. ; . . V . .. . V . . . . v V .. . . V . . . V p .‘ . V . . . . r . m . . . . . . V . V v . . . . . V V . . . . . ‘V . V. V . V . . . . . . . V V , . . . . V . .V. V. . . V . . . ‘. V a . . . .. V. . . .. . V . . . . . . . .. V .x . ‘ A . . . . . ‘ . . . V ,V‘ . V . . ,4 V V . . V . . . . . . . . . V . v .. . . , . . . . V . r . . .. . . . . . .. . .. V .. . «V .. V V . V . . . V . V ‘ . . V . 1. . . . . ’.vu." -.- nu- ...-..-.. m; _ h. . . .i mmahfih . V . . V V. . V V (tat. This is to certify that the dissertation entitled NEURODYNAMICS OF EPISODIC MEMORY CONSOLIDATION presented by JEREMY LEE SMITH has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for the PHD. degree in NEUROSCIENCE Major Professor’s Signature / MM DPQ Date MSU is an affinnalive-action, equal-opportunity employer o.-.-v-.-.-.-.-.-.-.-.-.--.-.—.-u-.-.-.—-.--.-.-.—.—.—.-.-—-—-n-.-.—.—.-.--.---._.-—._._ _.--.-.-.-.--.-.—._._ PLACE IN RETURN BOX to remove this checkout from your record. TO AVOID FINES return on or before date due. MAY BE RECALLED with earlier due date if requested. DATE DUE DATE DUE DATE DUE NEURODYNAMICS OF EPISODIC MEMORY CONSOLIDATION By Jeremy Lee Smith A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Neuroscience 2008 ABSTRACT NEURODYNAMICS OF EPISODIC MEMORY CONSOLIDATION By Jeremy Lee Smith The present study endeavored to implement conventional FMRI analyses and two novel approaches to BOLD signal characterization with consolidation — Shannon entropy and voxel-response cross-correlation with task (VTCC) — to assess the brain's ability to optimize memory network assemblies with consolidation. In this dissertation the current state of the episodic consolidation literature is reviewed. These concepts are then related to the anatomy and fimctional architecture of episodic memory systems. Past and present FMRI and electrophysiological methodologies are also discussed and related to the study of physiological changes in the memory system with consolidation. A paired-associate task was used to examine changes in fimctional modularity and connectivity with episodic memory consolidation. This task required subjects to learn to associate visually presented words with abstract pictures, then identify correctly-matched, incorrectly-matched, and control pairings during a testing session. A hypothesized improvement subject recall performance with consolidation was not substantiated, but instead remained level between pre- and post-consolidation subjects, suggesting that access to the memory traces remained approximately constant over the consolidation interval despite network changes. Improvements in performance were correlated with decreased recruitment of premotor cortex and visual areas V2, V3, and V4, but not lllfl I.\ III. {ll III I. Ill .1! medial temporal, primary motor, or primary visual cortices. Changes in activation volume, BOLD signal magnitude, the mean correlation between activated voxels and the paired-associate task, and BOLD signal Shannon entropy, were also assessed. The present findings indicate that neurons still participatory in trace retrieval were individually more active than their pre-consolidation counterparts, that BOLD signal entropy increased with consolidation in perirhinal cortex and in components of the ventral visual stream, and, moreover, that BOLD signal response rates increased significantly with consolidation in all areas except left hemisphere primary motor cortex. These findings are discussed in the context of basic scientific research in memory network dynamics, as well as the methodological contributions to neuroscience. Copyright by JEREMY LEE SMITH 2007 ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS The author wishes to thank Mrs. Katey Smith, Mr. William Smith, Ms. Barbara Parker Smith, Mr. and Mrs. Donald and Marcia Brown, Mr. Kenneth M. Cobb, and Dr. Craig Branch for their ongoing support during the writing of this dissertation; Drs. Alessandra M. Passarotti, Laura Symonds, Sue Barman, Christine Larson, David Zhu, Jerry Gebber, and Cheryl Sisk for indispensable advice and guidance; Dr. David Zhu, Mr. Jeremy Grounds, and Mr. Dave McFarlane for unremitting patience and assistance with daily technical issues; Dr. Matt Hoptman, for critical feedback on this manuscript; and the students and faculty of the Neuroscience Program, without whose encouragement and helpful comments this work would likely have suffered exceedingly. TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES ..................................................................................................... VIII LIST OF FIGURES ........................................................................................................ X LIST OF FIGURES ........................................................................................................ X EPISODIC MEMORY AND MEMORY CONSOLIDATION ........................................ 1 Connectivity and Functional Architecture in Episodic Retrieval ................................... 4 Declarative memory: the semantic and episodic systems .......................................... 5 Role of other memory systems in episodic retrieval ................................................ 10 REGIONS OF INTEREST ............................................................................................ 13 Premotor cortex as a visuospatial attention area ......................................................... 17 Visual areas ............................................................................................................... 18 Primary motor cortex: a control area .......................................................................... 20 FMRl SPATIAL AND TEMPORAL SPECIFICITY ..................................................... 21 BOLD signal physiology ........................................................................................... 21 Spatiotemporal resolution of F MRI ............................................................................ 23 Modeling the hemodynamic response functions ......................................................... 26 AIMS AND THEORETICAL TREATMENT OF CONSOLIDATION NEURODYNAMICS .................................................................................................... 27 Consolidation as an optimizing process ...................................................................... 28 Evidence for interim physiological changes in retrieval substrates ............................. 3O Computational efficiency and cortical demands: activation volume and entropy ........ 32 Cortical reinstatement and retrieval orientation effects in visual and medial-temporal areas .......................................................................................................................... 35 HYPOTHESES ............................................................................................................. 38 METHODOLOGY ........................................................................................................ 43 Subject pool and participants ..................................................................................... 43 Paradigm ................................................................................................................... 44 Data acquisition ......................................................................................................... 47 Behavioral analysis .................................................................................................... 48 Modular analysis ....................................................................................................... 49 vi Deconvolution ....................................................................................................... 49 Percent BOLD signal change from baseline ........................................................... 52 Activation volume .................................................................................................. 53 Voxel-task cross-correlation ................................................................................... 54 Shannon entropy of the voer time series ............................................................... 55 MEMORY CONSOLIDATION AND RETRIEVAL PERFORMANCE: EFFECTS OF STIMULUS MODALITY AND PAIR CONCORDANCE ............................................ 59 Behavioral results ...................................................................................................... 61 Reaction time ......................................................................................................... 62 Accuracy ................................................................................................................ 63 Changes in BOLD Physiology with Consolidation ..................................................... 71 R01 analysis Of metabolic demand ......................................................................... 71 R01 analysis of cortical recruitment ....................................................................... 73 R01 analysis of BOLD response rate ...................................................................... 75 Retrieval orientation and cortical reinstatement: Interpretation of Physiological Findings in the Context of Signal Entropy .................................................................. 78 NEURODYNAMICS OF EPISODIC MEMORY CONSOLIDATION: CONCLUSIONS AND DISCUSSION ...................................................................................................... 85 Effects of stimulus modality and testing interval on task performance ........................ 87 Disparity between behavioral predictions and findings ........................................... 87 Theoretical reconsolidative basis for the non-Kamin Ebbinghaus exception ........... 89 Interpretation of Metabolic Demand, Recruitment, and Signal Response Rate ............ 93 Disparity between physiological predictions and findings ...................................... 93 Caveats of the entropy metric ............................................................................... 100 Theory: Cortical reinstatement and Retrieval Orientation ......................................... 101 Possible cortical reinstatement effects .................................................................. 103 Closing remarks ....................................................................................................... 105 APPENDIX A: ANOVA TABLES FOR BEHAVIORAL DATA ................................ 107 APPENDIX B: MAIN-EFFECT ANOVA AND POST-HOC RESULTS FOR PHYSIOLOGICAL STATISTICS ............................................................................... 109 APPENDIX C: DECONVOLUTION COMMAND LINE ........................................... 134 vii LIST OF TABLES Table 1 Projections to the perihippocampal regions (Lavenex and Amaral 2000). Abbreviations: PPC, posterior parietal [association] cortex; SSC, somatosensory cortex; OFC, orbitofrontal cortex; PFC, prefrontal cortex; ST S, superior temporal sulcus; TE, V4, visual areas TE and V4. ................................................................... 8 Table 2 Regions typically involved in episodic retrieval tasks. LH, left hemisphere; RH, right hemisphere; BL, bilateral. Several of these regions will be used as target ROIs in the present study (see next chapter). ................................................................... 12 Table 3 Summary of the hypotheses for the present study. ............................................ 41 Table 4 Summary of the predictions for the present study with respect to subject performance (reaction time and accuracy), as well as physiological descriptions of the shape and temporal characteristics of the BOLD signal on the delayed match—to- sample portions of the paired associates task. Both behavioral and physiological findings are represented as being greater (+), less (—), or equal (no change, N/C) in post-consolidation subjects relative to pre-consolidation subjects. Abbreviations: HC, hippocampus, ERC, entorhinal cortex, EcRC, ectorhinal cortex, PRC, perirhinal cortex, V1, V2, V3, visual areas 1-3, F US], fusiform gyrus, M1, primary motor cortex, PMC, premotor cortex; RT, subject reaction time, Acc, accuracy; BOLD mag, percent BOLD signal change from baseline, recruitment, cortical recruitment (activation volume), BOLD rate, BOLD signal response rate (evaluated by the voxel-task cross-correlation, VTCC). Hypothesis 4 (H4), which predicts a predominance of right hemisphere activation in post-consolidation subjects, is not included in the table. .............................................................................................. 41 Table 5 A qualitative review of the results presented in the present chapter and the conclusions drawn fiom them. “Per-neuron activity” was determined by dividing the metabolic demand PC) of an ROI by the extent of its recruitment (AV) to obtain a rudimentary AP/mm estimate. Abbreviations: N/C, no change; fusi., fusiform gyrus; Vx, visual area x. ......................................................................................... 84 Table 6 Summary of the predictions (“Predicted”) and findings (“Actual”) for the present study with respect to subject performance (reaction time and accuracy), as well as physiological descriptions of the shape and temporal characteristics of the BOLD signal on the delayed match-tO-sample portions of the paired associates task. Both behavioral and physiological findings are represented as being greater (+), less (—), or equal (no change, N/C) in post-consolidation subjects relative to pre- consolidation subjects. Abbreviations: HC, hippocampus, ERC, entorhinal cortex, EcRC, ectorhinal cortex, PRC, perirhinal cortex, V l , V2, V3, visual areas 1-3, F USI, fusiform gyrus, MI, primary motor cortex, PMC, premotor cortex; RT, subject viii reaction time, Acc, accuracy; BOLD mag, percent BOLD signal change from baseline, recruitment, cortical recruitment (activation volume), BOLD rate, BOLD signal response rate (voxel—task cross-correlation) .................................................. 86 LIST OF FIGURES Figure 1 Connections of the rhinal and perihippocampal cortices. PHC, perihippocampal cortex; PRC, perirhinal cortex; EcRC, ectorhinal cortex; ERC, entorhinal cortex; HC, hippocampus. Adapted from (Lavenex and Amara12000) and (Save, Nerad and Poucet 2000). ........................................................................................................... 7 Figure 2 (A) Interconnections among constituent regions of the MTL and other areas of interest in the present study. Blue arrows indicate neuronal projections between areas as determined by anatomical studies of the cortex and temporal lobe. Arrow locations do not accurately reflect the anatomical location of interregional projections (Sources: Deacon, Eichenbaum, Rosenberg and Eckmann 1983; Schacter, Buckner, Koutstaal, Dale and Rosen; Burwell and Amaral; Burwell and Amaral; Lavenex and Amaral; Save and Poucet 2000a; Save and Poucet 2000b). (B) The anatomical locations of MTL areas in the Talairach-Tourneaux template brain, obtained from the AFNI suite’s Talairach Daemon. Abbreviations: LH, left hemisphere; RH, right hemisphere; ctx., cortex; BA, Brodmann area. ..................... 16 Figure 3 The training/testing paradigm for the present study. Stimuli consisted of 20 non- rehearsable, non-generalizable abstract pictures paired with animal nouns. Word stimuli were presented either audibly (computer-based training/testing) or visually (FMRI training/testing). Following a training session, during which the correct pairings of stimuli were learned, subjects were tested on their recall either immediately (pre-consolidation time point; 50% of subjects) or 7 days after training (post-consolidation time point; 50% of subjects). For the purposes of testing, subjects were presented with 33 correctly-matched, 33 incorrectly-matched, and 34 control (scrambled) stimulus pairs, and subjects were required to identify each type correctly by button press. ....................................................................................... 44 Figure 4 (A) Example of a paired associate as presented for the FMRI version of the training/testing paradigm: note that the word component of FMRI training and testing associates was presented below the abstract visual component. The centroid of the presented paired associate was positioned in the center of the subjects' field of View. (B) A schematic of a single run fi'om the FMRI testing paradigm: match, mismatch, and control-condition stimuli were presented with a constant interstimulus interval of 12.5 seconds. Presentation order was randomized across the three types of stimuli; x-axis represents TR (1 TR = 2.5 seconds). .................... 46 Figure 5 Mean reaction times for the paired-associate task by testing group (computer- based, CBT, versus FMRI subjects), pair concordance (matched, mismatched, or control), and time point (pre- versus post-consolidation). FMRI subjects, who were given unimodal (visual) training and testing stimulus pairs, were slower at identifying both correctly-matched and mismatched stimulus pairs than CBT subjects, who were given dual-modal (visual and auditory) training and testing stimulus pairs. This effect was seen at both pre- and post-consolidation time points. However, neither FMRI nor CBT subjects were significantly faster at identifying correctly-matched stimulus pairs after one week. Neither group improved with respect to reaction time with consolidation for control stimuli, nor did CBT and F MRI subjects differ significantly with respect to reaction time for control stimuli at either time point. Error bars represent standard errors of the means. *p < .05. ........ 62 Figure 6 Mean subject accuracy for the paired-associate task by testing group (computer- based, CBT, versus FMRI subjects), pair concordance (matched, mismatched, or control), and time point (pre- versus post-consolidation). F MRI subjects, who were given unimodal (visual) training and testing stimulus pairs, were more likely to identify mismatched associates as matched associates (exhibited more false positives) than CBT subjects, who were given dual-modal (visual and auditory) training and testing stimulus pairs. However, neither FMRI nor CBT subjects improved with respect to the number of false positives over the putative memory consolidation period, nor did either group improve with respect to accuracy with consolidation for matched associates (false negatives) or control stimuli. CBT and FMRI subjects did not differ significantly with respect to accuracy for correctly- matched associates or control stimuli at either time point. Symbols (light grey) indicate individual data points; error bars represent standard errors of the means. *p < .05; **p < 0.01 .................................................................................................... 65 Figure 7 Group statistical parametric maps (SPMs) of voxel BOLD signal correlation with presentation of match, mismatch, and control-condition stimuli by region of interest. Voxel BOLD signal changes shown are significant at a full-model F = 3.786 (p = 0.001, corrected); colors represent whether changes are positive (yellow) or negative (blue) relative to the baseline. Group SPMs were obtained by studentizing individual BOLD datasets and registering them to a common Talairach- Tourneaux coordinate system (Talairach and Tourneaux, 1988). Datasets were then subjected to cluster analysis using a minimal 5mm, 10u1 Gaussian kernel (to correct for type 11 error: see Salrnond, Ashburner, et al. 2002; Geissler, Lanzenberger, et a1. 2005) and averaged by condition. The location of the calcarine fissure (visual area V1) is shown on an axial slice in each case for reference (yellow boxes). Hemodynamic response functions for representative subjects were also obtained for ectorhinal area 36 (red box) and visual area V3/19 (green box) and are shown in Figure 9. Abbreviations: Fus, fusiform gyrus; Ins, AIC, anterior insular complex; ST G, superior temporal gyrus; IPL, inferior parietal lobule; SPL, superior parietal lobule; DG, dentate gyrus; pul, pulvinar nucleus of the thalamus, Ver, cerebellar vermis, Cal, calcarine fissure. Numbers represent Brodmann areas: 3a, digit somatosensory area; 4, digit motor area; 31, posterior cingulate cortex ................... 67 Figure 8 Estimated event-related average hemodynamic response functions (HRFs) for pre- (dotted line) and post- (solid line) consolidation right hemisphere ectorhinal cortex (EcRC, Brodmann area 36) and visual area V3 (Brodmann area 19) seed locations indicated in Figure 7. HRFs were obtained from the same studentized respresentative subject data as in Figure 7. Talairach-Toumeaux coordinates for each region are also given (see Talairach and Toumeaux, 1988) ............................. 69 xi Figure 9 Percent BOLD signal change fi'om baseline (BOLD signal magnitude) in significantly task-correlated voxels constituting regions of interest at pre- and post- consolidation for matched associates. Analysis by main effect ANOVA indicated a significant difference in BOLD magnitude between pre- (T1) and post- (T2) consolidation subjects (p < 0.001) overall, however, post-hoe analysis by Tamhane T2 failed to indicate any differences in per-region signal change with consolidation. These findings suggest that the BOLD response magnitude is not affected by consolidation in significantly task-correlated voxels in these regions. Error bars indicate standard errors. ......................................................................................... 72 Figure 10 Activation volume of significantly task-correlated voxels at pre— and post- consolidation for matched associates. Analysis by main effect ANOVA indicated a significant difference in activation volume between pre- and post-consolidation subjects overall (p < 0.001). Post-hoe analysis by Tamhane T2 further indicated significant reductions in cortical recruitment in the fusiform gyrus, premotor cortex, and visual areas V2 and V3. These findings suggest that the number of significantly task-correlated voxels is affected by consolidation in premotor areas, area V2, and ventral visual areas, but not in medial temporal, primary motor, or primary visual areas. Error bars indicate standard errors; *indicates a significant difference in activation volume, p < 0.05. ROI significances computed by post-hoc Tamhane T2. .............................................................................................................................. 74 Figure 11 Mean voxel BOLD response-stimulus presentation covariance (voxel-task cross-correlation, VTCC) in significantly task-correlated voxels constituting regions of interest at pre- (T1) and post- (T2) consolidation for matched associates. Analysis by main effect ANOVA indicated a significant difference in VTCC between pre- and post-consolidation subjects overall (p < 0.001), and post-hoe analysis by Tukey HSD revealed significant differences in VTCC in all areas (p < 0.05). These findings suggest that the BOLD-task covariance is affected by consolidation in significantly task-correlated voxels in medial temporal, motor, premotor, and visual regions. Error bars indicate standard errors; *indicates a significant difference in VTCC, p < 0.05. ROI significances computed by post-hoc Tukey HSD unequal-N test. ........................................................................................................................ 76 Figure 12 Mean BOLD signal Shannon entropy (TSE) in significantly task-correlated voxels constituting regions of interest at pre- and post-consolidation for matched associates. Analysis by main effect ANOVA indicated a significant difference in BOLD TSE between pre- and post-consolidation subjects overall (p < 0.001). Post- hoc analysis by Tamhane T2 firrther indicated significantly greater signal entropy in the firsiform gyrus and visual areas V2 and V3 (p < 0.01). These findings suggest that the BOLD signal entropy is affected by consolidation in significantly task- correlated voxels in extrastriate ventral visual areas, but not in medial temporal, premotor, or primary motor or visual cortex. Error bars indicate standard errors; *indicates a significant difference in TSE, p < 0.01. ROI significances computed by Tamhane T2. .......................................................................................................... 81 xii KEY TO SYMBOLS AND ABBREVIATIONS Acc Accuracy ANOVA Analysis of variance AV Activation volume BA Brodmann area BOLD Blood oxygenation level dependent CBF Cerebral blood flow CBT Computer-based testing CMRO; Cerebral metabolic rate of oxygen DMTS Delayed match to sample DNMS Delayed nonmatch to sample dVS Dorsal visual stream EcRC Ectorhinal cortex EEG Electroencephalography EPI Echo-planar imaging ERC Entorhinal cortex EWS Extrastriate ventral visual stream FMRI Functional magnetic resonance imaging HbIdI-lb Hemoglobin/deoxyhemoglobin HC HRF IRF LFP LH LTD LTP M1 MTNS MTL Hippocampal formation Hemodynamic response function Impulse response function Local field potential Left hemisphere Long-term depression Long-term potentiation Primary motor cortex Visual area V5/MT Medial temporal lobe PC I AP Percent BOLD signal change over PET PHC PHG PMC PRC PRS R baseline Positron emission tomography Parahippocampal gyrus Perihippocampal cortex Premotor cortex Perirhinal cortex Perceptual representation subsystem Pearson product-moment correlation RH ROI RT Sh SNR SPM TSE V1 VTCC vVS xiii Right hemisphere Region of interest Response time Shannon bits Signal-to-noise ratio Statistical parametric map Tesla Shannon entropy of the time series Visual area V1 (etc.) Voxel-to-task cross-correlation (indicative of BOLD signal rate) Ventral visual stream Denoised BOLD signal response vector Raw BOLD signal (EPI) response vector EPISODIC MEMORY AND MEMORY CONSOLIDATION Consolidation refers to the process by which short-term memories are gradually indexed, transferred to cortex, and made permanent (Abel and Lattal 2001; Eichenbaum 2001; Haist, Bowden Gore and Mao 2001). Episodic memories are quite labile until stabilized through this process, which results in stronger connections among the elements (nodes) that are relevant to the memory and weaker connections among elements that are irrelevant to the memory. Neuroimaging and neuropsychological studies have provided reasonable evidence that the consolidation mechanism proceeds in two stages. In the first stage, short-term memory traces are encoded in the hippocampal CA3 region and linked by long-term potentiation to form stable networks. In the second stage, the medial temporal lobe (MTL) and association cortex backprojections, which are Hebb-modifiable by entorhinal 0 rhythm entrainment (firing rate coherence: Treves and Rolls 1994; Chrobak and Buzsaki 1998b) are reactivated and reinforced. It is believed that the backprojections themselves drive the “repositioning” of the index of a memory from the hippocampal complex to the frontal lobe over a period of several years (Treves and Rolls 1994; Rolls 2000). The present study encompasses seven days between training on an association task and subsequent testing. Consequently, only Stage 1 consolidation effects — long-term potentiation of the short-term memory traces, evinced by increased association strength (retrieval efficiency, discussed in a later chapter) between the hippocampus and primary, secondary, and association areas — are expected. Electrophysiological studies have produced reasonable evidence that particular spectral (frequency-related) changes in the interactions of neural populations occur at this stage, particularly with respect to coherence (the correlation between the timing and amplitude of population responses) and spectral power (Bodizs, Bekesy, Szucs, Barsi and I-Ialasz 2002; Cantero, Atienza, Salas and Dominguez-Marin 2002b). At least one neuroimaging study has also demonstrated an overall decrease in the recruitment of brain areas after a period of consolidation (Buchel, Coull and Friston 1999). Both findings corroborate the intuitive prediction of changes due to Hebbian processes: brain regions necessary for learning, such as executive, attentional, and working memory regions, are recruited at the pre—consolidation (encoding) time point, as are regions involved in the perception of peripheral (distractor) stimuli; connections between these regions and the memory network undergo long-term depression, however, and are not activated at the post-consolidation recall time point (Alvarez and Squire 1994; Chrobak and Buzsaki 1998b; Chrobak and Buzsaki 1998a). Electrophysiological studies cannot unambiguously resolve signal sources, however, and thus cannot be used to create a final, systems-level interpretation of mnemonic binding; nor has any functional magnetic resonance (FMRI) study, to the knowledge of the author, attempted to operationalize changes in retrieval efficiency with these measures. The present study therefore endeavors to assess changes in cortical recruitment, metabolic demands, and FMRI signal complexity1 and rate of change as correlates of memory consolidation. ‘ The terms complexity, entropy, and chaoticity are used interchangeably in the present dissertation, but it should be pointed out that all three terms are used in a technical (as opposed to lay) sense to refer to the Shannon (information-theoretic) entropy of a signal. More specifically, the Shannon entropy is a quantification for the degree of a vector’s unstable aperiodicity. The BOLD time series Shannon entropy (TSE) is described in depth in the “Shannon entropy of the voxel time series (TSE)” section of the Methodology chapter; the reader is also referred to Shannon’s seminal 1948 paper, A Mathematical Theory of Communication (Shaman, C. E. (1948). A mathematical theory of communication. Bell Sys Tech J 27: The consolidation period has been shown to result in the decreased lability of memory traces, increased long-term potentiation (LTP) and firing rate coherence among mnemonic network constituents, and increased long-term depression (LTD) with decreased recruitement among peripheral networks (Bailey, Montarolo, Chen, Kandel and Schacher 1992; Alvarez and Squire 1994; Zhang, Endo, Cleary, Eskin and Byrne 1997; Chrobak and Buzsaki 1998b; Chrobak and Buzsaki 1998a; Buchel, Coull et al. 1999; Eichenbaum 2001; Bodizs, Bekesy et al. 2002; Cantero, Atienza et al. 2002b). Thus, one might expect the memory consolidation interval to serve as a temporal window during which binding and synaptic integration among memory traces are “tuned” to optimal efficiency (see McClelland, McNaughton and O'Reilly 1995; Squire and Alvarez 1995; Chrobak and Buzsaki 1998b; Abel and Lattal 2001; Eichenbaum 2001; Haist, Bowden Gore et al. 2001; Ross and Eichenbaum 2006; Wang, Hu and Tsien 2006). This does in fact appear to be the case, as previously-stored memory traces in medial temporal cortex and isocortex are reactivated and entrained to so-called sharp wave-ripple complex oscillations during the presumptive consolidation period of sleep, thus reenforcing synaptic weights (Buzsaki 1998; Siapas and Wilson 1998; Stickgold, Scott, Rittenhouse and Hobson 1999; Datta 2000; Maier, Nimmrich and Draguhn 2003; Gais and Born 2004; Walker and Stickgold 2004). Conventional wisdom holds that, in spite of this putative consolidation-related mnemonic network optimization, recall performance declines with time, except when the items to be encoded are repeatedly presented; yet research into the neurodynamic consequences of the consolidation phenomenon at the whole-brain level have only recently been undertaken, and many of the published studies on the subject have focused 379—423, 623-656.) on the hippocampal, rather than cortical and medial-temporal, contributions to recall. Furthermore, recent experimental manipulations of encoding depth have indicated that subjects who are well-trained to remember specific memory items may exhibit an improved proficiency in cortical reinstatement, the reactivation of previously-encoded memory traces, and an improved capacity, or retrieval orientation, for processing stimulus features so as to better match them with stored representations, which suggests that the strength of sensory and retrieval-mode binding processes may differentially influence recall performance (Weldon, Roediger and Challis 1989; Rugg and Wilding 2000; Robb and Rugg 2002; Wheeler and Buckner 2003). However, these phenomena have not yet been studied in the context of a between-subjects paired associates task, nor in the context of episodic memory consolidation. CONNECTIVITY AND FUNCTIONAL ARCHITECTURE IN EPISODIC RETRIEVAL Demarcations of the various memory systems have classically been established through neuropsychological observation of human patients, notably through experiments with the amnesic patient H.M., and lesion studies in animals (Eichenbaum 1996; Eichenbaum, Dusek, Young and Bunsey 1996; Eichenbaum, Schoenbaum, Young and Bunsey 1996). These data have helped to establish the idea that memory can be dissociated into several cognitive networks, each of which is responsible for a particular type of mental representation of the past. At least five such networks are commonly referred to in the literature: two declarative memory systems (episodic and semantic memory) and the procedural, perceptual, and working memory systems. Schacter and Tulving have proposed three criteria that a network should meet in order to be considered an independent and complete memory system (Schacter and Tulving 1994). First, the network should be composed of regions that perform interrelated tasks that permit the storage and retrieval of a particular class of information. Second, the network can be characterized in terms of operational qualities that are, as a whole, unique to that network. And third, the operations of the network can be dissociated from those of other networks through ablative or neuropsychological studies. Based on these criteria, at least five major memory systems have been identified. These include semantic memory (long-term memory for facts and concepts, including associative memory for objects and words, independent of an episode in memory), the perceptual representation system (long-term, non-associative memory for Objects and words), procedural memory (long-term habit learning, classification skills, grammatical rules, and sequence memory), working memory (short-term retention of information for cognitive problem solving), and episodic memory (long-term retention of events: Eichenbaum 1996; long-term retention of events: Eichenbaum, Dusek et al. 1996; long- terrn retention of events: Eichenbaum, Schoenbaum et al. 1996; Gabrieli 1998). Many of these five systems can be further dissociated into subsystems, as discussed below. Declarative memory: the semantic and episodic systems Semantic memory is memory for information in the abstract: factual memory, such as memory about historical events in which the subject did not participate. It is dependent on the medial temporal lobes (MTL; including rhinal cortex and the hippocampal and perihippocampal areas), and the fact that this anatomical division is shared with episodic memory (discussed below) incorporates semantic and episodic memory into the descriptive heading “declarative memory.” In addition, semantic memory seems to recruit anterior and lateral regions of the temporal lobe in the left hemisphere (Eichenbaum et al., 1996; Gabrieli, 1998). Episodic memory itself is perhaps the most complex and best understood of the five memory systems. It is involved in memory for events that are the personal experience of the subject, and which are imbued with “warmth and intimacy,” as characterized by William James (James 1893). Like semantic memory, episodic memory has been linked with the MTL, which is comprised by the entorhinal cortex, perirhinal cortex, perihippocampal cortex, perihippocampal gyrus, and the hippocampal formation. The connections of the MTL are extensive; the hippocampus is connected to various unimodal and polymodal association areas through the perihippocampal gyrus and to the thalamus (see Figure 1). Furthermore, prefiontal areas (particularly the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex, precentral sulcus and inferior frontal gyrus; the dorsolateral prefiontal cortex, posterior middle frontal gyrus; and the left anterior prefi'ontal cortex are engaged during the recall effort (ecphory: Buckner, Koutstaal, Schacter, Dale, Rotte and Rosen l998a; Buckner, Koutstaal, Schacter, Wagner and Rosen l998b; Ranganath, Johnson and D'Esposito 2003), whereas the hippocampal formation itself is recruited during actual recall (Eichenbaum et al., 1996). Right prefrontal cortex, which is involved in attention, has also been disambiguated from episodic retrieval (Cabeza, Locantore and Anderson 2003). The medial temporal areas have traditionally been classified as subserving novelty detection (recognition: perirhinal and entorhinal cortices and the posterior Figure 1 Connections of the rhinal and perihippocampal cortices. PHC, perihippocampal cortex; PRC, perirhinal cortex; EcRC, ectorhinal cortex; ERC, entorhinal cortex; HC, hippocampus. Adapted from (Lavenex and Amaral 2000) and (Save, Nerad and Poucet 2000) Isocortex Visual (object) cortex Visuospctial, polysensory, and auditory association cortices PRC : = PHC ll EcRC . . ERC \ Long-term memory acquisition, . consolidation, and recall; I"SUIa HC responds to saliencies across temporally»dispersed stimuli Visual long-term memory, exact function unknown Encodes stimulus saliencies and stores or “indexes” memory traces in abstracted, or non- sensory, form hippocampusz), recency judgements (posterior hippocampus), visuospatial learning (medial entorhinal, anterior hippocampus), as well as the association of salient simultaneous or temporally-dispersed stimulus features (declarative associative learning and contextual learning: perirhinal, perihippocampal, entorhinal and ectorhinal cortices and posterior hippocampus) (Winters and Bussey 2005c; Witter and Moser 2006; 2 The fimctional neuroarchitectural divisions in question differ from quadripeds to bipeds. The fimctional differentiation of the hippocampus, originally cited as following a “dorsoventral” axis, has more recently been retooled as following a “septotemporal” axis, since the rodent dorsal hippocampus corresponds to the primate posterior hippocampus, whereas the rodent ventral hippocampus corresponds to the primate anterior hippocampus. See Moser, M. B. and E. I. Moser (1998). Functional differentiation in the hippocampus. Hippocampus 8(6): 608-19. Dudukovic and Wagner 2007; Kerr, Agster, Furtak and Burwell 2007; Kumaran and Maguire 2007). The individual medial temporal areas elude ascription to particular computational operations, however, due not only to the difficulty of constructing tasks that dissociate suppositional functions from each other, but also by the intricate interconnectivity, and therefore interdependence, of these areas (Preston and Gabrieli 2002). The structure of the perihippocampal region consists of the entorhinal cortex, Brodmann areas 28 and 34; medial (area 35) and lateral (area 36) perirhinal cortex; and the parahippocampal cortex. Information flows from isocortex to the perirhinal and parahippocampal cortex, to the ERC, dentate gyrus, CA3 field, and subiculurn, and back to isocortex. Information is initially processed in isocortex and then passed through the entorhinal (ERC), perirhinal (PRC), and parahippocampal (PHC) cortices to the hippocampal complex, where it is presumably optimized. After this Optimization, the information is returned to the isocortical area of origin, resulting in long-term memory consolidation. Parahippocampal lesions cause deficits as severe as hippocampal lesions alone, yet the role of areas surrounding the hippocampus, such as the entorhinal cortex, is Table l Projections to the perihippocampal regions (Lavenex and Amaral 2000). Abbreviations: PPC, posterior parietal [association] cortex; SSC, somatosensory cortex; OF C, orbitofiontal cortex; PFC, prefrontal cortex; ST S, superior temporal sulcus; TE, V4, visual areas TE and V4. Modality Projection TE (visual object information) PRC PPC and V4 (visuospatial) PHC 880 Both Ventrolateral cortex, OFC, cingulate, Both retrosplenial cortex, PFC, dorsal STS (polysensory cortices) Auditory association cortex PHC distinct from the role of the hippocampus itself. For example, ERC+PRC lesions prevent the formation of mnemonic representations of visually paired associates in the inferotemporal cortex. However, different areas of isocortex have different projections to the perihippocampal region as well, resulting in sensory specificity in many areas of the MTL (see Figure 1 and Table 1). Feedback, feedforward, auto- and reciprocal connections result in a conversion Of unimodal information to polymodal, supermodal, or amodal information, and allow abstractions in the HC complex before being returned to isocortex. MTL afferents to unimodal sensory cortex can interact with lower level information processed or stored in unimodal cortex. Efferent projections fiom entorhinal and perirhinal cortex are necessary for maintenance and consolidation of pair codings in auditory area TE and may be responsible for short-term memory maintenance (since these areas activate during the delay phase of a delayed nonmatch to sample, or DNMS, task). Perirhinal neurons are multimodal and probably encode more than sensory attributes. The entorhinal cortex is less involved in object/sensory analysis and coding than is the perirhinal cortex. These neurons may respond only to simultaneous activation from multiple (multimodal) areas; the ERC, for example, is thought to respond to “compound” stimuli. ERC and PRC 0 patterns are coherent during REM sleep, which is thought to participate in memory consolidation. The 0 origin is probably driven by the ERC and/or subiculum. The PRC does not get input from the medial septum, the HC/ERC pacemaker, but it does receive inputs fiom the neurons in the deep ERC, which have intrinsic 0 firing patterns (Lavenex and Amaral 2000). The hippocampus, in contrast, responds only to supermodal or amodal information and returns abstracted representations to the originating areas of isocortex. Role of other memory systems in episodic retrieval Three non-declarative memory systems — the perceptual representation system, procedural memory, and working memory — may also interact with the declarative memory system, depending on task demands (see Table 2). The perceptual representation system (PRS), memory for word and Object identification, can be subdivided into three subsystems: the visual word form subsystem (extrastriate visual cortex and temporal and frontal cortex), which appears to process orthography, the auditory word form subsystem (superior temporal and fi'ontal cortex), which processes auditory information, and the structural description subsystem (occiptotemporal regions, e. g. inferior temporal and fusiform gyri), which processes parts of objects and matches them to global structure. Although the PRS has been functionally and anatomically dissociated from declarative memory through PET and lesion studies, it is possible, if not likely, that a declarative memory task involving word or visual recognition (such as the task proposed in the present dissertation) would also engage the PRS (Eichenbaum et al., 1996; Gabrieli, 1998). Procedural memory is memory for habits and skills. Procedural memory does not recruit the MTL and is almost therefore anatomically independent of declarative (semantic and episodic) memory. Neuropsychological and pathological studies of amnesic patients have revealed that procedural memory is dependent upon a 10 corticostriatal system, as well as the motor cortex and basal ganglia (Eichenbaum et al., 1996; Gabrieli, 1998). Working memory enables short-term retention of information over several seconds (Eichenbaum et al., 1996; Gabrieli, 1998). Like the PRS, working memory can be anatomically and functionally categorized into two slave subsystems: the phonological loop (auditory and parietal cortex), which contains auditory information for working memory, and the visuospatial sketch pad, which is anatomically composed of the visual association cortex, inferior parietal lobule, and inferior prefi'ontal cortex (Eichenbaum et al. , 1996). ll Table 2 Regions typically involved in episodic retrieval tasks. LH, left hemisphere; RH, right hemisphere; BL, bilateral. Several of these regions will be used as target ROIs in the present study (see next chapter). Working Maintenance Precentral sulcus (BL) memory VISUOSPATIAL SKETCH PADZ: Visual association cortex Inferior parietal lobule Inferior prefrontal cortex (area 45) Inferior frontal gyrus (BL) Anterior frontal gyrus (LH) Medial frontal gyrus (RH) Phonological loop. Parietal cortex Auditory cortex Recognition Precentral sulcus (BL) Inferior frontal gyrus (BL) Posterior middle frontal gyrus (BL) Middle frontal gyrus (BL) Anterior prefrontal cortex (LH) Long-term Retrieval Entorhinal cortex (9 source)3 memory Medial septum (9 source) Perihippocampal cortex3 Perirhinal cortex3 Dorsal hippocampus5 Right frontal cortex6 PRS Recognition AUDITORY WORD FORM SUBSYSTEMZ: Superior temporal gyrus Right frontal cortex STRUCTURAL DESCRIPTION SYSTEM: Inferior temporal gyrus Fusiforrn gyrus ;,(Ranganath Johnson etal. 2003) :(Eichenbaum, Schoenbaum etal. 1996) :(Lavenex and Amaral 2000) :(Chrobak and Buzsaki 1998b) Z,(Bannerman Rawlins, McHugh, Deacon, Yee, Bast, Zhang, Pothuizen and Feldon 2004) :‘,(Buckner Koutstaaletal1998b) 7(Haxby, Honrvitz,UJeI1eider,Maisog, Pietrini and Grady 1994) 12 REGIONS OF INTEREST Regions of interest (ROIs) for the present study were delineated according to the AFNI implementation of the Talairach-Tourneaux human brain atlas (Talairach and Tournoux 1988) by warping the individual datasets to Talairach space. In this section, the significance of the various ROls, according to recently published literature, will be discussed in order to place them into the context of the previously-stated hypotheses. Ten ROls were selected for the present study based on their functional significance as long-term memory, visual, or motor regions. Four, including hippocampus and entorhinal, ectorhinal, and perirhinal cortex, constitute the majority of the MTL with the exception of the periamygdaloid area (also known as the semilunar gyrus), posterior PHG, the prepyriform area (also called piriform cortex; laterally bounded by the retrosubicular area 48), and the presubiculum, which are not known to play a pivotal role in declarative memory storage or recall. An additional four ROIs, including visual areas V1, V2, and V3 and the fusiform gyrus, were included in order to facilitate sampling from lower (V1 and V2) and higher-order (V3 and fusiform) visual areas, particularly in the ventral visual stream. Primary motor (M1) cortex and premotor (PMC) cortex were also included as control and motor attention areas (see below), respectively. MEDIAL TEMPORAL AREAS: LONG-TERM MEMORY R018 The hippocampus (HC), entorhinal cortex (ERC: Brodmann areas 28 and 34), ectorhinal cortex (EcRC: Brodmann area 36), and perirhinal cortex (PRC: Brodmann area 35) were 13 identified as regions of interest for the present study based largely on the neuroanatomical and neuropsychological studies of Lavanex and Amaral (2000), Eichenbaum and colleagues (1996), Gabrieli (1998), and Nadel and colleagues (2000). The functional neuroanatomy of these long-term memory regions is further described in the introductory chapters. To review: information in conveyed from isocortex to the perirhinal and parahippocampal regions, depending on the modality of the stimulus; PRC appears to be specialized for visual object information, whereas PHG seems to be related to the storage and conveyance of visuospatial information. It has been suggested that PRC and PHG are necessary for the maintenance and consolidation of pair codings, and as such are of particular interest in the present study. Furthermore, current anatomical models suggest that information exchange takes place between the PRC/PHG and HC, parietal cortex, anterior cingulate, and the insula (Lavenex and Amara12000). The PRC is located along the banks of the rhinal sulcus and is bounded medially by entorhinal area 28 and laterally by ectorhinal area 36; the PHG is defined for the purposes of this dissertation as consisting of the periamygdaloid area (also known as the semilunar gyrus), posterior PHG, the prepyriform area (also called piriform cortex; laterally bounded by the retrosubicular area 48), and the presubiculum, and is not included as an ROI in the present dissertation. Output from the PHG/PRC region is sent to the ERC (Lavenex and Amaral, 2000), which also interacts with parietal association cortex (PAC) for spatial perception and cognition. The ERC in turn sends its output to the dentate gyrus (in the hippocampus). The function of the ERC is not well understood, however, except that it plays a role in all phases of long-term memory acquisition, consolidation, and recall, and 14 seems to function in the pairing of stimuli that are temporally dispersed (Egorov, Hamam, Fransen, Hasselmo and Alonso 2002; Frank and Brown 2003). The ERC is divided into Brodmann areas 28 and 34; both areas are located on the medial-caudal aspect of the temporal lobe. Information is sent from the hippocampal dentate gyrus to the CA3 field and finally to the subiculum (also in the HC), which returns abstracted information to isocortex. The nature of abstraction in the hippocampal complex is not well understood; however, it is believed that the HC responds only to supramodal or amodal information, such as salient unified features of a memory, and it is this supramodal and amodal information that is returned to cortex. As discussed previously, the HC itself has been shown to activate during acquisition, consolidation, and recall, although the time course of its influence upon these phenomena is a matter of controversy (Eichenbaum, Schoenbaum et al. 1996; Nadel and Land 2000; Nadel, Samsonovich, Ryan and Moscovitch 2000). The ectorhinal cortex (EcRC: area 36) has been shown to have backprojections onto V1 and is heavily interconnected with area TE in simians (Naya, Yoshida and Miyashita 2003a; Naya, Yoshida, Takeda, Fujimichi and Miyashita 2003b; Clavagnier, Falchier and Kennedy 2004). It appears to be part of a visual long-term memory system (along with other visual areas) in these higher primates, but its role in humans has not been decisively determined. Extrapolation from animal data, however, suggests that area 36 may also participate in long-term visual memory in humans. EcRC is located primarily in the fusiform gyrus (this area is excluded from the fusiform gyrus ROI in the 15 (A) Unimodal association cortex, polymodal association cortex, frontal cortex, cingulate, V4 Polymodal association cortex I mugt,‘r ERC(34) I? ' ‘ ‘ AA... ' 3,, Orbitofrontal , primary motor (M1), primary visual (V1), primary somatosensory (S1), supplementary somatosensory (SSC), and visual association cortex Unimodal association cortex, polymodal association cortex , frontal cortex (V3) (B) FRONTAL POLE Entorhinal ctx. Entorhinal ctx. (BA 28) (BA 28 and 34). Ectorhinal ctx. (BA 36) Hippocampus Peri r‘h1' nal ctx. (BA 35) Ectorh1nal ctx. (BA 36) LH RH OCCIPITAL POLE Figure 2 (A) Interconnections among constituent regions of the MTL and other areas of interest in the present study. Blue arrows indicate neuronal projections between areas as determined by anatomical studies of the cortex and temporal lobe. Arrow locations do not accurately reflect the anatomical location of interregional projections (Sources: Deacon, Eichenbaum, Rosenberg and Eckmann 1983; Schacter, Buckner, Koutstaal, Dale and Rosen; Burwell and Amaral; Burwell and Amaral; Lavenex and Amaral; Save and Poucet 2000a; Save and Poucet 2000b). (B) The anatomical locations of MTL areas in the Talairach-Toumeaux template brain, obtained fiom the AFNI suite’s Talairach Daemon. Abbreviations: LH, lefi hemisphere; RH, right hemisphere; ctx., cortex; BA, Brodmann area. 16 present study) and is bounded laterally and caudally by inferotemporal area 20, medially by PRC area 35, and rostally by temporopolar area 38. MTL areas, which, again, serve to associate salient features Of encoded declarative stimuli, also appear to be affected by consolidation processes. Intuitively, this would mean that changes in plasticity would drive the amalgamation of pre-consolidation medial temporal interneurons serving to link memory traces stored in isocortex, such that fewer interneurons are required for the linkage at post-consolidation (see Figure 2). PREMOTOR CORTEX AS A VISUOSPATIAL ATTENTION AREA Brodmann area 6 has been cited as an attentional area since its activation is strongly correlated with tasks involving covert shifts of attention, and particularly in tasks that demand shifts in visuospatial attention (Nobre, Sebestyen, Gitelman, Mesulam, Frackowiak and Frith 1997). Furthermore, area 6 has been subdivided into medial and lateral regions, which are differentially activated by verbal and spatial memory sequencing tasks, respectively. This finding was also supported by a repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) inference study, in which TM stimulation to the medial Brodmann area 6 selectively disrupted verbal but not spatial cognitive processing, whereas TM stimulation to the lateral Brodmann area 6 disrupted spatial but not verbal cognitive processing (Tanaka, Honda and Sadato 2005). Area 6 comprises parts of the superior and middle fi'ontal gyri as well as the rostral precentral gyrus and is bounded medially by the cingulate sulcus and laterally by the lateral sulcus. l7 VISUAL AREAS It is beyond the scope of this dissertation to discuss in depth the fimction and history of research into visual cortex. Here, the fimctional architecture of the visual streams from V1 to V5 will be highlighted (with the exception of V4/1PL, discussed above). For a more thorough review of human visual neuroanatomy, the reader is referred to the recent review by Wichmann and Muller-Forell (Wichmann and Muller-Forell 2004); the seminal paper on the dorsal and ventral streams by Ungerleider and Mishkin (Ungerleider and Mishkin 1982) is also a good reference. Visual area VI , located in Brodmann area 17, gets input from the thalamic lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN) and responds selectively to (i.e., its cells are tuned to) orientation-specific contours and (in some cases) respond to color (Hubel and Wiesel 1959; Hubel and Wiesel 1962). V1 exhibits complex within-region circuitry, as well as interactions with the LGN, in addition to visual areas V2 and MT/VS, through unidirectional and reciprocal projections, which may serve simple attentional firnctions in visual selection (Livingstone and Hubel 1984b; Livingstone and Hubel 1984a; Albright 1993). V1 is bounded by Brodmann area 18 and surrounds the calcarine fissure. It is anatomically identifiable by the presence of the bands of Gennari. Visual areas V2 and V3, located in Brodmann areas 18 and 19, respectively, are the first of many so—called extrastriate visual areas. V2 shares both feedback connections with area V1 and feedforward connections with V3, V4/IPL, and V5/MT. Neurons comprising area V2 are, like area V1, tuned to feature orientation and color, but 18 additionally have figure-ground tuning (Qiu and von der Heydt 2005). V2 is also interconnected with areas V4 and MT, and exhibits some attentional modulation (Albright 1993; Rao 2005). Visual information is relayed from V2 to V3 as part Of a ventral visual stream (vVS), which has a role in object recognition, and from V2 to V5/MT as part of a dorsal visual stream (dVS), which functions in spatiotemporal tasks (Ungerleider and Mishkin 1982). V3 (Brodmann area 19), part of the vVS, is tuned to aspects of visual motion (Braddick, O'Brien, Wattam-Bell, Atkinson, Hartley and Turner 2001). Area V3 has strong interconnections with both area V2 and area V4/IPL (Albright 1993). Brodmann area l8/V2 is bounded by area l7/V1 and area 19/V3; area 19/V3 is further bounded by areas 37 and 39. The firsiform gyrus is a functionally heterogenous area most recently shown to be involved in face processing (Kanwisher, McDermott and Chun 1997; Haxby, Gobbini, Furey, Ishai, Schouten and Pietrini 2001; Haxby, Hoffman and Gobbini 2002; Rossion, Schiltz and Crommelinck 2003; Yovel and Kanwisher 2005). However, it also seems to be involved in other aspects of visual processing and computation, including visual working memory, lexical tasks, and mental manipulation of orientation of visual stimuli (Orban and Vogels 1998; Pammer, Lavis and Comelissen 2004; Uncapher and Rugg 2005a; Uncapher and Rugg 2005b). The fusiform gyrus consists of ectorhinal area 36 (which was included as the EcRC ROI but excluded from the fusiform ROI in the present study) as well as area 37. Area 37 is bounded caudally by area 19/V 3, rostrally by area 20/V 5 and area 21, and dorsally by area 39. 19 PRIMARY MOTOR CORTEX: A CONTROL AREA Finally, primary motor cortex (Ml, Brodmann area 4) was included in the analyses in order to assess its connectivity with cortical and subcortical areas during our episodic retrieval task. It has been shown that increased within-M1 neural firing rate coherence is related to corticospinal (descending voluntary) control of muscle movement, and, furthermore, that this coherence is further correlated with coherence between M1 and motor neurons in the beta frequency range (Gerloff, Braun, Staudt, Hegner, Dichgans and Krageloh-Mann 2006). Area 4/Ml is bounded rostrally by premotor area 6 and caudally by area 3. 20 F MRI SPATIAL AND TEMPORAL SPECIFICITY The present study endeavors to assess changes in cortical recruitment, metabolic demands, and FMRI signal entropy and rate of change as correlates of memory consolidation. In order to formulate a proper methodology to perform this assessment, however, the arguments must be made that (a) the BOLD signal is substantially coupled to, and co-registered with, neural activity; (b) that BOLD-FMRI is satisfactorily colocalized to neural activity, so one may infer details about the underlying neural activity; and (c) that one may extrapolate, based on certain features of the F MRI dataset, where in the brain the T2*—weighted signal change originates (i. e., at which locations the BOLD signal arises from increased metabolic activity as opposed to passive diffusion of deoxyhemoglobin). This chapter, then, will turn to the principal methodology of the present dissertation, functional magnetic resonance imaging (FMRI). The underlying physiology of the F MRI Signal will be examined and the implications of this physiology for investigations of firnctional neural architecture, as well as the spatial and temporal resolution of the FMRI method and its colocalization with aspects of neural electrophysiological phenomena, will be highlighted. BOLD SIGNAL PHYSIOLOGY FMRI is an extension of anatomical MRI that relies on a T2*-weighted, blood oxygenation-level dependent (BOLD) signal (Ogawa, Lee, Kay and Tank 1990) that 21 arises indirectly from the metabolic sequelae of postsynaptic potentials, particularly local field potentials (LFPS), which correspond to cooperative population activity (synchronized somatodendritic potentials: see Logothetis, Pauls, Augath, Trinath and Oeltermann 2001; Logothetis 2002; Logothetis 2003; Logothetis and Pfeuffer 2004; Logothetis and Wandell 2004). LFPS fluctuate relatively slowly; the magnitude of this fluctuation is not related to cell size but with the geometry and extent of dendritic arborization. Thus, the parallel arrangement of apical dendrites heavily influences LFP measurements due to their “open field” arrangement. LFPS reflect a net effect of cellular events — synaptic events, voltage responsive membrane oscillations, afterhyperpolarizations, and afierdepolarizations due to Kca channels (Logothetis, Pauls et al. 2001). Anatomically, the density of brain vasculature seems to be related to the location of perisynaptic elements, rather than the location of neural somata, which suggests that cerebral blood flow (CBF) is related to neurotransmitter release rather than perikaryal metabolic demands. This is further verified by increases in CBF only with orthodromic, not antidromic, microstimulation. Logothetis (2001) has noted that glucose uptake, the postsynaptic effects of glutamate, and the restoration of ion gradients after an action potential — all of which can be categorized under the heading “synaptic events” — are the most energy-consuming processes in the brain and drive CBF changes. Of these, presynaptic and postsynaptic currents drive the energy demand and are the dominant elements in LFPS (Logothetis et al., 2001). Just as cortical areas are delineated anatomically by differences in cyto- and myeloarchitecture, they can be delineated functionally by differential physiological properties (electrophysiology) and connectivity (neuroimaging, tract tracing). 22 Neuroimaging methods like FMRI have the advantage over invasive methods in that they permit longitudinal studies (e.g. on learning, plasticity, reorganization). The BOLD contrast signal used in FMRI is derived from magnetic field inhomogeneities caused by differential deoxyhaemoglobin (de) concentrations. de is confined to the intracellular Space of erythrocytes, which are in turn confined to the vasculature. Thus, the field gradient responsible for the BOLD signal is a result of the susceptibility differences between the de-containing compartments and the surrounding space. FMRI pulse sequences are sensitive to these differences and alter the signal when de concentration changes. Upon neural activation the increase in de concentration would be expected to enhance the difference in magnetic susceptibility, thereby decreasing the BOLD signal; however, the exact Opposite occurs. This is due to an increase in CBF that overcompensates for the increase in oxygen uptake: an oversupply of oxygenated blood is delivered to the site (Raichle 1998). Thus, changes in CBF corresponding to neural processes are reflected by an increase in the BOLD signal. SPATIOTEMPORAL RESOLUTION OF F MRI The development in the late 19803 of high-field (1.5T) MR imaging techniques (echo- planar imaging and spiral imaging) and discovery of two endogenous “contrast” mechanisms — BOLD (Ogawa, Lee et al. 1990) and differential net longitudinal magnetization with changes in tissue perfusion — initiated a revolution in firnctional neuroimaging, largely replacing paramagnetic tracer technology for in-depth studies of cognition. The temporal relationship of neural and vascular events (neurovascular 23 coupling) was actually first described by Angelo Mosso in 1881, but the nature of this relationship has only recently been defined (Logothetis et al., 2001). Whisker barrel stimulation in murine models demonstrated that CBF increases within approximately 2 sec of stimulation onset and peaks within 5-7 sec. However, paramagnetic tracer studies have revealed that gross cerebral volume changes actually lag behind changes in CBF, likely due to a “capacitance system” in the capillaries and post-capillary beds, and I-Ib/HbOz changes even precede CBF changes. Thus, changes in hemoglobin concentrations, local CBF, and gross CBF are related temporally but perhaps not causally. The implication of the BOLD signal technique is that the block design, which was necessary for single-photon and positron emission tomography (since they require a quasi-equilibrium state of > 60 sec), became superfluous for MRI, and event-related designs, such as the one proposed in the present dissertation, were made possible. Consequently, the short history of fast (event-related or ER)—flV1RI has been characterized by investigations of ever-smaller sampling windows. Blamire et al. demonstrated detectable signal changes after stimulus durations as short as 2 seconds; Bandettini et al. decreased this window to as little as 500 msec (Bandettini, Wong, Hinks, Tikofsky and Hyde 1992; Blamire, Ogawa, Ugurbil, Rothman, McCarthy, Ellermann, Hyder, Rattner and Shulman 1992). More recently, Savoy et al. established that the BOLD signal could be detected after a 34 msec-duration visual stimulus and Boynton et al. showed that responses can be summated linearly over time (with some limitations) (Savoy 2005). The computability of a hemodynamic response function (HRF) is directly related to the strength of the field; as field strength increases (e.g. 1.5T to 3T) the signal—to-noise ratio (SNR) increases. In the lower magnetic fields, time series can be reconstructed with (a) 24 overlap correction methods similar to those used in ERP research and (b) averaging across trials, which brings out the modal tendencies in the HRF. Rosen et al. note that temporal sensitivity is a filnction of the SNR (and by implication, field strength) as well as the haemodynamic response (HRF) variance and the methods used to characterize it (Rosen, Buckner and Dale 1998). They also report that variance in HRF latency within and between brain regions is not merely a matter of vasculature diameter. Although larger vessels do exhibit longer latencies (a significant factor within regions such as V1) the longest delays are seen in anterior PFC, an area with a minimum of large vessels. Savoy et al. (1995) have reported latencies of 500 msec in V1; Buckner et al. report latencies of 500 to 1000 msec between visual and prefrontal areas; and at the other extreme, Schacter et al. note delays of 2 to 4 sec between various prefi'ontal regions (Buckner, Bandettini, O'Craven, Savoy, Petersen, Raichle and Rosen 1996; Schacter, Buckner et al. 1997; Savoy 2005). The spatial resolution of F MRI has been examined most closely with manganese contrast, which is paramagnetic and emphasizes the calcium-dependent sites of neural activity. Manganese enters voltage-operated calcium channels and mimic calcium’s role in neurotransmitter release. In a recent study with manganese MRI contrast in rats, Duong et al. demonstrated a co-registration of < 200 um between synaptic activity and BOLD contrast, suggesting that the BOLD signal (at high fields [9.4T]) resolves synaptic activity to a satisfactory degree (Duong, Silva, Lee and Kim 2000). It is therefore reasonable to conclude that the BOLD signal satisfactorily exemplifies the localization of [post]synaptic activity. 25 MODELING THE HEMODYNAMIC RESPONSE FUNCTIONS Aguirre et al have investigated the variability in HRF shape and timing within subjects (different brain regions), across subjects, and at different times of the same day or on different days (Aguirre, Zarahn and D'Esposito 1998). Formerly the best estimates for the HRF were (I) a Poisson distribution (Friston et al, 1994) or (2) a gamma distribution with two free parameters and a phase delay (Boynton et al, 1996). It was determined that the mean tirne-to-peak in subjects was 4.7:t1.1 sec and ranged from 2.7 to 6.2 sec; HRFS across subjects were significantly more variable than those within a subject, even across several days. Additionally, the Poisson fiinction accounted for only 25% of the HRF variances; the gamma function accounted for 70%, which was a significant difference (p < 0.001). When a single subject-specific HRF estimate was used (generated empirically from the single-subject data) approximately 96% of variance was accounted for. Therefore, the authors conclude that an empirical estimate of the HRF is much more desirable than an a priori estimate. For blocked flVIRI paradigms, in which the HRF is oversampled, an a priori estimate will not decrease statistical sensitivity significantly; however, the authors note that an empirical HRF lends to the analysis a measurable increase in sensitivity — as it will for event-related designs (which are much more sensitive to the statistical decrement imposed by the a priori firnction). Therefore, the HRF varies within and between subjects and a deconvolution analysis (discussed in the Methodology chapter) will be utilized to overcome this variance in the present dissertation. 26 AIMS AND THEORETICAL TREATMENT OF CONSOLIDATION NEURODYNAMICS A paired-associate task (described in the Methodology chapter) was utilized to examine changes in task performance and BOLD physiology with episodic memory consolidation. This task required subjects to learn, via a computer-based training session, to associate visually presented words with abstract pictures, then identify correctly matched, incorrectly-matched, and control condition pairings during a testing session, which took place within an MRI magnet. Two FMRI time points were established by dividing the subject pool into a pre—consolidation time point group (TPI), whose constituents were tested in the MRI magnet immediately after the training session, and a post-consolidation time point group (TPz), who were tested in the MRI magnet seven days after the initial training session. This paradigm, coupled with a non-invasive, systems-level approach, firnctional MRI (FMRI), enabled the assessment of changes in subject memory performance and physiology with consolidation. Although many FMRI investigations of mnemonic functions have been performed, a majority has dealt with modular cortical activity correlated with a given memory function; fewer are the studies of such memory fiinctions as a system (e.g., a recent analysis of cortical connectivity during episodic encoding by Summerfield, Greene, Wager, Egner, Hirsch and Mangels 2006), and none, to the knowledge of the author, have attempted to analyze differences in the FMRI signal entropy or signal response rate in episodic memory areas with consolidation. 27 This scarcity of neuroimaging-based mnemonic neurodynamics research is likely due to the indirect relationship between neural activity and the F MRI signal: the BOLD signal, upon which most FMRI studies are founded, is an epiphenomenon of glycolytic activity concomitant to synaptodendritic metabolic demands (Ogawa, Lee et al. 1990). As such, neural activity and the BOLD signal are said to be related through a black box or “transfer function” of unknown parameters, and these parameters vary both across brain areas within a given subject as well as across subjects and task demands. Nevertheless, the correlation between the BOLD signal and neural activity (more Specifically, the local field potential phenomenon) has been shown to exhibit a rough linearity across low to moderate metabolic magnitudes, such that neuroimaging research may generate probative, albeit admittedly indirect, neurodynamics data (Logothetis, Pauls et al. 2001 ). CONSOLIDATION AS AN OPTIMIZING PROCESS A discussion of the theoretical basis of the hypotheses of the present dissertation iS best approached through a conceptualization of the episodic memory system as a self-tuning network that aims to minimize its energy expenditure relative to the storage and recall of a given memory trace. For the purposes of the present dissertation, retrieval efficiency was defined as the Speed, recruitment, and retrieval success or eflicacy of the mnemonic (long-term memory) and supporting (working memory, attentional and executive, and visual and motor) networks recruited during trace recall. Retrieval “speed” and “success” were characterized behaviorally by subject response time and accuracy, respectively, on the 28 paired associates task, whereas retrieval-concomitant “recruitment” was characterized physiologically by the degree of cortical recruitment and metabolic demand relative to subject retrieval speed and success. It should be noted that this definition of“eff1ciency” is closely related to the use of the term in thermodynamics in that it is conceptually a ratio of useful output (behavioral trace retrieval performance) versus energy expenditure (metabolic intensity per unit recruited volume): performance or performance recruitment PC /AV ' where PC represents the percent BOLD signal change from baseline and A V represents the degree of cortical recruitment (activation volume). Although it is beyond the scope of the present work to attempt to typify this ratio as a quantitative index of neural systems efficiency (although such attempts have been made: see Chance 2007), this conceptualization of retrieval efficiency was used exhaustively in the origination of the theory and hypotheses in the current study. It is imperative to note that, by this definition, increased efficiency over a consolidation interval could be argued to have occurred in any of several cases. Namely, the output-to-energy expenditure ratio would increase if (a) subject performance on a recall task improved without a corresponding decrease in metabolic demand or cortical recruitment; (b) metabolic demand and cortical recruitment decreased without a corresponding improvement in subject performance; (c) metabolic demand decreased without corresponding improvements in cortical recruitment or subject performance; or 29 (d) cortical recruitment decreased without corresponding improvements in metabolic demand or subject performance. EVIDENCE FOR INTERIM PHYSIOLOGICAL CHANGES IN RETRIEVAL SUBSTRATES Prior behavioral, electrophysiological, and neuroimaging studies of memory performance in well-trained versus poorly-trained subjects have in fact provided some evidence that performance on episodic memory tasks are dependent upon the “attunement” of perceptual and trace retrieval circuits toward task Specificity, i.e., the identification of previously learned stimuli, and, therefore, do exhibit some hallmarks of optimization (Tulving 1983; Weldon, Roediger et al. 1989; Rugg and Wilding 2000; Robb and Rugg 2002; Wheeler and Buckner 2003). In particular, well-trained subjects appear to exhibit an enhanced capacity for quickly identifying, through covert attentional mechanisms, distinctive characteristics of presented stimuli in order to expedite retrieval of those features from memory; and, fithhermore, such subjects also exhibit an enhanced capacity for reactivating, upon recall, the cortical networks recruited during encoding. These phenomena have become known as retrieval orientation and cortical reinstatement (or ‘transfer-appropriate processing”) effects, respectively (Lockhart 2002; Vaidya, Zhao, Desmond and Gabrieli 2002; Herron and Rugg 2003b; Herron and Wilding 2004; Homberger, Morcom and Rugg 2004; Herron and Wilding 2006a; Homberger, Rugg and Henson 2006b; Mulligan and Lozito 2006; Stenberg, Johansson and Rosen 2006; Woodruff, Uncapher and Rugg 2006). Clearly, such changes in perceptual and retrieval circuits, if they exist, are relevant to an understanding of pre- and post-consolidation 30 retrieval efficiency; however, these phenomena have not yet been examined in a time- dependent or consolidation context. It is logical to assume that the shifts in recall neurophysiology predicted by the retrieval orientation and cortical reinstatement phenomena, as well as the Hebbian changes in neuronal efficacy that would ostensibly cause them, would likely occur both within the trace network and the visual cortices recruited for future recognition of the stimuli the traces represent. To the knowledge of the author, the ideal test of this assumption — the presence or absence of retrieval orientation and/or cortical reinstatement effects in the context of a MTL lesion — has not been performed. Given the robust evidence for close connectivity between episodic and attentional brain regions, however, it is unlikely that these phenomena are asymmetric across the regions that mediate attention to remembered stimuli and the memory traces which drive them (Chun and Turk-Browne 2007); therefore, correlates of retrieval orientation and cortical reinstatement were expected in medial temporal, attentional, and visual areas, except where direct descending attentional input is lacking (e. g. Vl: Rao 2005). It was of particular interest in the current work to substantiate or refute retrieval orientation effects in the right hemisphere fusiform gyrus and ventral visual stream, where such effects have been reported (Vaidya, Zhao et al. 2002; Woodruff, Uncapher et al. 2006). The ventral, or object identification, visual stream and fusiform gyri, especially in the right cortical hemisphere, are believed to subserve more holistic or “global” feature processing (Brown and Kosslyn 1993; Johannes, Wieringa, Matzke and Munte 1996; Yamaguchi, Yamagata and Kobayashi 2000). Therefore, three predictions were made: 31 I) that increased synaptic weights within visuospatial networks concomitant to the cortical reinstatement phenomenon would increase the efficacy of neuronal firing, such that the BOLD signal rates of all areas (except primary visual and primary motor cortices) would be accelerated (H. 3); 2) that post-consolidation subjects would exhibit shifts in the laterality of metabolic demands in extrastriate areas relative tO pro-consolidation subjects, thereby evincing changes in visual attention with consolidation (H. 4); and 3) that increased synaptic weights within visuospatial networks concomitant to the cortical reinstatement phenomenon, as well as the differential modulation of the activity of those networks by descending attentional neurons due to retrieval orientation effects, would moderate the requisite amount of information processing in all extrastriate visual areas, such that both left- and right-hemisphere post-consolidation visual cortices would exhibit lower metabolic demands than at pre-consolidation (H. 4). COMPUTATIONAL EFFICIENCY AND CORTICAL DEMANDS: ACTIVATION VOLUME AND ENTROPY A fundamental assumption in the present study is that the number of neurons required to accomplish an encoding or computational function decreases as the encoding or computational efficiency of a neuron or population of neurons increases, an assumption that is supported by molecular, electrophysiological, and neuroanatomical evidence that repeated neuronal activity drives changes in proximal and distal dendritic morphology, which in turn effect changes in synaptic integration and therefore the computational 32 functions of the neuron (Holmes and Levy 1990; Zador, Koch and Brown 1990; Bailey, Chen, Keller and Kandel 1992; Bailey, Montarolo et al. 1992; Yuste and Denk 1995; Mallot and Giannakopoulos 1996; Shepherd 1996; Zhang, Endo et al. 1997; Hausser, Spruston and Stuart 2000; Verzi 2004; Frith 2005; Segev 2006). Thus, if memory consolidation is a process of mnemonic network optimization, the number of recruited neurons (or voxels, in the present case) would be expected to decrease. Two important exceptions in the present study are the premotor cortex, which appears to subserve attentional functions, and primary motor cortex, which subserves the motor response. Neither ROI was expected to differ with respect to recruitment nor metabolic demands with consolidation, since no changes in their computational fiinctions were expected. In the present study, therefore, the spatial extent of BOLD signal activation is quantitatively assessed through statistical analysis of the activation volume (in mm3) per region of interest. However, activation volume measures are susceptible to noise in the MR magnet environment and intersubject variability (Saad, Ropella, DeYoe and Bandettini 2003), and furthermore, it is not informative with respect to the nature or degree of information processing in signals fi'om the neural populations (remnant voxels) that remain correlated with the task after consolidation. Moreover, interpretation of observed decreases in activation volume is further complicated by the indirect relationship between neuronal activity and the BOLD signal which underlies the definition of “activated” voxels. A change in activation volume may, however, be due to any number of factors — internalization of salient stimulus features, circuit multiplexing, or increased neuronal computational efficiency — but in the absence of data indicating that these changes in recruitment are accompanied by increases in (or stability of) the 33 task-specificity and magnitude of neuronal activity, it is equally justifiable to conclude that such decreased recruitment is a consequence of, for example, subject inattentiveness or task habituation. If however, a region of interest was shown to exhibit a higher information load or entropy, suggesting an increase in computational efficiency, and/or a higher covariance between the regional BOLD signal and stimulus presentation, suggesting task attunement, a stronger argument could be made that network Optimization had occurred in the region. In the present study, therefore, computational efficiency and task attunement are quantitatively assessed through statistical analysis of the entropy (voxel Shannon time series entropy, or voxel TSE, a novel application to FMRI) and voxel/task cross-correlation (VTCC, or the BOLD signal response rate), respectively. The particulars of both metrics are discussed in the Methodology chapter. An analysis of task-related signal change and voxel TSE will also serve to support or refute an inference that remnant voxels exhibiting a post-consolidation increase in entropy are as metabolically active as (or more metabolically active than) significantly task-correlated voxels at pre-consolidation. This issue is of interest because, again, the assessment of BOLD signal entropy is a novel application for an FMRI-based study of human neurodynamics, and it is as yet unclear how faithfully BOLD signal entropy corresponds to neuronal signal entropy; a statistical concurrence of task-correlated increases in metabolic intensity and voxel TSE would suggest that remnant voxels both carry more information and are more metabolically active after consolidation, thereby supporting the assumption that the metrics discussed above are in actuality assessing the degree of synaptic integration (and therefore the computational intensity) within the region of interest. 34 CORTICAL REINSTATEMENT AND RETRIEVAL ORIENTATION EFFECTS IN VISUAL AND MEDIAL-TEMPORAL AREAS Cortical reinstatement and retrieval orientation effects should be evinced by a decrease in cortical recruitment in long-term memory (LTM) and visual substrates, consequent to Hebbian competition over the consolidation interval, as well as an increase in BOLD response rates subsequent to the onset of neural activity, as correlates of enhanced synaptic weights of neurons still participatory in the LTM network after consolidation (Kida, Josselyn, de Ortiz, Kogan, Chevere, Masushige and Silva 2002; Bozon, Davis and Laroche 2003; Bozon, Kelly, Josselyn, Silva, Davis and Laroche 2003). Furthermore, LTM and visual circuit attunement (reinstatement) and retrieval orientation effects should be at least correlated with behavioral performance improvement, consequent to the efficacy Of the post-consolidation network. However, one would not expect consolidation to have the same effect magnitude on all retrieval-related networks: differential retrieval efficiency concomitant to memory consolidation was expected to manifest differently in attentional areas, which support the trace retrieval process, than in medial temporal and visual networks, which actually store and retrieve previously-stored memory traces. Shifts in the orienting Of attention from local features to global features with consolidation, corresponding to retrieval orientation effects, were predicted to be evinced by bilateral activation in pre-consolidation subjects and right-hemisphere dominance (fewer left—hemisphere and more right-hemisphere clusters: Brown and Kosslyn 1993; Johannes, Wieringa et al. 1996; Yamaguchi, Yamagata et al. 2000), particularly in extrastriate ventral. visual stream (EVVS) components in post-consolidation subjects 35 (H.3-4: Knierim and Van Essen 1992b; Knierim and van Essen 1992a; Polat and Norcia 1998; Haxby, Gobbini et al. 2001; Lerner, Hendler, Ben-Bashat, Hare] and Malach 2001). lntemalization, a consolidation-driven shift in attention from external lexical and visual features to comparison of external features with stored representations of salient stimulus features (Tulving 1983) and a possible consequence of cortical reinstatement, was also predicted to be evinced by a decrease in activation volume as well as metabolic demand in striate visual cortex (V1) and EVVS components with consolidation. In contrast, “mnemonic” areas of the MTL are believed to function as a “linkage” Site between traces stored in isocortex, as discussed in the Regions of Interest chapter (see also Sakai and Miyashita 1991; Miyashita, Morita, Naya, Yoshida and Tomita 1998; Egorov, Hamam et al. 2002; Egorov, Heinemann and Muller 2002; Frank and Brown 2003; Naya, Yoshida et al. 2003a; Naya, Yoshida et al. 2003b; Clavagnier, Falchier et al. 2004). Thus, recruitment of the ectorhinal, perirhinal and entorhinal cortices was predicted to decrease while metabolic demand remained constant between pre- and post- consolidation time points. Intracellular competition among trace neurons over the consolidation interval should result in an amalgamation of, and diminution in, the number of these trace neurons (Alvarez and Squire 1994; Chrobak and Buzsaki l998b; Chrobak and Buzsaki I998a; lKida, Josselyn et al. 2002; Bozon, Davis et al. 2003; Bozon, Kelly et al. 2003). This diminution should be reflected in a decrease in recruitment of “mnemonic” medial temporal areas (ectorhinal, perirhinal, and perihippocampal cortices). Furthermore, retrieval orientation effects were assumed to depend on both visuospatial information (subserved by entorhinal cortex and anterior hippocampus) and visual recognition (subserved by the majority of the MTL and posterior hippocampus). 36 However, it is worthy of note that, as discussed in the introductory chapters of this dissertation, there exists as yet no clear consensus as to the computational operations of the individual MTL areas; instead, most research has focused on a “visuospatial” versus “mnemonic” functional categorization scheme (see, for example, Preston and Gabrieli 2002). Moreover, the simple DMTS task utilized in the present study was not designed to disambiguate these visuospatial and mnemonic firnctions. 37 HYPOTHESES In the present dissertation, mnemonic neurodynamics are investigated with respect to differences in behavior, with respect to differential static, or modular, brain activity, and with respect to differences in metabolic demands, between pre-consolidation and post- consolidation subjects. In particular, these differences were evaluated in two phases. In the first phase, conventional FMRI statistics — percent signal change fiom baseline and activation volume (task-correlated cortical recruitment) were applied in order to calculate changes in regional recruitment and the correlation between these changes and improvements in subject performance on a forced-choice delayed match-to-sample variant (paired-associate) task. In the second phase, two novel statistical assessments, the cross-correlation or covariance (VTCC) between stimulus presentation timing and subsequent BOLD responses and the BOLD signal complexity (the Shannon entropy of a voxel time series, or voxel TSE) — were employed to aid in the interpretation of the data returned by the conventional metrics. It should be noted that both behavioral and physiological results are interpreted in the present study in the absence of direct (e.g., molecular or cellular) evidence that memory consolidation occurred over the 7-day interval. In this context, a MATCH condition accuracy significantly greater than chance (i.e., a normalized accuracy Of 2 66%) was taken as evidence that consolidation had indeed occurred, Since (ostensibly) neither successfiil retrieval nor recognition effects can occur after intervals longer than 6 38 hours in the absence of stage IA protein synthesis (Abel and Lattal 2001.; Walker 2004; Walker and Stickgold 2004; Wang, Hu et al. 2006). The present study attempted to quantitatively describe differences in neurophysiology between pre- and post- stage I consolidation subject groups and correlate such differences, if present, with subject performance on the recall of previously learned (correctly-matched) paired associates, in order to test the working hypothesis that episodic memory consolidation is a process of optimization of the memory trace network for recall. More precisely, it was hypothesized that, based on recent evidence of consolidation-interval competition among neurons for inclusion in the final memory trace (Alvarez and Squire I994; Chrobak and Buzsaki 1998b; Chrobak and Buzsaki 1998a; Kida, Josselyn et al. 2002; Bozon, Davis et al. 2003; Bozon, Kelly et al. 2003), consolidation would effect decreased cortical recruitment in anatomical substrates of long-term memory storage and recall, but that neurons surviving competition would “compensate” computationally for their excluded counterparts. Thus, metabolic demands were expected to remain constant in mnemonic substrates (1.6., the medial temporal lobe) across pre- and post-consolidation subjects. Furthermore, consolidation was hypothesized to result in “attunement” of retrieval networks to recognition of previously- leamed stimuli, such that post-consolidation subjects would exhibit faster visual, mnemonic, and motor attention (i.e., premotor cortex) BOLD signal response rates relative to the presentation of previously-leamed associates, increased BOLD signal entropy, and better overall subject memory performance. 39 Thus, consolidation-related optimization was hypothesized, first, (H.1) to correlate positively with recall performance (corresponding to plasticity changes among memory traces), resulting in decreased subject response time and increased subject accuracy in identifying previously learned associates. Second (H. 2), anatomical substrates of pre- and post-consolidation memory storage and recall were hypothesized to exhibit equivalent metabolic demands, evinced by the BOLD signal magnitude, with decreased cortical recruitment, evinced by the spatial extent of task-correlated activation (activation volume), relative to the presentation of previously learned associates, in medial temporal (hippocampus, entorhinal, ectorhinal, and perirhinal) and attentional (premotor) ROIS, corresponding to greater activity at the level of the individual neuron. Extrastriate visual (VI-V3 and fusiform) ROIS, in contrast, were predicted to exhibit decreased metabolic demand and decreased cortical recruitment, corresponding to diminished activity at the level of the individual neuron as a consequence of retrieval orientation effects (see below). Third (H. 3), all substrates, with the exception of primary motor cortex, were expected to exhibit faster rates of change, evinced by faster voxel BOLD signal rates, as a correlate of cortical reinstatement, as discussed previously. And finally (H. 4), consolidation was hypothesized to drive changes in synaptic integration, thereby effecting differential sensory processing (retrieval orientation) of the learned associates, as evinced by right-hemisphere dominance in the ventral visual stream (Robb and Rugg 2002). These hypotheses are summarized in Table 3; related predictions are summarized in Table 4. 40 Table 3 Summary of the hypotheses for the present study. See Table 4: H. 1 Consolidation correlates positively with recall performance for RT previously learned associates due to plasticity changes among ACC memory traces. H. 2 Consolidation correlates negatively with task-oriented recruitment BOLD mag./ (i.e., activation volume) of recall substrates. However, post- Recruitment consolidation trace neurons are individually more active than pre- consolidation trace neurons, such that net metabolic demand remains constant over the consolidation interval, except in visual areas, which are less recruited overall at post-consolidation. H. 3 Consolidation correlates positively with BOLD signal response VTCC rates (cortical reinstatement) in mnemonic, visual, and motor attention substrates. H. 4 Consolidation correlates positively with metabolic demands in (N/A) right-hemisphere components of the ventral visual stream, including area V3 and the fusiform gyrus, corresponding to differential retrieval orientation. Table 4 Summary of the predictions for the present study with respect to subject performance (reaction time and accuracy), as well as physiological descriptions of the shape and temporal characteristics of the BOLD signal on the delayed match-to-sample portions of the paired associates task. Both behavioral and physiological findings are represented as being greater (+), less (—), or equal (no change, N/C) in post-consolidation subjects relative to pre-consolidation subjects. Abbreviations: HC, hippocampus, ERC, entorhinal cortex, EcRC, ectorhinal cortex, PRC, perirhinal cortex, V1, V2, V3, visual areas 1-3, F USI, fusiform gyrus, M1, primary motor cortex, PMC, premotor cortex; RT, subject reaction time, Acc, accuracy; BOLD mag, percent BOLD signal change from baseline, recruitment, cortical recruitment (activation volume), BOLD rate, BOLD signal response rate (evaluated by the voxel-task cross-correlation, VTCC). Hypothesis 4 (H4), which predicts a predominance of right hemisphere activation in post-consolidation subjects, is not included in the table. RTIH ACC/H BOLD RECRUITMENT/H 1 1 M AG. [H2 2 BOLD RATE/H3 Eric ”"3 — + EcR ”’0 " + c N/C — + PRC ”/0 _ * V1 -- + — N/C + V2 — — + V3 — — + FUSI _ .. + M1 N/C N/C N/C PMC N/C N/C + 41 All four of the above hypotheses (see Tables 3, 4) follow from the assumption that the phenomenon of consolidation is, in essence, a process of Optimization of the mnemonic net. However, it should be noted that activation volume, regional signal change from baseline, voxel TSE, and VTCC will only be assessed and discussed for brain activity that was significantly correlated with the presentation of correctly-matched stimulus pairs (matched associates) in order to minimize the effect of BOLD Signals corresponding to stable task-related sensory and lexical networks (i.e., responses to control stimuli) and task-related but generalized attentional, executive, and search-and-retrieval mechanisms (i. e., responses to mismatched stimulus pairs). Thus, the findings discussed in the present dissertation will be related exclusively to the recognition and processing of matched associates and the related memory traces putatively stored in the subject's cortex. 42 METHODOLOGY Stage I episodic memory consolidation was assessed using a three-phase training and testing paradigm. In the training phase, subjects were given 20 pairs of abstract pictures and animal words (tiger, bear, jellyfish, etc.) to memorize. These 20 pairs were repeated 10 times in order to stabilize the pairings in memory; subjects were then immediately tested on their retention using a computer-based testing script. During testing, subjects were presented with 100 total stimulus pairs (33 correctly matched words and pictures, 33 incorrectly matched words and pictures, and 34 control stimuli: scrambled pictures and backwards words) in a slow event-related paradigm. Only metrics concerned with correctly-matched stimuli will be assessed. in this dissertation. Subjects were then randomly assigned to one of two groups. Group A proceeded directly to the FMRI magnet and underwent the same testing paradigm encountered previously; Group B underwent the testing paradigm 7 days later. Thus, Group A represented a presumptive pre-consolidation cohort, whereas Group B represented a presumptive post-consolidation cohort (see Figure 4). SUBJECT POOL AND PARTICIPANTS Subjects for the present study were obtained from undergraduate and graduate students at Michigan State University and from the surrounding community. 93 subjects (43 female; mean age 19.45 yrs) were subjected to a computer-based (non—FMRI) version of the task, and 13 subjects (6 female, age = 26.08 i- 10.57 yrs) comprised the final testing (F MRI) 43 group. Of these, 6 (2 female) were assigned to the pro-consolidation group and 7 were assigned to the post-consolidation group (4 female). lnforrned consents were obtained in accordance with the Michigan State University Committee on Research Involving Human Subjects (UCRIHS). The study was also approved by UCRIHS (IRB #02-634). Demographically, the subject pool consisted of 1 Hispanic, 0 African-American, 1 Indo- European, 1 Asian, and 10 white subjects. Participants denied claustrophobia and significant neurological history, and were screened for handedness using the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory (Oldfield 1971). Only right-handed subjects were asked to participate in the study. PARADIGM Stimulus presentation scripts were designed, prepared, and tested using E-Studio software (Psychology Software Tools, Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania). Subjects were given a computer- 30 min. (50% of 4 subjects) L ‘ r 7 days (50% of subjects) A k ‘ bee jellyfish hsifyllej hsifyllei Figure 3 The training/testing paradigm for the present study. Stimuli consisted of 20 non- rehearsable, non-generalizable abstract pictures paired with animal nouns. Word Stimuli were presented either audibly (computer-based training/testing) or visually (FMRI training/testing). Following a training session, during which the correct pairings of stimuli were learned, subjects were tested on their recall either immediately (pre—consolidation time point; 50% of subjects) or 7 days after training (post-consolidation time point; 50% of subjects). For the purposes of testing, subjects were presented with 33 correctly- matched, 33 incorrectly-matched, and 34 control (scrambled) stimulus pairs, and subjects were required to identify each type correctly by button press. 44 based training session, during which they learned 20 pairs of presumably non- rehearsable, non-generalizable abstract pictures and simultaneously-presented animal nouns, such as rabbit, monkey, koala, jellyfish, and parrot. The 20 pairs were each repeated ten times to ensure thorough learning, stipulatively defined as a 66% or better accuracy on the MATCH condition (DMTS) task. Following the training session, 50% of the participants were randomly selected to return after a 30-minute interval for the testing session; the other 50% were asked to return in seven days for testing (see Figure 3). In either case, subjects were instructed not to think about the task, nor attempt to remember or rehearse the presented stimuli. The paradigm was tested with 93 subjects (43 female) using a computer-based version of the task. For these pilot subjects, the “animal words” were spoken by the computer simultaneously with the abstract picture presentation; thus, this version of the task was “dual modality.” However, technical difficulties in incorporating the audio system inside the MRI magnet necessitated the substitution of lexical stimuli for the auditory components for training as well as testing sessions for MRI subjects, such that the MRI version of the task was constituted wholly of visual stimuli (see Figure 4). Stimuli did not differ in any other respects. Differences in task performance between pilot and MR1 groups were evaluated and are discussed in the “Changes in BOLD Physiology with Consolidation” section in the Memory Consolidation and Retrieval Performance chapter. 45 FMRI Paradigm. With the exception of stimulus modality, training and testing were consistent across computer-based and FMRI groups. During the testing phase, (A) l jellyfish (B) -lH—l—I—l——I——H———H— II I II I llllll MM ISATC” II I II I lllll CONTROL +I—HHH1—H—H—l—H—l—Hl- Figure 4 (A) Example Of a paired associate as presented for the FMRI version of the training/testing paradigm: note that the word component of FMRI training and testing associates was presented below the abstract visual component. The centroid of the presented paired associate was positioned in the center of the subjects' field of view. (B) A schematic of a single run fi'om the FMRI testing paradigm: match, mismatch, and control-condition stimuli were presented with a constant interstimulus interval of 12.5 seconds. Presentation order was randomized across the three types of stimuli; x-axis represents TR (1 TR = 2.5 seconds). subjects were asked to determine, based on their training session, whether presented stimulus pairs were correctly or incorrectly matched, and to respond accordingly by button press (thumb for match, index for mismatch). Additionally, control stimuli, consisting of heavily pixelated versions of the testing stimuli and either backwards audio (CBT subjects) or scrambled letters (for FMRI subjects), were presented at random 46 intervals. Subjects were asked to respond to these control stimuli by pressing both response buttons simultaneously. MATCH, MISMATCH, and CONTROL condition stimuli were presented in random order3 with a constant interstimulus interval (181) of 12.5 seconds (Figure 4B). Subjects were given four runs of 25 stimuli and each stimulus was presented for one second, for a total run time of seven minutes in addition to 8 seconds of longitudinal magnetization equilibration time (total, 7 minutes 8 seconds). DATA ACQUISITION Behavioral measures acquisition. Correctly matched and mismatched paired associates and control stimulus pairs were presented to subjects within the MRI magnet, as described above, on an LCD display. Stimulus presentation was controlled, with millisecond accuracy, through an implementation of E-Prime and Integrated Functional Imaging Systems (IFIS) software (Psychology Software Tools, Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania). Subject response time and accuracy data were acquired through an electronic button response system tethered to the subject's right hand. MR acquisition. Anatomical and functional data acquisition was carried out on a GE 3 Tesla MRI magnet (General Electric Medical Systems, Wisconsin, United States) with an 8-channel head coil. Passive control of sagittal and lateral subject head motion was established by using foam supports and medical tape secured to the subject's 3 Random ordering of stimuli was performed by assigned a given stimulus an integer between 1 and 100 using a random seed generation script in the Linux Boume Again shell. 47 forehead, respectively. Single-shot gradient echo-planar image (EPI) functional slices (27.7 msec TE, 22 cm FOV, 3 mm slice thickness x 34 inferior-to-superior slices, 80° flip angle) were co-registered to high-resolution Tl-weighted sagittal anatomical volumes (5 msec TE, 24 cm FOV, 1.5 mm Slice thickness x 120 slices, £15.63 kHz bandwidth, 8° flip angle) for each subject. Anatomical and functional data was subjected to a 6- parameter (roll, pitch, yaw, and axial, sagittal, and coronal shift) iterated linear least squares motion correction algorithm using the programs ZdImReg and 3dvol reg, part of the AFNI FMRI analysis suite (NIMH), and also temporally realigned to correct for slice acquisition offset. A low-frequency bandstop was applied over the (-°°, 0.020] H2 interval to remove low-frequency artifacts in the data. A high-frequency bandstop was also applied on the [0.080,+°°) Hz interval in order to remove pulsatile cardiac (40-60 bpm) and respiratory-related (12-16 bpm) motion components. Although stimulus presentation order was randomized, stimuli of the same condition were presented at an average of 0.057 H2, well within the data pass range of (0.020, 0.080) Hz. The bandstop filters were not expected to affect the entropy calculation (described below) since entropy is primarily influenced by moderate-probability events in a signal (e.g.random—walk-type phenomena), rather than pulsatile components, which are higher-probability (more predictable) Signal components. BEHAVIORAL ANALYSIS Reaction time and behavioral data were analyzed with Minitab 14.0; performance statistics-related figures presented in the Results chapters were generated with SYSTAT 10.2 using the statistical results from Minitab. Inclusion criteria were established as 48 control stimulus response accuracy of at least 75%, in order to differentiate between high- and low-compliance subjects, and a paired-associates response accuracy (i.e., the mean of matched- and mismatched-associates accuracies) of at least 66%, in order to differentiate between relatively low-performers and moderately-high to high-performers. Low- performer data was not incorporated into the dataset for the present study; MRI data would also have been excluded for low-perforrners, however, none of the 13 MRI subjects exhibited data outside of the inclusion range. For the purposes of analysis, both behavioral and MRI data were categorized as responses to correctly-matched stimuli (MATCH condition), incorrectly-matched stimuli (MISMATCH condition), or control stimuli (CONTROL condition). Reaction times and accuracies were then calculated for each condition and are reported for each condition separately, but only MATCH condition performance will be explored further in the present study. MODULAR ANALYSIS Deconvolution In order to differentiate between BOLD/neural responses to MATCH, MISMATCH, and CONTROL stimuli, as well as responses to fixation crosses embedded in the paradigm, a deconvolution analysis was performed on the data using the AFNI program 3dDeconvolve. Deconvolution analysis de-overlaps contributors to the raw MR signal such that subject motion, arteriovenous signal, baseline trends, and task-related Signal components can be isolated and analysed as statistical parametric maps (SPMS) (see Ward 1998). 49 Mathematically, given a raw voxel BOLD Signal vector y(t), the “true haemodynamic response vector x(t), an Dirac delta function denoted 5(t), an a priori polynomial vector y, a Gaussian noise component 8, and k so-called orthogonal functions I//(t) , representing subject motion and signal drift components (the error term for the linear regression of signal components) respectively, it is assumed that the raw voxel BOLD signal is a linear composition, or convolution, of the Dirac delta fiinction, polynomial, and orthogonal vectors: y(t) =x® 7(t)®I//(t)+€(t)- The objective of deconvolution analysis, then, is to remove the motion, noise, and drift components by linear least-squares estimation of x(t). Thus, given Z(t)=Zy(t)5(t-r)=x(t)[1|r|5|W1+8(t), where 8 represents the Dirac delta function, the sum-square error, SSE, can be minimized, 2 r T T min SSE = Eli—(Z - x”, 2)] [Z - fix;— 2)]. dt x x x x Using 3dDeconvolve, voxels that activated in response to matched associates, but not mismatched associates, control stimuli, or fixation crosses, were isolated by a subtraction method to create a MATCH condition SPM. Motion components were also deconvolved against the raw signal, and correlation coefficients between the voxel response vectors and an ideal hemodynamic response waveform (composed by convolving the binary stimulus onset vector with an empirically-detennined hemodynamic response model waveform r(t): R.W. Cox, waver FA Q, 2005): 50 0for all {x, |—oo_ 0.23, corresponding to p _<_ 0.01 (corrected), were considered to be statistically significant. Activation volume (see also Ward 1998) Activation volume (AV) is defined as the number of voxels significantly correlated with a reference waveform representing a task of interest. Specifically, given a constant rectangular prism voxel volume L WH over N voxels in a deconvolved subject dataset that exhibits an F -statistic (following the variable naming conventions in the “Percent Signal Change” section): 53 = de+x[SSE(B) —SSE(B +x)] (de —de+x)[SSE(B + x)] F where SSE and df denote the residual sum of squares and degrees of freedom used in the deconvolution analysis, respectively, then A V a N(L . W . H) Activation volume has been utilized in FMRI studies of auditory and motor cortex recruitment, as well as comparisons of neurological patients and control subjects following cortical recovery from stroke (Cramer, Weisskoff, Schaechter, Nelles, Foley, Finklestein and Rosen 2002; Lasota, Ulrner, Firszt, Biswal, Daniels and Prost 2003); however, it has been cautioned that activation volume should not be interpreted in the absence of other measures of brain function, since it is susceptible to intersubject variability (Saad, Ropella et al. 2003). Therefore, AV was utilized in the present study only in conjunction with voxel—task cross-correlation, percent signal change, and voxel entropy (see below). Voxel-task cross-correlation (VTCC: see also Ward 1998) The voxel-to-task cross-correlation (VTCC) is defined as the Pearson product-moment correlation coefficient between a voxel BOLD signal time series and a reference waveform (defined above). Following the variable naming conventions fi'om the “Percent Signal Change” section, 54 [X(t) — ”x(t) :IZ [r(t) _flr(t)] JZ[X(t)-fl,(,)]22[r(t)-l1..(,)]2 p such that, when Signal drift (7) is accounted for, _x(t) TTr r _r(t) TTr r p: 3&0) 7 y 22;‘(t) 7T 7 .110) TTr 7 J0) TTr r x(t) 7T 7 Z r(t) 7T 7 Note that motion components are not considered in the above equation since they were removed from the raw signal during deconvolution (B.D. Ward, 3dfim+ Reference Manual, 2000). VTCC was computed for individual subjects' MATCH condition SPMS using the AFNI program 3df im+. For the purposes of establishing a baseline, signal drift (7) was assumed to be a first-order polynomial (i. e., a linear trend). Only VTCCS with an internal AFNI threshold of R _>_ 0.23, corresponding to p 5 0.01 (corrected), were considered to be statistically significant. Shannon entropy of the voxel time series (T SE) In contrast to metrics based on BOLD signal intensity, many estimates of the amount of information in a time series based on information theory (also known as 55 “communications theory”) make no assumptions about the shape, origin, or stationarity of the Signal under inspection, meaning that the conclusions based on such metrics are not biased due to underlying models of neurovascular coupling (Gonzalez Andino, Grave de Peralta Menendez, Thut, Spinelli, Blanke, Michel, Seeck and Landis 2000; de Araujo, Tedeschi, Santos, Elias, Neves and Baffa 2003). Of interest in the present study is an information theory-based metric known as the Shannon entropy of a time series (TSE), which was employed in order to quantify the “chaoticity” — that is, the disorder, or, more specifically, unstable aperiodicity (Shannon 1948) — of the BOLD response in discrete brain regions. This quantification of time series entropy is of interest in the present study because, by definition, the disorder of a time series is directly and positively related to the amount of information it carries, much as the instability in an audio signal is related to the complexity of the sound it represents (Shannon, 1948). Moreover, it has been suggested by electrophysiological studies that entropy is a direct aid to synchrony among cell populations since signal instability facilitates changes in neural firing rate oscillation (Fell, Fernandez and Elger 2003). In the case of a voxel or group of voxels, such as F MRI regions of interest (ROIS), the Shannon entropy may be related to “chaotic” coupling between neural activity and the BOLD Signal, or to chaos in the neural responses themselves, due to a surplus of neural connections prior to, for example, synaptic pruning. With respect to the former case, several instances exist in which neural activity and the BOLD signal become decoupled; for example, in patients with a history of stroke or vascular disorders, or subjects who have had caffeine or nicotine prior to their FMRI scan, or at very high and very low levels of neural activity (Logothetis, Pauls et al. 2001; Jacobsen, Gore, Skudlarski, 56 Lacadie, Jatlow and Krystal 2002; Laurienti, Field, Burdette, Maldjian, Yen and Moody 2002; Hamzei, Knab, Weiller and Rother 2003; Liu, Behzadi, Restom, Uludag, Lu, Buracas, Dubowitz and Buxton 2004; Krainik, Hund-Georgiadis, Zysset and von Cramon 2005). There is also some evidence that infants and toddlers have immature neurovascular coupling that may complicate interpretation of fMRI results as compared to adult fMRI data (Martin, Joeri, Loenneker, Ekatodrarrris, Vitacco, Hennig and Marcar 1999; Poldrack 2000). By age 8, however, the hemodynamic responses Of children and adults are comparable (Kang, Burgund, Lugar, Petersen and Schlaggar 2003). In healthy, compliant adults and in children over three years of age, therefore, neural activity and the BOLD signal are believed to be tightly coupled (Martin, Joeri et al. 1999; Logothetis 2002). For this reason, it was inferred that, in the majority of subjects, voxel Shannon time series entropy (voxel TSE) arises from disorder in the underlying neural activity, and, given that the BOLD signal arises primarily from metabolic events at the synapse (Logothetis, 2002), this disorder likely occurs at synaptic connections. The precise relationship between BOLD signal chaos and neural activity, however, is as yet unknown. It was surrnized that voxel TSE is directly correlated with the stability of synaptic integration, which is operationalized here as the ease with which multiple inputs undergo spatial or temporal summation at the synapse to create “coherent” output. Intuitively, spatial and temporal summation could be complicated by (a) an overabundance of inputs into a single output (morphological complexity), or (b) inefficiency of the connection at the synaptic junction (connectivistic complexity — due to, for example, a lack of Hebbian entrainment). This metric is therefore being applied in 57 the present study in order to determine whether TSE differs Significantly in particular ROIS between pre- and post-consolidation groups. The Shannon time series entropy (TSE) was calculated using the wentropy function in the MATLAB Wavelet Toolbox (The MathWorks, Inc., Natick, Massachusetts). AS implemented in the went ropy program, the Shannon entropy H(t)ROI, in Shannon bits (Sh), of the average time series across all voxels n meeting the threshold criterion (see above) in a region of interest was calculated as l n H(t)ROI E -;Zin210g(xi2), x=l i where xi represents a data point in the time series Of a voxel x (x 6 R01). The Shannon entropy of the voxel time series is calculated directly from the bandpass-filtered voxel response vector as described in de Araujo et al. (de Araujo, Tedeschi et al. 2003). The contribution of response rates to the entropy therefore has a Poisson distribution, such that very low- and high-probability responses contribute little to the overall TSE, and the entropy calculation is biased toward x, values that appear with moderate frequency. What this means conceptually, then, is that the voxel TSE is a quantification of how disordered a voxel response is, and consequently, it is also a measure of the amount of time- or frequency-encoded information that is potentially contained within the time series. With regard to the hypotheses outlined in the previous chapter, this means that the amount of disorder in voxel response can be used as an indirect measure of the amount of information carried in the BOLD response. 58 MEMORY CONSOLIDATION AND RETRIEVAL PERFORMANCE: EFFECTS OF STIMULUS MODALITY AND PAIR CONCORDANCE Experience and intuition suggest that episodic memory recall declines with the passage of time. Yet episodic memory networks undergo a period of consolidation, during which neuronal connections are assumed to be optimized - meaning that the mnemonic circuits involved in the memory are indexed, transferred to isocortex, and made permanent. In vivo as well as in vitro studies in humans and higher primates have confirmed that decreased memory and memory trace lability, increased intemeuronal firing coherence, decreased recruitment of memory-irrelevant (peripheral) brain areas, and heightened long-term depression (LTD) of firing coherence between these peripheral areas and mnemonic networks are hallmarks of episodic consolidation (Alvarez and Squire 1994; Eichenbaum I996; Eichenbaurrr, Dusek et al. 1996; Eichenbaum, Schoenbaum et al. 1996; Chrobak and Buzsaki l998b; Buchel, Coull et al. 1999; Abel and Lattal 2001; Eichenbaum 2001; Haist, Bowden Gore et al. 2001; Bodizs, Bekesy et al. 2002; Cantero, Atienza and Salas 20023; Cantero, Atienza et al. 2002b). The “consolidation as optimization” hypothesis is further supported by the striking changes in proximal and distal dendritic morphology that have been observed in a consolidation-concomitant long- term potention (LTP) context (Bailey, Chen et al. 1992; Bailey, Montarolo et al. 1992; Zhang, Endo et al. 1997; Verzi 2004; Verzi, Rheuben and Baer 2005). Such changes in morphology have been shown to modulate synaptic integration, thereby modifying the computational functions of the neuron (Shepherd and Brayton 1987; Holmes and Levy 1990; Zador, Koch et al. 1990; Yuste and Denk 1995; Shepherd 1996; Hausser, Spruston 59 et al. 2000; London and Hausser 2005; Segev 2006). It would therefore be reasonable to expect that, as a consequence of this modified neuronal computation, post—consolidation networks would be comprised of fewer neurons that nevertheless individually exhibit more complex firing rate patterns. This expectation could be substantiated or falsified by evaluating pre- and post-consolidation glycolytic magnitude, firing rate entropy and magnitude, and the number of neural populations recruited for recall. Conventional wisdom holds that, in Spite of this putative consolidation-related mnemonic network optimization, recall performance declines with time, except when the items to be encoded are repeatedly presented; yet research into the neurodynamic consequences of the consolidation phenomenon at the whole—brain level have only recently been undertaken, and many of the published studies on the subject have focused on the hippocampal, rather than cortical and medial-temporal, contributions to recall. The primary aim of the present FMRI study was to test the hypothesis that Optimization does indeed enhance retrieval efficiency, where an “enhanced retrieval efficiency” is defined, for the purposes of this study, as (a) improvements in behavioral performance — subject response time and accuracy — and (b) decreased overall metabolic demand. The working hypothesis was tested in 93 subjects on a computer- based bimodal paired associates (delayed match-to-sample or DMTS) task, and in 13 other subjects on an FMRI-based unimodal version of the same task. It Should be noted that both behavioral and physiological results are interpreted in the present study in the absence of direct (e. g, molecular or cellular) evidence that memory consolidation occurred over the 7-day interval. In this context, a MATCH 60 condition accuracy significantly greater than chance (i.e., a normalized accuracy Of 2 66%) was taken as evidence that consolidation had indeed occurred, Since (ostensibly) neither successful retrieval nor recognition effects can occur after intervals longer than 6 hours in the absence of stage IA protein synthesis (Abel and Lattal 2001; Walker 2004; Walker and Stickgold 2004; Wang, Hu et al. 2006) Please refer to the Methodology chapter for details of the data acquisition and analysis procedures. BEHAVIORAL RESULTS Subject performance, operationalized as reaction time and accuracy, were assessed with two ANOVAs. Whereas FMRI and CBT training sessions differed only in the modality of stimulus presentation, testing sessions differed with respect to both environment and presentation modality. Therefore, the first MANOVA tested the effect of paradigm type (FMRI versus computer-based testing, CBT) and time point (pre- versus post- consolidation) on reaction time and accuracy for previously learned (MATCH or delayed match to sample, DMTS) stimuli, mismatched stimuli, and control stimuli. Whereas MATCH and MISMATCH reaction times and accuracy were expected to differ between the two paradigm types due to attentional demands, CONTROL performance was predicted to remain constant across paradigm types and time points, and serve instead as a measure of subject compliance. Furthermore, it was predicted that accuracy would increase whereas reaction time would decrease as a function of time point but not paradigm type, consistent with the stated hypothesis that task performance would improve with memory consolidation. 6] 1500‘ Iii fr \ § ‘R\\\\\\\\\\\\\\ 1000‘ RESPONSE TIME (ms) 0.. 9.. Q Q 0. 9. 9. 9. 9. e To. 9’. O... o l l . . Time point Pre Post Pre Post Pre Post Pre Post Pre Post Pre POst Testing grp. CBT FMRI CBT FMRI CBT FMRI Match Mismatch Control Figure 5 Mean reaction times for the paired-associate task by testing group (computer- based, CBT, versus FMRI subjects), pair concordance (matched, mismatched, or control), and time point (pre- versus post-consolidation). FMRI subjects, who were given unimodal (visual) training and testing stimulus pairs, were slower at identifying both correctly-matched and mismatched stimulus pairs than CBT subjects, who were given dual—modal (visual and auditory) training and testing stimulus pairs. This effect was seen at both pre- and post-consolidation time points. However, neither FMRI nor CBT subjects were significantly faster at identifying correctly—matched stimulus pairs after one week. Neither group improved with respect to reaction time with consolidation for control stimuli, nor did CBT and FMRI subjects differ significantly with respect to reaction time for control stimuli at either time point. Error bars represent standard errors of the means. *p < .05. Reaction time (H. 1) Reaction time was defined as the response time, in msec, between the presentation of stimulus pairs and button press response to matched (MATCH reaction time), mismatched (MISMATCH reaction time), and control (CONTROL reaction time) stimulus presentations separately. A two-way (2x2) ANOVA using paradigm type (FMRI versus computer- 62 based training/testing, CBT) and time point (pre- versus post-consolidation) as independent factors indicated that neither F MRI nor CBT subjects were faster at identifying previously-learned (correctly-matched) stimulus pairs at post-consolidation than at pre-consolidation (F MRI pre-consolidation matched associate reaction time p. i 0' = 1701.8 i 120.4 msec, post-consolidation reaction time p. i O' = 1615.7 3: 207.3 msec; CBT pre-consolidation matched-associate reaction time u i 0' = 1288.9 :1: 174.8 msec, post-consolidation reaction time u i O' = 1201.4 i 112.6 msec; F(1, 54) = 2.22, MSE = 23399, p = 0.119), although pre- and post-consolidation FMRI subjects were slower at identifying correctly-matched associates than their CBT counterparts (F(1, 54) = 67.07, MSE = 23399, p < .001). Moreover, FMRI subjects were slower at identifying mismatched stimuli than were CBT subjects at both pre- and post-consolidation time points (FMRI pre-consolidation mismatched associate reaction time u i O = 1376.0 i 739.0 msec, post-consolidation reaction time u i 0' = 1599.6 i 253.2 msec; CBT pre- consolidation mismatched-associate reaction time p. i” 0' = 1315.6 i 182.3 msec, post- consolidation reaction time p. i 0' = 1262.3 i 164.2 msec; F(1, 54) = 4.87, MSE = 74470, p = 0.032), although there was no significant difference in reaction time for mismatched stimuli as a function of time (consolidation: F(1, 54) = 1.18, MSE = 74470, p = 0.314). The reaction time findings thus contradicted the predictions following from H. I. (See Figure 5 and Appendix A.) Accuracy (H. 1) Accuracy was defined as the number of correct identifications of matched (MATCH accuracy), mismatched (MISMATCH accuracy), and control (CONTROL accuracy) stimulus 63 presentations separately. Using a two—way 2x2 ANOVA, FMRI and computer-based training/testing (CBT) subjects were found to exhibit comparable accuracy on correctly- matched stimulus pairs (FMRI pre-consolidation accuracy 11 i o = 76.79 i- 9.13%, post- consolidation accuracy It i O = 71.29 i 15.35%; CBT pre-consolidation accuracy [.1 i O' = 73.74 i 11.79%, post-consolidation accuracy p. i O' = 79.06 1- 11.56%), indicating that neither FMRI nor CBT subjects differed with respect to the number of false negatives (F(1, 54) = 0.350, MSE = 143.80, p = 0.554), nor did the number of false negatives increase or decrease in either group as a function of time (F(2, 54) = 1.370, MSE = 143.8, p = 0.264). FMRI subjects were found to exhibit lower accuracies, however, for mismatched stimulus pairs (pre-consolidation accuracy 11 i 0' = 72.39 i 10.74%, post- consolidation accuracy u i 0' = 66.54 i 15.06%) than their CBT counterparts (pre- consolidation accuracy u i O' = 79.22 i 13.60%, post-consolidation accuracy It i o = 82.78 3: 13.30%), indicating that the number of false positives were higher in FMRI subjects than in CBT subjects (F(1, 54) = 6.74, MSE = 181.1, p = 0.012), although neither group exhibited a significant change in accuracy for mismatched stimulus pairs as a function of time (F(2, 54) = 0.65, MSE = 181.1, p = 0.526). These findings suggest that both F MRI and CBT subjects identified the correctly matched associates learned during training with statistically equal accuracy, but FMRI subjects were more likely to incorrectly identify mismatched stimuli as a previously learned paired associate (See Figure 6). Moreover, the putatively post-consolidation subjects in both CBT and FMRI groups identified the previously-learned paired associates as accurately as pre- consolidation subjects, but, conversely, subjects neither improved at correct rejections of 64 100 < %% 'V 1;: .0. . 904 80 4 r11 70- O o e To. v 0 3i? .0 O v 0 .0 Q .0... .'.' v 9:"? 0J5 90¢ 90%? , ‘ 66% o o .0? v v.3 v . 6.6 % ,...V O 9 92¢ .' c ’0; 60- 0 .y. 0 A ' 3 .9. ACCURACY (% Correct) EiEiEiE: 2:535 35 50. O O vrvvv .000 0 92020202320 RV??? rooooo flfifi£§b Pre Post Pre Post CBT FMRI CBT FMRI Match Mismatch Control 40 I I Time point Pre Post Pre Post Testing grp. FMRI Figure 6 Mean subject accuracy for the paired-associate task by testing group (computer- based, CBT, versus FMRI subjects), pair concordance (matched, mismatched, or control), and time point (pre- versus post—consolidation). FMRI subjects, who were given unimodal (visual) training and testing stimulus pairs, were more likely to identify mismatched associates as matched associates (exhibited more false positives) than CBT subjects, who were given dual-modal (visual and auditory) training and testing stimulus pairs. However, neither FMRI nor CBT subjects improved with respect to the number of false positives over the putative memory consolidation period, nor did either group improve with respect to accuracy with consolidation for matched associates (false negatives) or control stimuli. CBT and FMRI subjects did not differ significantly with respect to accuracy for correctly—matched associates or control stimuli at either time point. Symbols (light grey) indicate individual data points; error bars represent standard errors ofthe means. *p < .05; **p < 0.01. mismatched stimuli nor improved at accurately identifying learned associates (correctly- matched stimulus pairs) as a function of memory consolidation. An absence of statistically significant effects of time (consolidation: F(1, 54) = 1.43, MSE = 277.6, p = 0.238) or testing group (CBT versus FMRI: F(1, 54) = 1.03, MSE = 277.6, p = 0.316) on accuracy for control stimuli indicated equivalent subject task compliance in all four 65 conditions (FMRI pre-consolidation accuracy 11 i 0' = 97.50 i 4.07%, post-consolidation accuracy u i O’ = 94.79 i 11.13%; CBT pre-consolidation accuracy u i 0' = 86.13 i 24.01%, post-consolidation accuracy p. i O' = 93.72 i 9.40%). Thus, accuracy did not appear to reflect memory consolidation effects, contradicting H. 1 (see Figure 6). Thus, the multivariate ANOVA performed on CBT and F MRI subject response time and accuracy failed to confirm the stated hypothesis (H. 1) that the putative period of memory consolidation was positively correlated with subject recall performance, although the observed differences in recall performance between the CBT (bimodal) and FMRI (unimodal) paradigms may also indicate that such changes are sensitive to the number and type of modalities used to present the stimulus pairs to be learned. Interestingly, however, the conservation of subject response time and accuracy across pre- and post-consolidation subject groups suggests the possibility that access to memory traces was not reduced over the consolidation period, over and above a reduction in trace neurons as a consequence of the intraneuronal Hebbian competition that is believed to occur within hours of episodic memory encoding (Kida, Josselyn et al. 2002; Bozon, Davis et al. 2003; Bozon, Kelly et al. 2003). 66 Figure 7 Group statistical parametric maps (SPMS) of voxel BOLD signal correlation with presentation Of match, mismatch, and control-condition stimuli by region of interest. Voxel BOLD signal changes shown are significant at a fiIll-model F = 3.786 (p = 0.001, corrected); colors represent whether changes are positive (yellow) or negative (blue) relative to the baseline. Group SPMS were obtained by studentizing individual BOLD datasets and registering them to a common Talairach-Tourneaux coordinate system (Talairach and Toumeaux, 1988). Datasets were then subjected to cluster analysis using a minimal 5mm, 10p] Gaussian kernel (to correct for type 11 error: see Salmond, Ashburner, et a1. 2002; Geissler, Lanzenberger, et al. 2005) and averaged by condition. The location of the calcarine fissure (visual area V1) is shown on an axial slice in each case for reference (yellow boxes). Hemodynamic response functions for representative subjects were also obtained for ectorhinal area 36 (red box) and visual area V3/19 (green box) and are shown in Figure 9. Abbreviations: Fus, fiisiform gyrus; Ins, AIC, anterior insular complex; ST G, superior temporal gyrus; IPL, inferior parietal lobule; SPL, superior parietal lobule; DG, dentate gyrus; pul, pulvinar nucleus of the thalamus, Ver, cerebellar vermis, Cal, calcarine fissure. Numbers represent Brodmann areas: 3a, digit somatosensory area; 4, digit motor area; 31, posterior cingulate cortex. 67 3 786. p = 0.001 t-statistic at F 3.786, p = 0 001 I-sratistic at F .nuo boo 68 Figure 8 Estimated event-related average hemodynamic response functions (HRFS) for pre- (dotted line) and post- (solid line) consolidation right hemisphere ectorhinal cortex (EcRC, Brodmann area 36) and visual area V3 (Brodmann area 19) seed locations indicated in Figure 7. HRFS were obtained from the same studentized respresentative subject data as in Figure 7. Talairach-Tourneaux coordinates for each region are also given (see Talairach and Toumeaux, 1988). 69 NORMAUZED MRI SIGNAL INTENSITY (A.U.) NORMAUZED MRI SIGNAL INTENSITY (A.U.) Ectorhi nal Cortex: (41 . -33 . -6) 1430 1300 1200 1130 1000 800 TIME (TR: 1 TR = 2.5 sec) visual Area v3: (36.-83,7) 2103 1903 1700 1500 1300 1100 3 4 900 TIME (TR: 1 TR = 2.5 sec) - - Pre/M —-Post/M - - 'Pre/MM —Post/MM - - -Pre/C -—Post/C - - Pre/M —Post/M - - Pre/MM —Post/MM - - Pre/C —Post/C 70 CHANGES IN BOLD PHYSIOLOGY WITH CONSOLIDATION Initial three-way 2x11(2) ANOVAS were performed only on F MRI subject data to test the effects of the training-testing interval (“time point”: day 0 versus day 7), region of interest (ROI), and ROI hemisphere (left versus right) on various physiological measures — metabolic demand (percent BOLD signal change from baseline, PC), cortical recruitment (activation volume, AV), BOLD Signal rate (voxel-task cross- correlation,VTCC), and BOLD signal entropy (time series Shannon entropy, TSE) — collapsed across all ROIs, using hemisphere as a nested variable within ROI. Inspection of the three-way ANOVA results indicated that only the “time point” factor had had Significant effects; therefore, the effect of the putative consolidation period on regional metabolic demand, cortical recruitment, and BOLD response rates were subsequently assessed with a main-effect ANOVA, using time point as the independent factor. Physiological measures were collapsed across cerebral hemispheres and subjected to t- tests using “time point” as the grouping variable. (See also Appendix B.) Group and representative subject percent BOLD signal change SPMS Obtained from studentized data are shown for the x, y, and 2 planes crossing through ectorhinal area 36 in Figure 7, and estimated studentized hemodynamic response fiinctions from selected voxels fi‘om entorhinal cortex and area V3 are shown in Figure 8. The reader may refer to these figures to supplement the discussion of results in the following sections. R01 analysis of metabolic demand (H. 2) 71 The main-effect ANOVA indicated a significant effect of time point on regional metabolic demand over all ROIs (F(2,286) = 81.068, MSE = 2.003, p < 0.001). However, post-hoe analysis by Tamhane’s T2, applied due to heteroscedasticity between pre- and post-consolidation subjects (Levene’s test), failed to return any significant differences at % BOLD Signal A from Baseline 3.0. 2.5- 2.0‘ 1.51 1.0- 0.5 ' 0.0 - Time ct. ROI ? N > HC PRC V r2 T1T2 T1 T2 T1 T2 T1 T2 Ti T2 T1 T2 co > _— I Figure 9 Percent BOLD signal change from baseline (BOLD signal magnitude) in significantly task-correlated voxels constituting regions of interest at pre- and post- consolidation for matched associates. Analysis by main effect ANOVA indicated a significant difference in BOLD magnitude between pre- (T1) and post- (T2) consolidation subjects (p < 0.001) overall, however, post-hoc analysis by Tamhane T2 failed to indicate any differences in per-region signal change with consolidation. These findings suggest that the BOLD response magnitude is not affected by consolidation in significantly task-correlated voxels in these regions. Error bars indicate standard errors. 72 the level of the individual ROIs. These findings may therefore suggest that decreases in metabolic demand (pre-consolidation BOLD change = 1.145zt0. 123%, post-consolidation BOLD change = 0.993zlz0.114%) were only apparent when all regions were pooled, possibly due to the large per-ROI variance in the BOLD signal change data. The present data thus failed to indicate any changes in metabolic demand with consolidation despite the Observed decrease in per-region recruitment, contradicting some of the predictions following from H. 2 (Figure 9). Interestingly, the fusiform gyrus was found to exhibit significant differences between pre- (6532.292i538.623 uL) and post- (5340.595i498.668 ILL) consolidation activation volumes (3 difference of 18.24%, p 5 0.05) in the right hemisphere. The left- hemisphere fusiform gyrus did not differ significantly with respect to activation volume with respect to time point. R01 analysis of cortical recruitment (H. 2) The ANOVA also indicated a significant effect of time point on cortical recruitment in response to the presentation of previously-learned (matched) stimulus pairs (3885.042i114.835 ILL, post = 3148.1262t106.3l6 uL, an average decrease of 18.97%; F (2,286) = 1010.692, MSE = 1.76 X 109, p < 0.001). Post-hoe analysis by Tamhane’s T2, applied due to heteroscedasticity between pre- and post-consolidation subjects (Levene’s test), further indicated that post-consolidation subjects used a smaller cortical volume concomitant to recall than pre-consolidation subjects in the fusiform gyrus (pre = 6314.938i380.864 uL, post = 5408.875i352.611 uL, a 14.35% reduction in recruitment; 73 14000« 12000 « 10000‘ Activation Volume (pL) T2 T1 T2 T1 T2 9 9 T2 T1 T2 T1 T2 T1 T2 T1 T2 T1 T2 T1 T2 T1 T2 T ._- o :I: PRC V1 Figure 10 Activation volume of significantly task-correlated voxels at pre- and post- consolidation for matched associates. Analysis by main effect ANOVA indicated a significant difference in activation volume between pre- and post-consolidation subjects overall (p < 0.001). Post-hoc analysis by Tamhane T2 fiirther indicated significant reductions in cortical recruitment in the firsiform gyrus, premotor cortex, and visual areas V2 and V3. These findings suggest that the number of significantly task-correlated voxels is affected by consolidation in premotor areas, area V2, and ventral visual areas, but not in medial temporal, primary motor, or primary visual areas. Error bars indicate standard errors; *indicates a significant difference in activation volume, p < 0.05. ROI significances computed by post-hoe Tamhane T2. p 5 0.05), premotor cortex (pre = 11717.083:I:380.864 ILL, post = 96170421352611 uL, a 17.92% reduction; p 5 0.05), and visual areas V2 (pre = 8528.167fl80864 ILL, post = 6586.702i352.611 uL, 22.77% reduction; p 5 0.05) and V3 (pre = 8356.250:I:380.864 pL, post = 6791.256i352.611 uL, 18.73% reduction; p 5 0.05). (See Figure 10.) 74 ROI analysis of BOLD response rate (VTCC; H. 3) A main-effects ANOVA was also applied to the cross-correlation, or covariance, between the voxel BOLD response and a binary reference waveform representing the presentation of previously-learned (correctly-matched) stimulus pairs. This cross-correlation is denoted the VTCC for the present study, and represents the “goodness-of—fit” of the voxel response vector to the binary waveform, such that larger VTCC values correspond to a better fit between the two vectors, while smaller VTCC values correspond to a poorer fit. The VTCC therefore approximates the rate of the BOLD response relative to the presentation of correctly-matched stimuli, both in terms of time-to-peak (BOLD Signal rising phase) and peak-tO-baseline (falling phase). The main-effect ANOVA indicated a Significant difference in overall VTCC between pre- and post-consolidation subjects, such that post-consolidation VTCC (mean R = 0.330:t0.012) was greater than pre-consolidation VTCC (mean R = 0.475:l:0.011) when all ROIS were pooled (F(2,286) = 1365.942, MSE = 0.018, p < 0.001). VTCC data was homoscedastic (Levene’s test), and post-hoe analysis by the Tukey HSD test for unequal sample sizes confirmed that regional VTCCS were indeed greater in post- consolidation subjects than in pre-consolidation subjects by an average of 30% (p $0.05) in all ROIs studied, including primary motor cortex. These findings confirmed the prediction (H. 3) that mean regional BOLD signal response rates would increase significantly over the putative consolidation interval (Figure l 1). 75 1.0 0.8 ‘ 0.6 . 0.4 - 0.2 J Task-Voxel Vector Cross-Correlation (R) i 0.0 Time p1 : T1 T2 T1 T2 T1 T2 T1 T2 T1 T2 T1 T2 T1 T2 T1T2 T1 T2 T1 T2 T1 T2 [ROI ‘- 0 00 V --' O O O ‘— 5 o: “I ‘1’ B :I: 2 a: > 3‘ 9 qu 0 O u. o_ D. II LI LU uJ Figure 11 Mean voxel BOLD response-stirnulus presentation covariance (voxel-task cross-correlation, VTCC) in significantly task-correlated voxels constituting regions of interest at pre— (T1) and post- (T2) consolidation for matched associates. Analysis by main effect AN OVA indicated a significant difference in VTCC between pre- and post- consolidation subjects overall (p < 0.001), and post-hoe analysis by Tukey HSD revealed significant differences in VTCC in all areas (p < 0.05). These findings suggest that the BOLD-task covariance is affected by consolidation in significantly task-correlated voxels in medial temporal, motor, premotor, and visual regions. Error bars indicate standard errors; *indicates a significant difference in VTCC, p < 0.05. ROI significances computed by post-hoe Tukey HSD unequal-N test. Interestingly, primary motor cortex was observed to exhibit significant differences between pre- and post-consolidation VTCC in the right, but not left, hemisphere (right- hemisphere pre-consolidation Ml VTCC = 0.437i0.055, right-hemisphere post- consolidation M1 VTCC = 0.556i0.051, p 5 0.05). As this was the only unilateral effect in an otherwise globally-affected VTCC dataset, and subjects responded only with their right hand, this surprising finding may suggest that left hemisphere primary motor cortex 76 may be exempt from the purported effects of the consolidation interval, thereby acting as a “negative control” area as previously proposed. The involvement of right hemisphere M1 in right-handed tasks has been reported anecdotally in the F MRI literature, but there is as yet no clear consensus on its anatomical basis or its firnction. 77 RETRIEVAL ORIENTATION AND CORTICAL REINSTATEMENT: INTERPRETATION OF PHYSIOLOGICAL FINDINGS IN THE CONTEXT OF SIGNAL ENTROPY The above results suggest that episodic consolidation correlates with a generalized decrease in voxel recruitment in response to matched associates in premotor areas, area V2, and ventral visual areas, but not in medial temporal, primary motor, or primary visual areas, although the BOLD signal in all ROIS (with the exception of left-hemisphere Ml) exhibited a faster response rate with respect to matched stimulus presentation in post- consolidation subjects relative to pre-consolidation subjects. Furthermore, per-ROI BOLD Signal magnitudes in response to matched associates, corresponding to metabolic demand, did not appear to change significantly with consolidation in spite of this decreased recruitment. One interpretation of these findings (see H. 4) is that the instantiation of differential visual processing strategies with consolidation — particularly, covert shifts in attention from local visual stimulus features to more global features (“retrieval orientation”) — and facilitation of network reinstatement through consolidation-related increases in the synaptic strength among trace neurons (“cortical reinstatement”) enhance the ability of trace neurons and ancillary retrieval networks to exchange and process information through synaptic integration, such that fewer neurons are required for trace encoding and/or retrieval (Rugg and Wilding 2000; Lockhart 2002; Robb and Rugg 2002; Vaidya, Zhao et al. 2002; Mulligan and Lozito 2006). This hypothesis is testable in that (a) it predicts the engagement and disengagement of particular brain regions upon recall, which may be revealed through parametric manipulation of memory task parameters, and (b) it predicts that the neural networks involved in the sensory processing of learned 78 mnemonic stimuli will differ with respect to the volume of information carried by the neurons constituting those networks. The former prediction has been tested successfully with fiinctional MRI, but, surprisingly, the latter prediction has not (e.g., Rugg and Wilding 2000; Mulligan and Lozito 2006). In the present study, the regions involved in sensory processing of matched associates were the fusiform gyrus and visual areas V1, V2, and V3. However, BOLD signal entropy in area VI was not expected to differ significantly between time point groups due to the absence of corticocortical or corticothalamic afferents from attentional areas onto V1, which would ostensibly control differential sensory processing of presented associates (Rao 2005). Interestingly, the present data indicated that recruitment of the fusiform gyrus and visual areas V2 and V3, as well as premotor cortex — but not area V1 — differed substantially between pre- and post-consolidation subjects. Given that neither metabolic demand nor VTCC were observed to differ from region to region, it was Of interest in the present study to attempt to quantify the entropy of BOLD signals in ROIs that exhibited significantly reduced cortical recruitment as a firnction of the time point factor. A main-effect ANOVA on the Shannon entropy of the mean regional BOLD signals (time series entropy, TSE; see the Methodology chapter), a complexity metric, using time point as the independent factor, indicated a significant effect of time point on TSE overall (F(l,242) = 10.355, MSE < 0.001, p 5 0.001), such that regional post- consolidation BOLD signal entropy (see the Methodology chapter) was greater than pre- consolidation signal entropy overall (pre-consolidation TSE = 0.0322t0.001 Sh, post- 79 consolidation TSE = 0.034:I:0.001 Sh, a 5.88% increase). Post-hoe analysis by Tamhane T2, employed to compensate for heteroscedasticity in the TSE data, firrther indicated significant effects of time point on TSE at the ROI level in the fusiform gyrus (pre- consolidation TSE = 0.048:t0.002 Sh, post-consolidation TSE = 0.055:l:0.002 Sh, a 12.73% increase) and visual areas V2 (pre-consolidation TSE = 0.066zt0.002 Sh, post- consolidation TSE = 0.073:I:0.002 Sh, a 9.59% increase) and V3 (pre-consolidation TSE = 0.069i0.002 Sh, post-consolidation TSE = 0.073i0.002 Sh, a 5.48% increase), but not premotor cortex (pre-consolidation TSE = 0.103:t0.002 Sh, post-consolidation TSE = 0.104i0.002 Sh), which had also exhibited differential cortical recruitment between the time point groups. Further post-hoe analysis by Tamhane T2 indicated a significant difference between pre- and post-consolidation TSE in the right (pre = 0.0692t0.002 Sh, post = 0.0750002 Sh, +8.00%, p 5 0.05), but not left (pre = 0.068:t0.002, post = 0.072i0.002, +5.56%), hemisphere visual area V3. (See Figure 12.) 80 Thus, of the four ROIS —— fusiform gyrus, premotor cortex, and visual areas V2 and V3 —— that had previously been found to exhibit decreased cortical recruitment between pre- and post-consolidation subjects, three — fusiform gyrus, V2, and V3 — were also observed to exhibit increased signal entropy. However, these observations are likely to be mediated through a third explanatory variable, since premotor cortex, which exhibited differential activation volume as a function of time point, did not exhibit E (D ui‘ 0.10‘ In t: a 0008‘ * g a C m g 0.064 t C C (U .C (I) 0.041 E C .9 0’ 0.024 o _l O In 0.00- . Time pt_ r1r2 T1 r2 r1 T2 r1r2 T1 T2 T1 T2 T1 T2 r1r2 T1 T2 r172 r1 r2 ROI 0 o o ; gt ; I II E o. Fusi. ‘- O 2 n: o LIJ ERC-28 ERC-34 Figure 12 Mean BOLD signal Shannon entropy (TSE) in significantly task-correlated voxels constituting regions of interest at pre- and post-consolidation for matched associates. Analysis by main effect ANOVA indicated a significant difference in BOLD TSE between pre- and post-consolidation subjects overall (p 5 0.001). Post-hoe analysis by Tamhane T2 further indicated Significantly greater signal entropy in the firsiform gyrus and visual areas V2 and V3 (p < 0.01). These findings suggest that the BOLD signal entropy is affected by consolidation in significantly task-correlated voxels in extrastriate ventral visual areas, but not in medial temporal, premotor, or primary motor or visual cortex. Error bars indicate standard errors; *indicates a significant difference in TSE, p < 0.01. ROI significances computed by Tamhane T2. 81 differential signal entropy, whereas the converse was true for perirhinal cortex. It should be noted, however, that ROIs that are comparatively more active at the per-neuron level need not exhibit comparatively greater signal entropy. For example, the BOLD signal may increase in amplitude while maintaining its spectral characteristics; in this case, since no additional frequency components are introduced into the vector spectrum, its Shannon entropy will be equivalent. This could be the case with an attentional area such as the premotor COrtex, which might exhibit an increased post-consolidation spectal power concomitant to “driving” descending (attentional) input to, for example, visual cortex, but would not necessarily exhibit an increase in its time series entropy. It is also possible that one or both of these variables are modulated by different physiological effects. For example, premotor cortex, which appears to fimction as an attentional as well as motor planning area (Nobre, Sebestyen et al. 1997), and therefore a possible source of modulation of sensory processing, would only exhibit appreciable differences in firing rate entropy in the synaptodendritic compartments of visual cortices. As another example, perirhinal cortex, which functions in the maintenance and consolidation of pair codings (Eichenbaum, Schoenbaum et al. 1996; Nadel, Samsonovich et a1. 2000), may not be subject to the putatively consolidation-related cortical recruitment effects reported for premotor, visual, and ventral visual areas, since it likely serves as a “linkage site” between trace networks for paired stimuli. Moreover, the present data are not sufficient to make a conclusive case for the hypothesis that increases in BOLD signal entropy and decreases in cortical recruitment are directly related, or even that the entropy of an indirect measure of neural activity such as the BOLD signal corresponds to the entropy of neural spike train vectors. Therefore, although the present 82 fmdings are probative, more studies are needed both to reproduce a correlation between BOLD signal entropy and cortical recruitment with consolidation, and to directly test the correspondence between BOLD and electrophysiological entropy. If this link is established by future research, however, BOLD signal entropy might be utilized to draw inferences about changes in the nature and degree of neural activity by noninvasive means. Thus effects of both retrieval orientation and cortical reinstatement — two means by which episodic systems could become optimized with consolidation — may have been substantiated in the present dissertation. Retrieval orientation effects may be argued to have been evinced in that extrastriate visual areas were significantly less recruited, and their BOLD signals more complex, in post-consolidation subjects, phenomena that may be related to more “efficient” (e. g., holistic) stimulus processing and differential neuronal firing rate responses in post-consolidation subjects. However, the more robust finding predicted in the context of H. 4 — that right-hemisphere (global) ventral visual stream regions would exhibit higher metabolic demands in post-consolidation subjects, was not boume out. Potential novel correlates of cortical reinstatement were also observed, in that BOLD signal response rates (VTCC) increased globally; again, however, future studies are required to demonstrate that this effect is not due to, for example, angiogenesis over the consolidation interval. These findings are summarized in Table 5. 83 Table 5 A qualitative review of the results presented in the present chapter and the conclusions drawn from them. “Per-neuron activity” was determined by dividing the metabolic demand (PC) of an ROI by the extent of its recruitment (AV) to obtain a rudimentary AP/mm3 estimate. Abbreviations: N/C, no change; fusi., fusiform gyrus; Vx, visual area x. Demand Recruitment Entropy Resp. Rate Conclusion Medial- N/C N/C N/C increased - temporal Primary N/C N/C N/C increased visual Consolidation increases speed of recruitment only Primary N/C N/C N/C increased motor Premotor” N/C decreased N/C increased Consolidation correlates with higher per-neuron activity but not greater signal entropy V2, V3, fusi. N/C decreased increased increased Consolidation correlates with higher per-neuron activity and greater signal entropy *It should be noted that, although Brodmann area 6 is referred to in this table and the rest of the dissertation as premotor cortex or PMC to follow convention, it does in fact appear to be an attentional (specifically, “motor-attentional”) region. See the Regions of Interest chapter for the relevant discussion. 84 NEURODYNAMICS OF EPISODIC MEMORY CONSOLIDATION: CONCLUSIONS AND DISCUSSION In the introduction to the present dissertation, it was proposed that an interval of episodic memory consolidation may function as a temporal window during which memory traces are optimized and the memory retrieval process made more efficient. This “efficiency” was operationalized schematically, but not quantitatively, as an increase in, or conservation of, subject retrieval performance (usefiil output) — reaction time and accuracy — on a delayed-match-to-sample (DMTS) task and a decrease in regional metabolic intensity (energy expenditure), characterized by both regional BOLD magnitude and the degree of cortical recruitment, or the number of voxels significantly correlated with the DMTS task. By this stipulative defmition, increased efficiency over a consolidation interval could be argued to have occurred in any of several cases. Namely, the useful output-to-energy expenditure ratio used to illustrate the present work’s approach to retrieval efficiency would increase had (a) subject performance on the DMTS task improved without a corresponding decrease in metabolic demand or cortical recruitment; (b) metabolic demand and cortical recruitment decreased without a corresponding improvement in subject performance; (c) metabolic demand decreased without corresponding improvements in cortical recruitment or subject performance; or (d) cortical recruitment decreased while subject performance and cerebral metabolic demand was conserved. The results provided in the present work indicate that, over a seven day interval putatively corresponding to a Stage I consolidation period, retrieval efficiency did in fact improve as in case (d). That is, neither subject response 85 Emu 85 cosmiomno .8 88065 80: :+.. 2:. doumE—omaoo 3 Bacchus: on 9 333% .3025 3ch m> 88 obiflfio: «3 £8.33 833:8:0905 9 038—2 amen E 5388 0.8.: AXE haemogcmu 053 Emcwa Odom m> 88 82335: 98.33. 89385063 9 038—2 -58 E baa 3..an 3.585an oh? 822 38%“: 940mm :2 Bonn—mas: 3m: 85 2233030 an 8:865 20: :+.. 2:. dose—58:8 E Bacofiag on o. @28ng 5:8 88.: >3th obfiflfio: a2 5 88.. 8.8%“: :EwE Odom .3833 FEES—8:00-05 3 9:28 amon— E 3:38.. $2 3% 3 33355.6 33 mg .5863 Bosnian: 2w: 35 nous/.530 ca 88065 ob: :I: 2:. .cosauzogoo .3 Bacobacd on 9 333% macaw 88.38 obfiflfio: c2 05 .«o u5§_aoo.m _ 92 + + I 92 02 92 05¢ 92 5+ 92 92 92 92 92 :2 + + + El I WWZ I _ma A V+ + + I I Z I n> n+ + + | I 22 I 92 92 ~> 22 + + 92 I 22 I 9> 92 + + 92 I 02 92 um“. 22 + + 92 I 92 92 cam. 22 + + 92 | 92 22 emu 22 + + 22 I 92 92 o: .2506. umuofimhn. _m30< UQOEQQ $30.4. 8869a _m30< _m30< mm... m._.<¢ Odom ksztDmomm .0 :23363 Eofizenx: .858 .Emfizfomx 62:33 80¢ owcmno Rama 940m :8ch ..mos QQOM S2333 dew 6:5 8:88 803% Km 38:8 88an GEE .xotoo SSE basin <3 £33 8.5.53 55K .m-_ 32¢ :25; .m; .N; .S .528 3:553 438.33 838288905 8 0338 $8.33. comaE—oéooamoq 5 A92 .owcmso 05 :53 co .AIV 32 At 58on wEB 8 328858 0.8 mwfiwzm _mawo—ommxam 95 $8323 Eom .v—mfi 8368mm 3:8 2: mo £5an ogcmfornoafi Benn—ow 05 so 18% 940mm 2: no 8558820 .8888 28 33.0. 05 («o meosmtomoc Enema—063a mm :95 mm .3083on was 2:: E508: oocgotoa 80.33 8 80%»: 53> beam :5on of com €953»; mwfiwcm can Fuouavoisv mcocomooa 2: mo $885 w 935. 86 time nor subject accuracy, the numerator portion of the illustrative efficiency ratio, nor metabolic demand (BOLD signal magnitude), a measure of energy expenditure, differed significantly between putatively, pre- and post-consolidation subjects, whereas cortical recruitment (activation volume), did decrease significantly at the ROI level, particularly in premotor, lower visual, and ventral visual areas. It is therefore argued that retrieval efficiency did in fact increase, at least in these particular ROIs, between pre- and post- consolidation testing groups. Here, the results (Tables 5 and 6) and discussion portions of previous chapters will be elaborated, and their potential contributions to existing mnemonic theory will be discussed. EFFECTS OF STIMULUS MODALITY AND TESTING INTERVAL ON TASK PERFORMANCE Disparity between behavioral predictions and findings Upon analysis, the computer-based testing (CBT) group was found to differ significantly from the F MRI group in that FMRI subjects were slower at identifying correctly-matched and mismatched associates than their CBT counterparts, and exhibited more false positives than CBT subjects overall, but neither CBT nor FMRI subjects differed with respect to accuracy for previously learned associates at either time point. Neither group was faster or more accurate at identifying learned associates after consolidation, and neither group improved with respect to the number of false positives or false negatives with time. These differences were not likely due to differences in training or testing 87 environments or subject compliance, as both F MRI and CBT subjects were trained in the same environment and exhibited the same reaction times and accuracy for control stimuli at both time points. Instead, the performance differences could be ascribed to the requisite differences in attentional demands between testing groups: FMRI subjects, who were given lexical rather than auditory stimuli, were required to divide their visual attention between two simultaneously-presented stimuli that were wholly within the visual modality, in contrast to CBT subjects, who were required to divide their attention between two simultaneously-presented stimuli in two modalities. Such divided-attention effects have been well documented in both behavioral and neuroimaging literature, and it is generally agreed that bimodal attentional demands consistently have an effect on reaction time during encoding but not retrieval, unless the task stimuli being retrieved, such as simultaneous retrieval of different (“discordant”) lexical and auditory words, require access to the same representational areas of cortex (e.g., Grunwald, Boutros, Pezer, von Oertzen, Fernandez, Schaller and Elger 2003; Naveh—Benjamin, Kilb and Fisher 2006). In addition to these attentional effects, saccade times were likely to have a greater influence on FMRI subject reaction time than in CBT subjects, for whom saccade times were relatively minimized. Neither factor appeared to contribute to decreased accuracy for matched associates, however, as evinced by the absence of significant differences between the two groups. This is likely due to the indexing facility of the hippocampal formation, which appears to be independent of memory trace modality (Eichenbaum 2001; Ross and Eichenbaum 2006). More puzzling, however, was the observed consistency in reaction time accuracy across time points, contradicting the well-known Ebbinghaus retention 88 curve and Wickelgren power law, which predicts a power law relationship between memory performance and the delay between encoding and retrieval, such that retention declines with time (see, for example, Ebbinghaus 1913; Wickelgren 1974; Anderson and Schooler 1991; Rubin, Hinton and Wenzel 1999; Wixted 2004). Here, a possible explanation of this “non-Kamin” Ebbinghaus exception is presented based on the current literature in episodic reconsolidation. Theoretical reconsolidative basis for the non-Kamin Ebbinghaus exception Although improvements in memory performance —— the so-called Kamin effect, an exception to the monotonicity of the Ebbinghaus curve — have been demonstrated in rodents, the mollusk Sepia ofiicianalis and honeybees, these studies have been done, without exception, in the context of affective stimuli, such as avoidance responses, olfactory memory, and appetitive responses, and it is generally agreed that the Kamin effect is neuroendocrinologically mediated through the amygdalar body (Kamin 1957; Kamin 1963; Messenger 1971; Sanders and Barlow 1971; Woodruff and Kantor 1983; Rudy and Morledge 1994; Belcadi-Abbassi and Destrade 1995; Gerber and Menze12000; Rudy and Matus-Amat 2005). The present study, however, utilized stimuli that, being abstract, should have had neither affective influences nor rehearsability, eliminating the possibility of deep encoding. Thus a larger question remains: how could subjects maintain retrieval accuracy and comparable response or retrieval times on a paired-associates task with no emotional content? A possible answer may lie in the physiology of episodic consolidation. As discussed in the introductory chapters of this dissertation, episodic memory consolidation 89 appears to occur in two major stages (Chrobak and Buzsaki l998b; McGaugh 2000; Abel and Lattal 2001; Wang, Hu et al. 2006). In the first stage, memory traces are encoded in ' hippocampal area CA3 and interlinked through Hebbian processes over a period of hours to months to form metastable mnemonic networks that, in the second stage of consolidation, are gradually re-encoded into cortical, hippocampus-independent networks over a period of months to years (McClelland, McNaughton et al. 1995; Squire and Alvarez 1995; Ross and Eichenbaum 2006). Stage I consolidation, with which the present study is concerned, has been firrther subdivided into three phases. In Stage I phase 0, a short-term facilitation phase, recently- encoded memory traces are associated through a protein synthesis inhibitor-insensitive AMPAR-NMDAR-PKC pathway, which appears to last up to six hours (Abel and Lattal 2001; Walker and Stickgold 2004; Wang, Hu et al. 2006). In Stage I phase IA, a stabilization phase, the memory traces undergo protein synthesis-dependent long-term potentiation through a PKA-tPA-BDNF pathway (Abel and Lattal 2001; Walker and Stickgold 2004; Wang, Hu et al. 2006). Memory traces can remain in this state for 6-12 hours before proceeding to Stage IB, an enhancement phase which appears to be primarily if not solely dependent on sleep (Stickgold, Scott et al. 1999; Datta 2000; Gais and Born 2004; Walker and Stickgold 2004). The profile of protein synthesis shifts in Stage [B to include several plasticity-related proteins, such as TrkB and the glutamate- aspartate transporter GLAST, probably in preparation for increases in dendritic arborization and other structural changes that are the hallmarks of this phase (Cirelli and Tononi 2000b; Cirelli and Tononi 2000a; Abel and Latta12001). 90 Stages IA and IB are also characterized by competition among neurons constituting a memory trace through a calcium-dependent calmodulin/CREB pathway, such that neurons exhibiting a higher influx of calcium will upregulate transcription, thereby outcompeting neighboring cells for inclusion in the final memory trace (Kida, Josselyn et al. 2002; Bozon, Davis et al. 2003). Thus it is likely that trace networks are destabilized prior to the onset of cytostructural changes in consolidation phase IB due to factors modulating short-term facilitation, such as AMPA and NMDA receptor turnover and differential activity- dependent calcium influx at encoding, as well as cellular competition within the network in stage IA (Abel and Lattal 2001; Bozon, Davis et al. 2003). Consequently, the number of cells constituting a trace would be smaller at the onset of phase IB than immediately after encoding. The consistency of retrieval accuracy and retrieval time across time points in the present study therefore indicates persistence in trace network accessibility over and above this loss of trace neurons: that is, the trace is retrieved as easily after consolidation than after encoding despite a decrease in the number of neurons in the memory trace. One possible explanation for this persistence in trace accessibility is that the sum of synaptic weights remains approximately equal across pre- and post-consolidation time points, both within the trace network and between the trace network and other retrieval- associated brain areas, while the activation thresholds of the neurons constituting the trace network decrease. This conjecture would necessitate an increase in the synaptic weight, and decrease in the activation threshold, per trace neuron across the consolidation interval. These two parameters are not entirely independent, since the effective firing 91 rate, and therefore the probability of activation, of a trace neuron is a fitnction of both the presynaptic neuron's firing rate and synaptic weight and the activation threshold of the trace neuron. Extending this logic to a population of neurons, then, the sum synaptic weight of a trace network could be kept constant by an increase in the postsynaptic cell's firing rate through a decrease in the postsynaptic cell activation threshold, or by an increase in the presynaptic cell's firing rate through a decrease in the presynaptic cell activation threshold. Although this model is, at present, speculative, it would indicate that the trace retrieval time and accuracy could theoretically be maintained while also maintaining a relatively constant level of accessibility. Consequently, the neural populations involved in long-term memory storage and retrieval should both activate more quickly and produce a more complex signal after consolidation. These predictions were tested in the present study through measurements of the voxel BOLD signal/task cross-correlation (VTCC) and Shannon entropy of the voxel BOLD signal time series vector (TSE), respectively. Furthermore, the number of recruited voxels (activation volume) and degree of metabolic activity (percent BOLD signal change from baseline) were assessed in order to support or refiite the assumption that the number of neural structures required for trace retrieval decreased with consolidation. 92 INTERPRETATION OF METABOLIC DEMAND, RECRUITMENT, AND SIGNAL RESPONSE RATE Disparity between physiological predictions and findings As discussed in the previous chapter, the present results suggest that metabolic demand was conserved across all observed R015 and was not accompanied by a decrease in recruitment of the MTL, as was predicted. In fact, excluding increased BOLD signal response rates, hippocampal and perihippocampal areas, as well as primary visual and motor cortices, appeared to be unaflected across the conservation interval, even when signal entropy was taken into account. Moreover, components of the extrastriate ventral visual stream (EWS), which were predicted to demonstrate both decreased metabolic demand and decreased recruitment over the consolidation interval —thereby reflecting a diminution of per-neuron activity — instead exhibited no change in metabolic demand but significant reductions in recruitment (except the left hemisphere fiIsiform area, which was unaffected), indicating that EWS neurons participating in trace retrieval after the consolidation interval were individually more active than their pre-consolidation counterparts. BOLD signal entropy data further revealed that, of all ROIs inspected in the present study, only EWS areas exhibited an increase in entropy with consolidation. It must therefore be concluded that the consolidation-concomitant Hebbian changes in episodic retrieval networks (and particularly in the MTL) described in the literature are not necessarily reflected in changes in cortical recruitment, metabolic demand, or BOLD signal entropy, which may derive from cellular and/or network phenomena that are unrelated, or secondarily or more distantly related, to Hebbian effects. 93 In the introductory chapters of the present dissertation, it was hypothesized that areas comprising the MTL, which serve to associate salient features of encoded episodic stimuli, would also be affected by the proposed optimizing processes of consolidation. Intuitively, this would mean that changes in plasticity would drive the amalgamation of pre-consolidation medial temporal interneurons serving to link memory traces stored in isocortex, such that fewer interneurons are required for the linkage at post-consolidation. That the entorhinal, perirhinal, perihippocampal, and ectorhinal cortices and the hippocampus are both involved in and affected by stage I visual episodic memory consolidation has been reasonably well established: short-term glutamate receptor blockades, trauma, or neurotoxic lesions in the MTL disrupt visual long-term memory (Zola-Morgan, Squire, Amaral and Suzuki 1989; Meunier, Bachevalier, Mishkin and Murray 1993; Burwell, Bucci, Sanbom and Jutras 2004; Winters, Forwood, Cowell, Saksida and Bussey 2004; Winters and Bussey 20050). Because day 7 subject accuracy was significantly greater than chance — which would not be the case if stage IA protein synthesis had not taken place — it must also be assumed that at least stage IA consolidation, and therefore plasticity effects, had also occurred (Abel and Lattal 2001; Walker and Stickgold 2004; Wang, Hu et al. 2006). Therefore, if these physiological changes do in fact occur as hypothesized, it is possible that they may do so on a spatial scale much smaller than an EPI voxel, or may not be correlated with changes in BOLD signal features. In such cases, confirmation of the present hypotheses in the medial temporal lobe will require direct electrophysiological observation in vivo. Resolution issues, however, do not account for the absence of entropy effects in MTL areas. Therefore, if the theoretical reasoning underlying the entropy metric (see the 94 Methodology chapter, the “Retrieval Orientation and Cortical Reinstatement” section in the Memory Consolidation and Retrieval Performance chapter, and “Caveats” below) is correct, and BOLD signal entropy does in fact primarily reflect neuronal spike train information load, it must be concluded that these firing rates are no more complex after the consolidation interval than before, and, by deductive reasoning, the same (or very similar) computational operations must be taking place in the pre- and post-consolidation MTL. This conclusion contradicts the prediction that recruitment of, and demand from, the MTL areas would suggest increased (compensatory) per-neuron activity, as well as the underlying hypothesis that trace retrieval efficiency increases in the MTL over the consolidation interval. Thus the question arises: are current models of MTL function consistent with a position that the pre- and post-consolidation computational operations of the MTL are identical, in spite of well-documented changes in plasticity in the MTL with consolidation? As discussed previously, neither the processing functions nor the temporal course of involvement of particular medial temporal areas relative to memory consolidation or storage are well understood; however, tetanic stimulation of hippocampus and frontal (primary motor), entorhinal, and perirhinal cortices in vivo indicates that these areas are not only plastic but also capable of effecting, and being affected by, long-term potentiation (LTP) via neuronal afferents and efferents (Ivanco and Racine 2000). Moreover, it has been proposed that the characteristic latency between direct stimulation and steady-state LTP in each of these areas may reflect their hierarchical (yet symmetrical) or time-sensitive fiInctions. For example, the hippocampus, which potentiates quickly and sensitively to novel stimuli, may serve to stabilize memory traces 95 for the duration of stage I, and most of stage II, consolidation; isocortex, which potentiates slowly but exhibits enduring LTP once it is effected, likely serves as the long- terrn storage site for memory traces. In this view, components of the parahippocampal region, which exhibit plasticity latencies and stabilities intermediate between those of the hippocampus and isocortex, may perform their computational functions on a temporal interval between the effective periods of the hippocampus (stage I consolidation) and isocortex (stage II consolidation) (McClelland, McNaughton et al. 1995; Ivanco and Racine 2000; Eichenbaum 2001). However, parahippocampal activation is frequently reported during all phases of memory tasks (Eichenbaum, Schoenbaum et al. 1996; Gabrieli, Brewer and Poldrack I998; Lavenex and Amaral 2000; Nadel and Land 2000; Egorov, Hamam et al. 2002; Frank and Brown 2003; Naya, Yoshida et al. 2003a; Naya, Yoshida et al. 2003b; Clavagnier, Falchier et al. 2004). Furthermore, lidocaine inactivation of rodent perirhinal cortex, localized to Brodmann areas 35 and 36, has been shown to preclude performance on DNMS tasks when administered over intervals corresponding to episodic memory encoding, consolidation, and retrieval, which suggests that parahippocampal areas are vital to the functions of the episodic system in all three phases of the memory trace lifetime (Tassoni, Lorenzini, Baldi, Sacchetti and Bucherelli 1999; Winters and Bussey 2005b; Winters and Bussey 2005a; Winters and Bussey 2005c). Theories that address the problem of computational dissociations among parahippocampal areas by positing phasic or temporally hierarchical functions must therefore do so in light of the seemingly constant involvement of these areas in the trace network. 96 Despite the current controversy regarding the temporal upper bound of medial temporal involvement in trace retrieval, most theories of MTL function in declarative memory concur with respect to its involvement in trace storage and conclude, with some degree of consensus,4 that both the physiological characteristics of the medial temporal structures and the protracted nature of consolidation itself subserve the merging of the memory trace into the myriad of other isocortical memory traces, while simultaneously precluding, upon trace retrieval, a debilitating cascade of memory recall precipitated by the overlapping representations of similar, but non-identical, experiences known as catastrophic hypermnesia (McNaughton and Wickens 2003; Tse, Langston, Kakeyama, Bethus, Spooner, Wood, Witter and Morris 2007). In this context, then, the ostensible role of the parahippocampal region is to modulate information transfer to the hippocampus based on the novelty of the information, such that novel and salient stimulus features, but not features similar to those found in previously-encoded traces, will elicit hippocampal activation (see, for example, Fernandez and Tendolkar 2006). This theoretical function of the parahippocampal region is in line with behavioral, electrophysiological and neuroimaging data on correlates of repetition suppression, recognition, and recency effects in the MTL (Henson and Rugg 2003; Weis, Klaver, Reul, Elger and Fernandez 2004; Gonsalves, Kahn, Curran, Norman and Wagner 2005; 4 The most conspicuous exception to this consensus is the multiple memory trace theory propounded by Lynn Nadel. Nadel’s theory maintains that trace retrieval never becomes completely independent of the hippocampal formation and, therefore, the MTL is always required for trace recall. It could be argued, however, that the hippocampal synaptic weight upon trace retrieval approaches, but would never physiologically attain, zero; by this argument, the upper bound of MTL involvement proposed by Nadel (infinity) and that proposed by other theorists (possibly, the lifetime of the organism) may differ mathematically, but in practical terms, the upper bound will always be the lifetime of the organism. Cf, for example, Moscovitch Moscovitch, M., L. Nadel, G. Winocur, A. Gilboa and R S. Rosenbaum (2006). The cognitive neuroscience of remote episodic, semantic and spatial memory. Current opinion in neurobiology 16(2): 179-90. and Nadel Nadel, L., A. Samsonovich, L. Ryan and M. Moscovitch (2000). Multiple trace theory of human memory. computational, neuroimaging, and neuropsychological results. Hippocampus 10(4): 352—68.. 97 Montaldi, Spencer, Roberts and Mayes 2006). Moreover, it is 'of particular relevance to the present discussion because it minimizes the distinction between encoding- and retrieval-concomitant roles for the parahippocampal areas and emphasizes instead a so- called gatekeeper fiInction, by which neurons engaged in encoding are potentiated toward novel stimuli (Fernandez and Tendolkar 2006). The gatekeeper theory thus renders computational distinctions among the various MTL components (where such distinctions cannot be ascribed to the sensory specificity or aspecificity of their projections: cf. Kerr, Agster et al. 2007) less impedimentary and sustains the contention that the parahippocampal areas might retain uniform computational firnctions over and above changes in plasticity. EWS areas, with the exceptions of left hemisphere visual area V3, which exhibited greater putative per-neuron activity but not greater signal entropy, and left hemisphere fusiform gyrus, which exhibited greater signal entropy but no change in metabolic demand or recruitment, were observed to exhibit greater per-neuron activity and BOLD signal entropy at the post-consolidation time point. Based on the theory proposed in this dissertation (cf. “Caveats” below), this finding would indicate that —- in contrast to predicted decreases in per-neuron activity in the extrastriate ventral visual stream (EWS) consequent to differential retrieval orientation with consolidation — post- consolidation EVVS neurons exhibit more complex firing rates, a possible reflection of greater information load, than their pre-consolidation counterparts. Interestingly, primary visual cortex (visual area V1) appeared to be unaffected by consolidation (except for possible correlates of cortical reinstatement, described below), a finding which may indicate that the consolidation-correlated effects observed in the other visual ROIs may 98 be attentionally modulated (see the Regions of Interest chapter). Although this hypothesis remains speculative pending direct measurement of neuronal firing rate entropy during a DMTS paradigm, it is interesting to note that premotor cortex — an attentional area — exhibited greater per-neuron activity but, like parahippocampal areas, conserved entropy (and, per the discussion above, conserved computational functions) at post-consolidation. These characteristics are consistent with a hypothetical attentional region that modulates retrieval orientation in post-, but not pre-consolidation subjects. Thus, the first component of the hypothesis stated above — that the neural populations involved in long-term memory storage and retrieval should activate more quickly following consolidation — may hold true for gatekeeper, ecphoric, and trace networks, whereas the second component —— that neural populations involved in storage and retrieval should produce a more complex signal after consolidation — may hold true only for the trace networks themselves, that is, the EWS. Although this model has not yet, to the knowledge of the author, been proposed in the literature, it is highly testable by fiIture DMTS studies that directly examine firing rate entropy. The present model predicts that previously-learned visual DMTS stimuli will elicit greater post- consolidation than pre-consolidation firing rate entropy in visual cortices, where the traces are stored, but not in attentional or executive cortices, which subserve ecphory, or medial temporal structures, which appear only to link trace nodes and encode stimulus novelty. Furthermore, entropy effects should not occur in primary visual cortex. In contrast, DNMS stimuli should elicit greater firing rate entropy in medial temporal, and perhaps visual, but not attentional or executive, cortices. It would also be informative to 99 perform these future experiments in an FMRI context in order to attempt the replication of metabolic demand findings. Caveats of the entropy metric It should be noted that the BOLD signal has been shown to be primarily determined by the magnitude of perivascular oxidative glycolysis as a result of cooperative, or synchronized, somatodendritic potentials (Boynton, Engel, Glover and Heeger 1996; Logothetis 2002; Buxton, Uludag, Dubowitz and Liu 2004; Behzadi and Liu 2005), and appears to be more closely associated with energy demand concomitant to neurotransmitter release than with perikaryal metabolic demands (Logothetis 2002; Logothetis 2003; Logothetis and Pfeuffer 2004; Logothetis and Wandell 2004). The persistence with consolidation of BOLD signal magnitude within the observed ROIs would therefore suggest that the degree of perisynaptic activity, including pre- and post- synaptic currents, neurotransmitter release, and, consequently, neural firing rates, remained approximately constant across pre- and post-consolidation subjects. It could therefore plausibly be proposed that firing rates increased, relative to the post-encoding state, in the recall-associated neural populations of the post-consolidation EWS, and, consequently, BOLD signal entropy would be expected to increase with consolidation within these areas. Again, the hypothesis that firing rates increased in recall areas with consolidation was tested by computation of the Shannon, or information-theoretic, entropy of the BOLD response from each Brodmann area. This voxel BOLD signal time series Shannon entropy (TSE) was found to increase in three of the four regions that exhibited decreased 100 cortical recruitment between the two subject groups, indicating that the BOLD signal per Brodmann area was more complex in still-participatory neural populations after the consolidation interval than in the neural populations recruited for recall immediately after encoding in those Brodmann areas. Although this entropy arguably arises fiom increased neural firing rates, the TSE results could also be the result of nonlinearities in neurovascular coupling or due to other physiological variables, such as cytogeometry, as discussed in the Methodology chapter. However, as was argued previously, in healthy, compliant adults and in children over three years of age, neural activity and the BOLD signal are believed to be somewhat linearly coupled (Martin, Joeri et al. 1999; Logothetis 2002) and it was inferred that, in the majority of subjects, voxel TSE arises from entropy in neurotransmitter release in the perisynaptic compartment (Logothetis 2002). The precise relationship between BOLD signal chaos and neural activity, however, is as yet unknown. Thus, while the findings of the present study are probative, they must be interpreted with caution, since more investigation is indicated as to the nature and origin of the entropy in the BOLD signal that the voxel TSE represents. THEORY: CORTICAL REINSTATEMENT AND RETRIEVAL ORIENTATION An implicit assumption underlying the generation of the hypotheses of the present dissertation was that subjects would exhibit non-conscious (covert) shifts in attention from local to global presented visual stimulus features after the consolidation interval, a phenomenon called retrieval orientation (Rugg and Wilding 2000; Robb and Rugg 2002). In the Aims and Hypotheses chapter, it was proposed that changes in retrieval orientation would be evinced by a shift fiom bilateral visual cortex activation in both 101 dorsal and ventral visual streams at pre-consolidation to right-hemisphere dominance in the ventral visual stream at post-consolidation (H. 4). This prediction follows fiom divergent activation profiles observed in functional MRI studies in which mnemonic demands were parametrically manipulated (Herron and Rugg 2003a; Homberger, Morcom et al. 2004; Herron and Wilding 2006a; Herron and Wilding 2006b; Homberger, Rugg et al. 2006b; Homberger, Rugg and Henson 2006a; Stenberg, Johansson et al. 2006; Woodruff, Uncapher et al. 2006). However, in contrast to those studies, which utilized the conventional BOLD magnitude metric, no diflerences in signal intensity were observed in the ventral visual stream in the present study, contradicting the predictions following from H. 4. Interestingly, however, increases in BOLD signal entropy, which may correspond to the complexity of neural firing rates, were observed in visual area V2, as well as area V3 and the fusiform gyrus (V4), two components of the ventral visual stream. This observation was particularly surprising in area V3, in that right, but not left, hemisphere V3 differed significantly with respect to signal entropy as a function of time point: area V3 is considered the first region that comprises the ventral visual stream (Ungerleider and Mishkin 1982), and, fiIrthermore, the right hemisphere ventral visual stream is believed to function in the processing of global visual stimulus features (Knierim and Van Essen 1992b; Knierim and van Essen 1992a; Brown and Kosslyn 1993; Johannes, Wieringa et al. 1996; Polat and Norcia 1998; Yamaguchi, Yamagata et al. 2000; Haxby, Gobbini et a1. 2001; Lerner, Hendler et al. 2001). Area V2 and the fiIsiform gyrus, visual area V4, are also known to be heavily interconnected (Albright 1993), and, although V2 is not considered to be a ventral visual component, the finding that both V2 and V4 102 exhibited greater entropy in post-consolidation subjects may indicate that increases in BOLD signal entropy may indeed correspond to a ventral visual network effect. Notable, too, is the absence of a significant effect of time point on either recruitment or signal entropy in area Vl, since, as discussed previously, V1 activity has not been shown to be attentionally modulated, and it is the attentional network that would likely modulate differential retrieval effects with consolidation, if they exist. Admittedly, however, the present data are insufficient to substantiate the previously stated hypothesis that retrieval orientation effects occurred in the DMTS task; yet these findings may nevertheless be seen as seminal in that differential sensory cortex effects were observed in domains other than the conventional BOLD magnitude statistic. Possible cortical reinstatement effects A second assumption in the dissertation was that post-consolidation subjects would exhibit faster reactivation of retrieval networks, and therefore BOLD response rates relative to the presentation of previously-learned stimuli, as a consequence of increased synaptic weighting after the consolidation interval. This effect has been referred to as ‘transfer-appropriate processing” or cortical reinstatement (Lockhart 2002; Vaidya, Zhao et al. 2002; Mulligan and Lozito 2006). BOLD response rates (VTCC) were in fact greater in post-consolidation subjects in all observed ROIs with the exception of left hemisphere primary motor cortex, which did not differ with respect to VTCC as a function of time point. Thus, the present data did substantiate the hypothesized time point effect on network reinstatement. 103 Notably, regional BOLD signals in all Brodmann areas except left hemisphere primary motor cortex were also found to exhibit a significant increase in covariance (voxel-task cross-correlation, VTCC) with a reference vector representing the temporal presentation of previously learned (matched) stimuli with consolidation, meaning that both the rising and falling phases of the BOLD response occurred at a faster rate relative to the onset and offset of correctly-matched stimulus presentation in post-consolidation subjects. This result could feasibly be interpreted as a consolidation-related decrease in the activation threshold of the neurons constituting the voxels of interest, such that the roughly linear relationship between neural activity and the hemodynamic response remained unchanged with consolidation but increased presynaptic weighting or decreased postsynaptic neuron activation thresholds facilitated a faster response onset and offset in the neural activity underlying the BOLD signal. In this case, the altered BOLD signal trajectory could reflect heightened efficiency of synaptic integration, such that neural populations were activated more quickly (lower neuronal time constants facilitate higher presynaptic firing rates or vice versa) and maintained activity for shorter durations. Alternatively, however, a change in the metabolic components of neurovascular coupling, such as increased receptor or ion channel expression, neurogenesis, differential enzyme kinetics, and alterations to the pathways of signal transduction or time course of the relevant neurochemical cascades, could also explain the accelerated trajectory of the hemodynamic response. Additionally, although the positive VTCC-TSE relationship appears to be preserved in visual areas V2 and V3 and the fusiform gyrus, it should also be noted that this relationship was not preserved in perirhinal or premotor cortex. If, therefore, the TSE and VTCC metrics do in fact correspond to neuronal spike train 104 complexity and synaptic efficacy respectively, the conclusion must also be drawn that one does not necessarily predict the other, and may in fact be mediated through a third explanatory variable.CLOSlNG REMARKS The present study yielded probative, albeit preliminary, neurodynamics data that have not yet been reported in prior FMRI-based studies, particularly in the context of episodic memory consolidation. Notably, unique physiological profiles of recruitment, metabolic demand, BOLD signal rates, and BOLD signal entropy were reported that suggest that post-consolidation trace recall and retrieval support networks are characterized by increased trace retrieval efficiency at the regional (rather than behavioral) level, and differential sensory processing, which may reflect changes in synaptic integration both within the episodic memory system and supporting networks. The reader should be cautioned, however, that two of the methods used in the present dissertation — BOLD signal entropy and VTC C -— have not been replicated or interpreted by other researchers, and their reliability must be established by repeated investigations in the fiIture. It is hoped that the present study will be utilized as groundwork for firture neurodynamics research, especially with respect to neuroimaging investigations. 105 APPENDICES 106 APPENDIX A: ANOVA TABLES FOR BEHAVIORAL DATA A two-way 2x2 ANOVA was utilized to test for the effect of computer-based (CBT) versus FMRI testing scenarios and time point (immediate versus day-7 testing), the independent factors, on task performance (reaction time and accuracy, the dependent variables). Abbreviations: m, match; mm, mismatch; c, control; Acc, Accuracy; RT, reaction time; FMRI, FMRI/unimodal testing group; CBT, computer-based (pilot/bimodal) testing group. Analysis of Variance for match-condition accuracy Adjusted SS for Tests Source DF Seq ss Adj ss Adj MS F P FMRI v CBT 1 77.8 51.0 51.0 0.35 0.554 TimePointCFMRI v CBT) 2 393.0 393.0 196.5 1.37 0.264 Error 51 7334.3 7334.3 143.8 Total 54 7805.1 s = 11.9921 R-Sq = 6.03% R-SqCadj) = 0.56% Analysis of Variance for mismatch-condition accuracy Adjusted SS for Tests Source DF Seq SS Ad' SS Ad' MS F P FMRI v CBT 1 1366.0 12 1.5 12 1.5 6.74 0.012 TimePointCFMRI v CBT) 2 235.9 235.9 118.0 0.65 0.526 Error 51 9236.4 9236.4 181.1 Total 54 10838.3 9.77% Analysis of Variance for control-condition accuracy Adjusted SS for Tests S = 13.4576 R-Sq = 14.78% R-SqCadj) Source DF Seq SS Adj SS Adj MS F P FMRI v CBT 1 327.1 354.8 354.8 1.28 0.264 TimePointCFMRI v CBT) 2 639.2 639.2 319.6 1.15 0.325 Error 51 14190.8 14190.8 278.3 Total 54 15157.0 5 = . 08 R-Sq = 6.37%’ R-Sq(adj) = 0.87% Analysis of Variance for match-condition response time Adjusted SS for Tests Source DF Seq SS Adj SS Adj MS F P FMRI v CBT 1 1557152 1569349 1569349 67.07 0.000 TimePointCFMRI v CBT) 2 103726 103726 51863 2.22 0.119 Error 51 1193331 1193331 23399 Total 54 2854209 s = 152.966 R-Sq = 58719% R-Sq(adj) = 55.75% 107 Analysis of Variance for mismatch-condition response time Adjusted SS for Tests Source DF Seq SS Adj SS Ad' MS F P FMRI v CBT 1 446193 36 692 36 692 4.87 0.032 TimePointCFMRI v CBT) 2 176495 176495 88247 1.18 0.314 Error 51 3797985 3797985 74470 Total 54 4420672 s = 272.892 R-Sq = 14.09% R-SqCadj) = 9.03% Analysis of Variance for control-condition response time Adjusted SS for Tests Source DF Seq SS Ad' SS Ad' MS F P FMRI v CBT 1 37284 2 640 2 640 0.23 0.631 TimePointCFMRI v CBT) 2 190481 190481 95240 0.75 0.478 Error 51 6479807 6479807 127055 Total 54 6707572 5 = 356.448 R-Sq = 3.40% R-Sq(adj) = 0.00% 108 APPENDIX B: MAIN-EF F ECT ANOVA AND POST-HOC RESULTS FOR PHYSIOLOGICAL STATISTICS Initial three-way 2x1 1x2 AN OVAs were performed on F MR1 subject data only to test the effects of the training-testing interval (“testing group”: day 0 versus day 7), region of interest (ROI), and ROI hemisphere (lefi versus right) on various physiological measures — metabolic demand (percent BOLD signal change from baseline, PC), cortical recruitment (activation volume, AV), BOLD signal rate (voxel-task cross-correlation, VTCC), and BOLD signal entropy (time series Shannon entropy, TSE) — collapsed across all ROIs. Inspection of the three-way ANOVA results indicated that only the “testing group” factor had had significant effects; therefore, physiological measures were collapsed across cerebral hemispheres and subjected to t-tests using “testing group” as the grouping variable. " TSE Testing group ' ROI Hemi. 1 Testing group -1: ._1_1,_______ _,, a- I Pre-Con. Results of the initial 2x11x2 three-way ANOVA, the main-effect ANOVA on “testing group,” and the post-hoe t-tests are given below. Between-Subjects Factors Value Label N I 1 Pre-Con. W _ * *———'3_2I 2 Post-Con. _ _ ‘_ 1541' 1 'Ml _ 26] 2_ MC , __ 391' 3 ERC-28 _ _ _ _26I 4 ERC-34 26I '5 Fusi. _ "1 ~—— ‘26] 6 HC _ _ _ __2§I :7 :PMC _ 11 W 26] s PRC i _ _ _ 26J 9 v1 26I .105 :v2 _——_ ‘26} 11 v3 . _ _2—61 1 S LH L — — T M _“—143I 1-11 fl __ _ .'_.2.___ R11 1- _ 143I Descriptive Statistics I gHemi. I Mean I Std. Deviation I N I lin— ” I ”.02251’1683Im .0002462143I 6I IRH i .022656133IWI 0009594639775I @6611, ’ i J 02253400467" “.0006720651I 12] 109 , Post-Con. =V3 Total EcRC ERC-28 ERC-34 F usi. HC PMC PRC V1 I991 V2 1907849723I llO 1 6I 0032996832 010768713071 1 11111 00821210482811 6» .9977695151 .9991997299I 12; .995074413 .0021863221f 6i .9059787711 1111199219970931 16: .095976592' 119929692969I‘ 1123 .994996960? 11 11 .9019542099I 6I 1.0046709261 11 11 .0018783719I 6. .9047639431 1 .0018300495I 12! .9474995851 1 1 0199516547I 6I .948122593% 1 0200966724I 6; 1.947778964? 1 111;U10190958069I 12! 1.0952229941 1 9021049161I1 .904649119. 1 11 9018678021I 116I 19049929571 11 .0019212903I 112i 102977950j 119921694997I 6. 11913121618199? 11 11 19944204021 I 6; 1.109122976' 11 1 11.0033206321I 1121 .008478885I 1 0013976280I1“ 61 .903396942I111.0014128557I 116I .9995972881 9013402017I 112I .914195912I __ 90501047091 6I .0144525981 .9051148192I 6i .914929995I 11111 111 00482917291 12I .965361417 1 1 .0169169178I 16I .066979599I 119171777214I 6I .965715968I .9162586746I 12; 1968396898; 11 111.9110750137I 116I 11969499193I 11 11111.0110375615I1116I 196991799871111 9105557898I 12I 19915779931 0338500076I1166I .031783942 9341854976I 661 .091699938 11111111 0338882348I 132I 1.022697929‘ 1 111 110001433439~ 7I .022986486 9009733493: 71 “-52.231571 ___.°0.£2533§8' 91 .009000927 0012446179 7; .008701497I 11 1 9912300932 1117} ’ 0089151212 IT10ta1l I I ' 0011989426 14; 711.111 119951949276 1.0013244864I 7I ERC-28 IRH 11 i 9091757986 I 1111 90127786021I 7II _ 7 __ : '_jf;_i:i_¢ “”1? 1‘41 9:11.00553365390034912375 7} ERC-34' iRH 1 1 199513195117 1 11 19903253545 7| I161” I 99572161939 11111 .10—003428892 I 11413 31:11 1 9&18319‘ 111 _ (51370275251 _7I Fusi. RH 054964086.‘ .0038352458I 7I rT1otal ”9&786111413: . 9936264012 II 1411 IL? _. - 3909918633-; I _ - 0000473126I _ 7_I _HC {RH 905387920 _9.000446476I 7} I99! 093123261 9935535331 111 11.11 .1037317141 0006860585I 7; PMC IRH1104786289 0008732983I 7g IT6t1a1 11104259000: 11 0009320111I 141 111111 1 - 1098964633 1_ 0003591451T17‘I PRC 1R9” _903995128 __ _ .0003618858I ___7I 3116131 ___ 903979879 .0003467357I 14I ILH 916223814 .0000610444I 71 :v1 IRli 1 1 01189559161 .0000243196I_I1_7I » ITotal .016364700I .0001528675I MI I _ III1L111 1 11.10719774141I 11 111 11 00.02680646I 7I va IRl-11111.(I)7—3?1-'a(1)10 999229072137 1171I IT61311 _ __ 9726102571 _ 2-- (10069772181-111I . [111 _ ___;0724360431;__ 1 V_ .0003597663I 7] IV3 IRH I .0745170219IEI 11 .0002322144I 7I 1 - I161? :_ 1 __ .07347653_6__11 .0011182709I 14I I 1 I111 _22 _ _933818210I _ fl 9340363952I1171'1/I ‘ ITotal IRH_ .I_ _ _._034l9666§I___fi .0346751205I 77I I. - 4311.1 - - _ .034007437I 0342453058I 154 J I I131“ .022563115I _ .0001948588I 13I I iM1 IR_H____ - -d __ __._()22834015_I ___ .0006447040I___3 I I i IT61a1 22-- .022698565I - _ .0004866363I 26I ITotal 1511908469602? - .0023807497I 13I I IEcRC [111111; 1 11.008233409I1111111 .0023140481I 13I I I E61717, 11 11 1 0083515061 11 .0023033547I 216:1 ' IERC-zé I911 11 1 911151911018 11 .0017406991I 13I lll ..._.. _. if ‘_. ._ -_ —_ _ _. __ fl [RH 2 22 2 2005444340 00168291201131 (Total 2 .005467724 00167762841 26! 1LH 0045222092128 2 22 ._ 222—2 0032—58331 E321 ERC-34 .1111 005019214 .00127916661 131 1Tot8l2 * 2 005120066, _ 2222 00125884612261 11.112 22 05129681612 22 2 22 02123659280712222131 Fusi. 1211112 I 2' 2 05180264747 22.0218749E1E1 13; (Total 2 2 2 .051551645 2 01341559552128 11.11 f 2 005678234 1111 2 00142822741 131 HC 2RH 2 2 005044161 2 ' 06566502121321 Total * 0053611982 22 ‘2' 2.2003614574471226 1L}! 2 2 2 21203383408 22 4 0015275990121 31 PMC 1RH .104085585 .00302382531 131 [Total .103734496 — 2 2 222.06232422220212261 'L11 2 2 22 0037402802 _2 2 22 00097057261 131 PRC 2RH * 0037832242122 43.520927226270321“ 13! [Total 22 0037617602 2 22 - 200095422461 261 11.1122 2 22 015287444 .003401524521222213I VI ERH f 2 015558053 22 '2 2033469229821 131 {Total ' 22 015422248 222222.003896271261 LB 1 2 068923877 _ 2 .01144827891 2131 V2 11111 22 069932669222 _2222 2 2201169712191 131 fTotal 22 .0654221322734—222422 2 01135120041 261 11.11 2 070521792 2 2 22 2.002174541803122 131 v3 11111 2 7. . 1 .0722127—229461 2 “10075982398 131 1Total 1 '07‘3123991____,.- 1‘ __ 0074195146 261 11.11 . 032784236 0338494023 1431 Total 11111 2 22 03%011 2‘ 0343499673 1431 Total 22 03293—32668 — 22 034041054212861 1LH 2 f 2396. 761666666667661—343. 7640694090449001 61 1 Ml 11111 2 1 26277083333383ng 270. 7955140261128001 61 3 . 1Tota1 _; 2512.250000000000001 318 7314700496328001 121 I Iui 1152 375000000000001 36.8 9429410491546001 61 2AV sPre-Con. EcRC 111211 _1 2 121402560600000000001 347.4163568400313001 61 i 12Tot:l ' 7221146 43450860600601 341.7213299306109001 121 2 115121 2 780. 583333333333001 2‘ 2216.6233289991331001 61 ERC-28 1112211 2 . 7366666666662666—00l 22 22— 2" ‘ 621 __{Tota1 .2 2 758. 62500000000000 2 219434634759350001 1221 112 m "7' 'ERC-34 IRH I Total 111 'Fusi. IRH _ Total 1LH HC 31111 1 Total ILH PMC IRH ‘ Total 'LH PRC IRH rfiifi Total v1 IRH 'vz inn v3 ‘ RH Total I RH I Total ‘Ml IRH I 5 5 11.11 IERC-ZB IRH— " ' iTotal . 5 1 ERC-34 % ~ : IRH "'1911291666666667001 480. 778376130965000I 14I Post-C on IT'otal' 3' 566. 79166666666600 237. 8'81032065750600 6 I 5 555. 50000000000000 ' 226. 227374559313600 I - 6 561.14583333333300 I 221 .403284300939000 I 12 I 6097. 583333333330001" 2661727280670705000I W6; -.fi __7 __ _-_ ___— __. _—__— ___.-- _-__.._..J 6532 29166666666000 I. 2492. 086492362708000I 6 I 63 14. 93750000000000 I 2468. 775000161 103000 I1 629.16666666666600 I 257.032763800000100I 6I 628 91666666666600‘ '7 210 8727499385984001'_6I ..7 _ _ V . _T ___. 629. 04166666666600 I 224.148079943648500 12 ‘ 11183.166666666660001 2085.183167413996000I 6'; 12251.00000000000000: 52359.040207372481000I 6I 1 1717. 08333333333000 I 21.94 747900032477000 I 12I ' 463. 29166666666600 I“ 157. 056949596847400 I 6' I 468. 95833333333300 I 149. 098576172499500 I :6 1 466.12500000000000 I ' 146.0332] 1509013800 I 1689.16666666666700. 631. 852230878918000I '6I _ _ __ [__2_.- __ ___—___— 18016.2500000000000I 568. 038395489248000I 6I 1745. 39583333333300T1 575. 83654—0276339000I 12' I 8454. 000000000000001 3109.152597573815000I' 6I 8602. 33333333333000' 1 2939.190290992855000Im 6 E ___m ___m -.,1 8528. 16666666666000 15‘2885 612516381395000I121 8185 70833333333000I 2290 067299320408000I 6I __.T__.._ 8526. 79166666666000 I 2735.842500] l 1559000I 6 I A ,2, -l,__—_.—_._._ —,w____._ __-L_ H— 8356. 25000000000000 I 2412. 000784429994000 I 12I 3781 .69318181818300I 4048.169774201730000I 66I 3988 39015151515100I 4309. 866734091935000I 66I 3885 04166666666600I—4l66 369184832200000I 132 I “51771.88095238095200I 583 713556355354000I7 7i 2050. 70238095238100' 338. 747214889271200 I ‘_I _ 980 98809523809500I 171099632744774900I 7I 937 916666666666001 200. 789535334887400I 7I 959. 45238095238100 I 180. 606275725684200 I I4 I W...”— ' 63.9 73809523809500| 186. 390311481808800I _ 7I 640. 90476190476100I 181. 309954506570400 I 7 I 640. 32142857142800 I 176. 655390475714000I I 1451 2m- - __ _ J 528.17857142857100 I 125. 600450693003900 l 7 i ' 494. 21428571428500 104. 505325678828900I ~47 I F usi. HC ; PMC PRC V1 V2 : V3 1 Total 1.. EcRC Total Totai 51119642857142800 1112. 3930337019393001 1141 1LH 5477.1547619047601001 129256611173258130001 7i 13H 5 1 5340 5555869E§3i6901 1156092512226161000111171 1Total 5408 87499999999000 1180. 2519988188160001 14g 1} [HA 1 f 1 507. 6666666666660011 1 97.855051535989200 1 7 1RH 465 26190476119004001 '1 11128967123468622001 1715 17611111 1 486,464—281571385001 1 103. 8568711966121001 1:141 1”! 1 9458. 726190476190001 11896. 560—1987022680001 71 1RH 1 9775. 357142857140001— 1829. 605962001238000 111—71 1Total 9617 04166666666000 1797. 8038833954350001 141 1LH 375. 25000000000000 1 1 80. 4054828147102001 71 {RH 346. 73809523809520 82. 9430922236387001 71 7Tot1a1 1 360 99404761904760 1 79 8618228720206001 1141 .FLHh 133148809523809500 480. .4003910430190001___7J 1RH 1380. 083333333333001 3198.0434440275751001 71 1T0tal 1 I 1355. 785714285714001 1111424. 5906584686622001 11141 1L1H“ 1 1 1 6487. 77380933435001 “21104297206324278300011M71 [RH—1 6685.6310952380950001 1690.2528067112440001 71 [T6011 6586. 702380952380001 1804. 293010532696000 1 1411 mu 6615 04761904761000' 1486 3162570169970001 71 SR}! 1 6967 464285714280001 1867. 0766500767720001 :71 1To¢1al 6791 .255952380950001 16315485488218540001 141 1LH _ 310671753246753200111 3278. 0672960445610001 771 1RH 1 .1 11 3189. 533549783549001113368 2376713690550001 _771 {Total 31148. 125541125540001 331—28400112044670001 1541 1LH11 11 2060. 301112820512182001 569.8579100620220001 131 1112* _ 317012820512820001 421.3854201033076001 131 1Total 2188. 657051282051001 508.1731913470500001 261 11.111 11060.089743581974—3001 281.5350601923699001 131 113111: __111_110314.166666661666_001 1., 285.4791354995239001 131 -_ 1:93" _ J 1045. 753205128205001 _ 278.170298039777100 261 11.11 704. 743589743589001 205.581780782596600 131 1 RH ” 685.1025M4m1 1207.0975547146885001 131 1 17691 : __ 694 923076923076001 202. 4200279988192001 _261 1LH 545. 999999999999001 178 5139467007176001 1131 1181111 1 1 I 522. 500000000000001 ”11116627225427169343001 131 176115—111 534. 250000000000001 1 169.6529693226735001 1261 11 In 1 1 1 5763. 506411E641—0001 1 1972.5607045597040001 131 114 IPC Pre-Con. IRH I -_.--.., _ I Total I ILIH HC R]! I v1 ,HH ' v2 RH I v3 'RH I 'Total I l-I I LHI Total IRH I r ,2 “.33 fifi :LH Ml [RH I I ___-- - - I Total :ILH EcRC IRH i I ____. __.. __ - . - I Total ERC-28 Total ' IRH I I 5890 60897435897000 5827. 05769230769000 I l907. 439834005929000 IIIIl l902.l76409737l95000 I3 26 I 563. 74358974358900 I 190.498534131919800I 13I 415. 88461538461530 I I I I 403.14743589743580 I III I 409. 516025641025701 1496.5705128I2051200 I gr‘w 1574. 64102564102500 I 1535. 60576923076900 I II 7395. 26282051282000 I_ 7570. 26282051282000 I 540 79487179487100I 179.197260357568300I 13: 552. 26923076923000 I 181 575260349366300II I26 I Ii0254.621794871731§50I 2100.176776238846000I 13I I 10917 96153846153000I 2375 409863419525000I 13I I I” I I10I58—6I2I9T66I6I66666000I 2222 610191900255000I 26: 1.248891014470694OOI 13I 129.321887710571400I I133 124 725585496121900I 26 562 303189691070000I 13I 5511376313727428000I 13‘: 528. 087298421646000I I261 2674.990017255117000I 13 I 2452. 973657833618000 I 7482.76282051282000 I 2516.115091177523000 I 26I II 7339. 96794871794000 I 1988.447561372168000III13I * 7687.15384615384000 I 2348.681255731743000 I I13; I 7513. 56089743589000 I 3418. 24475524475606I II 3558. 23659673659500I: 3488. 24067599067600 I .85089145 I .38727509 I .61908327 I .6I8080263II _._ _ ___ I I1.17851141I .92965702I 174269839 I II 2139. 403893 164543000 III 26' I 3656. 043869491610000I 143I 3838. 530913552827000 I E3 3742. 473090933987000 I 286 I 1.537649016I 6II IIIIIIII5558IIII982I74I I6I 1I.1_2I862I5I7I67I 1I2I III I IIII7I9911136I7' 6' 1.422076321 I 6; 1.130066059I I12I 2 259973998 I 6I I.90825018I II I32547428I III77425600II L06495|08II .91960354IIII 1.67876346 r-_ __ _ .891324727I 6I ___—__4 1.694875235 I 12 I 574329137I 6I 1 .686967330I 6 1 .2110128951 12: I2I. 149966979 II 198403296 i I 1831398211 I .J I I II I 6 2463441070I II6 2.210184443' I123 115 E Post-Con. HC , F usi. .lflC PMC PRC V1 .8313 V2 RH .V3 RH 041 31111 Total I LH RH Total .3, __M .. 'LH EcRC ERC-28 LIB Total 3 ILH ERC-34 ERH I Total I111 éRH Erouu Q .811 “EL“ _ 108606636 3 .69826655 3.39216644; .81440086 ".93062219 .87251152 3.88558190. .63907800: .76232995; 124357595 .95398377 109877986 3 3 2.07361138 167178188 '1. 87269663 1. 89407285 3 2 05014999 1972111421 1 .15679284 'L13335483 114507383 31.28997136; .59479899 .94238517 85655908 58876108: .72266008E .74865004 I 1.28373235 1.24;345362 ‘ .99605 183;r 1. 10803735 1 .45942736 .92817579 131251708 ____l 12034643 ‘1 .52380035 32784621 "— ‘0983872273' 6; '33 .8686665263 61 3 33' 67051042333_32j 1 .323246837I ' 6: 7 16837757023 3363 _ _ 136647656393 123 A 1395247788I—338 " 9452760993-6i _ 1143503746I '12I ‘1'6486988363d"_63 ___"___—__ M63 3 3 1.395487637; 123 3 _ 233827217373 3 6g ___-3 187269666233_~6E _- '22366609648I 123 2.333836180I 6; 3 “—33322442076943I 6I 2- ' 2278870050I” 12 3‘ 1 669374766I37661 _ 1546187388I 66 '1 602850797I 132I 21306085183 71 ‘- .6628718893— 7; 33' ‘3 _ 1.558223273333743 ' .936790008I 37E 3.532664040E 71 .74518166OE 14E 3373333 .53593001333 73 -3961349252I 73 733 .790588253E 14I .961041938I 7I L483619664E 71 7'33 3 L214671079E 14E ___33;:::m.909673342E3 73 1.7089ISOSSE 7: 33 3'1 .330253114El:143 ___”"3_“_:601616115 ' 7E 373m_33fl_ 367622419 __7E 116 Total Total 4253232811_ 4701746081841 1LH 1.12738879 1.6059947371 71 PMC 11111 _1.29061159.j_ 22492676861—7I 11:61:11 126966107191 8 -_‘ 18795210081 14; 11 4739348 _ if; “‘7‘ PRC 11111 _ 113793967 _ 11371829171 _71 _1T61a1 1056:4842 1.1489490821 141 1111 #:6812513 3157196931 __71 VI 11‘” 119914236; :774550423_[_ .71 1Total 89427785 .609590648 141 1L11 I. 1.189081461 8- __ 14480037661 —71 1v2 11111 1267383601 71389250583851 71 Tota1 - 122132561 1136387037171 1_41 1LH *' 1 987883201 “5287—4202471nj v3 11111 1 1090301591 _ 1.0012938981 71 1Total 1038814901 .8609635881 141 1L11 __ 946869361 _ 1.1013870111 Total 1111 103818028 __ _ ‘4 “12020372651 771 @6611 99252482 _ 1. 1499501781 1154 LH 108732101 _ _ _ 18184618541 :31 Ml 1111149901873: 600044.8701 131l 176ml ' ”— 783868—911 — 13601752971 261 . 1L1: .77544072 1“ 1*.8444992361-1—31 EcRC 111118603541 7 10383308051 .13“; 1T'oka1 81817713, 8 19282941071 261 1 L11 ' _- 18207441591" "1.593036019118181 ERC-28 18117 1088744341 “___ 9073986051 __ 176m 1.114809296 7 1.2716164611 261 “HH— _— 853984421 7932433101 131 ERC-34 11111 _ ___—1277361381 1 m 1.5258550241 131 1fota1 ' ~7‘1.115672901 - “—1.2028190951 261 81:11” 1- 1 1274600871 _ 1.5784216891 131 1Fusi. 11111 . 1“ 1.622447481 1 20273541471 131 ' 176611" 7 1448524181 _ _ 17889152091 261 . 1L11 ' 321769271-— .4863903181 131 'HC 111111. 1 .498809441m .631396776 131 . 112.31 7 ”741386861_M_“ "559520513 261 1PMC L11 1 .982932821 ”1430230644 1 131 1 VTCC ; Pre-Con. PRC V1 V2 'V3 Total ‘Ml ; EcRC ; ERC-28 iji' 1 Total —._.r _..._ __. 1L1! 1R]! i11na1 1 1 105369773 910769582 ; 92070144? .988663391 1.597326011” 11145402896311111 w 1.525677481 1.4058257311 11 1.53330854112111 1 4.6956714 1 11.04375712fi 1 108210699§1 1 1 .062932061 .43743954 1 44125479111 1 .247305871 11 .258324071 11 11 .252814971 23754938 .230128715 233839071 211'12212227619421 223253831 2254366371 .268864961 321416881 .29514092 112446264. 1 .93370705[ 112 94098075i11 103634603§ ___. 7.22 -b ,_2__ _ 445070041 11 .2691267111 .307905131 r _. ___. 1.9351852741'113: 1668726705 1 261 ..... ___.) 1 .2605686421 13} 1 0420677851 131 1.1332018471 26: 1125444544111131 .9688335751 131 1 .0299957581 261 1 201492455511 1 1311 1—12571614437111 111131 1.7719163201 26, 1.6701785851 131 1.7983967961 131 -_.., . ,_ _. . ..___-____ ..___J 1. 701650583 261 1 3913741961 1431 1.3674511871 143; 1 1 .377176277i 2861 .1305505531 61 .0915168951 61 7,_ _____-.. “__.—m, __.—___; 1107563351 12 __2_‘r__ __.1 .1334320421 61 .1349967471 1161 .1280998761 121 .1368883031 61 .1160805211_6J __.—___ 1 1 J 1 .12106747611 12 1194239771 61 115075707111 61 .1118357961 12; .1354434321 61 .1799594141 6! .1543127521 121 157984561316} 1709309991 61 ___4' 1.288515921 .158227091 1 121 2439341621 .45319019 1467038757 2111 1322243231 6% 122111173154911 6} .1200501311 121 118 ' Post-Con. 'PRC IRH V1 'IL11 v2 IRE I I Ir6699 7191 _ . ‘v3 Inu1 Total . RH 511 EcRC RH ERC-28 IRH I .27174938 _" 236126992 .28650965 '.29852083' .357278877 " .31789985 .397507711_' .49445642] .49098206I .418660991 .44236813; .430514067 .32011963 7 .339170937 3 152964528 51595200i .55604798: .53599999I I37605029I' 137929010. .37767919. .346955711 . 4 q '138931757f '“ '.37997376"_ '.44337374; ”1 .460852183 .45211296{ .371134611I7I3I .359045161 .370629957 '3 3 2-_. ..__._ PM .153988830 6 1 1.69674973 1 6 ' __155249116 '121 . -MW»—.V »___ J I 157580325I 6I .195611962I 6| ‘1170556456I 121 '1992134955I 61 .9958994621 6I .6897332601 121 . 102052433 I 61 .087301008161 5691386816I 12I .148107329I 661 I “7.152711849I 66I .150156719I 1321 .1453165751 71 .160584513I 71 _ .148596682I 141 _—_'__I .1139486251 7I __- 1 147133333 I 71 126439946I 14I .112547768 I 7 I 1152905761 7I 16786200021 7! .110174571I 141 _ __.—J 079476069I' 7I 076564501I 14I .182738848I 71 1378154971 71 __.155758398I_ 141 ;43954893‘ 120255395I 71 .49183727' 9 .46569310§ _.58688586I 57030632 578596091 .42188486 I 42773914; I 165582236I 7I 141650238I 141 1366171701 7I .119373759 I 7 ' .12355259IIWWE4I .131923089I 7I __ mum!“ __r..f___.__1 .155150921! 71 . Total Vl 1V2 V3 Total Ml EcRC ERC-28 ERC-34 Fusi. 1R}! [1 __ {Total} I :Lll 1 RH 1 Total -r _ __. LB 1 1 final . Total l .___, 6 4248075676 .138389686l8 146' 53505787. 1620122921 71 _ 54571616966666 6 61661660055617 5403879368 .1357201931 1461 66543528551'66666 —.l3500265l16766i 54444573766666 “61105914992166_71 54398714666666 66 61165745561 141 6 .5590749376 666 .l0582248l1 71 6 571131937 ' 6666*66" .12157069016 671 .56510343: 66: 966—"16967637016141 .46887548 .1464307811 771 64808293116u66 66666 661461585491mm771 474852401 66 6 1459391011 1541 483237251 1378860871 131 .501385621 _ 66 .1420244591 131 49227143; 666 76 1374517031 261 31662975: 67 6:1355472511 6131 33:59:21 1321333171131 .320044711.1398663571 261 296460481 66 __‘666M_Ti3177174616 131 .366854981.1327156101 131 .3612577316”6 129664963 261 6.314687655 12677726516 131 66 6 ;382610215666666m__ .120513543 1 131 6'6666 .36864893. 66 6-6721213424891— 261 6.36283122 66 .18038689016131 6666 6396497421 666—661688827621 13% 6 6 37966432 6 .1716437291 6261 66 .360892521 66 _ 66215952987416'131 406945511 .18705885016 131 66666U6 383919621 '6666 1719389701 261 ___“51'§1§Z§§§? __”-221“___1499259401 131 522644371 _m_2322 1254221971 131 2.22 52071644 ___nm__1554398491 261 .352591561 .157005490| 131 966666 6§36936389166666 16833644516 131 666 _ ”.36097772T 66166 6 .1597101261 26.5 66m66 42588693 666666 66666196386471666131 12f) 11111111 44951435 1 18573388 I 131 1Total 1 1.1433700614 .1875413401 261 1111:1131 11 3 11.417161133143111. 11 13353511111131 'v2 ‘11111— f 1 4398351218111 1 1 ” 1213139361 131 =Total . 47798479 1.260760351 261 1.111.111 1 f 11 ' 1494361810141 11 1334—7393—6414 1131 V3 119i-" *1 _ 33376313 __. 1%..151 TTotal : .50298526 .1208782961 261 . 1111.11 1 1 46012118931 ’ 1 1614411343911 1431 Total 1RH .4154485216471377611431 #6141 1 11407333331 ' 111644—87034 2861 Box' 5 Test of Equality of Covariance Matrices(a) 1 1Box1'sM 11m 11111 1 2091. 6891 F 1 1 I 1 1 34031 an 430 on 11 1 11 11—1 1358317791 181g: 1 . 11111 1 11311 1 1.0001 Tests the null hypothesis that the observed covariance hiatrices of the dependeht variables are 111eq1ual across " groups. 1 a Design: COHoRT+Co110RT* Ro1+Co_1101RT* ROI * HEM] 1' 1 _.___,,_H,_ T. __T__“u 1 Multivariate Tests(d) 1 _____ 7 A 1 Partial ‘ El‘fec t Value F 1Hypothesis1 Error 1 Sig. 1 Eta 11 Noncent. Observed (If 1 dl’ 1 1 Squared 1Parameter Power(a) 1 ' 1 1 1 #11 11 1 11 Yfld_fi1—*MWT W 1 1 15.12:? 1.332 119.678 8.000T480000T .0001 .6661 957.427‘ 1.0001 Wilks' '1 1 1 11_ 111 1 1*fl1_—_|1fi 11 1 1111311 1 11111 H 11 11311141 1 | .006 708.227(b) 8. 000 478. 000 .0001 .922 ? 5665. 814 1.000 1L8Mbd8 l - 1 1 T .COHORT “T ” " W“ ‘”'" 1 1 ' 131113111111 S 1108 3331 3222.910; 8. 000 476 0001 000 982 25783. 277 1.000 1. 2-2- _ --- . . __.-.. i i __ _ _. Roy's '1 1 , 1T 1 1 1 1 l Largest T107815 6468.893(c)! 4.000 240 000! 0.00; 9911258755721 1.000 TT .. __T ___- ___________ T T ___. .T_.________.,___.-______ 1 ma” 1 1.329' 6.022? 80 000 T.968 000 0001 .3321 481.766 1.000 Trace 1 1 1 1 i Wilks' i i T T 1 ICOHORT Lambda 1 .005 32.573. 80 000 945. .2461 000 .729i 2541.679. 1.000 1*ROI I __ -flTi _ T 2 __.__;__.______.._TT .--. ._ 111 1 1%:311131151 127. 988 379.964 80 00011950 0001 000 970130397132 10001 1 1116;; 1.127.592 I 1454313616161 210. 000 242 0001 .000 11 9.92 331817131311 11 1.00011 4__._. r.._— __.—__.. __-_ ___—__.- ____ ___—__.— ______ -. ___— ____- ____.__ _. _ __4 121 ; Largest ’Root ""3"“ .077 .217 88.000 968.000 1.0003 .019 3 19.086 1 .363 '- Trace 1 | . ' i . * 1T“ ‘5'“ ' ‘ .“ "‘1 WW“ .924 .216 88.000 947.548 1.000 .0191 18.789 .356I COHORT Lambda 1 1 *ROI* :7 ~7 2', I I T T A“. WT ‘_ -__ A M _ _ _i- HEM. #3:?“ s .080 .215 88.000 950.000 1.000 .020 . 18.944 . .359 ; ‘Roy S — i m" I — _ ' " I I m T f T 'T __u T Largest .041 .456(C) 22.000 242.000 .984; .040 . 10.037 3 .3581? .Root - ' a Computed using alpha = .05 i b Exact statistic ‘ 3c The statistic is an upper bound on F that yields a lower bound on the significance level. y d Design. COHORT+C0110RT * R01+coHORT * ROl * HEMl I Levene' s Test of Equality of Error Variances(a) 1 1 . -2." ._ _ _ __ .1 F dfl : an Sig. TSE 5.014 43 242 1 .000} AV 4.497 43 242 : 000 f PC 3.011 . 43 242 ' .000: VTCC .491 f 43 j 242 ' .997! Tests the null hypothesis that the error variance of the dependent variable IS equal across group. i a Design: COHORT+COHORT ROI+COHORT ROI * HEM] i Tests of Between-Subjects Effects 1. ’ "' 7 i I ‘ A ‘T _ _ _ A H I I 1 I Partial T Source iDependent . Type III Sum of ; df _ Mean Square i F ! Sig. Eta Noncent. Observed. ‘Variable . Squares . T Parameter Power(a) 1 1 Squared | {1513.63105) 44 .014 386. 273T .000T .986 I 16996. 012T 1.000! Mod 1 'Av 705049098413016) 160238431 457 92. 055T .000 .9441 4050.407 3 1 .000! e - --—--- 1; . PC 378. 903(d)1 44 8. 611 1 4. 299; .0001 .439 I 189153 I 1.000I EVTCC 50.93416) g 44 1.158 64. 449 i 000 I .921 g 2835. 752 1 1 .000| gTSE ‘ .311 . 2 ' ..155 4179. 749T .0001 .972 i 8359.498i 1 .000! in 3518595376. 323 2 1759297688 161 T1010 692T“ .000 .893 i 2021.383i 1.0001 COHORT . ----—— -- -— --—- — : - 1: IPC 324. 784. 2 162. 392' 81 .068T T000. .401 I 162.137i 1.000; iVTCC 49. 069 2 24. 534 1365. 942 | .000 .919 2731. 884 1 .000; TTSE .321 i 20 .016 431. 7771 000 j .973 i 8635. 548 1 .0001 $33?” 7v . 3526000367.240I 20 176300018. 362. 101.282T000; .8931 2025. 637 1.000; ' il’C 43.192 20 2.160 1 078 .373 ' .082 .I 21.562 .7821 122 ___—l .091 ' .000 I > ' .295 __ .000 ' VTCC 1.819 20 5.064E 9101.286ij1 TSE 35913-005; 22 16313-006 E 704411.000; .004. _ "1967 __ _ .068. 5333331” 5895240. Q7; 22' 12679863480 7 .154i 1.000 : 014 1 fl 3 387 NEE-”.126... HEM] ‘PC 10.927 22 .497 248 1.000E .022; 5.455» .189. VTCC .046: 22' _ .002' .117 : 1.000 _ .011 2.582“ W .104; TSE .009242 1‘ 317213-005 _ E E 1— ‘ E E ‘ Error AV 42124621170134EZ42 1740682091 1 1 fl— 1 W _ E ___ fl 5 PC 484 762 242 2. 003 1 EVTCC 4 347 242 .018 ' 8 1 1 E TSE .640 286 7 Total AV 74717372608264EZ86 7 2 - - - - PC 863.665 286 VTCC 55.281 286 1 1 fl 1 aComputed using alpha: .05 1 — _ - ___ — ‘8 - H bRSquared= 986 (AdjustedRSquared- .983) if _- _ _ _ _ 1 c R Squared— — .944 (Adjusted R Squared= .933) L i 1 E d R Squared: 439 (Acljusted R Squared = .337) 1 1 i 1 hm -_ eRSquared= .92l (AdjustedRSquared= 907) — — F 71 8 if -- 1 CUSTOM HYPOTHESIS TESTS Contrast Results (K Matrix) E ETesting group Simple E E Dependent Variable . _ E Contrast“) .TSEE AV E PC EVTch JContrast Estimate E002! -7369161 .153'5 .145E HVpanesgefiaue 10" E __ 0939—910, 2- 0 EDifference (Estimate- Hypothesized) E 002E -736 9161 .153? 145E Levelsz.Levell Eskin—m _ __ _ ___ W 001E 156493E .168E: 0161 31;“ 4 EH _ _ E001 “000' 364 .000E 1 ELEM" “00" 1045.179I 4.83. “4E Easimliglclzidence Interval for ll}f’fg‘Lfi . _m ___ ___ ; i 355:; 004 -428. 653E 1.78E .1772 a Reference eategory— — l 123 Multivariate Test Results ___—”___ ___rj— __ _.___ ___—___ Observed ‘ 1 1 I a Computed using alpha— = .05 ESTIMATED MARGINAL MEANS 1. Grand Mean 7 1 Dependent Variable' r Mean Hypothesis 1 Error 1 . 1 Partial Eta 1 —I\_loncent. Value f F ‘1 df 1 df 1 Slg' 1 Squared 1 Parameter . Power(a) Pillai's trace .353 32.581(b) r 4. 000 239. 0001 .000 ‘ .353 130.323 1 1.0001 , 1 . : wm“ .647 32.581(b) 40002390001000 .353 130.323: 1.0001 lambda 1 1122:1111.“ .545 32.581(b) 4000 239 000 000' .353 i 130. 323 1.0001 fogs ""3"“ 7 .545 32.58116) 4000 , 239000.000 .353 _ 130.323 1.0001 a Computed using alpha — .05 r _ _—h .___._ _-_ M b Exact statistic Univariate Test Results ' _ 22.3.HF__ 3. 3__fi. 177 _1 _ 1 1 1 Partial Source Dependent Sum of df Mean Square F Sig. . Eta 1 Noncent. Observed 1 , Variable Squares - 1 Parameter Power(a) . , _ .Squared 1 1 TSP: .000 1 .000 10.355 001 1 .041 1 10.3551 .8931 rAV 38597994. 423 ' 1 38597994. 423 .22. 1741 .000 i .084 1 22.174 ' .997? Contrast; - -- . - —- —~~—— -J_ 1PC 1.654 1 l . l .654 826; .364 .003 .826- .148 .vrcc 1.499 1 1.499 83.4381 :000 .256 1 83. 438 1 0001 .TSE 0097242 .0007 ' IAv 421246276134 242: 1740687. 091 F , i 1 Error ~77 - W- -f—wu—-- . PC 484 762 242 2.003 . J 1VTCC 4.347 242.018 ' 1 1 l ._ ___. 1 Std. Error 1 95% Confidence Interval—"1 1 ' Lower Bound 1 Upper Bound 1 Lower Bound 1 Upper Bound 1 TSE ‘ .033 1 .000 ' 032 1.0341 EAv- 357167.584 .' 78.247, __ 3362—452 H 36%7—151 115C — 1369 "Vi—.084. * V 0903 ; 1 2341 '9'ch 7.402. __ "H.008 _ .387 24181 124 Dependent Variable Testing group I + w» A Hi 44 2. TESTING GROUP Pre-Con. .Post-Con I Pre-Con. Post-Con Pre-Con. . Post-Con Pre-Con. TSE AV PC VTCC Dependent (1) Testing . Variable group Pre-Con. TSE . - - ‘Post-Con. Pre-Con AV Post-Con. IPre-Con. PC :Post-Con. Pre-Con. VTCC IPost-Con. I Post-Con. I I(J) Testing I group I I Post-Con. Pre-Con. IPost-Con. [Pre-Con. Post-Con. Pre-Con. IPost-Con. Pre-Con. Based on estimated marginal means Estimates I22 I Mean Lower Bound 3.885 042 31418126; 1.145: i .993: .330 M 8.475" *' I Std. Error I 95% Confidence Interval I J IUpper Bound Lower Bound I Upper Bound I I I 1 Pairwise Comparisons Mean IDifference (I- J) 1 Lower Bound I - .002(*): 002(*) 736. 916(*)I -736916(*)I .153 : —.153I 8 i -1450)? " 5457-61" _WhT" 001 .031I 033I 8.000 ”“0337 _ _ 8 035; 881914.835} 8 "8658838 i ”-7171‘245 I _ 106.316 I 2538-7821—833375488' ‘ _ 123i ' “8802': __ __.—1.388I 114g 8 8.768%"— __ i217: __.0i82 _ — 830—7— — __. 38534 i .011" h .454. _ _— 8436AI J m, _ Std. Sig.(a) I 95% Confidence Interval I Error I for Difference(a) Upper Lower I Upper Lower Bound Bound Bound . Bound w— 8:0013'I _BOII -.004I -.001I _ .001; 8.001885" 6.01I W—‘88004I ”#156493 I 8.000} 8 $428858}? TogfiéI ‘156—493 "—10005? 71045. 179 — 348883? .168 8647—88—11—78 ' .483! .168; 8‘ 364—I‘m“ -.483E t— .i78I .016: .000] — 1778 -_ “ti—144 DWI—“66°.“ m—114I AWE: I -__,_ _ _ ___ WI '* The mean difference IS significant at the .05 level. a Adjustment for multiple comparisons: Bonferroni. 125 Multivariate Tests Value F Hg4pothesisT : Error IS—i— Partial Eta I None—git. I Observed 4 df ’ df I g : Squared Parameter , Power(a) I Pillaistrace .353 32.581(b) 4000 239.000 000; .353? 130323 1-0004 Wilks' I a .647 32.581(b) 4.000I 239.000I.000 .353 130.323 1.0004 lambda 3334-4“ .545 32.58l(b) 4.000 239. 000.000 .353 130.323 1.000. :‘o‘zyt‘mgm .545 32. 5811b), 4000: 239.000I 0.00, .353: 130.323i 1.000: I Each F tests the multivariate effect of Testing group. These tests are based on the linearly independent pairwise comparisons among the estimated marginal means. a Computed using alpha- —- .05 b Exact statistic Univariate Tests _. _T __ __ ___ T _ ' Partial 4 Dependent Sum of I I 4 Noncent. 4 Observed I Variable Squares df MC“ Square ‘ F 98'g Eta Parameter I Power(a) I Squared I , ‘_. . - _ ..- __ I _ ___ 3__ .__ _,_ __ TSE Contrast .000 I 1 000 10. 355 001 .041 I 10.355 ' .893 Error 4 .009I 1242 3. 7215-005I I I . . AV Contrast 38597994.423-I1 38597994. 423 I22.174 .000 ' .084 I 22.174 , .997 I IError 421246276.134:IZ42 1740687. 091I I I PC Contrast 4 1 6541 1 1 .654: .826 .364 3 .003 1 .826 .148! I Error , 484.762 I242 2.003 I 4 ' I . j Contrast 1.I499 I l 1 .499'183. 438 .000 .256 83.438 ' 1.000I VTCC . . 9 . ;m—-— , mm._— -—9 I Error I 4.347 I242 .018? I 4 I i The F tests the effect of Testing group. This test is based on the linearly independent pairwise comparisons I among the estimated marginal means. I I I a Computed using alpha: — .05" 3. Testing group * ROI I I I Mean I Std. Error I I 995% Confidence Interval —— I Dependent Variable I Testing group R0] §~ - - II I I Lower Bound Upper Bound I Lower BoundI Upper Bound I h I II ______ ””7 M71” .023I .002I .019I .026I IEcRC .008 : .002 : .004 i .011 I . IERC-ZBI .005 - .002 I .002 ; .009: TSE Pre-Con. IAN ~~~~~~ 9 - --~ -—— —- -9 -— —- --—~9—-———————- 5 ERC-34 4. .005 , .002 .001 . .008 ; IFusn 048 .002 9 044 051 HC 005 .002 001 008 4 126 Post-Con. - Pre-Con. .AV 1 Post-Con. 1 1 131MC 1PRC 1 v1 v2 v3 1 1 M1 1EcRC “11213028 ; ERC-34 1 1 F681. 1H1C PMC 1PRC 11 1v1 1 v2 v3 ~ Ml '1 E6RC {ERC-1128 3 ERC-3417 31 Fusi. 1111C 11 1864C 113RC11 L V1 1 V21 {V3 3M1 EcRC 1ERC-28 3 1ER1C-34 1 I Fusi. 1-- , __ 133C 3 { 9617.042 1PMC 11ka 31311 1 11812 1_V13 _ 1 6314.938 11.103 .004 .014 .066 1 .069 11 .0231 1 .009 .006 1 1 .005 .055 1 .006 .1104 .0041 .016 .073 1 11 11 .073 1 2512.250 . 1111 146. 438 1 758. 625 56l.l46.1 629.042 ' 11717083 466.125 1745. 396 8528.167 8356.250 . 1911292111 959. 452. 1 640 321 51ll96 1 5408. 8715 ' 1 1 486. 464 360. 994 111111355. 786 6586. 702 67912561 11 127 ‘ 11.06211 1 _ 100111 1‘1 11.107: 11 1.1002 114.1128005 ___577 1.002 11_1.10111 _-__ .0181: .0021 .0621 #1- 1.1061911; 1.010211 fl 0651—” 107211 .0027 “1111.020; 1 110121611 11.002 11 11-11——.006 _____ 11101123 .0021 1 11.003131 11111116001911:- 111.0021 1“ 20023 11 _ 1.010911; .002 .052: .058; 1.002 11.100321 _ 1.00911 1 11.0102 _ 110111 1_ 11.111107; .0021 1 _0011. “—— 00.71; 1.00231 111 — .0113’11—6—602013 1.002j 11.061911 ___ .0761 1.0021111 1 .070161_ .0777 380.8643 1 1762. 0191; 1‘3262481; 380.8643 396. 2061 18966691 3888641 8394 116311111150—881561 380.8641 389—0861 131131 T 3773 1 380.8641 ' 151516467061—_ 71065116911 1 :64 _:__ __"“ 380. 8641 1.0966 852; 12467 3153 380.8641 11 -284 106 1 1216.356; 3801.864 1 1991511641 2495. 627; 380. 8641 1 17777—9353 1 9278. 3981 Q 38:88—95 .‘_‘__'_.Q7__68€°19r__.91£:§13 352.611 1216 7123 2605. 871; ' 1352.6113111112648731 1111116546321 11352116111, 111 111—54. 12518 '1 1“ 1334.901 1 113526111 1 -183. 3833 12057761 __ 3512611 1 54713—2961 111—6111184541; 352611 -208.1151 1181044; 335371" 1.3283562 __193133311 352.611 -333. 585 1055. 573. 1 11352—6111 1111 _661—206 #20501 3615 3 3521.611 1_ 581912423171 __ 1728112182 ‘ 352.6111 —16096.677; _ 74858351 PC 7 VTCC Pre-Con. Post-Con. Pre-Con. Ml 1E RC3; ”.6197 1" .409; @1861“ 1.4241 __ .9301 .409 1 .125 1 1.734 1.325 1 .4091 .521 2.130 1 1.8311 F .4091 11.0271— 1' 2.6301 __ _. _ ‘ ..__._.. _ _ _ ._ _... _ -.____ _T. ._ --—— r- - ——-—— - - —— 'T" —————-— —— —— -———-——-——---fi .920 .409 ; .1151 1.724 ——"_-’"Y‘ --— *—.— ——-— __. ,7 .392 .409 1 44133 1.197 __ __.. -' .-_ --. _-__ . . ___. .873 i .409 l .068 g 1.677 .762 ' .409 —.042 1 1.5671 _ 1.099; _ .4091 "12—941“ "19041 1.873 '1 2409.1“ 1.068? 10561“ ’ .3381” ".311—[___1862“ __ ___;_-..1 2.6781 1.972 ”.4691" _' 11.1671" ”—2.717? 9“? __- " @117 7]:Fféi2_§_:§_6__§71 723 1 378 -.022 1 4681 535.3: _ 537Q81’l’ji'j g1"'—j1§_1‘1 1.284 . .378 g .539 2.029 1.1201 1 .378} ' " ‘ 3'7—51“"_1.8654 .426; .378' 43191 1.171 __,. __ . ___-.. . __ _-._._ .. _ ___ __-- , _1 1.209 1 .378 ’ .464 1 1.954 .894 1".3781' i '.149 1328 _37‘81 .483 : G73 1 039 3781 .2941 1 784 1V2 __1 — 401 _ 039 __ .3251 477 1V3 1 431 039 1 .354; 507 1 — 1M1 I 5361 ' 036| .____ ”.4651 1 607 1 EcRC ' 378 ' 036 1 .307 g 448 fPost-Con. 1ERC-281 _359 1 ”0361 — “288 1 430 ' ERC-341 - 1313—01 #036 ”1359 1 4511 IFusi - 452 1 036 .382 f 523 1 128 4. Testing group * ROI * Hemi. .466111 ‘1036. _ __ .395 _: -—i:"_§g61 .579 .036 .508 .6491 .425. .6361 _ .354 __ 14951 .540" .036f .4701'__ 16111 .544' .036 _ '_ .473'___ 61§1 .565" .036' _ _ .495' __.6361 129 __ _ _.___.._--__1 A ' 1 _ ' _.-. 3 ' 1 . 1 1 Mean 1 Std. Error | 95% Confidence Interval 1 [Dependent 1Testing ROI 1H i 12- 1 ~~-4 - - 1+“— 1'r A -—— ———— Variable 'group 1 cm ' , Lower Upper Lower Upper 1 1 1 Bound 1 Bound 1 Bound 1 Bound 1 “4] L11 3 .0231 .0021 .0181 .0271 1 1RH .023 - .002 ' .018 ' .0281 1lJi .008 .002 .0032 .0131 EcRC 1—-- ~- 4 -—+- n 1-“. _-- 1 1 RH .008 .002 ' .003 . .0131 1 ‘1—‘ ‘ '*7‘ r "" f --“_: ERG E1.11 ‘ .005 .002, ,_ .0001 __.0_1_01 123 11m .005 .002 F .000 g .0101 Hanc- 1w 1 995' .0025 4.521180% 1 _ -9191 134 1 RH .005 .002 i .000 g .010 1' 11J1 .047 .0021 .0431 .0521 Fusi. ’r- . - . - - r - - -—--- 11m 1 .048 .002 1 .043 . .0531 llJl j .005 .002? .000 .0101 .Pre-Con. HC 1 g— ; - - -1 — -- 1- _ ---— . 3 RH 1 .005 r .002 . .000; .0101 1 1L}! 1 .103- .002 .098 .1081 PthI 1 : - —w 1 - __.- ._ __.» TSE 1 1RH 1 .103 .002 . .098 , .1081 1 1111 i .003: .002. 40011 .0081 PRC 1 n». » - . w— . —--w1-—————», inn 1 .mm .mn1 -00u .mm1 :VW. 11J1 1 .014' .0021 .0095 .0191 - 11111 f .014- .0021 .010; 0191 . _—7 __ .-', - _ . -__._. 1 __. ., __.—__J 1 5 1lJi 1 .065- .002: .0601 0701 .V2 . » --1 - — -—; — ~— ' 1RH .066; .0021 .061 1 0711 1L}! 1 .068 .002: .063 .0731 1V3 11— 1 __- 1 __- 1 ~~—— —-_— —~— ' ! 1nu1 1 .0691 .0021 .0651 0741 1 3 1L}! 1 .023: .002! .018? 0271 1 .841 1——-—; __,-_ —— m ,. — -» - .1— —4 ~4— 1 1 rRH 1 .023 1 .002 1 .018 1 0281 1Post-Con. 1 RC 11.11 i .0091 .002? .004? 0141 1 l c _"'1 _.-7i -, .-_- , . .L_. __ 1 1 1RH 1 .009 .002 .004 1 013 1 58110 11.11 1 .006: .002 .001 1 .010! __ 22,, A, W; 22, -,2_ _____ _ -_-_ - _'_ “_____'__._ ______ _..'_. ._._____._ .____ _. --___._J 28 RH ' ERC— «lfflfl 34 [R11 rL11 Fusi. 1 _-_ 33 .RH LH HC 13+- 1RH ,L11 PMC 1 -: :RH ‘ L11 PRC . 1 1RH FL11 v1 1: - RH 1L11: v2 1 l 11:"- v3 1 — —- 1R11 1L" 1 M1 1- 1R11 1 rL11 » EcRC 1 1R]! . L r--- . ERC- 1U} :28 1R“ . _ -1 . ERC- 1'1”“ 34 1RHI 1LH : Fusi 1-3-—- 1 11 AV Pre-Con. 1L}! l 11C ;—---——— IRH 1 TLH 1 =PMC r____- ___“ . L11 1RH . 1m 1 'VI 1‘“— 1R“ 11.11 1 1R11 < v3 L11 I 1 l_—.-.——— I. 11111 1 .006—7' _ 4.0027 ' .90171_——010_1 .0063 h 2.002 __. .001-1 “— 0701 ”.005 7 .6027 7— 7 ".007 [___01—01 .055 - .002 1 .050. .0591 2 .055 7.002 7— ___.0505”_ — .9603 .006" M .002" _— __ ”.0027 __-0111 .005 ”.00—2W 001~ 6101 7.10477 2.0027277“ —.0991'—__77081 .105 .002 .100; .109; .0047 7 7' .002— 7 -.0011 7__'.0_091 .004 2 .002? '7” "27-001 1___ __.0691 .017 .002 .012: 0211 .072 .002 .067 : .077 1 ' .0723 ".0027 _ ___—.0697; ___—“.6781 .072 .002 ; .0685 0771 .075: .0021 _____ “97.91“ ___.(291 2396 792 538. 623: 1335. 8041 3457. 779: 2627. 708 7 538 623 7 156687211 ”73688—6961 13313333 333633: __77133333' [3.3333631 1140. 500: 538.623. 79.5131 2201487. ' 333333? ._ 333 633' 33.33.33 ..33333331 736.667; 538. 623 -324. 3211 1797. 6541 566 792—I” 573—8 6213 __ 494.1961—7627—7791 - 555500 7 5386—23 505.4871 1616487 6097.583; 538.623 ' 5036 5961 7158. 5711 653272927 __ 538.623 5471 .3041 7593. 2791 629.7671 57387623; —-431 821: ””176E7541 628. 917 538. 6231 —432. 071. 1689.904! 11183767 7 538.6231 ‘101221-79 "722441541 12257696777 5386231 111900131 133119871 463292" "—538. 6231 -597. 69671 1524. 2791 7 468 9581— 538623 :7 :592. 0291 __:E@1 1689.167 538. 623 628.1791 2750.1541 18016251 538.623I_—-—_7747()_;3-81 28626121 8454000: ”7538—623: 7 7393. 01731 7779574987; ' 8602.333? “538.623 7 75413467799663 3211 ' 8185.708 _ 538.623 . 7124.721j _ 779246 6961 130 Post-Con. Pre-Con. 8526. 7921 “538. 6231" 7465. 8941 IIII9I58I7II.I77I91 1771.881 . 498.6683 II 789 5981 I 2752164? 2059702IIII_I498.-1568I 1968419? II 39329861 980.988 I I 498.668 -1 .2951 19632711 937 917II “7496.668 II 44.367: II 19292091 639 738 I 49846681 I -342. 5457 1622—02—11 640. 905 "498.668 I 434379 IIIII1623988 528.179 I7 498.668 454.1051 15494621 494.274 II I498. 668 I 4889691” 14764981 5477.155 II—419I866I81 II 44948711 64594381 53407595 ' 498 6681 4358.312 ' 63228791 597.667 7 4986681 I 474.6171IIII14I899591 I 465.262 498668 7-5779211 14475451 9458—72617III4986681 84764431 II10441.0101 I 9775.357 4986681 I 8793. 07417767576411 375.250 74236581 697—0331 13575331 7139-71? _ ’ 19:69.8 " 33335.51 1188797531 1331.488: 498.668 349. 2051 2313 7711 "1380.083 I49§66IS17I 397 8001 III23II62367I: 648777477" 498.6681 II 551754901 III7479957? 6663763117“ 4986681 IIII5793348 II 7667914‘ 6615 0481 “4986681 :5632764 7597—3311 6967 464 498—6681 II 59851811 II794I9.74I8 .851I;—_ .578; -III.287I1 I IIIIII179I89I1 II.387I1 .578I'I I— "—4751; I IIIIII15251 681‘ 5781 -.457-. 1.819: 1:179? I —.5781 —— .040; 2.3171 1.743; “7578; "___—605: 2.8811 998, __ 3.5.7.8: ___ _5 231°] __ I_2I:i°‘I”_6_1 .774 1 .578 ' -.3641 1.9121 1_.065II7_I— 578 ___-.0731 “22.2631 1.679 .578 .541 2. 8171 ‘ _ .9813; _ .578 _ ___-1052. 1.2241 .698 . 578 , -.440 1 836? .8147 Ts78IIu— —;3_24II1I——I1.9531 .931g — _ .578 I 1208 2.0691 886 .578 1 2253 1 2.0241 'VTCC Post-C on. ; Pre-Con. V1 | v2 g v3 ? Ml EcRC } 'ERC- 11‘ {_W ‘23 :RH gERC- “J1 34 {RH . Fhfl. [—-—- ‘ HC PMC 'r PRC V1 r *7 V2 ;V3 m‘i ELH wn ._. '”—"“f_ _. ILH EcRC “- ; [RH 1 1 | r "__—— _TI ' ' 'ERC- flJi L34 LRH 'ng'i. TLH [Rji"?" r fV~~—~w~ 'ERC- flJi . E fa" fl w 1‘ .__ .l ___ . l :Aifl‘ J05? "bxn .534 5 lih9j _”1fi4f ’Iiiif ‘"1£3; _ _Efifl 4M6; oyfl N. N -——1' .Iéi'“ "_xmé7m” “153? .337 E lfiffl ”'“ESéi 12mg zsnfl "3:032; “_Lioé L&B! Lani _Zfidfl —"31éfl "““£§n} ‘ _ 11$2E ' _"_f§m§ lagn ___ .* ___. _ J 2J8| ' 234% _"”—§an Hus; ___ __- _ J 53in 33] 366i ‘ Post-Con. fl .055" 7200 416F — A ”.332: _ T5471 _ __.—3597 _ __.—575: __ _'.164§ _ "380—1 __ .193 ___ E91 __ 191‘; _ “ 7106—1 fl — 2301 _ __.445'"i RH .404 .055 _ .297 — 5121 3 1LH 419 .055 311 ' .526: V W 7 7 3 .7. __ -7 . _ _ ___ __ __- ___J 3 RH .442 .055 .335 .5501 .M TLH 516. .051, .416 6161 1 __ .- —— — —— 4 — 1RH .556 . 051 fi .456 .656; , ‘H__. _ _ . _ _ _ _____;_ _ __ . * 2____..__-J 1LH .376 .051 2 276 476! EcRC .44— . — — —— - ———----@ 1 1RH .379 ' .051 5 280 .4791 [ERG 1LH . .347? .051 1 .247. .4471 {23 1RH .371 .051 f .271 : .471 1 :m- it," 339., _ __ 051 _ 23° _ 5581 134 1RH ' 371 9 .051 , .271 . .4701 ' T1LH 1 .443 : 051 .3443 543 1 Fusi — .:. - — — 2-, — — ~ ———-— ------ ——— ———-1 . gRH . .4611 .0511 3611 5611 FHC 1w .440 .051 1 340 .5391 1 1RH ' .4921 .051 . 3921 5921 T 11. .587 1 .051 3 487 . .6871 PMC . ..'. . 1 _ _ .3. ___~ _ __-5J 1 1 .570 .051 1 471 . 6701 1 1 1 .422 .051 .322 1 5221 PRC F” — <1 ———— — - ——— — ——' 1 1 428 051 1 .328 1 5281 iv1 TLH .535 051 1 .435 1 .635! g 1RH .546 .051 5 .4461 .6451 ivz iLH 1 .5443 .051 g .444? .6431 1 :----——— +—#——-———-- ——— — — ___ -----— :. 1RH .544 .051 1 .445 .6441 133 .051—1— "____ ' —' _ Y— .4511 APPENDIX C: DECONVOLUTION COMMAND LINE The following command line templates were used to invoke the AFNI program 3dDeconvolve for Figure 7 of the present dissertation. 3dDeconvolve was run on individual, motion-corrected (“3 dreg”) subject datasets which had been concatenated and detrended across the four runs (“allruns”). The baseline was specified to consist of six motion parameters — roll, pitch, yaw, and translations along the x, y, and z axes - as well as a linear trend. Computation of voxel impulse response functions (IRFs: see Figure 8) was found to be optimal at a maximum time lag of 4 TRs, or 10 seconds. 3dDeconvolve \ -input 3dreg $subj.allruns+orig \ —automask —GOFORIT 2 -jobs 4 —num_stimts 9 \ -stim_file l match.1D -stim_label l M \ —stim_file 2 mismatch.1D -stim_labe1 2 MM \ -stim_file 3 control.lD -stim_label 3 C \ -stim_file 4 '$subj.allruns.lD[O]' —stim_1abel 4 ROLL \ —stim_label PITCH -stim_file S '$subj.allruns 1D[l]' \ -stim_label RAW —stim_file 6 '$subj.allruns.1D[2]' \ -stim_label dP -stim_file 7 '$subj.allruns.lD[3]' \ —stim_label dI -stim_file 8 '$subj allruns.lD[4]' \ ~stim_label dS —stim_file 9 '$subj.allruns.1D[5]' \ ~stim_base 4 —stim_base 5 —stim_base 6 ~stim_base 7 \ -stim_base 8 —stim_base 9 \ —iresp l $subj.match_IRF_maxlag_4 \ ~iresp 2 $subj.mismatch_IRF_maxlag_4 \ -iresp 3 $subj.control_IRF_maxlag_4 \ -sresp 1 $subj.match_sdIRF_maxlag_4 \ -sresp 2 $subj.mismatch_sdIRF_maxlag_4 \ —sresp 3 ssubj.control_sdIRF_maxlag_4 \ -stim_maxlag 1 4 -stim_maxlag 2 4 —stim_maxlag 3 4 \ -fout -rout -tout -vout -nobout -full_first \ —bucket 3dd.$subj.allruns_maxlag_4 \OUJQONU'I 134 REFERENCES Abel, T. and K. M. Lattal (2001). Molecular mechanisms of memory acquisition, consolidation and retrieval. Current opinion in neurobiologz 11(2): 180-7. Aguirre, G. K., E. Zarahn and M. D'Esposito (1998). The variability of human, BOLD hemodynamic responses. Neuroimage 8(4): 360-9. Albright, T. D. (1993). Cortical processing of visual motion. Reviews of oculomotor research 5: 177-201. Alvarez, P. and L. R. Squire (1994). Memory consolidation and the medial temporal lobe: a simple network model. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 91(15): 7041-5. Anderson, J. R. and L. J. Schooler (1991). Reflections of the environment in memory. Psychol Sci 2(6): 396-408. Bailey, C. H., M. Chen, F. Keller and E. R. Kandel (1992). Serotonin-mediated endocytosis of apCAM: an early step of learning-related synaptic growth in Aplysia. Science (New York, N. Y 256(5057): 645-9. Bailey, C. H., P. Montarolo, M. Chen, E. R. Kandel and S. Schacher (1992). Inhibitors of protein and RNA synthesis block structural changes that accompany long-term heterosynaptic plasticity in Aplysia. Neuron 9(4): 749-58. Bandettini, P. A., E. C. Wong, R. S. Hinks, R. S. Tikofsky and J. S. Hyde (1992). Time course EPI of human brain function during task activation. Magn Reson Med 25(2): 390-7. Bannerman, D. M., J. N. Rawlins, S. B. McHugh, R. M. Deacon, B. K Yee, T. Bast, W. N. Zhang, H. H. Pothuizen and J. Feldon (2004). Regional dissociations within the hippocampus--memory and anxiety. Neuroscience and biobehavioral reviews 28(3): 273-83. Behzadi, Y. and T. T. Liu (2005). An arteriolar compliance model of the cerebral blood flow response to neural stimulus. Neuroimage 25(4): 1100-11. Belcadi-Abbassi, W. and C. Destrade (1995). Post-test apamin injection suppresses a Kamin-like effect following a learning session in mice. Neuroreport 6(9): 1293-6. 135 Blamire, A. M., S. Ogawa, K. Ugurbil, D. Rothman, G. McCarthy, J. M. Ellermann, F. Hyder, Z. Rattner and R. G. Shulman (1992). Dynamic mapping of the human visual cortex by high-speed magnetic resonance imaging. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 89(22): 11069-73. Bodizs, R., M. Bekesy, A. Szucs, P. Barsi and P. Halasz (2002). Sleep-dependent hippocampal slow activity correlates with waking memory performance in humans. Neurobiology of learning and memory 78(2): 441-57. Boynton, G. M., S. A. Engel, G. H. Glover and D. J. Heeger (1996). Linear systems analysis of functional magnetic resonance imaging in human V1. J Neurosci 16(13): 4207-21. Bozon, B., S. Davis and S. Laroche (2003). A requirement for the immediate early gene zit268 in reconsolidation of recognition memory afier retrieval. Neuron 40(4): 695-701. Bozon, B., A. Kelly, S. A. Josselyn, A. J. Silva, S. Davis and S. Laroche (2003). MAPK, CREB and zif268 are all required for the consolidation of recognition memory. Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London 358(1432): 805-14. Braddick, O. J., J. M. O'Brien, J. Wattam-Bell, J. Atkinson, T. Hartley and R. Turner (2001 ). Brain areas sensitive to coherent visual motion. Perception 30(1): 61-72. Breakspear, M., M. J. Brammer, E. T. Bullmore, P. Das and L. M. Williams (2004). Spatiotemporal wavelet resampling for functional neuroimaging data. Hum Brain Mapp 23(1): l-25. Brown, H. D. and S. M. Kosslyn (1993). Cerebral lateralization. Current opinion in neurobiology 3(2): 183-6. Buchel, C., J. T. Coull and K. J. Friston (1999). The predictive value of changes in effective connectivity for human learning. Science (New York, NY 283(5407): 1538-41. Buckner, R. L., P. A. Bandettini, K M. O'Craven, R. L. Savoy, S. E. Petersen, M. E. Raichle and B. R. Rosen (1996). Detection of cortical activation during averaged single trials of a cognitive task using fimctional magnetic resonance imaging. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 93(25): 14878-83. Buckner, R. L., W. Koutstaal, D. L. Schacter, A. M. Dale, M. Rotte and B. R. Rosen (1998a). Functional-anatomic study of episodic retrieval. II. Selective averaging 136 of event-related flVIRI trials to test the retrieval success hypothesis. Neuroimage 7(3): 163-75. Buckner, R. L., W. Koutstaal, D. L. Schacter, A. D. Wagner and B. R. Rosen (1998b). Functional-anatomic study of episodic retrieval using MRI. 1. Retrieval effort versus retrieval success. Neuroimage 7(3): 151-62. Burwell, R. D. and D. G. Amaral (l998a). Cortical afferents of the perirhinal, postrhinal, and entorhinal cortices of the rat. The Journal of comparative neurology 398(2): 179-205. Burwell, R. D. and D. G. Amaral (l998b). Perirhinal and postrhinal cortices of the rat: interconnectivity and connections with the entorhinal cortex. The Journal of comparative neurology 391(3): 293-321. Burwell, R. D., D. J. Bucci, M. R. Sanborn and M. J. Jutras (2004). Perirhinal and postrhinal contributions to remote memory for context. J Neurosci 24(49): 11023- 8. Buxton, R. B., K Uludag, D. J. Dubowitz and T. T. Liu (2004). Modeling the hemodynamic response to brain activation. Neuroimage 23 Suppl 1: S220-33. Buzsaki, G. (1998). Memory consolidation during sleep: a neurophysiological perspective. Journal of sleep research 7 Suppl 1: 17-23. Cabeza, R., J. K. Locantore and N. D. Anderson (2003). Lateralization of prefrontal activity during episodic memory retrieval: evidence for the production-monitoring hypothesis. Journal of cognitive neuroscience 15(2): 249-59. Cantero, J. L., M. Atienza and R. M. Salas (2002a). Effects of waking-auditory stimulation on human sleep architecture. Behavioural brain research 128(1): 53- 9. Cantero, J. L., M. Atienza, R. M. Salas and E. Dominguez-Marin (2002b). Effects of prolonged waking-auditory stimulation on electroencephalogram synchronization and cortical coherence during subsequent slow-wave sleep. J Neurosci 22(11): 4702-8. Chance, F. S. (2007). Receiver operating characteristic (ROC) analysis for characterizing synaptic efficacy. Journal of neurophysiology 97(2): 1799-808. Chrobak, J. J. and G. Buzsaki (l998a). Gamma oscillations in the entorhinal cortex of the freely behaving rat. J Neurosci 18(1): 388-98. 137 Chrobak, J. J. and G. Buzsaki (l998b). Operational dynamics in the hippocampal- entorhinal axis. Neuroscience and biobehavioral reviews 22(2): 303-10. Chun, M. M. and N. B. Turk-Browne (2007). Interactions between attention and memory. Current opinion in neurobiology 17(2): 177-84. Cirelli, C. and G. Tononi (2000a). Gene expression in the brain across the sleep-waking cycle. Brain research 885(2): 303-21. Cirelli, C. and G. Tononi (2000b). On the functional significance of c-fos induction during the sleep-waking cycle. Sleep 23(4): 453-69. Clavagnier, S., A. Falchier and H. Kennedy (2004). Long-distance feedback projections to area V1: implications for multisensory integration, spatial awareness, and visual consciousness. Cognitive, aflective & behavioral neuroscience 4(2): 117- 26. Cramer, S. C., R. M. Weisskoff, J. D. Schaechter, G. Nelles, M. Foley, S. P. Finklestein and B. R. Rosen (2002). Motor cortex activation is related to force of squeezing. Hum Brain Mapp 16(4): 197-205. Datta, S. (2000). Avoidance task training potentiates phasic pontine-wave density in the rat: A mechanism for sleep-dependent plasticity. J Neurosci 20(22): 8607-13. de Araujo, D. B., W. Tedeschi, A. C. Santos, J. Elias, Jr., U. P. Neves and O. Baffa (2003). Shannon entropy applied to the analysis of event-related flVIRI time series. Neuroimage 20(1): 31 1-7. Deacon, T. W., H. Eichenbaum, P. Rosenberg and K W. Eckmann (1983). Afferent connections of the perirhinal cortex in the rat. The Journal of comparative neurology 220(2): 1168-90. Dudukovic, N. M. and A. D. Wagner (2007). Goal-dependent modulation of declarative memory: Neural correlates of temporal recency decisions and novelty detection. Neuropsychologia 45(11): 2608-20. Duong, T. Q., A. C. Silva, S. P. Lee and S. G. Kim (2000). Functional MRI of calcium- dependent synaptic activity: cross correlation with CBF and BOLD measurements. Magn Reson Med 43(3): 383-92. Ebbinghaus, H. (1913). Memory. A Contribution to Experimental Psychology. New York, Teachers College, Columbia University. 138 Egorov, A. V., B. N. Hamam, E. Fransen, M. E. Hasselmo and A. A. Alonso (2002). Graded persistent activity in entorhinal cortex neurons. Nature 420(6912): 173-8. Egorov, A. V., U. Heinemann and W. Muller (2002). Differential excitability and voltage-dependent Ca2+ signalling in two types of medial entorhinal cortex layer V neurons. The European journal of neuroscience 16(7): 1305-12. Eichenbaum, H. (1996). Learning from LTP: a comment on recent attempts to identify cellular and molecular mechanisms of memory. Learning & memory (Cold Spring Harbor, N. Y 3(2-3): 61-73. Eichenbaum, H. (2001). The long and winding road to memory consolidation. Nature neuroscience 4(1 1): 1057-8. Eichenbaum, H., J. Dusek, B. Young and M. Bunsey (1996). Neural mechanisms of declarative memory. Cold Spring Harbor symposia on quantitative biology 61: 197-206. Eichenbaum, H., G. Schoenbaum, B. Young and M. Bunsey (1996). Functional organization of the hippocampal memory system Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 93(24): 13500-7. Fell, J ., G. Fernandez and C. E. E1ger(2003). More than synchrony: EEG chaoticity may be necessary for conscious brain functioning. Medical hypotheses 61(1): 158-60. Fernandez, G. and I. Tendolkar (2006). The rhinal cortex: 'gatekeeper' of the declarative memory system. Trends Cogn Sci 10(8): 358-62. Frank, L. M. and E. N. Brown (2003). Persistent activity and memory in the entorhinal cortex. Trends in neurosciences 26(8): 400-1. Frith, C. (2005). The neural basis of hallucinations and delusions. Comptes rendus biologies 328(2): 169-75. Gabrieli, J. D. (1998). Cognitive neuroscience of human memory. Annual review of psychology 49: 87-115. Gabrieli, J. D., J. B. Brewer and R. A. Poldrack (1998). Images of medial temporal lobe fimctions in human learning and memory. Neurobiology of learning and memory 70(1-2): 275-83. 139 Gais, S. and J. Born (2004). Declarative memory consolidation: mechanisms acting during human sleep. Learning & memory (Cold Spring Harbor, N. Y 11(6): 679- 85. Geissler, A., R. Lanzenberger, M. Barth, A. R. Tahamtan, D. Milakara, A. Gartus and R. Beisteiner (2005). Influence of fMRI smoothing procedures on replicability of fine scale motor localization. Neuroimage 24(2): 323-31. Gerber, B. and R. Menzel (2000). Contextual modulation of memory consolidation. Learning & memory (Cold Spring Harbor, N. Y 7: 151-158. Gerloff, C., C. Braun, M. Staudt, Y. L. Hegner, J. Dichgans and I. Krageloh—Mann (2006). Coherent corticomuscular oscillations originate from primary motor cortex: evidence fiom patients with early brain lesions. Hum Brain Mapp 27(10): 789-98. Gonsalves, B. D., I. Kahn, T. Curran, K. A. Norman and A. D. Wagner (2005). Memory strength and repetition suppression: multimodal imaging of medial temporal cortical contributions to recognition. Neuron 47(5): 751-61. Gonzalez Andino, S. L., R. Grave de Peralta Menendez, G. Thut, L. Spinelli, O. Blanke, C. M. Michel, M. Seeck and T. Landis (2000). Measuring the complexity of time series: an application to neurophysiological signals. Hum Brain Mapp 11(1): 46- 57. Grunwald, T., N. N. Boutros, N. Pezer, J. von Oertzen, G. Fernandez, C. Schaller and C. E. Elger (2003). Neuronal substrates of sensory gating within the human brain. Biological psychiatry 53(6): 51 1-9. Haist, F ., J. Bowden Gore and H. Mao (2001). Consolidation of human memory over decades revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging. Nature neuroscience 4(11): 1139-45. Hamzei, F., R. Knab, C. Weiller and J. Rother (2003). The influence of extra- and intracranial artery disease on the BOLD signal in FMRI. Neuroimage 20(2): 1393-9. Hausser, M., N. Spruston and G. J. Stuart (2000). Diversity and dynamics of dendritic signaling. Science (New York, N. Y 290(5492): 739-44. Haxby, J. V., M. I. Gobbini, M. L. Furey, A. Ishai, J. L. Schouten and P. Pietrini (2001). Distributed and overlapping representations of faces and objects in ventral temporal cortex. Science (New York, N. Y 293(5539): 2425-30. 140 Haxby, J. V., E. A. Hoffinan and M. I. Gobbini (2002). Human neural systems for face recognition and social communication. Biological psychiatry 51(1): 59-67. Haxby, J. V., B. Horwitz, L. G. Ungerleider, J. M. Maisog, P. Pietrini and C. L. Grady (1994). The functional organization of human extrastriate cortex: a PET-rCBF study of selective attention to faces and locations. J Neurosci 14(11 Pt 1): 6336- 53. Henson, R. N. and M. D. Rugg (2003). Neural response suppression, haemodynamic repetition effects, and behavioural priming. Neuropsychologia 41(3): 263-70. Herron, J. E. and M. D. Rugg (2003a). Retrieval orientation and the control of recollection. Journal of cognitive neuroscience 15(6): 843-54. Herron, J. E. and M. D. Rugg (2003b). Strategic influences on recollection in the exclusion task: electrophysiological evidence. Psychonomic bulletin & review 10(3): 703-10. Herron, J. E. and E. L. Wilding (2004). An electrophysiological dissociation of retrieval mode and retrieval orientation. Neuroimage 22(4): 1554-62. Herron, J. E. and E. L. Wilding (2006a). Brain and behavioral indices of retrieval mode. Neuroimage 32(2): 863-70. Herron, J. E. and E. L. Wilding (2006b). Neural correlates of control processes engaged before and during recovery of information from episodic memory. Neuroimage 30(2): 634-44. Holmes, W. R. and W. B. Levy (1990). Insights into associative long-term potentiation from computational models of NMDA receptor-mediated calcium influx and intracellular calcium concentration changes. Journal of neurophysiology 63(5): 1148-68. Homberger, M., A. M. Morcom and M. D. Rugg (2004). Neural correlates of retrieval orientation: effects of study-test similarity. Journal of cognitive neuroscience 16(7): 1196-210. Homberger, M., M. D. Rugg and R. N. Henson (2006a). ERP correlates of retrieval orientation: direct versus indirect memory tasks. Brain research 1071(1): 124-36. Homberger, M., M. D. Rugg and R. N. Henson (2006b). fMRI correlates of retrieval orientation. Neuropsychologia 44(8): 1425-36. 141 Hubel, D. H. and T. N. Wiesel (1959). Receptive fields of single neurones in the cat's striate cortex. The Journal of physiology 148: 574-91. Hubel, D. H. and T. N. Wiesel (1962). Receptive fields, binocular interaction and functional architecture in the cat's visual cortex. The Journal of physiology 160: 106-54. Ivanco, T. L. and R. J. Racine (2000). Long-term potentiation in the reciprocal corticohippocampal and corticocortical pathways in the chronically implanted, freely moving rat. Hippocampus 10(2): 143-52. Jacobsen, L. K, J. C. Gore, P. Skudlarski, C. M. Lacadie, P. Jatlow and J. H. Krystal (2002). Impact of intravenous nicotine on BOLD signal response to photic stimulation. Magnetic resonance imaging 20(2): 141-5. James, W. (1893). The Stream of Consciousness. Psychology. W. James. New York, Holt. Johannes, S., B. M. Wieringa, M. Matzke and T. F. Munte (1996). Hierarchical visual stimuli: electrophysiological evidence for separate left hemispheric global and local processing mechanisms in humans. Neuroscience letters 210(2): 111-4. Kamin, L. J. (1957). The retention of an incompletely learned avoidance response. J Comp Physiol Psychol 50: 457-460. .Kamin, L. J. (1963). The retention of an incompletely learned avoidance response: some further analyses. J Comp Physiol Psychol 56: 719-722. Kang, H. C., E. D. Burgund, H. M. Lugar, S. E. Petersen and B. L. Schlaggar (2003). Comparison of functional activation foci in children and adults using a common stereotactic space. Neuroimage 19(1): 16-28. Kanwisher, N., J. McDermott and M. M. Chun (1997). The fusiform face area: a module in human extrastriate cortex specialized for face perception. J Neurosci 17(11): 4302-11. Kerr, K. M., K L. Agster, S. C. Furtak and R. D. Burwell (2007). Functional neuroanatomy of the parahippocampal region: The lateral and medial entorhinal areas. Hippocampus. Kida, S., S. A. Josselyn, S. P. de Ortiz, J. H. Kogan, I. Chevere, S. Masushige and A. J. Silva (2002). CREB required for the stability of new and reactivated fear memories. Nature neuroscience 5(4): 348-55. 142 Knierim, J. J. and D. C. van Essen (1992a). Neuronal responses to static texture patterns in area V1 of the alert macaque monkey. Journal of neurophysiology 67(4): 961- 80. Knierim, J. J. and D. C. Van Essen (1992b). Visual cortex: cartography, connectivity, and concurrent processing. Current opinion in neurobiology 2(2): 150-5. Krainik, A., M. Hund-Georgiadis, S. Zysset and D. Y. von Cramon (2005). Regional impairment of cerebrovascular reactivity and BOLD signal in adults after stroke. Stroke; a journal of cerebral circulation 36(6): 1146-52. Kriegeskorte, N., R. Goebel and P. Bandettini (2006). Information-based fimctional brain mapping. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 103(10): 3863-8. Kumaran, D. and E. A. Maguire (2007). Which computational mechanisms operate in the hippocampus during novelty detection? Hippocampus. Lasota, K. J., J. L. Ulmer, J. B. Firszt, B. B. Biswal, D. L. Daniels and R. W. Prost (2003). Intensity-dependent activation of the primary auditory cortex in functional magnetic resonance imaging. Journal of computer assisted tomography 27(2): 213-8. Laurienti, P. J., A. S. Field, J. H. Burdette, J. A. Maldjian, Y. F. Yen and D. M. Moody (2002). Dietary caffeine consumption modulates fMRI measures. Neuroimage 17(2): 751-7. Lavenex, P. and D. G. Amaral (2000). Hippocampal-neocortical interaction: a hierarchy of associativity. Hippocampus 10(4): 420-30. Lerner, Y., T. Hendler, D. Ben-Bashat, M. Hare] and R. Malach (2001). A hierarchical axis of object processing stages in the human visual cortex. Cereb Cortex 11(4): 287-97. Liu, T. T., Y. Behzadi, K. Restom, K Uludag, K Lu, G. T. Buracas, D. J. Dubowitz and R. B. Buxton (2004). Caffeine alters the temporal dynamics of the visual BOLD response. Neuroimage 23(4): 1402-13. Livingstone, M. S. and D. H. Hubel (1984a). Anatomy and physiology of a color system in the primate visual cortex. J Neurosci 4(1): 309-56. Livingstone, M. S. and D. H. Hubel (1984b). Specificity of intrinsic connections in primate primary visual cortex. J Neurosci 4(1 1): 2830-5. 143 Lockhart, R S. (2002). Levels of processing, transfer-appropriate processing, and the concept of robust encoding. Memory (Hove, England) 10(5—6): 397-403. Logothetis, N. K (2002). The neural basis of the blood-oxygen-level-dependent functional magnetic resonance imaging signal. Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London 357(1424): 1003-37. Logothetis, N. K. (2003). The underpinnings of the BOLD functional magnetic resonance imaging signal. J Neurosci 23(10): 3963-71. Logothetis, N. K., J. Pauls, M. Augath, T. Trinath and A. Oeltermann (2001). Neurophysiological investigation of the basis of the flVIRI signal. Nature 412(6843): 150-7. Logothetis, N. K and J. Pfeuffer (2004). On the nature of the BOLD flVIRI contrast mechanism. Magnetic resonance imaging 22(10): 1517-31. Logothetis, N. K and B. A. Wandell (2004). Interpreting the BOLD signal. Annual review of physiology 66: 735-69. London, M. and M. Hausser (2005). Dendritic computation. Annual review of neuroscience 28: 503-32. Maier, N., V. Nimmrich and A. Draguhn (2003). Cellular and network mechanisms underlying spontaneous sharp wave-ripple complexes in mouse hippocampal slices. T he Journal of physiology 550(Pt 3): 873-87. Mallot, H. A. and F. Giannakopoulos (1996). Population networks: a large-scale framework for modelling cortical neural networks. Biological cybemetics 75(6): 441-52. Martin, E., P. Joeri, T. Loenneker, D. Ekatodramis, D. Vitacco, J. Hennig and V. L. Marcar (1999). Visual processing in infants and children studied using fiJnctional MRI. Pediatric research 46(2): 135-40. McClelland, J. L., B. L. McNaughton and R. C. O'Reilly (1995). Why there are complementary learning systems in the hippocampus and neocortex: insights from the successes and failures of connectionist models of learning and memory. Psychological review 102(3): 419-57. McGaugh, J. L. (2000). Memory--a century of consolidation. Science (New York, NY 287(5451): 248-51. 144 McNaughton, N. and J. Wickens (2003). Hebb, pandemonium and catastrophic hypermnesia: the hippocampus as a suppressor of inappropriate associations. Cortex; a journal devoted to the stuafv of the nervous system and behavior 39(4- 5): 1139-63. Messenger, J. B. (1971). Two-stage recovery of a response in Sepia. Nature 232(5307): 202-3. Meunier, M., J. Bachevalier, M. Mishkin and E. A. Murray (1993). Effects on visual recognition of combined and separate ablations of the entorhinal and perirhinal cortex in rhesus monkeys. J Neurosci 13(12): 5418-32. Miyashita, Y., M. Morita, Y. Naya, M. Yoshida and H. Tomita (1998). Backward signal from medial temporal lobe in neural circuit reorganization of primate inferotemporal cortex. Comptes rendus de l'Academie des sciences 321(2-3): 185- 92. Montaldi, D., T. J. Spencer, N. Roberts and A. R. Mayes (2006). The neural system that mediates familiarity memory. Hippocampus 16(5): 504-20. Moscovitch, M., L. Nadel, G. Winocur, A. Gilboa and R. S. Rosenbaum (2006). The cognitive neuroscience of remote episodic, semantic and spatial memory. Current opinion in neurobiology 16(2): 179-90. Moser, M. B. and E. 1. Moser (1998). Functional differentiation in the hippocampus. Hippocampus 8(6): 608-19. Mulligan, N. W. and J. P. Lozito (2006). An asymmetry between memory encoding and retrieval. Revelation, generation, and transfer-appropriate processing. Psychol Sci 17(1): 7-11. Nadel, L. and C. Land (2000). Memory traces revisited. Nature reviews 1(3): 209-12. Nadel, L., A. Samsonovich, L. Ryan and M. Moscovitch (2000). Multiple trace theory of human memory: computational, neuroimaging, and neuropsychological results. Hippocampus 10(4): 352-68. Naveh-Benjamin, M., A. Kilb and T. Fisher (2006). Concurrent task effects on memory encoding and retrieval: further support for an asymmetry. Memory & cognition 34(1): 90-101. Naya, Y., M. Yoshida and Y. Miyashita (2003a). Forward processing of long-term associative memory in monkey inferotemporal cortex. J Neurosci 23(7): 2861-71. 145 Naya, Y., M. Yoshida, M. Takeda, R. Fujimichi and Y. Miyashita (2003b). Delay-period activities in two subdivisions of monkey inferotemporal cortex during pair association memory task. The European journal of neuroscience 18(10): 2915-8. Nobre, A. C., G. N. Sebestyen, D. R. Gitelman, M. M. Mesulam, R. S. Frackowiak and C. D. Frith (1997). Functional localization of the system for visuospatial attention using positron emission tomography. Brain 120 ( Pt 3): 515-33. Ogawa, S., T. M. Lee, A. R. Kay and D. W. Tank (1990). Brain magnetic resonance imaging with contrast dependent on blood oxygenation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 87(24): 9868-72. Oldfield, R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia 9(1): 97-1 13. Orban, G. A. and R. Vogels (1998). The neuronal machinery involved in successive orientation discrimination. Progress in neurobiology 55(2): 117-47. Pammer, K, R. Lavis and P. Comelissen (2004). Visual encoding mechanisms and their relationship to text presentation preference. Dyslexia (Chichester, England) 10(2): 77-94. Polat, U. and A. M. Norcia (1998). Elongated physiological summation pools in the human visual cortex. Vision research 38(23): 3735-41. Poldrack, R. A. (2000). Imaging brain plasticity: conceptual and methodological issues--a theoretical review. Neuroimage 12( l ): 1-13. Preston, A. R. and J. D. Gabrieli (2002). Different fianctions for different medial temporal lobe structures? Learning & memory (Cold Spring Harbor, N. Y 9(5): 215-7. Qiu, F. T. and R. von der Heydt (2005). Figure and ground in the visual cortex: v2 combines stereoscopic cues with gestalt rules. Neuron 47(1): 155-66. Raichle, M. E. (1998). Behind the scenes of functional brain imaging: a historical and physiological perspective. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 95(3): 765-72. Ranganath, C., M. K. Johnson and M. D'Esposito (2003). Prefrontal activity associated with working memory and episodic long-term memory. Neuropsychologia 41(3): 378-89. 146 Rao, R. P. (2005). Bayesian inference and attentional modulation in the visual cortex. Neuroreport 16(16): 1843—8. Robb, W. G. and M. D. Rugg (2002). Electrophysiological dissociation of retrieval orientation and retrieval effort. Psychonomic bulletin & review 9(3): 583-9. Rolls, E. T. (2000). Memory systems in the brain. Annual review of psychology 51: 599- 630. Rosen, B. R., R. L. Buckner and A. M. Dale (1998). Event-related fiJnctional MRI: past, present, and future. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 95(3): 773-80. Ross, R. S. and H. Eichenbaum (2006). Dynamics of hippocampal and cortical activation during consolidation of a nonspatial memory. J Neurosci 26(18): 4852-9. Rossion, B., C. Schiltz and M. Crommelinck (2003). The fimctionally defined right occipital and fusiform "face areas" discriminate novel from visually familiar faces. Neuroimage 19(3): 877-83. Rubin, D. C., S. Hinton and A. Wenzel (1999). The precise time course of retention. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 25(5): 1161-1176. Rudy, J. W. and P. Matus-Amat (2005). The ventral hippocampus supports a memory representation of context and contextual fear conditioning: implications for a unitary fiinction of the hippocampus. Behav Neurosci 119(1): 154-63. Rudy, J. W. and P. Morledge (1994). Ontogeny of contextual fear conditioning in rats: implications for consolidation, infantile amnesia, and hippocampal system function. Behav Neurosci 108: 227-234. Rugg, M. D. and E. L. Wilding (2000). Retrieval processing and episodic memory. Trends Cogn Sci 4(3): 108-115. Saad, Z. 8., K. M. Ropella, E. A. DeYoe and P. A. Bandettini (2003). The spatial extent of the BOLD response. Neuroimage 19(1): 132-44. Sakai, K and Y. Miyashita (1991). Neural organization for the long-term memory of paired associates. Nature 354(6349): 152-5. Sahnond, C. H., J. Ashburner, F. Vargha-Khadem, A. Connelly, D. G. Gadian and K J. Friston (2002). Distributional assumptions in voxel-based morphometry. Neuroimage 17(2): 1027-30. 147 Sanders, G. D. and J. J. Barlow (1971). Variations in retention performance during long term memory formation. Nature 232(5307): 203-4. Save, E., L. Nerad and B. Poucet (2000). Contribution of multiple sensory information to place field stability in hippocampal place cells. Hippocampus 10(1): 64-76. Save, E. and B. Poucet (2000a). Hippocampal-parietal cortical interactions in spatial cognition. Hippocampus 10(4): 491-9. Save, E. and B. Poucet (2000b). Involvement of the hippocampus and associative parietal cortex in the use of proximal and distal landmarks for navigation. Behavioural brain research 109(2): 195-206. Savoy, R. L. (2005). Experimental design in brain activation MRI: cautionary tales. Brain research bulletin 67(5): 361-7. Schacter, D. L., R. L. Buckner, W. Koutstaal, A. M. Dale and B. R. Rosen (1997). Late onset of anterior prefrontal activity during true and false recognition: an event- related fMRI study. Neuroimage 6(4): 259-69. Schacter, D. L. and E. Tulving (1994). What are the memory systems of 1994? Memory Systems 1994. D. L. Schacter and E. Tulving. Cambridge, MIT Press: 1-38. Scouten, A., X. Papademetris and R. T. Constable (2006). Spatial resolution, signal-to- noise ratio, and smoothing in multi-subject fiinctional MRI studies. Neuroimage 30(3): 787-93. Segev, I. (2006). What do dendrites and their synapses tell the neuron? Journal of neurophysiology 95(3): 1295-7. Shannon, C. E. (1948). A mathematical theory of communication. Bell Sys Tech J 27: 379-423, 623-656. Shepherd, G. M. (1996). The dendritic spine: a multifunctional integrative unit. Journal of neurophysiology 75(6): 2197-210. Shepherd, G. M. and R. K Brayton (1987). Logic operations are properties of computer- simulated interactions between excitable dendritic spines. Neuroscience 21(1): 151-65. Siapas, A. G. and M. A. Wilson (1998). Coordinated interactions between hippocampal ripples and cortical spindles during slow-wave sleep. Neuron 21(5): 1123-8. 148 Squire, L. R. and P. Alvarez (1995). Retrograde amnesia and memory consolidation: a neurobiological perspective. Current opinion in neurobiology 5(2): 169-77. Stenberg, G., M. Johansson and I. Rosen (2006). Conceptual and perceptual memory: retrieval orientations reflected in event-related potentials. Acta psychologica 122(2): 174-205. Stickgold, R., L. Scott, C. Rittenhouse and J. A. Hobson (1999). Sleep-induced changes in associative memory. Journal of cognitive neuroscience 11(2): 182-93. Summerfield, C., M. Greene, T. Wager, T. Egner, J. Hirsch and J. Mangels (2006). Neocortical connectivity during episodic memory formation. PLoS biology 4(5): 6128. Talairach, J. and P. Tournoux (1988). Co-planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain. New York, Thieme. Tanaka, S., M. Honda and N. Sadato (2005). Modality-specific cognitive function of medial and lateral human Brodmann area 6. J Neurosci 25(2): 496-501. Tassoni, G., C. A. Lorenzini, E. Baldi, B. Sacchetti and C. Bucherelli (1999). A peculiar pattern of temporal involvement of rat perirhinal cortex in memory processing. Behav Neurosci 113(6): 1161-9. Treves, A. and E. T. Rolls (1994). Computational analysis of the role of the hippocampus in memory. Hippocampus 4(3): 374-91. Tse, D., R. F. Langston, M. Kakeyama, 1. Bethus, P. A. Spooner, E. R. Wood, M. P. Witter and R. G. Morris (2007). Schemas and memory consolidation. Science (New York, N. Y 316(5821): 76-82. Tulving, E. (1983). Elements of Episodic Memory. Oxford, Clarendon Press. Uncapher, M. R. and M. D. Rugg (2005a). Effects of divided attention on MRI correlates of memory encoding. Journal of cognitive neuroscience 17(12): 1923- 35. Uncapher, M. R. and M. D. Rugg (2005b). Encoding and the durability of episodic memory: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. J Neurosci 25(31): 7260-7. 149 Ungerleider, L. G. and M. Mishkin (1982). Two cortical visual systems. Analysis of Visual Behavior. D. J. Ingle, M. A. Goodale and R. J. W. Mansfield. Cabridge, MA, MIT Press: 549-586. Vaidya, C. J., M. Zhao, J. E. Desmond and J. D. Gabrieli (2002). Evidence for cortical encoding specificity in episodic memory: memory-induced re-activation of picture processing areas. Neuropsychologia 40(12): 2136-43. Verzi, D. W. (2004). Modeling activity-dependent synapse restructuring. Bulletin of mathematical biology 66(4): 745-62. Verzi, D. W., M. B. Rheuben and S. M. Baer (2005). Impact of time-dependent changes in spine density and spine shape on the input-output properties of a dendritic branch: a computational study. Journal of neurophysiology 93(4): 2073-89. Walker, M. P. (2004). Issues surrounding sleep-dependent memory consolidation and plasticity. Cell Mol Life Sci 61(24): 3009-15. Walker, M. P. and R. Stickgold (2004). Sleep-dependent learning and memory consolidation. Neuron 44( 1): 121-33. Wang, H., Y. Hu and J. Z. Tsien (2006). Molecular and systems mechanisms of memory consolidation and storage. Progress in neurobiology 79(3): 123-35. Ward, B. D. (1998). 3dDeconvolve Reference Manual. At_1as of Fugctional Neuroimaging (AFNI) Suite. NIMI-I. Weis, S., P. Klaver, J. Reul, C. E. Elger and G. Fernandez (2004). Neural correlates of successful declarative memory formation and retrieval: the anatomical overlap. Cortex; a journal devoted to the stuay of the nervous system and behavior 40(1): 200-2. Weldon, M. S., H. L. Roediger, 3rd and B. H. Challis (1989). The properties of retrieval cues constrain the picture superiority effect. Memory & cognition 17(1): 95-105. Wheeler, M. E. and R. L. Buckner (2003). Functional dissociation among components of remembering: control, perceived oldness, and content. J Neurosci 23(9): 3869-80. Wichmann, W. and W. Muller-Forell (2004). Anatomy of the visual system. European journal of radiology 49(1): 8-30. Wickelgren, W. A. (1974). Single-trace fragility theory of memory dynamics. Memory & cognition 2: 775—780. 150 Winters, B. D. and T. J. Bussey (2005a). Glutamate receptors in perirhinal cortex mediate encoding, retrieval, and consolidation of object recognition memory. J Neurosci 25(17): 4243-51. Winters, B. D. and T. J. Bussey (2005b). Removal of cholinergic input to perirhinal cortex disrupts object recognition but not spatial working memory in the rat. The European journal of neuroscience 21(8): 2263-70. Winters, B. D. and T. J. Bussey (2005c). Transient inactivation of perirhinal cortex disrupts encoding, retrieval, and consolidation of object recognition memory. J Neurosci 25(1): 52-61. Winters, B. D., S. E. Forwood, R. A. Cowell, L. M. Saksida and T. J. Bussey (2004). Double dissociation between the effects of peri-postrhinal cortex and hippocampal lesions on tests of object recognition and spatial memory: heterogeneity of firnction within the temporal lobe. J Neurosci 24(26): 5901-8. Witter, M. P. and E. I. Moser (2006). Spatial representation and the architecture of the entorhinal cortex. Trends in neurosciences 29(12): 671—8. Wixted, J. T. (2004). The psychology and neuroscience of forgetting. Ann Rev Psychol 55: 235-269. Woodruff, C. C., M. R. Uncapher and M. D. Rugg (2006). Neural correlates of differential retrieval orientation: Sustained and item-related components. Neuropsychologia 44(14): 3000-10. Woodruff, M. L. and H. M. Kantor (1983). Fomix lesions, plasma ACTH levels, and shuttle box avoidance in rats. Behav Neurosci 97(6): 897-907. Yamaguchi, S., S. Yamagata and S. Kobayashi (2000). Cerebral asymmetry of the "top- down" allocation of attention to global and local features. J Neurosci 20(9): RC72. Yovel, G. and N. Kanwisher (2005). The neural basis of the behavioral face-inversion effect. Curr Biol 15(24): 2256-62. Yuste, R. and W. Denk (1995). Dendritic spines as basic firnctional units of neuronal integration. Nature 375(6533): 682-4. Zador, A., C. Koch and T. H. Brown (1990). Biophysical model of a Hebbian synapse. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 87(17): 6718-22. 151 Zhang, F., S. Endo, L. J. Cleary, A. Eskin and J. H. Byrne (1997). Role of transforming growth factor-beta in long-term synaptic facilitation in Aplysia. Science (New York, NY 275(5304): 1318-20. Zola-Morgan, S., L. R. Squire, D. G. Amaral and W. A. Suzuki (1989). Lesions of perirhinal and parahippocampal cortex that spare the amygdala and hippocampal formation produce severe memory impairment. J Neurosci 9( 12): 4355-70. 152 Itilllllflllliljfiflilfllt1111111171