11...? 5.. :39“? .1 LIBRARY Michigan State Universrty This is to certify that the I dissertation entitled THE TRANSGRESSION, REVERSE PREFAVOR, AND CONCESSION COMPLIANCE GAIN ING STRATEGIES IN EXCHANGE RELATIONSHIPS presented by Colin Richard Baker has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for the PhD degree in Communication p—KML. Maj Protessor’s Signature I). - I 3 - 200 7 / Date MSU is an affirmative-action, equal-opportunity employer PLACE IN RETURN BOX to remove this checkout from your record. TO AVOID FINES return on or before date due. MAY BE RECALLED with earlier due date if requested. DAIEDUE DAIEDUE DAIEDUE 6/07 p:/ClRC/DateDue.indd-p.1 THE TRANSGRESSION, REVERSE PREFAVOR, AND CONCESSION COMPLIANCE GAINING STRATEGIES IN EXCHANGE RELATIONSHIPS By Colin Richard Baker A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University In partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Communication 2007 ABSTRACT THE TRANSGRESSION, REVERSE PREFAVOR, AND CONCESSION COMPLIANCE GAIN ING STRATEGIES IN EXCHANGE RELATIONSHIPS By Colin Richard Baker This study presents and tests two new compliance gaining strategies: the reverse prefavor and the transgression, steeped in the family of compliance strategies utilizing the concession and based on the norm of reciprocity. The reverse prefavor is a strategy first requiring compliance agents to gain compliance on a small initial request and then offers a concession on a product or service framed as a repayment for the prior compliance target’s help. The transgression is similar; however, it is based on the concession as an apology rather than as reward. Also tested was an expectation of compensation induction. An experimental survey methodology was utilized, which allowed for the measurement of motivations to comply with the initial request and other variables expected to mediate the relationship between the strategies and compliance. Results indicate that several variables had significant relationships (viewing the concession as a reward, viewing the concession as apology, and empathy) with compliance. Sympathy and anticipation of compensation were found to have mediated relationships with target compliance. Copyright by Colin Richard Baker 2007 For Tiffany. For whom I have no egoistic motivations. And for whom I strive to anticipate every want, and when I fail, she need not employ complex compliance strategies, but merely a direct request. iv ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS As I reflect on the process of writing a dissertation and those who helped me to complete the document, it has become apparent that this project was merely one of many important experiences culminating in my view of the world and in my abilities as a scholar. In this regard, there are many people who I wish to acknowledge, and weighing their contribution is a chore that at this point I am unwilling to undertake. So I recognize them in no particular order. My advisor, Dr. William A. Donohue, demonstrated a level of flexibility and encouragement which, in my experience, is rare among distinguished faculty. My conversations with him regarding behavior, cognition, and the interactive nature of communication have led to significant reflection regarding the nature of human communication inquiry. He was also a champion for me to the end and without his encouragement I may still be writing. Dr. Franklin J. Boster served on committees for both my master’s and doctoral degrees. I am hard pressed to think of anyone who has contributed more to my educational experiences, perhaps reflected most in my dissertation topic. He has been my instructor for numerous classes and instructor of record where I was teaching assistant, collaborated with me on both ad hoc and formal research projects, endured many committee meetings and read many manuscripts, and provided me with numerous memorable instances of mentorship. I am troubled that my recognition of his efforts and influence here is likely to be the only formal recognition that he will receive. Truly, more is deserved. My outside committee member, Dr. Jennifer Butler-Ellis has been extremely helpful to me as a role model and in giving valuable advice. She has always been tactful, yet honest and this approach enabled me to make informed decisions even if I didn’t always make the best ones. I would encourage those who follow me to listen to her more than 1. She also gave valuable comments on my dissertation that resulted in a much more polished and well developed product in theoretical and methodological terms. I had the opportunity to observe Dr. Hee Sun Park begin her employment at Michigan State University and develop into an amazingly productive scholar. She has demonstrated the direct correlation between hard work and success and that learning is not complete when classes are. She has also been very supportive and encouraging of me throughout the process of writing my dissertation and during graduate school in general. I also wish to acknowledge the influence of Dr. Vernon D. Miller who taught how to write and to carefully consider the power of words. In my time as graduate student Marge Barkman was a friend, surrogate mother, and helpful staff member all in equal measure. Without her patient persistence I would certainly have some outstanding requirement. Dr. Mathew Seeger, my current department chair demonstrated a level of patience and support throughout the dissertation process that was instrumental in my success. There are others who, without their help, I would have not been able to complete this project. Michael R. Krofi aided me in correcting several technical issues, and Dr. Patric Spence and Dr. Kenneth Lachlan helped me in acquiring subjects. Tiffany Baker, my loving wife, has acted as editor on this and every other manuscript I have written. Without their help I would still be typing computer code, recruiting subjects and editing. vi TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES ............................................................................................................. ix LIST OF FIGURES ............................................................................................................ x CHAPTER I ........................................................................................................................ 1 Norm of reciprocity ............................................................................................................. 5 Norm of Reciprocity as Social Influence ........................................................................ 8 Giving and Receiving Favors ....................................................................................... l l Concession as Reward: The Reverse Prefavor ............................................................. 15 Concession as Apology: The Transgression ................................................................. 17 Test of the Model ........................................................................................................... 23 Alternative Responses to the Compliance Request ....................................................... 25 CHAPTER II ..................................................................................................................... 27 METHOD ......................................................................................................................... 27 Design ........................................................................................................................... 27 Participants ................................................................................................................... 27 Procedure ...................................................................................................................... 28 Scenario Inductions ...................................................................................................... 3O Instrumentation ............................................................................................................. 34 CHAPTER III ................................................................................................................... 39 RESULTS ......................................................................................................................... 39 Motivations ................................................................................................................... 48 Tests of the Proposed Models ....................................................................................... 50 CHAPTER IV ................................................................................................................... 59 DISCUSSION ................................................................................................................... 59 Viewing concession as reward and apology ................................................................. 60 Implications for the norm of reciprocity and mutual concessions ................................ 61 Initial non-compliers ..................................................................................................... 66 Limitations and future research .................................................................................... 68 REFERENCES ................................................................................................................. 81 Appendix A ....................................................................................................................... 71 Motivation to gain rewards scale .................................................................................. 71 Appendix B ....................................................................................................................... 72 Motivation to avoid punishment scale .......................................................................... 72 Appendix C ....................................................................................................................... 73 vii Empathy scale ............................................................................................................... 73 Appendix D ....................................................................................................................... 74 Sympathy scale ............................................................................................................. 74 Appendix E ....................................................................................................................... 75 Reward seeking scale .................................................................................................... 75 Appendix F ........................................................................................................................ 76 Rectification seeking scale ............................................................................................ 76 Appendix G ....................................................................................................................... 77 Concession viewed as reward scale .............................................................................. 77 Appendix H ....................................................................................................................... 78 Concession viewed as apology scale ............................................................................ 78 Appendix I ........................................................................................................................ 79 Distress scale ................................................................................................................. 79 Appendix J ........................................................................................................................ 80 Compliance Items ......................................................................................................... 80 viii LIST OF TABLES Table 1: Study design and number of subjects per condition. .......................................... 27 Table 2. Mean, standard deviations, medians and percent of subjects complying with target request as measured by number of tickets purchased. .................................... 40 Table 3. Mean, standard deviations, and medians of subjects complying with target request as measured by Likert compliance scale. ..................................................... 41 Table 4. Descriptives for motivations to help by condition. ............................................ 49 Table 5. Motivation to avoid material punishments: Correlations, corrected correlations and path coefficients. ................................................................................................ 51 Table 6. Motivation to gain material rewards-Likert measure of compliance: Correlations, corrected correlations and path coefficients. ............................................................ 52 Table 7. Motivation to gain material rewards-dichotomous measure of compliance: Correlations, corrected correlations and path coefficients. ....................................... 53 Table 8. Motivation to gain material rewards-Likert measure of compliance, for anticipation of compensation conditions only: Correlations, corrected correlations and path coefficients. ................................................................................................ 54 Table 9. Results for predicted null model of the effect of motivation to reduce negative affect and altruistic helping on compliance: Correlations, corrected correlations and path coefficients. ....................................................................................................... 56 Table 10. Results for the revised model of the effect of altruistic helping on compliance: Correlations, corrected correlations and path coefficients ........................................ 57 ix LIST OF FIGURES Figure I. Heuristic Model of the effect of the Transgression and Reverse Prefavor compliance strategies and their relationship with compliance. ................................ 22 Figure 2. Predicted mediation model of motivation to avoid material punishments- compliance relationship. ........................................................................................... 23 Figure 3. Predicted mediation model of motivation to gain material rewards-compliance relationship ................................................................................................................ 24 Figure 4. Predicted null model of the effect of motivation to reduce negative affect and altruistic helping on compliance. .............................................................................. 24 Figure 5. Proposed model results of motivation to avoid material punishments- compliance relationship. ........................................................................................... 51 Figure 6. Proposed model results of motivation to gain material rewards-Likert measure of compliance relationship. ....................................................................................... 53 Figure 7. Proposed model results of motivation to gain material rewards-dichotomous measure of compliance relationship .......................................................................... 53 Figure 8. Model results of anticipation of compensation motivation to gain material rewards-Likert measure of compliance relationship. ................................................ 54 Figure 9. Results for predicted null model of the effect of motivation to reduce negative affect and altruistic helping on compliance .............................................................. 56 Figure 10. Results for the revised model of the effect of altruistic helping on compliance ................................................................................................................................... 58 CHAPTER I The Norm of Reciprocity is ofien proposed to be the underlying mechanism for the effectiveness of several compliance gaining strategies such as the unsolicited prefavor (i.e., Boster, Rodriguez, Cruz, & Marshall, 1995; Regan, 1971), door-in-the-face strategy (Cialdini et al., 1975) and that’s-not-all technique, (Cialdini, 2004). It takes the general form of reciprocal concessions where a compliance target feels a normative obligation to reciprocate the goodwill concessions of a compliance agent with a concession of their own, thereby eliciting higher rates of compliance. For example, door-in-the-face is a strategy where a compliance agent retreats from an extreme request likely to be rejected to the more conservative target request (Cialdini et al., 1975). Salespeople often use the operating mechanisms of this principle by offering a sale on certain items such as, “I want to sell a car to you today, so I will knock off $200 dollars.” Because the salesperson makes a concession, the compliance target is believed to feel obligated to also make a concession, and may purchase an item that he/she did not particularly want or pay more for the item than desired. As articulated by Cialdini (2001), the norm of reciprocity is a particularly powerful mechanism in social influence strategies because it allows the influencer to choose both the favor (or concession) and the way in which the favor is repaid. Gouldner (1960) views the norm of reciprocity process as a universal (though not situationally invariable) tendency to accept favors, to give favors, and to return favors. The norm of reciprocity has been shown to have great effects on the positive reciprocation of favors (i.e., Regan, 1971) and the negative reciprocation of transgressions (i.e., Eisenberger, Lynch, Aselage, & Rohdieck, 2004). In general, the obligation explanation for the utility of reciprocal concessions addresses just one of these three features of the norm of reciprocity as detailed by Gouldner — the obligation to return favors. Surprisingly, little attention has been given to other pertinent issues described by the norm of reciprocity, namely, the propensity to give favors, and the compulsion to accept favors within the compliance gaining literature. An important presumption for the effectiveness of reciprocal concessions as a compliance strategy is that the target must view the behaviors enacted by the compliance agent as a legitimate concession (Cialdini, 1975). According to Cialdini (2002) the authenticity of the concession is not merely a function of the target’s perception of the concession’s cost but also the target’s beliefs regarding the compliance agent’s motives. Consumers are becoming much more savvy to the strategies used to gain their compliance and are cued especially in sales situations where defensive mechanisms are activated (Cialdini, 1975). By introducing a plausible explanation for the sale or concession, a compliance seeker may reduce the possibility that defensive mechanisms are engaged and capitalize on the norm of reciprocity functions of the obligation to give favors and accept favors. The central focus of this research is to investigate the utility of reciprocal concessions in strategies that will increase the likelihood of compliance with a request. The newly developed strategies presented attempt to deactivate the defensive mechanisms by invoking the functions of the norm of reciprocity. The general form of the typical reciprocal concessions message is “I did this for you now you should do this for me in repayment.” Yet to be investigated, however, is whether this works in the opposite direction. That is, “You did this for me now you should let me do this for you in repayment.” The enactment of a service by the target can be made in several different contexts. Two contexts are proposed here: one in which the helping behavior is other- oriented (the reverse prefavor), where a target may make a decision on whether to engage in helping behavior intended to bring relief to the compliance agent; the other is ego- oriented (the transgression) where compliance targets are all but required to help the other because, by not doing so, they hurt themselves. These strategies are then followed by a concessionary compliance message framed as repayment for the compliance target’s prior act of kindness. This research has both theoretical and practical value. Exploring all facets of the norm of reciprocity will help us to gain a more complete understanding of human behavior. Specifically, one’s obligations to accept favors and to give favors have not been addressed directly in the compliance gaining literature. Additionally, some researchers have questioned the utility of the obligation explanation articulated by the norm of reciprocity to explain the effect of a concession on compliance. The development of alternative uses of obligation as an explanation for compliance may help to uncover intricacies in its use as an explanatory mechanism or may provide more evidence suggesting further explication of alternative causal explanations. Practical implications of this research are also significant. The identification of a set of influence strategies that further expand the repertoire of tools available to a person is particularly important in settings where behavior is governed largely by exchange. Such relationships exist in organizations, sales situations, and the interactions of strangers (Clark, 1981; Clark & Mills, 1979; Mills & Clark, 1982). As Regan (1971) notes, “[there are] many situations where we want compliance or assistance from another person, but either do not have or choose not to use the resources which would put a large amount of pressure on him” (p.627). This seems particularly important when future interaction is anticipated or desired. In terms of organizational research, this program of study is particularly relevant. Exchange relationships within organizations can be identified at the dyadic, group and organizational levels. According to Organ (1988) perceptions of fairness may lead someone to view their relationship with the organization as one of social exchange rather than strictly economic exchange. This shift can bring about extra role behaviors (organizational citizenship behaviors) that may be directed toward a specific person, the work group, or the organization (Moonnan, 1991). Alternatively, organizational citizenship behaviors may be enacted in response to competitive work environments and lead to the expectation by peers and supervisors that an employee continue enacting these extra role behaviors (VanDyne & Ellis, 2004). As such, feelings of under compensation in the exchange relationship can eventually lead to a state of psychological reactance. Additionally, individuals may have psychological contracts within organizations that are contractually grounded obligations based on exchange. Psychological contracts are typically thought to be between the organization and individual members (Morrison & Robinson, 1997) and are determined by beliefs about the reciprocal obligations between the parties. Much of the work on psychological contracts has emphasized the negative emotional (Robinson & Rousseau, 1994; Tumley & F eldman, 1999) and behavioral (Robinson, 1996) ramifications of psychological contract breach; however, more recent work has begun to imply outcomes when an employee is overcompensated by the organization and the reciprocal benefits the organization may invoke (Ellis, 2007). Moreover, Ho (2005) suggested that positional peers’ and friends’ perceptions of psychological contract fulfillment influence our own views of fulfillment. Graen and Uhl-Bien (1995) also noted that the relationships between superiors and subordinates are inherently exchange based until (if) relationships are developed. This suggests that traditional strategies by leaders (managers) of social influence and legitimated power must be utilized until a more transactional (communal) relationship is developed. Similarly, work on team-member exchange (Seers, Petty, & Cashman, 1995) has identified relational quality among peers as producing and reproducing the context in which the task and social aspects of work take place. They also suggest that it is the reciprocally exchanged behaviors between the team and the individual that reinforce both the member’s role and the identity of a group as a team. In both team-member exchange and leader-member exchange, “the reciprocity must be analyzed in terms of the resources each party may bring to bear on the exchange process,” (Seers, et al., p. 21) and thus, the acceptance, giving, and returning of resources is critical in role management, role negotiation and relationship development within organizations. In sum, relationships within organizations require that, until transactive (communal) relationships are developed, organizational members rely primarily on exchange to negotiate organizational life. As such, social influence strategies that rely on exchange may be productive in gaining desired outcomes. Norm of reciprocity Gouldner (1960) suggested that the norm of reciprocity, in its universal form, makes two interrelated, minimal demands: 1) people should help those who have helped them, and 2) people should not injure those who help them (p.171). The identification and elucidation of the norm of reciprocity grew out of the fimctionalist perspective of sociology. Functional theory is concerned with the problems of how individual actors are prepared by socialization to play a role in social systems. The functional theory, first developed by Durkheim, and more recently Parsons, has at a minimum three main concepts. First, society is viewed as a collection of interdependent parts with a tendency toward equilibrium. Second, there exist functional requirements that must be met in a society to ensure its survival. And third, functional phenomena are seen to exist because they serve a function (this assumption was questioned by Gouldner). Accordingly, functionalists view persons as having expectations of others’ actions and reactions to their own behaviors, and these expectations are derived from accepted norms and values of society. Social norms are generally accepted and agreed upon, and as the behaviors are repeated, these expectations are accepted as roles. While Gouldner was not the first to notice the norm, he was the first to present a definition for the norm and describe it in detail rather than limiting himself to a discussion of the frequency of its existence (e.g., Thumwald, 1932) or its importance in holding together the fabric of mankind (e.g., Simmel, 1950). Gouldner credits such issues to his contemporaries, such as Becker (1956) who referred to man as Homo reciprocus, and to his predecessors like Hobhouse (1906) who, according to Gouldner, stated that “Reciprocity... is the vital principle of society,” and Thumwald (1932) who indicated that the “principle of reciprocity pervades every relation of primitive life.” This statement perhaps foreshadowed work to be conducted 40 years later in research of evolutionary biology (Trivers, 1971), which contends that reciprocal tendencies are hardwired into our brains through natural selection for altruistic and reciprocal behaviors. The writings of Gouldner, Homans, Thaibaut and Kelly, and Blau are generally credited with a shifi in thinking from a functional perspective to a social exchange approach to understanding human relationships. The norm of reciprocity, as described by Gouldner (1960) and his contemporaries, suggests that the norm functions to stabilize societies. It does so because one who receives a favor is obliged to repay the favor, and while they are in debt, they are morally obliged to not harm the favor giver. The norm, however, does not suggest that favors will be reciprocated in a value-for-value sense, but rather without strict measure. From a societal stability standpoint, this serves to make the fulfillment of debts somewhat fluid and unknown to the parties and therefore creates stability. It would be foolish to bring harm to someone owing us a debt, and if we owe, we are morally bound to not harm those who have provided favors. To the extent that we are unsure whether a favor has been paid or repaid, we will not harm. By not following the norm and therefore not repaying favors, the taker may invite social sanctions from the favor giver and observers of the interaction by way of ostrication from the system. As ‘ described by Gouldner, the norm of reciprocity functions across social classes and thereby functions to stabilize society by reducing the proclivity of those with higher social status from taking advantage of those with less power. The norm is flexible in terms of the timing of the repayment of favors and the extent to which people are expected to reciprocate based on their access to resources, societal position, and lot in life. In sum, because of a variety of social functions, the norm of reciprocity as proposed by sociologists is a moral norm obligating the recipient to return a favor previously given. In recognition of the ubiquitous nature of the norm of reciprocity and the power with which it governs human behavior during social interaction, it has been a pervasively utilized strategy of social influence (Cialdini, 2001). Norm of Reciprocity as Social Influence Cialdini (2001) described the norm of reciprocity as a form of influence jujitsu. His metaphor of click then whirr further illustrates his belief that the norm of reciprocity is an ingrained mechanical rule of human behavior. When we are afforded a favor, it is in many ways unconscionable that we refuse to reciprocate. Using the rule as an influence strategy allows one actor to both decide the favor and also the form of the repayment (Cialdini, 2001). Indeed, the norm of reciprocity and its reciprocal concessions counterpart have been proposed to be the underlying mechanism in the effectiveness of some of our best documented influence strategies, such as the unsolicited favor and the door-in-the-face strategy. According to Cialdini, the reciprocation rule functions to instigate mutual concessions in two ways. First, it obliges a favor recipient to respond in kind and second, because of this requirement, “people are freed to make the initial concession and thereby to begin the beneficial process of exchange” (p. 37). He based these conclusions on the notion that during social interaction, participants often begin with positions that are unacceptable to one another and that for common goals to be met, compromise must be achieved (Cialdini, 1975). In order for someone to retreat from an opening position, an “implicit prescription” of mutual concessions is essential. Without this rule guiding behaviors, attempts at compromise would not be initiated and the interaction would not continue. His argument includes evidence from the negotiation literature, which finds that subjects increase their rates of concessions as their opponent’s concessions increase (Chertkoff & Conley, 1967; Benton, Kelly & Liebling, 1972; Komorita & Brenner, 1968), that extreme unmoving positions are met by subjects’ refusal to move from their own positions (Benton, et al., 1972), and that even fair and equitable initial offers are refused by subjects when confederates would not move from them (Komorita & Brenner, 1968). These arguments taken together suggest that the retreat from an extreme position to a more moderate position may induce a target to move from their own initial position of refusal to one of compliance. While meta-analysis has largely confirmed the effectiveness of the rej ection-then-retreat compliance strategy (Dillard, Hunter, & Burgoon, 1984; Fern, Monroe, & Avila, 1986; O'Keefe, 1998), others have suggested that this effect can be explained by guilt (O’Keefe & Figge’, 1999), the prosocialness of the organization, single or multiple requestors, and magnitude of the concession (Dillard, 1991; Dillard, et al., 1984; O'Keefe & F igge', 1997).. More recently, Hale and Laliker (1999) argued against these criticisms of the mutual concessions explanation. In a direct test of mutual concessions and what they termed the social responsibility explanation, Turner, Tamborini, Limon, and Zuckerman-Hyman (2007) found support for mutual concessions particularly among strangers. Dillard (1991) estimated that the mean effect size (r) for the door-in-the-face ranged from .15 to .25 in previous meta-analysis. Thus, the DITF strategy provides a favorable comparison strategy for new influence strategies of this ilk. Obligation is a powerful motivator. As such, it has often been used as an explanation for the effect of an unsolicited favor to remit compliance. One such study frequently cited was conducted by Regan (1971), who sought to understand whether the effectiveness of a prefavor on compliance was because of obligation or whether it could be attributed to increased liking for the favor doer. Regan recognized that by giving a favor, both things may occur simultaneously. Regan conducted the experiment in a lab setting where the participant received a soda in the favor conditions and where liking (or more precisely, disliking) was independently manipulated. The dependent variable of compliance was measured by the number of raffle tickets subsequently purchased by the subject from the confederate. According to Regan, the results of the study indicated that subjects in the favor condition purchased significantly more tickets than those in other conditions. Interestingly, liking was not correlated with the number of tickets sold in the favor condition, but liking was correlated with the number of tickets sold in the no favor and control conditions. This suggests, according to the author, a qualified conclusion that liking may be irrelevant when a favor is given prior to a request. The author concluded that by providing a target with an unsolicited favor, a compliance agent could increase compliance with a subsequent request, and the effect was at least in part due to felt obligation as would be predicted by the mechanisms operating in the norm of reciprocity. Similar conclusions were drawn by Cialdini (2001) in his report of a field study in which he observed the persuasive strategies of the Hare Krishna religious sect. These enterprising influencers would give a flower to someone walking by and then ask for a donation to their cause. Cialdini estimated that in this way, the organization was able to increase donations substantially because of the reciprocation rule. Many passersby would avoid the Krishnas by walking well out of their way, but even in these cases, the Krishnas’ tactics were often successful in obtaining a donation. Cialdini concluded that by first doing us a favor, strange, disliked or unwelcome others can enhance the likelihood that we will comply with their request. Also interesting here is that targets are 10 often aware of the expectations that accompany the receipt of a gift and may actively avoid entering into exchange relationships with others. The target of an unsolicited favor does not actively seek to enter into an exchange relationship with a favor giver but rather is forced into a debt. Targets may attempt to avoid entering into the exchange by refusing the favor at the outset of the interaction while others may diminish the value of the favor. Cialdini (2001) suggested that targets may reframe the favor as the social influence strategy that it is, and thereby attain freedom from feelings of obligation. Other research suggests that when we receive an unsolicited favor we may engage in psychological reactance (Brehm, 1966). When we believe our freedoms are limited by, perhaps, our emotions of obligation or indebtedness as a consequence of accepting a prior favor, we may respond by directly opposing the agent’s request and thereby regaining our freedom to choose. Combined, these issues indicate several active and passive defenses that may be exhibited by a compliance target. In order to improve on the concession as a compliance strategy it may be beneficial to attempt to increase the occurrence in which targets enter freely into a mutual exchange, to reduce the proclivity of a compliance target to foresee the subsequent compliance message, and to accentuate the exchange nature of the relationship. Many of these issues are difficult to overcome using the unsolicited favor strategy and other mutual concessions-based strategies; however, many of these practical problems are eliminated with a sequential strategy that calls for the target to make the first concession. Giving and Receiving Favors An alternative approach that capitalizes on other aspects of Gouldner’s (1960) ideas may focus not only on our dutiful obligation to repay debts, but also to give and 11 receive favors. By exploring these additional aspects of the norm of reciprocity, we will gain valuable insight regarding the utility of the norm of reciprocity to explain findings and make predictions in social influence contexts. According to Goffman (1971), the ritual of a favor exchange is an episode that “asks license of a potentially offended person to engage in what could be considered a violation of his rights at the same time (the receiver) exposes himself to denial and rejection” (p.114). Asking for a favor places the receiver in a position of vulnerability and it is the role of the receiver to express gratitude (Flynn, 2003). This is accomplished by emphasizing politeness in the favor request, through the use of nonverbal cues such as smiling, and by thanking the favor giver. These behaviors and feelings are markedly different than those of the giver. Goffman (1955) explained that instead of reacting with annoyance and frustration, the giver is obligated to reestablish social order in the relationship and to “save” the potentially embarrassing situation. According to Flynn (2003) this is accomplished by offering compliance without observable hesitation, refraining from reminding the receiver of the favor, and by making statements that diminish the worth of the favor. Besides the enactment of specific roles during the giving and receiving episode, other research has addressed the reasons why someone might engage in performing favors. Baston and Shaw (1991) summarized several decades of literature on this topic by proposing that there are perhaps both egoistic and altruistic motives for giving aid. The corresponding internal responses to seeing someone in need of aid will instigate distinct motivational states, and the decision to help is based on a relative benefit/cost calculation by weighing the cost of helping versus the benefit (either self-or other-oriented) of 12 helping the needy individual. Their research suggests that there are two broad forms of egoistic motivations: arousal reducing egoistic motivations and reward seeking/punishment avoiding egoistic motivations. First the egoistic motivations to seek rewards and avoid punishments will be considered. Rewards and punishments as expected by a potential helper may be material, social, or internal. Rewards may be material, as in being paid (Fischer, 1963), or anticipated reciprocal favors (Baston & Shaw, 1991). Rewards may be externally oriented, as in gaining social approval (Bauman, Cialdini, & Kenrick, 1981), praise (Batson and Flory, 1990; Bendapudi, Singh, & Bendapudi, 1996), power and reputation (Brunero, 2002), and avoiding censure (Reis & Gruzen, 1976). Or they may be internally oriented rewards such as avoidance of guilt, avoidance of painful memories, the attainment of warm fuzzy feelings (Brunero, 2002), and avoiding pain of guilty feelings (Sober & Wilson, 1989). If the situational markers provide expectations of rewards for helping and/or punishments for not helping in the situation, this may cue egoistic motivations to gain these rewards or avoid these punishments. The form of the reward may be explicitly stated, or merely suggestive that a reciprocal favor should be performed to repay the receiver’s debt. Baston and Shaw further suggested that a benefit analysis is then computed by the observer where the expected benefit of helping, and thereby reducing their negative affective state, is compared to the expected cost of helping, having another help, or escaping from the situation. Finally, the behavioral response will be to act to gain an expected reward or avoid a foreseeable punishment or to not act — whichever is most positively valenced in the relative benefit analysis. 13 Considered next are egoistic and altruistic emotional responses to viewing someone in need. Arousal reducing egoistic motivations are based on an observer’s internal vicarious emotional response (i.e., empathy) of personal distress (Hoffman, 1981). Hence, by viewing a person in need of help, an observer may feel distress and is motivated to reduce this negative affective state. In this way, empathy is differentiated from sympathy. Sympathy refers to the heightened awareness of the suffering of another person as something to be alleviated (Wispé, 1986). Feeling sympathy for someone constitutes an altruistic motivation to have their affliction reduced (Eisenberg, 1991). When feeling sympathy, a person feels for another and when feeling empathy, a person feels with another (Wispé, 1986). These differences are critical in the motivations for which we help another. In one case (empathy), we seek to reduce our own negative affect and in sympathy, we altruistically seek to relieve the negative state of the other. By feeling with someone it is therefore possible to have a vicarious emotional response to their pain and pleasures. These motivations are then evaluated similarly to the motivations to gain rewards/avoid punishments. That is, the costs of helping are compared to the benefit of helping to gain an expected reward and the appropriate behavioral response (to help or not) is enacted. Because the propensity to act altruistically and/or the ability to experience a vicarious emotional response (i.e., empathy) are based on idiosyncratic internal differences among people, these may be somewhat difficult for an influence agent to invoke. An influence agent may, however, quite easily adjust an influence target’s perceptions of their expectations for rewards and their expectations for punishment by 14 providing messages or drawing attention to situational markers that indicate the potential rewards/punishments for complying or not complying. Concession as Reward: The Reverse Prefavor When these principles are applied to the realm of social influence, an influence agent may be able to capitalize on a favor owed to another. In the reverse prefavor compliance strategy a compliance agent sets up a situation where they are in distress and ask for assistance from the target. Subsequently, the compliance agent will offer them a concession on a good or service by either sweetening the deal with the that’s-not-all concession strategy or by reducing the price with a door-in-the-face-strategy. In either case the concession target request is delivered as a “special opportunity” and repayment for the earlier favor. By framing the concession as a repayment for the prior favor, the repayment of the prior favor becomes the salient aspect in the exchange rather than trigger defensive mechanisms often associated with a concession strategy. Additionally, if a compliance agent can induce a compliance target to provide them with a small initial favor, the target may be obligated to accept repayment for that favor. The general form of the reverse prefavor sequential influence strategy is for a compliance agent to illicit a paltry favor from a potential consumer followed by a repayment of the favor in the form of a “concession” or “special opportunity” (i.e., sale or discount on goods) which is the target request. H1: A reverse prefavor compliance strategy will be associated with higher rates of compliance on a target request than a direct request or concession strategy alone. 15 A target may provide help because they see the potential for certain rewards such as praise, karma, or a fixture favor as a result for helping, or they may wish to avoid some punishment such as social sanctions for not helping. They also may feel empathy with the compliance agent and have an egoistic drive to reduce this negative state. Or, they may feel sympathy for the compliance agent and act altruistically to relieve the suffering of the compliance agent. Because several motivations could drive a target to comply with the initial request, it is expected that there will be relatively high rates of initial request compliance. Being driven to comply with a request based on altruistic or empathic motivations may be counter productive for a compliance agent utilizing this sequential strategy because the target may obtain relief or well being by complying with the initial request. Therefore, when targets help because of the egoistic motive to relieve their empathic state or when acting altruistically, there is no need to repay the help they offered. Because the strategy relies on compliance with the first request to set up the subsequent return favor compliance message, it may be most productive to solicit compliance with the initial small request by activating egoistic motivations of expecting rewards as opposed to sympathy/empathy. If a target helps on an initial small request based on motivations to gain a reward, we might expect that they will have high rates of compliance with a concession that satisfies these expected outcomes. Therefore, the rates of compliance with the target concession request is proposed to be a function of the motivations by which a compliance target provides aid on the initial request. If the motivation is reward-focused, the compliance agent will be able to capitalize on the favor owed (see Table 1 for visual explanation of research design). If someone seeking help provides a message indicating that the potential helper can expect repayment, they may 16 be able to induce a motivation to help based on the expectation of rewards. It is expected that due to this increased motivation to gain rewards, repayment may be more easily provided in the form of a concession. In conditions where an expectation of reward message is not provided, help may be more readily given based on empathy/sympathy and thus repayment is unexpected and unnecessary. H2: A reverse prefavor compliance strategy will be more effective when paired with a message indicating an expectation of reward than when no message indicating expectation of reward is presented. The intent of the reverse prefavor strategy is to provide a logical explanation for a concession, thereby reducing activated defensive mechanisms and increasing rates of compliance. Because of our normative obligations to give and receive favors, it is expected that compliance targets will allow the favor to be repaid in the form of a concession. Asking for help in the initial reverse prefavor request is likely to elicit emotional and motivational reactions in the target that may affect the likelihood of subsequent compliance. The strategy is expected to be most successful when egoistic motivations of gaining rewards and/or avoiding punishments for not helping are activated. An alternative strategy that more directly triggers egoistic motivations of gaining rewards or reducing punishments is the transgression. Concession as Apolog): The Transgression Transgressions are those acts that wrong or morally offend another or inflict psychological or physical pain or injury (Worthington & Wade, 1999). In the transgression compliance strategy, a'compliance agent commits a seemingly unintentional action or inaction that has negative consequences for both the compliance l7 agent and target. The compliance agent subsequently requests aid from the compliance target in order to remedy the situation, and the compliance target is compelled to help because, if they do not, they are also injured by the action/inaction of the compliance agent. Committing a transgression against a target will instigate the egoistic motivation of reducing punishments; as such, the target is expected to help in an effort of self- preservation. By limiting the compliance target’s freedom to choose alternative actions, it is expected that the target will frequently agree to the initial small request. However, because they do not freely choose to do so, they are not acting in an altruistic or sympathetic manner. Therefore, the minor favor they provided is an act of exchange and the view of the target is that their exchange partner is obligated to repay the favor. Transgressions are objectively wrong or injurious and are particularly destructive when they are repeated, heavily charged with negative emotion, severe, and unaccompanied by transgressor guilt or apology (Worthington & Wade, 1999). If the inequity is not recognized and acted upon by the offender, this may result in negative reciprocation to be performed by the person who was slighted. Ultimately, this may perpetuate a continued process of negative actions by both parties until the situation spirals out of control (Anderson & Pearson, 1999). However, if the offender attempts to repair the relationship, the banned individual may allow equity to be restored in the relationship and thereby avoid the possibility of a negative behavior spiral. According to F rantz and Bennigson (2005), apologies are our most basic and pervasive conflict resolution technique. With an apology, a transgressor confesses culpability but concurrently demonstrates that he/she may be worthy of a second chance. McCullough et a1. (1998) observed that when victims received an apology, they had 18 greater feelings of closeness with the offender, greater empathy for an offender, and behaved with less avoidance and revenge. Similarly, Ohbuchi, Kameda and Agarie (1989) determined that victims were less angry, had lower levels of aggression, and had a better impression of a transgressor when they received an apology. Apologies can come in several forms. Scher and Barley (1997) found that offers of reparation, expressions of responsibility, and promises of forbearance were web] in the effectiveness of an apology. Goffman (1971) mentions corrective action as an element of an apology; however, Benoit (1995) submits that offenders may take corrective action without admitting culpability or apologizing. According to Benoit, a transgressor may take corrective relationship repairing action when they pledge or provide reparation for the injury, take steps to prevent recurrence of the offensive act or both. It has also been suggested that when transgressors provide compensation for a misdeed, they present amends to the victim in order to soothe the victirn’s ill feelings (Schonback, 1980). Restitution may be in the form of goods, services, or monetary reimbursement and may therefore reduce the negative feelings of the offense (Benoit, 1995) Ultimately, a transgressor is required to make amends for their offensive actions. They may do this through an apologetic message that emphasizes their culpability or through reparative action. In the case of the transgression compliance strategy, the compliance agent would use the discount on the sale of an item (concession) as a reparative action. The compliance target must now make a decision to either accept or reject the compliance agent’s “attempt” to repair the relationship, whereby acceptance of the concession would presumably lead to increased relational harmony. Rejection, a form 19 of punishment, would lead to further relational discord. Rejecting another’s attempts to rectify an inequitable relationship, in effect to fail to accept repayment for a prior help violates our most basic norms (Gouldner, 1960). In this way, the act of concession simultaneously repays the help given to the compliance agent and rectifies the prior harm exacted upon the target. Because the concession is the form of apology and not paired with a verbal admission of culpability, if the target rejects the offer of concession they receive neither the benefit of repayment nor the contentment of rectification. For these reasons, targets are apt to accept the concession as apology and comply with the request. H3: A transgression compliance strategy will be associated with higher rates of compliance on a target request than a direct request or concession strategy alone. In addition to the transgression, motivating compliance targets to comply so that they may minimize their punishment for not aiding, a compliance agent may simultaneously provide indication that they will reward the target in the future for their help. This has the added benefit of inducing the egoistic motivation to gain expected rewards in addition to the already present egoistic motivation to avoid punishments. H4: A transgression compliance strategy will be more effective when paired with a message indicating an expectation of reward than when no message indicating expectation of reward is presented. The transgression compliance strategy is expected to activate the egoistic motivation to avoid punishments. By providing targets with a message indicating that the favor will be repaid to them by the agent, it is expected that an additional egoistic motivation will be generated: the motivation to gain rewards. Additionally, it has been 20 suggested that the transgression compliance strategy provides mechanisms that will enhance the effectiveness of the concession compliance strategy, and as such, is expected to outperform the concession and direct request as strategies to gain compliance. Presently, it is unclear whether a reverse prefavor or a transgression will be more effective in obtaining compliance on a target request. The reverse prefavor is expected to motivate someone by way of avoiding punishments and gaining rewards; however, without a specific induction where a compliance agent suggests that they will earn rewards for helping it is unclear whether unspecific internal (i.e., wellbeing) and external (e. g. social approval) rewards and internal (i.e., guilt) and external (i.e., social sanctions) punishments for helping will be enough to induce compliance. However if the norm of reciprocity fiinction to receive favors is present, compliance targets will be compelled to accept the repayment of their prior favor regardless of whether they view it as a good deal. The transgression is proposed to invoke a strong motivation to avoid punishments, which is expected to lead to feelings that relationship repair is owed and will view the concession as the apology that they seek, to which they will comply at high rates. The possibility does exist, however, that the initial small favor in the transgression will be agreed to at a high rate, but that because of psychological reactance or other factors stimulating relationship withdrawal, targets will not comply with the target concession request. Because of these myriad factors it is difficult to know whether the reverse prefavor or the transgression will be more effective in inducing compliance with the target request. Therefore a research question is posed: RQl: Is a reverse prefavor or a transgression compliance strategy more effective in obtaining compliance with a target request? 21 Hgmmnmmmmop W398 mam—<3 $530630: om woémam > d m _ .m mum—wraoam ummwaoa n moor Con—comma: AamommaoV “HQ ocgommrm Wong—moong Nae—<8 mm 008 :38 088 we emu. i p 8:833: Q3: Woémam 306 m. m r O . §.maH 898m" Aamommmov Ile 82% rillv Woo a Viewed as =9 target request Punishments Apology Figure 2. Predicted mediation model of motivation to avoid material punishments- compliance relationship. The gaining rewards model presented in Figure 3 is expected to be triggered most directly by the presentation of an anticipation of rewards message whereby someone 23 seeking help may indicate that they will repay any assistance given. By presenting such a message, someone seeking help may stimulate the egoistic motivation to gain rewards. This in turn causes them to engage in reward seeking behaviors after they help the needy individual. If the concession request is presented as a reward for the earlier good deed, then it is expected that compliance with the request will increase. G . M t . l + R d + C _ + Compliance am a ena ewar oncessron w/ concession Rewards ‘=={> Seeking i=9 Viewed as ‘=i> target request Reward Figure 3. Predicted mediation model of motivation to gain material rewards-compliance relationship. It has also been proposed that the observation of a needy person in the reverse prefavor strategy may produce feelings of empathy and/or feelings of sympathy in the observer (see Figure 4). These emotions are expected to be positively related to helping in the initial small request for aid. By aiding the person in distress it is expected that the " Rectification Concession Empathy =0 Seeking =9 Viewed as Apology Compliance Likert - Measure -- Reward Concession Sympathy =9 Seeking =1) Viewed as Reward Figure 4. Predicted null model of the effect of motivation to reduce negative affect and altruistic helping on compliance. 24 vicarious negative affect of empathy and the feelings of sympathy will be neutralized. Because helping behaviors have neutralized these affective responses, repayment will be unnecessary and therefore the concession will be irrelevant and unrelated to compliance. Alternative Responses to the Compliance Request While the arguments for the processes by which the presented inductions will affect compliance have been discussed in some detail, this is the first known research to present such strategies, to test their effectiveness, and to observe the intervening processes. This being the case, it is prudent to explore alternative processes that may help to explain the effectiveness of the reverse prefavor and the transgression compliance strategies. One such issue is related to the concessionary compliance message. The general form of the concession compliance message in this context, “You did this for me now you should let me do this for you in repayment” has been proposed to aid in compliance because the concession will be viewed as both an apology and a reward. There is the possibility, however, that the message could be perceived to indicate ones’ high level of obligation toward the favor doer. This perception of obligation may be viewed as a secondary circumstance of personal distress in the agent and, particularly because the target’s prior action of help caused the distress, they may be driven to help relieve it. The most cognitively accessible form of relief in this context would be to comply with the concession. This may be principally true with persons prone to feeling empathy and/or sympathy. However, viewing the agent in their obligatory state may also trigger compliance because of the motivation to gain rewards such as social approval/feelings of good will or to minimize punishments such as social censure (see 25 Figure 5). Therefore, while the motivations to help with the initial small request are predicted to be most important in the path from helping to compliance, a secondary motivation to help relieve a person’s distress, the negative affect state of obligation, may be present. The impact of perceived distress on compliance will also be investigated. RQ2: Is distress related to compliance? 26 CHAPTER 11 METHOD Design To test the hypotheses, this study utilized a 2 (reverse pre-favor, transgression) X 2 (expectation for compensation, no expectation for compensation) independent groups factorial design with two offset comparison groups (concession and direct request), resulting in six independent samples (see Table 1). Experimental conditions were manipulated by providing subjects with messages intended to induce each of the conditions. Table 1: Study design and number of subjects per condition. Reverse Pre-favor Transgression Anticipation of N = 124 N = 100 Compensation (19.5%) (17.5%) No . . . N = 110 N = 112 AntICIpatIon of o 0 Compensation (21'8 /°) “9'6 A) Concession N = 63 Control (1 1.10%) Direct N = 60 Request 0 Control “0'5 A) Participants Participants consisted of 577 undergraduate students from three campuses who participated in the study in exchange for course credit. One campus was a large Midwestern university (N = 277, 48.0%), another was a small Midwestern Christian college (N = 39, 6.76%) and the last was a large private Eastern college (N = 261 , 45.2%). Seven of the surveys returned were removed from analysis due to incomplete 27 responses, resulting in 570 participant responses used in analysis. Subjects’ ages ranged from 17 to 35 years of age with a mean of 19.63 (SD = 1.49) years. Most of the participants were Freshman (N = 168, 29.6%), Sophomore (N = 136, 23.9%) or Junior (N = 166, 29.2%) standing with fewer college Seniors (N = 96, 17.3) participating in the study. Participants were also more often female (N = 348, 61.5%) than male (N = 218, 38.5%). The ethnicity of respondents was mostly White (N = 426, 74.7%), followed by Black (N = 52, 9.1%), Hispanic/Latino (N = 34, 6.0%), Asian-American (N = 30, 5.3%), Asian (N = 10, 1.8%), Native American (N = 1, 0.2%) and other (N = 13, 2.3%). Procedure The study was conducted through the use of an experimental survey methodology where each participant is presented with a scenario in which each of the variables (RPF/I‘ransgression and anticipation/no anticipation of compensation) is manipulated. Students were recruited to participate in the survey by either an announcement given in class by the researcher or by a representative at the school sending out an email to prospective subjects. Students were directed to a website where they read the informed consent sheet and were given instructions. Participants then proceeded to the survey by way of a random link generator, which randomly assigned each participant to a condition. The random assignment to conditions resulted in approximately an even number of participants in each of the four experimental conditions (see Table 1). Because of the need for precise estimates for variables in the experimental groups, the algorithm used to randomly assign favored control experimental conditions was a rate of 2: l , resulting in the control conditions having approximately one half the number of subjects as any experimental condition. Experimental conditions consisted of 28 an initial favor request scenario and items measuring their motivation to help (gain rewards, avoid punishments, empathy, sympathy), reward seeking, and rectification seeking. The initial compliance item was counter balanced among experimental conditions to either precede or follow these measures. This was done to ensure that items measuring motivation did not affect respondents’ endorsement of the initial compliance measure, however temporally the decision to comply would follow the motivations to comply. Subjects were then presented with a continuation of the first scenario where they were asked to comply with the concession request presented as repayment for the subjects’ prior help. After this second scenario, subjects reported their view on whether the concession was an appropriate way to repay/apologize, the perceived amount of distress in their classmate and final compliance. Again, the final compliance items were counterbalanced and presented either just after the second scenario induction or after the items measuring concession as reward, concession as apology, and distress. If, however, a respondent reported that they would not help on the initial request, they were presented with a scenario that offered them a concession but did not present it as repayment for the prior favor. These subjects did not receive items measuring their view of the concession as reward, concession as apology, or other’s distress. The counterbalancing of items resulted in a survey design where approximately one-quarter of those assigned to experimental conditions were presented with both the initial compliance item and target compliance item immediately after the scenario inductions. Another one-quarter of participants received both compliance items after the measures of other relevant variables. One-quarter of subjects received the initial compliance item immediately afier the initial request, and the target request measure was 29 after scale measures of concession as reward/apology, and distress. The final quarter of participants received the initial request afier the motivation to help items and the target request immediately after the target request induction. In all, this resulted in four differently ordered surveys for each of the four experimental conditions. Those assigned to the control conditions (direct request control or concession request control) were presented with a scenario similar in content to the experimental conditions, however they were not asked for an initial small request and only completed the measure of target compliance. All participants also reported demographic information. Scenario Inductions The scenarios are representative of situations in which students may find themselves while enacting their role as a student. The general premise of the reverse prefavor/transgression scenario is that a fellow student asks the subject immediately prior to an exam if they have an extra blue book exam booklet that they might have to take the exam. The scenario indicates that the subject does have an extra exam booklet and must decide whether to provide a favor to their classmate in the form of sharing their exam booklet. The scenario continues to the following week, where the participant is presented with a concession (target request) to purchase raffle tickets. The following reverse prefavor and transgression scenario was presented to participants: It is nearing the middle of the semester and you have two classes that both meet on Tuesday and Thursday. Unfortunately for you, both of your classes have an exam scheduled on the same day. Luckily, you have about two hours between classes to decompress. At your first exam of the day, you arrive about five minutes before class and are waiting for class to begin. As with most students in large lectures, you typically sit in about the same place every class period but rarely talk to many other students. You do, however, notice the familiar faces of people with whom you have not often talked previously. One of your classmates, who you know to be fairly intelligent, sits in their usual place two seats down from you and 30 appears to be a bit flustered. This classmate turns to you and asks, “You wouldn’t happen to have an extra blue book exam booklet would you? I don’t have one.” You recall that the professor had mentioned that he “had gotten out of the business of carrying extra blue books long ago.” You do happen to have two blue books but only because you need the other one for your next exam later in the afiernoon. If you give this person your exam booklet you will have to walk about five minutes out of your way to get another one before your next class. However, you estimate that you will have about two hours to get to your next class and it is a nice day. Reverse Pre-Favor/Transgression. In the reverse prefavor conditions presented above, a compliance agent presents a situation that indicates that they are in distress. If the compliance agent decides to help their classmate, helping behavior will cost the compliance target minimally and these helping behaviors are directed at aiding their classmate. In transgression conditions, a compliance agent presents a situation that indicates that by their action or inaction they are in distress and because of their behavior or misbehavior their target has also been harmed. The compliance target may provide a help that aids both the compliance agent and himself. In this case, the transgression will be induced by adding the following statement to the general reverse prefavor scenario: “Your professor has mentioned that they will assign partners for this exam based roughly on where people sit. Before you can respond to the request for a blue book, your professor comes by and assigns you to work with this classmate. You will be without a partner on the exam if your classmate doesn ’t have a blue book ” By adding these statements to the scenario, the agent’s action of forgetting their blue book is now a transgression against the target. Anticipation of Compensation. Conditions receiving an expectation of compensation message are expected to result in an increased egoistic motivation to obtain 31 rewards. In effect, this ensures that the relationship is transactive in nature and cues the target to look for their reward. The expectation of compensation message is, “I will figure out a way to repay you” and will be delivered at the end of the second paragraph. The no anticipation of compensation message conditions will not receive this message. Direct Request/Concession Scenario. The direct request and concession strategies are comparison groups to which the newly developed strategies of the reverse prefavor and transgression will be compared. The direct request and compliance scenarios will be similar in content to the reverse prefavor and concession scenarios; however, they will not provide a request for help or a situation that requires help. The scenario utilized in both the direct request and concession scenario are presented below. It is nearing the middle of the semester and you have two classes that both meet on Tuesday and Thursday. Unfortunately for you, both of your classes have an exam scheduled on the same day. Luckily, you have about two hours between classes in order to decompress. At your first exam of the day you arrive about five minutes before class and are waiting for class to begin. As with most students in large lectures, you typically sit in about the same place every class period but rarely talk to too many other students. You do, however, notice the familiar faces of people you have not often talked to previously. One of your classmates sits in their usual place two seats down from you and appears to be a bit flustered. This classmate strikes up a conversation with you about how he got stuck at work later than expected and almost did not have time to pick up a blue book exam booklet for use on the exam. Compliance Request. A concession compliance request will be provided in the transgression, reverse prefavor, and concession control conditions; whereas a direct request for the more moderate price will be used in the direct request condition. Previous research has suggested that the concession as a strategy is more effective when compliance agents represent a pro-social organization or organization that the target likes. Therefore the compliance agent will represent the seemingly beneficial organization “old 32 high school.” Previous research has effectively used the sale of raffle tickets as a measure of compliance. Other benefits to the use of raffle tickets include the presumption that no one already has raffle tickets from the agents’ old high school, and it allows for the measure to be continuous. While some people may simply not ever purchase raffle tickets, there is no reason to expect that this artifact would have a significant effect on outcomes due to the random assignment of subjects to conditions. The general form of the concession compliance request is presented below. The next week you attend class as usual. After class, this same classmate says to you, “I am selling raffle tickets to raise money for my old high school, where the prizes include an Ipod Video, gift certificates to the book store, and some other cool stuff. [Insert compliance message here] You don’t have to pay me now. Can I put you down for some? You can buy as many as you want at this price, I can get more.” Your classmate seems really sincere that this is a good deal. At the beginning of the quoted message the compliance agent, in the reverse prefavor and transgression conditions, will deliver the additional message, “You really saved me last week. I have been thinking about a good way to pay you back for helping me out with that blue book and I think I have come up with something that you will really like.” This is intended to suggest that the deal they are about to present is repayment/rectification for the prior favor or transgression. Additionally, the reverse prefavor/transgression conditions will receive the message, “We usually sell the tickets for $2 each, and they let us keep half as an incentive to sell them, but I would like to repay you by selling them to you for only $1 each.” The concession control condition receives the message, “We usually sell the tickets for $2 each, and they let us keep half as an incentive to sell them, but I can sell them to you for only $1 each.” Whereas the direct request control receives the message, “I can sell them to you for only $1 each.” 33 Instrumentation Motivation to gain rewards. We may help someone in distress because of our motivation to gain rewards, to avoid punishments, to reduce the negative vicarious emotion of empathy, or because of an altruistic response to viewing others’ suffering. Each of these will be measured as induction checks of the transgression, reverse prefavor, and anticipation of reward inductions. Rewards for helping behaviors can come in various forms: material rewards, external rewards, and internal rewards. Someone who helps because of the motivation to gain material rewards seeks to earn payment for their good deed or gain other anticipated reciprocal favors. This motivation is expected to increase in conditions where participants receive an “anticipation of rewards” induction. The motivation to gain material rewards was measured with seven Likert-type items (see Appendix A). Items included, “By helping now, I will gain in the end,” and the reverse coded item, “If I help I do not expect repayment in this situation.” Item response options range from “strongly disagree” to “strongly agree” and higher scores indicate a motivation to gain material rewards. All subsequent items were measured similarly except where indicated otherwise. The mean of the measure was moderate (M = 3.24, SD = 1.14), and standardized item alpha was sufficient (a = .80). The motivation to seek external rewards was measured with four items including, “People will like me more if I help my classmate,” and “If I help in this situation people will think I am a good person.” The mean for this scale was moderate (M = 3.97, SD = 1.20), and standardized item alpha was sufficient (or = .87). The motivation to gain internal rewards was measured with four items including, “1 would get warm fuzzy feelings if I helped,” and “I helped 34 because it feels good to do so.” The mean was moderately high (M = 5.03, SD = 1.13), and standardized item alpha was (a = .85). Motivation to avoid punishments. Motivation to avoid punishments also has three different dimensions. Most important to the study is the propensity to avoid material punishments because this is proposed to be directly influenced by the transgression compliance strategy (see Appendix B). This was measured utilizing six items including, “By providing this favor, I help both my classmate and myself,” and “The actions of my classmate hurt me if I do not help.” The mean for the scale was moderate (M = 3.42, SD = 1.13), and alpha was (a = .82). The motivation to avoid external punishments was measured with four items. Representative items include, “Others would show their disapproval of my actions if I do not aid my classmate,” and “If I ever needed help, people would not help me if I do not help my classmate.” This scale yielded a moderate mean (M = 3.37, SD = 1.07), with an item alpha ((1 = .80). Motivation to avoid internal punishment was measured with four items, such as “Giving a favor in this situation may help me to avoid guilty feelings,” and “I would have painful memories of this situation if I did not help.” The mean for items measuring this variable were moderate (M = 4.30, SD = 1.17), and the scale items demonstrated an a = .79 level of reliability. Empathy. Empathy refers to the attempt by one self-aware person to comprehend nonjudgmentally the positive and negative experiences of another (Wispé, 1986). By feeling with someone it is possible to have a vicarious emotional response to their pain and pleasures. For this study, four items were developed that are intended to measure state empathy (see Appendix C). A prototypical item is “Seeing my classmate in this 35 situation makes me feel distressed.” Means for the empathy measure were moderate (M = 4.04, SD = 1.24), with a standardized item alpha level of .85. Sympathy. Sympathy refers to the heightened awareness of the suffering of another person as something to be alleviated. Four items measuring state sympathy were constructed (see Appendix D). These items were loosely based on the Empathic Concern measure in Davis’ (1983) Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI). An exemplar item is “I have tender, concerned feelings for my classmate.” The mean for the state sympathy scale was moderate (M = 4.85, SD = .99), and had a moderate alpha level (a = .78) Reward seeking. Persons who engage in helping behaviors that are motivated by gaining material rewards are expected to seek those rewards. Four items were designed to measure the extent to which a person believes they are owed a favor (see Appendix E). Exemplary items include “My classmate owes me for helping them” and “I will figure out a way for my classmate to return the favor.” This measure had a moderate mean (M = 3.0, SD = 1.08), and standardized item alpha of a = .82. Rectification seeking. Those who have been injured by a prior harm are expected to seek rectification. Four items, designed to measure the extent to which someone feels that they are owed an apology, have been developed for this study (see Appendix F). Representative items include “I would appreciate some acknowledgement by my classmate for the situation I was put in” and a reverse coded item, “There is no need for my classmate to say they are sorry.” The mean for the scale was moderate (M = 3.74, SD = 1.09), and alpha level of o. = .67. Concession viewed as reward. If a concession is viewed as a reward for prior helping behavior it has been proposed that this will lead to greater compliance with the 36 concession request. Four items were developed for this study to measure whether the concession is viewed as a reward (see Appendix G). Items include “This was a nice way to thank me for my prior help” and “My classmate thoughtfully returned my earlier favor.” The scale had a moderate mean (M = 3.61 , SD = 1.33), and had a standardized item alpha of .78. Concession viewed as apology. A concession that is viewed as rectification for a prior transgression is expected to increase the acceptance of the concession compliance request. Five items were constructed that are intended to measure a target’s view of the appropriateness of the concession as an apology (see Appendix H). Example items include “The deal my classmate gave me was a good way to say ‘sorry’” and “By offering this deal my classmate was making a request for forgiveness.” The mean for the scale was moderate (M = 3.75, SD = 1.23), and had an alpha level of a = .74. Distress. A person’s perceptions of another’s distress triggers motivations to help. It has been proposed that the perception of the compliance agent in a state of obligation or indebtedness may be viewed as distressfiil situation. If the target believes the agent to be in distress, they may help the agent for a variety of reasons not associated with seeking rewards or rectification. Distress is measured with six items (see appendix 1) including, “My classmate feels unsettled about owing me a favor” and “By being helped previously, my classmate believes they are in a bind.” The scale mean was moderate (M = 4.17, SD = 1.01) and had an alpha level of .87. Compliance. Subjects in the transgression and reverse prefavor conditions will be asked to report whether they agree to the initial small request for help with one item, “Would you help your classmate by giving them a blue book.” Subject’s response 37 categories are “yes” [I will give a blue book to my classmate] or “no” [I will not give a bluebook to my classmate]. It is important to have a clear commitment of compliance with the initial small request in order for the subsequent target request to have meaning. If a subject does not commit to helping their classmate they will be asked about their motivations for helping but will not receive the experimental compliance request, but rather will receive a compliance request identical to the concession control. Target request compliance will be measured by the single item, “How many tickets will you buy from your classmate at $1 each?” and a four item Likert-type scale including items such as “I would buy several tickets,” and the reverse coded item “There is no way that I would buy any tickets.” Please see Appendix J for the four scale items. The mean of the scale was moderate (M = 3.59, SD = 1.09) and the scale showed high reliability (or = .94). Of the 570 total participants in the study, 252 (44.1%) reported that they would not purchase any tickets in response to the single item final target request. Other scores on this measure ranged from 1 to 20 tickets purchased, resulting in a high number of responses to be deemed as outliers. The distribution of the measure had high skewness (1.94) and high kurtosis (5.3 8). These indicators suggested that the distribution of the measure deviated from normality and thus the variable was dichotomized into those that complied and those that did not comply with the target request. Both indicators of compliance along with the Likert—type measure of compliance will be used where appropriate. 38 CHAPTER III RESULTS Confirmatory factor analysis (Hunter & Gerbing, 1982) was employed to test the factor measurement model for internal consistency and parallelism for all multi-item scales. Based on these analyses, three items were removed from the motivation to gain rewards scale. Two items (“My classmate is unlikely to repay me for the favor I provide,” and “If I help I believe this classmate will not attempt to help me in return,”) were removed because of low correlation with other scale items, and one item (“By helping now, I will gain in the end.”) was removed because of substantial cross loading with other factors. This resulted in the retaining of five items that measure the motivation to gain material rewards. Additionally, one item (“I feel required to help my classmate”) intended to measure motivation to avoid material punishments yielded significant cross loadings resulting in high residual errors in the test for parallelism. This item was also removed from fin‘ther analysis resulting in a five-item measure for motivation to avoid material punishments. High residual errors were also found in the test for heterogeneity (parallelism) in two items (“The deal I got was because my classmate owed me an apolog)’;” and “This was an inappropriate way to admit fault”) in the measure of concession viewed as apology. These items were also removed from further analysis resulting in a three-item measure for this variable. After removal of these five items, the data were deemed consistent with the hypothesized factor model based on the small errors between predicted and observed correlations in tests of parallelism and internal consistency. 39 Of the 447 participants who were randomly assigned to an experimental condition, 87.2% (N = 390) reported that they would agree to the initial request while 12.8% (N = 57) refused. The refusals to comply with the initial small request were not distributed evenly between conditions. As expected, participants exposed to the transgression compliance strategy (N = 195, 92%) complied at a higher rate [380, 447) = 8.12, p = .004, phi = -.135] with the initial small request than participants in the Reverse Pre Favor (RPF) (N = 195, 83%) conditions. Hypothesis one predicted that a reverse prefavor compliance strategy will be associated with higher rates of compliance on a target request than a direct request or concession strategy alone. A one-way independent groups ANOVA was conducted comparing persons who agreed to the initial small request in the reverse prefavor condition, concession and direct request conditions. As predicted, there were significant differences among the three groups [F ( 2, 317) = 4.62, p=.01] in the number of tickets Table 2. Mean, standard deviations, medians and percent of subjects complying with target request as measured by number of tickets purchased. Reverse . Pre-f av o r Transgressnon Anticipation of 1.68 (2.79) 1.49 (2:43) Compensation Med " 0 Med " 0 39.8% 46.0% No 1.91 (2.34) 2.34 (3.23) Anticipation of Med = 0 Med = 1 Compensation 49.9% 55.4% 2.86 (3.66) Concession Med = 2 Control 73.0% 0...“ 253,52? Request 76 7% Control ' 40 that they reported they would purchase and significant differences between the three groups with respect to the Likert-measure of compliance [F ( 2, 317) = 18.64, p<.001]. Unexpectedly, however, the reverse prefavor had the lowest rates of compliance on the number of tickets purchased measure of compliance (see Table 2) (M=1.79, P(1.40 S p S 2.18) = .95), and the Likert-type measure of compliance (see Table 3) (M=3.20, P(2.95 S p S 3.44) = .95) when compared to the concession compliance strategy [number of tickets purchased (M=2.86, P(1.93 S ,u _<_ 3.78) = .95); Likert measure of compliance (M=4.30, P(3.88 S p S 4.71) = .95)] and the direct request compliance strategy [number of tickets purchased (M=2.73, P(2. 12 S ,u S 3.35) = .95); Likert measure of compliance (M=4.48, P(4.06 S p S 4.90) = .95)]. In general these data reveal a pattern where the concession and DR compliance strategies do not differ and are significantly better at inducing compliance than the RPF. Table 3. Mean, standard deviations, and medians of subjects complying with target request as measured by Likert compliance scale. Reverse ' n Pr e-fav or Transgressno Anticipation of 3.01 (1.76) 3.11 (1.68) Compensation Med = 2.66 Med = 2.75 No . . . 3.36 (1.70) 3.36 (1.74) Antrcrpatlon of _ = Compensation Med - 3.00 Med 3.50 Concession 4.30 (1.62) Control Med = 4.75 Direct 4.48 (1.66) Reques‘ Med = 5.00 Control 41 Interesting, however, are those forty subjects who received a reverse prefavor induction that did not comply with the initial prefavor request. These subjects received the standard concession request without the “You really saved me last week I have been thinking about a good way to pay you back for helping me out with that blue book and I think I have come up with something that you will really like,” message. Compliance for these subjects was significantly higher than those indicating that they would comply with the initial small request in terms of both tickets purchased [M=3.53, P048 5 p S 4.57) = .95)] and the Likert measure for compliance [M=4.49 P(4.01 S ,u S 4.97) = .95]. When the continuous dependent variable “tickets purchased” compliance measure is dichotomized, into those who comply and those who do not comply, a similar pattern emerges between subjects who complied with the initial request, and subjects who did not comply with the initial request. A chi square test was employed to test whether differences in compliance existed between initial compliers, initial non-compliers, direct request and concession controls. The chi square test indicated that non independence did exist among the variables [780, 358) = 37.93, p<.001, phi = .33] and visual inspection of the cell residuals between predicted and expected cell counts indicated that when subjects agreed to the initial request in the RPF condition, they complied less (44.1%, N = 86) than when they did not agree to comply with the initial request (80%, N = 32), or when in direct request (76.7%, N = 46) or concession only (73%, N = 46) control conditions. When initial compliance is not taken into account, and initial and non-initial compliers are combined, results indicate similar findings. Specifically, the mean number of tickets purchased is still lower [M=2.09, P(1.71 S p S 2.46) = .95], though not significantly lower, than controls. Additionally, the Likert-type measure of compliance 42 (M=3 .42, P(3.19S ,u S 3.64) = .95) remained significantly lower than controls, and analysis of the dichotomous measure of compliance [380, 358) = 20.29, p<.001, Cramer’s V = .24] also indicated that the RPF (50.2%) was worse than control conditions in inducing compliance. Hypothesis two predicted that the reverse prefavor compliance strategy will be more effective when paired with a message indicating an anticipation of compensation than when no message indicating anticipation of compensation is presented. A t-test was conducted, which compared compliance rates on the tickets purchased indicator between those who had received the anticipation of compensation message [M=1.68, P(1.14 S p S 2.22) = .95], with those who did not receive the message [ME] .91, P(1.36 S ,u S 2.46) = .95). The results of the test indicated that there was no difference [t(193) = -.60, p = .55, r = .04] when subjects received the anticipation of compensation induction compared to those who did not receive the induction. Similar findings [t(233) = -1.22, p = .23, r = .09] were observed in the data with the Likert compliance measure between those who had received the anticipation of compensation induction [M=3.06, P(2.72 S p S 3.40) = .95], with those who did not receive the message [M=3.36, P(3.01 S p S 3.71) = .95]. Identical non-significant findings resulted from the chi square test of independence [780, 195) = 1.64, p<.201 , Phi = .09] with the dichotomized dependent measure with 39.8% (n=41) of subjects in the anticipation condition complying and 48.9% (N = 45) of subjects in the no anticipation condition complying. Interestingly, while these findings do not show significant differences, all measures of compliance point to a consistent pattern of less compliance with the final concession request when subjects are given the anticipation of compensation induction. 43 Hypothesis three predicted that a transgression compliance strategy will be associated with higher rates of compliance on a target request than a direct request or concession strategy alone. A one way independent groups ANOVA was conducted comparing those who complied with the initial small request in the transgression condition, with the concession and direct request conditions. First, when considering the compliance item “How many tickets would you purchase,” there were significant differences among the three groups [F(2, 317) = 3.15, p=.044, r =.13], with the transgression having the lowest rates of compliance (M=1.94, P(1.53 S y S 2.35) = .95), followed by the direct request (M=2.73, P(2.12 S p S 3.3 5) = .95) and the concession (M=2.86, P(1.93 S ,u S 3.78) = .95). A similar pattern of results was observed in the data when the Likert type scale of compliance was investigated [F (2, 317) = 17.58, p<.001, r =.28], with those who indicated that they would comply with the initial request, and were thus exposed to the entire transgression induction, complying less (M=3.24, P(3.00 S ,u S 3.48) = .95) than either the direct request (M=4.48, P(4.06 S p S 4.90) = .95) or the concession control groups (M=4.30, P(3.88 S p S 4.72) = .95). Inspection of the confidence intervals indicates that the direct request and concession controls did not differ in their levels of compliance; however, subjects in these groups reported that they would comply at a higher rate than subjects exposed to the transgression compliance strategy. Similarly, a chi square test for independence revealed significant differences between the three groups [{(2, 318) = 19.81, p<.001, r = .25] when compared on the dichotomized measure of compliance. Inspection of the cell residuals between predicted and expected cell counts indicated that similar levels of compliance were observed in the concession and direct request control groups but these groups differed from the subjects 44 who agreed to the initial request in the transgression condition. Specifically, subjects in the transgression condition complied with the final request approximately half of the time (49.7%, N = 97), and compliance in the concession control (73.0%, N = 46) and direct request (76.7%, N = 46) was closer to three-quarters of the time. Those subjects who did not comply with the initial small request in the transgression conditions reported higher levels of compliance with the final target request when considering the tickets purchased measure of compliance (Mr—2.56, P(1.22 S ,u S 3.89) = .95), however this was not significantly higher. The Likert measure of compliance, however, did show that those who would not help on the initial request (M= 4.18, P(3.49 S [u S 4.87) = .95) were significantly more likely to comply with the target request. Inspection of the mean confidence intervals indicates that these subjects had similar levels of target compliance as subjects in direct request and concession control conditions. When the final target compliance scores of initial compliers and the eighteen non- compliers are combined, the propensity to purchase tickets (M=-1.99, P(1.59 _<. p _<_ 2.38) = .95) does not change substantially. Similarly, the Likert measure of target compliance does not change substantially (M=3.32, P(3.09 S p _<_ 3.55) = .95), nor does its relative underperformance when compared to direct request or concession compliance strategies. When the overall scores for the transgression compliance strategy is compared to the dichotomous measure of target compliance, only 50.9% of subjects complied with the target request and this remained significantly lower [x2(2, 335) = 18.58, p<.001, r = .24] than the two control groups. 45 Hypothesis four predicted that the transgression compliance strategy will be more effective in gaining compliance with a target request when paired with a message indicating an expectation of reward than when no message indicating expectation of reward is presented. A t-test was conducted, which compared those who had received the anticipation of compensation (W156, P(1.08 S ,u S 3.04) = .95), with those who did not receive the message (M=2.37, P(1.77 S p S 2.04) = .95) in the transgression condition for the target compliance indicator of tickets purchased. The results of the test indicated that when subjects in the transgression condition, received an anticipation of compensation induction they reported that they would purchase fewer tickets [t(204.46) = -2.07, p = .04, r = .14, equal variances not assumed] compared to those who did not receive the induction. The Likert type measure of compliance indicated that while subjects’ reports of compliance were lower in the anticipation condition (M=3.2 l , P(2.89 S ,u S 3.54) = .95) than in the no anticipation of rewards condition (M=3.42, P(3.10 S p S 3.74) = .95) it was not significantly lower [t(210) = -.893, p = .37, r = .06]. Finally, a chi square test for independence was conducted to determine whether a relationship existed between the anticipation of rewards induction and compliance on the dichotomized measure of target compliance. Results of the test indicated that while there may have been a tendency for subjects in the anticipation condition to comply with the target less (46.0%) than those who did not receive the induction (55.4%) the groups were not significantly different [{(1, 212) = 1.85, p=.174, phi = .09, one-tailed]. Research question one asked whether a reverse prefavor or a transgression compliance strategy was most effective in obtaining compliance with a target request. A two way independent groups ANOVA was conducted to investigate the veracity of a 46 main effect for strategy type (reverse prefavor/transgression) on the tickets purchased continuous measure of compliance. Consistent with the previously tested relationships in H2 and H4, the anticipation of rewards induction had no effect on compliance [F (l, 447) = 2.20, p= .14, n.s., r =.07]. More pertinent to answering the research question proposed, there was no main effect for strategy type [F (l, 447) = 1.99, p=.66, n.s., r =.02], and the strategy by anticipation of rewards interaction was also not able to explain variance in compliance scores [F(1, 447) = .043, p=.84]. In terms of the Likert measure of compliance, similar results were found. Specifically, the main effect for anticipation was not found to affect compliance [F(l , 447) = 2.20, p=.l4, n.s., r =.07], the main effect for strategy was not found to affect compliance [F(1, 447) = .451,p=.50, n.s., r =.03], and the interaction was not found to affect compliance [F (1, 447) = .043, p=.835]. In addition, a chi square test was conducted to investigate possible differences in compliance on the dichotomized measure. The data revealed no difference [780, 477) = .02, p=.88, n.s., phi = .07] between the transgression and compliance measure. These data suggest that there was no difference between the transgression and the reverse prefavor compliance strategies in their ability to induce compliance with a target request. When initial non compliers are removed from analysis, a main effect for strategy type (i.e., RPF, Transgression) is not detected [F(], 389) = .17, p=.68, n.s.] and the strategy by anticipation interaction is also not significant [F(] , 389) = 1.17, p=.28, n.s.] on the purchase tickets compliance measure. The main effect for anticipation approached significance [F(1, 389) = 3.61, p=.06, 772 = .009] however the effect was small (see Table 2 for means, medians and standard deviations). Additionally, the Likert scale measure of compliance revealed no significant differences [F(3, 389) = 1.47, p = .23, n.s., ”p2 =.008] 47 and the results did not show a difference among the four groups [x2(2, 390) = 4.74, p = .192, r = .09] on the dichotomized compliance measure. Research question two asked whether distress is related to target compliance. Distress was unrelated to the three measures of compliance [tickets purchased (r = .03, p=.51); Likert measure (r = .05, p = .35); dichotomous measure (r = .04, p = .45)]. Distress also did not differ among treatment conditions [F(3, 389) =.51, p=.67, n.s., ”p2 = .004]. Motivations Descriptive statistics for all measured motivations to help by condition are presented in Table 4. The transgression compliance strategy was predicted to be associated with relatively high rates of the motivation to avoid material punishments. An independent groups ANOVA was conducted to test for the predicted main effect. As predicted, the data indicated a significant main effect for compliance strategy [F (3, 445) = 64.72, p<.001, ”p2 =.13] with subjects in the transgression (M=3.83, P(3.69 S p S 3.97) = .95) compliance strategy reporting significantly more motivation to avoid material punishments than subjects in the RPF conditions (M=3.03, P(2.90 S p S 3.17) = .95). This analysis also revealed a small main effect for anticipation, with subjects who did not receive an anticipation message (M=3.30, P(3.17 S ,u S 3.44) = .95) more strongly endorsing the motivation to avoid material punishments [F(l , 443) = 6.79, p=.009, 77,,2 =.02] than those who did receive the anticipation message (M=3.56, P(3 .42 S ,u S 3.70) = .95). A significant, but small, interaction was also present [F(1, 445) = 6.91, p=.009, 77p2 =.02] with cell means indicating that the transgression x no anticipation condition produced the highest motivation to avoid material punishments. 48 Table 4. Descriptives for motivations to help by condition. Rewards - Material Reverse PF Transgression Anticipation 3.38 (1.14) 3.12 (1.14) No anticipation 3.04 (1.13) 3.38 (1 .13) Rewards - lntemal Anticipation 5.11 (1.11) 4.95 (1.19) No anticipation 5.20 (1.03) 4.84 (1.18) Rewards - External Anticipation 3.99 (1.25) 4.01 (1.15) No anticipation 4.11 (1.18) 3.80 (1.19) Avoid Punish. - Material Anticipation 3.03 (.89) 3.57 (1 .19) No anticipation 3.03 (.90) 4.09 (1.18) Avoid Punish. - Internal Anticipation 4.28 (1 .13) 4.35 (1 .15) No anticipation 4.29 (1.14) 4.29 (1 .13) Avoid Punish - External Anticipation 3.39 (1.05) 3.26 (1.08) No anticipation 3.25 (1.07) 3.56 (1.06) Empathy Anticipation 4.03 (1.36) 3.87 (1.32) No anticipation 4.13 (1.13) 4.13 (1.13) Sympathy Anticipation 4.93 (.94) 4.65 (1.12) No anticipation 4.96 (1.00) 4.81 (.89) Interesting to note, however, is that even though the results are consistent with what was expected, the mean amount of endorsement of the variable was at or below the neutral point on the Likert scale. Expectation of compensation was expected to increase the motivation to gain material rewards. Analysis of this expected main effect revealed that no main effect existed, however a significant interaction effect was present [F (3, 445) = 7.49, p=.006, ”p2 =.02] with inspection of the cell means, revealing that the RPF x anticipation and the 49 transgression x no anticipation conditions performed better than the other cells. However, the effect size was small. The reverse pre favor was expected to potentially motivate someone to help through a variety of different processes including: gaining internal and external rewards, avoiding internal and external punishments, empathy, and sympathy. In terms of the motivation to gain internal rewards, a significant but small main effect was found [F(l , 443) = 5.82, p=.Ol 6, ‘77,} =.01], whereby subjects in the RPF conditions (M=5.16, P(5.01 S p S 5.30) = .95) were motivated by internal rewards at a higher rate than subjects in the transgression conditions (M=4.90, P(4.75 S p S 5.05) = .95). No significant differences between conditions were observed for the motivation to gain external rewards, avoid internal or external punishments or for empathy. A significant main effect for compliance strategy was found in the measure for sympathy. As expected, subjects in the RPF conditions (M=4.95, P(4.82 S ,u S 5.07) = .95) reported higher [F(], 446) = 5.35, p=.O2, "p2 =.01] levels of sympathy for their classmate than subjects in the transgression conditions (M=4.73, P(4.60 S p S 4.86) = .95). Tests of the Proposed Models All correlations were corrected for attenuation due to error of measurement by using standardized item alpha coefficients for each variable. In the case of the dichotomized measure of compliance, measurement was assumed to be perfect. The proposed models were then tested with path coefficients based on the corrected correlations. The proposed model for the relationship between avoiding material punishments and compliance with a concession target request was presented in Figure 2. Afier 50 correcting for attenuation due to measurement error, low correlations, ,6 = .22, (.08 S ,8 S .36) = .95, between the motivation to avoid material punishment and rectification seeking and between rectification seeking and the concession viewed as apology variable [fl = .01, (-.13 S ,B S .15) = .95] were observed (see Table 5). These relationships did not meet generally accepted levels of significance nor were they substantial. Therefore, it is concluded that the data do not fit the predicted modelI (see Figure 5). Table 5. Motivation to avoid material punishments: Correlations, corrected correlations and path coefficients. Motivation Avoid Rectification Concession Compliance Material Seeking as Apology - Likert Punish Motivation Avoid Material Punish '22 '17 '08 Rectification ,, _ Seeking .16 .01 .17 Concession as .13* .01 .52 APO'OQY Compliance - . .. Like it .07 -.13 .43 ’ significant at the p<.05 level (two-tailed) "‘* significant at the p<.01 level (two-tailed) Note: Raw correlations are presented in the lower half of the matrix. Correlations corrected for attenuation due to error of measurement are presented in the top of the matrix, and those in bold are used to test the model. Avoid 22 Rectification -01 Concession -52 Compliance - material =9 Seeking =9 Viewed as 1:4) Likert Punishments Apology Figure 5. Proposed model results of motivation to avoid material punishments- compliance relationship. The proposed model for testing the utility of gaining material rewards was presented in Figure 3. It suggests that a person who helps because they are motivated by gaining some material reward will then engage in reward seeking. Those engaged in 51 reward seeking will then be more apt to view the concession as a reward, which will subsequently increase compliance. By inspection of the path coefficients, the expected model did not fit the data due to a substantial negative relationship (,6 = -.30) observed between reward seeking and the concession viewed as reward variable. However, the new model was tested for fit for both the Likert (Figure 6) and dichotomized (Figure 7) measures of compliance. Please see Table 6 and Table 7 for raw correlations and path coefficients (corrected correlations) for the Likert and dichotomized measure of compliance, respectively. First, the model predicting the Likert measure of compliance will be considered. In examining the fit of the model, the path coefficients reached substantial size. The error between the predicted and reproduced correlations were low between the motivation to Table 6. Motivation to gain material rewards-Likert measure of compliance: Correlations, corrected correlations and path coefficients. Motivation Concession Material Slam? Viewed as (33:22 Rewards 9 Reward Motivation Gain Material Rewards '76 "17 "09 Reward Seeking .64“ -.30 -.10 Concession ., ., Viewed as Reward "14 "24 '76 Compliance - _ c. leert -.08 .09 .65 gain material rewards and concession viewed as a reward (.06). The error between compliance and reward seeking (.08) was also low; however, the error between compliance and gaining material rewards (.13), 2 =1 .56, p =.l2 approached significance. The overall model fit was adequate [38(3) = 3.99, p=.26]. 52 . . _ Compliance Garn Matenal .76 Reward --30 Concessron -76 Likert , Rewards 1:) Seeking =9 Viewed as =14 Measure Reward Figure 6. Proposed model results of motivation to gain material rewards-Likert measure of compliance relationship. Alternatively, the model (see Figure 7),which included the dichotomized measure of compliance had lower errors (averaged .07) between predicted and observed unconstrained correlations and had reasonable effect sizes. Evaluation of the individual links suggested a much better fit with the model and the overall model was more consistent with the data [78(3) = 3.57, p=.3 1]. Table 7. Motivation to gain material rewards-dichotomous measure of compliance: Correlations, corrected correlations and path coefficients. Motivation Concession Material 816:3? Viewed as 683%); ' Rewards g Reward ‘ Motivation Gain Material Rewards '76 "‘17 "09 Reward Seeking .64“ -.30 -.07 Concession .. n Viewed as Reward "14 "24 '59 Comply - Dichot -.08 -.06 .52“ , Compliance Gain Material .76 Reward --30 Concessron -59 Dichotomous Rewards 1:1) Seeking :1) Viewed as 1:1) Measure Reward Figure 7. Proposed model results of motivation to gain material rewards-dichotomous measure of compliance relationship. 53 The original thought in developing this path model was that an anticipation of rewards message, “I will figure out a way to repay you” may induce compliance with the initial small request based on an egoistic motivation to gain material rewards. When the model is evaluated by considering only those subjects who received the anticipation of rewards induction, the relationships among the variables become even more substantial (see Table 8), the errors between predicted and observed unconstrained correlations are reduced, and the fit of the overall model (see Figure 8) is enhanced considerably [352(3) = .57, p=.96]. Table 8. Motivation to gain material rewards-Likert measure of compliance, for anticipation of compensation conditions only: Correlations, corrected correlations and path coefficients. Motivation Concession Material Slam? Viewed as 0322:: ' Rewards g Reward Motivation Gain Material Rewards '72 "25 "20 Reward Seeking .61“ -.35 -.10 Concession .. .. Viewed as Reward "14 "28 '79 Compliance - _ .. - c. n Likert .1 8 .24 .67 . , Compliance Gain Matenal .72 Reward -.35 Concessron -79 Likert - Rewards =9 Seeking =9 Viewed as =9 Measure Reward Figure 8. Model results of anticipation of compensation motivation to gain material rewards-Likert measure of compliance relationship. 54 Taken together, these findings indicate a departure from previous thought on the progression from the motivation to gain rewards to final compliance. That is, people who are motivated to help based on the possibility of gaining instrumental rewards are likely to seek those rewards. Offering a subsequent concession is often not viewed as an appropriate reward. Because compliance is based to a large extent on the perception that the concession is a form of reward, compliance is ultimately decreased. This is particularly relevant to those subjects who are given an anticipation of rewards induction. These people, while seeking the reward to which they feel entitled, do not see the concession as an appropriate benefit. Finally, the last predicted model asserted that the vicarious negative egoistic response of empathy and the other oriented (altruistic) response of sympathy would be neutralized by helping on the initial small request. Therefore, subjects who were motivated by these affective responses were not expected to feel like a return favor was owed to them (reward seeking), or feel as if they are owed an apology, or relationship repair (rectification seeking). Because reward seeking and rectification seeking were predicted to have mediated relationships on compliance through concession as reward and the concession as apology, respectively, sympathy and empathy were proposed to be negatively related or unrelated to final compliance. Inspection of the parameter estimates reveals several unsubstantial relationships in the model. The raw correlations and parameter estimates are presented in Table 9. As expected, empathy and sympathy generally produced negative and weak relationships with reward seeking and rectification seeking and resulted in a poor fit for the overall model [78(3) = 184.62, p<.001]. 55 Table 9. Results for predicted null model of the effect of motivation to reduce negative affect and altruistic helping on compliance: Correlations, corrected correlations and path coefficients. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 1 Empathy .66 -.08 .15 .09 .76 .18 2 Sympathy .54“ -.29 -.14 .13 .14 .12 3 5,9391%; -.07 -.23** .65 -.3o -.08 -.10 4 831:0 .11* -.10 .48“ -.21 -.01 —.16 5 affix? .07 .10* -.24** -.15** .75 .76 6 23°32? .06 .11* -.06 .01 .57" .52 7 03:22 ' .16" .10* -.09 -.13* .65“ .43" However, careful inspection of the parameter estimates reveals some interesting findings. First, while not strong, the path between empathy and rectification seeking is in the positive direction [,6 = .15, P(-.O7 S ,6 S .37) = .95]. This may indicate a slight tendency for people who help in order to reduce their egoistic negative emotional state to .15 - - -.01 Concession .52 Rectification , Empathy ==> Seeking =9 Viewed as \ Apology Compliance Likert - Measure .76 -.29 Reward -.30 Concession Sympathy =9 Seeking =9 Viewed as Reward Figure 9. Results for predicted null model of the effect of motivation to reduce negative affect and altruistic helping on compliance 56 expect some sort of apology. Second, the negatively valenced path between sympathy and reward seeking was stronger than expected. Significant errors, small parameters, and an interest in further investigating the sympathy to reward seeking relationship justified revision of the proposed model. Therefore, empathy and rectification seeking were removed from the model due to insignificant relationships with other variables. The concession viewed as reward variable was also removed because it was now exogenous due to the removal of rectification Table 10. Results for the revised model of the effect of altruistic helping on compliance: Correlations, corrected correlations and path coefficients. Reward Concession Compliance Sympathy Seeking as Reward - Likert Sympathy -.29 .13 . 12 Reward *1: . - Seeking -.23 .30 .10 Concession . w: as Reward .10 —.24 .76 Compliance , n _ Like rt .10 -.09 .65 seeking. What remained was a causal string leading from sympathy to reward seeking to concession viewed as a reward and finally to compliance. The parameter estimates of the revised model are sufficiently strong (see Table 10). The errors observed between predicted and observed unconstrained correlations are low and localized tests of individual links reveal that errors do not deviate significantly from zero. The overall fit for the model is satisfactory [352(3) = 3.14, p=.3 7]. This indicates a pattern of relationships between these variables that suggests those who feel sympathy for someone in need, and are thus motivated to relieve the other’s pain, tend to 57 , Compliance -.29 Reward --30 Concessron -76 Likert - Sympathy => Seeking =9 Viewed as =9 Measure Reward Figure 10. Results for the revised model of the effect of altruistic helping on compliance. engage in less reward seeking. Reward seeking is negatively related to viewing a concession as a reward. Thus, those seeking to be rewarded for their help do not view the concession as appropriate while those not seeking rewards for their prior good deed evaluate the concession more positively. Viewing the concession as a reward ultimately leads people to take advantage of the reward and therefore comply to a greater extent. 58 CHAPTER IV DISCUSSION The norm of reciprocity, as described by Gouldner (1960), is proposed to govern a persons’ tendency to accept favors, to give favors, and to return favors. There is a rich history of social influence research suggesting the norm of reciprocity, and the mutual concessions it requires, are effective means of gaining compliance. This study sought to employ these processes - particularly the nonn’s requirement that people accept returned favors — in forwarding two new compliance strategies. The reverse prefavor compliance strategy was expected to increase compliance by motivating internal and external reward seeking and punishment avoidance. The transgression compliance strategy was expected to improve compliance based on a person’s desire to avoid instrumental punishments and then seek rectification for the transgression. It is important to have a large repertoire of compliance strategies from which to choose when attempting to influence exchange partners. While many of the relationships that were proposed to exist based on the relative effectiveness of the strategy manipulation and the anticipation of rewards manipulation were not found in the data, evidence for several important relationships were revealed. This chapter will discuss the significant relationships observed, as well as the proposed relationships that did not meet traditional levels of significance or meaningful size of effect, and place these in the context of extant research. Limitations and future research will also be forwarded. 59 Viewing concession as reward and apology The present data indicated that there were substantial positive relationships between viewing the concession as a reward and compliance, and viewing the concession as an apology and compliance. These relationships were expected to be high. Because many subjects did not comply with the final request, however, it may be more accurate to view the uncovered relationship slightly differently. That is, those who did not view the concession as a reward or apology did not comply. While this certainly would not be true in other situations applying the concession as a compliance strategy, because of the context of the situation and the framing of the request, these relationships seemed to explain a fair amount of variance in compliance scores. Reward seeking, on the other hand, was negatively related to viewing the concession as a reward and rectification seeking was unrelated to viewing the concession as a punishment. This suggests that when people seek rewards for a prior favor, they have provided that the concession is not viewed as an appropriate response, or conversely, that when not seeking rewards they find the concession to be a nice way to thank them. Here, subjects scored low on both reward seeking and rectification seeking. Originally, it was thought that a small initial favor request or transgression against a target would induce compliance in the initial request and thereby allow for the return of the favor or rectification of the harm done. Small favors and small transgressions may not be the most effective way of gleaning compliance on the target request. Perhaps a larger initial request or transgression may provide higher levels of reward seeking/rectification seeking, and at higher levels of these variables, the view of the concession as reward/rectification may be reversed. In the present study, with the small favor provided, 60 many people did not seek relationship repair. Because of this, when presented with the concession, they may not have seen the need for it, or perhaps they felt that if they accepted the concession, they would be overcompensated for such a minor favor previously provided. Another important finding was that sympathy was negatively related to reward seeking. Thus a path from sympathy to reward seeking, to viewing the concession as a reward, and ultimately to compliance was found to fit the data. Sympathy has been proposed to be an altruistic motivator (Baston & Shaw, 1991). The relationship between sympathy and reward seeking provides some evidence for this; however, one might expect that the relationship between these variables is stronger if sympathy is entirely fl altruistic. Anticipation of compensation also was found to have an impact on target compliance particularly in the transgression condition. The effect, however, was small and in the opposite direction than expected; that is, receiving the anticipation message diminished compliance with the target request. It is possible that indicating to subjects that they will be repaid proxied an apology and thereby diminished the need for firrther relationship repair in the form of a concession. Alternatively, the message may have indicated that something good was to come from the prior help, and subjects, when faced with an undesirable repayment, tended to either withdraw from the relationship or wait for a repayment that was congruent with their expectations. Implications for the norm of reciprocity and mutual concessions Several caveats to the norm of reciprocity were first observed by Gouldner (1960) and, more recently, scholars wishing to invoke the norm to gain compliance with a .1 l '"l request (i.e. Boster et al., 1995) Gouldner suggested that the norm functioned to stabilize societies by requiring that when a favor was given, a recipient was required to not harm the favor giver, and that a giver would not harm a recipient. This suggests that it is perhaps most functional for a lag to exist between the giving of a favor and the repayment of that favor. In the current study, the compliance strategies called for a favor to be given to the compliance agent and it was expected that the compliance agent would be able to capitalize on this favor owed. The data suggest that this form of repayment was not particularly acceptable to compliance targets. Thus they may anticipate allowing the favor to be repaid at another time with a repayment deemed to be more acceptable. Alternatively, mutual concessions are expected to work by first retreating from an initial extreme position and, because of this concession by the compliance agent, a compliance target is expected to reciprocate the gesture and thus move from a position of non compliance to a position of compliance (Cialdini, 1975; Hale & Laliker, 1999; Turner et al, 2007). The concession, in norm of reciprocity terms, is seen as a favor that requires repayment. Cialdini, 1975) has argued however that consumers are becoming much more savvy to this strategy and thus if the concession is not seen as a legitimate offer of retreat, defensive mechanisms are activated. The strategies in the present research were developed, in part, to disable these defensive mechanisms by providing a justification for the concession. It is possible, however, that the act of providing a concession — moving from an extreme position to a less extreme position — satisfied the obligation to repay the prior favor given to the compliance agent. This retreat is presumed to be, at least in part (Cialdini, 2004) viewed as a concession that is subsequently reciprocated; however, less is known about the verbal retreat and how this by itself might 62 satisfy a debt. In the present research, it was expected that the retreat (concession) would be viewed as repayment only when the compliance target actually took advantage of the offer; however, one reason for relatively low levels of compliance may be that the offer of the concession was viewed as repayment. Gouldner suggests that by not adhering to the norm of reciprocity a person may invite social sanctions or ostrication from the system. In this, Gouldner suggests that q - i regulation of the system is conducted by those in the system. By being presented with the reverse prefavor and the transgression inductions, a compliance target is positioned as one up in the social arrangement. Not allowing repayment of a prior favor is a violation of the norm of reciprocity as it was originally described. It may be that someone in this one up position feels freer to leave the exchange without guilt. The compliance target may offend the compliance agent by violating the norm, but if they wish to escape from the exchange then this is ineffectual. In the scenarios presented there was no indication that others were observing the interaction, so possible ostrication from the larger system may also be minimized for this violation. Ultimately violating the norm of reciprocity may be more serious when one refuses to repay a debt; however, violating the norm by refusing repayment may be a less serious violation, or at times, may even be expected (Flynn, 2003). Additionally, evidence of the norm of reciprocity has been found to exist among strangers (exchange relationships) but not friends (communal relationships) (Boster, et a1. 1995). As Cialdini (2001) notes, mutual concessions is a powerful compliance strategy because a compliance agent can make the initial concession and thereby begin the process of exchange. The general concession strategy calls for the compliance agent to 63 put themselves in a one-up position in the exchange. This may make the relational parameters salient to the target, which would indicate that the parties are in an exchange relationship and that the exchange is presently unequal. The reverse prefavor and the transgression, by design, however place the compliance agent in a one-down position. This may specify a different set of relational parameters to the compliance target. The target may view the willingness of an agent to place themselves in a one down position as indicating that the agent views the target as a communal relational partner rather than an exchange partner. Therefore, the exchange of favors may become less based on exchange and more on altruism. Organizational literature, for example, has noted that trust can develop quite quickly in certain situations (McKnight, Cummings, & Chervany, 1998), which may move a relationship from one grounded in exchange to a communal relationship. If this is the case, it might explain why compliance in the treatment conditions was lower than expected. Put simply, given the way the relationship is redefined the concession and therefore compliance may be unnecessary. Raw compliance The raw level of compliance in the experimental conditions appears promising, with 44.1% of subjects in the RPF conditions reporting that they would comply with the target request and 49% of subjects reporting that they would comply in the transgression conditions. The RPF and transgression compliance strategies were predicted to perform better than control conditions but were found to not be as successful in gaining compliance. As argued previously, the sequential strategies first require that a compliance agent perform a transgression against someone or ask them for a prior help and then offer a concession as repayment or rectification. There are many times, particularly in 64 organizations, when one purposely or inadvertently causes harm or slights another (i.e. Anderson & Pearson, 1999). These data suggest that when relationships are strained do to a favor already being owed or a harm previously enacted, it is possible to gain compliance with a request by presenting it as a concession. It is difficult to know whether a concession within this context would outperform other strategies; however, nearly 50% compliance in conditions where the requester has either caused some harm or is already in debt to the target seems like a successful strategy. High levels of compliance in the control conditions do not reflect the levels of compliance in other similar compliance literature using the sale of tickets as a final compliance request. Traditionally, the compliance request of tickets purchased has been used in studies evaluating the effectiveness of the unsolicited favor. Goie and Boster (2005) reported subjects in their control conditions as purchasing .68 tickets, while Boster et al., (1995) reported .69 tickets purchased for strangers and 2.94 for friends in control conditions. In Goei, et al., (2003) the authors reported control conditions purchased 1.4 tickets, and Regan (1971) reported that subjects purchased .8 tickets from unpleasant confederates and 1.5 tickets from confederates in control conditions. In this study, subjects in control conditions reported that they would purchase a mean of 2.86 (mode = 2) tickets in the concession control and 2.73 (mode = 2) tickets in the direct request control conditions. It is unlikely that these differences are due to drawing from different populations since many of the subjects in previous studies are from the same “Midwestern University” as the present study. Hence, it appears that compliance scores may be unnaturally high in the control conditions. Ultimately it is difficult to know 65 _"~ whether the inflation in compliance scores is limited to the control conditions or if the levels of compliance across conditions are proportionally increased. Why the treatment conditions fared worse that control conditions is not entirely clear. In addition to the issues mentioned above One explanation may be the concession strategy used. The concession strategy utilized in the study is a variation of the that’s-not- all (TNA) technique (Burger, 1986). That is, a price is given for the tickets and before the subject can reply, the deal is improved. Some have argued that the effectiveness of the strategy is not entirely based on the concession explanation, but rather because the initial price sets an anchor point of reference for the value of the item (Burger et al, 1999; Cialdini, 2004). Thus when deciding whether or not to comply, the high anchor point tends to increase the likelihood that the better deal will seem more attractive, and within the range of acceptance. The present study expected to increase compliance over the concession control, in part, by giving a reason for the concession and deactivating defensive mechanisms. It was expected that if subjects had a valid reason why they, in particular, were getting a bargain, it would work to increase compliance. If, however, the anchor point or some other mechanism drives the effect generally, the present scenario may have worked to draw attention away from this and instead made the irrelevant relational variables more salient, negating the effectiveness of other, more useful mechanisms. Initial non-compliers Those who did not comply with the initial request complied more with the subsequent concession compliance message than subjects who complied with the initial request in both the RPF conditions and transgression conditions. There are several 66 reasons why this may be the case. First, these individuals could be high in equity sensitivity, described by Husman, Hatfield, and Miles (1987) as entitleds, and seek to be overcompensated at the expense of others. They may not want to help when they see little benefit to themselves in doing so as in the initial small request. However, they may be prone to subsequently view the concession as being a good deal and therefore take advantage of it. ran Another possibility is that, because they denied the person initially, they may feel that not complying with a request for a second time may be in poor form. Internal and external motivations for gaining rewards and avoiding punishments may become high 1”“ when presented with the second request. Denying someone’s request once may be justified, however when faced with not helping a second time, it may be inconsistent with their view of themselves as a helpful person or they may become particularly concerned that others will view them as unhelpful with repeated denials of aid. Third, they may have not helped initially because they viewed the compliance agent negatively due to not being prepared for the exam. It is possible that their view of the compliance agent changed when they were presented with more information. In this situation, raising money for their old high school is something a good person would do. Giving a deal on the tickets is something a good person would do. Hence, some subjects may be more likely to withhold favors to pe0ple they view negatively, and give favors to those individuals who they view positively. Fourth, the process of complying with a concession after an initial rejection does resemble the door-in-the-face strategy (Cialdini et al., 1975). However, in this case, instead of the DITF taking the form of rejection-then—retreat it takes the form of rejection- . z rejection-then retreat. Perhaps this is too much to bear for even the most stubborn compliers. A final reason why final compliance may have been increased in conditions where subjects did not comply with the initial request may be due to not receiving the message setting up the concession. These subjects did not receive the additional “You really saved me last week I have been thinking about a good way to pay you back for helping me out with that blue book and I think I have come up with something that you will really like. ” This message was intended to frame the concession as relationship repair for the treatment conditions. It is possible that it had the unintended consequence of setting subjects’ reward expectations too high, resulting in a negative reaction when faced with the concession. Therefore, this message may have reduced compliance in treatment conditions making it appear that it was initial non-compliers that had increased compliance. Limitations and firture research Future researchers wishing to test the veracity of the RPF and transgression would do well to first establish its usefulness under several conditions rather than test the many paths of why it might bring about compliance. One possibility would be to give the control conditions the entire scenario, including the initial request. This would allow for a better understanding of different compliance strategies that might work when a compliance agent already “owes” a favor or an apology. For example, the relative effectiveness of the direct request and concession could be compared to conditions where the “here is how I am going to repay you” message is delivered. Alternatively, it was thought that a verbal thanks (or apology) may reduce the reward seeking/rectification 68 seeking behaviors and subsequent compliance. Given the results of the present study, it may be beneficial to reduce reward seeking (rectification seeking) in order to increase compliance. Thus, paired with a verbal thanks or apology, a concession may be more effective in gaining compliance. A second limitation to the present study is that the control conditions varied in a variety of ways other than the delivery of a specific compliance message. Subjects in control conditions did not complete the items measuring the thirteen other variables. While attempts were made to keep the scenarios relatively similar in content, the scenarios were significantly shorter in control groups and were potentially more cognitively accessible when determining whether they would comply than experimental groups. Future research may also seek to better understand the anticipation of compensation message. In the present study, the anticipation message may have had a slightly negative relationship with compliance. It appears that subjects did not view the concession as a reward/apology when they were seeking one. Perhaps an induction that reduces rather than increases reward/apology seeking may be beneficial. A message such as, “I cannot see how I could ever repay you but...” may work to increase compliance with the subsequent request. The scenarios in the study also included trading favors that are relatively easily quantifiable in dollar and cents. Future research can determine if subjects pay particular attention to the monetary value of exchanged favors, and if they do, whether other types of favor exchange might show higher levels of compliance. This study sought to determine whether the concession could be framed as an apology or as a reward. The 69 apology and the reward may be useful for framing other types of requests. There may be times when it is unclear to a compliance target what the advantages and disadvantages are for complying. In these cases, it may be profitable for a compliance agent to frame the request as a reward. For example, in an organizational setting, someone may wish to get out of participating in a cross functional team. They may attempt to induce a coworker to take their place by telling them, “Because I owe you a favor I want you to take my place P in this prestigious team.” «- Another limitation of the present research is that the scenario inductions required subjects to effectively imagine themselves in the situations presented and successfully : perceive the inductions. Some of the measures may be particularly susceptible to this artifact. For example, the measure of distress was a measure of how distressed the subject believed the compliance agent to be with regard to owing relationship repair. In order to respond to items on this scale, a subject must first be able to imagine himself in the situation, then be able to imagine himself in the position of the compliance agent, and then imagine what the compliance agent was feeling. That is a lot of imagining. We might expect that some people would be unable to do this effectively; particularly those who are low in perspective taking and fantasy (see Davis, 1983). While it is hoped that the inductions were successful, and perhaps this is evidenced by several observed significant relationships, clearly a laboratory or field study would allow for the opportunity for expanded confidence in the data. Moving to a more realistic methodology where subjects feel the effects of the reverse prefavor and transgression may ultimately result in findings that deviate from those presented here. 70 "112*” Appendix A Motivation to gain rewards scale Rewards (material) 1. If I help it would be because I expect that the favor I provide to my classmate will be repaid sometime. I would be looking for a reward if I were to help. _ This classmate will do something for me because of the favor that I provide. If I help it would be because if the situation presents itself, my classmate will do me a favor. My classmate is unlikely to repay me for the favor I provide. (R) If I help I do not expect repayment in this situation. (R) If I help I believe this classmate will not attempt to help me in return. (R) By helping now, I will gain in the end. PP!" .°°.".°‘.V' Rewards (external) 1. People will like me more if I help my classmate. 2. I believe that people would admire me if I help. 3. If I help in this situation it would increase my reputation. 4. If I help in this situation people will think I am a good person. Rewards (internal) 1. I would get warm fuzzy feelings if I helped. 2. It would make me feel good to help out my classmate in this situation. 3. I would help in this situation because of the way it makes me feel inside. 4. Helping in this situation would make me feel like I am a nice person. 71 Appendix B Motivation to avoid punishment scale Punishment (material — no partner) The actions of my classmate hurt me if I do not help. I would be harmed if I do not help my classmate. I feel required to help my classmate. By providing this favor, I help both my classmate and myself. If I do not help my classmate I will lose out. This favor I may provide aids me as much as it aids them. 9999?)!“ Punishment (external) 1. If I ever needed help, people would not help me if I do not help my classmate. 2. My peers would think bad things of me if I do not help. 3. Others would show their disapproval of my actions if I do not aid my classmate. 4. My classmate would not like me if I do not help them. Punishment (internal) 1. Giving a favor in this situation may help me to avoid guilty feelings. 2. I would feel lousy if I did not help my classmate. 3. I would have painful memories of this situation if I did not help. 4. I would be in the wrong if I did not help here. 72 Frm " I" Appendix C Empathy scale 1. Seeing my classmate in this situation makes me feel distressed. 2. I feel the agony that my classmate is feeling. 3. When I see my classmate in this situation I feel kind of protective toward them. 4. Because this classmate feels worried, I also am feeling worried. 73 Appendix D Sympathy scale 1. I feel sorry for my classmate. I have tender, concerned feelings for my classmate. In this situation I don’t have much pity for my classmate. (R) My classmate has my sympathy. 5‘5”!" 74 Appendix E Reward seeking scale 1. There is no reason for this classmate to return the favor I did for them.(R) 2. My classmate owes me for helping them. 3. My classmate is indebted to me for what I did. 4. I will figure out a way for my classmate to return the favor. 75 Appendix F Rectification seeking scale 1. My classmate should apologize for all the trouble they caused. 2. If I were in my classmate’s position I would show remorse for putting me in that position. 3. There is no need for my classmate to say they are sorry. (R) 4. I would appreciate some acknowledgement by my classmate for the situation I was put in. 76 i Appendix G Concession viewed as reward scale 1. This was a nice way to thank me for my prior help. 2. The offer made was not a fair way to pay me back.(R) 3. I liked the creative way that I was rewarded for my earlier assistance. 4. My classmate thoughtfully returned my earlier favor. 77 Appendix H Concession viewed as apology scale 9:599)!“ The deal my classmate gave me was a good way to say “sorry”. By offering this deal my classmate was making a request for forgiveness. My classmate regrets having put me out so they made it right the best they could. The deal I got was because my classmate owed me an apology. This was an inappropriate way to admit fault. (R) 78 Appendix I Distress scale 1. My classmate feels unsettled about owing me a favor. 2. By being helped previously my classmate believes they are in a bind. 3. My classmate wants us to be even. 4. This classmate feels distress over needing to repay me. 5. Because I helped my classmate previously, now they feel bad. 6. My classmate feels obligated to me. 79 Appendix J Compliance Items 1. I would buy several tickets. 2. There is no way that I would buy any tickets.(R) 3. In this situation I am likely to purchase some tickets. 4. Purchasing tickets is a good idea at this time. 80 r“- Appendix K Endnote 1 While not predicted, an alternative model where rectification seeking is removed, fits the data quite well 98(1) = 3.57, p=.3l] 81 REFERENCES Anderson, L. M., & Pearson, C. M. (1999). Tit for tat? The spiraling effect of incivility in the workplace. Academy of Management Review, 24, 452-471. Baston, C.D., & Shaw, L.L.(l99l). Evidence for altruism: Toward a pluralism of prosocial motives. Psychological Inquiry, 2, 107-122. Batson, C.D., & Flory, JD. (1990). Goal-relevant cognitions associated with helping by individuals high on intrinsic, end religion. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 29, 346-360. Bauman, D.J., Cialdini, R.B., & Kenrick, D.T. (1981). Altruism as hedonism: Helping and self-gratification as equivalent responses. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 40, 1039-1046. Becker, H. (1956). Man in reciprocity, New York: Prager. Bendapudi, N., Singh, S.N., & Bendapudi, V. (1996) Enhancing helping behavior: An integrative framework for promotion planning. Journal of Marketing, 60, 33-40. Benoit, W.L. (1995) Accounts, excuses, and apologies: A theory of image restoration strategies. Albany: State University of New York Press. Benton, A. A., Kelley, H. H., & Liebling, B. (1972). Effects of extremity of offers and concession rate on the outcomes of bargaining. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 24, 73-83. Blau, RM. (1964). Exchange and power in social life. New York: Wiley. Boster, F.J., Rodriguez, J.I., Cruz, M.G., & Marshall, L.(1995). The relative effectiveness of a direct request message and a pregiving message on friends and strangers. Communication Research, 22, 475 -— 484. Brehm, J.W. (1966) A Theory of Psychological Reactance. New York: Academic Press. Brunero, IS (2002). Evolution, Altruism and "Internal Reward" Explanations. The Philosophical Forum 33, 413—424. Burger, J.M. (1986). Increasing compliance by improving the deal: The that’s not all technique. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 277-283. Burger, J.M., Horita, M., Kinoshita, L., Roberts, K., & Vera C. (1997) Effects of time on the norm of reciprocity. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 19, 91-100. 82 Clark, MS. (1981). Noncomparability of benefits given and receive: A cue to the existence of friendship. Social psychology Quarterly, 41, 375-381. Clark, M.S. & Mills, J.R., (1979). Interpersonal attraction in exchange and communal relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 3 7, 12-24. Cialdini, RB. (2001). Influence: Science and practice (4th ed.) Needham Heights, MA: Allen & Bacon. Cialdini, RB. (2004). Social influence: Compliance and conformity. Annual Review of Sociology, 55, 591-621. he Cialdini, R.B., Vincent, J.E., Lewis, S.K., Catalan, J., Wheeler, D., & Darby, BL. (1975) Reciprocal concessions procedure for inducing compliance: The door-in-the-face technique. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 31, 206-215 Chertkoff, J. M., & Conley, M. (1967). Opening offer and frequency of concession as bargaining strategies. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 7, 185-193. 0.... Davis, M.H. (1983). Measuring individual differences in empathy: Evidence for a multidimensional approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44, 113-126. Dillard,J. P. (1991). The current status of research on sequential-request compliance techniques. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 1 7, 283-288. Dillard,J. P., Hunter,.l. E., & Burgoon, M. E. (1984). Sequential request persuasive strategies: Meta-analysis of foot-in-the-door and door-in-the-face. Human Communication Research, 70, 461-488. Eisenberg, N. (1991). Values, sympathy, and individual differences: Toward a pluralism of factors influencing altruism and empathy. Psychological inquiry, 2, 128-131. Eisenberger, R., Lynch, P., Aselage, J. & Rohdieck, S. (2004). Who takes the most revenge? Individual differences in negative reciprocity norm endorsement. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 787-799. Ellis, J. B. (forthcoming). Psychological contracts: Does work status affect perceptions of making and keeping promises? Management Communication Quarterly. Fern, E. F., Monroe, K. B., & Avila, R. A. (1986). Effectiveness of multiple request strategies: A synthesis of research results. Journal of Marketing Research, 23, 144-152. Fischer, W.F. (1963). Sharing in preschool children as a function of amount and type of reinforcement. Genetic Psychology Monographs, 68, 215-245. 83 ' Flynn, RI. (2003). "How much should I give and how often? The effects of generosity and frequency of favor exchange on social status and productivity". Academy of Management Journal, 46 , p. 539- 555. Frantz, C.M., & Benningson, C. (2005). Better late than early: The influence of timing on apology effectiveness. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 41 , 201-207. Goffman E. (1971). The Presentation of Self in Everyday Life. Harmondsworth: Penguin. Goei, R., Boster, RI. (2005). The roles of obligation and gratitude in explaining the effect of favors on compliance. Communication Monographs, 72, 284-300. F“- Goie, R., Massi-Lindsey, L.L., Boster, F .J . Skalski, P.D., & Bowman, J.M. (2003). The mediating roles of liking and obligation on the relationship between favors and compliance. Communication Research, 30, 178-197. Gouldner, A.W. (1960) the norm of reciprocity: A preliminary statement. American , Sociological Review, 25, 161—178. L Graen, G. B., & Uhl-Bien, M. (1995). Relationship-based approach to leadership: Development of leader-member exchange (LMX) theory of leadership over 25 years: Applying a multi-level multi-domain perspective. Leadership Quarterly, 6, 219-247. Hale, J.L. & Laliker, M. (1999). Explaining the door-in—the face: Is it really time to abandon reciprocal concessions? Communication Studies, 50, 203-211. Ho, VT. (2005). Social influence on evaluations of psychological contract fulfillment. The Academy of Management Review 30, 113-121 Homans, GO (195 8) Social behavior as exchange. American Journal of Sociology, 63, 597-606. Hobhouse, LT. (1906) Morals in evolution: A study in comparative ethics. London: Chapman & Hall. Hoffman, M. L. (1981). Is altruism part of human nature? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 40, 121-137. Hunter, J.E. & Gerbing, D.W. (1982). Unidimensional measurement, second order factor analysis and causal models. In B.M. Staw & L.L. Cummings (Eds.), Research in Organizational Behavior, 4, 267-320. Greenwich, CT: JAI Press. Husman, R.C., Hatfield, J.D., & Miles E.W. (1987). A new perspective on equity theory: The equity sensitivity construct. Academy of Management Review, 12, 222-234. 84 ' Komorita, S. S., & Brenner, A. R. (1968). Bargaining and concession making under bilateral monopoly. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 9, 15-20. McCullough, M.E., Rachal, K.C., Sandage, S.J., Worthington, E.L., Brown, S.W., & Hight, TL. (1998). Interpersonal forgiving in close relationships 11: Theoretical elaboration and measurement, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 1586—1603. McKnight D.H., Cummings, L.L., & Chervany, N.L. (1998). Initial trust formation in new organizational relationships. The Academy of Management Review; 23, 473 — 490. F3“ Moorman, R. H. (1991). Relationship Between Organizational Justice and Organizational Citizenship Behaviors: Do Fairness Perceptions Influence Employee Citizenship? Journal of Applied Psychology, 76, 845-855. Morrison, E. W., & Robinson, S. L. (1997). When employees feel betrayed: A model of [ how psychological contract violation develops. Academy of Management Review, - 22, 226-256. Ohbuchi, K. Kameda, M. & Agarie, N. (1998). Apology as a ggression control: It’s role in mediating appraisal of and response to harm. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 219-227. Okeefe, D.J. & Figge, M. (1999). Guilt and expected guilt in the door-in-the-face technique. Communication Monographs, 66, 312-325. Organ, D. W (1988). A restatement of the satisfaction-performance hypothesis. Journal of Management, 14, 547-557. Regan D.T. (1971). Effects of a favor and liking on compliance. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 7, 627-639. Reis H.T. & Gruzen, J. (1976). On mediating equity, equality, and self-interest: The role of self presentation in social exchange. Journal of Experimental and social Psychology, 12, 487-503. Robinson, S.L., (1996). Trust and breach of the psychological contract. Administrative Science Quarterly, 41 , 574-599 Robinson, S.L., Rousseau, D.M. (1994). Violating the psychological contract: Not the exception but the norm. Journal of Organizational Behavior 15, 245-259. Scher, S.J., & Darley, J .M. (1997)How effective are the things people say to apologize? Effects of the realization of the apology speech act. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 26, 127-140. 85 ' Seers, A. Petty, M.M., Cashman, J.F. (1995). Team-member exchange under team and traditional management. Group and Organization Management, 20, 18-38. Simmel, G. (1950). The sociology of George Simmel, translated and edited by Kurt H. Wolff, Glencoe, IL: Free Press. Sober, E. & Wilson, DS. (1989). Unto others: The evolution and psychology of unselfish behavior. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press Thumwald, R. (1932). Economics in primitive communities. London: Oxford University Press. Trivers, R.L. (1971). The evolution of reciprocal altruism. Quarterly Review of Biology, 46: 35-57. Turner, M.M., Tamborini, R. Limon, M.S., & Zuckerrnan-Hyman, C. (2007). The moderators and mediators of Door-in-the-face requests: Is it a negotiation or a helping experience? Communication Monographs, 74, 333-3 56. Tumley, W. H., D. C. Feldman. 1999. The impact of psychological contract violations on exit, voice, loyalty and neglect. Human Relations 52, 895-922. Van Dyne, L. & Butler Ellis, J. (2004). Job creep: A reactance theory perspective on OCB as overfulfillment of obligations. In J. Coyle-Shapiro, L.M. Shore, S. Taylor, & L.E. Tetrick (Eds), The employment relationship: Examining psychological and contextual perspectives (181-205). Oxford England:Oxford University Press. Wispé, L. (1986). The distinction between sympathy and empathy: To call forth a concept, a word is needed. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 314-321. Worthington, E.L. & Wade, N.G. (1999). The psychology of unforgiveness and forgiveness and implications for clinical practice. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 18, 385-319. 86 r[rillrrrrlurrrrr1]“