(kn. “a. a z I r m w % bums" :55 1:1 ,‘ J: Lu. . .3 4:1 : ‘ 7431: . . . a :33“. » :0 1...: I: 2...... ,3. 10.3.5». 1 .t .266... . . ::i\ 1,1.O.I£ I. (2.1! .v}. 1:. 7....le via P-L.v§.l fa 2 LIBRARY \ (Z c Michigan State te "' ' 7C University This is to certify that the dissertation entitled EXCAVATING READERS: FINDING MIDDLE SCHOOL BOYS' LITERACIES IN A PERMEABLE LANGUAGE ARTS CLASSROOM presented by SEAN KO‘lTKE has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for the Ph.D. degree in Educational Psychology and Educational Technology CV Major Professor’s Signature IO .5 l. O 7’ Date MSU is an afiinnative-action, equal-opportunity employer PLACE IN RETURN BOX to remove this checkout from your record. TO AVOID FINES return on or before date due. MAY BE RECALLED with earlier due date if requested. DATE DUE DATE DUE DATE DUE 6/07 p:/C|RC/DateDue.indd-p.1 EXC AV AT ‘ EXCAVATING READERS: FINDING MIDDLE SCHOOL BOYS’ LIT ERACIES IN A PERMEABLE LANGUAGE ARTS CLASSROOM By Sean Kottke A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Program in Educational Psychology and Educational Technology 2007 EXCAVAT Adolescent opponuniti language at for improxl Howeter. l readers mi girls’ read academic ‘v‘emaculz gladers f Pmonal Selectim TeaIiSfic bOE‘S‘ s 31710113: Super: SEX eCIj A Panic ABSTRACT EXCAVATING READERS: FINDING MIDDLE SCHOOL BOYS’ LITERACIES IN A PERMEABLE LANGUAGE ARTS CLASSROOM Sean Kottke Adolescent literacy researchers frequently recommend that teachers incorporate lots of opportunities for students to self-select personally interesting texts to read as part of the language arts curriculum, and the literature suggests that this may be especially powerful for improving boys’ motivation for and attitude toward reading for multiple purposes. However, the literature does not contain a detailed analysis of the texts that adolescent readers might choose in such a permeable curriculum, the extent to which boys’ and girls’ reading selections might overlap or differ, or the extent to which choices made for academic purposes resemble those that students might make for recreational (or vernacular) purposes. During the 2004-2005 school year, texts selected by 48 sixth graders for a variety of academic contexts in which students were encouraged to choose personally interesting reading materials were documented and analyzed, as were the text selections made by a subgroup of focal students for vernacular purposes. Fantasy and realistic fiction were the most popular genres among participants' text selections, with boys’ selections more evenly spread across a wider range of genres than were represented among girls’ selections. Sports-themed texts from multiple genres and media as well as supernatural mystery/thrillers had an especially greater prominence among the text selections made by boys. Boys selected texts from a wider range of media than did girls, particularly for vernacular purposes. This trend was observed at both the group and AWL... individual l1 boys' text set serial worksi skewed towl almOSI excli‘ academic p. multimedia interactive contexts \\ likely to se academic t selections. Vernacula. selections. reflective Vemaculd the datab genres m1 l ' l his flags; . l | range of individual levels. While serial works were extremely popular among both boys and girls, boys’ text selections for vernacular purposes were more heavily concentrated around serial works than were girls’ and their text selections for academic purposes were more skewed toward non-serial works than were girls’. Girls’ serial reading interests were almost exclusively print-based, and were visible in the text selections they made for academic purposes. Boys, on the other hand, were more likely to be interested in multimedia series, that is, series that had unique manifestations in print, visual and interactive media. While the majority of both boys’ and girls’ text selections across contexts were works created specifically for a young adult audience, boys were more likely to select more linguistically difficult texts to read for vernacular purposes than for academic texts, while the opposite trend was observed to a lesser degree among girls’ selections. While the distribution of genres within the text selections made by girls for vernacular purposes was echoed in the distribution of genres within their academic text selections, the distribution of genres within boys’ academic text selections was more reflective of boys’ vernacular print-literacy practices than the overall portrait of their vernacular literacy practices. Boys expressed a number of strong genre preferences, and the database of text selections reveals that these boys engaged frequently with these genres not only in their vemacular literacy lives, but also within the more permeable contexts of their Language Arts curriculum. However, as permeable as the teacher made his classroom to the texts of students’ vernacular literacy practices, a focus on print literacy practices over the course of the school year limited the extent to which the full range of boys’ vernacular literacy practices could become visible within academic literacy contextsi Copyright by SEAN KOTTKE 2007 Aum To Angus fascinate. And to ‘5 school it To August and Alexis, whose text selections and pop culture commitments constantly fascinate, and who always wondered why 100 pages wasn’t enough. And to Nick, who has no idea what letting me hang out at the library every day in middle school has wrought Thanks firs: David Won recommend of eminent early cours the pages t. thanks to l in lanuary Fellovt'shi and. espet me to spe students ‘ IllElI fres classrom he has e! COllCagU Sharing I TO all 01 SChOOL Finally ACKNOWLEDGMENTS Thanks first go out to my Dissertation Committee, Drs. Nell Duke, Matthew Koehler, E. David Wong and Chairperson Mark Conley. Their support, guidance and recommendations at all stages of this dissertation have been invaluable. This same team of eminent scholars also aided me at every stage of my doctoral career, from advising and early coursework through preliminary exams, practicum research and comps. Nothing in the pages that follow could have been accomplished without their expertise. Special thanks to Matt for getting the ball rolling on this doctoral career with his initial phone call in January 2002 to let me know that I’d received the University Distinguished Fellowship. That call was the best birthday present of my life. The faculty, administration and, especially, students, of Harwell Middle School deserve deepest thanks for allowing me to spend a year with them observing and asking questions. As of this writing, the students who participated in this study are embarking on their next great journey, starting their freshman year at Homestead High School. While Ed Connors, the teacher whose classroom served as the site of this study, is no longer teaching language arts at Harwell, he has embarked on his next great journey into the waters of principalship. To my colleagues and students at Miller College, thank you for patiently listening to my ideas, sharing your stories and occasionally kicking me in the rear to get this writing done. To all of the Humanities Block students who passed through my room at Tomball High School, thank you for letting me learn as much fi'om you as I hope you learned fiom me. Finally, I owe an enormous debt to my parents, who have supported me in every way possible at every stage in my education and who let me hole up in their guest room for many marathon writing sessions. Thank you for your generosity and patience. vi l on or t __ llST OF Fl CHAPTER INTRODU Self-selcl V’isualiz. I Literatur The Stutf CHAPTER REVIEW ( lntrodue Theoreti Review Summa: CHAPTEl t—tETHOd Chapter Setting Researd Partici,“ Proceti. Data A ROle Of CHAPTE RESELT Introcl QUESI: Quest; Quest. Que-s: CHAPT; lmmg The TI The(; The T COHL TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES ............................................................................................................. ix LIST OF FIGURES ............................................................................................................ x CHAPTER 1 INTRODUCTION .............................................................................................................. 1 Self-selection .................................................................................................................. 6 Visualization ................................................................................................................... 9 Literature Circles .......................................................................................................... 10 The Study ...................................................................................................................... 11 CHAPTER 2 REVIEW OF THE LITERATURE .................................................................................. 15 Introduction ................................................................................................................... 15 Theoretical Perspective ................................................................................................. 17 Review of the Literature ............................................................................................... 20 Summary and Research Questions ............................................................................... 35 CHAPTER 3 METHODS ....................................................................................................................... 39 Chapter Overview ......................................................................................................... 39 Setting ........................................................................................................................... 39 Research Design Overview ........................................................................................... 56 Participants .................................................................................................................... 58 Procedures ..................................................................................................................... 59 Data Analysis ................................................................................................................ 66 Role of the Researcher .................................................................................................. 73 CHAPTER 4 RESULTS ......................................................................................................................... 75 Introduction ................................................................................................................... 75 Question One ................................................................................................................ 76 Question Two ................................................................................................................ 95 Question Three ............................................................................................................ 104 Question Four ............................................................................................................. 1 18 CHAPTER 5 DISCUSSION ................................................................................................................. 122 Introduction ................................................................................................................. 122 The Texts — Currents of Diversity .............................................................................. 124 The Contexts — (Im)Permeable Semiotic Social Spaces ............................................. 135 The Teacher - Negotiator of Literacy Events and Practices ...................................... 141 Conclusion .................................................................................................................. 1 52 vii APPENDlX TABLES”. APPENDN norREs- APPENDI.‘ LIST OF T Books Magazin Motion l IClEViSlt Video (71 REFEREX APPENDIX A TABLES ......................................................................................................................... 157 APPENDIX B FIGURES ........................................................................................................................ 172 APPENDIX C LIST OF TEXTS SELECTED BY MEDIUM AND TITLE ......................................... 188 Books .......................................................................................................................... 1 89 Magazines and Comic Books ..................................................................................... 203 Motion Pictures and DVDs ......................................................................................... 203 Television Series ......................................................................................................... 205 Video Games ............................................................................................................... 205 REFERENCES ............................................................................................................... 207 viii Table A]: Table A2: Table A3: Table A4: Number ti. Table A5: Number o. Table A6: Academic Table A7: .4 CU (16m Tc Table A8: Gender ur Table A0: .‘ICUdt’m 11L Table A} .4 (11(1an IL Table A} 3004.--. Table, A} 3005 Table A Table A Table A. LIST OF TABLES Table A1: Median Household Income Distribution by District ..................................... 157 Table A2: Enrollment Distribution by Race/Ethnicity by District ................................. 158 Table A3: Text Selections by Gender and Context ......................................................... 159 Table A4: Most Popular Texts Selected By All Students for Any Purpose, Based on Number of Text Selections Documented for Each Text (in Parentheses) ....................... 160 Table A5: Most Popular Authors Selected By All Students For Any Purpose, Based on Number of Text Selections Documented for Each Author (in Parentheses) ................... 161 Table A6: Distribution of Focal Students’ Text Selections by Genre, Gender and Academic/ Vernacular Context ........................................................................................ 1 62 Table A7: Fog Readability Levels of Focal Students ’ Text Selections by Gender and Academic/ Vernacular Context ........................................................................................ 163 Table A8: F lesch-Kincaid Readability Levels of Focal Students’ Text Selections by Gender and Academic/ Vernacular Context .................................................................... 164 Table A9: Distribution of Focal Students’ Text Selections by Medium, Gender and Academ ic/ Vernacular Context ........................................................................................ 1 65 Table A10: Distribution of Focal Students ’ Text Selections by Series, Gender and Academic/ Vernacular Context ........................................................................................ 166 Table A11: ERAS Recreational and Academic Subscale Comparisons by Gender, Fall 2004 ................................................................................................................................ 1 67 Table A12: ERAS Recreational and Academic Subscale Comparisons by Gender, Spring 2005 ................................................................................................................................ 1 68 Table A13: F all/Spring ERAS Recreational Subscale Comparisons by Gender ............ 169 Table A14: F all/Spring ERAS Academic Subscale Comparisons by Gender ................ 170 Table A15: F all/Spring ERAS Recreational Subscale Comparisons by Homeroom ..... 171 ix Figure Bl: Figure 82: I Figure 83: Figure B-l: Figure B5 Figure Bo: I 5.. dillt'di Figure B" Figure BS student... Figure BL: Figure Bl qut’Sr’ltnln Figure B Figure B Flgure B Figure B Figure 3 FlguIe Ii LIST OF FIGURES Figure Bl: Classroom library of Mr. Ed Connors ......................................................... 172 Figure B2: Classroom library of Ms. Julie Price ........................................................... 173 Figure B3: Reading corner in Ms. Julie Price ’s classroom ........................................... 174 Figure B4: Visualization quiz administered by Mr. Connors ......................................... 175 Figure BS: Genre Project assignment sheet ................................................................... 176 Figure B6: Sample book review form for all genres except biography, completed by Ciera S., a focal student ............................................................................................................ 177 Figure B7: Continued from Figure 36, back side of sample book review form ............. 178 Figure B8: Sample book review form for biographies, completed by Ciera S., a focal student ............................................................................................................................. 179 Figure B9: Book Project assignment sheet ..................................................................... 180 Figure B10: T imeline of classroom activities, data sources (with associated research questions) and data analysis strategies employed in study ............................................ 181 Figure B11: Text Selections of All Students by Age of T ext (Base Year 2005) ............... 182 Figure B12: Mr. Connors’ classroom during gym class, after DEAR ............................ 183 Figure B13: Second view of Mr. Connors’ classroom during gym class, after DEAR... 184 Figure B14: Third view of Mr. Connors ’ classroom during gym class, after DEAR ..... 185 Figure B15: Fourth view of Mr. Connors ’ classroom during gym class, after DEAR 186 Figure 316: The Long Tail ............................................................................................. 187 g‘ "R.AT.06 tinting on "Reading I read over 2 | I I 2003-200- Ham‘ell ll motivatio maximizi might ab academi. | exPerle: 0f self-5. at 50me had for : suburb“. I “turn . .\ a: ma; CHAPTER 1 Introduction “R.AT.O6.01: Students will be enthusiastic about reading and do substantial reading and writing on their own.” (Michigan Department of Education, 2006, p. 60) “Reading books can be a lot of ftm when they’re not the same ones that they make you read over and over and over till you want to shoot yourself.” (Portman, 2006, p. 60) I first became aware of Ed Connors” approach to reading instruction during the 2003-2004 school year, when my daughter was enrolled in his sixth grade homeroom at Harwell Middle School. At the time, I was immersed in the literatures on reading motivation and adolescent boys’ literacy practices, and had become convinced that maximizing opportunities for students to self-select texts for language arts instruction might abate the well-documented free-fall of children’s attitudes toward reading for both academic and recreational purposes over the middle school years. Indeed, my own experiences teaching ninth grade English had given me first-hand evidence of the power of self-selection to build reading motivation among teenagers (particularly boys) who had at some point in middle school lost much of whatever enthusiasm they had previously had for reading. For seven years, I taught language arts in the gifted/talented program in a suburban comprehensive high school. The final four years of my teaching career were within an interdisciplinary Humanities Block, which I designed in conjunction with a ’ All names of people, schools and municipalities are pseudonyms. social stur freshman honors 56 would QT into two 5 arts and 5 activities that incor T1 boundarit AP optior students 5 int'estiga': weeks of their tho of the fir “€11 as d SChOOI t- that We Commu: abut ti”, p ’0'? 3:? social studies teacher. Ninth graders would enroll in designated honors sections of freshman English and World Geography, while tenth graders would enroll in designated honors sections of sophomore English and World History. My teaching partner and I would typically share 30-50 ninth graders and 20-40 tenth graders, each group separated into two sections that would meet concurrently. Instructional units in the paired language arts and social studies classes were thematically coordinated, and the majority of activities in both classes involved cooperative grouping on short- and long-term projects that incorporated content and skills from both disciplines. Two required elements of the Humanities Block transcended disciplinary boundaries and served to distinguish the Humanities experience from other honors or pre- AP options available at the school. One element was the I-Search Project, in which students selected a topic of personal interest and designed a yearlong program of investigation into that topic. Students generated research questions during the first few weeks of school, then spent the majority of the first semester reviewing the literature on their chosen topic. This research culminated in the writing of an I-Search paper at the end of the first semester. During the second semester, students engaged in further research as well as designed materials for a three-hour, culminating exhibition at the end of the school year. The exhibition was designed like a science fair, with students displaying their work in the high school commons while peers, other teachers, administrators and community members were invited to circulate among the displays and query the students about their projects. The second distinguishing feature of the Humanities Block experience was a program of self-selected reading, in which students chose their own reading materials for self-defined design for e- difl’erent w we requirel example. n for a third I (for exam l required tl‘ each gradi local adm reading w increase r O COmporte When we each gra. ABET Stu cellifiriné While CC “I111 CVg SemESIe Smdemr PFOJCCt self-defined purposes each grading period and made brief presentations of their own design for each item that they read. Over the course of four years, we experimented with different ways to encourage students to read broadly and for multiple purposes. One year, we required that each grading period’s reading selections be from a different genre (for example, non-fiction/biography one grading period, a novel the next, a poetry collection for a third). Another year, we assigned point values to different kinds of reading materials (for example, a poem might carry one point, while a novel might carry 10) and then required that students complete a certain number of points' worth of independent reading each grading period. This program was developed in accordance with a mandate from our local administration to increase students' sheer amount of reading, whether or not that reading was tied to specific classroom activities or state objectives. The goal was to increase reading, period. Over the course of our four years of administering the self-selected reading component of the Humanities Block, we witnessed the most enthusiasm from students when we dropped all general restrictions on the reading that students could choose from each grading period and instead worked with students to fashion individual reading diets. After studying each student's reading history (based upon an autobiographical essay centering on prior literacy experiences and the particular experiences each student had while completing summer reading assignments), I had individual reading conferences with every student, during which we jointly developed a reading contract for the semester. Included on the contract were an inventory of possible titles (based upon the students' prior reading interests as well as the topics they chose for their I-Search Project), and an expectation for the number of pages that the student would read over the course of tl‘. encounteret As i facilitator r: from srude recreation: resolutely sharing bo- school. du applied to avenue or annual mt UllClEl’SIar det'elOpr: lm'CSIlg; could ml found m arranger- Primari— POSsibi: pareIII-j course of the semester. Contracts were revised each grading period as students encountered additional reading materials of interest or discovered new reading interests. As my role as teacher began to morph from repository of literacy knowledge to facilitator of individual students' literacy development, I began to hear more testimonials from students about their rediscovery of a passion for reading once their academic and recreational literacy practices had an opportunity to converge. Boys especially who had resolutely rejected reading as a hobby came to embrace reading and take great joy in sharing books and other reading materials. They had been turned off, they said, in middle school, during which they perceived reading being recast as an instrumental activity to be applied to high stakes tests and other information management tasks, rather than as an avenue of pleasure. Sharing my stories of excavating readers with other teachers at annual meetings of the National Council of Teachers of English (NCTE), I came to understand the value of choice as a powerful motivator in adolescent literacy development. The convergence of my experiences with the findings of empirical investigations into choice increased my enthusiasm for finding ways in which teachers could incorporate choice into their classrooms, but it also increased my frustration as I found many teachers skeptical about the possibilities. When my daughter enrolled in Mr. Connors’ homeroom and explained the unique arrangement that he had made for incorporating students' self-selected texts as the primary vehicle of literacy instruction into his classroom, I became intrigued by the possibilities this classroom offered to investigate boys’ reading choices and habits. At a parent-teacher conference early in the school year, Connors outlined for me the rationale behind his pedagogy. Connors noted that his teaching practice was heavily influenced by N. Robb’s TE achjet'eme the first 01 the only 0 substantia [thE]. 2 reasoned. stand muc C ornpreh: Departme Language InOdel. Robb’s Teaching Reading in Middle School (2000), and he felt that the cornerstone to achievement of Michigan’s Grade Level Content Expectations (GLCE) for reading was the first objective listed in the domain of Reading Attitude for every grade level, K-8 (and the only one for grades 3-8): “Students will be enthusiastic about reading and do substantial reading and writing on their own” (Michigan Department of Education [MDE], 2006, p. 60). If students are unmotivated to read widely and enthusiastically, he reasoned, they will neither choose to read on their own for recreational purposes nor stand much of a chance of mastering the other GLCEs in other domains such as Comprehension, Fluency, Word Study and Metacognition. Although the Michigan Department of Education lists Reading Attitude last among domains within the English Language Arts Reading Strand (MDE, 2006), Connors argued that attitude should be seen as the foundation for success in other domains of reading achievement, not as a pedagogical lagniappe to be addressed after the work of teaching comprehension is done. Furthermore, over the course of five years of teaching sixth grade language arts, he noticed that the boys in his class have typically had poorer attitudes toward reading than girls. To promote positive reading attitudes among all his students — but especially boys — Connors built his language arts curriculum around three key elements that his own reading of the professional literature suggested carry great power in generating and sustaining positive attitudes and high motivation toward reading: opporttmities for self- selection of personally interesting texts, an emphasis on building students’ visualization skills, and a reading workshop framework for instruction based on the Literature Circles model. Having witnessed my own daughter’s reading attitude and motivation increase over the co observing ( teaching la well as no young ado incorporat here. with research s Tl reading a of C 01th norms of attemptsK reader ir. around s DCWSpaF “induct enlol lc “Thin p I0 read. the do: over the course of her year in Mr. Connors’ class, I decided to spend the next school year observing Connors and his students to learn more about whether and how his approach to teaching language arts, which resonated so strongly with my own teaching experiences as well as my research into adolescents’ attitudes toward reading, might encourage more young adult readers (especially boys) to join the “literacy club.” How Connors incorporated each of the above components into his instruction will be briefly described here, with more details about both the specific practices enacted in the curriculum and the research supporting those practices to follow in subsequent chapters. Self-selection The amount of fieedom given to students to self-select personally interesting reading materials for their language arts instruction was for me the most striking element of Connors’ language arts cuniculum in terms of both its departure from traditional norms of reading instruction for middle school students and its similarity to what I had attempted to do in my own classroom. Connors’ own background as an unmotivated reader in middle school informed his decision to structure his language arts curriculum around students’ self-selected texts. He recalled a healthy reading diet of magazines, newspapers and other kinds of sports-related texts, but this enthusiastic reading was conducted entirely exclusive of the reading instruction he received in school. He did not enjoy reading as defined and assigned by his middle school teachers. “I don’t really like it when people give me books to read,” he said. “It’s too schoolish. I like to read what I like to read.” Connors admits that even as an adult, he is “a voracious reader,” but only within the domain of texts that he self-selects. One kind of text that Connors avidly collects and reads is professional books for teachers. 65 educational over whole monograpl (Atwell. 19 Readers V reading an he explain Ta ch IO teachers, especially books about language arts instruction. In his reading of the educational literature, he has noticed “a trend toward the Reader’s Workshop approach over whole-class novel approaches” among the best practices advocated in professional monographs such as Teaching Reading in Middle School (Robb, 2000) and In the Middle (Atwell, 1998). Connors believes that by incorporating students’ self-selected texts into a Reader’s Workshop setting, he can learn more about what his students are interested in reading and can expand their reading interests through a process he calls “webbing.” As he explains: Take a student who likes the Series of Unfortunate Events books. This is about children who have to overcome something. Here is another book, totally unrelated to the Series of Unfortunate Events, that deals with the same thing. And gradually, your reading interests expand. You can’t just stick with horse books or Series of Unfortunate Events in upper grades, where the reading gets more complex. Through “webbing” off of students’ self-selected texts, Connors creates a permeable language arts classroom (Dyson, 1993, 2003) in which students’ personal reading interests are honored and Connors can promote positive attitudes toward reading a variety of different kinds of texts. The Homestead Public School District, in which Harwell Middle School is located, organizes the school year on a trimester basis, and Mr. Connors used a different strategy for incorporating students’ self-selected texts into the reading curriculum each trimester of the 2004-05 school year. During the first trimester, students were expected to engage in independent reading with self-selected texts, recording titles and number of pages read each night on a calendar, which Connors checked at the end of each week. Upon C C ortnO comprt receive the firs logs an student genres: each gt \vorksh student Becaus School Dimest Were 11] Upon completion of a text, students would complete a brief book report worksheet. While Connors did lead the class in read-alouds and mini-lessons on various reading comprehension strategies (e. g., visualizing, drawing inferences) for which students received participation grades, the overall language arts grade that students received for the first trimester was determined almost entirely by the students’ independent reading logs and book reports. During the second trimester, Connors introduced the Genre Project, in which students’ self-selection of texts for independent reading was structured around five genres: realistic fiction, historical fiction, fantasy, biography, and science fiction. For each genre, Connors provided students with a differently formatted book report worksheet, and completed book reports were kept in a folder in the back of the room for students to peruse and discover new titles that they might be interested in reading. Because students complained about a relative paucity of science fiction titles in the school and classroom libraries, Connors changed the assignment mid-way through the trimester to allow students to choose their fifth book from any of the other genres if they were unable to find a science fiction text that interested them. Finally, the third trimester was devoted to a culminating Book Project, in which students were to select a book from any genre to read and around which to structure a poster presentation to be delivered to the class during the final weeks of the school year. The only restrictions on text selection for this project were that the book have more than 150 pages, and that it be a title that the student had not read previously. Every day throughout all three trimesters, Connors included Drop Everything and Read (DEAR) time, usually first thing in the morning, during which students were free to read any text that they II] A r identified 2 Parsons. 2 skill for are 1978). Ac the math Connors l building 3 visualize and not e narrative games. I higher l; This. a upon w; arts CUr activiti. explOI; mcnta: Omit. that they liked. Visualization A reader’s mental ability to visualize the actions and world of a text has been identified as a successful reading comprehension strategy (e.g., Harvey & Goudvis, 2000; Parsons, 2006; Robb, 2000; Sadoski, 1983; Wilhehn, 1997) as well as an indispensable skill for aesthetic engagement with a text (Kaj der, 2006; Parsons, 2006; Rosenblatt, 1978). According to Kajder (2006), “[e]ngaged reading is a visual experience, evoking the creation of an imagined story world often referred to as a ‘mental movie’” (p. 44). Connors believed that helping students with their visualization skills is the place to start building positive attitudes toward reading. He reasoned that students who are unable to visualize meaning in a text will not be able to engage with a text beyond the surface level and not enjoy reading print in the way that they enjoy engaging with other media narratives that directly appeal to their visual imaginations such as television and video games. By Connors’ logic, an inability to visualize meaning in a text could impair other higher level reading comprehension processes, such as the ability to draw inferences. Thus, a strong emphasis on building students’ visualization skills formed the foundation upon which Connors built most other cognitive and affective objectives in his language arts curriculum. This concern for building visualization skills permeated the reading and writing activities that Connors set up for his students. He presented vocabulary lessons as explorations of “picture words,” emphasizing the power of many words to evoke vivid mental images (Robb, 2000). Mini-lessons based on short readings from the Daybook of Critical Reading and Writing (Claggett, Reid & Vinz, 1999) consistently focused on identifilng they evoke (Spinelli. l led into St: the novels issued by in texts tl: conjuring brought i: classes tl. their text. drew pic: frarnevvt dev'eIOp-l. Sittdents relucm a Snider gTOUp C into a g talked ; T0 faC. identifying “picture words” in poetry and prose and representing the visual images that they evoked in a variety of ways. Read-alouds from novels such as Maniac Magee (Spinelli, 1990) and A Series of Unfortunate Events: The Bad Beginning (Snicket, 1999) led into small-group activities that involved storyboarding and acting out key scenes from the novels. The culmination of this emphasis on visualization skills was an invitation issued by Connors to his students during the second trimester of the school year to bring in texts that they enjoyed reading and that they felt were especially effective in terms of conjuring vivid imagery in the reader’s imagination. On the specified day, students brought in a variety of traditional and non-traditional texts, and Connors then led his classes through a series of writing tasks in which students identified “picture wor ” in their texts, described the effect that those words had on them as they read, and finally drew pictures of the images that they saw. Literature Circles The third component of Connors’ language arts pedagogy — a reading workshop framework based on Daniels’ (2002) Literature Circles model — was perhaps the least developed element of the curriculum during the year I spent observing Connors and his students, despite Connors’ strong faith in the power of positive peer support to motivate reluctant readers. During the 2003 -2004 school year (the year in which my daughter was a student in Mr. Connors’ class), most mornings’ DEAR periods were followed by small group conversations about the materials that students had been reading, which segued into a brief, informal whole class discussion in which both Connors and his students talked about what they were reading and swapped recommendations for future reading. To facilitate peer discussions after DEAR time and during regular language arts lessons, 10 students W per cluster] was. to $3} classroom. Ho faced a grt previous } students 5 faces. but the classr observati a return It Connors activities Pairs of: from the Was rnc-r implica: students were seated in clusters of two tables the entire year (seating four or five students per cluster), and students were free to choose with whom they wished to be seated. There was, to say the least, a great deal of conversation about reading in Connors’ language arts classroom. However, during the 2004-2005 school year, Connors and his teaching partner faced a group of students that they felt was in general less well-disciplined than the previous year’s class. Connors began the year as he had his four previous years, with the students seated in rows facing the chalkboard in order to facilitate matching names and faces, but rather than rearranging the tables into clusters as he had previously, he retained the classroom’s original arrangement for the entire year. Only twice during my observations were tables arranged in clusters, and only then for particular activities, with a return to the original arrangement immediately after the activity was completed. Connors did retain small group conversations throughout the year’s language arts activities, but in contrast to the previous year, these conversations were mostly limited to pairs of students (rather than to groups of four or five) with little to no “travel” away from the seats in which they began the day. This resulted in a classroom environment that was more teacher-directed than I had anticipated, and I believe has important implications for interpreting the findings of this study, which I will address in Chapter 5. The Study Connors indicated in a series of interviews that a key assumption underlying all of the strategies he incorporated into his language arts instruction was that students come to school with rich literacy lives that operate under the radar of the literacy practices valued and assessed by traditional language arts pedagogy. Rather than being wholly averse to 11 [he pract languagt' happen I classroor able to re external] may be 1. variety 0 educator: than mar T vernacul: This is al toward ri miffed \t Comic be Studl’ln : f0und a: (3003'). the practice of reading, a child who appears unmotivated to read and underachieves in the language arts may actually be a voracious reader of a wide variety of texts that do not happen to fall within the narrow limits of ‘that counts as reading,” as defined by classroom norms. Students who in a classroom context appear aliterate (that is, they are able to read well yet deliberately avoid doing so) thus may be adopting a stance against externally defined norms for reading in the classroom context, but outside of that context may be leading a literacy life characterized by reading deeply into and across texts from a variety of print and non-print genres, engaging in the very sorts of literacy practices that educators believe good readers should engage in, although with a different array of texts than many educators like to assign. This is precisely the attitude toward reading for academic versus self-defined (or vernacular) purposes that Connors claimed to have adopted as a middle school reader. This is also the attitude that many of the boys in my own language arts classrooms took toward reading, defining themselves as non-readers while sifting through backpacks stuffed with well-read magazines, newspapers, Internet printouts, technical manuals and comic books for the barely-read Turtlebackc edition of whatever classic novel we were studying that day (Cavazos-Kottke, 2005). Such contextual aliteracy is also the attitude found among the participants in studies by Smith and Wilhelm (2002, 2006), Newkirk (2002), Fink (2006) and Kajder (2006), who perfectly summarizes the attitude of the alternately voracious and aliterate reader with this quote from her student Gus: Here’s what it is I don’t read books. I do all we do in class, and pick up a lot. I talk to the gang in here that does read, and pick up some more. I go online. But, be real. When I want to read, this isn’t it I read the paper in the morning. I read 12 Lt. fi‘ M 4; (”I MI. Cor his bOyg develoI lucraci 91%31'0 maten'E implen articles and guides on PS3. I read what I get when friends text me. I read a ton of blogs, but I aggregate them or else it would take scads of time I go there for the cheats I used to read liners of CD’s, but now I just read online because I download MPB’s from iTunes. I had to read my car manual last night because one of the lights got busted. But, I know that stufldoesn ’t count. (p. 5, emphasis added) Kajder continues with her thoughts on Gus’ comments: As a reader, he read authentically and purposefully. He was reading selectively and critically, evaluating online gaming sites and blogs. His reading involved multiple genres and modalities. He was savvy enough to know how to navigate in-class tasks, but he needed to be drawn into higher-order tasks that addressed texts as tools for learning. And clearly, his perception of his own competency as a reader was impacting his work in the classroom. Gus was a reader. Just not in the English classroom (p. 5) Mr. Connors’ assumption about the vernacular literacy lives of his students, particularly his boys, thus carries the warrant not only of personal experience but also of published research into adolescent literacies. Connors was convinced that the best way to facilitate his students’ literacy deveIOpment was to break down as many barriers as possible between the vernacular literacies his students carried with them and the academic literacy practices enacted in the classroom. The above three strategies — self-selection of personally interesting reading materials, an emphasis on visualization skills, and a reader’s workshop framework — were implemented with this goal in mind. The present study was designed to interrogate 13 I Connors a assumption particular g llhat are ti classroom? enact in the. practices ir between he students' a Connors‘ 1 reading ar mastery Q Connors’ assumptions about his students’ literacies and the practices he based on those assumptions, to discover the extent to which those barriers were broken down for a particular group of sixth graders over the course of one year in his language arts class. What are the vernacular literacies engaged in by Connors’ students outside of the classroom? Do they differ markedly from the academic literacy practices the students enact in this classroom? When invited to bring the texts of their vernacular literacy practices into the classroom context, do students choose to do so? Are there differences between boys and girls in this regard? Does any of this have a noticeable impact on the students’ attitudes toward reading? Ultimately, how successfully do students in Mr. Connors’ permeable classroom achieve R.AT.06.01, “Students will be enthusiastic about reading and do substantial reading and writing on their own,” (MDE, 2006) the one Michigan Grade Level Content Expectation that Connors sees as the bedrock upon which mastery of all other objectives rests? These were the questions that launched this study. 14’ 1 "3 mar" In T Krassotkir. .. . [They] (p. 292). t. conclusio motivatior Valued sil love of re turn asso. Conclusit label The (p. 451) 35 the n c0115.:q le Ell-1n CHAPTER 2 Review of the Literature Introduction In The Brothers Karamazov (Dostoevsky, 1880/1952), the preteen boy Kolya Krassotkin claims that studying Greek and Latin in schools “is simply a police measure ...' [They] were introduced because they are a bore and because they stupefy the intellect” (p. 292). One hundred twenty five years later, some researchers have drawn similar conclusions in their attempts to explain boys' disengagement with school and low motivation for reading. Newkirk (2002) argues that the educational establishment has valued silent reading more as a means of social control than as a means of nurturing a love of reading, the tacit goal being to tame impulsive behavior in children (which is in turn associated more with boys than with girls). Clark (1998) comes to a similar conclusion regarding foreign language instruction. O'Brien, Stewart and Moje (1995) label the dominant instructional paradigm of secondary schools a “pedagogy of control,” (p. 451) in which a one-way flow of information from teacher to student has been reified as the most efficient model for the delivery of information to students. A major consequence (unintended or not) is the branding of a narrow range of knowledge as legitimate, implicitly and often explicitly devaluing much of students' extracurricular funds of knowledge as illegitimate (Alvermann, 1998; O’Brien, Stewart & Moje, 1995). Perhaps the most strident recent indictment of schools as institutions of social control comes in a historical essay by Gatto (2003): “it is in the interest of complex management, economic or political, to dumb people down, to demoralize them, to divide them from one another and to discard them if they don't conform. [Schools are] drill centers for 15 the habits a children or Stu number of girls on a ti Schmelzer Operation Sommers. boys tend themselve Skills (Bi: Brozo. 2( Particular (mite V01 ’iaPPrOp 2003 )_ ClaSSl’0( read'mE descn'b the habits and attitudes that corporate society demands. Mandatory education serves children only incidentally; its real purpose is to turn them into servants” (pp. 37-3 8). Studies of adolescent boys’ reading habits and achievement have highlighted a number of problematic trends. Cognitively, boys consistently underperform relative to girls on a number of standardized measures of reading and writing ability (Brozo & Schmelzer, 1997; Gurian, 2001; Newkirk, 2002; Organisation for Economic Co- Operation and Development & UNESCO Institute for Statistics [OECD-UIS], 2003; Sommers, 2000; Smith & Wilhehn, 2002; Young & Brozo, 2001). Affectively, many boys tend to have such a strong aversion to literacy that they will resist identifying themselves as “readers,” actively avoiding opportunities to read despite having adequate skills (Bintz, 1993; Moss, 1999; OECD-UIS, 2003; Smith & Wilhelm 2002; Young & Brozo, 2001). This tendency among boys to reject the descriptor of “reader” is particularly odd, given that most boys profiled in the some studies do indeed read (many quite voraciously), although not from sources typically sanctioned by schools as “appropriate” reading material (N ewkirk, 2002; Smith & Wilhelm, 2002; Wilhehn, 2002) Why did these boys resist identifying themselves as readers, and are there classroom contexts in which they may have been more likely to count their personal reading habits as constitutive of an identity as a reader? This case study was designed to describe the different literacies in which middle school boys engage within the context of a language arts classroom built around students' self-selected reading materials. To what extent would middle school boys in such a literacy curriculum draw distinctions between academic and personal literacy practices and to what extent might their attitudes toward 16 my reading ch (in en the t M; literacy is by Barth is the lite draw trpt aWar-ene abQUIaj with On Idemm [which radiCal Scho 01 Thus. 1 reading change over the course of a year’s experience with such a reading curriculum? Given the linkages between reading attitude, reading frequency and reading achievement (Bean, 1998), answering those questions should yield a better understanding of the personal literacies that middle school boys engage in outside of the radar of a traditional literacy curriculum, which might in turn help teachers better scaffold meaningfirl literacy instruction for boys. Theoretical Perspective My conceptual framework for understanding literacy is rooted in the idea that literacy is a social practice in which peOple engage for multiple purposes. As articulated by Barton and Hamilton (2000), the fundamental unit of analysis from such a perspective is the literacy practice, a “general cultural way of utilising written language which people draw upon in their lives, [involving] values, attitudes, feelings, social relationships awareness of literacy, constructions of literacy and discourses of literacy, how people talk about and make sense of literacy [as well as] the social processes which connect people with one another [including] shared cognitions represented in ideologies and social identities” (pp. 7-8). In short, “literacy is best understood as a set of social practices [which] are observable in events which are mediated by written texts” (p. 9). Different literacies are associated with different contexts. For example, the literacy practices encouraged and rewarded by certain literacy classrooms may appear radically different from those in which middle schoolers eagerly engage outside of school. While both may involve social interactions around a common text, the discourses governing those interactions as well as the texts themselves may differ considerably. Thus, it is more useful to conceive of an individual's literacy in the plural, that is, an 17 individual social or it. literacy. A l ’ vemaculai literacies : power anc| p. 13). In and revva example reflects ' ans PTO: are the ‘ radar 0 relativt Practic Classrc SubVe Pracri chilclr 12mg11 IOI- el’ individual practices multiple literacies over the course of his or her life, with particular social or institutional contexts enforcing different boundaries for “what counts” as literacy. A literacy as social practice perspective distinguishes between dominant and vernacular literacy practices within an individual's repertoire of literacies. Dominant literacies are “part of whole discourse formations, institutionalised configurations of power and knowledge which are embodied in social relationships” (Barton & Hamilton, p. 12). In other words, dominant literacies are those literacies that are publicly practiced and rewarded within major institutional contexts, such as school and government. For example, close reading and textual analysis of literary texts is a literacy practice that reflects the dominant literacy enforced and rewarded by traditional secondary language arts programs (O’Brien, Stewart & Moje, 1995). Vernacular literacies, on the other hand, are the “underground” literacy practices in which people engage, typically outside of the radar of institutional authority. Sharing a comic book with a peer and debating the relative strengths of a superhero and his or her archenemy is an example of a literacy practice in which many young people engage, but which typically occurs in non- classroom contexts. This is not to say that vernacular literacy practices are necessarily subversive of or antithetical to the goals of traditional academic literacy practices. These practices are simply the funds of knowledge (Moll, Amanti, Neff & Gonzalez, 1992) that children utilize outside of the classroom and that are not typically embraced by the language arts curriculum enacted in many classrooms. A key element of this perspective on literacy is the purpose that an individual has for engaging in a given literacy practice, and a major factor in many boys' opting to 18 disenf-Ia student Csiksze bervveer like to p for futur on preps purposes work” 01 way to ft social wt personal] 1998; Dr Hardenb: VtVOOCimz benteen meOSes (1902;1t educafic aCIletj ; Commict demon- disengage from active literacy practice in academic contexts is the positioning of a student and classroom at cross-purposes. Newkirk (2002), Clark (1998), Bean (1998), Csikszentrnihalyi (1990b) and Ivey (1999), among others, see a fundamental mismatch between school’s purposes for teaching literacy and the purposes to which students would like to put their abilities. Whereas schools stress the instrumental importance of literacy for future individual success (an especially visible phenomenon in classrooms centered on preparing students for high-stakes testing), children tend to value literacy for social purposes. Newkirk (2002), Finders (1997) and Dyson (1993, 2003) emphasize the “social work” of writing, in which students incorporate their peers into their creative fictions as a way to forge social bonds and exchange social capital. Other investigators focus on the social work of reading, by which students network, build relationships and solve personally relevant problems through discussions of shared reading (Bean, 1998; Davies, 1998; Dutro, 2001; Galda, Ash & Cullinan, 2001; Guzzetti, Young, Gritsavage, Fyfe, & Hardenbrook, 2002; Millard, 1998; Raphael, McMahon, Goatley, Bentley Boyd, Pardo & Woodman, 1992; Raphael & McMahon, 1994; Smith & Wilhelm, 2002). This mismatch between what schools want students to do with literacy and the immediately relevant purposes that students see literacy as valuable for pursuing was one of Dewey’s (1902/ 1990, 1915/1990, 1938) most persistent complaints about “traditional” modes of education, in which “the center of gravity is outside the child. It is in the teacher, the textbook, anywhere and everywhere you please except in the immediate instincts and activities of the child himself” (1915/ 1990, p. 34). Contemporary researchers have made common cause with Dewey in identifying this mismatch of purpose as particularly demotivating for students of literacy and in suggesting that an academic context for 19 literacy in hteracies l facilitating In disengage focus on i and conte teachers 2 lwill the contrast v instructit practice: these re; Promisi: Rtadinz (Eccles Guillrie ofmot; attivit'. literacy instruction that creates spaces for students' personal interests and vernacular literacies has potential for supporting positive attitudes toward literacy as well as for facilitating achievement in academic literacy practices. Review of the Literature In this section, I will begin by reviewing a number of theories for boys’ disengagement from academic literacy practices, including developmental theories that focus on boys' lower self-efficacy as a result of early negative experiences with reading, and contextual theories that focus on a mismatch of purposes for reading between teachers and students as well as the gender politics of reading that emerge in adolescence. I will then review the research on boys' vernacular literacy practices and how they contrast with the dominant literacy practices of school. Finally, I will review a number of instructional recommendations based on this understanding of adolescents' literacy practices. I will focus on two models — Book Club and Literature Circles — that synthesize these recommendations into an approach to literacy instruction that appears especially promising for promoting boys' participation in academic literacy practices. Reading Competency and Motivation Feelings of competency form an essential component in several domain-general (Eccles, Wigfield & Schiefele, 1998; Schunk, 1985) and literacy-specific (Clark, 1998; Guthrie & Wigfield, 2001; Hinchman, Alverrnann, Boyd, Brozo & Vacca, 2003) models of motivation. Social learning theory (Bandura, 1977) privileges self-efficacy for a given activity (a competency judgement based upon feedback from myriad sources) as a primary factor in determining one's future engagement in that activity. Self-determination theory (Deci & Ryan, 2000) conceptualizes motivation as a three-way interaction 20 betweei theory tofinl level at undera anuud experi underg interat by a y the at 1999'. readir (Ivey that t COnt rcadi between self-assessments of competence, autonomy and relatedness. According to flow theory (Csikszentmihalyi, 1990a), the kind of intense engagement that is most conducive to further development is impossible without an appropriate match between one's skill level and the demands of a particular task. Similarly, developmental explanations for boys’ disengagement with and underachievement in reading typically argue that boys tend to have more negative attitudes toward reading than girls do because many boys' earliest school reading experiences are marked by fiustration. At five or six years of age, the average girl has undergone sufficient cognitive development to position her for success in learning how to interact with an abstract symbol system. However, many boys at this age lag behind girls by a year or two, and so the age at which literacy instruction begins is more amenable to the average girl's developmental readiness than the average boy's (Kindlon & Thompson, 1999; Soderman, 1986). Many boys are thus set up to experience frustration with early reading experiences, which may predispose them to disengage from reading in the future (Ivey, 1999). However, a biographical study of twelve successful adults with dyslexia suggests that there may be more to literacy motivation than a sense of competency or self-efficacy. Contrary to the researcher’s expectations, all participants in the study reported avid reading despite having significant disabilities from a young age (Fink, 1995). Many had read widely enough from an area of passionate interest to call themselves experts well before they achieved reading fluency norms established by their respective schools and well before they had been instructed in strategies for overcoming their disabilities. The same phenomenon appears in autobiographical and anecdotal accounts of boys whose 21 f moti‘ them as we gener studel other det'elc of lm't suppor Purpos bem'eer an incor in class: 1978'). l motivation to read, sparked by access to personally interesting reading materials, allowed them to rapidly overcome gaps in reading achievement due to dyslexia (Buchanan, 2001) as well as to chronic disengagement from literacy instruction (Brozo, 2002). In these studies, avid reading within domains of personal interest and self- generated purposes for reading allowed individual vernacular literacies to blossom, and students' attitudes and abilities in dominant literacy practices showed positive gains. In other words, the development of students' vernacular literacies facilitated the development of their skills in dominant literacies. Several themes emerge from a review of investigations into particular features of recreational and academic contexts that support (or hinder) boys' positive engagement with multiple literacies. Purposes For, and Stances Toward, Reading One important theme, introduced above, is the congruence (or lack thereof) between students' and teachers' purposes for engaging in literacy practices. On one level, an incongruity between students' and schools' purposes can shut down active participation in classroom literacy events. Through the lens of Reader-Response Theory (Rosenblatt, 1978), a mismatch between efferent and aesthetic stances toward reading might also bring students and schools to cross-purposes. The efferent stance, favored by standardized tests and the New Critical close reading paradigm, focuses readers’ attention on what information or concrete understanding they can take away from a text. It is at the heart of the pedagogy of control (O'Brien, Stewart & Moje, 1995). The aesthetic stance, on the other hand, involves engagement with a text at an experiential level, in which readers connect their “lived-through experiences” with those of literary protagonists. Rosenblatt claimed that the aesthetic stance is the most appropriate to take toward 22 literafuf“ can inVll Kottke. I adopt an “back-to engagent exposed classroor 2000; Be Pardo & 1992 ). Lherac} literature, although one can imagine informational texts (for example, biographies) that can invite aesthetic response beyond their efferent purposes (Alexander, 1997; Cavazos- Kottke, 2006). Fowler (1988) and Barrs (2000) theorized that encouraging students to adopt an aesthetic stance would better motivate them to engage with reading than a “back-to-basics” emphasis. Several studies have demonstrated that greater student engagement — as well as levels of achievement equal to those displayed by students exposed to a traditional, skills-based curricula — occurs in the context of literacy classrooms in which aesthetic stances toward reading have been emphasized (Barrs, 2000; Bean, 1998; Cox & Many, 1992; Raphael, McMahon, Goatley, Bentley, Boyd, Pardo & Woodman, 1992; Raphael & McMahon, 1994; Wiseman, Many & Altieri, 1992) Literacy and Gender Discourse Most of these studies emphasize that a congruence of purposes for literacy mastery and an aesthetic stance toward engaging with a text are motivating for all students, not just boys. Why then don’t we see equal levels of disengagement from the school-sanctioned practices of literacy between the genders? Why do boys disengage more readily than girls? Many researchers point to the predominantly feminine environments of elementary and middle school instruction (particularly in literacy) as a prime source of boys’ disengagement and underachievement in literacy (Barrs, 2000; Dressman, 1997; Dutro, 2001; Kindlon & Thompson, 1999; Martino, 1995, 1999, 2000a, 2000b; Millard, 1998; Sadker & Sadker, 1994; Smith & Wilhelm, 2002; Young & Brozo, 2001). As boys become more aware of themselves as actors within a gender regime (typically during the 23 middle sch lenitrhnllb' into a fat associated well under behind so should be hackles a \1 some for that mar. Cheer. b the soci: bounda- Bots' I approvi Close Parad. Nadir Vol-ac bQFS t ulStEa middle school years), they begin to adopt markers of masculinity and reject markers of femininity in their construction of a gender performance that will facilitate their induction into a favored social clique. Why boys are more adamant about rejecting anything associated with femininity than girls are about rejecting markers of masculinity is not well understood. Martino (2000a, 2000b) suggests that homophobia is the driving force behind such rejections, although he does not address the question of why homophobia should be stronger among boys than among girls. To be seen as a “sissy” raises more hackles among adults and peers than to be seen as a “tomboy.” Whatever the source of this double standard, it remains clear that rejection of some forms of literacy is a standard feature of the masculine “identity kit” (Gee, 1989) that many boys appropriate in adolescence. Reading widely and well, and with good cheer, becomes strongly associated with femininity, and hence serves as a litmus test in the social negotiation of boys’ gender identities. Boys who transgress these gender boundaries may risk especially virulent social ostracism (Martino, 1999, 2000a). Boys’ Vernacular Literacy Practices Despite the exclusion of the label “reader” from the masculine identity kits appropriated by many adolescent boys, it would be a mistake to call such boys aliterate. Close study of the literacy practices of middle school and adolescent boys reveals a paradox. While many boys refuse to identify themselves as readers, openly denigrating reading as something irrelevant to their lives, most boys actually do read, some quite voraciously (Hinchman, et al., 2003; Newkirk, 2002; Smith & Wilhelm, 2002). Many boys do not reject reading per se, but rather school-defined norms of reading, choosing instead to engage with texts that are considered “disreputable” by school standards (and 24 id. hence. “no I control ox I resistancel literacy pr Vemacula literacy or indit'idua the textuz of readir. hence, “not really reading”) in order to resist what they perceive as encroaching external control over their identities (Newkirk, 2002). Disaffected girls often take this line of resistance, too (Bintz, 1993). The “pedagogy of control” that privileges a narrow range of literacy practices as dominant erects boundaries of legitimacy that often exclude students' vernacular literacies (O'Brien, Stewart & Moje, 1995). To indulge in an alternative literacy outside of the school establishment’s literacy norms is to assert one’s individuality and gender identity. Inside of this fi'amework are arguments that examine the textual content of the literacy curriculum as well as that of students’ preferred kinds of reading to find sources of disengagement from literacy. What kinds of texts most engage boy readers? Not surprisingly, boys’ favorite types of reading material are not often in great supply in school or classroom libraries (Worthy, 1996; Worthy, Moorman & Turner, 1999). Also not surprisingly, boys’ least favorite types of reading material are ofien abundant in schools (Worthy, Moorman, Turner, 1999). Texts that are most likely to engage boy readers tend to be short, highly visual (both in description and graphic accompaniment), challenging to a boy’s notions about the world, edgy, closely connected to real life (even in a metaphoric sense, as is the case with fantasy novels), current and humorous (Newkirk, 2002; Smith & Wilhelm, 2002; Wilhelm, 2002; Worthy, 1996). Broadly, genre fiction (such as science fiction, fantasy, horror and mystery), series fiction, magazines, and graphic novels and comic books constitute the texts of many boys' vernacular literacies (Cavazos—Kottke, 2006; Hall & Coles, 1999; Nell, 1988; Newkirk, 2002). However, few teachers surveyed were inclined to honor these reading preferences for inclusion in the literacy curriculum, despite evidence that so-called “light” reading does not stunt children’s literacy 25 developmc‘ Se‘ about stud representa classroom arguments education Brozo an; as boys (‘ Wilhelm. to better C Dressma adolesce context- iS intere. interest I [10m ta”: (2000): materia mCllnek‘ could S Yemat development (Galda, Ash & Cullinan, 2001). Several authors have explored the instructional implications of our knowledge about students’ reading preferences. Brozo and Schmelzer (1997) advocate for greater representation of positive male archetypes in the shared reading materials used in literacy classrooms as one means of orienting boys toward engagement with reading. Like arguments for bilingual education that stress building early success into students’ educational experience by teaching content area coursework in students’ native language, Brozo argues that boys must be booked with material that speaks to their life experience as boys (Young & Brozo, 2001). Other authors (N ewkirk, 2002; Smith & Wilhelm, 2002; Wilhelm, 2002) present similar arguments for changing the content of literacy instruction to better incorporate boys’ reading preferences. Other researchers urge caution in taking such findings at face value, however. Dressman (1997), Bintz (1993) and Smith and Wilhelm (2002) assert that children’s and adolescents’ reading preferences are never stable constructs, but are instead highly context-sensitive as young people perform different aspects of their identity. What a boy is interested in reading at home today might be very different from what he expresses an interest in reading at school tomorrow, and both preferences may in turn differ radically fiom what he is motivated to read next week. Young (Young & Brozo, 2001) and Barrs (2000) both note that simply substituting current texts for more “boy-friendly” reading materials can be just as oppressive to readers as the status quo. Indeed, if boys remain inclined to define themselves in opposition to school norms of literacy, shifting nomis could simply lead to resistance on a different fiont as the institutionalization of vernacular literacies might strip such reading materials of their original appeal 26 (Alterman Shakespea work" (’19 new Eminl expresses advocates finding be Choice A between i into the 1 them anc 199313 €51nge Worth} attit'et literac may ‘ Hidi. (Alvermann, 1998; Dressman, 1997; Neilsen, 1998). In the words of William Shakespeare, “If all the year were playing holidays, / To sport would be as tedious as to wor ” (1974, p. 851). The boy in a recent MAD Magazine feature who throws away the new Eminem CD, proclaiming it “totally ruined” because his mother bought it for him expresses the same sentiment (Hempel & Sinberg, 2005, p. 23). Barrs (2000) instead advocates not changing the texts, but rather changing boys’ orientation toward them, finding better ways to engage them on an aesthetic level. Choice A different solution, proposed by many researchers, is to disrupt the opposition between vernacular and dominant literacies by building as many opportunities as possible into the literacy curriculum for students to choose both reading materials that interest them and means of expressing their insights about those readings (Appleby, 1989; Bintz, 1993; Fink, 1995; Guzzetti, Young, Gritsavage, Fyfe & Hardenbrook, 2002; Hektner & Csikszentrnihalyi, 1996; Hunt, 1970; Millard, 1998; Whalen & Csikszentrnihalyi, 1991; Worthy, Moorman & Turner, 1999). A “permeable curriculum” (Dyson, 1993, 2003) that actively incorporates students' personal interests, “unofficial” texts and vernacular literacies into the classroom discourse is a model for nurturing intrinsic motivation that may even extend beyond the literacy classroom to other content areas (Alvermann, 1998; Hidi, 1990; Neilsen, 1998). This line of argument dovetails neatly with those calling for increased sensitivity to the purposes students bring to literacy activity (e. g., Duke, Purcell-Gates, Hall & Tower, 2006), which finds its roots in Dewey (193 8). This is not a case of dumbing down the curriculum or letting the inmates run the asylum, as none of these studies support teachers’ total abdication of authority for determining the direction 27 .£.L!.u_.i§3 of literaCl individua proximal 1998). G motivate: det'elopn influence list of ch. includes. and stud learning with pee e\‘aluati and rep;- Charact: COhere: Coordjn Pm de the fol [Itera3 mere}. of literacy instruction. Rather, the teacher’s role is reconceived as facilitator of individuals’ literacy development, bringing instructional goals within children’s zones of proximal development and within sight of children’s immediate experience (Alvermann, 1998) Guthrie and Wigfield (2001) synthesize these contextual insights into what motivates children to engage in literacy in their engagement model of reading development. In their model, particular instructional processes and contexts positively influence the cognitive, motivational and social aspects of engagement. Their extensive list of characteristics of learning environments that support engagement with literacy includes, among other items, the following elements: connections between the curriculum and students’ personal experiences; teacher support for students’ autonomy in choosing learning goals and reading materials; use of interesting texts for instruction; collaboration with peers for social construction of knowledge; personalized and student-centered evaluation; and, teachers’ active involvement in developing individuals’ reading skills and repertoire. They caution that a classroom that exhibits only one or two of these characteristics may do little to increase students’ engagement, and that an overall coherence of instructional processes is necessary. Strategies should be integrated and coordinated to create an entire classroom culture that supports engaged reading. Over the past decade, a number of instructional paradigms have been designed to do just that. In the following section, I will discuss two of these: Literature Circles and Book Club. Literature Circles and Book Club Defined Despite their many similarities, Book Club and Literature Circles are not interchangeable terms. Book Club was developed in the early 1990's by a team of 28 . {I researcherl as a teachi ttithln the | WritingR I group disl | skills take I reading or_ apically ' might als chance tc artistic re prompt c indit'idu, instructi their “T; about th lflStruct: thlSlm Widely ; “titans researchers led by Tafly Raphael and Susan McMahon (see Raphael & McMahon, 1994) as a teaching strategy that includes four major components, all of which are experienced within the course of a single instructional period: Community Share, Reading, Writing/Representing, and Book Club discussion. Community Share is a time for whole group discussion and/or instruction. Any direct teaching of literary concepts or language skills takes place during this time, as does whole class discussion of ideas raised by the reading or by small group discussions. Reading is the time set aside for reading texts, typically involving individual silent reading, although large- or small-group oral reading might also be utilized. The period of Writing/Representing gives individual students a chance to reflect on what they have read, in the form of either a written journal or an artistic response in a particular medium. Students may write/represent in response to a prompt offered by the teacher or may be encouraged to respond freely, reflecting their individual reactions to a given text. The Book Club phase of the lesson is the heart of this instructional approach, a phase during which students break into small groups to share their writing/representations and to discuss both teacher- and student-generated questions about the reading material. While the order of these phases may vary within a given instructional period, all four are necessary components of the Book Club program as envisioned by Raphael and McMahon. In its original conception, Book Club was more widely implemented with younger students than with secondary students, among whom Literature Circles were the discussion strategy of choice. Literature Circles is also an instructional strategy developed by a number of action researchers in the 1990's to create more authentic, discussion-based contexts for engaging with literature in the middle school and secondary language arts curriculum (see 29 Daniels. 2 engaged ir strategy be experience material a response 1 contraSI lt Circle ac: reading. I SllOLlld bt the $ch “pen: inlIOdL Daniels, 2002). In contrast with Book Club, which specifies four distinct activities to be engaged in within a single instructional period, Literature Circles is a more informal strategy based on eleven principles for increasing authenticity of classroom literacy experience. A Literature Circles classroom incorporates student choice of reading material and small temporary groups of students who are free to form new groups in response to new book choices. Diflerent groups of students can read diflerent books, in contrast to Book Club, in which the whole class usually reads a core text. Literature Circle activities should be implemented according to a regular and predictable schedule and student small group discussions are to be guided by students' notes, taken during reading. Discussion topics are to be student generated and small group discussions should be marked by natural conversation, that is, students talking about what they have read in a manner similar to how they might converse about books outside of the classroom. In a Literature Circles classroom, the teacher serves as a facilitator to encourage discussion, often participating actively within small-group discussions rather than acting as a monitor or a purveyor of discussion questions. Evaluation is collaborative, with teacher and students working together both to determine standards for assessment and to evaluate the quality of students' Literature Circle participation. Finally -— and for Daniels (2002), most importantly — a sense of playfulness should pervade the entire experience. As originally conceived, Literature Circles are more informal than Book Club, to the extent that there is no set of prescribed activities that constitute a Literature Circles experience. However, as Daniels (2002) acknowledges, one of the most popular means of introducing Literature Circles to students is to provide students with “role sheets” that 30 mecify par intent of r books too the classrc outside of Daniels (2 particular books. tht means. lr. than \tith character strenuou] Benefits I/) file Sim; ACCOIdi: “ill be ‘ for the Llleram; “Semit- ”Stiller; Sptcifi; : specify particular tasks for individuals to perform in the group discussion. The original intent of role sheets was to provide a launch pad upon which natural conversation about books could begin. As students become as comfortable conversing about books within the classroom as they would feel conversing about other topics of personal interest outside of school, the role sheets were supposed to disappear. Unfortunately, according to Daniels (2002), some teachers found comfort in holding students accountable to particular roles, and thus instead of being used as a key to unlock conversation about books, the role sheets themselves often became the end of conversation rather than the means. In many quarters Literature Circles became synonymous with role sheets rather than with free-flowing, authentic conversation about books. Daniels has objected to this characterization, and in his most recent book about Literature Circles has made a strenuous appeal for the original intentions of the strategy. Benefits of Book Clubs] Several authors suggest that book club activities may increase motivation because the structure of such activities aids students in becoming more self-determined learners. According to the self-determination theory of motivation (Ryan & Deci, 2000), students will be more motivated to engage in activities that satisfy their needs for relatedness, competence and autonomy. Such activities can satisfy adolescents' needs for relatedness by presenting opportunities for peer interactions (Combs, 2003; Raphael, Goatley, McMahon & Woodman, 1995; Riddle, 1996) as well as for informal interactions with teachers and more knowledgeable others (e.g., McMahon & Raphael, 1997). Teachers in ' For the sake of readability, I have chosen to use the general lower-case term “book clubs” to embrace both Literature Circles and Book Club activities as well as small-group discussion activities that closely resemble either strategy but may leave out one or more key element of the originally conceived instructional paradigm. I will use the upper-case Literature Circles and Book Club when speaking specifically about the original programs as envisioned by their designers. 31 . my; boolclubt (thriller. participatt ditersity t by engagi connectio stakes on competen Blunt. Lir (Katz & l who part Confiden assertior actit‘itie Choice : Particir atlit‘iti Noll. 1 by lncr Other a: elltl Ofl 199M book club-oriented classrooms report more inclusive discussions (Ahang, 1999; Chandler, 1997; George & Stix, 2000; Peralta-Nash & Dutch, 2000), with more students participating in small- and whole-group conversations and demonstrating respect for the diversity of their peers' opinions. In one classroom, a sense of relatedness was nurtured by engaging parents and students in book club activities, increasing home/school connections (Dias-Mitchell & Harris, 2001). Other authors suggest that the informal, low- stakes atmosphere of conversation about books has a positive impact on students' sense of competence as readers by increasing their self-efficacy for reading and discussion (e. g., Blum, Lipsett & Yocom, 2002) and by bolstering students' confidence in their own ideas (Katz & Kuby, 2001/02; Kong & Fitch, 2002/03). Chandler (1997) reports that students who participated in a summer book club program returned to school with greater confidence in their ability to formulate “valid readings” (that is, evidence-based assertions; see Rosenblatt, 1978), which they promptly displayed in similar literacy activities during the regular school year. Finally, allowing students a high degree of choice in reading materials and discussion topics, as well as giving them opportunities to participate in the evaluation process have been singled out as elements of book club activities that increase students' sense of autonomy (Burns, 1998; Katz & Kuby, 2001/02; Noll, 1994; Riddle 1996). Beyond those authors who have argued that book club activities motivate students by increasing their sense of competence or expectancy for success in reading, several other authors have made the case that book clubs motivate by also working on the value end of the Expectancy-Value model of motivation (e. g., Eccles, Wigfield & Schiefele, 1998). By giving students greater ownership over the entire reading experience, from 7‘ it‘ll” ‘ I i book selection to guiding and evaluating class discussions of books, students may come to value reading as a vital part of their adolescent identity (Raphael, Goatley, McMahon & Woodman, 1995; Raphael, Kehus & Damphousse, 2001). Lesperance (2002) observed that students who participated in a school-wide book club came to value recreational reading more as they perceived that their personal reading interests were validated through book club activities, while Ahang (1999) found that students enjoyed and valued a wider range of reading materials through their exposure to other students' reading interests in book club activities. As Hall (2003) notes in “The 'Oprahfication' of Literacy,” the kinds of literacy practices enacted in book clubs have become a defining feature of vernacular literacies in the past decade, making the book club format a more authentic context for the study of literature than the traditional close, analytical reading approach. While the above theories can help illuminate a general relationship between participation in book club activities and motivation for reading, many authors have argued that book clubs may be especially effective with adolescents because they address this age group's unique developmental needs. Raphael, Kehus and Damphousse (2001) make the case that Book Club accommodates young adolescents' physical needs by providing opportunities for movement around the classroom, which can help students cope with their sometimes wildly fluctuating energy levels. Book clubs can capitalize on adolescents' newly developed formal operational skills by offering opportunities for perspective-taking (Raphael & McMahon, 1994), and for “vicariously [experiencing] events they might not want to live out, but need to ponder and imaginatively explore” (Smith, 2000, p. 31). Such “subjunctive engagement with text” (Broughton, 2002, p. 35) 33 . m‘flu‘ is a unique adolescent (Erickson. structure ' who play 2001). \\ relationsl' and incre Studies h; identity c low Slahi aclltitles rtgintes identitie. amldtll: is a unique affordance of adolescent cognition that can also aid the grand task of adolescent social/emotional development, the need to construct a personal identity (Erickson, 1963). In addition to the vicarious experiences garnered from reading, the very structure of book club activities provides adolescents with direct experiences with peers who play such a vital role in adolescent socialization (Raphael, Kehus & Damphousse, 2001) While the bulk of the literature on book clubs has argued strongly for a relationship between participation in book club activities and positive attitudes toward and increased motivation for engaging in multiple literacy practices, a small number of studies have explored the consequences of book club participation on adolescents' gender identity development. Smith (2000), Dutro (2001) and Broughton (2002) noted that the low stakes discussion among small groups of peers that is at the heart of book club activities empowered both girls and boys to take more critical stances toward gender regimes within their classrooms and to construct more flexible, comfortable gender identities for themselves. Dutro’s (2001) observational study of a Book Club approach in a middle school classroom revealed that as boys’ engagement with the text intensified, policing of gender boundaries disappeared. Indeed, high status boys who had given the most stereotypical gender performances at the start of the observation period were transformed into avid readers of books from the “American Girl” series. This suggests that gendered resistance to dominant literacy practices might be addressed more effectively by nurturing a classroom culture in which boys directly engage with texts than by forcing boys to confront latent assumptions about gender through critical literacy activities. 34 ‘I gums—J lo boys' resi‘ I comprehe self-select and liter. many of t readers. i into a pen literacy i play a Sp boys fror “Umber t discours exclusit titties or Small- a SUbject 0{the s to read Studen: ‘0 Cloc‘a Summary and Research Questions To summarize, research suggests that an effective means for overcoming many boys' resistance to active participation in academic literacy practices is to implement a comprehensive program that privileges (among other features) student autonomy through self-selection of personally interesting texts and student-centered discussion. Book Club and Literature Circles are two frameworks for instruction that incorporate and coordinate many of the best practices identified by research as especially motivating for adolescent readers, incorporating self-selection, aesthetic engagement and a strong social component into a permeable approach to language arts instruction that values students’ vernacular literacy practices. A few key studies have further demonstrated that such programs can play a special part in disrupting the development of a gender regime that alienates many boys from active participation in academic literacy practices. However, there are a number of limitations in the current literature on book club activities and gender discourse that the present study was designed to address. For one, the majority of studies on book club based classrooms have focused exclusively on the tasks in which students engage in such contexts. The chief (and often times only) source of data in these studies is the in-class discourse of students involved in small- and large-group discussions, with very little consideration of the texts that are the subject of those discussions (e. g., Johnson, 2000; McMahon & Goatley, 1995). Only one of the studies of book club based classrooms has analyzed the texts that students choose to read (Dutro, 2001), and none of the studies have analyzed the processes by which students select reading materials in the context of such programs. In this study, I sought to docmnent and analyze the texts that a group of middle school boys chose to read in the 35 context of interesting Se classroom: determine events. In honoring documen freedom l | work of l analyzed I those set COmpara~ Periheab cornpare ClaSSYOO PTESEm longituc i“Spire .. context of a book club based classroom in which students self-selected personally interesting texts that formed the basis of their literacy instruction. Secondly, none of the published studies on Book Club or Literature Circle classrooms have attempted to document the vernacular literacy practices of students to determine the extent to which those practices manifest themselves in academic literacy events. In other words, despite the promise of such permeable instructional contexts for honoring students' extracurricular funds of knowledge, the published literature has neither documented nor analyzed what happens when adolescents are given a high degree of freedom to cross boundaries between vernacular and academic literacy practices. The work of Dyson (1993, 2003) on young elementary school students learning to write has analyzed children's extracunicular firnds of knowledge and explored the ways in which those vernacular literacy practices permeate academic contexts. However, there is not a comparable study of the vernacular literacy practices of adolescent boys in a similarly permeable classroom environment for reading instruction. In this study, I sought to document the vernacular literacy practices of a class of middle school boys in order to compare them with the academic literacy practices in which they engaged to determine the extent to which the borders between literacy practices actually became permeable in a classroom structured around students' self-selected texts. Thirdly, the majority of the published studies on book club based classrooms present either one-shot or short-term observations of classroom interactions, rather than a longitudinal analysis. This is ironic given many authors' claims that such activities can inspire once reluctant readers to develop a long-term reading habit. In this study, I documented and analyzed students' reading selections over the course of a full academic 36 . *1 year to ex literacy pr Ft participati of attitude club has: based cla attitudes. address ti toward re between there we: reading i academi _r_ bQOlC clt. gmllps C Same lC;t a SChoo'; qualllitg' mane c Header,” year to examine what kinds of changes took place in students' academic and vernacular literacy practices. Fourth, despite the claim that students had better attitudes toward reading after participating in book clubs, none of the publications reviewed here offers a measurement of attitude, only one (Raphael & McMahon, 1994) makes comparisons with a non-book club based classroom, and none draw comparisons between students within a book club based classroom. This begs the question: how do we know that the students had better attitudes, and what or whom are those attitudes better than? In this study, I attempted to address this gap in the literature by taking multiple quantitative measurements of attitude toward reading in two book club based language arts classes to make comparisons between individuals and groups of students within those classes and to explore whether there were significant differences between, for example, boys and girls of different reading abilities within the same classroom over the course of the year. In summary, I sought in this study to document and analyze the vernacular and academic literacy practices of middle school boys in the context of a language arts classroom that scaffolds instruction on students' self-selected reading materials and on book club style interactions. My goal was to draw comparisons between individuals and groups of students within two book club based language arts classes (both taught by the same teacher) in terms of both the literacy practices they made visible over the course of . a school year and the attitudes toward reading for multiple purposes they expressed on quantitative and qualitative assessments of reading attitude. I hope through this study to enhance the adolescent literacy literature by documenting both the vernacular and academic literacy practices of a class of middle school boys over the course of a full 37 academic year, and by comparing these practices with those of girls in the same class. These results should also enhance the teacher education literature by providing insight into the ways in which a teacher can fulfill a consultative role in facilitating individual students' literacy development in a permeable classroom. This study was guided by four overarching research questions: 1. What are the vernacular literacy practices of a class of middle school boys and what kinds of texts do these boys engage with in those practices? 2. How and to what extent do these boys' vernacular literacies become visible within a language arts program structured around students' self-selected texts? 3. How and to what extent do the vernacular and academic literacy practices of these boys differ from those of the girls within the same class? and, 4. What attitudes toward vernacular and academic literacy practices do these boys hold, how do they compare with the attitudes of other students, and how do those attitudes change over the course of an academic year in a book club based classroom? 38 l 0t the Home | school \V. has been The dent. of the de: profile th classroor in Chaptp account r the conc‘ participa dillerem The Con l ilh'lng 0f Ofiiciaj' area, it; halite: CHAPTER 3 Methods Chapter Overview Observations and interviews for this study took place at Harwell Middle School in the Homestead Public School District over the course of the 2004-2005 school year. This school was chosen because of Mr. Connors' unusual approach to teaching reading, which has been briefly discussed in prior chapters and will be described with more detail below. The demographics of the school and district population also provided a good combination of the demographic profiles of surrounding school districts in the region. I will first profile the district and school before telescoping down to a discussion of the particular classroom that was involved in this study, expanding on the description previously given in Chapter 1 of this dissertation. Following discussion of the setting, I will present an account of the methods used in this study to answer the research questions presented at the conclusion of Chapter 2. Included in this account will be information on the participants, data gathering and analysis strategies, and a timeline demonstrating how different methods were employed during different phases of the school year. Setting The Community The Homestead Public School District serves approximately 3,000 K-12 students living on the western side of Ilium, a mid-size city in Michigan's Lower Peninsula. Officially, Homestead is an “urban fringe” district. It encompasses two large residential areas within Ilium's city limits as well as all of the unincorporated neighborhoods between Ilium and Birch Creek, the closest suburban community to the west. Ilium's 39 populatior wealth or west arter which fre facilities. and scrap end. The Homeste, notjust g T $51,343 the Iliun: Poor‘s [s The distr [he SEW; distribut indicate. reIlectit- malOI er goi'errm industry While 8 BaChElr. population is predominantly working and middle class, with few observable extremes of wealth or poverty. Traveling westward fiom downtown Ilium along the city's main east- west artery, you officially enter Homestead by passing underneath a railroad bridge upon which freight trains shuttle between a complex of four large automobile production facilities. These factories constitute a vertically integrated system that takes in raw steel and scrap metal at one end and turns out new, fully assembled automobiles at the other end. They also comprise the single largest employer of working households in Homestead, which lies snugly between the Ilium and Birch Creek Public School Districts not just geographically, but also economically and demographically. The median household income in the Homestead Public School District was $51,243 in 2002, slightly above the state average and roughly midway between those of the Ilium ($33,220) and Birch Creek ($64,445) public school districts (Standard and Poor's [S&P], 2003). Median housing values for the three districts are similarly ranked. The distribution of households by income in Homestead is relatively homogeneous across the seven income brackets used by Standard & Poor's, whereas the household income distributions for Ilium and Birch Creek skew in opposite directions (see Table A1). As indicated by Table A1, this relatively even distribution across income categories is more reflective of the overall distribution in the state. In addition to the automotive industry, major employers of Homestead citizens include the State of Michigan (many state government offices lie within Homestead and its immediate vicinities) and the service industry, as Homestead's neighborhoods orbit one of Ilium's largest commercial zones. While 81% of adults in Ilium have at least a high school diploma and 27.1% have a Bachelor's degree or higher, 90.4% of Homestead adults have at least a high school 40 -_. diploma are 91.60i Creek in i percentag free or re students r county m 0 suburban by racial Ilium an. SUburbs . Of Black impolite Categori ; raider pe- Passing of the s: Ilium a: 7779 Sch l bIOCks t Honles: diploma and 36.1% have a Bachelor's degree or higher, and for Birch Creek the figures are 91.6% and 34.4% respectively (S&P, 2003). Similarly, Homestead is more like Birch Creek in terms of the number of students classified as economically disadvantaged as a percentage of overall student enrollment. While 57.1% of the students in Ilium qualify for free or reduced-price lunches, 16.6% of Homestead students and 13.9% of Birch Creek students receive these services. These figures compare to a state mean of 30.7%, and a county mean of 18.9% (S&P, 2003). Other features of the Homestead community indicate a similar blend of urban and suburban trends. As Table A2 shows, the distribution of Homestead's student population by racial/ethnic background forms a middle ground between the race/ethnicity profiles of Ilium and Birch Creek. Proceeding from the urban environment of Ilium to the semi-rural suburbs of Birch Creek, the proportion of White students increases, while the proportions of Black, Hispanic and Native American students decreases. Although the relative proportions of Birch Creek's student body are closer to the state averages for most categories, the greater representation of minority students in Homestead should afford a wider perspective on the diversity of literacy practices within the community. Finally, the passing and excelling rates of Homestead students on the most recently available results of the state's assessment of student progress stand midway between the rates posted by Ilium and Birch Creek across grade levels and subjects. The School Harwell Middle School is situated on the far east side of Homestead, only three blocks fiom the automobile plant that stands on the border between the Ilium and Homestead districts, and four blocks fi'om one of Ilium's three large comprehensive high 41 schools. l high scho surroundi homes of represent factory er VFW ant inall of} sixth grat single jui itselfis s a thick Cl admirtist Cafetor’u gUidanc ClaSSl'Oc Sixth gr Rachel. band an COUISe ( their ho schools. Harwell is, in fact, much closer to this high school than it is to Homestead's own high school, which is more centrally located in the district. The neighborhoods surrounding Harwell are solidly working class, filled with the modest, mid-20'h century homes of automobile plant workers. The area also serves as home to several union halls, representing the various trades employed at the factories, at least two bars frequented by factory employees, many small and home-based automobile repair facilities, and the local VFW and American Legion posts. There is not a more traditional rust-belt neighborhood in all of Homestead, or perhaps in all of Ilium. Students come to Harwell Middle School, Homestead's only campus for fifth and sixth graders, from four elementary schools. They leave Harwell for the Homestead's single junior high (grades 7 and 8) and, eventually, Homestead High School. Harwell itself is shaped like a stout letter H, with large rectangular east and west wings united by a thick central hub. The hub contains the school's library/media center and the administration offices. The east wing of the school contains the school's gymnasium, cafetorium, swimming pool, art and music rooms. The west wing is the home of the guidance offices, all of the regular classrooms and all of the student lockers. Fifth grade classrooms are south of the central hallways leading past the library to the east wing, and sixth grade classrooms are to the north. Students in both grades are assigned to two- teacher teams for their core academic instruction, and are able to choose between choir, band and orchestra for music. Due to budgetary constraints, there are no foreign language course options for students at Harwell. All students at Harwell are assigned to a team of two teachers, one of whom is their homeroom teacher. Teaching duties for the core content areas are shared between 42 I‘“___“‘:'l E. the two ta tell as re groups. I lie flat: D students' general r lflStructir MDmp silently Students. Silltlents Mignm bOLh tea EMT- Cot; homeIOQp £01305 j COIL? Org ' Smiling .. the two teachers, and students switch between their teachers multiple times each day, as well as report to various special classes, such as PE, music and art in their homeroom groups. The Classroom During the 2004-2005 school year, Mr. Connors taught language arts to 48 students1 who were enrolled in two homerooms, one assigned to Mr. Connors and the other assigned to Ms. Julie Price. Teaching duties for each homeroom were divided such that Connors was responsible for language arts and science instruction and Price was responsible for math and social studies instruction. Students began each day in their respective homerooms and switched classrooms at least twice daily in order to receive instruction in all core areas of the curriculum. Reading in a variety of formats and for a variety of purposes was built into the general routine of both Connors’ and Price’s classrooms, independent of reading instruction in the content areas. Both homerooms began each day with 20 to 30 minutes of Drop Everything and Read (DEAR) time, during which students were expected to read silently fiom self-selected texts. No restrictions were placed on the kinds of texts that students could read during these periods, although as will be discussed later in this study, students mostly read texts that they had selected to read to fulfill language arts assignments during their DEAR time. In addition to the regularly scheduled DEAR times, both teachers also had their classes engage in brief, impromptu DEAR periods after 1 Mr. Connors and Ms. Price began the year with a group of 50 students split evenly between their two homerooms. One student in each homeroom moved out of the school district during the first trimester, and mid-way through thesecond trimester, one new student moved into the district and was added to Mr. Connors homeroom. At various times during the school year, a handful of students were moved from Mr. Connors’ and Ms. Price’s homerooms into other teachers’ homeroom classes, and vice versa. Forty-eight students were in Connors’ classes for at least two trimesters and participated in all major reading assignments, and the dataset for this study is limited to information about these 48 students. 43 . h‘; ' ' . I l-I I. students DEAR in B. which st ‘ classroon baskets n Biograph Genre Pri area with time. Th Price) cr Should 1 Other th Classro texts e nomp bl‘Oug 56 le Q students completed assigned tasks in independent work times and frequently included DEAR in the plans they left for substitutes. Both classrooms featured small libraries of books from a variety of genres from which students could check out reading materials (see Figures B1 and B2). These classroom libraries were organized by genre, with Connors’ library containing separate baskets marked Realistic Fiction, Historical Fiction, Fantasy, Science Fiction and Biography, which are the five genres from which students were to select books for the Genre Project. Price’s classroom library also featured a reading comer, a small carpeted area with bean bag chairs and special lighting where students could spend their DEAR time. This area also included a reading recommendation wall, where students (and Ms. Price) could post notecards with comments on books they had read (see Figure B3). It should be noted at this point that neither classroom library featured any reading materials other than books, which represents an implicit constraint on the permeability of the classroom context. While students were not explicitly constrained from reading non-book texts during DEAR sessions, the lack of availability of magazines, newspapers or even non-print texts in the classroom libraries did restrict choices of students who had not brought reading materials fiom home. Nevertheless, the school library did contain a wide selection of magazines, newspapers and videotapes for student check out, in addition to books. Connors’ and Price’s students made regular weekly class visits to the school library over the course of the year, and during DEAR time, Connors would allow one student at a time to visit the school library to check our or return materials. Both teachers included read-alouds from popular novels in their regular daily routines. During the first trimester of the 2004-2005 school year, Connors read A Series 44 ofL’itfort l homeroor 2000). At. classes (it I Number r Swallow content 3 classes 0 10 and 2* thereof) would 0‘ Clarificat Series 0.7 read-alor in Iclatir of Unfortunate Events: The Bad Beginning (Snicket, 1999) to his homeroom, while to her homeroom, Price read Jaguar (Smith, 1997) and Joey Pigza Loses Control (Gantos, 2000). Additionally, Price read the following books to both her and Connors’ homeroom classes during the second and third trimesters: The Magician ’s Nephew (Lewis, 1955), Number the Stars (Lowry, 1990), Warriors Don’t Cry (Beals, 1994), and Joey Pigza Swallowed the Key (Gantos, 2000). Read-alouds were typically scheduled as buffers after content area lessons and before whole class movements, such as to lunch, recess, special classes or the other teacher’s classroom. These read-aloud periods usually lasted between 10 and 20 minutes, always involved the teacher reading a chapter (or substantial portion thereof) fiom a book, and were never followed by extension activities. Read-alouds would occasionally be interrupted for definitions of unfamiliar words, questions for clarification or brief commentary by the teacher on particular moments in the narrative. A Series of Unfortunate Events: The Bad Beginning was especially suited to this style of read-aloud, as the book’s narrator frequently interrupts the story to define words he uses in relating the story, often giving off-beat definitions that are tangentially related to the common uses of the words, affording Connors the opportunity to discuss the denotative and connotative meanings of the words with his students. Beyond these routines and activities that both teachers built into their daily schedules to promote reading, Connors’ language arts curriculum, as would be expected, involved a great deal of reading. As discussed in the first chapter of this study, Mr. Connors’ approach to language arts instruction places a premium on attempting to bridge the gap between students’ vernacular and academic literacy practices in an effort to promote more positive attitudes toward reading. Each trimester of the 2004-2005 school 45 -Im‘ I eat {camp students' tisualiza Connors‘ d | class he Reading Daybooi taken di the publi Words“ i Students compos not incl Sources In these idellllll: PTCSenr Studen: Corners ' Sioden: {limp}C year featured a unique suite of activities geared toward that end, but incorporating students’ self-selected texts into reading instruction, emphasizing the development of visualization skills, and maintaining a book club framework were components of Connors’ language arts curriculum that persisted across the three grading periods. First trimester. During the first trimester, a typical day in Connors’ language arts class began with a shared reading of a selected text, usually from the Daybook of Critical Reading and Writing (Claggett, Reid & Vinz, 1999, hereafter referred to as “the Daybook”), followed by a writing response activity. The response activity was often taken directly from the activities presented in the Daybook with each selection. Beyond the published activities, Connors would also challenge students to identify “picture words” in the reading selection, words that conjured up especially vivid images in students’ minds as they read. In addition to the Daybook, each student also had a composition book that they used as a journal in which to complete follow-up activities not included in the Daybook, engage in various response activities to selected texts fi‘om sources other than the Daybook and write drafts of essays for major writing assignments. In these journals, students would frequently be asked to record the picture words that they identified in the text and sketch the images that these words conjured in their minds. In this way, Connors would both engage students with the intended focus of the activities presented in the Daybook (such as how to identify point of view in a story) and reinforce students’ abilities to visualize meaning from text, the skill that Connors saw as the cornerstone to effective (and affective) engagement with print. Connors checked students’ Daybooks and journals each night and assigned daily grades based solely on completion of the day’s activities. 46 ‘LI‘ 4* 1““ ‘ —‘—n. I1." J indepcnd pages in containi: negotiate student's student i pages th‘ SUmItiar each to student readin Studei Slgne grad arts der llldt am Place “Otis Connors’ language arts students were also expected to complete a course of independent reading during this first trimester. Students were to read a certain number of pages from self-selected “chapter books” (defined very loosely by Connors as books containing “at least 100 pages”) each month. The precise ntunber of books required was negotiated individually between Connors and his students, in accordance with each student's reading level and the particular length and reading level of books that the student chose. Students kept weekly reading logs in which they recorded the number of pages they read each day, the author and title of each book they read, and a brief summary of what happened in the reading they accomplished each day. After completing each book, students were expected to write short book reviews to be shared with other students to recommend (or in very rare cases steer other students away from) further reading. Every two weeks, Connors assigned independent reading grades by assessing the students’ reading logs for completion and for the number of days on which a parent had signed the log to confirm that the student had read at home for at least 30 minutes. These grades were given the greatest weight in the calculation of students’ cumulative language arts grade for the trimester. Consistently throughout the first trimester, Connors structured activities in his language arts classes to integrate students’ independent reading with the mini-lessons derived from the Daybook. To illustrate how Connors brought students’ self-selected independent reading into his language arts lessons, I provide the following description of a typical day’s activities in Connors’ class during the first trimester. This lesson took place early in the school year, soon after students had been taught the concept of “picture words,” and it began with Connors reading aloud the following information from the 47 Connori and as s only to also to Volume to this the} Daybook about point of view: A Story’s Point of View. The third ingredient for a successful story is point of view — the angle from which a story is told. Ifthe point of view changes, so does the story. Active readers know that understanding point of view is as easy as deciding who the narrator (or storyteller) is. Read another excerpt from Danny the [sic] Champion of the World. As you read, highlight or circle any information you find about the narrator. Use the response notes to make observations about what point of view the author is using. (p. 45) Connors then gave students approximately seven minutes in which to read the selection, and as suggested in the directions given in the Daybook, he encouraged the students not only to write “response notes” about point of view in the margins of their Daybooks but also to circle any “picture words” they encountered while reading. After soliciting student volunteers to share their response notes with the whole class, Connors directed students to this follow-up question given in the Daybook: How would the scene the narrator describes be different if it were told from the father’s point of view? Use what you know about Danny and his father to rewrite the kite-flying scene from the father’s point of view. Be as creative as you like, but make sure you give readers an adult angle on the events. (p. 46) Students were given a few minutes to write responses to this question in their Daybooks, then encouraged to share their responses first with a partner and then with the whole class. Connors then directed students to share the picture words that they had circled as they read the selection. In their journals, students recorded their picture words and made 48 sketches of the scenes that they imagined in their minds as they read the selection. After sharing their sketches with partners and discussing how their sketches were inspired by the picture words presented in the selection, students were challenged to find picture words in the books that they had been reading for the independent reading component of language arts and sketch the mental images that those words inspired. Connors then called on students to share the picture words and sketches they made from their independent reading selections. As a further challenge, Connors asked students to think about the point of view in their independent reading selections, calling on volunteers to share their insights. During the next session of language arts, students were given a few minutes to revisit the selection from Danny Champion of the World (Dahl, 1975), review the picture words they had previously identified and enhance the sketches they had made in preparation for a closed-book (but open-journal) “visualization quiz” (see Figure B4). This basic lesson structure, in which students explored a literary element — such as characterization or point of view — and a reading comprehension strategy — such as visualization — in both a short Daybook selection and self-selected texts was repeated throughout the first trimester. During the subsequent trimesters, Connors continued to utilize this framework, although less frequently as students’ self-selected texts were given more prominence in in-class activities and as more time was devoted to building students’ writing skills. Because my primary interest was in students’ literacy practices as revealed through their text selections for academic and recreational purposes, my data gathering was limited to the reading activities in Connors’ language arts class. In the next sections, I will thus only describe how the reading curriculum for the respective trimesters reflected Connors’ commitments to student choice, building visualization skills 49 and soc literary readir were llltm and social practices in the literacy classroom, themes that emerge from the review of the literature on the kinds of classroom contexts that support positive attitudes toward reading and high motivation to read. Second trimester. While several mini-lessons such as the one described above were presented during the second trimester to reinforce students’ ability to visualize meaning fi'om text, Connors also built mini-lessons on other topics, including making personal connections to text and identifying and retelling main events/ ideas in different genres of writing. The Daybook continued to be a source of reading selections for mini- lessons, but Connors also incorporated readings from Time for Kids, a classroom magazine that all students received weekly, and short picture book biographies such as John Henry (Lester, 1994) and Freedom River (Rappaport, 2000). Connors also used short films, such as Wallace & Gromit: The Wrong Trousers (Park, 1993) and Rikki T ikki T avi (Jones, 1975), as texts for students to practice their skills in retelling main events and drawing personal connections to text. Most of the mini-lessons during the second trimester were based on selections from the novel Maniac Magee (Spinelli, 1990), which Connors read aloud to the class. During these mini-lessons, students were occasionally given copies of Maniac Magee in which to follow along with the read-aloud, but just as often they were challenged to record picture words or other responses to the novel without the aid of the text. This was done to stimulate students’ mastery of picture words in both print and oral language. A typical mini-lesson with Maniac Magee would involve Connors reading a chapter aloud to the class, during which students would record picture words and attempt to represent the mental images evoked by those words via sketches in their journals. On one occasion, 50 groups of students expanded upon the sketches they had made individually to recreate a key scene from the novel first in storyboard format, then as a skit that was performed for the whole class. The main focus of reading instruction during this trimester was on expanding students’ exposure to different reading genres through their self-selections. To that end, Connors not only incorporated a greater variety of informational texts into his mini- lessons, but he also introduced the Genre Project, in which students were challenged to select at least one book from each of five genres: fantasy, realistic fiction, science fiction, historical fiction, and biography (see Figure B5 for the assignment sheet Connors gave students). These five genres coincided with the genre divisions used by Connors for his classroom library, and the only restriction that Connors placed on students’ selections for this project was length. All selections were to be a minimum of 100 pages long, except for biographies, which were to be at least 60 pages long. This distinction was made in recognition of the different demands made on readers by non-fiction and narrative fiction, Connors’ sense that students were relatively inexperienced in reading long-fonn non- fiction texts, and the large selection of biographies in the school library that contained fewer than 100 pages. Midway through the trimester, Connors added “free choice” as a sixth option for students to select in completing the project because of the relative paucity of science fiction books in the school library. After finishing a book, students completed book review forms, and students’ overall grade for the Genre Project was based on the number of reviews they completed. Connors provided students with a generic book review form for all genres except for biography, which had its own form (see Figures B6, B7 and B8). These reviews focused 51 on topics such as making text-to-life connections, identifying and retelling main ideas/events, and exploring story elements such as setting, all of which were addressed in mini-lessons. Connors assessed each book review for completion, and then made all book reviews available in a binder for students to peruse and discover additional titles that might pique their interests. At several points throughout the trimester, Connors solicited student volunteers to give brief book chats about their Genre Project selections as well as had students engage in informal, small-group conversations about the books they were currently reading. Connors’ twin emphases on developing students’ visualization skills and honoring students’ vernacular literacy practices cuhninated midway through the second trimester in an activity in which Connors encouraged his students to: Bring in something that you enjoy reading, something that you think you are interested in reading, something that you can make a picture in your head as you read. Many students don't enjoy reading because they approach it as reading words, rather than creating pictures. I want you to think that reading is more fun than you thought it could be by the end of the year. Connors put no restrictions on the kinds of text that students could bring in for this activity, even encouraging students to bring in video game instruction booklets, owner’s manuals for electronic appliances or DVD case inserts, all of which he noted were the kinds of texts that he regularly enjoyed reading. On the day of the activity, students wrote extensively in their journals about the materials that they brought in. They first wrote a general description of the text they had brought and then a brief explanation of what they most liked about the text. Next, they 52 shared their texts with each other in small groups, and individual volunteers were solicited to share with the whole class. Connors had the students engage in ten minutes of DEAR with their texts, and students were expected to record picture words in their journals as they read. Finally, students wrote about how the picture words they identified helped them generate mental images as they read and made sketches of those mental images. Third trimester. Connors’ language arts curriculum culminated in the third trimester with the Book Project. Very little class time during this marking period was spent on reading mini-lessons. Most language arts sessions were either devoted to writer’s workshop activities or to the Book Project, which built on the skills developed in the reading mini-lessons of the prior trimesters. Connors built more DEAR time into language arts lessons during the third trimester than previously and specified that students read their Book Project selections during that time, unless they had already completed the book. Connors was also out of the classroom for various reasons more often during this trimester than previously, and DEAR time for the Book Project was a routine feature of the plans left for his substitutes. It should also be noted that there were fewer language arts sessions during this last trimester. The annual human growth and development unit was scheduled not only during the third trimester, but also during language arts time, as between Price and Connors, only Connors had the proper certification to teach this material to the two homerooms. For the Book Project (see Figure B9), students were to select a single book from any genre that was at least 150 pages long‘(bonus points were offered to students who selected biographies, science fiction, or books with over 250 pages) to read and create a 53 poster presentation about. These poster presentations represented a culminating exhibition of the concepts students had explored and the reading skills they had developed over the course of the school year. Posters were to contain basic information about the book (e.g., author, title, genre), descriptions of the main character and setting, a retelling of major events in the book (including the chief problem to be solved by the main character and how that problem was resolved), the book’s main theme, text-to-life connections, a brief review, and sketches of vivid moments from the story. All of these literary elements had been taught individually via mini-lessons using the Daybook or other short texts in conjunction with students’ self-selected reading materials during the previous trimesters, and the Book Project gave students an opporttmity to demonstrate mastery of those topics cumulatively using a text of their own choice. Connors aided individual students in the selection of appropriately challenging and personally interesting texts for the Book Project during class visits to the school library shortly after assigning the Book Project. Students were also encouraged to bring texts from home or to borrow texts from the classroom library as their reading interests led them. During the library visits, Connors met with all students individually to record their book choices and guided any students who were having trouble finding something interesting to read toward texts that resonated with reading selections they had made and enjoyed in previous activities. After book selection was completed, students were given one month in which to read their chosen texts and prepare their presentations. During this period, language arts class time was primarily devoted to bolstering students’ writing skills in an expository essay unit to prepare for the MEAP writing test, but Connors incorporated brief mini- 54 _. 4v- ' lessons 1 their Bod hoiects Project. posters. present proud ch: CO Ch COL lessons to review each of the literary elements that students were expected to address in their Book Project presentations and encouraged students to focus on reading their Book Project selections during DEAR time. In the week prior to the due date for the Book Project, Connors devoted two language arts sessions to helping students construct their posters, providing each student with poster board upon which to construct their presentations. Markers, colored pencils, construction paper and gluesticks were also provided for student use. The final presentations were scheduled over the course of three language arts sessions, during which writing workshop activities and visits to the semi- annual Scholastic Book Fair were also scheduled in order to maintain variety and student engagement. Activities related to the human growth and development unit were also scheduled during this period in fulfilhnent of state and district requirements for health instruction. Connors assessed students’ Book Project presentations using a simple checklist rubric, and the grade for this presentation constituted the entirety of the reading component of students’ language arts grades for this trimester. Opportunities for choice. As described in the preceding section, Connors’ classroom afforded several opportunities for students to self-select reading materials for academic purposes and potentially make their vernacular literacy practices visible within the classroom. The most potentially permeable context was DEAR time, as Connors placed no explicit restrictions on what students could spend their time reading. However, DEAR time was not wholly impermeable; unless students brought texts from home, choices of reading materials were restricted to books from the classroom library and books and periodicals from the school library. The next most potentially permeable context was the culminating activity on visualization skills, as students were explicitly 55 encouraged to bring a text from home that was effective at conjuring visual images. While Connors described a wide range of texts that students could bring in, from the traditional (books, magazines) to new media (DVD cases, video games), students were restricted to bringing in a single text, andthe text did have to include print. Independent Reading (first trimester), the Genre Project (second trimester) and the Book Project (third trimester) represent the least potentially permeable of the academic contexts for which Connors’ students were given the opportunity to self-select texts. In all three of these contexts, students were restricted to selecting books, and in the Genre Project, choices were restricted to books falling within five pre-specified genres. Thus, Connors’ curriculum was not a completely open-choice environment, but rather offered varying levels of permeability to the texts of students’ vernacular literacy practices. Because the potential of a permeable classroom to scaffold students’ development in academic literacy practices on their vemacular literacies depends on both the openness of the teacher to those literacies and the willingness of students to take advantage of opportunities to make those literacies visible, the structural and de facto constraints placed on students’ text selections for academic purposes are important delirniters to recognize before enmnerating the procedures used to document Connors’ students’ text selections. The second research question of this dissertation was included to assess whether and to what extent boys would take advantage of these opportunities for choice. Research Design Overview To document the vernacular and academic literacy practices of a group of sixth- grade boys, I designed a case study (Yin, 2002), patterned after the work of Finders (1997) and Hynds (1997). I spent the 2004-2005 school year at Harwell Middle School 56 observing and interviewing students in Connors’ language arts classes. As will be described in greater detail below, the focus of my data gathering was on the texts that students in these classes selected: 1) to fulfill specific reading assignments in the language arts curriculum; 2) to read during Drop Everything and Read (DEAR) periods; and 3) to interact with outside of school in their free time. The basic unit of documentation and analysis in this study is the text selection, defined here as an individual student’s interaction with a single text in a particular context, and different data gathering methods were employed to document the text selections that students made for different purposes. For texts selected by students to fulfill academic assignments, the sources of data were archives of student work in the Genre Project and Book Project saved by Mr. Connors for inclusion in students’ reading portfolios, observations of student Book Project presentations and student activities during language arts sessions, observations of read-aloud periods, and interviews with individual students. These records account for choices made over the course of the full academic year by all students in Mr. Connors’ homeroom as well as those made by students in Price’s homeroom who came to Mr. Connors for language arts instruction. For texts selected by students for DEAR periods, I observed students in Connors’ homeroom during their daily DEAR period at least twice each week during the second and third trimesters of the school year, during which most of their major language arts assignments were completed. I also observed students in both of the homerooms for which Mr. Connors was responsible for language arts instruction during occasional impromptu DEAR periods within regular language arts instructional time. 57 Finally, to document texts selected by students for extracurricular, recreational purposes, I interviewed a group of 22 focal students on multiple occasions over the course of the second and third trimesters of the school year. These interviews involved both self-reports of recreational reading and text interactions by students and observations of students browsing for personally interesting texts at a local book store and at a school-sponsored book fair. Participants The participants in this study were the two sixth grade language arts classes taught by Mr. Ed Connors at Harwell Middle School during the 2004-2005 school year. This age group was chosen because many studies have demonstrated a significant decline in boys' and girls' attitudes toward reading and school in general during the middle/junior high school years, indicating that boys at this age may be particularly vulnerable to disengagement from reading (Andermann & Maehr, 1994; Bean, 1998; Eccles, Midgley, Wigfield, Buchanan, Reuman, Flanagan & Maclver, 1993; McKenna, Kear & Ellsworth, 1995). Among the 48 students in Connors’ two language arts classes were 24 boys and 24 girls. The ethnic composition of the students was as follows: 50% White (14 boys, 10 girls), 31.25% African-American (6 boys, 9 girls), 8.3% Mixed ethnicity (2 boys, 2 girls), 6.25% Latino (2 boys, 1 girls), 2.1% Asian (1 girl), and 2.1% African (1 girl). The average grade equivalent reading level of Connors’ students as measured by the Gates- MacGinitie Reading Test administered at the end of the 2003-2004 school year was 6.64 (SD = 3.24, range = 2 to 13). The average grade equivalent reading level for boys was 6.19 (SD = 3.01, range = 2 to 13), and the average grade equivalent reading level for girls 58 M.“ was 7. 10 about tobeust all field ll he (ran 300 of( SOL tria qua telat inch was 7.10 (SD = 3.46, range = 3 to 13). This difference was not statistically significant (t = -0.09364, p > 0.1). In Connors’ and Price’s classes, each student was assigned a number to be used throughout the year to facilitate record-keeping. These numbers were used in all field notes and other documents in this study to ensure anonymity. From the whole group of Connors’ language arts students, 22 students volunteered to be interviewed further about their reading attitudes and habits over the course of the year. This sample contained 11 boys and 11 girls, and represented the range of ethnic backgrounds found in the two homerooms: 50% White (7 boys, 4 girls), 27% Afiican-American (2 boys, 4 girls), 13.6% Mixed ethnicity (1 boy, 2 girls), 4.5% Latino (1 boy), and 4.5% African (1 girl). The average reading level of this sample was 7.25 (range = 2 to 13) as measured by the Gates-MacGinitie Reading Test at the end of the 2003-2004 school year. This is higher than the average reading level for the whole group of Connors’ language arts students (6.09, range = 3 to 13), but not significantly different (t—=0.669, p>0.5). Procedures Figure B10 provides a graphic illustration of the classroom activities, data sources, data analysis strategies, and research questions addressed during each trimester of the 2004-2005 school year. Over the course of the year, I took a concurrent triangulation strategy toward data collection (Creswell, 2003), gathering qualitative and quantitative data from multiple sources to find converging information and themes relative to my original research questions. Qualitative data that I gathered for this study included field notes from classroom observations, transcriptions of audiotaped open- ended and semi-structured interviews with focal students and conversations with Mr. 59 Connors, samples of written work - including book reports — completed by focal students, and lists of books selected by all students in both homerooms over the course of the school year. Quantitative data I gathered included aggregate and individual results from the Gates-MacGinitie Reading Test-given to all students at the conclusion of the prior school year, a reading attitude assessment (the Elementary Reading Attitude Survey), administered by Connors at the beginning and end of the school year, and aggregate and individual results from a word reading assessment administered by Connors during the third trimester of the school year. What follows in this section is an in-depth discussion of the data gathering and analysis procedures employed in this study, organized chronologically. Data collection for this study began with a series of in-depth conversations with Ed Connors. I spoke with Connors three times during the 2003 -04 school year to gather specific information about how he structured his language arts curriculum around students’ book choices and his justifications for the different activities he included in that curriculum These conversations were informal and open-ended, and were not audiotaped. Field notes were taken and read back to Connors at the time of the conversation for confirmation and clarification. During the 2004-05 school year, three additional conversations were conducted with Connors - one each trimester — focusing on his assessment of students’ progress over the course of the preceding trimester, planned activities for the following trimester, and his background as a reader. These conversations were, like their predecessors, informal and not audiotaped. Field notes taken during these conversations were also shared with Connors for confirmation and clarification. At the start of the first trimester of the 2004-05 school year, all students in 60 Connors’ language arts classes2 were administered the Elementary Reading Attitude Survey (ERAS; McKenna & Kear, 1990), a 20-item, 4-level Likert-type assessment in which respondents circle a drawing of the cartoon character Garfield that most closely approximates their feelings about each item. Ten items on the survey assess respondents' attitudes toward recreational reading (sample items: “How do you feel about spending free time reading?” “How do you feel about reading for fun at home?”), and ten items assess respondents' attitudes toward academic reading (sample items: “How do you feel about the stories you read in reading class?” “How do you feel when it’s time for reading class?”). For sixth graders, McKenna and Kear (1990) report a Cronbach’s alpha of 0.87 for the recreational subscale, 0.81 for the academic subscale, and 0.89 for the full scale, indicating that the ERAS is a highly reliable measure of reading attitude across these two dimensions. Norms for the ERAS were established by a national norming project involving 18,138 students grades 1-6 (including 2,442 sixth graders). For sixth grade, the normed means are as follows: for the recreational subscale, 27.9 (SD = 6.2), for the academic subscale, 24.7 (SD = 5.8) and for the full scale, 52.5 (SD = 10.6). Connors administered the ERAS as part of his routine instruction in order to gather data about his students’ reading habits and attitudes. Scores were calculated and individual results were placed in each student’s language arts portfolio to be shared with parents during conferences later in the school year. Individual and aggregate results from the ERAS were provided to the researcher as part of the classes’ archival data. After the ERAS was administered early in the first trimester of the 2004-2005 school year, I gave a brief presentation to both of Connors’ language arts classes (as well 2 Due to absences, only 41 of the 48 students completed the ERAS at the start of the 2004-2005 school year, and Mr. Connors did not conduct a make-up administration for the absent students. 61 as to a group of parents who came to the first Parent Night of the school year) to solicit volunteers for further interviewing, distributing informed consent and assent letters. Sixteen students (6 boys, 10 girls) responded to the original call for volunteers, returning signed consent/assent forms midway through the first trimester. Each volunteer was then individually administered the interview protocol of the Motivation to Read Profile (MRP; Gambrell, Palmer, Codling & Mazzoni, 1996) at the start of the second trimester. In the MRP interview, participants discuss particular texts and authors that most interest them, as well as give a richer account of their reading habits (sample items: “Tell me about the most interesting story or book you have read this week.” “What are some things that get you really excited about reading books?”). All interviews except one (at the student’s and parents’ request) were audiotaped and then transcribed. At the conclusion of the standard MRP questions, I asked each student to list texts that they had read to fulfill the independent reading component of the language arts curriculum during the first trimester. As Connors had not maintained records of the independent reading selections made by his students during the first trimester, I had to rely on students’ recollections of the texts they selected for this component of the language arts curriculum, which is one limitation on the comprehensiveness of the dataset’s representation of text selections made by focal students for academic purposes. Midway through the second trimester, Connors identified five additional boys and one additional girl whose reading habits he felt might be of interest in this study and who had expressed a belated interest in participating. These students were immediately given another copy of the informed consent and assent letters, which were all returned within a week. As with the first group of focal student volunteers, I administered the MRP 62 interview protocol to this second wave of volunteers, and all interviews but two (at the student’s and parents’ request) were audiotaped and transcribed. These students were also asked to list texts that they remembered reading to fulfill the independent reading component of the language arts curriculum during the first trimester. All student interviews were conducted during specials periods (i.e., PE, art), which were usually scheduled between Connors’ language arts sessions, as well as during language arts sessions that were not devoted to reading activities. Students were pulled out from the classroom and interviewed in the school library and the school conference room when the library was not available. All focal students were asked to generate their own pseudonyms for this study. Classroom observations began near the start of the second trimester and continued through the end of the school year. I visited Connors’ class two to three times per week in this period, arriving for each observation at the start of the school day in order to observe Connors’ homeroom students during DEAR time and stayed until the lunch break. This afforded me the opportunity to observe regularly scheduled DEAR sessions, both language arts class periods taught by Connors (to his and Price’s homerooms), read- alouds conducted by Connors with his homeroom, and any additional, impromptu DEAR time incorporated into the morning’s activities. Field notes were taken during all observed sessions, focusing primarily on documenting the texts selected by individual students (both focal and non-focal) for DEAR and reading class activities. Students’ behaviors with their selected texts were recorded in field notes, as were scripts of teacher behaviors during all reading activities. Text selections made by all students in the Genre Project were documented by examining the notebook containing all book reports students 63 had submitted, which Connors made available for students to peruse as they sought recommendations for future reading. Text selections made by all students in the Book Project were documented first by observing students during their initial library visits to select books for the project and second by observing the student presentations at the end of the third trimester. A second wave of interviews with focal students was conducted late in the second trimester after the Genre Project had concluded and extending into the start of the third trimester. These interviews were focused on the texts that the students had selected for the Genre Project and that they had brought to class for the culminating activity on visualization skills (see pp. 51-52). Focal students were first asked to list the texts that they had selected for the Genre Project, both by title and genre (copies of the book reports that students had submitted were supplied by Mr. Connors to help spur students’ memories if they were unable to remember all of the titles they had read). For each text that they had read, focal students were asked why they chose that particular text for this assignment, how and where they obtained the text, what the text was about, and what their feelings about the book were. The same questions were asked regarding the texts selected for the culminating activity on visualization skills. Focal students were then asked to identify their most and least favorite texts from those they selected for the Genre Project, as well as their most and least favorite genres of the five included in the project. Finally, focal students were asked to identify any other texts they had been engaging with outside of school and to describe them briefly. All but two of these interviews (at the students’ and parents’ request) were audiotaped and transcribed verbatim soon thereafter. Six focal students (3 boys, 3 girls) volunteered for a third and final round of 64 inter obsei 2006 indit local The brou their Scht obse lVCl't' eng: sele line that Am interviews that were scheduled late in the third trimester and modeled on the bookstore observation protocols developed by the author in a previous study (Cavazos-Kottke, 2006). For this interview, I met with three of the focal students (2 boys, 1 girl) individually and two of the focal students (2 girls) together at Schuler Books, a large, locally-owned independent bookstore. Each student was given a shopping basket and was told to fill the basket with reading materials that he or she would be interested in reading. The students were given unlimited time to complete this task, and while each student browsed, field notes were taken, focusing on the sections in which students conducted their browsing. Because of transportation issues, one boy who volunteered for the third interview was unable to meet at Schuler Books, but was able to meet at the semi-annual Scholastic Bookfair that was held at Harwell near the end of the third trimester. The same observation and interview protocols as used with the focal students at Schuler Books were followed with the focal student at the Bookfair. Each student was then interviewed individually (except for the two girls who engaged in their browsing together) about the selections he or she made. For each selection the focal student made, he or she was asked the following questions: 1. Describe this item in your own words. 2. Why did you select this item? 3. What is it about this item that interests you? Finally, each participant was asked to make a list of texts (either specific titles or genres) that he would have liked to include in the basket but could not find in the bookstore. Answers to the questions were taken down in field notes by the interviewer as these interviews were not audiotaped. Each participant received a $10 gift card to the bookstore 65 as comrl produc] constatr interyii categoi data. C to doc datab: earlie indiy as compensation for participation in this third interview. Data Analysis Qualitative data (including field notes, interview transcripts and written work produced by focal students relating to their text selections) were analyzed using the constant-comparative method (Strauss & Corbin, 1998) to discover themes within interview and transcript data. Individual documents were read multiple times and categories were constructed from concepts and unifying themes that emerged from the data. On the first reading, transcripts, field notes and student-produced artifacts were read to document titles of texts that students read for academic and vernacular purposes, and a database of text selections was then constructed using Microsoft Access. As discussed earlier, the text selection is the basic unit of analysis for this study, and is defined as an individual text selected by an individual student for a particular academic or vemacular purpose. Thus, a particular book chosen by only one student to fulfill the requirements of a project qualifies as a single text selection, whereas a single book chosen by three different students for various purposes qualifies as three text selections. Fields for title, author, and student number were created and filled in for each text selection upon initial documentation from the field notes. In some cases (most especially among, but not limited to, biographies selected in the Genre Project), precise title and author information about particular text selections was not provided. In these cases, as much information as possible about the text as could be gleaned from the field notes and student artifacts was input to the database, while “N/A” was input to any field for which precise documentation was impossible. To generate more information about the texts selected by students in this study, all text selections in the database were further analyzed recursively, 66 iterating salient a behey result o of test i hecrea‘ Project catego either on yit iterating between raw data and emergent categories until no new categories became salient and no texts remained unaccounted for by the set of categories that emerged, and the key properties of these categories were then formalized as fields in the database. As a result of this analysis, nine additional fields were created. The first new field created was Context of Text Selection. Categories for context of text selection were the following: Self-report Independent Reading, Self-report Recreational, Self-report Interest, DEAR Shared, DEAR Solitary, Genre Project, Book Project, Visualization Text, Book Fair/Bookstore and Multiple Contexts. This last category was developed to account for the few cases of texts that students selected for either the Genre or Book Projects that were also brought in for the culminating activity on visualization skills. The next field created was Medium. Categories in this field were the following: Book, Magazine, Game, Movie/Video, TV Show, Short Story and Consumer Product. An associated field was created to indicate whether or not the text was part of a series of texts. This field contained just two categories — Series or Not Series — and was reserved for texts with the Medium classifications of Book, Game, MovieNideo and Short Story, because within these media, texts were selected that satisfied both categories. Because they are periodical in nature, texts classified as Magazine and TV Show (at least all particular texts selected by students in this study from these media) are inherently series, and thus did not need to be distinguished from non-series texts with the same Medium classification. The next field created was Year of Publication/Production. For this field, the year in which the text first appeared was documented for the media of Book, MovieNideo and 67 Short 51 that we] TV Shc For the selectiq ClOCllIllt Source books ‘ library physic consul Websit levels ar e fir- thEir \ ClasSi Detiog bOok [’3th_ fOCus Short Story. For the medium of Magazine, the current year was used, as all magazines that were documented among the text selections were current issues. For the medium of TV Show, the year in which the program first premiered on television was documented. For the medium of Consumer Product, N/A was documented in the database. For any text selections for which an exact year of publication or production was impossible to document due to incomplete information, N/A was input to the database in this field. Sources used to determine year of publication or production were the copyright pages of books when available, and Amazon.com, iBistro (Homestead Public School’s online library catalog), and the Internet Movie Database (imdb.com) for texts that were not physically available for inspection. Conflicts between sources were resolved by consulting additional information sources on the web, such as publishers’ and authors’ websites, other libraries’ online catalogs, and Wikipedia. Two fields were created to document readability levels of the texts. Readability levels were only sought for texts with the Medium classification of Book, as these texts are first and foremost print-based and all existing readability calculation methods can by their very nature only be run on print-based texts. Readability was not sought for texts classified as Magazine, despite the fact that they are print-based, because by their periodical nature, readability may vary both within and between issues. In this regard, books are in some sense more fixed in their readability than magazines or other periodical texts. Multiple readability scores were sought because individual calculation methods focus on different elements of language in a text, and having multiple scores can give a fuller appreciation of the linguistic complexity of a given text than any single score can give. Readability levels were first obtained using Amazon.com, which reports Fog and 68 esur calcr nunt level loge levej The “on ther 3Cl'0 cour Ieirt to C0: Flesch-Kincaid readability scores for many of its products. From those text selections for which Amazon.com did not report readability scores, 66 were selected to be calculated manually. To calculate readability levels using the F lesch-Kincaid formula, three 200- word samples from different parts of a text were selected. The average sentence length (L) was calculated for each sample by dividing the number of words in the sample (200) by the number of sentences in the sample. Where necessary, the number of sentences was estimated to the nearest tenth. The average number of syllables per word (N) was calculated for each sample by dividing the number of syllables in the sample by the number of words (200). The readability level for the sample (expressed in terms of grade level) was then calculated for each sample using the following formula: (LX039) + (N X1 1 .8) - 15.59. The readability levels for the three samples were then averaged together to obtain the Flesch-Kincaid score for the whole text. To calculate readability levels using the Fog readability test, three 100-word samples from a text were selected. The average sentence length was calculated for each sample by dividing the number of words in the sample (100) by the number of sentences in the sample. Where necessary, the number of sentences was estimated to the nearest tenth. The average sentence length across samples (L) was then obtained by averaging together the figures obtained for each sample. Next, in each sample, the number of words with three or more syllables was counted, and these figures were averaged together to yield N. The readability level for the text (expressed in terms of grade level) was then calculated using the following formula: (L + N) x 0.4. Mean readability levels of texts selected by boys and girls for vernacular and academic contexts on both readability measures were calculated, and t-tests were run to compare means. 69 vvere g the int: follow For ter catego first f Pictur catege The c MFA Shovv 4 "\X}, Patti] eVer The final three fields that were created to code each text selection in the database were generated to capture different aspects of genre. The first of these fields documented the intended audience of the text. For texts with the Medium classification of Book, the following categories emerged for Genre-Audience: Young Adult and General Readers. For texts with the Medium classification of TV Show or Movie/Video, the following categories for Genre-Audience were used: G, PG, PG-l 3, R and General Viewers. The first four categories correspond to the movie ratings classification system of the Motion Picture Association of America, and texts categorized by Medium as Movie/Video were categorized by Genre-Audience in accordance with their MPAA ratings, when available.3 The category of General Viewers was used for all Movie/Video texts that did not carry MPAA ratings (such as direct-to-video programs) as well as for all texts classified as TV Show. TV Show texts were assigned to this category rather than using the ratings classification system promoted by the Academy of Television Arts and Sciences because TV ratings are assigned to individual episodes of television series based upon the content of particular programs, rather than assigning general ratings to whole series. Because the TV Show text selections documented in this study referred to favorite series watched by students, it was not possible to categorize TV Show selections by individual episode ratings. Finally, for text selections categorized by Medium as Game, the following categories for Genre-Audience were used: T-Teen, M-Mature, and E-Everyone. These ratings correspond to those assigned to video games by the ESRB. All games documented in this study were video games, and none of the video games documented among the text 3While some readers may be concerned that any MovieNideo texts selected by sixth graders carried R ratings, it should be noted that no MovieNideo text selections made by students in this study carried the even more restrictive MPAA rating of NC-l7. 70 selectio intheC selectir Fiction by Me. vvere c studyi progra categr categt as Spt. 0f Lil; Ante. HiSt. selections carried ratings of Y-Youth or A-Adult. Thus, those ratings were not included in the Genre-Audience categorization scheme. The second field dedicated to Genre was used to indicate whether the text selection was fiction or non-fiction. The following categories for Genre- Fiction/NonFiction emerged: Fiction, Non-Fiction, Mixed and N/A. All texts classified by Medium as Book were categorized either as Fiction or Non-Fiction, all Movies/Videos were categorized as Fiction (no documentaries were selected by any students in this study) and all Games were categorized as N/A. TV Shows billing themselves as “reality programs” were categorized as Non-Fiction, while all others (except one) were categorized as Fiction. The sole exception was WWE Smackdown!, which was categorized as Mixed because it explicitly bills itself as “sports entertainment” rather than as sports programming, carving a middle ground between unscripted sports competition and scripted drama. Most Magazines were categorized as Non-Fiction, with the exception of those such as American Girl that regularly feature works of short fiction in addition to information articles. Such texts were categorized as Mixed. The third and final field dedicated to Genre was used to capture the subject matter of the text selections. The following categories emerged in the analysis: Action, Automotive, Animals, Comic Book, Comedy, Fantasy, Gaming, Science Fiction, Historical Fiction, Biography, Mystery/Thriller, Realistic Fiction, Sports, Multiple Genres, Inspirational, Poetry, History, General Non-fiction, Reference, Popular Culture, and Reality/Simulation. Unlike the two previous sets of genre categories, categorizations by Genre-Subj ect Matter were not cleanly associated with particular Medium classifications. 7l £;W,..l‘ selectio | title amp of faCIt' _ | Public. | and Ge used It fields ‘ ICC Ul'S I studen selecti Studeri 20023' analyp .. .l .‘leldi | regar. lEXLS Cale: Salle; fI‘Om To summarize the coding procedures for text selections in this study, all text selections were initially documented in a database and categorized according to author, title and student number. Further iterative analysis of the text selections yielded a number of factors by which each text selection could be further categorized: Medium, Year of Publication/Production, Series/Not Series, Genre-Audience, Genre-Fiction/Non-fiction and Genre-Subj ect Matter. The Query and Report functions of Microsoft Access were used to generate a series of reports in which text selections were grouped by each of the fields used to categorize each item in the dataset, and these reports were analyzed recursively to discover patterns in the text selections made by the whole group of students, between subgroups of students (i.e., differences between boys’ and girls’ selections in different contexts), and within the sets of text selections made by individual students over the course of the school year in a process of cross case synthesis (Yin, 2002) Interview transcripts, artifacts of student work and field notes were further analyzed using the constant comparative method (Strauss & Corbin, 1998) beyond their yielding of information to contribute to the text selection database. Emerging themes regarding the reasons why particular texts appealed to students and the functions of those texts within the students’ academic and vernacular literacy lives were identified and categorized as these documents were analyzed recursively until no new themes became salient. ERAS scores and achievement data were used to triangulate emerging themes from the qualitative analysis, to gauge students' attitudes, abilities and changes in both over time, and to group student data for qualitative analysis. Simple t-tests were run on 72 ‘- «Ma-44" the fall anuude focalar reading set of t unit. fr school nextla “inch gener Studen literac Finallj classr COmei IWQ F trirne fOCal [0 the SUPE .’ the fall and spring ERAS data to determine the extent to which differences in reading attitude for recreational and academic purposes existed between boys and girls, between focal and non-focal students and between homerooms, as well as the extent to which reading attitudes differed between fall and spring administrations of the ERAS. This case study used multiple, nested units of analyses. At the highest level, the set of text selections made by all students in Connors’ language arts classes formed a unit, fi'om which general patterns of students' reading selections over the course of a school year, and mean levels of reading ability and attitude could be determined. The next level of analysis was on the boys in Mr. Connors' classes, to determine the extent to which boys' literacy practices within the classroom differed from girls' as well as from general trends for the class as a whole. The text selections made by individual focal students constituted the next unit of analysis, to develop rich descriptions of individual literacy practices as exemplars of (or contrasts to) more general trends seen in the class. Finally, the smallest unit of analysis was the individual text selection. Role of the Researcher As discussed previously, my role in the classroom was primarily as an observer of classroom activities and interviewer of individual students pulled from the classroom context during the second and third trimesters of the 2004-2005 school year. I made only two presentations to Connors’ classes over the course of the year. Early in the first trimester, I introduced the goals of my study and solicited volunteers to participate as focal students. Near the start of the third trimester, I donated three books by Eoin Colfer to the classroom library (Artemis Fowl, Artemis Fowl and the Arctic Incident and The Supernaturalist) and was asked by Connors to speak briefly about each of the books as a 73 mode quali: Conn learni entire atno CODY model for the kinds of book talks he wanted students to engage in. This author talk qualified me for an Extra Effort Award, a good citizenship honor system by which Connors and his students would recognize positive contributions made by students to the learning environment. Beyond these two occasions, my interactions with students were limited almost entirely to scheduled, semi-structured interviews with focal students and occasional informal chats with individual focal students during free time or transitions between class sessions. During the library visitations to select texts for the Book Project, Connors encouraged students to solicit my opinion on particular titles they were considering; however, such consultation was limited to conversations with focal students and a couple of general suggestions in response to Connors’ inquiries regarding ideas for titles on particular subjects (e. g., a good baseball book). While I spoke individually with Connors during all of my visits to his classroom, at no time did I offer unsolicited commentary on classroom proceedings. My conversations with Connors were mainly of the informal interview variety, with me asking him questions about what would be occurring in class on a given day, how successfully he felt particular activities turned out, and how he saw his background as a reader influencing his beliefs about reading instruction. On a couple of occasions, Connors asked me to offer an opinion on ideas for classroom activities, but never did I actively engage with Connors in detailed conversations geared toward developing future lesson plans or classroom activities, nor did I ever attempt to persuade Connors toward or dissuade him from a particular course of action in his classroom. 74 and ve sought aria: CHAPTER 4 Results Introduction This study was designed to investigate four research questions about the academic and vernacular literacy practices of a class of middle school boys. Specifically, this study sought to answer: 1. What are the vernacular literacy practices of a class of middle school boys and what kinds of texts do these boys engage with in those practices? 2. How and to what extent do these boys' vernacular literacies become visible within a language arts program structured around students' self-selected texts? 3. How and to what extent do the vernacular and academic literacy practices of these boys differ from those of the girls within the same class? and 4. What attitudes toward vernacular and academic literacy practices do these boys hold, how do they compare with the attitudes of other students, and how do those attitudes change over the course of an academic year in a book club based classroom? Because literacy practices are not directly observable, but rather are implied from literacy events, before directly answering these research questions it is necessary to provide some definition of the literacy events upon which this study’s results are based. Over the course of the 2004-2005 school year, the 48 students in Mr. Connors’ language arts classes made a grand total of 672 documented text selections across academic and vernacular literacy contexts, and as detailed in Chapter 3, each text selection was analyzed through a number of lenses, including medium, genre and readability. A full list 75 OlICX' byfiu texts: docur repres these nude Read senes ”hat knnh enga fort cont of texts for which complete author and title information was available that were selected by students in this study is given in Appendix C. Table A3 summarizes the numbers of text selections made by boys and girls for each of the academic and vernacular contexts documented in this study. Totals for all academic contexts except Independent Reading represent text selections made by all 48 of Connors’ students, and as noted in Chapter 3, these were documented through direct observation of classroom behaviors and analysis of students’ written work. Totals for all vernacular contexts as well as for Independent Reading4 represent text selections made by the 22 focal students who volunteered for a series of one-on-one interviews, during which these selections were documented. Question One What are the vernacular literacy practices of a class of middle school boys and what kinds of texts do these boys engage with in those practices? The vernacular literacy practices in which the boys who participated in this study engaged can best be described first by considering the particular texts that boys selected for vernacular purposes and secondly by considering the social and psychological contexts in which boys engaged with those texts. Texts. In their vernacular literacy practices, there were few individual titles or authors around which boys’ text selections congregated very strongly. Four authors, Jonathan Rand, Matt Christopher, Gordon Korman and Jude Watson, appear in the dataset exclusively among boys’ text selections. Of these four, only Rand and Christopher were named as favorite authors exclusively by boys in follow-up interviews 4Because Mr. Connors did not maintain a record of the titles read by students for Independent Reading credit during the first trimester, text selections coded as Independent Reading are based entirely on self- reporting from focal students during the first round of interviews. None of these text selections are attributable to non-focal students. 76 with ft selecti select elever comp the er stnki texts scho: l or m selec “(hi the 1 20 s free SIUI \l”:~ me. Ill; with focal students, and only Rand and Christopher’s works appeared among the text selections of more than five boys. However, only Rand’s books appear among the text selections made by boys for vernacular purposes in significant numbers; only one of the eleven text selections by Christopher was made for recreational reading purposes, compared with ten of the 33 Rand selections. The overall diversity of titles and authors in the entire dataset of text selections, regardless of gender or context, is one of the most striking findings of this study. As can be clearly seen in Table A4, which lists all of the texts that were selected by five or more students for any purpose over the course of the school year, and Table A5, which lists all of the authors whose works were selected five or more times, while a few titles and authors were represented multiple times in the text selections, no single title or author appeared in a majority of the students’ text selections. While Lemony Snicket was represented in the dataset by 72 text selections, making him the most frequently selected author, his works appear among the text selections of only 20 students, less than half of the sample of 48 students. Jonathan Rand, the second most frequently selected author with 33 text selections, appears among the text selections of 15 students, although unlike Snicket, Rand’s works were selected exclusively by boys. While this represents less than a third of the whole set of students, it does constitute a majority of the boys, indicating that Rand’s popularity among boys was more widespread than any other author’s popularity among the whole group. Furthermore, the majority of the text selections made fiom the top 15 most selected authors, with the exceptions of J K Rowling and Electronic Arts, were made for academic purposes, indicating that academic text selections were slightly more likely to congregate around a set of popular authors than were vernacular text selections. Looking at the dataset through the lens of which 77 titles even than popu selec illust Mic/4 Chri Chn feat We] the Cir les M: :17; 0f [\r titles are most represented among students’ text selections (Table A4) demonstrates an even more extreme level of diversity, as the most popular titles were selected by no more than seven students. However, this diversity should not be mistaken for randomness, and in the relative popularity of the works of Jonathan Rand and Matt Christopher among boys’ text selections, a number of clear trends in boys’ vernacular literacy practices can be illustrated. For one, both are primarily authors of series fiction, Jonathan Rand for his Michigan Chillers and American Chillers series of supernatural thrillers, and Matt Christopher for his prolific output of sports fiction novels. While it might be argued that Christopher is not technically a writer of series fiction to the extent that his novels do not feature recurring characters or storylines that extend from book to book, he is nonetheless well known enough in the young adult sports fiction market that his name alone carries the same connotations as the name of a series. Students in this study looked for “Matt Christopher books” in their library visits in the same way that they looked for “Lemony Snicket books” or “Chillers books.” The particular title that they ultimately sought was less important than the text’s affiliation with other closely related texts. In the case of Matt Christopher’s novels, the lack of an overarching series title, such as Goosebumps or American Chillers did not prevent students from regarding his individual books as elements of a larger corpus, effectively branding these books as a series under the banner of Matt Christopher. A preference for serial works among boys’ text selections extends beyond these two authors to become a defining characteristic of the boys’ vernacular literacy practices. All of the authors named by boys in one-on-one interviews as their favorites are primarily 78 writers of series fiction, and nearly all of the authors listed on Table A5 are represented in the database of text selections for their serial works, rather than for their stand-alone creations. In the overall set of 672 documented text selections, 391 (or approximately 58%) were coded as being part of a series, and all of the texts listed on Table A4 are serial publications.5 Among the 160 text selections made by boys for vernacular purposes, 102 (or nearly 64%) were serial in nature, and boys’ preference for series texts cut across all media documented in this study to include movie, television and video game series in addition to books. While fiction outnumbered non-fiction in the database of text selections by a factor of 3 to 1, a preference for series works was also prevalent in the non—fiction text selections made by boys for vernacular purposes, manifest in such series as the Eyewitness and Dorling-Kindersley information books, The Encyclopedia of Danger, and even “reality television” series such as American Chopper and The Apprentice. The works of Jonathan Rand and Matt Christopher continue to serve as useful lenses for understanding the vernacular literacy practices of the boys in this study when we consider additional trends in boys’ text selections such as audience and genre. Both Rand and Christopher write exclusively for children and young adult readers, and the vast majority of text selections made by students in this study across all media and across all contexts were created and marketed specifically for young adults and teens, with 5 The only possible exception to this claim among the texts listed in Table A4 is The Hobbit, as it was originally written by JRR Tolkien as a stand-alone volume that would serve as a prequel to his previously published epic, The Lord of the Rings, but it has since come to be regarded by many readers - including this author — as an introductory text more appropriately read prior to the three volumes of The Lord of the Rings than afterward. This was also how focal students interviewed in this study regarded The Hobbit, more like the first volume of a tetraology than like a separate volume to be read after working through the original trilogy. Nonetheless, wherever one regards the proper place of The Hobbit within Tolkien’s Middle Earth writings, there is no argument that it belongs within the corpus of said writings, and the centrality of the events, characters and setting of The Hobbit within the larger story of Middle Earth demonstrates that the novel is rightfully regarded as an exemplar of series fiction. 79 comparatively few selections made from texts intended for older audiences. Of the 648 text selections that were classifiable by Audience, 527 (over 78% of the total set of text selections) were coded as YA, or Young Adult, while another 64 text selections were coded as General Readers. None were Written for an exclusively adult audience. Movies/Videos were categorized using the MPAA rating system, which yielded 11 PG- rated MoviesNideos, 12 rated PG-l3, and 3 straight-to-video DVDs that did not carry official MPAA ratings but were marketed for General Viewers. Games were categorized using the ESRB rating system, which yielded 11 Games rated T-Teen, 7 rated E- Everyone, 2 rated M-Mature, and one (RuneQuest) for which an official rating was unavailable as it was developed prior to the advent of the ESRB rating system. As discussed in the previous chapter, all TV Shows were coded by Genre-Audience as General Viewers. While these numbers represent the distribution of texts by intended audience for the whole dataset, young adult oriented texts carry as high a profile among the texts selected by boys for vernacular purposes as they do among the texts selected by the whole group. This finding is supported by the lists of most popular books and authors given in Tables A4 and A5. Only one item on Table A4 — Sports Illustrated — was not published explicitly with the young adult market in mind, although it is by no means an inappropriate text for this age group. Similarly, among the authors and publishers listed on Table A5, only Time Inc. (publisher of Sports Illustrated) and Electronic Arts (producer of video games) produced texts that were selected by students in this study that fell outside of the young adult niche market, yet the texts they produced were created for mass appeal to include that market as well as older consumers. For example, all of the 80 Electronic Arts games selected by students in this study were rated either T-Teen or E- Everyone. This finding is further corroborated by analyzing the readability levels of boys’ text selections for vernacular purposes, Where such data was available. Both Fog and Flesch-Kincaid readability scores were obtained for 60 text selections (all Books), while only F lesch-Kincaid scores were obtained for an additional 20 selections (again, all Books). The mean Fog readability level was 8.29 grade equivalents, with a range of 4 to 14.8, and a standard deviation of 1.98. The mean Flesch-Kincaid readability level was 6.11 grade equivalents, with a range of 2.9 to 12.9, and a standard deviation of 1.78. This discrepancy between readability levels as measured by different methods is to be expected because, as described in the previous chapter, each method focuses on a different aspect of a given text’s linguistic complexity. When considered together however, these figures indicate that the readability of the average text selection made by boys for vernacular purposes was closely equivalent to, if not considerably higher than, the students’ grade level. In terms of genre, boys expressed a strong preference in their vernacular literacy practices for works of imaginative fiction (an umbrella genre category that includes fantasy, science fiction and horror) and sports-themed fiction and non-fiction, the two genre categories with which the works of Jonathan Rand and Matt Christopher (respectively) are most closely identified. This is clearly demonstrated on Table A6, which presents the distribution of text selections made by focal students (the only students from whom text selections for vernacular purposes were documented) by gender, genre and academic or vernacular context. The first column on this table presents the 81 generic distribution of boys’ text selections for vernacular purposes, demonstrating a strong preference for fantasy above all other categories, followed by a cluster of five genres (action, mystery/thriller, realistic fiction, science fiction and sports) with nearly equal representation. Grouping to gether'the genres of mystery/thriller, fantasy and science fiction under the heading of imaginative fiction captures all of the most popular series from which boys selected texts for vernacular purposes, including Rand’s Michigan and American Chillers, Lemony Snicket’s A Series of Unfortunate Events, RL Stine’s Goosebumps and Fear Street, and a host of Star Wars-themed texts. Sports- themed texts, on the other hand, are not exclusively fiction. Not only does this category include a Matt Christopher novel, it also includes magazines such as Sports Illustrated and Slam, biographies of famous athletes, sports record books, sports video games, and video game reference guides. Further, the high profile of imaginative fiction and sports- themed texts among boys’ text selections for vernacular purposes is not due to the overrepresentation of a few focal boys’ voracious text consumption; rather, examples of both kinds of texts are spread throughout the vernacular text selections of nearly all of the focal boys. Finally, boys’ vemacular practices were generally marked by texts of quite recent vintage, and the works of Jonathan Rand and Matt Christopher represent convenient points on a continuum of text age that bracket nearly all of the texts selected by boys for vernacular purposes. The range of ages for texts selected by boys for vernacular purposes was 0 (indicating the text was published in 2005) to 68 years. The median age of a text selection was 5 years, and all of the Jonathan Rand titles that appear among boys’ text selections were published within five years of the period in which data was gathered. On 82 the other side of the median, text ages are more sparsely distributed between 6 and 68 years old (Tolkein’s The Hobbit being the oldest outlier), with a cluster of texts written between the late-19603 and mid-19803, a span of years that coincides with Matt Christopher’s most productive period. Figure B11 displays a frequency distribution graph of the number of text selections by age of text for all the text selections documented in this study across academic and vernacular purposes. Comparable to the subset of text selections made by boys for vernacular purposes, the mean age of a text selection in the whole dataset was 10.8 years, while the median was 5 years, the mode was 4 years, and the standard deviation was just under 15 years. As can be seen from a Figure B11, the distribution of text ages is extremely positively skewed, indicating a strong overall preference for texts published within the past five to ten years. Thus, the works of Jonathan Rand and Matt Christopher provide convenient rallying points for summarizing the findings with regard to the texts that comprise the physical landscape of the vernacular literacy practices of the boys who participated in this study. In their vernacular literacy lives, these boys gravitated toward works of imaginative fiction and sports-themed texts, in particular selecting materials created for and marketed toward a young adult audience. In general, the boys selected texts with reading levels closely approximating their grade level, and often slightly higher, and they were more apt to select texts written within the past half decade (with a few notable exceptions for 20th century classics). Lastly, the boys preferred texts that were part of a series, whether the series involved books, movies, video games or, as will be demonstrated in the next section, a combination of multiple media. Contexts. More critical to understanding boys’ vernacular literacy practices than 83 the particular texts with which boys engaged are the ways in which boys went about reading those texts, the stances they adopted toward them and the contexts within which their engagement with those texts occurred. Few individual texts were inherently interesting in and of themselves, and boys did not “just read,” selecting single texts one at a time and reading them in isolation. More compelling to the boys in this study were the connections that they could draw between diverse constellations of texts. As noted above, the boys were particularly drawn to serial works, but those serial interests were not constrained by medium. The boys displayed the strongest interest in books, magazines, movies, DVDs and games that could be connected not just to other texts, but to other texts from different media. They not only took great pleasure in following characters, plots and entire narrative worlds across multiple media outlets and delivery systems, their interactions with these multimedia textual constellations formed important building blocks of boys’ personal and social identity. More than intertextual, the vernacular literacy practices engaged in by the boys in this study can best be characterized as transmedial (Semali, 2002; Semali & Fueyo, 2001). When readers make intertextual connections between books, magazines, films, TV shows and other media texts, they typically note similarities and differences between characters, themes and plot points shared by two or more texts. In other words, they recognize points of comparison and contrast between texts. What transmediation represents, as distinct from an exercise in recognizing intertextual connections, is a synthesis of a new understanding — or “poem,” to borrow a term from Rosenblatt (1978) — that is not confined to a reader’s interaction with a single text. A transmedial understanding is a bricolage of ideas drawn from a set of narrative and informational 84 texts. Transmediating readers (or consumers, to better capture the breadth of print and non-print texts with which boys in this study engaged) adopt a stance toward texts that is neither exclusively aesthetic, imaginatively entering into and experiencing narrative worlds, nor exclusively efferent, extracting discrete bits of information from expository text. Rather, such readers iterate along a continuum between these stances, synthesizing the experiences of entering the visualized narrative worlds of some texts and digesting information gleaned from other texts to create an engaging personal, multitextual (and multimodal) diegesis. The ways in which Gatorade and Link, two focal boys who described themselves as “major Star Wars fans,” oriented themselves toward the texts that they consumed for vernacular purposes are illustrative of the transmedial literacy practices demonstrated by many of the boys in this study. For Gatorade and Link, the climax of the 2004-2005 school year came in mid- May with the theatrical release of Star Wars Episode III: Revenge of the Sith. In the months leading up to the film’s premiere, Gatorade and Link read books from the Jedi Apprentice and Boba F ett series that extend upon narrative threads suggested in the first two films of the trilogy, played the PlayStation 2 game Star Wars: Battlefront, which allows players to fight through battles both depicted and merely referred to in the Star Wars films, and watched Clone Wars on DVD, an animated series that covers events leading directly into the opening action of Episode 111. While each of these works could stand alone as a unique narrative experience, when taken together, these texts create a comprehensive portrait of the Star Wars universe that no single text provides on its own. Furthermore, while all of these texts derive from common source material, no two of these Star Wars-themed texts are straightforward adaptations of each other in the way 85 that a typical movie novelization is a transposition of a film’s screenplay into prose. What Gatorade and Link understand about the Star Wars universe is a synthesis of narrative information gleaned from a constellation of texts spanning multiple media, with each text contributing uniquely to their Star Wars knowledge base and worldview rather than recapitulating diegetic material consumed elsewhere. Even in the case of adaptations, the current fashion in transmedia storytelling is that each media incarnation of a core storyline offer a unique narrative experience, with various elements of plot and character development distributed exclusively among particular versions of the story such that a full understanding of the diegesis resides in a synthesis of transmedia narrative experiences (Jenkins, 2006). Link played the Episode III video game to its conclusion and read two different Episode III novelizations a week before the film’s release. While the game, novel and film share a core narrative arc and cast of characters, each contains certain plot points, action sequences and details on character relationships that the other versions do not offer, and as Jenkins (2006) notes in his analysis of The Matrix, these distinctions are not merely artifacts of the affordances and limitations of the particular media mustered into service in the transmedia enterprise, but reflect deliberate creative decisions on the part of the producers. While “each medium does what it does best,” a successful transmedia franchise survives by “pitching the content somewhat differently in the different media” such that “each work offers fresh experiences” (p. 148). Redundancy derails the transmedia experience, as both the broad and nuanced distinctions between transmedia texts become the substance of natural conversations. As Link engaged with each successive incarnation of the Episode [11 story, he would take advantage of transition points in the school day to breathlessly report 86 points of comparison and contrast between the texts to me (a confessed Star Wars fanatic and fellow consumer of these same texts), as well as offer his opinion on how certain aspects of the game or book should have been included in the movie and vice versa. Further examples of transmediation abound in the vernacular literacy practices of the boys in this study. After watching nightly episodes of The Simpsons with his father, B-Rad would often play a few missions in Simpsons Hit ‘n Run on the PlayStation, extending his enjoyment of the characters’ antics from a primarily receptive medium (television) to an interactive one (video gaming). Axel read the Amazing Spiderman comic book each month, then followed Spidennan’s adventures through the first two motion pictures directed by Sam Raimi, which are not wholesale adaptations of material from the comic books, but rather fresh takes on the Spiderman mythos that frequently play upon viewers’ familiarity with a large canon of pop cultural texts. Axel enjoyed each individual text in this converging array of media texts on its own terms, but making a commitment to follow the series within and across multiple media platforms provided Axel with an engaging narrative experience that no single text or medium could provide. One might be tempted to argue that what I am labeling transmediation here is less an authentic literacy practice than it is a case of adolescents performing a role of good consumer, at least from the perspective of the corporate producers of such media texts. Indeed, the stereotypes of the mindless consumer and the obsessed fan often merge in the popular imagination, especially in the case of fans of science fiction franchises (Jenkins, 1992). As fashion-conscious teenagers spend large amounts of money to pursue “a look” that is often more about displaying particular brand names than about comfort or practicality (Quart, 2004), science fiction and fantasy fans are ofien stereotyped as 87 uncritically omnivorous consumers of texts that bear the imprimatur of their favorite narrative fi'anchises, no matter how well or poorly crafted those texts may be (Alvermann & Hagood, 2000; Jenkins, 1992). However, such an interpretation denies a high degree of agency that readers, viewers and game players regularly bring to their text selections within such transmedial enterprises, an agency that was indeed seen among the boys in this study. While both Link and Gatorade were avid fans of Star Wars, neither was an uncritical consumer of all things Star Wars, nor was each boy’s constellation of transmedial text selections within the Star Wars realm identical. Link noted that while he enjoyed the games and the movies, the novels were his favorite medium, as he preferred “to make pictures in my head.” Link even pursued his Star Wars interests into non-fiction reading, selecting a George Lucas biography for the Genre Project, which he noted he would have gladly read “for fun” without the assignment. Gatorade, on the other hand, liked the novels the least, not because he disliked reading, but because in the books, “there’s not as much light saber fighting,” a key feature in his favorite Star Wars movies and games. Each boy thus carved out his own unique textual niche within the larger Star Wars mediascape (King & O’Brien, 2002), and while Link and Gatorade’s patterns of text selections were no doubt shared with millions of other young adolescent (and adult) Star Wars fans, it is important to note that each boy felt his fandom as an “individual, or special taste,” an “experience of identity” (Alvermann & Hagood, 2000, pp 444-445; also, Alverrnann, Huddleston & Hagood, 2004). That is, no matter how widely their interests in Star Wars were shared by other people, both Link and Gatorade referenced Star Wars texts as unique markers of their identity, not simply as texts they read, watched or played with, but rather as part of 88 who they are. Further evidence against the premise that these students’ transmedial allegiances were more artifacts of orchestrated corporate media campaigns than cases of authentic literacy practices comes from students’ relatively narrow allegiances within the Series of Unfortunate Events fi'anchise, indicating that not all transmedia enterprises are created (or received) equally. While Lemony Snicket’s novels were popular with many boys and girls in Connors’ classroom, the much-publicized movie version, starring Jim Carrey, was not. On a number of occasions during his read-alouds of The Bad Beginning, the first book in the series, Connors talked up the movie, which was released along with tie-in video games, fast food promotions and new book publications during the winter holiday break of the 2004-05 school year. However, among Connors’ students, there was no buzz about the movie once it was released, no discussion (formal or informal) that would indicate that anyone had even seen the movie, tried the videogame or otherwise consumed any tie-in product other than the original novels. Given that the movie grossed over $30 million its opening weekend, and would ultimately gross close to $120 million domestically, it is likely that at least a few of Connors’ students saw it in the theater during its original release, but while the books continued to be popular selections throughout the remainder of the school year, A Series of Unfortunate Events as an exercise in transmedia narrative did not capture these students’ imaginations. In his Book Project presentation on The Hostile Hospital (the eighth book in the series), Gatorade articulated a strong personal connection to Klaus, one of the series’ protagonists: “Klaus loves to read books and everything he reads he remembers. Klaus is like me because sometimes I get into a series of books and I remember what it’s about like Klaus, when 89 he remembers what’s in the books he reads once I started reading it, I couldn’t stop.” However, in an informal conversation after the study concluded, Gatorade admitted that while he had seen the film, he did not enjoy it very much, primarily because he did not feel the same personal connection with the cinematic depiction of Klaus as he did with his literary incarnation and because he felt the filmmakers took too many liberties with the books’ storyline in their attempt to make a single movie that covered the material of the first three books. In contrast to his transmedial experience with Star Wars, Gatorade preferred Lemony Snicket’s books over their movie adaptations, indicating at the very least a nascent critical perspective on his transmedial consumption, which provides some evidence against the argument that transmediation is simply a capitulation of “authentic” personal taste to a corporate-driven consumerist imperative. This is not to argue naively that adolescents’ tastes are immune to advertising and media hype; Gatorade, and thousands (if not millions) of adolescents did go see the film, after all. The point I wish to reinforce here is that through the transmedial text selections that many adolescents (in this study, almost exclusively boys) make in their vernacular literacy practices, adolescents experience and express a sense of personal identity, and that while that experience might be generally shared with masses of other adolescents, individuals act as agents of their own textual identity construction, purposefully aligning with particular texts and rejecting others within particular mediascapes. Given the overwhelming popularity of imaginative fiction among the focal boys’ text selections for vernacular purposes, it should not be surprising that the richest examples of these boys’ transmedial allegiances come from this realm. However, the role of transmediation at the intersection of these boys’ vernacular literacy practices and 90 social identity construction is most clearly seen among the boys in this study in a different mediascape: sports. For as much as Link and Gatorade situated their identities as readers in their consumption of imaginative fiction texts, these interests remained largely isolated to individual transmediation. A' number of other boys, focal and non-focal, were documented in literacy events with Star Wars texts, but on only one occasion was any sharing of these interests observed between boys.6 Indeed, during one DEAR session two non-focal boys sitting near each other were observed reading multiple books from the Jedi Apprentice series, yet at no point before, during or after the DEAR time did either boy acknowledge the other’s similar reading habits, and when not being read for DEAR, the books were kept hidden fi'om view, zipped up in their respective binders. Whether any of the non-focal boys aligned their textual identities with Star Wars to the extent that Link and Gatorade did, as well as whether any sharing of these interests took place in non-academic contexts, is unknown, but the fact remains that within the academic contexts documented in this study, transmediation in the Star Wars universe was visible almost exclusively as a private literacy practice. This is not the case with boys’ consumption of sports-related texts, particularly those dealing with basketball. Montel and Tiger, both focal boys, engaged with a variety of basketball-themed print and non-print texts across vernacular and academic literacy contexts. Moreover, these literacy events were frequently shared experiences, as Montel and Tiger would together pore over an issue of Slam! magazine, an informational book on the history of the Philadelphia ‘76ers, or Michael Jordan’s autobiography For the Love “The day afier the theatrical release of Star Wars Episode III: Revenge of the Sith, the following exchange was documented in the field notes: Connors asks the class if anyone has seen Star Wars. Non-focal boy: “Yeah, Star Wars was stupid.” Link throws down reading, “What!? People who say Star Wars is stupid are stupid!” 91 of the Game during DEAR time. On multiple occasions, other boys would join them for sometimes brief, sometimes extended periods. During the Genre Project, biographies about sports stars, including Michael Jordan and Magic Johnson, were circulated among multiple boys, and during the culminating activity on visualization skills, some boys brought multiple sports texts to share with other boys who had forgotten to bring their own sports texts from home. When Connors’ students made a class trip to the library to select books for the Book Project, some boys (such as PB, Gatorade and Chipmunk) selected works of imaginative fiction, which they either immediately began reading by themselves in a quiet part of the library or set aside to read later, using the remainder of the library time to chat with friends. Tiger, Montel, Axel, B-Rad and two non-focal boys, on the other hand, all selected Matt Christopher sports fiction novels, then sat together at a table and passed the books around, commenting on which ones they preferred to read for the project. During the oral presentations of Book Project posters, Connors allowed time after each student’s presentation for discussion, time that was usually taken up by brief comments (e. g., “Good”) or stock questions (e. g., “Did you like it?”) from students, or brief discourses on the selected books by Connors himself (e.g., after a presentation on Walter Dean Myers’ Scorpions, Connors offered recommendations for other novels by Myers). However, after presentations on works of sports fiction, student-generated discussion (led by boys) was much more animated, with references to shared athletic experiences that connected with the book being discussed. For example, afier Montel’s presentation on Johnny Long Legs, a Matt Christopher novel in which the protagonist carries the eponymous nickname, multiple boys recalled similar nicknames given to Monte], an unusually tall and slender boy, by his teammates. After a non-focal student’s 92 presentation on a football-themed novel, a number of boys made lighthearted comments about the on-field performance of the presenter during the previous football season, in contrast to the heroics ascribed to the novel’s protagonist. These observations illustrate ways in which literacy events with particular sports texts constituted social practices among the boys in Connors’ classroom, but the mediascape in which those practices were situated encompassed a much broader array of text forms than these print media examples. The dataset on which this study is based almost certainly underrepresents the number of non-print texts with which focal students engaged in their vernacular literacy practices. This is especially true in the realm of sports, where live and broadcast sporting events were excluded from analysis as were televised sports information programs such as ESPN SportsCenter. Field notes document several informal conversations between boys about professional sports games, especially during the NCAA and NBA basketball tournaments (which took place during the final trimester of the school year), clearly indicating that boys not only engaged with more non-print sports texts than the dataset would indicate, but that these texts also played a central role in forging and maintaining boys’ social identities. Despite the exclusion of an incalculable number of such texts, several non-print sports texts did nevertheless make it into the dataset, including DVDs (e.g., Friday Night Lights, Remember the Titans, Rocky) and videogarnes (e.g., NBA Street 3, ESPN 2K5) either brought to class for the culminating activity on visualization skills or self-reported by focal students as part of their recreational media consumption. Expanding the definition of media text even further to embrace all of the “extensions of man” (McLuhan, 1966) by which students conveyed information about 93 their professional and collegiate team allegiances, boys in particular were likely not only to wear clothing bearing the colors and logos of both of Michigan’s Big 10 schools, but also to comment on each other’s fashion choices in this regard, especially when pairs of students would wear the apparel of rival teams. Also popular among the boys in Connors’ classroom were “baller bands,” rubber wristbands bearing inspirational messages and/or team colors and logos. Boys would wear these bands prominently, collect them, swap them and frequently share stories about where new and unique bands could be purchased. While some boys, particularly Link, would also express interests in the Star Wars and Nintendo mediascapes through t-shirts and other apparel, these fashion choices did not inspire conversation among boys in the way that sports-themed apparel did. What these boys’ use of a broad range of sports-themed texts illustrates is a strong nexus of literacy practices and identity construction. Crawford (2004) notes that “material objects need to be understood as social agents” (p. 120), and it is through this semiotic process of “perforrnative consumption” that individuals appropriate, interpret and manipulate a variety of verbal and non-verbal sign systems to align their social identities with those of groups or other individuals with whom they would like to be affiliated. Gee (2005) labels mediascapes that afford such purposes “affinity spaces” (p. 214), multitextual (and multimodal) environments through which individuals forge social alliances based on common interests and a shared grammar of signs, symbols and Discourses. Fan communities that build up around such transmedial enterprises as Star Wars, Japanese anime and manga, ABC’s Lost and the Harry Potter novels and films are all prototypical examples of affinity spaces (see Carey, 2005; Jenkins, 1992, 2006), but so too are those that arise around sports leagues like the NBA and particular teams like 94 the Boston Red Sox (see O’Nan & King, 2005). Fine (1987) notes that for middle school age children, a key “imperative of development” (p. 105) is the grth and expansion of social competence across multiple social worlds, which is essential to achieving a sense of industry, the critical psychosocial milestone for young adolescents according to Erikson. Fine concludes from his ethnography of boys’ Little League teams that sports participation at this age serves as a convenient (although not always positive) crucible for such peer alignment and socialization, an idea echoed widely in the popular literature on boys’ and young men’s athletics (Griffin, 1998; Messner, 1992; Whitson, 1990). Less discussed in the literature is how sports fandom — or in Gee’s terms, participation within a sports-based affinity space — at this age might serve similar purposes. One conclusion that might be drawn from this study is that it may very well, as the most socially vibrant literacy events in which some focal boys engaged revolved around transmediation in a sports mediascape. It should be noted that sports mediascapes did not constitute the only affinity spaces in which boys engaged in identity construction through transmediation, as the examples of Link and Gatorade with their Star Wars passions illustrate. However, the affinity spaces that arose around basketball and other sports were the most socially active ones to become visible within Connors’ permeable classroom. Question Two How and to what extent do these boys' vernacular literacies become visible within a language arts program structured around students' self-selected texts? The second research question was included to assess whether the preferences expressed among students’ text selections across the contexts documented in this study authentically reflect their abiding personal interests. That is, when given the opportunity 95 to select personally interesting reading materials for academic literacy tasks, do students bring texts that indeed reflect their personal interests or do they in some sense “play school,” making text selections and engaging in literacy practices that conform to prior expectations for what counts as appropriate reading for school or that achieve some other aim apart from exploring personal interests? As the discussion on the previous question suggests, many aspects of the boys’ vernacular literacy practices did become visible in Ed Connors’ language arts program. While many of the interactions over sports-themed texts described above were documented during transition periods in the school day, and thus outside of the curricular space managed by Connors, text selections that boys made for various Language Arts activities nonetheless did resemble the kinds of text selections that boys made for recreational literacy pursuits. As with the first question, comparisons between the boys’ vernacular and academic literacy practices can be usefully made by considering both the kinds of texts that boys selected for academic literacy tasks and the extent to which the permeable curriculum set up by Connors afforded the social and psychological contexts that defined boys’ engagement with those texts in their vernacular literacy lives. Texts. In terms of genre, boys in this study did for the most part take advantage of Connors’ permeable classroom structure to make visible the texts that constitute the physical landscape of their vernacular literacy practices. As the first two columns of Table A6 demonstrate, while boys selected texts from a wider range of genres for vernacular purposes than for academic purposes, the most popular genres among the text selections made for vernacular purposes closely correspond to the most popular genres among text selections made for academic purposes. Fantasy and the other imaginative 96 fiction genres were popular in both academic and vernacular text selections, as were sports themed texts (some of which are included under the realistic fiction heading). There are three notable exceptions to this general conclusion: biographies and historical fiction were represented more strongly among boys’ text selections for academic purposes relative to those made for vernacular purposes; the action genre was totally absent among boys’ text selections for academic purposes; and fantasy had a considerably stronger presence among boys’ vernacular text selections. The first exception is attributable to the fact that biographies and hiStorical fiction were two of the five genres in the Genre Project. We would expect boys to make selections fi'om these genres for academic purposes because they were assigned to do so. Had these genres not been included in the Genre Project, we might expect that biographies and historical fiction would appear in lower numbers, comparable to their presence among boys’ text selections for vernacular purposes. The second exception is attributable to the fact that all of the text selections from the action genre were movies or DVDs, a medium with extremely limited representation among text selections for academic purposes. The only occasion on which boys brought DVDs to school for academic literacy tasks was during the culminating activity on visualization skills, for which Connors explicitly encouraged students to bring in “something that you enjoy reading that you can make a picture in your head as you read.” No additional restrictions were put on the kind of text that students could bring in; indeed, when a student asked if a shampoo bottle would be appropriate, Connors said it would be fine, provided it contained text with enough “picture words” to conjure a vivid image in the student’s mind as she read. Nevertheless, only two boys brought in favorite DVDs for that activity, and action movies constitute 97 the rare text genre that remained an exclusive feature of boys’ vernacular literacy lives. The third exception is partially attributable to the fact that the number of fantasy titles among boys’ text selections for vernacular purposes is inflated by certain focal boys’ heavy self-reporting of fantasy books arnong their extracurricular reading. Despite this, works of fantasy (and imaginative fiction in general) did nevertheless appear in appreciable numbers in the academic and vernacular text selections of all of the focal boys whose text selections are represented on this chart. The finding that the most frequently selected genre for boys’ academic text selections is represented even more strongly among boys’ vernacular text selections should not affect the above interpretation, but rather reinforce the conclusion that the chief difference between boys’ text selections for vernacular and academic purposes in terms of genre is more quantitative (i.e. more texts from more genres) than qualitative. A more substantial divergence between the text selections made for academic and vernacular purposes by the boys in this study is in the realm of readability. Although boys’ text selections across contexts were overwhelmingly aimed at young adult audiences, boys’ text selections for academic purposes scored significantly lower on scales of readability than did their text selections for vernacular purposes. Tables A7 and A8 display t-test comparisons between the mean readability levels of focal boys’ and girls’ text selections for different purposes, using Fog and F lesch-Kincaid readability measures, respectively. On both scales, boys’ vernacular text selections scored more than one full grade level equivalent higher than those made for academic purposes, suggesting that boys selected more linguistically challenging texts for their vernacular literacy practices than for academic reading. This suggests that while boys were selecting texts 98 for school reading activities that corresponded with their personal interests in terms of subject matter, the boys were nonetheless opting to read easier texts for school than they might select for leisure reading. One could interpret this as the boys playing the “game of school” (Fried, 2005), choosing to minimize the effort put into academic tasks in order to get by without appearing to be an overachiever or a “loser,” while saving more challenging texts for engagement at their own pace, on their own leisure time. There is some evidence to support such a conclusion. Tiger, whose previous year’s Gates- MacGinitie score was among the highest of Connors’ students, indicating a twelfth grade equivalency, opted to read a Matt Christopher novel with a fifth grade readability level for the Book Project. Axel, reading at a seventh grade equivalent, did the same, as did every boy reading at or above grade level who selected 3 Jonathan Rand title for the Book or Genre Project (the average readability for books in Rand’s series is between third and fourth grade equivalent). However, the lower readability levels of texts selected for academic contexts does not negate the fact that these books frequently did have strong connections to the students’ personal interests. The Book Project selection made by Tiger, an avid basketball fan and player, was a basketball themed Matt Christopher novel, and Axel, an avid football fan and player, selected for his Book Project a football themed Matt Christopher novel. While Jonathan Rand’s books were popular among boys for Book and Genre Project selections, they were equally popular among boys’ vernacular text selections. Thus, despite this difference between contexts in text selections’ readability levels, it could be concluded that the high visibility within academic contexts of the most popular genres of reading material from boys’ vernacular literacy practices is a good indication that boys’ reading choices in Connors’ class were reflective of 99 authentic personal interests. In terms of medium, boys’ text selections for vernacular purposes came from a wider range of media than did their selections for academic purposes. This should not be a surprising finding, considering the fact that in three of the five academic contexts in which participants’ text selections were documented, students did not have the option to select texts other than books (see Chapter Three). While Connors did allow students to self-select personally interesting reading materials for Independent Reading and the Book and Genre Projects, students nevertheless had to choose books. While no such parameters were established for DEAR time, and students were explicitly encouraged to bring in non-book texts for culminating activity on visualization skills, few boys took advantage of that openness to explore non-book media in their academic reading choices. This can be clearly seen in Table A9, which presents the distribution by gender, medium and academic/vemacular context of all of the text selections made by focal students in this study. The only non-book medium that appeared among focal boys’ text selections for academic contexts in any appreciable number was magazines. Thus, while print media elements of boys’ vernacular transmedial interests were well represented across academic contexts, the curriculum was less amenable to non-print texts. As permeable as Connor made his curriculum to afford the appearance of texts from many boys’ most cherished vernacular literacy practices, such as Link and Gatorade’s Star Wars interests or Tiger and Montel’s basketball interests, those transmedial allegiances manifested themselves in academic contexts entirely as print media texts. While each of these boys participated in their respective affinity spaces through engagement with a wide range of non-print texts, such texts remained 100 exclusively a part of the boys’ extracurricular literacy lives. This certainly reflects a structural limitation on the classroom’s permeability; as discussed in Chapter Three, neither the Book Project nor the Genre Project made allowances for non-book texts, let alone non-print ones. It may also refleCt a sense on the part of students that such texts were not welcome within academic contexts, despite Connors’ encouragement of multimodal texts during the culminating activity on visualization skills, which is where the majority of the non-print and non-book texts that became visible in Connors’ classroom made their appearance. For DEAR time, Connors did not dissuade students from reading non-print or non-book texts, but he did not actively encourage such reading either. The almost total invisibility of any such texts during DEAR time among both boys and girls suggests that despite Connors’ creation of a permeable classroom structure, students’ sense of appropriate DEAR reading may have been conditioned by prior book- centric language arts experiences, a notion that Connors’ approach did not disrupt. The key here is not so much that multimodal or non-book texts were allowed to become visible, but that their visibility went largely unrecognized and unrernarked upon by Connors over the course of the school year. As permeable as Connors attempted to make his curriculum to the vernacular literacy practices of his students, that curriculum remained a solidly print-centric literacy environment, even in the face of evidence that other semiotic systems were implicated in the most engaging of students’ (particularly boys’) vernacular literacy practices. Contexts. The relative invisibility of non-print and non-book texts within the text selections that boys made for academic purposes necessarily limits the extent to which transmediation, a hallmark of the boys’ vernacular literacy practices, could become 10] visible in Connors’ classroom. Nevertheless, Connors did include two activities over the course of the year that had potential to capitalize on boys’ ability to negotiate narrative meaning across media and semiotic systems. In one activity, Connors read aloud Rudyard Kipling’s “Rikki Tikki Tavi,” then asked students to identify the main and supporting details of the story. He then showed the Chuck Jones animated movie version of “Rikki Tikki Tavi.” Discussion after the film focused on the extent to which the film covered the main and supporting details of the original. In the other activity, Connors read aloud a chapter from Maniac Magee that contained a good deal of physical action. Students were then challenged, in groups, to create a storyboard of the main events of the chapter and act them out. Subsequent discussion focused on the extent to which students were able to identify the key events of the chapter and visualize them to make sense of the narrative. In both of these activities, Connors focused on core skills that he had stressed throughout the year: the ability to identify main and supporting details in a text and the ability to visualize story information from the printed word. The Book Project was set up to be a culmination of this skill building, as two of the tasks students had to complete in their poster presentations were to summarize the main events of the story and sketch out two moments from the book (see Figure B9). Both activities involved a certain degree of intertextuality; in the first, students made comparisons and contrasts between story and film versions of “Rikki Tikki Tavi,” and in the second, students compared perfomative texts that they had created with the original version. This attention to intertextual connections is as close as any activity in Connors’ curriculum came to engaging students in transmediation. However, neither activity involved true transmediation in the sense that students were not challenged to synthesize 102 new understandings of the characters or narrative worlds from the available texts. Further, none of the activities that incorporated students’ self—selected texts afforded transmedial readings. Whether the projects involved book reports, poster presentations, keeping a log of pages read, or generating lists of “picture words” and sketching out key moments, students were encouraged to focus on one book or text at a time. While Connors did spend some time talking with students about how good readers make inferences, raising their metacognitive awareness about how writers can imply ideas without directly stating them, these discussions dealt solely with how good readers synthesize information within individual texts, not across multiple texts as takes place in transmediation. Aside fiom the two activities described above, Connors did not encourage students to make intertextual connections with any of their readings, self- selected or teacher-selected, preferring instead to build students’ skills in making text-to- self connections (see Figures B7, B8 and B9 for examples of this emphasis). Focusing on text-to-self connections is not bad practice. It is, after all, one of the core “strategies that wor ” (Harvey & Goudvis, 2000) promoted by literacy researchers as a hallmark of what good readers do, and can play an important role in supporting young adult’s identity formation. However, giving official curricular recognition only to the print media at the textual core of students’ vernacular literacy practices may undermine the efforts toward permeability that Connors made in opening his language arts classroom to children’s self-selected texts. An impenneability to the non-print media that students engage with in their vernacular literacy practices as well as the transmedial ways in which students engage with those texts limits the extent to which academic literacy tasks could truly be scaffolded on students’ vernacular literacies. Thus, while a 103 great number of books that accurately represent the textual core of boys’ vernacular literacy practices did become visible in Connors’ permeable classroom, many other kinds of texts that play equally important roles in those practices as well as critical aspects of the literacy practices themselves remained invisible. This is an ironic missed opportunity, as transmediation involves a sophisticated synthesis of the very strategies that Connors emphasized in his language arts curriculum — visualization, drawing personal and intertextual connections, making inferences, and summarizing main and supporting details — only applied across a broader, multimodal mediascape than Connors’ classroom practices afforded. Question Three How and to what extent do the vernacular and academic literacy practices of these boys difler from those of the girls within the same class? Girls and boys: Texts. Quantitatively, boys as a group made more documented text selections, and they made those selections fiom a wider range of genres and media than did girls. Of the 672 text selections documented across all contexts in this study, 379 (56.4%) were made by boys and 293 (43.6%) by girls. This overall difference is likely attributable to the large discrepancy between the number of recreational reading texts self-reported by focal boys and girls (see Table A3). Boys also made more text selections per capita than girls (15.75 to 12.208, respectively), although this difference is not statistically significant (t=l .1 l6, df-——46, p>0.1). As can be seen on Table A6, of the 21 genres from which focal students made text selections over the course of the school year, boys’ selections for vernacular purposes represented 17 genres compared to 12 for girls. For academic purposes, boys made selections from 14 genres, while girls’ selections 104 came from 13. Similar trends are found when comparing the different media represented by boys’ and girls’ text selections, as depicted on Table A9, with boys’ vernacular text selections representing five different media compared to three for girls. Boys’ tendency to select reading materials from a wider range of genres than girls held across all academic and vernacular contexts in which text selections were documented in this study, as did the tendency for their selections to represent a wider variety of media. In terms of authors and titles represented, the full dataset of girls’ text selections across all contexts was as diverse as the boys’, with no single title or author dominating girls’ choices for any purpose. While four of the authors listed on Table A5 were selected exclusively by boys, only one author was selected exclusively by girls: Heinrich Bauer North America, publisher of J-I 4 magazine, which is also the only title listed on Table A4 that was selected exclusively by girls. Like boys, girls overwhelmingly selected works created for the young adult audience, although in contrast to boys, readability levels were more stable across their selections for academic and vernacular contexts than they were for boys. As shown in Tables A7 and A8, less than one third of one grade level equivalent separates the mean readability levels of girls’ selections for vernacular and academic purposes on both the Fog and Flesch-Kincaid scales. Although the trend runs in the opposite direction than for boys, with girls’ academic text selections scoring slightly higher than their vernacular text selections, the difference is not statistically significant on either scale and stands in stark contrast to the full grade level difference in readability scores between boys’ vernacular and academic text selections. When asked “what are some things that get you really excited about reading?” 105 during their first interview, focal boys were more likely than girls to make explicit references to defining characteristics of their most favored genres in their responses. Chipmunk indicated a preference for books that feature “disgusting, interesting and completely weird things that go beyond your imagination,” while Link prefers books that encourage him “to make pictures in my head.” Billy Bob, an avid reader of Michigan/American Chillers titles, responded that what gets him most excited is simply “reading about stuff that I like if it doesn’t interest me, I won’t want to read it. But if it’s basketball or (like the one I’m reading) werewolves or mega—monsters, or all that stuff, I’ll like it because that’s the kind of stuff I like.” Sparty answered succinctly: “Action unlike a textbook.” According to B-Rad, “if they're starting to like get in a fight, it's kind of exciting, because you kind of wonder what's coming next. But sometimes they set up like fighting, fighting, fighting really hard and they get hurt, but that's exciting because you want to know what happens next.” Both Gatorade and Montel referred to “unsolved mysteries” and “scary stuff,” while Tiger listed “scary books, science fiction and sports” as his primary areas of interest. In all of these comments, boys revealed that what gets them most excited about reading is the chance to experience many of the hallmarks of what is often popularly known as “genre fiction,” which encompasses fantasy, science fiction, horror, mystery and action/adventure. For these boys, the genre of a particular text is an important consideration in deciding whether or not to engage with it, as the text’s generic affiliation provides some guarantee that favored content may lie within. When prompted with the same question, focal girls had very different responses. Ciera S. said that what gets her excited about reading is “what happens in the books, just 106 the things that go on, and some of the things that the characters do, and sometimes the titles are really catching, or my friends recommend them and I want to see what they are to me.” Alicia indicated a preference for “books that have pictures, not a lot of pictures, but some pictures so I can see what’s going on.” Monkey noted that what gets her excited about reading involves a social component: “if someone tells me about a book, I’ll check it out and read it. If I didn’t like them, I’d still tell people about them, it’s just I’d find a different type of book that I think I’d be interested in, that might be interesting to them.” Paris answered, “to see what happens next,” while Vampira pointed to “how much details [sic] they have in them,” and Spongebob couldn’t think of anything that she felt got her excited about reading. These responses differ from those given by focal boys to the extent that the girls tended not to refer to particular genres or textual content, but rather to more general characteristics of the reading experience or textual elements that are not genre- specific. In general, genre was much less of a salient characteristic in choosing to engage with a given text than it was for the focal boys. Nevertheless, girls’ text selections did demonstrate some clear genre preferences. While a narrower range of genres was represented among girls’ selections than among boys’, fantasy and realistic fiction were still the most popular genres for girls as well as for boys across academic and vernacular contexts, a pattern clearly demonstrated on Table A6. However, in contrast to boys, girls’ realistic fiction text selections were exclusively books about family or peer relationships, featuring female protagonists, such as The Sisterhood of the Traveling Pants or Are You There God? It ’5 Me Margaret, while boys’ selections in this genre covered a wider variety of themes, including outdoor adventure stories (e. g., Hatchet), sports-themed dramas (e. g., Ironman), and humorous 107 school-based novels (e. g., Dork in Disguise), although boys almost exclusively selected novels with male protagonists. Further, while boys’ interests in imaginative fiction stretched beyond fantasy to include science fiction and supernatural thrillers, girls’ interests in this area were strictly limited to fantasy novels. The only academic or vernacular context in which a genre other than fantasy or realistic fiction was the most popularly selected text for girls was the culminating activity on visualization skills, where teen fashion and celebrity magazines like J-I4 were the most popular. Serial works were as popular among girls as they were among boys, although the proportions of girls’ series—based text selections were more evenly distributed between academic and vernacular contexts, while for boys, series-based texts were more strongly concentrated in vernacular contexts. Among focal students, 117 serial works were documented in academic contexts (approximately 48% of focal students’ academic text selections), while 152 serial works were documented in vernacular contexts (just under 62% of focal students’ vernacular text selections). Table A10 is a contingency table presenting the frequency distribution of series and non-series text selections across academic and vernacular contexts for focal boys and girls. A 2x4 chi-square analysis indicates a significant, although low, association between the variables (x2 (3, N = 489) = 12.0405, p < 0.05, (PC: 0.16). One trend we can see from this distribution is that both focal boys and girls made more overall text selections from serial than from non—serial works, and that non-serial works were selected in geater numbers for academic purposes than for vernacular purposes by both focal boys and girls. Additionally, both focal boys and girls selected more serial than non-serial works for vernacular purposes, although the ratio of serial-to-non-serial works in vernacular contexts is higher for boys (1.75:1) than 108 for girls (1.39:1). However, serial works were selected in greater numbers for vernacular purposes than for academic purposes by focal boys, while serial works were represented slightly more in focal girls’ academic selections than among their vernacular selections. What this distribution pattern suggests is that while serial works play an important role in both the academic and vernacular literacy practices of focal boys and focal girls, boys were more likely to pursue their series-based interests outside of school while girls were likely to pursue their series-based reading in both contexts. An important contrast between the series-based interests of boys and girls in this study that might help explain this discrepancy is that, as discussed earlier, boys’ serial interests tended to range across a variety of media, while girls’ serial interests were almost exclusively print-based. Indeed, as shown in Table A9, girls made very few text selections outside of print-based media for either vernacular or academic purposes, and thus the generally print-centric permeability of Connors’ language arts curriculum more easily afforded the appearance of girls’ print-based serial commitments than it did the multimedia commitments of many boys’ serial interests. Some girls, like Riley and SpongeBob, took advantage of the element of choice in Connors’ language arts assignments to explore book series that they were interested in, such as A Series of Unfortunate Events. Connors used the first volume in the series, The Bad Beginning, as a read-aloud, and some students who had not read from the series before became eager to read further volumes. SpongeBob devoted as many of her Genre Project selections as she could, as well as her Book Project selection, to this series, reading up through volume 7 by the end of the school year. Riley followed a similar path, making it through volume 6 with her project book selections, as did Ciera 8., who 109 devoted much of her early independent reading time to the series, and Paris, who read up through volume 9. Brittany credits the series for “getting me to love reading,” and used her project choices to finish as many Series of Unfortunate Events titles as she could get hold of, as well as to launch into a new b00k series, The Spiderwick Chronicles. Not every focal girl was interested in the Series of Unfortunate Events, and not every focal girl chose serial works to fulfill academic reading obligations (e. g., Kalekidan, an English language learner from Ethiopia, who made no serial selections in any academic context, or Vampira, who read the first five books of the Harry Potter series over the course of the school year, but did not claim that reading for any assignment). However, eight of the eleven focal girls read at least two or more books from different series for academic purposes over the course of the year and all eleven read at least one series-based book for vernacular purposes. To be fair, many boys did pursue print-based series interests, as well as the print components of transmedial series interests, in academic contexts, too. Thor, for example, selected volumes from the Dragonslayer ’s Academy and Charlie Bone series for the Genre and Book Projects, and all of the focal boys except for Monte] read at least two or more books from various series (the most popular being Michigan/American Chillers) for academic purposes over the course of the year. Meanwhile, Gatorade read a Jedi Apprentice novel and Link read a George Lucas biography for the Genre Project, both books representative of their transmedial interest in Star Wars. However, the main idea here is that the series-based texts with which boys engaged within academic contexts scratch the surface of boys’ serial interests, much of which, by their transmedial nature, could not become visible within an otherwise permeable curriculum. 110 Girls and boys: Contexts. In addition to differences in the kinds of texts they selected for various purposes, girls also demonstrated different reading behaviors than boys, most notably during DEAR sessions. Girls were more likely than boys to select texts for DEAR sessions that they had also selected to fulfill other academic requirements, such as the Genre or Book Projects. Of the 58 text selections made by girls for DEAR sessions, 23 (or approximately 40%) were texts that they had also selected to read for the Genre or Book Projects. Of the 94 text selections made by boys for DEAR sessions, 17 (or approximately 18%) were texts that they had also selected to read for the Genre or Book Projects. This difference suggests that girls as a whole were in one sense more strategic in their text selections for DEAR time than were boys, taking advantage of DEAR time to read texts in fulfillment of other language arts projects rather than to read wholly for pleasure. This is not to claim that reading a text for pleasure and reading a text to fulfill an assignment are mutually exclusive purposes, especially in the context of a permeable curriculum. What it does suggest is that girls were more likely than boys to make instrumental text selections during DEAR time, choosing texts in the service of satisfying other academic requirements. While boys as a group selected more texts to read during DEAR sessions over the course of the school year, within a given DEAR session, girls were more likely to commit to a single text than were boys, who in turn were more likely to ”circulate between two or more texts. The following excerpt from field notes taken during one DEAR session is a typical description of boys’ reading habits during DEAR time: “Before DEAR time starts, Link returns 1 book to the classroom library (Dork in Disguise) and takes another (Ghosts). Later, he returns Ghosts and gets Lawn lll Weenies from his locker. One boy [non-focal] reads, then returns his first choice from the classroom library. Another boy [non-focal] had taken Captain Underpants #1 , then hung out at the classroom library until [Mr. Connors] asks him to return to his seat. He then read another book that he had brought. Tiger has youth bible with him on his desk, but still goes to school library, returning with a Larry Bird book. Montel reads Everest, from the classroom library. Sparty reads an American Chillers volume. Axel takes four paperbacks from the classroom library, all volumes from the Narnia series, and he takes the series box as well. He spends DEAR time shuffling the books around in the box and farming the pages. When [Connors] approaches, he selects one of the books, puts the others back (his desk abuts the classroom library) and reads silently.” In this excerpt, Sparty and Montel each read one book for the whole DEAR session, but their behaviors were atypical for the boys in the classroom. The rest of the boys described in this excerpt circulated between two or more books during the reading time, and the depth of their reading commitment ranged between Link’s active reading of all three books he had with him on this occasion to Axel’s flirting with multiple texts before briefly settling down on one book to read. This tendency of many boy readers to roam between multiple texts during the course of a single DEAR session was documented during nearly every regular DEAR session that was observed,7 and stands in strong contrast to the typical behaviors exhibited by many girl readers, who tended to commit to 7As discussed in the previous chapter, 20 to 30 minutes of DEAR time was regularly scheduled at the start of each day, while impromptu DEAR sessions were frequently added to the classroom routine at transition points during the day as students completed independent work in one subject and anticipated the advent of the next activity. These impromptu DEAR sessions rarely lasted more than 10 minutes and did not afford much time to engage with more than one text. The contrast between some boys’ roaming between multiple texts and many girls’ tendency to devote their full DEAR session to a single text is more characteristic of the regularly scheduled DEAR sessions than of the impromptu DEAR sessions. 112 single texts not only throughout an individual DEAR session, but often across sessions. For example, J B, a non-focal female student, spent several weeks of DEAR time reading Harry Potter and the Order of the Phoenix exclusively. PS, another non-focal female student, read Are You There God? It ’5 Me Margaret over the course of several DEAR sessions. Few boys demonstrated such a commitment to a single text from one DEAR session to the next, frequently starting fresh each DEAR session with a new text selected that same day from the school or classroom library, as demonstrated in the above vignette. Montel and Sparty, who read from one book for the entirety of the DEAR session described above, were not observed reading these same books during another DEAR session. During DEAR sessions, girls also tended to be more willing to put their reading on display than did boys. Figures B11 through 814 are four photographs taken in Mr. Connors’ classroom one morning after a regular DEAR session, while students were away for gym class. Near most seats in these images can be seen the large zip-up binders in which Connors’ students kept their handouts, notes, homework and school supplies. Because students in Connors’ and Price’s classes switch between their classrooms multiple times each day and do not have regular desks for storing supplies, these binders serve as portable carry-alls in lieu of backpacks or frequent trips to student lockers. Note that books can be seen atop some of the binders, particularly in Figure B15, the lower right-hand corner of Figure B12 and the lower left-hand corner of Figure B13. In each of these instances, the binders with books atop them belong to girls, while most of the binders that are zipped up with no books atop them belong to boys. These images are typical of what was observed throughout the school year when Connors’ students would 113 leave the classroom for a specials class (e. g., gym, music or art) or recess, especially after a regular or impromptu DEAR session. At the end of DEAR time, girls were more likely to leave their DEAR texts on display, while boys were more likely to either conceal their DEAR texts in their binders or return them to the classroom library. With rare exception, girls were more likely than boys to keep their DEAR texts (which as noted above were more likely to be texts that fulfilled other assignments) conveniently accessible outside of DEAR time, and were frequently observed reading from their DEAR texts as the course of the day afforded brief opportunities for impromptu, unofficial silent reading. For example, PS, a non-focal girl, was observed on several occasions over the course of a single morning reading from Are You There God? It ’5 Me Margaret in brief spurts during daily announcements, teacher-directed mini-lessons, and transitions between activities within Language Arts instruction. Link was also observed over the course of that same morning reading from The Wish List whenever an opportunity presented itself, but Link’s behavior in this regard is atypical for boys observed in this study. Boys! [and girls]. We might conclude from the foregoing discussion that Connors’ permeable classroom afforded greater continuity between girls’ vernacular and academic literacy practices than it did for boys. The most popular genres among girls’ text selections for academic literacy practices were a more accurate reflection of the most popular genres selected by girls for vernacular purposes than was the case for boys, whose academic text selections were drawn from fewer genres than those made for vernacular purposes. Texts selected by girls for academic purposes were also more in line with those made for vernacular purposes than were boys’ academic selections in terms of readability and series status, and girls did not demonstrate as much interest as boys did in 114 {chitin I. i media that were generally excluded from Connors’ otherwise permeable classroom. Finally, most girls usually had books at the ready to engage with whenever a moment might present itself and were more likely than boys to put their reading materials on display. However, while many boys were eager to engage in social identity development by making aspects of their vernacular literacy practices visible through sanctioned and unsanctioned literacy events in Connors’ classroom, there is evidence to suggest that at least some girls were unwilling to take advantage of the permeability afforded by Connors’ classroom to make visible important aspects of their vernacular literacy practices. Conversely, there is also some evidence that not every literacy practice that girls made visible in Connors’ classroom was a clear analogue to an “authentic” vernacular literacy practice. Despite many surface continuities between girls’ vernacular and academic literacy practices, it may be that instead of capitalizing on the permeability of Connors’ classroom as a potential nexus between vernacular literacies and social identity, some girls “played school,” leaving cherished aspects of their selves at the schoolhouse door. Prior scholarship has suggested that middle school boys tend to focus their reading more on non-book texts, such as magazines, newspapers and even baseball cards, as a function of an increasingly efferent stance toward print by which boys seek out texts that will reinforce their knowledge base on favorite activities, such as sports and other hobbies (Beatty, 2000; Farris, 1995; Sullivan, 2003; Wheeler, 1984; Wicks, 1995). While not every boy who participated in this study demonstrated a strong interest in sports, sports-oriented texts were almost exclusively selected by boys, many of whom played a 115 variety of sports themselves and who clearly deepened their interest and participation in their favorite sports through their reading choices. B-Rad and Axel, two focal boys with a passion for football, made a number of football-oriented text selections, and B-Rad discussed how he had tried to adapt a play described in a Matt Christopher novel during one of his games. Tiger and Monte] played tOgether on a basketball team, and their text selections reflect their abiding interest in the sport, with basketball-themed informational books, biographies, sports fiction novels, magazines and even an inspirational work (The Right Way to Win: How Athletes Can Place God First in Their Hearts) appearing among texts they selected across all contexts. As noted earlier, Tiger and Montel’s DEAR readings of sports-themed texts frequently turned into shared experiences with other boys Sharing a similar passion for basketball, SpongeBob and Ciera S., two focal girls, also played together on a basketball team and attended every Homestead High School girls’ basketball game that their schedules allowed together, including tournament games as Homestead’s girls’ team competed for the state championship in its division during the period of this study. They also attended several home games of the local university women’s basketball team, even following the team to Indianapolis to watch it play in the Final Four of the NCAA tournament. During the bookstore browsing activity, which these girls participated in together, they each selected non-fiction books on the NBA, the WNBA and NCAA women’s basketball. It might be argued that these two girls were even bigger fans of basketball than any of the boys in this study. However, any texts reflecting such interests were entirely limited to vernacular literacy events, and between the two girls, only SpongeBob brought a single sports-themed text into any academic 116 context: a copy of Sports Illustrated, which she selected for the culminating activity on visualization skills and in which she focused entirely on an article about an NFL star, not an article about basketball. Furthermore, neither girl participated in any formal or informal sports-related classroom converSations, despite ample opportunities to do so. As visible as boys’ sports interests became in Connors’ classroom, these girls’ vernacular interests in sports remained virtually invisible within the otherwise permeable academic context. This is particularly ironic, given that Connors was himself a junior varsity boys’ and girls’ track coach and frequently initiated brief, informal conversations on current sports topics with the whole class. However, only the boys would respond to Connors’ prompts. Although no other girls who participated in this study demonstrated as strong an interest in sports as did SpongeBob and Ciera S., there is no reason to suppose that the other girls in Connors’ classroom had any less of an interest in sports than boys, given ample research demonstrating that in the wider population, women are just as likely as men to identify themselves as sports fans (Costa, 2003; Dietz-Uhler, Harrick & J acquemotte, 2000). Why even the most passionately dedicated girls’ interests in sports did not become visible within Connors’ permeable classroom while boys’ sporting interests flourished can only be speculated. While neither Mr. Connors nor any of the boys in this study appeared to actively discourage the girls from sharing or engaging with their sports interests within the academic contexts documented here, the invisibility of girls’ sports interests in Connors’ otherwise permeable classroom indicates that at some level for these students, sports as a visible social practice was a male preserve, regardless of the prominence of sports within some girls’ extracurricular lives and vernacular 117 literacy practices. The causes of this phenomenon are beyond the scope of this study; it could be that SpongeBob and Ciera S. simply didn’t feel like reading about sports at school or it could be the result of a complex constellation of social forces operating across the students’ lifespan. Nevertheless, the virtual absence of sports texts among the text selections made by girls for academic purposes, despite evidence that sports formed an important element of these girls’ social identities outside of school, suggests that as amenable as Connors’ classroom was to many aspects of girls’ vernacular literacy practices, it did not serve as a nexus of literacy and identity for some girls as it did for many boys. Question Four What attitudes toward vernacular and academic literacy practices do these boys hold, how do they compare with the attitudes of other students, and how do those attitudes change over the course of an academic year in a book club based classroom? As described in Chapter 3, students in Mr. Connors’ Language Arts classes were administered the Elementary Reading Attitude Survey (ERAS) at the start of the 2004-05 school year and again at the end to assess the extent to which students’ attitudes toward reading for academic and recreational purposes might change in the context of the permeable curriculum implemented by Connors. While 41 students were present for the first ERAS administration in the fall, and 42 were present for the spring administration, only 36 students were present for both administrations. Within this group were 19 boys and 17 girls (including 10 focal boys and 8 focal girls), and it is the scores from these students that were used for the analyses that are reported here. At the start of the school year, boys’ attitudes toward academic and recreational 118 reading were almost identical, and group means were comparable to national norms for sixth graders in the fall as published by McKenna and Kear (1990). Table A1 1 presents the mean scores for these subscales for the fall administration by gender. As can be seen in the table, the mean scores for girls on both subscales were higher than those for boys, and simple t-test comparisons of these means reveal that the difference on the recreational subscale was significant while the difference on the academic subscale was not. When comparing subscale scores within groups, McKenna and Kear indicate that a difference of 3 points or more between subscales should be regarded as a significant discrepancy in attitude toward reading for different purposes. For neither boys nor girls does the difference between mean scores on the academic and recreational subscales exceed this threshold. By the spring administration, mean scores for both boys and girls on both subscales of the ERAS had dropped. While the drop in boys’ attitudes toward academic reading was statistically significant, the drop in boys’ attitudes toward recreational reading was not. Drops on both subscales for girls were significant, however. Table A12 shows t-test results indicating that at the end of the year, the gaps between boys’ and girls’ scores were relatively unchanged from the fall administration, with the difference between boys’ and girls’ attitudes toward recreational reading remaining significant. Paired sample t-test comparisons of boys’ and girls’ recreational subscale scores from fall to spring administrations of the ERAS (Table A13) demonstrate that while scores for both boys and girls dropped, only the drop for girls was statistically significant, and both groups’ mean scores for the recreational subscale are within one standard deviation of national norms for sixth graders. Comparing boys’ and girls’ academic subscale scores 119 from fall to spring administration (Table A14, paired sample t-test comparisons), we can see that scores for both boys and girls dropped, and in contrast to the recreational subscale, both drops are statistically significant, yet still within one standard deviation of national norms. Finally, while the difference between the mean scores for boys on the academic and recreational subscales (24.37 and 24.95, respectively) at the spring administration does not exceed the 3 point threshold specified by McKenna and Kear, the difference in girls’ mean scores on the two subscales does (26.15 and 29.77 respectively). This indicates that girls held more positive attitudes toward reading for recreational than for academic purposes at the end of the year, while boys’ attitudes were comparable across contexts. To summarize the findings regarding reading attitude, both boys’ and girls’ attitudes toward academic reading as measured by the ERAS dropped significantly over the course of the 2004-05 school year, and while both groups’ attitudes toward recreational reading dropped, only the drop in girls’ scores was statistically significant. The difference between boys’ recreational and academic subscale scores remained stable between fall and spring administrations, and stayed within the 3 point threshold specified by McKenna and Kear for judging a significant discrepancy between attitudes for recreational and academic reading. However, the difference between girls’ recreational and academic subscale scores widened between the fall and spring administrations to exceed the 3 point threshold, indicating that girls as a group held more positive attitudes toward recreational reading than toward academic reading at the end of the school year. A comparison of boys’ and girls’ mean subscale scores on the spring administration with national norms reveals that both means for both groups fall within one standard deviation 120 of the national norms for sixth graders. Possible explanations for this overall decline in reading attitude among Mr. Connors’ language arts students will be offered in the following chapter. 121 CHAPTER 5 Discussion Introduction Over the past decade, one of the overriding concerns of adolescent literacy research has been to better understand the extracunicular literacy practices in which middle and high school students engage, on the premise that more detailed knowledge of these “real world” literacies could provide educators a scaffold upon which to construct more effective academic literacy learning environments. However, as Alvermann et al. (2007) argue, this is an area that remains “largely unstudied and/or underreported ... despite continued calls for exploring the potential of such literacies to serve as springboards for actively engaging youth in academic reading tasks” (p. 34). Our contemporary understanding of the literacy practices that adolescents engage in for personal (or what in this study are termed vernacular) purposes emerges from a body of scholarship based on large-scale surveys of recreational reading and leisure time activities (e. g., Hall & Coles, 1999; Nippold, Duthie & Larsen, 2005), in-depth interviews with individual or small groups of students (e.g., Cavazos-Kottke, 2006; Reeves, 2004), best practice reports of classroom contexts and activities that promote active engagement with core teacher-selected texts (e.g., Brozo, 2002; Smith & Wilhelm, 2006), and ethnographic accounts of adolescents participating in extracurricular media clubs (e. g., Alvermann, Hagood, Heron-Hmby, Hughes, Williams & Yoon, 2007; Alvermann, Huddleston & Hagood, 2004). Absent from the literature is an exploration of what a permeable classroom structure that attempts to embrace the texts of students’ vernacular literacy practices — not as on-ramps to engagement with core teacher-selected 122 texts but as the core texts themselves — might reveal about adolescents’ literacy practices as well as how adolescents’ attitudes toward reading might fluctuate in the context of such a classroom. This study was designed to enrich our understanding of adolescents’ vernacular literacy practices, particularly those of boys, through an analysis of the text selections made by a group of sixth grade students in just such a permeable classroom structure and through a comparison of the text selections made by a subset of those students for vernacular purposes outside of that classroom context. Specifically, this study sought answers to four main questions: 1. What are the vernacular literacy practices of a class of middle school boys and what kinds of texts do these boys engage with in those practices? 2. How and to what extent do these boys' vernacular literacies become visible within a language arts program structured around students' self-selected texts? 3. How and to what extent do the vernacular and academic literacy practices of these boys differ from those of the girls within the same class? and 4. What attitudes toward vernacular and academic literacy practices do these boys hold, how do they compare with the attitudes of other students, and how do those attitudes change over the course of an academic year in a book club based classroom? As the previous two chapters have detailed, Ed Connors’ sixth grade Language Arts classroom at Harwell Middle School was the site for a year long investigation into these questions. Text selections made by all students in Connors’ two Language Arts classes for a variety of academic purposes were documented and analyzed. Text selections made 123 by a focal group of students from Connors’ classes for vernacular purposes were also documented and were compared with the text selections that those focal students made for academic purposes. Comparisons were also made between the text selections made by boys and girls for academic and vernacular purposes, both at the whole group level and within the subgroup of focal students. Results of these analyses were detailed in Chapter Four. In this chapter, the significance of these findings for better understanding the literacy practices of adolescents, especially boys, as well as implications for instruction and for teacher education will be discussed. To situate this study’s findings within the current scholarship on boys and literacy, the discussion in this chapter is organized around three main focal points: the texts that defined the physical landscape of the participants’ literacy practices; the curricular and extracurricular contexts that defined the social and psychological landscape of those practices; and lastly, the role of the classroom teacher as a facilitator of literacy practices across and within those contexts. Limitations of the current study and possible directions for firture research will be addressed throughout. The Texts —- Currents of Diversity The Long Tail The first big idea that emerges from a consideration of the texts selected by the boys and girls in Mr. Connors’ Language Arts classes for vernacular and academic purposes is the high degree of diversity in the overall set of text selections. A teacher or librarian looking to this study for guidance as to particular authors or titles to keep in stock to meet the demands of students’ reading appetites would not get very far with this 124 dataset beyond a recommendation to offer an ample array of Michigan/American Chillers books and some Matt Christopher classics to appeal to boys, and perhaps to keep a few sets of Lemony Snicket’s A Series of Unfortunate Events handy. Ultimately, there were no big “hits” with the whole group of students, a microcosmic illustration of the “Long Tail” phenomenon (Anderson, 2006). According to Anderson, when consumers are free to select from a virtually limitless array of different texts (such as when shopping for songs on iTunes or for books at Amazon.com, for example), very few individual titles emerge as big hits. Instead, the vast majority of songs downloaded on iTunes or books purchased at Amazon.com are selected in relatively small numbers by relatively few consumers. If a frequency distribution of the number of downloads of the top 1,000,000 best selling songs on iTunes were plotted, the result would be a curve with a very steep, sudden decline, followed by a long steady plateau that never reaches zero (see Figure 816 for a prototypical Long Tail curve). The narrow head of the curve is where we find a few big hits, appealing to a broad spectrum of listeners; the Long Tail of the curve is where we find not only the vast majority of the iTunes inventory, but also the vast majority of downloads, period. In other words, the market is not driven by hits that appeal to large agglomerations of consumers, but rather by songs that have niche appeal to relatively small groups of people. Anderson (2006) finds this phenomenon not just in online marketplaces such as iTunes and Amazon.com, but also in television ratings, movie attendance, and web searching behaviors. The most recent statistics released by Nielsen BookScan, which tracks approximately 70% of book sales across 6,500 retail locations, provide additional confirmation of the Long Tail phenomenon: of the approximately 1,466,000 unique titles sold in the United States in 2006, 1,123,000 (or 125 just under 78%) sold fewer than 99 copies, while only 483 (or less than a tenth of one percent) sold more than 100,000 copies. (Nielsen Bookscan, in Harper’s Index, 2007). While a marketplace of reading materials with a virtually infinite inventory was not literally available to Connors’ students, they did nonetheless have access to two large classroom libraries (Connors’ and his team-teacher, Ms. Price’s), a well-stocked and constantly updated school library in a network with hundreds of other school and public libraries statewide, two large public libraries within five miles of the school, and one major chain bookstore in the Homestead district. Whether available on the shelf of a local library, by interlibrary loan from a networked library, or for purchase at a brick-and- mortar or online bookstore, Connors’ students did not lack for access to texts, and the focal students interviewed in this study indicated that they utilized all of these sources to find texts that they were interested in reading. Given that over half of the text selections documented in this study were only selected once by a single student and that the most popular texts appeared among the text selections of fewer than 20% of the students, we can see the Long Tail phenomenon in small-scale. A previous investigation into the reading preferences of a small group of talented middle school boys found little to no overlap in the particular authors and titles selected by that study’s five participants, and the question of whether a similar pattern might be seen with a larger, mixed gender group of general readers was raised (Cavazos-Kottke, 2006). The results of this study suggest that rather than congregating around a canon of popular texts, the crop of text selections yielded by a larger and more diverse group of readers becomes spread over an even larger array of authors and titles. How might teachers interested in supporting students’ academic literacy skills by 126 creating opportunities for aspects of their vernacular literacy practices to enter into the classroom context deal with such potentially unruly diversity? Acknowledging that students’ vernacular literacy lives are characterized by a much wider range of print and non-print texts than the traditional language arts curriculum (or for that matter the average pre-service teacher whose apprenticeship of observation likely took place in the context of such a curriculum) may be prepared to deal with, Anstey and Bull (2006) offer the following perspective: There are two possible responses to these ideas when selecting texts for use in the classroom. One is for the teacher to limit the texts and review their content carefully, using only texts that meet an agreed-on set of criteria regarding the topics, values, and attitudes of the content. This approach would ensure that students met only appropriate texts in school, but it would not equip them to deal with texts outside of school that presented contrasting ideas, values and attitudes, often very carefully disguised. A second response, which more appropriately supports multiliteracies’ goals, is for teachers to ensure that a broad range of texts are available and help students develop the skills to analyse these texts. In this way, students will become more discriminatory and have skills to deal with texts they encounter in many contexts. (p. 37) It is important to note that Anstey and Bull do not claim that taking the second stance toward students’ vernacular literacy practices will necessarily improve students’ attitudes toward reading, raise achievement or foster life-long reading habits. Rather, they modestly argue that helping students develop critical reading skills with the texts that characterize their vernacular literacy lives will simply empower them to more effectively 127 “deal with” the various texts they encounter there. As Alvermann et al. (2007) note (and I discuss in Chapter Two of this dissertation), the literature lacks conclusive data on the extent to which reading habits, attitudes or achievement may be affected by scaffolding students’ development in academic literacy practices upon self-selected texts that represent students’ vernacular literacy practices. Setting aside the fact that the non-experimental design of this study does not allow for causal claims, the drops in reading attitudes documented among students in this study certainly do not lend empirical support to an argument that reading attitudes may improve in the context of student choice, particularly given the structural impermeability of certain elements of Connors’ curriculum to the non-book and non-print texts that played a part in many boys’ vernacular literacy lives. Nevertheless, it should be noted that none of the drops in reading attitude documented here represent serious declines beyond those which might be expected among sixth graders, and thus while the results of this study do not make an unequivocal statement on the power of choice to positively affect young adolescents’ attitudes toward reading, they by no means suggest that choice is demotivating. Rather, the comments of Billy Bob, a focal boy, lend some support to those arguing for greater choice as a positive in adolescent literacy instruction, no matter how elusive causal data may be: I recently had a change in attitude toward reading. The books I was trying to get into before, I didn’t like them, so I found a book from the Chillers. I started reading them and really liked them. Reading about stuff that I like if it doesn’t interest me, I won’t want to read it. But if it’s basketball or -— like the one I’m reading - werewolves or mega-monsters, or all that stuff, I’ll like it because 128 that’s the kind of stuffI like. What Billy Bob describes here is a kind of self-discovery that we might hope to see in all reluctant readers, given a space to explore personal interests and become captivated by a book. In contrast to the general trend of attitude scores among Connors’ students, Billy Bob’s attitudes toward both academic and recreational reading improved over the course of the 2004-05 school year. While a causal link cannot be made between Billy Bob’s attitude and the opportunities for choice provided by Mr. Connors, it is clear that Billy Bob saw the chance to experiment with different reading materials on his own recognizance as a positive force in his literacy development. Billy Bob’s experience resonates with me because it echoes testimonials I heard from boys in my own teaching practice who discovered (or rediscovered) a love of reading upon experiencing the right book. Unfortunately, as the results of this study demonstrate, the right book may be different for every student, making a commitment to offering a high degree of student choice across the cuniculum an especially challenging proposition. When given ample opportunities to self—select reading materials for a variety of academic and vernacular contexts, the fact that there was relatively little repetition of authors and titles across students’ text selections suggests that educators hoping to offer young adolescent readers an attractive selection of interesting texts in a classroom or school library would be advised to stock their shelves with a wide assortment of different authors and titles, rather than investing in class sets of a limited selection of notable texts. Given a strong preference for texts published within the last five to ten years, another implication for practice from this wide diversity of authors and titles is that teachers would also be well advised to maintain an active interest in current publications 129 and to continually refresh their awareness of new titles and authors that might appeal to their students. While the annual lists of children’s, young adults’ and teachers’ book choices published by the International Reading Association (IRA) provide a convenient snapshot of titles and authors popular with large groups of readers, it is important to note that the process by which the IRA compiles these lists renders that snapshot two years old by the time it reaches press. The 2007 lists, for example, are based solely on books published in 2005, and it should be noted that of the 300 titles presented on the ten most recent young adults’ book choices lists (representing books published between 1996 and 2005, the period from which the vast majority of texts selected by students in this study were published), only 13 of those titles appear in the database of this study, and only four of the authors whose works appear on these lists (J K Rowling, Gordon Korman, Phyllis Reynolds Naylor and Louis Sachar) are among the most frequently selected authors in this study. Neither Lemony Snicket nor Jonathan Rand, who did publish books during this period and who were the two most frequently selected authors in this study, appear on any of the IRA’s young adults’ choices lists. If we want to open our classrooms to the texts of students’ vernacular literacy practices in order to scaffold academic learning on students’ existing funds of knowledge and perhaps increase motivation for engaging in academic literacy tasks (an ideal that Alvermann et al. (2007) remind us remains elusive in adolescent literacy research and practice), we have to accept that students are not likely to congregate around a single messianic text or even a small group of texts. Rather, we need to be able to cope with a potentially unruly collection of texts, many of which we may not particularly enjoy ourselves. We might hope that lots of students will pick up Harry Potter and the 130 Sorcerer ’s Stone so that we can teach about mythic archetypes, explore connections to other classics of fantasy literature and marvel at the linguistic cleverness of the names that J K Rowling gives her characters. Certainly, some students will go in that direction, but we will also spend our time pointing out misuses of “its” and “it’s” in the first chapter of Kreepy Klowns of Kalamazoo or explaining why students have to use complete sentences in their writing while Jonathan Rand gets to sell a million novels composed largely with sentence fragments. My point here is not to discourage the practice of self-selection in language arts instruction, but rather to point out that when we encourage students to bring themselves and their tastes into the classroom, we potentially invite in a degree of diversity unprecedented by any of our prior experiences learning about literature. The apprenticeship of observation through which most language arts teachers passed as students likely involved a systematic study of a canon of texts that, no matter how progressively calculated to include diverse voices, was defined in advance by a content area expert and rarely open to student-defined interests. If we offer our students the challenge to select reading materials in accordance with their own tastes, abilities and interests — and they take us up on it — we will then be faced with the daunting task of recognizing each student’s unique selections, because as Alvermann and her colleagues remind us, adolescents will regard such choices as an “experience of identity” (Alvermann & Hagood, 2000, p. 445; also, Alvermann, Huddleston & Hagood, 2004). The demotivational consequences of failing to recognize the “repertoires of practice” that children might choose to make visible in such a permeable environment can be knotty, especially if students interpret a teacher’s reluctance to embrace the texts of their 131 vernacular literacies as a referendum on their identity (Nixon & Comber, 2006, p. 128; see also Reeves, 2004). Underlying Patterns Diversity should not be mistaken for chaos, however. While boys may appear to be “all over the place” in their reading habits (J. Rothschild, personal communication, July 7, 2007), the results of this study fortunately demonstrate that there are some predictable trends in boys’ and girls’ text selections that may better assist educators looking for guidance in stocking a class library or preparing to engage with a permeable classroom. As detailed in the previous chapter, books constituted the majority of the text selections made by the whole group of students across all contexts as well as those made by boys for vernacular purposes, although boys were more likely than girls to select texts from across a range of print and non-print media. Compared to their text selections for academic purposes, boys’ text selections for vernacular purposes were more likely to come from a series, were spread across a wider range of genres, and were on average more advanced in terms of readability, even though their selections were predominantly texts marketed to a young adult audience. By far the most popular genre among boys’ text selections for vernacular purposes was fantasy, followed by action, realistic fiction, mystery/thriller, sports and science fiction, which were represented in roughly equal numbers of text selections. Girls’ text selections for vernacular purposes were more narrowly limited to works of fantasy and realistic fiction. It should be noted that these trends do not exist independently of each other in the dataset. The finding that boys favored series works in their text selections for vernacular purposes cut across the different media from which their text selections were made, and multiple media texts 132 were found within each of the most popular genre categories. For example, a popular science fiction franchise like Star Wars manifests itself in the full dataset of vernacular text selections as well as within individual focal students’ sets of text selections as series of books, video games and movies. Series interests were not confined to a particular medium, but frequently crossed over into multiple media. These trends in boys’ text selections are in line with much contemporary scholarship on young adults’ reading preferences. The popularity of imaginative fiction (including fantasy, science fiction, and most of the texts classified in this study as mystery/thriller, such as books from the Michigan/American Chillers and Goosebumps series) among young adolescent boys has been well established in prior studies, as has the popularity of series fiction (Cavazos-Kottke, 2006; Coles & Hall, 2002; Hall & Coles, 1999; Knowles & Smith, 2005; Nell, 1988; Newkirk, 2002; Sullivan, 2003, 2004; Wicks, 1995; Worthy, Moorman, Turner, 1999). The results of this study indicate that this is also true for girls, but what makes boys’ preferences unique in this regard is that they are spread across a wider range of genres within this category, whereas girls’ selections are more narrowly focused around fantasy. A preference among boys for texts that have strong visual appeal, both in terms of vivid language and graphic accompaniment, has also been documented in prior scholarship (Kajder, 2006; Smith & Wilhelm, 2002, 2006; Sullivan, 2003). The finding that the boys in this study made more overall text selections than girls does not have a precedent in the literature. Indeed, in the few studies that directly compare the leisure reading habits of boys and girls, the opposite trend is found (Coles & Hall, 2002). That boys’ preferred reading materials come from a wider array of genres 133 than do girls’ preferred materials is also a new finding. Prior scholarship on boys’ genre preferences either has found very little difference from girls’ preferences (e. g., Worthy, Moorman & Turner, 1999) or has focused on a narrow range of “boy-friendly” genres without comparison to girls’ genre preferences, including non-fiction, humor, sports/outdoors and fantasy/science fiction (e. g., Sullivan, 2003, 2004). While sports and science fiction texts were found almost exclusively among boys’ text selections, a preference for non-fiction and humor was not expressed any more strongly among boys in this study than among girls. The majority of non—fiction texts in this dataset were biographies and magazines, selected in roughly equal numbers by boys and girls for the Genre Project and the culminating activity on visualization skills, respectively. One limitation of this study is that despite the clear variety of texts with which students engaged in their vernacular literacy practices, this variety is underreported to the extent that certain types of texts that focal students did mention in the course of interviews and informal conversations were not added into the text selection database. These kinds of texts were excluded from data analysis chiefly due to the logistical difficulties in isolating a discrete student-with-specific-text unit of analysis to assess students’ selections of these kinds of texts. For example, most of the focal students mentioned in interviews that they frequently sought information about topics of personal interest on the Internet, but particular sites were neither solicited nor documented in the course of the interviews. Several focal students also made references to reading “the paper,” without further elaborating on which papers they read, which sections of those papers they read, or the frequency with which they read them. Wicks (1995) found that newspapers were the most frequently read texts among the boys he surveyed, suggesting 134 that this text form might have had more prominence among the vernacular literacy practices of the students in this study had newspaper text selections been included in the dataset. Televised sporting events (with the exception of WWE Smackdown.’, which is a weekly series with ongoing narrative arcs, more similar to a television serial drama than a “traditional” sports broadcast; see Alvermann, Huddleston & Hagood, 2004) and sports news programs were not documented, despite clear references to consumption of such texts and despite the fact that since the 1960s, the prevailing aesthetic of American sports broadcasting has been to cast the televised athletic competition into a rudimentary narrative framework, such that as a text, it has more in common with the average television drama than with the experience of a live sporting event (see Crawford, 2004, and Jay, 2004). Finally, participants’ engagement with popular music texts was not documented. In the case of music, only one focal student, Gatorade, made a reference to the Gwen Stefarri song “Hollaback Girl” during his bookstore browsing interview, and this was the sole occasion on which contemporary popular music was directly mentioned by any participant in the entire study. Given that other scholarship has found listening to popular music to be the leisure activity in which adolescents most frequently engage (e. g., Nippold, Duthie & Larsen, 2005; Perlstein, 2003), this omission is perhaps the most glaring, and future research should seek ways to document participants’ text selections in this as well as the preceding areas such that these kinds of texts can be factored into the overall portrait of adolescent literacy practices and that their representation therein can be reliably compared with that of the other kinds of text documented in this study. The Contexts — (Im)Permeable Semiotic Social Spaces T ransmediation 135 A major feature of boys’ vernacular literacy practices that was documented in this study, but not fully capitalized upon by Ed Connors’ otherwise permeable curriculum, is a tendency to engage in transmediation, or the synthesis of a complex meta-narrative across a range of media and delivery systems that each offer unique perspectives or bits of diegetic information on characters, stories or entire narrative worlds. The most obvious examples of transmediation come from the world of imaginative fiction (e.g., the Star Wars universe unfolding across films, novels and video games), but as this study has argued, sports fandom as enacted by many boys in this study offers an additional example of transmedial reading, with the added bonus that boys’ transmediation across sports texts frequently involved an observable social component. The process by which boys in this study engaged in the work of social identity construction through transmedial allegiances became visible in Connors’ classroom, although much of the social activity took place outside of sanctioned curricular space and Connors’ language arts activities were largely impermeable to non-print media. Writers on transmediation fi'equently describe it as a “natural” feature of everyday literacy practices, especially among contemporary adolescents, but rarely offer empirical evidence that adolescents actually engage in such practices. This assumption of naturalness is conveyed through rhetorical appeals to the reader to consider how one negotiates meaning by synthesizing information and ideas fiom a range of print and non- print media texts in one’s daily life, and descriptions of teens simultaneously listening to digital music, surfing the Internet, watching videos and chatting with fiiends via instant messaging programs (e. g., Anstey & Bull, 2006; King & O’Brien, 2002; Semali, 2000, 2002). What evidence these authors do offer is usually more indicative of multitasking — 136 simultaneous engagement with multiple semiotic systems — than true transmediation. Frequently cited is the Kaiser Family Foundation’s Generation M report (Roberts, Fueho & Rideout, 2005), which found that on the average day, young people are exposed to approximately 8 1/2 hours of media content in just 6 V2 hours of real time, indicating that at least a quarter of that time involves simultaneous exposure to multiple media. However, Generation M only reports media use; it does not investigate the meaning-making strategies employed by media consumers. Flores-Koulish (2005) found a similar level of saturation in her study of the media habits of pre-service teachers, providing some description of the ways in which her participants synthesize meaning in a multimodal, multitextual mediascape. Bruce (2002) provides anecdotes of teenagers engaging with a variety of media texts, using such texts as “boundary objects” that allow adolescents “to connect with each other by means of and through shared artifacts ... [and] are integral to the ways that these young people make meaning, communicate, and construct their social lives” (p. 3). These authors all agree that it is incumbent upon teachers to facilitate the development of students’ critical faculties as they transmediate in order to nurture what Gardner (2006) calls “the synthesizing mind,” one which is able to construct new knowledge from a variety of disciplines and information sources and one which Gardner and all of the above cited authors celebrate as essential for effective citizenship, leadership and occupational success in the modern age. However, while all of these authors provide convincing arguments for the importance of developing such competencies, for expanding schooled notions of literacy to embrace transmedial practices, and for the centrality of transmediation in everyday literacies, none aside from Flores-Koulish (2005) and Bruce (2002) offer empirical support for their assumptions of 137 the naturalness of transmediation. The talented boy readers studied in a previous investigation (Cavazos-Kottke, 2006) did demonstrate transmedial literacy practices, in one exemplary case constructing a detailed understanding of the Marvel Comics universe by transmediating between novel, comic book and film versions of The Incredible Hulk and X-Men as well as Discovery Channel documentary programs and tie-in books on evolution and even Darwin’s On the Origin of Species. This study fills a gap in the literature by providing empirically gathered support for claims that transmediation plays a significant role in the vernacular literacy lives of adolescents, and that the affinity spaces in which those transmedial literacy practices are carried out provide stages on which young adolescents construct social identities. Extending on a limitation suggested earlier in this chapter, the extent to which multiple media play a role in boys’ vernacular literacy practices is likely underrepresented in this study. Text selections for vernacular purposes were added to the database conservatively; focal students had to make reference to a specific text or series by title in one of the interviews for it to be entered into the database, and no text selections were added to the database by inference. For example, Link brought a strategy book for the Ruby and Sapphire versions of the GarneBoy Advance game Pokemon for analysis in the culminating activity on visualization skills, and this book was entered into the database as an academic selection for the visualization activity. In his journal, Link wrote seven reasons for why he found this book interesting, including “It has been my specialty since I was like 5, 6 or 7 I'm a mastermind at it.” On another occasion, Link brought a copy of Nintendo Power magazine to read during DEAR time, and he spent the period reading a series of articles on the upcoming Emerald version of Pokemon. It 138 would be reasonable to infer from the evidence that Link plays Pokemon and that the Pokemon series of games plays a significant role in his vernacular literacy practices. However, Link never discussed his video gaming interests in any of the interviews, even though he spoke in great detail about other texts he enjoyed outside of academic contexts, including movies and magazines as well as books. Another example is Chipmunk, who read an issue of Game Informer for his visualization activity text, yet did not discuss any particular video game titles during his interviews. It is likely that Chipmunk enjoys video gaming as part of his vernacular literacies, but no game text selections are attributed to him in the database. Thus, despite the stronger representation of a variety of media texts within the dataset of text selections documented for boys’ vernacular literacy practices than those for girls, the true extent to which multiple media texts play a role in the vernacular literacy lives of focal boys is likely underestimated in the dataset. Girls, Sports and Silence Although Ed Connors’ perrneably structured language arts cuniculum provided a portal for certain limited aspects of these transmedial vernacular literacy practices to become visible in an academic context (particularly where print-based media could be involved), it is important to note that while many boys took advantage of the opportunity to make those practices visible, some girls did not. The example discussed in Chapter 4 involved the appearance of sports—themed transmedial literacies exclusively within the visible literacy practices of boys, despite evidence that at least two girls in Connors’ language arts class were accomplished athletes and avid sports fans. Sociologists have long discussed how sports operates as a “male preserve,” a social practice that serves to reproduce and maintain society’s larger gender inequities by providing women fewer 139 opportunities for sports participation, leadership and media coverage than are accorded men, despite legislative efforts such as Title IX to address such inequities and despite evidence that women are as interested in sports as men are (Bryson, 1987; Costa, 2003; Dunning, 1986; Theberge, 1985). Wenner (1998) argues that sports spaces, such as sports bars and other venues where men gather to view or talk about sports, serve as “segregated sites of ‘male solidarity’ where men can ‘rehearse’ their identification with other men” (p. 310). Indeed, Fine (1987) observed in his ethnographic account of middle school age Little League baseball teams that while boys enjoyed the attention of girl spectators, they never acknowledged girls’ presence at their games, and that through participation in and talk about sports with other boys, players simultaneously forged peer and gender identities. Cross-gender fiiendships were not uncommon among the boys Fine studied, but they were restricted to relationships marked by either romance or mutual teasing. Cordial friendships based on common interests, such as sports, were not observed. Literacy events structured around sports in Connors’ class were indeed a male preserve, and while neither Connors nor the boys in his classes were ever observed trying to enforce such a gender regime on the girls in the class, they did nothing to disrupt it. Girls like SpongeBob and Ciera S. were neither actively discouraged from nor encouraged to bring their basketball passions into the community of learners that Connors attempted to nurture through his permeable classroom. Nevertheless, this important aspect of these girls’ personal identities remained invisible within virtually all academic contexts, flourishing almost exclusively as an unrecognized extracurricular pursuit. Despite Connors’ openness to his students’ vernacular literacy practices, it is in the gendered nature of sports discourse among the visible literacy practices enacted in his 140 classroom that the above warning about the need to explicitly recognize the materials that children and adolescents bring in their “virtual school bags” (Nixon & Comber, p. 129) must be heeded. It is not enough to simply make the classroom permeable to adolescents’ vernacular literacy practices and then Watch what pours in. The teacher in such a classroom must be actively engaged with the texts and contexts of each student’s vernacular literacies. The teacher in a permeable classroom must educate (from the Latin educare, meaning “to draw out”) in the truest sense of the word, taking an active interest in the students’ vernacular literacies and funds of knowledge and drawing out those literacies upon which students’ further development can be most compellingly scaffolded (Moll, Amanti, Neff & Gonzalez, 1992). A truly educative experience is one that thoughtfully recognizes where individual learners stand, then transforms them in such a way that broadens their minds and spirits to new experiences and knowledge and enables future learning. This is an ideal to which Ed Connors aspired in creating his permeable classroom. However, as the missed opportunities to engage these girls’ sports literacies or to fully integrate transmediation into the curriculum suggest, this ideal remained unfulfilled. The Teacher - Negotiator of Literacy Events and Practices Social Engagement in Theory When asked to describe his own reading habits, Ed Connors notes that he had never been a big reader of fiction. He admits that as an adolescent, he disliked reading for academic purposes, but that he was a “voracious reader” of informational texts that he was able to self-select. Even as an adult, Connors says, “I like to read what I like to read. I don’t really like it when people give me books to read. It’s too schoolish.” While he still 141 does not read much fiction, he is an avid collector of “teacher books,” and in those books, he says that in recent years he has noted “a trend toward a Reader’s Workshop approach over whole class novel approaches,” a recommendation that conveniently dovetails with his own feelings about being assigned books to read. His favorite teacher book is Laura Robb’s Teaching Reading in the Middle School (2000), a book that Franzak (2006) highlights in her review of current paradigms in adolescent literacy instruction as a “hybrid” between reader response and strategic reading approaches “that combines a reading workshop (thereby meeting the need for choice) with focused instruction on reading strategies” (p. 216). As described in Chapter 3 of this dissertation, Connors’ language arts curriculum clearly bears the imprimatur of Robb’s influence, from his commitment to maximizing opportunities for student self-selection of personally interesting texts to his use of the Daybook of Critical Reading and Writing (Claggett, Reid & Vinz, 1999) as a launch pad for reading comprehension strategy instruction. Connors’ own “hybrid” approach was to use students’ self-selected texts as vehicles for scaffolding students’ development in key comprehension strategies such as visualization, drawing personal and intertextual connections, making inferences, and summarizing main and supporting details. Prior to this study during the 2003-2004 school year, I had a number of opportunities to informally observe Connors’ teaching both firsthand and vicariously through the experiences of my sixth grade daughter. What I saw were student work tables arranged into small clusters, daily DEAR sessions that began with mostly quiet (sometimes shared) reading and culminated in lively conversations among peers and between Connors and his students, and frequent challenges to students to exercise newly 142 learned reading strategies in their reading of their self-selected texts. Not only did this structure closely match the Reader’s Workshop approach advocated by Robb and other adolescent literacy advocates, it allowed Connors to execute his teaching in a consultative manner, simultaneously helping individual students more effectively engage with their favored reading materials while guiding readers toward engagement with increasingly difficult text. Connors described his strategy toward guiding his students’ book choices as one of “webbing” children’s reading interests. For instance, suppose a student enjoyed reading A Series of Unfortunate Events. Connors would attempt to determine what aspects of the series captivated the child’s interest, then suggest a different title that might have a similar appeal to the individual: This is about children who have to overcome something. Here is another book, totally unrelated to the Series of Unfortunate Events, that deals with the same thing. And gradually, like we’re doing with writing, your reading interests expand. You can’t just stick with horse books or Series of Unfortunate Events in upper grades, where the reading gets more complex. Connors took a special interest in the reading habits of his male students and felt that, based on his understanding of best practices from his own professional reading, this kind of permeable classroom structure would afford him an excellent opportunity not only to directly support boys’ development as skilled and engaged readers, but also to nurture a classroom community of readers. Such was the state of Connors’ approach to teaching language arts when this study was designed, and Chapter 2 of this dissertation reviews the literature in support of the permeable classroom structure and pedagogical strategies that Connors adopted. 143 Social Disengagement in Practice During the 2004-2005 school year, while Connors continued to articulate his vision of building a community of readers through a pedagogy that privileged student choice and direct instruction in a constellation of reading comprehension strategies that he believed were central to engaged reading, Connors modified both the physical layout and daily routines of his class from what they had been the previous year in order to address classroom management issues. As can be seen in Figures B12 through B15, student tables were no longer clustered into small groups, but rather set end-to-end in rows. Only once were the tables moved into groups, to facilitate a book club-style discussion over students’ Book Project choices, which took place in the middle of the final grading period of the school year. Brief discussions between pairs of students seated at the same table frequently took place during language arts lessons, typically following the Think-Pair-Share format in response to a Daybook prompt. Instead of beginning and ending with text-based peer conversations, DEAR sessions during this school year usually began with a brief announcement that DEAR time was commencing, and ended with the whole class’ attention directed toward the front of the room, where individual students would take turns reciting the schedule for the day. During DEAR time, not only did Connors rarely interact with students, students rarely interacted with each other, Tiger and Montel’s occasional shared readings proving the exception to the rule. Only twice during the observation period did DEAR sessions end with volunteers sharing with the whole class what they had been reading. Ironically, while Connors continued to articulate the value of creating a community of readers, cooperative book club-type activities and text-centered social interactions were minimized in the enacted language arts curriculum. 144 Connors’ more socially disengaged approach may help explain the overall drop in reading attitude scores, as detailed at the end of Chapter 4. While attitudes toward reading for academic purposes dropped significantly among all students over the course of the school year, a statistically significant decline in attitudes toward recreational reading was seen only among girls, while the decline observed among boys did not reach statistical significance. While not reported in the previous chapter, a comparison of the attitude toward recreational reading subscores between Connors’ and his team-teacher Ms. Price’s classrooms may provide additional insights into the overall portrait of student attitudes toward reading. As can be seen in Table A15, while attitudes toward recreational reading dropped among all students in both classes, only the drop among Connors’ homeroom students was statistically significant. What was different in Price’s classroom? Even though she was not the designated language arts teacher, Price did build more socially engaged literacy practices into her classroom. While Connors read two novels aloud over the course of the year (The Bad Beginning and Maniac Magee), Price read six (The Magician 's Nephew, Warriors Don ’t Cry, Number the Stars, Jaguar, Joey Pigza Loses Control and Joey Pigza Swallowed the Key). As shown in Figure B3 (see Chapter 3), Price maintained a reading comer, a small carpeted area with bean bag chairs and special lighting where students could spend DEAR time and which also included a reading recommendation wall, where students (and Ms. Price) routinely posted notecards with comments on books they had read. During the Genre Project, Connors started putting up Post-It notes with the names of books that students had read, but did not continue this after the first wave of student book reports were graded. While none of Price’s classroom activities were observed in the 145 course of this study, these contrasts between the two teachers nonetheless suggest that Price implemented more routines to build a community of readers among her students than did Connors. What this contrast in routines illustrates is that while Connors’ ostensibly permeable language arts curriculum afforded visibility of the texts at the heart of many students’ vernacular literacy practices, it was Price’s class that afforded greater recognition of those texts. As Franzak (2006), Nixon and Comber (2006), Reeves (2004), and Alvermann and Hagood (2000) emphasize, creating classroom contexts that afford the visibility of students’ vernacular literacy practices may help build a bridge between in- and out-of-school literacies, but only a “cursory acknowledgement” of those practices within academic contexts “shortchanges students’ literacy knowledge” (Franzak, p. 224). These authors emphasize the importance of building close relationships with students to see texts through their eyes, “exploring with students the satisfactions these texts provide” (Reeves, p. 258). While Connors voiced a similar position in discussing the value of webbing students’ reading habits, he did not consistently do this with his students. In fact, the only occasion on which Connors was observed discussing text selections with individual students was during the library visits to choose books for the Book Project. Even then, most interactions with students were restricted to the students telling Connors which book they wanted to read and Connors either approving the choice or suggesting that the student seek a different title. This lack of close guidance of students’ text selections as well as the lack of consistent, explicit recognition of a great number of the texts that students made visible within his classroom may have served as a counterweight to the permeable framework 146 that Connors tried to establish, resulting in an unintended level of impermeability that may have factored into the drop in reading attitude scores. Self—determination theory (Ryan & Deci, 2000) provides a framework for understanding how that process might work. Connors’ emphasis on student choice afforded students a sense of autonomy, and to the extent that students chose texts within their independent/instructional range, students’ sense of competence was attended to by the classroom structure. However, the relative lack of recognition afforded students’ visible text choices by Connors as well as the de-emphasis of class routines and activities in which students could engage socially with each other over their text selections may have worked against supporting students’ sense of belonging While in Connors’ classroom. Further, while a permeability to certain kinds of texts at the core of some students’ vernacular literacy practices may have supported some students’ sense of belonging, an impermeability (whether intentional or not) to other kinds of texts may have served to diminish that sense for other students. Print-centrism and the Pedagogy of Control As discussed above, this impermeability manifested itself most strongly in the near total invisibility of the texts of some girls’ vernacular literacy practices within academic contexts, although boys were not immune, especially in terms of recognition of their non-print text selections. Noteworthy in this regard is Connors’ response to the text selections that students made for the culminating activity on visualization skills. While students were encouraged to bring in any kind of text, all of the exercises in the culminating activity were focused entirely on the print, with no discussion of the multimodal aspects of many of the texts that students brought in. While it could be argued that since the purpose of the activity was to assess students’ abilities to visualize 147 from print, the teacher need not have felt obliged to address any other semiotic systems at play in the texts that students brought, completely ignoring the non-print aspects of the texts may send the message to students that those aspects are of lesser value. Considering the fact that these extralinguistic features are the very aspects of the selected texts that many students noted in their journals as being among their top reasons for favoring these texts in the first place, an exclusively print-centric approach to studying these texts undercuts the first purpose of a permeable classroom: to better understand what kinds of texts students engage with in their vernacular literacy practices and to scaffold academic literacy learning on those practices. In their argument for an expanded notion of literacy that embraces multiple semiotic systems and acknowledges the multimodal and transmedial nature of many adolescents’ vernacular literacy practices, King and O’Brien (2002) note that “print represents the essence of control in the classroom” (p. 42). At best, a permeability to just the print aspects of students’ vernacular literacy practices may provide a limited perspective on those practices. Students might make strategic choices to explore the print media texts of their favorite mediascapes within such a classroom and reserve engagement with affiliated non-print media texts to extracurricular contexts. This is what Gatorade and Link did with Star Wars texts and what Tiger and Monte] did with basketball-themed texts. At worst, however, a permeability that is exclusively print- centric or that fails to adequately recognize the funds of knowledge that students bring with them may ultimately result in an impermeable classroom, as the texts that students are most enthusiastic about engaging with are not allowed to become either visible or recognized. Students in this case might opt to make their entire vernacular literacy lives 148 invisible and instead “play school.” This could partially explain why not even the print media manifestations of Ciera S. and SpongeBob’s basketball fandom became visible in Connors’ classroom. Given the importance of non-print texts to a transmedial literacy experience, this relative impermeability to non-print texts means that the classroom was not just impermeable to certain kinds of texts at the heart of many students’ vernacular literacy practices, but that it was to a certain extent impermeable to the practices themselves. In their provocative argument for considering the power of fashion in constructing compelling educational scenarios, Wong & Henriksen (in press) draw a distinction between the objects of fashion and the experience of fashion, noting that while many approaches to making classroom experiences relevant to the lives of young people embrace the former, few take the time to consider how the latter might be incorporated into learning activities to support engagement with academic literacies. That is, while a teacher might build a lesson around a book, movie, song or television program that is popular with his or her students, a failure to consider how students engage with such texts in their non-school lives may prove of limited motivational value in promoting engagement with academic learning. This is the conclusion drawn by Alvermann, Hagood and Huddleston (2004) when reflecting on ways to scaffold academic learning around students’ interests in professional wrestling. Huddleston attempted to discover the elements of professional wrestling and the experience of watching it that most captivated a student with whom he was working, and was able to successfully scaffold the student’s understanding of story grammar and the structure of Shakespearean tragedy by helping the student engage with canonical texts in a manner similar to the ways in which he 149 engaged with wrestling matches. Alvermann and Hagood, on the other hand, attempted to use students’ knowledge base of professional wrestling as an on-rarnp for a traditional reading and writing task, and were disappointed by the students’ lack of engagement with and enthusiasm for the task. The authors conclude that shoehorning the texts of students’ vernacular literacy practices into relatively traditional academic literacy tasks — pouring new wine into old casks — has less potential for promoting student engagement than an approach that honors and builds upon the ways in which students relate to those texts in their vernacular literacy lives. As Smith and Wilhelm (2002, 2006) argue, “it’s not the text, but the context” that matters (p. 55), and they recommend reframing the study of classic literature by appealing to the ways that adolescents engage with pop cultural and vernacular texts in out-of-school contexts, pouring old wine into new casks, in effect. Connors’ print-centric approach to language arts was permeable to a certain range of texts that were important to students’ vernacular literacy practices, but by recognizing few of the multimodal and non-print texts that students engaged with in those practices as well as few of the ways in which students individually and socially negotiated meaning between multiple print and non-print texts, Connors’ language arts curriculum was ultimately impermeable not just to some students’ vernacular texts, but to many students’ vernacular practices. To borrow Wong and Henriksen’s terms, while some objects of fashion became visible in Connors’ classroom, many students’ experience of fashion did not, which may help account for the failure to see increases in students’ attitudes toward reading for either academic or recreational purposes. It should be emphasized that this impermeability was not a barrier intentionally placed by Connors to keep out students’ vernacular literacies, although enacting an 150 unintended pedagogy of control may yield similar motivational results as an overt approach. Rather, the lack of recognition of students’ transmedial literacy practices may be more the result of a lack of attention to media literacy in Connors’ teacher education and professional development. Conspicuously absent from Robb’s Teaching Reading in Middle School, Connors’ “Bible” for language arts instruction, is any mention of teaching with multimodal texts or helping students develop media literacy skills, which could help explain why Connors dealt very little with non-print media in his curriculum. Indeed, as Flores-Koulish (2005) discovered, few teachers emerge from teacher education with an understanding of media literacy, let alone a conceptual framework for teaching it. Given the importance of transmediation in the vernacular literacy practices of the boys in this study, one clear implication for practice is that teacher education must do a better job of addressing the ways in which children and adults synthesize meaning in a multimodal, multitextual mediascape. F lores-Koulish’s study provides a basic model curriculum for media literacy education for pre-service teachers, while the work of Ladislaus Semali (2000, 2002) has developed a detailed approach to teaching for transmediation in teacher education based on a critical literacy framework. Anstey and Bull (2006) and Kist (2005) provide additional models. However, despite these calls for better media literacy education for teachers (and these calls are not new; Semali (2000) traces the history of media literacy advocacy back to the writings of McLuhan and Postman in the early 19605), an informal survey of four of the most recent and heavily promoted teacher education textbooks for reading and language arts instruction reveals a continued print-centric emphasis. While Strickland, Galda and Cullinan (2004) include viewing and visually representing along with reading, 151 writing, listening and speaking among the six language arts, neither of these skills receives a firll chapter treatment as do the other four, devoting only two pages of a chapter on content area literacy to advice on pre— and post-viewing discussion strategies for videos. Unrau’s (2008) new text on content area reading and writing is rooted in a multiliteracies perspective and acknowledges the importance of non-print literacies, yet devotes fewer than four full pages (out of 480) to media literacy. Frey and Fisher’s (2006) only reference to media literacy is in a figure presenting NCA Standards for Speaking, Listening, and Media Literacy in K-12 Education (p. 216). Like the bulk of Unrau’s text, Frey and Fisher devote their discussion solely to reading comprehension strategies with print media. Only Wiseman, Elish-Piper and Wiseman (2005) devote a full, substantial chapter (50 pages) to “Literacy and Visual Representation, Interpretation, and Evaluation,” which addresses basic visual literacy (decoding visual symbols), visual literacy with picture books, computer technology, and multimedia (including television). While resources devoted exclusively to multiliteracies and media literacy are abundant (e. g., Anstey & Bull, 2006; Kist, 2005), unless pre-service teachers have an entire class dedicated to understanding media literacy (as Semali has implemented at Pennsylvania State University), the media literacy education they receive within the context of a larger language arts methods course is not likely to be extensive. Conclusion This study was designed to enrich our understanding of adolescent boys’ extracurricular literacy practices, through a case study of a language arts classroom that was structured around providing students with maximum opportunities to self-select personally interesting texts. A great deal of diversity was found in the set of text 152 selections made by boys and girls across academic and vernacular contexts. While few individual authors or titles could be singled out as “hits,” some trends in genre were apparent. Among boys, the metagenres of imaginative fiction, which includes fantasy, science fiction, horror and supernatural thrillers, and sports, which includes sports fiction, biography, informational text, and reference works, were the most popular. For comparison, among girls, fantasy and realistic fiction were the most frequently selected genres. Boys’ vernacular literacy practices were found to be transmedial in nature and were strongly implicated for many boys in their construction of a social identity. Boys were observed participating in affinity spaces, affiliating themselves with other boys through shared consumption of a variety of print and non-print media texts. While the classroom that was studied was intended to be permeable to the texts of students’ vernacular literacy practices, it was found to be heavily print-centric, which in some cases afforded only a limited perspective on the full range of texts with which students engage in those literacy practices. Ultimately, boys were more likely than girls to take advantage of the permeability of the classroom to explore texts that strongly reflected their transmedial interests. Boys also tended to read and consume texts from a wider range of genres and of print and non-print media than were girls. Despite the openness of the classroom to students’ vernacular literacy lives, there were significant drops in attitude toward reading for academic and recreational purposes over the course of the year. It is suggested that while students did make many of the texts of their vernacular literacies visible within the permeable classroom, the lack of recognition of those texts by the teacher as well as a general level of social disengagement in the classroom may be a factor in the declining attitude scores. 153 A chief limitation of the present study is an underrepresentation in the dataset of text selections of many non-book and non-print texts that played a role in students’ vernacular literacy practices. It is recommended that future inquiries into students’ multimedia literacy habits find ways to factor in those texts in such a way that children’s engagement with them can be usefully compared to that of their other text selections. Another major limitation is the disconnect between the teacher’s stated beliefs about building a community of readers in his classroom and the overall lack of social engagement among students and between teacher and students observed in the classroom. Thus an understanding of what a permeable classroom with a strong social component might reveal about adolescents’ literacy practices and how it might affect students’ attitudes toward reading for a variety of purposes remains incomplete. This study enriches the literature of adolescent literacy by providing empirical support for theories of transmediation as a key feature of adolescent literacy, an exploration of the nexus between young adolescents’ literacy practices and social identity construction, and an illustration of the strong diversity that exists across the textual landscape of young adolescents’ vernacular literacy practices. Future directions for research include developing better ways to account for the non-print literacies in adolescents’ lives and exploring explanations for how certain affinity spaces (such as sports) develop gendered boundaries. Implications for practice include a strong call for teachers to stay current on publications for young adults, to nurture closer relationships with students to better understand the nature and functions of their vernacular literacy practices, and to adopt an expanded understanding of literacy that encompasses both print and non-print semiotic systems. As Thursday Next, the intrepid hero of Jasper Fforde’s 154 eponymous series of science fiction novels, observes “publishing these days doesn’t necessarily restrict books to being just books” (2007, p. 181). Neither should literacy education. 155 APPENDICES 156 APPENDIX A Tables Table A1 Median Household Income Distribution by District Ilium Homestead Birch Creek State Average Less than $15,000 (%) 24.0 14.5 11.3 18.1 $15,000-$29,999 (%) 21.0 16.2 14.5 18.6 $30,000-$49,999 (%) 19.4 18.1 16.4 18.0 $50,000-$74,999 (%) 13.9 15.1 14.3 14.0 $75,000-$99,999 (%) 9.2 12.9 14.0 11.3 $100,000-$l49,999 (%) 10.8 19.5 24.3 16.2 $150,000 or More (%) 1.6 3.7 5.2 3.0 Source: Standard & Poor's, 2003 157 Table A2 Enrollment Distribution by Race/Ethnicity by District Ilium Homestead Asian/Pacific Islander (%) 5.1 5.2 Black (%) 39.4 22.6 Hispanic (%) 14.6 7.1 Native American (%) 1.2 0.7 White (%) 39.6 64.4 Other (%) 0.0 0.0 Source: Standard & Poor's, 2003 158 Birch Creek State Average 0.5 2.9 3.0 0.1 93.5 0.0 Table A3 Text Selections by Gender and Context A — Independent Reading A — DEAR Shared A —- DEAR Solitary A — Genre Project A — Book Project A — Visualization Activity V — Bookstore Browsing V — Self-report Interest V -— Self-report Recreational Totals Boys Girls Totals 12 23 35 21 7 28 56 28 84 89 104 193 22 24 46 19 21 4O 15 23 38 23 16 39 122 47 169 379 293 672 /(9, N = 672) = 47.13, p < 0.05, C = 0.458434 A =Academic context V = Vernacular context 159 Table A4 Most Popular Texts Selected By All Students for Any Purpose, Based on Number of Text Selections Documented for Each Text (in Parentheses) (7) Lemony Snicket, A Series of Unfortunate Events, Book the First: The Bad Beginning (7) Lemony Snicket, A Series of Unfortunate Events, Book the Sixth: The Ersatz Elevator (7) Lemony Snicket, A Series of Unfortunate Events, Book the Second: The Reptile Room (7) Lemony Snicket, A Series of Unfortunate Events, Book the Third: The Wide Window (6) Lemony Snicket, A Series of Unfortunate Events, Book the Fifth: The Austere Academy (6) Lemony Snicket, A Series of Unfortunate Events, Book the Eighth: The Hostile Hospital (6) Lemony Snicket, A Series of Unfortunate Events, Book the Fourth: The Miserable Mill (6) Lemony Snicket, A Series of Unfortunate Events, Book the Seventh: The Vile Village (6) JRR Tolkien, The Hobbit (5) J K Rowling, Harry Potter and the Sorcerer ’s Stone (5) Lemony Snicket, A Series of Unfortunate Events, Book the Ninth: The Carnivorous Carnival (5) Lemony Snicket, A Series of Unfortunate Events, unspecified volumes (5) RL Stine, Goosebumps, unspecified volumes (5) Heinrich Bauer North America, J-I4 Magazine (5) Time Inc. Magazine Company, Sports Illustrated 160 Table A5 Most Popular Authors Selected By All Students For Any Purpose, Based on Number of Text Selections Documented for Each Author (in Parentheses) (72) Lemony Snicket (33) Jonathan Rand (20) J K Rowling (18) RL Stine (11) Matt Christopher (10) Roald Dahl (9) Jack Gantos (9) Gordon Korman (9) JRR Tolkien (7) Dav Pilkey (7) Time Inc. Magazine Company (6) Eoin Colfer (6) CS Lewis (5) Beverly Cleary (5) Electronic Arts (5) Heinrich Bauer North America (5) Phyllis Reynolds Naylor (5) Louis Sachar (5) Jude Watson 161 Table A6 Distribution of Focal Students ' Text Selections by Genre, Gender and Academ ic/ Vernacular Context Boy Boy Girl Girl Vernacular Academic Vernacular Academic Totals Action 16 0 0 0 16 Animals 2 3 2 5 12 Automotive 4 0 0 0 4 Bible/Inspirational 0 3 0 l 4 Biography 1 10 3 15 29 Comedy 7 l 0 O 8 Comic Book 4 3 l 0 8 Fantasy 49 24 36 44 153 Gaming 2 3 0 O 5 General Non-Fiction 5 l 2 4 12 Historical Fiction 3 l3 3 1 l 30 History 1 l O 0 2 Multiple Genres 0 0 1 0 1 Mystery/Thriller 15 16 3 5 39 Poetry 0 0 0 l 1 Popular Culture 0 0 2 3 S Realistic Fiction 16 21 29 22 88 Reality/ Simulation 5 0 0 0 5 Reference 5 O 0 1 6 Science Fiction 12 8 2 3 25 Sports l3 l9 2 2 36 Totals 160 126 86 117 489 [760, N = 489) = 194.2599, p < 0. 05, «DC = 0.36 162 Table A7 Fog Readability Levels of Focal Students’ Text Selections by Gender Academic/Vernacular Context and Boy Boy Girl Girl Vernacular Academic Vernacular Academic Mean 8.29 6.91 7.63 7.89 Standard Deviation 1.98 1.64 1.80 1.71 N 60 69 60 76 T-test Comparisons (two-sample assuming gual variances) Boy Vernacular to Boy Academic: t(127) = 4.33, p < 0. 05, d = 0.76 Girl Vernacular to Girl Academic: t(134) = -0.87, p >0. 05, d = -0.15 163 Table A8 F lesch-Kincaid Readability Levels of Focal Students’ Text Selections by Gender and Academic/Vernacular Context Boy Boy Girl Girl Vernacular Academic Vernacular Academic Mean 6.11 5.02 5.82 6.00 Standard Deviation 1.78 1.33 1.47 1.40 N 80 72 67 81 T-test Comparisons (two-sample assuming equal variances) Boy Academic to Boy Vernacular: t(150) = 4.28, p < 0.05, d = 0.69 Girl Academic to Girl Vernacular: t(146) = -0. 76, p > 0.05, d = -0.13 164 Table A9 Distribution of Focal Students’ Text Selections by Medium, Gender and Academic/ Vernacular Context Boy I Boy Girl Girl Vernacular Academic Vernacular Academic Totals Book 103 117 81 111 412 Game 1 9 0 0 1 20 Magazine 6 8 2 4 20 MovieNideo 21 l 3 O 25 Short Story 0 0 0 1 1 TV Show 1 1 0 0 0 11 Totals 160 126 86 1 17 489 {(15, N = 489) = 105.87, p < 0.05, C= 0.27 165 “-Vl'. Table A10 Distribution of Focal Students ’ Text Selections by Series, Gender and Academic/ Vernacular Context Boy Boy ‘ Girl Girl Vernacular Academic Vernacular Academic Totals Series 102 55 50 62 269 Non-Series 58 71 36 55 220 Totals 160 126 86 1 17 489 12(3, N = 489) = 12.04, p < 0.05, ac: 0.16 166 Table A1 1 ERAS Recreational and Academic Subscale Comparisons by Gender, Fall 2004 ERAS Recreational Subscale, Fall 2004 Girls Boys Mean 31.75 27.13 Standard Deviation 5.87 6.64 N l7 l9 t(34) = 2.20, p < 0.05, d = 0.74 ERAS Academic Subscale, Fall 2004 Girls Boys Mean 29 27.71 Standard Deviation 5.63 6.77 N 17 19 t(34) = 0.62, p > 0. 05, d = 0.21 167 Table A12 ERAS Recreational and Academic Subscale Comparisons by Gender, Spring 2005 ERAS Recreational Subscale, Spring 2005 Girls Boys Mean 29.77 24.95 Standard Deviation 6.31 5.77 N 17 19 t(34) = 2.40, p < 0.05, d = 0.80 ERAS Academic Subscale, Spring 2005 Girls Boys Mean 26.15 24.37 Standard Deviation 5.11 5.62 N 17 19 t(34) = 0.93, p > 0. 05, d = 0.33 168 Table A13 Fall/Spring ERAS Recreational Subscale Comparisons by Gender ERAS Recreational Subscale, Girls Mean Standard Deviation N t(16) = 1.77,p < 0.05, d = 0.32 ERAS Recreational Subscale, Boys Mean Standard Deviation N t(18) = 1.32,p > 0.05, d = 0.35 Fall Spring 31.75 29.77 5.87 6.31 17 17 Fall Spring 27.13 24.95 6.64 5.77 l9 19 169 Table A14 Fall/Spring ERAS Academic Subscale Comparisons by Gender ERAS Academic Subscale, Girls Fall Spring Mean 29 26.15 Standard Deviation 5.63 5.83 N l7 l7 t(16) = 2.73, p < 0.05, d = 0.50 ERAS Academic Subscale, Boys Fall Spring Mean 27.71 24.37 Standard Deviation 6.77 5.62 N 19 19 t(18) = 2.26, p < 0.05, d = 0.54 170 Table A l 5 F all/Spring ERAS Recreational Subscale Comparisons by Homeroom ERAS Recreational Subscale, Connors, by administration Fall Spring Mean 29.8 1 25 .94 Standard Deviation 5.69 6.09 N 16 1 6 t(15) = 3.13, p < 0.05, d = 0.66 ERAS Recreational Subscale, Price, by administration Fall Spring Mean 28 .91 28.26 Standard Deviation 7.41 6.65 N 20 20 t(19) = 0.44, p > 0. 05, d = 0.09 171 APPENDIX B Figures Figure Bl Classroom library of Mr. Ed Connors 172 Figure B2 Classroom library of Ms. Julie Price 173 Figure B3 Reading comer in Ms. Julie Price ’s classroom 174 Figure B4 Visualization quiz administered by Mr. Connors ’- WW 1,. l' . l' 1) What day of the week did Danny and hlS father fly the kite? 2) Where did they get the string to fly the kite? 3) Where did they release the kite7 4) What was the kite made of? 5) How long did the kite stay up in the our7 175 Figure B5 Genre Project assignment sheet Qenre Book Reviews A genre is a type of book. The following are different genres of books: realistic fiction, science (fiction, historical fiction, biography, and fantasy. Below are a description of each genre. Realistic Fiction: The story happens now, it is not true, and it could happen. Science Fiction: The story happens in the future, it is not true, and it includes futuristic technology. Historical Fiction: The story happened at least 25 years ago, it is not true, and it could have happened. Biography: This story is true and is written by an author about r“ another person. Fantasy: The story could happen at anytime, it is not true, it often includes magic and special powers. For the second marking term each student is expected to read one book from each genre. Each book must be at least 100 pages except for the biography which must be at least 60 pages. When the student has finished reading their book, they must complete a genre book review form. All forms will be due Friday February 18. The grading will be as follows: 100%: 5 books, 90% = 4 books, 80%: 3 books, 70% = 2 books, 60% = 1 book, 0%: 0 books. 176 _ Figure B6 Sample book review form for all genres except biography, completed by Ciera S., a focal student Li“ Name: Book: L' w' ~. U’“" " Genre 3- ”'4‘ G d W Page: V33 ,, 3&0 l Date: \\ W 1) What was the main setting of the story? (Time and Place the story happened);“r immune“ b“"“} We" id 32“)? '3 m Si ”libaofi and ROCL «CUM» 3"“ 530.933 .-,- '3“; W) Li (Y 3, M) “i (‘1 \\ \9 3‘... 3. 3.3.33"; ism l? tt i)“ 1, ()fik {if i‘fh 1 'y QT (:(‘KK «9 C1 {\gk h“ \f) {3 (31.9“ o 177 Figure B7 Continued fi'om Figure B6, back side of sample book review form 2 o \ ‘ I 4) How was the problem solved? M Q“ 3* ‘43); if? ¥:SC)O mm a w w mu W m 3‘ OVU’ XAOW \{erét X0 SCkQJ\ Qfik Wm} [3“ W W m Secong \Q(o\3\€m WAS QVCA \A . N Am 9 «7‘ .\ “Do/x Q“ Obi U \‘<\ K) . '\\’QE \ Qo‘ KR *0 c N 5) List five connections that you had to the story. ‘ Lil miiy riaeci HAW) N. so so 3; O l ))L\<\( kaofih’c '?\\\w’i \0V *0 SUV“ (mo 1 MW poqie. 3. \ , o\- 0\ 5d“ 2: / J LMV AA" +0 ”pity jéé fin/(LAO 5""7/ I {Q WicrrlJW'Ul ? Q ‘ \) I¥i fifq-(flh “A L {M1951 “H: Pia/\o S/wk: fi\ v s *% M5“ *cr Cm M~J$ekfi