‘_ n “.1... . M: I I J "W 1 ,- t‘.‘ -X “mfg”- I ‘lu‘h‘ I ..41‘g9,1.1,' {I‘m “-1‘ I‘M 1;!” ' {HEM ""129. «111: . 2" , . Wan-v" . J 11:}!11"'1 "’L IIJ1‘\‘I, .; 111.: .1Jf'.. J‘y‘u‘fl" jI H‘“: I 1:12 ’ 411‘. 112 .1 JJ‘I'n.‘W £21 4.111331 ASTHM- .1. 3......5. F I _J. .I‘-‘ {1.31.1 1V1foI-1AI1131“.. 7153"“ .“ (ism "9\1\“~. ,m ‘1. . _ Ifi'm '9. PM“ ‘i' ,1JI: f I‘ "‘4‘th “3;: 'lfl' 133% ‘34 3.21”, “lit, "33.? 1' III?" '5‘“ I '{Eh {j q Jill’ J_ “I“: .J’J'.’ . I 1.1 .1 mi, 111'. . "L: 'J’J} "Lyn-N , 'fl 1.. ‘4: 1 1w“. , I. .1 9.1.1:.- gafinr‘ “3": Sin-"gem? g 1' . ... 3 “4:11.535; ”1:21:15 IP11? 11' - ;‘ ‘ ‘32:“... II, '321‘119 . -. .. 29‘ ‘M. “1553931“ ‘64‘531 UL 'J‘JIIJ.“ $4M“ 2 £144.11 :1. 5111111. $712“ 15 ., 1 1'11} " "3" 1311‘ 1"{46 fl 11NML1'T1111 111;;(11113111111111111111111111‘1‘; If)? 1111‘551Ig1111111aifi111‘31.113%)? “iii :1 I: ~ .1 ‘ 1,. . ' ' “.31“ 1..-... . fifi‘fi-fi‘éw J‘ ' ‘W‘ " 3491”» .1“ v.1”dh .magp; :A‘ .11 ‘I J H'fibfl‘ I 9111111351 .9. . 1... "w n’: 'J‘Jr‘zt'. :,:J."|\..u 1-‘(1‘1‘an12‘211 ".191. .1 .': 'U‘ I' if“ (‘I 961;“ 1.3" . . .u.J.:.JII~J-J .111 ”41"" a. J: ’9' fly“ “.6 ‘4’“. hmilur‘ 5.2,. 4:11,} "319172 LgJ , ‘L'fi-M ‘34“! :3!" ~ g if“ ‘9‘“: right. 'u'J‘V‘ 1‘ ‘ I”? 3? $11133. J... 1. "$3.: . . I15. ‘ '1 . Km; .7 538‘}: I I. c ’1‘“ ’2: . ~32... 9.....311-J ‘ ~‘« ‘1‘ lei; ‘15 u £1“ 1 . ""‘N‘fJ‘I M12. ' J Us: 1 2'1 Q‘WLL‘U ‘51::3: .1 .11 .11 1171111 13:11... 9.111111% ”'9: ,J . 1 4... Tug?“ :8 IL!“ 411! 9.1.2:??? 5,... .J "w‘ gmm 4‘1.“ 2min *J I._.,1,1I‘£§1.‘.Hnt If $1311. 22?.“ ..‘I (1 1‘6” . ,J ‘ El , ‘J I ‘ ’ L‘a‘qu -.'l ._ '5 J J1]; 3 1 fl.l.«,...f‘. " I ‘ 1" 1") t u: 4:1)? 19“ 23"” ”it? @3211 9 J31 ‘2'?!" “I 11.43““ 3.‘ 94 “1:. a. :. $111321 ‘ . ' 2 "#75"; “11%“: ‘I‘ I . 9.7. iii: 53' “ht-I '15:. F‘ .431 m. it 4! 91! “I‘m, (5)45} ‘IIIIJ 1_‘3L:I1‘p" R 2431' r“. . ”.155 mg mp} n1“ $1.11. 53;: "qu 13f 111-1 " 4 “11111112 .. if"? J .1: 29:. #1:»; “1le l' ‘3' ‘zl'ipuJ V ¢ ‘ :~—' H}? “'V'JJ‘.‘ -_ . “a hzhwxfl ‘ ‘ ' .. 1 3%,: 1:43! 1“ é'fif‘? (‘1 "flaked,“ I Liv yuan-n»: hymn.“ . C“ 1. 5:; .1339 u"\ Jaw. .. W: L ‘1 w. 9 {3}” 7 ‘Ll‘giti5‘fiJ-m .w ‘- .3115 9:35.315; va‘é‘fi: \{ ”337‘. 111% “2.32:2. ”liq, mug urn _ "‘9‘ .953?“ .r‘ H 3,: wk «s ”via ‘12:“, 315’?“ ' 233.51. 353? ‘ . wl‘d“ ‘% . A! 31%?“ 1331‘ 34“? . ' 33:51 $.51»; $6,355: :15». ’ .. . :6: ~33? ‘49; ‘92: 91 ‘II 111 .31. "a" 2...: J 932:: .35 g‘n‘ 59‘s; ’JJI; THESIS LIBRARY Michigan State University This is to certify that the dissertation entitled THE INTENTION T0 REMAIN CHILDLESS: SEPARATION RESPONSE, SEX ROLE IDENTITY AND FAMILY BACKGROUND presented by Linda Sue Cohen has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for __Bh4JL__degree in m JZ%W Major professor Belem; MSU is an Affirmative Anion/Equal Opportunily Institution 0- l 2771 MSU LIBRARIES m - RETURNING MATERIALS: Place in book drop to remove this checkout from your record. FINES wiII be charged if book is returned after the date stamped beIow. THE INTENTION TO REMAIN CHILDLESS: SEPARATION RESPONSE, SEX ROLE IDENTITY AND FAMILY BACKGROUND BY Linda Sue Cohen A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Psychology 1984 317;?b00 ABSTRACT THE INTENTION TO REMAIN CHILDLESS: SEPARATION RESPONSE, SEX ROLE IDENTITY AND FAMILY BACKGROUND BY Linda Sue Cohen The purpose of the present study was to gain a better understanding of why women decide not to parent. More specifically the intent was to identify some of the develop- mental, personality and family background variables that distinguish women who state an intention not to parent from those who intend to mother. The variables studied were attachment and individuation need, sex role identity, iden- tification with mother and perceived warmth, control and cognitive involvement of parents. All subjects were drawn from.Introductory Psychology courses at Michigan State University. The sample was come posed of 34 women who were very certain they intend to parent and 26 women who were moderately or very certain they intend not to parent. Attachment and individuation needs were measured by the Separation Anxiety Test (Hansburg, 1972, 1980). The Bem Sex Role Inventory (Ban, 1981) was used to evaluate sex role identity and identification with mother. Perceptions of parents were measured using the Parent Behavior Form (Kelly and Worrell, 1976). Linda Sue Cohen The hypothesis that women who intend not to parent would show a lower attachment and higher individuation need than women who intend to mother was not supported by the data. Contrary to prediction there were no differences between groups in attachment and a trend suggested that future childfree women are actually lower in individuation than future parents. As predicted, childfree women were more likely to have a masculine sex role identity, were lower in feminity, were less identified with their mothers, and saw their mothers as less warm and more rejecting than women who intend to parent. Also, as predicted there were no differences between groups in perceived control exerted by parents. However, contrary to expectations, no differ- ences between groups were found in warmth of father and cognitive involvement of parents. The results were discussed in the context of an object relations theory of female deve10pment. Methodological issues including instrument reliability and validity, sam- pling bias and research design were also examined. In memory of my Mother who would have delighted in sharing the joy of this achievement ii ACKNOWLEDGMENTS To Dr. Albert Rabin, who encouraged me to explore the ques- tions of interest to me, provided professional guidance and generously agreed to continue as chairperson of my committee after his retirement. To Dr. Linda Jackson for her careful reading of the text, valuable feedback and, most of all, for her interest in this project. To Dr. Charles Hanley for his statistical advice. To Dr. Gilbert DeRath for taking the time from his busy schedule to serve on my committee. To Beth Grant and Michelle Powers for their perseverance during the tedious task of rating projective stories. And a special thanks to my friends and valued colleagues; To Terry, who continues to impress me with his knowl- edge, sensitivity, understanding and capacity to make me laugh, especially at myself. And to Cheryl for her warmth, acceptance, unwavering support and help in placing things in perspective. iii TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES . . . . . . . . INTRODUCTION 0 O O O O O O O 0 REVIEW OF THE LITERATURE . . . Traditional Analytic Theories of the Wish to Parent . . . . . A Psychoanalytic Reformulation of Mothering . Voluntary Childlessness: Norm O O O O O O O 0 Sex Role Identity . . . . Family Background . . . . HYPOTHESES O O O O O O O O O 0 METHOD 0 O O O O O O O O O O O SUbjeCtS O O O O O O 0 0 Instruments . . . . . . . Procedure . . . Hypotheses Operationally Defined Statistic Analysis . . . RESULTS 0 O O O O O O O O O 0 Distribution of Parenting Hypotheses . . . . . . . Additional Findings . . . DISCUSSION 0 O O O O O O O O O The Sample . . . . . . . Demographics . . . . . . Response to Separation . Sex Role Identity . . . . Deviation from the O O O O O O O O O O 0 Intentions Identification with Parents Family Background Variables Implications for Future Rese r h iv 0 O a O O C vi 10 19 27 29 29 35 51 53 56 57 57 64 REFERENCES 0 O O O O O O O O O I O O O O O O APPENDICES Iaaza'ubaucou1> Background Information Questionnaire Separation Anxiety Test . . . . . . . Chart for Controlled Association . . Pattern Summary Chart . . . . . . . . Scoring Criteria for Projective Stories Bem Sex Role Inventory . . . . . . . . Parent Behavior Form . . . . . . . . . Parent Behavior Form Scale Descriptions Consent Form . . . . . . . . . . . . . 105 112 114 139 140 141 144 147 154 156 Table 10 LIST OF TABLES Comparison of Demographic Variables for the Student Population, the Intent to Parent Group and the Intent not to Parent Group .. .. .. Interrater Reliability for Projective Ratings of Attachment and Individuation on the Separa- tion Aan-ety TeSt O O O O O O O O O O O O 0 O 0 Distribution of Parenting Intentions in the Total Student Sample . . . . . . . . . . . . . Analysis of Variance Comparing Women Who Intend to Parent and WOmen Who Intend Not to Parent on Attachment Need As Measured by the Separation Anxiety Test . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Analysis of Variance Comparing Women Who Intend to Parent and Women Who Intend Not to Parent on Individuation Need As Measured by the Separa- tion Anxiety Test . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Chi Square Comparing Number of Women Who Intend to Parent and Number of Women Who Intend to be Childfree with a Masculine Sex Role Identity on the Bem Sex Role Inventory . . . . . . . . . Analysis of Variance Comparing Women who Intend Not to Parent on Perceived Similarity to Mother on Sex Role Characteristics as Measured by the Bem Sex Role Inventory . . . . . . . . . . . . Analysis of Variance Comparing Women who Intend to Parent and Women who Intend Not to Parent on Perceptions of Parent Behaviors on the Parent Behavior Form . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Analysis of Variance Comparing Women who Intend to Parent and Women who Intend not to Parent on Components of Attachment and Individuation on the Separation Anxiety Test . . . . . . . . . . Analysis of Covariance Comparing Women who Intend to Parent and WOmen who Intend not to Parent on Mean Attachment and Individuation Scores on the Separation Anxiety Test with the Number of Test Responses as a Covariate .. vi 31 45 57 58 59 61 62 63 66 69 Table 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 Chi Square Comparing Women who Intend to Parent and Women who Intend Not to Parent on Sex Role Identity Measured by the Bem Sex Role Inventory Analysis of Variance Comparing WOmen who Intend to Parent and Women who Intend not to Parent on Masculinity and Femininity on the Bem Sex Role Inventory 0 O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O 0 Analysis of Variance Comparing Women who Intend to Parent and Women who Intend not to Parent on Perceived Masculinity and Feminin- ity of Parents on the Bem Sex Role Inventory . Analysis of Variance Comparing Women who intend to Parent and Women who Intend Not to Parent on Perceived Similarity to Father on Sex Role Characteristics on the Bem Sex Role Inventory . Analysis of Variance Comparing Women who Intend to Parent and Women who Intend Not to Parent on Mean Scores on Parent Behavior Scales of the Parent Behavior Form . . . . . . . . . . . . . Analysis of Covariance Comparing Women who Intend to Parent and Women who Intend Not to Parent 0n Mean Scores of Parent Behavior Factors of the Parent Behavior Form with Social Desirability as the Covariate . . . . . Analysis of Covariance Comparing Women who Intend to Parent and WOmen who Intend Not to Parent on Mean Scores on Parent Behavior Scales of Parent Behavior Form with Social Desirability as the Covariate . . . . . . . . . vii 69 70 71 72 73 75 76 INTRODUCTION There has been a dramatic decrease in fertility in the United States since the early 1960%h The fertility rate changed from 3.6 children per woman in 1961 to less than 1.8 children per woman in 1975. A major factor among others, that has contributed to this change is an increase in inten- tional childlessness1 (Silka and Kiesler, 1977). The incidence of voluntary childlessness in the United States has fluctuated with the social, economic and politi- cal climate of the country. It reached a peak during the Depression of the 1930's and early 40's and rapidly declined following World War II as men returned home from the ser- vice, the feminine mystique took hold, and the post war baby boom ensued. This trend continued until the 1970!s when an upsurge in intentional childlessness began (Renne, 1976; Veevers, 1974). Statistics show that by the mid—1970's fifteen percent of a sample of college students “LS. Bureau of the Census, 1976), sixteen percent of high school stu- dents (Silka and Kiesler, 1977) and 4.6% of all wives under thirty (U.S. Bureau of the Census, 1976) stated an intention to remain childless throughout the childbearing years. 1The author recognizes that the terms childless and childfree carry with them different connotations, with childless suggesting a void and childfree implying freedom from a burden. For lack of a more neutral term, childless and childfree are used interchangeably in this document. 1 2 Traditionally mothering was considered to be an innate instinct and motherhood a biological inevitability (Shields, 1975). However, the growth of the women's liberation move- ment, rising concerns about overpopulation and the develop- ment of more sophisticated and effective birth control tech- nology have resulted in increased flexibility in prescribed roles for men and women, a critical look at parenting and increased consideration of a childfree lifestyle. Despite these recent changes, our society is still dominated by a pronatalist ideology (Bardwick, 1971; Maxwell and Montgomery, 1969; Russo, 1976, 1979; Veevers, 1974). Women continue to be expected to mother and those who choose not to are stereotyped as selfish, immature and emotionally disturbed (Calhoun and Selby, 1980; Jamison, Franzini and Kaplan, 1979) and are subjected to considerable pressures and sanctions by society, family and friends (Houseknecht, 1977; Levine, 1978). Veevers (1973) has suggested that it is this pronatal- ist attitude that has impeded the growth of psychological research of intentional nonparenthood. Only one study to date and that unpublished has attempted to explore the psychodynamic factors underlying the intention not to par- ent. Drawing on psychoanalytic theories of psychogenic infertility, Levine (1978) hypothesized that childless women would demonstrate more intense sibling rivalry, greater unresolved oedipal concerns and a weaker identification with their mothers than women who mother. The data provided at least partial support for all three hypotheses. 3 The results of Levinefls study suggest that the psycho- dynamic history of women with differing reproductive inten- tions is a significant yet virtually unexplored area of study. The present investigation attempted to contribute to this body of research by proposing and testing an object relations theory of intentional childlessness in college age women. It also attempted to demonstrate that the intention not to parent2 is related to sex role identity and a partic- ular constellation of family background variables. 2The author recognizes that "the intent to parent" is not necessarily synonymous with "the intent tn) have a. child.’I 'The terms are, however, used interchangeably in this document. REVIEW OF THE LITERATURE Ti'l' JEJI'T] . EIIII'IIE I Traditional psychoanalytic theories of the wish to parent essentially propose that this wish is rooted in early biologically based needs and fantasies. While the particu- lar needs that this wish symbolically represents varies from theorist to theorist, all adhere to a biophysiological ex- planation of reproduction. Freud's theory is perhaps the most widely known and criticized of all psychoanalytic theories. Freud believed that the womanus reproductive need emerges from the female castration complex. At about the age of three the little girl realizes that she does not have a penis as male chil- dren do. 'The little girl believes that she has been cas- trated, views this as a narcissistic wound and comes to see herself as inferior. Discovering that the mother is also deficient in this respect, the little girl turns away from her mother in anger for depriving her of the penis. The child turns to the father and hepes to receive a penis from him. She gradually comes to realize that she cannot have a penis but can have a child by the father. The wish for the penis is then replaced by a wish for a child by the father who then becomes the little girlfs new love object (Freud, 1925, 1931, 1933). 5 Other analytic theorists propose that the wish for a child predates the female castration complex. Deutsch (1924) and Kestenberg (1956) contend that this wish is related to the need to master vaginal sensations that occur in early childhood. Erikson (1968) alternatively proposes that the wish for a child is rooted in the little girl's knowledge of inner space, the knowledge that she is capable of carrying a deve10ping child within her. This everpresent awareness is not only a source of her wish to mother, but is a major determinant of her entire personality character. The intention to parent may include both a wish to give birth as well as a desire to nurture a child. To fully comprehend why women decide to have children, both factors must be considered. However considerable insight into the intention to have a child can be gained from the literature describing why women wish to mother. I E I J l' E E 1 l' Ellll . Feminists, disenchanted and enraged by biologically based psychoanalytic formulations regarding the psychology of women, have recently turned to sociocultural explanations to account for women's mothering. Theorists and feminists have proposed that womenfis continued sense of responsibility’ to parent is largely a cultural phenomenon promoted by the patriarchal society to maintain women in a repressed posi- tion inferior in power and status to men (Bernard, 1974; Friedan, 1965; Strouse, 1974). They see parenting as a confining trap which has kept women from achieving their 6 full potential as individuals and View voluntary childless- ness as the “Ultimate liberation” (Movius, 1980; Peck, 1971; Peck and Senderowitz, 1974). To embrace this explanation of mothering is to turn women into passive victims of social forces (Flax, 1978). It suggests that the responsibility for mothering is sup- ported and perpetuated primarily by men. It denies the influential role women have in reproducing mothering from generation to generation and ignores psychodynamic sources of motivations and behavior. Mothering not only fulfills a social responsibility, but meets a deeply rooted psychologi- cal need. To the extent that this need is met in our soci- ety through parenting, women will continue to mother and, in so doing will be instrumental in perpetuating the existing social structure. Thus there is a continual interplay be- tween cultural and intrapsychic forces. Both these aspects must be considered to fully understand the continued process of womens' mothering (Chodorow, 1978). In an attempt to integrate these two perspectives, Chodorow'(1978) posits that.a woman's attitude toward par- enting is rooted in her early object relations. She does not dismiss the impact of culture. Rather she suggests that through interaction with one's family, a microcosmic re- flection of our society's structure, a woman learns that women mother and men are engaged in extrafamilial pursuits. These expectations are internalized and integrated into her psychic structure, becoming a part of her concept of self, her definition of self in relation to others and her 7 expectations of others. She will, in turn, produce daughters who will need to mother thus reproducing mothering from generation to generation. The psychological process by which this occurs is delineated below. Personality theorists concur that girls remain depend- ent on their mothers longer than boys (Bardwick, 1971; Deutsch, 1944; Douvan and Adelson, 1966; Maccoby, 1966; Rossi, 1966). The male child being of a different gender from the mother, is treated as a psychological other from birth. This results in an early resolution of separation- individuation issues and rapid entry into the oedipal phase, where issues of competition, jealousy and castration anxiety predominate (Chodorow, 1978). Quite in contrast the rela- tionship between a mother and her infant daughter is likely to be characterized by overidentification. The mother thus tends to see her daughter as a narcissistic extension of herself (Chodorow, 1978). This is more likely to stimulate memories of her own early parenting and reawaken conflicts in relation to her own mother. To the extent that this occurs she may project her own needs onto the child and be unable to accurately respond to the needs of her daughter who is in reality a separate human being with motivations, intentions and feelings of her own (Benedek, 1970). It is during the rapprochement subphase of deve10pment that the child first becomes fully aware of her own sepa- rateness. The child's needs oscillate between the require- ment for separateness and developing sense of self, and the continued need for reliance on the mother (Mahler, Pine and 8 Bergman, 197SL. Fear of separation or losing the mother's love increases and the need to have the mother accessible and responsive to the child's needs is of optimal signifi- cance (Flax, 1978L. When.the mother has resolved her own dependency conflicts from childhood she will promote her daughter's independence. However, if the mother is ambiva- lent about giving up the symbiotic relationship or is reliv- ing her own infancy through her daughter, she will be less likely to provide her daughter with the needed encouragement to become more independent. Autonomy, rather than being experienced by the child as a way of pleasing the mother, is then experienced as a rejection for which the daughter is likely to be rejected. Thus the individuation process in girls is usually curtailed at this point (Flax, 1978). During the oedipal situation a new move toward individ- nation is made. Chodorow (1978) suggests that the turn to the father during the oedipal period is not for a penis or a baby as traditional Freudian theory suggests, but rather is a way for the little girl to establish her autonomy. Be— cause the father is relatively inaccessible and because his relationship with his daughter develops at a later age, the relationship never achieves the same intensity as the origi- nal attachment to the mother. Thus turn to the father is never complete, as traditional theory suggests. While the little girl may resolve the oedipal conflict to the extent that she establishes her father as her heterosexual object choice and identifies with the mother, the mother remains 9 internally and externally a significant love object (Chodorow, 1978). The major task of adolescence, termed by 8103 (1962) as the “second individuation process" is to relinguish internal love objects in order to free one up to establish signifi- cant extrafamilial attachments (8103, 1962). Because both mother and father have been retained as internal objects the process of relinquishment is particularly conflictual fOr the adolescent girl. The conflict is further complicated by the mother's own conflict over the daughter's separation. Because mother and daughter are ambivalent, both remain convinced that independence will bring about rejection (Chodorow, 1978). Chodorow further theorizes that because women's preoed— ipal attachments to mother are never fully relinquished, women tend to be more attuned to the needs and feelings of other people, define themselves in terms of their relation- ship with others, and retain the wish to reexperience pri- mary identification. In adulthood women seek satisfaction of their relational needs with men. However, because men in our society have repressed their relational needs in order to achieve a masculine identification, the satisfaction of a womanfs‘attachment needs can never be adequately met in a male-female relationship. It is therefore sought for in symbiotic union with a child. Thus the wish for a baby represents an attempt to reestablish a sense of merger with the mother. 10 II] I £1.13] . D . l' E II N Not all girls follow this normative path. Research consistently demonstrates that voluntarily childfree women are more autonomous than their parenting peers. Childless women tend to assume "personal rather than other directed guidelines“ (Veevers, 1974), are more able to formulate and carry out plans on their own (Mikus, 1980) and possess independent sources of self-esteem (Lewis, 1972; Rossi, 1965). Perhaps one of the most notable findings is that these women are able to maintain self-esteem in the face of significant criticism, sanctions and pressures exerted on them by family, friends and society because of their choice not to parent (Veevers, 1973); This pattern of self sufficiency appears to have been established early in development. In interviews these women report having been seen by their families as more rebelli- ous, with rebelliousness being manifested in verbal dis- agreements and value differences (Levine, 1978). Other findings have reported that intentionally childless women have achieved greater psychological distance from their families during adolescence than other women (Houseknecht, 1977, 1979; Lott, 1973), tend to challenge conventional values (Levine, 1978) and in adulthood are more likely to have changed religious affiliation or profess no religious affiliation (Gustavus and Henley, 1971; Levine, 1978; Veevers, 1973). The independence of childless women.has been'clearly established by the data. However, other empirical research ll raises questions about the nature of this autonomy. The existence of a firm sense of autonomy, developed out of a secure attachment to the mother facilitates and enhances attachment. Truly autonomous individuals are able to move freely between states of autonomy and relatedness. They are not only able to form mature love relationships but are able to depend on others and be depended upon without undue anxiety (Bowlby, 1973). Research on childless women suggest that their autonomy is a more fragile nature. A thread that consistently runs through the literature is the tenacity with which these women safeguard their autonomy. They tend to have fewer social supports (Houseknecht, 1977), show less interest in interacting with people, prefer to be alone, avoid group membership, tend to live further from their families of origin and choose professions which require them to work alone (Silka and Kiesler, 1977). In their adolescent years they tend to date little, are more likely to marry later than other women (Renne, 1976; Veevers, 1973) and identify their husbands and their "first love." Even in their mar- riages they are highly protective of their independence. For many, the decision not to have children is made because of a fear of disrupting the egalitarian nature of their relationships (Levine, 1978), leaving them dependent on their husbands and burdened by the dependency of their children (Veevers, 1975). Bowlby (1973) theorizes that for some girls early de- velopmental conflict with the mother results in a premature 12 or defensive autonomy. Repeated separations of sufficient duration may result in the child detaching herself from reliance on the mother and becoming narcissistically depend- ent on herself. This pattern results in a heightened drive towards individuation. However, because it involves a re- pression of attachment need, the development of relation- ships is restricted (Hansburg, 1972). It is the contention of this study that the above described object relational pattern underlies the intention not to parent in adolescent women. Pregnancy and mothering involves a psychological re- gression to the infantile stage of primary identification (Benedek, 1960). It is this regression that provides the mother with the basis for empathic understanding of her child's needs and is the source of her capacities for giving love and nurturance so essential for her childfls growth and I development (Balint, 1939; Benedek, 1970; Winnicott, 1965). Most.women, because they define themselves in relation to others and retain the need for merger are not excessively threatened by regression involved in the parenting process (Chodorow, 1978). However, for the excessively self suffi- cient woman, parenting and its accompanying regression not only evokes conflict, but threatens her fundamental self definition which is based on a sense of separateness, a capacity to function autonomously and a denial of relation and connection to others. The theory presented above to explain the intention not to parent suggests that these women haveea heightened need L; 13 for individuation and an attenuated attachment need. Hansburg (1972) proposes that the strength of onefls attach- ment and individuation needs will be reflected in how«one deals with separation. He describes a continuum along which the balance between attachment and individuation need var- ies. On one end are those individuals he labels "anxiously attached}. 'These peOple will meet both mild and strong separation experiences by seeking support from others. They are likely to avoid separation experiences by staying in close proximity'to their attachment figures. Chzthe other end of the continuum are "excessively self sufficient" peo- ple. These individuals find it difficult to utilize support from others even at times of permanent separation. They are likely to meet both temporary and permanent separations with individuation behavior. Hansburg (1972) contends that most individuals will fall between these two extremes. They will meet temporary separations with minimal anxiety and will respond with individuation behavior. However, when con- fronted by a permanent separation they will tend to seek support. The women who intend not to parent appear to fit in the group that Hansburg identifies as excessively self suffi- cient. It was therefore predicted that women who intend not to parent would show a higher individuation and lower at- tachment response following a separation than women who intend to mother. It was further predicted that they would react to strong separation situations with a greater indi- viduation than attachment response. 14 The theory proposed here to account for the intention not to parent is largely an extension of Chodorow's theory of mothering. However, Chodorow not only provides an object relational basis for understanding reproductive intentions, but also prOposes that the balance between attachment and individuation need is a critical factor in the deve10pment of sex role identity. fi£x_BQl£_Id£nL1L¥ fl '1' . 1.! E S E J Ii l'l Historically sex role identity was considered to be a unidimensional concept with masculinity and femininity rep- resenting its polar extremes. .According to this conceptual- ization, individuals were labeled either masculine or femi- nine, not both, and sex role was inextricably linked to biological gender (Monroe-Cook, 1979). The goal of sex role identity was to develop in chil- dren of each gender "sex-appropriate characteristics so that they may be capable of executing successfully the sex roles society has assigned to them" (Spence and Helmreich, 1978, p. 3). Boys were expected to develOp independence and other instrumental traits to prepare them for their extrafamilial occupational roles. Girls, on the other hand were expected to develop expressive characteristics which would ensure adequate fulfillment of their roles as wives and mothers. For both boys and girls deviation from this normative pat- tern, i.e., masculinity in girls or femininity in boys, was considered pathological (Spence and Helmreich, 1978). 15 Empirical instruments based on this theoretical model forced individuals into one of these categories by opera- tionally defining one in terms of the absence of the other (Constantinople, 1973). Constantinople posited that the use of these instruments hampered the deve10pment of a dualistic conceptualization of sex role identity. D 1.! E S E ] Ii l'l The dualistic model of sex role identity pr0poses that masculinity and femininity operate independently and there- fore may co-exist within the same individual. The theoreti- cal basis for a dualistic formulation can be found in the writings of Bakan (1966) and Jung (Campbell, 1971L. Bakan writes: For the male and in the female we have instances of differentiation of function, especially with respect to their roles in reproduction. If we think of agency and communion as two major func- tions associated with all. living substance, then although agency is greater in the male and commun- ion greater in the female, agency and communion nonetheless characterize both. (Bakan, 1966, p. 122). In a similar formulation, Jung introduces the concepts of anima (femininity) and animus (masculinity). He posits that in women the anima is conscious and therefore dominant while the animus is unconscious. For men the opposite is true; the animus dominates while the.anima is latent, Both.Jung and Bakan emphasize the importance of achieving a balance between these two dimensions. It was not until the early 1970's that the empirical use of unidimensional instruments was seriously questioned l6 (Spence and Helmreich, 1978). Following Constantinople's critique in 1973, a large body of research has developed which provides empirical support for the duality of sex role identity (Bem, 1974; Berzins, Welling and Wetter, 1978; Spence and Helmreich, 1978; Spence, Helmreich and Stapp, 1976; Heilbrun, 1976). Using instruments which enable indi- viduals to rate themselves separately on the domains of masculinity and femininity, subjects attribute to themselves varying degrees of socially valued feminine and masculine characteristics, that appear to operate independently of each other. This dualistic conception of masculinity and femininity has led to the broadening of our understanding of sex roles and to the identification of four distinct sex role cate- gories to replace the traditional two groups. These four categories have been labeled "masculine" to refer to those high in masculinity and low in femininity; "feminine" for those high in femininity and low in masculinity; "androgy- nous" indicating those high in both and "undifferentiated" to designate those low in both masculinity and femininity. It has been demonstrated that both males and females can be characterized as either masculine, feminine, androgy- nous or undifferentiated and that psychological adjustment is related more to category than to the establishment of 'gender-approPriate" characteristics. Research indicates that androgynous individuals show highest self-esteem (Bem, 1975; Spence et a1., 1975) and are better able to adapt their behavior to the specific requirements of a situation 17 than are others who tend to respond in sex stereotyped ways (Bem, 1974, 1975; Bem and Lenney, 1976). Data further show that masculine individuals have higher self-esteem than do feminine subjects, regardless of biological gender (Bem, 1975; Spence et al., 1978). This has been attributed to the fact that masculine characteristics are more valued in Amer- ican society than are feminine traits (Deaux, 1976; Rosen- krantz, Vogel, Bee, Broverman and Broverman, 1968; Spence and Helmreich, 1972) and that feminine individuals are psy- chologically less separate and therefore more dependent on others for maintenance of self esteem. Traditional psychoanalytic theory posits that the de- velopment of sex role identity arises from the resolution of the oedipal conflict resulting in identification with the same sex parent. While this may have been adequate to explain the deve10pment of unidimensional sex typed behav— ior, it fails to account for androgyny or cross-sex identity (Monroe-Cook, 1979). In an alternative formulation, Chodorow (1978) theo- rizes that sex role identity is primarily a manifestation of the attachment-individuation balance. She prOposes that femininity is associated with continued attachment need while masculinity reflects the extent of individuation. According to her formulation, traditional feminine identity indicates a high attachment and low individuation need. In contrast, masculine identity involves a repression of at- tachment need and a heightened drive towards individuation. Chodorow does not specifically address the androgynous or 18 undifferentiated categories. However, her theory can logi- cally be extended to include these groups. Androgynous women are those who are high in both attachment and individ- uation, while those with an undifferentiated sex role iden- tity are low in both these areas. II] ! £1.13] 3 S E J Ii ! d Only'one study to date has explored the relationship between childlessness and sex role identity. In a study comparing married childless women with mothers, Levine (1978) postulated that childless women probably define them- selves in less traditional terms than do other women and consequently hypothesized that they would be more androgy- nous than mothers. The prediction was not supported by the data. Levine found no differences between groups on sex role identity. However, she suggested that differences may have been attenuated by use of the long form of the Bem Sex Role Inventory, which was at that time in its developmental stages. The present study concurs with Levine that childless women.are likely'to demonstrate a nontraditional sex role identity. However, on the basis of the previously stated hypothesis that women who intend not to parent are high in individuation need and low in attachment need (a pattern associated with a masculine sex role identity) it was pre- dicted that women who intend not to parent would be more likely to identify themselves as having a masculine sex role identity than women who intend to mother. 19 W A relatively neglected are in the exploration of non- parenthood has been in the domain of family background variables. Existing studies can be divided into two groups: 1) demographic research and 2) womean perceptions of their early family life. Demographic investigations compose most of the early research on the intentionally childless woman. In one of the earliest studies Lewis (1972) found that childless women tended to be raised in nontraditional homes where 1) mother was dominant over father, 2) religion was unimportant, 3) mothers were likely to have some higher education and come from a higher socioeconomic level that their husbands and 4) parents were more likely to be previously divorced. Subsequent research has failed to corroborate these findings. Studies have repeatedly reported that women who remain childless by choice come from families which are quite conventional. Childfree women tend to be raised in intact families (Levine, 1978; Veevers, 1973) where there is no more prevalence of divorce or separation than in families of those who wish to, or have become mothers (Bram, 1974; VLott, 1973). Furthermore, mothers of the intentionally childless tended to be full time housewives during the subjects' childhoods (Levine, 1978; Veevers, 1973) with those who worked being forced to do so for financial reasons (Levine, 1978). This discrepancy may be due to differences in sampling. 20 In one respect mothers of childless women were even more conventional than mothers of women who mothered. Con- trary to expectations, Bram (1974) found that mothers of women who mothered were slightly more likely to work full time during the womanfislchildhood. Mothers of nonparents tended to work only when the children were older. This occurred even though childless women came from families of lower socioeconomic status (Lewis, 1972; Strong, 1967; Veevers, 1973). When employed, mothers of childless women were no more likely to be employed in male identified pro- fessions than mothers of women who mothered (Bram, 1974). Empirical data regarding family size have reported conflicting results. Some investigations have found that nonparents tend to come from small families (Lewis, 1972; Veevers, 1975) and are oldest (Veevers, 1973) or only chil- dren (Bram, 1974; Veevers, 1973L. Other researchers have found no differences in family size or ordinal position (Houseknecht, 1979; Levine, 1978; Silka et al., 1977). In a review of demographics, Bram (1974) concluded that there was a weak association between family history and fertility behavior. Consequently more recent research has focused on the childfree woman's retrospective perceptions of her family. Bram (1974) found no difference in perceived role con- flicts for mothers or fathers, nor was there a difference in the "value of children" attributed to parents of childless women and mothers. However, important differences have been found between childfree women and parents on the perceived 21 quality of early family relationships. Women who were in- tentionally childless were more likely to see parents as less nurturant (Lott, 1973), the emotional climate of their homes as lacking in warmth (Bram, 1974; Houseknecht, 1979; Levine, 1978) and to have seen children as a major source of friction between parents (Bram, 1974; Levine, 1978). In interviews, Levine (1978) found that both mothers and childless women experienced significant conflicts with families in the process of growing up. However, the inten- sity of these conflicts were stronger for the childfree group than for the mothers. Some childless women reported that their mothers had confided in them about their marital conflicts. This had left them feeling angry and resentful toward both parents. Other nonparents felt pressured to be successful where their parents had failed and experienced guilt for the sacrifices parents had made for them. Both.Lott (1973) and Levine (1978) report that child- less women were even more ambivalent toward their fathers than their mothers. However, neither elaborated on the nature of these conflicts. The preceding review represents the sum total of liter- ature available regarding the family background variables associated with a childfree life style. The recent focus on perceptions rather than factual data has provided promising results. However, the conclusions tend to be based on single questions about family closeness (Bram, 1974; Lott, 1973) or on global perceptions gleaned from interviews (Levine, 1978). Therefore, one of the goals of this study 22 will be to identify parental background variables which differentiate intended childless women from women who intend to parent using instruments with established reliability and validity. Ii l'E' !' W] III] The first variable that will be considered will be identification with mother with respect to sex role charac- teristics. There has been some controversy among theorists on the significance of identification in the development of sex role (Orlofsky, 1979). Research has failed to resolve the conflict. Some researchers have concluded that identi- fication plays a major role in the determination of sex role (Heilbrun, 1973, 1976), while others have relegated it to a secondary position (Lynn, 1976; Maccoby and Jacklin, 1974). In an investigation that provided partial support for both these viewpoints, Orlofsky (1979) found that the influence of identification differed according to sex role category (Androgynous, masculine feminine or undifferentiated). Thus, although there is disagreement about the importance of its contribution, most agree that to a greater or lesser extent, identification does play a role in the deve10pment of sex role identity. Benedek (1970) proposes that a woman's attitude toward parenting is rooted in the little girl's early identifica- tions with mother. Research has further demonstrated the importance of a woman's identification with her mother in achieving a satisfactory adjustment to pregnancy and 23 mothering (Ballou, 1978; Benedek, 1970; Colman and Colman, 1971). Consequently, it is surprising thatonly one study to date~has explored the relationshiptbetween identification with mother and intentional childlessness. Comparing child- free women and mothers Levine (1978) found that women who have chosen not to parent show a weaker identification with mother than women who are mothers. Levine, however, did not explore the similarity of these women to their mothers in regard to masculine and feminine characteristics. It is expected that similar results would be found in this area of sex role identity. Some support for this expectation is provided by previous demographic research which has reported that mothers of childless women lead highly conventional lives, while their daughters have chosen a life style that deviates from the traditional female role. It is furthermore consistent with Orlofsky“s (1979) results that masculine women are less similar to their mothers than androgynous, feminine and undifferentiated women. The pre— diction in this investigation was therefore made that women who intend not to parent would be less similar to their mothers in respect to sex role identity than women who intend to mother. ParenLBehaxior Three additional variables were explored: 1) Mother and Father warmth vs. rejection, 2) Mother and father con— trol and 3) mother and father cognitive involvement, which in 24 are the three orthogonal factors identified by Kelly and WOrrell (1976) to describe parent behaviors. As previously indicated, the findings of other investi- gations that childless women have parents who are less warm and nurturing was based on questionable methodology. Fur- l I l l l thermore there was no direct theoretical or empirical basis l on which to formulate hypotheses on the remaining two vari- ables. Consequently research in the area of sex role iden- r tity was utilized. Because it is being hypothesized that l women who intend not to parent are masculine in sex role identity, the family background of masculine women is par- 1 ticularly relevant to the present research. l Only two studies to date have explored the development- ; a1 background of women with different sex role identities. The first study done by Kelly and Worrell (1976) found that I masculine women described their parents in highly positive terms. Their descriptions were most similar to the androgy- nous women, with both groups seeing their parents as more encouraging of their cognitive pursuits and less controlling than parents of feminine and undifferentiated women. Com- l pared with the masculine group, androgynous women were slightly more positive in describing their mothers, charac- terizing them as more actively involved and encouraging of their academic curiosity. The results of the second investigation were strikingly different. Orlofsky (1979), also studying college students, reported that masculine women described both parents more negatively than any of the other three groups. They saw I ., i 25 their parents as less accepting, involved, egalitarian, more rejecting, and less encouraging of their cognitive develop- ment. Orlofsky cites the discrepancy between the two studies, I but makes no attempt to account for the differences. One might certainly speculate that the masculine sex role cate- l gory encompasses two distinct groups which have reached a I masculine identity through alternative developmental paths. l Accepting this premise, a comparison reveals that women who l intend not to parent appear more similar to Orlofsky's masculine women. In the first section of this document, it is pr0posed that a disruption in the early childhood rela- tionship between the future childless woman and her parents results in the development of defensive autonomy. In de— scribing his group of masculine women Orlofsky similarly concludes: . . . if a source of strength is to be found in masculine women from these data, it appears that their strength and independence may be self- protective, an autonomy and toughness reached by self-protective withdrawal from cold excessively rejecting parents (Orlofsky, 1979, p. 509). It was consequently assumed that women who intend not to parent would describe their own parents in much the same i way as the masculine women in Orlofsky's study. It was r therefore hypothesized that future childless women would describe both father and mother as more rejecting than future mothers, which was consistent with the results of previous investigations of childless women. In accordance with Orlofsky"s results it was further predicted that women L...—_—__ 26 who intend to parent would see their parents as less cogni- tively involved than women who intend to mother. Finally it was expected that there would be no difference between groups on perceived parental control. HYPOTHESES WW Hypothesis I: Hypothesis II: Women who intend to remain childless will be more self sufficient and will seek fewer attachments following a separation than women who intend to parent. Women who intend to remain childless will be more likely to remain self sufficient than to seek attachments in situations of strong separation than women who intend to parent. _$_e_x_Ro_1_e_Id_enLi_tx Hypothesis III: Women who intend to remain childless will be more likely to have a masculine sex role identity than will women who intend to parent. E .1 E I i M . I] Hypothesis IV: Women who intend to remain childless will show less similarity to their mothers with regard to sex role characteristics than will women who intend to parent. 27 Hypothesis V: Hypothesis VI: Hypothesis VII: 28 Women who intend to remain childless will see their parents as less warm and more rejecting than will women who intend to parent. Women who intend to remain childless will see their parents as no different in exert- ing control than will women who intend to parent. Women who intend to remain childless will see their parents as less involved in their cognitive development than will women who intend to parent. METHOD W The subjects for this study were selected from a pool of women enrolled in Introductory Psychology Courses at Michigan State University during Winter and Spring Terms, 1983. In both courses, subjects are given extra credit for participating in ongoing research. Subjects were obtained through two alternative proce- dures. With the professor's approval the Biographical In- formation Questionnaire was administered to entire sections at the beginning or end of classes. The subjects were told that there were two parts to the study and that only a small number of them would be selected to participate in part II. They were given no further information on what basis the discrimination would be made. In those classes where the professor would not allow the questionnaires to be administered during class time, sign up sheets were placed in the classrooms. On these sheets, subjects were informed of the name of the experiment ”Future Role Expectations,” the number of credits to be awarded for participation and the place and time to meet with the experimenter if interested. At the designated time subjects were administered the Biographical Information 29 30 Questionnaire and were given the same information provided to students tested in the classroom. In all, 762 women answered and returned the screening questionnaire. Table 1 gives a summary of the demographic characteristics of the college pOpulation of women from which the study sample was subsequently drawn. The study sample was composed of two groups of women. The first group included 34 women who were very certain they intend to parent. The second group was composed of 26 women who were moderately'or very certain that they intend not to parent. This determination was made on the basis of answers to the following questions included on the Background Information Questionnaire: Do you intend to parent: Yes No Undecided How certain are you about this intention? Very certain Moderately Certain Moderately Uncertain Very Uncertain All subjects also met the following criteria: 1. Never married 2. Never having given birth to a child 3. Not currently pregnant 4. Physically able to give birth to a child Three hundred thirty-eight women met the criteria for inclusion in the parenting group, while 27 women met the inclusion criteria for the childfree group. All 27 women 31 Table 1. Comparison of Demographic variables for the Stu- dent Population, the Intent to Parent Group and the Intent not to Parent Group. GBQQB Intend Intend Not VARIABLE POpulation to Parent to Parent N 702 34 26 Age Mean 19.18 18.96 19.16 S.D. 1.59 1.31 2.49 Marital Status Single 95.20% 100.00% 100.00% Married 1.70% 0 0 Separated .30% 0 0 Divorced .90% 0 0 Student Status Freshman 63.20% 58.80% 57.70% Sophomore 20.70% 20.60% 19.20% Junior 10.00% 17.60% 7.70% Senior 5.80% 2.90% 11.50% GPA Mean 3.17 2.65 3.26 S.D. .62 .41 .39 Major No Preference 20.80% 11.80% 7.70% Agriculture 2.60% 2.90% 3.80% Humanities 2.40% 0 3.80% Business 23.90% 38.20% 19.20% Communications 10.30% 17.60% 7.70% Education 1.30% 2.90% 0 English 5.10% 0 3.80% Human Ecology 4.80% 8.80% 0 Natural Science 9.80% 2.90% 19.20% Nursing 2.80% 2.90% 0 Social Science 13.10% 11.80% 23.10% Veterinary Med. 1.60% 0 11.50% .60% 0 0 Race Caucasian 90.60% 94.10% 88.50% Black 6.30% 2.90% 0 Other 3.00% 0 11.50% 32 Table 1. (Cont.) GBQUE Intend Intend Not VARIABLE P0pulation to Parent to Parent Religion None 5.60% 0 33.60% Protestant 31.60% 44.10% 15.40% Catholic 40.50% 38.20% 42.30% Jewish 4.80% 8.80% 0 Lutheran 3.00% 2.90% 0 Christian 1.40% 0 3.80% Other 9.80% 2.90% 0 Parents' Marital Status Married 76.40% 85.30% 65.40% Separated 3.00% 2.90% 3.80% Divorced 15.80% 5.90% 26.90% Widowed 4.10% 5.90% 3.80% Age sow1 0 - 5 2.90% 0 6 - 10 2.90% 15.20% 11 - 18 8.70% 15.20% Father's Education Post Graduate 24.60% 23.50% 38.50% College Graduate 33.80% 35.30% 11.50% High School Graduate 34.30% 32.40% 34.60% Not High School Grad 6.70% 5.90% 15.40% Mother's Education Post Graduate 11.00% 11.80% 11.50% College Graduate 34.20% 41.20% 15.40% High School Graduate 49.70% 38.20% 65.40% Not H.S. Graduate 4.40% 5.90% 7.70% Father's Occupation2 1 26.80% 20.60% 30.80% 2 20.50% 32.40% 15.40% 3 17.40% 8.80% 7.70% 4 7.80% 14.70% 7.70% 5 10.00% 5.90% 3.80% 6 3.70% 0 19.20% 7 2.10% 8.80% 3.80% Unemployed 4.70% 0 0 33 Table 1. (Cont.) QEQHE Intend Intend Not VARIABLE Population to Parent to Parent Mother's Occupation2 1 1.90% 0 0 2 21.40% 26.50% 23.10% 3 10.30% 8.80% 3.80% 4 22.80% 11.80% 19.20% 5 3.40% 0 0 6 5.60% 0 15.40% 7 1.40% 2.90% 11.50% Housewife 22.40% 26.50% 15.40% Unemployed 4.30% 14.70% 7.70% 5353 Class I 25.20% 17.60% 30.80% II 18.90% 35.30% 11.50% III 23.60% 23.50% 11.50% IV 17.70% 8.80% 19.20% V 4.10% 8.80% 15.40% Children in Family Mean 3.50 3.29 3.65 S.D. 1.62 1.64 1.38 Sibling Position Only 5.10% 2.90% 7.70% Oldest 26.90% 38.20% 38.50% Middle 32.60% 23.50% 30.80% Youngest 35.20% 35.30% 23.10% Marital Intentions Yes 87.01% 100.00% 34.60% No 1.00% 0 19.20% Undecided 11.30% 0 46.20% Childbearing Intentions Yes 78.50% 100.00% 0 No 1.70% 0 100.00% Undecided 19.50% 0 0 Births No 97.70% 100.00% 100.00% Yes 1.40% 0 0 Previous Pregnancies No 98.70% 100.00% 100.00% Yes 0 0 0 seq 34 Table 1. (Cont.) GBQUB Intend Intend Not VARIABLE POpulation to Parent to Parent Physical Problems4 No 95.40% 97.10% 100.00% Yes 4.00% 0 0 Work Intentions Yes 95.70% 97.10% 100.00% No .40% 0 0 Undecided 3.60% 2.90% 0 lAge SDW = Subject's age at the time of separation, divorce or widowhood of parents. 2Occupation 1 = Higher executives, prOprietors of large concerns and major professionals. = Business managers, proprietors of medium sized businesses, lesser professionals. Administrative personnel, small independent busi- nesses and minor professionals. Clerical and sales workers, technicians and owner of little businesses. Skilled manual employees. Machine Operators and semi-skilled workers. Unskilled employees. \IO‘U'I ah I» M II II 3SES determined by Hollingshead Two Factor Index (1957). 4 = Physical problems that would prevent pregnancy. who stated an intention not to parent agreed to participate. One subject left school before she could be tested. For each intentionally childless woman identified, a woman in- tending to parent was randomly selected from the same class or group. This was done to minimize sampling bias. Eight additional women who stated an intention to parent were randomly selected to participate in the study to achieve the previously agreed upon 60 subjects. This was done to reduce 35 statistical error. Four of the women originally selected for inclusion in the intend to parent group, chose not to participate. Four other women were contacted and all agreed to be tested. Table 1 shows a comparison of the demographic characteristics between the two research groups and the population from which they were drawn. Infiilnmfinlfi Background Information Questionnaire The Background Information Questionnaire (see Appendix A) was developed specifically for this study. It consists of 19 questions designed to gather demographic data about the subjects and their families. Four additional questions regarding subjects' marital, parenting and work intentions, and pregnancy history were also included. The questionnaire was administered to all potential subjects and was used to identify those women who met inclusion criteria for the study. Separation Anxiety Test (SAT) 5 ._ . !' E3 . . . The Separation Anxiety Test (see Appendix B) is a semi- projective instrument which is designed to assess reaction patterns to separation in adolescents (Hansburg, 1972; 1980). There are two forms, one for males and one for females. The test consists of 12 pictures, each of which depicts a different situation in which a child, either a boy or a girl, is separating from a significant other. Six of 36 the pictures represent mild or temporary separations and six depict strong or permanent separations. Each of the pictures is accompanied by a title describ- ing the type of separation situations and 17 statements describing how the child in the picture might react or feel. Each of the 17 items were judged by four highly experienced clinical psychologists to represent a different psychologi- cal mechanism one might use to deal with separation. These mechanisms can then be grouped according to eight response patterns representing different ego and superego functions. These include: 1) attachment need; 2) individuation need; 3) hostility; 4) painful tension; 5) reality avoidance; 6) self-esteem preoccupation; 7) self-love loss; and 8) identi- ty stress. Only the first two patterns, attachment and individua- tion need, were used for this study. Each of these reSponse patterns consists of three components. Attachment includes feelings of rejection, loneliness and empathy. Individua- tion consists of a feeling of well being, adaptation and sublimation. .All subjects were administered the female form of the SAT. Subjects were instructed to look at each picture, read the title underneath, and then indicate those statements which represent how the girl in the picture feels. They were then instructed to identify as many statements as seemed appropriate. / Each subject's protocol was then scored individually and recorded on the Chart for Controlled Associations (see 37 Appendix C). Next, the total number of attachment responses were calculated by adding the number of rejection, loneli- ness and empathy responses. The individuation score was similarly calculated by adding the adaptation, well-being and sublimation scores. These were then placed in the Pattern Summary Chart (see Appendix D). ‘The overall attach- ment and individuation percentages for mild and strong pic- tures were calculated. K 1.1.! Validity studies were done on 250 children between the ages of 11 and 15 between 1967 and 1970 (Hansburg, 1972; 1980). The subjects were drawn from a number of different settings in the New York City Long Island area including the Pleasantville Cottage School, two group residences for the Jewish Child Care Association (JCCA), two Catholic Charities care facilities, the psychiatric clinic of the JCCA, several public schools and a private Jewish day school. The results indicated that girls showed a slight but nonsignificantly higher attachment need than did boys. In- stitutionalized children showed a lower attachment need than other children. Jewish children were higher in attachment need than Catholics. There were no differences across age groups in attachment need. In regard to individuation, the highest individuation responses were found in children living in nuclear family settings, while the lowest scores were obtained by children 38 with weak attachments to their families and those living in institutional settings. The children who were best adjusted showed a slightly higher overall attachment than individuation score. On pictures representing strong separations attachment exceeded individuation.while the reverse was true for mild separa- tions. A positive correlation was also found between the extent to which individuation exceeded attachment and diffi- culties in object relations. Hansburg (1972) discovered that an adequate balance between attachment and individuation is represented by an attachment need of 20-25% and an individuation need of 16- 28%. Those individuals independently judged to be symbiotic had attachment responses exceeding 25% and individuation responses lower than 16%. Self—sufficient subjects had attachment responses less than 20% and individuation re- sponses greater than 28% Sherry (1981) validated the SAT with a graduate and undergraduate college population. Based on pretest and interview data be judged the test to be applicable to these groups. B 1‘ 1.1.! Internal consistency, computed by the split-half method, revealed an overall reliability of .885. No indi- vidual scale reliabilities were reported (Hansburg, 1972). Coefficient alpha calculated for this research sample was .747 for attachment and .732 for individuation. 39 E . l‘ £3 . . I' The Separation Anxiety Test was also administered pro- jectively. For this administration only the pictures with descriptive titles were shown to the subject. They were given the following directions which are virtually identical to those given when administering the TAT (Murray, 1938). I am going to show you a series of twelve pictures and ask you to make up a story about each one. When I hand you the picture, first read the title underneath and then make up the story. Please include in your story the following four things: 1) what is happening in the picture; 2) what led up to the situation; 3) what are the people think- ing and feeling; and 4) what is going to happen. When you are finished with your story, place the picture face down on the table in front of you. All stories were tape recorded and later transcribed. The protocols were then rated for attachment and individua- tion needs according to the scoring system developed spe- cifically for this study. The attachment score was composed of four components: loneliness, rejection, relatedness, and affiliation. The individuation score included three compo— nents: object constancy, adaptation and exploration/ initiative. Each of the twelve stories per protocol was rated on these seven components by assigning a score of one if pres- ent or zero if absent. The attachment score was then calcu- lated by adding the loneliness, rejection relatedness and affiliation scores across the twelve cards. The individua- tion score was similarly calculated by adding the object constancy, adaptation and exploration/initiative scores. Comparisons were made and hypotheses tested. 40 Persianmsnt_of_§coring_§riteria The scoring criteria for attachment were developed from the theoretical and empirical literature of separation anx- iety and attachment. There is some disagreement in the literature as to whether separation anxiety is a primary anxiety associated with object loss or a derivative of the more basic anxiety associated with unsatisfied bodily needs. There is, however, a growing consensus that separation from an attachment figure is accompanied by anxiety and some predictable patterns of response throughout the life cycle (Antonucci, 1976; Bowlby, 1973). Loneliness is a common response to the disruption of the attachment bond at all ages, though the character of attachment may differ depending on the developmental stage at which the .... .... .2 .8555 ...-8.. 2:2 .23 2.2.... ...... 25.2.5... 329.3: 3.82.3335: P¢<=U >I<223m ZIP—hi.- APPENDIX E SCORING CRITERIA FOR PROJECTIVE STORIES Attachment Rejection l) A person with whom the child has had an ongoing 2) relationship does not love, like or care about the child or does not love/like/care about the child to the extent they previously did. The child feels rejected, abandoned or rebuffed by another person. Loneliness The child feels sad, lonely, miserable, unhappy, empty, a sense of loss, or grief because of being separated from another person. Relatedness 1) 2) 3) 4) The child has feelings of security, content- ment, confidence which is associated with the current, remembered or expected presence of another person. The child feels responsible for or obligated to another person. The child loves, likes, cares about, under- stands, is interested in or is concerned about another person. The child has influence over, is influenced by, wants to be like or admires another person. 141 142 Affiliation The child seeks out another for the purpose of giving or receiving contact, attention, reassur- ance, admiration, approval, acceptance, emotional support, affection, understanding, comfort, advice or to confide in. lndiyiduation Object Constancy/Sense of Self l) A child has a feeling of security, contentment or confidence in his/her capacity to function in the absence of the person from whom the child is being separated. 2) The child exhibits the ability to maintain a feeling of relatedness to the person from whom s/he is being separated. 3) The child expresses confidence that the rela- tionship will continue and survive periods of separation. Adaptation (implies separation has been disruptive)/ Self Maintenance 1) The child accepts the separation and attempts to change in order to overcome obstacles/prob- lems created by the separation or to reestab- lish some previous level of functioning. 2) The child adjusts to the situation, adapts or does the best s/he can do. 3) 143 The child demonstrates the ability to carry on routine tasks (school, play, social relation- ships) in the absence of the person from.whom s/he is separating. Exploration/Initiative l) The child seeks out new experiences or opportu- 2) 3) nities. The child feels excited about the prospect of having new experiences. The child desires independence or freedom for the purpose of his/her growth and development. An act simply for the purpose of defying or getting back at someone is not a criterion for scoring this category. APPENDIX F 144 BEM SEX ROLE INVENTORY INSTRUCTIONS All your responses should be made on the printed (computer) sheets with a #2 pencil. Before you begin, full in your student number and sex and FILL IN THE CORRESPONDING CIRCLES on each of the computer sheets attached to this form. On the following pages is a list of 30 items. Each consists of a pair of characteristics, with the letters A-E in between. For example: Not at all A....B....C....D....E Very artistic artistic Each pair describes contradictory characteristics, that is, you cannot be both at the same time, such as very artistic and not at all artistic. The letters form a scale between the two extremes. You are to choose a letter which describes where YOU fall on the scale. For example, if you think you have no artistic ability, you would choose A. If you think you are pretty good, you might choose D. If you are only medium, you might choose C, and so forth. Now begin answering the questions on the answer sheet. Be sure to answer elm question, use a #2 pencil, and answer quickly because YOUR FIRST IMPRESSION IS BEST. After you have finished the questionnaire describing yourself, using the segond.printed computer answer sheet, choose the letter which best describes YOUR MOTHER on each of the 30 items. ll. 12. Almost never defend my own beliefs Not very affectionate Not very conscientious Not very independent Not very sympathetic Not very moody Not very assertive Not very sensitive to the needs of others lot very reliable. Not a very strong personality Not very understanding Not very jealous Not very forceful Not very compassionate Not very truthful Not much leadership ability Not too eager to soothe hurt feelings 145 AOOOOBOOOOCOOOODOOOOE A....B....C....D....E A....B....C....D....E A....B....C....D....E A....B....C....D....R A....B....C....D....E A....B....C....D....E AOOOOBOOOOCOOOODOOOOE AOOOOBOOOOCOOIODOOOOE ADOOOBOOOOCOOOODOOOOE A....3....c:...n....z A....B....C....D....E A....B....C....D....E A....B....C....D....E AOOCOBOOOOCOOOODOOOOE AOOOOBOOOOCOCOODOOIOB ACOOOBOOOOCOOOODOOOOE Code No. Almost always defend my own beliefs . Very affectionate Very conscientious Very independent Very sympathetic Very moody Very assertive Very sensitive to the the needs of others Very reliable Very strong personality Very understanding Vefiy jealous Very forceful Very compassionate Very truthful Much leadership ability Very eager to soothe hurt feelings 18. 19. 20. 2'2. 23. 24. 25. 26. 27. 28. 29. 30. REMINDER -9 FILL IN THE CIRCLES FOR Vhry secretive Not willing to take risks Not very warm Not very adaptable Submissive Not very tender Conceited Not willing to take a stand Not very fond of children Not very tactful Not very aggressive Not very gentle very conventional SHEET. 146 AOOOOBOOOOCOOIODOOOOB AOOOIBOOOOCOOOODOOOOB A....B....C....D....E A....n....c....n....t A....s..,.c....n....e A....B....C....D....E A....B....C....D....E A....B....C....D....E AOOOOBOOOOCOOOODOOOOB AOOOOBOOOOCOOOODOOOOE ACCOOBOOOOCOOOODOOOOE aOOOOBOOOCCOOOODOOOOB ACCCOB.OOOCOCCOD.CIIE Not very secretive Willing to take risks Very warn Very adaptable Dominant Very tender Not conceited Very willing to take a stand Very fond of children Very tactful Very aggressive Very gentle Not very conventional SEX, 8 STUDENT NUMBER ON YOUR ANSWER APPENDIX G IT 4‘ 147 PARENT BEHAVIOR FORM Directions: On the following pages you will find a series of statements which a person might use to describe his parents. Read each statement and decide which answer most closely describes the way each of your parents has acted toward you. Indicate your answer on the separate answer sheet. If you are under 16, answer the questions as they describe what is happening now. If you are over 16, answer as you would have around the age of 16. If you have been living with someone other than your real mother or father, answer the questions in terms of those individuals. 148 ‘ MOTHER Makesmefeelbettcaftctalkingovcmy worriuwitth. 2. Oftanpreisesme. 3. 10. 11. Letsmehelptodecidehowtodothingswe're worln'ngon. . .Reallywaflenntoulljuathowlfeelabout thins!- . Wantametoknowhewandwhynaturalthinp happeninthewaytheydo. Emegesmetodevebpafterschoolsldlla andhobbiee. . Letamedressinanywaylpleaae. . Tellametothinkandplanbefonlact. . Isunhappythatl'mnotbetterinschoolthanl am. Ssestoitthatlknowexactlywhatlmayor maynotdo. Inaiatathatlmuatdoeractlyael'm told. 12. IfItakesomeoneelse'ssideinanal-gument. 13. 14. 15. 16. 17. 18. 19. 20. 21. sheisooldanddiatanttome. Thinhlemjuatsomeoneto“putupwith." Tellsmeneitherofushaea brain. Isconsiderateofothers. Isabletomakemefeelbettcwhenl amupset. Bellevesinshowinghclovefame. Dosen’tgetangryifldiaecnewithhcidea'a. Lilaemetoasectmyownideeewithher. Lilreatodiscuaecnrrentevutawithme. Providedmewithpunleewhenlwaayoung. nDom'ttellmewhattimetobehomewhenl goout. 23.?ellamethatgoodhardworkwillmakeliie worthwhile 24. Says that myteechersoftenexpecttoollttleof me. 25. Wantstolmowexactlywherelamandwhatl amdm'ng. Believeeinhavingalotofrulesandstickingto them. . Says I'ma big problem. Makes mefeel I'm not loved. . Seyathatthingawithsugartaetssmr. 853,838 . Makes good decisions about family problems. 31. Makes me feel free when I'm with her. 32. Tells me how much she loves me. 33. Allows me to be myself. 34. Likes when I'm able to criticize my own or others’ ideas effectively. ‘ 35. Talks with me about philosophical ideas. 36. Hastakenmetolookatpaintings. sculpture andarchitecture. 37. Lets medoenything I like todo. 38. Sea to it that I keep my clothes neat. clean. andinorder. 39. Wantsmetoknowalotoffactsregardlessof whethrornottheyhavemeeningforme. 40.Dosan'tletmegoplaoesbecausesomething mighthappentome. 41. Believes that all my bad behavior should be punishedinsomeway. 42. Almost always complains about what I do. 43. Ismintuestsdinmeetingortalkingwith myfriende. a 47. 48. 49. 50. 51. . Expectsmetostareatthesunf‘ahonrs. . lshardwakingandeflcint. . Comfutsmewhnl'mafraid. Tellsmel'mgoodlooking. Dosan'tmindifllddhcabmtthinga. Wantametokeepanopumindabontmyown oroth-e'belieia. Pointsonttbebeeutieso‘nanne. Hastakmmetoseeapdamanoeinaplayor correct. szDomn'tpeymnchattentimtomymisbo 53. 54. 55. 56. 57. 58. 59. 60. 61. havitr. Wantsmetohavethesemerelip‘ousbeliefsas shedoa. Seysshewonldliketoseemeentca professionwhichrequireea'iginalthinking. Isalwaystellingmehowlshouldbehave. Haema'emleathatlunremembc.sois‘ oftenpnnishingme. Tellsmelamimnnmre. Doesn'tshowthtsheloveeme. Tellsmetheeerthiasquare. lsarssponsibleperson. Chetamenpwhenlamsad. 62. Sayslmalrehchappy. 63. 64. 65. 66. 67. 68. Enjoysitwhenlbringfriendatomyhome. Ispleeeedwhenlbringupa'lginalideaa. 'l‘alhwithmeabonthowthingsaremade. Playsclaaeicalmueicwhenlamhome. Doeanotinsistlobeyiflcomplainorprotat. TanghtmetobelieveinGod. 149 69. ‘ 7o. 71. 72. 73. 74. 75. 76. 77. 78. 79. 81. Wants metopursueaoaminascientifioally relatadfield. Wantstooontrolwhatevcldo. Seeetoitthatlobeywhenshetellamssome- thing.‘ Oftenblowshertopwhenlbotherher. Doesn’tseemtothinkofmevcyoften. ReedstomeinGnefiandLatin. Istruthfnl. Hasagoodtimeathomewithme. Givesmealotofcereandattention. Allowsdiscnssionofrightandwmng. Likeswhsnlaakquesfionsaboutallkindsof thine: . Encouragesmetodiamssthecausesandpos- sible solutions of social. political. economic or international problems. Buysbooksformetoreed. 82. Excaseemybedcondnct. 83. 84. 87. 39. Encourages me to pray. Seysshewaildlikemetobeanimportantor famoupcsonsomeday. .Keeperemindingmeaboutthingslamnot allowedtodo. Punishes mewhenl don'tobey. Wheneverwe get into adiscuesion. she treats memorelikeachildthananamilt. .Changeshermindtonnhthingseesiafor herself. Gives me green lollipope evuyday. . Uses goodjudgement. 91. lseasytotalkto. 92. Beoomeeveryinvolvedinmylife. 93. Iseesywithme. 94. Tellsmetostandupforwhatlbelieve. 96. Feelelshouldreedasmuchasposeibleaimy own. 96.3mmtobediflcentfromothc punk. 97. Canbetalkedintothingseesily. 98. Feelshnrtwhnldon'tfollowhisadvioe. 99.Expectsmetobesuocseainlineverything1 try. 100. Is always gettingafterme. 101.Believesinpunishingmetocorrectandim- provemymanners. 102.Whenldon'tdoashewante.says I'm not gratefulforallhehasdonefa'me. 103. Doesn'tgetmethingsunlesslaskoverand overagain. 104. Buysmethousanddollarsuitsordressea. 105. Is honest in dealing with othts. 106.8eemstoseemygoodpnintsma'ethanmy faults. 107. Says l'mverygoodnatm'ed. 108. Triestobeafriendrathcthanaboee. 109. Givesmereesonsforrulesthathemakee. 110. Encairagesmetoreednewsperiodicalsand watchnewshreedoestsonTV. 111. fisquiresmetoarriveatmyownconchnions whenIhaveaproblemtosolvi. 112. Seldominsiststhatldoanything. 113. Feelshurtbythethingsldo. 114. Is more concerned with my being bright rather than steady and dependable. [.1 01 O 115. Decideswhatfriendslcangoaroundwith. 116. Loses his tamper with mewhen I don't help aroundthehouse. 117. Tellsmeofallthethingshehasdoneforme. 118. Asksotherpeoplewhatldoawayfromhome. 119. Expectemetomakeallofmyownclothee. 120. Obeysthelaw. 121. Smileeatmevcyoftsn. 122.1salwaysthinkingofthingsthatwillplease me. 123. 'l‘riestotreetmsasanequal. 124.1‘rainsmetobsrationalandobjectiveinmy ll'l' 125. Encouragesmetofoolaround withnewidees eveniftheyturnouttohavebeenawastsof time. 126. Wants me to find out answers for myself. 127. Doesnotbothertoenfm'cerules. 128. Seemstoregetthatlamfiowingupandam spendingmoretimeawayfromhome. 129. PrefI-s metobegoodinacademicworkrather thaninsports. 130. Tellsmehowtospendmyfreetime. 131. Doesn’t give me any peace until 1' do what he 9‘”- 132.1slesefriendlywithmeif1don'tsesthings his way. 133. Almostalwayswantstoknowwhophonedme orwrotetomeandwhattheysaid. 134. Says I should never ride in an automobile. 135. Makes guests feel at home. 1. 151 FATHER Makaamefealbettarafta'talkingovrmy 2. Oftenm-eieesme. 3. ‘0 10. 11. 12. 13. 14. 15. 16. 17. 18. 19. 20. 21. Letamabalptodecidehowtodothlngswe’re workingon. Reallywantametotallhimjuethowlfeal aboutthinga. . . Wantsmetoknowhowandwhynaturelthings happenintheweythsydo. .Encmragaametodevelopeftrschrnlskills andhobbiea. . Letsmedressinanywaylpleasa. . Tellsmetothinkamiplenbeforelact. .lsunhappythatl’mnotbsttuinschoolthanl Seestoitthatlknowexactlywhatlmeyor maynotdo. Inaiststhatlmustdoerectlyesl'mtold. IfI takesomeoneelsa’ssideinanargumentha isébldanddistanttoma. Thinkslamjustsomeonato“putupwith." Tellsmaneitheroiushesabrein. Isconsiderateofotha's. Isabletomekamafealbettxwhanlamupsat. Believesinshowinghialovefa'me. Doesn’tgetangryifldisegreawithhisidaas. Likeametoessartmyownideaewithhim. Likestodiscusscun'enteventswithme. Providedmewithpunleswhenlwesyoung. 22.Doesn'ttsllmewhettimetobehomewhenl go out. 23.Tellsmethatgoodherdworkwillmakelife worthwhile. 24. Saysthatmytsachersoftsnexpacttoolittleof me. 25. Wentstoknoweaactlywherelamandwhatl amdoing. 26. Believesinhavingalotofrnlesandstickingto them. 27. Says I’mabigmblem. 28. Makeamefeall'mnotloved. 29. Saysthatthingswithsugertastesour. 30. Mekeagooddecisionsabout family problems. 31. Makeamefeelfreawhenl'mwithhim. 32. Tellsmehowmuchhalovesme. 33. Allowsmetobemyself. 34. Likeswhenlamablatocn’ticizemyownor othce'idaaseffectively. ’ 35. Talks with me about philosophical ideas. 36.Hastakenmatolookatpaintings.sculpture andarchitecture. 37. Letsmedoanythingllikatodo. :flfiklmnmulhmmmeMMMmMm and in order. 39. Wentsmetoknowalotoffactsreprdleseof whetherornottheyhavemeaningforme. 40.D9aen’tlatmagoplecesbeoeusesomething mightheppsntome. 41.3elievesthatallmybadbehavia'shouldbe punishedinsomeway. 42. Almost always complains about what I do. 43.1sneverintarestsdinmeetingortalkingwith myfriefnds. 44. 45. 46. 47. 48. . 49. 50. 51. Expectametostareatthasunforhours. lshardwa-kingandeificient. Comfatamewhen I'mairaid. TellsmeI’mgoodlooking. Doesn'tmindiflkidhimabontthings. Wantametokeepenopanmindabontmyown oroth-e'beliefe. Pdntsoutthebaentieaofnemra. Hestakmmetoseaapcfmnancainaplayor cones-t. 52Doasn't peynmchatuntimtomymisbe- 53. 54. 55. 56. 57. 58. 59. 60. 61. hevia. Wantsmetohavethesannreligiousbeliefsas hedoaa. Sayshewouldliketossamsentcaprofession which . . . III‘ I' Isalwaystellingmahowlshouldbehave. Hesmoreruleathenlcanramembc.sois oftmpuniahingme. Tellemelamimmature. Doesn'tshowthathelovesme. Tellemetheearthissquare. Iseresponsibleparson. Che-'smeupwhenlamsad. 62. SaysInnkehimhappy. 63. 64. 65. 66. 67. 68. Enjoys it when I bring friends to my home. lspleasadwben 1 bn'ngupmideas. Talks with me about how things are made. Plays classical music when 1 am home. Does not insist 1 obey if I complain or protest. Taught me to believe in God. 69. 70. 71. 152 Wantsmatopursueacereerinascientiflcally relatedfield. - ‘ Wants to control whatever I do. Seastoitthatlobaywhenhatellsmesome- thing. 72. Oftenblowshistopwhenlbothm'him. 73. Doesn'tseamtothinkoimeveryoften. 764. RaedstomeinGreakandLatin. 75. 76. 77. 78. 79. 81. Istrnthful. Hesagoodtimeethomewithme. Givasmealotofcereendattention. Allowsdiscussionofrightendwrong. Likaswlnnlaskquestionsaboutallkindsof l' . .Encourageametodiscussthecausesand possible solutions of social. political. economic or intentional problems. Buysbooksim'matoread. 82. Excnsaamybadconcluct. 83. 84. 85. 87. 89. Encourages me to prey. Sayshewouldlikemetobeanimportantor femouspersonsomeday. Keepsremindingmeaboutthingslamnot allowedtodo. Punishes me when I don’t obey. Whenevu'wegetintoadiscussion. hetreats memorelikaachildthananadult. .Chengeshismindtomakethingseasierfor himself. Gives me green lollipops everyday. . Usesgoodjudgement. 91. lseasytotalkto. r—x 92. Becomeavcyinvolvedinmylife. 93. Iseesywithma. 94. Tellsmetostandupfa'wbatlbelieve. 95. Feelslahouldreadaenmchaspoaaibleonmy own. 96.Encouregeamatobedlffuentfromother people 97. Canbetalkedintothingeeasily. 96. Feelshurtwhmldon'tfollowhcadvioa. 99.8mmtobasuccassfn'lineva-ythingl try. 100. Isalwaysgettingaftmme 101.8eliavesinpnniahingmatoem-rectandim- provemymennme. 102When1don'tdoushewantasaysl'mnot gratefulfcallshehesdomfam. - 103.Doasn'tgetmethingsunlaealaskoverand ovcagain. 104. Buysmethousenddollerssuitserdreases. 105. Iehonaatindaalingwithoth-e. 106.8aamstosaamygoodpointsmorethanmy faults. 107. SaysI’mvrygeodnstured. 108. Trieatobeafriemiratherthanabosa. 109. Gives mereasonsfm'rulaathetshemekes. 110. Encourages me to read news periodicals and watchnewsbieedcastsonTV. 111. Requireamatoerriveetmyownconclusions whenlhevaepoblamtosolve. 112. Seldomineistathatldoenything. 113. Feelshurtbythethingaldo. 153 114.1smoreconctnedwithmybsingbrightrather thanstesdyanddepsndable. 115. Decidaswhatfriendslcengoaroundwith. 116. Loeaahttenmcwithmewhmldon’thelp aroundthehouea. 117. Tellsmaofellthethingsshehesdonefcrme. 116. Asksothmpsqlswhatldoawayfromhome. 119. Expectematomekeellofmyownclothes. 120. Obsysthelaw. ‘ 121. Smilaaatmevaryoftan. 1221salwaysthinkingofthingsthatwillpleese me. 123. Triestotreatmeasanequel. 124.Trainsmetoberationalandobjectiveinmy l'l' 125. Encouragesmetofoolaroundwithnewideas eveniftheyturnouttohavebeenawasteof time. 126. Wantsmetofindoutanswersformysali. 127. Doaanotbothartoenfa'carules. 128. Seemtoregletthetlamgrowingupandam spandingmaetimeawayfromhome. 129. Prefers me tobegoodinacademic work rather thaninsports. 130. Tellsmehowtospendmyfreatime. 131. Doesn't give me any peace until I do what she “I“ 132. Is less friendly withma if I don’t see things her way. 133. Almost always wants to know who phoned me orwrotstomeandwhattheysaid. 134. Says I should never ride in an autombile. 135. Makes guests feel at home. APPENDIX H Parent Behavior Form Scale Descriptions .hmqmomg .5 Active Involve-ent AI Equalitarianisn 8 Cognitive Independence CI Curiosity CU Cognitive Competence - CC Lax Control LC Confer-ity CO Achievement AC Strict Contror' SC Punitive Control' PC Hostile Control BC Rejection R The order of the scales is determinedgby the correlation of each cluster with the lead scale of acceptance. Therefore, the scales range roughly on an acceptance-rejection dimension. Scales high on the list have a. closer correlation with Acceptance. Scales on the lower end of the list have a negative relationship with acceptance and scales near the middle have low or variable relationships depending upon the parent being considered. 3%u*¥*‘”9§ Active Involvement: Equalitarian- isn: The parent is seen as warm. loving, accepting. Listens to problens. nurturant and caring, concerned about feelings. easy going. hes a positive view of child and enjoys his companionship. The parent becomes actively nurturant and initiates open indications of positive feelings. Parent takes an active role in colnunicating his feelings and concern for the child. Hunts child to know how parent feels about his. Becomes actively involved in child's activi- ties. Tries to treat the child as an equal. Allows open expression of child's feelings. even if negative. Accepts disarrcencnts, listens to child's opinions. Accepts child's friends and ideas. Hen-punitive and nonecritical. Encourages child to think for himself, to come to his own conclusions. whats child to express his individ- uality with parent and with others as well. Encourages critical thinking while keeping an.open mind about his own and others' ideas. Encourages originality, analysis of ideas. Emphasis on child developing own sources of infatuation rather than taking on parents' ideas. LC! CO: AC: QC: IIC : .1555 wants the child to ask questions about life, the world and him- self. Enjoys intellectual dialogue with child. Wants child to appreciate nature and how it evolved. Wants child to keep in- formed on current events and new ideas. wants child to develop skills and to be competent at a variety of tasks. Vents child to develop cultural and aesthetic inter- ests. Provides wide exposure to cultural activities. Encourages individuality and competence at problem solving. Provides a wide latitude of freedom for child's activities. Bots not set down many specific rules for child to follow. Allows child to avoid obeying rules that do exist and ignores misbehavior that occurs. Is never coercive or demanding. Allows child free- dom to develop his own rules. wants child to adopt values of hard work, religious involvement. obedience to rules'and orderliness. Takes an active role in teaching and enforcing these values. Tends to view the child as an extention of himself in these values and feels hurt when child does not conform. Pears losing control over child. Pas high goals for achievement for child. Feels child could do more to be meeting these goals. Communicates to child that he falls short of parent expectancies for him. Wants child to excell in an outstanding career involving professional or scien- tific areas. would like child to be famous. Expects child to be academically superior and successful in all of his endeavors. Has many rules that he communicates and enforces carefully. V Supervises child's activities and is restrictive about free move- ment. Constantly reminds about rules, tries to monitor all be- havior. Tells child what to do in his free time and with whom he may associate. - Insistent and coercive about conformity to all rules. Punishes all misbehavior. Punishes frequently for a variety of infringe- ments. Has many rules. Loses temper when child does not comply and nags until he does. Communicates his dissatisfaction with everything child does. Tells child he is a big problem. Gives blanket criticisms, loses his temper easily, becomes cold when child disagrees with him. Con- trols child through accusations. guilt induction and psychological withdrawal from the relationship. - Communicates his active dislike and dissatisfaction with child. Never shows love or concern. Makes it clear that child is of little importance to him. At the same time, he is intrusive' about child's activities and price into his private life. APPENDIX I l. 2. 3. l. 5. 6. 156 HICHIGAR STATE UNIVERSITY Department of Psychology , DEW RESEARCH CURSE!!! FOE! I have freely consented to take part in a scientific study being conducted by: Linda Cohen under the supervision oi: Albert 1. Robin. Ph.D. . Academic Title: _ Professor The study has been erplained to me and I understand the ex- planation that has been given and what my participation will involve. . I understand that I am free' to discontinue my participation ' in the study at any time without penalty. I understand that the results of the study will be treated in strict confidence and that I will remain anonymus. within these restrictions. results of the study will be made available to me at my request. . . I understand that my participation in the study does not guar- antee any beneficial results to me. I understand that. at my request. I can receive additional erplamtion of the study after ny participation is completed. Signed: mm as new mam: ‘ Women's Roleé L M'c/ITI'ITILHINHLTIJEILHfi/flHjiujflfifll'lflflyiflffiiflflfl'ES