VEkBAE. CUES 0F ORGANIZATIONAL lNFORMATEON EN MESSAGE fiECQDiNG 'z‘he-sis Ed»: 613 Degree 6F Ph. D. it‘dCMiGAN STATE UNEVERSITY Mahmwfi M. Ei-Okby {9’62 E'Hiul ‘- This is to certify that the thesis entitled VERBAL CUES OF ORGALIZATIOL‘AL lllFO RMA'IIOL II; MESSAGE DECOUIL G presented by Mahmud m. El-Okby has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for Mdegree in Communication MM? \ Major professor Date August 7. 1962 0-169 LIBR AR Y Michigan State University ABSTRACT VERBAL ours or ORGANIZATIOICAL limos-Emma; m MESSAGE DECODING by Mahmud m. El-Okby This is a study of one of the basic features of language-in-action. It is an attempt to elucidate the notion of organizational information in communication, applied to the message as content-in- form. From the point of view of the decoder, it categorizes such information within a psychological frame of reference. It demonstrates the various types of cues manipulated for the organization of linguistic response in the vocal-auditory inter-personal communication situation, and presents a psycholinguistic analysis of a recorded panel discussion, based on examination of a number of assumptions. In the course of such examination, it presents a synthesis of some of the pertinent findings in interrelated disciplines, all of which are concerned with language-in-communication. It offers suggestions for further research in an area thereby initially explored. Taking into consideration such factors as the attention value of a message, the similarity of experience supposedly shared by communicator and Mahmud H. El-Okby receiver, coupled with their more or less equivalent familiarity with the symbolic representation Of such experience in language form, the question now arises whether the clarity of received messages and facility Of communication may be assisted by investigations into the structure Of content. Perhaps a basic principle of communicability in any language is a formative principle having to do with the organization of content units. Consequently, one could assume the operation of verbal cues eliciting a specific type of information. One could probably talk of the organization value Of messages in communication, depending on the way such cues are calculated to Operate with the decoder. 1. Contouring and Accentuation cues are presumed to Operate at the 'Projection' level Of linguistic reSponse. 2. Transition, Combination and Integration cues are supposed to Operate at the 'Integration‘ level. 3. These cues are two main types under each category: syntactic, and contextual. 4. They Operate synchronously, and are not mutually exclusive. The theoretic framework within which analysis and interpretation are conducted is based on: 1. Recommendations of the 1953 Report on Psycholinguistics by the Committee on Linguistics and Psychology Of Mahmud M. El-Okby the Social Science Research Council. 2. Applications Of some of the basic concepts Of Information Theory. 3. Some of the findings Of structural linguistics in the areas Of syntactics and supra-segmental phonemics. 4. Some of the assumptions Of discourse analysis. 5. Pertinent concepts of the psychology Of perception in general, and of the Gestalt psychology of visual perceptual organization in particular. VERBAL CUES 0F ORGANIZATIONAL INFORMATION IN MESSAGE DECODING By ”3d Mahmud uz‘ml-Okby A THESIS Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PEILOSOEEY Department of Communication 1962 A CKLT OW LEDGLE it‘ll TS For generous cooperation in the develOpment of this thesis, I wish to acknowledge my gratitude to Professors John Ball, David K. Berlo, Hideya Kumata, Donald J. Lloyd, Malcolm S. MacLean Jr., Herbert P. Phillips, and Dean John Winburne. Special thanks are due tO Professor hacLean, chairman of my advisory committee, Who has impressed me by his watchful care and most scrupulous concern, as well as by his broadness Of outlook. And to Professor Berlo, Head Of the Department of Communication, I am indebted for his making available both time and research facilities. As for Professor John Ball, he has contributed a great deal to whatever I may have achieved, thanks to his most competent and sincere guidance. He has obliged me by taking personal interest in my progress all the way through, and by providing ready assistance whenever needed. Professor Donald Lloyd Of Wayne State University has been so generous as to serve on my doctoral committee, and give me the benefit of sharing his valuable experiences in the field of modern language research. I am very thankful to him. 11 Dean John Winburne Of University College has been very kind and cooperative in demonstrating his methodology of discourse analysis, and providing ample materials from his research centre. I have utilized some of his concepts, as will be acknowledged with gratitude in the chapter on analysis. Thanks are also due to Miss Anne Beard for her willingness to revise the bibliography with great care and consideration. I am very much obliged to my wife, Fatma, for her diligence and perseverance in revising my manuscripts, even While writing her own, Master‘s,thesis. 111 TABLE OF CONTENTS CKNOWLEDGKENTS PERT I Chapter I. INTRODUCTION . . . . . . . . . . . II. DEFINITIONS . . . . . . . . . . . III. CUES AND CATEGORIES . . . . . . . . PART II I. ANALYSIS . . . . . . . . . . . II. CONTOURING CUES . . . . . . . . . III. ACCENTUATION CUES . . . . IV. TRANSITION CUES . . . . . . . . . V. COMBINATION CUES . . . . . . . . VI. INTEGRATION CUES . . . . . . . . APPEHDIX I . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Corpus Material APPENDIX II . . . . . . . . Suggestions for Further Research BIBLIO GRAmY O O O O O O O O O O O I 0 iv Page ii 15 25 52 52 67 100 124 148 158 165 CHAPTER I This study is an investigation into the structure of message content, from the point of view of a decoder. It is conducted'through psycho- linguistic analysis Of a sample of formal inter-personal communication. The communication situation, which is only partially represented by recorded speech, is a complex process, with many simultaneous component processes, which affect the behavior of both speakers and listeners engaged in some form Of interaction. It is a panel discussion among eminent American creative thinkers, taking place in one auditorium of’Miami University, Oxford, Ohio, and attended by several hundred peOple, who are supposedly interested in creativity in the various art forms. The time is around 7.50 run. The tOpic of the discussion is "Form versus Content in Art." As may be suggested by the title, such a tOpic sounds debatable. The notion of 'versus' implies presentation of a controversial issue, over which panel and audience are likely to be divided. 2 It is unquestionable that an analysis aimed at the identification of only 'verbal' cues, manipulated by members of the panel, to serve the specific function of 'Organization', would leave out a great many 'non-verbal' cues, exchanged among members of the two interacting sides: the panel on the one hand, and the audience on the other. Such non-verbal cues relate to social-cultural, psychological, and physical factors and conditions, both personal and situational, which affect the communication process in no small measure. However, as far as this investigation is concerned, the focus is on thos verbal cues, which help organize content units in the process of perception. It is true that the exchange of messages on the gestural-visual band, for instance, could be utilized in reinforcing those cues delivered on the vocal- auditory, relating to the organization of content. Still, this investigation has to have limits, which encompass relationships between certain linguistic and non-linguistic phenomena. In linguistics, because the focus Of study is on the 'code', rather than on the 'message', it has been possible to maintain a difference between two types of structure - the structure of content and the structure of expression - and to limit investigations to the latter, leaving the former to semanticists, content-analysts, psycho-linguists, etc. Also, in linguistics, because the structure Of expression has to be maintained in its own rights, independently of the structure of content, the levels of structure have to be purely linguistic; in the main, they are divided between phonematization and grammatization. Consequently, the units of analysis are the phoneme, the morpheme, and the 'syntagme'. Territory beyond the sentence is 'Out of bounds'. In the study of verbal communication, however, the focus is on the 'message', whole and entire, as content and as expression. As expression and as content, it is viewed as having structure, Which is perceivable and explainable in terms Of signals. All we have in the form of verbal items constitute a number of 'cues' manipulated by an encoder for a decoder. Those cues which are selected for Observation in this study are segmental and supra-segmental features of language-in-action, suggestive of a specific type of information. They do not tell the decoder, in any direct way, what the message is about; but they tell him What goes with what. They are supposed to help the decoder organize the content units of received 4 messages at different levels of perceptual organisation. Accordingly, in Verbal Communication, the units of analysis do not have to be phonemes, morphemes, and 'syntagmes'. As far as the decoder is concerned, his ones can be modulations of speech sounds, or they can be speech items, single or combined, as words, phrases, sentences, or utterances, etc. All that matters is the correspondence between the level of perceptual organization and the nature of the given one. For instance, if at the 'Prcjecticn' level, perceptual organization is described as " a summation of points of maximal stimulation, and a suppression of other activity,"1then it is reasonable to suppose that the prosodic features of speech - pitch, stress, Juncture - may be considered cues providing organizational information needed at this level of structuring. 0n the other hand, if at the 'Integraticn' level, percpptual organisation is described as an * ordering of semantic units ... concordal agreement and certain other relational phenomena, etc.,"2thmn it is reasonable to suppose that 'eyntactic‘ items of transition from one 1 Charles Osgood and T Sebeck, ede., Es cholinguistics (Indiana University Publication, 1954 , p. 4. 2 Osgood, 22. cit., p. 95. 5 sentence to another, or that redundant 'contextual' items in combination - may be considered cues of organizational information needed at this level. Part I of this study is a theoretic frame- work, which seeks to elucidate the notion of 'orgenizational information' utilised in message decoding. The major thesis expounded in this part may be summed up as follows: 1. There are verbal cues which.help a decoder organise the content of received messages in the course of vocal-auditory interpersonal communication. 2. These cues are supposed to elicit information at two levels of organization of linguistic response, namely those of 'Pchecticn', and 'Integration'. 1 5. These cues may be subsumed under the corresponding categories of 'Contouring' and ‘Accentuaticn' for projection, and of 'Transiticn', 'Combination', and 'Integraticn' for integration. 4. The Operation of these cues may be described in terms of the Gestalt principles of visual perceptual organisation. Part II is the actual analysis of recorded materials. It consists in: l. Devising means of identifying cues of organizational information. 2. Identifying such cues in the sample under observation. 5. Testing a number of assumptions on the basis of evidence from the corpus material, with the understanding that conclusive evidence should come from future field research in each of the specified areas. It is necessary to emphasize that this study is of an 'exploratory' nature. It presents a new way of looking at language-in-action. It presents a new orientation: the communication point of view, in which form is combined with content, and structure with function. CHAPTER II DEFINITIONS Verbal. Some students of language refer to the supra-segmental or prosodic features Of pitch, stress, pause, and Juncture as 'non-verbal‘. However, such features are here considered as part and parcel of the segmental units. It would be impossible to consider bundles of distinctive features as human speech components, without the hierarchies of configurational features which give them shape and constitution. Accordingly, the term 'non-verbal' would be used to refer to gestural and other phenomena, that do not fall within the scope of this enquiry. Meanwhile, linguo-perceptual phenomena would be referred to as ‘non-linguistic'. 9333, These are segmental and supra-segmental features of language-in-communication, suggestive of a specific type of information to be dealt with in this study. The word 'similarly' is a cue to some parallel event or situation. It may be viewed as a 'combination' cue, according to the projected theoretic framework. A succession of the /-4-3/ intonation contour (Pike system) in the course of an utterance signals a 'sustain' in the encoding-decoding process. Such an intonation contour may be considered a combination cue. One of the established meanings of the term 'cue' in the context Of message decoding, is any 'hint', or 'allusion', which may be considered suggestive of certain denotations, connotations, relationships, etc. Another is a 'sign', or 'indicator' of a certain course of action. A third is synoymous with a 'clue', though this meaning is often associated with problem situations. In any case, the basic feature of a cue is 'indirect' intimation. What is particularly spelled out here is the notion of 'mediation' between stimulus and response; what is 'given‘ is only a hint that ripples into further hints, which finally trigger the desired response. Should the same word Occur twice in a message, it would act as two different cues reinforcing each other in establishing certain relationships at levels of organization. Organizational Information. "In the case of decoding, we want to know ... what segments contribute to sequential and integrational organization in 1 language perception." The notion of organizational information maintained in this study is an attempt to extend the concept of 'information' in the direction of its normal usage, which is, of course, different from the particular denotation that it has in Information Theory. Although 'chance' may be conceived of as a factor of communication as regards the probability of Occurrence of coding symbols, there is no intention of investigating the possibility that chance may still control the probability of occurrence of 'concepts' and their arrangements, as words and word combinations. This, however, would not prevent possible extensions of some of the basic concepts of Information Theory, that could be adapted to the wider range of the present investigation. It is very often observed that in dealing with configuraticnal aspects of perception, the terms 'form' and 'structure' are sometimes used synonymously, and sometimes with some discrimination as to whether the notion of 'constituents' is implied, in which case, 'structure* would be preferable. The concept of organization is assumed to add a further I Osgood and Sebeck, eds., Ps cholin uistics (Indiana Unisersity Publication, 1954l, p. VI. 10 distinction based on De Saussure's famous dichotomy of 'langue' versus 'parole',2and on the principle of linguistic duality expounded in particular by Gustav Herdan.3 Speaking of linguistic structure at the 'code' level, one would normally have in mind the two chief categories of 'phonematization' and 'grammatization'. However, speaking of how phonemic and grammatical relationships are utilized in the encoding-decoding processes of individual 'messages', one would think in terms of the organization of linguistic units. Structure of Content. Similarly, the structure of content is, in fact, an ‘Organization' of content units at the 'message' level, that one has access to, to be distinguished from the so-called 'semantic structure'? which.has to do with a summation of the organizations of content units used by all members of a speech community. In order to study the organization of content units with reference to the linguistic perception of message decoders, it may be useful to consider in the 2 Ferdinand de Saussure. Course in General Linguistics (New York: Philosophical Library Inc.,1959), p. 13. 5 Gustav Herdan. Lan ea c as Choice and Chance (Groningen: NoordhofI N. 7., I953), Part IV. 4 Charles F. Hockett. A Course in Modern Linguistics (New York: Macmillan CO., 1958), p. 142. 11 first place, the general levels of organization of linguistic responses. Charles Osgood distinguishes threefi: a skill level: an integrational level, and a representational level. These happen to correspond to the three levels of organization of sensory input6 namely: 1. Projection Level: having to do with the "summation of points of maximum stimulation, and suppression of other activity."7 2. Integrative Level: having to do with grammatical relationships: "Ordering of semantic units, concrete relational classifications of these, concordal agreement, and certain other relational phenomena, appear to belong to the 'integrational' level."8 5. Representational Level: with which this enquiry is not 'directly' concerned. Information as to what the content of a message is, though related to, is still different from information as to the organization of the elements of such a content. Osgood, 22, cit., p. 95. Ibid. , p. 54. Ibid. , p. 54. m-QO‘OU Ibid. , p. 95. 12 Within this frame of reference, a few categories of organizational information are hypothesized in this study, namely: Contouring, Accentuation (at the projection level): Transition, Combination, and Integration (at the integrative level). Message. A message is technically defined as that part of the total output of a source unit, which may simultaneously be a part of the total input to a destination unit. It may be the utterance, or sentence, signalled by a terminal juncture, or the 'entry', signalled by a change of speakers. Decoding. This is the process, Whereby auditory patterns of stimulation elicit both structural and representational responses in the receiver, through the activity of the central nervous system. ngcholinguistic. The distinction often made between 'psycholinguistics' and 'psychology of language' is probably based on whether the researcher is a linguist or a psychologist in the first place, on whether his area of competence is the verbal event or the mental process. In either case, however, the basic concern is the relationship between linguistic and non-linguistic features. 13 Analysis. The procedure by which certain verbal cues are: 1) identified in the recorded material: 2) empirically tested in their assumed capacity to elicit certain responses: and 3) associated with specific categories of organizational information. Formal Inter:personal Communication. It is supposed that the exchange of verbal messages in the face-to-face situation Of some conference or group discussion may offer more adequate samples for this type of analysis than in other communication situations. The whole point of Specifying 'formality' is to avoid analyzing the trite expressions of every- day life chatting, which would normally be responded to at a level of no small automaticity. Though intercepted by the voice of the announcer three times after the introduction, and by that of the moderator at points of transition, the recorded materials from a symposium held at Miami University in March.of 1955, still present the formal communication situation in which the speakers address one another, not without reservations of course, due to their consciousness of their face-to-face audience. Vocal-Auditory Band. Interpersonal communication channels are often treated as consisting of different 14 bands over which messages move synchronously. As far as this study is concerned, the gestural-visual band is out of the question, and so are those bands connected with other sense modalities. One is here concerned with the decoding of messages given by word of mouth and received aurally. CHAPTER III CUES AND CATEGORIES A) Pro action Level Contouring. A contour may probably be defined as one or more lines Joining points of equal elevation on a surface. An initial step in decoding speech may be very much like 'joining points of equal elevation' on the surface of a continuum. It is the attempt to discover the hierarchical structure Of every utterance, as the bundles of distinctive and ccnfigurational features are simultaneously unreeled. The aim on the part of the decoder is to distinguish the gross units of which content is made up, or rather, the minimum thematic sequences that make a difference in reSponse. He would want to know, for instance, from the contour cues of the utterance that there is an actor-action relationship, or a modifier-modified relationship: that there is a positioning of Opposites, or a drawing together of similarities, no matter what these are in detail, etc. 16 Psychologically, contouring is the outcome of intensity differences, which actually consists in greater contrast. A triangle of light projected very dimly on a screen is nothing more than a blur of light. But as the intensity Of illumination is increased, a contour will emerge. Increased awareness Of the divisions of an utterance that constitute belongingness of units to one level of structure or another, is probably based on similar intensity difference. Assuming that Immediate Constituents are signalled by Juncture patterns, among other ccnfigurational features, one might expect that the more contrastive and differentiated the junctures become, the more intensity difference is available, and the more sharply defined is the contour. Identification of Cues. All the prosodic features Of speech are supposed to operate synchronously to give the decoder the contouring cues in question. However, it is here suggested that Juncture, in particular, may be singled out as the chief variable affecting the simultaneous Occurrence of relationships of grouping and separation involved in contouring. The other supra-segmental phonemic features, namely pitch and stress, will be considered redundant features. 17 Accentuation. This feature has to do with the degree of articulation or differentiation that the decoder perceives as maintained in connection with certain points of the contour. The placing of accent on any unit of some behavioral pattern is usually associated with the notion of prominence or distinction in comparison with other units of the same pattern. While contouring is supposed to enable the decoder to 'figure out' the gross form of content, accentuation is assumed to help him distinguish the main features of the Outline. Here he seems to Observe which units carry more weight than others; which are stressed on account of their presumed significance to the source; which are underscored to create more awareness in the receiver. Psychologically, accentuation is probably based on all three interrelated notions of 'figure on ground', 'contrast', and 'fixation'. Any stimulus which contrasts strongly with its background will show as a distinct figure on ground: it will be readily and particularly attended to. Elements that are maximally accentuated in the structure of content are the ones to stand out as more significant than others in the background. They are the ones that call attention to themselves for special recognition. 18 Identification of Cues. Again, more than one of the prosodic features may be considered cues to this type of information. While stress, as a chief variable, may be chosen for close examination in this connection, the other features may be considered redundant. B) Integration Level Transition. "The relations between transitional sequences in messages and transitional sequencing mechanisms in the communicator is the field of sequential psycholinguistics."1 Integration is another level of organization of linguistic responses. One of the basic phenomena to be investigated at this level is transition, which is message decoding would normally suggest a progressive motion from one unit of the message to another, or a passage from one stage of its thematic deve10pment to the next. In analyzing the message in terms of the structure of content, transition as a feature of organizational information is not viewed with reference to the 'probability‘ of occurrence of microlinguistic units, but in terms of the 'possibility' of occurrence of 1 Charles Osgood and T. Sebeck, eds., Ps cholinguistics 4} (Indiana University Publication, 195 , p. 6. 19 conceptual frames, as content units. Gustav.Herdan‘s concept of language as choice and chance assumes great significance in this context. One's interest in the organization value of the signal or cue at the point of transition does not centre on how many times a particular linguistic unit will 'probably‘ occur as a succeeding unit, but rather on the 'possibility' of proceeding thematically from there somewhere. Information theorists are obviously interested in the former organization value of signals; their approach is statistical. "The statistical procedure usually ignores, however, a matter which is basic to linguistics - the distinguishing of levels of structure.E Therefore, the present investigation is concerned with the latter organization value of signals. The approach is analogical, consisting in the use of substitution frames based on two notions: 1) that of the level of structure; and 2) that of the context of communication. This is very much.the same approach of the individual decoder in the communication situationt He cannot proceed from one content unit to the next without suing: l) as to the Immediate Constituent level; and 2) as to the thematic environment. If an utterance were 2 Ibid. , p. 94 20 to be broken up into single words, or into sections of equal length, it would lose its sequential value in the process of decoding. One reason is that the levels of syntactic structure would in that case be obliterated and the contour utterly confused. Another is that contextual cues would also be shuffled, throwing away every chance that the decoder might have of guessing what is likely to come next. Psychologically, transition seems to be accomplished through the simultaneous operation of systematic and non-systematic principles of association. Principles of subordination, coordination, etc. would belong to the former category; while principles of similarity, contiguity, etc. would pertain to the latter. The Operation of both level or syntactic cues and contextual cues of transition seems to be explainable in terms of systematic and non-systematic principles of association. Identification of Cues. In structural linguistics, it is natural that the transition phenomenon should receive only partial treatment. Only levelzcues seem to be identifiable. According to C.C.Fries, the links between what he calls 5 Charles C. Fries. The Structure English (Kew York: Harcourt Brace & Co.:_1952), p. Ell. 21 a 'situation' sentence and a 'sequence' sentence must be structural units, or rather, grammatical in a strict linguistic sense. Hence, he has to limit his investigation - and understandably so - to the'so-called' definitive article, the 'so-called' adverbs, and the demonstratives. However, his inclusion of such items as 'else' and 'other' now leaves the way open for the warranted addition of the whole category of contextual cues, within the frame of reference of psycholinguistic analysis. Apart from these two main types of cues, the prosodic features also play an important part in signalling transition from one content unit to the next. Combination. In the course of progression from one unit of a message content to the next, there occurs an accumulation of parts, an addition of thematic segments that for some reason or other may be viewed as tacking together. This phenomenon is here referred to as combination. Obviously, what is involved in combination of elements is redundancy, or repetition and reiteration. However, it is not Just redundancy, but redundancy in transition. While pursuing a point in the course of development, the decoder would encounter certain items being repeated along the line of progression. He might or might not be fully aware of the repetition and reiteration, but the end result for 22 him is accumulative. Combination enriches context, and this in turn affects prediction of subsequent events. Speaking in terms of Information theory, prediction would then be made on the basis of both 'discrete', and 'oumulative' probabilities, or rather, possibilities in the present context. Given only knowledge of the immediately antecedent event at any one of these levels, uncertainty as to the subsequent event is maximal (within limits imposed by the structure of the hierarchy). As we increase our knowledge by taking into account more and more of the sequence of antecedent events, as well as subsequent events in the case of decoding, uncertainty as to the subsequent event decreases. 4 Psychologically, this is due, very likely, to stimulus patterning. The stimulus presented in one form after another, thereby accumulating additional information for the decoder, becomes more specific, and hence, more precisely associated with a given reSponse than with other reaponses. This is what one might call the psychological function accomplished by combination of elements. However, the principles of perceptual organization underlying combination as a structural feature might perhaps be 'similarity', 'contiguity', etc. Identification of Cues. Here again, it is necessary to recognize the two chief classes of level, or syntactic cues, and of contextual cues. Prosodic features may be considered redundant. ~~4 Osgood, gp. cit., p. 97. Integration. It is assumed that listening to a verbal communication involves an attempt on the part of the decoder to accomodate, as far as possible, the presented structure of relationships into the total framework of his previous experiences. Hence, the notion of integration as a feature of organization. The encoder usually provides certain cues to facilitate this operation. Such verbal units have the value of incorporating new relationships into a generally acceptable frame of reference, namely the logical, through 'substitution', 'modification', and 'amplification'. What is involved in integration seems to be the outcome of all the foregoing Operations: not that these Operations follow a consecutive linear order, of course, but that they all contribute to this phenomenon simultaneously. When contour and accent are discriminated, when the elements of content are noted in succession, as well as in combination, the integration of newly established relationships into a framework already in existence becomes possible. Psychologically, integration is accountable in terms of the principle of fusion or summation in perceptual organization. Hence, the pertinence of the notion of 'consistency' in particular with regard to comprehension. "Whether the comprehended thought becomes our own, whether we retain it,“and whether we can 24 use it, does not depend on comprehension as such. The process of 'fitting in“ only defines more closely the meaning.”5 Identification of Cues. Cues to this type of organizational information happen to be identified by the structural linguist in terms of 'function words', which would qualify as level cues, according to the present classification. These are the signals that help the decoder establish the communicated structural relationships utilized for reasoning. Within the presented framework of studying the message as content-in-form, it would be necessary to recognize the other major class of contextual cues, which consist of 'content words'. 5 David Rapaport, (trans.). Organization and Pathology of Thought. (New York: Columbia UniversityfiPress, 1951), p. 45. CHAETER I ANAIESIS Within the theoretic framework sketched in Part I of this study, the corpus material in Appendix ;_will be analyzed with the aim of identifying those ones which are assumed to elicit the interrelated categories of organizational information previously discussed. The prOposed analysis is psycholinguistic: it has to do with the relationship between linguistic and non-linguistic phenomena. Under 'Contooring‘, it deals with the relationships between Juncture and intonation, as two of the prosodic features of speech. and the 'gross structure' of content in the process of decoding. under 'Accentuation', it is concerned with the relationships between stress and Juncture phenomena, and the 'prominence' or maximal stimulation of specific content units in the same process. Analysis in terms of these two categories is hypothetically representative of the 'Prodectionl, or skill level of organisation of linguistic response. it the 'IntegrationJ level, hypothetically 25 26 represented by the three Operations of 'Transition', 'Combination', and 'Integration', analysis of speech is also assumed to treat relationships between linguistic and non-linguistic features. Transition is synonymous with 'progression' from one content unit or group of units to another in the process of perception: combination is assumed to consist in the 'clustering' of such units: and integration has to do with their 'cohesion' against a background of experience possessed by the decoder. Under allthtee categories of perceptual organization, identifiable verbal cues are partly 'grammatical' and partly 'lexical' items: partly 'eyntactic', and partly 'Oontextual'. This analysis is 'auditory', as distinguished from 'inetrumental' analysis in structural linguistics. It is realised that in order to describe linguistic features significant to communication, such features must first be 'discovered' by deliberate auditory analysis. However, for confirmation of the sheer physical characteristics of speech.in field experiments, observations made by the analyst would have to be supplemented by instrumental measurements. The analyst, who is a decoder of the messages he listens to, could not possibly qualify as a casual listener under the same conditions of the 27 particular communication situation. While losing all the nonverbal cues previously alluded to, he has the advantage over the casual listener in other respects. For instance, in the process of perception, a decoder would normally utilize in particular a two-dimensional view of things: one prospective and the other retrospective. The analyst has a better chance of utilizing perception both ways: prior training and availability of 'play-back' would naturally act favorably for him. However, the aim of this analysis is exploration: otherwise, it could not have been planned to cover so much territory. It is supposed to demonstrate: 1) what cues to look forin field research dealing with the organization of message content in the process of decoding: 2) at what levels of linguistic response they are supposed to Operate: 5) how to identify them in a corpus material: and 4) how to go about testing a few assumptions empirically, in a more or less tentative way. While the reproducibility of one identification is maintained, and the quantitative side of measurement initially demonstrated, the given interpretations 'would need further checks from future research. The data under observation consists in excerpts from a symposium on a 'supposedly' 28 contrOversial issue: Form versus Content in Art. The various contributions, referred to as 'entries', which are made by members of the panel in discussing this issue, are given serial numbers in the order of their occurrence. And because the brief statements made by both the moderator and the announcer provide links in between the exhhanged messages, the contributions Of these two individuals are also included in the general count of entries to facilitate reference. At the projection level of organization of linguistic response, hypothetically represented by 'contcuring' and 'accentuation', any utterance composed of a group of words may be analysed, however brief such an utterance may be. While data for such analysis could be chosen from any part of the corpus material, it will be convenient at this level to treat only the shorter entries. At the integration level of organization, however, discourse units must be long enough to permit observatimn of such.phenomena as 'transition', 'combination!, and 'integration'. The longer entries will therefore provide data for analysis in terms of these categories. The procedure of analysis is two-fold: general and specific. The general procedure consists of three steps: 1. 2. 3. 29 Transcription of recorded material, special attention being given to terminal Junctures, as well as to pauses of hesitation and all instances of hemming and hawing. Selection of entries to be charted in terms of the prosodic features of pitch, stress and Juncture. Tabulation of content units for the rest of the entries to help identify contextual cues. Dean John Winburne's method of content analysis is considered very helpful in this respect. The specific procedure of analysis starts when the data.hsyebeen reduced to these charts and tables. It is the more 'presentable', or rather, more condensed form of the data now on display for the reader of this study. This procedure is suited to It 1. the requirements of testing specific assumptions. also consists of three steps: Formation of assumptions under each.oategory, suggested by the charted data. Generally, Assumption ;.would be about the assumed capacity of a particular type of cues to elicit a certain feature of organizational information. Assumption ;;_would be concerned directly or indirectly with the 'predictability' of occurrence for each of the specified types of cues, through preliminary investigation of some correlation 2. 30 between one feature that is supposedly known to the decoder with another that is pending occurrence, or else, through an investigation of the Operation of some fundamental principle like the binary principle, on which.a decoder might consciously or unconsciously base his predictions. Assumption Il;.would be concerned with one of the main characteristics of interpersonal communication among a group, such as feed-back effects and mutual interaction. Should certain terms occur in any of the assumptions, which need clarification of the intended meaning, such clarification is done, as a rule, in the course of interpretation. (step 3) Identification of Cues. One or two entries are usually chosen for observation. If the specified cues are prosodic features of speech, the patterns of these features are charted consistently according to one system, such as the Pike system of phonemic notation. All intonation contours are charted in terms of such.a notation, and so is the case with the patterns of tentative and terminal junctures. Adherence to one particular system is only meant to permit collation for purposes of comparison or contrast. Any other well known system, such as the Trager and Smith, could have been used for the same purpose. 5. 31 On the other hand, if the specified cues are 'syntactic', a consistent classification of such cues is maintained all the way through, accompanied by a brief note on the basis of such classification in terms of the communicative function served by these cues. As for the identification of 'contextual' cues, this is done, as a rule, along the lines drawn by Dean John Winburne's research in content analysis. His major and minor 'sensemes' are basic notions on which the present investigator has built tables of major and minor content units, as well as tables Of differentiated cue clusters. Interpretation. In a process of differential analysis, mere identification and reproducibility are all the evidence needed to support a given description. In other words, verification is inherent in a demonstrable contrast: in a comparison of points of similarity or difference. However, because interpretation under circumstances dictated by the nature of this research would still have the limitations previously alluded to in this chapter, the presented verification of assumptions may be considered tentative. More exhaustive treatment under conditions permissive of more experimental rigor would still be needed. CHAPTER II CONTOURING CUES The notion of contouring may be summed up in a few words to bear in mind in the course of analysis. It is a presumed tendency on the part of a decoder to respond to certain verbal cues, ihich signal two main structural features of simultaneous cccurrenc: grouping and separation of items. Whenever grouping of elements occurs, separation of these from other elements will concurrently take place. Verbal cues, which.are supposed to signal the two structural relationships in question, may be more than the ones selected here for observation. Only two of the prosodic features of speech.will be considered in this analysis, namely, juncture and intonation. It is assumed that juncture in particular is probably the chief variable affecting word grouping and segmentation, as far as field properties are concerned. Although intonation is recognised as a factor of perceptual organisation at this level, it is assumed to operate in other capacities connected with communication of feelings and attitudes. 32 33 With a view to the purpose of this analysis, it may be helpful to distinguish between two types of Junoture. a) Tentative Junoture / may be operationally defined as a pause of combined phonological-and-structural significance, occurring in the course of an utterance. It signals a'sustain' in the encoding-decoding process. Therefore, it can occur at the end of a 'sentence', if such an end is not relatively 'conclusive'. It is often accompanied by a -4-3 or a ~2-3 intonation contour, according to the Pike system of phonemic notation. b) Terminal iuncture # is a pause invariably occurring at the end of an utterance, the given signal being, relatively 'conclusive'. is far as the present corpus material is concerned, this pause is more often than otherwise accompanied by the -2-4 intonation contour. 1. Assumption; Junotures and intonation contours are supposed to operate 'iointly' as cues for the 'simultaneous' separation and grouping of items, as represented by the words of an utterance. 2. ldentification of Cues (Entry 10., Ciardi) But can you go on to the human qgegtimn / until you know what stone will4dg / and what reinforced concrete will4dg / 34: and what steel will do / ~4-s and what glass will do / ~4-5 and what plastic facings will do # -2-4 Uh until your knowledge of these materials is such / -2-5 that you..you can put them in second place # ~2-4 (Entry 13., Oiardi) I'd like to ask William.Butler Yeats that question / uh and his answer is / 0 body swayed to music o brightining glance / how shall I tell the dancer from the dance # . ~2-4 You can't separate these th.. these two things / lhere is no dancer/ unless.. the..the dance is nowhere until someone dances it # ~2-4 And he's not a dancer until he's danoéng # 3. Interpretation Entry 10 is divided into two sections by means of the terminal juncture after ”will do'. This juncture may be viewed as performing two organisational functions simultaneously. While it separates the preceding frame from the one following it, it actually groups or unifies all the preceding segments, marked off by tentative 35 pauses. A possible interpretation of this phenomenon may be given in terms of the Gestalt principles of proximity and similarity, on the one hand, and of contrast on the other. In position, 'what stone will do', 'what reinforced concrete will do', etc. are contiguous segments, marked off as 'segments' by means of contiguous pauses. And in duration, these consecutive pauses, or tentative juncturee are very much similartiflowever, they become the more similar, when the terminal juncture after “will do' occurs, presenting a significant contrast in the form of a markedly dissimilar pause. In the particular utterance being examined, other aspects of similarity may be noted: for instance, the intervals between tentative junctures are almost equal in length, varying between 5 and 6 words each. Also the segments between tentative junctures are almost identical in structure. Although these aspects may be viewed as contributing to contouring relationships of grouping and separation, yet they do not pertain to juncture cues as such. 1 look at the juncture pattern for Entry 10 may help clarify this and further illustrations. 9ws} Gwsfi Ewe? Ewe? Ewe? ewe; 9wsy' 9wsg "flab It is suggested that the occurrence of B, while separating the i-B frame from the forthcoming B-c frame, actually 36 gives more unity and cohesion to the segments i-Bl, 2, 3, etc. By the same token, when 0 occurs, it would potentially set the 3-0 frame apart, while furthering the unity, not only of 3-01 and 3-08, but also that of the whole B-C frame with the A-B frame. Similarity of duration between B and c as terminal junctures, coupled with their relative proximity could be some of the underlying factors. 9"} 6'8}; 8's? 9"; Eve 94"? 1mg maul D # 3'3# The above terminal juncture pattern of Entry 13 would probably give a better idea of the possible effect on contouring of the distance between these junctures. Judging by the same criterion of contiguity, it would appear that 3-0 and 0-D have a strong tendency to unite. Regardless of the content of these segments, the sheer structural phenomenon is borne out by experiments with metronome beats distributed over longer and shorter distances. It may be noted in addition that, in terms of progression, the distance between terminal juncturee tends to get shorter, as the speaker moves on to conclude his point. It is not clear as yet, if this is more or less generally the case, or if it is 37 characteristic of the style of individual speakers in argument. In any event, the observation is true of both patterns here. With regard to the operation of tentative junctures in their assumed capacity to signal both separation and grouping relationships, one would probably find an exact replica of what happens in the case of terminal junctures. only on a smaller scale. The same principle of contrast may account for the separation of frames or segments between points of tentative juncture, while the same notions of contiguity and similarity may explain word grouping within these segments. For example, in the A-B frame of Entry 13, the components of the i-Bl segment, already sharing a rough similarity of spacing, together with proximity in sequence, would, upon the occurrence of juncture 1, become the more similar and the more contiguous, while getting separated from the forthcoming components of segmentwd-Bz. Considering intonation in the presumed scheme of joint operation with juncture, the general current of speech is supposed to flow at pitch level 3, according to the Pike system of notation. From there, glides take place, up to levels 2 and 1, and down to level 4. Presumably, any departure from level 3, whether up or down, is in a sense, a signal of separation, unless a glide back ensues. Similarly, any return or glide back 38 to level 3 from up or down is, in a sense, a signal of grouping or unification. Meanwhile, recurrence of the same contour is in itself a signal of grouping from similarity. The data being examined shows three types of intonation contours:/-2-3, 4-3, and ~2-4./ The /-2-3/ and /—4-3/ contours glide both ways up and down, but they do return to level 3. As such, they seem to indicate temporary separation from the main current, followed by a junction. These are the contours observed to accompany the tentative junctures. On the other hand, the /-2-4/ contour observed to accompany terminal junctures in both entries, seems to represent departure down to level 4, with no 'apparent' return to level 3, or rather, a stronger type of separation. But as the stream of speech flows on, the decoder is made aware that such a separation is only momentary, though for a comparatively longer duration than in the case of the other two contours. To sum up, as far as evidence goes, the foregoing descriptive account of the synchronous operation of junctures and intonation contours in their assumed capacity for contouring could be suggestive of empirical ways whereby the assumption may be verified in field research.and utilised in application. 39 The second assumption to be examined in this chapter has to do with the predictability of junctures, as verbal cues of organizational information. It is considered worthmhile to find out how far a decoder's expectations of the occurrence of junctures may be 'forcseen'. If the Gestalt principles of perceptual organization may be accepted as adequate 'grammatical' descriptions of how juncture-and-intonation cues help arouse in a decoder the response of lumping and dividing speech items upon their reception, these psychological principles alone are not enough to explain the contouring phenomenon in language perception. The question now arises, whether the occurrence of juncture, or rather, its presumed operation in the encoding-decoding process is grammatical also in the purely linguistic sense, that may be discussed in terms of subject-predicate relationships, modifier-modified relationships, etc. It is reasonable to suppose that the relationships aroused by the verbal cues in question are grammatical, both in the broad ccnfigurational sense, encompassing such.notione as similarity, contiguity, contrast, etc., and in the strict sense of linguistic structure, based on the notion of immediate constituents. The presented examination.of assumptions I and II deals with juncture cues in this double frame of reference respectively. 1e 2e 40 Assumption II In order for junctures to operate as organizational cues of contouring, they must fall at syntactic points of structure. Identification of Cues: Juncture and Corresponding 10 En (Entry 1., Announcer) Early in the discussion / Ll John Ciardi had made the statement / L2 that a rhythm is a way of knowing something # Asked to elaborate on what he meant / 1.1 Hr. Oiardi said / L2 Example: ( L2) L1 L2 Asked to elaborate/ on what he meant/fur. Oiardi said/[... 2 L1 L1 (Entry 19., Neutra) Well technology lid to-dzy which envelope us / L3 in such a terrible fashion / 1.1 has produced / Ll unbearable situations / L4 biologically unbearable situations in our community /L2 in our cities # We have been so in love / L2 and the technicians / 1.5 and the technologists / L4 41 and also the architects with materials / L1 that it was overlooked inst man the consumer / L3 is the greatest subject # To understand his responses and reactions I say / L1 is the thing # (Entry 29., Ciardi) And..and..it's..it's..it's an experience # It says / L1. , 3 in making this / 1.2 ‘I live / L3 better / L4 more richly / L4 more truly / L4 than 1 do / L5 by not making thi' # Whether it means anything or not / Ll I'm more alive in the process # As Robert Frost said / L2 a while back / Ll a poem / L2 is a momentary stay / L3 against confusion # You can't get clarified to stay so # Uh you name think that # You have to do it all over again # 42 But for a minute / Ll the poem clarifies a thing # It uh it's an experience or life / L1 caught # I think that is the subject of all art # 3. Interpretation ‘ In order to find out how far the given Junctures tally with syntactic points of structure, the corresponding Ic pattern is sketched in every case, as shown by the example in Entry 1. 21.12.22 corrflspondingfIc Patterns Entry Patterns 1 # 1-2 # # 1-2 # 19 # 4-3-1-3-4-2 # # 2-5-4-1-3 # # 1 # 29 # 1-2-3-4-4-4-5 # # 1 # # 2-1-2-3 # # 1 # # 1 # 43 The patterns for Entries l.and 29.share one characteristic, which Entry 19 does not have. It is the regularity of the hierarchical structure of constituents. The levels would normally take a regular ascending order ( # l-2-3-, etc. # ), a regular descending order ( # 3-2-1 # ), or'both. It would appear that the obvious irregularity of patterns in Entry 19 may give a more or less accurate picture of a Jerky way of speaking, where pauses are made at various levels of the syntactic structure, irrespective of the regularity of gradation. So, although Junctures may fall at syntactic points of structure, yet, when the hierarchy of levels gets shuffled, as it does in Entry 19, the decoder is furnished with such combinations as 'well technology‘, 'to-dsy which envelops us', 'architects with materials', etc. In the light of such evidence, it would seem reasonable to modify the original assumption through the additional specification of the feature of regularity pertaining to the hierarchical order of linguistic structure. Without this particular characteristic, Juncture could not possibly perform its function, as a one of organisational information. It could sometimes introduce into the ccmmmnication situation more entropy than the ordinary hesitation pause: for such entropy would be associated with the ambiguity of 'queer' combinations. 44 anaesther possible modification of the assumption may have to do with the range of levels at which a speaker would normally be able to manipulate Juncture cues with a minimum of entropy for the decoder in oral interpersch communication. It would appear that manipulation of such cues above the third level of structure would require adequate support from emphatic stress and pitch variations, or else run the risk of breaking up the unity and continuity of the structure of content in the decoding process. Entry 29 gives an excellent example of a successful manipulation of Juncture ones at the 4th and 6th levels in the pattern # 1-2-3-4-4-4—5 # . K Above all, however, the notion of a common context to be shared by both encoder and decoder would assume importance here. the occurrence of Juncture would be predictable, if it fulfilled the two conditions of the regularity of order, and the range of operation. However, it would be more predictable, if these two individuals shared approximately the same degree of assimilation of the language structure. It has been demonstrated through the foregoing examination of Assumption 11 how encoders differ in their manipulation of Juncture cues, and how the decoding process is affected by such a difference with respect to the organisation of items at the projection level. 45 ' Assuming that the difference touches the encoder in terms of manipulation, and the decoder in terms of prediction and subsequent operation, it would be understandable to think of contouring as 'given' and of contouring as 'expected', not as a presentation of the same or similar patterns of organisation in every instance, but of characteristically different patterns at times. In short, the notion of 'relativity' seems impelling in this connectimn. fhere is reason to believe that while the grammatical base for Juncture is an actuality with language users, differences will keep occurring between source and receiver, depending on the factors already noted and discussed. If an encoder chose to place his junctures at points a, b, and d, while the decoder expected them at points b, e, and f, there would be difference between 'given' and 'expected'. To the extent that difference existed, the organisation.of content units at this level with the help of such cues would be affected. 1. Assumption III the organisation value of Juncture cues depends on the amount of agreement between contouring as 'given! and contouring as 'expeeted‘. 2. identification of Cues Coupled Juncture and 10 patterns for both 'given' and 'expected'. 46 (Entry 3., Announcer) 9.41.9.9. Conflict with John Ciardi's point of view / L1 was provided by Hillard Sheets # Responding to the question / L2 Is the big idea / L3 more important than concern with the actual material medium of the painter / Ll Mr. Sheets said /L2 Expected Conflict with John Ciardi's point of view / L1 was provided by Millard Sheets # Responding to the question / L2 Is the big idea / 1.5 more important / Le than concern with the actual material medium of the p./ 1.1 Mr. Sheets said / L2 (Entry 21., Announcer) 9.1-1.1! A question which seemed for a moment to go unanswered/ Ll provided one of the best expressions / L2 of the view point of Millard Sheets # the panel was asked what they meant by the word art / 1.1 and whether they are not in fact talking about several different things / L2 when they used the word art # 47 galacted ‘ A question / L2 thioh.seemed for a moment to go unanswered / Ll provided one of the best expressions / L2 of the view point of Millard Sheets # the panel was asked what they meant by the word art / L1 and whether they were not in fact talking about several different things / L2 when they used the word art # (Entry 11., Neutra) aim Most assuredly I have to know first the human beings / L2 Before I know I‘Ld what a.. cold wet stone will do/is do to my.. skin where I have through.shich.1 have heat losses # the biological side of it / L3 and the empathic side of it / 1.2 understanding human beings / L3 and systematically understanding them / Lt as well as empathically understanding them / L1 is the premise # to know / L1 1 what to do about stone steel and any other thing # I have to understand physiological optics before I know how to use glass or mirrors and so forth # 48 Eco ted Most assuredly / Ll I have to know first the human beings / L2 before I know what a cold witsstnme will do to my skin/L3 where I have (through which I have) heat losses # ' fhe biological side of it / L3 and the empathic side of it / L2 understanding human beings and systematically understanding them as well as empathically understanding them / Ll is the premise / L2 to know what to do about stone steel and any other thing # I have to understand physiological optics 1(8an .2- before I know how to use glass or mirrors and so forth # 3. Interpretation ' rhe following table shows the result of a oongruency test in which given and expected patterns are compared. In either case, every Juncture point is tagged by the I0 levels at which it occurs. TABLE Comuencz Test W Entry Given Pattern Expected Pattern s # l # # 1 # # 2-3-1-2 # # z~5~4-l-2 # 49 TABLE Ooggguencz fest Entryt Given Pattern Expected Pattern 21 # 1-2 # # 3-1-2 # # 1-2 # # 1-8 # ll # 2-4-6 # # 1-2-3 # # s-s-s-e-l # # 3-2-1-2 # # 1 # # 1 # The comparison between given and expected patterns shows 1 discrepancy for Entry 3, l for Entry 21, and 7 for Entry 11. In terms of a simple numerical calculation, it is easy to see that for the deceder (here the present investigator), the organisation value of juncture cues employed in both.Entries 3 and 21 must be higher than that of uses in Entry 11. Especially as the total number of words for the entries in question is approximately the same on either side of the comparison, it would be tempting to compute a ratio of organisation value for employed cues, and the results would look more or less satisfactory. 50 However, it is not so much.the number of discrepancies between given and expected patterns that really counts, as it is the type of such discrepancies. In the foregoing interpretation of observations connected with assumption II, it has been pointed out that variability with regard to the operation of these verbal cues may be viewed in terms of: a) the regularity of transition from level to level, b) the range of juncture location between the two poles of Immediate and Ultimate constituents, and o) the degree of assimilation of a language structure for both encoder and decoder. . Accordingly, the one discrepancy noted in Entry 3 would not affect contouring in any drastic way, especially as both given and expected patterns are, otherwise, identical. The decoder would be more satisfied, if one more juncture point were added at level 4: in other’wurds, if the range were a little wider to suit an utterance carrying so mueh.content weight..Almost the same thing may be said with regard to the one point of disagreement in Entry 21. fhe added juncture, however, is not out of the range already covered: it is of the nature of adding one more stroke to a finished contour. In contrast with these points of disagreement are the ones noted in comparing 'given! and 'expected' for Entry 11. These are, for the most part, connected with the observed irregularity of transition from 51 level to level. Also there is a lack of balance in. contour delineation caused by a lop-sided distribution of Junctures, as may be exemplified by the utterance starting, 'The biological side of it,’ etc. By the time level 1 is reached, three more words are given, followed by a semi-terminal Juncture, which seems to create uncertainty that could have been avoided. In brief, the evaluation of Juncture cues in terms of the proposed comparison between given and expected patterns would seem plausible. However, in a field study situation, the quantitative side of it would have to be developed in the light of specific types of discrepancies. GHAPTER III ACCENTULIION CUES While contouring is described as a perceptual operation.ccnnected*with the establishment among content units, as represented by *words', of relationships having to do with.the simultaneous occurrence of grouping and separation, accentuation may be operationally defined as a presumed tendency on the part of a decoder to respond to certain verbal cues, which.signal two other structural features of simultaneous occurrence, namely, projection and suppression of items. It would be impossible to think of the relative prominence of certain items in a speech.continuum, without the concurrent levelling of other items in the vicinity. There could be no projection without suppression. It is assumed that word stress, as distinguished from syllable stress or accent, is a basic verbal cue to this particular operation. Other prosodic features, such.as intonation and juncture, do get into the picture. However, the present analysis is concerned only with ‘word stress, as a chief variable affecting the prominence and suppression of items, and with Juncture as a concomitant variable. For the purpose of this auditory 52 53 analysis, word stress stands for two main types: a) normal , which is the more common types of 'sentence' stress, and b) emphatic or peak stress *, wish is the strongest type that may be distinguished among all the rest. 1. 2. Assumption I Stress and iuncture operate 'iointly'- as once for the simultaneous proJeotion and suppression of items, as represented by the words of an utterance. identification of Cues ‘ (Entry 15., Oisrdi) 339.}; all you're 335135 is that / oh when you say you want a £222. to have M / or an architect to have M / or an 29.2.91 to have M / all: you're really _ag_l_z_i_ng is that he be a 9.2959. being} oh and _I_°don't think that's 3.2.1 much to grit the th- 222.221 # ‘ uh I don't care gighe £1 / the'ohances are he's a 9.9.2.9. 2125 # If he's a 93;}; human being / he'll say 393}; things about being 1.3293 / hewill have 531;; experiences in his art # If he's a lag-go; human being / he will have m ones / but the only way he 9;; have an experience in his art form is technical # 54 But oh you see that's that's the dirty word # One thinks immediately / * empty correctness # as Uh.that's the .. not it at all # It's the way of 52335 # The lggggggg_must be spoken # TABLE I. Accentuatcd Units Stress only Stress and Juncturc asking Really ideas poet ideas architect ideas artist human being asking all artist too I is grnat I human being who human chances are small technical small dirty word small immediately larger empty correctness larger all only going can spoken language 55 W Content Units Major Minor poet architect ideas human being asking artist ideas human being say he ideas human being asking artist things being human he experiences human being think he once thinks experiences you technical you small dirty word I small empty correctness ? I small way of going you larger language - spoken one larger art art form 3. Interpretation ill stressed units are supposed to have one thing in common, though, of course, in varying degrees. They all represent variations in intensity, compared 56 with other units in the vicinity. Whether pre-Junctural, post-Jonltural, or medial, these stressed units represent points of prominence, against a more or less levelled background. Without these points, the whole configuration of content units would be vague, and sometimes ambiguous. In the utterance, "all you're really m is that he be a human 11155," the stressed units are respectively, a function word and two content units, one minor and one major. the major unit, receiving the emphatic stress, happens to be a key unit, upon which the whole argument is based. It is the only unit repeaud four times without change or modification. The minor unit, within the larger context of the whole symposium, is not actually minor. There is a conflict between members of the panel, as may be seen in the form of asking and answering. is for the function word given prominence in this utterance, it is the most significant in the whole frame. The speaker makes use of it to give point to his argument. In order for a decoder to give due attention to such words, it is necessary that they should be distinguished in a fashion, from the other words in the background. In terms of the Gestalt principle of figure-on-ground, when part a of a pattern is presented with more intensity than parts b, o, and d, part a will be perceived as a figure. It is not difficult to find out empirically that stressed units in a speech continuum are the 57 only units to be distinguished against a background of noise that could drown almost everything else. When stressed units happen to be pre- or post- junctural, their prominence from increased intensity, ‘would, through the additional effect of gradually arrested movement, or of gradually started or resumed movement, be given a more clear-cut form against the 'levelled' background. This appears to be very much.the case with words like 'ideas', 'human being', 'technical', 'all', etc. The stress responded to under the circumstances is characterised by a more extended rendition of the syllable carrying it. This would probably increase the attention value of such.a cue, particularly if in terms of intensity, the stress happens to be of the peak type, with respect to the entire utterance. in example of this is: "Uh.I_don't care ions is / the chances are he's a M M" So much for the ccnfigurational aspect of the Joint operation of stress and Junctore, as may be viewed from an examination of this portion of data. Quantitatively, stressed units are 3? in all: 17 of these are pre-Junctural: d are post-Junctural; and 16 are of medial occurrence in their respective utterances. It is quite obvious that a substantial number of atrgaggd words occur medially, irrespective of Juncture points. Naturally, the aim of the original assumption is not 58 to prove or disprove the joint occurrence of stress and juncture as such, but rather to find out how far the enquiry could be pursued on the basis of such a count in terms of major and minor contmut mnits. Stress and intonation could also be profitably investigated in terms of a joint partnership along these lines. Judging by evidence from the analysis of Entry 16, the assumption being examined here could be tentatively qualified in the light of findings from Tables 1, and 2. The table of accentuated units shows which.of these are differentiated by stress, in the main: 'only' is not literally meant. It also shows which of these are additionally differentiated by juncture. The table of content units is prepared with a view to discovering which of the total number of 37 units belong to the major category, and which.belong to the minor one. Comparing the information provided by these two tables, one finds that out of 19 units differmntiated by means of both stress and juncture cues, 16 may be identified as major content units. Therefore, it is probable that one way of qualifying the original assumption is to say that "stress and juncture operate jointly, when they are applied to words of major content items." This is very much open to question, however: further investigation is needed. 59 1. Assumption II 2. content units differentiated through the combined operation of stress and juncture appear to be generally more 'significant' than those projected by stress in a medial position. Identification of Cues (Entry 15., Stevenson) I think that Mr. mm the..the rig}; point of 2;! # I can't ccno:ive of the kind of music that would interest gone / without absolute control of materials first # (Entry 18., Ciardi) But I'm saying what's wrong with having a technical meion # I that's wrong with .1533; your 221%! # What‘s wrong with believing that the only way you can get mrtssed what you _a_re__ / is through your M3 # mm 1 isosntuated Units Stress Stress and Juncture Ciardi view right anyone eonoeive absolute first 60 TABLE 1 Accentuated Units W Stress Stress and Juncture loving technical-passion expressed medium are medium EABLB 2 content Units m Major linor point of view’ 1 think conceive anyone materials first Oiardi technical passion saying medium loving medium getting expressed I you wrong you wrong wrong 61 3. Interpretation ‘ The notion of 'siginificance'secms to suggest at least two main issues in this context: a) whether such.units are significantly 'looated' in the utterance; whether by virtue of their positions, they are likely to call the decoder's attention more "20381:: . b) whether such.anits belong to a major content category. These two issues are touched upon briefly in this study. A third issue, which needs a much more thorough investigation in field research, has to do with the possible correlation between these two features of significance in the study of individual style. lith regard to the attention value of units located in the pre-junctural position, the table of accentuated units seems to indicate that in 2 cases out of 7, these units seem to receive the highest stress. Therefore, if they de call attention mere than stressed units in the medial position, then intensity alone as a field property would not explain the phenomenon. It is probable that the change of movement at points of juncture, coupled with the accompanying variations of pitdh level, might account in part for the assumed significance of this location. According to the Gestalt principle of common movement, units may be viewed in separation, if they move toward or away from each other. The notion of separation is involved in the 62 operation of both juncture and intonation as concomitant variables. In the case of stressed units before terminal junctures, it is true that the anticipated drop to level 4 - with words like 'view' and 'medium', would cause a stress to gather additional intensity. Meanwhile, it is quite probable that the down-glide, signalling a separation from pitch level 3, followed by a temporary arrest of movement, should contribute to the attention value of these units . The question of whether the significance of such units has to do with their belongingness to major content categories may be tentatively answered from Table 2. There is some indication that the assumption is well worth investigation. The element of choice may have been gleaned through the initial quantitative enquiry into how much of the total of major content units receives accentuation or differentiation, from more than one set of coca. The implication.of this enquiry is that the distinction between major and minor content units, supposedly operating with both encoder and decoder, seems to underscore the notion of relativity. While single or double suing is relative to the encoder‘s awareness of such a distinction, the amount of attention given " by the decoder is also relative , partly to his awareness of such a distinction, and partly to his awareness of such.cues, as physical phenomena, to say nothing of motivational factors. The picture is even much.more complex; what is relevant to this investigation however, is the notion of 'seleotion' for the decoder. Accordingly, it would be meaningful to consider two sides of a comparison between what is 'given' in terms of accentuated units, and what is 'expected', and to find out what the relationship between these two could mean with respect to this feature of organisation. What is 'given' is available for empirical checks. What is 1expected', however, may be difficult to determine with unquestionable precision. The nearest approximation would probablgxfor individuals to study a particular message, silently, outside the communication situation, with the purpose of locating the central content units, or those that they consider to be essential for understanding the message in question. It is assumed that sueh units would be the expected loci of accentuation. ' Accordingly, the expected pattern would vary from one individual to another. This variability is the feature that could be utilised in determining the organisation value of these cues (stress cues) at this level of language perception. 1e 2e 64- Assumption III The organisational value of 'normal' and 'emphatic' types of stress depends on the amount of agreement- between accentuation as 'given' and accentuation as 'expected'. Identification of Coca - (Entry 9., Neutra) The (material) was put(§hgg§)of everything gigg_# And it is g (considered opinion) / and so I can (gggily) say and beyond (suspicion) / that I haven't gggg_ new (materials) in a (922$25) :21 /’ I would say that all.these (material) (considerations are (231) much in (2.9.0.29) Min gm# I (do) think that the (best) (material) the architect ever gets under his (25953) / are the (human bgiggg) / who are his (clients) # And oh to understand (human.§giggg) / to be (fascinated) by understanding their (responsiveness) their (responses) their(reactions) / is the (g_r_e_a_t_) (gt) of structuring an(envircnment) and arranging (stimuli) 325 than # Given stress: Expected stress: ( ) 65 (Entry 12., Houghton) It would (appear) / (off’hggg) / that this (panel) is moving in that (age-old) discussion of (3333) versus (content) / over on to the side of (£25!) # And uh let's not sorry about whether we're saying (anythingfat (£13) # ' The (important) thing is the (m) the (rhythm) the (32523) the (;;22) # Uh and ;,would like to come (£395) to the support of (MP point which was that / (£9.12) and (m) and (21152;) and (line) in (themselves) are (£239 enough.# And that (somehow) / there must b_e_ the (balance) of £13 (£033) and (m) and (52132!) and (iggg) / addressed to (something) that relates to (human.bgiggg) # 3. interpretation While accentuation as 'given' is denoted by the underlined stressed words, accentuation as 'expected' is provided through identification of the words supposed to carry special weight in terms of content, which would entitle them to a characteristic prominence among other words in each utterance. These sspposedly prominent words are denoted by means of brackets. 66 Congruency Test: For each of the two entries, a comparison is made between given and expected patterns, and the organisation value of stress cues is computed as follows: a is the total number of points or differentiated units, both given and expected. 933531. is the number of points in agreement between the two patterns. Organisation value is presumed to be a fraction of Congrs./ N. The result shows a ratio of 23/59 for Entry 9., as against 27/36 for Entry 12. How significant such a result may be is hard to tell now. However, this line of enquiry appears to be promising. CHAPTER IV TRANSITION CUES Analysis at the 'projection' level of message decoding has been conducted in terms of the two presumed Operations of 'contouring' and 'accentuation'. At this level, the organization of message content with the help of the prosodic features of speech is viewed as a gross structuring of units in the aggregate. The only relation- ships under investigation have been those of grouping versus separation, and projection versus suppression. At the 'integration' level of message decoding, however, organisation of content is viewed in terms of its more diverse relationships, which may be subsumed under three main categories: transition, combination, and integration. Naturally, the overlapping of such relationships, both among themselves and with other relationships at the 'representational' level, would be inevitable. But it would thereby suggest the essential unity of the process of language perception. There could be no transition without combination of items, and there could be no integration without consistency among the various combinations in view of the constant need to substitute, modify, and amplify content units. 67 68 While integration has to do with.the aspect of 'cohesion' among content units, transition is concerned with that of 'progression'. There is a presumedtendency on the part of a decoder to view events in a sequence, as proceeding from one point to another. Such a tendency would probably underlie the questions: "Where do we go from here? What next?" etc. The concept of motion is basic to transition. Verbal cues supposed to elicit this category of organisational information, though numerous, cannot be heterogeneous. The operation of these cues represents one form of speech.dynamics, one form of human behavior, which would naturally be expected to have some basic pattern. Consequently, it is considered pertinent in the present investigation to attempt discovery of such a pattern. Linguistic research, having been so far confined to utterance or sentence limits, and guarded against the study of content, could not possibly give a more or less complete picture of an organisational feature like transition, which.operates in a wider frame of reference. Howsver, because form and content are inseparable, linguistic research must have given at least one side of the picture, namely the syntactic. Still, the notion of syntax within utterance or sentence limits is incomplete. In the present enquiry, it is extended to cover relationships between utterances and treated within a communication frame of reference. 69 If the linguistic unit performing the function of transition from one sentence or utterance to the next happens to be a 'content' unit, and not a 'function' word, it is considered logical, from a communication point of view, to treat such a unit as a function unit, and accept it as a syntactic cue. There is no sense in studying syntax in terms of individual utterances. And if people can proceed from one utterance or sentence to the next, using units of speech other than the so-called 'function' words, these units, no doubt, fulfil a syntactic function. Any hesitation in accepting this premise would mean denial of the way language operates in communication. 1. 'iseumption I Transition in message decoding is effected partly through the operation of 'syntactic' cues, and partly through that of 'contextual' cues. 2. Identification of Cues (Entry 4., Sheets) TABLE 1 §yntactic Cues Reference Cues Shift Cues Putting it another way And I think But by the same token It seems to me But I believe 70 TABLE 1 Syntactic Cues Reference Cues Shift Cues I think I believe I don't think 1 think And I think TABLE 2 Contextual Cues Differentiated Cue Clusters greatest art have advanced great arts aesthetic understanding great ideas organisation great art painting... a complete unit ideas accords of color human values handling space great ideas texture values ideas technical building progress TABIE 2 Contextual Cues MW Differentiated Cue Clusters quantities easier to produce experimental age new vocabulary new power empty art ideas will come back insight power force ideas (Entry 6., Radiman) TABLE 1 Syntactic Cues Reference Cues Shift Cues 72 (Entry 6., Fadiman) W Syntactic Cues _- L _-_ j Reference Cues Shift Cues That And The How They You That What...? But You Most of 'thea' And They now They Now But Really The first And of course But Suppose The first Here The second Here The New The And The Is...? 73 TABLE 2 Contextual Cues Differentiated Cue Clusters 2.42.2 'mdi' ideas poet's ideas summed up prose writer's ideas list 'them' in the words themselves ideas ideas of Shakespeare ideas notions about the world ideas notions ideas as he expresses them ideas 'of' young_novelists of very bad .. novelists ideas ideas rhythms Two dates one date another date lighty seven years ago four score and seven years ago same date the first the second Liam-.0 r and! rhythm 74 W Contextual Cues Differentiated Cue Clusters rhythm that first statement second statement fggt_same statement different statement four score etc. beginning of great idea idea igga.lees operative idea operative eighty seven years ago 5. Interpretation Transition seems to be a product of linkage at points of terminal Juncture and 'elsewhere'. The former type of linkage, which.is the more obvious, is furnished by means of syntactic cues. These cues seem to fall into two major divisions: one of them 'refers' back to something of previous occurrence in speech} the other 'shifts' on to what is forthcoming. Of course both types suggest the onward motion, and both provide the connection between what has gone and what 75 is coming; but this distinction appears to be pertinent especially in a comparison between one style of speech and another. It may also be correlated with particular types of context in speeche This is a point that may be picked up for future research. While Table l for Entry 4 shows §,reference cues as against §_shift cues, Table l for Entry 6 shows l1, as against l§,.Also, the §.cues from.Entry 4 belong to one variety: they introduce amplifications either through similarity or through contrast. "We've advanced ... in raising the level of understanding ... . 'Putting it another way', the organisation of a painting etc." Also, "I think we've made a tremendous progress ... . 'By the same token', I don't think that the things we are producing ... will ever held their place etc." Again, "I think that the experimental age ... will contribute tremendously to the younger painters ... . 'But' it's a pretty empty art ... etc." As for the shift cues in the same Entry, they also belong to one variety without a single exception. That particular variety introduces a 'persunal opinion', or a 'personal conviction'. Eight such cues say: 'I think', 'I believe'. 0n the other hand, Table l for Entry 6 shows other varieties of both reference and shift cues. One class of reference case is for 'definitives' like 'the', 'the first'; another for 'demonstratives' like 'that', etc.: another for 'pronouns': and another for 'contrastive reference', like 'but'. 76 Shift cues for the same Entry consist of these groups of items: emphatic items like 'really', 'and of course': intermission items like 'now', 'here': items inviting audience participation or involvement, like 'you', 'euppose', 'what...?' etc. Rinally, the word 'and' is of particular interest. While it operates here as a 'ehift' cue, it is observed to act elsewhere as a 'combination' cue, serving the function of addition or annexation. However, this double frame of operation is understandable in terms of the inevitable overlapping of organisational phenomena already discussed. The only solution for the practical purpose of analysis would be to make this arbitrary decision: if 'and' is located at the head of a sequence sentence, i.e., following a terminal Juncture (actual or potential), it is automatically considered a 'shift' cue: if it is located anywhere else, it is considered a 'combination' cue. In introducing syntactic cues, riference has been made to those speech items which provide linkage at points of terminal Juncture, as distinguished from those that establish linkage elsewhere. The latter are given the name of 'contextual' cues. Contexm.is supposed to be structured in.the course of speech through a process of 'differentiation' and constant 'reference'. While differentiation reveals the speaker's awareness of the sides of an issue, constant 77 reference indicates his progression. How far he proceeds, or how fast - these are questions pertaining to the degree of transition. The important point to note here is that progression is achieved through a form of redundancy, once the 'primary' cues are given, and the differentiation ee tab lished . Table 2 for Entry 4 shows the differentiation established horisontally by means of two columns: meanwhile, transition is represented vertically down the columns through the occurrence of redundant cues which get attached to the corresponding 'primary' cue (here underlined), as the speaker goes on. In a comparative frame of reference, Sheets speaks of: l) the greatest art of past epochs: 2) the fact that painters 'have advanced': 3) that advancement is recorded in terms or 'quantities': 4) that this art is 'empty' art; and 6) that painters need more 'insight' . I Each of these items is considered a primary use, which is continually reinforced by redundant cues of reference to itself. A speaker could handle two or more primary cues at a time, and have them simultaneously reinforced. That is how he is enabled to proceed with his argument. It may be observed that while some of these rein- forcement cues are so close to the corresponding primary one as to be identical repetitions; others are only slightly related. This is understandable in terms of the 78 concurrent development of the content units initially introduced by the primary cues. Transition from one point of the structure of content to the next is naturally accompanied by some development or amplification of the primary cue, by which the particular point has been previously ushered in. Table 2 for Entry 6 shows the same principles in operation: differentiation horisontally, and transition accompanied by amplification vertically. The obvious difference between the charted sequence here and in Table 2 for Entry 4 is an indication of two styles of treatment. Sheets differentiates between classical and modern art in terms of two primary cues, and moves on from past to present, where he remains to the end of the entry. That is why most of his content units cluster in the second column. Fadiman, on the other hand, holds the two sides of the comparison all the way through. He introduces his two primary cues of 'ideas' versus 'words' almost simultaneosuly, and keeps piling up reinforcements on both sides till he moves up to the climax with the amplification: 'young novelists', 'full of ideas', etc. Then he makes a big shift to the question of rhythm; but there, again, he maintains the same balance of cues ‘with.'one date' here, and 'another date' there: with one having 'rhythm‘ and the other 'no rhythm'. 79 This is one way of exploring a pattern for transition cues. While syntactic cues may be considered explicitly relational, and explicitly transitional, contextual cues are implicitly so. They further progression through the referred to clustering phenomenon, which is explainable in Gestalt terminology. In a sense, every reinforcement one must be 'eimilar' to the primary cue to which it belongs. And the more 'contiguous' it is to the primary cue, or to the preceding reinforcement cue, the more operative it is likely to be in achieving unity with the rest of the cluster. Curiously enough, although syntactic cues are the more obvious, they seem to be the less operative as tools of transition. A simple test would be to remove them completely from the message under study, and observe the results. Progression could be effected through the operation of contextual cues alone. But this is not the way people speak; people use, and will go on using 'reference' cues and 'shift' cues, and any serious displacement of these could affect comprehension of their messages. Experiments along these lines might be both interesting and profitable. 80 1. Assumptionggg The pattern of transition cues, as manipulated by individual communicatiors, seems to show two interlocking frames: a macro-structure and a micro-structure. 2. Identification of Cues ’ (Entry 2., Ciardi) TABIE 1 Syntactic Cues Reference Cues Shift Cues It A while back He Then It Now That How...? Something And That You It You It You Whether What It There One of the There It In It In That May...? It What...? ' 81 TABLE 1 Syntactic Cues Reference Cues Shift Cues That What...? It Because And TABLE 2 Contextual Cues Differentiated Cue Clusters R. Frost .. put it rhythm said way of knowing said something... wave length said something... way it goes had to say something... primitive rhythm (English Language) 29.!!! ee page misged gay of going know have missed meter of the race know aiming at iambic know have missed other languages, other ways know' have hit kind of beat 82 TABLE 2 Contextual Cues Differentiated Cue Clusters know have missed rhythmic (sequences) know visual know series of lines know in time know series of sounds know know (structure) in rhythm structure way of forming bringing to shape basic to form rhythm (way of knowing) way of knowing .. ourselves human behavior .. poem doing .. writes.. poem doing .. receives poem poem.nevsr idea .. aims . . experience what idea feels like TABLE 2 Contextual Cues m Differentiated Cue Clusters live in it experience experience (pass) rate (rhythm)..unfolding rhythm wave length (way .. testing) way one knows TABLE 3 Macro-Structure Transition Frames 1. I was interviewing Robert Frost ... 2. a. put it 5. He said ... 4. Then.he said ... 5. What he had to say.nexm ... 84 TABLE 5 Macro-Structure W Transition Frames 6. That's your way of knowing it. 7. One of the questions I'd like to raise ... 8. But I want to say very emphatically ... 5. lgtggpretation The distinction already made between syntactic and contextual cues serving the function of transition, coupled with the tentative discovery that such a function appears to be largely fulfilled through the operation of the latter type of cues - have suggested further investigation into what basically affects a decoder's comprehension of the sequence of events. If contextual cues appear to be so operative in revealing sequential progression, where in particular do they exercise maximum power? One's immediate impression is that probably the notion of a 'macrc-structure' might offer a solution. If the big land-marks are visible, if the various turns and points of Junction are held in view, it is very likely that one should find one's way about, without hitting blind alleys . 85 The problem would then be how to identify those big land-marks which make up the macro-structure. One way would be to chart all differentiated cue clusters, and to mark primary cues, as is shown in Table 2. Presumably when all primary cues are identified, there will be nothing else to look for. These will be the constituents of the macro-structure, without which the decoder would be at a loss, regarding the sequence of events. If for any reason, he were to become momentarily inattentive and miss these particular cues, the chances are that he would lose points of significance in terms of sequence. This is not to say, however, that all primary cues are of equal significance. According to Table a, rhythmic 'pace' is reinforced only once, but to 'kncw‘ whether one has hit or missed something, through-one's awareness of rhythmic sequences, gets the heaviest cluster of l; reinforcemeut cues. Should the decoder miss as many as §Dcr even 2_ of these, he would be able to follow the presented sequence nontheless. But if he were to miss the name of Robert Frost or subsequent reference to his name, he would not only miss the sequence of presentation at this point, but also the whole idea of source credibility attached to the better known.imerican poet. Another way of identifying constituents of the transition macro-structure may be to obtain communal fl-_ 36 Judgement on the matter from various decoders, and study the results. Table 3 shows the investigator's own impression of the minimum number of transition frames, without which the decoder could miss the sequence at one point or another. Frames 1-5 are essential for identifying the source of quoted information. Frame 3; signals a start for Ciardi's own personal reactions to achieve independence of what Robert Frost said or thought. Frame 1’signals a shift to the next major point, namely what sort of human behavior a poem is. Frame §_signals his conclusion that a poem is never about an idea, but always about the experience of an idea. Tables 2 and 3 would make it possible to compare the resultant macro-structures identified in these two different ways: the quantitative and the impressionistic. Table 2 Table 3 R. Frost .. put it 1. A while back I was ... know .. have missed 2. He put it ... 3. He said ... 4. Then he said ... 5. What he had to say next .. ghyjhg, 6. That's your way of knowing.. human behavior .. 2232? 7. One of the questions ... idea... experience 8. But I want to say ... 87 The fact that in either case the decoder is the analyst would certainly be expected to affect the results. However, the assumption regarding the correspondence between constituents of the macrostructure and maJor content units seems to be Justified. According to Table 2, primary cues are no other than the maJor content units of the Entry. Therefore, it would be safer to employ the quantitative method of identifying the macro-structure, as represented by Table 2. As for the micro-structure, Which has no separate existence per se, probably two things may be said about it in brief. First, it would naturally be expected to merge into the macro-structure at every point, both syntactically and contextually. Second, the distinction between 'macro' and 'micrc' is relative. In Gestalt terminology, one thinks of the inter- dependence cf action of the 'parts' within the 'whole'. Therefore, it is possible to think of the interlocking frames as a number of interJacent circles, ranging from the smallest to the largest. This would probably explain the position of the cues put between brackets in Table 2, as distinguished from the underlined.cues on the one hand, and from any of the reinforcement cues on the other hand. Finally, concerning the effect on comprehension of the presence or absence of a macro-structure, it 88 may be tested through total or partial removal of its constituents, followed by comparison of results for various decoders. The underlying assumption is that predictability of forthcoming items in a particular order is calculated in terms of macro-structure units. For instance, when Ciardi starts by quoting Robert Frost, the chances are that when he moves on to discuss rhythm, the motion is more or less expected: and when his discussion is concluded by the statement 'It's a way of knowing something,‘ the conclusion is more or less foreseen, because it is made precisely in terms of_the initial remark quoted from Robert Frost. In other words, once the decoder is let in on the gradual unfolding of the 'big frame', his expectations of items in the 'followbthrough' would be more or less accurate. 1. Assumption III In interpersonal communication of the group situation, the total frame of transition is revealed through an exchange of cues, occurring with the change of speakers in succession. 2. Identification of Cues 89 TaamullA) Syntactic Cues Panel Entry Reference Cues Shift Cues Announcer l Giardi 2 A while back Announcer 3 Sheets 4 They Adams 5 radiman 6 I Just want Adams 8 ’ Neutra 9 Well Adams 14. Stevens on 15 I think Adams 17 ' Ciardi 18 But Announcer 21 Sheets 22 I think Adams 26 Ciardi 27 I think Announcer 32 ' Oiardi 33 No 90 TABIE 1(3) Syntactic Cues yr Panel Entry Reference Cues Shift Cues Fadiman 6 Ciardi 7 That Neutra 9 Ciardi 10 May...? Ciardi 10 Neutra 11 Most assuredly Neutra ll Roughtcn 12 It would appear Heughton 12 Ciardi 13 I'd like ... Stevenson l5 Ciardi 16 Really Ciardi 18 Neutra 19 Well Neutra 19 Ciardi 20 Absolutely Sheets 22 Ciardi 23 But that Ciardi 23 Sheets 24 That 91 TABLE 1(B) Syntactic Cues Panel Entry Reference Cues Shift Cues Sheets 24 Ciardi 25 I mean Ciardi 27 Sheets 28 That Sheets 28 Ciardi 29 It Ciardi 29 Sheets 30 I think Sheets 30 Ciardi 31 I think W Contextual Cues Entry Differentiated Cues 1 , Ciardi rhythm way of knowing something 2 Robert Frost 2 rhythm way of knowing something 92 TABLE 2 Contextual Cues Entry Differentiated Cues 3. conflict with Ciardi Millard Sheets big idea material medium 4 both important 4 ideas will come back into art 5 (Adams) 6 anecdote 6 idea operative what.. meant, John 7 ,part of it 7 human feeling technical passion 8 (Adams) 9 material of architect 9 once eternal issues 10 question 10 materials second place? 11 human beings first fault of F. Avenue renewal ll fault of P. Avenue renewal 12 panel moving side of form 12 content along with form 93 W Contextual Cues I L Entry Differentiated Cues 13 question .. W. B. Yeats l3 dancer dance 14 (Adams) 15 Ciardi right 15 materials first 15 have ideas being human 16 language spoken 1? (Adams) lightning bugs have passion 18 technical passion wrong? 18 getting expressed through medium 19 technology envelope us 19 understand human the thing 20 saying same thing 20 human within form 21 question art? several different things M. Sheets 94 TABLE 2 Contextual Cues W Entry' Differentiated Cues 22 question pertinent 22 poetry and music two different arts problem 23 nonsense 24 m 25 mean remark 25 parable? 26 (Adams) 27 may have something to say 27 It says: I make this 28 idea! ‘ 29 experience 29 subJect of all art 30 great idea: 31 greater experience 32 write a poem convey idea? 33 not an idea an experience 33 ideas .. textbooks 95 3. Interpretation While Assumption I treats transition cues in terms of categories, Assumptions II and III consider the frames within which.they operate. Speaking of a 'macro-structure' with reference to Entries, or individual units of discourse, would naturally suggest an examination of the 'total frame' of transition in a group communication situation. i To a certain extent, the total frame of transition for the recorded excerpts which constitute the corpus material of this study, is in a way 'meddled with' by virtue of the nature of 'editing', even if it consisted of nothing more than putting excerpts together, providing the most essential links in between. In other words, the 'presented' sequence is slightly different from the 'actual' sequence here and there. However, the fact remains that it 'is' a sequence calculated to give the transition cues necessary for one's perception of the total frame. Such.aframe is therefore examined with this understanding of the circumstances attending its proJectimn. A general look at the transition cues occurring with the change of speakers shows the two maJor types of syntactic cues previously identified and discussed in the presented examination of Assumption I. 96 What is interesting to note, however, is the almost systematic occurrence of only shift cues, when the turn is between Adams, the moderator, and any of the speakers (Table 1A), and of both reference and shift cues, when the turn is between members of the panel engaged in the conduct of the discussion (Table 13). The obvious reason is that Adams says nothing beyond TMr. So-and-so wants to speak," thereby breaking the 'referential' sequence, and making it necessary for the next speaker to start with a shift cue. On one single occasion, however, he takes part in the discussion (Entry 17): "I think it can be rather arbitrary; As Mr. Neutra pointed out, even lightning bugs have passion," upon which Ciardi retorts: "But I'm saying what's wrong with.having a technical passion?" It is only here that the turn from moderator to 'speaker' produces a reference cos of transition. With the announcer, however, the case is bound to be different. He is not an actual participant in the communication, but he is, in a sense, superimposed. Because he can control his own entrances and exists, these could not be looked upon as affecting subsequent cues. The opposite would rather be true. He is the one to take the cue from the speaker’he introduces: he is not a 'cue giver' but 97 a 'cue taker'. This being the case, he sounds twice like ‘Adams' (Entries 1 and 21), and twice like a participant (Entries 3 and 32). Table 3 shows the contextual cues occurring in the first and last sentence of every Entry. It may be observed how these always bear some reference to the main content units treated throughout the discussion. While the first sentence is supposed to pick up these significant cues from the foregoing speaker, the last may be expected to hand these down to the following speaker, as modified, amplified, or perhaps substituted for by other cues. As previously mentioned, the effect of editing on the total frame of transition should be noted. In other words, whenever transition occurs between the announcer and any member of the panel, it should be remembered that this. transiticnvis not 'actual', but calculated to resemble actuality. These shifts, though valuable in these terms, should still be identified as premeditated and superimposed. Therefore, they may be studied separately as is indicated by Entry numbers. Therewith, it is suggested that the moderator's brief statements with which he ushers in one speaker after another should also be noted separately. In a sense, these statements are also premeditated. In spite of his presence with the group, he is not really involved 98 in the communication situation, especially as he puts in very little or nothing in the way of participation. His sole contribution is to turn in speakers. This being the case, the name Adams is Just mentioned in Table 2, with.nothing to follow. But when he does contribute something, it is treated as with the other members of the panel. The assumption that the first and last sentences uttered by panel speakers as they follow one another would normally contain 'pickup' signals of reference to significant content items of previous occurrence - seems to be Justified in quite a number of cases. The closest fit to this pattern of operation is probably demonstrated in shifts between the announcer and the next panel speaker. In the shift between Entries 1 and 2, the pickup items are 'rhytha' and 'way of knowing something': they are the very “ ‘words with which.Oiardi closes Entry 2. In the shift between Entries 21 and 22, again, the pickup items are identical. It is on these two occasions that the announcer cannot be mistaken for a panel speaker, or an actual participant. The fit in transition is too close to be true. In the 3-4 and 32-33 shifts, however, transition is more or less like that between the actual participants in the discussion. The pickup 99 signals are there, but they are not exact replicas. They are in a way similar to those in the 6-? shift: they sound more incidental. In fact, it is this incidental quality that may be considered characteristic of all other shifts between panel speakers. There are always pickup signals, which vary from the most to the least incidental, from the most to the least casual. If the speaker chose to start with some anecdote, or to introduce his point at some length, it might appear for a moment that he had failed to pick up his cue from his forerunner..And yet such a one would sooner or later pome.up.Again, should a speaker be interrupted before he was able to formulate his conclusions, the pickup signals for his follower might be anywhere, not necessarily in the final utterance. In other words, the first and last sentences do not always contain the pickup items. Though syntactically, both reference and shift cues among speakers seem to have a more or less fixed location, yet contextually, the so-called pickup signals could occur anywhere. And it is through the operation of these two maJor types of cues that the total frame of transition may be envisaged. CHAPTER'V COMBINAEION CUES Combination, as a category of organisational information, is here located between transition and integration for the simple reason that it is basic to both. It is not a 'different' category: it is 'the' category to account for communication in symbols. It is the principle of redundancy in action. With a separate chapter devoted to its treatment individually, it may appear as a separate entity. This is far from being the case with any of the categories postulated in this study. It is only for the sake of analysis that combination, like transition, is considered temporarily 'in its own rights'. Combination of items, as may be seen from the examination of assumptions in the preceding chapter, is fundamental to the identification of cues serving the function of transition. The whole phenomenon of cue clustering shown to account for progression is explainable in terms of combination. Therefore, with. respect to the decoder, whose task is basically the identification of cues, it is reasonable to suppose IOU 101 his having a tendency to add or accumulate items repeated identically, analogously, or by association. It is assumed that combination cues fall into the same two classes of 'syntactic' and 'oontextual'. Syntactic cues of combination, in the sense of 'annexation', are limited to the words 'and', 'or', and their equivalents, such as 'in addition', 'further- more', etc.; apart from these, a host of composite syntactic cues may be used for combination, then selected for 'reiteration' in close proximity. In other words, when units like 'and', or 'or' are used to combine content elements like 2., Q, 3, etc., the combination is something like a plus b plus c. The Gestalt depends more on contiguity than on similarity. But when function units like 'that is', 'not until', etc. are reiterated in succession, and in close proximity, they produce combinations which are like gg_, 32., etc. The Gestalt depends on both similarity and contiguity. This is considered the basis for distinguishing between 'annexation' and 'reiteration' ones on the broad syntactic side. They are not two kinds: but they produce two varieties of combination. It appears that any syntactic cue, including 'and', and 'or' can Operate as a reiteration cue for combination, once it is selected for reiteration in close proximity. 102 Contextually, combination cues are the same two classes of 'primary' and 'reinforcement' cues. Differentiation being established, the decoder would expect the same recurrence of items, noted before, producing combinations which have either 'identical', or 'analogous' constituents. Both of the Gestalt principles of similarity and contiguity seem to be in operation here. 1. Assumption I Combination of items in message decoding at the integration level is produced partly through 'annexation' and partly through 'redundancy'. 8. Identification of Cues (Entry 7., Ciardi) TaBLE 1 gyntactic Cues Annexation Cues Eeiteration Cues basic (and) true That is husband (or) citisen This is about notation (or) etc. It is That means beautiful and intense That means beautiful and intense Till the passion beautiful and intense Until the passion 103 TABLE 2 Contextual Cues Differentiated Cue Clusters human feelings beauty good husband good citizen humanity ggglg_.. beautiful intense beautiful souls intense souls delivering sermons beautiful sermons intense sermons it notion it x anything technical art form technically concerned notation way hands behave business playing piano technical art arts technical m poetry passion technical kisses both blubber human feeling technical passion 0110 104 (Entry 9., Neutra) TABIE 1 gyntactic Cues Annexation Cues Reiteration Cues complex (and) shifting thirty (or) forty years to the Philistines (and) structuring (and) arranging and and whether . . 01‘ 01' or 01‘ 01‘ and and and gg,have we have what have you 105 W Contextual Cues Differentiated Cue Clusters material of the architect complex shifting best material new materials human beings stainless steel clients plastics human beings reinforced concrete responsiveness (old stuff) responses new materials reactions material considerations them eyes ears auditive reception thermal reception inner ear sense of position sense of acceleration human beings 106 3. Interpretation When Ciardi employs such.combinations as 'basic and true', 'husband or citizen', 'about notation or about the way the hands behave ...' the function units 'and' and 'or' are supposed to do the business of combination in an explicit way. These units, by themselves, seem to signal combination, positively or negatively. And when some correlation is established in terms of content value between 'basic' and 'true', or between 'husband' and 'citisen', etc., as would normally be expected, combination may be considered to be implicitly signalled in addition. And should the whole frame 'basic and true' be reiterated in some analogous form, combination would get still further implicit reinforcement. Whenever this is the case with annexation cues, they are placed in a medial position between the two columns of Table 1. The frame ‘beautiful and intense' is both.annexational and reiterative: its constituents are both explicitly and implicitly combined. In fact, they are more implicitly than explicitly combined. Consequently, in a cluster count, they would go with reiteration cues. In order to show'how'many primary cues are responded to, whether these are syntactic or contextual, underlinings are used to indicate these cues. Whatever is listed under them is supposed to belong to them. 107 Table l for Entry 7 shows §.clusters of reiteration cues, as against §.individual annexation cues. While the former are supposed to combine ;9_ items, the latter combine §_: that is, if one were to equalize items aegigglg.in every case. Actually, if the sentence starting: 'That is ... ' contained two or more content units, to be combined with some two or more constituents of its parallel 'That is', then the net result would be a combination of 'two', not of 'four', or more. This is done for the sake of equalization. Table l for Entry 9 shows g,clusters of reiteration cues combining I§Dconstituents, as against é.individual annexation cues combining §_constituents. Therefore, as far as evidence goes, it appears that reiteration overweighs annexation, as far as syntactic signals are concerned. Contextually, however, the picture of combined items is entirely redundant. it this point, it should be re-stated that redundancy must be conceived of as a form of repetition, sometimes identical, and 'more often' analogous, or purely associative. It must be broadly maintained, in order to account for the progression and development of items in a message organisation. As previously mentioned, combination is not performed for its own sake. A comparison between cue clusters in Table 8 for Entry 7 108 and those in the corresponding Table for Entry 9 shows different forms of redundancy utilized for combination. Probably the most striking difference lies in greater variety among cluster constitueits in Entry 7. The grouping of such items as 'beauty', 'good husband', 'good citisen', and 'humanity' under the primary one 'human feeling' (Table 7) is certainly different from attaching 'rsspcnsiveness', 'responses', and 'reaotions' to 'human beings' (Table 9); or from attaching 'stainless steel', 'plastics', etc. to 'new materials (Table 9). While the former type of clustering is accomplished through subtle, ingenious repetitions, the latter is done through blank, unqualified, or slightly qualified repetitions. However, in either case of contextual cue clustering, the form of combination is redundant, and the underlying principles of perceptual organisation are similarity in various degrees, and contiguity. is far as syntactic combination cues are concerned, it has been demonstrated how these tend to be more reiterative than annexational. The results seem to argue strongly for redundancy in the sense defined by the described operation. 109 l. Assumption II The principle of linguistic duality seems to underlie the operation of combination cues. 2. Identification of Cues ' (Entry 13., Houghton) TABLE 1 §yntactic Cues innexation.0ues Reiteration Cues that form, etc. . that (and) that somehow .. that and and and and and TABLE 8 Contextual Cues Differentiated Cue Clusters content form 110 TABLE 2 ContextualVCues W Differentiated Cue Clusters Content ‘ form saying anything form something related word to human beings rhythm what use shape a point line something to say end Mrs Sheet's point end balance something close form to his heart rhythm shape line form shape rhythm line formalism formal materials words for sake of words material means to end content along with form 111 (Entry 22 . , Bheets) TABIE 1 gyntactic Cues Annexation Cues Reiteration Cues idea (or) content It,isn't literature rhythm (and) all ... It is music to go over ..(and) defend It is a special ... speaks (and) writes You're not dealing You're dealing Itfs important It's terribly ... It's not the ... It isn't Just ... that never say ... that doesnfit say ... that is written ... 112 TABLE 2 Contextual Cues W Differentiated Cue Clusters different kinds of art 2.2222. music it special area of human feeling tone rhythm music music no idea no content doesn't say anything work of art form manner form literature ideas clearly uniquely stated not mere rhythm not Just rhythm i122. context basic ideas concepts feelings beliefs lecturer speaks and writes beautifully fascinating hear him speak doesn't say anything never say anything two different arts problem 113 3. Interpretation _ The notion of 'linguistic duality' has been borne out by linguistic research at various levels, but within the narrow framework of 'phonematisation' versus 'grammatisation'.:Eowever, from the point of view of communication, one cannot conceive of a linguistic principle as separate in operation from the whole conceptual framework of human experience, as may be represented by message content. The presented procedure of identifying combination cues in the structure of content would not be possible if the basic scheme of language operation was not so amenable to analysis in terms of a system of duality which encompasses both the structure of content, and the structure of expression. Fundamentally, it is possible to think of combination cues as 'syntactic' and 'contextual'. Syntactic cues are those grammatical units that may be identified as 'Joiners' of content units. Table l for Entries 12 and 22 comprise a.number of such connecting units. Contextual cues are those content units, which carry the sum and substance of the message. Whatever may have been left out of Table 2 for these two Entries belongs to the category of minor content units: those that are subsidiary ramifications, whieh receive no further development 114 in the course of the message. Should one be interested in giving a detailed picture of content units, such minor elements would certainly make their appearance in the table. It is then possible to think of syntactic cues serving the function of combination as 'annexational' and 'reiterative'. This does not mean that the two classes are mutually exclusive, but that they produce two different types of combination. All the annexation cues in Table l for Entry 12 are simultaneously reiterative cues, with only one single exception. 0n the other'hand, none of the annexation cues in Table l for Entry 22 is selected for reiteration. This seems to be a matter of individual style. Meanwhile, contextual cues, serving the same function of coalescence may be divided into: 'identically redundant', and 'analogously redundant'. While there are various forms of analogous repetition, none of them can be confused with identical forms. It has been stated earlier how both classes of redundant units are essential for communications However, so long as the aim is to achieve progression with the concurrent development of content units, there would normally be more items of the analogously redundant type than of the identically redundant. Under 'content' in Table 2 for Entry 12, there is not a single item identically 115 repeated. Uhdcr 'form', there is one in this particular cluster. * Considering each.oue cluster individually, the internal structure has two components: a 'primary' cue, followed by 'reinforcement‘ cues. Sometimes, the primary cue does not appear to be central in the cluster following it.:Eow far organisational primacy would coincide with conceptual centrality appears to be relative to the point of view of the beholder: the encoder on one side, and the decoder on the other. For example, considering the cluster under 'music' in Table 2 for Entry 22, should the item 'special area of human feeling' occur first, it would be arbitrarily called a 'primary' use for this particular cluster, according to the presented scheme of analysis. And should a decoder think it proper to call 'music' still the primary cue, the difference between the 'given' order and the 'expected' order would not really matter in this connection, as long as there 'is' a primary use with other reiterative cues in the‘vicinity. This is one interpretation of the principle of 'linguistic duality', viewed as underlying one aspect of perceptual organisation in message decoding, namely combination. 116 l. Assumption III In interpersonal communication of the group situation, a speaker's reaction to opposition in argument seems to be reflected in increased density of combination cues. 2. Identification of Cues Entries: 13, 16, 18, and 20., Ciardi TABLE 1 §yntactic Cues Entry‘ iAnnexation Cues Esiteration Cues 13 None until someone ... _ until he ... 16 post or an architect or an artist Ig_he's a small ... If he‘s a larger ... Thgtfs the dirty word. That's not it ... It's the way 18 None Ehgtfs wrong with ... ‘ What's wrong with ... What's wrong with ... 20 None If_you're going ... If you violate ... If you violate ... 117 TABIE l Syntactic Cues Entry Annexation Cues Reiteration Cues 20 No one objects ... No one objects ... W Contextual Cues Entry 13., Ciardi Differentiated Cue Clusters W. B. Yeats question answer 9.191 oasis. dancer dance dancer dance someone it dancer dancing Entry 16., Ciardi ideas human being poet W 118 Contextual Cues Entry 16., Ciardi Differentiated Cue Clusters ideas ideas ideas things 03.! human being human being human being being“human human being poet architect artist artist he he he he he experiences experience art art form technical dirty word empty correctness? way of going, language spoken 118 TABLE 2 Contextual Cues Entry 18., Ciardi Differentiated Cue Clusters passion loving getting expressed Entry 20., Ciardi music structures premises technical premises pianist what fingers doing what fingers doing ‘wropg technical wrong medium wrong medium same thing, at it differently poem not something said something happening happens poet way thing flows form empty technically? human good human 120 TABLE 5 Comparative Distribution Density per cluster per one Density 13 L1 16 18 20 60 807 146 55 174 20 IO) [0’6 5* 5i; NOTE: The underlined figures for Entries 10 and 7 stand for available samples of 'normal' distribution, as far as Ciardi is concerned. These are calculated to permit a comparison within understandable limits. 3. Interpretation . In debating this controversial issue, Ciardi appears to be the predominant figure, fighting for form and technical passion. As long as his opposition can be kept under control, he is naturally relaxed: he speaks at some length, interjects incidental remarks, and proceeds at a moderate pace. On two occasions, however, he seems to be gradually shaken by his opposition: once, following Houghtonis (Entry 12) and once, following Sheets's (Entry 22). On the latter occasion, he retorts violently with the phrase:"That's nonsense, Mr. Sheets," and says little under the circumstances, that could be studied in terms of the effect of opposition on his manipulation.of combination cues. Boughton's opposition being milder, Ciardi goes on at a faster tempo, apparently saying more in less words. This is the general impression a decoder would probably get, as he listens to Entries 13, 16, 18, and 20. Table 1 shows no annexation cues in g,Entries. All syntactic cues are reiterative: 5 clusters for Entry 13; §.for 16: l_for 18: and §_for 20. This distribution of syntactic cues seems to be more or less normal, as far as Ciardi is concerned. See Entry 7, for example. | I'll-It'll! ’utlfnw I... 122 is for contexxual cues, the general picture shows density with brevity. Entry 13 shows §_clusters, with a total of ;§_units. Apart from the grammatical structure in which these content units are set, practically nothing is said, which.is not redundant in a measure. The density ratio seems to rise even higher in Entry 16, as may be seen in Table 3 of Comparative Distribution. What is noticeable in addition is that the speaker seems to sacrifice motion and variety for the sake of greater emphasis: his reinforcement cues are, generally speaking, more or less identical repetitions of his primary cuss: the only variation for 'human being' is 'being human': 'experience' has no reiterative form i but 'experience': 'technical' is made synonymous with some 'dirty word', for lack of a more suitable term, to maintain the diversity to which he is accustomed. Entry 18 is probably the most representative of increased density of combination cues coupled with brevity. It is not only that the three main content units are trebly reiterated, but the whole syntactic frame of the question is constantly repeated. Nothing else is said beyond this. Entry 20 shows a return back to normal, according to Table 3. Comparing Entry 20 with Entry 16, one observes how the total number of words rises, while density per one falls. On the other'hand, the samples of 'normal' distributisn 123 shown as underlined in Table 3 demonstrate, in a very 'tentative' fashion, how, under the effect of opposition in argument, density per cue rises in Entries 13 and 16, while the number of words falls. In other words, the speaker seems to reinforce his primary cues more than he usually does, and simultaneously, to reduce the total number of words. In any case, with a sample like this, it is difficult to tell accurately how 'significant' such differences may be. Above all, what we have here is the reaction of one individual. Whether this is generally the case with.others is open to research. V ..I’III]. aid .II afl_.&m m . ...... . .Ix — CHAPTER'VI INTEGRATION CUES Combination of the elements of a message content has been introduced as basic to transition on the one hand, and to integration on the other. It has also been stated that while transition is synonymous with progression from one end of a message to the other, integration is presumed to mean cohesion among parts. The assumptions to be examined in this chapter'have to do partly with what cohesion of parts is, and partly with what it does, as far as the decoder is concerned. Preliminary to the analytic process at this stage, however, one question should be raised and taken account of. In an investigation into what constitutes organisational information in linguistic communication, are we interested in the 'logic' of such.communication? And if so, what is our standpoint, as far as the 'quality' of reasoning is concerned? In answer to the first part of the question, one would say that this investigation is concerned at this point with the linguistic features of £24 125 organisation for reasoning, not with the reasoning process as such. In other words, when people reason with one another, they are supposed to use certain linguistic techniques. They might 'substitute' some items, 'modify' other items, 'amplify' other items, etc. These are the organisational linguistic techniques, which are here postulated as basic to any form of reasoning. Naturally, we are interested neither in the 'mode' of reasoning, nor in the 'quality' of reasoning. These are questions of more interest to the logician, and they are naturally outside the scope of this study. Therefore, whether the communicator is reasoning or rationalising, all we propose to do is to notewhat type of organisational cues he manipulates, where, how often, and with what effect on the decoder. Our aim is to discover a basic pattern, which would be expected to recur with other speakers of the same language. When it is stated or implied that 'substitution' cues, for instance, help a decoder to accomodate a suggested relationship better, and therefore perceive it better, it should be understood that one is not concerned at all with whether such a decoder is convinced, whether he will accept or reject the presented relationship. Integration cues are, again, either syntactic or contextual. Syntactic cues are those grammatical ~‘ III}! lllslll..1 ‘.!r 126 units viewed as providing links for speech segments, which 'substitute' content units, 'modify', or 'amplify' them. Contextual cues are, according to the basic scheme of combination, either 'primary' or 'reinforoement' ones. The same principle of redundancy adduced to account for combination is again utilized to achieve the integration or cohesion of parts, often referred to as the 'development' of content units. Naturally, contextual cuss would fit into the same functional framework of syntactic cues, which holds 'substitution', 'modification', and 'amplification'. 1. Assumption I Integration of content units is accomplished 'Jointly' by syntactic and contextual cues. 2. Identification of Cues (Entry 27, Ciardi) TABLE I gyntactio Cues W Substitution Medification Amplification of (what) goes (when) spring parable: (There) discovered (that) (while) he was sw.. juggling: (He) this is (what) (while) he was cl.. specialists: (One) 127 TABIE l ‘ Syntactic Cues Substitution Modification Amplification It is (that) anything.. idea (that) Desecration: (This) moral: (I) TABLE 2 Contextual Cues Differentiated Cue Clusters short story parable of artistic process Juggler 1:551:25 Juggler monastery_ sweeping island of peace sweeping monastery dedicated to praise Juggled sessions of Juggling cleaning Juggler 128 TABLE 2 Contextual Cues Differentiated Cue Clusters 292.159. specialists wrote hymns raised flowers illuminated manuscripts did decorations Desecration What Luce says Desecration Sheets says. poetry about ideas 10881138 sweat Virgin come down wipe sweat 2.9.12.1. real one gist anything intensely done anything positively done any good shape praise statement experience prayer I say experience of ideas one idea praise 129 (Entry 29., Ciardi) TABLE 1 Syntactic Cues f:- Substitution Modification Amplification think (that) is.. (whether .. or) Experience: (It) (more .. than) (as) R. Frost TABLE 2 Contextual Cues Differentiated Cue Clusters (idea) Experience live better more richly more alive in process Robert Frost pggm momentary stay against confusion not stay so (clarified) for a minute clarifies thing experience caught of all art 3. Interpretation One's view is here limited to inter-sentential syntactic cues signalling the three structural features specified as basic to the process of integration amnng decoded content units. In Table l of Entry 27, some units are listed as 'substitution' cues. What is meant in the first case, for instance, is that when the listener hears Ciardi say:"I want to make it into a parable of..", he expects the occurrence of 'something', an obJect or action, which could be 'artistic thinking‘, 'creative thinking', 'the artistic process', or anything suitable in this context. However, as soon as the decoder receives the cue: 'what', his expectations switch over to an expanded statement of what was of probable occurrence, and so he gets: " .. what goes on in the artistic process." This expansion of a content unit pending expression is introduced by means of such.cues as 'what', 'that', etc. And this expansion is presumably calculated to give better accomodation to integration between the idea of the parable and that of the artistic process, by way of introduction. This is not to say that other factors may not be recognised as affecting a speaker's choice of the expanded frame rather than the short frame. And this is not to say that the expanded frame always provides a better contribution to integration, but 131 that it offers another alternative to a decoder who might need it in order to perceive the presented relationships better. It is very likely, however, that in oral interpersonal communication, a decoder would be happier with the expanded form, which.is actually more redundant than the short form. In any case, this point would need further confirmation.from field research. Moving on to the second column of Table 1, one observes another set of cues used to signal another type of expanded frames, referred to as 'modification' frames. The chief difference between these and substitution frames is a difference of function» modification frames are supposed to delimit actions already expressed or pending expression. In the case of "When spring came around, he found himself recovered," the whole frame,'when spring came around' may be looked upon as 'delimiting' the forthcoming action of the Juggler 'finding himself recovered'. In this case, modification is 'prospectively' applied to action 'pending expression'. 0n the other'hend, "They were Just about to run in and throw him out, when they saw the Virgin, etc.," is an illustration of how an expanded frame is signalled to modify 'retrospectively‘ some action already expressed. ...; (.0 N The third column comprises a composite number of cues, which signal the structural relation- ship given the name of 'amplification' in the sense of illustration or supplementation of something of prior occurrence. In Entry 23, Ciardi requests three more minutes to give a 'parable', and his request is granted with the words: "Speak, master." Thereupon, he starts: 2... There's a lovely short story of Anatole France's, etc. etc. 3 The cue shown between brackets in this column is supposed to signal an amplification of the unit 'parable'. Similarly, ‘he' is related to 'Juggling‘, 'one' to 'specialists', 'this' to 'Desecration‘, and 'I' to 'mcral'. To say that 'there‘, 'he', 'one', 'this', and '1', etc. are 'amplification' cues is not to contradict or denounce one's calling them previously by the name of 'transition' ones. They are both transition and amplification cues: they fulfil complementary functions in one and the same process of language perception. It is only that one is here interested in some exploration of the different functions fulfilled by these and other units of speech. Along these lines, one may read the results shown in Table l of Entry 29. "That is the subJect of all arts," is a substitution frame. "Whether it means anything or not, " is a modification frame, prospectively acting upon 'Ciardi being alive in the process'. As for the list of amplification cues, it is sufficiently varied in scope, although it comprises only three units. The notion of 'experience' in this context happens to be one of those delicate concepts, that can hardly be communicated even to artists, without misapprehension. It therefore needs amplification.in various ways. One way, according to Ciardi, is to denote what it means in the broadest possible terms: "It says,etc. ," goes the first cue. moanWhile, a comparison is needed to demonstrate the advantage of living through an experience: " better, more richly than I do etc.," goes the second cue. That does not seem enough to convince, not ohly a panel of fellow artists and creative thinkers, but also a large audience of mixed thoughts and mixed feelings. So, out goes the name of Robert Frost again: "As Robert Frost said a while back,etc." The one "as", followed by the particular name of Robert Frost, is calculated to usher in amplifying information characterized by source credibility. As a matter of fact, one could list more cues under this category than Just three: in a sense, development of content items consists in amplification all the way through. However, the most conspicuous cues that could be listed systematically here may be arbitrarily determined as units introducing: 1) comparisons implicitly or explicitly expressed: and 2) appositional statements. 13h With respect to contextual cues, there is practically nothing to add to what has already been demonstrated in the previous chapter about differentiated sue clusters: how they are supposed to accumulate, and how they operate with the decoder in terms of primary and reinforcement frames. What is closely related to integration, however, is the scheme of their operation in terms of the three structural features of 'substitution' 'modification', and 'amplification'. A leek at the contents of Table 2 for Entry 27 will show how the total structure of differentiated cue clusters can be accemodated within the three- dimensional frame of reference. 'Juggling', 'sweeping', and 'the artistic process' seem to be handled as inter-changeable substitutes: and so are the 'monks', 'Luce', and probably 'Millard Sheets'. The 'moral' modifies everything said: anything 'intensely well done', anything 'positively well done', etc. is, according to Ciardi, synonymous with the artistic experience. The whole parable as has been previously alluded to, is an amplification of the notion of the artistic process. Similarly, Table 2 of Entry 29 shows substitution in the inter-changeable character of a poem as 'a momentary stay against confusion', and as 'an eXperience of life caught'. It shows modification through specifying the ephemeral nature 135 of organization in the artistic experience. And it shows amplification in terms of Robert Frost's contribution to John Ciardi's initial statement about the notion of experience. Finally, the essential unity of both syntactic and contextual cues seems to be reflected in their fitness into the same scheme of integration, which .in Gestalt terminology is based on principles of consistency and symmetry in the main. Through substitution, modification, and amplification of content units, the decoder is supposedly enabled to perceive better consistency or more symmetry among parts of the presented content. 1. Assumption II The scheme of integration cues seems to reveal a binary principle in action. 2. Identification of Cues ...; O O) c.) (Entry 53., Ciardi) TABLE 1 §yntactic Cues Substitution Modification Amplification (What) I am oon.. (When) you put Idea in action: (What) interests behaves (when) (In) philosophy Creative thinking: .. is (what) portion/ around(which) (He) said tell me (how) irrationalities(that) Basie things: tell you (how) things (that) (You) can't get .. in (What) it means experiences (that) in (what) it does Profound obsourit. the fact (that) (As) told us (what) TABLE 2 Contextual Cues W Differentiated Cue Clusters Idea Experience ideas experience ideas into monographs ideas into human being portion of a life enactment of idea TABLE 2 Cont extual Cues )u Differenti ated Cue Clusters idee concept thing in the abstract idea he Neutra 'pure idea' lost 18th cent. cause experience idea in action in philosOphy_ pragmatic consequences how human being behaves accepts idea .. motivation way of measuring ideas experience what idea means to human b. what it does inside him ghole bush of dendrites idea with all glands on with all fears on with all anxieties on with.all Joys ready with all irrationalities basic irrationalities emotions Il.‘illl|lfl’lllll ll} I, | :1!) 3‘3 H TABLE 2 Contextual Cues Differentiated Cue Clusters problem solving rational Dean of engineering five steps limit problem qualitative analysis quantitative mathematics check quant. mathematics creative thinking, mechanical implementation/ creative thinking chess playing problem solving problems to solve defining 'girls' qualitative analysis quantitative mathematics text-book ideas basic things of life get married get married beget child die decently cardinal points of life hinge points important points irrationalities profound obscurities g. marvellous obscurities experiences .. stir us resonance .. vibration I1 I III" I III I. l . I‘ll! 5. Interpretation To repeat an earlier statement, the pattern postulated for the main linguistic features of organization utilized for the integration of content units is presumed to underlie any reasoning process. It is neither the reasoning process as it 'should be', nor even as it 'is'. Wh other we reason or rationalize, whether we follow an inductive or a deductive procedure, or both, the underlying structural features of linguistic expression are presumably these two in the main: 'substitution', and 'mcdification'. A content unit or a combination of content units is either 'replaced', or 'changed' in the course of reasoning. It may be wholly or partially replaced: and in the process of change, it may be qualified, tempered, delimited, or else, expanded, broadened, deve10ped, etc. This dual scheme of integration cues does not contradict the tripartite scheme being so far expounded: it only presents a possible reduction in binary terms. As a matter of fact, the operation of die binary principle seems to be so pervasive as almost to dictate a scheme of dualities all the way through. In the first place, Table l for Entry 55 presents one of two categories of integration cues, namely the syntactic. A look at column l.will show two varieties 140 of substitution cues: one occupying an initial position in the sentence; the other occupying a terminal position. It would be interesting to investigate the relative significance of these two positions from the point of view of the decoder. moving on to column 2, one observes two varieties of modification cues: those in the upper part modify both prospectively and retrospectively, according to their position in the sentence. This has already been discussed in the previous examination of Assumption 1. Those in the lower part of the column must always follow whatever they modify. They never precede it. As it now stands, column g comprises two types of amplification cues: one is distinctively analogical and extensional; it often suggests a comparison implicitly or explicitly. This type is here represented by the unit 'as'. It is as distinctively analogical and extensional as 'and' is distinctively 'combinational', and as 'then' is distinctively 'transitional'. The other type is a composite group of shift cues employed to introduce the amplifying combination of items. However, when column §.is mashed into column g, the result will be a compound of two chief elements: one characterized by introducing further development and extension (now amplification): and the other 14-1 comprising different features of change (now modification). 0n the other hand, Table 2, which stands for the other chief category of integration cues, namely the contextual, shows, in turn, a dual distribution of these cues, as represented by the two columns of differentiated cue clusters: one for the notion of 'idea', and the other for the notion of 'experience'. It may be argued that the observable duality of distribution may very probably reflect the two-sided issue under discussion: 'form' versus 'content', and that the Juxta-position of 'idea' versus 'experience' is probably foreshadowed by the two sides of the topic. It is also possible that this basic duality is a characteristic of Ciardi's personal style. All this may be true to a certain extent. Observation, howeger, seems to support the assumption that 'duality' is inherent in the very process of differentiation, without which there could be no distribution of cues, no communication. A content analysis of any of the Entries treated in this study would invariably reveal the binary structure of content in every case. In Entry 4, for instance, the major content units cluster around 1) the great art of the past; and 2) the empty art of the present. Entry 9 centres on two points: 1) technical concern with raw materials: 142 and 2) technical concern with human response. Within this dual frame, the same features of substitution and modification are observable about contextual cues in Table 2 for Entry 35. In a sense, all clusters under 'experience' are offered as substitutes for those under 'idea'. The 'whole bush of dendrites' would replace the 'pure idea': 'creative thinking' would replace 'problem solving', etc. Meanwhile, the notion of 'pragmatio consequences' in philosophy is adduced to amplify the 'idea in action'. The notion of the 'basic irrationalities' is modified by the very mention of the item 'emotion', and by the thought of 'dying decently', etc. There is ample evidence here of the operation of the binary principle. 1. Assumption III In interpersonal.communication over a controversial issue, the total structure of content consists in a 'systematic' integration of 'salient' contextual cues, manipulated on both sides of the controversy. 2. Identification of cues 143 TABLE 2A Contextual Cues Contention Reaction Entry 2., Ciardi rhythm way of knowing basic to form poem not about idea about experience Feature of Integration: Exchanged Remarks: Entry 6., Fadiman Lincoln's four score and .. idea operative Ideas of Shakespeare as he expresses them cannot list them Amplification Fadiman: Is that all what you meant, John? Ciardi: That's very much a part of it. Entry 9., Neutra best materials human beings Entry 18. Eiardi technical passion loving medimm Entry 10., Ciardi human_question second Entry 19., Neutra technology:enve10ps us man, greatest subject 14-4 TABLE 2A Contextual Cues Contention Reaction Entry 18., Ciardi Entry 19., Neutra Feature of Integration: Modification Exchanged Remarks: Neutra: To understand his responses and reactions, I say, is the thing. Ciardi: Absolutely. We're saying the same thing but coming at it differently, etc. a Entry 15., Ciardi Entry 15., Stevenson human Question Mr. Ciardi right second materials first Feature of Integration: Substitution for similar Reference: ' Agreement acknowledged ‘11}{lljlil1l 14-5 TABLE ZB Contextual Cues Contention Reation Entry 4., Sheets Entry 12., Houghton both terribly important balance of form ideas form fair equality rhythm shape line addressed to .. human b.. Lincoln's four score .. utilized to make a point Feature of Integration: Amplification Reference: Houghton : I would like to come back to the support of Mr. Sheet's point which was etc. Entry 22., Sheets Entry 25., Ciardi four score .. out of context nonsense two different arts problem Feature of Integration: Substitution for different _ expected Reference: Disagreement declared 14-6 TABLEwEB Contextual Cues W Contention Reaction Entry 27., Ciardi Entry 28., Sheets poetry .. experience fi one idea .. praise an idea Entry 29., Ciardi Entry 50., Sheets an experience praise .. great idea Entry 51., Ciardi greater experience Feature ofIntegration: Substitution for different Reference: Disagreement declared 5. Interpretation In a communication situation like this, where members of the panel form two sides, the total integration of contributions from both sides would presumably be a product of the operation of cues signalling approval on the one hand, and disapproval 'llllllilllll'lllli“ 14-7 on the other. In terms of the basic three-dimensional scheme postulated for integration cues, signals of agreement between two or more members of the panel would be associated with a 'substitution' of something similar or with 'amplificaticn'. On the other hand, signals of strong disagreement would be associated with a 'substitution' of something completely different, or with mere rejection followed by a pause of suspense. Half-way between are signals of mild disagreement, which suggest some form of 'modifioation'. All three types of signals have naturally been transmitted, received, and reacted to by every member of the panel. Table 2A for one side of the controversy, and Table 23 for the other side give a briefing of the exchanged cues at points of contact. The question of the saliency of these cues is determined partly by their recurrence, as previously shown in Tables of differentiated clusters, and partly by their appearance as the points 'at issue'. As for the sequence followed in these two tables, it does not show the actual order of presentation, but the order found to reveal the systematic nature of the process of agreement and disagreement, as corresponding to the postulated pattern of integration cues. - .-.-.2 ..,-- 3. ‘ TL??? '22er mini-"- 0 .... -.. M Ad ' 'Zn-Ir. :12 ' A-‘J' 524.320.3133. 33 Cvj..;.lamTvvt1 fin ‘m" ( Excerpts from a Syn mp0 osiu um heLd at Miami Universit by, Oxford, Ohio f/A/rn/Z?’ /;J’/ —Q-~---‘m~~e‘mmm-m~mwflmma......-0.“-“nu-Che.” we H 3‘ LeAel “KY.“ J. Ciardi, poet and poetry editor of the Saturday Rev iew C. Fadimen, author lecturer and critie . N. Houghton, managing direetor of the Phoenix Theater, NGW'YOEK R. Mantra, Los Angeles architect ML Sheets, aitist designer and directo; of L. A. Art Institute H. Stevenson, composer musics an. chairman of the Composition Dept., Un3.vereity of QoLLho: Californsa. Phil Atiams, director of the Cincinnati Art ML seam ~---~~---*-m-~--~-m—-——-e’-a~e“----mm ‘ '4 pn—E rev NEE 0333333 73 ART “.6 ‘ ...-.— Early in the diSCLt.seions John Ciardi had made the ste't emeht that a rh.ytnm is a way of knowing SOTPthnS} Asked to elaborate on what he moantlflro Ciardi said Ciardi. .2. "fimmwm A.while be ck I was interviewing Robert Frost uh in Florioa and he put it uh as well as I LLh I've ever heard it said# Uh it‘s somet hing thAt's be on bus sing around the ed.ge of my my head for a long t:x.o and he he hsted locate it# He said when you start a poem you don't xnow:oore it's going? It's on its way uh ani you don't A new fibers it's coming out but you do know when you' we missed 1% Uh then he said now there’s a questioof M33 do you know ah me you've missed what you dzLu.n‘tE :now you were ai.ming etf3 Uh and he d.oesn't know# You do knot” You do hm3w wELen you ve mzssed itJ Yen kn3w you hope you know e‘hen i We ve hit it bL:t you can be positive you know when y3u we m:Lssed itJ Eh the v.rh what he aghat he had to say next on it was t1 at the 3E qu is in it” That's that' a your way of. knowing it# ‘ometuxrs eoou‘ the wave length of the to .y t goes3 There‘s something 3.3 prin.tivo as the be at of Mother Gronif That‘s thatEA the rhythm! of Lhe English lcuvu""nr It's the wraoL of the re Eh the? 3 there’ 3 a kindU of wzv of T03R23 In ELgiioh leis Lasxc 11y iambicfi ‘38 a kind of O .‘ / In other 183.3 J“”“8 it 3h 33 goes Giver V‘vsf 1‘ 7:2"; beat# newthxic eeueAcoo” Whether tAAV're eh ..o ..9 uh visual as in a series of lines or in timenae 3.3 a series of sounds a1 atrscture‘s involvei in rhythm# Structure is your way of Earning a poem it's yo3r ray of formimg mythirgfi wringin: it to ehepef May l’hove Lo insist that a rhythm is a way of gnowing sogz“hion 3 That it‘s very basic to form; 149' 2. It's a way of knowing something about ourselves# One of the questions I'd like to raise is what sort of human behavior is a poo what is a human being doing when he writes a poem# What is a human being doing when he receives a poem# Because I'd I don't know how the others would feel about this but I went to say very emphatically a poem is never about an idea# It's always and only about the experience of an idea And that's a very different thing# It's what what an idea feels like when you live in 13# That is it is an experience and it has to come to you at a pace# Experience has to happen at a certain rate# And the rhythm of the unfolding as well as the rhythm and and the wave length of the language are the basic ways uh poetry has of testing the vali 1ty of the esthetic experience# It's the way one knows# Announcer.& Conflict with John Ciardi's point of view was provided by Millard Sheets# Reoponding to the question is the big idea more important than concern with the actual material medium of the painter Mr Sheets said: Shbets.4. They're both terribly important# And I think the greatest art uh a a I look back upon the great arts.of many epochs contains both of these qualities in fair equality# It seems to me that ... if we do look our own age comparatively we must recognize that ... wedva advanced ... I I believe very honestly tremendously in the last 20 ... 25 years in America and throughout the world in raising the level of esthetic understanding and feeling about painting# Putting it another way the organization of a painting as a complete unit ... the accords' of color the new concepts of handling space within a ace texture values and all the things that are part of t e the technical building of of a of a painting ... I think wo8vc made a tremendous progress over the perfectly banal cheap kind of literal painting that that degenerated 1n the late 19th and earl 20th century where where realism was an and in itself‘ But by the same token I don't think that the things that we are producing to day in these large .. .quantities which are so much easier to produce than dealing with great ideas will ever hold their place in terms of of comparison with great art of of many epochs of the past# I believe that idea: and deep human values are permanent I don't think that the twent eth century has ruled those out for a moment# I think that the experimental age that we've gone through will contribute tremendously to the younger painters coming on in the next few generations-who will inherit with one stride new vocabulary new power# But it's chirctcy empty art if you compare it to great 1deas# And I t nk that the ideas will come back into-art if we as a. society have the f the insight and the power and the force to develop these ideas, 150 3. Adams 51 Mr. Fadiman wants to speak of that too Mr. Sheets May he# Fediman K5 I Just want to add a little anecdote which may be familiar to all of you involving the great French uh painter Derein and the great French poet Mallarme# Derain was a very intelligent man who was a friend of Mallarme who once said to him Mallarme he said I cannot understand why I ... I cannot write poetry as you do# I have such remarkable ideas# And Mallarme said that's your trouble poetry is made with words# not ideas# New ... that may seem aradoxical# You might say this is art for art's sake; Not at all# ' The poet's ideas and the prose-writer's ideas are in the words themselves# They are not somethi which can be summed up# What are the ideas of Shahespe No one knows No one knows# You cannot list theefi And if your English professor here tells u to list Shakespeare's ideas you must see that he is fired# ow that doesn't mean Shakespeare has no ideas# Not at all Not at all# But his notions about the world and you may call them ideas if you will those notions are only perfect and clear when they are ressed precisely as he expresses them and in no other way Now a great many ... I I in the course of m Job as an editor I used to meet a great many young novelists Moetsgf them very bad young novelists but they had a lot of idea They're full of ideas# Really they had many more ideas than Dickens ever had in all of his life I assure yo But the ideas were not of the.sort that could he married to this particular medium narrative prose# The first question asked of Mr. Ciardi was in connection with what he meant by his statement about rhythms# And of course he said he couldn't ... answer you exactly and he's quite right# But I think we can make it a little clearer this way we can say ... suppose I gave you two dates Here's one dat Eighty seven years ago Here's another dat Four score and seven years as New both the same date aren't they# The first date has no rhythm# The second date has rhythm# Mr. Ciardi could tell-you Just what‘ rhythm that is and why it is that Lincoln chose to say that rather than eighty seven # Now the second statement is a . different statement than the first etatement# Curiously enough# The fact is the sanefl The statement is different# The statement four score and seven years ago is the beginning of'a great idea and you know what_the idea was You've read the Gettysburg Address# And we remember that idea is operative # The idea would be less operative if Lincoln had said eighty seven years ago# Does that make ... Is that all what you meant.John# 151 A» Ciardi 7 That's very much a part of it# This this is such a basic and true notion and yet it's one the audience resists so uch As soon as I've had this experience uh uh ever since I began with the Saturday Review if you try to say anything technical about the art form that means you have no human feslings# Uh that that means you hate beauty uh and you’re not really a very good husband or citizen# It's it's it's as if’a musician destroyed his humanity b being technically concerned about ... notation or about he way the hands behaVe in the business in the business of uh playing the pian People think consistently that so long as their souls are beautiful and intense they're poets# Now I think there are Inany beautiful and intense souls delivering sermons for example delivering perhaps beautiful and intense sermons# But they're not poems# Poetry is a technical art# All arts are technical# And until the passion is technical it doesnt gp# I made an aphorism for that once# Till passion is technical kisses are blubber# Uh the that's that's you've you've got to :9 both the human feeling and the technical passion have to e on Ad§§§4l Mr. Neutra would like to speak to that too# Nggtgg . 7 well uh p s the material of the architect is a little bit more comp ex and shifting than the material of the poet and that's why I would uh I could make a much - clearer statement about it from my point of view# About 30 or 35 or to yearspgfo modern architecture started to be explained to the listines and to all the good people who didn't know why we need a new architecture# You know uh we have all these new materials we have the stainless steel and the plastics and the reinforced concrete what' have you and we can't go on with this old stuff# The material was put ahead of everything else and it is my .. considered opinion and'so I can easily say and beyond sus icion that I haven't used new materials in a daring way# would say that all these material considerations are very much in second place in my in my mind# I do think that the best mate the architect ever ets under his hands are the human beings who are his clients; And uh to understand human-beings to be fascinated by understanding their responsiveness their responses their reactions is the great art of structuring an environment and arranging stimuli for them# Whether it's for their eyes or for their ears or for their.audit1ve reception or for their thermal reception or for their inner ear sense of position or acceleration and so forth# He has to understand human beings and.this is his material# I suppose that every issue of the trade magazines bring new materials and new engineering and new installations and they are always played up as the decisive factor in architecture# But once upon a time archetecture was concerned with eternal issues 152 5. Cigdi I“ May I may I ask one question of that Mr Neutra# Uh I'm interested in your emphasis but can you can you go on to the human question until you know what stone will do and what reinforced concrete will do and what steel will do and what glass will do and what plastic facings will do# Oh until until your knowledge of these materials is such that you you can put them in second place# Neutra M ’ ' ‘ Most assuredly I have to know first the human beings before I know what a cold wet stone will do to my skin where I have heat losses# The biological side of it and the empathic side of it understanding human beings and systematically understanding them is the premise to know what to do about stainless steel and any other thing# I have to understand physiological optics before I can know how to use glass mirrors and so forth I don't start with mirrors and glass# And this is the great fault of Park Avenue re uh renewal# Hggghtgg /2, ‘ It would appear off hand that this panel is moving in that age old discussion of form versus content over on to the side of fbrm# And uh let's not worry about whether we're saying anything at all# Uh the important thing is the word the rhythm the shape the line uh and I would like to come back to the support of Mr. Sheets' point which was that form and rhythm and shape and line in themselves are not enough and that somehow there must be the balance of this form and shape and rhythm and line addressed to something that relates to human beings# I don't think anybody would really dispute me but the uh may be they would this panel I think would# But we have heard so much emphasis on the formalism on the formal materials as opposed to what use these were to be put to that I would like to to try to put my weight back on the side of of suggesting that certainly four score and seven years ago is better than eighty seven years ago and one does understand the meaning of the difference but Lincoln was utilizing the phrase in order to make a point and he had something to say# And he wasn't just playi with words for the sake of the words in themselves They were the material which was a means to an end and the end was something that he had very much close to his heart to say to other people and the content is important along with the form in which it's said# clildi xi. I'd like to ask William Butler Yeats that question uh and his answer is 0 body swayed to music 0 bri ht'ning glance how shall I tell the dancer from the dams Ibu can't se to these things these two things# There's no dancer ees .. the the dance is nowhere until someone dances it and he's not a dancer until He's dancing# 153 6. Adams x4. '" I'd like to ask a musician who practices the most Eurely artificial and abstract of all art forms# Mr. Steven ow do you speak to this pcintfi §§eyensgg /51 I think that Mr. Ciardi has the the right point of yiew# I can’t conceive of the kind of music that would interest anyone without absolute control of materials first# Ciardi /5; ““””'" Really what you're asking is that when you say you want a post to have ideas or an architect to have ideas or an artist to have ideas all you’re really asking is that he be a human being# Uh and I don‘t think that's too much to to grant the the artist# Uh I don't care who he is the chances are he‘s a human being# If he‘s a small human being he'll any small things about being human# He will have small experiences in his art# If he's a larger human.being he will have larger ones but the only way he can have an experience in his art form is technical# But uh you see that's that's the dirty word# One thinks immediately empty correctness# Uh that's the not it at all# Its the way of going# Thelanguage must be spoken# Adwms I77 .3“. I think it can be rather arbitrary# As Mro Neutra pointed out even lightning bugs have passions# Ciardi / J)- But I'm saying what’s wrong with having a technical passion# What's wrong with loving your medium# What's wrong with believing that the only way you can get expressed what you are is through your mediumfi New_1t;_§ / f ' ' ' Well technology today which envelope us in such a terrible fashion has produced unbearable situations biologically unbearable situations in our community in our cities#.We have been so in love and the technicians and the technologists and also the architects with materials that it was overlooked that man the consumer is the greatest subject# To understand his responses and reactions I say is the thing# 154 7. some 20— Absolutely# We're saying the same thing but coming at it differently uh may be across the hedge of of uh our different art forms we see this differently# But a poem is not something said# It's something happening# And how does it happen unless one engages it in the terms of what it's already started# Uh if you're going to write music you have to you have to watch your uh structuree# If you violate your premises you’re going to be in trouble# If you violate your technical prenises you're going to be in trouble# No one objects to a pianist being worried about what his fingers are doing# Uh he couldn't be a good human pianist without worrying about what his fingers are doing# Uh no one objects to this in a dancer# Uh yet if a poet says that he is in love with the way one thing flows into another in his poem ho'o s posed to be empty tochnically# This is the only way he canugo human within his form# 522222222 2% , A question which seemed for a moment to go unonsworod rovidod one of the boot expressions o the view int of'Mi ard Shoets# The panel was asked what th meant y the word art and whether they were not in fact tal ing about several difforont things when they used the word art# Shag: .22, - I think this this question is no of the most pertinent questions that has been asks In other words we are talking about several different kinds of art# What's true in music it doesn't have to represent an idea or a content# It isn't literature it's music# It's a special .aroa of human feeling that best expresses itself through tone rhythm and all the other definitions that you might givo for music# But in the word in in the art of literature you have another proble You're not dealing with more rhythm alone you're dealing with ideas# The four score and seven years ago is out of context# It's imposrtant# It's terribly important the definition that uh the comparison you've given Mr. Fadiman# But it's not the basic reason that w respect that particular address# It isn't just the rhythm; There are some basic ideas concepts feelings beliefs that were very clearly uniquely statod# Now I feel that tho the problem that we are talking about here is is one that we can be wo can leave everyone confused about by merely stressing the importance because we are nearly always as autists as critics on the defensive about what constitutes an organic work of art# The form the manner in which it's done# And therefore we are a little prone to go over on that side continuously and defend the importance of the form# But I wont to a lecture one time which someone summed up like this He speaks and writes beautifully# Of course he doesn't say anyt ing but it's Just fascinating to hear him speak# I dont think we bags time in a world like this to listen to things that are Just spoken well that never say anything# I would love to hear music that doesn't say anything that is written 155 8. .tseautifully# But they are talking about two different arts :and I think we've got to face the fact that this is our problem Ciardi 23- ' """ But that's that's nonsense Mr. Sheets# I mean that's the kind of thing one says Shgegs 24 . That ' a bad# (”£291 25’. I mean this remark that you quote is a piece of tuonsense# May I take about three minutes to give a a parable# I uh ...‘ I A953; 2 6: Speak masters? Ciardi 2 7— Uh I think it may have something to say# Uh.there's a lovely short story of Anatole France's and I want to make it into a parable of what goes on in the artistic process# 0 You recall the Juggler of Notre Dame uh went through France for a long time and made himself a good living by Juggling# He would get to a county fair and spread a rug on the ground and get down on his back and toss balls up in the air and Juggle them with his hands and his feet and his nose and all went well and he made a good living out of it# Then some years later one winter ill and broke and homeless he found himself wanderin down a back.road of France and he came to a monaster 9 brothers took him into the monastery and they kept him through the winterand tended him# When spring came around he foun himself recovered# He was,well# But he decided he didn't want to leave this monastery# He was going to stay there he had found a little island of peace# But he also discovered that this monastery was dedicated to the . praise of the Virgin and he liked that# But all or the monks were specialists of some sort One wrote hymns in praise of ‘the Virgin and one raised flowers for her alter and another illuminated manuscripts and another did decorations of a sort and only the juggler had nothing to give her# So he'took.to sweeping out the chapel# But one day while he was sweeping out he was taken by an urge to to do Something real for the Virgin and he took out his mat and Juggled for her# There There There after he uh he began to sneak in little sessions of Ju ling . before the statue while he was cleaning out the chapel;8 And one day one of the monks looked in and saw this happenin and called all the others and said Desecration of the temple; This is what the what Luce says about the novel every once in a while-the desecration of the temple# And the monks looked in through the window and sure enough there was the Juggler juggling in front of the_statu They were just about to run in and throw him out when they saw the Virgin come down from 156 the pedestal and wipe the sweat from the juggler's brow# And I uh I’ll leave you to pick out the moral# I th I think it‘s a real one but I think the gist of it is that anything intensely well done any anything positively done any good shape is a praise is a statement is an experience# It's a prayer# I don‘t know hOW'muCh more meanin there is than that# Mr. Sheets says poetry is about ideas; I have to go back to my original phrasing# It's not about ideas# It's about the superience of ideas and basically the one idea that a poem has to give is praisefi It says I make this# Sheets 2¢fi- . ' That's an idea# one; 2‘7" - " And and it's it's it’s an experience# It says in making this I live better more richly more truly than I do by not making this# Whether it means anything or not I'm more alive in the process# As Robert Frost said a while back a poem is a momentary stay against confusion# Yen can't get clarified to stay so# Uh you musn't think that# You have to do it all over again# But for a minute the poem clarifies a thing? It uh it’s an experience of life caught# I think that is the subject of all art# Sheets 3 O- I think praise is a great idea# Ciardi '3’- I think it's a greater experience# announcer 39~ John Ciardi was asked if he was not when he writes a poem in fact trying to convey an idea# Ciardi. 3?- "uw"m”' Uh no it is not an idea I want to convey to the reader# Let him find his own ideas# I'm trying to give him an experience# Uh yes ideas exist# I leave those to academicians to put into monographs# What I'm conveying what interests me is when you put this idea into a human being what portion of a life follows in the enactment of this idea# That‘s an experience# It's the idea in action#sfih if you we in philosophy they speak of pragmatic consequences# Sometimes it's hard to evaluate an idea a-concepty The e The only real measure turns out to be .0 all right I don't know how to definedthis thing in the abstract# But tell me how a human being bah so when he accepts this idea as a motivation and then I'll to 1 you how I feel about it# Thatis a way of measuring ideas you see# But it’s the experience # All all I’m interested in for any idea is what it means to a human being what it does inside him# Uh Mr. Heutra was speaking earlier of the fact that there’s no such thing as pure idea# That was a lost 18th century cau5s# Zeufve‘gotgthis ghole bush of dendrites around lL‘- 157 10. your glands on with all your fears on with all your anxieties on with all your Joys needy with all your irrationalities# And I refuse to be rational about this process# It's not a matter of problem solvin ' Uh it‘s it's a matter of those basic irrationalities we call the emotions and that do the most important things of our lives# Uh I found myself on a panel with the dean of engineering# And this panel was called a panel on creative writing# On creative thinking# And he told us what creative thinking was# He said it consists of five steps you define the limits of your problem uh you perform a qualitative analysis you then perform the quantitative mathematics check the quantitative mathematics and find the mechanical implementation of your aolutio ~ And that’s not creative thinking that's chose playing# That's problem solvin I have a great deal of respect for it we all have problems to solve# But I defy you t& do any of the basic things of a life in these terms# You can't get married this way by defining the girl and performing qualitative analysis and quantitative mathematics# You cant get married on can't beget a child you can't dic decently this way And all the cardinal points of our life all the hinge points all all the things that make our lives important to us are irrationalitiea# They're they're they're full of_very profound obscurities as the Bible is full of ringing marvellous obscurities because they involve those experiences that stir us# Resonance is the word here Thorbe a vibration# That is nothing to do with tefltbook ideas as stated APPENDIX II This study has as many implications as can be applied to message decoding in any form of inter-personal communication. When research of this type is further developed, the time may come when speaking 'and‘ listening can be taught by notation. Playing it by ear will, of course, remain the privilege of those who either lack training, or do not think it necessary to have such training. The rest will have access to information of increasing precision about how messages need to be encoded on the basis of how they are decoded. One of the implications of this study is that clarity of message content may be provided for in terms or cues, associated with specific features of perceptual organisation. The value of manipulating these cues in such a way as to achieve a well organized content has always been hasily perceived. Because the concept or organization itself’has been vague, what constitutes a cue to such information could not have a clearly defined function. As a matter of fact, the very notion of 'cuing' in this context may have been Just initiated in this study. 158 159 One would normally hear about 'well chosen words', and 'well constructed sentences', or about discourse characterized by 'unity', 'brevity', 'variety', etc., without ever getting to the roots of revealing a basic pattern of organization encompassing all these items. The problem of how to improve speech or writing has been more often than otherwise attacked from the point of view of 'vocabulary' or of 'grammar' in the narrow sense of 'linguistic' grammar, which whether 'prescriptive' or 'descriptive', must be recognized as a body of summary statements about a 'code'. What should receive more emphasis is the grammar of 'messages', as content-in-form, 'intra' as well as 'inter-sentential': and the psycholinguistic perceptual 'grammar', introduced in this study as cues and categories of organizational information. The first two chapters on contouring and accentuation cues emphasize the importance of the prosodic features of speech, as consisting in their capacity to outline and demarcate the structure of content units in a more or less general way. This is not to say that representational mediation would cease to operate, but that it would operate in unison with the referred to organizational cues. 160 At the integration level, this is borne out by the possible division of cues (in the.next three chapters on transition, combination, and integration) into 'syntactic' and 'contextual'. Syntactic cues are immediate constituent, or level, cues. They are both structural and 'semantic', i.e., representational. Contextual cues are both semantic and 'structural'. The one clustering phenomenon operates on the same principle of complementary distribution observed about allophones as related to phonemes. In brief, according to the orientation and main thesis of this study, it is recommended thatthe listener's or reader's expectations be given priority of consideration in any attempt to tackle problems of inadequate verbal communication: that organization of content in a broad sense, based on psychological principles, be the focus of investigatimu. In inter-personal communication, the value of fluency and speed is very often exaggerated. If we mean to 'communicate', it does not always pay to be ready with ‘words'. The chief characteristic of fluency is 'profusion', which is in many cases lacking in organizational cues. Particularly in a critical age like ours, speakers, salesmen, all public relations people, including teachers, might do well to contribute to research in this connection. 1‘31 Probably the most distinctive feature of personal style in speech or writing is organization. So far, almost nothing has been done, that the author is aware of, in terms of investigating the relative potency of oral cues relating to organization. More experimental work along these lines would give a tremendous push to the study of individual style. The focus would then shift to oral communication, which in itself is a big gain, especially if the aim of study is to improve the ability to predict in the taco-to-face situation. In oral inter-personal communication, more cues are available than in written communication. Particularly those cues that tell more about the individual are usually the ones we miss in analyzing written messages. For instance, speaking only of the organizational aspect, when we analyze oral communication, we have a chance to study pause and hesitation phenomena, which are almost completely obliterated in the written form. We can project accentuated items of content with great precision. We can probably detect signs of certainty or uncertainty, applicable to the communicator, his audience, or his topic, from a study of the feature of 'redundancy', which in this investigation, is given very broad implications. 162 The situation affecting the decoder's ability to predict forthcoming events in a message may be described as entropic in so far as it presents uncertainty to him. Such a situation is supposed to lose as much of entropy as it gains in redundancy. Research dealing with transitional and other integrational phenomena would provide ample chance of seeing these concepts in application. One may choose to consider the effect of pause and hesitation on the decoder, as he tries to put the pieces of the puzzle together. Passes of particularly longer duration, which fall between syntactic Juncture points, and thus seem to interrupt the continuous flow of speech, are assumed to correlate with the rise of entropy on the part of the decoder. They operate as 'noise'. While the normal juncture pauses are an aid to the decoder in a process of structuring relationships, hesitation pauses are a hindrance. It is suggested that hesitation pauses be experimented on in field study situations, with the aim of determining their effect on organization. Finally, this study has various implications for research in language learning. With the focus gathering on the 'message', as content-in-form, both 'vocabulary' and 'grammar' will be viewed in truer perspective. Research.could demonstrate to teachers 163 and learners how 'words' stand for content units, sometimes as separate, and sometimes as combined items. It could demonstrate how the code-structure students are taught at school is actually embedded in a psycholinguistic structure, based on principles of similarity, contiguity, contrast, etc. It could demonstrate how the organization of content units is inseperable from the so-called 'word order', and how sentence order is as important as 'word order'. Meanwhile, focus on the 'message' as content-in-form would rid language learning of the artificialities of code analyses. Learners would naturally be interested more in a study of how 'individual messages' are 'communicated' than in summary descriptions of how 'language' operates in general terms. And, as far as speech.training is concerned, research could show that more is involved in improving articulation, for instance, than a mere sharpening of blurred edges across word boundaries, to facilitate auditive reception, namely the provision of organization cues. Also, teachers telling their students to introduce more 'pitch variation', would come to realize that this is desirable, not only for the sake of change, not only to avoid monotony with a resultant drop in attentive reception, but particularly to provide the decoder with more organizational cues. In brief, the more research gets communication- oriented, the more it is directed to the investigation of relationships between linguistic and non-linguistic phenomena, the more meaning language would have for both speaker and listener. After all, when we speak or listen, we communicate with somebody somewhere. BIBLIOGRAPHY Ball, John, and Williams, Cecil B. Report Writing. Lew York: The Ronald Press 00., 1955. Bartlett, Sir Frederic. Thinkin , Ln Experimental and Social Study. London: George Allen & anin, 1956. Bendig, A. "Twenty Questions: An Information Analysis," Journal g§_Experimental Psychology, 1955, 46, 545-48. Berelson, Bernard. "Content Analysis," in G. Lindzey (ed.), Handbook of Social Psychology. Cambridge: Addison- WesleyPress, 1954. Berlo, David K. The Process of Communication. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston, Inc., 1960. Bloch, Bernard, and Trager, G. L. Outline of Linguistic Analysis. Baltimore, Md.: Waverly Press Inc., 1942. Special Publications of the Linguistic Society of America. Bloomfield, L. Language. Law York: 1955. Bosenquet, Bernard. Logic or the Lorphology of Lgowledge. Oxford University Press, London: Humphrey Lilford, 1911. (Second edition, vol. 1.) Boulding, Kenneth. The Ima e. Ann Arbor: University of Lichigan Press, $95 Brown, Roger, and Dulaney, D. "A Stimulus- -ReSponse Analy- sis of Language and Leaning, in P. Henle (ed. ), La as e, Thought and Culture. Ann Arbor: University E Michigan Press, 1958, 49-95. Brown, Roger, and Lenneberg, E. "A Study in Language and Cognition," Journal g; Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1954, 49, 454-62. Bruner, J. S., Goodnow, J. J., and Austin, G. A. §_Study g£_Thinking. New York: Wiley, 1956. Cantril, Hadley, and Allport, Gordon. The Psychology 2; Radio. New York: Harper, 1955. Carnap, Rudolph. The Logical Syntax of Languag_. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul Ltd., 1951. Carroll, John B. "The Analysis of Verbal Behavior," Ps - chological Review, 1944, 51, 102-19. Carroll, John B. "Communication Theory, Linguistics, and Psycholinguistics," Review e£_Educational Research, 28, 79-66. 185 166 Carroll, John 3., and Oasagrande, J. B. "The Effect of Linguistic Categories on Sorting Behavior," in E. E. Laccoby, T. H. Lewcomb, and E. L. Hartley (eds.), Read- ings lg Social Psychology, Lew York: Henry Holt aFEET, Carroll, John B. "A Factor Analysis of Verbal Abilities," Psychometrike, 6, 279-507. Carroll, John B. "The Interdisciplinary Summer Seminar on Linguistics and Psychology," Social Science Research Council, Item 5, 40-42. Carroll, John B. The Study e£_Lan as e. Cambridge, Hass.: Harvard University Press, 955. Carroll, Lewis. The Game of Logic. London and Lew York: Macmillan and 00., 1887} Oasagrande, Joseph, and Carroll, J. B. "The Effect of Lin- guistic Categories on Sorting Behavior," in E. E. Maccoby, T. M. Newcomb, and E. L. Hartley (eds.), Readin s in Social Psychology. New York: Henry Holt & 00., I953. Chase, Stuart. Power 9: Words. New York: Harcourt, Brace and 00., 1954. Cherry, Colin. gg_Human Communication. Lew York: Massachu- setts Institute of Technology,‘1957. Cofer, Charles H. "Verbal Behavior in Relation to Reasoning and Values," in Groups, Leadership and Len. Pittsburgh: Carnegie Institute ofTTechnology, 1951. Current Trends Lg Psychology and the Behavioral Sciences. Pittsburgh: Hep . 0 sychology, College of the Univer- sity of Pittsburgh, 1954. Current Trends lg_Psycnological Theory. Pittsburgh: Dept. of Paychology, College ofvthe University of Pittsburgh, 1951. Current Trends ;g_Information Theory. Pittsburgh: Dept. of Psthology, College of the University of Pittsburgh, 1951. as Saussure, Ferdinand. Course ;g_Genera1 Linguistics. Charles Bally, Albert Sechehaye‘Teds.) in collaEOra- tion with Albert Redilinger. (trans.) Baskin Wade. Lew York: Philosophical Library, 1959. Ellis, W. D. §_Source Book e£_Gestalt Psychology. Lew York: Harcourt, Brace and 00., 1956. 167 England, A. 0. "Getting Your Message Across by Plain Talk," Journal 2; Applied Psychology, 1950, 54, 182-85. Fano, R. M. The Transmission g£_1nformation. Cambridge, Lass.: Research Laboratory of’ElectronIcs, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, 1949-50. Technical Reports 13:03 e 65-149 s Flavell, J. H. "A Test of the Whorfian Theory," Psycholog;- cal Reports, 1958, 4, 455-62. Flesch, R. How 33 Write, Speak and Think More Effectively. New York: Harper and Brothers, i960. Flesch, R. "Shirt-Sleeve English in One Easy Lesson," Read- er's Digest, 67:46-48, August 1950. Fries, C. C. The Structure e: English. Lew York: Harcourt, Brace and COe, 1952e Gonzales, Richard C., and Cofer, Charles N. The Role g: Adjectives es Context Lg_the Organization of Heanin - fol Materials. ColIege Park: University ET'LaryIand, 1957. nepaffment of Psychology Technical Report No. 15. Gonzales, Richard C., and Cofer, Charles N. The Effects 3: Modifiers on the Organization of Verbal Material §e_Shown in CIfisterngin Free ReEEll. College Park: University of Maryland,—T95V} department of Psychology Technical Report No. 6. Gonzales, Richard C., and Cofer, Charles N. A Study o£_ Clustering in Verbs Adverbs and Four Word Seguences. College Park? University of MaryIand, 1957. Depart- ment of Psychology Technical Report No. 16. Greenberg, Joseph H. Essays ;§_Linguistics. Chicago, 111.: University of Chicago Press, 1958. Greenberg, Joseph. "Current Trends in Linguistics," Science, October 1959, 150, To. 5585. Harris, Z. "From Morpheme to Utterance," Language, 1946, 22 , 161'83 e Hayakawa, S. I. Language lg_Thought and Action. New York: Harcourt, Brace & Co., 1949. Herdan, Gustav. Language gs Choice and Chance. P. Hoord- hoff H. V.: Groningen, 1956. Hill, A. Introduction 33_Linguistic Structures. Lew York: Harcourt, Brace and 00., 1958. Hockett, C. F. é_Course in Modern Linguistics. Lew lork: The Macmillan Co., 1958. an»- ..- 168 Hockett, C. F. "Problems in Morphemic Analysis," Language, 1947, 25, 521-45. Hockett, C. F. "Review of Shannon and Weaver: A Mathematical Theory of Communication," Language, 1955, 29, 69-92. Hoijer Harry (ed.) Lan uses in Culture. Chicago: Univer- sity of Chicago Press, 1952. Hovland, Carl I. The Order e§_Presentation ;3_Persuasion. New Haven: Yale University Press, 1957. Howes, D. H. "The Definition and Measurement of Word Prob- ability." Unpublished Ph. D. thesis, Harvard Univer- sity, 1950-51. Howes, D. H. "The Intelligibility of Spoken Messages," American Journal g£_Psychology, 1952, 65, 460-65. Howes, D. H., and Osgood, c. E. "On the Combination of Associative Probabilities in Linguistic Contexts," American Journal 3: Psychology, 1954, 67, 241-58. Howes, D. H. "On the Interpretation of Word Frequency as a Variable Affecting Speed of Recognition," Journal 3:.Experimenta1 Psychology, 1954, 48, 106-12. Howes, D. H. "On the Relation between the Intelligibility and Frequency of Occurrence of English Words," Journal 3; the Acoustical SociegyLQELAmerica, 1957, 29, 296-505. Hull, 0. L. Principles 23 Behavior. Lew York, 1945. Huppe, Bernard F., and Kaminsky, Jack. Logic and Language. Lew York: Alfred A. Knopf, 1956. Jacobson, R., Fant, C. G., and Halls, M. Preliminaries §o_Speech Analysis: The Distinctive Features and their Correlates. Cambridge, Lass.: Acoustic Laboratory, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, 1952. Techni- cal Report Lo. 15. Joos, Martin. "Description of Language Design," The Journal o: the Acoustical Society 3: America, 1950, 22 (6), 708. Harwoski, T. R., Odbert, H. S., and Osgood, Charles. "Stu- dies in Synthetic Thinking: II The Role of Form in Visual Response to Music," Journal 3: General Psychol- ggy, 1942, 26, 199-222. Katona, G. Organizing and Hemorizing. New York: Columbia University Press, I940. Katz, David. Gestalt Psychology. Translated by Robert Tyson. Lew York: The Ronald Press 00., 1950. fi-“3 ARIA“; _.' 169 Hoffka, I. Principles of Gestalt Psychology. Lew York: Harcourt-Brace, 1955} Kohler, Wolfgang. Dynamics lg_Psychology. Lew York: Grove Press, Inc., 1960. Horner, Stephan. Conceptual Thinkinv. Cambridge, Bng.: Cambridge University Press, 1955. Lado, Robert. Linguistics Across Cultures. Ann Arbor, Mich.: University of Lichigan Press, 1957. Lenneberg, Eric. "A Hots on Cassirer's Phi1080phy of Ian- guage," Phil. Phenomenol. Res., 1955, 15, 512-22. Lenneberg, Eric, and Brown, R. "A Study in Language and Cognition," Journal e: Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1954, 49, 454-62. Lenneberg, Eric, and Brown, Roger. "Studies in Linguistic Relativity," in E. E. Haccoby, T. M. Newcomb, and E. L. Hartley (eds.), Readings in Social Psychology. New York: Henry Holt & Co., 1958. Linsky, Leonard (ed.) Semantics. Urbana: University of Illinois, 1952. Lloyd, Donald J., and Warfel, Harry R. American English %% its Cultural Setting. Lew York: Alfred A. Lnopf, 55. Logan, Frank, 33 gL. Behavior Theory and Social Science. Kew'Haven: Publishéd byfithe Institute ofLHuman Rela- tions, Yale University Press, 1955. haclay,‘Howard, and Osgood, Charles E. "Hesitation Pheno- mena in Spontaneous English Speech," Word, April 1959, 15, 1&0. 1e Miller, George A. "Information Theory and the Study of Speech," in R. A. Patton (ed.), Current Trends ;§_ Information Theor . Pittsburgh: University ET'Pitts- Bhrgh Press, 1954. Killer, George A., and Ricklider, J. C. R. “The Intelligi- bility of Interrupted Speech," The Journal of the Acoustical Society gg America, 1950, 22, 167:75. Miller, George. Language and Communication. Lew York, 1951. Miller, George A. "Psycholinguistics," in Gardner Lindsey (ed.), Handbook 3; Social Psychology. Vol. II. Cam- bridge, Hass.: Addison-Wesley, 1954. 170 Miller, George A. "Speech and Communication," The Journgl_ g£_the Acoustical Society e§_America, 1958, 50, 597-98. Miller, George A. "Speech.and Language," in S. S. Stevens (ed.), Handbook of Experimental Psychology. Lew York: Wiley and Sons, 1951. Iiller, G. A. "What is Information Leasurement?," American Psychologist, 1955, 8, 5-11. Miller, Gerald, Zavos, Harry, Vlandis, John, and Rosenbaum, Lilton. The Effect of Differential Reward to Speakers on Speech Patterns. "gtate University of Iowa, n. d. Dittoed. "+7” Morris, Charles W. "Foundations of the Theory of Signs," in International Encyclopedia of Unified Science. Yul. %, N0. 2. Chicago: University_ of Chicago Press, 1958, 7-157. Morris, Charles. Signs, Language and Behavior. Lew York: Prentice-Hall, Inc., 1946. Hegel, Ernest. "Principles of the Theory of Probability," in International Encyclopedia of Unified Sciences. Vol. I, L0. 6. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1959, 545-422. Newman, S. E. "Effects of Contiguity and Similarity on the Learning of Concepts," Journal of :xperimental Psychol- ogy, 1956, 52, 549- 55. Ogden, C. K., and Richards, 1. A. The Leaning of IEeaning. London and New York, 1948. Osgood, Charles. "Factor Analysis of Leaning," Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1955, 50, 525-58. Osgood, C. E. Isthod and Theory in Experimental Psycholggy. Lew York: 1955. Osgood, Charles E., Suci, George J., and Tannenbaum, Percy H. The Measurement of Meaning. Urbana: University of Illinois Press, 1957. Osgood, Charles. "Nature and Measurement of Leaning," Psy- chological Bulletin, 1952, 49, 197-257. Osgood, Charles, and Sebeck, Thomas (eds. ) "Psycholinguis- tics, " Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, (supplementT—TOT vol: 49 TOctbber, l954), Lo. 4. Bloom- ington: Indiana University Press, 1954. Payne, S. L. "Can You Lake it Brief?, " Chap. 8 of The Art of Asking Questions. Princeton: Princeton University Press, 1951,129-57. 17.1 Petermann, Bruno. The Gestalt Theory and the Problem g£_ Configuratigg, TranSlatEd by Leyer Fortes. LEEdon: Routledge and Kegan Paul Ltd., 1950. Pike, K. L. The Intonation g§_American Eng;ish. Ann Arbor: University of Lichigan Press, 1956. Pike, K. L. Phonemics. Ann Arbor, Mich.: University of Lichigan Press, 1947. Publications in Linguistics. Pike, K. L. Phonetics. Ann Arbor, Lich.: University of Michigan Press, 1955. Raffel, G. "Two Determinants of the Effect of Primacy," American Journal 3; Psyghology, 1956, 48, 654-57. Rapaport, David (trans.) Organization and Pathology 3: Thought. New York: Columbia University Press, 1951. Reichenbach, Hans. Experience and Prediction. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press, 1958. Ruesch, Jurgen, and Bateson, Gregory. Communication. Lew York: W. W. Lorton and Co., Inc., 1951. Sapir, Edward. Language. Lew York: Harcourt, Brace & Company, 1921. Sapir, Edward. Selected Writings. Edited by David Landelbaum. Berkeley: University of Ca ifornia Press, 1949. Saporta, Sol, and Sebeck, Thomas A. "Linguistics and Con- tent Analysis," Trends ;g_Content Analysis. Urbana: University of Illinois Press, 1959. Saul, E. V., and Osgood, Charles. "Perceptual Organization of Materials as a Factor Influencing Ease of Learning and Degree of Retention," Journal of Experimental £31} chology, 1950, 40, 572-79. Shannon, 0. E., and Weaver, W. The Lathematical Theogy a: Communication. Urbana: University of Illinois Press, 1949} Skinner, B. F. Verbal Behavior. Lew York: Appleton- Century—Crofts, 1957. Solomon, P., and Werner, H. "Studies on Contour: III. Negative After Images," American Journal 3: Psychology, 1952, 65, 67-74. Sondel, Bess. The Humanity_g£ Words. New York: The World Publishing Company, 1958. 172 Trench, Richard C. Trench 33 the Study gg'Words. Lew York: Redfield, Clinton Hall, 1852. Voeglin, Charles. "Distinctive Features and Leaning Equiva- lence," Language, 1948, 24, 152-55. Voeglin, Charles. "Linguistics Without Leaning and Culture Without Words," Proc. Ind. Acad. Sci., 1949, 55, 47-49. Abstract. Warfel, Harry. Language: §_Science g§_Human Behavior. Cleve- land: Howard Allen, Inc., 1962. F‘ Werner, H. "Change of Leaning: a Study of Semantic Processes Through the Experimental Method," Journal g§_General Psychology, 1954, 50, 181-208. I Werner, H., and Kaplan, E. "Development of Word Leaning Through Verbal Context: An Experimental Study," Journal 3: Psychology, 1950, 29, 251-57. 5 Werner, H. "A Psychological Analysis of Expressive Language: in Heinz Werner (ed.), Qg_Expressive Langpage: Papers Presented g3 the Clark University Conference gg_Express- gyg_Language Behavior, Worcester: Clark Universi y Press, 19 o Werner, H., and Wapner, S. "Toward a General Theory of Per- ception," Psychological Review,1952, 59, 524-58. Wertheimer, m. Productive Thinking. New York, 1945. Whatmough, J. Language. New York, 1957. Whorf, Benjamin Lee. Language, Thought and Reality. Edited by John B. Carroll} Lew York: John Wiley & Sons, Inc., 1956. ROOM USE ONLY. 1 L ~ , ... “:11