.64... L . 59. _> .w{% J. '9‘ . t v... nut-i- I." I. .7“ ‘ ”GAL. 79‘ (V LIBRARY Michigan State University ~— This is to certify that the dissertation entitled COMPLEX POSTTRAUMATIC STRESS SYMPTOMS AMONG A COMMUNITY SAMPLE OF BATTERED WOMEN presented by KERRY LYNN LEAHY has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for the PhD. degree in Psychology V; “U ,qum Date MSU is an affirmative-action, equal-opportunity employer «-—.------.-o—-—-— — A ,-c-----n-o-.-o--c----- PLACE IN RETURN BOX to remove this checkout from your record. To AVOID FINES return on or before date due. MAY BE RECALLED with earlier due date if requested. DAIEDUE DAIEDUE DAIEDUE 6/07 p:/ClRC/DateDue.indd-p.1 C OMPL ( COMPLEX POSTTRAUMATIC STRESS SYMPTOMS AMONG A COMMUNITY SAMPLE OF BATTERED WOMEN By Kerry Lynn Leahy A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Psychology 2007 COMPI Objective: The air: violence (Dl'l \ieti complex posttraum; potential mediating representations of e detelopment of (‘P Method: Cluster at €Xperiences of DV (threats of agareSsi ABSTRACT COMPLEX POSTTRAUMATIC STRESS SYMPTOMS AMONG A COMMUNITY SAMPLE OF BATTERED WOMEN By Kerry Lynn Leahy Objective: The aim of this study was to identify longitudinal patterns of domestic violence (DV) victimization and to examine the relationship between these patterns and complex posttraumatic stress disorder (CP) symptoms. An additional aim was to test the potential mediating role of social support and the potential moderating roles of adult representations of childhood attachment and child maltreatment history in the development of CP symptoms among a community sample of female victims of DV. Method: Cluster analysis techniques were used to summarize 164 women’s individual experiences of DV victimization over six years. Frequency, severity, and type of abuse (threats of aggression, physical DV, and sexual DV) were considered separately in the cluster analyses. Regression analyses were used to examine the relationships between DV trajectories and CP symptoms, and DV duration and CP symptoms. Baron and Kenny’s (1986) procedures for testing mediation and moderation were applied to examine social support as an intervening variable and adult representations of childhood attachment and child maltreatment history as vulnerability factors in the proposed DV-CP Symptom link. Results: Cluster analyses based on DV frequency, DV severity, threats of DV, and physical DV produced nearly equivalent solutions; each produced Minimal, Moderate, and High DV subgroups. The cluster analysis based on sexual DV produced a 2-group solution: Minimal and High sexual DV. The relationships between all cluster solutions and CP symptoms, as well as DV duration and CP symptoms were significant, such that greater I) of social support: 'ti DV and CP sympti. link. but adult repr suggest that female atiectixe and helm accounted for by C exacerbate DV-rel; support suggests tl community-d“ ell i understood. Resu such that greater DV was related to more CP symptoms. DV was related to poor quality of social support; however, social isolation did not account for the relationship between DV and CP symptoms. Child maltreatment history moderated the DV-CP symptoms link, but adult representations of childhood attachment did not. Conclusions: Findings suggest that female victims of DV (even those with moderate levels of DV) are at risk for affective and behavioral dysregulation that transcends simple PTSD and may be better accounted for by CP. In addition, the experience of abuse in childhood appears to exacerbate DV-related CP symptoms. The lack of significant findings regarding social support suggests that mechanisms for the development of CP symptoms among community-dwelling women who experience mild to moderate levels of DV are not well understood. Results have significant clinical implications for the psychological treatment of women who experience DV. First and iorerr her thoughtful Her mentoring been inx'aluab my commute Alex to“ E}. to Support m L'I‘liV'ersity c aim 10 the g foi- this sum [0 Share the Finally. I a ACKNOWLEDGMENTS First and foremost, I would like to thank my dissertation chair, Alytia Levendosky, for her thoughtful guidance, flexibility, and patience throughout the production of this work. Her mentoring and dedication to my success far exceed that which is expected and have been invaluable in my professional and personal development. I would also like to thank my committee members: Anne Bogat for her theoretical knowledge and editing skills, Alex von Eye for his statistical expertise, and Tom Luster for his generosity in agreeing to support my work. I am indebted to Natalia Katenka, a graduate student at the University of Michigan, who taught me about cluster analysis techniques. Thank you also to the graduate and undergraduate research assistants who assisted in data collection for this study, and to the Mother-Infant Study participants for their time and willingness to share their life experiences — without them, this research would not have been possible. Finally, I am very grateful to my husband and family who provided continued support and encouragement throughout this process. iv IISI OF TABLE} IISI OF FIGL'RI. Introduction ........... Problem of'Dl Mental Health (' .4 Theoretical .ll. Direct Effect ( Social lsoltitit Personal l'ulr Eridencefbr a ( Social Support. Adult Represent Child Maltreatt Ht'pot/reses (“It Htpmlrcs‘w‘ r lltpotlrexis j Htpotltesis j Ht‘potlres‘is _ .l'pOtltt'sis . .lpotlzt's‘is Methods ......... Participants rocedures ecruitmcr . ampling .. ata C(lllL raining... I ‘ aniple RC TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES ........................................................................................................... viii LIST OF FIGURES ........................................................................................................... xi Introduction ......................................................................................................................... 1 Problem of DV ................................................................................................................ 6 Mental Health Consequences of DV ............................................................................... 7 A Theoretical Model of Complex Posttraumatic Stress Disorder for DV Victims ....... 13 Direct Effect of DV on CP Symptoms ....................................................................... 14 Social Isolation as an Explanatory F actor for the Development of CP ................... 21 Personal Vulnerability Factors for the Development of C P ..................................... 22 Evidence for a Complex PT SD Syndrome .................................................................... 27 Social Support ............................................................................................................... 36 Adult Representations of Childhood Attachment .......................................................... 40 Child Maltreatment History .......................................................................................... 44 Hypotheses and Rationale ............................................................................................. 45 Hypotheses 1a-1 d ...................................................................................................... 46 Hypothesis 2 .............................................................................................................. 49 Hypothesis 3a ............................................................................................................ 49 Hypothesis 3b ............................................................................................................ 50 Hypothesis 4 .............................................................................................................. 51 Hypothesis 5 .............................................................................................................. 52 Methods ............................................................................................................................. 53 Participants ................................................................................................................... 53 Procedures .................................................................................................................... 53 Recruitment ............................................................................................................... 53 Sampling ................................................................................................................... 54 Data Collection ......................................................................................................... 55 Training ..................................................................................................................... 56 Sample Retention ...................................................................................................... 57 Measures ....................................................................................................................... 58 Demographics ........................................................................................................... 58 Trauma Exposure ...................................................................................................... 58 Complex PT SD .......................................................................................................... 61 Social Support ........................................................................................................... 63 Adult Representations of Childhood Attachment ...................................................... 64 Results ............................................................................................................................... 67 Missing Data ................................................................................................................. 67 Hypothesis Testing ........................................................................................................ 68 Hypotheses Ia-Id ...................................................................................................... 68 Analyses for Hypothesis 1 ......................................................................................... 68 Hypothesis 2 .............................................................................................................. 72 Analysis for Hypothesis 2 .......................................................................................... 73 Hypothesis 3a ............................................................................................................ 81 Analyses for Hypothesis 3a ....................................................................................... 82 Hypothesis 3b ............................................................................................................ 86 Analyses for Hypothesis 3b ....................................................................................... 86 Hypothesis 4 .............................................................................................................. 92 Analyses for Hypothesis 4 ......................................................................................... 92 Hypothesis 5 .............................................................................................................. 94 Analyses for Hypothesis 5 ......................................................................................... 95 Discussion ......................................................................................................................... 98 Longitudinal Trajectories of DV Victimization ............................................................. 98 Relationship between DV Victimization and CP Symptoms ....................................... 104 Effect of Partnership Status on C P Symptoms ............................................................ 108 Social Support ............................................................................................................. 1 10 Adult Representations of Childhood Attachment and Child Maltreatment History 1 14 Summary ..................................................................................................................... 119 Study Strengths ........................................................................................................ 120 Study Limitations and Future Research Implications ............................................. 121 Clinical Implications ............................................................................................... 123 APPENDIX A: Demographics ....................................................................................... 172 APPENDIX B: Severity of Violence Against Women Scales ........................................ 174 APPENDIX C: Childhood Trauma Questionnaire — Short Form ................................... 176 APPENDIX D: Self-Report Inventory for Disorders of Extreme Stress ........................ 179 APPENDIX E: Norbeck Social Support Scale ............................................................... 185 APPENDIX F : Perceptions of Adult Attachment Questionnaire ................................... 188 APPENDIX G: Imputed Variables with Corresponding Matching Variables ................ 193 APPENDIX H: Clustering Results for Severity of DV, Threats of DV, and Physical DV .. ......................................................................................................................................... 196 APPENDIX 1: Cluster Results for Severity of DV, Threats of DV, and Physical DV among Women in Long-term DV Relationships ................................... 202 APPENDIX J: Cluster Results for DV Severity and Threats of DV among Women in Short-term DV Relationships ................................................................. 208 vi REFERENCES ............................................................................................................... 212 vii Table 1: Nu ant Table 2: De Table 3: Ch Table 4: C“ Table 5: Crt Table 6: C“ Table 7: Cr: Table 8: Cr. Table 9: Cr Table 10; CI Table 1 1: CI Rt Table 11C] Ctr Table 13. C 81 a Table 15: C Table 1: Table 2: Table 3: Table 4: Table 5: Table 6: Table 7: Table 8: Table 9: Table 10: Table 1 1: Table 12: Table 13: Table 14: Table 15: LIST OF TABLES Number of Participants Missing Variable-level Data by Study Variable and Time Period ...................................................................................... 128 Descriptive Data for Continuous Variables in Conceptual Model ......... 129 Cluster Mean Profiles for DV Frequency ............................................... 133 Cross-tabulation of DV Frequency and DV Severity Cluster Solutions ...... ................................................................................................................. 134 Cross-tabulation of DV Frequency and Threats of DV Cluster Solutions ................................................................................................................. 135 Cross-tabulation of DV Frequency and Physical DV Cluster Solutions ...... ................................................................................................................. 136 Cross-tabulation of DV Severity and Threats of DV Cluster Solutions ....... ................................................................................................................. 137 Cross-tabulation of DV Severity and Physical DV Cluster Solutions... 138 Cross-tabulation of Threats of DV and Physical DV Cluster Solutions ....... ................................................................................................................. 139 Cluster Mean Profiles for Sexual DV ..................................................... 140 Cluster Mean Profiles for DV Frequency among Women in Long-term DV Relationships ........................................................................................... 141 Cross-tabulation of DV Frequency and DV Severity Cluster Solutions among Women in Long-term DV Relationships .................................... 142 Cross-tabulation of DV Frequency and Threats of DV Cluster Solutions among Women in Long-term DV Relationships .................................... 143 Cross-tabulation of DV Frequency and Physical DV Cluster Solutions among Women in Long-term DV Relationships .................................... 144 Cross-tabulation of DV Severity and Threats of DV Cluster Solutions among Women in Long-term DV Relationships .................................... 145 viii Table 16: Table 17: Table 18: Table 19: Table 20: Table 21: Table 22: Table 23: Table 24: Table 25: Table 26: Table 27: Table 28: Table 29: Table 30: Table 31: Cross-tabulation of DV Severity and Physical DV Cluster Solutions among Women in Long-term DV Relationships .................................... 146 Cross-tabulation of Threats of DV and Physical DV Cluster Solutions among Women in Long-term DV Relationships .................................... 147 Cluster Mean Profiles for Sexual DV among Women in Long-term DV Relationships ........................................................................................... 148 Cluster Mean Profiles for DV Frequency among Women in Short-term DV Relationships .................................................................................... 149 Cluster Mean Profiles for Physical DV among Women in Short-term DV Relationships ........................................................................................... 1 50 Cross-tabulation of DV Severity and Threats of DV Cluster Solutions among Women in Short-term DV Relationships .................................... 151 Cross-tabulation of DV Severity and Physical DV Cluster Solutions among Women in Short-term DV Relationships .................................... 152 Cross-tabulation of Threats of DV and Physical DV Cluster Solutions among Women in Short-term DV Relationships .................................... 153 Cluster Mean Profiles for Sexual DV among Women in Short-term DV Relationships ........................................................................................... 1 54 Means and Standard Deviations for Number of Network Supporters by DV Frequency Cluster ............................................................................ 155 Means and Standard Deviations for Number of Network Supporters by Sexual DV Cluster .................................................................................. 156 Means and Standard Deviations for Number of Network Supporters by DV Duration Group ................................................................................ 157 Means and Standard Deviations for Emotional Support by DV Frequency Cluster ..................................................................................................... 158 Means and Standard Deviations for Emotional Support by Sexual DV Cluster ..................................................................................................... 159 Means and Standard Deviations for Emotional Support by UV Duration Group ...................................................................................................... 160 Complex PTSD Symptom Scores as a function of DV Frequency Cluster and Attachment Status ............................................................................ 161 ix Table 33: Table Ill: Table H2: Table H3: Table ll: Table II: Table 13: Table ll : Table l2: CUT”; Aria. Cllni (Tu Rel. Table 32: Table H1: Table H2: Table H3: Table 11: Table 12: Table 13: Table J 1: Table J2: Complex PTSD Symptom Scores as a function of Sexual DV Cluster and Attachment Status ................................................................................... 162 Cluster Mean Profiles for DV Severity ................................................... 196 Cluster Mean Profiles for Threats of DV ................................................ 197 Cluster Mean Profiles for Physical DV .................................................. 198 Cluster Mean Profiles for DV Severity among Women in Long-term DV Relationships ........................................................................................... 202 Cluster Mean Profiles for Threats of DV among Women in Long-term DV Relationships ........................................................................................... 203 Cluster Mean Profiles for Physical DV among Women in Long-term DV Relationships ........................................................................................... 204 Cluster Mean Profiles for DV Severity among Women in Short-term DV Relationships ........................................................................................... 208 Cluster Mean Profiles for Threats of DV among Women in Short-term DV Relationships ........................................................................................... 209 Tigure l: figure I: Figure 3: Figure 4: Figure 5: F1Sure 6: TiElite 7: Time 8: Figure 9; Figme H1: Figure H2: Figure H3; FIEUre 11 '. Figure 12 FlgUre 13 ‘ “glue J1 Theoretit Victims Cluster Cluster Cluster TCILIIIU‘ CIUSK TCI'JIIt CIUSI relari (‘IU\ PCT at Clu Tel: .‘xrc Cl C Figure 1: Figure 2: Figure 3: Figure 4: Figure 5: Figure 6: Figure 7: Figure 8: Figure 9: Figure H1: Figure H2: Figure H3: Figure 11: Figure 12: Figure 13: Figure .1 1: LIST OF FIGURES Theoretical model for the development of CP Symptoms in female Victims of DV ......................................................................................... 163 Cluster mean profiles for DV frequency ................................................ 164 Cluster mean profiles for sexual DV ...................................................... 165 Cluster mean profiles for DV frequency among women in long-term DV relationships ............................................................................................ 166 Cluster mean profiles for sexual DV among women in long-term DV relationships ............................................................................................ 167 Cluster mean profiles for DV frequency among women in short-term DV relatibnships ............................................................................................ 168 Cluster mean profiles for physical DV among women in short-term DV relationships ............................................................................................ 1 69 Cluster mean profiles for sexual DV among women in short-term DV relationships ............................................................................................ l 70 Mean number of network supporters by DV frequency cluster and time ”.7" ................................................................................................................. l 1 Cluster mean profiles for DV Severity ................................................... 199 Cluster mean profiles for Threats of DV ................................................ 200 Cluster mean profiles for Physical DV ................................................... 201 Cluster mean profiles for DV Severity among women in long-term DV relationships ............................................................................................ 205 Cluster mean profiles for Threats of DV among women in long-term DV relationships ............................................................................................ 206 Cluster mean profiles for Physical DV among women in long-term DV relationships ............................................................................................ 207 Cluster mean profiles for DV severity among women in short-term DV relationships ............................................................................................ 21 0 xi Figure 13: CIUS relat; Figure J2: Cluster mean profiles for threats-of DV among women in short-term DV relationships ............................................................................................ 21 1 xii Domestic Violcn. mittens of aggressiur public health problem \\ risk for developing a \ :1 disorder tPTSDl. a ssm exhibited by indi\ idua“. actual or threatened \ it Horanitz. 8; Raxxlings abned \\ omen are his 31-.199S:Kemp et al. nor appear to fully ca ta'ticularlv given tlt c % ‘ PTSD mm, r the Criteria Were (1 CV Cir . tumscnbed tram SV 'ndrom com I P c Calmll‘e The p S\ Introduction Domestic violence (DV; defined here as male physical violence, sexual violence, and threats of aggression perpetrated against female romantic partners) is a significant public health problem with serious mental health consequences. Abused women are at risk for developing a variety of mental health problems, including posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD), a syndrome developed to account for the mental health symptoms exhibited by individuals who experience a traumatic event (i.e., an event that involves actual or threatened violence or death, Cascardi & O'Leary, 1992; Kemp, Green, Hovanitz, & Rawlings, 1995; Kemp, Rawlings, & Green, 1991). Rates of PTSD in abused women are high, ranging from 45% to 84% (Houskamp & Foy, 1991; Kemp et al., 1995; Kemp et al., 1991; Vitanza, Vogel, & Marshall, 1995). However, PTSD does not appear to fully capture the psychological sequelae of DV for all battered women, particularly given the high rates of co-morbid psychiatric disorders experienced by many abused women (e. g., Nixon, Resick, & Nishith, 2004). PTSD may not fully account for the mental health consequences of DV because the criteria were deve10ped to reflect the psychological response to an acute or circumscribed traumatic event in which the primary stressor is no longer present (e. g., automobile accident, natural disaster), whereas DV is a repeated, prolonged interpersonal trauma (M. A. Dutton & Goodman, 1994; Herman, 1992a; 1992b; van der Kolk, 2002a). Repeated, prolonged interpersonal trauma is defined as interpersonal physical, sexual and/or psychological abuse without a discrete beginning or ending. An alternative syndrome, complex posttraumatic stress disorder (CP) was developed specifically to capture the psychological consequences of repeated, prolonged interpersonal trauma and therefore, may better account for the mental health effects of DV experienced by some women. CP is characterized by psychological disturbances in three major areas: 1) a poly-symptomatic psychiatric presentation, including somatization, dissociation, and depression, 2) poor self-perceptions and interpersonal functioning, and 3) vulnerability to experiences of future harm, both self-inflicted and perpetrated by others (Herman, 19923; 1992b). The present research proposes and examines a theoretical model for the development of CP symptoms in female victims of DV. This model includes a direct link from DV to CP symptoms with DV frequency, severity, duration, and abuse type differentially affecting the development of CP symptoms. Specifically, in this model, CP symptoms are proposed to be the direct result of DV exposure with women who experience high levels of DV frequency, severity, and duration developing more symptoms as compared to women who experience less frequent, severe, and chronic abuse. In addition, sexual DV is more likely to cause CP than physical DV or threats of DV. The model also assumes that women in long-term DV relationships are more likely to develop CP symptoms than women in one or more short-term DV relationship(s). Furthermore, the model includes an indirect link from DV to CP symptoms through social support, suggesting that the development of CP symptoms is partially due to poor social support related to DV. Finally, the model includes two moderator variables— insecure adult representations of childhood attachment and child maltreatment history— which are expected to exacerbate DV-related CP symptoms (see Figure 1). This model for the development of CP symptoms in battered women is influenced by Herman’s (1992a; 1992b) conceptualization of CP. Herman (1992a; 1992b) asserts that repeated, prolonged interpersonal trauma occurs when a relationship of coercive control develops between the perpetrator and victim. The perpetrator establishes a coercive relationship with the victim over time through his abusive actions, such as threats of violence, actual violence, and psychological abuse techniques. In a coercive relationship, the perpetrator has the power to exercise continued abuse and to shape the attitudes, beliefs, thoughts, feelings, and behaviors of the victim, eventually undermining the victim’s psychological resistance and mental health. Thus, subordination to coercive control results in CP, a complex constellation of observable, mental health symptoms that transcend the arousal, intrusive, and avoidant symptoms of PTSD (Herman, 1992a; 1992b) Although the model for this study draws primarily on Herman’s (1992a; 1992b) formulation of CP for theoretical support, Herman’s (1992a; 1992b) theory has significant limitations. Specifically, Herman (1992a; 1992b) does not adequately describe how and why victims of interpersonal trauma are vulnerable to future harm perpetrated by others. In addition, CP theory does not explicitly address how the specific configuration of abuse experienced by victims may contribute to differential levels or patterns of CP symptoms. Furthermore, individual differences in risk for CP are not discussed. Thus, the theory does not fully explain the mechanisms underlying risk for repeated harm and does not specify how abuse characteristics and factors apart from the abuse may directly affect or interact to affect CP symptoms. As a result, the current study draws on constructivist, dynamic-maturational, and attachment theories to provide further support for the proposed model that suggests that the specific configuration of abuse differentially affects the development of CP symptoms and includes two factors that affect vulnerability to DV-related CP symptoms. Empirical research has provided evidence for a CP syndrome among repeated, prolonged interpersonal abuse victims in general, particularly among victims of sexual abuse (McLean & Gallop, 2003; Pelcovitz et al., 1997; Roth, Newman, Pelcovitz, van der Kolk, & Mandel, 1997). Studies suggest that CP is specific to trauma victims (Pelcovitz etal., 1997), with exposure to an interpersonal stressor, severity and duration of abuse, sexual abuse, and early-onset of abuse presenting particular risks for the development of CP (McLean & Gallop, 2003; Pelcovitz et al., 1997; Roth etal., 1997). The syndrome has subsequently been referred to under a variety of names, including “Disorders of Extreme Stress” (DES: Davidson, 1993), “Disorders of Extreme Stress Not Otherwise Specified” (DESNOS: Pelcovitz et al., 1997; van der Kolk et al., 1996), and “lasting personality changes following catastrophic stress” (World Health Organization [WHO], 1992, p.91). In the current DSM-I V nomenclature (American Psychological Association [APA], 1994), the CP symptom constellation is incorporated under “Associated Features of PTSD.” Although findings provide evidence for the existence of a CP syndrome, there are some noteworthy limitations in the existing empirical literature. For example, prior studies of CP have relied primarily on treatment-seeking Caucasian samples, and have rarely identified the relationship between victim and perpetrator. This methodology has perhaps limited the application of findings to the general population of interpersonal abuse victims, the majority of whom do not seek treatment, and has precluded the identification of the specific nature of the violence. Therefore, the validity of the CP construct has not been explored among battered women specifically, even though DV qualifies as an interpersonal trauma, which is often re-occurring and chronic in nature. Previous studies have also failed to test the assumptions and mechanisms outlined in CP theory, instead relying on findings supporting the presence of CP symptoms among interpersonal abuse victims as providing confirmation for Herman’s (1992a; 1992b) formulation of CP theory. The present research begins to address these limitations, and thus was designed to contribute to the further development of an empirical and theoretical literature on CP by proposing a theoretical model for the development of CP symptoms in victims of DV based on trauma and attachment theories, and testing this model in a community sample of abused women with lifetime histories of DV ranging in severity from mild to severe. Women were recruited during pregnancy as part of a larger, on-going longitudinal investigation of the impact of DV on women and children (The Mother-Infant Study: Levendosky, Bogat, Davidson, & von Eye, 2000). The current study examines CP symptoms as opposed to diagnostic status due to the non-clinical nature of the sample. Clinically significant CP is expected to be rare in this population, but the core features of CP are expected to occur with sufficient frequency to determine their relationship to DV. Specifically, this study utilizes a person-oriented, developmental approach by examining CP symptoms in relation to groups of women who have experienced similar longitudinal patterns of DV victimization based on a variety of abuse characteristics (e.g., DV severity) across six waves of data collection. In addition, the study examines the importance of DV partnership and the process of social isolation in the development of CP symptoms. Finally, the study tests social support as a mediator and adult representations of childhood attachment and child maltreatment history as moderators of the hypothesized DV-CP symptom link. Study findings are expected to add specificity to the existing body of knowledge on CP, specifically in relation to DV, and have important clinical implications for understanding the experiences of battered women, such that psychological symptoms can be linked, where appropriate, to histories of abuse. Problem of DV DV is a significant public health problem, accounting for 20-64% of all violence against women according to estimates from two national crime surveys (Rennison, 2003; Tjaden & Thoennes, 2000a). Lifetime prevalence estimates for DV are high and similar across studies with rates of spousal or partner rape ranging from 8-10% (Finkelhor & Yllo, 1987; Russell, 1990; Tjaden & Thoennes, 20003) and rates of physical assault steady at about 22% (Bureau of Justice Statistics [BJ S], 1998; Tjaden & Thoennes, 2000a). Twelve-month prevalence estimates of DV are more variable. Annual prevalence rates based on national surveys range from less than 2% to almost 14% (Rennison, 2003; Schafer, Caetano, & Clark, 1998; Tjaden & Thoennes, 1998, 2000b, 2000c). Community-based studies employing a broader definition of DV, including psychological abuse, produce higher estimates ranging from 18-57% (Meisel, Chandler, & Rienzi, 2003; Smith, Thornton, DeVellis, Earp, & Coker, 2002; Waltermaurer, Ortega, & McNutt, 2003). Based on US. Census estimates for the number of women age 18 or older, these findings suggest that at minimum 1.5 million women experience DV annually in the US with approximately 4.8 million incidents of DV perpetrated against women each year in the US. (Tjaden & Thoennes, 1998, 2000b). Furthermore, battered women are at risk for injury, (Rennison, 2001; Tjaden & Thoennes, 1998, 2000a, 2000b), mental health problems (e.g., Kemp et al., 1995), and even death at the hands of an intimate partner (Rennison, 2003). Thus, findings from both national and community-based surveys indicate that DV affects a significant percentage of women with great personal costs to physical and mental health. Mental Health Consequences of DV Battered women are at risk for developing PTSD, a syndrome developed to capture the psychological consequences of traumatic events, including DV (Ceballo, Ramirez, Castillo, Caballero, & Lozoff, 2004; Coker et al., 2002; Frasier et al., 2004; Houskarnp & Foy, 1991; Kemp et al., 1995; Kemp et al., 1991; Mertin & Mohr, 2000; Nixon et al., 2004; N011, Horowitz, Bonanno, Trickett, & Putnam, 2003; Pico-Alfonso, 2005; Sharhabani-Arzy, Amir, Kotler, & Liran, 2003; Stein & Kennedy, 2001; Stein, Kennedy, & Twamley, 2002). Rates of PTSD among battered women are high, ranging from 45% - 81% depending on sample characteristics, type of DV, and history of prior interpersonal trauma (Houskarnp & F oy, 1991; Kemp et al., 1995; Sharhabani-Arzy et al., 2003; Stein & Kennedy, 2001). A recent meta-analysis found that the weighted mean prevalence of PTSD among abused women was 64%, providing a combined prevalence estimate across 11 individual studies of PTSD (Golding, 1999). DV appears to be the greatest predictor of current PTSD symptomatology even after controlling for the effects of other interpersonal abuse experiences, including childhood abuse and non-partner related adulthood victimization (Pico-Alfonso, 2005). In addition, a limited number of studies suggest that frequency and severity of DV are related to risk and severity of PTSD (Cascardi & O'Leary, 1992; Houskarnp & Foy, 1991; Mertin & Mohr, 2000; Mitchell & Hodson, 1983; Pico-Alfonso, 2005). However, it is difficult to obtain reliable prevalence estimates of PTSD by DV severity because the majority of the DV literature does not report on severity of violence. Thus, rates of PTSD among women experiencing mild to moderate levels of DV are unclear. Finally, Bogat, Levendosky, Theran, von Eye, and Davidson (2003) found that chronic experiences of DV were related to higher PTSD symptoms scores than less chronic DV experiences, suggesting an effect of duration of abuse on PTSD symptomatology. Although PTSD accounts for some of the psychiatric consequences of DV, battered women experience co-morbid psychopathology and a range of psychiatric symptoms not captured in the PTSD construct, including depression, generalized anxiety, somatization, and dissociation. For example, a large number of studies consistently indicate that battered women report symptoms of depression (Cascardi, Langhinrichsen, & Vivian, 1992; Cascardi & O'Leary, 1992; Christopoulos et al., 1987; Coker et al., 2002; Doyle, Frank, Saltzman, McMahon, & Fielding, 1999; Frank & Dingle, 1999; Kemp et al., 1991; Mitchell & Hodson, 1983; Moore, Pepler, Mae, & Michele, 1989; Nixon et al., 2004; Olson et al., 2003; Stein & Kennedy, 2001; Wolfe, Zak, Wilson, & Jaffe, 1986). Clinical depression and PTSD are often co-morbid. For instance, Stein and Kennedy (2001) found that PTSD and major depressive disorder (MDD) co-occurred more often than would be expected by chance among women with recent experiences of DV recruited from DV agencies and medical clinics. Specifically, 43% of women with current DV-related PTSD also suffered from MDD. Similarly, Nixon et a1. (2004) found that PTSD and MDD co-occurred in 49% of a help-seeking sample of battered women with recent abuse experiences. Co-morbid PTSD and MDD diagnoses were more common than PTSD alone (27%). In addition, individuals diagnosed with both disorders reported more severe PTSD and MDD symptoms than individuals with PTSD alone (Nixon et al., 2004), indicating that severity of distress increases with multiple diagnoses. The literature on DV also reliably indicates that battered women experience more general symptoms of anxiety in addition to those subsumed under the PTSD diagnosis (Coker et al., 2002; Kemp et al., 1991; Moore et al., 1989; Olson et al., 2003; Wolfe et al., 1986), as well as somatic symptoms (Frasier et al., 2004; Lown & Vega, 2001). For example, Lown and Vega (2001) found that a community sample of battered women was more likely than a non-abused sample of women to report one or more significant somatic symptom(s) for five symptom groups: gastrointestinal, cardiopulmonary, neurological, sexual, and reproductive (Lown & Vega, 2001). To illustrate, women with histories of DV were twelve times more likely to report urinary retention, nine times more likely to report general pain, amnesia, and paralysis, and four to six times more likely to report sexual or reproductive symptoms, such as painful intercourse or menstruation, as compared to non-abused women (Lown & Vega, 2001). Other frequently cited somatic complaints among battered women include headaches; dizziness; gastrointestinal disturbances, such as abdominal pain, nausea, and indigestion; joint pain; numbness, tingling, or pain in the extremities; racing or pounding heartbeat; shortness of breath; and chest pain (Frasier et al., 2004; Lown & Vega, 2001). Though not well researched, one study suggests that DV may be related to dissociation. Dorahy, Lewis, and Wolfe (2007) compared 33 women who resided in a shelter for abused women to a sample of 33 non-abused women recruited from the general population in Northern Ireland. The researchers found that women in the DV group scored significantly higher on scales assessing dissociation as compared to women in the non-abused group. Furthermore, peritraumatic dissociative episodes (i.e., those occurring during domestic assaults) were related to higher on-going dissociative experiences. However, these results must be interpreted with caution as the DV group also reported more childhood maltreatment than the non-abused group of women. Thus, it is difficult to parse apart the effects of childhood maltreatment and DV on dissociation. In addition to placing battered women at risk for depression, anxiety, and dissociation, DV may lead to disruptions in personality and behavioral functioning, two areas of disturbance that are not well accounted for by PTSD. Specifically, DV may lead to alterations in personal identity, including the development of poor self-perceptions, and may place women at risk for future harm both self-inflicted and perpetrated by others. Battered women interviewed for psychiatric evaluation reported low self-esteem, as well as feeling incompetent, unworthy, and unlovable (Hilberman & Munson, 1978). Research studies support clinical work, indicating that battered women suffer from low self-esteem (D. G. Dutton & Painter, 1993; Kemp et al., 1991; Mitchell & Hodson, 1983). In addition, qualitative data and empirical research indicate that DV is related to feelings of shame and guilt (Hilberman & Munson, 1978; Street & Arias, 2001). Shame, which refers to global, negative evaluations of the self, may alter personal identity. Furthermore, clinical reports on battered women (Walker, 1984) and findings from a national sample of women (Umberson, Anderson, Glick, & Shapiro, 1998) suggest that victims of DV have a diminished sense of personal control, which results in feelings of powerlessness and helplessness. Preliminary findings also suggest a possible link between DV and personality disorders. Stanley and Penhale (1999) conducted a small pilot study of 13 mothers with 10 severe mental health problems whose children were involved in the child protection system. A detailed analysis of the nature of the mental health problems revealed that 6 of the women had been formally diagnosed with a personality disorder, and all 13 women had experienced a history of DV. Furthermore, Sansone, Reddington, Sky and Wiederman (2007) reported a relationship between borderline personality disorder and DV among a sample of female primary care patients. Specifically, 64.0% of abused women scored in the clinical range on a measure of borderline personality disorder, as compared to only 11.1% of non-abused women. These preliminary findings implicate DV as an important contextual factor for women’s mental health problems, particularly personality disorders. Poor self-esteem coupled with depression and anxiety may lead to self-harm in an attempt to manage intense painful affect or to self-validate negative perceptions of the self. Compared to nonabused women, battered women are more likely to engage in risky, damaging health behaviors, such as cigarette smoking (Frasier et al., 2004; Weaver & Etzel, 2003), and alcohol (Roberts, Lawrence, O'Toole, & Raphael, 1997) and substance abuse (Coker et al., 2002; Doyle et al., 1999; Plichta, 1996), and those who are more severely abused are more vulnerable to the negative effects of the unhealthy behaviors. For example, Weaver and Etzel (2003) found that almost 60% of a sample of severely battered women reported that they were current smokers. Among those current smokers, women who experienced more severe sexual violence, and greater domination and isolation were more likely to exhibit greater symptoms of nicotine-related physical dependence (Weaver & Etzel, 2003). Risky sexual behaviors, such as having multiple sexual partners and engaging in high-risk types of sexual activity have also been 11 observed in victims of DV (Champion, Shain, Piper, & Perdue, 2001 ). In addition to behaviors, sexually permissive attitudes and sexual preoccupation (e. g., positive attitudes toward pornography and thinking about sex frequently) were positively related to interpersonal victimization, including DV, in a study of young women (N 011 et al., 2003). These risky behaviors and attitudes are self-destructive, and may also indirectly increase the likelihood of encountering a perpetrator of assault through impaired judgment (in the case of substance use) and exposure to highly sexual situations (in the case of sexual behaviors and attitudes). In its extreme form, self-harm may manifest as suicidal ideation and behavior, a common sequel to DV. Research suggests that battered women are more likely to report suicidal ideation (Coker et al., 2002), as well as suicide attempts (Doyle et al., 1999; Frank & Dingle, 1999; Kernic, Wolf, & Holt, 2000; Olson et al., 2003; Roberts et al., 1997) than non-abused women. Specifically, battered women are five times more likely to attempt suicide than non-abused women (Stark & Flitcraft, 1996). In addition to self-harm, survivors of DV are at risk for interpersonal revictimization inflicted by others. Women involved in abusive romantic relationships are frequently exposed to other experiences of adult victimization (Pico-Alfonso, 2005). Pico-Alfonso (2005) found that female victims of DV had higher rates of non-partner related, adulthood physical, psychological, and sexual victimization than non-battered women. The poly-symptomatic psychiatric presentation, poor interpersonal functioning, and negative self-perceptions commonly observed among battered women, as well as research indicating that battered women are at risk for self-harm and other experiences of 12 interpersonal victimization suggests that PTSD does not sufficiently capture the psychological sequelae of DV for all abused women. Many battered women experience a range of mental health problems that exceed the re-experiencing, avoidance, and arousal symptoms of PTSD, and may co-occur. In order to fully account for the mental health outcomes documented among victims of DV and other repeated, interpersonal traumas that transcend the symptoms of PTSD, Herman (1992a; 1992b) proposed a psychological syndrome of repeated, prolonged, interpersonal trauma—complex posttraumatic stress disorder (CP). A Theoretical MOdel of Complex Posttraumatic Stress Disorder for DV Victims The current study proposes a theoretical model for the development of CP symptoms in women exposed to DV, and tests this model by examining longitudinal trajectories of DV victimization in relation to CP symptoms. This model includes a direct link between DV victimization and CP symptoms, and assumes that particular abuse characteristics and DV relationship length differentially affect the development of CP symptoms in abused women. Specifically, the model suggests that women who experience greater frequency, severity, and duration of abuse, as well as sexual abuse are at greater risk for developing CP symptoms than women who experience less frequent, chronic, and severe forms of abuse. The model further assumes that women involved in long-term DV relationships are more vulnerable to developing CP symptoms as compared to women in one or more short-term DV relationship(s). In addition, the model includes an indirect link from DV to CP through social support, suggesting that the development of CP symptoms is due, in part, to poor social support related to DV victimization. Finally, the model includes two moderator variables that increase 13 vulnerability to CP symptoms in response to DV: insecure adult representations of childhood attachment and experience of child maltreatment (see Figure 1). Direct Effect of DV on CP Symptoms The proposed model is integrative and draws on trauma and attachment theories to explain the development of CP symptoms in DV victims. First, in this model, DV has a direct effect on CP symptoms. According to Herman’s (1992a; 1992b) conceptualization of CP, the perpetrator uses systematic and repeated abuse to create a coercive relationship, in which the perpetrator becomes powerful and the victim has little power. Over time, the perpetrator’s use of coercive methods, which may include a combination of physical violence, sexual violence, threats of aggression, and/or psychological abuse, depletes the victim’s external resources (e. g., social support) and internal resources (e. g., initiative, feelings of control), ultimately resulting in a complex group of mental health symptoms characterized by behavioral and affect dysregulation (Herman, 1992a; 1992b). Impact of DV on interpersonal functioning and personal identity. The development of interpersonal and identity disturbances involves a complex process. The perpetrator uses violence and threats of aggression to create an environment of fear, in which the victim is afraid to assert power by not complying with the perpetrator’s wishes or following his rules. The perpetrator capitalizes on this environment and uses a variety of psychological abuse tactics to decrease the victim’s independence and social support, which further limits the victim’s power. For example, the perpetrator may limit the victim’s economic independence by making important financial decisions without consulting the victim or by restricting her access to money. The perpetrator may 14 decrease the victim’s social support by intercepting telephone calls and letters, monitoring his victim’s whereabouts, and ruining sentimental items, such as pictures and gifts that are symbolic of the victim’s connection to others. Thus, the perpetrator uses DV to undermine the victim’s sense of autonomy and to limit her social connections, forcing the victim to become dependent on the perpetrator for economic security, information, emotional sustenance, and in severe cases, bodily needs (Herman, 1992a). Over time, forced dependency on the perpetrator results in constriction of initiative, referred to as “passivity” and “learned helplessness” among battered women, as well as a bond of identification with the perpetrator called “traumatic bonding” (Herman, 1992a, 1992b; Walker, 1979). The victim possesses ambivalent feelings toward her perpetrator; she may fear him, and paradoxically, feel confused, worthless, and helpless without him (Walker, 1979). Eventually, the victim may view all relationships “through a lens of extremity” as she experiences her relationship with the perpetrator (Herman, 1992b, p. 385). This new understanding of relationships may lead to poor interpersonal functioning, in which the victim behaves ambivalently with others, alternating between extreme dependency and withdrawal. Impact of DV on mental health. In addition to interpersonal and identity disturbances, Herman’s (1992a; 1992b) conceptualization of CP suggests that victims of repeated, interpersonal trauma, including DV, suffer from a range of psychiatric symptoms, including somatization, dissociation, and depression. According to Herman (1992a; 1992b), the experience of prolonged, repeated, and unpredictable violence, such as DV, results in the chronic expectation of danger, which in turn, causes hyperarousal— chronic arousal of the nervous system. Hyperarousal leads to observable somatic 15 complaints (physical or bodily problems that defy medical explanation) including insomnia, headaches, and gastrointestinal problems, as well as irritability and agitation (Grinker & Spiegel, 1945; Herman, 1992a, 1992b; Kardiner & Spiegel, 1947). The need to cope with overwhelming affect associated with abuse activates dissociation, a basic psychological defense mechanism that impairs capacity for cognitive integration (Terr, 1991). Dissociation is initially an adaptable, conscious form of self- hypnosis employed to mentally avoid misery and pain, as information about traumatic experiences is disconnected, disjointed, and relegated to separate aspects of consciousness preventing interference with everyday level of consciousness (Luxenberg, Spinazzola, & van der Kolk, 2001). However, with repeated abuse, the dissociative process becomes automatic, involuntary, and relatively fixed (Terr, 1991). Prolonged use of dissociation may result in amnesia, depersonalization, concentration and attention difficulties, and a disrupted sense of time. In its extreme form, dissociation may lead deformations of identity such as those characteristic of dissociative identity disorder (Herman, 1992a, 1992b). Abuse and betrayal at the hands of an intimate partner (one who has theoretically committed to loving and protecting the victim) may disrupt the victim’s spiritual faith and belief systems. This is particularly true when others are aware of the abuse and do not intervene appropriately, allowing the violence to continue, as well as when others engage in victim-blaming or denial of the abuse. Information that is incompatible with previously formed belief systems results in cognitive and affective changes, most prominently a state of depression (Herman, 1992a, 1992b; Rorty & Yager, 1996). In addition, the internalization of unexpressed anger toward the perpetrator (which the 16 victim is unable to express appropriately for fear of retribution) may manifest as symptoms of depression, low self-worth, and in extreme cases, deliberate self-harm, such as self-mutilation and suicidal preoccupation (Herman, 1992b). Risk for repeated harm among DV victims. Finally, Herman’s (1992a; 1992b) CP theory suggests that the self-defeating behaviors associated with internalized anger (i.e., self-injurious behavior and suicidality), as well as other forms of compulsive self-harm, including alcohol and substance use and risky sexual behavior may represent attempts to manage painful affects by numbing or distracting the self from emotional pain. It is also possible that self-harming behaviors serve to communicate pain to others, and/or serve a self-validating function by confirming negative beliefs about the self (e. g., “I am bad or evil,” “I deserve to be punished”). Unfortunately, these coping strategies are maladaptive and may lead to impaired judgment (in the case of alcohol and substance use) and involvement in dangerous situations (in the case of risky sexual behavior) which increase vulnerability to harm perpetrated by others (e. g., stranger perpetrated harm and involvement in repeated abusive relationships). Vulnerability to self-inflicted harm and harm inflicted by others may also represent unconscious behavioral reenactments of traumatic experiences, in which the victim unsuccessfully attempts to achieve cognitive and affective mastery over the trauma (Freud, 1995; Herman, 1992a, 1992b). Although Herman’s (1992a; 1992b) formulation of CP describes how the perpetrator comes to control his victim and break down her psychological resistance, resulting in pathological relationship functioning, identity disturbances, and multiple psychiatric symptoms, CP theory does not fully explain why victims of repeated, prolonged abuse are vulnerable to repeated harm perpetrated by others. Two additional 17 theoretical perspectives on trauma provide support for the link between DV and the proclivity to reenact abusive relationships, a core symptom of CP. Specifically, a constructivist theory of trauma suggests that individuals develop constructs or templates to understand and predict their own behavior as well as the behavior of others (Harter & Neimeyer, 1995). Women living in DV relationships develop idiosyncratic constructions of relationships based on their experiences with an abusive partner. Therefore, non- abusive relationships may lie outside of the abused women’s range of constructions useful for anticipating behavior. As a result, abused women may unwittingly choose similar abusive partners because the consequences of such relationships, although negative, are at least predictable (Harter & Neimeyer, 1995). Alternatively, in her formulation of a dynamic-maturational approach to trauma, Crittenden (1997) suggests that the victim of DV learns after repeated failures to protect herself from abuse that her actions are futile and that she is incapable of keeping her physical or psychological self safe. At this point, the woman becomes completely vulnerable to repeated harm because she has reached a state of learned helplessness in which all efforts at self-protection have ceased. Overall, trauma theories (Crittenden, 1997; Freud, 1995; Harter & Neimeyer, 1995; Herman, 1992a, 1992b; Terr, 1991; Walker, 1979, 1984) suggest a direct link between DV and CP symptoms. However, not all women share the same experiences of DV, and different experiences of abuse may differentially influence CP symptoms. In her formulation of CP, Herman (1992a; 1992b) does not articulate how the specific configuration of DV and other coercive techniques may vary across batterers and contribute to differential levels or patterns of CP symptoms among abused women. In 18 contrast, the proposed model suggests that certain abuse characteristics (i.e., DV severity, frequency, duration, type, and partnership) differentially affect the development of CP symptoms in response to DV, and draws on other trauma theories to support such hypotheses. Differential effects of abuse characteristics on CP. Specifically, severity of DV is one abuse characteristic in the proposed model that influences the development of CP symptoms, such that women with patterns of DV characterized by severe levels of abuse develop more CP symptoms as compared to women with less severe trajectories of abuse. The development of a coercive relationship, which ultimately undermines mental health, is most likely to occur when abuse is severe. Women who experience severe DV have few options to escape perpetrator control due to the risk of serious danger. Insubordination, attempts to escape, or any exertion of power on behalf of the victim may result in serious bodily injury or death when the abuse is severe. Therefore, severe abuse may be an effective method of achieving and maintaining control over the victim. Likewise, frequency of abuse differentially influences the development of CP symptoms in the proposed model. Women who experience frequent DV may learn through punishment that any behavior that is not sanctioned by the perpetrator is likely to result in abuse. Women in this situation are at the mercy of the perpetrator because protection of physical safety requires submission. In addition, when abuse is frequent, the victim lives in a constant state of fear and physiological arousal resulting in high levels of somatization, and may employ dissociation on a regular basis in an attempt to escape psychological and physical pain. Women who experience frequent DV may feel completely helpless because any initiative of self will almost indefinitely result in abuse. 19 Feelings of ineffectiveness may result in'a state of learned helplessness, leaving women vulnerable to future harm because all efforts at self-protection cease (Crittenden, 1997). In contrast, if violence is relatively infrequent, women may be able to assert more power and take more “risks” because assertiveness rarely results in DV. Thus, in cases of infrequent abuse, women may be able to resist perpetrator control. Type of DV is another abuse characteristic relevant to the development of CP in the proposed model. Sexual abuse may place women at relatively greater risk for the development of CP than other types of abuse because sexual abuse represents the most intimate form of betrayal and an assault on reproductive control. Thus, in addition to representing an intimate invasion of the self, sexual abuse undermines a primary function of human evolution (Crittenden, 1997), leaving sexually abused women powerless to control even the most basic human function. Furthermore, the proposed model suggests that duration of DV is a characteristic of abuse, which influences the development of CP symptoms. The CP construct was developed to reflect the impact of traumatic experiences, which are “prolonged” in nature without a well-defined beginning or ending, in contrast to psychological disorder resulting from a single traumatic event. According to trauma theory, a perpetrator establishes a relationship of coercive control, which ultimately leads to CP symptoms, over time with repeated use of coercive techniques (Herman, 1992a, 1992b). For example, the process of social isolation occurs over time through the gradual implementation of restrictive rules preventing social contact with others. Finally, the proposed model suggests that relationship length with the perpetrator is relevant to the development of CP symptoms. Herman’s (1992a; 1992b) CP theory 20 implies that a relationship of coercive control and the resulting CP symptoms develop over time with a single perpetrator. Although the perpetrator may be able to create fear in his victim after one abusive incident, autonomy, social support, and spiritual faith, for example, do not deteriorate considerably after one episode of abuse. The creation of a relationship of coercive control and the eroding of the victim’s psychological resistance is a gradual process. Involvement in multiple short DV relationships where subordination to coercive control never begins is less likely to result in CF symptoms. Thus, the current study examines longitudinal trajectories of DV based on DV severity, frequency, type, and duration in relation to CP symptoms separately for women in long-term DV relationships (defined here as three or more consecutive years) and for women in one or more short-term DV relationship(s). Social Isolation as an Explanatory F actor for the Development of CP Although limited data and sample size preclude a test of all of the mechanisms underlying the theoretical relationship between DV exposure and CP symptoms, the current research examines the process of social isolation, and its role in the DV-CP symptom link. Specifically, the proposed model suggests that social support partially mediates the relationship between DV victimization and CP symptoms. According to trauma theory (Herman, 1992a; 1992b), the DV perpetrator decreases his victim’s social contacts and support through the use of psychological abuse, such as discouraging visits with family and fiiends. Because the victim is isolated from others, the perpetrator becomes the victim’s primary source of support. As dependency on the perpetrator increases, the victim may view the perpetrator as both a source of fear and a source of solace. Eventually, the victim may generalize this ambivalence to other relationships, 21 resulting in unhealthy interpersonal functioning, in which the victim alternates between dependency and withdrawal in relationships with others, a primary disturbance characteristic of CP (Herman, 1992a, 1992b). In addition, as the victim’s social support deteriorates, she is less likely to have the external resources to leave a violent relationship, making her vulnerable to continued abuse and its direct mental health consequences, such as somatization and dissociation. Finally, the deterioration of social support reinforces the isolation and withdrawal symptoms of depression. Thus, DV leads to social isolation, which in turn, affects the woman’s mental health. In this study, social support is examined over time to test whether the process of social isolation occurs in victims of DV, and thus, whether poor social support can be considered a reasonable indicator of social isolation and examined as a mediator in DV-CP symptom link. Personal Vulnerability F actors for the Development of CP In addition to testing for mediation, the model addresses individual differences in risk for developing CP symptoms in response to DV. Herman’s (1992a; 1992b) conceptualization of CP does not address individual differences in vulnerability to CP symptoms. However, characteristics of traumatic events, as well as characteristics of the individual likely interact to influence adaptation to trauma. Perhaps the failure to address personal vulnerability to CP symptoms was a deliberate decision to emphasize that psychological problems experienced by trauma victims are effects, not causes of abuse. Nonetheless, preexisting individual differences certainly contribute to the particular psychological response to abuse. As a result, the proposed model includes two factors suggested by attachment theory that may influence personal vulnerability to trauma 22 exposure. such a maltreatment hi .\ Adult ref that an individua develops in relati risk for psychopa .\'lohr.2001). Spc vulnerability to tr: According by an innate dispt‘ system is aCIIVaIL‘l illness. and by ext the primary attach and inner security Theoretica available and SCUK exposure, such as DV: adult representations of childhood attachment and child maltreatment history. Adult representations of childhood attachment. First, attachment theory suggests that an individual’s strategy for responding to conditions of danger, which originally develops in relation to the primary caregiver, may be related to individual differences in risk for psychopathology in combination with other risk factors, such as DV (Cassidy & Mohr, 2001). Specifically, an insecure attachment organization may create psychological vulnerability to trauma exposure, including DV. According to Bowlby (1969), the founder of attachment theory, infants are driven by an innate disposition to form an attachment system with primary caregivers. This system is activated under conditions of danger and distress by internal cues, such as illness, and by external threats to safety, which drive the infant to seek protection from the primary attachment figure. Caregiver responses determine the infant’s sense of safety and inner security. Theoretically, infants with caregivers who are physically and emotionally available and sensitive develop primary, secure attachment strategies. In situations of threat, securely attached infants seek caregiver protection and then return to exploration when the distress or threat has passed (Main, 1990). However, infants with unresponsive, unavailable, or insensitive caregivers adopt secondary defensive behavioral strategies for gaining caregiver access and preserving the attachment relationship. Insecure infants either deactivate the attachment system, shifting attention away from a rejecting caregiver and minimizing displays of distress to avoid pain and conflict within the attachment 23 svstem. 0f hyper; insistent demand> Early atta mental representa the attacllmsmI fi "”3 behavior in subse interpersonal relati attachment relatior as a relatively stab In addition interpersonal beha regulation ('Cassid; in the contexr of a Intemalize self-re t‘ : about internal caln giving bond derail attachm system, or hyperactivate the attachment system with exaggerated displays of distress and insistent demands for care and attention to maintain caregiver contact (Main, 1990). Early attachment experiences with the primary caregiver crystallize into internal mental representations or working models representing the relationship among the self, the attachment figure, and the external world. Internal working models guide social behavior in subsequent relationships, and affect expectations and perceptions about future interpersonal relationships throughout the lifespan (Bowlby, 1977, 1980). Thus, the early attachment relationship plays a vital role in the development of a sense of self and serves as a relatively stable template for future relationships. In addition to influencing an understanding of the interpersonal realm and shaping interpersonal behavior, early attachment experiences affect the development of emotion regulation (Cassidy & Mohr, 2001; van-der-Kolk & F isler, 1994). Children who develop in the context of a supportive relationship with the primary caregiver gradually internalize self-regulating functions initially provided by the caregiver designed to bring about internal calm under conditions of stress. However, disruption of the early care- giving bond derails this critical developmental process. Individuals with a damaged attachment system may not successfully learn how to internally regulate affective impulses or engage in adaptive self-soothing behaviors. In response to situations of overwhelming affect, such as trauma exposure, these individuals may be forced to rely on basic defenses, including dissociation, or engage in dangerous external behaviors, such as self-mutilation or sexual promiscuity in attempt to modulate affect (Cassidy & Mohr, 2001; van-der-Kolk & Fisler, 1994). Therefore, a damaged attachment system disrupts essential developmental processes, including the development of healthy emotion and 24 behavior regular 1 theoretically plat especially in con attachment is a r affective. relatio: attachment stratc emergence of p53 insecure attachm a predisposing in | Child ma; (defined here as l neglect experieng mOderator of the 1993a 1993b) su child maltreatmer such as DV; how: experience of earl behavior regulation, adaptive coping strategies, and mature interpersonal functioning, theoretically placing a child at risk for short- and long-term psychological problems, especially in combination with other risk factors (Bowlby, 1977). As a result, disrupted attachment is a pathway to diverse forms of psychopathology, including a range of affective, relational, and behavioral disturbances captured by CP. Therefore, an insecure attachment strategy may be a risk factor for sensitivity to later risk conditions and the emergence of psychopathology, including CP symptoms. The present research examines insecure attachment based on adult recollections of childhood care-giving experiences as a predisposing individual-level factor for CP symptoms in relation to DV. Child maltreatment history. Finally, in this model, child maltreatment history (defined here as physical, sexual, and emotional abuse, and physical and emotional neglect experienced during childhood and adolescence) is prOposed as a possible moderator of the hypothesized DV-CP symptom relationship. Trauma theory (Herman, 1992a, 1992b) suggests that the experience of prolonged, interpersonal trauma, including child maltreatment, renders victims vulnerable to repeated experiences of later abuse, such as DV; however, trauma theory does not conceptually address how and why the experience of early abuse may affect psychological response to later abuse. Attachment theory provides some insight into this shortcoming of trauma theory. Specifically, attachment theory suggests that disrupted attachment to the primary caregiver, as often occurs in situations of child maltreatment (D. Barnett, Ganiban, & Cicchetti, 1999; E. A. Carlson, 1998), derails the critical developmental process of learning to internally modulate physiological arousal and affective states. As noted above, failure to internalize self-regulating functions forces the individual to rely on basic 25 defenses. such a substance abuse early reliance or development of experiences of c negatively affect many individual: in which the indi coping strategies of the attachmen psychologically poor coping mec more adaptive st context in which Effects of later e: the proposed Stu DV-related CP 5 defenses, such as dissociation, and external, maladaptive coping mechanisms, such as substance abuse, to calm internal discomfort (van-der-Kolk & F isler, 1994). In addition, early reliance on external coping strategies and immature defenses may prevent the development of more psychologically sophisticated defensive strategies. Thus, experiences of child maltreatment may disrupt the attachment system, which in turn, negatively affects capacity to regulate emotions. Furthermore, Gold (2000) asserts that many individuals who experience child maltreatment also grow up in ineffective families in which the individual does not learn necessary skills for living, including adaptive coping strategies for modulating emotions. Poor emotional regulation, whether the result of the attachment process or a deficient learning environment, may leave individuals psychologically vulnerable to both current and future traumatic experiences, as the use of poor coping mechanisms and basic defensive strategies becomes automatic over time and more adaptive strategies are not learned. Thus, early experiences of abuse and the context in which it occurs may make an individual more sensitive to the psychological effects of later experiences of DV, increasing the risk of DV-related CP symptoms. In the proposed study child maltreatment is examined as a potential vulnerability factor for DV-related CP symptoms. By specifying the relationships among abuse characteristics and CP, and including a potential mediator and two potential moderators of the hypothesized DV-CP symptom link, the current research agenda was designed to add to a theoretical literature on CP, particularly in relation to DV. This model is supported by trauma and attachment theories, as well as a growing body of empirical literature linking previously isolated mental health symptoms, which can be understood in the context of a traumatic reaction 26 to repeated. prob attributed to mull A handful providing evident trauma confirmin reexperiencing. a psychological im Several studies e: of the DSM-Il ' fr among a combin. adolescents and z 1997; van der K( For exam dissociation. son‘ to repeated, prolonged interpersonal abuse, including DV, rather than erroneously attributed to multiple psychological conditions and enduring personality factors. Evidence for a Complex PT SD Syndrome A handful of empirical studies have examined CP among trauma victims, providing evidence for a CP syndrome in victims of repeated, prolonged interpersonal trauma confirming Herman’s (1992a; 1992b) theoretical proposition that the reexperiencing, arousal, and avoidance symptoms of PTSD do not adequately capture the psychological impact of interpersonal trauma exhibited by a subset of individuals. Several studies examining complex human adaptation to trauma were conducted as a part of the DSM-I V field trial for PTSD, a multisite investigation of post-trauma pathology among a combination of treatment-seeking (n=395) and community recruited (n=125) adolescents and adults (Newman, Riggs, & Roth, 1997; Pelcovitz et al., 1997; Roth et al., 1997; van der Kolk et al., 1996). For example, van der Kolk and colleagues (1996) investigated PTSD in relation to dissociation, somatization, and affect dysregulation—the primary psychiatric symptoms of CP. Correlations revealed that PTSD and its associated features are highly related, and to a greater extent than may be suggested in the DSM-I V (APA, 1994). Participants with a diagnosis of lifetime PTSD scored significantly lower on scales assessing dissociation, somatization, and affect dysregulation than those with current PTSD, but significantly higher than those who never had PTSD. Subjects who developed PTSD in response to interpersonal trauma (physical and/or sexual abuse) in adolescence or adulthood had significantly fewer associated features than those who endured childhood abuse, but significantly more than those who experienced disaster. Thus, PTSD, dissociation, 27 somatization. 3 characteriSIiCS ( trauma (van de’ Based o fifty trauma EXT structured inten SIDES) to operz Kolk et al. (199! several field tria studies generally victims of early- or before age 13 example, Pelcov victims and 38° c compared to onl; diagnosed among stressors that did FIEld trial example’ Netvma somatization, and affect dysregulation often occur together, in part as a function of abuse characteristics (i.e., age of onset, nature of trauma), representing a complex response to trauma (van der Kolk et al., 1996). Based on a systematic review of the literature and contributions from a survey of fifty trauma experts, Pelcovitz and colleagues (1997) developed a criterion set and structured interview (i.e., the Structured Interview for Disorders of Extreme Stress: SIDES) to operationally define the complex symptom constellation described by van der Kolk et al. (1996) and Herman (1992a; 1992b). The SIDES was subsequently used in several field trial studies to examine the utility of the CP construct. Findings from these studies generally indicate that CP is a syndrome specific to trauma that primarily affects victims of early-onset interpersonal abuse (i.e., physical and/or sexual abuse beginning at or before age 13: Newman et al., 1997; Pelcovitz et al., 1997; Roth et al., 1997). For example, Pelcovitz et al. (1997) reported that 63% of early-onset interpersonal abuse victims and 38% of late-onset interpersonal abuse victims met criteria for CP, as compared to only 10% of those who witnessed a disaster. Furthermore, CP was rarely diagnosed among non-trauma exposed individuals (3% among individuals exposed to stressors that did not qualify for DSM-I V PTSD criteria). Field trial studies also suggest that CP is highly co-morbid with PTSD. For example, Newman (1997) reported that 53% of trauma-exposed individuals who met criteria for current PTSD also met criteria for CP. The rate of co-morbid PTSD and CP increased to 72% when victims of interpersonal abuse were examined separetely from individuals who experienced other types of trauma (Roth et al., 1997). Results also 28 indicate that C I’ the Nevvman et L Addition, ofthe current P’l lie. the inability current lives) as t (that individuals \v thematic disruptir trauma; interpersc Cerrience of a m e.‘tperience and the subset (22:50) oftl symptoms were or {1 . ndrng suggests th CP construct may 1 Category (Newman Findings frr abuse differentially of sexually abused of PTSD also met c indicate that CP typically occurs in conjuction with PTSD; less than 3% of participants in the Newman et al. (1997) study were diagnosed with CP alone. Additional findings from Newman et al.’s (1997) study speak to the heterogeneity of the current PTSD construct. Individuals with CP exhibited greater thematic disruption (i.e., the inability to resolve trauma-related issues and create adaptive meaning in their current lives) as compared to those with pure PTSD and those without PTSD, suggesting that individuals with CP are the most pervasively affected by trauma. Differences in thematic disruption among the three groups appeared to be a function of the nature of the trauma; interpersonal abuse was associated with greater thematic disruption than was the experience of a non-interpersonal stressor, controlling for age of first traumatic experience and the number of traumatic experiences. Further analyses performed on a subset (n=50) of the same sample indicated that severity of intrusive and avoidant symptoms were greatest in the group of individuals with co-morbid PTSD and CP. This finding suggests that those with CP represent the most severe cases of PTSD. Thus, the CP construct may represent a marker of severity, rather than a distinct diagnostic category (Newman et al., 1997). Findings from a final DSM-I V field trial study suggest that type of interpersonal abuse differentially affects risk for CP. Specifically, Roth et a1. (1997) reported that 76% of sexually abused women (regardless of physical abuse status) with a lifetime diagnosis of PTSD also met criteria for lifetime CP, as compared to 53% of those women who reported physical abuse only. Multiple regression analyses revealed that women who reported both sexual and physical abuse were 14.5 times more likely to meet a diagnosis of CP than those who had not experienced both types of abuse, suggesting a cumulative 29 impact of abuse . compared to no women. especial higher risk of de CP occurred at a abuse demonstra“ Given the CP S-‘mPIOmS e) dEGmF‘ Dickin: tit-omen in prim; childhood Sexu; the research t ea suffered from s and averaged n CSA and dUrat: and family hist impact of abuse. In addition, sexual abuse resulted in a risk of CP 4.4 times as great as compared to women who experienced physical abuse only. Thus, sexually abused women, especially those who had also experienced physical abuse, had a significantly higher risk of developing CP than those who experienced physical abuse alone; though, CP occurred at a high base rate among physically abused women (27%). Taken together, these results suggest that physical and sexual abuse are risk factors for CP, with sexual abuse demonstrating greater specificity. Given the specific risk for CP associated with sexual abuse, two studies examined CP symptoms exclusively among sexual abuse survivors. For example, Dickinson, deGruy, Dickinson, and Candib (1998) provided evidence of a CP syndrome among women in primary care outpatient settings with a history of sexual abuse, including childhood sexual abuse (CSA) and/or adult sexual assault (n=99). Using cluster analysis, the research team identified a homogenous subgroup of sexual abuse victims who suffered fi'om symptoms of depression, PTSD, dissociation, and somatoform disorder, and averaged more than six psychiatric diagnoses. Furthermore, increased severity of CSA and duration of abuse, within family perpetration, greater number of perpetrators, and family history of antisocial behavior were associated with greater overall symptomatology. This symptom profile may be better characterized by CP, as opposed to a plethora of distinct, discrete, apparently unrelated disorders. Building on these findings, McLean and Gallop (2003) examined the full CP construct among a small sample (n=65) of adult female CSA survivors obtaining services from urban mental health clinics in Canada. The researchers found a significant degree of overlap between CP and borderline personality disorder with virtually all women 30 meeting criteria significantly hill lie. abuse occu. years of age). su- borderline persoi onset of abuse al. higher lifetime re onset group. mak the development revealed that part Incidents) were 5 In a more SimPIOms amon experiencing CU} Sample; hOWQVC adolescence exh features of (11330 absence of PTS I meeting criteria for both disorders. Furthermore, diagnoses of both disorders were significantly higher among participants reporting early- versus late-onset sexual abuse (i.e., abuse occurring at or prior to 12 years of age versus abuse occurring at or beyond 13 years of age), suggesting a relationship between early abuse and co-morbid CP and borderline personality disorder. However, the group of women who experienced an early onset of abuse also reported higher paternal rates of abuse, no single incidents of abuse, higher lifetime revictimization, and higher biparental neglect as compared to the late- onset group, making it difficult to disentangle the relative contribution of these factors to the development of CP and borderline personality disorder. A separate set of analyses revealed that paternal sexual abuse and frequency of abuse (i.e., experiencing 10 or more incidents) were significant predictors of diagnostic status. In a more recent study, Ford, Stockton, Kaltman, and Green (2006) assessed CP symptoms among a community sample of female college students who were not experiencing current trauma. Findings suggest that full CP is rare among a community sample; however, women who experienced interpersonal trauma during childhood or adolescence exhibited elevated levels of CP symptoms independent of PTSD (i.e., the CP features of dissociation, altered relationships, and systems of meaning occurred in the absence of PTSD: 32-43% discordance). Furthermore, women who experienced repeated interpersonal trauma reported more CP symptoms than women who experienced a single incident of non-interpersonal or interpersonal trauma, and women who experienced a single incident interpersonal trauma reported more CP symptoms than women who experienced non-interpersonal trauma or no trauma. Generally, few CP symptoms were reported by women with no trauma history. 31 These fr syndrome speci interpersonal in a severe form o= constitute a dist: response to traut by acute stress d between. Finally. '. confirmation fo “psychometric 1 (MCMI) among (p.289). C onsi included a com Chronic depresj Profound isolat directed aggreg increased Sym] Other t)‘lths of r levels (J_ G A' These findings are consistent with field trial studies in suggesting that CP is a syndrome specific to trauma, and that risk for CP is greater when abuse is frequent and interpersonal in nature. However, unlike field trial studies that suggest that CP represents a severe form of PTSD (e.g., Newman et al., 1997), these findings suggest that CP may constitute a distinct posttraumatic reaction to interpersonal abuse. Thus, the human response to trauma may be more accurately conceptualized along a continuum anchored by acute stress disorder at one end, CP at the other end, and simple PTSD somewhere in between. Finally, literature on personality assessment of trauma victims provides additional confirmation for a CP syndrome. Allen, Coyne, and Huntoon (1998) delineated a “psychometric prototype of CP” based on the Million Clinical Multiaxial Inventory-III (MCMI) among a sample of psychiatric inpatient females with childhood abuse histories (p.289). Consistent with Herman’s (1992a; 1992b) formulation of CP, this prototype included a complex pattern of symptomatic and personality disturbances: severe and chronic depression and anxiety, high prevalence of somatization, low self-esteem, and profound isolation coupled with insecure attachment, masochism, dependency, and self- directed aggression. In addition, CSA demonstrated the strongest relationship to increased symptom scores on the clinical syndrome scales of the MCMI as compared to other types of childhood abuse, and duration of abuse predicted higher clinical symptom levels (J. G. Allen et al., 1998). Another study on the same sample of women revealed five distinct clusters based on MCMI personality disorder scales: four clusters exhibited personality profiles consistent with CP and one showed an absence of personality disturbance (J. G. Allen, 32 Huntoon. 8; EV relative promin symptomatic at most severel," C and dependenc lovv self-esteen substance use. Allen et al.. 19 exhibit signifii resilience. Overal childhood trat PTSD, and fit support the e) imerpersonal develODment severity of p— also Suggem j that Cp can ( How, Primarily Ca addition, a ll general Were Huntoon, & Evans, 1999). Although the four disturbed clusters were distinguished by the relative prominence of particular personality problems, all the groups demonstrated symptomatic and personality disturbances captured by the descriptive label of CP. The most severely disturbed cluster of women showed a pattern of depression, masochism, and dependency (as did women in the remaining disturbed clusters), profound isolation, low self-esteem, paranoia, dissociation, aggression, PTSD, anxiety, somatization, substance use, psychoticism, poor coping skills, and poor interpersonal functioning (J. G. Allen etal., 1999). The identification of a subgroup of abused women who did not exhibit significant psychopathology in response to trauma may be indicative of personal resilience. Overall, results from studies examining the long-term impact of sexual abuse or childhood trauma, those from studies conducted as a part of the DSM—I V field trial for PTSD, and findings from the personality assessment of victims of childhood abuse support the existence of a CP syndrome among victims of repeated, prolonged interpersonal abuse. Several abuse characteristics appear to be related to the development of CP, such as age of onset, severity, duration, and type of abuse, as well as severity of PTSD symptoms and experiencing more than one type of abuse. Findings also suggest high rates of co-occurring PTSD and CP, though there is some indication that CP can occur in the absence of PTSD. However, these studies have several limitations, including the reliance on primarily Caucasian, treatment-seeking samples and retrospective reports of abuse. In addition, all but one of the studies examining CP among interpersonal abuse victims in general were based on the same sample of women—the field trial’s sample—or a subset 33 of that larger sample. These limitations may restrict the external validity of findings and introduce the possibility of recall bias. Furthermore, relationships between perpetrators and victims were not specified in any of the studies, with the exception of the McLean and Gallop (2003) study, in which paternal sexual abuse was identified as a risk variable. Thus, the extent to which battered women were included in the Dickinson et al. (1998) study of sexual victimization or the field trial sample is unclear, making it difficult to determine the applicability of these findings to victims of DV. Although a consensus is emerging in the literature suggesting the integrity of CP as a diagnostic construct for understanding the psychological responses of victims of repeated, prolonged interpersonal trauma, particularly CSA victims, Herman’s (1992a; 1992b) formulation of CP does not focus exclusively on sexual abuse. Rather, CP can theoretically result from a wide-range of repeated, prolonged interpersonal abuse experiences, including DV (Herman, 1992a). The present study is the first to examine CP symptoms among a community sample of battered women from relatively diverse backgrounds with a range of lifetime DV experiences. Specifically, this study examines CP symptoms in relation to various longitudinal patterns of DV victimization based on several theoretically- and empirically-derived abuse characteristics (e. g., DV severity). Furthermore, symptoms are examined separately for women in long and short-term DV relationships to address the relevance of DV partnership in the development of CP symptoms. The current study examines the full range of CP symptoms using a set of criteria specifically designed to assess the CP syndrome (CP symptoms are reported rather than diagnostic status given that the sample is community-based and fewer women are likely to meet clinical thresholds than would be expected among treatment-seeking 34 (n In samples). This study is designed to provide information about how individual differences in DV victimization, including whether the violence occurs in the context of one or multiple violent relationships, relate to CP symptoms, further specifying a theoretical model for the development of CP in battered women. Although the CP construct as a whole has not yet been examined among battered women specifically, clinical and empirical literature regarding the impact of DV on women suggest the presence of symptoms consistent with the constellation of symptomatic, relational, and behavioral disturbances subsumed under the CP nomenclature with frequency, severity, and duration of abuse predicting severity of mental health symptoms (see above for review of Mental Health Consequences of DV). In addition to noting symptoms consistent with the CP syndrome among abused women, studies of DV indicate that battered women report more structural and subjective dependency on the perpetrator, view themselves as powerlessness, and experience greater social isolation as compared to their non-battered counterparts (Farris & F enaughty, 2002; Forte, F ranks, Forte, & Rigsby, 1996; Rokach, 2006). For example, Farris and F enaughty (2002) found that physically abused women reported that their partners discouraged friendships and leaving the home, which resulted in social isolation. These findings provide some support for potential explanatory processes underlying the relationship between prolonged interpersonal abuse and CP proposed by Herman (1992a; 1992b) in her theoretical conceptualization of CP. Although limited data and sample size prevent the examination of each of these potential mechanisms underlying the DV-CP link, the current study examines the process of social isolation, and the potential mediating role of social support in the hypothesized DV-CP symptom relationship. 35 relationsl D\-’ on sr levendo consistcr tempura Carlson. severity member support; then di &th For 6x3; “Omen dining 1 qUality When [I Writro] elamir infOIm SUPDOr Social Support Previous empirical studies have not examined the role of social support in the relationship between DV and CP; however, several studies find a direct negative effect of DV on social support (0. W. Barnett, Martinez, & Keyson, 1996; Curry & Harvey, 1998; Levendosky et al., 2004; Mitchell & Hodson, 1983, 1986; Thompson et al., 2000) consistent with the proposed theoretical model of CP, with only one study finding comparable levels of social support between battered and non-battered women (B. E. Carlson, McNutt, Choi, & Rose, 2002). Generally, studies indicate that frequency and severity of DV is related to poor social support, such as few social support network members; low perceived social support, including emotional, informational, and practical support; and decreased likelihood of receiving social support from friends and relatives when disclosing abuse (Matud-Aznar, Aguilera-Avila, Marrero-Quevedo, Moraza-Pulla, & Carballeira-Abella, 2003; Mitchell & Hodson, 1983, 1986; Thompson et al., 2000). For example, El-Bassel, Gilbert, Rajah, F oleno, and Frye (2001) found that abused women reported few individuals to turn to for support, experienced insufficient support during their relationships with abusive partners, and expressed dissatisfaction with the quality of support they had received. These women also considered the threat of DV when making choices about seeking support. Although study findings suggest that DV perpetrators may use dominance and control to isolate women and prevent them from accessing social support, these studies examined social support at one point in time. Cross-sectional studies do not provide information on the direction of causality; thus, it is possible that women with poor social support are more likely to experience DV, rather than DV precipitating loss in support. 36 Furu test ‘ over viol: SUP; med in tt rela sup maj abu One hea rel; Ion In: hea Furthermore, examining social support at one point in time does not permit researchers to test whether social support deteriorates in response to DV. Examination of social support over time is necessary to determine whether the process of social isolation occurs in violent relationships. Thus, the current study examines longitudinal trajectories of social support in relation to DV victimization. Furthermore, social support is examined as a mediator in the DV-CP symptom link, such that DV predicts poor social support, which in turn, is related to CP symptoms. Although prior studies have not examined the role of social support in the relationship between DV and CP, several prior studies have examined the role of social support in the relationship between DV and other symptoms characteristic of CP. The majority of these studies have treated social support as a moderating variable, buffering abused women from the negative effects of DV (e.g., Levendosky et al., 2004); however, one study has examined social support as an intervening variable between DV and mental health outcomes. Thompson et al. (2000) found that social support mediated the relationship between DV and psychological distress and PTSD symptoms in a sample of low-income, African American women seeking routine medical care at an urban hospital (n=l 38). Findings from this study suggest that the negative effects of DV on mental health outcomes may be explained by low levels of social support. Other studies of DV, social support, and mental health outcomes also support the possibility of a mediation model. Several studies find a direct effect of DV on social support (e.g., Levendosky et al., 2004: see above for review), and an association between social support and mental health outcomes in the context of DV (e. g., Coker, Watkins, Smith, & Brandt, 2003). Studies examining the relationship between social support and 37 mental health symptoms in battered women generally indicate a positive association between support and psychological well-being, and a negative association between support and mental health problems captured by CP (Coker et al., 2002; Coker et al., 2003; Kemp et al., 1995; Tan, Basta, Sullivan, & Davidson, 1995; Thompson et al., 2000). Specifically, studies have linked structural and functional social support to general levels of psychological functioning (Thompson et al., 2000), depression (Anderson, Saunders, Yoshihama, Bybee, & Sullivan, 2003; B. E. Carlson et al., 2002; Fowler & Hill, 2004; Mitchell & Hodson, 1983; Tan et al., 1995), anxiety (B. E. Carlson et al., 2002), somatization (Matud-Aznar et al., 2003), suicide attempts (Meadows, Kaslow, Thompson, & Jurkovic, 2005), self-esteem (Mitchell & Hodson, 1983), and PTSD (Astin, Lawrence, & Foy, 1993; Thompson et al., 2000) in community and shelter samples of abused women. For example, Levendosky et al. (2004) found that structural and functional support predicted mental health outcomes, controlling for abuse severity. Three structural support variables—total number of supporters, abuse disclosure, and nonhomophily (i.e., fewer network supporters experiencing DV)—predicted better mental health among pregnant women in violent romantic relationships. Functional support was also a significant predictor for the mental health of battered women. Specifically, more criticism predicted lower self-esteem, and greater practical aid predicted higher self-esteem and lower anxiety. In addition, Coker and colleagues (2002) reported that abused women with greater emotional support were less likely to report current poor mental health, anxiety, depression, PTSD symptoms, and suicidal ideation and actions than were abused women reporting lower emotional support and controlling 38 for 35 00 1h. for frequency and severity of abuse, race, age, and Medicaid insurance status. A study on a sub-sample of the same participants (n=19l) revealed similar findings. Higher scores on emotional support were associated with better self-perceived mental health among those women reporting current physical, sexual, or psychological abuse (Coker et al., 2003). F urtherrnore, a woman’s experience of battering was indirectly associated with mental health through emotional support; however, mediation was not adequately tested (i.e., direct effects and mediation models were not statistically compared to determine the difference in model fit). Finally, Matud et al. (2003) reported that social support correlated positively with self-esteem, and negatively with depression, anxiety, insomnia, and somatic symptoms in battered women. Overall, studies suggest that social support plays an important role in the relationship between DV and mental health symptoms, including those symptoms characteristic of CP. Specifically, studies support a link between DV and social support, and a link between social support and mental health outcomes in battered women. Moreover, one study finds that poor social support accounts for increased psychological distress and PTSD symptoms in response to DV (Thompson et al., 2000). Taken together, these findings are consistent with a mediation model. However, only one prior study has tested for mediation and no studies have examined social support in relation to CF specifically. The current study examines whether both structural and firnctional aspects of social support mediate the relationship between longitudinal patterns of DV victimization and CP symptoms. In addition to investigating the process of social isolation and the role of social support in the proposed relationship between DV and CP symptoms, the current study 39 examines two potential factors that may moderate or affect the strength of the hypothesized DV-CP symptom link: adult representations of childhood attachment and child maltreatment history. These factors are proposed to increase personal risk for CP symptoms in response to DV victimization. Adult Representations of Childhood Attachment One factor believed to qualify the impact of partner abuse on CP symptoms is insecure attachment based on adult recollections of childhood care-giving experiences. The study of adult representations of childhood attachment involves the assessment of internal working models, which can be classified as secure or insecure, parallel to infant attachment organization (Main & Goldwyn, 1994). Adult attachment classifications are typically identified based on the quality and quantity of discourse while describing and evaluating childhood attachment-related experiences (Main & Goldwyn, 1994). Adult attachment classifications represent an index of self-representations and strategies for processing emotions and thoughts related to childhood attachment relationships. Individuals with secure adult representations of childhood attachment emphasize the importance of early relationships and describe relationships with caregivers in a thoughtful, coherent, consistent, and succinct manner. In contrast, individuals with insecure adult representations of childhood attachment provide descriptions of relationships with caregivers that are incoherent, inconsistent, confiised, or conflicted. Such individuals minimize the influence of childhood attachment experiences by idealizing, normalizing, or devaluing childhood attachment relations, or provide confusing, lengthy descriptions of care-giving experiences, which indicate an angry, passive preoccupation with childhood attachment figures (Main & Goldwyn, 1994). 40 Although researchers are beginning to consider adult representations of childhood attachment in studies of childhood experiences of trauma and psychopathology (e.g., J. G. Allen et al., 1998; see Cassidy & Mohr, 2001 for review), researchers have not examined the role of adult representations of childhood attachment in the relationship between adult experiences of trauma, such as DV, and psychopathology. However, a growing body of theoretically-based empirical literature links insecure adult representations of childhood attachment in normal and patient samples of adolescents and adults to a wide range of psychological symptoms reflected in the CP construct, suggesting that insecure adult attachment in relation to early care-giving experiences is a risk factor for poor mental health. For example, Pianta, Egeland, and Adam (1996) found a relation between attachment insecurity and overall symptoms of psychopathology among a high-risk, multi-problem sample of pregnant women, the large majority of whom had histories of DV and child maltreatment. Specifically, in comparison to women classified as secure, women classified as insecure scored significantly higher on the Psychopathic Deviation, Paranoia, and Schizophrenia scales of the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory — 2 (Butcher, Dahlstrom, Graham, Tellegen, & Kraemmer, 1989) endorsing symptoms of impulsivity, insensitivity to others and social norms, suspiciousness, hostility, persecution, isolation, self-preoccupation, and inferiority. Furthermore, self-reported symptom levels exceeded the clinical cutoff, consistent with mental illness (Pianta et al., 1996). Insecure attachment was also linked to clinical status in a study of adolescent psychiatric patients; Adam et al. (1996) found a low prevalence of secure attachment 41 representations in the psychiatric sample as a whole (16%) in comparison to normative data on adolescents (56%: van Ijzendoom & Bakermans-Kranenburg, 1996). Studies assessing specific psychopathology outcomes indicate that adult representations of childhood attachment are related to affective disorders, self-harm, impulsive behaviors, and alterations in relational functioning—symptoms consistent with CP (Adam et al., 1996; J. P. Allen, Hauser, & Borman Spurrell, 1996; Cole-Detke & Kobak, 1996; Fonagy et al., 1996; Rosenstein & Horowitz, 1996; van Ijzendoom & Bakermans-Kranenburg, 1996). Two studies provide evidence for a relationship between insecure adult representations of attachment and affective disorders, including major depressive, dysthymic, schizoaffective, and eating disorders (Cole-Detke & Kobak, 1996; Rosenstein & Horowitz, 1996). For example, Cole-Detke and Kobak (1996) found that 76% of college women reporting elevated symptom levels of eating pathology and depression were classified as insecure, whereas 62% of women reporting no disorder were secure. Evidence also suggests that insecure adult representations of childhood attachment are related to self-harm and impulsive, risky behavior. For example, Adam, Sheldon-Keller, and West (1996) conducted a case-comparison study of 133 adolescents in psychiatric treatment, about half of whom had a history of severe suicidal ideation or behavior, and found that insecure attachment organization increased the probability of membership in the case group differentiating adolescents with a history of suicidal behaviors. Two additional studies of psychiatric patients linked insecure adult representations of childhood attachment to substance abuse disorder (J. P. Allen et al., 1996; Rosenstein & Horowitz, 1996). 42 In addition to Axis I psychopathology, evidence suggests a relationship between insecure adult representations of childhood attachment and relational disturbances reflected in Axis II psychopathology. Two studies found significant associations between adult representations of childhood attachment and borderline personality disorder (Fonagy et al., 1996; Rosenstein & Horowitz, 1996). For example, Fonagy et al. (1996) reported that adult psychiatric inpatients diagnosed with borderline personality disorder were significantly more likely to have insecure adult representations of childhood attachment (e.g., 75% of participants with borderline personality disorder were classified as insecure) as compared to matched controls. Furthermore, Allen et al. (1998) suggested a relationship between insecure adult representations of childhood attachment and other Axis II features, including paranoid and schizotypal behavior, based on the personality assessment of psychiatric inpatient females with histories of child abuse. Finally, van IJzendoom and Bakermans-Kranenburg (1996) provided a summary of the literature on the relation between attachment processes and psychiatric disorders in adolescents and adults by conducting a meta-analysis comparing the distributions of current representations of childhood attachment between participants in 14 clinical samples and 13 normative samples. The researchers found a deviant distribution of attachment classifications among the mentally ill, such that samples of adolescents and adults with oppositional, depressive, and borderline psychopathology were more likely to have insecure representations than the normative group; the large majority of the mentally ill were classified as insecure. Although no studies have investigated the relation between adult representations of childhood attachment and post-trauma psychopathology in the context of DV or other 43 interpersonal traumas, theoretical arguments and empirical findings suggest that disrupted attachment is a pathway to diverse forms of psychopathology, including a range of affective, relational, and behavioral disturbances captured by CP. Therefore, an insecure attachment strategy may be a risk factor for sensitivity to later risk conditions and the emergence of psychopathology, including CP symptoms. The current study tests the potential of attachment insecurity as a predisposing individual-level factor for CP symptoms in relation to DV. Child Maltreatment History Although previous studies have not examined child maltreatment as a potential vulnerability factor for DV-related CP symptoms, several studies have linked child maltreatment to CP. Specifically, two groups of researchers found a high prevalence rate of CP diagnoses among child maltreatment victims utilizing the full CP structured criteria set (McLean & Gallop, 2003; Pelcovitz et al., 1997), one study found a relationship between child maltreatment and elevated CP symptom levels (Ford et al., 2006), and three studies noted complex patterns of symptomatic and personality problems consistent with the CP syndrome among victims of childhood maltreatment (J. G. Allen et al., 1998; Dickinson et al., 1998; van der Kolk et al., 1996: see section on Evidence for a Complex PTSD Syndrome for details of these studies). Although findings from these studies suggest a connection between early experiences of abuse and CP, the role of child maltreatment in the relationship between DV and CP has not previously been explored. The current study examines child maltreatment as a moderator of the hypothesized relationship between DV and CP symptoms. 44 Hypotheses and Rationale Several mental health professionals have addressed post-trauma psychopathology that exceeds the avoidance, reexperiencing, and arousal symptoms constituting the diagnostic construct of PTSD. Most notable are Herman (1992a; 1992b), who was the first to define a CP syndrome, and Pelcovitz and colleagues (1997), who developed a diagnostic criterion set to assess CP symptoms. These authors argue that the current diagnostic formulation of PTSD fails to capture the complex psychological consequences of experiencing prolonged, repeated, interpersonal trauma, which occurs when the victim is captive, under control of the perpetrator. Instead, Herman (1992a; 1992b) proposed the construct CP to capture the profound impact of repeated, prolonged interpersonal traumatic experiences on emotion- and behavior-regulation, self-concept, and cognitive and interpersonal functioning. Research has validated the CP construct among victims of interpersonal abuse; however, the syndrome has yet to be explored among battered women specifically. The current study proposes a theoretical model for the development of CP symptoms in women exposed to DV based on trauma and attachment theories. The model includes a direct link from DV to CP symptoms, as well as an indirect link through social support. In addition, this model suggests that abuse characteristics and DV partnership may differentially influence CP symptom development. Finally, the model includes two moderator variables, attachment insecurity and experience of child maltreatment, which are expected to increase personal vulnerability to CP symptoms in response to DV. This model is tested by identifying longitudinal patterns of DV victimization based on several important abuse characteristics, and examining these 45 patterns in relation to CF symptoms among a community sample of female DV victims. This study has several theoretically-driven hypotheses: Hypotheses I a-I d Women with longitudinal patterns of DV victimization based on a variety of abuse characteristics will suffer from CP symptoms. Specifically, women classified into groups with high levels of DV frequency will report more CP symptoms than women classified into groups with lower levels of DV frequency (1a). Women classified into groups with high levels of DV severity will report more CP symptoms than women classified into groups with lower levels of DV severity (1b). Women classified into groups with high levels of sexual DV will report more CP symptoms than women classified into groups based on threats of DV or physical DV (1c). Finally, women with longer duration of DV will report more symptoms of CP than women with shorter duration of DV (1d). Herman’s (1992a; 1992b) formulation of CP suggests that interpersonal abuse victims, including battered women are at risk for the development of CP, characterized by co-occurring affective, somatic, identity, relational, and behavioral disturbances (see Theoretical Model section for details about the development of CP symptoms in response to DV). Although CP has not been examined in battered women specifically, two lines of clinical and empirical inquiry support the general theoretical link between DV and CP. First, research examining the relation between interpersonal victimization and CP provides evidence for a CP syndrome among victims of interpersonal abuse in general (e.g., Roth et al., 1997). Second, the DV literature indicates that abused women are at risk for co-morbid psychiatric disorders (e. g., Nixon etal., 2004), personality problems 46 (e.g., Walker, 1984), and experiences of repeated harm (Pico-Alfonso, 2005)—primary areas of disturbance in the CP syndrome. Although trauma theories and the empirical literature suggest that battered women are at risk for CP, battered women likely have varying experiences of abuse. Particular patterns of DV victimization due to theoretically important abuse characteristics may differentially influence the development of CP symptoms. (1a). For example, severity of abuse may be relevant in the development of CP symptoms. According to trauma theory, a relationship of coercive control, which ultimately breaks down the psychological resistance of the victim resulting in CP symptoms, is more likely to occur in cases of severe abuse where the victim has few options to escape perpetrator control due to the risk of serious danger than in cases of less severe abuse (Herman, 1992a, 1992b). Consistent with theoretical predictions, Dickinson et al. (1998) found that severity of sexual abuse was related to greater overall symptomatology reflected in the CP syndrome, including depression, somatization, PTSD, and dissociation. In addition, the DV literature suggests that DV severity is related to post-trauma psychopathology, specifically PTSD prevalence, in abused women (Houskamp & F oy, 1991). (lb). Similarly, frequency of DV may be an influential DV characteristic on the development of CP symptoms. Subordination to coercive control leading to CP symptoms is more likely to occur in cases of frequent abuse than in cases of infrequent abuse because defiance against the perpetrator usually results in abuse. Thus, self- protection requires submission. In addition, frequent abuse increases the use of dissociation to escape emotional and physical pain, as well as arousal levels resulting in somatic symptoms. Women who experience frequent DV may feel helpless because any 47 initiative of self will almost indefinitely result in abuse. Feelings of futility may result in a state of learned helplessness, leaving the women vulnerable to future harm (Crittenden, 1997). Empirical evidence suggests that frequency of DV is an important contributor to CP. For example, one study found that frequency of sexual abuse was a significant predictor of CP (McLean & Gallop, 2003). In addition, studies suggest that frequency of DV increases the risk of depression and low self-esteem (Cascardi & O'Leary, 1992)— both symptoms of CP. (1c). Type of DV is another abuse characteristic relevant to the development of CP. According to Herman (1992a), CP is a syndrome experienced by survivors of prolonged, repeated interpersonal trauma, regardless of the specific nature of the interpersonal trauma. However, sexual abuse may place women at relatively greater risk for the development of CP because sexual abuse represents the most intimate form of betrayal and an assault on reproductive control, thereby undermining a primary function of human evolution (Crittenden, 1997). Consistent with this idea, Roth et al. (1997) found that sexual abuse posed a greater risk for CP than physical abuse, suggesting that CP may demonstrate greater specificity for sexual abuse. (1d). Finally, duration of DV may be another characteristic of abuse, which influences the development of CP symptoms. Herman’s (1992a; 1992b) CP construct was developed to capture the consequences of “prolonged” traumatic experiences. A perpetrator establishes a relationship of coercive control, which ultimately leads to CP symptoms, over time with repeated use of coercive techniques. Consistent with this theoretical argument, Dickenson et al. (1998) found that duration of sexual abuse was related to multiple symptoms included in the CP syndrome. F urtherrnore, Bogat et al. 48 (2003) reported that chronic experiences of DV were related to worse psychosocial outcomes captured by CP than less chronic abuse experiences. Hypothesis 2 Women in long-term DV relationships (defined here as three or more consecutive years in the same relationship) will be more vulnerable to CF symptoms related to longitudinal patterns of DV victimization as compared to women in one or more short- term DV relationship(s). Herman’s (1992a; 1992b) formulation of CP implies that the development of a relationship of coercive control, which ultimately leads to symptoms of CP, occurs over time with a single perpetrator. Thus, involvement in one or multiple short-term DV relationships where subordination to coercive control never begins is less likely to result in CF symptoms. Although empirical studies suggest that duration of interpersonal abuse is related to CF symptoms (J. G. Allen et al., 1998; Dickinson et al., 1998), previous studies have not addressed whether or not the violence occurs in the context of one abusive relationship or multiple violent relationships. The current study is the first to investigate whether duration of DV with a single perpetrator is an important factor in the development of CP symptoms. Hypothesis 3a Women who experience DV will lose social support over time. In addition, women classified into groups with high levels of DV frequency, DV severity, threats of DV, physical DV or sexual DV will report lower levels of emotional support and fewer network supporters across all time periods than women classified into groups with lower levels of DV frequency, DV severity, threats of DV, physical DV, or sexual DV. Women with longer duration of DV will also report lower levels of emotional support and fewer 49 network supporters over time than women with shorter duration of abuse. According to Herman’s (1992a; 1992b) CP theory, the perpetrator uses DV to socially isolate the victim. Previous studies have noted a relationship between frequency and severity of DV and impaired social support (e. g., Levendosky et al., 2004; Mitchell & Hodson, 1983, 1986); however, longitudinal trajectories of social support have not been studied in relation to longitudinal patterns of DV victimization. Examination of social support over time will be an indicator of whether or not it is possible that DV perpetrators use social isolation as a method of achieving control over battered women as suggested by the proposed theoretical model. Hypothesis 3b Social support will mediate the relationship between longitudinal patterns of DV victimization and CP symptoms, such that small network sizes and low levels of emotional support will partially account for CP symptoms in victims of DV. Theoretically, poor interpersonal functioning (a primary characteristic of CP) is the outcome of traumatic bonding between perpetrator and victim, a process which is enabled when the perpetrator isolates the victim, such that the victim must depend on the perpetrator for support and other needs (Herman, 1992a, 1992b). In addition, limited external support resources make it difficult to leave an abusive relationship, thereby subjecting the victim to continued abuse and its mental health consequences. Thus, the perpetrator of DV socially isolates his victim, which in turn, leads to the development of CP symptoms. Although this is the first study to examine the role of poor social support in the development of CP symptoms among battered women, one study found that social support mediated the relationship between DV and psychological distress and PTSD 50 symptoms (Thompson et al., 2000). Furthermore, several studies have linked DV to poor social support (e. g., Levendosky et al., 2004), and poor social support in abused women to mental health problems captured by the CP construct (e. g., Meadows et al., 2005). Hypothesis 4 Adult representations of childhood attachment will moderate the relationship between longitudinal patterns of DV victimization and CP. Specifically, insecure adult representations of childhood attachment will be a risk factor for the relationship between DV and CP symptoms. Attachment theory suggests the importance of early experiences with the primary caregiver in critical developmental processes, such as the development of a sense of self, internalized mental representations of relationships, and capacity to regulate affective states (Bowlby, 1977). A damaged attachment system disrupts these important processes leading to unhealthy interpersonal functioning and the utilization of basic defenses and maladaptive coping strategies to regulate affective states (van-der- Kolk & F isler, 1994). Thus, disrupted attachment to the primary caregiver has long-term negative consequences for social, emotional, and behavioral functioning, which may affect an individual’s response to traumatic events, such as DV. Although this is the first study to examine the role of attachment in the development of CP among battered women, prior literature suggests strong connections between a damaged attachment system and several psychiatric problems constituting the CP construct (e.g., Cole-Detke & Kobak, 1996). Hypothesis 5 Child maltreatment history will moderate the relationship between longitudinal patterns of DV victimization and CP symptoms, such that women with histories of 51 frequent and severe child maltreatment will be more vulnerable to DV—related CP symptoms as compared to women with less frequent and severe experiences of childhood maltreatment. Attachment theory suggests that disrupted attachment to the primary caregiver, as often occurs in situations of child maltreatment (D. Barnett et al., 1999; E. A. Carlson, 1998), derails the crucial developmental process of learning to internally modulate physiological arousal and affective states (Bowlby, 1977). Thus, experiences of child maltreatment may disrupt the attachment system, which in turn, negatively affects capacity to regulate emotions. Poor emotional regulation may leave individuals psychologically vulnerable to both current and future traumatic experiences, as the use of basic defensive and maladaptive coping strategies become automatic over time and more adaptive strategies are not learned. Thus, early experiences of abuse may make an individual more sensitive to the psychological effects of later experiences of DV, increasing the risk of DV-related CP symptoms. Empirical research suggests a link between child maltreatment and CP (e.g., J. G. Allen et al., 1998). However, this is the first study to examine how child maltreatment may influence psychological response to adult trauma, specifically DV, in terms of CP symptoms. 52 Pa inesuga olD\'an 3000'). l througho Soenn mxnm particip. death (7 tenant 63.800 Amen Lhtav Single nido, 0r eq] dcgre Methods Participants Participants included a subsample of the 206 women recruited for a larger investigation, the Mother-Infant Study—a prospective, longitudinal study of the impact of DV and other risk factors on women and children’s functioning (Levendosky et al., 2000). The subsample consisted of women who reported experiencing DV at any time throughout the course of the larger study as defined by endorsement on any item on the Severity of Violence Against Women Scales (Marshall, 1992: see Measures section for description of scale). Analyses indicated that the total DV group consisted of 181 participants. However, 17 of these women were excluded from the current study due to death (n=2) or failure to participate in the final three waves of data collection (n=15), resulting in a final sample of 164 women. The 164 women identified themselves as 62.8% Caucasian, 25.0% African American, 5.5% Latina, 4.9% multi-racial, 1.2% Native American, and .6% Asian American. Based on demographics collected at study entry, the average age of women was 25.0 years (SD=4.9); 55.5% of the women were single/never married, 34.1% were married, and 10.4% were separated, divorced, or widowed; and 17.1% did not complete high school, 29.3% earned a high school diploma or equivalent, 42.7% had some college or trade school, and 11.0% earned a bachelor’s degree or higher. Finally, the median monthly income was $1,300 (range: $0-$7,000). Procedures Recruitment Participants for the larger study were recruited from three counties in mid- Michigan (representing urban, rural, and suburban areas) through contact with fifty-one 53 sites, including obstetric/ gynecological clinics or other women’s health clinics (39%), social service programs (26%), childbirth classes (5%), legal agencies (2%), and DV shelters (1%), as well as by posting flyers in public areas, such as libraries, laundromats, and grocery stores (27%). Two flyers were distributed: one invited pregnant women to participate in a study about mother-infant relationships, and the other targeted women who had DV experiences (i.e. being pushed, slapped, kicked, punched, or worse) during pregnancy. Sampling To participate in the study, women were required to be between eighteen and forty years of age, in their third trimester of pregnancy, and involved in a romantic relationship that lasted for at least six weeks sometime during pregnancy. Women with a limited knowledge of the English language were excluded from the study if it was believed that they would have difficulty understanding assessment protocol. The sampling procedure for the larger study involved a five-minute telephone interview to screen for eligibility based on demographic information and the presence of a romantic relationship during pregnancy. After approximately half of the sample had been enrolled in the study (n=96), additional screening procedures were instituted to ensure that a reasonable number of subjects experienced DV during pregnancy. During the telephone screen, the Conflict Tactics Scale (Straus, 1979) was administered in order to track the number of abused and non-abused women. For the purpose of recruitment, women were categorized as abused if they experienced physical violence during pregnancy according to the CTS. This screen was used to exclude women who had not experienced DV during pregnancy once it was determined that enough non-abused 54 women were already enrolled. After 137 women had been recruited and interviewed, it was discovered that many of the “non-abused” women had experienced DV in a prior relationship. Thus, the telephone screen was then also used to enroll women who had never experienced DV, in addition to those who experienced DV during their current pregnancy. Overall, 161 women were excluded because they did not meet age, language, relationship status, or battering experience criteria, or due to interview scheduling difficulties. There were no significant demographic differences between excluded women and participants on age, race, education, marital status, or abuse status. Data Collection Data for the Mother-Infant study was collected at six waves, beginning in the women’s third trimester of pregnancy (T1) and continuing every year thereafter through the child’s fifth birthday (T2-T6). For the current study, data for each variable from all available waves was utilized: DV and social support data from T1-T6, attachment data from T1, and childhood maltreatment and CP data from T6. Procedures for data collection were similar at each time period to ensure standardization. Every twelve months, women were contacted by phone, letter, or electronic mail to schedule data collection appointments. Assessment protocol was administered in an interview format by graduate and undergraduate research assistants. Interviews were conducted in a private space at participant homes or at university project offices. At each appointment, research assistants explained interview procedures and informed participants about anonymity and confidentiality policies. Participants completed an informed consent form that specified voluntary participation with the option of withdrawing from the study at any time without negative consequences. 55 Interviewers administered questionnaires aloud and recorded participant responses in order to control for varying levels of literacy among participants. Research assistants were blind to the abuse status of the women, which was guaranteed by administering questionnaires referring to DV near the end of the protocol. Interviews lasted approximately one to four hours to complete depending on the wave of data collection. Women were paid cash to thank them for their participation and to reimburse them for their time. Payment amounts varied depending upon the length of the interview. All women were provided with a list of community resources at the end of each interview. Confidentiality was maintained by assigning all participants an identification number, which was placed on all data rather than participant names. Participant names and corresponding identification numbers were kept in a separate tracking file apart from all data. The tracking file and data were kept in locked project offices, in locked filing cabinets, and on password-protected computers in password-protected files. Training Undergraduate and graduate research assistants were trained to strictly adhere to assessment protocol, confidentiality, and tracking procedures. Duration of the training was three to seven months depending on the nature of each interview. Training consisted of reviewing questionnaires and interview procedures, role-playing, reading and discussing relevant research articles, conducting mock interviews, and observing real interviews completed by experienced interviewers. In addition, interviewers learned skills to be empathic, non-judgmental, and to troubleshoot in difficult situations (e. g., the intrusion of a partner who is unhappy about a woman’s participation in the study). Finally, interviewers conducted two to five interviews supervised by senior graduate 56 student research assistants to ensure consistent and standard administration of measures. Supervisors reviewed completed interviews to ensure accurate data collection. Research assistants attended weekly training meetings throughout the data collection process to discuss concerns or problems, and to receive qualitative feedback from experienced interviewers. This meeting also served as a debriefing period for research assistants who experienced difficult interviews. Sample Retention In order to limit attrition and preserve a high retention rate, participants were contacted every ninety days. Participants were mailed a letter and a blank contact information sheet to be completed and returned in a postage-paid envelope. Letters were mailed in envelopes with “address service requested” such that the post office would send an updated address for those participants who had relocated and registered a new address. If participants did not return the information sheet or if the post office did not send updated address information within three weeks, research assistants attempted to contact the participant via alternative methods. First, participants were contacted directly by telephone or electronic mail. If women could not be reached directly (due to a disconnected phone, for example), efforts were made to contact them through three recontact people given to us by each participant at the T1 interview. If all other tracking methods failed, research assistants would go to participants’ and recontact people’s homes. Women were paid ten dollars for their participation in each tracking contact. 57 Measures Demographics A brief questionnaire was administered to obtain basic demographic information from participants, including age, ethnicity, romantic relationship history, marital/relationship status, education level, and family income (see Appendix A). Trauma Exposure Severity of Violence Against Women Scales (S VA WS.‘ Marshall, 1992). The SVAWS is a 46-item instrument designed to assess DV, defined as adult male violence and threats of violence against female romantic partners (see Appendix B). The questionnaire is composed of nine dimensions: symbolic violence; mild, moderate, and serious threats of physical violence; minor, mild, moderate, and serious actual physical violence; and sexual violence. Example items include, “Hit or kicked a wall, door, or furniture,” “Threatened to hurt you,” “Pushed or shoved you,” “Beat you up,” and “Physically forced you to have sex.” Women reported on their experiences of DV 1) with their most recent previous partner at the time of the pregnancy interview, 2) with their current partner during the year prior to pregnancy, 3) with their current partner during pregnancy, and 4) with all romantic partners during the five years following the birth of their child. Ratings are on a 4-point scale with response choices ranging from “Never” to “Many times.” To capture various abuse characteristics, each participant was assigned DV frequency, DV severity, threats of DV, physical DV, and sexual DV scores corresponding to each wave of data collection (Tl-T6). DV scores were summed across partners at each wave when a participant endorsed DV with more than one romantic partner during the 58 wave. DV frequency was calculated by summing frequency scores for individual items (possible scores range from 0-138 for any one partner). DV severity was calculated by multiplying frequency scores for individual items by physical impact weights provided by the author, and summing the weighted frequency scores for individual items (possible scores range from 0-96). Threats of aggression was calculated by summing frequency scores for individual items pertaining to threats (possible scores range from 0-5 7). Physical DV was calculated by summing frequency scores for individual items pertaining to physical abuse (possible scores range from 0-63). Sexual DV was calculated by summing frequency scores for individual items pertaining to sexual abuse (possible scores range from 0-18). In addition, each participant was assigned an overall DV duration score ranging from 1 to 8 corresponding to the number of data collection waves at which DV was endorsed (with the caveat that T1 could represent 3 observation points: current partner during pregnancy, current partner the year prior to pregnancy, and most recent previous partner). The SVAWS has good psychometric properties. Marshall (1992) reported coefficient alphas for the nine subscales ranging from .89 for symbolic violence to .96 for both mild and serious physical violence. In the current study, coefficient alphas for the entire scale ranged from a low of .92 for T7 partner 1 to a high of .99 for T3 partner 2. Dutton, Landolt, Starzomski, and Bodnarchuk (2001) found significant correlations between women’s reports of abuse on the SVAWS and their partners’ reports of abuse according to the Propensity for Abusiveness Scale, providing evidence for the validity of the SVAWS. 59 Childhood Trauma Questionnaire —— Short Form (CTQ—SF: Bernstein et al., 2003). The CTQ-SF is a 28-item, self-report measure for retrospectively assessing multiple types of abuse and neglect experienced during childhood and adolescence (see Appendix C). The questionnaire has five clinical scales each consisting of five items: physical, sexual, and emotional abuse, and physical and emotional neglect. The instnunent also includes a 3-item minimization/denial scale to detect under-reporting of maltreatment. Only the 25 items on the clinical scales were used for purposes of the current study. Example questions include, “People in my family hit me so hard it left me with bruises or marks,” “Someone tried to touch me in a sexual way,” and “People in my family called me things like ‘stupid,’ ‘lazy,’ or ‘ugly.”’ Items were rated on a 5-point Likert scale with response options ranging from “Never” to “Very Often True.” A composite child maltreatment score was calculated for each woman by summing frequency scores for individual items on all clinical scales. Possible total scores range from 25 to 125 with higher scores reflecting greater frequency and severity of abuse. The CTQ is a reliable and valid measure of childhood trauma across a variety of individuals who differ in terms of age, sex, ethnicity, socioeconomic status, psychopathology, and life experiences. Test-retest reliabilities range from .79 for the physical neglect scale to .86 for the entire measure in a sample of adult substance abusers (Bernstein & Fink, 1998). Bernstein and Fink (1998) reported internal consistency reliability coefficients ranging from a median of .66 for physical neglect to a median of .92 for sexual abuse across a variety of clinical and non-clinical samples. Coefficient alpha for the entire CTQ was reported to be .91 in a large community sample (Scher, Stein, Asmundson, McCreary, & Forde, 2001) and was .95 in the current study. 60 Confirmatory factor analyses support the invariance of the 5-factor solution across six diverse groups of people with differing maltreatment histories, including adult substance abusers from a northeast metropolitan city and southwest rural area, adolescent psychiatric inpatients, female HMO members from the northwest, and two normative community samples (Bernstein & Fink, 1998; Bernstein etal., 2003; Scher et al., 2001). The CTQ represents an improvement in content validity due to its broader content coverage reflecting multiple domains of abuse described in the literature (Bernstein & Fink, 1998). Finally, the CTQ demonstrated convergent validity with a clinician-rated, semi-structured interview of child abuse (Bernstein & Fink, 1998) and therapists’ independent ratings of abuse (Bernstein & Fink, 1998; Bernstein et al., 2003). Complex PT SD Self-Report Inventory for Disorders of Extreme Stress (SIDES - SR: van der Kolk, 2002b). The SIDES-SR is a 45-item, self-report inventory designed to assess CP or DESNOS (see Appendix D). The measure consists of 6 major scales: (1) alteration in regulation of affect and impulses, (2) alterations in attention or consciousness, (3) alterations in self-perception, (4) alterations in relations with others, (5) somatization, and (6) alterations in systems of meaning. Example items include, “When I feel upset, I have trouble finding ways to calm myself down,” “I am active sexually in ways that I know put me in danger,” “I sometimes feel so unreal that it is as if I am living in a dream, or not really there, or behind a glass wall,” and “I suffer from chronic pain in my back, yet doctors could not find a clear cause for it.” Women rated lifetime presence or absence of DV-related CP symptoms. Thus, in order to isolate the relationship between DV and CP symptoms, the effects of other interpersonal traumas (e. g., child abuse) on CP were 61 methodologically controlled by instructing women to report CP symptoms specifically related to experiences of DV. For the current study, participants received a continuous score reflecting the number of CP symptoms endorsed. The SIDES is a useful tool for the investigation of psychological response to extreme stress. The measure has good behavioral anchors and internal consistency (van der Kolk, 2003). van der Kolk (2003) reported coefficient alphas ranging from .68 to .82 for the subscales with a full-scale alpha of .93 among study participants with a diverse history of trauma. In the current study, coefficient alpha for the entire scale was .95. van der Kolk (2003) also indicated that the SIDES self-report and interview versions demonstrated convergent validity: Pearson r correlations were significant and ranged from .60 -.78 for the subscales with a full-scale correlation coefficient of .86. These correlations represent 74% shared variability between the two instruments (van der Kolk, 2003). Findings from a second study further support the convergent and discriminant validity of the SIDES instruments: the interview and self-report versions of the SIDES correlated more strongly with each other (r=.85) than with self-report and clinician-rated measures of PTSD symptoms (correlation range: 51-62). In addition, the construct validity of the SIDES has been supported in several studies (Blaustein, Spinazzola, Simpson, & van der Kolk, 2000; Spinazzola et al., 1994, November; van der Kolk et al., 1996). For example, in one study individuals showing impairment on the SIDES as compared to those with PTSD alone exhibited more comorbid Axis I and Axis II psychopathology, as well as a specific pattern of DSM-IV diagnoses reflective of shared underlying deficits with the DESNOS construct (Blaustein et al., 2000). 62 Social Support Norbeck Social Support Scale WSSQ: Norbeck, Lindsey, & Carrieri, 1981). The NSSQ is designed to measure multiple dimensions of social support, including emotional support, practical aid, and network structure (i.e., size of support network, duration of the relationships, frequency of contact, and recent loss of important relationships). Example items include, “How much does this person make you feel liked or loved,” and “If you needed to borrow $10, a ride to the doctor, or some other immediate help, how much could this person usually help?” Participants listed each significant person in their lives, and rated the amount of social support each person provided on a 5-point scale ranging from “Not at all” to “A great deal.” For purposes of the current study, women received two social support scores for each available wave of data collection: 1) number of network supporters, and 2) mean level of emotional support (determined by summing participant ratings across network members for the corresponding questions and dividing by the total number of supporters). The possible number of network supporters ranges from 0-24. Possible scores for emotional support range from 0-4 with higher scores indicating greater emotional support. See Appendix E for questions assessing emotional support. The NSSQ provides a reliable assessment of social support. Norbeck and colleagues (1981) reported l-week test-retest reliability correlations ranging from .85 to .92 across measured dimensions of social support. Stability over a 7-month interval is high, with correlations ranging from .58 to.78 (Norbeck, Lindsey, & Carrieri, 1983). The instrument also appears to be internally consistent; correlations between pairs of related items range from .89 to .97 (Norbeck et al., 1981). Validity of the NSSQ has also been 63 established (Norbeck et al., 1981, 1983). The measure was reportedly relatively free from social desirability response bias among a group of nursing graduate students (N orbeck et al., 1981). In addition, moderate correlations were found between the NSSQ and two other instruments designed to measure social support (Norbeck et al., 1981, 1983). Findings of positive correlations between interpersonal needs for inclusion and affection and amount of available social support provided evidence for the construct validity of the NSSQ (Norbeck et al., 1983). Finally, Norbeck et al. (1983) found confirmation for the predictive validity of the NSSQ, reflecting the stress-buffering role of social support. Adult Representations of Childhood Attachment Perceptions of Adult Attachment Questionnaire (PAA Q, previously referred to as the Inventory of Adult Attachment: Lichtenstein & Cassidy, 1991). The PAAQ is a 60- item questionnaire for measuring representations of early attachment experiences (see Appendix F). The instrument assesses 3 broad areas: childhood relationship with the primary caregiver (e. g., “My mother was not very affectionate”), current relationship with the primary caregiver (e.g., “No one gets under my skin like my mother”), and accessibility of childhood memories (e. g., It’s hard for me to remember my early relationship with my mother in any detail). Items form 8 subscales, several of which were designed to be equivalent to scales on the Adult Attachment Interview (AAI: Main & Goldwyn, 1994): 1) Rejection by mother, 2) Loved by mother, 3) Role Reversal with mother, 4) Anger towards mother, 5) Derogation of attachment experiences, 6) Forgiving of childhood problems, 7) feelings of Vulnerability about the relationship with mother, and 8) No Memory of childhood attachment experiences. Items are rated on a 5-point 64 all. sul {It scale with response options ranging from “Strongly Disagree” to “Strongly Agree.” Higher scores on each subscale indicate higher levels of the measured construct (e. g., more rejection, more love, etc.). Based on an earlier confirmatory factor analysis of the PAAQ performed as a part of the larger Mother-Infant Study, six subscales were used here to measure adult representations of childhood attachment: Rejection, Love, Anger, Derogation, Vulnerability, and No Memory. The previous factor analysis revealed that these six subscales loaded onto a general attachment factor reflecting attachment security (Huth- Bocks, Levendosky, Bogat, & von Eye, 2004). For the current study, participant attachment representations were determined by summing item totals on these six subscales (after reverse-scoring necessary subscales, such that higher scores reflected greater security), and then dichotomizing composite scores, such that the lower third of cases represented “insecure” and the upper two-thirds of cases represented “secure” adult representations of childhood attachment. Lichtenstein and Cassidy (1991) established the concurrent validity of the PAAQ by comparing PAAQ responses to AAI responses in a sample of mothers. Analogous scales were highly correlated with each other, with the exception of the Role Reversal and Derogation subscales (Lichtenstein & Cassidy, 1991). The PAAQ is also a reliable instrument. Lichtenstein and Cassidy (1991) reported test-retest reliabilities ranging from .64 (Forgiving) to .86 (Loved) for a sample of college students. Alpha coefficients ranged from a .62 (Derogation) to .90 (No Memory) in the same sample, and from .51 (Derogation) to .94 (N 0 Memory) in a sample of mothers. In the current study, 65 coefficient alpha for the composite scale was .94. These findings suggest that the PAAQ has good psychometric properties. 66 Results Missing Data There were two types of missing data due to attrition and data collection errors: data missing at the item level and data missing at the variable level. Data missing for single item variables (e.g., income) were considered missing at the variable level. See Table 1 for missing data at the variable level. Data missing at the item level included the following: one participant was missing an individual item on the SVAWS at T1, two participants at T1 and one participant at T4 were missing an individual item on the social support scale, and one participant was missing four items on the social support scale at T5. Scale scores with missing data at the item level were prorated. Missing data at the variable level were estimated using the hot deck method available in Prelis 8.72 (J oreskog & Sorbom, 2005). The hot deck method substitutes real values for missing values by obtaining real values from another participant whose scores on specified variables most closely match those of the participant with missing data. In this study, variables from prior waves of data collection corresponding to missing variables were specified as matching variables (e. g., T2 DV Frequency was imputed based on T1 DV Frequency). For those missing variables in which corresponding data was not collected at a prior wave or imputation was not successful based only on corresponding variables from previous observations, matching variables were selected based on conceptual closeness and correlational data. See Appendix G for a list of imputed variables and corresponding matching variables. Descriptive data for measures before and after estimation were similar, suggesting that imputation procedures were reasonable (see Table 2). 67 Hypothesis Testing Hypotheses Ia-I d Women with longitudinal patterns of DV victimization based on a variety of abuse characteristics will suffer from CP symptoms. Specifically, women classified into groups with high levels of DV frequency will report more CP symptoms than women classified into groups with lower levels of DV frequency (1a). Women classified into groups with high levels of DV severity will report more CP symptoms than women classified into groups with lower levels of DV severity (1b). Women classified into groups with high levels of sexual DV will report more CP symptoms than women classified into groups based on threats of DV or physical DV (1c). Finally, women with longer duration of DV will report more symptoms of CP than women with shorter duration of DV (1d). Analyses for Hypothesis 1 To classify participants into homogeneous groups based on longitudinal patterns of DV victimization, five sets of cluster analyses were conducted using R 2.3.1 (Maechler, Rousseeuw, Struyf, & Hubert, 2005; R Development Core Team, 2006). Cluster analysis is a person-oriented approach to data analysis that classifies individuals into groups or clusters based on pre-specified clustering variables, such that individuals within a cluster are more similar to each other than they are to individuals in other clusters. Cluster analysis is useful for longitudinal studies because it reduces the large number of possible developmental trajectories into a manageable number of groups while minimizing the loss of important information about individual differences (von Eye, Mun, & Indurkhya, 2004). 68 For the present study, cluster solutions were produced separately for DV frequency, DV severity, threats of DV, physical DV, and sexual DV. Clustering methods were the same for all analyses and decisions about the choice of clustering methods were guided by von Eye et al.’s (2004) discussion of characteristics of clustering methods. Hierarchical agglomerative clustering procedures were employed using Ward’s method for the clustering algorithm and Euclidean distance as the proximity measure. Ward’s method was selected because it maximizes within-group homogeneity and is less sensitive to outliers than other agglomerative methods. Euclidean distance was a natural choice for a base measure because it indicates the geometric distance between individuals in the multidimensional space of the clustering variables. Selection of the number of interpretable clusters was based on examination of the dendrogram and silhouette plots, as well as whether the clusters made theoretical sense. In the first cluster analysis, DV frequency scores for each of the 6 waves of data collection were entered as clustering variables. This cluster analysis suggested a 3-group solution. Cluster mean profiles for DV frequency are presented in Table 3. Cluster 1 (n=89) was named Minimal DV because these women reported low levels of DV frequency across all time periods. Cluster 2 (n=62) was named Moderate DV; women in this cluster experienced a moderate level of DV frequency at T1 , decreasing to lower levels of DV from T2-T6. Cluster 3 (n=13) was named High DVbecause DV frequency was high at T1, although frequency of DV decreased to lower levels over time. (See Table 3, Figure 2). The next 3 sets of cluster analyses based on severity of DV, threats of DV, and physical DV also suggested 3-group solutions with similar longitudinal profiles of DV 69 victimization as those produced by DV frequency. Thus, to determine whether these groups were equivalent across the 3 solutions and to those produced by the DV frequency cluster analysis, cross-tabulations were performed between all possible pairs of cluster solutions. Results indicated that the women who comprised the Minimal, Moderate, and High DV groups were relatively similar across solutions with the percentage of overlap ranging from 77% for the threats of DV and physical DV solutions to 92% for the DV severity and physical DV solutions (see Tables 4, 5, 6, 7, 8, and 9). Therefore, the cluster solution for DV frequency is the only solution from the first 4 sets of cluster analyses discussed further (see Appendix H for results obtained for severity of DV, threats of DV, and physical DV cluster analyses), though later findings pertaining to DV frequency can be generalized to DV severity, threats of DV, and physical DV since the solutions were nearly equivalent regardless of the abuse characteristic used to create the groups. For the final cluster analysis, sexual abuse scores for each of the 6 waves were entered as clustering variables. In contrast to previous analyses, this cluster analysis suggested a 2-group solution: one large cluster (n=159) and one small cluster (n=5). The large cluster was called Minimal Sexual DVwith women in this group reporting almost no sexual abuse at any of the observation periods. The small cluster was called High Sexual DV because women in this cluster reported sexual abuse at all time periods, with the exception of T5 and T6, in which no abuse was reported. Cluster mean profiles for sexual abuse are presented in Table 10 and Figure 3. To examine the relationship between DV victimization and CP symptoms, two analyses of variance and one regression were conducted. First, a one-way, between- subjects analysis of variance (ANOVA) was used to test whether number of CP 70 symptoms differed as a function of DV frequency cluster membership (Minimal DV versus Moderate DV versus High DV). The ANOVA indicated a statistically significant difference, F (2,161) = 14.92, p<.05, in number of CP symptoms across the 3 groups. The Minimal DV group had a mean CP symptom score of 3.78 (SD=7.08), the Moderate DV group had a mean score of 7.13 (SD=7.63), and the High DV group had a mean score of 16.00 (SD=12.42). Post-hoe Tukey tests indicated that the Moderate and High DV groups differed significantly from the Minimal DV group (p<.05 in both cases) and that the High DV group differed significantly from the Moderate DV group (p<.05) in the expected directions. Thus, women who experienced greater frequency of DV reported a greater number of CP symptoms relative to women who experienced less frequent DV. Furthermore, this result can be generalized to DV severity, threats of DV, and physical DV, such that women who reported greater DV severity, more threats of abuse, and more physical DV reported more CP symptoms relative to women who experienced less severe DV, fewer threats of abuse, and less physical DV, given that the cluster analyses produced nearly equivalent solutions for DV frequency, DV severity, threats of abuse, and physical abuse. A second one-way, between-subjects ANOVA was performed to test whether number of CP symptoms differed as a function of sexual abuse groupings (Minimal Sexual DV versus High Sexual DV). The ANOVA indicated a statistically significant difference, F (1 ,162)=9.l6, p<.05, in number of CP symptoms across the 2 groups. The Minimal Sexual DV group had a mean CP symptom score of 5.67 (SD=8.10) and the High Sexual DV group had a mean score of l 7 (SD=12.79); thus, women who reported 71 more sexual abuse reported more CP symptoms than women who experienced less sexual DV. To examine whether sexual DV demonstrated better specificity for CP symptoms than threats of DV and physical DV, the R2 resulting from the sexual DV ANOVA and the R2 resulting from the DV frequency ANOVA (which would be nearly equivalent to that produced by an ANOVA with threats of DV or physical DV groupings as the between-subj ects factor) were compared. The ANOVA with DV frequency cluster membership as the between-subjects factor produced an R2 of .16, whereas the ANOVA with sexual DV cluster membership as the between-subjects factor resulted in an R2 of .05. Thus, results suggest that sexual DV does not demonstrate better specificity for CP than threats of DV or physical DV. Finally, a linear regression was conducted to determine whether duration of DV predicted number of CP symptoms. In this model, CP symptoms was regressed on DV duration. The model resulted in an R2 =.16, F(l,162)=30.06, p<.05 with an unstandardized estimate b=1.66, t(162)=5.48, p<.05. Thus, for each additional time period that DV was reported, number of CP symptoms increased by 1.66. Hypothesis 2 Women in long-term DV relationships (defined here as three or more consecutive years) will be more vulnerable to CP symptoms related to longitudinal patterns of DV victimization and DV duration as compared to women in one or more short-term DV relationship(s). 72 Analysis for Hypothesis 2 First, participants were divided into two groups: women involved in at least one DV relationship for three or more consecutive years (n=132) and women involved in one or more short-term DV relationships lasting less than three consecutive years (n=32). Next, the analyses described above for Hypothesis 1 were repeated separately for the two groups of women. Group 1 .' Women in long-term DVrelationships. For the first cluster analysis, DV frequency scores for each of the 6 waves were entered as clustering variables. This cluster analysis suggested a 3-group solution. Cluster mean profiles for DV frequency are presented in Table 11. Cluster 1 (n=76) was named Minimal DV because these women reported low levels of DV frequency across all time periods. Cluster 2 (n=34) was named Moderate DV; women in this cluster experienced a moderate level of DV frequency at T1, decreasing to lower levels of DV from T2-T6. Cluster 3 (n=22) was named High DV because DV frequency was high at T1, although frequency of DV decreased to lower levels over time. (See Table 11, Figure 4). The next 3 sets of cluster analyses based on severity of DV, threats of DV, and physical DV also suggested 3-group solutions with similar longitudinal profiles of DV victimization as those produced by DV frequency. Thus, to determine whether these groups were equivalent across the 3 solutions and to those produced by the DV frequency cluster analysis, cross-tabulations were performed between all possible pairs of cluster solutions. Results indicated that the women who comprised the Minimal, Moderate, and High DV groups were relatively similar across solutions with the percentage of overlap ranging from 75% for the DV frequency and severity solutions to 89% for the DV 73 severity and physical DV solutions (see Tables 12, 13, 14, 15, 16, and 17). Therefore, the cluster solution for DV frequency is the only solution from the first 4 sets of cluster analyses discussed further (see Appendix I for results obtained for severity of DV, threats of DV, and physical DV cluster analyses). For the final cluster analysis, sexual DV scores for each of the 6 waves were entered as clustering variables. In contrast to previous analyses, this cluster analysis suggested a 2-group solution: one large cluster (n=127) and one small cluster (n=5). The large cluster was called Minimal Sexual DV with women in this group reporting almost no sexual abuse at any of the observation periods. The small cluster was called High Sexual DV because women in this cluster reported sexual abuse at all time periods, with the exception of T5 and T6, in which no abuse was reported. Cluster mean profiles for sexual DV are presented in Table 18 and Figure 5. To examine the relationship between DV victimization and CP symptoms, two analyses of variance and one regression were conducted. First, a one-way, between- subjects AN OVA was used to test whether number of CP symptoms differed as a function of DV frequency cluster membership (Minimal DV versus Moderate DV versus High DV). The ANOVA indicated a statistically significant difference, F (2,129) = 9.24, p<.05, in number of CP symptoms across the 3 groups. The Minimal DV group had a mean CP symptom score of 4.21 (SD=7.58), the Moderate DV group had a mean score of 7.38 (SD=8.79), and the High DV group had a mean score of 12.68 (SD=9.71). Post-hoe TUkey tests indicated that the High DV group differed significantly from the Minimal DV group (p<.05) in the expected direction. Furthermore, the difference between the High and Moderate DV groups approached significance (p=.05). There was no difference in 74 mean CP symptom scores between the Moderate and Minimal DV groups. Thus, the pattern in means suggests that women in long-term relationships who experience frequent DV suffer from more CP symptoms than women in long-term relationships who experience a moderate or infrequent level of DV; however, there appears to be no difference in the number of CP symptoms between women in long-term relationships who experience moderate or minimal levels of DV. Furthermore, this result can be generalized to DV severity, threats of DV, and physical DV, such that women in long- term DV relationships who reported high levels of DV severity, threats of abuse, and physical DV suffer more CP symptoms as compared to women who experienced moderate or low levels of DV severity, threats of abuse, or physical DV, given that the cluster analyses produced nearly equivalent solutions for DV frequency, DV severity, threats of abuse, and physical abuse. A second one-way, between-subjects ANOVA was performed to test whether number of CP symptoms differed as a function of sexual abuse groupings (Minimal Sexual DV versus High Sexual DV). The ANOVA indicated a statistically significant difference, F (1,130) = 7.91, p<.05, in number of CP symptoms across the 2 groups. The Minimal Sexual DV group had a mean CP symptom score of 6.02 (SD=8.39) and the High Sexual DV group had a mean score of 17.0 (SD=12.79); thus, women in long-term DV relationships who reported more sexual abuse reported more CP symptoms than women in long-term DV relationships who reported less sexual DV. Finally, a linear regression was conducted to determine whether duration of DV predicted number of CP symptoms. In this model, CP symptoms was regressed on DV duration. The model resulted in an R2 =.17, F(l,130) = 26.18, p<.05 with an 75 unstandardized estimate b=1.80, t(130)=5. 12, p<.05. Thus, for each additional time period that DV was reported, number of CP symptoms increased by 1.80. Group 2: Women in short-term DVrelationships. For the first cluster analysis, DV frequency scores for each of the 6 waves were entered as clustering variables. This cluster analysis suggested a 3-group solution. Cluster mean profiles for DV frequency are presented in Table 19. Cluster 1 (n=15) was named Minimal DV because these women reported low levels of DV frequency across all time periods. Cluster 2 (n=l3) was named Moderate DV; women in this cluster experienced a moderate level of DV frequency at T1, decreasing to lower levels of DV from T2-T6. Cluster 3 (n=4) was named High DV because DV frequency was high at T1 and T2, although frequency of DV decreased to lower levels over time. (See Table 19, Figure 6). For the second cluster analysis, physical DV scores for each of the 6 waves were entered as clustering variables. This cluster analysis also suggested a 3-group solution, though the longitudinal profiles of DV victimization differed from those produced in the DV frequency analysis. Cluster mean profiles for physical abuse are presented in Table 20. Cluster 1 (n=19) was named Minimal Physical DVbecause these women reported low levels of physical abuse across all time periods. Cluster 2 (n=5) was named Early Moderate-High Physical DV because women in this cluster experienced a moderate level of physical abuse at T], a high level of physical abuse at T2, and then almost no physical abuse for the remaining time periods. Cluster 3 (n=8) was named Early High Physical DV because physical abuse was high at T1 dropping to minimal levels of abuse for T2- T6. (See Table 20, Figure 7). 76 The next 2 sets of cluster analyses based on severity of DV and threats of DV suggested 3-group solutions with similar longitudinal profiles of DV victimization as those produced by physical abuse. Thus, to determine whether these groups were equivalent across the 2 solutions and to those produced by the physical DV cluster analysis, cross-tabulations were performed between all possible pairs of cluster solutions. Results indicated that the women who comprised the Minimal, Early Moderate-High, and Early High DV groups were relatively similar across solutions with the percentage of overlap ranging from 78% for the DV severity and threats of abuse solutions to 88% for the DV severity and physical DV solutions (see Tables 21, 22, and 23). Therefore, a decision was made to not report any further on the DV severity and threats of DV cluster solutions (see Appendix J for results obtained for severity of DV and threats of DV cluster analyses). For the final cluster analysis, sexual abuse scores for each of the 6 waves were entered as clustering variables. In contrast to previous analyses, this cluster analysis suggested a 2-group solution, one large cluster (n=29) and one small cluster (n=3). The large cluster was called Minimal Sexual DVbecause women in this group reported little to no sexual abuse at any of the observation periods. The small cluster was called Early High Sexual DV because women in this cluster reported sexual abuse at T1 and T2 with no abuse at any of the remaining time periods. Cluster mean profiles for sexual abuse are presented in Table 24 and Figure 8. To examine the relationship between DV victimization and CP symptoms, three analyses of variance and one regression were conducted. First, a one-way, between- subjects ANOVA was used to test whether number of CP symptoms differed as a 77 function of DV frequency cluster membership (Minimal DVversus Moderate DV versus High DV). The ANOVA indicated a statistically significant difference, F (2,29) = 13.66, p<.05, in number of CP symptoms across the 3 groups. The Minimal DV group had a mean CP symptom score of 2.47 (SD=3.42), the Moderate DV group had a mean score of 2.54 (SD=4.03), and the High DV group had a mean score of 16.50 (SD=11.91). Post-hoe Tukey tests indicated that the High DV group differed significantly from the Moderate and Minimal DV groups (p<.05 for both instances) in the expected directions. However, there was no difference in mean CP symptom scores between the Moderate and Minimal DV groups. Thus, the pattern in means suggests that women in short-term relationships who experience frequent DV suffer from more CP symptoms than women in short-term relationships who experience a moderate or infrequent level of DV; however, there appears to be no difference in the number of CP symptoms between women in short-term relationships who experience moderate or minimal levels of DV. A second one-way, between-subjects ANOVA was performed to test whether number of CP symptoms differed as a fimction of physical abuse groupings (Minimal versus Early Moderate-High versus Early High Physical DV). The ANOVA indicated a statistically significant difference, F (2,29) = 23.98, p<.05, in number of CP symptoms across the 3 groups. The Minimal Physical DV group had a mean CP symptom score of 2.32 (SD=3.35), the Early Moderate—High Physical DV group had a mean score of 16.4 (SD=7.80), and the Early High Physical DV group had a mean score of 1.25 (SD=3.54). Post-hoe Tukey tests indicated that the Early Moderate-High group differed significantly from the Minimal group (p<.05) in the expected direction. Furthermore, the Early High group differed significantly from the Early Moderate-High group in the expected 78 direction. There was no difference in mean CP symptom scores between the Early High and Minimal groups. Thus, the pattern in means suggests that women in short-term relationships who experience moderate to high levels of physical abuse over time suffer from more CP symptoms than women in short-term relationships who experience minimal physical abuse. In addition, women in short-term DV relationships who experience moderate levels of DV at one time point and high levels at another time report more CP symptoms than women who report high levels of DV at just one point in time. This finding is expected given that women in the Early High group experienced less DV overall as compared to those in the Early Moderate-High group. Finally, there appears to be no difference in the number of CP symptoms between women in short—term relationships who experience high levels of physical abuse at one point in time but none at others and those who experience minimal physical abuse at all time periods. These findings can also be generalized to DV severity and threats of DV given that the cluster analyses produced nearly equivalent solutions for physical DV, DV severity, and threats of abuse. Therefore, women in short-term relationships who experience moderate to high levels of DV severity or threats of abuse over time suffer from more CP symptoms than women in short-term relationships who experience minimal levels of DV severity or threats of abuse. In addition, women in short-term DV relationships who experience moderate levels of DV severity or threats of DV at one time point and high levels at another time suffer more CP symptoms than women who report high levels of DV severity or threats of abuse at just one point in time. Finally, there is no difference in the number of CP symptoms between women in short-term relationships 79 who experience high levels of DV severity or threats of abuse at one point in time but none at others and those who experience minimal DV severity or threats of abuse across all time periods. A third one-way, between-subjects ANOVA was performed to test whether number of CP symptoms differed as a function of sexual abuse groupings (Minimal Sexual DV versus Early High Sexual DV). The ANOVA indicated a statistically significant difference, F (1,30) = 84.33, p<.05, in number of CP symptoms across the 2 groups. The Minimal Sexual DV group had a mean CP symptom score of 2.41 (SD=3.61) and the High Sexual DV group had a mean score of 22.00 (SD=1.73); thus, women who reported more sexual abuse reported more CP symptoms than women who report less sexual DV. Finally, a linear regression was conducted to determine whether duration of DV predicted number of CP symptoms. In this model, CP symptoms was regressed on DV duration. The model resulted in an R2=.05, F (l ,30)=1 .52, n.s. with an unstandardized regression estimate b=.88, t(30)=1.23, n.s. Thus, duration of DV was not related to CP symptoms among women in one or more short-term DV relationships. Findings regarding the relationship between DV and CP symptoms for women in long- and short-term relationships were compared by examining differences in the magnitude of R2 for each analysis. For women in long-term DV relationships, DV frequency cluster membership (which was deemed equivalent to DV severity, threats of DV, and physical DV cluster membership) accounted for 13% in the variation in number of CP symptoms, sexual DV cluster membership accounted for 6% in the variation in number of CP symptoms, and duration of DV accounted for 18% in the variation of 80 number of CP symptoms. For women in short-term DV relationships, DV frequency cluster membership accounted for 49% in the variation in number of CP symptoms, physical DV cluster membership (which was deemed equivalent to DV severity and threats of DV cluster membership) accounted for 62% in the variation in number of CP symptoms, sexual DV cluster membership accounted for 74% in the variation in number of CP symptoms, and duration of DV accounted for 5% in the variation of number of CP symptoms. Thus, it appears that DV was generally a better predictor of CP symptoms for women in short-term DV relationships compared to those in long-term DV relationships. The only exception to this was with duration of DV, which accounted for more variation in CF among women in long-term DV relationships than in women in short-term DV relationships. Although it appears that DV may be a better predictor of CP among women in short-term than in long-term DV relationships, the cluster solutions produced in the analyses for Hypothesis 1 were used in all remaining analyses because those cluster solutions were based on the entire sample of women and the larger sample size was needed to increase power in remaining analyses. Hypothesis 3a Women who experience DV will lose social support over time. In addition, women classified into groups with high levels of DV frequency, DV severity, threats of DV, physical DV or sexual DV will report lower levels of emotional support and fewer network supporters across all time periods than women classified into groups with lower levels of DV frequency, DV severity, threats of DV, physical DV, or sexual DV. Women 81 with longer duration of DV will also report lower levels of emotional support and fewer network supporters over time than women with shorter duration of abuse. Analyses for Hypothesis 3a Repeated measures analyses were conducted to examine whether abused women lose social support over time. Analyses were conducted separately for DV frequency cluster membership, sexual DV cluster membership, and duration of DV, as well as for number of network supporters and emotional support. The cluster solutions produced for the overall sample of women (i.e., those produced in the Analysis for Hypothesis 1) were used for these analyses. Overall, results did not support study hypotheses regarding main effects and the effect of time on social support. However, details of these results are reported below. Number of Network Supporters. A two-way repeated measures ANOVA was conducted with DV frequency cluster membership as the between-subjects factor and time as the within-subjects factor. The Geisser-Greenhouse correction was applied for within-subjects tests to adjust for asphericity. The ANOVA indicated a significant main effect for time [F (4.02, 646.35)=28.07, p<.05], such that number of supporters decreased over time from T1 to T6 for all groups of abused women. The ANOVA also indicated a marginally significant main effect for DV frequency cluster membership [F (2, 161)=2.97, p=.05]. The interaction of time and cluster membership was not statistically significant [F (8.03, 646.35)=8.03, n.s.]. Means and standard deviations for number of network supporters as a function of cluster membership and time are presented in Table 25. Mean number of network supporters by cluster membership and time is also illustrated graphically in Figure 9. 82 Games-Howell comparisons were used to follow-up the main effect of cluster membership. The comparisons between the Minimal and High DV groups and between the Moderate and High groups were not significant. However, the comparison between the Minimal and Moderate DV clusters was significant (p<.05). The pattern in means suggests that women who experience moderate levels of DV have fewer supporters averaged across time as compared to women with low levels of DV. However, contrary to expectation, women with high levels of DV do not have significantly fewer supporters than women with low or moderate levels of DV frequency. A two-way repeated measures AN OVA was conducted with sexual abuse cluster membership as the between-subjects factor and time as the within-subjects factor. The Geisser-Greenhouse correction was applied for within-subjects tests to adjust for asphericity. The ANOVA indicated a significant main effect for time [F (4.05, 655.89)=4.25, p<.05]. The main effect of sexual abuse cluster membership was not statistically significant [F (1 , l62)=.06, n.s.]. The interaction of time and cluster membership was also not statistically significant [F (4.05, 655.89)=.59, n.s.]. Means and standard deviations for number of network supporters as a function of cluster membership and time are presented in Table 26. Thus, results indicate that number of network supporters decreased over time from T1 to T6 for all groups of abused women; however, there was no mean difference in number of supporters averaged across time between women with high versus low sexual DV. In order to perform a repeated measures ANOVA with duration of DV, the continuous DV duration variable was converted to a categorical variable by dividing participants into 4 groups. Group 1 consisted of women who experienced DV for a 83 minimum of 1 time period and a maximum of 2 time periods (n=3 8), Group 2 consisted of women who experienced DV for a minimum of 3 time periods and a maximum of 4 time periods (n=50), Group 3 consisted of women who experienced DV for a minimum of 5 time periods and a maximum of 6 time periods (n=53), and Group 4 consisted of women who experienced DV for a minimum of 7 time periods and a maximum of 8 time periods (n=23). Next, a two-way repeated measures ANOVA was conducted with duration group membership as the between-subj ects factor and time as the within- subjects factor. The Geisser—Greenhouse correction was applied for within-subjects tests to adjust for asphericity. The ANOVA indicated a significant main effect for time [F (4.02, 643.13)=57.23, p<.05]. The main effect for group membership was not statistically significant [F (3, 160)=1.27, n.s.]. The interaction of time and group membership was also not statistically significant [F (12.06, 643.13)=.82, n.s.]. Means and standard deviations for number of network supporters as a function of cluster membership and time are presented in Table 27. Thus, number of network supporters decreased over time from T1 to T6 for all groups of abused women; however, there was no mean difference in number of supporters averaged across time among women with chronic versus less persistent experiences of DV. Emotional Support. A two-way repeated measures ANOVA was conducted with DV frequency cluster membership as the between-subj ects factor and time as the within- subjects factor. The Geisser-Greenhouse correction was applied for within-subjects tests to adjust for asphericity. The ANOVA indicated a significant main effect for time [F(4.42, 711.33)=5.54, p<.05] and a marginally significant main effect for DV frequency Cluster membership [F(2, 161)=2.66, p=.07]. The interaction of time and cluster 84 membership was not statistically significant [F(8.84, 711.33)=1.15, n.s.]. Means and standard deviations for emotional support as a function of cluster membership and time are presented in Table 28. In contrast to expectation, findings indicate that emotional support increased over time from T1 to T6 for abused women. A two-way repeated measures ANOVA was conducted with sexual abuse cluster membership as the between-subjects factor and time as the within-subjects factor. The Geisser-Greenhouse correction was applied for within-subjects tests to adjust for asphericity. The ANOVA indicated a significant main effect for cluster membership [F (1 , 162)=4.66, p<.05], such that women with high sexual abuse report less emotional support than women with low sexual abuse. However, the main effect of time was not statistically significant [F(4.39, 710.56)=1.64, n.s.], suggesting no change in mean level of emotional support over time. The interaction of time and cluster membership was also not statistically significant [F (4.39, 710.56)=.77, n.s.]. Means and standard deviations for emotional support as a function of cluster membership and time are presented in Table 29. In order to perform a repeated measures analysis with duration of DV as a predictor variable, the 4-group categorical duration variable as described above was used as the between-subjects factor in a two-way repeated measures ANOVA with time as the within-subjects factor. The Geisser-Greenhouse correction was applied for within- subjects tests to adjust for asphericity. The ANOVA indicated significant main effects for both time [F (4.39, 701 .57)=7.88, p<.05] and group membership [F (3, l60)=4.07, p<.05]. The interaction of time and group membership was not statistically significant [F (13.15, 701.57)=1.20, n.s.]. Means and standard deviations for emotional support as a 85 function of group and time are presented in Table 30. Contrary to expectation, results indicate that mean emotional support increased over time from T1 to T6 for abused women. Games-Howell comparisons were used to better understand the main effect of group membership. Comparisons between the 1 and 2, 1 and 3, 2 and 3, and 2 and 4 groups were not significant. However, the comparison between groups 1 and 4 was significant (p<.05), indicating that emotional support was generally lower among women who experienced chronic DV occurring for at least 7 observation periods as compared to women who reported DV for 2 observation periods or less. Hypothesis 3b Social support will mediate the relationship between longitudinal patterns of DV victimization and CP symptoms, as well as the relationship between DV duration and CP symptoms. A nalysis for Hypothesis 3b According to Baron and Kenny (1986), to test for mediation a series of three regression equations must be estimated. First, the dependent variable is regressed on the independent variable. Second, the mediator is regressed on the independent variable. Third, the dependent variable is regressed on both the independent variable and the mediator. To establish mediation, four conditions must be satisfied: 1) the independent variable must predict the dependent variable in the first equation, 2) the independent variable must predict the mediator in the second equation, 3) the mediator must predict the dependent variable in the third equation, and 4) the effect of the independent variable on the dependent variable in the third equation must be zero (for complete mediation) or less than the effect in the first equation (for partial mediation). 86 To test the mediating role of social support, Baron and Kenny’s (1986) procedures for establishing mediation were followed: first, regressing CP on DV; second, regressing social support on DV; and third, regressing CP on both DV and social support. This series of regression equations was conducted separately for various facets of DV (i.e., DV frequency and sexual DV clusters as produced in the Analysis for Hypothesis 1, and duration of DV) and for two different indicators of social support: number of network supporters at T6 and emotional support at T6. Overall, analyses did not support mediation, though details of these results are reported below. Number of Network Supporters. The first series of equations tested whether number of network supporters mediated the relationship between DV frequency and CP symptoms. Because DV frequency is a categorical variable (with values representing the Minimal, Moderate, or High DV frequency clusters), a dummy variable coding scheme designating the Minimal DVcluster as the comparison group was incorporated into the series of regression equations. First, CP was regressed on DV frequency. The unstandardized regression estimates were 3.78 for the Minimal DV group [t(161)=4.56, p<.05], 3.35 for the Moderate DV group [t(161)=2.60, p<.05], and 12.22 for the High DV group [t(161)=5.27, p<.05]. The R2 was .16 [F(2,161)=14.92, p<.05]. Second, number of network supporters was regressed on DV frequency. The unstandardized regression estimates were 7.45 for the Minimal DV group [t(161)=18.45, p<.05], -1.40 for the Moderate DV group [t(161)=-2.22, p<.05], and -1.45 for the High DV group [t(161)=- 1.28, n.s.]. The R2 was .03 [F(2,161)=2.79, p<.10]. Finally, CP was regressed on DV frequency and number of network supporters. The unstandardized regression estimates were 2.92 for the Minimal DV group [t(l60)=l .99, p<.05], 3.51 for the Moderate DV 87 group [t(160)=2.68,p<.05], 12.39 for the High DVgroup [t(160)=5.31,p<.05], and .12 for number of network supporters [t(160)=.71, n.s.]. The R2 was .16 [F (3,160)=10.08, p<.05]. Although the overall models specified in each of the three equations were significant or marginally significant, the main effect of number of network supporters was not significant in the third equation. Furthermore, the relationship between DV frequency and CP symptoms was not zero or reduced in the third equation. Therefore, the mediation hypothesis was not supported. The second series of equatiOns tested whether number of network supporters mediated the relationship between sexual abuse and CP symptoms. Although sexual abuse is a categorical value, there are only two levels (representing the Minimal and High Sexual DV clusters). Therefore, dummy coding is unnecessary. However, the variable was recoded, such that a value of zero represents the Minimal Sexual DV cluster and a value of one represents the High Sexual DV cluster. First, CP was regressed on sexual abuse. The unstandardized regression estimates were 5.67 for the Minimal Sexual DV group [t(162)=8.67, p<.05] and 11.33 for the High Sexual DV group [t(162)=3.03, p<.05]. The R2 was .05 [F (1,162)=9.16, p<.05]. Second, number of network supporters was regressed on sexual abuse. The regression estimates were 6.82 for the Minimal Sexual DV group [t(162)=22.27, p<.05] and -.42 for the High Sexual DV group [t(162)=-.24, n.s.]. The R2 was 0 [F(l,162)=.06, n.s.]. Finally, CP was regressed on sexual abuse and number of network supporters. The unstandardized regression estimates were 5.65 for the Minimal Sexual DV group [t(161)=4.27, p<.05], 1 1.33 for the High Sexual DV group [t(161)=3.02, p<.05], and 0 for number of network supporters [t(161)=.02, n.s.]. The R2 was .05 [F (2,161)=4.55, p<.05]. The overall model specifying sexual abuse as a 88 predictor of number of network supporters was not statistically significant. F urtherrnore, number of network supporters was not a significant predictor of CP symptoms, and the relationship between sexual abuse and CP symptoms was not zero or reduced in the third equation. Therefore, the mediation hypothesis was not supported. The third series of equations tested whether number of network supporters mediated the relationship between DV duration and CP symptoms. First, CP was regressed on DV duration [b=1.66, t(162)=5.48, p<.05; R2=.l6 [F(l,162)=30.06, p<.05]. Second, number of network supporters was regressed on DV duration [b=-.31, t(162)=- 2.10, p<.05; R2=.03 [F(l,162)=4.39, p<.05]. Third, CP was regressed on DV duration and number of network supporters. The unstandardized regression estimates were 1.70 for DV duration [t(161)=5.55, p<.05] and .14 for number of network supporters [t(161)=.86, n.s.]. The R2 was .16 [F(2,161)=15.38,p<.05]. Although the first two conditions were satisfied, number of network supporters was not a significant predictor of CP symptoms, and the relationship between DV duration and CP symptoms was not zero or reduced in the third equation. Therefore, the mediation hypothesis was not supported. Emotional Support. The fourth series of equations tested whether emotional support mediated the relationship between DV frequency and CP symptoms. Because DV frequency is a categorical variable (with values representing the Minimal, Moderate, or High DV frequency clusters), a dummy variable coding scheme designating the Minimal DV cluster as the comparison group was incorporated into the series of regression equations. First, CP was regressed on DV frequency. The unstandardized regression estimates were 3.78 for the Minimal DV group [t(161)=4.56, p<.05], 3.35 for the Moderate DV group [t(161)=2.60, p<.05], and 12.22 for the High DV group 89 [t(161)=5.27,p<.05]. The R2 was .16 [F(2,161)=14.92, p<.05]. Second, emotional support was regressed on DV frequency. The unstandardized regression estimates were 3.47 for the Minimal DV group [t(161)=67.72, p<.05], -.05 for the Moderate DV group [t(l61)=-.66, n.s.], and -.12 for the High DV group [t(161)=-.85, n.s.]. The R2 was .01 [F (2,161)=.48, n.s.]. Finally, CP was regressed on DV frequency and emotional support. The unstandardized regression estimates were 11.15 for the Minimal DV group [t(l60)=2.50, p<.05], 3.24 for the Moderate DV group [t(l60)=2.52, p<.05], 11.97 for the High DV group [t(160)=5.18, p<.05], and -2.13 for emotional support [t(160)=-1.68, p<.10.]. The R2 was .17 [F (3,160)=1 1.00, p<.05]. The overall model specified in equation 2 was not statistically significant. In addition, the main effect of social support was only marginally significant, and the relationship between DV frequency and CP symptoms was not zero and only slightly reduced (for the Moderate and High DV groups) in the third equation. Therefore, the mediation hypothesis was not supported. The fifth series of equations tested whether emotional support mediated the relationship between sexual abuse and CP symptoms. Although sexual abuse is a categorical value, there are only two levels (representing the Minimal and High Sexual DVclusters). Therefore, dummy coding is unnecessary. However, the variable was recoded, such that a value of zero represents the Minimal Sexual DV cluster and a value of one represents the High Sexual DV cluster. First, CP was regressed on sexual abuse. The unstandardized regression estimates were 5.67 for the Minimal Sexual DV group [t(162)=8.67, p<.05] and 11.33 for the High Sexual DV group [t(162)=3.03, p<.05]. The R2 was .05 [F (l,162)=9.16, p<.05]. Second, emotional support was regressed on sexual abuse. The unstandardized regression estimates were 3.45 for the Minimal Sexual DV 90 group [t(162)=90.57, p<.05] and -.31 for the High Sexual DV group [t(162)=—1.43, n.s.]. The R2 was .01 [F (1,162)=2.05, n.s.]. Finally, CP was regressed on sexual abuse and emotional support. The unstandardized regression estimates were 13.26 for the Minimal Sexual DV group [t(161)=2.84, p<.05], 10.65 for the High Sexual DV group [t(161)=2.84, p<.05], and -2.20 for emotional support [t(l61)=-1.64,p=.10, n.s.]. The overall model specifying sexual abuse as a predictor of emotional support was not statistically significant. Furthermore, emotional support was only a marginally significant predictor of CP symptoms, and the relationship between sexual abuse and CP symptoms was not zero and only slightly reduced (for the High Sexual DV group) in the third equation. Therefore, the mediation hypothesis was not supported. The sixth series of equations tested whether emotional support mediated the relationship between DV duration and CP symptoms. First, CP was regressed on DV duration [b=1.66, t(162)=5.48, p<.05; R2=.16, F(l,162)=30.06, p<.05]. Second, emotional support was regressed on DV duration [b=—.04, t(162)=-2.19, p<.05; R2=.03, F (l ,162)=4.78, p<.05]. Third, CP was regressed on DV duration and emotional support. The unstandardized regression estimates were 1.60 for DV duration [t(161)=5.21, p<.05] and -l.50 for emotional support [t(161)=-l.17, n.s.]. The R2 was .16 [F(2,161)=15.75, p<.05]. Although the first two conditions were satisfied and the relationship between DV duration and CP symptoms was slightly reduced in the third equation, emotional support was not a significant predictor of CP symptoms. Therefore, the mediation hypothesis was not supported. 91 Hypothesis 4 Adult representations of childhood attachment will moderate the relationship between longitudinal patterns of DV victimization and CP symptoms, as well as the relationship between DV duration and CP symptoms, such that insecure attachment will exacerbate CP symptoms related to DV victimization. Analysis for Hypothesis 4 The differential effect of DV on CP symptoms as a function of attachment was examined separately for three facets of DV (frequency, sexual abuse, and duration) using Baron and Kenny’s (1986) recommended procedures for testing moderation. According to Baron and Kenny (1986), the specific analytic method for testing moderation varies according to the categorical or continuous nature of the independent and moderator variables under study. In this study, the moderator variable attachment is categorical. However, the nature of the independent variable DV changes depending on the particular facet of DV examined; DV frequency and sexual abuse are both categorical variables representing cluster membership as determined in the Analysis for Hypothesis 1, while DV duration is a continuous variable. Therefore, two cases are presented here. In case 1, both the independent and moderator variables are categorical, whereas in case 2, the independent variable is continuous and the moderator variable is categorical. Thus, analytic procedures and results are reported separately for each case. Case I : Categorical Independent Variable and Categorical Moderator. To test for moderation when both the independent and moderator variables are categorical, Baron and Kenny (1986) recommend conducting a two-way, between-subjects ANOVA. A moderator effect represented by the product of the independent and moderator variables 92 is included in the ANOVA with a significant interaction indicating moderation. This procedure was followed to test the potential moderating role of attachment in the relation between DV and CP symptoms for both DV frequency and sexual abuse. [It should be noted that the use of a multiplicative term to represent the moderator effect only makes sense if the levels of the independent variable follow a natural order, which is the case in this study. For example, DV frequency cluster 1 represents Minimal abuse, cluster 2 represents Moderate abuse, and cluster 3 represents High abuse] First, a two-way, between-subjects AN OVA was computed to test the differential effect of DV frequency (Minimal, Moderate, High) on number of CP symptoms as a function of attachment (insecure versus secure). Cell means are presented in Table 31. The interaction term was not significant [F (2,158)=.09, n.s.]. Thus, a moderator hypothesis was not supported. This same procedure was followed to test the differential effect of sexual abuse (Minimal, High) on number of CP symptoms as a function of attachment (insecure versus secure). Cell means are presented in Table 32. Likewise, the interaction term was not significant [F (l ,160)=. l 3, n.s.], indicating that the moderator hypothesis was not supported. Case 2: Continuous Independent Variable and Categorical Moderator. To test for moderation when the independent variable is continuous and the moderator is categorical, Baron and Kenny (1986) suggest using regression to estimate the effect of the independent variable on the dependent variable separately for each level of the moderator and then comparing the resulting unstandardized regression coefficients among the independent samples. This procedure was followed to examine the potential 93 moderating role of attachment in the relationship between DV duration and CP symptoms. First, CP was regressed on DV duration for participants with insecure representations of attachment. The unstandardized regression estimate was 1.67 [t(52)=2.87, p<.05], and the R2 was .14, [F(1,52)=8.21, p<.05]. Next, CP was regressed on DV duration for participants with secure representations of attachment. The unstandardized regression estimate was 1.61 [t(108)=4.54, p<.05], and the R2 was .16, [F (1,108)=20.57, p<.05]. Finally, these estimates were compared to test the null hypothesis Ho: B5=B,, where Bs is the unstandardized regression coefficient for participants with secure representations of attachment and Bi corresponds to the unstandardized regression coefficient for participants with insecure attachment. To perform this analysis, the attachment variable was coded, such that a value of 1 represented secure attachment and a value of 0 represented insecure attachment. Then, attachment, DV duration, and the interaction of these two variables were entered as predictors in a regression equation. The interaction term in this equation tests the null hypothesis Ho: BS=B,. Results failed to reject the null hypothesis [t(160)=-.10, n.s.; R2=.16, F (3,160)=10.24, p<.05], indicating that the unstandardized regression coefficient for DV duration predicting CP symptoms was not significantly different for participants with insecure versus secure representations of attachment. Thus, moderation was not supported. Hypothesis 5 Child maltreatment history will moderate the relationship between longitudinal patterns of DV victimization and CP symptoms, as well as the relationship between DV 94 duration and CP symptoms, such that the experience of child maltreatment will exacerbate the relationship between DV and CP symptoms. Analyses for Hypotheses 5 To examine the potential moderating role of child maltreatment history in the relationship between DV and CP symptoms, Baron and Kenny’s (1986) procedures for testing moderation were employed. Again, this hypothesis was examined separately for three independent DV variables: frequency cluster membership, sexual abuse cluster membership, and duration. The procedures used to test moderation, however, differ somewhat from those described above to examine the moderating role of attachment because child maltreatment history is a continuous variable. Thus, we have two different cases altogether. In the first case (case 3), the independent variable is categorical and the moderator is continuous. In the second case (case 4), both the independent and moderator variables are continuous. Analyses and results are reported separately for each case. Case 3: Categorical Independent Variable and Continuous Moderator. To test for moderation when the independent variable is categorical and the moderator variable is continuous, Baron and Kenny (1986) suggest conducting a linear regression, in which the dependent variable is regressed on the independent variable, the moderator variable, and the product of the independent and moderator variables. The moderator hypothesis is supported if the interaction is significant after controlling for the main effects of the independent and moderator variables. This was followed to test the potential moderating role of child maltreatment history in the relation between DV and CP symptoms for both DV frequency and sexual abuse. 95 First, regression was used to test whether the effect of DV frequency on CP symptoms changes linearly with respect to child maltreatment history. Because DV frequency is a categorical variable (with values representing the Minimal, Moderate, or High DV frequency clusters produced in the Analysis for Hypothesis 1), a dummy variable coding scheme designating the Minimal DV cluster as the comparison group was incorporated into the regression equation. This regression produced two interaction terms (Moderate DV frequency * child maltreatment history) and (High DV frequency * child maltreatment history). The unstandardized regression estimates were -.13 for the interaction of Moderate DV and child maltreatment history [t(158)=-2.04, p<.05] and .27 for the interaction of High DVand child maltreatment history [t(158)=2.28,p<.05], and the R2 for the model was .33 [F (5,158)=15.24, p<.05]. The moderator effect was significant, as indicated by the significant DV by child maltreatment interactions after controlling for the main effects of DV and child maltreatment. Thus, the moderation hypothesis was supported, such that child maltreatment exacerbated DV—related CP symptoms with the greatest exacerbation in the High DV group. Likewise, regression was used to test whether the effect of DV sexual abuse on CP symptoms changes linearly with respect to child maltreatment history. Although sexual abuse is a categorical value, there are only two levels (representing the Minimal and High Sexual DV clusters produced in the Analysis for Hypothesis 1). Therefore, dummy coding is unnecessary. However, the variable was recoded, such that a value of 0 represented the Minimal Sexual DV cluster and a value of 1 represented the High Sexual DVcluster. The unstandardized regression estimate was .43 for the interaction term, which was marginally significant [t(160)=1.81, p<.07], and the R2 for the model was .21 96 [F (3,160)=14.57, p<.05]. Thus, results suggest that child maltreatment history moderates the relationship between sexual abuse cluster membership and CP symptoms, such that child maltreatment exacerbates DV-related CP symptoms with women in the High Sexual DVcluster at the greatest risk for DV-related CP symptoms as child maltreatment increases. Case 4: Continuous Independent Variable and Continuous Moderator. Although this case is unique in that both the independent and moderator variables are continuous, the test for moderation is the same as the test specified above in case 3, with one exception: a dummy coding scheme for the independent variable is unnecessary. Therefore, to test whether the effect of DV duration on CP symptoms changes in a linear fashion with respect to child maltreatment history, CP was regressed on DV duration, child maltreatment history, and the interaction of these two variables. The overall model was significant [R2=.27, F (3,160)=1 9.79, p<.05]; however, the unstandardized regression estimate for the interaction of DV duration and child maltreatment was not significant [b=.02, t(160)=1.14, n.s.]. Thus, the moderator hypothesis was not supported in this case. 97 Discussion Overall, findings offer partial support for the proposed model of the development of CP symptoms in a community sample of women with lifetime experiences of DV. Findings suggest that abused women report elevated symptoms of CP, and that women who experience childhood maltreatment may be particularly vulnerable to CF symptoms in response to DV. DV was related to poor quality of social support; however, social isolation did not account for the DV-CP symptom relationship. Thus, risk factors for CP are partially explained here, though mechanisms for the development of CP symptoms among abused women living in the community are not yet understood. Specific findings pertaining to each hypothesis are discussed below. Longitudinal Trajectories of DV Victimization Results from cluster analyses suggest that women’s individual experiences of DV over time based on frequency, severity, threats of aggression, or physical abuse scores can be summarized by three longitudinal profiles of victimization. The first profile is characterized by low levels of DV across time. The second profile is characterized by moderate levels of DV at T1 with lower levels of DV across time. The final profile is characterized by high levels of DV at T1 decreasing to lower levels over time. These profiles suggest that abused women differ initially in terms of level of DV, but violence generally decreases over time for all abused women, regardless of initial level of DV. Furthermore, at the end of the study period (T6), there are no mean differences in level of DV among the three patterns. There are several possible explanations for the observation that DV is generally greater at T1 with declining levels of DV over time. First, this finding may reflect the 98 sampling strategy utilized for the Mother-Infant Study. Specifically, participants were selected for an overrepresentation of DV at T1 to ensure that an adequate number of women who experienced DV were recruited for the study. Second, participants were recruited during pregnancy, and the T1 assessment of violence combined DV experienced during and prior to pregnancy. Previous research suggests that women may be particularly vulnerable to violence prior to and during pregnancy with a decreased risk following delivery (Martin, Mackie, Kupper, Buescher, & Moracco, 2001; Torres et al., 2000). For example, Martin et al. (2001) found that the prevalence rate of physical abuse against women was 6.9% in the 12 months prior to pregnancy, 6.1% during pregnancy, and 3.2% approximately 4 months postpartum. In addition, the absence of abuse before and during pregnancy was strongly protective against postpartum abuse. These findings are consistent with trajectories observed in the current study; women who reported low levels of violence at T1 reported low levels of violence across time, and those who reported moderate to high levels of DV at T1 reported decreasing levels of abuse over time. However, the Martin et al. (2001) study was cross-sectional, relied on retrospective reports of abuse, and assessed physical violence against women more generally (though in the large majority of cases, violence was perpetrated by a romantic partner). Thus, the current study is unique in that findings are specific to DV, the assessment of violence included threats of aggression in addition to actual violence, and the study design was longitudinal and prospective. Research on women who are not pregnant also finds decreasing rates of DV across time, consistent with results from the current study. For example, Jasinski (2001) found that prevalence of DV dropped from 11.5% to 8.9% over a 5-year period among 99 romantic couples who were together during the length of the study. Furthermore, epidemiological research suggests that following early adulthood (age 24), risk for DV declines with age (Rennison, 2001). In the present research, the mean age of participants was 25.0 years (SD=4.9) at study entry; thus, based on epidemiological research, it is consistent that DV decreased as time elapsed and participants aged. From a theoretical perspective, these trajectories can be understood in a variety of ways. For example, pregnancy may be a time of increased stress for a couple and as a result, increase the risk for DV (J asinski, 2001). This may particularly be true of couples who will be new parents, couples who are expecting an unplanned child, couples with financial hardship, or couples in which the male partner is not the father of the child (Jasinski, 2001; Stewart & Cecutti, 1993; Torres et al., 2000). It has also been suggested that violent male partners may experience feelings of jealousy in relation to the unborn child, increasing risk of pregnancy DV (Campbell, Harris, & Lee, 1995). For some couples, DV may decrease postpartum because the birth of a child triggers violence cessation; for example, J asinski (2001) found that men who ceased to perpetrate DV against female partners over the course of their five-year study were almost twice as likely to have experienced the birth of their first child during the first wave of data collection. The present finding that DV generally decreases over time is also consistent with Herman’s (1992a; 1992b) theory that acts of violence within a relationship may decrease across time because the perpetrator no longer needs to use violence in order to control the victim. The perpetrator may use violence initially to instill fear and establish a relationship of coercive control, but once he has achieved this objective, actual violence 100 romantic couples who were together during the length of the study. Furthermore, epidemiological research suggests that following early adulthood (age 24), risk for DV declines with age (Rennison, 2001). In the present research, the mean age of participants was 25.0 years (SD=4.9) at study entry; thus, based on epidemiological research, it is consistent that DV decreased as time elapsed and participants aged. From a theoretical perspective, these trajectories can be understood in a variety of ways. For example, pregnancy may be a time of increased stress for a couple and as a result, increase the risk for DV (Jasinski, 2001). This may particularly be true of couples who will be new parents, couples who are expecting an unplanned child, couples with financial hardship, or couples in which the male partner is not the father of the child (J asinski, 2001; Stewart & Cecutti, 1993; Torres et al., 2000). It has also been suggested that violent male partners may experience feelings of jealousy in relation to the unborn child, increasing risk of pregnancy DV (Campbell, Harris, & Lee, 1995). For some couples, DV may decrease postpartum because the birth of a child triggers violence cessation; for example, J asinski (2001) found that men who ceased to perpetrate DV against female partners over the course of their five-year study were almost twice as likely to have experienced the birth of their first child during the first wave of data collection. The present finding that DV generally decreases over time is also consistent with Herman’s (1992a; 1992b) theory that acts of violence within a relationship may decrease across time because the perpetrator no longer needs to use violence in order to control the victim. The perpetrator may use violence initially to instill fear and establish a relationship of coercive control, but once he has achieved this objective, actual violence 100 may no longer be necessary to maintain power over the victim. Other methods of control not measured in the current study, such as control over economic and legal resources, or undermining or discrediting the victim may continue throughout the course of a relationship. Thus, psychological abuse tactics may be effective due to prior violence. This position is supported by Marshall’s (1996; 1999) investigations on the effects of psychological abuse, which suggest that a perpetrator can be abusive and controlling without necessarily being physically or sexually violent, as well as other studies that suggest that psychological abuse can occur independent of violence (Alexander, 1993; D. G. Dutton & Painter, 1993). Further examination of alternative methods of coercion over time may serve to further clarify women’s individual experiences in abusive relationships. Findings also indicate that longitudinal profiles of DV victimization are nearly equivalent regardless of whether the profile is based on DV frequency, DV severity, threats of DV, or physical DV scores. Thus, each of these indicators of abuse provides similar information about women’s experiences of DV victimization over time, suggesting that measurement of only one of these indicators may be necessary to define developmental trajectories of DV. This finding is not surprising given that each of these scores were derived from the same measure and were highly correlated (e.g., Tl DV frequency highly correlated with T1 DV severity, T1 threats of DV, and T1 physical DV). DV frequency and severity scores were positively related because the calculation of these scores was based on endorsement of the same items. However, threats of DV and physical DV scores were based on summing frequency counts for separate items. Thus, the positive correlation between these two types of scores indicates that threatened 101 and actual violence do not often occur in the absence of each other; women who experience threatened violence are also likely to experience physical violence. This finding is consistent with two prior studies of large community samples of women in long-term heterosexual relationships conducted by Marshall (1996; 1999), who found that threats of aggression and acts of violence as assessed by the SVAWS were highly correlated (r=.82 and r=.85). In contrast, the cluster solution produced for sexual DV was not equivalent to the solutions produced for DV frequency, DV severity, threats of DV, and physical DV. Instead of a three-group solution representing mild, moderate, and high levels of violence, the cluster analysis based on sexual DV scores produced a two-group solution. Women who comprised Group I experienced almost no sexual abuse across time, whereas women in Group 2 experienced high sexual DV at T1 with sexual abuse decreasing to lower levels over time. The decline in sexual violence over time in Group 2 is consistent with the decline observed in DV frequency, DV severity, threats of DV, and physical DV groupings, and is supported by the empirical and theoretical literature noted above, which suggests a decline in DV over time (Herman, 1992a, 1992b; Jasinski, 2001; Rennison, 2001). In addition, results indicate that sexual DV is not as highly correlated with the other indicators of abuse (e. g., the correlation between sexual DV and threats of DV at T6 was .23, the correlation between sexual DV and physical DV at T6 was .29, and the correlation between threats of DV and physical DV at T6 was .71). This is consistent with Marshall’s (1996) study of women in long-term heterosexual relationships that found that the correlation between sexual DV and threats of DV was .49, and the 102 correlation between sexual DV and physical DV was .50 (as compared to the correlation of .82 observed between threats and physical DV). Thus, sexual DV may be qualitatively different than threats or actual physical aggression. Specifically, sexual abuse may represent the most intimate form of abuse and is an assault on reproductive control, a basic human function (Crittenden, 1997). The distinctive nature of sexual abuse may partially explain why the cluster solution for sexual DV was not equivalent to those produced by other indicators of abuse, and thus, should be considered in addition to other indicators of abuse when trying to understand women’s experiences of DV and the differential effects of DV based on the constellation of harmful acts sustained. This recommendation is supported by Marshall (1996) who identified differential effects of DV on women’s physical and mental health, help-seeking behaviors, and perceptions of relationships depending on the type and configuration of abuse sustained. For example, sexual aggression was related to participation in psychotherapy, whereas threats and physical violence were not. Furthermore, sexual DV contributed to women’s help- seeking behaviors significantly more often than physical violence. It is also possible that the sexual DV cluster analysis did not produce a moderate- level DV group similar to that produced with other abuse indicators because sexual abuse occurred infrequently for the majority of women. Although 54.9% of women reported experiencing sexual abuse at some point throughout the course of the study, the frequency of these acts was low (the highest mean score on sexual abuse was 2.31 at T1). Thus, sexual DV may be more common than previously believed among battered women residing in the community, but acts of sexual aggression are infrequent for most abused women, consistent with results obtained in previous DV research among community 103 samples (e.g., Marshall, 1996). It is important to keep in mind, however, that although results here indicate that individual trajectories of sexual DV victimization can be summarized by two longitudinal patterns, this solution may not generalize to shelter samples of abused women who may experience greater frequency and severity of sexual abuse. Relationship between DV Victimization and CP Symptoms Consistent with the proposed model, results suggest a significant relationship between DV victimization and CP symptoms. Specifically, findings indicate that mean number of CP symptoms differed as a function of DV cluster membership, such that women who experienced greater levels of DV (regardless of the specific indicator of abuse) suffered from more CP symptoms as compared to women in clusters characterized by lower levels of DV victimization. Thus, women in groups characterized by greater DV frequency, DV severity, and threats, physical, and sexual DV reported more symptoms as compared to women in groups characterized by lower DV frequency, DV severity, and threats, physical, and sexual DV, respectively. Furthermore, duration of DV was related to CP symptoms, such that the more observation periods in which women reported abuse, the greater the number of CP symptoms. The relationship observed between DV and CP symptoms is consistent with CP, dynamic-maturational, and contructivist theories of trauma, which together suggest that victims of prolonged, repeated interpersonal trauma, including DV, suffer from a complex constellation of problems, including somatization, depression, dissociation, negative self-perceptions, chaotic interpersonal functioning, re-victimization, and self- inflicted harm—problems that exceed the re-experiencing, avoidance, and hyperarousal 104 symptoms of PTSD (Crittenden, 1997; Harter & Neimeyer, 1995; Herman, 1992a, 1992b). Specifically, Harter and Neimeyer (1995) suggest that individuals develop templates for relationships based on experiences with an abusive partner resulting in CP symptoms, particularly re-victimization. Crittenden (1997) argues that the victim of repeated abuse reaches a state of learned helplessness after repeated failures to protect oneself, which leaves the victim vulnerable to coercive control by the perpetrator. It is this relationship of coercive control, in which the perpetrator undermines the victim’s autonomy and gains control over the victim’s cognitions, actions, and feelings through repeated threats of violence, actual violence, and other coercive techniques (e. g., severing the victim’s interpersonal connections with others) that Herman (1992a; 1992b) argues leaves victims psychologically vulnerable to CP. According to theory, the relationship of coercive control, which ultimately breaks down the psychological resistance of the victim leading to CP is more likely to occur when abuse is chronic, severe, or frequent, consistent with findings from the current study. Specifically, the development of a coercive relationship occurs over time with repeated use of coercive techniques and therefore, is more likely to occur when abuse is chronic. Submission to coercive control is more likely to occur when abuse is severe or frequent because defiance against the perpetrator places the victim in grave danger (in the case of severe DV) or inevitably leads to physical or psychological injury (in the case of frequent DV). Furthermore, frequent abuse may lead to chronic arousal of the fight/flight response, resulting in anxiety and somatic symptoms reflected in CP. Though the current study did not directly measure the development of a coercive relationship, violence is one 105 technique used to achieve control over an individual and thus, may be one indicator of the existence of a coercive relationship. Results supporting a relationship between DV and CP are also consistent with empirical studies which suggest that DV is related to a range of mental health problems reflected in the CP construct (e.g., Champion et al., 2001; Coker et al., 2002; Dorahy et al., 2007; D. G. Dutton & Painter, 1993; Lown & Vega, 2001; Olson et al., 2003; Pico- Alfonso, 2005; Sansone et al., 2007; Stein & Kennedy, 2001; Umberson et al., 1998), as well as to studies which have validated the CP construct among interpersonal abuse victims in general, and particularly among CSA survivors (e.g., Dickinson et al., 1998; Ford et al., 2006; McLean & Gallop, 2003; Pelcovitz et al., 1997; Roth et al., 1997). Studies of CP among interpersonal abuse victims also suggest that abuse characteristics such as frequency (McLean & Gallop, 2003), severity (Dickinson et al., 1998), and duration of abuse (J. G. Allen et al., 1999; Dickinson etal., 1998) are related to an increased risk for CP symptoms, consistent with findings from this study. However, the current study adds to the existing empirical literature in several important ways. For example, this is the first known study to examine and find evidence for CP symptoms specifically among victims of DV. In addition, the current study uses a person-centered approach summarizing women’s individual experiences of DV over time and linking these longitudinal trajectories of DV to CF symptoms. Furthermore, in the current study, CP was explored among a community sample of women with a range of DV experiences in order to investigate whether women with relatively less severe and frequent abuse as compared to women residing in battered women’s shelters would report core features of CP. 106 Although findings suggest that greater DV was associated with more CP symptoms as predicted, results also indicate that even women with moderate levels of DV suffer from elevated levels of CP symptoms. Thus, CP may not occur exclusively among individuals who experience severe levels of abuse, as might be expected based on an understanding of current CP theory. This finding is similar to results reported in a prior study by Ford et al. (2006), in which infrequent interpersonal trauma was associated with elevated levels of CP symptoms among a college sample of women. Though full CP may be rare in community samples of interpersonal abuse victims, subclinical levels of CP may nonetheless cause significant impairment in functioning. For example, previous research has indicated that subclinical PTSD is related to significant psychosocial and physical health impairment (Schnurr & Green, 2004; Stein et al., 1996). Furthermore, subthreshold psychiatric symptomatology has been linked to future psychiatric morbidity (Katzelnick et al., 2001). Contrary to expectation, the current study found that sexual DV did not demonstrate greater specificity for CP symptoms as compared to other types of abuse (threats and physical DV). This finding runs contrary to Crittenden’s (1997) position that sexual abuse may have the most devastating consequences for mental health as compared to other types of abuse because it represents the most intimate form of betrayal, as well as to the existing empirical literature on CP, which suggests that sexual abuse is a stronger risk factor for CP than other types of violence. Specifically, Allen et al. (1998) found that adult women who experienced CSA had higher scores on symptoms consistent with CP than women who did not experience CSA. Furthermore, Roth and colleagues (1997) 107 found that sexual abuse placed individuals at greater risk for developing full CP than physical abuse alone. However, the Allen et al. (1998) study only included victims of childhood sexual abuse, and it is unclear in the Roth et al. (1997) study whether participants experienced sexual abuse during childhood or adulthood. Findings in the current study may be a function of when the sexual abuse occurred during the lifespan. It is possible that the experience of sexual abuse in adulthood, particularly within a romantic relationship in which sexual intimacy is appropriate, does not have as detrimental of an impact as it does when the sexual abuse is experienced during childhood. Thus, the impact of sexual abuse may be equivalent to other forms of aggression, such as physical abuse, when it occurs later in life. In fact, Herman (1992a; 1992b) does not specify that sexual abuse has a differential effect on CP as compared to other forms of prolonged interpersonal abuse, and Marshall (1999) found that psychological abuse made a greater contribution to women’s mental health problems than sexual DV, though CP was not assessed. Alternatively, this finding may be a function of the infrequency with which sexual violence occurred in the current sample; the high sexual DV group contained only five women as the majority of participants reported a low frequency of sexual aggression. Additional studies including more women who experience frequent sexual DV may be needed to better understand this finding. Effect of Partnership Status on CP Symptoms Conceptually, DV contributes to CP via the development of a relationship of coercive control, which the perpetrator establishes over time (Herman, 1992a, 1992b). Thus, DV was hypothesized to be more predictive of CP symptoms for women in long- 108 term violent relationships as compared to women in short-term violent relationships in which there is limited opportunity for a relationship of coercive control to develop. As predicted, duration of DV among women in long-term DV relationships accounted for more variance in CP symptoms than among women in short-term DV relationships. In contrast, DV groupings (patterns of DV victimization) among women in short-term DV relationships accounted for more variance in CF symptoms as compared to DV groupings among women in long—term DV relationships. Therefore, although DV clusters were predictive of CP for both women in long- and short-term violent relationships, the present research seems to suggest that DV is generally a better predictor of CP among women in short-term violent relationships. According to Herman (1992a; 1992b), the perpetrator establishes a relationship of control leading to CP over time through a m of coercive techniques. Thus, it is possible that in long-term relationships, other factors (not measured here) in addition to DV, such as psychological abuse and economic and legal subordination, contribute to CP, whereas in short-term violent relationships, it is primarily DV that drives CP. Another interesting finding pertains to group differences in CP symptom levels for women in short- versus long-term DV relationships. Specifically, mean number of CP symptoms differed significantly in the expected direction between the High and Minimal DV groups, but not between the Moderate and High DV groups among women in long-term violent relationships. The pattern in means was different among women in short-term violent relationships. For these women, mean number of CP symptoms differed significantly in the expected direction between the High and Moderate and the High and Minimal DV groups, but not between the Moderate and Minimal DV groups. 109 These findings appear to suggest that DV only has to be moderate (not high) for women in long-term violent relationships to experience an elevation in CP symptoms, whereas the violence has to be high for women in short-term violent relationships to experience an elevation in CP symptoms. It is possible that women involved in long-term violent relationships may experience significant stress symptoms even if violence is not high because a relationship of coercive control has been established over time and the perpetrator relies on other coercive techniques rather than violence to maintain control, an explanation which would support current CP theory. Social Support Overall, results regarding social support did not support the theoretical model that social isolation partially accounts for the relationship between DV and CP symptoms. However, there were several interesting findings pertaining to DV and patterns in social support, which are discussed in detail below. For example, results generally suggest a main effect for emotional support, such that women in groups characterized by greater DV have less emotional support averaged across time as compared to women with lower DV. However, there was no main effect for number of supporters; thus, women in groups characterized by greater DV did not have significantly fewer supporters than women in groups with less DV. Taken together, these findings suggest that the average quality rather than quantity of social support differs according to level of DV. These findings are consistent with theory and prior literature that suggests that DV is related to poor social support (e. g., Levendosky et al., 2004; Mitchell & Hodson, 1983; Thompson et al., 2000), however, they run contrary to prevailing theory and literature that suggests that poor social support among battered women is due to social isolation (O. W. Barnett 110 et al., 1996; Herman, 1992a, 1992b; Hilberman & Munson, 1978; Mitchell & Hodson, 1983; Tan et al., 1995; Walker, 1979). This finding may be a function of the varying severity of DV experienced by women across samples. Specifically, the majority of studies that find that poor social support is a function of social isolation utilize shelter samples, which may include a large percentage of women who experience severe levels of DV. In contrast, the current study examined abused women residing in community settings, most of who reported mild to moderate levels of DV. This explanation is difficult to test, however, as most DV studies do not include information on the severity of violence experienced by participants. Alternatively, poor quality of social support may stem from abused women’s failure to reach out to network members for support due to shame, embarrassment, fear of perpetrator retribution, or concern about endangering the safety of family and friends— reasons frequently cited by female participants in a qualitative study about social support and DV for not appealing to family and friends for support (El-Bassel et al., 2001). It is also possible that abused women request support from others in their network, but are dissatisfied with the quality of support received because family and friends respond with a lack of empathy or are unable to provide adequate support due to their own personal circumstances. For example, results from a study on the entire sample of Mother-Infant Study participants during pregnancy found that abused women, as compared to nonabused women, had more network supporters who experienced DV themselves (homophily: Levendosky et al., 2004). In addition, homophily was positively related to disclosure of abuse and negatively associated with emotional support within the DV group. This suggests that abused women may turn to others for support, but do not 111 receive adequate support, perhaps because supporters who are also abused may have few psychological resources to provide quality support. Thus, impaired social support among abused women may be best explained by homophily, rather than social isolation. Repeated measures results regarding the effect of time were significant for both number of supporters and emotional support. As predicted in the proposed model, repeated measures analyses indicate that number of supporters decreased over time averaged across groups of abused women (regardless of the indicator of abuse used in the analyses). In contrast, emotional support increased over time averaged across DV frequency clusters and DV duration groups (though the effect of time was not significant when sexual DV clusters were defined as the between-subjects factor, perhaps because of the small sample size in the High Sexual DV group, n=5). Thus, findings regarding the effect of time generally suggest that abused women lose supporters over time, but that perceived quality or satisfaction with support increases. Findings regarding the effect of time for emotional support may be best understood in light of current study findings on longitudinal trajectories of DV, which suggest that DV declines over time for all groups of abused women. Thus, as DV decreases over time, quality of support increases, which is consistent with findings from previous cross-sectional studies citing an inverse relationship between quality of social support and DV (e. g., Thompson et al., 2000). In contrast, number of supporters decreased over time. Although this finding was predicted in the proposed model, it is believed that the decrease in network size may be a measurement problem, rather than a valid indicator of social isolation, particularly given the lack of a main effect for number of supporters. The social support questionnaire utilized in the current study required participants to answer a number of questions 112 pertaining to each network member. Thus, over time, participants may have learned that the interview takes less time to complete if fewer supporters are identified, which may explain the observed decrease in network size. Alternatively, it is possible that the decrease in network members is a function of the normal developmental trajectory of social support across the lifespan. For example, studies of age differences in social support generally indicate a decline in quantity of available supporters with age (e. g., Fischer, 1982). Therefore, the decrease in network members observed in the current study may be a function of participants aging, rather than an indicator of social isolation by the DV perpetrator. Furthermore, even though network size may erode over time, studies on perceived social support suggest that quality of social support received is relatively stable with age or increases in relation to particular sources of support (e. g., children: Coventry, Gillespie, Heath, & Martin, 2004). Thus, the longitudinal pattern in social support observed in the present research (i.e., decrease in number of network members and increase in emotional support) may be explained by normative developmental patterns in social support, rather than DV-related factors. Number of supporters and emotional support were expected to mediate the relationship between DV and CP symptoms; however, given the findings noted above, social isolation does not appear to be a primary route to DV-related CP symptoms, at least among a community sample of abused women. Thus, it is not surprising that mediation was not supported. Although findings do not necessarily discount that social support may be an explanatory factor for CP among women with more severe levels of DV in which the victim may be held captive by the perpetrator, the absence of mediation suggests that social isolation or even the perception of isolation, reflected in poor quality 113 of support, do not explain the development of behaviors characteristic of CP for abused women living in the community. It may be that coercive techniques other than disconnection from fiiends and family are central in these violent relationships, such as psychological abuse (e.g., discrediting the victim) or control over economic and legal resources. These coercive techniques may serve to undermine the victim’s autonomy, power, and initiative, creating dependency and traumatic bonding with the perpetrator, even though the victim is not isolated. Perhaps perceived level of dependency on the perpetrator, rather than social isolation, may better explain the link between DV and the interpersonal and identity disturbances captured by CP for abused women residing in the community. Adult Representations of Childhood Attachment and Child Maltreatment History The present research examined two factors that were hypothesized to moderate the DV-CP symptom link: adult representations of childhood attachment and child maltreatment. The inclusion of both of these factors was based primarily on attachment theory, which suggests that disruption of the early care-giving bond (reflected in attachment insecurity and that often occurs in situations of child maltreatment) disrupts critical developmental processes (e. g., emotion and behavior regulation), which leave individuals psychologically vulnerable to traumatic experiences in adulthood, including DV (D. Barnett et al., 1999; Bowlby, 1977; E. A. Carlson, 1998; van-der-Kolk & Fisler, 1994). Interestingly, results indicate that child maltreatment moderated the relationship between DV clusters (regardless of the abuse characteristic used as the clustering variable) and CP symptoms, such that child maltreatment exacerbated DV-related CP symptoms with the greatest exacerbation among women with the highest levels of DV; 114 h0‘ €F§ at at rel so Gr nta ha\ eve For disc. givir disru rESea Sittia however, attachment insecurity based on adult representations of early care-giving experiences did not. One possible explanation for the lack of significant findings regarding attachment is that adult representations of care-giving experiences may not adequately reflect early attachment experiences that disrupt developmental processes due to discontinuity in attachment over time. In fact, several longitudinal studies have found a low concordance rate between infant attachment organization and adult attachment status based on recollections of care-giving experiences among adolescents using the AAI and AAI Q- sort procedure (Jones, 1996; Weinfield, Sroufe, & Egeland, 2000; Zimmermann, Grossmann, & Fremmer—Bombik, 1998), suggesting that for many individuals attachment may not be stable from childhood through adulthood. Furthermore, additional studies have found that attachment is less stable among individuals who experience traumatic life events (Waters, Merrick, Treboux, Crowell, & Albersheim, 2000; Weinfield et al., 2000). For example, Weinfield (2000) found that child maltreatment accounted for some discontinuity in attachment status over time. Thus, adult representations of early care- giving experiences (particularly in the presences of traumatic events) may not capture disruption in infant attachment that negatively affects critical developmental processes that create psychological vulnerability to conditions of stress, such as DV. However, literature regarding the stability of attachment is inconsistent. Other researchers have observed 70% to 77% correspondence rates between childhood Strange Situation behavior and adolescent and adult attachment status with regard to care-giving experiences, suggesting that attachment is stable over time (Benoit & Parker, 1994; Hamilton, 2000; Waters et al., 2000). If attachment is in fact stable over time, an 115 alternative explanation may be plausible. It is possible that adult representations of childhood attachment accurately reflect disruptions in early developmental processes; however, the PAAQ may not be a good measure of adult attachment with regard to care- giving experiences. Longitudinal studies that suggest continuity in attachment used the AAI (e. g., Waters et al., 2000), which is the gold standard of adult attachment measures (regarding care-giving experiences) with the greatest psychometric support. In addition, studies cited here to support the moderation argument linking adult attachment with regard to care-giving experiences to psychopathology (J. P. Allen et al., 1996; Cole- Detke & Kobak, 1996; F onagy et al., 1996; Rosenstein & Horowitz, 1996) utilized the AAI. The PAAQ was selected for the current study to increase efficiency due to the time intensive nature of training interviewers, and administering and scoring the AAI. Although the PAAQ was developed to predict adult attachment status with regard to care- giving as assessed by the AAI with significant correlations found between 4 of the 6 overlapping subscales, and preliminary research suggests that the PAAQ has adequate validity and reliability (Lichtenstein & Cassidy, 1991), the PAAQ has rarely been used; therefore, its psychometric properties have not been investigated thoroughly. In addition, a clinical cutoff for insecure attachment on the PAAQ has not been established in the prior literature. Furthermore, studies comparing self-identified adult attachment based on care- giving experiences to AAI status have not found significant associations (Crowell, Treboux, & Waters, 1999; Hesse, 1999). For example, Hesse (1999) found that participants who were classified as insecure according to the AAI were likely to identify themselves as secure on questionnaire measures, and individuals who were secure on the 116 AAI were likely to identify themselves as insecure. F urtherrnore, (Hamilton, 1995) found no relation between self-reported adult attachment based on care-giving experiences and infant attachment among adolescents using the Inventory of Peer and Parent Attachment (IPPA: Arrnsden & Greenberg, 1987), but in another study found that infant strange situation behavior was related to AAI status (Hamilton, 2000). Thus, adult representations of early care-giving experiences may correspond to infant attachment, but not be satisfactorily identified through self-report measures. Unlike the AAI which assesses attachment based on coherence of discourse rather than content, self-report measures of adult attachment with respect to care-giving experiences, such as the PAAQ are subject to deficits in autobiographical memory about early relationship experiences with the primary caregiver, as well as social desirability bias. Finally, women may report on attachment experiences more favorably as a way to avoid negative feelings associated with acknowledgment that early care-giving experiences were poor. However, it is alternatively possible that the PAAQ adequately measures disrupted attachment, but the clinical cutoff for insecure attachment utilized in the current study may not have accurately captured women with severe enough attachment disturbances that would be indicative of disruption in developmental processes believed to contribute to psychological vulnerability to traumatic experiences. In contrast, it is arguable that child maltreatment, which is an extreme circumstance, is a more sensitive measure of disrupted care-giving experiences that would fail to provide opportunities for normal development. If this is the case, the finding that child maltreatment moderated the DV-CP symptom link is consistent with theoretical literature suggesting that disruptions in early developmental processes of emotion and behavior regulation 117 stemming from disrupted attachment lead to psychological vulnerability to later conditions of risk (e.g., Bowlby, 1977). It is also possible that the assessment and cutoff selected for attachment in the current study was not problematic, but rather that attachment insecurity alone is simply not a strong enough risk factor to be related to CP symptoms, which are suggestive of severe psychological problems. Child maltreatment however, is a strong enough risk factor to contribute to CF because it is a more severe experience than disrupted attachment alone. The experience of childhood abuse is often indicative of a very disturbed childhood, which is often associated with insecure attachment as well as a host of other risk factors for psychopathology (e. g., poverty, parental substance abuse, parental psychopathology, domestic violence: see Zielinski & Bradshaw, 2006 for review), which is not necessarily the case with insecure attachment in and of itself. Thus, it may be the combination of childhood maltreatment and other risk factors associated with the environment within which childhood maltreatment occurs that increases vulnerability to CP in response to DV. This explanation is consistent with Gold’s (2000) understanding of the impact of child maltreatment. Specifically, Gold (2000) proposes that it is not child maltreatment alone, but rather the combination of child maltreatment and contextual factors (though not measured here) associated with early abuse that contribute to psychological vulnerability in adulthood. Furthermore, Gold (2000) suggests that poor emotion regulation, which increases vulnerability for mental health problems in response to conditions of risk (e. g., DV), may stem from the deficient learning environment associated with child maltreatment. He argues that many individuals who experience child maltreatment are raised in ineffective 118 families with caregivers who do not teach effective skills for living. Thus, poor emotion regulation that increases vulnerability to future trauma may result from the failure to learn adaptive coping strategies for modulating emotions in one’s family of origin. Therefore, the finding that child maltreatment exacerbates DV-related CP symptoms may be due the absence of opportunities to learn effective strategies for regulating emotions, rather than due to problematic attachment. Although the exact explanation as to why child maltreatment moderates the DV- CP symptom link may not yet be clear (e. g., is moderation the result of disrupted developmental processes due to insecure attachment, due to the cumulative impact of risk factors associated with child maltreatment, or related to the inability to learn emotion regulation skills because of the deficient family environment associated with child maltreatment), moderation is consistent with several previous studies linking child maltreatment and CP in adulthood (J. G. Allen etal., 1998; Dickinson et al., 1998; McLean & Gallop, 2003; Pelcovitz et al., 1997; van der Kolk et al., 1996). Summary Overall, findings from the current study provide partial support for the proposed model. Specifically, results support the theoretical link between DV and CP with level and type of abuse differentially affecting CP, and suggest that child maltreatment history moderates the relationship between DV and CP. Thus, this study is an important first step toward establishing an empirical link between experiences of DV and CP, and uncovering a personal vulnerability factor for experiencing DV—related CP symptoms. However, DV appears only to be a risk factor for women who experience moderate to high levels of DV. Women who reported mild levels of DV across time (Minimal DV 119 group) reported few CP symptoms, suggesting that this group of women may be similar to women who do not experience DV in that they do not appear to be at increased risk for developing significant posttraumatic sequelae. Although an association was generally found between DV and an elevation in CP symptoms, it is clear that the mechanisms for developing CP in response to DV are not fully understood as poor social support was not explained by social isolation, and social support did not mediate the association between DV and CP. Thus, explanatory factors for the development of CP symptoms in response to DV require further theoretical and empirical consideration. Nonetheless, findings do not discount prevailing trauma theory and literature, but rather suggest that such theories may not apply to all abused women, particularly those living in community settings with mild to moderate levels of DV. Study Strengths This study had several strengths. Specifically, this study combined person- and variable-centered analytic strategies to classify women’s individual experiences of DV victimization over a six-year period and to explore the relationship between these patterns and CP symptoms. Although prior studies have examined CP among victims of repeated, interpersonal trauma, some of whom may have experienced DV (e.g., Pelcovitz et al., 1997), this is the first known study to assess CP specifically in victims of DV. In addition, prior studies have relied on retrospective reports of abuse, whereas the current study assessed DV prospectively, eliminating the potential for recall bias. This is also the first known study to test Herman’s (1992a; 1992b) assertion that social isolation is a primary route to developing CP. Again, although this idea should not be discounted, current findings suggest that it may not be applicable to women who experience mild to 120 moderate levels of DV. Finally, the cmrent research was a first step toward identifying personal vulnerability factors that may exacerbate CP symptoms related to prolonged, interpersonal trauma. Study Limitations and Future Research Implications Although this research has several important findings, results must be interpreted in light of study limitations with future research addressing such limitations. For example, the current study did not statistically control for the potential effects of other traumas on CP. Prior literature has suggested that CP demonstrates greater specificity for chronic interpersonal trauma as compared to single-incident, non-interpersonal traumas, such as a natural disaster (Pelcovitz et al., 1997); however, participants may have experienced other types of interpersonal trauma that were not measured (e.g., adult sexual assault by a non-romantic partner) that may contribute to particular symptoms of CP. In an attempt to partially control for the effects of other traumas, participants were asked to endorse CP symptoms related specifically to experiences of DV, but women who experience multiple interpersonal traumas may not be able to separate the effects of specific traumas. For example, a woman with lifetime experiences of DV and adult sexual assault by a non-romantic partner may be unable to distinguish whether the DV, sexual assault, or both contributed to depressive affect or chronic physiological arousal resulting in somatization. In future studies, DV researchers should consider assessing and parsing out the effects of other interpersonal traumas on CP, or alternatively testing a cumulative stress model of CP, particularly given that trauma survivors are often subject to repeated victimization. 121 Another weakness of the current study was the cross-sectional measurement of CP symptoms. In contrast to DV, which was measured longitudinally, CP was only measured at the end of the study period (T6). Thus, the immediate effects of DV on CP symptomatology could not be assessed, and women who may have experienced CP symptoms earlier in the study period, but not at T6, were not identified. Prospective, longitudinal measurement of CP symptoms would help researchers identify patterns in CF in relation to changes in DV. Furthermore, it is possible that DV-related CP symptoms fluctuate in relation to other life stressors or mental health intervention. The inclusion of additional risk factors or factors restoring mental health in future studies may serve to clarify the relationship between DV and CP and contribute to the further development of a theoretical literature on the development of CP among women who experience DV. Finally, the sample size in the current study was relatively small, which may have precluded the identification of relationships among study variables, particularly with regard to sexual abuse. Future studies should include larger sample sizes to increase power and allow a latent variable approach to testing models for the development of DV- related CP. A latent variable approach would permit the inclusion of multiple indicators to reduce the bias of measurement error, as well as allow researchers to include all relevant factors in one model to examine the interaction among variables. A larger sample size would also allow researchers to include all types of abuse, including psychological abuse and other methods of coercion in one cluster analysis, rather than multiple separate cluster analyses. This would permit researchers to more precisely 122 def abu lim finc suci exp and inve isolz Hen seve for tl COntr help to ta, Clim' Provj inte dlag Chara define configurations of abuse and identify the differential impact of such patterns of abuse on CP. In addition to future research implications noted above in relation to study limitations, studies with other samples of abused women are needed to replicate the finding that DV is related to CP, particularly given that this is the first study to support such a link. As the relationship between DV and CP is further supported empirically, explanatory factors for the development of CP in response to DV need to be considered and tested. Although social isolation was not found to be a mediating variable in this investigation, researchers should examine the potential explanatory role of social isolation in studies that include women with a broader range of DV experiences as Herman’s (1992a; 1992b) theory may be more applicable to women who experience severe DV. Finally, future studies should include additional risk and protective factors for the development of CP among abused women. This type of research would contribute to the development of effective prevention and intervention programs, and help mental health professionals identify individuals who may be at greatest risk for CP to target for such programs. Clinical Implications The current study provides empirical evidence for a link between DV and CP, providing additional cause for challenging our current view of posttraumatic stress reactions in order to facilitate an improved understanding of the psychological impact of interpersonal trauma, including DV. Currently, clinicians must rely on established diagnostic concepts, such as personality disorders, to describe the complex symptom and character presentations of women with trauma histories, including battered women. 123 According to the DSM-I V, the application of personality disorders to trauma victims is not incorrect; victims may meet symptom criteria as they are currently defined, especially as constriction and negative symptoms are predominant in the clinical presentation. However, the use of personality disorders to describe trauma victims may result in the failure to recognize the traumatic origins of symptoms, the failure to understand the full impact of victimization on the trauma victim, and the mistake of attributing symptoms to enduring personality traits (Herman, 1992a). The risk of misunderstanding may be particularly high among the group of interpersonal trauma survivors who no longer meet criteria for PTSD, but exhibit other trauma-related sequelae captured by CP (see Dickinson et al., 1998 for a complete description of this profile of women). In these cases, the link between symptoms and traumatic origins may be easily lost with the current classification system. In addition to the risk of misunderstanding the experience and underlying pathology of trauma victims, the application of personality disorders may serve to further stigmatize a group of women who may already experience a great deal of embarrassment, shame, and self-blame. Several personality diagnoses have acquired a pejorative tone among clinicians; borderline personality disorder, a diagnosis that commonly overlaps with CP (McLean & Gallop, 2003), is particularly uncomplimentary and laden with negative connotations. The diagnostic picture for victims of interpersonal abuse is further complicated by the fact that clinicians are often forced to diagnose several co-occurring Axis I and Axis 11 conditions in an attempt to capture the multiple and pervasive disturbances frequently present among trauma victims. Multiple diagnoses may muddle the conceptualization, 124 firrther misrepresenting the underlying pathology, and add to the flawed sense of self often observed among interpersonal abuse victims. Misunderstanding the experience of interpersonal abuse victims and the consequent assignment of co-morbid conditions has significant implications for treatment planning and success. Some may argue that the issue is one of labeling—what names we assign to particular mental health disturbances. However, it would be naive to assume that labels serve only a descriptive and communicative function in a profession that is becoming increasing dominated by empirically-validated, manualized treatments designed for particular disorders. The current classification system, without the recognition of CP, contributes to the application of multiple diagnostic constructs, which may not recognize the underlying link between trauma and symptoms, and thus, may lead to inadequate treatment planning and transient improvement among trauma victims. The CP construct provides clinically meaningfirl information, which may aid in treatment planning. Given the significant degree of diagnostic overlap between personality disorders and CP among victims of interpersonal abuse (e.g., McLean & Gallop, 2003), it may be clinically useful to separate this group of trauma victims from Axis II psychopathology and subsume them under CP, a diagnosis which combines state and trait psychopathology. This would eliminate the problems of stigma, co-morbidity, misunderstanding (if not misdiagnosis), and possible mistreatment for this particular group. Recognition of symptoms common among abuse victims and knowledge regarding the range of human responses to trauma and exploitation would improve 125 treatment planning. The assessment of CP would help clinicians identify areas of psychological impairment that are essential for treatment planning and may cut across current diagnostic boundaries. Once clinicians understand the totality of the impact of trauma on psychological and interpersonal functioning, they can work with victims to ,9 ‘6 “reconnect fragments, reconstruct history,” and “make meaning of present symptoms in the light of past events” (Herman, 1992a, p. 3). Thus, given the association observed in the present research between CP and DV, mental health professionals can begin to develop a greater understanding of the full impact of DV on women, placing them in a better position to develop effective treatments with long-lasting success. Ide and Paez (2000) advocate for a multidimensional and integrative treatment approach for victims with CP, which now appears to be relevant to battered women. This approach to treatment involves a variety of tactics, including social skills training, behavior and affect management, alcohol and substance use rehabilitation, and improving family functioning, in addition to the exploration of traumatic themes. Inpatient treatment may be indicated to provide adequate safety for victims with suicidal ideation or self-harm tendencies prior to outpatient treatment (Ide & Paez, 2000). Although social support was not found to mediate the relationship between DV and CP, frequency of DV was related to poor social support. Therefore, it is appropriate for practitioners to target social support in treatment for abused women by incorporating strategies to enhance the quality of informal social support. Creating an environment of support rather than blame will empower women whose sense of control and initiative have been depleted, and communicate a societal commitment to zero tolerance for partner abuse. Prevention programs that facilitate the strengthening of social ties for women at 126 risk for DV (e. g., women with a history of childhood maltreatment) may be beneficial. These efforts could potentially reduce the likelihood of involvement in an abusive relationship characterized by coercive control. A comprehensive treatment program for battered woman should directly address post-trauma psychOpathology as well as factors that play an important role in the relationship between DV and mental health problems, such as child maltreatment. Consequently, a thorough treatment plan for an abused woman should include an assessment of childhood maltreatment history and exploration of traumatic themes related to childhood abuse if indicated, in addition to those related to DV. Ideally, victims of child maltreatment should be identified and targeted for early intervention in order to reduce vulnerability to CP. 127 Table 1 Number of Participants Missing Variable-Level Data by Study Variable and Time Period Variable T1 T2 T3 T4 T5 T6 DV Frequency 0 5 5 9 10 13 DV Severity 0 5 5 9 10 13 Threats of DV 0 5 5 9 10 13 Physical DV 0 5 5 9 1 0 1 3 Sexual DV 0 5 5 9 10 13 Child Maltreatment 17 CP Symptoms 17 No. Supporters l 5 5 9 10 13 Emotional Support 1 5 5 9 10 1 3 Long/Short—term DV 6 Partner Income 1 Note. There was no missing data for study variables not included in the table. Blank cells indicate that data for study variable was not collected at that particular time period and therefore, missingness is not applicable. 128 N_.m Q..— NoTo vmdmé 84. $4 bto>om >Q 8. who mN.N N26 3.3.: m2. Nm.~ bto>om >Q mu. mm.m ca; N26 5.36 3.0 gm bigom >Q E. 05.x omd N26 cadmé 8.x Rd bigom >Q E. 5.2 mmé ~26 95.3.9 02: one £55m >0 NH swam om._m wmmé node—6 swam om._m bto>om >Q E. wmw vmd onmé $6 Worm cod 55:3.“— >D E. m: _ m; 33 was :2: :1. 55:3; >o we who mm.m emmé 36 No.2 mo;o moses—cor; >Q vb Ed 8% £3 to on: own sconce". >a E 3.2 9; cnmé 2:5 002 S; caucus—coun— >Q NH 3.34 3.2 Evé 37o 3.3 mm.mm 5:262”— >Q Tr AcocmEzmobav Acoszzmobav owcmm owcmm Acorn—:38 $83 323653-303 033:3 QM. :32 o_£mmom 3334. QM. 5.22 921$ 33: 3339880 S $35.8; mzoantnob S\SaQ 9:52.9me N 2an 129 mom 5N v2 «.3 m2 :3 >Q Ream :- Ea :2 £3 85 8a 3. >9 Rumba E v: R; 3:. 9; m3. 8; >2 3%.: 3 win a. 8:. h To NE a. >9 .8755 E- »? SN 82. 21. 3.0 gm >0 .833; fl o: 3m £3 85 z: ”2 >3 .8553 S 8.2 ”we a To :5 8.2 ”v.2 >o. samba : mum m _ .N E I. 3.0 26 92. >o go 38:: 8 $0 m2 3 To $5 m3 0: >9 he 38:: B one am 3 To 3; go SN >o ho $2.: E N3 3m 3 To 25 m: w; >96 emefi fl we.” N3 3 To 35 2.” m3 >o E 385 E 3.: 3.2 :2 :33 $2 $2 >o mo 33:: E Acoumfitmo-oav 323550-23 owcmm owcmm $55650 -303 Acouaficmo-amoav 03%; a. :82 ”3:8“. .32 mm :32 .9253 N 03$ 130 32.—235 :3” 3.0 vmé mm; 56 mad 00 85:52 2. automazm mm.m 86 vmé w T. Nag.” 2.0 .«o “3:52 3‘ £88.55 omé ow.» vmé vm-m 3; 3.x 0o 39:52 C. flotoaasm cod 0 _ .0. vmé _m-o wwd an no .3532 NH muotoqum w; 2.: «Né «m6 ta 3.: 00 59:52 C. ow. mo. omé 9o co. no. >0 338m oh em. 2. cmé m6 3. 2. >0 338m mrr mm; mm. 3.6 o-o vm._ vm. >0 :3me E‘ 20 mm. omé w-o m: cm. >0 Esxom 2‘ mm.~ xv. cmé h To OWN cw. >0 :3me NF €23§fi923 Acoszcmoéav owcmm owcmz €238:va -303 AcotmEBmoémoav 038:; QM :82 03580 _~Bo< 0m :82 .5253 m 2an 131 M82 2? M: 3 SN 8: >9 8 .8225 on: :w 3 878 3-3. 8:: 3.; 88888282 2:6 83 88 3-8 2-8 2; 5.8 2.882% mu 3. 3% 8.8 3: M8. 3% 228% 8285 E No 83. 8-8 :8. mm. mom 8&5 .2285 B Q. 88 «.8 if Q. 9;. 2285 8:285 E Q. mom 8.8 if 3. 83 228% .8285 E mm. m3. 8.8 8.8 mm. 2% 228.8 8:285 S 3. an E 382 a» N3 228% 8:285 ; flotoamzm SM 38 2-8 E; 2X 88 8 .2882 E AcotaEcmoéav 928858-23 owcmm owcmm 0532250 -603 Acowwficmofioav 2828> QM :82 03680 .mEo< QM :32 03:53 N DEN—L 132 Table 3 Cluster Mean Profiles for DV Frequency DV Frequency Cluster T1 T2 T3 T4 T5 T6 Cluster 1: Minimal DV 7.78 1.91 2.56 1.35 2.35 3.17 Cluster 2: Moderate DV 53.47 12.77 9.63 6.82 6.86 2.66 Cluster 3: High DV 138.20 20.08 5.92 9.31 1.77 4.15 133 |l Table 4 Cross-tabulation ofDVFrequency and DVSeverity Cluster Solutions DV Frequency Cluster Cluster 1 Cluster 2 Cluster 3 DV Severity Cluster Minimum DV Moderate DV High DV Cluster 1: Minimum DV 89 24 0 Cluster 2: Moderate DV 0 38 5 Cluster 3: High DV 0 0 8 Note. Percentage of overlap between DV Frequency and DV Severity cluster solutions = 82%. 134 Table 5 C ross-tabulation of DV Frequency and Threats of DV Cluster Solutions DV Frequency Cluster Cluster 1 Cluster 2 Cluster 3 Threats of DV Cluster Minimum DV Moderate DV High DV Cluster 1: Minimum DV 85 11 0 Cluster 2: Moderate DV 4 47 1 Cluster 3: High DV 0 4 12 Note. Percentage of overlap between DV Frequency and Threats of DV cluster solutions = 88%. 135 Table 6 Cross-tabulation of DV Frequency and Physical DV Cluster Solutions DV Frequency Cluster Cluster 1 Cluster 2 Cluster 3 Physical DV Cluster Minimum DV Moderate DV High DV Cluster 1: Minimum DV 89 17 0 Cluster 2: Moderate DV 0 41 3 Cluster 3: High DV 0 4 10 Note. Percentage of overlap between DV Frequency and Physical DV cluster solutions = 85%. 136 Table 7 Cross-tabulation of DV Severity and Threats of DV Cluster Solutions DV Severity Cluster Cluster 1 Cluster 2 Cluster 3 Threats of DV Cluster Minimum DV Moderate DV High DV Cluster 1: Minimum DV 91 5 0 Cluster 2: Moderate DV 22 29 1 Cluster 3: High DV 0 9 7 Note. Percentage of overlap between DV Severity and Threats of DV cluster solutions = 77%. 137 Table 8 Cross-tabulation of DVSeverity and Physical DV Cluster Solutions DV Severity Cluster Cluster 1 Cluster 2 Cluster 3 Physical DV Cluster Minimum DV Moderate DV High DV Cluster 1: Minimum DV 106 0 0 Cluster 2: Moderate DV 7 37 0 Cluster 3: High DV 0 6 8 Note. Percentage of overlap between DV Severity and Physical DV cluster solutions = 92%. 138 Table 9 Cross-tabulation of Threats of DV and Physical DV Cluster Solutions Threats of DV Cluster Cluster 1 Cluster 2 Cluster 3 Physical DV Cluster Minimum DV Moderate DV High DV Cluster 1: Minimum DV 87 19 0 Cluster 2: Moderate DV 9 29 6 Cluster 3: High DV 0 4 10 Note. Percentage of overlap between Threats of DV and Physical DV cluster solutions = 77%. 139 Table 10 Cluster Mean Profiles for Sexual DV Sexual DV Cluster T1 T2 T3 T4 T5 T6 Cluster 1: Minimal Sexual DV 1.88 .30 .22 .08 .10 .08 Cluster 2: High Sexual DV 16.00 5.60 1.60 5.20 O 0 140 Table l l Cluster Mean Profiles for DV Frequency among Women in Long-term DV Relationships DV Frequency Cluster T1 T2 T3 T4 T5 T6 Cluster 1: Minimal DV 8.32 2.21 3.34 1.75 2.71 3.69 Cluster 2: Moderate DV 45.59 7.74 3.50 7.24 7.91 1.62 Cluster 3: High DV 112.80 15.45 15.77 9.09 6.41 6.36 141 Table 12 Cross-tabulation of DV Frequency and DVSeverity Cluster Solutions among Women in Long-term DV Relationships DV Frequency Cluster Cluster 1 Cluster 2 Cluster 3 DV Severity Cluster Minimum DV Moderate DV High DV Cluster 1: Minimum DV 75 18 0 Cluster 2: Moderate DV 1 16 14 Cluster 3: High DV 0 0 8 Note. Percentage of overlap between DV Frequency and DV Severity cluster solutions = 75%. 142 Table 13 Cross-tabulation of DVF requency and Threats of DV Cluster Solutions among Women in Long-term DV Relationships DV Frequency Cluster Cluster 1 Cluster 2 Cluster 3 Threats of DV Cluster Minimum DV Moderate DV High DV Cluster 1: Minimum DV 71 3 0 Cluster 2: Moderate DV 5 31 8 Cluster 3: High DV 0 0 14 Note. Percentage of overlap between DV Frequency and Threats of DV cluster solutions = 88%. 143 Table 14 Cross-tabulation of DV Frequency and Physical DV Cluster Solutions among Women in Long-term DV Relationships DV Frequency Cluster Cluster 1 Cluster 2 Cluster 3 Physical DV Cluster Minimum DV Moderate DV High DV Cluster 1: Minimum DV 76 13 1 Cluster 2: Moderate DV 0 21 6 Cluster 3: High DV 0 0 15 Note. Percentage of overlap between DV Frequency and Physical DV cluster solutions = 85%. 144 Table 15 C ross-tabulation of DVSeverity and Threats of D V Cluster Solutions among Women in Long-term D V Relationships DV Severity Cluster Cluster 1 Cluster 2 Cluster 3 Threats of DV Cluster Minimum DV Moderate DV High DV Cluster 1: Minimum DV 74 0 0 Cluster 2: Moderate DV 19 24 1 Cluster 3: High DV 0 7 7 Note. Percentage of overlap between DV Severity and Threats of DV cluster solutions = 80%. 145 Table 16 Cross-tabulation of DVSeverity and Physical DV Cluster Solutions among Women in Long-term DV Relationships DV Severity Cluster Cluster 1 Cluster 2 Cluster 3 Physical DV Cluster Minimum DV Moderate DV High DV Cluster 1: Minimum DV 88 2 O Cluster 2: Moderate DV 5 22 0 Cluster 3: High DV 0 7 8 Note. Percentage of overlap between DV Severity and Physical DV cluster solutions = 89%. 146 Table 17 C ross-tabulation of Threats of DV and Physical DV Cluster Solutions among Women in Long-term D V Relationships Threats of DV Cluster Cluster 1 Cluster 2 Cluster 3 Physical DV Cluster Minimum DV Moderate DV High DV Cluster 1: Minimum DV 72 17 1 Cluster 2: Moderate DV 2 21 4 Cluster 3: High DV 0 6 9 Note. Percentage of overlap between Threats of DV and Physical DV cluster solutions = 77%. 147 Table 18 Cluster Mean Profiles for Sexual DV among Women in Long-term DVRelationships Sexual DV Cluster T1 T2 T3 T4 T5 T6 Cluster 1: Minimal Sexual DV 1.82 .07 .27 .10 .11 .09 Cluster 2: High Sexual DV 16.00 5.60 1.60 5.20 O 0 148 Table 19 Cluster Mean Profiles for DV Frequency among Women in Short-term DV Relationships DV Frequency Cluster T1 T2 T3 T4 T5 T6 Cluster 1: Minimal DV 8.40 1.20 9.00 .20 .73 .13 Cluster 2: Moderate DV 49.77 11.31 3.23 4.92 2.31 1.85 Cluster 3: High DV 91.75 71.75 .25 4.50 0 0 149 Table 20 Cluster Mean Profiles/or Physical DV among Women in Short-term DVRelationships Physical DV Cluster T1 T2 T3 T4 T5 T6 Cluster 1: Minimal Physical DV 3.05 .47 3.90 0 .21 .47 Cluster 2: Early Moderate-High Physical DV 21.20 33.40 0 2.40 0 0 Cluster 3: Early High DV 27.88 1.75 2.25 .50 .75 0 150 Table 21 C ross-tabulation of DV Severity and Threats of DV Cluster Solutions among Women in Short-term DV Relationships DV Severity Cluster Cluster 1 Cluster 2 Cluster 3 Threats of DV Cluster Minimum DV Moderate DV High DV Cluster 1: Minimum DV 16 0 l Cluster 2: Moderate DV 3 2 0 Cluster 3: High DV 3 0 7 Note. Percentage of overlap between DV Severity and Threats of DV cluster solutions = 78%. 151 Table 22 Cross-tabulation of DVSeverity and Physical DV Cluster Solutions among Women in Short-term D V Relationships DV Severity Cluster Cluster 1 Cluster 2 Cluster 3 Physical DV Cluster Minimum DV Moderate DV High DV Cluster 1: Minimum DV l9 0 0 Cluster 2: Moderate DV 2 2 1 Cluster 3: High DV 1 0 7 Note. Percentage of overlap between DV Severity and Physical DV cluster solutions = 88%. 152 ll Table 23 Cross-tabulation of Threats of DV and Physical DV Cluster Solutions among Women in Short-term D V Relationships Threats of DV Cluster Cluster 1 Cluster 2 Cluster 3 Physical DV Cluster Minimum DV Moderate DV High DV Cluster 1: Minimum DV 16 1 2 Cluster 2: Moderate DV 0 4 1 Cluster 3: High DV 1 0 7 Note. Percentage of overlap between Threats of DV and Physical DV cluster solutions = 84%. 153 Table 24 Cluster Mean Profiles for Sexual DV among Women in Short-term DV Relationships Sexual DV Cluster T1 T2 T3 T4 T5 T6 Cluster 1: Minimal Sexual DV 2.07 .10 .03 0 .06 O Cluster 2: Early High Sexual DV 2.67 11.67 0 0 0 0 154 Table 25 Means and Standard Deviations for Number of Network Supporters by DVFrequency Cluster Cluster 1: Minimal DV Cluster 2: Moderate DV Cluster 3: High DV Time Period Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD T1 11.70 5.79 10.60 4.33 10.00 4.00 T2 7.74 3.82 6.34 3.32 7.77 5.93 T3 8.74 4.50 8.00 4.17 8.15 5.43 T4 6.39 3.28 5.74 3.09 5.85 3.41 T5 6.80 3.87 5.07 2.13 7.77 5.69 T6 7.45 4.32 6.05 3.09 6.00 3.06 155 Table 26 Means and Standard Deviations for Number of Network Supporters by Sexual DV Cluster Cluster 1: Minimal Sexual DV Cluster 2: High Sexual DV Time Period Mean SD Mean SD T1 11.21 5.21 9.00 3.08 T2 7.22 3.91 7.00 3.16 T3 8.43 4.49 8.00 3.16 T4 6.09 3.24 6.40 2.61 T5 6.19 3.65 7.20 1.79 T6 6.82 3.89 6.40 2.51 156 Table 27 Means and Standard Deviations for Number of Network Supporters by DV Duration Group Group] Group 2 Group 3 Group 4 (1-2 Time Periods) (3-4 Time Periods) (5-6 Time Periods) (7-8 Time Periods) Time Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD Period T1 11.84 5.49 11.72 5.85 10.26 4.37 10.78 4.72 T2 7.58 3.51 7.02 3.81 7.43 4.40 6.52 3.44 T3 8.68 4.17 8.60 4.55 8.47 4.93 7.44 3.50 T4 6.58 3.56 6.06 3.49 6.06 3.00 5.52 2.48 T5 7.26 3.86 6.28 3.84 6.06 3.66 4.74 1.71 T6 7.74 4.44 7.14 4.63 5.96 2.76 6.48 2.71 157 Table 28 Means and Standard Deviations for Emotional Support by DV Frequency Cluster Cluster 1: Minimal DV Cluster 2: Moderate DV Cluster 3: High DV Time Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD Period T] 3.29 .44 3.14 .54 3.16 .54 T2 342 .45 3.29 .60 3.25 .53 T3 3.42 .41 3.35 .42 3.32 .64 T4 3.54 .35 3.45 .49 3.35 .52 T5 3.45 .46 3.38 .51 2.97 .86 T6 3.47 .47 3.42 .49 3.35 .51 158 Table 29 Means and Standard Deviations for Emotional Support by Sexual DV Cluster Cluster 1: Minimal Sexual DV Cluster 2: High Sexual DV Time Period Mean SD Mean SD Tl 3.23 .48 3.01 .67 T2 3.36 .52 3.12 .68 T3 3.40 .41 2.83 .72 T4 3.50 .42 3.28 .66 T5 3.40 .53 2.96 .39 T6 3.45 .48 3.14 .56 159 Table 30 Means and Standard Deviations for Emotional Support by DVDuration Group Group] Group 2 Group 3 Group 4 (1-2 Time Periods) (3-4 Time Periods) (5-6 Time Periods) (7-8 Time Periods) Time Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD Period T1 3.29 .46 3.21 .51 3.20 .52 3.19 .44 T2 3.50 .44 3.41 .39 3.31 .47 3.08 .83 T3 3.46 .41 3.44 .33 3.35 .50 3.22 .45 T4 3.56 .37 3.59 .36 3.42 .44 3.33 .54 T5 3.52 .45 3.50 .38 3.29 .56 3.14 .74 T6 3.57 .40 3.41 .52 3.47 .38 3.23 .63 160 Table 31 Complex PTSD Symptom Scores as a function of DV Frequency Cluster and Attachment Status Secure Attachment Insecure Attachment DV Frequency Cluster Mean SD Mean SD Cluster 1: Minimal DV 3.54 7.09 4.50 7.18 Cluster 2: Moderate DV 6.94 6.87 7.39 8.72 Cluster 3: High DV 14.86 14.30 17.33 10.98 161 Table 32 Complex PTSD Symptom Scores as a function of Sexual DV Cluster and Attachment Status Secure Attachment Insecure Attachment Sexual DV Cluster Mean SD Mean SD Cluster 1: Minimal Sexual DV 5.04 7.66 6.96 8.86 Cluster 2: High Sexual DV 17.33 15.37 16.50 13.44 162 . >0 00 08:03 00080.0 E 08899? 00 00 208020000 05 .80 0on 1003000020. .~ 0.5ME toaazm _0_0om manExm 95.005000 0% $0.0m .003. 620050 2002:3259. A r £80100. 0 003800 .bt0>0m >0 080$ 20.500202 EEO E08582 goat—EU mo 8208:8053. :32 163 3 *— ‘“— Group3 """" Group2 8 _ '''' Group1 O S .— 5‘ C O _ 0) co 3 U 2 U- o > <0 ‘ O o _ v o _ N O\ 8 0- .fi. ' O o o. -- I I I I I I 1 2 3 4 5 6 TIme Period Figure 2. Cluster mean profiles for DV Frequency. 164 ° —_ Group2 3' """" Group1 m o_ m ‘- 3 .0 < 75 3 X a) (D o m_ 0 ..~ 0 .‘............~........ I l l I l I 1 2 3 4 5 6 Two Period Figure 3. Cluster mean profiles for Sexual DV. 165 — Group3 O """" Group2 g — """ Group1 o a co > 0 C 3 a 8 - Ll. > o o _ v a a ‘ °——°\ ..._.,8NQ._';_.. 1 2 3 4 5 6 Tune Period Figure 4. Cluster mean profiles for DV Frequency among women in long-term DV relationships. 166 ° — Group2 53 ‘ """ Group1 3 3 2 - .0 <2 E 3 X G) U) > D O o — \O-—-—.'_-—O-—-— n—n-O l I l l l l 1 2 3 4 5 6 Time Period Figure 5. Cluster mean profiles for Sexual DV among women in long-term DV relationships. 167 ° — Grom3 """" Group2 8 _ """ Group1 O o _ > (O 0 C 8 0 U' \ Q 5 LI. 0 __ ‘ > v o o _ N 0. k a .‘\ / -.' . ...Q o _ 0' .%.:>.|;;;=8 I I I m I I 1 2 3 4 5 6 TIme Period Figure 6. Cluster mean profiles for DV Frequency among women in short-term DV relationships. 168 “—- Group3 o """" Group2 m ‘ """ Group1 to _ N Q) U) 3 .o o _ < N .3 ">’. to _ c T- o. > o o _ ID —-I . /' ‘ -7“""""'.° \ ‘.'-. o q .Nafilmo I I I I I I 1 2 3 4 5 6 Time Period Figure 7. Cluster mean profiles for Physical DV among women in short-term DV relationships. 169 $3 _ ° _ Group2 """ Group1 g _ 00 J a) (D 3 .0 < E co — 3 X G) a) > o v — o N - 0‘ \ \ \ \ O _ .-—-—.-—.——.-—--0 I I I I I I 1 2 3 4 5 6 TIme Period Figure 8. Cluster mean profiles for Sexual DV among women in short-term DV relationships. 170 D MInImal DV I Moderate DV h DV . 0¢.«0¢.i.ioioiOiOioioioi‘oioiOI.‘ .ow..o..wowowowowowowowowowowo4.4.»...«o. 016‘ 1 , COM.“ 000060000... 0 O 000 0 0 o t t t . i t i b . . no». . . or. . ouowon. 3%.». , . . fl , . , 5 , , T ........................ . .. .............'.‘..'..’.. i, , .M ...4.u.- ..'.u.<.n.4.toi. Vu.»ou...now.u.u.wowou.u .«oduoc 005...... Q..‘.“.' ‘ ‘.““.‘ 0.9.0... .090... v .0.......... . o.....o...... , : T1 — _ iT 4 2 0 8 6 4. 2 o 1 1 1 29:23.6 {9302 we eonfiaz cues. 171 Time Period Figure 9. Mean number of network supporters by DV Frequency cluster and time. APPENDIX A Demographics From T1: 1. Yourdateofbirth: __/__ /__ (m0) (dY) (yr) 2. Choose the one that best describes your current marital/relationship status (choose only one): (a) single, never married (see below) (b) married For how long? (in months) (c) separated For how long? (in months) (d) divorced For how long? (in months) (e) widowed For how long? (in months) If (a) is circled: Are you currently in a relationship? YES NO If YES, go to Question 3. If NO, were you in a relationship that lasted at least 6 weeks during your current pregnancy? YES NO 3. First name of your current partner or the partner you were with for at least 6 weeks during your pregnancy: 4. Prior to your current romantic relationship, specified in Question #3 (a) were you ever married? 1 = YES 2 = NO (b) did you ever live with a partner? 1 = YES 2 = NO (0) were you ever separated? l = YES 2 = NO (d) were you ever divorced? l = YES 2 = NO (e) were you ever widowed? 1 = YES 2 = NO 5. What is your racial or ethnic group? (Circle one) 1 = Native American 2 = Asian American/Pacific Islander 3 = Black, African American 4 = Latino, Hispanic, Chicano 5 = Biracial (mixed): Specify 6 = Caucasian, White 7 = Other: 6. What is your total family income per month (estimate)? 172 APPENDIX A (cont’d). 7. What is the highest level of education you have completed? (Circle one) 1 = grades 1, 2, 3, 4, 5, or 6 (circle specific grade) 2 = grades 7, 8, 9, 10, 11, 12, GED (circle specific grade) 3 = some college Where? 4 = AA degree Where? 5 = BA/BS Where? 6 = some grad school Where? 7 = graduate degree Where? MA? Ph.D.? Law? MD? 8 = other; Specify (e.g., Beauty School, nursing school) From T3-T6: 8. When we interviewed you during your pregnancy, you had been involved with [name of TI partner: fill in from assignment sheet] for at least SIX weeks during the pregnancy. What has happened to this relationship since we interviewed you then? (Read all choices and circle one) (1) I am still in a relationship with him/her. (2) We have been together off and on since the interview, and we are currently together. (3) We have been together off and on since the interview, and we are currently not together. (4) I have not had a relationship with him/her since the interview. 9. Since your child was one year old (or two, three, or four years old depending on wave of data collection), please list the first names of the people with whom you had a romantic relationship that lasted at least 6 weeks. Please start with the most recent or current relationship and go back in time. Include the person listed in Question #8 (T 1 partner) if appropriate. 9a. Was this person the same person you were involved with one year ago? YES NO Is this person the father of your child? YES NO Are you currently involved? YES NO Anyone else? 9b. Was this person the same person you were involved with one year ago? YES NO Is this person the father of your child? YES NO Are you currently involved? YES NO Anyone else? 90. Was this person the same person you were involved with one year ago? YES NO Is this person the father of your child? YES NO Are you currently involved? YES NO 173 APPENDIX B Severity of Violence Against Women Scales INSTRUCTIONS: You and your partner have probably experienced anger or conflict. Below is a list of behaviors he may have done. Describe how often he has done each behavior to you during the last year and how many times your child saw or heard what happened by choosing a letter from the following scale. How often: A= never B= once C= a few times D= many times Stressfulness: 0= event did not occur l= not at all stressful 2= a little stressful 3= very stressful During the last year 1 Times your child saw or heard what happened 1 Stressfulness i 1. Hit or kicked a wall, door or furniture 2. Threw, smashed or broke an object 3. Driven dangerously with you in the car 4. Threw an object at you 5. Shook a finger at you 6. Made threatening gestures or faces at you 7. Shook a fist at you 8. Acted like a bully toward you 9. Destroyed something belonging to you 10. Threatened to harm or damage things you care about 1 l. Threatened to destroy property 12. Threatened someone you care about 13. Threatened to hurt you 14. Threatened to kill himself 15. Threatened you with a club-like object 16. Threatened you with a knife or gun 17. Threatened to kill you 18. Threatened you with a weapon l9. Acted like he wanted to kill you 20. Held you down, pinning you in place 2 I. Pushed or shoved you 22. Shook or roughly handled you 174 APPENDIX B (cont’d). How often: A= never B= once C= a few times D= many times Stressfulness: 0= event did not occur l= not at all stressful 2= a little stressful 3= very stressful During the last year 1 Times your child saw or heard what happened i Stressfulness I 23. Grabbed you suddenly or forcefully 24. Scratched you 25. Pulled your hair 26. Twisted your arm 27. Spanked you 28. Bit you 29. Slapped you with the palm of his hand 30. Slapped you with the back of his hand 31. Slapped you around your face and head 32. Kicked you 33. Hit you with an object 34. Stomped on you 35. Choked you 36. Punched you 37. Burned you with something 38. Used a club-like object on you 39. Beat you up 40. Used a knife or gun on you 41. Demanded sex whether you wanted to or not 42. Made you have oral sex against your will 43. Made you have sexual intercourse against your will 44. Physically forced you to have sex 45. Made you have anal sex against your will 46. Used an object on you in a sexual way 175 APPENDIX C Childhood Tramna Questionnaire — Short Form These questions ask about some of your experiences growing up as a child and a teenager. Although these questions are of a personal nature, please try to answer as honestly as you can. For each question, circle the response that best describes how you feel. WHEN I WAS GROWING UP... 1. I didn’t have enough to eat. Never True Rarely True Sometimes True Often True Very Often True 2. I knew that there was someone to take care of me and protect me. Never True Rarely True Sometimes True Often True Very Often True 3. People in my family called me things like “stupid,” “lazy,” or “ugly.” Never True Rarely True Sometimes True Often True Very Often True 4. My parents were too drunk or high to take care of the family. Never True Rarely True Sometimes True Often True Very Often True 5. There was someone in my family who helped me feel that I was important or special. Never True Rarely True Sometimes True Often True Very Often True 6. I had to wear dirty clothes. Never True Rarely True Sometimes True Often True Very Often True 7. I felt loved. Never True Rarely True Sometimes True Often True Very Often True 8. I thought that my parents wished I had never been born. Never True Rarely True Sometimes True Often True Very Often True 9. I got hit so hard by someone in my family that I had to see a doctor or go to the hospital. Never True Rarely True Sometimes True Often True Very Often True 176 APPENDIX C (cont’d). WHEN I WAS GROWING UP... 10. There was nothing I wanted to change about my family. Never True Rarely True Sometimes True Often True Very Often True 11. People in my family hit me so hard that it left me with bruises or marks. Never True Rarely True Sometimes True Often True Very Often True 12. I was punished with a belt, a board, a cord, or some other hard object. Never True Rarely True Sometimes True Often True Very Often True 13. People in my family looked out for each other. Never True Rarely True Sometimes True Often True Very Often True 14. People in my family said hurtfiil or insulting things to me. Never True Rarely True Sometimes True Often True Very Often True 15. I believe that I was physically abused. Never True Rarely True Sometimes True Often True Very Often True 16. I had the perfect childhood. Never True Rarely True Sometimes True Often True Very Often True 17. I got hit or beaten so badly that it was noticed by someone like a teacher, neighbor, or doctor. Never True Rarely True Sometimes True Often True Very Often True 18. I felt that someone in my family hated me. Never True Rarely True Sometimes True Often True Very Often True 19. People in my family felt close to each other. Never True Rarely True Sometimes True Often True Very Often True 20. Someone tried to touch me in a sexual way, or tried to make me touch them. Never True Rarely True Sometimes True Often True Very Often True 177 APPENDIX C (cont’d). WHEN I WAS GROWING UP... 21. Someone threatened to hurt me or tell lies about me unless I did something sexual with them. Never True Rarely True Sometimes True Often True Very Often True 22. I had the best family in the world. Never True Rarely True Sometimes True Often True Very Often True 23. Someone tried to make me do sexual things or watch sexual things. Never True Rarely True Sometimes True Often True Very Often True 24. Someone molested me. Never True Rarely True Sometimes True Often True Very Often True 25. I believe that I was emotionally abused. Never True Rarely True Sometimes True Often True Very Often True 26. There was someone to take me to the doctor if I needed it. Never True Rarely True Sometimes True Often True Very Often True 27. I believe that l was sexually abused. Never True Rarely True Sometimes True Often True Very Often True 28. My family was a source of strength and support. Never True Rarely True Sometimes True Often True Very Often True 178 APPENDIX D Self-Report Inventory for Disorders of Extreme Stress What follows are descriptions of typical reactions someone could have after traumatic experiences such as you have had. Please indicate if you had similar feelings SOON AFTER THE EXPERIENCE OR AS LONG AS YOU CAN REMEMBER. 1. Small problems get me very upset? For example, I get angry at a minor frustration. I cry easily. YES NO I find it hard to calm myself down after I become upset and have trouble getting back on track. YES NO When I feel upset, I have trouble finding ways to calm myself down. YES NO I feel angry most of the time. YES NO I have thoughts or images of hurting somebody else. YES NO I have trouble controlling my anger. YES NO I worry about people finding out how angry I am. YES NO I have been in accidents or near accidents. YES NO I find myself careless about making sure that I am safe. YES NO 179 ll APPENDIX D (cont’d). AFTER THE EXPERIENCE OR AS LONG AS I CAN REMEMBER... 10. I have deliberately tried to hurt myself (like burning or cutting myself) 11. 12. 13. 14. 15. 16. 17. 18. 19. YES NO I have thought about killing myself. YES NO I make active efforts to keep myself from thinking about sex. YES NO It bothers me to be touched in general. YES NO It bothers me to be touched in a sexual way. YES NO I actively avoid sex. YES NO I find myself thinking about sex more than I want to. YES NO I find myself driven to engage in sexual activities without really feeling that I had a choice. YES NO I am active sexually in ways that I know put me in danger. YES NO I expose myself to situations that might be dangerous. For example, I get involved with people who might hurt me. I go to places that are not safe. I drive too fast. YES NO 180 APPENDIX D (cont’d). AFTER THE EXPERIENCE OR AS LONG AS I CAN REMEMBER... 20. There are parts of my life that I cannot remember, or I am confused about what happened, or I am unsure whether certain important things did or did not happen to me. YES NO 21. I have difficulty keeping track of time in my daily life. YES NO 22. I ‘space’ out when I feel frightened or under stress. YES NO 23. I sometimes feel so unreal that it is as if I am living in a dream, or not really there, or behind a ‘glass wall.’ YES NO 24. I sometimes feel like there are two or more totally different people living inside me who control how I behave at different times. YES NO 25. I have the feeling that I basically have no influence or control over what happens to me in my life? YES NO 26. I feel that I have something wrong with me after what happened to me that can never be fixed? YES NO 27. I feel chronically guilty about all sorts of things. YES NO 28. I am too ashamed of myself to let people get to know me. YES NO 181 APPENDIX D (cont’d). AFTER THE EXPERIENCE OR AS LONG AS I CAN REMEMBER... 29. 30. 31. 32. 33. 34. 35. I feel set apart and very different from other people? YES NO People make too big a deal about the dangerousness of situations that I get involved in. YES NO I have trouble trusting people. YES NO I avoid having relationships with other people. YES NO I have difficulty working through conflicts in relationships. YES NO I find that other traumatic experiences keep happening to me. YES NO I have hurt other people in ways similar to how I was hurt? YES NO 36. I have trouble with (circle items that apply), yet doctors have not found a clear cause for it. YES NO a. Vomiting b. Abdominal pain c. Nausea d. Diarrhea e. Intolerance for food 182 APPENDIX D (cont’d). AFTER THE EXPERIENCE OR AS LONG AS I CAN REMEMBER... 37 . I suffer from chronic pain (circle items that apply), yet doctors have not found a clear cause for it. YES f. FVVFW NO In my arms and legs In my back In my joints During urination Headaches Elsewhere 38. I suffer from (circle items that apply), yet doctors have not found a clear cause for It. YES I. m. n. 0. NO Shortness of breath Palpitations Chest pain Dizziness 39. I suffer from trouble with (circle items that apply), yet doctors have not found a clear cause for it. YES arsseearep NO Remembering things Swallowing Losing my voice Blurred vision Actual blindness Fainting and losing consciousness Seizures and convulsions . Being able to walk Paralysis or muscle weakness Urination 183 APPENDIX D (cont’d). AFTER THE EXPERIENCE OR AS LONG AS I CAN REMEMBER... 40. I suffer from (circle items that apply), yet doctors have not found a clear cause for 41. 42. 43. 44. 45. 1t. YES NO 2. Burning sensations in my sexual organs or rectum (not during intercourse) aa. Impotence bb. Irregular menstrual periods cc. Excessive pre-menstrual tension dd. Excessive menstrual bleeding I feel hopeless and pessimistic about the future. YES NO I do not expect to be able to find happiness in love relationships. YES NO I am unable to find satisfaction in work. YES NO I believe that life has lost its meaning. YES NO There have been changes in my philosophy or religious beliefs or in the religious beliefs or philosophical beliefs I grew up with. YES NO 184 APPENDIX E Norbeck Social Support Scale Please list each significant person in your life below. Consider all the persons who provide personal support for you or who are important to you. Use only first names or initials. Use the following list to help you think of the people important to you, and list as many people as apply in your case. «spouse or partner «family members or relatives (specify: mother, father, grandmother, etc.) «friends «work or school associates «neighbors «health care providers «counselor or therapist «minister/priest/rabbi «God, Allah, or Supreme Being «other You do not have to use all 24 spaces. Use as many spaces as you have important persons in your life. l. 8. 15. 2. 9. 16. 3. 10. 17. 4. 11. 18. 5. 12. 19. 6. 13. 20. 7. 14. 21. 185 APPENDIX E (cont’d). For each person you listed, please answer the following questions by writing in the number that applies. 0 = not at all 1 = a little 2 = moderately 3 = quite a bit 4 = a great deal Question 1: How much does this person make you feel liked or loved? 1. 8. 15. 2. 9. 16. 3. 10. 17. 4. 11. 18. 5. 12. 19. 6. 13. 20. 7. 14. 21. Question 2: How much does this person make you feel respected or admired? 1. 8. 15. 2. 9. l6. 3. 10. 17. 4. 11. 18. 5. 12. 19. 6. 13. 20. 7. 14. 21. 186 APPENDIX E (cont’d). For each person you listed, please answer the following questions by writing in the number that applies. 0 = not at all 1 = a little 2 = moderately 3 = quite a bit 4 = a great deal Question 3: How much can you confide in this person? 1. 8. 15. 2. 9. 16. 3. 10. 17. 4. 11. 18. 5. 12. 19. 6. 13. 20. 7. 14. 21. Question 4: How much does this person agree with or support your actions or thoughts? 1. 8. 15. 2. 9. 16. 3. 10. 17. 4. ll. 18. 5. 12. 19. 6. 13. 20. 7. 14. 21. 187 APPENDIX F Perceptions of Adult Attachment Questionnaire The majority of the following statements refer to your early childhood relationship with your mother (when you were approximately 3 to 8 years old). If someone else was the principal person responsible for your care in childhood, please respond to the questions which refer to “mother” with that person in mind. A few of the questions have two parts. For example “when I caused trouble as a child I knew my mother would forgive me.” Some people might feel like they never caused trouble as a child; however, they consider their mothers very forgiving. How then do you answer? Only answer AGREE or STRONGLY AGREE if you agree with both parts of the statement. If you agree with only one part of the statement, answer NEUTRAL. If you disagree with both parts of the statement, answer DISAGREE or STRONGLY DISAGREE. l= STRONGLY DISAGREE 2 = DISAGREE 3 = NEUTRAL (NEITHER DISAGREE NOR AGREE) 4 = AGREE S = STRONGLY AGREE 1. In childhood, I felt like I was really treasured by my mother. 2. In childhood I sometimes felt like my mother was really lonely when I was not with her. 3. My mother was not very affectionate. 4. When I was a young child and little things went wrong, I did not feel sure I could count on my mother to take care of me. 5. As a child, I couldn’t stand being separated from my mother. 6. My mother can make me feel really good, but when she is not nice to me she can really tear me apart. 7. In my family of origin, we don’t make a show of expressing our feelings. We prefer keeping feelings to ourselves. 8. Neither my mother nor myself are perfect, but somehow we made it through my childhood. 9. I remember when I was frightened as a child, my mother holding me close. 188 APPENDIX F (cont’d). l= STRONGLY DISAGREE 2 = DISAGREE 3 = NEUTRAL (NEITHER DISAGREE NOR AGREE) 4 = AGREE 5 = STRONGLY AGREE 10. When I was a child, my mother sometimes told me that if I was not good she would stop loving me. 11. My mother is selfishly caught up in herself to the exclusion of everybody else. 12. My family was not particularly intimate, but this has never bothered me. 13. It’s hard for me to remember my early relationship with my mother in any detail. 14. In childhood, I sometimes felt that my mother and I were so alike that I didn’t know where she ended and I began. 15. If anything happened to my mother, I wonder if I could survive it. 16. I remember as a child feeling a desire to protect my mother. 17. Even though I went through rough times with my mother during my childhood, somewhere along the line I managed to let go of the majority of those angry, hurt feelings. 18. In childhood, I knew I was low on my mother’s priority list. 19. My mother was an all-around excellent mother. 20. No one gets under my skin like my mother. 21. As a child, I never thought separations from my parents were any big deal. 22. I often felt responsible for my mother’s welfare. 23. In childhood, my mother sometimes threatened to leave me or to send me away if I wasn’t good. 24. To this day, my mother has no clue who I am or what I am all about. 189 APPENDIX F (cont’d). 1= STRONGLY DISAGREE 2 = DISAGREE 3 = NEUTRAL (NEITHER DISAGREE NOR AGREE) 4 = AGREE 5 = STRONGLY AGREE _— 25. 26. 27. 28. 29. 30. 31. 32. 33. 34. 35. 36. 37. 38. 39. Even with all our past difficulties, I realize my mother did the best for me that she could. I have forgotten what most of my early childhood was like. I always knew my mother was there for me; no matter what, I could depend on her. There are times when I feel like shaking my mother and saying “wake up and see me for who I am.” In childhood, I often had the impression that my mother was not listening to me. She often tuned me out. During my childhood, I sometimes felt like I was my mother’s whole life. My mother and I are more accepting of each other’s differences than we have been in the past. When I was young, I often feared something dreadful would happen to my mother or father. I remember my mother telling me that I didn’t pay enough attention to her or love her enough. I often take my mother’s opinions about me to heart and lose sight of my own opinions about myself. My mother is a real nag. My mother and I were so alike we often could finish each other’s sentences. I think people put too much emphasis on the mother/child relationship. I remember very little about my early childhood (ages three to seven). The concept of the loving, supporting mother is pure myth. 190 APPENDIX F (cont’d). l= STRONGLY DISAGREE 2 = DISAGREE 3 = NEUTRAL (NEITHER DISAGREE NOR AGREE) 4 = AGREE 5 = STRONGLY AGREE 40. My relationship with my mother has gone through major changes over the course of my childhood and adolescence. 41. Even as an adult, I sometimes feel like I will never dig myself out from under my mother’s influence. 42. As a child, I sometimes got the feeling that without me my mother would have fallen apart. 43. I couldn’t have asked for a better mother. 44. If my mother was not fair to me as a child, I realize now it was because she was dealing with her own problems. 45. If something really bad happened to me in childhood, I did not feel I could count on my mother to support me. 46. When I was a child, I sometimes got the feeling that my mother wished I was never born. 47. I remember when I was a child feeling scared that one or both of my parents would die unexpectedly. 48. My mother can devastate me with her criticisms. 49. In childhood, my mother often told me she was sacrificing herself for me. 50. I don’t think my early childhood relationship with my mother has any significant influence on who I am today or my present relationships. 51. My mother was always there for me when I needed her. 52. When I acted bad as a child, my mother would, at times, threaten to send me away. 53. I never felt like my mother gave me enough attention. 191 APPENDIX F (cont’d). l= STRONGLY DISAGREE 2 = DISAGREE 3 = NEUTRAL (NEITHER DISAGREE NOR AGREE) 4 = AGREE 5 = STRONGLY AGREE 54. For all our past problems, my mother and I can still enjoy a good laugh together. 55. During my childhood, my mother would often turn to me and tell me lots of things that upset and bothered me. 56. In childhood, I often worried about my mother’s state of health. 57. I find it difficult to remember my early childhood. 58. My mother was a perfect mother. 59. My mother’s issues are still interfering with my life. 60. When I think back to my early childhood experiences, I discover things about myself and my parents that I’ve never considered before. 192 APPENDIX G Imputed Variables with Corresponding Matching Variables Imputed Variable Matching Variables Income T1 N0. Supporters T2 N0. Supporters T3 N0. Supporters T4 N0. Supporters T5 N0. Supporters T6 N0. Supporters T1 Emotional Support T2 Emotional Support T3 Emotional Support T4 Emotional Support T5 Emotional Support T6 Emotional Support Age, Race, Marital Status, Education Age, Race, Marital Status, Education, Income, Attachment Security T1 No. Supporters, Education, Income T1 & T2 No. Supporters, Education, Income T1, T2, & T3 N0. Supporters, Education, Income T1-T4 No. Supporters, Education, Income Tl-TS No. Supporters, Education, Income Age, Race, Marital Status, Education, Income, Attachment Security T1 Emotional Support T1 & T2 Emotional Support T1-T3 Emotional Support T1-T4 Emotional Support Tl-TS Emotional Support 193 APPENDIX G (cont’d). Imputed Variable Matching Variables T2 DV Frequency T3 DV Frequency T4 DV Frequency T5 DV Frequency T6 DV Frequency T2 DV Severity T3 DV Severity T4 DV Severity T5 DV Severity T6 DV Severity T2 Threats of DV T3 Threats of DV T4 Threats of DV T5 Threats of DV T1 DV Frequency T1 & T2 DV Frequency T1-T3 DV Frequency T1-T4 DV Frequency Tl-TS DV Frequency T1 DV Severity T1 & T2 Dv Severity T1-T3 DV Severity T1-T4 DV Severity T1-T5 DV Severity Tl Threats of DV & Tl DV Frequency T1 & T2 Threats of DV & DV Frequency T1-T3 Threats of DV & DV Frequency T1-T4 Threats of DV & DV Frequency 194 APPENDIX G (cont’d). Imputed Variable Matching Variables T6 Threats of DV T2 Physical DV T3 Physical DV T4 Physical DV T5 Physical DV T6 Physical DV T2 Sexual DV T3 Sexual DV T4 Sexual DV T5 Sexual DV T6 Sexual DV Child Maltreatment CP Symptoms Tl-TS Threats of DV & DV Frequency Tl Physical DV & DV Frequency T1 & T2 Physical DV & DV Frequency T1-T3 Physical DV & DV Frequency Tl-T4 Physical DV & DV Frequency T1 -T5 Physical DV & DV Frequency T1 Sexual DV & DV Frequency T1 & T2 Sexual DV & DV Frequency Tl-T3 Sexual DV & DV Frequency T1-T4 Sexual DV & DV Frequency T1-T5 Sexual DV & DV Frequency Attachment Security, T1-T6 DV Frequency Child Maltreatment, T1-T6 DV Frequency 195 APPENDIX H Clustering Results for Severity of DV, Threats of DV, and Physical DV Table H1 Cluster Mean Profiles for DVSeverity DV Severity Cluster T1 T2 T3 T4 T5 T6 Cluster 1: Minimal DV 7.49 1.88 1.60 .75 1.18 1.46 Cluster 2: Moderate DV 43.16 9.22 7.34 5.49 6.27 1.90 Cluster 3: High DV 98.93 11.26 5.03 6.52 1.33 3.33 196 APPENDIX H (cont’d). Table H2 Cluster Mean Profiles for Threats of DV Threats of DV Cluster T1 T2 T3 T4 T5 T6 Cluster 1: Minimal DV 5.24 1.38 .99 1.24 1.67 1.58 Cluster 2: Moderate DV 25.04 8.79 6.50 4.27 5.21 2.44 Cluster 3: High DV 57.16 10.88 5.56 3.44 1.38 3.00 197 APPENDIX H (cont’d). Table H3 Cluster Mean Profiles for Physical DV Physical DV Cluster T1 T2 T3 T4 T5 T6 Cluster 1: Minimal DV 2.83 1.05 .54 .26 .50 1.03 Cluster 2: Moderate DV 21.89 5.14 5.64 2.16 3.21 .91 Cluster 3: High DV 56.00 3.86 2.00 1.64 .86 .93 198 APPENDIX H (cont’d). 8 _ *- — Group3 """" Grorp2 """ Group1 o _ (D e 8 a a) > m a) > o g _. O _ N .-\~ 8\:.. -......‘ o \e-—-—:-—-_::>.n-i= ' I I I I 1 2 3 4 5 6 Tune Period Figure H1. Cluster mean profiles for DV Severity. 199 APPENDIX H (cont’d). — Group3 """"" Group2 8 _ """ Group1 O _ v (D (D 3 .0 < “a P.— 22 (U 2! .c I— o_ N o _ ‘- °.\\ .~\§ 8&8 ...... .....Q I I I I I I 1 2 3 4 5 6 'I'Ime Period Figure H2. Cluster mean profiles for Threats of DV. 200 APPENDIX H (cont’d). *— Group3 o """" Group2 ID “ """ Group1 o _ v (D (I) 3 .0 < o _ _ (‘0 .3 (D > .C o. o _. N g _. ...... 0.. .-~-~:\.—......-....°..Qo I I I I I 1 2 3 4 5 6 Time Period Figure H3. Cluster mean profiles for Physical DV. 201 APPENDIX I Cluster Results for Severity of DV, Threats of DV, and Physical DV among Women in Long-term DV Relationships Table II Cluster Mean Profiles for DV Severity among Women in Long-term DVRelationships DV Severity Cluster T1 T2 T3 T4 T5 T6 Cluster 1: Minimal DV 7.21 1.33 1.74 .90 1.31 1.64 Cluster 2: Moderate DV 43.50 6.41 7.10 6.85 8.35 2.60 Cluster 3: High DV 98.93 11.26 5.03 6.52 1.33 3.33 202 APPENDIX I (cont’d). Table 12 Cluster Mean Profiles for Threats of DV among Women in Long-term DVRelationships Threats of DV Cluster T1 T2 T3 T4 T5 T6 Cluster 1: Minimal DV 4.08 .72 1.30 .73 1.31 1.60 Cluster 2: Moderate DV 24.91 8.09 5.32 5.30 7.02 3.23 Cluster 3: High DV 58.06 9.29 7.64 6.36 1.29 3.43 203 APPENDIX I (cont’d). Table I3 Cluster Mean Profiles for Physical DV among Women in Long-term DV Relationships Physical DV Cluster T1 T2 T3 T4 T5 T6 Cluster 1: Minimal DV 3.06 .72 .99 .64 .57 1.17 Cluster 2: Moderate DV 21.11 1.52 1. .33 1.82 3.22 .22 Cluster 3: High DV 54.33 6.33 7.73 1.53 3.87 2.80 204 APPENDIX I (cont’d). 100 1 —“- Group 3 """"" Group 2 """ Group 1 80 I O .— g co 0.) > a) (D > o g .- Q d N \0 .~\ 0"\"~GS'——w'- O”"’".‘~... I T I I I I 1 2 3 4 5 6 TIme Period Figure 11. Cluster mean profiles for DV Severity among women in long-term DV relationships. 205 APPENDIX I (cont’d). O (D — Group 3 """" Group 2 o _ """ Group 1 to DV Threats of Abuse 30 I amt:\8 ...o.... .~ ...... . .s. o __ \‘\.-_-—e-_-_.:_\e-‘::O I I I I I I 1 2 3 4 5 6 Time Period Figure 12. Cluster mean profiles for Threats of DV among women in long-term DV relationships. 206 APPENDIX 1 (cont’d). "— Group3 8 _ """" Group2 """ Group1 o _ v (D In 3 .0 < _ O __ 8 m '5 > .0 CL .3 a — o _ ‘— ‘ o 0‘ Q/ o — \~‘8. :__.&H.Ivh= g?::---".m. I I I I I 1 2 3 4 5 6 TIme Period Figure J1. Cluster mean profiles for DV Severity among women in short-term DV relationships. 210 APPENDIX .1 (cont’d). — Group3 8 _ """"" Group2 """ Group1 to _ N a) (I) 3 .0 O _ < N “5 {3 9.’ 53* .C I'- > o g- m —1 .~ o°\ 9-—-—-. e _ O _ ‘9 ...... 83:.--ofi' I I I I I 1 2 3 4 5 6 TIme Period Figure J2. Cluster mean profiles for Threats of DV among women in short-term DV relationships. 211 REFERENCES Adam, K. S., Sheldon Keller, A. E., & West, M. (1996). Attachment organization and history of suicidal behavior in clinical adolescents. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 64(2), 264-272. Alexander, R. (1993). Wife battering: An Australian perspective. Journal of Family Violence, 8, 229-251. Allen, J. G., Coyne, L., & Huntoon, J. (1998). Complex posttraumatic stress disorder in women from a psychometric perspective. Journal of Personality Assessment, 70(2), 277-298. Allen, J. G., Huntoon, J ., & Evans, R. B. (1999). Complexities in complex posttraumatic stress disorder in inpatient women: Evidence from cluster analysis of MCMI-III personality disorder scales. Journal of Personality Assessment, 73(3), 449-471. Allen, J. P., Hauser, S. T., & Borman Spurrell, E. (1996). Attachment theory as a framework for understanding sequelae of severe adolescent psychopathology: An 11-year follow-up study. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 64(2), 254-263. American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed. ). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association. Anderson, D. K., Saunders, D. G., Yoshihama, M., Bybee, D., & Sullivan, C. M. (2003). Long-term trends in depression among women separated from abusive partners. Violence Against Women, 9(7), 807-838. Araujo, A. B., Mohr, B. A., & McKinlay, J. B. (2004). Changes in sexual function in middle-aged and older men: Longitudinal data from the Massachusetts male aging study. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 52(9), 1502-1509. Armsden, G. C., & Greenberg, M. T. (1987). The inventory of parent and peer attachment: Individual differences and their relationship to psychological well- being in adolescence. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 16(5), 427-454. 212 Astin, M. C., Lawrence, K. J ., & Foy, D. W. (1993). Posttraumatic stress disorder among battered women: Risk and resiliency factors. Violence and Victims, 8(1), 17-28. Barnett, D., Ganiban, J ., & Cicchetti, D. (1999). Maltreatment, negative expressivity, and the development of Type D attachments from 12 to 24 months of age. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 64, 97-118. Barnett, O. W., Martinez, T. E., & Keyson, M. (1996). The relationship between violence, social support, and self-blame in battered women. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 11(2), 221-233. Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51(6), 1 173-1 182. Benoit, D., & Parker, K. (1994). Stability and transmission of attachment across three generations. Child Development, 65, 1444-1456. Bernstein, D. P., & Fink, L. (1998). Childhood Trauma Questionnaire manual. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation. Bernstein, D. P., Stein, J. A., Newcomb, M. D., Walker, E., Pogge, D., Ahluvalia, T., et al. (2003). Development and validation of a brief screening version of the Childhood Trauma Questionnaire. Child Abuse and Neglect, 27(2), 169-190. Blaustein, M. E., Spinazzola, J ., Simpson, W., & van der Kolk, B. (2000). Psychological sequelae of early trauma: Comorbid diagnoses or diagnostic entity?, 16th annual meeting of the International Society for Traumatic Stress Studies. San Antonio, TX. Bogat, G., Levendosky, A. A., Theran, 8., von Eye, A., & Davidson, W. S. (2003). Predicting the psychosocial effects of interpersonal partner violence (IPV): How much does a woman's history of IPV matter? Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 18(11), 1271-1291. Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss. Vol 1. Attachment. New York: Basic Books. Bowlby, J. (1977). The making and breaking of affectional bonds: 11. Some principles of psychotherapy. British Journal of Psychiatry, 130, 421-431. 213 Bowlby, J. (1980). Attachment and loss: V01. 3. Loss, sadness, and depression. New York: Basic Books, Inc. Bureau of Justice Statistics. (1998). Bureau of Justice Statistics sourcebook of the criminal justice statistics--1997. US. Department of Justice. NCJ171147 (pp. 198). Butcher, J ., Dahlstrom, W. G., Graham, J ., Tellegen, A., & Kraemmer, B. (1989). MMPI- 2: Manual for administration and scoring. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press. Campbell, J. C., Harris, M. J ., & Lee, R. K. (1995). Violence research: An overview. Scholarly Inquiryfor Nursing Practice, 9(2), 105-126. Carlson, B. B, McNutt, L. A., Choi, D. Y., & Rose, 1. M. (2002). Intimate partner abuse and mental health: The role of social support and other protective factors. Violence Against Women, 8(6), 720-745. Carlson, E. A. (1998). A prospective longitudinal study of attachment disorganization/disorientation. Child Development, 69, 1107-1128. Cascardi, M., Langhinrichsen, J ., & Vivian, D. (1992). Marital aggression: Impact, injury, and health correlates for husbands and wives. Archives of Internal Medicine, 152, 1178-1184. Cascardi, M., & O’Leary, K. (1992). Depressive symptomatology, self-esteem, and self- blame in battered women. Journal of Family Violence, 7(4), 249-259. Cassidy, J ., & Mohr, J. J. (2001). Unsolvable fear, trauma, and psychopathology: Theory, research, and clinical considerations related to disorganized attachment across the life span. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 8(3), 275-298. Ceballo, R., Ramirez, C., Castillo, M., Caballero, G. A., & Lozoff, B. (2004). Domestic violence and women's mental health in Chile. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 28(4), 298-308. Champion, J. D., Shain, R. N., Piper, J ., & Perdue, S. T. (2001). Sexual abuse and sexual risk behaviors of minority women with sexually transmitted diseases. Western Journal of Nursing Research, 23(3), 241-254. 214 Christopoulos, C., Cohn, D. A., Shaw, D. 8., Joyce, S., Sullivan-Hanson, J ., Kraft, S., et al. (1987). Children of abused women: I. Adjustment at time of shelter residence. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 49(3), 611-619. Coker, A. L., Smith, P. H., Thompson, M. P., McKeown, R. E., Bethea, L., & Davis, K. E. (2002). Social support protects against the negative effects of partner violence on mental health. Journal of Women 's Health and Gender Based Medicine, 11(5), 465-476. Coker, A. L., Watkins, K. W., Smith, P. H., & Brandt, H. M. (2003). Social support reduces the impact of partner violence on health: Application of structural equation models. Preventive Medicine: An International Journal Devoted to Practice and Theory, 3 7(3), 259-267. Cole-Detke, H., & Kobak, R. (1996). Attachment processes in eating disorder and depression. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 64(2), 282-290. Coventry, W. L., Gillespie, N. A., Heath, A. C., & Martin, N. G. (2004). Perceived social support in a large community sample: Age and sex differences. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 39, 625-636. Crittenden, P.-M. (1997). Toward an integrative theory of trauma: A dynamic-maturation approach. In S. Toth & D. Cicchetti (Eds), Developmental perspectives on trauma: Theory, research, and intervention (pp. 33-84). Rochester, NY: University of Rochester Press. Crowell, J ., Treboux, D., & Waters, E. (1999). The Adult Attachment Interview and the Relationship Questionnaire: Relations to reports of mothers and partners. Personal Relationships, 6(1), 1-1 8. Curry, M. A., & Harvey, S. (1998). Stress related to domestic violence during pregnancy and infant birth weight. In J. C. Campbell (Ed.), Empowering survivors of abuse: Health care for battered women and their children (pp. 98-108). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications, Inc. Davidson, J. A. (1993). Issues in the diagnosis of posttraumatic stress disorder. In J. Oldham, M. Riba & A. Tasman (Eds), Review of psychiatry (V 01. 12, pp. 141- 155). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press. 215 Dickinson, L., deGruy, F. V., 111, Dickinson, W., & Candib, L. M. (1998). Complex posttraumatic stress disorder: Evidence from the primary care setting. General Hospital Psychiatry, 20(4), 214-224. Dorahy, M. J ., Lewis, C. A., & Wolfe, F. A. M. (2007). Psychological distress associated with domestic violence in Northern Ireland. Current Psychology: Developmental, Learning, Personality, Social, 25(4), 295-305. Doyle, J. R, Frank, E., Saltzman, L. E., McMahon, P. M., & Fielding, B. D. (1999). Domestic violence and sexual abuse in women physicians: Associated medical, psychiatric, and professional difficulties. Journal of Women 's Health and Gender Based Medicine, 8(7), 955-965. Dutton, D. G., Landolt, M. A., Starzomski, A., & Bodnarchuk, M. (2001). Validation of the propensity for abusiveness scale in diverse male populations. Journal of Family Violence, 16(1), 59-73. Dutton, D. G., & Painter, S. (1993). Emotional attachments in abusive relationships: A test of traumatic bonding theory. Violence and Victims, 8(2), 105-120. Dutton, M. A., & Goodman, L. A. (1994). Posttraumatic stress disorder among battered women: Analysis of legal implications. Behavioral Sciences and the Law, 12, 215-234. El-Bassel, N., Gilbert, L., Rajah, V., Foleno, A., & Frye, V. (2001). Social support among women in methadone treatment who experience partner violence. Violence Against Women, 7(3), 246-274. Farris, C. A., & Fenaughty, A. M. (2002). Social isolation and domestic violence among female drug users. American Journal of Drug and Alcohol Abuse, 28(2), 339-352. Finkelhor, D., & Yllo, K. (1987). License to rape: Sexual abuse of wives. New York: Free Press. Fischer, C. S. (1982). To Dwell Among Friends. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Fonagy, P., Leigh, T., Steele, M., Steele, H., Kennedy, R., Mattoon, G., et al. (1996). The relation of attachment status, psychiatric classification, and response to psychotherapy. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 64(1), 22-31. 216 Ford, J. D., Stockton, P., Kaltman, S., & Green, B. L. (2006). Disorders of extreme stress (DESNOS) symptoms are associated with type and severity of interpersonal trauma exposure in a sample of healthy young women. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 21(11), 1399-1416. Forte, J. A., Franks, D. D., Forte, J. A., & Rigsby, D. (1996). Asymmetrical role-taking: Comparing battered and nonbattered women. Social Work, 41(1), 59-73. Fowler, D. N., & Hill, H. M. (2004). Social support and spirituality as culturally relevant factors in coping among African American survivors of partner abuse. Violence Against Women, 10(11), 1267-1282. Frank, E., & Dingle, A. D. (1999). Self-reported depression and suicide attempts among US. women physicians. American Journal of Psychiatry, 156(12), 1887-1894. Frasier, P. Y., Belton, L., Hooten, E., Campbell, M. K., DeVellis, B., Benedict, S., et al. (2004). Disaster down east: Using participatory action research to explore intimate partner violence in eastern North Carolina. Health Education and Behavior, 31(4), 69s-84s. Freud, S. (1995). Beyond the pleasure principle (1. Strachey, Trans). New York: W. W. Norton and Co, Inc. (Original work published 1920). Gold, S. (2000). Not trauma alone: Therapy for child abuse survivors in family and social context. Philadelphia: Brunner/Routledge. Golding, J. M. (1999). Intimate partner violence as a risk factor for mental disorders: A meta-analysis. Journal of Family Violence, 14(2), 99-132. Grinker, R. R., & Spiegel, J. P. (1945). Men under stress. Oxford, England: Blakiston. Hallstrom, T., & Sarnuelsson, S. (1990). Changes in women's sexual desire in middle life: The longitudinal study of women in Gothenburg. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 19(3), 259-268. Hamilton, C., E. (1995). Continuity and discontinuity in attachment from infancy through adolescence, Biennial Meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development. Indianapolis. 217 Hamilton, C., E. (2000). Continuity and discontinuity of attachment from infancy through adolescence. Child Development, 71(3), 690-694. Harter, S. L., & Neimeyer, R. A. (1995). Long-term effects of child sexual abuse; Toward a constructivist theory of trauma and its treatment. Advances in personal construct psychology, 3, 229-269. Herman, J. L. (1992a). Trauma and recovery. New York: Basic Books, Inc. Herman, J. L. (1992b). Complex PTSD: A syndrome in survivors of prolonged and repeated trauma. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 5(3), 377-391. Hesse, E. (1999). The Adult Attachment Interview: Historical and current perspectives. In J. Crowell (Ed.), Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications (pp. 434-465). New York: Guilford Press. Hilberman, E., & Munson, K. (1978). Sixty battered women. Victimology, 2(3-sup-4), 460-470. Houskarnp, B. M., & Foy, D. W. (1991). The assessment of posttraumatic stress disorder in battered women. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 6(3), 367-375. Huth-Bocks, A. C., Levendosky, A. A., Bogat, G., & von Eye, A. (2004). The Impact of Maternal Characteristics and Contextual Variables on Infant-Mother Attachment. Child Development, 75(2), 480-496. Ide, N., & Paez, A. (2000). Complex PTSD: A review of current issues. International Journal of Emergency Mental Health, 2(1), 43 -49. Jasinski, J. L. (2001). Pregnancy and violence against women: An analysis of longitudinal data. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 16(7), 712-733. Jones, E. E. (1996). Introduction to the special section on attachment and psychopathology: Part1. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 64, 5-7. Kardiner, A., & Spiegel, H. (1947). War stress and neurotic illness (2nd ed.). Oxford, England: P. B. Hoeber. 218 Katzelnick, D., Kobak, K. A., DeLeire, T., Henk. H. J ., Greist, J. H., Davidson, J. R., et al. (2001). Impact of generalized social anxiety disorder in managed care. American Journal of Psychiatry, 158(12), 1999-2007. Kemp, A., Green, B. L., Hovanitz, C., & Rawlings, E. I. (1995). Incidence and correlates of posttraumatic stress disorder in battered women: Shelter and community samples. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 10(1), 43-55. Kemp, A., Rawlings, E. 1., & Green, B. L. (1991). Post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) in battered women: A shelter sample. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 4(1), 137-148. Kernic, M. A., Wolf, M. E., & Holt, V. L. (2000). Rates and relative risk of hospital admission among women in violent intimate partner relationships. American Journal of Public Health, 90(9), 1416-1420. Levendosky, A. A., Bogat, G., Theran, S. A., Trotter, J. 8., von Eye, A., & Davidson, W. S., 11. (2004). The social networks of women experiencing domestic violence. American Journal of Community Psychology, 34(1-2), 95-109. Levendosky, A. A., Bogat, G. A., Davidson, W. S., & von Eye, A. (2000). Risk and protective factors for domestic violence. ROI Grant from Centers for Disease Control, Injury Prevention Center: Total Budget: $875,811 (10/1/00-9/31/03). Lichtenstein, J ., & Cassidy, J. (1991). The Inventory of Adult Attachment (INVAA): Validation of a new measure of adult attachment, Society for Research in Child Development. Seattle, Washington. Lown, E., & Vega, W. A. (2001). Intimate partner violence and health: Self-assessed health, chronic health, and somatic symptoms among Mexican American women. Psychosomatic Medicine, 63(3), 352-360. Luxenberg, T., Spinazzola, J ., & van der Kolk, B. (2001). Complex trauma and disorders of extreme stress (DESNOS) diagnosis, part 1: Assessment. Directions in Psychiatry, 21 , 373-392. Main, M. (1990). Cross-cultural studies of attaclunent organization: Recent studies, changing methodologies, and the concept of conditional strategies. Human Development, 33(1), 48-61. 219 Main, M., & Goldwyn, R. (1994). Adult attachment scoring and classification system.Unpublished manuscript, Department of Psychology, University of California, Berkeley. Marshall, L. L. (1992). Development of the Severity of Violence Against Women Scales. Journal of Family Violence, 7(2), 103-121. Marshall, L. L. (1996). Psychological abuse of women: Six distinct clusters. Journal of Family Violence, 11(4), 379-409. Marshall, L. L. (1999). Effects of men's subtle and overt psychological abuse on low- income women. Violence and Victims, 14(1), 69-88. Martin, S. L., Mackie, L., Kupper, L. L., Buescher, P. A., & Moracco, K. E. (2001). Physical abuse of women before, during, and after pregnancy. Journal of the American Medical Association, 285(12), 1581-1584. Matud-Aznar, M. P., Aguilera-Avila, L., Marrero-Quevedo, R. J., Moraza-Pulla, O., & Carballeira-Abella, M. (2003). Social support in battered women. International Journal of Clinical and Health Psychology, 3(3), 439-459. McLean, L. M., & Gallop, R. (2003). Implications of childhood sexual abuse for adult borderline personality disorder and complex posttraumatic stress disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 160(2), 369-371. Meadows, L. A., Kaslow, N. J ., Thompson, M. P., & Jurkovic, G. J. (2005). Protective factors against suicide attempt risk among African American women experiencing intimate partner violence. American Journal of Community Psychology, 3 6(1/2), 109-121. Meisel, J ., Chandler, D., & Rienzi, B. M. (2003). Domestic violence prevalence and effects on employment in two California TANF populations. Violence Against Women, 9(10), 1191-1212. Mertin, P., & Mohr, P. B. (2000). Incidence and correlates of posttraumatic stress disorder in Australian victims of domestic violence. Journal of Family Violence, 15(4), 411-422. 220 Mitchell, R. E., & Hodson, C. A. (1983). Coping with domestic violence: Social support and psychological health among battered women. American Journal of Community Psychology, 11(6), 629-654. Mitchell, R. E., & Hodson, C. A. (1986). Coping and social support among battered women: An ecological perspective. In S. E. Hobfoll (Ed), Stress, social support, and women: The series in clinical and community psychology (pp. 153-169). New York: Hemisphere. Moore, T. E., Pepler, D., Mae, R., & Michele, K. (1989). Effects of family violence on children: New directions for research and intervention. In G. Cameron & B. Pressman (Eds), Intervening with assaulted women: Current theory, research, and practice (pp. 75-91). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Newman, E., Riggs, D. S., & Roth, S. (1997). Thematic resolution, PTSD, and complex PTSD: The relationship between meaning and trauma-related diagnoses. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 10(2), 197-213. Nixon, R. D. V., Resick, P. A., & Nishith, P. (2004). An exploration of comorbid depression among female victims of intimate partner violence with posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Affective Disorders, 82(2), 315-320. Noll, J. G., Horowitz, L. A., Bonanno, G. A., Trickett, P. K., & Putnam, F. W. (2003). Revictimization and self-harm in females who experienced childhood sexual abuse: Results from a prospective study. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 18(12), 1452-1471. Norbeck, J. S., Lindsey, A. M., & Carrieri, V. L. (1981). The development of an instrument to measure social support. Nursing Research, 30(5), 264-269. Norbeck, J. S., Lindsey, A. M., & Carrieri, V. L. (1983). Further development of the Norbeck Social Support Questionnaire: Normative data and validity testing. Nursing Research, 32(1), 4-9. Olson, B. D., Curtis, C. E., Jason, L. A., Ferrari, J. R., Horin, E. V., Davis, M. 1., et al. (2003). Physical and sexual trauma, psychiatric symptoms, and sense of community among women in recovery: Toward a new model of shelter afiercare. Journal of Prevention and Intervention in the Community, 26(1), 67-80. 221 Pelcovitz, D., van der Kolk, B. A., Roth, 8., Mandel, F ., Kaplan, S., & Resick, P. (1997). Development of a criteria set and a structured interview for disorders of extreme stress (SIDES). Journal of Traumatic Stress, 10(1), 3-16. Pianta, R. C., Egeland, B., & Adam, E. K. (1996). Adult attachment classification and self-reported psychiatric symptomatology as assessed by the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory«2. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 64(2), 273-281. Pico-Alfonso, M. A. (2005). Psychological intimate partner violence: The major predictor of posttraumatic stress disorder in abused women. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 29(1), 181-193. Plichta, S. B. (1996). Violence and abuse: Implications for women's health. In K. S. Collings & M. M. F alik (Eds), Women 's health: The Commonwealth Fund survey (pp. 23 7-270). Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins University Press. Rennison, C. M. (2001). Intimate partner violence and age of victim, 1993-99. US. Department of Justice. NCJ 187635 (pp. 1-12): Bureau of Justice Statistics. Rennison, C. M. (2003). Intimate partner violence, 1993-2001. US. Department of Justice. NCJ 197838 (pp. 1-2): Bureau of Justice Statistics. Roberts, G. L., Lawrence, J. M., O'Toole, B. I., & Raphael, B. (1997). Domestic violence in the emergency department: 1. Two case-control studies of victims. General Hospital Psychiatry, 19(1), 5-11. Rokach, A. (2006). Lonliness in domestically abused women. Psychological Reports, 98(2), 367-373. Rorty, M., & Yager, J. (1996). Histories of childhood trauma and complex post-traumatic sequelae in women with eating disorders. Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 19(4), 773-791. Rosenstein, D. S., & Horowitz, H. A. (1996). Adolescent attachment and psychopathology. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 64(2), 244-253. 222 Roth, S., Newman, E., Pelcovitz, D., van der Kolk, B., & Mandel, F. S. (1997). Complex PTSD in victims exposed to sexual and physical abuse: Results from the DSM-IV field trial for posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 10(4), 539-555. Russell, D. E. H. (1990). Rape in marriage (exp. and rev. ed. ): Bloomington, IN, US: Indiana University Press. Sackett, L. A., & Saunders, D. G. (1999). The impact of different forms of psychological abuse on battered women. Violence and Victims, 14(1), 105-117. Sansone, R. A., Reddington, A., Sky, K., & Wiederman, M. W. (2007). Borderline personality symptomatology and history of domestic violence among women in an internal medicine setting. Violence and Victims, 22(1), 120-126. Schafer, J ., Caetano, R., & Clark, C. L. (1998). Rates of intimate partner violence in the United States. American Journal of Public Health, 88(11), 1702-1704. Scher, C. D., Stein, M. B., Asmundson, G. J. G., McCreary, D. R., & Forde, D. R. (2001). The Childhood Trauma Questionnaire in a community sample: Psychometric properties and normative data. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 14(4), 843-857. Schiavi, R. C., Schreiner-Engle, P., Mandeli, J ., Schanzer, H., & Cohen, E. (1990). Healthy aging and male sexual function. American Journal of Psychiatry, 147(6), 766-771. Schnurr, P. P., & Green, B. G. (Eds). (2004). Trauma and health: Physical health consequences of exposure to extreme stress. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Segraves, R. T., & Segraves, K. B. (1995). Human sexuality and aging. Journal of Sex Education and Therapy, 21(2), 88-102. Sharhabani-Arzy, R., Amir, M., Kotler, M., & Liran, R. (2003). The toll of domestic violence: PTSD among battered women in an Israeli sample. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 18(11), 1335-1346. 223 Smith, P. H., Thornton, G. E., DeVellis, R., Earp, J ., & Coker, A. L. (2002). A population-based study of the prevalence and distinctiveness of battering, physical assault, and sexual assault in intimate relationships. Violence Against Women, 8(10), 1208-1232. Spinazzola, J ., Roth, S., Derosa, R., Efrom, L., Lifton, N., & Davidson, J. (1994, November). The specificity of the Disorders of Extreme Stress construct Poster session presented at the 10th annual meeting of the International Society for Traumatic Stress Studies. Chicago, IL. Stanley, N., & Penhale, B. (1999). The mental health problems of mothers experiencing the child protection system: Identifying needs and appropriate responses. Child Abuse Review, 8(1), 34-45. Stark, E., & F litcraft, A. (1996). Women at risk: Domestic violence and women 's health. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications, Inc. Stein, M. B., & Kennedy, C. (2001). Major depressive and post-traumatic stress disorder comorbidity in female victims of intimate partner violence. Journal of Aflective Disorders, 66(2-3), 133-138. Stein, M. B., Kennedy, C. M., & Twamley, E. W. (2002).Neuropsychologica1 function in female victims of intimate partner violence with and without posttraumatic stress disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 52(11), 1079-1088. Stein, M. B., Walker, J ., Anderson, G., Hazen, A., Ross, C., Eldridge, G., et al. (1996). Childhood physical and sexual abuse in patients with anxiety disorders and in a community sample. American Journal of Psychiatry, 153(2), 275-277. Stewart, D. E., & Cecutti, A. (1993). Physical abuse in pregnancy. Canadian Medical Association Journal, 149(9), 1257-1263. Straus, M. A. (1979). Measuring intrafamily conflict and violence: The Conflict Tactics (CT) Scales. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 41 (1), 75-88. Street, A. E., & Arias, I. (2001). Psychological abuse and posttraumatic stress disorder in battered women: Examining the roles of shame and guilt. Violence and Victims, 16(1), 65-78. 224 Tan, C., Basta, J ., Sullivan, C. M., & Davidson, W. S. (1995). The role of social support in the lives of women exiting domestic violence shelters: An experimental study. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 10(4), 43 7-45 1. Terr, L.-C. (1991). Childhood traumas: An outline and overview. American Journal of Psychiatry, 148, 10-20. Thompson, M. P., Kaslow, N. J ., Kingree, J. B., Rashid, A., Puett, R., Jacobs, D., et al. (2000). Partner violence, social support, and distress among inner-city African American women. American Journal of Community Psychology, 28(1), 127-143. Tjaden, P., & Thoennes, N. (1998). Prevalence, incidence, and consequences of violence against women: Findings from the National Violence Against Women Survey. US. Department of Justice, National Institute of Justice, Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. NCJ 172837 (pp. 1-16). Tjaden, P., & Thoennes, N. (2000a). Full report of the prevalence, incidence, and consequences of violence against women: Findings from the National Violence Against Women Survey. US. Department of Justice, National Institute of Justice. NCJ 183781. (pp. iii-61). Tj aden, P., & Thoennes, N. (2000b). Extent, nature, and consequences of intimate partner violence: Findings from the National Violence Against Women Survey. US. Department of Justice, National Institue of Justice. NCJ 181867 (pp. iii-57). Tjaden, P., & Thoennes, N. (2000c). Prevalence and consequences of male-to-female and female-to-male intimate partner violence as measured by the National Violence Against Women Survey. Violence Against Women, 6(2), 142-161. Torres, 8., Campbell, J. C., Campbell, D. W., Ryan, J., King, C., Price, R, et al. (2000). Abuse during and before pregnancy: Prevalence and cultural correlates. Violence and Victims, 15(3), 303-321. Umberson, D., Anderson, K., Glick, J ., & Shapiro, A. (1998). Domestic violence, personal control, and gender. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 60(2), 442- 452. van-der-Kolk, B.-A., & Fisler, R.-E. (1994). Childhood abuse and neglect and loss of self-regulation. Bulletin of the Menninger Clinic, 58(2), 145-168. 225 van der Kolk, B. A. (2002a). The assessment and treatment of Complex PTSD. In R. Yehuda (Ed), Treating trauma survivors with PTSD. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press, Inc. van der Kolk, B. A. (2002b). Self-report Inventory for Disorders of Extreme Stress (SIDES-SR). In Psychological Trauma Assessment Package. Brookline, MA: The Trauma Center. van der Kolk, B. A. (2003). Psychological trauma assessment package. Brookline, MA: The Trauma Center. van der Kolk, B. A., Pelcovitz, D., Roth, S., Mandel, F. S., McFarlane, A., & Herman, J. L. (1996). Dissociation, somatization, and affect dysregulation: The complexity of adaption to trauma. American Journal of Psychiatry, 153(Suppl), 83-93. van Ijzendoom, M. H., & Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J. (1996). Attachment representations in mothers, fathers, adolescents, and clinical groups: A meta- analytic search for normative data. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 64(1), 8-21. Vitanza, S., Vogel, L. C. M., & Marshall, L. L. (1995). Distress and symptoms of posttraumatic stress disorder in abused women. Violence and Victims, 10(1), 23- 34. von Eye, A., Mun, E. Y., & Indurkhya, A. (2004). Typifying developmental trajectories - a decision making process. Psychology Science, 46(1), 65-98. Walker, L. E. (1979). The battered woman. New York: Harper & Row. Walker, L. E. (1984). Battered women, psychology, and public policy. American Psychologist, 39(10), 1 178-1 182. Waltermaurer, E. M., Ortega, C. A., & McNutt, L. A. (2003). Issues in estimating the prevalence of intimate partner violence. Assessing the impact of abuse status on participation bias. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 18(9), 959-974. Waters, E., Merrick, S., Treboux, D., Crowell, J ., & Albersheim, L. (2000). Attachment security in infancy and early adulthood: A twenty-year longitudinal study. Child Development, 71 (3), 684-689. 226 IIIIIII Weaver, T. L., & Etzel, J. C. (2003). Smoking patterns, symptoms of PTSD and depression: Preliminary findings from a sample of severely battered women. Addictive Behaviors, 28(9), 1665-1679. Weinfield, N. S., Sroufe, L. A., & Egeland, B. (2000). Attachment from infancy to early adulthood in a high-risk sample: Continuity, discontinuity, and their correlates. Child Development, 71 (3), 695-702. Wolfe, D. A., Zak, L., Wilson, S. K., & Jaffe, P. (1986). Child witnesses to violence between parents: Critical issues in behavioral and social adjustment. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 14(1), 95-104. World Health Organization. (1992). The [CD-10 classification of mental and behavioral disorders: Clinical descriptions and diagnostic guidelines. Geneva: World Health Organization. Zielinski, D. S., & Bradshaw, C. P. (2006). Ecological influences on the sequelae of child maltreatment: A review of the literature Child Maltreatment, 11(1), 49-62. Zimmermann, P., Grossmann, K. E., & Fremmer-Bombik, E. (1998). Attachment in infancy and adolescence: Continuity 0fattachment.Unpublished manuscript, Regensburg, Germany. 227