m ! u h h. h. m ! “‘IV‘P‘Hflh LIBRARY Michigan State University This is to certify that the thesis entitled Factors Affecting How Individuals Explain Their Behavioral Intentions to Others presented by Hye Jeong Choi has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for the MA. degree in Advertising, PR & Retailing W Major Professor’s Signature 07/3 0/02008 Date MSU is an affirmative-action, equal-opportunity employer PLACE IN RETURN BOX to remove this checkout from your record. TO AVOID FINES return on or before date due. MAY BE RECALLED with earlier due date if requested. DATE DUE DATE DUE DATE DUE 5/08 K.IProj/Acc&Pres/CIRCIDaIeDue indd FACTORS AFFECTING HOW INDIVIDUALS EXPLAIN THEIR BEHAVIORAL INTENTION TO OTHERS By Hye .Ieong Choi A THESIS Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment ofthe requirements for the degree of MASTER OF ARTS Department of Advertising. Public Relations & Retailing 2008 ABSTRACT FACTORS AFFECTING HOW INDIVIDUALS EXPLAIN THEIR BEHAVIORAL INTENTION TO OTHERS By Hye Jeong Choi Previous research mainly investigated which ofattitudinal and normative determinants would be more prominent in terms of predicting behavioral intention. By taking an approach drastically different from previous TRA research, this thesis focused on how attitude toward a behavior and perceived norms would influence individuals’ intentions to use various reasons when explaining their behavioral intention to others. Also, this study examined how different cultures would influence people’s citation of the same components which were related to behavioral intention. The study did not show that one culture always use attitudinal components to explain individuals’ behavior intention while the other always use normative components to explain individuals‘ behavior intention. It provided the evidence that TRA components would influence differently depending on the type of behavior. Furthermore, the relationships that attitudinal and normative components had with behavioral intention to engage in behaviors were not the same for the relationship that attitudinal and normative determinants had with intentions to use attitude-related reasoning and norm-related reasoning. The behavioral type and the norm types were important factors for the relationships among attitude, norms, and various types of intentions. Copyright by Hye Jeong Choi 2008 ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS I would like to express my sincere and deepest appreciation to several wonderful persons. First of all. I really thank my family. Without their love, support, and sacrifice, I would not complete my study here at MSU. Especially. my parents" understanding and supporting were critical for me to continue my study. Next. I give my special appreciation to Dr. Hee Sun Park. my academic advisor, for her precious time and effort guiding my thesis and study. If it were not her helping advising and understanding. I would not finish my thesis and my degree. I owe her so much. I also appreciate Dr. Teresa Mastin and Dr. Mira Lee, for their invaluable helps as committee members. While studying with them, I was impressed with their insights. They also inspired and encouraged me to think broadly. Finally, I can not help giving my best appreciation to God for showing me the way I should go and guiding my life as the true mentor. I am leaving Michigan with many wonderful memories. I loved the nature as much as the people in Michigan. TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES ................................................................................. vi LIST OF FIGURES .............................................................................. viii INTRODUCTION .................................................................................... I CHAPTER I: LITERATURE REVIEW ........................................................... 3 Determinants of Behavioral Intentions ................................................... 3 Explaining Ones’ Behavioral Intentions ................................................... 4 Cultural Differences in Explaining One’s Behavioral Intentions ...................... 6 Hypotheses and Research Questions ...................................................... 7 CHAPTER II: METHODS ........................................................................ I0 Overview of Design ........................................................................ IO Participants .................................................................................. 10 Procedure .................................................................................... I I Measures .................................................................................... 12 CHAPTER III: RESULTS ......................................................................... I6 Summary ..................................................................................... I6 Research question I ........................................................................ I7 Research question 2 ........................................................................ I9 Research question 3 ........................................................................ 22 Hypothesis I &2 ............................................................................ 29 CHAPTER IV: DISCUSSION .................................................................... 35 Summary .................................................................................... 35 Implications for socially desirable and undesirable behavior ......................... 37 Implications for behavior involving interpersonal relationships ..................... 39 Implications for cross-culture ............................................................ 40 Limitations .................................................................................. 42 Conclusion .................................................................................. 42 TABLES & FIGURES .............................................................................. 43 APPENDICES ....................................................................................... 73 BIBLIOGRAPHY .................................................................................... 98 LIST OF TABLES Reliabilities, Correlations, Means. and Standard Deviations for Purchasing Green Products ............................................................................................. 43 Reliabilities, Correlations, Means, and Standard Deviations for Telling the Truth.........45 Reliabilities, Correlations, Means. and Standard Deviations for Lying for a Friend ...... 47 Reliabilities, Correlations, Means, and Standard Deviations for Downloading Unauthorized Media Files Online ................................................................. 49 Regression Results for Intentions to Purchase Green Product and to Download Unauthorized Media Files Online .................................................................. 5| Regression Results for Intentions to Tell the Truth and to Lie ................................ 52 Regression Results for Intentions to Use Attitudinal Reasoning for Explaining Purchasing Green Product and Downloading Unauthorized Media Files Online ......................... 53 Regression Results for Intentions to Use Attitudinal Reasoning for Explaining Truth- TeIIing and Lying ..................................................................................... 54 Regression Results for Intentions to Use Subjective Norms-Reasoning for Explaining Purchasing Green Product and Downloading Unauthorized Media Files Online .......... 55 Regression Results for Intentions to Use Subjective Norms-Reasoning for Truth-Telling and Lying ............................................................................................. 56 Regression Results for Intentions to Use Descriptive Norms-Reasoning for Explaining Purchasing Green Product and Downloading Unauthorized Media Files Online...........57 Regression Results for Intentions to Use Descriptive Norms-Reasoning for Explaining Truth-Telling and Lying ............................................................................. 58 Regression Results for Intentions to Use Injunctive Norms-Reasoning for Explaining Purchasing Green Product and Downloading Unauthorized Media Files Online .......... 59 Regression Results for Intentions to Use Injunctive Norms-Reasoning for Explaining Truth-Telling and Lying ........................................................................... 60 Summary of Unstandardized Simple Slopes ..................................................... 6] Means and Standard Deviations for Reasoning/ Behavioral Type/ Culture ANOVA for Purchasing Green Products and Downloading Unauthorized Media Files Online ........ 62 vi Means and Standard Deviations for Reasoning/ Behavioral type/ Culture ANOVA for Telling the Truth and Lying for a Friend ......................................................... 63 4 (Reasoning) X 4 (type of behaviors) X 2 (cultures) ANOVA Results ..................... 64 Comparisons of Reasoning Types for Koreans and Americans for Purchasing Green Product ............................................................................................... 65 Comparisons of Reasoning Types for Koreans and Americans for Truth-Telling..........66 Comparisons of Reasoning Types for Koreans and Americans for Lying for a Friend....67 Comparisons of Reasoning Types for Koreans and Americans for Downloading Unauthorized Media Files Online ................................................................. 68 vii LIST OF FIGURES Comparisons of Reasoning Types for Purchasing Green Products in Korea and in US .................................................................................................................................. 69 Comparisons of Reasoning Types for Downloading unauthorized media file via online in Korea and in US ................................................................................. 70 Comparisons of Reasoning Types for Truth-telling in Korea and in US .................. 7I Comparisons of Reasoning Types for Lying for a Friend in Korea and in US ........... 72 viii FACTORS AFFECTING HOW INDIVIDUALS EXPLAIN THEIR BEHAVIORAL INTENTION TO OTHERS Introduction Individuals often face situations where they need to explain to others why they have engaged or are about to engage in a certain behavior. Some individuals may express to others what they have in mind as the main reasons for engaging in a behavior, whereas other individuals may opt to cite reasons they would help to receive positive impressions from other people. For example, when students are asked why they intend to study hard for an exam, some students may tell others that they intend to study hard because they personally think studying hard is good and useful even though the more central reason for them to study hard is because their parents expect them to study hard. One main aim ofthe current paper is to investigate whether or not individuals’ internal reasons for forming their behavioral intention will be consistent with the external reasons they use for expressing their behavioral intentions to other people. That is, when individuals’ attitudes are stronger reasons for forming behavioral intention, would they also cite their attitudes externally as the reasons for their behavioral intentions when asked to explain their behavioral intentions? In this paper, internal reasons and external reasons are distinguished. Internal reasons for forming behavioral intentions focus on what individuals may consider for engaging in a behavior. Attitudinal components and normative components central to theories such as Theory of Reasoned Action (TRA) are the internal reasons. External reasons are the ones that individuals tell others when explaining their behavioral intentions to others; individuals may use reasons pertaining to their attitudes and/or their perceptions of norms. Another main aim ofthe current paper is to examine cultural differences between Koreans and Americans for the relationship between internal reasons and external reasons. The culture of Korea has been characterized as more collectivistic and less individualistic than that of America (Hofstede, I980; Kim, I994). Individualism has been defined as emphasis on individuals’ own attributes and their self-concepts independent of other people, whereas collectivism has been characterized as emphasis on interpersonal harmony and fitting in with others and self-concepts construed in relation with others (Triadis, I995). An individualistic culture stresses that individuals should have autonomy in their relationships with others (Markus & Kitayama, I991) and their personal goal should be more respected than a group’s goal (Triandis, Mccusker, & Hui, 1990). On the other hand, individuals in collective cultures consider themselves as an element of a whole group (Markus & Kitayama, I99]; Triandis, I989), so it is more important to achieve the group goal and keep the group’s survival than each individual’s goal and survival. Considering such cultural differences, it is expected that Koreans and Americans can differ in the extent to which they are likely to use attitudes and norms as the reasons they use when explaining their behaviors to others. The following sections provide a brief rationale, hypotheses. and research questions. Chapter I Determinants of Behavioral Intentions Among the various reasons for why individuals intend to engage in a behavior, two major types of determinants have been examined: attitudinal and normative components. The first type of components has to do with individuals’ own view about performing the behavior. In other words, individuals themselves may consider engaging in the behavior as good and beneficial, possibly because engaging in the behavior leads to useful outcomes for them. The other component pertains to individuals’ perception about what other people think about the individuals performing the behavior. That is, individuals often consider the opinions of others who are important to them and want to comply with those important others’ wishes (Ajen & Fishbein. 1973). One ofthe theories that specify these attitudinal and normative components as determinants of behavioral intentions and behaviors is Theory of Reasoned Action (TRA) (Ajen & Fishbein, I980). Attitudes toward behavior and subjective norms as two components of TRA have been shown to be related to intentions to engage in various behaviors such as donating blood (Warshaw, Calanton, & Joyce, 1986) and purchasing (Warshaw, I980). Meta-analyses also showed that attitudinal and normative components had strong correlations with behavior intentions (Sheppard, Hartwick, & Warshaw, I988; Sheeran & Talyor, I999; Armitage & Conner, 2001). Research showed that TRA had a good validity in Korea for purchasing intentions (Lee & Green, I991) and intentions to study for a final exam (Park & Levine, I999). Concerning normative components, individuals’ perceptions of descriptive and injunctive norms have been shown to be related to behavioral intentions. Descriptive norms are defined as what people commonly do and injunctive norms are defined as what people approve and support (Cialdini, Reno, & Kallgren, I990). Studies have shows that descriptive norms explain additional variance in behavioral intentions beyond attitudes 3 and subjective norms (e.g., Rivis & Sheeran, 2003). Injunctive norms also are related to the behavioral intention such as alcohol consumption (Real & Rimal, 2007), playing the lottery (Walker, Coumeya, & Deng, 2006). Even though injunctive norm and subjective norms are determined by expectations of other’s belief, they are different in terms of social sanction (Bendor & Swistak, 2001). Park and Smith (2007) provided empirical evidence that individuals' subjective norms, perceived descriptive norms, and perceived injunctive norm are distinguished from one another and are differently interacted with behavioral intentions and attitudes. Explaining Ones’ Behavioral Intentions When there is no need for individuals to explain their behaviors to others, individuals may freely consider their own personal assessment of the behaviors. But for explaining to others about their behavioral choices, individuals may need to consider what others would think about the reasons explained to them. When there are multiple reasons for engaging in a behavior, each reason can vary in the extent to which it generates desirable impressions (Cooley, 1902). Even when there is only one good reason for engaging in a behavior, individuals may not want to let others know the real reason and rather may want to use other reasons when explaining to others why they intend to do the behavior. For example, when running for a public office (e.g., a senator seat, the US. President), an individual may have his or her own selfish reason deep down in his or her heart (e.g., obsession with power), but may avoid revealing it to others and tell to many others that he or she simply wants to serve the country and honor what others want and expect him or her to do. When juxtaposing internal reasons for forming behavioral intention with external justification for forming behavioral intention, two possibilities exist. One is that individuals are consistent in their internal thoughts and external justification concerning 4 their behaviors. For a behavior that individual internally weigh their attitudes, rather than perceived norms, as a stronger factor for forming behavioral intention, individuals may externally express their attitudes to others when asked to explain why they are about to engage in the behavior. For a behavior that individuals internally weigh their norm perceptions, rather than attitudes, as a stronger factor for forming behavioral intention, individual may externally express their perceived norms to others when asked to explain why they are about to engage in the behavior. Another possibility for the relationship between internal reasons for forming behavioral intention with external reasons for forming behavioral intention is that individuals’ internal thoughts are inconsistent with their external justification concerning their behaviors. Individuals may externally express their perceived norms to others when asked to explain why they are about to engage in the behavior even for a behavior that individuals internally weigh their attitudes as a stronger factor for forming behavioral intention. Individuals may externally express their attitudes to others when asked to explain why they are about to engage in the behavior even for a behavior that individuals internally weigh their norms as a stronger factor for forming behavioral intention. Individuals may invoke different reasons when explaining a certain action to themselves versus to others. One reason could be impression management, which refers to the attempt to control individuals’ image consciously or unconsciously in social interaction (Schlenker, I980). In general, people have the need to belong to the society and want to be accepted by others. When individuals need to explain their behavioral intentions to others, individuals may need to consider what others would think about them (Buss, I980). Expressing a certain type of reasons for one’s behavioral reason can garner desirable or undesirable impressions (Leary & Kowalski. I990). For example, for a certain behavior, individuals may think that expressing their attitudes as the reason for engaging in a behavior makes them look confident about their attitudes. Alternatively, for a behavior that may benefit others more so, individuals may think that expressing their perceived norms (e.g., "others who are important to me wanted me to do it," as an example of subjective norms, "many people do it" as an example of perceived descriptive norms, and "many people approve of the behavior" as an example of perceived injunctive norms) as the reason for engaging in a behavior makes them look caring and/or considerate of others. Cultural Differences in Explaining One 's Behavioral Intentions Cultures can vary in the extent to which individuals consider other people in how they think, behave. and communicate. For example, cultures can affect how individuals think ofthemselves in relation to others (Markus & Kitayama, 1991). Compared to Hindus, Americans more often refer to general dispositions and less frequently refer to contextual factor when they have to explain the behavior of third party (Miller, 1984). Masuda and Nisbett (2001) also demonstrated that East Asians relatively more focus on the environment or context than do Americans. For instance, commercials in USA appeals personal benefits more than do Korean advertising where Korean commercials more frequently showed the scene of conversation together than American commercials (Cho et. al., 1999). Morris and Peng’s (1994) study showed that that Chinese—language newspaper described more situational factors than English-language newspaper did while English- language newspaper described more personal dispositions than C hinese-Ianguage newspaper did. Although it is universal that individuals perceived a communicative behavior differently depending on the relationship they involved (e.g., parents vs. strangers) (Knapp, Ellis, & Williams, 1980), cultural differences could exsit in such a way that Koreans, compared to Americans, were more likely to tell personal things to the members of their ingroup (Gudykunst, Yoon, & Nishida, 1987). 6 Meta-analyses and literature review articles have shown that attitudes toward behavior, rather than subjective norms, have a stronger relationship with behavioral intention (Sheeran & Taylor, 1999; Sheppard, Hartwick, & Warshaw, I988; Trafimow & Finlay, 1996; Hausenblas, Carron, & Mack, 1997). That is, many studies using TRA have shown that, in general, attitudes are stronger reasons for forming behavioral intentions than subjective norms. Attitudes often being a stronger factor for behavioral intentions may be more naturally expected for people in America whose culture is considered as individualistic. On the other hand, subjective norms have been often hypothesized to be a stronger determinant of behavioral intentions among people in Asian cultures, many of which are considered as collectivistic. Empirical findings, however, have shown that, even for Asians such as Koreans and Chinese, attitudes are only the significant factor or the stronger factor for behavioral intention than subjective norms (Park & Levine, 1999). One of the reasons for such findings could be that, although the cultural characteristics of collectivism may lead individuals to consider what their important others think about their performing the behavior, attitudes might be still a stronger determinant of behavioral intention as long as individuals do not have to consider whether others would know about their behavioral intentions and reason for such behavioral intentions, at least at the time of answering survey items. Thus, when individuals imagine a situation where they face others and explain to them about their behaviors, it may become more salient in the individuals' mind what others would think about their behaviors and also explanations for the behaviors. Hypotheses and Research Questions One set of the main variables in the current study are the two components of TRA (attitudes toward behavior and subjective norms) and individuals’ perceptions of 7 descriptive and injunctive norms as the internal reasons for behavioral intentions to engage in a behavior. Another set of the main variables are intentions to use attitudes- related reasons and intentions to use norms-related reasons when explaining behavioral intentions to others. For the behavioral intentions to be examined in the current study, it will be first examined whether or not attitudinal and normative components will be significantly related to each behavioral intention. Then, it will be examined whether or not the attitudinal components and normative components (subjective norms, descriptive norms, and injunctive norms) as the internal reasons are significantly related to intentions to use attitudes-related and norms-related reasons used for explaining behavioral intentions to others The research questions are specified as follows: Research Question 1: Among attitudinal and normative components, which one will be more strongly related to behavioral intention to engage in a behavior? Research Question 2: Among attitudinal and normative components concerning performing a behavior, which one will be more strongly related to intention to use attitudes-related reasons when individuals are supposed to explain their behavioral intentions to others? Research Question 3: Among attitudinal and normative components concerning performing a behavior, which one will be more strongly related to intention to use nonns-related reasons when individuals are supposed to explain their behavioral intentions to others? Based on the aforementioned discussion of cultural differences, it is expected that differences between Koreans and Americans may be more pronounced when they are supposed to explain their behavioral intentions to other people. For example, Japanese believe that they need to adjust their action in accordance with the situations (public vs. 8 private setting) whereas Americans believe that their behavior should be consistent with both situations (Doi, 1986). Briley, Morris and Simonson (2000) found out that Chinese and Japanese are more likely to change their decision on from two extreme options to compromise alternatives when they have to explain their decision while Americans are unlikely to change their decision. Although Koreans are not necessarily the same as Japanese and Chinese in their orientations, considering the characteristics of Americans, it is expected that Americans and Koreans also are likely to differ. The following hypotheses are advanced: Hypothesis 1: Koreans will indicate stronger intentions to use norms-related reasons than attitudes-related reasons when they are supposed to explain their behavioral intentions to others. Hypothesis 2: Americans will indicate stronger intentions to use attitudes—related reasons than norms-related reasons when they are supposed to explain their behavioral intentions to others. Chapter 2 Method Overview of design The current study recruited participants from Korea and the US which were considered as collectivistic and individualistic, respectively. according to Hofstede (1980). Four versions of questionnaire were prepared because each questionnaire contained measurement items for one of four behaviors (purchasing green products, downloading unauthorized media files via the Internet, lying for a friend or telling the truth). Participants completed one of the four versions randomly. Participants were asked to indicate their behavioral intentions to engage in one of four behaviors, attitude toward the behavior, subjective norms, descriptive norms, and injunctive norms concerning one of four behaviors. Participants also indicated the degree to which they would intend to employ each of four different reasoning types (attitudinal reasoning, subjective, descriptive, and injunctive norms reasoning) when they would have to explain to others their behavioral intention to engage in one of the four behaviors. Participants A total of72| undergraduates were recruited in the US. and Korea. Three hundred forty two students (135 males, 205 females, and 2 unclassifiable) enrolled in Michigan State University and 379 students (260 males, 135 females. and 13 unclassifiable) in large universities in Korea completed the survey. American participants. Of the participants. 59.9% were Caucasian, 17.8% were African American, 0.9% were Native American, 6.1% were Asian American, 5.0% were Hispanic, 1.2% were Pacific Islander, 5.3% were mixed, and 3.8 % were un-identifiable. Ages ofthe participants ranged from 18 to 48 years old (M: 21.35, SD = 3.16). Eighty participants (60.0% females) completed questionnaire that contained measurement items 10 about purchasing green products, 85 participants (64.7% females) completed questionnaire about truth-telling, 83 participants (60.2% females) completed questionnaire about lying for a friend, and 94 participants (55.3% females) completed questionnaire about downloading unauthorized media files via the Internet. Korean participants. All participants in Korea had identical ethnicity. The average age ofthe Korean participants was 22.56 with SD = 2.94, ranging from 18 to 42. Eighty five participants (18.8% females) completed questionnaire about purchasing green product, 90 participants (30% females) completed questionnaire about truth-telling. 91 participants (29.7% females) completed questionnaire about lying for a friend, and 1 13 participants (31.9% females) completed questionnaire about downloading unauthorized media files online. Procedure Some American students received the extra credits in exchange for their voluntary participation, while Korean students and other American students voluntarily participated without any compensation. Korean participants were contacted via the researcher’s personal network and snowball sampling and also in classrooms in universities, whereas U.S. participants were contacted in various places on campus including classrooms and libraries. Korean participants completed the questionnaire in Korea and American participants completed the questionnaire in English. Since the questionnaire with different language should be equivalent, it was necessary to ensure correct translations between languages. To do this, a bilingual native Korean speaker first translated the questionnaire from English to Korean. Next, a bilingual English speaker translated the Korean version back to English. Ultimately the original English version and the back-translated English version were compared for consistency. ll Measurements For the purpose ofthe current study, four behaviors (purchasing green products, downloading unauthorized media files via the Internet, lying for a friend in a situation, and telling the truth in a situation) chosen because ofthe possibility that cultural differences in the relationship between TRA components and intentions could vary with behavior types. For a proper cross-cultural comparison. it was necessary to select behaviors that would be commonly and prevalently performed in both countries. It was also desirable that chosen behaviors could illustrate cultural differences in how people would form their behavioral intentions and explain them to others. The four behaviors chosen for the current study seemed to fit these criteria. Downloading unauthorized media files from the Internet and purchasing green product were chosen because these two behaviors were gaining attentions increasingly from Korea and the US for their popularity and potential costs and benefits to both countries. While telling the truth and lying for a friend would be behaviors familiar to both Koreans and Americans. these two behaviors could show cultural differences in reasons to engage in the behaviors (Triandis et al., 2001). Unlike downloading unauthorized media files from the Internet, for telling the truth and lying for a friend, a scenario was provided to participants to increase participants’ involvement and interest in the behaviors (see Appendices 3 and 4 for the scenario). Based on a survey example provided by Ajzen and F ishbein (1980), the questionnaire was designed to measure each participant’s behavioral intention to engage in one of the behaviors, attitude toward the behavior, and subjective/descriptive/injunctive norms concerning the behavior. After responding to the scale for typical TRA components, each participant indicated the extent to which they would intend to use the altitudinal and normative reasoning when explaining their behavioral intention to others. The reliabilities (Cronbach’s a) ranged from .74 to .96 with most ofthe variables having reliabilities l2 above.80. Tables I to 4 show the reliabilities on the diagonal of each table for each behavior type. Tables I to 4 also show correlations among the variables and means and standard deviations of each variable for each behavior. Behavioral intentions. The measurement for behavioral intention included five items for unauthorized downloading, five items for purchasing green products, six items for lying for a friend, and six items for telling the truth with a 7-point response scale format (e.g., 1 = strongly disagree, 7 = strongly agree) (See Appendix 1 for purchasing green products, Appendix 2 for unauthorized downloading, Appendix 3 for lying for a friend, and Appendix 4 for telling the truth). An example item was “I intend to download unauthorized media files via the Internet.“ For measuring behavioral intention to tell the truth, two items (“I will lie for my friend in this situation.” [a recoded item] and “I have an intention to tell the truth in this situation") were deleted in order to improve reliability. The reliabilities for the four retained items for behavioral intention to tell the truth for each culture were included in Table 2. Perceived norms. Subjective norms, descriptive norms, and injunctive norms were separately measured with four items with a 7-point response scale format (I = strongly disagree, 7 = strongly agree). These items were modified ones from the scales used in a previous study (Park & Smith, 2007). For example, “Many people have illegally downloaded media files online” was used to measure individuals’ perceptions of descriptive norms. Among the four items measuring injunctive norms, one item (“Telling the truth in this situation is a socially approved behavior”) was removed to improve the reliabilities for measuring injunctive norms concerning truth-telling and lying for a friend. The reliabilities for the three retained items for measuring injunctive norms concerning truth-telling and lying for a friend for each culture were included in Tables 2 and 3. Attitudes toward behavior. Direct measure for attitude toward behavior included 13 nine 7-point semantic differential evaluative scales; Bad-Good. Unpleasant-Pleasant, Considerate-Inconsiderate, Unfavorable-Favorable, Positive-Negative, Pointless- Worthwhile, Enjoyable-Unenjoyable, Unsatisfying—Satisfying. and Useless-Useful. A higher score indicated more positive attitude. ranging from 1 (e.g., extremely bad) to 7 (e.g., extremely good). An example item was “Downloading unauthorized media files via the Internet is __.” Intention to use anNudes-related reasons. The items to measure intention to use attitudes-related reasons were similar to the items used to measure attitude toward behavior. Participants were asked to indicate the extent to which they agreed with each attitude-related reasoning statement as a way to explain their behavior. Participants responded on semantic differential evaluative scales (e.g., 1 = extremely useless, 7 = extremely useful) to the statement, “When I have to explain why I will downloaded unauthorized copyright-protected media files via the Internet. I intend to say to other people that downloading unauthorized copyright protected media files via the Internet is 99 Intentions to use norms-related reasons. The items to measure intention to use norms-related reasons were similar to the items used to measure individuals’ perceptions of subjective norms, injunctive norms, and descriptive norms. Participants were asked to indicate the extent to which they agreed with each norms-related reasoning statement as a way to explain their behavior (e.g., When I have to explain why I will downloaded unauthorized copyright-protected media files via the Internet, I intend to say to other people that many people would endorse my downloading unauthorized media files online.). All the measures used a 7-point response format (I = strongly disagree. 7 = strongly agree). In order to improve the reliabilities of measurement items for injunctive norms-related reasons for truth-telling and lying for a friend, one item was deleted. This 14 removed item was the same item that was removed from the measurement of injunctive norms. The reliabilities for the three retained items for measuring intentions to use injunctive norms-related reasons concerning truth-telling and lying for a friend for each culture were included in Tables 2 and 3. Chapter 3 Results Summary Before testing hypotheses and answering research questions, the data were checked for linearity. Scatterplots did not show any considerably curvilinear relationships between independent and dependent variables. Besides. t-test was used in order to check the gender difference. Even though there were statistically significant differences between men and women for some of the variables, gender did not substantially influence the overall findings. Thus, gender difference will not be further discussed. Hypotheses were tested with a 3-way mixed ANOVA. The within-subject factor was 4 types of reasoning (intentions to use attitude-related reasonin g, subjective norms- related reasoning, descriptive norms-related reasoning, and injunctive norms-related reasoning). The between-subject factors were 4 types of behaviors (purchasing green products. telling the truth, lying for a friend, downloading unauthorized media files via the Internet) and 2 cultures (Korea and the US). Hierarchical multiple regression analyses provided answers to the research questions. One dummy variable labeled as culture was created with Korean participants as the reference group coded with O and American participants as the comparison group coded with 1. All continuous independent variables were mean-centered in order to protect against unessential multicollinearity when testing interactions among the variables. Attitudinal and normative components (i.e.. the first—order effect predictors) were entered in the first block along with culture (i.e., the dummy variable representing the national group). In addition, the interaction term between the attitudinal component and culture and the interaction terms between each of the normative components and culture (i.e., the second-order effect predictors) were entered in the second block. 16 Research Question 1 RQI asked which of the attitudinal and normative components would be more strongly related to behavioral intentions. The findings showed that, in general, attitude toward a behavior and subjective norms were statistically significant predictors of intentions across the four behaviors. Attitude toward a behavior was the strongest predictor of intentions to purchase green products and to download unauthorized media files via the Internet. whereas subjective norms were the strongest predictor of intentions to tell the truth and to lie for a friend. Hierarchical multiple regression results for each behavior type are discussed below. For any significant interactions, simple regressions were conducted to examine the patterns ofthe interactions. Table 15 summaries simple regression results for significant interactions. Purchasing Green Product As shown in Table 5, the overall model including the first-order effect and second- order effect predictors for behavioral intention to purchase green products was significant. 2 F(9, 154) = 16.73 p < .001. adjusted]? = .47. When the five first-order effect predictors were entered to the first block, attitude (B [unstandardized coefficient] = 0.77, SE = 0.12, [3: .52, p < .001) and subjective norms (B = 0.34, SE = 0.10. B = .29, p < .001) were statistically significant. The results showed that individuals who had more positive attitude toward the behavior and stronger subjective norms had higher intention to purchase green products in the future. Culture and descriptive and injunctive norms were not statistically significant. When the interaction terms were entered to the second block. they together explained an additional 4.0% of the variance in behavioral intention. The significant interaction involving culture as the moderator of the relationship between attitude toward behavior and behavioral intention indicated that the relationship between attitude and 17 behavioral intention differed in Korea and the US. The simple regression showed that attitude was more strongly related to behavioral intention for Americans (B = 1.06, SE = 0.17, B = .31,p < .001) than for Koreans (B = 0.41, SE = .17, [3 = .56.}? < .05). Downloading Unauthorized Media Files via the Internet The analysis showed that the overall model including all the predictors was 2 significant. F (9. 197) = 39.26. p < .001. adjusted R = .63 (see Table 5). When the five predictors were entered to the first block, attitude (B = 0.66, SE: 0.07, ,b’ = .54, p<.001), subjective norms (B = 0.29, SE = 0.07, B = .26, p<.01), descriptive norms (B = 0.15 SE = 0.06, B:- .14. p < .01), and culture (B: —0.38. SE = 0.16. B: —.13_. p < .05) were statistically significant while injunctive norms (B = 0.02. SE = 0.07. B = .02, p = .73) were not statistically significant. None of the interaction terms were statistically significant. The results showed that Koreans had stronger intention to download medial files than Americans did and that individuals who had more positive attitude toward the behavior and stronger subjective and descriptive norms strongly intended to download unauthorized media files online both in Korea and the US. Truth- Telling As shown in Table 6. the overall model including all the predictors was significant, 2 F(9, 164) = 14.15,p < .001, adjustedR = .41. Among the first-order predictors, attitude (B = 0.41, SE = 0.1 I, B = .28, p<.001), subjective norms (B = 0.28, SE = 0.08, B = .28,p < .01), injunctive norms (B = 0.25, SE = 0.10, [3 = .21,p < .05), and culture (B = 0.43, SE = 0.19, B = .15, p < .05) were statistically significant for behavioral intention to tell the truth, whereas descriptive norms (B = —0.7 1 , SE = 0.10. B = —.06. p = .40) was not significant. There were no significant interactions, indicating no cultural differences in the relationship between predictors and behavioral intention. The results showed that 18 Americans had stronger intention to tell the truth than Koreans did and that individuals who had more positive attitude and stronger subjective and injunctive norms were more likely to tell the truth. Lyingfor a Friend Table 6 shows the regression results. The overall model with all the predictors was 2 significant, F (9, 162) = 16.04, p < .001 adjustedR = .44. Among the five predictors in the first block, attitude (B = 0.38. SE = 0.10. [3 = .30, p < .001) and subjective norms (B = 0.36, SE = 0.08, [3 = .37.p < .001) were statistically significant predictors of intention to lie for a friend. The results showed that increases in attitudes and subjective norms were associated with increases in behavioral intention to lie for a friend. Descriptive norms (B = 0.16, SE = 0.09, B = .14, p = .72) and injunctive norms (B = —0.03, SE = 0.09, B = —.03, p = .63) and culture (B = —0.09, SE = 0.18, B = —.04,p = .72) were not statistically significant. When the interaction terms were entered to the second block, they together explained an additional 6.0% of the variance in behavioral intention. The significant interaction involving culture as the moderator of the relationship between attitude toward behavior and behavioral intention revealed that attitudes toward the behavior were more strongly related to behavioral intention for Americans (B = 0.70, SE = 0.14, B = .47, p <.001) than for Koreans (B = 0.10, SE = 0.13, [3 = .08,p = .45). Research Question 2 RQ2 asked which of the two components of TRA (attitude and perceived norms) would be more strongly related to intention to use attitude related-reasoning (the dependent variable) when individuals needed to explain their behavioral intention to others. Injunctive norms (for purchasing green products), and subjective norms (for lying for a friend and downloading unauthorized media files) were significant predictors for 19 intention to use attitudinal reasoning. but attitude toward a behavior was the strongest predictor for intention to use attitudinal reasoning for each of the four behaviors. In addition, there were significant interactions between subjective norms and culture (for purchasing green product) and between injunctive norms and culture (for downloading unauthorized media files). Intention to Use A ttitudinal Reasoning Purchasing green product. Table 7 presents the regression results. The overall model including first-order effect and second-order effect predictors was significant, F (9, 2 155) = 40.38, p < .001, adjusted R = .68. Among the five predictors in the first block, attitude (B = 0.65, SE = 0.06. [3 = .66. p < .001) and injunctive norms (B = 0.27, SE = 0.06, B = .30, p < .001) were statistically significant predictors of intention to use attitudinal reasons, whereas descriptive and injunctive norms and culture were not statistically significant. The results showed that individuals with more positive attitude toward the behavior and stronger injunctive norms had higher intention to use attitudinal reasoning. The significant interaction involving culture as the moderator ofthe relationship between subjective norms and intention to use attitudinal reasoning indicated that the simple slope was negative and significant for Americans (B = -0.14, SE = 0.06, [3 = -. I 9, p<.05), but the simple slope was positive and not significant for Koreans (B = 0.12, SE = .10, B: .13, p = .24). Downloading. Table 7 presents the regression results. The overall model including the first-order effect and second-order effect predictors was significant. F (9, 197) = 47.65 2 p < .001 adjusted R = .67. Among the five predictors in the first block, attitude (B = 0.69, SE = 0.06, ,8 = .67,p < .001) and injunctive norms (B = 0.15. SE = 0.06. [3 = .15. p < .01) were statistically significant predictor of intention to use attitudinal reasons. whereas 20 descriptive (B = 0.04, SE = 0.05, B = .05.p = .33) and injunctive norms (B = 0.03, SE = 0.06, B = .30. p = .55) and culture (B = 0.22, SE = 0.13, B = .09. p = .09) were not statistically significant. The results showed that attitude toward the behavior and injunctive norms were positively related to intention to say attitudinal reasoning when explaining to others one’s behavioral intention to download. As shown in Table 7, the interaction involving culture as the moderator ofthe relationship between injunctive norms and intention to use attitudinal reasoning was significant. The finding indicated that the simple slope was negative for Americans (B: —0.1 1. SE = 0.08. B = —.10. p = .18), but the simple slope was positive for Koreans (B = 0.1 1, SE = 0.07, B = .12. p = .12). Truth-telling. As shown in Table 8. the overall model was significant, F(9, 165) = 2 15.37,p < .001 adjustedR = .43. When the five predictors including in the first block, attitude (B = 0.62, SE = 0.08, B = .58, p < .001) was a statistically significant predictor of intention to use attitudinal reasons. whereas subjective norms (B = 0.03. SE = 0.06, B = .04. p = .66), descriptive norms (B = 0.09, SE = 0.06, B = .10,p = .16) injunctive norms (B = 0.06, SE = 0.07, B = .07. p = .42), and culture (B = 0.08, SE = 0.14, B = .04, p = .55) were not significant. Additionally, none of the interaction terms in the second block were significant. The result showed that individuals with more positive attitude toward telling the truth were more likely to say attitudinal reasons when they had to explain to others their behavioral intention to tell the truth. Lying for afriend. As shown in Table 8, the overall model with all the predictors 2 was significant, F (9, 162) = 28.62.p < .001, adjusted R = .59. Among the five predictors in the first block, attitude (B = 0.61, SE = 0.06. B = .61.p < .001) and subjective norms (B = 0.14, SE = 0.05. B =.18. p < .01) were statistically significant, yet descriptive norms (B = —0.08, SE = 0.06. B = —.10, p = .20), injunctive norms (B = 0.03. SE = 0.06. B 21 = .03.}? = .65), and culture (B = —0. 16, SE = 0.12. B = —.08. p = .17) were not significant. The results showed that the more positive individuals’ attitudes were and the attitudes individuals’ subjective norms were, the more strongly they intended to say attitudinal reasons in order to explain their behavioral intention to others. There were no significant interaction terms. Research Question 3 RQ3 asked which ofthe attitudinal and normative components would be more strongly related to intention to use normative reasoning when explaining one’s behavioral intentions to engage in behaviors. Although subjective norms were most strongly related to subjective norms-related reasoning across the four behaviors, attitude toward a behavior (for downloading unauthorized media files via the Internet and telling the truth) and injunctive norms (for telling the truth) were also significantly related to intention to use subjective norms-related reasoning. Besides, in two types of behaviors (purchasing green product and downloading), the relationship between attitude and intention to say subjective norms-related reasons differed depending on the culture. Descriptive norms were most strongly related to intention to say descriptive norms-related reasons across the four behaviors. Attitude toward a behavior was also significantly related to the norms- related reasoning across the behaviors even though the magnitudes were relatively smaller than descriptive norms. Besides, the direction of the relationship between attitude toward a behavior and intention was different from the relationship between descriptive norms and intention for lying for a friend. In addition, subjective norms were significantly related to intention to say descriptive norms-related reasons only in the behavior of lying for a friend. Although injunctive norms were most strongly related to intention to say injunctive norms-related reasoning across the behaviors, attitude toward a behavior (for purchasing green product and downloading) and subjective norms (for telling the truth and lying for a 22 friend) also significantly explained the variance of intention to say injunctive norms- related reasons. Intention to Use Subjective Norms-Related Reasoning Purchasing green product. As shown in Table 9. the overall model including the first-order effect and second-order effect predictors was significant. F (9. 155) = 37.89, p 2 < .001, adjusted R = .70. Among the five predictors in the first block, subjective norms (B = 0.74. SE = 0.06. B = —.73. p < .001) and culture (B = —0.34. SE = 0.16. B = —. l 3, p < .05) were statistically significant predictors of intention to say subjective norms-related reasons. The results showed that Koreans had stronger intention to say subjective norms- related reasons than Americans did and that subjective norms were positively related to intention to say subjective norms-related reasons. On the other hand, Attitude (B = 0.1 1, SE = 0.08, B = .09. p = .16). descriptive norms (B = 0.05. SE = 0.06, B = .05, p = .38), and injunctive norms (B = —.07, SE = 0 .08, B = —.06, p = .38) were not statistically significant. When the interaction terms were entered to the second block. the interaction involving culture as the moderator ofthe relationship between attitude toward behavior and subjective norms-related reasoning was significant. The finding showed that for Koreans (B = 0.32, SE = 0.10, B = .28 p < .01), the simple slope was positive and significant, whereas for Americans (B = —0.03, SE: 0.12. B = —.03,p = .77), the simple slope was negative and not significant. Koreans who had more positive attitudes toward purchasing green products indicated stronger intention to say subjective norms-related reasons than Americans did. Downloading unauthorized mediafiles. Table 9 presents the relevant regression 2 results. The overall model was significant. F (9, 196) = 33.48. p < .001. adjusted R = .59. Among the five predictors in the first block, attitude (B = 0.20. SE = 0.07, B = .19, p < .01 ), 23 subjective norms (B = 0.55. SE = 0.07. B = .55.p < .001), and culture (B = —0.32, SE = 0.15, B = ~—. 12, p< .05) were statistically significant, while descriptive norms (B = -0.20, SE = 0.05, B = —.02. p = .72) and injunctive norms (B = .10, SE = 0.06, ,6 = .10, p = .13) were not statistically significant. The results revealed that Koreans had stronger intention to say subjective norms-related reasons than Americans did and that individuals with more positive attitude toward downloading indicated stronger intention to say subjective norms- related reasoning. When the interaction terms were entered to the second block, they together explained an additional 3.0% ofthe variance in intention to say subjective norm- related reasons. The significant interaction involving culture as the moderator of the relationship between attitude toward behavior and intention to say subjective norms- related reasoning revealed that simple slope was negative and not significant for Americans (B = —.04, SE = 0.10, B = —.04. p = .68), but simple slope was relatively large and significant for Koreans (B = .42, SE = 0.09, B = .41, p < .001). Truth-telling. As shown in Table 10. the overall model was significant, F (9. 165) 2 = 19.86. p < .001. adjusted R = .49. When the five predictors in the first block, attitude (B = 0.33, SE = 0.09, B = .24. p < .001), subjective norms (B = 0.46, SE = 0.07, B = .49, p < .001), and injunctive norms (B = 0.17. SE = 0.08. B = .16.p < .05) were statistically significant, but descriptive norms (B: —0.08, SE = 0.07, B = —.07. p = .28) and culture (B = —0.04, SE = 0.17, B = —.02, p = .82) were not significant. None of interaction terms were statistically significant. Lyingfor afriend. As shown in Table 10. the overall model was significant, F (9, 2 162) = 20.32.p < .001, adjusted R = .50. Among the five predictors in the first block, subjective norms (B = 0.53. SE = 0.07, B = .56. p < .001) and culture (B = —0.52. SE = 0.15, B = —.22. p< .01) were statistically significant, but attitude (B = 0.06, SE = 0.09. B 24 = .05, p = .46), descriptive norms (B = —0.02, SE = 0.08. B = —.02. p = .83), and injunctive norms (B = 0.09, SE = 0.08, B = .09, p = .26) were not significant. The results showed that Koreans indicated stronger intention to say subjective norms-related reasoning than Americans did and that subjective norms were positively related to intention to say subjective norms-related reasoning. When the interaction terms were added into equation, interaction terms failed to explain the significant increase in the proportion of variance of intention to say subjective norm-related reasons. Fchange (4.162) = 0.57. p = .39. R change = .01. In other word, culture did not serve as a moderator. Intention to Use Descriptive Norms-Related Reasoning Purchasing green product. As shown in Table I l, the overall model was 2 significant, F (9. 155) = 19.56. p < .001, aajustedR = .51. Among the five predictors in the first block, attitude (B = 0.29, SE = 0.10, B = .23. p < .01), descriptive norms (B = 0.60, SE = 0.07, B = .57. p < .001) were statistically significant predictors of intention to say descriptive norms-related reasons, but subjective norms (B = 0.15, SE = 0.08, B = .15, p= .05), injunctive norms (B = —.09, SE = 0.10, B = —.08. p = .37). and culture (B = —0.06, SE = 0.19, B = —.03. p = .74)were not statistically significant. When the interaction terms were entered to the second block, however, interaction terms did not explain any additional variance in intention to use descriptive norms-related reasons, Fchange (4.155) 2 = 1.6I,p = .18, R Change = .02. None ofthe interactions were significant. Downloading. Table 1 1 presents the regression results. The overall model including first-order effect and second-order effect predictors was significant. F (9, 196) = 25 2 17.62.p < .001. adjusted R = .42. Among the five predictors in the first block, attitude (B = 0.30, SE = 0.08, B = .26. p < .001) and descriptive (B = 0.44, SE = 0.07, B = .44, p < .001) were statistically significant. while subjective norms (B = 0.1 1. SE = 0.08, B =.10, p = .19), injunctive norms (B = 0.01 . SE: 0.08. B = .01, p: .87), and culture (B = 0.25, SE = 0.19, B = .09, p = .19) were not statistically significant. When the interaction terms were entered to the second block. they failed to explain an additional variance of intention to 2 say descriptive norm-related reasoning. Fchange (4.196) = 0.78. p = .54. R change = .01. None of the interactions were significant. Truth-telling. As shown in Table 12. the overall model was significant. F(9, 165) 2 = 1 1.13, p < .001, adjusted R = .34. Among the five predictors in the first block, attitude (B = 0.34, SE = 0.09, B = .28. p < .001) and descriptive norms (B = 0.37, SE = 0.07, B = .38, p <.001) were statistically significant. but subjective norms (B = 0.05, SE = 0.07, B= .06.p = .53). injunctive norms (B = 0.08. SE = 0.08. B = .08. p = .33) and culture (B = —0.09, SE = 0.17,,8 = —.04. p = .58) were not significant. When the interaction terms were entered to the second block, yet. the interaction terms failed to account for a significant increase in the proportion of explained variance, Fehange (4,165) = I.50,p = .21, 2 R change 2 -02- Lyingfor afriend. As shown in Table 12, the overall model was significant, F(9, 2 162) = 8.50,p < .001, adjustedR = .28. Among the five predictors in the first block, attitude (B = —0.21, SE = 0.10, B = —.18.p < .05). subjective norms (B = 0.21, SE = 0.08. B = .23. p < .05). and descriptive norms (B = 0.32, SE = 0.10, B = .32. p < .01) were 26 statistically significant. but injunctive norms (B = 0.15. SE = 0.09. B = .15. p = .I l) and culture (B = —0.07, SE = 0.18. B = —.03. p = .69) were not statistically significant. When the interaction terms were added into equation, interaction terms failed to explain the significant increase in the proportion of variance of intention to say subjective norms- 2 related reasoning. Fchange (4.162) = 2.00, p = .10, R change = .03. None ofthe interactions were significant, indicating that culture did not serve as a moderator. Intention to Use Injunctive Norms-Related Reasoning Purchasing green product. A shown in Table 13, the overall model was significant, 2 F(9, 155) = 13.30.p < .001. adjusted R = .40. Among the five predictors in the first balock, attitude (B = 0.33, SE = 0.10. B = .27, p < .01) and injunctive norms (B = .43, SE = 0.10, B = .39. p < .001) were statistically significant. On the other hand, subjective norms (B = 0.05, SE = 0.08. B = .05. p = .57), descriptive norms (B = —0.09, SE = 0.08, B = .10, p = .22), and culture (B = —0.l6. SE = 0.20. B = —.07. p = .41) were not statistically significant. When the interaction terms were included into the equation, the interaction terms failed to account for a significant increase in the proportion of explained variance, 2 Fchange (4.155) = 0.85. p : .50, R change = .01. There were no significant interaction terms. Downloading. As shown in Table 13. the overall model was significant, F(9, 196) 2 = I9.55,p < .001, adjusted R = .45. Among the five predictors in the first block, attitude (B = 0.28, SE = 0.08, B = .26.p < .001) and injunctive norms (B = .41. SE = 0.07, B = .40, p < .001) were statistically significant, while subjective norms (B = 0.1 1. SE = 0.08, B = .11. p = .16), descriptive norms (B = 0.02. SE = 0.06. B :02. p = .78). and culture (B 27 = .03. SE = 0.17. B = .01.p = .88) were not statistically significant. When the interaction terms were entered to the second block, they together explained an additional 3.0% ofthe variance in intention to say subjective norm-related reasoning, F (4, 196) = 2.39,p = .05, R change = .03. The significant interaction involving culture as the moderator ofthe relationship between attitude toward downloading and the intention to say injunctive norms-related reasoning revealed that for Americans (B = .08, S = 0.1 1. B = .07, p = .50), simple slope was positive and not significant, however, for Koreans (B = 0.45, SE = 0.10, B = .43,p < .001), simple slope was relatively large and significant. That is. attitude toward downloading was a positive predictor of intention to say injunctive norms-related reason for Koreans when they had to explain to others they behavioral intention to download. Truth-telling. Table 14 presents the relevant results. The overall model was 2 significant, F (9. 165) = I I.13.p < .001. adjustedR = .34. Among the five predictors in the first block, subjective norms (B = 0.27, SE = 0.07, B = .32. p < .001) and injunctive norms (B = 0.27, SE = 0.08, B = .28. p < .01) were statistically significant predictors of intention to say injunctive norms-related reasons, whereas attitude (B = 0.15. SE = 0.09, B = .13. p = .09). descriptive norms (B = 0.1 1, SE = 0.07. B = .12. p = .17) and culture (B = — 0.05, SE = 0.16, B = —.02. p = .74) were not significant. The results showed that the stronger subjective norms and injunctive norms individuals had. the more strongly they intended to say injunctive norms-related reasoning when they would explain their behavioral intention to others. When the interaction terms were included into the equation, yet, the interaction terms failed to account for a significant increase in the proportion of 2 explained variance, Fehange (4,165) = 1.50, p= .21. R change = .02. None ofinteraction 28 terms were statistically significant. Lyingfor afi'iend. As shown in Table 14, the overall model was significant, F (9, 2 162) = 8.50.p < .001. adjusted R = .28. Among the five predictors in the first block. subjective norms (B = 0.23. SE = 0.09. ,b’ = .23. p < .01) and injunctive norms (B = 0.47, SE=0.09, B=.44, p<.001) were statistically significant for intention to use injunctive norms-related reasons, but attitude (B = 0.04, SE = 0.10. B = .03. p = .69). descriptive norms (B = —0.05. SE = 0.10. B = —.04. p = .64). and culture (B—‘ —0.32. SE = 0.19, B = — .12, p = .09) were not significant. The results showed that the stronger subjective norms and injunctive norms individuals had, the more highly they intended to use injunctive norms-related reasons when explaining to others their behavioral intention to lie. None of the interactions were significant. indicating that culture did not serve as a moderator. Hypotheses I & 2 Hypothesis I predicted that Koreans would intend to employ norms-related reasoning more strongly than attitudinal reasoning when they would need to explain their behavioral intention to others. Hypothesis 2 predicted that Americans would intend to use attitudinal reasoning more strongly than norm-related reasoning. In order to test the hypotheses, 4 (reasoning type: attitudinal reasoning vs. subjective norms-related reasoning vs. descriptive norms-related reasoning vs. injunctive norms-related reasoning-repeated) x 4 (behavior type: purchasing green products vs. downloading unauthorized media files online vs. telling the truth vs. lying for a friend) x 2 (culture: US. vs. Korea) mixed ANOVA was performed. All cell means and standard deviations for this mixed ANOVA are provided in Tables 16 and 17. Table 18 shows a summary of ANOVA results. The analysis did not reveal a significant main effect for culture. F ( 1 . 71 I) = 0.49, p = .48, n = .00. However, there was a significant main effect for type of behaviors, F(3. 29 2 711): 12.10. p < 001.11 = .03. Post hoc comparisons using Turkey’s procedure (p < .05) showed that intention to say reasons was higher for purchasing green products (M= 4.49a, SD = 1.19) than for telling a truth (M= 4.21b, SD = 1.15). lying for a friend (M= 3.80b, SD = 1.15), and downloading unauthorized media files online (M = 3.91b. SD = 1.68). In addition, The ANOVA yielded a significant main effect for the type of reasoning, F (2.89, 2 2056.55) = 65.92, p < .001. n = .03. To further examine the difference among reasoning types, two complex comparisons (—3. 1, 1. I and 0, 1. —2._ I) and one pairwise comparison (0.—1 , 0, 1) were conducted. This analysis yielded that intention to say attitude-related reasoning (M= 4.12, SD = 1.24) were higher than intention to say norms-related reasoning (M= 4.01), t (718) = —2.65.p = .008. Among intentions to say the norms-related reasons, intention to say descriptive norms-related reasoning (M = 4.26, SD = 1.29) was higher than intention to say subjective norms-related reasoning (M = 3.68, SD = 1.31) and intention to say injunctive norms-related reasoning (M = 4.09. SD = 1.27), t (718) = 8.60, p < .001. Finally, intention to say subjective norms-related reasoning was lower than intention to say injunctive norms-related reasoning. I (718) = —9.29. p < .001. However, these main effects were qualified by a significant 3-way interaction among culture, 2 reasoning. and behavioral type. F(8.68. 2056.55) = 16.60. p < .001. n = .02. A series of paired t-test were conducted to discern which reasoning type was preferred over others for each behavior and for each culture. Korea Tables 19 through 22 showed the results of paired t-tests. For purchasing green products among Korean participants. intention to say attitude-related reasoning (M = 4.86, SD = 0.98) received the highest score. compared to intentions to say subjective norms- 30 related reasoning (M= 4.20. SD = 1.07), descriptive norms-related reasoning (M = 4.25. SD = 1.18), and injunctive norms-related reasoning (M = 4.36, SD = 1.10). There were significant differences between attitudinal reasoning and subjective norms-related reasoning, t (84) = 7.59. p < .001. between attitudinal reasoning and descriptive norms- related reasoning. I (84) = 5.99.p < .001 and between attitudinal reasoning and injunctive norms-related reasoning, I (84) = 5.25. p < .001. On the other hand, there were no significant differences between subjective norms-related reasoning and descriptive norms- related reasoning, I (84) = —0.88. p = .38. between descriptive norms-related reasoning and injunctive norms-related reasoning. t(84) = —l .68. p = .10. When Koreans had to explain their intention to download unauthorized media files via the Internet, however, descriptive norms-related reasoning (M = 4.23, SD = 1.38) was more strongly preferred to attitudinal reasoning (M= 3.88, SD = 1.26), subjective norms-related reasoning (M= 3.56, SD = 1.25), and injunctive norms-related reasoning (M= 3.78, SD = 0.94). Although there was not a significant difference between attitudinal reasoning and injunctive norms-related reasoning t (1 12) = 0.88, p = .38, there were significant differences between attitudinal reasoning and subjective norms-related reasoning, I (1 12) = 4.25.p < .001, between attitudinal reasoning and descriptive reasoning, I (1 12) = -3.1 1,p < .001. Besides, there were significant differences between subjective norms-related reasoning and descriptive norms-related reasoning, t (1 12) = —6.44. p < .001, and between descriptive norms-related reasoning,t(112)= 3.40, p < .001. For the behavior oftelling the truth, injunctive norms-related reasoning (M= 4.02, SD = 0.94) was preferred over attitudinal reasoning (M= 3.88. SD = 0.97), subjective norms-related reasoning (M = 3.92, SD = 1.06), and descriptive norms-related reasoning (M = 3.89, SD = 0.95). Nonetheless, none of comparisons were statistically significant (e.g., between injunctive norms-related reasoning and attitudinal reasoning. I (89) = —1.42. q 31 p = .16). For lying for a friend. however, intention to say descriptive norms-related reasoning (M = 4.28, SD = 0.84) was the strongest, compared to attitudinal reasoning (M = 3.83, SD = 0.84), subjective norms-related reasoning (M= 4.09, SD = 0.91), and injunctive norms-related reasoning (M= 4.12. SD = 1.04). There were statistically significant differences between attitudinal reasoning and descriptive norms-related reasoning, I (90) = —4.22. p < .001 and between attitudinal reasoning and injunctive norms-related reasoning. t (90) = —2.96, p = .004. The findings indicated that Koreans would use different types of reasoning, depending on the type of behaviors. Overall, intentions to use norms-related reasons were higher than intention to use attitudinal reasoning for downloading, telling the truth, and lying for a friend, whereas intention to use attitudinal reasons was higher than intention to norms-related reasons for purchasing green products. However, the difference of preference between norms-related reasoning and attitudinal reasoning was not always statistically significant across the behaviors. Thus, inconsistent with hypothesis 1 or partially consistent with hypothesis 1 to a certain extent, Koreans did not necessarily have stronger intention to say norms-related reasons than attitudinal reasoning consistently across various behaviors. US. Tables 19 through 22 showed the results of paired t-tests. American preferred attitudinal reasoning (M = 5.54, SD = 0.87) over subjective norms-related reasoning (M= 3.12, SD = 1.23), descriptive norms-related reasoning (M = 4.21, SD = 1.40), and injunctive norms-related reasoning (M= 4.63, SD = 1.29) when they had to explain their intention to purchase green products. In addition to significant differences between attitudinal reasoning and subjective norms-related reasoning. I (79) = 14.43, p < .001, between attitudinal reasoning and descriptive norms-related reasoning. I (79) = 8.28. p 32 < .001, and between attitudinal reasoning and injunctive norms-related reasoning. t (79) = 7.27, p < .001. there were statistically significant differences between subjective norms- related reasoning and descriptive norms-related reasoning, t (79) = -6.71.p < .001, between subjective norms-related reasoning, I (79) = -8.40. p < .001, and between descriptive norms-related reasoning and injunctive norms-related reasoning, I (79) = —2.95. p = .004. On the other hand, for the behavior of downloading unauthorized media files online, intention to say descriptive norms-related reasoning (M= 5.01 , SD = 1.37) was higher than attitudinal reasoning (M= 4.25, SD = 1.29). injunctive norms—related reasoning (M= 4.10, SD = 1.37), and subjective norms-related reasoning (M= 2.99, SD = 1.32). There were significant differences between attitudinal reasoning and subjective norms, t (92) = 9.40, p < .001, and between attitudinal reasoning and descriptive reasoning, t (92) = —5.79, p < .001 although the mean difference between attitudinal reasoning and injunctive norm-related reasoning was not statistically significant, t (92) = 1.09, p = .28. When Americans had to explain their behavioral intention to tell the truth, they preferred subjective norms-related reasoning (M = 4.43. SD = 1.48) over attitude-related reasoning (M= 3.88, SD = 1.05), t (84) = —3.75,p < .001. Even though there was no significant difference between attitudinal reasoning and descriptive norms-related reasoning (M= 3.81, SD = 1.30), t (84) = 0.41,p = .68. there was a significant difference between attitudinal reasoning and injunctive norms-related reasoning (M= 4.41 , SD = 1.33), t (84) = —3.61 , p = .001. In addition, there were statistical differences between subjective norms-related reasoning and descriptive norms-related reasoning, I (84) = 3.31, p = .001. and between descriptive norms-related reasoning and injunctive norms-related reasoning, I (84) = —4.22, p< .001. On the other hand, for the behavior of lying for a friend. Americans indicated the strongest intention to say descriptive norms-related reasoning. compared to injunctive norms-related reasoning (M= 3.45. SD = 1.43). attitudinal 33 reasoning (M= 3.04, SD = 0.95). and subjective norms-related reasoning (M= 3.10, SD = 1.32). There was a significant difference between attitudinal reasoning and descriptive norms reasoning, I (82) = —7.77. p < .001, even though there was not significant difference between attitudinal reasoning and subjective norms-related reasoning. t (82) = —-0.48. p = .63. Americans also intended to say different reasoning depending on the behavioral type. When they had to explain their behavioral intention to purchase green products, they were more likely to employ attitudinal reasoning over norms-related reasoning. On the other hand, when they had to explain their behavioral intentions to lie for a friend or to download unauthorized media files online, they were more likely to use descriptive norm- related reasoning than attitudinal reasoning. Americans had stronger intentions to say subjective norms-related and injunctive norms-related reasoning than attitudinal reasoning when they needed to explain their behavioral intention to tell the truth. Thus, the data were inconsistent with hypothesis 2 which predicted that Americans would prefer attitudinal reasoning over norms-related reasoning when explaining their behavioral intention to others. 34 Chapter 4 Discussion Summary The current study aimed to examine how attitude toward a behavior and norms would differently related to behavioral intentions to engage in behaviors, intentions to say attitudinal reasoning, and intentions to say norms-related reasoning. For cultural differences, the current study compared Koreans and Americans regarding whether attitude and norms relevant for behavioral intentions would be also relevant intentions to say attitudinal reasoning and norms-related reasoning. In other word, it was expected that the consistency between internal reasoning and external reasoning would be different for individuals from different cultures. The results ofthe present study provided support for previous claims (Hagger, Chatzisarantis, & Biddle, 2002) that attitude toward a behavior was the significant predictor of behavioral intention across behavioral type and culture. That is, the current findings showed that attitude toward a behavior was the most highly reliable predictor of behavioral intention whether the behaviors were a socially desirable behavior (e.g., purchasing green products), a socially undesirable behavior (e.g., downloading unauthorized media files via the Internet), or decision-making in a dilemmatic situation (e.g., telling the truth and lying for a friend). However, normative components such as subjective norms, descriptive norms, and injunctive norms could be also the significant predictors of behavioral intention depending on the type of behaviors. For downloading unauthorized media files online, subjective norms as well as descriptive norms were significant predictors of behavioral intention. Because many college students may frequently engage in downloading unauthorized media files online, they might think it would be approved by their friends to do it despite its socially undesirable aspects. 35 Individuals may believe that a greater proportion of people in the population engage in a certain behavior that the actual proportion in the population do. For example, college students commonly believed that their peers drank alcohol more than college student themselves did (Neighbors, Dillard, Lewis, Bergstrom, & Neil, 2006). In addition, college students believed their peers had more positive attitude toward heavily drinking than college student themselves did (Prentice & Miller, 1993). This perceived norm was the significant predictors of heavy drinking for college students (Borsari & Carey, 2003). Thus, because the perceived descriptive norms of downloading unauthorized media files were strong, they were more likely to intend to engage in downloading unauthorized meida file on the Internet. Norms affected attitudinal reasoning although attitude was most strongly related to attitudinal reasoning across the different behaviors. By contrast. attitude toward a behavior influenced norms-related reasoning, depending on the type of behaviors. In addition, various perceived norms differently influenced intention to say norms-related reasoning across the different type of behaviors. Although there was correspondence between the type of individuals’ perceived norms and the type of norms that individuals intended to say (e.g., individuals’ perceived injunctive norms was mostly strongly related to individuals’ intention to say injunctive norms-related reasoning), intention to use descriptive norms-related reasoning was also affected by attitude toward a behavior. Except for lying for a friend, the more positive attitude people had, the more likely they intended to explain their behavioral intention with descriptive norms-related reasoning. On the other hand. the less positive attitude people had, the more likely they intended to explain their behavioral intention to lie for a friend with descriptive norms-related reasons, whereas the more positive attitude toward a behavior individuals had. the more likely individuals intended to lie for a friend. For lying 36 for a friend, the findings seemed to be reasonable because individuals were less likey to lie for a friend ifthey believed lying was morally wrong or bad. Furthremore, individuals were more likely to excuse their behavior of lying for a friend with descriptive norms- related reasons, probably because they had a negative feeling for lie. In this case individuals were more likely tojustify their behavior with norms which individuals believed to be prevalent in the society maybe because accounts declining the social conflict (either concrete or potential) could be utilized as a tool to decreas the gap between expectation and action (Toby. 1952). Thus. people might have believed that saying their unethical behavior with norms provided some room to justify their behavioral intention. Implication for socially desirable and undesirable behaviors According to mixed ANOVA. attitudinal reasoning for purchasing green products was rated the highest than any other reasonigs while attitudinal reasoning for lying for a friend was rated the lowest than any other reasonings. The current findings indicated that individuals selected additional reasonings for socially desirable behaviors. That is, when individuals needed to explain to others their behavioral intentions of socially desirable behaviors, individuals’ perceived injunctive norms were a significant predictor of intention to say attitudinal reasoning, although individuals’ perceived injunctive norms were not a significant predictor of behavioral intention to engage in the behaviors. In other word, individuals used attitude-related reasons due to social approval of their behavior, even though injunctive norms were not the reason why they intended to buy green products. Although individuals preferred employing attitudinal reasonings, they might have thought about whether many people would approve the behavior, maybe because desire for beingining to society is a fundanmental motivation for individuals (Baumeister & Leary, I995). Leary et a1. (2003) demonstrated that social approval or disapporal affected state self-esteem referring to how people believed they were evaluated by others 37 in the immediate social context. That is, they could use attitudinal reason more strongly because they believed many people would approve their behavior. In turns, this choice would increase their self-esteems. However, subjective norms were a significant predictor of intention to say attitudinal reasons for socially undesirable behaviors such as lying for a friend when individuals had to explain their behavior intention. Individual might be still concerned with how their important others would think about their behavioral intention, even though they had the strongest intention to use attitudinal reasons. Furthermore, regression analysis revealed that intention to lie for a friend with descriptive norms-related reasons is related to not only descriptive norms but also subjective norms. In general, individuals would consider how other people would think about their behavioral intention when they would have to explain their behavioral intention. Explaining their own behavioral intention should be consistent with popular ideas in a particular society. Otherwise, explanation would not be accepted by others (Scott & Lyman, 1968).These ideas would be different in terms of behavior. When people had to explain their socially undesirable behavioral intention to others, they would consider how their significant others would think about their behavioral intention. That is, individuals would select reasons between attitudinal reasoning and normative reasoning in order to manage their impression to others when others would pay attention to the individuals’ behaviors. Furthermore, intention to say injunctive norms-related reasoning was related to attitude toward a behavior for purchasing green products and downloading unauthorized media files. Apparently, people would use norm-related reasoning in spite of their positive attitude toward a behavior. That is, it is possible to employ norms-related reasoning over attitudinal reasoning in order to hide their positive attitude toward behaviors because it is not socially approved to show the positive attitude toward behavior or it is socially 38 desirable to be modest. For example, people might think that an individual is not ethical if he or she says how much he or she loves to download unauthorized media files online. Thus, people may use norms-related reasoning in order not to take a risk despite their favorable attitude toward a behavior or in order to show their modesty. Nonetheless. this pattern does not fit every significant relationship between four different reasoning/behavioral intention and TRA components. For example, Injunctive norm was a significant predictor of intention to tell the truth, but not of intention to lie for a friend, probably because telling the truth is a morally right thing to do. However, injunctive norms were not significantly related to intention to download unauthorized media files via online. Besides, for telling a truth classified as one of socially desirable behavior, the more positive attitude toward a behavior individuals had, the more strongly they employ subjective norms-reasoning whereas for downloading unauthorized media file via Internet, the more positive attitude individuals had, the more strongly they say their behavioral intention with subjective norms. Implicationfor behavior involving interpersonal relationships Private behaviors (e.g., purchasing green products vs. downloading unauthorized media files online) and the behavior involving interpersonal relationships (e.g., telling the truth to a supervisor vs. lying for a friend) could have different patterns. Intention to lie for a friend was most likely to be influenced by subjective norms over attitude toward a behavior. Because individuals might have considered friendship as important, they were more likely to lie for a friend even though lying may not be ethical. Besides, the intention to tell the truth was predicted by subjective norms. attitude toward a behavior, and injunctive norms. Specifically, subjective norms had an effect on behavioral intention to tell the truth as much as attitude toward a behavior did. Interestingly, subjective norms were also the significant predictor of behavioral intention to lie for a friend. It is possibly 39 considered that as an employee. his/her supervisor would be one of more important people than his/her friend. Furthermore, the behavior involving interpersonal relationships was more likely to be affected by subjective norms when individuals intended to use injunctive norms-related reasoning whereas private behaviors were more likely to be influenced by attitude toward a behavior. Implication for cross—culture There were differences between Koreans and Americans in the light of the relationship between attitude/norms-related reasoning and attitudinal/normative components. First, the relationship between subjective norms or injunctive norms and attitudinal reasoning differed according to culture. When Koreans and Americans had to explain their purchasing intention of green products. Koreans who had more subjective norm were more likely to say attitudinal reasons while Americans who had less strong subjective norm were more likely to employ attitude-related reasons. Also, Koreans with less strong injunctive norms were more likely to say their intention to download unauthorized media files online, while Americans with stronger injunctive norms strongly had the intention to say about their behavioral intention. In other word, in purchasing green product case, when Americans employed attitude-related reasons to explain their behavioral intention to others, they were more concerned with how much they liked the behavior, how much they believed many people would support the behavior, but they were less concerned with how much their significant others would approve the behavior. Besides, when Americans would intend to say attitudinal reasoning to explain their behavioral intention of downloading unauthorized media files online, they were more concerned with how they liked the behavior, how much they believed many people including their important others would support the behavior. However, when Koreans intended to use attitude-related reasons to justify their behavioral intention to purchase 40 green products. they were more concerned with how much they liked the behavior, how much many people including important others would support the behavior, whereas in order tojustify their behavioral intention of downloading unauthorized media flies via the Internet with attitude-related reasons, Koreans were more concerned with how much they liked the behavior, how much their important others would approve it, but less concerned with how much many people would approve it. To sum up, Koreans and Americans indicated different relationships between attitude-related reasoning and normative components depending on the type of behaviors. Second, norms-related reasoning was also significantly related to attitude toward a behavior depending on the culture across the type of behaviors except for lying for a friend. For example. Koreans who had more positive attitude toward purchasing green products, downloading unauthorized media files. or telling the truth were more likely to use norms-related reasoning. On the other hand, Americans with less positive attitude (or even negative attitude) were more likely to have the intention to explain the behavioral intention with norms-related reasoning. In general. despite positive attitude toward a behavior, Koreans were more likely to use norms-related reasoning than Americans. T-test analysis revealed that Koreans would intend to employ the subjective norms-related reasoning more strongly than Americans would for explaining intentions to purchase green products and to lie for a friend. In addition, Americans would intend to employ the descriptive norms-related reasoning less strongly than Koreans would when the behavior was about downloading. As shown in Figure 3, when they had to explain their behavior intention to tell the truth, Americans were more likely to use subjective norms-related and injunctive norms-related reasoning compared to attitude and descriptive norms-related reasoning. However. Koreans did not show much differences among the different types of reasoning. 41 Limitations The study did not examine individuals’ preferred choice on a specific type of reasoning with TRA components and injunctive and descriptive norms. Participants were not asked to select one reason over the other, but to indicate their varying preferences for all the TRA components and injunctive and descriptive norms. Thus, it cannot be concluded that Koreans would prefer explaining their behavioral intention only with norms-related reasons, while Americans would prefer explaining their behavioral intention only with attitude-related reason in spite ofthe differences among behaviors. Nonetheless, in reality. peOple may use most ofthe several types ofdifferent reasoning such as attitudinal reasoning and norms-related reasoning at the same time when they explain their behaviors. This study examined how each explanation was related with individual’s attitude and perceived norms. But it did not investigate the reasons why there were different relationships between each reasoning type and TRA components. Future research can examine the reasons why the relationships between each reasoning type and TRA components varied across different behaviors. Conclusion This study showed that there were significantly different relationships between behavioral intentions and attitudinal/normative components and also between attitude/norms-related reasoning and attitudinal/normative components. In other word, this study examined how these relationships were different from the typical TRA relationships. Besides, these relationships differed depending on the behavioral type. Even though there was no main effect for culture. there were significant interactions between culture and behavioral type. 42 Table I. Reliabilities, Correlations. Means. and Standard Deir'iationsjor Purchasing Green Products. IQ Koreans (tif= 83) 2 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. Attitude Subjective norms Descriptive norms Injunctive norms Behavioral intention to purchase green products Intention to use attitudinal reasoning Intention to use subjective norms-related reasoning . Intention to use descriptive norms-related reasoning . Intention to use injunctive norms-related reasoning M SD .94 .73*** .59*** .70*** 6D*** .84*** .73*** .71*** 77*** 4.75 (0.96) .71*** .79*** .63*** .71*** 8°*** .76*** .74*** 4.11 (1.07) .94 ‘78*** .57*** .59*** .59*** .74*** ‘6l*** 4.24 (1.18) .59*** .70*** .70*** '68*** .74*** 4.39 (1.08) .97 .57*** .59*** .65*** .63*** 4.17 (1.30) .96 _70*** .64*** .66*** 4.86 (0.98) .94 .86*** .83*** 4.20 (1.07) .92 .87*** 4.25 (1.18) .92 4.36 (1.10) Americans (41’: 78) 1. 2. 3. Attitude Subjective norms Descriptive norms 4. Injunctive 5. norms Behavioral intention to purchase green products .91 31** '39*** .66*** '00 UI .26* 23* .44*** .89 .40*** .12 .83 .30" .96 43 Table 1 (cont’d). IQ DJ UI 6. 7. Intention to use attitudinal reasoning Intention to use subjective norms- related reasoning . Intention to use descriptive norms- related reasoning . Intention to use inj unctive norms- related reasoning AI SD .68*** 24* .34** 5u49 «192) .05 .73*** 24* L105 (L21) 6D*** .30“ 4.25 (1.30) *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001 Reliabilities (Cronbach’s a) are reported on the diagonal. .48*** .09 24* '46*** .L95 (L02) 4o*** 28* .09 zf26 (L67) .94 .01 56* .51*** 554 «187) .88 .40*** .91 5§*** 4sn (L40) Due to missing data. d/‘was = 77 for correlations involving this variable. 44 .89 4.63 (1.29) Table 2. Reliabilities. (I'tn‘relations. illeans. and Standard Deviations/or Telling a Ttruth Koreans (df= 88) 1. Attitude 2. Subjective norms 3. Descriptive norms 4. Injunctive norms 5. Behavioral intention to tell the truth 6. Intention to use attitudinal reasoning 7. Intention to use subjective norms- related reasoning 8. Intention to use descriptive norms- related reasoning 9. lntention to use injunctive norms- related reasoning A4 SD .91 '60*** .40*** .47*** .55*** 67*** .59m .58*** .50*** :19: «198) .87 4§*** .55*** .51*** .37*** .59*** §D*** .57*** :189 (L04) .90 '63*** .45*** .45*** .38*** .57*** :190 (L01) .90 57*** .47*** .64*** 67*** 4.01 (1.04) .74 99*** 'fi Ur * * * .48*** .54*** :195 (L09) .50*** .55*** .43*** :188 «197) .90 .69*** .71*** 3.92 (1.06) .83 .76*** 3.89 (0.95) .84 4103 «194) Americans ((17: 83) 1. Attitude 2. Subjective norms 3. Descriptive norms 4. Injunctive § I'IOI’ITIS .54*** .07 26* .91 -.02 .39*** .89 .50*** .91 45 Table 2 (cont’d). 5. Behavioral intention to tell the truth 6. Intention to use attitudinal reasoning 7. Intention to use subjective norms- related reasoning 8. Intention to use descriptive nonns- related reasoning 9. Intention to use injunctive norms- related 9 reasoning M SD .49*** .67*** .30** .36** 3.70 «189) 47*** .69*** .18 .48*** 4.98 (1.48) -.01 -.01 45*** 23* 3.86 (1.31) .34“ .24* .32** .26* 4§*** 4.68 (1.23) .90 .44*** .44*** .07 .24* 4.77 (1.53) .89 .47*** .92 .37*** .61*** 3.88 (1.05) 4.43 (1.48) .91 .51*** .91 3.81 (1.30) 4.41 (1.33) *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001 Reliabilities (Cronbach’s 0.) are reported on the diagonal. Item 4 in injunctive norms and in injunctive norms-related reasoning was in order to improve reliability. Items 4 and 5 were deleted in order to improve reliability. 46 Table 3. Reliabilities. Correlations. Means, and Standard Deviationsfor Lyingfor a Friend. FJ Koreans W= 89) I. Attitude 2. Subjective norms 3. Descriptive norms 4. Injunctive norms 5. Behavioral intention to lie 6. Intention to use attitudinal reasoning 7. Intention to use subjective norms- related reasoning 8. Intention to use descriptive nonns- related reasoning 9. Intention to use injunctive norrns- related reasoning M SD .89 .49*** 23* '38*** 79** 77*** .44*** 4|*** 3.81 (0.88) be Ur §7*** §8*** §l*** .35** .66*** .49*** 4;*** 4.04 (1.11) .77*** .53*** .06 .38*** .47*** .30** 1147 (L07) .88 .48*** .26* .41*** 4|*** .47*** 4.27 (1.07) .37*** .22* ‘123 (L09) .90 .41*** 26* .Sl*** 3.83 «184) .80 .59*** .77 .6]*** .58*** .84 4.12 (1.04) 4.09 (0.91) 4.28 (0.84) Americans (91/: 80) 1 . Attitude 2. Subjective norms 3. Descriptive norms 4. Injunctive :8 ”011113 .86 .39*** .46*** '60**# .90 47 Table 3 (cont’d). I\) 5. Behavioral intention to lie 6. Intention to use attitudinal reasoning ‘ 7. Intention to use subjective norms- related reasoning 8. Intention to use descriptive norms- related reasoning .65*** .68*** .31** -.02 .59*** .57*** ‘63*** 31** .28* .22* Id 'JJ * .39*** .45*** .94 §D*** .30** .08 .91 .49*** .80 .56*** .93 9. Intention to use injunctive norms- related # reasoning M SD .13 3.01 (0.91) .44*** 3.37 (1.36) 4D*** 4.25 (1.30) .60*** 4.95 (1.02) 79* ‘126 (L67) 79** 5.54 (0.87) .68*** .76*** 4.21 (1.40) .90 4.63 (1.29) *p< .05, **p< .01, ***p< .001 Reliabilities (Cronbach’s 0.) are reported on the diagonal. Due to missing data, df for correlations involving injunctive norms was 79. \ Due to missing data, (If for correlation between attitudinal reasoning and attitude/normative reasoning was 81. Item 4 in injunctive norms and in injunctive norms-related reasoning was in order to improve reliability. 48 Table 4. Reliabilities, (’(n‘relations. Means, and Standard Deviations/or Downloading Unauthorized .Media Files Online. i0 Koreans (cif= l | l) I. Attitude 2. Subjective norms 3. Descriptive norms 4. Injunctive norms 5. Behavioral intention to download 6. Intention to use attitudinal reasoning 7. Intention to use subjective norms- related reasoning 8. Intention to use descriptive norms- related reasoning 9. Intention to use injunctive norms- related reasoning M SD .95 .70*** .36*** .54*** .77*** .80*** 71*** .51*** .64*** id '4) I0 V (L .80 .48*** .66*** .74*** .67*** 7|*** .54*** .57*** .89 .48*** .47*** }o** .30** '56*** .30*** 4.78 (1.48) .81 §4*** 43*** .53*** .44*** .54*** :166 (L15) .88 6|*** .57*** .52*** .58*** .96 _79*** .60*** .67*** .90 .65*** .90 .61*** .89 3.78 (1.28) Americans (4f: 93) 1. Attitude 2. Subjective norms 3. Descriptive norms 4. Injunctive nonns . J .93 .59*** .21* “*** .87 .07 .50*** .87 49 Table 4 (cont’d). IJ 5. 6. 7. Behavioral intention to download Intention to use attitudinal reasoning Intention to use subjective norms- related reasoning . Intention to use descriptive norms- related reasoning . Intention to use injunctive norms- related reasoning M SD 7§*** _80*** .46*** _4,... .50**# 4.07 (1.24) .58*** .57*** .74*** ’58** .45W 2 .99 (1.38) '29** .30** .50W .30** 5.96 (0.99) .48*** .56*** .48*** .34M '66:“- 4.30 (1.36) .89 .69*** .40*** _40... .42W 3.72 (L68) .94 .51*** _50... ‘125 (L28) .88 .34“ '41] 'fl 'I‘ f *I’ :199 (L32) .83 .57... 5.01 (1.36) .88 410 (L37) *p< .05, **p < .01, ***p< .001 Reliabilities (Cronbach’s or) are reported on the diagonal. 50 Table 5. Regression Resultsfor Intentions to Purchase Green Product and to Download Unauthorized Media Files Online. B SE B t sr Purchasing Green Product First Block Intercept 4.29 0.14 29.78*** ATT 0.77 0.12 .52 6.45*** .3 SN 0.34 0.10 .29 3.57*** .21 DN —0.02 0.09 —.01 —0.1 8 —.01 [N 0.08 0.12 .05 0.64 .04 Culture] —0.19 0.24 —.06 —0.79 —.05 9 F(5. 158) = 26.28, p < .001, adj. R“ = .44 Second Block ATT X Culture 0.65 0.24 .28 2.68** .15 SN X Culture 0.1 1 0.23 .06 0.48 .03 DN >< Culture —0.27 0.20 —.16 —I .36 —.08 IN >< Culture 0.06 0.26 .03 0.23 .01 Fchange (4, 154) = 3.08. p < .05, chhange = .04 The overall model: F(9, 154) = 16.73. p < .001, adj. R2 = .47 Downloading unauthorized files online First Block Intercept 4.01 0.96 41.83*** ATT 0.66 0.07 .54 9.30*** .40 SN 0.29 0.07 .26 4.11*** .18 DN 0.15 0.06 .14 2.67** .12 IN 0.02 0.07 .02 0.34 .02 Culture] —0.38 0.16 —.13 —2.42* —.10 2 F(5, 201 ) = 57.34. p < .001. aa’j.R = .62 Second Block ATT X Culture 0.27 0.14 .15 1.88 .08 SN >< Culture —0.09 0.15 —.06 —0.60 —.03 DN X Culture 0.12 0.12 .06 1.00 .04 IN >< Culture 0.03 0.14 .02 0.24 .01 9 Fchange (4, 197) = 1.84, p = .12, Ruchange = .01 The overall model: F(9, 197) = 39.26, p < .001, adj. R2 = .63 *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001 sr: semipartial correlation Regarding multicollinearity, the first-order and second-order predictors had variance inflation factor (VIF) ranging from 1.48 to 8.80. Cohen, Cohen, West, and Aiken (2003) mentioned 10 as the traditional rule of thumb threshold value, although there is “no good statistical rationale for the choice of any of the traditional rule of thumb threshold values” (p. 424). BI: Behavioral intention ATT: Attitude toward behavior SN: Subjective norms DN: Descriptive norms IN: Inj unctive norms AR: Attitudinal reasoning SNR: Subjective norms-related reasoning DNR: Descriptive norms-related reasoning INR: Injunctive norms-related reasoning dummy—coded with Americans = 1 and Koreans = 0 51 Table 6. Regression Resultsfor Intentions to Tell the Truth and to Lie. B SE B t sr Truth-Telling First Block Intercept 4.14 0.12 33.40*** .23 ATT 0.41 0.11 .28 3.89*** .20 SN 0.28 0.08 .28 3.43M —.05 DN —0.71 0.09 —.06 —0.84 .15 IN 0.25 0.10 .21 2.61* .13 Culturel 0.43 0.19 .15 221* 2 F(5, 168) = 24.57,p < .001. adj.R = .41 Second Block ATT X Culture 0.15 0.22 .07 0.70 .04 SN X Culture 0.15 0.18 .11 0.82 .05 DN X Culture —0.28 0.18 —.18 —1.54 —.09 IN X Culture 0.03 0.20 .02 0.17 .01 2 Fchange (4. 164) = 1.08. p = .37, R change = .02 The overall model: F(9. 164) = 14.15. p < .001. adj. R = .41 Lying for a Friend First Block Intercept 4.01 0.11 35.87*** ATT 0.38 0.10 .30 3.92*** .23 SN 0.36 0.08 .37 4.46*** .27 DN 0.16 0.09 .14 1.67 .10 IN —0.03 0.09 —.03 —0.36 —-.02 Culture] —().09 0.18 ~.04 ——0.49 -—.03 9 F(5, 166) = 22.75.p <. .001. adj.R = .39 Second Block ATT X Culture 0.60 0.19 .32 3.17“ .18 SN X Culture 0.12 0.16 .09 0.76 .04 DN X Culture —0.33 0.19 —.21 —1.73 -.10 IN X Culture —0.10 0.19 —.07 ~0.49 —.03 2 Fchange (4. 162): l.84.p = .001. R change = .06 2 The overall model: F(9, 162) = 16.04.p < .001, adj. R = .44 *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001 sr: semipartial correlation Regarding multicollinearity, the first-order and second-order predictors had variance inflation factor (VIF) ranging from 1.48 to 8.80. Cohen, Cohen, West, and Aiken (2003) mentioned 10 as the traditional rule of thumb threshold value, although there is “no good statistical rationale for the choice of any of the traditional rule of thumb threshold values” (p. 424). Bl: Behavioral intention ATT: Attitude toward behavior SN: Subjective norms DN: Descriptive norms 1N: Injunctive norms AR: Attitudinal reasoning SNR: Subjective norms—related reasoning DNR: Descriptive norms-related reasoning INR: Injunctive norms-related reasoning dummy-coded with Americans = l and Koreans = 0 52 Table 7. Regression Resultsfor Intentions to Use Attitudinal Reasoningfor Explaining Purchasing Green Product and Downloading Unauthorized Media Files Online. 8 SE B t sr Purchasing Green Product First Block Intercept 5.20 0.73 71 .19*** ATT 0.65 0.06 .66 10.78*** .48 SN —0.70 0.05 —.09 —0.60 —.06 DN -0.03 0.05 -.03 —0.18 —.03 IN 0.27 0.06 .30 4.56*** .21 Culturel ——0.02 0.12 —*.01 —0.14 —.01 F(5, 159) = 68.51,p < .001. adj.R2 = .67 Second Block ATT X Culture —0.05 0.12 — .04 — 0.43 —.02 SN X Culture —0.25 0.12 —.23 — 2.16* —.10 DN X Culture —0.07 0.10 —.07 —0.73 —.03 IN X Culture 0.14 0.13 .10 1.06 .05 Fchange (4. 155) = 2.34. p =.058. R2change = .02 The overall model: F(9. 155.): 40.38. p <1 .001, adj. R2 = .68 Downloading unauthorized files online First Block Intercept 3.95 0.78 51.06*** ATT 0.69 0.06 .67 12.16*** .49 SN 0.15 0.06 .15 2.64“ .11 DN 0.04 0.05 .05 0.99 .04 IN 0.03 0.06 .30 0.59 .02 Culture 0.22 0.13 .09 1.71 .07 F(5, 201) = 82.02. p < .001. adj.R2 = .66 Second Block ATT X Culture —0.03 0.1 l —.02 —0.28 —.01 SN X Culture —0.15 0.12 —.11 —1.30 —.05 DN X Culture 0.15 0.10 .08 1.45 .06 IN X Culture 0.23 0.11 .17 2.05* .08 2 Fchange (4, 197) = 2.21.1) = .12. R change : .01 2 The overall model: F(9. 197) = 47.65. p < .001. adj. R = .67 *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001 sr: semipartial correlation Regarding multicollinearity. the first-order and second-order predictors had variance inflation factor (VIF) ranging from 1.48 to 8.80. Cohen, Cohen, West, and Aiken (2003) mentioned 10 as the traditional rule ofthumb threshold value, although there is “no good statistical rationale for the choice of any ofthe traditional rule ofthumb threshold values" (p. 424). BI: Behavioral intention ATT: Attitude toward behavior SN: Subjective norms DN: Descriptive norms IN: Injunctive norms AR: Attitudinal reasoning SNR: Subjective norms-related reasoning DNR: Descriptive norms-related reasoning INR: Injunctive norms-related reasoning dummy-coded with Americans = 1 and Koreans : 0 53 Table 8. Regression Results/or Intentions to Use Attitudinal Reasoningfl1r Explaining Truth- Telling and Lying. 2 F(5, 169) = 26.68, p < .001. adj.R = .42 B SE B t sr Truth-telling First Block Intercept 3.84 0.09 43.16*** .47 ATT 0.62 0.08 .58 814*“ .03 SN 0.03 0.06 .04 0.44 .08 DN 0.09 0.06 .10 1.48 .05 IN 0.06 0.07 .07 0.81 .03 Culturel 0.08 0.14 04 0.55 Second Block ATT X Culture 0.15 0.15 .10 1.09 .06 SN X Culture 0.15 0.13 .21 1.56 .09 DN X Culture —0.28 0.13 —-.07 —0.61 —.04 IN X Culture —0.03 0.14 —.16 —1.32 —.08 Fchange (4. 165) = 1.84, p = .12, chhange = .02 7 The overall model: F(9. 165) = 15.37. p < .001. adj. R“ = .43 Lying for a friend First Block Intercept 3.53 0.07 4864*" ATT 0.61 0.06 .61 9.65*** .48 SN 0.14 0.05 .18 2.67M .13 DN —0.08 0.06 —. | 0 —1.23 —.06 IN 0.03 0.06 .03 0.45 .02 Culture —0. 1 6 0.12 —.08 —1.39 —.07 F(5, 166) = 48.76, p < .001. adj.R2 = .58 Second Block ATT X Culture —0.10 0.13 —.07 0.81 —.04 SN X Culture 0.15 0.11 .14 1.39 .07 DN X Culture 0.24 0.13 .19 1.87 .09 IN X Culture -0.15 0.13 —.14 —l.19 —.06 2 Fchange (4, 162) = 1.84, p = .098, R change = .02 2 The overall model: F(9, 162) = 28.62. p < .001, adj. R = .59 *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001 sr: semipartial correlation Regarding multicollinearity. the first—order and second-order predictors had variance inflation factor (VIF) ranging from 1.48 to 8.80. Cohen, Cohen, West. and Aiken (2003) mentioned 10 as the traditional rule ofthumb threshold value, although there is “no good statistical rationale for the choice of any of the traditional rule of thumb threshold values” (p. 424). BI: Behavioral intention ATT: Attitude toward behavior SN: Subjective norms DN: Descriptive norms IN: Injunctive norms AR: Attitudinal reasoning SNR: Subjective norms-related reasoning DNR: Descriptive norms-related reasoning INR: Injunctive norms-related reasoning I . . dummy-coded w1th Amer1cans = l and Koreans = 0 54 Table 9. Regression Resultsfor Intentions to Use Subjective .I'V(n-'ni.s-Reasoning/or Explaining Purchasing Green Product and Downloadng Unauthorized Media Files Online. 8 SE B t SI Purchasing Green Product First Block Intercept 3.84 0.10 40.21 *** ATT 0.1 l 0.08 .09 1.41 .06 SN 0.74 0.06 .73 11.50*** .52 DN 0.05 0.06 .05 0.88 .04 IN —0.07 0.08 —.06 —0.87 —.04 Culture —0.34 0.16 —.13 —2.16* —.10 7 A.— F(5, 159) = 64.65. p < .001. adj.R = .66 Second Block ATT X Culture —0.35 0.16 —.18 —2.18* -.10 SN X Culture 0.15 0.16 .10 —0.96 .04 DN X Culture 0.13 0.13 .10 1.00 .05 IN X Culture ~0.20 0.17 —.11 —1.15 —.05 2 Fchange (4, 155) = 2.14.17 =.079, R change = .02 2 The overall model: F(9. 155) = 37.89. p < .001. adj. R = .70 Downloading unauthorized files online First Block Intercept 3.45 0.09 3830*“ ATT 0.20 0.07 .19 2.99** .14 SN 0.55 0.07 .55 8.34*** .38 DN —0.20 0.05 —.02 ~0.36 —.02 IN 0.10 0.06 .10 1.53 .07 Culture —0.32 0.15 —. 1 2 —2.17 —.10 F(5, 200) = -4.5-.p < .001, adj. R2 = .57 Second Block ATT X Culture ——0.46 0.13 —.29 —3.52** —.16 SN X Culture 0.24 0.13 .17 1.79 .08 DN X Culture 0.19 0.12 .11 1.62 .07 IN X Culture 0.03 0.13 .02 0.24 .01 2 Fchange (4, 196) = 3.60. p = .007, R change = .03 2 The overall model: F(9, 196) = 33.48, p < .001, adj. R = .59 *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001 sr: semipartial correlation Regarding multicollinearity. the first-order and second-order predictors had variance inflation factor (VIF) ranging from 1.48 to 8.80. Cohen, Cohen, West. and Aiken (2003) mentioned 10 as the traditional rule of thumb threshold value, although there is “no good statistical rationale for the choice of any ofthe traditional rule ofthumb threshold values” (p. 424). Bl: Behavioral intention ATT: Attitude toward behavior SN: Subjective norms DN: Descriptive norms IN: Injunctive norms AR: Attitudinal reasoning SNR: Subjective norms-related reasoning DNR: Descriptive norms-related reasoning INR: Injunctive norms-related reasoning dummy-coded with Americans = 1 and Koreans = 0 55 Table 10. Regression Results/or Intentions to Use Subjective Norms-Reasoning/or Truth- Telling and Lying. 8 SE t sr Truth-telling First Block Intercept 4.18 0.1 I 38.98*** .20 ATT 0.33 0.09 .24 362*” .35 SN 0.46 0.07 .49 6.49*** —.06 DN —0.08 0.07 —.07 -1.09 .1 1 IN 0.17 0.08 .16 204* —.01 Culture ——0.04 0.17 7-03 —0.23 F(5. 169) = 34.64, p < .001. adj. R2 = .42 Second Block ATT X Culture —0.00 0.19 —.00 —0.02 —.00 SN X Culture 0.30 0.16 .24 1.90 .10 DN X Culture —0.01 0.16 —.00 —0.04 —.00 IN X Culture —0.21 0.17 —-.14 —l.22 ~—.07 Fchange (4, 165) = 1.19. p = .32. R2c11ai1ge = .01 The overall model: F(9, 165) = 19.86. [K .001. adj. R2 = .49 Lying for a friend First Block Intercept 3.88 0.10 39.76*** ATT 0.06 0.09 .05 0.74 .04 SN 0.53 0.07 .56 7.58*** .41 DN —0.02 0.08 —.02 ~02 —.01 IN 0.09 0.08 .09 1.13 .06 Culture —0.52 0.15 — 22 —3.39** —.18 F(5. 166) = 36.50,p < .001. adj. R2 = .51 Second Block ATT X Culture —0.1 7 0.17 —.09 —0.98 —.05 SN X Culture 0.13 0.15 .10 0.87 .05 DN X Culture 0.00 0.18 .00 0.02 .00 IN X Culture 0.08 0.17 .06 0.48 .03 '1 Fchange (4. 162) = 0.57. p = .389. Rbchange = .01 The overall model: F(9, 162) = 20.32. p < .001, adj. R2 = .50 *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001 sr: semipartial correlation Regarding multicollinearity, the first-order and second-order predictors had variance inflation factor (VIF) ranging from 1.48 to 8.80. Cohen, Cohen. West, and Aiken (2003) mentioned 10 as the traditional rule ofthumb threshold value, although there is “no good statistical rationale for the choice of any ofthe traditional rule ofthumb threshold values" (p. 424). BI: Behavioral intention ATT: Attitude toward behavior SN: Subjective norms DN: Descriptive norms IN: Injunctive norms AR: Attitudinal reasoning SNR: Subjective norms-related reasoning DNR: Descriptive norms-related reasoning INR: Injunctive norms-related reasoning dummy-coded with Americans = 1 and Koreans = 0 56 Table l 1. Regression Results/or Intentions to Use Descriptive Norms-Reasoningfor Explaining Purchasing Green Product and Downloading Unauthorized Media Files Online. 8 SE B t W Purchasing Green Product First Block Intercept 4.26 0.12 3612*“ ATT 0.29 0.10 .23 3.00** .17 SN 0.14 0.08 .14 1.83 .10 DN 0.60 0.10 .57 8.10*** .45 IN —0.09 0.19 «.07 —0.90 —.05 Culture] —0.06 0.17 —.03 -0.33 —.02 2 F(5. 159) = 33.42.); < .001. alt/1R = .50 Second Block ATT X Culture -—-0.18 0.20 —.09 —0.90 —.05 SN X Culture —0.32 0.19 -.22 —1.67 —.09 DN X Culture 0.24 0.16 .17 1.46 .08 IN X Culture 0.05 0.22 .03 0.21 .01 2 Fchange (4, 155) = 1.6I,p =.l75, R change = .02 7 A. The overall Model: F(9.155) = 19.56 p<.001. adj. R = .51 Downloading unauthorized files online First Block Intercept 4.47 0.1 1 3968*" ATT 0.30 0.08 .26 3.65*** .19 SN 0.11 0.08 .10 1.32 .07 DN 0.44 0.07 .44 6.74*** .36 IN 0.01 0.08 .01 0.18 .01 Culture] 0.25 0.19 .09 1.33 .07 2 F(5, 200) = 31.23. p < .001. adj.R = .42 Second Block ATT X Culture 0.02 0.17 .01 —0.11 .01 SN X Culture —0.13 0.17 —.08 —0.74 —.04 DN X Culture 0.23 0.15 .12 1.59 .08 IN X Culture 0.00 0.16 .00 0.03 .00 2 Fchange (4, 196) = 0.78, p = .538, R change = .01 2 The overall model: F(9. 196) = 17.62, p < .001. adj. R = .42 *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001 sr: semipartial correlation Regarding multicollinearity. the first-order and second-order predictors had variance inflation factor (VIF) ranging from 1.48 to 8.80. Cohen. Cohen, West, and Aiken (2003) mentioned 10 as the traditional rule of thumb threshold value, although there is “no good statistical rationale for the choice of any of the traditional rule of thumb threshold values” (p. 424). BI: Behavioral intention ATT: Attitude toward behavior SN: Subjective norms DN: Descriptive norms IN: Injunctive norms AR: Attitudinal reasoning SNR: Subjective norms-related reasoning DNR: Descriptive norms-related reasoning [N R: Injunctive norms-related reasoning dummy-coded with Americans = 1 and Koreans = 0 57 Table 12. Regression Results for Intentions to Use Descriptive Norms-Reasoning/or Explaining Truth- Telling and Lying. 8 SE B t sr Truth-telling First Block Intercept 3.90 0.1 1 36.54*** .23 ATT 0.34 0.09 38 3.67*** .04 SN 0.05 0.07 .06 0.64 .31 DN 0.37 0.07 .38 5.08*** .06 IN 0.08 0.08 .08 0.97 —.03 Culture] ~0.09 0.17 —-04 0.55 2 F(5, 169) = 18.62. p < 001.6417? = .34 Second Block ATT X Culture 0.05 0.19 03 0.29 .02 SN X Culture 0.03 0.15 .03 1.17 .01 DN X Culture 0.28 0.16 .22 1.76 .11 IN X Culture —0.39 0.17 —.30 —2.30* —.14 Fchange (4. 165) = 1.50, p = .205. chhange = .02 The overall model: F(9. 165.): 1 1.13, p <' .001, adj. R2 = .34 Lying for a friend First Block Intercept 4.35 0.1 1 38.19*** ATT “0.21 0.10 —.18 ~2.08* ~.14 SN 0.21 0.08 .23 2.57* .17 DN 0.32 0.10 .32 3.37** .22 IN 0.15 0.09 .15 1.61 .11 Culture] —0.07 0.18 —.03 —0.40 —.03 F(5, 166) = 13.38. p < .001, adj.R2 = .27 Second Block ATT X Culture —0.25 0.20 —.14 —1.24 —.08 SN X Culture —.0.08 0.17 —.07 —0.48 —.03 DN X Culture 0.24 0.20 .17 1.21 .08 IN X Culture 0.19 0.20 —.15 0.95 .06 2 Fchange (4, 162) = 2.00. p = .097. R change = .03 2 The overall model: F(9, 162) = 8.50.p < .001, adj. R = .28 *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001 sr: semipartial correlation Regarding multicollinearity, the first-order and second-order predictors had variance inflation factor (VIF) ranging from 1.48 to 8.80. Cohen, Cohen, West, and Aiken (2003) mentioned 10 as the traditional rule of thumb threshold value, although there is “no good statistical rationale for the choice of any of the traditional rule of thumb threshold values” (p. 424). B1: Behavioral intention ATT: Attitude toward behavior SN: Subjective norms DN: Descriptive norms IN: Injunctive norms AR: Attitudinal reasoning SNR: Subjective norms-related reasoning DNR: Descriptive norms-related reasoning INR: Injunctive norms-related reasoning dummy-coded with Americans = l and Koreans = 0 58 Table 13. Regression Results-for Intentions to Use Injunctive Norms-Reasoningfor Explaining Purchasing Green Product and Downloading Unauthorized Media Files Online. 3 SE t sr Green Product First Block Intercept 4.57 0.12 3821*” ATT 0.33 0.10 .27 3.32" .20 SN 0.05 0.08 .05 0.58 .04 DN 0.09 0.08 .10 1.24 .08 IN 0.43 0.10 .39 4.40*** .27 Culture —0.16 0.20 —.07 —0.84 —.05 F(5. 159) = 23.34.p < .001, adj.R2 = .41 Second Block ATT X Culture —0.09 0.21 —.05 —0.42 —.03 SN X Culture —0.29 0.20 — 22 —1.48 —.09 DN X Culture 0.14 .17 .10 0.83 .05 IN X Culture 0.09 0.22 .05 0.40 .02 Fchange (4, 155) = 0.85, p =.496. R2c112111ge = .01 The overall model: F(9. 155) = 13.30. p < .001, adj. R2 = .40 Downloading unauthorized files online First Block Intercept 3.91 0.10 3755*” ATT 0.28 0.08 .26 367*“ .19 SN 0.11 0.08 .1 1 1.43 .08 DN 0.02 0.06 .02 0.28 .02 IN 0.41 0.07 .40 563*“ .30 Culture 0.03 0.17 .01 0.15 .01 F(5. 200) = 32.39, p < .001, adj.R2 = .43 Second Block ATT X Culture ~0.37 0.15 — 23 —2.41* —.13 SN X Culture 0.02 0.16 .01 0.11 .01 DN X Culture 0.17 0.13 .09 1.26 .07 IN X Culture 0.24 0.15 .17 1.59 .08 Fchange (4, 196) = 2.39, p = .053. Rfichange = .03 2 The overall model: F(9, 196) = 19.55. p < .001, adj. R = .45 *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001 sr: semipartial correlation Regarding multicollinearity. the first-order and second-order predictors had variance inflation factor (VIF) ranging from 1.48 to 8.80. Cohen, Cohen, West. and Aiken (2003) mentioned 10 as the traditional rule ofthumb threshold value, although there is “no good statistical rationale for the choice of any of the traditional rule of thumb threshold values” (p. 424). BI: Behavioral intention ATT: Attitude toward behavior SN: Subjective norms DN: Descriptive norms IN: Injunctive norms AR: Attitudinal reasoning SNR: Subjective norms-related reasoning DNR: Descriptive norms-related reasoning INR: Injunctive norms-related reasoning dummy-coded with Americans = 1 and Koreans = 0 59 Table 14. Regression Results/or Intentions to Use Injumrtive Norms-Reasoningfor Explaining Truth- Telling and Lying. B SE B t sr Truth-telling First Block Intercept 4.24 0.1 1 4035*M .10 ATT 0.15 0.09 .13 1.70 .23 SN 0.27 0.07 .32 3.83*** .09 ON 0.11 0.07 .12 1.58 .20 IN 0.27 0.08 .28 3.33“ —.02 Culture' —0.05 0.16 —-03 —0.33 2 F(5, 169) = 18.62.p < .001. adj.R = .34 Second Block ATT X Culture 0.03 0.19 .02 0.16 .01 SN X Culture 0.08 0.15 .08 0.53 .03 DN X Culture ~0.03 0.16 —.03 —0.22 —.01 IN X Culture —0.03 0.17 —.03 —0.20 —.01 2 Fchange (4. 165) = 1.50, p = .21, R change = .02 The overall model: F(9. 165) = 11.13. p < .001, adj. R2 = .34 Lying First Block Intercept 3.96 0.12 3393*” ATT 0.04 0.10 .03 0.40 .02 SN 0.23 0.09 .23 2.70** .17 DN —0.05 0.10 -.04 —0.48 —.03 IN — .32 0.09 .44 5.00*** .31 Culturel —0.07 0.19 —.12 —1.71 —.10 F(5. 166) 2' I3.38.p < .001, adj.R2 = .27 Second Block ATT X Culture ~0.37 0.20 —.19 —1.80 —.11 SN X Culture 0.06 0.17 .04 0.35 .02 DN X Culture 0.31 0.21 .19 1.49 .09 IN X Culture 0.07 0.20 .05 0.35 .02 2 Fchange (4. 162) = 2.00, p = .10. R change = .03 7 4.1 The overall model: F(9, 162) = 8.50.p < .001, adj. R = .28 *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001 sr: semipartial correlation Regarding multicollinearity, the first-order and second-order predictors had variance inflation factor (VIF) ranging from 1.48 to 8.80. Cohen, Cohen. West, and Aiken (2003) mentioned 10 as the traditional rule ofthumb threshold value, although there is “no good statistical rationale for the choice of any ofthe traditional rule ofthumb threshold values” (p. 424). BI: Behavioral intention ATT: Attitude toward behavior SN: Subjective norms DN: Descriptive norms IN: Injunctive norms AR: Attitudinal reasoning SNR: Subjective norms-related reasoning DNR: Descriptive norms-related reasoning INR: Injunctive norms-related reasoning dummy—coded with Americans = 1 and Koreans = 0 60 Table 15. Summary of Unstandardized Simple Slopes. Type of Criterion 5:33:31? Simple Slopes for Groups behav10rs Variables Reference Group Comparison Group Purchasing Bl ATT Koreans Americans Green Products b = 0.41, p = .01 b = I.06,p < .001 AR SN Koreans Americans b=0.12,p=.24 =—0.I4,p=.02 SNR ATT Koreans Americans b = 0.32, p = .003 b = —0.03, p = .77 Downloading AR IN Koreans Americans Medial Files b=—0.Il,p=.18 b=0.11,p=.12 SNR ATT Koreans Americans b = 0.42.p <00] 11 = —0.04,p = .68 INR ATT Koreans Americans b = 045,17 <.001 b = 0.08,p = .50 Truth-Telling DNR IN Koreans Americans b = 0.32.p = .001 = —0.07, p =.58 Lying Bl ATT Koreans Americans b:0.10,p=.45 b=0.70,p<.001 Note. BI: Behavioral intention AR: Attitudinal reasoning SNR: Subjective norms-related reasoning DNR: Descriptive norms-related reasoning INR: Injunctive norms-related reasoning ATT: Attitude toward SN: Subjective norms behavior DN: Descriptive norms IN: Injunctive norms 61 Table 16. Means and Standard Deviations for Reasoning/ Behavioral T ype/ Culture ANO VA for Purchasing Green Products and Downloading Unauthorized Media Files Online. M SD Purchasing green Korea Intention to use attitude reasoning 4.86 0.98 products Intention to use subjective norms-related reasoning 4.20 1.07 Intention to use descriptive norms-related reasoning 4.26 1.18 Intention to use injunctive norms-related reasoning 4.36 1.10 US. Intention to use attitude reasoning 5.54 0.87 Intention to use subjective norms-related reasoning 3.12 1.23 Intention to use descriptive norms-related reasoning 4.20 1.40 Intention to use inj unctive norms-related reasoning 4.63 1.29 Total Intention to use attitude reasoning 4.09 1.1 I Intention to use subjective norms-related reasoning 3.68 1.27 Intention to use descriptive norms-related reasoning 4.23 1.29 Intention to use injunctive norms-related reasoning 4.63 1.29 Downloading Korea Intention to use attitude reasoning 3.88 1.26 unauthorized files Intention to use subjective norms-related reasoning 4.25 1.29 Intention to use descriptive norms-related reasoning 4.23 1.38 Intention to use injunctive norms-related reasoning 3.78 1.28 US. Intention to use attitude reasoning 4.25 1.29 Intention to use subjective norms-related reasoning 3.01 1.32 Intention to use descriptive norms-related reasoning 4.57 1.42 Intention to use inj unctive norms-related reasoning 4.08 1.37 Total Intention to use attitude reasoning 4.05 1.28 Intention to use subjective norms-related reasoning 3.31 1.31 Intention to use descriptive norms-related reasoning 4.58 1.42 Intention to use injunctive norms—related reasoning 3.78 1.28 Table 17. Means and Standard Deviationsfor Reasoning/ Behavioral type/ Culture ANO VA for Telling the Truth and Lyingfor a Friend. M SD Telling the truth Korea Intention to use attitude reasoning 3.88 0.97 Intention to use subjective norms-related reasoning 3.92 1.06 Intention to use descriptive norms-related reasoning 3.89 0.95 Intention to use injunctive norms-related reasoning 4.03 0.94 US. Intention to use attitude reasoning 3.88 1.05 Intention to use subjective norms-related reasoning 4.43 1.48 Intention to use descriptive norms-related reasoning 3.81 1.30 Intention to use injunctive norms-related reasoning 4.41 1.33 Total Intention to use attitude reasoning 3.87 1.01 Intention to use subjective norms-related reasoning 4.16 1.30 Intention to use descriptive norms-related reasoning 3.85 1.13 Intention to use injunctive norms-related reasoning 4.21 1.16 Lying for a friend Korea Intention to use attitude reasoning 3.83 0.84 Intention to use subjective norms-related reasoning 4.09 0.91 Intention to use descriptive norms-related reasoning 4.28 0.84 Intention to use injunctive norms-related reasoning 4.12 1.04 US. Intention to use attitude reasoning 3.04 0.95 Intention to use subjective norms-related reasoning 3.10 1 32 Intention to use descriptive norms-related reasoning 4.36 1.43 Intention to use injunctive norms-related reasoning 3.45 1.43 Total Intention to use attitude reasoning 3.45 0.98 Intention to use subjective norms-related reasoning 3.62 1.22 Intention to use descriptive norms-related reasoning 4.32 1.16 Intention to use injunctive norms-related reasoning 4.09 1.27 Table 18. 4 (Reasoning) X 4 (type ofbehaviors) X 2 (cultures) ANO VA Results F 112 df§ Sig. Reasoning 68.92 .03 2.89, 2056.55 p < .01 Behavioral type 12.10 .03 3. 71 1 p < .001 Culture 0.49 .00 I. 711 p = .484 Behavioral type x Culture 7.02 .02 3, 71 1 p < .001 Reasoning x Culture 27.38 .01 2.89, 2056.55 p < .001 Reasoning x Behavioral type 55.87 .07 2.89. 2056.55 p < .001 Reasoning x Behavioral type x 1 18.07 .02 2.89. 2056.55 p < .001 Culture Note. ‘ Greenhouse-Geisser Correction was used because the assumption of homogeneity was violated. 64 Table I9. Comparisons ofReasoning Typesfor Koreans and Americans for Purchasing Green Product. t-test p 4f: 84 Korea Attitudinal reasoning versus Subjective norms-related reasoning 7.59 < .001 Attitudinal reasoning versus Descriptive norms-related reasoning 5.99 < .001 Attitudinal reasoning versus Injunctive norms-related reasoning 5.25 <.001 Subjective norms-related reasoning versus Descriptive norms-related -0.88 .384 reasoning Subjective norms-related reasoning versus Injunctive norms-related -2.43 .017 reasoning Descriptive norms-related reasoning versus Injunctive norms-related -1.68 .096 reasoning df= 93 US. Attitudinal reasoning versus Subjective norms-related reasoning 14.43 < .001 Attitudinal reasoning versus Descriptive norms-related reasoning 8.28 < .001 Attitudinal reasoning versus Injunctive norms-related reasoning 7.27 < .001 Subjective norms-related reasoning versus Descriptive norms-related -6.71 < .001 reasoning Subjective norms-related reasoning versus Injunctive norms-related -8.40 < .001 reasoning Descriptive norms-related reasoning versus Injunctive norms-related -2.95 .004 reasoning 65 Table 20. Comparisons of Reasoning Types for Koreans and Americansfor T ruth— Telling. t-test p (If: 89 Korea Attitudinal reasoning versus Subjective norms-related reasoning -0.36 .718 Attitudinal reasoning versus Descriptive norms-related reasoning -0.16 .875 Attitudinal reasoning versus Injunctive norms-related reasoning -I .42 .161 Subjective norms-related reasoning versus Descriptive norms-related 0.28 .778 reasoning Subjective norms-related reasoning versus Injunctive norms-related -I .40 .166 reasoning Descriptive norms-related reasoning versus Injunctive norms-related -1.97 .052 reasoning df= 93 US. Attitudinal reasoning versus Subjective norms-related reasoning -3.75 < .001 Attitudinal reasoning versus Descriptive norms-related reasoning 0.41 .684 Attitudinal reasoning versus Injunctive norms-related reasoning -3.61 .001 Subjective norms-related reasoning versus Descriptive norms-related 3.31 001 reasoning Subjective norms-related reasoning versus Injunctive norms-related 0.14 .891 reasoning Descriptive norms-related reasoning versus Injunctive norms-related -4.22 <.001 reasoning 66 Table 21. Comparisons of Reasoning Ttpesfor Koreans and Americans/or Lyingfor a Friend. t-test p df= 84 Korea Attitudinal reasoning versus Subjective norms-related reasoning -2.60 .01 l Attitudinal reasoning versus Descriptive norms-related reasoning -4.22 <.001 Attitudinal reasoning versus Injunctive norms-related reasoning -2.96 .004 Subjective norms-related reasoning versus Descriptive norms-related -233 .022 reasoning Subjective norms-related reasoning versus Injunctive norms-related 4139 .697 reamnfing Descriptive norms-related reasoning versus Injunctive norms-related 1.71 .091 reasoning dj'= 82 US. Attitudinal reasoning versus Subjective norms-related reasoning -0.48 .684 Attitudinal reasoning versus Descriptive norms-related reasoning -7.77 <.001 Attitudinal reasoning versus Injunctive norms-related reasoning -2.55 .013 Subjective norms-related reasoning versus Descriptive norms-related -8.87 <.001 reasoning Subjective norms-related reasoning versus Injunctive norms-related -2.88 .005 reasoning Descriptive norms-related reasoning versus Injunctive norms-related 8137 UJN—- Subjective norm related reasons When I have to explain why I will lie for my friend in this situation. I intend to fly to other co-workers “Lying for my friend in this situation is ‘ I. Most people who are important to me think that I should lie for my friend in this situation. 2. Most people whose opinion I value consider that I should lie for my friend in this situation. 3. It is expected of me that I lie for my friend in this situation. 4. Most people who are significant to me consider that I should lie for my friend in this situation. Descriptive norm related reasons When I have to explain why I will lie for my friend in this situation, I intend to s_a_y to other co-workers “Lying for my friend in this situation is ” I. Many people have lied for their friend in this situation. 2. Many people had deceived others to help their friend in this situation. 3. Many people would be deceitful in this situation. 4. Lying for a friend in this situation is a common behavior that many people do. Injunctive norm related reasorg When I have to explain why I will lie for my friend in this situation. I intend to s_ay to other co-workers “Lying for my friend in this situation is ” I. Many people would approve of my lying for my friend in this situation. 2. Many people would endorse my lying for my friend in this situation. 3. Many people would support that my lying for my friend in this situation. 4. Lying for a friend in this situation is a socially approved behavior.* Note. * Item was deleted when we compute the construct in order to improve reliability. 8] Demographic informtifl I. Age 2. Gender Male __ Female __ 3. Your Ethnicity (check one): __ Caucasian __ Hispanic __ African American __ Pacific Islander _ Native American __ Mixed (please specify) _ Asian American _ Other (please specify) Note. * Item was deleted when we compute the construct in order to improve reliability. 82 Appendix4 Participants will respond using the following scale: I 2 3 4 5 6 7 Strongly Disagree Neutral Strongly Agree As you read the following story, please try to imagine as vividly as possible that the following story is happening to you You and your friend are currently working for a computer store on campus. Your friend is always IO to 20 minutes late. One day, a manager warned your friend to be on time. Otherwise, he would be fired. A day later, your friend did not show up to work. He sent you a text message, indicating that he is at his house and will be l5 minutes late. In addition, he asks you to cover for him. The manager comes up to you and asks where your friend is and whether he came to work on time. Behavioral intention on telling a truth in the situation I. I will try to tell the truth in this situation. 2. I have it in my mind to tell the truth in this situation. 3. I intend to lie for my friend in this situation. (Recorded) 4. I will lie for my friend in this situation. (Recorded)* 5. l have an intention to tell the truth in this situation.* 6. I plan to lie for my friend in this situation. (Recorded) Attitude toward tellipg a truth in the situation Telling the truth in this situation is . Bad- Good . Unpleasant-Pleasant . Inconsiderate-Considerate . Unfavorable-Favorable . Negative-Positive . Pointless- Worthwhile . UnenjoyabIe-Enjoyable . Unsatisfying Satisfying . Useless- Useful \OOOQONLII-DDJN— Subjective norms I. Most people who are important to me think that I should tell the truth in this situation. 2. Most people whose opinion I value consider that I should tell the truth in this situation. 3. It is expected of me that I tell the truth in this situation. 4. Most people who are significant to me consider that I should tell the truth in this situation. Descriptive porms 1. Many people have told the truth in this situation. 2. Many people had spoken the truth in this situation. 3. Many people would be completely truthful in this situation. 4. Telling the truth in this situation is a common behavior that many people do. Note. * Items were deleted when we compute the construct in order to improve reliability. 83 Injunctive norms 1. Many people would approve of my telling the truth in this situation. 2. Many people would endorse my telling the truth in this situation. 3. Many people would support that I tell the truth in this situation. 4. Telling the truth in this situation is a socially approved behavior.* Attitude-related [‘83ng When I have to explain why I will tell the truth in this situation, I intend to say to other co- workers “Telling the truth in this situation is " . Bad- Good . Unpleasant-Pleasant . Inconsiderate-Considerate . Unfavorable-Favorable . Negative-Positive . Pointless- Worthwhile . Unenjoyable—Enjoyable . Unsatisfying -Satisfying . Useless- Useful \OOONQUIQ-WN— Subjective norm related reasons When I have to explain why I will tell the truth in this situation. I intend to say to other co- workers “Telling the truth in this situation is ” I. Most people who are important to me think that I should tell the truth in this situation. 2. Most people whose opinion I value consider that I should tell the truth in this situation. 3. It is expected of me that I tell the truth in this situation. 4. Most people who are significant to me consider that I should tell the truth in this situation. Descriptive norm related reasons When I have to explain why I will tell the truth in this situation. I intend to say to other co- workers “Telling the truth in this situation is ” I. Many people have told the truth in this situation. 2. Many people had spoken the truth in this situation. 3. Many people would be completely truthful in this situation. 4. Telling the truth in this situation is a common behavior that many people do. Injunctive norm related reasons When I have to explain why I will tell the truth in this situation. I intend to say to other co- workers “Telling the truth in this situation is ” I I. Many people would approve of my telling the truth in this situation. 2. Many people would endorse my telling the truth in this situation. 3. Many people would support that I tell the truth in this situation. 4. Telling the truth in this situation is a socially approved behavior.* Note. * Items were deleted when we compute the construct in order to improve reliability. 84 Demographic information 1. Age 2. Gender Male Female _ 3. Your Ethnicity (check one): _ Caucasian _ Hispanic __ African American __ Pacific Islander __ Native American _ Mixed (please specify) _ Asian American __ Other (please specify) Note. * Items were deleted when we compute the construct in order to improve reliability. 85 ETIEA XIEE‘JOI EiElE DICIOI Higgi- ‘lU FD IO HU f9 [D g IUIO OH“ 3; Q HO HIJ IF] 1- III 2.4. [UK I: HI] 0 [[0 0'0 llO 3i? UIO E E l0 Olfl l_9 09.! fl 2 [ID \J 00.“ 02+. M H7 ll :23: -IO OHI l9 OII Em ma mm a m m m a f. llf I9 [D f— 1”; §5HAI II’SIEJOI EEEl-‘E UlClOl Egg ETD—92% CIEEEE’ J?!“ or 52 Q ”0' F 0 Mr E Hr EIEOII/H IIEIEJOI EEEl-E EZIOILI gil 33% 992$ ClE—EEE’ E L: §5HAI IIEi‘JOI EEEIE [3| 1: _2> Ilf llf f9 mmwm Efi remmmfluo m g a I: > awegwxw 9.4 L] r2 "’9” r9 m0 mlo \J E E] 2 H7 5Hkl IIEEJOI BEBE DICIOll—ngg $EL.§ Cl i mm a m m m m 9i m HQ LIED-EU 3| Ell E’s’Cl-Dl Elli % éfilfll-M 81 %2 El mEAEXIaoaaEAao ear—a 21 o—e 22+ o— gaaomeggamu flmatkmflfltl §D|ELIEIII ego—g aaam13cvaa "ELI 8i Cl CI LOCDVO'DUT-DQON‘ an rlH IE IE f9 :iJ DEE I. LIOIIDII §9§l CHE-35194 Algtgg LHDI PJElBi'ié- §6HM XIEEJOI EEEl—E DlClOl fll%% 992% ClE—EE ‘é’OlOl QUE gl‘éflll. 2. LHDI EEEIE 945% DIE [H3153 AIEIEE- LHDI PJEILJJE géH/H XI’EIEJOI EEEIE DICIOI Egg“ EP—EIEE CIEEE t£OIOI éllllfl éfil’éllll. 3. LHDI EBIQJQJOII/d EHEEI EQIOILI gil HIE—lgg ME 31% LOIIJ-ll ‘7'" DICHEIOIIIE ‘éOIC. l 4. LHDI EAIOU E Algggl CH'wZJ-‘E-E— LHDl EEIQ’C‘JOIIM LID—32$ DICIOI 1‘1ng; ClE—EE EECII’. gl‘élfll 86 DICIOIEICéI—E> EEIQE L.— L.— —EI E IEéJOIIAI XI’EIEIOI E a. E ELI. OILI gil XIIEEE $1le 0a 05 E E W H mm as 0:: E E 9 o: as AEA o: E :5 ma 2: I1" L an we E Q E E a a mwwM ICE 3. I U. DICIOI L— ; "El E HM IIE‘EIOI E 5 2% 20 A E til I P_ 4. H _o E am 0_ E, m: E as o: m.“ E mm 0:: DIEIOIHI‘EE EEIQE DICIOI THEE EEISBE L;. I.— h. — El EIE E E EHAI XIEEJOI E E EEHAI XI’SIEOI E A E til 0 E I ELIE EE XIII? EOICI. o: E E u: E o: DIEIOI THEE EEIQE C EEIE 'HA-I II ZSIEH OI E 5 EIU'E E J—E”§§OICI. I H DICIOI L— .L_—._ El E HM XI 751*“E’JOI E C o *OICI 2E 14;: _, I C l— O 'H lag 3?— CI Ill-AI BI 22 CI OIIZI ISIAEI-DIiI‘RACI 7: 23 II ‘Ba-ICI-DI ECI CI- 4 A a H UU XI DICIOI LHDI 9H PJEIEiI-E- E LI I . nlrl. C am 9 G we a 5.2 mfi Di ow m.“ MD .TM ow mm N 9 am I m_. _J 52 ow 2.3.4.5.6.7.8.9. O mom mag $92: EIEEE Sizer—.1 EIEE .L_— _ El CH 2E 87 I B I L— E 8 C. I,OI_C_>_ C1 are “r— “LIOIIJII ER 11 ESHAI XI’EIEJOI E DICIOI L. l_. ’EI CIE—AIPEIEOIIJII 2 g t— LI II’SI-ZJOI E $2 Z“I’ééOIIZI. I. 2. LIE LIE/K EEOIIJII 'LHDI EEEIE -IEE DIE CHE—E 9| AIEEE LHDI QJEIBi'E E 5H/d IIEIEJOI EEEIE DICIOI HIO 'E EEIEE CIEEE tI’:IOIOI ELIE EEIELI." EIII. EE J—‘I"é§.OlCI. 3. LIE CIEAIEEOIIJII “LHDI EEI‘BJQOII/H SHERRI EQIOILI EEI IIIXIEE AIE BdE LIOIIJII DICHEIOIXIE EOILI " ELI—I. 93E JSIESIOICI. 4. LIE CIE AIEIEOIIJII ‘LHDI EAIOCIDIE AIEEBI LHEEE LHDI EEIE’C‘DIOIIAI EEIEE DILIOI HIEE IEEE tEELID. XQIIQIELI BID. DEE JSIEOILI LHDI 9H @EIBIJE EoH/H XIXI IEJOI EEEIE DILIOI HIEE 5?— EIEE LIEEE EEXI EESHOI E [1H, I. LIE LIE/\IEEOIDII "ELIE AIaI DICIOIEIEE EEIEE LIE I 2. LIE CIEAIEEOIIJII "E’s! IIIX-IEE EDHELI. " EEI Egg X-IEOILI. 3. LIE EA AIEEOIIDII ‘IIXIEJ I flan: OIUI EEIEEE D—IOI LIEEEEIE $3,111." BID. E-aIEI XIEOI OZ ' E E. 2:4. fl . . Ii}. .9. III IOII E III 4. LIE LIE AIEIEOIIJII OIEIl'E ESHH XIXIE’JOI EEEIE DILIOI EIEE EEIEE LIE-EEO IE 33E E’.:IE AIEEOI ELAEE BIE EEOILI' BID EE X-IEOICI. EE’EIPJ HEQJ Ola?— LHDI 9H OEIEILJIE EEH/d XI’EIEJOI EEEIE EIILIOI HIEE EEIEE CIEEE t(IzIEXI EE6HOI E IDI. I. LIE UgEO/KIEEOIDII El—SIE AIEEE LHDI EBIQJE ESHAI SHEEI EQIOILI EEI XIV-‘1 EEDHOIE 31E EIEE EOICI.’ BID. EE EIEOICI 2. LIE DEE/HE EOIIJII "EL—IE AIEEE LHDI EBIQJE ESH/d IIXIEJOI EEEIE EIILIOIILIEE EEIEE CIEEE EE ERIE EOICI." BID. EE JfiI’c‘JOILI. 3. LIE CIEAIEIEOIDII E’sIE AIEEE LHDI QJEIIi'E ESHM XIX IaOI EE’JEIE DICIOI HIEE EEIEE LIES—IE EE IIIIE EOICI." all). EI'I JEI’ééOICI 4. LIE CIEAIEIEOIIHI 'EflElbi'E EGHAI XIXIEJOI EEEIE UICIOI HIEE EEIEE CIEEE ‘E—JE EE AIEIEEE EQJOI EIE EEOILI" all 9%! I JSIEOIIZI Ell—TIEJIIEE EE GI 93% (Pl) __ All G2 13%: LIA-I __ 04g 88 ‘111 11111 CD @5323 HIE EUH EE- EE’Xf—E—E— 7 E EE (1) = 0H9?- E2|51II EE (5) 0H5"? E9183 SHO“ I1d91 0 EE- ESE EEBI 9.1%} 1. L1:- 311‘01 HUME EEEMIE EEUH‘E EEOICL 2. ME 31201 HUME E1 ERIE-E- ’E 311E011}. 3. L15 313401 EO1E ZE'EEMIE EOEUHEBM—l E 310111. 4. 1.1-:- HIEOI EC1E1E EEEMIE EEUHE 1101111. 5. L15 312101 EO1E "'EEMIEE E *o”?—.*O| 91121. SE 313101 HINE EEEXHEE EUH81E 33E”- 1. USES-E111 2. DIEII EEG-3199C} 3. EEE1C1-Al 1%2'111 4. EEC-£21K! $C1-E1Efi‘EC1 5. E35 8* afl-EEEU 6. EE’EOIU-EEEOIU 7 9.10181121-DRIS’AC1 8. EDIEBRI 9§C1-EE‘951C1 9. EQJEMI 3111-7291510 35—43519 RE 1 1.10113“ E52121 CHE—EB! NEE-E LH31 319101 H|M1C1€1EE§JE§MIEE EUHB‘HOt 5.31112 81345313 2 LH31 EEC—3'15 915E 31E CHE—E94 NEE—E— LHD1 HUME EEE MIEE 101 EU]. AOHD—fEU. 3 HUME EEEMIEE LHD1A1E 31E— L101|31| DICHEIWIIE O'OIC1. 4. LH31 EMOUIE MEEPJ [HES—E LH31 HUME EEEMIEE— A101 @5131 E21530 3133319 RE 1. B—JE MEEE HUME EEEMIEE EUHECL 2. El—JE NEE-E HIEOI EC1E1E E'EEMIEE £111. 3. NEOI HUME EEEMIEEE 9:!01 EEG. 4. 31210| EO1E EEEMIEE 1E EE Psi-E— NEE-OI ELEEE 51E EEOIU 89 09 012 J?! 12 1L1 DEE 1. 9.1: 11%;:- 11131 31201 :01: 'l‘1EX1IEE ME a; 1:12:21 210m. 2. 131: 11:15::- LHD1H|M1E$JEEMIEE ME 21—;— Egg £019, 3 131: MEE—E 1.1131 111111: 1.112111%; 21111151: 31:3- x1x161 $0151 4. 912101 :01; SEEMIEE ME 31E 11122-1222; $9521:- EEOIU EHEE‘E 01% HIEOI EOHE E31013). 911 LH31 E'EEMIEE’E ‘ E91310“ EH L1E UEMEEOHJH "ME MIEOI HIM1C1€1E EEEMIEE ” 813’. E E 51253101111. 1. MEG-E111 2. DIEII (EU-DIES 3. EE51E1-AIE1EC1 4. EELEEXI EU-EEEEU 5. EE 9.1 EU-EEEU 6. EEEOlU-EEEOIU 7 QUIEU-DH’EIQAU 8. EDIEERI EC1-EEE181E1 9. EESRI 93111-994811] 3153175181 771121.91 01% HIEOl EOHE EEE1D 52H LHD1’SEEMIEE E 31.931011 HHS:1 EEE 6HO1E [[H. L15- GEMEEOHNI 1.1011311 E1953 E94 M‘EEE LH31 31EO| HUMHE1E 311% 1EX1|EE EUHOHO151C1D E *- 1.11. 191 El 2 L1: HEMP-£01131 11131 E 3151 mag-:91 MEEE 1.1131 1111115 agamgg A101 9:11:13). c121é1c1. a1 :1'1512210151. 3. L1:- C1cA1EE‘011311‘EHUM 831EX1IEE 1191;11:212L1o1131191t1151o111: @0151." 31:1. gig 2112210111. 4 L-:—1 :A wagon)“ 11191 E110191: 11:1:21 112:: 11131 111111; 32:1m11g1o1 1:11:12 111218.151." 2.12 :1; 311510151. Ea 1211.391 0111 111§01 gong E?811 5211 11131 "EEMIEE 2111131011 [112:1 1:19;: 51101211111, 1. L1E UEMEEOHJH "EEE MEEE HUME JSEEMIEE EUHEU." €1.71 DEE 5122101121. 2. L1E UEMEEOHJH E’s’E MEEE HlEOl EC1E1E "EEMIEE £1111" E11 EE EEOIU. 3. L15- UEMEEOHJH 31EO| HUME jSEEMIEEE 95101 EEG" 31?. EE 511E011}. 9O 4 L1: L1EA1EE011311 912101 EO1E EEEMIEE 11: 31E 91E A1EE01 ELEQE 51-:— EEOIH 31:1 EE EEOIG. EEEE 2112121 0111 111E101 EOHE E21512 211 11131 '1E1E111EE E E91310“ 012:1 EEE 01101E1011 1. L1E EA1E’E011311 EE— A1E‘EE LHD1 31EOI E0112 EEEMIEE ME EE- EEE E01121. 312 EE E 1 2. L1E GEME‘EOIIHI EE 1E1EE LH91 HUME EEEMIEE ME EE EEE E01111." 31—1 E_E 3517301 3. ME GENE E0113“ EE NEE—E LHD1 HUME EEEMIEE :rIDH81E EE X1IIE E011}. " B12 EE Zc-Pc‘s‘OlD. 4. 1.1-E C1E—A1E‘E01131131E01 EO1E E1 1EM|EE ME EE MEIEQE EEEIE EEOIC1EIQ1 EE EEOID. EEEMEE EE— G1 SE (9.1) _ MI 02 EE 1:121; 011/11 91 am am a Ao Ir Ac ._0 H .A Mo a8 :0 0“?“ $0“) é Co l. 91 OIODIDF [3% "410“?“ ’éi/KIO“ UH Ll X1 3} Ed 01! 3“ 2M4 II EELIC}. } L'J ME. “+81 E‘fiié—LIC}. o ”ELIE. '6 a E : EX} .EMOH 5%! DE 850“?“ :13} 318%}. g2 % LIE}. 8H g3} EQUEO 9| Oil I- __0D 0 , E0 2. U 05 mg am fl :5 E (1) = GHQ 73-! _, 77% o: :2: 0:: H E am Q 05 a: HE E B FU-AIBHI'C} 3 0| Cl'_g§ EOI C} ”EL—J- A: $0 L'LlLl-Irlllllr g [JON 31! L} 2. DlflXl (Baffl-DIEC} 3. 1. 1. .2 ma *diléj-Cé EOIDI $I6H dilE Jill/bi)“. 532'“; M Note: 92 E a: 81E fig L101|31|3|CHE|O1XIE $0111. 4. LH71 EAIOUI |r E § % a 2 IE H. W go Mo 0: 2: m o: o: o: Ea Ea Ea ma M 0: ma 1 151. L— 5 Oil/cl 51a";- OIOFDI 128 ”SEMI/H 91E JCHEQJ Mag Da‘SJCL O H __o E am 2 E and Ea as 0: ma 0: a $12 801 a a 1 A aa OE ao Ea Ea NA )1 E Ho. A12 01- aaa 2 A19. a OI é‘JEWHM LH31 XIXISa EDIE}. a X I— O1| kl 1r * Ir .L m as ._Hoo E 9 ma go a: o_ 5 ._|oE Ir IA 0: mi E am ma 0a a a :12: E LH31 52H 01 OIU. 0: mi 5 fl 0a a g 5 M LI :5 Ac Eat-[Z1 2. DIEII EEG-DIED o1 EDIE-3533401111 oHJ-Al 81 21' C1 8111101119151 _C1- 3 no N N :2 E, 3 Ea on 4.5.6.7.8.9. UU u aa 3. 1 . a... W % g .2 ma 2 LHD1 9H OI 101W LHD1 0| * ELLE (DE 1 A El OICL rm; 8 L... B 93 x 5 “ L10“ 31! 1C1." '5 I— 011311 Dfil- I: EDI]! $|5H dilE 3115M. C13. ’34 I. l— 5 $2 "HOII 5 L1E GE— Eiggfi O alga DaF *dag Note: 1. 2. LIE GE’— E’é’EEEOHJH “LHD1 EEEE 915-:- DHSJ $519.1 Mggg LH31 OI Qé’Oil/x-l agg— Da’EHOII 811112 @42153111. 313. gig ZSPéiOIl—J. 3. UE DE al’é‘gEE-OHHI "LH31 782% Da’EIE 338— 1,1011)“ DICHElO-1XIE $0151 ' 312 g; J3’30“} 4 LIE GE EEEEEOHDH "LH31 ENG-DIE Mes-E91 CHE—E? LH31 OI QEWHH Egg géHOF 5102 2'4345JE1 813 9:32 75153011}. DIEEE Hggl 0'791‘ LHDI 9H OI QEJOHH a/g‘a E?=_*3*I| £93101? IIH. 1. u-ts G—E— aggaaom “Ea-:8 WEE-8 0| ggmw Egg Gaga." am 01-. 351330151. I 2. ME 51% EQ§§EWHI “egg MLE‘E—E—Z— mag gamma 29g; Olomém." am 323 wow. 3. 1.1-:— DE aggfigmlm “egg mega OPE‘E gamma ea:- 20129 Avg; gar-3:1" am 9:32 £53010. 4. us a; fl’géfigéflfll “0121 28 gamma was; gas as- Egg M‘E’EOI 2551519.; 51: @gom." an 21% 22030151. ggag 3:391 01% LHD1SBH OI QEJOHM agg— Dagll ggwm ‘2 EH. 1. LIE [11% aggagmml “£8 M‘E‘EE OPE-LC:D QEWHM LHDI E’a‘g 05-51: 31% (3’32 fiOICI. 312 ”a”? J—5.”