. .1 u... if ... a 5 v. ..f . ‘ Lumw“ st... ”94....3... «a»: z. , N .mu 1.434!» #4} r x. 9.... {sauna it, A ... "Orv zriu: . a? V , 4W" ‘ L...“ V _ ‘ :‘l ‘7‘" Q. \ 5 A; 2!: “my 7.0.; a“; A. v mu? . 3.“ r1 .3. h). . . “Hafiz! all. «tiPUrhII If K)‘ ‘ III a II. «1.2. ‘l. \ufilIIAOItV Trix... it. it!!! . .I 9! . a... $3,! IJBRARY Michigan §tate University This is to certify that the dissertation entitled ORCHESTRATING PRODUCTIVE DISCUSSION: A STUDY OF DIALOGIC DISCOURSE AND PARTICIPATION IN SCIENCE CLASSROOMS presented by LINDSEY MOHAN has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for the Doctoral degree in Counseling, Educational Psychology, and Special Education / Major Professor's Signature a .‘f/zt 1/ 200? Date MSU is an afi‘innative-action, equal-opportunity employer PLACE IN RETURN Box to remove this checkout from your record. TO AVOID FINES return on or before date due. MAY BE RECALLED with earlier due date if requested. DATE DUE DATE DUE DATE DUE 5/08 K'IProyAchrelelRC/DateDue indd ORCHESTRATING PRODUCTIVE DISCUSSION: A STUDY OF DIALOGIC DISCOURSE AND PARTICIPATION IN SCIENCE CLASSROOMS By Lindsey Mohan A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Counseling, Educational Psychology, and Special Education 2008 ABSTRACT ORCHESTRATING PRODUCTIVE DISCUSSION: A STUDY OF DIALOGIC DISCOURSE AND PARTICIPATION IN SCIENCE CLASSROOMS By Lindsey Mohan Although we expect students to engage in the discourses of science, there is relatively little research that documents how teachers accomplish this in their classrooms. The goal of this study was to develop a theory of how dialogic discourse helped this goal be realized in two classrooms. Classroom discussions of two exemplary science teachers (approximately ten hours for each teacher), along with video recall sessions and teacher interviews were analyzed using open-coding method to identify dialogic discourse indicators, such as authentic teacher questions, Open invitations, student questions and new ideas, expressing value for student contributions, positioning through agreement of disagreement, assigning authorship, and evaluating accounts (either collectively or individually). The space afforded in these discussions allowed for student participation with science practices, which were guided by classroom socioscientific norms, such as norms for collective validation of observations and explanations. Discussions, especially those characterized by dialogism, enabled student participation with science practices and discourse. Implications of what these discussions afford students in terms of participation within a social community, and how illustrations of rich discussions and exemplary teaching can be used in pre-service and in-service professional development are discussed. Limitations and directions for future research are also considered. Copyright by LINDSEY MOHAN 2008 ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS This dissertation is a culmination of six years as a doctoral student and teacher, during which time I had the opportunity to collaborate with remarkable people. First, I would like to acknowledge three faculty members and mentors that have given me guidance and support during my studies—Dr. Mary L. Lundeberg, Dr. Charles W. Anderson (“Andy”), and the late Dr. Michael Pressley. All provided invaluable help at times when I needed it most. Dr. Lundeberg taught me to balance my work with the things that mattered most: my family, friends, and my love of the outdoors. Dr. Anderson was an amazing sounding board when I struggled to pull all the pieces together. The late Dr. Pressley was responsible for bringing me into the field of educational research, gave me immeasurable support during my undergraduate and graduate studies, and taught me to be the researcher I am today. Thank you, too, to the other members of my dissertation committee, including Dr. Angela Calabrese Barton and Dr. Mary M. Juzwik. Each provided excellent feedback and insight into my work. I would also like to acknowledge the support I received from Dr. Jack Smith, especially during my early studies in graduate school. He helped me muddle through difficult questions and was thoughtful about my work. I would also like to thank Dr. Jere Brophy who stepped in to give me feedback on my research when l was struggling to overcome the loss of an important mentor in my life. My experience in graduate school would not have been the same without my friends and colleagues. I would first like to thank Alison Billman for always being supportive and interested in my work (and a great stats buddy, and pottery partner tool). Thanks to Donna Forrest-Pressley for letting me crash at her place on my visits to Michigan. Thanks to Lisa Raphael Bogaert and Sara Dolezal Kersey for allowing me to be part of their work on motivation in classrooms. Thanks also to Katie Hilden and Lauren F ingeret, my amazing office buddies, who I miss dearly now that I am gone. And thanks to other friends I made who always provided interesting conversations about my work, and endless diversions away from it (especially Erin Wibbens, Blakely Tsurusaki, Howard Glasser, Annie Moses). 1 would also like to thank the remarkable teachers I was able to observe and talk with during this study—their teaching is truly an inspiration and I was lucky to be invited into their classrooms. Thanks to Pat Christensen and Wendell Hocking, as well as the numerous exemplary teachers I collaborated with over the years. I owe my family thanks—my parents, Tim and Karen Mohan, who pretended to know what in the world I was talking about! Thanks to my brother Nathan, and his wife Jan, who provided opportunities for me to regain my sanity. Thanks especially to my sister, Audrey, who was the one who truly understood (and sympathized!) with my life in academics. Also I would like to thank my stepdaughter, Aubrey, for providing endless entertainment and diversion while I completed my dissertation. Certainly, none of this would be possible without the support of my husband, John Hawkins. He listened when I needed him (even when he was thousands of miles away), he read my work, rubbed my shoulders after hours of writing at my desk, and volunteered to be my personal ‘geek squad’ when I was learning how to use and manage video data. He supported me in too many ways to count, but most importantly he encouraged me to go forward when I doubted myself, and helped me to enjoy the most important things in life. Thank you, Johnny. This material is based on work supported in part by the National Science Foundation, under special project number ESI - 0353406 as part of the Teacher Professional Continuum program. Any opinion, finding, conclusions or recommendation expressed in this publication are those of the author and do not necessarily reflect views of the supporting institutions. vi TABLE OF CONTENTS List of tables .................................................................................................................. ix List of figures .................................................................................................................. x Introduction .................................................................................................................... 1 Purpose of the Study ................................................................................................... 4 Scientific Literacy as an Educational Goal ................................................................... 6 Defining Literacy .................................................................................................... 6 Scientific Literacy as Participating with Practices .................................................... 8 The Central Role of Discourse in Classrooms ............................................................ 15 Research Questions ................................................................................................... 22 Methods ........................................................................................................................ 25 Design ....................................................................................................................... 25 Participants ............................................................................................................... 26 Researcher ................................................................................................................. 30 Data Sources ............................................................................................................. 30 Data Analysis ............................................................................................................ 3] Criteria for Evaluation and Validation ....................................................................... 39 Findings and Discussion ................................................................................................ 42 Introduction to Findings ............................................................................................ 42 Part 1: Dialogic Indicators and Norms for Discussion .................................................... 43 Diversity of Accounts ................................................................................................ 43 Multiple Accounts were Elicited ............................................................................ 43 Student Accounts were Valued. ............................................................................. 47 Linking Accounts ...................................................................................................... 54 Accounts were Authored. ...................................................................................... 55 Accounts were Positioned ...................................................................................... 5 7 Talking about Talk .................................................................................................... 65 Challenges to Participating in Dialogic Discourse ..................................................... 67 Balancing Teacher and Student Voices. ................................................................. 67 Balancing Structure and Spontaneity. .................................................................... 76 Interplay Between Writing and Talking. ................................................................ 77 Part2: Classroom Socioscientific Norms ........................................................................ 80 Dialogism and Emergence of Norms ......................................................................... 80 Validation of Observations ........................................................................................ 8] Validation of Explanations ........................................................................................ 91 Meta-level Discussion about Models and Consensus ............................................... 102 Summary and Discussion of Emerging Theory ............................................................ I 1] vii Conclusions and Implications ...................................................................................... l 15 Limitations and Directions for Future Research ....................................................... l 18 Conclusion .............................................................................................................. 122 Appendix A: Coding Book .......................................................................................... 134 Appendix B: Ms. Hocking’s interview ........................................................................ 139 Appendix C: Ms. Christensen’s interview ................................................................... 143 Appendix D: Terms ..................................................................................................... 147 References .................................................................................................................. l 50 viii LIST OF TABLES Table 1 Participant Demographics ........................................................... 123 Table 2 Relationship between. Dialogic Indicators and Discussion Norm .............. 124 ix Figure 1 Figure 2 Figure 3 Figure 4 Figure 5 LIST OF FIGURES Experience, Patterns, and Explanations (EPE) model ........................... 129 Inaccurate or incomplete observations of light beams traveling through a curved jar .............................................................................. 130 Accurate observation of two beams of light traveling through a curved jar and what is observed when one beam is covered.............................13l Diagram of water collecting on the inside of the plastic-covered water dish ..................................................................................... 132 Relationship between dialogism and socioscientific norms and practice..... 133 ORCHESTRATING PRODUCTIVE DISCUSSION: A STUDY OF DIALOGIC DI SCOURSE AND PARTICIPATION IN SCIENCE CLASSROOMS How do students come to appropriately and publicly communicate with forms of talk or text that are part of being a competent hypothesizer, evidence provider, maker of distinction, checker of facts? (0 ’Connors & Michaels, 1996, p. 64) INTRODUCTION In recent decades, science educators have been engaged in extended discussion about effective teaching of science. The products of science education reform efforts in the 1990’s were standards and standards-based curricula, which were used to make science teaching in American schools more coherent and consistent. The National Research Council (NRC) and the Association for Advancement in Science (AAAS) supported scientific literacy as the contemporary goal for science education. In elaborating their vision of scientific literacy, NRC (1996) stated, “everyone needs to be able to engage intelligently in public discourse and debate about important issues that involve science and technology” (p. 1). Indeed, this need is greater than ever, as nations can no longer afford to have citizens unable to participate in national and global affairs. Many global problems we face require actions based on information that science can provide (e.g., global warming, habitat destruction and deforestation, shrinking of tropical rainforest and artic ecosystems, the spread of the AIDS virus, etc.). Yet, even in educated and democratic societies, such as the United States, a majority of citizens remain unable to use science as a resource to engage in public discussions or to inform their everyday actions. Science education in the United States has been criticized for producing citizens who are poorly prepared to use science in their lives (Eisenhart, F inkel, & Marion, 1996). Citizens—both students and adults— rarely invoke science as a resource for reasoning and communicating, which begs the question: Why are they unable to do so given that many receive at least ten or more years of scientific study in schools? One possible reason is that the culture of science in schools is one that supports the transmission of already known scientific “knowledge” from teachers to students, emphasizing recitation of facts in the course of independent, rather than collective, study. As Sharma and Anderson (2007) discussed, a distorted image of science is substantiated when “scientists’ science” is recontextualized for learning science in schools. The messiness of the scientific enterprise is packaged as “scientific truths” for students to memorize. This is not to say that science teachers knowingly intend for students to learn science through memorization, rather the typical culture of science in schools does not encourage teachers and students to move beyond those modes of participation. As Sharma and Anderson (2007) and others (e.g., Doyle, 1983; Lave & Wenger, 1991) have argued, classrooms are characterized by the continual negotiation for grades that occurs between teachers and students—what Lave and Wenger (1991) called “learning to display knowledge for evaluation” and Doyle (1983) described as the “performance for grade exchange”. Sharma and Anderson further explained that “instead of having students learn science through inquiry—a pedagogical practice that is relatively much more ambiguous and risky for students, students learn science by reproducing simple canonical knowledge and practices in exchange for grades and compliance. Complexity and meaningfulness is bartered away for ease and efficiency.” (p. 10). Instead of inquiry, which is supported by science standards, students learn science through a superficial display of information, or what has been termed procedural display (e.g., Anderson, 2003; Bloome, Puro, & Theodorou, 1989; Heath, 1983; Lave & Wenger, 1991). Furthermore, the discourse that dominates classroom interaction is overwhelmingly authoritative—there is one story being told from the teacher/textbook to students through monologic classroom activities such as lecture, taking notes, completing question-answer worksheets, following lab procedures, and participating in the well documented communication pattern of IRE (initiate, respond, evaluate) (Mehan, 1979). Thus, novice learners do not learn to question knowledge represented in textbooks, the Internet, or from teachers, because the scientific knowledge and discourse demands what Bahktin (1981) called “our unconditional allegiance” (p. 342). This is unfortunate given that the scientific knowledge and discourse presented to students are often not inline with what students bring to school. Science teachers rarely take advantage of the vast experiential base that students bring to their classroom. Indeed, students have few opportunities to negotiate scientific knowledge and discourse with what they already know about the world, creating a potential separation of what students come to know in and out of schools (Resnick, 1987). As such, students may not reconcile the stories they have developed about the world with scientific accounts they encounter in school, and simply learn to do science in schools, without recognizing the utility of science outside the classroom. As Bauersfeld (1993) explained: Knowledge (in a narrow sense) will be for nothing once the user cannot identify the adequateness of a situation for use. Knowledge, also, will not be of much help, if the learner is unable to flexibly relate and transform the necessary elements of knowing into his/her actual situation (p.4, as cited by Yackel & Cobb, 1996) It is reasonable to assume the outcomes of teaching and learning science “in a narrow sense” will only exacerbate the difficulties students have with applying science to their lives, and further compromise our ability to educate a citizenry able to understand and deal with problems facing our society. Knowledge will serve no purpose when treated as information to display in exchange for grades and performance. While learning about what we already know in science has its own place in schools, it does not produce students who can reason and communicate in ways that will advance our scientific knowledge. It does not produce students who can engage intelligently in scientific discourse because they have not recognized how to appropriate scientific knowledge and discourse in usable ways. If the ultimate goal of science education is to produce citizens capable of participating in discourse around evidence and explanations, then a first step is to immerse students in classroom discourse likely to achieve this goal. Purpose of the Study The view taken in this study is that scientific literacy means “the capacity to understand and participate in evidence-based discussions” (e.g., Anderson et al., 2006). That is, scientifically literate citizens are ones who can engage in the ongoing dialogue about global issues involving scientific knowledge and discourse, using evidence to develop and evaluate explanations. Likewise, scientifically literate individuals recognize the scientific enterprise is inherently social, and one that values specific ways of thinking and communicating. In most American classrooms, students do not engage in evidence- based discussions, nor do they participate in collective validation of explanations using evidence. While evidence is a critical rhetorical tool for practicing scientists, it has little weight in the science classroom (Shanna & Anderson, 2007). Instead, classroom “discussions” require little involvement on the part of students, and almost no attention to evidence. Classrooms can be characterized by a variety of ways members interact and we know students participate in more or less productive ways, depending on the structure of talk (e.g., Cazden, 2001; Lemke, 1990; Moje, 1995; Wells & Arauz, 2006). The research community has paid a great deal of attention to the negative consequences of recitation, and other monologic forms of instruction. Less is known, however, about how teachers encourage student participation that moves beyond recitation, and how rich discussions of evidence and explanation influence student participation with science content. This dissertation study documents the exception to the rule: two exceptional teachers who engage their students in collective validation of observations and explanations during rich discussions. It is a study of how two teachers help “students come to appropriate and publicly communicate with the forms of talk or text that are part of being a competent hypothesizer, evidence provider, maker of distinctions, checker of facts” (0’ Connors & Michaels, 1996, p.64). The purpose of this work is not only to provide examples of discourse in science classrooms that is less authoritative, but also to show how dialogic discourse provides opportunities for students to participate in thoughtful consideration of evidence and explanations. In this introductory chapter, I will discuss scientific literacy as an educational goal, and the interpretation of this goal as competent participation with science practices guided by socioscientific norms. I will then consider how making sense of discourse is critical to teaching and learning in classrooms, especially in the case of science. Scientific Literacy as an Educational Goal Scientific literacy is not a new term used by science educators; the term has been broadly conceived and often used since Hurd first introduced it in 1958 (DeBoer, 2000). In order to make sense of scientific literacy, I will first consider how the deliberate use of the term literacy has important implications as an educational goal. Defining Literacy What does literacy mean? What are the characteristics of a literate individual? Bybee (1997) stated that, “although the definitions have varied, being literate has consistently referred to mastering the processes needed to interpret culturally significant information” (p. 70). Traditional definitions of literacy, such as that supported by the United Nations Educational, Scientific, and Cultural Organization (UNESCO), define literacy as the ability to understand, interpret, and communicate with printed and written materials. While some literacy researchers may include orality in their view of literacy, many separate the two. For example, Moje (2000) and Norris and Phillips (2003) claimed that because literacy is literally translated to mean mastery of printed text or letters, the term does not usually include orality or other symbols systems (e.g., numeracy, graphical literacy). Moje (2000) argued that conflating the term “literacy” with other acts, such as speaking or listening breaks “the hold that print literacy has in social institutions” (p.655). Yes, print literacy is especially important to literacy, but unlike those who emphasize print literacy, I adopt a view of literacy similar to that of Michael Halliday. Halliday (1993) argued that from a linguistic point of view, treating print literacy as separate from orality does not make sense conceptually. Because speaking and writing are closely connected, and the act of writing involves both inner speech with oneself and talking with others, a view of literacy that does not include orality ignores a powerful relationship between language activities and the practices they support. For this reason I considered literacy to involve the communication of a social language through different literacy acts and genres, including reading, writing, and oral communication (and multiple symbol systems connected to language) that represent a particular way of thinking, valuing, or acting. This definition of literacy is similar to Discourse used by Gee (1996), who stated, “Discourses are distinctive ways people talk, read, write, think, believe, act, and interact with things and other people to get recognized (Gee, 2004a, p.39)(see Appendix D for further discussion). My use of the term literacy is influenced by and compatible with sociocultural approaches to the study of literacy, especially those grounded in New Literacy Studies (e.g., Street, 2001). Furthermore, others have argued that a view of literacy must pay attention to oral communication in order to make sense of the literacy of a community. In a series of papers published in Research in the Teaching of English in 2006, several researchers (i.e., David Bloome, Anne Dyson, James Gee, Martin Nystrand, Vicki Purcell-Gates, and Gordon Wells) responded to a paper by David Olson about the relationship between writing and speech activities. The researchers generally agreed that writing and speech have different forms and are used for different purposes; yet separating the two activities (and therefore separating “literacy” and “orality”) is not a useful distinction. Rather, focusing on the social practices of a community, and the relationship between the genres of discourse (including written and spoken, and other non-verbal texts) to those practices, provides powerful insights to what it means to be literate in a social group or community. James Gee, for example, explained that it is more useful to focus on social practices valued in a community, which include acts of writing and speaking as modes of communication, and concluded that, “the study of literacy, then, becomes the study of practices in which literacy is embedded, not just the study of reading and writing” (p. 155). Scientific Literacy as Participating with Practices Literacy, as understood in terms of participation with spoken and written language embedded in social practices has important implications for the way 1 conceptualized scientific literacy as an educational goal. As explained earlier, I chose to define scientific literacy using a definition by Anderson et al., (2006)—that scientific literacy means being able to participate in evidence-based discussions. In unpacking this definition, scientific literacy involves successfully participating with social practices that use evidence to develop and evaluate explanations. This definition also highlights the necessary role of discourse in participating with these practices. In a recent report to the National Research Council, Duschl, Schweingruber, and Shouse (2007) explained that their framework for scientific literacy “rests on a view of science as both a body of knowledge and an evidence-based, model-building enterprise that continually extends, refines, and revises knowledge” (p. ES- 1). Similar to the way I have conceptualized scientific literacy, they argued that students should “participate productively in science practices and discourse” (p. ES-2), that involve scientific argumentation, explanation, and evaluation of evidence. While I agree with these goals, it is important to consider what participation with practices and discourse potentially looks like in science classrooms. What comes to count in science classrooms is influenced in part by the knowledge and discourse of practicing scientists, and what the broader community values with respect to participation. While I am not making the claim that science classrooms should replicate what happens in the science community, expectations for what students should come to know and do are based on participation in the practitioner community. An important goal in science is to develop scientific models that have the power to explain and predict how things happen in the world. The more explanatory power a model has, the more widely it is accepted and used by the community. The process of developing these models involves thoughtful design and careful collection of data It involves teasing out patterns in data, and ultimately developing model(s) to explain those patterns. The process of making sense of the data, patterns, and models is not straightforward, and typically involves multiple cycles of investigation/experimentation and theory building over long periods of time (although sometimes important discoveries are made serendipitously). Importantly, the process of making sense of data and models requires ongoing dialogue between members of the science community through written and oral presentation and critique of scientific work, thus the importance of being able to participate with discourse that is valued by the community. As members of the science community participate with science practices described above and in collective validation of scientific work, they use standards and criteria that have been accepted as guidelines for evaluating whether something is what it is—whether the data, patterns, and models actually describe and explain what they are intended to explain. These standards serve as a way for individuals in the community to develop their own research programs, as well as ways for individuals and the community to evaluate the work of others. The following are examples of standards and criteria: Data and Observations— standards for evaluating data and observations involve questions about whether or not the data/observations are accurate, precise, and replicable. Pattems- developing patterns involve making sure patterns fit the data, and that they also fit models. For instance, standards used in statistical analysis (e.g., sample sizes, what counts as a large enough effect size, what is significant, what to do with outliers, etc) are criteria for investigating patterns. Other ways to investigate patterns include developing criteria for deciding when things are or are not examples of patterns. Models— models are validated based on the explanatory power of the model (in terms of explaining patterns and data) and whether or not the model is testable, coherent, and parsimonious. When taken together, participating with practices (e.g., observing, experimenting, pattem-finding, modeling, theorizing etc.) guided by accepted standards for validation represent socioscientific norms, or patterns for regular or normative participation in the scientific enterprise. Socioscientific norms serve as guidelines for conducting research (which often involves relationships with the material world) and engaging in careful evaluation of the work of others (which involves predominantly social relationships). To 10 some extent these norms are specialized to various domains within science (e.g., biological sciences might have slightly different practices and standards from other sciences, so the norms for that community may be specific to their domain). Scientists ' Science and School Science Like scientists, students are expected to participate with practices in which they collect and analyze data, develop patterns and models, and then apply those models (i.e., engage in what has been termed scientific inquiry). They are expected to conduct investigations and participate in experimentation (Duschl et al., 2007; NRC, 1996), participate in model building and theorizing (Duschl et al., 2007; Herrenkohl, Palinscar, DeWater, & Kawasaki, 1999; Crawford, Kelly, & Brown, 2000), and develop logical arguments (NRC, 1996; Osborne, Eduran, & Simon, 2004), all of which involve thoughtful activities around observations, patterns, and models, and connections between the three. The practices are both social activities and activities with the material world that are deeply rooted in science, and involve learning how to use and appropriate tools valued in science, such as scientific language and scientific principles. As defined in this study, there are two important features about science practices that should be briefly pointed out. First, practices involve participation with the material world and also the social world. Secondly, participation with practices involves individual activity, as well as collective validation and skeptical review (Duschl et al., 2007; Herrenkohl & Guerra, 1998; NRC, 1996). Thus, standards for what counts as valid become influential for how community members participate. As explained by Anderson (2003), scientific study in schools is about making sense of these experiences, patterns, and explanations (see Figure I). Anderson argued ll that students should participate with practices where they develop accounts that coordinate observations/data with models. Furthermore, they should also learn how to differentiate between observations, patterns, and models in their accounts (C.W. Anderson, personal communication, February, 22, 2008). Thus products of participation with practices are transformed accounts. Take, for example, the account “the glass is getting wet because it is sweating”. This account does not differentiate between observations “getting wet” with an informal model “sweating”. While sweating is not necessarily scientific in the context of this example, it describes what happens to things when they get warm. The glass, for example, may sweat because it is cooler than the air around it—so the warm air causes it to sweat just like humans sweat on warm days. Unfortunately, in this account it is unclear as to what “sweating” really means, and there is no clear way to differentiate between what can be measured and what is inferred. Scientific accounts, on the other hand, do a better job at differentiating between observations, patterns, and models. For instance, take the account “Condensation, such as water droplets collecting on a glass, happens when water vapor is cooled to its dewpoint.” In this account, the observable (water droplets on glass) and measurable (dewpoint) characteristics of the account are differentiated from the pattern (condensation). The transformation of accounts, however, is not an easy task, especially when the culture of schools science (and the authority of scientific discourse) often hinders students’ progress toward this transformation. Challenges to Participating in Scientists ’ Science While students in science classrooms can potentially participate in “scientists’ science” that has been recontextualized for schools (Shanna & Anderson, 2007), there are 12 several important differences between the two communities that prevent students from truly participating in what scientists do. A notable difference is that students are not expected to develop scientific models, or identify new ways in which they are applied (at least in authentic ways), which constitutes a great deal of activity of practicing scientists. Instead students make sense of the explanatory power of models developed by scientists, which are translated into “teachable” and “simplified” content. A second notable difference is that while scientists draw from a broad base of experiential data in their work, students are rarely required, or given opportunities to do the same in the classroom (Sharma & Anderson, 2007). Instead, students typically participate in scripted lab investigations intended to help them see the transparency of models they are testing. Students may not even be required to share experiences they believe are relevant to what they are studying. Shanna and Anderson explained that while the ratio of experience to models is high for scientists, the opposite is true for students— doing science in schools is about learning the scientific models that are the products of centuries of scientific research; it is not about making sense of the vast array of experiences students have in their lives. As such, students spend a great deal of time memorizing definitions, laws, theories, and models without making connections to their experiences. A third difference is that evidence is a necessary tool for scientists to convince and persuade their colleagues that what they found is indeed valid. Sharma and Anderson (2007) said, “empirical evidence comes across as the major and constant reference point in the trajectory of scientific statements from tentative speculations to taken-for-granted ‘truths’ in science labs” (p.6). Evidence is treated substantially different in science 13 classrooms. Evidence is not used as a tool for persuasion, but rather a way to evaluate whether students completed investigation in the “right” way and is used to determine what students did wrong when they did not get the “correct” data or come to the “same” conclusions. Classrooms are also constrained by curricular standards and goals, so that much of what students participate in is scripted at some level (e.g., a scripted lesson, adopted district curriculum and standards, state and national standards). Teachers must preplan a great deal of the experiences students have in the classroom (to varying degrees of flexibility). At best, teachers can organize those experiences in ways that optimize opportunities to participate with science practices that are characteristic of scientists’ science, but rarely can classroom activity reflect the nature of the scientific enterprise as experienced by scientists themselves. Even with the best attempts to support student participation with science practices, the normative ways of participating in school science are not identical to the norms that guide participation by scientists. They are norms that develop as teachers and students interact with science that has been reformulated as content for the classroom. The norms in classrooms are based largely on expectations and goals teachers have for their students. When teachers are successful at supporting student participation with the practices and science standards of scientists science described above, they can establish classroom socioscientific norms as a way to guide participation. In some ways, the classroom socioscientific norms are localized to the classroom community in which they are embedded, but they also reflect the broader culture of science. 14 As described earlier, participation in science occurs through interaction with the material world, and through social interaction. Scientific discourse is a critical medium for participation, especially during social exchanges with fellow class members and the teacher. The following section will consider the role classroom talk and discussion plays in optimizing student participation with each other, and with content. I will explain why I have chosen spoken discourse as the focus of this study, and how it relates to goals for scientific literacy. The Central Role of Discourse in Classrooms Spoken language plays a unique role in the process of learning. Spoken language unites the cognitive and social aspects of learning and provides people with ways of coordinating thought and action. Spoken language is uniquely important in schools since a great deal of classroom instruction and interaction occurs through the medium of talk (Cazden, 2001) and potentially contributes to students’ conceptual understanding and comprehension (Wells & Arauz, 2006). In making the case for focusing on spoken discourse during science discussions, I draw from work in sociocultural tradition, namely that of Lev Vygotsky (1978) and Mikhail Bakhtin (1981, 1986). Mikhail Bakhtin was a contemporary of Lev Vygotsky, and both scholars contributed a great deal to theoretical advances in the fields of psychology, linguistics, philosophy, and education. They both recognized the central role language plays in thinking, as a tool for developing shared knowledge in a community, and for individual sense making. Vygotsky argued that language was the “tool of tools”—it not only served as a tool for mediating social activity, but also as a tool for individuals to internalize and 15 reflect upon social interactions (i.e., inner speech). Vygotsky explained that novice learners acquired habits of minds, practices, and shared knowledge of a cultural group by learning how to use the tools and signs of the group. Vygotsky believed the development of language was paramount to the development of higher mental functioning, so he emphasized speech development in the process of learning. Bakhtin (1981) was also interested in the interplay between thought and language within the context of literature. He introduced the idea of dialogism or dialogic discourse, which described a type of discourse in which speakers and listeners co—constructed new meanings together. He explained that utterances were responsive in the sense that they responded to preceding utterances, and utterances were also anticipatory of future utterances. He also explained that utterances were not only responsive in the immediate context, but rather reflections of historical discourse and how words and utterances had been used before. Dialogism emphasizes a linking of utterances. There is also a sense that dialogic discourse is dynamic—that discourse evolves over time depending on how utterances are linked. Bakhtin also introduced the idea of heteroglossia (i.e., multiple voices), which described the competing voices in a narrative or story, and one’s ability to take on and transform the words of others. Bakhtin explained that heteroglossia brings about conflict and tension created through multiple voices when individuals learn to appropriate discourse. As Nystrand (1997) explained, “It is precisely this tension— this relationship between self and other, this juxtaposition of relative perspectives and struggle among competing voices— that for Bakhtin gives shape to all discourse and hence lies at the heart of understanding as a dynamic, sociocognitive event” (p.8). l6 Vygotsky’s notion of internalization is somewhat controversial if interpreted to be a passive process of adopting with words of others (Cazden, 2001). Although Vygotsky likely did not intend for internalization to be interpreted in such a way, the concept of appropriation from Bakhtin’s work provides a less controversial alternative to describing how novices, in this case students, come to appropriate academic social languages, and also how adults, such as teachers, learn to appropriate the language of children. The important point is that both internalization and appropriation represent processes of adopting and reconciling discourses, and by learning how to use language in specific contexts. Language and the scientific discourse (which includes symbol systems related to social language) are especially central to making progress toward goals of scientific literacy. Engaging in evidence-based discussions is about more than “discussion” defined in a narrow sense. It is about knowing how to interpret the way others appropriate scientific discourse, as well as knowing how to do it yourself. It is also about engaging in an ongoing dialogue around issues that use scientific discourse as the medium for communication—potentially just through listening and interpreting information, but also for using information to engage in informed decision-making. According to sociocultural perspectives, such as those of Vygotsky and Bakhtin, language is a critical tool for making this transformation happen. Science classrooms are the obvious arenas for supporting students with engaging in scientific discourse. It is unfortunate, however, that most classrooms do not encourage student participation with discourse that is aligned with goals of scientific literacy. In fact, there is considerable evidence that most classrooms still rely on recitation as a way 17 of having students participate in class (Cazden, 2001; Nystrand, 1997), which is also true in science classrooms (Lemke, 1990; Wells & Arauz, 2006). Bakhtin argued that all discourse is inherently dialogic, but that authoritative discourse, such as monologic or transmission modes of communication, are used by institutions and governments for control (Lotman, 1988, Wells & Arauz, 2006). In contexts in which there are few experts, or knowers, and many novices, or learners, authoritative discourse prevails. It is the type of discourse used for experts to share what they know with novices, without recognizing novices as sources of knowledge in their own right. Schools implicitly impose authoritative discourse in order to control what is taught to students, such as managing the pace and organization of curricula, and to maintain the authority of the teacher in the classroom. When students are viewed as empty vessels to be filled with knowledge, teachers (as experts) often resort to monologic activities to provide students (as novices) the information they need. As such, a great deal of time in these classrooms is spent in lecture or recitation activities, or in completing textbook or worksheet activities (e. g., worksheets, chapter review questions, and laboratory procedures). In these activities what comes to count as knowledge in the classroom in based on “right” and “wrong” answers. It is not difficult to see how this type of discourse may limit student participation during classroom activities, and ultimately affect their learning of content. While authoritative discourse is a common, and sometimes necessary, way to teach students already known information, it also reduces opportunities for students to be active agents 18 in their own learning. Monologic classroom activities do not produce students who can think and communicate in ways that will advance our scientific knowledge. Dialogic discourse, on the other hand, is dynamic— evolving as the community, and individuals in that community, engage with the content. Bakhtin contrasted authoritative discourse with discourse that is internally persuasive. Internally persuasive discourse is the transformation of authoritative discourse into one’s own voice or words. In communities in which internally persuasive discourse prevails, there is a sense of reciprocity between more knowledgeable and less knowledgeable members of the group, and with this reciprocity is give-and-take between the discourses of experts and novices. In their paper, Sharma and Anderson (2007) described how scientific discourse is both authoritative and internally persuasive. It is authoritative because scientific discourse has evolved as one with “a remarkable degree of standardization in the rhetoric of science communication” (p. 2). When scientific work is written and communicated to others, authors remove as much as possible the human activity involved in the research— that is, authors use a neutral voice to communicate scientific facts, or truths. Yet, even when presented in such an authoritative manner, scientific discourse is open to change, which involves negotiation not only between scientists in the community, but also with nature itself. Sharma and Anderson also explained that in the process of recontextualizing scientific discourse and practice into school curriculum, the internally persuasive elements of the discourse are removed. Unlike scientists who are continually engaged in a dialogical relationship with each other and nature, students engage in the experiences crafted through school curriculum, and are rarely asked to call upon their own experiential base, or to question the authoritative discourse presented to them in schools. 19 While I described these two types of discourse as a dichotomy, Burbules and Bruce (2001) pointed out that a more useful theoretical frame “will need to move beyond the simple dichotomous monologue/dialogue distinction to, at the very least, a spectrum along which various pedagogical communicative relations can be classified from the relatively univocal and directive to the relatively reciprocal and open-ended” (p. 1107). Classroom activities, such as lectures, recitations, dialogue, etc. can be characterized in terms of their place on a continuum between monologic and dialogic forms of communication. Dialogism, as conceived by Bakhtin, is rarely observed in school science. This is unfortunate given that students face numerous challenges when Ieaming to negotiate everyday language and accounts with scientific language and accounts (Lemke, 1990) and those who cannot adopt and appropriate the social language sanctioned by the science community are potentially marginalized (Moje et al., 2004). The authoritative discourse that dominates science classrooms prevents large numbers of individuals from learning to appreciate and value science, and use it in their lives. Researchers have argued that, “any promotion of scientific literacy should empower people to be literate in the discourses of science” (Yore & Treagust, 2006, p. 293) and that researchers and educators must recognize that “students possess multiple Iiteracies that they can bring to bear on various academic tasks” (Hand et al., 2003, p.610). This involves reconciling vernacular and scientific discourses, and learning when and how to appropriate discourse in context. As Gee (2004b) noted, “success in school is primarily contingent on leamers’ willingness and ability to cope with the academic 20 language” (p. 14) and the extent to which students learn to appropriate scientific discourse as opposed to their “lifeworld language”. Other researchers have documented ways in which discourse is critical to understanding science teaching and learning. Elizabeth Moje, for example, has documented multiple discourses students negotiate while in school and how students make compromises between discourses of home, community, and academic disciplines. Moje et al., (2004) focused on how middle and secondary students negotiated the space between everyday and academic discourses (i.e., what is called the “third space”) and stated that, “what is critical to our position is the sense that these spaces can be reconstructed to form a third, different or alternative, space of knowledge and Discourses” (p. 41). The third space represent a new, hybrid discourse that is jointly constructed in the community, with elements of scientific discourse, as well as students’ home discourse. In his notable study, Lemke (1990) provided insights as to how students and teachers appropriate scientific language. His premise was that the learning of science is learning to “talk science” and that “talking science does not simply mean talking about science. It means doing science through the medium of language” (p. ix). He explained that teachers and students often talk past one another because they have not developed shared meaning of words, or a systems in which members of the classrooms jointly construct and use language in the classrooms. Looking carefully at discourse, whether more monologic or dialogic, has important implications for how students participate in science, and ultimately their progress toward scientific literacy. Giving students opportunities to participate with 21 scientific discourse is likely one step toward negotiation of vernacular discourses with the scientific discourse encountered in and outside of schools. Furthermore, when scientific literacy is conceptualized as participating with practices (many of which are social) that are intimately connected to language, then the type of discourse supported in classrooms in critical for making sense of student outcomes. Outcomes for what students learn in terms of participating in evidence-based discussions are hugely different when considering different discourse patterns—that is, a discourse pattern that minimizes student participation and only recognizes one discourse (scientific discourse) will likely lead to markedly different outcomes for students compared to discourse that increases student participation and supports students in negotiating discourses. Research Questions The teacher is critical to supporting students in acquiring the discourse of science. As O’Connor and Michaels (1996) explained, “teachers are continually engaged in a form of language socialization” (p.65). Engaging students in recitation of scientific facts as a replacement for discussion is not an effective solution to the challenges students face in acquiring scientific discourses. True dialogue, on the other hand, potentially provides students with Opportunities to negotiate discourses and Ieam to appropriate scientific discourse in contexts that move beyond recitation in the classroom. Classroom discussions are activities that can potentially bring the dialogical relationships students have with each other and nature to the foreground. Studies on dialogism, particularly during discussions, are potentially promising if researchers can identify what makes discussions dialogic, how teachers and students engage in this form of talk, and the potential outcomes dialogism has for learning content. As Heath (2000) 22 pointed out, researchers interested in discourse in classrooms face the challenge of linking the discourse to learning outcomes afforded to students (i.e., dialogue is not an end in itself). They face the challenge of identifying how social dialogue is not only transformed during social activity, but how it becomes transformed for individual use (Gee & Green, 1998; Gee, Michaels, & O’Connors, 1992). Making sense of how teachers and students continually position themselves not just in relation to each other, but also the content, and how they develop normative ways of interacting in their communities is an important first step toward understanding how discourse facilitates participation in science. While there are theoretical underpinnings of dialogism (i.e., Bahktin, 1981) and observations of dialogic discourse in classrooms, (e.g., Cazden, 2001; Gallas, 1995; Nystrand, 1997), more research on dialogism is needed, especially to make sense of what this type of discourse affords for learning science. The following research question guided the initial data generation and analysis process: 1. What distinguishes dialogic discourse from recitation in science discussions? What are the markers in the discourse that indicate the class is participating in talk that is more or less dialogic? As the study progressed, two additional research questions were considered: 2. What comes to “count” during science discussions—that is, what does dialogic discourse afford in terms of student participation with science practices, and how does the discourse support the establishment of socioscientific norms? 3. In science, there is usually a best explanation or scientific model that has been collectively agreed upon by the science community, so how do science teachers 23 use dialogic discourse, and the tension and conflict embodied in this type of talk, to make progress toward the best explanation? How do students’ accounts transform by participating in the discussions? 24 METHODS Design While theory and research on dialogism is useful in understanding how discourse might be analyzed, and some characteristics of discourse that might be observed in science discussions, it does not provide an a priori framework that is applicable to the present study. Thus, I used methods of open and axial coding adapted from Strauss and Corbin’s (1998) grounded theory methodology, to conduct both microanalysis using line- by-line and episode level coding. I employed the method of constant comparison, in which I systematically reviewed data and compared new data to the emerging theory, revising the theory as more data was analyzed. There are several important points to make concerning my choice in selecting grounded theory methodology. Grounded theory, as conceived originally by Glaser and Strauss (1967) and more recently by Strauss and Corbin (1998) does not assume that researchers begin a study as “blank slates”, but rather, they have theoretical sensitivities that may guide their data generation and analysis. I have described some of my theoretical sensitivities in the introduction to this paper. Whenever possible, researchers using grounded theory should recognize theoretical comparisons “as tools for looking at something somewhat objectively rather than naming or classifying without a thorough examination of the object at the property and dimensional levels” (Strauss & Corbin, 1998, p. 80). Thus, grounded theorists strive foremost to empirically ground the emerging theory in their data, but they also attempt to recognize and connect the theory to current research. Throughout the Findings and Discussion section, I have tried to make the connections to other research whenever possible. 25 Participants 1 recruited participants using a nomination method searching for “exemplary” science teachers who used ample discussions in their classrooms. I considered exemplary teachers to be those with a track record and reputation for innovative teaching and high student engagement during science class, as deemed by school principals, curriculum specialists, and/or science education faculty. I received nominations for six exemplary teachers between grades 4-8. Initially I observed each of these teachers to confirm that they used ample discussion and had high participation from students during this time. I chose four of the six teachers to participate in the study. The four teachers were selected primarily because they were most similar in age range (grades 4-6) and time spent with students (half to full day) compared to the two other teachers who taught grade-8 and only spent an hour with their students each day. Although I continued to observe and videotape the four teachers, I narrowed my analysis down to two teachers. I chose not to analyze data from Ms. Adam’s classroom because she had a student teacher who helped with the lessons, and because she was focusing on small group interactions during the time I videotaped. This minimized the whole group discussions that occurred, which were the focus of this study. I chose not to analyze Ms. Daniels’ classroom because she co- taught with another teacher, making it difficult to tease apart her role in discussions from her team teacher’s role. The two teachers observed and analyzed in this study were nominated by multiple sources for their reputation as exemplary teachers (see Table 1). Both teachers opted to use their real names in this report, as opposed to pseudonyms, however student names have been changed to protect their identity. 26 [Insert Table 1 Here] Ms. Christensen taught in an urban elementary school serving students between kindergarten and 5th grade. The school was located in a residential community of a mid- sized Midwestem city. The school had land surrounding the main building where students played during recess time. Many of the families the school served lived in nearby apartments and homes. The school served roughly 270 students, with two classrooms per grade level. The average teacher-student ratio was 1:16, lower than the state average of 1:17 (www.publicschoolreview.com). Ms. Christensen grade—5 class, however, included 30 students—l7 male students and 13 female students. Ms. Christensen was Caucasian and she estimated the ethnicity of her students was 43% African-American, 23% multi-racial, 10% Caucasian, 10% Hispanic, and 7% Asian, and 7% Indian. She did not know the socioeconomic status of her students, although she described her students as coming from working and middle class families. Public records indicated that during the year I collected data, 55-60% of the students in her school qualified for free or reduced lunches (www.greatschools.net, www.publicschoolreview.com). Furthermore, Ms. Christensen’s school was a Title 1 school, and received additional funding because of the financial situation of students and their families. Ms. Christensen had 17 years of teaching experience at the elementary level and had received both her bachelor’s and master’s degrees at the local university. She currently taught in an urban elementary school. Ms. Christensen was involved in a professional development project at the local university that supported her in standards- based, inquiry oriented professional development. As part of this project she was a 27 member ofa science study group that involved other teachers, curriculum specialists, and university science educators. Ms. Christensen spent the entire school day with her students, except for special activities, such as art, music, and library activities. Ms. Christensen’s school district had adopted science curriculum developed by Biological Science Curriculum Study (BSCS), so much of her classroom activities were based on this curriculum. 1 observed Ms. Christensen during a three-week period toward the end of the school year. We selected this time of year because her student teacher would no longer be leading classroom lessons, therefore my data could capture how Ms. Christensen, as opposed to her student teacher, orchestrated discussions. 1 observed eleven consecutive science lessons. The unit during this time was based on materials developed by BSCS, and focused on water cycling, weather, and phase changes of water. Ms. Christensen used a great deal of whole class discussions, coupled with some writing activities, small group and partner work, and one day of investigations. The science lessons, which were mainly comprised of whole group discussion, typically lasted about one hour and occurred at the end of the school day. Ms. Hocking taught in an elementary school serving only grades 5-6. The school was located in one of the more affluent residential neighborhoods in the suburban city in which it was located. The school served approximately 250 students with five classrooms per grade level. The teacher-student ratio was the state average of 1:17 (www.publicschoo1review.com). Ms. Hocking’s class size was 22 students—13 males and 8 female students. Ms. Hocking was Caucasian and she estimated that her class was 71% Caucasian, 14% African-American, 5% African, 5% Pakistani, and 5% Hispanic 28 (the school average was 56% Caucasian, 19% African-American, 15% Asian, 7% Hispanic, and 3% unspecified). She explained that the socioeconomic status of her students was diverse, from low income to upper middle class families. Public records indicated that 28% of the students in Ms. Hocking’s school district were eligible for free or reduced lunch (www.greatschools.net). Ms. Hocking did not spend the entire school day with her students, but rather slightly more than a half-day with her homeroom class and slightly less time with another teacher’s class. The class used in this study was her homeroom group of students. She taught science and language arts to her students. Ms. Hocking had 17 years of teaching experience with 7 years at elementary and 10 years at the middle school level. She received her bachelor’s degree in music from a midwestern university and then completed a post-baccalaureate program in education. She had taken one year of courses toward her PhD in education. Ms. Hocking was involved in an ongoing teacher professional development project that used problem-based learning as a model for studying dilemmas to strengthen teachers’ understanding of content and practice. As part of this project Ms. Hocking met monthly with a study group consisting of other teachers and local university science educators. The study group reflected on practitioner-oriented books, and on teacher-selected problems they shared with the study group. I observed Ms. Hocking for a 3-month period between mid-February to mid-May of the school year. I observed her classroom approximately once a week, and occasionally visited on consecutive days. Like Ms. Christensen, Ms. Hocking science lessons lasted approximately one hour and occurred at the end of the school day. However, Ms. Hocking used more small group and hands-on activities during that time. I 29 observed her teaching three units—force and motion, light and color, and phase changes of the moon. Eight of my eleven observations occurred during the lengthy light unit, so I focused my analysis on these observations. Researcher In qualitative research, an important concern to consider is the role of the researcher. l was involved with the participants throughout the data generation process, and because I worked closely with teachers, I assumed the role of a participant observer (Spradley, 1980). In this role, I interacted with teachers and students regularly during classroom observations and with teachers before and after observations, during interviews, and through phone calls and electronic mail. Because of my use of videotaping, I was sometimes limited to staying behind the camera monitoring the video and audio recording. Data Sources Addressing issues of reliability and validity in ethnographic research involves triangulation across a variety of data sources, which serve as sources of corroboration for each other. This study used multiple sources, including 1) observations and videotape of classrooms instruction and transcription of entire videotaped lessons, 2) videotaped and transcribed interviews with teachers, and 3) videotaped and transcribed video recall sessions with teachers. Videotaped observation of classroom instruction. I observed each teacher at least 13 times and videotaped at least 10 times during the study using one video camera with multiple audio feeds. The video camera was placed to capture as much of the classroom 30 as possible, and also to be as unobtrusive as possible. During small group work, the camera remained on the teacher as she circulated around the room, and audio was captured from some small groups, as well as a microphone that the teacher carried with her as she worked with groups. Interview and video recalls. Teachers participated in an interview and video recall session at the end of the study that lasted approximately two hours. The interviews were semi-structured, ethnography interviews (Spradley, 1979), in which questions were developed around the teacher’s purpose for discussions (and written) activities, including the teacher’s focus on specific science practices during those activities. In addition, I selected 8-10 video clips for video recall, with each clip lasting between 30 seconds to 3- 4 minutes. For each video clip, I developed 2-4 questions to guide discussion about the clip and teachers were asked to talk at length about what they remembered of the activity shown in the clip. I selected video clips to address a variety of questions that arose during the observations. Questions were intended to obtain the teacher’s perspective about the activity in the clip and to clarify questions I had developed over the course of the study (see Appendices B and C for interview and video recall questions). Data Analysis Transcription and “chunking”. Videotapes and interviews were transcribed completely and then inserted into excel. Using excel, I first reviewed all of the observations and divided them in terms of episodes. Episodes were identified by major shifts in topic, which sometimes coincided with shifts in classroom activities. Episodes were further divided in terms of “segments” which was my way of breaking down the topics or activities in a related episode. 31 Microanalysis. In coding the first transcript I used open coding, conducting a line- by-line analysis of each utterance in the transcript. After this point I identified three broad categories in which to code: dialogic indicators, social norm indicators, and science norm and practice indicators. Although the subcategories of each were generated using open coding, my identification of three categories was a first step toward axial coding. Each utterance in a transcript received up to three codes corresponding to the three broad categories: 0 Dialogic indicators- 1 developed categories to capture the characteristics of discourse that signaled it was dialogic. I identified indicators at the level ofa discourse move, but considered subsequent exchanges to see how the move, or utterance, was realized in a sequence of talk. For example, open-ended teacher questions (or authentic teacher questions) were dialogic indicators coded at the level of an utterance, however, I considered the responses to the question to verify whether it was truly open-ended. 0 Discussion norm indicators- 1 developed categories for norms only during instances of discussions. The emerging categories did not include norms that guided other classroom activities, such as seatwork, recitation, etc. The categories and subcategories reflected patterns in the way the group made use of the contributions, or accounts, shared during discussion. For example, assigning authorship to accounts became a pattern in the way the class paid attention to how they linked and positioned accounts and how accounts evolved through the course of discussion. 32 ' Socioscientific norm and practice indicators- Because the emergence of norms depended on the science practices engaged in at the time, I deveIOped categories for science practices and socioscientific norms simultaneously. For example, when students participated with the practice of making observations, the teacher helped to establish norms for what counted as a precise or replicable observation in the classroom (practices and norms will be discussed in more detail in the results and discussion section and in Appendix D). Example of Microanalysis. In order to demonstrate how the microanalysis occurred, 1 selected a brief transcript, in which I illustrate my coding of dialogic indicators, social norms, and socioscientific norms and practices. Note that this transcript will be discussed in more detail in the results section, and that I do not intend to explain the indicators fully in this section. Rather my goal is to demonstrate how I conducted line-by-line analysis on the texts. Since I developed dialogic indicators using grounded theory, the following example does not represent how the dialogic indicator codes developed or evolved over the course of the study. Rather, the example transcript below is coded using dialogic indicators represented by the final set of categories and subcategories from the analysis. The key below shows how I identified interesting utterances and coded those utterances in terms of dialogic indicators. Note that the key is only useful for the transcript below and not intended as a key for the other transcripts throughout the results and discussion. Key for Dialogic Indicators 33 Dialogic Indicators Font Student Questions (taken up) Bold New Student Ideas (taken up) Italicized Uptake that Aligns/Opposes Underlined Authentic teacher questions Typewriter font Value and Respect Chalkboard Font Ice in Antarctica DEVON: How come there’s water in Antarctica that’s not frozen? JACKSON: Question book. MS. CHRISTENSEN: Yeah, that’s a good one. That’s one I bet you could get on the Internet. Tell us where that water would be that’s not frozen. Is it out on the surface? Where is it? Somebody who knows something about Antarctica, where is that water that’s not frozen? Thomas? Manuel? Where is that water that’s not frozen? MANUEL: In the ocean or under some other ice. MS. CHRISTENSEN: Go put that in the question book. Good question. What do you think Clarice? CLARICE: This is the sun, and this is the water- and - this is all melting down here. And then the sun is shining on this part (points to drawing). And it’s all the way over here. But it’s not — it’s still shining on this, but it’s not shining as much as it in on this part. MS. CHRISTENSEN: What do other people think about that? What do other people think about that? Thomas, What do you think? THOMAS: I think that the water is not frozen under because —— well, it is probably not solid, but if you go farther down, it’s still very cold — very freezing, but it’s not all the way frozen. CLARICE: Maybe because — 34 MS. CHRISTENSEN: Do you have an idea of Why that would be? (to Thomas) CLARICE: I have an idea. Maybe because when it’s still shining on ice and everything, all the small whatever comes to the top, but it can ’t go under because it’s not— (doesn’t finish) MS.CHRISTENSEN: You know, this is an interesting idea. Keep your hands up. This is an interesting idea, because I think it’s the same thing that you talked about when you talked about the mountains. That there’s an uneven heating of the earth — of the surface of the earth. So that ’s a very interesting thought, isn ’t it? What do you think, J a an? JAVAN: I don’t know how to explain it. Well, kind of- I don’t know. It’s not Me I do disagree with Clarice- but I also agree with Thomas. But I can’t explain it. Can I come and show you up on the board? I also coded the same transcript in terms of discussion norms that became apparent during discussions marked by dialogic indicators. It is important to point out the discussion norms and dialogic indicators were closely related, so during the axial coding process these two types of indicators were mapped on to each other. Further discussion of both will be combined in the first part of the results section. The key below shows how I coded discussion norms at the level of utterances, and uses the final categories that emerged from the grounded theory process. Key for Discussion Norm Indicators Discussion Norm Indicators Font Elicited multiple accounts Bold Valued student accounts Italicized Positioned Accounts Underlined 35 Ice in Antarctica DEVON: How come there ’s water in Antarctica that ’s not frozen? JACKSON: Question book. MS. CHRISTENSEN: Yeah, that’s a good one. That’s one I bet you could get on the Internet. Tell us where that water would be that ’s not frozen. Is it out on the surface? Where is it? Somebody who knows something about Antarctica, where is that water that ’s not fiozen? Thomas? Manuel? Where is that water that’s not frozen? MANUEL: In the ocean or under some other ice. MS. CHRISTENSEN: Go put that in the question book. Good question. What do you think Clarice? CLARICE: This is the sun, and this is the water- and - this is all melting down here. And then the sun is shining on this part (points to drawing). And it’s all the way over here. But it’s not — it’s still shining on this, but it’s not shining as much as it in on this part. MS. CHRISTENSEN: What do other people think about that? What do other people think about that? Thomas, what do you think? THOMAS: I think that the water is not frozen under because — well, it is probably not solid, but if you go farther down, it’s still very cold - very freezing, but it’s not all the way frozen. CLARICE: Maybe because — MS. CHRISTENSEN: Do you have an idea of why that would be? (to Thomas) CLARICE: I have an idea. Maybe because when it’s still shining on ice and everything, all the small whatever comes to the top, but it can ’t go under because it’s not- (doesn’t finish) MS.CHRISTENSEN: You know, this is an interesting idea. Keep your hands up. This is an interesting idea, because I think it ’s the same thing that you talked about when you talked about the mountains. That there ’3 an uneven heating of the earth — of the surface of the earth. So that’s a very interesting thought, isn ’t it? What do you think, Javan? 36 JAVAN: I don’t know how to explain it. Well, kind of- I don’t know. It’s not like I do disagree with Clarice- but I also agree with Thomas. But I can’t explain it. Can I come and show you up on the board? The transcript was also coded in terms of science practices and socioscientific norms. In this example the class was participating with the practice of connecting their observations and experiences with patterns and models. The class was trying to establish what was necessary for things to be frozen (cold temperature), or what keeps things from being frozen (sun), which gave rise to a socioscientific norm about what counts as appropriate ways to connect observations, patterns, and models. The bold font represents utterances that were particularly important for making sense of the socioscientific norms and practices in the discussion. Ice in Antarctica DEVON: How come there’s water in Antarctica that’s not frozen? JACKSON: Question book. MS. CHRISTENSEN: Yeah, that’s a good one. That’s one I bet you could get on the Internet. Tell us where that water would be that’s not frozen. Is it out on the surface? Where is it? Somebody who knows something about Antarctica, where is that water that’s not frozen? Thomas? Manuel? Where is that water that’s not frozen? MANUEL: In the ocean or under some other ice. MS. CHRISTENSEN: Go put that in the question book. Good question. What do you think Clarice? CLARICE: This is the sun, and this is the water- and — this is all melting down here. And then the sun is shining on this part (points to drawing). And it’s all the way over here. But it’s not — it’s still shining on this, but it’s not shining as much as it in on this part. MS. CHRISTENSEN: What do other pe0ple think about that? What do other people think about that? Thomas, what do you think? 37 THOMAS: I think that the water is not frozen under because — well, it is probably not solid, but if you go farther down, it’s still very cold - very freezing, but it’s not all the way frozen. CLARICE: Maybe because — MS. CHRISTENSEN: Do you have an idea of why that would be? (to Thomas) CLARICE: l have an idea. Maybe because when it’s still shining on ice and everything, all the small whatever comes to the top, but it can’t go under because it’s not- (doesn’t finish) MS.CHRISTENSEN: You know, this is an interesting idea. Keep your hands up. This is an interesting idea, because I think it’s the same thing that you talked about when you talked about the mountains. That there’s an uneven heating of the earth - of the surface of the earth. So that’s a very interesting thought, isn’t it? What do you think, Javan? JAVAN: I don’t know how to explain it. Well, kind of- I don’t know. It’s not like I do disagree with Clarice- but I also agree with Thomas. But I can’t explain it. Can I come and show you up on the board? “Big Picture” analysis. After I coded approximately 25 % of my data, I recognized that line-by-line analysis was not capturing the “big picture”, so I started writing episode summaries. The episode summaries were pieced together to form a timeline for each teacher. They included narrative summaries of each of the three original categories described above. They also included an additional feature of the talk—the accounts that were shared and taken up by the group. In order to make sense of “the what " being discussed, 1 used accounts of science objects—that is, how students, the teacher (or textbook) pieced together observations, patterns, and models. I considered accounts to be mainly about observations (e.g., the glass gets wet) or explanations (e.g., the glass sweats or water condenses on the glass) or combinations of the two (e.g., the glass is getting wet because it is sweating). I used the term emerging accounts to describe 38 the narratives and stories that students have about their experiences that do not use scientific knowledge and discourse. I use the term scientific accounts to refer to those that adhere to scientific standards for the way observations, patterns, and models are used to explain and predict. The term transitional accounts was used to describe those that reflect bits and pieces or emerging and scientific accounts. Theoretical saturation. For each teacher I reached theoretical saturation for dialogic indicators and discussion norm indicators that occurred during classroom discussions, meaning there were no new major insights emerging from the data. At this point I made minor adjustments to the organization of the emerging categories and subcategories for these indicators. The goal of this study was not to reach theoretical saturation in terms of the categories of science practices, socioscientific norms, and accounts, namely because I focused mostly on whole group discussions as opposed to looking at practices across different community activities. Instead the goal was to make sense of dialogic discussions, and then identify the relationship between discourse and student participation with practices guided by norms. In Part 2 of the results section, I selected examples of socioscientific norms to discuss these relationships, but the selected examples do not reflect saturation of science practices and norms in these two classrooms. Criteria for Evaluation and Validation Reliability and validity. Reliability and validity were achieved through the use of five techniques: I) triangulation of multiple sources of data, 2) constant comparison method and negative case analysis, 3) member checking, including teacher review of the emerging results (Merriam, 1998), 4) inter-rater reliability, in which an independent 39 researcher coded 10% of the data for dialogic and discussion norm indicators using the emerging coding book in Appendix A, and 5) comparison of emerging findings to the existing research literature. In using Strauss and Corbin’s (1998) method of constant comparison, my analysis involved multiple cycles of generation and revision to emerging categories (i.e., open and axial coding). As new categories emerged, they were continually compared to existing categories and existing data. When the emerging findings for each teacher became more complete, I focused on documenting examples that contradicted the emerging theory. That is, I conducted a thorough negative case analysis. The results were shared with both teachers describing the findings from their classrooms. The teachers were encouraged to review the results and given opportunities to elaborate if they felt the results were inadequate or misleading. Both teachers responded positively to the emerging findings, and also provided additional information about their use of classroom discussion. During the results and discussion, suggestions that occurred during the member checking process will be considered. An independent researcher not familiar with the literature on dialogism or familiar with research on science teaching and learning coded 10% of the data (e.g., roughly 1 observation per teacher) using the coding book in Appendix A. The independent researcher coded only dialogic indicators and discussion norms, as the analysis of science practices and socioscientific norms involved more than the 10% of data being used for reliability checking. The initial reliability was 82% agreement. Most of the disagreement occurred around distinguishing between authentic teacher questions and questions that attempted to elicit evaluation from students. The researchers discussed the differences 40 and minor revisions were made until both agreed 100% that the dialogic and discussion norm indicators did in fact capture what occurred during discussions. In the results sections, I have tried to make connections to the existing research base whenever possible as a way of building a theory about dialogism in science discussions that is not only grounded in these two classrooms, but also recognizes relevant studies in the existing literature. Therefore, the following sections represent both findings and discussion of those findings. 41 FINDINGS AND DISCUSSION Introduction to Findings The goal of this study was to develop a theory of how dialogic discourse supports student participation in science. What emerged from the data were complex relationships involving discourse, norms, practices, and accounts. Because of the complexity of the results, I present my findings in two sections. In the first section 1 illustrate the emerging theory about dialogic discourse during discussions, and the central role discourse plays in creating space for student participation. 1 ground my claims about dialogic discourse with classroom examples, and consider how my claims relate to current research in science education. As I clarify the emerging theory, I make the case that attention to discourse is critical if one wants to analyze what is afforded to classroom communities when this type of talk is supported. In the second section I explain how dialogic discourse created space for students to participate with science practices, and how the teachers used discussions as a way of establishing socioscientific norms. I will illustrate the emergence of practices and norms using briefcase studies focusing on validation of observations and validation of explanations using evidence. I also consider meta-level discussions about the use of scientific and informal models and the need for consensus. I explain how the teachers encouraged their students to move toward more sophisticated scientific accounts in terms of discourse and practice, and how socioscientific norms influenced the process of transforming those accounts. 42 PART I: DIALOGIC INDICATORS AND NORMS FOR DISCUSSION Dialogic Indicators were markers for instances where discourse exhibited features of true dialogue. In developing these indicators 1 used line-by-line analysis, but also considered how dialogue played out over a sequence of talk. Dialogic indicators were grouped by similarities in how they influenced the direction and type of talk supported. With dialogic indicators grouped in such a way, the norms that helped sustain participation during discussions were mapped onto the indicators (as shown in Table 2). [Insert Table 2 here] The discussion norms were patterns in the way the group made use of the contributions shared during discussion, which represented the standards and expectations for regular participation. In Table 2, Diversity in Accounts, Linking Accounts, and Meta- Talk were broad categories that linked similar norms in terms of their purposes during discussions. Diversity of A ccounts During discussions, there was often a diverse set of accounts under consideration by the group. Eliciting multiple accounts from students not only provided more opportunities for more students to participate during discussions (and to share their naive accounts), but it also communicated value for what students had to say. Multiple Accounts were Elicited. Gaining access to the floor during discussions can be challenging for students in classrooms using monologic discourse activities, especially given the limited opportunity and time students participate in talking during these activities. In recitation, students gain 43 the floor when they already know the right answer (i.e., teacher calling on students with hands raised). Access might also be granted to students not paying attention (i.e., by calling on students Off-task), so even though these students are given Opportunities to participate, they are not prepared to do so. Inevitably, recitation creates a place for “smart kids” to display what they know for the teacher and further exacerbates the disparities in student participation. Students that do not know the right answer are spectators during recitation, and may be further disadvantaged because they have limited involvement in what is occurring in the classroom. It is important to point out, however, that participation during discussion is not only about those who are doing the talking, but rather involves both actively speaking or listening during the discussion (Cazden, 2001 ). Sometimes “quiet” students may be actively engaged in listening to what is talked about. There are also times when the speaker’s participation is not fully realized if the speaker cannot gain the attention of others in the discussion. In the two classrooms in this study, discussions involved the consideration Of more than one account. At times the teachers elicited ten or more contributions from students. For example, during Ms. Christensen’s “science talks”, each student had an opportunity to share their account with the group, and also an Opportunity to question or comment on another student’s account. Likewise, in Ms. Hocking’s classroom students used “talking chips” early in the year as a way to distribute the speaking responsibilities throughout the classroom. In this way, gaining and holding the floor was made easier for students who did not already know the “right” answer, or who were hesitant to speak out during discussions. The teachers did not want “quiet” students or “confused” students to be left out Of the conversation. 44 Authentic teacher questions and open invitations were not the only ways Ms. Christensen and Ms. Hocking elicited multiple accounts from students, or the only way students gained access to the floor. Rather these dialogic indicators (or markers in the discourse), represented important ways the teacher communicated to students that the floor was Open to anyone wanting to share their ideas, even if the ideas were speculative, and that the teacher expected more than one student to contribute. Both discourse moves were considered dialogic indicators because they elicited talk from students that was not pre-scripted, and supported the emergence of multiple voices. Authentic teacher questions, as previously discussed by Nystrand (1997) and Gallas (1995) are those that do not have “right” or “wrong” answers. They are questions that elicit a variety Of possible responses. For example, when the teacher asked the question, “Does anybody have anything else to add?” she communicated that the floor was still Open, especially to those students who wanted to “add” something new to the discussion. As Ms. Hocking explained in her interview, “[The] nature of the questions that I ask is how, without giving away, without short-circuiting their thinking process, because I don’t want to short-circuit it, I want to encourage it and motivate it, and if I ask close-ended questions, then that cuts Off the discussion in their heads and in the groups.” Ms. Hocking was clearly conscious of her use Of Open and close-ended questions, and how these questions influenced the participation Of her students. Maintaining an Open invitation was equally important because it served as an explicit reminder to students that the teacher expected high participation from everyone during discussion. Explicit invitations to participate also helped to distribute the speaking responsibilities (and access to the floor) to a larger number Of students, as Opposed to 45 engaging only those who always had something to say (Hadjioannou, 2007). Importantly, as Ms. Hocking clarified in her member check, the Open invitations was not necessarily and “anything goes” session—that is, students’ contribution needed to be focused, at least to some extent, on the topic being discussed. The following excerpt from a classroom discussion reveals several dialogic indicators. The underlined text represents moves (and overall patterns) in the way Ms. Christensen encouraged students to contribute diverse accounts to the conversation using authentic questions and Open invitations: Water on the Plastic MS. CHRISTENSEN: Is there anything else that you want to say? Anything different that you have to say than what’s already been said? Melody, you need tO pay attention. You need to pay attention to the discussion. Anything different? (Pause) Okay. How about investigation two? And that was the one with the dishes of water? And we had some over in the sun, some under the sink. _W_hat happened? Allen. what happened? ALLEN: Well one- the plastic - without plastic over there [points to window]- It was completely dry. And then the one that was in there [points under sink] had a little water in it. And the one with the plastic over there [in window] had water on the top Of the plastic. And the water in that one [under sink with plastic] did too. MS. CHRISTENSEN: Okay. Does anyone h_a_ve_ anything to add to that? Anything different than what Allen said? Julia? JULIA: Like when he said that the one with the plastic on it, the water was on top of the plastic- it was there because it tried to evaporate, but it just basically was stopped inside. MS.CHRISTENSEN: What do you think? Did you hear what she said, Alice? ALICE: NO. MS.CHRISTENSEN: Okay. Nice and loud. 46 JULIA: I said that the one that had the — the ones that had the plastic on it- That the water was on the plastic is the water on the plastic because it tried to evaporate, but plastic was in the way. As Ms. Christensen’s class discussed Observations Of their investigations, she encouraged them to contribute diverse ideas, not only by asking open-ended questions (“What happened”), but also by explicitly calling for new or different ideas from students, with Allen’s and Julia’s accounts representing substantially different types of responses to the question. Allen’s account was about Observations he made during the investigation, while Julia’s account was an attempt to explain those Observations using the idea Of evaporation. Student Accounts were Valued. Ms. Christensen and Ms. Hocking communicated (explicitly and implicitly) that they valued what students had to say, especially when students’ ideas were different from other accounts on the floor. Valuing students’ accounts not only supports student engagement in discussions (Mohan, Lundeberg, & Pressley, in revision), but it also allows teachers to access students’ prior knowledge, or what Gallas (1995) referred to as accessing the “windows into children’s thinking”. In their interviews, both teachers acknowledge the importance of knowing about what students brought to the class. For example, Ms. Christensen explained in her interview that “student wisdom” was an important consideration for her orchestration Of discussions: Kids don’t believe what you say most Of the time, but believe what they believe; what they know, and what they know may or may not be what they teacher is saying. If they’re not going to believe what I say and they really have much more authority with each other, so I guess [I’m] kind Of helping them- helping the conversation to go in the way that I want it to go, but coming from them because again, they have a lot Of wisdom that they just need to put out on the table. 47 Science teachers face the challenge of honoring what students bring to their class, while simultaneously steering students toward more sophisticated scientific accounts, which oftentimes means the one best explanation or “right” answer. They also face the challenge of not only figuring out the naive conceptions (or misconceptions) students bring to the classroom, but also how to incorporate those conceptions into the discussion alongside scientific accounts, and eventually when and how the class will reconcile the two. This involves reconciling the teachers’ intentions and expectations with the life experiences Of children in the classroom (Gallas, 1995). Misconceptions can be problematic for teachers, but as Ms. Christensen alluded to, students strongly believe their emerging accounts and it is difficult to convince them Otherwise, especially if they do not recognize how substantially different those accounts are from scientific accounts (e. g., evaporated water goes into clouds versus evaporated water condenses to form clouds). At times, the scientific explanations needed to convince students are beyond the scope Of the course (e.g., scientific models that explain patterns such as condensation, evaporation, refraction may not be appropriate for upper elementary students). Ms. Christensen explained in her interview that balance needed tO occur between honoring students’ experiences and giving them a reason to move toward more scientific accounts: Kids learn by their experiences; by what they see in the world. There’s a lOt of input, and for me to just be one piece Of input, if I were to give them just information, I guess I think kids don’t have that kind of faith in what one person says, or what an adult says. They have to have that conversation. It has to have all those layers [writing, reading, talking with multiple voices at play], and it has to be complex. And it has to have places where it challenges what they’re saying- what they’re thinking- so that they can either say, ‘well I don’t think so’ or, ‘I do think so’. 48 Ms. Christensen believed scientific accounts (i.e., accounts from her or the textbook) represented one voice in the conversation, with emerging accounts from students representing the larger majority Of what was on the table. She thought, however, that at some point the emerging accounts had to be questioned—there had to be “places where it challenges what they’re saying.” Ms. Christensen clarified her position during the interview using a specific instance when Kamal came up to her after class to ask what she thought about the question they were discussing: I happen to think it’s the sun that drives it [water cycle]; the heat energy. And once the heat energy enters in, that puts it all in motion. But Kama] did ask me what my ideas were. I may tell them at some point what I think. I wondered why he wanted to know what 1 thought—if it’s the right answer because I don’t want to give him the right answer. But I’m willing to give him my Opinion, as just one Of the Opinions on the table. For Ms. Christensen, the introduction Of scientific accounts from her or through the textbook was not about giving students the “right” answer, but rather about when and how scientific accounts were introduced to students. The challenge for her as a science teacher was to figure out when scientific accounts should be treated “as just one Of the Opinions on the table” and when they should be used to directly challenge emerging accounts. Both teachers used students’ emerging accounts (and vernacular language) as starting points in discussions. Ms. Hocking explained in her interview that students’ ideas, even those that were “wrong” or naive, had their place in discussions: I think that they need to hear people making sense of what they’re seeing. They need to hear explanations, whether they’re right or wrong, initially because that sort Of discussion—entertaining back and forth, entertaining ideas and suggestions, and ideas and Opinions—I think that’s an important part of it—the give and take. But I also think that during the discussion if we’re aiming toward something, for instance, the transparent, translucent, opaque activity, I’m going to be sort Of corralling them toward these final working definitions, so out Of the 49 discussion I’m hoping will arise an [scientific] explanation for the phenomena that they were exploring. Besides explicitly communicating value for student accounts, the teachers showed value for what students had to say by taking up their questions and allowing their new ideas to shift discussions. Similar to what Nystrand and his colleagues found, student questions led to what they referred to as “dialogic spells”, or extended episodes of dialogic discussion (Nystrand, Wu, Gamoran, Zeiser, & Long, 2003). For example, consider the following discussion from Ms. Christensen’s class: Ice in Antarctica DEVON: How come there’s water in Antarctica that’s not frozen? JACKSON: Question book. MS. CHRISTENSEN: Yeah, that’s a good one. That’s one I bet you could get on the Internet. Tell us where that water would be that’s not frozen. Is it out on the surface? Where is it? Somebody who knows something about Antarctica, where is that water that’s not frozen? Thomas? Manuel? Where is that water that’s not frozen? MANUEL: In the ocean or under some other ice. MS. CHRISTENSEN: GO put that in the question bOOk. Good question. What do you think Clarice? CLARICE: This is the sun, and this is the water- and — this is all melting down here. And then the sun is shining on this part (points to drawing). And it’s all the way over here. But it’s not — it’s still shining on this, but it’s not shining as much as it in on this part. MS. CHRISTENSEN: What do other people think about that? What dO other people think about that? Thomas, what do you think? THOMAS: I think that the water is not frozen under because — well, it is probably not solid, but if you go farther down, it’s still very cold — very freezing, but it’s not all the way frozen. 50 CLARICE: Maybe because — MS. CHRISTENSEN: Do you have an idea Of why that would be? (to Thomas) CLARICE: I have an idea. Maybe because when it’s still shining on ice and everything, all the small whatever comes to the top, but it can’t go under because it’s not- (doesn’t finish) MS.CHRISTENSEN: You know, this is an interesting idea. Keep your hands up. This is an interesting idea, because I think it’s the same thing that you talked about when you talked about the mountains. That there’s an uneven heating of the earth - of the surface of the earth. So that’s a very interesting thought, isn’t it? What do you think, Javan? JAVAN: I don’t know how to explain it. Well, kind Of- I don’t know. It’s not like I do disagree with Clarice- but I also agree with Thomas. But I can’t explain it. Can I come and show you up on the board? Embedded in this transcript are numerous dialogic indicators, making it clear the discussion following Devon’s question was similar to dialogic spells observed by Nystrand et al., (2003). In fact, the discussion illustrates the complexity Of dialogue around eliciting diverse accounts. It began with a student question (i.e., How comes there’s water in Antarctica that’s not frozen), which was taken up by the teacher. By taking up Devon’s question, Ms. Christensen communicated to everyone that she took student questions seriously. She then asked an authentic and open-ended question to the class (i.e., Where is the water that’s not frozen?) that modified the original question by Devon. Ms. Christensen also indicated she valued students’ ideas when she asked, “Somebody who knows something about Antarctica, where is that water that’s not frozen?” By asking this question, Ms. Christensen recognized students had something to say in response to both Devon’s and her questions. Both questions led to diverse responses from Clarice, Manuel, Thomas, and Javan. In her interview Ms. Christensen explained that student questions were uniquely important in her classroom: 51 I think when they’re asking questions and looking for trying to make sense of all this stuff, that’s when they’re really, really learning. SO the whole point Of them asking questions is really important to me. Now how do I get them to do that? I think I give them lots of opportunities to do that and then I value when they do ask questions. I value their questions. One of the ways that I show I actually value it and that encourages kids to ask questions is [I] don’t let kids laugh at other kids’ questions, and I expect that everyone is going to be listening. I expect them to ask those questions loudly enough so that other people can hear. I mean I think if there are kids who talk in soft voices and other kids can’t hear them and I allow that to happen what I’m saying is your question is not important enough for everybody to hear. So I want to make sure that everybody hears the question; that it’s valued in the community- in the classroom- and that then we try to answer them. I think that tells them it’s important to ask questions; we’re not going to just let it go by. An important emerging finding in this study is that not only did students’ questions have weight in shifting the course of discussion, but new student ideas were another way for students to stimulate dialogic discourse. At times, students recognized connections or contributed something new to the discussion that led to rich dialogue around their contribution. Ms. Hocking explained in the interview that new student ideas were often sources of creativity and curiosity she capitalized on in her classroom: I think its important for them to know that if you see the idea and you go off on a tangent you’re going to discover the light bulb (laughs), or whatever else. SO, I think that in part I want them to be creative and I want them to be curious. I think that if you constantly restrained them, you know, have constraints placed on them where they have to follow a script where they have to do it the same as everybody else in the room—you’re going to nip something in the bud. For example, in Ms. Hocking’s class students were making Observations of a roller coaster apparatus (though not explicitly labeled “roller coaster”) and trying to explain what was happening. In the example below, Ellen contributed a comparison between roller coasters and the apparatus, which Andrew questioned, leading eventually to rich discussion around forces (i.e., gravity) and roller coasters. 52 Old-fashioned Roller Coasters MS. HOCKING: What’s happening here? BEN: The ball- the marble rolls down the slope and as it does it gains speed, and eventually it has enough speed to overcome gravity, so when it goes in this swirl it comes back, then up. The energy left goes up here and it stOps. ANDREW: Hopefully stops. MS. HOCKING: Okay, could somebody else add to what Ben said? ELLEN: I think when it goes down like a roller coaster, if you don’t have a steep enough hill to go down, then you don’t have enough time to even go over like the ball- ANDREW: Why a roller coaster? ELLEN: I mean like if it’s like an Old-fashioned one, something where you get pulled up like a wire and go like that- MS. HOCKING: Yeah, that’s an interesting question because roller coasters are built differently now than they used to be. The old-fashioned wooden roller coaster, what was the force that powered it? Ms. Hocking took up Ellen’s new idea about roller coasters (and Andrew’s question for Ellen) to move the group toward talking about gravity, and how gravity related to what they Observed on the apparatus. Ellen’s account was not necessarily a question asked of the group, but rather a way for her to explain what she observed using her prior knowledge about roller coasters. Andrew, however, was confused about the connection being made until Ellen clarified that she meant “Old-fashioned” roller coasters. The clarification served as a way for Ms. Hocking to bring in the idea of forces, and gravity more specifically. 53 Discussions in these classrooms depended a great deal on the diversity in accounts being shared, both as a way of accessing what students’ already knew, and as a way to stimulate rich discussion around the differences in those accounts (which will be discussed further in the following section). Importantly, however, was that the discourse observed in the examples above can be likened to what Burbules (1993) labeled as “conversation” because the discourse was both divergent and inclusive. Indeed, much of the discourse and accounts that held the floor were from students. It was not the case, however, that the class never came to agreement or attempted to converge toward one best explanation. At times the teachers had to be satisfied with not receiving closure at the end of a lesson (Cazden, 2001). Despite allowing for greater diversity in accounts, Ms. Hocking and Ms. Christensen also felt strongly that scientific accounts needed to be used to challenge emerging accounts, and their responsibility was to help students develop more scientifically sophisticated accounts. In the following section I will discuss how the teachers accomplished this task during discussions. Linking Accounts An important characteristic Of dialogic discourse is that utterances in dialogue are linked and what emerges from the dialogue is joint construction of meaning (Bakhtin, 1981). While dialogue may include diverse accounts from different members participating in the conversation, the goal of dialogue is to work toward a shared understanding, and eventually one that is transformed for personal use. As this process of co-construction occurs, there needs to be a way for utterances to be linked and positioned. In these two classroom, the process of linking utterances (or accounts) was observed 54 through group members referencing and giving authorship to accounts, and by positioning their own accounts in relation to others on the floor. Accounts were Authored. The teacher or students assigned authorship of accounts by referencing the original contributor, often by name. For example, in the following transcript Ms. Hocking elicited multiple accounts from students, and as students shared their accounts, she revoiced what was said for the group, naming or referencing the author: Penny and Jar Filled with Water MS. HOCKING: Okay. So if light’s bouncing Off the penny all the time, how come you can’t see it? That’s the big question. He {Andrew} says because you’re looking through water. Sanjit, do you want to add to that? SANJIT: I think because if the water takes the form of the Object, so maybe it goes through the water and goes in different ways. MS. HOCKING: Okay. Goes through the water, goes through different ways. Tyler, you want to add something to what Sanjit said. TYLER: I think it’s transparent, so it’s, like, a convex near, so when it - the light goes through and hits the penny, as it comes out it gets all spread out. MS. HOCKING: Okay, he’s [Tyler] comparing it to the convex mirror. Convex or concave, which one? TYLER: Convex. MS. HOCKING: Convex, okay, so curved with the outer curve. Okay, so he says he thinks it’s got something to do with the shape of the jar. The shape of the jar somehow does something to the way the light behaves. Well, light can only travel in a straight path, right? But we know with mirrors that it can change direction, right? 55 What was interesting about this transcript was that Ms. Hocking not only referenced the author of the account, but she also allowed for Tyler to verify that she had correctly understood his explanation. By linking authors with their accounts, it allowed for members to track the way accounts changed through the course of discussion. Ms. Christensen described this as a process in which students have to commit (to some degree) to the accounts they share with the group—students needed to formulated an account they felt was reasonable and made sense in the context of what was being discussed. When sharing accounts, “authors” were expected to defend their accounts if necessary. As the teacher, Ms. Christensen maintained a record in which she tracked student accounts during discussion, and at times she pointed out to students when they changed their accounts to have students explain why they changed their account. For example, in the following transcript Ms. Christensen pointed out to the class that Maria and Alisa had changed their accounts since the previous lesson: What Drives the Water Cycle- Part 1 MS.CHRISTENSEN: Okay, now I want to go back to our original question. I want to refocus our discussion and hold on to the ideas. I wrote them down so we wouldn’t lose those ideas. But I want to refocus our discussion on what is it that makes- or what is it that drives the weather? What is it that drives the weather? And I want to review a little bit about the notes that I took. You know what, if you want to take notes on our discussion, then get your pencils ready, get your papers ready. But there were some things that I had noticed about the discussion. And that is that we had several peOple who had said that it was the sun that drives the weather. We had a couple of people who had previously said it was the air that drives the weather, and then changed their ideas. And that was Maria and Alisa. BEN: Wasn’t it what drives the water cycle? 56 MS. CHRISTENSEN: What drives the water cycle, I’m son'y. Yes, you’re right. Thank you. What drives the water cycle? And Maria and Alisa had changed their ideas because before you had said the air. You changed your ideas to the sun, that it was the sun that drives the water cycle. So let’s go back and refocus. Tisha, I see that you also had changed your ideas so that you believed it was the sun. But some people had other ideas and I want to talk about those too. Ms. Christensen focused on instances in which students changed their accounts as a way of initiating discussion. By assigning authorship to students’ accounts she was able to track (for herself and the group) how those accounts changed throughout the discussion, which helped to make sense of how the accounts were linked to the people who generated them, and linked to each other. It is important to point out that while the accounts were authored, the accounts reflected the voices of Others, so authorship did not imply only an individual process of construction, but was also a reflection of what the group had discussed. Accounts were Positioned In classrooms using recitation and other monologic forms of discourse, conflict and tension in discourses is problematic, especially when the class is working toward one best explanation—in this case scientific accounts. So, in classrooms using recitation, emerging accounts may only get recognized as being “wrong” or “non-scientific”. Differently, in these two classrooms there was a diverse set of accounts under consideration, and conflict and tension between those accounts was used to stimulate rich discussion. In Ms. Christensen’s and Ms. Hocking’s classrooms, emerging accounts were used as a resource for bringing out tension and conflict that occurred between the discourse and knowledge students brought to the classroom, and the discourse and 57 knowledge expected of them as they learned in a science classroom. Dialogic indicators that signaled instances where the class negotiated this tension and conflict included the teacher or students responding to accounts by either agreeing or disagreeing, or creating alignment or Opposition between two or more accounts. The indicators, such as taking up ideas and agreeing or disagreeing, marked instances where the developing dialogue was not only responsive to what had been said previously, but the dialogue also showed the class was trying to organize the accounts. The following example is a continuation from the “What Drives the water cycle” excerpt above, and shows how students took up previous accounts and either aligned or opposed those accounts: What Drives the Water Cycle- Part 2 MS. CHRISTENSEN: Kamal, when we talked on Wednesday, can you remind us what it was that you said drives the water cycle? KAMAL: The water. MS. CHRISTENSEN: The water drives the water cycle and I know there were a couple of people who agreed with you. DO you want to tell us your idea again, tell us your reason for saying that? KAMAL: Because lots Of people said the sun, but if the sun doesn’t have anything to evaporate, then like water— nothing could evaporate, so what’s the point. (Long pause). MS. CHRISTENSEN: What do other people think about that? David? DAVID: He [Kamal] said that the water drives the water cycle so he means like- so if the water vapor- he said like if the water didn’t evaporate and the sun makes the water evaporate and condense it to precipitation. MS. CHRISTENSEN: Can you tell us more about that? I’m not sure. So were you agreeing with Kamal, or are you disagreeing? DAVID: Disagree. 58 MS. CHRISTENSEN: Disagree. And tell us again, why? DAVID: Because that’s if you don’t have any water to evaporate- if you don’t have water then you don’t have the water cycle, because the water, it’s hard to explain. MS. CHRISTENSEN: It is hard to explain. Think you can help [to Ben]? GO ahead. BEN: Well, I kind of agree with David and I don’t agree with Kamal, but the water IS the water cycle. The sun- you need the sun to evaporate the water. Without the sun you can get like it can start in the clouds and condense, and can precipitate, but it can’t re-evaporate- it stops right there because there’s no sun. SO that adds to part Of it. The water is pretty much just the water cycle. MS. CHRISTENSEN: What do other people think? Allen? ALLEN: I kind of agree, and kind Of disagree. Cause without the sun the water would freeze, and get very cold, we couldn’t survive, I think. And, but if there wasn’t water, nothing would be able to evaporate. MS. CHRISTENSEN: SO what is your idea about what drives the water cycle? ALLEN: Now I think water and sun. MS. CHRISTENSEN: What do other people think? How about someone who hasn’t talked yet? In this example, as well as the extended discussion in which it was embedded, Ms. Christensen helped the class work toward identifying heat energy (or the sun) as driving the water to cycle. Although the class later questioned the usefulness of the question, and the different interpretations students had about the question, it was still one that sparked rich discussion and debate between students. Students generally fell into one Of three groups: those that identified the sun or heat energy as driving the water cycle, those that identified water as driving the cycle, and those who could not label only one “driver”. 59 The original discussion of this question occurred as a “science talk” in which every student in class had the opportunity to share their response to the question, and then question other student responses. The excerpt above occurred when the class revisited the original discussion focusing on instances where students disagreed or changed their accounts. In the first line, Ms. Christensen gave authorship to Kamal by asking, “can you remind us what it was that you said.” She singled out Kamal because his account was substantially different from the accounts of other students and the scientific account the class was working toward. She also gave Kamal some degree of authority by saying, “I know there were a couple of people who agreed with you,” naming Kamal as the leader or spokesperson for his position. Kamal’s response, “Because lots of people said the sun” created an opposition between his claim that water drives the water cycle and other students’ claim that the sun drives the water cycle. Both David and Ben tried to justify why they disagreed with Kamal’s account, whereas Allen saw some usefulness and validity on both sides of the debate. Ms. Christensen encouraged the students to position their accounts, for instance, asking, “Can you tell us more about that? I’m not sure. So were you agreeing with Kamal, or are you disagreeing?” The students, however, positioned themselves without explicit calls from the teacher. By positioning the accounts, the class worked towards converging on a smaller number of accounts to consider and identifying the key disagreement between the accounts. Positioning encouraged students to come to consensus, both as a matter of convention (e. g., how things are labeled, defined, or categorized), and consensus about 60 models with the most explanatory power. For example, in Ms. Hocking’s class the students categorized materials in terms of how they interacted with light. Ms. Hocking explained to the class that they needed to come to consensus in their groups (and as a whole class) about the categorizations, and eventually how they defined the groups using scientific labels of transparent, translucent, and opaque: Light Interacting with Matter MS. HOCKING: A consensus is sort of like a general agreement among most pe0ple. So we can generally all agree. Okay, I want to get a sense of where peOple’s groups are deciding to put things. Nick, where did your group decide to put them? What’s the thing that lets through the most light according to your group? MS. HOCKING: Anange them into three distinct groups based on what they have in common. You can have as many as one, two, or three things in a group. SANJIT: Oh, I know. Transparent, translucent, and Opaque. CHRIS: How about this one lets most Of the light. This one lets some of the light, and then none. During this activity, the students first tested and grouped materials in terms of how they interacted with light. Then they decided the characteristics that best described each group. Some students, such as Sanjit, had already encountered the words “opaque”, “translucent”, and “transparent”, whereas other students had not. In such activities, the diversity of accounts was limited and coming to consensus about groups of materials was a matter of convention so that they group developed a joint understanding of the words, and the materials in which those words describe. At other times coming to consensus involved making decisions about which models had the most explanatory power. For instance, in Ms. Christensen’s class, the 61 students did not agree about “what drives the water cycle” and a lengthy discussion ensued about whether or not they were actually disagreeing about the model in the first place, and the importance of coming to consensus: Consensus about What Drives the Water Cycle MS. CHRISTENSEN: So what I want to do today, and I don’t want to take much time to do this, I want to wrap up that discussion and when I say wrap up what I mean is I want to come to some kind of consensus, if we can, on what it is that drives the water cycle. And I think the main— the main disagreement that I heard was at the end of our last discussion, which was on Friday, was Kamal was saying that water is the driving force and that water has to be there for the sun to evaporate it and so he held tightly to his idea that it was the water. And there were several people that were talking to him, Ben being one who was saying that it was, and I think Maria was the other, who had actually changed their ideas and now were arguing that the sun is the driving force or the thing that drives the water cycle. So, how do we come to some kind of consensus? How do we come to a bringing together of our ideas? What do you think Ben? BEN: Maybe the two can like argue it and try to decide what it is. MS. CHRISTENSEN: Okay, and how do you think we should do that? How do you- BEN: We could argue about what we disagree— we could go up to the board and explain it to everybody and then maybe we can figure out what the other person is saying and compare and contrast and make them understand- MS. CHRISTENSEN: Okay so you’re trying to convince each other by drawing and using words? Sounds like a good plan. So who would like to start off? MS. CHRISTENSEN: I don’t think I realized or appreciated just exactly how deeply you would think about it and how difficult and complicated the question was. But as I’ve listened to your questions, I’ve come to feel that maybe there really isn’t an answer. The reason that I asked the question in the first place, was because in your book when it came to what we were reading, “the role of the sun”, it said on page 79. You want to open up your book. “The role of the sun”, and we read through this. I’m waiting for you to get your book opened up to page 79. “Evaporation and condensation in earth’s atmosphere depends on the heat energy of the sun. The sun heats up the water on the surface of lakes, 62 rivers, oceans and even puddles. Water evaporates from all these sources adding water vapor to the air. When the sun heats earth unevenly, either from place to place, or from day to night, warm air cools and the water vapor condenses back to liquid water. This movement of water from the surface of the air, surface of the earth to the air and back to the surface is called the water cycle. What do you think drives the water cycle?” And I thought that paragraph made it pretty clear to me just what drove the water cycle. Now after having the discussion that we’ve had, I see that it’s much more complicated than that, and maybe the question is not nearly as important as the discussion that you’ve had about it. Do we need to come up with what drives the water cycle? Do you think we need to come to a consensus on that? Coming to consensus in this discussion was initially about developing some sort of agreement or shared understanding of a scientific account of water cycling (and the role of heat energy in water cycling processes). Ms. Christensen started the discussion setting up different “sides” or positions that had been shared in response to the question. But the discussion was also about where the disagreement was occurring: was it a matter of interpreting the question differently, such as Kamal interpreting the question as “what is the most important thing to the water cycle”, or were students truly disagreeing about a model for water cycling, and the role heat energy plays in water cycling processes? A third layer to the discussion was a meta-level question about the importance of coming to consensus, and having enough evidence to participate in this process. By having this discussion with students, Ms. Christensen not only supported the discussion norm of positioning ideas, but she also emphasized her goal for discussions was to have students come to agreement, and that she wanted students to have enough evidence to do that. Other markers in the discourse that showed the class was trying to link and position accounts were instances where individuals and the group evaluated accounts on the floor. While calls for evaluation often occurred as open-ended teacher questions (e.g., What do you think about what Kamal said?), by calling for students to evaluate each 63 other’s accounts, authority was given to students. At times, the evaluations occurred as class votes (e.g., How many peOple agree? How many people disagree? How many aren’t sure?) By allowing students to engage in the evaluation process, they were required to make decisions about why they did or did not agree, and to explain their decisions using evidence or some form of justification. For example, in the following three utterances Ms. Hocking encouraged students during discussions not only to make a decision, but to justify their choices with evidence: Example I: So she says if I move the jar towards that end of the table, the cork is going to move to the opposite end. Now why? What evidence do you have there? What’s your reasoning? What do you think? Example 2: You think the jar is first. Chris, do you know why? Can you defend that decision? Why did you guys think that? Example 3: Exactly the same. Okay, now I need some justification for why you put them where you put them. Ms. Hocking also wanted her students to be discerning and critical about when they chose to agree or disagree. As she explained in her interview, she did not want her students to disagree without reason to do so, or to align themselves with “smart” kids without questioning the accounts from those students: I need to nudge them toward what’s been tested and proven true by scientists, so I don’t want them just going out on a limb and disagreeing because they don’t see it. I want them to be able through experiences that l craft, in which they can discover that yes indeed this is the case... I want them to become discriminating, so that they don’t always assume that the smartest kid in the group has it. Because oftentimes one person will come in center stage and say, “well this is what I think” and everybody else will jump on board without questioning it. So I want [them] to question. While Ms. Christensen and Ms. Hocking recognized that students bring diverse accounts to the classroom, they also had goals for identifying the inadequacy of emerging 64 accounts, and use conflict between accounts to support the class in converging toward a scientific account. In this way they not only engaged their students in divergent and critical talk, such as the debate observed in Ms. Christensen’s classrooms, but they also engaged students in convergent and critical talk characteristic of inquiry and consensus- making (Burbules, 1993). Talking about Talk The ability to discuss does not come naturally for many students, especially given that students have little practice doing so in schools. Ms. Christensen and Ms. Hocking explicitly talked about talk with their students, focusing on the listening and speaking responsibilities of group members. They emphasized that discussions, either as a whole class, in small groups, or conversations with peers were an important part of learning. While the listening and speaking responsibilities were not necessarily markers for dialogic discourse themselves, they illustrated the expectations of normative participation in during discussion. Ms. Christensen reminded her class during discussions that she expected them to actively listen and be thoughtful about what was said: Expectations for Discussion You know, I forgot to go over the procedures or the expectations for when we’re talking in groups. So I’m going to go over them right now. Because I see that some people have forgotten. When someone else is talking, you want to make sure that you listen to them when they’re talking. You want to make sure that you’re being a good listener. And if someone makes a comment, says something, you’re going to talk about their ideas. You’re not going to talk about a person. You’re not going to make a comment or say something about the person. You’re going to talk about the ideas. We’re going to talk about lot of ideas. And you need to accept other people’s ideas. Questioning them is fine, but you do need to listen to them, and try to understand what it is that they’re saying. If don’t understand what they’re saying, then you’re going to ask questions, to help you 65 understand what they’re saying. Okay. SO now that we’ve gone over that, got that refreshed in your mind, you need to make sure that you’re being a good listener, ready to participate in the discussion. . .You need to listen. Ask yourself questions. Ask the class questions, so that you can understand it. That’s how you learn, by asking yourself questions, by asking other people questions, by getting ideas, thinking “does that make sense to me?” For Ms. Christensen listening was an active process, in which students were expected to continually question themselves (e.g., Does that make sense to me?) and develop questions for their peers. Ms. Christensen also expected students to talk loudly so that everyone in the room could hear (e.g., “Talk so they can hear you. That’s your responsibility”). Ms. Hocking also expected high participation of students in terms of both listening and speaking. She expressed concern in her interview that students did not have enough practice articulating their ideas in schools, and that discussion, along with writing, were important activities for helping students articulate and formalize their ideas: They don’t know how to use the language to explain what they are seeing. In part it’s because they don’t have a complete understanding of it. But also even if they can show it to you, they can’t translate what they are seeing into words on the page, but also they sometimes can’t explain it orally. So for me that’s one of the toughest, toughest things in science—the communication piece for them. . .I guess I want them to be able to be articulate in explaining not just science phenomena, but articulate in explaining their positions, their ideas, their Opinions, and part of that comes from being able to observe and being able to explain and articulate what it is that you see and what you know. Ms. Hocking expected students to also listen carefirlly and to question each other’s accounts, as part of her goal for helping students become more discerning and critical during discussion. She modeled for students during discussions how to write explanations, how to read science texts carefully, and how to be metacognitive during discussion by asking questions of oneself (e.g., “Okay, so as a note to myself, I might say, ‘From side view it’s gone; penny not visible.’ I’m not 66 writing too many verbs here. I’m keeping it pretty basic”). She also felt that supporting students in articulating their ideas, both orally and in writing, was one of her toughest challenges. Challenges to Participating in Dialogic Discourse During the interviews, both teachers brought up interesting issues about the challenges they faced in orchestrating productive discussions. The challenges generally revolved around balancing the voice of the teacher or textbook with the voice of students. Challenges also emerged around issues of time and curricular goals, and when and how to incorporate students’ unpredictable questions. A final issue emerged around how teachers used and integrated writing and talking activities. Balancing Teacher and Student Voices In classrooms using monologic discourse activities the teacher and textbook control the discourse—an authoritative discourse—and the teacher takes the role of evaluator of what counts as scientific (or “right”) versus what does not count. Both Ms. Christensen and Ms. Hocking viewed themselves as more than evaluators. They believed their job was to monitor the pace and content of the discussion, helping the class get back on track if needed, and helping certain students gain the floor (i.e., facilitate the discussion). They also believed they needed to help students make connections and position themselves within the discussion. As facilitators this involved managing the “verbal traffic” of the discussions (Cazden, 2001). In her interview, Ms. Christensen explained her perceived role in discussions: I see myself as the facilitator and that is to move the conversation along. To make sure we don’t get bogged down with stories. To make sure everybody has a chance to get there ideas out... even though it may appear that we ramble around, 67 I do have a pathway that I’m trying to get them down. I’ll use an analogy. I’ve got the pathway. I’m letting them put the bricks down on the pathway, so they’re building, but I have a direction that I want to go with the discussion and certain things that I want to get out. And we don’t always get out what I want us to get out, and sometimes we get sidetracked. Like the sleet discussion was a sidetrack, and it was just way too interesting to abandon. When I hear something like that I think its almost exciting because I think here’s a chance to kind of fix up, explore, or take apart, and pick at something that I know someone in here has information about. I know there’s someone in here who can either reason it out or has read something about it. And so I guess I see my job at that point is to slow the conversation down so that we can do that exploring of the idea and to give kids who may have read something or seen something, or watched a video, or something that has that piece of information, or can help someone else understand. My job is to make sure that we slow it down, so they can do their job. Facilitating discussion, as opposed to controlling discussion, was a challenging task for the teachers, especially when they had a “pathway” and “goal” to make progress toward. It involved carefully balancing their own voice with the voice of students, and relinquishing the floor so that students talked to one another, rather than filtering their ideas through the teacher. Gallas (1995) described this process as bringing the student voice to the foreground, while silencing the teacher (at least to some degree). The teachers, however, retained the responsibility of helping students to make sense of what each other was saying. The following example illustrates how Ms. Christensen facilitated discussion in her classrooms, directing students to talk with one another: Comparing Investigations and Weather MS. CHRISTENSEN: What does it have to do with clouds and the weather? Let’s have volunteers who have not talked today. Kamal? We haven’t heard from you today. What does this have to do with the weather and have to do with clouds? What do you have? What did you write down? KAMAL: Well weather — when it’s snowing outside, it’s always cold, and when we did the experiment, it was cold in the glass? 68 STUDENTS: What? What did he say? MS. CHRISTENSEN: Okay. So the temperature —. If you have something to say, raise your hand. ILmu have a question for Kamal. Alisa? ALISA: I still don’t know how that has to do with weather. MS. CHRISTENSEN: What does it have to do with weather? So your question to Kamal is what does it have to do with weather? You’re not understanding it? _Vfliat would you say to her. Kamal? KAMAL: Like when it’s snowing, it’s always cold. And so is the glass when the ice melts it. Ms. Hocking also thought students needed Opportunities to talk to one another, and her role was to facilitate that dialogue. Although she expressed concern that she was too involved and controlling of the dialogue, she also stepped back from the conversation to let students talk to each other: Speed and Getting Around the Loop MS. HOCKING: Colt, what’s your question? What do you want to ask? Maybe people can answer it for you. COLT: Well, you just answered it. MS. HOCKING: Speak up so we can hear you. COLT: You guys just answered it. I wanted to know what would happen if you pushed it from the other side and if it would gain enough speed to go around this loop. BRIAN: No, if it did gain enough speed to go around the loop- CHRIS: It would just go around the loop and fall right down. BRIAN:: Then it would it would have to go up this slope, but it would not have enough energy left over. It would barely get over there. CHRIS: It doesn’t have enough speed because this isn’t steep enough. 69 One of the issues that emerged with facilitating discussions was deciding when hands did or did not need to be raised. By asking students to raise their hands, the teacher was placed in a position of authority and control because she decided who would gain the floor. At times, however, students also led discussions by calling on peers who had questions for them. Deciding how students gain the floor, whether through hand raising, talking chips, or other means was problematic when teachers wanted to be facilitators, participants, or even spectators to the ensuing discussion. Ms. Christensen explained in her interview that her ideal conversation would be one in which she was able to participate, but not necessarily lead: In my ideal conversation what I do is I hand the conversation over to them, so that I can withdraw from it because I think they know what they know and don’t know, and they have the intelligence in this class to help each other learn. I guess I think they have all the intelligence they need; they have all the questions; they can discuss with each other and that’s what I want them to do. I want them to form that community and respect for each other, by listening and really hearing what they’re saying. I want them to take control of that conversation, so that I’m still back there listening and if we get off track I can get them back on track. Those are the best conversations for me—when they’re talking to each other and I’m a participant rather than the one who is directing it. The description of Ms. Christensen’s ideal conversation demonstrates the challenges teachers faced when trying to orchestrate discussion in which students had much more control as compared to recitation. How do teachers honor students’ wisdom and “hand the conversation over to them”, while simultaneously accomplishing the curricular goals? It is important to point out that no matter how much stepping back the teachers did during a conversation, a power asymmetry still remained (Edwards & Mercer, 1987). The students naturally tended to filter their contributions through the teachers, so that the pattern of dialogue continued to follow a teacher-student pattern as opposed to student-student pattern. There were instances where the students lead and 70 controlled discussions, and even called on peers to ask questions, but the student-student dialogue did not occur without teacher prompting and teacher facilitation. The key to balancing teacher and students’ voices was how much control the teacher had, and when and how the scientific, or authoritative voice, entered the conversation. There was no doubt that at some point the teachers would need to introduce a scientific account—or take on the authoritative voice. As Bransford, Brown, and Cooking (2004) explained, even in constructivist classrooms teachers tell students the “right” information or story at some point. The when and how differentiates whether the classroom is constructivist or didactic. As Ms. Christensen alluded to, students had the intelligence to be able to compare accounts, especially comparing scientific and emerging accounts. Yet, they needed the teacher to introduce more sophisticated accounts (to challenge their thinking) and to help structure activities, especially discussions, in ways that helped them position and critique those accounts. Therefore, while these discussions are more dialogic in nature compared to recitation, they should be viewed as examples of teacher-led dialogism. Making progress toward adOpting scientific accounts and discourse was the primary goal for these teachers. The process of making this happen required students to have awareness of discourses (and differences between discourses and accounts) (Moje et al., 2001) and to make compromises by accepting new discourse when one’s vernacular discourse was limited (Gee, 2004b). The teachers supported students in being metacognitively aware of language. One way the teachers did this was by encouraging students to pay careful attention to the meaning of words and how they were used in the classroom. Because science uses 71 specialized language (Bazennan, 1988; Duschl et al., 2007; Palincsar, Anderson, & David, 1993; Shanna & Anderson, 2007) and the scientific register is replete with words that have very specific meanings (Lemke, 1990), understanding the meaning of words and how they are pieced together is incredibly important to acquiring scientific discourse. In other words, learning how to do science is not just memorizing a vocabulary list, but knowing how to put those words together to build scientific accounts. Dialogic discourse was critical for identifying instances when words were being used appropriately and inappropriately. It provided opportunities for the class to develop shared meanings about scientific words (e.g., “What do you mean by evaporation”), and how words were pieced together to form accounts (“Water vapor goes up into clouds” versus “Water vapor condenses to form clouds”). In the following transcript Ms. Christensen’s class considered how molecules were related to water: Molecules Make Up Water MS. CHRISTENSEN: The question was - we talked about molecules and what molecules have to do with the evaporation. But what do molecules have to do with water vapor, condensation—all those things? CARTER: Well, this is an idea. Maybe as the wategoes through the cycle. maybe the molecule does. too. Like if the water evaporates. maybe the molecules may evaporate. MS. CHRISTENSEN: Hmm. What do other people think about that? Lesley? LESLEY: I think the molecules have to do with water and water has to do with water vapor and condensation and all that. MS. CHRISTENSEN: Can you explain how? MS. CHRISTENSEN: Because like molecules are in water and so that’s how they connect. And then from the molecules in the water- molecules- oh dang- 72 Molecules and water, they go together; and then water and condensation and all that are like —I don’t know. MS. CHRISTENSEN: Okay. You want to add something, Allen? ALLEN: I thought molecules made up water— MS. CHRISTENSEN: Okay ALLEN: That’s what made water. MS. CHRISTENSEN: Okay. So if I ask you to go to the board and draw a droplet of water and what you would see under a very, very powerful microscope, would you do that for us, Allen? ALLEN: Yeah- Sure (student goes to board). You want me to draw a droplet? Both Carter and Lesley had accounts that molecules are in water, while Allen explained that molecules made up water. This example showed how slight nuances in language indicated substantially different meanings (and different accounts). Careful attention to language and words was important because it served as a way of identifying key differences, which were then used coming to consensus. While Carter and Lesley had encountered a scientific account about water molecules from their books, they had not reconciled it with their emerging accounts. Allen, on the other hand, had adOpted the scientific account of molecules. A challenge for Ms. Christensen and Ms. Hocking was to decide when the slight nuances in language were indications of substantially different accounts, and when they were examples of students talking about the same account using slightly different words. The teachers probed and asked questions about the meaning of words as one way to deal with this challenge. For example, when talking about reflection, it became clear to Ms. Hocking that students did not have a shared understanding of the word: 73 What’s a Reflection? HEATHER: Well, if it doesn’t reflect- wait, it does reflect. MS. HOCKING: Does it reflect? HEATHER: Yeah. MS. HOCKING: How so? Show us what you mean. HEATHER: Like see? There’s like a little reflection thingy. (demonstrates with flashlight) MS. HOCKING: What’s a reflection? I guess that’s maybe what we need to clear up. What is a reflection? CHRIS: A mirror image of something. MS. HOCKING: A mirror image of something? HEATHER Or it’s kind of like a reflection of like maybe it’s called light reflection. MS. HOCKING: Maybe it’s called light reflection. Well, maybe it is. This is really interesting. Heather and Chris had assigned different meanings to the word “reflection”, which prevented them from agreeing or developing an explanation. The teachers and students had to be attentive to how language was used so they could develop what Paul Cobb and his colleagues have identified as a “taken-as-shared” meaning for accounts and objects (e.g., Cobb, Stephan, McClain, & Gravemeijer, 2001; Yackel & Cobb, 1996). Attention to language not only occurred in how students used words, but also how words were used in texts read during class. While reading from texts, Ms. Christensen told students to listen and write down words to talk about: Okay, good. I want you to listen as I read the book about — listen for words, listen for sentences that are going to help you understand those words that we started 74 talking about yesterday. We started talking about evaporation, precipitation, condensation, and there were other words that we had talked about. You know, we’ll run across them in the reading. I want you to listen for those words and then I want you to write those words down so that we can talk about it. A potential challenge for students in making sense of scientific words is that sometimes meaning of those words may be different from meaning in everyday uses of the word. Devon, for example, had developed a meaning of the word evaporation that was substantially different from how it was being used in science class: Meaning of Evaporation MS. CHRISTENSEN: What do you mean evaporating? THOMAS: Cause like the water molecules or something goes- go into the- goes up- I don’t know, it has something to do with heat. Heat makes it evaporate. MS. CHRISTENSEN: What do you think? (to Devon) DEVON: I thought it was when something disappeared or — MS. CHRISTENSEN: Something? Or — What is the “something”? DEVON: The water. It’s anything, I think. ‘Cause ashes, when they just disappear in movies— They say that something evaporates. MEGAN: Ashes? DEVON: Ashes when people die. Um, in this movie — a man got shot, and then his ashes came down. Then they just disappeared. MS. CHRISTENSEN: So you think that’s evaporating? What do you think about that? Derek, what do you think? In many ways Ms. Christensen and Ms. Hocking helped students navigate the “third space” in order for them to construct shared meanings about words and accounts. While this presented a challenge for the teachers, exemplary teachers, such as Ms. 75 Hocking and Ms. Christensen, had a sense of when it was important to point out differences between how words and accounts were used. They also recognized when differences in language were preventing individuals and the class from participating, and tried to solve these problems by making students metacognitively aware of language and how it was used in the classroom. Balancing Structure and Spontaneity. Teachers are required to cover increasingly large amounts of curriculum during the school year, with standards-base reform helping structure what is taught to students. Obviously teachers face the challenge of accomplishing all their curricular goals in the limited time they have, which forces teachers to choose between strongly adhering to their curricular plans or being flexible in pursuing questions generated by students. Ms. Christensen and Ms. Hocking expressed concern about balancing the two, although both felt strongly that having students ask questions (which the class pursued) was an important piece of learning science. They struggled with decisions about how long they could pursue student questions, and at what point to come back to their goals, and how to tie students’ questions in with the content the class was already covering. In the Ice and Antarctica transcript, for example, Ms. Christensen attempted to pull out parts of the discussion most relevant to what the class had previously talked about (i.e., differential heating of Earth). She explained in her interview that she was always making decisions about when to pursue and not pursue student questions: Yeah, and I guess I feel I’m making decisions all the time about is this something that fits what we’re studying or is this something that we’re going, although it might be an interesting conversation, its something that really they have curiosity about. And I tr)I to incorporate their questions, incorporate their curiosity, but 76 maybe it belongs in the question book, so that we can come back to it later, or so that its preserved and doesn’t get lost. Ms. Christensen used a “question book” as a way to record student questions, especially those not discussed by the class. It was a record of questions that had been asked by her students over several years, so students in the current year could read about questions asked in previous years. During computer lab time, the class took the question book with them so that they could look up answers using the Internet. They were also allowed to read the question book during silent reading time. Interplay Between Writing and Talking. Although this study focused on instances of discussion, it became apparent that both teachers believed the activities of talking and writing were intimately connected, which further supported the view that studies of literacy should pay attention to printed language, as well as oral use of that language (e.g., Gee, 2006; Halliday, 1993) Both activities helped students to formulate and communicate their accounts in unique ways. The challenge for Ms. Christensen and Ms. Hocking was deciding how and when to use these activities, which Ms. Christensen referred to as the “layering process”. Ms. Christensen explained in her interview that the acts of talking and writing serve different purposes at different times: It kind of pushes the enveIOpe, so that they have to commit to an idea. They have to think it through and then commit to an idea of what they think is going to happen... its the layering process, and I think the discussion kind of stirs up all the ideas, and the writing gets them to think in their own head—to make sense of it in their head—and then to commit. . .what I would expect to happen is they were committing—getting it down on paper—so its like the language was being formalized and then when we went to the discussion, they could take it to a higher level or a different level. 77 Like Ms. Christensen, Ms. Hocking also used speaking and writing for different purposes in her classrooms. In her interview she shared beliefs about writing and speaking that were similar to Ms. Christensen’s beliefs: In part the written piece can be helping them formulate in their own minds an explanation that makes sense to them, and it also does give them something to talk about because if they spend time thinking and writing, they’re going to have something to say. . .But in terms of generating conversation and discussion, I really think that they have to sit and sift through the stuff in their brain, and they have to make sense of it, and writing forces them to be able to communicate their ideas in some way or another whether its accurate or not. For both teachers writing served two primary purposes. First it allowed students to “make sense of it in their heads” (Ms. Christensen) and “sit and sift through stuff” (Ms. Hocking). “It” and “stuff” likely referred to a number of things around making sense of naive and scientific accounts. In a way, the teachers believed writing gave students an opportunity to take what they heard from other students, the teacher, and the text, and to reconcile those accounts and discourse with their own personal experiences and discourse. The teachers also believed writing was a chance for students to “commit to an idea” (Ms. Christensen) and “forces them to be able to communicate their ideas in some way or another” (Ms. Hocking). By committing, students had to make decisions about how to respond to a writing prompt, and select what they believed to be the most appropriate response. Writing was also a way to “formalize” explanations or accounts—a way for students to construct accounts using appropriate grammatical rules for sentences and paragraphs, and to use scientific words properly in the context of an account. As Halliday and Martin (1993) explained, “writing puts language in chains; freezes it, so that is becomes a thing to be reflected on” (p. 118). This occurred in Ms. Christensen’s class, 78 for instance, when students engaged in writing in response to a prompt, and then shared discussed their writing during science talks. As Ms. Christensen explained, she layered the speaking and writing activities because participating in each type of activity was beneficial to the other. For example, discussions helped to “stir up all the ideas”, and writing helped to take discussions to a “higher level or a different level.” This finding corroborates other research, such as Rivard and Straw (2000) who found that middle school students benefited most from engaging in speaking and writing activities, as opposed to just one or the other. 79 PART 2: CLASSROOM SOCIOSCIENTIFIC NORMS Dialogism and Emergence of Norms Dialogic discourse, as discussed in the first part of the results, was a type of talk that builds from diversity in voices and the constant positioning and repositioning of those voices. It allowed for students’ voice to have weight in the classrooms, and provided opportunities for students to reconcile what they brought to science with new accounts and discourse they encountered. By having a diverse set of accounts on the floor, the class was able to take advantage (and honor) the vast experiential base of their members and by linking and positioning accounts, the class was able to identify how accounts were different, and determine ways to reconcile those accounts. Yet, engaging students in dialogic discourse was not an end in itself. It would be misleading to claim that simply talking, even if the talk was more dialogic, was the primary goal the teachers had for their students. Rather, what did dialogic discourse afford in terms of opportunities to participate with each other and content? What came to “count” as knowledge and practices in these classrooms and how did the discourse help achieve these goals? During the study it became apparent that through the interactive discourse, teachers were able to support students in participating with the science practices described in the introduction of this paper. When participation with practices used standards and criteria specific to those practices, normative ways for participating emerged. I refer to these normative ways of participating as classroom socioscientific norms. In the following results, I will illustrate how the interactive discourse allowed for 80 the emergence of classroom socioscientific norms, focusing on the following norms as key examples: 0 Validation of observations: What counted as precise, accurate, and replicable? When students participated with the practice of making observations, what was expected of sharing details of how observations were made, and the degree of accuracy and precision in those observations? ' Validation of explanations: What counted as appropriate connections between observations, patterns, and models? When students participated in the practice of constructing and critiquing explanations, how were appropriate connections between observations, patterns, and models determined, and how was evidence used to support these connections? After discussing these norms, I will consider meta-level discussions about the application and explanatory power of models, and the need for coming to consensus. Validation of Observations During the time I observed Ms. Hocking’s class they were engaged in a unit about light, focusing mostly on understanding reflection, refraction, and colors. In Ms. Hocking’s class students participated with practices of making observations and collecting data nearly every lesson. Sometimes these investigations were conducted as whole class demonstrations, but mostly they occurred in small groups where observations were later discussed by the whole class. One of Ms. Hocking’s goals for students during this unit (and for the year) was to become more observant and analytical, which she explained in her interview: 81 One of the things that I’ve told them all year long—and this is true almost across the board—is that if I do nothing else, I want to make them observant and analytical about what they observe. And I think that science provides a lot of opportunities for them to be able to do that because I think that they need to be observant in lots and lots of arenas and to be able to think and analyze about things they are seeing. . .I do manipulate a lot, but I think it’s a type of manipulation that forces them to look at salient elements that otherwise they might miss because if their observation skills aren’t honed—if they’re not recognizing what really is having the effect or what really is important—they may not be able to explain it. But if they’re observations are poor, then I think my questioning—my facilitation—has to do with helping them focus their observations. Ms. Hocking devoted a great deal of discussion time to sharing observations, so there were ample opportunities for the class to collectively validate whether the observations were accurate and precise, and whether or not students explained observations with enough detail to be replicated. Ms. Hocking was especially attentive to standards of replicability and precision. As she explained in her interview, she felt her questioning during discussion was key to establishing normative ways of making and sharing observations. In the following transcript Ms. Hocking questioned students to help them clarify how they made their observations, so the focus was not on the Observations themselves, but rather how they were collected: The Penny and the Jar MS. HOCKING: Do you have anything to add? You’ve got quite a bit written down there, James. Do you have some other things you want to add to the observations? What did you see? Oh, tell us about that one. JAMES: Oh, well, when I, like, looked at it from a diagonal on the side, it looked kind of bigger. MS. HOCKING: So were you looking through the water or the jar and the water? 82 JAMES: The jar and the water. MS. HOCKING: The jar and the water. So when you looked at it sort of diagonallygyou were able to make the penny apgtar larger than it is? Okay. Yeah, Heather? HEATHER: When you look directly at the water line, you can only see half of the penny. MS. HOCKING: Oh, Lou mean, when you looked right at the level of the water? (demonstrates to class) HEATHER: Yeah, if you looked kind of up, but looked at the water line, there was only half of the penny showing. MS. HOCKING: Oh, okay, yeah, yeah, you could only see part of the penny. Yeah, that was - kind of looked oval. Yeah, I saw that. Yeah. JANICE: Okay. I said that if I put the jar up to my eye and then I kind of went up and looked at the penny, it would be smaller, it would look smaller, it appeared smaller. MS. HOCKING: Were you looking straight down through thejar? (demonstrates to class) JANICE: Yes... MS. HOCKING: So you just lifted it away from the penny and as you got farther and farther away from the penny, the penry seemed to do what? JANICE: It appeared smaller than without the jar. MS. HOCKING: Oh, okay, so you were able to change the size of it by moving the position of the thing you were looking through, the glass and the water. In this excerpt, Ms. Hocking was interested in what her students observed. There were no “right” and “wrong” answers to the questions she asked, only sufficient or insufficient details. The questions were not directed at explaining or evaluating observations, but rather were focused on ‘procedures’ for obtaining those observations (e.g., “So were you looking through the water or the jar and the water?”; “You mean, 83 when you looked right at the level of the water?”). By using questions Ms. Hocking was able to clarify details of how observations were made and establish an acceptable level of detail that should be shared with the group, so the class not only understood the observations, but also could replicate them. She also communicated that observations needed to include ‘measurable’ information, such as angle or distance at which the observation was obtained. The exchange that occurred between Janice and Ms. Hocking demonstrates how Janice’s account transformed as Ms. Hocking questioned her. When Janice shared her observation of the penny and jar, her original account of the observation was ambiguous. It did not provide the necessary details to describe what she actually did. Ms. Hocking helped to clarify Janice’s procedures through questioning. Janice also revised her original observation twice to make her language more accurate, from “it [penny] would be smaller, it would look smaller, it appeared smaller. Later she followed up her account with the comparison “appeared smaller than without the jar”. Janice modified her language to share a more precise observation of what appeared to happen to the penny, and she also made a comparison about the penny’s appearance that could be replicated and measured. In this way, Janice’s account reflects one in transition—one in which Janice attempted to take on the scientific discourse as she described her observation. Precise observations, such as Janice’s, were important in Ms. Hocking’s classroom, especially when the class did not agree with observations being shared. The following transcript provides an example of an instance when the class did not agree with an observation being shared (see Figures 2 and 3): 84 Where does Light Bend? MS. HOCKING: Draw a sketch in your book to indicate what you think is happening and write a few statements about what you see happening. What did you observe? (Chris comes to the overhead to draw his observation for class, and then sits down) MS. HOCKING: Hmm. Did it do that? (asking the class) SAM: Kind of. MS. HOCKING: Did it? Is that what you observed? ELLEN: Yeah. KATIE: Yes. SANJIT: Yep. MS. HOCKING: Really? Whoa. I’m very curious about this because th_at’s not what I saw. TREVOR: No I said that it was inside. (disagreeing with Chris’ observation) MS. HOCKING: flat’s not that I saw. Hang on let’s think about this. According to what he |Chris| is saying it went through, it bent, it crossed, and then it went back parallel. TREVOR: No. MS. HOCKING: Did they do that? KELLY: No. MS. HOCKING: This is a little trickier than it seems. Let’s tg again. We’ll have another one. We’ll have another go at it. Okay. Who thinks they can figure it out--let ’s use a diflerent color this time. Who thinks they can- Brian go ahead. You really haven’t had a chance up there. BRIAN: I don’t really know what happened when it was in the jar. MS. HOCKING: Leave that part white then. BRIAN: I saw the light goes through. Then I saw it go like that. 85 MS. HOCKING: Okay. Mmm. That’s what I saw too. Wasn’t it going straight once it left? Didn’t Must stay in a straight line? Because remember light does travel in a straight line unless it encounters something else like a new medium to travel through. So once it left thejar isn’t it just gong in a straight line? STUDENTS: Yes. Yeah. MS. HOCKING: Yeah. Okay. But Brian said he wasn’t sure what was happening in the jar. Sam, do you know what happened in the jar? SAM: Yes. (Sam walks to overhead projector and fills in Brian’s observation) MS. HOCKING: Does anybody else think they know what happened in the jar? Draw a picture for yourselfjust like we have done up here and if your lines aren’t accurate go ahead and. . .(tums to Sam’s drawing) it crossed twice? SANJIT: NOpe. MS. HOCKING: Did it cross itselftwice? DEREK: No. No. MS. HOCKING: Here’s a good question for you. If it crossed itself twice, when you blocked one slit-- SANJIT: It would be the same. MS. HOCKING: What one would disappear? SAM: Oh. SANJIT: The same slit that was blocked. MS. HOCKING: Ahhh. So Sam, how can we make it refract inside the glass without- Derek and Chris think it has to bend a little bit in the jar. The discussion continued with two more students drawing their observations on the overhead and the class discussed and voted on which were most accurate. The class was working toward agreeing on an accurate and precise observation of what happened to light when it encountered a curved jar. 86 Unlike the previous Penny and Jar transcript, Ms. Hocking’s questions in Where Does Light Bend were mostly close-ended. This is likely because Ms. Hocking was searching for an observation that was precise, and in this case, there was not much room for diversity. A salient feature in the Where Does Light Bend example was the tension that occurred between observations and the way class members were positioned in the discussion. Some students agreed with Chris’ observation, while others disagreed. Ms. Hocking also weighed in by disagreeing with Chris (e.g., “that’s not what I saw.”) and Sam (e.g., “did it cross itself twice?”) and agreeing with the partial observation shared by Brian (e.g., “that’s what I saw too.”). By allowing space for students to share their observations and make sense of the differences, the class was able to participate in collectively validating which observations were most precise. Importantly, when Ms. Hocking disagreed with Chris’ observation, she did not give students the correct answer. Instead, she had students use different colors markers as they drew their observations on the overhead, so the class was able to consider the differences between those observations (see Figure 2 and 3). Although there was a sense that there was one ‘right’ answer (or observation), by allowing for more and less accurate observations to be compared, the students were able to see why precision of observations matters. In this excerpt the class did not reach a precise account of what happened to the two light beams when they encountered the curved jar (although they did later in the lesson), so the class focused on making sense of why the observations were inaccurate. Sam, for example, “kind of” agreed with Chris’ original observation, and then implicitly 87 agreed with Brian by volunteering to complete Brian’s observation on the overhead. When the class disagreed with what Sam drew, it became clear that he had not connected his observation of the two light beams with evidence collected about what happened when one of those beams was covered (i.e., since the curved jar caused the two light beams to cross once, when covering one beam, the reflection on the white paper disappeared from the “opposite” side) (see Figure 3). In this case, the evidence collected during the investigation did not support Sam’s observation of the beams. Instead the evidence was used to dispute Sam’s observation. Sanjit realized the beams could not have crossed twice given the evidence from the investigation. This transcript is an example of how the class learned to coordinate the evidence from their investigations to develop precise observations, and questioning inaccurate accounts was critical to achieving this goal and forcing the students to think critically about evidence. During the time I observed Ms. Christensen’s class, they were learning about processes that changed water from one state to another, and the water cycle as a whole. The class began the unit by conducting three investigations: boiling water (investigation 1), comparing evaporation and condensation of water placed in a sunny and dark condition with plastic covering and not covering the water dishes (investigation 2), and condensation on the outside of a glass of ice water (investigation 3). As Ms. Christensen explained in her interview, the investigations served as a “common experience” for members of the class. The class returned to the investigations repeatedly when trying to make sense of their Observations. 88 Like Ms. Hocking, Ms. Christensen helped to establish norms for validating observations. She explained in her interview that she wanted “them to describe what it is that they see and be able to do that accurately.” The first example discussion came from a discussion of students’ observations of condensation on the outside of a glass of ice water. Fog on a Cold Glass of Water MS. CHRISTENSEN: The one with the glass and the water and the ice that was set up over here? What did you notice? JACOB: I noticed that there was something like fog coming down on the side. MS. CHRISTENSEN: Where was the fog? You said there is something like a fog? JACOB: Yeah. A fog — MS. CHRISTENSEN: And where was it? JACOB: I think it was on the outside. MS. CHRISTENSEN: On the outside of the glass. How many people noticed that there was something like a fog on the outside of the glass? Now when you say a fog what do you mean? A fog. Can you give us a better description of that? Corinne, did you see it? CORINNE: No. MS. CHRISTENSEN: No? You didn’t see it? Who did see it? Who was able to see i_t_? Camille? EV_h_at ditLvou see? Describe it. CAMILLE: It was clouds. If it were the one where the water and fog, you know? It was so cold. MS. CHRISTENSEN: There was — fog, where? CAMILLE: Inside of where all the water was. MS. CHRISTENSEN: On the inside. They’re having trouble understanding. You need to turn this way so that they can hear. Natalie, did you Observe that one? 89 NATALIE: Well it was outside in the sun and the ice melted. MS. CHRISTENSEN: The ice started to melt. But there were other things that were happening. What else was happening? Lesley? What else was happening? LESLEY: I saw the fogging through the ice. Some of the ice started to melt, and they have a coating of— molecules. And then sooner or later, like a fog, doubled up and- MS. CHRISTENSEN: The fog doubled up. Tell us what you mean by that. LESLEY: Okay like in the beginning, it was like you could still see that there was a regular fog. And then every time, like — the more time in the air, it fogged up more. And it left it- MS. CHRISTENSEN: When — that — you’re calling this — this moisture- fog. Where was it? On the inside of the glass or the outside of the glass? LESLEY: I think it was on the outside. MS. CHRISTENSEN: On the outside. Did anyone touch it? Did anyone see? Clearly _s;c_e_2 Jaclyn? Even in this short excerpt, dialogic discourse indicators were present. Ms. Christensen began with an authentic and open-ended question eliciting observations (i.e., “what did you notice?”). She took up Jacob’s observation and vernacular discourse of “fog” on the glass, and encouraged the class to focus on describing the “fog” and where it was located. Although, the meaning of “fog” was likely understood by many students, Ms. Christensen recognized the meaning may not have been known or shared by all. She asked Jacob to elaborate (“Now when you say fog, what do you mean?”). It appeared Ms. Christensen used this strategy as a way to ensure the class had a shared meaning of the 66f0g99. Ms. Christensen called for group evaluations (“How many people noticed that there was something like a fog on the outside of the glass?”) to establish that most class 90 members agreed with Jacob’s initial observation. She also called for subsequent individual evaluations, such as when she asked Lesley where the fog was located. She was not satisfied with relying solely on Jacob’s observation, and thus, elicited further observations to corroborate what had been shared. Through this type of talk, Ms. Christensen indicated to her students that observations can be validated when community members agree on their accuracy, and also when multiple people have similar observations. She also privileged observations from students who could “clearly see” what happened (or had touched the glass). In this example, even though students were participating with the practice of making observations, their observations did not appear to follow rules for accuracy or precision without cuing from the teacher. For instance, Jacob described “something like a fog coming down the side”, in which Ms. Christensen reacted by asking, “where was it?” Ms. Christensen’s reaction was intended to help establish the location of the fog—was it on the inside or outside of the glass? She asked students repeatedly “where was it [the fog]?” and “can you give us a better description?” As such, she communicated that when making observations students needed to be thoughtful, considering how they explained observations to their peers, and not just themselves. Validation of Explanations During the water cycle unit in Ms. Christensen’s class, they focused on learning about the processes of evaporation and condensation. In developing accounts about how water changed from state to state, students participated with practices of connecting observations with patterns and models, which involved the construction and critique of 91 explanations (Ford, 2008) and using evidence to decide what counted as appropriate connections. The next transcript, Condensation on Plastic, was taken from a discussion where the class reconsidered the evaporation and condensation investigations after reading a scientific account from their books. The book explained: How does heat energy help evaporation and condensation? Liquid water is made up of many molecules moving all around. When heat is removed from liquid water, it can be cooled so far that the molecules stop moving around. They lock together in a pattern, and the liquid becomes solid. The opposite is also true. When heat is added to liquid water, the molecules of water begin to move around faster and faster. Some reach the surface of the water, and escape into the air. These molecules evaporate. As long as the air is warm enough to keep the molecules moving, water will stay in the air as water vapor. But, if the air touches a cooler surface, and cools down, the air cannot hold as much vapor. The air releases the extra water in the form of drops on the cold surface. The water vapor condenses. The book introduced a simplified scientific model for explaining patterns of condensation and evaporation. It introduced students to the idea that heat energy affects evaporation and condensation, and that molecules speed up or slow down depending on heat energy. The following transcript illustrates how the class participated with connecting observations (e. g., water on plastic or outside of glass) with bits and pieces of patterns and models (e.g., condensation happens when vapor changes to liquid on cool surfaces; this involves molecules slowing down). Condensation on Plastic MS. CHRISTENSEN: Think back to the investigations. What is the investigation that is an example of what happens in what they’re explaining in th_at last paragraph? “As long as the air is warm enough to keep the molecules moving, water will stay in the air as water vapor. But if the air touches a cooler surface and cools down, the air cannot hold as much water vapor. The air releases the extra water in the form of droplets on the cold surface. The water vapor condenses.” Natalie, what’s an example of that? 92 NATALIE: About like — we were talking about how come the water didn’t get out of the one that had the plastic stuff on it, and probably not because the heat — the investigation said that if it’s still heated then it’ll come out, but if its cool it won’t come out. MS. CHRISTENSEN: Questions for her? JAVAN: Can’t hear her. MS. CHRISTENSEN: Questions? And I think some of the questions, Natalie, are louder. Say it louder. But who has a question for what she said? Maria? MARIA: When you said that the stuff that had the stuff over it — MS. CHRISTENSEN: You’re talking about the water over here [window] and the water under here [cabinet] that had the plastic wrap over it? MARIA: When you said that you think it didn’t come out because that it was cooler, but it didn’t- DAVID: It was warmer. MARIA: Wellkwanner because there was something over it to block it coming out. MS. CHRISTENSEN: You know what? I’m thinking about that one, and I’m remembering that the plastic wrap had water on the plastic. How do you think that water got on the plastic? Anvfiideas about that? Because I’m thinking what Natalie is saying is that there was some condensing. That it condensed in th_at container. Is that wh_at you were saying, Natalie? NATALIE: Uh—huh. MS. CHRISTENSEN: Because it went from a liquid state to a gas. How did that happen? How did that happen? Because in this paragraph, it says that it has to touch a cooler surface. What do you think that cooler surface was. Natalie? NATALIE: The plastic. MS. CHRISTENSEN: The plastic. So you thinlLthe plastic was cooler. What do other people think about that? That it was condensing onto th_at plastic. because the plastic was cooler? What do you thinkabout that? Tisha. what do you think about that? TISHA: I think the plastic was warmer. 93 MS. CHRISTENSEN: You think the — TISHA: Because the sun was shining on the plastic. MS. CHRISTENSEN: So you think it would be hotter. How many people believe that the plastic would be cooler? (over half of students raise hands). How many people believe that that plastic would be warmer? (less than half of students raise hands). Because Tisha says the sun is shining on it. Who would like to explain their ideas about that? Akira? AKIRA: I think it would be warmer. too. because since most of the like isn’t going out to the side, because its (plastic) blocking it (water), and so all of the air is in one crowded space in a little cup, and so it would be hotter. MS. CHRISTENSEN: So you think it’s hotter? What do other people think? Chanise. what do you think? CHANISE: Well, I think it contained a lot of vapor. I think it was trying to evaporate but it really couldn’t evaporate so it just had to get on the plastic. MS. CHRISTENSEN: So you think it just couldn’t get out. That’s what you’re thinking? Ben, what do you think? BEN: I think it’s kind of like —I don’t know if it’s investigation two. Kind of like investigation three. MS. CHRISTENSEN: So you’re going to change investigation. Okay. go ahead and tell us why you say that. BEN: Because you said that once they condense, like all the water condenses, if it gets too much then it starts to have water droplets. Like, condensing and then the water or the sweat on the outside, those are water droplets that were running Off. MS. CHRISTENSEN: Okay. How many people are there that thought of the other investigation? The investigation that Ben’s thinking about? The one with the glass and -— okay (lots of hands). I think what we would have to do with th_at investigation. Natalie. is take another look at it. Set it up again. and take a look at it. and see wh_at we thought about it— even if we could touch th_at plastic. 1 think that would be an interesting thing to do. So I think what we’ll do is set th_at back In). and see if we can figure th_at out. But the one that I was thinking of is the same one that Ben was thinking of. Because what happened was, we had ice cubes in here. And what happened when we had ice cubes? What did it do? What did it do to the water? Jacob? JACOB: it makes it cold. 94 MS. CHRISTENSEN: It cooled it off. So it made the temperature chilly. It made the temperature chilly. Well then, if it did that, what did it do to the glass? To the container? What happened to the container? Anybodmouch the glass that had ice cubes in it? STUDENTS: Uh-huh. Yea. Ms. Christensen initiated the discussion asking the class to make connections between what was described in the book, and their observations of the investigations (e.g., “what’s an example of that?”). The main purpose of the discussion was to collectively agree upon the appropriate example(s) or observation(s) of condensation, and to explain why. At this point the class had developed criteria for what counted as examples of condensation, based mostly on what they read in the book (i.e., condensation happens when vapor changes to liquid on cooler surfaces). Natalie first identified investigation two as an observation of condensation. In this investigation students had four dishes filled with water. Two dishes were covered with plastic, while two were left uncovered. The students placed a covered and uncovered dish on a windowsill, while the other two dishes were placed in a dark cabinet. Natalie remembered an observation that the dish in the windowsill covered with plastic had collected water on the inside of the plastic (see Figure 4). Because of the observation of water collecting, Natalie shared this investigation as an example of condensation. Natalie’s claim that investigation two was an example of condensation was an appropriate connection between observations, patterns, and models. When students questioned Natalie’s account, Ms. Christensen first verified that the class understood her correctly (i.e., “Because I’m thinking what Natalie is saying is that there was some condensing. . .Is that what you’re saying Natalie?”), which helped the class identify the main disagreement between accounts—that Natalie had identified “the plastic” as a 95 cooler surface, which was not collectively agreed upon by the class. Maria, David, Tisha, and Akira questioned whether the plastic was cooler because they had the intuitive sense that plastic in sunlight would be warmer. Since the class had not tested the temperature of the plastic in the original investigation, Ms. Christensen suggested the class redo the investigation to test whether the plastic was cool or warm (i.e., “Set it up again, and take a look at it, and see what we thought about it— even if we could touch that plastic”). This solution was one way to gather evidence to support either side of the disagreement. By making this suggestion, she communicated to the class that they did not have enough evidence to determine whether the investigation was or was not an example of condensation, so the connection made by Natalie was not agreed upon or recognized as an example of condensation until more evidence was collected to support this claim. Interestingly, Ben introduced investigation three as a possible observation of condensation. Investigation three involved observing what happened as water collected on the outside of glass of ice water. Ben’s account was not disputed because the teacher and other students agreed the investigation met the criteria for what counted as an observation of condensation—that it involved vapor changing to liquid on the cool surface of the glass. The class did not need the same evidence (i.e., temperature measurements as proof of the cooler surface) in order to agree that Ben made an appropriate connection. It is important to point out that the interactive discourse was an important medium through which to participate in validating the explanations made by Natalie and Ben. The students were able to agree and disagree with each other and position accounts within the discussion. Ms. Christensen encouraged them to question one another in this 96 process (i.e., “Questions for her?) At one point Ms. Christensen called for a class vote about how many students agreed or disagreed that the plastic was warmer. This type of discourse helped the class to identify areas of disagreement and focus their discussion on those areas. In considering what Natalie and Ben shared with the group, we can start to see how the students’ accounts of condensation are transitioning from “fog” mentioned in Fog on a Cold Glass of Water, to incorporating scientific ideas about condensation. As the class continued to modify their understanding of condensation, they were asked to make sense of other observations and connect those observations to patterns and models. In the following example, the class considered whether clouds were made of air/vapor or liquid, which initiated another discussion of condensation. It also provides an example of how student accounts transitioned from identifying clouds as objects, to thinking of clouds in terms of forms of water: Clouds and Condensation Part 1 MS. CHRISTENSEN and STUDENTS: “Water vapor is invisible.” We knew that. “Liquid water is visible.” We knew that. “Which form Of water do you think makes up clouds?” MS. CHRISTENSEN: Raise your hand if you think you know—if you have an idea about that. Remember, any time you give an answer, you always have to tell why. Why do you think that? Derek, what do you think and why do you think it? DEREK: I think the water vapor because if it was liquid water, then we would see it. It couldn’t go back- MS. CHRISTENSEN: Okay, what do other people think about that? What do you think about what Derek said? What do you think about - what is it that we see when we look at a cloud? Tisha? TISHA: I think that it’s water vapor because like when he said like if it was like liquid water, then we’d be able to see it. We’d evaporate it and when it doesn’t evaporate it comes back. And that’s how it works. 97 MS. CHRISTENSEN: What are they asking about in this question? What’s the question about, Tisha? Read the question to us. TISHA: Which form of water do you think makes up clouds? MS. CHRISTENSEN: Okay. Because I hear you and Derek saying you would be able to see it if it was liquid water. Can you see a cloud? STUDENTS: Yes. Yea. MS. CHRISTENSEN: Mmm. What do you think, Maria? MARIA: I think it’s the molecules ‘caumesterdaywhen we talked about when it gets cold the molecules slow down and they stick together. MS. CHRISTENSEN: And what do they turn into? STUDENTS: Water. MARIA: It- like molecules sticks together. it forms a cloud and then it’s actually water. MS. CHRISTENSEN: So you’re saying that you disagree with Tisha and Derek is that right? That you think it’s the liquid water? And the reason th_at you think this is what? Say that again. MARIA: Because of when it gets cold. MS. CHRISTENSEN: So you’re thinking of the idea of condensing. And what happens when water condenses? When it gets cold, it condenses and it goes to liquid water. What do otherppeople think_? (Pause) Is the fact that your hand is not up that you’re not sure? Is that- no, I know what you think, Maria. We heard it Go ahead, Ben. BEN: Is it- I think it’s kind of like — well if— I think isn’t like a cloud made of water and air? MS. CHRISTENSEN: What do other people think about that? So you’re thinking that it’s both—that it’s— BEN: It’s the water vapor and water. MS. CHRISTENSEN: So you’re thinking that it’s both? BEN: Because you can’t see water vapor, but a cloud is pretty much just air. And then- but you can still see it, which means it has to be some water. 98 MS. CHRISTENSEN: Mmmm. ALLEN: I think that makes sense. MS. CHRISTENSEN: It does make sense, doesn’t it? Go ahead, Alisa. ALISA: The idea what Bobby said- it’s a good idea because the clouds they don’t look like liquid water- they’re different but they just look like they’re made of air- they don’t look like they’re made out of anything else- MS. CHRISTENSEN: The way they look. And what does it look like they’re made out of? Do they look like a lake? STUDENTS: No. MS. CHRISTENSEN: They look different than a lake, don’t they? In this discussion the class tried to make sense of an unusual example of liquid water. Clouds are remarkably different from other forms of liquid water, such as lakes and puddles, which was pointed out when Ms. Christensen asked, “do they look like a lake?” Application of the criteria for developing explanation that connected between Observation, patterns, and models (e.g., water vapor is invisible, liquid water is visible, vapor condenses to form liquid on cool surfaces), was not as straightforward for students when talking about clouds. Maria introduced the idea that clouds were liquid water because clouds were formed through condensation (i.e., “When it gets cold the molecules slow down and they stick together... molecules sticks together, it forms a cloud and then it’s actually water”). She participated with the practice of connecting an observed phenomenon of clouds with what she knew about scientific models. The class eventually took up Maria’s account about condensation and clouds. 99 Clouds and Condensation Part 2 MS. CHRISTENSEN: Well right now, what we’re doing is trying to figupe out what it is that happens for it to condense. Jaylyn, what did you just say? JAYLYN: It happens on a cold surface. MS. CHRISTENSEN: On a cold surface. So we know that it has to cool down, doesn’t it? For water to condense, it has to cool down. Now, what do we know about the higher we go? What do we know? Natalie? NATALIE: The thinner the air is. MS. CHRISTENSEN: The thinner the air is—and what else? What else? Derek? DEREK: The thinner it gets. MS. CHRISTENSEN: The thinner the air is — yeah, we talked about the thin air. Go ahead? DEREK: The colder it gets. MS. CHRISTENSEN: The colder it gets. Whoa—does that make sense? STUDENTS: _Ypay MS. CHRISTENSEN: This water vapor is going to go up — it’s going to — it’s all around us; it’s going to be up, it’s going to be down, it’s going to be all around us. And up there, it’s going to get colder. So what Ben said was it cools off and it condenses - remind me, Ben, what it was I said. BEN: Water vapor condenses — when water evaporates, the water vapor goes up and then it condenses into clouds. MS. CHRISTENSEN: And then it condenses into clouds. Does that make sense? STUDENTS: Yes. ALISA: Simon says he doesn’t get it. MS. CHRISTENSEN: You don’t get it Simon? Can you ask a question that helps us help you? Can you tell us what part of it — because it is a cycle. It’s very complicated. What part is it that you have questions about? SIMON: Well, how are they [clouds] made of water? 100 MS. CHRISTENSEN: Thank you for bringing that up, Simon—for letting us know. Asking questions is the way that you’re going to learn about it. So let’s do this, Simon, you hold on to your question. And that might even be something that you want to do research on. But hold on to your question and we’ll read through this and we’ll write a summary and then see. Okay? See if it helps you. All right. Everyone needs to be looking at their book. In the first line of this example Ms. Christensen focused the discussion on establishing an agreed upon criteria for determining instances of observations (“what we’re doing is trying to figure out what it is that happens for it to condense”). In some sense she wanted the rest of the group to participate as Maria had done in the previous example—by connecting observations, patterns, and models using the account of condensation happening when gas turned to liquid on cold surfaces. Clouds represented a peculiar challenge because the class had to establish that temperatures are colder at high altitudes and “particles” in the atmosphere are “cooler surfaces” (note that they do not talk about these particles, or CCN’s, in this example). Clouds did not fit neatly into students’ experiential base about liquid water, and therefore many students thought clouds were water vapor, even though they conceded that vapor was invisible. Maria seemed to readily adopt that clouds were liquid because they were an example of condensation (i.e., her account was transitioning to include scientific knowledge and discourse), while the remainder of the class needed additional prompting to make this connection. Again, the challenge was establishing the “cooler surface” required for clouds to form, as was the case with condensation on plastic. In the end, several students, including Simon, remained confused because he was not able to reconcile his observation of clouds with what he had learned about condensation. 101 The previous examples about water collecting on a glass, on plastic, and as clouds were about the class developing a shared understanding of condensation, and shared way to develop explanations that connected observations to patterns and models. Experience and evidence became important factors in the connections and explanations developed. Evidence also determined whether the class needed to gather more evidence in order to proceed with collectively validation (e.g., testing temperature of plastic to verify whether it was a cooler surface). The role of the teacher was to help students identify key disagreements and help them decide if they had enough evidence to support connections being made. Meta-level Discussion about Models and Consensus A substantial portion of the unit on water cycling in Ms. Christensen’s class was devoted to the question of “What drives the water cycle?” Early in the unit the class read a scientific model of water cycling from their books. At that time, Ms. Christensen and the student focused on using the model to make sense of evaporation and condensation. The model from the book was: Evaporation and condensation in earth’s atmosphere depends on the heat energy of the sun. The sun heats up the water on the surface of lakes, rivers, oceans and even puddles. Water evaporates from all these sources adding water vapor to the air. When the sun heats earth unevenly, either from place to place, or from day to night, warm air cools and the water vapor condenses back to liquid water. This movement of water from the surface of the air, surface of the earth to the air and back to the surface is called the water cycle. What do you think drives the water cycle? Yet, the question asked of students—what drives the water cycle—required students to think critically about the model. The students responded in several ways to the question. After two discussions devoted completely to the topic, the students were still 102 divided into three main groups—those that believed the sun or heat energy was the driving force, those that believed water was the driving force, and those that believed several things were driving the water cycle. The students engaged in debating their ideas, with “leaders” from each group coming to the front of the room, drawing their ideas on the board, explaining their position to the class, and then taking questions from peers. Even after participating in these rich discussions the students still disagreed. In the following transcript Ms. Christensen and her students considered whether they needed to come to consensus about “what drives the water cycle”: Consensus about Water Cycling MS. CHRISTENSEN: “Evaporation and condensation in earth’s atmosphere depends on the heat energy of the sun. The sun heats up the water on the surface of lakes, rivers, oceans and even puddles. Water evaporates from all these sources adding water vapor to the air. When the sun heats earth unevenly, either from place to place, or from day to night, warm air cools and the water vapor condenses back to liquid water. This movement of water from the surface of the air, surface of the earth to the air and back to the surface is called the water cycle. What do you think drives the water cycle?” And I thought that paragraph made it pretty clear to me just what drove the water cycle. Now after having the discussion that we’ve had, I see that it’s much more complicated than that and maybe the question is not nearly as important as the discussion that you’ve 1m about it. Do we need to come up with what drives the water cycle? Do you think we need to come to a consensus on that? STUDENTS: Yeah. (some ‘no’s). MS. CHRISTENSEN: I’m hearing yeses and nos. Why do you say yes? Who has an idea? Why do you say yes we need to come to a consensus? Lesley? LESLEY: Maybe that we need to know more about the water so we have a clearer whatever about the water cycle. MS. CHRISTENSEN: So you’re saying that we do need to, because what it may mean is that we need to study the water cycle more. We need to get more information on it? Okay. Ben? BEN: I think we need to come to a consensus on Kamal’s question. Not maybe completely what drives the water cycles, but what’s most important? Like, what is 103 the whole thing about? What like really is the most important, not what drives the water cycle, but what is the most important- most important part of the water cycle? Is the sun the most imponant part? Does everything depend on that? Does everything depend on the air? Does everything depend on the water? MS. CHRISTENSEN: And how do you suggest we do that Ben? BEN: Like, you just- we pretty much just answered that question right here in this paragraph. It depends on the heat energy of the sun to get evaporation of the puddles or lakes. And without that- that’s what everything depends on. MS. CHRISTENSEN: Kamal. does this paragraph satisfy you? KAMAL: I give up- it’s just- MS. CHRISTENSEN: No, but I don’t want you to give up. What I want you to do is think about your ideas. Did reading this paragraph satisfy you or not? KAMAL: Kind of. MS. CHRISTENSEN: Kind of? But I’m seeing you shake your head no. It didn’t, did it? KAMAL: Kind of. MS. CHRISTENSEN: Kind of. Okay. Kamal, kind of. (students laugh). What does “kind of” mean? KAMAL: Like, when you understand- kind of think- I kind of think both the sun and water. MS. CHRISTENSEN: I think we have a very good understanding of what the water cycle is because of the way you’ve talked about it. The question is not do you have a good understanding of it, although I agree with Lesley you can always have more information that might give you a clearer understanding. KAMAL: I think it’s kind of the sun and the water. The book- well I think that air is kind of lower. MS. CHRISTENSEN: So, you know what? I’m looking back at my notes. And that’s a move for you because before you said water drives it. So now you’re willing to say water plus the sun? KAMAL: It’s like this is the water and the sun. this is the air (student demonstrates levels of importance using his hands). cause the air is not that 1m portant. 104 MS. CHRISTENSEN: So you’re thinking the air is not as important? Okay. Natalie? What do you think? Ms. Christensen engaged the class in discussing consensus by asking an open- ended, authentic question. She was genuinely interested in how the students would feel if they did not find closure on the topic. She gave authority to the students to decide what the next step would be (e.g., study more; debate more; leave it alone). For instance, she asked Ben, “How do you suggest we do that?” By engaging students in dialogue around the question about consensus she communicated that she valued what they thought. She also communicated that it was important for the class to step back and reevaluate their goals and whether or not consensus would bring them closer to those goals (e.g., “I think we have a very good understanding of what the water cycle is because of the way you’ve talked about it. The question is not do you have a good understanding of it.”). The exchange between Ms. Christensen and Kamal further demonstrated that true dialogue was critical for getting students’ understanding of models on the table. Ms. Christensen refused to allow Kamal’s “kind of” response to be sufficient. By communicating that she valued what he thought (e.g., “No, but I don’t want you to give up. What I want you to do is think about your ideas. Did reading this paragraph satisfy you or not?”), she was eventually successful at getting him to talk about his account in relation to the model from the book. The discussion that occurred in this example, and throughout the entire observation, was about explaining water cycling using different models. The passage from the book, for example, was a model representing a scientific account of water cycling that identified heat energy as critical to water cycling processes. Kamal’s original 105 account did not identify “the sun” or “heat energy”, so his model for water cycling did not include a “driving force”. A third model that was used during this observation was an analogy of a car to explain water cycling—that is, the class used a car to represent water cycling and tried to identify “the driver” of the car and how that explained the “driver” of the water cycle. Through discussing, debating, and evaluating these models, the class was able to identify instances where the models were and were not helpful, thus participating with practices that looked critically at the explanatory power of models. In the example above, Kamal was challenged to reconcile his model (that did not include heat energy) with what was explained in the book when Ms. Christensen asked him, “Does this paragraph satisfy you?” Kamal struggled to answer partly because he realized his model was insufficient, but also because he agreed with the model read in the book (“I think it’s kind of the sun and the water. The book- well I think that air is kind of lower”). Kamal showed he was trying to adjust and revise his ideas to reconcile them with the book, but that he was not completely convinced about the importance of the sun or heat energy. Kamal had not adOpted a model of water cycling that recognized the role of heat energy is making molecules evaporate and condense, which indicated he struggled to make connections between observations, patterns, and models. Ben, on the other hand, made these connections. He focused his argument that sun/heat energy was the driving force of water cycling because of its importance in getting water to evaporate. For this reason, Ben more readily accepted the model from the book (i.e., “We pretty much just answered that question right here in this paragraph. It depends on the heat energy of the sun to get evaporation of the puddles or lakes. And without that- that’s what everything depends on.”). 106 Metaphors and analogies in science are unique types of models that try to bridge naive accounts with scientific accounts by describing complex ideas in ways more familiar to students (Gallas, 1995). Ms. Christensen and Ms. Hocking had to coordinate a complicated set of models that were on the floor, including scientific models, metaphors, analogies, and narratives. In some ways they allowed all to stay on the table for consideration, but were trying to help students see the explanatory power of scientific models, and the limitations of informal models. Thus, both teachers privileged the scientific models. The privileging of scientific models was necessary for encouraging students to take up those models in terms of accounts and discourse. It was also necessary because informal models, such as metaphors and analogies, are helpful to a limited degree. In the following transcript (which is composed of three excerpts from a larger discussion), Ms. Christensen’s class considered the utility of a “car analogy” to explain water cycling, and to identify a “driver” in the cycle: What Drives a Car? MS. CHRISTENSEN: Okay. Well, let me ask you this. Thank you Natalie. We have Kamal’s idea of what we need. We have Natalie’s idea, all three. Here’s what I would like to toss out to you. We have (teacher draws car on board), what’s that? STUDENTS: A car. MS. CHRISTENSEN: Who is driving the car? (lots of student chatter). Who drives the car? STUDENTS: You. People. MS. CHRISTENSEN: A person. We all agree thpt it’shaymrson that drives the car. right? So is this kind of like that? Kind of like Natalie’s ideas and kind of like Kamal’s ideas? Are these three ideas kind of the same? And that’s when an analogy is helpful to you. when that happens. Allen. what do you think? 107 ALLEN: Okay, I think everything on the movie that makes up weather, makes up that water cycle, land, air and sun- the heat. MS. CHRISTENSEN: So Kamal has got all three of those parts. Natalie’s got all of those parts. How does this kind of work to go along with this or to explain that? Does it? ALLEN: Yeah. MS. CHRISTENSEN: Is it helpful to you? ALLEN: Yeah, because you need certain things to drive a car. Like you couldn’t drive a car without a steering wheel and um- MS. CHRISTENSEN: So you’re agreeing with what Natalie and Kamal have drawn over here. How does that analogy of the car and who drives the car work for you. Thomas? Can you work those two twigs together? And they fit together at all? THOMAS: Yeah. If there was no car, it’s like the water; if there was no car you could not have a car at all, and if there was no water- (pause) MS. CHRISTENSEN: So what would that be? (pause). Now let’s say we’ve all agreed that it’s the driver who drives the car. So you’re saying the car is not the driver, it’s not what drives the car. It’s being driven. So the car is not what’s driving the car. It’s the driver. How does that fit in with Kamal’s ideas and Natalie’s ideas? How do those fit together? Do they fit together? Whpt do you think, do they fit together? Marisha. do you think they fit together? (PauseLI-Iow many people think they do fit together? Raise your hand if you think they do fit together. (Lots of hands). How do they fit together? (pausQ. Hard to put in to words, isn’t it? BEN: But I also think that the water is pretty much the car, because if you don’t have a car, you can’t drive it. So without water, you don’t have a water cycle. The sun- have you ever heard of like maybe- My mom used to tell my dad when my sister was taking her driver’s test, when she was there the guy would say “Backseat driving” when my dad was in the backseat, so I’m thinking maybe the sun is driving it and then the wind is like backseat driving because it’s also part of it. MS. CHRISTENSEN: Yeah, it kind of makes sense. Janice, I saw your hand go up. What did you want to say? 108 JANICE: I was going to say people because you need to put your feet on the pedal, or else the car is not going to go. MS. CHRISTENSEN: So how does that relate to the water cycle? How doesyour foot on the pedal relate to the water cycle? JANICE: The engine gets the cars to go, and without the engine, the car’s not going to go. MS. CHRISTENSEN: Okay, so we get the car now. I understand what you mean about the car. How does that relate to the water cycle? What is the water cycle? Like, the engine? What is the water cycle, the foot on the gas pedal? In the water cycle. what is thgperson (teacher mimics quotations when she says person)? Like this [quotes] means not really. but kind of. Or similar to? JANICE: The sun. MS. CHRISTENSEN: The sun? And why do you say that? JANICE: Because the sun- the person- MS. CHRISTENSEN: Diego, that’s really distracting. You need to make sure you’re being a good listener. [00:58:40] JONDASHAE: Never mind. MS. CHRISTENSEN: Oh no, tell us. JANICE: The person or the sun gives heat to the car. The person does. MS. CHRISTENSEN: The person is like the heat giver. And so that’s why you say the sun, because it’s the heat giver. To some degree the car analogy was beneficial to students. It helped Ben develop an account that made connections to his experiences outside of school (e.g., backseat driving). It helped Janice identify that the sun was like the person pressing the pedal in a car. While these students were able to use the analogy, there were many students that could not agree on which parts of the car represented parts of the water cycle. An analogy such as this helped focus some students on salient features that “fit together” between the 109 analogy and the developing scientific model for water cycling (e.g., the driver). Unfortunately it also caused some students to focus on “mapping” parts of the car onto water cycling. While the mapping likely helped on some level, it also caused students to related the car and water cycle in ways that detened them from the primary focus of the activity—to use the “driver” of the car to identify the “driver” of the water cycle. Ms. Christensen and Ms. Hocking faced the challenge Of balancing the different types of models and helping students realize when models were helpful and when they were limiting. Ms. Christensen pointed out during her member checking that a challenge for her was deciding how and when to use different models and then making sense of how those models benefit students differently. At the age level in which these two teachers taught, the task became even more daunting because oftentimes the explanatory power of scientific models was beyond the scope of the course, and so identifying limitations to informal models, such as narrative, metaphors, and analogies, was not straightforward for their students. Ms. Christensen explained during her member check that she realized she was just “one layer” in her students’ education. She understood some students needed more time and more thoughtful discussions before they could make sense of scientific models, and that she had to be satisfied if some students continued to rely on narratives, analogies, and metaphors as opposed to scientific models. At best, she hoped she was laying a foundation so that her students could make sense of scientific models more easily in the future. 110 SUMMARY AND DISCUSSION OF EMERGING THEORY Figure 5 summarizes the emerging theory, and howl found dialogic discourse to support students’ participation in science. The dialogic discourse brought out diversity in accounts, which allowed for teachers to capitalized on students’ experiences and to use students’ vernacular language and emerging accounts as starting points for discussions. By linking and positioning accounts, the class identified sources of tension and conflict, which allowed for space to reconcile discourse and accounts that differed. The interactive discourse made possible the establishment of norms for collective validation of observations and explanations. The collective validation process involved the class participating with practices and developing agreed upon criteria of what counted as normative participation with those practices. The practice of making observations was guided by standards for precision, accuracy, and reliability, so that normative ways of participating depended on these standards. The practice of connecting observation, patterns, and models was guided by norms for what were appropriate ways to explain those connections. Evidence became a critical tool for participation in validating observations and explanations. Evidence was used to verify the accuracy of observations and it was used to determine whether the class had enough justification to support explanations that made connections between observations, patterns, and models. The teachers faced challenges when making decisions about how to handle the tension that occurred between emerging and scientific accounts. They had to balance their own voice and the authoritative discourse of science with vernacular discourse of students. They had to teach their students to be metacognitively aware of language and words. They had to make progress toward their own curricular goals (and mandated lll requirements) while simultaneously encouraging students to ask questions and contribute new ideas. Furthermore, they acknowledged that talking alone was not sufficient, and that writing was important to supporting students to articulate their ideas and acquire scientific discourse. They also had to help students make sense of explanatory power of models, and at times the teacher and students discussed the process of coming to consensus and when this was necessary. It became apparent during data collection and analysis that the type of talk supported in these classrooms changed depending on the teacher’s goals for the lessons. At times, the teacher wanted to elicit diverse accounts, while at other times, the teacher encouraged the class to converge on a smaller set of accounts, or single account. Burbules (1993) suggested that one way classroom discourse can be viewed is in terms of divergence and convergence. This study corroborates the critical role of convergence and divergence in classroom talk, and suggests that teachers’ pedagogical purposes influence how discussions play out in terms of what happens to the accounts that are shared with the group. Although not analyzed in this study, I suggest the purpose of discussion, and whether discussion emphasizes convergence or divergence is strongly related to teachers’ instructional models and sequencing of classroom activities. For example, teachers attempting to elicit prior knowledge early in a science unit, likely support divergent classroom discussion. Teacher with little interest in prior knowledge or misconceptions of students may utilize divergent discussions rarely. Furthermore, teachers interested in coming to agreement on scientific explanations, especially toward the end of study within a unit, will likely emphasize convergence rather than divergence. The discussions used in the results section also showed a great deal of teacher control, where students filtered their contributions through the teacher. While superficially this may appear to be similar to recitation (i.e., the back-and-forth exchanges between teacher and students), an important finding from this study was that even in situations in which teachers remain greatly involved in the dialogue, the discussion approached the dialogic end of the spectrum between monologue and dialogue (Burbules & Bruce, 2001). The discussions could be labeled “teacher-controlled dialogue” since teachers retained power over the direction, pace, and content of discussions. Yet, even though the students filtered comments through the teachers, the teachers tried to minimize their voice in the discussions (e.g., “What do you think?”; “Anything else to add?”). Although not shared in the results, there were instances in which students led the classroom discussions, first sharing their ideas, and then calling on and answering questions from students. There were also instances where students talked directly to one another during discussion without filtering their comments through the teachers. The student-led discussions, however, occurred less often compared to those in which the teacher played a major role. One of the challenges for the teachers, which Ms. Christensen pointed out during her description of the “ideal conversation”, was getting students to engage in back-and-forth discussion, where the conversation was handed over to students. Too often, students are well practiced in talking to the teacher, rather than their peers, during classroom discussions. Interestingly, during member checks with the teachers, it was mentioned that the teachers’ content knowledge sometimes influenced the amount of control they wanted during discussions. Occasionally teachers felt the need for more control when their 113 content knowledge was lacking. There were also times the teachers allowed for divergence, instead of convergence, when they were “stalling” until they understood the content themselves, or could make sense of the students’ emerging accounts. 114 CONCLUSIONS AND IMPLICATIONS Teaching science content is unique in that much of learning could potentially occur through interaction with the material world (e.g., experimentation, investigations through hands-on activities) in addition to interaction in a social community. Both are important, but the latter is especially critical for teaching students ways of thinking and communicating that are valued in science. Classrooms focused primarily on hands-on activities have been observed to leave little instructional time for discussion, although students show and report higher levels of engagement when discussions are used (Blumenfeld, Puro, & Mergendoller, 1992; Mohan et al., in revision). While the teachers in Mohan et al. (in revision) study, incorporated hands-on activities in their instruction, they used these judiciously. These teachers understood that hands-on investigations were only as effective as the discussions that accompanied them. Productive and rich discussions are important for student participation in science. This study contributes to the existing literature on dialogism in classrooms by documenting examples of how this type of discourse is encouraged by teachers during discussions. It provides a framework for identifying talk that is more dialogic in terms of discussion norms and discourse moves that indicate the students and teacher are engaged in heteroglossic, linked, and dynamic talk (Bakhtin, 1981). The framework points to two broad norms that guided the sharing of accounts during discussions—seeking diversity and linking accounts. The framework corroborated previous findings about dialogism, such as Nystrand’s work (1997) on authentic teacher questions, student questions, and uptake, and added to this literature by expanding the framework to include moves that 115 elicited evaluation, issued open invitations, assigned authorship, and took up new ideas from students. More importantly, this study linked dialogic discourse to what was afforded in terms of participation with content. When teaching science, teachers face the challenge of balancing authoritative discourse of science, while capitalizing upon (and honoring) what students bring to the class. This involves careful negotiation between discourses, and helping students to take on scientific discourse and accounts that offer more explanatory power, when their vernacular discourse is limited. Importantly, the process of negotiating discourses involves collective validation of accounts, including validation of observations and explanations. Very few teachers naturally support students in participating with science practices or encourage collective validation, particularly through the use of open forums, such as discussions (Lemke, 1990; Wells & Arauz, 2006). In the last decade, a great deal of effort has been made to design curricula to support teachers and students in using practices, particularly ones related to theory-building and skeptical review (Crawford et al., 2000; Herrenkohl et al., 1999; Henenkohl & Guerra, 1998; Hogan & Corey, 2001; Rosebury, Warren, & Conant, 1992) and argumentation skills (Crawford et al., 2000; Osborne et al., 2004; Warren et al., 1992). Unfortunately, students may be reluctant to participate with such practices, especially ones involving collective, or group, activities, since they have limited experience outside of individual tasks for individual evaluation (Hogan & Corey, 2001) or because group tasks may violate or conflict with students’ sense of individual ownership (Calabrese Barton, 2007). Interventions designed to support reforrn-based teaching can be even more problematic if the intervention occurs in 116 short time frame, which risks the chance of the teacher and/or students not buying into the reformed-based methods. On the other hand, documenting discussions in the classrooms of exemplary teachers opened a window to what was made possible for science participation during discussions without the intervention of specially designed curricula (although the teachers did use high quality materials in their classroom). Exemplary teachers have a remarkable grasp of what their students bring to the classroom and a repertoire of tools for engaging students in serious and cognitively demanding tasks (e.g., Blumenfeld et al., 1992; Mohan et al., in revision). Ms. Christensen and Ms. Hocking were able to support students in being observant and analytical, and they encouraged their students to collectively evaluate the observations and explanations that held the floor during discussions—a daunting task for even the most skilled teachers. The complexity of this type of instruction is intimidating, yet these two teachers recognized that such complexity is necessary for students to make substantial progress toward more sophisticated scientific understanding. This study also adds to the existing literature of discourse in science by focusing on the role of the teacher in supporting negotiation of discourses. Numerous studies of discourse in science have focused on how students learn to negotiate discourse, with less attention to the means in which the teacher supports this process (e.g., Calabrese Barton et al., 2008; Moje, 2000; Moje et al., 2004). This study, on the other hand, attempted to provide insight what the teacher was doing during discussions and how they supported students in adopting scientific discourse and accounts. 117 Limitations and Directions for Future Research While this study sheds light on what is possible during science discussions, there are several important limitations to this work. These limitations involve ways to assess the learning that occurred during discussions, the relationship between individual and collective participation, the generalizeability of results, and teacher professional development and experience. Assessing Learning in Discussions One of the main limitations of this research was that there was no assessment of student learning as a result of participating in these discussions. Accountability, assessment, and evidence-based pedagogical practice are hugely important in educational research today. While the study provided evidence that discussions were beneficial for student participation with practices and norms, there were no formal or consistent assessments of what students learned about science content. There were no me and post- assessments and only limited attention to student work beyond what occurred during discussions. This is unfortunate because few conclusions can be made about how dialogic discussions supported learning, which is key for getting both researchers and educators to pay attention to the critical role discussions play in instruction. Other work (e.g., Nystrand, 1997) suggests that discussions are beneficial for learning English content, so the same may be true in science. Future research needs to establish ways to measure student learning of science content during discussions, and how discussions can be analyzed both separately and in combination with other activities in the classroom. Interestingly, Ms. Christensen pointed out during her member check, that assessing student learning during discussion was especially difficult because she had not 118 found a successful way for making sense of the learning that occurred through actively listening. She had a sense that many of her students were participating during discussions, even when those chose not to speak, and suggested that assessing Ieaming and participation during discussion consider not only the speakers, but what is involved in actively listening, and how listening influences the transformation of accounts. Individual and Collective Participation. A second limitation, which is related to the first, is that this study did not focus on consistently differentiating between the participation of individuals and that of the group. Although dialogic discussions provided more opportunities for more students to participation, I conducted no analysis that looked at the access of individual students and how their participation evolved in relation to what occurred in the group. While I make some claims about the transformation of individual student accounts during discussions, further analyses need to be conducted to link transformation of those accounts to the transformation of the collective knowledge of the group. One possibility for conducting such analyses might involve a framework like Cobb & Yackel (1996) in which they integrate social and psychological perspectives. In this framework they coordinate individual beliefs about roles, content, and activity with communal norms and practices, so that one could potentially analyze, for example, the classroom mathematical practices in relationship to an individual’s mathematical conceptions and activity. The analyses would involve collecting data regarding group dynamics, as well as assessments of individuals (either written or interview). Although this study did not collect the necessary data to complete this type of analysis, future studies on individual and collective 119 participation are potentially promising, especially for identifying how students may benefit from discussions in different ways. Generalizeabilty This study documented the discussions in two science classrooms at the upper elementary/middle school grades. While the small sample size allowed for rich description, it limited the degree to which these results can be generalized, especially when thinking about younger and older populations of students. As work on dialogic discussions continues, researchers need to document discussions in classrooms at every grade level, and in a variety of content areas. While attempts were made in this study to connect the emerging findings to current research, the process of making these connections was limited by the very fact that few empirical studies of dialogism exist in the literature, especially in science. The emerging theory from this study will need to be re-examined as other studies are conducted. More research on discussions in science should be conducted before such a theory is readily generalizeability to a wider population of classrooms in science. Important to the issue of generalizeability is not only whether the emerging theory applies to different classrooms and age groups, but also to teachers and their classroom communities year-to-year. Both Ms. Christensen and Ms. Hocking commented during their member checks that they wondered if my results would be different had I studied a different year in their classrooms. The did not question the emerging categories described in the results, but rather whether discussions might have included more or less diversity, and more or less positioning, depending on the group of students in their classroom that year. For example, Ms. Christensen explained that during the year I observed her class, 120 students more easily engaged in positioning their accounts, while the following year this did not come as readily to the group. Rather than focusing on re-examination of the emerging theory in other classrooms, and at other age levels, it may also be important to re-examine the theory in the same classroom across several groups of students. Teacher Experience and Professional Development As described briefly in the participant section, both teachers in this study had been closely connected to the local university for years. They had been involved in numerous professional development experiences, as was true the year this data was collected. One question that was not answered in this study was how they learned to orchestrate good discussions, and what occurred in their professional past that directed them toward using rich discussions? Answering these questions is important not only to shed light on the professional path and trajectory for developing teacher expertise, but it also has important implications for the training of in-service and pre-service teachers. Other researchers (Mohan, Lundeberg, & Reffitt, 2008; Pressley, 2003) have suggested that expert teachers are not bom, but rather develop through years of targeted professional development. Ms. Hocking and Ms. Christensen certainly had years of professional development that helped them to become the reflective teachers they were during this study. Future studies should look at expert teachers and how they have developed within the profession. Furthermore, understanding the teaching of exemplary teachers has potential to impact the training of other teachers. Capturing the productive discussions that occurred in these classrooms could potentially lead to the development of case studies used with in-service and pre-service teachers, who need a glimpse into an expert teacher’s classrooms. For science teachers in particular, providing examples of how exemplary 121 teachers successfully handled the uncertainty that came with dialogic discussions (and inquiry teaching in general), and how they successfully dealt with naive conceptions, may give other teachers confidence to use similar practices in their own classrooms. Conclusion In traditional science classrooms, in which the authoritative discourse of science dominates, scientific models, as well as emerging accounts that students bring to the classroom, are not challenged. Student emerging accounts may become more elaborate, adopting bits and pieces of scientific models. Students may be successful in reciting their accounts in narrow contexts, but since they failed to reconcile their vernacular discourse with scientific discourse, application of their accounts is limited, especially when applied outside of the classroom. Students need Opportunities to participate in rich discussions that support them in challenging both emerging and scientific accounts, not only as a way of reconciling discourses, but also as a way of thinking critically about the explanatory power afforded by scientific models that is not made possible with informal models. This type of participation also engages students in the collective activities that are so central to becoming legitimate members of the science community. 122 Table l Participant demographics Teacher Grade level Experience Setting Christensen Grade 5 17 years Urban; 30 students Hocking Grade 6 17 years Suburban; 21 students 123 Table 2 Relationship of Dialogic Indicators to Discussion Norms Norms of Discussions Dialogic Indicators Classroom example Diversity of Accounts Multiple A uthentic teacher accounts are questions that elicit elicted numerous, diverse accounts Open Invitation Student Value and Respect accounts are valued T: Anybody have any more predictions about what you think is going to be happening? T: Anything else? Anything else to add? T: Okay, I’m seeing the same hands over and over again. If you haven’t volunteered, if you haven’t offered anything to the discussion, you need to get your hand up. T: Yell it out. Yell it out. What you have to say is valuable. T: What do you mean by evaporation? 124 Table 2 continued A uthentic student question New Student Idea T: I’m having a problem when I hear people laugh. When a question is asked, if you laugh, it’s going to make people feel like their ideas are not as valid as other people’s ideas. . .this is one of the procedures that we follow- this is one of our guidelines. And that is that you are commenting on the ideas, talking about the ideas, asking questions to help you understand. Not laughing at people. Not laughing at their ideas. S: Since tears are like a salty liquid, why do your eyes not sting when you cry? But then when you go in the ocean, it stings? S: [introduces rollercoaster comparison that is taken up]. I think when it goes down like a roller coaster/ if you don’t have a steep I25 Table 2 continued enough hill to go down/ then you don’t have enough time to even go over like the ball- T: Yeah/ that’s interesting because roller coasters are built differently now than they used to be. The old- fashioned wooden roller coaster/ what was the force that powered it? Linking Accounts Accounts are authored A uthorship and Referencing T: But Jason brought up a very important point about support/ about support forces/ right? Would you say that the support force on this side/ is different from the support force on that side? S: Like when he said that the one with the plastic on it, the water — was on top of the plastic- it was there because it tried to evaporate, but it just basically was stopped inside. 126 Table 2 continued Accounts are positioned Uptake that aligns Uptake that creates opposition T: So that you’re saying - basically, you’re saying the same thing that Thomas said. They’re all up in the air. The water molecules that were in the cup now are up in the air- as water. S: I agree with Allen. Because water evaporates into the clouds. And then it precipitates and then does it all over again ‘til the water cycles. T: So you’re saying that you disagree with Gina and David, is that right? That you think it’s the liquid water? And the reason that you think this is what? Say that again. S: Because lots of people said the sun, but if the sun doesn’t have anything to evaporate, then like water- nothing could evaporate, so what’s the point. 127 Table 2 continued Questions that elicit T: What do you think about what evaluations Sarah said? Meta-talk Responsibilities Speaking T: Talk so they can hear you. That’s during Responsibilities your responsibility discussions Listening T: And remember, you want to make Responsibilities sure that you are listening to everyone and that you are asking questions if you don’t understand what they’re saying. Commenting on ideas, not on people 128 Models Theories Learning Using knowledge: r om , . Application expenence: Patterns in data: Laws, inquiry generalizations, graphs, tables Observations, measurements, data using attribute-value descriptions Figure l. Experience—Pattems—Explanations (EPE) model (Anderson, 2003) Chris’ Observation (g) Light box Baby Food White Brian’s Observation i Light box Baby Food Jar White Paper Sam’s Observation Light box Baby Food Jar White Paper Figure 2. Inaccurate or incomplete observations of light beams traveling through a curved jar 130 Accurate Observation > Light box Baby Food Jar White Paper Observation covering one beam of light Light box Baby Food Jar White Paper Figure 3. Accurate observation of two beams of light traveling through a curved jar and what is observed when one beam is covered. I31 v A 4 .0. D V A Water collecting on plastic ‘V V V Water in bottom of dish Figure 4. Diagram of water collecting on the inside of the plastic-covered water dish 132 Dialogic Discourse Diversity of Accounts 0 Multiple accounts are elicited o Authentic questions 0 Open Invitation ' Student accounts are valued 0 Value and respect 0 Student questions taken up 0 New student ideas taken up Linking Accounts ' Accounts are authored o Authorship/referencing 0 Accounts are positioned o Uptake that aligns o Uptake that opposes 0 Questions to evaluate I What it affords: ° Experiential base of students 0 Vernacular language and naive accounts What it affords: ' Connecting OPM’s using evidence 0 Reconciling vernacular and scientific discourses (and naive and scientific accounts) 0 Shared accounts and meaning of words Practices and Norms: 0 Validation of accounts 0 Validation of observations 0 Validation of explanations o Explanatory power of models 0 Consensus Figure 5. Relationship between dialogism and socioscientific norms and practice. 133 CODES APPENDIX A: CODING BOOK Explanation and Definition Classroom 1 Example Classroom 2 Example Multiple accounts are elicited (MI)- discourse moves in that called for more than one idea to be shared and considered ATQ TE-OI Authentic Teacher Question- questions asked by the teacher that does not have a rigid, pro-specified answer or right/wrong answer pertaining to content. Open Invitation- Teacher encourages all students to participate with an open invitation (sometimes with handraising, sometimes without) T: Anybody have any T: This is your big more predictions about question - big, big what you think is going to be happening? T: Okay, I’m seeing the same hands over and over again. If you haven’t volunteered, if you haven’t offered anything to the discussion, you need to get your hand up. question: why does the image appear to be behind the minor? And is this true with plane mirrors? Is your image in the minor the same as the image behind these red things? Does it work that way? T: You don’t have to raise your hand/ just talk. Student accounts are valued (WW)- indication that the teacher values student knowledge and student ideas. Students know their ideas are consequential and that they are expected to help others understand. VM-RE Value and Respect- teacher expresses value for students, genuinely cares about what they have to say, and encourages others to be respectful. T: Yell it out. Yell it out. What you have to say is valuable. T: I want to refocus our discussion and hold on to the ideas. I wrote them down so we wouldn’t lose those ideas. 134 T: Okay/ would you guys listen to Derek. I want Dereld I want you to explain this to them. Would you explain it to them? ASQ Authentic Student Question-questions asked by students that pertains to the content, and asked out of curiosity, confusion, interest, etc. NSI New Student Idea- An idea or comment expressed by a student that shifts the direction of the discussion T: I’m having a problem when I hear people laugh. When a question is asked, if you laugh, it’s going to make people feel like their ideas are not as valid as other people’s ideas. S: Well I first have a question. If- um— left something out, why don’t the waters from the ocean evaporate? Maria's connected boiling water to cooking experiences which was taken up by the group into a lengthy discussion of observation while cooking. S: Since tears are like a salty liquid, why do your eyes not sting when you cry? But then when you go in the ocean, it stings? S: Um/ I think when it goes down like a roller coaster/ if you don’t have a steep enough hill to go down/ then you don’t have enough time to even go over like the ball- Accounts are authored (A U)- linking accounts involves assigning authorship to the accounts under consideration TU-R or Assigning author or SU-R referencing- when the teacher or student revoices an idea, giving authorship of an idea to a member in the group S: Like when he said that the one with the plastic on it, the water — was on tOp of the plastic- it was there because it tried to evaporate, but it just basically was stopped inside. 135 T: But Derek brought up a very important point about support/ about support forces/ right? Would you say that the support force on this side/ is different from the support force on that side? T: Let me ask you to quote your answer, Maria I want to give everyone a chance to think about this. Maria said that she thought -— see what you think. She thinks that the water level went down because — because the steam was coming up out of the cup. And so the water was coming up out of the cup. What do you think about that? T: Somebody said something about friction being necessary to hold it/ in the loop. Chris: That was Ellen. Ellen: Because it’s like/ see you know how it makes this noise/ this helps to understand because if it’s/ it’s like staying on here and if/ it’s like too smooth/ even if you have this little circle or something/ then you’d have/ like a skateboard ramp. Accounts are positioned (POS)- linking account involves positioning accounts by either disagreeing or agreeing with accounts on the floor and by evaluating accounts. TU-AL or SU-AL Uptake that aligns ideas- when the teacher or students specifically align two or more ideas or two or more people as a way of building agreement (may include revoicing moves) T: So that you’re saying -— basically, you’re saying the same thing that Mario said. They’re all up in the air. The water molecules that were in the cup now are up in the air- as water. T: Okay. Because I hear you and Corey saying you would be able to see it if it was liquid water. Can you see a cloud? S: I agree with Allen. Because water evaporates into the clouds. And then it precipitates and then does it all over again ‘til the water cycles. 136 T: SO there’s some sort of energy transfer or something going on? Okay, now Tim, you said the same thing. You thought that light energy that doesn’t go all the way through something might change to/ might change to heat energy. T: What do you think is going to happen? Think about what happened when Beth and Jason and I were sort of marching with our meter stick? What happened? TU-O or SU-O GEQ Uptake that opposes- when the teacher or students specifically Opposes two or more ideas or two or more people as a way of pointing out disagreement (may include revoicing moves) Calls for Individual or collective evaluation- question asked by teacher that requires students to evaluate the previous idea(s). S: I think it’s the molecules ‘cause yesterday when we talked about when it gets cold- um the molecules slow down and they stick together. T: So you’re saying that you disagree with Trisha and Corey, is that right? That you think it’s the liquid water? And the reason that you think this is what? Say that again. S: Because lots of people said the sun, but if the sun doesn’t have anything to evaporate, then like water- nothing could evaporate, so what’s the point. T: What do you think about what Alise said? T: See if you agree with what Thomas says. See if you agree with what other people say. If you agree, say you agree and give your reasons. If you don’t agree, say you don’t agree; give your reasons. 137 S: I think I agree with Beth’s first reasoning because I don’t think you can use/ Newton's second law/ because if you use enough force/ then maybe it will actually move the opposite way. T: This is really interesting. He says they’re clear. You say they’re translucent, why won’t these work? S 1: No/ no. There’s less gravitational potential energy right here. 82: I know but it’s going to go down faster so there’s more potential energy/ that’s going to make it go down. T: What do you think? He says the force is being transferred. Can we transfer force? What do you think? Meta-talk- students are coach in how to speak clearly and loudly, and how to participate as active listeners SR LR Snake: responsibilities- indication that it is important to talk loud and clear for everyone to hear and as a way of making sense of your ideas. Lime: responsibilities- indication that it is important to listen carefully to and consider every idea T: Talk so they can hear you. That’s your responsibility. T: Okay. Say it nice and loud. I want to know what other people think about that. T: And remember, you want to make sure that you are listening to everyone and that you are asking questions if you don’t understand what they’re saying. Commenting on ideas, not on people. 138 T: Can you say that louder/ I can't hear you. T: This is - you know, they put a lot into this first sentence. 1 want you guys to listen to this and see if you can tease out the really important parts. APPENDIX B: MS. HOCKING’S INTERVIEW PART I: Background Information Information about students: Total Students: Number of Males/Females: M F Ethnicity ofclass: Economic status of students: Information about teacher: How many years have you taught in total? At elementary level? At middle school level? At high school level? Other? Certifications? What degree(s) do you hold? (both science and/or education) Bachelors? Masters? Specialist? Other? Other background information: 139 PART 2: Specific Clips: Guiding questions we might consider in each clip: ' What goals do you have for what you want students to learn about science in the clip? ° How do you see these goals as connecting to what students need to know about science and the practices valued in the science community? ' How do you see discussion as playing a role in achieving your goals in this clip? ° How do you see discussion as playing a role in student learning of science in the clip? 1. Testing & Exploring— teacher tells students to explore light/matter any way they like You do a lot of open-ended explorations: Why do you use the approach of exploration first? What are your goals for students when they participate in these explorations? How do you see this as helping them use practices valued in science? 2. Control Variables In this clip, the students and you talk about control variables after students have explored. You scaffold their thinking about the control variables. What was your purpose for doing this at this particular point in the lesson? What caused you to do this brief “mini-lesson”? 3. Coming to consensus In this clip, students develop groups based on how light interactions with matter. What do you see your role is when helping students to reach consensus? How do you see talking about consensus as contributing to what students learn about science? You also have students develop working definitions for words. How do you usually teach vocabulary to students? What do you want students to do with their working definitions? 4. Science method & Directions for penny/jar- In this clip you have students explain their ideas about the scientific method and then explain what you expect of students during the next few lessons. Mostly you communicate expectations of students. What were you listening for when asking about scientific methods? What do you want students to learn about science when talking about this? 5. PI] small group 1- curvature and reflection In this clip you circulate to a small group working with their jar and penny. What do you see your role as facilitator while you do this? What are you listening for? 6. P/J Kevin’s observation- shares with class In this clip, you selected an observation from one student to share with the class. Why did you select Kevin’s observation? How do you choose what observations to share and what not to share? 140 7. Trial and Error This was a lesson that did not go as planned. You talk explicitly to students about this. What was your purpose for talking about the failed experiments? Do you think talking about them influenced your students’ understanding of science? 8. Predicting light boxes & Evaluating predictions In this clip, students first draw their predictions on the overhead. After investigating in groups, the students them evaluate the predictions as a class. What was your goal for students in these clips? How do you see predicting and evaluating predictions relating to your goals for students? 9. Sketches of light box In this clip you have students sketch what they observed with the light boxes. What explanations and sketches were you expecting? When someone sketched the “wrong” observation, what did you feel you needed to do? What did you want students to learn from this? PART 3: General views about goals of science discussions: 1. How do you get students to engage in discussions about science? What practices do you use? 2. What practices do you encourage students to use in the discussions? How to you support them in using those practices? 3. How do you see writing as supporting discussions? 4. How do you see hands-on activities and investigations as supporting discussions? 5. When students answer in a wrong or unexpected way, such as showing misconceptions or steering the conversation in an unexpected direction, what do you feel you need to do as the teacher? 141 . How do you think discussion and communication influence students’ Ieaming of science? What evidence do you have? . What other things do you think I should ask about your science teaching? What things seem important to you? . If I were to make a case study from the lessons I’ve taped, which lesson would you want me to choose and why? . Why is questioning important to you? What types of questions do you try to ask your students? How do you help students to ask their own questions? 142 APPENDIX C: MS. CHRISTENSEN’S INTERVIEW PART I: Background Information Information about students: Total Students: Number of Males/Females: M F Ethnicity ofclass: Economic status of students: Information about teacher: How many years have you taught in total? At elementary level? At middle school level? At high school level? Other? Certifications? Where did you earn each degree that you hold? (both science and/or education) Bachelors? Masters? Specialist? Other? Other background information: 143 PART 2: Specific practices from videos: Specific Clips: Guiding questions we might consider in each clip: ° What goals do you have for what you want students to learn about science in the clip? 0 How do you see these goals as connecting to what students need to know about practices valued in science? ° How do you see discussion as playing a role in achieving your goals in this clip? ° How do you see discussion as playing a role in student learning of science in the clip? C lip 1: What makes weather? This clip was from a lesson that occurred at the beginning of the unit. Students brainstormed their ideas about “What makes the Weather?” Why did you do this activity at this point in the unit? What was your goal for this activity? What were listening for from your students? Clip 2: Observations ofInvestigtion I In this clip, students are observing investigation 1. What do you expect of students Observations? How to you help students articulate/describe their observations? How do you see hands-on activities as supporting discussions? C lip 3: Explaining and Predicting (around minute 43: 151 In this clip, you ask students to write explanations for two questions in response to investigation 1 and to make predictions for investigation 2. Why do you have students make predictions? How do you see writing as supporting discussions (or vice versa)? C [in 4: What are clouds made of? In this clip, students discuss what makes up clouds. What are your expectations for the types of explanations that students give? Why do you have students evaluate each other’s explanations? How do you think discussion like this support student learning? C [in 5 .' Adding to chart In this clip, students add more ideas to the water cycle chart. What was your purpose for this activity? What were you listening for from students? C Lip 6: Listener responsibilities In this clip you remind students of their responsibilities during a science talk. Can you explain more about your expectations for “listener responsibilities” and “speaker 144 responsibilities”? How do you help students become good listeners/speakers throughout the school year? Why is this so important to you? What do you think students learn because of these expectations? Clip 7: Begin science talk In this clip, the class begins their science talk. Can you describe the typically science talk in your class? What are your goals for the science talk? What do you hope students learn during the talks? How do you see the ‘quick write’ as supporting the discussion? What is your role during the science talk? Clip 8: Adding comments to science talk In this clip, you give students who passed an opportunity to add to the science talk. Can you talk a little more about your purpose here and what you hope students will do during this time? C lip 10: Hail and melting In this clip, Maria had asked the question about why hail doesn’t melt (since it falls in the summertime). Ben asks for a clarification of “melt”. Students in your class ask lots of questions- How do you help students learn to ask question? Why do you think this is important? Why do you have students answer and evaluate their classmate’s question/ideas? Clip I I: Revisiting science talk In this clip, you bring students back to the science talk and tell the class who changed their mind. What is your purpose for doing this? What do you expect from your students in this discussion? Clip 12: Kamal and Ben In this clip Kamal and Ben are questioning each other’s explanations. How do you see this type of activity as important for what you want students to learn about science? How do you help students come to some consensus? PART 3: General views about goals of science discussions: 10. How do you get students to engage in discussions about science? What practices do you use? 145 1 1. What practices do you encourage students to use in the discussions? How to you support them in using those practices? 12. When students answer in a wrong or unexpected way, such as showing misconceptions or steering the conversation in an unexpected direction, what do you feel you need to do as the teacher? 13. How do you think discussion and communication influence students’ Ieaming of science? What evidence do you have? 14. What other things do you think I should. ask about your science teaching? What things seem important to you? 146 APPENDIX D: TERMS Literacy: communication of a social language through different literacy acts and genres, including reading, writing, and oral communication (and multiple symbol systems connected to that language) that represent a particular way of thinking, valuing, or acting. (Similar terms: Discourse (James Gee); Articulacy (Michael Halliday)) Discourse: Distinctive ways people talk, read, write, think, believe, act, and interact with things and other people to get recognized (Gee, 1996) Important Note: How are literacy and Discourse alike or different? Science Literacy and (big D) Discourse (Gee, I 996) appear to be very similar depending on how one chooses to define “literacy” or “Discourse Some scholars, such as Moje define “literacy” as specifically the reading and writing of print and do not include orality or other symbolic systems. However, my definition of “literacy” is somewhat closer to Gee ’s Discourse, yet there is more emphasis on how natural language of a community is appropriated through written and spoken discourse, rather than a “way of being” as Discourse typically emphasizes. The terms “Discourse ” as used by Gee and the term “Literacy ” as I define it are in no way incompatible and choosing one seems to depend more on the community in which the research is conducted and the focus of the research. Since the science education community has used Scientific literacy as a goal, I have chosen to use Scientific literacy as opposed to Scientific Discourse. I47 Scientific Literacy: The capacity to understand and participate in evidence-based discussions that thoughtful consideration of evidence and explanation, as well as participating in the collective process of validating and skeptical review. Science practice: Social activities in which members interact with other people or the material world using tools valued in science, such as scientific principles and specialized language. Monologic discourse: A type of discourse that is univocal in which discourse is used for the transmission of knowledge or information from a “knower’ or ‘eXpert’ to ‘learner’ or ‘novice. In this way knowledge flows in one direction and the ‘knower’ does not acknowledge that the learner has knowledge to contribute. Examples include recitation and lecture (derived from Bakhtin, Nystrand, Wells, Lottman, etc) Dialogic discourse/dialogic exchange: A type of discourse that is multi-vocal (or heteroglossia), in which discourse is used for collaborative knowledge building between multiple knowers who are simultaneously learners. In this type of discourse knowledge comes from multiple sources (derived from Bakhtin, Nystrand, Wells, Lottman, etc) Discourse move: A single utterance or contribution made by a participant that has a specific purpose or function. True Discussion: Type of discourse structure, involving free exchange of information among 3 or more people (and lasting for more than 30 seconds- from Nystrand, 199 7) 148 Recitation: Describes a teacher-student interaction in which students recite previously learned information in response to teacher questioning (Example: IRE). This does not necessarily mean students are doing much oral participation, because the teacher’s voice often dominates and teachers may actually do the answering as a way of elaborating on students’ short answers. Lecture: Describes a type of communication is which information is given, often by one person, to an audience with minimal audience participation in response to the information. 149 REFERENCES American Association for the Advancement of Science. (1990). Science for all Americans. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Anderson, C. W. (2003). Teaching science for motivation and understanding. Retrieved April 17, 2008 from http://www.msu.edu/~andya/TEScience/Assets/Files/ TSMU.pdf Anderson, C.W., and others (2006). Environmental literacy blueprint. Unpublished manuscript, East Lansing, MI Bakhtin, M.M. (1981). The dialogic imagination. Austin, TX: University of Texas Press Bakhtin, M.M. (1986). Speech genres and other late essays. Austin, TX: University of Texas Press. Bazerman, C. (1988). Shaping written knowledge: The genre and activity of the experimental article in science. Madison, WI: University of Wisconsin Press. Bloome, D., Puro, P., & Theodorou, E. (1989). Procedural display and classroom lessons. Curriculum Inquiry, 19(3), 265-291). Bransford, J.D., Brown, A.L., & Cocking, RR. (2000). How people learn: Brain, mind experience, and school. Washington, DC: National Academic Press. Burbules, NC. (1993). Dialogue in teaching: Theory and practice. New York: Teachers College Press. Burbules, N.C., & Bruce, BC. (2001). Theory and research on teaching as dialogue. In v. Richardson (ea), Handbook ofResearch on Teaching, 4th Edition (pp.l 102- 1121). Washington, DC: American Educational Research Association. Bybee, R. (1997). Achieving scientific literacy: From purpose to practice. Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann. Calabrese Barton, A., Tan, E., & Rivet, A. (2008). Creating hybrid spaces for engaging school science among urban middle school girls. American Educational Research Journal, 45 (I), 68-103. Calabrese Barton, A. (2007). Science learning in urban contexts. In S.K. Abell & N.G. Lederman (Eds), Handbook of Research on Science Education (pp. 319-343). Philadelphia: Lawrence Erlbaum. 150 Cazden, C. B. (2001). Classroom discourse: The language of teaching and learning (2nd ed.). Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann Cobb, P., Stephan, M., McClain, K., & Gravemeijer, K. (2001). Participating is classroom mathematical practices. The Journal of the Learning Sciences, 10 (1&2), 1 13-163. Cobb, P., & Yackel, E. (1996). Constructivist, emergent, and sociocultural perspectives in the context of developmental research. Educational Psychologist, 31 (3/4), 175-190. Crawford, T., Kelly, G. J., & Brown, C. (2000). Ways of knowing beyond facts and laws of science: An ethnographic investigation of student engagement with scientific practices. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 3 7(3), 237-258. DeBoer, G. E. (2000). Scientific literacy: Another look at its historical and contemporary meanings and its relationship to science education reform. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 37(6), 582-601. Doyle, W. (1983). Academic work. Review of Educational Research, 53, 159-199. Duschl, R.A., Schweingruber, H.A, & Shouse, A.W. (2007). Taking science to school: Learning and Teaching science in grades K-8. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press Edwards, D., & Mercer, N. (1987). Common knowledge: The development of understanding in the classroom. New York: Methuen. Eisenhart, M., Finkel, E., & Marion, S. F. (1996). Creating the conditions for scientific literacy: A re—examination. American Educational Research Journal, 33(2), 261- 295. Ford, M. (2008). Disciplinary authority and accountability in scientific practice and learning, Science Education, 92(3), 404-423. Gallas, K. (1995). Talking their way into science: Hearing children's questions and theories, responding with curricula. New York: Teachers College Press. Gee, J. P. (1996). Social linguistics and Iiteracies: Ideology in Discourses (2nd ed.). Bristol, PA: Taylor & Francis. Gee, J. P. (2004a). Discourse analysis: What makes it critical? From R. Rogers (Ed) An Introduction to critical discourse analysis in education (pp. 19-50). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Gee, J. P. (2004b). Language in the science classroom: Academic social languages as the 151 heart of school—based literacy. In E. W. Saul (Ed.), Crossing borders in literacy and science instruction: Perspectives on theory and practice (pp. 13-32). Newark, DE: International Reading Association. Gee, JP (2006). Oral discourse in a world of literacy. Research in the teaching of English, 41(2), 153-159. Gee, J .P., & Green, J .L. (1998). Discourse analysis, Ieaming, and social practice: A methodological study. In P.D. Pearson & A. Iran-Nejad (Eds.), Review of research in education (pp.l 19-170). Washington, DC: American Educational Research Association. Gee, J.P., Michaels, S., & O’Connors, MC. (1992). Discourse analysis. In M.D. Lecompte, W. Millroy, & J. Preissle (Eds), Handbook of qualitative research in education (pp.227-29l ). New York: Academic Press. Glaser, B.G., & Strauss, AL. (1967). The discovery of grounded theory: Strategy for qualitative research. Chicago: Aldine Press. Hadjioannou, X. (2007). Bringing the background to the foreground: What do classroom environments that support authentic discussions look life? American Educational Research Journal, 44(2), 370-399. Halliday, M.A.K. (1993). Toward a language-based theory of Ieaming. Linguistics and Education, 5(2), 93-116. Halliday, M.A.K., & Martin, JR. (1994). Writing science: Literacy and discursive power. Pittsburg, PA: University of Pittsburg Press. Hand, B. M., Alvennann, D. E., Gee, J., Guzetti, B. J., Norris, S. P., Phillips, L. M., et al. (2003). Message from the "island group": What is literacy in science literacy. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 40(7), 607-615. Heath, SB. (2000). Linguistics in the study of language in education. Harvard Educational Review, 70(1), 49-59. Heath, SB. (1983). Ways with words: Language, life, and work in communities and classrooms. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Herrenkohl, L. R., Palincsar, A. S., DeWater, L. S., & Kawaski, K. (1999). Developing Scientific communities in classrooms: A sociocognitive approach. The Journal of the Learning Sciences, 8(3&4), 451-493. Henenkohl, L. R., & Guerra, M. R (1998). Participant structures, scientific discourse, and student engagement in fourth grade. Cognition and Instruction, 16(4), 431- 473. 152 Hogan, K., & Corey, C. (2001). Viewing classrooms as cultural contexts for fostering scientific literacy. Anthropology & Education Quarterly, 32(2), 214-243. Lave, J ., & Wenger, E. (1991). Situated learning: Legitimate peripheral participation. New York: Cambridge University Press. Lemke, J. L. (1990). Talking science: Language, learning, and values. Westport, CT: Ablex Publishing Lotman, Y.M. (1988). Text within text. Soviet psychology, 26(3), 32-51. Mehan, H. (1979). Learning lessons: Social organization in the classroom. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Merriam, SB. (1998). Qualitative Research and Case Study Applications in Education. San Francisco: Jossey Bass. Mohan, L., Lundeberg, M.A., & Reffitt, K. (2008). Studying teachers and schools: Michael Pressley’s legacy and directions for future research. Educational Psychologist, 43 (2), 1-12. Mohan, L., Lundeberg, M.A., & Pressley, M. (in revision). Where is that lab going? Engagement practices in science and the value of discussion. Moje, E. B. (1995). Talking about science: An interpretation of the effects of teacher talk in a high school science classroom. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 32(4), 349-371. Moje, E. (2000). “To be part of the story”: The literacy practices of gangsta adolescents. Teachers College Record, 102 (3), 651-690. Moje, E. B., K.M., C., Kramer, K., Ellis, L., Carrillo, R., & Collazo, T. (2004). Working toward third space in content area literacy: An examination of everyday funds of knowledge and discourse. Reading Research Quarterly, 39(1), 38-70. National Research Council. (1996). The national science education standards. Washington, DC: National Academies Press. Norris, S. P., & Phillips, L. M. (2003). How literacy in its fundamental sense is central to scientific literacy. Science Education. 87, 224-240. Nystrand, M. (1997). Opening dialogue: understanding the dynamics of language and learning in English classrooms. New York: Teachers College Press. Nystrand, M., Wu, LL, Gamoran, A., Zeiser, 8., Long. DA. (2003). Questions in time: 153 Investigating the structure and dynamics of unfolding classroom discourse. Discourse Processes, 35(2), 135-198. O'Connor, M. C., & Michaels, S. (1996). Shifting participant frameworks: Orchestrating thinking practices in group discussions. In D. Hicks (Ed.), Discourse learning and schooling (pp. 63-103). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. Osborne, J., Erduran, S., & Simon, S. (2004). Enhancing the quality of argumentation in School science, Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 41 (10), 994—1020. Palincsar, A. S., Anderson, C. W., & David, Y. M. (1993). Pursuing scientific literacy in the middle grades through collaborative problem-solving. The Elementary School Journal, 93(5), 643-658. Pressley, M. (November, 2003). Balanced elementary literacy instruction in the United States: A personal perspective. Keynote address given at the annual meeting of the International Literacy Conference, Toronto. Resnick, L. B. (1987). Learning in school and out. Educational Researcher, 16(9), 13-20. Rivard, L. P., & Straw, S. B. (2000). The effect of talk and writing on learning science: An exploratory study. Science Education, 84, 566-593. Rosebury, A.S., Warren, 8., & Conant, FR. (1992). Appropriating scientific discourse: Finding from language minority classrooms. The Journal of the Learning Sciences, 2(1), 61-94. Shanna, A, & Anderson, CW. (2007). Recontextualization of science from lab to school: Implication for science literacy. Science & Education, Online. Spradley, J. (1979). The ethnographic interview. New York: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston. Spradley, J. (1980). Participant observation. New York: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston. Strauss, A. L. & Corbin, J. (1998). Basics of Qualitative Research: Techniques and Procedures for Developing Grounded Theory. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Street, B. (2001). The New Literacy Studies. In E. Cushman, G.R. Kintgen, B.M. Kroll, & M. Rose (Eds), Literacy: A critical sourcebook (pp. 430-442). Boston: St. Martin’s Press Vygotsky, LS. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes. Cambridge: Harvard University Press. Wells, G., & Arauz, R. M. (2006). Dialogue in the classroom. The Journal of the Learning Sciences, 15(3), 379-428. 154 Yackel, E., & Cobb, P. (1996). Sociomathematical norms, argumentation, and autonomy in mathematics, Journal of Research in Mathematics Education, 27(4), 458-477. Yore, L. D., & Treagust, D. F. (2006). Current realities and future possibilities: Language and science literacy--empowering research and informing instruction. International Journal of Science Education, 28(2-3), 291-314. 155 lljillilllill]llillljijljlllllililiilll 93 02