W N MW! H. g \ ___/ -7 \ RORSCHACH ANATGMY RESPONSES ARE 30mm fiévkeé‘xg Yixasls ‘50? {En Degree 05 M.. A. VH3.” SHE’E UHE‘JERSETY '1. L ‘ 5. T‘ ' I 3023351 .5. Conan £959 :~w_«_:.« Yr, ‘JI‘rfin-T ". m .' LIBRARY 1 gain Harm State University r. {{'l. ‘ ‘I‘s ., . ’1‘!!! \II: x?“ I! )l.l._|lllllul , b l 11""...‘1'1 I‘ RORSCHACH ANATOMY RESPONSES AND SOMATIC COMPLAINTS BY ROBERT J. COHEN A THESIS Submitted to the College of Science and Arts Michigan State University of Agriculture and Applied Science in partial fulfillmnt of the requirements for the degree of MASTER OF ARTS Department of Psychology 1959 i: [4 -‘ .“ § L‘ \' ‘§‘\§ ACKNOWLEDGMENT I wish to express my gratitude to Dr. Gerald F. King for his active participation and assistance in the different phases of this study. In this manner, he contributed toward making this a meaningful experience for me. I would like to thank Drs. Albert I. Rabin and Joseph Reyher for serving on my oral examination committee. I appreciate the help given by the following psychol- ogy graduate students by way of offering critical suggestions and/or classifying data: Messrs. Paul Berg, Harold Davis, Jacques Levy; and.Melvin Weinberg. Robert J. Cohen RORSCHACH ANATOMY RESPONSES AND SOMATIC COMPLAINTS BY ROBERT J. COHEN AN ABSTRACT submitted to the College of Science and Arts Michigan State University of Agriculture and Applied Science in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of MASTER OF ARTS Department of Psychology 1959 RORSCHACH ANATOMY RESPONSES AND SOMATIC CORPLAINTS Robert J. Cohen (Gerald F. King, Ph.D., Major Professor) ABSTRACT Designed as a study of the concurrent validity of Rorschach anatomy (Ag) responses, this study investigated the commonly posited relationship between An responses and hypochondriacal symptoms (somatic complaints). A controlled interview, the Rorschach, and the wechsler- Bellevue Verbal Scale (Form I) were administered to 100 recently hospitalized neuropsychiatric patients, all of whom were males. The Rorschach protocols were scored for number of general Ag re- sponses, skeletal anatomy (ngég , and visceral anatomy (23:59) responses. Responses to the interview were classified for somatic complaints in accordance with the following schema: presence or absence of somatic complaints, multiple somatic complaints, focal somatic complaints, and diffuse somatic complaints, plus central- ity of the somatic component in the patient's over-all problemo Acceptable levels of reliability were obtained for Ag responses (inter-rater) and somatic complaints (inter-rater and interview- reinterview). High general _A_n_, M, and M groups were formed and each compared with a control Nofiég group on the classifications used for somatic complaints. With age, IQ, and number of Rorschach responses controlled, tests of significance with chi square revealed no iv reliable differences in any of the comparisons. Consistent trends in favor of the high anatomy groups were not even obtained. The results were viewed as adding to the large number of Ror- schach studies reporting negative findings for standard interpreta- tions, a situation which seems to call for explorations with new Rorschach interpretations. In regard to the relationship between A_n_ responses and hypochondriacal symptoms, some recent conceptual and empirical contributions suggest that future research in this area should take into consideration an additional variable, level of hostile drive strength. ’2 Approved ,SanQ/a Major Pro essor THO/Jug Mb“? Date Table of Contents Page I. Introduction... ..... .... ......... ........ ..... 1 II. Methodology.................... .......... ..... 4 Analysis of Somatic Complaints............ 5 Analysis of An ReSponses.................. 7 Reliability............................... 7 III. Results....................................... 10 High-An Group vs. No-Ag Group............. 10 High-ggfgn Group vs. No—An Group.......... 13 High-Elfign Group vs. Nofign Group.......... 13 Statistical Summary....... ..... ..... ..... . 13 IV. Discussion.................................... 19 V. Summary....................................... 22 VI. References.................................... 24 Appendices........................................ 25 Appendix A (Analysis of Somatic Complaints: Instructions to Raters) Appendix B (Classification of Somatic Complaints: Reliability) Table Table Table Table Table Table Table List of Tables Page Comparison of the Rorschach Anatomy (Ag) Groups on Age, IQ, and Number of Rorschach Responses (3) ................................ 8 Comparison of the High-An and No-An Groups on Somatic Complaints ........................ 11 Comparison of the Highfigg and No-An Groups on Centrality of the Somatic Component in Overall Problem .............................. 12 Comparison of the High—nggn and No-gg Groups on Somatic Complaints ................. 14 Comparison of the Higheggjgn and Nofign Groups on Centrality of the Somatic Component in Overall Problem ........................... 15 Comparison of the Highjyijgn and Nofign Groups on Somatic Complaints....... ........ . ........ 16 Comparison of the High-ylrgn and Nojgn Groups on Centrality of the Somatic Component in Overall Problem ........................... 17 ii I. Introduction In Psychodiagnostics, Rorschach indicates that "in subjects who are not physicians" anatomy (An) responses represent ”either a complex impelling the subject to try to give the impression of intelligence or a tendency to hypochondriacal rumination, or to both" (9, pp. 198-199). Along with Klopfer (4), Beck (1) shares the belief that anatomy associations indicate an excessive concern with health. Rapaport (7), while accepting feelings of intellectual adequacy and bodily preoccupations as meanings of An, suggests that responses stem from two additional sources, namely, generalized anxiety and extreme blocking. In making interpretations of Ag responses, most clinicians take into consideration certain qualitative differences. Rapaport (7) distinguishes between skeletal (§k) and visceral (2;) Ag, ascribing different meanings to each type. Phillips and Smith (6) include the following ‘under Ag reSponses: general anatomy, x—ray, bony anatomy, ‘Visceral anatomy, and gums and teeth. They say that '“persons with psychosomatic disorders....produce bony anatomy contents beyond expectancy" (6, p. 127). It has been generally acknowledged that inferring hypochondriacal traits from a Rorschach protocol is more complex than merely noting the presence of An_(4, 5, 6). Mons outlines the problem in the following manner: The response must therefore be examined and assessed in relationship to (several) factors. Their relative number, their special character - e.g., whether scientific or merely morbid - and their relation to colour will help decide in each instance whether one is dealing with a justifiable association or with a hypochondriacal tendency. The anatomy responses must therefore always be viewed with some suspicion, and only be dis- carded as ‘normal' when their number and quality can be logically accounted for by a 'normal' thought content (5, p. 81, 82). The current investigator is aware of only one empirical study of the relationship between A3 and Zhypochondriasis. Rav (8) obtained, by group adminis- 'tration, Rorschach protocols from a large sample of linselected normal males. Instead of using an outside <2riterion of hypochondriasis, he hypothesized that the rnxmber of Ag responses should be correlated with other Eiigns on the Rorschach indicative of hypochondriasis, (e.g., high 2Q, high 5, etc. The results failed to snipport any of the predictions. While one might disagree ‘With.some of his predictions, the study can be viewed as a test of the Rorschach's internal interpretative con- sistency. Although a review of Rorschach literature reveals differences of opinion regarding the interpretations of Ag responses, there is general agreement concerning one interpretation, i.e., investigators believe that it taps something called hypochondriasis, somatic pre- occupation, or concern with health. It is surprising that this basic interpretation has received so little controlled empirical attention. The present research represents an attempt to test the concurrent validity of An responses, using reported somatic complaints as an outside criterion. While a quantitative analysis is employed, the study takes into consideration the type of Ag responses and the nature of the somatic complaints. II. Methodology A study done by King (3) contributed the raw data employed in the present investigation. Thus, it is desirable that a brief summary of the methodology he used be presented here. For a more detailed account of this research design, the reader is referred to the original source. In his experiment, a controlled interview, the Rorschach Test, and the Wechsler-Bellevue Verbal Scale (Form I) were administered to 100 carefully screened, recently admitted male neuropsychiatric patients at the Fort Custer Veterans Administration Hospital, Battle Creek, Michigan. The controlled interview was used to obtain data to test certain hypotheses concerning the neuropsychiatric patient's orientation toward his illness. The first section of the interview focused on the patient's conception of his problem. A copy of the outline of this section follows: Introduction. As a patient here in the hospital, the hospital staff is interested in you and your problem. If we are to help you, we must get certain information about you. I am going to ask you some questions. I would like you to listen carefully and answer the questions the best you can. Think each question over before answering. I would appreciate your talking slowly because I want to write down as much as I can of what you say. 1. (Nature of the Problem) Like every person who comes to this hospital, there is a reason. We will call this your problem. Now, first of all, I 'would like you to tell me in your own words what your problem is. (If hesitant, the subject should be encouraged. The question can be repeated and paraphrased. If paraphrasing is necessary, only minor variations should be used. If the subject's account of his problem is brief and confined to such general descriptive terms as tense, nervous, emotionally upset, etc., more information should be obtained by asking the general question: "What are you tense (nervous, etc.) about?" At the end of the subject's account, he should be asked: "Anything else?") Every other subject of the first 50 was reinterviewed six to eight days later by another person. The reinterview teas essentially a repetition of the interview except for the introduction. Ihnalysis g; Somatic Complaints The 100 interview protocols were scored for the prresence or absence of a) somatic complaints, b) multi- ple somatic complaints, c) focal somatic complaints, d) cij.ffuse somatic complaints, as well as e) centrality of Somatic component in the patient's overall problem. The fOllowing is a copy of the definitions of these categories that were included in the instructions1 to the judges: Presence 9; somatic complaints. Somatic complaints are defined as any verbalization, spontaneous or otherwise, indicating some degree of discomfort and/or * lTTue complete instructions are available in Appendix A. malfunctioning in any bodily organ or locus (e.g., stomach trouble, headaches, backaches) as well as any overall disturbance in bodily status (e.g., fatigue, malnutrition, loss of weight). Various responses symptomatic of anxiety (e.g., nerves, jumpiness, tension) are not to be classi— fied as somatic complaints unless they are in some way explicitly connected with bodily disturbances. Examples of the latter would be the following. (I worry so much that my head aches. I become very jumpy, even my muscles twitch. This tension and restlessness gets so bad that I get a sinking feeling in my stomach.) With this frame of refer- ence, it is still difficult to make decisions about certain symptoms as to whether they are somatic or not, e.g., sleeplessness (insomnia) and loss of appetite. Symptoms of this nature are to be classified as somatic since they represent dis- turbances in cyclical bodily activities. Multiple somatic complaints. The criteria for multiple somatic complaints is two or more somatic complaints. Focal somatic complaints. Focal somatic complaints are ones in which the disturbances are localized in Specific organs or regions of the body (e.g., stomach aches, pain in arm muscles). Diffuse somatic complaints. The disturbances tend to encompass the entire body in diffuse somatic complaints, with no particular focus or localization (e.g., run-down, tired). ' Classifications for centrality 9f somatic component _ig overall problem. Central: The patient gives the major emphasis to somatic factors (regardless of type) in his account of his problem. Peripheral: The patient includes somatic complaints in his account of his problem, but they are secondary in importance to other factors non-somatic in nature. Absent: The patient does not report somatic complaints in his account of his problem. Analysis pf Ag Responses In this study, the 100 Rorschach protocols were scored for the number of general_§p, Skfgp, and_y;5Ap responses. The criteria for forming the Rorschach anatomy groups were two or more Ag responses for the High-5p group, two or more §krgp responses for the High-gkfgp group, one or more Viegp responses for the Highfiyifigp group, and no “Ag responses for the Nojgp group, which yielded prelimi- nary groups of 34, 26, 23 and 42 Se, respectively. The distribution of these groups in terms of age, Verbal IQ, and number of Rorschach responses (3) were examined for the purpose of equating the groups on these variables. Table 1 gives the results of equating the groups on age, IQ, and .3, along with the final N for each group.2 Reliability The author scored all of the Rorschach protocols for the number of Ag, Skffip, and_yijgp responses, according to Beck's (1) definition of Ag and dictionary (10) defi- nitions of visceral and skeletal. Using the same criteria, another judge independently scored every other protocol. There was 94 per cent agreement for Ag, 90 per cent agree— ment for §Eféflo and 88 per cent agreement for ygsgp. 2The groups were equated by discarding gs with extreme scores . mm.o oeuma me.om oa.aa omauom em.ooa Hm.m seuom m~.0m mm cauoz mm.s oeuma mm.om H®.HH mmauew cm.ooa em.s Neumm mH.Hm om marmmrcmam mm.o canoe mo.mm mm.HH mmaumm so.soa mm.e Neumm mm.om mm mmummunmam ma.o oeuma Hm.om om.aa mmaumm mm.soa mm.e Neumm 6H.Hm mm sarcasm mm mmcmm fl. mw. mmcmm m. mm. mmcmm .fl m. mesonw m. Amev 0H amnno> mom mo Honsdz cam AMQ newcommmm gownomnom .oH Hmano> .mmm so masono Amflv mfioumsm nomnomuom may mo somHHdeou H mHQmB The interview protocols were equally divided into two samples of 50, and each sample was rated in accordance with the definitions of the categories of somatic com- plaints by three independent judges. The only common judge in the two samples was the author. The mean inter- rater reliability for the five judges on the interview categories was as follows: 97 per cent for somatic com- plaints, 95 per cent for multiple somatic complaints, 95 per cent for focal somatic complaints, 86 per cent for diffuse somatic complaints, and 87 per cent for centrality of somatic component in overall problem. More details concerning the obtained reliabilities can be found in Appendix B. One of the judges scored the 25 reinterview pro- tocols for the five categories of somatic complaints. The following interview-reinterview agreement was obtained: 96 per cent agreement for somatic complaints, 96 per cent agreement for multiple somatic complaints, 100 per cent agreement for focal somatic complaints, 84 per cent agree- ment for diffuse somatic complaints, and 80 per cent agreement for centrality of somatic component in overall problem. 10 III. Results A preliminary analysis revealed that 59 of the 100 .Ss were judged to have somatic complaints. Of these, 38 had multiple somatic complaints, 42 had focal somatic complaints, and 42 had diffuse somatic complaints. Twenty- four §s reported somatic complaints of central importance in the overall problem, while the somatic complaints of the remaining 35 Se were classified as peripheral. In analyzing the data, each of the Rorschach anatomy groups was compared with the Noagp group for the five classifications of somatic complaints. Contingency tables were constructed, and significance was tested by chi square. flighsgp Group Mg. Npfigp Group Tables 2 and 3 provide a comparison of the High-Ag and No—Ap groups for the categories of somatic complaints. As can be seen, none of the differences were statistically significant. The data in Table 2 indicate that more Se in the Highsgp group reported the presence of somatic complaints, multiple somatic complaints, and diffuse somatic complaints than did SS in the No-Ap group. In Table 3, it is seen that the Highféfl group gave more cen- ‘trality to somatic components in the overall problem than .ll :.ucmmflm: on a a:ucwmmnd= Op mnmwwu m umwx i ooo.o mmm.o emo.o e~o.a mumsmm are me ma as am om we 6H 6H warez me «a ma as me as as am mmrnmam a m a m a m we «a masons muchHmEou mucamadsou mpcamadEOU mpcamamEoo UHumEom UHmeOm oHumEOm UHumEom mmsmmao swoon washbass mucamadeoo oaumfiom co masonw mmroz cam mflrnmam esp mo GOmHndeoo N dance 12 Tfifle3 Comparison of the High-Ag and No-Ap Groups on Centrality of the Somatic Component in Overall Problem Groups Central Peripheral Absent Highfigp 9 12 ll Nofigp 6 10 16 Chi square = 1.704 13 the No-Ap group. However, all of the trends are minor in nature. mgr-area ___Gro_u2 ye- Ale-Ar; seam Tables 4 and 5 provide a comparison of the High-Skagp and Noagp groups for the categories of somatic complaints. It can be readily seen that none of the chi squares were statistically significant. An examination of Table 4 reveals that there are not even consistent trends in favor of the High-Skrgp group. Table 5 shows that the High-Skjgp group placed slightly more emphasis on the centrality of the somatic component than did the No-Ap group. giqggyiegp Group ys. Ngfigp Group A comparison of the Highsygagp and No-Ap_groups, on somatic complaints is seen in Tables 6 and 7. Again, the pattern of cell frequencies reveals little difference or consistent trends between the groups. All chi squares were low and not significant. Spatistical Summary The results offer a fairly simple summary: none of the RorSChaCh Ag groups reported significantly more somatic complaints of any type or gave more emphasis to somatic complaints than the control No—Ap group. Consistent trends l4 HmH.o Hmn.o Hmm.o mmm.o seesaw ago as me He Hm ow we on ma maroz an an Ha as me o m an marmmunmam a m < m m m a m mdsouw mucflmamEou musfimadeou mucamadEou mucamameou UHumEOm oaumeom UHumEOm UHumEOm mmsmmao Hmoom meanness mucamadeoo oaumEom so mdsoaw mwroz Ucm mflLMWIanm mnu mo cowenmmEoo fl mHQME 15 Table 5 Comparison of the High-Skrgp and Noefip Groups on Centrality of the Somatic Component in Overall Problem Groups Central _Peripheral Absent Highfigkagp 7 7 8 No-An 6 10 16 Chi square 2 1.572 l6 mmo.o Hmo.o eoo.o mmm.e mumswm are on me as am om we as ea warez as m m as NH m a ea mmrmmrcmam a a a a a a a m masons mucamamEou muchHQEOU muCHMHQEOU mucamameoo oaumfiom Uflumeom uaumEom oaquom mmsumao swoon sameness mucamamfioo oaumEOm no masonw caloz one C¢IH>I£mHm may mo consummaou o OHQME 17 Table 7 Comparison of the Highfiylegp and No—Ap Groups on Centrality of the Somatic Component in Overall Problem Groups Central Peripheral Absent Highayifigp 7 7 6 No-An 6 10 16 Chi square I 2.498 18 in favor of the Ag groups were not even obtained. It should be pointed out, however, that the statistical tests with the three Ap_groups (High-Ag, High-Skrgp, and High— .yifiép) were not independent, as the three groups showed considerable overlap in terms of common gs. Derived from chi squares computed from median tests3, the phi coefficients among the three types of Ag reSponses were as follows: Ap vs. ghféfl, .89; Ag vs. Zifflfl, .50; and Skrgp vs. yifigfl, .36. w Chi squares: Ag vs. §Efléfl, 79.6; Ag vs. yifiéfl, 25.7; and §Efifl£ vs. ylfflfl, 13.2 (all significant beyond the .01 level of confidence). 19 IV. Discussion In considering negative results, the immediate question usually arises as to what is the most adequate interpretation. Should the conclusion be that the results do g2; lend support to the notion that number of Rorschach Ag responses is related to hypochondriacal complaints? Or, would it be more appropriate to say that the results are inconclusive due to certain methodological deficiencies? Let us turn to the methodology employed in this research. The results cannot be attributed to differences in age, IQ, or number of Rorschach responses as the groups were equated for these variables. There is the matter of the controlled interview, a crucial aspect of the methodology. This instru- ment yielded fairly good inter—rater and interview-reinterview reliability. It should also be pointed out that the same controlled interview yielded positive results in another Rorschach study (3). What remains are the gs and the setting of the study, "functional" neuropsychiatric patients in a neuropsychiatric hospital. It is granted that it would be desirable to try a variation of this study with another population (e.g., general hospital patients); but if the EXDsited relationship between Ag responses and hypochondriasis 20 is a general one, it should have held up in the present study. The most appropriate interpretation then seems to be that the results do not support a relationship between number of Ag responses and hypochondriacal complaints. This study contributes to the growing reservoir of negative research results for standard Rorschach inter- pretations. It would appear that the situation calls for explorations with new interpretations for some of the Rorschach variables. King's (3) study of human movement (g) indicates that new conceptual schemas can lead to fruitful results. In regard to Ag responses, Phillips and Smith have recently offered the following different interpretation of this variable: ”Anatomy content reflects a sensitivity to, and concern with, the expression of destructive impulses. Paradoxically, those individuals who act out their destructive impulses do not develop anatomy content...” (6, p. 123). Using this frame of reference, Wolf (11) com- pared a group of patients who had histories of hostile acting out with a group classified as "non-actors," finding that Ag reSponses were a significant factor only when hostile drive level, as derived from Rorschach content, was taken into consideration. He offers the interpretation that Ag responses in the presence of high hostile drive operate as 21 a control factor which channels these impulses into somatiza- tion and other substitutive activities. Further, Ag responses produced in individuals with low hostile drive probably have some other meaning. Wolf's findings and interpretations indicate that level of hostile drive should be taken into consideration in any future study of the relationship between Ag responses and hypochondriacal complaints. A possible procedure for the present data would be to divide the As, on the basis of Rorschach content, into two groups, one with high and one with low hostile drive strength. The suggestion is that Ag responses would be related to hypochondriacal symptoms in the high group but not in the low one. 22 V. Summary Designed as a study of the concurrent validity of Rorschach anatomy (Ag) responses, this study investigated the commonly posited relationship between Ag responses and hypochondriacal symptoms. A controlled interview, the Rorschach, and the Wechsler- Bellevue Verbal Scale (Form I) were administered to 100 recently hospitalized neuropsychiatric patients (all males). The Rorschach protocols were scored for number of general ‘Ag responses, skeletal anatomy (ggfiAg), and visceral anatomy (ylfAfl) responses. Responses to the interview were classified for somatic complaints in accordance with the following achema: presence or absence of somatic complaints, multiple somatic complaints, focal somatic complaints, and diffuse somatic complaints, plus centrality of the somatic component in the patient's overall problem. Acceptable levels of reliability were obtained for Ag responses (inter— rater) and somatic complaints (inter-rater and interview- reinterview). High general Ag, fig-Ag, and ygfiAg groups were formed and each compared with a control NofiAg group on the classifications used for somatic complaints. With age, IQ, 23 and number of Rorschach responses controlled, tests of significance with chi square revealed no reliable differences in any of the comparisons. Consistent trends in favor of the high anatomy groups were not even obtained. The results were viewed as adding to the large number of Rorschach studies reporting negative results for standard interpretations, a situation which seems to call for explora- tions with new Rorschach interpretations. In regard to the relationship between Ag responses and hypochondriacal symptoms, some recent conceptual and empirical contributions suggest that future research in this area should take into con- sideration an additional variable, level of hostile drive strength. 10. ll. 24 VI. References Beck, S. J. Rorschach's Test. Vol. II. .5 variety .9: personality pictures. New York: Grune and Stratton, 1946. Beck, S. J. Rorschach's Test. Vol. III. Advances Ag interpretation. New York: Grune and Stratton, 1952. King, G. F. A theoretical and experimental considera- tion of the Rorschach human movement response. Psychol. Monogr., 1958, 72, No. 5 (Whole No. 458). Klopfer, B., Ainsworth, Mary D., Klopfer, W. G., and Holt, R. R. Developments Ag the Rorschach Technique. Yonkers: World Book Co., 1954. Mons, W. Principles and practice pg the Rorschach Personality Test. London: Faber and Faber, 1947. Phillips, L., and Smith, J. G. Rorschach interpretation: Advanced technique. New York: Grune and Stratton, 1953. Rapaport, D. Diagnostic psychological testing. Vol. II. The theory, statistical evaluation, and diagnostic application p§_g.batte§y p§_tests. Chicago: Year Book Publishers, 1946. Rav, J. Anatomy responses in the Rorschach Test. .g. proj. Tech., 1951, 15, 433—443. Rorschach, H. Psychodiagnostics: A diagnostic test based pg perception. (Trans, by P. Lemkau and B. Kronenberg) New York: Grune and Stratton, 1942. webster's New Collegiate Dictionary. Springfield: G. and C. Merriam, 1951. Wolf, I. Hostile acting out and Rorschach Test content. _g.gproj. Tech., 1957, 21, 414-419. Appendix A 25 26 Analysis p§_Somatic Complaints Instructions 39 Raters You are asked to read carefully the following proto— cols and make certain judgments or ratings. The protocols represent close to verbatim recordings of the responses of hospitalized neuropsychiatric patients when they were asked to describe their problems (reasons for being in the hospi- tal). Thus, each patient has given his version of his illness. Presence of somatic complaints. Score each protocol for the presence or absence of somatic complaints. If a somatic complaint (or complaints) is included as part of the problem by the patient, record A for present in the appropriate column of the Rating Sheet. If somatic com- plaints are absent, record an A. Use the following- definition of somatic complaints as a frame of reference in making the judgments: Somatic complaints are defined as any verbalization, spontaneous or otherwise, indicating some degree of dis- comfort and/or malfunctioning in any bodily organ or locus (e.g., stomach trouble, headaches, backaches) as well as any overall disturbance in bodily status (e.g., fatigue, malnutrition, loss of weight). Various responses symptomatic of anxiety (e.g., nerves, jumpiness, tension) are not to be classified as somatic complaints unless they are in some way explicitly connected with bodily disturbances. Examples of the latter would be the following. (I worry so much that my head aches. I become very jumpy, even my muscles twitch. This tension and restlessness gets so bad that I get a sinking 27 feeling in my stomach.) With this frame of reference, it is still difficult to make decisions about certain symptoms as to whether they are somatic or not, e.g., sleeplessness (insomnia) and loss of appetite. Symptoms of this nature are to be classified as somatic since they represent disturbances in cyclical bodily activities. Multiple somatic complaints. The criterion for multiple somatic complaints is two or more somatic complaints. Score each protocol for the presence (2) or absence (A) of multi— ple somatic complaints in the appropriate column of the Rating Sheet. Focal somatic complaints. Focal somatic complaints are ones in which the disturbances are localized in specific organs or regions of the body (e.g., stomach aches, pain in arm muscles). Consider only focal somatic complaints and score each protocol either gior A. Diffuse somatic complaints. The disturbances tend to encompass the entire body in diffuse somatic complaints, with no particular focus or localization (e.g., run-down, tired). Consider only diffuse somatic complaints and record either A or‘A for each protocol. Centrality p; somatic component Ag overall problem. Now evaluate the importance of any somatic component in the patient's overall version of his problem. You are to judge how much emphasis is given somatic factors by the patient in relation to other non—somatic factors. Use the following 28 categories in rating each protocol. Central (g): The patient gives the major emphasis to somatic factors (regardless of type) in his account of his problem. Peripheral (g): The patient includes somatic complaints in his account of his problem, but they are secondary in importance to other factors non-Somatic in nature. Absent (A): The patient does g2; report somatic complaints in his account of his problem. Appendix B 29 30 Classification of Somatic Complaints: Reliability First 50 Interview Protocols Somatic Complaints Judges Percentage of Agreement A vs. B 94 A vs. C 98 B vs. C 96 Multiple Somatic Complaints A vs. B 90 A vs. C 96 B vs. C 94 Focal Somatic Complaints A vs. B 98 A vs. C 96 B vs. C 96 Diffuse Somatic Complaints A vs. B 80 A vs. C 84 B vs. C 84 Centrality of Somatic Component A vs. B 80 A vs. C 92 B vs. C 84 Second 50 Interview Protocols Somatic Complaints D vs. E 98 D vs. F 98 E vs. F 100 Multiple Somatic Complaints D vs. E 98 D vs. F 98 E vs. F 96 Focal Somatic Complaints D vs. E 90 D vs. F 92 E vs. F 98 Diffuse Somatic Complaints D vs. E 86 D vs. F 94 E vs. F 88 Centrality of Somatic Component D vs. E 84 D vs. F 88 E vs. F 96 ROOM USE ONLY