7'J‘rvitii’ '. ”a [A 5.7 - '4, .~- €&-1‘ .‘u‘fifi‘;~ .JQII. b‘ut‘fi‘ UnéW ELI- This is to certify that the thesis entitled Coping Responses and Adaptational Outcomes of Children Undergoing Orthopedic Surgery presented by Paul M. Robins has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for Ph.D. degree in School Psychology 06». Ram...“ Major professor [fine February 14, 1986 0-7639 MS U is an Affirmative Action/Equal Opportunity Institution MSU LIBRARIES .5315.- RETURNING MATERIALS: Place in book drop to remove this checkout from your record. FINES will be charged if book is returned after the date stamped below. COPING RESPONSES AND ADAPTATIONAL OUTCOMES OF CHILDREN UNDERGOING ORTHOPEDIC SURGERY BY Paul M. Robins A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY College of Education Department of Counseling, Educational Psychologyr and Special Education 1986 ABSTRACT COPING RESPONSES AND ADAPTATIONAL OUTCOMES OF CHILDREN UNDERGOING ORTHOPEDIC SURGERY BY Paul M. Robins This study examined the relationships between coping responses and adaptational outcomes of children undergoing elective orthopedic surgery. Knowledge of these relationships is needed in order to prevent possible negative reactions due to hospitalization and surgery. A sample of 27 children, ages 6 through 17, undergoing orthopedic surgery at a large, urban children's hospital participated in the study. The Roberts Apperception Test for Children, used to measure surgical coping responses, was administered preceeding and following surgery. The Revised Behavior Problem Checklist, used to measure surgical adaptation, was administered prior to discharge and one month following discharge. The Personality Inventory for Children, used to measure cognitive functioning and premorbid adjustment, was administered immediately prior to surgery. Eight hypotheses related to children's responses to Paul H. Robins hospitalization and surgery were developed and tested. Descriptive case analyses were also presented. Children who scored higher on measures of surgical coping generally did not exhibit better surgical adaptation than those who scored lower. However, children who perceived appropriate and constructive limits placed on their behaviors exhibited better surgical adaptation one month later. In addition, children who used greater numbers of coping responses exhibited less anxiety and withdrawal after surgery. Children's coping responses did not significantly differ preceeding surgery versus following surgery. Children with higher cognitive functioning, shorter periods of hospitalization, and fewer hospital admissions did not exhibit better surgical adaptation. Girls did not show better surgical adaptation than boys, nor did older children show better surgical adaptation than younger children. Children with better premorbid adjustments showed more positive long-term surgical adaptations than did children with poorer premorbid adjustments. The results suggested that the majority of children demonstrated significant “stress resistance." Factors contributing to such resistance and implications for children at risk for developing psychological complications were discussed. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS Writing a dissertation is much like running a marathon: ultimately, it is a very lonely experience, but aid is essential along the route in order to keep going. I have been fortunate, for a number of_ individuals helped me complete the project. Don Hamachek functioned as my academic advisor and dissertation chairperson. His utterly pragmatic approach in all phases of the research helped me learn that the dissertation, although a necessity, was only a starting point in my career. I appreciated his prompt and careful readings of the manuscripts and his support for my developing abilities as a researcher and psychologist. Steve Raudenbush helped me design the study and analyze the data. His questions encouraged me to increasingly clarify my research hypotheses, while his expertise as a methodologist, but more importantly as a teacher and scholar, helped me make sense of sometimes overwhelming amounts of data. Our work sometimes occured over long distance telephone; he was both responsive and responsible and was very appreciated. Harvey Clarizio and Martha Karson also functioned as committee members. Harvey's academic expertise and input during long individual discussions, especially ii during my early years of graduate study, stimulated my academic growth and competence. Martha helped me develop and expand my clinical skills and believe in the process of psychotherapy and change. The staff at Children's Hospital of Michigan assisted me in the nuts and bolts of the research. Particular appreciation is expressed to Steve Spector, Director of Psychology Training. He helped me further refine the study and was instrumental in obtaining the subject population. Joseph Fischhoff, Chief of Psychiatry, kindly read and critiqued my proposal, and sponsored the research. Appreciation is extended to Richard LaMont, Chief of Orthopedic Surgery, and David Aronson, Assistant Chief of Orthopedic Surgery, as they graciously agreed to support the research with their private and clinic patients. Pat Newman, Department of Orthopedics, and her office staff helped me recruit and track subjects, despite many other job obligations. I am indebted to the children and parents who participated in the study. Despite anxiety and physical discomfort they shared their experiences with me in order to possibly help other children facing similar operations. My parents have always encouraged and supported my goals, both academic and personal. Their belief in me has never dimmed. Finally and most important, to Robin I am most iii thankful. She shared with me on a daily basis both the victories and defeats of the dissertation process. Her love, caring, encouragment, and competence helped provide the emotional groundwork so necessary in order for me to see the finish line. iv TABLE OF CONTENTS Page LIST OF TABLES COO...I00......OOOOOOOOOOOOOOOO..0. Vii LIST OF FIGURES .0...O0......OOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOO Viii Chapter I. THE PROBLEM ............................... 1 Introduction ............................ 1 Statement of the Problem ................ 2 Need for the Study ...................... 2 Purpose 0.0.00.0...OOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOIOOOOO 4 Theory .0...0......OOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOO 5 The Concept of Stress ................. 5 Interactionism as Applied to the Concepts of Stress and Coping ....... 9 A Cognitive Model of Stress Within an Interactional Framework ............. 11 Definition Of Terms OOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOO 15 Hypotheses OOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOO 16 II. REVIEW OF THE LITERATURE .................. 17 Stress and Children ..................... l7 Hospitalization and Surgery as Stressors in Childhood ................ 22 The Structure of Coping Responses........ 31 Common Coping Responses................ 31 The Relationships Between Coping Responses and Outcomes............... 34 Summary of Stress Research............... 44 Conclusions.............................. 46 III. DESIGN OF THE STUDY OOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOO 49 Selection and Description of the Sample . 49 measures 0..OOOOOOOOOOOOOIOOOOOOOOOIIO... 52 Procedures for Collecting Data .......... 63 Design................................... 66 Research Hypotheses ..................... 67 Ana1YSj-S O00......OOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOO 69 Supplementary Descriptive Data Analysis.. 74 summary...000......OOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOO 74 Page Iv. RESULTSOOOOOOOIOCOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOO00.....0. 77 Summary Statistics of Surgical Coping and Adaptation......................... 77 Summary Statistics of Intrinsic and Extrinsic Stress Factors............... 78 Test of the Hypotheses................... 81 Descriptive Case Analysis ............... 91 summary Of the FindinQBOOOOOOIOOI00...... 101 V. DISCUSSION AND CONCLUSION ................. 104 summary OOOOOOOOOO0.000000000000000.0.0.... 104 Discussion of the Hypotheses .............. 107 Limitations Of the Stuay OOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOIO 125 Implications of the Study ................. 127 Directions for Future Research ............ 129 conC1USion O0.0.00000000000COOOOOOOOOOC.0.0 131 APPENDICES A. CONSENT FORM OOOOOOOOOOOCOIOOOO0.00...0.... 133 B. ROBERTS APPERCEPTION TEST FOR CHILDREN .... 136 C. PERSONALITY INVENTORY FOR CHILDREN ........ 142 D. REVISED PROBLEM BEHAVIOR CHECKLIST ........ 151 LIST OF REFERENCES 0..O...OOOOOOOCOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOO 156 vi Table 3.1 3.2 4.1 4.2 4.3 4.5 4.6 4.7 4.8 LIST OF TABLES Page Description of Subject Sample Agexsexxnace (N=27) OOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOO 52 Description of Subject Sample by Hospital History (N=27) ................. 52 Descriptive Statistics for Surgical Coping and Adaptational Outcome Scales ......... 79 Descriptive Statistics for the Intrin81c variables OOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOIO 81 Descriptive Statistics for the Extrinsic Variables ..................... 81 Correlation Coefficients Between the Total Surgical Coping Scores (RATC) and the Total Surgical Adaptation Scores (RBPC) . 84 Paired Difference T-test Results for Presurgical (RATC-l) and Postsurgical (RATC-Z) Coping Response Scales ......... 86 T-test Results for Postsurgical Adaptation (RATC-l) and Long-term Surgical Adaptation (RATC-2) Mean Scores for Males (M) and Females (F) ............... 89 Summary of Bivariate Correlation Coefficients for Hypotheses Related to Intrinsic Stress Factors ................ 89 Summary of Bivariate Correlation Coefficients for Hypotheses Related to Extrinsic Stress Factors ................ 91 vii LIST OF FIGURES Figure Page 1. An Interactional Model of Stress, Coping, and Adaptation .................. 12 2. Data Collection Points and Measures ....... 66 viii CHAPTER 1 THE PROBLEM Intredngtign Hospitalization affects millions of children yearly. Three and a half to four million children under 15 years of age are hospitalized each year and one third of all young people will be hospitalized once or more by the time they become adults (Prugh & Jordan, 1975). The psychological effects of hospitalization on children has long been an area of inquiry (cf., Vernon, Foley, Sipowicz, & Schulman, 1965, for review). Children view hospitalization as a very stressful experience (Coddington, 1972; Dohrenwend & Dohrenwend, 1980), and as many as 20—36% of hospitalized children show demonstrable emotional difficulties after . hospitalization (Cook, 1967; Davenport & Werry, 1970). Very little research has examined the correlations between surgical coping responses and adaptational outcomes in children undergoing surgery using psychometrically sound instruments. It is not enough to know what goes wrong in the lives of children (Blom, 1958; 1984). Much can also be learned by studying how children successfully cope with the stresses associated with hospitalization and surgery. Statement 91 the Preblem Children experience many emotional problems due to hospitalization, as documented in the pediatric literature (Prugh & Jordan, 1975). Aspects of the hospitalization experience itself are stressful for children (Cook, 1967; Prugh & Jordan, 1975; Rutter, 1983; Willis, Elliot, 5 Jay, 1982; Wolff, 1973; Wright, Schaefer, & Solomons, 1979). Furthermore, surgical procedures add to the perception of stress (Kliman, 1968; Vernon et a1., 1965; Wolff, 1973; Wright et a1., 1979). Yet, the vast majority of children do not experience long-term psychological upset following hospitalization and surgery. In fact, some children experience psychological benefit from the experience (Lambert, 1984; Vernon & Shulman, 1964). What characteristics differentiate these children from those who do not cope so well? More specifically, are there cognitive strategies used by successful copers that differentiates them from nonsuccessful copers? Need in: the Stud! There is a need for this study because of the following three points: (1) Although the research on stress and coping over a 20 year period is voluminous, most studies are based on adult samples. Except for a few doctoral dissertations, research on how children cope with surgery as it occurs is scarce. (2) Previous research investigating children's adaptation to surgery has focused strictly on trait or personality factors of children (Lazarus et a1, 1980; Monat & Lazarus, 1977). An investigation of cognitive processes that mediate the impact of surgery and affect postsurgical adaptation in pediatric populations might contribute to our knowledge of how children themselves actively cope with a very stressful experience. Cognitive processes can be learned (Spivack, Platt, & Shure, 1976) and are more amenable to intervention than personality traits. , (3) This study investigates how normal, nonreferred children cope with a severe stressor, i.e., their sense of mastery (Anthony, 1974) or competency (Garmezy, 1983). Too often research has focused on children coping poorly with a specific stressor. Pediatric surgery patients face a very disturbing experience over which they have little control. An empirical investigation describing the relationships between surgical coping responses and postsurgical adaptation aids in understanding the strategies both good and poor copers use (Garmezy, 1983; Rutter, 1983). The information can be used by pediatric psychologists, physicians, nursing staff, and other hospital personnel as empirical groundwork to plan and structure more effective interventions for children experiencing surgery. Pnrpeee The primary purpose of this research is to study the relationships between coping responses and adaptational outcomes of hospitalized school-aged children undergoing elective orthopedic surgery. The coping responses of children successfully adapting to hospitalization and surgery will be compared with the coping responses of children not adapting as well. An interactional stress model, emphasizing cognitive, situational (e.g.,1ength of hospital stay and number of previous hospital admissions) and personality variables (e.g., presurgery intellectual functioning and premorbid adjustment) will be employed. The central hypothesis of this study is that cognitive processes act as mediators of stress in children undergoing hospitalization and surgery, such that children who score higher on measures of surgical coping exhibit better postsurgical psychological adaptation. In addition, it is hypothesized that cognitive processes, individual differences, and situational variables all affect children's surgical adaptation. Theelx The Ceneept 9f Stress Coping responses and adaptational outcomes in children undergoing surgery can best be understood by a stress and coping model. The application of this model to the process of pediatric surgery has several advantages. One, it offers a dynamic and interactive framework for the study of children's functioning in a hospital setting. Two, there is practical utility in the stress and coping model in its focus on the adaptive process, as opposed to psychopathology. Three, the model is consistent with current naturalistic approaches to research on pediatric hospitalization and surgery in that stress is defined as an event that is subsequently appraised by the individual, resulting in coping responses. The present section is theoretical and briefly presents the concepts of stress, coping, and adaptation. Included are: a) the concept of stress, b) interactionism as applied to stress, including coping and adaptation, and c) a cognitive stress model within an interactional framework, upon which the present research is based. There are three main variations in use of the concept of stress (Lazarus & Launier, 1978): a) stress as a form of stimulus, b) stress as a response condition, and c) stress as a psychological state. Many researchers define stress as a form of stimulus, a condition or conditions that disrupt or endanger well- established personal and social values. Dohrenwend and Dohrenwend (1980), among others, conceptualize stress as a series of life events, either inherent in the life cycle or occuring naturally, e.g., marriage, change of jobs, illness, separation, and death. Such events are said to produce change in life patterns or activities of the individual for better or for worse. Stress is used to describe situations characterized as new, intense, quickly changing, and sudden (Appley & Trumbull, 1977). Numerous researchers have studied stress associated with life events and have developed scales to assess the impact of such changes (e.g., Coddington, 1972; Hefferin, 1982; Holmes 5 Rahe, 1967). An unsettled issue at this time is whether change per se or the perceived desirability of the change is the actual basis for the stressfulness of life events. Other researchers define stress as a response condition (e.g., Selye, 1976). The presence of emotional activity is used post-facto to define the existence of stress. Stress here refers to any response, bodily or behavioral, which deviates from normative value for the individual or an appropriate reference group (Appley and Trumbull, 1977). Selye (1976), the ”father“ of stress research, refers to the physiological manifestations of stress. His model, the “general adaptation syndrome“ (GAS), exemplifies a total commitment to the concept of physiological nonspecificity of stress responses (Monat & Lazarus, 1977). A third group of researchers, most notably Lazarus (Lazarus, 1966, 1977; Lazarus & Launier, 1978) and Sarason and Sarason (1981) refer to stress as a psychological or mental state describing adaptive commerce between the person and environment, and emphasize possible cognitive factors leading to the evaluation of threat and consequent stress responses. Here, the individual's interpretation and evaluation of stimuli functions as the basis for a response to the stress experience (Scott, Oberst, & Dropkin, 1982). Emotions and physiological responses are viewed as by- products of cognition. Cognitive processes determine the quality and intensity of an emotional reaction which Lazarus (1977) sees as the mediating variable between the event and the response. Each position regarding stress described above has its adherents and detractors. A stimulus or life events based definition is incomplete, for a given situation may or may not be stressful to a person. For example, why does stress occur in some people but not to others exposed to the same stimulus? Except for sudden, life endangering situations (Appley, 1977; Monat & Lazarus, 1977) it is seldom that a particular stimulus is a stressor to all individuals exposed to it. A definition which is strictly response based is also incomplete, inasmuch as an individual may exhibit increased autonomic nervous system activity at one moment in time, but fail to do so in the same situation at another moment in time. What accounts for the difference in the physiological response to stress? Finally, a cognitive appraisal approach assumes an internal mechanism not readily visible to either the individual or an observer, which, because the processes must be inferred, causes certain problems when it comes to defining and assessing stress. The picture is further complicated in reference to the concept of stress as applied to children. Children's tolerance for stress is lower than adults; they have fewer Options available and thus are more vulnerable to the effects of stress (Chandler, 1982). Children are in the process of developing controls that tend to be more established in adults. Their cognitions also are egocentric and centered, making awareness of multiple perceptions difficult (Ginsburg & Opper, 1979). Cause and effect relationships demand a certain level of cognitive development, i.e., concrete operations, something which preschool aged children generally do not possess. Children's problem-solving strategies are less planful; they are less apt to be organized sequentially, are more redundant, and future events are less likely to be taken into account (Maccoby, 1983). The concept of stress as applied to children thus raises many issues in terms of definition and assessment that are unresolved at this time. For purposes of this research, definitions of stress and coping are based on an interactional model, which focuses on the interplay between the person and environment, and which is understood and explained within a cognitive framework (Lazarus, 1966; Lazarus & Folkman, 1984; Lazarus & Launier, 1978; Sarason & Sarason, 1981). ”Psychological stress in a particular relationship between the person and the environment that is appraised by the person as taxing or exceeding his or her resources and endangering his or her well-being'I (Lazarus & Folkman, 1984, p. 19). Coping, a related term, refers to "constantly changing cognitive and behavioral efforts to manage specific external and/or internal demands that are appraised as taxing or exceeding the resources of the person“ (Lazarus & Folkman, 1984, p. 141). Wmnwmmmu Streasandcmins All interactional theories make important common statements. The essential differences between the theories center on the amount of emphasis on person, environment, and/or process variables. Heuristically, interactional theories are useful insofar as they each 10 maintain that a variety of events can occur simultaneously within and outside of a person and that an event can only be understood in relation to the sequence or pattern of which it is a part. The consequence of stress cannot be understood merely in terms of the stressful event (Holroyd 8 Lazarus, 1982) or trait factors of the person alone. Rather, stress is best described in terms of an interaction between the person and the environment (Laux & Vossel, 1982). An individual's cognitive appraisal of a stressful event is crucial to understanding how that person may respond to the stress, or, for that matter, whether stress is perceived at all (Magnusson, 1982). Prediction from trait measures regarding how a person actually will cope with specific threat situations is very poor (Lazarus, Cohen, Folkman, Kamer, & Schaefer, 1980). General coping traits assume a stable pattern or style of coping in most stressful situations, but since coping styles show little generalizability within persons and between situations, traits are usually poor predictors of how a person will cope with a given stresssor (Monat & Lazarus, 1977). Lazarus and Cohen (1974) found that trait measures did not predict how people coped with children. Children, due to the very nature of their cognitive, affective, and physiological development, do not demonstrate stable personality characteristics. To 11 fully understand children's behavior, one must consider both the developmental characteristics of the child and the environmental context in which behavior occurs (Achenbach, 1974; Quay & Werry, 1974). Unfortunately, most research investigating children's coping responses and adaptational outcomes to hospitalization and surgery have assumed a trait model, thereby limiting the fruitfulness of much research already completed. A Cegnitixe Stress Hedel Hithinanlnteraetienalmmemk Although stress has been defined as a stimulus, a response, and a hypothetical state, a generally agreed on view is that stress is an state, i.e., an interaction between the person and the environment that is appraised by the person as taxing or exceeding the resources of the person (Lazarus & Folkman, 1984). If stress can be understood in cognitive terms, differential reactions to stressful situations might be better understood. Figure 1 depicts the model of stress developed for purposes of this study. It is based on a theory of interactionism, whereby stress is defined as the interaction between the individual, his or her resources and capabilities, and the demand(s) of the situation. Stress, thus theorized, is an ongoing process as the individual moves through situations and time. It is a chain of events involving two related processes: coping responses and adaptational outcomes. The model and 12 WWZOUHDO A Pastemerx ------- >Eellenzun RATC-l RATC-2 PIC RBPC-1 RBPC-2 Design The overall design of this study was descriptive, the purpose being to explore the relationships between coping responses and other instrinsic and extrinsic factors and adaptational outcomes in children undergoing orthopedic surgery. It was apparent from a review of the literature that an interactional theoretical framework encompassing both individual and situational variables was necessary in order to more fully understand the complexities inherent in the constructs of stress, coping, and adaptation. One of the major problems with research in the area of coping and adaptation has been the causal and overly restricting experimental approaches to research used in lieu of more exploratory approaches with solid theoretical foundations. A microanalytic process-oriented approach to coping was necessary to more fully understand coping processes and explain adaptational outcomes (Lazarus & Folkman, 1984). A correlational approach was chosen for three reasons: a) The variables necessarily under investigation were very complex and did not lend themselves to the experimental method and controlled I manipulation. b) Correlational research permitted the simultaneous measurement of several variables and their interrelationships within a naturalistic setting. c) Correlational research permitted an analysis of the degrees of relationship between variables rather than all-or-nothing questions (Isaac & Michael, 1971). A correlational research design in the form of a naturalistic field investigation was used in this study because of the flexibility involved in this approach. Such flexibility facilitated the inclusion of a variety of carefully selected variables and the study of a complex area. This type of research design did not directly address issues of causality. Rather, it was hoped that the results from the study would provide direction for intervention with children undergoing potentially stressful hospital events such as orthopedic surgery, generate further questions regarding the affects of hospitalization and surgery on children, and broaden the knowledge base concerned with the definition and measurement of stress and coping in children as applied to hospitalization and surgery. Researehflxnetheses This study investigated the relationships between coping responses and adaptational outcomes of children undergoing elective orthopedic surgery. Factors intrinsic and extrinsic to the child were studied. HmthesesRelatedtenpinsBesnenses The first two hypotheses were developed to study the relationships between coping responses and adaptational outcomes. Hypethesis 1: Children who obtain higher T-scores on each of the surgical coping response scales a) Reliance on Others, b) Support Others, c) Self- Sufficiency, d) Problem-Identification, e) Limit Setting, f) Unrealistic Solution, 9) Constructive Solution, and h) Insightful Solution, as measured by the Roberts Apperception Test for Children (RATC), will exhibit better post and long-term surgical adaptation, as measured by the Revised Behavior Problem Checklist (RBPC), than will children who obtain lower T-scores. is II: There will be significant differences between T-scores on each of the surgical coping response scales, as measured by the RATC, administered preceeding surgery and following surgery. Hymtheseshelatedtelntrinsiefitressfaeters The second set of hypotheses were developed to investigate how factors intrinsic to the child were related to surgical adaptation. EXPQ£h£§i§ 111: Children who obtain lower T-scores on the cognitive functioning scales Intellectual Screening (IS), Achievement (ACH), and Development (DVL), as measured by the Personality Inventory for Children (PIC), will exhibit better post and long-term surgical adaptation, as measured by the RBPC, than children who obtain higher T-scores. Hypothesis 11: Children who obtain lower T-scores on the premorbid adjustment scale Adjustment (ADJ), as measured by the PIC, will exhibit better post and long- term surgical adaptation, as measured by the RBPC, than children who obtain higher T-scores. Hypetnesis 2: There will be a significant correlation between post and long-term surgical adaptation, as measured by the RBPC, and age of the 69 child. Hypethesis yl: Girls will exhibit better post and long-term surgical adaptation, as measured by lower scores on the RBPC, than will boys. HmihesesRelatedteExtrinsieStressEaeters The final set of hypotheses were developed to investigate how factors extrinsic to the child were related to surgical adaptation. y11: Children hospitalized for fewer days will exhibit better post and long-term surgical adaptation, as measured by lower scores on the RBPC, than will children hospitalized for more days. VIII: Children with more prior hospital admissions will exhibit poorer post and long-term surgical adaptation, as measured by higher scores on the RBPC, than will children with fewer prior hospital admissions. Analysis Statistical procedures were used to test the eight hypotheses. The statistics used included: the t-test statistic, Pearson product-moment correlations, and multiple regression analysis. The form in which the variables were expressed and the nature of their relationship determined which of the correlational statistics was used (Borg & Gall, 1979). The analytic procedures used are discussed below for each hypothesis. Significance level was set at .05. Prior to any statistical analysis, scattergrams and histograms were generated for each variable. If the histograms indicated a markedly skewed distribution or if the scattergrams indicated a curvilinear 7O relationship, a log transformation of the original variable was undertaken in order to restate the relationship in a linear form. Log data was then used in all subsequent analysis. A three step analytic strategy was employed to test the hypotheses. The first level, or provisional analysis, consisted of bivariate correlations generated according to the specific hypotheses under investigation. These correlations were then examined in terms of statistical significance. A second intermediate step involved computing a correlational matrix of all of the variables studied. The purpose of the correlational matrix was to examine whether confounding existed, i.e., whether the correlations between two variables obtained through the provisonal analysis might be spurious because of the effects of a third independent variable. If in fact the independent variable was confounded or correlated with one or more independent variables, a multiple regression analysis was computed between the two correlated independent variables and the dependent variable in order to partial out the effects of the confounding. Multiple regression is a statistical technique through which one can analyze the relationship between a dependent variable and a set of independent variables. The multiple regression analysis permitted examination of the relationship between the independent 71 and dependent variables, while holding constant the effects of the second independent variable. This three step strategy was undertaken in order to empirically demonstrate whether the correlational effects obtained were in fact valid. In order for the null hypothesis to be rejected, significant correlations had to be obtained at this third level of analysis. Hypethesis 1 refers to the possible relationship between children's coping responses and post and long- term adaptation to orthopedic surgery. The surgical coping variables (RATC) and the post and long-term adaptation variables (RBPC) are expressed as T-scores, which are continuous scores. The Pearson product-moment correlation (r) was used, as the variables were continuously expressed. Correlation analysis summarizes the relationship between two variables, i.e., the degree to which variation in one variable is related to variation in another. It measures the goodness of fit of a linear regression line to the data (Nie, Hull, Jenkins, Steinbrenner, & Bent, 1975). The product-moment corrrelation is the most stable correlation, with the smallest standard error of the correlation techniques. One tailed t-test statistics were then computed in order to determine whether the correlation coefficients obtained were statistically significant. HXPQEhfifiifi 11 tests whether the number of 72 children's coping responses differs significantly before orthopedic surgery as opposed to following orthopedic surgery. Two-tailed matched-pairs or dependent t-tests were computed between pre and postsurgery coping scores (RATC) in order to test this hypothesis. The t-test statistic is used to determine whether two means, proportions or correlation coefficients, differ significantly from one another. The hypothesis does not predict direction, but rather tests whether there are significant differences, either positive or negative, in the magnitude of children's coping responses. EXPchefiifi 111 is concerned with whether there is a significant linear relationship between children's cognitive functioning and their post and long-term surgical adaptation. Both the PIC Cognitive Triad Scales and the RBPC scales are constructed using T- scores. Thus, both variables were expressed continuously and the Pearson product-moment correlation was computed in order to obtain the strength and direction of their linear relationship. Hypethesis l! is concerned with whether there is a significant linear relationship between children's premorbid adjustment and their post and long-term surgical adaptation. Both the PIC Adjustment scale and the RBPC scales are constructed using T-scores and are thus continuously expressed. The Pearson product-moment correlation was computed in order to obtain the strength 73 and direction of the relationship. Hypethesis 1 refers to the relationship between age and children's post and long-term surgical adaptation. Both age and surgical adaptation are continuously expressed. In order to measure the linear relationship between the two variables, the Pearson product-moment correlation was computed. Hypethesis yl tests whether girls or boys demonstrate better post and long-term adaptation to orthopedic surgery. A one-tailed independent t-test was performed between the mean scores of males and females on the RBPC. HXEQIhfifiifi 211 is concerned with the linear relationship between length of hospitalization and post and long-term adaptation to orthopedic surgery. Once again, both variables, i.e., days and RBPC, were continous. Thus, the Pearson product—moment correlation was computed in order to determine the degree to which variation in the number of days hospitalized was related to variation in post and long-term surgical adaptation. HXPQLhfifiifi 2111 is concerned with the relationship between number of overnight hospital admissions and children's short and long term adaptation to orthopedic surgery. Both variables, i.e., number of admissions and surgical adaptation, as measured by the RBPC, are continuous. Thus the Pearson product-moment correlation was computed. 74 Supplementary Descriptixe Data Analysis Certain aspects of the present study did not lend themselves to quantitative analysis and the rigor of testing for statistical significance. The intensive data collection used and the difficulties associated with obtaining sufficiently large numbers of pediatric patients undergoing stressful orthopedic surgeries restricted the study to a relatively small sample. In an attempt to capture the richness of the data obtained, a qualitative case-study approach was used to describe two children's coping with their operations. Additional clinical material was obtained from these children, including interviews, scores on test variables not included in the hypotheses, and review of their medical charts. These case studies are reported in Chapter IV. Summarx The primary purpose of the present study was to study the relationships between children's coping responses and adaptational outcomes to orthopedic surgery. A descriptive research design in the form of naturalistic field investigation was used in conjunction with correlational analytic techniques to address the questions posed. A sample of 27 children undergoing orthopedic surgery participated in the study. Mothers of the patients completed the Personality Inventory for Children, used to measure premorbid adjustment and 75 cognitive functioning of their children, prior to the surgery. They also completed the Revised Behavior Problem Checklist, used to measure surgical adaptation, immediately before discharge and again one month later. The children completed the Roberts Apperception Test for Children, used to measure surgical coping responses, less than one day prior to their operations and again before their discharge. Hypotheses related to both intrinsic and extrinsic factors related to surgical adaptation were developed and procedures to test these hyotheses were outlined. Pearson's product-moment correlations were used to summarize the degree to which variation in the independent variables were related to variation in the dependent variables. A correlation matrix was also computed in order to more fully examine the possibility of confounding of the independent variables. Multiple regression analyses were then performed in order to partial out or hold constant the effects of one independent variable while examining the relationship between a second independent variable and the outcome measure. In addition to coping responses, the relationships between other independent variables and surgical adaptation were examined, including age, sex, length of hospitalization, and number of overnight hospital admissions. 76 The procedures used for data analysis included: one and two-tailed t-tests and correlational models. Supplementary descriptive case analysis was also used to aid in description of children's experiences in the hospital. Descriptive research such as this, while not addressing cause and effect, has heuristic value. The results from the present study will begin to build theory and broaden the knowledge base concerned with how children cope with stressful events such as hospitalization and surgery and how to help prepare high risk children for orthopedic surgery. CHAPTER IV RESULTS This chapter presents summary statistics of the major variables and the test results of the hypotheses under investigation. Each hypothesis is restated in statistical form and the statistical procedures described. The results of the analysis are then presented. SunnarxStatistiesefsngiealmningandAdantatien The Roberts Apperception Test for Children (RATC) was used to measure coping responses of children to orthopedic surgery. The RATC was twice administered: once 24 hours or less preceeding surgery and again immediately preceeding discharge from the hospital. The total scale RATC T-scores, which are composed of mean T- scores of the eight subscales, ranged from 39 to 64 for the first RATC administration (RATC-l) and from 39 to 63 for the second RATC administration (RATC-2). The overall mean T-score for RATC-1 was 51.76 (SD = 7.74). The overall mean T-score for RATC-2 was 52.43 (SD = 6.49) The Revised Behavior Problem Checklist (RBPC) was used to measure post- and long-term surgical adaptation. 77 78 The RBPC was twice administered: once immediately prior to discharge from the hospital, and again one month later. The total scale RBPC T-scores, which are composed of mean T-scores of the six subscales, ranged from 39 to 63 for the first RBPC administration (RBPC-1) and from 42 to 63 for the second RBPC administration (RBPC-2). The overall mean T-score for RBPC-1 was 52.43 (SD = 6.49). The overall mean T-score for RBPC-2 was 50.14 (SD = 7.77). Table 4.1 presents descriptive statistics for all coping response and adaptational outcome scales. Snmmaufitatistieeeflntrineieandfixtrinsiefitreas tasters Four intrinsic factors, hypothesized to affect adaptational outcomes to orthopedic surgery, were investigated: cognitive functioning, premorbid adjustment, age, and gender. Cognitive functioning was measured by the Cognitive Triad scales of the Personality Inventory for Children (PIC), administered immediately prior to or during surgery. Higher scores on the PIC indicate the increased possibility of psychopathology or deficit. Three scales compose the Cognitive Triad: Achievement, Intellectual Screening, and Development. T-scores for each subject were obtained on the three scales, added together, and divided by three in order to obtain a measure of cognitive functioning. Cognitive functioning 79 Table 4.1 Descriptive Statistics for Surgical Coping and Adaptational Outcome Scales Standard Standard Scale N Mean Deviation Error Presursieal Casing lBATC:ll Total 25 51.76 7.74 1.55 Reliance on Others 25 56.4 12.59 2.52 Support Others 25 47.12 15.47 3.09 Self-Sufficiency 25 50.44 9.24 1.85 Limit Setting 25 59.8 14.50 2.90 Problem Ident. 25 51.08 11.47 2.29 Unrealistic Solut. 25 49.92 9.52 1.90 Constructive Solut. 25 51.44 13.66 2.73 Insightful Solut. 15 50.27 6.13 1.58 Restaursieal Canine lRATC:21 Total 21 52.43 6.50 1.42 Reliance on Others 21 53 9.46 2.06 Support Others 21 51.86 13.14 2.87 Self-Sufficiency 21 50.52 9.37 2.05 Limit Setting 21 60.10 15.70 3.42 Problem Ident. 21 51.71 10.34 2.26 Unrealistic Solut. 21 49.52 9.20 2.00 Constructive Solut. 21 52.10 10.76 2.35 Insightful Solut. 12 51.92 8.20 2.37 Restaursieal Adaptatien 18829211 Total 27 49.93 6.72 1.29 Conduct Disorder 27 46.26 7.40 1.42 Socialized Aggress. 27 48.78 7.26 1.40 Attention Problems 27 51.19 11.53 2.22 Anxiety-Withdrawal 27 50.93 11.82 2.27 Psychotic Behavior 27 49.70 6.06 1.17 Motor Excess 27 51.74 9.82 1.89 Lenszterm Surgical Adaptatien 18822221 Total 22 50.14 7.77 1.66 Conduct Disorder 22 49.60 10.42 2.22 Socialized Aggress. 22 - 48.18 6.30 1.34 Attention Problems 22 49.90 9.30 1.98 Anxiety-Withdrawal 22 49.77 9.66 2.06 Psychotic Behavior 22 51 8.71 1.86 'Motor Excess 22 52.14 10.60 2.26 80 ranged from T-scores of 43 to 78. The mean T-score was 54.80 (SD = 9.06). Premorbid adjustment was measured by the Adjustment scale of the PIC. T-scores ranged from 40 to 85. The mean T-score was 54.58 (SD = 11.69). The age of the child was documented from the birthdate on the medical chart. Children's ages ranged from 6 to 17 years. The mean age was 12.6 (SD 8 2.86). There were 18 boys (67%) and 9 girls (33%) in the study. Table 4.2 presents the descriptive statistics for the instrinic variables. Two extrinsic variables, hypothesized to affect chidren's adaptation to orthopedic surgery, were included in the study: length of hospitalization in days, and total number of overnight hospital admissions. This data was obtained both through a review of the child's medical charts and questions answered by the parent. The length of stay ranged from a minimum of two days to over 10 days. The mean length of stay was 4.60 (SD= 2.65) The total number of hospital admissions ranged from one to over four. The mean total number of admissions was 2.67 (SD = 1.28). Table 4.3 presents the descriptive statistics for the extrinsic factors. 81 Table 4.2 Descriptive Statistics for the Intrinsic Variables Standard Variable N Mean Deviation Cognitive Funct. 26 54.80 9.06 Premorbid Adjust. 26 54.58 11.69 Age 27 12.56 2.86 Sex Males 18 12.44 2.90 Females 9 12.78 3.0 Table 4.3 Descriptive Statistics for the Extrinsic Variables Standard Variable N Mean Deviation Length of Hosp. 27 4.60 2.65 Total Admissions 27 2.67 1.27 Testeftheflxnetheses firmneses Related te Seeing Beanenses andSurgiealmantatien Hypethesis 1: Children who obtain higher T-scores on the surgical coping scales, as measured by the Roberts Apperception Test for Children, will exhibit better total post and long-term surgical adaptation, as measured by the Revised Behavior Problem Checklist, than will children who obtain lower T-scores. He: rho = 0 (Total RATC-1 and Total RBPC-l) Hl: rho < 0 (Total RATC-l and Total RBPC-1) The bivariate relationships between the surgical coping scales and the surgical adaptation scales were determined by computing Pearson product-moment correlations. One-tail t-tests were used to determine 82 whether the correlations obtained were significant. The correlation between Total Presurgical Coping and Total Postsurgical Adaptation was not significant (r = -.18, p >.05). The null hypothesis is retained. HQ HZ The correlation between Total Postsurgical Coping rho = 0 (Total RATC-2 and RBPC-l) rho < 0 (Total RATC-2 and RBPC-1) and Total Postsurgical Adaptation was not significant (r = -.17, p >.05). The null hypothesis is retained. He: rho = 0 (Total RATC-1 and RBPC-2) El: rho < 0 (Total RATC-1 and RBPC-2) The correlation between Total Presurgical Coping and Long-term Surgical Adaptation was not significant (r = .31, p >.05). The null hypothesis is retained. He: rho = 0 (Total RATC-2 and RBPC-2) HA: rho < 0 (Total RATC-2 and RBPC-2) The correlation between Total Postsurgical Coping and Total Long-term Surgical Adaptation was not significant (r s .36, p >.05). The null hypothesis is retained. He: rho = 0 (Subscales RATC—l/RATC-Z and RBPC-l/ RBPC-2) Hi: rho < 0 (Subscales RATC-l/RATC-Z and RBPC-l/ RBPC-2) The correlations ranged from -.03 to .47 between the Surgical Coping Subscale Scores and the Total Surgical Adaptation scores. Three correlations were significant: (a) Presurgical Self-Sufficiency and Long- term Surgical Adaptation (r = .44, p (.05), (b) Presurgical Problem Identification and Long-Term 83 Surgical Adaptation (r = .47, p (.05), and (c) Postsurgical Self-Sufficiency and Long-Term Surgical . Adaptation (r = .47, p (.05). The correlations ranged from -.03 to .41 between the Total Surgical Coping Scores and the Surgical Adaptation Subscale Scores. Two correlations were significant: (a) Total Presurgical Coping and Postsurgical Adaptation Anxiety-Withdrawal (r = -.39, p (.05), and (b) Total Postsurgical Coping and Long-Term Surgical Adaptation Anxiety-Withdrawal (r = .41, p (.05). Following the three-step strategy outlined in Chapter III, a correlational matrix of all independent and dependent variables was generated. Multiple regression equations were computed after identifying all possible sources of confounding in the independent variables in order to isolate the effects of the confounding independent variable. Multiple regression analysis revealed two significant linear relationships, both in the hypothesized directions: (a) Presurgical Limit Setting and Long-term Surgical Adaptation (t - -2.16, p (.025) and (b) Total Presurgical Coping and Postsurgical Anxiety-Withdrawal (t = -2.01, p (.05). Although there are two significant correlations between subscale scores, the correlations between the total scores are not significant, and the null hypothesis is thus retained. The results of these analysis are presented in Table 4.4. 84 Table 4.4 Correlation Coefficients Between the Total Surgical Coping Scores (RATC) and the Total Surgical Adaptation Scores (RBPC) RBPC’l RBPC’Z Correlation Coefficients Between the Surgical Coping Subscale Scores and the Total Surgical Adaptation Scores RBPC-l RBPC-2 RATC-1 Reliance On Others —.16 .33 Support Others -.12 .22 Self-Sufficiency .12 .44*(a) Limit Setting -.18 .16** Problem Identification .14 .47*(b) Unrealistic Solution -.32 -.12 Constructive Solution -.13 -.03 Insightful Solution -.09 .36 RATC-2 Reliance On Others -.35 -.09 Support Others -.07 .26 Self-Sufficiency .15 .47*(c) Limit Setting -.09 ~ .25 Problem Identification .04 .29 Unrealistic Solution -.19 -.04 Constructive Solution -.04 .37 Insightful Solution -.16 .30 Correlation Coefficients Between the Total Surgical Coping Scores and the Surgical Adaptation Subscale Scores RATC-1 RATC-2 RBPC-1 Conduct Disorder -.08 -.09 Socialized Aggression -.03 -.07 Attentional Problems -.18 -.12 Anxiety-Withdrawal -.39* -.34 Psychotic Behavior -.08 -.03 Motor Excess -.07 -.06 RBPC-2 Conduct Disorder .25 .34 Socialized Aggression .22 .29 Attentional Problems .32 .38 Anxiety-Withdrawal .15 .41*(d) Psychotic Behavior .29 .23 85 Table 4.4 (cont'd.) . Motor Excess .31 .23 (a) (b) (C) (d) (.05 bivariate correlation only. (.025 when effects of Adjustment held constant. >.05 when effects of Adjustment held constant. >.05 when effects of Adjustment held constant. >.05 when effects of Age held constant. P P p >.05 when effects of Adjustment held constant. P P Hypethesis ll: There will be significant differences between T-scores on each of the surgical coping response scales (a) Reliance on Others, (b) Support Others, (c) Self-Sufficiency, (d) Limit Setting, (e) Problem Identification, (f) Unrealistic Solution, (9) Constructive Solution, and (h) Insightful Solution, as measured by the Roberts Apperception Test for Children administered preceeding surgery versus following surgery. u a 0 u # 0 HQ Hl Two tail t-tests for paired or dependent samples were computed. None of the t-values were significant at p (.05. These results appear in Table 4.5. There were no significant differences in the number of surgical coping responses used presurgery as opposed to postsurgery. The null hypothesis is retained. Hinethesesaelatedtelnstrinsieatressaaeters Hypethesis lll: Children who obtain lower T—scores on the cognitive functioning scales, as measured by the Personality Inventory for Children (PIC) will exhibit better post and long-term surgical adaptation, as measured by the RBPC, than children who obtain higher T-scores. : rho = 0 (Cognitive Functioning and Postsurgical Adaptation : rho > 0 (Cognitive Functioning and Postsurgical Adaptation) rho = 0 (Cognitive Functioning and Long-term Surgical Adaptation) rho > 0 (Cognitive Functioning and Long-term Surgical Adaptation) SEES 86 Table 4.5 Paired Difference t-test Results for Presurgical (RATC-l) and Postsurgical (RATC-2) Coping Response Scales Scale N i1 X2 t-value Total 20 51.76 52.43 -.79 us Reliance on Others 20 56.40 53.00 1.35 NS Support Others 20 47.12 51.86 1.92 NS Self-Sufficiency 20 50.44 50.52 .03 NS Limit Setting 20 59.80 60.10 .08 NS Problem Identification 20 51.08 51.71 .15 NS Unrealistic Solution 20 49.92 49.52 .37 NS Constructive Solution 20 51.44 52.10 -.37 NS Insightful Solution 12 50.27 51.92 1.34 NS The bivariate relationship between cognitive functioning and surgical adaptation was determined by the Pearson product-moment correlation. Correlations between cognitive functioning and post and long-term surgical adaptation were not significant (r = .06, p >.05, and r = .21, p >0.5, respectively). Further multiple regression analysis with cognitive functioning did not reveal any significant correlations. The null hypothesis is retained. Hypethesis 111 Children who obtain lower T-scores on the premorbid adjustment scale Adjustment, as measured by the PIC, will exhibit better post and long-term surgical adaptation, as measured by the RBPC, than will children who obtain higher T- scores. He: rho = 0 (Premorbid Adjustment and Postsurgical Adaptation) Hl: rho ( 0 (Premorbid Adjustment and Postsurgical Adaptation) 87 E : rho = 0 (Premorbid Adjustment and Long-term Surgical Adaptation) H2: rho ( 0 (Premorbid Adjustment and Long-term Surgical Adaptation) The Pearson product-moment correlation was computed in order to investigate Hypothesis IV. Both the correlations between premorbid adjustment and postsurgical adaptation (r s .39, p (.05) and long-term surgical adaptation (r c .70, p (.0005) were significant. Multiple regression analysis revealed that the relationship between premorbid adjustment and postsurgical adaptation was confounded by the independent variable “days hospitalized“ and was not significant with the effect of days hospitalized held constant (t = .99, p >.05). However, the relationship between premorbid adjustment and long-term surgical adaptation was significant despite partialing of all possible confounding variables (t = 2.88, p (.005). The null hypothesis is rejected in favor of the alternative hypothesis H2. Hypeenesis 1: Different age children will show different post and long—term surgical adaptation. He: rho = 0 (Age and Postsurgical Adaptation) Hl: rho f 0 (Age and Postsurgical Adaptation) He: rho = 0 (Age and Long-term Surgical Adaptation) H2: rho f 0 (Age and Long-term Surgical Adaptation) The relationship between age and surgical adaptation was investigated using the Pearson product- moment correlation. Provisional analysis indicated that 88 age was significantly correlated with postsurgical adaptation (r = -.38, p (.05), but not with long-term surgical adaptation (r = -.025, p >.05). Further analysis using multiple regression indicated that age was confounded with total postsurgical coping. The standardized beta value for age was -.35. The correlation between age and postsurgical adaptation with postsurgical coping held constant was not significant (t = -1.5, p >.05). However, the correlation between age and postsurgical adaptation with presurgical coping held constant remained significant (t - -2.07, p (.025). Nonetheless, the null hypothesis is retained. Expethesis 21: Girls will show better post and long- term surgical adaptation than will boys. He: u a 0 (Postsurgical Adaptation) Hl: u £ 0 (Postsurgical Adaptation) He: u = 0 (Long-term Surgical Adaptation) H2: u £ 0 (Long-term Surgical Adaptation) This hypothesis was tested by t-test comparisons of the RBPC-l and the RBPC-2 means for the male and female groups. There were no significant differences in postsurgical adaptation (t = -0.59, p >.05) and long- term surgical adaptation (t = -0.443, p >.05) between males and females. Table 4.6 describes the scores for males and females. The null hypothesis is retained. Table 4.7 is a summary table and presents the bivariate correlations for Hypothesis III-VI, hypotheses related to instrinsic stress factors. 89 Table 4.6 T-test Results for Postsurgical Adaptation (RATC-l) and Long-term Surgical Adaptation (RATC-2) Mean Scores for Males (M) and Females (F) Surgical Adaptation Mean (M) Mean (F) t-value Postsurgical Adap. 49.67 51.38 -0.60 NS Long-term Surgical Adap. 49.57 51.13 -0.44 NS Table 4.7 Summary of Bivariate Correlation Coefficients for Hypotheses Related to Intrinsic Stress Factors _ Long-term Postsurgical Surgical Adaptation Adaptation III Cognitive Functioning .06 .21 IV Premorbid Adjustment .39*(a) .70** V Age -038*(b) -002 * p (.05 ** p (.0005 (a) p >.05 when effects of Days Hospitalized held constant. (b) p >.05 when effects of total Presurgical Coping held constant amethesisaelatedteExtrinsieStressEaaters Hypethesis yll: Children with longer periods of hospitalization will exhibit less successful post and long-term surgical adaptation than will children with shorter periods of hospitalization. : rho = 0 (days hospitalized and postsurgical adaptation) : rho > 0 (days hospitalized and postsurgical adaptation) rho = 0 (days hospitalized and long-term surgical adaptation) rho > 0 (days hospitalized and long-term surgical adaptation) EEEE 90 The correrlations between days hospitalized and surgical adaptation were determined by computing Pearson product-moment correlations. The correlation between days hospitalized and postsurgical adaptation was not significant (r = .32, p >.05). The correlation between days hospitalized and long-term surgical adaptation was significant (r = .41, p (.05). Multiple regression analysis indicated that the variable “days hospitalized“ was confounded with the variable “premorbid adjustment.“ The relationship between days hospitalized and long-term surgical adaptation was not significant with the effects of premorbid adjustment held constant (t - .83, p >.05). The null hypothesis is retained. Hypethesis ylll: Children with more prior hospital admissions will exhibit less successful post and long-term surgical adaptation than will children with fewer hospital admissions. He: rho = 0 (Admissions and Postsurgical Adaptation) Hl: rho > 0 (Admissions and Postsurgical Adaptation) He: rho = 0 (Admissions and Long-term Surgical Adaptation) H2: rho > 0 (Admissions and Long-term Surgical Adaptation) The correlations between hospital admissions and surgical adaptation were determined by computing Pearson product-moment correlations. The correlation between admissions and postsurgical adaptation was not significant (r s .17, p >.05). The 91 correlation between admissions and long-term surgical adaptation was also not significant (r = .04, p >.05). Futher analysis using multiple regression did not reveal any significant effects. The null hypotheses are retained. The results of the correlations for hypotheses related to extrinsic stress factors are presented in Table 4.8 Table 4.8 Bivariate Correlation Coefficients for Hypotheses Related to Extrinsic Stress Factors Long-term Postsurgical Surgical Adaptation Adaptation VII Days Hospitalized .32 .41*(a) VIII Hospital Admissions .17 .04 * p (.05 (a) p >.05 when effects of Adjustment held constant D . l' C E J . Two case studies are presented in an attempt to capture the richness of the data obtained. Gary was a nine year old male, undergoing operations on both of his legs due to bone growth deformities, who demonstrated a problematic surgical adaptation. Alex was a 17 year old male with severe scoliosis who, despite a demanding operative procedure and several postoperative complications, exhibited satisfactory surgical adaptation. 92 Casemfiau Gary was a 9-8 year old white male with an admitting diagnosis of Perkins Disease. The surgical procedure involved orthopedic surgery on both legs in order to correct bone growth deformities. Gary remained in the hospital for 10 days. He had a significant history of prior hospitalizations involving surgery. Two weeks prior to the surgery, his mother indicated that he was experiencing many behavioral difficulties related to his upcoming hospitalization and surgery, including difficulties following directions, poor frustration tolerance, overdemanding, and defiance. His mother was a single parent and expressed exasperation with Gary's behaviors. Test Results: Gary's mother completed the Personality Inventory for Children (PIC) while Gary was undergoing surgery. Gary's profile was valid and had clinically significant elevations on scales indicative of poor scholastic achievement, sadness, fearfulness, poor self-control and defiance, and overactivity, restlesssness, and distractability. Mothers of children obtaining similar profiles are frequently seen as overly permissive and have difficulties setting limits on child demands. Gary clearly presented many behavioral concerns prior to his hospitalization and surgery. Sequential analysis of Gary's scores on the Roberts Apperception Test for Children (RATC), administered 93 one day prior to his surgery, indicated significant elevations (T > 60) on the following scales: Reliance on Others, Self-Sufficiency, Limit Setting, Anxiety, Depression, Rejection, and Unresolved Conflict. There were no significantly low scores (T ( 40). Gary's presurgical coping responses were characterized by high rates of limit setting, self- sufficiency, and reliance on others. Gary's high scores on Limit Setting suggest excessive concerns about pleasing his parents and authority figures, as others were frequently required to correct his wrong doings, e.g., “They did something bad and got grounded for three days,“ and “A gang tried picking her up. Her mother and dad said do not walk by the street again.“ His reliance on others was extremely high, indicating Gary's preoccupation with going to the hospital and getting help from others. In addition, he tended to use unrealistic solutions to problematic situations or left problems unresolved, e.g., “They're talking to their son about going into the hospital. They're sad. After that they were happy,“ and “He's getting into bed and he's nervous about tomorrow. That's all.“ Gary's high scores on Self-Sufficiency as a coping strategy, on the other hand, suggests that he was able to be self-reliant and displayed much affection for others. His self- sufficient coping responses were an important compensatory resource to the anxiety, depression, and 94 rejection he experienced, e.g., “He's glad to be back home from the hospital,“ and “He's got the answer done and he was happy.“ Examination of Gary's clinical scale scores suggest much sadness, worry, and rejection. The high rejection score reflects Gary's concern that parental conflict, accidents, or death might leave him feeling rejected and abandoned. In combination with his high rejection score, Gary's anxiety and depression suggest that his hospital admission and surgery had an overwhelming affect on his behavior and feelings. The RATC was readministered 10 days following Gary's surgery, prior to his discharge from the hospital. Sequential analysis indicated significant elevations on the following scales: Self-Sufficiency, Limit Setting, Constructive Resolution, Anxiety, Aggression, and Depression. There were no significantly low scores. Although several of Gary's coping strategies were the same preceeding and following surgery, e.g., self- sufficiency and limit setting, his reliance on others significantly decreased. In addition, Gary verbalized more constructive solutions to interpersonal problems, e.g., “If anyone pushes him, walk away, don't fight. He never fights again in school“ and “The father is trying to think how he can get them home safe. So he figured out to pick them up after school in the car.“ Gary also 95 expressed fewer feelings of rejection. Examination of his clinical scale scores indicates that Gary's level of aggression and depression were significantly higher following surgery than preceeding surgery. The wide scatter of Gary's scores suggests that being in the hospital was quite demanding. Physical restriction imposed by having full-length casts on both legs might have increased Gary's postsurgery levels of aggression and depression. Gary's postsurgical adaptation, measured 10 days following surgery, was very problematic. He exhibited significant behavioral concerns on all subscales of the Revised Behavior Problem Checklist (T > 73). Particularly noteworthy were his scores on the Attentional Problems and Anxiety/Withdrawal scales (greater than two standard deviations above the mean). Gary's long-term surgical adaptation, measured one month following discharge, continued to be problematic. There was an increase in aggressive behaviors, although he did demonstrate a decrease in anxious, fearful and depressed behaviors. Cenclusien: Gary's case illustrates two important points relative to this study. One, his level of premorbid adjustment was predictive of his surgical adaptation, both 10 days after surgery and 30 days following discharge. Problematic behaviors, both internalizing and externalizing, exhibited prior to 96 hospitalization, placed Gary at increased risk for experiencing problematic surgical adaptation. Two, although Gary used a wide variety of coping responses, including high levels of self-sufficiency and increased constructive resolution skills, the demands of the situation exceeded his ability or resources. He clearly experienced much anxiety about being in the hospital and having an operation. For example, his first response on the RATC was: “They're talking to their son about going to the hospital. They're sad.“ Rather than appraising the situation merely as a challenge to his resources, it was perhaps appraised as a threat to his well-being because of the compromised resources he had prior to the surgery. The situation was stressful for Gary and he demonstrated significant post and long-term surgical adaptation as a result. Case 121 Alex Alex was a 17-3 year old white male with a history of congenital scoliosis. Alex's scoliosis was diagnosed within the past year when an x-ray was taken following a football injury he sustained. A myelogram confirmed scoliosis of the thorasic region. Congential scoliosis is often caused by a defective embryologic development.o Alex suffered from the presence of a hemivertebra, or the asymmetric fusion of two vertebrae, eventually involving a 62 degree 97 curvature of the spine. The operation involved the insertion of rods along the spine and a fusion of the vertebrae involved, called a spinal fusion with Harrington rod instrumentation. Following the operation, Alex was placed in a circle bed to increase his movement, circulation, and skin integrity. A body cast was placed five days later. Alex remained in this cast for approximately six months. Alex developed several complications during and postsurgery. He lost over 90 cc of blood and was placed in the Intensive Care Unit for two days as a result. In addition, he developed an immunologic problem as a result of the operation. Postsurgery he was unable to urinate, necessitating that he be cathaterized. Test Results, Alex's mother completed the Personality Inventory for Children (PIC) while Alex was in the ICU. There was a tendency to minimize Alex's problem behaviors. There were no clinically significant elevations on the PIC. Overall, Alex's premorbid adjustment, as evidenced by the PIC, was excellent. Sequential analysis of Alex's scores on the Roberts Apperception Test for Children, administered one day prior to his surgery, indicated clinically significant elevations (T > 60) on the following scales: Constructive Resolution, Aggression, and Depression. There were no significantly low scores. Alex's presurgical coping responses were 98 characterized by high rates of constructive and insightful resolutions to problem situations. Frequently, new insights or problem solving abilities which generalized to new situations were expressed: “The son feels left out. The mother realizes this and tells him that she loves him just as much and the kid realizes it and learns to like his brother,“ and “He finally realizes that if he doesn't do it, he'll get bad grades and fail.“ Recognition of support by others was another coping strategy: “They'll talk about their problem,“ “She wakes up and her friend tells her to go home to sleep in bed,“ and “The dad asks him how much he needs, gives it to him, and lets him go to the show.“ However, low levels of problem identification were evident, suggesting much denial of conflict. Alex's anxiety about his operation was evident by significantly high levels of aggression and depression on the clinical scales. The wide variation in Alex's scores suggested that being in the hospital was an acutely disturbing situation. Sequential analysis of Alex's postsurgical coping responses indicated signficant scores on Self- Sufficiency and Insightful Resolution. Other clinically significant scales were Anxiety and Depression. Alex's postsurgical coping responses were characterized by high rates of self-sufficiency, 99 perception of appropriate limits, ability to recognize problem situations, and insightful solutions to problem situations. Self-sufficiency included responses such as: “He realizes that the quicker he does it, the sooner he can do what he wants,“ and “He thinks fighting is stupid and doesn't solve anything.“ Alex also used significantly more coping responses postsurgery in which problem situations were recognized: “He lost his job,“ and “A couple of guys are giving the black boy a hard time.“ Increased problem identification following surgery suggests that denial and avoidance served adaptive functions; it was necessary to deny the stressful aspects of the surgery in order to successfully adapt to a situation in which he had little control. Ten days following the surgery, however, Alex no longer needed to use high levels of denial and avoidance, as he had overcome a difficult postoperative recovery, including loss of blood, pain, traction, and inability to urinate, and could exhibit more control over his body and the environment. Alex also used more age appropriate and sophisticated resolution skills following surgery, e.g., “He realized that the quicker he does his homework, the sooner he can do what he wants. He does it and then goes outside,“ “They realized that just because he's black, he has feelings too. Eventually they became friends and play together,“ and “He helps the parents 100 and realizes that the parents love him as much as the baby.“ Alex's scores on the measure of postsurgical adaptation, administered 10 days following his surgery, indicated a significant elevation only on Motor Excess. All other scales were below the mean, suggesting no other significant behavioral reactions to the hospitalization and surgery. Follow-up assessment one month later indicated no problem areas. Qenelusien: This case study illustrates two important conclusions relevant to the present study. One, Alex's level of premorbid adjustment predicted his post and long-term surgical adaptation. He demonstrated no significant adjustment difficulties prior to his hospitalization and scoliosis operation. Alex's surgery and postoperative recovery were particularly demanding: he lost much blood, necessitating a stay in the ICU; he developed a serious immunologic problem postoperatively: and was unable to freely urinate. In spite of these obstacles, however, he demonstrated very positive adaptation using coping strategies such as support from others, self-sufficiency, and adequate problem- resolution skills. Two, a coping strategy characterized by denial and avoidance presurgy may have helped Alex more successfully adapt to the anxiety, aggression, and depression he experienced while in the hospital. Once 101 he overcame the immediate challenges imposed by the surgery, however, Alex returned to prestress levels of recognizing differences and difficulties. Denial and avoidance might have served an adaptive function for Alex in a stressful situation in which he had little control. Summarxeffindings Research questions about children's coping responses and adaptation to orthopedic surgery have been investigated by the testing of eight hypotheses and qualitative case analyses. The first two hypotheses investigated the main research question, which concerned the linear relationship between coping responses and surgical adaptation. The statistical hypothesis that there was a significant linear relationship between the magnitude of coping responses used and post and long-term surgical adaptation was not accepted for the total scales. However, there were two significant relationships amongst the subscales: (a) presurgical limit setting and long-term surgical adaptation, and (b) total presurgical coping and postsurgical anxiety-withdrawal. The second hypothesis that the magnitude of children's coping responses significantly differs preceeding surgery versus following surgery was not supported. The next four hypotheses investigated the linear 102 relationship between intrinsic stress factors and surgical adaptation. Hypothesis III, which investigated whether cognitive functioning was related to surgical adaptation, was not supported. Hypothesis IV, indicating that children with better premorbid adjustment will show better surgical adaptation, was supported for long-term surgical adaptation. The hypothesis (V) that age is related to surgical adaptation was equivocal. Although age was significantly correlated with postsurgical adaptation, it was confounded with postsurgical coping. Hypothesis VI investigated whether girls would show better surgical adaptation than would boys. The hypothesis was not supported. The last two hypotheses were related to the relationship between extrinsic stress factors and surgical adaptation. Hypothesis VII, which indicated that length of hospitalization was related to surgical adaptation, was supported using bivariate correlations. However, multiple regression analysis indicated that days hospitalized was confounded with the variable premorbid adjustment. The null hypothesis was thus not rejected. The last hypothesis, that children with more hospital admissions would exhibit less successful surgical adaptation, was not supported by any analysis. 103 Qualitative analyses of the data suggested that the relationships between surgical coping responses and adaptational outcomes were complex and affected by many idiopathic intrinsic and extrinsic factors. A discussion of the meaning and significance of these results is found in the next chapter, Chapter V. CHAPTER V DISCUSSION AND CONCLUSION Snmmaxx This study was undertaken in an effort to understand the relationships between coping responses and adaptational outcomes of children undergoing elective orthopedic surgery. Knowledge of these relationships is needed in order to prevent possible negative reactions due to hospitalization and surgery. The reactions and psychological adjustment of children to hospitalization and surgery can be hypothesized from a stress and coping model. This model is based on a theory of interactionism, whereby stress is defined as the interaction between the individual, his or her resources and capabilities, and the demands of the situation. Accordingly, factors both within the person and situational or extrinsic factors are important in understanding coping and adaptation. Psychological stress is thus defined as a relationship between the person and the environment that is thought to be a threat to one's well-being. Theorists advocating interactional models of stress and coping include: Lazarus and Folkman (1984), Sarason and Sarason (1981), Moos and Billings (1982), and Rutter (1983). 104 105 A descriptive research design in the form of a naturalistic field investigation was used, in conjunction with quantitative and descriptive analysis of the data. A sample of 27 children, ages 6 through 17, undergoing orthopedic surgery at a large, urban, children's hospital participated in the study. They completed the Roberts Apperception Test for Children, used to measure their coping reponses, at two points: once immediately preceeding surgery and again prior to discharge from the hospital. In addition, the child's parent completed the Revised Behavior Problem Checklist, used to measure surgical adaptation, at two points: once prior to discharge from the hospital and again one month post-discharge. Premorbid adjustment and cognitive functioning of the child were measured prior to the surgery, using the Personality Inventory for Children. Length of stay number of hospital admissions, age and gender were obtained from the parent and the medical chart. Eight hypotheses related to children's responses to hospitalization and surgery were developed and tested. The main hypothesis that children who score higher on measures of surgical coping will exhibit better surgical adaptation than those who score lower was not accepted for the total scores using statistical hypothesis testing. However, presurgical limit setting and long- term surgical adaptation were significantly correlated 106 in the predicted direction, such that children who perceived appropriate and constructive limits placed on their behaviors, as assessed prior to their surgery, also exhibited better surgical adaptation upon one month follow-up. In addition, total presurgical coping and postsurgical anxiety/withdrawal were significantly related: children who used greater numbers of total coping responses, as assessed prior to their surgery, also exhibited less anxiety and withdrawal after surgery. Children's coping responses did not significantly differ preceeding surgery versus following surgery. Furthermore, the hypotheses that children with higher intellectual functioning, shorter periods of hospitalization, or fewer hospital admissions will show better surgical adaptation were not supported. Girls did not show better surgical adaptation than did boys. The hypothesis that younger children will experience poorer short-term adjustments to hospitalization and surgery was supported using bivariate correlations but was not supported with the effects of postsurgical coping held constant. There was no significant correlation between age and long-term surgical adaptation. Finally, the hypothesis that premorbid adjustment would predict surgical adaptation at 30-day follow-up was supported. Qualitative data analysis, using case studies, 107 suggested that the relationships between coping responses and adaptational outcomes did exist, although they were complex and not easily demonstrated empirically. The following sections discuss the significance and meaning of the results of the research questions. Implications for children entering the hospital for orthopedic surgery and future research questions are also discussed. Discussieneftheflxnetheeee WWMWW Children's overall surgical coping responses were not significantly linearly related to their surgical adaptation using statistical hypotheses testing techniques. However, there were two exceptions: presurgical limit setting was related to long-term surgical adaptation, and total presurgical coping was related to postsurgical anxiety and withdrawal. A possible explanation for the results of Hypothesis I is that hospitalization and orthopedic surgery were not appraised as taxing or exceeding the child's resources or endangering his or her well-being. An interactional model of stress and coping states that an individual's cognitive appraisal of a stressful event is crucial to understanding how that person may respond to the stress or whether stress is perceived at all (Magnusson, 1982). An essential question is thus whether the children appraised the orthopedic surgery as 108 exceeding their resources and endangering their well- being? If so, at what point might they have appraised it as stressful--prior to the surgery, during anesthesia induction, or while in traction postsurgery? Were there factors that helped protect these children against the possible threat of hospitalization and surgery to their resources or well-being? There is general agreement in the literature that the presence and use of a wide variety of social resources facilitates children's adjustment to stressful life events. It has further been demonstrated that parent- child contacts during hospitalization are helpful in reducing the level of stress experienced by the child (Peterson, Mori, & Carter, 1985). Although there is solid evidence that even minor surgery is stressful for children (Burstein & Meichenbaum, 1979; Lambert, 1984: Skipper 5 Leonard, 1968), interviews with the children prior to and following the surgery indicated that the majority felt well-prepared and cared for while in the hospital. Although sometimes expressing anxiety about the surgical procedures, the older children frequently expressed confidence in their own ability to cope with the experience, particularly if they had successfully been through a similar procedure in the past. Younger children frequently expressed confidence in the ability of their parents or the nursing staff to care for them. 109 In fact, many of the parents functioned as paranurses, staying by their children's bedside and comforting and supporting them throughout their stay. It is possible that these children thus did not appraise the situation as a threat to their resources or well-being because of the support they received from parents and staff. Examples of coping responses used by children demonstrating support by others included: “the doctor is talking with the girl“, “mother is trying to make her feel better,“ “they are talking about going to the hospital and the parents are asking him how he feels,“ “the lady is hugging her child because he might be getting out of the hospital,“ and “he's explaining to the boy how its gonna be at the hospital.“ There is a notable exception, however. Qualitative observations indicated that children whose parent(s) did not remain in the hospital throughout the entire stay frequently exhibited much anxiety related to separation. This anxiety became particularly exacerbated when undergoing stressful procedures such as blood work or physical examinations by the orthopedic resident. At these times the child either cried and could not be easily comforted or, in the case of several adolescents, remained silent and withdrawn. A second possible explanation for the above results is that the children's expectations about the surgery influenced their postoperative adaptation (Burstein & 110 Meichenbaum, 1979; Cohen & Lazarus, 1973). Melamed and her associates concluded that appropriate psychological treatments prove effective for preparing children for hospitalization and medical procedures (Melamed et al., 1982). The children in the present study did not all participate in a preparation program. The hospital offered an optional Saturday morning program consisting of a film and discussion with the nursing staff, parents, and children in an effort to prepare children and their families for surgery. In addition, ward staff again helped prepare children and their families once the child was admitted. The child's doll was frequently used to demonstrate and model to the child the medical procedures necessary. Children thus were prepared on different levels; such preparation was not controlled for in the present study. Examples of children's coping responses indicative of the effects of preparation include: “the doctor is explaining to the girl what will be done,“ “they say it is just like going to sleep,“ and “he has been there before and knows what to expect.“ Another explanation for the nonsignificant correlations between total surgical coping and total surgical adaptation is measurement related, involving the choice of a multi-band outcome measure. The Revised Behavior Problem Checklist measures four broad behavior syndromes, in addition to two minor scales. Although 111 both externalizing and internalizing behaviors are noted in the literature in conjunction with hospitalization, interview data with the subjects and their parents suggest that attention and anxiety/withdrawal behaviors are predominant while in the hospital, while aggressive behaviors surface at home following discharge from the hospital. The restriction to a single broad band measurement instrument might be more effective in assessing children's behaviors following surgery. One overriding limitation effecting the results obtained is methodological. Although Table 4.4 presents many correlations in the .30 to .38 range, these were not statistically significant because of the relatively small sample size. Frequently, an increase of less than five subjects would have significantly added to the power of the statistic. The presence of statistically non-significant results in a correlational study employing a small “N“ does not mean that these relationships do not exist, however. A question is thus raised over meaningful versus statistical significance, suggesting that a more qualified conclusion be drawn about the relationship between children's surgical coping responses and adaptation; this relationship cannot be adequately described by the presence or absence of statistically significant correlations alone. A more qualified conclusion, based on both empirical and qualitative data 112 analysis, is that relationships between surgical coping responses and adaptational outcomes exist. However, the inherent complexities in these relationships do not easily lend themselves to the rigor of hypothesis testing. Both intrinsic and extrinsic factors, such as the presence of social supports, may affect the relationship between children's coping responses and adaptation to surgery. Further study, using more restrictive measures but greater sample size, is warranted. While the major aspects of Hypothesis I were not supported, two correlations involving subscales of coping and adaptation were significant in the predicted direction using regression analysis: (a) presurgical limit setting and long-term surgical adaptation and (b) total presurgical coping and postsurgical anxiety and withdrawal. Presurgical limit setting reflects the child's perception of reasonable and appropriate limits placed by adults in response to a violation of rules or expectation, as assessed one day or less prior to surgery. Long-term surgical adaptation refers to observable behaviors one month following discharge from the hospital. While significant correlations might be expected merely out of chance occurance, the magnitude and direction of these correlations are notable. The results suggest that children who perceived appropriate 113 limits placed on their behaviors by adults, as assessed one day or less prior to their surgery, exhibited better long-term surgical adaptation than those who perceived fewer limits in their environment. One possible explanation for this result is once again the role that social supports, particularly intra-familial, play in children's adaptation to orthopedic surgery. Children whose coping responses include firm limits and guidelines on their behavior, i.e., support from their environment, demonstrate better long-term surgical adaptation. Appropriate and firm limit setting is associated with better child adjustment in the develomental literature (Baumrind, 1968). Such limits provide the child with a sense of stability, security, and trust. It is the presence of a caring, authoritative adult that is important. In addition, the correlation is between limit setting and lengztetm surgical adaptation. It is probable that limit setting does not occur only when a child is in the hospital, but rather prior to and after the surgery as well. Examples of coping responses used by subjects included: appropriate punishments for doing something wrong, telling the child what not to do, limiting or restricting the child's behavior, having to take medicine, and getting grounded for bad school grades. The correlation between total presurgical coping 114 and postsurgical anxiety and withdrawal was also significant. A possible explanation for this correlation is the role that action strategies play in reducing the impact of a stressful event. It has been hypothesized that active cognitions play an essential role in coping with stressful situations. Action strategies help form and maintain a set of illusions by looking at the known facts in a particular light in order to maintain the most positive picture possible (Curry & Russ, 1985; Taylor, 1983). Children who used greater numbers of coping responses prior to their surgery thus demonstrated less anxiety and withdrawal while in the hospital. Rather than passive acceptance of the situation, these children used active strategies to gain a sense of mastery and control the meaning of the experience. Passive acceptance, helplessness, and depression have been linked to higher morbidity and mortality rates in a number of investigations (Lazarus & Folkman, 1984). On the other hand, high levels of cognitive arousal are related to successful surgical adaptation (Ray 8 Fitzgibbon, 1981). Effective coping might have depended not only on what the subjects did, but also on how much they did (Curry & Russ, 1985). Active cognitions might thus have functioned to help the subjects form and maintain a set of illusions by looking at the known facts in a particular light in 115 order to maintain the most positive picture possible. Other researchers (Alloy & Abramson, 1979; Cohen & Lazarus, 1973: Taylor, 1983) have concluded that avoidance and illusion may be cognitive processes that serve essential adaptive functions. If one is to maintain hope, the opposite of depression, then one must select the positive aspects of the situation and believe that one's actions make a difference. Breznitz (Turkington, 1984) maintains that a sense of hope is essential for successful adaptation to stressful situations. Hope involves active work, as opposed to passive acceptance. Coping responses indicative of children's efforts to maintain hope included: “He went to the hospital and found out it wasn't a bad place after all,“ “his surgery wasn't bad because he didn't even know,“ “when the person gets out of surgery, then he'll be alright,“ and “maybe her mom and dad might be separated and they might be getting together again.“ Hypothesis II predicted that the number of coping responses would significantly differ preceeding surgery versus following surgery. The hypothesis was not supported. The results obtained suggest that coping efforts, in a quantitative sense, remained relatively stable throughout the child's hospital stay. Qualitative examination of the subject's coping responses on the RATC administrations again suggest stability. The children's stories demonstrated quite 116 similar thematic content one day prior to their surgery and before their discharge from the hospital. One further way to investigate the stability of children's coping reSponses to orthopedic surgery is to examine the test-retest reliability coefficients obtained for the sample. Overall test-retest reliability for the sample is remarkably high (r 8 .86), given the nature of stimulus bound thematic apperception tests such as the RATC to be influenced by state factors of the child. More weight can thus be given to the conclusion that coping responses are stable over time, as opposed to predictions from an interactional model of stress to the contrary. Whelatedtelntrinsiefiressfaetm Four hypotheses related to intrinsic stress factors were developed and tested. Intrinsic factors hypothesized to affect adaptational outcomes to orthopedic surgery were: cognitive functioning, premorbid adjustment, age, and gender. Hypothesis III predicted that the child's cognitive functioning would be significantly related to post and long-term surgical adaptation. Correlations were not significant at the p (.05 level: further regression analysis did not reveal significant correlations. The hypothesis was not supported. Not only was cognitive functioning not significantly related to surgical adaptation, but examination of the correlation matrix 117 indicates that cognitive functioning was not related to measures of surgical coping. One possible explanation for this result is that the advantages of good scholastic achievement and ' intellectual functioning do not protect children from the adversities imposed by hospitalization and surgery. Although this finding seems to contradict the work of Rutter (1983), it should be noted that his finding referred to long-term functioning. On the other hand, the present study focused on adaptation to a specific stressor over a relatively short period of time (three to thirty days). The value of superior cognitive functioning might be evident over the long haul, across many stressors, as opposed to discrete episodes of stress measured in days. Hypothesis IV predicted that children's premorbid adjustment, i.e., the variety and frequency of problem behaviors prior to admission to the hospital for orthopedic surgery, was related to their surgical adaptation. Initial results indicated that premorbid adjustment was significantly correlated with both post and long-term surgical adaptation at the p (.05 level. Further regression analysis indicated that the relationship between premorbid adjustment and postsurgical adaptation (short-term) was confounded by the number of days the child was hospitalized. However, the relationship between premorbid adjustment and long- 118 term surgical adaptation was significant at the p (.005 level. These results raise several important issues. The most important is that children's overall psychological adjustment was the most powerful predictor, investigated in the present study, of their behavioral responses to hospitalization and orthopedic surgery. Children with poorer premorbid adjustment were more likey to develop behavioral concerns at home one month following their operations. Premorbid adjustment was an important individual difference that mediated the impact of hospitalization and surgery over the first month following discharge. One possible explanation for the finding that premorbid adjustment predicted long—term surgical adaptation is the role that a child's psychological resources play in his or her adaptation to stressful events. Reseachers, most notably Garmezy (1976; 1983) consistently refer to the role that a positive personality disposition plays in protecting children who are exposed to stressful events. The ability of these children to meet and conquer the adversities imposed by the rigors of hositalization and painful orthopedic surgery, including traction, forced dependency, and loss of privacy might depend, in large part, upon the psychological resources they bring into the stressful encounter. 119 Why was “premorbid adjustment“ confounded with the variable “days hospitalized? The importance of psychological resources in protecting children from the effects of hospitalization and surgery might also depend upon the severity of the stress they encounter while in the hospital. There is much research to support the hypothesis that the simple accumulation of stressful events is significant; the more stressors encountered, the more severe the reactions. The severity of the children's orthopedic surgery could roughly be measured by the number of days they remained in the hospital. Children undergoing spinal fusions for scoliosis generally remained in the hospital for 10 days and remained in body casts for up to six months following discharge from the hospital. Removal of excess bone growth or heel cord lengthening, on the other hand, necessitated a hospital stay of two to three days. Because the relationship between premorbid adjustment and postsurgical adaptation was confounded by the number of days the child was hospitalized, the results suggest that in the short run, the affects of premorbid adjustment are not signficant when adjusted for the severity of the stressor. However, it is in the long run that the importance of a child's psychological resources is more evident. Although researchers using case studies have suggested that children with preexisting psycholgical 120 problems will experience more severe reactions to hospitalization and surgery (Barnes, Kenny, Call, & Reinhart, 1972), there has been no research to date in which the reactions of children with and without preexisiting psychological problems are compared (Lambert, 1984). The results of the present study thus represent an important first step in studying the correlations between premorbid adjustment and adaptational outcomes to orthopedic surgery. Hypothesis V predicted that different age children will show different post and long-term surgical adaptation. The literature states that the age period of greatest risk is about 6 months to 4 years. On the other hand, the advanced metacognitive processes of older children could also foster greater anxieties in children (Maccoby, 1983), as they might forsee the possible threats to their well-being associated with hospitalization and surgery but are unable to protect themselves. The results of Hypothesis V were equivocal. Age was significantly correlated with postsurgical adaptation (p (.05), such that younger children experienced greater postsurgical difficulties than older children. However, age was confounded with total postsurgical coping, but not with presurgical coping. One explanation for this result is that hospitalization and orthopedic surgery are stressful for 121 younger children with deficient coping responses. Young school-aged children who utilize fewer numbers of cognitive responses exhibit poorer adadptation while in the hospital. Younger children who are more active in using coping responses, on the other hand, may be at less risk for developing adjustment difficulties postsurgery. In addition, older children as a group have developed cognitive processes that allow them to actively respond to being in the hospital. The results suggest that older children utilized greater numbers of cognitive responses. Once the child returned home, however, his or her age was not signficiantly associated with surgical adaptation. It is perhaps only during the intense stress associated with being in a hospital that age plays a role in surgical adaptation. At home the surroundings are no longer unfamiliar, as the child is reunited with his or her family. Hypothesis VI predicted that girls would show better post and long-term surgical adaptation than would boys. The hypothesis was not supported. There were no significant differences between males and females on any of the total surgical adaptation measures. Researchers such as Rutter (1983) state that boys tend to be more vulnerable to the effects of stress and, in particular, hospital admissions. However, the boys in this sample did not quantitatively exhibit poorer 122 surgical adaptation, as compared with the girls. In retrospect, a more pertinent line of investigation with regard to gender is whether boys and girls use different coping responses. For example, do boys use physical aggression and controlling behaviors and girls use verbal aggression and orienting behaviors as Lambert (1984) suggests? Further research is needed to determine if the quality of girls and boys coping responses to orthopedic surgery differ, regardless of their surgical adaptation. Wfielatedteflznineiefitreeefaetere Hypothesis VII predicted that length of hospitalization was a significant factor in children's surgical adaptation, such that children with longer periods of hospitalization would exhibit less successful post and long-term surgical adaptation than would children with shorter periods of hospitalization. Although the correlation between days hospitalized and long-term surgical adaptation was significant in the predicted direction, it was not significant with the effects of premorbid adjustment held constant. The hypothesis was not supported. One possible explanation for the results is that the children hospitalized for longer periods of time had serious orthopedic conditions that affected their level of premorid adjustment. The long-term cases in the 123 sample consisted of children undergoing spinal fusions for correction of scoliosis. There is a large body of literature describing the affects of chronic physical illness or disability on children's adjustment (Moos & Tsu, 1977). Scoliosis is a deformity involving lateral curvature of the spine and is often progressive through childhood. Many nonsurgical and conservative treatments are available such as exercises to improve posture, bed rest, traction, and plaster cast or brace. Surgical treatment is indicated when curvatures cannot be stisfactorily improved, or their improvement satisfactorily maintained, by nonsurgical measures (Raney, Brashear, & Shands, 1971). The scoliosis patients in this sample had been in and out of the orthopedic clinic many times. Scoliosis is a disfiguring deformity for children and adolescents that would have psychosocial consequences. In addition, the scoliosis was frequently associated with other physical conditions, including muscular dystrophy. The variable days hospitalized was very much linked to the severity of the orthopedic condition. It is thus not surprising that the variable days hospitalized was signficantly confounded with the variable premorbid adjustment. Together, these two independent variables accounted for 19% of the variance in postsurgical adaptation (p >.05) and 51% of the variance in long-term adaptation (p (.001). 124 Hypothesis VIII predicted that children with more prior hospital admissions would exhibit less successful post and long-term surgical adaptation than would children with fewer hospital admissions. The hypothesis was not supported. There is continued debate in the literature whether previous hospitalization positively or negatively affects the degree of emotional upset during or following subsequent hospitalizations (Lambert, 1984). The children in this sample with many prior hospitalizations did not experience more difficulties postsurgery or in the month following discharge from the hospital. Qualitative analysis indicates that in general the benefits of orthopedic surgery outweight the drawbacks. Conversations with parents and children prior to the surgery indicated much variation in children's reactions to coming again to the hospital for surgery. Many parents stated that their child expressed great displeasure about the upcoming hospital admission and exhibited increasingly irritable and argumentative behaviors. Still other parents, usually of adolescents, stated that their child was ready and eager to get their body “fixed,“ although apprehension was nonetheless expressed by the child. There are thus likely many other variables not directly addressed in the present study, such as the child's and parent's view of the 125 orthopedic difficulty and orthopedic surgery, that affected whether prior admissions was related to the child's surgical adaptation. Limitatiens Qt the Study The most significant limitation of the study concerns the size of the sample. A sample of 27 children affects both the empirical results and the generalizations that can be made, or the external validity. Quite frequently, relatively large correlations were obtained in the predicted direction in the testing of the hypotheses, but because of the small sample size, these correlations were not statistically significant at the p (.05 level. The conclusions based on the empirical results are thus limited by methodological constraints. In addition, the generalizability of the results is limited. With a sample of 27 subjects, it is more difficult to generalize the results beyond the sample itself. Generalization to a larger patient population is often made using the Tukey-Cornfield Bridge argument (Glass & Stanley, 1970). This is a logical argument allowing for inferrences to be made from non-randomized samples to populations of interest, provided that the characteristics of the sample are described in detail. Of the 33 subjects eligible and asked to participate in the study, 27, or 82%, completed the study. This 126 participation rate is quite high and compares favorably with other published studies in the field (Curry & Russ, 1985). The subject pool consisted of adequate numbers of minority children. However, boys outnumbered girls three-to-one. Socioeconomic status was varied, as both private and orthopedic clinic patients participated. Use of the Tukey-Cornfield Bridge argument allows for the cautious generalization of these results to the larger pediatric orthopedic population undergoing similar operations. Another limitation of the study was the use of a projective test to measure surgical coping responses. Although the Roberts Apperception Test for Children has adequate psychometric properties, projective tests in general have certain limitations in terms of construct validity. Other methods of assessing children's coping responses, such as structured interviews, were considered in the present study, but also have limitations associated with using retrospective self- report measures. However, the use of projective measures is well-supported by stress researchers (Moos, 1974), and is one viable method of exploring coping processes, particularly when such processes cannot be readily observed. In addition to the above limitations, naturalistic field investigations are unable to control for many confounding variables. Although an effort was made to 127 control for confounding among the independent variables under investigation by using multiple regression analysis, other variables were not controlled for and might be confounders. Confounding variables might include expectations regarding the surgery, and the amount of social support and preparation the child received prior, during, and following surgery. mnlieatienseftheaeseareh The results of this research have implications for hospital staff and parents of children entering the hospital for orthopedic surgery. The clearest finding of the study is that children's premorbid adjustment functions as a powerful predictor of surgical adaptation. Children experiencing various and frequent problem behaviors prior to their surgery exhibit higher rates of both internalizing and externalizing behaviors one month after they are discharged from the hospital. It is thus important to adequately screen for children at risk for problematic postsurgical adaptation prior to orthopedic surgery. Appropriate secondary prevention strategies can then be used. Intervention programs specifically aimed at this population of children can be implemented. Such interventions might include increased parental and hospital staff support, behavioral programs, modeling procedures, systematic desensitization, and exposure therapy (Melamed et al., 1982; Peterson, Mori, & Carter, 1985). This implication 128 is important to nursing staff, pediatric and orthopedic residents, and pediatric psychologists involved with providing care for children undergoing orthopedic surgery. It is also important that children are encouraged by hospital staff and parents to use action-oriented cognitive strategies prior to orthopedic surgery in order to limit behaviors indicative of anxiety and withdrawal following the surgery. The use of coping strategies such as reliance on others, self-sufficiency, problem-identification, and problem resolution should be encouraged so that the child may maintain a sense of hope. Traditional puppet therapy, modeling procedures, and exposure therapy can be used, depending on the age of the child, to help encourage active use of strategies, as opposed to passive acceptance of the situation. In this context, the defiance sometimes noted of children by hospital staff can be viewed as a positive attempt of children to cope with a situation that is not totally under their control. Finally, the results suggest that it is important that parents and hospital staff continue to impose appropriate but firm limits on the children's behavior prior to the orthopedic surgery. Children's perceptions of such limits are important in terms of their adaptation one month following discharge from the hospital. 129 Directiensfertntnreaeseareh The present study, while directly assessing the relationships between coping responses and adaptational outcomes of children undergoing elective orthopedic surgery, merely begins the process of describing the relationships. The results of the study indicate the need for further investigation of children's responses to various surgeries and medical procedures, pose new questions for researchers in the stress and coping field, and provide directions for future research. More information is needed regarding how children appraise the stress associated with hospitalization and major surgical procedures. Are there particular points that they appraise as stressful, e.g., admission, first night in the hospital, transport to the operating room, anesthesia induction, postoperative pain and restriction, or discharge? Interviews with the child as the stress occurs is advantageous. However, assessment of cognitive appraisal during the actual coping episode is not always possible or appropriate in terms of the treatment process. A coping interview, based on prospective and retrospective self report, can be used (Curry and Russ 1985). A second needed line of inquiry is whether children's expectations about their surgeries influence their surgical adaptation. For example, if children 130 believe that the positive results of the surgery will outweigh the negatives, do they demonstrate more’ positive adaptational outcomes than those who do not expect good results? Use of elective orthopedic surgery patients would allow the researcher to interview children at various points prior to the surgery and compare their expectations to their surgical adaptation. The role of social supports in helping children cope with the adversities of hospitalization and surgery is powerful and requires future research. What are the essential aspects of social supports that help children cope with surgery? Are children of single parent, foster care, or institutionalized families at increased risk for developing emotional difficulties? Social support might be measured in a number of ways, including number of visitors, time spent visiting, hospital staff time, and time on the telephone. In order to explore gender issues in relation to adaptation to orthopedic surgery, comparisons of the coping responses used by girls and boys is necessary. Do boys use more physical agression and controlling responses, while girls use more verbal aggression and orienting responses? Implications from this line of inquiry could assist with predicting and explaining differential reactions to surgery and help prepare boys and girls for surgery based on their coping strengths. Use of more narrow band outcome measures is 131 necessary in order to more fully describe surgical adaptation. Internalizing symptomatology is more prevalent while the child is in the hospital, while externalizing symptomatology becomes more prevalent after the child returns home. Although research on stress and coping is exploratory in nature, use of larger sample sizes is necessary, once the variables of interest have been narrowed. Further research is necessary in order to increase the statistical power of the analyses while not prematurely narrowing the scope of the inquiry. Cenelusien The results of this research highlight the resiliance in children under stress. Most children exposed to a stressor of marked gravity, associated with a heightened probability of maladaptive outcomes, demonstrated behavior marked by adapation and competence. The “stress resistance“ of these children is noteworthy and is a starting point for research on protective factors and the processes they activate. However, those children demonstrating poorer premorbid adjustment are at risk for developing adverse reactions to orthopedic surgery. This subgroup of children requires special attention as they prepare to enter the hospital; their stress resistance is 132 lower as they are less apt to have available effective protective factors. This minority poses a special challenge to parents and hospital staff to insure the most advantageous physical and psychological adaptation possible. APPENDICES APPENDIX A CONSENT FORM i133 l‘ ‘ Children’s HOSPITAL OF M|CH|GAN DETROIT MEDICAL CENTER - 3901 Boaubion Boulevard/Detroit. Michigan 48201 Dear Parent(s): Your child will soon be undergoing orthopedic surgery at Children’s Hospital of Michigan. Children and their parents often find hospitalization and surgery to be stressful. I am a Psychologist in the Department of Psychiatry/Psychology and am studying how children cope with their surgery. I would like to have you and your child participate in a research study I am conducting. This work may improve our understanding of surgical stress in order to develop new ways of helping children facing similar operations. It is sponsored by your child’s surgeon, Dr. LaMont, the Department of Psychiatry/Psychology, and Michigan State University. You will be asked to complete two questionnaires about your child, both while s/he is at the hospital and when you see Dr. Lauont for a follow-up visit. Altogether these questionnaires require about one hour of time. Your child will be asked to make up stories for 16 pictures for which there are no right or wrong answers, requiring about one hour of time. The questionnaires are currently used by the Department of Psychiatry/Psychology. Your participation is completely voluntary. Enclosed is a consent form that more fully describes your participation in the study. If you are willing to participate, please sign the form and leave it with the receptionist, or mail it to me in the stamped envelope provided. I will call you at home in order to more fully explain the study and answer any questions you might have. Thank you for your consideration. Sincerely, aJLRdm'a/ H4- aul Robins, H.A. Psychiatry/Psychology 133 A Priuxc. Voluntary. Non-prom Hospital Orgamzcd an 1886 13A Consent Form No. 1985-03 Children and their parents often find hospitalization and surgery to be stressful. Paul Robins, M.A. is a psychologist, and is trying to learn more about the stresses of pediatric surgery for children. This work will help improve the understanding of surgical stress in order to develop new ways of helping children. The work is sponsored by Children's Hospital of Michigan and Michigan State University. He would like us to help him learn about how children cope with surgery by taking part in a study. We will be asked to complete some questions which ask about: a) background information (child's age, gender, days hospitalized, etc.); b) child's personality (likes, dislikes, skills, etc.); c) child's behavior (attention seeking, shy, etc.). Your child will be asked to make up stories for 16 pictures for which there are no right or wrong answers, both prior to and following his or her surgery. We understand that it will take approximately one hour of our time and one hour of our child's time to participate in this study. The investigator will give us the questionnaires both in the hospital and when we bring our child to the doctor for a follow-up visit. All information obtained will be kept confidential. No names will be placed on any of the questionnaires, nor will our names be used in any reports of the study. When the study is completed, we will be mailed a written summary of the results if we so desire. This is a research study, and it does not involve any psychological treatment. However, it may be of benefit to us in that we may learn how children cope with hospitalization and surgery. We may withdraw from this study at any time. If we do withdraw from this study, the hospital and the doctors will still give us the best care that they can, both currently and if we are treated there in the future. Information about what we learned from this study may be published, or given to other people doing research, but our names will not be used. 135 Participation in this study does not entail any known risks. However, in the unusual event of emotional upset resulting from completing the questionnaires or stories, no compensation and no free psychological treatment or reimbursement is offered. Any questions we have asked about this study have been answered. If we have other questions later on, Paul Robins, M.A. will answer them for us. He can be reached at 494-4878 weekdays from 8:00 a.m. to 5:00 p.m. We also may call Dr. Flossie Cohen, who can be reached at 494-5566. She is Chairman of the Children's Hospital of Michigan Research Grants and Investigation Committee and will answer any questions we have about our rights as part of a research study. By signing this paper we are saying that we have read and understood it and that we agreed to take part in this study. Name of Participant Signature of Child (if over age of 13) CHM # (PDB #) Signature of Participant or Parent or Legal Guardian Date Physician's Signature Witness The child assented to participate in this project. Investigator APPENDIX B THE ROBERTS APPERCEPTION TEST FOR CHILDREN R337? .. km d‘t~‘:.,_‘, ‘-.:»f‘ on 1| Coal 26 Can! an . cm 4 hut, label-l W Ochoa All“ CHM WW @323 Q .4351 it *3! E M' " Q’ ‘ )5 \5 k“ ’ fly” .4 ,V,’ cause Card” Call mam Feet/mm 'mlym v . 2.3,, .2“ .. l ‘ ’- ' a «c- .é‘w f . Card. Card" Call" cums macaw mam he 9mm i ,| I K «"3. I[ g, 1K3 «a ~ - .r" «a -~ gait: r E; k ‘4' J‘ - Ikgflv «1"? ' M100 Card“ Card!“ muons-en. W mm Figure I The RATC Stimulus Cards 3 136 137 Summary Score Sheet - ' Protile Scales Supplementary Clinical Indicators Identification Resolution 2 Aggression Depression Unresolved Italadaplive Refusal Resolution 3' Resolution 1 E 2 a o h 3 Rejection Interpersonal Matrix ass 6—7 0-9 10-12 13-15 'For ages 13-15 only. Resolution 3 should be plotted as a prolile scale. 138 O Ayala. a 'taIiJ Lung“- llllllllll N u _,&IIIII8HIIIIIIIIILIlll&lllllslllI‘lgllllélllléllll Ines lll-lz meme wow 1 or area sets 'u no so: use tea tea me an out n: _ - — D '5 -- _ - — W 3 " - ts " - - - to - "E - - _ - . _ : - _ _ '5 75. ._ _ ._ E I: ._ : - to as; — _ — _ - _ - " — _ s 2 . : . - _ ‘ — o: _ , _ .- E - Io ‘ '° - - . 5: _ - _ - _ — _ 5 - ”E - - - - — s - ‘55 - a .. 4- E _ ° - ' - - o to: s o - o o as; ’ - : i - ‘ 3 ° ° : - so; _ _ 3- : _ — o 25- ‘ ' It awe an t- ees so: use an see are so use In low m___._____.______m “I '3.“ W am It IO! "C I. M ”I It! I” In H I! IJ ._ _ IS 0 - q 9 I I — - — — 5 5 — 5 - — _ — - .E, - - - 5 _ - D s _ _ - - U. - - - - - - - - ‘ a I?IIIIYIIII?IIII?IIII?Illll‘lllI?”ll?l|ll?llll?llll?llll l I ‘8 $llll£llll*_ £Illléllll£llll$llll Llllléllll lllllllllllllll ‘8 139 The RATC differs from other apperception tests commonly used with children, e.g., the CAT and TAT, in five major respects: a) it is specificially designed for children and thus depicts children in all 16 stimulus cards, b) it emphasizes everyday interpersonal events, as opposed to psychosexual or adult concerns, c) the stimuli show realistic drawings of children and adults executed in a uniform style by the same artist, d) it employs easily scored, objective measures which yield high interrater agreement, and e) it provides normative data for a sample of 200 well-adjusted children ages 6 through 15. The More Scales The eight adaptive scales used in the study as measures of surgical coping are each described below (McAruthur & Roberts, 1982). The parentheses indicate the name of the scale for purposes of the study. Reliance on chete. The Reliance on Others scale assesses a child's tendency to make up stories in which the character reaches out to others for help in solving a problem, either external or intrapsychic. The scale measures the adaptive capacity to use outside help to overcome a problem. Examples include: “he went to tell his parents,“ “he talked it over with his mom,“ and “please can't I hold him.“ - . The Support-Other scale reflects a tendency to support others by giving assistance, emotional support, or material objects. Common examples of thematic content include granting a request, buying a present, or giving professional help: “he reads him the story,“ and “I love you.“ uppottzchild. (Self-Sufficiency) The Support- Child scale measures self-sufficiency and maturity as indicated by assertiveness or the experience of positive emotions. Common examples of thematic content include curiosity, positive feelings about self, pride in work, joy, expectation, affection, and a good dream: “he's excited,“ and “he's thinking hard about all the answers.“ Problem Identification. The Problem Identification scale indicates the child's ability to formulate concepts beyond the nature of the card. The ability to engage in and articulate problem situations is considered adaptive. It requires a lack of defensiveness, verbal ability, and the capacity to view« others in complex, dynamic ineractions. Common examples include marital separation, not knowing what to do, inability to handle school work, getting lost, lying, environmental disaster, and having problems: “he's wondering what's going on,“ and “they want to go surfing and they don't have a ride.“ Limit Setting. The Limit Setting scale reflects the child's perception of reasonable and appropriate 1&0 limits placed in response to a violation of rules or expectations. Low scores may reflect a lack of parental involvement or concern. Common examples include scolding, punishment, explaining what the child did wrong, making the child do something over, or showing the child how to do something the right way: “the father took him to his room and he went to bed without his supper,“ “his dad's telling him not to do that any more,“ and “he had to clean up the paint on the walls.“ Reeeiutien i. (Unrealistic Solutions) The Resolution 1 scale reflects a child's tendency to seek easy or unrealistic solutions to problem solutions that have magical, wish-fulfilling, or unrealistic quality. Common themes include living happily ever after, and suddenly not having a problem. Resolutign 2. (Constructive Solution) The Resolution 2 scale indicates a constructive resolution of a problem either external or intrapsychic. The resolution is limited to the present situation. It is not accompianed by any new insights or approaches which may be applied to new problem situations. This scale involves resolution without explanation of the process or “working through“ of the problem, e.g., “he figures out it was just a dream and he goes back to sleep,“ and “she'll go get help and make sure she's 0K.“ Beselutien 3. (Insightful Solution) The Resolution 3 scale indicates a constructive resolution which goes beyond the immediate problem or conflict situation. The character in a story acquires new insight or develops problem-solving abilities which generalize to new situations. It occurs developmentally for older children. The authors investigated the relationship among the stimulus cards, i.e., which cards evoked similar responses in children and what were the underlying dimensions. A factor analysis was performed and yielded four factors, which accounted for 89% of the common variance. Roid (1983) explored two kinds of factor structure using more complex factors methods: a) the structure of the 13 basic profile scales, and b) a hierarchical analysis of the structure of individual cards in the context of selected profile indicators. The findings replicated the three-factor structure of profile scales described by McArthur and Roberts (1982), i.e., Adaptive, Clinical, and Developmental. In addition, specific combinations of cards elicited a structure of profile indicators that suggested the presence of four higher-order factors: a) internalization, b) externalization, c) aggressive—destructive, and d) degree of maturity in the child's resolution of stories. An initial test of criterion-related validity was 1141 conducted by the authors (Zachary, 1983). In order to affirm the ability of the RATC to discriminate between clinic and nonclinic groups, the RATC was administered to a heterogeneous sample of 200 clinic chidren. A multiple regression was calculated and resulted in a multiple R of .79, which was highly significant, P(12, 387)=48.53; p<.001. 62% of the variance in group membership was accounted for by the 13 profile scales. In addition, clinic and nonclinic children differed significantly on 12/15 rating variables at the .01 level or better, providing further evidence of the RATC's validity. Muha (1977) assessed the ability of the RATC to discriminate successfully between clinic and nonclinic families. 10 cards were administered to clinic and nonclinic families. Family functioning was also observed and rated, using a standardized evaluation scale. The RATC protocals were scored on 13 RATC measures. Results indicated that the two groups differed significantly on 8 of the 13 measures. All significant differences were in the expected directions. McArthur (1976) compared the clinical effectiveness of three projective tests, i.e., the RATC, CAT, and TAT. Stimulus cards from each test were selected and administered to 98 white males ages 8 and 11, who were judged by their teachers to be well-adjusted. Results indicated that the RATC elicited a significantly lower percentage of stereotyped responses than either the CAT or TAT. The author maintained that projective stimuli should elicit a minimum of stereotyped responses and hence the RATC is useful in a manner for which it is intended. Hersh (1978) investigated the predictive validity of the RATC and obtained mixed results. Subjects included 25 children, ages 6 to 13, referred for psychiatric evaluation. A wide range of presenting complaints were included. Therapeutic change was assessed by comparing children's scores on the RATC and four additional measures pre and post treatment. The four outcome measures were published and unpublished rating scales, filled out by the child's parents or teachers, thus functioning as independent criterion scores against which to measure the validity of the RATC. Results indicated that the children improved over the course of theraPY: as assessed both by changes in the children's average scores of the RATC, and by the independent criteria of parent and teachers' ratings. However, the RATC scales did not correlate significantly with the independent ratings of the severity of the children's presenting problems at intake, or the direction and magnitude of changes in individual children as the result of therapy. APPENDIX C THE PERSONALITY INVENTORY FOR CHILDREN PERSONALITY INVENTORY FOR CHILDREN REVISED FORMAT " ADMINISTRATION BOOKLET by ROBERT D. WIRT. Ph.D. PHILIP D. SEAT. Ph.D. WILLIAM E. BROEN. Jr.. Ph.D. Revision by DAVID LACHAR. Ph.D. Why *- VEST!!! minimum - habitation one Dim w ”031m. w us Armies. Cal-torn. sous _— This inventory consists oI statements about children and tamily rela- tionships. The inventory items are presented in tour parts. DIRECTIONS: First Iill in the information requested on the answer sheet; then read each ol the statements in this booklet and decide whether it is true or Ialse as applied to your child. Swim 0, ans“, sheet correctly Look at the example at the answer sheet shown mm“, at the right. In the example the parent decided that statement 25 was true as applied to the child I F and statement 26 was Ialse as applied to the child. 25 O G) 26 o o II a statement is TRUE or MOSTLY TRUE. as applied to your child. use a pencil to blacken the circle labeled T (See 25 in the example). It a statement is FALSE or NOT USUALLY TRUE. as applied to your child. blacken the circle labeled F (See 26 in the example). . In marking your answers on the answer sheet. be sure that the number of the statement agrees with the number on the answer sheet. Make your marks heavy and black. Erase completely any answer you wish to change. 00 not make any marks on this booklet. Continue to answer the inventory items unless instructed to stop at the end oI Part I. II. or III. Copyrth . l9". l98l by WESTERN PSYCHOLOGICAL SERVICES Not to be reproduced in whole or in part withOut written permssion at Western Psychological Services All rights reserved 2 3 4 5 6 7 0 9 Primed in U SA “RH?! 142 143 DO NOT MAKE ANY MARKS ON THIS BOOKLET v PART] |2. l3. l4. l5. |6. |7. 2|. 22. 23. 24. 23. . My child often plays with a group of children. . My child hardly ever smiles. . Other children often get mad at my child. . My child worries about things that usually only adqu worry about. . My child has many friends. . My child seems avenge or above avenge in intelligence. . My child's manners sometimes embarrass me. . My child has a good sense of humor. . My child sometimes secs things that aren't there. . My child is worried about sin. . Other children don‘t seem to listen to or notice my child much. My child sonretirncs undresses outside. My child has little self-confidence. I often wish my child would be more friendly. My child can comb his (her) own hair. My child is usually rejected by other children. My child seems to enjoy destroying things. . Now and then my child writes letters to friends. . Thunder and lightning bother my child. . The school says my child needs help in getting along with other children. My child often asks if I love him (her). Other children look up to my child as a loader. My child could ride a tricycle by age five years. My child sometimes gets angry. My child frequently complains of being hot even on cold days. 27. 3|. 32. 33. 35. 37. 39. 4|. 42. 43. 45. 47. 49. . My child's behavior often makes others angry. Recently my child has complained of eye trouble. . Others think my child is talented. . My child frequently has gas' on the stomach (sour stomach). . . My child is good at lying his (her) way out of trouble. My child often cheats other children in deals. My child is good at leading games and things. A: 3... time my child had speech difficulties. . Pestcring others is a problem with my child. My child can out things with scissors as well as can others of his (her) age. . My child doesn‘t seem to care to be with others. My child has difficulty doing things with his (her) hands. . Others think my child is mean. My child seems to know everyone in the neighborhood. ‘ . My child would never take advantage of others. My child can be left home alone without danger. My child jumps from one thing to another. My child has been in trouble for attacking others. . My child seems too serious minded. My child hasmore friends than most children. . When my child gets mad. watch out. My child really has no real friend. . My child is as happy as ever. My child often complains that others don‘t understand him (her). GO ON TO THE NEXT PAGE 5|. 52. 53. 55. 57. 58. 59. 6|. 62. 63. 65. 67. 69. 70. 7|. 72. 73. 74. 7'3. 144 . My child has very few friends. My child likes to play active games and spans. Sometimes I worry about my child‘s lack of concern for others‘ feelings. Often my child is afraid of little things. . My child tends to see how much he (she) can get away with. My child almost never argues. . My child often disobeys me. My child likes to show off. Others have said my child hasa lot of ”personality." My child goes to bed on time without complaining. . My child likes to “boss” others around. Reading has been a problem for my child. A scolding is enough to make my child behave. My child sometimes disobeys his (her) parents. . My child is in a special class in school (for slow learners). My child usually plays alone. . My child sometimes eats too many sweets. My child often brings friends home. . My child learned to count things by age six years. My child could print his (her) first name by age six years. My child doesn't seem to learn from mistakes. My child can‘t seem to wait for things like other children do. My child always does his (her) homework on time. My child is usually a leader in groups. Sometimes my child lies to avoid embarrassment or punishment. Other children make fun of my child’s different ideas. 76. 77. 78. 79. 82. 83. 84, ' 85. 87. 89. 9|. 92. 93. 95. 97. 98. I00. Sometimes my child‘s muscles twitch. My child worries about talking to others. My child first talked before he (she) was two years old. School teachers complain that my child can‘t sit still. . My child has some bad habits. Several times my child has spoken of a lump in his (her) throat. ‘ My child frequently has nightmares. My child almost never acts selfishly. My child is usually in good spirits. My child seems fearful of blood. . My child seems more clumsy than other children his (her) age. My child will do anything on a dare. . My child sometimes becomes envious of the Wnions or good fortune of others. Shyness is my child's biggest trouble. . Usually my child gets along well with others. My child gets lost easily. My child often has headaches. My child seems to get along with everyone. . My child is easily embarrassed. My child is very popular with other children. . My child gets confused easily. My child is almost always smiling. My child loses most friends because of his (or her) temper. . My child is shy with children his (her) own age. My child was difficult to toilet train. My child wants a lot of attention when sick. GO ON TO THE NEXT PAGE |02. |03. |08. I09. 110. m. ”2. ”3. m. us. “6. iii. “8. “9. I20. I2l. I22. l23. I24. l25. l27. 145 My child can count change when buying something. My child can tell the time fairly well. . Many times my child has become violent. . My child can take a bath by him (her) self. . Recently my child has complained of chest pains. . There is seldom a need to correct or criticize my child. My child has as much pep and energy as most children. Recently the school has sent home notes about my child's bad behavior. Sometimes my child will put off doing a chore. My child often talks about death. My child has been difficult to manage. Sometimes my child‘s room is messy. My child is usually afraid to meet new people. My child almost never needs punishing or scolding. My child could eat with a fork before age four years. Often my child complains of blurring (blurred vision). My child needs protection from everyday dangers. My child respects the property of others. Frequently my child will put his (her) hands over his (her) ears. Everything has to be perfect or my child isn’t satisfied. Spanking doesn‘t seem to affect my child. My child talks a lot about his (her) size or weight. My child often will cry for no apparent reason. My child will worry a lot before starting something new. . My child usually looks at the bright side of things. My child often has crying spells. l28. l29. |3|. Sometimes my child gets hot all over without reason. My child seems tired most of the time. . Others have remarked how smart my child is. My child takes illness harder than most children. 60 ON TO THE NEXT PAGE (unless instructed to stop at the end of Part |) 146 PARTII l32. My child tends to pity him (her) self. I58. Most of my child‘s friends are younger than he (she) is. I33. Others always listen when my child speaks. I59. There is a lot of swearing at our house. l34. Several times my child had complaints. but the doctor could find nothing wrong. I60. My child never takes the lead in things. I35. I often wonder if my child is lonely. I6I. My child takes criticism easily. I36. Usually my child takes things in stride. I62. My child sometimes swears at me. I37. My child is likely to take remarks the wrong way. I63. My child is not worried about disease. l38. Little things upset my child. I64. My child seems bored with school. I39. My child keeps thoughts to him (her) self. I65. The child's parents are now separated or divorced. I40. It has been a long time since our family has gone I66. My child gets exhausted so easily. out together. I67. I can‘t get my child to do his (her) school lessons. |4I. My child has never mentioned his (her) heart racing or pounding. I68. My child stays close to me when we go out. I42. My child has usually been a quiet child. I69. Often my child goes about wringing his(her) hands. I43. At times my child has seriously hurt others. I70. The child‘s parents have broken up their marriage ‘ several times. I44. My child has never had cramps in the legs. ”I. Sometimes my child runs errands for me. I45. At times my child yells out for no reason. - I72. It is not too unlikely that my child will stay in the I46. My child is liable to scream if disturbed. house for days at a time. I47. My child has no special talents. I73. My child has had brief periods of time when he (she) seems unaware of everything that is going on. I48. Our family seems to enjoy each other more than - most families. I74. My child has never had face twitchings. I49. My child broods some. I75. My child usually runs rather than walks. I50. My child could do better in school if he (she) tried. I76. My child is different from most children. I5l. M h'ld liked to be cuddled. ’ c ' m" 117. My child is afraid of dying. I52. ' has bee table hak . 0‘" "mm“ " my m (‘ y) us. My child believes in: God. l53. T h'ld' f t ' lo of the h'ld. he c ' . ‘ h" seems j“ us c . I79. My child doesn‘t seem to care for fun. I.I fid h'ld'htbe " . 5‘ am ‘ ra my c ‘ m“ '0." insane I80. Often my child will sleep most of the day on a I55. My child seldom talks about sickness. holiday. I56. My child has had convulsions. I8I. My child often stays in his (her) room for hours. l57. My child often gets up at night. I82. My child has never had any paralysis. GO ON TO THE NEXT PAGE I83. I84.. I85. I86. I87. I88. I89. l9l. I92. I93. I94. I95. I96. I97. I98. I99. 20l. 202. 203. 205. 207. 147 My child seldom breaks rules. How to raise the child has never been a problem at our house. Several times my child has threatened to kill him (her) self. My child usually doesn‘t trust others. My child has many friends of the opposite sex. My child seems unhappy about our home life. Others often remark how moody my child is. . The trouble with my child is a “chip on the shoulder.“ Nothing seems to scare my child. My child doesn‘t wem to be interested in practical things. My child can‘t seem to keep attention on anything. The child‘s parents are not active in community affairs. My child tends to swallow food without chewing it. My child loves to stay overnight at a f riend‘s house. School has been easy for my child. My child can‘t sit still in school because of nervousness. I do not approve of most of my child‘s friends. . Constipation has never been a problem for my child. My child is often restless. Several times my child has been in trouble for stealing. My child seldom complains of stomachaches. . My child has never failed a grade in school. My child is afraid of strangers. . The child's parents can't seem to live within their income. My child loves to work with numbers. . My child has never been in trouble with the police. 2|0. 2| I. 2|2. 2|3. 2|4. 2|5. 2|6. 2|7. 2|8. 2|9. 22I. 222. 223. 224. 225. 226. 227. 228. 230. 23I. 232. 233. . My child seldom visits a doctor. My child's favorite stories are fairy tales or nursery rhymes. The child's father doesn‘t,understand the child. Dizzy spells are no problem with my child. The child's father drinks too much. My child tends to brag. ’ My child would rather be with adults than with children his (her) own age. My child tends to be pretty stubborn. My child seldom talks. Our whole family seldom gets to eat together. Reading is my child's favorite pastime. . The child‘s father usually makes the important decisions at our house. “Bad days” are frequent with my child. My child insists on keeping the light on while sleeping. My child seems to prefer adults to children. My child is dependent on others. My child gets common colds more often than most children. The child's parents disagree a lot about rearing the child. Often my child locks himself (herself) in the bedroom. Often my child will laugh for no apparent reason. . My child sometimes skips school. My child is not as strong as most children. Others have remarked how self-confident my child is in a group. ‘ Others often remark how sensible my child is. My child seems to understand everything that is said. 00 ON TO THE NEXT PAGE 234. 235. 236. 237. 238. 239. 240. 24 I. 242. 243. 244. 245. 246. 247. 248. 249. 25I. 252. 253. 255. 256. 257. 258. 259. 260. 148 Sometimes the child's father will go away for days after an argument. Money seems to be my child's biggest interest. I have often found my child playing in the toilet. The child‘s father sometimes gets drunk and mean. My child is a healthy child. My child thinks others are plotting against him (or her). Usually my child plays inside. The child's father seldom misses work. Often my child takes walks alone. The child‘s parents have set firm rules that must be obeyed. Often my child will wander about aimlessly. Several times my child has threatened to run away. At times my child has difficulty breathing. There is always a lot of argument at our dinner table. - My child plays with friends who are often in trouble. My child seldom has nose bleeds. . My child has never been expelled from school. My child whines a lot. My child has never run away from home. My child shows unusual talent. . Speaking up is no problem for my child. I had an especially difficult time with temper tantrums in my child at an early age. Sharing things has been no problem for my child. The child‘s parents always discuss important matters before making a decision. My child smokes at home. The child's father frequently“blows up"at the child. My child is shy with adults. 26I . 262. 263. 264. 265. 266. 267. 268. 270. 2‘” . 272. 273. 274. 275. 276. 277. 278. 279. 280. go? Miami! I have heard that my child drinks alcohol. My child is rather absent-minded. My child is afraid of the dark. My child boasts about being sent to the principal in school. My child never has fainting spells. The child‘s father is too strict with the child. My child will never clean his (or her) room. My child is able to keep out of everyday danprs. . Most of my child‘s time is taken up watching television. 'Frequently my child has a high fever. The child's father is hardly ever home. Sometimes I don't understand what my child means. My child is exceptionally neat and clean. My child speaks of him (her) self as stupid or dumb. There is a lot of tension in our home. Several times my child has threatened to kill others. The child‘s father spends very little time with the child. My child seldom has back pains. The child's father has very little patience with the child. The child‘s parents frequently quarrel. GO ON TO THE NEXT PAGE unless Instructed to stop at the end of Part II) 149. Use of the mother as respondent has particular advantages and helps overcome many of the limitations associated with requesting the child to respond to numerous self-report descriptions. Referred children are often non-compliant and hence would not accept a technique requesting them to read and respond to a large number of questions. There are wide differences in verbal comprehension abilities in children that restrict the types and richness of items possible. In addition, the PIC validity scales signal respondent defensiveness or exaggeration. Rather than representing the mother's intentional distortion or the mother's personality, the PIC has established predictive accuracy (Lachar, Kline, s Boersma, in press). Informant Response Style Lie agele,1Ll. This rationally developed scale was constructed to identify a defensive response set in the informant. L reflects the absence or denial of behavior problems. L elevation increases when the respondent intentionally attempts to portray the child as having fewer problems than is actually the case. Eregneney seele 121. This scale consists of seldom ensorsed items. The F scale obtains extreme elevations for profiles generated by All True, All False, and Random Sort response sets. The F scale also reflects the severity of symptoms. Defensiceness Scale IDBEl. This empirically constructed scale is composed of items that separated mothers judged to be high-defensive from mothers judged to be low-defensive. It reflects fake-good response sets. General Adjustment Adjustment Scale iADll. This scale is a screening measure to identify children in need of a psychological evaluation and as a general measure of poor psychological adjustment. It was empirically constructed by comparing the items endorsement rates for 600 normal boys 7 to 12 years of age to the item endorsement rates for 200 maladjusted boys 7 to 12 years of age. The Cognitive Triad Aehieyement agele,LA§Hl. This empirically derived scale identifies children with limited academic abilities, poor achievement, and poor psychological adjustment characterized by impulsivity, limited concentration, over or under assertiveness with peers, and disregard for parental expectations. These children usually possess adequate intellectual capacity. Intellectual Screening Scale IISl. This scale was empirically constructed to identify children with impaired intellectual functioning. Items were identified by constrasting the protocols of retarded children with normal, nonretarded disturbed, and psychotic children. 150 (DyLl. This scale was rationally derived and reflects retarded development in motor coordination, poor school performance, and lack of any special skill or abilities. PIC scales were constructed using two methods. An empirical approach involved comparing a group of people without known psychopathology, i.e., 'normals,' with groups of other persons believed to have certain problems of living, i.e., "criterion'l groups. A second approach was the rational method. Here the researchers asked persons knowledgeable in the field to pick items from the total pool which they believed could be related to the disposition under question. Those items for which there was high agreement among the judges made up a scale which was tested using a criterion group to determine if the scale in fact discriminated among groups. Due to the limitations posed by the task of completion of a GOO-item inventory by a parent informant, a Revised Format Manual Supplement and Administratin Booklet were published (Lachar, 1982). The Revised Format Administration Booklet retains all 600 original inventory items, but presents these items in a different order. Completion of the first 280 items of the Revised Format Administration Booklet allows the scoring of the factor scales, validity and screening scales, and the clinical scales. The shortened profile scales have retained their reliability properties. Both coefficient alpha estimates of internal consistency and three test-retest estimates of temporal consistency suggest that the shortened versions have retained their reliability. In addition, the equivalence of shortened and full-length scales was evaluated (Lachar, 1982). The results again suggest strong concordance between shortened and full scales with a variety of populations. Lachar (1982) concluded that all available evaluations of the shortened profile scales suggest that they provide reliable and valid data regarding child functioning. APPENDIX D THE REVISED BEHAVIOR PROBLEM CHECKLIST Identification Number 53(3231F53IJIRCEIEIRXI IBEEIIJ§\IIECJI? (ZIIEZ(3!<1.3[£51P Please rate your child’s behavior after surgery on the following 89 items. If an item does not constitute a problem or if you have had no opportunity to observe or have no knowledge about the item, circle the zero. If an item constitutes a mild problem, circle the one: if an item constitutes a aevere problem, circle the two. Please complete every item and remember to rate your child’- behavior only following surgery. Thank you for your cooperation. 151 152 Identification Number I>C3£31rlit3£31?1[7r151.3E23111P3EC311 IBIEIIJK‘IJECJI? (ZIIEE(3!(I.JEES1T Please complete the following two queetiona. 1. How many nights did your child remain in the hospital? Circle One: 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10+ 2. How many times has your child been admitted overnight to a hospital, including this past hospitalization? Circle One: 1 2 3 4+ Please rate your child’s behavior since discharge from the hospital on the following 89 itemm. If an item does not constitute a problem or if you have had no Opportunity to observe or have no knowledge about the item, circle the zero. If an item constitutes a mild problem, circle the one: if an item constitutes a mevere problem, circle the two. Please complete every item and remember to rate your child’m behavior only mince discharge from the hospital. Thank you for your cooperation. d PP?N99‘§9.~:‘ .8;5.2.5.6awsssssswssszsswsss 0' d 153 Restless; unable to sit still ......................................... O 1 2 Seeks attention; "shows-off" ....................................... 0 1 2 Stays out late at night ............................................ O 1 2 Self-conscious; easily embarrassed ................................. O 1 2 Disruptive; annoys and bothers others ............................... 0 1 2 Feels inferior .................................................... O 1 2 Steals in company with others ..................................... O 1 2 Preoccupied; “in a world of his own;" stares into space . . . . . . . . . . : ..... O 1 2 Shy. bashful .................................................... O 1 2 Withdraws; prefers solitary activities ................................. O 1 2 . Belongs to a gang ............................................... 0 1 2 . Repetitive speech; says same thing over and over ..................... 0 1 2 . Short attention span; poor concentration ............................. 0 1 2 . Lacks self-confidence ............................................. O 1 2 . lnattentive to what others say ...................................... O 1 2 . Incoherent speech. what is said doesn't make sense ................... 0 1 2 . Fights ......................................................... 0 1 2 . Loyal to delinquent friends ........................................ O 1 2 . l-las temper tantrums ............................................. 0 1 2 . Truant from school. usually in company with others .................... 0 1 2 . Hypersensitive; feelings are easily hurt .............................. 0 1 2 Generally fearlul; anxious ......................................... 0 1 2 Irresponsible, undependable ....................................... O 1 2 Has "bad" companions. ones who are always in some kind of trouble . . .. O 1 2 Tense. unable to relax ............................................ 0 1 2 Disobedient; difficult to control ..................................... O 1 2 Depressed; always sad ....... « .................................... O 1 2 Uncooperative in group situations ................................... 0 1 2 Passive. suggestible: easily led by others ............................ O 1 2 Hyperactive; "always on the go" ................................... O 1 2 Distractible; easily diverted from the task at hand ...................... 0 1 2 Destructive in regard to own and/or other's property ................... 0 1 2 Negative; tends to do the opposite of what is requested ................ O 1 2 lmpertinent; talks back ............................................ 0 1 2 Sluggish. slow moving. lethargic .................................... O 1 2 Drowsy; not ”wide awake" ........................................ 0 1 2 Nervous. jittery. jumpy; easily startled ............................... 0 1 2 Irritable. hot-tempered; easily angered ............................... O 1 2 Expresses strange. far-fetched ideas ................................ 0 1 2 Argues; quarrels ................................................. O 1 2 Sulks and pouts ................................................. 0 1 2 Persists and nags; can't take “no" for an answer ..................... O 1 2 Avoids looking others in the eye .................................... 0 1 2 Answers without stopping to think .................................. O ‘l 2 Unable to work independently; needs constant help and attention ........ O 1 2 Uses drugs in company with others ................................. 0 1 2 Impulsive; starts before understanding what to do; doesn't stop and think . . 0 1 2 Chews on inedible things .......................................... O 1 2 Tries to dominate others; bullies. threatens ........................... 0 1 2 Picks at other children as a way of getting their attention; seems to want to relate but doesn't know how ..................................... 1 2 Steals from people outside the home ................................ 0 1 2 (please go on to next page) 151i Expresses beliefs that are clearly untrue (delusions) ................... Freely admits disrespect for moral values and laws .................... Brags and boasts ................................................ Slow and not accurate in doing things ............................... Shows little interest in things around him or her ....................... Does not finish things; gives up easily; lacks perseverance ............. is part of a group that rejects school activities such as team sports. clubs. projects to help others .......................................... Cheats ......................................................... Seeks company of older. “more experienced” companions ........... ,. . Knows what's going on but is listless and uninterested ................. Resists leaving mother's (or other caretaker's) side .................... Difficulty in making choices; can't make up mind ...................... Teases others ................................................... Absentminded; forgets simple things easily ........................... Acts like he or she were much younger; immature. “childish” ........... Has trouble following directions .................................... Will lie to protect his friends ....................................... Afraid to try new things for fear of failure ............................ Selfish; won’t share; always takes the biggest piece ................... Uses alcohol in company with others ................................ School work is messy. sloppy ...................................... Does not respond to praise fromadults .............................. Not liked by others; is a “loner" because of aggressive behavior ......... Does not use language to communicate ............................. Cannot stand to wait; wants everything right now ...................... Refuses to take directions. won’t do as told .......................... Blames others; denies own mistakes ................................ Admires and seeks to associate with “rougher" peers .................. Punishment doesn't affect his or her behavior ..... _. .................. Squirms. fidgets ................................................. Deliberately cruel to others ........................................ Feels he or she can't succeed ..................................... Tells imaginary things as though true; unable to tell real from imagined . . . Does not hug and kiss members of family; affectionless ................ Runs away; is truant from home .................................... Openly admires people who operate outside the law ................... Repeats what is said to him or her; "parrots" others' speech ............ 88388383 38$8338§3833§3333§?38838838838 CD SA AP aw PB Raw Score ......... Says nobody loves him or her ...................................... ' ‘d‘ddd‘ dddd"d‘d‘dddd‘ddd‘d‘dddddddddd ‘I'Score ........... MNMNNNNNNNNNNNNNNMMNNNNNNNNNNNN NNMMNNN 155 3329 Subscales . Disorder (CD). The CD subscale re resents a dimen31on of aggress1ve, noncomplaint, quarre some,' interpersonally aliented, acting-out behavior. It is also linked with noncompliance with a medial regimen, lower level of moral reasoning, and attribution of hostility to others. ' ' Aggressive Diserder (SA). The SA subscale also represents a dimension of acting—out, externalizing behavior. However, unbridled agressiveness and interpersonal alienation are not present. There are strong bonds with others, socialized delinquency, greater susceptibility to peer influence, without cognitive deficits. Attention arablems (AP). The subscale reflects problems in concentration, perseverance, impulsivity, and direction-following which leds to a deficient ability to come to grips with the demands of both home and school. ' - ' (AW). This subscale represents the internalizing dimension of disorder which subsumes such characteristics as anxiety, depression, fear of failure, social inferiority, and self-concern, reflecting subjective distress. ' Behaxigr (PB). The PB scale is comprised both of items that are clearly related to overt psychosis, and items related to language dysfunction. notorziension Excess (ME). ME involes both gross motor behaviors and apparent motoric tension. LI ST OF REFERENCES LIST OF REFERENCES Achenbach, T. (1974). Denelopmental.psychopatholcgy. New York:M The Ronald Press Co. Alloy, L. B., a Abramson, L. Y. (1979). Judgment of contingency in depressed and nondepressed students: Sadder but wiser? Jcarnai cf Rsxshclesrt General, lQfi. 441-485. Anthony, E.J. (1974). The syndrome of the psychologically invulnerable child. In E. J. Anthony 5 C. Koupernik (Eds. ), The child in his family; Children at ssxshietris risk. New York: Wiley. Appley, N. 8., & Trumbull, R. (1977). On the concept of psychological stress. In A. Monat & R. S. Lazarus, (363.), Stress and Casings An antholear- New York: Columbia University Press. Arnstein, H. s. (1960). Hhat to tell year child aheut births deathi illnessi discreet and ether famil! w. New York: Bobbs-Merrill. Barnes, C. M., Kenny, F. M., Call, T., s Reinhart, J. B. (1972). Measurement of anxiety in children for Open heart surgery. Pediatrics, 12, 250-259. Baumrind, D. (1969). Authoritarian vs. authoritative parental control. Adelescence, 1, 256-261. Bellotti, H. J. (1983). An investigation of two theoretical constructs of stress: The effects of stress appraisal and stress accumulation on psychological adjustment. (Doctoral dissertation, University of Conneticut, 1982). Dissercaricn Abstracts Internetienal .1. 601B. Billings, A.G., & Moos, R. H. (1982). Stressful life events and symtomes: A longitudinal model. Heairh MM”!‘ 1' 99'117. Blom, G. E. (1958). The reactions of hospitalized children to illness. Pediatrics, 22, 590- 599. Blom, G. E. (1984). Children who cope: Some implications for intervention and prevention. In Boger, R. P., Blom, G. B., a Lezotte, L. E. (Eds.), Child nurturing in the lfidflis. New York: Plenum Press. Blotcky, M. J., & Grossman, I. (1978). Psychological 156 157 implications of childhood genitourinary surgery. Jcnrnal cf the American Academy cf Borg, W. R., & Gall, N. D. (1979). Educaricnal Research. Third Edition. New York: Longman Inc. Bronheim, S. M. (1982). Familiarity, behavioral modeling and affect-arousal as related to children's coping with stress. (Doctoral dissertation, Catholic University of America, 1981). Dissertaricn Abstracts Internaticnal $3. 12478. Burstein, S., a Meichenbaum, D. (1979). The work of worrying in children undergoing surgery. acnrnal cf Abncrmal Child Eexehclcsxp 1, 121-132. Chamberlain, D. C. (1980). Sources of stress, psychological structure, and coping response. (Doctoral dissertation, Indiana University, 1979). Dissertaticn Abstracts Internaticnal in. 34598. Chandler, L. A. (1982). Children under stress: Understanding emcticnal adjustment reacticns. Springfield, 11: Chas. Thomas. Chandler, L. A., & Lundahl, W. T. (1983). Empirical classification of emotional adjustment reactions. American Jcnrnal cf Drthceexchiatrr. 51. 460-467. Coddington, R. D. (1972). The significance of life events as etiological factors in diseases of children. acurnal cf Psychcscmatic Research, 15. 7-18. Cohen, P. C., & Lazarus, R. S. (1973). Active coping processes, coping dispositions, and recovery from surgery- Psychescmatic Medicine, 35. 375-389- Cook, R. E. (1967). Effects of hospitalization upon the child. In J. A. Baller, (Ed.), The hespitalized child and his family. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins Press. Curry, S. L., & Russ. S. (1985). Identifying coping strategies in children. acarnal cf Clinical Child Pexehclegri ii. 61- 69. Davenport, H. T., s Werry, J. S. (1970). The effect of general anesthesia, surgery, and hospitalization upon the behavior of children. American acnrnal Di QLLhQBEXthELIXr $9: 806- -811. Dohrenwend, a. s., a Dohrenwend, a. p. (1980). What is a 158 stressful life event? In H. Selye (Ed.), guide ta stress research. New York: Van Nostrand Reinhold. Endler, N. S., a Magnusson, D. (1976). Toward an interactional psychology of personality. Perchclcsical Bulletin. £1. 956- 974. Fleming, R. (1980). Toward an integrative approach to the study of stress. acurnal cf Perscnality and Sccial Psychclcgu if, 939— 949. Fluger, I. (1981). Two cognitive coping strategies and arousal reduction in field dependent- independent males. (Doctoral dissertation, University of Kansas. 1980). Dissertaticn Abstracts Internaticnal.12. 1007A. Garmezy, N. (1976). Vulnerable and invulnerable children. Theory, research, and intervention. Unpublished manuscript #337, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis. Garmezy, N. (1981). Children under stress: Perspectives on antecedants and correlates of vulnerability and resistance to psychopathology. In I. A. Rabin, J. Aronoff, A. M. Barclay, & R. A. Zucker (Eds. ), further erchraticns in eerscnalitu. New York: Wiley. Garmezy, N. (1983). Stressors of Childhood. In N. Garmezy & M. Rutter (Eds. ), Stress; caning; and derelepment in children. New York: McGraw Hill. Ginsburg, 3., & Opper, S. (1979). Piaget.s thecry cf intellectualm Micement Englewood Cliffs. NJ: Prentice-Hall. Goggin,.E. L., Lansky, S. B., & Hassanein, K. (1976). Psychological reactions of children with malignancies. qurnal cf the American Academr cf Child Psychiatry. 15. 314-325. Glass, G. V., 5 Stanley, J. C. (1970). Statistical methcds in education and psychclcgy. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. Goldstein, M. J. (1973). Individual differences in fessonse to stress. American qurnal cf Ccmmunity Psychclegxr l. 113- 137 Haan, N. (1977). Coping and defendinsi Precesses cf self— encircnment crganizaticn. New York: Academic Press. 159 Hefferin, E. A. (1982). Life-cycle stressor. An overview of research. In D. C. Sutterley & G. P. Donnelly (Eds. l. Coping uith stressi A nursing DeraseeLiEe- Aspen Systems Corp. - Hersh, J. A. (1978). A concurrent validation of the Roberts Apperception Test as a measure of the therapeutic progress of children. (Doctoral dissertation, California School of Professional Psychology. 1978). Dissertation Abstracts International. 12. 4032-8. Holmes, T. H., s Rahe, R. H. (1967). The social readjustment rating scale. Journal Di Earshosomatic Research. 11. 213-218. Humphrey, J. H., & Humphrey, J. N. (1981). Reducing stress in children through creatiue relaxation. New York: Charles Thomas. Hyson, M. C. (1983). Going to the doctor: A developmental study of stress and coping. acarnal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 21. 247- 259. Ilfeld, F. W. (1980). Coping styles of Chicago adults: Description. Journal of Human Stress. l. 2-10. Isaac, S., a Michael, W. B. (1971). Handhcck in research and exalnaticn. New York: Robert R. Knapp. Jemmott, J.B., a Locke, S. E. (1984). Psychosocial factors, immunologic mediation, and human susceptibility to infectious diseases: How much do we know? Parcholosical Bulletin. 25. 78-108. Johnson, J. H. (1982). Life events as stressors in childhood and adolescence. In B. Lahey a A. Razdin gadgi;.2gguancee in clinical child Darcholosxr Y911 Kagan, J. (1983). Stress and coping in early development. In N. Garmezy & M. Rutter (Eds.), Stressi cabinet and deuelopment in children. New York: McGraw Hill. Kalb, R. C. (1977). The effects of the cognitive control of leveling-sharpening on the response of children to stress. (Doctoral dissertation, Fordham UniversitY. 1976). Dissertation Abstracts Iflifilflfliignfllr 33: 1884'188580 Kania, M. E. (1979). Identification of resource variables serving to moderate life events and concomitant life satisfaction of elementary age 160 children. (Doctoral dissertation, The University of Utah. 1978). Dissertation Abstracts International. to, 23703. Kashani, J. H., Venzke, R. & Millar (1981). Depression in children admitted to hospital for orthopedic procedures. British Journal of Psychiatry. lid. 21- 25. Kliman. G. (1968). Psychological emergencies of childhood. New York: Grune s Stratton. Knight, R. B., Atkins, A., Eagle, C. T., Evans, N., Finkelstein, J. W., Fukushima, D., Katz, D., & Weiner, H. (1979). Psychological stress, ego defenses, and cortisol production in children hospitalized for elective surgery. Psychosomatic Medicine. il. 40-49. Lachar, n. (1982). Personality lnyentory for Children 12191 reyised format manual supplement. L08 Angeles: Western Psychological Services. (W-152GS). Lachar, D., & Gdowski, C. L. (1979). Actuarial Assessment of child and adolescent personalityr An interpretiye guide for the Personality lnyentory for Children. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services. Lachar, D., Kline, R. H., s Boersma, D. C. (in press). The Personality Inventory for Children: Approaches to actuarial interpretation in clinic and school settings. In H. M. Knoff (Ed.), The Psychological assessment of child and personality. New York: Guilford Press. Lambert, S. A. (1984). Variables tht afffect the school- age child's reaction to hospitalization and surgery: A review of the literature. maternalzChild Nursing Journal. 13. 1-18. Laux, L., s Vossel, G. (1982). Paradigms in stress research: Laboratory versus field and traits versus processes. In L. Goldberger and S. Breznitz (Eds.), aandhook of stress. New York: The Free Press. Lazarus. R. s. (1966). Psychological stress and the coping process. New York: McGraw Hill. Lazarus, R. S. (1977). Cognitive and coping processes in emotion. In A. Monat s R. S. Lazarus (Eds.), Stress and cooingr An anthology. New York: Columbia University Press. 161 Lazarus, R. 8., Cohen, J. B., Folkman, S., Kanner, A., & Schaefer, C. (1980). Psychological stress and adaptation: Some unresolved issues. In H. Selye (Ed. ). Guide to stress research. New York: Van Nostrand Reinhold Co. Lazarus, R. S., DeLongis, A., Folkman, S., & Gruen, R. (1985). Stress and adaptational outcomes: The problem of confounded measures. American EEXQthQQifiL. 40. 770-779. Lazarus, R. s., a Folkman, S. (1984). Stressr appraisalr and coping. New York: Springer Publishing Co. Lazarus, R. 8., & Launier, R. (1978). Stress related transactions between person and environment. In L. A. Pervin & M. Lewis (Eds. ), Perspectiyes in interactional psychology. New York: Plenum Press. Lefcourt, H. M., Martin, R. A., & Salch, W. E. (1984). Locus of control and social support: Interactive moderators of stress. Journal of Personality and Social WI ‘1! 378'389- Levine, M. D., Carey, W. B., Crocker, A. C., & Gross, R. T. (1983). Deyelopmental behayioral pediatrics. Philadelphia: W.B. Saunders Co. Maccoby, E. E. (1983). Social-emotional development and response to stressors. In L. Goldberger & S. Breznitz (Eds.), Sandbook of stress. New York: The Free Press. Magnusson, D. (1982). Situational determinants of stress: An interactional perspective. In L. Goldberger & S. Breznitz (Eds.), Handbook of stress. New York: The Free Press. Masten, A. S. (1983). Humor and creative thinking in stress-resistant children. (Doctoral dissertation, University of Minnesota, 1982). Dissertation Abstracts WI no 37373- McArthur, D. (1976). A comparison of the stimulus influence of three thematic projective thechnques with children. (Doctoral dissertation, California School of Professional PsycholOQY. 1975). Dissertation Abstracts International. 31. 46933. McArthur, D. S., & Roberts, G. E. (1982). Roberts Apperception Test for Children. Manual. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services. 162 McNamee. A. 5. (Ed.). (1982). Helping children cope. Association for Education International. Melamed, B. 6., Robbins, R. L., & Graves, S. (1982L Preparation for surgery and medical procedures. In D. C. Russo 8 J. W. Varni (Eds. ), Behayioral pediatricst Research and Practice. New York. Plenum Press. Milgram, N. A. (1982). War related stress in Israeli children and youth. In L. Goldberger a S. Breznitz (Eds.), The handbook of stress. New York: The Free Press. Monat, A. & Lazarus, R. S. (1977). Stress and copingr An anthology. New York: Columbia University Press. Moos, R. H. (1974). Psychological techniques in the assessment of adaptative behavior. In G. V. Coelho, D. A. Hamburg, & J. E. Adams (Eds.), Coping and adaptation. New York: Basic Books. Moos, R. H., s Billings, A. G. (1982). Conceptualizing and measuring coping resources and processes. In L. Goldberger & S. Breznitz (Eds.), Handbook of stress. New York: The Free Press. Moos, R. H., a Tsu, V. D. (1977). The crisis of physical illness: An overview. In R. H. Moos (Ed.), Coping Hith physical illness. New York: Plenum Press. Mueller, D., Edwards, D., & Yarvis, R. (1977). Stressful life events and psychiatric symptomatology: Change or undersirability? Journal of Health and SDCial Behayior. 1a. 307-317. Muha, T. W. (1977). A validation study of the Roberts Apperception Test as a measure of psychological dysfunction in families. (Doctoral dissertation, California School of professional Psychology, gagig. Dissertation Abstracts International. 3B. Nie, N. H., Hull, C. H., Jenkins, J. G., Steinbrenner, K.. & Bent. D. a. (1975). Statistical package for the social sciences. New York: McGraw-Hill. Otero, R. F. (1983). The handling of stressful psychosocial life-events as a function of a hierarchical configural pattern of competence. (Doctoral dissertation, University of Maryland). Diggertation Abstracts International. 11. 4157- B. 163 Pearlin, L. T., & Schooler, C. (1978). The structure of gaging. Journal of Health and Social BehaYior.12. Pellegrini, D. S. (1980). The social-cognitive qualities of stress- -resistant children. (Doctoral dissertation, University of Minnesota, 1979). Abstracts InternationaL 41. 1925- 19263. Perkins, D. V. (1982). The assessment of stress using life events scales. In L. Goldberger & S. Breznitz (Eds.), The handbook of stress. New York: The Free Press. Peterson, L., Mori, L., 5 Carter, P. (1985). The role of the family in children' 5 responses to stressful medical Procedures. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology. 1.4. 98- 104. Prugh, D. G., & Eckhardt, L. O. (1980). Children's reactions to illness, hospitalization, and surgery. In H. I. Kaplan, A. M. Freedman, & A. J. Sadock (863.). Comprehensiye textbook of psychiatry. III. New York: Williams & Williams, Co. Prugh, D. G., a Jordan, K. (1975). Physical illness or injury: The hospital as a source of emotional disturbances in children and family. New York: Brunner/Mazel. Quay, H. C., & Peterson, D. R. (1983). Interim Manual for the Peyised Behayior Problem Checklist. University of Miami: Coral Gables. Quay, H. C., & Werry, J. S. (1979L Psych disorders of childhood. New York: John Wiley 8 Sons. Raney, R. H., Brashear, H. R., s Shands, A. R. (1971). Shandis handbook of orthopedic surgery. St. Louie: C.V. Mosby. Ray, C., & Fitzgibbon, G. (1981). Stress arousal and coping with surgery. Psychological Aedicine. 11. 741-746. Rie, H. E., Broverman, H., Grossman, B. J., & Olga, N. (1968). Immediate and long—term effects of interventions early in prolonged hospitalization. Pediatrics. 11. 755-764. Roid, G. H. (1983, March). Dimensions of child personality and psychopathologyi Factor structure 164 of the Roberts Apperception Test for Children. Paper presented at the meeting of the National Association of School Psychologists, Detroit, MI. Rutter, M. (1979). Protective factors in children's responses to stress and disadvantage. In M. W. Rent, a J. E. Rolf (Eds.). Primary oreyention of Parchobatholodxi Social competence in children iyoli 31. Hanover: University of New England. Rutter, M. (1983). Stress, coping, and development: Some issues and some questions. In N. Garmezy & M. Rutter (£03.). Stressl cosinor and derelopment in children. New York: McGraw Hill. Sarason, I. G., & Sarason, B. R. (1981). The importance of cognition and moderator variables in stress. In 0- Magnusson (Ed. ). Touard a psycholoox of situationsr An interactional perspectiye. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Scott, D. W., Oberst, M. T., & Dropkin, M. J. (1982). A stress and coping model. In D. C. Suterley & G. P. Donnelly (£66.). Cosine uith stressl A nursing perspectiye. Rockville, MD: Aspen Systems Seal, S. R. (1973). Psychophysiological reactivity of coping and cognitive styles in children. (Doctoral dissertation, McGill University, 1972). Dissertation Abstracts International. 3A. 8843. Selye. H. (86.). (1976). Guide to stress research. New York: Van Nostrand Reinhold Co. Shapiro, F. I. (1973). Cognitive controls and adaptation in children. (Doctoral dissertation, Boston College. 1972). Dissertation International. 13. 5026-50773. Shure, M. B. (1981). Social competence as a problem- solving skill. In J. Wine 8 M. Syme (Eds.), Social competence. New York: Guilford Press. Skipper, J. R., & Leonard, R. C. (1968). Children, stress, and hospitalization: A field experiment. Journal of Health and Social Behayior. 2. 275-287. Spielberger, C. D., Auerbach, S. M., Wadsworth, A. P., Dunn, T. M., & Taulbee, B. S. (1973). Emotional reactions to surgery. Journal of Consulting and Clinical EEXQh91992. 59. 33-38. Spivack, G., Platt, J. J., & Shure, M. B. (1976). The Problemzsolyino approach to adjustmeuta A guide to 165 research and interyention. San Francisco: Jossey- Bass. Sturmer, R. A., & Rothbaum, F. (1980). The effects of. stress on children' 3 human figure drawings. Journal of Clinical Psychology. as. 324-331. Sutterly, D. C. a Donnelly, G. F. (Eds. ). (1982). Coping pith stressr A nursing perspectiye. Rockville. MD: Aspen Sytems Corporation. Taylor, S. E. (1983). Adjustment to threatening events: A theory of cognitive adaptation. American EEYChplDQiSt. 33. 1161-1173. Thompson, R. J. (1981). Enhancing coping with the stress of chronic illness: A heuristic review. Documentsl MS 2225. American Psychological Association. Turkington, C. (1984, April). Israeli researcher finds hope eases stress, affects outcome. ABA MonitDL. P. 18. Vaillant, G. E. (1977). Adaptation to lire. Boston: Little, Brown. Vernon, D. T. A., & Bigelow, D. A. (1974). Effect of information about a potentially stressful situation on responses to stress impact. Journal or Personality and Social Psychology. 22. 50-59. Vernon, D. T. A., Foley, J. M., Sipowicz, R. R., & Schulman. J. L. (1965). The psychological responses of children to hospitalization and illness. Springfield, IL: Chas. C. Thomas. Vernon, D. T., & Schulman, J. L. (1964). Hospitalization as a source of psychological benefit to children. Pediatrics. 11. 694- 696. Vernon, D. T. A., Schulman, J. L., 8 Foley, J. M. (1966). Changes in children's behavior after hospitalization: Some dimensions of response and their correlates. American Journal of Diseases of Children. 111, 581-593. White, R .W. (1976L Strategies of adaptation. In R. H. Moos (Ed. ), Human adaptation. Lexington, MA: D. C. Heath. Wilcox, B. L. (1979). Life event recency and social support as mediators of the relationship between stressful life events and psychological adjustment. 166 (Doctoral dissertation, University of Texas at Austin, 1978). Dissertation International. so. 1390-13913. Willis, D. J., Elliot, C. H., & Jay, S. M. (1982). Psychological effects of physical illness and its concomitants. In J. M. Tuma (Ed.), Handbook for the practice of pediatric psychology. New York: Wiley- Wirt, R. D., & Lachar, D. (1981). The Personality for Children. In P. McReynolds (Ed.), Advances in psychological assessment. Vol. 5. San Francisco: Jossey—Bass. Wirt, R. D., Lachar, D., Klinedinst, J. R., a Seat, P, D. (19772. Multidimensional description of child personality: A manual for the Personality Inyentory for Children. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services. Wolff, S. (1973). Children under stress. Harmondsworth: Penguin Books, Ltd. Wright, L., Schaefer, A., & Solomons, G. (1979). Encyclopedia of pediatric psychology. University Park Press. Wrubel, J., Benner, P., & Lazarus, R. S. (1981). Social competence from the perspective of stress and coping. In J. Wine 5 M. Syme (Eds.), Social competence. New York: Guilford Press. Youssef, M. M. (1981). Self-control behaviors of school- aged children who are hospitalized for cardiac diagnostic procedures. MaternalzChild Nursing Jflfllnfllr 19: 219-271- Zachary, R. A. (1983, March). A comparison of yellzadjusted and clinic childrenls thematic responses to the Roberts Apperception Test for Children. Paper presented at the meeting of the National Association of School Psychologists, Detroit, MI Zeiss, A. M. (1980). Aversiveness versus change in the assessment of life stress. Journal of Psychosomatic Research. 24. 15-19. Zeltzer, L.. Kellerman, J., Ellenger, L., & Rigler, D. (1980). Psychologic effects of illness in adolescence. II. Impact of illness in adolescents- crucial issues and coping styles. The Journal of Pediatrics. 21. 132- 138. AAAAA \W "THifixfiflxfifljfififiuMilifliflifiwflfigi