A STUDY 0F ENTERNATiONAL THREA? Thesis for the flame or! M. .43.. MECHEGAN STA‘E‘E UNE‘4’ERSEW J'a‘é‘an (10%, fir. W635 IHEIIB LIBRARY Michigan Stave University ABSTRACT A STUWY OF INTERNATIONAL THREAT by John Cobb, Jr. This is a study of sources of international threat as perceived by people of different nationalities. It attempts to establish certain correlates of international threat in terms of country characteristics which include ideology, size of pOpulation and per capita wealth. It is assumed here that nations may be defined as organizations, i.e., that the citizens of any nation are interconnected by channels of communication and that they exhibit certain regularities of behavior. One of these regularities is assumed to be a high degree of consensus cone cerning which other nations are threatening or dangerous. Perceived in- ternational threat is defined here as a mechanism which controls the growth of an international community of nations. A sample of 24 countries was selected which was stratified by ideology (Communist and non-Communist), population (large and small), and level of per capita energy consumption (rich, medium and poor). These three country characteristics-~plus the nationality of the subject-~were hypothesized to be related to how threatening a country is judged to be. The sample of subjects included peOple of five different nationalities. Except for a special subsample of experts in international relations, the sample was made up of graduate students at Michigan State University. Nationality subsamples were made up of Four persons each, and the entire sample consisted of only 2n peOple. With such extremely John Cobb, Jr. small and non-random subsamples, no claim can be made that findings are representative of the five nationalities studied (Brazilians, Egyptians, Indians, Formosan Chinese and Americans). To operationalize international threat subjects were asked to reSpond to a 21-point scale marked "safe/not threatening" at one end and "dangerous/ very threatening" at the other. Subjects made an X on this scale for each of the 2n countries to indicate how threatening they judged it to be. These responses were coded as scores of 0—20 with 20 representing maximum threat. These threat scores were analyzed in various ways. A Q factor analysis of these sets of reSponses produced a five-factor solution in which American, Indian and Brazilian "types" of responses were discernable, plus two Egyptian "types." This offers an indication that the nationality of the respondent was of importance--(83% of the variance was accounted for)--in defining which countries are judged as threatening and not threatening. It also suggests that despite the lack of sampling pro- cedures, the fact that three of the nationality subsamples are indicated to be different "types" of people lends some substance to the hope that nationality differences and similarities have been tapped. The analytic technique used to test most hypotheses was a factorial analysis of variance. For this sample-owhich included subjects from "neutralist" countries such as Egypt and India--Communist countries were seen as more threatening than non-Communist countries, and large countries were perceived to be more threatening than small countries, regardless of ideology. The nationality of the subject again proved to be a strong effect influencing judgements of international threat. John Cobb, Jr. The per capita wealth or energy consumption of a country was also found to influence judgements about threat, but its effects were far more complex than those hypothesized. This variable interacted with all of the others in the analysis, and severely restricted the statistical in- terpretation of the analysis of variance of threat scores. In general, poor countries were seen as most threatening, rich countries as next most threatening and countries of intermediate wealth as least threatening. Among the three country characteristics examined, ideology was found to have the strongest influence upon threat judgements. But the nationality of the subject was an even stronger influence. A STUDY OF INTERNATIONAL THREAT by John Cobb, Jr. A THESIS Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of MASTER OF ARTS Department of Communication 1964 ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS I wish to express my appreciation to my advisor, Dr. Malcolm S. MacLean, Jr. for guidance and assistance with this study from planning 'to presentation; to Dr. Eugene Jacobson, Dr. Randall P. Harrison and Dr. John T. McNelly for constructive criticism and encouragementt to Albert Talbott for assistance with the analysis of variance design and for supervision of data processing; and to Mrs. Shirley Sherman for typing this thesis. ii CHAPTER I II III IV TABLE OF CONTENTS INTRODUCTION: A Theoretical Exploration of the Concept Of Threat e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e 0 Individual Organization . . . . Perception of Threat Stress and Strain e e e e e e e e Perceived Threat in an International Context VARIABLES AND HYPOTHESES Threat, Size and Wealth Myths and Realities . . Nationality as a Variable Hypotheses O O O O O 0 Threat and Nationality of O O O O O 0 Respondent O O O O O O 0 "Myths and Realities" Operationalized Threat and Perceived Similarity . . . Threat and world Government . . . PROCEDURES AND METHODS OF IdeolOgy POPUlatiOn e e e e 0 Health and Energy . . The Sample of People Analytical Procedures Othar Analyses e e e Criteria Data . . . . World Government . . O O O O O 0 O O O O O O O O RESULm O 0 O O O O O O 0 Analysis of Variance of Ideology POPUlfltiOn e e e e e e IdeolOgy and Population Energy 0 e e e e e e e Ideology and Energy . . Population and Energy . ANALYSIS 0 O O O O O O O Threat Scores 0 O O O O O O o e O O Ideology, Population and Energy Nationality Subsamples Energy and Nationality Ideology, Energy and Nationality Population, Energy and Nationality Some Additional Findings iii 0 O O O O O O O O O O 0 Ideology of Country and Nationality Population of Country and Nationality Multiple Correlations . . . . . . . O O O 0 O O O O of Subject 0 0 O O O O O O O O O O O I O O O O O C O O O O O O O O O O O O 0 O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O C O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O C O O O O O O O O O O C O O O O O O O O O C O C O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O of Subject 0 O O O O O O O O O O O O 0 so... 0 O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O 0 Page 10 13 16 19 22 23 25 27 29 29 31 32 33 3“ 35 36 39 “2 an “5 “6 #7 52 5k 55 56 57 60 61 63 66 68 7O 73 75 77 83 85 CHAPTER TABLE OF CONTENTS QANCIYSISeeeeeeee Similarity e e e e a e e 0 Correlations with Criteria World Government . . . . . V DISCUSSION . . . . . . . . 0 REFERENCES . BIBLIOGRAPHY O O O O 0 Q 0 Q 5 5 . APPENDIX A: Ideology . . POPUlation e e a e e e e 0 Energy a a e e e e e e e Nationality a e e e e e e Questionnaire e a e e e 0 iv (continued) 0 O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O 0 O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O Page 86 90 91 93 96 97 100 102 106 115 117 121 TABLE 8a 8b 9a 9b 10a 10b lla 11b 12a 12b 13. lua lab 15 LIST OF TABLES The Country Sample with Criteria Data. . . . . . . Age and Field of Study of Subjects . . . . . . . . Average Threat Scores for Subjects and Countries . Per Capita Energy Consumption Averages for SUbjCCt. .nd CCUDtPICQ o o o o o o o o s o o o o 0 Average Population Estimates for Subjects and Countries o o o o e o o a o a o s o a s s s o o o Average Ideology Estimates for Countries . . . . . Summary of Four Analyses of Variance . . . . . . . Ideology: Average Throat Scores for Communist and non-Communist Countries o o o o o s o o s o o o o “‘1“ Ideology EffCCt o o o e o o a o o s o s o 0 Population: Average Throat Scores for Large and 5m.11 countrIO‘ o o o e o o o o o o o o o o o o o M‘In POPUl‘tIUD Bff‘Ct o e s o o e o o o o o s o o Ideology and Population: Average Throat Scores. . Ideology-Population Interaction . . . . . . . . . Energy: Average Throat Scores for Exporimontor- Classified High, Medium and Low Energy Countries . Main Enorgy EffOCt o s s o o o o o o o o o o o o o Enorgy: Average Throat Scores for Subject-Classified High, Hedium and Low Energy Countries- . . . . . . Main Energy Effect, Subject-Classified Analysis . Experimenter Classification of Countiros by Per Capita EUCng CODIUIPti.n e s e o e o o s o s o s Ideology and Energy: Average Threat Scores . . . . Ideology-Energy Interaction . . . . . . . . . . . . Ideology and Energy: Average Threat Scores, Subject- Clasaified An‘lysis o s o o o o o o s o o o s o o a V Page 38 “0 Q7 49 SO 51 53 SH 55 55 S6 56 56 57 S7 58 58 59 60 60 61 LIST OF TABLES (continued) TABLE Page 16a Papulation and Energy: Average Threat Scores . . . . . 61 16b Population-Energy Interaction . . . . . . . . . . . . 61 17 Countries Experimenter Classified by Energy Level and POpulatiOn o o e e e e e e e e e e o e o e e e e o 62 18 Population and Energy: Average Threat Scores, Subject-Classified Analysis . . . . . . . . . . . . . 62 19a Ideology, Population and Energy: Average Throat Scores e e e e e e e e e e o e e e e e o e e e e e e o 63 19b Ideology-Population-Energy Interaction . . . . . . . . 63 20 Countries Experimenter Classified by Ideology, Population and Energy e e e e e o e o e e e e e e e e 6“ 21 LOVGIS Of Perceived Threat e e e e e e e e e e o e e o 65 22a Ideology, Population and Energy: Average Threat Scores, Subject-Classified Analysis . . . . . . . . . 66 22b Ideology-Population-Energy Interaction, Subject- CIESBifin Analysis e e e o e o o e e e e o e e e e e 66 23a Nationality: Average Threat Scores for Six Subsanples. 67 23b “310 Nationality EffOCt e e e e e e e e e e o o e o e 67 2ua Ideology and Nationality: Average Threat Scores . . . 68 2% Ideology-Nationality Interaction . . . . . . . . . . . 68 25a Ideology and Nationality: Average Threat Scores, Subject-Classified Analysis e e e e e e e o e e o e e 69 25b Ideology-Nationality Interaction, Subject- C13831fied AhllYSia e e e e e e e e e e o e e o e e e 69 26a Population and Nationality: Average Threat Scores . . 70 26b Population-Nationality Interaction . . . . . . . . . . 70 27a Ideology, Population and Nationality: Average Threat Scores e e e e o o e e o e e e e e e e e e e e 72 27b Ideology-PopulationcNationality Interaction . . . . . 72 vi TABLE 28a 28b 29a 29b 30a 30b 31a 31b 32a 32b 32c 33 3k 35 36 37 38 39 NO 1:1 “2 LIST OF TABLES (continued) Energy and Nationality: Average Threat Scores . . . . Energy-Nationality Interaction . . . . . . . . . . . Energy and Nationality: Average Threat Scores, SUbjGCt'CICSSified Analy313 e e e e e e e e e e o e e Energy-Nationality Interaction, Subject- ClflS'ifiCd AnalySis o e o e e e e e e e o e e e o e e Ideology, Energy and Nationality: Average Threat Scores Ideology-Energy-Nationality Interaction . . . . . . . Population, Energy and Nationality: Average Threat Scores . . . . . . . o . . . . . . . . . . . . . e e Population-Energy-Nationality Interaction . . . . . . Pour-factor Interaction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Pmfihamefihm:MwnemmuS«ms.... Summary, Experimenter-Classified Analysis . . . . . . Differences Between Two Analyses . . . . . . . . . . Intro-Individual Correlations Between Sets of Threat Scores and Sets of Ideology, Population and Energy Estimates . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . e Multiple Correlations: Three Predictors of Threat . . Q Analysis: Rotated Factor Loading: . . . . . . . . . More and Less Threatening Countries for Nationality TYPO: e a a e e e e o e e e e e e o e e e e e e e o o Correlations of Similarity Estimates with Threat and Ideology Estimates e e e o e e e e e e e e e e e o e Correlations of Subjects' Estimates and U.N. Criteria Data e e e e o e e o e e o e e e e e e o e e e e e o Classification Scores by Country Characteristics . . 0 Mean Threat Scores for Subjects Classified by Responses to World Government Statements . . . . . . . . . . . Subsample Means for world Government and Threat Scores vii Page 73 7“ 7a 75 76 76 77 77 78 80 81 82 8k 85 87 90 92 93 94 9M CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION A Theoretical Exploration of the Concept of Threat Note: This chapter examines various sources of theory and research in an effort to provide a broad theoretical field in which perceived international threat may be defined and studied. A communication model derived from cybernetic theory is proposed. The variables and hypotheses of immediate theoretical concern in this study are discussed in Chapter II. The purpose of this study is to investigate the relationship be— tween perceived threat and danger represented by a given country or class of countries and the nationality of the perceiver. There will also be an effort to ascertain the relationship between degree of perceived threat and certain country characteristics such as national population, per capita wealth, and political ideology. In this study, threat is the dependent variable of major concern. How is threat to be dealt with in theoretical terms? Efforts to study threat have been made at various levels of generality. Several of these levels will be examined here. The most abstract efforts to include threat in scientific theory have been in the field of cybernetics. Here efforts have been made to formulate theory which deals with communication and control in a wide diversity of systems--astronomical and chemical as well as biological and social. The notion of threat is used in this body of theory in terms of defining the elements of an on-going system or organization. This study is concerned with three levels of organizations: (1) individuals, (2) nations, and (3) groups or blocs of nations, so this theoretical approach should be of some value here. A In dealing with organisations in general, cybernetic theorists make several axiomatic assumptions. In The_Human Use of Human Dermal, Norbert Wiener says, "...certain organisms, such as man, tend for a time to maintain and often even to increase the level of their organization... The process by which we living beings resist the general stream of corruption and decay is known as homeostasis... The oxygen and carbon dioxide and salt in our blood, the hormones flowing from our ductless glands, all are regulated by mechanisms which tend to resist any untoward changes in their levels. These mechanisms constitute what is known as homeostasis, and are negative feedback mechanisms." (1) Neiner goes on to say, "It is the pattern maintained by this homeostasis which is the touchstone of our personal identity... We are but whirlpools in a river of overflowing water. No are not stuff that abides, but patterns that perpetuate themselves." (2) In this approach, both personality and culture may be conceptualized as patterns in the process of transmission which change as they are constrained by communication with other patterns. H. Ross Ashby, another cybernetic theorist, suggests that external threats and disturbances are not necessary for defining organization, p=£_22, but that they are necessary in defining a viable organization or an organism which acts so as to further it's own survival. Ashby says that "there is not any property of an organization that is 'good' in any absolute sense; all are relative to some given environment, or to some given set of threats or disturbances, or to some given set of problems.” (3) ' He also says that "the organization (whether of a cat or an automatic pilot or an oil refinery) is judged 'good' if and only if it acts so as to keep an assigned set of variables, the 'essential' variables, within assigned limits." (fl) Here an effort will be made to develop a communication model of the international situation in terms of Ashby's cybernetic theory. It will be conceptualized as a situation in which previously isolated, independent systems become interconnected in a larger system. ”An example," of such a situation, says Ashby, "is the embryo nervous system, which starts with cells having little or no effect on one another, and changes, by the growth of dendrytes and formation of synapses, to one in which each part's behavior is very much affected by the other parts." (5) The proposed communication model would be one which starts with nation-states having little effect upon one another, and changes, by the growth of communication to one in which each country's behavior is very much affected by that of the other countries. In this view it is the establishment of increasingly large communi- cation channels and flow of messages between countries which has replaced relative independence with conditionality and imposed new constraints upon the behaviors of countries. Here it is assumed that to the extent that channels of international communication exist, to that extent international organization exists in that constraints have been imposed upon what were relatively in- dependent parts. (Of course, it is not the formal, conscious, planned sort of international organization that is being discussed here.) In these terms, organization need only be defined as conditionality between parts and regularity of behavior. A further implicit assumption is the one already implied in the definition of homeostasis: systems or or— ganizations tend toward states of equilibrium. In this theoretical context, threat has no intrinsic qualities, but, rather, expresses a relationship. First, an organization must be defined as a number of interdependent parts: the behavior of each part is constrained by the behavior of other parts. Each part has a number of psosible states: each variable has a number of values. The laws or regularities of the system are such that certain values of states are preferred over others. A threat, then, is any element or event which tends to drive the outcome away from the preferred values of the system's variables. For a mammal, body temperature must be maintained within certain limits. Anything which tends to push body temperature too high or too low represents a threat to the organism. For a business organization, income and expenses must be kept in balance. Anything which pushes income below expenses represents a threat to the organization. To describe the proposed model of international relations further, two more concepts must be introduced: boundary maintenance and systemic linkage. The tendency of a system to maintain its independence and relative isolation from other systems will be called boundary maintenance. The tendency for a system to increase its communication and thus its interdependence with other systems will be called systemic linkage. An extreme example of boundary maintenance in the international sphere is the present 0.5. policy toward Cuba. An example of systemic linkage is the change of status of Hawaii and Alaska from territories to states. Other examples of systemic linkage are the installation of the ”hot line" between Washington and Moscow, and the proposed commercial flights between New York and Moscow. In general, an increase in communication and trade indicates systemic linkage, and an absence, or a reduction in these things indicates boundary maintenance. Here it will be assumed that if a country is perceived as threatening or dangerous, a policy of boundary maintenance will be carried out. If a country is not seen as threatening or dangerous, the potentiality of increased communication and interdependence exists. In this view threat is a negative feedback mechanism which results in the reduction of systemic linkage. Such an idea is neither new nor novel. In a book published in lens, two English anthropoIOgists discussed social change in terms of equilibrium and disequilibrium: "The positive or cultural forces of social change are new ideals, ideas and intuitions of beauty, and the discovery of new uses for material resources, of new facts and of new techniques... "The negative or structural forces are intollerable opposition between groups and categories of peOple that threaten, unless some change is quickly made, to destroy law, logic and convention. These are the forces of dis- equilibrium." (6) Godfry and Monica Wilson go on to say that, "So long as foreigners are regarded as dangerous enemies to whom one has no obligations, trade or other cooperation with them is impossible." (7) Some insight into the nature of international isolation and independence as contrasted with the interdependence created by communi- cation with other countries is offered by the following discussion of Tibet by the current Dalai Lama: "Perhaps the best known quality of Tibet in the recent past was its deliberate isolation. In the world outside, Lhasa was often called the Forbidden City. There were two reasons for this withdrawal from the world. The first, of course, was that the country is naturally isolated. Until the last decade, the route from the border of India or Nepal to Lhasa was a journey of two months across high Himalayan passes which were blocked for a large part of the year. From my birthplace in the borderland between Tibet and China the journey to Lhasa was even longer, as I have already told-~and that borderland itself was over a thousand miles from the sea coast and ports of China. Isolation was therefore in our blood. we increased our natural isolation by allowing the fewest possible foreigners into our country, simply because we had had experience of strife, especially with China, and had no ambition whatever except to live in peace and pursue our own culture and religion, and we thought to hold ourselves entirely aloof from the world was the best way of ensuring peace. I must say at once that I think this policy was always a mistake, and my hope and intention is that in the future the gates of Tibet will be kept wide open to welcome visitors from every part of the world." (8) Here a change from a policy of isolation to a policy of increasing external communication is suggested. Visitors from fereign countries were judged as dangerous: now they are considered safe. A policy of boundary maintenance has been abandoned in favor of a policy of systemic linkage. These opposite sorts of behavior are discernable in many kinds of systems. In molecular systems with high mutual attraction like molecules stick together and expose the least possible surface areas to foreign contact. An example is a ball of mercury. In molecular systems of high capillarity, there is a strong attraction for alien molecules: water spreads out and clings to glass. Nations in this century rarely assume that the total external environment is threatening and maintain the extreme policy of isolation pursued by Tibet. More often such a social system discriminates some ' elements in-its environment which are relatively safe or benign from other elements which are relatively threatening or dangerous. And it usually has in its repertory of responses both adaptive and defensive reactions to unfamiliar or changed aspects of the environment. From the theoretical viewpoint which has been developed here, threat or danger posed by other countries provides negative feedback which restrains and controls the growth of a global international system made up of nation states. It is also possible with such a model to use another unit of analysis, and consider the "Communist bloc" and the ”non-Communist world" as organizations or systems. There is certainly more communication and commerce within these groups of countries than between them. In terms of the model developed here, the degree of organization existing in the non-Communist world should be indicated in consensus among individuals in non-Communist countries that Communist countries are threatening. However, not as much organization would be expected at this level of analysis as would be expected when nations are the units of analysis. Stated in general terms, the model deveIOped here assumes that personalities, national cultures and larger patterns of culture may all be conceptualized as patterns in the process of transmission which maintain stability by homeostasis. They change and modify themselves if, and only if, they are constrained by communication with other patterns. Thus both stability and change are taken into account in this communication model at various levels of organization. (It is interesting to note that in describing A ngss-Polity Survgy, Banks and Textor state that the basic concept used is "pattern.") (9) As communication imposes conditionalities or constraints upon the behavior of each individual, he may be considered as an interdependent part of a system. Thus the clan, tribe, feudal community and nation may be considered systems to the extent that the communication net imposes constraints upon the behavior of its members. The socialized individual finds that to maintain his essential biological variables in equilibrium, it is necessary to maintain equilibrium within one or more larger social systems of which the in- dividual is a constrained or interdependent part. The present age of nationalism may be defined in terms of the formalized boundary maintenance which takes place at national borderS. This suggests that national communication networks impose constraints upon the attitudes of their members concerning external threats. In the terms used here, one index of national organization would be consensus concerning a pattern of external threats. Individual Organization It is assumed here that individual and social change is always mediated by communication. How does this operate at the level of individuals? Theodore Newcomb sees interpersonal communication as an ABX system in which each message modifies the state of the two-person system in the direction of symetry. In terms of what has been said previously, this "process of co-orientation" can be viewed as change in a two-person social system which is mediated by communication. Just what happens when a new and relatively novel message is received? The following description is in the nature of a hypothesis which owes much to the work of D. E. Berlyne. In most general terms the problem will be case as the introduction of a new element of information into a system, and the consequent reorganization of the system. A healthy organism devotes part of its time and energy to environ- ment scanning or information seeking behavior. (Natural selection has provided some assurance that the sensory apparatus is appropriate for attending to external elements which are potential problems, disturbances or threats to survival or well being.) The sense data or new information which filters into the brain is checked against old information which has been gathered during the past and stored in the memory. The result of the check between sense data and memory data is a same/different reaction. When the check between new and old infermation yields a ”different” response, the healthy individual focuses the senses on the new or un- expected or changed element in the perceptual field. Since the individual has no learned responses or attitudes concerning the new element, there are conflicting response tendencies. The conflict brought about in the self by new information which is contrary to expectations about the environment may have a number of outcomes. An "exploratory drive" may impel the individual to orient itself toward the uncertain stimulus for maximum stimulation from it, to approach it, and examine it. There is some evidence that unfamiliar objects or events bring about exploratory behavior on the part of healthy organisms. (This is something like the capillarity of water mentioned earlier in 10 which there is a strong attraction for alien molecules.) The first thing that happens during the ”orientation reaction" is a scanning-rescanning by the appropriate senses in an effort to increase information and reduce uncertainty. Pavlov called this reaction to a perceived change in the environment the "what is it?" response. Berlyne calls it the orientation reaction. He points out that the same external conditions may bring about a reaction of curiosity, fear, adaptive behavior, or even laughter. Perception of Threat The next step in the process is an evaluative reaction of safe] dangerous. If the evaluative reaction is "dangerous," than a fight/flight or attack/retreat decision is made and appropriate behavior is carried out to maintain the physical and psychological integrity of the in- dividual. If the evaluative reaction is "safe," than a number of choices are opened including more information seeking and another evaluation. It would seem that a likely sequence of action in response to new information or a novel stimulus is information seeking, tentative "safe" evaluation, more information seeking, another safe/dangerous evaluation, etc. This sort of behavior is exemplified in the gingerly approach to an uncertain stimulus. As long as the safe/dangerous evaluation remains tentative, approach/avoid/approach behavior is likely. From the subjective view- point of the individual, an attitude, orientation or policy has not yet been decided upon. An evaluation of "dangerous" brings about a defensive reaction, while an evaluation of "safe" brings about an adaptive reaction. Both 11 adaptive and defensive reactions are generalized and pervasive physiological states, and both act to counteract stimulation or in- formation gathering, and reallocate available energy. The defensive reaction has been thoroughly explored and fitted out with terminology by Freudian psychologists. Ego-defensive behavior includes denial, identification with the agreesor, repression, pro- jection, etc. In Irving Sarnoff's definition, ego defense is a response to intolerable fear to minimize incapacitating the ego's perceptual function. He says that threatening events exert the greatest strain on the ego's perceptual function. (10) In Berlyne's terms the defensive reaction counteracts stimulation: energy is diverted from the sense organs in favor of the musculature. (In terms of a national system, this would be equivalent to allocating less money to the diplomats and more to the military.) One limitation of the intra-individual approach outlined above is that it takes little note of the importance of other individuals in the environment. Theodore Newcomb, in writing about the acquaintance process as a prototype of human interaction suggests that between two given people the process goes on indefinitely: "Participants, however familiar with one another they may already be, acquire information about each other, assess one another's attitudes, and either reinforce existing states of orientation toward each other and toward the common world, or change them, or develop new ones." (11) This implies that perhaps the other persons and groups making up the interpersonal environment of the individual should constantly be considered as relatively novel stimuli in Berlyne's schema. Each new 12 encounter offers an opportunity for increasing information and reducing uncertainty and for the evaluation of any information which is classified as different from that received during previous encounters. Newcomb sees the individual as confronted with the constant problem of maintaining three kinds of equilibrium: (1) intrapersonal, (2) with other individuals and groups, and (3) with the world that we have in common with these persons and groups. The communication input which provides the constraints within and among these systems consists of (1) direct sensory experience, and (2) the testimony of other people. As long as this communication input is internally consistent, no question arises about the ”real" nature of things. But when there is conflicting evidence perceived in communi- cation input, then disturbing questions arise about "myths" and "realities,” and conflicting response tendencies are activated. "Thus," says Newcomb, "we become sensitive to the acquired, drive- like state of strain" which, like other states of drive, may be tolerated for a time at least, but whether endured or appeased, influences behavior." (12) Two kinds of tendencies in balance-promoting changes in orien- tations discussed by Newcomb are the autistic and the realistic. ”Insofar as they are characterized by distortion of the testimony of others in balance-maintaining fashion-~30 that one's orientations neither toward those others nor toward the common world need be changed-owe have referred to them as autistic. And insofar as balance is achieved by changing one's own orientations (either attitudes or attractions) rather than by distortions of others orientations, the adoptions, may, l3 contrarywise, be regarded as realistic." (13) Stress and Strain This pressure toward equilibrium in interpersonal systems Newcomb calls ”strain." "Human socialization necessarily includes the acquiring of many attitudes that are consensual with others' (especially adults') attitudes. To survive requires the acquiring of unfavorable attitudes (like those of adults) toward possibly drowning water, or devouring beasts, or macerating machinery. To adapt comfortably to a stabilized family, community, or social order one must acquire the right attitudes (attitudes like those of one's associates who have already adapted com- forably to family, community, etc.) toward cabbages and kings and all else that is culturally approved or disapproved. Socialization, in short, includes the building of danger signals when attitudinal dis- crepancies with trusted others are perceived." (13) These danger signals provide negative feedback necessary for the maintenance of equilibrium between an individual and a social system. Richard S. Lazarus has studied a variable which he calls "stress." "This anticipation of potential harm or motive thwarting is the key to the concept of threat. I regard threat as the central intervening variable in psychological stress." (1a) In discussing the role of evaluation or "discriminating between dangerous or threatening conditions and benign ones," Lazarus says, "The process of appraising which circumstances are harmful and which are benign is crucial to the production of stress reactions... It seems likely that beliefs or expectations about events based both upon past experience and the present stimulus configuration determine whether a ll) stimulus will be appraised as threatening." (15) What has been labeled a safe/dangerous evaluation in previous pages is called "cognitive appraisal" by Lazarus. "The concept of cognitive appraisal implies that the same stimulus can be threatening or not, depending upon the interpretation the person makes concerning its fUture personal significance... The threat is not simply an attribute of the stimulus; rather it depends for its threat value on this appraisal, which in turn depends upon the person's appraisal of the meaning of the stimulus fer the thwarting of motives important to him." (16) In an experimental situation in which stress reaction was indexed by skin conductance, Lazarus demonstrated that the stress reaction to the same visual event could be significantly modified by varying simultaneously presented verbal commentary (i.e., cognitive appraisal). His conclusion was that the defensive verbal messages--constructed along lines specified by the theory of ego-defense--significantly reduced the threatening impact of a visual event (a motion picture). Lazarus has demonstrated that "the same stimulus may be threatening or not, depending upon the manner in which it is interpreted." (17) Perhaps some individuals habitually interpret their environment as being more threatening. Or in Berlyne's terminology, they may habitually make use of a defensive reaction. John A. Hammes has found that highly anxious individuals rate en- vironmental stimuli higher on a "danger-egression-threat" continuum than less anxious individuals. He operationalized high and low anxiety by Taylor Manafest Anxiety Scale items in multiple choice form on the Heineman Forced Choice Anxiety Scale. Hammes concluded that his findings 15 support the view that ”manifest anxiety" may be considered as a per- sonality characteristic. (l8) Authoritarianism is a well-known concept related to dichotomizing of complex issues, acceptance of authority figures and maintaining a view of the world as hostile and threatening. Non-authoritarianismo-as indexed by a low score on the F-scale--is associated with perceptiveness of others. Low scorers tend to be more sensitive to interpersonal re- lations among their peers. In Berlyne's terms they use their senses more and their muscles less. Newcomb and others have found that nonauthoritarians are more accurate in estimating the attitudes of people with whom they interact. And 0. J. Harvey has found that "higher authoritarianism was reflected in tendencies ‘to form judgmental norms or concepts of novel stimuli more quickly, as manifested in faster reaction time and less variability in the judgments entering into the formation of a concept." (19) In terms of the hypothetical cognitive circuitry described earlier, it would seem that high F-scale scorers move from orientation to evaluation more quickly. And the faster reaction time suggests that they are able to allocate energy from the sensory apparatus to the musculature more quicklyo-a change associated with the defensive reaction. Harvey found that high F—scale scorers demonstrated shorter delay in perception of movements and that "...authoritarianism disposes toward faster and more rigid structuring of novel stimuli." He also found that "authoritarianism disposes the individual toward increased closed- ness of his conceptual system and hence toward warding off events that deviate very far from his simple and narrow-banded interpretative schema." 1.6 This suggests that authoritarianism is related to autisim, and this was validated by Newcomb. He found that in an interpersonal re- lationship with peers, "the relationship between authoritarianism and autisim increases with acquaintance, and may be reversed at early stages of acquaintance if objects of orientation are important enough to induce very strong forces toward balance." (20) Perceived Threat in an International Context The foregoing pages have been an effort to make a case for the position that judgments of "safe/dangerous" or "threatening/not threatening" about aspects of the social reality of an individual are not a randomly selected attitudinal dimension but rather a fundamental evaluative operation which takes place as a system, organization, organism or person maintains equilibrium in a world which includes external threats, disturbances and problems. It has further been suggested how this conceptual scheme can be fitted into a communication model which defines individuals, nations, and blocs of nations as systems whose degree of independence of other systems is defined by degree of communication with other systems. Minimum communication with other systems equals maximum independence of the system. Maximum communication between systems imposes maximum constraints and conditionalities upon system behavior. This suggests that growth is taking place and that the systems are becoming sub-systems of a larger system as communication increases. It is thus suggested that the current international situation can be defined in terms of "levels of interdependence." (21) l7 ~Within the highly integrated nation-states of the mid-twentieth century, the integration of the system is expressed in terms of many constraints upon the behavior of the individuals, and among these constraints are those which impose consensus concerning threats or dis- turbances external to the national system. Ross Stagner has taken a similar position in suggesting that "man come to value his nation, or other social group, as an essential part of his environment, and mobilizes energy to protect it. Further, as a part of this process, he distorts the input of information in such a fashion as to protect valued aspects of his social environment, and these distortions contribute in no small degree to the intensity and bitterness of social conflicts." (22) Stagner says that ”for both theoretical and practical reasons, we should focus on how members of groups perceive other groups, and their goals and tactics," and that "social conflicts are rational if we grant the accuracy of the way in which the participants perceive the issues." (23) In detailing the role of perception he says that "it can magnify certain information inputs, giving them greater weight; and secondly, the obvious collary, it can diminish the importance of other cues. Finally, actual distortions may occur in quality and magnitude." (24) In this study, Stagner's admonition to focus on how members of groups perceive other groups will be heeded by utilizing a sample made up of peOple of different nationalities. Although primary interest is in perceived threat in terms of nations and groups of nations, the ex- tremely small sample of 2“ people certainly precludes generalizing about 18 nationalities. Findings must be interpreted not as generalizations but rather as explorations which indicate that further research may be fruitful. CHAPTER II VARIABLES AND HYPOTHESES The myth is that every Communist state is an un- mitigated evil and a relentless enemy of the free world; the reality is that some Communist regimes pose a threat to the free world while others pose little or none... J. Hilliam Fulbright "...the peOple whose decisions determine the policies and actions of nations do not respond to the 'objective' facts of the situation, whatever that may mean, but to their 'image' of the situation." Kenneth Boulding "National Images and International Systems" 19 20 On Wednesday, March 25, 196“; Senator J. William Fulbright, Chairman of the Senate Foreign Relations Committee, made a speech before the Senate in which he said: "It is not Communist dogma as espoused within Russia but Communist imperalism that threatens us and other peOples of the non- Communist world. Insofar as a great nation mobilizes its power and resources for agressive purposes, that nation, regardless of ideology, makes itself our enemy. Insofar as a nation is content to practice its doctrines within its own frontiers, that nation, however repugnant its ideology, is one with which we have no proper quarrel." The study to be described here is concerned with the countries, and the kinds of countries, which are perceived as threatening "us and other peoples of the non-Communist world," and how this is related to such things as power, resources and ideology. Senator Fulbright believes that simply because a country has a different ideology should not be sufficient grounds for judging it to be threatening or hostile. In this study people of different nationalities were asked to say to what degree 2n countries are dangerous or threatening to the security of themselves and their families. They were also asked to say which of the countries have governments which are Communist, and which, non-Communist. The correlation of the responses concerning threat and ideology makes it possible to say to what degree each of the people questioned agree with the view that the threat represented by a country is independent of its ideology. Senator Fulbright does not argue that these two things are independent, but rather that there is not a one-to-one relation between the two. In the same speech he said: 21 "I believe that the Communist world is indeed hostile to the free world in its general and long-term intentions but that the existence of this animosity in principle is far less important for our foreign policy than the great variations in its intensity and character both in time and among the individual members of the Communist bloc. Only if we recognize these variations, ranging from China, which poses immediate threats to the free world, to Poland and Yugoslavia, which pose none, can we hope to act effectively upon the bloc and to turn its internal differences to our own advantage and to the advantage of those bloc countries which wish to maximize their independence.” Thus Senator Fulbright argues that it is important to discriminate among Communist countries which represent different degrees of threat. He then states as pertinent examples that China poses immediate threats to the free world while Poland and Yugoslavia pose none. In this study the respondents were asked to express just this kind of judgments about a sample of 2a countires which includes China, Poland Yugoslavia, and also Cuba, a country which Senator Fulbright mentioned in the following terms: "I think we are bound to conclude that Castro is a nuisance but not a grave threat to the United States... Cuban communism does pose a grave threat to other Latin American countries..." The data from this study yields information about the degree to which a subsample of Americans see Cuba as a threat and comparisons may be made with the responses of a subsample of Brazilians. It will then be possible to say to what degree the Americans and Brazilians questioned agree with Senator Fulbright's view of the threat posed by 22 Cuba to Americans and Latin Americans, and further to compare these responses with those of Egyptians, Chinese and Indians questioned. While the samples are far too small to make generalizations about national populations, the data will yield some indications of whether members of a nationality make similar judgments about threat, ideology, etc. and whether the variation in reSponses within a nationality subsample are as great as the variation of the whole sample of five nationalities. Threat, Size and Health In the March 25 speech, Senator Fulbright did not go into great detail about the relationship between the theat a country poses and its power and resources. However, in discussing the situation in Panama he did cite the per capita income of Panama ($u29) and the Canal Zone ($u,228) as evidence of "the profound social and economic alienation between Panama and the Canal Zone, and its impact on the national feeling of the Panamanians, that underlines the current crisis." In this investigation, one hypothesis is that the wealth of a country is related to how threatening it is judged to be. That is, rich Communist countries will be seen as more threatening than poor Communist countries. Among non-Communist countries it is eXpected that the opposite relationship will prevail: rich countries will be judged to be less threatening than poor countries. One further point should be made about Senator Fulbright's dis- cussion of the crisis over the Panama Canal. He described Panama as "a small nation with a weak economy...” and stated, "I am unable to understand how a controversy with a small and poor country, with virtually no military capacity can possibly be regarded as a test of our 23 bravery and will to defend our interests." The Senator evidently does not see Panama as much of a threat, and part of the reason for this is that it is both small and poor. Here it will be hypothesized that a country's size (i.e., national population) as well as its wealth help to determine whether it is seen as posing a threat to the welfare and security of the person responding. Myths and Realities The central theme of Senator Fulbright's Speech was the divergence between "cherished myths" and "objective facts." He said: "As long as our perceptions are reasonably close to objective reality, it is possible for us to act upon our problems in a rational and appropriate manner. But when our perceptions fail to keep pace with events, when we refuse to believe something because it displeases or frightens us, or because it is simply startingly unfamiliar, then the gap between fact and perception becomes a chasm, and action becomes irrelevant and irrational. "There has always--and inevitably-obeen some divergence between the realities of foreign policy and our ideas about it. This divergence has in certain resPects been growing, rather than narrowing; and we are handicapped, accordingly, by policies based on old myths rather than current realities... "We are confronted with a complex and fluid world situation and we are not adapting ourselves to it. We are clinging to old myths in the face of new realities, and we are seeking to escape the contradictions by narrowing the permissable bounds of public discussion, by relegating an increasing number of ideas and viewpoints to a growing category of 2N» 'unthinkable thoughts.'" The issue raised by Senator Fulbright is a broad one, and only certain apsects of its can be examined within the confines of this study. However, some of the divergences between "perceptions" and "realities" will be examined here. For example, it is possible to ask a person to make estimates of the pOpulations of a sample of countries and to compare these estimates with data in official United Nations publications. A correlation coefficient will tell how closely these two sets of data agree, and will give an indication of whether the respondent rank ordered the countries in the same way as the U.N. statisticians. But there are difficulties in making such comparisons, and China makes a good example. It is easy enough to ask someone to make an estimate or guess of the population of China, and this was done in this study. But if this is the "perception," then what is the "reality"? China reports no figures to the United Nations, and any figure printed will be another estimate-~a carefully considered one no doubt, but still an estimate. There are other countries in the sample which have taken no national census in recent years, and, here again, pepulation figures will be estimates. If there are difficulties in specifying the "reality" of which countries are large and which countries are small, and which are rich and which are poor, the difficulties increase when ideology is to be discussed. Which countries have governments which are "very communist," "somewhat communist" or "non-communist"? There are no tables of numbers to consult to discover whether a government is more or less communist. 25 The reSpondents in this study were asked to rate the governments of 2% countries on a eight-point scale starting with "most non-communist" and ending with "most communist," and they completed the task without major difficulties. Thus the "perception" is easy enough to elicit, but the "reality" is harder to find. At this point, it seems best to abandon the reality- perception distinction and only to specify whose perception or classifi- cation is being discussed, and under what conditions and at what time the estimate was made. Each person participating in this study gave his own estimates concerning the degree of threat represented by each of 2% countries and also its population, per capita wealth (energy consumption) and ideology. Since the responses of any one person do not agree 100% with any other person in the sample, it might be argued that fragments of 2a different perceptual worlds of 2a people are represented by the data. Or one might say that 2a varying realities are represented. At any rate, each of these 24 sets of responses may be compared with a 25th set--data selected from current United Nations publications. Nationality as a Variable Two samples are under consideration in this study: a sample of 2a countries, and a sample of 2n people. Some characteristics of each sample have already been mentioned. The People were selected primarily on the basis of their nationality. There are five nationality subsamples each made up of four persons. (The sixth subsample of people was made up of international relations experts.) 26 All respondents were adult male college graduates. Each person in the sample had lived in the country where he was born until he was at least 21 years old with no extended periods of residence in foreign countries, and in all cases the parents of the respondent were of the nationality of the country where the respondent was born. In other words, the persons who participated in the study were all relatively pure national types in that they had been born and raised in the country where their parents had been born and lived. One of the primary purposes of the study is to find out whether people of different nationalities, when asked the same questions in approximately the same time and place reSpond in significantly different ways to questions about international threat. In the preceeding pages, a discussion of the concept of threat in systematic, international, interpersonal and intrapersonal terms has drawn on a large body of theoretical and practical material. How can this be utilized in a small-scale exploratory study? Heinz Eulau suggests that: "The most feasable alternative is to deal with modest propositions that require simultaneous manipulation of only a few variables, but to do so in a larger conceptual system that, although it cannot be tested directly, serves the very useful purpose of guiding an investigation and giving it theoretical significance." (25) If threat is to be the dependent variable of major concern, what are the independent variables which will help to account for variation in threat? Rosecrance has examined international politics from a "systems" viewpoint derived from Ashby, and he provides a statement which may assist in developing a few "modest propositions": 27 "The variety of the disturbance is in turn dependent upon certain intra-actor variables. The number of Options which an elite in control of an actor will exercise in the international system is a function of its particular ethos (that is, of the direction in which control is to be exercised), its control of disposable resources, and the quantity of disposable resources available." (26) This statement will be freely interpreted along with the remarks of Senator Fulbright to develop hypotheses concerning international threat. Thus an effort will be made to take into account viewpoints from both the practical and theoretical worlds of international politics. Some hypotheses have already been suggested. They will now be restated along with further hypotheses. Hypotheses The first two hypotheses suggest that there is a relationship between perceived threat and the ideolOgy of the government of a country and its total national pOpulation. In a sample of peOple made up of Egyptians, Indians, Brazilians, Formosan Chinese and Americans, (1) Communist countries will be perceived as more threatening than non-Communist countries, and (2) large countries-- regardless of ideology-—wi11 be perceived as more threatening than small countries. The first hypothesis may be interpreted as expressing a relation- ship between the ideOIOgy of the stimulus country and the ideology of the respondents which are assumed to be non-Communist in varying degrees. The second hypothesis is more directly related to a country characteristic, size of population. Although it is also possible to think of this as a relationship between the pOpulation size of the country being judged and the pOpulation of the country of the person 28 responding, this is not implied in the hypothesis. Regardless of the pepulation of the home country of the reSpondent it is hypothesized that large countries will be seen as more threatening than small countries. The third hypothesis relates international threat to wealth or what Rosecrance calls "the quantity of disposable resources available." In this study this will be considered as an energy variable, and it will be indexed as per capita energy consumption. It is assumed that such an index taps the same sort of underlying characteristic that is indexed by per capita gross national product. Edward S. Mason of Harvard University made an international study of per capita energy consumption and per capita gross national product, and concluded that "no country at this state of history can enjoy a high per capita income without being an extensive consumer of energy." (27) The sample of countries utilized in this study will be divided into equal subsamples of countries of high, medium and low per capita energy consumption. The hypothesis is that there will be no significant differences between the degree of threat represented by countries of different energy levels, but that there will be an interaction between energy level and ideology. Specifically, it is predicted that rich Communist countries will be seen as more threatening than poor Communist countries, but that rich non-Communist countries will be seen as 3555: threatening than poor non-Communist countries. No further hypotheses will be made about possible interactions between ideolOgy, population and energy, but any significant interactions among these variables will be reported and examined. 29 Threat and Nationality of ReSpondent Although it is expected that there will be significant differences in mean threat responses of nationality subsamples, there are no detailed hypotheses. This aspect of the study is exploratory, and the general prediction is that there will be significant differences in the mean threat reSponses of nationality subsamples when countries are divided by ideology, pepulation or energy level. In line with the view of a country as an organization which constrains the behavior of its citizens, it is also hypothesized that each nationality subsample will produce a pattern of threat reSponses for the country sample which will be different from that of the other nationality subsamples. In quantitative terms, this is to predict that inter-subject correlations of sets of threat responses will be significantly higher within nationality subsamples than between them. To summarize, it is predicted that perceived international threat is related to certain country characteristics (ideology, pepulation and energy level) as well as to the nationality of the respondent. "Myths and Realities" Operationalized One difficulty with the hypotheses suggested above is that a country which, according to United Nations statistics, has a small population may be considered by one or more subjects as having a large population. For example, when the countries of the world are rank ordered on the basis of their populations, Australia is clearly one of the smaller countries; yet many peeple might judge it to be a country with a large population. 30 This is the problem of "myths and realities" raised by Senator Fulbright. In this study, this problem will be dealt with by using two different criteria for making judgements about the characteristics of a country of interest here. In one case, the experimenter will classify the countries in the sample as to ideology, population and energy level--largely on the basis of the latest available U.N. figures. In the other case the subjects will classify the countries in the sample by giving their own estimates or judgements about the ideolOgy, population and energy levels of the countries. Thus the same variables of classification will serve as the basis for two classifications of threat reSponses, and two analyses. The experimenter-classified analysis will provide a basis for generalizations concerning specific countries with characteristics Specified by U.N. statistics. The subject-classified analysis will provide a basis for making statements, for example, about small, poor, non-Communist countries, although subjects may not always agree which countries in the sample have these characteristics. In both cases the dependent variable will be the threat responses of each subject to each country in the sample. The estimates or judgements of each subject about the ideology, population and energy level of each country makes it possible to use this limited view of the perceptual world of each subject to make intra- subject examinations of the relation of perceived international threat to perceived country characteristics. In this way it is also possible to test the assumption that these variables are significantly related on a subject by subject basis. 31 It is also possible to ask whether the relationship between subject estimates of country characteristics and U.N. figures on country characteristics help to explain the threat reSponses of an individual. Perceived populations and energy levels of countries for each subject will be correlated with U.N. data, and these data will be examined in terms of the "mean threat score" of each individual. This should give some idea of whether lack of information or uncertainty is related to how threatening the extra-national world is judged to be. Threat and Perceived Similarity It has already been hypothesized that non-Communist subjects will perceive Communist countries as threatening. It is possible that other kinds of perceived differences make a country or a nationality seem threatening. In order to investigate this possibility, each subject will be asked to say which nationalities (from the country sample) are most similar to his own and most different to his own. Here the subject will supply his own criteria for similarity and difference. The hypothesis will be that these "difference scores" will be positively correlated with the threat scores of an individual. These difference scores will also be correlated with ideology scores to provide a crude projective technique for assessing the ideology of each subject on a Communist to non-Communist scale. If Communist nationalities are regarded as "different" and non-Communist nationalities are regarded as similar, the correlation will be highly positive. It is assumed that the multinational sample will provide some variation in correlations, although they are all expected to be positive. 32 Threat and World Government Threat has been defined here as a mechanism which increases national boundary maintenance and reduces the possibility of systemic linkage. The threat responses discussed here have been thought of primarily as bilaterial with an individual country as a stimulus and a person of a given nationality as the respondent. However, it has also been suggested that summing and averaging the threat scores of an individual should give an indication of how dangerous he judges his international environment to be. If this is the case, then a person with a high mean threat score should be against supra-national arrangements which links his country with other countries in an international system. To investigate this, subjects were asked to indicate one of five degrees of approval or disapproval with the following statement: I think that my country should give up some of its soverignty to become part of a world government-~in the same way that a state or privincial government gives up some of its power to a national government. The hypothesis is that when subjects are cast into five categories according to the five possible responses to the statement, subjects who most strongly disapprove of world government will have highest mean threat scores, and subjects who most strongly approve of world government will have lowest mean threat scores. CHAPTER III PROCEDURES AND METHODS 0? ANALYSIS Two samples were utilized in this study: (1) a sample of 2a countries used in the questionnaire, and (2) a sample of 24 people who reSponded to the questionnaire. The country sample was constructed by the experimenter by selecting countries on the basis of (l) ideology, (2) population size, and (3) energy consumption per capita. The initial criterion for selection of countries was that they appear in the United Nations Statistical Yearbook for 1962. An effort was also made to limit the sample to countries which would be familiar to college graduates whose professional interests were not in inter- national relations. Hithin these limitations a country sample of maximum geographic diversity was chosen. Further details concerning the country sample will be described on the following pages. But first, this will be prefaced by a general description of the measuring instrument. It consisted of a brief introduction and six sections. (Appendix A is a cepy of the questionnaire.) The first five sections each asked for reaponses concerning the same 2H countries, and the sixth section included a world government question and blanks for information about the subject's sex, age, occupation or field of study, educational background, country of birth, nationality, parent's nationality, and foreign countries lived in for at least one year before the age of 21. 33 3a The order of presentation of the sections was as follows: (1) threat represented by countries (2) similarity of nationalities (3) energy consumption per person by countries (4) national populations and (S) ideologies of governments (Communist to non-Communist). In each section the order of presentation of countries was alphabetical, except for the section on national population in which this order was reversed. Thus if the order of presentation has affected the variability of reSponses, it is the same for all subjects. Ideology An initial selection of some 50 countries was made from the latest available United Nations publications--principally the Ugh. Statistical Yearbook and the U.N. Demggraphic Yearbook. These were divided by the experimenter into Communist and non-Communist countries. The increasing number of "neutralist" countries posed problems which were resolved by the experimenter by casting some of them into each ideological category. Neutralist nations are here defined as 20 countries whose representatives (1) attended a 1961 conference in Belgrade, Yugoslavia, and (2) signed a letter to the Secretary General of the United Nations asking that the 196k General Assembly be postponed until after a meeting of heads of state of non-alligned nations scheduled to meet in Cairo, Egypt, in October, 1964. (28) Five of these countries appear in the country sample. Cuba and Yugoslavia are experimenter classified as Communist countries while Algeria, Indonesia and the Congo are classified as non-Communist countries. (Banks and Textor classified Cuba as quasi-Communist rather than Communist. Otherwise, the countries classified as Communist here 35 are identically classified by Banks and Textor.) The subjects in this investigation were also asked to classify the governments of each country in the sample on the basis of ideology. They classified the countries according to degree of Communist or non- Communist government orientation on a eight-point scale. Distribution was forced normal so that only one country was allowed to be scored "most non-Communist" and one, "most Communist" in the sample, etc. This allowed for the countries to be dichotomized according to ideological orientation on the basis of the judgements of the subjects. Countries scored 1 through u were called "subject classified as non-Communist" while those scored 5-8 were called "subject classified as Communist." These two bases of classification were used with each of the three major independent variables in this study. This allowed for the judgements concerning threat to be cast into two matrices: one on the basis of experimenter classifications, and the other on the basis of subject classification. The same type of analysis was carried out on the two matrices. Experimenter classifications were made primarily on the basis of United Nations published data, and the divergences between the two matrices will yield some idea of how the perceptions and judgements of the subjects vary from this criterion. Pepulation National population figures for mid-1962 were the latest available in the United Nations Demoggaphic Yearbook for the countries under consideration, and these were used for experimenter classification of countries according to pepulation size. A dichotomy was also wanted on 36 this variable, and countries were classified as "large" if their populations were reported as greater than 16 million in mid-1962, and "small" of their pOpulations were 16 million or less. Subjects were asked to estimate the populations of each of the countries in the sample to the nearest million, and this provided the basis of subject classifications on this variable. In this case, the mid-point which discriminated "large" from "small" countries was determined by the subjects' estimates. That is, the 12 countries which were considered largest by each subject were classified as "large," and the rest, as "small." If two or more countries at the subject's pOpulation midpoint were estimated as having the same pOpulation, they were randomly cast into the two categories by the experimenter. Wealth and Energy The initial decision was to use per capita wealth as one of the three major independent variables of the investigation. However, an examination of the U.N. Yearbook of National Account Statistics,¢1962, indicated that the latest year that a reasonably complete set of figures was available referred to the year 1958. Also, there were no figures for most Communist countries. An investigation of other possible indices indicated that per capita figures for consumption of commercial energy were available for a large number of countries for the year 1961, and that Communist countries were included. Also, it had been demonstrated that for data from 62 countries, there was a high positive correlation between per capita gross national product and per capita energy consumption. (29) 37 The pre-selection of countries was classified into three levels of energy consumption: high, medium and low. In the final sample of u countries there are eight countries in each of these categories. But because this variable is nested within the two mentioned previously, the greatest variability within each level is present in the wealth or energy variable. Subject classifications of countries on this variable were based on individual estimates of the wealth or energy consumption per capita on a 15-point scale. Part of the instructions on this part of the questionnaire said: Now you are to make the best estimate that you can about the wealth or poverty of the same list of countries. In order to avoid the problem of equivalency of different currencies, you are asked to judge each country in terms of the amount of coal, hydroelectric power and other sources of commercial energy available to the average person in each country... In general, you should consider the scale below and the one on the next page, as asking you to make distinctions between richer and poorer countries. The richer that you think a country is--that is, the greater the income of the average person--the farther to the right you should make your "X". However, in case of doubt, you should consider the commercial energy consumption per person as the indicator of wealth... Note that the scale values (0 to 8000) increase more rapidly as the scale moves from the low values on the left to the high values on the right. Thus an effort was made to elicit distinctions between countries perceived as rich and poor, and at the same time introduce the general notion of the criterion index for this variable which was per capita energy consumption. In summary, a sample of 29 countries was constructed by the experimenter in which 12 were classified as Communist and 12 as 38 non-Communist. The sample was so selected that there were 12 "large" countries and 12 "small" countries. Six of the Communist countries were "large," and six were "small"; six of the non-Communist countries were "large," and six were "small." Within each of these four groups of six countries, there were two relatively poor countries, two countries of intermediate wealth, and two relatively rich countries. The final result is a 2x2x3 design with two countries (one replication) in each cell. The direction of the nesting assumes that ideolOgy is most important in judgements concerning international threat, and that population size and wealth are successively less important. This design assumes that all of the cells can be filled with countries of apprOpriate characteristics as described by U.N. data. How well this was achieved is described by the following table: TABLE 1 The Country Sample With Criteria Data Energy Population Consumption Index (millions) (per capita) Non-Communist Small Rich: New Zealand 2.9 2033 Switzerland 5.6 1951 Medium: Formosa 11.3 529 Alberia 11.6 25h Poor: Congo lu.8 6H South Viet-Nam 14.9 60 Large Rich: United States 186.6 80u2 France ”6.9 251k Medium: Mexico 37.2 959 Japan 9n.9 1298 Poor: Pakistan 96.6 68 Indonesia 97.7 140 Communist Small Poor: Albania 1.7 337 North Korea 8.“ 528 Medium: Cuba 7.7 866 Hungary 10.1 2996 Rich: Czechoslovakia 13.9 6125 East Germany 16.0 u9u2 39 TABLE 1 (Continued) Large Poor: North Viet-Nam 16.7 528 China (mainland) 686.U 528 Medium: Yugoslavia 18.8 sou Roumania 18.7 1433 Rich: Poland 30.3 3182 Soviet Union 221.5 2921 The rich-medium-poor order is reversed in the Communist countries so that this listing of pairs of countries represents hypothesized rank order of threat. The prediction is that countries at the top will be least threatening and countries at the bottom will be most threatening. The cells are certainly not as homogeneous as might be desired. This is eSpecially true in the Communist countries, but very little selection was possible in this case. Bulgaria and Mongolia are the only Communist countries in the U.N. publications which are left out of the sample. The Sample of PeOple The main sample of subjects consisted of 20 male college graduates who were all pursuing graduate studies at the same university. To this was appended a special subsample consisting of four university professors with professional interests in international relations. An effort was made to introduce nationality as a variable in the investigation by breaking the sample into five subsamples of four persons each. The subsamples consisted of Brazilians, Indians, Chinese (from Formosa), Egyptians and Americans. In all cases the nationality of the subject was that of the country of birth, and none of the subjects had been outside his home country for as much as a year before reaching 21 years of age. And in all cases the parents were both of the same HO nationality as the reapondent. (Brazil and Egypt have similar levels of energy consumption and per capita wealth while India is somewhat poorer. India and Egypt are both "neutralist nations" in the terms mentioned earlier, and Brazil had pursued a similar course quite recently.) Except for the Americans, the sample of people was drawn from a list of foreign students attending Michigan State University. In an effort to obtain a stronger nationality effect in this small sample, subjects were selected from students who had come to the United States to do graduate work, and no undergraduates were included. Selection of subjects was not random but rather was based on the willingness of the subjects to respond to the questionnaire. No effort was made to control for age or field of study. Data from the questionnaires yields the following information: TABLE 2 Age and Field of Study of Subjects Brazilians Questionnaire A e Field Date Subject #11 3O Businesministration W Subject #12 26 Business Administration 3/23/6“ Subject #13 #3 Sociology u/u/su Subject #1u ( no) Business Administration 3/23/6u E tians UAR Subject £31 30 Education 3/25/69 Subject #22 30 Biochemistry (none) Subject #23 23 Education 3/27/6u Subject #24 35 Social anthropology 3/26/6u Chinese (Formosa) Subject §3I 27 Agricultural Economics 3/24/6u Subject #32 27 Communication 3/23/6u Subject #33 Chemistry u/u/su Subject #3u 32' Agricultural Engineering M/S/6u , Indians Subject FBI 25 Agricultural Engineering 3/29/6” Subject ##2 25 Electrical Engineering 3/26/6u Subject #93 28 Biochemistry 3/2u/6u Subject #uu 27 Plant Pathology 3/2u/6u ul TABLE 2 (continued) Americans Subject-m— 35 Communication 3/27/61; Subject #52 29 Communication 3/2u/6u Subject #53 Communication (none) Subject #su '-' Communication (none) U.S. Ex erts— Subject #6! 5H International Relations 3/3/6u Subject #62 #9 Agricultural Economics u/20/6u Subject #63 ___ Economics (none) Subject #SH no Journalism 3/27/6u Here it Will be assumed that nationality is a much stronger determinant of international attitudes than field of study, and while the selection of the sample of peOple does not allow for a rigorous check of this assumption, the duplication of departments in different nationality subsamples will allow for an informal examination of this assumption. A factor analysis of the subjects in terms of their 2h international threat reSponses will provide one means of checking this assumption. An examination of the resulting factors should give some indication of whether, say, biochemists and agricultural engineers are on the same factor or on different factors according to their nationalities. The same analysis should give an indication of whether the two sub-samples of Americans (graduate students and faculty international relations Specialists) are similar in their responses concerning inter- national threat. Host of the responses to the questionnaires were made during the last week of March, 196“, and the remaining ones were made during April. Three of the 0.8. experts made their responses some three weeks later u2 than the rest of the sample. However, it is assumed that this time differential was not significant enough to introduce variability in reSponses. Analytical_§rocedures The basic analytical tool will be a factorial analysis of variance in which the factors are ideology (two values), population (two values), wealth (three values) and nationality (six values). The alpha level will be set at the conventional .05 level, and greater levels of significance will be mentioned. Subjects' judgements about the threats of 2H countries will be cast into two matrices (1) experimenter classified, and (2) subject classified, and identical analyses of variance will be carried out on each one. No hypotheses will be made about differences in the two sets of results, but the differences will be examined and discussed. The eXperimenter classified analysis is apprOpriate for making statements about specific pairs of countries and groups of countries. Here summation will be across subjects on reSponses about one or more pairs of concrete countries. In the subject classified analysis, it will no longer be possible to make generalizations about specific countries, since these will vary from one individual to another. Here the common quality of the rows and columns being summed will be values of the variables of classification: ideology, population and wealth. And here generalizations will refer to these kinds of country characteristics. The analysis of variance table for this design will consist of fifteen possible sources of variance: four main effects, six two-factor 1+3 interactions, four three-factor interactions, and one four-factor interaction. Error terms will be drawn from six other analysis of variance tables--one for each of the six subsamples of peOple. The sum of squares for individual subjects (within subsamples) will be pooled for the six subsamples and divided by the correSponding degrees of freedom to produce a mean square error apprOpriate for testing the significance of a main "nationality" effect. Similarly, sums of squares for subject (within nationality)- ideolOgy interaction for the six subsamples will be pooled and divided by the correSponding degrees of freedom to produce an appropriate denominator for the F-ratio testing the main ideology effect as well as the ideology-nationality interaction. The six other error terms to be used in this analysis will be computed in the same manner. This computation of a series of different error terms should add more precision to the analysis than is possible when a residual error term is used in a fixed effects model. Individual subjects will be considered as "replicates" of a given nationality. Also, the set of error terms used here allows the use of analysis of variance procedures with subsamples of subject reaponses in which the variance is not homogeneous across subsamples. Simple effects will be tested by t-tests in which the numerator is the difference between a pair of cell means and the denominator will be the mean square error term for the source of variance under con- sideration. (30) 141+ Other Analyses In order to examine the assumptions concerning the relationships between the four main variables being examined, intra-individual correlation coefficients will be computed for pairs of the sets of responses for each subject. This will determine the direction and the strength of the relationship between threat and ideology, threat and population, and threat and energy. Here, individual differences will be visible which were examined only indirectly--in mean square error terms--in the analysis of variance. To examine the inter-relationships between ideology, population and energy, and their effectiveness in predicting the threat scores of an individual, multiple correlation coefficients will be computed for each individual. These will give an indication of how much of the variation in threat scores can be accounted for by ideology, population and nationality together, and in the three possible pairings. To examine the relationship between threat scores and subsamples of subjects, a Q factor analysis will be computed. The hypothesis will be that each nationality will have a different pattern of threat scores, and that these differences will produce different factors for each nationality. Responses on the 21-point threat scale will be treated as equivalent to a Q-sort of countries on a threat dimension. Each subject's set of threat reSponses will be correlated with that of every other subject, and the resulting matrix of intercorrelatinns will be submitted to factor analysis. A principal axis solution will be obtained and submitted to varimax rotation to produce orthOgonal simple structure. The factor loadings for the individuals loading highest on each factor will be weighted apprOpriately to produce a typical set of threat responses for each factor, and these weighted responSes will be converted to z-sccres. Similarly, sets of responses for each factor concerning 0 ideology, population, energy, and similarity of nationality will be produced. These will provide a check on hypothesized relation- ships between threat and the independent variables in terms of individual countries. Reaponses concerning similarity of nationality have been mentioned briefly in the description of the questionnaire and in the Q analysis. These responses are similar to the ideology responses in that the in- structions require that the nationalities of the 2“ countries be ranked on an 8-point scale in a normal distribution pattern. Correlations of similarity and threat scores for each individual will give an indication of the direction and strength of the relation- ship between these two sets of responses. Correlations of similarity and ideology scores for each individual should yield a rough index of the ideology of each subject. Criteria Data The United Nations data on national populations and per capita energy consumption used to construct the country sample has already been mentioned. Correlations of these data with sets of individual responses will be used to give an indication of how closely each subject's reSponses correSpond to the criteria. Also, a composite index will be constructed by noting in how many cases each subject's responses on ideology, population, and energy 1&6 result in classifying a country in the same coll--of a total of twelve-- as that in which U.N. data classifies it. A perfect score here would be 2n. Those data can be srrsysd by subjects, and also by countries. World Govern-out Finally, the subjects will be divided into five groups an the bssis of their response to the world government ststssont on the last psgs of the questionnaire. The hypothesis here is s corrolationsl one: strong agreement with the world government statement will be associated with low nosn threst scores, and strong dissgresnont will be associated with high Issn thrust scores. CHAPTER IV RESULTS The principal findings of this study consist of an analysis of variance of threat scores. But before these results are detailed, a few general descriptive findings will he reported. Below are the average threat scores of individual subjects and individual countries in the two samples: TABLE 3 Average Threat Scores for Subject and Countries “7 Sub ects Countgigg‘ Brazilian #11 1.1667 Albania 5.0u17 Brazilian #12 1.2917 Algeria 3.0583 Brazilian #13 8.5u17 China (mainland) 13.9167 Brazilian #1# .8750 Congo “.3333 Egyptian #21 5.7083 Cuba 6.8750 Egyptian #22 2.8333 Czechoslovakia 5.0u17 Egyptian #23 1.2083 East Germany 6.2917 Egyptian #2“ .5ul7 Formosa 3.6250 ’Chinese #31 11.1667 France ' u.usas Chinese #32 “.2500 Hungary 5.5000 Chinese #33 1.8333 Indonesia “.6667 Chinese #3“ 11.0“17 Japan 3.0000 Indian #ul 1.6250 Hexico 1.5833 Indian #02 2.7083 NOV Zealand .6667 Indian in: 2.7917 North Korea 6.1250 Indian #uu 1.6667 North Viet-Nam 6.9167 Anorican #51 11.3333 Pakistan 5.2500 American 652 9.7083 Poland “.7917 American #53 10.2500 Roumania “.6250 American #5“ 8.H583 South Viet-Nan 3.n167 48 TABLE 3 (continued) U.S. Expert £61 .7917 Soviet Union 9.7917 0.8. Expert #62 5.5000 Switzerland .3333 0.3. Expnrt #63 10.5000 Unitod States 3.7083 0.3. Expert #su 1.6667 Yugoslavia n.0u17 The questionnaire obviously produced considerably variation in response, both in terms of subjects and of countries. The 21-point scale was scored from zero to twenty. with no further information than that, the expected mean score for any person or country would be 10. Switzerland and New Zealand have the lowest means and also the lowest standard deviations (not shown). China, the Soviet Union and North Viet-Nam all have the highest means and the highest standard deviations. It seems that it is easier to get international consensus concerning countries which are not threatening than for which countries are threatening. The countries are listed in the sane order as that which was used in the questionnaire. (They were equally spaced in the questionnaire; the groupings of four are arbitrary.) It is possible that the order of presentation might have affected threat scores, but if there is such an effect it seems to be a minor one. Just as threat responses were averaged for subjects and for countries, it is also possible to average other sets of responses from the questionnaire. Per capita wealth or energy estieates were made on a 15-point scale scored from zero to 70. “9 TABLE 0 Per Capita Energy Consumption Averages for Subjects and Countries Sub ects Brazilian #11 18.7 Brazilian #12 15.0 Brazilian #13 7.1 Brazilian #1“ 15.0 Egyptian #21 18.9 Egyptian #22 5.7 Egyptian #28 8.0 Chinesa #31 8.5 Chinese #32 17.1 Chinese #33 21.0 Chinese 33% 9.8 Indian #91 13.0 Indian #uz 16.7 Indian #us 6.0 American #51 20.0 American #52 13.“ American #53 19.9 American #59 13.5 U.$. Expert #61 10.7 U.S. Expert #62 18.5 0.8. Expert #63 1u.u UeSe Export #6“ So]. In terms of the scale and scoring procedure used here, the average Countries Albania 5 0 Algeria 3 2 China (mainland) 5.0 Congo 1 5 Cuba 6.2 Czechoslovakia 13.2 East Germany 13.9 Formosa 6.3 France 30.3 Hungary 10.6 Indonesia 2.9 JCPHD 2'4. 0 "Cu“ 7e 1 New Zealand 21.2 North Korea 3.0 North Viet~Nam 2.8 Pakistan 3.0 Poland 9.0 Roumania 6.6 South Viet-Nam 2.7 Soviet Union 27.8 Switzerland 38.9 United States 68.3 Yugoslavia 12.1 0.". N 0050‘” e e e e 000° H0) 000010 es a 0000 HP” ”Pm". e e 0000 energy consumption per capita of the 2n countries in this sample is 17.0 when U.N. criteria data is used. are above this figure, and 17 are be variation in how this sample of subjects judges the average per capita wealth in this sample of countries. The average estimates of 7 subjects low it. There is considerable Data 50 Subjects were asked to estimate the population of each country in the country sample to the nearest million, and these estimates may be treated just as the threat scores and energy estimates were: TABLE 5 Average Population Estimates for Subjects and Countries Subjects Brazilian #11 Brazilian #12 Brazilian #13 Brazilian #1u Egyptian #21 Egyptian'#22 Egyptian #23 Egyptian #29 Chinese #31 Chinese #32 Chinese #33 Chinese #38 Indian #91 Indian #uz Indian #ua Indian #uu American #51 American #52 American #53 American #Sn UeSe EXPCrt #61 0.8. Expert #62 0.6. Expert #63 U.S. Expert #6:. (in millions) Countries Albania Algeria China Congo Cuba Czechoslovakia East Germany Formosa France Hungary Indonesia Japan Mexico New Zealand North Korea North Viet-Nam Pakistan Poland Roumania South Viet-Nam Soviet Union Switzerland united States Yugoslavia 78.8 26.0 21.0 28.0 232.8 15.2 186.0 31.2 U.N. Data (for countries) 1.7 11.5 686.“ 1“.8 The mean for the U.N. population data used as criteria was 69.7 million. The mean average estimates of 13 subjects are above this criterion, and 10 are below it. Taken at face value, this indicates 51 that some subjects think that these countries are much more highly pepulated than do others. The second and third columns of figures refer to populations of countries in millions. When U.N. data are used as criteria, the populations of 18 countries are overestimated, and 6 countries are under- estimated. The questionnaire also asked for estimates of the ideology of each country on an 8-point non-Communist to Communist scale. Subjects were asked to judge the present governments of the countries in the sample. Responses were forced into a normal distribution, and as a result all subjects have the some means. Therefore only country averages will be presented in this case: TABLE 6 Average Ideology Estimates for Countries United States 1.29 Roumania 5.00 Switzerland 2.33 Poland 5.12 Formosa 2.87 Hungary 5.25 Japan 2.92 Yugoslavia 5.81 France 2.96 North Korea 5.50 New Zealand 2.96 North Viet-Nam 5.62 South Viet-Nam 3.82 Albania 5.79 Mexico 3.59 East Germany 5.79 Pakistan 3.83 Cuba 5.79 Congo 3.92 Czechoslovakia 5.83 Algeria ”.08 Soviet Union 7.25 Indonesia n.17 China 7.62 S2 A low score indicates a non-Communist government and a high score indicates a Communist government. The fact that the twelve countries in the left hand column of Table 6 were all experimenter classified as non-Communist and the other twelve countries as Communist indicates general consensus between the subjects and the experimenter with regard to this variable. Standard deviations are not shown, but the highest ones were for Albania and Formosa. All other standard deviations were below 1.0. Analysis of Variance of Threat Scores The findings to be reported in the following pages are the results of four identical factorial analyses of variance of threat responses. These responses or scores have been cast into two different matrices, one in which countries are experimenter classified as to ideology, papulation, and wealth, and another in which the countries are subject classified with regard to these variables. In each of these two approaches to classification, the threat responses are quantified in two different ways (1) as raw scores, and (2) as standardized scores. This produces the data for the four analyses. Threat responses for each country were marked on a 2l-point scale and are scored from zero to 20. In this analysis the 2n countries in the sample are treated as 12 pairs of countries which are similar in ideology, population and wealth or energy level. The raw scores of pairs of countries are summed so that the sum of any two "replicate" countries may vary from zero to no. Standardized scores were produced by computing means and standard deviations for each subject, and translating the raw scores into 53 standard scores with a mean of 50 and a standard deviation of 10. Raw scores then, refer to absolute scale values while standard scores refer to relative threat for a given individual. In the following table the results of the four analyses are summarized. TABLE 7. Summary of Four Analyses of Variance Ideology (Communist/Non-Communist) Population (Large/Small) Ideology-Population Energy (Rich/Hedium/Poor) Ideology-Energy Population-Energy Ideology-Population-Energy Nationality (six subsamples) Ideology-Nationality Population-Nationality Ideology-Population-Nationality Energy-Nationality Ideology-Energy-Nationality Population-Energy-Nationality Ideology-Population-Energy-Nationality Threat Scores Experimenter Classified Raw Std. .0005 .0005 0001 .005 .0005 .0005 .01 .025 .0005 .0005 .005 .0005 .05 Contr. .005 .001 .025 .01 .01 .001 .005 Subject Classified Raw Std. .0005 .0005 .025 .005 .01 .025 Controlled .05 .005 .05 .01 .05 58 These findings will be discussed in detail in the following pages. The discussion will center upon the analysis of the experimenter- classified raw scores, and the other analyses will not be discussed in detail except in cases where they differ significantly from it. For example, the main ideology effect was highly significant in all four analyses, and no purpose would be served by presenting details from the four analyses. Only significant results are noted in the table above, and blanks indicate a lack of significance at the .05 level. The order of presentation will be that of the analysis of variance tables: (1) ideology, (2) population, (3) energy, and (8) nationality. That is, the three nested "treatments" will be presented first, and the "levels" last. Ideology The hypothesis is that Communist countries will be perceived as more threatening than non-Communist countries. The results are as fellows: TABLE 8a Ideology: Average Threat Scores for Communist and non-Communist Countries Threat Scores N Mean S.D. (observations) Experimenter—classified non-Communist countries 188 6.89 8.88 Experimenter-classified Communist countries 188 13.18 12.28 The AOV table shows the following results as the contribution of ideology to the variance in threat responses: 55 TABLE 8b Main Ideology Effect Source of Sum of Mean Observed Variance Squares ‘2§_ Square Error F F.95 Ideology 3186.6806 1 3186.6806 132.7963 23.9968 8.81 with 1/18 df The very large F values observed--significant beyond the .0005 level in all four cases-osuggest that no subtle distinction is being examined here. Even though 12 of the 28 subjects are from what are sometimes considered neutralist countries, the circumstances surrounding the data gathering (i.e., on the campus of a 0.5. university) no doubt contributed to this resounding rejection of a null hypothesis which seems even less tenable after the fact than it did before. Population The theoretical hypothesis here is that, ideology aside, countries with large populations will be seen as more threatening than those with small populations. Countries were again dichotomized with equal numbers of Communist and non-Communist countries in each category. The results were: TABLE 9a Population: Average Threat Scores for Large and Small Countries N (Observations) Mean S.D. Experimenter-classified small countries 188 8.38 10.62 Experimenter-classified large countries 188 11.28 11.32 56 TABLE 91) Main Population Effect Source of Sum of Mean Observed Variance Squares 2£_ Sguare Error F F.95 Population 589.3889 1 589.3889 35.8188 16.8566 8.81 with 1.18 df The conclusion is that whether countries are classified as large and small by United Nations data by the experimenter or by the estimates of each subject, it is possible to reject the null at the .05 level of confidence and state that large countries are perceived as more threatening than small countries by subjects of five different nationalities. Ideology and Population Are these two factors independent in their effects upon threat scores, or do they interact? Here are the results of the experimenter classified analysis: TABLE 10a Ideology and Papulation: Average Threat Scores for non-Communist and Communist, Large and Small Countries Small Large Non-Communist 5.2778 7.6988 Communist 11.8861 18.7917 Here there are 72 observations in each cell. The AOV table reads: TABLE 10b Ideology..Popu1ation Interaction Source of Sum of Mean Observed Variance Squares ‘25_ Square Error F F.95 Idso.-Pop. 18.2222 1 18.2222 10.1667 1.3989 8.81 with 1/18 df 57 The interaction is not significant, and the effects of ideology and p0pu1ation of a country upon its perceived threat are independent for this sample of people. Energy In contrast with the first two dichotomous factors, this variable is three-valued. Countries were clasified as high, medium or low in per capita energy. The hypothesis is that there will be no significant difference between the three cells when threat scores are classified in this manner. The results of the experimenter classified analysis are: TABLE 11a Energy: Average Threat Scores for Experimenter-Classified High, Medium and Low Energy Countries N Mean Threat Scores Std. Dev. (observations) High energy (rich countries 96 8.9879 10.8808 Medium energy countries 96 8.0729 9.9792 Low energy (poor) countries 96 12.8167 12.1877 TABLE 11b Main Energy Effect Source Sum of Mean Observed of Variance Squares 2: Square Error ' F F. 95 Energy 1013.3125 2 506.6563 20.8977 28.7177 3.26 with 2/36 df The very large observed P value suggests that there is a highly significant main energy effect, but the direction in the differences 58 between the means makes it difficult to interpret from the data presented here. The following pages will show that the energy or wealth factor interacts significantly with other factors, and that generalizations about wealth or energy must be limited by these other factors. When subjects classify the countries in the sample on per capita energy consumption, the results are quite different: TABLE 12a Average Threat Scores for Subject-Classified High, Medium and Low Energy Countries N Threat Scores (observations) Mean Std. Dev. High energy (rich) countries 96 9.8687 10.7233 Medium energy countries 96 9.3858 11.0328 Low energy (poor) countries 96 10.5833 11.2331 TABLE 12b Main Energy Effect, Subject-Classified Analysis Source of Sum of Mean Observed Variance Squares 2E Sguare Error F F. 95 Energy 85.8958 2 82.9879 50.8388 .8888 3.26 with 2/36 df In this case there is no significant difference between these three sets of observations. This is in marked contrast to the findings in the experimenter-classified analysis. Again, part of the explanation is that energy interacts with other factors in the analysis, and this will be detailed on the following pages. 59 However, it should be noted that the relative order of the means remains stable across the two analyses: the countries of intermediate wealth are seen as least threatening, the rich countries next most threatening, and the poor countries as most threatening. Because of the contrast in the results, it seems worthwhile to return for a moment to the experimenter-classified matrix, and to specify the three groups of countries which resulted in highly significant differences in threat scores: TABLE 13 Experimenter Classification of Countries by per capita Energy Consumption High per capita Medium per capita Low per capita Energy Consumption Energy Consumption Energy Consumption New Zealand 2033 Formosa 529 Congo 68 Switzerland 1951 Algeria 258 South Viet-Nam 60 United States 8082 Mexico 959 Pakistan 68 France 2518 Japan 1298 Indonesia 180 Czechoslovakia 5125 Cuba 866 Albania 337 East Germany 8982 Hungary 2896 North Korea 528 Poland 3182 Yugoslavia 908 North Viet-Nam 528 Soviet Union 2921 Romania 1833 China (mainland) 528 Mean 3,838 Mean 1,092 Mean 282 3.0. 2,088 3.13. 682 3.6. 223 It is the threat scores of these specific countries which are used in the energy consumption cells in the experimenter-classified matrix. It is obvious that the subject classified matrix cast these 60 some countries into these three categories in patterns which were different enough to alter the significance of the results. Ideology and Energy In the experimenter-classified analysis, these two factors interacted as follows: TABLE 18a Ideology and Energy: Average Threat Scores Mean Threat Scores Rich Medium Poor Non-communist countries 8.7917 5.8333 8.8333 Communist countries 13.1082 10.3125 16.0000 Here there are 88 observations per cell. The AOV table shows: TABLE 18b Ideology: Energy Interaction Source of Sum of Mean Observed Variance Sguares ‘25. Sguare Error F F.95 Ideo.-Energy 185.8803 2 92.9201 17.6019 5.2790 3.26 with 2/36 df The interaction of ideology with energy is significant at the .01 level of confidence. (Mean differences of 1.5 are significant.) When ideology is controlled, poor countries are most threatening at both levels, but there are significant differences between rich and medium Communist countries. This difference is not significant in the case of the non-Communist countries. When energy level is controlled, Communist countries are more threatening at all three levels. 61 Results in the subject classified analysis were as follows: TABLE 15 Ideology and Energy: Average Threat Scores, Subject-Classified Analysis Rich Medium Poor Non-Communist countries 5.3750 6.8125 7.35u1 Communist countries 13.5625 11.9583 13.8125 Here the relationships between the cell means are the same as in the experimenter-classified analysis, but the pattern has been attenuated enough to reduce it to statistical insignificance. Papulation and Energy Cell means for the experimenter classified analysis are as follows: TABLE 16a Population and Energy: Average Threat Scores Rich Medium Poor Small countries 6.1667 9.5208 9.u583 Large countries 11.7292 6.6250 15.3750 Again in the 2x3 table, each cell mean is based on “8 observations. Results from the AOV table are: TABLE 16b Population-Energy Interaction Source of Sum of Mean Observed Variance Sguares ‘RE_ Ssuare Error F F.95 Pop.-Bnergy 119u.6319 2 597.3160 22.9“68 26.0305 3.26 with 2/36 df 62 This is the largest P value observed in the eXperimenter- classified analysis, and it seems worthwhile to examine the simple effects in detail. Each of the six means refers to threat scores for four specific countries: TABLE 17 Countries Experimenter Classified by Energy Level and Papulation Rich Medium Poor New Zealand Formosa Congo Small Switzerland Algeria South Viet-Nam Czechoslovakia Cuba Albania East Germany Hungary North Korea united States Mexico Pakistan Large France Japan Indonesia Poland Yugoslavia North Viet-Nam Soviet Union Rumania China (mainland) Differences between each pair of means--exc1uding diagonals-- were tested with t-tests. All differences were significant with the single exception of the difference between small, poor countries and small medium (energy) countries. In contrast, the greatest difference was between large, poor and large, medium (energy consumption) countries. In the subject-classified analysis, the corresponding cell means were: TABLE 18 Population and Energy: Average Threat Scores, Subject-Classified Analysis Rich Medium Poor Small countries 7.2292 9.1667 9.9167 Large countries 11.7083 9.6081 11.7500 63 Tests of difference between means revealed only three which were significant. The results may be summarized as follows: When wealth is controlled, large countries are seen as more threatening than small countries at the rich and poor level, but not at the intermediate level. When population is controlled, there is only one significant difference in the table: large, poor countries are more threatening than large countries of medium wealth or energy consumption. Ideology, Population and Energy When these three factors are taken into consideration at the same time, as variables of classification, the 288 observations which form the basis for this analysis are cast into twelve cells with 2n observations in each cell. The mean threat scores in each cell refer to two countries. TABLE 19a Ideology, Population and Energy: Average Threat Scores Non-Communist Countries Communist Countries Small Large Small Large Rich 1.0000 8.5833 11.3333 19.8750 Rich Medium 7.0833 “.5833 11.9583 8.6667 Medium Poor 7.7500 9.9167 11.1667 20.8333 Poor The figures above are from the experimenter-classified analysis. The AOV table data are: TABLE 19b Ideology-Population-Energy Interaction Source of Sum of Mean Observed Variance Sauares 2:. Sguare Error F P.95 Id.-Pop.-Bn. 825.0986 2 313.M253 18.69““ 11.3683 3.26 with 2/36 df 614 The observed F is significant at the .01 level, and therefore it seems useful to examine the countries in each cell: TABLE 20. Countries Experimenter Classified by Ideology, Population and Energy Non-Communist Countries Communist Countries Small Large Small Large Rich New Zealand United States Czechoslovakia Poland Switzerland France East Germany .Soviet Union Medium Formosa Mexico Cuba Yugoslavia Algeria Japan Hungary Roumania Poor Congo Pakistan Albania North Viet-Nam South Viet-Nam Indonesia North Korea China (mainland) The means in the last table are difficult to summarize. Ex- ceptions are evident for the main effects which have been reported. (For examining this table, a difference of 3.1625 between two means indicates a significant difference.) Communist countries have been found more threatening than non- Communist countries, but Yugoslavia and Roumania are seen as no more threatening than four of the six non—Communist cells. However, with population and energy controlled, there are significant ideology differences in each pair of six cells. Large countries have been found more threatening than small countries, but when ideology and energy consumption are controlled, the Opposite is found at the medium energy consumption level: Formosa and Algeria are seen as more threatening than Mexico and Japan. And Cuba and Hungary are seen as more threatening than Yugoslavia and Roumania 65 by this sample. The large-small differences are not significant in the case of poor non-Communist countries, and are greatest in the case of poor Communist countries. Poor countries are more threatening than rich countries only in the small, non-Communist cells and the large, Communist cells. The generalization that poor countries are more threatening than medium energy consumption countries is sustained for the large countries, but rejected for the small countries in this sample. It is difficult to summarize these findings, but it is possible to note six levels of threat which are attributed to the countries in these cells, plus a seventh level which overlaps two levels: TABLE 21 Levels of Perceived Threat Least threatening Most threatening New Zealand Mexico Congo Albania Soviet Union China(mainland) Switzerland Japan South Viet-Nam North Korea Poland North Viet-Nam United States Czechoslovakia France East Germany Yugoslavia Cuba Roumania Hungary Pakistan Indonesia Formosa Algeria In the subject-classified analysis, it will not be possible to specify the countries in each cell. Here are the cell means: 66 TABLE 22a Ideology, Population and Energy: Average Threat Scores, Subject-Classified Analysis Non-Communist Countries Communist Countries Small Large Small Large Rich 3.8583 7.2917 11.0000 16.1250 Medium 7.0833 6.5ul7 11.2500 12.6667 Poor 7.0833 7.6250 11.7500 15.8750 TABLE 22b Ideology-Population-Energy Interaction, Subject-Classified Analysis Source of Sum of Mean Observed Variance Sguares 2£_ Sauare Error F F.95 Id.-Pop.-En. 11.1319 2 5.5660 3u.2766 0.1623 3.26 with 2/36 df Here the ordinal relationships between the means in most cases remains the same as in the experimenter-classified analysis, but the magnitude of the differences has decreased, and the error term has in- creased. The result is that when ideology is controlled there are no significant differences between population or energy levels. However, when population and energy are controlled, there are significant differences between the two values of ideology in every case. Nationality Subsamples In previous pages threat scores for the entire sample of people have been cast into cells on the basis of three "treatment" variables. Now, "levels" or subsamples of subjects will be brought into the analysis. 67 To put it another way, the focus of interest changes from characteristics of the stimulus country to characteristics of the respondent. The first question: Is there a main nationality effect? That is, are there significant differences between mean threat scores of sub- samples fOr the entire sample of countries? TABLE 23a Nationality: Average Threat Scores for Six Subsamples N Threat Scores (observations) Mean S.D. Brazilians ”8 6.0833 10.01u5 Egyptians 48 5.1858 7.3802 Chinese (Formosan) us 18.1u58 12.887u Indians #8 4.3958 6.7026 Americans #8 19.8750 9.u3u3 0.3. Experts #8 9.2292 9.5910 Since there is no breakdown of countries, the data here is the same for the experimenter-classified and the subject-classified analysis: TABLE 23b Main Nationality Effect Source of Sum of Mean Observed Variance Sguares ‘25_ Sauare Error F P.95 Nationality 8899.091? 5 1779.8083 503.0u63 3.5381 2.77 with 5/18 df The main nationality effect is significant at the .05 level, but it is limited by interactions to be reported on the following pages. 68 Ideology of Country and Nationality of Subject When threat scores are divided into those of Communist and non- Communist countries, are there significant differences across the nationality subsamples? Here are the results in the experimenter- classified analysis: TABLE 24a Ideology and Nationality: Average Threat Scores Non-Communist Communist Difference Brazilians .6667 11.5000 10.8333 Egyptians n.5u17 5.7500 1.2083 Chinese 9.8333 18.8583 8.6250 Indians u.oooo “.7917 .7917 Americans l3.u167 26.3333 12.9166 U.S. Experts 6.US83 12.0000 5.5u17 TABLE 2ub Ideology-Nationality Interaction Source of Sum of Mean Observed Variance Sguares 2P; Sauare Error F F. 95 Nationality- 1509.9861 5 301.9972 132.7963 2.27M1 2.77 with 5/18 df Ideology Here the interaction approaches but does not reach significance at the .05 level. The differences between Communist and non-Communist means are presented for comparison with the subject classified analysis which follows: 69 TABLE 258 Ideology and Nationality: Average Threat Scores, Subject-Classified Analysis Non-Communist Communist Difference Brazilians 1.2083 10.9583 9.7500 Egyptians “.5“17 5.7500 1.2083 Chinese 9.7916 18.5000 8.708“ Indians 3.9167 “.8750 .9583 Americans 13.1250 26.6250 13.5000 U.S. Experts 6.5000 11.9583 5.“583 TABLE 25b Ideology-Nationality Interaction Source of Sum of Mean Observed Variance Sauares 9}; Sguare Error F F.95 Nationality- 1““5.2813 5 289.0562 101.1030 2.8590 2.77 with 5/18 df Ideology Here the interaction between ideology and nationality is signifi- cant. An examination of pairs of means with t-tests indicates that when subject nationality is controlled, there are no significant differences between Communist and non-Communist countries. Neither are there significant differences across nationalities in the case of non-Communist countries. However, in the case of Communist countries, there are significant differences between the Americans and the Egyptians in the sample, and also between the Americans and the Indians. Here nationality of subject is a somewhat stronger effect than ideology of country. 70 Population of Country and Nationality of Subject Does the perceived threat of large, as opposed to small, countries vary significantly across nationality subsamples? Here are the mean cell values and P-ratio data for the experimenter-classified analysis: TABLE 26a Population and Nationality: Average Threat Scores Small Large Difference Brazilians 5.5833 6.5833 1.0000 Egyptians 3.6667 6.6250 2.9583 Chinese 11.7083 16.5833 “.8750 Indians 1.5000 7.2917 5.7917 Americans 19.1667 20.5833 1.“166 U.S. Experts 8.6667 9.7917 1.1250 TABLE 26b Population-Nationality Interaction Source of Sum of Mean Observed Variance Sguares 2E. Sauare Error F F.95 Pop.-Nat'1. 25“.6111 5 50.9222 35.81“8 1.“218 2.77 with 5/18 df Here, there is no significant interaction between the nationality of the subject and the population size of the country. In other words, judgements of the degree of threat associated with large in contrast to small population countries is relatively stable across nationality sub-groups in this sample. 71 In the table, a significant difference between a pair of means must be greater than 6.27. When nationality is controlled, there is no significant difference between the threat scores of large and small countries, although the means for large countries are higher in every case. When population is controlled, there are significant differences between nationalities. For small countries the mean for the American subsample is greater than for any other subgroup; fer large countries the American mean is greater than the rest with the exception of the Chinese. For large countries, the Chinese mean is greater than that of the Egyptians, Indians, Brazilians and U.S. experts. For small countries, it is greater than that of Egyptians and Indians. And for small countries only, the mean of the U.S. experts is greater than that of the Indian subsample. In this table, nationality of the subject is a stronger effect than the population of the country being judged with regard to threat. Chinese and Indian subjects make the greatest distinction between large and small countries in the experimenter-classified analysis. Ideology, Population and Nationality In the following table of means, control for ideology has been added to the two factors just previously discussed: 72 TABLE 27a Ideology, Population and Nationality: Average Threat Scores Non-Communist Communist Small Large Small Large Brazilians .1667 1.1667 11.0000 12.0000 Egyptians 2.7500 6.3333 “.5833 6.9167 Chinese 7.8333 11.8333 15.5833 21.3333 Indians 1.“167 6.5833 1.5833 8.0000 Americans 13.5833 13.2500 2“.7500 27.9167 U.S. Experts 5.9167 7.0000 11.“167 12.5833 TABLE 27b Ideology-Population-Nationality Interaction Source of Sum of Mean Observed Variance Sauares 2f; Sguare m F £1.23 Id.-Pop.- “1.1111 5 8.2222 10.1667 0.8087 2.77 with 5/18 df Nationality This triple interaction is not significant. However, it seems useful to examine the means in this table with t-tests. Here any difference greater than 3.5600 is significant. When papulation and nationality are controlled, there are not always significant differences between Communist and non-Communist countries. They exist for the Brazilian, Chinese, American and U.S. expert subsamples, but not for Egyptians and Indians. These differences are stable across large and small countries. 73 When ideology and nationality are controlled, small-large differences appear in the case of Communist countries for Chinese and Indians, and in the case of non-Communist countries for Chinese, Indians and Egyptians. When ideology and population are controlled, nationality differences are evident in all four columns. These will not all be detailed, but it can be seen that the similarity between the Indian and Egyptian sub- samples is stable across the four columns. It is also interesting to note that Americans can not only be clearly discriminated from the other nationality subsamples, but from the U.S. experts as well. Energy and Nationalitv Are there significant differences in threat scores across nationality subsamples for countries of three different energy consumption levels? Here are the findings from the experimenter-classified analysis: TABLE 28a Energy and Nationality: Average Threat Scores Rich Medium Poor Brazilians 6.7500 5.5625 5.9375 Egyptians 7.6875 2.6875 5.0625 Chinese 12.5625 12.5625 17.3125 Indians 1.7500 1.5625 9.8750 Americans 16.9375 18.0000 2“.6875 U.S. Experts 8.0000 8.0625 11.6250 7“ TABLE 28]: Energy-Nationality Interaction Source of Sum of Mean Observed Variance Sauares 2E; Sauare Error F F. 95 En.-Nat. 862.7708 10 86.2771 20.“977 “.2091 2.10 with 10/36 df The interaction is significant at the .01 level. When nationality is controlled the only significant differences between rich, medium and poor countries are those between medium and poor countries for Indians and Americans. (A significant difference here must be greater than 5.3806). When energy level is controlled, a number of differences be- tween nationality subsamples are visible, but not all are stable across the three columns. The corresponding figures for the subject-classified analysis are: TABLE 29a Energy and Nationality: Average Threat Scores, Subject-Classified Analysis Rich Medium Poor Brazilians 6.6250 6.1250 5.5000 Egyptians 8.5000 3.000 3.9375 Chinese 12.6875 1“.1875 15.5625 Indians 5.1875 2.6875 5.3125 Americans 16.8125 20.6875 22.1250 U.S. Experts 7.0000 9.6250 11.0625 75 TABLE 29b Energy-Nationality Interaction Source of Sum of Mean Observed Variance Sguares '2: Ssuare Error F F.95 En.-Nat 715.0625 10 71.5062 50.83““ 1.0667 2.10 with 10/36 df In contrast with the previous data, the interaction of energy and nationality is not significant in the case of the subject-classified analysis. The error term has increased considerably, and here a significant difference between a pair of means must be greater than 13.3039. When nationality is controlled, all differences between threat scores for countries of different energy levels disappear. When energy level of country is controlled, some nationality differences remain. Americans can be discriminated from Brazilians, Egyptians and Indians for medium and poor countries, but not for rich countries. Here, again, nationality is the stronger effect. Ideology, Energy and Nationality What happens to the findings when ideology is added to the two factors examined above? Here are the results of the experimenter. classified analysis: TABLE 76 30a Ideology, Energy and Nationality: Average Threat Scores Non-Communist Communist Rich Medium Poor Rich Medium Poor Brazilians 1.625 0.250 0.125 11.875 10.875 11.750 Egyptians 8.750 2.000 2.875 6.625 3.375 7.250 Chinese 5.750 11.625 12.126 19.375 13.500 22.500 Indians 1.500 1.500 9.000 2.000 1.625 10.750 Americans 7.500 12.375 20.375 26.375 23.625 29.000 U.S. Experts 3.625 7.250 8.500 12.375 8.875 1“.7SO TABLE 30b Ideology-Energy-Nationality Interaction Source of Sum of Mean Observed Variance Sguares 2E. Sguare Error F F.95 Id.-En.- 53“.“931 10 S3.““93 17.6019 3.0366 2.10 with 10/36 df Nat'lty. In this table a significant difference between a pair of means must be greater than 9.2“1. This triple interaction is significant at the .01 level. When energy level and nationality are controlled, there are differences in threat scores between Communist and non-Communist countries for Brazilians at three energy levels, for Chinese at two energy levels (rich and poor), and for Americans at two levels (rich and medium). When ideology and nationality are controlled, the only significant difference for energy levels of countries is the discriminations that the American subjects make between rich and poor non-Communist countries. 77 And when ideology and energy are controlled, there are nationality differences in every column except the one for rich, non-Communist countries. In no case are there significant differences between Egyptians and The similarity of Egyptians and Indians is also stable across Indians. the two analyses. Population, Energy and Nationality The final triple interaction to be examined is similar to the last section, but ideology has been replaced by pOpulation. The data from the experimenter-classified analysis follows: TABLE 31a Population, Energy and Nationality: Average Threat Scores Small Countries Large Countries Rich Medium Poor Rich Medium Poor Brazilians 5.250 7.500 “.000 8.250 3.625 7.875 Egyptians 2.875 2.500 5.625 12.500 2.875 “.500 Chinese 8.375 13.875 12.875 16.750 11.250 21.750 Indians 1.500 1.375 1.625 2.000 1.750 18.125 Americans 12.625 21.500 23.375 21.250 1“.500 26.000 U.S. Experts 6.375 10.375 9.250 9.625 5.750 1“.000 TABLE 31b Population-Energy-Nationality Interaction Source of Sum of Mean Observed Variance Sauares 2E; Ssuare Error F Pop.-En.- 9“6.618l 10 9“.6618 22.9“68 “.1253 2.10 with 10/36 df “at. ltye 78 The interaction of these three factors is significant at the .01 level. In this table a difference of 12.0“7 between a pair of means is the minimum for significance. When energy and nationality are controlled, in 13 of the 18 cases here the mean threat score for large countries is greater than for small countries, but in only one case does the difference attain significance: the judgements of Indians concerning poor countries. When pOpulation and nationality are controlled, there are no energy effects for small countries, and only one for large countries which reaches significance: in the judgement of the Indian subsample, large, poor countries are more threatening than large rich countries. When pOpulation and energy are controlled, there are significant nationality effects in every column except the one for small, rich countries. On the next page are displayed both the experimenter-classified and subject-classified tables of means subdivided by all four factors. Each of the 72 cells consists of four observations. The AOV table data are: TABLE 32a Four-factor Interaction Source of Sum of Mean Observed Variance Sauares EF- 5 uare m F 32.9.: Experimenter-Classified Id.-Pop.- 218.6181 10 21.8618 18.69““ 1.169“ 2.10 with 10/36 En.-Nat1. df Subject-Classified Id.-Pop.- “35.6180 10 “3.5618 33.3715 1.3053 2.10 with 10/36 En.-Natl. df muneaxm .m.0 mceownee< snowman omecwzo mcmuummwu mcewawnenm .munonxm .m.0 q. mceofinoa< 7 mcemvcH onecanu mceuua>wm mamwawumnm mb.md 00.0w 00.HH mm.:m 00.5 mm.m noom Eran»: mN.HA 0m.0m 00.N om.0m mh.w 00.HH mb.NH 0m.bn mw.b mm.mn om.md 0m.:H scam momnpcsou owned 00.0H 00.30 mb.md mw.mm 0m.m 0m.mH mm.0 om.HN mh.H mb.NH 0m.w mm.b Loom suave: newpucsoo swung 0m.NH mu.0m 0m.m 00.nm mb.m mm.na some 00.0d mn.mw mN.H 00.5H 0m.: mb.0 Loom mN.HH 00.0w mh.H mb.0H mm.: 00.0H sane»: mh.h 0m.:m 00.0 0m.mH 0m.m mb.m scam newnunooo Hamew ”OHEHCflPOU u. a.“ GSEEOO 0m.0H 00.3“ mb.H mb.0H 00.0 00.0 noon 0m.0 0m.NH 0m.h mh.:H 0m. 00.: noon 00.0 mN.HH 0m.H mm.mH mN.H 00.0 ashes: 00.5 mN.:H mm.d 00.m 00.na mm.m scam mofinucsoo omnmq eoemmmmaao-poomnsm 0m.HH mb.mm 0m.H mm.:a mm.: 0m.:H ease»: mm.wa 0m.:m 0m.H mh.mH 0m.m 0m.0a goes mewsucsoo HHmEm moanucnoo umficneeoo vofiwwmmeaounoucoEmLomxm 00.0 00.0H 0m.0H mw.mH 0m. mm. Loom newnucsoo umwcseeooucoz mm.m 0m.b mh.H mh.m mm.m 00.0 Esfivmz mb.0 mm.:H 0m.H 0m.0a mm.mH mm.m coax newnpcsoo ownmq mofipucooo unficseeooncoz monoow peonch ommno>< “czowxmosm nouommnssom nan mqm Q (5 I 6? c> é? c> <3 0 zuczwémflW/‘b 9’ to Q ‘0 O O Argentina_:_:_:_:__:__.__.__:_:L:_:_:_:_:_Argentina Bulgaria_:__° ° : ' : ' ° ° :X:_° °__:__Bulgaria Portugal__° ° ° ° : ' ° ' ° : ° ° ° :__Portugal 0 O O O O O O O C C 0 .-~.-._*----* In the example above, one person has estimated that in Argentina the average energy consumption per person is 1000-1500 units in Argentina, and 1500-2000 units in Bulgaria. For practice, before turning the page, make an estimate of the wealth per person of Portugal. Then turn the page. wealth of each country. (mainland)China: : lllllll Make an "X" on each of the lines below to indicate the per ca ita Energy ConsumptiggnflgPer Person Albania :__:__° ° 0 O O C O 0 Algeria :__:_: ° ' O. 0 O 0 O O 0 Congo:_: ~ . . O O 0 O O O O O O 0 -—_‘--“-‘—~— Cuba :__: : Czechoslovakia-g . . . e e e e O I O O -—_-——-_-- East Germany:__: ° : 0 (Taiwan) Formosa ;-:_. . . O I O O O O O O O O O O ’-—--—_----— France :___: : ° ~ Hungary :_. . . . Indones ia :__: ° ° ° Japan :__: ' ° ' Mexico :__: ° ° ° New Zealand: : ' ' ' North Korea ::: ' ' ° North Viet-Nam: : : ' ° Pakistan :__:___° ° ° Poland:_: Roumania:_: ° ' ° South Viet-Nam: : - - - Soviet Union:_:_° ° ‘ Switzerland:_: . . . United States:__:__° : ' YugoslaVia :__:_' ° ‘ 0 O O O O O O O O O O O O 0 O O O O O 0 O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O 0 O O O O O 0 O O O O O O O O O O O 2 O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O —-—_‘__—*---- O O O O O 2 .__Albania Make sure you have made an "X" on each line to indicate the wealth per person of each country. Then turn the page. For detailed instructions, see the prev one page. .__Algeria ____ _China( mainland )_ _Congo _- __Cuba ......... _Czechoslovakia East Germany :Formosa( Taiwan: _France .._._.... _Hungary _Indonesia Japan -Hexico -New Zealand —North Korea —North Viet-Nar— :Pakistan “ Poland :Roumania _South Viet-Nam____ ._Soviet Union _Switzerland _United States _Yugoslavia In the space provided below, please make your best estimate of the total national population of each of these countries to the nearest million. For example, if yourbest guess of the total population of Italy is fifty million, write a "50" in the space by the name of the country: 0’0 Italy Now make your best estimate of the populations of the countries listed below: Yugoslavia Japan United States Indonesia Switzerland Hungary Soviet Union France South Viet-Nam Formosa (Taiwan) Roumania East Germany ‘______ Poland Czechoslovakia Pakistan Cuba North Viet-Nam Congo North Korea China (mainland) ______ New Zealand Algeria Mexico Albania Before turning the page, please make sure that you have made at least a rough guess about the pOpulation of each country to the nearest mi 11101). Below are listed the same countries you have seen on previous pages. This time you are to examine them in terms of the degree to which each government is communist or non-communist. 1. Look at the names of all the countries listed below. Which country has a government which is the most non-communist, that is, which government has the least communist influence of-Ehy on the list? Hark a "l" in front of this country. 2. Which country has a government which is the most completely communist one on the list? Place an "8" in front of this country. 3. From the countries you have not yet marked, select two countries which have the most non- communist governments. Hark a "2“-innfront of each of these 512 comries. “. Choose two of the remaining countries which have the most communist governments. MEFF'a "7" in front of each of these two countries. 5. Now choose four of the countries you have not yet marked which have the most Eggrcommunist governments, and place a "3" in front of these four countries. 6. Choose four of the countries you have not yet marked which have the most communist governments, and place a "6" in front of these four countries. 7. If you have followed carefully the instructions above, there should now be ten countries on the list below that you have not yet marked. Select five of the ten which have the most Egg-communist governments, and place a "“" in front of these five countries. 8. Place a "5" in front of the five countries which are left. ‘ Albania Mexico Algeria New Zealand China (mainland) North Korea Congo North Viet-Nam Summary_of_lnstructions Cuba Pakistan 1 one (most non- Communist) Czechoslovakia Poland 2 twos “ threes East Germany Roumania 5 fours "' ' 5 fives Formosa (Taiwan) South Viet-Nam “ sixes 2 sevens France Soviet Union 1 eight "'"'""' """' (most Hungary Switzerland Communist) Indonesia United States Japan Yugoslavia After you have placed a number in front of each of the countries according to the instructions above, turn the page. Please indicate your disapproval or approval of the following statement: I think that my country should give up some of its soverignty to become part of a united world government--in the same way that a state or provincial government gives up some of its power to a national government. Strongly disapprove Approve Uncertain Disapprove Strongly approve Thank you for your cooperation. Your responses will be treated in a completely confidential manner, and no names of individuals will be used in any report of this study. Please fill in the biographical in- formation requested: Name__ _(name not necessary) Sex: ____Male ____Female Age: ‘4‘ Occupation or field of study: College or university graduate Yes No In what country were you born? What is your nationality? ___ Is either your mother or your father of a nationality different from that of the country in which you were born? ____Yes ___No Name any country--or countries--other than your own that you lived in for more than one year before the age of 21:_¢ ‘4 Date THE END COMMENTS: M'TITI'I‘HLMTILEJ/fllflzfi!Lifliujfyifllflflfljfiflmfl'Es