APPROACH LEAMENG AND EXTINCTION AS A FUNCTION OF AVOEDING PREDATORY ATTACKS Thesis far ”to Dwm of Hi. D. MECBEGAN STATE UNIVERSITY Joseph Weldon Jennings, Jr. 1985 TH ES‘S L I B R A R Y Michigan Saw University APPROACH Lilo-1531.340 A33.) EXTIEUTIOS AS A FURCTIOH OF A‘JOIDIHG PREDAE’ORY ATTACKS by Josoph Holden Jonnings, Jr. Thin thooio prooonto o nodol tor opproaoh loarning and oxtinction in pro: organiono ao o function 01 ropootod prodotony ottooto, o nothod to: tooting tho letl. one data thot boar on tho nodol. Tho loorning,lodo1 involvod tho osounption that tho haste toot oontronting tho proy otganisn to to loorn to o)prooeh o rosion of tho ontironnont to obtain noodod rein- !oroo-ont (orator) and than tloo this am rogion to avoid tho diroot phyoieal attack of an approaching prodator. Tho rooponoo of approaching the goal. it wao arguod. 1o condi— tions! to tho otilnlt accompanying the onotional rooponnoo of too: and trnotrotion. root is olioitod by the operation of tho nrOGotor. Attor a sufficient nunbor of opproacheo to tho goal rogion, toorwprodunoé stimuli oorvo as partial aiacrininotivo ottnnli olioiting tho opproooh responoo. Frustration to olioitod by tho tormination of pooitivo rotatoroonont noooooitotod by tho prey loaving tho souroo a! roiltoroulont tn ordor to avoid tho predator's attack. An Joseph Weldon Jennings, Jr. with fear, after a sufficient number of approaches to the goal region, frustrationuproduoed stimuli serve as partial discriminative stimuli eliciting the approach roeponee. Two hypotheses were derived. Both concerned reeietanco to extinction of the approach reeponee. The first hypothesis predicted that prey without prior experience in approoching the reinforced region under threat of predatory attack would chow lees resistance to extinction than had they had such prior experience. The eecond hypothesis predicted that prey allowed less time in the goal region (3 eeconde) prior to the onset of predatory attack would chow more resistance to extinction of the approach response compared to prey having more time in tho goal region (11 seconds) prior to predatory attack. Both hypotheeee were supported by the reeults. The apparatus ueed in the three experimente involved o safe box in which the prey (female, hooded rat) was safe from any attack. This cote box was connected with the goal rosion by e 3 foot onelooed alloy. The goal region was actually a three feet continuation of the alloy with water available Just within the region. At the farthest end ct tho goal region wee the predator. The predator was a 4 inch aluninun dieo carrying 11,000 volt. .001 ampere charge on its curtace. The electrical charge wee produced by an automobile ignition coil. This mechanical predator was Joseph Weldon Jennings, Jr. prepelled the length of the goal region and alley in pursuit of the prey. I: the prey allowed the mechanical predator within 1/8 inch of itself it received a shock. The prey would continue to be shocked until it moved faster than the predator on the way towards the safe box. Each subject (prey) was run individually once a day for ten minutes. During a session. various measures of the subject's behavior were taken. These measures included: number of traversale between the safe box and the goal region; the tics spent in the safe box. alley and goal region during a session; the number of shocks received; and slount or water consumed. Suggestions were made for the extension of this work to further laboratory analysis or prey~predator situations and to field work in this area. ‘i? /7 7» _ ’ /4' ", /(\ Approvefi i:,,- met“: ( ( gm“ dormittee Chairman / Date & ; egg/51!. [75, ” 7 r_ I a I 0 V x \ -' \. p U“ -mo..~r~;.-*M‘--~M‘.. ‘v‘ ’h“m.‘ ' - 'I :0 0 ’v ‘ .“f ' . my; c" i - ’ 1,.1‘. «hm muafiu ..~- E. NQZ‘ gm‘& .- w . APPROACH LEARSIKG AED EXTIBCTION AS A FUHCTIOH OF AVOIDIEG PfiLDATOHY ATTACKS By Joaeph Weldon Jennings, Jr. A THESIS Submitted to Michigan State University in partial tuliillmen of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOS §HY Department of Psychology 1965 In more ways than I can recount, this thesis was made posaiblo by a: wife's unatinting love. Carolyn 10 the author of my happiness and achievements. ii AthOuLhflGamfith The author acknowledges the great assistance given bun by his major professor. ML Ray Dannyy His wise counsel and encouragement helped make this study pooaibla. I also owe a debt of thanks to the other members of my guidance committoes Doctors Stanley C. Ratnor and William Stellwagon of the Department of Psychology and Doctor John A. King of the Department of Zoology. I also wioh to thank Mr. Royal Olson for his suggestions and help in building the apparatua and Miss Jean Mcflartin for her many patient hours spent in transcribing data. 111 TABLE OF COfiTEfiTS ACKEOHLEDGEHSfiTS . . . . 0 LIST OF TABLiS . . . . . . LIST OF FIGURES . . . . . . IEQROD CTICE o I o o a o o FTUStrfltiOno o o o a 3 Fear .0... 0.... Fear and Frustration Campos Predator 0 o I o o o o Reoiotauco to Extinction General Considerations Hypotheses o c o o o o EXPERIHEKT I o o o o o o o mathOd o o 6 u o o o 0 Results and Liaouooion EXPELLIFELLQT II o a o o o a. o Methadooooooao 0 Results and Discussion . EXPERIEEgT III. 0 o o o I o Hethofl Q a a o~o o o 0 Results and Biscuoaion . CQEJCLUSIOIES A313 SUM‘iAh‘Y . . “V“?DWH‘V” hufiunénc&u . o o o n o o o AP‘EEDICEfi o o o o o o o 0 1v 0 0.. 3d. O o O O Page iii vii 9 12 15 18 20 21 Q1 59 55 56 61 73 73 76 ‘0' K." LC: U1 Tohlca I. Illumination, in foot candlco, of the £1003 Of the a???‘rfltg8 7,. ,. o o o I ‘C O 0: II. Boise lovelo, in decibolo,.recorded with the Boise Level Meter microphone beside the drinking tUbG . o o o o 0}. 0‘. 0 III. Kciee levolo, in decibels, recorded with 30156 Level Meter microPhone in the miédle Of thfi safe box I o o o o o o o 0 IV. Humericel key to the various-componente of the experimental apparatus shown in Figure I. o o o o o o o o o o o o o o 0 V. Analysis of Variance of response rates aorooe sessions of Phase 2 versus Phase 4 (Experiment I). . . . . . . . . . . . VI. Mean water consumption (ml.) per session of rhaoea 1 and 3 of Experiment I. . . . VII. Frequency of shocks received per session of the four phases of Experiment I. . . VIII. Group composition in termo of the mean reoponoo rate across Phase 1 (Experiment II). C D O C O O O 0 O O U 0 0'. 0 II. Analysis of Variance of Group A veroue Group B response rate across Phase 2 (EIperimont II). o o I o o o o o o o o o X. Mean water consumption (ml.) by $3 of Groups A and B during Phaeee I and 3 of EXPeriment II. 0 O o o o o o o o o o o a II. Frequency of shocks received by fig of Groups A and B during the four Phases of Experiment 11. o o o o o o o o o o o o 0 III. Group composition in terms of mean mac or me To roe once rate across Phase 1 (Experiment 111?. O O O O I O O O O O O O O I I 33 39 54 54 58 61 71 72 75 LIST OF TABLES (cont.) rail! XIII. XIV. moan water ooneumption by g3 of Group C and D during Plaoe l and 3 of EXperlmOntIIIoboocoooooooo Frequency of shocks received by.§g of Groupe C and D during the four Phoeee OffiPB-‘fi'mentlllootoot-ace... vi as 86 Figure I. II. III. 1V3. IVb. V. VI. VIIa. VIIb. VIII. IX. I. II. LIST 0? FIGURES Isometric drawing of the apparatus . . . . 32 leomotrio drawing of the mechanical predator o o o o o 0 mean response rate per ooeeion of the four pheeee of EXPeriment I. . o . . . . . 50 Mean total time spent in the safe box, alley, and goal region per session of Phaeea l and 2 of Experiment I. . . . . . 51 Mean total time opent in the safe box, alley, and goal region per session of Phases 3 and 4 of Experiment I. o . . . . 52 mean time in the goal region per response per eeeeion of the four phases of Ekperiment It .0 o a o o o o o o o o o o o 5) Heen response rate per session of the four phases of EXperiment II. . . . . . . 67 Mean total time spent in the safe box, alley, and goal region per eeesion or Phases 1 and 2 of Experiment II. . . . . 68 Mean total time spent in the safe box, alley, and goal region per eeeeion of Pheeea 3 and 4 of Experiment II. . . . . 69 Mean time in the goal region per response per session of the four phases of Experiment II. o o o o o o o o o o o o o 0 7O Keen reoponee rate per oeeeion or the four phases of Experiment III. . . . . . 82 mean total time spent in the safe box, alley, and goal region per eeeeion of the four phases of Experiment III. . . . . . . . . 83 Kean time in the goal region per responee per eeeaion of the four pheeee of EXPGIiment III. o o o o o o o o o o o o o 84 vii r This roooaroh deals with possible acquired ohongoo in the behavior of prey organisms under repeated oxpoouro to attack by a predator. A body or literature exists on the lubjoot of prey reactions to predators but it has little relationship to acquired changoo, particularly under con- trolled conditions. There is a lack of data for oovoral roaoonl.‘ On. in that most observations which have any boating on the topic havo boon made in the field. that 19, undo: rolativoly uncontrolled conditions (Seton. 1355; Rudiger. 19503 Elton, 1953). A130, thooo studios have otton involved opooioa-apooifio response pattopno (Simmons, 1935: Molzach, 1961:.Bindo, 1961: Curti, 1935). Further, in many of those studioo tho past history of the organism was unknown and coulo not Q. controlled (Griffith, 1360} Richard- son. 1942; Joalln, 1964): What 13 wanted 19 a situation in which 1) tho onvironnontol and sequential oopooto of the situation one known and controllable; 2) the oomo animal can to obaorvod nepoatodly. Since the ncoo £13k control is paramount, an apparotuo was dovelupod that was assumed to be on anologuo of a oituatlon in which a predator ooulfi confront a prey. The pilot opporatua as first devoIOpod involved, as does the inal apparatus, throo operationally dlotinct orooo. Theoo I were 1) the “safe box', analogous to the prey'e lair, which consisted of a 5' x 12' high box. The prey (hooded rat)- had eeey exit and entrance to this ”safe box' via a 3' hole near the floor. 2) This hole led directly into e 6 foot long alloy 4' wide and 12' high (the ”unsafe region"). 3) In the middle of the alloy (3 ft. from either end) there was a dish containing approximately one ounce of water (the goal region). Thee the rat had to traverse three feet of alloy to reach the goal region. .At the farthest point from the 'eefe box", there was a 3%” hollow steel sphere oerrying a high tension (10,000 volts), very low amperage charge on its surface. This sphere was suspended by on are from a movable platform outside the alley which could be propelled the length of the alley up to i inch of the "safe box“ door. This sphere. its high tension charge, and ite movement were eegggpg to be a nechenioel analogue of a predator. This apparatus defines the task for the water deprived rat, which was to learn to obtain water by moving from the “safe box“ to the water dish and at the same time elude the nechsnicel predator. After a brief delay following gig entry into the goal. the mechanical predator begins to move down the elley. I: the rat fails to return to the ”safe box” in sat:- 101¢at time and allows the mechanical predator to come l/B inch it receives I punishing shook. It then continues to be pursued and possibly shocked until it reenters the 'eete box”. One might Justifisbly ask why the presentation of apparatus details at this point. It is necessary for the reader to appreciate these details in order to understand the nature of the problem. The pilot work demonstrated that rats learned to cape with the conditions imposed by the apparatus as evidenced by an incremental increase in response rate (trevereele between the “safe box“ and the goal) while at the same thus swiftly achieving an active avoidance of the mechanical predator at a probability in excess of 98%. After an asymptotic response rate was achieved, the primary reinforcement (water) was removed in order to observe the rate of extinction. This was done for more than curiosity's sake. There was reason to believe that the animal might show greater resistance to extinction than a group of animals which had equal access to the water but had not learned to cope with the predator. Pilot work suggested that this was a valid expectation under the conditions imposed by the apparatus. l;§g1;§£$gn. The role of frustration and frustration pro- duced stimuli in resistance to extinction will be considered first. under the conditions imposed by the apparatus, trus- tretion is present during both acquisition and extinction. During the acquisition the rat must leave the goal to escape or avoid the mechanical predator. Leaving the goal region results in the termination or withdrawal of rein- forcement. feroter (1957. 1958) found that stimuli signaling I"time out“ from positive reinforcement acquire aversive properties. He also showed that the withdrawal of a positive conditioned reinforcer had the functional prepertiee of a negative reinforcer, 1.0., suppressing the rate of respond- ing. Therefore. the rat is assumed to be frustrated each time it must leave the goal at the approach of the mechanical predator. ' On each trial, then. the rat will respond by being frustrated. We might. therefore, expect that stimuli of the goal region will become cues capable of eliciting frus- tration response in the rat as it approaches the goal. Thus, after repeated trials, the rat anydevelop a conditioned anticipatory frustrations responses to the approaching goal cues. But. through repeated approach responding. the stimuli produced by the anticipatory frustration response should become partial discriminative stimuli for the goal approach response. In other words, at least late in acquisi- tion training, the stimuli generated by anticipatory frustra- tion will become cues eliciting continued approach to the goal. Therefore. when primary reinforcement is discontinued in extinction. rats which have learned to approach the goal in reeponee to frustration produced ones will be acre rueietent to extinction than to rate which have not been frustrated in their ettenpte to obtain water. Another he: to eay ell this ie no follcues Honreinforce- lent of the approach response during.extinctiou elicite frustration reeponee, but, it frustration-produced etimuli here already become conditioned elicitore of the approach response. then the frustration engendered by noureinforcement is leee effective in eliciting reepoueee incompatible with continued approach. In other words, because frustration produced internal stimuli occur in both acquisition and extinction. the two conditione are less diecriuinable to the rat than had it not orpericnced frustration during acquisi- tiou. Such arguments as the preceding have been put forward by imael (1958. 1962) and Spence (1360) to explain how partial reinforcement producee result in an increased resis- tance to extinction in instrumental reward situations. These workers have pointed out that the intensity of frustration elicited by noureinforcement should increase gradually during acquisition ea anticipatory reward increases. Under such conditions, g; are loco likely to show a passive avoidance response to alley and goal cues during extinction compared to §giwhich have not had such training. Therefore, the termin- ation of reinforcement which results as the animal avoids the lwchhnical predator is viewed as having the some effect as partial reinforcement. That ie, the intensity of true- traticn elicited by withdrawal of reinforcement should increase gradually during acquieition ac anticipatory reward increases. Assuming that chaining back also occura, then §§.will be trained such that anticipatory frustration will elicit reeponoeo which are compatible with the instrumental roe- pence. Thus. the internal emotional response of frustration elicited during extinction should result in greater reeie- tauoe to extinction than would be seen in animale not originally trained to approach while at the eaue time being frultrated. £325. The mechanical predator repreeente a source of punieh— rent it not successfully eluded. Therefore, any persistent reeponding on the part of the rat during extinction appears to be ueaochietic, as its behavior would eeeu to court disaster. handler (1964) hoe reviewed the problem of naeo~ ohien and notes an.uopubliohed observation by HowrerClQSO). A rat was trained to run an alley to escape an electrified alley; otter training, the check was aduinietered g£;;_in a small area Just prior to the eecape box. the reeulte indicated that keeping this email region electrified resulted in increased reeietanoe to extinction even though the rat could have paeeively avoided any further ehoek by not running the alley. Evian (1949) found that rate trained like Movror'e would run the alley taster and display greater resistance to extinction than control rats that were not shocked during extinction. Brown, Martin and Morrow (1964) were able to accentuate the effects reported by Howrer and Gwinn. In two experi- ments, rats were trained to escape shocks in a starting box and alloy by running down the alley into an uncharged gocl box. During extinction, shock was no longer administered in the start box, but some groups received shock in part or all of the alley. Control rats were not shocked during extinction. In the first eXperiment attempted, the control and experimental groups performed alike, that is the experi- mental animals were not more resistent to extinction for being shocked during extinction. In the second experiment they changed the procedure so that the magnitude of extinction- shock and the number of escape trials were reduced. This was done to make the transition from acquisition to extinction less discriminsble. Under these conditions the animals shocked during extinction took longer to extinguish than non- shocked control animals. Azrin and Hols (Asrin, 1959. 1960; Azrin and H012, 19613 Hols and Asrin, 1961) did a series of studies which have even greater significance for the present research. Their procedure was such that a positively reinforced response engendered punishment. The method involved the pigeon's upecking response in a key peck Skinner box. fiftcr the birds reaponéiug was shaped and food reinforced on various cohe- dulea, shock of varying intensities and durations was administered as a second and acditional contingency to key pecking. The data from Azrin (1360) indicates that pigeons will deliver checks to themselves several hundred times to receive intermittent food rewards. Although several varia— tions combine to produce this behavior, the results can be interprcted ac inflicating that shook under these conciticns does little to interfere with typical intermittently re;n- forced rcaponéing. H012 and Azrin (1961) report stronger evidence that pigeons may increase their respcnce rates as a function of punishment alone. The subjectc were run under two daily conditions. In one, the response contingency was a VI food reinforcement and CR! chock. The second involved extinction without the reintcrcement~chock contingency. Assume that punishment or cues accocicted with punish— ment elicit an internal rccyonse. The label for this internal reagonse will be 'fcar'. The internal response of fear is assumed to be involved in increased resistance to extinction of the approach recponce by the prey organism for the some reasons as were true for frustration. Namely. 1.) During acquisition, roar produced ctimuli become ccn~ diticncd clicitorc for the approach response. 2.) Fear exists in both the acquisition and extinction conditions, as do its attendent stimuli. 3.) Because of the presence of fear produced stimuli in both acquisition and extinction conditions, the two conditions are less discriminable for the prey animal than for an animal which has not eXperienced fear during acquisition. gear and Frustration Cemented.~ A study by Brown and Wagner (1964) provides support for the functional similarity of ”fear“ and frustration. Three groups of rats were trained in‘a simple runuay. During acquisition. Group l was exposed to nonreintorcement on a 50% reward schedule. Group 2 was exposed to gradually increasing punishment along with con- eistent food reward. Group 3 was never punished or non- reintorced. Half of each group was then tested for the decremental effecte of either consistent nonreiniorcement or consistent punishment. Group 1 and 2 fig were more resistant than Group 3 fig not only to the decremental variable which they had been trained on, i.e., punishment or nonreinforcement, but also to the alternate toot variable. These results were interpreted in support of a commonality between the emotional consequences of punishment and nonreinfcroement. The results showed that responding for food and shock was greater than for neither. More importantly, the behavior was maintained even when the food reinforcement wee withdrawn from the first condition. In fact, delivering shock alone during the 10 second or extinction condition resulted in an increased rate of responding. To quote Hole and Asrin (1961), ”These experiments demonstrate that a relatively severe punishment can increase responding...Thie procedure o1 selectively pairing a stimulus with a reinforcer is the usual procedure for estab- lishing a discrimination. This discriminative property that the punishment acquired produced the apparent anomaly. Indeed. the discriminative property came to exert an even greater effect on responding than did the aversive property." (p. 231) a quote tron.3rown and eagner (1964) serves to clarify the cannon effects of "fear“ and frustration on resistance to extinction. ”If there is more than a conceptual simil- arity between the emotional responses of fear and antici— patory frustration, it would be reasonable to expect some degree of transfer of behaviors learned in the presence of one to occasions when the other ie aroused. Thus it might be expected that §g_whioh have learned to approach in the presence at anticipatory frustration would also persist in approaching in the presence of rear. Likewise §§_trained to approach in the presence or fear might be expected to continue to approach in the presence of anticipatory trus- tration. In this context, the present findings of a partial tranator between the learned resistances to punishment and 11 extinction would argue for a degree of commonality between the two emotional responses." (p. 507) The reasons for a I'partial transfer” rather than a complete transfer observed by Brown and Wagner can be clarified by Carlsmith‘s (1961, reviewed by Church 1963) study. Carlsmith found that the mean number of trials to a criterion of extinction was uninfluenced by the conditions of punishment (shock or loud sound) but there was a large and significant interaction. If the same aversive stimulus was used as a punishment that was used as s U03 for avoidance training, resistance to extinction was much greater than if the other aversive stimulus was used as punishment. This was interpreted as supporting a discrimination hypothesis. Thus facilitation may occur in cases of punishmnnt of negative instrumental acts because of a reinstatement of specific stimuli present earlier in training. Therefore. we may View the effects of the type of situation proposed for study as involving the simultaneous conditioning of the internal responses of fear and frustra~ tion. Both internal responses appear to work in concert to facilitate resistance to extinction since the animal has been trained such that the anticipatory responses of fear and frustration elicit responses compatahle to continued approach to the goal. Also, since the anticipatory responses should be developing a pace under the some external stimulus 12 conditions, anticipatory responses of fear and frustration should be elicited by the some external stimuli. Thus the situation confronting the rat or other enihel can be viewed as involving fear and fruotration as internal responeee. After sufficient eXperience stimuli concomitant with these responses come to elicit responeee compatable with approach to the goal region. Therefore. resistance to extinction, should be greater in animal. which have had the experience at learning to cope with the repeated atteoke of a predator compared to those which have not had to learn to cope with a predator. Predator. To say that the prey muet learn to ccpe with the predator, is to say that the prey learns to deal effect- ively with the actions of the predator. The predator'l action towards the prey ie labelled "threat". The predator can be conceived of as representing two types of threat to the prey. One is potential, the other actual. ihet is, the animal can behave differentially depending upon behavior of the predator. The animal must move towards the mechanical predator ot obtain reinforcements. It is therefore exposing itself to potential threat. The potential aspect refers to the fact that the mechanical preaator is not waiting beoide the water source to spring instantly upon the rat. It io at a distance from the rat and begins to approach the rat only after the rat hue arrived 13 at the water source. Once the mechanical predator is in motion the threat is termed “actual" rathsr_than potential. The above View of the dual mode of predator action finds support in Keehn's (1959) work. Keehn's study involved an avoidance situation (rats were held in an electrifiablc activity wheel) where interval responses served to postpone the onset of the next trial. Animals were trained with and without a warning signal. Those animals which received the warning signal were free to postpone the onset of the signal as well as the shock. What Keehn found was that the animals supplied with a warning signal behaved so as to postpone the shock but not the warning signal. He argued that the signal was not a secondary negative clicitor. Rather, the signal had tho properties of a discriminative stimulus because it marked the time in which the appropriate avoidance response would be reinforced. Keehn's work resembles Sidman and Baron's (1957) although Sidnan and Baron's was not as well controlled. They gave their animals considerable avoidance training before the signal was introduced. Sidman and Boron interpreted their results such as Keshn did by suggesting that the discriminated avoidance situation may be considered a multiple schedule in which one avoidance contingency prevails in the presence of the warning stimulus and another in its absence. Thus in the situation where a specific stimulus precedes a noxious event. such as the sights and sounds of the mechanical l4 predator as it ceases to be a potential threat and becomes an actual threat to the rat. this stimulus comes to die« tinguieh between occasions when avoidance reeponaee will be reinforced and when they will not. For the analogous prey- prodator situation, an avoidance response prior to the onset of actual threat should be punished by too early a termination of reinforcement. That is, the animal should learn to etay at the source of primary reinforcement at least until the warning signal of an approaching predator has been perceived. Additionally. a study by Kelvin and Brown (1964) may polaibly indicate that even the onset or the predator's approach might not be sufficiently aversive to immediately Ilicit the avoidance response. In this study a noxious bright light preceded food delivery to rate, for 20, 40, or 80 pairings. After this training, the light was then used alone in an escape learning test. It was found that light- food pairings diminished the light'e aversivenese, the effect increasing with frequency of pairings. Loss of aversivonees was attributed to the light'a having acquired tendencies to elicit food seeking which competed with eecepe responses. is an example of a competing response incompet- iblo with escape responses, Melvin and Brown site a loco- motor movement toward the former location of the food cup at the back or the apparatus; in other words movement in a direction opposite that necessary to escape. 15 figgietsncetg_§;tinotion. By invoking a competing response explanation of resistance to extinction, it is possible to understand why the organism may persist at the source of the primary positive elicitor at least up to and possibly slightly beyond the onset at the actual threat from the approaching predator. Therefore,it seems reasonable to suppose that the effects of frustration and tear compete with the develOpment of a passive avoidance response to the one: of the alley and goal regions. The terms, “frustration" and “tear" are considered internal responses elicited in the organien by external stimuli that may be labeled negative elicitore, i.e.. potential and actual threat and the termination or a hrinary positive elicitor. The effect of the frustration and tear an overt behavior may be comparable. as flelvin and Brown (1964) have indicated. Therefore, in attest. fear and frustration may be olassed together as internal response normally antagonistic to the internal response of relaxation es hypothesised by Denny and associates (Denny and Adelssn (1956): Denny one Heisman (1964) ). This View at “frustration- fear“ is in accord with Amsel’o (1962) proposition that extinction is an sotiVe not a passive process. It is on active process neéiated via disorisinoble internal stimuli. Also, it is maintained that the internal response produced stiluli, generated within an animal by external 16 stimuli which have fcorful and frustrating associations con to discriuinitivc stimuli eliciting continued approach towards tho situation associated with potential threat of punishment and withdrawal of rain: rccmcnt. Therefore, in the prey—predator analogue, the n nreinforccuont of approach responses in the face of threatening cuss should result in greater resistance to extinction in animals which have previously learned to approach threatening stimuli than in animals which were previously able to obtain primary rein- forcement without tearful or threatening consequences. Further, on internal response of fcsr~£ruotration is viewed as a continuous variable. That is to say, the fear—frustration response may vary in magnituoc. While a certain magnitude or Icor~frustrsticn may become a conditioned segment of the approach response, an increase in tho fear- fruotrstion response over and above the conditioned level should provide responses which compete with continued approach. Bronco. the increased magnitude of the competing recr- frustration rooponco over~aud~sbove the previously conditioned level should require some time to become ccntigiouoly associated with the external stimulus cooplox, especially the stimuli of tho goal rcgion. That is, extinction takes some time to occur as it torso time for tho competing roo- ponsoa to build up. Possible support for this latter suggestion is found in a study by Lonny (1959). In the first 17 of two experiments, two groups of rats were trained to press a her one trial a day for 10 or 50 trials. Both groups were then given 75 extinction trials, one per day. A control group, with no prior training, was given 25 unrcwsrded trials, also one per day. The bar was always removed from the box as soon as § had depressed it. During extinction, the latency of the bar-pressing response greatly increased for the control animals, but reached a low, stable, level in the two groups which had previously received either 10 or SO reinforced trials. There was no evidence of extinction in these two latter groups. In the second experiment, rats were trsinod to prose a her five trials 3 day until oach,§;g_latency was at least cqual to the group in the first experiment which had 10 rein- forced trials. The: were then given extinction trials until a Salinute no-rolponco criterion was attained. But, the III in this case was 5 minutes not one day. Under these conditions, all a; extinguished in less then so trials. Denny interpreted the results from this study as follows: 1.) bar—retraction immediately after discrete bar-proooing docs not itself prevent extinction; and 2.) one trial a day, or at least highly spaced trials, is essential to the Virtual prevention of extinction. "The importance of bar— roctriction use definitely suggested, however, since E observed that approach to the ever present food trey extin- guished during the non-rewarded trials even her responding 18 did not.” ”One compelling observation by §.in Experiment 11 was that on the trial or two just before § extinguished, all §;§_hegen to make vigorous attempts to escape from the box. An implication here is that frustration effects may accumulate with a 5-minute intertrial interval and become sufficiently strong to instigate competing responses.” (p.85) I The implications of Denny's study for the present study lies in the similarity in conditions. Removal of the bar, in the Denny study. in viewed as similar to the animal's withdrawal from the goal region compelled by the "actual threat“ of the approaching mechanical predator. During extinction, the conditioning of competing frustration responses to the goal stimuli £111 be impeded if the animal must quickly avoid the oncoming mechanical predator. 0n the other hand, if the prey organism is permitted to spend increased tine in the goal region the cues of the goal region would become elicitors o: the frustration response and thereby hasten extinction. I general aneigegationg, To summarize the intent of this study is to impose experimental control on an assumed prey-predator situation where the temporal and environmental aspects of the situation are known. or equal importance is the fact that the behavior of the prey animal can be repeatedly observed and recorded. Through manipulation of factors controlling reeietence to extinction, an attempt will be made to underetand how the prey organism learns to - .‘-v 19 to cepe with a situation involving potential and actual threat of predation. The experimental apparatus regardleoe of claims to an analogy to the naturally occur*in5 eituetion, could actually be considered a conglomerate of familiar luberotory apparatus. Indeed, the experimental apparatus involveo nopecto very similar to those found in a shuttle box, a straight alley, and perhaps an obstruction box. Con idered in another light, it should be remembered that many animals have leirs, burrows, or noete. They must forage to stay alive for rarely are the needs of the organ~ ion met without leaving the neet. By foraging, the animal expoeee itself to predatore. Alec, in the competition with other members of its own species, they (the prey organiem) muet take maximal advantage of the limited sources of ouetonn“ce available to it (Wynne-Edwards, 1963; Calhoun, 1362). The complexity of the naturally occurring situation dictotce the use of a laboratory situation paralleling the natural in ito essentials. Because of the temporal and apatinl eecuoncee of events, the prey-predator situation can also be characterieed on involving ”approach-then-uvoid behavior.” The study of avoidance behavior, from the outeet, has been viewed by some workere as having a direct bearing on the prey-predator oitueticn. A3 Pavlov (1927) has noted, "The strong carni- vorous animal preys on weaker animals, and if they waited to defend themselves until the teeth of the foe were in :p i) their flesh, would speedily be exterminatod. The oooe takes on a different aspect when the defense roflcx is called into play by the sights era sounds of the enemies approach. Khan the prey hoe a chance to cave itself by hiding or by flight' (p.14). Hull (1934) also referred directly to this tapic when he wrote, ”In the violent otruggle for existence pictured by organic evolution.... those animals which responded by flight and other defense reactions in advance of injury would be far more likely to ooonpe who horco woulé have immensely greater choncee of survival and ultimate reproduction than would arimala which did not possess such a tendency.” (p.454). error-.1 13:12:; From the foregoing considerations, two hypothesee more formulated even though tlifi otuéy io Quito explanatory in format. 1. In the prey-predator analogue, the prey which had previously learned to approach threatening stimuli in order to obtain positive reinforcement shows greater reeiotanco to extinction than an animal which had obtained the some positive reinforcement without fearful or threatening consequences and/or without being forced to leave the goal region preme- turcly (because of threat). 2. In he prey-predator analogue, the prey which hfid previously learned to approach threatening stimuli in order to obtain posi- tive reinforcement shows greater resistance to extinction if forced to leave the goal region sooner (because of threat) then a prey which is not force& to leave the goal regioo a! 90011. Suhigotq. The gg'wero S naive female hooaod rats from the colony maintained by the Papartment of Psychology of Michigan State University. All 23 were 110 days old at the start or the experiment. Beginning two weeks prior to the start of the experi- ment, each §Iwas handloa for five minuteo a day. Beginning one week prior to the start or the experiment, §3.were placed on a water deprivation oohedulc of ten minutes of access to water in the individual homo cages every 24 hours. Food waa constantly available. fipogggtgg, An isometric drawing of the apparatus is presented in Figure I, togother with labels for the major apparatus components. The major component of the apparatna consisted of a long, rectangular box measuring 8 feet long by 5 inches wide and 2 feet high. This box wao closed at both endo; at one end by a fixed end wall, at the other by a hinged door which swung outward and downward from the top. All four sides of this box were corotructed of 1/8 inch Masonite with the finished surface facing inward. Thin flasonite box was firmly mounted to the top and to one side of a very rigid box platform. This platform measured 8 feet long by 1 foot wiée by 5% inches high and was constructed by 3/4 inch plywood for the toy (which also 21 P) l“ '2 served as the bottom of the hasonitc box) and 5/4 inch finished pine planking on four aides. Two rectangular frames were rigidly attached to the platform. One frame was attached to the platform 22-3/4 inches from the end with the hinged door. The frame con» sisted of two 5/4 inch by 5 3/4 inch pine uprighte, one on either side of the platform, rising 29% inches. The tope of these uprighte were connected by a 13 5/8 inch, 3/4 by 5 3/4 inch piece of pine. it the OppOSite end of the plat- form was the other an& identically constructed frame although it was not inset from its end of the platform. The Opening formed by this frame and the end of the platforh was covered by a aheet of % inch plywood. This made the frame especially rigid and formed a vertical mounting surface for an electric motor. The motor was a 115 volt D.C., .60 ampere, 1725 rpm, 1/20 h.p., ESE-33, reversable Bodine. It was mounted so that its shaft was 20% inches from the top of ita plywood base with its shaft lying horizontally. A 5/8 inch pally wee attached to tho motor's shaft. The current for the motor was supplied by a varies to control motor epoch and a full-wave bridge rectifier. The varioue euperstructures of the apparatus were attached to the horizontal, overhead members of the two upright frames. This included a pulley and belt system, microswitcaes, the track for the mechanical predator and r~ ‘9 \21 a mirror. The overhead par of the mechanical prceator depenoed from a 6 foot track consisting of a sliding .5. closet door track of extruded alum‘num (Soars Catalogue Io. Under this arrangement, two feet of the Hasonito rectangle was not between the two upright frames. Thio aegment of the larger box was the "safe box” and was phy- sically separated from the remainder by a 22 inch high partition with a 2% inch diameter hole with the bottom edge of the hole 2 inches from the bottom of the partition. This hole communicated to he remaining 6 foot segment of the box. Cn the other end of the safe box was the previously mentioned hingefi door. The mechanical predator was drawn to and fro along the overheea track via a long loop of high tensile strength cotton string attached by screw eyes to either end of the overhead component of the mechanical predator. This 100p of string ran immediately under the track and was guided by pulleys around the ends of the horizontal segments of the uprights and over the tap of the trace. At the end of the rue on which the elecuric motor was mounted, the string loop was driven by a e inch diameter pulley. This pulley wet connected by a short shaft to a 1 1/8 inch pulley which wee ériren through a 21 inch rubber belt from the pulley moulted on the motor shaft. The movement of the mechanical predator up and down 24 the 6 feet of the alley was controlled by two microswitches (Micro V4—l4). Each switch was suspended over the inner edge of the upright frames by angel brackets. They were actuated by rotational force, so from the shaft of each switch a finger of flexible piano wire hung down into the path of the overhead component of the mechanical predator. The resting position of the mechanical predator was at the end of the alley opposite the safe box. To place it in operation, fihmomentarily depressed a hand held switch which looked closed a relay supplying current to the electric motor. On reaching the safe box and of the track the mechanical predator deflected the finger actuating the microswitch at that end. This action unlocked the first relay and locked on a second which fed a reversed current to the armature or the electric motor. The mechanica predator, therefore. reversed direction and returned towards its start ng position. Just before reaching this end of its track, the mechanical predator deflected the finger from the other microswitch which unlocked the second relay and opened the circuit to the electric motor, thus stopping the mechanical predator until its cycle was begun again by E. The mechanical predator consisted of two connected components, an overhead apparatus and another at floor level. The latter component was what the gs had direct interaction with. A drawing of the complete unit is to be had in Figure II. The overhead component consisted of the four, wheeled track runners supplied with the closet door track. Two runners were hung in the two parallel J-troughe of the track. The two runners in each track were separated on 5 inch centers and ran in Oppoeeed pairs front and back. Mounted between the track runners with its bottom surface flush with the bottom edges of the runners was a 7 inch long, 1 7/16 inch wide and 1 5/8 inch high block of pine. mounted flush to the bottom of the block was a piece of i” Masonite measuring 7 inches . long by 3 7/8 inches wide in the horizontal plane. Sne- ponded below this horizontal Masonite platform by encir- cling Haeonito mounts was a 6 volt, Allstate ignition coil (Sears catalogue no. 28A8240). The 0011 was mounted so that its long axis was aprallel to the floor of the Illey, its electrical terminals projected towards the safe box and of the alley, and ite bottom was flush with the back edge of the Mhsonito platform from which it hung. Projecting downward from the horizontal Masonite platform.wee a 17 inch, 1/8 inch diameter bronze rod. The rod hung from a point 3/3 inch from the front edge of the Masonite platform and 2 3/8 inches from either side wall of the alley. This placed the rod directly in front of the high tension electrode of the ignition coil and was connected to this electrode by a coil spring assuring good .1, I. ootrioal condutlon and a fat iguo Ir eo linkage Thin bronze rod thus cozduo od high voltage currc 2L dovrnard to the lower component of the mechanical predator. ) The lower co W; est consistei o; a 4 inch, 12 gauge alminmn dioc 1.131121;- sprayed with no t Lute p.112; 1: for inoroo: cd also ri; lnability by Q, Tho dioo Lao bolted to an additional threaded iILCh of the bronze rod bent at a right m'le to tho vertical segment and pointing fo wards towards the safe box and of the alley. The mounting hole Of the disc was in its exact cantor. Thus the flat ourface of too disc was mrpondicular to the fl oor anfi facing tho sofa box end of the alloy.. Tho 6 volt current was supplied to the ignition coil from an Allstate battery charger (Home l 30. 608.L600) first being fed through a Motorola automobile radio vibrator (Type 485522000). The current was fed to the overhead compor ent of tllo mechanical predator from a fine gouge, inoulatoo d, twin lead hanging from the top of a 2 inch wide, inch tlli 0k, 43 inch high poot fastened 63 inches to center from the 333 ebox end of the platform. The twin lead wire was 40 inohoo in longth and was c‘nnootcd to the mechanical predator 1y 0 vertical otand o; f with an arm projecting over the oide of the alley. This 338 ”tn aallowod positive aleotric supply to the coil without twisting or stretching 27 the wire or allowing it to foul the mechanical operation of the predator. This system was capable of delivering a 11,600 volt, .001 ampere shock to the gg’if they ailowed the disc witnia 1/8 inch of any pirt of their bodies as valtuge at this pressure was quite capable of arcing through the fig hair. This shock gunisnad §,for failure to avoid in aurficiont time. The shock was assumed to be roughly analogoua to the effects of tho teeth and claws of a predator on the EL; body without the necessity of actually producing lasiona of the skin. The floor or the apparatus on which §.actually saved consistea of § inch hardware cloth. In the 6 foot alley this floor was formed by benéing down the edges of a 6 foot run of-hardwara cloth 90 that the surfacg was raised 1 1/3 inches above the wooden bottom of tha alley. This provided a self-cleaning surface besides being an electrical ground When g received a shock. The floor of the safe box was also formefi o: i inch hardware cloth with an inch of additional material folded downward and slightly under for aaditional rigiaity. this was dong as this floor moved up and down by pivoting on fulcruma immediately out$ide of and to one side of-the safe box. The lever aupparting the flour 2 inches above the wooden subfloor was formed from a 3/16 inch bronze rod. This rod baa bent into a Q aidci lcC'Lxrglc thin¢ two 7 irch abort aides and a it; irch side. Three inches of each of the short c;des 13 Qj€(.ti2d past the fulcrums through the wall 5 the sa¢s box to be atts ched to and suprort th hardware cloth floor. The remainin g 4 inches of the chart {9 idc and thc counecting 163 inches of rod were outside the safe buy. Lith thc fulcrum; outside the safe box, §_could ti,“inr the £100: downwaré. A minimum of 50 grams wa, B u necessary to tip the fluor. The lever was counter balanced a W .1 a;c 1;g t.u9 163 inches arm by a brags weight. Resting in the middle of the 16§~1nch arm was a pivoting mercury switch (an thus cilant). The mercury switch was pivoted on a separate 2 inch arm. Thus whcn_§ wag in the eafe ho x the 16:: nch extc-dal arm rais ed anfi it tilted the mercury switch upwards. When this occured, the mercury snitch 0p ad a normally closed circuit to a recording pen on a four pen Gc*brands recorder (Model 30. 24). This tilting floor and mercury switch system formed an automatic recoréing detector for the duration of the QE presence in or absence frcm the 8”f@ box duri1g an experi- mental Be 2‘1 on. Another pen of the recorcer automatically recorded the operation of t1 3 mechanical predator. A third pen was uaed to record the time spent in the "goal region”. The 29 goal region was that port of 332:; the inches from the safe box The beginning of «a alley. b1 for m orange cardboard lying on the wooaen subfloor. had about the some brightness, to 3, as wooden floor. By means of a through the pen recorder, the time the 6 foot alley beginning and extending the romainder of this goal region was marked '3 convenience by the front edge of a nouoro of light This orange the surrounding hand held switch, 3 recorded, 3 spent in the goal region. g depressed and held the button when ever and as long as any part of §_wa3 in the goal region. By moans of those recordings, a permanent, accurate record was formed of tho rumber of sofa ho" and goal region entrance and exits and the amount of time spent in each and in the inleyway connecting the two. A stainless steel firinli 7‘1”“.‘3 ‘0'- V n; tube 'ectod into the goal region 2& inches from the ease of the goal region or ;6 inches from the safe box. inches in o he (22‘ ware cloth floor. 0 . the side wall but througa a roceosoo translucent white "fial With its éna l The tube did not plastic 23 The tube projocteé 1% inch above the hard- projoct directly through O inches in inch 603;. The drinkir; tabs #J‘ ouspenfio: on the outaifie from a rulher stoppered graduated cylinder. Through this means, é could measure the amount of water av‘fi consumed during a daily session. 30 In spite of the high walls of the app aratus and over- head aurora tructures, §;could cle: zrly ob: 3rv Ea behavior in the apparatus through overhead mirrors. 033 mirror was mounted above the safe box, aszothor over the alloy. The laboratory was W3“ 11 lllomL33t3d my dif fu 336 fluroeso.nt lamps, but due to the 53033133 of the apparatus, the ill 3Lu3 3’33 12* 3id3 the apgara.tu3 was r3 3*uo ed an further refiuced the low albedo of £33 interior walls. Table I gives the ill 32ira‘ion in foot candles along the floor of *Exc apgaratus. r} ’1. -1-9 I“; I “.-JJ‘¢.¢‘ *m Tllumination, in fact c3;dles, of the floor of the apparatus measured at one foot intervals from the mats box end. . sgf- :x r- . 3;, 3 oistance in :3 9 hr alloy 153; rebign 33me Q 11 2* "' 4 6 a lOOt Cunu1032.03.8.81.5 108W 2: 8 .4- 30 2. . *readinga of 1.6 and 1.8 were made immediately on either side of €33 33f box wall, 33103 333 located at ox3otly two feet. The entire apparatus was raised 20% inches above the laboratory floor for fiLg convenience in taking fig in an& out of the apparatus and for observing §§ through the mirrors. The zuan’ng 3pp3r3tus was in one room while all con— trol circuitrj. power aupplies, and the pen recorders were in an adjacent room. The wiring connecting the two was fed through a hole in the wall. This was 3333 to isolate £3 from as much extraneous noise as poooiblc; especially circuitry cm*ttor ac3033ohyiru the recording of 3g.various activities. Pilot work ficmonotratcd dcmonot'otod that abrupt noises tended to inhibit the g3, The operation of the mechanical procatcr produced noise. $313 noise was considerco part oi the stimulus complex oicoallin~ actual throat to the 3. Below are two tables lioting noise levels within the apparatus during the operation of the mochonicol predator. ‘1'“. A "." I “i ‘ 1.73..)Hul I T mu...- .5 Noise level 3, in decibels, recorded with the fioioe Level “333? rlorco onc brzioo the (: aging tuoc. floodings were taken at one foot into-V313 as tho mes clm onioul predator move 3 toxcrdc tPc oofo box. All roooingo in?lucc 3.; ambient noise 19.31 of 72 db. ,- .“1 ._,..- _ . ~~ {30-213. I’C‘L‘XLOH alloy 1 fie toncc in Iccmt G 1 E 1 i 5 ‘001501l 78.0 79.0 77.0 77.5 78.0 73.0 78.5 Hoisc lovclo, in dcoibcls, rccorccc 3:33 tlc Loiae Level Motor microphone in the middle of the oofo box. cadinus 1': CI"? ta! Kié'; got 0210 f0 “CD it IQlV'lS 3.“ :11; 143,\LL~1_...\.(...L. lxredatcl‘ .movcd towards the safe box. All readings incluoo an ambient noise level of 72 db. («‘3 7 154' ,- 3; new: r .3 '1 .q-vr -7 41:):’:m fi‘Jh- -, «A. 1 ‘u «- '1‘ ‘ a 3 once 1ng t 0 l 2 i» 4 5 6 ; I~.:~g..:--.;Q~~- «v-0..- org-2:" «Tibia-y’all-J" —'—..--r~v '- 3| II. "F -. ‘i‘r.’ :um: ' 1 ,: 7'P‘ ‘— Lccibolr ; .3} 3.5 (5.3 (int; {3.) I ‘.3 .0 l Figure 1. Isometric drawing of the The key to the numbered ‘ following appara .. . 9 componen.s page. vmh4adfzs l...’ 2'32; .1“?! ca ajporoivq m ‘ ‘pr 7* IV 4.. '13.)..3 \- ~0- mm 1’- .-O 4. b- V - a». .'--v.. ..- ,~- . . ,. "V. 1'7 -s.- :- '._. .~ 1-. . 5—. 1 41". .fiingt.’ U3 t £ ~t'o‘l -:-CL\;‘ LEA lfi‘Z'UJAbzab‘J 0:3. Liar—7 ('ufik‘fil‘lfllun V0.01 o]*L.w~ L;3; orfor. 5&2 1 - 2 - 3 - W i (1‘ “J 0. U1 1 I I 3 t a... ‘5 .1. 0'. 1 f. .C. .’ q .l. - I .5} - C: - 4' w . as b "' ‘ . 3.. .. "‘ rw. WV .53.. ‘1' «my .3 i-;*io i. :33 u'MpP3ougo are wistod in '1 Corroroni ‘¢.|-p. ‘- . » W' 4...... -. almighiuia £4901. C4 1 C: ‘0 C. S... c" (72‘ f- C» a; t—J r- {.7 5 . (D h E C {2, *3 5‘ king tube hgrdwaro 3"o.1 io her 303.33336 of moohani«: i predator Elli 3.111.} ' U".¥'...'t011 miorosyiioh 31'1018 urgflgo unrubouru Sguare ‘3 ovorLe3d compononi ofn ohaziioal predator Figure 11. Icometric drawin5 of the mechanical predator. L Below: Numerical key to the ‘ alphabetized componerte. l :R o e \)03 -o:hu\¢wanahl I Qomgonentg aluminum diec aluminum track automobile ignition coil brace bronze rod high voltage electrode insulator connecting brace and bronze rod Masonite platform pine block standoff string track runner award (OI-'0 wMaJ t2 5 35 £53ng, A within subjects or eteaay state design was used to provide maximal control of individual differencee and unknown varieblee. The four eonéitione or phases of the experiment were: 1) Free Access; 2) Extinction; 3) Requi- sition; 4) he-extinction. Each phase lasted 15 Gaye. The experiment, therefore, required 60 days to complete. Regardless of the phase, each § was run for one, on minute session per d3y. In Ihaae l or Free Access g baa unlimited access to the goal region throughout, without fear of pursuit from the predator. In Yhase 2, §_waa exposed for the first time to conditions where goal entries were both nonreinforced and subject to potential and actual threat from the prefiator. In ?hase 3, goal entries were again reinforceé as in Phase 1, but such approach wee followed by attack from the predator. Thus, § in this phase were subject to both fear and frustration. In Phase 4, g was re—extinguiohod with the expectation that extinction would take longer than in Phase 2. firgeedure. The four experimental phases are described in detail below. Phase 1 consisted of fifteen days of free access to water in the experimental apparatus. Buring this hase, entrance into the goal area or region éid not result in the approach of the mechanical predator. Through- out this pause. the mechanical predator was present and 36 electrically charged though always stationary at the end of the alley opposite the safe box. In Phase 2. the water reinforcement was not available although the drinking tube was still in place. Also, any entry by any part of §,into the goal reb ion resulted in the approach of the mechanical predator after a fixed delay of 7 seconds. For Phase 3. water reinforcement was reinstated. In all other respects it was identical to Phaeo 2. During this phase, 3 learned to approach the goal stimuli when these stimuli were associated with frustration and threat or punishment. The last phone, Phase 4. was procedurally identical to Phase 2. the goal entry response was the major dependent variable. This response was defined as any traversal trom.the safe box to the entry of guy part otI§ into the goal region. Ercept for the differential treatment: given during H Phase 1 and 3, the remainder of the running procedures were uniform throughout the four phases. ‘All §§|wero continuously meintained on the water deprivation schedule to which they care adapted prior to the beginning of the experiment. This schedule was ten minutes of access to water in the individual hone cages once a day not sooner than thirty minutes after an.§_had been run for that day. The running order of the animals. for each day, was eyetemu atioally varied so that no S was consistently run in the same position within a day as on the proceeding day. §g. 37 were brought, individually, from the colony room into the tasting room, run, and then returned to the colony room. This was done to prevent any g3 exposure to apparatus coundc while not being run. An equally uniform running procedurc was used across all phases. A g was placed in the safe box; g then turned on the switch controlling the paper feed for the remote pen recorfior. ‘E then started a stopwatch while at the ammo timo lifting a panel from in front of the safe box hole. The otcpwatchtaa used to takc the session length and the 7 second delay between §LQ entry into the goal region and operation of the mechanical predator. The panel prevented §g_leaving the safe box until the stapwatch woo started. §_thon cat in a position whore he could observe § via the overhoad mirrcro on the apparatuc. In this pos- 1tion E also Operated the switches which controlled the remote recording of the time spent by §.in the goal area on each entry and the switch which turned on the motor driving the mechanical predator. .§ also cht notes on g g behavior during tho session. Throughout Phases 2, 3, and 4. the mechanical predator was actuated only after a constant delay of 7 seconds after the entrance of.§ into tho goal region. A 7 second aolcy was chosen on the basic of pilot work. Dolayo shorter than 7 seconds were found to retard the acquisition of tho approach response which would have prolonged Phase 3. In Phases 2, 3, and 4, the mechanical predator was prepellcd to and fro at a conotant velocity of 1 foot per second. This velocity was chosen so that firhad to retreat from the goal and into the safe box at minimally a fast walk or slow run to avoid contact with he advancing disc. This was done to reduce variability in flight from the goal. The advantages or disadvantages of compelling §_to run as swiftly as possible were unknown. The response by glupon which actuation of the mechan- ical predator was contingent woo the same in Phases 2. 3, and 4. S was in no way forced to remain in the goal region after entering. RESULTS AED DISCUSSION §§ati§tical Agalzsio. The data are presented in graphic and tabular form at the and of this section and in Appendix I. (5 Figure III gives the rossonso rate (number at times 3_cntors tho goal region in s 10 minute session) across sessions for the four experimental phases. A analysis of variance response rates during the two extinction phases (Phases 2 and 4) showed that there is a significant difference in the absolute nagnitudoa of response rates. The response rate is aigni~ Iicantly higher in Phase 4 than in Phase 2. This is clear evidence that the learning during Phase 3 had the prodicted effect of conditioning the approach reoponao to the goal region under threat of attack from the predator. TABLE V Analysis of variance (Edwards. 1960) or response rates across sessions of Phase 2 versus Phase 4. 64?;Vfflean gggagea_ F Treatment 1267.30 1 1267.30 7.44“ Error 1363.62 8 170.45 Scasious 169.87 14 12.13 1.72 Treatment X Sessions 140.80 14 10.06 1.43 Error zoggza ;;2 7.04 Total 3730.37 149 * significant at the .05 level The treatment means and standard deviations for Phase 2 are 3 v1.84, SD =- .717. For Phase 4 they are I «7.65, so al.981. 39 40 The nonsignificant Sessions effect indicetee that when the treatment effect is averaged ecross sessions there remains no effect peculiar to sessions alone. The nonsigni- ficaet interaction of treatments X sessions indicates that the extinction process was of the some form in Phase 2 and 4. differing only in magnitude. The low response rate recorded during Phase 1 must be interpreted in terms of £3; task. During Posse 1, fig were never forced to leave the goal region. Observations of fig; behavior during Phase 1 indicated that most of the time accumulated in the goal region was spent drinking (see Table VI for the mean water consumption across sessions of Phases 1 and 3). The remainder of the time during a Phase 1 session was spent exploring the aposratue. This orplcrstory behavior probably accounts for the feet that there was a reoponoe rate greater than 1 response per session.: Figures liaend IVb show the mean total time spent in the safe box, alley ondp goal across sessions during the four phases. Figures IVs and IVb show that during Phase 1, the gg spent most of each 600 second or 10 minute session in the goal region. This was not the case during the remaining phases where most of the time was spent in the safe box. A further means of comparing the resistance to extinction of £2 responding was through the difference between regression coefficients of the curves from Phaeee 2 and 4. Inspection of the response rates during Phase : ineioatea thst the effect 41 of the extinction sessions was completed by the eighth session. Therefore, the slopes of both curves were derived for the first eight aessiona. For those 2, tho regression coefficient is bxy” -.285. For Phase 4, the regression coefficient is bxy' -.480. The poolea error term 13 33"447’ This yields a t~rotio of .436, d.f. = 12, uhich is not significant at the .05 level. This teat shows that when the differences in absolute magnitude are eliminated, there is no significant difference in'the rate of extinction in Phases 2 and 4. The test of regression coefficients inuicateo that the tasks prior to extinction is or the utmost importance. glg rate of reoponding will be greater during extinction, in terms of the absolute number of responses, after they have learned to cope with predatory attacks to obtoin reinforce- ment, a was learned in Phase 3, compared to conditions which did not equip the §g to cope with predatory attacks (Phase 1). Once extinction has begun, though, either type of prior experience results in the same relative rate of extinction. Another way to analyze fig,behnvior is through the mean time spent in the 3031 per response. This information is available in Figure V. Inspection of Figure V and response ratio given in Figure III suggest a relationship between roaponae rate and tho time spent in the goal region per rea- ponao. A Pearson product moment correlation between response rate and mean time in goal per response computed across h H Beaalana in Phage 1 yieldafi an E n - .fils (Hignificmflt at the .0035 level). For th33 2, tha correlation has £.2 + .521 (significant at the .D;S level). Fer Phaaa 5, the correlation was + .713 (significant at the .SQS level). Emile fer rhaae 4. the correlation was glm * .644 (eignificfint at the .335 level). A3 far as Phase 1 in concerneé. thare 13 a strcng inverse relationship between reeponae rate and tima in anal per rea- ponae. Zba fact that this rclatiomshifi is not § 1.00 Can moat probably be attributed to expleratcry behfivier. For the remaining 3 phases the ralatienanip seems to be a direct one. Euring the last 3 phaaea, the reayonae rate and time spefit in the gnal per renpnnaa can be considered fume ioaa of the aa&e ftctor. Thia factor appears to be the extent to which fear aafi frustration prcduced stimuli are congitioxed elicitvra of the goal reaponat. this aaeumea that both refipanfia rate and time in £031 per reaponae may bath be indicative of tna degree to which canditioning hug takan place. Inayectian of Figure V shows that during Innae 2 the mean time Eiéflt 1n tfie @931 pa? reaporma wag alwtya leaa thlfl the 7 aacond fie- lay periwd. This may indicate thét fitaying in tha gfial region was an noxious ua eateriag thfi goal regian. in rhasea 3 and 4, though, time npemt in tag goal par IQBPOHfie egualed or exceafieé the 7 aecenu delay period axcapt in Phase 3 and late in Phase 4: E rly in Phuae 3 little conditioning had takan pléca, late.in Phase A, extincticn wag well afivunced. the resultm with respect to time in the anal were not specifically predicted althaugh the poaaihility uaa 43 discussed in the introduction under the section labeled Predator. Phases 2, 3. and 4 were compared statistically on mean time in goal region par response by means of matched-pairs E-taats. The comparisons were made across the 15 session by pairing sessions in order. The results showed that £3 spent significantly more time in the goal per response during Phanu 3 compared with Phase 2 (Td a 7.12, é.f. I 14, signifi- cant n the .001 level). The gig bah-Avior am m differ oigniricantly botwcen Phase 3 and 4 (Td a 1.84. d.f. = 14, not significant at the .05 level). It appears that once the conditioning to the posited fear and frustration cues had taken place more time was spent in the goal region than prior to conditioning. Also, once conditioning toox place, tho behavior producing the increased time was very resistant to extinction. ObgngationaL_Bata. While §ngere free to explore the apparatus during Phase 1, they aoon learned to avoid an area approximately 2 inchea in front of the disc. The disc was charged at all tin... After fig had received a shock apiece during the firat session (oneug took 2 shocks in a row), they continued to show interest in the disc but only from a dis- tanoe. In the remaining sessions of Phase 1, three different fig ventured clone enough to be shocked more than once (see Table VII for the frequency of shocks across sessions of the four phases). 45 region. Also, no Phase 2 progreoeod, the fig behavior in the safe box changed. During the early oeooiono, §§ frequently appearcfi very eggitotcd. The; paced about the safe box and frequently crouched facing the safe box hole. In later sessions, the g3 spent increasing amounts of time oitting quietly in one or another corner of the safe box. Also, by later in Phase 2, most Eg,had entirely abandoned any attempts to drink. What time was spent in the goal was need to crouch or nervously econ the surroundings. Table vII chews that during ?haec 3, the‘flg received more chock. than our ng any other phase. Three fig’who received shocks during the first session or this phase because once they had begun drink :3, they were very loath to discontinue. Either they did not stop drinking until shocked or moved towards the cafe box too slowly and hecitnntly as if it were equally noxious to leave the water and to remain and face the approaching predator. This hesitant, conflict like retreat from the goal region wan the usual reason for receiving shocks during Phase 3. Quite frequently during the first 3 sessions of ?hase 3, the £3 won a flee the reel beforo'tho 7 second delay had elapsed. This was not the case during the letter sessions of Phase 3 as can be eeen in Figure V. Observations made on the §g behavior during the latter part of Phase 3 ouggeeted that the frequent hesitant retreats from the goal region were the results of partial extinction I 46 of the avoidance response. ieouming that the argumente put forth in the Introduction are correct, a partial extinction of the avoidance response could take place. ibis is especially true in the letter oeeeicnn of Phase 3 es the stimuli which ehould elicit avoidance becomes inetead con- ditioned elicitore for approach and entry into the goal region. As was previously mentioned, the heeitrnt retreat iron the goal was the ueuel reason. §g were ehocked during the latter part of Phase 3. Also, during the latter Part of this phase, besides an increasing amount of time spent in the goal region perreeponee meny fig would, with increasing frequency, emerge from the safe box and begin approaching the goal reginn soon after the mechenical predator had reached its closest approach to the safe box and woe then on its return to the opposite end. Occasionally an §_would approach and enter the goal region by following on the very heels of the withdrawing meohenicel predator. Also on the inoreone at the end of this phase were burote of eggiteted appearing behavior in the safe box. The So would suddenly begin bounding about the safe box in a very vigorous way. These bursts of behavior were usually accompanied by very swift dashes to the goal region. Finally, in the letter sessions the usual approach to the goal region was a swift run from the safe box door without oboerveble signs of heeitancy or warineee. Table VII shows that during Phase 4, the fig received as many shock: as in Phase 2. In Phase 2, though. 8 of the 47 10 shocks ware rcceiveé during the firnt gezsinnn while in Phase 5 th~ 10 shanks were Shattered throughout fihn phase. (3 Obanrvat1~nn of thn‘gg’ bahavior during Phana 4 nuggeatsfl that the reasons for raceivfn: a shock were different than during Phnsn ?. In Phfise A it nppnnred that an tho ntrnngth of the approach decranned 30 did the avoidance of the mechanical predator. Perhaps as tha stimuli elicitiv; Approach infit their power to do so, these etinuli Rlno lont the pnwnr to elicit avoidance. Thin might be a tannhie suppnnitinn in terms of the hypntheaia. In the early aesgionm of Phase A, the 33 on naveral ocoaainnn woulfl, on entering the gnal raginn, cnmfilnfiely ignora the drinking tube and continua moving tnunrda the statinnnry disc. The closer they approached the dine the more wary their behavior aphenred to bannme. No fi’qu avar seen tn come closer than 6 inchaa from the disc. A9 seamians prngreanefl, the drinkifig tube wag mare and more ignored but not in favor of approaching the disc, but in favor of can- tiouwly scanning tha nurrounfla crou?hin3 by the drinking tube facing in tha directian of the 6130. During the early massinns of Phase 4, on a number or occasiona fig'wnre seen to chew and tug vigoroualy at the tube. Also during the early sessions, the §g_would on occasion bound and leap about the safe box eapecially after having attemptad to drink. They would sometimes chew the edge of the safe box hole and the hardware cloth floor. The 48 incidence of standing on the hind legs increased during this phase together with vigorous sniffing. Occasionally an‘g would attempt to jump straight up the sides of the safe box walla. hone succeeded in reaching the top though. Perhaps these behaviors were indicative of an increased magnitude of frustration. Gggcrhl Considerations: Becouoe of the orploratory naturo of this experiment, the decision was made to use the some §g_in both extinction phases (Phases 2 and 4). The assumption was made that. in rats, ro-oxtinotion is not retarded and, in Iaot,may be foscilitatod (Rorth and marten, 1962). Any fascilitation of ro-oxtinction would work against the hypothesis. In the future, though, it is recommended that independent groups be used so that r0~oxtinotion will be unneceooary and thus making assumptions unnecessary. The model used in this eXperimont to predict differential resistance to extinction represents a combination of exist- ing theories such as competing response theory, frustration theory, and secondary reinforcement theory. The theory which best fits the general noool is Elicitation Theory (Denny and Adelman, 1956). Thoorico of the drive reduction, inhibition, and generalization decrement were not viewed as being copablo of predicting the present results. It is hard to conceive of any way in which drive reduction or inhibition theories could predict those reoulto. Generalization docremont theories 49 would stand a better chance by noting that the stimulus conditions between Phases 1 ané 2 were change to a much greater extant than between Phases 3 and 4. It is difficult to see how a generalization decrement position would com- pletely account for the results in that all fig received chock during Phase 1 so that the advent of punishment in Phase 2 was not an entirely novel state of affairs. In any case, only the present model could account for the results in the following experiments. 50 4.0 TIIIIYTTITTTIFW PHASEZ 3.0- 2.0_. I.O__ COLLllLllJllllllll 0123456789l01ll2|5|4|5 SESSION ‘ I5.OTIIITWTITIITIr PHASE4 I0.0_ 5.0- 011111111L1_1411 Ol23456789l0|ll2l3l4l5 SESSION 6.0TIIITITIITTIII 5.0.. u '— é MILOL «a 2 o (L m 3 23.0.. PHASE' < u 2 2.04111i1111111111 Ol23456789|on|2|3l4l5 SESSION 30.0 ITWITTTFITrITr u, PHASE 3 p. (20.0 a r- u U) 2 o c. 3m a ' ‘ z < u ‘2 O LllljllLJlJllL Ol23456789l0llltl3l4ls SESSION Figure 111. Experiment I. graph to graph. Mean response rate per session of the four phases of Take note of the change in ordinate units from MEAN TOTAL TIME (Ssc.) MEAN TOTAL TIME (5“) 51 600 I I I I T I I I I I I I I I I 500,. PHASE I 400, 300 _ GOAL 200; ALLEY IooL. SAFE. sex 0 J L I L 41 l L L L 1 l I J L I o I 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 I0 II I2 I5 l4 I5 SESSION 600 __:_.. 500_ ALLEY 400; 300— SAFE BOX zooL PHASE 2 IooL o L I L J I i l 41’ L L_ L l J_ l L I C) I z 3 ‘4 5 6 7 8 9 Io II I2 I3 I4- IS SESSION Figure IVs. Mean total time spent in the safe box, alley, and goal region per session of Phases 1 and 2 of Experiment I. MEAN TOTAL TIME (5“) MEAN TOTAL TIME (Scc.) 52 coo 500, 400 r ALLEY soo_ 200.. SAFE BOX PHASE 3 I00. 0 J I l l l l i l 1 1 L l l l l o I 2 545 678 9|OIIIZI5I4IS SESSION 600 r I I I T T I I I I I I I 500 F ALLEY 400_. 300% SAFE BOX 200_ PHASE 4 IooL o I I lo I I I L I L, LL_ L L I I I OI 25456783IOIII2I5I4I5 SESSION Figure IVb. Mean total time spent in the safe box, alley, and goal region per session of Phases 3 and h of Experiment I. MEAN TIME IN GOAL PER RESPONSE (Sec) MEAN TIME IN GOAL PER RESPONSE (See) I? 0 5 I00 8 O O 4 O O 0 0‘ 0 U‘ 0 53 I l I I I I I I I I I I I If ‘ PHASE I " r— —- P— —I I— d F ‘ I L LJ 1 l l l I l 1 L1 1 L OI23456789IO|II2|5I4I5 SESSION I I I I I I I I I I T FT T I PHASE 3 I I III I I I I I I I I I I I OI 2 3456759IOIIIZI3I4I5 SESSION Figure V. I5 IO 0 I5 O IIIjIIITIIITTII PHASE 2 I IIJ I III I I I III I I I L o I 2 345 6 7 89IOII IZISMIS SESSION IIIIIIIIIIIIII 23456789IOIIIZI3I4I5 SESSION I O I Mean time in the goal region per response per session of the four phases of Experiment I. at the 7 second mark indicates the time of onset of the mechanical predator from the time of an 3's entry into the goal region. The broken, horizontal line 54 o A n o a a o o o H o w a H o v 33A m N N m u w o n a A m H o o m n 32$ 0.. o o o o o o o c m o o o A a m 3.5 o o o A o o o o n A o o o H w a 25E hazeuvcuu noonm NA 2 W m a w m! 44 In a H 542$ .H wnoawumnum Ho Gunman know one we moanmon hag co>fiouon exoonn we hosenrehm v.n p.w wnn ¢.¢ v.m u.n ch mow o.w w. w. m. do A. a. H0¢Q3.No .dl ma wa nfi‘ NA NA CHII MW n 1N o m «1 INI N fl 0.5 0.6 0.5 +.o m.> o.m 0.0 mow N.> ¢.b v.» mos mob o.> w.n have) no nfiJaflfidSaprIstlswufignew .H aucldnoanm no m use a seesaw we moan-ea new A.Hlv neuvgnshsoe Ream) use: g ue v. I II: ‘0. III II-‘ I gargantuan II This experiment constituted the test of the second hypothesis. Given the confirmation of the first hypothesis obtained in Experiment I, it was assumed that the internal stimuli from fear-frustration continued to serve as discrim- inative stimuli. Additional support for the present analysis through a procedure which allowed for the manipulation of the opportunity to make competing responses in association with the discriminative stimuli. I It was assumed that extinction is an active process involving the conditioning of frustration responses to the goal stimuli so that the goal stimuli elicit responses in competition with continued goal entry. But. since fear- fruetretion stimuli are assumed to be capable of eliciting goal entry because of the conditioning which took place during acquisition, it would appear that an increased inten- sity of the internal stimuli would be necessary to elicit the occupating responses. Presumably. this should occur when reinforcement is removed. To demonstrate a predictable differential, it was further assumed that the longer gg’were allowed to remain in the immediate vicinity of the goal stimuli during non-reinforce- ment the greater the likelihood that frustration responses would occur and compete with goal entry. A group allowed to 55 56 spend a longer time in the goal region should display lees resistance to extinction than a group spending less time in the goal region. fietggg Subjegjg. The §3_vere 10 naive female hooded rats from the colony maintained by the Department of Psychology of Kiehigan State university. All garnere from 110 to 120 days old at the start of the experiment. Beginning two weeks prior to the start of the experi- ment. each §_wss handled for five minutes a day. Beginning one week prior'to the start of the experiment, §3.were placed on a water deprivation schedule of ten minutes of sccess to water in the individual home cages every 24-hours. Food was continuously available. Also, during the last week prior to the beginning of this experiment, gg,were. individually, allowed ten minutes per day to explore the experimental upperetus and drink in the apparatus. Apparafing. The apparatus used in Experiment I was used in this experiment without modification. ‘zzgggggggp the procedure used called for a combination within and between subjects design. The four conditions or phases of the experiment were: 1.) Acquisition; 2.) Extinction; 3.) Reecquieition; 4.) Be-extinction. than. 1 lasted fifteen days as did Phase 2. Phase 3 looted ten days. Phase 4 lasted five days. Regardless of h 57 the phase, each g‘wee run for one, ten minute session per day. During Phase 1 or Acquisition, the gs learned to approach the goal. They were water reinforced, but. they entered the 5081 under conditions of threat from the mechanical predator which in turn resulted in termination of reinforcement on each trial. This phase was prooedurally identical to Phase 3 of Experiment I. Any entry by any part of an §_into the goal resulted in the approach of the mechanical Predator after a fixed delay of 7 seconds. At the end or this phase. the 10 §§.were divided into two groups. Each group had approximately the some mean response rate across the sessions of Phase 1. The groups were eeeembled from pairs of §§, more or less matched for mean response rate eoroee sessions (see Table VIII below). The response was defined as a traversal from the safe box into the goal region. All the measures taken in Experiment I_were taken in Experiment II. The groups were randomly assigned to the eXperimentel conditions of Ehase 2. In Phone 2 ell gs, regardless of group, underwent extinction. Ibis is, that the water reinforcement was no longer available upon reaching the goal. The independent verieble wee the enount of time the §g|were allowed in the goal prior to the approach of the mechanical predator. There was two conditions of the independent variable, 3 seconds Group composition in terms of the mean response rate aorooo Phaoo 1. 2.2222 A W W o H w : agpjgct geag {espouse gate gugjggt goon response rats 1. 17.5 6. 17.6 2. 16.1 7. 15.9 3._ 12.6 8. 12.3 4. 11.9 9. 10.1 5. ___fi&§_ , 10. _~_§&;~_ 63.9 64.0 2:12.78 2:12.80 and 11 seconds. Group A received the 3 second condition; Group B the 11 second condition. Three seconds and 11 seconds are both 4 seconds away from the 7 second delay usad in Phase 1. These values were chosen for two reasons: 1) pilot work indicated that a 3 second delay strongly retarded initial acquisition of the approach response. Because the second hypothesis predicts greater resistance to extinction for Group A (3 seconds) compared to Group B (11 coconda) making the short delay a time known to retard responding during acquisition strengthens the design it differences develop in the predicted direction. 2) Both 3 and 11 seconds represent equal changes in time from 7 seconds and should constitute approximately an equal amount of generalization decrement, if any. Bidirectional 59 ‘gencralization gradients are typically symetrical. It must be understood that regardless of the time used, either 3 or 11 seconds, the §g|were not forced to remain in the goal for any length of time. They were simply allowed more or lees time in which to remain in the presence of the goal cues. Phase 3 was identical to Phase 1 with the sole exception that Phase 3 was ten days long. fhat is, each snimel was given ten daily sessions, one session per day. Phase 3 with its return to e 7 second predator delay was used to allow the groups to return to their response rates achieved towards the end of Phase 1. ihis was necessary too, as Yhase 4 or Reoxtinction constituted a test of the reliability of the results obtained in Phase 2. In Phase 4, reinforcement was not available. Group A which had received a 5 second delay during fihase 2 now received an 11 second delay. Group B-which had received an 11 second delay in Phase 2, now received e 3 second delay. By reversing the conditions for the two experimental groups from those imposed during Phase 2, two things were accomplished. first, this procedure was a stringent test of the hypothesis. If the results were again in the pre- dicted direction, this would be interpreted as strong support for the hypothesis and eliminate the possibility that differences found in Ehsse 2 were due to some uncontrolled differences between the groups. Secondly, the reversal of 60 conditions made pooeible a within-subgects analysis. Excegt for the differencee.in procedure diecueoed above, all other procedures need in this experiment were identical to thoee used in Experiment I. F .USULTS fC DIS CU'TSIOH‘V Thad data are prasented in gr9phlo and tabular form at the and of this section 911d in Appendix II. Figure VI 51999 the r99p0n9e rate for Groups A and B aoroas aeaaions of the four experimental phases. Analysiu of variance of the responae rates of Group A versus Group B fiurin 9 Ph1se 2 shows that the two groups did not differ significantly in resistance to extinction (see the analysis of variance Table IX). By this te9t, the second hypothesis was not supported. TABLE 9' analysis of variance (Efiwa.rda, 1960) of Group A versua Group 3 r9. 990999 rata across Phase 2. frcatment 198.80 1 128.60 .40 Error 2559.22 8 319.53 39991099 1036. 97 14 79.16 7.999 Treatment X Sesaiona 145 70 14 15.39 1.44 Error 112 11.L Total 49 ‘aignifioant at the .01 level The treatment mean and 9t9nd9rd deviation for Group 9 are fi’u 8.04, 99-” 3.417. For Group B they are :1 . .19, CD a 2.693. The significant 999910n9 effect indicates that when tha treatmen t effect 19 aver9ged acro99 sessions there in an effect peculiar to sessions alone. Inspection of tha Fhaae a graph in Figure VI suggestea a negatively accelerated function usually associated with extinction. 61 62 Regardless of the fact that for 11 of the 15 sessions Group A.maaho were higher than Group 3 means, the treatment effects ware insufficient to produce a significant difference. Inspection of the graph of Phase 2 in Figure VI shows that the greatast absolute difference between group means occured during the first session. A T-tost of thcec two meana'gavo a value of T=1.405 with 8 dofo which is not significant at the .05 level. Thus. for the first session of Phase 2, the results were in the predicted direction but not to a significant extent. ‘ Turning to Phase 4, Figure VI shows that for at least ‘tho first session, the results are again in the predicted direction. The data. also, took a form very much like the first fivc sessions of Phase 2. Inspection of the data indi- outed once more that if there was any significant difference betweon the group means it would be in the first session. A T-toat of these two neaoo gave a value of T=.846 with 8 d.f. which was not even significant at the .20 level. Thus. for the first session of 11131 9, the results were in the prefiictcd but to no significant extent. A further analysis of the response rate data was made by using each g as its own control. That is, each 99} rea- ponac rate under the 3 second delay condition was matched against its resyonoe rate for the 11 second condition. A matched-pairs T-test was used to analyze. This gave a TC 9 2.04 with 9 d.£. which is significant at the .10 level but 63 not at the .05 level. hith individual differences controlled, the orpected trend is more apparent. The lack of clearly significant results not withetenéing, the response rate data euggeet that predicted effecte are real. The effect may be only of short duration. The effect is replicable if Phase 4 is consioered e replication of Phase 2. The difficulty appears to lie in an ineufficient number of subjects, as the main effects are obscured by a high degree of voriebility. Inspection of the re3ponec rate data from rheee l inci— cates a great similarity to the results obtained in rheae 3 of Experiment I. This suggests that there was no savings for the fig in Experiment I at the beginning of Phase 3. This was not the case for 23 in Phase 3 of Exreriment II. The combined mean reeponee rate of Groups i and B for the first session of Phase 1 of Experiment II is ? =-3.9. The combined mean response rate of Groups A and B to: the first o'coion of Phase 3 of Experiment II is ‘ a 16.7. This gave a eevinge score of 303% in terme of response rate. A matched-pairs T- teet of the mean response rates of the first sessions of Phases 1 and 3 gave a Td a 4.870 with 9 d.f. A T-retio of this line ie significant at the .CCl level. Inspection of hence 2 end 4 hi the response rates for the first eeeeiore of also Buggected a savings. The mean combined rccronee rate for Groups A and B for session 1 of Phase 2 is _ x 15.9. For .li' 64 the first cccoiou of Phase 4, the moon is x a 8.9. Skis represents a éfifi savings for the first session. A matched- paira T-toat of the near rccponcc rates gave a Td a 2.609. With a 9 d.f. the T-valuc is significant at the .05 level. These savings represent a significant amount of positive transfer. The following analysis is of particular importanccibr the major hypothesis. In Figures VIIa and VIIb, the graphs for Ihaoe 2 and 4 show a difference in the moan total time spent in the goal region between the group receiving the 3 second and the group receiving the 11 second delay concition. This difference is seen more clearly in Figure VIII. iho graphs of the mean time in the goal per response for Fhaaeo 2 and 4 Show that when a group is allowed more time in the goal region, moré time is spent there. Also, the graphs for Phases 2 and 4 of Figure VIII show that the two curves, session for session. arc about equally displaced from the 7 second point. On the ave~c5e, a group under the 11 second condition spent less than the full delay interval in the gocl region per response. while a group under the 3 second condition Spent moro than the delay interval. In line with the argument put forward in the discussion, it 13 suggested that comething less than 11 seconcs was ourficient for the maguitudo of the frustration response to increase erough beyond previously conditioned lcvols so that the §§ spontaneourly fled the 65 goal region. Under the 3 second delay condition, on any one trial. the magnituae of the frustration responae éid not increase sufficiently to result in the §§_spontaneou31y fleeing the goal region. This brings up the queotion of why there woo no decline of time in the goal region per reayonse in Phase 2 (Group A). bxya + .270; (Grouy B), bxyg +.230. In Phase 2, the responoo of remaining in‘the goal upon entering may be more resistant to extinction than the entry reoponoe. lhis would seem reesona le assuming that the alley cues decline in approach eliciting power in relation to their dis once from the @031. Therefore, as extinction progresood, the ayproach gradient would collapse towards tho goal. The negative slopes seen in the Phage 4 graph of Figure VIII (Group l, b = ~.230; Group B, b -.280) may be the result of xy xyu repeated extinction making the functional reinforcing pro- perties of the conditioned elicitors generally more vunerahle to the extinction process. This is all very hypo— thetical, but worth conaidering for future research. In the later sesoiona of Phase 1 and throughout Those 3 there was a decided increase in the mean time in the goal ‘ region per response beyond the 7 second delay period. Tnis 13 the some effect as seen in Phase 3 of Exporiment I and probably occuro for the same reasons previously prepoood. Table 1 gives the freouoncy of shocks by soosion for Groups A and B. Inspection of tho shook frequencies for 66 Phases 2 and 4 show that any éecreased zesista co to extinn W ction shown by the 35 under the 11 second delay conéition 1'“) cannot be attributed to a higher ahock frequency. Ehe _3 under the 3 second delay conditionr Vlvays received tho are eat~ est amount of ehocks. Obsezmx tiona of the So behavior indicates that a hesitancy to retreat from the goal region or healtanoy to enter the Qbre box was the usual reason on g receivoé a shook.l This behavior was menife: to d more freq.uen tLy i.n the group unfier the 3 second conoition comcared to the group under the 11 second condition. This differential amount of hesitancy may have a common cause with the rasponoo of re“ maini ng in he goal region upon entry. The reasons for this were proviouoly éiscusaed. Beyond the differentm 13 in observed behavior patterns reported abovo, the behavior of both groups during Phases 1 and 3 closely resembled the behavior observed for tho §§_ in Phaée 3 of Experiment I. There was an ezual 01 Hit? (II- between observed behavior of the go qu i '3 .5 Fhuae d of Experiment I ano the behavior of the fig in Experiment II during Phage 2 axtd 4. With the exception of the di forential gi- tendency to hcs1tant retreats ano trnder:: oy to (toy in la 1"! 3031 region, war no'; able to (18cc1n up; reliable difference ( - ‘ in the manifestation of fear or frustration between Groups A and B. MEAN RESPONSE RATE MEAN RESPONSE RATE 67 .300 I I I I I I I I I I I I I I r /\/ 2°”? PHASEI ‘ Io.o_. _ \J 0 11111141111111 OI23456789IOIII2I5I4IS SESSION .300 I I I I I I I I II 20.0 /\ _ pj/ \, /\ loo _ s PHASE3 O LIIIIIllll 0|23456759I0 SEsyoN GrogpA' Figure VI. of EXpsrimont II. 3&0 ’I’I I I I I T I I I I I I T"T W- PHASE 2 IQO _ o I I l I I I I I I I I I I I O I 2 3 4 S 6 7 8 9IOIII2I3I4I5 SESSON 300 I I I I I 20.0 _ .I PHASE4 I0.0 _\ .. \ o I I I I L O I 2 3 4 S SESSION Group 8 -—-~—-— Mean response rate per session of the four phases MEAN TOTAL TIME (Sec) MEAN TOTAL TIME (Sec) 600 500 400 300 200 I00 68 PHASE I O I Z 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 IO ll I2 I3 l4 I5 300 £00 IOO SAFE 50X I J l I l l l I l l l l 2 3 4 5 6 7 ‘e 9 I0 II I2 I3 I4- I5 SESSION Group A ——- Group B __ Figure VIIa. Mean total time spent in the safe box, alley, and goal region per session of Phases 1 and 2 of Experiment II. 69 600 500 ._ a O O I (A O O l MEAN TOTAL TIME (Sec.) '8 O I SAFE BOX PHASE 3 I00 _. .. a l J I I I I l l l I O I Z 3 4 5 G 7 8 9 Io SESSION 600 I I ,2.___'.——:=_""I‘ W 500_. ./ _ ’1‘ 3400 *' SAFE sax - {’3 u300 2 PHASE 4 I- _J 200 __ _ < ’— O I»- ma _ _+ z ( “J 2 O L l l 1 1 O I 2 3 4 S SESSION Group A ———- Group B __ Figure VIIb. Mean total time spent in the safe box, alley, and goal region per session of Phases 3 and h of EXperiment II. MEAN TIME IN GOAL PER RESPONSE (scc) MEAN TIME IN GOAL PER RESPONSE (8“) I 5.0 I0.0 70' OLIIIIllllllllll OI234sc7eemnmwum sesmou ISO IIFTFITTI PHASES Iw_ . W LAvJ _________________ wL _ o I I I I I I I I I I OI25456789m SESSION Group A I50 II I0.0 TTIIIIrIIIITII PHASE 2 _----———————.———_——n_--.—.————-——_--.-- 11111141111111 OI 254 5 6789IOIIIZI3I4I5 I50 II 10.0 50 0 SESSION IFIWT PHASE 4 -—--———-————q 1L111 O I 2 3 4 5 SESSION Group B -——--——- Figure VIII. Mean time in the goal region per response per session of the four phases of EXperiment II. The broken, horizontal lines at the 3, 7, and 11 second marks indicate the time of onset or the mechanical predator from the time of an gig entry into the goal region, depending on the group and the phaselsee the text). 71 0.6 $6 0.6 won wow mo+ o.» N.m new ecn weasozoo .Hfl Mum 93.5 fin. «a we we to in ad 3. 3. a: 3...:28 4s m... .325 Ilqm. m .19 H m m eIINIIMIIIwI .3339“ wig .6 ex. e.» e4 0A n4 mg m.“ mam m.” or.“ In a. e. a. message Jam .m muons .. e o.» «A. In O; 0.6 e. n ed ON Ga 04 .3 a. a. e. 3538 Jan .4 goes .HH 383.:th «O n 2.3 H mesefim magma m 9.3 < smack... no ab :23" a: nowumaaeace have) seem "I .2 IV .‘vl. .I\ 72‘ A o H H H ”enough.“ Neonm ”m M593 0 o H o 0 Omega.» MOO d. :93 m e n M a mmowmamm «Iummmm N a o a e w m a v w hasaaeosu neon“ .m geese v m u H n m u HI o H hoseuasmmImmmmm .a apnea Ids. m m r m m w n m a 0 on ”Iummmm O o o o o o o o M a o H o a o nos-seoua use.» .n means . , : homage .4 98.3 MIumuaM a m o o a o a m. H a m H. n . n «a nesuavsnu gamma .n anonu muummmqu VIYII. . I 4.... . I! ,I I~ ah LIBI:T III Due to the failure to obtain statistically eignifice-t results in Experiment II, Experiment II was replicated in this experiment. The number of subjects was increased. Subjegtg, The §g_were l4 naive female hooded rate from the colony maintained by the Departmentof Psychology of Michigan State University. All §§,were from 110 to 120 days Old at the start of the experiment. Beginning two weeks prior to the start of the experi- ment. each.§,uas handled for five minutes a day. Beginning one week prior to the start of the experiment, §g,were placed on a water deprivation schedule of ten minutes of access to water in the individual home cages every ZI-houre. Food was continuously available. Also, during the lent week prior to the beginning of the eXperiment, g; were, individually allowed ten minutes per day to explore the exyerimental apparatus and drink in the apparatus. A arat 9. ins apparatus used in Experiments I and II was M used in this exPeriment without modification. Procedure. The procedure for this experiment wee identical to that need in Experiment II with but one exception. In the present OXperiment, Phases 2, 3 and 4 (Extinction, Ho- acquietion, and Re-extinction respectively) were all reduced 73 74 in length to five days each. That is. five daily sessions per each g, one session per day. This reduction in phase length was justified by the results from Experiment II. Exporbment II results showed that if significant differences were to occur in Phases 2 and 4, they would occur during the first few days. EIperiment II also showed that no more than five days were needed for Phase 3 to return ggbto their steady response rates obtained at the end of Phase 1. It was apparent from Experiment II that Phase 1 or Acquisition had to remain fifteen days long. As in Experiment II. at the end of Phase 1. the 14 fig were divided into two groups. Each group had approximately the name mean response rate corona oeeeione. Again, the groups were assembled from.peirs of §§,more or less matched for mean response rate across sessions (see Table III below and Figure in the results section). A responee was de- fined as a traverse from the safe box into the goal region. 75 TABLE 11; Group composition in terms of the mean response rate across Phase 1. ‘ W? 1; w: W... 5”... 1.1219 D 1 __ :3. §EEJ£££.:§F33 r9399fifli_£§£2_.._ »1§221§££ Roan geaponag rate 1 "" 17080 P: '- 21047 2 - 14.47 9 - 14.40 3 - 12.73 10 - 13.20. 4 - 11.93 11 - 9.53 5 ” 9.00 12 ¢ 9.53 6 "" 8080 13 "' 600° 7 - .2322 14 - 5,60 ‘ 77093 ' 79073 I a 11.13 I =- 11.39 Each group was then randomly assigned to the experimental conditions in Phase 2. EESULTS goo DISCUfiSIGH The date are presented in graphic and tabular form at the end of this oeeoion and in Appendix III. Figure IX .giveo the response rate for Groups C and D eoroae eeoeione or the four experimental phases. As in lxyerimeet II, inspec- tion of the graph of Phase 2 in Figure II shows that the greatest éifference between group means occured, no might be expected during the first session. A Tnteet of these two means ave a value 1:1.703 with 12 d.£., thio difference is not signi- ficant at the .05 level. A likely reason for this nonsignificant result lies in the response rote of one § of Group C. Thio a made only 1 reoponoe during the first eeeoion. The response ratee for the remaining §§_of Group I in the first session ranged from 13 to 24. The response rate of the one g badly skewed the data. khan the date from g with the lowest responne rate was discarded from Eggg Groups 0 and D, a T- teet of the reoulting two means (Group C, Y'2 17.8}: Group B X‘z 10.83) gave a T32.79 which with 10 d.f. is significaot at the .02 level. Thus. for the first session of Phase 2. the rooulto wore in the predicted direction and were eigni- rioant when the data of a highly Coviont g were excluded. Turning to Phase 4, Figure IX shows that the results are again in the predicted direction. Once again, the greatest @ifference between group means occured during the first seeeion. i T-teet of theae two moane gave a value 76 77 of Ta 3.20 which with 12 d.f. is significant at the .01 level. To be consistent with the analyeie in EIperiment II, the data from the §,with the lowest reoponae rate for this session was discarded from.§g§h Groups 0 and D. The two lowest performing §g_1n Phase 4 were not the some two lowest performing §g,in Phase 2. After the removal of the data for the two poorest performing §ghin session 1 of Phase 4, the mean for Group 6 become 7.33 versus the previoue mean of 6.71. For Group D, tho mean became 15.6? versus the previoue mean of 14.71. A intact or these reeulting tub memos gave a in 3.10 with.10 d.t.'uhioh is significant at the .02 level. Thus. for the first session of Phase 4. the results were clearly in the predicted direction. The second hypothesis was supported. A further anelyeia or the response rate data was made by using each.§ as its own control (No data were excluded). That is, eachwfiflg response rate under the 3 second delay condition was matched against its response rate for the 11 second condition. A natched~paire T-teet was need to analyze the reeulte. This gave a Tia 3.64 with 13 d.f. which is significant at the .01 level. This matched-paire analysis clearly supports the predicted effect. Inspection of the response rate data from Phase 1 indicateo a great similarity to the reeulte obtained in Phone 3 of Experiment I and Phase 1 of Experiment II. In this orperiment as in Experiment II, there is some savings 78 in roaoquioition. The combineo mean rosponee rate of Groups C an& D for the first session of Phase 1 of EXperimcnt III is Kin 3.93. The combineo mean response rate or Groupe C and D for the first eeeeicn of Phase 3 of Experiment III is 3'2 10.76. This represents a aavinge of 288% in terms of reopenee rate. A matched—pairs Tuteat of the mean response ratee of the first oeoaione or Phaeee l and 3 (using each 3 an its own control) gave 3 Ti 3 3.205 with 13 d.f. which is significant at the .01 level. ‘Inepeotion of the response rates for the first eeeeione of Pheeee 2 and 4 revealed only a 14% savings. Compare this savings with the 44% savinge round in Experiment 11. A matchedwpaire Tnteat between Phases 2 and 4 of Experiment III gave a Ta u .675. d.f. a 13. The 14% savings represents a nonoignificent savings in reeponfiing. Probably the shortening of Phase 2 (or extinction) from Ezporiment II to Experiment III accounts for the appreciable lose in savings. The five ecseione of Phase 2 in Experiment 111 may have been an insufficient number of sessions to allow the doveloyment of an appreciable amount of positive trane~ fer. A3 in Exyeriment II, the nelyeie of tire measures it of particular importance for the major hypothesis. ¥igure 1 presents the mean total time spent in the goal region for the group receiving the 3 second delay ccnd tion and the group receiving the 11 eeccnd dele‘. The difference noted here we: also econ in Experieent II. Thie filiference can be 79 better appreciated in Figure XI. The graphs of the mean time in the goal per response for Phases 2 and 4 show, as they did in Experiment II, that when a group is allowed more time in the goal region, more time is spent there. In Figure II. the two curves are not quite so equally displaced from the 7 aocond point as was the case in EXporimont II. However, the effects were very similar. The 11 second group again spent less than the full delay interval in the goal region not rooponao while the group under the 3 second condition Spent more than the delay interval. As in Experiment II, the eons explanation could apply: Hamely, that on ll second delay is sufficient timo for the frustration response to increase beyond the previously conditioned levels, while a 3 second delay is not., One of the moat interesting aspects of the extinction phones for those data is that the time in goal par rooponoo tends to inoreaoa rather than decrease. This in especially true in Phase 4 for the 11 oecond delay group (Group C). The inoreaae in goal time in Group C in Phase 4 is possible be- cauao the predator does not otart moving until after an ll second ficlay. Why goal time per response increases when there in no water available can only be explained, in part at least, by tho assumption that staying in tho goal is atill the strongest or propotont response in this stimulus situation. The slope constants for tho curves soon in the graph of Phase 2 in Figure KI are bx a + .110 for Group C and y 80. bxy a + .08: for orcpp r. These'alight positive slopes might suggest that there was little it any margin for further confiitioning at the outset of Phase 2. Tho slight negative 510pe computed for the Group D curve of time in tho goal region per response in Fhaso 4 suggests that since this group started out spending a deal of time in the goal per response compared to Group C in Phaso 2, Group D'o performance may have been near maximal in the first session nné thus had no place to go but Gown. As in Exporimont II, in the final four sessions of Phase 1 and throughout Phase 3, their is a reliable increase in the mevt time spent in the goal region per response beyond the 7 second delay period. Once again, this is the some effect as oeon in ?hase 3 of prerinent I. Reasons for this were proposed in Experiment I. Table XII gives the frequency of shocks for each session anfi each phone for Groups 0 and D. As in Experiment II, inspection of the shock frequencies for Phones 2 and 4 again showa that decreased resistrfico to extinction shown by the _ §§,un&cr the ll aocond delay condition cannot be attributed to a higher shook frequency. Tho‘fig under the 3 oec.nd delay c ndition always received the greatest number of shocks during the firot oeooicn and too entire phase. A3 in Experiment II, the observations of £9 behavior again inéicated that hesitoncy in retreating rem the goal region and entering the safe box 81 were major raascna for an 83 receiving a shock. Again, tha obmerved differential in amount of hesituncy was seen. Finally, g was not able to discern any reliable difference in the manifestation of fear or frustration between Groups C and D. £32 zo.mmum zo_mmwm zo.mmmm n v n N . o m ¢ n w 0 ¢ n m _ o _ q ‘ fl ‘d 0 4w 4 _ _ o 1‘ _ _ _ o q\anHHHH”//\\\\ 11/ u //// .1o. I o. _\ .10. m um/\\ M510 I0. 0. fl JO. 3 W ................................ : I-------I--------.-II----:----I : C .v wm c.m v.m >.¢ m.~ moasmmao .Ha_w W VII m. m H rmowmvoc nIcmwxm m.m m.m o.m $.é o.m m.m n.m n.m w.m ¢.m m.m m.“ H. o. o.H moasmsoo .Ha.w o.m m.m ~.m o.m 0.5 m.m ~.m m.¢ h.m N.m m. o.~ m. 0.0 H.H neawmnoo .fla m ma ¢fiIIIwH «a: HflIIIoH m w p w w v n m a rccflmme H mmmdrm n ass a ommnm maauzc a ans 0 .HHH pgmafihwmxm mo QSOHO M0.MW hp aoHS ch fioavmfifimmoo Hmwma nabm .m among .0 macho .Q Queue .0 nacho 91’. I... I.o” .‘u .2. 86 VN'?‘\O 3" N 3 r"! H ("I "\J H H H F" vi “vac: «\nn4 anca wflmcs “(V r“! O {\J 1-! Cd {\3 (“Jr-4 IIIIIIII (JIM N houmswwgu MocAw mumomrmum Moon; II!!! is. mxcwwnou HON o hoiwsammm Manw o hoxwfiwmmm goon; H muowmmmm hommnwmuw Moon: mommfivmuw Xuosw euomeum HHO mahocmsawmw ”conm wfihusmfimmmu Moozm H anofimmmm .HHH pcmafiuomum mo momwam axon 98» unfladv Q and o masons no mMWhn no>woomu whocmm mo hummuwoum bHH mam¢w Q vi: mfioku Adena macaw macho macaw awoke muonu nachu 1 'f. . u . ‘I c. ,. . I I . . . I. u. ~V' . . 111‘ 1....I it :0 I1 u. . . Q r 0‘ . A . . .1 ’I ni“ III! ' I . . t. .. a . If .. . P QOIQLUSIOfiS RED SUfiHiRY In the introduction to this study, a model was presented for approach learning and extinction in prcy argoniame co 3 function of repeated predatory attacks. The learning model involved the assumption that the booic task confronting the prey organism is to learn to approach a region of the environ~ ment to obtain needed reinforcement and then flee this came region to avoid the direct physical attack of on approaching predator. The roeponoe of approaching the goal, it was argued, is conditioned to the ctimuli accompanying the emotional responoee of fear and frustration. Fear is elicited by the operation of the predator. After a sufficient number of approaches to the goal region, fear-produced etimuli serve as partial discriminative stimuli eliciting the approach response. Fructration is elicited by the termination of positive reinforcement necessitated by the prey leaving the source of reinforcement in orécr to avoid the predator’s attack. As with fear, after a sufficient number of approaches to the goal. fruetraticn-produced stimuli serve as partial diocrimw inative stimuli eliciting the approach stimuli reoponoo. Two predictiono were mace concerning resistance to extinction. The preaiction was made that prey without prior experience in approaching the reinforced region under threat of preéntory attack would chow leoo reciotnnce to extinction 87 n b 80 than had they have such prior experience. Alec. it was predicted, from the model, that prey allowed less time in the goal region prior to the onset of predatory attack would show more resistance to extinction of the approach response compared to prey having more time in the goal region prior to predatory attack. These predictions were supported by the results. The nodal makes use of apparently justified assumption that under appropriate learning conditions. normally noxiouo and cvereive stimuli can come to serve no stimuli helping to increase resistance to extinction. Convincing evidence for this assertion in to be had by noting thst in Experiments II and III the groups which show the greatest resistance to extinction are also the groups which receive the greatest number of checks. The results supporting the two hypotheses and the other results which case to light during this study must be generalized with great caution. Direct application of the present findings to the field must be guite tenetive. However, two conclusions seen telly warranted. First, this study has set forth e means of eyetemstically studying a couple: problem. The problem is to find a way to evaluate the type and form of learning which a prey organism displays as it learns to cope with its predators. It is felt that this study represents a step in the right direction. The basic took confronting the subjects in the present study may bo limited in scope in terms of all possible confrontations of prey and predator, but the situation is readily modifiable along several dimensions. These dimensions are, for instance: The special relationship of cafe region, goal, and intorvon mg Space; the initial locus of the predator; the detectability of the predator before and after the beginning of its approach towards the prey orginioms the stimuli characteriz¢ ing the predator such as size, speed of a promch, typo of stimuli signaling opproach of the predator: the possibility of varying the type of and number of alternative paths leading to and from the goal region; variation in the type of reinforcement and deprivation conditions of the prey; and of course. the type of prey organiom used. The second point is that an attempt has been made to formulate the problem. ibis formulation or theorizing gives a structured framework for future research in the fluid. Tho present study could serve to focus field efforts towards the gathering of detailed information on the continuing behavior of individual prey organisms. This study could servo to equip the observer with a set of expectations on the learning of prey organisms. Theoc expectations could then be supported, modified, or dioconfirmed by careful obacrvationo in the field. By thin neana a reciprocal ro- lationohip between laboratory and field work might be developed. Shear common sense would dictate that the conditions confronting the prey orgauiom in the field are much less c: O stable than in the laboratory. Lvenoo, some tentative suggestions can be put forth from the results of the present 'hyporotable' study. It in suggeotod that increased resistance to extinction of opproach to a region previously associated with reinforcement might prove to be adaptive for the prey organism. This might be so in that the con- ditions which make reinforcement available in the first place may repeat themselves. Thio would make it unnecessary for the organism to search new and wider arena for other sources of reinforcement. This effect might help explain territor- iality in orgenioms which have fairly demarooted foraging ranges. Also, increased resistance to extinction under conditions of short delay in predatory attack compared to long delays might prove adaptive. More returns to a previously reinforced region could disclose other eourceo of reinforcement close to the original region. Also, through the conditioning of approach response to fear-frustration stimuli it 16 possible that a prey organism may be more resistant to abandoning new, potential sources of food or water (reinforcement) if it is attacked in this region while being reinforced. These suggestions are put forth to show further avenues of study in the area of modification of prey behavior through the medium of predator encountera. Finally, it is hoped that the present study will alert 91 other workers to the need to View behavior modification in light of its adaptive significance for the organism display- ing the behavior change. In the last analysis, learning should be viewed as one way an organism deals with a hostile environment. nag-13 pjytwnyn?q .~ '4! .‘f;..'..,.;. "v _.L,j ‘Amsel, A. Tkie role of frustrative nonreward in noncontinuous reward situations. Pczchol. Bu11., 1J38, £2, 102-119. .Amsel, A. Frustrative nonreward in partial reinforcement and discrimination learning: Some recent history and a ghgoreticel extension. Fsvchol. ReV., 1962, fig, 306- _t_. *'*“**"‘“*' Azrin, N.H. Punishment and recovery fluring fixed-ratio performance. 1, E312.Anc1. I? er sv.. 1359, L, 301~335. Azrin, 3.3. Effects of punishment intenaity during variable- interval reinforcement. 1. exz. gag}, §g§g1.. 1:60. 2. 123-142. Azrin, L.H. and H012, w.c. Punishment during fixed-interval reinforcement. i. exp. Anal. EQhéng, 1961, 1, 345-347. Brown, J.. flsrtiu, R.C.. and Morrow, Mu. colt-punitive behavior in the rat: Facilitative effects of punishmezt on resistance to extinction. i, 20mg. pgxeiol. £3 1chol.. 1&6“. 21' 147’133. Brown, R.T. and tagner, 1.3. Resistance to punishment and extinction following training with shock or nonreinforce- lent. Q, exg, Esxcho;.. 1964, gg, 503-507. Calhoun, J.B. ‘ e ” ~ of the Forwey Rat. weshingtonz rubl'o flea th service Pablication so. luofi, 1‘64“ Church. R.M. The varied effects of punishment on behavior. 22mm” 1963. 12. 369-462. Curti. R.W. Native tear responses of white rats in the presence of sets. gszchol. Monog.. 1955. 5Q, 78-98. Denny, M.R. One bar-press per day: Acquisition and extinction. g, exg. Ana . behev., 1959,‘§. 61-85. Denny, H.R. and Adelmen, H.H. Elicitation Theory II: The formal theory and its application to instrumental escape and avoidance conéitioning. unpublished theoretical paper, nichigen State University, 1956. Denny, h.R. and Aeismrn, h.G. Avoidance behavior so a function of lenrth of nonshock confinement. 1, come. lb hgelol. Pszclol., 1964, fig, ESP-257. . . . ‘ . 6 ‘, I . I U a 0 V Edwards. AOL. Row York: Ritehart and 00., 1960. Elton, 0.3. The use of cats in farm rat control. Brit. 1, “Rig. B8h3Vo. 1955. 13 151-155. Forster, 0.3. Withdrawal of positive reinforcement as punish- Ferster, 0.8. Control of behavior in chimpanzees and pigeons by time out from positive reinforcement. Pczchcl. gram-2., 1958, 12; (8, whole to. 461.) Griffith, C.R. The behavior of white rats in the presence of cats. Psxchobiol., 1920, g, 19-28 (abstract) Guinn, G.T. The effects of punishment on acts motivated by fear. i2 exo. Psvohol., 1949, 23, Eco—£69. fiedeger, 3. Wild Animals in Captivitz, London: Butterworth, 1950. Hinds, R.A. Factors governing the changes in strength of a partially inborn response, as shown by the mobbing behavior of the chaffinch (Fringills coelebs) III. The interaction of short and long term incrementsl and decremental effects. £239. £21. Soc. §., 1961, 153, 3id“4£00 H012, W.C. and Arrin, H.H. Discriminative preperties of punishment. i, cry. Anal. Behav., 1961, i, 2:5-232. Eull, C.L., Learning: 11 The factor of the confiitioned reflex. In C. Murchison (ed.). Handbook of genera; Er eri ante szcgolggz. Worcester; Clerk Univer. rress, Joslin, 5., Fletcher, H., and Emlen, J. A comparison of the response to snakes of lob~uni wild-reared Rhesus monkeys. Animal Bohev., 1964, lg, 346-356. Keehn, J.D. The effect of a warning signal on unrestricted avoidance behavior. Brit. 1, Psvchol., 1959, 22. 1¢5*1550 Melee k, K. On the survival of mallard ducks after 'hebituation' to the hawksheped figure. Behavior, 1961, $1: 9’16. 34 Melvin, K.B. eni Brown, J.S. fieutrwllzation of on aversive light stimulus as a function of number of paired presentations with food. g, 0033. h-eiol. Pevczcl., Earth, A.J. and Morton, m.L. Succeeeive acquisition and extinctions of an instrumental response. i. 0333. physiel. Psychol.. 1962. 55. fl74-377. Pavlov, I.P. genditioned Reflexes. London: Oxford Univer~ airy Preee., 19:7. Richerdeon, W.B. Reaction towards snakes as shown by the Wood Rat (Restore elbignia) g, coma. Fovcho;.. 1942, 34, 1-10. Sandler, J. Moeochiem: in empirical analysis. Psxcicl. 31111.; 1954, fig. 197-3304. Seton, E.T. Lives 9! GameewAnimele, 3 vol. newton Center, Masc.8 Chirlee T. bronford, 1353. Sideen, M. and Boron, J.J. A comparison of two types of warning stimulus in on avoidance situation. i, 2332. ghycicl. Fojchg;., 1957, 2;, 282~287. Simmons. K.E.L. Ehe nature of the predator~reectione of waders towards humans; with Special reference to the role of the aggressive, escape and brooding drives. Bohgvior, 1955. Q, 150-173. Spence, K.W. Behav or The and Learning. Englewood Cliffs, N. .a 'rentice~ all, 1930. Wynne-Edwarie, v.0. Animal Diepereicn in Relation to Social Behovior. Eflinburgh: Oliver and Boyd, 1962. . .Q.‘ ,V’r‘ 0. oh. vu W Cu , a4” ‘ O“ .H O c O 7D b- O W n ’n O J a .H . . “J. .. . u L. H. u. . c .5. a... ”no 7.. .. 4‘ - l , 4 ,A. II I. a ‘ v a a .. n . . . w ; u n o D." . o \ o u Q 1 - 1 1 . a . V g . . r J.“ g E31, 9. .2 .. X ,n . ..,J.f..;r - q a - é..- . 3 v 1:593.» J. ‘ m. i. p a V : {ng n .. o .. , a..¢ 1 6‘ mt}? a in ‘ a»: .W .V. » . t f :1 A.‘ H1 4". \ I 0V .. \< . J x I u a o o . ~ ( f. a . Mn . a 3%.. .4 - 3...... 2‘ r...“ v .. .. L .l . w. .’ ,W .. 4 1.. O . . . . O . ) ... .y u a~ i. ,LD , 1 ‘ u 4 .ow .1 n . J 01”.; A I 05W: A (al.\ . \ .‘4 V c u‘ ‘4 . 44,4 ~ u. .\ (1 a u 4 .41.“ .jH _ .\2 . M." ed Hog H «no-«nomnm we nunuam .33 on». no 330.2. ”on :33 033:... 1; u.- unouauuuou vangua- and H ”Hagan. once: ‘.u.\, .bt’...‘.. S . t . . I. :1 . .01 t. . C .3. a .. \ Q 0 a. 4 u... . .I 1 1.! 8 ¢ . I . o . \ . ‘ T » A .. ‘ I. . . l 3!, .. v . o 9 ' Lu . 1 . .k‘ . L D O . I 9 . 1 A . V ‘ l .. .ul \IO nan. A . . n . 1 O F 4 I l . o o I o I I I ' n .33» _ J ‘ J ._ q a e. . . m. .- .Kiwawl..rnfl+tl;fli wlle... ... m . I4.m'.«fl.l.mi!i. ll..f.l.ii.mti. l. 3 in. H .n mm: .3 .. NI'H¢.AI'B'~t' (aid at Eryiflatfl . u . O4. , . .0 ad dd 4% 2 Na d «D! . . .rflofiq «4. . .... .. , . .lr. 1,0? o . q 04.1 m a . .. L... ~32 N - 9..-.-m. wn.w._mnw_.w:.swflwsr . m 3a . Agliléi ..« w , . atlagbrfl~cfivfin . . vag «ggnggvgv 5K...» Expat! Kufitafl. Ea: , OMKQJWI'AKImK an“?! KJfiimKvaitr E.. EHJLHKHL an a a.“ n... c . o .1 v. . u we} . mm m.” a mu .3 Wm fl “I! a w w m c m m. a % Human." HH 335$ no 3333 55% 3». Ho nod-non “on n 28 4 33.8 «a seam." amino.“ 3», no uaowadatu 235$- 23 Bio! HH gnu; cl IL ,L H 5. .r’ . “. .\. .l.‘ « ,. v .. .. Lu MU_. ”n... we Wu“. w... Uop‘ maxim}: ., \Jifl": a. 2L. 4 m 4w m , ‘w‘ H amowmn w @wfinm n magma a. mu W M N H ”fiat—”mm, JANA-Q V . . mmdw w mm. meu MH.¢ Qw .L x V. \F U. 11.3N0’Jl’. .. O H w:.: a r. cc 4 mm m tw.n mack uni \ “.1 )JU ‘ RN10 1. 1.”, M \r.« ‘ ,;V H “I” . Np a «5; w« c a Mud; .hmcr M .q :9 PER punt Jr. a“ ‘ . H m m .H a much \ 11 w m w mmufimmm H manna HHH aumawhegxm mo aewmna know an» no mowmamu 969 Q vcm o mmfionu Ho moawu @mzoammn an» we acoaawfib¢v vhmcamum was «swam HHH NHQWflmmd