é IIHUIWIHWUINIWIHIWNIUWNW'IIIWI 081 ‘ Mama This is to certify that the thesis entitled AERODYNAMIC EVALUATION OF THE CONNECTED DISCOURSE Date OF NORMAL-HEARING AND SEVERELY TO PROFOUNDLY HEARING—IMPAIRED ADULTS presented by Peter Feudo, Jr. has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for M.A. August 11, 1978 degree in Andi°108Y & Speech LIBRARY Michigan State University ' Sciences 5 gég. s—‘k Major professor AERODYNAMIC EVALUATION OF THE CONNECTED DISCOURSE OF NORMAL-HEARING AND SEVERELY TO PROFOUNDLY HEARING-IMPAIRED ADULTS BY Peter Feudo, Jr. A THESIS Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of MASTER OF ARTS Department of Audiology and Speech Sciences 1977 Accepted by the faculty of the Department of Audiology and Speech Sciences, College of Communication Arts, Michigan State University, East Lansing, Michigan. September 26, 1977 \"‘\—-———cg~c \— i ‘ \.\\ Linda L. Smith, Ph.D. Chairperson \L [/ givq Leo V. Deal. Ph.D. Paul A. Cooke, M.S. ABSTRACT AERODYNAMIC EVALUATION OF THE CONNECTED DISCOURSE OF NORMAL-HEARING AND SEVERELY TO PROFOUNDLY HEARING-IMPAIRED ADULTS by Peter Feudo, Jr. The purpose of this study was to investigate aerodynamic characteristics of connected discourse produced by normal-hearing and severely to profoundly hearing- impaired adults. Eight normal-hearing and eight orally trained hearing4impairedadults participated. Each subject read a total of eighteen sentences. Six sentences were five syllables in length; six sentences were ten syllables in length, and six sentences were fifteen syllables in length. For a spontaneous speech sample, each subject des- cribed the activities depicted in a picture of a pet shop scene. All utterances were sensed by a pneumotachograph and recorded by an Optical oscillograph. A simultaneous audborecording was made. Inspiratory and expiratory vol- 'umes and expiratory time were measured. Three judges assessed the word intelligibility of the hearing-impaired speakers' utterances. The results indicated that the hearing-impaired subjects displayed greater ranges of inspiratory air volumes and spontaneous expiratory air volumes and greater expiratory time than the normal hearing subjects. Additionally, in going from contrived to spontaneous utterances, the hearing—impaired subjects varied expiratory air volumes with a systematic change in speech intelligi— bility. The therapeutic importance of these results was discussed in support of increasing emphasis on aerodynamic controls for developing the speech of the hearing-impaired. LIST OF TABLES . LIST OF FIGURES Chapter I. II. III. IV. V. TABLE OF CONTENTS INTRODUCTION . . . . Statement of the Problem . . EXPERIMENTAL PROCEDURES . . . Subjects. Stimuli . Instrumentation . . Method . Analysis 0 RESULTS . Speech Intelligibility f the Data Inspiratory Data . . Expiratory Volume Data Expiratory DISCUSSION SUMMARY . Time Data LIST OF REFERENCES . . . . . APPENDI A. B. CES . . . Auditory Thresholds for Pure Tone Averages Results of Individual Subject Subject Subject Subject Subject Subject Subject Subject #l(male) . #2(male) . #3(female) #4(female) #5(female) #6(female) #7(female) #8(female) ii Hearing Evaluations . Page iv vi 10 ll 13 l4 l7 l7 19 24 26 3O 39 41 45 46 48 49 50 51 52 53 54 55 56 C. D. E. Case Histor and Release Form . Sentence Stimuli . . . . . . . Instructions. . . . . . . . . . iii LIST OF TABLES Table Page 1. The number of errors in word intelligibility scored by three judges and the mean percent correct word intelligibility for hearing- impaired subjects . . . . . . . . . . . . . 18 2. The mean number of inspirations taken by hearing-impaired subjects for each of three sentence lengths. . . . . . . . . . . 19 3. The mean inspiratory air volume and stan- dard deviation (SD) (in cc) for each of three sentence lengths for normal-hearing subjects and hearing—impaired subjects. . . 22 4. The mean overall per syllable inspiratory air volume and standard deviation (SD) (in cc), expiratory air volume (in cc), and expira- tory time (in sec) for normal-hearing sub- jects and hearing-impaired subjects . . . . 23 5. The mean expiratory air volume and standard deviation (SD) (in cc) for each of three sentence lengths for normal—hearing sub- jects and hearing-impaired subjects . . . . 25 6. The mean expiratory time and standard deviation (SD) (in sec) for each of three sentence lengths for normal-hearing subjects and hear- ing-impaired subjects . . . . . . . . . . . 27 7. The mean and standard deviation (SD) of data for hearing-impaired subject #8 (female). . 28 8. The percent correct word intelligibility and the mean number of inspirations for the poor intelligibility hearing-impaired subjects during the sentence condition . . . . . . . 30 iv 9. The change in intelligibility and expiratory air volume (in percentage) for six hearing- impaired subjects. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 35 LIST OF FIGURES Figure Page 1. Schematic representation of instrumental array 0 O Q Q . C C C Q C ' C O C O O Q C O O 12 2. Schematic representation of the production of a lS-syllable sentence by normal—hearing subject #7(female) and by hearing-impaired subject #4(fema1e) . . . . . . . . . . . . . 32 vi CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION The speech production of hearing-impaired indi- viduals has been a tOpic reported in the literature for many years. The majority of the literature which has described the speech production of the hearing-impaired has utilized perceptual bases for such descriptions. Per- ceptual comparisons have been made of the speech of hear- ing-impaired speakers and normal-hearing speakers rather than judgments.amo a museum mconmouoflz _ Ell: HwUSpmGMHB OHSmmmHm Hmaucmummmao A xmmEmomm \ Z adamaamam A. nmmumonomu085mcm 13 The system was calibrated prior to each subject by directing a constant source of air with a known velocity through a precision bore flowrator glass tube (Fischer and Porter Co., #FP—l/2—27-G—10/27) connected to the pneumo- tachograph and differential pressure transduction system. The rate of air flow was measured on the flowrator and equated with specific galvanometric deflections. Subjects were run on various days. Recording time was minimal. Normal-hearing subjects utilized up to 93 seconds. Hear- ing-impiared subjects utilized up to 162 seconds. Repeated calibration and balancing of the recording system were used in order to prepare the equipment to be sensitive to the large range of values which were recorded. Method The subjects received written and oral instruc- tions in the experimental procedures (See Appendix E). Each subject completed a case history, indicated his com— prehension of the vocabulary which comprised the sentence stimuli, and received a hearing evaluation. At the con- clusion of these tasks, the subject proceeded to the recording room for aerodynamic evaluation. The experimen- ter explained the test procedures and demonstrated the use of the facemask. Prior to the sentence stimuli, the sub— jects were given three practice sentences corresponding to each pair of test sentences. Prior to the picture stimulus, the subject was given a practice picture. The l4 practice items were used for adjustment to speaking into the facemask. When the subject had practiced and had indicated willingness to proceed, the test items were presented. The presentations of the sentence and picture stimuli were counterbalanced. The six sentences had been typed on individual cards and were presented three times each for a total of eighteen stimulus cards. Sentence order was randomized for each subject. Analysis of the Data For each sentence and sentence length mean values and ranges for three dependent variables were cal- culated from the oscillographic recordings. Those de- pendent variables were (1) inspiratory air volume, (2) expratory air volume, and (3) time of expiratory air flow. An Ott Compensating Polar Planimeter was utilized for cal- culating the volume measurements. By manually circumstracing the recorded deflec— tions and the baseline, the planimeter displayed a dimen- sionless value for the traced areas. The planimeter measurements were varified by carrying out two tracings beginning in different but symmetrically opposed positions along the perimeter of the area. The mean value of these two measurements was correct because of the automa- tic compensation of the planimeter utilized. By reversing the direction of the trace, the planimeter value returned to zero proving its accuracy. The verified planimeter 15 measurement was entered into the following forumla (L2) yielding the air volume within a particular inspiratory or expiratory deflection. AV = (L2)(K2) mHm wmoammmem mo mo Ho moo moo nvo wmo NHo moo some am some ow some 0m some am smmE mHanHMm mom mannaawm mom mosmusmm wosmuswm mosmusmm sump msomsmusomm pump mosmucmm oHanHMmimH oHanHmmloH mannaammlm MEDAO> MH¢ MMOBoc unopsmum can some wnall.h mamas 29 sentence stimuli. For the hearing-impaired subjects, these ranges are 1.75 sec to 3.43 sec, 3.27 sec to 6.18 sec, and 3.84 sec to 7.02 sec, respectively. The mean overall ET and their standard deviatflxm; appear in Table 4. For the normal-hearing subject group, these range from .23 sec/syllable to .30 sec/syllable in sentence speech and .18 sec/syllable to .28 sec/syllable in spontaneous speech. Group means are .27 sec/syllable and .24 sec/syllable for sentence and spontaneous speech. For the hearing—impaired subject group, mean overall ET ranges from .29 sec/syllable to .54 sec/syllable in sen- tence speech and from .30 sec/syllable to .49 sec/syllable in spontaneous speech. Respective group means are .41 sec/syllable and .39 sec/syllable. Mean expiratory time ranges are increasingly greater for hearing-impaired subjects. Rank order of all normal-hearing and hearing-impaired subjects indicates that hearing-impaired subjects rank 1, 2, 3, 4, 5, 7, 8, 12 in ET utilized for producing the five syllable sentence stimuli, 1 through 8 for the ten syllable sentence stim- uli, and l, 2, 3, 4, 5, 6, 7, 9 for the fifteen syllable sentence stimuli. For the hearing-impaired subjects, increased expiratory time was significant to the 1.0% level in the 5 syllable sentence task and significant to th 0.2% level in the 10 syllable and 15 syllable tasks, and in the overall per syllable sentence and spontaneous data (Wilcoxon Sum of Ranks Test). CHAPTER IV DISCUSSION This experiment documents air stream mismanage- ment during the connected speech of the hearing-impaired. Mismanagement occurs during inspiratory and expiratory events. The hearing-impaired speakers with poor intelli- gibility in the sentence condition inspire more frequently than the hearing-impaired speakers with 100% intelligi- bility and the normal-hearing speakers. Table 8 indicates Table 8.--The percent correct word intelligibility and the mean number of inspirations for the poor intelli- gibility hearing-impaired subjects during the sentence condition. mean number of inspirations subject % intelligible 5-sy11. 10-syll. 15-syll. 3(female) 90 1 1 1.33 2(male) 86 1 2.16 1.66 1(ma1e) 71 1 2.16 2.16 4(female) 03 1.83 4.5 6 that as intelligibility decreases the number of inspira- tions increases. These inspirations occur randomly throughout sentence productions. The inspirations do not occur simultaneously with the initiation of noun or verb phrases. The inspirations occur within phrases and within words. In addition to decreasing word intelligibility, 30 31 random inspirations decrease the listeners‘ abilities to recognize syntactic patterns within utterances (John and Howarth, 1965). This mismanagement is ammenable to therapy. John and Howarth (1965) substantiated treatment for rela- tive time factors of syllable length, sentence length, and non-linguistic pauses during connected discourse to the exclusion of articulation emphasis. Repeated imitation of a normal-hearing speaker's rate and rhythm yielded increasing meassage comprehension by increasing word intel- ligibility and syntactic intelligibility (by 56% and 203%, respectively, in John and Howarth, 1965). The significantly increased expiratory time utilized by the hearing-impaired speakers is consistent with the early literature (Hudgins, 1934; Rawlings, 1935, 1936; Voelker, 1935, 1938). The hearing-impaired speakers' expiratory time was 52% and 63% greater than that of the normal-hearing speakers in the sentence and spontaneous conditions, respectively. These data coupled with expira- tory air volumes similar to normal-hearing speakers yielded decreased air flow rate peak magnitudes. Such decreases are a parameter of blurred consonant production (Hutchinson and Smith, 1974). Figure 2 represents the pro- duction of a lS-syllable sentence by normal-hearing subject #7 (female) and by hearing-impaired subject #4 (female). The normal-hearing subject inspired 788 cc of air immedi- ately prior to the intelligible production of the sentence 32 Figure 2.-—Schematic representation of the production of a 15-sy11able sentence by (A) normal—hearing subject #7 (female) and by (B) hearing-impaired subject #4 (female). Pattern (A) illustrates on inspiration of 788 cc and expiration of 1060 cc during 3.32 seconds. Pattern (B) illustrates seven random inspirations of 1040 cc and expiration of 1020 cc during 5.94 seconds of expiratory time. Pattern (B) represents completely unintelligible speech by the hearing— impaired subject. The stimulus is, "You know how to stick to things until you find out the answers." 33 oo ovoa u ofisao> Had huoumuflmmcH mpaoomm wo.m u mafia muoumuwmxm {a E i>§>§§ oo omoa u mEsHo> Hflm muoumuflmxm Amy zmmaamm 00 wow u mESHo> Had whoumuwmmsH mcsoomm mm.m n mafia muoumuflmxm ' oo omoH u mEdHo> Had mnoumnflmxm Amv zmmaamm 34 and expired 1060 cc of air in 3.32 seconds. The hearing— impaired subject inspired 1040 cc of air in 7 random inspirations and expired 1020 cc of air in 5.94 seconds throughout a production which was entirely unintelligible. Visual analysis of the two air flow rate (AFR) traces notes a lack of clear peaks in the AFR of the hearing-impaired speaker. That trace denotes air flow turbulence with an absence of clear consonant production. Although expiratory air volumes are similar, the disturbance of time factors and the manner in which the air flow was expired lead to unintelligible speech. This mismanagement also appears responsive to therapy. Hutchinson and Smith (1974) demonstrated the ability of hearing-impaired adults to condition aerodynamic and concurrent intelligibility changes in monosyllabic stimuli. Their subjects successively approximated Optic- ally recorded information from equipment similar to that used in this experiment. Further research should attempt to increase the length of stimuli practiced in therapy. Perhaps a two- stage treatment program would be effective if based on decreasing expiratory time prior to direct conditioning of air flow. Control for the spontaneity of stimuli should be continued. The present experiment reveals the occurrence of a distinct aerodynamic event effecting the decreased word intelligibility of hearing-impaired speakers from 35 contrived sentence stimuli to spontaneous stimuli. The group mean intelligibility decreases 14%, whereas indivi- dual subjects exhibit a range from no change to -36% (see Table 9). Table 9 was obtained from data presented in Tables 1 and 4. Table 9.--The change in intelligibility and expiratory air volume (in percentage) for six hearing-impaired subjects. hearing-impaired intelligibility expiratory air subject changes volume change 1(ma1e) -28% +209% 3(female) -18% +26% 5(female) —04% -47% 6(female) -04% -41% 7(female) -36% +208% 8(fema1e) 0% -44% Excluding Subject #2 and Subject #4 because of incomplete data, the six remaining subjects can be reviewed in two groups regarding intelligibility change in the two stimuli conditions. Subjects #7, #1, and #3 decrease their intel- ligibility 36%, 28%, and 18%, respectively. Subjects #5 and #6 decrease their intelligibility only 4%, while Subject #8 remains 100% intelligible. These changes in intelligibility are consistent with changes in expiratory air volumes. The group of Subjects #7, #1, and #3, while markedly decreasing word intelligibility, increase their EAV by 208%, 109%, and 26%, respectively. Subject #7, who displays the greatest decrease in intelligibility and 36 the greatest increase in EAV, is 100% word intelligible during the production of contrived sentences. During that condition, Subject #7 inspires apprOpriately. Subject #7, along with Subjects #1 and #3 appear to lose physiologic control when required to speak spontaneously. In the spon— taneous condition, these subjects produce 5, 9, and 9 syllables respectively per inspiration. The remaining hearing-impaired subjects and the normal-hearing subjects produce from 10 to 36 syllables per inspiration. Whether the result of ineffective laryngeal or articulatory valving, the changes in EAV are contrary to the performance of the hearing-impaired subjects with highly intelligible spon- taneous speech and to the performance of the normal-hearing subjects. Subjects #5, #6, and #8, with minimal or no de- crease in intelligibility, decrease their EAV in the spon- taneous condition by 47%, 41%, and 44%. This approaches the 65% decrease (range of 31-81%) which the normal-hearing subjects exhibit. For intelligible spontaneous speech, such information illustrates the necessity to decrease and retain expiratory air volumes from the amount utilized in contrived sentences. We are concerned with the percentage of EAV retained by each subject going from the contrived conditions to the spontaneous condition as opposed to the absolute value of EAV. The retention of EAV by intelli- gible speakers indicates the recognition and use of a valving adjustment for spontaneous speech. Further 37 research to account for this adjustment is warranted. The data presented are consistent with other research findings. Forner and Hixon (1977) assessed res- piratory events of profoundly hearing-impaired speakers. Their evaluation utilized the apparati for respiration kinematics established by Hixon, Goldman, and Mead (1973). Subjects (young adults) communicated predominantly by manual or total (simultaneous oral and manual) communication and were judged moderately to severely deviant in articulation, rhythm, stress and linguistic phrasing. Data collected during utterances of continuous discourse displayed frequent deviancies in one or more of the following areas: (1) lin- guistic programming, (2) respiratory adjustments, (3) laryn— geal or upper airway adjustments. Similar to the present thesis, Forner and Hixon noted that most of their subjects inspired frequently during nonpunctuated word strings which increased the amount of breaths per minute. Thus, the num- ber of syllables supported per breath was decreased. Forner and Hixon noted tracings which were not accmpanied by vocalization and noted breath haltings which did not involve inspiration. Many subjects expired volumes up to 250 cc during these haltings. This further decreased the available volumes which, as seen in the present thesis, were decreased initially by large volume expenditures. Additionally, the breath haltings involved a phenomenon similar to the cessation of measurable respiratory activity 38 by this thesis' Subject #8. Possibly, this subject retained intelligibility by conserving necessary air volumes. The support of the Forner and Hixon data for the data of the present thesis is clear. A range of respira- tory behaviors is seen during the connected utterances of severely to profoundly hearing-impaired adults. Relation- ships to speech intelligibility are noted. These relation- ships need to be expressed in therapeutic programs at the levels of linguistic, respiratory, and laryngeal mechanics. An appropriate component of such programming is the analy- sis of the communicative behavior of the hearing-impaired in terms of speech intelligibility and aerodynamic func- tioning. This component, apparently unbiased by minimal variations in extent of hearing loss, establishes initial and continuous evaluation clearly representative of the needs for effective expressive communication. CHAPTER V SUMMARY The purpose Of the study was tO investigate aerodynamic characteristics Of connected discourse pro— duced by normal-hearing and severely-to-profoundly hearingh impaired adults. The results added support for the use Of aerodynamic evaluation in speech—disordered populations while providing normative information from normal-hearing and hearing-impaired populations. This is intended to provide a basis for future investigations. The results established aerodynamic mismanagement during the speech Of the hearing-impaired evidenced by increased frequency Of inspirations for hearing-impaired subjects with poor speech, increased ranges Of inspiratory and expiratory air volumes, and greatly increased expira- tory time. The general result of these characteristics was a blurring Of aerodynamic peaks necessary for intel- ligible speech. Further mismanagement was noted in proceeding from contrived to spontaneous stimuli. Hearing- impaired subjects who suffered minimal change in speech intelligibility executed aerodynamic adjustments Opposed tO those Of the normal-hearing and minimal change hearing- impaired subjects. 39 40 Previous therapeutic investigations were des- cribed and, in conjunction with this study, are supportive Of therapeutic emphasis on aerodynamic management. Con- nected discourse has been seen as an appropriate and relia- ble stimulus for use in future investigations. Further investigations should consider the ex— tent Of hearing—impairment and the status of respiratory physiology. In this study,speech intelligibility and aero- dynamic measures did not vary in relation to hearing loss. For example, hearing—impaired Subject #6 (female) had the poorest pure tone average while exhibiting 100% intelligi— ble speech in the contrived condition and 96% intelligible Speech in the spontaneous condition. The status Of the subjects' respiratory physiology was not evaluated beyond screening for gross abnormalities. Perhaps further evalua- tion would explain the lack Of respiratory control exhibited by the hearing—impaired subjects Of the present investi- gation. LIST OF REFERENCES 41 42 LIST OF REFERENCES Angelocci, A., Kopp, G., and Holbrook, A., The vowel for- mants Of deaf and normal-hearing eleven to fourteen year Old boys. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 29, 156-170 (1964). Bzoch, K. R., Variations in velopharyngeal valving as a function Of syllabic change in repeated CV syllables. Paper presented at the Annual Convention Of the Ameri- can Speech and Hearing Association, Chicago (1965). Colton, R. H. and Cooker, H. S., Perceived nasality in the speech Of the deaf. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 11, 553-559 (1968). DiCarlo, L. M., Speech, language, and cognitive abilities of the hard-of—hearing. Proceedings of the Institute on Aural Rehabilitation, 45-66 (1968). Forner, L. L. and Hixon, T. J., Respiratory kinematics in profoundly hearing-impaired speakers. Journal Of Speech and Hearing Research, 20, 323-408 (1977). Gilbert, H. R. and Dixon, H. P., Simultaneous oral and nasal airflows during production of stop consonants by hard Of hearing subjects. Paper presented at the Annual Convention Of the American Speech and Hearing Associa- tion, Las Vegas (1974). Hixon, T. J., Goldman, M., and Mead, J., Kinematics Of the chest wall during speech production: Volume displace- ments Of the rib cage, abdomen, and lung. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 16, 70-115 (1973). Hudgins, C. V., A comparative study Of the speech coordina— tions Of deaf and normal subjects. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 44, 3-48 (1934). and Numbers, F. C., An investigation of the intelligibility Of the speech of the deaf. Genetic Psychology Monographs, 25, 289-392 (1942). Hutchinson, J. M. and Ringel, R. L., Aerodynamic patterns of stuttered speech. Paper presented at the Annual Con- vention of the American Speech and Hearing Association, Detroit (1973). and Smith, L. L., An aerodynamic evaluation of consonant production in the adult deaf. Paper presented at the Annual Convention Of the American Speech and Hearing Association, Las Vegas (1974). 43 Isshiki, N., Vocal intensity and air flow rate. Folia Phoniatrica, 17, 92—104 (1965). Isshiki, N. and Ringel, R., Air flow during the production Of selected consonants. Journal Of Speech and Hearing Research, 7, 233-244 (1964). John, J. E. J. and Howarth, J. N., the effect Of time dis- tortions on the intelligibility Of deaf children's speech. Language and Speech, 8, 127-134 (1965). McClumpha, S. L., Cinefluorographic investigation Of velo- pharyngeal function in selected deaf speakers. Master's thesis, University Of Florida (1966). M011, K. L., Cinefluorographic techniques in speech research. Journal Of Speech and Hearing Research, 3, 227—241.(1960L McGlone, R. E., Air flow during vocal fry phonation. Jour- nal Of Speech and Hearing Research, 10, 289-298 (I967). , Air flow in the upper register. FOlia Phonia- trica, 22, 231-238 (1970). O'Donnell, M., All Through the Year: Harper and Row Basic Reading Program. New York: Harper and Row (1967). Rawlings, C. G., A comparative study Of the movements of the breathing muscles in speech and quiet breathing of deaf and normal subjects. American Annals Of the Deaf, 80, 147-156 (1935). , 81, 136-150 (1936). Silverman-Dresner, T. and Guilfoyle, G. R., Vocabulary Norms for Deaf Children. Washington, D. C.: Alexander Graham Bell Association for the Deaf (1972). Smith, C. R., Residual hearing and speech production in deaf children. CSL Research Report, 4 (1973). Subtelny, J. D., Worth, J. H., and Sakuda, M., Intraoral pressure and rate of flow during speech. Journal Of Speech and Hearing Research, 9, 498-518 (1966). VanHattum, R. J., Communication therapy for problems asso- ciated with cleft palate; in Dickson, Communication Disorders: Remedial Principles and Practices. Glen- view, Illinois: Scott, Foresman, and Co. (1974). 44 and Worth, J. H., Air flow rate in normal speakers. Cleft Palate Journal, 4, 137-147 (1967). Voelker, C. H., A preliminary strobophotoscopic study Of the speech Of the deaf. American Annals Of the Deaf, 80, 243-259 (1935). , An experimental study of the comparative rate Of utterance Of deaf and normal hearing speakers. American Annals Of the Deaf, 83, 274-284 (1938). Warren, D. W. and Wood, M. T., Respiratory volumes in speech: A possible reason for intraoral pressure differences among voiced and voiceless consonants. Journal Of the Acoustical Society Of America, 45, 466—469 (I969). APPENDICES 45 APPENDIX A AUDITORY THRESHOLDS FOR PURE TONE AVERAGES 46 47 om moa mm om mm mos mm moa mos mos mos ms me me mos cos moa mos mm mm mm om om mm mm mm mm mm moa mz moa mz mm mz mos on ma om m» cm ms mm Em mz mm om om mz mm mm moa cos mm mm moa cos .<.ar.m um ooom um oooa as com .a .9 .m as ooom um coca .mmMImmmm umm usmmm .moma .Hmza "mu .aem mo as we ooom can .Nm OOH .Nm com um muomnnsm mm mm OOH on mm om mm mm Nm oom Amamfimmvm AmHmEmmoo Amamfimmvm AmHmEmmvm Adamsmmoe AmHmEmmvm Amamsom Aoamsoa pomflnsm UOHHMQEHImsflHmmn How mommuo>m msou muse can mpaonmmunu sofluospsoo Hfl¢1|.¢ xflpsommm APPENDIX B RESULTS OF INDIVIDUAL HEARING EVALUATIONS 48 49 SUBJECT #1(MALE) Frequency in Hertz (Hz) m mxnmmmfifle spasm: 0 fl“ 306wppnk nausea Hwawnn manna: an a o / / A mm _ A _ _ _ _ HO X MR 0. 0. a... 5.. h. 9.. 1. SO c / // r a R Tu E o ooapwwmsom Om onnnnca W O 2 h 6 8 m 8 o m o m m m m o m m 0 a 125 c m" m m m. a“ w 3me .325 no 5 ~33 o 0 7 9 caozmwune magnum: mob f 8.. .1 t S o air pressure in mm of water 50 SUBJECT # 2 (MALE) Frequency in Hertz (Hz) M A mxomomwflm twnSws . O HM Bovwwwnw sonamw Hwawnu naunmsmum n. _ E u o x mm m n" a... L «L 3.4 1.. .3 / // / no r a R L as o ooappwssom Om shamans W 0 2 k 6 8 Wu. 0 a o 0 w o m z m m m o m a o a 125 ommmwmwm ”Rm 3.. .1 f b .1 .33 .825 no 5 :33 30535. magnum: m a air pressure in mm Of water 51 SUBJECT #3(FEMALE) Frequency in Hertz (Hz) fl“ mxomumwmo swarm: Bone socwwwnk sonanw Hwawnm/I z/I mhflwnsaun a o . L COMPLIANCE _4 C I nox m m mas. .. a 1. .. //// a R .u E o ooapwwmsom On osnmnsa m o 2 .Q 6 8 8 o 0 w o m . m o u m 0 a 125 o m m m, w m m m m m m .32 .325 no 5 «2.3 30:353. 333: i f b u m u m o air pressure in mm of water 552 SUBJECT #4(FEMALE) Frequency in Hertz (Hz) m L mxommmwcm that»: O h” aocwwwnw sonaeH Huawnm unwnnsouu B E m _// 21% m1 . 4_da) d _ uox mm .w a.._.....” .. a A 2 100 0 o o A ”M _ ._. 7 w _ c // r aRL E o ooafiwwmsoo Om canonsa W O 2 k 6 8 m 8 o m o m z m G m m m an o a 5 u ,..m m m. w m w m m m m Ammoa .Hm24o no :« Hm>oa Odonmmuna magnum: .1 a m stiff o air pressure in mm of water 553 SUBJECT #5(FEMALE) Frequency in Hertz (Hz) 0 $0 1000 200 0 '00 O 0 800 0 TYMPANOGRAM 2.50 125 O 0 000000 1 mmfihw6789mu Amwma .Hm24o mp :« Hw>wA caonmmuns mcwummm mobil 2 u 6 8 OOSOHMOSOO Om mononsa stiff m . mxommm+mg oaosmmuns mcwummm m mxomsmwoq oaocmmuns meadow: m L mxommmmOQ annmmuna magnum: 0 AJ II. 0 H ooapwwmsom Om mmnancs O 2 TYMPANOGRAM mObi n4 6 8 f 8.. .1 t 3 m... o air pressure in mm of water APPENDIX C CASE HISTORY AND RELEASE FORM 57 58 Name Birthdate Age Address Phone PLEASE FILL IN ALL APPROPRIATE QUESTIONS. Have you ever had facial surgery or surgery on your ears What kind When and where What medications do you take now DO you have an acute allery (occurs suddenly) Age Of onset of hearing loss Cause Of hearing loss How has your hearing changed DO you wear a hearing aid What kind DO you know sign language How Often do you sign DO you know fingerspelling How often do you fingerspell How Often do you write messages in face to face communi- cation What percent Of your communication is oral as Opposed to manual or written 59 TO what extent was your education oral manual or combined manual What was the last school you attended Where and when have you received speech theraphy I attest tO the truth Of the above information and my willingness tO participate in this experimentation. Signature Witness Date Date APPENDIX D SENTENCE STIMULI 60 61 Practice Sentence Items We sat on the floor. I want to wish you a happy birthday. The two boys worked at the job Off and on but not all the time. Test Sentence Items Dad went on a trip. She made a picture. The ice is too cold to hold in your hands. That big tree will shade us from the hot sun. You know how to stick tO things until you find out the answers. Now each person in the Office is talking about the dog. APPENDIX E INSTRUCTIONS 62 (l) (2) (3) (4) 63 Please fill out the case history form. I will answer any questions you have. Read these words. If you do not understand a word, please tell me. Next, we will go to Room B-5 for a hearing evaluation. Next, we will go to Room 209-C. You will be seated at the equipment table. a. Hold the rubber mask to your face, like this (example given). Make the mask tightly cover your mouth and nose. DO not let air leak out the sides of the mask. In front Of you is a stack Of cards. Each card has one sentence typed on it. You will read every sentence into the mask. When you are ready, I will turn on the machine, and you will begin reading the sentences. Let's practice. Read these three sentences. Read as you usually do. Are you ready to read the other sentences? b. Hold the rubber mask to your face. Make the mask tightly cover your mouth and nose. DO not let air leak out the sides of the mask. You will look at this picture and hold the mask to your face. 64 I want you to tell what you see in the picture. When you are ready, I will turn on the machine, and you will begin talking about the picture. Let's practice. Tell me what you see in this picture. Talk as you usually do. Are you ready to tell me about another picture? (l) (2) (3) (4) 65 Please fill out the case history form. I will answer any questions you have. Read these words. If you do not understand a word, please tell me. Next, we will gO to Room B-5 for a hearing evaluation. Next, we will gO tO Room 209-C. You will be seated at the equipment table. a. Hold the rubber mask to your face, like this (example given). Make the mask tightly cover your mouth and nose. DO not let air leak out the sides Of the mask. You will look at this picture and hold the mask to your face. I want you to tell what you see in the picture. When you are ready, I will turn on the machine, and you will begin talking about the picture. Let's practice. Tell me what you see in this picture. Talk as you usually do. Are you ready to tell me about another picture? Hold the rubber mask to your face. Make the mask tightly cover your mouth and nose. DO not let air leak out the sides of the mask. In front of you is a stack Of cards. Each card has one sentence typed on it. 66 You will read every sentence into the mask. When you are ready, I will turn on the machine, and you will begin reading the sentences. Let's practice. Read these three sentences. Read as you usually do. Are you ready tO read the other sentences?