A STUDENT-INSTRUCTOR DEMONSTRATION LABORAIOIIY METHOD III. AN INDIVIDUAL *§-3i;;fl:r;'fif LABORATORY METHOD IN A BEGINNING COLLEGE FOODS counsa ’ Thom IN» Ila Dag-m of M D. g:j_ MICHIGAN SUITE WWII“! " . Mary C Coleman 7 .5?ij i:‘ ‘ “ 1966 ----- THESIS LIBRARY " (0, (,1. » Mgdligan Sun :Jm, This is to certify that the thesis entitled ‘ I A Student - Instructor Demonstration Laboratory 5 Method Vs. An Individual Laboratory Method f In A Beginning College Foods Course . presented hg Mary C. Coleman i - has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for PhoD. degree in Foods 0 \ ~~‘ , / )fié j ‘ i / Major professor Date February 14. 1966 0-169 ABSTRACT A STUDENT-INSTRUCTOR DEMONSTRATION LABORATORY METHOD VS. AN INDIVIDUAL LABORATORY METHOD IN A BEGINNING COLLEGE FOODS COURSE by Mary C. Coleman The study described in this thesis was designed to evaluate the effectiveness of an experimental approach (Student-Instructor Demonstration) to the laboratory instruc- tion in a beginning food preparation course at Michigan State University. Students were stratified on the basis of their University Entrance Examination scores then randomly assigned to two concurrently scheduled laboratory sections. The method of laboratory instruction and assignment of instructor were determined randomly for the groups of students participating in the study fall term, 1965. The instructor and teaching method used for the student groups studied winter and spring terms, 1964 were established to replicate the fall term design. A total group of 104 students originally qualified for the study. Students from the larger laboratory sections were randomly excluded from the study to produce samples of equal numbers for statistical analyses. The final samples studied totaled 88 students. Mary C. Coleman The EN 200 Pretest and Questionnaire provided data con- cerning the non-numerical characteristics of the samples studied as well as an indication of proficiency in areas related to food preparation at the onset of the course. The Student Evaluation of EN 200 form was used as a measure of student attitude toward the course. The EN 200 Final Examination was employed as the measure of student achievement at the completion of the course. An analysis of variance factorial design was used to analyze student scores on the two latter instruments. Hypotheses Tested and General Findings The First Hypothesis of the Study was: A ”Student-Instructor Demonstration Method” is at least as effective as an "Individual Method” of laboratory instruction in achieving common course objectives in a beginning foods preparation course for students of both above and below median general ability. The statistical tests and numerical data indicate no significant differences (5% level) attributable to method of instruction or other factors investigated with the exception of ability level in the winter—spring, 1964 samples. The Second Hypothesis of the Study was: Student attitudes toward a "Student—Instructor Demon— stration Method" are at least as favorable as are student attitudes toward an "Individual Method” of instruction in the laboratory portion of a beginning food preparation course. Mary C. Coleman The statistical tests and numerical data indicate no significant differences (5% level) attributable to method of instruction or other factors investigated. Under the conditions of the study, there were no indi- cations that the Individual Method of laboratory instruction was superior to the Student-Instructor Demonstration Method. When individual laboratory and teacher—demonstration (lecture- demonstration) laboratory methods of instruction have been compared in recent studies in science courses (1,2,5,4), no significant differences have been reported between the two methods of instruction on the basis of the criteria used. If real differences, attributable solely to method of in— struction, do exist, it may be possible that suitable instru— ments for their measurement have not yet been developed. It is also possible that the method of instruction has only a minor influence on the outcome of studentsI experience in a course. Verification of the latter postulation would indicate that the method of instruction could be freely alternated to meet the interest of the course instructor or needs resulting from expanding student populations. REFERENCES 1. Bradley, Robert L. Lecture Demonstration vs. Individual Laboratory Work in a Natural Science Course at Michigan State University. (Unpublished Ph.D. Dissertation, College of Education, Michigan State University, E. Lansing) 1962. 2. Dearden, Douglas M. An evaluation of the laboratory in a college general biology course. J. Experi. Ed. gg: 241 (1960). 5. Mary C. Coleman Bay, May S. An Experimental Study to Compare the Indi- vidual Laboratory and Modified Demonstration Methods for Teaching Food Preparation. (Unpublished Masters Thesis, School of Home Economics, Michigan State College, E. Lansing) 1952. Trotter, Virginia Yapp. A Comparison of the Laboratory and the Lecture-Demonstration Methods of Teaching Survey of Food Preparation for Freshmen Home Economics Students at the University of Vermont. (Unpublished Ph.D. Disser— tation, The Ohio State University, Columbus) 1960. A STUDENT-INSTRUCTOR DEMONSTRATION LABORATORY METHOD VS. AN INDIVIDUAL LABORATORY METHOD IN A BEGINNING COLLEGE FOODS COURSE BY Mary CL Coleman A THESIS Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Foods and Nutrition 1966 ACKNOWLEDGMENTS The writer wishes to acknowledge Professor Joe L. Saupe for converting the task of writing this dissertation into an exciting learning experience. She wishes to thank Barbara B. Deskins for assisting in the many facets of developing and teaching FN 200, thus making this study a more valid one. A special thanks is extended to Professor Dena C. Cederquist for making unusual adjustments in teach- ing and course schedules to accommodate the requirements of the study. Her warm interest, encouragement and patience have been appreciated. Gratitude is extended to Professors Grace A. Miller, Bernard S. Schweigert and Pearl A. Aldrich for assisting in planning a program of graduate study and critically reading the manuscript. Special recognition is paid to Rosann Bongey for her assistance in typing and to Dorothy Arata, supervisor in charge of thoughtful encouragement. Thanks is extended to General Foods Corporation for providing funds which made this study possible. ii Chapter I. II. III. IV. TABLE OF CONTENTS BACKGROUND AND PURPOSE . . . . . . The Need for Research on Teaching Methods Background of the Study . . . . Definitions . . . . . . . . . Purpose of the Study . . . . . Population . . . . . . . . . . Prospectus . . . . . . . . . . REVIEW OF LITERATURE . . . . . . . Research on Teaching Methods . Laboratory Instruction . . . . Individual vs. Teacher—Demonstration ‘ Laboratories . . . . . . . Summary . . . . . . . . . . . . DESIGN, PROCEDURE, AND INSTRUMENTS Design . . . . . . . . . . . . Procedure . . . . . . . . . . . Instruments of Measurement . . Summary . . . . . . . . . . . . RESULTS AND DISCUSSION . . . . . . Preliminary Results . . . . . . Statistical Tests Employed . . Summary . . . . . . . . . . . SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS . . . . . Specific Hypotheses Investigated Method of the Study . . . . . . Limitations of the Study . . . General Conclusions . . . . . . Recommendations for Future Study General Discussion . . . . . . LITERATURE CITED . . . . . . . . . . . . APPENDICES . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . iii Page \IOUOUIPCNP I-\ a) 15 17 18 18 20 25 52 55 55 47 6O 62 62 65 64 65 65 66 68 74 Table 10. 11. 12. LIST OF TABLES Distribution of Instructors and Methods of Laboratory Instruction for Three School Terms and Two Class Times . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Specifications of the EN 200 Final Examination. Selected Characteristics of the Student Samples Studied . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Medians and Ranges of CQT-T Scores of the Four Groups from Which the Student Samples were Selected in a Study of Two Methods of Labora— tory Instruction in EN 200 . . . . . . . . . . Means and Standard Deviations of CQT—T Scores of the Student Samples Studied . . . . . . . . Means and Standard Deviations of CQT-T Scores for Above and Below Median Students for Three School Terms and Two Class Meeting Times. . . . Frequency Distribution of EN 200 Pretest Scores Frequency Distribution of Student Evaluation of EN 200 Scores (N = 88). . . . . . . . . . . . . Distribution of Item Difficulty for the EN 200 Final Examination . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Distribution of Item Discrimination Indices for the FN 200 Final Examination . . . . . . . . . Frequency Distribution of EN 200 Final Exami— nation Scores (N = 88). . . . . . . . . . . . . Analysis of Variance of EN 200 Final Exami— nation Scores as Influenced by Time of Day, Ability Level, Method of Instruction and Instructor. Fall Term, 1965 . . . . . . . . . iv Page 20 52 55 56 58 59 4O 45 44 44 46 49 LIST OF TABLES — Continued Table 15. 14. 15. 16. 17. 18. 19. 20. 21. Analysis of Variance of EN 200 Final Exami- nation Scores as Influenced by Ability Level School Term and Method of Instruction— Instructor. Winter and Spring Terms, 1964 . . . The Means of the FN 200 Final Examination Scores for the Student Sample Studied Fall Term, 1965 . The Means of the FN 200 Final Examination Scores for the Student Sample Studied Winter and Spring Term, 1964 o o o o o o o o o o o o o o o o o o o Analysis of Variance of Rating Scale Scores for the Student Evaluation of FN 200 Form, Fall Term, 1963 O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O C Analysis of Variance of Rating Scale Scores for the Student Evaluation of FN 200 Form, Winter and Spring Terms, 1964 . . . . . . . . . . . . . The Means of the Rating Scale Scores for the Student Evaluation of FN 200 Form for the Student Samples Studied Fall Term, 1965. . . . . The Meansof the Rating Scale Scores for the Student Evaluation of FN 200 Form for the Student Samples Studied Winter and Spring Terms, 1964 . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . A Summary of Student Responses to Four Open-End Questions Used as a Portion of an Evaluation Form in a Beginning Foods Course (N = 88). . . . A Summary of Mean Scores of the Student Samples Studied in an Evaluation of Two Methods of Laboratory Instruction in a Food Preparation Course . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Page 49 51 52 55 55 54 55 56 59 APPENDIX A. LIST OF APPENDICES Laboratory Outline FN 200, Fall, 1965. (Individual Method of Laboratory Instruction) Laboratory Outline FN 200, Fall, 1965. (Student-Instructor Method of Laboratory Instruction) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . FN 200 Pretest and Questionnaire . . . . . . Student Evaluation of EN 200 form. . . . . . vi Page 75 Chapter I BACKGROUND AND PURPOSE The primary purposes of Chapter I are to indicate the need for research on teaching methods and to present the background and purposes of this study. The Need for Research on Teaching Methods The selection of a method of instruction is often based on tradition rather than the current objectives of the .course. Enarson (1) has said: The time is ripe for searching analysis and sweeping innovations in course content and method. The young teacher emulates his own instructors rather than seeking out the most appropriate methods and tools for a given teaching situation. It has been suggested by Eckert (2) that the teacher's job is to structure the situation in such a way as to stimulate and aid the learner in acquiring the needed competencies. Teaching methods must be chosen in terms of the ends sought. A comment by Buxton (5) describes the spirit of many additional articles which have sought to give greater direction to research on teaching: It is difficult to analyze the research on teaching at the college level. It is relatively scanty and often not very well done. The following prediction by Hannah (4) is particularly relevant to the methods selected to provide learning experi— ences for students in laboratory courses: We probably will have twice as many students on campus in 1975 as today (1965). We shall have none too much time to teach the why, leaving much of the training in techniques to business and industry and other segments of the economy which employ the college graduate. Many college food courses are heavily weighted with in— dividual laboratory work. This traditional pattern is based on the philosophy that appreciable time must be devoted to the development of food preparation skills and techniques. Such a method of instruction is costly in terms of student time, faculty teaching time, laboratory space, equipment, and food expenditures. With particular reference to the teaching of food preparation, Sweetman (5) has said: I believe that we must move rapidly in the direction of reducing laboratory time to a minimum and using the time we do retain for demonstrating principles. Her views have been re—stated by a number of home economists in higher education with the specific directive that we carefully evaluate our current methods of instruc— tion (6,7,8,9,10). From the foregoing comments, it seems clear that the laboratory portion of a college foods course must be evalu— ated in terms of 1) use of student—faculty time and 2) func— tion in implementing the objectives of the course. The Background of the Study Foods and Nutrition 200 (EN 200) is a five-credit, beginning, food preparation course consisting of two one— hour lectures and three two—hour laboratories per week. The course carries an organic chemistry prerequisite. The university catalog issued for 1965—64 describes the course as follows: Scientific principles of food preparation with special emphasis on the physical and chemical changes involved. At the time this study was conducted, the course was required in the programs of students majoring in the follow- ing areas: Dietetics Foods Hotel, Motel and Club Management Institutional and Hospital Management Research in Foods and Nutrition Restaurant Management The course could be elected by students majoring in other programs providing the students had taken the pre— requisite course in organic chemistry. This latter group represented a very small proportion of the total course enrollment. During spring term, 1960, six members of the Foods and Nutrition Staff at Michigan State University met as a com— mittee to reconsider the objectives and methods of instruc— tion in FN 200. At this time the following objectives were adopted for the course: 1. To develop an ability to recognize good or poor quality in representative food products and to understand the reasons for these quality differ— ences. j‘ 2. To learn the function of ingredients, the effects of the ratio of ingredients, and the influence of the kind and extent of manipulation on the prepa— ratiOn of representative food proddcts. 5. To acquire an understanding of the application of chemical and physical principles to food prepa— ration. 4. To acquire an understanding of and ability to use the terminology relevant to the science of food preparation. In an effort to utilize the laboratory portion of the course as effectively as possible in achievement of course objectives and use of student—faculty time, modifications in the traditional method of laboratory instruction were investigated by two instructors in the Foods and Nutrition Department. After three school terms of pretesting, labora— tory lessons that appeared to be most effectively presented as demonstrations by the instructor were identified. The remaining laboratory lessons were modified so groups of students were responsible for the preparation of demonstra- tions. This method of instruction has been identified as the ”Student-InstructoriDemonstration” method of laboratory instruction and is defined more precisely below. Definitions Student—Instructor Demonstration Laboratory Method (Experimental Method)--For the purpose of this study the above term is defined as a method in which the instructor presents approximately one—third of the experiments as lecture—demonstrations. The remaining two-thirds of the laboratories are conducted as student—prepared demonstrations with a summarizing discussion led by the instructor. Stu- dents are required to evaluate a series of food products of varying quality as a final laboratory measure of students“ ability to critically appraise food products. These food products are not prepared by the student. Individual Laboratory Method (Control Method)—~For the purpose of this study the above term is defined as a method by which individual students or pairs of students perform an experiment with a final summarizing discussion led by the instructor. Students are required to prepare and evaluate a series of food products as a final laboratory measure of their ability to critically appraise food products. The instructors involved in the formulation of the teaching by the ”Student-Instructor Demonstration Laboratory Method” postulated that this method provided a laboratory experience for students which more rigorously implemented the first three objectives of the course. Favorable stu- dent attitude toward the course was also encouraging. These subjective appraisals were difficult to interpret as, concurrent with the development of this modified method of laboratory instruction, admission requirements to the uni— versity were providing a potentially more able group of students. Purpose of the Study It has become the purpose of this\study to compare, as objectively as possible, the "Student-Instructor Demon— stration Laboratory Method" with the more traditional "Individual Laboratory Method” in a beginning, college, food preparation course. To nurture this purpose the following objectives are pertinent: A. To provide an experimental test of the two hypothe- ses stated below: 1. A "Student-Instructor Demonstration Method" is at least as effective as an "Individual Method" of laboratory instruction in achieving common course objectives in a beginning food prepae“ ration course for students of both above and below median general ability. 2. Student attitudes toward a "Student—Instructor Demonstration Method" are at least as favorable as are student attitudes toward an "Individual Method" of instruction in the laboratory portion of a beginning food preparation course. B. To investigate the following additional factors as they might condition variations in student achieve- ment and attitude under the two laboratory methods of instruction: 1. Student factors: Sex, academic major and previous food preparation experience. 2. Scheduling factors: Academic term, class hours, and laboratory instructor. Population It was the intent of this investigation to study a sample of students that would be representative of current enrollments in a beginning food preparation course (FN 200) at Michigan State University. We.assume that the conclu— sions reached concerning the laboratory portion of the course in question will have application to future enroll— ments in the course as long as the student population which the course services undergoes no major change in composition. /’ Prospectus The following chapter contains a review of criticisms of the procedures that have been used to study and compare methods of teaching. It also describes methodological studies concerning laboratory instruction in general college science courses as well as in college food preparation courses. The design of this study, description of sample selection procedures, treatments, and instruments are found in Chapter III. Chapter IV reports preliminary results concerning the sample and instruments along with a report of the statistical techniques used in testing the hypotheses. The results of these analyses are reported and discussed. The final chapter is a summary of the findings and conclusions of the study with recommendations for future investigation in the area. Chapter II REVIEW OF LITERATURE This chapter reports on three areas of the literature which are relevant to the study under consideration. First are those articles which question past research on teaching methods. The second part of the review relates to those reports which evaluate the function of the laboratory in the learning process. The final part of the chapter reports on comparative studies between "individual" and "teacher— demonstration” laboratories in natural science at the college level of instruction. Research on Teaching Methods The role of research in teaching has been thoughtfully stated by Campbell and Stanley (11): The experiment is the only means for settling disputes regarding educational practice, the only way of veri- fying educational improvements, and the only way of establishing a cumulative tradition in which improve- ments can be introduced without the danger of faddish discard of old wisdom in favor of inferior novelties. A number of subject matter specialists and administrators in higher education support the spirit of this directive by strongly encouraging the study of teaching methods (12,15,14, 15,16,17). There are others (18,19,20) who indicate that the primary emphasis of educational research should be the analysis of content and basic studies of learning. The authors, whose thinking has made the greatest contribution to the present study, are those convinced of the need for evidence concerning methods used in instruction, but have identified inherent weaknesses in many of the ”methods of teaching" studies reported. Good (21) has concluded that, in a number of studies on teaching methods, the sample size was too small and the experimental period too brief to provide a firm basis for a generalization of results. Biased sampling and lack of suitable control groups have been identified as deficiencies in the design of many studies of teaching methods (22,25). Bias has been introduced into studies when a single in— structor is responsible for teaching by the two or more methods of instruction being compared (24,25). Stanley (26) and others (27,28) encourage greater use of factorial de- signs in methodological studies to allow several variables to be studied simultaneously. The use of randomization to establish comparable study group, rather than matching, has been difficult for many educational researchers to accept (29). On the basis of these criticisms it may be concluded that a well designed study of teaching methods should in— volve replication and randomization. Control measures, including the involvement of at least two persons in teaching 10 by each method of instruction being studied, should be undertaken. While the number of students in the sample should be large, the sample should be representative of the usual number of enrollments in the course. A statistical test which is both powerful and capable of analyzing more than one variable at a time should be used. An experimental teaching method may be deemed equivalent or superior to a traditional method as a result of an in- creased effort made by students because they are aware of being in an experimental group (50;51,52;55)._ Students' motivation for passing or earning excellent grades may com- pel them to study harder when ineffective teaching methods are being used; thus obscuring difference resulting from methods of instruction (54,55). Teaching methods conducive to optimal achievement for some students may be detrimental to the achievement of others, thus making the comparison of teaching methods difficult to evaluate (56,57). Informing both the experimental and control groups that they are participating in an experiment may reduce any dif— ference related to a "Hawthorne" effort on the part of some students by chance discovery of their participation in a study. The use of other measures, in addition to course examinations, should reduce the bias imposed by extra study on the part of students being instructed by a less effective method. 11 Laboratory Instruction Tyler (58) has listed four questions which he feels must be answered in developing any curriculum or plan of instruction: 1. What educational purposes are to be attained? 2. What educational experiences can be provided that are likely to attain these purposes? 5. How can these educational experiences be effec— tively organized? 4. How can we determine whether these purposes are being attained? Theoretically and traditionally laboratory instruction has been well justified as an educational experience which assists in attaining the objectives of science courses. It has attempted to accomplish this function by providing students with an opportunity to practice careful observa- tions of outcomes prior to drawing conclusions. It has also been useful in acquainting students with the methods and materials of the specific area of science in question. In a historical review of the purpose and character of laboratory instruction, Blick (59) writes that laboratory work has been one of the most important and commonly used procedures for science instruction since the beginning of the twentieth century. Both Blick (40) and McKeachie (41) support the view that laboratory instruction provides first hand experience in observation and manipulation of the materials of science, a method superior to others in meet— ing such course objectives as development of understanding and appreciation. The laboratory is a means to an end and 12 not an end in itself. McKeachie (42) points out: One would not expect laboratory teaching to have an advantage over other teaching methods in the amount of information learned. Rather we might expect the dif- ferences to be revealed in retention, in ability to apply learning or actual skill in observation or manipu- lation of materials. Kruglak (45) does not believe it is possible for stu— dents to understand what science is without some first—hand experience with the objects, tools and methods of science. He lists the following objectives for laboratory instruction: 1. To illustrate and give meaning to lectures discus— sions and text books. 2. To develop skill in accurate, independent and orderly observation. 5. To demonstrate relationship between fact and inferences. 4. To provide practice in planning and carrying out an experimental problem. An opposing view is taken by Ginsberg (44) and McGrath (45) who both indicate that the important concepts in any science field can be and have been taught without formal laboratory classes. Ginsberg (46) and Blick (47) deplore the many laboratory courses which simply keep the student occupied with "busy—work." If the science laboratory has a unique contribution to make in implementing the objectives for a given course, and if Kruglack's (48) objectives for laboratory experience are valid, then an experience which provides demonstrations using the methods and materials of the science in question, combined with student participation, may be expected to pro— vide an educational experience at least as valuable, in 15 terms of student attitude and achievement, as an individual laboratory experience. Individual vs. Teacher-Demonstration Laboratories Cunningham (49) evaluated 54 studies which purported to compare the effectiveness of the individual laboratory with the teacher—demonstration technique. The studies included unpublished Masters and Ph.D. theses as well as published research reports. He encountered violations of good experi— mental design such as lack of random sampling, lack of suit- able control groups and lack of replication. These violations made the generalization of results impossible in many of the studies reviewed. When Cunningham pooled the conclusions of the authors of the studies, he found twenty investigators favoring the teacher-demonstration method, six favoring the individual laboratory and two that found no difference be— tween the two methods of instruction. He did not report any conclusions reached by the remaining six studies. Cunningham's findings reflect the criticisms of methodologies reported earlier in this review. Bradley (50) compared lecture—demonstration with indi— vidual laboratory work in a general education, natural science course at Michigan State University. His sample con- sisted of eighty students during spring term, 1960 and 82 students during spring term, 1961. His study appears to have many criteria of a well designed research study. Questionable 14 aspects of his study would include: the use of term-end examination grades as the only criterion and the assumption that his sample was equivalent for two consecutive school years. A measure of any difference in student attitude toward the course, or natural science, as a result of method of instruction would have strengthened his study. He found that the two methods of laboratory instruction did not result in student achievement which was significantly different. Dearden (51) studied the complete class of 924 University of Minnesota general biology students in an evaluation of four laboratory treatments. The laboratory treatments were designated as: individual (traditional), demonstration, workbook exercise, and term paper (submission of a single term paper in lieu of any other laboratory participation). Students were stratified on the basis of college and sex, and were randomly assigned to one of the four types of labora- tory treatments. Scores on standardized tests of general biological knowledge, scientific thinking and biological attitudes were used as measures to compare the influence exerted by the laboratory method. Dearden concluded that no one laboratory treatment was superior to another in increas— ing the scores of students on the three tests administered at the end of the course or when students were re-tested three months later. No one method of laboratory instruction was more effective than the others in promoting higher test scores among students evaluated as being of high, medium or 15 low academic ability. Dearden made no attempt to study any interaction effect due to instructors participating in the study. All students attended lectures which were assumed equivalent in content. The factors controlling students‘ performances on the measures used in this study may have been influenced greatly by the common lecture portion of the course. A study reported in 1941 by Bloye and Long (52) compared a conventional (individual) laboratory with one taught by the teacher-demonstration method in a beginning college food preparation course. Students were familiarized with the dif— ferences between the two laboratory methods at the onset of the course. They were then encouraged to enroll in the laboratory best suited to their past experience in food preparation. Examination scores were used as criteria but no report was made of statistical comparisons. The authors concluded that both methods of laboratory instruction were equally effective when measured by student achievement in the course. Lack of any evidence of randomization, repli— cation or control make a generalization of these conclusions impossible. A similar study was initiated at Michigan State Univer— sity in 1952 (55). When examination scores were used as criteria, no significant differences were observed between an individual method and a teacher—demonstration method of laboratory instruction in a beginning college foods course. 16 Once again lack of randomization and replication in the study make the results difficult to apply to other teaching situ- ations. Trotter (54) compared the individual laboratory and lecture-demonstration methods of instruction in a beginning food preparation course at the University of Vermont. She matched two groups, each containing fourteen students, on the following basis: . College Entrance Exam Board"Scores Spitzer Study Skills Test Score Critical Thinking Test Score Listening Comprehension Test Score High School Home Economics Experience 4-H Club WOrk . Home Experience . Employment Experience (DxlOBU'HP-CNNP: Each of the two matched groups of students was taught four units of laboratory work by the individual laboratory method, than four units by the lecture-demonstration method. The same instructor was responsible for all the teaching in the course. The examination scores in the course were used as criterion. The same examination was used as a pretest and as a final examination. In addition, portions of the examination were used as tests after each unit of work. A practical laboratory examination was also used as cri— terion. Students were requested to evaluate the course; however, the precise procedure for this evaluation was not described. No significant differences were observed between the two teaching methods as determined by examination scores. The lack of replication, small sample size (N = 28), and lack 17 of reliability data for the practical laboratory examination must be questioned since students had experience with the same questions on a pretest and unit tests during the course. Conclusions concerning the effectiveness of the lecture- demonstration laboratory method are to be questioned further since only four laboratory units, during the semester course, were taught by this method. Summary In spite of a well identified need for methodological research, conclusions of past studies are difficult to gen- eralize to other teaching situations. They lack one or more of the requisites of self—contained experiments, namely: randomization, replication and control. There is evidence and opinion to support the theory that some form of laboratory instruction aids in meeting the objectives of science courses. The specific nature of this laboratory experience has not been adequately defined. When individual laboratory and teacher—demonstration (lecture—demonstration) laboratory methods of instruction have been compared in recent studies (55,56,57,58) no sig— nificant differences have been reported between the two methods of instruction on the basis of the criteria used. Chapter III DESIGN, PROCEDURE, AND INSTRUMENTS The purpose of Chapter III is to describe the design, procedure and instruments used to gather data for this study. Johnson (59) lists the following requirements for a self—contained experiment: control, randomization, and replication. His directives have provided the structure for building the design and procedure used. The purpose and population of the study have been described in Chapter I. Design The original design of the study may be described as a pretest—posttest control group design (60). With this design, a pretest is used to indicate the level of proficiency of a student, in a course of study, at the onset of the course. The pretest score is then used as a control variable in ana— lyzing the student's posttest score with the analysis of covariance. After observing the poor correlation (r = 0.19) between pretest and final examination scores, it was concluded that the performance of a student on the_EN 200 pretest was not related to his score on the final course examination. The pretest was used in the study as an indicator of a 18 19 students knowledge of selected principles of chemistry and food.preparation at the onset of the course but it was not used as a control variable in the statistical test employed. The final design of the study may be more precisely referred to as a posttest-control group design (61). Students were stratified on the basis of their College Qualification Test—Total (CQT-T) scores, then randomly assigned to two concurrently scheduled laboratory sections. The method of laboratory instruction and assignment of in— structor were determined randomly for the groups of students participating in the study fall term, 1965. The instructor and teaching method used for the student groups studied winter and spring terms, 1964, were established to replicate the fall term design for the 10—12 class time. Each term, students taught by the two methods of labora— tory instruction attended common lectures and were compared by two criteria. These criteria were, attitude toward FN 200 as determined by their score on the Student Evaluation of EN 200 form and achievement in the course, as reflected in their score on the FN 200 Final Examination. A summary of the teaching methods, instructors and time distribution of the design are presented in Table 1. 20 Table 1. Distribution of Instructors and Methods of Laboratory Instruction for Three School Terms and Two Class Times Instructor School Terms Class Time Method of Instruction A Fall, 1965 10—12 Student-Instructor Demon— stration A Fall, 1965 2—4 Individual B Fall, 1965 10—12 Individual B Fall, 1965 2-4 Student—Instructor Demon— stration A Winter, 1964 10—12 Student—Instructor Demon— stration B Winter, 1964 10—12 Individual A Spring, 1964 10—12 Student—Instructor Demon— stration B Spring, 1964 10—12 Individual Procedure The Sample* Samples, consisting of 48 students, were selected from the total course enrollment of FN 200 fall term, 1965. All students enrolling in a laboratory section for whom CQT—T scores were available were originally selected. This group consisted of 58 students. Students were randomly deleted *The patience of the reader is requested when reference is made to the term "sample." In some contexts, the term refers to 104, 102, 88, 48, 40, 6, or 5. When the sample size is larger than five or six, the term will be used in the plural. 21 from the study in the larger class sections to provide sections of equal numbers of students (six above and six below median) for statistical treatment. This adjustment, to numerically equate laboratory sections, resulted in samples of 48 students. Samples, consisting of 40 students, were selected from the total course enrollment of EN 200 winter and spring terms, 1964. All students enrolling inna laboratory section which met concurrently with another section, and for whom CQT—T scores were available, were originally selected. This group consisted of 46 students. Students were randomly deleted from the study in the larger class sections to pro- vide sections of equal numbers of students (five above and five below median) for statistical treatment. This adjust— ment, to numerically equate laboratory sections, resulted in samples of 40 students. Assignment to Laboratory Sections Prior to randomization, the CQT—T median score was determined for students enrolled for a given school term. Students were randomly assigned to either the two concurrently scheduled laboratory sections in a manner which assured an equal number of above and below median CQT—T score students in each laboratory section. The name of each student was written on a separate slip of paper which was folded and placed in a container designated as above median or below median CQT—T score. A random selection of folded papers was 22 made from the containers to determine the composition of each laboratory section. The two laboratory sections, scheduled to meet at the same time of day, were designated to be taught by the "Individual" and "Student—Instructor Demonstration" method by the flip of a coin. The assignment of instructors to labora— tory sections was also determined by the flip of a coin for fall term. The instructors and teaching methods for winter and spring terms were assigned to provide a replication of the fall term design. Treatments Laboratories—~Each student was provided with a labora— tory manual (62,65) specifically designed for the laboratory situation to which he was assigned (Individual or Student— Instructor Demonstration). The matched laboratory lessons, outlined in the two manuals, were governed by common course objectives and identical review questions were at the end of each laboratory lesson. The manuals differed only in their instructional approach to meeting the objectives out— lined for each laboratory session. The content of each manual had been pretested with previous EN 200 classes to insure adequacy of directions and appropriateness of the experiments. The laboratory manuals were co-authored by Barbara B. Deskins and this writer. The Laboratory outlines are presented in the Appendix. 25 The physical arrangement of the two laboratory rooms was similar but not identical. Both classrooms were equipped with individual.work units for twenty students. In neither laboratory were facilities excellent for conducting the Instructor Demonstrations. The Classrooms were equally accessible to supply and storage rooms. The Instructor Demonstration laboratories required ad- vance preparation which was unnecessary for the Student Demonstration and Individual laboratories. A graduate student assisted in the advance preparation required for the Instructor Demonstration sessions and class members shared in the re- sponsibility of post-laboratory ”clean-up”. On the nine occasions when the instructor performed a demonstration in the Student-Instructor Demonstration laboratories, class time was reduced from the usual two hour period to approximately one to one and one-half hours. Instructors-—The two instructors, who shared equally in the lecture and laboratory instruction in FN 200 throughout the study, had similar previous experience in teaching food preparation courses. Pertinent factors in the academic background of each instructor were as follows: Instructor A - B.S. degree in Nutrition from Michigan State University in 1951. M.S. degree in Nutri— tion from the State University of Iowa in 1955. Completion of a minor in Food Science as a partial requirement for a Ph.D. degree at Michi- gan State University. Instructor B - B.S. degree in Food from Cornell Uni- versity in 1951. M.S. degree in Foods from the University of Wisconsin in 1955. 24 .Examinations--Two one-hour examinations, using objective questions, were administered in the lecture portion of the course each term. Although the examinations covered the same areas of content each term, the questions differed. Each term four fifteen— to twenty-minute laboratory quizzes were given. The discussion questions used for the quizzes dif; fered each term but covered identical areas of course content. The same final examination was used throughout the study and will be reported fully in the section of this chapter devoted to instruments of measurement. Additional Control Measures-—To control all phases of the study, other than the variables compared, the following measures were observed: 1. At the first course meeting, students were informed that they were participating in a study "aimed at determining the most effective method for teaching the course." This procedure was followed to allow any student bias, related to being a participant in the study, to be equally dispersed throughout the groups. The specific nature of the study was not described to students, but they were informed that the methods being studied had been tested with other classes and would not place any students in a disadvantaged position in the course or in their professional training. 2. The two instructors responsible for the laboratory instruction shared equally, and in the same sequence, in the preparation and delivery of course lectures. Both instructors were closely related to the de— velopment of the Student-Instructor Demonstration Method of presenting the laboratory portion of FN 200. They shared the responsibilities of develop— ing the laboratory manuals, the quizzes and hour examinations throughout the study. 5. Mimeographed course outlines and reading lists were in identical sequence for the three terms of study. These materials were distributed to students at the first class meeting of each school term. 25 4. The same textbook was used throughout the study. 01 The number of class meeting times per term was held constant during the study. 6. Hour examinations and laboratory quizzes were given at the same interval in the course each of the three terms. 7. The same two foods laboratories and lecture room were used. 8. Neither of the instructors participating in the study was absent from a scheduled laboratory or lecture meeting of the class. to Laboratories and lectures were held on the same days of the week throughout the study. The time of day when lectures were held did differ each term (1pm, 11am, 10am, fall, winter, spring respectively). Laboratories were held at either 10—12 noon or 2=4 pm. 10. The school terms studied were consecutive thus mini— mizing differences in the sample that could have been possible due to differences in University entrance requirements. 11 An equal number of above and below median College Qualification Test-Total score students were assigned to each of the concurrently scheduled laboratories. This procedure was intended to control any influence the general ability level of the class may have had on individual student performance in the course. Instruments of Measurement The instruments, described in this section, were designed and/or selected on the basis of the two hypotheses associated with the study. A secondary, but very important, purpose for the instruments was to provide the instructors who taught EN 200 with a better understanding of the competence and potential of the students as well as providing a partial basis for evaluating their performance in the course. 26 College Qualification Test (CQT) Since 1957, the present form of the COT (64) has been administered to all freshmen and transfer students entering Michigan State University as an indicator of general scholas— tic ability. The 80—minute test measures verbal ability and numerical reasoning as well as information from a broad range of subject matter areas. The reliability of the test is reported (65) to be in the mid 0.90's for the total score and from the high 0.70's to the low 0.90“s for the subtests. The COT—Total (CQT—T) scores are assumed to provide a more adequate estimate of student ability than grade point average due to the variation in the class level of students comprising the samples (66). EN 200 Pretest and Questionnaire, Theoretically, the most precise method of measuring the achievement of a student during his enrollment in a course, is to compare his scores on a single, highly reliable and valid instrument at the beginning and completion of the course. Smith and Tyler (67) support this theory with the statement: Practice effects of taking a test once will probably not be a serious factor influencing the scores on a second administration of the test several months later. They did not support their theory with evidence from research findings. Their theory was rejected in the study being re— ported here for the following reasons: 1. 27 The ten—week period between pretest and final exami- nation provided a very short interval for any prac- tice effects from completing the pretest to be eradicated uniformly among participating students. The use of an identical final examination throughout the study was considered a necessary control measure. Possible communication between students concerning the identical nature of the pretest and final exami- nation could have lowered the "security" on the final examination. Smith and Tyler suggested (68) that a pretest should be based on basic concepts and principles without unduly discouraging the student with unfamiliar terminology or formulation. The comprehension and application of principles appropriate for use at the completion of FN 200 may have discouraged or stimu— lated some students. In either case, differences may have resulted which were attributable to the pre- test rather than the method of laboratory instruction. The considerations important in the construction of the EN 200 Pretest were: 1. The questions should be based on principles of chem— istry and food preparation considered to be of im— portance in FN 200. The pretest should be brief enough to administer at the introductory lecture in the course and prior to any formal course instruction. The pretest should provide an estimate of students knowledge of selected principles of chemistry and food preparation at the onset of the course. A nineteen~item multiple-choice pretest was constructed to meet the above considerations. A copy of the EN 200 Pretest and Questionnaire is available in the Appendix. In addition to sampling the proficiency of students in Specific areas of chemistry and food preparation, the pre- test was designed to serve as a supplementary means of intro— ducing students to the nature of the course. The total class 28 performance on the pretest provided the course instructors with information concerning general areas of strengths and weaknesses of student knowledge of selected principles of food preparation and chemistry. The latter was useful in selecting more appropriate levels of instruction for several portions of the course. The individual student scores on the pretest were not studied or recorded until the final course grades for stu— dents were submitted. This prevented instructor bias toward students with high or low pretest scores. The questionnaire portion of this instrument was con— structed to obtain the following information from each student enrolled in EN 200: name of student student number grade level specific college major previous food preparation experience The previous food preparation experience, indicated by stu— dents completing the questionnaire, was difficult to evaluate. The type and duration of experience in: high school, 4H-Club, armed forces, restaurant, hotel, resort, as well as other areas--for which the student was requested to list the specific experience, was recorded. A student was classified as having previous food preparation experience if he listed one or more months of experience in any of the areas specified above. 29 Student Evaluation of FN 200 The two considerations in constructing the Student Evaluation of EN 200 form were: 1. To provide an instrument for measuring student attitude toward FN 200. 2. To provide one means of identifying strengths and weaknesses in the course which have been useful in the on-going re—evaluation of EN 200 course content. To meet these objectives, a sixteen item rating scale was constructed after examining several rating scale for— mats (69,70,71). A copy of the evaluation form used in this study is available in the Appendix. The instrument also contained four "open—end” questions which were designed to obtain specific comments from students concerning the best liked and least liked portions of the course lecture and laboratory. Lehmann (72) encourages the use of student rating scales of attitude toward a course as a method of evaluating in— struction. Although he discusses several types of rating scales, he indicates no preference for one format of con— struction over the others he describes. The rationale for measuring student attitude toward the course was discussed earlier. The Student Evaluation of EN 200 form was administered to each laboratory group in the next to the last laboratory session each term. The form was completed by students and returned to a box in the laboratory at the end of the laboratory period. The lesson presented concurrent to the 50 completion of the evaluation form provided ample time for this assignment. Students were requested to express their true opinions and not sign their names to the evaluation form. To make possible a comparison of the responses of above and below median ability students, students were requested to list a code letter on their evaluation form. The name and code letter of each student in a laboratory section was posted for the class period when evaluation forms were filled out. The following code was used: x = above median CQT-T score students y = below median CQT-T score students 2 = students for whom CQT—T scores were unavailable. The possibility of a student attaching an incorrect code letter to his evaluation form was considered less serious than the bias which may have resulted by having the student identify the appraisal form with his name. FN 200 Final Examination A 156 question, objective, comprehensive examination was prepared by four members of The Foods and Nutrition Department at Michigan State University. The four authors were teaching EN 200 at the time they prepared the exami- nation. This writer was not one of the authors of the examination. The examination was composed of fill—in—the-blank, matching, and multiple—choice questions. The questions were 51 subjected to an item analysis for three school terms prior to being used in this study. All questions demonstrating a negative discriminating power were deleted or improved during this pretesting period. The content validity of a test refers to the adequacy of its items, or parts, in sampling a specific domain of content and behavior (75). The use of four specialists in the area of foods instruction as authors of the EN 200 final examination assisted in providing the test with content validity. The breadth and depth of items comprising the examination were determined independently by two instructors familiar with the objectives and total content of the course. These data are recorded in Table 2. With the exception of the rather large number of questions dealing with leavening, the distribution of questions for each area of course con— tent reflects the emphasis given to each area in the body of the course. The classification of questions according to four levels of learning (knowledge, comprehension, appli— cation and analysis) is thought by this author, to be repre— sentative of levels of learning necessary for the achieve— ment of the course objectives described in Chapter I. The degree to which students have achieved the objectives of the course should therefore be indicated by their scores on the final examination. Table 2. 52 Specifications of the EN 200 Final Examination Numbers of Examination Questions Course Content Knowl- Compre- Appli- Analy— edge hension cation sis Total Meat, Poultry, Fish 11 10 9 0 50 Milk and Cheese 5 5 1 0 9 Eggs 6 6 0 0 12 Baked Products 7 6 5 0 18 Leavening 12 4 2 0 18 Fats and Oils 7 6 4 1 18 Sugar 2 1 2 0 5 Starch 11 6 5 1 21 Fruits and Vegetables 9 5 1 1 16 Beverages 6 1 0 0 7 Food Sanitation 0 0 1 0 1 Measuring 0 0 0 1 1 Total 74 50 28 4 156 Percent (47.4) (52.1) (17.9) (2.6) (100) Summary The posttest control—group design used in this study has been described. Replication of this design was carried out for two consecutive school terms on samples of FN 200 students. This chapter has also described the control measures employed throughout the study as well as the prom cedures for assigning students to laboratories. Laboratories, instructors, and instruments used for testing the hypotheses of the study have also been described. Chapter IV RESULTS AND DISCUSSION The testing of the hypotheses concerning the effective- ness of two methods of laboratory instruction, as stated in Chapter I, constituted the major purpose of this study. The present chapter reports the methods used in testing these hypotheses as well as the results of the tests and a discussion of these results. Preliminary results, describ- ing the student sample and instruments used, are also pre- sented and discussed. Preliminary Results Sample For the duration of the study, records were kept of each of the 88 students comprising the samples. The follow— ing information was recorded for each student on separate pages of a bound notebook: Name Student Number CQT—T Score FN 200 Pretest Score EN 200 Final Examination Score Specific Major Previous Food Preparation Experience Sex Terms of Enrollment 55 54 FN 200 Laboratory Section Name of Laboratory Instructor Class Level Grades in the Following Courses Taken at Michigan State University: American Thought and Language (ATL 111, 112, Naturilsécience (NS 181, 182, 185) Introductory Chemistry (Cem 101, 102, 105) Some characteristics of the student samples are recorded in Table 5. The randomization procedures used appear to have been effective in attaining an equitable distribution of college majors, sex, students with previous food preparation experience. The differences in mean grade point averages for the two groups in the selected courses must be attributable to chance factors. There was a higher proportion of senior class level students in the fall term group than in the winter—spring term group (16 vs. 4). This may be explained, in part, by the fact that FN 200 is a prerequisite for a second foods course for the large majority of students enrolling in the former. If a student reached senior classification prior to enrolling in FN 200, it would simply be a case of good schedule planning to complete this course early in the senior year. The close, positive correlation between major and sex may be explained by the predominance of female stu— dents in Home Economics and the predominance of male students in Business Administration. ‘ Ao.enavewumasnmo .mm ommm .HHH HwDQMSO CH cw>Hm mum moocwflungm mo mQOHpmHHUme meB Suemnm>flcb weepm cmmflnoflz um mwmnsoo ommnu Uwuwamfiou 0:3 mucmpoum Sago A.¢.m.ov ommuo>< DQHQN oUMHuo .wwHDOU mflu. CH UQOEHHOHQO ou Hoaum mocoHHoQXo mo whoa Ho QDGOE moo msflpnomou unconoum How Umpnooou mm3 :mmhz < "wocoflummxm mpoom mDOH>me .moum hoswm HOSME m UonHUmU pow Doc Um: 053 wuchSDm HDOM pan ucmndmumom pom .QOHDMOSUM mUHEocoom wEom CH HonmE wco UOUDHUCH msoum mHQB .unofiommcmz .ucmemmcmz Hmuflmmom tam HMCOHDSUHuwGH ~HVHH 05 Q “omeuo Immmcmz QSHO one .Houoz .HouomIIGH muonmE popoaocfl moonm mflnfi "QOHumuumHQHEU< mmocwmsm .noumomwm soapflnusz paw mooom use .mooom .mUHDoDoHQIIcH mnoflmE UDUDHUQH moonm mHSB ”moHEocoom oEoE uwmoaaoom NN.N mm.m mm.m m Sm we om Sm am mm MN m mm SH Hmuoa mm.N wm.m «m.m m we mm w Sfi ma om oa m mm S mcfluamu Iuopcaz . mponpwz Swom am.m mm.m mm.m 0 mm 0N ma om ma mm Na 0 mm oa Hamm . IIHMDOB wfi.m mm.m HN.N a ma mm oa we we mm m a mm S Hmuoe mH.N mm.N mfi.m a S we m S Ofi ma m a me e mcflumm I ,Immucflz Sfi.m mw.m mm.m 0 ea ma S fie m ON a 0 am m Hamm Hm5©H>HUQH Sm.m Sm.m mm.N N we mm OH we we am we a om oa HMDOB coaumupm om.m mm.m mN.N N m oa a OH m we m a ma m moflumw . IcoEwQ Iuoucflz HODUSHDmCH mN.N mm.m mw.m 0 ea ma m m m ma m 0 Sa S Hamm IDQoUSDm Emu mz 99¢ mom 02 02 wow .Hm .Hb .0m 2 m Hwauo .EU¢ .om Emma COHDUSHpmcH Ummmusoo Q.mxm mpoom w50H>on Ho>oq mmmHO xmm .msm oEom Hoogom mo ponuoz sz .¢.m.w MAHOSMEV omoaaoo Umatdum moHQEmm Dempspm orp mo moapmflnouomumnu popooamm .m wanna 56 Instruments Each of the following instruments has been described in Chapter III. The function of the following portion of this chapter is to report the results of tests the data collected from the instruments were subjected to, as well as the gross results obtained by administering the instruments to the student samples. College Qualification Test—Total Score (CQT-T) Median and range CQT—T scores for the four groups of students, for which scores were available (N = 104), are presented in Table 4. Table 4. Medians and Ranges of CQT-T Scores of the Four Groups from Which the Student Samples Were Selected in a Study of Two Methods of Laboratory Instruction in FN 200. Number of CQT—T Scores Term Time Students Median Range Fall, 1965 10—12 noon 29 154 97~175 Fall, 1965 2~4 pm 29 156 .88~167 Winter, 1964 10—12 noon 21 159 94-165 Spring, 1964 10-12 noon 25 155 99—165 Total 104 The median scores appear to be homogeneous although no statistical procedure was used to verify this observation. When the median CQT-T scores for FN 200 students were com— pared to the fiftieth percentile scores (74) of the total 57 freshmen classes of which the majority of these students were members, these FN.200 students appeared to have slightly higher numerical.scores than the students with whom they entered the University. The samples of FN 200 students were potentially as "able” as the students comprising the total freshmen classes with whom they entered the University when CQT-T scores were used as criterion. The means and standard deviations of the CQT—T scores of the student samples of 88, randomly drawn from the group of 104, are recorded in Table 5. Although the numerical differences are slight, and presumably due entirely to the randomization procedure used, it is interesting to note that slightly more able students may have been drawn into the Student—Instructor Demonstration Laboratories, fall term, 1965 while the reverse situation took place during winter-spring terms, 1964. When the method of laboratory instruction is disregarded, it appears that the sample of students Spring term may have been slightly more able than students studied fall and winter terms. One facet of this study was observing any differences in student performance and attitude toward EN 200 attributr able to the potential ability of students as indicated by their CQT—T scores. The means and standard deviations of the students classified as above or below median ability (using CQT-T scores as criterion) are recorded in Table 6. From these data, the groups of students studied spring term 58 Table 5. Means and Standard Deviations of CQT—T Scores of the Student Samples Studied Method of School Number of CQT—T Scores Instruction Term Students Mean S.D. Student— Fall 24 157.4 18.78 Instructor Demonstration Winter- Spring 20 151.9 18.25 Total 44 154.6 18.54 Individual Fall 24 128.7 19.97 Winter— Spring 20 157.6 19.05 Total 44 155.1 19.85 Total Both Fall 48 155.1 19.41 Methods Winter 20 155.1 19.44 Spring 20 156.1 12.90 Total 88 155.9 19.20 appear to be more homogeneous than the groups studied fall and winter terms. FN 200 Pretest and Questionnaire A frequency distribution of student scores on the FN 200 Pretest are presented in Table 7. In the original design of the study, this writer postulated that the performance of students on a pretest, given prior to any formal instruction in the course, would measure their knowledge of selected 9 5 .mQOHDMH>mU Unmpcmum ohm monogpamumm CH mHGQEDZU .mucmpspm m Umnflmucoo mdoum 30mm Q .mpcmpdpm w posemucoo msoum sowmm mm Hence Amm.mvm.mma xom.mavm.mae Aam.eavo.maa Amo.mvs.mea ea cmflemz Boamm meoeums euom Aao.mvs.mea Amfi.mva.ema Amm.aavm.ama ASm.mvm.mea we cmflemz m>on< Amuoe we Hmuoe Amm.m VN.SNA Amm.mava.mfia Amm.muvm.aoa AeN.mvS.mafi emaemz onmm lam.0Hve.mefi Aom.mve.mma Amm.oevs;mea Amm.mvm.seu anaemz m>ona Hmsefl>flecH we Hmuoe Aom.mvm.mmu Amw.mavm.m0H Aem.muvm.mea Amm.mvS.omfi cmflems onmm aOADAAumeosmo Aam.avo.mefi Amm.Svm.mae Amm.mavm.amuuxSm.mvm.omfi cmflemz m>ona AooosuomeH upcmesum .Q.m com: .Q.mr and: .Q.m cmmz .Q.m smog mpchSDm ouoom coepusuumcH coon NHION zoos mfiuoa em «IN coon NAIOA mo 9.506 No eoeumz gamma .meflumm emmfi .Amucas enema «game Amnssz Q mpcmospm CMHUGZ Bonm paw m>ofl< How monoom BIBOO mo mQOHDMH>oQ onmncmum Ugo mcmoz mmEHB mcfluomz wmmHO 039 Ugo mEHmB Hoonom mmHSB Mom .@ OHQMB 40 Table 7. Frequency Distribution of EN 200 Pretest Scores Score Number of Students 14 1 15 6 12 10 11 9 10 9 9 17 8 11 7 14 6 8 5 7 4 8 5 0 2 1 1 1 Total 102 Mean 8.42 Standard Deviation 2.79 Reliability (Kuder—Richardson #20) = 0.54 principles of chemistry and food preparation at the onset of the course. It was assumed that high or low pretest scores would correlate with high or low final examination scores and that pretest scores could, therefore, serve as a control variable in the statistical analysis. When the pretest and final examination scores were compared by Pearson's Product Moment Coefficient of Linear Correlation (75), a correlation of 0.19 was found between the two tests. A further study of the pretest revealed a reli— ability (Kuder—Richardson #20) of 0.54 while the reliability at 41 of the final examination [Cronbach (76), Coefficient Alpha] was 0.88. The relatively low reliability of the pretest and the low correlation between the tests indicated that using the pretest scores as a control variable in the final examination analysis would be pointless. The main function of the pretest throughout the study was as an indicator of students' proficiency in selected principles thought to be of importance in food preparation. The questionnaire attached to the pretest provided the major portion of information, reported on Table 5, concerning selected characteristics of the student sample. These data have been discussed earlier in Chapter 4. Student Evaluation of EN 200 A group of 99 students, from the total of 104 students for which CQT—T scores were available, submitted evaluation forms. Evaluation forms were randomly eliminated from larger laboratory sections to provide an equal number of evaluations from each laboratory group for a given statisti— cal test. The sixteen questions which formed the rating scale portion of the instrument were graded to determine a numerical score for each evaluation form. The four cate— gories provided for reSponding to the rating scale items were weighted as follows: 42 Category Marked Points Awarded Greatly Moderately Little None l—‘NCNIP The reliability (77) of the rating scale was calcu- lated to be 0.88, indicating that the scale was consistent in its measurement. The mean score for the evaluation was 51.8 with a range of scores from 50-61 (out of a possible range of 16—64). A frequency distribution of student scores on the evaluation form rating scale is presented in Table 8. The content validity is evaluated as satisfactory by this writer because the items on the rating scale are di— rectly related to the specific objectives outlined for FN 200. A copy of the Student Evaluation of EN 200 form will be found in the Appendix. The four open-end questions, which formed a portion of the questionnaire, will be dis— cussed later. FN 200 Final Examination The 104 examination papers, of all students for whom CQT-T scores were available, were subjected to a test of reliability formulated by Cronbach (78). The computed reliability was 0.88. Anastasi (79) stresses the fact that any reliability coefficient derived from a single administration of a single form of a test is more precisely designated as a coefficient of internal consistency. 45 Table 8. Frequency Distribution of Student Evaluation of FN 200 Scores (N = 88) Score Range Number of Students 60—64 1 55-59 26 50—54 59 45—49 15 40—44 5 55—59 0 50—54 2 Mean 51.8 Standard Deviation 5.08 Reliability (Coefficient Alpha) 0.88 A reliability coefficient of 0.88 indicates that the final examination used in this study was consistent in its measure— ment. Feldt (80) reports the most valid method for conducting an item analysis consists of selecting the examination papers having the highest 27% and the papers having the lowest 27% of the scores and analyzing them using a procedure described by Truman (81). This procedure was followed. The mean percentage of correct responses by both high and low groups was used to determine the difficulty level for each question (82). The discrimination index for each question was de— termined by using Flanagan's Coefficient Table (85). The frequency distributions of item difficulty and discrimination will be found respectively in Tables 9 and 10. 44 Table 9. Distribution of Item Difficulty for the EN 200 Final Examination Percentage of Students Number of Answering Correctly Items 90-100 56 80—89 45 70-79 28 60-69 19 50—59 14 40—49 6 50—59 6 20—29 4 Total 156 Mean 74.98 Table 10. Distribution of Item Discrimination Indices for the EN 200 Final Examination Discrimination Indexa Number of Items .70—.79 2 .60—.69 10 .50—.59 14 .40—;49 24 .50—.59 51 .20—.29 21 .10—.19 26 0—.09 19 —.01— —.10 5 —.11— —.20 5 —.51— -.40 1 Total 156 Mean 0.29 aComputed from Flanagan’s Coefficient Table in Walker, H. M. and J. Lev Statistical Inference. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston (1955) Table XIII, pp. 472—75. 45 The mean difficulty level for items used on the final examination was 74.98% correct responses. Saupe (84) reported that items with a difficulty level of fifty percent are more precise than items of a higher or lower difficulty level in estimating the relative standing on the character— istics tested. The high reliability coefficient (0.88) for FN 200 Final Examination would indicate that, in spite of questions which were comparatively easy for students to answer correctly, the test was consistent in its measure— ment. Tolerance of the instrument for a rather high proportion of "easy” questions, without a consequent loss of reliability, may be directly related to the large number of items used. The mean index of discrimination for the items on the final examination was 0.29, with 65.4% of the items having an index above 0.19. In a discussion of the discrimination indices of test items, Ebel (85) has proposed the following classification: Index of Discrimination Item Evaluation 0.40 and greater very good 0.50—0.59 reasonably good but subject to improvement 0.20—0.29 marginal items below 0.19 poor items Ebel also suggests that if low discrimination items are not due to technical weaknesses, and if there are good reasons for including them, they may be used. In future revisions of the EN 200 Final Examination, the number of questions 46 with a discrimination index of under 0.19 obviously must be re—evaluated. However, their low discrimination indices did not seriously detract from the reliability of the test. A frequency distribution of FN 200 Final Examination scores is reported in Table 11. The mean score of the student samples on the examination was 267.5 (77.02% cor— rect). Scores ranged from 200 to 514. The highest possible score on the instrument was 547. Table 11. Frequency Distribution of FN 200 Final Examination Scores (N = 88). Interval Number of Score Students 511—520 1 501—510 5 291-500 4 281-290 12 271—280 18 261—270 16 251~260 10 241—250 12 251—240 7 221-250 2 211-220 0 200~210 1 Total 88 Mean 267.5 Standard Deviation 21.1 Reliability (Coefficient Alpha) 0.88 Although the validity of FN 200 final examination can- not be expressed in terms as tangible as its reliability, 47 its content validity was considered satisfactory for three reasons. First, the examination represented the combined efforts and judgment of four specialists in the area of teaching college food preparation courses. Second, the examination was pretested and revised for three school terms prior to its use in the study to eliminate negatively discriminating and/or ambiguous questions, as described in Chapter 5. The third support for evaluating the content validity of the examination as satisfactory is reflected in its specifications as summarized in Table 2. The classi— fication of the examination questions has been discussed in Chapter 5. Statistical Tests Employed The hypotheses, concerning differences in learning out— comes associated with methods of laboratory instruction, were tested by means of an analysis of variance factorial design (86). A five percent (0.05) critical region was used in testing each hypothesis. The assumption necessary for the use of the analysis of variance technique [normal population with equal variance (87)] was thought to be valid. The randomizing procedure presumably produced groups of equivalent normalcy. The ”Fmax” test (88), designed to determine whether equivalence of variance between groups exists, was employed. Results of this test indicated that, at the five percent level of significance, the groups were equivalent in variance. 48 The First Hypothesis of the Study was: A "Student—Instructor Demonstration Method” is at least as effective as an "Individual Method” of laboratory instruction in achieving common course objectives in a beginning foods preparation course for students of both above and below median general ability. Indication of achievement of common course objectives was determined by the scores of students on the FN 200 Final Examination. Students were classified as above or below median in ability, on the basis of their CQT—T scores, in relation to the median score for their comparison group. An analysis of variance of the final examination scores was completed for the fall term samples. These data are reported in Table 12. The analysis of variance data for the winter—spring term samples are reported in Table 15. The results of these statistical tests indicate no signifi— cant differences in student achievement in EN 200 as a result of the two methods of laboratory instruction em— ployed. The lack of significant difference in final examination scores among students of above and below median ability fall term (Table 12) was unexpected. Although the randomization procedure could be expected to disperse the above and below median students equitably throughout the groups, the small number of students in each group may have resulted in a less than equitable distribution. This may account for the lack of significant differences in final examination scores among the above and below ability students fall term. 49 Table 12. Analysis of Variance of EN 200 Final Examination Scores as Influenced by Time of Day, Ability Level, Method of Instruction and Instructor. Fall Term 1965 Degrees of Sum of Mean F a Source Freedom Squares Square Ratio F 95 Ability 1 867.00 867.00 1.469 N.S.b Time 1 0.09 0.09 < 1.00 N.S. Instructor 1 541.54 541.54 < 1.00 N.S. Method 1 850.09 850.09 1.440 N.S Ability x Time 1 596.74 596.74 < 1.00 N.S. Ability x Instructor 1 1.55 1.55 < 1.00 N.S. Ability x Method 1 1140.75 1140.75 1.907 N.S. Within (error) 40 25614.55 590.56 Total 47 27211.67 aF (1 40) = 4 08 .95 ’ ' bN.S. = Not significant (5% level) Table 15. Analysis of Variance of FN 200 Final Examination Scores as Influenced by Ability Level, School Term and Method of Instruction — Instructor. Winter and Spring Terms, 1964 Degrees of Sum of Mean F a Source Freedom Squares Square Ratio F 95 Ability 1 2608.22 2608.22 10.68 S.b Term 1 15.22 15.22 < 1.00 N.S.C Method-Instructor 1 15.22 15.22 < 1.00 N.S. Ability x Term 1 172.25 172.25 < 1.00 N.S. Ability x Method- Instructor 1 442.25 ,442.25 1.81 N.S. Method—Instructor x Term 1 508.05 508.05 1.26 N.S. Ability x Term x Method-Instructor 1 70.22 70.22 < 1.00 N.S. Within (error) 52 7817.60 244.50 Total 59 11444.97 aF 95 (1,30) = 4.17 bS. = Significantly different (5% level) CN.S. = Not significant (5% level) 50 Group means of the final examination scores, for the samples studied fall term, are recorded in Table 14. Similar data for the samples studied winter—spring terms, 1964 are recorded in Table 15. There was a slight numerical superiority in total examination scores for the laboratory sections instructed by the Student—Instructor Demonstration method, fall term, 1965. Any difference, attributable to teaching method, during winter and spring terms, 1964 appear numerically negligible. Determining whether observed numeri- cal differences were due to method or the student samples was impossible. However, numerical differences were not significant when these data were subjected to the analysis of variance technique (Tables 12 and 15). The Second Hypotheses of the Study was: Student attitudes toward a "Student—Instructor Demon— stration Method" are at least as favorable as are student attitudes toward an "Individual Method” of instruction in the laboratory portion of a beginning food preparation course. Student scores on the rating scale portion of the Student Evaluation of FN 200 were used as indicators of student atti— tude toward the course. An analysis of variance of these scores was completed for the fall term samples and is re- ported in Table 16. The analysis of variance of the evalu— ation scores for the winter—spring term samples are reported in Table 17. The results of the statistical tests employed indicate no significant differences in student attitude toward EN 200 as a result of the method of laboratory in— struction employed. 51 Table 14. The Means of the EN 200 Final Examination Scores for the Student Samples Studied Fall Term, 1965 Method of Above Median Below Median Instruction Students Students Total Instructor Student- A Instructor Demonstration a (A.M.) 268.2(6) 274.8(6) 271.5(12) Individual (P.M.) 275.2(6) 250.8(6) 265.0(12) Total 271.7(12) 262.8(12) 267.2(24) Instructor Student— B Instructor Demonstration (P.M.) 268.2(6) 264.0(6) 266.1(12) Individual (A.M.) 265.8(6) 251.7(6) 257.8(12) Total 266.0(12) 257.8(12) 261.9(24) Sub—Totals A.M. 267.0(12) 265.2(12) 265.1(24) P.M. 271.7(12) 257.4(12) 264.5(24) Student— Instructor Demonstration 268.2(12) 269.4(12) 268.8(24) Individual 269.5(12) 251.2(12) 260.5(24) Grand Total 268.9(24) 260.5(24) 264.6(48) aNumbers in parentheses indicate the number of student scores comprising the mean. Table 15. The Means of the FN 200 Final Examination Scores for the Student Samples Studied Winter and Spring Terms, 1964 Method of School Above Median Below Median Instruction Term Students Students Total Student- Winter 279.6(5)a 265.6(5) 271.6(10) Instructor Demonstration Spring 282.0(5) 252.4(5) 267.2(10) (Instructor A) Total 280.8(10) 258.0(10) 269.4(20) Individual Winter 271.2(5) 265.2(5) 267.2(10) (Instructor B) Spring 279.4(5) 268.4(5) 275.9(10) Total 275.5(10) 265.8(10) 270.0(20) Total Winter 275.4(10) 265.4(10) 269.4(20) Spring 280.7(10) 260.4(10) 270.5(20) Total 278.1(20) 261.9(20) 270.0(40) aNumbers in parentheses indicate the number of student scores comprising the mean . Group means of the rating scale scores for the fall term and winter—spring 19 respectively. Individual Method Individual Method The two lower mean scores (Table 18, of Instruction, of Instruction, Instructor A, Instructor B) were, term samples are presented in Table 18 and and Table 19, in each case, the result of a single low score rather than a group tendency. .. 55 Table 16. Analysis of Variance of Rating Scale Scores for the Student Evaluation of FN 200 Form, Fall Term, 1965 Degrees of Sums of Mean F a Source Freedom Squares Square Ratio F.95 Ability 1 46.06 46.06 2.270 N.S.b Time 1 42.25 42.25 2.081 N.S. Instructor 1 17.55 17.55 < 1.00 N.S. Method 1 46.06 46.06 2.270 N.S. Ability x Time 1 12.96 12.96 < 1.00 N.S. Ability x Instructor 1 49.19 49.19 2.424 N.S. Ability x Method 1 62.98 62.98 5.104 N.S. Within (error) 40 811.52 20.29 Total 47 1088.55 1 I aF (1,40) = 4.08 .95 bN.S. = Not significant (5% level) Table 17. Analysis of Variance of Rating Scale Scores for the Student Evaluation of EN 200 Form, Winter—Spring Terms, 1964 Degrees of Sum of Mean F a Source Freedom Squares Square Ratio F.95 Ability 1 0.02 0.02 < 1.0 N.S.b Term 1 0.22 0.22 < 1.0 N.S. Method—Instructor 1 50.62 50.62 1.05 N.S. Ability x Term 1 42.05 42.05 1.41 N.S. Ability x Method- Instructor 1 11.05 11.05 < 1.0 N.S. Term x Method 1 15.65 15.65 < 1.0 N.S. Ability x Term x Method—Instructor 1 24.02 24.02 < 1.0 N.S. Within (error) 52 950.9 29.72 Total 59 1074.57 aF (1,30) = 4.17 .95 bN.S. = Not significant (5% level) 54 Table 18. The Means of the Rating Scale Scores for the Student Evaluation of EN 200 Form for the Student Samples Studied Fall Term, 1965 Method of Above Median Below Median Instruction Students Students Total Instructor Student— A Instructor Demonstration a (A.M.) 51.6(6) 52.5(6) 51.9(12) Individual (P.M.) 44.5(6) 51.8(6) 48.1(12) Total 48.1(12) 52.1(12) 50.1(24) I Instructor Student- B Instructor Demonstration (P.M.) 52.0(6) 50.6(6) 51.5(12) Individual (A.M.) 50.6(6) 51.8(6) 51.2(12) Total 51.5(12) 51.2(12) 51.2(24) Sub—Totals A.M. 51.1(12) 52.0(12) 51.5(24) P.M. 48.2(12) 51.2(12) 49.7(24) Student— Instructor Demonstration 51.8(12) 51.4(12) 51.6(24) Individual 47.5(12) 51.8(12) 49.6(24) Grand Total 49.7(24) 51.6(24) 50.6(48) aNumbers in parentheses indicate the number of student scores comprising the mean. 55 Table 19. The Means of the Rating Scale Scores for the Student Evaluation of FN 200 Form for the Student Samples Studied Winter and Spring Terms, 1964 Method of School Above Median Below Median Instruction Term Students Students Total Student— winter 52.4(5)a 54.0(5) 53.2(10) Instructor Demonstration Spring 54.0(5) 54a6(5) 54.5(10) (Instructor A) Total 55.2(10) 54.5(10) 55.7(20) Individual Winter 51.4(5) 54.0(5) 52.7(10) j (Instructor B) I) Spring 55.6(5) 49.0(5) 51.5(10) { Total 52.5(10) 51.5(10) 52.0(20) Total Winter 51.9(10) 54.0(10) 52.9(20) Spring 55.8(10) 51.8(10) 52.8(20) Total 52.8(20) 52.9(20) 52.8(40) aNumbers in parentheses indicate the number of student scores comprising the mean. "Open—End" Questions on the Student Evaluation of FN 200 Form The responses to the four "open—end” questions on the Student Evaluation of EN 200 form are summarized in Table 20. The responses to the "open—end" questions indicate little difference attributable to method of laboratory instruction. There does appear to be a discrepancy of ”no response" to the questions which dealt with what the students liked most and least about laboratory. A total of twenty—one ICOEon HQCUCHumCHIUCooduw ou oUCoHommH UHNHUomm UmmeHUCH no MCCHQ oommm pmoH H mmCommom OZ “COHDmCmeHm ooow CHHB I90 mo SDHHMHO .UOCCoE huoumqumH HMCUH>HUCH Ho COHumnbw H COCqu mCHCUmoB auCon>H5vw wCu Ho :mCHCDOC: CUCoHHoaxm .ww>Huoofl .quuCOU mmMSOU n HmquoHHouCH mmmmHU mo mumnmmofium poMHHmHU Ho poxHH .%53 mo COHDMCmmem 0C CCH3 HODUSHumCH MO me500 waHHme no UmMHH H HmCoHuoEm ammem CDOC .moNNHsv .memHU mCHCmmB .HwCuHmm SHODCHOQCH .osao .HmHnoumE mo COHumNHCmmHO Ho CoHumngoHQ H HMOHCCUoB .SHOHCHOQCH oCm ouspomH CCOQ usonm unmoH oCm umoE UmMHH SoCu DMCS poxmm mHoS mquosum ,Q m HN OH HH w m m mmCommom OZ 0 o o N H H 0>Hpmmoz om mH mH Nm wH mH o>HuHmom HmsuooHHopCH N H H H o H o>Hummoz H H m m m m w>HuHmom HCCOHDOEM mN mH NH mN HH NH o>Hummoz H m H H H m o>Hanom HmoHcrome o o o a H m o>Humooz Bored: N H H m m o o>HpHmom mCHCOCoB SHODMHOQMH mH S w NH m w mmCommom OZ 6 5 m H a o m a o>Honooz NN MH m SH m w 0>HCHmom HmsuooHHopCH H o H N N o o>Huwmoz H m H o O o ®>HDHmom HCCOHDOEM mm mH «H mm NH mH o>Hooooz m N S NN HH HH o>HuHmom HCUHCCUoB onsuooq Hopoe CoHowE CmHUoE HMDOB CoprZ CmHowz QomCommwm OHQOB Bonm o>ofi< soHom o>ofl< HCCUH>HCCH COHumnumCoEoo MOCUSHDmCHIpCooCCm COHDUCHmeH SHODCCOQMH mo porno: ANN H Zv ownsoo mooom mCHCCHmom m CH Euom COHumon>m Cm mo COHuHom m we tomb mmCoHumoCO UCMICoQO noon on momCommom uCoooum mo SCCEESm < .ON wHQMB 57 students taught by the Individual method of Instruction did not respond, or listed "nothing" or its equivalent, to these questions compared with eight students taught by the Student-Instructor Demonstration method of laboratory in— struction. One of the twenty—one ”no responses” by students in the former group was in answer to the question: "The one thing I liked most about laboratory was?" The other twenty "no responses" by students taught by the Individual method, as well as the eight "no responses" by students taught by J) the Student—Instructor method, were in answer to the question: *- ”The one thing I liked least about laboratory was?” It might be inferred from the latter response that the students could think of nothing they disliked about laboratory. "Lack of adequate discussion time” and ”washing dishes" were the most frequently mentioned responses to: "The one thing I liked least about laboratory” by students taught by both laboratory methods. Reference was infrequently made, to the instructor demonstrations, by the students when dis- cussing either the most or least liked part of their labora— tory experience. There appears to be a definite lack of a more positive attitude, attributable to method of laboratory instruction, on the part of groups instructed by either of the two methods of laboratory instruction. This observation supports the results of statistical analyses on the rating scale portion of the Student Evaluation of EN 200 form. 58 Student Samples A summary of pertinent data concerning the student samples is presented in Table 21. The CQT—T mean scores were numerically higher for the student sample taught by the Student—Instructor Demonstration method of laboratory instruction fall term, 1965 and lower for the sample taught by this method winter-spring, 1964 than were the mean scores for students taught by the Individual method. The lowest CQT-T mean score, reported for fall term students taught by the Individual method, may partially account for their I numerically lowest FN 200 final examination mean score. The differences between the CQT-T means of above and below median ability students in a given laboratory section fluctuated (Table 6). In addition, there appeared to be less variance among the group taught by the Student— Instructor Demonstration method spring term with 21.2 CQT—T score points differentiating the above and below median averages that term. There were 42.0 points difference between the above and below median averages in the laboratory section taught by the Individual method winter term. This observation supports the premise that the randomization procedure among small groups may not have resulted in an equitable distribution of ability levels among the groups studied. Pretest It may be noted, in Table 21, that the mean pretest scores for students randomly assigned to the Individual 59 m.Hm M.SmN H.m m.MMH mCowz UCCHO m.om m.mwN m.m H.MMH C802 O.Nm S.OSN m.w N.SMH mCHHQm IHopCHB S.mH H.0mN N.m S.®NH HHom HCDUH>HUCH S.Nm H.mwN N.S ©.HMH Com: m.mm «.mwN H.S «.HMH mCHHQm IHmDCHZ COHDMHDmCOECQ CODUSCDmCH ©.Hm w.me H.S H.SMH HHmm Iprpspm wHOUm OON Zm mo wuoom COHDmCHmem muoum unwound onoom EACH COHDUDHmeH COHDCDHm>m DCoUDDm HnCHm OON Zm OON Zm BIBOU Hoonom mo UOCDGS COHponmmHm Uoom CH COHDUCCCmCH SCODCHOQCA no moonuoz 039 MO COHumnHm>m Cm CH UoHUCDm monEmm uCoUSDm 0CD No mouoom Coos mo wnmfifism N .HN oHQMB 60 method of laboratory instruction were numerically higher than the mean scores for students assigned to the Student— Instructor Demonstration method of laboratory instruction. These data suggest that, throughout the study, students assigned to the Student—Instructor Demonstration method of laboratory instruction may have had less knowledge of measures tested by the pretest than students randomly assigned to the Individual method of laboratory instruction. Numerical differences in mean pretest scores between the two methods of laboratory instruction did not appear to be paralleled by differences in previous food preparation experience or in differences in grade point averages (Table 5). These numerical differences suggest that students taught by the Student—Instructor Demonstration method, for reasons not known, had less knowledge about selected principles of chemistry and food preparation at the onset of the course than did their counterparts taught by the Individual method of laboratory instruction. The low correlation (0.19) between the pretest and final examination scores for the samples of students suggest that any differences in performance on the final examination, attributable to a slightly higher pretest score, were not likely to be responsible for differences noted in final examination scores. M This chapter has presented the results of the statistical tests of the two hypotheses concerning the effectiveness of 61 two methods of laboratory instruction in a beginning food preparation course. Descriptive characteristics of the measuring instruments and student samples were also pre— sented. Hypothesis one, dealing with student achievement on the EN 200 Final Examination, indicated no significant dif— ferences, attributable to teaching method, under the con- ditions of the test. From these results it may be inferred that the Student-Instructor Demonstration method of labora— tory instruction is as effective as the Individual method, as measured by students' achievement on the FN 200 Final Examination. Hypothesis two, concerning student attitude toward EN 200, indicated no significant differences under the con— dition of the test. From these results it may be inferred that the Student—Instructor Demonstration method of labora— tory instruction is as effective as the Individual method in producing favorable attitude toward the course among student samples studied. Chapter V SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS The study described in this dissertation was designed to evaluate the effectiveness of an experimental approach (Student-Instructor Demonstration) to the laboratory in— struction in a beginning food preparation course. Studies, appraising the effectiveness of instruction, especially in courses heavily weighted with laboratory time, are in need of careful evaluation. The Specific Hypotheses Investigated The two hypotheses tested in conjunction with this study are stated explicitly in Chapter I. The first hypothesis concerned the achievement of students of above and below median ability when subjected to two methods of laboratory instruction. The statistical tests and numerical data indicate no significant difference attributable to method of instruction or other factors investigated with the ex— ception of ability level in the winter—spring, 1964 samples. The second hypothesis dealt with the attitude of students toward their experience in EN 200 as influenced by method of laboratory instruction. Again, no significant differences attributable to method of instruction or other factors investigated were observed. 62 65 Under the conditions of the study, there were no indi— cations that the Individual method of laboratory instruction was superior to the Student-Instructor Demonstration method. These conclusions are based on the outcome of the students' achievement on the EN 200 Final Examination and the students' attitude toward the course as measured by the rating scale in the Student Evaluation of EN 200 form. The Method of the Study ‘ '4‘ Students were stratified on the basis of their CQT—T scores, then randomly assigned to two concurrently scheduled laboratory sections. The method of laboratory instruction and assignment of instructor were determined randomly for the groups of students participating in the study fall term, 1965. The instructor and teaching method used for the student groups studied winter and spring terms, 1964 were established to replicate the fall term design. A total group of 104 students originally qualified for the study. Students from the larger laboratory sections were randomly excluded from the study to produce samples of equal numbers for statistical analyses. The final samples studied totaled 88 students. Two instructors, with equivalent experience in teaching food preparation courses, shared equally in the lecture and laboratory instruction in the course throughout the study. The instructors had worked together prior to the study to 64 pretest and establish suitable laboratory experiences for both Student-Instructor Demonstration and Individual labora— tory instruction groups. The CQT-T scores were used to determine the classifi— cation of the student samples as above or below median in general ability. The EN 200 Pretest and Questionnaire pro— vided pertinent data concerning the non—numerical character- istics of the samples studied as well as a slight indication of proficiency in areas related to food preparation at the onset of the course. The Student Evaluation of FN 200 form ‘ was used as a measure of student attitude toward the course. The EN 200 Final Examination was employed as the measure of student achievement at the completion of the course. Limitations of the Study The FN 200 Final Examination was not designed to measure the course objectives specifically associated with the laboratory portion of FN 200. It is possible that the lecture material in the course related more closely to the items on the final examination than did the laboratory portion of the course. The development and use of a reliable and valid laboratory practical examination might measure the outcome of the students laboratory experience more precisely. The FN 200 Pretest, as previously discussed, appeared to relate poorly to the performance of students in EN 200. A more adequately designed pretest may provide a more precise teaching and experimental tool. 65 Larger numbers of students in each group studied would provide a more favorable situation for the effective opera— tion of the randomization procedure described earlier. The procedure would have a better opportunity to produce a truly equitable distribution of student characteristics. General Conclusions In general, this study was an attempt to gain a better understanding of the effectiveness of the laboratory portion of a beginning food preparation course. The intent of this study was to weigh carefully any attributes of an alternate approach to the instruction against the effectiveness of a traditional method of instruction. The following general conclusions appear to be justified: 1. The Student—Instructor Demonstration method of in— struction was as effective as the Individual method in student achievement, when compared statistically, using student scores on the FN 200 Final Exami— nation as criterion. 2. The Student—Instructor Demonstration method of in- struction was as effective as the Individual method in producing a favorable attitude toward FN 200, when compared statistically, using student scores on the Student Evaluation of FN 200 form as criterion. Recommendations for Future Study The design of this study did not provide for an evalu— ation of the comparative effectiveness of the Student vs. the Instructor demonstrations. It may be possible that varying the distribution of these two demonstration techniques 66 could produce results quite different from those observed in this study. The development of a pretest, which demonstrated a stronger relationship to the students' "readiness" for work in FN 200 could make possible a stratification of students in future studies, which might provide more precise statisti— cal testing than stratification according to CQT—T scores. This study was initiated in a fashion far too naive. A future study might simply attempt to establish the relationship between course objectives, course content and course evaluation procedures. This would, in itself, be no small task. General Discussion It is pertinent to compare the results of this study with those reported by others who have studied the effective— ness of methods of instruction at the college level. When individual laboratory and teacher—demonstration (lecture— demonstration) laboratory methods of instruction have been compared in recent studies in science courses (89,90,91,92), no significant differences have been reported between the two methods of instruction on the basis of the criteria used. It is interesting to contemplate the possible reasons for the lack of significant differences in measured student traits even when such different approaches to teaching, as 67 the traditional laboratory vs. a single term paper, were compared (95). If real differences, attributable solely to method of instruction, do exist, it may be possible that suitable instruments for their measurement have not yet been developed. It is also possible that the method of instruction has only a minor influence on outcome of students' experience in a course. Verification of the latter postulation would indicate that the method of instruction could be freely alternated to meet the interest of the course instructor or needs resulting from expanding student populations. I Most basic to the determination of the climate most effective for learning, is the need for a precise understand— ing of the learning process itself. This need, for under- standing the learning process, is as basic to educational practice as is the working knowledge of intermediary metabol— ism to an understanding of the function of nutrients. N I 6. \1 O 10. 11. 12. 15 14. LI TERATURE CI TED Enarson, Harold L. Innovations in higher education. J. Higher Ed. 54: 495 (1960). Eckert, Ruth E. Improvement of college teaching. J. Home Econ. 41: 750 (1955). Buxton, Claude E. College Teaching: a Psychologist's View. New York: Harcourt, Brace and Co. (1956) p. VI (Preface). Hannah, John A. Education for the jet age. J. Home Econ. 55: 745 (1965). Sweetman, Marion D. To think like home economists. J. Home Econ. 55: 7 (1961). Ayers, Irma. New directions in action. J. Home Econ. 52: 499 (1960). Hussemann (Strong), D. L. Food and nutrition teaching in ferment. J. Home Econ. 49: 94 (1957). Brown, Sara A. in Harris, Chester W. (ed.) Encyclopedia of Educational Research, 5rd. ed., New York: Macmillan Co. (1960) p. 670. Eppright, Ercel S. College teaching of foods and nutrition. J. Home Econ. 41: 595 (1955). Failing, Jean. Report of the committee on the improve- ment of instruction in Am. Assn. of Land—Grant Colleges and Univ. Proceedings, 69th Annual Convention, E. Lansing, Mich. (Nov. 1955) p. 277-78. Campbell, Donald T. and Stanley, Julian C. in Handbook of Research on Teaching, N. L. Gage (ed.) Chicago: Rand McNally and Co. (1955) p. 172. Sweetman, Marion D., loc. cit. Hussemann (Strong), D. L. loc. cit. Eppright, Ercel S., loc. cit. 68 15. 16. 17. 18. 19. 20. 21. 22. 25. 24. 25. 26. 27. 28. (1' Failing, Jean, loc. ci Enarson, Harold L., loc. cit. Schuller, H. The madness of method in higher education. J. Higher Ed. 55: 90 (1951). Coladarci, Arthur. Towards more rigorous educational research. Harvard Ed. Review 59: 5 (1960). Dressel, Paul L. and Mayhew, Lewis B. General Education: Explorations in Evaluation. Washington, D.C.: Am. Council on EduCation (1957) p. 51. Travers, Robert. A study of the relationship of psycho— logical research to educational practice, in Training Research and Education, Robert Glaser, ed. Pittsburg: U. of Pitt. Press (1962) Chapt. 17. pp. 525—58. I Good, Carter V. Colleges and universities, methods of teaching, in Encyclopedia of Educational Research, Monroe, Walter S. (ed.). New York: Macmillan Co. (1950) pp. 275—79. McKeachie, W. J. Research in teaching at the college and university level in Handbook of Research on Teaching, Gage, N. L. (ed.). Chicago: Rand McNally and Co. (1965) p. 1122. McKeachie, W. J. Procedures and techniques of teaching, in The American College: A Psychological and Social Interpretation of the Higher Learning, Sanford, Nevitt (ed.). New York: John Wiley and Sons (1962) pp. 512-64. Ibid. McKeachie, W. J., in The American College: A Psycho— logical and Social Interpretation of the Higher Learning, loc. cit. Stanley, Julian C. Controlled experimentation in the classroom. J. Experi. Ed. 55: 195 (1957). Bowers, Norman D. and Soar, Robert S. Studies of human relations in the teaching—learning process: V Final Report; Evaluation of Laboratory Human Relations Training for Classroom Teachers. Nashville, Tenn.: Vanderbilt U. Press (1961) p. 122. McKeachie, W. J. The improvement of instruction. Review of Ed. Research 55: 551 (1960). 29 50. 51. 52. 55. 54. 55. 56' 57. 58. 59. 40. 41. 42. 45. 44 45. 46. 47. 70 Campbell, Donald T. and Stanley, Julian C., loc. cit. Good, Carter V., loc. cit. Eckelberry, R. H. Editorial comment. Ed. Research Bulletin _2_5: 24 (1944). McKeachie, W. J., in Handbook of Research on Teaching, loc. cit. I McKeachie, W. J., in The American College: A Psycho— logical and Social Interpretation of the Higher Learning, loc. cit. McKeachie, W. J., in Handbook of Research on Teaching, loc. cit. McKeachie, W. J., in The American College: A Psycho— logical and Social Interpretation of the Higher Learning, loc. cit. Ibid. Stanley, Julian C., loc. cit. Tyler, Ralph W., Basic Principles of Curriculum and Instruction (Syllabus for Ed. 560). Chicago: U. of Chicago Press (1950). Blick, David J., The purpose and character of laboratory instruction. J. Chem. Ed. 55: 264 (1955). Ibid. McKeachie, W. J., in Handbook of Research on Teaching, pp, cit., p. 1144. Ibid. Kruglak, Haym, in Accent on Teaching, French, Sidney J. (ed.). New York: Harper and Bros. (1954) pp. 184-88. ' Ginsberg, Benson E. To what extent should laboratory requirements be part of science education for non—science majors? Current Issues in Higher Ed. (1960) pp. 70—72. McGrath, Earl J., et al. Toward General Education. New York: Macmillan Co. (1948) p. 96. Ginsberg, Benson E., loc. cit. Blick, David J., loc. cit. 48. 49 50. 51. 52. 55. 54. 55. 56. 57. 58. 59. 60. 61. 62. 71 Kruglak, Haym, loc. cit. Cunningham, H. A. Lecture-demonstration vs. individual laboratory method in science teaching-—a summary. Sci. Ed. 59: 70 (1946). Bradley, Robert L. Lecture Demonstration vs. Individual Laboratory Work in a Natural Science Course at Michigan State University. (Unpublished Ph.D. Dissertation, College of Education, Michigan State University, E. Lansing) 1962. Dearden, Douglas M. An evaluation of the laboratory in a college general biology course. J. Experi. Ed. 55: 241 (1960). Bloye, Amy I. and Long, Alma. An experiment in teaching food preparation to college freshman. J. Home Econ. 55; 470 (1941). Bay, May S. An Experimental Study to Coppare the Indi— vidual Laboratory and Modified Demonstration Methods for Teaching Food Prgparation. (Unpublished Masters Thesis, School of Home Economics, Michigan State College, E. Lansing) 1952. Trotter, Virginia Yapp. A Comparison of the Laboratory and the Lecture—Demonstration Methods of Teaching Survey of Food Preparation for Freshmen Home Economics Students at the University of Vermont. (Unpublished Ph.D. Disser— tation, The Ohio State University, Columbus) 1960. Bradley, Robert L., loc. cit. Dearden, Douglas M., loc. cit. Bay, May S., loc, cit. Trotter, Virginia Yapp, loc. cit. Johnson, Palmer 0. Statistical Methods in Research. New York: Prentice Hall, Inc. (1949) p. 282. Campbell, Donald T. and Stanley, Julian C., pp. cit., p. 185. Ibid., p. 195. Coleman, Mary C. and Deskins, Barbara B. A Laboratory Manual for Introductory Food Prgparation: FN 200, I (Individual method of instruction). E. Lansing: Michigan State University Mimeographing and Printing Dept. (1965). 65. 64. 65. 66. 67. 68. 69. 70. 71. 72. 75. 74. 75. 76. 77. 78. 72 Ibid., II (Student—Instructor Demonstration method of instruction). Bennett, George K., et al. College Qualification Tests. New York: The Psychological Corp. (1961). Froehlich, Gustav J., in The Fifth Mental Measurements Yearbook, Buros, Oscar K. (ed.). Highland Park, New Jersey: The Gryphon Press (1959). Entry No. 520, p. 445. Chansky, Norman M. A note on the grade point average in research. Ed. and Psyc. Measurement 54: 95 (1964). Smith, E. R. and Tyler, R. W. Appraising and Recording Student Progress. New York: McGraw—Hill Book Co., Inc. (1942) p. 155. Ibid.] pp. 238—59. , Learning Appraisal Form. Oregon State Uni— versity. Copyright, 1961. . Student Opinionnaire. Michigan State University. Undated document. Smalzried, N. T. and Remmers, H. H. A factor analysis of the Purdue rating scale for instructors. J. Ed. Psyc. 54: 565 (1945). Lehmann, I. F., in Evaluation in Higher Education, Paul Dressel (ed.). Boston: Houghton Mifflin Co. (1961) p. 357. Saupe, Joe L., in Evaluation in Higher Education, Paul Dressel (ed.). Boston: Houghton Mifflin Co. (1961) p. 447. Warrington, W. G. Report to the Board of Trustees. E. Lansing: Michigan State University, Jan. 10, 1964. Mimeographed document. Table 1, p. 5. Walker, Helen M. and Lev, Joseph, Statistical Inference. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston, Inc. (1955) pp. 255—56. Cronbach, Lee J. Coefficient Alpha and the internal structure of tests. Psychometrika 45: 297 (1951). Ibid. Ibid. 79. 80. 81. 82. 85. 84. 85. 86. 87. 88. 89. 90. 91. 92. 95. 75 Anastasi, Anne, Psychological Testing, 2nd. ed. New York: The Macmillan Co. (1961) pp. 105-54. Feldt, Leonard S. Note on the use of extreme criterion groups in item discrimination analysis. Psychometrika _55: 97 (1965). Truman, L. Kelly, The selection of upper and lower groups for the validation of test items. J. Ed. Psyc. 5Q: 17 (1959). Ebel, Robert L. Measuring Educational Achievement. New Jersey: Prentice—Hall, Inc. (1965) pp. 559-60. Walker, Helen M. and Lev, Joseph, pp. cit., Table XIII, pp. 472—75. Saupe, Joe L., pp, p4p., p. 451. I Ebel, Robert L., pp. p45., p. 564. l Walker, Helen M. and Lev, Joseph, pp. p4p., p. 551. 4545., p. 201. 4545., p. 192. Bradley, Robert L., loc. cit. Dearden, Douglas M., loc. cit. Bay, May S., loc. cit. Trotter, Virginia Yapp, loc. cit. Dearden, Douglas M., loc. cit. ‘I(|.‘ II)‘ “||r APPENDICES 74 APPENDIX A FN 200——Food Preparation Fall Term, 1965 Laboratbry Outline I Date Experiment Reading Assignment Quiz Fri. Sept. 27 Food Measurement Text pp. 74-94 and Scoring Handbook of Food Prepa— ration p. 18 Mon. Sept. 50 Crystallization Text pp. 475—486 Wed. Oct. 2 Vegetables I Text pp. 229-246 Fri. Oct. 4 Vegetables II Mon. Oct. 7 Fruits Wed. Oct. 9 Salads X Fri. Oct. 11 Starch I Text pp. 516—529 pp. 597—600 Mon. Oct. 14 Starch II Wed. Oct. 16 Effect of Heat on Protein x Fri. Oct. 18 Eggs I Text pp. 557—560 Mon. Oct. 21 Eggs II Wed. Oct. 25 Gelatin Text pp. 428-452 Fri. Oct. 25 Foams I Text pp. 560—567 Mon. Oct. 28 Foams II Text pp. 514—521 Wed. Oct. 50 Milk Cookery Text pp. 509-514 Fri. Nov. 1 Cheese Text pp. 521-528 Mon. Nov. 4 Meat, Poultry,Fish dry heat cookery Text pp. 402—416 75 76 Date Experiment Reading Assignment Quiz Wed. Nov. 6 Meat,Poultry,Fish moist heat cookery Text pp. 416—428 Fri. Nov. 8 Baked Products gluten development X Mon. Nov. 11 Baked Products function of ingre— Text pp. 545—545, dients 556—557 Wed. Nov. 15 Baked Products Text pp. 557—562, manipulation 601—602 Fri. Nov. 15 Baked Products steam leavening Mon. Nov. 18 Baked Products air leavening Wed. Nov. 20 Baked Products Text pp. 595—596, C02 leavening 605-615 (chemical) Fri. Nov. 22 Baked Products Text pp. 585—594 C02 leavening (yeast) Mon. Nov. 25 Fats—-shortening power Text pp. 554-556 Wed. Nov. 27 Special Assignment Fri. Nov. 29 Holiday Mon. Dec. 2 Fats—-emulsions + shortening power x Wed. Dec. 4 Fats——deep fat cookery Fri. Dec. 6 Laboratory Final Exam 96 Classes cancelled (President Kennedy's Funeral) 77 HNMIHHm .Qm uNoB N HH mamom mN .uoo .Coz SNMIOmm .mm meB N H mEmom mN .uoo .HHm NMHINNH .mm uNoB N CHpmHomIImemeoHCB CHououm mN .Coo .poz N mammIImuoCoNUHCB Cpronm HN .uoo .Coz ONMISmm .mm uNoB N mmmm NH .uoo .Hum N N CHououm Co Comm mo poommm NH .uoo .003 N HH roumpm HH .uoo .Coz mNmImHm .mm pNoB N H Counpm HH .uoo .Hnm N N mpmHmm m .qu .pw3 N muHsum S .uoo .uoz N HH moHCmoomo> H .uuo .Hum onIomm .CC uxoe x H moanouooo> N .600 .663 mmHImSH .mm HNdB N COHCCNHHHmCmNHO on .mem .COZ NH .m COHumHmmoum poom mo Nooanmm mCHCoom HmIHS .mm pNoB N pCm uCoEmHCmmoz poom SN.Dmom .Hum COHumuuw CoHme NHSO quECmHmw< mCHpmwm ICoEwQ- Immoum NHODMHOQCH opmo Hosomoe uCopdum HH oCHHCSO NCODCCOQCH mme .ECoB HHmm ConmHomon COOMIIOON Zm m NHQmem¢ 78 AHCHQCCN m.>poCCmN pCopHmonv pmHHmUCmo mowmmHU NuHme>HCD * uCoECmHmm< HCHoomm SN .>oz .po3 OONISmm .wmmlemm .Qm uNoB N C®3om mCHCopCOCmIImumm mN .>oz .COE Ix. HmmImmm .Qm uNoB N humoohv mCHCo>moH moo mpospoum poxwm NN .>oz .Hum MHmImom .mmmImmm .mm pNoB AHCUHECCUV mCHCo>ooH moo muonpoum poxmm ON .>oz .poB N mCHCo>ooH CHM muospoum poxmm wH .>oz .Coz N mCHCo>m®H Eoopm muosponm poxmm mH .>oz .Hum NONIHON .NwmISmm .Qm DNoB N COHDCHSQHCCE . mpospoum poxmm MH .>oz .poz Smmemm .memlmem .mm uNoB N mpCoHponCH mo COHCOCCN mpodpoum poxmm HH .>oz .Coz N N pCoEmoHo>op CopsHm muospoum poxmm w .>oz .HHm mNHImHH .mm DNoB N NHwNooo Coo: pmHoE CmHm pCm NHCHDON .Dooz w .>oz .poz mHHINOH .md uNoB N Suwxoou umoC Sup CmHm pCm NHDHCOC .pooz H .>OZ .Coz NNMIHNM .mm uNoB N omoono H .>oz .Hnm HHMIoom .oa axes x Sunrooo AHA: om .noo .663 COHDmHum COHDCH NHCO uCoECmHmm< mCHpmom ICoEoQ Immoum Snowmnofloq opmm Hmsomma qupCum poSCHuCoo I m NHQZMNNN 79 mem HCCHm NHOpmquoq m .000 .Hum N NH0MOOO Cow Q00pllmpom w .O0Q .p03 N N H030m mCHC0DHOCm + wConHCE0IImumm N .000 .C02 mooHHom om .>oz .Hum COHummpm COHumH NHDO DC0ECmHmm¢ mCHpo0m ICOE0Q Iom0nm NCODCHOQCH 0Com H0C000B CC0USDm p0DCHDCOU I m NHQmemd APPENDIX C FN 200 Pretest and Questionnaire III—B Introductory Note: The primary purpose of this pretest is to help your instructors determine which subject matter areas planned for EN 200 may need special emphasis for the mem— bers of your class. RESULTS OF THIS TEST WILL NOT BE USED IN THE DETERMINATION OF YOUR FINAL GRADE IN THE COURSE. Directions: In each of the following multiple—choice questions there is only ppp correct answer listed. Record your answers on the scor— ing sheet provided. If you are not sure or do not know the answer to a specific question, indicate it by marking number 5 (Do Not Know) on your scoring sheet. Be sure to record only one answer for each question. When you have completed the test, check your scoring sheet to be sure you have recorded a total of 19 answers. 1. The commercial process used for converting edible oils into solid fats is known as: 1. Hydrogenation 2 Oxidation 5. Hydrolysis 4. Saponification 5. Do not know. 2. Which of the following is an example of a disaccharide 1. Glucose 2. Cellulose 5. Galactose 4. Sucrose 5. Do not know. 5. When organic acids combine with glycerol to form glycerol esters, the resulting product is generally referred to as: 1. A starch or sugar 2. A fat or oil 5. A protein 4. An amino acid 5. Do not know. 80 Ip. CD 9. 81 An example of a material composed largely of polyhydroxy aldehyde and ketone molecules is: 1 Vitamin A 2. Sugar 5. Butter 4. Amino Acid 5. Do not know. The term “pH" is used to express: 1. Specific gravity 2. Hydrolysis 5. Ions in solution 4. Acidity and alkalinity 5. Do not know. A Centigrade temperature measurement may be easily con— verted to its Fahrenheit equivalent by the following formula: 1. (9/5 x 0c) — 32 2. (5/9 x 0C) - 32 3. (9/5 x 0c) + 32 4. (5/9 x 0c) + 32 5. Do not know. To reduce cooking losses (shrinkage) in meat, poultry or fish which of the following is an advisable procedure? 1. Cook at a low temperature for a long time 2. Cook at a high temperature for a long time 5. Cook by charcoal broiling 4. Cook in a pressure saucepan 5. Do not know. A rib roast that has a round purple stamp on the exterior fat stating (in part) "InSpected and Passed" indicates: 1. The animal that produced the roast was healthy at the time of slaughter 2. The roast has been inspected by the local Board of Health 5. The roast is of U. S. Prime Grade 4. The roast should be cooked by a moist heat method (such as stewing) 5. Do not know. A basic factor to consider when choosing a cooking method for a piece of meat, poultry or fish is: The type of bone present in the cut . The type of connective tissue present in the cut The type of fat present in the cut . Whether the cut is fresh or frozen, then thawed Do not know. 1 2 5 4 5 10. 11. 12. 15. 14. 15. 82 Which one of the following foods would be the best source of high quality protein (protein composed of the amino acids essential for growth)? 1. Enriched bread 2. Navy beans 5. Gelatin 4. Eggs 5. Do not know. To delay or prevent rancidity a product high in fat, such as corn oil, should be stored: 1. In a humid atmOSphere 2. In a air-tight container 5. In a copper lined container 4. At a temperature of 800E or above 5. Do not know. To minimize the amount of fat absorbed by a food during the frying process: 1. Use a fat such as margarine to fry food in 2. Use a fat such as butter to fry food in 5. Use a fairly high cooking temperature (5600 F) 4. Use a moderate to low cooking temperature (212 5. Do not know. OF) In a baked product, such as cake, which of the following ingredients functions as a tenderizer for the product? . Sugar . Eggs . Milk . Flour . Do not know. O'II-PCNNH A reaction whereby starch molecules are broken into shorter chains may occur when: 1. Starch is heated (such as in boiling potatoes) 2. Starch is subjected to dry heat (such as in toasting a slice of bread) 5. Starch (present in flour) is used in preparing bread dough 4. Starch is used in making chocolate pudding 5. Do not know. The formation of large crystals of sugar in a cooked fudge frosting, giving a "gritty" texture, may result from: 1. Beating the frosting after some cooling ($00 OF) 2. Beating the frosting while it is hot (212 F) 5. Using cream of tartar in the recipe 4. Using corn sirup in the recipe 5. Do not know. 85 16. When making gravy, a smooth product may most easily be produced by 1. Cooking the gravy at a lower temperature (2120 F) 2 Cooking the gravy at a high temperature (5500 F) 5. Adding liquid to the meat drippings in the pan, stirring then adding flour and stirring until thickened 4. Adding flour to meat drippings in the pan, stirring, then adding liquid and stirring until the mixture has thickened 5. Do not know. 17. To prevent undesirable bacterial growth in food: 1. Be particularly cautious when storing foods largely of fatty composition 2. Be particularly cautious when storing foods largely of carbohydrate composition 5. Use 150-1700F as a steam table temperature for hold— ing foods to be served to customers 4. Use 100—1200F as a steam table temperature for hold- ing foods to be served to customers 5. Do not know. 18. One tablespoon is equivalent to: 1. 2 teaspoons 2. 5 teaspoons 5. 4 teaspoons 4. 5 teaspoons 5. Do not know. 19. Three—fourths (5/4) of a cup is equivalent to: 1. 11 tablespoons 2. 12 tablespoons 5. 15 tablespoons 4. 14 tablespoons 5. Do not know. 84 Questionnaire 1. Name 2. Student Number 5. Grade Level (please check): Sophomore Junior Senior Other (please specify) 4. Specific Major (please check): Dietetics Foods Hotel, Motel and Club Management Home Economics Education Institutional and Hospital Management Research in Foods and Nutrition Restaurant Management Other (please specify) 5. Previous Food Preparation Experience (please check): Type of Experience Length of Experience (in months) High School ——— 4-H Club—— Armed Forces ——— Restaurant —- Hotel or Resort—— Other (please ppecify)-—— APPENDIX D Student Evaluation of FN 200 Date Laboratory Section Code Letter Note: By the use of check marks (x) indicate for each item whether "greatly," "moderately,” "little,” or ”none" best expresses what you have gained through your ex- perience in FN 200. PLEASE DO NOT SIGN YOUR NAME. Greatly Moder— Little ately None As a result of my experience in EN 200, my over all knowl- edge of foods has increased. FN 200 has contributed to my vocational preparation. My knowledge of important facts and terms in foods has increased. The clarity of the EN 200 course objectives (what I have been expected to learn) has contributed to my progress in the course. The examinations and quizzes in EN 200 influenced my atti— tude concerning the course. My previous work in chemistry contributed to my understand— ing of EN 200. My ability to make practical applications in the area of foods has increased. 85 86 APPENDIX D - Continued Greatly Moder— ately Little None The lecture portion of EN 200 has increased my understand— ing of the basic principles of food preparation. LO 0 The laboratory portion of EN 200 has increased my under— standing of the basic princi— ples of food preparation. 10. The lecture portion of FN 200 has caused my interest in foods to grow. 11. The laboratory portion of FN 200 has caused my interest in foods to grow. 12. I have enjoyed the lecture portion of FN 200. 15. I have enjoyed the laboratory portion of EN 200. 14. Demonstrations, presented by the instructor, increased my satisfaction with the labora— tory portion of EN 200. 15. Laboratories involving student preparation of products, in- creased my satisfaction with the laboratory portion of EN 200. 16. As a result of my experience in EN 200, my interest in tak— ing more foods courses has increased. PLEASE WRITE IN YOUR COMMENTS TO THE FOLLOWING QUESTIONS: 17. The one thing I 44ked most about lecture was 87 18. The one thing I liked most about laboratory was 19. The one thing I disliked most about lecture was 20. The one thing I disliked most about laboratory was “'IIII'IIIIIIIIILIIIII'IIIIIIII'IIIIIIIII’II“