INTER - GENERATIONAL-CLEAVAGES AND POLITICAL BEHAVIOR: ASURVEY STUDYOF THE “1971 ¥ , ‘ PRESIDENTIAL ELECTION IN URUGUAY Dissertation for the Degree of Ph; D.' MICHIGAN STATE UNIVERSITY ‘ ULISES F. GRACERAS - 1977 L 7 hi‘ '; - t r. Uniwrsity This is to certify that the thesis entitled Inter-Generational Cleavages and Political Behavior: A.Survey Study of the 1971 Presidential Election In Uruguay presented by Ulises F. Graceras has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for Ph. D. degreein Sociology it \3- WWW Major professor Date February 11, 1977 “‘19 C I CI 0 I INTER—GENERATIONAL CLEAVAGES AND POLITICAL BEHAVIOR: A SURVEY STUDY OF THE 1971 PRESIDENTIAL ELECTION IN URUGUAY By Ulises F. Graceras A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Sociology 1977 ABSTRACT INTER-GENERATIONAL CLEAVAGES AND POLITICAL BEHAVIOR: A SURVEY STUDY OF THE 1971 PRESIDENTIAL ELECTION IN URUGUAY By Ulises F. Graceras This research provides a detailed survey analysis of the 1971 Presidential Election in Uruguay, focusing on the different behaviors and attitudes shown in this opportunity by the older and the younger voters. In general, this study corroborates generational theory as it has been presented by Ryder, Riley, and, less recently, Mannheim. Although this research depicts age—based cleavages as the foundation of political behavior in Uruguay, its theoretical framework goes far beyond the scope of generational theory. The picture of a culture sharply divided into two opposed worldviews corresponding to the two poles of the generational spectrum is related to the process of political destabilization which the society under scrutiny underwent recently, after having enjoyed many years of stability and institutio- alized conflict resolution. Eckstein's theory of congruent authority patterns is discussed as one attempt to establish the basic requisites which democratic societies should fulfill if they are to remain stable. The broad Ulises F. Graceras context of social congruence theory is then applied to the case of modernizing societies whose stratification systems are mainly age—based. The research strategy followed in this study is two-fold: in the first place, hard institutional data are analyzed to show that Uruguay fits the above specifications. This analysis of current statistical data also indicates that the country has suffered substained economic deterioration after many years of prosperity and growth. Second, survey data from the Presidential election which predated the overthrow of democratic structures in Uruguay are analyzed in attempt to Show the phenomenon of generational polarization. A number of findings are drawn from the data employed in the study. .Among the more important are the following: 1.— Older voters tended to feel more poorly rewarded than did younger ones. Similarly, older respondents voiced more complaints about their standards of living having deteriorated than did their younger counterparts. 2.— Nevertheless, younger voters tended to evaluate the overall national situation more negatively than did older voters. 3.— The effect of education on political attitudes was ambivalent: higher educational attainments made persons more likely to complain about the situation and more inclined to support left parties when they were young, less likely to do so if they were old. 4.— Age groups at both extremes of the generational spectrum appeared sharply divided into two opposed fields on the issues debated during the campaigning, and in their approaches to political life in general. 5.- Group responses, in general, tended to be configurational. Ulises F. Graceras The above evidence is interpreted in this thesis as a symptom, rather than as the cause, of a highly conflictual situation which led to a collapse of pre-existing political institutions. ACKOWLEGMENTS A number of people and institutions made it possible for me to develop and complete the present dissertation. Data collection and data analysis for this study were funded through an area assistantship from the Comparative Program in Sociology at Michigan State University. I shall never forget the role played by Professor Frederick Waisanen, my committee chairman, who provided continuous advice and encouragement throughout the different stages of my work. Professor Waisanen displayed a high quality professional role model which I will always try to emulate. During the years we worked together, my major professor permanently showed superlative human qualities of flexibility and comprehension which fostered understanding and cooperation in spite of differences in cultural backgrounds. I am also grateful to the other members of my guidance committee: Professor Philip Marcus provided important critical evaluation of my ideas and contributed to developing my studies and to the execution of my dissertation problem. Professor Denton Morrison also served on my committee and provided me with useful criticisms on the manuscript. With special importance, I want to ackowledge the helpfulness and support of Professor Allan J. Beegle. His academic excellence and outstanding human qualities have earned him the kind of respect that ii I hope my students will direct toward me. Were it appropriate to dedicate this dissertation, the dedication should be to him. Foreign students pursuing an advanced degree face, in addition to the usual sifficulties implied in scholastic work, the problem of adjusting to a different environment, and of overcoming the practical problems of everyday life with limited resources. I specially wish to ackowledge help in this respect to MSU Foreign Student Advisor August G. Benson, and to Mr. Elliot Ballard. Dr Theodore Hedrick and my fellow student Dr. Armando Villarroel also helped me greatly on this matter. I also received help and encouragement from various Foreign Service officers serving in Uruguay at different times: William Gussman, James Tull, and Russell Olson. None of these persons, of course, is responsible for the ideas presented in this thesis, but they did help me complete my training program in the United States. This, as I see it, is a personally relevant and much appreciated application of the theory of international cooperation. iii IIRI'EIL TABLE OF CONTENTS Page LIST OF TABLES vi Chapter I. INTRODUCTION 1 II. THEORY 6 Age Groups as Analytical Categories 7 Age Groups as Interest Groups 9 The Segmental View of Society: A Model of Conflict Group Formation 10 Conflict Groups and Social Integration 14 Age GRoups as Major Conflict Groups 17 Life Cycles and Conflicting Self Identities 23 III. OPERATIONAL DEFINITIONS Democracy 32 Stability Age-Stratified Society 36 The Dependent Variable: Political Attitudes 38 Inequity Perception 38 Commitment-Alienation 39 Ruling Class Identification 40 Change vs. Status Maintaining Orientations 40 Party Preferences and Party Identification 41 Self—Investment in Political Roles: Political Activism Index 43 Clientele-Oriented Partisanship 44 Primary Group Identification/Conflict 44 The Independent Variable: Age 44 Education: The Intervening Variable 45 The Control Variables Occupation 47 Income 48 iv Chapter IV. THE RESEARCH SETTING A Stable Democracy Decisional Effectiveness and Low Violence Rates An Age-Stratified Society Economic Crisis After Economic Growth Increasing Conflict and Decreasing Stability V. HYPOTHESES Inequity Perception Commitment/Alienation Ruling-Strata Identification Change vs. Status-Maintaining Orientations Attitudinal Configuration Party Identification and Party Preferences Self-Investment in Political Roles: Political Activism Index Clientele-Oriented Partisanship Primary Group Conflict VI. METHODOLOGY The Data The Instrument The Sample Data Analysis VII. FINDINGS Value Polarization Inequity Perception Commitment-Alienation Ruling-Strata Identification Change Orientations Attitudinal Configuration Behavioral Polarization Self-Investment in Political Roles Clientele-Oriented Partisanship Primary Group Identification The Control Variables The Alternative Hypotheses VIII. SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS Summary of Findings Conclusions: The Theory of Generations Conclusions: Democratic Theory Page 49 51 53 56 63 64 7O 7O 71 72 73 74 74 75 75 76 77 77 78 81 82 83 83 83 98 105 118 122 127 136 137 142 146 150 159 159 162 164 LIST OF TABLES Table Page 1. Uruguay. Population 20 Years Old and Over by Age Groups Percentages 50 2. Political Participation and Left Vote in Uruguayan Elections (1906-71). Percentages 52 3. Integration—Stability Index. Yearly Average of Diverse Indicators 54 .1}. .-Employment Status of Montevideo Male Students by Father's Occupation. Percentages 59 5. Employment Status of Montevideo Students by Sex and Father's Occupation 6O 6. Uruguay. Gross National Product and Per Capita Gross National Product 65 7. Composition of Uruguay's Gross National Product (1957-68). Percentages 66 8. Uruguay. Buying Power of Salaried Workers 67 9-30*. Respondents Self Perceived Recognition Level by Socio-Demographic Categories. Percentages 86 9-31 . Respondents Assigning Themselves the Highest Recognition Scores by Age and Education. Percentages 87 9-32 . Respondents Assigning Themselves the Lowest Recognition Scores by Age and Education. Percentages 88 9-33 . Respondents Self Perceived Pay Level by Socio— Demographic Categories. Percentages 91 9-34 . Respondents Assigning Themselves the Lowest Pay Scores by Age and Education. Percentages 93 *Tables with an ”9” prefix refer to findings vi Table Page 9-35. Self Perceived Deterioration of Standard of Living by Respondents Socio-Demographic Characteristics. Percentages 94 9-36. Self Perceived Deterioration of Respondents Standard of Living by Age and Education. Percentages 96 9—37. Alienation Index. Derogatory Statements by Socio Demographic Categories. Percentages 100 9—38 Commitment/Alienation Index by Age Groups. Percentages 101 9-39. Commitment Index. Appreciative Statements by Age and Education. Percentages 102 9-40. Alienation Index. Derogatory Statements by Age and Education. Percentages 103 9-41. Commitment/Alienation Index. Percentage- Differences 104 9—42. Respondents Terming the Local Situation "Worse" and ”Much worse" by Socio Demographic Characteristics. Percentages 107 9-43. Respondents Perception of the Local Situation as Compared to 5 Years Before by Age—Groups. Percentages 110 9-44. Respondents Terming the Local Situation "Worse” and "Much worse” by Age and Education. Percentages 111 9-45. Respondents Terming the Local Situation ”Worse” and "Much worse" by Education and Age. Percentages 112 9-46. Ruling Strata Identification. Respondents Saying That the Local Situation May Improve Depending On Who is Elected President. Percentages 115 9-47. Respondents Saying that the Local Situation May Improve Depending on Who is Elected President. Percentages. 116 9-48. Respondents' Assessment of the Importance of Election Issues. Percentages 120 vii Table Page 9.49. Conservatism/Radicalism Scores by Age Groups Percentages. 124 9.50. Respondents Receiving Top Conservatism Scores by Socio-Demographic Characteristics. Percentages 125 9.51. Respondents Party Identification by Age Groups. Percentages 128 9.52. Left Party Identification by Age and Education. Percentages 130 9-53. Conservative Party Identification by Age and Education. Percentages. 131 9.54. Voting Intention by Age Groups. Percentages 133 9.55. Left Vote by Age and Education. Percentages 134 9.56. Conservative Vote by Age and Education. Percentages 135 9.57. Political Activism Scores by Age Groups.Percentages 136 9.58. Political Activism Scores by Educational Levels. Percentages 138 9.59. Respondents Having Received a Favor from Someone in a Political Club by Socio-Demographic Characteristics. Percentages 140 9.60. Respondents Who Received Favors from Someone in a Political Club by Age and Education. Percentages 141 9.61. Primary Group Identification by Age Groups. Percentages 144 9.62. Primary Group Identification: Respondents Extent of Agreement with Relatives Living in the Same Household by Age and Education. Percentages 145 9.63. Conservative Vote by Age and Income Levels. Percentages 148 9.64. Respondents Party Identification by Occupational Categories. Percentages. 149 9.65. Respondents Party Identification by Social Class. Percentages 152 9.66. Voting Intention by Social Class. Percentages 153 viii Table Page 9.67. Change (Protest) Dimensions by Age Groups. Percentages and Percentage Differences 135 9.68. Protest Dimensions by Occupational Categories. Percentage Differences 156 9.69. Protest Dimensions by Occupational Categories. Percentages 157 ix CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION The question of how societies manage to remain stable, regardless of external pressures and/or internal tensions has justifiably constituted a central concern for sociologists at least since the times of Durkheim.1 At high conceptual level, this matter involves an option between the major paradigms that dominate social theory: the systemic and the segmental views of societies. When the first approach is taken, as it is the case in Parsons (1951), Levy (1952), and Apter (1958), social change is regarded as a consequence of the system's having failed to fulfill one or various among its functional requisites. On the other hand, segmental and/or conflict theorists interpret the processes of social change as merely the manifestation of a more general historical evolution which is purportedly taking place.2 In both cases, the theorist situates himself at a very high level of abstraction. When more specific problems and situations are considered, grand theory does not furnish the guidelines appropriate for the conceptualization and the operationalization of the variables. l.- Durkheim (1960); see also Wilensky (1961) 2.- Classic segmental views of societies are presented in Pareto (1961 and 1966), and Marx and Engels (1959). For a comprehensive discus— sion of the epistemological implications involved in the latter, see Leff (1961 and 1969) The present study concerns itself with the problem of stability and change in democratic societies. The theory here presented starts by examining the question of stability in general, and then tackles the problem within the more specific context of democratic polities. The last three decades have witnessed the growth of a branch of sociology primarely concerned with voting behavior and public opinion formation in electoral campaigns (Lazarsfeld 25 31,, 1944; Campbell 3: '22:, 1954; Berelson 33.2}:’ 1954). This branch of knowlwedge is largely based on empirical research utilizing survey studies. On the other hand, theorists of democratic societies such as Lipset (1960), Sartori (1962), as well as Kornhauser (1954) in a more restricted manner, have dealt with social conditions that are instrumental in, or detrimental to, the emergence and survival of such form of government. The latter has been the subject of historical speculation and secondary analysis of survey and institutional data. In order to reconcile these two approaches, it is useful to consider the theory of social congruence, prOposed by Eckstein (1961 and 1966). Eckstein rejects the approach of early political sociologists who concentrated in the study of the State and of governmental institutions and processes, and concerns himself with power dependency relations, at all levels of society.3 3.- Medels of electoral behavior where computer simulation tech— niques were used in an attempt to fill the gap in theory construction have been presented by McPhee (1963), and deSola Pool et a1. (1964). This approach runs into the additional difficulty that—Th5_background data suitable for simulation schemes, and specially the opinion and attitude components of it, explicate but one part of the electoral behavior. A democratic society is stable, Eckstein claims, when the patterns of authority that exist at different societal levels are congruent with each other. Authority congruence theory is still a very general formulation, unless a view is presented in terms of what dimensions should be scrutin- ized in trying to identify the actual sources of power and authority at each societal level. It would also be necessary that a definition be put forth in order to decide when a society is to be considere stable or ‘unstable. For this purpose, a segmental model, inspired in the theories byIBentham (1908), Duverger (1968 and 1973), and Dahrendorf (1963) is used in this study. The result is a multidimensional model of conflict— _ group operation wherein the lines of cleavage along which confrontations and allegiances occur are dependent upon the circumstances present in each.situation. The situation that serves as the basis for this study is that of Uruguay, a developing society whose parliamentary institutions and political parties collapsed recently after many years of stability. In this particular case, I contend that the stratification system of Uru— guay is mainly age-based, which makes age-groups the major conflict- groups involved. Under those circumstances, this study can profit from the considerable amount of sociological literature dealing with the interplay between age, generations, or life-cycles, and political attitudes (Ryder, 1968; Cutler, 1974; Riley, 1968 and 1972). Ultimately, the present study can contribute to the evaluation of current theories on the political effects of aging. Two other questions are dealt with laterally in this work: one of them belongs in the area of sociology of knowledge, the other in the field of social research methodology. The question of how social positions, or statuses, are related to individual beliefs and group values, which goes back to Marx's materialism (Marx, 1964: 15—22) and to Mannheim's relativism (Mannheim, 1952: 134-90), appears here inevitably in connection with the values held by different age—groups. Although no specific discussion of the underlying philosophical question is undertaken here, most of the evidence presented points to the fact that political groupings pgr_§g_ have to be accounted for as an independent or at least as as intervening factor in many of the processes studied. At the methodological level, this study grew out of the tradition that combines historical analysis, survey research, and sociological interpretation (Lipset, 1960; Alford, 1963). After attempting to demonstrate that the society under scrutiny meets the requirements of the proposed model, I concentrate on the interpretation of survey research findings in the light of the preceding theory. The surveys used were taken by the author during the Presidential campaign of 1971 in Uru- guay, which is here regarded as a critical turning point in the process of destabilization of the local political structures. Some of the conclusions which are drawn in the following chapters go well beyond the scope of generational theory. Indeed, the more general problem of how democratic systems of government survive or perish as a consequence of internal strain is recurrently at stake in this work. It is my belief that secondary elaboration on this matter is warranted, given the high extent of logical configuration of the theory. CHAPTER II THEORY Throughout the history of the discipline, students of political sociology have endeavored to establish relationships between group membership and value orientations. In his classic elaboration on the social factors that underlie political phenomena, Lipset (1960: 229-58) related ideological traits to social class affiliation, and coined the graphic expression ”democratic class struggle" with reference to the social cleavages that purportedly express themselves through party preferences. Within a more limited context, Berelson £3 31. (1954) conducted empirical studies of the processes of opinion formation which predate a voting decision, and concluded that primary group orientation and reference group membership are good predictors of individual political preferences. The sociological study of politics, therefore, embraces two basic tasks of conceptualization: first, to depict lines of cleavage in a given society that may lead into interest group formation and, second, to explore hypothesized connections between (1) group membership and (2) attitudes and behavior. The purpose of this study id to analyze the interrelations between social cleavages and political attitudes during a period of serious inter- generational conflict in a partially modernized 4 society where age constitutes the.major stratification dimension. The analysis below will center upon the beliefs and behaviors observable during a Presidential campaign which predated the collapse of the existing parliamentary structures. At stake is the question of how inter-generational relation— ships are affected by serious changes in the interest structure, and what the ultimate consequences of this situation are in terms of the stability of the political structures. Age Groups as Analytical Categories The importance of age as an analytical category for the study of social conflict and political beliefs came to the attention of social scientists in the politically heated atmosphere of Pre-Nazi Germany, where conflicting generational views seem to have greatly contributed to the collapse of the Weimar Republic (Mannheim, 1952: 218; Bracher,l957: 85—6; Neumann, 1942: 239). A revival of such an interest has occurred today, paralleling the appearance of a generational gap at all levels of society, and the acceleration of the pace of social change. Correlations between aging (as a socio-psychological process) and behaviors have been reported, among others, by Kuhlen (1951) who found conservative traits to increase with age; Pollak (1943) showed that levels of personal disengagement correlate with individual age, and Cumming and 4.- Modernization is referred to in this study as a social change process broader than mere socio-economic development, and with reference to structural and value transformations. Henry (1961) showed that the perception of future is a function of age. In all these empirical studies, age was conceptualized as the independent variable, yet the research findings were never integrated into a comprehensive theory of aging. An alternative approach, generally referred to as the theory of generations is represented by Mannheim and his followers. 5 Theorists in this tradition envisaged generations as human aggregates bound together by a shared perception of certain critical historical juncture (Mannheim, 1972: 120; Haberle, 1951: 122). The generational approach has been contrasted with its alternative, the aging hypothesis, in a considerable amount of literature. Campbell et_al. (1960: 120-40) applied both perspectives to the study of party affiliations in the United States, and concluded that generational theory had the greater explanatory power. A similar conclusion is suggested by Lipset (1960:281) after overviewing several decades of political behavior research. Cutler (1974: 453—62) reviewed the current literature on the subject more recently and reached the same results. In addition, the generational factor appears as the major analytical concept in studies of how foreign policy issues are perceived in the United States by Farris (1960: 61) and Almond (1960: 117—20). A recent survey of student attitudes toward European integration led Ingelhart (1967) to the same conclusion. 5.— See Mannheim (1952), and Haberle (1951). A comparative present— tation of the generational views of Mannheim and Haberle can be found in Hornback (1974: 30—9). Age Groups as Interest Groups Regardless of the high extent of configuration it exhibits, and of the considerable amount of research it has generated, the generational approach is still plagued with two major shortcomings: first, it is no simple task to determine precisely what is to be considered a generation in terms of life-span; second, a critical problem is involved in relating age-groups to the interest structure. Mannheim himself envisaged this latter difficulty: It is a matter for historical and sociological research to discover at what level in its development, and under what conditions, a class becomes class-conscious and similarly, when individual members of a generation become conscious of —heir common situation and make this consciousness the basis of their group solidarity. 6 The first problem has been dealt with recently in a rather practical fashion by reducing the ambiguous term "generation" --basically coined in the tradition of literature and history -— to more parsimonious concepts. It has been proposed, for instance, that the term "cohort" be substituted for generation. Unlike the latter, which calls for a general definition, a cohort can be thought of as any age group with shared life experiences. This makes it possible for the concept to be operationalized relative 6.- Mannheim: Sociology of Knowledge, quoted in Wolff (1971: 272) 10 to the research interests born in mind by the researcher in each dif- ferent study (Ryd-r, 1965). The second problem is much more complicated. One possible way of dealing with it is indicated by Morrison'e£_§l, (1972) who distinguished intrawgenerational differences in the perception of environmental issues in connection with the extent to which the issues affected the occupational opportunities available to to each group within the generation. For this approach to be extended to a complex political situation, it becomes necessary to elaborate upon the relationship between interest-based cleavages and group orientations and to examine how these orientations may be conducive to either social continuity or social change. The concepts of conflict-group, and self-investment will furnish the basic elements for the fellowing elaboration. The Segmental View of Society: “A'Mode1 of Conflict4Groungormation Group theorists have traditionally conceived of society as being composed of a number of conflicting groups. Following Bentham (1908), theorists in this tradition have defined groups in terms of masses of activity resulting from common interests, and sharing a common tendency with regard to decisions.7 It follows from this formulation that the 7.- The problem is extensively treated in Eckstein (1963) 11 major interpretive concept involved in "group theory" is that of interests, for it is interest—conflict which, according to this view, forms the basis of the struggle of groups which constitutes politics, a struggle from which decisions are made concerning the allocation of scarce social goods. Although this approach ccomplished the task of relating group goal orientations to the interest structure, the implied definition of interest9 is still far from being precise. For example, group theory does not provide the necessary guidelines as to ascertain when an interest group would emerge from a given social situation, nor does it cast light on the question of how cohesive the resulting group would be, nor how intensively the group would engage in social conflict. To overcome this limitation of group theory, it is helpful to think in terms of self-investment theory, as suggested by Faunce (1972). The concept self inveStment refers to processes whereby an actor develops and further ranks different self identities in connection with different role performances. 8.- The term "interest" first appeared in the writings by Kant, who distinguished between actions oriented toward the pursuit of material goals ("interested" actions) and "disinterested" actions. Following Bentham, most group theorists have thought of interests in terms of shared attitudes. For a more detailed discussion of the topic, see Sartori—(1965: 199) 9.- Cf. Truman (1953), Hagan (1958), Key (1954). The number of possible self identities open to an individual in any given social situation is contingent upon the extent of role dif— ferentiation within the inmediate social context. By the same token, the number of theoretically possible self—identities increases with modernization. Differences in the extent to which self-investment occurs in different roles is another consequence of the social structure, insofar as the actor will most likely develop self—identities in connection with the roles susceptible to produce the highest rewards within the prevalent value system. Lack of sufficient reward resulting from the performance of roles with low self-investment may be harmless for an individual's self image. However, if actors do not perceive themselves as being properly rewarded in connection with the performance of high self—investment roles, a situation of dissonance develops which may be resolved by one of the following mechanisms: self —investment may be shifted to a different role, or the equity of the distributive structure may be questionned. 10 When a dissonance situation such as the one described above results from circumstances affecting a considerable number of people, as it usually happens in a period of rapid socio—economic change, or when there is a sudden modification of the economic condition, 10.— In essence, this is an application of the basic compensatory mechanisms (social influence, misperception, and social locomotion) depicted by balance theorists. See particularly T.N. Newcomb (1961). it can be hypothesized that groups of individuals with shared tendencies toward questionning the legitimacy of the authority structure will emerge, and that these groups will develop varying degrees of self awareness. Group goal—orientations, thus, originate from shared perceptions of the insufficiency or the inapropriatedness of the resources allocated to different statuses. The above concept closes the bridge between self-investment theory and conflict theory as the latter has been presented by Dahren— dorf (1959: 216). According to Dahrendorf, conflict groups come to exist in connection with the different positions relative to the authority structure. In other words, in any imperatively coordinated association, the existing political order allocates different shares of social goods with different status groups. It is this process, together with the judgement by the individuals involved on the fairness of such distribution, which determines that individuals align one way or another, along the most relevant lines of cleavage. Anothe consequence of this theoretical perspective is that any social situation can be envisioned as consisting of, or as leading into, the opposition of two major conflict groups: In any imperatively coordinated association, two quasi-groups united by common latent interests can be distinguished. Their orientations of interests are determined by possession or exclusion from authority. From these quasi—groups, interest groups are recruited, the articulate program of which defends or attacks the legitimacy of existing authority structures. (Dahren— dorf, 1959: 183) l4 Conflict4GrOups'and Social Integration The above model of group formation and of conflict formulation carries along important consequences in terms of integration and change in societies. For the specific purpose of this study, exclusive reference is made to democratic society. In the case of democratic polities, Dahrendorf's argument has been shrewdly followed by Duverger: Struggle and integration are not only contradictory aspects of politics: they are also complementary aspects of it. In studying the antagonic factors, it was noted that many of them are ambivalent to a certain extent: They generate conflict but, under certain circumstances, they can also contribute to limit conflict and to develOp integration. (1968: 246) Duverger concludes that conflictive and integrative factors are but two aspects of the same social processes; "the two faces of Janus”, metaphorically, in reference to the pagan god whose two faces appeared on both sides of the Roman coins. The resulting view of social integration is one of an implicit and lasting compromise of forces between major conflict groups. In this sense, the Duverger-Dahrendorf model of social integration ressembles Mosca's definition of a "political formula": the only notable dif- ference between the two conceptions rests with the symbolic, ll.- In a more recent work (Duverger, 1972) the author used the same metaphor with regard to the two contradictory aspects of develop- ment in modern industrial societies: technological progress, and deterioration of the quality of life. 15 or sociospsychological emphasis in Mosca's: "the legal and moral basis, or principle, on which the power of the political class rests. " CMosca, 1939: 186) Before further discussing the above model of social integration, it is worth_underscoring some fundamental differences between this view and the classic conflict and consensus conceptions of society. As in classic conflict theory, it is here contended that it is essential for the understanding of any social processes that the existing social cleavages, or the significant dimensions of the stratification system, be identified: the difference between these two approaches rests upon the fact that, while conflict theorists, for instance, envision the same classebased cleavages as being universal, the view here presented argues that social cleavages are contingent ‘upon the particular integrative mechanisms that have developed in a given social system. The rejection of a universal, unidimensional, explanation (which is implicit in the model here presented) is clear in the following quotation from Gordon Leff: All such universalistic explanations tend to reduce all phenomena to the same medium whther it is nationality, climate, or the unconscious. In each case, the claims for such under— standing can only be substained at the expense of all other counter—claims. There is the added difficulty in the case of social wholes, although there can be empirical tests for them, they remain theoretical constructs which as wholes never go beyond the level of an abstraction: so soon as we attempt to designate a particular group or occurrence as belonging to a certain class, we are compelled to recourse to the realist position of invoking an essence or nature. We are thus reduced to circularity: that the group in question is a manifestation of class because of social action being class action. (1969:171) 16 The use of s systemic view does not help to overcome the problem of circularity in our explanation either. As Eckstein pointed out: The use of systemic requisites is also unlikely to lead us to the desired level of explanation. To some extent these categories will merely help establish that the political systems mentioned do indeed fit our definition of unstable democracy: for example that they failed to maintain the democratic "pattern" by the surrender of power to non—democratic adjuncts of the system. To some extent, they will only lead us to the more familiar truistic theories. (1961: 4) In contrast to systems or consensus views, the model here presented does not see integration as resulting from a value system being shared at all levels of society, but more simply from a situation of compromise among influential groups. Whatever the nature and the internal structure of this compromise situation (e.g., whether it is based on a wide value consensus, or on coercion; whether it refers to the interaction between an upper stratum of rulers and a lower class, or to the interplay among a multiplicity of social segments) it will suffice for the purposes of this study that such a compromiseresults in the polity being able to manage social tensions and to process conflict within prescribed institutional patterns. In other words, stability sim ply refers here to the fact that,in a society, grievances are resolved through legitimate channels so ruling out use of unstructured forms of action. 17 Age GrOups as Major CenfliCtéGroups Let us now examine the case when generations, or age differences, become the lines of cleavage in a society. Such a situation implies that age had become the basic dimension in the stratification system, and that, consequently, age cohorts, or generations, are made the major conflict groups. At stake here are the consequences that derive from such a situation, in terms of social stability or social integration. Granted that the normal operation of competing groups inevitably puts some strain on any society, it is reasonable to assume that such a strain would acquire unique proportions when the case is that of conflicting generations. Research on political socialization conducted by Hyman (1969) and others 12 depicts this process as family—centered. Since this process largely conditions the way in which values are passed from one generation to another, as well as the learning of political attitudes and norms of behavior by the younger age groups, one might assume that inter—generational differences can to a certain extent explain social change. (Eisenstadt, 1956: 292—95). One might also assume that generational differences, socialization processes, and social change, are related in delicate balance. The injection of unusual doses of conflict into such a balance will most likely result in serious consequences for the preservation of the system. 12.- Cf. Easton and Hess (1962), Hess and Torney (1967),Langton (1969), Dawson and Prewitt (1969), Easton and Dennis (1968). 18 The above question is two-fold: first, it involves examining the attitudinal and behavioral differences among generations in general; second, those differences should be accounted for in the specific case of an age—stratified society. In general there is unanimity among the different author who have dealt with the subject; older people tend to be more conservative, and less supportive of change, than do the younger groups (Riley, 1968; Ryder, 1965; Carlson and Karlsson, 1972). The explanation for these empirical differences is not that unanimous, however. Three major groups of theories can be distinguished: (l) theories that stress the influence of different environments (school vs. ”real world”) (Riley, 1968: 476; Sarnoff, 1962: 392); (2) those that emphasize metabolic and psycho—physiological aspects (Carlson and Karlsson,l972; Ryder, 1975); and, (3) those that stress the influence of the different social locations that different generations have. (Riley'etpals,1972:l39; Ryder, 1965: 856). The third theoretical orientation, which emphasizes specifically sociological factors, points out consistently that an increasing extent of commitment toward the existing social structures normally results from the process of aging. In the first place, as Waisanen (1969) has contended, older actors have greater investments in the social system; second, as noted by Ryder (1965: 856), as individuals grow older, they tend to avoid and/or reject dissonant items; third, with the passing of years, individuals come to adjust to the existing structure so that any pr0posed change will imply risking unknown consequences for them (Riley 33 31., 1972: 139). 19 It can be concluded from the above considerations that age differentials in political behavior and value orientations do exist, and that those differences can be readily understood on the grounds of current sociological theory. We can now turn into discussion of the second part of the problem, that is, what kind of political behavioral differences we might expect accross generations in (1) a modernizing society where (2) age palys a major role as a stratification dimension. A society with a democratic system of government has supposedly reached a certain extent of modernization. Indeed, as Eisenstadt (1956, 59) points out, modernization results in increasing role differentiation as well as in the adoption of universalistic value- orientations, which, in turn, somewhat conflict with the particularistic value system that would still prevail within primary groups in that society. Age stratification, then responds, Eisenstadt contends, to the need of managing, or of graduating, the tensions emerging from the above situation. On the other hand, one of the postulates of social congruence theory is that democratic stability results from the congruence between family and governmental authority structures (Eckstein, 1966: 227—43). As a case of extreme congruence, Eckstein envisions an ideal society whose patterns of domination would be identical at all levels. The closest empirical referent to the ideal type above, is represented by the authority patterns of the British government and the British political parties. In Eckstein's words: ”both 20 patterns consist of a curious and very similar mixture of democratic, authoritarian, and, so to speak, constitutional elements.”(Eckstein, 1961: 6). Eckstein ackowledges that identity or isomorphism of all authority patterns can rarely, if ever, be found in real life situations: Some social relations simply cannot be conducted in a democratic manner, or can be so conducted only with the gravest disfunctional consequences. Take for example, those social units which link different generations ——families and schools. An infant cannot be cared for democratically. Families and schools can be permissive, but this is merely to say that they can be authoritarian in a lax and lenient manner (1961: 8) Having granted this difficulty, Eckstein then moves another step forward, which adds substancial clarification to the concept of social congruence: By congruence I do not mean any resemblance at all among authority patterns. Where authority relations are not all highly similar, the term refers rather to a particular patterns of resemblance among them, one which makes stringent requirements, but not requirements impossible to fulfill -—a pattern of graduated resemblances so to speak. (1961:10) In other words, social congruence theorists conceive of society as composed of a series of authority strata, on t0p of which are the formal structures of government, and at bottom are the primary relation social control mechanisms. Between these two extremes, the theorist situates all other social structures and institutions: the higher the level, the closer its authority pattern would resemble that of the government. This is to say that, although it is granted that differences may exist among the authority patterns of different social levels, 21 these differences must necessarily be a question of degree, rather than one Of substance. Accepting that congruence can never be perfect, due to the necessarily more authoritarian nature of family and primary group structures, social congruence becomes a function of the intermediate structures. In the light of this theory, the collapse of the Weimar Republic, for instance, is explained as a result of the lack of intermediate structures that would have filled the gap between a highly democratic form of government, and a highly autocratic culture. (Eckstein, 1961: 59) This interpretation parallels Eisenstadt view of Pre-Nazi Germany as a society sharply divided into two antagonic value systems, cor- responding to two opposing generations. (Eisenstandt, 1956: 317) The fact that the stratification system of a particular culture is largely based on age at the expense of other dimensions simply shows the predominance in that society of ascription over achievement as a mechanism of status acquisition. Such a structure of inequality can be therefore found in societies whose modernization is at the best partial, or incomplete. In these cases, mainly due to the effects of cultural diffusion, individuals may develop some of the self identities characteristic of the industrial societies. Very much in the same way as it happens in developed societies, the process of learning occupational roles is here channelled through formal education. Culturally defined patterns of consumption develOp through the socialization process along with occupational training. 22 Unlike in industrial societies, however, the probability that an individual will be able to meet his vocational goals, achieve his desired status, find occupation, and finally be able to fulfill his expectations in terms of consumption, depends primarely on personal contacts and family background. In this way, status ascription through family position is "legitimized" in terms of "modernism" by simultaneous, or concurrent educational achievement. While the individual in this manner strives for upward mobility through formal education, he ultimately depends on family contacts for placement purposes. Family ties, thus become indirectly a part of the social control mechanism inasmuch as they administer sanctions by regulating effective access of individuals to occupations. The above discussion illustrates the joint functioning of conflicting and integrative mechanisms: on the one hand, the actor's acquiring an education with the consequent develOpment of a job- oriented self identity, represents the change element whereby individuals strive to move upward in the social hierarchy; on the other hand, the network of personal ties and family oriented self identities on which basis the individual will ultimatelly be assigned a status, represents the integrative element. In this way, the younger generations are dependent upon the older. That such a situation may endure without the subjacent conflict becoming manifest, depends not solely on the legitimizing effect of the prevailing value system, but also upon the society's substained economic growth so as to allocate a satisfactory amount of goods and resources with each status group. 23 ‘Life—Cycles and'Conflicting 'Self -Identities When economic stagnation follows a period of substained growth and prosperity, we have what has been characterized by Davies (1971:46) as a typical pre-revolutionary situation. Under these circumstances, it is theorized that the greater the gap between raising expectations and the resources available, the higher the amount of conflict. When the above situation arises in a society with an age-based stratification system, inter—generational conflict is expected to become manifest. Ultimately, this study is concerned with the manifestations of such conflict, as well as with the long run consequences of the situation involved in terms of the preservation of the existing political structures. The model here presented presupposes a balance between role expectations connected with educational achievement, and socially ascribed statuses. Indeed, it is implicit in the model that certain self identities will predominate at given stages of life, under normal circumstances. Maximal differences in this respect are expected for the two extremes of the generational spectrum: the age group comprising those who have already retired from work, and the one in which those who have not yet entered the labor market are included. The younger group will show strong self investment in roles defined by their educational background and the corresponding expectations in terms of occupational statuses, as well as in consumer 24 roles which normally symbolize those statuses in that culture. Self investment in family roles among the younger generation will occur to the extent whereby primary group connections are instrumental to the individual's becoming self—sufficient and working out his own position in society. At any rate, as long as individuals assess their situations as being conducive to attaining a social position conmensurate with their investment in education -—and indirectly, with their perceived importance in terms of family identification -- they are unlikely to challenge the authority structure neither at the primary group nor at the society level. On the other hand, if the rewards received are not satisfactory, individuals are likely to perceive the authority structure as being inequitable. By with drawing self from professionally oriented roles, and from family roles (neither of which are any longer considered as rewarding) individuals may then turn into identification with others in their same situation, and develop varying degrees of generational, or group, awareness. The values of the newly formed group may include strong self investment in political roles aimed toward changing the existing order which would materialize in increased political participation and the advocacy for change orientations. When this point has been reached, a revolting generation has originated, and the society is in the treshold of change. What has occurred, basically, is that the previously existing balance of self identities has been broken, and the moderating influence of the integrative element has ceased to operate. The status 25 ascription mechanism is now questioned by the younger groups; education is no longer the means of legitimizing the ascribed statuses: it becomes instead the unrestrained impulse toward change-proneness. Among youth, those who have attained the highest educational level reach also the highest feeling of deprivation and the least extent of commitment toward the existing order. At the other extreme of the generational spectrum, those who have already completed their occupational experiences will stick close to those self identities linked to maintaining the distributive structure as it is. This is not to say that members of the older 'generation will not perceive the economic situation as being highly detrimental for themselves, nor that they would always consider that they are being highly recognized. On the contrary, people of older age will complain about the worsening of the situation, and look at them- selves as one of the least rewarded groups in terms of both economic rewards and social recognition. The intergenerational difference in this respect rests upon the fact that, while the youth will blame the ruling stratum for what is happening, the older groups will tend to consider that the existing state of affairs is but the inevitable consequence of the circumstances, something that no one can be reasonably made responsible for. This belief will materialize into the older generation tending to support the view that the situation cannot be improved, but will necessarily get worse, and that it will be so, irrespective of who wins the elections. This point of view is understandable, since in an age-stratified society, the older the actor, the higher the extent of his identification 26 with the ruling stratum, and the higher his commitment to the established order. People in this situation have reached the point where they can no longer think of change, or contend that the distributive structure is unfair, without seriously hurting their own self-esteem. Indeed, as educational attainment for the older generation plays a role opposite to the one that it has for the younger generation, we have that the more educated one person is, if he is of older age, the higher his commitment to the system, and the less change—prone he is expected to be. The above consideration accounts for the grounds of the hypothesized inter-generational differences with respect to various issues: commitment to the prevailing order and value system; support to change orientations; political preferences and political identification; types of political participation, and perception of the problems debated during the election campaign. It is also predictable that primary group homogeneity would be high in a society where personal and family connections provide the fundamental means of upward mobility. Contrariwise, when a family- centered authority structure is eroded by the individual's perception that he is being unfairly treated, then intra-family conflict increases. In a synchronic study, such as the present one, dissent with the political views predominating in family circles will be maximal for the younger generation. This assumption forms the basis for another set of hypotheses in this study. One last paragraph should now be dedicated to the specific situation of the intermediate generations, ranging from the age of 27 entrance in the job market to that of retirement. The period of life encompassed between those two limits is here regarded as a cycle in which first half, individuals strive to establish themselves within the occupational structure, while implementing their procreational families; during the second half of the cycle, however, an average individual would have already attained everything that was within his reach, and would subsequently try to maintain his social position rather than continue to struggle for upgrading himself. This view is advanced by Faunce (1972: 16) who sees self investment in the occupational roles as increasing to a midpoint, at which further investment in these roles no longer produces significant returns. At this stage, a shift to other self identities normally occurs. When the process involves passing from predominantly job-related identities to predominantly family-related identities in cultures such as the one referred to here, the basic individual attitude concomitantly moves from a change-orientation to a status-maintaining;orientation. At stake here is, rather than the quantitative effect of aging, the qualitative effect of the individual's position in terms of life—cycles, as these are determined by the individual's passing from one family status to another. This criterion has been applied by Lansing and Kish (1952) to the study of family life-cycles. Inasmuch as aging, a biological process dependent on time, can be thought of as a continuous variable, the sociological concept of age represents a discrete one, consisting of a series of statuses which individuals occupy succesively along with theirs fulfilling certain structural requirements. f ‘ I 28 At each level in this process, individual self-investment goes to different roles (occupational, family, social, etc.) in varying proportions; also at each stage, the wide variety of self identities resulting from the performance of a number of roles becomes integrated, or organized, around a key, or anchor, role, corresponding to that status which the individual looks upon as his main, or key, social characteristic. Along with their progressing through all these organizational stages, individuals are being continuously socialized; i.e., they reelaborate their systems of symbols on the grounds of current cultural patterns. It is not by accident, therefore, that high investment of the self goes to those roles which, in terms of the group's preferences and values, are perceived as potentially rewarding. It follows from the above that, with each more toward occupancy of higher maturity statuses, there is also an increase in the actor's extent of social integration. The more culture integrates the actor in the group, the more he becomes committed to the particular type of social organization within which he is operating. As an indirect consequence of self investment actions, the performance of some roles becomes increasingly important in terms of the individual's self image; this increases the vulnerability of the actor to social sanctions, i.e. to external control by the group. As was noted in discussion of Eckstein's view of stability, this concept implies a graduation of authority patterns, ranging from the informal social control mechanism implicit in group pressure at the primary relation level, to more formal power structures at higher 29 levels of organization. The assumption of succesive statuses throughout the life sycle presupposes a step—by-step movement through a progression of organizational patterns, each of which corresponds to a specific value system and cultural context, which provides the meaning for the interaction and regulates mutual expectations of the actors. Inter-generational balance is a specific situation within the above theoretical framework. In early stages of life, one's predominant identity is defined in terms of one's orientational family's social position. Actors later redefine their self image while developing educational goals and occupational expectations. By so doing, adult individuals invest self in roles connected with job performance, peer group interaction, various kinds of social activities, and their own procreational families. In a modern society, the passing from youth to adult roles implies a progression from less formal to more formal relations, as well as a move from particularistic to universalistic criteria. In fully traditional societies, this graduation does not exist: particularism and ascription permeate all levels of social life, role differentiation is minimal, and life is perceived as an integrated whole. Societies wherein modernization affects just certain aspects of social life while others remain traditional pose an interesting problem. In such social milieus, passin from one structural step to a higher level one does not automatically guarantees the passing to a more complex, or more formal, organizational level. In practice, for instance, individuals in such societies may qualify for occupational statuses since they have completed all 30 educational requirements, yet they may not be eligible for those positions because the local value system poses greater emphasis on social position and family contacts than it does on formal training. Under those circumstances, individuals who see their expectations frustrated are not likely to develop the extent of commitment which we expect to find in the previously discussed situations. If culture is defined as a system of common meanings on which bases individuals are integrated into their groups, the case presented above can be interpreted as the Opposition of values implicit in the diffusion of technical and vocational education on the one hand, and a traditional culture on the other. under those circumstances, one would expect that individuals whose expectations were raised by formal education, and who see their chances of social mobility frustrated would become alienated. Seeman (1959) distinguished five conceptual varieties of the term alienation in current sociological literature: powerlessness, meaninglessness, normlessness, isolation, and self-estrangement. In this study, the relevant dimension is normlessness. As Seeman (1959: 787) pointed out, the idea of normlessness has been imprecisely over-extended to include a wide variety of both social conditions and psychic states, including personal disorganization, cultural breakdown, and reciprocal distrust. Only the latter, i.e. reciprocal distrust, is emphasized in the present study. Reciprocal distrust underlies situations which Merton characterizes by the loss of commonly held standards and the consequent development 31 of manipulative attitudes (Merton, 1949: 223). From the individual point of view, then, this type of alienation may be defined as a high expectancy that socially unapproved behaviors are required to achieve given goals (Seeman, 1959: 788). Since it has been indicated that education provides the dynamic element of change—orientations, while age provides that of status— maintaining orientations, it follows from this that change orientations will vary directly with education, inversely with age. Finally, if the Mannheimian perspective on generations is correct, one would expect that, for each generation, it would be possible to find a highly structured worldview. Indeed, if there is some truth in the claims of the authors who coined the concept ”nuclear personality" 13 we tend to increase our selectivity we the messages we accept, in order to avoid possible dissonance situations. In the latter sense, we would expect the extreme generations to show a highly configurational value pattern, and to tend toward the rejection of messages originating in sources other than their own political parties. Having presented the conceptual foundations for this study in the present chapter, it is now necessary that operational definitions be put forth, and that research setting and methodology be specified. These matters constitute the subjects of the next two chapters. l3.- Cf Klein (1951); Brown (1954); Festinger (1952) CHAPTER III OPERATIONAL DEFINITIONS In the preceding chapter, I presented a theory of democratic stability with special reference to the question of age and political orientations in age—stratified, modernizing, societies. Before further considering the problem, it becomes necessary that criteria be provided as to determine what is to be considered a democracy, when it is proper to speak of stability, when a society can be thought of as being age— stratified, and finally, how do we conceive and measure our independent, dependent, and intervening variables. a) Democracy Lipset has defined democracy in complex societies as: A political system which supplies regular constitutional opportunities for changing the government officials, and a social mechanism which permits the largest possible part of the population to influence major decisions by choosing among contenders for political office. (1960: 27) Lipset's definition, which is largely inspired in previous elaboration by authors such as Schumpeter and Max Weber, is far too vague as to be operationalized, unless it is interpreted as saying that 32 , 33 democracies are political systems of government wherein officials are periodically changed by means of constitutionally regulated elections in which all those who have reached the age to vote are permitted to participate. 14 The considerable ubiquity of the term democracy has been stressed by Sartori (1965: 3—16) who underlined the difference between political, social, and economic democracy. From Sartori's lucid discussion of the problem, it becomes clear that, when we speak of democratic systems of government within the Western world, we are referring to political and liberal democracy (1965: 353:84). Lipset’s definition can now be complemented by Sartori's: Democracy is the power of active democratic minorities, the word democratic meaning that the recruitment of these minorities must be open, and that they must compete according to the rules of a multi—party system. (1965: 90) We have now all the ingredients necessary for our operational definition: democracy will be here said to exist whenever there is a system of government wherein public officers are elected periodically, by means of universal suffrage, according to constitutional prescriptions, and with participation of a multiplicity of political parties. b) Stability Eckstein stresses that stability must be regarded as something more complicated than mere endurance, as he proposes a definition of democratic l4.- Lipset's requisites are: a "political formula" specifying which institutions, political parties, a free press, etc. are legitimate; one set of political leaders in office, and another, or several other sets of recognized leaders attempting to gain office (1960: 45). 34 stability based on three requisites: persistence of pattern, decisional efficiency, and authenticity (1966: l-2). The difficulties concerning the Operationalization of Eckstein's definition can not be overemphasized. In the first place, this definition calls for strictly determining what length of time a pattern of govern- ment must go unchanged to be considered persistent. It is hard to conceive precisely of one such dimension in such a way that it can be applied to all historical circumstances. A second difficulty arises with Eckstein's exaction for the authenticity of democratic government. The author himself is aware of the vagueness of this concept, and tried to clarify its meaning by adding that: "democratic structures must not be mere facades for actual government" (Eckstein, 1966: 2), and that: elections " must decide in some way, the outcome of competition for power and policies”. (Eckstein, 1966: 2). These two final remarks add very little meaning to the above conceptualization, since the latter requirement had already been included in our definition of democracy, and the former one simply restates the quest for authenticity in a different manner, but still without precision. When these two formulations are removed from the concept, we are left with Eckstein's requisite for decisional effectiveness as the remainer and most powerful component of the definition. In attempting to operationally define democratic stability under the perspective of decisional effectiveness, one can greatly benefit from the model of democratic problem-solving presented by Almond (1969: 73-128), who envisages democratic rule as the combined 35 operation of two kinds of social mechanisms: on the one hand, interest aggregation and interest articulation entities (interest groups), and on the other, conflict solving agencies (political parties). It follows from this view that the extent of decisional effectiveness in a democratic system can be measured by the amount of conflict that is channelled through the political parties and the parliamentary process. It can be contended that the above conceptualization does not yet provide firm grounds for operationalization. Nevertheless, if conflict is conceived of within a zero—sum situation, it them becomes the case that all conflict not being solved through the political process would be expressed through unstructured forms of protest, and non— institutional pressure gropp action. The above dimension can be accounted for by taking a count of all strikes, confrontations, etc., taking place in a given society during a specified period of time. Each of these dimensions can be sub-clas— sified into different degrees of seriousness or gravity. In the case of the present study, the following conflict- dimensions were researched: total number of strikes in a year (includes all kinds of strikes), number of strikes lasting one week or longer, number of strikes paralyzing public services (banking, mail, public hospitals, electricity, telephones, public transportation, or sanitary services) number of public servant strikes, numer of 24—hour general strikes, number of violent confrontations or riots, number of times the Army and/or the Police were called on by the government to intervene in connection with a labor problem, number 36 of unions mobilized, number of working days during which emergency laws were in effect, and number of guerrilla operations reported. Each of these indicators were computed over a one~year period. Given the difficulty of weighting each indicator in comparison to the others mentioned, it was decided that all dimensions would be listed in a table, but no attempt would be made as to compute a composite index. Growth rates were computed for each indicator separately with the first count, whenever it took place, considered as a one—hundred value. c) Age-Stratified Society All societies are in some way age-stratified, in the sense that some roles (childhood, adulthood, etc.) are dfined on the basis of the incumbent's age. Age-based hierarchies are also of paramount importance within family and kinship systems, and even in the cases of the more developed societies, the patterns of authority and solidarity that are learned within the family context provide some socio—psychological grounds for the development of individual social integrative mechanisms (Eisenstadt, 1956: 51). At stake here is not the kind of age stratification that exists in every society and continues to exist even in the most universalistic- ally oriented social systems, but the specific case of societies where seniority is the m§12£_ dimension that regulates social mobility. In trying to capture the above meaning into one operational definition, one inevitably runs into the difficulty that no single quantifiable indicator can accurately measure the extent of age-stratification in a society. 37 The problem could be more easily solved were it possible to account for the proportion of older age people in top positions in every occupational and social hierarchies; this endeavor proves unfeasible, however, since the terms included are too vague, and the task involved is clearly herculean. Our resulting definition is two-fold: on the one hand, it calls for quantitative evidence on the effects of education as a mobility variable; on the other hand, it relies on a review of the current opinions of leading area experts, to assess the inter-generational power—balance situation. In conclusion, we shall here term ”age—stratified" a society: first, when in that society education does not operate as an independent means of social mobility; in other words, when individual life chances in that social context are contingent upon personal positions relative to the informal structure which regulates access to, and mobility within, the occupational structure, instead of depending basically on the amount of formal education the individual has received. This conception must be supported by hard statistical data. This is, of course, equivalent to a pattern of ascription and particularism. Second, our definition requires that, according to reputable descriptions, generational positions and social hierarchies be strongly inter-correlated in that society. 38 d) The Dependent Variable: PolitiCal "Attitudes The dependent variable in this study is represented by the political attitudes observed during the campaigning. This conceptual category is conceived as a syndrome of highly inter—related issues. Some of these have been colapsed into composite indexes, but the most part have been dealt with on an independent basis. The following items are included: l.— Inequity Perception.- The ladder technique 15 was employed to measure the respondent's extent of self—perceived deprivation. The basic instrument for this purpose consisted of a 9—step ladder which was presented to the interviewees while they were asked to imagine that the top of the ladder represented those people who were doing best in a certain respect, while the bottom represented those who were doing worst. The dimensions thus measured were: first, recognition; second, pay. Interviewees were asked to situate themselves at certain point on the ladder. The answers were recorded on a 9-point scale which was later translated into High-Medium-Low categories. Additionally, the ladder was flashed once again, this time asking the respondent to try to imagine that the t0p represented the kind of life he/she had five years ago, then to locate a point on the ladder representative of the standard of living he/she had at the present. 15.- For a detailed description of this technique see Cantril and Roll (1971). 39 Responses for this item were also recorded on a 9—point scale, then collapsed into thirds. ‘Measurement of these three variables is handled separately. They altogether attempt to provide different, yet coincident measures of the perception of the impact the local economic situation had upon the respondent's personal standard of living. Individuals who rank high in the soci0«demographic scales, but locate themselves at the bottom third of the pay and recognition scales will be said to perceive the system as highly inequitable. Likewise, individuals of all social groups who located themselves at the bottom level of the standard of living scale will fall under the same category. 2.- Commitment-Alienation.- Commitment will be here defined on a single item measure, on the grounds of the actor's conviction that hard work, honest behavior, and competent professional or job performance, lead into what we shall here call "living well". Highly committed individuals will then be expected to think that a high standard of living results from one's compliance with the above norms, which is another way of expressing their belief in some basic fairness of the distributive structure. On the other hand, alienation will be defined as the Opposite attitude, that is, a tendency toward describing deviant patterns of behavior as being associated with a rewarding life. The instrument deviced to measure commitment was a two—fold, Open question, whereby interviewees were requested to think of those "who "live'wel 'in this countryfl, and then to describe these people's 4O possessions and most common activities. Answers to the question were recorded verbatim, then coded into coneptual categories which were finally grouped into three broad types: "derogatory statements", ”non-evaluative statements”, and "appreciative statements". A first category response will be interpreted as an indication of alienation, while responses of the third class will indicate commitment. Because of the impossibility of continuous variable measurement, responses were so dichotomized. The above limitation of this study, together with some other defficiencies of the instrument will be discussed later. 3.— Ruling Class Identification.- It is here assumed that the higher the respondent's identification with the ruling stratum, the stronger will be his tendency to attribute the worsening of the situation to circumstances other that the national leadership. The problem was explored by means of a projective question whereby interviewees were asked whether they believed that whoever would be elected for President would ultimately make a difference in the course of affairs. A negative response was interpreted as an evidence of ruling—strata identification. Again in this case, the responses were sorted into a dichotomous clas— sification. 4.— Change vs. Status—Maintaining Orientations.— Change orientations will be here defined in terms of the respondent's taking stands that challenge the authority structure, which will materialize into the respondent's assigning high priorities, in terms of necessary action, 41 to anti«administration issues. On the other hand, status-maintaining orientations will express themselves through a respondent's giving the highest priorities to pro-administration, and anti-Left items. The instrument provided a series of items which respondents were asked to evaluate in therms of whether something should or should not be done about the problem mentioned, then to express whether the recommended (or not recommended) action was urgent or not. Items were thus classified into: "urgent", ”not urgent”, and "nothing-to-be— done_about” categories. Items over whose alledged urgency there was practical unanimity were dropped from the index. The remaining six issues were combined into a Likert_type scale whereby respondents received +1 to +3 scores for each "change—oriented” item, -1 to -3 scores for each "status-maintaining" issue. The resulting score, ranging from a maximum of +10 to a minimum of —3 were collapsed into four categories. Roughly speaking, the index provides a measure of the respondent's location within the radicalism- conservatism continuum. 5.— Party Preferences and Party Identification.- Conservative Vote will be here defined as the respondent's expressing his/her intention to vote Colorado in the coming election. The Colorado Party was the administration party at the time this survey was taken. Indeed, the President, who was also the head of the Colorado Party, and was seeking re-election, had drawn a hard line against organized labor, left parties, and guerrilla movements. For all concepts, at the time when this 42 survey was taken, irrespective of what stands the same party might have taken in the past,16 the Colorado image was that of a "law and order" party. Moderate Vote will be here defined as the respondent's expressing his/her intention to vote Blanco. Although various authors have considered the Blanco party of Uruguayas a conservative group, mainly formed by big landowners and the upper classes,17 within the specific context of the 1971 Election, and given the stands taken by the Blanco's Presidential candidate, it seems more reasonable to characterize this party in terms of ”moderate", i.e. as an Opposition party which stood middle of the road between the hard governmental line, and the liberal, or even radical stands of the left groups. Finally, Left Vote will be here defined in terms of the respondent's expressing his/her intention to vote for the Frente Amplio (Broad Front), a newly formed party resulting from a coalition of left groups which included the Communist and the Socialist parties. l6.- Some authors have presented the view that the Colorado Party of Uruguay is, or at least was, an urban middle class, and lower class political movement, prone to liberal and reform—orientations, while the Blanco Party represented oligarchic and landowning interests. (Cf Taylor, 1960: 52-3; Lindhal, 1962: 8]). This view, which has enjoyed great popularity among Uruguayan writers, has never been substantiated with historical evidence. At any rate, even if the above assumption were accepted, it does not apply to the picture found during the 1971 Election. l7.- For a detailed discussion of the origins and evolution of the Uruguay Political parties, see Pivel Devoto (1942) and Pivel Devoto pt 31: (1966). 43 By the same token, conservative, moderate, and left Party Identifications will be here defined in terms of the respondent's seeing themselves as Colorado, Blanco, or left supporters on a long-term basis, rather than exclusively for the purpose of voting in this particular election. The two items, voting and Party Identification, were explored through two independent pre—coded questions. Answers were recorded on a nominal scale which included all the above parties as mutually exclusive categories, in addition to the residual categories of "no answer", "undecided”, and "other". 6.- Self-Investment in Political Roles; Political Activism Index.- The extent to which respondents have developed political self- identities was measured by means of a composite index combining the following items: talking politics with relatives, talking politics with friends, knowing the number of votes required by the Constitition for the President to be re-elected, attending political rallies or demonstrations, contributing either money or work to a political party, and knowing personally one or more political leaders. Respondents were given one point for each affirmative question when the answer was dichotomized, one or two points when the affirmative option offered two alternatives ("from time to time”, or "frequently"). As it can be seen, self-investment in political roles was basically measured under the form of political activism. 44 7.- Clientele—Oriented Partisanship.- Clientele oriented partisanship was measured via respondents' ackowledgement of having received at least one favor from someone in a political club. The issue was explored by means of a pre-coded question which offered three options: no favor ever received, important favor received, and unimportant favor received. 8.— Primary Group Identification/Conflict.- Primary group identifi- cation was measured in this study by a respondent's declaring that his political views were shared by every relative living with him/her, or these views were not shared by anyone or just a few of the relatives living in the same household. e) The Independent Variable: Age The independent variable in this study will be represented by the respondent's categorized age. The age categories were designated as to correspond with the average occupational life cycle. Age was measured in this study by self report of years lived. Respondents were grouped by age into four categories. To represent the theoretically possible positions relative to the job market, we started by assuming that, in a service-oriented job— structure, access to which is mediated by secondary school education, the age limits for the cycle would correspond to the average ages of graduation and of retirement: 18 and 50 years respectively. Eighteen is. also the voting age in Uruguay. 45 As it will be extensively discussed later, the process of entering the labor market in Uruguay is a slow one. This consideration, along with the fact that the average graduation age for the pre-professional level is 20, justifies the claim that our younger generation, (18—25) embraces basically those who have not yet fully entered the job market.18 The intermediate generations are here defined as composed by those respondents falling in the age span between the two extreme generations (26-50) years; this lapse has been arbitrarily divided into two sub- categories: the first one (26-35 years) accounts for the period of high self-investment in the occupational role within which individuals establish themselves in the job market; the second sub-period (36-50 years) corresponds to the period wherein the actor, having already attained a stable situation, may be inclined to progressively with- drawing self from the occupational role, and becoming oriented to status maintainance. Respondent's ages were investigated and recorded by means of a pre—coded questionnaire item. Whenever this information was not volunteered by interviewees, an estimate was entered by the interviwer. Therefore, the only missing data in this category are those for selected individuals who could never be reached. f) Education: The Intervening Variable There are three educational levels in Uruguay: elementary, secondary, and University. The first two levels are free and compulsory, and it is generally accepted that completion of both 46 of them is desirable if not necessary in order to obtain a non-manual job. In this sense, secondary education in Uruguay follows the model of the French'Lypge, which goes back to the Napoleonic Reform.18 Nonetheless, secondary education in Uruguay is divided in two "cycles", the second of which is called pre-professional, or "preparatory". This cycle is shaped according to University standards,19 and supposedly is aimed toward those seeking a professional license through one of the Facultades which compose the University. Three educational levels were distinguished in this study: elementary, secondary and advanced. The first level corresponded to all those having at most completed elementary school. The second level included all those who have had at least some secondary school. The third level included all those respondents who have had at least some 18.— For a detailed description of the situation of education in Uruguay, see: Ministerio de Instruccion Publica (1965). 19.- Like in most Latin American countries, University education in Uruguay is intended toward attaining professional licenses in the so called "liberal" professions. Each of the classical professions are taught in one independent "Facultad" which means school. The word Facultad was the designation for the body of professors in classical Medieval universities. This meaning has been retained by some European and all American universities. French education, however, changed this custom, and the word Faculty was there used with reference to the whole educational institution, including physical facilities, the administra— tive staff, and the teachers. This latter meaning was ad0pted in Latin America, whose University pattern closely follows the French. The question is extensively discussed in Ribeiro (1966). 47 pre-professional education, teacher's training, or technical or business school. g) The Control Variables The control variables have been conceptualized as follows: l.- Social class.- A composite index was computed to establish the respondent's social class. This index included the interviewees' home owner or tenant statuses, the type of car they owned, their educational backgrounds, the type of household where they lived, the areas where they lived, the breadwinner's current occupation, and the family income. ' According to estimation by knowledgeable judges, the above items were weighted as follows: 6.4%, 8.0%, 9.6%, 20.0%, 16.0%, 20.0%, and 20.0% respectively. Respondents were clasified in four social classes: Lower, Lower— Middle, Upper-Middle, and Upper. The proportions of classes resulting from this study were consistent with previous research by the author, as well as with the findings reported by local sociologists 20 and with 21 Census data. 2.- Occupation.- Interviewees were asked their current occupations, which, after they had been recorded literally, were collapsed into the following seven categories: unemployed, retired, blue-collar worker, 20.— Cf Instituto de Ciencias Sociales (1961); Solari 33.31: (1963; Canon (1966). 21.- Direccion General de Estadistica y Censos (1969). 48 small business, big business, and finally, a residual category including professionals, bankers, financiers, etc. 3.«'Inc0me.— This is the kind of information that most Uruguayans would not volunteer. To illustrate the point, it suffices to say that tje Uruguayan Census does not contain any information about income. The instrument employed in this study was a modification of the ladder technique,as it is commonly used in commercial research: a 9sstep ladder was flashed, each step corresponding to one particular income level, and the interviewee was asked to indicate the step where he/she and his/her family stood. The resulting income distribution was checked against the occupational distribution in this study, as well as against pay and standard of living scales, with satisfactory results. Income information was collapsed into four categories: High, .MediumeHigh, Medium—Low, and Low. Having explained how the variables in this study have been conceptualized, and what instruments have been used to measure these variables, we are now in a position to formulate our hypotheses. But before that step is taken, I will try to demonstrate that the research setting selected for this investigation meets the requirements set up in the theory. CHAPTE R I V THE RESEARCH SETTING The social and political circumstances of Uruguay seem to offer an ideal Opportunity for this study. In the first place, during most of the Twentieth Century, this country had a republican form of govern- ment, with the President and the members of the Parliament being elected freely and periodically through universal suffrage. Second, for considerable time, the two—party system of Uruguay, as well as the organized pressure groups, appeared capable of handling conflict efficiently: national problems were solved within the framework of institutional procedures while non-structured fOrms of protest, as well as all kinds of political violence,were virtually non—existent. Third, regardless of the considerable extension of education that took place in Uruguay at all levels, access to the labor market continues to be regulated to a large extent through family and personal connections. This age~based occupational structure seems to be associated with a social and demographic situation wherein the older age groups have been gaining influence over the younger ones for the last thirty years (See Table 1). 49 “momfiv seam ache eouaae< floom.nmo.fie m00©.0hmu moom.4NmU moom.4oev moom.hmmV uz o.OOH e.mm N.mH w.mm 4.mm momu w namm.mwev flooe.kmfiv meko.mfifiv hmom.omfiv an4.H4NV "z o.ooH H.0N 0.0H o.wm m.mm mama H®>O 5o paw uHo whee» om coflumasmom I.xmsm592 I.H oHnt 51 Fourth, ultil after World War II, the Uruguayan economy experienced substained growth, but thereafter the country went into a lasting recession. Fifth, simultaneously with the advent of economic difficulties, Uruguay underwent increasing pressure—group activity, and a proliferation of wild-cat strikes, and other forms of non-institutionalized protest: public services were often the object of stoppage, while even the most conservative unions became more and more radicalized; student unrest and various manifestations of a generational crisis (Jara and Banales, 1969) became evident at this time. Finally, an extremely active guerrilla group Operated in Uruguay during the last ten years, posing a continuous challenge to the authorities. This situation culminated in June, 1972, when the President, with strong military support, decreed the end of the Congress, banned political parties, and established several forms of control over the news. It follows from the above discussion that, since the turn of the century, Uruguayan society has travelled all the way through the stages of high integration with high institutional stability, decreasing integration with increasing conflict, and, finally, collapse of what has been defined in the present study as the democratic structures. a) A Stable Democracy Table 2, below presents the data on political participation and left vote in Uruguay between 1906 and 1971. The two earlier elections were Senatorial ones, since, at that time, there was no direct 52 TmfleZ Political Participation and Left Vote in Uruguayan Elections (1906-1971).— Percentages.- Percent of Year Voting population Percent Voting Left Who Voted Total MOntevideo 1906 64.9 1916 62.9 1926* 81.7 1.3 8.9 1930 80.1 0.7 1.5 1932 37.3 6.9 14.8 1934 52.2 6.9 13.1 1938 56.2 8.9 16.5 1942 66.9 8.1 14.1 1946 67.4 12.7 21.5 1950 70.9 8.8 14.1 1954 67.9 10.5 18.6 1958 71.3 9.9 17.1 1962 76.6 8.9 14.5 1966 74.3 10.0 16.4 1971** 96.7 18.2 30.1 Source: Electoral Court, Uruguay * In 1926 a National Electoral Registry was introduced, to substitute for the previously existing ballot system. ** Vote was compulsory by the first time in this election. ._,\ . 53 Presidential election in Uruguay. Also, beginning with the 1926 Election, the present system of Voter Registration was introduced, in substitution for the previously existing ballot system. Electoral statistics prior to 1926 are quite imperfect in Uruguay, which justifies the missing data at the top of the table. From Table 2, at least three conclusions can be drawn: First, Uruguay had political authorities elected on a periodical basis since 1900; second, elections took place with a considerable percentage of the voting-age population taking part; third, the Uruguayan constituency was largely conservative, as indicated by the small percentage of left votes cast in every election. b) Decisional Effectiveness and Low-ViolenceéRates. The information we have compiled on group conflict in Uruguay is presented in Table 3 below. These data substantiate the claim that in the earlier periods studied here, the Uruguayan polity managed conflict efficiently. The table includes 10 different indicators of conflict not channelled through institutional procedures. The indicators habe been ordered tOp to bottom in a scale of increasing violence, ranging from strikes to guerrilla operations. A primary examination of the data in Table 3, demostrates that the most severe indicators of violence tended not to appear until late in the period studied. 54 .Hm:mm HHoz . HmoaH eem .omIkmmH eoHHoa ago How macaw: HoIMopmmH :Haes may «0 wee: are use .HHAQHV memHv NosmHHeom HemmemH can memHV .Hm .uo HcmomHa 509m «new :qu wouoshpmcou oon comm com ooH III III III "oumm szogo Hm NW N H III III III meoHHaHmao aHHHHHoso oomm mmmm oom mnH III ooH III ”puma cuzoeo ooH HoH NH n III e III mmzme :HV mzmH HocoweoEm em ooH III III III II- III ”mpam Hezoao N 0 III III III III III eONHHHnoz mcoch ooH oom III ooH III ooH III ”puma auscuo m 0 III m III m III mcoHuao>90ch oOHHom\xEm< e.mmm n.00e III em ooH ooH III ”69mm cpzoau n vH III H m m III mcoHpmucowmcou ucoHoH> omH ooH ooH omH mm ooH III ”oamm :HSOHO e w m o H m III moxHHHm Hanocom esoHIVN omH ooH ooH QNH mm ooH III “oumm cuzoho o w m o H m III mmxHHpm ucm>eom OHHHSH com com com coo com com ooH ”open :pzouo a m m e m A H WIUH>Hom UHHnsa :H mmxHHHm coo com com com com com ooH ”epmm :p3090 0 m m m m m H HowcoH Ho Hoe: H wcHummH moxHHpm m.noo H.nHo m.omm H.0me m.mmm m.mme ooH ”puma :HBoHo mmm on owe moe Nam 0mm HNH moxHHum mo Hones: HHHOH onImomH moInomH ooImomH eoImomH moIHomH ooImmmH. wmrmmmH whee» HOHmOHucH I.mpoumOHw:H omHo>Ho mo ommao>< xHHwe> I.xoecH quHHnmumIcoHueamoucH I.m oHnmb 55 In addition, all the literature dealing with Uruguay, portrays the local society as extremely stable. For instance, the country had the lowest rate of deaths from domestic violence in the world (Taylor 22.3}:’ 1972: 114); suicide rates in Uruguay are also among the lowest in the world (United Nations, 1966). Descriptions of Uruguay as a highly developed political society can be found in Fitzgibbon (1954), and Pendle (1952). Both these authors coincide with Taylor (1960) in depicting Uruguay as a society where individuals were allowed practically unrestricted freedom to express their views, and where political groups were able to voice dissent with no limitations, and without any fear of further persecution; indeed, there is also unanimity among these authors in their perception of Uruguay as a peaceful and highly integrated society. (Fitzgibbon, 1945: 8-10; Pendle, 1952: 14-16; Taylor, 1960: 23-28) Duverger (1954: 105) includes the Uruguayan political parties in his discussion of classical two—party systems, and stresses that these parties are among the oldest in the world. Although Almond (1960: 480-81) among others, has refused to consider the party system of Uruguay as truly two-party, because the same party won all consecutive elections between 1906 and 1958, the question remains basically one of semantics and does not involve any denial as to the legitimacy of the political process in Uruguay as fully democratic. Extensive discussions of this matter can be found in Fitzgibbon (1951, 1956, 1957, and 1961) and Gil (1953). 56 This view of Uruguay as a democratic and higly integrated society is reinforced by the conclusions reached by those authors who studied the country's social organization. All these writers coincide in their appreciation of what has been termed "the Switzerland of Latin America" (Fitzgibbon, 1954: 18), or "South America's First Welfare State" (Pendle, 1952: 1). In addition to this, Harvard histo— rian Vanger (1963), and Swedish social historian Lindhal (1962), both of whom have dealt with the processes of political and social development of Uruguay, found that the social philosophy which under- lied these processes was humanitarianism. Thus, all the evidence available altogether point to the fact that Uruguay meets the requirements of our definition in terms of having a democratic form of government and a stable society: it had periodic elections, and freedom of expression; political participation was channelled through universal suffrage and a two-party system; the amount of decisional efficacy the government had was high, as measured by the low extent of conflict not channelled through institutionalized patterns; and finally, all conceivable indicators of social disfunction, or social disintegration, were extremely low in Uruguay, by all international standards. c) An Age—Stratified Society Inasmuch as there is consensus among scholars who have studied Uruguay as to this country's having developed the trappings of democratic rule and modern life, there is also unanimity of opinions that the local value system was mainly particularistic, and that seniority and 57 family connections provided most of the grounds for the ascription of statuses and the regulation of occupational mobility. The above conclusion is clearly stated by Solari: It is quite clear that particularism is a very important phenomenon in Uruguayan society, and that it clearly predominates upon universalism. A multiplicity of facts point to this: the present system of selection for government jobs is based on kinship ties, affiliation with a political club or party, or personal connections, etc., all of which are particularistic criteria. A similar phenomenon is very extended in private activities, where selection of personnel on the grounds of universalistic criteria, standardized tests, etc, is exceptional,and, is very often applied only to those candidates who have been previously selected on the grounds of their personal connections. (1964: 162) A similar conclusion is reached by Weil et al.: Upper mobility is limited by the importance of personal and family connections in obtaining a job or a social position. Selection for positions in the government or private enterprises is generally based on kinship, membership in a certain club or political faction, or friendship, rather than on universalistic criteria such as standardized tests, educational level, grades, or experience. When such criteria are applied, the candidate has already been selected on the basis of personal relationships. (1971:77) Similarly, 24.7% of all University students who answered the 1960 Census at the University of Montevideo declared that they would eventually rely on particularistic means to obtain a job (Instituto de Ciencias S0— ciales, 1964: 73). Indeed, among those who expressed this belief, 46.8% expected their friends and relatives to be instrumental in their finding jobs (Instituto de Ciencias Sociales, 1964: 76). 58 The prOportion of students relying on particularistic orientations for acquisition of jobs increased with age, as well as with the student's feeling that it will be hard for him to find employment (Instituto de Ciencias Sociales, 1964: 80); both the above variables reached their peaks among the group aged 24-25 which is the age level at which, the report says, Uruguayan students normally face the concrete task of job-hunting. In addition to this, the 1960 University Census depicts University students as having little or no conflict with the older generation. 60.5% of a1 students said that they were satisfied with the life orien- tation they had learned from their parents, and thought that their own homes in the future will most likely resemble very much those of their parents. Also, 60% of all students declared that they usually received advise either from their parents, from other relatives, or from other people of older age. (Instituto de Ciencias Sociales, 1964: 33-34) Stronger evidence about the importance of age in the ascription of statuses in Uruguay comes from a study of student work from Labbens (1965). As shown in Table 4, student's father occupation is not a good predictor of whether a student would work or not, (Labbens, 1965: 91) even when it is noted that the sons of white collar and blue collar workers tend to work more frequently than do other students. Labbens notices that male students tended to work much more than did females, irrespective of the student's social class (1965: 93); the author also compared student employment at the time of the student's first enrol- lment versus employment at Census time, i.e., years later, and found Table 4.— 59 Employment Status of Montevideo Male Students By Father's Occupation.- Percentages.- Father's Occupation Student Employed at: Census Time Enrollment Difference Landowners 34.6 14.4 154 Farmers 48.0 25.8 86 Industry 48.0 21.7 121 Business Executives 50.0 21.7 130 Miscellaneous 50.0 21.2 135 Professionals 51.7 16.5 214 Craftmen, Small Business 57.0 31.3 91 Technical workers 59.7 28.7 163 Administrators 60.0 26.3 123 Blue Collar Workers 60.0 43.5 38 White Collar Workers 61.3 34.4 78 TOTAL 56.2 28.5 97 After Labbens (1965) 60 Table 5.— Employment Status of Montevideo Students by Sex and Father's Occupation.— Percentages.- EMPLOYED NOT EMPLOYED Father's occupation Male Female TOTAL Male Female TOTAL Landowning 2.2 0.8 3.0 3.1 3.1 5.0 Farming 2.0 0.9 2.9 1.7 1.7 3.4 Crafts 16.2 6.2 22.4 10.0 10.0 21.1 Industry-Commerce 5.5 1.8 7.0 4.8 4.8 9.9 Professions 5.8 1.3 7.1 4.5 4.5 8.0 Executives 5.6 1.6 7.3 3.8 3.8 7.0 Administrators 8.0 2.5 10.6 4.4 4.4 7.1 Technical Workers 2.7 1.0 3.7 1.5 1.5 3.1 White Collar Workers 14.5 5.6 20.1 7.4 7.4 17.9 Blue Collar Workers 8.9 4.2 13.1 4.5 4.5 11.7 Other 1.7 0.8 2.5 1.4 1.4 3.4 TOTAL 73.2 26.8 100.0 47.1 52.9 100.0 After Labbens (1965) 61 that students from the lower strata already had a job when they entered the University, while students whose parents were situated at higher occupational levels tended to find jobs later on (Labbens, 1965: 85). Labbens then examined the type of occupations held by students and concluded: Student jobs are not the kind of accesory jobs which enable a student to afford basic living expenses while in school, such as in the case of those working as waiters, or waitresses for restaurants, or as attendants for gasoline stations, as it happens in Europe, and even more frequently in the United States. Student work does not constitute a form of help which permits the job holder to subsist while in school; it is rather the first step in an occupational career. It all happens as if the student status would make the person eligible for the job. (1965: 92) In short, the study of student work in Montevideo by Labbens strongly suggests that a University student status in Uruguay is a means of legitimizing those occupational and social statuses which have been ascribed on the grounds of personal contacts. In this sense, education is still a means of mobility, but within a limited social sphere. This conclusion is totally consistent with Solari's findings in the sense that upper social mobility in Uruguay tended to be restricted to those who had reached educational levels higher than their fathers. (Solari 23.31, 1965: 56). The same conclusion is reached by Solari ££.El‘ (1967) in a study of education, occupation, and development in Uruguay, and by Iutaka (1963), who conducted a comparative study of social mobility and occupational opportunities in Montevideo and Buenos Aires. 62 Research findings and conclusions for other Latin American countries can also be compared with Uruguay; they lend support to the argument presented here. Bonilla (1967: 243) pointed out that only 49% of the holders of degrees in Law and 52% of the engineers who graduated from Latin American schools were professionally active. This seems to indicate that, in Latin American societies, University degrees are regarded as status symbols rather than being considered as the means of earning one's life. The same idea is reported in a study of student politics by Walter (1968: 11) who noticed that University doctorates in Argentina lent their holders considerable prestige. The author also noted that 40% of all students enrolled in the University of Buenos Aires were in the areas of Law and Medicine. The Montevideo University Census of 1969 found 46.86% of all students in Law and Medicine (Instituto de Ciencias Sociales, 1964). By 1968, when another University Census was taken, those two fields accounted for 47.61% of the total enrollment (Universidad de la Republica, 1968). Labbens (1965: 73) compared the situation of employed students in Uruguay with those of students in five Central American countries, and found that the less open the country's social structure, the higher the tendency of students not to be employed when they entered school. From this,one can conclude, as Labben did, that student work and formal education in the Latin American countries act as a means of of making young individuals relatively independent from parental and family rule. Scholarships and fellowships appear to have a similar function in developed countries. Thes conclusions are basically in fit with our MI- 63 proposition in the sense that education and job—oriented self- identities form the dynamic element of change-orientations. d) Economic Crisis After EconOmic Growth That Uruguay was prosperous in the aftermath of the Second World War has been extensively substantiated in the works by Pendle (1952), Taylor (1960), Brannon (1968), and Hanson (1938), among others. For our purposes it would suffice to say that Per Capita Gross National Product in Uruguay during the 50's has been estimated at the 700 hundred dollar level (Solari 33.31:, 1966). The fact that economic deterioration followed this situation of prosperity, and that the economic crisis was severe, is supported by data in Tables 6, 7, and 8. Table 7 shows that both the Gross National Product, and the Per Capita Gross National Product went considerably down in Uruguay between the years 1957 and 1968. It can also be noted from the table, that while there were some years (1961, 64, 65, 66) where GNP showed some recuperation, and reached the same level as it had in 1957, Per Capita GNP declined steadily throughout the period considered. Table 8 illustrates the composition of Uruguay's GNP for 3—year periods between 1957 and 1968. This table shows that the services grew more than did any other sector of the economy during the period; which means that employment and expenditure in this particular area were being used by the government as a means of compensationg for the recession in the others. 64 Finally, Table 9 compares the buying power of salaried workers for the period 1957-67. This table shows that actual value of wages by 1967 had been reduced by one fourth in a ten—year period. The impact this condition must have had upon Uruguay's large middle classes is easily imaginable, even more when at the same time the expectations of middle class Uruguayans were continuously raising, as it seems to be reflected in the fact that between the years 1955 and 1965 , while the nation's GNP decreased by ten percent, enrollment in secondary schools increased ten—fold. (CIDE, 1965: 95; Solari §£_§l, 1967: 73) e) Increasing Conflict and Decreased Stability Paralelling the economic crisis, the last two decades witnessed the growth of non-institutionalized forms of protest in Uruguay. Table 2 above, presents data in this regard. The table contains a series of indicators of non-institutionalized conflicts in terms of the defini- tions provided earlier. Items in the table are listed in progressive order of conflict intensity, ranging from regular strikes to long— lasting strikes, stoppage of essencial public services, violent confrontations, and guerrilla operations. At first glance , it can be seen in the table that the bottom items, that is to say, those items which are high violence, did not appear until late in the period; indeed, they increased at a very fast rate thereafter. The table also shows a consistent progression in all conflict items listed. 65 Table 6 Uruguay. Gross National Product and Per Capita Gross National Product. (1957-68) Per Capita Year GNP* GNP** 1957 100 100 1958 96 95 1959 94 91 1960 97 93 1961 100 94 1962 98 91 1963 97 89 1964 100 91 1965 101 91 1966 105 91 1967 98 85 1968 99 85 *Base: 1957= 17.304 (Millions of Uruguayan Pesos) **Base: 1957: 7.120 (Millions of Uruguayan Pesos) Source: Cuentas Nationales; Central Bank of Uruguay, 1969. 66 .momH .xmnmsgb mo Hcmm Hmhucou .mon:0Homz mmpcosu "oopzom momma GHHHDMSHD mo mcoHHHHz cHse .Hocuo paw .mOOH>Hom Hmpcoecho>ou .moOH>Hom HmHocmaHm .oosmhsmcH .mxcmm “0p :oHpHeew :H .mooH>Hom xHeuHcmm paw .quOHHpoon «Houwz .m:OHpm0H::EEou ”movsHocHI www.mH Hmm.mH emH.mH Hmm.4H IIIz oo.ooH oo.ooH oo.ooH oo.coH HHom oo.H m4.e ew.m m4.4 m4.m wcHazoon: Ho.a- Hm.k mA.4H Nw.NH No.4H omega mm.HI wm.4 00.4 AN.4 AH.© coHposHHmcou em.H mm.mm eo.mm ok.kH mm.mm waspoamsaaz 4m.H am.eH mm.mH we.HH mm.4H oHsoHsoHHm< ooeoHoHHHa weIeemH meImemH moIoomH mmIemmH Hes I HHV Hey Hmv HNV HHS I.mommuemueoa I.Hw0InmmHv .uostHm HmcoHumz mmOHm m.xm:wsyz mo cOHuHmomEou -.A mHHaH Table 8.— 67 Urugpay.+'Buying_Power of Salaried Workers. (1957-67) Year 1957 1958 1959 1960 1961 1962 1963 1964 1965 1966 1967 Index 100 97. 89. 89. 89. 92. 91. 87. 81. 82. 76. Source : Institute of Economics, School of Montevideo. of Economics, University 68 Together with the manifestations of labor unrest that are depicted in Table 2, severe forms of protest were also represented by a succession of student protests, and by the appearance and continuous operation of an extremely active guerrilla group: The Tupamaros. From all this, it can be inferred that, prior to the 1971 Election, Uruguay had moved from its original condition of a relatively stable, integrated society, to a situation of turmoil and convulsion, charac- terized by student and labor strikes, union occupations of factories, frequent mobilization of workers by the government in response to the above situations; violent confrontations confrontations between students and the Police also took place, with five students killed during 1967-68. Nonetheless, all kinds of guerrilla activities were reported, ranging from bank robberies to kidnappings and aasasinations. The above considerations provide strong support to the contention that Uruguay definitely fits the model presented in this study: original stability under democratic rule, age stratification, economic deterioration, and increasing social conflict. As a corollary, after a series of conflicts between the Executive and the Legislative branches of the government, on February 13, 1973, the President of Uruguay ordered the Congress disolved. By the same decree, a National Security Council (with a majority of military members . . . . . . 21 on it) was created, and party act1v1t1es were prohibited. 21.- Information on the issue was carried in the city editions of The New York Times on February 14, 1973, February 15, 1973, and March 2, 1973. See also the editorial to the New York Times on February 16, 1973. 69 By all means, it seems that the 1971 Presidential Election in Uruguay was a critical time in the political history of that country. The hypotheses in this study refer to the reactions of different age-groups under such historical circumstances. CHAPTER V HYPOTHESES The theoretical propositions presented in a previous chapter will now be organized into testable hypotheses. These will be presented according to the following organization of items: 1) Inequity-Perception 2) Commitment/ Alienation 3) Ruling-Strata Identification 4) Change/Status-Maintaining Orientations 5) Attitudinal Configuration 6) Party Identification and Party Preferences 7) Self Investment in Political Roles — Political Activism 8) Clientele-Oriented Partisanchip 9) Primary-Group Identification/Conflict 1.— lneqpity Perception As discussed above, different age groups are expected to hold different evaluations of the authority structure, in terms of how fairly or unfairly they are being treated. In principle, this can be summarized as follows: 70 71 H l.- The intensity with which the authority structure is perceived as being inequitable will be at maximum for the younger generation, at minimum for the older. The above proposition can be sub divided into the following: H 1.1.- Self perception of being poorly 22 recognized as compared to the rest of society will be maximal among the younger group, minimal among the older. H 1.2.- Self perception of being poorly paid as compared with the rest of society will be higher among the younger generation, lower among the older one. H 1.3.- Self perception of standard of living having deteriorated as compared with five years ago will be maximal among the younger age group, minimal among the older generation. 2.- Commitment/Alienation Commitment was conceptualized in our previous discussion as a function of time an actor has been exposed to the social system. There- fore, high commitment to the system is more likely to be found among older age groups. On the other hand, alienation, which was defined as the opposite to commitment, is more likely to appear among the younger generation. 22.-Recognition refers here to what interviewees interpreted in common language as "being recognized at work", and in terms of locating one's position somewhere between "the most and the least recognized people in this country. 72 Measures of commitment and of alienation in our instrument come from the coiding of the Open ended question whereby individuals were asked to describe the possessions and the ways of life they perceived as being characteristic of "those who live well in this country". The question is intended as an indirect measure of the actor's belief upon the correspondence, or lack of correspondence, between living well, and complying with the system's professed values. Thus, commitment and alienation will be here measured by the proportion of appreciative statements, and of derogatory statements, respectively, found among each group's responses to the above question. H 2.- Commitment will be maximum among the older group, minimum among the younger. Alienation will be maximum among the younger group, minimum among the Older. 3.— Ruling Strata Identification I hypothesized that the higher the actor's identification with the distributive structure, the lesser his tendency to depict the current situation of Uruguay as being ”worse" or "much worse" as compared to five years before. The five year period was chosen for this test, because it is the normal length of a Presidential mandate in Uruguay. H 3.1.- The tendency to describe the current local situation in terms of its being "worse” and "much worse” than it was five years before, will be maximal among the younger group, minimal among the older. 73 It is also assumed that the higher the actor's identification with the existing order, the stronger his/her tendency to attribute the worsening of the situation to circumstances other than national leader? ship; this feeling, in turn, will be expressed through consideration that things will continue to be the same, independently of who is elected President. H 3.2.— Belief in the possibility of changing the local state of affairs by means of a change in the national leadership will be stronger for the younger generation, minimum for the older group. 4.— Change vs. Status—Maintaining Orientations It is here hypothesized that when individuals feel that they have been not sufficiently rewarded, they are likely to challenge the authority structure, and become advocates of change. Change—orientations have been defined in a previous chapter in terms of supporting anti- administration campaign issues; on the other hand, status—maintaining orientations will manifest themselves through the actor's supporting pro-administration issues: H 4.1.— Change—orientations will be maximal among the younger group, minimal among the older. H 4.2.- Status-maintaining orientations will be maximal among the older group, minimal among the younger. 74 5.4 Attitudinal Configuration l hypothesized that syndromes of highly interrelated beliefs and behavior will be reported at both extremes of the generational spectrum. Configuration is indicated by a concentration of high radicalism and conservatism Score values at both poles of the generational continuum: H 5.— The attitudes and behavior of the contrasted age categories are configurational, i.e, at each age category, the variables that bear upon change-orientations, and of status-maintaining orientations, are interrelated. 6.- Party Identifications and Party Preferences This item can be in some manner considered as a continuation of the previous ones, given that party preferences are but the voting expression of the political attitudes mentioned above. The terms Left, Conservative, and Moderate vote, have been defined in a previous chapter: H 6.1.- Left Party Identification and Left vote will be maximum among the younger generation, minimum among the older. H 6.2.- Conservative Party Identification and Conservative vote will be maximum among the older group, minimum among the younger. 75 7.4‘SelféInVeStment'in Political Roles 'Political"Activism‘lndex I hypothesized that withdrawal of self from family roles, and from previously held occupational roles, will lead into the deve10pment of self identities connected with the performance of political roles. The latter will be measured in this study in terms of political activism scores. A Likert—type scale discussed above, accounting for various dimensions of individual political involvement was used to measure political activism. H 7.1.— Political activism scores will be higher among the younger generation, lowest fer for the older group. H 7.2.- Political activism scores will be maximal for the most educated, minimal for the least.‘ 8.- Clientele-Oriented Partisanship Despite the higher political involvement that is expected to be found among the younger generation, there is one special type of political participation which we expect to be higher for the older group: I call this form of political involvement "clientele—oriented" partisanship, previously defined as the extent to which individuals ackowledge that they have received at least one favor from someone in a political club: 7+4-p—_ 76 H 8.— Clientele-Oriented partisanship will be maximal among the older group, minimal among the younger. 9.- Primary Group Conflict Given the context dealt with in this study, I hypothesize: if strong self investment in political roles takes place, and if change- orientations emerge, individuals will withdraw self from family defined identities. In other words, younger individuals, who are the most affected by the unfavourable situation, and therefore the more prone to take anti-establishment stands, will tend to be less likely to share their political views with other relatives living together, than are older family members: H 9.- In households, older family members are more likely to agree with some, or all, relatives they live with, than will the younger cohort. CHAPTER VI METHODOLOGY Two different kinds of data were analyzed in this study; figpp, historical evidence; these institutional data were discussed in support of the broad propositions concerning the Uruguayan society as a whole. These propositions have been dealt with in Chapter 111. Second, the hypotheses that have been formulated in Chapter V were subject to verification by means of survey operations. The present chapter is concerned with the instrument, the sample, and the analyses in those Operations. a) The Data Two surveys were taken in the metropolitan area of Montevideo: 45 days (N= 980), and 15 days (N= 880) prior to Election Day. MOntevideo is a relatively modern city with a population of 1.163.000 (Direccion Gral. de Estadistica y Censos, 1969). 46.34% of all Uruguayans, and 55.49% of Uruguay's urban population live in this 77 78 city, which has traditionally been considered as the urban— European, or cosmopolitan, liberal, middle—class, counterpart to Uruguay's countryside. With only 3.23% of its total population being rural, MOntevideo is Uruguay's Department (province) with the highest extent of industrial deveIOpment, as well as the administrative, commercial, and cultural center of the country. Montevideo newspapers and radio stations reach nationwide audiences in this country whose size is nearly that of the North American State of North Dakota. MOntevideo is also the home of the only Uruguayan university, and it also houses all the branches of the central government. An idea as to how MOntevideo predominates by and large over the urban dimension in Uruguay is evident in the fact that the second largest city of Uruguay, Salto, has only 46.000 population. b) The Instrument Before the research instrument is described, a word of caution is necessary with the peculiarities of the campaign. The 1971 Presidential Election in Uruguay took place in an atmosphere of tension, with increasing fears of violence and persecution being felt by different groups, and with almost generalized distrust on the part of individual voters to opening the door to strangers who wanted to ask questions about politics. 79 These conditions tended to escalate along with the development of the election campaign. Related to this was the fact that various candidates recurrently launched attacks against pollsters who were accused of manipulating public opinion by means of fabricating survey results. Also, since the left groups tended to carry their campaigns on the basis of home visits, the average citizen living in Montevideo grew up reluctant to entertain anyone inclined to talk politics, unless, of course -e was himself a left partisan. However, even if the latter was the case, there was still the fear that interviewers who would not identify themselves as left party memebers would collect information for Police files. Obviously, the questionnaire used in our polls had to adapt to these difficult circumstances. As a first consequence, the instrument was designed in the shortest possible form; also, a certain number of questions likely to raise suspicion on the part of the interviewees were dropped from the questionnaire, even when some of those items would have been valuable for the investigation of political behavior. The instrument employed in the first survey consisted of 75 items, 5 of which are open—ended questions. The remaining pre-coded questions offered the minimum number of alternative responses we could think of. Except in a few cases, interviewers were never asked to come in the respondent's homes, and interviews took place by the interviewee's front door, which, incidentally, in very many cases, was not even wide open during the interviewing. 80 In essence, the questionnaire consisted of three parts: First, a series of direct questions were aimed to finding out the respondent's political identification, party preference, and previous voting record, the questionning then moved into some of the areas most commonly investigated in voting research: parental political affiliation, and respondent's type and extent of political activism; seCOnd, a combination of the ladder technique, and of semantic differentials was used to assess the respondent's state of satisfaction or disa satisfaction with his/her social situation, the extent to which respondents identified with the authority structure, and with the prevailing value system, and how respondents evaluated the candidates running for the Presidency; £2139, several objective indicators were used to gather the necessary information on the respondents' socio- demographic characteristics. The questionnaire used in the second survey was basically the same as the one described above although somewhat shorter. The ladders and semantic differentials were removed from the second version of the instrument, in consonance with the increasing difficulty in interviewing. This second survey was aimed toward obtaining another measure of the respondents' political preferences and identifications, their perceptions of the national situation, and the extent they were informed about local affairs. 81 c)'The'Sample Sample size was established according to the usual criteria of using the minimum possible number of cases as to fulfill the requisites of efficiency, representativeness, reliability, and flexibility (Parten, 1950: 293). For our purposes, it was decided that survey results which could be read with a maximum error of 1 2% were sufficiently accurate, thus the sizes of the samples would be 980 and 880 for the first and the second survey respectively. Given the limitations of the existing maps of Montevideo, and the lack of accuracy of Census and statistical data in general in Uruguay, it was decided that the best possible sample design in terms of reliability was the one known as ”area probability”. Although this sampling scheme turns interviewing into a burdensome and expensive operation, it is the oldest and most experimented sampling technique in general use (Bureau of the Census: 1945; Brown, 1958) Additionally, I was personally familiar with it, having used the technique several times in previous survey research in Montevideo. The basic frame follows the same pattern established since the early 40's by the US Bureau of the Census. In this case, MOntevideo was subdivided into 13 segments whose respective populations were known. By means of a fotogravimetric plan of the city, the number of interviews for each segment were apportioned to the area's share of the total Montevideo pOpulation. The sampling unit in this operation was the household, defined as the space provided with sanitary services, where one or more individuals live on a permanent 82 basis. Once households had been selected at random from randomly selected segments, one individual was selected at random from each household in the sample. The latter was done following the procedure developed by Deming (1952: 85) whereby household occupants are listed by sex and age, then one among them is picked up according to a match that has been previously drawn on the questionnaire. d) Data Analysis A series of circumstances determined that the analysis of the data had to rely primarely on contingency tables, tabular presentation of data, and percentage difference analysis, rather than being based on higher level statistics. From earlier theoretical discussions, it can be inferred that the model employed in this study is not linear. A second difficulty in terms of data analysis is represented by the fact that most measurement in this study were made by means of nominal scales. Finally, there is a great variation in the sizes of the sub—samples employed. In addition to the above considerations, the conclusions derived from our data can easily be drawn from simple tabular presentation of data and its corresponding control operations; ultimately, the final support for the theory here presented comes less from statistical test of hypotheses, and more from patterns of data that successively support a series of theoretical propositions. If the data patterns are consistently in hypothesized direction, i.e., to have high convergence, there would be reason to infer some viability in the theoretical framework. CHAPTER VII FINDINGS The precendent theoretical propositions, as well as the resulting hypotheses, can be conveniently classified into a value grQEPI and a behavioral group. In presenting the findings in this study, I will use the above classification, thus allocating a first section to the expected inter—generational polarization of values, and a second one to the polarization of behaviors observable among contrasted age groups. Additional paragraphs will subsequently account for the control operations and the alternative hypotheses. a)Value Polarization 1.— Inequity Perception. According to our theoretical-hypothetical system, the data should depict a situation of bipolarity affecting the values held by the extreme generations. Such a value opposition is hypothesized to exist at the three following levels: a) whether individuals see 83 84 themselves as being highly or poorly recognized; b) whether they see themselves as being highly or poorly paid; and, c) whether they perceive themselves as being among the most or the least affected by present economic crisis. All the three dimensions altogether form the core of the following hypothesis: H 1.- The intensity with which the distrib— utive structure is perceived as being inequitable will be maximal for the younger generation, minimal for the older. As suggested in a previous chapter, the above proposition can be sub-divided into three parts. Each of those will now be dealt with separately. H 1.1.— Self-perception of one's being poorly recognized at work, as compared to the rest of society, will be highest among the younger group, lowest among the older. The data in Table 30 below do not support the hypothesis. In fact, the observed relationship is the inverse of that hypothesized: low recognition scores increase with age. It is worth noting, however, that, had the hypothesis been formulated differently, that is to say with reference to high recognition scores, it would have been accepted: high recognition scores are higher for the older group, and lower for the younger one. Although rejection of this hypothesis does not at all invalidate the core of the assumed polarization, the data in the table bring to our attention another question of considerable importance. In the first place, it should be noted that the older generation has the highest scores on both perceived low-pay and perceived high-pay 85 dimensions. In the second place, it is also evident from the table that in addition to age, other variables account for important differences in the scores: low recognition scores are higher among the less educated, and among the lower classes and income groups as well. Occupational groups which are not currently active (the unemployed and the retired) also show high low-recognition scores. Finally, low-recognition scores are higher for the Left voters than they are for supporters of any other political group. On the grounds of these data, one is tempted to conclude that strong belief in the inequity of the system (in terms of self-perceived recognition at work) is likely to originate from three sources: economic deprivation, as it is the case for the poor, or the less educated; occupational marginality, as it happens for the retired, or the unemployed; and political dissent,as for the Left supporters. Whether these three dimensios operate separately, or form a syndrome of interrelated factors is a question of considerable importance, and also of great complexity, with which we cannot deal in detail at this point. On a more restricted basis, our analysis will now concentrate on the combined effects of age and education, by examining the data presented in tables 31 and 32. The percentages in both these tables testify to what we call the ”ambivalent" effect of education: in Table 31, high recognition scores increase in consonance with age at the advanced and secondary levels of education, but remain stable across age-categories at the elementary education level. 86 Table 30 Respondents Self-Perceived Recognition Level by Socio-Demographic Categories.- Percentages.- LOW MED HI_ N_ SEX: Male 17.7 40.0 14.7 430 Female 17.4 43.1 13.2 524 AGE: 18—25 11.4 47.4 8.8 114 26-35 15.9 42.4 13.9 151 36-50 15.5 45.1 17.5 348 51 and over 22.5 35.2 12.3 324 EDUCATION Elementary 18.9 39.1 10.3 614 Secondary 14.8 42.4 19.8 243 Advanced 12.2 45.5 17.1 123 SOC CLASS Lower 22.9 41.5 9.2 477 Low-Mid 12.9 44.8 16.4 402 Up-Mid/Up 7.4 26.5 32.4 68 OCCUPATION Unemployed 31.2 24.7 7 8 77 Retired 24.8 38.6 4.6 153 Blue Collar 20.9 45.6 13.7 182 White Collar 14.3 51.4 15 9 315 Small Busin 13.2 38.2 19.7 76 Big Busin 8.8 32.4 38.2 34 Prof. etc. 7.0 41.9 27.9 43 PARTY PREF Undecided 17.3 42.9 10.5 133 Colorado 13.4 46.6 19.0 232 Blanoo 14.4 42.1 16.9 195 Left 27.4 38.6 12.2 197 INCOME Low 22.1 35.4 8.8 240 Med-Low 17.0 47.5 15.1 522 Med-Hi 11.4 44.3 15.7 70 High 11.1 27.8 41.7 36 TOTAL 17.0 40.7 13.5 980 87 women ohm mowmucoogom :oflnz :o mhmofisc exp oumofiwcfl mmmoapcopmm :fi mogsmflm one AQNHV fioNV mmm V fimm V fivv V H.AH H.mm o.wm o.om w.o voucm>e< flammV mug V flfim V mom V fimq V w.m~ 0.5H 4.6m 4.HN m.w xgmecoumm flwmmV “HmNV flNmNV mom V “mm V m.oH 4.-H m.NH n.m o.mH xgmpcoaofim go om-om mm-om mm-wH onefiomEmzh wcflcmflmm< mucowcommmm Hm manmh 88 .wmmmn ogm mowmpcoopom aofiaz co mgonasc exp opmoflwcfl mommnpcopmm :fi moazmfim oak AONHV flow V fimm V mmm V qu V N.NH A.“ 0.8H 0.8H H.m emocm>e< flamNV mnv V Aflm V flom V mmv V w.4H n.5m o.HH m.NH H.HH xpmwcooom Awan fiHmNV flNmNV mom V mmm V m.wH o.mm o.n~ o.wH 0.8H xnmpcmEon qo om-om mm-om mm-wH oneHomEonh mcficwfimm< masovzommom mm ofinmh 89 From this it can be inferred that feelings of being higly recognized increase with age, once one has achieved a certain educational level. It can also be seen that high recognition increases with education among the older, while it decreases with education among the young. The data in Table 32 are consistent with the above reported findings: low recognition feelings increase with age at the elementary education level, then the feelings remain stabilized for all age groups at the advanced education level. The figures in Table 32 also suggest that there are no differences in the extent to which the intermediate generations feel recognized, as compared to the other age groups, at the advanced educational level. Nevertheless, there is need for caution about drawing inferences from these apparent differences; the subsamples at the university level of education are of too small size to permit firm conclusions. Let us now summarize our previous discussion, and decide on what can be salvaged, if anything, from our hypothesis. It is true that generational polarization exists with respect to one's perceiving oneself as being lowly, or poorly recognized; but the highest extent of high self-perceived recognition was found among the older group, not the younger. Indeed, the polarizing effect of aging seems to have been masked by the effect of education. The older and poorly educated do perceive themselves poorly recognized, and since they are numerous, they shape the general population's distribution. On the other hand, at the higher educational level, the younger group rank higher than the older group in low-recognition scores. 90 Furthermore, the lowest percentage of individuals who felt they were highly recognized was found among the younger group, while the lowest percent of these responses appeared for the older generation. What seems to have happened is that, in this particular case, the effect of education enhances the effect of age: the probability of younger individuals feeling highly recognized when they have attained high levels of education is low, while the same probability is considerably higher for the like members of the older age—group. The following hypothesis confronts us with a situation very much the same as the above one: H 1.2.- Self-perception of one's being poorly paid as compared with the rest of society will be highest among the younger generation, lowest among the older. Table 33 below presents our data on self-perceived pay-level, cross- tabulated by all the variables considered in this study. In general, these data reaffirm the conclusions drawn in the discussion immediately above. It can be observed in the table that one's placing oneself at the bottom payment-level is more likely to occur among those social and occupational groups who felt they were poorly recognized. These groups were: the older generation, the lower social classes, the unemployed, and the retired, with the addition, in this particular case, of the blue collar workers; the lower income groups, and the left voters, also form part of this category. In this case, again, the relationship observed between age and inequity perception goes in a direction opposite to that expected. Table 33 91 Respondents Self Perceived Pay-Level by Socio-Demographic Categories.— Percentages.- SEX Male Female AGE 18-25 26-35 36-50 Over 50 EDUCATION Elementary Secondary Advanced SOC CLASS Lower Low-Mid Up-Mid/Up OCCUPATION Unemployed Retired Blue Collar White Collar Small Busin Big Busin Profes., etc. PARTY PREF Undecided Colorado Blanco Left INCOME Low Med-Low Med-Hi High TOTAL LOW 42.3 42.0 33.3 33.1 40.8 51.9 45.8 34.6 30.9 52.4 34.1 19.2 50.6 62.1 54.4 36.2 32.9 26.5 23.3 47.4 38.4 40.0 55.3 56.3 42.9 17.7 25.0 41.1 36.0 43.0 39.1 29.6 30.5 43.2 44.7 30.2 43.3 41.2 23.4 23.5 30.2 49.2 43.4 52.9 30.8 46.1 41.0 27.4 25.8 41.0 52.9 44.4 35.4 t—INI—JO N {00040 H N-bO A1400 OONO OOU'l-b r—INNl—II—Il O\©O\L00\| b—JNNO U'li—‘NOO 4.3 IZ 430 524 114 151 348 324 614 243 123 477 402 68 77 153 182 315 34 34 43 133 232 195 197 240 522 70 36 980 92 When we examine the distribution of respondents with low pay scores by age and education, as in Table 34, we note that the ”Low's” for perceived pay increase with age at the elementary and secondary educational levels, but remain stable at the level of advanced education. At all ages, ”Low” pay-scores tend to decrease when the educational level increases. From this it appears that all age and educational groups perceive their own pay-levels in a fashion very similar to their own self perception of recognition. Our third hypothesized dimension within the inequity perception item, was that of the deterioration of the actor's standard of living. Specifically: H 1.3.- Perception of one's standard of living as having deteriorated, as compared with five years ago, wil be maximum for the younger generation, minimum for the older. Again in this case, we have choosen to present our data in percentages of responses, cross-tabulated by all the variables considered in this study. Table 35 shows that the perceived high deterioration of one's standard of living does not vary across age groups. In fact, there is one inter-generational difference with respect to self perceived deterioration of one's own standard of living: there were relatively more respondents who perceived the deterioration of their own standards of living as being low, among the younger generation, than they were among the older group. This is the opposite of what we had expected to find. 93 .vommn ohm mmwmpcoohmm sofizz :o mamassc ecu opmofiwca memmnucohmm :H moHSmHm och AONHV flew V mmm V Amm V A44 V m.om w.om o.Nm o.mm m.nm emocm>e< AmmmV “54 V mum V flow V mmv V 8.4m w.84 w.om 4.0m o.mm sumecooom AwemV mHmNV AmmmV flea V Amm V w.me o.mm n.m4 n.mm 0.04 sampcoaofim 3o om-om mm-o~ mm-mH one~om5655 mcflcmflmm< mucownommom vm oHDmH 94 Table 35 Self-Perceived Deterioration of Standard of Living by Respondents' Socio-Demographic Characteristics.- Percentages.- LOW MED HI N SEX Male 11.2 35.1 22.3 430 Female 16.6 33.4 18.1 524 AGE 18—25 21.1 36.0 17.5 114 26-35 12.6 35.8 21.9 151 36-50 11.5 34.8 18.4 348 Over 50 15.7 31.5 21 3 324 EDUCATION Elementary 12.2 32.7 18.6 614 Secondary 18.1 31.3 20.2 243 Advanced 13.0 39.8 22.8 123 SOC CLASS Lower 14.7 35.0 20.5 477 Low—Mid 13.9 32.3 20.4 402 Up-Mid/Up 13.6 37.3 16.9 68 OCCUPATION Uenmployed 9.1 32.5 32.5 77 Retired 16.3 28.1 26.1 153 Blue Collar 17.0 42.3 19.8 182 White Collar 14.6 35.9 17.5 315 Small Busin 18.4 30.3 17.1 76 Big Busin 14.7 38.2 20.6 34 Prof., etc. 16.3 44.2 11.6 43 PARTY PREF Undecided 18.8 38.3 20.2 133 Colorado 15.9 27.6 10.3 232 Blanco 15.4 39.5 21.5 195 Left 15.2 38.1 37.6 197 INCOME Low 24.6 34.2 15.0 240 Med—Low 20.3 34.1 15.0 240 Med-High 18.6 31.4 14.3 70 High 13.9 63.9 11.1 36 TOTAL 13.8 33.3 19.5 868 95 In conclusion, the way respondents perceived their standards of living, as compared to the ones they had five years before, was sensitive to their occupational statuses (higher deterioration was reported among the unemployed and the retired, than was among the other occupational groups), and to the respondent's political affiliation (considerably higher deterioration was found among the leftists, than it was among supporters of other parties) but this dimension had no sensitivity to either age or educational differences. Table 36 presents the data on self-perceived deterioration of the respondents' standard of living by age groups and educational levels. In this talble, percentages of High's" and"Low's” of perceived deterioration were constrasted with each other, and the percentage differences were computed. Since ”Low's” were subtracted from the "High's", the resulting scores indicate "net” deterioration in terms of absolute values. The table shows low scores for the young respondents with lower educational levels, and also for the older age group at the higher educational level. It also shows that self—perceived deterioration of one's standard of living increases with age at the lower educational levels, but, also that this relationship reverses itself at the level of advanced education. With respect to the intermediate groups, deterioration seems to increase with education for the younger generation (26-35 years) while it remains stabilized for the older cohort (36—50 years). We can now say that while our original inequity-perception hypotheses are far from being verified, a pattern of reasonable alternative expla— nations emerges from the data. 96 .msogm :owo kn wo>flmoaom m0 :OprHOHpopov :uoc: exp mmogmxm mopoom mcflpHSmoH 6:9 .mommpcoohom :Lmfl: Eoym mommpCOURom :304: mcflpomaHQSm kn downgEoo who: moQCoHomwflo 0.0+ 0.0- 0.0+ 0.NH+ 0.ma+ .0040 AMNHV mom V “00 V flmm V A44 V "2 0.0a m.HH 0.0a 0.NH 0.0a 300 0.00 A.“ 0.0m 0.4m 0.00 0000 ”04004>0< H.N + m.N+ 0.0+ n.0u+ 0.00. .0000 namNV mh4 V flfim V “00 V “04 V "2 H.0H m.mm N.mH 5.0H H.Hm 300 N.0N 0.0N 0.0a 4.HN H.HH :00: ”sawecooom 4.0+ 0.5+ 0.0+ H.A+ 0.4- .0040 AwamV mamNV AmmmV A04 V 500 V "2 N.NH p.40 0.0a 0.4H 0.NH :00 0.0a 0.00 5.5H 4.HN 0.0 :00: manageme®fim 04400 om Hm>0 00-0m 00-0N 00-00 .40400 0m>Hmumma 200940000 004 -.mommucoomom .:0fipwodvm 6:0 ow< an wcfl>fla mo chmwcmum .mucovcommom mo COflpwhofiHouoo po>flmuhomuwfiow om ofinmh 97 Young people who have been exposed to advanced education have not increased their feelings that the system is fair, but appear in our survey as being less satisfied with their position in society than do their generational peers who have had lesser education. On the other hand, for the older generation, the more educated one is, the less likely than one would complain about being insufficiently rewarded. It seems that either education generates some expectations among the younger generation that are not fulfilled by the opportunities avail— able to them; or that the values the young acquire through the educa- tional process tend to alienate them from the society's predominant value system. The same dualism of possible explanations applies to the older age groups: it is possible that the more educated among the older generation tend to complain less than the others because they are the most socialized into the predominant value system by means of a "functional” education, but it is also possible that they be more satisfied with their social positions simply because their educational attainments have helped them weather the economic crisis with the as— sistance of the more sound social and economic position they already had. At any rate, the evidence we have hitherto presented does not provide an answer for this question. Nevertheless, it is plausible to state the problem from now, for, if the underlying force in the shaping of the observed responses is a value factor (as it would be the case of alienation through education) both the older and the younger group will react similarly in terms of political values, as long as they share the same level of education. 98 If, contrariwise, a structural factor, namely the opportunities available to each group in the job market, constitutes the best predictor of political feelings, then older individuals will tend to be conservative, since they reached their statuses before the crisis broke out, while the younger, who entered the job market when good positions have already become scarce, will tend to become radical. 2.- Commitment-Alienation Social commitment, conceived of as the extent to which the actor shares the values and has learned the ways in his/her particular social system, is here measured by the percentage of ”appreciative” statements found among each group's responses. Typical examples of "appreciative” statements would be: ”Those who live well in this country work hard”; or: ”They work hard and generate jobs for others”; or: ”They enjoy what they have earned through their work.” Alienation, in our conceptualization, is simply the contrary of commitment, i.e., the extent to which the actor does not share the prevalent values. As perceived through our instrument, alienation mate- rializes in derogatory statements with reference to those who live well in the country. Typical examples of ”derogatory” statements would be: ”They do nothing, live well, exploit the others, and have plenty of comfort"; or: ”They travel a lot, have nice homes and cars, and live on the money they have made out of smuggling." 99 The hypothesis here is as follows: H 2.1.- Social commitment will be maximum for the older generation; minimum for the younger. Alienation will be maximum for the younger, minimum for the older. Table 37 presents the data on alienation for all social categories. From this table, it can be concluded that alienation does not vary across generations; indeed, the index is sensitive to other variables, namely social class, (the upper classes are more alienated than the lower ones); income (higher income groups appear more alienated than lower ones); and political preference (left voters are considerably more alienated than the others). Percentage differences can be computed from Table 38, by subtracting appreciative statements from derogatory (or viceversa). By no means are these indicators of commitment/alienation influenced by age-differences. Cross-tabulating the percentage distributions of derogatory and appreciative statements found for each age category by educational levels, produces the results reported in Tables 39 and 40. From these tables percentage differences can be computed: alienation percentages minus commitment percentages equals what we will here call ”net” alienation scores. A plus sign preceding the figure indicate that the alienation level found for that particular category was over the commitment one. (See Table 41) In drawing conclusions from these data, we are limited by the small sizes of the samples at the advanced level of education. 100 Table 37 Alienation Index.- Derogatory Statements by Socio-Demographic Characteristics.- Percentages.— Percent. N SEX: Male 23.0 (430) Female 21.0 (524) AGE: 18—25 24.6 (85) 26-35 24.5 (112) 36-50 19.8 (228) Over 50 22.5 (220) EDUCATION: Elementary 19.2 (614) Secondary 23.5 (243) Advanced 27.6 (123) SOCIAL CLASS: Lower 18.9 (614) Lower-Middle 24.9 (243) Upper-Middle/Upper 26.5 (68) OCCUPATION: Unemployed 15.6 (77) Retired 22.2 (153) Blue Collar Workers 22.5 (182) White Collar Workers 24.1 (315) Small Business 21.1 (76) Big Business 20.6 (34) Profes., etc. 20.9 (43) PARTY PREFERENCE: Undecided 18.8 (133) Colorado 20.7 (232) Blanco 25.6 (195) Left 30.5 (197) INCOME: Low 20.8 (240) Medium Low 21.8 (522) Medium High 31.4 (70) High 30.6 (36) TOTAL 21.3 (980) 101 .6600@ 600 momwucoohom :o0:3 co whomesc 0:0 opmo0vc0 memospcohmm :0 mmHDM0m 0:0 0000V 0000V 0000V 0000V 000 V "2 4.00 0.00 4.00 0.00 0.00 0>0000800mm< 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.00 0>000300>0 :02 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.40 0.40 0000000000 04000 00>0 0:4 00 00-00 00-00 00-00 0020200400 00 0000 004 u.wowmp:oQMom -.mm5000-om< kc xowcH :00umc600<\p=oEufieEou wm ofinwh 102 .wmmmc 000 00mmucoohom :00:3 :0 00035:: 0:0 opmoHUC0 mmmosucohmm C0 moHSWHM 0:0 0000V 000 V 000 V 000 V 044 V 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.40 -.00 00000>04 0000V 004 V 000 V 000 V 004 V 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.00 000000000 0000V 0000 0000V 000 V 000 V 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.40 0000005000 04000 00 00>0 00-00 00-00 00.00 200040000 .mw< a.momwp:600®m 1.:00uwoswm 0:0 ow< an mquEopmpm o>0um0oohmm< -.xowcH ucoau0fifiou mm oHan 103 .0000: 000 mowmucoQHQQ :00:3 :0 000:552 0:0 oumo0wC0 momo:pco00m :0 mo0SM0m 0:0 0000V 000 V 000 V 000 V 044 V 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.00 00000>04 0000V 004 V 000V 000V 004 V 0.00 0.00 0.00 4.00 0.00 000000000 0000V 0000V 0000V 000 V 000 V 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.00 0000000000 04000 00 00>0, 00-00 00-00 00.00 200040000 u.mowmpcoohom .:00umoswm 0:0 om< @00000mopmu .mucmwcommom 0: mucosopmpm x0oummoaoo n.xop:0 000000000< ow oHQmH 104 :0000:000< 500m -.:O0uw:000< :uos: opmo0©:0 mmpoom wc000300m .mowmpcoowom mowmpcoogom ucoauflesou mc0pomhu::m 0: 00039500 moocohommfiw ommucmoaom 0.00+ 0.00+ 0.00+ 0.0+ 0.0+ 00000>0< 0.0+ 0.0 0.0- 0.00+ 0.0+ 000000000 0.0+ 0.0+ 0.0+ 0.00- 0.4- 0000000000 :r0<000 00 00>0 00-00 00-00 00-00 2000<000m 00<. -.moo:ohommflo owmp:0000m -.xowc0 :00umao00<\pcoEpwanu 0v manmh 105 From the rest of the figures, however, it is possible to decide that alienation increases dramatically with education among the younger groups (18-25 and 26-35 years), and that it also increases with age at the elementary educational level. Although the figurcs at the bottom row in Table 40 seem to indicate that alienation levels also increase proportionate to age for the most educated respondents, this conclusion is not warranted, in view of the small samples involved. It seems more plausible to assume that, at the higher level of education, alienation remains stable for all groups. In conclusion, the data suggest that alienation increases with age in the absence of education, and that, it also increases with education among the younger groups. These results are basically consistent with the picture that has been obtained from the previously examined distributions. 3.— Ruling Strata Identification We shall now deal with the following proposition: H 3.- The estent to which individuals identify with the ruling strata, more specifically with the national leadership, is higher for the older group, lower for the younger one. In order to be tested, the above proposition was sub-divided into: H 3.1.- Assessing the national situation as "worse" or "Much worse”, as compared to five years before, will be maximum among the younger group, minimum among the older. 106 Since five years is the length of a Presidential mandate in Uruguay, the hypothesis combines the economic and the political dimen— sions in one single evaluation of the local state of affairs. That the economic situation had worsened during the period is undeniable; the economic indicators of this deterioration were presented in a previous chapter. Differences in the perception of this phenomenon would there— fore depend on either the way in which —ne social group in particular has been favorably or unfavorably affected by the general crisis situation, (structural element) or by the group's perception of the national sit— uation in whatever (and not necessarily economic) terms the situation is assessed (value element). Given that the age groups we are dealing with have been designed as to correspond with the positions in the labor market, the structural element can be identified by observing the reactions of relevant occupational sectors and then comparing those reactions to generational reactions. At this stage in our work, though, we shall concentrate in analyzing our variables just in terms of age—groups and educational categories, leaving the aforementioned endeavor for the final paragraph in this chapter. When the population of ”Worse's” and ”Much worse's” is cross—tabulated by all the social and demogrphic variables in this study, we have the results appearing in Table 42. It can be seen in the table that, effectively, the maximum percent— ages of ”Worse's” and ”Much worse's” is for the younger generation, whereas the minimum is for the older one (in fact, the smallest percentage in the table is for the 36—50 years age-cohort, which does not invalidate Table 42 107 Respondents Terming the Local Situation "Worse” and ”Much Worse” by Socio Demographic Characteristics. Percentages.- SEX AGE EDUCATION SOCIAL CLASS OCCUPATION PARTY PREFERENCE INCOME TOTAL Male Female 18-25 26-35 36-50 Over 50 Elementary Secondary Advanced Lower Lower Middle Upper Middle Upper Unemployed Retired Blue Collar Workers White Collar Workers Small Business Big Business Prof., etc. Undecided Colorado Blanco Left Low Medium Low Medium High High Percent. 62.8 62.2 70.2 66.2 57.2 63.0 56. 65. 75. 0&0 62. 61. 67. 0. \IOO©CN 72. 66. 67. 60. 63. 64. 76. \IVWOOVN 63. 40. 76. 93. ®®©\l 70. 61. 65. 88. CNN-b 60.8 (430) (524) (114) (151) (348) (324) (614) (243) (123) (477) (402) (59) (9) (77) (153) (182) (315) (76) (34) (43) (133) (232) (196) (197) (240) (522) (70) (36) (980) 108 our proposition). In addition to this, percentages show significant variations across educational levels (maximum for the most educated); occupational strata (maximum for the unemployed); political affiliations (maximum for the left voters); and income groups (maximum for the highest level). As compared with the previously studied variables, respondents' judgements about the national situation show similarities, and one highly significant difference. Similarly to the other variables, maximum percentages of responses evaluating negatively the local state of affairs are found among the left partisans; it should be noted, though, that unlike the situation with the previously considered variable, there are high percentages of "Worse's" and ”Much worse's” for the upper middle classes, the Moderate voters, the higher income groups, and the higher occupational strata. (This latter finding should be considered with caution, since the group is an heterogeneous composite including big landowners, bankers and industrialists, together with university professionals, who are likely to behave similarly to the higher educational level). At any rate, it seems that our data do more than confirm the existence of the relationship we had hypothesized; they also underscore what we would call the "political dimension” in the voters' assessment of the situation. ”Worse's” and ”Much worse's" appear heavily at both extremes of the income spectrum, as well as on both poles of the occupational scale. Although this does not rule out the possibility of an ”economic” motivation for the judgement expressed by respondents, 109 the finding support the assumption that it is not only the way in which the economy objectively hits every social group, but rather that each group's special sensitivity to the local state of affairs determines the group's evaluation of the situation. The political affiliation data in the table also point to this fact: almost every leftist in the sample (roughly 94% of all Left voters) termed the situation "Worse” and ”Much Worse”; the minimum percentage for all the categories in the table was found among the Conservative (Colorado) voters, while moderate voters stood inbetween. A combined analysis of the effects of age and education was also carried for all respondents' evaluations of the situation. The results of this analysis are presented in Tables 43 to 45. Table 43 shows that 70.2% of all interviewees aged 18-25 termed the situation ”Worse” and ”Much worse", as compared to 63% of the older generation, and to 60.8% of the total population. Although the percentage differences are very slim, the figures for those answering "Better" and ”Much Better” in the table seem to confirm that this other dimension fluctuates inversely with respect to the previous one. Since age and education seem to complement each other in predicting interviewees' responses, we attempted to ascertain which one, among these two variables, was the better predictor. This study was undertaken by means of cross-tabulating the general distribution table both ways, by education and age, and viceversa. Table 44 shows a heavy concentration of ”Worse's” and ”Much worse's” for both the younger groups with high educational levels, and the older groups with low educational attainments. Table 45, in turn, shows that 110 .0000: 000 m0m00c0000m :00:3 :0 00005:: 0:0 000000¢0 0000:00000m £0 m005M0m 0:H 0000V 0400V 0040V 0000V 0400V "z 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.00 00000 0002\00000 4.00 4.00 0.00 0.00 0.00 0000 0.0 0.00 0.00 0.4 0.0 000000 0002\000000 0<000 00 0000 00-00 00-00 00.00 "002000 200000000 .000. n.00m00c00000 u.mm:ouu:0m< km 0how0m 0000» m 00 00000500 00 00000300m 0000: 0:0 mo 0000900000 .m0c0wnomm0m M? 00:09 lll 00000 04000 00000 00000 000 0 "z 0.00. 0.00 0.0 0.0 0.00 0.04 00000>0< 0.00. 0.40 0.00 0.00 0.04 0.00 000000000 0.m0+ 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.00 0000000000 .0000 00.0000 .00000. ,00000 00000. 0: -03 .2:0 $0 $0 -00. 0: 0022250 .mw<, 1.00w00c00000 n.00>00 0000000050m 0:0 mso0wl0m< x: :00003 :002: 0:0 :00003: 00000000m 00000 0:0 mcwe000 00000comm0m 00 00:0H 112 00000 000 0 00000 0448 Hz m.0- m.m m.m m.m 0.0 00:0500000:: 0.0N- N.0m N.00 w.om 0.m0 om 00>o N.0N: m.mm N.00 0.Nm 0.0m omuom 0.00+ w.00 0.NN w.mm 0.00 mm-om w.0m+ 0.m0 0.0m 0.00 w.m mmuw0 .000: 00< 0000:000w 0000:0E0Hm mo< onH00QEH 002 :0000500m 0000: 0:0 00:0 m:0x0m 00:00:0000: 00 00:0H 117 expectations that go unfulfilled, thus alienating the person from the rulers and the established order. The origin of these expectations can be said to be two—fold: it is either education for those who have gone beyond the level of elementary school, or simply the process of aging for those who have not moved beyond that educational level. At any rate, it can be reasonably argued that respondents in this survey who had had secondary education lend themselves to test of the hypotheses better than do those in the other educational levels. Age— differentials at the elementary level of education also mean occupational or status differences. Those in the younger group who have but primary school are the most likely not to have entered the job market, while those who are at the same educational level but belong in the older age group are most likely the holders of poor jobs, or perhaps retired. Since the development of public education in Uruguay, goes back a few generations, the ceteris paribus condition cannot be assumed to exist when dealing with different age groups sharing the same low educational level. If the above claim is accepted, then it is noticeable that precisely at the secondary level of education, our hypothesis holds true: the higher the age, the lower the hopes that the situation may change depending on who is elected President. The theorized antagonistic effects of age and education in an age-stratified society are thus detected under the evidence presented. 118 4.- Change—Orientations We will now move from the realm of mundane interests, clearly represented by concerns such as whether things are getting worse or not, or how has the economic crisis affected one's own buying power, into a more abstract sphere of interpretation. Along with the electoral campaign in progress, it was ascertainable to political observers who witnessed the Presidential election of 1971 in Uruguay, that a few local issues had become the cornerstones of two opposed political orientations. On the one hand, the administration had drawn a "law and order” line, emphasizing the maintainance of order, and anti-guerrilla warfare, along with a substained anti-communist stand. Concomitantly, pro-administration supporters had voiced strong opposition to some reforms proposed by left and moderate groups, such as an agrarian reform, and the nationalization of the banks. The emphasis in the conservative campaign was not on reform, but rather on the "defense” or the preservation of the democratic institutions which, in the opinion of pro-governmental spokemen, were being threatened by continuous labor unrest and student protest. As a remedy for these problems, the administration resorted to enacting "emergency laws" whereby constitutional ”rights and guarantees of the individual” (the equivalent to the American Bill of Rights) were temporarily suspended. In opposition to this, left and liberal groups conducted political campaigns \dthfliemphasized the need for social reforms, like those mentioned above, and advocated for the inmediate lifting of the emergency 119 laws. Reform advocates argued that the evil at that time in Uruguay was not represented by organized labor and rebellious students, but by governmental inefficiency and outdated social structures. Change, rather than maintainance of order, was therefore the main professed goal of anti—administration parties. In addition to this, for the left groups, the question of limiting what they perceived as excessive or unwarranted American influence in Uruguay was also insistently discussed. The two hypotheses in this paragraph refer to the expected relationship between age—groups and political orientations: H 4.1.- Change orientations will be maximal among the younger group, minimal among the older. H 4.2.— Status Maintaining orientations will be maximal among the older generation, minimal among the younger. Respondents were presented with each issue, then they were requested to say whether they thought something or nothing should be done about that particular problem; in case the answer was an affirmative one, interviewees were asked whether they considered that the problem should be taken care of urgently or not. Results from the observations are presented in Table 48. The table presents percentage distributions of responses for each age~category. The left section of the table shows the distributions of answers requesting "urgent” action on the topics; the right section presents the percentages of responses considering that those topics deserved no action at all. Table 48 evidences a clear polarization of the two contrasted generations in terms of change and status maintaining orientations. 120 .0:00000:000o m:0:000:002-05000000 .0:00000:0000 0m:0:00 00000 00000 00400 00000 04000 00000 00000 00400 00000 04000 n z 00 00 00 00 00 04 04 04 00 40 00000 000000000 0000 0 0 0 0 00 00 00 00 00 00 000000000000 00000 00 00 00 00 00 04 44 04 04 00 0000000000 00 00000 00 00 00 00 00 04 00 00 04 00 00000000000 00000 0 4 0 0 4 00 00 00 00 00 000000 00000000002 4 0 0 0 4 00 00 00 00 00 0000000 00000000 00000 00.>0 00-00 00-00 00-00 00000 00 .00 00-00 00-00 00-00 00000 >mpomcoo ESE0xme 0000000000 .m+ we eneflxma 0 00 .5000000000 ESE0xme 000060000 56003 .0. mo 8:50:05 0 5000 00:00 mmhoom .Uommn 0H0 mowmpmoohom 06003 no mHmnES: may opwoflwc0 momocpconm may c0 moaswfim one 00000 04000 04000 00000 00000 Hz 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.0 0.0 0+ 00 0 0.04 0.04 0.04 0.44 0.00 0- 00 0- 0.00 0.00 4.00 0.04 0.04 4- 00 0- 0.0 0.4 0.0 0.0 0.00 0- 00 0- 00000 00 0000 00-00 00-00 00-00 00000 -. 000 -.mommpcoohom 1.003000 ow< 0n mohoom 500000000m\50000>0o0:ou av mHQmH 125 Table 50 Respondents Receiving Top* Conservatism Scores by Socio-Demographic Characteristics.- Percentages.- Percent. SEX Male 11.3 Female 15.8 AGE 18-25 7.5 26-35 7.3 36-50 13.3 Over 50 19.1 EDUCATION Elementary 15.7 Secondary 13.4 Advanced 6.7 SOCIAL CLASS Lower 15.2 Lower Middle 11.7 Upper Middle/Upper 14.7 OCCUPATION Unemployed 21.4 Retired 16.0 Blue Collar Workers 13.1 White Collar Workers 11.8 Small Business 11.1 Big Business 20.6 Prof., etc. 11.9 PARTY PREFERENCE Undecided 17.5 Colorado 24.6 Blanco 12.0 Left 1.6 INCOME Low 18.0 Medium Low 11.0 Medium High 12.1 High 16.7 TOTAL 13.9 (430) (524) (114) (151) (348) (324) (614) (243) (123) (477) (243) (123) (477) (153) (182) (315) (76) (34) (43) (133) (232) (195) (197) (228) (499) (66) (36) (874) *Upper third of the scale. 126 In general, it can be concluded that, in 1971 in Uruguay, compalining or not complaining about the economic situation was not a question of age, but rather of socio-economic status; when age seemed to have had an influence on the shaping of the responses, age was also correlated with the economic variable. The older were the more likely to be retired, the more likely to have low levels of education, the more likely to hold poorly paid positions. This conclusion may appear contradict our thesis that Uruguay is an age—stratified society. The contradiction is not real, however, That a society be stratified on the grounds of age does not require that all those at the higher age level would enjoy higher statuses. Age- stratification only means that seniority, rather than formal instruction, is the basis for upper mobility. This conclusion can be considered to have been verified by the findings. High levels of perceived recognition were found to a considerable extent among those older generation members who had high educational levels, while low scores of self perceived recognition appeared at the same educational level for the younger age group. Older people who did not complain about being sufficiently recognized and/or paid, were those affected by socio-economic circumstances likely to have prevented them from reaching high statuses. That age plays a role in the ascription of statuses in a society does not mean that economic differences no longer shape individual life-chances to a significant degree. With regard to the second aspect of the question, it was observed in this study that those groups which complained the most about the objective worsening of the situation were not the most likely to blame the establish- ment for the deterioration of their standards of living, or to advocate 127 social reform. The latter seemed to be a function of a subjective element, not necessarily related to the objective situation of economic deprivation. In the next section we shall explore the political behavior in our sample of Uruguayn voters, and try to ascertain whether the above discussion is also applicable to this topic. b) Behavioral Polarization On the grounds of the theoretical formulations in this paper, we anticipate that party preferences and party identifications will also appear as polarized in our survey. Party sympathies and voting are ultimate— ly the behavioral representation of the value orientations we have previous— ly dealt with. The expected relationships are as follows: H 6.1.— Left Party identification and Left vote will be maximum among the younger group, minimum among the older. H 6.2.— Conservative Party identification and Conservative vote will be maximum among the older group, minimum among the younger. Identification with a political party has been defined here as the extent to which respondents see themselves as partisans of that particular group in terms of long—lasting sympathies rather than exclusively in terms of their voting in the next election. Observed frequencies corroborated the expected relationships in this case, as shown in Table 51. The table shows the younger group 12.7% over the older, and 7.3% over the total population in Left identification. 128 04000 00400 00000 04000 Hz 0.00 0.0 0.00 0.00 0.00 0030:< 0.000 0.00 4.00 0.00 0.00 0.00 0000000002 4.0 0.4 0.00 0.00 0.00 0000 4.00 0.00 0 00 0.00 0.00 00000002 04.Nm w.mv mém NKN 01000 m>0p0>00m0ou 0<000 00 00>0 00-00 00-00 00-00 2000<000002000 000<0 00< -.mmm0u000000 -.005000 om< 00 00000000000000 00000 .00000000000 0m 00£0H 129 Similarly, the data show that the older group identifies with the Conservative Party to a higher extent than do other groups, whereas the younger generation shows, in this respect, the smallest percent in the table. In previous discussions, it has been underscored that, within the particular social context here considered, age and education are expected to have ”opposite” effects with respect to the promotion of change and status maintaining orientations. We therefore analyzed the above results controlling for education. The picture so obtained sheds light on the interplay between these two variables. In essence,the findings lend support for generational theory, as presented in this study: left partisanship tends to decrease when age increases at all levels of education. Furthermore, the percentages of left partisanship found among the younger generation tend to increase with education, while the oppsite is the case for the older group. Although some of the percentages in the table are based on small samples, discernible tendencies emerge from the information presented. (See Table 52) Percentages in Table 53 complete the picture: Conservative sympathies tend to increase with age for all educational levels. The ”ambivalent” effect of education is also shown in this case: as far as the subsamples in the table permit meaningful comparisons, the data show that when level of education increases, conservative partisanship goes down among the young, up among the older group. Voting data in this study were subject to analytic treatment similar to that reported above. General distributions of responses on 130 .00002 000 mommpqoohom 300:3 no maonasc 0:0 oumoflwcfl memosucohmm 0:0 :0 mohsmflm 0:9 00000 000 0 000 0 000 0 000 0 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.00 00000>0< 00000 000 0 000 0 000 0 000 0 0.00 0.0 0.00 0.0 0.00 000000000 00000 00000 00000 000 0 000 0 0.0 0.0 0.0 0.0 0.0 0000000000 .:z0o 00-00 00-00. 00-00 2000<0000 m0< u.mommu:oohom u.cofiuwosum paw om< kn :00000000pcowH Aphmm umoq mm oaamh 131 .0000: 000 mowwucooaom :00:3 :0 000:55c 0:0 00000::0 momm:u:000m 0:0 :0 mohzwfiw 0:9 00000 000 0 000 0 000 0 000 0 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.00 00000>0< 00000 000 0 000 0 000 0 000 0 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.00 000000000 00000 00000 00000 000 0 000 0 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.00 0000000000 0<000 00>0 000 00 00-00 00-00 00-00 2000<0000 mw< -.mommucoogom -.:ofiumo:©m was ow< x: :000000M0pCowH 00000 nowmhofiou0 o>wum>00mcou mm oHQMH 132 voting intention for all age groups are presented in Table 54. In general, these results follow the same pattern observed for party identification; the only difference being that the percentage of prospective left voters is considerably higher than the percentage of identifiers for that party. The explanation for this can be found in the fact that the Left party appeared for the first time in this election; it therefore would collect votes from individuals who would still indentify with other political groups. It is also likely that the left coalition, appearing by the first time as a real alternative to the "traditional” two parties of Uruguay, would have collected most of the "protest" vote that goes normal— ly into the opposition party in a two party system. Finally, since the Left party received strong support from young voters, it is also likely that some of these young partisans would not conceive of their support for the party in terms of a long-lasting sympathy. The ”ambivalent" effect of education can also be detected in Tables 55 and 56, where voting data have been cross-tabulated by age and education: left vote increases with education among the young, decreases with educa- tion among the old; conservative vote fluctuates in the opposite direction. Furthermore, at all educational levels, left vote decreases with age, while conservative vote increases with age. In essence, the observed relationships between age, party identifica- tion, and political conservatism, is similar to the findings reported by Campbell e£_al, (1960: 62) for the United States, and by Eysenck (1954222) with respect to Britain. In general, older people in all countries tend to develop stronger party identifications than do younger individuals; the former also tend to be more conservative than the latter. 13$ 00000 00000 00000 00000 00000 Hz 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.0 003000 0.000 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.00 000000000 0.0 0.0 0.0 0.0 - - 00000 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.00 0000 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.00 00000002 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.00 00000>000000 00000 00 00>0 00-00 00-00 00-00 000> 00020020 -.mommp:ooaom u.mm5000 0w< 0: :OHuCopsH wcwpo> 0m 00:05 134 .Uomw: 0H0 mommucoohom :00:3 co m00:E:: 0:0 000 mmmo:p:o0mm 0:0 :0 moazmflm 0:9 00000 000 0 000 0 0 000 000 0 0.00 0.0 0.00 0.00 0.00 00000>0< 00000 000 0 000 0 000 0 000 0 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.00 000000000 00000 00000 00000 000 0 000 0 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.00 0000000000 00000 00 0000 00-00% .00-00 00-00_ 200000000. .00<_ -.mmmwu:00000 z.cofluwo:vm 0:0 om< x: mpo> 0mm: mm ®H:m9 135 .0000@ 000 mommpc000wm 000:3 co mhmnfidc 0:0 000 0000:02000m 0:0 :0 mmgdwfim mcb 00000 000 0 000 0 000 0 000 0 0.00 0,00 0.00 0.00 0.00 00000000 00000 000 0 000 0 000 0 000 0 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.00 000000000 00000 00000 00000 000 0 000 0 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.00 0000000000 0<000 00 00>0 00000 00-00 00-00 200000000 000 n.00w00000000 u.:00pmo:©m 000 mm< 0n mpo> o>000>0mmcou om oHan 136 00000 00000 00000 00000 00000 Hz 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.00 000: m.wo m.00 m.wo. 0.00 w.wm 300 .IMo omuom mmnom mmuw0r wmmoum It; “I: .1 mu< u.momwucmo000 1.0m3000 ow< 00 mo0oum Emfl>flpo< 000000000 mm mfinmh db 136 2.- Self-Investment in Political Roles Not only are the younger generations more prone to change orientations and left voting, but they also show a greater extent of political activism as compared to the older group. Table 51 shows the distribution of political actigism scores for all age groups. Scores resulted from respondents being assigned one point for each affirmative answer to the following: (a) attending political meetings or rallies, (c) contributing money or work to a political party, (d) knowing personally one or more political leaders, (b) talking politics with relatives, (e) talking politics with co-workers, and, (f) knowing the number of votes needed by the President in order to be re-elected. The hypothesized relationship is as follows: H 7.l.— Political activism scores will be maximum among the younger group, minimum among the older. As shown in Table 56, when political activism scores are dichotomized into ”High" and ”Low" categories, inter-generational polarization becomes quite evident: the percentage of ”Low" scores for the younger generation is 10% below that for the general population, and 12.7% below the total percentage for the older age group. On the other hand, ”High" political activism scores are 10% higher among the younger group than they are for the general population. In addition to this, there is a 12.9% difference between the two extremes of the generational continuum,in the direction hypothesized. Cut-off points were modified to provide a more refined analysis of our data while dealing with political activism and education: 137 H 7.3.— Political activism scores will be maximum for the most educated, lowest for the least educated. Table 58 shows that 49.2% of the respondents with higher education are in the ”High" political activity category, as compared to only 31.1% of the total population, and 27.5% of those with elementary education. Similarly, 43.1% and 43.2% of all respondents with elementary and secondary education respectively, scored "Low” in political activism, as compared to only 23% of the most educated who also received low scores . 3.- Clientele-Oriented Partisanship Noted by Merton (1953) political parties in the Western democracies perform the task of redistributing social benefits among lower—middle and lower class individuals. Such a ”latent” function of the political machin— ery can be seen as a characteristic of an era of affluence. If we apply this concept to the case of Uruguay at two time periods, pre— and post— economic recession, it is not unreasonable to assume that political par— ties would have operated as favor-granting agencies during the times of prosperity, while no longer being able to perform those roles in the recent times because the goods that were allocated through this system had become increasingly scarce. If this is so, it is also likely that, at different times in the process, different patterns of political socialization would have developed. Therefore, in a synchronic study such as this, it should be possible to find the expression of those patterns through comparison of 138 00000 00000 00000 00000 Hz 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.00 000: 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.00 000002 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.00 300 00< .000wcooom 000000000m mmoum 200000000 n.00mmucmohoa .000>00 Hmcoflpmoswm 0n 00000m Em0>000< 000000000 mm oHQMH of the behavior of older and younger generations. We are here assuming that older people, especially if they belong in the lower or lower- middle classes, are likely to have made use of the favor~granting mechanisms of the political parties, while it is less likely that this would have happened among younger individuals. The matter was investigated by asking respondents if they had ever received a favor from someone in a political club. The hypothesis is as follows: H 3.1.— Clientele—Oriented Partisanship will be maximum among the older generation, minimum among the younger. Percentage distributions of the responses given to the above questions were cross tabulated by all socio—demographic categories. (See Table 59) As we anticipated, the highest percentages of affirmative responses in the table are for: (a) the older age-group, (b) the lower social classes, (c) the lower income groups, (d) the lower oc- cupational strata, and, (e) conservative and moderate voters. Percentage distributions of responses to the above question were also cross—tabulated by educational levels, as shown in Table 60. It should be noted that: first, for all educational categories, the percentage of respondents ackowledging that they have received at least one favor from someone in a political club, tends to increase wih age; it is also noteworthy that, for all educational levels, the percentage of affirmative responses given by younger interviewees are very small. In fact, all these percentages fall within the sampling error of this survey. 140 Table 59 Respondents Having Received a Favor from Someone in a Political Club by Socio-Demographic Characteristics.- Percentages.— TYPE OF FAVOR Important Not Important N SEX: Male 5.1 5.1 (430) Female 4.6 3.6 (524) AGE: 18-25 0.9 2.6 (114) 26—35 4.0 3.3 (151) 36-50 5.5 4.9 (348) Over 50 6.2 4.3 (324) EDUCATION: Elementary 6.0 4.2 (614) Secondary 1.6 4.1 (243) Advanced 2.9 6.5 (123) SOC. CLASS: Lower 6.7 4.2 (477) Low-Mid 3.0 3.5 (402) Up-Mid/Upper 3.4 3.4 (68) OCCUPATION: Unemployed 0.4 5.2 (77) Retired 7.2 2.6 (177) Blue Collar W. 4.9 5.5 (182) White Collar W 3.8 5.1 (315) Small Business 3.9 2.6 (76) Big Business 2.9 2.9 (34) Prof., etc. 4.7 2.3 (43) PARTY PREF. Undecided 4.5 2.3 (133) Colorado 7.8 6.0 (232) Blanco 6.2 6.7 (195) Left 3.0 3.6 (197) INCOME: Low 6.7 3.3 (240) Med-Low 5.2 5.0 (522) Med-High 2.9 5.7 (70) High 2.8 2.8 (36) TOTAL 4.7 4.2 (980) 141 Amway flow 8 Ammv fimm U Ava v "z m.o I I o.w o.w m.v pcmunomefl uoz He mg fine 94 ma B8BS§J o.ww m.mm o.vw o.ww o.mm ”xzm po>amoo~ Ho>oz "Umocm>w< “mama has V Asa V Aom v Ame v "z N.H H.N N.N I I - II pcwppomafl poz m.m m.¢ H.H w.H I - pampnomeH N.am v.0w m.mm v.6m o.oofi saw co>flmomp nm>oz mNmmmmmmmm fleaov fifimmv Ammmv non U mmm v "z N.v m.e w.e m.m o.v pawpn0defl uoz 0.0 v.0 m.n n.m I-I- pamphomEH n.mn n.mw N.mn m.vn o.wo xcm po>flooon no>mz ”xnmucoEon qo om-cm mm-om mNIwH mo>9 onhmm vo>flooom 0:3 mucowcommom ow manmk 142 Political participation by each of our indicators, attending ral- lies, contributing money or work to parties, and talking politics, is more characteristic of the younger group than it is of the older; on the other hand, as we have just seen, the experience of having had some direct contact with, or having received a favor from political leaders in a club, was more common for older people than it was for the young. 4.- Primary Group Identification The question of family influence on party preferences and political views is among the most researched topics in political sociology. Social theorists such as C.H. Cooley (1909) advanced the view that primary groups form a bridge between individuals and the ”greater" society; this position has been later buttressed by an impressive body of data. (Shils, 1951; Katz §£_313 1955; Hyman, 1959). In addition to the value homogeneity of primary groups in a general rule intra-family cohesion is supposedly enhanced in the partic- ular case of Uruguay because of the importance of the family as a mediating agency for the individual's finding a job and, more generally, for his social mobility.(Solari, 1965; Weil, 1971; Instituto de Ciencias Sociales, 1964) We have hypothesized that inter—generational relations were deteriorating in Uruguay at the time this survey was taken, then the survey would detect differences in the extent to which members of different generations agree or disagree with other family members. 143 For operational purposes, a family (in this case equated to primary group) was defined as a group of relatives who live in the same household on a permanent basis. Interviewees were, therefore, asked whether their political views were shared by either all, a few, or none of the relatives wiht whom they lived together. The hypothesis is as follows: H 4.1.- In households, older family members are more likely to agree with some, or all relatives they live with than will the younger cohort. The distribution of responses for this variable tabulated by age groups is presented in Table 61. The table shows that for respondents aged 51 and over, 76. % shared their views with everyone in the same family, while only 64.1% of the younger group gave the same response. Of the younger group, 23.3% declared that only a few of the relatives they lived with shared their political views, as compared with only 18.2% of the older generation who gave the same answer. Finally, the percentage of respondents whose views were not shared by anyone else in the household were 12.6% and 5.1% for the younger and the older generations respectively. It is interesting to verify if the ”ambivalent" effect of education Operates also at this level of intergenerational relations. Table 62 presents the data on family homogeneity, cross-tabulated by age, and educational levels. The data show that education does appear to moderate the intergenerational differences in this respect. When the percentage of those agreeing with their entire families are subtracted from the percentage of those who disagreed with everyone in the same 144 Aamav mmsmv flammv Amway Amend "z ”.5 h.w N.w 0.x o.NH ocoz a.aH N.wH H.©H s.mH m.mm 3am < w.Ns 5.6a w.ms N.Hh H.40 Hfia qo omIom mm-om mm-mH unaccommoh gun: oowwa 0:3 m0>fiPdH®H W0 .Hmn—ESZ -.mowmucounom I.mm:ono ow< kn :ofiumofiwflpcouH macaw xhwaflsm Ho oHan .pcoEoogwm machm xgwaflnm :uocz mo ogoom m ma masogomwflv mcflufizwoa och .:oohw< HH<: Eogm :ocoz\3om <: wcfluomHuLSm an woodmaoo one moocopommflq 14S H.mv n.0m o.ac 0.0m m.mm .mmflo “ONHV mom V flmN V Amm V flee V "2 e.am w.om o.m o.ON H.4m mcoz\3ma a m.so m.Ho o.Nn o.on o.mo HH< ”emocm>e< w.wm H.am N.mm 0.4m 5.0N .mmflo AmmmV has V mam V flow V “ma V I2 a.Hm m.mN N.mm m.om H.Hm ocoz\3ma < m.oo c.mm 4.00 m.qo m.am HH< ”sameqooow v.mm m.sa o.Ha V.Hm o.oH .mmno flwsmV flHmNV AmmmV nos V Amm V I2 m.sH 0.0H m.mH o.om o.Nm ocoz\3oa < m.nm m.mo w.oo v.Hm o.wv HH< ”xnmucosofim q m.ezmozoammm mmo 0:2 mm>HHfiq mo>fluwfiom Aufls psoEoonw< mo psouxm .mpcmvcommom ”coauNUAMAHCovH adouu AHmEflHm proanmb,- 146 house, or just agreed with a few family members, the resulting dif- ferences tend to increase with age for the elementary and secondary levels of education, while at advanced level the scores remain stable. It should be noted, however, that the advanced education samples are small size; we cannot draw firm conclusions from this evidence alone. As a conservative conclusion, it appears that intra-family conflict (which is the Opposite of primary group identification) tends to decrease with age, independent of education. The evidence above testifies to the existence of marked inter- generational differences on the matters of voting, party identification, political activism, and primary group identification. On all these mat- ters, a clear polarization of the two extreme generations emerges from the data; indeed, education contributes to the effect of age, but does not affect the directionality of it, as was observed earlier for political opinions and attitudes. Across generations, indeed, these data show that behavioral differences are more clearly delineated than are differences in value orientations. c) The Control Variables In research such as this, it is customary that control operations be performed to rule out the possibility that influences other than the independent variable would be responsible for the observed effect. In our case, we are considerably limited in the number and types of control operations available, due to the relatively small size of the sample. This circumstance, unfortunately, rules out the possibility of doing defensible multiple control analysis. As an alternative, I have choosen 147 to cross—tabulate Voting and Party Identification by Age-Groups, controlling by Income and by Social Class respectively. Both variables are usually dealt with in political behavior research. As shown in Table 63, the hypothesized relationship between age and conservative vote exists at practically all income levels. The percentage distributions in Table 64 show that occupation has little or no effect on voting behavior, except for some occupational groups which are clearly related to the age structure, like the retired, or the professionals. In fact, the data in Table 62 suggest that the relationship between working class membership and Left sympathies which has been observed in other cultures (Lipset, 1960; Doggan, 1968) does not hold true in Uruguay. Uruguayan left groups draw considerable support from white collar workers and professionals. Rather than confirming the assumption of working class leftism, our data are more congruent with the opinion voiced by Allardt (1970: 49) who envisioned these forms of dissent as originating from relatively deprived middle classes. Although the evidence presented in our control tables is far from conclusive to the end of ruling out the existence of all possible spurious variables, it is plausible to assume that no other relevant variable among those usually considered in political behavior research seems to have as powerful an effect as do age and education in the determination of political attitudes. 148 AOOHV Afim V ”ma V hmfi V mm V N.mm w.mm o.mm m.mm m.NH emu: ANNmV novfiV ANHNV “mm V hon V N.wm o.mm 4.AN o.mm A.mm sages: hoeNV mANHV Ame V flmm V hmm V N.om m.mm N.NN o.NH o.mH 304 ao omIom mMISN mN-wH mzoqu mo< -.momwu:mohom I.wHo>oq mEoocH was om< kn ouo> m>fipm>Homcou mo ofinwe 149 nome “m4 V flaw V hos V AmHmV flNwHV flmmHV Asa V "2 H.mH m.m m.m m.s m.m m.m w.a m.o Hozmc< p_:oo o.om o.wH o.om m.om m.mm n.4N m.oH m.aH eoefiuouc: a.m o.mN w.» m.m H.NH N.w w.a H.m “was 4.NN m.oH a.mm v.NN H.4N m.ms w.oN m.mm ouasoeoz v.Nm N.om m.mm N.wm m.om m.am N.H4 H.mm o>flpm>wmmcoo I m m m. Mm mm a w m .- g m mm. w; u. a V c n T X. e I I T S e 3 I D a o a I. m m m. s m p. M n m m. m H D. oneHmfipm>gmmcou sEmflpw>Homcou q QmQZmHzH mm3 coaucoch wcfluo> co ofipme 154 bottom on the pay level and recogintion level scales, and perceiving one's standard of living as having drastically deteriorated (to the extent that the present standard of living approximates the "worst conceivable one”). Comprable scores of these dimensions for each group can be developed by standardizing percentages. ”Standardize" is here meant to indicate subtracting the ”total” percentage of responses in one dimension, from the percentages found in the same row for each age- group. The resulting scores are indicative of each age-group's situation relative to the total popoulation. If this process is carried for each group, and a composite score then computed, we have a group index of deprivation feelings (or of economic dimensions) suitable for inter—group comparison. By the same token, it is possible to compute scores of Left vote and alienation for each group which, if combined into one single indicator will produce a score of ”protest”. We can subsequently compare the ”economic" scores for each group versus the ”political” scores. Results for these operations are presented in Tables 67-69. The tables show no correspondence for the two indexes, except in some manner for the 36-50 years group. The most impressive aspect of the data is that both extreme generations show index configura— tions opposite to each other: the younger generation scores very high in political protest dimensions, while its ”economic” deprivation score is the lowest in the table. On the other hand, the older group shows one of the lowest scores in the table with respect to ”protest” (political) scores together with the highest deprivation (”economic") score. 155 .ommpCoUAmm m.>sowoumo sumo Scam QMMpcmogmm :HmuOH: mcfluomnpnzm An wouSQEou* H.wfi+ a.NI a.o- 4.mfi- flofisocoomV v+m+m 0.4- N.4- m.m+ N.ON+ flfimofiufifioaV mIH m.oH+ H.A- N.m+ w.4+ meHmoazoo m.H+ m.H- N.m+ m.m+ coflpmcoflfi< -.m w.H+ H.H- 4.N+ o.N- cofipmgoflpmumo can: -.4 m.m+ m.HI H.H- o.m- :ofiuflcmouom 30g -.m w.oH+ m.o- o.w- w.a- Ana-303 -.N N.m- A.N- 5.0+ m.oH+ ouo> paws -.H amoHoom npmmpoan\mw:mcu floan flaNmV flwvmV flHmHV AVHHV "z m.HN m.NN w.mH m.am c.4m :ofipacofiu< I.m m.mH m.HN v.wH a.HN m.AH :oflumgoflwmpma awn: -.s 0.5H m.mm m.mH m.mH 4.HH canpficmooom 304 I.m H.He m.Hm w.oa H.mm m.mm Ana-30s -.N H.mH m.mH 4.0H w.mm o.om opo> “was -.H qo om-om mmISN mN-wH onmzmzHo mw< I.mowmucmosom I.mmzosw om< an mcoflmcoafio humouosmV omcmgu no ofinwh 156 HmcofimeSUUo numb Eonm scamcoeflw code now emancmohom :HmHOH: .oMMocoonom m.xyowopmo mcfluowwunzm an woodmaoo mopoom 0.0 H.om- c.0H- A.w- m.m+ m.mN+ H.Hv+ a.m4+ me+m+NV :ofieocoom: 0.0 w.nm+ o.m- N.NI m.v+ 0.0+ 0.0m- m.n- mm+HV :Hmoflufifiom: o.o H.m- o.vN- m.oHI 0.0+ n.0m+ m.na+ m.vm+ mHHmomzou o.o v.o- n.oI N.oI o.m+ N.H+ H.0H- m.m- cofiumcmfifi< -.m 0.0 m.mI H.H+ v.m- o.N- m.o+ 0.0+ o.mH+ soapmHOHHoooQ cwflm -.v 0.0 o.oHI N.w- w.m- m.m- m.m+ w.n+ N.¢H+ coauflcwooom 304 -.m 0.0 N.NHI m.w- m.N- 0.0 + o.mH+ n.0m+ N.mH+ xmm 304 -.N 0.0 v.nm+ m.n- o.N- m.m+ m.N + H.v- N.NI ouo> “we; -.H d 8 S M M M 8 H R .IV I T_. m 0 U- 0 T. 9 u 0 o 8 e x r. 1 n 1 a m .1. a n am. Me n. .w "I. t n l I D 9 I S Cu. 5 D S O p O m m. m N H M onmzmzHo o . I. I a P . u 9 l . 1 onH upon -.H .1 d H S M M M an H H O l I. m 0 U. 0 T. 9 u l 0 an E l I. I n n. m wlv. T: on U. a m“ wed 9 1 d . n I m m m. m. onmzmzHo e m. m. s m. I x a 3 u S T. I 9 O . I. T. B D- . u E I . l one