‘1'.“ . N J v “.15. {3.252751%} 7.5» wwmfifljm my. , ‘ ‘42! 4 5.... ‘. 3‘ 9. .5?!“ u v . ‘ lyrihf'. .‘m x 7. '3“ .. z"- '4 .. -‘r. ‘ 15‘ .a £5"le ' m. w . . 4.. v- .-.~;. 5.5. Iii” :- ' ' '< '1' fizz-'4“ .1}. H ~21“?! ‘ .21 “Iva 1215.523 ' T. k IjizdéL 3‘“:qu “ xsfizgri‘ 22;..17212 ,2 ”‘ , Kz-HEEK 713% .. ,7” 1‘1pr :hénq’d. Etiéluuy 'I‘ .1- II [313,313“. I. ”a ‘ We'ik l i ~r’Q‘ s {'2 "3‘st ”35% ‘13.“ I ‘ 3:5? '11 11;: . 1‘" {£15. H131 M' "E71: ' I - . E «5:3: ,:.. Wat-1):; ”"7519; 1 . f . ,1», . .z, 2?... , is :ng .‘WL‘ '3‘“: "‘12 21- mfiwm . ~. ~- 1. -‘ ._' - “w -. 3“» 1;» "mfg P51“ ‘4 . . 1:: 3:: + 3,12 - ' 23%;! ~évff3'1v @' L 3% EEK 3' Riv " ‘ " J'gfi-Al‘n 72.2.7... 7 m 113:},1» “:3 — 7.7+ M 1 It?!" . “'3 1 NE; N§gp MM“, M11131“ " '“nxr‘1*}"“' is“? 1". ' ‘1‘“? ”3:3“ {WK-717*” :37 - ' ." V Yatgxiffi mfif wan ~ 7 7:1 QEWW Vii“! . “1‘21 .93 46“.: 21.1..“ 2‘01: :47 7 “3;le ‘0 3 ‘~‘“ .; , 36-... 4: J3} may "I: ‘ V ‘ ' ‘ J. in: - ‘ ‘L‘yL‘h ‘t’ ' ‘ «t? “+37 "“LV‘ . ' .JL- 1.“?"11' 31"u..\1-. K33}; W", .. '4“ , 5' ._ . . Aq‘fliqv’i LEE}; aging“ .‘ 4;” - 8:" .¢ wh a" 2-mkv .. ““2391 v‘ . . it ééfi‘g’w- . ‘ . Tm ‘ mash“ ma, 4:15;“ at" .1.» ' J‘Q'AVIc—‘UL‘! Hu- . , ”.31.:“1‘:’i.‘::;.:m. » . .- _‘ 1 ’..‘£§1.‘. ; "52.5.1 EEE‘EE ‘ “1* . . ~ ' $1: 7+ .- "51"“ 15% ... I ‘ 5.3.? ; Wmfik gem-J. ‘ . “+32: 'E‘Téfoqu'." A, ‘7". a)?” s '0 ; ,4 I. rfiéfiffifm "‘ ' ,2. “$4333. F§1®¥L ‘ vw~ ”1»... "34’ 2.3“ ‘M 71.x. ‘ k ”‘3'. ‘1‘ ’33; . ”us... "i. '” Vg'1‘1%::‘$j2§:fi ".7. .fiflgazhm; v.13- ”3.3131... .1 . ,. . ... v - ..,. ~ ‘... . . , .14 {a w - :1 n " t z ‘ 241;. 1:. v. m“ ,. fl 3" .., Ill “35"ka 3’. ”oh" «Jo‘- (‘3. 9'30 3 I 2 ‘ '4'" , t. f - .-- h . . o - . E Li’gvvo‘c}9w£y g '- _ .— m-ww “..m (val This is to certify that the dissertation entitled A SURVEY OF SOUTHWESTERN NEW HAMPSHIRE DRIVERS' KNOWLEDGE OF SYMBOLIC SIGN MESSAGES presented by C. GEORGE BOWER has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for Ph. D. degree in EDUCATION Major professor _. Date NOVEMBER 4, 1985 MS U is an Affirmative Action/Equal Opportunity Institution 0-12771 MSU LIBRARIES m RETURNING MATERIALS: Place in book drop to remove this checkout from your record. FINES will be charged if book is returned after the date stamped below. A SURVEY OF SOUTHWESTERN NEW HAMPSHIRE DRIVERS' KNOWLEDGE OF SYMBOLIC SIGN MESSAGES BY C. George Bower A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Secondary Education and Curriculum 1985 Copyright by C. George Bower 1985 ABSTRACT A SURVEY OF SOUTHWESTERN NEW HAMPSHIRE DRIVERS' KNOWLEDGE OF SYMBOLIC SIGN MESSAGES BY C. George Bower Drivers' knowledge of symbolic sign messages that are employed as traffic control devices has run: been reliably assessed. Symbolic messages have seen increasing use in recent years despite inconclusive evidence of driver knowledge. The principal objective of this study was t1) assess driver's knowledge enui recognition <3f symbol signs. Differences between groups (ME drivers, delineated tnr age, sex, driving experience, and training, were studied through the use* of a :fifteen item. domain referenced test. The content validity of the instrument was set at r = .979 and reliability vans set at r‘ = .9455. The test sample was comprised of two hundred and twelve (212) drivers, selected on the basis of license renewal date, from southwestern New Hampshire. The Pearson Product Moment and the Bi—Serial correlation coefficients were employed to test association between test \muiables. Intragroup differences were measured using tflma t—test, ANOVA, time eta statistic, and the test of linearity. The study results showed a strong linear relationship in the variable age, in that as age increased, symbol knowledge decreased. A significant difference of 5.168 between the mean test scores of male (81.571%) and female (76.404%) (drivers vuu; observed. Significant differences were also found within the eight driving experience classifications and a positive linear relationship was established. in“; presence (ME driver education among test subjects produced a mean score difference of +6.562. Sign recognition and sign knowledge ‘were found to in; related based on the obtained correlation coefficient of r = .739. To my wife, Jackie For accepting and sharing a goal I have pursued; for promoting this endeavor when interest diminished; and, for many hours of assistance that is herein recognized. ii ACKNOWLDGMENTS In the preparation of this report, I received the very generous assistance of a group of people to whom I am deeply indebted. A sincere note of appreciation is extended to these individuals: To Paul Paquereau, for his role in providing access to SCSS and expert consultations on the data analysis and computer generation of tables; To Peter Salinger, for writing the data entry utility and assisting with the data organization for SCSS analysis; To time content ‘validity’ review team, consisting of Fred Vanosdall, Dr. Paul Specht, Dr. Thomas Titzkowski, and John Perroni, for their diligence in helping to validate the test instrument; To Zuni Clark, for lending her extensive Wang word processing background to solving my technical printing and formatting problems; To the members of my doctoral committee, Dr. Peggy Riethmiller, Dr. Joseph Dzenowagis, and Fred Vanosdall, for their guidance and assistance in attaining this goal; and, Without any hesitation, a very special acknowledgement is due Dr. Donald Smith, who, as the Chairman of my program, supplied the encouragement and technical references that were essential to making this doctoral program a meaningful developmental experience. iii TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . vi LIST OF APPENDICES . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . vii CHAPTER I THE PROBLEM 1 Statement of the Problem 1 Background of the Study 1 Purpose of the Study 3 Significance of the Study 4 General Question to be Answered 6 Research Hypotheses 7 Basic Assumptions 8 Delimitations 8 Definition of Terms 9 Summary 10 II REVIEW OF LITERATURE 11 Historical Perspective . . . . . . . . . . 11 The Benefits of Symbol Signs . . . . . . . . 12 Effectiveness of Symbol Signs . . . 15 Perception and Recognition of Symbol Signs . 20 Drivers Knowledge of Symbol Signs . . . . . 24 Symbol Sign Testing Techniques . . . . . . . 29 Symbol Sign Design . . . . . . . . . . . . . 35 Summary . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 39 III DESIGN AND METHODOLOGY OF THE STUDY. . . . . . 41 Source of Data . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 41 Sample Employed . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 42 Research Materials . . . . . . . . . . . . . 42 Procedure of the Study . . . . . . . . . . . 45 Research Design . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 47 Research Hypotheses . . . . . . . . . . . . 49 iv Analysis of Data Summary IV ANALYSIS OF DATA Demographic Information Age and Sex Driving Experience and Driver Education Test Item Analysis Crosstabulations and Intragroup. Statistics Analysis of the Variable Age Analysis of the Variable Sex Analysis of the Variable Driving Experience . . . . . . . . . Analysis of the Variable Training Summary V SUMMARY, FINDINGS, CONCLUSIONS, RECOMMENDATIONS AND DISCUSSION Summary Findings Conclusions Recommendations for .Further Research Recommendations Discussion REFERENCES APPENDICES 49 51 52 52 52 53 55 59 6O 63 64 67 69 71 71 72 75 76 77 77 83 87 Table LIST OF TABLES Age and Sex Distributions Driving Experience and Training Summary Table: Correct Response and Recognition Rate for Test Items Breakdown of Knowledge by Age Group Summary of Statistical Analysis for the Variable Age T—Test Results for the Variable Sex Cell Statistics for the Variable Driving Experience . . . . Summary of Statistical Analysis for the Variable Driving Experience T—Test Results for the Variable Driving Experience vi Page 53 54 57 61 63 64 65 67 68 Appendix Page A. B. C. D. D1. D2. D3. D4. D5. D6. E. LIST OF APPENDICES Test Instrument and Answer Form Validity and Reliability of the Test Part I: Part II: Part III: Instrument Manual Test Instrument Validity Test Instrument Reliability Content Validity Assessment Readability Assessment of the Test Instrument Analysis Tables of the Test Variables Mean Scores of Test Subjects and Sex . Mean Scores of Test Subjects and Training . Mean Scores of Test Subjects Driving Experience and Sex Mean Scores of Test Subjects by Age by Age by by Driving Experience and Training Mean Scores of Test Subjects and Training by Sex Correlation Matrix for Test variables Breakdown of Test Item Responses El. E2. E3. E4. E5. E6. E7. E8. Item 1: No Left Turn Item 2: Right Turn Only Item 3: Keep Left Item 4: Do Not Enter . Item 5: Center Lane Left Turn Only Item 6: Right Curve . Item 7: T— Intersection. Item 8: Merging Traffic 87 92 93 95 96 126 128 129 I30 131 132 I33 134 I35 136 137 138 139 I40 141 I42 143 E9. E10. E11. E12. E13. E14. E15. Item Item Item Item Item Item Item 9: 10: 11: 12: 13: 14: 15: Merge Left . . . . . Divided Highway Ahead Slippery Road . . . Low Clearance — 12 Feet 6 Inches Narrow Bridge Workers On Road Two Way Traffic viii 144 145 146 147 148 149 150 CHAPTER I THE PROBLEM Statement of the Problem The problem addressed in this research was the assessment of drivers' knowledge of selected traffic symbol signs. A test was administered in December 1982 to southwestern New Hampshire drivers during the license renewal process. The study was intended to discern differences between groups of drivers, which were based on age, sex, training and driving experience, and their knowledge of the selected symbol signs. Background of the Study One (M5 the primary controllable nmdes (ME information presented to drivers in the traffic environment is traffic signs. Messages on signs are essentially presented 1J1 two types: word and symbolic messages. The driving task involves tine driver's ability tx) interpret tflue meaning (of signs accurately' under 51 wide variety (of conditions and often with specific time limitations. Symbol signs have been used as traffic control devices on the highways of this country almost since the invention of the automobile. In order to develop a Inore uniform motorist communication system, time federal government decided in the early 1970's to adopt some of the international symbol signs. Since then, symbol signs have been demonstrated to be an effective form of communication with drivers in this country and 111 foreign countries. (1,8,14) More than one hundred symbol signs have been accepted by the federal government for use on streets and highways. The major categories consist of regulatory, warning, guide, information and construction signs. There has been extensive use of symbol and pictographic signs in the traffic system. in“; greatest demand exists in the recreational and service sectors and can be seen widely in roadside advertising. An interest 111 tourism anui education. in tflma United States, by foreign individuals, along with the growth of a multilingual population, creates a need for symbol signs in the transportation system. In some sections of the country, English has become the second language. The science (ME symbol sign development involves very complex perceptual considerations that have been researched both here anui abroad. Symbols Ck) not necessarily change the content of a sign, but there is still difficulty in expressing content through symbols. The word message "road narrows" and the symbolic message are both intended to cover the same condition, but communicating the information so that there is a singular interpretation is difficult. Relatively little research has been conducted to determine drivers' knowledge of the symbol signs since their implementation, and no assessment has been made previously in the State of New Hampshire. Purpose of the Study Symbol signs and signs in general are intended to provide drivers information about roadway and traffic conditions in order to improve both safety and efficiency. The extent to which symbol signs accomplish this objective, and to which drivers understand the messages presented by the symbols, has not been adequately documented. The purpose of this study was to assess drivers' knowledge of selected symbol signs that are used as traffic control devices. A secondary purpose of the research was to determine drivers' familiarity with the selected signs. This study was directed at identifying the symbol signs essential to the driving task that are most recognizable and those that are least recognizable. The audience(s) most affected by misinformation was also studied. Public information accompanying the development and use of symbol signs has been limited at best. Symbol signs are generally considered to be self—evident and appear without word messages. Whether the signs are learned through trial and error or are self-explanatory" is not Clear. Driving experience was correlated with sign knowledge to obtain a partial answer to this questions. The study was designed to answer the research questions relative to a selected group of symbol signs deemed most critical to the driving task by traffic experts. No attempt was made to determine drivers' knowledge of all symbol signs. Significance of the Study Since the institution of the system of symbol signs, little research emphasis has been placed on determining drivers' knowledge of these signs. Previous research (1,14) has centered on gathering information on symbology and the science of symbols. The characteristics of symbols, the attention demanded, and the message conveyed by abstract forms have received considerable attention from both the traffic and psychological research communities. (14,19) Drivers rely almost entirely on information received from the traffic environment in performing the driving task. Processing the information accurately and rapidly is dependent, in part, on the quality and the fornl of the stimuli presented. Symbol signs are used to communicate messages in such a mode as only to require recognition and interpretation of the sign content without the need to comprehend and interpret the meaning of word messages. (1) The study also intended to identify the groups, based on the study variables of age, sex, driving experience and training, in which symbol sign knowledge needs the most improvement. Previous research has indicated deficiencies in certain age classifications of drivers. (1,2,5) Large amounts of money are spent by the State of New Hampshire each year on the installation and maintenance of signs about which little is known regarding drivers' use and understanding. While previous research has employed laboratory techniques and intact groups, this effort tested a random sample of drivers using a valid and reliable test instrument. The study provided information that can be applied to the transportation system to affect cost and efficiency. Cost is a factor in as much as some signs may be unnecessary or inappropriate for their purpose. Efficiency is considered from the standpoint of communicating a message in a clear and singular fashion. Public dissatisfaction with the traffic control devices used in the traffic system is voiced almost daily through the media and to governmental officials. (2) Traffic signs that do not convey the proper information or present inaccurate or easily misinterpreted information, are common on the highways. An attempt was made to evaluate the input function of the driving task for the driver as an information processor. By comparison, output in the driving task is far simpler to measure than input. Information density and information processing capabilities are determining factors in the reception of stimuli from the environment (13). The process of discrimination and the ability of the driver to discriminate affect input and the overall performance of the driving task. (13) The relationship of symbol sign research to this issue involves the ability of the driver to process information accurately and rapidly' and. the determination of the signs anui symbols that effectively discriminate in presenting messages. Signs should discriminate in terms of form, content, and intensity. This study was intended to identify the signs of major importance and their ability to project a discriminating or singular message. Recognition and understanding received emphasis in the study rather than the concepts of detection and identification. By assessing recognition and understanding, the question of what the information presented on the sign meant to the driving task was sought. General Questions to be Answered The major research questions answered by this study are as follows: 1. Do drivers in southwestern New Hampshire understand the meaning of selected symbol signs? 2. Are the variables of age, sex, driving experience, and formal training factors affecting drivers' understanding of symbol signs? 3. Have drivers seen or are they familiar with the selected symbol signs in the traffic environment? Are certain selected symbol signs better recognized and understood by drivers than other symbol signs? Researchgfiypotheses The null hypotheses tested in this research were: H01: H02: H03: H04: The was set at There is rm) significant difference between Inale and female drivers' knowledge of symbol signs. fill-#2 = 0 There is ru> significant difference in 'the knowledge of symbol signs between drivers with formal training and those without formal training in the driving task. 11—11. = 0 There is I“) significant difference 111 the knowledge CHE symbol signs between the age classifications of drivers. #1-#2-#3-#4-#s-#e-#7= 0 There is IN) significant difference in the knowledge of symbol signs between the experience classifications of drivers. ux-uz-uz-m-ks—m-uv—Afi 0 level (ME significance for 5H4. statistical tests .10. Basic Assumptions Four assumptions were made in conducting this research: 1. The content validity and reliability of the test instrument, established as part of the research, were sufficient for the purposes of this study. 2. Individual responses of the test subjects were based on true symbol sign knowledge. 3. Symbol sign knowledge can be tested through the use of a valid and reliable test instrument. 4. Expressed opinions are held opinions. Delimitations For the purposes of this study, the following delimitations were established: 1. New Hampshire residents are eligible to obtain a drivers license at age 16 with the renewal required at three year intervals. Age groupings were set at 16—19, 20—29, 30—39, 40—49, 50—59, 60—69, and 70 and older. 2. The symbol test was administered to drivers who were scheduled for renewals during the month of December in 1982. 3. Only drivers from southwestern New Hampshire were tested as a part of this study. New license and out of state transfer applicants were not tested. Definition of Terms SYMBOL SIGNS: The internationally accepted emblem and picture signs, without accompanying words, used to provide information to drivers. DRIVING EXPERIENCE: For the purposes of this study, a value representing the number of mules driven by a driver in a period of one week. FORMAL TRAINING: The completion of a formal driver education course as a prerequisite to licensing. WORD MESSAGES: The statement or legend accompanying a symbol sign to provide the driver a second source (HE information about the sign. (9) KNOWLEDGE: The driver's understanding, awareness, and comprehension of the symbol sign message. FAMILIARITY: The fact of whether or not a driver has seen or recognized a symbol sign. DOMAIN—REFERENCED TEST: A test instrument using a sample of questions to evaluate a body of knowledge. DIAGRAMATIC SIGN: A sign using symbolic messages to indicate a path of travel on a highway. (14) DISCRIMINATION: "Hue ability t1) distinguish between confusing (n: ambiguous stimuli from the 10 driving environment; the presentation of a singular message by a symbol. (14) 10. DETECTION: The perceptual act of seeing a symbol sign in the traffic environment. 11. IDENTIFICATION: The act of being able to accurately name the symbol sign seen in the traffic environment. 12. RECOGNITION: The act of being able to interpret the meaning of the message of a symbol Sign seen in the traffic environment. 13. VISUAL CUES: The elements of color, contrast, motion, intensity, enui positirnh that serve as visual stimuli in the traffic environment. 14. PICTOGRAPHIC SIGNS: Signs employing picture symbols as opposed to abstract symbols to communicate a message. (16) §EEEEEY In the preceding discussion, a. need inns been indicated for symbol sign research and for the investigation (M5 the» relationship between. the test variables and symbol sign knowledge. Chapter Two of this report presents findings of previous sign research that was applicable to this study. The relationship between sign messages and the driving task is defined as well as related research methodologies. CHAPTER II REVIEW OF LITERATURE Introduction The majority of the research conducted in relation to symbol signs has focused on the individual characteristics of the signs such as legibility distance, attention value, and understanding of the message symbol. No comprehensive study has been found to date that deals with all the essential tasks of detecting, recognizing, and acting on a wide variety of symbol signs, using a true random sample of the driving population. Historical Perspective In 1949 the United Nations Protocol for Standardized Signs was adopted in an attempt to bring a degree of uniformity to the international motorists' communications networks. It was not until 1962, through the work of several Japanese citizens, that the International Committee for Breaking the Language Barrier was established in New York. This organization proposed the adoption of symbols as an international traffic communication medium and actively lobbied for implementation. The 1962 United Nations Conference on Road Signs produced a rudimentary guide for international signs but not all were symbolic in nature. Relatively few countries participated in the development of the sign proposal and 11 12 thus, it received only modest support. (13) In preparation for the 1964 Olympics, the Japanese government became the first to adopt symbol signs in a uniform manner. (It is interesting to note that both the Chinese and the Japanese alphabets are derived from symbolic representations for use as characters.) Soon after, the United Kingdom and the Canadian province of Quebec instituted symbol sign systems for their highways. (10) Not until 1971 did the United States government adopt a limited number of symbol signs for use on highways. A year earlier, the U.S. Park Service implemented a system of symbol signs in parks and other recreational areas in order to better accommodate foreign visitors. Since the mid—1970's, the United States has devised a system of symbol signs for highway communications and provided for extensive implementation. However, this system is; not in full accord with the standardized internationally accepted symbols. (13) The Benefits of Symbol Signs The benefits of signs employing symbols to communicate messages (symbol, diagrammatic, and changeable message signs) have not been clearly defined in sign research. Mast and Kolsrud (14) conducted. a study that indicated diagrammatic signs produce tangible benefits in sizable reductions in hazardous inaneuvers, improved lane positioning, and in reduced indecision at gore areas. 13 Their field study suggested that the greatest benefits are derived in areas where unexpected or unusual maneuvers are common. Multiple message displays incorporating symbols can address a broader range of traffic management problems and roadway and environmental conditions, according to the research findings of Allen, et a1. (1). These signs also have the advantage of being able to inform drivers of the type of action(s) required for a given situation with specific information. Symbols convey information in much the same way as conventional signs except that no reading skills are utilized in the communication process. The accuracy of well designed symbols in conveying an intended message is very high. Not only can a symbol be used to present a condition or situation with one statement but the degree and intensity can also be communicated to the audience. (4) For example, Roberts (17) found that diagrammatic signs more adequately communicate the driver's position relative to an exit within an interchange than conventional signs. An advantage is gained by having the operator visualize the approach to the interchange prior to entering the area where decisions are demanded. The decisions on position and direction are then mentally stored and the task becomes one of updating and executing the decisions. Drory and Shinar (6) described several drawbacks of symbols used as communication devices. The foremost 14 problem was that as the graphic component(s) of the sign became more complicated, the time required by the driver to interpret the information increased. Drivers also need additional time to read and interpret the information displayed on complex diagrammatic signs in comparison to conventional signs. Intricate graphics increase the information content in the sign message but at the same time place increased processing demands on the vehicle operator. Allen (1) found that some diagrammatic and symbol signs may actually enhance the problems drivers encounter at interchanges with single right exits, common Cloverleaf, and very complex interchanges by producing indecision. This situation leads to two general recommendations regarding the use of symbols. First, simple graphic designs must be employed whether the message is complex or elementary. Sensory overloads are as much a consideration in signs as in roadway design. The second recommendation, involving response time, remains an essential consideration even though the information content of the sign is heightened by a symbol without additional verbage. A major variable is the processing and execution time needed after the sign has been observed and the information content transmitted to the driver. (1) The Transportation Research Board (TRB) (18) determined that cost tends to be a factor in favor of symbol signs; especially in comparison with the application 15 of diagrammatic and changeable message signs. Special symbols and legends demand individualized construction at additional cost. Maintenance and installation costs are much higher for the diagrammatic and changeable message signs than for mass produced conventional signs. Most benefits are derived from the ability of the symbolic representations to transmit simple and meaningful messages without the use of lengthy legends, according to Allen.(l) The integrity and density of the information content presents both positive and negative results depending upon the specific application. The function of the symbol sign is limited though by the variable abilities of vehicle operators who have widely divergent capabilities in perceiving, processing, and acting on the information transmitted. (1) Effectiveness of Symbol Signs Most research supports the superior performance of symbol signs in communicating messages as compared to conventional signs. Diagrammatic signs, changeable message signs, and symbol signs are more effective in most situations requiring special or detailed applications due to the specialized design to fit the demands. As described earlier, the information content of the symbol is greater and requires significantly less reading and response time than conventional signs. The effectiveness of symbol signs is directly related to the attention value of the design 16 and this element may, in fact, be the key to transmission of information. (6,14,18) Allen (1) demonstrated that symbol signs provide legibility as good as, and in many cases better than, conventional word message signs. In the same study, Allen (1) investigated the influence of the geometric complexity of the graphics in establishing inherent meaning. He found that increasing time geometric: complexity tended. to make the symbol more obvious. Symbols with inherent meaning were more easily recognizable and may be immediately familiar to the driver upon initial exposure. Complex graphics make the symbol distinctive and isolate its meaning from outside interference. Singular meaning is essential to a symbol anui when. attained, the application was limited to those situations and demands most appropriate to the design. Allen (1) also compared the single symbol to 23 single word employed on a sign. The two were equivalent and required a little over two seconds for reading and comprehension by the driver. This fixation was relatively long when combined with other driving tasks. Warning signs, according t1) Drory and Shinar (6), receive shorter fixations than directional signs. Most snmfixfl. signs fall into time warning classification which may account for the shorter fixations. The symbols used in vnnnmjm; signs are generally simple in both content and context. The directional signs employ rmnxa graphically complex symbols and word messages by necessity in order to communicate 17 sufficient information to guide motorists. When signs have the capacity to present multiple messages using several symbols or words, TRB (18) findings define the advantages over single message signs. Single message signs are appropriate only when the situation is recurrent and the same driver response is solicited each time the sign is encountered. They are applicable for only the intended purpose and the accuracy and credibility of the sign.:hs questionable over time. The nmltiple message signs can alert drivers to a broad range of problems without presenting unnecessary distractors. TRB research findings indicated that reading and comprehension should increase for multiple message signs if simply the bulk and complexity of the information displayed is considered. Longer scanning and fixation times are also implied. At issue is whether or not the multiple message sign improves the quality of the information reception process while increasing the fixation times. While the process (ME percepticni is questionable, tflua fact that the multiple message sign improves the attention (n? the driver by providing variable information is aa decided advantage. (18) Legibility, recognition time and efficiency in presenting the sign message are the most obvious benefits of symbolic messages. When the complexity of the symbol is sufficient to increase the reading and comprehension times this element becomes 23 detriment unless adequate response 18 time is allocated to the driver in each application. (18) Roberts (11) defined the attention value of diagrammatic guide signs as seeming to be greater than that of conventional signs, which is probably due to the increased size of the legend. The symbols used on diagrammatic guide signs presented a more formidable character for viewing than conventional word messages. An essential element of any sign, symbol signs included, was the attention value inherent to the design. Attention value was the degree to which the sign components collectively demanded observation or review by the driver. The yellow/black warning signs were described by Allen (1) as having the highest attention value of all signs while the white on green service signs have the second best recognition. Although the white on green had high attention values, it also produced some of the highest rates of recognition errors. Allen cited geometric complexity of the symbol as a second crucial factor, but it also produced other perceptual elements influencing driver recognition and behavior, as described earlier. Attention value was determined by visual cues such as color, contrast, motion, intensity, position, size, repetition, and shape. These factors caused drivers to visually divert attention from one orientation to another. Communicability of information to drivers is the basic requirement of all signs. Roberts (17) studied the communicability of symbolic messages and discovered that “Ar—— 19 they more clearly presented information relative to exit directions at advanced locations than did conventional signs. Mast and Kolsrud (l4) corroborated this finding in research showing that diagrammatic signs produced the greatest benefits where unexpected or unusual maneuvers can be anticipated. The diagrams of the exit areas, lanes and routes clearly defined driver choices and removed from the driving task the necessity to read and respond to lengthy word messages within a short time span. Multiple message signs can address a broader range of traffic, roadway, and environmental situations, according to research by the TRB (18). These signs are most effective in alerting drivers to the conditions they are to encounter and in informing them of the appropriate actions required in the specific situation. In conducting a study on the effectiveness of changeable message signs, the TRB (18) found that the efficiency of regulatory, advisory, and warning signs, was nearly impossible to quantify because of the factors producing bias in the study. The difficulty was in determining whether the test sign or a later relevant cue affected driving behavior. As the efficiency of the symbolic message improves, the demand placed on the driver increases while the time required for recognition decreases. Symbol signs are not appropriate in every situation as some very complex conditions are virtually impossible to represent accurately with simple graphics. I!) 20 The TRB study did determine that the changeable message sign was a step up from the conventional symbol signs with regard to attention value. Problems existed however, with fully reliable data that would allow the researchers to quantify the difference. The greatest value of the changeable message signs was in communicating information under adverse weather and roadway conditions. The requirements for traffic control devices defined in the Manual on Uniform Traffic Control Devices for Streets and Highways (16) are all essential in insuring the effectiveness of signs. The effectiveness of sign messages is dependent upon proper application of the sign, attention value of the graphics, selective or limited use, and a clearly recognizable message. Perception and Recognition of Symbol Signs Drory enui Shinar (6) asserted that traffic control devices are lfififli to inforni drivers about something that the roadway (n: the environment does run: or cannot inform them. Signs provide response time to situations that are not known to time driver. Markowitz, Lees, 6%: al., (13) defined a classification (ME probability' signs or those intended to inform the driver of sxnme condition cm: hazard that rmnr exist in the traffic environment. The driver's response was determined by the probability that the condition(s) described by the sign exists in the environment. Past experiences enui environmental stimulus 21 were evaluated to assess the potential of the hazard or condition being present. Earlier discussion by Allen (1) noted that the attention value of a symbol is a dominant factor in driver recognition and understanding. Drory and Shinar's (6) research into the attention value of signs also defined several principles (ME driver attention. They tinnui that as the demands of the driving task increase, the individual differences between drivers, i.e. fatigue, emotional state, etc., are reduced as more mental capacity is required. When the situations presented in the driving environment are less demanding, the individual differences become rmnma pronounced. Fatigue (uni be suppressed temporarily ii? the attention demanded of the driver is at a high level, but as the demands diminish, the driver may relax to the point of being inattentive. Drory and Shinar (6) stated that the upper limit of sign recognition is determined by attention. The attention value of the sign and the attention of the driver to line driving task anxe both relevant. When the driving task (Lanands attention, such as vnlfli a winding road, attention increases and 5%) does sfirni recognition. Their research used road design factors as a study variable. A 1969 study of symbol recognition, conducted in Ottowa, Canada, by McLean (15), used a questionnaire to test eight sign symbols. The symbols selected tin: study 22 were: No Parking, No Stopping, One Iknu: Parking, Pavement Narrows, Bump, Construction Flagman, Slippery Road Ahead, and One Lane Traffic. Three of the symbols exhibited a high degree of recognition: the "P" 1mmmi as a substitute for the word "parking," Flagman Ahead, and Slippery Road Ahead. The research report specified neither the criteria employed to define a high degree of recognition nor the symbol sign selection criteria. Gordon (8) conducted research on the subject using diagrammatic and conventional signs. While (diagrammatic and symbol signs are rmfl: equivalent, time design concepts (avoiding word messages) are the same. In six of the ten locations studied, reaction time for the diagrammatic signs was shorter. Two of the locations reached significance at the .05 level using a t—test for corrected measures where N=30. The test measures were based on the speed and accuracy of the subjects lane changes as determined by observers. Reaction time demanded recognition on the part of the driver. Some of the diagrammatic signs were more suitable and effective for particular interchange designs. Conventional signs did produce fewer lane placement and selection errors. Gordon's findings support the assertions made in other research that symbols and diagrams are more effective in lrwv task load situations inn: that the value of the signs begins to decline as the complexity of the tasks increase. The research on sign recognition does not 23 clarify the apparent contradiction between. the improved response time produced through the use of symbols and the degradation (if responses anmi maneuvers when symbols are employed in complex situations. The fact that the subjects in Gordon's study expressed a personal preference for conventional signs may tme indicative of driver acceptance (if the symbols that the not appear as "friendly." Other research cited previously described the difficulty of expressing complex situations with graphics. Recognition is improved when the route and destination information. is matched with the major diagrammatic components of time sign, according to Roberts (17). The components of time sign allow time driver ti) be more discriminating when making lane selection and travel directicni decisions. Graphic and situational complexity tend to be less problematic in the presence of adequate discriminating symbol qualities. Markowitz, Lees, et a1. (13), determined. that the shape of the sign has rm) inherent meaning ti) drivers and that it: is probably time least important design element. But since shape is; helpful tin: sign recognition iii some cases (stop, yield and other singular purpose shapes), the effect on recognition must be considered. The research of Mackie (12), Dewar (4), Roberts (17), Drory and Shinar (6), and others depended upon recall of signs when driving. The classic experiment asked the subject to specify a sign that had been passed when 24 stopped after travelling through a test area. Drory and Shinar (6) claimed that recall is related to attentional and. motivational factors rather than those of memory' or sensory inputs. There was a failure to account for the process and effect of discrimination as a perceptual consideration in these studies. Drivers' Knowledge of Symbol Signs Kato (11) proposed ten criteria for the development of symbol signs with universality. These recommendations serve as some of the primary test criteria in the design of symbolic messages. 1. Is it easy to associate the symbol with its message? 2. Does the symbol fit different cultures and different local situations? 3. Does the symbol fit the changing times? 4. Is the symbol pleasing and acceptable without controversy? 5. Does the symbol conform with existing international symbols or other elements? 6. Is the symbol or its element capable of systematic application for a variety of interrelated concepts? 7. Is the symbol easily reproducible? Is it applicable for many different purposes? 8. Is the symbol distinguishable from other symbols? 25 9. Can the sign be perceived from different angles and perspectives, under different light conditions? 10. Can the symbol withstand vandalism and contamination? In a) 1966 study' entitled Progress in Learning the Meaning of Symbolic Traffic Signs, (12) Mackie administered a questionnaire to 476 drivers to test their knowledge of symbolic traffic signs. The instrument was designed to collect data relative ti) the understanding of the shapes, colors, and meanings of the signs. The variables consisted of age, sex, social class, geographic area, and experience driving outside of the country. A secondary area of concern centered on the method of learning the meaning of the symbol signs. When compared ti) a similar test conducted 111 1965, Mackie observed significant differences in several areas. The mean value of knowledge on identical test items improved 16% in the latter study' (P (.001). While the percentage of correct answers increased, the percentage of incorrect answers remained tmonstant. The (jifference twas accounted for through a reduction in the rmana: of partly correct responses. The most widely recognized symbol sign was the Advanced Warning — Traffic Signal Ahead sign, which had almost universal recognition. The least recognized symbol sign was the Parking Regulation grouping. 26 Two important factors identified in the research are that seeing the sign while driving was an indicator of drivers' knowledge of the symbol signs, but not the complete answer, and that the most common method of learning signs was self reported to be guessing. There twas EH1 inconsistent level of knowledge among the groups of British drivers on the various symbol signs. No significant differences were reported between male and female drivers but there were significant variations between knowledge and age. The drivers who knew the principles of the sign classification system (3% of the population) gave significantly' more correct responses than drivers who did not know the classification system. (12) Similar findings were reported by Dewar and Swandon (5) in a 1971 report. They found 1m) significant differences iii Canadian cjrivers vdiii regard ti) sex, but again established a significantly lower level of knowledge among older drivers; especially those over 6%) years YES NO RANKING: I 2 3 COMMENTS: 15. WINDING ROAD UPON SEEING THIS SIGN A DRIVER SHOULD: A. PROCEED AT A NORMAL SPEED 8. EXPECT A SERIES OF HILLS IN THE ROAD (C. EXPECT A SERIES OF CURLES IN THE ROAD HAVE YOU EVER SEEN THIS SIGN WHILE DRIVING7 YES NO RANKING: I 2 3 COMMENTS: 15 112 LARGE ARROW UPON SEEING THIS SIGN A DRIVER SHOULD: (A. SLOW AND PREPARE TO MAKE A SHARP RIGHT TURN B. EXPECT A SHARP LEFT TURN C. E\PECT A CLOSED LANE AHEAD II HAVE YOU EVER SEEN THIS SIGN WHILE DRIVING? YES NO RANKING: 1 COMMENTS: Ix! 3 CROSS ROAD UPON SEEING THIS SIGN A DRIVER SHOULD: A. E‘PECT A DIVIDED HIGHWAY (B. WATCH FOR VEHICLES APPROCHING ON CROSSROAD C. PREPARE TO MAKE A SERIES OF TURNS HAVE YOU EVER SEEN THIS SIGN WHILE DRIVING9 YES NO RANFING: I 2 3 COMMENTS: SIDE ROAD UPON SEEING THIS SIGN A DRIVER SHOULD: A. PREPARE TO MAKE A LEFT TURN 8. STOP FOR ON-COMING TRAFFIC 1C. WATCH FOR VEHICLES APPROACHING ON SIDE RCHD 6 HAVE YOU EVER SEEN THIS SIGN WHILE DRIVING7 YES NO RANKING: l 2 3 COMMENTS: 113 1?. T SYMBOL UPON SEEING THIS SIGN A DRIVER SHOULD: (A. PREPARE TO TURN LEFT OR RIGHT B. PROCEED AT A NORMAL RATE OF SPEED C. REDUCE SPEED AND PROCEED WITH CAUTION HAVE YOU EVER SEEN THIS SIGN WHILE DRIVING7 YES NO RANKING: l 2 3 COMMENTS: 28. Y SYMBOL UPON SEEING THIS SIGN A DRIVER SHOULD: A. SLOW AND PREPARE FOR A SERIES OF CURVES (B. PREPARE TO TURN LEFT OR RIGHT C. STOP FOR ON—COMING TRAFFIC HAVE YOU EVER SEEN THIS SIGN WHILE DRIVING” YES NO RANKING: 1 COMMENTS: ILI 3 21. SIGNAL AHEAD UPON SEEING THIS SIGN A DRIVER SHOULD: PROCEED AT A NORMAL SPEED SLOW AND PREPARE TO MAKE A RIGHT '.:M PREPARE TO STOP IF SIGNAL IS REL FICUD l HAVE YOU EVER SEEN THIS SIGN WHILE DRIVING7 YES NO RANKING: 1 2 3 COMMENTS: 114 Page 18 22. MERGE UPON SEEING THIS SIGN A DRIVER SHOULD: lA. WATCH FOR TRAFFIC ENTERING FROM RIGHT 8. SLOW AND PREPARE TO STOP C. PREPARE TO MERGE WITH TRAFFIC HAVE YOU EVER SEEN THIS SIGN WHILE DRIVING° YES NO RANKING: I 2 3 COMMENTS: 23. RIGHT LANE ENDS UPON SEEING THIS SIGN A DRIVER SHOULD: A. WATCH FOR ON-COMING 'RAFFIC X8. MERGE LEFT IF TRAVELLING IN RIGHT LANE C. PREPARE FOR A SERIES OF CURVES HAVE YOU EVER SEEN THIS SIGN WHILE DRIVING’ YES NO RANKING: 1 2 3 COMMENTS: 24. DIVIDED HIGHWAY UPON SEEING THIS SIGN A DRIVER SHOULD: A. PREPARE FOR TWO-WAY TRAFFIC AHEAD 8. MOVE RIGHT TO AVOID A HAZARD (C. EXPECT A DIVIDED HIGHWAY AHEAD HAVE YOU EVER SEEN THIS SIGN WHILE DRIVING7 vES NO RANKING: I 2 3 COMMENTS: 115 Page 19 25. DIVIDED HIGHWAY ENDS UPON SEEING THIS SIGN A DRIVER SHOULD: XA. EXPECT THE END OF A DIVIDED HIGHWAY 8. MOVE RIGHT TO AVIOD A HAZARD C. ExPECT A DIVIDED HIGHWAY AHEAD HAVE YOU EVER SEEN THIS SIGN WHILE DRIVING” YES NO RANKING: I 2 3 C OMMENTS : 26. TWO-WAY TRAFFIC UPON SEEING THIS SIGN A DRIVER SHOULD: A. PREPARE FOR A DIVIDED ROADWAY AHEAD QB. EXPECT A TWO-WAY ROADWAY AHEAD L. WATCH FOR MERGING TRAFFIC HAVE YOU EVER SEEN THIS SIGN WHILE DRIVING7 YES NO RANKING: l 2 3 COMMENTS: HILL UPON SEEING THIS SIGN A DRIVER SHOULD: A. AATCH FOR TRUCKS CROSSING AHEAD B. WATCH FOR STOPPED VEHICLES iC. REDUCE SPEED AND PREPARE FOR A DOWN—GRADE HAVE YOU EVER SEEN THIS SIGN WHILE DRIVING° YES NC RANKING: I 2 3 COMMENTS: 116 Page 28 28. SLIPPERY WHEN WET UPON SEEING THIS SIGN A DRIVER SHOULD: 1A. PREPARE FOR SLIPPERY ROAD IF SURFACE IS WET B. EXPECT A HIGHWAY FOR CARS ONLY C. REDUCE SPEED AND PREPARE FOR A SERIES OF TURNS HAVE YOU EVER SEEN THIS SIGN WHILE DRIVING° YES NO RANKING: 1 2 3 COMMENTS: BICYCLE CROSSING UPON SEEING THIS SIGN A DRIVER SHOULD: 29. A. KEEP OUT OF BICYCLE LANE (B. WATCH FOR BICYCLE CROSSING OR TRAFFIC C. YIELD RIGHT OF WAY TO BICYCLES HAVE YOU EVER SEEN THIS SIGN WHILE DRIVING? YES NO RANKING: I 2 3 COMMENTS: 38. PEDESTRIAN CROSSING UPON SEEING THIS SIGN A DRIVER SHOULD: WATCH FOR SCHOOL CROSSING AHEAD HITCH HIKING PROHIBITED . WATCH FOR PEDESTRIAN CROSSING AHEAD DMD R HAVE YOU EVER SEEN THIS SIGN WHILE DRIVING’ YES NO RANKING: I 2 COMMENTS: 117 Page 21 31. DEER CROSSING UPON SEEING THIS SIGN A DRIVER SHOULD: XA. WATCH FOR DEER CROSSING THE ROADWAY B. WATCH FOR CATTLE CROSSING AHEAD C. WATCH FOR WILDLIFE PRESERVE AHEAD HAVE YOU EVER SEEN THIS SIGN WHILE DRIVING? YES NO RANKING: 1 2 3 COMMENTS: FARM MACHINERY A. WATCH FOR MAINTENANCE WORK AHEAD' B. PREPARE TO STOP XC. WATCH FOR FARM MACHINERY ON ROADWAY 32. HAVE YOU EVER SEEN THIS SIGN WHILE DRIVING? YES NO RANKING: I 2 3 COMMENTS: 33. DOUBLE ARROW UPON SEEING THIS SIGN A DRIVER SHOULD: XA. PREPARE TO PASS ON EITHER SIDE OF A HAZARD B. KEEP RIGHT C. REDUCE SPEED AND PREPARE TO STOP HAVE YOU EVER SEEN THIS SIGN WHILE DRIVING? YES NO RANKING: 1 2 3 COMMENTS: 118 Page 22 34. LOW CLEARANCE UPON SEEING THIS SIGN A DRIVER SHOULD: A. EXPECT PAVEMENT TO NARROW TO 12 FEET 6 INCHES X8. EXPECT A LOW CLEARANCE OF 12 FEET 6 INCHES C. USE PARKING SPACES 12 FEET 6 INCHES LONG HAVE YOU EVER SEEN THIS SIGN WHILE DRIVING? YES NO RANKING: I 2 3 COMMENTS: SCHOOL ADVANCE UPON SEEING THIS SIGN A DRIVER SHOULD: 35. A. WATCH FOR CROSSING FOR THE BLIND B. PREPARE TO ENTER A HEAVILY POPULATED AREA (C. PREPARE TO ENTER A SCHOOL ZONE HAVE YOU EVER SEEN THIS SIGN WHILE DRIVING? YES NO RANKING: I 2 3 COMMENTS: SCHOOL CROSSING UPON SEEING THIS SIGN A DRIVER SHOULD: 36. 1A. WATCH FOR CHILDREN CROSSING THE STREET 8. WATCH FOR A CRCSSING FOR T~E 1““ C. PREPARE TO ENTER A PECES'RIAN IRIESIKO AREA HAVE YOU EVER SEEN THIS SIGN WHILE DRIVING? YES NU RANKING: 1 2 3 COMMENTS: 119 37. STOP AHEAD UPON SEEING THIS SIGN A DRIVER SHOULD: A. EXPECT ON-COMING TRAFFIC TO STOP AT INTERSECTICN kB. REDUCE SPEED AND PREPARE TO STOP C. NOT TURN RIGHT AT INTERSECTION HAVE YOU EVER SEEN THIS SIGN WHILE DRIVING? YES NO RANKING: I 2 3 COMMENTS: 38. YIELD AHEAD . UPON SEEING THIS SIGN A DRIVER SHOULD: A. NOT TURN LEFT AT INTERSECTION 8. EXPECT ON-COMING TRAFFIC TO STOP 1C. REDUCE SPEED AND PREPaRE TO STOP IF NEIESS-RY HAVE YOU EVER SEEN THIS SIGN WHILE DRIVING? YES NO RANKING: I 2 3 COMMENTS: GUIDE SIGNS 39. ADVANCE TURN ARROW THIS SIGN MEANS: !A. THE ROUTE BEING TRAVELLED TURNS LEFT AHEAD E. CENTER LANE LEFT TURN ONLY C. ALL TRAFFIC MUST TURN LEFT HAVE YOU EVER SEEN THIS SIGN WHILE DRIVING” '35 NO RANKING: 1 2 3 COMMENTS: 120 Page 24 48. DIRECTIONAL ARROW THIS SIGN MEANS: I A. NO TURNS PERMITTED {8. THE ROUTE BEING FOLLOWED PROCEEDS STRAIGHT C. ALL TRAFFIC MUST GO STRAIGHT HAVE YOU EVER SEEN THIS SIGN WHILE DRIVING? YES NO RANKING: I 2 3 COMMENTS: INFORMATION SERVICE SIGNS 41. PICNIC TABLE THIS SIGN MEANS: f¥,, A. REST AREA 8. CAMPING AREA AHEAD xc. ROADSIDE PIZNIC TABLE AHEAD HAVE YOU EVER SEEN THIS SIGN WHILE 3RIHIHG° ~E3 <0 RANKING: l 2 3 CIMMENTS: 42. TELEPHONE THIS SIGN MEANS: (A. ROADSIDE TELEPHONE AHEAD 8. EMERGENCY SERVICE AREA C. END OF EMERGENCY TELEPHONE NETWCRK HAVE YOU EVER SEEN THIS SIGN WHILE DRIVING’ YES NO RANKING: l 2 3 C OMMENTS : 121 43. HOSPITAL THIS SIGN MEANS: A. HOTEL PARKING (8. DIRECTION TO HOSPITAL C. LODGING INFORMATION HAVE YOU EVER SEEN THIS SIGN WHILE DRIVING? YES RANKING: I A 3 COMMENTS: 44, CAMPING THIS SIGN MEANS: A LODGING INFORMATION A. e. PEST AREA xc. CAMPING AREA HAVE YOU EVER SEEN THIS SIGN WHILE DRIVING? YES RANKING: I 2 3 COMMENTS: 45. BIKE ROUTE THIS SIGN MEANS: BEEIEEE, IA. BICYCLE ROUTE 9. NO BICYCLES c. BICYCLE CROSSING AHEAD HAVE YOU EVER SEEN THIS SIGN WHILE DRIVING? (ES RANK I HG: I 2 3 COMMENTS: NO NO NO Page 25 122 Page HIKING TRAIL THIS SIGN MEANS: A. PEDESTRIAN CROSSING i8. HIKING TRAIL C. SCHOOL ZONE HAVE YOU EVER SEEN THIS SIGN WHILE DRIVING? YES NO RANKING: I 2 3 COMMENTS: 47. RESTAURANT THIS SIGN MEANS: A. LODGING INFORMATION 8. PICNIC AREA iC. RESTAURANT OR DINING FACILITY HAVE YOU EVER SEEN THIS SIGN WHILE DRIVING? YES NO RANKING: I 2 3 COMMENTS: 48. SERVICE STATION THIS SIGN MEANS: i (A. SERVICE STATION B. REST AREA C TRAVEL INFORMATION HAVE YOU EVER SEEN THIS SIGN WHILE DRIVING? YES NO RANKING: 1 2 3 COMMENTS: 26 123 49. LODGING THIS SIGN MEANS: I.-. A. REST AREA 18. LODGING OR MOTEL SERVICES C. TRAVEL INFORMATION HAVE YOU EVER SEEN THIS SIGN WHILE DRIVING? YES RANKING: I 2 3 COMMENTS: CONSTRLCTIZN SIGNS 58. ADVANCE FLAGGER THIS SIGN MEANS: MEN WORKING DETOUR AHEAD FLAGMAN AHEAD (”AIDD HAVE YOU EVER SEEN THIS SIGN WHILE DRIVING? YES RANKING: I 2 3 COMMENTS: SI. NARROW BRIDGE THIS SIGN MEANS: iA. NARROW BRIDGE AHEAD B. OBSTRUCTION IN ROADWAY C. SOFT SHOULDER HAVE YOU EVER SEEN THIS SIGN WHILE DRIVING? YES RANKING: I 2 3 COMMENTS: NO NO NO 124 52. PAVEMENT ENDS THIS SIGN MEANS: A. BUMP XB. PAVEMENT ENDS C. ROAD NARROWS HAVE YOU EVER SEEN THIS SIGN WHILE DRIVING? YES RANKING: I 2 3 COMMENTS: 53. WORKER THIS SIGN MEANS: A. FLAGMAN AHEAD B. ROAD MACHINERY AHEAD *C. WORKERS IN ROADWAY HAVE YOU EVER SEEN THIS SIGN WHILE DRIVING? YES RANKING: 1 COMMENTS: 3 h] \ TWO-WAY TRAFFIC “THIS SIGN MEANS: iA. TWO-WAY TRAFFIC AHEAD 8. STOP FOR ON-COMING TRAFFIC C. DIVIDED HIGHWAY AHEAD HAVE YOU EVER SEEN THIS SIGN WHILE DRIVING? YES RANKING: I 2 3 COMMENTS: NO NO NO m” ."‘m‘1s““‘; gr, '. . ’ 55. 125 RIGHT LANE ENDS THIS SIGN MEANS: A. NO TURNS *8. ROAD NARROWS FROM RIGHT C. TWO-WAY ROADWAY AHEAD HAVE YOU EVER SEEN THIS SIGN WHILE DRIVING? YES NO RANKING: I 2 3 COMMENTS: 126 APPENDIX C READABILITY ASSESSMENT OF THE TEST INSTRUMENT 127 Keene State College Inter—Department Memorandum To: George Bower Dam: October 20. 1982 Front Glenna Mize, Ph.D. Reading Specialist - Safety Center Using the Frye Readability Formula attached, the estimated average reading difficulty of the three-page form is 4.2. Page Sentences Syllables Estimated Grade Level 1 9 133 5.3 2 8 117 3 3 8 115 3 A 7.5 125 5.5 11.5 f 4 - 4.2 The addition of the sign should reduce the reading difficulty even more because of the visual representation. GJM:jb 128 APPENDIX D ANALYSIS TABLES OF THE TEST VARIABLES 129 Tab1e 01 Mean Scores of Test Subjects by Age and Sex BREAKDOWN OF AGE AND SEX ACROSS - AGE DOWN - SEX Age Groups 16-19 20—29 30-39 40—49 50-59 60-69 70+ TOTAL FEMALE MEAN 85.000 78.000 82.686 77.706 63.667 64.700 71.000 : 76.404 STDOEV 8.485 19.494 9.489 11.537 12.698 16.125 17.088 1 15.538 N 4 30 35 17 15 10 3 : 114 HALE MEAN 87.000 82.880 84.222 82.650 75.063 77.400 77.667 : 81.571 STDOEV .000 11.319 11.804 15.618 20.407 11.327 4.041 : 14.087 N 2 25 27 20 16 5 3 98 TOTAL MEAN 85.667 80.218 83.355 80.378 69.548 68.933 74.333 1 78.792 STDOEV 6.653 16.341 10.496 13.933 17.804 15.563 11.690 1 15.073 N 6 55 62 37 31 1S 6 : 212 Tab1e 02 Mean Scores of Test Subjects by Age and Training ACROSS — AGE DOWN -TRA1NING - DRIVER EDUCATION 20—29 76.333 27.717 80.978 13.425 50-59 69.920 17.888 25 68.000 19.037 60-69 68.933 15.563 15 0 70+ 78.600 5.857 53.000 .000 TOTAL 16—19 NO TRAINING 0 0 0 TRAINING 85.667 6.653 6 TOTAL 85.667 6.653 6 AGE GROUPS 30-39 40-49 81.708 77.739 12.376 15.789 24 23 84.395 84.714 9.140 9.118 38 14 83.355 80.378 10.496 13.933 62 37 69.548 17.804 31 74.333 11.690 131 Table 03 Mean Scores of Test Subjects by Driving Experience and Sex BREAKDOWN OF EXPERIENCE AND SEX ACROSS - EXPERIENCE DOWN - SEX DRIVING EXPERIENCE CLASSIFICATIONS (MILES PER WEEK) 1 . 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. 8. TOTAL FEMALE MEAN 73.500 69.400 77.279 75.214 78.211 80.700 73.500 : 76.404 STDDEV 9.192 16.426 13.539 20.565 15.175 8.015 9.192 : 15.538 N 2 10 43 28 19 10 2 0 : 114 MALE MEAN 66.667 77.091 81.688 80.273 80.700 89.000 87.765 : 81.571 STDDEV 34.962 17.207 13.446 12.464 12.374 3.464 10.680 : 14.087 N 0 3 11 32 22 10 3 17 98 TOTAL 73.500 68.769 77.241 78.667 79.317 80.700 82.800 87.765 2 78.792 9.192 20.187 14.182 17.296 13.648 10.147 9.960 10.680 2 15.073 2 13 54 6O 41 20 5 17 : 212 Driving Experience CTassifications in Hi1es per Heek 1. 0 2. 1-20 2. 20-50 3. 50-100 5. 100-200 6. 200-300 7. 300—400 8. 400+ ".I.‘ --- :- 132 TabTe 04 Mean Scores of Test Subjects by Experience and Training BREAKDOWN OF EXPERIENCE AND TRAINING ACROSS — EXPERIENCE DOWN - TRAINING - DRIVER EDUCATION EXPERIENCE CLASSIFICATIONS IN MILES PER WEEK' 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. 8. TOTAL NO TRAINING MEAN 67.000 60.200 75.333 73.971 75.643 77.778 80.250 84.000 : 75.356 STDDEV .000 22.163 14.893 20.126 17.310 12.091 9.430 10.187 : 16.994 N 1 5 24 34 14 9 4 1O : 101 TRAINING MEAN 80.000 74.125 78.767 84.808 81.222 83.091 93.000 93.143 : 81.919 STDDEV .000 18.240 13.647 10.104 11.212 8.043 .000 9.547 2 12.352 N 1 8 30 26 27 11 1 7 111 TOTAL MEAN 73.500 68.769 77.241 78.667 79.317 80.700 82.800 87.765 : 78.792 STDDEV 9.192 20.187 14.182 17.296 13.648 10.147 9.960 10.680 : 15.073 N 2 13 54 60 41 20 5 17 : 212 'Driving Experience C1assifications in Mi1es per Heek 1. 0 2. 1—20 2. 20-50 3. 50-100 5. 100—200 6. 200-300 7. 300—400 8. 400+ 133 Table D5 Mean Scores of Test Subjects by Sex and Training BREAKDOWN OF SEX AND TRAINING ACROSS - SEX DOWN - TRAINING - DRIVER EDUCATION FEMALE MALE NO TRAINING MEAN 72.865 78.000 75.356 STDDEV 17.255 16.476 16.994 N 52 49 101 TRAINING MEAN 79.371 85.143 81.919 SDTDEV 13.368 10.176 12.352 N 62 49 111 TOTAL MEAN 76.404 81.571 78.792 STDDEV 15.538 14.087 15.073 N 114 98 212 134 Table D6 Correlation Matrix for Test Variables NUMBER OF CASES = 212 CORRELATION, SIGNIFICANCE AGE -.272 .000 SEX .171 .022 .006 .374 TRAINING .218 -.541 -.044 .001 .000 .263 EXPERIENCE .226 -.047 .393 -.059 .000 .250 .000 .198 KNOWLEDGE AGE SEX TRAINING 135 APPENDIX E BREAKDOWN OF TEST ITEM RESPONSES Table E—l Item 1: No Left Turn 1. THIS SIGN MEANS: A. NO TURNS B. LEFT TURNS ALLOWED C. NO LEFT TURNS ALLOWED RESPONSE N TOT % NM % CUM % A 18 8.5 8.6 8.6 B 10 4.7 4.8 13.4 C 181 85 4 86.6 100.0 CORRECT 0 3 l 4 NA NA NO RESPONSE RESPONSES TO ITEM 1 = 209 "Have you ever seen this sign while driving?" YES: 187 (88.7%) NO: 22 NO RESPONSE: Number of Responses % of All Responses 0 a of Valid Responses 0 Cumulative s 3 137 Table E-2 Item 2: Right Turn Only 2. THIS SIGN MEANS: A. THE INDICATED TURN MUST BE MADE B. NO TURNS ARE ALLOWED C. THE LANE MAY BE USED TO MAKE A LEFT TURN RESPONSE N TOT % NM % CUM % LABEL A 126 59.4 60.0 60.0 CORRECT B 1 .5 5 60.5 C 83 39.2 39 5 100.0 0 2 .9 NA NA NO RESPONSE RESPONSES TO ITEM 2 = 210 "Have you ever seen this sign while driving?" YES: 199 (93.8%) NO: 9 NO RESPONSE: 4 N = Number of Responses TOT % = % of All Responses % of Valid Responses CUM % = Cumulative % 138 Table E—3 Item 3: Keep Left 3. THIS SIGN MEANS: A. KEEP LEFT B. LANE CLOSED AHEAD C. VEHICLE APPROACHING ON LEFT RESPONSE N TOT % NM % CUM % LABEL A 178 84.0 87.7 87.7 CORRECT B 20 9.4 9.9 97.5 C 5 2.4 2.5 100.0 0 9 4.2M NA NA NO RESPONSE RESPONSES TO ITEM 3 = 203 "Have you ever seen this sign while driving?" YES: 128 (63.4%) NO: 79 NO RESPONSE: 5 N = Number of Responses TOT % = % of All Responses NM% = % of Valid Responses 0 Cumulative a o\° II CUM Table E—4 Item 4: Do Not Enter 4. THIS SIGN MEANS: A. NO PARKING ANYTIME B. DO NOT ENTER THIS STREET OR ROAD C. EXIT ONLY RESPONSE N TOT % NM % CUM % A 5 2 4 2.4 2.4 B 200 94 3 95.2 97.6 CORRECT C 5 2 4 2.4 100.0 OM 2M 9M NA NA NO RESPONSE RESPONSES TO ITEM 4 = 210 "Have you ever seen this sign while driving?" YES: 205 (96.7%) NO: 4 NO RESPONSE: 3 N = Number of Responses TOT % = % of All Responses NM% = of Valid Responses CUM % = Cumulative % 140 Table E—5 Item 5: Center Lane Left Turn Only THIS SIGN MEANS: A. DIVIDED HIGHWAY AHEAD B. LEFT TURNS ONLY AT INTERSECTION C. CENTER LANE FOR LEFT TURNS ONLY RESPONSE N TOT % NM % CUM % A 23 10.8 12.4 12.4 B 102 48.1 54.8 67.2 C 61 28.8 32.8 100.0 CORRECT OM 26M 12.3M NA RESPONSES TO ITEM 5 = 186 "Have you ever seen this sign while driving?" TOT NM% CUM o\° o\° YES: 49 (23.1%) NO: 161 NO RESPONSE: Number of Responses % of All Responses % of Valid Responses Cumulative % NA NO RESPONSE 2 141 Table E—6 Item 6: Right Curve 6. UPON SEEING THIS SIGN A DRIVER SHOULD: A. SLOW AND LOOK FOR RIGHT CURVE B. WATCH FOR TRAFFIC ON SIDE ROADS C. PROCEED AT NORMAL SPEED RESPONSE N TOT % NM % CUM % A 200 94.3 94.3 94.3 CORRECT B 3 1.4 1.4 95.8 C 9 4.2 4.2 100.0 RESPONSES TO ITEM 6 = 212 "Have you ever seen this sign while driving?" YES: 206 (97.2%) NO: 6 NO RESPONSE: 0 N = Number of Responses TOT % = % of All Responses NM% = % of Valid Responses CUM % = Cumulative % 142 Table E—7 Item 7: T Intersection 7. UPON SEEING THIS SIGN A DRIVER SHOULD: A. PREPARE TO TURN LEFT OR RIGHT B. DRIVE AT A SAFE SPEED C. REDUCE SPEED AND DRIVE WITH CARE RESPONSE N TOT % NM % CUM % A 170 80.2 86.3 86.3 CORRECT B 4 1.9 2.0 88.3 C 23 10.8 11.7 100.0 0 15 7.1 NA NA NO RESPONSE RESPONSES TO ITEM 7 = 197 "Have you ever seen this sign while driving?" YES: 117 (55.2%) NO: 93 NO RESPONSE: 2 N = Number of Responses TOT % = % of All Responses NM% = % of Valid Responses CUM % = Cumulative % 143 Table E—8 Item 8: Merging Traffic 8. UPON SEEING THIS SIGN A DRIVER SHOULD: A. WATCH FOR TRAFFIC ENTERING FROM RIGHT B. PREPARE TO STOP C. MERGE WITH TRAFFIC RESPONSE N TOT % NM % CUM % A 175 82.5 83.7 83.7 CORRECT B 1 .5 .5 84.2 C 33 15.6 15.8 100.0 0 3 1.4 NA NA NO RESPONSE RESPONSES TO ITEM 8 = 209 "Have you ever seen this sign while driving?" YES: 173 (81.6%) NO: 35 NO RESPONSE: 4 N = Number of Responses TOT % = % of All Responses NM% = % of Valid Responses CUM % = Cumulative % 144 Table E—9 Item 9: Merge Left 9. UPON SEEING THIS SIGN A DRIVER SHOULD: A. WATCH FOR ONCOMING TRAFFIC B. MERGE LEFT C. WATCH FOR LOW SHOULDER RESPONSE N TOT % NM % CUM % A 31 14.6 15.2 15.2 B 152 71.7 74.5 89.7 CORRECT C 21 9.9 10.3 100.0 0 8 3.8 NA NA NO RESPONSE RESPONSES TO ITEM 9 204 "Have you ever seen this sign while driving?" YES: 149 (70.3%) NO: 57 NO RESPONSE: 6 N = Number of Responses TOT % = % of All Responses NM% - % of Valid Responses CUM ° Cumulative s o\° 1| 145 Table E-lO Item 10: Divided Highway Ahead 10. UPON SEEING THIS SIGN A DRIVER SHOULD: A. LOOK FOR TWO WAY TRAFFIC AHEAD B. MOVE RIGHT TO AVOID OBSTRUCTION C. EXPECT A DIVIDED HIGHWAY RESPONSE N TOT % NM % CUM % A 20 9.4 10.0 10.0 B 30 14.2 15.0 25.0 C 150 70.8 75.0 100.0 CORRECT O 12 5.7 NA NA NO RESPONSE RESPONSES TO ITEM 10 = 200 "Have you ever seen this sign while driving?" YES: 129 (60.8%) NO: 78 NO RESPONSE: 5 Number of Responses 0 s of All Responses 6 of Valid Responses Cumulative % 146 Table E-ll Item 11: Slippery Road 11. UPON SEEING THIS SIGN A DRIVER SHOULD: A. LOOK FOR WET SLIPPERY ROAD B. EXPECT A ROAD FOR CARS ONLY C. LOOK FOR A SERIES OF CURVES RESPONSE N TOT % NM % CUM % A 193 91.0 92.8 92.8 CORRECT B 1 .5 .5 93.3 C 14 6.6 6.7 100.0 0 4 1.9 NA NA NO RESPONSE RESPONSES TO ITEM 11 = 208 "Have you ever seen this sign while driving?" YES: 178 (84.0%) NO: 21 NO RESPONSE: 4 N = Number of Responses TOT % = % of All Responses NM% = of Valid Responses 0 % Cumulative 6 ('3 C.‘ 3 o\° 11 147 Table E-12 Item 12: Low Clearance — 12 Feet 6 Inches 12. UPON SEEING THIS SIGN A DRIVER SHOULD: EXPECT ROAD TO NARROW TO 12 FEET 6 INCHES EXPECT LOW CLEARANCE OF 12 FEET 6 INCHES USE PARKING SPACES 12 FEET 6 INCHES noo> RESPONSE N TOT % NM % CUM % 2.9 2.9 100.0 CORRECT NA NA NO RESPONSE otbfi’ N o N KO rewim tooim o q H RESPONSES TO ITEM 12 = 208 "Have you ever seen this sign while driving?" YES: 191 (90.1%) NO: 18 NO RESPONSE: 3 Z 11 Number of Responses TOT % % of All Responses NM% % of Valid Responses CUM % 0 Cumulative a 'A\ 148 Table E—13 Item 13: Narrow Bridge 13. THIS SIGN MEANS: A. NARROW BRIDGE B. OBSTRUCTION IN ROAD C. SOFT SHOULDER RESPONSE N TOT % NM % CUM % A 176 83.0 92.6 92.6 CORRECT B 7 3.3 3.7 96.3 C 7 3.3 3.7 100.0 0 22 10.4 NA NA NO RESPONSE RESPONSES TO ITEM 13 = 190 "Have you ever seen this sign while driving?" YES: 48 (22.6%) NO: 158 NO RESPONSE: 6 N = Number of Responses TOT % = % of All Responses NM% % of Valid Responses CUM % = Cumulative % 149 Table E—l4 Item 14: Workers On Road 14. THIS SIGN MEANS: A. FLAGMAN AHEAD B. ROAD WORK C. WORKERS ON ROAD RESPONSE N TOT % NM % CUM % A 33 15.6 16.2 16.2 B 39 18.4 19.1 35.3 C 132 62.3 64.7 100.0 CORRECT 0 8 3.8 NA NA NO RESPONSE RESPONSES TO ITEM 14 = 204 "Have you ever seen this sign while driving?" YES: 98 (46.2%) NO: 109 NO RESPONSE: 5 N = Number of Responses TOT % = % of All Responses NM% = % of Valid Responses CUM % = Cumulative % 150 Table E-lS Item 15: Two Way Traffic 15. THIS SIGN MEANS: A. TWO-WAY TRAFFIC B. STOP FOR ON-COMING TRAFFIC C. DIVIDED ROAD RESPONSE N TOT % NM % CUM % 208 98.1 98.1 98.1 CORRECT 4 1.9 1.9 100.0 03> RESPONSES TO ITEM 14 = 212 "Have you ever seen this sign while driving?" TOT NM% CUM o\° YES: 176 (83.0%) NO: 31 NO RESPONSE: = Number of Responses % of All Responses % of Valid Responses Cumulative % AAAAAAAAAAAAAAAAA ‘1“111.111111611111111111‘3 30 “111111 293