To” reason or the desire to stay in the relationship and is based on personal choice. Another typology of commitment was fonivarded by Weigel and Ballard- Reisch (2002) who identified two categories of behavior that directly identify one’s level of commitment to a partner. The first category is expressing commitment, defined as directly telling one’s partner of one’s commitment and intention to remain in the relationship. The second category is creating a relational future, which is accomplished by communicating commitment in a more indirect manner, such as celebrating milestones or planning for a future together. Results from their study suggested that people use a combination of indirect and direct behaviors to communicate the importance of the relationship as well as commitment level to one’s partner. The sample for this study consisted of half students and half participants in community education extension classes. The mean age of participants was 29. Psychological Processes Related to Commitment Positive illusions, derogation of alternative partners, and relationship superiority bias have been found to influence the strength of commitment to one’s partner. Positive illusions involve filtering out negative information, comparing the current relationship to less well-off relationships, and weighting partner’s positive traits more heavily. Positive illusions have been found to help individuals deal with doubts and fears regarding their relationship. In addition, Martz et al. (1998) found that the level of commitment significantly predicted how positive an individual’s evaluation of a current romantic relationship was, as well as the individual’s optimism regarding the future of the relationship. Derogation of alternative partners is illustrated when people are satisfied with their current romantic relationship and avoid attractive alternatives or reduce interest in seeking information about possible attractive alternative partners. Three studies conducted by Johnson and Rusbult (1989) found a strong link between highly committed individuals and the tendency to negatively evaluate relationship alternatives. Their results indicated the higher the relationship threat, and the higher the attractiveness, temptation, and availability of an alternative, the more likely individuals were to devalue alternatives. For example, if one finds another person attractive one may make the alternative seem less desirable by deciding that the alternative is really not in that great of shape or has a bad attitude. A high level of commitment also may cause an individual to be inattentive to their alternatives altogether. Miller (1997) found that commitment was related to attentiveness to alternatives, such that the more attentive to alternatives an individual was, the more one actively sought information about their other options, and the less committed they were to partners. Relationship superiority bias is the tendency for people to evaluate their relationship more positively than the current relationship warrants. This serves to maintain the commitment level and may help people cope with relationship challenges and doubts (Murray & Holmes, 1993). Research has supported the claim that strong commitment to a relationship increases a rose-colored, relationship-enhancing view (Agnew, Van Lange, Rusbult, & Langston, 1998). Commitment Talk One question that often is ignored in commitment research is how couples actually communicate commitment. Knapp and Taylor (1994) identified three forms of commitment talk: (a) indirect - where the messages implied a sense of commitment but did not specifically mention it, (b) partner to third party — one partner overhears the other talking about their relationship to a third party, and (c) direct focus on commitment but indirectly referencing another person or couple (a combination of indirect and direct commitment talk). For example, if Steve and Katie discuss another couple focusing on the subject of commitment, the discussion may be very informative of how Steve and Katie themselves view commitment. Direct commitment talk does occur, but it is rare because it generally occurs when possible extreme changes in the relationship are anticipated. When the relationship has the potential to change dramatically, high emotions of anger or excitement, depending on the change, most likely will be involved (Knapp & Taylor, 1994). Individuals may choose to discuss commitment with their partner for various reasons including to express affection, to determine relationship exclusivity, to understand sacrifices made for the relationship, and to discuss the future of the relationship (Knapp & Taylor, 1994). Additionally, how commitment is communicated also may be a cause for discussion. It commitment is expressed at an inappropriate time for example, this action may initiate discussion of commitment. Therefore, a variety of reasons exist for people choosing to discuss commitment, which leads to the first two research questions: RQ1: What reasons do college students report for discussing or not discussing relational commitment? R0 2: What events tn'gger their discussion of commitment? While discussing commitment with one’s partner, the issues of quality (how it is discussed) and quantity (how often it is discussed) are relevant, as both have been shown to be positively associated with successful relationships. Emmers-Sommer’s (2004) results suggest that the quality and quantity of the communication is important to relational intimacy and relational satisfaction. Her results also suggest that it may be beneficial to speak of commitment (or any relationship matter) in great depth, often, and in a controlled manner (i.e., manage the production of coherent conversations and coordinate verbal and nonverbal behavior). If the quality and quantity of communication play a role to the success of a relationship, why then do so many couples ignore discussing their specific expectations regarding commitment? The answer may be found in the literature on uncertainty. Uncertainty Uncertainty occurs when an outcome is unknown, such as how an individual will respond in an initial encounter. Berger and Calabrese’s Uncertainty Reduction Theory (URT, 1975) detailed the process of interactions among strangers and considered ways in which individuals attempt to reduce cognitive uncertainty and increase predictability. Uncertainty Reduction Theory Berger and Calabrese (1975) suggested that in initial meetings people try to reduce anxiety and stress by making sense of interpersonal interactions. Uncertainty is an individual’s lack of confidence to predict another’s behavior in an interpersonal interaction. Reduction of anxiety and stress is accomplished by reducing uncertainty about the self, the partner, and the relationship. A central assumption of URT is that when individuals interact their main concern is to successfully predict the behavior of the other. This can be accomplished in two ways: (a) proactive processes - the development of predictions about the other’s behavior before the other acts, and (b) retroactive processes — the explanation of a behavior after it has occurred. Berger and Calabrese (1975) developed seven axioms to explain how uncertainty influences communication between two strangers. The axioms that have particular relevance to the current study include; Axiom 1: As the amount of verbal communication between strangers increases, the level of uncertainty for each person in the relationship will decrease; Axiom 3: High levels of uncertainty cause increases in information-seeking behavior, as uncertainty levels decline, information seeking behavior decreases; and Axiom 4: High levels of uncertainty in a relationship cause decreases in the intimacy level of communication content. Low levels of uncertainty produce high levels of intimacy. Berger and Calabrese (1975) suggested that, given high levels of uncertainty, the amount of verbal communication in an interaction will increase and the level of uncertainty will decrease. One way to reduce uncertainty is to discuss the topic that is causing the uncertainty with the other individual(s) involved. Although URT traditionally has been used to study relationships in their initial stages, research by Solomon and Knobloch (2001; 2004; Knobloch & Solomon, 1999; 2002; 2005) has extended this original conceptualization to more intimate relationships. Knobloch and Solomon (2002) have further specified the 10 concept of uncertainty to include dimensions of self uncertainty, which is when one doubts their own involvement in the relationship, and relational uncertainty, which includes the doubts people have about the relationship. Both of these concepts are relevant to commitment talk because topic avoidance is particularly prominent when the individual wants to avoid judgment or embarrassment (self- protection), and avoid conflict or relationship destruction (relationship protection). Thus, evidence suggests that uncertainty corresponds with a lack of communication about high risk issues, most likely because individuals prefer to maintain ambiguity rather than confront the risks involved in direct communication (Aflfi & Burgoon, 1998). These findings imply that uncertainty does not always produce information-seeking behaviors and, instead, occasionally leads to a desire to maintain the elevated uncertainty levels. Knobloch and Solomon’s (2002) research has found that individuals in a relationship with high uncertainty will not be likely to engage in information- seeking behaviors. Furthermore, they have found that relational uncertainty appears to be especially persistent beyond the initial stages of relationship development (Solomon & Knobloch, 2001). Relational uncertainty can make interactions with a partner more difficult, diminish dyadic well-being (Knobloch & Solomon, 2005), intensify people’s reactions to relationship events (Solomon & Knobloch, 2004), and enhance doubt about the status of the relationship itself. However, relational uncertainty is not all negative, it can add excitement, romance (Berger & Bradac, 1982), spontaneity, or novelty (Brashers, 2001) to the relationship, which may ll help to explain why some individuals do not discuss commitment in specific relationship terms. Taken as a whole, Knobloch and Solomon’s work suggests that an individual in a relationship with high self or relational uncertainty will not be likely to engage in information-seeking behaviors. Although their work has specified the concept of uncertainty into two distinct dimensions (i.e., self and relational), their findings conflict with Berger and Calabrese’s (1975) notion that high uncertainty will increase information-seeking behavior. Therefore the following research questions are posited: R03: How does self uncertainty influence commitment talk? RQ4: How does relational uncertainty influence commitment talk? One explanation for why individuals would avoid commitment talk is that they may sometimes prefer not knowing information rather than finding out negative information (Afifi & Burgoon, 1998). Individuals also may prefer to remain uncertain rather than cause embarrassment to themselves or their partner. This is further explored in the literature on face. Face The concept of face was discussed first by Goffman (1959) as patterns of interactions, both verbal and nonverbal, in which evaluations are made of self and others. Goffman (1959) defined face as the self presentation of identity in interaction, and it depends on the self, the situation, and others involved in the interaction. 12 Face is something that can be threatened by how the speaker or the person being addressed behaves or responds. For example, discussing certain issues, such as commitment, may threaten one or both parties” face depending on how each individual behaves during the interaction. As relationships become closer, each partner’s face becomes inextricably bound in the shared relational identity, and when face is threatened, one or both partners experience anxiety and discomfort (Cupach & Carson, 2002). There are advantages and disadvantages related to each partner’s face when discussing commitment. Discussion about commitment can promote togetherness and closeness. However, in any discussion of commitment there is also the prospect of humiliating individuals and damaging relationships. Many individuals view talking about sensitive topics, such as commitment, as more threatening under conditions of relational uncertainty (Knobloch & Carpenter- Theune, 2004). Research by Bello and Edwards (2005) examined face concerns and the influence of various forms of equivocation. Equivocation was defined as the use of ambiguity or vagueness and is used to protect face when an individual seeks to avoid a hurtful truth or a deception. Their findings suggested that clarity is an important concern for communicators and that many individuals may value honesty more than politeness. However, others have found that the fear of threatening self or the relationship motivates some to avoid topics (Afifi & Burgoon, 1998). 13 Politeness Brown and Levinson (1987) furthered the concept of face with their work on politeness by distinguishing between two types of face: (a) negative face - the basic claim to territories, personal preserves, rights to non-distraction and (b) positive face - the positive consistent self-image claimed by each member in the relationship. Communication that threatens relationship issues may be hazardous to a relationship along two distinct lines; (a) individuals expect that such communication will damage their self image; (b) individuals also believe that such communication will jeopardize the well-being of their relationship. Brown and Levinson’s work (1987) suggests that the face of each person depends on the face of everyone else being maintained, and in general, it is in the best interest of everyone involved to maintain the face of one another. Therefore, any discussions of commitment potentially threaten the face of the other and setf. Face Threatening Acts Fear of face threats may be one explanation for why commitment is not often discussed between partners in the earlier stages of a romantic relationship. A Face Threatening Act (FTA) is an act that threatens the positive or negative face of either the speaker or the individual being addressed and may vary in magnitude (Goldsmith & MacGeorge, 2000). Acts that primarily threaten the addressee’s negative face include those acts that predicate some future act of the addressee, and in so doing put some pressure on the addressee to do (or refrain from doing) the act. An example of this would be if Katie requested that 14 Steve only smoke on the porch. Acts that predicate some positive future act of the speaker toward the addressee, and in so doing put some pressure on the addressee to accept or reject them, and possibly to incur a debt are also considered acts that may threaten the addressee’s negative face. For example, if Steve offered to help Katie move, Katie may feel that if she agrees to accept his offer she would then owe him a favor in the future. Those acts that primarily threaten the addressee’s positive face are those that show the speaker has a negative evaluation of some aspect of the addressee's self-image. For example, if Steve expresses disapproval of Katie’s loud and gregarious personality by saying, “Do you always have to be so noisy and obnoxious?” Katie’s positive face would be threatened. Threats that directly damage the speaker's positive face are those acts that involve damaging one’s own self-image. For example, if an individual trips and falls down a set of stairs in front of someone he or she holds in high regard, this would be a threat to the speaker’s positive face. Threats that primarily threaten the speaker's negative face would include any hazard to the individual’s basic claim to freedom of action and freedom from imposition. An example of this would be if Steve told Katie that he would go to church with her on Sunday, even though he did not want to. See Table 1 for examples of both positive and negative face threats to the speaker and hearer. Some overlap in the classification of FTAs does exist because some FTAs fundamentally threaten both negative and positive face. Examples of FTAs that may threaten both negative and positive face are complaints, interruptions, 15 threats, strong expression of emotion, and requests for personal information (Brown & Levinson, 1987). Table 1. Examples of Positive and Negative Face Threats Negative Positive Acts that primarily threaten the Acts that primarily threaten the Addressee's negative face. Addressee’s positive face. Hearer Threats Expressions of Disapproval Warnings Disagreements Suggestions Challenges Advice Mention of Taboo Topics Acts that primarily threaten the Speaker's Acts that primarily threaten the negative face. Speaker's positive face. Speaker Excuses Self-Humiliation Expressing Thanks Acting Stupid Acceptance of Offers Confessions Future Unwanted Commitments Admissions of Guilt A positive face threat has been found to be more strongly associated with lingering damage to the relationship than a negative face threat, as indicated in a study conducted by Cupach and Carson (2002). This signifies that FTAs to an individual’s own desire to be liked and accepted can be very damaging to the relationship. Consequently, discussions of commitment may simultaneously threaten both positive and negative face, and ultimately damage the relationship. Specifically, the person initiating the conversation about relational commitment may threaten their own positive face with concern for self-humiliation. The mention of the taboo topic of commitment may threaten the positive face of the listener. The speaker, due to feeling humiliated by bringing up the subject of relational commitment, may offer excuses to the listener, thus threatening the 16 negative face of the speaker. Therefore, the following research questions are posited: R0 5: How does concern for own face influence commitment talk? R0 6: How does concern for partner’s face influence commitment talk? Research supports the idea that it may be beneficial and insightful to further research the effect of face in relationships. According to Afifi and Metts (1998), “results from their study suggested that politeness theory (Brown & Levinson, 1987) may provide a good theoretical framework for future research efforts in this area.” (p. 386). Method Participants Participants (N = 190) were drawn from upper level undergraduate and graduate communication (N = 72, 37.9%), geography (N = 52, 27.4 %), and nursing (N = 66, 34.7%) students at a large mid-westem university. Participation was voluntary and took place in a classroom setting on campus. Fifty four of the participants were male (28.4%) and 136 (71.6%) were female. The participants ranged in age from 19 to 39 (M = 22.17, SD = 1.83), with one participant not completing this question. The sample was 82.6% Caucasian, 6.3% African American, 4.7% Asian American, 1.6% Hispanic, .5% Pacific Islander, 2.6% mixed, and 1.6% of the sample did not denote their race. Of the sample 97.4% indicated that they were heterosexual, 1.6% homosexual, and .5% bisexual. The average length of time participants had been with their romantic partner was 17 25 months (2.08 years), SD = 23.3 months (1.94 years), and the most frequently reported length of time was 6 months. To assure that the three departments tested (communication, geography, and nursing) were not significantly different, means and standard deviations were compared across all scales. There were no significant differences, thus the three departments were combined into one large group for the remaining analyses. Independent and Dependent Variables The independent variables are self and relational uncertainty, and concern for self and other’s face. The dependent variable is commitment talk. Instrumentation The data were collected using a self-report survey; The Relationship Questionnaire (see Appendix A) included both Likert-type items and open-ended questions. Concern for self face and partner’s face was assessed using Oetzel and Ting-Toomey’s (2003) face concerns scale. Self and relational uncertainty were measured by Knobloch and Solomon’s (1999) uncertainty scales, with lower values indicating greater degrees of self and relational uncertainty. Previous research has shown that this scale has high reliability scores of ac =.80 or higher (Knobloch & Solomon, 1999). Commitment level was measured by commitment stage and the amount of time invested in the relationship. To measure how committed an individual islwas to their current or most recent relationship the Rusbult, Martz, and Agnew’s (1998) Commitment Scale was used. Expression of commitment and 18 creation of relational milestones were assessed according to Weigel and Ballard- Reisch’s (2002) Behavioral Indicators of Commitment instrument. The open—ended questions asked participants to recall and describe their most memorable conversation regarding commitment in the last calendar year. Participants also were asked to describe the reasons and events that triggered discussion of commitment. If they had not experienced a conversation pertaining to commitment in the last year with a romantic partner they were asked to give reasons why they had not discussed it. Only 30% (N = 56) of the recalled conversations actually described a word for word conversation. In contrast, over half, 55% (N = 104) of the recalled “conversations” were summaries of what had been talked about. Due to the lack of actual recalled conversations, the decision was made to code the data for topics of commitment talk that emerged rather than the nature of the talk (i.e., face-threatening acts, politeness, or indirection). Coding of Open-ended Responses The author and one coder derived the coding categories through an inductive analysis of the open-ended responses. Each coder took approximately half of the completed surveys and created a list of the topical areas that consistently emerged from the data. The unit of analysis was the message itself, rather than individual thought units. Each message was evaluated for topical categories. When a response revealed a new topic, a category was created to accommodate that topic. The coders applied the same procedure to the other half of the surveys and compared their results. The first category scheme was 19 developed after discussion and practice coding between the author and the coder. The final category structure emerged after a second iteration of the entire process. The author then trained an additional independent coder and each of them coded and compared approximately one forth of the surveys to check for coder agreement. Once it was established that the coders understood the category scheme, each person then coded 100 more surveys. Cohen’s kappa was used to measure intercoder reliability for these 100 surveys. The kappa’s are reported below along with the category descriptions, and all were found to be “good” or “excellent” according to Fleiss (1981).1 Categories from the Open-ended Responses Topics of commitment talk. Nine categories emerged in response to the question that asked participants to recall their most memorable conversation about commitment with a romantic partner. The first category was the status of the relationship, which was defined as any discussion about the commitment level of the relationship that maintains or moves the relationship forward. The second category, uncertainty, was defined as any uncertainty about the relationship, commitment, or the future. The next category was decrease in commitment level and was defined as any decrease in commitment level such as a break-up. Long distance was the forth category that emerged from the data and was defined as any struggle or issue that was related to a long distance relationship. The fifth category was marriage, which was defined as any discussion about proposals, engagements, marriage, or honeymoons. The sixth category was labeled trust, and was defined as any conversation regarding trust, 20 infidelity, jealously, or discussion of ex-partners. Caring was the seventh category and was defined as a positive expression of loving, caring, or feelings. Conflict inequity was the eighth category and was defined as one person feeling differently than the other. The ninth category was labeled as miscellaneous and included statements that did not fit the categorization structure. Cohen’s kappa for the intercoder reliability on this item was k = .87 (See Table 2 for categories and examples). Table 2. Topics of Memorable Commitment Conversations Categories Example Status of the Relationship "Should we date exclusively?" Uncertainty 3:120:31: rhabits were making him doubt my commitment to our Decrease in Commitment Level "I don't want to go out with you anymore." Long Distance "The whole long distance thing is harder than I expected" Marriage ’Will you many me?" Trust "I would never cheat on you, you know that." Caring "Love you baby." "I am committed to us, you just expect me to be committed in the Conflict lnequity way you want." Events that trigger discussions of commitment. Twelve categories were created to describe the events or conversations that triggered the discussion of commitment. The first category, upcoming or recent life changing event, was defined as any event that would occur in the near future that would have an impact on either the partner or the relationship. The second category that emerged was celebrations, which was defined as any celebration, holiday, vacation, or wedding. The duration of the relationship was the third category and was defined as any mention of length of time dating. The fourth category was 21 labeled the status of the relationship, and referred to any discussion of maintaining or moving the relationship fonivard. Uncertainty was the fifth category and included any uncertainty about the relationship, commitment, or the future. A decrease in commitment level was any decrease in commitment level or a relational break-up and was category six. The seventh category, physical contact, included sex, kissing, cuddling, or any type of physical contact. External issues was the ninth category, and was defined as any discussion of outside issues not related to the relationship, such as movies or the weather. The tenth category was labeled external networks, and included any discussion with or about other people. Relationship conduct was the eleventh category and was defined as everyday relationship occurrences such as talking, going on a date, or hanging out. The final category was miscellaneous and included statements that could not be categorized by the current structure. The Cohen’s kappa for the intercoder reliability on this item was k = .858 (See Table 3 for categories and examples). 22 Table 3. Triggers of Commitment Conversations Categories Upcoming or Recent Life Changing Events Celebrations Duration of the Relationship Status of the Relationship Uncertainty Decrease in Commitment Level Physical Contact Conflict External Issues External Networks Relationship Conduct Example "It really stinks that you have to go back to school at the end of the summer." "It was our anniversary." 'We were dating for about 6 months." "I wanted to know how he felt about our future." "She wanted to know where we were in our relationship." "A recent break up." 'We had our first kiss." ’We were fighting about me not coming home as much." "A commerical." ”A friend asked us if we were thinking about getting married." "In the car, eating french fries." Protecting partner’s face. If participants reported concern for their partner’s face during the conversation, they were asked what they said or did to protect their partner’s face. Six categories emerged from the data. The first category was labeled attention to partner and was defined as anything that protects the partner’s feelings or does not embarrass the partner. Another category was conflict avoidance — conflict escalation, which included not wanting to fight, staying calm, being rational, keeping the conversation light, and avoiding assigning blame. Attention to self was the third category, and was defined as a focus on self, not embarrassing self, or taking responsibility. Attention to the conversation included conversational awareness, such as politeness, turn-taking, apologizing, listening, and honesty. The next category was re-affinning commitment, which was defined as reciprocating feelings, expressing feelings, and confirming commitment or relationship status. The final category was miscellaneous, which included anything that did not fit into the previously defined 23 categories. Cohen’s kappa for the intercoder reliability of this item was k = .72 (See Table 4 for categories and examples). Table 4. Avoid Embarrassment to Self or Partner Categories Example Attention to Partner "I didn't discredit anything he said." Conflict-Avordance ' Avond "I didn't want to push the issue." Escalation Attention to Self "I just need to focus on myself right now." Attention to Conversation "I asked politely." Re-Affirm Commitment "l recipricated the I love you." Protecting self face. If participants reported concern for their own face during the conversation, they were asked what they said or did to protect their own face. The data revealed the same six categories as described above, for protecting partner’s face. Cohen’s kappa for the intercoder reliability of this item was k = .81. Reasons for not talking about commitment. If the participant had not had a conversation about commitment but wanted to, participants were asked to describe the reasons for their choice, and seven categories surfaced. The first category was labeled partner’s disposition and included anything to do with partner’s personality, attitude, traits, and openness. Uncertainty about partner was the second category and was defined as uncertainty related to how partner feels about the relationship and/or uncertainty about how the partner would react. The third category was to protect partner, which was defined as protecting the partner’s face and or feelings. Protect self was the fourth category and was defined as protecting one’s own face and or feelings. Relationship issues was the fifth category and was comprised of issues related to the relationship such as 24 timing, the relationship future, and the relationship status. The sixth category was external issues and was defined as any outside issues not related to the relationship. For example, one of the participants was dating a study abroad student and knew their partner would be returning to their home country at the end of the year. The final category was miscellaneous and included statements that did not fit into the current category structure. Cohen’s kappa for the intercoder reliability of this item was k = .90 (See Table 5 for categories and examples). Table 5. Reasons for Not Discussing Commitment Categories Example Partner's Disposition "He's a jerk." Uncertainty about Partner "Scared he would dump me." Protect Partner "Not wanting to hurt her feelings." Protect Self "Fear of appearing dramatic." Relationship Issues "I was worried it would changewhat we had." External Issues "I knew I was leaving to another country." Close-ended Measures Several scales were used for this research and are described below. All scales consisted of 7-point Likert type items with response options ranging from 1, strongly disagree, to 7, strongly agree, unless othenrvise noted. When assessing each scale’s reliability, three criteria were used to determine whether an item should be excluded from the scale: a) face validity, b) inter-item correlations, and c) the overall Cronbach’s alpha for the scale. Concem for self face. Six items were used to assess how concerned the participant was for their own face when discussing commitment with their partner. Example items included “I was concerned with protecting my personal pride,” and 25 "I was concerned with maintaining my dignity.” One item was deleted to improve the scale’s internal consistency. For the remaining concern for self face items acceptable reliability was obtained (a = .87). Concern for partner’s face. Six items were used to assess how concerned the participant was for their partner’s face when discussing commitment. Example items included “I was concerned with maintaining the dignity of my partner,” and “I tried to be sensitive to my partner’s self-worth.” This scale had reliability (Cronbach’s d) of .88. Relationship uncertainty. The Likert type items ranged from 1 (completely uncertain) to 7(completely certain). The reliability (Cronbach’s a) for this scale was a = .91 and had six items that included, “how certain are you about whether or not you and your partner feel the same way about each other?” and “how certain are you about the future of the relationship?” Self uncertainty. The self uncertainty scale had eight items including, “how certain are you about how committed you are to the relationship?” and “how certain are you about whether or not you want this relationship to last?” The reliability (Cronbach’s a) for this scale was a = .96. Demonstration of commitment. The Likert type items for the demonstration of commitment ranged from 1 (all of the time) to 7 (never). Nine items, including, “showed affection,” and “included partner in future plans” were used to measure how demonstrative participants were of their commitment. The ' reliability (Cronbach’s q) for this scale was a = .93. 26 Commitment level. Ten items were included on the commitment level scale. Example items from this scale were, “I want our relationship to last for a very long time,” and “I would feel upset if our relationship were to end in the near future.” This scale had a reliability (Cronbach’s a) of .94. Results Descriptive Statistics Six scales were used in this study to measure commitment level, demonstration of commitment, concern for self face, concern for partner’s face, uncertainty about the relationship, and uncertainty about self (See Table 6). Table 6. Descriptive Statistics for Close-Ended Scales M SD Concern for Face Scale 4.53 1.27 Concern for Partner's Face Scale 5.29 1.17 Relationship Uncertainty Scale 5.35 1.42 Self Uncertainty Scale 5.65 1.29 Demonstration of Commitment Scale 5.21 1.45 Commitment Level Scale 4.53 1.49 Note. N = 190. Scale Correlations Four of the scales used in this study measured concern for self face, concern for partner’s face, uncertainty about the relationship, and uncertainty about self. The data indicated that concern for self face was positively correlated with concern for partner’s face, r = .25, p < .001. These results indicate that as concern for own face increases, concern for partner’s face also increases. Concern for self face was negatively correlated with self uncertainty, r = -.16, p < 27 .05, and relationship uncertainty, r = -.15, p < .05. Thus, as concern for self face increases the less personal certainty one experiences about their relationship, and the less relational uncertainty they experience. The data also indicated a positive correlation between relationship certainty and concern for partner’s face, r= .25, p < .001, and a positive correlation between relationship uncertainty and self uncertainty, r = .77, p < .001 (See Table 7). Table 7. Scale Correlations CPFS CSFS SUS RUS Concern for Partner’s Face Scale ,, .. (CPFS) 1 .25 0.14 .25 Concern for Self Face Scale (CSFS) .25“ 1 -.16" -.15" Self Uncertainty Scale (SUS) 0.14 -.16" 1 0.77“ Relationship Uncertainty Scale (RUS) .25“ -.15* 0.77“ 1 Note. 'p < .05, two-tailed. “p < .001, two—tailed. Discussion of Commitment RQI asked what reasons college students report for discussing or not discussing relational commitment. The second portion of R01 is answered by the twenty-six percent of participants that did not discuss relational commitment with their partner. Of those, the responses given most often included uncertainty about partner (31%), protect self (20%), and relationship issues (16%). Uncertainty about partner included fear of rejection from partner and uncertainty about how the partner would react if commitment was addressed. To protect one’s self was the next most common category and included the participant feeling scared and/or not wanting to embarrass themselves. The category of relationship issues consisted of bad timing and not wanting to ruin the friendship. 28 When participants did not talk to their partners about commitment they often turned to others for advice and guidance regarding their romantic relationship. Fifty—five percent of the participants that did not talk to their partners about commitment talked about it with friends. Participants also talked with family members (22.4%), roommates (4.1%), and undisclosed others (6.1%). Some participants, however, did not talk to anyone about their relational commitment to their partners (12.2%). R02 and the first part of R01 were answered when participants that did talk about commitment with their romantic partner were asked to describe what events or conversations generated the discussion. The responses to this question were open-ended and thus, some participants’ responses included more than one category. Four of the 12 categories, which can be found above, were most frequently mentioned as the events or conversations that triggered the discussion about commitment and they were: (a) relationship conduct, (b) upcoming or recent life changing events, (c) status of the relationship, and (d) external networks. Twenty-two percent of the participants’ conversations about commitment with their romantic partner were prompted by relationship conduct. Examples of relationship conduct included talking on the phone, going on a date, or taking a walk. Upcoming or recent life changing events were described as generating commitment discussion by 21% of the participants. This category included graduation, a new job, and geographical issues such as moving. Status of the relationship was chosen by 18% of the participants and examples consisted of 29 discussing the relationship future and where the relationship was going. The external issues category was revealed by 18% of the participants. External issues such as watching a TV show or reading a book were included in this category. Uncertainty The second set of research questions (R03 and R04) asked how uncertainty about self and uncertainty about the relationship influenced the decision to discuss commitment. The data indicated statistically significant positive correlations between self uncertainty and status of the relationship, r= .16, p < .05; marriage discussion, r = .25, p <.001; and discussion of caring, r = .15, p < .05. The data also showed a statistically significant negative correlation between self uncertainty and decease in commitment level, r= -.24, p < .001. No other statistically significant correlations emerged for self uncertainty and discussion of commitment. Relationship uncertainty was found to have statistically significant positive correlations with discussing the status of the relationship, r = .20, p < .001; marriage, r= .28, p < .001; and caring, r= .15, p < .05. Relationship uncertainty also was found to have statistically significant negative correlations with discussing a decrease in commitment level, r= -.33, p < .001; and conflict inequity, r = -.17, p < .05. The data revealed no other statistically significant correlations between relationship uncertainty and discussion of commitment (see Table 8). 30 Table 8. Correlations Between Uncertainty and Topics of Commitment Talk Self Uncertainty Relationship Uncertainty Status of the Relationship 45* .20" Uncertainty -.11 -.13 Decrease in Commitment Level -24“ -.33** Long Distance -.06 -.11 Marriage .25" .28“ Trust -.01 .02 Caring .15* .15' Conflict lnequity -.13 -.17* Note. *p < .05, two-tailed. “p < .001, two-tailed. Face Research questions 5 and 6 asked how concern for own face and concern for partner’s face influenced commitment talk. The data revealed no statistically significant correlations between concern for partner’s face and topics of commitment talk (see Table 9). Concern for own face, however, had a statistically significant negative correlation with discussing the topic of marriage, r = -.23, p < .001. No other statistically significant correlations emerged between concern for self face and topics of commitment discussion (see Table 9). 31 Table 9. Correlations Between Face and Topics of Commitment Talk Concern for Self Face Concern for Partner's Face Status of the Relationship _03 .09 Uncertainty .09 .10 Decrease in Commitment Level -.11 -.03 Long Distance .05 -.03 Marriage -.23** .01 Trust -.01 -.07 Caring -.09 .03 Conflict lnequity .07 -.09 Note. *p < .05, two-tailed. **p < .001, two-tailed. Post Hoc Analysis Upon further examination of the data, several observations appeared pertinent to better understand the relationship between uncertainty, face, and commitment talk. Therefore, additional analyses were performed. Triggers of commitment discussion. The data revealed that self uncertainty and relationship uncertainty were correlated with what events triggered discussions of commitment. The data indicated statistically significant negative correlations between self uncertainty and decreases in commitment level triggers, r= -.16, p < .05; and conflict triggers, r= -.16, p < .05. Relationship uncertainty also was negatively correlated with decreases in commitment level triggers, r= -.18, p < .05; and conflict triggers, r = -.16, p < .05; and positively correlated with status of the relationship triggers, r = .14, p < .05. The data indicated no relationship between concern for partner’s face and the events that triggered a commitment conversation. Concern for self face, 32 however, had a statistically significant positive correlation with uncertainty as a trigger for discussion, r = .19, p < .001. Prevention of commitment talk. According to the data, reasons that prevented discussion of commitment were correlated with both self uncertainty and relationship uncertainty. Self Uncertainty showed a statistically significant negative correlation with uncertainty about partner, r = -.25, p < .001, and wanting to protect partner, r = -.16, p < .05. The data indicated a negative correlation with relationship uncertainty and uncertainty about partner, r= -.24, p < .001. Again, the data revealed no relationships between concern for partner’s face and the prevention of a commitment conversation. However, the data revealed statistically significant positive correlations between concern for self face and partner’s disposition, r = .15, p < .05; uncertainty about partner, r = .24, p < .001; and wanting to protect self, r= .17, p < .05. Commitment level. The instrument used for this analysis was the Rusbult, Martz, and Agnew’s (1998) Commitment Scale. The data suggested that the more committed to the partner participants were, the more likely they were to discuss the status of the relationship, r = .19, p < .001; marriage, r= .33, p < .001; and caring, r= .16, p < .05; and the less likely they were to discuss a decrease in commitment level, r = -.29, p < .001. Reasons for not discussing commitment with one’s partner were also correlated with how committed the individual was to the relationship. Commitment level was negatively correlated with uncertainty about partner, r = -.20, p < .001, such that the more committed 33 an individual was to their partner the less likely they would not discuss commitment due to uncertainty about their partner. Surprisingly, there was not a statistically significant correlation between relationship conduct and commitment level. Finally, the more committed individuals were to their partners the less likely they were to not discuss commitment in order to protect their partner’s face, r = -.20, p < .001. Discussion The premise of this research was to evaluate how uncertainty and concern for face affect the decision to discuss commitment with one’s partner. When partners engage in commitment talk it allows them to discuss and address the status of the relationship and the direction in which the relationship is headed. Commitment talk, however, can potentially threaten the face of both partners (Baxter & Wilmot, 1985). The findings in this study support that both face and uncertainty play a role in determining if and how commitment will be discussed between romantic partners. Even though the data was limited in this study due to the fact that many participants did not transcribe a conversation word for word, Knapp and Taylor’s (1994) identified forms of commitment talk could easily be detected. Examples of indirect commitment talk were plentiful and included discussion of the future in general, long distance issues, and expressions of caring. The combination of indirect and direct communication was observed in a handful of reported conversations where the couple discussed parents or friends’ infidelity and what it meant to the relationship. Direct commitment talk occurred much more often 34 then expected and perhaps this was the case because participants were asked to recall their most memorable commitment conversation. Being prompted to recall a conversation about commitment likely triggers the recall of direct, rather than indirect conversations. Research supports that direct conversations often occur when there is a potential for something to change dramatically (Knapp & Taylor, 1994). The data collected here supports that notion as upcoming or recent life changing events prompted 21 % of the commitment conversations. Although the number of times commitment was discussed was not obtained in this research, the quantity of communication was alluded to in a number of responses. Sixteen percent of the commitment conversations were prompted by relationship conduct, which includes everyday relationship occurrences that are often ongoing. Some participants directly stated that their relationship commitment is frequently discussed. Research has suggested that communication quantity is important to relational intimacy and satisfaction (Emmers-Sommer, 2004), however, there was not a statistically significant correlation between relationship conduct and commitment level. Future research should consider further evaluation to ascertain the relationship between quantity of communication and commitment levels in other samples. Quality of communication also has been described as a possible factor influencing relational intimacy and satisfaction (Emmers-Sommer, 2004) and therefore, the more in-depth and serious the conversation the more beneficial the conversation may be to the relationship. Discussing commitment in a controlled manner is a factor when determining the quality of communication. The 35 participants often paid special attention to the conversation and used conversational politeness, apologies, and listening techniques when discussing commitment issues. Forty percent of the participants discussed the status of the relationship, which included re-affirming commitment and moving the relationship forward. The reasons most often cited for not talking about commitment had to do with protecting one’s own face and uncertainty about how the partner would react. If the participant was high in uncertainty about partner they were not likely to engage in commitment discussion. These results parallel Knobloch and Carpenter-Theune’s research (2004), which found that relational uncertainty and topic avoidance appear to overlap. Research questions one and two examined the reasons college students discuss or chose not to discuss relational commitment, and the topics that triggered these discussions. The data suggested that many commitment conversations occur during everyday relational encounters including a date or watching a movie. These findings are important because they illustrate how commitment issues and topics are thought of often and are common conversational staples among individuals involved in a romantic relationship, rather than a taboo topic. A taboo topic according to Baxter and Wilmot (1985) is a topic that is identified as ‘off limits’ to one or both of the relationship parties. These results conflict with previous research (Baxter & Wilmot, 1985) that found explicit relational communication was rare and state of the relationship talk dominated the data as taboo. Future research needs to focus on determining if 36 commitment talk is seen as a taboo topic or if over time the new generation has become more comfortable discussing such topics that may leave them vulnerable. The data also revealed the common trend of discussing their romantic commitment with third parties, most often reported, friends. When participants do not talk to their partner about commitment over half of them turn to friends. This is a significant finding that demonstrates the importance friends may have on how the relationship is viewed and how commitment is discussed in the future. Future research should investigate how third parties, especially friends, impact how individuals approach the discussion of commitment later in their relationship. Research questions three and four asked how self and relational uncertainty influence commitment talk. The data support Knobloch and Solomon’s (1999; 2002; 2005; Solomon & Knobloch, 2001; 2004) findings that individuals in high self or relationship uncertainty will not be likely to engage in information-seeking behaviors (e.g., commitment talk). The number one reason given for not discussing commitment was uncertainty about partner. This uncertainty about partner did not increase the participants’ desire to discuss commitment as Uncertainty Reduction Theory would have predicted. Although uncertainty sometimes produced commitment discussions, it often did not. In fact, as self and relational certainty decreased the less likely participants were to discuss the topics of status of the relationship, marriage, and caring. Perhaps the dimensions of relational and self uncertainty need to be addressed under the 37 premise of a different theory as URT was never intended to be used to explain encounters beyond initial interactions. Relationship uncertainty and self uncertainty were highly correlated. Knobloch and Solomon (1999) also found a substantial correlation between self and relationship uncertainty. After a factor analysis, their results indicated that relationship uncertainty covaried with self uncertainty. Although Knobloch and Solomon raised the question of distinctness between the two scales, they came to the conclusion that the two scales were not unidimensional. It is possible that the correlation between relationship and self uncertainty was high in this study because the participant was projecting about how their partner felt. For example, if the participant was highly certain about self in the relationship than that individual may be more likely to project that their partner feels the same resulting in high relationship certainty. The final set of research questions addressed how concern for own face and concern for partner’s face influenced commitment talk. Overall, the data suggested that concern for partner’s face had little impact on the commitment conversational topics or the triggers of the conversations. Concern for own face, though, was related to both the topics and the triggers of the commitment conversations. The data showed that as concern for own face increased the less likely a commitment conversation would take place due to partner’s disposition, uncertainty about partner, and wanting to protect self. The data also suggested that as concern for own face increased the less certain participants were about their self and relational commitment. These results indicate that concern for own 38 face together with self and relational uncertainty impacted the decision to discuss relational commitment. Additional research needs to further examine the relationships between concern for own face, uncertainty, and the topics and triggers of commitment talk. Limitations Some limitations of this study include the characteristics of the sample, the method by which the data was collected, and the direction of causality. First, males were underrepresented in this investigation. The females in this study made up over 70% of the participants. Consequently, the results of this study can be generalized with more confidence to the female population. A related limitation is the overall race, age, and education of the participants was very homogenous, which reduces the generalizability of the findings. Additionally, the method in which the data was collected is also a limitation. The participants were asked to report by recalling a conversation, which is most likely affected by time and the current status of the relationship. Also, data were only collected from one partner, the absence of data from the other partner leaves a gap in the data and does not provide a complete picture of why couples do or do not discuss commitment. Finally, because of the nature of the findings it is impossible to speak of causality associations between uncertainty, face, and commitment talk. While relationships can be discussed, utilizing recall data precludes analysis of cause because temporal ordering cannot be established between the variables. 39 Conclusion This research explored how self and relational uncertainty and concern for self and partner’s face affect the decision to discuss commitment with one’s partner. The findings in this study maintain that both face and uncertainty play a role in determining if and how commitment will be discussed between romantic partners. It is important for future scholars to continue to focus on how face and uncertainty play a role in decisions to discuss intimate relationship topics with partners as well as social networks. Finally, it may be useful to broaden our theoretical focus and consider how face concerns influence the uncertainty that couples face when broaching discussions of commitment. Notes [1] For data entry each category was given a binary code with the number one signifying that the category was present (i.e., reported by the respondent) and the number zero signifying that the category was not present (i.e., not reported by the respondent). The goal for this coding was to create a representative list of categories, one that would demonstrate the breadth of responses reported by the participants rather than the frequency of each response. This choice was made in order to facilitate data entry purposes because the participants wrote quite extensively in response to each question. For example, an entry for a participant who reported their most memorable conversation about commitment reported, ‘I would never cheat on you, you know that. . . It (cheating) would never happen because I know you would kick me to the curb’ would receive only one code for the trust category being present, rather than two. Thus, the data are representative of the range of categories reported rather than the depth reported for each category. 40 REFERENCES 41 REFERENCES Adams, J. M., & Jones, W. H. (1997). The conceptualization of marital commitment: An integrative analysis. American Psychological Association, Inc., 72, 117-1196. Afifi, W. A., & Burgoon, J. K. (1998). 'We never talk about that": A comparison of cross-sex friendships and dating relationships on uncertainty and topic avoidance. Personal Relationships, 5, 255-272. Afifi, W. A., & Metts, S. (1998). Characteristics and consequences of expectation violations in close relationships. Joumal of Social and Personal Relationships, 15, 365-392. Agnew, C. R., Van Lange, P. A. M., Rusbult, C. E., & Langston, C. A. (1998). Cognitive interdependence: Commitment and the mental representation of close relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 939- 954. Arriaga, X. B., & Agnew, C. R. (2001). Being committed: Affective, cognitive, and conative components of relationship commitment. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 27, 1 190-1203. Baxter, L. A., & Wilmot, W. W. (1985). Taboo topics in close relationships. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 2, 253-269. Bello, R., & Edwards, R. (2005). Interpretations of messages the influence of various forms of equivocation, face concerns, and sex differences. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 24, 160-181. Berger, & Bradac, J. (1982). Language and social knowledge: Uncertainty in interpersonal relations. London: Edward Arnold. Berger, 0., & Calabrese, R. J. (1975). Some explorations in initial interaction and beyond: Toward a developmental theory of interpersonal communication. Human Communication Research, 1, 99-112. Brashers, D. E. (2001). Communication and uncertainty management. Journal of Communication, 51, 477-497. Brown, R, & Levinson, S. C. (1987). Politeness: Some universals in language usage. Cambridge and New York: Cambridge University Press. Bui, K.-V. T., Peplau, L. A., & Hill, C. T. (1996). Testing the Rusbult model of relationship commitment and stability in a 15-year study of heterosexual couples. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 22, 1244-1257. 42 Cupach, W. R., & Carson, C. L. (2002). Characteristics and consequences of interpersonal complaints associated with perceived face threat. Joumal of Social and Personal Relationships, 19, 443-462. Dindia, K., & Canary, D. J. (1993). Definitions and theoretical perspectives on maintaining relationships. Joumal of Social and Personal Relationships, 10, 163-173. Emmers-Sommer, T. M. (2004). The effect of communication quality and quantity indicators on intimacy and relational satisfaction. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 21, 399-411. Emmers, T. M., & Canary, D. J. (1996). The effect of uncertainty reducing strategies on young couples’ relational repair and intimacy. Communication Quarterly, 44, 166-182. F leiss, J L. (Eds.). (1981). Statistical methods for rates and proportions (2"d ed.). New York: Wiley. Goffman, E. (1959). Presentation of self in everyday life. Garden City, NY: Doubleday. Goldsmith, D. J., & MacGeorge, E. L. (2000). The impact of politeness and relationship on perceived quality of advice about a problem. Human Communication Research, 26, 234-263. Johnson, & Rusbult, C. (1989). Resisting temptation: Devaluation of alternative partners as a means of maintaining commitment in close relationships. Joumal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 967-980. Johnson, M. P. (1973). Commitment: A conceptual structure and empirical application. Sociological Quarterty, 14, 395-406. Johnson, M. P. (1999). Personal, moral, and structural commitment to relationships: Experiences of choice and constraint. In J. M. Adams & W. H. Jones (Eds), Handbook of interpersonal commitment and relationship stability (pp. 73-87). New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum. Kelley, H. H., & Thibaut, J. W. (1978). Interpersonal relations: A theory of interdependence. New York: Wiley. Knapp, M., & Taylor, E. (1994). Commitment and its communication in romantic relationships. In A. L. Weber & J. H. Harvey (Eds.), Perspectives on Close Relationships (pp. 153-175). Needham Heights, MA. 43 Knobloch, & Carpenter-Theune, K. E. (2004). Topic avoidance in devel0ping romantic relationships: Associations with intimacy and relational uncertainty. Communication Research, 31, 173-205. Knobloch, & Solomon. (1999). Measuring the sources and content of relational uncertainty. Communication Studies, 50, 261-278. Knobloch, & Solomon. (2002). Information seeking beyond initial interaction: Negotiating relational uncertainty within close relationships. Human Communication Research, 28, 243-257. Knobloch, & Solomon. (2005). Relational uncertainty and relational information processing. Communication Research, 32, 349-388. Martz, J. M., Verette, J., Arriaga, X. B., Slovik, L. F., Cox, C. L., & Rusbult, C. E. (1998). Positive illusion in close relationships. Personal Relationships, 5, 159-181. Miller, R. (1997). lnattentive and contented: Relationship commitment and attention to alternatives. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 758-766. Murray, 8. L., & Holmes, J. G. (1993). Seeing virtues in faults: Negativity and the transformation of interpersonal narratives in close relationships. Joumal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 707-722. Oetzel, J. G., & Ting-Toomey, S. (2003). Face concerns in interpersonal conflict: A cross-cultural empirical test of the face negotiation theory. Communication Research, 30, 599-624. Rusbult, C. E. (1983). A longitudinal test of the investment model: The development (and deterioration) of satisfaction and commitment in heterosexual involvements. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 101 -1 17. Rusbult, C. E., & Buunk, B. P. (1993). Commitment processes in close relationships: An interdependence analysis. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 10, 175-204. Rusbult, C. E., Martz, J. M., & Agnew, C. R. (1998). The investment model scale: Measuring commitment level, satisfaction level, quality of alternatives, and investment size. Personal Relationships, 5, 357-391. 44 Solomon, D. H., & Knobloch, L. K. (2001). Relationship uncertainty, partner interference, and intimacy within dating relationships. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 18, 804-820. Solomon, D. H., & Knobloch, L. K. (2004). A model of relational turbulence: The role of intimacy, relational uncertainty, and interference from partners in appraisals of irritations. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 21, 795-816. Van Lange, P. A. M., Agnew, C. R., Harinck, F., & Steemers, G. (1997). From game theory to real life: How social value orientation affects willingness to sacrifice in ongoing close relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 1330-1344. Weigel, D. J., & Ballard-Reisch, D. (2002). Investigating the behavioral indicators of relational commitment. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 1 9, 403-423. 45