232‘. 7. .. . if: , ‘2»? 1.! i1 . , E i. am. at»? .0. .l 5119...: flvl - 1'13 :. .2: . . .i 4!. I... .F..\ ‘ :3. :IIMEI Ii. » 1‘. Eat. . . ‘r : z: I. :.r .153 .«x .... .. i... . a . .1... 5. . .11.. in... I» :I vhlq 15.251.) . 013:2...‘4L It: 25338.. (i J sknki a..- [.33 55:9!!! cl $- . fl .. ld‘~ .0533;th .- t tycituaru. ’t.‘ It: «.5, .lrtzlgrn 4'... FEE? .2... a 1:: tfiflmflmflmmhzn am .Lnr 4.3.53 . «mu. 1“an is 2%, wig” v ‘ .uu : , 333:7..4, .5... V s?:%.. a. . . kw... .rwwfi. . B ,3 . . Inst: r3..v|\* n .n i “2%.. ‘ , \l . . .. I. 31»... . in. 3.3M mm 2 Ki,- 5’; LIBRARY Michigan State University This is to certify that the thesis entitled A Tool for Assessing Citizen Deliberative Decisions about Contaminated Sites presented by Monica Day has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for the MS. degree in Resource Development jaw m Major Professor’s Signature July 29, 2008 Date MSU is an affirmative-action, equal-opportunity employer -.—-—-—c—-— PLACE IN RETURN BOX to remove this checkout from your record. TO AVOID FINES return on or before date due. MAY BE RECALLED with earlier due date if requested. DATE DUE DATE DUE DATE DUE 5/08 K'lProilAcc8Pres/ClRC/DateDue.indd A TOOL FOR ASSESSING CITIZEN DELIBERATIVE DECISIONS ABOUT CONTAMINATED SITES By Monica Day A THESIS Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of MASTER OF SCIENCE Resource Development 2008 ABSTRACT A TOOL FOR ASSESSING CITIZEN DELIBERATIVE DECISIONS ABOUT CONTAMINATED SITES By Monica Day Despite acceptance of the benefits of public participation for clean up of contaminated sites, agencies often struggle with executing public involvement methods. The literature provides guidance on evaluating methods, but tools for assessing group decisions were difiicult to find. This study tested an application of the National Issues Forums (N IF) method for clean-up of dioxins on the Tittabwassee River. The research recruited people to attend a forum in their neighborhood, discuss clean up options, and develop consensus about the preferred solution. This thesis assessed the group decisions according to six dimensions. Forum participants differed in some ways from the neighborhoods at large and the quality of the group decisions varied. The study concludes that while the subjectivity of any assessment method makes evaluation of outcomes difficult, based on the tool developed for this study, the NIF method can produce good decisions in neighborhoods impacted by contaminated sites. TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES ................................................................................... iv CHAPTER 1: INTRODUCTION ................................................................... 1 Research question ................................................................................................................ 3 Setting and approach ............................................................................................................ 4 Organization of thesis .......................................................................................................... 5 CHAPTER 2: LITERATURE REVIEW ............................................................................. 6 Scholarship on assessing group decision-making .............................................................. 15 CHAPTER 3: INTRODUCTION TO THE CASE STUDY ............................................. 19 Case: Tittabawassee River and Floodplain Dioxin Contamination ................................... 19 Study design and data used ................................................................................................ 20 Neighborhoods surveyed ................................................................................................... 26 CHAPTER 4: COMPARISON OF NEIGHBORHOODS TO FORUM PARTICIPANTS .................................................................................... 29 CHAPTER 5: ASSESSING GROUP DECISIONS .......................................................... 47 Data used in assessing decision quality ............................................................................. 47 Assessment factors ............................................................................................................. 47 Calculation of the six factor scores .................................................................................... 51 Process Fairness ............................................................................................................. 51 Consensus ...................................................................................................................... 51 Decision Stability ........................................................................................................... 52 Preference Change ......................................................................................................... 52 Resolution of Tradeoffs ................................................................................................. 54 Knowledge Gain ............................................................................................................ 55 Converting factor scores into an individual decision assessment ...................................... 57 Constructing a group assessment ....................................................................................... 62 Other data related to factors ............................................................................................... 64 Group decision assessments ............................................................................................... 74 Good group decisions .................................................................................................... 74 Partially developed group decisions .............................................................................. 79 Forums with too little information to assess .................................................................. 84 Summary of assessments and reasons for differences observed ........................................ 84 CHAPTER 6: CONCLUSION .......................................................................................... 90 APPENDIX: RESPONDENT QUOTATION TABLES BY FORUM ....................... 97 BIBLIOGRAPHY. . . . . .. ................................................................................................ .105 iii LIST OF TABLES Table 3.1 Recruitment - participation at each stage of inquiry ................................ 26 Table 4.1 Comparison of income, education and racial composition of forum participants to neighborhoods ..................................................................................... 32 Table 4.2 Reasons for not participating in a forum .............................................. 34 Table 4.3 Comparison of rates of forum participation by gender as reported in baseline survey .................................................................................................. 35 Table 4.4 Comparison of sources of knowledge of the issue between forum participants and non participants as reported in the baseline survey ........................................ 37 Table 4.5 Illustration of key knowledge responses across neighborhoods ................... 39 Table 4.6 Comparison of interests of forum participants to non-participants as reported in the baseline survey .................................................................................. 41 Table 4.7 Comparison of preferred solution of forum participants to non participants as reported in the baseline survey .................................................................... 43 Table 4.8 Frequency of blocking statements among forum participants ..................... 46 Table 5.1 Consensus positions by forum ........................................................ 66 Table 5.2 Change in mean level of acceptance of clean up scenarios ....................... 67 Table 5.3 Examples of tradeoffs and resolutions ............................................... 69 Table 5.4 Change in percentage of correct quiz answers ...................................... 70 Table 5.5 Change in knowledge self-assessment ............................................... 71 Table 5.6 Process fairness ......................................................................... 73 Table 5.7 Decision stability ....................................................................... 74 Table 5.8 Maple street group three (MSG3) .................................................... 77 Table 5.9 McCliggott Road (MCG) ............................................................. 79 Table 5.10 Gratiot and River Roads Group two (GRR2) ...................................... 80 iv Table 5.11 Falcon View Estates (F VE) .......................................................... 82 Table 5.12 North Saginaw (NSA) ................................................................. 83 Table 5.13 Gratiot and River roads group one (GRRI) ........................................ 85 Table 5.14 Quality of group decisions based on proximity to river ......................... 86 Table A] MSG3 Preferred Solution Quotes .................................................... 97 Table A2 MCG Preferred Solution Quotes ...................................................... 98 Table A3 GRR2 Preferred Solution Quotes .................................................... 100 Table A4 F VE Preferred Solution Quotes ...................................................... 101 Table A5 F VE Preferred Solution Quotes ...................................................... 102 Table A6 NSA Preferred Solution Quotes ..................................................... 103 Table A7 GRRl Preferred Solution Quotes .................................................... 104 CHAPTER 1: INTRODUCTION Citizen participation began to be institutionalized in the United States during the rise of regulatory rule making in the first half of the twentieth century. Participation was included in rule making to protect individual interests and facilitate public scrutiny of government (Renn, 1995). During the 19503 and 19603 the value of participation to improve public policy to improve policy, plans and projects was starting to be recognized (Renn, 1995). Since then, nearly every federal agency has institutionalized some form of citizen participation in their regulatory decision making procedures (Renn, 1995). Citizen participation has been touted for reducing costs and leading to better projects by reducing the likelihood of litigation and decreasing planning time and changes later in the process (Abel & Stephan, 2000; Woolcock & Narayan, 2000). In addition to easing policy making, administrators who make a good faith effort to engage the public meaningfully can also increase citizen competence and desire to participate, and engender trust between citizens and government paving the way to work collaboratively to solve public problems. Benefits of quality participation efforts include stronger agency—citizen relationships, improved agency credibility, better policy, and a stronger democracy (Abel & Stephan, 2000). Despite evidence that citizen participation yields high quality outcomes (Beierle, 2002), citizen participation continues to be a source of frustration for agencies and continues to be underfunded. Making policy about the clean up of contaminated sites has been particularly challenging for public health agencies because of the costs involved in action and the risks involved in inaction. Contaminated sites posing health risks to wildlife and humans exposed to the contaminants, including reproductive problems, birth defects, cancer, tumors, neurological defects, immune dysfunction, and liver and kidney ailments. Strategies to reduce exposure to the contaminant may require land use changes that impinge further on communities and individuals. The United States Environmental Protection Agency (EPA), the agency responsible for oversight of the clean up of contaminated sediments, recognizes benefits of early, active, and continuous community involvement in risk management strategies (Council, 1996). In fact, citizen participation programs have become a standard component of decision making processes for contaminated site remediation. Despite these efforts, risks persist, the costs of clean-up may seem overwhelming, or the remediation efforts may seem unacceptably long. Because of the serious health implications, and because clean—up involves disruption of communities, contaminated site remediations are contentious public issues that call for sophisticated citizen participation efforts. Agencies and other organizations leading and undertaking environmental clean— ups are challenged with how to most productively engage the affected public in clean-up decision making. This study suggests an approach for assessing citizen preferences produced during a deliberative public involvement process known as the National Issues Forums. A contaminated site remediation case in central Michigan provides the context for the research. Citizen Participation The present study draws upon literature on citizen participation in public decision making. Institutional barriers to meaningfully engaging affected citizens in public decision-making recognized by this research include representative democracy and a technocratic approach to risk management. The proposed solution is for public agencies to build social capital with affected citizens by creating new opportunities for citizen involvement, especially deliberative opportunities. The approach seeks to engage affected citizens meaningfully in the ethical decisions inherent in contaminated sediments clean- up decisions by using a structured group decision-making process that emphasizes working-through to resolve tradeoffs in clean-up choice making. Cleanup of environmental contaminants is ofien approached by the responsible institutions as largely a technical problem (Council, 1996). While science and mathematics play a critical role in hazard characterization and risk quantification, risk communication, including decision making, requires normative judgments regarding acceptance of risks. The technical experts who are able to quantify the risk provide critical information for making risk management decisions. However, the people who are exposed to the contaminants are in the rightful position to determine what risk is acceptable. Supporting productive communication between these groups, experts and citizens, is the contemporary challenge of risk communication (McComas, 1998). Normative risk decisions that will affect many people cannot be made if people are uninformed or have not thought through the consequences of their decisions. A process of learning about and defining the options, and thinking through the tradeoffs is needed. Research question This research seeks to find out whether, when provided with objective technical background information on the problem and potential solutions, affected citizens can attain high quality clean-up decisions using a facilitated, structured decision-making approach. To answer this question, the research first assesses to what degree forum participants are representative of the area residents. Setting and approach To answer this question 378 residents were surveyed of whom 51 (15%) then attended nine structured decision making discussions held in neighborhoods along the Tittabawassee River in the Saginaw River basin in central Michigan. At the time of this study, the Tittabawassee River downstream of Midland was in the risk assessment phase of a clean-up of high levels of dioxin in the river sediments and floodplain. The source of the dioxin was from a chemical production facility that disposed of wastes directly into the Tittabawassee River before environmental regulations were established in the 19703. Citizen involvement in cleanup decision making is challenged by scientific uncertainty and low engagement of affected citizens. Scientific uncertainty around the health effects from dioxin exposure at levels that are in the Tittabawassee floodplain has been a vulnerability of the issue that has been exploited by powerful corporate actors. Their rhetoric has been effective at polarizing the views of the citizenry, and interest groups have formed for and against cleanup of the dioxin to try to influence the decision- making. Responsible for leading the cleanup, decision makers such as Michigan DEQ and USEPA are interested in knowing more about the interests of the “silent majority” who are not engaged with the polarized interest groups. However, those not involved in the debate are unlikely to participate in standard public involvement opportunities. This research was part of an effort to create an institutionally sanctioned opportunity for the “silent majority,” the affected but unaffiliated citizens, to discuss several cleanup scenarios together with others in their neighborhood and come to consensus about their preferred choice. Organization of the thesis Following this introduction, chapter 2 presents a survey of relevant literature. Chapter 3 presents the case study and the larger research project of which this thesis is a part. Chapter 4 presents demographic data from the study area and compares forum participants to the larger population. Chapter 5 develops a method for determining the ability of forum participants to reach about how to address the dioxin issue and displays an assessment of the group decisions. Chapter 6 concludes the thesis. CHAPTER 2: LITERATURE REVIEW Public participation research is motivated by normative and practical goals. People want a living democracy and effective public policy. Despite decades of research touting the benefits, in practice, public participation activities often flounder. This research is motivated by a real-world problem of making more just and satisfying environmental clean-up decisions. An exploration of some of the scholarship on the topic contextualizes the study. Problem Contaminated sites are complex environmental problems that have historically been addressed in the United States with a technocratic approach (Fischer, 2000). The technocratic, synoptic, or rational-comprehensive model is a scientifically based, expert-driven approach that is characterized by a high degree of specialization and is “rule bound,” hierarchical, and closed off to feedback (Kelly, 2004; Lachapelle et al., 2003). Risk assessment is an example of a technocratic approach to addressing uncertain dangers. A typical risk assessment for addressing a contaminated sediments problem includes: 0 Hazard identification: characterization of the harm associated with the contaminant; 0 Risk measurement or quantification: researching the probability of illness or harm due to exposure to the contaminant; 0 Risk communication: examining possible corrective actions including technical aspects of different approaches to conducting a clean-up, deciding which clean-up approach to take; and, 0 Risk management: conducting a clean-up, and evaluating the effectiveness (Thompson, 2004). Science can and should be used to describe and understand the technical aspects of the risk; however, it cannot determine what the target for protection or repair should be or resolve other normative issues (J asanoff, 1991). Normative problems, or situations requiring discerning what’s important and deciding “how things ought to be,” require more than facts and information; they require judgment. The individuals exposed to the hazard should be part of deciding what level of risk is acceptable. However, engaging citizens in decision making around risk management situations is challenging for agencies. The emotionally charged atmosphere of contaminated sites requires excellent public involvement methods. If citizens are from the normative decision making process, agencies risk degrading relationships with the public and their credibility. Furthermore, where special interest groups dominate, as is often the case with contaminated sites, citizens have an increased threat of vulnerability to manipulation. Excluding the public from the decision making process, in the zero—sum atmosphere of representative government is at best going to leave a minority of the public dissatisfied with the solution chosen and at worst satisfy none. Decision makers attempt to limit the influence of special interest groups, falsely assuming that if they “stick to the science” they can maintain a scientific-rational basis for decision making. However, science cannot solve the normative components of the decision making. What is lost in the technocratic approach to risk analysis is insight into the decision at hand about what is acceptable to the people who are most affected by the problem (Gregory, 2002). Recognizing the risks of excluding and alienating the public, public institutions have expanded risk analysis to include consideration of public interests and values and a public involvement component (Council, 1996). It is now widely accepted that contaminated site decisions require an understanding of science, engineering issues, regulatory requirements, economic impacts and community values. The US. Environmental Protection Agency (EPA), the agency responsible for oversight of the clean-up of contaminated sediments, recognizes benefits of early, active, and continuous community involvement in risk management strategies. Thus, public involvement programs have become a standard component of decision making processes for contaminated site remediation. Although newer programs offer citizens a range of ways they can become involved, most opportunities are found on the lower rungs of the ladder of public involvement: information and education. Amstein (1968) developed the “ladder of citizen participation” to delineate the gradations of participation based on degree of influence citizens have over the outcome. The first two rungs: manipulation and therapy, Amstein (1968) describes as non-participation because they are used primarily to change citizen’s views to match the interests of the decision makers. Public involvement activities at the manipulation rung are really public relations tools intended to persuade the public. The middle set of rungs: informing, consultation, and placation involve openly engaging the public, but fall short of ensuring that the public’s interests will affect the outcome. Examples of these middle rung public involvement activities include distributing information by the media, public meetings, and advisory committees. The top three rungs: partnership, delegated power, and citizen control are rungs where citizens have gradations of influence over the decision outcome (Amstein, 1968). Examples include citizen juries, planning committees, and negotiation. The lower levels of public involvement are ofien insufficient for blending expert opinion and citizen interests into satisfying contaminated site clean-up decisions (Gregory, 2000). Traditional avenues for public participation such as public hearings are designed for the purpose of providing citizens with an opportunity to voice their opinions or views of proposed government policies, plans or projects. Such a process includes providing the public access to the proposal; a comment period is offered for public input including a public hearing to listen to public reactions; and decision-makers then usually have discretion in how they use the public input in their decision (McComas, 1998). Where emotions run high and the problem is technically complex, as is usually the case with contaminated sites, these relatively unstructured avenues for input are not likely to yield useful information. There may be too little participation, or the public may be organized into special interest groups where decisions may yield clear winners or losers. In fact, these venues for public input may even exacerbate conflict between citizens and experts leading to “decision paralysis” and public dissatisfaction (Fischer, 2000; Lachapelle et al., 2003). With inadequate public involvement processes, instead of informing the decision making process, decision makers operating from a technocratic approach may view the public’s expectations and desires as unrealistic and ill-informed. Despite believing in theory in the value of public involvement, in practice, the public is viewed in large part as a problem to be managed. The agency often responds by emphasizing the need for more information (Yankelovich, 1991). It is reasoned that if the public were informed enough, or understood the science better, they would adopt the same or similar solution that the experts select, thus resolving the conflict. However, simply adding to the information base rarely results in great success. While education is important, alone it is most likely inadequate for arriving at solutions that are satisfying to the public and the agency (Arvai & Gregory, 2003). For conflict resolution it is important for experts and the public to have two-way discourse about the technical aspects of the problem, but also about other aspects of concern to the public such as potential economic repercussions, aesthetic implications, tourism effects, health consequences, or impacts on cultural traditions (Arvai & Gregory, 2003; Slovic, 1997). Agencies may dismiss such concerns fi‘om the process because they fall outside of their authority. Due to the specialized nature of their responsibilities, agencies are typically constrained to focus their attention on technical solutions within their domain. By just having an open, non-directed public involvement process such as public hearings or some other open public comment period, the agencies will probably not be able to get the kind of information about public interests that can be useful for making satisfying normative judgments in risk management (Gregory, 2000). With open input many of the affected people may not participate, skewing the kind of information gathered. Also, those who do participate may not have worked through the issues to provide fully developed public opinion (McComas, 1998). 10 Public Involvement Methods - Deliberation and resolution of tradeoffs Aware of the need for participatory practices for complex problems, practitioners and scholars have been pursuing alternative public involvement methods since the early 19903 (Abel & Stephan, 2000). Conflict resolution, negotiated rulemaking, mediation, bargaining, citizen juries, citizen panels, citizens” advisory committees, citizen initiatives, study circles, planning cells, and consensus groups are some of the methods that have been tried (Renn, 1995). Many such methods emphasize the role of discourse between citizens and agencies for their effectiveness (Lachapelle et al., 2003; Slovic, 1997). Using dialog and deliberation is one useful and practical way to engage citizens in decision making. The aim is to facilitate the discussion to help participants to resolve disagreements and differences in a non-adversarial manner, and steer them toward win- win outcomes (Spano, 2001 ). Facilitation is also important to ensure that power differences are held in check. Deliberative public involvement methods are credited with their legitimizing effects for the agency, increasing sustainability of the decision, improving policy relevance, contributing to citizen empowerment and capacity building, and resolving difficult conflicts (Kelly, 2004; Kothari, 2001; Pellizzoni, 2003). Contrasting the technocratic approach, Gregory (2002) articulates the importance of wrestling with tradeoffs in risk management decision making: With so much attention given to community participation in risk-based deliberation, an obvious question is: Why do there remain so many stalled, unpopular, or otherwise problematic risk-management initiatives? We believe that a primary reason is the inability of most community-based risk initiatives to deal effectively with the difficult value trade-offs that emerge over the course of nearly all risk policy consultations. 11 Yankelovich (1991) also emphasizes the importance of resolving tradeoffs for reaching public judgment. The term is used for a type of public opinion “that exists once people have engaged an issue, considered it from all sides, understood the choices it leads to, and accepted the full consequences of the choices they make.” He describes how public judgment is cultivated and identifies its characteristics. He proposes that “the quality of public opinion be considered good when the public accepts responsibility for the consequences of its views.” According to Yankelovich, if the consequences of a particular choice are acceptable and they are willing to be responsible for those consequences, it is a good choice. Two additional characteristics of quality public opinion include the firmness with which an opinion is held and “consistency,” the extent to which their thinking is integrated rather than compartmentalized. Most contaminated sites do not have a clear technical solution. The public may not accept an agency proposal if there are substantial unwanted outcomes or if the public has not worked through the tradeoffs between non-action and potential solutions (Jasanoff, 1991). By not incorporating public values (Smith, 2003), or dealing with tradeoffs of critical concerns (Gregory, 2002), traditional public involvement methods often fail to yield satisfactory outcomes, may undermine the legitimacy of the agency, and may poorly address the problem. Public involvement theorists emphasize dialogue between decision-makers, agency representatives, other stakeholders and the public (Slovic, 1997) to incorporate values into the decision making process (Gregory, 2002). Giving attention to values in risk management decision making is consistent with the conflict resolution practitioner‘s recommendation to focus on interests, not positions in order to find mutually beneficial l2 solutions (Gregory, 2002). However, for as much as discursive methods are recommended, they are not widely used effectively. Theorists propose various explanations for this shortcoming. One that has already been mentioned is the technocratic approach of many public agencies. According to Yankelovich (1991), this approach makes poor use of values because expert knowledge is culturally revered while public or lay knowledge and feelings are culturally devalued. According to Yankelovich (1991), there is a cultural bias against the emotive in favor of expert knowledge. Similarly, the “objective” conception of risk: the probability of harm based on mathematics and scientific knowledge, is not how the lay public usually views risks in their lives. Instead the lay public’s risk perceptions are largely based on personal experiences with risk. The social construction of risk is important to contaminated sites decision making because how individuals conceive of the risk will impact the decision making process. This difference between how the experts and policy makers conceive of risk and the people who are living with the risk creates a communication challenge for engaging the public in dialogue. A second explanation for why pubic public policy makers do not deal with public values well is for cognitive and psychological reasons: they are unpleasant and difficult issues emotionally to deal with (Gregory, 2002). More specifically, drawing on the decision sciences and behavioral psychology literature, Gregory (2002) identifies six reasons that value tradeoffs are difficult: 1) they are multi-dimensional, for example there may be human health concerns, economic concerns, social impact concerns or moral concerns; 2) there is often outcome uncertainty making it difficult to come to a decision; 3) citizens are usually inexperienced and unfamiliar with making public policy decisions; 13 4) thinking and feeling carefully is difficult but necessary for disentangling values tradeoffs; 5) incorporating emotional elements into the decision is difficult for the administrators; and 6) such decisions often require learning over time to be made well (2002) To address these barriers, proponents recommend structured, values-centered decision making (Gregory, 2002; Slovic, 1997; Yankelovich, 1991): In contrast to the conventional, science-based process of risk analysis, a values-based approach starts with the interests of citizens and attempts to structure alternatives based on their concerns and expressed trade-offs (Gregory, 2002). Values are defined as: an expression of concerns or interests about what matters to individuals, in terms of both their preferences for different goods or activities and the underlying moral or ethical beliefs that give rise to these choices (Gregory, 2002) Tradeoffs occur whenever getting more of one thing of value requires giving up something else that is also desirable (Gregory, 2002). Why do the hard work of sorting through values tradeoffs? Gregory (2002) explains that the resulting benefit is more satisfying decisions: Addressing trade-offs is rarely an end in itself: in most cases, a focus on trade-offs is recommended because it leads to a better definition of the risk-management problem a more open discussion and better understanding of the fundamental values of participants, and an improved and more broadly accepted set of recommended risk-management alternatives. The theory suggests that one path to reaching sustainable risk management decisions is to engage the affected public meaningfiilly in a deliberative decision making process about the values tradeoffs inherent in each option. A call for this approach is not 14 new (Council, 1996); however, empirical studies of the feasibility of such an approach are rare. Scholarship on assessing group decision-making Although important, collective decision-making about contentious environmental problems is difficult work. Furthermore, how can practitioners know if group decisions satisfy the affected public? Survey of the literature resulted in an abundance of theory about what constitutes a good public involvement process, but little concrete guidance for practitioner for evaluating public involvement outcomes. An assessment tool, which will be described in chapter 5 was developed based on scholarship on evaluating public involvement methods. Public judgment, process fairness, and resolution of tradeoffs are core concepts introduced here. ‘ Reaching “public judgment” is a term made popular by Daniel Yankelovich (1991), to refer to decisions where people have settled on and accept a position. It indicates where struggling has ceased and acceptance can leave people feeling good about taking action. Several factors from Yankelovich’s theory were modified for the Tittabawassee River Contaminated sediments Case. Yankelovich (1991) emphasizes three characteristics of public judgment: 1. Responsibility for the consequences of views. 2. Consistent, dominated by holistic thinking vs. inconsistent or compartmentalized thinking. 3. Stability of views. The characteristics are compared to public opinion as determined from polls. Yankelovich (1991) provides guidance for how over time on various major national or 15 international issues, the public incorporates information with their values and accepts or rejects policy choices. Yankelovich (1991) defines public judgment as carefully thought- through opinion. When making a decision, agencies and policy makers value knowledge about what the public wants and feels about the issues. However decision makers need quality public input about the problem, not simply public opinion. Public judgment, if captured can provide guidance for policy makers about what will and what will not be acceptable to the public. It incorporates people’s interests, values, feelings and ethics about an issue. Public judgment, more than public opinion, can help to develop more lasting decisions that serve the interests of more people and are therefore more just. Deliberative risk communication theory (Beierle 2002; Arvai and Gregory 2003; Habermas 2006) is another literature that provides guidance for how to assess decision making. Much of the deliberative risk communication theory suggests that practitioners apply structure to public involvement processes, rather than just accepting undirected open comment such as may be provided at a public meeting. Process guidance emphasizes issues of fairness (Habermas, 2006), resolution of tradeoffs (Arvai & Gregory, 2003), level of knowledge (Arvai & Gregory, 2003), comfort with decisions (Arvai & Gregory, 2003), how well choices reflect their concerns (Arvai & Gregory, 2003), and achieving consensus (Webler & Tuler, 2000) as indications of quality decisions. Webler and Tuler et al. (2001) identified five distinct ways that the public decides whether the deliberative process was a good one. These include 1) the process “acquires and maintains popular legitimacy,” 2) the process “facilitates an ideological discussion,” 16 3) the process is fair, 4) the process minimizes power differences, 5) the process results in compromise (Webler, Tuler, & Krueger, 2001). The public decision reached during the process must satisfy the population who is affected by the decision in addition to the regulatory requirements. Renn, Webler, and Wiedemann (1995) present suggestions for how to evaluate various public involvement methods: citizen advisory committees, citizen panels, citizens juries, citizen initiatives, negotiated rule making, mediation, compensation and benefit sharing, and Dutch study groups. They suggest several factors that should be present if 9, 6‘ 9, ‘6 the process is good: “agreement on values,” “fairness, competence, efficacy,” and “applicability.” Process fairness, or procedural justice, is commonly regarded as a basic prerequisite for effective communication. Jurgen Habermas is a leader in the theory. Others such as Suskin & Kruschank have made the theory more accessible for practitioners. Arvai and Gregory have published extensively on the nuances of resolving tradeoffs and balancing values in environmental decision making (2000, 2002, 2003). Level of knowledge and achieving consensus are two common criteria of quality decisions. However both of these criteria have been critiqued and are not always included in evaluations of decisions or processes. For example, many practitioners emphasize the importance of clarifying values over gains in knowledge. Furthermore, according to many practitioners, quality public deliberations may not result in consensus. Gregory (2000) finds that if public deliberations result in a clarification of what is important to the public and thus provides substantive input for decision makers, then the deliberation was effective. 17 Consistent (holistic thinking), comfort with decisions, and how well choices reflect participants’ concerns were not incorporated into the evaluation framework. Coming together to support a chosen solution requires that some people (those who enter with disparate preferences from the group position) change their perspective. 18 CHAPTER 3: INTRODUCTION TO THE CASE STUDY This research examined the prospects for using deliberative methods to help engage a broad spectrum of people and to help them make better decisions about environmental management. For the case of decision-making about cleaning up the Tittabawassee River and floodplain the research asks if participants in the forums reached informed and lasting decisions. This chapter introduces the case and the larger study of which this research is a part, the sources of data used in the research, and the analysis undertaken. This study is one part of a larger Michigan State University research study sponsored by the United States Environmental Protection Agency. The larger study assessed, from a variety of perspectives, the efficacy of public issue forums as a method for achieving meaningful engagement of unaffiliated citizens in contaminated site communities. The community living in proximity to the dioxin contaminated sediments and floodplain of the Tittabawassee River in central Michigan was chosen as the test case. Case studies allow the researcher to illuminate a particular problem by studying a case in-depth (Patton, 2002). Case: T ittabawassee River and Floodplain Dioxin Contamination Higher than normal levels of dioxin have been found in the sediments and floodplain soils of the Tittabawassee River. Dioxins, as a group of more than 210 chlorine-based chemicals, tend to be persistent in the environment and many are toxic even in very small amounts. Testing has revealed dioxins levels in excess of the state’s dioxin standard of 90 parts per trillion (ppt) and a number of areas in and around the river (EPA, 2003; Diebel, 2005). 19 The source of the dioxin has been traced to production practices in the first half of the twentieth century conducted at Dow Chemical Company’s Midland facility. In 2003 the Michigan Department of Environmental Quality issued the Dow Midland Plant a 10- year Resource Conservation and Recovery Act Operating License that governs hazardous waste management activities at the Midland facility. It includes requirements for corrective action for dioxin contamination along the Tittabawassee River and floodplain. The Michigan Department of Community Health issued a fish consumption advisory for the Tittabawassee River downstream from Midland. Like many other contaminated sediments cases, the dioxin issue is contentious within the local community. The main point of the debate is the level of risk posed by the dioxin contaminated sediments and soils. Dow Chemical, the responsible party, has argued that the 90 ppt dioxin standard is unnecessarily low because there is no evidence that people and wildlife are being hurt by the dioxin. The public are aligned on different sides of the debate, some siding with Dow while others supporting the MDEQ. One of the stakeholder groups, the Tittabawassee River Watch, is establishing a class-action lawsuit against Dow to obtain medical monitoring of citizens living on the floodplain. Among stakeholders, the issue has become polarized and the level of distrust amongst the parties is high. There has been extensive media coverage of the issue within the region. Study design and data used The National Issues Forums (N IF) designed by the Kettering Foundation provide a deliberative format for decision making about complex and contentious social problems through structured choice-making (Mathews & McAfee, 2000). NIF facilitate decision- making by first naming and framing the issue for participants in a compact and objective 20 document: the “Issue Guide.” The Issue Guide then serves as the basis for a discussion of the tradeoffs between the example solutions to the problem. Deliberative discourse requires that citizens have knowledge of the basic elements of the public policy problem, the relationships among those elements, and about the consequences and tradeoffs associated with alternative policies. To achieve this, the study team prepared an Issue Guide on the Tittabawassee contaminated sediments issue. The document provided the study participants with a framework for sorting through the contaminated soils and sediments issue and the tradeoffs associated with alternative courses of action. Prior to writing the issue guide, the study team conducted an intelligence gathering exercise to gain an understanding of the range of perspectives on the Tittabawassee dioxin contamination and potential solutions. Face-to-face, in-depth interviews were conducted with representatives of stakeholders associated with the dioxin issue including representatives of the MDEQ, Dow, local governments, state and local environmental organizations and citizen groups that had formed because of the dioxin issue. The issue guide was developed based on information learned during the intelligence gathering efforts. The document attempted to set forth in a reader-friendly manner the Tittabawassee contamination problem, how the risk management process is used to make decisions, what possible courses of action might be taken, and what tradeoffs are inherent in each of the choices. Concerns beyond the level of risk from exposure to dioxin were compared for each outcome option including the ability to reduce dioxin exposure, role of govemment, financial cost, impact on floodplain 21 residents, impact on property values, and key challenges. Every effort was made to present the information in an unbiased manner so as to not lead the reader to any one of the possible solutions. The reader was encouraged to form his or her own solution to the problem. Issue forum participants were recruited from selected neighborhoods situated along the 22-mile stretch of the Tittabawassee River affected by contaminated sediments. Half of the selected neighborhoods were lower socio-economic while the remainder were middle class. Local residents who were not members of any recognized stakeholder groups associated with the Tittabawassee case were the target population because one of the goals of the research is to assess the NIF approach for engaging populations who are affected by the decision but who are frequently underrepresented in public decision making. A screen was developed to determine whether an individual met our definition of an “unaffiliated resident.” The target issue forum size was 6-10 to ensure enough people to generate a deliberative conversation but not too many that some individuals would stop engaging. The issue forums were organized by neighborhood with each participant attending one forum. Neighborhoods were determined by a minimum of 50 residences, aerial photographs to identify neighborhoods on or near the floodplain, census units for delineating boundaries, and windshield neighborhood visits to ground truth. The forums were conducted at neighborhood venues such as schools, church halls, and YMCAs. At each forum, a trained neutral moderator facilitated the discussion. The forums lasted approximately two hours. 22 Recruitment to the forums was done by knocking on doors in the study area. Recruitment by door knock is only practical for problems that are confined to a relatively small area. However, it was conducted both to improve the response rate and accuracy of the survey and also to encourage higher attendance at the forums. This recruitment method, while more intensive than mailed invitations, was employed to have a larger sample size and more representative sample than a mail survey. For these reasons it is a practical method for real world public decision making. An effort was made to reduce sampling error in canvassing neighborhoods. Every house in a neighborhood was approached. In an effort to reach people at home who may be working different shift schedules, each neighborhood was canvassed twice; doors unanswered during the first visit were knocked again later that day to attempt to reach those who may have been out during the first round of canvassing. To provide a basis for comparing forum participants to the larger population in the study area, a baseline survey was conducted in the area with as wide a sample as possible. The team of four interviewers canvassed the neighborhoods by knocking on each door and asking adult residents to answer a brief questionnaire. The survey instruments were pre-tested with a group of residents from a community outside of the study area. To increase consistency in data collection, the four interviewers who conducted the door-to-door surveys underwent a training workshop prior to working in the field and daily debriefings were conducted. To measure the effect of the issue forums on preferences and knowledge, the study participants were surveyed at three points in the study process. First, if respondents to the baseline survey expressed interest in participating in a forum, they were offered a 23 $25 gift certificate as incentive and as gesture of appreciation for their participation. Those who expressed an interest in participating were given an informed consent document to read and sign and the voluntary terms of their participation were explained verbally. Individuals who signed the consent document were asked a set of 14 supplementary questions; henceforth this is referred to as the pro-forum survey. After completing the pro-forum survey, each respondent was given a copy of the Issue Guide and was asked to read it prior to attending the issue forum. The second survey came at the end of each issue forum. The participants were given a brief questionnaire to complete before leaving the venue. For comparison purposes, many of the same knowledge and preference questions from the Neighborhood Survey were repeated. The self-administered questionnaire also sought feedback on their experience with the issue guide and forum. The third and final survey instrument was mailed one month after the respondent attended an issue forum. Once again, many of the same knowledge and preference questions were asked as in the earlier questionnaires. Participants self-administered the questionnaire and mailed it back to the researchers. The purpose of the third survey was to determine the stability of participants’ views, i.e. whether their views remained consistent immediately after the forum and a month later. My role in the larger study involved preparing for data collection, data collection, and data management. I helped to test the research instruments, make modifications to the surveys, provide training to other surveyors, prepare surveys and answer booklets, administer the surveys, mail the follow up surveys, and analyze the data. I assisted with 24 the issue forums by setting up the rooms, greeting participants, recording and displaying participant comments, and distributing self-administered surveys at the end of issue forum discussions. To recap, the data used in this thesis includes a baseline survey covering a wider sample within the study area; a three-step survey of the forum participants, and data collected during the forum itself on the process that each group took to arrive at a decision. Table 3.1 shows the sample sizes and the proportion of canvassed households participated in different stages of the study. Of the doors knocked, 46% did not come to the door, and 21% said “no” when asked if they would like to answer questions. The remaining 33% of the households in the neighborhoods answered the baseline survey. 14% of the doors knocked the resident agreed to participate in the forum and completed a pro-forum survey. This group excludes 3% of total households that were screened from the study because they were affiliated with groups that had vested interests in the clean- up decision. Many of the pre-forum survey respondents dropped out and did not actually attend the forum; a total of 5% of initial doors knocked resulted in a forum participant. 25 Table 3.1 Recruitment - participation at each stage of inquiry % of % of pre % of % of % of remaining forum baseline eligible to survey N doors doors 3 rv It r nd nts knocked Answered u ey comp e e espo e respondents pre forum suwgy Doors Knocked 1042 100 Doors Answered 550 53 100 Baseline Survey 336 32 61 100 Completed Screened* 30 3 5 9 100 Pre forum Survey 142 I4 29 42 46 100 Completed Attended a 51 5 9 15 17 36 Forum * Stakeholder group afliliation screen Each baseline respondent was asked if they, or anyone in their household is affiliated with a series of nine different stakeholder groups Michigan Department of Environmental Quality, an elected government official, the Lone Tree Council, Michigan Department of Community Health, the Dow Chemical Company (current or retired employee), the Tittabawassee River Watch, a member of a lawsuit against either Dow or the Department of Environmental Quality, Michigan Department of Agriculture, or the Tittabawassee Voice. Of the 37 8 asked, 19 indicated that they are involved in a lawsuit against Dow or the DEQ, 14 with Dow Chemical Company, and one respondent indicated involvement with Michigan Dept of Community Health. These individuals were not asked to participate in the research (“screened” from participation) because the research was targeting participation fi'om non-affiliated citizens. The purpose of the research was to examine a different type of engagement of citizens than is typically done. Usually stakeholder groups are assembled and target the most active citizens. However this research sought to understand the average citizen, sometimes referred to as the “silent majority.” To target these individuals the screen was developed and those already involved in the issue in some way were excluded. Neighborhoods surveyed Nine neighborhoods in the affected area were delineated for canvassing. Freeland Mobile Home (FMH) is the furthest upstream and close to Midland and not in a floodplain. Freeland Town (FLT) is also located in a non-flooding area in Titabawassee Township. F alconview Estates (F VB) is a modular home park with more elderly residents 26 located outside of the flooded areas. Two neighborhoods, McGliggot Road (MCG) and Gratiot and River Roads (GRR) flood frequently and have confirmed elevated levels of dioxins on their property. During the study many households in these neighborhoods had been contacted by Dow or the DEQ and were involved in a lawsuit against Dow or the DEQ. Four neighborhoods in the City of Saginaw North Saginaw (N SA), East Saginaw (ESA), Maple Street East (MSE), and Maple Street West (MSW), were canvassed. These neighborhoods are located along the Saginaw River, do not regularly flood, and are racially diverse. The river and floodplain downstream of a manufacturing source in Midland, on the Tittabawassee River is known to be contaminated with dioxins. Although not densely populated, there are residential neighborhoods throughout the floodplain area. Individual homes located along the river that were not clustered were not sampled because many of them were known to be involved in a lawsuit against the manufacturer or against the DEQ (stakeholder groups were excluded) and because of difficulty in sampling widely spaced houses. Nine neighborhoods more densely populated and in proximity to the contaminated site were chosen for the study. Each residence was canvassed and if they were not in a “stakeholder group” they were asked to participate in the study. The data were grouped by region based on proximity and demographic similarities (see table 4.1). The groups are: “Frequently Flooded” (FF), “Adjacent to Floodplain (AFP)” and “City of Saginaw” (SAG). Nine forums were held throughout the canvassed neighborhoods, some without any forums, and some with more than one. Two neighborhoods, East Saginaw (BSA) and 27 Freeland Town (FLT) did not have a forum because no residents showed up to the meetings. One neighborhood, Gratiot and River Roads (GRR) had two forums (GRRI and GRR2) because so many people from that neighborhood agreed to participate in a forum. The surveyed respondents from the two Maple Street neighborhoods (Maple Street East and Maple Street West) attended one of three Maple Street forums named Maple Street One (MSGl), Maple Street Two (MSG2), and Maple Street Group Three (MSG3). Combined, nine forums were held across seven of the surveyed neighborhoods. 28 CHAPTER 4: COMPARISON OF NEIGHBORHOODS TO FORUM PARTICIPANTS This chapter demonstrates how the forum participants are similar or dissimilar to non-participants and the general neighborhood population. Income, education, race, gender, knowledge, interests (reported as “other impacts”) and preferred solution are detailed in tables 4.], 4.2, 4.3, 4.4, 4.6 and 4.7. In addition, tables 4.5 and 4.8 illustrate the key knowledge responses and frequency of blocking statements within forums. Table 4.2 lists the reasons people cited when they declined to participate in a forum. The information is provided to illustrate some differences between participants and non- participants to understand to what degree forum participants were representative of the target population. Demographics of forum participants are compared to census data about the neighborhoods’ characteristics in Table 4.1. Income, percent earning a high school diploma, and percent white are listed for the different neighborhoods sampled where a forum was held. Median household incomes ranged from $13,250 to $65,200. Incomes of forum participants were on average somewhat lower than the median incomes of their respective neighborhoods, according to the census. MCG, GRRl, FVE, MSG2, and NSA forum participants had lower incomes than their respective neighborhoods and GRR2, MSGI , and MSG3 participants had higher incomes than their respective neighborhoods at large. The lower incomes of forum participants may be due to the fact that individual homes on large lots in some census tracts were not canvassed because they were not within the neighborhood. For example, the census tract that included Freeland mobile home park 29 had homes located along the river on large lots outside of the mobile home park that were not canvassed. In all of the neighborhoods, the percentage of adults with a high school diploma or higher was at least 77%. Some of the forums appear to have slightly higher graduation rates because no one was in attendance who did not have a high school diploma (MCG, FMH, and MSW). Likewise, according to the census, none of the neighborhoods were 100% white, although some of the forums were 100% white (MCG, FMH). Maple Street East, Maple Street West had the highest percentage of non-white participants. 30 Table 4.1 Comparison of income, education and racial composition of forum Jarticipants to neighborhoods Pre-forum survey Neighborhood Focus Group Geographic Demographic Census figures for forum location variable figures . . participants only M cGliggot McGliggot Frequently median income $59,896 Qi=8) $35,000 Road (MCG) Road (MCG) flooded % h-S- diploma 36 100 % white 96 100 Gratiot and median income $51,] 1 1 (n=7) $30,000 Riv(e(lj[l:|:£j()is l % h.s. diploma 87 86 Glam" and Frequently % white 96 86 River Roads flooded (GRR) Gratiot and median income 351,11 1 (:14) $62,500 River Roads 2 % h.s. diploma 87 100 (GRR2) % white 96 100 Median income $38,929 (n=l 1) $25,000 Falcon View Falcon View Adjacent to _ Estates (FVE) Estates (FVE) floodplain % 115- diploma 92 82 % white 95 91 Freeland Freeland Median income $65,200 (n=4) $20,000 M b'l H M b'l H Adjacent to . o ' e ome o ' e ome floodplain % h.s. diploma 90 100 (FM H) (F MH) % white 99 100 median income $53,173 Freeland No forum Adjacent to . No forum held Town (FLT) held floodplain %h-S- diploma 97 %white 97 . Median income $32,400 (n=4) $40,000 MSG] City of ' Saginaw % h.s. diploma 78 100 % white 64 50 Maple Streets Median income $32,400 (n=3) $15,000 East and MSGZ City of _ West (MSE) Saginaw % h.s. dlploma 78 66 (MSW) % white 64 67 f Median income $32,400 (n=4) $41,250 City 0 MSG3 Saginaw % h.s. diploma 78 100 % white 64 75 31 Table 4.1 Continued Pre-forum . . survey figures Neighborhood Focus Geographic Demographic Census for forum Group location variable figures . . participants only North North . median income $25,167 (n=6) $15,000 S , S . City of _ (:grfw (afilsrfiy Saginaw 0/0 h.S. diploma 77 83 % white 67 83 . , median income $13,250 East Saginaw No forum City of o , No forum held (ESA) held Saginaw A’h's' diploma 57 %white 8 Table 3.2 showed participation at each stage of the study. There were 378 people who started a baseline survey. Of those, thirty people were screened from participating because they or someone in their household reported that they were affiliated with one of the stakeholder groups identified on the baseline survey. The remaining reasons volunteered why they could not participate are listed in Table 4.2. The responses were open ended and were not solicited. Thirty people said that they could not attend because of an accessibility issue (they had to stay home because of being someone’s primary caregiver, they couldn’t physically get to the forum, etc). Twenty-eight people said they didn’t have time. Not everyone declining to participate in the study offered a reason, and some respondents offered more than one reason. 32 Table 4.2 Reasons for not participating in a forum Reason for not participating (30ml % and Screened for Affiliation 23(30) Caregiver, mobility issue, health, literacy or other disability. 23(30) Lack of time 22(28) “I’m not interested” & “moving” 13(17) Prefer to not get involved in things 9(12) Work conflict 6(8) “1 don’t have anything to offer” 2(3) Total * 1 00(128) * Total number of responses does not equal the number of respondents because some provided an answer and some did not. The surveyor did not ask for a reason. These were volunteered reasons after being asked if they would like to participate in a forum. Table 4.3 shows the participation rates of women compared to men. The third column, “Rate of participation in forums within groups” shows what percentage of men and women participated in their respective groups from those who were surveyed. For example, 17% of the men surveyed attended a forum while only 10% of women surveyed attended a forum. Of the 51 people attending a forum 30 of them were men and 21 were women. 33 Haw—a ab Goa—Elmo: o». 3:8 on moi—B 6:35:32. 5392. am 26842— ? ammo—mun 2:43. .x. 35 e\ BE wow—53o 38 om 5.8 e\.. om U 3. A5 Hon.— AS on 53-33:: Amy o». moi—B wanna—33m um wanna—come: «Eon—fie 3 2.3.3.2. non—iguana a @2638? o». no: mo moi—E <2..qu . 33:: . . . . can—:38 2.33 25.. 534.2qu newton: 1.95 manna—33 u a 38s mud—Gm»... Zorn A33 ficfiv $08 5s. 3 3:53 Gowv mafimwv 333 Ho. 3 H053. A331... SowaMV :56: _.. Emfiom 5 En 33.880 88 o». moan. pedomnmam mm m uoaogfimo om so: 68.39va ammvonmo 38: mm cane—Boa 3. ESQEM 90 R82: 0». mow—5D 333358 3. En page: om so: most: 3383:? m3 oxmBEo m2. Ea :55 3S. GA e\e mm 85$an 3. EF ammvosmom GmmV 8%Wwwom GS Rmvoamam Ma: 88382: cm 5838mm Eli A223 aomvonaga $50 $323 .529 awed o». Ba 5%.. 8633 $63 so” ~5on 305 99., rues—mama .95 39: :5 $28. Em: Ed :38 3A3 _ K 3 32:55 2va ANCNS 303V _ C a hos 33v #253 mu: 8 ma 5 HOH>H... QumV Sofimov H838 5.08; <5 3850 Emma“: :55ch cm 303a muméanaa 959.2: 95303 Ea moan @3303 8:5 :98 808 Sun 050 Rmvonma. Eases—m $30 £53 83033 93 SQ runs; :85 om Em mmmco 226 no” gram 8 88mm 90:. _n$35K), and 100% of participants had a high school diploma. MSGl had similar demographics but a not enough information decision. Freeland Town (FLT) was an exception. Freeland Town had high levels of education and income, but low levels of participation (no forum held). They are adjacent to the floodplain, potentially explaining their low levels of participation. However respondents from FLT expressed high levels of awareness of the issue as compared to the City of Saginaw neighborhoods (Table 4.5), only one person surveyed answered “no” to “have you heard of the issue?” Also, with higher education levels and higher incomes, the barriers present in attending were less in Freeland Town than for neighborhoods where people have fewer resources such as in Saginaw. When surveyed residents from that neighborhood reported higher than average comments that they “don’t like to get involved in things” and that talking about the issue causes people anxiety. Those comments suggest perhaps a lack of desire to confront an emotionally difficult problem. Freeland Town is also the neighborhood in closest proximity to the City of Midland, where the company responsible for the dioxin contamination is located. The desire to avoid the problem to avoid conflict may be stronger among residents in the Freeland Town neighborhood, especially since they do not flood and are therefore not likely to face direct exposure to the contaminants. 87 Education, income and salience (including location) do not provide adequate explanation about the partially developed (mid quality) decisions. These decisions came from across all three neighborhood groupings (frequently flooded, City of Saginaw, and adjacent to floodplain). The partially developed decisions also had higher numbers of participants (n) (Table 5.14). One explanation for lower quality decisions may be re the numbers: perhaps it is more difficult to come to consensus the more people trying to make a decision. However, upon investigation, a comparison of the factor assessments across groups does not show a pattern that it is lack of agreement on a decision that caused the poor quality decision, although that does seem to be somewhat of a factor for all three partially developed decisions. A clearer pattern is observed when making individual factor assessment comparisons across the groups: the presence of 0’s for certain factors signal lower quality individual decisions. All the individuals with good decisions had no unresolved tradeoffs (0’s) and all individuals expressing unresolved tradeoffs (0’s) had partially developed decisions. Similarly, for most individuals scored 0’s for knowledge gain or preference change, the individual score was less than “good.” The exception was preference change. Several individuals scored a 0 for preference change, but had an overall individual assessment of good. Also, the presence of “blocking statements” may explain why some groups were unable to develop quality decisions. Table 4.8 shows that with the exception of NSA, forums with partially developed decisions (GRRI, NSA, FVE) had higher frequencies of “anti-solution attitudes” (blocking statements) expressed by their participants on the baseline survey. Blocking statements include 7 categories of comments expressing negative attitudes for reaching a solution such as “nothing can be done” and “talking 88 about it causes stress.” Two of the three forums with partially developed decisions (GRRI and F VE) had 11 and 12 “anti-solution” attitudes respectively. Alternatively, the three groups with good decisions (MCG, GRR2, and MSG3) had far fewer “anti- solution” attitudes with 6, 3, and 2 respectively. The exception to this trend was North Saginaw (NSA) where only one anti solution comment was expressed. The individual assessment table (5.12) and associated quotes (Appendix) provides insight into why NSA was unable to develop a good decision. The reason for the lower decision score for NSA was that for one participant (#176) the decision wasn’t stable. He or she went back to their original view that dredging is too difficult to be successfirl. Another participant (#196) was given a low individual score because his or her final questionnaire response was not specific. If the decision were more firm for these participants then this would have been a good quality decision. In summary, proximity to known contaminated areas, income and education, resolution of tradeoffs, saliency, barriers to participation, and presence of “anti-solution” attitudes, are some observed associations with low rates of participation and lower quality decisions. Higher levels of salience, higher levels of income and education, mobility, resolution of cross pressures, and lower levels of anti-solution attitudes, are associated with higher response rates and quality decisions. 89 CHAPTER 6: CONCLUSION Democratic governance depends on effective public participation. Contaminated. sites clean-ups, siting of landfills, and location of new highways are among the more challenging public policy problems. Engagement is needed, but working with the public is often emotionally challenging due to the possible health impacts on the people living near the site. Although risk managers have been advised to incorporate public participation early and often when developing risk management policy, too often the public involvement efforts fail to result in satisfying policy outcomes (Gregory 2002). Some of the problems with existing options include the perceived amount of time and energy required to engage the public, loss of control of the decision, and difficulty in engaging the silent majority. Recent literature on public involvement suggests practitioners employ deliberative methods and actively seek participation of underrepresented populations. The larger study tested the National Issues Forums for developing decisions about a contaminated site. This thesis assessed the quality of the group decisions that resulted from the deliberations. The first part determined the extent to which forum participants were representative of the sampled neighborhoods. From the data collected and the low numbers it is difficult to assess definitively, but it appears that people with certain characteristics participated at higher rates than others. Those living in the contaminated floodplain and those with more knowledge about the issue participated at higher rates than those living outside of the contaminated area or who have no knowledge about the issue. People who reported that they heard about the issue from Dow or DEQ mailings, or from a public meeting (survey did not differentiate if the public meeting was about this 90 issue) also participated at disproportionately higher rates than residents reporting gaining knowledge of the issue from other sources of information such as newspaper and word of mouth. Alternatively, people who reported that they heard about the issue from TV or radio participated at lower rates. Some barriers to participation reported by survey respondents included lack of time, need to stay home to be a caretaker, lack of transportation or other mobility problems. Conflict avoidance and concern about openly sharing their views about what should be done was also expressed by some residents. Two of nine neighborhoods had no forum because no showed up both outside of the flooded areas. One neighborhood in the frequently flooded area had two forums because so many residents expressed interest. Survey respondents expressing blocking views when asked about what should be done or other impacts participated at higher rates than respondents expressing altruistic concerns, and problem solving attitudes such as views that dioxin harms the environment or impacts land uses, or that the solution is to raise awareness about the issue or regulate the problem. Participants were more likely than non participants to be opposed to dredging. To assess the quality of the group decisions, a decision tool was built with 6 factors identified in the literature from Yankelovich, Gregory, Jasanof, Slovic, Webler and Tuler. Three focus groups developed good decisions, three came to partially developed group decisions, and three groups did not have enough information for a complete assessment. The decisions from the frequently flooded neighborhood forums tended to be of good quality while decisions from the adjacent to floodplain neighborhood forums tended to be of low quality or were not held. The decisions from the Saginaw neighborhood forums were spread across the quality spectrum. 91 The assessment tool combined individual factor scores of 1 (yes), 0 (no) and n.a. (not enough information). Consensus or personal agreement with the group decision, and stability of the decision were the two major factors contributing to a decision assessment. Despite a mathematical possibility in the assessment, certain lesser factors (consensus, knowledge change, preference change, and resolution of cross pressures) signaled lack of developed individual decisions when the factors were assessed 0 (for “no”). For example, where tradeoffs are unresolved, individuals did not learn during the forum individuals do not develop quality decisions. The expression of blocking views also signaled difficulty reaching agreement. One implication of this finding is that the decision assessment tool can be useful for helping practitioners charged with helping groups make decisions, identify issues that need further exploration. For example, where “0’s” were observed for resolution of tradeoffs, practitioners can look back at the data and record the critical concerns that need to be overcome before participants may be ready to come to a decision. In this case, the forums raised a number of critical concerns regarding action to clean up the Tittabawassee River and floodplain. For example participants were sometimes blocked from coming to a lasting decision to dredge because of their concern about contaminating downstream residents. On the other hand, participants were blocked from a comfortable decision of doing nothing because of unresolved concerns about people remaining exposed to dioxin. This type of analysis can yield valuable knowledge about the public’s views. It is significant for practitioners also that not a lot of data must be gathered to get quality insight. Simply asking an open ended question (important) about people’s 92 preferred solution before, after and one month following their involvement in a deliberation can provide useful information. One of the challenges faced by risk managers that this study attempted to find a solution to is that of engaging and learning about the interests of the “silent majority.” Personal, face to face invitations to forums being held in their neighborhoods at a time convenient to them, and offering a $25 gift card, did not appear to be enough to get those other than for whom the issue is most salient, to participate. One risk of engaging a non- representative sample (people with views differing from the general population) is that it is difficult to determine if the outcomes will serve the interests of those who did not participate. The implication for future research is that more it may be necessary to do more to attract people to participate. It may be important to raise awareness of the problem in areas with lower literacy and higher poverty rates. Providing child care, transportation, or recruitment by community members, known and trusted by residents may also yield additional participation for underrepresented groups. Perhaps most importantly for improving citizen participation in decision making is for the agency to demonstrate taking the citizens views and preferred solutions into serious account in deciding a clean-up plan of action. Finally, settling on assessment criteria can be difficult because assessing outcomes is inherently subjective. One implication of this tool is that it could be replicated and used in other settings to evaluate group decisions and identify issues that need further definition. The tool can help groups know when deliberations can cease (a stable agreement is found), and take action on the chosen course. On the other hand, when a decision is poorer, it can help practitioners identify those issues and attitudes that 93 need work to come to a decision. The assessment tool could possibly be adapted by interchange other factors that the researcher or practitioner may wish to include. One implication is that practitioners wishing to use an abbreviated method to reduce transaction costs associated with group decision making, can reduce their data collection to just one open ended question asked at three points in time. Agencies need manageable public involvement methods that yield quality group decisions. The engagement approach outlined in this thesis and the group decision assessment tool gains ground toward the goal of developing methods that are usefirl for public involvement in risk assessment and other difficult collective decision making scenarios. Research limitations & validity A limitation of the study is that even though forum participants were reasonably representative of their neighborhoods’ population, a representative sample does not imply that the general public will be satisfied with the conclusions of the citizens in the issue forums. Participating in the process may be a critical component to resolving cross- pressures, so others may not be satisfied with the decision if they did not go the decision making process, because they themselves did not think through the tradeoffs between the various scenarios to come to group consensus. The research artificially excludes special interest groups from the dialogue. In a real clean-up decision making process, special interest groups would likely participate and from this research we can’t predict how more polarized activist participants would have affected the decision outcomes. Our design deviated from what would likely occur 94 in an open process where more powerful stakeholders would have access to manipulating the situation. The purpose is to gain insight about the effectiveness of the NIF for cases where the tradeoffs are difficult and decision-makers wish to gain reliable information about the affected public’s interests and concems. Generalization to a particular population is not possible with purposeful sampling and small samples (Patton, 2002). From this study we can explore the variation within this community, investigating the nuance and potential patterns within the case. Credibility of the research conclusions is sought through rigor of data collection and analysis, triangulation of data and analysis, and by making researcher biases explicit. During data collection, an effort was made to minimize reactivity. During the survey data collection, the survey instruments remained the same and surveyors spoke with each other to check that they were conveying the same information in the same manner. Possible influences of differences in appearances such as gender, age, or race could not be controlled for, but an attempt was made to reduce variation by wearing uniform University shirts, wearing identifying name badges, and approaching homeowners in a friendly and professional manner. The affiliation with Michigan State University aided in being perceived by residents as a neutral and credible organization. Different residents perceived surveyors in different ways: a few residents expressed suspicion that surveyors were undercover spies for Dow or that we represented environmental groups. Neutrality was attempted, but residents’ preconceptions affected their reaction to researchers. For example, during forums, maintaining the neutrality of the facilitators was attempted by not voicing personal opinions, and attempting to treat all 95 responses the same. Similarly, when administering surveys, surveyors avoided expressing their opinions or validating the views of respondents. An attempt was made to minimize participant reactivity to the survey by asking questions worded in the same fashion at different times and asking the same questions of all participants. In addition to these measures seeking to minimize participant reactivity, researchers followed rigorous and systematic data collection procedures. Issue forum groups were audio and video tape recorded and the facilitator used the same semi- structured format for each issue forum. 96 APPENDIX 400.0 5 2.me 3202.2. mega: 0:080 93:0 00210:" 9.030 :3 «003. 260.3 82.3 0:0 3.9.26.3 0<2 :30 Mmmm $02.00 5309.20? 2H2 H.033 0:0 30:3 22 _ 35x 50< 0:050 00:00:56 0: :0H Who—“mmnflmmfioflw .H = $0 00on - 226.0 503 0 0% 3:00. 30: . . . 0.0 m 2: 03.: m 52. 520: 8 000 a 50 _0<0_0 200 0<2 m 3:050 50 :2 00on 0:0 20850 503 00 0308200 :00? 20.0 00:00 2 :30 0:0 : 30< 00:; 50: 5. 0:0 000 a 520 .0 m:< 200. ._.:0: no :03 :m<0 H0 0 0 00305.8 00305.8 320 - ocH _ 55x 2 <0: :2 H30 520. 0005 z. ”2 00on 0<2 0:0 0<2 50 60.0 20:5 60 0.00: z :0. <<:0H0<2 .H 900 0 :0H 00on 0:0 .2 :220 mem :0 :m 8.2 8 002 z :0. 85.00. 9080 so :04 00040. _H 00030 8 30 52 30 002 00.5.0: 52 2080 30 20mm ":2 0003 H0 £20 £050 00 0 00320350 8 :06 2.0520 HNN 25.00. ":0 :6302 00:00:42.0:0 0:0 06% 30 000<0 302.0:00 H0020. 0:0: mm .20. 3000 20mm 0<2 m 02.00 2 :30 226.8 50 2000 2 H30 :<2 50... 20 030.0200 8 00 30 302 00:500. 00:00::20 0: H30 :32 0006... 950 0030 8:: 2 00305303 200 0: 50 :9 30:22 0: 00300300 0: 10622 30 :2 0.0on 52 £050 00 00on H0 :06. .000 30. 2.200 :0 0.05:0 .30 ":0 :<2 is 000000. 5520 <0: 0050 000 30 302 0202020 2.03 003:0 58 30 m: 2 83:2 2258 30:22 30 «00.0030 0:006. 00S: :<2. 2230 50 :6: 030032.30 0: m <02? 2 502? .05. 0:0 «0020 202000 _: 3000 2000 $520 050%000 £050 00 302 3200020. 97 400.0 >0 2.00 040002.00 moan: 0:080 0300100000? 000: 00000 00300... 330.00 :0. 00100 006300.303. .00. 0: 02. 0600a 00.20 00382.00 000.0 0000 #3 m000...:0 3300.030? 0.401033 0:0 303: .00: 0033103603 :00 8 :9... 00<0.00303. .0. :0 :05. ”muwmwmwmoflflwwmfifiwmg :0300. 0030. 000000 0300 00. 0000.00 <3: :05. 0003 : 0a 0 .00 0.. 003 .0 > 00303000: 0. :0:¢0..~.:0 3:0 00.. 0:0 0:03.00. #003303 0.. 30 00.. 0:0 200.. 53.0: .2... 00 w. w o 000 x - £0. a 0 3 0:0: 0 3... 0 : 0 ~22 s3: :05. 6030.00.00. 0:03.000 0<0..00_0 .: 0 .05 .000 .0 3:0 300. 000. 0200320 :_0«0 00.2 o\o .: 0.0:: 0 0:0 0:0 0: 000: 00000 00300: 30300 0. 000.30 53: 3:0 030.03. 203 0030 .2030.“ 30.30.3000: 0:203... .: ”:0 0000 0:0 000.0 0000 . 00000 0.. 0030 300.0030 .30 030 0200.00. . . . . m.3:0.. .03 :30 0.00: 3:0 080.03 .5000 wmu Wm...“ .mofithmMWMwnw: HMS..N.:0 0:03.000 00: 00 08<0: 23:30 0:050 00 00:0. 00305000: 0.. 00302 0:0 30300000 wwo 00:4 .30.... 300330 0. 0:03.000 0:0 3.062002030 20350. 00300“ 53: 00.000: "000:0 0.. 00.00.00. 0:0 .: 030.. 0300 20. ..30 003:0 s3: :.0: 0:03.00. 00033030 8 000.? 03003000? . 00..0<00 <3: 30.0 .303000: 0: 0.00 04000 3:03 3:0 . . . . . . . 0:03.00. 0.00: :0 .2000 00 0003. ._.:0: 0030.: 0:0 ww. 0.00: ...0 ":0 30... 0.00030 0:03.00. ...3: 320 58:30:09 «00.000 ":0 00.. 0303.1: “00. 003.0. 0600 530.0 000.0 0030 : 00303 <3: ":0 00.. 0<0Q 000.. > 00303000: 0." 000: 00000 00302 0:0 0000.0 0:050 00 30.0 :030..~.:0 0.90:0 <.0 0:03.000 0:2 . . . . wwm 0.206 0.. 3:0 00:0.. 00:4 ":0 ”00:300.. :00 000: 33:0. <<03.:0:c0d._ 3% "”3300" "mwwfidm3 :0w. 000: 00:03 .30.: 0050000 0:0 3030 .0 .302: 0003 ":0 «0000.0 0: 0: 000 < _ 0 0 08 03. 0.00 0.4000. :0<0 :00303 00:00 0<<0< .403 .:0 :<0: Z0<0.. 0030 03.330 000.? ._.:0<.<0 00. uwu .0 03<0 3:03 .0 0: 00:0. 0: 03. 00. 0 0 I z> ...:0< 00:." :0<0 03. .000. 00s. .0..00 ":0 :<0: <<00 00 000 .3 £00.03 .400N0. 98 400.0 >0 00...... 0.000 0000.03 000: 00000 00300.. :0:..0..~0 :0E000 003300.003.“ .00. 0: 030.000.... 00.0.0 :030 2.00 000.0 0000 0. 0000.30 33030.0? 0.40.. .033 0:0 30:... 0.0.. _ .:.:x .:0 0.00 0:050 00 0.00:00 :0. :030 .:0 .003 .. 00. 00 3330. 2033.030 0:03.000 0. :00. 0.0:0 5...: 30300000 03:00.0. .:0 :00 0. .:0.30_ $030.00.. 000 00:.. 00.0.0. .0. :0.c.0 .0x0 000: 00000 00300. ...00 0:0 0000.02 ..000 .0 333.00 .:0 0.90:0 0. 0000.0... 0:03.00. 003 0. .. .: ..30 00:..0. 30000.00. :0:..0_.N.:0 00030. _. .:000 0000.0._...00 :0<0 000: .0300 0:0 :0<0 ...._0 0. :0 003.00 0.00 0.4000. 00.0:000 :030 00 00: 303 .33 830330.00 0.00 00... ...00. 0.. :0300. 003. 00... z 0:. 3.0 0 0. 0x.: 5...: 000: 00000 00300.. 0. 0 0:00 .0 00. 0 00300300 :030 _: 0 :0:-0..00.00 0.00: :3. 0.00. N- .0. 0.00: :0 0.. 0.000000 00.:..0:0 :000 .0 00 30030300. 99 ...00.0 >0 0mm» 03.02.00 00.5.0: 000.00 0.000 000.033 0.0000 00. 000.0 3 ..<0. 0. 00000.03. ..000 00:20.. 30:3 30.00:.0 000.:0. ..3000. 000 .02 00:00:..0..0: 0.000 0000 0. 0000.30 3300.03? 0.0.0: .033 000 300.0 .03., .u .000 0033.. 5.030... 05:00.. . 00.3: 0. 30:3:00 0. 00.00300. rdwmjmowmm. finnfifim 0. 00.0 0< 00s. .. :0. .:.0.00.00 3 0.00: :0 0.00 .03. 0.000 0.. 330<30 000 00 0.0 0. 0 0 «..0 0:. 0:. 0 305:0. 00:..0. 0. 20000.03 :00 00:.03.:0..0:. 0.000 00030 .0 0.00 20.0. .00 gm. . 0 0 5...: 0 8300:0082 000x000 .0 031.02 .00. 200.0 00000 300:.0330..0:. . :0. 0.30. 0 00.00:.0 <<0< 0. 03000333030. 0.00: :0 00030 0.300. 20.2 .00: .. E0 03 0:3 0. 0:0 . 000 3000033. 00:.. .392 30c... m0.0:._0: 00:00. O0..00..<0 0.003 300... 00:..0. ...00030 0:0 0300.:0 0.0.. . ~00 00:0.:0.0:. 00:.. .302 00:00:..0..0: 0.000. 3000.000. 0030.0.0 0.00: :0. 0.00 00.2 ..03 03.030 000 03.330. .. 0000:.. 0.000 0033. 0:0 0.00: :0 ._.0.0_ 0.00: :0. 30x0 00:00 .00. 00$. 00: 0.... 0:30. 100 ...00.0 >0 .u 0.00 0.. 0.00 0.00000 0.0.30 5...: 0.000 0.0.: . . . .<.0:00030:. 0.0030030 .0 303.... 5.3.0 305 0x.0. 0:0 30.3 0.4030 .0 A ”“4“” 0 ”M0JM030..0._..0..._0..“MM0 :0<0. 00.000. >00000 0<0.0 0x000:3 .0 303. 00:33 .0 .00:0 00:0 ...0. 0 : 0 0. . 0.0::30 0:0 .30.... .0 .00::0.00.00.-0.0_00.00_ 3000.0: 30.:000 .0 . . 3303. 0. 0.9.30 0. :0003 0.90: 00:.03.:0..0:. 300000.... 003030 5..0.03300.0..0: 0.00000. .. .. :00 .0 00 33030 ..03 .:0 :3. 00.83 _ . . . 5.050 ...3 .0 000 .:0 :3. 30.30.00 .0 .:0 moire ”mmn:000w.0 0030 03:0 00033:. 30:00 0x000:.0. ..000 0033.. 000: 00000. 0.0000 :0. 000.0 0:0 5.0.. .0. 0.0.0300.0..0: .0. 30 ..0000.03 5.3: 03:00.0 0000... 00.03 .033 >3. 0:0 303: .03.. 0. 0000.30 3300.03... 0.3. .033 0:0 303: .03. 0.. <<:0.0<0. .0 :00000. 0.0.0300.0..0: .......%..mw0 0. 3.030.002030 .0 0.00.. 005.: 30 20330 00: 00. 0.00 30 . 0:.000 .. .0 0 <0.< 0.00. :00_.: 0.00.03 . 33.. .. 00:00. ..003 3300 00 30< 0.0 03335900050 00 .0.. 0.0:0. . _ 00:. 33.. 303 .0 ..:0. 0:0 . .0 00:. ..:05. 00.33:. ._.:0. _. 00: 00 0.00:00 :0. ...:0< :000 .0 00 0.3.30 wo:....w.w..0 Immw.00.WM<0<:00_MMM.M 0.0000 :0 0.. 003033030 00..0 0:0 300 00:...0 00 3 0 : 5...: 53 0 2.0.0 0.:0< :0000 .0 00 00:0. 000.3030. 0.00000 0.3.0 5003 3 0 00.0 mm .0 .M< 0. 00 30 30403 .. < 0000.0 0350 0.0< 0<<0< .33 0.30330 5.0<. 00 :0. .0 00303303 0:< 30.0030 00:. 00 0 0 003.0330 30 0.00. 0.:0. 030. .. 00s. 00:.0 5.0... 0: 0 0:03.00. . . . 5.0 5. 0 3 0. 0 0: 0. . 0: 0 00.3.0: 5.0.0: 00:.0 0.00: :0 30 :3.. 0.00: 5.0.0.. _..0.0 0. 30.30. 0000.0 0: 30 . 00 ..3. 03:3 00 30.0 ”.00..“0M3 :3. 0:0 0.< 0 33.30. 0 . 00 00:.. _.:05.. 0.0.0300.0..0: 0.< 030030. 0 .. 0.0 _. :0 :05. 0000 00 <0: 20 . 33.. 0.000.:0 .0 30 5.0< .0 00. 0%... 102 ...00.0 0> 20> 033.30 00.33: 0330 0.0:0 000.03? 0033. .3003? 0.. 0.00%0. 0.0.0300.0..0: 0: .00 00000.03. 0000 .0 0000.30 3300.03... 0.3. 32.3 0.3 303... .03. 0... 0.000.:0 0:0 00:..0. .00 3000.030. 0... 0.00030 0:... .. <<:03 .0 .. 00330 .33... 030 .0030 03.. m 30300.0 0:00.. . . . . .mo :05. 5.0.0 0. 0.00000. 0:0 003.. 3. m ...0 .00: 0:::0.. >_00 “mmfiwwgco: 0. 030030 5...: :05. 0.00: :0 5.00. .0 .003. 0.0.0300.0..0: 3. .00 00.. .0 0.. 003 .00 .0300 .00. 0.0 0.300. 033333300. .300. 0.0. . . 0... 0.000.:0 .. .00 0000.0 33.30 3 .00 mm. M00 0:30.:0. 3.3. 5.03.. 0.. 0.000.:0. 0... 0.000.:0 00: 00 000.00 :0 3 0330.3 00: :0 ..000. P00. 2.0..0 0:3 .003 .0 80$ .30 0.. .00 .00 0 . 03000.00 ..3. 0:0 0:. 0 0030. 0.00.03 005. 0003.00. :00 00:000 0:00 0:. 0:05.. . . . . . _: 00.5.00: 0:0 0:. 00:0 03.... 00 _: 0:30:0. 0030.0. 5.03.. 00.. 030.030 5.0 0.0 :0<30 0.0::0 00.0. ..003 5.0.. 0:0:0: 0.0:0 0:0 30.3 0000.0 ..:0.03:0 .0 .03.. .00. 00: 0030 .500 0. ..000 0033.. 0.. 05.03 0. .00 00:03: 00 .00. 00:.0 000000 ...0 00. .. 0:. 0. .003. 0....30 .. 0.000.:0 0:0 0.0.00.00. 0. 5.00.00. .0 ..3 0: 0. :00. 0. :00 .00 0.00 :0 5.... 00.000 .. .0 0.00. 0003.00. :00 3 0: 000:. 3. 30303:. . .03.. .00 3303. .000 .0 ..0..00. 00.003. 0300.... 30000.03 00000.0... 3 .00 00000.03 0.000. 00303 .. 0:0 0.00 .. .33 00.:0 ..m 20 0:05.00 3.30.. <<:003. .0 0. 3:... 005. 0:0 mm» 0.03 003.:303. 0:050 20 30. .00 0... .0 0.00: .. :0. 103 ...00.0 >.. 0.00.. 033.30 00.53: 0:030 03:0 000.03? 3.33... 3.00000: 3 :.0: 003033000: 0300. 5.00.0.0 0.00. 00 :0330 0.005.003 0:. 00 30:..0. :000: 0000 .0 0000.30 3303.03... 0.3. 333 0:0 30:... .03. 0:30. 030.. .0 000 .. .003 .0 0 0.00 00.. 0. 00:00. .0 mum 0 0:.. .3 0 5. 0:30. 0.3.. 0. .00 000.3 .3000. 003.0 0000.0. _. 30.0 .0 0 ..0... .00: 0000.00. 0000.0.0 0.00: . 30<.:0. 0.000.:0. 0.0. :0 0. 8300:0000: .0 .0000 0.3030 00. 3.30.0 503 ::.:000.0 0.00 3:0. 3300.03... 303 0000.0 0:. o. 0.00 . . . 0.00... 00:.03.:030 0:0 0.. 0300.... 0:.. 0:. 0300.... 3.000003 0:0 30:03. 02:00:00.3: N00 “Mm.” mwmmmmflfimflfiflmmmw. 000:.0 00 .0030 302.03.... 0. 0.33:. 0:0 003.030 000.3 0.3.. 0. 30.0030 0:0 .00.. . 000.00. 00 000.3 030.00 .00.... 0.. 00:03:. 30.00:.0 0:0 00.00.30. ..:0 5.0.. .00.. 0.0 00.00 .0 0.. ..00. - - 00.:0 .00 080... . mum ”wwfiwhwfimm ”mafia.“ ”NW. m<0.:03 000.3 :0.. 0: 0000.0 0.0.. <<00. 2000 30.0 0:05.03 .0 005. 0000.0 0:0 5.00.0.0 0.0 00. 0 ..00. 3 .0 0 0... 0. 0 .0 0. .03. 00: 5.0 ..3 503.. <<0.. 0:0 000. 0.3030. 30.. 0.35.00 30 30:..0. _ 000.03 0.. .00.. 000.0000. . . . .oo ... 0000 0.0.: .0 00 .0... 0.0:0. <00... ...0 .0303 003033000: 3 000030 0.000 0:0 003. ~00 0.00: _. :0. 10030: 0000:. 00000.03 .0 00 0.00:00 .5 :0:-0000 0.000. .00.. 5.0:. .0.. ..0: .. :0. 0:.0 NNO 5.03 003033030. 00:. 20 20 ..:05.. _ 000.. .03.. .00.. 00: 00 03.330. _. 5.0:.0 00 .00 N00 000.... <<00. 0.0 .00.. 00.:0 .0 20 20 00. 303 :0 0:.... N00 .503 0.3.00. 00. 0x00 .03. 00300. 3300. m:0:3 000.3 030.00. 0:05.003. 00:. 0:0 005.. 053.0 0.0 0030.030 104 BIBLIOGRAPHY Abel, T. D., & Stephan, M. (2000). The limits of civic environmentalism. American Behavioral Scientist, 44(4), 614-628. Abelson, J ., Eyles, J., McLeod, C. B., Collins, P., McMullan, C., & Forest, P. G. (2003). Does deliberation make a difference? Results from a citizens panel study of health goals priority setting. Health Policy, 66(1), 95-106. Amstein, S. R. (1968). A ladder of citizen participation. Journal of the American institute of planners, 35(4), 216-225. Arvai, J., & Gregory, R. (2003). Testing alternative decision approaches for identifying cleanup priorities at contaminated sites. Environmental Science & Technology, 37(9), 1469-1476. Beierle, T. C. (2002). The quality of stakeholder-based decisions. Risk Analysis, 22(4), 739-749. Council, N. R. (1996). Understanding Risk: Informing Decisions in a Democratic Society. Washington, DC: National Academy Press. Diebel, A. Chibucos, M. & Beckwith, J. (2005). Dioxin Contamination of the Tittabawasssee River and Flood Plain: Overview of Issue Forum Outcomes. East Lansing: Michigan State University. EPA, M. S. U. U. (2003). Dioxin Contamination and the Tittabawassee River and Floodplain: Managing the Risks: An Issue Guide. Fischer, F. (2000). Citizens, Experts, and the Environment: The politics of local knowledge. Durham and London: Duke University Press. Gregory, R. (2000). Using stakeholder values to make smarter environmental decisions. Environment, 42(5), 34-44. Gregory, R. S. (2002). Incorporating Value Trade-offs into Community-Based Environmental Risk Decisions. Environmental Values, 1 I, 461-488. Habermas, J . (2006). Political Communication in Media Society: Does Democracy Still Enjoy an Epistemic Dimension? The Impact of Normative Theory on Empirical Research. Communication Theory, 16(4), 411-426. Jasanoff, S. (1991). Acceptable evidence in a pluralistic society. In D. G. Mayo & R. D. Hollander (Eds), Acceptable Evidence: Science and Values in Risk Management (pp. 29-47). New York: Oxford University Press. 105 Kelly, T. (2004). Unlocking the Iron Cage Public Administration in the Deliberative Democratic Theory of J iirgen Habermas. Administration & Society, 36(1), 38-61. Kothari, B. C. a. U. (Ed). (2001). Participation the new tyranny? London: Zed Books. Lachapelle, P., McCool, S., & Patterson, M. (2003). Barriers to effective natural resource planning in a "messy" world. Society & Natural Resources, 16(6), 473-490. Mathews, D. (1994). Politics for People. Urbana: University of Illinois Press. Mathews, D., & McAfee, N. (2000). Making choices together: the power of public deliberation. Kettering, OH: Ruffolo Press. McComas, K. C. W. S. (1998). Reassessing Public Meetings as Participation in Risk Management Decisions. Risk: Health, Safety & Environment, 9, 347-360. Patton, M. Q. (2002). Qualitative Research & Evaluation Methods (3 ed.). Thousand Oaks: Sage Publications. Pellizzoni, L. (2003). Uncertainty and Participatory Democracy. Environmental Values, 12, 195-224. Putnam, R. D. (1995). Bowling Alone: America's Declining Social Capital. Journal of Democracy, 6(1), 65-78. Renn, O. W., Wiedemann, P. (Ed.). (1995). Fairness and competence in citizen participation: Evaluating models for environmental discourse. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers. Slovic, P. (1997). Public perception of risk. Journal of Environmental Health, 59(9), 22- 23. Smith, G. (2003). Deliberative democracy and the environment. London, New York: Routledge. Spano, S. (2001). Public Dialogue and Participatory Democracy. Cresskill, New Jersey: Hampton Press, Inc. Thompson, P. (2004). Seminar on Ethical Considerations in Risk Assessment. Webler, T., & Tuler, S. (2000). Fairness and competence in citizen participation: theoretical reflections from a case study. Administration & Society, 32(5), 566- 595. Webler, T., Tuler, S., & Krueger, R. (2001). What is a good public participation process? Five perspectives from the public. Environmental Management, 27(3), 435-450. Yankelovich, D. (1991). Coming to public judgment: making democracy work in a complex world (1 st ed.). Syracuse: Syracuse Universrty Press. 106 utillliltiifllflljlllllflljitl