R8L§uEXP§fiAfiON PATTERNS AMONG {JNQ'EED STAKES AND MEXECAN Héfifi SCHCGL STUbEflTS: AN EMPERECAL S‘WDY O$ SCME APPLICABlUfiES OF THE SOCIAL SYSTEM SCHEMA Thesis f0? fin Degree 9! Ph. D. MiCHGAN S'M‘s'i UNIVERSSTY Frank Cemsf‘ock Nell H 1959 This is to certify that the thesis entitled ROLE-EXPECTATION PATTERNS AMONG UNITED STATES AND MEXICAN HIGH SCHOOL STUDENTS: AN EMPIRICAL STUDY OF SOME APPLICABILITIES OF THE SOCIAL SYSTEM SCHEMA presented by FRANK COMSTOCK NALL II has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for Ph.D. degree in Sociology an Anthropology Major professor Date December LI; 1958 0-169 LIBRARY Michigan State University .w. ' . r..- fly! I in. —~'.‘- ‘- ..-I. o _...h-“‘ ‘ 0“". ‘ — J5“. ‘ ‘4 . "I _ -. u" '- g-O-v-O--'~ (/1 <9 ROLE-EXPECTATION PATTERNS AMONG UNITED STATES AND MEXICAN HIGH SCHOOL STUDENTS: AN EMPIRICAL STUDY OF EDME APPLICABILITIES OF THE SOCIAL SYSTEM SCHEMA By Frank Comatock Nall II A THESIS Submitted to the School of Advanced Graduate Studies of Michigan State University of Agriculture and Applied Science in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTGZ OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Sociology and AnthrOpology Year 195 9 (A Approved jégfifit/‘x :y. 7% AN ABSTRACT The general focus of the study'was a sub-area of the problem of moral integration of society; A range of data drawn from high school students living within the metropolitan communities of El Paso, Texas, and Ciudad Juarez, Mexico, was gathered and analyzed. The frame of reference which structured the research was that of the social system schema. The study had two principal purposes: the exploration of the applicability of the social system schema to a limited problem in empir- ical analysis, and the furtherance of general sociological knowledge of the structure of social relations in the United States—hexican border area. Four principal prOpositions derived from the social system schema were formulated and subjected to empirical testing. ‘The relationshi, between cultural value standards and social role-expectations was exam— ined and it was inferred from the findings that social role-expectations do not necessarily represent an unmodified tranSposition of culture value standards. The relationship between the extent of di fcrentiation of social relations in a social system.and the universalistic-particular- istic dimension of role-expectations was examined also. It was inferred that as the extent of differentiation increases the incidence of univer— Salistic-type role-expectations increases also. Finally, the relation- Ship between the extent of differentiation in a system.and the self-col- lectivity'dimension of role-expectations was examined. It was inferred that as the extent of differentiation increases the incidence of collec- 1-. Q ,.-j a!" _" ‘ .'--- '.-- “" " q.... 0" .'~ ‘ .7 ‘ ‘ ._,....~ 1 " . Q A II- A 7" _ 0,- , . -— . v .o " -‘I '- . _.q . " .- , " , uv - . 1 . ....e . >4 I. -l.~¢ ‘ov-s .’ A .I- a .- . -o, . - "‘\---u . "‘--. 'u . ‘-’-.‘. . 'tivityaorientation toward the family decreases and the incidence of col- lectivityeorientation toward the peer group increases. Ethnic status differences were found to account for differences in the patterns of role-expectations exhibited by the United States students. Social status differences did~not appear to be related to the patterns of role-expectations exhibited, except with reSpect to the high status Lexicans and Anglo-Americans. These latter showed convergence in role- expectations types. It was concluded that the data imply either (I) that the system of social relations in which both the Lexican and United States students are involved lack a high level of order, or (2) that a high degree of moral integration may not constitute so vital a feature of the mainten- ance of stability and order as Parsons and others would surgest. This study favored the latter view. r.-- wr. -_ p, . -Auu n7" " .... .n e. or “' ' , J .o -’ ‘ l - .-.. .7 .. v.0 '-- .A . .1-‘. cu... . . l -. .'..,.- '--‘ . . ~..- -. ._ .., . .-. s. ‘ . ~.| ‘ o - v. ; , ,“'IA-- . § -' u . .i .. a. a,. Due-o. 7-. ._,‘_, . ~ u .._ - _ ..-I ‘ w. ' a- __ \-' 0-... n- . . "T 5‘4 s ,, ~. . e a "-. ~. " ._u ‘uIQ. -.. w '- A . .. ‘ ._._ x 1‘ ' 1-.-“- \- .-. _| 2-- . ' l-..‘ . ‘ 7- . a.‘ ". . ‘ -.. V‘w 'N - N... ' .. '\ u ._...s - ’ It-.. . ~.v _‘\.o '._" m, \ . 'V ‘. .. ,. O .0. a .g -. ‘- . ev- .- . n a .n. . '-.". u u‘ . ‘,\ .‘ A ' ‘ .'\ ‘ H ACKNOWLEDGEthTS I want here to express my thanks to some of the many peeple who in one way or another helped me to write this thesis. I am particularly indebted to Dr. William Form who acted as my major advisor, read and crit- icized several versions of the manuscript, and contributed numerous help- ful suggestions. I deeply appreciate his helpfulness and friendly coun- seling. Dr. Charles P. Loomis, former Head of the Department of Sociology and AnthrOpology, originally interested me in doing field work in Latin America and provided the means whereby that interest was implemented. I owe Dr. Loomis my gratitude for his continuing encouragement of my pur- suance of this study. fioreover, he has been a constant mentor throughout my academic career. I am eSpecially grateful to Drs. John Useem, Charles hoffer, Uilbur brookover, and Paul Morrison, members of my guidance committee, for their help in planning a program of graduate studies. 1 am also grateful to the Carnegie Corporation for the financial assistance which made possible the field work which was conducted on the Border Project. The public school authorities, teachers, and students in El Paso and Ciudad Juarez who par- ticipated in the study deserve Special recognition. Without their helpful cooperation the research most certainly could not have been accomplished. Two former faculty members of hichigan State University should be men- tioned here. Dr. John HcKinney stimulated my interest in systematic theory, particularly in the social system theory of Talcott Parsons. Mr. Kenneth - _ .. ,,_. ';",, ,A .~. " , J... _. -.- -s--"' .‘ . 1 ‘~ -“ ' ' I . .~... . c. - r .. a- -.I '-_. -. ~..-.-. to-.. . - . 'I . .. .- |~.-- . a. 'u‘-Ol - "‘- -‘-.- v. - ‘0 . ~ . - ' "n .n I. -‘ - ' 1--h . . "‘ ‘>,. .- a. ' "-.- . __ ‘ r -. ; "' wen-o. *‘ »..o-, . a . .,- .- I'd_ .. — . ‘ -~§~ , .‘ 0. § \7 "'.‘ a. ., . .'-b ~‘A , '\ .._ ~. -‘ ... . I. h n I ‘. ‘_.~- .1“. v "QA‘_ \ ul.‘ nl' ‘u- ' l~ua . ~. \ p .. . ‘ '. ea.-- "‘ n “_ A v \- ~‘,-' I x ‘ ‘ ' ‘- .‘ I. .‘M. ' "4-. .. . x ‘. _ la. v. *n. '7 . “'0. P ‘ ‘. V “c .' -. ‘_ -._ ‘ ‘. ‘, D‘ i. '.v. ~..‘~ ~r ‘ ‘ . Q .7 . ‘ n ,- ~ .- C..' ;\. .,.~ .P Ne w a . ‘I x .‘a .m- \. °.. I.“ 3 ‘h s . . ~‘. ._‘_ - '0 v. U‘. D ‘- 1" ACKNOWED GElhfiI-YI'S I want here to eXpress my thanks to some of the many people who in one way or another helped me to write this thesis. I am particularly indebted to Dr. William Form who acted as my major advisor, read and crit- icized several versions of the manuscript, and contributed numerous help- ful suggestions. I deeply appreciate his helpfulness and friendly coun- seling. Dr. Charles P. loomis, former Head of the Department of Sociology and AnthrOpol gy, originally interested me in doing field work in Latin America and provided the means whereby that interest was implemented. I owe Dr. Loomis my gratitude for his continuing encouragement of my pur— suance of this study. fioreover, he has been a constant mentor throughout my'academic career. I am eSpecially grateful to Drs. John Useem, Charles hoffer, Uilbur Brookover, and Paul Morrison, members of my guidance committee, for their help in planning a program of graduate studies. 1 am also grateful to the Carnegie Corporation for the financial assistance which made possible the field work which was conducted on the Border Project. The public school authorities, teachers, and students in El Paso and Ciudad Juarez who par— ticipated in the study deserve Special recognition. 'Nithout their helpful cooperation the research most certainly could not have been accomplished. Two former faculty members of Michigan State University should be men- tioned here. Dr. John KcKinney stimulated my interest in systematic theory, particularly in the social system theory of Talcott Parsons. Hr. Kenneth Tiedke, a close friend, was one of my outstanding teachers. I wish to thank my friends, hr. and hrs. mussell ncKee, for their help in preparing the manuscript. To my wife, Elizabeth Williams Hall, I owe special thanks for her unfailing aid and comfort during the lengthy period recuired to complete this thesis. Her incisive thinking contributed signiiicantly to this work. law-.v“ ‘ in ~-\- , A.. o \-. ..-. . .- “a... A.‘ 04‘ 0 .~ .. . . . -«A~- U- __ I Chapter I. II. III. IV. TABLE OF COI‘IDLN’L‘S II‘HRODUCTIOI‘I TO TIIE STUDY .ooooooooooooooooo0.0.0.0000... PerSpectives of the Study ............................ The Problem .......................................... The Social System Schema ............................. Kethodoloaical Critinue .............................. Types of Role-Expectations and Features of the Social Structure .................................. E'BEI‘HODOI-DGY 0......00....OOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOO. l‘lethOd .0000......0.0.0....O...OCOOOOOCOOOOOOOOOOOI... Procedures and Technioues ............................ DeveIOpment, Translation, Reliability and Pre-testing of dole-expectation Stories ........... heasurement of Dimension of Role—eXpectations: the Guttman Scales ................................ Selection of Reapondents and Questionnaire Administration .................................... VALUE STANDARDS AND SOCIAL STRUCTURAL COLPLEKES ....... Mexican Backgrounds .................................. Revolution and Change ................................ Rural-Urban Features ................................. Occupations and.labor ................................ Literacy ............................................. Formal I-lass Education o.o0.000000000000000coon-.00.... Government cococoo...0000000000000000.00.000.000.o0000 Commerce and Industry ................................ Sumary .00..0.0000000.oo.000000000000000.000000000000 THE BEARING OF THE EKPIRICAL FINDINGS ON THE GEEJRAL THEORLTICAL IRPLICATICNS ............................ General Implications: Proposition I .................. General Implications: PrOposition II ................. General Implications: Proposition III ................ General Implications: Proposition IV ................. smflry 00.0.0000...OOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOIOOOOOOOOOOC.... AL 13 2o 29 37 37 39 [#4. A6 57 62 66 67 7O 71 73 77 78 8O 83 89 92 9h c v C C O o O Q I O . O . o -- o n a u 0 O a D O I O n I I O C O O I . . I u n o c o O O O u . . . OOOIOI‘ V. SOCIAL ROLE-EXPECTATIONS AID ETHNICITY IN THE UZIIIIIISD STATJID‘ brI‘UDLIIII.‘ (III-CUPooooooooooooooooooooooooooooo 102 The Principal Ethnic Status Division of the United States Student Group............................. 102 Ethnic Status aithin the Spanish-American Groun ........ 110 smmflrsr .OOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOO0.0...... 120 VI. A FURTHJR EXALINfiTIGN OF TIS RALATIULSHIP SHPXLSH ROLE-EKPECTATICUS AND ETHNIC STATUS IN INS UI¢ITISD STA‘III—‘Ifi SrFLID.JIIYP (BI-0UP . o o o o o o o o o o o o o o o o o o o o o o o o o 0 L25 Language Spoken to Peers as an Index of Ethnic Status Differences among the Spanish-Americans ............ 128 Specification of the delationship between Ethnic Group Integration or Identification and Types of dole- Expectations ....................................... 130 Analysis of the Data .................................. 133 A Further Test of the Relationship between Role- Expectations Patterns and Ethnic Status Differences among Spanish-Americans ............................ 1L3 Smrlf .QCOOOOOOOOOOOOOOO0.00000000000000000000000000. 159 VII. EFFECTS OF CROSS-CULTURAL EDUCATION ”N MEXICAN STUDSHTS' ROLEPL‘XPJ-SCrl'All‘IOI-IS .00....0.00.0...0000000000.00.0000... 167 AnalZJSis Of the Data 0000......OOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOO 17’; smmry .OOOOOOOOOOCIOO0...0.0..OOOOOOOOOOOOOOCOOOOOOQQ 18U VIII. THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN ROLE—EXPSCTATIOLS AVD SOCIAL STATUS 18h Intranational Comparisons ............................. 191 Cross-cultural Comparisons ............................ 193 81.1.1er0.00.00.00.00...oooooooooooooooooooo000.000.... 203 IX. SUIHEZARY AI‘ID COIICIUSICIIS .OOOOOOOOOOOOOO...OOOOOOOOOOOOOOO 205 methodology and Instruments .......................... 205 Empirical Findings ................................... 208 Types of Role-expectations and fithnic Status Differences 211 Types of Role-expectations and Social Status Differences 215 Effects of Cross-cultural fiducation on Mexican Students' Role-expectations ................................. 217 The Problem of Loral Integration and the Social System Schema ..................................... 218 floral Integration and Social Structure .............. 226 O D \ Q o o I O I O b O D Q I - u - l I O . . q a c o O u n I Q C u u D Q - I ‘ I C o ' O 0- o u y o . - O ‘- 0 a O O I Q § 0 o o - . I ' I C 0 O C TABLE 1. TABLE 2. TABLE 3. TABLE 4. TABLE 5 . TABLE 6. TABLE 7. TABLE 8. TABLE 9. TABLE 10. TABLE 11. . LIST OF TABLES CLASSIFICATION OF RESPONSE CATEGORIES BY PATTERN VARIABLESTYPEQQOOOOQOOO0000000. I43 SCALOGRAM PATTERN FOR RESPOerRITS T0 was ON SMEI MID GM'SQIARE ANALYSIS 0 o o o o o o o o o 53 SCALOCRAM PATTERN FOR RESPONDENTS TO ITEMS ON SCALE II AND CHI-SQUARE ANALYSIS . . . . . . . . . Sh SCALOGRAN PATTERN FOR RESPONDSITS TO ITmS ON SCALE III AND CHI-SQUARE ANALYSIS . . . . . . . . . 55 SCALOGRAJA PATTERN FOR RESPONDENTS T0 ITEIS ON SCALE IV AND GTE-SQUARE ANALYSIS . . . . . . . . . . 5'6 DISTRIBUTION BY NATIrTALITY OF SCALE TYPES DERIVED FROL: RESPONSES To ROLE-CONFLICT SITUATIONS INVOLVING AN ECONOLCEC ROLE VERSUS AFANNLIALROLR(SCALEI)............. 86 DYSTRIRUTI ON BY NATIONALITY OF SCALE TYPES DARTVSD TRON RTSPONSPS TO ROLE-COT-TPLICT SITUATICNS INVOLVING AN INS’I‘RUZFHTTAL ROLE VERSUS APRIRIDSRIPROLMSCALEII)............ 86 DISTRIBUTION BY NATIONALITY OF SCALE TYPES DRRTVRD TRON RESPONSES TO ROLE—CONFLICT SITUATIONS INVOLVING SRF-ORIEEV‘I'ATION VARSUS COLLECTIVITY- ORIENTATION TOWARD TRE FALZLLY (SCALE III) . . . . . 91 DISTRIBUTION BY NATIONALITY OF SCALE TYPES DERIVED FROM RESPONSES To ROLE-CONFLICT SITUATICNS INVOLVING SEF-ORIE‘JTATION VERSUS COILEGTIVITY-ORIENTATION TOWARD THE PM GROUP (SCALE IV) . . . . . . . . . 91 DISTRIBUTION BY ETHNIC GROUPS OF SCALE TYPES DERIVED FROM RESPONSES To ROLE-CONFLICT SITUATIONS INVOLVING AN EGONORIC ROLE VERSUS A FAITZILIAL ROLE (SCALE 1) . . 10A DISTRIBUTION BY ETaIIC GROUPS OF SCALE TYPES DERIVED TRON RESPONSES To ROLE-CONFLICT SITUATIONS INVOLVING AN INSTRUMENTAL ROLE VFRSUS A FRIENDSHIP ROLE (SGALEII) 107 C a I a . g a o I g u ' O I I I u u I C Q O u o - . . a o v p o . ‘- I a . - . . a . . O - o v 4 Join. 1-1.. I .L‘dl‘u un‘. '~.< a A“ . . 0.x- :- 1- . Jazz. 1?: . ~- 7" .I’V A 3:. ~ n .y' N p. t.‘ TABLE 1. TABLE 2. TABLE 3. TABLE 4. TABLE 5 . TABLE 6. TABLE 7. TABLE 8. TABLE 9. TABLE 10. , LIST OF TABLES CLASSIFICATION OF RESPONSE CATEGORIES BY PATTERN VARIABLESTYPB.................. ‘43 SCMOGRAM PATTERN FOR RESPCIIDI‘EIITS TO ITFA‘LS ON SCALEI AND CH‘SQJIEE ANALYSIS 0 o o o o o o o o a 53 SCALOCBAM PATTERN FOR RESPONDENTS TO ITEIAZS ON SCALE II AND CHI-SQUARE ANALYSIS . . . . . . . . . 5h SCALOGRAN PATTERN FOR RESPONDEITS TO ITENS ON SCALE III AND cm-SinRE ANALYSIS . . . . . . . . . 55 SCALOGRAM PATTETN FOR RESPONDENTS TO ITEIZS ON SCALE IV AND CHI-SQUARE ANALYSIS . . . . . . . . . . 56 DISTRIBUTIOF‘T BY NATI!"T-’:‘LITY OF SCALE TYPES DERIVED FROL‘L RESPONSES TO ROLE-CONFLICT SITUATIONS INVOLVING AN ECONOE‘JIC ROLE VERSUS ‘FAWILIALROLE(SCALEI)cocoa-coocoo. 86 DISTRIBUTION BY NATIONALITY OF SCALE TYPES DERIVED IRON RESPONSES TO ROLE-CONFLICT SITUATIONS INVOLVING AN INSTRUIETTAL ROLE VERSUS ATRIENIASHIPROLE(SCALEII)............ 86 DISTRIBUTION BY NATIONALITY 0F SCALE TYPES DERIVED TRON RESPONSES TO ROLE—CONPLICT SITUATIONS INVOLVING SEE-ORIENTATION VERSUS COLLECTIVITY- ORIENTATION TONARD THE EATILLY (SCALE III) . . . . . 91 DISTRIBUTION BY NATIONALITY OF SCALE TYPES DEzIVED TRON RESPONSES To ROLE-CONFLICT SITUATIONS INVOLVING SEE-ORIEITATION VERSUS COILECTIVITY-ORIENTATION TOWARDTHEPE’ERCROUP(SCALEIV)......... 91 DISTRIBUTION BY ETHNIC GROUPS OF SCALE TYPES DERIVED FROM RESPONSES To ROLE. CONFLICT SITUATIONS INVOLVING AN ECONOAEC ROLE VERSUS A EANELIAL ROLE (SCALE I) . . 10A DISTRIBUTION BY ETHNIC GROUPS OF SCALE TYPES DERIVED IRON RESPONSES To ROLE-CONFLICT SITUATI ONS INVOLVING AN INSTRUMENTAL ROLE VERSUS A FRIEJDSIHP ROLE (SCALEII)...................... 107 ,“n-q Ln -‘-o yw-q- ‘- q ‘0'." " “‘04“ V- ‘. . I 4' f‘ (H >; 4! O I o.» I TABLE 12. TABLE 13. TABLE 1.6,. TABLE 15. TABLE 16. TABLE 17. TABLE 18. TABLE 19. TABLE 20. TABLE 21. TABLE 22. DISTRIBUTION BY ETHNIC GROUPS OF SCALE TYPES DERIVED TRON RESPONSES TO ROLE-CONPLICT SITUATIONS INVOLVING SEF-ORIHVTATION VERSUS COLLECTIVITY- IRRITATION TONARDTNEFANILY (SCALEIII) . . . . . . . . . . . . DISTRIE'LTTICIT EST ETHNIC GROUPS OF SCALE TYPES DERIVED FROM RESPONSES TO ROLE-CONFLICT SITUATIC-‘NS INVOLVING SELF- ORI EITATI ON VERSU S COLLECTIVITY—ORI HIT ATI ON TOWARD THE PEER GROUP (SCALE IV) . . . . . . . . . . RRATICNSHIP BETTIEEII ETHIIC STATUS AND MIII RANK SCORES ON EACH OF THE PATTERN VIELIABLES SCALES . . . TYPODOGYOFETIHIICTRIJTS.............. DISTRIBUTION BY LTPTIC STATUS TYPES OF SCALE TYPES DERIVED FNULE RESPONSES TO ROLE-CONFLICT SITUATIONS INVOLVING AN BCONOYIG ROLE VERSUS A PANTLIAL ROLE (SCALEI)...................... DISTRIBUTION BY ETPTIG STATUS TYPES OF SCALE TYPES DMVED TRON RESPONSES To ROLE-CONFLICT SITUATIONS INVOLVING AN INSTRUAZEIITAL ROLE VERSUS A FPUINDSRTP ROLE(SCALEII)................... DISTRIBUTION BY ETTNIC STATUS TYPES OF SCALE TYPES DERIVED TRON RESPONSES TO ROLE-CONFLICT SITUATIONS INVOLVING SELF-OR STTATION VERSUS COLLECITIVTY- OBIS-NATION TONARD TRB FAMILY (SCALE III) . . . . . DISTRIBUTION BY ETHNIC STATUS TYPES OF SCALE TYPES DERIVED TRON RESPONSES To ROLE—CONFLICT SITUATIONS INVOLVING SALT-ORIENTATION VE‘ASUS COLLECTIVITY— ORIENTATION TONAIO) THE. PAIR GROUP (SCALE IV) . . . SPANISH-AMERICAI'IS BY LAI‘IGUAGE SPOKEN TO PEERS, DISTRIBUTION OF SCALE ”YPES DERIVED TRON RESPONSES TO ROLE-CONFLI CT SITUAT ONS INVOLVING AN ECONOLIC ROLE VERSUS A FAISLIAL ROLE (SCALE I) . . . . . . . SPANISH-AMERICANS BY LANGUAGE SPOKEN TO BEETS, DISTRIBUTION OF SCALE TYPES DERIVED FROM RESPOI‘TSES TO ROLE-CONFLICT SITUATIONS INVOLVING AN INSTRUI‘AHITAL ROLE VERSUS A FRIENDSHIP ROLE (SCALE II) . . . . . . SPANISH-AMERICANS BY LANGUAGE SPONIN TO PAIRS, DISTRIBUTIOT—I 0F SCALE TYPES DERIVED F303;: RESPONSES TO ROLE—CONPLI CT SITUATIONS INVOLVING SELF-a ORIHITATION VERSUS COLLECTIVITY-ORI ETTATI ON T NARD TREFN.;1Y(SCALEIII) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 107 107 109 116 117 119 135 ' 135 138 .q' .- M «‘0 09‘- I 1.... q! .\ I‘ A...” Q 7-, IA. ~o 3.3;} J‘;‘3 .. .5 - ..r - I)..- .. ; ‘9... fl TABLE 23. TABLE 25. TABLE 26. TABLE 27. TABLE 29. TABLE 30. TABLE 31. SPANISH-ALIELICAEIS BY LECGUAGE SPOKE: TO PEEELS, DISTRIBUTION OF SCALE TYPES DELIVED TRON RESP NSES To ROLE—CONPLI CT SITUATITII’S INVOLVING STEP—ORIENTATION VERSUS GOLLECTIVITY-ORIEJTATION TONARD THE PEER GROUP (SGALEIV)...................... 138 ANGLO-ANEPI CANS AND SPA’II SI-z-ATEI CAIIS SPENLING ETGLI SR TO BEARS, DISTRIBUTION OP SCALE TYPES DETIVED FROM RESPONSES TO ROLE—COUPLICT SITUATIONS INVOLVING AN NOTIC ROLE VERSUS A PAILIAL ROLE (SCALE I ) . . . 1A5 ANGLO-AI'EERICAIIS AIID SENIISH-AAEBICAIIS SPEMII‘IG ENGLISH TO PETERS, DISTRIBUTION OF SCALE TYPES DERIVED FROIJI RESPOZ‘ISES TO ROLE- COI‘IELI CT SITUATIOI‘IS IIIVOLVIZIG AII IIISTHUEVLLIITAL ROLE VJISUS A FFCIPI‘IDSELIP ROLE (SCALE II) 115 ANGLO-AVEI CANS AND SPANISH-ASHIICAIIS SPEAKING NIGLISR TO PAIRS, DISTRIBUTION CF SCALE TYPES DERIVED TRON RESPONSES TO ROLE—CORILICT SITUATIONS IT-IVOLVIJG SELE- ORIETTATI ON VITSUS COLLECTIVITLORIEzTA-TI ON TOAARD TREPLILY(SCALEIII)............... 1&6 AITGLO-A:IRICANS AND SPATVI STA-ALERT CANS SPEAKING SIGLISR TO PEBBS, DISTRIBUTION OF SCALE TYPES DERIVED PR 2:: RESPONSES TO ROLE—CONFLICT SITUATIO2-rs INVOLVING SELF- ORIEI’I‘ATIGN VERSUS GOLLECTIVITY-OPJENTATION TONARD TREPEERCBOUP(SCALEIV)... . . . . .. 11:6 AIEIICANS AND SPANISH-AARICANS SPEAKING SPANISH TO PENIS, DISTRIBUTION OF SCALE TYPES DERIVED TRON RESPONSES TO ROLE-C ’NELICT SITUATI ONS INVOLVING AN INSTRUNENTAL ROLE VERSUS A FRIEJDSTNP ROLE (SCALE II) 1L8 EEXICANS AND SPAITI SIT-A4311 CANS SPEAKING SPANI SR TO PEEBS, DISTRIBUTION OF SCALE TYPES DERIVED TRON RESPONSES TO ROLE-CONFLICT SITUATIONS II-IVCLVII-IG SELE- ORI ANTATI ON VETSUS COLLECTIVITY-CRI EEITATI ON TOUABD THEPEERGIOUP(SCALEIV)............. 1A8 ANmo-AAmICANS AND SPANISH-NATIONS TAO ARE BILINGIAL ‘AITB PETS, DISTRIBUTION OP SCALE TYPES DERIVED TRON RESPONSES To ROLE.- CONPLICT SITUATIONS INVOLVING AN ECONOLIC ROLE VERSUS A PAINTIAL ROLE (SCALEI)..................... 150 ANGLO-NIBICNS AND SPE-IISN-AEAI Cit-IS TOTO ARE BILINGUAL LITE BEARS, DISTRIBUTION OP SCALE TYPES DERIVED TRON RESPOI‘TSES To ROLE-COIFLIOT SITUATIONS INVOLVING AN IIIS’I‘RUILEAZTAL ROLE VERSUS A FRIE’NDSEIP ROLE(SCALEII).................. 150 --A« .- {ad ,1. .~. I K . It“ I". ’ w: ‘J l‘ . u. “‘ u! ‘i ' l. I". “a. q I r I I . 1;. N/d .I‘; 7, \N 4:. :0.— 0,. Jul»- ?" b u b"‘ 3 i f" v C i, v f y I f . v . ‘ j g ' c I . . A I o: w L: w TABLE 32. TABLE 33. TABLE 34. TABLE 35 . TABLE 36. TABLE 3'7 . TABLE 38 TABLE 39. TABLE 40 ANGLO-ALIERICAI-TS AND SPEII SN..N:E>.ICAIS WHO ABLE BILINGUAL WITH PEERS, DISTRIBUTION OE SCALE TYPES DERIVED TRON RESPONSES TO ROLE-CONFLICT SITUATIONS INVOLVING SEP-ORIEVTATION VERSUS COLLECTIVITY- ORIENTATION TOWARD THE PAIAILY (SCALE III) . . . . . 151 ANGLO-AIVIERI CANS AND SPA’VI SH-AuEI'I CANS WHO ARE BILINGUAL WITH PESLS, DISTRIBUTION OF SCALE TYPES DERIVED IRON RBBPOI-JSES TO ROLE-CONPLICT SITUATIONS INVOLVING SEE-OMEEATION VERSUS COLLECTIVITY- ORIENTATION TOTARD THE PEER GROUP (SCALE IV) , 151 NEH CANS ND SPAVI SI—I-ANERI CANS WHO ARE BILINGUAL WITH PEERS, DISTRIBUTION OF SCALE TYPES DERIVED FROM RESPONSES TO ROLE—CONFLICT SITUATIONS INVOLVING AN ECONOMIC ROLE VERSUS A FALILIAL ROLE (SCALE I) . . . 151, MEXICANS AND SPA‘TISH-ALERICANS WHO ARE BILINGUAL WITH FEES, DISTRIBUTION OF SCALE TYPES DHZIVED FROM RESPONSES TO ROLE-CONFLICT SITUATIOI‘ES INVOLVING AN DISTRUMHVTAL ROLE VERSUS A FRI ExIDSI-HP ROLE (SCALE II) 151; mm CANS AND SPANISH-AER: CAI-2S )‘JI-IO A-E BILINGUAL NITN PEERS, DISTRIBUTION OF SCALE TYPES DERIVED FROM RESPONSES TO ROLE-CONFLICT SITUATIONS INVOLVING SEF-ORIENTATION VERSUS COLLECTIVITY-ORIEITATI ON TOWARD THE FAMILY (SCALEIII)..................... 155 AEXI CANS AND SPEECH-AER: CPS-IS 1330 AB BILIII" TAL RITA PEERS, DISTIEUTI??? OP SCALE TYPES DERIVED TRON RESPOI—ISES TO ROLE-COT’EIICT SI‘I‘L‘AIIL‘ITS INVOLVING SELE- ORIEETATION VERSUS COLLECTIVITI-OR EITATIUN TOVRRD THEPEELEYOUP(SCALEIV) . . . . . . . . . . . . 155 RELATIONSHIP BATTER; TRAIT OF LANGUAGE SPOKE: TO PEERS BY SPANISH-ANERI CAN STUDEA-ITS AIED PATPEU-IS CE ROLE- EXPECTATIONS EAIEBITEJ ON THE FOUR PATTERN VARIABLE ENGLISH LANGUAGE TRAINERS AND OTISR 11m CAI-:3, DISTRIBUTION OF SCALE TYPES DERIVED FROI-‘l. RESPOI-ISES TO ROLE-CONFLICT SITUATIONS INVOLVING AN ECONOLIC ROLE VERSUS A PANILIAL ROLE (SCALE 1) . . . . . . . . . . 175 ENGLISH LANGUAGE TRAINERS AND ANGLO-A‘L’EII CANS, DISTRIBUTION OF SCALE TYPES DERIVED RIO: RESPONSES TO ROLE. COI-IPLICT SITUATI ONS INVOLVING AN ECONOLEC ROLE VERSUS A FAEAEILIAL ROLE (SCALE I) . . . . . . . . . . 17S uu‘ . .C . --.n . LJ ' ' ~10 O ‘5. L1,; , . v" p "u .I l 1 A 5 ‘¢‘ Q... 1 . l‘ I by _ , . . . .‘.‘ .‘I 'R h! ’ u a 9 C I ' I I > I Q C I .\ - I . o g . n n ‘- .\ ‘ IN I I .f ' kw v ' 0 I I . . AA {. dd TABLE 4.1. TABLE 42 TABLE 43. TABLE 41.. TABLE 45. TABLE 46 TABLE 47 TABLE [.8 TABLE 49 ENGLISH LAI'EGUAGE TRAINERS AND OTHER EEC}: CANS, DISTRIBUTION OE SCALE TYPES DERIVED TRON RESPONSES To ROLE-GOIIFLICP SITUATIONS INVOLVING SEE-OBSETTATION VERSUS GOLLECTIVITY-ORI BETTAI‘I ON TONARD THE FALZELY (SCALEIII)..................... 178 ENGLISH LANGUAGE TRAINEE-S AND ANmIO-ALTERI CANS, DISTRIBUTION OF SCALE TYPES DERIVED FROIII RESPONSES TO ROLE- CONELICT SITUATI ON S INVOLVING SEF-ORI E'T'TATI ON VERSUS COLLECTIVITY-ORIBETATION TO‘a'iARD THE FAMILY (SCALEIII)..................... 178 ENGLISH LANGUAGE TRAINERS BY CLASS IN SPECIAL ENGLISH COURSE, DISTRIBUTION OF SCALE TYPES DELIVED TRON RESPONSES TO ROLE-COI-JELICT SITUATIONS INVOLVING AN ECONORUC ROLE VERSUS A FAMILIAL ROLE (SCALE I) . . . . 181 ENGLISH LANGUAGE. TRAINERS BY CLASS IN SPECIAL ENGLISH COURSE, DISTRIBUTION OF SCALE TYPES DERIVED FRDN RESP NSES TO ROLE-CONFLICT SITUATIONS INVOLVING SELF- ORIENTATION VERSUS COLLECTIVITY-ORIEITATION TONARD THEFANILY(SCALEIII)............... 181 HIGH STATUS NETLICANS AND ALL NERO-AYES ONES, DISTRIBUTI ON OF SCALE TYPES DERIVED TRON RESPONSES TO ROLE- CONELICT SITUATIONS INVOLVING SELF-OBI ENTATI ON VERSUS COLLECTIVITY-ORIETTATION TONARD THE FAI‘JLY (SCALEIII)..................... 195 HIGH STATUS NEALICANS AND ALL ANCLO-AEDBICANS, DISTRIBUTION OF SCALE TYPES DERIVED TRON RESPONSES TO ROLE—CONFLICT SITUATIONS INVOLVING SRF-ORIENTATI ON VERSUS COLLECTIVITY-ORI EHTATION TONALD THE PEER CIROUP(SCALEIV).................. 195 HIGH STATUS AEEICECANS AND ALL ANGLO-AI‘JE‘BI CARS, DISTRIBUTION OF SCALE TYPES DERIVED IRON RESPONSES TO ROLE. CONFLICT SITUATIONS INVOLVING AN ECONONIC ROLE VERSUS A FATELIAL ROLE (SCALE 1) . . . . . . . . . . 196 HIGH STATUS NEILICANS AND AIL NIGLO-ANERICANS, DISTRIBUTION OF SCALE TYPES DERIVED IRON RESPONSES TO ROLE-CONFLICT SITUATIONS INVOLVING AN INSTRULEITAL ROLE VERSUSAFRIEVDSRIP ROLE (SCALE II) . . . . . . . . . 196 LON STATUS EZEHCANS AND HIGH STATUS BIRD—AA-IRICANS, DISTRIBUTION OF SCALE TYPES DERIVE) PROD RESPONSES TO ROLE-CONFLICT SITUATIC‘TS EVOLVING NI ECOI‘EOALIC ROLE VERSUS A EAAELIAL ROLE (SCALE I) . . . . . . . . . . 198 In" '9 . (M E 52. '1‘"! ‘- .‘o.’ ‘ "'" In. 0-. ... P". ?""1P 3. TABLE 50. TABLE 51. TABLE 52 . TABLE 53 . TABLE 5h. TABLE 55. TABLE 5 6 . TABLE 57 . TABLE 58. IDW STATUS MEXICANS AND 10W STATUS MOLD-AMERICANS, DISTRIBUTION OF SCALE TYPES DERIVED FRCM RESPONSES TO ROLE-CONFLICT SITUATIONS 'INVCLVING AN ECONCMAIC ROLE VERSUS A FAMILIAL ROLE (SCAIE I) .................... 198 LOW STATUS MEXICANS AND ALL ANGID-AI’IERICANS, DISTRIBUTION OF SCALE TYPES DERIVED FROM RESPONSES TO EDIE-CONFLICT SITUATIONS INVOLVING AN DJSTRUMFNTAL ROLE VERSUSAFRIENDSHIP ROLE (SCALE II) ................o 199 HIGH STATUS MEXICANS, LCM STATUS MEXICANS, AND ALL ”CID-AMERICANS, DISTRIBUTION OF SCALE TYPES DERIVED FROM RESPONSES TO ROLE-CONFLICT SITUATIONS INVOLVING AN EONCMIC ROLE VERSUS A FAMILIAL ROLE (SCALE I) u. 199 HIGH STATUS MEXICANS, LOW STATUS MEXICANS, AND ALL ANGIO-AMERICANS, DISTRIHJTION OF SCALE TYPES DERIVED FROM RESPONSES TO ROLE-CONFLICT SITUATIONS INVOLVING AN INSTRUMENTAL RCLE VERSUS A FRIENDSHIP ROLE (SCALE II) 201 ALL UNITED STATES STUDENTS BY HIGH AND IDW STATUS, DISTRIBUTION OF SCALE TYPES DERIVED FROM RESPONSES TO ROLE-CONFLICT SITUATIONS INVOLVING AN ECONOMIC ROLE VERSUSAFAMILIAL ROLE (SCALE I) ............... 3'. ALL UNITED STATES STUDENTS BY HIGH AND Low STATUS, DISTRIBUTION OF SCALE TYPES DERIVED FROM RESPONSES To ROLE-CONFLICT SJTUATIONS INVOLVING AN INSTRUMENT ROLE VERSUSAFRIENDSHIP ROLE (SCALE II) '1 ALL UNITED STATES STUDENTS BY HIGH AND LOW STATUS , DISTRIBUTION OF SCALE TYPES DERIVED FROM RESPONSES TO ROLE-CONFLICT SITUAT ION S INVOLVING SELF-CHIENTATICN VERSUS COLIECTIVITY-ORIENTATION TCMARD THE FAMILY (SCALE III) ooooooooooocoo.oooooooo-oooooooooooooooo ii ALL UNITED STATES STUDENTS BY HIGH AND LGV STATUS , DISTRIBUTION (F SCAIE TYPES DERIVED FROM RESPONSES TO ROLE-CDNFLICT SITUATI ONS INVOLVING SEIF-(RIENTATION VERSUS COLLECT IVITY-CR IENTATION TWARD THE PEER (‘me (SCH-IE IV) .0O0.000....OOOOOOOOOOOOOOCOOOOOOOC 11 ALL MEXICAN STUDENTS BY HIGH AND 10'! STATUS, DISTRIBUTION OF SCALE TYPES DERIVED FRCM RESPONSES TO ROLE-CONFLICT SITUATIQVS INVOLVING AN EEONOMIC ROLE VERSUSAFAMILIAL ROLE (SCALE I) .............. iii 0-.- ,1 0'. 0.. O O O (Q Off :3 t -‘I! t «v " A . fl‘ ‘ v4. 9.... :- ALI' Sé. n...‘ ,' 4‘ ' A. '1. an .- du— :11 (I. TABLE 59. TABLE 60. TABLE 61. TABLE 62. TABLE 63 . TABLE 6h. TABLE 65 . ALL MEXICAN STUDENTS BY HIGH AND LOW STATUS, DISTRIBUTION OF SCALE TYPES DERIVED FROM RESPONSES TO ROLE-CONFLICT SITUATICNS INVOLVING AN INSTRUMENTAL ROLE VERSUS A FRIENDSHIP ROLE (SCALE II) ALL MEXICAN STUDENTS BY HIGH AND LCM STATUS, DISTRIBUTION CF SCALE TYPES DERIVED FROM RESPONSES TO ROLE-CONFLICT SITUATIOI'S INVOLVING SEIF-ORIEJTATION VERSUS COLLECTIVITY- ORIENTATICN TCMARD TI-E FAMILY (SCALE III) ............. ALL MEXICAN STUDENTS BY HIGH AND LON STATUS, DISTRIBUTION OF SCALE TYPES DERIVED FROM RESPONSES TO ROE-(DNFLICT SITUATIONS INVOLVING SEIP-CRIINTATION VERSUS COLLECTIVITY- ORIENTATION TONARD THE PEER GROUP (SCALE 1V) DISTRIBUTION OF SCALE TYPES DERIVED FROM RESPONSES TO ROLE-CONFLICT SITUATIONS DWOLVIN G AN ECONOMIC ROLE VERSUSAFMELIAL ROLE (SCAIIE I) OOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOCOCCO DISTRIHJTION CF SCALE TYPES DERIVED FROM RESPONSES TO ROLE-CONFLICT SITUATIONS INVOLVING AN INSTRUMENTAL ROLE VERSUS A FRIENDSHIP ROLE (SCALE II) ......... .......... DISTRIBUTION OF SCALE TYPES DERIVED FROM RESPONSES TO ROLE-CONFLICT SITUATI ONS INVOLVING SELF-ORIENTATION VERSUS COLLECTIVITY-ORIENTATION TOWARD TILE FAMILY (SCALE III) cooooooooooooooooooococoooooooooooooooooooo DISTRIBUTION OF SCALE TYPES DERIVED FROM RESPONSES TO ROLE—CONFLICT SITUATIONS INVOLVING SELF-ORIENTATION VERSUS COLLECTIVITY-ORIENTATION TOWARD THE PEER GROUP (SCALE IV) ...........................................o iii iv iv .0... .0... O... O. pu-o’a '- t I \ ”I" 0-— I l APPENDIX A. SUPPLEI'ENTARY STATISTICAL TABLES APPENDIX B. THE QUESTIONNAIRE BIBLIOGRAPHY CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION TO THE STUDY Research on the Border Project Prior to a discussion of the theory and methods it would be well to describe the general objectives of the research project of which the present study constitutes but one segment. Formally, the general project bears the title of Processes 31; Technological and m 9% in the Luigi—Cultural Settings _o_{ the _B_o_rg§£ £e_a_s_ g; the 92.3.1322 States, but we shall refer to it simply'as thelgggggg Project. The funds for carrying on the progran.of research of the Border Project were awarded.in the form of a grant frm the Carnegie Corporation to the Area Research Center of the Department,of Sociology and Anthropology, Michigan State College, in 195k. The progral.of research was to be projected over a period of five years, in areas where cross-cultural contact occurs as a persistent phen- omenon, and.where the liklihood of cultural diffusion from.one society to the other’light be expected to bear importantly upon modes of social org- anisaticn. \ Fro: its inception, the Border Project contemplated carrying out a comprehensive progran.of research in the MexicanpUnited States border area. This area meets the stipulations nentioned.above, in that it constitutes an arena in which two societies are juxtaposed, where persistent interac- tion is carried on between them in numerous spheres, and where each society is faced with similar problems of adaptation to a common environment. Such a setting contains, potentially, the seeds of diffusion. :18: ues ~r- $22.3» ' .338" [2&3 12:2: L 3.5:! the hex “ea has 1132.323; EFD. ... 5mini: -2- A brief description of some of the major pieces of research which have been undertaken up to the present by different researchers working on the Border Project may provide greater perspective to the present study. A major endeavor has been the analysis of the demographic fea- tures of the Mexican-United States border area. Demographic analysis of this area has proceeded at a steady pace. Tiers of counties on the United States side of the border and tiers of unmicipios on the Mexican side have been examined, and the comparative analysis of pepulation and social statistics is being undertaken at the time of this writing. A major goal of this demographic analysis is the delimitation of some facets of the cultural and social frontiers of the two societies. The study of the interpenetration in a border region of two societies as large and complex as those of Mexico and the United States has heretofore never been attempted and, thus, this aspect of the Border Project constitutes a pioneering research venture. Another major study done has consisted of the analysis of economic institutions and social interaction of business and political leaders in the conunities of Ciudad Juarez, Mexico, and El Paso, in the United States. This stub focused upon the interpenetrations of economic and political institutions. It was concerned especially with the perception along leaders of the Ciudad Juarez community of modes of economic and political organization practiced in the El Paso community. It was fur- ther interested in ascertaining the extent to which business and political leaders in Juarez manifested values and attitudes similar to those of leaders in El Pmel 1 William V. D'Antonio, "National Images of Business and Political Elites in Two Border Camities,“ Ph.D. Dissertation, Michigan State University, 1958. 112th gr. .‘Letcan i J 5":I~‘ ““99 Y3: x3 5* 4.. . A study by Rivera2 was concerned with the general phenomenon of migrant Mexican laborers. Here the interest lay in trying to ascertain the effects upon migrant laborers' values of contact with United States society and culture. In addition, the stucbr was aimed at discovering what might be the general effects upon family and community organization of the migration of family heads to the United States for lengthy periods of time. Another comparative study has been launched in the border area. This study is conerned with the analysis of the organization of cotton grow- ing conunitles which specialized in the production of a particular var- iety of cotton. The peculiarity of production of this variety is that, in order to bring a premium price on the market, it must be grown in areas which are planted exclusively in this variety. The problems in- volved in the organization of a farming community around the production of a single variety of cotton are likely to be mainfold, and the findings of this study will have relevance to the acceptance of this technological change in Mexico as well he in the United States. In emery, we might reiterate the aforementioned points. First, the separate studies done within the framework of the Border Project are directed toward the comparative analysis of phenomena occuring in the United Stateanexican border, area. Secondly, the foci of the studies is upon social, cultural, and technological interpenetrations, diffusion, and adaptations. With these general objectives in view, several significant 2 Julius Rivera, 'Attitudes toward the United States in a Mexican Border (Jo-unity, " Ph. D. Dissertation, Iiichigan State University, 1957. studies already have been carried to various stages of completion and others will be underway shortly. It is, then, within this framework that the present study was conceived and carried out. Perspectives of the Study This study is a canparative analysis of United States and Mexican high school students. The focus of the study is upon selected dimensions of students' expectations with respect to a limited number of status-roles. High school students' role-expectations were selected as an object of study in the hope of expanding substantive and theoretical knowledge concerning similarities and differences between the socio-cultural systems of the United States and Mexico. It was believed that, in part, the systematic study of certain dimensions of role-expectations might lead to a more complete understanding of what has been referred to as the moral integra- tion of society. The expectations which people manifest with respect to the range of legitimate demands which specified others may place upon them constitutes an important dimension of social integration. When the question of the rage of legitimate demands which may be placed upon a person is viewed in tens of the relevance of these demands to the integration of dif- ferent groups of which the individual is simultaneously a member we are ex- amining a problem of moral integration. Because the present study was conceived of within the context of a broad general program of research on social and technological change in the United States-Mexican border area, the central focus is the compare- tive analysis of a dimension of socio-cultural integration. It is abun- dantly evident that the United States is a society well along the way to approaching the polar extreme of an ideal-type urban society. The case of Mexico, on the other hand, is not so unambiguous, nor is there such a wealth of social science materials bearing upon the struc- ture of contemporary Mexican society.3 Mexico as a national society is considerably more variegated than the United States in numerous important respects and by no means as far along the way to approaching an ideal-type urban society as the United States. Nevertheless, there are features of Mexican society which ex- hibit basic similarities to social structure in the United States. It seems most accurate to say that Mexican society, or at least important segments of it, is in varying states of socio-cultural transition. or prime importance is the expansion of industrialization and urbanization in that countny. LPOpulation.movements are involved.in both of these major aspects of changing Mexican society. The establishment of a high degree of’stability with respect to the political order of the state has been accomplished during the past twenty years, and this has been accompanied hy the expansion of the population base of those partici- pating in the national political processes. It is also important to note that the social structure of rural life and agriculture has exhib- ited significant changes since the Revolution. Mechanization of tasks accompanied.hy the rationalization of practices associated with the var- ious facets of agricultural production have reached high levels of crys- 3 It should be observed that there is definitely a dearth of explicitly sociological studies with respect to systematic and directly compara- tive data.pertaining to specified dimensions of both societies. aI-‘KT' ' so... ‘3: .H“ 3' q:. 1;: r1.“ L '. ::e‘ flfl“ H ’ 'l ‘., L’vi“ “ ‘| .e e H A ‘ ‘L :4: t I; Lack." 12:33; :10: 42C ESL 3' -6- tallisation among some sectors of the producers while others show but slight modifications of traditional ways and methods. The expansion of legal-political organs of the state has accompanied the changes in other sectors of the society, and it is especially important to note that a typically bureaucratic form of social structure is exhibited in the organisation of governmental agencies. In the historical develOpment of Mexico, and other Latin American countries, family and locality groupings have loomed large as empirical foci of moral. integration of the social structure. The demands which fauly and kin might place upon the individual and, vice versa, those which the individual might place upon his family and kin, qapear to have constituted the central core and the widest range of demands which the individual recognised as legitimate social obligations. on the other hand, the world of work, of occupations, seems traditionally to have been segregated frat attachment to moral values.h Work, in a sense, consti- tuhd a necessary evil. But under conditions of an unexpanding and relatively precarious economy characterized by relatively small scale instrumental complexes apendent upon a low degree of interdependence and limited differentia- tion and specification of positions in the occupations, problems of moral integration of the social structure revolved largely around family, kin- ship, and locality groups (except, of course, with reapect to the problem 1‘ See, for example, T. Lynn Smith, ”Values Held by PeOple in Latin Amer- ica Which Affect Technical Cooperation", Rural Sociol , Vol. 15, No. l. I ' . o. ‘0.’ ' i2:‘.vweoh . :rintce 0c: in. meg: -‘O ' : I'LL-96:03 0; :1" ex" ’ U “3". e. . ‘1 ‘ p .' .3, rut. r "*‘-' '~‘~~' '-v at. -. 3 "I "" ea- ‘0... C 'Hevu -7- of territorial integration of political power.5) 0n the other hand, given an expanding econonw, the development of large-scale instrumental com- plexes with higher degrees of differentiation and specification of posi- tions in the occupations, and increasing pOpulation movement, problems of moral integration of the social structure arise with respect to the organization of relations in the instrumental complex and the organiza- tion of extra-familial relations in the expressive complexes. It is, then the general area of moral integration of society under which this thesis is subsumed. The particular facet of the prob- lem upon which this study focuses and the theoretical frame of reference, methods, and techniques of investigation will be disucssed shortly. More- over, a more detailed examination of the data bearing upon the structure of Mexican society will be presented. The examination of the moral in- tegration of society is, of course, not a recently developed interest of social science but stems from some of the pioneering theory and re- search in matters of society. The incisive work of Durkheim, in his The Division g_f_ Labor in Society,6 and Suicide,7 is focused upon the problem ~ The problem of territorial integration of political power has been a source of persistent conflict and disruption for many, if not all, Latin American states. Mexico has experienced grave problems with respect to territorial integration, and only in recent years has the territorial integration of political power become relatively well stabilized. Emile Durkheim, Th__e_ Division of _L2.___bor _ipS oci.e_tz Translated from the French by George Simpson.— Glencoe, -Ill.: The Free Press, 191:9. Mile Durkheim, Suicide, _A_ Stu Ln Sociology. Translated by J. A. Spaulding and George Simpson. lencoe, Ill. : Thre Free Press, 1951. 1 530 EC obi- é? )- :nn 513 3 a . 3. 1. OCT...' 0": LJ- .- 3-h-b“ e 32:3: I g 3 EEFG I‘ll“: "sne- . n 5-.- ed. a: . IIL! IAN. m . . . o aw ..W M... am 4.. A. {have 5‘ M a My“. h ”I“ of moral integration of society. The concept of M focuses directly upon dimensions of the state or degree of moral integration of social groups and social aggregates. The work of Elton Mayo8 and his associates may be viewed in large part as concerned with moral integration. Roeth- lisberger and Dickson,9 in their pioneering study of the social organiza- tion of a modern factory, were also concerned with this problem. Lewinlo and many of those studying under him have been interested in the problem as it pertains to wall groups in particular. Angelln directly and ex- plicitly focused upon moral integration in his survey of American cities. lloorel2 was also concerned with the macroscopic aspects of the problem in his survey of industrialization and labor in undeveloped areas of the world and in his study of a Mexican town. It must, of course, be emphas- ized here that the approach to this topic and the methods, techniques, and findings are considered as exploratory and not definitive. 8 Elton Mayo, The Hmmn Problems of an Industrial Civilization. Boston: 9 F. J. Roethlisberger and W. J. Dickson, Management 229. _t_h_e_ Worker. Boston: Harvard Univ. Press, 19314. 1° Kurt Lewin, Resolving Social Convlicts. New York: Prentice-Hall, 191.8. Also Leon Festinger, Stanley Schacter and Kurt Black, Social Pressuresin Informal Groups. New York: Harper and Brothers, 1950. 11 Robert C. Angell, 'I‘he Moral Integration of American Cities." American Journal 9_f. Sociolog, 1951. 12 Wilbert E. Moore, Industrializauon and Labor. Ithaca, New York: Doubleday Doran, 19 l. - e u ‘- I .no- U.- .5. v. I' The Problem As indicated, the main focus of this study is upon a limited aspect of the moral integration of society. Moral integration is conceived here as pertaining to the analysis of three different yet interrelated aspects of the organization of human affairs. 0n the one hand, it is conceived as pertaining to the substantive forms of the maintenance of stability and cor-ensue in the normative orientations of the members of collectivities. In this respect the concept refers to the orientations of members of a collectivity to a system or systems of normative or value standards which serve to integrate the group's affairs. Second, moral integration also may be thought of as referring to the existential order of human social life. In this respect, the concept refers to the orientations of members of a collectivity to a system or systems of exis- tential standards which serve to integrate the social interaction of the members. A third and derivative sapect of the problem of moral inte- gration is that of the relationship between the existential standards held by the members of a collectivity and the normative or value stan- dards which the group maintains. his study will touch upon each of these aspects of the problem of moral. integration. However, the principal concern will be with moral. integration as it obtains in the existential order. As suggested above, the expectations which persons exhibit with respect to the range of legit- imate demands which specified others may place upon them constitutes a dimension of moral integration at the level of the existential order. Principally, then, the study will be concerned with the extent to which 2333! file‘eri 32:5 .‘EfléCt ‘..-.OV'? . $55 as it; .... The grader-l 0‘: “C 3 :7“ .- my .31- E ‘hp w.;"‘.‘ 1 “- a!” «‘I. . “st in. \.:_‘:hs; .‘ U u : x a 5‘ Q ‘n" ”’33.“. . ‘ ‘ ~E:3 4 t_. ‘c: " .‘t‘a ‘1. ‘- -10... persons' role-expectations with respect to a series of mothetical sit- uations reflect a proclivity to recognize the demands of given collectiv- ities as legitimate and superordinate to other demands. The problem will be examined chiefly in the context of conflicting demands placed upon the individual by the status-roles he occupies in the M m, 33;; gm, and friendship p_e_e_r_ m. The methodology of the stuck will be discussed in detail below, but a word should be said here with respect to the reasons for having chosen to focus upon these classes of groups. The range of legitimate demands which the family may place upon the individual member has significance for the integration of all known societies. In some societies family and kinship units con- stitute the principal social structures, and most important social activ- ities of the individual are determined by the statuses he occupies in it. On the other hand, in highly differentiated societies family and kinship w count for somewhat less. That is, the individual's statuses in his family and kin group may not determine so stringently his activities in other social structures within the larger society. since in the case of Mexicans aid United States high school students, we are dealing with persons involved in societies manifesting high degrees of structural differentiation, we can be confided; that the family is by no means the sole collectivity which may place legitimate demands upon the individual. Nevertheless, there is sufficient reason to believe that the family con- stitutes a more important focus of integration in Mexico than in the United States (if, for the sake of discussion, we may speak in such broad tens). The range of demands which family and kin may legitimately place :55 hell's . ....0. £3) {"0 . exufid. a. 00--.. A. I ‘0 .t tench c 1'! 333212 3:: b - ‘ P u U‘ bib. 9 I. O '51. Hair, -..- \f.‘ e: 35 so: h". -11.. upon the individual, then constitutes an important and persistent factor to be reckoned with in the moral integration of society for the range of these demands m be expected to have consequences for the functioning of a society. Attention will be directed to hypothetical situations involving work groups as collectivities differentiated from the family and which may place a range of demands upon the individual in his status as collaborator. The question of the extent to which demands placed upon the individual by the work group are recognized as legitimate has far reaching consequences for the maintenance of stability within a highly differentiated social sys- tem, and especially with respect to the economic order of society. In the case of Mexico, and other Latin American countries, this constitutes a real problem with respect to the transition to industrialization, urb- anization, and secularization in general. In fact, it constitutes a broad problem of social order in any society undergoing processes of change in small-scale, traditional, and relatively undifferentiated social structural units. The case of modern Mexico is precisely of this type. The peer group in the United States and Mexico constitutes a struc- ture of social relations differentiated from both family groups and work groups. He shall examine the extent to which a range of demands placed upon the individual by the peer group are viewed as legitimate and binding. The comparative stuck of this aspect of moral integration is of some con- siderable interest owing to certain current ideas in sociology which view the youth peer group as a growing source of authority over the youth's normative orientation in American society.13 1‘3 See Dafld Riesman, £92 Lonely Crowd. New Haven: Yale Univ. Press, 1950. 4------- f ‘ ,‘.‘A .’ '...UM . a 'ac t"" 31.6. I'M . 9"?" i ”F .Jw—h ' ‘- , I 0" :Kuflcu Serial r21 {:0; an A inm'h 4 D O, - : .0 53-05. £2.21 3:5 “.4"-+T"'i um- w' _ mazes ., ‘ ”h J“fia~— .‘J‘b'v:. k. 2L2; 395:3.“- “a. .I. . v' w I a I- I‘. Q... .M K’.’ 3"“ "" \‘c .133‘ z...‘ «e O. 9 .' _ i M ‘. ‘3‘» r... ";.' \ :- ‘ ‘ $_‘{ "kl‘ a. -12- Four propositions implied by the social system schema were focused upon for study. with the perspective of the problem outlined above, these propositions and the substantive hypotheses derived from.them.may now be stated. Proposition I Social role-expectations represent a relatively unmodified trans- position of cultural value standards. Hypothesis I If Preposition I is true, then both the Mexican students and.the United States students will exhibit a.predominance of universal- istic-type role-expectations in their responses to the pattern variables scale items. Proposition II is the extent of differentiation and segregation of social relations increases in a social system, role-expectations will change from a predominance of particularism.to a predominance of universalism. Hypothesis II If Proposition II is true, then the United States student group will exhibit a greater tendency toward universalistic-type role- expectations than will the Mexican student group. Fraposition III is the extent of differentiation and segregation of social relations increases in a social system role-expectations will change from.a predominance of collectivity-orientation toward the family to a predominance of self-orientation. Hypothesis III If Proposition III is true, then the United States student group will exhibit a greater tendency toward self-orientation than will the Mexican student group. Proposition IV As the extent of differentiation and segregation of social relations increases in a social system role-expectations will change from a predominance of self-orientation to a predomi- nance of collectivity-orientation toward the peer group. Hypothesis IV If Proposition IV is true then the United States students will exhibit a greater tendency toward collectivity-orien- tation toward the peer group than will the Mexican students. t2: the 5:5. 2 3118! C029 ' (a .— .e.2.' a": s ' a L". ‘ M." 39"” ‘I:~u \ "ea '."$ b . fin. Inc-l ‘ u, . an. :~ a no. \ u y . “‘M,‘ 521331: ”O Q a Na - .33 . U a,_ f'. :n 1“». “ t -13- It is these propositions, then, which define the core of the study design, and the empirical research undertakings were devised to test the substantive hypotheses derived from them. but before proceeding to the outline of the research design a discussion of certain important features of the social system schema needs to be presented. The Social System Schema The social system is conceived of as one of three ordered systems within the frame of reference of the general theory of action.“ The two other concepts of ordered systems falling within the scope of the general theory of action are those of the personality system and the cultural system. In its most generic form Parsons defines the idea of a fully emerged social system as: ...a plurality of actors interacting with each other in a situation having physical aspects, actors motivated in terms of a tendency to optimization of gratification whose relation to the situation is defined Ed mediated in terms of culturally structured and shared symbols. The social actor, viewed as a person performing social acts, con- stitutes a first order conceptual abstraction of the schema, but this is too gross a term for the analysis of complex vstems of social action. Con- sequently, the concept of M'Elfi is introduced. In the social system Schema, then, status-roles and the organization of different classes of Status-roles into patterned relationships constitute for most purposes For an exposition of this theoretical schema see: Talcott Parsons, The Social Systan. Glencoe, Ill.: The Free Press, 1951. ‘I‘alcott Parsons and hdward Shils, ed. , Toward a General Theory pf Action. Cambridge: Harvard Univ. Press, 193'.- Talcott Parsons, The Structure 2; Social Action. INew York: McGraw-Hill, 1937. Talcott Parsons, Robert Bales, and dward Shils, Working Papers _i_n_ £12 Theog of Action. Glencoe, 111.: The Free Press, l9§3. 15 Parsons, 3.112 Social §ystemJ 22. cit., p. S. I g A q .339. 1"... . f‘ 0p. “0-,. ~: 0.- ..J: “.-:A.‘ - -' ' '0; U- v.- J tn. . II \ a ' w"~ ‘ fl ’ ‘ o t . \ I.‘ V\ \J. ‘v . A. ,-_. 0. “s ,- -11” the basic units of description of social structure. The concept status- role is derived frat the idea of a situation involving the partic1pation of one actor in a patterned network of relationships with other actors. Attention is directed to two aspects of the situation. 0n the one hand the position of the actor in relation to other actors in the system is focused upon. This aspect of the term is the actor's status. On the other hand, the performances of the actor are focused upon -- what the actor does in his relations with other objects of the situation. hhen focus is directed to what the actor does we are concerned with his role. Thus, the position which an actor occupies in a network of social relationships and the acts he performs as an incumbent of that position are termed his status-role. His concept, it will be noted, conforms eSpecially closely to the ideas of status and role which Linton elaborated.16 Integrally related to the concept of status-role is that of role-expec- tations. Role expectation is a construct pertaining to the analysis of action at the social system level. Hole-expectations, like culture, are residual products of interaction; empirically they are given as substantive social concepts and m be communicated as verbal statements. This concept, of role-expectation, is the principal vehicle by which the gap between the ideas of a social system and a cultural system may be bridged. We shall focus upon patterns of role-expectations in this thesis as the central core for the analysis of the problem of moral integration, aid the concept will be discussed in more detail shortly. The concept Egg-orientation is a construct pertaining to the analysis of the organization of action at the personality level. This concept is essentially a psychological term. Its referent is the personality system 16 Ralph Linton, The Study of Man. New York: Appleton-Centmy Co., 1936, Chapter VIII, “Status and—Rae." ' . en“. 1': L2: kid. 0: " 4- 17‘: can: .5 II- “- ... o; in D- i: ec:-~J." . me: can. on Op: ‘0 - ' 00' O . . ' "" 1c .00 -.- "O ha: a.... n‘.. p 5“. '3 at; “ iv In-.. I ..- m ‘h v'v ‘MA-“ "‘ In 'J":IU’ *. 4 ‘ u . . ‘3. 'v 41,- the basic units of description of social structure. The concept status- role is derived fraa the idea of a situation involving the participation of one actor in a patterned network of relationships with other actors. Attention is directed to two aspects of the situation. On the one hand the position of the actor in relation to other actors in the system is focused upon. This aspect of the term is the actor's status. On the other hand, the performances of the actor are focused upon -- what the actor does in his relations with other objects of the situation. then focus is directed to what the actor does we are concerned with his role. Thus, the position which an actor occupies in a network of social relationships and the acts he performs as an incumbent of that position are termed his status-role. His concept, it will be noted, conforms especially closely to the ideas of status and role which Linton e1aborated.l6 Integrally related to the concept of status-role is that of role-expec- tations. Role expectation is a construct pertaining to the analysis of action at the social system level. Role—expectations, like culture, are residual products of interaction; empirically they are given as substantive social concepts and m be communicated as verbal statements. This concept, of role-expectation, is the principal vehicle by which the gap between the ideas of a social system and a cultural system may be bridged. We shall focus upon patterns of role-expectations in this thesis as the central core for the analysis of the problem of moral integration, and the concept will be discussed in more detail shortly. The concept mg-ozientagog is a construct pertaining to the analysis of the organization of action at the personality level. This concept is essentially a psychological term. Its referent is the personality system 16 Ralph Linton, The Study of gin. New York: Appleton-Centuy Co., 1936, Chapter VIII, flStatus and- Role. " g j; 7“ . l‘ s I- new .—--- 331'. LE he etgir l ‘- his;- 1‘ ._. ,. ‘ e ‘3:- \ ‘9. \: :‘g‘ .. 'a" ‘4)? q “‘ -15- of the individual actor. It is basically an element for the analysis of the personality system in that it refers to an actor's "comdtment" to a set of normative standards. Commitment to normative standards, for Parsons, means their internalization by the actor, i.e., incorporation of the normative standards into the personality system as persisting elements in its structure. These persistent elements in the personality structure are identified as rigid- dispositions. Thus, when focusing on an actor's value-orientations certain persistent elements in his personality are imputed. The empirical referents of a value-orientation, it should be observed, are not given directly in reality but are inferred from action and its sym- bolic representations. With respect to empirical. reality, they are inferred frm overt and covert acts, and from oral or written statements. The concept value-orientation is, then, following Northrup, 17 a concept by postulation rather than a concept by intuition. It would appear to be a potentially use- ful theoretical concept with respect to the analysis of the integration of culture theory with personality theory, but it does not bridge the gap between personality 81d social theory. We may now move to an examination of the pattern variables typology. This typology is central to the whole structure of Parsons' schematic out- line of the interrelations of social, personality, and cultural systems. Moreover, it constitutes a proper theoretical-analytic group of concepts in that the latter refer to certain dimensions of the organization of role-expectations, value-standards, and value-orientations. Parsons' typology is intended to be applicable to the analysis of not just a single class of 1'7 F. 8. c. Northrup, The gogg_ ofthe Sciences and the Humanities. New Haven: Yale University Press, -1T, Chapter 3. Q rig-5,85 (e. u _v 1 “C I I? a.“ a... La. . u" "‘ H‘.‘ H “c u- IJrv-\. -| n this the remiss o: \ 'w-i‘ . aa‘gz ‘ f‘NA.‘- . R «E -16... status-roles (0.3., father in the United States or Kobana'u in Yaqui society) but to all classes of status-roles, anywhere. However, it is to be noted, that the typology itself does not define the substantive social referent nor specify the methods of their observation and. recording. These latter are problms of epistemic correlation which the researcher must devise for his enquiry (and which he must defend according to the cannons of scientific procedure.) 18 discuss In Values, Motives, and Systems pf £23951 Parsons and Shils the nature of the pattern variables and their relation to four distinct levels of the organization of action. They indicate that the pattern vari- ables are related to the organization of systems of action at the following levels: (a) behavioral, (b) personality, (c) social, and (d) cultural. In this study we are interested in the pattern variables as they bear upon the analysis of role expectations and value standards. That is, our interest in them is confined to their relevance as a classi- ficatory scheme to units of the social system and cultural system, not their relevance to tte personality system. Furthermore, since this study makes use of only two pairs of pattern variables, we shall not elaborate on the whole typology at this point. The two pairs of pat- tern variables which this study focuses upon are those of universalism- particularism, and self-collectivity orientation. We need to examine, them, what these two pairs of pattern variables signify at the level of (a) the cultural system, and (b) the social system. 18 Parsons and Shils, 220 22:20, ppe 78-8)40 9'7:0".’°s (1 wow "u- I °' 79 c: .- “lyw —_ I . : 3.7.1? 0. n~l. ’ I- .‘z": 30 Lee: -16- status-roles (e.g., father in the United States or Kobana'u in Yaqui society) but to all classes of status-roles, anywhere. However, it is to be noted, that the typolog itself does not define the substantive social referent nor specify the methods of their observation and recording. These latter are problems of epistemic correlation which the researcher must devise for his enquiry (and which he must defend according to the cannons of scientific procedure.) In Values, Motives, and Systems 2; _A_c_:_t_i_o_n Parsons and Shils18 discuss the nature of the pattern variables and their relation to four distinct levels of the organization of action. They indicate that the pattern vari- ables are related to the organization of systems of action at the following levels: (a) behavioral, (b) personality, (c) social, and (d) cultural. In this study we are interested in the pattern variables as they bear upon the analysis of role expectations and value standards. That is, our interest in them is confined to their relevance as a classi- ficatory scheme to units of the social system and cultural system, not their relevance to the personality system. Furthermore, since this study makes use of only two pairs of pattern variables, we shall not elaborate on the whole typology at this point. The two pairs of pat- tern variables which this study focuses upon are those of universalism- Particularism, and self-collectivity orientation. We need to examine, than, what these two pairs of pattern variables signify at the level of (a) the cultural system, and (b) the social system. —‘ 18 Parsons and Shils, _op. 9333., pp. 78-81;. ‘4... ’ col-t. ‘ .. I I a In. - a 32:3: :r. u- {as .‘r. p v I g ‘0' p. ’ ‘u-‘~-\. l' 0.... e . W "I- Aa- 4‘"‘“ .‘HVL. $311330 c -17- Let us first consider universalism and particularism as they per- tain to the cultural system. Parsons and Shils speak of universaligg at the cultural level as pertaining to "the normative pattern which obliges an actor in a.given situation to be oriented toward objects in the light of general standards rather than in light of the objects' possession of properties ... which have a particular relation to the actor's own properties..."19 Concerning particularism they identify this variable at the cultural level as pertaining to "...the normative pattern which obliges an actor in.a given type of situation to give priority to criteria of the object's particular relations to the actor's own properties ... over generalized attributes, capacities, or perfor- lance standards."20 The pattern.variable of universalism at the social system level is conceived of as a dimension of role expectations. Thus, Parsons and Shils define it as: "The role-eacpectation that, in qualifications for memberships and decisions for differential treatment, priority will be given to standards defined in completely generalized terms, independent of the particular relationship of the actor's own statuses ... to those of the object."21 Iikewise, of course, the pattern variable of particularism at the Bocial system level is conceived of as a dimension of role expectations. k 19 20 21 Ibid., 'p. 82. £29.- .922- .Laeo 9.1.12- ... ..- o 3‘ a K ‘ a .5 5 -.. ... “It :“v-s;~ ‘ Rs. 9 -17.. Let us first consider universalism and particularism as they per- tabs to the cultural system. Parsons and Shils speak of universalism at the cultural level as pertaining to ”the normative pattern which obliges an actor in.a.given situation to be oriented toward objects in the light of general standards rather than in light of the objects' possession of pr0perties ... which have a particular relation to the actor's own properties..."l9 Concerning particularism they identify this variable at the cultural level as pertaining to "...the normative pattern which obliges an actor in.a given type of situation to give priority to criteria of the object's particular relations to the actor's own properties ... over generalized attributes, capacities, or perfor- mance standards.'20 The pattern variable of universalism at the social system level is conceived of as a.dimension of role expectations. Thus, Parsons and Shils define it as: ”The role-expectation that, in qualifications for memberships and.decisions for differential treatment, priority will be given to standards defined in completely generalized terms, independent of the particular relationship of the actor's own statuses ... to those of the object.'21 Likewise, of course, the pattern variable of particularism.at the social system.level is conceived of as a dimension of role expectations. 19 1mm, p. 82. 2° 92.2- 2.12. 21 £92. 2.1.2- ‘fl ..'..q poOa-At- 4' L3: :IJ5V 6 - m-efl‘ag s .. ..‘o-v ... 2:22: of 39'. _ N" . ... anal se “an: A; h"'e.iys a r he 5.. é.“ .. ' V c ‘ ‘g..l\ ad. ’ .‘ O P ~93 u.- 3:64. 2 \" -18.. Parflcularism is defined as follows: "the role elqaectation that, in qualifications for membership and decisions for differential treatment, priority will be given to standards which assert the primacy of the values attached to objects by their particular relations to the actor's properties ... as over against their general universally applicable class properties."22 The second pattern variable pair with which this study is concerned is that of Elf-orientation and collectivity-orientation. At the cul- tural leiel self-orientation refers to: "the normative pattern which prescribes a range of permission for an actor, in a given type of sit- uation, to take advantage of a given Opportunity for pursuing a private interest, regardless of the content of the interest or its direct bear- ing on the interest of other actors."23 On the other hand, collectivity- orientation at the cultural level refers to: "a normative pattern which prescribes the area within which an actor, in a given type of situation, is obliged to tales directly into account a given selection of values which he shares with the other members of the collectivity in question. It defines his responsibility to this collectivity. ”21‘ The social system aspect of this pattern variable pair refers to role-expectations. At the social system level, then, self-orientation is defined as: "the role-expectation by the relevant actors that it is 22 £02.99.- 23 Ibid., p. 81. 2" .122. 2.4.2- Q E... :2? 1‘» ... o. ‘ . Q, “h" '-- a.-bu -0“ . . ...i- .- 9“ ‘5‘- U.‘ U..- ... O M. 0 .n :4 -U a ‘ \ ..- as Q -19- Enable for the incumbent of the role in question to give priority in the given situation to his own private interests, whatever their mot- ivational content or quality, independently of their hearing on the inter- ests or values of a given collectivity of which he is a member."25 Collec- tivity-orientation at the social system level refers to: "the role expec- tation by the relevant actors that the actor is obliged, as an incumbent of the role in question, to take directly into account the values and.in- terests of the collectivity of which, in this role, he is a.member. When there is a potential conflict with his private interests, he is expected in the particular choice to give priority to the collective interest. This also applies to his action in representative roles on behalf of the collectivity."26 The pattern variables typology, of course, consists of five pairs of categories. while this study makes use of duty two pairs, it might be well to indicate briefly the three other categories of the typology. With reapect to the gratification-discipline dimension of social action Parsons identifies a pattern variable pair which he refers to as Affectivity versus Affective Neutrality. With reapect to the choice between "modalities" of the social object he identifies the pattern variable of Achievement versus Ascription. Finally, he identifies the pattern variable pair of Specificity versus Diffuseness as pertaining to the actor's scope of in- terestlin an object. 25 .122. as. 26 ice. as. " e ‘1 i -‘l [as Q J 0x" " P” ‘- Nb. h In. 14,, 9: a ......Juaa- .1 .1 no a» .. _:.C: a la: ' e .9 h ‘ . 1. 21 321E “2.." .- , .- e a t.‘ .- "a .9 ' '1 5:" w“- 0 rs...“‘ ; :fi'0- e: ,, a u- “'2. gen, . h‘k ..ufi“ ’- N'. a ~20- Methodological Critique In any attempt to construct a schematic outline of the structure of social, cultural, and personality systems and the nexus of their in- terrelationships the problem of the maintenance of stability in each of the systems must be dealt with. This problem of the conceptualization of the maintenance of stability in each of the systems must be resolved in such a way as to allow a satisfactory conceptualization of the integ- ration of all three systems in combination. Parsons' problem is, then, one of the conceptualization of order in the structure and interrela- tions of society, culture and personality. It is the great virtue of Parsons' and Parsons' and Shils' work that they do, in fact, achieve a very sophisticated first approximation of the resolution of this prob- lem, tentative though it may be. Our discussion here will deal with certain important methodological steps which Parsons and Shils have taken in their attempt to articulate in their schema the resolution of this problem of order. Having already outlined the major features of Parsons' and Shils' formal distinctions as to the referents of the pattern variables of universalism-particularism and self-collectivity orientation at the cultural and social system levels we need to submit certain criticisms Of their position before moving on. If one scrutinizes their state- ments about the pattern variables at these two levels it becomes evident that there is involved a degree of over-simplification, if not equiv- O"cation, in their thinking. u m -q‘ v. - . ..e.‘ 3"" 'u _,eO-'I , .P ,’v'~n-' ‘ ‘ ulna“ A" F .s-nwleiba h I I ...-“roan“ ”a ’ I 1“,. '5 ‘ no \4 ' 3.3.. ....S J .‘~§' ltr'v:‘ ‘ ‘: n“. .‘\ h. u I‘ I I.” . L' ' a, . I. a \.‘ ’ Q I \i 3‘ -, . a '“r \ I“ -21- They posit an unmodified transposition of value standards ("norm- ative patterns") into role-expectations. In their exposition of the distinctions between the meaning of the pattern variables at the cul- tural and social system levels they focus upon the Special or limiting case of perfect integration of role-expectations with value-standards. Thus, in such a case, the actor (ego) expects alter to react to him in conformance with the value standard which prescribes haw alter ought to react. This is obviously a special case, and Parsons and Shils' con- centration upon it as the basis for elaboration of the interrelationship of the pattern variables at the cultural and social system levels is mis- leading. Ego's and alter's knowledge of a value standard prescribing what each M to do when performing their respective roles does not necessarily correspond with their expectations of each others' actions. Having indicated that Parsons and Shils tend too closely to equate role-expectations with value-standards we need to examine the basis of their error. In the most generic sense this arises from their unsuc- cessful grappling with the distinction between normative and existential prepositions. The distinction between normative and endstential propos- itions and, hence, the reason for distinguishing between value standards and role-expectations, may be put very briefly as follows. Normative Propositions are distinguishable from existential prepositions on the besis of the former being conceptions of what 93593 to be, while the letter are conceptions of what is or what is possibl . Normative prop- ositions are conceptions of the desirable or the idea}, while existential Propositions are conceptions of what is 33.132 in reality or what 1.3 be: lieved to be given. Thus, in the terms of the social system schema, value- '- a 3:32;} A.— p- L.” ‘1‘ m ..e «a n.“ .M“ .Q. n... t mu. Y. I“ .. \J ‘J .1 x, F '! U,- 78 "Yb: . . .‘n-n- '- 5“ $1233. . ‘ . . ‘H. Y -a~ won. 711 . 3:62;; t at“ '1— ~- '4. r- ..- -22... standards are of the order of nonnative propositions, and role-expec- tations are of the order of existential propositions. Although the distinction between these two orders of propositions is explicitly recognized -- it is discussed at some length by Kluckhohn in his chapter on Values and Value-orientations27 -- the logically pos- sible and empirically evichnt combinations of value-standards with role- expectations have not been spelled out. They have elaborated the case of perfect integration of a value-standard with a pattern of role-expecta- tions, but. they have not considered the other possible combination, that is, they have not considered the case wherein a pattern of role-expecta- tions contradicts or conflicts with a value-standard. In terms of the pattern variables typology this would be exemplified in the case wherein the value-standard was universalistic and the pattern of role expectations particularistic, or vice versa. It will be important to keep in mind this other possible combination of a value-standard and a pattern of role-emectations as it is highly relevant to the analysis of part of the aspirical data gathered in the present study. We shall have occasion to elaborate our ideas on this subject in a subsequent portion of this chapter in a discussion of Parsons' views on the organization of Latin herican societies. In order to understand wtw attention has not been given to the case wherein a pattern of role-expectations diverge from or contradict a 27 Clyde nuckhohn, "Values and Value-Orientations in the Theory of Action: An Emloration in Definition and Classification, " in Parsons and Shils, 22. 2333., pp. 388-103. 1 ' I “lg—__e_.d 1! 12336 13 1 . . . . Qe ’n.'§.g .. ‘ o. ’A..-y. ’ l .. Vina: I .‘n- ' F . ...: ‘E ‘ ‘5 1'! :~.. -‘<: C 0-3 v L: U‘. \. ‘ .. n=:c )3 i5 \ .. a ‘- K-a‘. value-standard we must proceed further into the structure of the schema. We must examine certain assumptions of Parsons concerning modes of in- tegration of cultural standards into a social system. The quotation of a short passage of Parsons will serve to establish his position and in- dicate the reference of our criticism of his methodology. Parsons states his methodological position as follows: There is a range of possible modes of orientation in the motiv- ational sense to a value-standard. Perhaps the most important dis- tinction is between the attitude of "expediency" at one pole, where conformity or non-conformity is a function of the instrumental in- terests of the actor, and at the other pole the "introjection" or internalization of the standard so that to act in conformity with it becomes a need-disposition in the actor's own personality struc- ture, relatively independently of any instrumentally significant consequences of that conformity. Th_e latter is to be treated as _t_h_e L-asic type_ ofin irksgration of motivation _w_i_th a normative patte structure of values. Parsons, then explicitly adopts as the basic mode of integration the case of conformance with a cultural standard as that of its intern- alization into the individual's personality as a need-disposition. This constitutes an extremely important methodological step, and it raises sale important questions. We feel it necessary for the purposes of this thesis, and for a general clarification of Parsons' schema, to show that the above indicated methodological position which Parsons has taken is unwarranted and that it has important ramifications in the structure of his schema. The question must be asked: what does Parsons mean by the term _b_a_si__c_ _tm? There are We alternative meanings which appear likely. These 28 Parsons, 22e 9-2”, Pe 37e Q Fat“; as i ' “'fiFAu '12 :t Hide. e v -P 3' .le ’ w e" .ar“‘ ‘ xi: 4” 1‘ a. “' .a .E H ... mags. “ I N\“‘ ‘ I ... L435 "E I"-.; I tat-.‘S: til 0... . . n‘e: ": a- . . § ‘I , \..‘ 4 “'31“; :i:*\. _2[,,- meanings are (a) the idea of an eyirical type, i.e., mean or modal type, and (b) the idea of a limiting 33.33 in a mathematical sense. We shall have to explore each of these alternative meanings briefly. (a) Viewed as meaning an empirical type his 'basic type' would have to mean that at the level of concrete reality internalization of value standards as need-dispositions represents either a mean or modal distrib- ution of concrete cases. That is, if we accept the idea of empirical type as the meaning which Parsons has in mind when he speaks of a I'basic type", then we have to assume that he is talking about concrete reality - that he has lmowledge of the incidence of eases in which conformity to value standards is maintained on the one hand via their internalization as need-dispositions and on the other hand via expedient conformity. Does Parsons produce any evidence to support his position? An «examin- ation of what he asserts as justification for adopting the case wherein internalization is the means by which conformance with a value standard is maintained does not, in fact, present any justification whatever for taking this methodological step. Parsons alludes to (empirical) evidence from the field of psychology as justification for taking this position in regard to internalization in the following passage: "... by and large we are on psychological grounds justified in saying ego's or- ientation will on balance tend to be oriented to stimulating the favor- able, gratification-producing reactions and avoiding provocations for the unfavorable, deprivation-producing reactions. "29 But it is clear 29 Parsons, 32. 9353., p. 37. J a 0 5H3 . n In: 2:: 2v. 'I' 3.3 292 Sv- Il‘ e.'. E . . ,. N 3.31, .25- that this does not constitute justification for his methodological pos- ition and nowhere does he produce evidence for’its justification. Viewed as meaning a limiting case in the mathematical sense Parsons' use of the term basic type seems plausible. In mathematics, a limit is the value a function (i.e., a relationship between two or more variables) would have if the independent variable or variables ever reached a par- ticular value. Although the function never attains this value, it is useful to hypothesise this limit for the purpose of discussing the values that this function takes on as it approaches the limit. In this sense, then, his term basic type would mean that maintenance of conformity with a value standard via internalization of the standard by an individ- ual as a need-diSposition constitutes a hypothetical (limiting) case which is never attained in concrete reality, but which.is, nevertheless, useful as a point of reference for examining the extent to which concrete cases of conformance to value standards approach this state. How, although this constitutes a plausible way of dealing with the examination of degrees of conformity to value standards it does not jus- tify Parsons' treatment of this topic. Again we must assert that, be- cause Parsons focuses upon the case of perfect integration, his treat- ment leads to the obscuring of vital insights into the operation of real societies. What he does is to focus almost exclusive attention on internalization, i.e., on the limiting case, for the elaboration of his ideas concerning the way in which conformity with a values standard is maintained. By doing so he fails to consider the case wherein con- fermity with a value-standard is maintained hy an orientation of exped- iency rather than hy its internalization as a need-disposition. T‘. In ‘8“ .QL'J '.."e- FLA £9514: a...” A“: a *l N... a. a.» u‘. start] ' I ...-Is I .9 to...- a; ti! :rea‘g "1" f’ .- " ~ ‘ In ~ I. r ,e ,- ”‘5‘ 31:». \ ...]. d-I V‘ 1:»: Finally, we should note here a further implication of Parsons' and Shils' methodology. This is the tendency toward the use of a psychol- ogical framework for the interpretation of social phenomena. It is most evident in Parsons' subsequent attempts at empirical analysis.30 Certainly mam who have devoted some time to the study of Parsons' work comeuay with the feeling that he introduces a disproportionate mount of essentially psychological terms into the structural frame- work of the social system schema. For the elaboration of his ideas concerning the integration of cul- tural elements into a social system Parsons chooses to focus upon the case wherein actors internalize cultural standards as need-dispositions in their personalities. but from the point of view of the analysis of social structure or of social interaction the question of whether or not the actors internalize cultural standards is not the most generic consideration. The principle consideration in the analysis of social interaction and of social structure needs to be that of determining What are the value standards, the status-roles and patterns of role- OXpectations, and how these two orders (normative and existential) are interrelated in the concrete empirical instance. There is no evident need, aside from formal requirements of the theory, to introduce such concepts as internalization and aged-m- 933p in order to analyse the elements of constraint and discipline y 3 O Talcott Parsons and Robert F. Bales, Family Socialization and Interaction Process. Glencoe, 111.: e ree'PE-ess, 19$. _— A. zit“ l “I!" ARI:O: .- e‘fl Vice. u ev ga§,w .| i \ 3‘ Op. “ It. in: A .' .vree' *1... 5' _. .1, I V e ‘.=’T“ " 9 he. Ii 2;“ I" . t H .‘e ‘ I “s- . “"531 11- .vn -27- which obtain in the individual's orientations to value standards and role-expectations in interaction situations. Moreover, the meaning of these terms in his schema remains highly ambiguous. But given the var- iety of personality theory which Parsons adopts, these concepts do, in fact, constitute necessary types of terms in his schema. As he poses it, these, or like concepts, are necessary in dealing with the problem of the transposition of terms of the cultural and social systems into those of the personality system. It readily may be noted that concepts like internalization and need-disposition do have a peculiar appropriate- ness for the description and analysis of the personality system in terms of certain psychoanalytic theories. The Freudian and neo-Freudian theories of personality are, of course, ones which provide terms which are most compatible with those in Parsons' and Shils' schematic outline of the structural interrelations of cultural, social, and personality systems. 'Ihe heart of the matter with which Parsons attempts to deal is that of solving the problem of the maintenance of stability in a social sys- tem, i.e., a problem of order. A resolution of the problem of order is produced, but it is an overly simplistic one, That is, the maintenance of a level of stability in a system of goal-directed behavior is con- ceived of as being obtained through the internalization of a value stan- dard as a need-disposition in the personalities of the participant actors. The adoption of such a methodological position tends to place a prepon- derant emphasis upon the study of the personality system as a pre-requis- its to m understanding of normative action. ... o I"" .. :pf. bud-W . ..- .p l~..' :8. fl. N-bcu S 3»: ‘~ vb. .. . m n.- ‘. '5 ... 0-.- F‘“ 0‘ V. e "“01" 1:521; an: to a. :22 Hui-Iv“ ~aes-’. \ I. ‘. , £53.13 in: , . ‘ ‘ 3.0.5.. "‘57-: . ""“h¢>‘ i..- . he -28- The implication of the schema with reapect to the relationship betIIeen cultural value standards and social role-eXpectations may be put concisely in the form of a proposition as follows: Proposition I Social role-expectations will represent a relatively unmodified transposition of cultural value standards. This prOposition, along with others, will be tested later by the analysis of a range of pertinent empirical data. In sumnary, then, we have attempted to point out how, in their attempt to articulate in their schema the terms of their resolution of the problem of order, certain methodological steps are taken by Parsons and Shils which tend to obscure important distinctions in the relationships obtaining between role-expectations and value standards. Owing to the adoption of the case of internalization as the primary focus for discussing the integration of value standards into a social system (in the form of role-expectations) they fail to consider the case wherein actors' role-expectations may contradict the value-stan- dard pertaining to the statuses which the actors occupy. This latter case, we contend, is of great significance to an understanding of the operation of empirical social systems. It is exemplified in the sit- uation wherein an actor has a conception as to how a bureaucratic of- ficer ought to act toward him (i.e., in conformance with specified gen- eral rules of office) and yet at the same time has a pattern of role- expectations that the officer will not likely conform to this standard but will likely act toward him in some other definable manner. 73.1282 32 fine sci-en :::~:':e 3:5: L“ .I 3:». 'l:" a. "‘9‘: \. at?! ff 3": A!“ 5 ‘“ "f“ quu -29- If one limits the focus of conceptualization of an ordered system of action to the case wherein value standards are integrated with pat- terns of role-expectations at the social system level through their in- ternalization as need-dispositions in the personality, then the utility of the schema is confined to the analysis of the most simple cases of concrete systems of social action. Types of Role-Expectations and Features of the Social Structure There remains to be treated in this chapter a further and important implication of the social system schema, one which will be assessed later against a range of empirical data. It is not an uncommon reaction a- long those who have attentively read Parsons' work on the social system to come away from it with the impression that the pattern variables, in a sense, "Just are", i.e., the relationship between specifically sociol- ogical features of the social system and the pattern variables is some- what elusive. The kind of relationship obtaining between forms of social structure and types of social role-expectations (as defined in terms of the pattern variables) is not always clearly spelled out. It is partic- ularly difficult to determine the peculiarly sociological significance of the pattern variables owing to Parsons' use of a paradigm which foc- uses upon their applicability to the personality system of actors or to closed systems of interaction between two actors. However, his general outline of the foci of crystallization for social structure constitutes an important point at which he explicitly indicates a conception of cer- tain relationships between types of role-expectations and the extent of \ It." 5”;'¢'::a:.: ..e Us I . fl: fig I; 58" u ‘e-nev-« " Hull: e r :i;;. c cf: ‘-< ‘1 .‘ I vi :re ~"ee: - i " he,“ ..‘gkv‘bh . i Q's . \lz‘fi; I‘. A ' “:~et‘fl.“ q"& s K 15. P is I“: ‘ ee.‘ a 5.? C'--. .r'» g'.‘. e ." - \‘ -30- differentiation and segregation of social relations in a social system. Here, at least, the pattern variables are “tied into" the expressly soc- iological features of the social system schema. Since in this study only two pairs of pattern variables are dealt with, the complete typology will not be considered here. It appears to be implicit in Parsons' whole approach to the outline of the unfblding social interaction situation that particularistic-type standards are first to emerge, and that only after relatively high de- grees of differentiation of social relations occur do universalistic- type standards begin to appear. These ideas, which Parsons discusses principally in terms of a diadic model, are carried into his concep- tualization of large-scale social systems of societal prOportions. Parsons is quite explicit about his conception of the relation- ship between the pattern variables and these features of social struc- ture. He conceives of a functional interrelation between the extent of differentiation and segregation of social relations in a social system and the predominance of certain types of social role-expectations held by participant actors. The implications of the schema with reSpect to the universalismpparticulariem and self-collectivity dimensions of role-expectations may be stated as follows in the form of two proposi- tions. PrOposition II As the extent of differentiation and segregation of social re- lations increases in a social system role-expectations will change from a predominance of particularistic to a predominance of universalistic types. f1 j v-‘m 3‘“ I l a”. 15 at: ‘ :vn‘ #" ....55: I .335- 3 ‘R‘ F.“ .9 I" -31- firOposition III {As the extent of differentiation and segregation of social relations increases in a social system role-eXpectations will change from.a predominance of collectivity—orientation toward the family to a predominance of self-orientation. With respect to Proposition III the intention is not to imply that little or no care and affection is exhibited toward family and kin man, here in.the context of a highly differentiated system of social relations. It is rather that as important social activities, especially those of an instrumental nature, become differentiated from the context of the fam- ily, the demands of these segregated roles will compete with those of the family. In order, then, that the activities in the differentiated social complexes may be carried on conflicts between family interest and self interest must more frequently be decided in favor of self inter- ests. Presumably, were the range of legitimate demands of the family to be too extensive, the demands placed upon the individual in his roles in.other social complexes either (1) would not be acceeded to (with consequent damaging disruption of the activities in these other role complexes) or (2) might be acceeded to at the cost of considerable social conflict and probably accompanying psychological stress of dams aging proportions for the individual. It should be noted that the theory generates no clear implication as to which pole of the self-collectivity dimension of role-expectations might be expected to predominate in actors' orientations in the context of social relationships Other than that of self versus family. Given this, some further specification of the theory might be attained as . I . la. . .s- ..- .-. .a— .. a u A: . u“ .... a4 . .6 .: ”_m .... ...“ u. a A e . 2. ..r T .5? 3. _. ..s . . ... ax H ._. as .r. .u. ... . We a a a ... ~v %. n1— “ la. & ... ... . at. .c ... k. t a: . u r . .. .... ... . a.. a. a“ u a u .v a 2. dx ‘a .- 3!. a... .uu an ..I W I e ‘. \e. mauve m. an“ JUN .a a a S I. . . . .. . .... L L a m... .. ... . ... . . a a." .3 .... t a .. 4 s ‘ 4i .1 e . o . H . . .e. .. y .» . v ‘ e , x . O. 3 _ fi 0 _ . . . z ‘ c 0 ll - k v . . m well as a greater measure of insight into the empirical cases at hand if the self-collectivity dimension were examined in the context of a different social relationship. There arises here, of course, a ques- tion as to what other social relationship might be examined so as to assemble data bearihg most crucially upon the theory. No definitive and fully defensible answer to this question is submitted by this thesis. Any of a considerable range of social relationships might be selected and arguments in defense of their selection might be advanced. How- ever, there is a particularly compelling reason to examine the relation- ship between the individual and his peer group. Parsons' own writings have dealt with this relationship and, of course, so have those of a number of other sociologists. Parsons has spoken of a "youth; culture" in Arnerica, 31 and the youth peer group has been discussed considerably in sociological literature. Elkin 81d Westley32 have attempted to summarize briefly a number of characteristic views bearing upon the sociological features of the per- iod of adolescence. As seen by Elkin and Westley, the chief features attributed to adolescence in American society are that it is a period of ”storm and stress" for the youth, and the relationship of the youth to his peer group stands out as one of the most salient social relation- 31 '1‘. Parsons, "Age and Sex in the Social Structure of the United States, " American Sociological Review, Vol. 7, No. 5, pp. 6014-616. 32 F. Elkin and W. Westley, “The Myth of Adolescent Culture," American SociOIOgical Review, Vol. 20, No. 6, pp. 680-685. \. P. ...": 1"- I.- .f .l::“ . ' um ”‘g a:- o i ‘H "Li - ‘25: 3.’ ' - "h .. l “I 'x‘ 0. ‘es_ ~..' ‘. . a... ‘ 01‘ . .‘ ‘va.“ I a VI. :' “£‘ ‘ Ha ‘ “ | .l .. 'tu‘ _ ""s.‘ ‘ I.l ‘ well as a greater measure of insight into the empirical cases at hand if the self-collectivity dimension were examined in the context of a different social relationship. There arises here, of course, a ques- tion as to what other social relationship might be examined so as to assemble data bearing most crucially upon the theory. No definitive and fully defensible answer to this question is submitted by this thesis. Any of a considerable range of social relationships might be selected and arguments in defense of their selection might be advanced. How- ever, there is a particularly compelling reason to examine the relation- ship between the individual and his peer group. Parsons' own writings have dealt with this relationship and, of course, so have those of a number of other sociologists. Parsons has spoken of a "youth. culture" in America, 31 and the youth peer group has been discussed considerably in sociological literature. Elkin and Westley32 have attempted to summarize briefly a number of characteristic views bearing upon the sociological features of the per- iod of adolescence. As seen by Elkin and Westley, the chief features attributed to adolescence in American society are that it is a period of ”storm and stress" for the youth, and.the relationship of the youth to his peer group stands out as one of the most salient social relation- 31 T. Parsons, "Age and Sex in the Social Structure of the United States," American Sociological Review, Vol. 7, No. 5, PP. bob-616. 2 3 F. Elkin and W. Westley, “The Myth of Adolescent Culture," American Sociological Review, Vol. 20, No. 6, pp. 680-685. , I... I V ’: 'I at g C- s O'- sra: it ~- V . . . .... _'_'.'... c- 4 L- ...-vow- . 4 17.6 15. ‘1. v ...—e- ...: . i We": VIJK ‘- 0 v it's. a: VI- .U~ 45 Id: ‘ D r w "‘3 ‘A unbsbnfl .. v IVA-O q: n. we 53.x I \ J . ~ ‘Q‘ «n “'Nwia‘. 3* . . -' 3;“, ‘. new ‘ t‘:a‘ “hc . . :vev, I... ta;‘ “:- a a I. O ‘l ‘\ 4. . I ”1:?“ . :N \l._ " VIII a,‘~. |/ I 40 -\::~.,_ d “'5. -32- 33 ships in which he is involved. Davis views the period as one of strain on the youth in a complex and changing society, a period fraught with indecision owing largely to the absence of consistent authority patterns. Williamth views this period as imposing stress upon the youth owing in large part to the lack of any clear cut cultural defin- ition of the ”youth role". Moreover, he recognizes that this stress resulting from "indeterndnacy", while a characteristic feature of Am- erican society, is seldom encountered in simpler and more stable soc- ieties. MeadBS discusses social features of the early socialization process in a non-Western society and explicitly contrasts the lack of stress during this period in Samoan society with the considerable amount in American society. Riesman36 has pointed to the association of social personality types with pOpulation factors, and asserts that the p'other directed” type is emerging in certain sectors of the United States and other Western societies as the predominant social personality. Whyte37 has noted the importance attached to the maintenance of "good relations" with one's peer group in certain sectors of American society. 33 Kingsley Davis, "Adolescence and the Social Structure", 2113 Annals 9_f_ the. American Academy 2; Political and Social Sciences, Nov. , 1914b, p. 11. 31” Robin Williams, American Society. New York: Alfred A. Knopf, 1952: p. 71s 35 Margaret Mead, Coming 93 5.8.9. in Samoa. New York: W. W. Morrow Co., 1928. Riesman, pp. gig" passim. 37 W. H. Whyte, ”The Transients," Fortune, May, June, July, August, 1953. I. ’0'. R 1 .- F u Q I M .u a. ‘ n a ..o . a .I., I In . 1. ‘\ I I O! . ‘ s . Q a a ‘ a ‘ c " r \e I a ... . Q W. F. “byte39 has described the complex structure of peer group relations among lower class young men in an urban community and shown the saliency of the peer group as a vehicle of social identification. ho has suggested several features which he considers espec- Parsons ially characteristic of youth in the United States. 1'he period is char- acterized.hy a compulsive independence of and antagonism to adult author- ity, involving recalcitrance to adult standards of responsibility. Also, compulsive conformity within the peer group of age mates, involving a low level of tolerance for deviation from the group's standards, is viewed by Parsons as characteristic of this period. In summary, it is suggested that these various views with reapect to the period of adolescence in the United States and the relationship of’the individual to his peer group imply that the American youth will tend to be predominantly peer group oriented in situations involving a conflict between self interests and peer group interests. The views of these writers also imply a general recognition that the features which characterize the period of adolescence in the united States are by no means universally encountered in other societies but are conceived as related to the high degree of complexity and rapid change occurring in this country. In contrast, relative “simplicity” and "stability" in a society are viewed as accounting for the absence of stress and confusion during the period of adolescence. 39 W} FL Whyte, Street Corner Society. Chicago: Univ. of Chicago Press, 19h}. T. Parsons, "Psychoanalysis and the Social Structure,“ Psychoanalytic watery, V01. 19, PP. 371‘38he ho 5" z"— . w -- °- ..- $ . v . _. R}: Q .‘ .-- \ . ‘ ‘- 'I- ‘::.- ‘e I». I“ \‘N .- 'v Cl . s “ \ ‘ ‘ -3h- W. F. Whyte” has described the complex structure of peer group relations among lower class young men in an urban community and shown the saliency of the peer group as a vehicle of social identification. to Parsons has suggested several features which he considers espec- ially characteristic of youth.in the United States. 1'he period is char- acterized by a compulsive independence of and antagonism to adult author- ity, involving recalcitrance to adult standards of reaponsibility. Also, compulsive conformity within the peer group of age mates, involving a low level of tolerance for deviation from the group's standards, is viewed by Parsons as characteristic of this period. In summary, it is suggested that these various views with reSpect to the period of adolescence in the United States and the relationship of the individual to his peer group imply that the American youth will tend to be predominantly peer group oriented in situations involving a conflict between self interests and peer group interests. The views of these writers also imply a general recognition that the features which characterize the period of adolescence in the United States are by no means universally encountered in other societies but are conceived as related to the high degree of complexity and.rapid change occurring in this country. In contrast, relative "simplicity" and "stability" in a society are viewed as accounting for the absence of stress and confusion during the period of adolescence. 39 ‘W. F. Whyte, Street Corner Society. Chicago: Univ. of Chicago Press, 19h3. ho T. Parsons, "Psychoanalysis and the Social Structure," ngchoanglytic Quarterly, Vol. 19, pp. 371-38h. > i n. l-Q-Ul -\ av ..ovvaal ".e..-‘. th'h‘- s . § '°‘ «v- «- ... ..u...‘ J' 13‘" . u s‘. - 311222.. '~ acnp; . . ‘da-~_ 0.. QA- ¢ .5..E~_ JCS “- If these views are to be congruent with the terms of the social system schema then it is suggested that what is referred to as the rel- ative complexity or simplicity in a society might be comprehended by the notion of different degrees of differentiation and segregation of social relations. Thus, the features cited above as characteristic of adolescence in the United States might be conceived as related to the. relatively high degree of differentiation and segregation of social relations in this society. Cast in this form the views referred to above may be considered congruent with the terms of the social system schema. Thus, it becomes possible to examine the self-peer group rel- ationship by stating a testable proposition formulated in the terms of the scheme. with reapect to the self-peer group relationship, the fol- lowing proposition mw be stated: Proposition IV As the extent of differentiation and segregation of social relations increases in a social system role-expectations will change from a predominance of self-orientation to a predominance of collectivity-orientation toward the peer group. Although Parsons' categories and the form of his statement of the relationship between them is perhaps unique, the idea of these general kinds of relationships may scarcely be considered novel. One of the fil‘zet to focus on this general area was. Toennies with his concepts of fiemeinschaft-likg and gesellschaft-_li_l§_e_ relationships. Redfield has ban concerned with the changes occuring in the way peOple relate t"heiselves to one another in the context of emerging degrees of differ- emSiation and segregation in social structures. Miner, also, in his saga: of: ' 1f. I.» 5“ :~o’ A4: '4 135 v (I. \‘I I.’R+ 2L. u‘c» land .. £::M en‘n:€ 23' conception of a ”folk-urban continuum", might certainly be considered among those concerned with this general type of relationship. In any event, the study will undertake a test of these implications by exam- ining a range of new data from two different socio-cultural systems, each characterized by different degrees of differentiation and segreg- ation of role relationships. ...‘ " 35..-: 3s 3 eagle "‘ par. , .u H ' . _. uv-I . .... -o .- ' U ‘ . an. -d'-‘ ‘Il. D ‘ .... u... a . A 3-. 5752.11 0 0"!- ...x.. conception of a ”folk-urban continuum", might certainly be considered among those concerned with this general type of relationship. In any event, the study will undertake a test of these implications by exam- ining a range of new data from two different socio-cultural systems, each characterized by different degrees of differentiation and segreg- ation of role relationships. 23 m . I ‘ 0 nec- - l I: “’h' '- My ea. -- . \ . 7.1% 1.“. 26“ 2 '11:: 3:15 1 on. W =Aassw: .3.“ CHAPTER II METHODOIDGY This chapter will present the research design for the investigation of the previously discussed implications of the social system theory. Since the major implications with which the stuchr is concerned have to do with the relationships obtaining between types of role-eXpectations, _culmral value standards, and the structural differentiation of the social system, it will be pertinent to examine the substantive nature of these categories as they apply in Mexico. It is with the structure of social relations, cultural value standards, and role-expectations in lkuico that.this study is especially concerned because Mexico constitutes a sufficiently different case from that of the United States to warrant a comparative test of the theoretical implications. The methodological “cps will be outlined below, containing a detailed discussion of the instruments used, a critique of the technical characteristics of four Guttman scales developed by the study, a discussion of the method of Selecting reapondents, and. an outline of the conditions under which the data were gathered. Method The method adopted by this study for the investigation of the pre- viously discussed problems is simple in outline yet complex and fragile in its dependence upon inferences. First, it was necessary to come to ‘ o vv-n‘ II: .Q‘ nth“ J04" pv- flzuu", C..- .” :o'.g...’: It .unv—s v ' II. I r.— ... b... 0“ .- ~ , ‘ Q-‘In . a ‘|. I " ‘2':":‘ b :-_-. “’ (n rs a: I c ' h‘ V“a‘~' ““':.}¢ :. ‘v.. . ‘va .‘a ‘F~ v.“ P‘-‘ \ ‘~ E ‘5 ., .Us.c .; fl' an \.,__‘: o: ‘ ‘.' -"# K h .. ‘. ‘ H .I [9,. -38- Some judgement as to the extent of structural differentiation and seg- regation of social relations exhibited in various Spheres of hexican society. It is important to note the degree to which the structure of instrumental exchange relations exhibits relatively considerable differ- entiation, and the extent to which this appears to be segregated from the structure of family and locality-group relations. 1his part of the study will be presented in Chapter III. A major point in.terms of method should be noted here. This study, in general, accepted Parsons' observations with respect to United States society and culimre. That is, it was assumed that major social relational complexes in the United States exhibit high degrees of differentiation and segregation of social relations. Certainly, it was felt that this adequately characterizes the general form of instrumental exchange rel- stions. With respect to culture, it was accepted that value standards of a universalistic nature generally predominate in the United States, and that this is especially true for the standards pertaining to instrum- ental exchange act‘iVities. 0n the other hand, although it is believed that role-expectations (existential standards) are more frequently de- fined universalistically in the United States than in Mexico, the study hazarded no assumptions in this respect, but considered this tOpic prob- lematical and a prOper subject of empirical investigation. Third, in order to test the implication that role-expectations re- Present a more or .less modified transposition of cultural value stan- dards, a range of role-expectations exhibited by members of each society Q . ’ I' a 2' ::..' 3.5»— . , .- ':"~‘ 3: Y», ...-n- r,..:-a’g CAu .‘t-a-ofl 5" 9“. an: zF u .7“. .61 -» W1 ‘ .‘L- can"; I Q ‘ I A I" s I q w . 3‘ l J.v¢y ...- 'Ql:-;s\~ o 'M-g...“ ' ...: g... .- m:.,~e o- n; .. . . H a....5. ~.. was examined. These role-expectations were examined with reapect to their relevance to the pattern variable pair of universalism and partic- ularism so that a comparison of them with the relevant cultural value stuflbrds could be made. The technique used.in gathering data on this point was that of questionnaires submitted to Mexican and United States lfigh.sdhool students. Two series of short stories were devised which included role-conflict situations. It is the responses to questions concerning these series of stories which constituted our data on the incidence of universalistic-type and particularistic-type role-expec- tations. The procedure will be diecussed in detail below. Fourth, it had to be ascertained whether role-expectations of self- orientatio n or collectivity-orientation predominate in situations in- volving fadly and peer group interests on the one hand, and self in- terests segregated from family and peer group interests, on the other hand. Guttman scales also were used in this connection and.will be dis- cussed below. Fifth, notions pertaining to variables other than those indicated above‘were introduced at relevant points throughout the analyses and their significance for the theory was examined. These pertained to such substantively important differentiating criteria as ethnic status and.social status. Procedures and Techniques The logic of the procedures adopted will be discussed in this secs tion. The pattern variables, following Parsons and Shils, constitute a ':p-O<' . a. -I 3" no. cub-‘ . 'O.‘ " no... it! I“. 3.. ‘v- in, ‘v- . . \ ‘5‘- ‘\ typological system.for the analysis of the moral dimension of social role-expectations, cultural value standards, and value-orientation patterns of the personality. It will be recalled that, at the level of the social system, the pattern variables of universalism and particularism apply to the differentiation of role-expectations with reSpect to "quali- fication for membership and decisions for differential treatment." Role- expectations which give priority for differential treatment of an object on the basis of that object's general class characteristics, independent of the relationship of its prOperties to those of the actor, are defined as universalistic-type role expectation. On the other hand, role-expec- tations which give priority for differential treatment on the basis of the relationship of the object's properties to those of the actor, rather than on the basis of their general class characteristics, are defined as particp ularistic-type role-expectations. With these definitions in mind, four series of short stories were deve10ped, each portraying an actor in a situation in which demands for differential treatment are placed upon him by Specified other actors. Each of these series of stories, then, formed.the basis for deve10pment of four Guttman scales, which, for analytical purposes, constituted the devices for measurement of universalism-particularism, self-orientation and collectivityhorientation dimensions of role-expectations. The devel- apnent of these four scales and their technical features will be discussed below, but first a word must be said with respect to the nature of the stories themselves. The following example of the form of these stories is drawn from the final questionnaire used for the study and, hence, may serve as the basis of this discussion. . 112225 -hl- Example of Short Story Device Ibu are the head of a large office. One of the supervisors leaves the company, and so you have to select a new one. Your brother also works in the office. He is a good worker, but there might be some other employees in.the office who are harder workers than he is. Your brother wants the job. In the above story, the situation is one in which an actor'is faced with demands deriving from two different status-roles which he simultan- eously occupies, i.e., those of brother and office head. The demands of his status-role within the sub-system of instrumental relations call for his subscription to universalistic criteria for choosing a new supervisor. 0n the other hand, the demands of his status-role within the sub-system.of kinship relations call for subscription to particularistic criteria with respect to differential treatment of the brother. The dilemma is one of conflicting value standards at the cultural level, while at the social system level it is one of incompatible role-expectations. Acknowledgement of the legitimacy of the demands of the actor's brother may be taken as constituting a definition of role-expectations in.particularistic terms, while a rejection of the legitimacy of the brother's demands may be taken as constituting a definition of role-ex- pectations in universalistic terms. In order to ascertain the incidence of particularistic versus universalistic definition of role-expectations a set of questions followed the story. Questions Following Short Story Device (a) In view'of the circumstances, what right does your brother have to expect.you to select him.for the Job? He has a definite right to expect me to select him for the job. He has some ri ht to expect me to alect him for the job. He has no right Eb expect me to select him for the Job. ’- CI gl’ ’- I I S?” C‘.’ ...—4 ’I. ‘V : AIS e F uug' ‘0- I.E he '_ a ‘w 4- A‘ . . A ‘N 0 \t ‘A. ‘ I‘v'. ‘\ a. .. \_ a - ‘V‘_“ ...“ ‘ 0‘"; a )n. . .f ‘fi N ‘ of a .. l"§‘.-, _v' Q A'- i‘d“ ...}. 2 (b) In view of your obligations to a brother and your obligations to the other employees and to the company, what do you think you would do? I would select my brother for the job. I would select the hardest worker for the job, even if this meant leaving my brother in his old job. In the example above, then, it may be inferred that a response in- dicating that the brother has a “definite right" to expect to be selected for the job constitutes an acknowledgement of the legitimacy of demands deriving from a particularistic relationship and, hence, the respondent's role-expectations relevant to this situation may be classified as p31;- ticularistic. 0n the other hand, it may be inferred that a reaponse in- dicating that the brother has "no right" to expect to be selected for the Job constitutes a rejection of the legitimacy of particularistic de- mands and, hence, the respondent's role-expectations may be classified as universalistic. In view of the liklihood that some portion of the respondents would not hold altogether clean-cut expectations with respect to the situation portrayed in the story, the category "some right" was included as a pos- sible response selection. But a response indicating that the brother has ”some right” to expect to be selected for the job could not be con- sidered an unambiguous acknowledgement of the legitimacy of demands de- riving from a particularistic relationship and, hence, the problem of classifying the reapondent's role-expectation would be unclear. It was because of the lack of clarity for the problem of classification of role- expectations that an additional series of questions (those in Part b) were asked of the respondent. As I” be seen above, these additional ? .ar- - . c a I'- _ . .n-0’_v' ' -..I"“ . o awn up--." . - . _-.- .— .1. o, - . P . 's 5v a - u e -. ‘ C .. C b. ‘ - |<-.... -» .'v use . o - . . . . _ . . _ ‘b" ..‘ I... ~‘ - .1... _, --....'1. .. §K H ‘- av “§ . "' .0. a ’eu -.- . at. ... TABLEii CLASSIMCATICN OF RESPONSE cancers -13.. BY PATTERH VARIABLES TIPES Response Category Pattern Variable Type Brother has a definite gjcht to expect to be selected for the job. Brother has n9 rgrht to eXpect to be selected for the job. Brother has some right to eXpect to be selected for the job, and respondent would gelect him for the job. Brother has some riyht to expect to be selected for the job, and respondent flogld g9; gelect him for the job. Particularistic Universalistic Particularistic Universalistic .... ' ... '..e' O ..l-' V -:p. eh: I h" . a In" .0. has V- : A ... 0 ‘-I a. ‘. . n .c J, 'I .U M D e V”- "‘:‘v-'-yl A ‘“‘~' 5. a ‘0 e ”‘e x. ‘ N F- ‘. I‘.‘ .‘Pv‘ _i'r":'- questions posed two alternatives of action for the respondent. On the one hand, he could select the brother for the job, and on the other hand, he could refrain from selecting the brother for the job. It was decided, then, that should the respondent indicate that the brother had "some right" to expect to be selected for the job, his response to the subse- quentnpair of alternatives would give a further indication of the "direc- tion" of his choice. In effect, it was decided that the response combin- ation of “some right“ fin Part a) and "select brother for the job“ (in 'Part b) is indicative of a predominance of particularistic elements in the role-expectations relevant to this situation. likewise, it was decided that the re8ponse combination of "some right" (in Part a) and "not select brother for the job" (in Part b) is indicative of a predominance of universalistic elementsin the role-expectations relevant to this sit- nation. The system of classification of reSponses to the above story is summarized in Table l, and illustrates the method of classifying the responses to all of the other stories included in the study. DevelOpment, Translation, Reliability and Pre-testing of Role-expectations Stories The four Guttman scales were constructed using both Mexican and United States student. Consequently this entailed devising stories in English and translating them into Spanish.1 Some forty-five stories 1 Aid in translation of these stories was given by Professor John Sharp, of the Department of Foreign Languages, Texas Western College, as well as by Mrs. Bertha House. Mrs. House was of great assistance in the translation of idiomatic usages, and the study profited greatly by her easy versatility in both languages. {$.a‘. ‘ lewd: : . - I33" ‘> " v . ”2'? 1' “'“ .5; U: ”is _ '33 inc: S "vazns 3‘“ 3. Q 1&3: r,“ ‘V . were devised, approximately eleven for each of the four dimensions of role-expectations in which the study was interested. Having developed a series of stories, it was necessary to establish some index of their reliability. The context in which reliability is spoken of here refers to the establishment of whether the stories con- stituwd hypothetical social situations which were "understandable" and held ”meaning" for the class of respondents to whom they were to be ad- ministered. In effect, this was a process of epistemic correlation. Responses, in the form of check marks on a written questionnaire, would scarcely be of much value to the research were the respondents not to understand the stories and the questions following each of them. Hence, about three weeks were spent in personal interview sessions with students drawn from two high schools in El Paso, Texas, and one in Juarez, Mexico.2 In each of the three schools, ten students were interviewed with respect to their comprehension of the list of forty-five stories. These inter- views were focused upon: (1) readability of the stories, (2) whether the stories portrayed situations which appeared plausible to the inter- viewees, and (3) whether the reaponse categories seemed to fit the sit- uations portrayed in the stories. The students who were interviewed were selected by a random pro- cess from the tenth, eleventh, and twelfth grade class lists. The *— 2 The two high schools in El Paso, Texas, were the Thomas Jefferson, and Technical, both of which belonged to the El Paso public school system. In Juarez, Mexico, students from the Escuela Secundaria y Preparatoria, a public school, were interviewed. 2.120 an: ,3 wt. w MP". Ffi'. 5“ ‘a é... invv 3:53 30:33 ~tcdete :1: 51;. .4. -ns- tenth.name on ten class lists was selected for interviewing. With res- pect to readability of the stories, the interviewees were questioned as to what they understood about the situations portrayed, whether the plot confused them and.whether there were words with which they were unfamil- iar. It was deemed.important to ascertain whether the stories portrayed situations which would be plausible to the respondents. Here the interview attempted to discover whether the stories portrayed sit- nations which would be readily subject to the respondent's projecting himself into the situation without feeling that such a situation would seldom or never be likely to occur in reality. Finally, an effort was lads to determine whether the reSponse categories appeared to the inter- viewees to fit the situations given in the stories. Attention was dir- ected to the wording of the reSponse categories with reference to clar- ity of meaning for the interviewees. The finding of these interviews with.high school students led to the elimination of fifteen stories as inappropriate. Thus» there remained thirty stories which appeared sat- isfactory for use in pre-testing for the deve10pment of the four Guttman scales. Measurement of Dimension of Role-expectations :‘ the Guttman Scales Some work on the measurement of role-expectations which is highly relevant to this study was done by Stouffer,3 using stories in which 3 Samuel Stouffer, "An.Empirical Study of Technical Problems in Analysis of Role Obligation“, in Parsons and Shils, ed., Toward a General Theogy 9; Action. Cambridge: Harvard Univ. Press, 195117 pp". H7'9'-"179'o"." J‘ d \. - self 5 -s as his 2‘. le :- s; i to s at «I coe It). a s..- ‘9- J 1" | 0-. on. . o .-.... a: -: ab . n 6 .r— C CI‘QV 1'8 . c i '9‘... -s “ "I ah a} g S- -h7- the respondent projects himself into the situation which the stony por- trays. Stouffer also attempted to show that reaponses to this type of device will form a Guttman scale. Following Stouffer's work, this study proceeded to develop four scales. In order to establish the status of the scales developed by this study it will be necessary to discuss the technical features of a Guttman scale. In his development of the theory of scale analysis Guttman distin- guishes at least four criteria for scalability.h These are: (a) reprod- umibility, (b) range of marginal distributions, (c) pattern of errors, and (d) number of items in the scale. The coefficient of reproducibility is an index of the degree to which the scale approaches a perfect scale. In a perfect scale, one could reproduce the respondent's rank.from his total score. The co- efficient of reproducibility is the probability of being able to do this. It 18 Obtained by error: CR'l-(W) Guttlsn has arbitrarily set an acceptable coefficient of reproducibility of 90 percent. The range of marginal distributions should be wide, with some items at either end of the distribution and some items around 50-50. Else- where Guttman.suggests that at least four items should be within the 70-30 range.5 Extreme marginals will produce a spurious CR. h Samuel Stouffer et. al., Measurement and Prediction. Princeton: Princeton Univ. Press, 1950. 5 L. Guttman, A Basis for Scaling Qualitative Data. American Sociological Review, Vol. IX, pp. 139-150. .. 0" Co- 9 ..-t‘ e Q U c511 - Q'“"‘ -..: '. '0 "w -w- 'Ilw‘ no.- ‘a‘b‘ Q .- 5—..:--e Ut‘ \ .‘~‘ a.— The errors of reproducibility must be random. That is, the errors must not be piled Up at any particular non-scale type. In a so-called quasi-scale, errors occur in the form of a gradient. l'his indicates that the errors are caused by one or two other major variables rather than by many small variables occurring at random. The more items included in a scale, the greater is the assurance that the entire universe of which these items are a sample is scalable. Guttman suggests that "if theitems are dichotomous ..., it is probably _ desirable that at least ten items be used, with perhaps a lesser number being satisfactory if the marginal frequencies of several items are in the range of 30 to 70 percent."6 Using these criteria of scalability, it was found that four scales could be develOped from twenty-nine of the thirty stories. These four sets of stories were then submitted to a second pre-test, after which the final form of the questionnaire was deveIOpedL Since thirty stories would have constituted far too many to incor- porate in a single questionnaire, the stories were divided into two groups for both the first and second pre-tests. These two questionnaires were then submitted to groups of United States and Mexican high school students. The students to whom they were submitted were selected by a random process from the tenth, eleventh, and twelfth grades. The 5th, 10th, 15th, 20th and 25th names from twelve class lists in the El Paso 7—— 6 Stouffer _e__t_._a_l_., Measurement and Prediction, 92. £33., p. 52. p \v V'o t“ - Q 0!!“ Vao- q A... ‘ I C .- ‘u 3 a n In: .chart al fare: to {7" . \K ...—0. U Q I I." I , I-AA.‘ a.-. a tenth, e '3 ..W " .5. I m... 9 3'? ho- ‘Uh - -L9- Technical High.School were selected, and each of the questionnaires was administered to these students on separate days. With respect to the Mexican high school, it was not possible to draw a pre-test sample from class lists. Hence, the two pre-test questionnaires were adminis- tered to three whole classrooms of students on separate days. These classrooms comprised, in the Mexican school system, the equivalents of the tenth, eleventh, and twelfth grades. Each of the Mexican high school classrooms contained approximately thirty students. Thus the first pro-test sample contained sixty United States studentsand ninety- one Mexican students. Details on the development of each scale are given below. Scale I. This scale was derived from the set of eight stories which focus upon situations in which the actor is faced with conflicting demands pertaining to (1) his status-role in a sub-system of instrumen- tal relations and (2) his status-role in a sub-system of kinship rela- 7 tions. The dimension measure is that of universalism versus partic- ularism. Seven of these items were found to be scalable with a C? of .91. The distribution of marginals for the first pro-test population of 151 was 110, 95, 91, 86, h7, h6, h5. Scale II. This scale was derived from.the set of nine stories which focus upon situations in which the actor is faced with conflict- ing demands pertaining to (1) his status-role in a subusystem of in- ? See Appendix B, stories numbered.1,2,3 and h. I”; ,1 I 0 ...er' ll ' fin ' F w‘v, 5‘, FR ES ,J i.- S’s tic: fuels 1 v F I Q .‘IIS’JD- ) interes NI. 0 “. SE Scale & 81:. ’ A a «a as s , a ‘H ‘ « N .l ..V o strmnental relations and (2) his status-role in a sub-system of friend- ship relations.8 These stories were found scalable with a CR of .87. The distribution of marginals for the first pre-test population of 151 was 88, 80, 68, 60, 56, h6, 140, 38, 38. Scale III. This scale was derived from the set of eight stories which focus upon situations in which the actor is faced with demands from a sub-system of unship relations which conflict with the actor's self-interest in the situation.9 The dimension measured is that of self-orientation versus collectivity-orientation. The stories were found scalable with a CR of .93. The distribution of marginals for the first pre-test population of 151 was 110, 101, 100, 86, 82, 58, 5h, 6. Scale IV. This scale was derived from the set of five stories which focus upon situations in which the actor is faced with demands from a sub-system of peer (friendship) relations which conflict with 10 The dimension measured is that his self-interest in the situation. of self-orientation versus collectivity-orientation. The stories were found to be scalable with a CR of .814. The distribution of marginals for the first pre-test pOpulation of 151 was 112, 101, 70, 3h, 25. 8 Ibid., stories numbered 5, 6, 7, and 9. 9 Ibid., stories numbered 10, ll, 16, 17, 18. 10 Ibid., stories numbered 12, 13, 1h, 15. -—51. For the second pre-test of these scales, they were again submit- ted to groups of United States and.Mexican students, selected in the manner described above. For purposes of easier arithemetical computa- tion, fifty United States and fifty.Mexican students were selected. Details on this population are summarized below. Scale I. For the second.pre-test population of 100 the OR was feund to be .90 and the distribution of marginals was 71, 59, 55, h8, 51c 32, 27. Scale II. For the second.pre-test population of 100 the CR was feund to be .89 and the distribution of marginals was 63, h8, ho, 39, 37, 32, 25, 25, 20. Scale III. For the second.pre-test population of 100 the OR was found to be .91 and the distribution of marginals was 8b, 77, 69, 67, 67. 57, 50, 16. Scale IV. For'the second.pre-test population of 100 the OR was found.to be .86 and.the distribution of marginals was 71, 59, hl, 38, 3h. The purpose behind the deve10pment of a scale, of course, is that, after the items have been shown to be scalable, a smaller number of items may be selected for administration to a larger population on the assump- tion that these items will discriminate along a single dimension. Thus the final form of the questionnaire was comprised of seventeen stories, four each selected.from Scales I, II, and IV, and.five from Scale III. (One of the five stories comprising Scale III was subsequently dropped.) These stories were selected as the most discriminating, as can be seen clearly by noting the marginal distributions of the four stories selected from each scale as compared with the marginal distributions of all the stories from which the scale was constructed. A.further test for spurious reproducibility is furnished by the chi-square goodness-of-fit test using as a theoretical distribution that distribution which.would be the case if the items were statistically independent of each other.11 If the items were statistically independent of each other, the frequency of the population answering tyes" to all four questions would be equal to the prOportion answering ”yes" to the first question times the proportion answering "yes" to the second ques- tion.times the proportion answering "yes" to the third question times the proportion answering ”yes" to the fourth question. There are size teen possible combinations of responses from four dichotomous questions, and the proportion of peOple who should fall in each group can be cal- culated.by forming products in this manner from the frequencies of "yes" and "no“ for each item. This test was used as a further check on the feasibility of using the four selected stories for each scale. The products formed and the chi-square values obtained are shown in Tables 2, 3, h, and 5. These tables also show the complete distribution of the responses to the selected stories for the second pre-test pOpulation. One of the difficulties in using the chi-square goodness-ofbfit test'is that no adequate answer has been found as to the number of 11 The study is indebted to Dr. Joel Smith,.Michigan State University, fer suggesting this test. :.i.\ .... .5- .... .l .. we ~A.l_ .. as ...- .L .. . a- . I In I r. as. c . . . a '1 'l. .I II __- III I I . I Iii Ill-III III!" I'll! ll- I' all!" ' I. linil 1'K'I‘l‘l‘.-- I! Illiclilllll | Insillilluullllulllllllllo'chi. l I I I'll--.lrl.r!||1|l.lllllrl+¢|.l :-.!.ouol.-|-|s I RIIIII. «...... h. _ 1 72:35.27»: L... ......T......t..uv ‘“3>.2:‘ 32<~5eniU—3 2Z< H $.52»: 23 “...—....L... ..L. H...~lh.~\?..:3:14;: :.rc~ ..,~vfi.~.‘.~.a\.q 3.3::94d33q 1.. in.a. .‘.h. .pmopuopd dream one so moanedmon mpcomohdop canes mans ma u .m.e .Hoo. asap seed a oe.mefl a eaessn-ano 884 a an a s Tapes NMMO. MP. NM. 0v. ON. .. .. e. .o I x I I VMbO. Mb. m0. Hm. ON. .. .. Q .. I I x I HQNO. 5N. $0. 0%. ON. a. .. o. .o X I I I has. 8. mm. as. as. .. .. .. .. xx- .. mOPO. M5. m0. 0%. @N. Ava .. .. .. e. I I I I > ammo. an. we. we. as. H .. .. H x u u x RS. 2.. 8. S. E. .. : .. ma .. .. ..x Z HFNO. bN. WC. Hm. ON. on .o e. o. \ I \ I 85. me. 8. a. as. .. .. .2 3 .. .. x x EH omwo. me. mm. 3. F. .. m .. m .. x .. \ mQMO. MP. NM. HM” 0N. .. e. o. o. I x ‘ I 38. A 2.. mm. R. as. .. a a s - x x x E 88. R. 8. a. E. a .. e c x. xx Homo. new. mm. as. as. m m .. N x x - \ wNHO. fiDN. NM. Hm. 0N. .. o. e. o. x X X I H 88. R. an. a. E. as 3 ma 3 x a. x \ I11"; 9 d a, a 1m n 1M3! m sin-Mm .oszMHmIGMIon mumm coma Bluegrass signified eatflcdsafism Beeps." 38m ow Go. anadnseEanoe no 833883 these: games as H gr. 8 mES 8 22.3888 m8 shes: 15888 N "AM4H ,rIIII.I..I III I I III C 3t 7. .. 7:13... A :5; .1 Fa :Tim. afgfi. 23.3.1.5... 3 p. a . «erases; [IILIIDIIII IIII I. I III I ll I|4,.‘..III .IIIIIIIII.III .I. I. III. IIIIIIIroIIIIIIIOIIIKII.| IIII III I I...II IIAIIIIIIIIII IIIIIIIII.III I I i I: I. I III . II AV}. >... «Z— u..~fia..-.—Zc.~.ne.: L.» J:: W-edv._.~'3v Iajnshzz.‘ 3:‘.5.«u|3_0 32.1 mm. 2.2.33 2; nu..3~.—.~ ....r «......ah.~:Z...~.....s.:~ ctfll $3....H‘.~ 2233:1235. . ‘..IC‘ . c O . ~ . o O O D O I 0 II I! II I a . o a e . . o o c o e o I. I I I O a O A J D I I I I O o a e a o e a I c I 1. o e . o o o a I I no I c c a a e o c c c c a c . ... I i ..l U o O O o I II I I v a s o O Q I ‘ o C O O O I I II I e c a e c a a a a c p c o r l. I I o . n v o o . c o o . .I I O a 0 C 0 G a o a I I u 4 t O O O O O I O O i Q o I I 11 I v o o . a a o . e . . o . I .l I o a . o o o a . e v a . . .l . I. n I D D n h o I o o o a | I I I o a . o o . a a c e . . a I .l I I .pmopummd Hmcfim one so noncommom mpcemoadop oHQsp mesa * as u .e.e .Hoo. acne mass m sa.sn u oasssa-ano momma. S“ cm .Vm no Hence SNO. PH. <5. 00. PM. N .c cc. .. X I I I bNmO. Mm. ON. 00. FM. .. N .. o. I x I I meso. mm. es. em. cm. .. .. s .. n u x . wOHO. 5H. ON. 00. FM. H H .. o. X X I I OOmH. mm. ..Nmn. 00. FM. AQNV .. .. .. .. I I I I > name. as. se. or. me. n .. .. m x u u x swam. mm. es. no. me. .. .. .. mm . a n \ >H mmHO. PH. db. ..NM. PM. o. c. o. o. X I X I name. mm. an. em. me. .. .. a a u u x \ HHH some. mm. em. as. me. .. s .. a - x - \ HFNO. mm. 0N. QM. \Lm. .. N N .. I X X I «see. mm. mm. «m. ms. .. oa OH OH . x x x HH some. as. «a. «m. me. e .. s s x u x \ ..NwHO. PH. CW. 8. M0. .. .. .. .. x X I \ 008. PH. 0N. QM. Nam. .. .. .. .. \ x x I +\mcoo. S. mm. em. 3. N. b. p. n. a. x x x H ..Hm H309 .2” ma S” m .2” 3“ S” m. .2“ mH S” m .03 so: on omsoonofi on? aoassnaasaao asaaansneam casaaaeaneaeasm scrapes cascm mason Ase. asaaanaesecansm cc pseacaaoeeov *MHmwAdzaa magnum—”mo 924 HH mqqom 20 QEBH OB mefimmzommmm mom ggdm “godwow m flamed. Ia. Ha." xvi—a .u—‘_jnl «__Mv III! III III|I I ‘< fiu‘ A\\§... _. a. an >.. .3 on... I... 73427.; 3;. n...L:>h£.?..rv.?......V: 3;: 2.3.35.3«3a «. ... IIIIII II '6 w 1 I17 I ~d‘..~qnv~—w:lvc-~fi.vw cu: J-Nfiwnr» —..~vN-.5.,Y..»V o. £.§.~n\- J 311‘ I D o. c. .. ca .- c. .I O. .Q a. '0 on On '0 O. .- .. s. .0 .0 I. e. .- .. .— 9. It .. or u. an .0 no .9 .s o- .v '0 .- .. ... u. a. ._ .8: 4125...“. '0 I I I I I. I II .. I. II I. I. I I I I. II I I II I. .I ..I I II II II II I. I I! I... (I I I. as I II n I I II I I . [I .pmmpnmpm adcwm map so mmmnommmp mpaommpmmp maps» mane -55... * «H u .m.c .Hoo. swap mmmfl m NH.MNH u mnaswmnfizo $8. 3 on 3 wk. 38. M®MOO QWO m. mm. mm. o. .0 oo 00 I x I I @0000 VW. qqo N00 NW. .0 o. 0 no I I \ I mmOOo 0H0 qu mm. NW. .0 co oo o. x I I I mbOOo GHQ 0m. mm. NW. 00 o. o. o. \ x I I OOMO. ..NW. 3. Mo NN. AOHV o. o. o. o. I I I I b 8.8. 3. 3. mm. mu. .. .. .. .. x .. .. \ mWOHo QWO VQO wm. mp. 00 oo 00 HH I I I \ >H @0000 0H0 QVO N00 NW. 00 o. co co \ I X I $2. 3w. 5.. 8. mp. .. .. 2 Z .. -xx H: .32. «w. m. mm. in .. m .. m .. x - x NVQOO VWO 0m. N0. NW. 00 no o. 00 I \ \ I mam. «m. R. 8. ms. .. mm mm mm - xxx HH OVMO. OH. +N+No N0. who .0 o. o. oo \ I X \ QQNOo OH. 0m. mm. mDo oo oo o. oo \ \ I \ NNHO. OH. @0- NW. NW. .0 oo oo o. \ \ \ I mmqo. 0H. mm. mm. mp. 0H 0H 0H 0." x x x x H ..E H309 w m m N v m m m NM Pm .oz ampH on mmnommoz omhy cofisflbma hflflpfifim Epnmfloufififloofloo Emfium 38m 38m Aha. hpwadpfiodvonmmm mo pnmfiowmmooov 533.424 gamuao 9/3 HHH mafia zo mafia ca mawfimzommmm mom figaedm ...Qmooéom v.mqm112‘ Iraljnul «.20 \u fiu‘ A\.§.- . n _ H 3.33.3. I; «.....n...... 3:. 5...- n q d..—an--:vvl-nnu§v~ ..- 2‘...‘.- .N 3 J .... —.C —.L .pr...J.» V MI... 52:}: a. ‘1...._.- st...‘ Zyfi.‘.~..~.<.§ D o v I v 4 v a u o o n I 4 c v o o o o O u ‘ U n b O a a y s . O n a a u o o r. U o o o o o o o . I a o v c o - O I O o a I o u: ‘\v~n I)» jfiuvfia '- I [I II ‘ ' II ‘ I. II I I. II I. I. I T (I II I. . I. II II I I. II II I! I. [I OI 1. In. '- I. II I. III II II x I II. I I II I. l I: II II II II II II I II II «I II. .pmmpnmpa Hacwm map so mmmcommmp mpcmmmpmmp mapmp mfina * ma u .m.o .Hoo. gagp mmwa m mH.mmH . opmsam-figo $8. fl a. mo 3. 38 MONO. WW. m. mm. NW. 00 o. oo o. I X I I ..NOmOo ..Nmo +\.No N00 NW. .0 oo 0 no I I X I OWOOQ 0A“. .NV. mm. NN. on o. co co \ I I I mbOOo @Ho 0%. mm. NN. on .0 o. o. \ X I I OOMO. Qwo 3. mm. NN. AOHV o. oo o. o. I I I I p OONOO 0H. $0 mm. mm... oo oo o. o. N‘ I I \ mmoa. «w. «a. mm. mp. .. .. .. Ha - u u x >H @9000 OH. 3. N0. NW. .0 oo o. co Na I \ I umua. «m. «a. mo. mu. .. .. Ha Ha - - x x HHH «and. «w. em. mm. mu. .. m .. m s x - \ NQQOO 4%. 0m. N6. NW. .0 o. no no I \ x I mpmm. «m. cm. mo. mu. .. mm mm mm . \_x \ HH OQMOO 0H. 4N9. NO. who o. o. o. .0 Vs I X \ nxuONOo OH. Om. mm. who o. co o. 00 Na \ I \ NNHO. 0H. Om. Nrwo NW0 .0 co oo .0 us \ \ I go. 3. on. .3. mu. 3 3 3 3 x x x x H ..E H309 .N h m N w nf m m w h o N .03 ampH Op omcommmd omhg 833ng baifipfim Epcofioufiyfioofloo 539$ 38m oiom Abw. mwflaapflosuongom mo pcmfiofimmooov $3332 gamugo 9,2 HHH 33m 20 9&5 2. mafimzommmm mom €33 gamooéom v.mqm¢9 fa..vc..,avitélu.; ..1q—! :4. SEC—<21 III! I.'|I IIIIIuII .I I- I‘llllll ‘II .IIIII'. .Ii'll Ills IIII I‘ll I ... 11:33.1 2.". I I. I I It“ III-III- IIII .I'I'IIIIIII’III' I'll A-nxf. 3:1 > H. A... A. — —._ n»...-.C.~;tu~ L; .... .33: .. .L.v mu. IIII.I.II.I-ll 1!. .SL. — VI ~v.~. NH.~.».h9§N~NI\4.~H‘.V- a. Iiuh‘.’ O I V l O O l I c U . 0 v o o 9 o O o a . c D O u o o u a a c O C o ' I 1 O I 9 d O . v 4 C 4 1 U Q 0 b C J :3 —.C firm azdv wrautfl 5&93~L..5<..~ 3‘15: .«v\~4§v\vma I. II. V. l I» o I ll I v: K II II I II I. II I- (I. ‘ II a .I I; —56- .pmmpumna HQCHM mzp so mmmcommoh mesomopmmn magma mflneiw H u .m.u .Hoo. sane mmOH m mH u .H.w .mo. swap mmoH m mH.mm u opascm-Hgo Rom. Hm .3 mm HH. H.309 NNMO. 00. HQ. HQ. 0N. .. .. .. .. I X I I @000. mo. om. Hw. mm. .. .. m .. u n x u oqmo. Hm. mm. HH. mm. m .. .. .. x u u- . WOHO. QM. HQ. .. 0N. .. .. .. .. \ x I I NOQO. 00. Om. HQ. ON. AOHV .. .. .o .. I I I I > ammo. Hm. on. HQ. Hp. H .. .. H x u - \ mmHH. me. am. HH. H5. .. .. .. HH - u n \ >H «HMO. Hm. mm. mm. mm. m .. m .. x u x . HmoH. we. mm. m. Hp. .. .. NH NH - u x \ HHH mmbo. ow. Hv. Hg. Hp. .. m .. m - x - x 3.5. 8. H... om. om. .. H q .. .. x x .. mmHH. co. HH. mm. Hp. .. mH HH MH - x x \. HH 0 mo. Hm. mm. mm. Hu. 0 .. o o x a x x 83. HH. d. Hq. HR H H .. H x x -.. \ mmmo. Hm. Hq. mm. mm. H H H .. x x x - mmmo. Hm. HQ. mm. Hp. «H «H «H «H x x. x x H ..E H38 0H NH HH «H OH NH HH 3 OH NH HH «H .02 EopH mmsommmm many =0HpsthpmHa HHHHHpmnoum wopcmeo-pr>HpooHHoo gnoppum onom oHaom “Ho. HHHHHpHosuoaaom Ho panoHHuooov ..mHmHHHzH mfiam-ao HE... E Sam go 293 8 mamazommam mg 33.2.. Hgaodom m mqm.22.‘ l.|IIIII'l III! 'I:."IIIII I I I'll \ .- I . v- . III.-.‘ ah - r 1.4 1.. u. I I. I'll III-IIIlI-I III. I III. III '.| I In. I... .l I It I I. I l . .I- ..‘III -I II I I -I III. [I I'll! Illlllil IIIII.IV||III In}. I III-I'll... III III «I... .fiJH H—.._n2213..1.u: LC .L:(___r.qul~20 ZZ< >5 2.2532. ...; T. of... .... H. «I ’Q.-§‘. . I d \ C ‘ a . a c v o . v c c . u p u o . o . ‘ O C A O I Q C h V . I . . . . . . v o . o 9 . o a 9 q . o n . c . . . . a . o o o n . c - . o . u u a a . . . . c . . 0 v o a . c O Q 5 w ‘ O ...:lCECESV LLNKHTL..~.....,..-~ v:a..« 24.3???‘1s . D O O O n C C Q ‘ u C O O i I u o o O 0 U n u o O O O I I . o o c O o . o n v a c o o O . v a o O . I . I Q a . . a . . . c n . . I c o t . \....a\v~\ £d.~4\.\vmwa I II I II. II I II. I I] II II 1 I I | I I I . II :I I n [I III II II I II II II I I... II I. .I .I '. II [I II -56- .pmmpnmng Hacfim map so momnommoh mpsmmmgmoh mapwp manaxw H u .H.u .Hoo. aan mmmH m mH u .m.@ .mo. swap meH N wH.mm n mesGmufino $8. .3 G 3 HH. H309 NNMOO 000 Go GO 0N0 o. o. o. o. I X I I @080 8. Om. go ON. oo o. m on I I Nu l oqNo. «m. mm. _. ON. N .. .. .. x n u- . WOHOQ ..NM. HQ. AWN. ON. 00 o. o. o. \ \ I I NQQOQ 00. Om. fiv- ONo AOHV o. oo o. o. I I I I > mmmo. «N. mm. HQ. Hp. H .. .. H x u s x mmHH. me. am. HN. Hp. .. .. .. NH . u u x >H mqmo. Hm. mm. mm. 0N. m .. m .. x - x . HmoH. 00. mm. m. Hp. .. .. NH NH . u x \ HHH mmpo. ww. HH. Hq. Hp. .. m .. m u x u \ N80. 8. Hi om. ON. .. H N .. .. x x .. mmHH. co. HN. om. Hp. .. HH mH NH - x x \ HH oqmo. Nm. am. am. H5. 0 .. o m x - x \ ooqo. Hm. HH. H.. H5. H H .. H x \ -.. x mmNo. Hm. HH. mm. mN. H H H .. x x x - mmmo. Hm. HQ. mm. Hp. NH NH NH .NH \ x. x x H ..E H309 0H NH HH 3 OH NH HH .HH OH NH HH NH .02 EmpH o omnommmm came qOHpspprmHn HpHHHpmpoum wmpqupo-pr>HHOOHHoo :uoppum oHuom onom HHo. HHHHHpHosconmom Ho pamHOHNmooov #3332. 528990 ea 3 828 no 93.3 2. msamzommam mom .635: Haaaoéom m may p h 9 .1 ¢ '~‘- U W a an ae Selec -e an a 55 a“. 50f in ‘e is: for e ace 15 w .49 _‘U .. 0"' Q «0 a, , h! "uég‘ ‘C ¢..i-d.. ‘P:° * xfit‘ v». H . 0v- ‘1‘" ..o I... - ...... ava degrees of freedom to be used. It is usual to lose one degree of freedom for each parameter which is estbmated. The question in this instance is whether each combination of responses is to be considered an estimated.parameter. However, it can be seen that the maximum possible nulber of degrees of freedom.must be fifteen. Since the dis- ‘ tribution of each of the scales is statistically significant for fifteen degrees of freedom, the question seems to be a moot point for the purposes of this thesis. Selection of Respondents and Questionnaire Administration The principal fieldwork on which the study is based was conducted in public high schools in El Paso, Texas, and.Juarez,.Maxico. The final questionnaire was administered to hOS students enrolled.in the El Paso Technical High School in El Paso, Texas. Of these, 2h3 were regular students and 162 were enrolled in special English courses only.12 For reasons pertaining to the organization of the program of studies at the school, it was infeasible to select a random sample of students for administration of the questionnaire. Nor was it pos- sible to select classrooms of students by a random process. Some teachers felt that they could not allow time to be taken from the 12 Of the 2h3 respondents regularly enrolled at the El Paso Technical High School only 15 indicated that they were following one of the technical courses of study. The rest were enrolled in one or an- other of the general academic curricula. ..... 22:21:; courses aims were re mi of the c aeration of a“: in 13th, .i .‘ case classr; .'s ‘ “I: ‘CI ‘ ‘.‘:'a 0: ‘ ‘a 8:0 Fw- - “ware in A ‘s '. _58— regular cones of studies. The problem of selecting a sizeable number of students to whom the questionnaire might be administered devolved into one of securing approximately equal numbers of classrooms devoted to teaching courses at the 10th, 11th, and 12th grade levels. All 12th grade students were required to enroll in a social science course, and so several of the classroans devoted to this subject were selected for ad- ministration of the questionnaire. Also, all students were required to enroll in 10th, 11th, and 12th grade English courses, and, thus, a portion of these classrooms were selected with the consent of the prospective teachers. The final questionnaire was administered, then, to classrooms of about thirty students each. It was necessary to accomplish the ad- ministration of the questionnaire within a period of forty minutes, which period of time was the scheduled length of the classes at El Paso Technical High School. This requirement of forty minutes for admini- stration of the questionnaire, of course, placed restrictive limits upon the length of the instrument. It was found during pre-testing of the questionnaire that forty minutes provided sufficient time to give dir- ections to the students and allow them to complete the entire instrument. The questionnaire was administered to students during the final two weeks of the school year, June 1 to 15, 1956. Students regularly en- rolled in special English courses at the El Paso Technical High School were administered the Spanish version of the questionnaire. The final questionnaire also was administered to 397 students en- rolled in the 10th, 11th, and 12th grades of the Escuela Secundaria y 53.530113 of Hair. 3! the E 2: select a ran ‘ o r'f‘, f‘n '- ation O_ -59- Preparatoria of Juarez, Mexico. For reasons pertaining to the organ- ization of the program of studies at this school, it was not possible to select a.random sample of students from the three grade levels, nor to sample systematically'classrooms. However, classrooms of students drawn from the three grade levels were secured in approximately equal members. The Director of the school arranged to have teachers bring, their classes to the school auditorium at designated hours, and the qmestionnaire was administered en.mass. Needless to say, the Spanish version of the questionnaire was administered to these students. Ad- ministration of the questionnaire under these conditions required only two days. VA: “‘5 ch AMA ‘ \ s " ""0 Ti" v«'“ u cu CHAPTER III VALUE STANDARDS AND SOCIAL STRUCTURAL COIELEXES This chapter will outline some of the major features of Mexican culture which imply the predominance in certain sectors and the emer- genes in others of universalistic type value standards. It will point out also that major segments of Mexican society manifest relatively high degrees of structural differentiation and segregation of social relations. and that large scale instrumental complexes are to be found in Mexico. Parsons' statements pertaining to the analysis of the group of societies which he calls Spanish-Amarican provide an appropriate _. starting point for this discussion. It will be recalled that his theoretical formulations imply that role-expectations represent a more or less unmodified transposition of cultural value standards. Parsons' analysis adheres to this general implication, with the result that it is somewhat curious and vague. It is by no means clear whether his analysis is meant to pertain exclusively to value-orientations (a category of the personality system) or to cultural value standards and social role-expectations, or to all three collectively. The structure of his dialysis in this unclear manner may be presumed to follow di- rectly from the development of his thesis. But, in any case, using the analytical device of the pattern variables Parsons proceeds to classify these societies as exhibiting a predominance of particularism :79! givers a1 :13: is re“ ‘ es '9 :f :arerege 0:1 tte-aver, if ‘r. so. I s‘ w 493.1 It is ‘ £331“! \ -61- over universalism, and ascription over achievement. Such a classifica- tion is subject to serious question both in terms of its intended scope of coverage of quite different societies and segments of those societies. Moreover, if his brief analysis is intended to apply to the dominant national cultures it fails to consider important spheres in which uni- versalistic-type and achievement-type value standards are very much in evidence. His analysis would seem to apply mst appropriately to that category of action orientation identified as role-expectations in his schema, rather than to cultural value standards. With respect to the application of his analysis to personality, little can be hazarded by Parsons or any other social scientist owing to the lack of available scientific data pertaining to the organization of action at the level of the personality system among Latin American peoples. It can scarcely be considered an appropriate or correct analysis to assert that parti- cularistic-type value standards constitute the exclusive or even major type of cultural values borne by the dominant sectors of Mexican so- ciety.1 It is not the intention of this chapter to examine, other than parenthetically, the structure of social relations in Mexican agricultural 1 See, for example, Manuel Gamio, Cultural Patterns in Modern Mexico, Qggrterly Journg. 9; lpter-American Relations, Vol. 1. 1939, pp. 49-61. Also Jose E. Iturriaga, La estructug social 1 cultural. gig Mexico. Mexico. D. 1., Ibndo de Cultural Economics, 1951. John Gillan, Modern Latin American Culture, Social Fbrces, Vol. 26, 1948, pp. 243-252. Lesley Byrd Simpson, Unplanned Effects of Mexico's Planned Economy, Th_g Virginia Quarterly Review, Vol. 29, No. 4, 1953, pp. 514-532. Fbr a Journalistic account of some stature, see Tomme Clark Call, Th__e_ Mexicg Venture: From Politicgl _t__q Industrial Revolution _i_n_ Mexico. Oxford University Press, 1953. 1 "E! ..-t veg-s 3. :2113221 cult: c a fi Hart '-°-‘i:. -61- over universalism, and ascription over achievement. Such a classifica- tion is subject to serious question both in terms of its intended scope of coverage of quite different societies and segments of those societies. Moreover, if his brief analysis is intended to apply to the dominant national cultures it fails to consider important spheres in which uni- versalistic-type and achievement-type value standards are very much in evidence. His analysis would seem to apply most appropriately to that category of action orientation identified as role-expectations in his schema, rather than to cultural value standards. With respect to the application of his analysis to personality, little can be hazarded by Parsons or any other social scientist owing to the lack of available scientific data pertaining to the organization of action at the level of the personality system among Latin American peoples. It can scarcely be considered an appropriate or correct analysis to assert that parti- cularistic-type value standards constitute the exclusive or even major type of cultural values borne by the dominant sectors of Mexican so- ciety.1 It is not the intention of this chapter to examine, other than parenthetically, the structure of social relations in Mexican agricultural 1 See, for example, Manuel Gamio, Cultural Patterns in Modern Mexico, Mterlz Journal of Later-American Relations, Vol. 1, 1939, pp. 49-61. Also Jose E. Iturriaga, La estructhLa social 31 cultu_r_'_a_1_ g3 Mexico. Mexico. D. 1., lbndo de Cultural Economica, 1951. John Gillan, Modern Latin American Culture, Social Forces, Vol. 26, 1948, pp. 243-252. Icsley Byrd Simpson. Unplanned Effects of Mexico's Planned Economy. 1h; Virginia Qaarterly Review, Vol. 29, No. 4, 1953, pp. 514-532. Fbr a Journalistic account of some stature, see Tomme Clark Call, Egg Mexicg Venture: From Political t_o_ Industrial Revolution in Mexico. Oxford University Press, 1953. I; ‘4 .1 " sofa; 'V‘V‘ales ’ .QN ’ ' 71.638 9.7111 3 pups, and ex lirarise, cult liieczssion ui the scope “Ext to an; 1:9 Suzie-cur -62— villages. It is quite evident, from anthropological and sociological field studies, that the structure of social relations in rural Mexican villages exhibits considerable variability according to regions, ethnic groups, and extent of isolation from the dominant national society. Likewise, cultural value standards vary among these small sub-systems. A discussion of the rural scene is beyond the purview of this chapter and the scope of the study. Parsons' brief analysis referred to above, subject to any coherent interpretation, would appear to apply best to the socio-cultural milieu of the small, isolated, rural, agricultural village. However, this study deals with non-rural persons who, as it will be pointed out, comprise a sizeable portion of the Mexican popu- lation, and who exhibit social habits and cultural values which may loosely be referred to as those of the dominant socio-cultural milieu. Mexican Backgrounds It is of prime importance to the examination of the dominant national society in Mexico today to understand that highly significant socio-cultural changes have occurred in the present century.2 In gen- eral. it may be said that immediat ely prior to the Revolution of 1910- 1920 the dominant national society was characterized by strong conser- vatism and traditionalism. Rural peasants and urban proletarians were effectively isolated from participation in the national political pro- cesses, and attempted political action was forcefully discouraged. 2 See Charles Cumberland, The Mexican Revolution, _‘I'hg fig! Leadeg, Decem- ber, Vol. 27, 1954, pp. 12—17. Also Henry Bamford Parkes, g Histogz 9; Mexico. New York: Houghton Mifflen Co., 1950. Nathan L. Whetten, Rural Mexico. Univ. of Chicago press, 1948. attached v11 latilords. 1 tiarxteri 2e, 22131133 3: :lea'iffl alt 91:5:8-‘1 in h 1'1! Chief an 2.19 "39.3313' 3a? Zen .. e for», t t‘, / -(‘J3_ The dominant religious system generally fostered the maintenance of this status quo. Overwhelming illiteracy prevailed among the rural peaSants and urban proletarians. A.semi-feudal social, political, and economic order characterized rural life, with large tracts of land and their attached villages under the domination and control of a relatively few landlords. Regional and sub-regional geographic and social isolation characterised.most of the country, and geographical mobility of the population appears to have been generally quite low. Strong class cleavages along lines of social, political, and economic interest existed in both the rural and urban settings. Agriculture provided the chief an almost exclusive source of gaining a livelihood. With the exception of the extractive industries, and the beginnings of tex- tile manufacturing, industrial employment was nearly non-existent. What manufacturing there was generally was restricted to small scale enterprises, and handicraft methods of production were the norm. The function of government was largely restricted to the maintenance of order and discipline in the society, thereby facilitating the economic and other interests of the dominant social class. Conspicuously lacking as functions of government in the pre—Revolution period were public edu- cation, public health, and civil rights protection for the masses. What little formal education there was available was under the jurisdiction of the Catholic church. Concern with problems of public sanitation was largely lacking. Provisions fer redress of flagrant violations of civil and property rights of the mass of peasants were largely ignored. In fact, civil and property rights generally appear to have been absent so far as the peasant was concerned. 2e iazinsnt 2‘ 312:: no. u“ than pr: :rier (Latest. embed vil‘. 1:113:13. F.- 21s'acteri 2e: ltlathn a; 21687;?” 315 meted in be! 1:93:18: an l *9 means—‘- (0) — «a' n ..Y “1 dis (O -‘J J»— The dominant religious system generally fostered the maintenance of this status quo. Overwhelming illiteracy prevailed among the rural peaSants and urban proletarians. A semi-feudal social, political, and economic order characterized rural life, with large tracts of land and their attached villages under the domination and control of a relatively few landlords. Regional and sub-regional geographic and social isolation characterised.most of the country, and geographical mobility of the population appears to have been generally quite low. Strong class cleavages along lines of social, political, and economic interest existed in both the rural and urban settings. Agriculture provided the chief an almost exclusive source of gaining a livelihood. With the exception of the extractive industries, and the beginnings of tex- tile manufacturing, industrial employment was nearly non-existent. What manufacturing there was generally was restricted to small scale enterprises, and handicraft methods of production were the norm. The function of government was largely restricted to the maintenance of order and discipline in the society, thereby facilitating the economic and.other interests of the dominant social class. Conspicuously lacking as functions of government in the pre—Revolution period were public edu- cation, public health, and civil rights protection for the masses. What little formal education there was available wasVunder the Jurisdiction of the Catholic church. Concern with problems of public sanitation was largely lacking. Provisions for redress of flagrant violations of civil and property rights of the mass of peasants were largely ignored. In fact, civil and property rights generally appear to have been absent so far as the peasant was concerned. In pre-‘F ensue affsi $149631 1;; "i’i '33 Print: si releti 791+ 1:5: Jamal : '55? 28193;; ’91:: in Villas 2 50's? 2 Elite. :he re 25!“ 13,339 “I“! A ”1-, v. .- . L‘ 0.. 5:83.311: of eti‘n' . .1.95_ 8 I‘ Egan a: b. i l . efifla -64.. In preoRevolution Mexico a hegemony in political, governmental, and economic affairs was exercised ‘w the urban upper class and the rural semi-feudal land-holding elite. Life among the peasant masses was car- ried on principally within the context of the small agricultural village, and relatively little geographical mobility occurred. The structure of instrumental relations revolved largely around agricultural tasks and the marketing of agricultural products. The involvement of the popula- tion in roles of responsibility and leadership in political, economic, and governmental affairs was restricted to the relatively small landed elite, the religious authorities, and the great merchants, while the masses labored under various conditions of peonage. Particularistic rights and obligations revolving around family, village, and the semi- feudal relationship between peon and patron composed the major confio gurations of the social order for most Mexicans, and the web of society exhibited a relatively low degree of structural differentiation.3 Revolution and Change The Revolution of 1910-1920 wrought significant changes in the fabric of Mexican society and culture. The hegemony of the semi-feudal landlords was overthrown. Land reforms in rural areas were carried out, often in the form of eJidal land grants to peasant villages. The Catholic church was more or less effectively barred from direct autho- rity over political and governmental affairs. Participation of the urban and rural masses in the local, state, and national political 3 Parkes, 92. cit., passim. Fifi”?! ‘81 3”,: wheel {sewn ‘ s .‘ reziered to .. l r:::_:a:ied t7 ’3 be increas "‘88? . the _ _ ‘ ‘.. intro: ~65-I processes was encouraged as a patriotic duty. The new government intro- duced radical policies4 and expanded greatly in terms of the services rendered to the national public. The years of active hostilities were accompanied by population movements which have continued to the present day. Today Mexico exhibits relatively high degrees of differentiation in its social structure. Government provides important services and possesses numerous agencies for the articulation of these, as well as for the maintenance of order in the society. Industry has made signi- ficant advances, although the country remains predominantly agricultural. Large scale commerce and trade are evident. Public primary and second- ary schools are established throughout the country. The urban popula- tion is on the increase, and geographical mobility in general appears to be increasing. Illiteracy among the masses is decreasing, yet remains high. Modern communications and transportation networks con- nect the major commercial, agricultural, and industrial centers into an increasingly coherent framework.5 4 Inssolution of latifundia, the ejidal system of land tenure, the en- couragement of organized labor, the curtailing of church power, and the introduction of widespread public education are some of the major changes which the new regime accomplished. 5 See Antonio Carrillo Flores, Industrial Mexico, Selected.Readings 9p Mexico: A,Seminar 93,1breign Affairs, mimeographed, Michigan State University, 1957. Fbr some salient social problems arising out of the changed conditions see Arturo Mbnzon, Planteamiento de Algunos Problemas Indigneas, America Indigena, Vbl. VII, No. 4, Octobre, 1947, pp. 323-331. lexico 1| :zlt‘sal Cour. ruthiris of tile in 195:; 111.7 Likev :29:le of t Lightly mre :5 mr 52,31 F9122! llVe: izcrem in t 11! IESO can 2:518:13; V! "’1 1‘ 3f 12: 1:323:11 in}. laiign Of 4. It is :7?! “he Be 9' °be 21 it '2 3.. 1115‘}. -6"... Rural—Urban Features Mexico remains to the present day a predominantly rural and agri- cultural country. According to thecensus of population of 1940, nearly two-thirds of those economically active were engaged in agriculture,6 while in 1950 there were about fifty-eight percent engaged in agricul- ture.7 Likewise, Mexico remains largely rural, with slightly more than one-half of the population residing in places of 2,500 or less in 1940. Slightly more than thirteen percent of the population lived in cities of over 50,000 inhabitants in 1940, while approximately twenty-two percent lived in cities of sore than 10,000 inhabitants. A si zeable increase in the population residing in urban centers is recorded in the 1950 census. By 1950 slightly more than forty-two percent of the population were living in places of 2,500 or more inhabitants. The census of 1950 listed twenty-four cities with populations exceeding fifty thousand inhabitants. These twenty-four urban centers included a popu- lation of 4,828,524, or about nineteen percent of the total population. It is precisely in the urban areas of Mexico that universalistic- type value standards and forms of large scale instrumental complexes are to be encountered. It is to be noted, further, that notonly large scale instrumental complexes exhibit relatively high degrees of differ- entiation of activities and the segregation of these activities from 5 Wilbert E. Moore, Industrialization and Labor. Ithaca: Cornell Univ. Press, 1951, p. 247. 7 Septim Censo General _dg Poblacion, 1950, Cuadro 21, p. 86. 3:19: life spit mall scale 1: :‘zese feature name: of e 7:31 differs 18213 of 3:: 1e paticipa; 5:9: IPEIB‘}: 315“ Patter :3 332:! as t .‘ ! Le ”3M .7:f&.f '1‘ {£251 ‘ .j‘¢ e y" a“ 1 ...‘3 ?‘~ I 'x1::§# N other life spheres of the individual, but it is characteristic that even small scale instrumental complexes in the urban milieu also exhibit these features. Wherever the instrumental complex involves the unequal statuses of enployee and proprietor in the collaborative endeavor struc- tural differentiation is implicitly present. Likewise, so far as the status of employee is concerned, the instrumental activities in which he participates are characteristically segregated from familial and other "personal” action interests. Certainly, in Mexico, this is the common pattern. The principal reasons for viewing the urban milieu, in contrast to the rural, as the focus of the above socio-cultural fea- tures are that very small scale instrumental complexes constitute the numerically greater portion of agricultural production enterprises, and the differentiation of status-roles with respect to the instrumental activities occurs principally as an extension of status-roles occupied in the family and 1d.nship unit. The small scale family farm or ejido plot employs little or no extra-familial labor, whereas even the small scale economic enterprise in the urban center is likely to employ some extra- fami 11 a1 labor. Occupations and Labor In 1940 approximately thirty percent of the labor force was en- gaged in non-agricultural occupations, whereas in 1950 approximately forty-two percent were in non-agricultural occupations. In 1940 approximately 640,000 persons were employed in manufacturing industries, while in 1950 the number had increased to 970,000.8 8 Whetten, gp. 911., pp. 180-197. unfattriné' :izzal catego: :crzatian :; s gap Cansi is size of mesh; 5:22 2:3 , Car; a»; a \.3‘ 3 have if a .-- 1‘ “a of : .titei i“ T . ‘ 5. N- J3. A__ z ' 1“ ‘2 L, ‘ Manufacturing at both of these periods was the largest single occupa- tional category outside of agriculture. Commerce employed approximately 550,000 in 1940 and about 680,000 in 1950. Communications and trans- portation employed some 150,000 in 1940 and approximately 210,000 in 1950. Considering the number of persons employed in manufacturing the size of membership in labor unions appears large. In 1942 there were some 650,000 members of unions. In general, Mexican labor unions are of the industry-wide type rather than of the crafts type. The two major labor confederations are the Confederacion Regional Obrera Mexicana (CROM), and the Confederacion de Trabajadores Mexicanos (CTM). Labor unions have been important instrumentalities in raising the standards of living cf Mexican industrial and commercial workers, and.have con- stituted important political forces in the nation. Being industrial- type organizations, encompassing thousands of workers, they may be considered large scale instrumental complexes. Moreover, the labor union movement, a sub-cultural ideological complex of considerable importance on the national scene, may be viewed as exhibiting major patterns of universalistic-type value standards.9 The dedication of the movement to the gaining of rights and benefits for employees 9 The labor unions or sindicatos in Mexico differ radically from labor organisations in the United States in one respect in particular. They are very strongly dominated by political forces. See Rank Tannenbaum, Mexicg: The Struggle for Peace and Bread. New York: Alfred A. Knopf Co., 1951, pp. 175-190. See also Marjorie Ruth Clar, Organized Labor ig_Mexico. University of North Carolina.Press, 1934, passim. t. v'aea'fm‘;t t. 5:;lifies the Tzeir varied Ezre‘l obse fze lab the o: gic ns -69. throughout the sphere of economic exchange relationships in the society ex- emplifies the predication of the movement on universalistic-type values. Their varied activities further imply this. It is relevant to note Moore's observations in this respect. The labor organi zation in Mexico, through the very nature of the occupational heterogeneity of its membership, is a strate- gic instrument of social control and change. The wide extension of sindicatos into the rural village, their support of village schools, adult literacy campaigns, adult education in agricul- tural and marketing improvements -- all this bespeaks the in- fluence of the unions toward social innovation. When to this is added local and national direct political participation, the sindicato becomes a major source of innovation and national economic development. The existence of a. labor market is becoming more evident in Mexico as is indicated by the numbers of persons who are wage and salary earn- ers. Slightly Imre than fifteen percent of the population were wage and salary earners in 1940, while fifty-two percent of the economically active portion of the population were wage and salary earners. By 1950, fifty-eight percent of the economically active population were wage and salary earners. Moore observes, with respect to the figures for 1940, that the relatively low percentage of wage and salary earners derived in significant part from the organization of economic activities on the basis of family groups in Mexico, and he notes that the economy operates with units of smaller scale than in the United States. While this appears to be so, it should be kept in mind that these observa— tions apply especially to the field of agricultural production in Mexico. The relatively low amount of persons being wage and salary 10 Moore, 92, cit., p. 240. c w . ..P“ 5 “2" ...-L r 2:19! of p! 2: .I. my a: I ,srten’. of t 3 3:39:91 it" '2' 13 112:3 1’ o‘ d. sate r913: :33! Of 0C0 ’31:; are 1: a 1 .E:‘ A. augShipa '.‘ ““e it 9-4 “1's— -70- earners thus derives principally from the fact that the preponderant number of persons engaged in agricultural production are proprietors on a very small scale. It is also to be noted that females comprise a such smaller portion of the economically active population in Mexico than in the United States. One indication of this is the proportion of wage and salary earners who are females. In 1940, only 5.3 percent of the females aged 15-64 were wage and salary earners, while in the United States the proportion was 23.9 percent. On the other hand, 52.5 percent of the males aged 15-64 were wage and salary earners in Mexico as compared to 60.9 percent in the United States. It will be useful to keep in mind these statistics in that they provide a rough indication of the relative lack of involvement of females in Mexico in non-familial types of occupations. That is, these figures imply that females in Mexico are involved to only a limited extent in economic exchange re- lationships which are differentiated and segregated from the activities of the family. However, of those females classified as economically active in 1940 more than two-thirds were wage and salary workers. Hence, it might be concluded that females with aspirations toward in- volvement in the complex of economic exchange relations are most likely to become involved as either wage or salary earners, i.e., as employees of business firms. Literacy The proportion of the population which can read and.write may be taken as a very rough indicator of the commonality of the dominant national culture. Without an ability to read and write, access to many {gets of the . a ‘I‘ atemrtry t.- ti): aged ten fifty-five p= cf write, as if the papal: 3533215“ ”are; An» 31:38 0 f E's -71- facets of the dominant national culture is severely restricted. It is noteworthy that literacy has increased slowly since the end of the Mexican Revolution. By 1940, about forty-nine percent of the popula- tion aged ten years and older could read and write, while by 1950 about fifty-five percent of the population aged six years and older could read and write, according to the national census. Likewise, the proportion of the population speaking Spanish, the dominant national language, may be considered as an index of involvement in the national society and culture of Mexico. In 1940, the proportion of the population speaking Spanish, was about eighty-five percent and by 1950 had increased to slightly more than eighty-eight percent. Conversely, the proportion of those speaking only an indigenous language or dialect declined from about seven percent in 1940 to about three and one-half percent in 1950. The proportion of the population speaking both an indigenous language and Spanish remained approximately seven and one-half percent in both years. Formal Mass Education It would seem a warranted generalization to assert that formal mass education leads to the sharing of ideas and values on the part of those subject to the educational system. From a substantive social per- spective this is certainly one of the primary manifest goals of national educational systems. The magnitude of the scale of education in Mexico is large and, hence, it will be important to examine it briefly The federal and state governments in Mexico have launched into an impressive program of mass education since the end of the Revolution. 1"h “ . 3 .jtv firmer . ‘is ‘5‘“: '. ac! eu‘a‘. a" ‘ l . - I. K ' :4 D V In , ....s. 0H . ~' .2 six grai- erratizna‘. 3"‘5‘ ‘R'J‘ILS .- II B _ -72- By 1950 there were some 748 "lower primary schools"11 financed by pub- lic funds, and some 115 privately financed ones. These enrolled approx» imately 115,000 pupils in 1940 and approximately 127,000 pupils in 1952. The six grade primary school, however, is the most common type of formal educational institution in Mexico, and in 1948 there were 23,257 of these. By 1952 the number of these latter had increased to 25,613. Of these, some 20,000 were located in rural areas and about 5,500 in urban areas. The size of the student population has been of considerable magni- tude also. In 1952, there were approximately 1,420,000 pupils enrolled in the rural primary schools, and some 1,870,000 enrolled in the urban primary schools. With respect to post-primary educational institutions, there were 1,210 schools registered in the country in 1952. Of these, 464 were secondary schools, 80 were preparatory schools, 234 commercial schools, 87 normal schools, 145 advanced professional schools. There were slightly less than 200,000 students enrolled in all of these di- verse types of schools in 1952, and of these about 75,000 were enrolled in secondary schools and about 22,000 in preparatory schools. The school systems constitute an important instrumental complex; many of the values associated with them may be categorized as univer- salistic. The Mexican school systems, although varying in details of their adaptation to national, regional, and local conditions, follow the general pattern of Western culture. The value emphasis is perhaps slightly different from the pattern in the United States in certain 11 This is a type of primary school which includes only the first three grades. :Zazsrooa re 31:92: to b: n.- ““ 3e .-‘ZEF‘ ‘ ._‘~ 0 ‘3‘; -73.. respects, but not radically so. Fbr example, rote memorization of lessons tends to be generally adhered to in Mexican schools, as does classroom recitation, whereas in the United States these activities appear to be no longer so highly valued. Academic competition, the allocation of grades on the basis of examinations, and the enforcement of discipline in classrooms are predicated on universalistic—type values. It is not an uncommon practice, in the writer's experience, that general norms of social morality are imparted within the context of the class- room. This is especially so with respect to the humanistic ideals of the Mexican Revolution. The public school system, in fact, has been a major institution for the inculcating of the aspirations of the Revo- lution. It is notable that the values of honesty and restraint in dealing with one's fellows, the concept of obligation to the nation, and many if not all of the commonly considered humanistic values consti- tute a not insignificant part of the value premises upon which the formal educational structure is predicated, and which in various forms are con- veyed to the students. Something has been said already about the scale of the educational systems in Mexico, and it would seem necessary to note here only that they are organized on a relatively large scale involving more than three million students and some twenty-five thousand schools. Government The sphere of governmental relations constitutes an important in- strumental and integrative complex in a nation-state such as Mexico. Certainly, it manifests a form of large scale social organization and 0.045, m , .55.... EJY‘. feieratei st' fat-.91 I’"°l w "‘51-‘98 CPL 3": o & -«--..es are 'a s: W.‘.‘ “we-i c a s .03. 111‘. ‘q u: ‘3‘! e 50 ‘, . «e t .‘a . l I f :v . O. f I t# pJ- :v‘ '0, I) (J m 0!! 8 *1 .v 0 ’4 I" (n t It I- I. [I] '~ 0.”; (I .. f? l) w l" (J I“ ”a ((1 "‘ I) '1 H: -74- exhibits maJor cultural values.12 Mexico is a federated republic with governmental relations organized on three levels, national, state, and local. The legislative, administrative, and judicial functions of the national government are performed by separate agencies, similar in model to those in the structure of United States government. Each of the federated states similarly possesses a constitution which defines the formal structure of the state governments and the Jurisdiction and sepa- ration of powers. The states, in turn, are subdivided into political entities called.nunicipios (municipalities). These latter political entities are relatively weak so far as general political and taxation powers are concerned. With respect to the general discussion to which this chapter is addressed one of the most important points to be noted about the admin- istrative branch of government in Mexico is that it is structured in a bureaucratic form.13 It is evident that not all of the features of Max 12 United States Department of Commerce, Government and Business in Mex- ico, Investment in.Mexico. Government Printing Office, 1955, Chap. II. 13 Max Weber‘s bureaucratic type includes the features (1) a clearly de- fined.hierarchy of offices, (2) members of the association are‘subject to authority only in respect to the impersonal obligations pertaining to the office, (4) the office is filled by a free contractual relar tionship, (5) offices are allocated initially on the basis of achieve- ment, that is, demonstrated technical aualifications, (6) the members of the association are remunerated by fixed salaries in money, (7) the office is treated as the sole, or at least the primary occupar tional activity of the incumbent, (8) there is a.system of promotion according to seniority or to achievement or both, and promotion is dependent on the Judgment of superiors, (9) the bureaucrat is subject to strict and systematic discipline and control in the conduct of the office. See H. H. Gerth and C. wright Mills, From Max Weber: Essays 33 Sociology. Oxford University Press, 1946, pp. 196-203. Ig'aer's 1162' i :elati :3 it. i ‘.. 21:: of V5,- 6 3956C» to ‘. 53737258 8; It" .' 0" I28 nast- e'.‘ . ‘b; ‘Is h a c~ 33““, '— 3’5 "‘3’ $1 ‘” n . Q ff D Weber's ideal-type bureaucracy are manifest in the structure of social relations in the case of governmental machinery of Mexico. But here we are concerned with cultural value standards and the extent of complexity and elaboration of social relations. The combination of these ideal- typical elements elaborated by Weber does appear to define a.configura- tion of values, i.e., of features considered desirable, in Mexico with respect to the structure of governmental relations. The constitutional and legal framework defining the legitimate normative standards pertain- ing to the structure of social relations in government in Mexico does encompass such features as Weber enumerates. Moreover, public reaction to the flagrant violation of certain of these values may be taken as further evidence of their substantive social acceptance.14 It is important to note that although the culture defines these features as desirable they are not thereby necessarily articulated in the structure of social relations as role—expectations. The distinction between the normative and the existential orders must be kept in mind continually. It is to be noted, then, that at the cultural level there obtains a configuration of values defining the mode of bureaucratic structure in government as desirable. Furthermore, this culturally defined desirable mode of structuring social relations appears to mani- fest some degree of articulation at the level of the social system of governmental relations. With respect to the argument of this thesis, it is to be observed that the configuration of standards defining the 14 See Whetten, 92. g11., especially Chapter XXI on Government in Hexico and pp. 545-554 with special attention to the mordida. elem: of f1: types of Beside :‘ze sphere : '82! 82.3215 :23. here is fins, are ; 1: c-mceive versalistic- $793331! :5: t"fie exclusiv 361131 Telat ignat 81 151.31". 5. 3‘s. . 4.21:: frat 1:es 8 no 1'38 :11! . V 197: 35a elements of bureaucracy clearly conform to the universalistic and speci- fic types of value standards of the pattern variables typology. Besides the bureaucratic rules pertaining to governmental offices, the sphere of governmental relations is predicated in part on cultural vahue standards taking the form.of legal prescriptions. What is impor- tant here is that law norms, in the framework of western legal institu- tions, are preeminently universalistic-type normative standards. Hence, to conceive of Mexico as possessing e.cultural tradition in which uni- versalistic-type value standards are of relative insignificance is to overlook maJor aspects of the culture. To be sure, law norms are not the exclusive value standards which pertain to the sphere of govern- mental relations. value standards of custom and convention are also important elements of the cultural framework pertaining to governmental affairs. But it is important to understand that law norms constitute a basic framework of values pertaining to governmental relations, and these norms are of the universalistic type. The sphere of Mexican governmental relations exhibits a high de- gree of structural differentiation and constitutes a large-scale in— strumental complex. A.vast bureaucracy administers governmental affairs at the federal level, while lesser scale bureaucracies operate within the Jurisdictions of the states and municipios. It would be super- fluous to attempt an enumeration of the myriad agencies occurring at the level of the federal government, much less at the state and muni- cipio levels. However, some of the more important agencies of the federal government are organized around the recognized national needs elem! 0‘ 1‘- fi: type: of Besiies tte sphere 0: use 3:232. 12:: he: is :im, are p 1: :nxeive , 'Efsalistic- train}: 2;: .22 exclusi- _v5_ elements of bureaucracy clearly conform to the universalistic and speci- fic types of value standards of the pattern variables typology. Besides the bureaucratic rules pertaining to governmental offices, the sphere of governmental relations is predicated in part on cultural value standards taking the form.of legal prescriptions. What is impor- tant here is that law norms, in the framework of western legal institu- tions, are preeminently universalistic-type normative standards. Hence, to conceive of Mexico as possessing a.cultural tradition in which uni- versalistic-type value standards are of relative insignificance is to overlook maJor aspects of the culture. To be sure, law norms are not the exclusive value standards which pertain to the sphere of govern- mental relations. Value standards of custom and convention are also important elements of the cultural framework pertaining to governmental affairs. But it is important to understand that law norms constitute a basic framework of values pertaining to governmental relations, and these norms are of the universalistic type. The sphere of Mexican governmental relations exhibits a high de- gree of structural differentiation and constitutes a large-scale in- strumental complex. A vast bureaucracy administers governmental affairs at the federal level, while lesser scale bureaucracies operate within the Jurisdictions of the states and municipios. It would be super- fluous to attempt an enumeration of the myriad agencies occurring at the level of the federal government, much less at the state and muni- cipio levels. However, some of the more important agencies of the federal government are organized around the recognized national needs 3f #1281135, u ' . 12331101! EL. a" "r M“; o“ in he VH‘U categary 3f 3.7.95 Value 5 stemzezt a: {timed by {‘ '::~' on In. ..‘te .1 ti Particg' n -,’ - I of education, health, public works, finances, postal service, commu- nications and transportation, public safety, military and naval forces. Certain nationalized industries also-fall within the scope of govern- ment administration, most notably the petroleum and railroad industries. It may be concluded, then, that a major configuration of values of the cultural tradition conforms to Parsons' universalistic-type category of value standards. However, to conclude that universalistic- type value standards obtain in the cultural tradition with respect to governmental affairs is not to assert that at the social system level universalistic-type role-expectations also obtain. he field data gathered by this study and to be presented in subsequent chapters constitute materials bearing upon the incidence of universalistic and particularistic types of role-expectations among certain classes of Mexicans. Commerc e and Industry Legal norms and formal rules of office of a.universalistic type, of course, pertain to social relational complexes other than that of government. The relatively large-scale industries referred to pre- viously in this chapter represent instrumental complexes which are directly subject to a wide variety of legal norms and exhibit a pattern of bureaucratic values in the definition of offices. The involvement of the separate industrial enterprises in market complexes implies that they subscribe to a.maJor configuration of universalistic-type value standards, i.e., a pricing system. In fact, the capitalistic econcuuc exchange relationship, so strongly based on pricing systems the 81399 .195. ...' J y- 158 O- 3 st- ‘ ‘4‘ to us- U‘Jal ..I“ b u.‘. 8 .e . .. i 1 i 0..- AL 0 . 3 t- 8 e a. ‘5 s‘b z ”- ‘ v a: .. a p u a y .a “a .5 Ops ‘. Q - A: u a... .1 a «M ..... r. E .... and the specification and limitation of obligations of the contracting parties, is the prototype of a pattern of universalistic-type value standards.15 Summary In summary, this chapter has attempted to point out that there are exhibited in the structure of Mexican society major instrumental complexes organised on a relatively large scale. These complexes, moreover, manifest the characteristic of relatively high degrees of structural differentiation and segregation of social relations. It was indicated also that universalistic-type cultural value standards are associated with these large scale instrumental complexes. Further- more, it is in the context of urban life that the large scale social organization, the pattern of sharp structural differentiation and segregation of social relations, and universalistic-type value stand- ards are to be found in association. However, that there should be encountered universalistic-type value standards in Mexican culture does not necessitate that at the social system level there must likewise be found universalistic-type role-expectations. It has been the purpose 15 It is to be noted that not all econondc exchange relationship come plexes are based principally on the universalistic-type values of a pricing system. Examples of economic exchange relationships in which criteria other than prices are prominent or even predominant are available. The potlatch of the Northwest Coast Indian groups constitutes one such relationship which was not based on a pricing system. The form of exchange relationship in which the nature of the social contact itself is more highly valued than the exchange price, as appears to occur with some frequency among certain Guate- malan and other Indian groups, constitutes another example of pricing being secondary to other value considerations. . ' ‘ A" :atimi .L. . o b, then. 19.. :3 speczf‘. ; -79- of this chapter merely to point out that there do obtain universalistic- type value standards in what has been referred to as the dominant national culture. It will be the purpose of the following chapter to present data concerning role-expectations of Mexicans who, according to specified criteria, might be expected with some degree of likelihood to be involved in the dominant national culture. ‘ a- +- ‘ 7‘ . s O Y I. 0 3:18 €2.5- . i - a b 5c. s.‘ .-9 .r s} .4 e. a .m t e. L e v .1 3 2 _ 0 me 3 Q Q .3 6 u r“ wk 5 as we I ..2 .4 .u . a .. «a .Q . a . a. ... ... .. is... a: .. .. CHAPTER IV THE BEARING OF THE EMPIRICAL FINDINGS ON THE GHNERAL THEORETICAL IMPLICATIONS This chapter will present a range of empirical findings bearing on the general theoretical impliCations discussed previously. verification of each of the four propositions formulated above will be assessed by testing several pertinent hypotheses. The propositions will be examined in order. The dominant national Mexican culture exhibits cognitive and value premises which are in large part evolved from Western Civilization, and it would be fatuous to suppose that universalistic-type value standards do not obtain as salient features of the culture. In order that the findings of this study may constitute an assessment of the implications of Parsons' theory that social role—expectations represent a more or less unmodified transposition of cultural value standards it must be admitted that both Mexican andenited States cultures exhibit salient configurations of universalistic-type value standards. Given this assump- tion the study may proceed to a comparison to certain patterns of role- expectations exhibited by the subjects of the study. The proposition bearing upon this implication of the social system schema is stated below. General Implications: Proposition I The first proposition examined concerned the relationship between cultural value standards and social role-expectations. The argument 7 Q.- s s}: on Ca . a. A: p - ettion . .A‘Igan“ 590* ‘1' b s .5.‘ '4... Is ‘ ...-‘7 ...-“HF- e p -g- 'A ~! -81... advanced previously against Parsons' conception of this relationship, while believed accurate and compelling, would be enhanced by empirical findings which were congruent with it, Likewise, were the argument uns sound, it could be denied by empirical finding which agreed.with Parsons' conception of the relationship. This study's interpretation of Parsons' conception of the relationship is stated below in the form onProposition I. Proposition I Social role-expectations will represent a relatively unmodified transposition of cultural value standards. Having stated the proposition concerning the theoretical implication in which the study was interested, an hypothesis bearing upon the data gathered by the study was posed and tested. This hypothesis is as follows: Hypothesis I If'Proposition I is true, then both the group of Mexican students and the group of United States students will exhibit a.predominance of'universalistic-type role-expectations in their responses to the pattern variables scale items. The most important empirical evidence gathered by this study which bears upon this hypothesis is to be found in the development of the two Guttman scales of universalism-particularism. These seales were discussed in detail in Chapter II. The fact that it was possible to arrange the responses of Mexican and United States students along a uni-dimensional continuum.of'universalisnhparticularism.censtitutes important relevant evidence which must be interpreted as failing to confirm Hypothesis I. The hypothesis is not confirmed by this evidence because the construc- tion of the scales depended upon securing representatives from both ‘ — .~ 9 P b‘firus' Db. itezs or. 8 .~‘.{ on x ."‘C“ ‘ Mar, T at '; '4- a .\ 'aida . _ .. ,~‘ ~dr‘r‘izu. -.‘ .L‘ i cultures. It was not possible to construct a scale with the present items on either of the groups separately. The United States students exhibited a strong tendency to give uni- versalistic-type responses to the test scale items, and were it not for the inclusion of the group of Mexican students it would not have been possible to develop a proper Guttman scale. The MexiCan student group, on the other hand, exhibited a strong tendency to give particularistic- type responses to the test scale items. That the Mexican student group should exhibit a stronger strain toward particularistic-type role-expec- tations is a finding which does not conform to the theoretical implica- tion. It is very important to note also that there was a.considerable degree of overlap between the Mexican and United States groups on both of the scales of universalism-particularism. This is to be seen in the scale score distributions presented in Tables 6 and 7. Some Mexicans ex- hibited more universalistically oriented role-expectations than the United States shudents and, vice versa, some United States students exhibited more particularistically oriented role-expectations than some Mexican students. It was believed that these findings could not be dismissed lightly. If it may be assumed that universalistic-type value standards obtain in both cultures with respect to the kinds of situations portrayed in the scale items, then it should not have been possible to construct Guttman scales because the students' role-expectations also ought to have been predominantly universalistic. Obviously, in an empirical operation of this nature, unanimity of response would not have been expected. It would not have been expected that all of the Mexican and United States students would respond to the test items in the same manner even if \ Q. tale-ex: 8C1 “*2 of cal M.“ 2:51: of e . "fi‘fln: ol “d'dia . . w... ." 1“ '9 NEJ. Ew‘ A bl Q" ~ ‘3-1::E£i] F‘: '1. [1 O 'I '1 I.) k” g, 9 -83— role-expectations were to represent a relatively unmodified transposi- tion of cultural value standards. Rather, there would be expected a margin of error which might be attributed to numerous factors only randomly associated with the response pattern, e.g., misinterpretation of the scale items, mistakes in marking the reply, and willful mdsre- presentation. But if this were the case then it still would not have been possible to construct Guttman scales because of the randomness of such variant responses. These findings, then, must be construed as failing to support Proposition I and, hence, they cast doubt on the correctness of the theoretical formulation. A.more adequate conceptualization of the re- lationship between social role-expectations and cultural value standards will be explored in a subsequent portion of this chapter. General Implications: Proposition II The social system schema's conceptualization of the relationship between types of role-expectations (as defined in terms of the pattern variables) and certain features of social structure was discussed above. The concern here is with the second proposition formulated above. This proposition is as follows: Proposition II As the extent of differentiation and segregation of social relations increases in a social system, role-expectations will change from a predominance of particularism to a.predendnance of universalism. This proposition was tested by reference to the empirical findings of the study. Without recourse to a detailed exposition, the assumption ‘. N3 ... ‘~ .81.,— was made that the social system of the United States exhibits a very high degree of differentiation and segregation of social relations. This assumption was conceived as pertaining eSpecially to the sphere of instrumental exchange activities. It was assumed that in the United States the major activities of an instrumental nature are performed by roles which are differentiated and segregated from expressly family and locality group roles. It also was assumed that the social system of Mexico exhibits a considerable degree of differentiation and segregar tion of social relations. Various aspects of the structure of social relations in Mexico were examined in Chapter III, and it was pointed out that on the average the economy's operating units are smaller and less complex, with comparatively fewer persons involved than in the United States. The important point, however, is that the dominant national scene in Mexico may not be characterized as a folk-type system of social relations, but exhibits on a smaller scale many if not all of the urban-type features which characterize the United States. With the above assumptions made explicit, an hypothesis was stated and the empirical findings of the study were examined for evidence of its confirmation. The hypothesis is as follows: Hypothesis II If Proposition II is true (that'universalistic-type role-expec- tations are a function of the degree of differentiation and segregation of social relations in a.social system) then the United fiates student group will exhibit a greater tendency toward.universalistic—type role-expectations than will the Mexican student group. -85- Scales I and II constituted the relevant empirical data for the testing of this hypothesis. If Hypothesis II holds, then the distri- bution of scale scores of the group of Mexican students will differ from that of the group of United States students, and the mean rank of the Mexican student group will be lower than that of the United States student group on Scales I and II. Scale I, it will be recalled, deals with universalism-particu- larism and was constructed on the basis of items involving a conflict between the expectations of a familial role (that of brother) on the one hand, and “economic" or "bureaucratic" role on the other hand. To score high on this scale means to rank high on universalism and, vice versa, to score low means to rank low on universalism. A null hypothesis was posed that the distribution of the group of Mexican students does not differ significantly from the distribution of the groups of United States students on Scale I. This statistical hypo- thesis was tested by a chi-square test of significance and it was found that the chi-square was significant at the .001 level. (See Table 6.) Hence the null hypothesis was rejected and the inference was drawn that the distributions of the two groups on Scale I do differ signifi- cantly. In order to determine which of the two groups were distributed nearer the universalistic end of the scale a ranking procedure was applied to the data with the fellowing result. The Mexican students were found to have a mean rank of 252, while the United States students had a mean rank of 329 on Scale I. Thus, the ranking showed that the L'J . 0 a) ~86- TABLE 6 3131‘ ammo: 131' 179.337.1111? or SCALE TYPES DERIVED FROM RESPONSES TO ROLE-CCTTFLIGT excursions Ir-rvoLVIrtc .1: Hostel-'3 ROLE VERSUS A FALKLIAL ROLE (SCALE 1) Ethnic Scale Type Gm”) I II III IV v N Mexican 76 86 9O 59 30 341 United States 15 51 58 59 38 221 N 91 137 148 118 68 562 Chi-square I 36.7 P less than .001, d.f. I 7 TABLE 7 DISTRIBUTION BY NATICIIALITY or SCALE TYPES DERIVED FROM RESP 1:333 T0 ROLE-CDIIFLICT SITUATIOZFS INVCLVING AN Insraumrrm. ROLE VERSUS A FRIEIDSE-IP ROLE (SCALE II) Ethnic Scale Type Gmup i 11 III 'fi IV V N Mexican 23 84 107 110 63 387 United States 16 24 44 59 74 217 N 39 108 151 169 137 604 Chi-square ' 36.7 P less than .001, d.f. : 7 .. . ..09- ....u. 0 " 11:. I”. -87— United States students ranked higher on universalism.or further toward the universalistic end of the scale than did the Mexican students. Hence, it may be concluded that these data confirm the first hypothesis stated above, and support the implication that universalistic-type role—expectations are a function of the degree of differentiation and segregation of social relations in a social system. 1 The ranking procedure is as follows. 1 The scale scores of the total distribution were considered to be ranked from lowest to highest. Then each scale score was given as a rank the mid-point of the pro- portion it occupied of the total cumulative freouency. This may be illustrated with a dummy table as follows. Groups Scale Scores I II _III Total Mexican 5 lO .5 20 United States 10 l5 5 80 Scale Score Frequency Cumulative Hank Frequency I 15 15 7.5 II 25 40 27.5 III 10 5O 45 The rank given each scale score was nultiplied by the frequency of each group falling that category. Then the total of the rank of each group was divided by the total freouency of each group to find the mean rank. Mexican United States I 5 x 7.5 n 37.5 10 x 7.5 = 75 II 10 x 27.5 a 275 15 x 27.5 =412.5 III 5 x 45 a 225 5 x 45 . 225 Total 537.5 712.5 Mean Rank 537.5 .- 26.875 213:5 = 23.75 30 s ‘ ‘ Qua-‘5‘ I‘ew ‘ . Iii‘ffi :e:tat: A0“; M z.. 0 . F. s.. s . h. .D ‘u s ‘— fistri ‘\ V.- w. "‘t ’9 'M‘ .' ='~ ' a...'~. .5. an )1 6‘. ~98: It will be recalled that Scale II also deals with the universalis- tic-particularistic dimension of role-expectations, and that it was constructed on the basis of items involving a conflict between the ex- pectations of a friendship role on the one hand, and such other roles as student at an examination, wdtness at a law court, member of a student body, and a fourth role which might be termed "witness to an accident". To score high on this scale means that the respondent exhibited a proclivity to interpret universalistic-type role—expectations as binding his resolution of the role conflict. To score low on this scale means, of course, that the respondent exhibited a tendency toward the interpretation of particularistic-type role-expectations as binding. As in the case discussed above, a null hypothesis was posed and then submitted to a chi-square test of significance. It was found that the chi-square value was significant at the .001 level. See Table 7. Hence, the null hypothesis was rejected, and the inference was drawn that the distributions of the two groups on Scale II do differ signifiCantly. A.ranking procedure was then applied to the data to determine which of the groups was distributed closer to the universalistic end of Scale II. It was found that the Mexican students had a mean rank of 279, while the United States students had a mean rank of 243. Thus, the ranking showed that the United States students fall closer to the universalistic end of the sCale than do the Mexicans. Hence, it may be concluded that these data also confirm Hypothesis II, and support Proposition II. It was observed that the socio-cultural system of Mexico from which the Mexican subjects were drawn and.that of the United States both 03-.2: tow .- aspc'l .- -.n‘ .‘l':° now-V' On 35 5.. i, I “Casi- I”... “p t, .. u is 1: ‘b: -._‘\ |r «‘3‘, its.‘ \: a" I.. ‘et e 'u 'h" I- o .9 mac. on: It will be recalled that Scale II also deals with the universalis- tic-particularistic dimension of role-expectations, and that it was constructed on the basis of items involving a conflict between the ex- pectations of a friendship role on the one hand, and such other roles as student at an examination, witness at a law court, member of a student body. and a fourth role which might be termed "witness to an accident". To score high on this scale means that the respondent exhibited a proclivity to interpret universalistic—type role-expectations as binding his resolution of the role conflict. To score low on this scale means, of course, that the respondent exhibited a tendency toward the interpretation of particularistic-type role-expectations as binding. As in the case discussed above, a null hypothesis was posed and then submdtted to a.chi-square test of significance. It was found that the chi-square value was significant at the .001 level. See Table 7. Hence, the null hypothesis was rejected, and the inference was drawn that the distributions of the two groups on Scale II do differ signifiCantly. A.ranking procedure was then applied to the data to determine which of the groups was distributed closer to the universalistic end of Scale II. It was found that the Mexican students had a mean rank of 279, while the United States students had a mean rank of 243. Thus, the ranking showed that the United States students fall closer to the universalistic end of the scale than do the Mexicans. Hence, it may be concluded that these data also confirm Hypothesis II, and support Proposition II. It was observed that the socio—cultural system of Mexico from which the Mexican subjects were drawn and.that of the United States both ..: 2.3 w 0 3':" 'fl .ni .-b s u 0 . a! , ,nu i 'ps‘ s._ . 1.: En“ ' tn .u filo Q . Eu. 0"- t ‘e «C. S exhibit relatively high degrees of differentiation and segregation of social relations. Hence, according to the theory, it would be expected that both the United States and Mexican students would exhibit rela— tively high incidences of universalistic-type role-expectations.v The scale scores of both groups on the two scales of universalism-parti- cularism showed that both groups did exhibit relatively high incidences of this type of role-expectation. The significant test of the implication, however, revolved around the notion that, although both the Mexican and United States students were involved in socio-cultural systems exhibiting high degrees of dif- ferentiation and segregation of social relations, the socio-cultural system of the United States exhibits considerably higher degrees of differentiation and segregation than that of Mexico. Hence, it would be expected that the United States students would exhibit a higher incidence of universalistic-type role expectations than the Mexicans. . The analysis showed that this was indeed the case and, thus, the find- ings were interpreted as supporting the theoretical implication at this general level. General Implications: Proposition III The third proposition stated earlier may now be examined. It reads as follows: Proposition III As the extent of differentiation and segregation of social relations increases in a social system role-expectations wdll change from a.predomdnance of collectivity-orientation toward the family to a predominance of self—orientation. -90- In order to verify this proposition an hypothesis was devised as follows: Hypothesis III If Proposition III is true, then the United States student group will exhibit a greater tendency toward self—orientation than will the MexiCan student group. With respect to this hypothesis, Scale III constituted the rele- vant data. If this hypothesis holds, then the distribution of scale scores of the Mexican students will differ from.that of the United States students and the Mexicans will rank lower than the United States students on Scale III. This scale, as was pointed out previously, deals with the self-collectivity orientation dimension of role-expectations. It was constructed of items involving a conflict between self interests and familial interests. To score high on this scale means to rank high on collectivity-orientation toward the fondly and to score low is to rank low on collectivity-orhntation toward the family. A.nu11 hypothesis was posed that the distribution of the Mexican student group does not differ significantly from the distribution of the United States student group on Scale III. See Table 8. This hypothesis was tested by a.chi-square test of significance and it was found that the chi-square value was significant at the .001 level. Hence, the null hypothesis was rejected and the inference was drawn that the distributions of the two groups on Scale III do differ significantly. In order to determine directionality, the same ranking procedure was applied to the data.on Scale III as was done in the cases of the previous two scales. The Mexican students were found to have a mean TABLE 8 DISTRIBUTION BY IIATIUNALITY or SCALE TYPES Damn) 830:: 33390113813 To ROLE-CONFLICT SITUATIGIZS INVOLVING SELF- ‘RIEJTATION VERSUS COLLECTIVITY- RIEIZTATION Tort-mo THE FAIJLY (SCALE III) Ethnic Scale Type emu" I a II III IV V N MexiCan 11 121 151 58 341 United States 40 108 54 10 212 N 51 229 205 68 553 Chi-square : 73.7 P 1638 than .001, def. 3 5 TABLE 9 DISTRIBUTION BY NATICT-TALITY OF SCALE TYPES 1112111131) FROM RESPONSES TO ROLE-CONFLICT SITUATIONS INVOLVING SELF-O. ETTATIC‘N VERSUS GOLLEC’PIVITY-ORIEIITATION TOWARD Tm Print 8301:? (SCALE Iv) = Emhnic Shale Type Gr°up I II III IV V N Mexican 59 100 112 78 37 386 United States 17 39 58 52 46 212 N 76 139 170 130 83 598 Chi-square I 25.6 P less than .001, d.f. ' 7 L. ~21 ‘I 5;“. &\‘ H hi .‘9. 'M.‘ “-- ‘. IN . a ....- ‘. rank of 317, while the United States students had a mean rank of 211 on Scale III. Thus, the ranking showed that the United States students rank higher on self-orientation than the Mexican students. Hence, it may be concluded that these data.confirm Hypothesis III, and support Proposition III. i.e., that collectivity-orientation toward the family is a function of the extent of differentiation and segregation of social relations in a social system. General Implications: Proposition IV This proposition deals with the selfecollectivity dimension of role-expectations in the context of the relationship of the individual to his peer group. The proposition is as follows: Proposition IV As the extent of differentiation and segregation of social rela- tions increases in a social system role-expectations will change from a predominance of self-orientation to a.predondnance of collectivity-orientation toward the peer group. In order to test this proposition the fbllowing hypothesis was formulated. Hypothesis IV If Proposition IV is true then the United States students will exhibit a greater tendency toward collectivity-orientation toward the peer group than will the Mexican students. The relevant empirical data for testing this hypothesis were con- stituted by Scale IV. This scale was constructed of items involving a conflict between self interests on the one hand and peer group interests on the other. To score high on this scale means that the respondent tended to select a collectivity-orientation resolution to the conflict _C)3_ situations, while to score low means that the respondent tended to select selfeorientation resolutions. A.null hypothesis was posed and submitted to a.chi-sauare test of significance. It was found.that the chi-square value on Scale IV was _significant at the .001 level. See Table 9. Hence, the null hypothesis was rejected and the inference was drawn that the distributions of the two groups on Scale IV do differ significantly. A.ranking procedure was applied to these data to determine direc- tionality. The Mexican students were found to have a mean rank of 275 while the United States students had a mean rank of 843 on Scale IV. Thus, the ranking showed that the Mexican students rank higher on self- orientation than the United States students. Hence, it may be concluded that these data.confirm Hypothesis IV, and the confirmation of this by» pothesis lends support to Proposition IV, i.e., that collectivity-orien- tation toward the peer-group is a function of the degree of differen- tiation and segregation of social relations in a social system. ‘The foregoing analysis dealt with implications of the social sys- tem schema at a very general level, that of the relationship between types of social role-expectations and the extent of differentiation and segregation of role relationships in social systems. Three general propositions derived from the schema were tested against a limited range of empirical data. It was inferred that the empirical data.presented by the study supported these three propositions and, hence, lend a measure of empirical confirmation to those sectors of the theory which were focused upon. The significance of these findings will be given attention in the summary to this chapter. :19 i: Q h to... -t¢’«’-u :‘fi‘rg c "e- s s frrzn. “Duels 't—s. at. t “It“, i'. .. me _.E _9[+_ Summary The analysis of the empirical data presented in this chapter has now been concluded. The first implication of the social system theory which was examined was that of the relationship between cultural value standards and social role—expectations. It was contended that Parsons posits too simplistic a.relationship between these two orders. A.prs— position was formulated which attempted to articulate in concise terms the implications of the theory with respect to the relationship between cultural value standards and social role-expectations. This was called Proposition I. Next, an hypothesis bearing upon this proposition was formlated and tested against the empirical data. These empirical data consisted of the two pattern variables scales of'universalismeparticue larism.(Scales I and II). It was noted that the development of these scales depended.upon the inclusion of representatives of the United States and Mexico. It was not possible to construct a Guttman scale of universalism—particularism based solely on either the United States student population or the Mexican student population. Further, it was noted that both the United States and Mexico exhibit important config- urations of universalistic-type value standards applicable to the situ- ations portrayed in the scale items. ‘It was reasoned that were social role-expectations to represent a relatively unmodified transposition of cultural value standards, then both the United States student group and the Mexican student group should have exhibited a predominance of uni— versalistic-type role-expectations. Had this indeed been the case it, of course, would.not have been possible to construct successfully the ~95- two Guttman scales of universalism-particularism on this population. The scales were constructed, however, and they met all of the criteria of scalability prescribed by Guttman. Moreover, the distribution of both the Mexican and United States students throughout all of the scale—- types on each of the pattern variables scales was interpreted as consti- tuting conclusive evidence which failed to support Proposition I. It was concluded, then, that the theoretical implication that social role-expectations represent a relatively unmodified transposi- tion of cultural value standards may not be considered confirmed by the empirical data presented in this chapter. On the contrary, the em- pirical data strongly urge the rejection of any simplistic conception of the relationship between these two orders and call for a. reformila- tion of Parsons' conceptualizations. Parsons' position tends to assume that cultural value standards are transposed relatively unmodified into social role-expectations. In doing so the form and results of system analysis tend to be predetermined. When the position is taken that social role-expectations represent a relatively unmodified transposi- tion of cultural value standards, system analysis is led to disregard the differences between these two orders or consider any differences observed as merely random events. Another important ramification of such a position is that the system analysis will tend to equate these two orders. Thus, if it is determined by analysis that universalistic- type value standards obtain at the cultural level with respect to a class of social relationships the simple transpositional view will tend to assume that universalistic-type role-expectations will also “I obtain among the population bearing such a culture. Parsons himself dis- regards the differences between social role-expectations and cultural value standards in his comparison of the United States and Latin America. It is suggested, then, that if the relationship between the existen- tial and normative orders were conceived of as a question of the extent of their convergence toward one another or divergence away from one another then the theory might be enhanced by relieving it of a.theore- tical implication which predetermines and tends to bias the analysis of substantive social reality. If the theory were to conceive of the re- lationship between social role-expectations and cultural value standards as a cuestion of their convergence or divergence, then the relationship between these orders in any given empirical instance clearly need not be pre-judged. On the contrary, the relationship then becomes problem- atical and a matter of significant empirical concern. With this view, the relationship between cultural value standards and social role- expectations emerges as an important focus of the maintenance of stability and order as well as a source of change in a socio—cultural system. Parsons' view of the maintenance of stability and order and of the initiation of social and cultural change lays heavy emphasis upon the personality system of the individual actor and upon the diadic social relationship.‘ As a result of this he achieves a rather high degree of closure in his schematic conceptualization, but the schema itself tends to become overly static. Problems of social change tend to be anchored too exclusively in processes obtaining in the personality I nu;- :0 A. 5”. . 'Mta h -97- systems of actors. Whereas it must be admitted that there may be per- sonality correlates of the socio-cultural systenn that the personalities involved in a given socio-cultural system must adapt to it in some viable form, it is questionable whether an understanding of the expli- citly social aspects of large scale human groups may best be achieved by laying such a heavy emphasis upon personality features. By considering the relationship between normative and existential orders as problematic a way is opened for introducing a.more dynamic character to the social system schema. This view brings into the pur- view of the theory the rolc of innovation, for example, in technology, as a focus of disjuncture between the existential and normative orders of a social system, or at least that sub-sector of a society most in- timately concerned with the given technological innovation. Although this view in itself may scarcely be considered novel, it is of con- siderable importance to Parsons' schema to lay more emphasis upon it if the schema is to embrace a.more dynamic view of socio-cultural sys- tems. The point which we wish to stress is not that role-expectations and value standards may never stand in an approximately unmodified re- lationship to one another, but that this might be expected to occur only under rather special conditions. Parsons' conception of the relationship between these two orders would seem most likely to apply to the small scale, isolated, non-literate society. In view of the empirical find— ings presented in this chapter, then, it may be concluded that the implication that social role-expectations represent a relatively un- modified transposition of cultural value standards may not be maintained 0’3. a.-l I.‘ n ... if ~98- except as a.special case. Hence, it is urged that the theory adopt a position which treats the relationship between these two orders as problematical. In addition to the above implication, this chapter also assessed certainly explicitly sociological implications of the theory against a range of empirical data bearing directly upon these implications. One of the major criticisms leveled against the social system theory is that it is too abstract and stands largely unconfirmed by empirical analysis. Thus, it was hoped that the findings of this study might achieve a measure of empirical confirmation of certain sectors of the theory. Since the theory is presented hy Parsons largely in a discur- sive form it was found necessary to fermulate several propositions I based on the implications of his discussion. There were also examined in this chapter, then, three propositions concerning the relationship between types of social role-expectations and the extent of differen- tiation and segregation of social relations in a social system. By- potheses predicated on these propositions were formulated and tested against the empirical data gathered by the study. The first of these was proposition II which stated that, as the extent of differentiation and segregation of social relations increases in a social system, role—expectations will change from a predominance of particularism to a predondnance of universalism. The assumptions were made that both the United States and Mexico exhibit high degrees of differentiation and.segregation of social relations, but that the United States ranks considerably higher in this respect than Mexico. - ‘ Q l a n ((l ECCI“. QAD:‘ nut. 'ACO .JJ. 5 ' u 1‘, w' u. .. we: :an ..F nl" —w\ -99.. Hence, it was reasoned that if Proposition II were true, then the United States student group would exhibit a greater tendency toward universal- istic-type role—expectations than would the Mexican student group. The subsequent analysis of the Mexican and United States students' scale scores on pattern variables Scales I and II showed that the United States students did exhibit a greater tendency toward.universalistic-type role-expectation than the Mexican students. Thus, the hypothesis was considered confirmed, and it was inferred that Proposition II was sup- ported by these data. The next implication of the theory which was examined was formulated in Proposition III. This proposition stated that as the extent of differ- entiation and segregation of social relations increases in a social sys- tem role-expectations will change from a predominance of collectivity- orientation toward the family to a.predominance of self-orientation. It was reasoned:that if this proposition were true, then the United States students would exhibit a greater tendency toward selfsorientar tion than would the Mexican students. The analysis showed that the United States students did exhibit a greater tendency toward self- orientation than the Mexicans. Thus, in this case also, the hypothesis was confirmed and it was inferred that Proposition III was supported by the data. The final implication of the theory examined in this chapter was formulated in Proposition IV. This proposition stated that, as the ex- tent of differentiation and segregation of social relations increases in a social system, role-expectations will change from a predominance 0r v.4 .D h 'n . a. .u ’A '- a- L» A.__l f’? ~103- of selfborientation to a predominance of collectivity-orientation toward the peer group. Again, it was reasoned that if this proposition were true, then the United States students would exhibit a greater tendency toward collectivity-orientation toward the peer group than would the Mexicans. In this case also, the hypothesis was confirmed. The analy- sis showed that the United States students did exhibit a greater tendency toward collectivity-orientation toward the peer group than did the Mex- ican students. Hence, it was inferred that Proposition IV also is sup- ported by the data. It is to be noted that the inferences drawn from the analysis of the empirical data were interpreted as supporting Propositions II, III, and IV. These data are considered to support these propositions but not to prove them.true. In formal logic a universally stated proposi- tion may be proved false by a single case of disconfirmation. Thus, Proposition I was considered false in view of the nature of the infer- ences drawn from the analysis of the empirical data. On the other hand, all cases of the class to which a universal proposition pertains must support that proposition if it is to be proved true. Since these pro- positions pertain to all of the class referred to as social systems, the data of this study cannot prove them true but may, at best, only support them. The data.presented here derive from the members of only two social systems, i.e., the United States and Mexico. Hence, we are limited in our conclusions to the statement that the data support Pro- positions II, III, and IV but do not prove them true. Nevertheless, this is the mode by which the scientific method must necessarily proceed, and ~10], hence, it seems warranted to contend that a measure of empirical con- firmation of‘Parsons' theory has been achieved. Propositions II, III, and IV'pertain to the relationship between types of social role-expectations exhibited by persons and the extent of differentiation and segregation of social relations obtaining in the social system in which these persons are participating members. It is recognised, of course, that to focus upon the extent of differentiation and segregation of social relations in a social system is to refer to a highly generalized feature of social structure. The notion comprehends an admittedly gross feature and, without further specification, would seem of greatest utility in the comparative analysis of relatively different social systems. while differences in the incidence of role- expectation types were noted as between the Mexican and United States students on each of the pattern variables scales, it was observed also that a wide range of role-expectation types was exhibited within each of these groups. It appears clear, then, that some factors subsumed ‘under this general notion need further specification in order to carry the analysis to a.point where it more adequately comprehends these intrargroup differences in role-expectations types. CHAPTm V SOCIAL'ROLEkEXPECTATIONS AND ETHNICITY IN THE UNITED STATES STUDENT GROUP The previous chapter dealt with implications of the social system theory at a very general level and involved a comparative analysis of the types of role-expectations exhibited by members of two distinct socio-cultural systems, i.e., the United States and Mexico. Although the analysis was interpreted as supporting the propositions under exam- ination, it was noted that a wide range of role—expectation types was exhibited by the members of both the United States and Mexican student groups. Hence, it is implied that factors sub-sumed under the general notion pertaining to the relationship between social role-expectations and.the extent of differentiation of social relations in a social system must account for such findings. The interest of this chapter is in attempting to isolate factors which more adequately might account for the wide range of role-expectations types exhibited by the United States students. Considering the ethnically complex nature of the United-States- {exican border, it would seem likely that ethnic status within the United States student group might be a factor related to the types of role-expectations exhibited by these students. This chapter will exam- ine the ethnic composition of the United States students to determine if ethnicity may be a factor accounting in some measure for the wide range of role—expectations exhibited by the United States student group. -103_ .A first approximation to the segregation of the United States students and Anglo-Americans on the pattern variables scales, and the mean rank of the Spanish—Americans should fall in an intermediate position between those of the other two groups. This, in turn, would account in some measure for the distribution of the total United States student group along the whole length of the scales instead of piling up at opposite ends from those of the Mexican group. In order to examine this notion the United States students were sub-divided into two groups, one composed of Spanish-name students and the other of non-Spanish-name students. By the sub—division of the United States into two groups a.rough comparison may be made of the Anglo-American, Spanish-American, and Mexican students on each of the pattern variable scales. Null hypotheses were posed that the distribu- tions of scale scores of the Anglo-AmeriCan, Spanish-American, and Mex- ican groups do not differ significantly from one another. These statis- tical hypotheses were tested on each of the pattern variable scales by a chi-square test of significance. The distributions of the scale scores of these three groups on Scales I, II, III, and IV are shown in Tables 10, ll, 12 and 13, respectively. With respect to the distribution of the scale scores of these three groups on Scale I (universalism versus particularism) it was found that the chi-square value was significant at the .001 level. See Table 10. Hence, the null hypothesis was rejected, and the infer- ence was drawn that the distribution of the scale scores of the three groups do differ significantly from one another. Thus, it may be inferred ~104- TABLE 10 DISTRIBUTION BY mmrrc camps 0F SCALE TYPES DEI-iIVED 1?ch snare}: I“ TO ROLE-CONFLICT SITUATImIS INVOLVING m: soc-mus ROLE VERSUS A FmtIJAL ROLE (SCALE I) Ethnic Scale Type Group I II III IV V N Mexican 76 86 9O 59 30 341 Anglo-American 9 25 28 31 11 104 Spanish-American 6 26 30 28 27 117 N 91 137 148 118 68 562 Chi-square = 45.2 P less than .001, d.f. ' 12 TABLEvll DISTRIBUTION BY mmnc GROUPS OF SCALE TYPES Dnuvm) F3011 RESPONSES TO ROLE-CONFLICT SITUATICNS I::VCLVI:~IG 1m INSTR warm ROLE VERSUS A FRIEISDSIIIP ROLE (SCALE II) Ekhnic Scale Type Gm“? I II III Iv v N Mexican 23 84 107 110 63 387 Anglo-American 9 14 21 28 29 101 Spanishpsmerican 7 10 23 31 45 116 N 39 108 151 169 137 604 Chi-square - 38.1 P less than .001, d.f. 3 12 -105... that the factor of ethnicity is associated with the distribution of scale scores, i.e., with the degree of universalisticetype role—expec- tations exhibited by the students. A.ranking procedure was applied to the scale score distributions in Table 10, and it was found that the Spanish-American students had a mean rank of 341, the Anglo-Americans a mean rank of 309, and the Mexican students a mean rank of 252. It may be seen from the mean ranks that the Spanish-American group ranks highest on universalism, the Anglo-American group second highest, and the maxican group, of course, lowest. Now, while it was suggested that ethnic status within the United States student group might be a factor associated with the types of role-expectations exhibited by the American students, the notion that the Spanish-American sub-group may stand in an intermediate position between the Mexicans and Anglo-Amer- icans is not supported by the data of Scale I. That the Spanish-Mer- ican group should exhibit a higher degree of universalistic-type role- expectations than the Anglo-American group constitutes an unanticipated finding which will be explored in greater detail below. With respect to the distribution of the scale scores of the Span- ish-American and Anglo-American groups on Scale II (universalism-par ticularism) it was found that the chi-scuare value was significant at the .001 level. See Table 11. Hence, the null hypothesis was rejected and the conclusion was drann that the distributions of the scale scores of Spanish-Americans, Anglo-Americans and Mexicans do differ significantly. It may be inferred, then, that the factor of ethnic statis is associated with the distributions of scale scores on Scale II, i.e., with the degree ‘I t. C: o n "c ..r... 1.1...“ of universalistic-type role-expectations exhibited. The mean ranks were calculated, and it was found that the Spanish-Americans had a rank of 363, the Anglo-Americans a rank of 321, and the Mexicans a rank of 279. Thus, again, the mean rank of the Spanish-Americans was found to be considerably higher than that of the Anglo-Americans and of course, much higher than that of the Mexican group. This finding, then, does not conform to the notion that the Spanish—Americans stand in an in- termediate position between the Mexican and Anglo-American groups owing to a condition of partial assimilation. The significance of this find- ing will be discussed below. Attention was turned next to the distributions of scale scores of the Spanish—Americans and Anglo-Americans on Scale III. (self-orien- tation versus collectivity-orientation toward the family). It was found that the chi-square value of Table 12 is signifiCant at the .001 level. Hence, as in the preceding cases, the null hypothesis was rejected and the conclusion was drawn that the distributions of scale scores of the Spanish-American, Anglo-American, and MexiCan student groups do differ significantly. It may be inferred, then, that the factor of ethnicity is associated with the distribution of scale scores on Scale III, i.e., with the extent of collectivity-orientation toward the family. However, the calculation of the mean ranks for each group showed only a negli- gible rank order difference between the Spanish-American and Anglo- American groups. While the Spanish—Americans ranked higher in terms of collectivity-orientation toward the family than did the Anglo-Amer- icans, the rank order difference was very small. The mean rank of the -107 .. TABLE.12 DISTRIBUTION BY EBB-TIC GROUPS OF SCALE TIRES DEIIVED FRO-L: RESPONSES TO ROLE-CONFLI or SITUATILNS IRVOLVIRG smF-ORIEIITATICN VflLSUS COLLECTIVITY-OI‘GEJTI‘ITICN TO‘.';1RD TRS mum (SCALE III) Ethnic Scale TYPG emu" I a II III IV V N Mexican 11 121 151 58 341 Anglo-American 22 54 24 7 107 Spanish-American 18 54 30 3 105 N 51 229 205 68 553 Chi-square = 75.7 P less than .001, d.f. I 9 TABLEIIB DISTRIBUTION BY Emma GROUPS OF SCALE TYPES DERIVED FROI-s’: RmBOIISRS To HOLE-CONFLICT SITUATILITS II-TVOLVII-TG SilF—OPJEI‘TI‘ATICN VERSUS COLLECTIVI’PI-OMRITATICTI TOWARD TILE PIER GROUP (SCALE IV) Ethnic Scale Type 6”” I II III IV V N Mexican 59 100 112 78 3'7 386 Anglo-Ameri can 3 17 28 3O 29 107 Spanish-American 14 22 30 22 17 105 N 76 139 170 130 83 598 Chi-square = 36.8 P less than .001, d.f. 3 l2 J 6)- ‘Js 5“ W“ a, ... u A. I. v. u s R! 0 ‘J. a 9‘9: ‘c‘u. ..‘vw ~-.. ‘\) . o “4' l‘. rc, -lO8- Anglo-Americans was 208, that of the Spanish-Americans was 214, and that of the Nexicans was 317. Although the difference is small, it is worth noting that the Spanish-Americans ranked closer to the family-orienta- tion and of the scale than did the Anglo-Americans. The Mexican group, of course, ranked considerably further toward the family-orientation end of the scale than either of the other two groups. This finding does conform to the notion that the Spanish-American group ndght occupy an intermediate position between the Mexicans and Anglo-Americans. How- ever, the rank differences between each of the groups on Scale III appear to be exceedingly small. Hence, it is believed that no defini- tive significance may be attached to this finding. A.more detailed analysis of this scale will be presented in a.subsequent chapter. The distributions of the scale scores of the Spanish‘American, Anglo-American and Mexican students on Scale IV (selfeorientation versus collectivity-orientation toward the peer group) are shown in Table 13. The chi—square value of these districutions was found to be significant at the .001 level and, hence, the null hypothesis was rejected. The conclusion was drawn that the distributions of the scale scores of the three groups do differ significantly. Hence, it was inferred that the factor of ethnic status is associated with the distributions of scale scores on Scale IV. The ranking prOCedure showed that the mean ranks of the three groups on this scale were as follows: Anglo-American group highest with a mean rank of 880, Mexican group lowest with a mean rank of 275, and Spanish-American group about midrway between the others with a.mean rank of 306. It is to be noted that this finding -109- an .83 8e Sofia: 2m «83 «mm :88... 8m .3223 a nasal an nasal 8m sass! .324 .324 .39: .39: 8m .8225 SN Sons-q «on nasal .3 n8?! . .83...» Same £3.53 ...-«33 once» once» spoon 0.38 3 93....» use .33... 3 .33.» and 335 I... . 35am. an... Sass 5:. cg s..- can»: fi .3 a .18 n .18 a .18 gm gab as." an. ho med“ 3 gm g a a: mafia DEE gym has? 3” 0.3.09 -110- does conform to the notion presented earlier, that the Spanish-American group might be expected to stand in an intermediate position between the Mexican and Anglo-American student groups. Table 14 provides a summary statement of the findings discussed above. An interpretation of these findings will be presented in the summary to this chapter. Ethnic Status Within the Spanish—American Group The nature of the findings discussed above indicated the need for further examination of the Spani sh-American group. The previous analy- 318 was predicated on the notion that the Spanish-Americans might be treated as an ethnic unity standing in an intermediate acculturative Position between the Mexicans and Anglo-Americans. That analysis, how- ever, showed that no such simple view of the ethnic status of the Span- ish- Americans may be entertained with respect to the patterns of role- expec tations exhibited by this group. These students, while sharing cert ain ethnic traits in common such as Spanish name, residence in a Spani sh-American neighborhood, association with Spani sh-American peers, and descent from Mexican parentage, do not share a uniform ethnic back- g”0‘34:ch in all respects. Thus, the notion was suggested that differ- ences in the ethnic status or acculturative backgrounds of the Spanish- AmeI‘it'san students might be related to the patterns of role-expectations w113-311 they exhibit. Given this notion, it followed that some means of sub‘dividing this group according to ethnic status or acculturative bee kgl‘ounds was called for. In searching for criteria which might reflect different ethnic -1ll- statuses or acculturative backgrounds within the Spani sh-American group three traits were deemed of considerable importance. These were (1) length of residence in the United States, (2) mother's place of birth, and (3) language spoken to parents. A discussion of the reasons favor— ing selection of these traits as criteria reflecting different ethnic status backgrounds will be presented below. Given these three traits, however, a typolog may be constructed. The chief interest in con— structing such a typology was to provide the study with a rough scale of ethnicity. Thus, what was needed was a series of ethnic status types which reflect potential assimilation into the ways of the dominant Anglo- American society. The combination of these traits into one of the polar tJ'Pes should, then, represent a minimim of potential assimilation, and the other combinations of traits should form a series of types progress- ing toward higher levels of potential assimilation. The first trait selected for construction of the typology was that °f length of residence in the United States. This was deemed an impor- tant differentiating trait with respect to ethnicity in that the Span- ish_ American group included students who themselves were first genera- tion immigrants from Mexico, having immigrated to the United States Within a period of relatively few years prior to the time that the study was conducted. Others were second generation immigrants, having been born in the United States of immigrant parents. Finally, some of the Btu~<1ents were descended from families which had lived in the United S . tat es for several generations. Upon examination of the responses it w as found that a natural breaking point occurred between those of "less 4.12... than eight years residence in the United States" and those of "rmre than eight years residence in the United States." The second trait selected for construction of the typology was that of the place of birth of the respondent's mother, i.e., Mexico or the United States. Mother's place of birth was selected over that of father because of the relatively numerous responses indiCating that the father's place of birth was unknown. This Category, then, was dichotomi zed into the classes of "mother born in the United States", and "nether born in Mexico”. Mother's place of birth was considered an important trait to include in the construction of the typology because of the potential effect which this might have upon the home life of the child. Were the Parent born in Mexico it reasonably might be expected that there would be a stronger Mexican milieu in the home than if the parent were born in the United States. The third trait selected was that of the language which the student Speaks to his parents. This trait was trichotomized into the categories "Engli sh spoken to parents", "Spanish spoken to parents", and "both English and Spanish spoken to parents“. This trait was deemed import- ant to include in the typologz for several reasons. Language habits in themselves reflect ethnicity. By definition one's social relation pat- terns cannot be fully assimilated to the dominant Anglo-American ways Of this Society if Spanish is spoken in the home. Moreover. language 18 not merely a means of communication but is also formulative of attitudes, e . specially attitudes about social relationships. The combinations of these traits will produce a total of twelve types, hit only five of the possible combinations are relevant to the Spanish-American students included in the study. The Spanish-American group was first dichotomi zed into the cate- gories “lived less than eight years in the United States" and "lived eight years or more in the United States". Then each of these two cate— gories was dichotomised into the categories ”mother born in Mexico" and ”mother born in United States". Finally, each of these latter cate- gories was trichotomi zed into the Categories "Spanish spoken to parents", "English spoken to parents", and "both Spanish and English spoken to and also indi- Parents." Table 15 shows these combinations of traits, Cates which ones were selected as scale types for analysis. Ethnic status type T1 was conceived as representing the minimum of potential £188flJIflJlation into the dominant Anglo-American patterns of social rela- tions , and ethnic status type T7 was conceived as representing the highest potential of assimilation. Table 15 TYPOIOGY OF ETHNIC TRAITS 5“ ength Of Less than 8 years 8 years or more res1(1ence in Unit ed States \ Mo the r 's place Mexico U. S. Mexico U. S. O f 131 1. th Language spoken S"I E" B“ S E B S E B number T1 T3 T4 T3 T5 T7 T6 Spanish, E z English, B .-. Both English and Spanish t“! Al -llZi- Once having constructed this typology an analysis was undertaken to determine the extent of association between the distributions of scale scores of the students on each of the pattern variable scales and the different ethnic types. Table 16 shows the distributions of scale scores of the Spanish-American students by ethnic types on Scale I. A null hypothesis was posed that the distributions of the scale scores of the Spanish-Marleen students by ethnic types do not differ This hypothesis was tested by a chi-square test of from one another. Hence, the Significance, and the chi-square value was not significant. null hypothesis was accepted, and the inference was drawn that these ethnic type differences are not related to the students' role-expec- tation patterns. Since the null hypothesis was accepted, it was not Warranted to calculate the mean rank order of the ethnic types on Scale I. A null hypothesis was posed with respect to Scale II, and submitted to a. chi-square test of significance. The chi-square value of these distributions on Scale II was not significant, hence the null hypothe- sis ‘was not, rejected. It was inferred that the ethnic types are not relat ed to the role expectation patterns exhibited by the students on Scale II. See Table 17. A null hypothesis was also posed with respect to Scale IV and tested by a chi-square test of significance. The chi-sauare value of t he distributions on Scale IV was also considered not significant, and s o 11'- was inferred that the ethnic types are not related to the patterns 0 f I‘ole expectations exhibited by the students in this dimension. See Table 18. -115... TABLE 16 DISTRIBUTICI-T BY any: STATUS TYPES or SCALE TYPES 1331117211) F301;? 3.1903333 To 120L2L-CC-ITFLI or s TUiz‘Icr's IIJVCL’lI-YG A}? ECOTICIIIC RC'LE Viz-.393 A rennin. HOLE (SCALE I) S 1 T3 ~x—x n r 8 Ethnicity C“ e p Type I a II 8.: III IV a v N T2 8 8 16 T 10 O 20 4 1 T5 4 5 9 T6 l9 19 38 T7 11 7 18 N 52 49 101 Chi-square 3' .8 P less than .99, d.f. '-"- '7 * '. w o 1 EXplanation of ethnic types may be found in Table 15. The present table does not include ethnic trees '1‘ and T due to insufficient Jl l 3 numbers for chi-square analysis. H Pattern variables scale types I, II, and III have been collapsed in this table, as well as types IV and V. DISTRIBUTION BY ETXTIC STATUS TYPE TABLE.IS ~117— 3 CF 30.1.3 TYPES 1331111131) FRCIi 11331011333 TO 1101....“ CCETFLI CT SITE-“£16313 ITTVOLVITI-C} SELF-GRIPITI‘ATICN vxasss CCLLECTIVITI-CRIE.’TA’1‘ICTI Tcriaio "2:112: msILY (SCALE III) w Ethrgcity Scale Type,” Type I, II, III IV a- v N T2 10 7 17 T 4 9 10 19 T6 21 9 30 T7 16 4 20 N 56 3o 86 Chi-square - 13.2 P less than .02, d.f. ' 5 ** EXplanation of ethnic types may be found in Table 15. The preSent table does not include ethnic types T1, T3 and TS due to insufficient numbers for chi-square analysis. Pattern variables scale types I, II and III have been collapsed in this table, as well as types IV and V. ~118- The distributions of the scale scores of the Spanish-American students by ethnic types on Scale III (selfeorientation versus col- lectivity-orientation toward family) appeared to be the only scale on which these ethnic types were associated with types of role-expects, tions exhibited by the students. A.nu11 hypothesis was posed, and a chi—square test of significance was applied. The chi—square value was 13.2 and is significant at the .02 level. Hence, the null hypothe- sis was rejected. It was inferred that the distributions of the scale scores of the Spanish-Americans do differ according to ethnic types. Since the distributions of the scale scores may be considered differ- ent, the mean ranks were calculated. Table 19 (Scale III) consists of four ethnic traits rather than seven, for it was necessary to leave out of the table Ethnic Types 1, 3 and 5 because they included insufe ficient numbers for the calculation of a chi-sonare test. It was found that Ethnic Type 4 ranked highest on collectivity-orientation toward the family. with an average rank of 50.6, Ethnic Type 2 ranked second highest with a rank of 45.7, Ethnic Type 6 ranked third highest with a.rank of 40.9, and Ethnic Type 7 ranked lowest with an average rank of 36.6. Now, although the findings in the case of Scale III above may not be dismissed, their interpretation is not readily accomplished. That Ethnic Type 4 should rank higher than Ethnic Type 2 on collectiv» ity orientation toward the family is an unanticipated finding. The construction of the types was based on the premise that certain com- binations of traits would render ethnic scale types which differed ~119— TADLE l9 DISTRIBUI‘ICIT BY LT"; TIC ST ATUS TYPIJIS CF 5 T“ TO ROLES-n 40:. TWIST SI! L’ATI;: S IIYVCL II: 33.. -CI" 21. :jnTILF \ Vuluus cc LLECTIVIr -CRI'UI.- TIJ To”. ’ Ins Pddfi GRC”.I (SCAL:.IV) ‘ t ___.—1 h __ m ’ Ethnicity Scale TYP9** Tipe* I & II III IV & V N T2 8 3 6 17 T4 11 5 3 19 T5 2 4 5 11 T6 9 8 13 30 T7 6 6 8 20 N 36 26 35 97 Chi-square = 8.8 P less than .80, d.f. 3 l2 Ekplanation of ethnic types maybe found in Table 15 The present t ble does not include et .ni-c types T1 and T3 due to insufficient numbers for chi-square analysis. Pattern variables scale types I and II have been collapsed in this table, as well as types IV and V. -120... from one another in terms of degrees of ethnicity. Ethnic Type 4, of the typology should represent a lesser degree of Spanish-American ethni- city than Ethnic Type 2, and yet Ethnic Type 4 ranks higher on collect- ivity-orientation toward the family. This is surprising because col- lectivity-orientation toward the family appears to be a differentiating characteristic of the Spanish-American group as compared with the Anglo- AmeriCan group. With this exception, however, the average rank scores of the four Ethnic Type groups do appear to follow the pattern of "the greater the degree of ethnicity, the greater the degree of collectivity- orientation toward the family." It is to be noted, of course, that on only one of the feur pattern variables scales does there appear an association between ethnic status types and types of role-expectations which the students exhibited. Al- though in the case of Scale III an association between ethnic status types and role-expectations types was found, this single finding may scarcely be considered to imply that there exists a.strong and pervasive relation- ship between the ethnic status background.of the Spanish-American stu- dents and the types of social role—expectations which they exhibited.1 Summary The preceding analysis waS‘undertaken in an attempt to determine if ethnic status within the United States student group might be a factor 1 Each of the traits comprising the scale of ethnicity was examined separately to determine if they differentiated the patterns of role— expectations of the students. None of the traits taken alone appears to differentiate. ~121- related to the types of role-expectations exhibited by these students. The wide range of role—expectations types exhibited by the United States student group was not accounted for adeouately by the general implica- tions of the theory examined in the previous chapter. Hence, it was suggested that factors sub—sumed under the theoretiCal notion pertaining to the relationship between types of role-expectations and the extent of differentiation and segregation of social relations in a social system must account for the wide range of role-expectations types ex- hibited. It was then suggested that the ethnically heterogeneous cone position of the United States student group might account for the wide range of role-expectations types exhibited by this group. Given a.know- ledge that Spanish-Americans retain in some measure social and cultural traits of Mexico, the notion was presented that such a socio-cultural affinity also might be reflected in the role—expectations types exhibited by the Spanish-American students. Thus, it was reasoned that the role~ expectations types exhibited by the Spanish-American student group might fall in an intermediate position between those of the Mexican and Anglo- American groups. Were this the case then the wide range of role-expec- tations types exhibited by the total United States group would have been accounted fbr sore adequately within the framework of the theory. The analysis showed, however, that only in the case of Scale IV (collectivity-orientation toward the peer group) did the Spanish-Amer- ican group definitely rank in an intermediate position between the Mexican and Anglo-American groups. See Table 14 for a summary of this finding and those referred to immediately below. In the case of Scale -l?2- III (collectivity-orientation toward the family) the Spanish—American group did rank in an intermediate position between the other two groups, but the differences between the ranks of the three groups were so small that it seemed.unwarranted to attach any definitive significance to the findings. 0n the other hand, in the case of Scale I (universalistic orientation toward brother) and Scale II (universalistic orientation toward friend) the Spanish-American group did not rank in an intermedi- ate position between the other two groups. On the contrary, the Span- ish-American group ranked higher on each of these two sCales than either the Mexican or Anglo-American groups. Moreover, the Spanish-American group ranked at the opposite end of these scales from the Mexican group. Two principal conclusions may be drawn from these findings. First,. they show that the Spanish-American.group did exhibit a.pattern of role- expectations different from those of both the Anglo-AmeriCan and Mexican groups on each of the pattern variables scales. Thus, it seems warranted to infer that ethnic status within the United States student group is a factor related to the types of role-expectations exhibited by these stu- dents. Second, it may be inferred that the Spanish-American group does not stand in any simple intermediate acculturative position between the Mexican and Anglo-American groups with respect to the dimensions of role-expectations examined by this study. These findings, then, clearly bear upon the notion that ethnic status within the United States student group may be a factor accounting for the wide range of role-expectations exhibited by this group. The finding that the Spanish-American group's scale score distributions -123- differed from.and ranked considerably higher than the Anglo-American group's on Scales I and II indicates that the wide range of role«expec- tations types exhibited by the total United States group is accounted for in some measure by the "spreading" effect of the Spanish-American group. While it is curious that the Spanish—American group should tend to spread the scale score distributions of the total United States group toward the universalistic end of these two scales, this is not a con- sideration bearing on the present oueetion. This matter will be examined later. The spreading effect of the Spanish-American group is again ap- parent in Seales III and IV, although it appears to have little effect in the former. This chapter also examined the relationship between social role- expectations and certain ethnic differences within the Spanish-American) group. The purpose here was to determine if Certain combinations of ethnic traits manifested among these students might be associated with the patterns of role—expectations which they exhibited on the pattern variables scales. It was hoped that by sub-dividing the Spanish-Ameri- can group into several ethnic status types a more sensitive index of ethnicity might be achieved and, thus, a more thorough analysis of the relationship between ethnicity and.patterns of role-expectations might be carried out. In conclusion, then, the findings of the chapter may be interpreted as indicated that ethnic status differences within the United States student group do constitute a differentiating factor with respect to the patterns of role-expectations exhibited on the pattern variables -121 Jr- scales. The Spanish-American group exhibited a distribution of scale scores on all of the pattern variables scales which was significantly different from those of both the Anglo-American and Mexican groups. The findings, however, do not conform to the notion that the Spanish- American group stands in an intermediate acculturative position between the Mexicans and Anglo-Americans. On the contrary, the Spanish-Ameri- can students ranked higher than the Anglo—Amaricans on both Scale I (universalistic orientation toward brother) and Scale II (universalis- tic orientation toward friend). Moreover, an examination of selected features of the ethnic status backgrounds of the Spanish—American students did not reveal a pervasive relationship between these ethnic differences and the types of role-expectations exhibited by the students. While the analysis indicates that ethnic status within the United States group is related to the patterns of role-expectations exhibited by the American students, the finding that the Spanish-Americans rank higher than the Anglo-Amaricans on two pattern variables scales was un“'Satziticipated‘and remains unexplained by the data of this chapter. 3“flirthemore, the precise manner in which the factor of ethnic status may be sub—sumed under the general notion pertaining to the relation- ship between social role-expectations and the extent of differentiation °f social relations in a social system remains to be explored. -125- CHAPTER VI A FURTHER EXAMINATION OF THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN ROLE-EXPECTATIONS AND ETHNIC STATUS IN THE UNITED STATES srmmm GROUP This chapter will present a further examination of the relation- ship between social role-expectations and ethnic status in the United States student group. First, a summary discussion will be presented of several views pertaining to the ethnic composition of the Spanish- Americans in the United States. A discussion will follow this, out- lining a conception that ethnic status differences within the Spanish- American group may be reflected by the variable of language spoken to peers. These notions, in turn, will lead to the presentation of fur- ther analysis of the role-expectations patterns exhibited by the Span- 1 ell-Americans on each of the four pattern variables scales. Finally, a mrther test of the association between ethnicity and social role-ex- PSCtations will be presented. This will consist of a comparison of the r0 le-expectations patterns of the different Spanish-American sub-groups Wi th those of the Anglo-Americans and Mexicans. A summary of the find-— ings will be presented at the end of the chapter. The previous chapter undertook to determine if the wide range of r0 le—expectations types exhibited by the United States student group might be accounted for in some measure by a "spreading" effect intro- duced by the Spanish-American students. It was inferred from the analysis that the role-expectations patterns exhibited by the Spanish- -126- American students did appear to have such an effect. Hence, it was interpreted that ethnic differences within the United States student group constitute a class of factors related to the types of social role-expectations exhibited by these students. Put in a more generic form, ethnic status differences within a social system characterized by a high degree of differentiation and segregation of social relations may be seen to constitute a class of factors related to the patterns of role-expectations exhibited by the members of such a system. However, it was noted that, while ethnic status appears to be related to the patterns of role—expectations exhibited by the students, the notion that the Spanish-American group stands in an intermediate accultura- tive position between the Anglo-Americans and Mexicans is untenable. Hence, a further examination of the Spani sh-American group is called for, and a specification of ethnic status differences within this group is needed if the role-expectations patterns which these students eJChibited are to be comprehended more adequately by the social system tiheory. It is to this problem that the study now turns. From the evidence available in the literature, it seems warranted to assume that not all Spanish-name persons are equally integrated into the life-ways of what is loosely referred to as the Spanish-American et1:).nic enclave in the Southwest of the United States. Likewise, it may be assumed that not all Spanish-name persons equally identify with the patterns of social relations which characterize this ethnic complex. NIlrtierous studies support these views. -127- Simmons'1 work with Mexican-Americans in South Texas shows that this ethnic group exhibits wide variation in social relations patterns. He identifies what may be considered a traditional core of the ethnic group as well as a sector which exhibits a high degree of acceptance of the dominant Anglo-America patterns. In addition, however, there may be identified a sector of the Spanish-American group which exhibits ” deviant patterns“ of social relations, i.e., patterns not fully con- gruent with either those of the traditional core or those of the domi- nant Anglo-Amaricans. Broom and Shevlry2 also have examined the problem of social differentiation as it involves Spanish-name persons in the United StateS, and their findings also indicate that the Spanish-Amer- ican ethnic group may be sub-divided into at least the three Categories mentioned above. Jones3 indicates some of the problems faced by the public schools in handling social relations involving Spani sh-American students, and it may be interpreted that different degrees or stages of adaptation to the school pattern are exhibited by these students according to the extent to which they are integrated into the traditional ethnic patterns or the dominant Anglo-American patterns of social relations. \ 0 zzie Simmons, Anglo-Americans and Mexican-Americans i_p_ South Texas, é Study i_1_1_ _I‘pminant-Subordinate Gregg Relations. Unpublished PhD Dissertation, Harvard University, 1952. 2 I-eonard Broom and Eshref Shevky, Mexicans in the United States: A I”Irroblem in Social Differentiation, $ciolggy and. Social Research, ‘72>1. 36, January, 1952, pp. 150-158. 3 Robert Jones, Mexican Youth in the U.S., The American Teacher, Vol. 28, March, 1944, pp. 11-15. -L’28- Tuck,4 Watson and Samara,5 Humphreys,6 and Senter7 each, in treat- ing the particular problem with which his paper was concerned, cast some light on different features of the composition of the Spanish-America or Mexican-American ethnic enclave in the United States. A central im- plication of what these writers have said is that the Spanish-American ethnic group may not be thought of as a unitary social entity, but rather that it is composed of persons differentiated from one another according to the extent of their integration into the traditional ethnic iliufe—ways or into the donunant Anglo-American patterns of social relations. Language Spoken to Peers as an Index of Ethnic Status Differences among the Spanish-AInericans Considering the views cited above, the Spanish-Americans may not be thought of as an ethnic unity. They may be divided into at least three major sub-groups according to the extent of their integration into the dominant Anglo-American society. These sub-groups may be Conceived as sectors of the very broad Spanish-American ethnic class. W1 thin this class, then, there Can be identified "traditional", 4 Ruth Tuck, Not With the First: l:!exican-Ameri¢ans i___ a Southwest City. New York: Harcourt Brace and Co., 1949. James B. Watson and Julian Samora, Subordinate Leadership in 8. Bi- cultural Community: An Analysis, American Sociological Review, Vol. 19, No. 4, August, 1954, pp. 413-421. 6 Norman Humphrey, The Cultural Background of the Mexican Migrant, Rural Sociology, Vol. 13, 1948, pp. 289-246. Ibnovan Senter, Acculturation Among New Mexican Villagers in Compari- son to Adjustment Patterns of other Spanish—speaking Americans, Rural Sociology, V51. 10, 1945, pp. 810-847. 4:29.. "assiudlatedfl, and "deviant" sectors. Given these notions pertaining tc> the ethnic differentiation of the Spanish-Americans it was deemed pnertinent to determine whether ethnic status differences of this order might be related to the patterns of role-expectations exhibited by the students. While the combinations of traits composing the ethnic status typology presented in the previous chapter included the variable of Ilaunguage spoken to parents, it did not include that of language spoken to peers. It was suggested, then, that the language spoken to peers ‘by' the Spanish-American students might reflect ethnic status differen- Cees unre adequately than did the previously discussed typology. more- o‘rer; considering the notions cited above with respect to the ethnic Biiatus differences among Spanish-Americans the categories of this vauriable are particularly appropriate. This variable may be sub- dinvided.into the categories of "Spanish spoken to peers", "English SEMiken to peers", and "both English and Spanish spoken to peers". Tknis, the notion that the ethnic status differences among Spanish- Anuericans which were discussed above may be reflected by the variable Of' language spoken to peers is immediately suggested. In considering the Variable of language spoken to peers as an in- dex of ethnicity, it was posited that the trait of speaking Spanish to Peers implies integration or identification with the traditional ethnic Ccnnplex of social relations. On the other hand, it was posited that 8Peaking English to peers implies integration or identification with the dominant Anglo-American patterns of social relations. This English- -130- speaking group, then, may be thought of as an assimilated sector of the general Spanish-American ethnic class. Finally, it was posited that the trait of speaking both English and Spanish to peers implies integration into or identification with a deviant pattern of social relations. It was reasoned that if these notionswith respect to the relationship be- tween language spoken to peers and ethnic status differences are correct, then the variable of language spoken to peers should differentiate the role-expectations patterns exhibited by the Spanish-American students. Operationally, this means that the scale score distributions of the Spanish-speaking, English—speaking, and bilingual sub-groups will differ SignifiCantly from one another on the pattern variables scales. Fur- thermore, the mean rank order of these language sub—groups will conform to the rank order patterns exhibited in the previous analyses of the Anglo-Merican group and the total Spanish-American group. The speci- fication of the expected mean rank order of these language sub-groups ‘ on (eaCh of the pattern variables scales will be made clear in the dis- cus sion below. Specification of the Relationship Between Ethnic Group Integration or Identification and Types of Role-Expectations It will be recalled that the distributions of the total Spanish- American group and those of the Anglo- Americans differed significantly on all four of the pattern-variable scales. See Tables 10, ll, 12, 13. The Spanish—Amarican group ranked higher on universalism than the Anglo-Americans on both Scales I and II. The Spani sh-Amarican group 8180 ranked higher than the Anglo-Americans on collectivity orientation -13 1. toward the family (Scale III), although the mean rank difference between them was not large. Finally, the Spanish-Americans ranked considerably lower than the Anglo-Americans on collectivity-orientation toward the peer group (Scale IV). That the Anglo-Americans had a mean rank lower than that of the total Spanish-American group on Scales I, II, and III, and higher on Scale IV provides a baseline for a further comparison. These findings were interpreted as implying that (l) the Anglo-Americans are less uni- versalistically oriented on the average than the Spanish-American group, and (2) that the Anglo-Americans have a lower incidence of collectivity- orientation toward the family, and (3) that the Anglo-Americans have a. higher incidence of orientation toward the peer group than the Spanish- Americans. The Spanish-speaking students were conceived as constituting the Seetor of the total Spanish-America group which is most traditional and. least integrated into the patterns of social relations of the domi- nant Anglo-American society. It follows, then, that of the three sub- groups differentiated according to language spoken to peers the Spanish- Speaking group should have the lower mean rank on the two scales of Universalism (Scales I and II). Also, it would be expected that the English-speaking group would rank higher on universalism than the Span- i 311‘ speaking sub-group. But if the trait of speaking English to peers (1°98 reflect a relatively high state of integration into the dominant An'glo--American patterns of social relations, this sub—group should not he.“ an absolutely high rank on universalism on Scales I and II. This English-speaking sub-group would not be expected to have an absolutely high rank on the scales of universalism because it is posited as being integrated into or identified with the Anglo-American norms, and the Anglo—AmeriCan group itself exhibits a middle rank between the Mexican group and the total Spani sh-American group. Finally, it was difficult to posit where the group which speaks p-74 both English and Spanish to peers might rank relative to the other two Spanish-American sub—groups. The difficulty arises directly from our conception that this trait represents ingegration or identification with patterns of social relations which are deviant from those of both the tradition Spanish-Americans and the dominant Anglo-Mericans. There appeared only one sound reason for positing that this group should rank higher or lower than either the Spani sh-speaking or English-speaking sub- groups. It might be conceived that this group is in process of transi- tion from a state of integration or identification with traditional Spanish-American social patterns to a state of integration into those 0f the dominant Arnie-American group. This notion will be explored in the following analysis. In addition to the above idea, however, there was suggested another Cone eption of the significance of the trait of speaking both English and Spanish to peers. This notion is that such a trait may represent inte- grat ion into or identification with an emergent pattern of role relations. Thus , rather than conceiving that bilingualism with peers represents a 8 tat e of transition, it may be viewed as representing the emergence of a 6‘1 3t inct pattern of role—orientations within the context of the ethnically ~133- sub-divided community. This notion, then, will be examined in the fol- lowing analysis also. There were suggested certain expectations with respect to the dif- ferences between the three Spani sh-American sub—groups on the scale of collectivity-orientation toward the family (Scale III), and collectivity- orientation toward the peer group (Scale IV). Ibllowing the ideas al- ready outlined, it would. be expected that the group which speaks Spanish to peers would rank relatively high on collectivity-orientation toward the family. The English-speaking group should rank. relatively low on this scale, considerably lower than the Spanish-speaking group. Again, however, in the case of the group which speaks both English and Spanish to peers it was not possible to posit directionality. 0n Scale IV (collactivity-orientation toward peers) it would be eXpected that the English-speaking group would rank relatively high and the Spani sh-speaking group relatively lower. Finally, as was the case in the other three scales, the position of the group which speaks both English and Spanish to peers could not be posited. Analysis of the Data The analysis of the distributions of the scale scores of the Span- ish‘ American group, sub-divided according to language spoken to peers, may :now be presented. With resoect to Scale I, a null hypothesis was p0 Sed that the distributions of the scale scores of the Elglish-speak- ing. Spanish-speaking, and bilingual sub-groups do not differ signifi- c ant 1y from one another. This hypothesis was submitted to a chi-souare -131,— test of signi ficance and it was found. that the chi-souare value was sig- nificant at the .05 level. See Table 20. Hence, the null hypothesis was rejected and it was inferred that the factor of language spoken to peers is related to the degree of universalistic-type role-expectations exhibited by the students. A ranking procedure was applied to these scale score distributions, and it was found that the Spanish-speaking group ranked lowest on universalism with a. mean rank of 57.0, the Eng— lish—speaking group ranked slightly higher with a mean rank of 59.6, and the bi—lingual group ranked highest with a mean rank of 64.8. It may be concluded, then, that these findings support our expectations with respect to the distributions of the scale scores of the three sub- groups on Scale I. Our expectations with respect to the rank positions of the Spani sh-speaking and English-speaking sub—groups are also supported by these data. The finding that the bi-lingual sub-group exhibited a distribution different from either of the other two sub—groups on Scale I is in accord "'1 th the conception that this trait represents involvement in or iden- tification with a deviant pattern of social relations. The bi-lingual Sub—group exhibited a, higher degree of universalistic—type orientation than either the English—speaking or Spani sh-speaking groups. The notion that the trait of speaking both English and Spanish to peers might imply a State of transition from identification with the traditional Spanish- AInezfican sub-group _to a state of identification with the Anglo-Mnerican group 13 not supported very clearly by this finding. Given the concep- tion of this trait as representing a transition state, then a new i" \‘f -13 5... SPANISH ATERICg-JTS BY L:£"GUnGE SPCI‘ZET TO P7212233, DISTRIBUTION OF SCALE ‘1: RESPOI‘TSES TO RCLE-CGl-TFLICT SIT ’aTIClI‘S IITVOL‘ 7'33 c gal 1101.3 VEiSTJS A FALILIAL ROLE (SCALE 1) Laneuage . . Spoien to Scale Type Peers I a II III Iv v It mgiish 12 12 13 7 41. Spanish 6 ll 2 5 24 Both 17 8 13 16 54 N 35 31 28 28 122 Chi-square 3 18.2 P less than .05, d.f. = 9 TABLE 21 SPAIIISZi-ICJERICA‘TS EY LMTGUAGE 390m: TO PEI'TiS, DISTRIBUTION CF SCALE TYPES DERIVED F3013 Rsssozrsns TO ROLE—003mm? SITUATIGI‘IS IIWCL :13 AN Il-ISI‘RULZETTM. ROLE vats-cs A Eastman? ROLE SCALE II) Language T r Spoken to Scale 3pe Peers I a II III Iv v N nigiish 10 11 7 15 43 Spanish 6 5 5 8 24 Both 4 9 19 22 54 N 20 25 31 45 121 Chi-square = 9.3 P less than 50, d.f. = 9 question is raised as to why this sub-group should rank even higher than the English-speaking group with respect to universalistic~type role-ex- pectations. A.possible explanation of the exceptionally high incidence of universalistic~type role—expectations among this sub-group might in- volve the further notion of "over generalization". Thus, if this group were in a.state of transition, a tendency to over generalize the atti- tudes of the group into which it was trying to move or with which it was attempting to identify might explain its higher incidence of uni- versalistic-type role—expectations. This is clearly a.notion of the order of an ex post facto explanation. However, a tentative corroborar tion of this notion's appropriateness might be obtained by its system- atic application to the subsequent findings. With respect to the notion that the trait of speaking both English and Spanish to peers may represent involvement in an emergent pattern of social relations the above finding is somewhat more conclusive. First, the finding indicates that the bilingual sub-group does not fall in an intermediate position between the Spanish—speaking and English-speaking sub-groups, but ranks higher than either of them. This indicates a distinctly different pattern of role-expectations for the bilingual sub-group. Second, this notion is favored over the "tran- sitional" idea above in that no additional explanatory process need be invoked. In the case of Scale II, a null hypothesis was posed that the dis- tributions of the scale scores of the English-speaking, Spanish-speak- ing, and bilingual sub-groups do not differ significantly from one ~137— another. This was submitted to a chi-square test of signifiCance, and it was found that the chi-scuare value could not be considered signifi- cant. See Table 21. Hence, the null hypothesis may not be rejected. It was concluded, then, that language spoken to peers does not consti- tute an important differentiating factor with respect to the extent of universalistic-type role-expectations exhibited toward friends. Further- more, it was concluded that these data do not support our expectations with respect to the distributions of the scale scores of the three sub— groups on Scale II. In the case of Scale III, a null hypothesis also was posed that the distributions of the scale scores of the English-speaking, Spanish- speaking, and bilingual sub-groups would not differ significantly from one another. This was submitted to a chi-sousre test of significance, and it was fbund that the chi-sonare value was significant at the .10 level. See Table 22. Although this is not a high degree of probabi- lity, it was considered high enough to warrant rejection of the null hypothesis. Hence, it was concluded that the factor of language spoken to peers is related to the extent of collectivity-orientation toward the family. A.ranking procedure was applied to these soale score dis- tributions, and it was found that the Spanish-speaking sub—group ranked highest on collectivity-orientation toward the family with a.mean rank of 65.2. The sub-group Speaking both Spanish and English to peers ranked second on this scale with a mean rank of 60.1, while the English-speaking sub-group ranked lowest with a mean rank of 49.4. It was concluded, then, that these findings support our expectations with respect to the distri- -138- TABLE 22 swam-hammers BY LmIOUAOE SPOM? TO FEERS, DIS'l’RIBU'l‘IL-bl OF SCALE TYPES DERIVED FROM RESPONSES TO ROLE-COFFLICT SITUATIC‘N INVOLvIr-IG SELF-ORIEN- TATION VERSUS COLLECTIVITY-OPJEJTATION Iona-m THE FAEZELY (3023.3 III) -__— —_fi_ v Language . Spoken to Scale Type Peers I & III III Iv a v N English 8 27 8 43 Spanish 3 4 8 15 Both 7 28 20 55 N 18 59 36 113 Chi-square B 10.3 P less than .10, d.f. = 6 TABLE 23 SPANISH-ARERICANS BY LANGUAGE 5130}an TO PMS, DISTRIBUTION OF SCALE TYPES DERIVED FROM RESPONSES TO ROLE-COPELICT SITUATICI‘JS INVOLVII-IG ems-cams- TATION vmiSUs COLLECTIVITY-Ol’lffiTTATICN Tor: ‘FLD THE Pam GROUP (SCALE Iv) Language 7 Spoken to Scale T3pe Peers I II III Iv v N English 4 7 9 10 13 43 Spanish 2 2 7 2 2 15 Both 9 17 11. 10 5 55 N 15 26 30 22 20 113 Chi-square = 24.1 P less than .02, d.f. 3 12 ~13 9- butions of the scale scores of the three sub—groups on Scale III. It also was concluded that these data.support our expectations with re- spect to the rank positions of the Spanish-speaking and English-speak- ing sub-groups. The finding that the sub-group which speaks both English and Span- ish to peers exhibits a distribution of scale scores different from either of the other two sub-groups on Scale III is in accord with the conception of this trait as representing involvement or identification with a deviant pattern of social relations within the ethnically sub— divided community. It is to be noted further that this finding is in accord with the notion that bilingualism may represent a state of transition from.ac- ceptance of traditional Spanish-American social patterns to an accept- ance of the dominant Anglo-American patterns. But if consistency in the interpretation of these findings is to be maintained then the additional notion of over generalization ought to be applied here Just as was done earlier in the case of Scale I. However, it is clearly inappropriate to apply the notion of over generalization to the interpretation of the rank position of the bilingual sub-group on Scale III. Hence, it must be concluded that the idea.of over generali- zation introduces an unsystematic element into the analysis. It must either be invoked in all cases or in none. If it is invoked as an ex- planation of why the bilingual sub—group did not rank in an intermediate position on Scale I then it also must be invoked with respect to the position of this sub-group on Scale III. But the notion is clearly -lh0- superfluous as an explanation of the rank position of the bilingual subvgroup on Scale III. Thus, it would appear best to abandon the notion of over generalization. It then must be observed that the findings with respect to the bilingual sub—group on Scale I are not consistent with the notion that bilingualism implies involvement in a transitional process. The finding that the bilingual sub-group exhibits a.different pat- tern of role-expectations from the other two Spanish-American sub-groups and that it ranks in an intermediate position between the Ehgiish~speak+ ing and Spanish-speaking sub-groups is congruent with the notion that bilingualism represents involvement in an emergent pattern of social relations. A.null hypothesis was posed with respect to Scale IV that the distributions of the English-Speaking, Spanish-speaking, and bilingual sub-groups do not differ significantly from.one another. See Table 23. This was submitted to a chi-souare test of significance, and it was found that the chi-square value was significant at the .02 level. Hence, the null hypothesis was rejected, and it was inferred that the factor of language spoken to peers is associated with the extent of collectivity-orientation toward the peer group. The application of the ranking procedure showed that the English-speaking sub-group ranks highest on collectivity-orientation toward the peer group with a mean rank of 67.2. the Spanish-speaking sub—group ranks second with a mean rank of 55.6, and the bilingual sub-group ranks lowest with a mean rank of 48.1. ~11; 1.. It may be concluded, then, that these findings also support our expectations with respect to the distributions of the scale scores of the three sub-groups on Scale IV. It also may be concluded that these data support our expectations with respect to the rank positions of the Spani sh-speaking and Engli sh-soeaking sub-groups. Again, in the case of Scale IV, the finding that the bilingual sub—group exhibited a distribution of scale scores different from either of the other two sub-groups is in accord with the conception of biling— ualism as a trait representing involvement in or identificationnwith a deviant pattern of social relations within the ethnically sub—divided community. The finding that the bilingual sub-group ranked lower on Scale IV than either of the other two sub-groups clearly is not in accord with the notion that bilingualism may be a trait representing a transitional state between the Spanish-speaking and English-speaking sectors of the Spanish—American ethnic group. On the other hand, this finding is con- gruent with the notion that bilingualism may be a trait representing involvement in an emergent pattern of social relations wdthin the con- text of the ethnically differentiated community. Prior to completing the analyses scheduled for this chapter some tentative conclusions drawn from the preceding findings will be pre- sented. First, it may be concluded that the variable of language spoken to peers differentiates the Patterns of role-expectations exhibited by the Spanish-American students on three out of the four pattern variables scales. The role-expectations patterns of the Spanish-speaking, English- _yg_ speaking, and bilingual sub-groups were differentiated from one another on Scale I (universalistic-orientation toward brother), Scale III (col- lectivity—orientation toward family), and Scale IV (collectivityoorientae tion toward the Peer group). Second, the findings were congruent with the notion that the variable of language spoken to peers may represent integration or identification with differentiated complexes of social relations in the ethnically heterogeneous border setting. The relative mean ranks of the language sub—groups on Scales I, III, and IV supported this notion. The mean ranks of the Spanish-speaking and English-speaking sub-groups stood in relative positions to each other on these scales as the mean ranks of the total Spanish-American and Anglo-American groups stood to each other. The findings also were congruent with the notion that bilingualism with peers may constitute an index of involvement in a deviant pattern of social relations. The role-expectations patterns exhibited by the bilingual sub-group were significantly different from those of the Spanish-speaking and English-speaking sub-groups on Scales I, III, and IV. Two alternative views were proposed with respect to the condeption that bilingualism may represent involvement in a deviant pattern of social relations. The first of these conceived of the bilingual sub- group as involved in a transition from integration into traditional Spanish-AmeriCan patterns of social relations to a state of integration into those of the dominant Anglo-Americans. This view, however, was not consistent with the findings and, hence, was rejected. The alternative view conceived of bilingualism as representing integration or identifi- 41,3- cation with an emergent pattern of social relations. The findings proved to be consistent with this view. A Further Test of the Relationship between Role—expectations Patterns and Ethnic Status Differences among Spanish-Americans Proceeding with the conception that the variable of language spoken to peers may represent integration or identification with differentiated patterns of social relations which are, in turn, reflected in the role- expectations patterns of the Spanish-American students a further means of testing this notion was suggested by the data. The trait of speaking Spanish to peers was posited as representing integration or identifica- tion with the social relational patterns of what was referred to as the traditional sector of the Spanish-American ethnic group. On the other hand, the trait of speaking English to peers was posited as represent- ing integration into or identification with the social relational pat- terns of the dominant Anglo-American society. Conseouently, this trait was thought of as reflecting an assimilated status. Finally, the trait of speaking both English and Spanish to peers was posited as represent- ing integration into or identification with a deviant pattern of social relations; a pattern of social relations deviating from those of both the tradition Spanish-Americans and the dominant Anglo-Americans. The findings of the Preceding analysis proved consistent with these conceptions, but further proof of their appropriateness may be obtained by an additional series of analyses. The notion pertaining to the Span- ish-American students who speak English to peers will be examined first. -lhh- If the English-speaking sub—group were assimilated into the dominant Anglo-American social patterns, then it would exhibit a range of role- expectations similar to that of the Anglo-American students. This may be confirmed by an analysis of the scale score distribufions of these two groups on the pattern variables scales. If the English-speaking sub—group is integrated into the patterns of social relations of the Anglo—Americans, then the scale score distributions of the English- speaking sub—group and the Anglo-American group will not differ signi- ficantly fronxone another. The distributions of the scale scores of the English—speaking sub- group and the Anglo-American group on Scales I, II, III, and IV are shown in Tables 24, 25, 26, and 27, respectively. Null hypotheses were posed with respect to each of these scales that the distributions of these two groups do not differ significantly from one another. These hypotheses were tested by a chi-square test of significance, and in each case it was found that the chi-square value was too low to warrant rejection of the hypothesis. On Scale I (Table 24) the chi-square value was significant at the .95 level, on Scale II (Table 25) at the .80 level, on Scale III (Table 26) at the .50 level, and on Scale IV (Table 27) the chi square value was significant at the .80 level. It was evi— dent that these levels of significance did not warrant rejection of the null hypothesis. It was inferred, then, that the role-expectations pat- terns of the English-speaking sub-group do not differ from those of the Anglo-American group on the pattern variables scales. The analysis may now turn to the Spanish-American sub-group which In TABLE 24 ANGLo-ntnu c.5353 IND SPANI swan—u cars Spnmm OF SCALE TYPE-“.3 DERIVED mom RESPOIISES PEERS, DISTRIBUTICN -11; 5... El? GLI SH T0 T0 ROLE-COE~’2FLI CT SITUATI-.113 INVOLVING n: ECOI‘TOEJC ROLE vmsus A FAILIIL ROLE (SCALE 1) Scale Type Ethnic emu" I & II III Iv v N Anglo-American 34 28 31 11 104 Spanish-American 12 12 13 7 [.4 Speaking English to peers N’ 46 40 44. 18 148 Chi-square “-1 1.1 P less than .95, d.f. ‘5 5 TABLE 25 AI‘IQO-AILE‘BICANS AND SPMTISH-BETZICANS SPEAKING HIGLISH TO PEERS, DISTRIBUTICN 0F SCALE TYPES DERIVED FROM RESPONSES TO ROLE-COIIFLI CT SITUATICIIS IIIVCLVING AN INSTRULLENTAL ROLE mews A FPJRIDSEEIP ROLE (SCALE II) Ethnic 5 Group cale Type I & II III IV V N Anglo-AmeriCan 23 21 28 29 101 Spanish-American 10 ll 7 15 43 Speaking English to peers ' N 33 32 35 41. 11.4 Chi-square I 2.7 P less than .80, d.f.35 TABLE 26 IIGLG— .IGI s Ito SPI Is::-II.:E.IGIIISSPL.-u:1GnIGLIs:.Io PEERS DISTRIBUTION OF SGILE TYPES D .31 ED FRCIJ 12.339017 ens TO ROLE- cortILIGI SITUATC SI"VcLVI G SLIP-CRIB TATICIT vn.sus COLLSC‘ I‘IVITY -Ol’.I ETTATICIT T0‘ III) THE III II.Y(33ALE III) Scale 6 Ethnic hp Group I 8. II III IV a V N Anglo-American 22 54 31 107 Spanish-American 8 27 8 43 Speaking English to peers N 30 81 39 150 Chi-square ' 2.2 P less than .50, d.f. I 3 TABLE 27 ANGLO- AI’J .ICz‘JT-S. ' CPAI’IS‘i-wIJICIA’S SP'miKl'llG 21"GLI‘3H TO PMS, DISTRIBUTION or SCILE IIPLs DILLIVED FRCII 12.33901. ens TO ROLE- CONFLICT SITUATIONS III-VOLVI G SilF-CRIHITATION VEPSUS COLLECTIVITT-OPJEIJTATION IoaVIID THE Pam GROUP (em: Iv) Sc T Ethnic 81" ype Gm“? I II III IV V N Anglo-American 3 17 28 30 29 107 Spanish-American 4 7 9 10 13 43 speaking English to peers N 7 24 37 40 42 150 Chi-square ' 3.6 P less than .80, d.f. ' 7 -1”- speaks Spanish to peers. The conception that this sub—group is inte- grated.or identified with a traditional Spanish-American pattern of social relations was assessed in similar manner to that above. It was reasoned that if this were true, then the Spanish-speaking sub—group's scale score distributions would not differ significantly from those of the Mexican group on the pattern variables scales. Null hypotheses were posed with respect to each of the pattern variables scales that the scale score distributions of the Spanish-Amer- ican sub-group which speaks Spanish to peers do not differ significantly from. those of the Mexican group.1 These were submitted to a chi-square test of significance. Upon setting up the problems for chi—square analysis, it was fbund that the theoretical freouencies in several cells on Scales I and III were too low to allow proper calculation of the chi-square values. Hence, no data is available on these two scales. However, a chi-square analysis was carried out on Scales II and IV. See Tables 28 and 29. In both of these latter scales the levels of significance were too high to warrant rejection of the null hypotheses. It was inferred, then, that the scale score distributions of the Spanish- speaking sub-group and the Mexican group do not differ significantly from one another on Scales II and IV. Thus, it was concluded that the 1 It should be noted that the questionnaires adminstered.to all of the students in the El Paso Technical High School, except the English language trainees, were written in English. Public school regulations proscribe the use of anything other than an English language instru- ment. Moreover, it was considered that an optional Spanish language questionnaire for use with Spanish-American students would have been an inappropriate instrument because of the very low level of literacy in Spanish among even high school level Spanish-Americans. TABLE 28 —U;8— MEGCMIS AND spmsawsmcmzs SPEAK ‘IG SPAIII SH TO PEERS, DI s'mIBUTIg-N CF sCALE Izms DEEIVEO FRI Rasputsas T0 ROLE-CCNELICI SITUATICNS INVOLVING AN II-TSTRULETTAL ROLE vmsvs A II B‘IDSHIP ROLE (SCALE II) Scale T Emhnic ype Gmup I A- II III IV v N Mexican 107 107 110 63 387 Spani sh-Am eri can 6 5 5 8 24 Speaking Spanish to peers N 113 112 115 71 All Chi-square = 5.3 P less than 050, d.f. : 5 TABLE 29 MEXICANS AND SPMTISH-ALL‘EI CAN- S SPEAKII'IG SPANISH TO PMiS, DISTRIBUTILN OF SCALE TYPAJS DERIVED FROM RESPOI‘ESES T0 ROLE- CON FLI CT SI TUATI 01‘? S IN VOLVI N G SE1. F- ORI HIT ATI ON VERSUS COLLECTIVITY-ORIE‘ITATION TOI’I'ILRD THE PEER GROUP (SCALE Iv) Scale T e Ethnic yp Gm‘p I II III Iv v N MexiCan 59 100 112 78 37 386 Spanish-American 2 2 7 2 2 15 speaking Spanish to peers N 61 102 119 80 39 401 Chi-square 8 -.9 -149- notion outlined above with respect to the Spanish-speaking sub~group is consistent with these findings. Attention was then directed to the Spanish-American subegroup which speaks both English and Spanish to peers. The conception posited of this sub-group was that it is integrated or identified with an emergent pat- tern of social relations in the ethnically heterogeneous border setting. Since this sub~group was conceived of as involved.in an emergent pattern of social relations, no attempt was made to predict directionality. The interest here was in deterudning with which other groups it might share similar patterns of role-expectations, and on which dimensions it might differ from the other groups. First, the bilingual sub—group was compared with the Anglo—American group on each of the pattern variables scales. This was done in order to determine if the bilingual sub-group exhibits patterns of role-expec- tations sindlar to or different from those of the Anglo-American group. The analysis described earlier in this chapter implied that the role- expectations patterns of the bilingual sub-group would differ from those of the Anglo-American group, but that analysis was not definitive in that it did not involve a direct comparison with the Anglo-American group. Tables 30, 31, 82, and 33 show the distributions of the scale scores of the Spanish-AmeriCan group which speaks both English and Spanish to peers Compared with those of the Anglo-American group on Scales I, II, III, and IV, respectively. A.null hypothesis was posed in each case that the scale score distributions of the two groups do not differ significantly from one another. These were submitted to a chi-souare test of signifi- <3ance. -149- notion outlined above with respect to the Spanish-speaking sub-group is consistent with these findings. Attention was then directed to the Spanish-American sub-group which speaks both English and Spanish to peers. The conception posited of this sub-group was that it is integrated or identified with an emergent pat— tern of social relations in the ethniCally heterogeneous border setting. Since this subbgroup was conceived of as involved in an emergent pattern of social relations, no attempt was made to predict directionality. The interest here was in determining with which other groups it might share similar patterns of role-expectations, and on which dimensions it might differ from the other groups. First, the bilingual sub—group was compared with the Anglo-American group on each of the pattern variables scales. This was done in order to determine if the bilingual sub-group exhibits patterns of role-expec- tations similar to or different from those of the Anglo-American group. The analysis described earlier in this chapter implied that the role- expectations patterns of the bilingual sub—group would differ from those of the Anglo-American group, but that analysis was not definitive in that it did not involve a direct comparison with the Anglo-American group. Tables 30, 31, 32, and 33 show the distributions of the scale scores of the Spanish-American group which speaks both English and Spanish to peers compared with those of the Anglo-American group on Scales I, II, III, and IV, respectively. A.null hypothesis was posed in each case that the scale score distributions of the two groups do not differ significantly from one another. These were submitted to a.chi-souare test of signifi- 081108. -150... TABLE 30 ANGLo-m-cnu CANS AND SprtISH-MSEICAIS mo ”IN BILIz-ICUAL NIIN PENIS, DISINIBNIICN 0F SCALE TIA-ES DAILIVID 111112.: RESPONSES TO ROLE-CONFLICT SITUATIQITS INVOLVING A1: Ascot-301.10 ROLE VERSUS A FAILIAL ROLE (SCALE I) Sc T Ethnic ale ype Gnu!) I a II III IV V N Anglo-American 34 28 31 11 104 Spanish—American 1'7 8 13 16 54 bilingual with peers N 51 36 44 27 158 Chi-square 3 10.6 P less than .05, d.f. 3 5 TABLE 31 Acme-manners AND SPNII SN-N'SIHCNS NSC ALE BILINCUAL WITH PENIS, DISTRIBUTIUN CF SCALE rues DARIVED 1111121.: NESPcz-rSES T0 ROIisl-COITETICT SITUAI'Im'S INVOLVING AN INSINUI.:AIITAL ROLE VERSUS A matDSI-IIP NCLE (SCALE II) Scale Type Ethnic Gmup I a II III IV . V N Anglo-American 23 21 28 29 101 Spanish-American 4 9 19 22 54 bilingual with peers N 27 30 47 51 155 Chi-square '-'- 7.3 P less than .20, d.f. '-"- 5 -151... TABLE 32 M'TEO-AIFA CAI S A: D SPECIES '11; 11110.; S 21" C 11.1.3 EILII'G AI. Z‘JTH F3313 DISTf’lBUI‘I IT CF SC ALE" TYPES D31 112D FBI 1' S'3PC SSS TC ROLE- CCUFLICTS ITU‘T C17.) " ITTVCLVI TC} 5113- C3132 LATIC'IT VERSUS CCLLECI‘IHTY O-:IE ITATICIT T0. 1A;'1D :1: FA ILY (SCALE III) :22 Ethnic Scale Type ”Imp I a II III IV a v N Anglo-American 22 54 31 107 ‘ Spanish-American 7 28 20 55 bilingual with peers N 29 82 51 162 Chi-square = 2.0 P less than .50, d.f. a 3 TABLE 33 NEED-EARRICMIS A1: D SPANISA-AL'EICAIS 1111C AAE LILINCVAL NINE FEARS, DISTRIBUTION OF SCALE TYPES D1. IVED FEO: EASE. ..SIO ROLE. CONFLICI SITUAEIONS INVOLVING SlF-ORIEITTATICN “DV'ERSUS COLLECTIVITY- CEIEI: IAIION IONAND THE PEEICHIP (SCALEIV) Ethnic Scale Type GT WP I II III IV V N Anglo-American 3 17 28 30 29 107 Spanish-American 9 17 14 10 5 55 bilingual with peers N 12 31+ 42 40 34 162 Chi-square : 20.1 P less than .001, d.f. = 7 ~152- In the case of Scale I (Table 30) the chi-square value was sig- nificant at the .05 level, in the case of Seals II (Table 31) at the .20 level, and in that of Scale IV (Table 33) at the .001 level. Thus, the null hypothesis was rejected in all three of these cases. It was concluded that the bilingual sub—group and the Anglo-American group exhibit different patterns of role-expectations on Scales I, II, and IV. In the case of Scale III (Table 82) the chi-square value was sig- nificant at the .50 level, but this probability was too high to allow rejection of the null hypothesis. Hence, it was concluded that the bilingual sub—group does not exhibit a pattern of role-expectations on Scale III different from that of the Anglo-American group. It was inferred from these findings thatthe bilingual sub-group is not gen- erally integrated into nor identified with the patterns of social re- lations of the Anglo-Americans. New, although it was inferred from the findings that the bilingual sub-group is not generally integrated nor identified with the social relations patterns of the Anglo-Americans, it remained to be determined if this sub—group were integrated into a set of traditional ethnic pat- terns. Hence, a.comparison of the bilingual sub-group with the Mexican group was made. It was reasoned that if the patterns of role-expecta- tions ethibited by the bilingual sub-group also differed from those of the Hexicans, then it might be inferred that the former is integrated into neither the traditional ethnic group patterns nor the dominant Anglo-American patterns, but is involved in a deviant pattern of social relations. ~153- An analysis, then, was carried out of the Spanish-American sub-group which speaks both English and Spanish to peers compared with the MexiCan group on Scales I, II, and IV. See Tables 34, 35, and 37, respectively. Null hypotheses were posed that the distributions of each group do not differ significantly from one another, and these were submitted to a chi- souare test of significance. The chi-square value on Scale I (Table 34) was significant at the .001 level and, hence, the null hypothesis was rejected. It was concluded, then that this finding is consistent with the notion that the bilingual sub—group is not integrated nor identified with the patterns of social relations of the traditional ethnic group. In the case of Scale II (Table 35) the chi-square value was sig- nificant at the .001 level also and, hence, the null hypothesis was re- jected. It was concluded that this finding is also consistent with the notion that the bilingual sub-group is not integrated nor identified with the patterns of social relations of the traditional ethnic group. An analysis was made of the bilingual and hexican group on Seals III (Table 86). A.null hypothesis was posed with respect to the scale score distri- butions of these two groups on this scale, and the hypothesis was tested by a.chi-square test of significance. The chi-square value was signifi- cant at the .001 level, and the null hypothesis was rejected. It was Concluded that this finding is consistent with the notion that the bi- lingual sub-group is not integrated into nor identified with the patterns of social relations of the traditional ethnic group. The chi-souare value on Scale IV (Table 37) was significant at the .95 level. However, this Was far too high a probability to warrant rejection of the null hypothesis. “1514,— TABLE 34 tm CANS AND SPAIII SH-Allflll CANS mo ARE BILll-IGUAL WITH PEERS, DISTRIBUTION OF SCALE TYPES DERIVED IRON RESPONSES TO ROLE- CONFLICT SITUATICNS INVOLVING AN ECONOLITLC ROLE VERSUS A FAMLIAL ROLE (SCALE I) Scale Type Ethnic Gm“? I a II III IV V N Mexican 162 90 59 3O 341 SpanishpAmerican 17 8 13 16 54 bilingual with peers N 179 98 '72 1.6 . 395 Chi-square = 24.1 P less than .001, d.f. = 5 TABLE 35 moms AND SPANISH-AMERI once NRC IRE BILINCUAL WITH Rims, DISTRIBUTION OF SCALE TYPES DIRIVSD IRON RESI’CIJSES T0 ROLE- CONFLICT SITUATIONS INVOLVING AN INSTRUIIINTAL ROLE VIRSUS A FRIS-IDSRIP ROLE (SCALE II) W Ethnic . Scale Type Group _ I & II III IV V N Mexican 107 107 110 63 387 Spanish-American 4 9 19 22 54 bilingual with peers N 111 116 129 85 441 Chi-square ' 26.8 P less than .001, d.f. : 5 -155.- TABLEVB6 hiEJCICANS AI‘ID SPMIISH-AMERICAWS ’b'fii'O MLE BILINGUAL WITH PEEIS, DISTRIBUTION OF SCALE TYPES DEHVED FROLI RESPOYSES TO ROLE- CONFLICT SITUATI CNS IT-IVOLVING 82L F—ORI ENTATI ON VERSUS COLLECTIVITY ORIEETATION TOW£RD THE FAMILY (SCALE III) Ethnic Scale Type Gmup I a II III IV a v N Mexican 11 12.1 209 341 Spanish-American '7 28 20 55 bilingual with peers N 18 149 229 396 Chi-square - 18, 3 P less than .001, d.f. '-'- 3 TABLE 37 MEXICANS AND SPANISH-AMERICIUVS WHO ARE BILIIIGUAL NITH PEERS, DISTRIBUTION OF SCALE TIPES DERIVED FROM RESPONSES TO ROLE- CONFLICT SITUATI ONS INVOLVING Sth-ORIRYTATION VERSUS COLLECTIVITI—ORIIRTATION TOWARD THE PEER GROUP (SCALE IV) Scale Type Ethnic Gr°up I II III IV v N lexiCan 59 100 112 78 37 386 Spanish-American 9 l7 I4 10 5 55 bilingual with peers N 68 117 126 88 42 441 Chi-square = 2.2 P less than .95, d.f. II ‘1 Hence, it was concluded that this finding does not support the notion that the bilingual sub-group is not integrated nor identified wdth the patterns of social relations of the traditional ethnic group. On the contrary, this finding implies that the bilingual sub-group is integrated into this dimension of the social relations patterns of the traditional ethnic group. In order to facilitate the comprehension of the preceding findings a table was constructed showing the relationship between the variable of language spoken to peers and the patterns of role-expectations exhibited by the Spanish-American students. Table 38, then, may serve as the basis for a brief discussion of these findings. The analysis showed that the English-speaking sub-group exhibited a pattern of role-expectations similar to those of the Anglo-Americans on all fbur of the pattern variables scales. These findings, then, are thoroughly congruent with the notion that the English-speaking sub-group is integrated into or identified with the social relational patterns of the dominant Anglo- Americans and, hence, that it may be conceived of as an assimilated sector of the general Spanish-American ethnic class. The analysis also showed that the Spanish-speaking sub-group exhibited patterns of role- expectations similar to those of the Mexicans on Scales II and IV. It was not possible to determine whether the Spanish-speaking sub-group exhibited role-expectations similar to or different from.those of the Mexicans on Scales I and III owdng to technical difficulties in the computation of the chi-square value. However, these partial findings are consistent with the notion that the Spanish-speaking sub-group is Table 38 41.57- REIATIONSHIP BETWEEN TRAIT OT LANGUAGE SPOKEN TO P537315 BY SPANISH- AMERICAN STUDENTS AND PATTERNS 0? RS E-EXPECTATIONS EXHIBITED ON -IE FOUR PATTERN VARIABLE SCALES Language Spoken to Scale I Scale II Scale III Scale IV Peers Engli sh Exhi bi t Anglo- Exhi bi t Anglo- Exhi bi t Anglo- Exhi hi 1: Anglo- American pat- American pat- nerican pat- American pat- tern of role- tern of role- tern of role- tern of role— expectations expectations expectations expectations Spanish No data EXhibit No data Exhibit Mexican pat- Mexican pat- tern of role- tern of role expectations expectations Both Exhibit Exhibit Exhibit Anglo- Exhibit English neither Anglo- neither Anglo— American pat- Mexican pat- and American nor American nor tern of role- tern of role- Spanish Mexican pat- Mexican pat- expectations expectations terns of role- expectations terns of role- expectations integrated into or identified with the social relational patterns of a traditional sector of the general Spanish-AmeriCan ethnic class. The analysis showed that the bilingual sub—group exhibited patterns of role-expectations different from both the Anglo-American and hexican groups on Scales I and II. These partial findings are consistent with the notion that the bilingual sub-group is integrated or identified with an emergent pattern of social relations. However, on Scale II this sub- group exhibited a pattern of role-expectations similar to that of the Anglo-American group, and on Scale IV a pattern similar to that of the Mexican group. Thus, neither of these latter findings are consistent with the above notion. The interpretation of these findings with respect to the social system theory and their significance as an addition to substantive knowb ledge of the United States—Mexican border area will be taken up in the summary to this chapter. -159— Summary This chapter examined the relationship between the variable of language spoken to Peers and patterns of role-expectations exhibited by the Spanish-American students. Several notions concerning this relation- ship were presented, and the findings were analyzed to determine if they were consistent with those conceptions. A summary of the findings along with an interpretation of their theoretical and substantive significance will be presented also. A brief review of selected literature pertaining to ethnic status differences among Spanish-Americans was presented at the beginning of this chapter. The views cited implied that the Spanish-Americans may not be conceived as an ethnically hemogeneous class but that they may be differentiated into at least three major sub-groups. Since the find— ings in the previous chapter indicated that the Spanish-AmeriCan students could not be considered an ethnically homogeneous group standing in an intermediate acculturative position between the Mexicans and Anglo-Amer— icans, it was deemed advisable to try to apply the notions suggested by the literature to the Spanish-American students in this study. The trait of language spoken to peers was selected as a variable deemed likely to reflect the ethnic status differences cited in the literature. In accordance with those views, then, it was posited that the trait of speaking English to peers represents integration or iden- tification with the patterns of social relations of the dondnant Anglo- Americans. It was posited that the trait of speaking Spanish to peers represents integration or identification with the patterns of social ~160— relations of a traditional sector of the Spanish-American ethnic class. Finally, it was posited that the trait of speaking both English and Spanish to peers represents integration or identifiCation with emergent patterns of social relations in the ethnically heterogeneous border com- munity. An analysis of the scale score distributions of the Spanish-American students sub—divided according to language spoken to peers showed that this variable does differentiate their role-expectations patterns. The scale score distributions of the English-speaking, Spanish-speaking, and bilingual sub-groups differed significantly from one another on Scales I, III, and IV. With the exception of Scale II, then, these findings were consistent with the notion that the variable of language spoken to peers reflects integration or identification with differentiated patterns of social relations. {Oreover, the mean ranks of these three groups re- lative to one another on Scales I, III, and IV conformed to the pattern which would be expected if the language variable reflects inegration into differentiated patterns of social relations. The findings described above prompted a.further series of analyseS. The purpose of these was to secure further evidence bearing on the notion that the variable of language spoken to peers reflects integration into or identification with differentiated patterns of social relationS. First, the scale score distributions of the English—speaking sub-group were cons pared with those of the Anglo-Americans. The analysis showed that the English-speaking sub-group's scale score distribution does not differ from that of the Anglo-Americans on any of the four scales. From this additional evidence, then, it may be concluded that the English-speak- ing sub—group is integrated or identified with patterns of social re- lations similar to those of the Anglo-Americans. The analysis also showed that the Spanish—speaking sub—group's scale score distributions do not differ from those of the Mexicans on Scales II and IV. Owing to technical difficulties it was not possible to calculate the chi-souare value of the distributions of these two groups on Scales I and IV. From this additional partial evidence, how- ever, it seems warranted to conclude that the Spanish-speaking sub—group is not integrated into patterns of social relations similar to those of the Anglo-AmeriCans. moreover, the evidence strongly suggests that the Spanish-speaking sub—group is integrated into the patterns of tradi- tional Spanish-AmeriCan social relations which do not differ signifi- cantly from those of the Mexicans. Finally, the analysis showed that the scale score distributions of the bilingual sub-group differed from those of both the Anglo-American and Mexican groups on Scales I and II. The analysis also showed that the scale score distributions of the bilingual sub—group did not differ from that of the Anglo-Americans on Scale III, and did not differ from that of the Mexicans on Scale IV. Hence, it seems warranted to conclude that the bilingual sub-group is integrated into certain patterns of social relations differing from those of both the Anglo-American, Span- ish-American and hexican groups, i.e., emergent patterns of social re- lations. It is clear, however, that this sub—group nay not be conceived of as completely involved in deviant patterns of social relations. On the dimension of collectivity-orientation toward the family (Scale III) the bilingual sub-group appears to be integrated into or identified with a pattern of social relations similar to those of the Anglo-Americans. On the dimension of collectivity-orientation toward the peer group (Scale IV) this sub-group appears to be integrated into or identified with a pattern of social relations similar to those of the Mexicans and Spanish—speaking Americans. If these findings are to be congruent wdth the general implications, of the social system theory examined in Chapter IV, then it would seem that the ethnic sub~division of a society must be viewed as a factor which limits the extension of given patterns of role-expectations through- out the population by restricting participation in common patterns of social relations. The findings of the present chapter were consistent with the notion that the Spanish-speaking sub-group is integrated or identified with traditional Spanish-American ethnic patterns but not with those of the dominant Anglo-Americans. Hence, idegration or iden- tification with traditional Spanish-American ethnic patterns must be viewed as limiting participation in the highly differentiated and seg- regated system of social relations posited as characterizing the dosi- nant mode of social organization in the United States. This view suggests, then, that the failure of the Spanish-speaking students to exhibit as high an incidence of universalistic-type role- expectations as the Anglo-Americans on Scales I and II need not be attri- buted to any unicue features of the traditional Spanish-American ethnic b ~163— enclave, but rather that the patterns of social relations of this ethnic sub-group are less differentiated and segregated than those of the larger social system in which it persists. Likewise, with respect to the di- mension of selfLorientation versus collectivity-orientation toward the family (Seale III), the failure of the Spanish-speaking students to ex- hibit as high an incidence of self—orientation as the Anglo-Americans may be atributed to their integration or identification with less differ- entiated and segregated patterns of social relations. The same inter- pretation is applicable to the role-expectations patterns exhibited by the Spanish-speaking students on the dimension of self-orientation versus collectivity-orientation toward the peer group (Scale IV). In contrast to the Spanish-speaking students, the English-speaking sub-group exhibited patterns of role-expectations which did not differ significantly from those of the Anglo-Americans on any of the pattern variables scales. Hence, this sub-group of Spanish-AmeriCans quite clearly appears to be integrated or identified with the dominant pat- terns of social relations of the society. That is, the role-expectar tions patterns exhibited by this sub-group strongly imply that it is integrated or identified with a highly differentiated and segregated system of social relations. The bilingual Spanish-American sub-group presents a case somewhat different from either of the other two sub-groups. The analyses showed that this sub-group exhibited patterns of role-expectations different from those of both the dominant Anglo-Americans and the traditional Span- ish-American sub-group on Soale I (universalistic—orientation toward ~164- brother) and Scale II (universalistic-orientation toward friend). Thus, with respect to these dimensions, the findings imply that the bilingual sub-group is integrated or identified with neither the dominant patterns of social relations of the society, nor with those of the traditional Spanish-American ethnic group. These findings are especially interest- ing because not only did the bilingual sub-group's role-expectations patterns differ from those of the dominant Anglo-Americans, but this sub- ' “ "‘7‘!"— ._.J group ranked higher than the Anglo-Americans on both Scales I and II. If consistency is to be maintained in our interpretations, then these I findings must be construed as implying that the bilingual sub—group is integrated or identified with an even more highly differentiated and segregated system of social relations ther the Anglo-Americans and English-speaking Spanish- AmariCans. On Scale III (collectivity-orientation toward the family) the bi- lingual sub—group exhibited a pattern of role-expectations which did not differ significantly from that of the Anglo-AmariCans. And on Scale IV (collectivity-orientation toward the peer group) the bilingual sub-group exhibited a pattern of role—expectations which did not differ signifi- cantly from that of the Mexicans or the traditional Spanish-American sub-group. Thus, considering the role-expectations of the bilingual sub-group on these two dimensions, it may not be concluded that this sub-group is integrated or identified with a.completely emergent pat- tern of social relations. Nevertheless, these findings contribute to a rather interesting portrait of a group integrated or identified with a semi-emergent pattern of social relations within the context of an ethnically differentiated social system. The chief features to be noted with respect to the bilingual sub-group are its more freouent rejection of particularistic-type demands deriving from both family and peers and its more freouent exhibition of selfeorientation in family and peer group relationships. On the average, the members of the bilingual sub—group would appear to be more independent of particularistic ties than the mem- bers of any of the other groups studied. Like the Anglo-Americans, this sub-group exhibited a relatively low incidence of collectivity-orienta- tion toward the family. But, while the Anglo-Americans exhibited a re- latively high incidence of collectivity-orientation toward the peer group, the bilingual sub-group ranked very low on this dimension also. Whfle in the case of the Anglo-Americans and English-speaking Span- ish-Americans the breakdown of strong kinship ties appears to be com- pensated for by the development of strong ties to the peer group the bilingual sub-group appears to lack both strong kinship and peer group ties. It also should be noted here that of the Spanish—American stu- dents included in this study about forty-five percent indicated that they spoke both English and Spanish to peers. Thus, the bilingual sub- group constitutes a very sizeable portion of the Spanish-American stu- dents studied here and probably constitutes a large sector of the Span- ish-American youth population throughout the border area. One more item needs to be considered before completing this summary. That the variable of language spoken to peers should differentiate so well the patterns of role-expectations of the Spanish-American students is of significance in view of the fact that the previously discussed typology did not do so. The typology discussed in Chapter V included the variable of language spoken to parents but not that of language spoken to peers. It also included the variable of place of mother's birth. It is to be noted, then, that the typOIOgy was constructed largely around items which tend to reflect the ethnic character of the student's home. On the other hand, the variable of language spoken to peers may not be conceived of as reflecting directly the ethnic char- acter of the student's home but rather his extra—familial social re- lations. Hence, the inference is strongly suggested that the factors determining the types of social role-expectations stumed here must largely lie in socialization processes occurring outside of the famdly complex. CHAPTER VII EFTECTS OF CROSS-CULTURAL EDUCATITN ON MEXICAK STUEEITS' ROLE-EXPECTATIONS This chapter wdll present an analysis of the effects of cross-cul- tural education on the role-exnectations of a group of Mexican students. Included as respondents in the study was a group of Mexican students en- rolled in special classes in English at the El Paso Technical High School. These shudents were residents of Mexico and crossed the international bor- der on school days to attend the special anliSh courses for foreign stu- dents. Fbr sake of clarity in the following discussion, these students will be referred to as English Language Trainees. This group of students was excluded from all of the previous analyses. The principal reason for its erclusion was to maintain a high level of homogeniety among the regular Mexican student group with respect to amount of cross—cultural contact. The inclusion of the English Language Trainees anong the other Mexican student group clearly would have reduced the homoeeniety of the latter with respect to this variable. The purpose of the analyses presented in this chapter was to deter- mine which of several pertinent views more adequately accounts for the role-expectations exhibited by the English Language Trainees. 1~"irst, several alternative views will be presented and the types of role-ex- pectations which would be expected in each case will be discussed. Second, an analysis of the role-expectations of this group subdivided according to extent of education inthe United States will be presented. /d '- Qu- Third, the role-expectations of these students will be compared with those of the regular Hexican students and also with those of the Anglo- AmeriCan students. Both of these analyses pertain to only two of the pattern Variables seales: Scales I and III.1 Finally, a summary of the findings will be presented at the end of the chapter. The three views which will be considered here as possibly account— ing for the role-expectations patterns exhibited by the English language Trainees are as follows: (1) that, while enrolled in special classes in English in an American high school, these students remain integrated or identified with Mexican patterns of social relations, (2) that these students may be in process of transition from interration into MexiCan patterns to integration into United States patterns of social relations, and finally (3) that they may be integrated into deviant patterns of social relations, patterns different from those of both the Mexicans and Anglo-AmeriCans. Each of these views is consistent with the social system frame of reference. The view which appeared most compelling to this study from its in- ception was that, while hexican students may be enrolled in an American th school, so long as they reside in Mexico and continue their associ- ations with Merican peers they will remain inbgrated or identified with Mexican patterns of social relations. Hence, they would be expected to exhibit role-expeCtations patterns similar to the regular Mexican high school students. 1 Owing to the rather tight course schedule it was not possible to 8d? minister the complete questionnaire to the English Language Trainees. Hence, only those items which constitute Scales I and III were pre- sented to these students. A.study of the effects of a.sross-cultural exchange program which the present writerz conducted in Mexico suggested the pertinence of this View to the study. The various studies undertaken by the Committee on Cross-Cultural Education of the Social Science Research Council like- wise point to this notion. While the individual studies conducted under the auspices of the committee were concerned largely with several dimen- sions of attitudes toward the United States, this variable is not so dif- ferent from that of role-expectations as to render their findings irre- levant to the present concern. In no case did those studies report that extent of educational training in the United States or amount of contact with United States society and culture constituted a factor which clearly differentiated "favorable" from "unfavorable" attitudes toward the United States. This was particularly so with respect to the Mexican student group. While it is not specifically elaborated, Beals' and Humphrey's study3 of Mexican students implies a structural-functional approach to the analysis of Mexican and other foreign students' attitudes toward the United States. It is implied, given that the Mexican student remains integrated into or identified with particular groups in Mexico and with the national ethos, that the place to seek the source and sustenance of 2 Frank Na11,AStudy of the International Farm Youth Ekchange Program in Mexico, Unpublished MA thesis, Michigan State College, 1954. 3 Ralph L. Beale and Norman Humphrey, No Frontiers t_o_ Learning. The Mexican Student in the United States, —unpublished— manuscript. -170— his attitudes toward the United States is not primarily in the situation of contact with American society and.culture. Rather, it is to be sought in the signifiCance of his attitudes with respect to the social system or sub-systems into which he is integrated or with which he identifies in Mexico. The notion here is clearly consistent with that of Persons, i.e., that attitudes have e_functional signifiCance with respect to the integration of a social system. Hence, so long as the individual remains integrated or identified with Mexico and particular groups within that country he will tend to formulate attitudes of an order which do not threaten his position in those sub-systems of social relations. The adoption of attitudes "too favorable" toward the United States could quite conceivably jeopardize a Mexican national's position in political, economic, and even social affairs. This view tends to minimize the sig- nificance attached to cross—cultural education as a situational context promoting modification of students' orientation patterns. Such a structural-functional view was precisely the one taken here with respect to role-exnectations, and it is consistent with the analyti- cal framework adopted in the analyses presented in the preceding chapters. Thus, since the English language Trainees resided in Mexico and must be presumed to be in continuous contact with nexican peers, it was posited that they would exhibit patterns of role-expectations which do not differ from.those of the regular Mexican high school students. Conversely; it would be expected that they would exhibit role-expectations patterns which do differ from those of the United States students. -171- The second view which was considered is that the English Language Trainees may be in process of transition from integration into Mexican patterns of social relations to integration or identification with United States patterns. A socialization or social learning view which focuses predouunantly upon the cross-cultural contact situation would hold that this situation would tend to promote modification of the for- eign students' orientations. It seems reasonably consistent with this view, then, to posit that the greater the extent of centact of the Mex- ican students with United States society and culture the more siudlar would be their role-expectations to those of the United States students. Such a view would seem to constitute the implicit assumption of many cross-cultural exchange programs. It is to be noted, however, that in fostering a methodological concentration on features of the cross-cul— tural interaction situation, this view tends to neglect the larger sig- nifiCance of foreign students' orientation patterns. It tends to over- look the significance which fbreign students' orientation patterns (e.g., role-expectations, value standards, ideological beliefs) have for their integration and identification with social complexes in their home countries. Since data was not available to present an analysis of the effects of general cross-cultural contact on the role-expectations patterns of the English Language Trainees, the study necessarily could consider only the effects of the formal education situation. Thus, it was reasoned that if the formal education situation promotes the integration or iden- tification of the English Language Trainees with United States patterns of social relations, than the role-expectations of these students should -l72- differ from those of the other Mexican students in the direction of greater similarity to those of the United States students. Furthermore, if the formal education situation promotes the integration of these stu- dents into United States patterns of social relations, then their role— expectations should differ from one another in the direction of greater similarity to those of the United States students according to the ex- tent of eduCation in this country. Before discussing the third view, an alternative notion to the one above needs to be considered. It is possible that a selective factor or factors may be operative with respect to the English language Trainees. Thus, if their role-expectations were to differ from those of the other Mexican students this might be accounted for by their having been inte- grated or identified with distinctive patterns of social relations in Mex- ico, prior to their arrival in the United States. If a selective factor of this nature were operative, then the Ehglish language Trainees' role- expectations would be expected to differ from those of the regular Mex- ican students. Thus, this would pose a problem of differentiating be- tween the effects of the cross—cultural eduCation situation on the role- expectations of these students and the effects of the temporally ante- cedent factors. which factor, then, might be attributed prime signifi- cance: partial or total integration into United States patterns of social relations as a result of the cross-cultural education, or integration into a sub-system.of United States-like social patterns in Mexico prior to arrival in the United States? ~rn- Without recourse to additional data concerning the social back- grounds of the English Language Trainees this problem may be solved in the fbllowing manner. If the findings were to indiCate that the Eng- lish Language Trainees' role-expectations differed from those of the other Mexican students in the direction of greater similarity to those of the United States students, this would establish that either the cross-cultural eduCation or factors antecedent to this situation were related to the types of role-expectations exhibited. However, if ante- cedent factors rather than the cross-cultural eduCation were to account for the similarity of the Trainees' role-expectations of the members of this group would not be expected to differ significantly from one an- other according to the extent of their cross—cultural education. Hence, a basis for differentiating between the effects of cross-cultural edu- cation and antecedent factors is provided. The third, and final, social system view which may be considered here as possibly accounting for the role-expectations patterns exhibited by the English Language Trainees is that they may be integrated or iden- tified with deviant patterns of social relations, patterns different from those of both the other Mexican students and the United States stu- dents. Here, the notions elaborated in Chapter VI with respect to the bilingual Spanish-American students may be suggested again. Enrollment in the special classes in English at an American high school may imply, for Mexican students, integration or identification with deviant pat- terns of social relations and, hence, the manifestation of deviant patterns of role-expectations. As in the previously discussed view, integration into deviant patterns of social relations may be the direct result of the cross-cultural education experience itself, or Mexican students involved in deviant patterns of social relations may possibly be more prone to seeking eduCation in the United States. In either case, we should expect the role—expectations exhibited by the English Language Trainees to differ from those of both the regular Mexican students and the United States students if they were integrated into a deviant pat- tern of social relations. Analysis of the Data The first series of analyses consisted of comparisons of the role- expectations of the English Language Trainees with those of the other Mexican students and those of the Anglo—Americans. Table 39 shows the distributions of the scale scores of the English Language Trainees and the other MexiCan students on Scale I (universalistic-orientation toward brother). A null hypothesis was posed that the scale score distributions of these groups do not differ significantly from one anoth r. This was submitted to a chi-souare test of significance, and the chi-snuare value was significant at the .10 level. Thus, the null hypothesis was rejected. It was inferred, then, that the scale score distributions of the English Language Trainees and the other Mexican students do differ significantly from one another on Scale I. The mean ranks of the two groups were calculated, and the English Language Trainees had a mean rank of 25 , while the other Mexican stu- dents had a mean rank of 251. This rank difference, however, was so us ‘ . ,, TABLE 39 ~175- ENGLISH LAAICUACE TRAINEES AND OTHER muons, DISTRIBUTION OF SCALE TYPES DERIVED FROM Basra-ISIS TO ROLE- "NFLICT SITUATILNS INVOLVIIZG AN 3001:0116 ROLE VERSUS A FAMILIAL ROLE (SCALE 1) Student Scale Type Gr°up I II III Iv v N Other Mexican 76 86 9O 59 30 341 Riglish Language 48 29 32 29 24 162 Trainees - N 124 115 122 88 54 503 Chi-square I 12.9 P less than .10, d.f. = 7 TABLE 40 ENGLISH LAIIGUAGE TRAINERS AND Aime—mam CAI'FS, DISTRIBUTION OF SCALE TYPES DERIVED FRCIA RESPONSES TO ROLE—CONFLICT SITUATILNS INVOLVING AN EGONOIJEEC ROLE VARSUS A FAIJLIAL ROLE (SCALE 1) Student Scale Type Gnu" I II III Iv v N Anglo-American 9 25 28 31 11 104 English Language 48 29 32 29 24 162 Trainees N 57 54 60 60 35 266 Chi-square = 55.0 P less than .001, d.f. = 7 _' '"E_ “1"" “ .- I u o o a - . . o small that it seemed unwarranted to attach much significance to it. Thus, although it was inferred from the chi-square analysis that the scale score distributions of these groups differ significantly from one another, the exceedingly small rank difference between them suggests that they differ in only a very minor degree. Mbreover, the English Language Trainees differ in the direction of a higher degree of parti- cularism, and this was shown to be a Mexican rather than an Anglo—Amer- ican norm. A.comparison of the scale score distributions of this group with those of the Anglo-Americans on Scale I only served to confirm the no- Ition that the English Language Trainees are not integrated or identified with Anglo-AmeriCan patterns of social relations. Table 40 shows the distributions of the English Language Trainees and the Anglo—Americans on Seale I. A null hypothesis was posed that these distributions do not differ significantly from one another, and this was submitted to a chi— souare test of significance. The chi-square value was significant at the .001 level, and so the null hypothesis was rejected. The calculations of the mean ranks showed that the Anglo-Americans ranked considerably higher than the English Language Trainees. The Anglo—Americans had a mean rank of 146, while the English Language Trainees had a rank of 124. It was concluded, then, that the English Language Trainees are not inte- grated nor identified with an Anglo-American pattern of social relations. These findings now may be considered with respect to their bearing on the three views advanced earlier in this chapter as possibly account- ing for the role-expectation patterns of the English language Trainees. —l77- It is clearly evident that in this dimension they may not be viewed as in process of transition from Mexican patterns to United States patterns of social relations. Likewise, the findings do not imply that a selec- tive factor is operative which draws Mexicans into the special English course who are already oriented to Ynited States patterns of social re- lations. It also follows that the English Language Trainees may not be considered integrated or identified with a deviant pattern of social relations in this dimension. It may be concluded, then, that the find— ings indicate that in this dimension the English Language Trainees re- main integrated or identified with Mexican patterns of social relations. Next, the scale score distributions of the English Language Train- ees were compared with those of the other MeriCan students on Scale III. Table 41 shows these distributions on Scale III. A null hypothesis was posed that the distributions do not differ from one another, and this was submitted to a chi-sonare test. The chi-souare value was signifi- cant at less than the .001 level and, hence, the null hypothesis was rejected. The English Language Trainees had a mean rank of 290, while the other Mexicans had a mean rank of 233. This was deemed a very large rank order difference, and indicated that the English Language Trainees exhibit a considerably stronger collectivity orientation toward the fam- ily than the other Mexican students. Thus, it was inferred that the English Language Trainees on this dimension are not integrated into a pattern of social relations similar to that of the other Hexican students. A final confirmation of the absence of evidence implying that the English Language Trainees are integrated or identified with the United ~17 8- TABLE 41 ENGLISH LANGUAGE TRAINEE": AND OTHE’» 1‘.EXICANS, DISTRIBUTION OF SCALE TYPES DERIVED FR-C'ifi. RESPONSES TO ROLE-CONFLICT SITUATION S INVOLVING SflF—ORIHITATICN VERSUS COLLECTIVITY-ORIHITATION TOWARD THE FANLILY (SCALE III) Student Scale Type Gmup I & II III Iv v N Other Mexican 11 121 151 58 311 Elglish Language 3 35 7O 54 162 Trainees N 14 156 221 112 503 Chi-square = 22.81 P less than .001, d.f. 3 5 TABLE 42 ENGLISH LANGUAGE TRAINERS AND meLo-mgmcms, DISTRIBUTION OF SCALE TYPES DERIVED FROE‘JE RESPONSES TO ROLE-CONFLICT SITUATIONS INVOLVING SELF-ORIENTATION VERSUS COLLECTIVITY-ORIENTATION TOWARD THE FAMILY (SCALE III) Student. Sula “p3 Gmup I & II III Iv v N Anglo-American 22 54 24 7 107 Ehglish.Language 3 35 7O 54 162 Trainees N 25 89 94 61 269 Chi-square 3 69.3 P less than .001, d.f. 3 5 -179- States students' patterns of social relations ras provided by comparing their scale score distributions on Scale III. Table 42 shows the dis- tributions of scale scores of these two groups on Scale III. A null hy- pothesis was posed and subjected to a chi-souare test of significance. Since the chi-sonare value was significant at less than the .001 level, i the null hypothesis was rejected. The English Language Trainees had a ? mean rank of 159, while the Anglo-Americans had a rank of only 83. if; " These findings clearly imply that the English Language Trainees may not be considered integrated or identified with this dimension of social relations of the Anglo-Americans. They also imply that the Eng- lish Language Trainees are not in a process of transition from Mexican to United States patterns of social relations. Likewise, they imply that a selective factor is not operative which tends to draw into the special English course Mexicans who are already oriented to Anglo-American norms. But, finally, the findings do imply that in this dimension these students are integrated or identified with a deviant pattern of social relations. An examination of their role-erpectations according to extent of eduCa- tion in the United States was carried out to determine whether the cross— cultural education situation or factors temporally antecedent to it may account for this pattern of role—expectations. The second series of analyses concerns the role—expectation patterns of the English Language Trainees sub-divided according to class in the special English course. Null hypotheses were posed that the distribu- tions of the English Language Trainees according to class in the special English course do not differ significantly on Scales I and III. These ~130— were submdtted.to a chi-souare test of SignifiCance. On Scale I it was found that the probability that the chi-souare is significant is less than .90, hence the null hypothesis was not rejected. (See Table 43.) Thus, it was inferred that the anlish Language Trainees do not differ signi- ficantly on Scale I according to class in the special thlish course. In the case of Scale III the probability that the chi-souare value is signifiCant was less than .50. (See Table 44.) Hence, the null hypo- thesis was not rejected in this case either. It was inferred, then, that the scale score distributions of the English Language Trainees according to class in the special English course do not differ significantly from one another on Scale III. It may be concluded from these findings that the extent of education in the special English course in the El Paso high school does not differ- entiate the patterns of role-erpectations of the English Language Train- ees on either of these two dimensions. Furthermore, these findings sup- port the earlier comparisons. They indicate that extent of eduCation in the El Paso high school is not related to the role-expectations patterns exhibited by the English Language Trainees. Moreover, with respect to Scale III, they indicate that while these students may be integrated or identified with a deviant pattern of social relations in this dimension this may not be attributed to the cross-cultural eduCation experience. Summary This chapter presented an analysis of the patterns of role—expec- atations of a group of Mexican students enrolled in special English courses in the Technical High School in El Paso. The purpose of the TABLE 43 ENGLISH LANGUAGE TRAINEES BY CLASS IN SPECIAL E-I-GLISH COURSE, DISTRIBUTION OF SCALE TYPES DERIVED FROM: RESPCI-ISES TO ROLE-CONFLICT SITUATIQES IZCVCLVING a Econ 'L'IC ROLE VERSUS A FA‘EIBIAL ROLE (SCALE 1) S Class in 0516 Type cm?” I II III IV v N Special filglish I 9 5 3 l 3 21 Special Ehglish II 23 10 1’7 10 9 69 Special Elglish III 5 6 3 6 3 23 Special English IV 11 8 9 12 9 1.9 N 48 29 32 29 24 162 Chiusquare I 11.8 P less than .90, d.f. 3 17 TABLE AA. ENQISH LANGUAGE TRAINEES BY GLASS IN SPECIAL EIGLISH COURSE, DISTRIBUTION OF SCALE TYPES DERIVED FROM RESPONSES TO ROLE- COIJFLICT SITUATI CNS INVOLVING SELF-ORIEIITA‘I‘I ON VERSUS COLLECI‘IVITY-ORII'ETATION TOWARD THE FAL‘ELY (SCALE 111) Scale Type Glass in 00““ I, II, III IV v N Special English I 4 5 12 21 Special English II 15 31 23 69 Special Eiglish III 8 9 6 23 Special Ehglish IV 11 25 13 49 N 38 7O 54 162 Chi-square '-'- 9.1 P less than .50, d.f. = 9 ..lij... analysis was to determine to what extent attendance at the school might have effected the role-expectations patterns of these Mexican students. Three views were suggested which might account for the role-expectations patterns of the English Language Trainees. The view which appeared most compelling to this study was that, while enrolled in special classes in English in an American high school, the English Language Trainees may remain integrated or identified with Mexican patterns of social rela- tions and, hence, exhibit Mexican patterns of role-expectations. While this view was supported by the findings on Scale I, it was not clearly supported by the findings on Scale III. The English Language Trainees' role-expectations on Scale III differed signifiCantly from those of the Anglo-AmeriCans, and there was no nuestion that they might be integrated or identified with Anglo-American patterns of social relations with re— spect to this dimension. However, the English Language Trainees' role- expectations also differed significantly from those of the other MexiCan students on Scale III. But the direction of the difference was toward greater collectivity-orientation rather than greater selfeorientation. Hence, the conclusion must be drawn that the English Language Trainees are partly integrated or identified with a deviant pattern of social relations, different from those of both the other Mexican students and the Anglo-American students. The notion was also suggested that the English Language Trainees might be integrated or identified with deviant patterns of social rela- tions and that such deviancy might be attributed to either the effects of the cross-cultural eduCation situation or factors temporally antecedent to the cross-cultural contact. It was reasoned that if it were the cross— cultural contact. It was reasoned that if it were the cross—cultural edu— cation situation which effected the role—expectation patterns of these students, then their role-expectations would differ according to the exe tent of their cross-cultural education experience. The findings, however, showed that extent of cross-cultural education did not differentiate the role-expectations patterns of these students. Hence, the difference between the role-expectations of the English Language Trainees and those of the other MexiCan students on this dimension may be attributed to the integration or identification of the former with a deviant pattern of social relations in Mexico. It may be concluded, then, that involvement in the formal educar tional complex of the special English courses does not appear to have effected the role-expectations patterns of the English Language Trainees. The findings do not imply that the English Language Trainees may be in process of transition from integration with Mexican patterns to inte- gration with United States patterns. CHAPTER VIII THE PELATIONSVIP B’Z'I‘V'IW ROLE-EXPECTATIONS AND SOCIAL STA’I’TTS This chapter will present various analyses of the relationship between role-expectations and social status. Eirst, the relationship will be examined with respect to the United States students. Second, it will be examined with respect to.the Mexican students. And, finally, both Nbxican and United States students will be grouped together and the relationship between role-expectations and social status will be analyzed. Fbllowing this, a series of analyses in which the high status MexiCans are compared with the Anglo-Americans will be presented. Final- ly, the low status Mexicans, high status Mexicans, and Anglo-Americans will be compared. A summary of the findings will be presented at the end of the chapter. This study adopted the variable of (student's) father's occupation as an index of social status. Occupation, as an index of status, war- ranted consideration because of its presumed significance as a major determdnant of social status. Parsons holds that position in the econ- omic structure of a.social system is of high significance in determining social status, relational patterns and, by implication, patterns of role—expectations. As was pointed out earlier in this study, it is Parsons' conception that, as the extent of differentiation and segrega- tion of social relations increases in a social system, universalistic— type role-expectations will gradually emerge as the predominantiype. While not proving this proposition, the findings presented in previous chapters did strongly support.it. Parsons'1 discusSion of this topic is liudted to generalizations concerning very broad features of society. He does not discuss the significance of involvement in the social system at different status levels as this may bear upon the types of role-ex- pectations exhibited by actors. However, while not providing a clear implication of the relationship between social status and role-expects, tions patterns, the theory is suggestive of certain notions. The social system theory would seem to suggest that type of role- expéctations say be related to social status differences, much like the relationship between role-expectations and ethnic status differences discussed previously. Not all members of a highly urbanized society are involved equally or participate in the same capacity in the networks of economic exchange and other social relationships.‘ In fact, the generic notion of an urban society would seem to be precisely that of a social system exhibiting high degrees of differentiation and segregae tion of social relations, i.e., one in which actors participate in dif- ferent and complimentary capacities. Parsons‘ conceptions, it was noted, imply an association between universalistic-type role-expectations and the extent of differentiation and segregation of social relations in a social system. To the extent, then, that social status differences may reflect degrees of involvement in the highly differentiated and segregated 1 Talcott Parsons, The Social gystem., Glencoe, 111.: The Free Press, 1951, pp. 114.127. ' complexes of social relations in an urban-type society, it would seem congruent with the theory to expect role-expectations to be related to this variable. Since Parsons did not provide a clear implication as to the effects on role-expectations patterns of being differently involved in the econ— omic structure of society, a.number of empirical studies were reviewed to determine if they might imply some consistent relationship and indi- cate its direction. Reference to several of these studies will be made below in order to indiCate what certain pertinent empirical researches imply with respect to the relationship between social status and patterns of role-expectations. Davis and Havighurstz indicate that lower status group members ex- hibit a tendency to evaluate highly the tenor of close personal rela- tionships on the one hand, and tend to place less value on the control of property than do middle status group members. From their findings it seems reasonable to infer that this may indicate that lower status persons exhibit a proclivity toward particularistic-type role-expectar tions on the one hand, and collectivity-orientation toward the peer group on the other hand. Sharp and Axelrodz in their study of Mutual aid among relatives in an urban population report little difference in mutual aid patterns 2 A. Davis and B. Havighurst, Father of the Man. Boston: Houghton Mifflin Co., 1947. 3 Mbrris Axelrod, Urban Structure and Social Participation, American Sociological Review, Vbl. 21, No. 1, February, 1956, pp. 13-18. _1-.;; :7— ; according to occupational position; about two-thirds of those in each occupational category engaged in mutual aid among relatives. Their findings may be interpreted as indicating that some degree of particu- laristic-type role-expectations as well as collectivity orientation toward the family is exhibited among the members of all major occupar tional categories, and that little difference with respect to role-ex- pectations is to be anticipated according to social status. Riesman4 suggests the emergence of a high evaluation of the tenor of peer group relationships and a concomitant lowered evaluation of expresSIy family relationships among the middle and upper middle status grmups in urban places; That is, as "other-directedness" supplants "inner-directedness" peer relationships become more highly valued. At the same time the sphere of family relationships appears to emerge as something resembling another peer group rather than as a.complex of relationships segregated from those of the "outside" world and meriting a consideration predicated on different values from those appropriate to non—familial social relations. Riesman, likewise, suggests that a high degree of “tradition-directedness" is characteristic of lower sta- tus groups. It may be interpreted, then, that lower status group mem- bers are more likely to exhibit self-orientation types of role-expectae tions with reSpect to situations involving conflict between self inter- ests and peer group interests than are middle status persons. With re- spect to situations involving conflict between self interest and family ‘4 David Riesman, The Lonely Crowd, passim. New Haven: Yale University Press, 1950. interests, it may be expected that the lower status group member (the "tradition-directed" man) would exhibit a greater tendency toward col- lectivity-orientation types of role-expectations. Axelrod5 reports that little difference was found between status groups with reenect to the extent of informal visiting engaged in with relatives and friends; all status groups appeared to have a high aro- portion of members who archange relatively frequent visits with relatives and friends. It also was found that there was some greater likelihood that a family member would belong to a formal organization if the head of the family were a white collar (middle status) worker. It might be concluded from these findings, then, that family and peer group (col- lectivity) orientations are not likely to vary with status. Bell and F'orce6 found that upper economic status was associated with higher participation in formal associations than was lower economic 7 reported lower status persons, while not involved status. Yomarovsky so extensively in formal associational activities, exhibited a consider- able amount of informal associational activity. 5 Morris Axelrod, Frban Structure and Social Participation, Ameriggg Sociological 9eview, Vbl. 21, No. l, Tebruary, 1956, pp. 15-16. 6 Wendell Fell and _L‘aryanne T. Force, Urban Neighborhood 'IErpes and Far- ticipation in Fbrmal Associations, American SociologiCal Feview, vol. 21, No. 1, February, 1956, pp. 25—34. 7 Mirra Komarovsky, Patterns of Voluntary Association among Urban Dwellers, American Sociological Review, Vol. 11, December, 1946, pp. 686-698. Ibtsons fbund that high economic status was associated with re- latively greater participation in voluntary associations than was low econosdc status among an urban population in Mexico. Dotson also found that women participated in voluntary associations much less frequently than men, and that religious affiliated associations were the major type in which women were participants in Mexico. F'oskett9 found that high status was associated with greater formal social participation than was low status, but that sex and age were not highly related to amount of participation. Education, on the other hand, was found to be highly associated with formal social participation. This was a direct relationship in which the higher amount of formal education the higher the index of formal social participation. 10 in an interesting analysis of interviews Schatzman and Strauss, with lower and middle status persons on the effects of a natural disaster, suggest that differences in conceptualizing, perceiving, and in modes or relating themselves to an interviewer are apparent between middle and lower status persons. Briefly, the middle status persons tended to generalize more about the effects of the disaster than did the lower status persons. The lower status persons tended to perceive the disaster 8 Floyd.Ibtson, A Note on Participation in Vbluntary Associations in a Mexican City, American Socioloeical Review, vol. 18, No;.4, August, 1953, pp. 880-586. ' 9 John M. Fbskett, Social Structure and Social Participation, American Sociologiga1_Review, vol. 20, No. 4, August, 1955, pp. 431—458. 10 Ieonard Schatzman and Anselm Strauss, Social Class and Modes of Com- munication, American Journal of Sociology, Vbl. LY, No. 4, January, 1955, pp. 329—538. more narrowly in terms of its consequences for themselves. Also, the lower status persons tended to expect a particularistic-type response and interest toward themselves on the part of the interviewers, while the middle status persons took a more impersonal view of the relation- ship. Schneider and Lysgaard,11 in their article on the "deferred grati- fication pattern“, likewise suggest status or class related variables as major differentiating factors with respect to the manifestation of this pattern in orientation to various situational contexts. It seems rea- sonable to conceive of the absence of this pattern as implying a.parti- cularistic emphasis in social orientations and presence of it as im— plying a universalistic emphasis (a proclivity to generalize beyond the immediacy of existence to a situation of future consequences.) ‘Lower status was associated with an absence of or lower degree of crystalli- zation of the pattern, and middle status was associated with the mani- festation of the pattern. The above citations serve to indicate the numerous interpretations which may be drawn from various empirical studies. Certain of these studies would imply that low status in the occupational structure of a highly differentiated society might be expected to produce a predomi- nance of particularistic-type role-expectations and a.strong collecti- vity—orientation toward the family. On the other hand, some would seem 11 Louis Schneider and S. Iysgaard, The Deferred Gratification Pattern, American Sociological Review, Vol. 18, No. 2, April, 1953, pp. 142— 149. to imply a strong balance in favor of self interests when these are in conflict with family interests. It also is implied that upper middle status seems to produce universalistic—type role-expectations, a strong proclivity to generalize beyond.the "personal“ meanings of a situation to their transcendent conseouences. In view of the absence of any clear implication pertaining to the relationship between social status and patterns of role-expectations, no attempt was made to posit direction- ality. This analysis may be considered primarily exploratory. Intranational Comparisons The occupational Categories of the United States census were adop— ted as a status index. The occupation of the person providing the chief support of the family in which the student respondent lived was taken as the basis of classification. It was, of course, not possible to classify the subjects themselves by occupation because only a very few held full time Jobs in addition to attending high school. The presen- tation of the analysis will be divided into three parts. The United States student group will be exandned.first, then the Mexican student group, and finally both of the groups combined. The distributions of scale scores of the United States students according to occupational categories on Scales I, II. III, and IV were arranged in tables for the purpose of chi-square analysis. However, it was found that this arrangement resulted in a considerable number of cells having frequencies too low to allow proper calculation of the chi-square values. The occupational categories Were then collapsed into two groups on the basis of their presumed reflection of status differences. The one collapsed category consisted of Professionals, Farmers, Proprietors, Managers, Officials, and Clerks. This collapsed category was deemed as representing an aggregate of the relatively high status occupations. The other collapsed category consisted of ckilled and Semi-skilled workers, farm laborers and other laborers, and servants. This collapsed category was deemed as representing an aggregate of the relatively low status occupations. All other occupational categories were excluded from the analyses. The reapondents were reclassified into high status and low status categories. The distributions of seals scores of the United States students according to status groups on Scales I, II, III, and IV were then arranged into tables and subjected to chi-square analysis. It was found that the chi—square values were not significant in any of these tables and, hence, the null hypotheses could not be rejected. It was inferred, then, that the role-expectations patterns exhibited by the United States students are not related to the social status of the students. The complete frequencies are presented in Appendix Tables 1, 2, 3 and 4. The distributions of scale scores of the Mexican students accord— ing to status groups on Scales I, II, III, and IV were arranged into tables and subjected to chi-square analysis also. The chi-square values were not found to be significant in any of these tables and, hence, the null hypotheses were not rejected. It was inferred,then, that the role-expectations patterns exhibited by the Rexican students are not related to their social status background. The complete fre- quencies are presented in Appendix Tables 5, 6, 7 and 8. -173 Finally, the distributions of scale scores of the combined group of United States and Hexican students sub-divided according to occupational categories on Seales I, II, III, and IV were arranged into tables and subjected to chi-sauare analysis. In this case there were sufficiently high_frequencies in each of the cells to permit chi-sonare analysis with— out collapsing the occupational categories. In no case, however, were the chi-sausre values sufficient to warrant rejection of the null hypo- theses. Hence, it was concluded that the patterns of role-expectations exhibited by the total group of students are not related to the variable of social status. In view of the preceding findings, this, of course, was to be anticipated. The complete frequencies are presented in Appen- dix Tables 9, 10, 11 and 12. Cross—Cultural Comparisons In view of the above findings, it was decided that a series of cross-cultural comparisons of the relationship between role-expectations and social status merited consideration. The notion which suggested carrying out a further series of comparisons was that, while social status differences do not appear to differentiate the role-expectation patterns of the students on a national basis, United States and Mexican students of similar social status might exhibit simdlarities in role- expectations patterns. The central idea here was that socio-cultural convergence or interpenetration in the border area may occur selectively according to social status. Hence, were this so, it might be reflected by similarities in role-expectation patterns. In considering the possibility that social status similarities may be related to similarities in role-expectations on a cross-cultural ba— sis, the first comparison made was between high status hbxicans and the combined high and low status Anglo-American group. Since earlier find- ings showed that the Anglo—American group's role-expectation patterns differed significantly from those of the Mexicans on all four of the pattern variable scales, this comparison was deemed the strategically most crucial one to make. First, the scale score distributions of the high status MexiCans and the combined Anglo-American group were compared on Scales III and IV. See Tables 45 and 48, respectively. The chi—square value on Scale III was significant at the .001 level, and on Scale IV at the .05 level, Hence, the null hypothesis was rejected in both cases, and it was in- ferred that the role-expectation patterns of the high status Mexicans and those of the combined Anglo-American group do differ significantly from one another in these two dimensions. It may be concluded, then, that sindlarity in social status does not appear to show convergence of role-expectation patterns in these dimensions. Next, the scale score distributions of the high status Mexicans and those of the combined Anglo-American group on Scales I and II were arranged into tables and subjected to chi-sonare analysis. In the case of Seale I, the chi-souare value was significant at the .40 level, and this was not sufficient to reject the null hypothesis. See Table 47. Hence, it was inferred that the role-expectation patterns of the high status Mexicans and the total group of Anglo-Americans do not TABLE 45 HIGH STATUS I-.LEK'ICAI‘JS AND ALL HELD-AMERICANS, DISTRIBUTION OF SCALE TYPES DERIVED FREE”; RESPONSES TO ROLE-CONFLICT SITUATIOl-FS INVOLVING SEF—ORIE‘ITATION VERSUS COLLECTIVITY-ORIE'ETATI ON TOWARD THE FAIJLY (SCALE III) Scale 6 Status Typ Gr°up I II III 1V V N High Status Mexicans 2 6 so 111 33 232 All Anglo-Americans 4 13 55 2o 6 98 N 6 19 135 131 39 330 Chi-square 3 36.6 P less than .001, d.f. 3 7 TABLE 46 HIGH STATUS hiEflCANS AND ALL AI‘ICLO-ALELICAI‘ES, DISTRIBUTION OF SCALE TYPES DERIVED FROM RESPONSES TO ROLE—CONFLICT SITUATIONS INVOLVING SEF-ORIH‘TTATION VERSUS COLLECTIVITY-OEUENTATION TOWARD THE PEER GROUP (SCALE IV) Status Scale Type Gm“? I II III IV v N High Status Mexicans 34 71 80 50 26 261 Ill Anglo-Americans 2 15 24 27 20 88 N 36 86 104 77 46 349 Chi-square I 17.5 P less than .01, d.f. I 7 TABLEIA7 HIGH STATUS IIIAICAIIS AND ALL AIGLO—AIAIIICAIS, DISTRIBUTION OF SCALE TYPES DERIVED mos-I RESPONSES TO ROLE-CONFLICT SITUATIONS INVOLVING AII ECONOMIC ROLE VERSUS A FAIvL'LLIAL ROLE (SCALE I) Scale 6 Status Typ GmuP I II III IV V N High Status Mexicans 51 54 61 44 23 233 All Anglo-Americans 6 22 28 26 ll 93 N 57 76 89 70 34 326 Chi-square 3 7.4 P less than .40, d.f. ‘ 7 TABLE 48 HIGH STATUS Ismcms AIID ALL prGLO-mh-II CAIIS, DISTRIBUTION OF SCALE TYPES DERIVED IROII RESPOZ-ISALS TO ROLE-COIIF'LICT SITUATIOIIS INVOLVING AII NS‘I‘RUEIETTAL ROLE VARSUS A FRIEIIDSIIIP ROLE (SCALE II) Scale 6 Status Typ “map I II III IV V N High Status Mexicans 17 50 72 77 46 262 All Anglo-Americans 5 12 23 21 27 88 N 22 62 95 98 73 350 Chi-square ' 2.2 P less than .95, d.f. ' 7 ‘7 (‘17:; differ significantly from one another. This was a particularly inter— esting finding since Mexican and.United States students do differ sig- nificantly from one another when social status is not controlled. The significance of this finding will be discussed further below. The scale score distributions of the high status Mexicans and the combined Anglo-American group then were compared on Scale II. See Table 48. In this case the chi-square value was significant at the .95 level, and this was not sufficient to reflect the null hypothesis. Hence, it was concluded that the role-expectation patterns of the high status Mexicans and the total Anglo-American group do not differ significantly from one another on this dimension. While the findings with respect to Scale I and II imply that the role-expectation patterns of high status Mexicans do not differ signi- ficantly from those of Anglo-Americans, the matter may not be dropped here. It will be recalled that social status did not appear to dif- ferentiate the role-expectation patterns of the Mexicans themselves. Hence, logically, the role-expectation patterns of the Anglo-Americans. should.not differ from those of the low status MexiCans. Further analy— ses showed, however, that the role-eXpectation patterns of the Anglo- Americans do differ significantly from those of the low status Mexicans on Scales I and II. See Tables 49, 50, and 51. It will be recalled, moreover, that Anglo-Americans' role—expectation patterns differed sig- nificantly from those of the total Mexican group on Scales I and II. See Tables 10 and 11. In view of these logically inconsistent findings a further analysis TABLE 49 —j‘ q :3— Low sums MEXICAHS AND HIGH STATUS Aime-Aaumcazs, DISI‘FJBLTION or SCALE TYPES DERIVED III-on RESPONSES TO ROLE-CONFLICT SITUATIONS INVOLVII‘EG AN ECONOMIC ROLE VERSUS A FAMILIAL ROLE (SCALE 1) Scale I 9 Status yp 61‘0“!) I II III Iv v N High Status Anglo-Americans 2 6 36 9 2 55 Lou'Status Mexicans 21 24 21 14 6 86 N 23 30 57 23 8 141 . Chi-square - 23.24 P less than .001, d.f. 3 7 TABLE 50 LOW STATUS EIEXICAITS AND LOW STATUS fl'filO-szCAI‘ES, DISTRIBUTION OF SCALE TYPES DEIVED FRO}?! RESPONSES TO ROLE-CONFLICT SITUATICNS II‘IVCL‘ 13G A}? ECONOLIIC ROLE VERSUS A FAMILIAL ROLE (SCALE I) E Scale Type Status . Group I & II III IV & V N Low Status Anglo-Americans 18 16 25 59 Low Status Mexicans 45 V 20 19 84 N. 63 36 44 143 Chi-square = 7.45 P less than .05, d.f. = 3 TABLE 51 LOW STATUS NECECANS AND ALL ANCEO-MZERICANS, DISTRIBUTION OF SCALE TYPES DEIDRE) FROM RESPONSES TO ROLE-CONFLICT SITUATIONS INVOLVING AN INSTRUI'E‘JTAL ROLE VERSUS A FRlflIDSHIP ROLE (SCALE II) Status Scale Type Gm”) I II III Iv . v N Low Status Mexicans 3 2'7 28 21 16 95 All Anglo-Americans 5 12 23 21 27 88 N 8 39 51 42 43 183 Chi-square 3 9.4 P less than .20, d.f. 3 7 TABLE 52 HIGH STATUS L'MCMIS, LOW STATUS L'MCANS, AND ALL ANGLO- AMMOANS, DISTRIBUTION OF SCALE TYPES DERIVED FROM RESPONSES TO ROLE-CONFLICT SITUATIONS INVOLVING AN ECONOLIC ROLE VERSUS A FAMILIAL ROLE (SCALE I) Scale Type Status Gmup I II III Iv v N High Status Mexicans 51 54 61 44 23 233 Low Status Mexicans 21 22 21 13 6 83 All Anglo-Americans 6 22 28 26 11 93 N 78 98 110 83 40 409 Chi-square I 15.9 P less than .20, d.f. S 12 was made. The scale score distributions of the high status Mexicans, low status Mexicans, and Anglo-Americans on Seals I were arranged in a table and submitted to a chi-square analysis. See Table 52. The level of significance of the chi-souare value was .20. This level of signi- ficance is too high to reject summarily the null hypothesis but, never- theless, may be taken as strong evidence that a difference does exist. On the assumption that a difference exists between these distributions, the mean rank of each group was Calculated. The low status Mexicans were found.to have a mean rank of 181.5, the high status Mexicans a mean rank of 198.8, and the Anglo-Americans a rank of 239.8 on Scale I. It would appear from the relative mean ranks of these groups that the distribution of scale scores of high status Mexicans differs slightly from those of both low status Mexicans and Anglo-Americans, lying as it were near a mid-point between these groups. Apparently this distribu- tion of high status Mexicans is arranged in such a way that it is bal- anced at a point between the low status Mexicans and Anglo-Americans where it does not differ from either group sufficiently to allow the difference to appear in the chi-sousre tests. The distribution of scale scores of the high status MexiCans on Scale I, then, may be interpreted in two different ways. It may be that high status Mexicans as a whole tend to fall in an intermediate pOSition between the low status Mexicans and the Anglo-Americans. On the other hand, it may be that some high status Mexicans resemble Anglo-Americans and other resemble low status MexiCans. Thus, these two patterns "aver- age out" at a point between low status MexiCans and Ang10,-. :3. :3 ;.: :1: :3 :j. :;: a. :1: >5- :‘,< >;: :;< )3 >;. :;: 3k :3: :;: 2;; 2|: 3i: :4: :k >;: at: )3: 51>}: 2:1 :1: up at: STORY 4 You are in charge of an office which provides a service to the public. Anyone who wishes to receive this service must make application through your office. It generally takes your office about a week to look over a person's application, check it, and give the final approval on it. But it is possible to push through an application in two days time if you give the order to do so. This morning your brother comes to your office and tells you that he needs your help. He needs to have his application checked and approved within two or three days. If he waits a full week as is usual it will make things very inconvenient for him. (a) How much right does your brother have to eXpect you to push through his application intwo days instead of makinghim wait a full week? my brother would have a definite right to expect me to push through his application in two days instead of making him wait a full week. Ly brother would have some right to expect me to push through his application in two days instead of making him wait a full week. My brother would have no right to expect me to push through his application in two days instead of making him.wait a full week. (b) Considering your obligations to a brother what do you think you would do in a situation like this? I think I would give the order to have my brother's application processed. I think that I would make my brother wait a full week. ' a. ...- .6- STORY 5 You are riding in a car driven by a close friend. Your friend turns a corner rather fast and the rear fender scrapes against a pole and is slightly damaged; Your friend decides to tell his father that the accident was un- avoidable and was not his fault. If his father thinks that the fender was damaged because of carelessness then your friend probably will not be allowed to use the car anymore. Actually, it is hard for you to decide whether the damage was or was not your friend's fault. (a) In a situation like this, what right does your friend have to expect 'you to support his story? Iy'friend has a definite right to eXpect me to support his story. Lw'friend has some right to eXpect me to support his story. L®'friend has 2g right to expect me to support his story. (b) Considering your obligation to your friend what do you think you would do? I think that I would support my friend' s story in a situation like this. *1 think that I would not support my friend's story in a situation like *this. *$***$#$$ufigfia$$ngblr*$*******$$$$$$$**$ STORY 6 You and a close friend sit near each other in class. Your friend has done very poorly on his examinations this term. One day an important examin- ation is to be given. If your friend doesn't pass this examination he will probably have to take the course over again next erm. Your friend asks you to help him on the examination by turning your paper so that he can see it during the exam. This way there is very little chance that either of you will be caught. (a) What right does your friend have to expect you to let him copy off your paper? My friend would have a definite right to expect me to let him.see my paper. my friend would have some right to exPect me to let him.see my paper. my friend would have 39 right to expect me to let him see my paper. (b) Considering your obligations to a friend in a situation like this, what do you think you would do? I would probably tell my friend that I would help him out. I would probably tell my friend that I was sorry, but I couldn't help him.sut. ..7- STORY 7 One day a close friend of yours stops to talk to you and ask your help. He says that last night he was out riding around in a car with some boys. It turned out that the boy driving the car had stolen it earlier that same evening. Anyway, the police had picked them up and all of the boys were accused of taking part in stealing the car. Your friend wants you to testify that he was with you at the time the car was stolen. Although he wasn't really with you at that time you are absolutely sure that he didn't help steal the car. If you will testify at court that your friend was with you at the time the car was stolen, then your friend may avoid very serious trouble with the police. (a) In a situation like this, what right does your friend have to expect you to testify in court in his favor? He would have a definite right to eXpect me to testify in his favor. He would have some right to expect me to testify in his favor. He would have 39 right to expect me to testify in his favor. * (b) Considering your obligations to him as a close friend, what do you think you would do? I think that I would testify in court that he was with me at the time the car was stolen so that he wouldn't get into serious trouble with the police. I think that I would not help him out in a situation like this, even if it meant that he would get into serious trouble with the police. """ " "' '.-'.‘l-.-.-' -.'.-'.'..‘ ‘.’... .'. * :1: a: 3,: a: a: 3,: :5 3:: 3.: >5 :2: :,: :5 =,: :j: :,. :3 :5 .5 ;.: >5 .5 .5 :.¢ 2:: :.. ... ;.: ., ..z :5. :_- :.. 9,. ... >3. STORY 8 Suppose that an important person, like the governor of the state, were to visit this school, and this class were asked to send three of its members to greet him. In a situation like this what three students in this class would you pick to do this? (1) (2) (5) .-n -5- THEY 9 You are standing in line to eat lunch at the school cafeteria. There is a long line of students, but you are up near the head of the line. A good friend of yours comes up to you and asks you if he can get in line with you. He says that he has to get to a meeting shortly and if he waits at the end of the line he will be late for the meeting. You are pretty sure that he doesn't have any such meeting, but you tell him that it's O.K. with you if he gets in line ahead of you. Then the other students begin to complain and tell him that he hasn't any right to get in line aheadtf‘the rest. If you don't back up your friend's story by telling the other students that he has an important meeting, then they will make your friend go to the end of the line. (a) In a circumstance like this, what right does your friend have to expect you to support him? .My friend has a definite right to expect me to support him. by friend has some right to expect me to support him. my friend has gg_right to expect me to support him. (b) Considering your obligations to a close friend, what do you think you would do? I think that I would support my friend so that he wouldn't have to go to the end of the line. I think that I would not support my friend, even if it meant that he would have to go to the end of the line. *********>§=32<*=2€*>F¥ >I‘*>l~‘*=!==s***$******A=$****** STORY 10 Most of your friends have joined a club. They have a good time together, and you want to join the club, too. You feel left out and lonely because you are not a member and all of your friends are members. Your parents have heard that a couple of the members of the club have bad reputations; they have heard that some of them have been in trouble with the police. Because of this your parents don't want you to join the club. If you don't join the club you won't be able to take part in most of the activities of your friends. (a) In a situation like this how much right do your parents have to expect you not to join the club? They would have adefinite right to eXpect me not to join the club. They would have gome right to expect me not to join the club. They would have 29 right to expect me not to join the club. (b) Considering your own interests and your obligations to your parents, what do you think you would do if the decision were left up to you? I think that I would join the club so that I could continue to be with my friends, even if my parents weren't very happy about it. I think that I would give up the idea of joining the club, and follow the wishes of my parents. ..9- SKEY 11 You go around with a small group of friends. You like them very much and they like you. Usually you have a good time when with them. So far as you are concerned they are a good bunch of kids. Then one evening your parents begin asking you a lot of questions about this group of friends of your. They have heard that some of them have been in trouble with the police at various times, and they are worried about your going around with them. Finally they tell you that they would prefer that you stop going around with these friends, and they ask you not to see them anymore. (a) In circumstances such as these, how much right do you think your parents would have to expect you to stop seeing your friends anymore? They would have a definite right to expect me to stop seeing my friends. They would have some right to expect me to stop seeing my friends. They would have pg right to expect me to stop seeing my friends. (b) Considering your own interests as well as your obligations to your parents, what do you think you would do if the decision were up to you to make? I think that I would continue to go around with my friends, even if this didn't entirely please my parents. I think that I would stop seeing my friends and stop going around with them if my parents wanted me to do so. **$*a**$#*****************$$$$$$$$$$a$$$ STORY 12 You go around with a group of friends about your own ago. One after- noon you and your friends are standing around talking and trying to decide what to do this evening. After standing and arguing for half an hour or so most of them decide they want to go over and watch television at one of the kid's home. The program they want to see begins in 15 minutes so you'll have to hurry. But you don't want to watch television tonight and you start arguing with the rest of them that there's a good movie downtown. They tell you to quit arguing and come along now or they will miss the beginning of the program. But you know that if you can talk a couple of them into coming to the movie with you, then the rest of them will probably come, too. (a) In a situation like this, what right does the group have to expect you to quit arguing and come along to watch television with them? The group has a definite right to expect me to come watch television with them. The group has some right to eXpect me to come watch television with them. The group has 29 right to expect me to come watch television with them. (b) Considering your own interests and the interests of the group what do you think you would do? I think that I would continue to argue and try to get some of them to go to the movie with me. , I think that I would stop arguing and go watch television with the rest of the group. ..9... flDRY 11 You go around with a small group of friends. You like them very much and they like you. Usually you have a good time when with them. So far as you are concerned they are a good bunch of kids. Then one evening your parents begin asking you a lot of questions about this group of friends of your. They have heard that some of them have been in trouble with the police at various times, and they are worried about your going around with them. Finally they tell you that they would prefer that you stop going around with these friends, and they ask you not to see them anymore. (a) In circumstances such as these, how much right do you think your parents would have to eXpect you to stop seeing your friends anymore? They would have a definite right to expect me to stop seeing my friends. They would have some right to expect me to stop seeing my friends. They would have pg right to expect me to stop seeing my friends. (b) Considering your own interests as well as your obligations to your parents, what do you think you would do if the decision were up to you to make? I think that I would continue to go around with my friends, even if this didn't entirely please my parents. I think that I would stop seeing my friends and stop going around with them if my parents wanted me to do so. **$*=. **¥VX*******XK****ia‘ =>* =*: ns=3w*** STORY 12 You go around with a group of friends about your own age. One after- noon you and your friends are standing around talking and trying to decide what to do this evening. After standing and arguing for half an hour or so most of them decide they want to go over and watch television at one of the kid's home. The program they want to see begins in 15 minutes so you'll have to hurry. But you don't want to watch television tonight and you start arguing with the rest of them that there's a good movie downtown. They tell you to quit arguing and come along now or they will miss the beginning of the program. But you know that if you can talk a couple of them into coming to the movie with you, then the rest of them will probably come, too. (a) In a situation like this, what right does the group have to expect you to quit arguing and come along to watch television with them? The group has a deflgite right to expect me to come watch television with them. The group has some right to eXpect me to come watch television with them. The group has 39 right to expect me to come watch television with them. (b) Considering your own interests and the interests of the group what do you think you would do? I think that I would continue to argue and try to get scie of them to go to the movie with me. , I think that I would stop arguing and go watch television with the rest of the group. THEY 13 You belong to a club. One day at a meeting of all the group an argument develops between you and another member. The argument gets pretty hot because both of you feel strongly about the subject. In the midst of the argument the other member tells you that you don't know what you are talking about, that you are stupid, and he thoroughly insults you. This, of course, makes you mad, but if the argument goes on or a quarrel starts it probably will break up the meeting of the group. (a) In a situation like this, what right does the rest of the group have to expect you to sit down and quit arguing? The rest of the group has a definite right to expect me to sit down and quit arguing. The rest of the group has some right to expect me to sit down and quit arguing. The rest of the group has 39 right to expect me to sit down and quit arguing. ' (b) Considering your own interest as well as the interest of the group, what do you think you would do in a situation like this? _I think that I would not let the other member get away with insulting me, even if it did break up the meeting of the group. I think that I would not break up the meeting of the group just because this other member insulted me. e12. SHEY 14 You and some of your close friends are standing around talking one evening. You have been thinking about going to a movie downtown which you believe will be a particularly good one. Tonight is the last night it will be showing in town. You try to interest your friends in going to see this movie, but they tell you it's no good. They want to go see a different movie, and they want you to come along with them. Your friends start telling you that you never seem to want to do what they do anymore. (a) In a circumstance like this, what right do your friends have to expect you to go along with them to see the movie they are interested in? my friends would have a definite right to expect me to go along with them. NV friends would have some right to expect me to go along with them. My friends would have ég right to expect me to go along with them. (b) Considering your interest in seeing the other movie, and your obligations to your friends, what do you think you would do? Go with my friends to see the movie they were interested in, and maybe see the movie I was most interested in at some later date. Go to the movie in which I was most interested, even if my friends did get mad about it. ************************************ TORY "15 You own a car. You and some of your friends have been planning to take a short trip over the week-end. You have promised them that they can go in your car, and so they have based all their plans on the fact that they will go in your car. Then, the day before you are supposed to leave with them on the trip a good friend of yours who lives in California comes to town. This friend will be in El Paso just for the week-end, and you would like to spend as much time with this friend as possible. If you stay in El Paso to visit with your friend fromlDalifornia as you would like to do then the trip you and your friends had planned on taking will be ruined. (a) In a situation like this, what right do your friends with whom you plan to take the trip have to expect you to go on the trip as planned? .My friends would have a definite right to expect me to go on the trip as planned. NV friends would have some right to expect me to go on the trip as planned. by friends would have g9 right to expect me to go on the trip as planned. (b) Considering your own interests in seeing your friend from California and your obligations to your other friends to take them.on the trip, what do you think you would do? I think that I would stay in El Paso and see my friend from.California, even if my friends whom I planned to take on the trip did get mad about it. I think that I would go ahead with the trip even if I would prefer to visit with my friend from California. THEY 15 You belong to a club. One day at a meeting of all the group an argument develops between you and another member. The argument gets pretty hot because both of you feel strongly about the subject. In the midst of the argument the other member tells you that you don't know what you are talking about, that you are stupid, and he thoroughly insults you. This, of course, makes you mad, but if the argument goes on or a quarrel starts it probably will break up the meeting of the group. (a) In a situation like this, what right does the rest of the group have to expect you to sit down and quit arguing? The rest of the group has a definite right to expect me to sit down and quit arguing. The rest of the group has some right to expect me to sit down and quit arguing. The rest of the group has 29 right to expect me to sit down and quit arguing. (b) Considering your own interest as well as the interest of the group, what do you think you would do in a situation like this? I think that I would not let the other member get away with insulting me, even if it did break up the meeting of the group. I think that I would not break up the meeting of the group just because this other member insulted me. .12. new 14 You and some of your close friends are standing around talking one evening. You have been thinking about going to a movie downtown which you believe will be a particularly good one. Tonight is the last night it will be showing in town. You try to interest your friends in going to see this movie, but they tell you it's no good. They want to go see a different movie, and they want you to come along with them. Your friends start telling you that you never seem to want to do what they do anymore. (a) In a circumstance like this, what right do your friends have to expect you to go along with than to see the movie they are interested in? up friends would have a definite right to expect me to go along with them. my friends would have some right to expect me to go along with them. my friends would have §9_ right to expect me to go along with them. (b0 Considering your interest in seeing the other movie, and your obligations to your friends, what do you think you would do? Go with my friends to see the movie they were interested in, and maybe see the movie I was most interested in at some later date. Go to the movie in which I was most interested, even if my friends did get mad about it. ************************************ I‘ORY '15 You own a car. You and some of your friends have been planning to take a short trip over the week-end. You have promised them.that they can go in your car, and so they have based all their plans on the fact that they will go in your car. Then, the day before you are supposed to leave with them.on the trip a good friend of yours who lives in California comes to town. This friend will be in El Paso just for the week—end, and you would like to spend as much time with this friend as possible. If you stay in El Paso to visit with your friend from California as you would like to do then the trip you and your friends had planned on taking will be ruined. (a) In a situation like this, what right do your friends with whom you plan to take the trip have to expect you to go on the trip as planned? my friends would have a definite right to expect me to go on the trip as planned. My friends would have some right to expect me to go on the trip as planned. my friends would have 29 right to expect me to go on the trip as planned. (b) Considering your own interests in seeing your friend from California and your obligations to your other friends to take them on the trip, what do you think you would do? I think that I would stay in El Paso and see my friend from California, even if my friends whom I planned to take on the trip did get mad about it. I think that I would go ahead with the trip even if I would prefer to visit with my friend from California. ”113+- swarr7rda BOYS Your father owns a store. Your parents want you to go to work in TMS the store after finishing high school. They hope that you will eventually take over the store when your father retires. Your own interests, however, don't lie along these lines. You have worked in the store and you know that you don't like that kind of work veby much. On the other hand, you have a real interest in becoming a skilled mechanic. You do know that you have a consider- able amount of talent for mechanics, and you are sure of getting a good job. (a) In a circumstance like this, what right do your parents have to expect you to give up mechanics and go to work in your father's store? my parents would have a definite right to expect me to give up mechanics and go to work in their store. my parents would have some right to expect me to give up mechanics and go to work in their store. my parents would have 2g right to expect me to give up mechanics and go to work in their store. (b)¢30neidering your own interests and your obligations to your parents, what do you think you would do if the decision were up to you? I would probably go ahead and follow my own interest and try to become a mechanic. I would probably go to work for my father in his store and give up the idea of becoming a mechanic. ***#***************$=L=351*>.==':¢c‘.-*2:*=2-¥>2==i81<>1<* NRnyFOR GIRLS Some close friends of your parents have a son who is just about your age. Ever since you were born, your parents have planned that you would marry this boy. You and this boy like each other, but you are not in love with each other. Besides that, you want to be a nurse. After finishing high school, you want to enter nurse's training. (a) In a circumstance like this, what right do your parents have to expect you to marry this boy as soon as you graduate from high school? by'parents would have a definite right to expect me to marry the boy. My parents would have some right to expect me to marry this boy. my parents would have‘gg right to expect me to marry this boy. (b) Considering your own interests and your obligations to your parents, what do you think you would do if the decision were up to you? I think that I would probably go ahead and follow my own interest and enter nurse's training when I finished high school. I think I would probably go ahead and marry the boy when I finished high school and give up the idea of becoming a nurse. ”.11.. STORY 17 FOR BOYS Your father owns a store. Your parents want you to go to work in fiHS STORY NR BOYS ' the store after finishing high school. They hope that you will eventually take over the store when your father retires. Your own interests, however, don't lie along these lines. You have worked in the store and you know that you don't like that kind of work vefiy much. On the other hand, you have a real interest in becoming a skilled mechanic. You do know that you have a consider- able ameunt of talent for mechanics, and you are sure of getting a good job. (a) In a circumstance like this, what right do your parents have to expect you to give up mechanics and go to work in your father's store? Ry parents would have a definite right to expect me to give up mechanics and go to work in their store. aw parents would have some right to expect me to give up mechanics and go to work in their store. my parents would have g2 right to expect me to give up mechanics and go to work in their store. (b) Considering your own interests and your obligations to your parents, what do you think you would do if the decision were up to you? I would probably go ahead and follow my own interest and try to become a mechanic. I would probably go to work for my father in his store and give up the idea of becoming a mechanic. *******************3}=3L:=1=*?:==é’-:.~'t=>.;=i==L-3it>2=‘rl3*>l<3!! TORY f-TFOR GIRLS Some close friends of your parents have a son who is just about HIS TORY DR [RLS your age. Ever since you were born, your parents have planned that you would marry this boy. You and this boy like each other, but you are not in love with each other. Besides that, you want to be a nurse. After finishing high school, you want to enter nurse's training. (a) In a circumstance like this, what right do your parents have to expect you to marry this boy as soon as you graduate from high school? my parents would have a definite right to expect me to marry the boy. Hy parents would have some right to expect me to marry this boy. My parents would have g9 right to expect me to marry this boy. (b) Considering your own interests and your obligations to your parents, what do you think you would do if the decision were up to you? ' I think that I would probably go ahead and follow my own interest and enter nurse's training when I finished high school. I think I would probably go ahead and marry the boy when I finished high school and give up the idea of becoming a nurse. J o. . . I .3 . e x, . . . . a. . V 4 a, I. . ' . i . . . . . . .. , . . . . l e . _ o o . . r. . A . . . . . o . . . . .. . .. . . . a. .. . . . , . .. . 1. . o \ .. . . 1 . . . a . . o . n , a . :15- flfiWIBFOR BOYS There is a girl at school whom you know and whom you like quite THIS STORY mm BOYS well. One day you ask her to go out on a date to a dance, and she accepts. When you get home from school you tell your mother that you have asked this girl to a dance and your mother gets all upset. She says that she doesn't like this girl and doesn't want you to date her. Your mother doesn't say why she doesn't like this girl, but she makes it clear that she doesn't want you to date her. Cnithe other hand, you have been looking forward to dating this girl, and if you don't take her to the dance after having asked her, then she probably won't ever accept another invitation. (a) In a circumstance like this, what right does your mother have to expect you to break your date with this girl? by'mother has a definite right to expect me to break my date with this girl. My mother has some right to expect me to break my date with this girl. hy'mother has g9 right to expect me to break my date with this girl. (b) Considering your own interests as well as your obligations to your mother, what do you think you would do? I think that I would go ahead and take the girl to the dance like I had planned. I think that I would call her and tell her that I wouldn't be able to take her to the dance. ***$$$$$m**$$$$¥$$¥$$$m$L$$¥$$v*$*** RRYI8FOR GIRLS There is a boy at school whom you know and whom you like quite WY IRLS well. One day he asks you to go out on a date to a dance, and you accept. When you get home from school you tell your mother that this boy has asked you to go to a dance with him and that you accepted his invitation. Then your mother gets upset and says that she doesn't like this boy and she doesn't want you to have a date with him. Your mother doesn't say why she doesn't like the boy, but she makes it clear that she doesn't want you to date him. On the other hand, you have been looking forward to having a date with this boy, and if you don't go out with him after having promised him that you would, then he probably won't ever ask you again. (a) In a circumstance like this what right does your mother have to expect you to call off the date with this boy? my mother would have a definite right to expect me to call off the date. my mother would have some right to expect me to call off the date. my mother would have g3 right to expect me to call off the date. (b) Considering your own interests as well as your obligations to your mother, what do you think you would do? I think that I would go ahead and go to the dance with the boy like I had planned. I think that I would call the boy and tell him that I wouldn't be able to go to the dance with him. I' 4. a l I.” ‘- SELECTED BIBLIOGRAPHY Angell, Robert C. Th2 horgl Integration 9; égerican Cities. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1951. Axelrod, Morris. Urban Structure and Social Participation, Amerjch W Em, V01. 21’ No. 1, Feb. 1956, pp. 13‘180 Beale, Ralph.L. and Norman Humphrey. Kg Frontiers 39 Lcarggng: Thg Eggiggg Spgdgnt 1g the United Stgtefi, unpublished manuscript. Bell, Randell, and Maryanne T. Force. Urban Neignborhood Types and Participation in Fbrmal Associations, Americap §g§iglggi§gl ReView, Vol. 21, No. 1, Feb. 1956, pp. 25-34. Broom, Leonard, and Eshref Shevky. Mexicans in the United States: A Problem in Social Differentiation, Socioloefi gag Social Resegrch. Vol. 36, January, 1952, pp. ISO-158. Call, Tomme Clark. Th2 hexiggg Venture: From Pgliticg; t9 Industrial Revolutign ip Magic . Oxford University Press, 1953. Cumberland, Charles. The Mexican Revolution, The New Leader, December, 1954, V01. 27, pp. 12-170 Davis, Allison and Hebert Havighurst. Father 9f thg Egg. Boston: Boughton Mifflin 00., 1947. Davis, Kingsley. Adolescence and the Social Structure, 23g Aggglg 9; Baliiisal and. facial ......asScien . NMember 1944, pp. 11-16. Dotson, Floyd. A.Note on Participation in Voluntary Associations in a Mexican City. Amazican Sgcjolgricg] Review, Vol. 18, No. 4, August 1953, pp. 380-386. Durkheim, Emile. Thg Division 9; Labor 1Q Socjepz. Translated from the French.by George Simpson. Glencoe, Illinois: The Free Press, 1949. flail-£1.62. A £611.12 in éasialpm. Translated by «LA- Spaulding and George Simpson. Glencoe, 111.: The Free Press, 1951. Elldn, F. and W. Westley. The Myth of Adolescent Culture. Americgn §gg19lgg1ggl Rgvigw, Vol. 20, No. 6, pp. 680-685. Festinger, Leon, S. Schacter, and Kurt Black. Social Preseuree 1e IDIQEEQI Grogpa. New York: Harper and Brothers, 1950. Flores, Antonio Carrillo. Industrial fiexico, ~1ected Readincs 9e Fegjg 3.5 Seminar ea Forei~n Affaire. mimeographed, hichigan State University, 1957. Fbskett, John M. Social Structure and Social Participation. Amegiceg §921219£i£§l Review, Vol. 20, No. 4, August 1955, pp. 431-438. Gamio, Manuel. Cultural Patterns in Modern mexico. geartezlx Jgurne] 21; Bier-Amman Matisse. Vol- 1, 1939, pp. 49-61. Gerth, H.H. and C. Wright Mills. From.ng Heber: Eseave 2g Sociology. Oxford University Press, 1946. Gillen, John. Modern Latin American Culture. §ecia1 Forcefi, Vol. 26, 1948, pp. 243'252. Huxlphrey, Norman. The Cultural Background of the Mexican Emigrant. Banal W, Vol- 13. 1948, pp. 239-246. Iturriaga, Jose B. La eatmctura M x eglturg-J fie tiexi e9. Mexico, D.F.: Fondo de Cultura Economics, 1951. Jones, Robert. Hexican'Youth in the U.S. The Aperican Teacher, Vol. 28, March 1944, pp. 3.1-1.5. Komarovsky, Mirra. Patterns of Voluntary Association among Urban Dwellers. geezieeg §9c1019§ica1 Re ’ew, Vol. 11, No. 4, December 1946, pp. 686-~98. Linton, Ralph. The Studv 9; Egg. New York: Appleton-Century Co., 1936. Mayo, Elton. The Human Problems eg ge industrial Civilization. Boston: .5 _— Harvard University Press, 1933. Mead, Margaret. Coming e_i: Age g me. New York: WM. Morrow Co., 1928. Monzon, Arturo. Planteamiento de Algunas Problemas Indigneas, America indigena, Vol. VII, No. 4, Cctdbre 1957, pp. 323-331. moors, Wilbert E. Industri ‘z t' n geengbor. Ithaca, N.Y.: Cornell University Press, 1951. Nall, Frank. A Study ef the Internatigna; Farm Youth Exchanre Prowreg ig‘yezie_. Unpublished EA thesis, Michigan State College, 1954. NorthrOp, F.S.C. The mg; 91‘ fige Sciences egg 1h_e I—hnaeities. Yale University Press, 1947. Parkes, Henry Bamford. A Historv‘eg Mexico. New York: Houghton Mifflin Co., 1950. Parsons, Talcott, The Structure 9: Social Action. Neerork: KcGraWb Hill, 1937. The Socjel Syetem. Glencoe, 111.: The Free Press, 1951. , and Edward Shils, editors. Toward g General Theogx 2: Ac n. Cambridge: Harvard University Press, 1951. , Robert Bales, and Edward Shils. W rE' n0 Pipers i_t Thegzy g1 Aciign. Glencoe, 111.: The Free Press, 1953. and Robert Bales. Family: Socializationfl dInterectjgn Ereceea. Glencoe, 111.: The Free Press, 1955. Age and Sex in the Social Structure of the United States. QHQI Egan ECiOlQé’ical Rev: 8W, v01. 7, NO. 5’ pp. 604-616. Psychoanalysis and the Social Structure. P c n t'c Riesman, David. The Lenelv Crewd. New Haven: Yale University Press, 1950. Rivera, Julio. antact; gag Attitudes toward the United §t ates 123 Mexicag Bgrde; Congpgitz. Unpublished Ph .D. Dissertation, Michigan State University, 1957. Roethlisberger, F.J. and W.J. Dickson. Manager ent 11d the worker. Boston: Harvard University Press, 1934. Schatzman, Leonard, and Anselm Strauss. Social Class and Modes of Communication. .Amenjcan Journal 2£.§921212£X; V01. LX, N0. 4, January 1955 , pp. 329-338. Schneider, Louis, and S. Lysgaard. The Deferred Gratification Pattern. W W limes. Vol. 18. N0. 2, April 1953, pp. 142-149. Senter, Donovan. Acculturation.Among New hexican Villagers in Comparison to Adjustment Patterns of other Spanish-speaking Americans. Repel W. Vol. 10, 1945, pp. 310-347. Simmons, Ozzie. An~lo~American e_e Mexican-Americans 13 South Texas, ‘5 Studx' n Dominant-Subprd.inate Group Relatipng. Unpublished Ph.D. Dissertation, HarVard University, 1952. Simpson, Lesley Byrd. Unplanned Effects of Mexico's Planned Economy. The W 9132122211 3.82123, Vol. 29. N0. 4, 1953, pp. 514-532. Smith, T. Lynn. Values Held by People in Latin America Which Affect Technical COOperation. Rural Sociology, Vol. , No. Stouffer, Samuel, e3. al.: Heasurenent egg Prediction. Princeton: Princeton University Press, 1950. Tannenbaum, Frank. Hexico: The trugrle fee Peace and Bread. New York: Alfred A. KnOpf Co., 1951. THOR, Ruth. Not With the First: Nexican-epericans ie 3 Southwest Citv. New York: Harcourt Brace and Co., 1949. United States Department of Commerce. Government and Busineee 13 hexicg, investment ig hexice. Government Printing Office, 1955. Natson, James B., and Julian Samora. subordinate Leadership in a Bi- Cultural Community: An Analysis. American Secjolosical Regiee, Vol. 19, No. 4, August 1954, pp. 413-421. Whetten, Nathan L. Rural Mexico. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1948. Whyte, W.F. _greet Corner Society. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1943. fihyte, W.H. The Transients, Fortune, May, June, July, August 1953. Williams, Robin. Armrjcgign ficie‘fl. New York: Alfred A. Knopf, 1952. 1,291: a .‘ '3 -} k . 1.2:1 \ ._ - ..-- _eu IIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIII lllllflifllllfllfllllfllfll [mm«minimum: 9 9