3 :. .V.,.,,....H,.,_,.,...,.z, +3..) L T NA ON IN A FAMILY CONT ré‘e' h D: SA SAND TRAN of? ,2. 'the be I, 0". CAT " is. '1‘ WW ‘ Th M 1 ' ‘DYADIC SYSTEM 0 C _ . a m . _. V“ .rtl.vh| . 91:11.. u ,,. .‘ 4 .... . This is to certify that the thesis entitled Dyadic Systems and Transactional Communication in a Family Context presented by L. Edna Rogers has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for Ph. D. Jegree in Communication {3;L ‘ 7 // /{/‘ .1 C@,.y{ Z. ‘(L/ggz...ce,/ Major professor Date October 16, 1972 0-7639 ‘ -..—._____._._———- ‘ amomc av ‘: ”BAG 8 SONS' . n" " wru- ‘. ' {M' 3‘1, 7! WC , 7r 3' ~ >7 ‘r :f: S smncponr 1 ' " “I \ 51.75151.) 1 ‘. .——-—~_ ABSTRACT DYADIC SYSTEMS AND TRANSACTIONAL COMMUNICATION IN A FAMILY CONTEXT BY L. Edna Rogers This research investigated communication behaviors of dyadic systems. The study, designed within a general systems theoretical frame, emphasizes the relational and process aspects of interpersonal communication. The research problem involves a theoretical description of system characteristics of dyads, the indexing of system states concerning role relationships and the measurement of system patterns and processes of communication. The major purpose of the study was to compare the interper— sonal behaviors and transactional communication patterns of dyadic systems exhibiting different levels of perceived role discrepancy strain. The data for this study were obtained by personal interviews with a random sample of 65 husband—wife dyads. Two types of information were gathered: (1) self-report questionnaire data from each husband and wife separately, L. Edna Rogers and (2) discussion interaction data from each husband and wife pair as they discussed four different topics. An important aspect of this study was the develop- ment and application of a transaction level interaction coding system (in conjunction with Ericson, 1972, and based on the earlier work of Sluzki and Beavin, 1965, and Mark, 1970) which was utilized in a relational analysis of the dyadic discussions. Relational communication refers to the transactional characteristics of message exchange by which system members reciprocally control and define the ongoing nature of their relationship. The major transactional patterns investigated were symmetry, complementarity and transitory. The principle dyadic variable under study was role discrepancy, a measure of the degree of inequity strain perceived to be present in the dyadic System. Length of existence, or history, of the system was utilized as a controlling variable. A comparison of dyads differentiated by role discrepancy level, showed significant differences in dyadic communication behaviors. Compared to high role discrepand dyads, lower role discrepancy dyads spent more time together, talked with one another more, talked about more topics, particularly more personal topics, were more satisfied with their communication relationship and with their marriage. L. Edna Rogers The relational analysis of the dyads' topic dis- cussion interaction showed that high discrepant dyads as compared to low discrepant dyads, expressed fewer support messages, had more wife one—up control movements, fewer husband one-down transitory transactions, fewer unsuccess- ful talk-overs, and more symmetrical transactions. It was concluded from this research that the general systems approach provided sound theoretical guide for relating system states and communication be- haviors, and that the relational communication analysis procedures gave not only useful message-level descrip- tions of interaction, but has particular value for operationalizing and describing transactional—level communication patterns. DYADIC SYSTEMS AND TRANSACTIONAL COMMUNICATION IN A FAMILY CONTEXT BY L. Edna Rogers A THESIS Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Communication 1972 COpyright by Lilian Edna Rogers 1972 ii Accepted by the faculty of the Department of Communication, College of Communication Arts, Michigan State University, in partial fulfillment of the require- ments for the Doctor of PhilOSOphy degree. flaw y K W/ Director ofTT eSlS flcée/ 1/ 64m,» Chairman Guidance Committee: , My flaw \ 4\ iii To my parents iv ACKNOWLEDGMENTS I wish to acknowledge my appreciation to Dr. Richard V. Farace for serving as my committee chairman and to Dr. Erwin P. Bettinghaus, Dr. Hideya Kumata and Dr. Beatrice Paolucci for being members of my guidance committee. Gratitude is also expressed to Dr. David K. Berlo. The support, time and helpfulness of each has been greatly appreciated. I am particularly grateful to my friends and colleagues, Phil Ericson, Frank Millar and Ken Villard. Because of them the project was successful. I also wish to thank Lee Russell and Judy Osborne for their contribution to the study. A Special appreciation for understanding is given to my son, David Rogers. TABLE OF CONTENTS Chapter I. II. III. INTRODUCTION . . . . . . . . . . . . Overview and Importance of the Research PrOblem o c o o o o o O o o q o o o A General Statement of the Research Problem . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Dyadic Dimensions . . . . . . . . Dyadic Evaluation Dimensions . . Dyadic Communication . . . . . . smary I O O O O O O O O O O Q C C THE RESEARCH PROBLEM IN PERSPECTIVE: REVIEW OF LITERATURE . . . . . . . . Dyadic Dimensions . . . . . . . . . Uniqueness of the Dyad as a Small Need for a Classification System The Search for Group Properties . Review of Attempts to Define and Classify Dyads . . . . . . . . Basic Dyadic Dimensions . . . . Communication Variables . . . . . . Review of Selected Observational Systems Overview of the Observational Approach Family Communication Research . . . Family Studies . . . . . . . . . Length of Marriage . . . . . Sex Differences . . . . . . . Role Perceptions . . . . . . Family Interaction Studies . . . RESEARCH METHODOLOGY . . . . . . . Sampling Procedures . . . . . . . . vi 0 Page 10 12 14 16 18 18 18 21 22 27 31 34 34 48 49 49 49 52 53 57 64 64 Chapter Data Collection . . . . . . . . . . . . . Procedures . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Type Of Data 0 O O O O O O O O O O O 0 Description of the Sample . . . . . . . . Operationalization of Variables . . . . . Role Discrepancy . . . . . . . . . . . Relational Communication Patterns . . . Coding System . . . . . . . . . . . Code Categories . . . Control Dimensions . Transactional Codes . Analysis Levels . . . suma ry I O I O O O I O O O O O O I Coding Procedures and Reliability for the Discussion Data . . . . . . . . . IV. ANALYSIS PLAN AND RESEARCH HYPOTHESES . . SGlf-Report Data 0 o o o o o o o o o o o 0 Discussion Data . . . . . . . . . . . . . Exploratory Questions . . . . . . . . . V. RESEARCH FINDINGS O O O O O O O O O O O V O self-Report Data 0 O O O O O O O O O O O 0 Time Allocations . . . . . . . Topic Discussion Frequency . Communication Control . . . . Desired Talk-Time . . . . . . Communication Satisfaction . Marital Satisfaction . . . . Discussion Data . . . . . . . . . . . . . Message and Transactional Descriptions Message Types . . . . Message Control . . . . . . . . . . Transactional Types . . . . . . . . Summary of Message and Transactional Descriptions . . . . . . . . . . . vii Page 66 66 69 70 71 72 75 8O 82 87 91 93 101 102 107 108 112 115 116 116 117 135 141 143 147 153 157 158 158 164 165 167 Chapter Message and Transactional Hypotheses . . . Activity Level Hypotheses . . . . . . . Message Level Hypotheses . . . . . . . Transactional Level Hypotheses . . . . Summary of Message and Transactional Hypotheses . . . . . . . . . . . . . Exploratory Questions . . . . . . . . . . Summary of Exploratory Questions . . . Transactional Examples . . . . . . . . . . VI. SUMMARY AND IMPLICATIONS . . . . . . . . . Self—Report Description Summary . . . . . Time and Talk-Time Allocations . . . . Discussion Frequency and Communication Control . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Desired Talk—Time, Communication and Marital Satisfaction . . . . . . . . Relational Communication Analysis Summary Message Type and Control Direction . . Transactional Activity and Flexibility Transaction Types . . . . . . . . . . . Configuration Patterns . . . . . . . . Implications O O I O O O O I O Q O O O O 0 Major Conclusions . . . . . . . . . . . Research Suggestions . . . . . . . . . APPENDICES O I I O O O O O O O O O O O O O O r O I BIBLIOGRAPHY O I O O I O O O O O C O O O O O O 0 viii Page 168 170 175 180 195 195 211 214 220 220 220 222 223 224 224 225 226 228 228 228 232 236 281 LIST OF TABLES Table Page 1. Number of percent of dyads by interview category . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 66 2. Percentage agreement between coders on three digit message codes . . . . . . . . . . . 106 3. Mean score estimates of total time with spouse on a weekday . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 119 4. Mean score estimates of total time with spouse on a Sunday . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 121 5. Mean estimates of time spent with spouse on a weekday . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 124 6. Mean estimates of time spent with children alone on a weekday . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 127 7. Mean estimates of time spent outside the family on a weekday . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 130 8. Mean estimates of talk-time with spouse on a weekday O O O Q 0 O O O O O O O O O O O O O 1 32 9. Mean estimates of talk-time with spouse on a Sunday . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 133 10. Mean discussion frequency across ten topics . . 136 11. Significance levels of mean discussion fre- quency scores by topic . . . . . . . . . . . . . 139 12. Response frequencies and total frequency percentages for self-report communication contrOl by item I O O O I O O O O O O O O O O O 142 13. Mean scores of desired talk-time on a weekday . 144 14. Mean scores of desired talk-time on a Sunday . . 145 ix Table Page 15. Mean desired talk-time for a weekday and a Sunday by wife—husband . . . . . . . . . . . . . 147 16. Mean scores on general communication satisfaction measure . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 148 17. Mean scores on composite communication satisfaction scale . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 150 18. Mean marital satisfaction scores . . . . . . . . 155 19. Summary of major self—report dyadic findings . . 156 20. Frequencies and percentages of message types by wife, husband, and dyad and rank order of percentage usage above 1 percent . . . . . . . . 159 21. Frequencies and percentages of second digit message types by wife, husband, and dyad and rank order of dyad percentages . . . . . . . . . 162 22. Frequencies and percentages of third digit message types by wife, husband and dyad and rank order of dyad percentages . . . . . . . . . 163 23. Percentages of control direction by wife, husband and dyad . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 165 24. Percentages of transaction types . . . . . . . . 166 25. Percentages of the three general transaction types I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I 16 7 26. Mean discussion time . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 171 27. Mean silences per minute . . . . . . . . . . . . 173 28. Mean number of transactions . . . . . . . . . . 174 29. Mean proportions of support messages . . . . . . 176 30. Mean proportions of talk-over messages . . . . . 178 31. Mean proportions of successful talk-over messages I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I 179 32. Mean proportions of unsuccessful talk-over messages I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I 180 Table 33. 34. 35. 36. 37. 38. 39. 40. 41. 42. 43. 44. 45. 46. 47. 48. 49. 50. Mean difference scores from random usage of each transactional type . . . . . . . . Mean proportions of actions . . . . . . Mean proportions of transactions . . . Mean proportions of transactions . . . Mean proportions of (++) transactions . Mean proportions of transactions . . . Mean proportions of transactions . . . Mean proportions of transactions . . . Mean proportions of transactions . . . Summary of discussion data hypothesis Mean proportions of Mean proportions of Mean proportions of Mean (++) proportions of transactions . Mean (++) proportions of transactions . Mean proportions of Mean proportions transactions Mean propor configuration trans— H+ W+ complementary H4 W+ complementary competitive symmetry H+ W+ transitory H+ W+ transitory O I o O I I o o I H+ W+ transitory H+ W+ transitory one-up messages . . one-down messages one—across mess submissive xi testing Page 182 184 186 187 188 190 191 203 204 205 206 Table Page 51. Most frequently expressed transactional pattern by discrepancy level . . . . . . . . . 208 52. Frequencies and sub—total percentages of configuration patterns by discrepancy level I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I 210 53. Summary of exploratory question findings . . . 213 xii LIST OF FIGURES Figure Page 1. Message transactions . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 80 2. Message type and control direction . . . . . . . 88 3. Matrix of transactional types . . . . . . . . . 92 4. An example of ten transactional configurations . 97 5. Transactional configuration typology . . . . . . 98 6. Transactional patterns of high, medium, and low discrepancy dyads . . . . . . . . . . . . . 215 7. Transactional patterns of high discrepancy short and long history dyads and a low discrepant dyad . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 217 xiii LIST OF APPENDICES Appendix I. INTRODUCTORY LETTER . . . . II. SELF-REPORT QUESTIONNAIRE . III. DISCUSSION TOPICS . . . . . IV. INTERVIEWER INSTRUCTIONS . V. DISCUSSION TIME BY TOPIC . VI. SECOND AND THIRD DIGIT CODE EXAMPLES . . . . . . . . . VII. TRANSCRIBING PROCEDURES . . VIII. INSTRUCTIONS FOR CODERS . . xiv CATEGORY Page 236 237 254 256 265 266 269 272 CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION A developing theme in the area of family research is one that focuses on interaction processes. Since Burgess' (1926) innovative definition of the family as a "unit of in- teracting personalities" this theme has gained momentum. Concurrent with this movement toward interaction research is a growing interest in communication theory. In attempting to understand the "psychosocial interior" (Hess and Handel, 1959) of the family, researchers and clinicians have increas- ingly recognized the necessity of studying interpersonal communication behavior. 7 Inquiry into the nature of interpersonal relation- ships has required a theoretical approach emphasizing the interdependence of interactors and the notion of process. This approach is found in general systems theory (Hall and Fagan 1956,Bertalanffyhl962); it necessitates a shift of concern away from individual actions to transactions as they occur over time. The study of system characteristics, rather than individual behavior or component characteristics, in- volves a variety of new conceptual and methodological pro- blems. Research efforts dealing with ongoing interaction at a systems level have been delayed and detoured by these 1 difficulties. Nevertheless, strong beginnings are being made and this dissertation is aimed at furthering the study of interpersonal transactional communication. Overview and Importance of the Research Problem The key features of the general systems model are the emphasis on organization, the recurrent patterns of interrelated component actions, and the transmission of' information that underlies organization. When this model is applied to social systems, the exchange and processing of symbolic information becomes of central importance. By definition, system components influence one another and in human systems, this mutual interdependence is mediated through communicative acts. As system theorists point out, a distinguishing characteristic of human systems over mechanical or organic systems, is that they are based primarily on the flow of information rather than energy. Further, they are not only oriented toward structural maintenance, but toward structural evolution. Buckley (1967) refers to these processes respectively, as morphostasis and morphogenesis. Human systems may be described as adaptive, informa- tion processing systems. They are open systems, operating continually within a potentially influential and changing environment. Likewise, there are internal tensions, or pressures, that affect the goal-directed behavior of system members. The level of tension within the system varies over time, but some degree of tension is characteristic of human systems and is considered a motivator of action. Cybernetic self-governing principles operate to moni- tor and control system actions. The dynamics of self-regula- tion in human systems are based on a selective set of inter- nal criteria, i.e., rules of distributive justice (Homans, 1958), that function to maintain action limits for the sys- stems.'The level of tension or perceived inequity within a system is increased as component behaviors deviate from the criteria rules toward the tolerance limits. With rule stretching or breaking, the maintenance of a system's organ- ization or "working agreement" at that given point in time become problematic. McCall and Simmons (1966) state that a working agreement exists "when the cognitive processes of one person,are not in gross conflict with the expressive processes of the otherperson" (p. 142). The attainment and maintenance of such an agreement or consensus validation (Sullivan, 1953) is always somewhat problematic and rarely stable. The cybernetic notion of feedback implies that reciprocal definitions of relation— ships are continually being negotiated via control communica- tion, and as a system moves further from an equity "balance", error-correcting communication increases. Transactional feedback may result in maintenance of a given steady state or in the elaboration of a new transitory steady state. The multiple sources of potential strain on human systems, as well as the plasticity of human adaptive alternatives, func- tion to give a fluid, dynamic nature to the structural pat- terns of social systems. It is within this general systems orientation, with its emphasis on communication processes, that the present research problem was generated. This research deals with dyadic systems, i.e., two-person interaction systems. It is viewed as an application of cybernetic principles to the in- teraction of husband-wife pairs. The major purpose of the study is to compare the transactional communication patterns of dyadic systems that have lower levels of perceived in- equity, to dyadic systems of higher levels of perceived inequity. The communication conceptualizations basic to the present study are those that have been developed in apply- ing the systems approach to the family area. This is notably the accumulative efforts of Bateson (1936), Jackson (1959, 1965), Watzlawick, et_§l., (1967), Haley (1963), Sluzki and Beavin (1965) and others whose prime orientation has been one of viewing interaction as a transactional phenomenon. A transactional level of concern refers to the circular af- fect of interactors upon one another, such that person A's behavior affects person B's behavior and B's response af- fects A which in turn affects B. This does not negate the value of lower levels of interaction analysis, e.g., the ef- fect of one person's behavior on another, but it goes beyond it. A transactional focus requires more adequate models of communication processes than allowed by traditional mona- dic oriented theories; it presses for at least a dyadic level of analysis. In emphasizing the inadequacy of present termi— n0109Y and methodology for describing and explaining trans- actions within the family system, Jackson states: It is only when we attend to transactions between individuals as primary data that a qualitative shift in conceptual framework can be achieved. Yet our grasp of such data seems ephemeral; des- pite our best intentions, clear observations of interaction process fade into the old, individual vocabulary, there to be lost, indistinguishable and heuristically useless. To put the problem another way, we need measures which do not sum up individuals into a family unit; we need to measure the characteristics of the supra-individual family unit, characteristics for which we presently have almost no terminology (1965, p. 4). In developing a "transactional" language, a basic distinction was made by Ruesch and Bateson (1951) between the report and command aspects of messages. Later theorists have referred to this distinction as the content and the meta or relational level of communication. Every message has both a content (report) and a relationship (command) aspect; the former conveys information about facts, opinions, feelings, ex- periences, etc., and the latter defines the nature of the relationship between the communicants . . . In every communication, then, the participants offer to each other definitions of their relation- ship . . . . (Jackson, 1965, p. 8). Jackson conceptualizes the family as a rule-governed system, characterized by certain repetitive interaction se— quences. Based on Bateson's (1936) earlier work, two prin- ciple patterns of transactions have been defined on the basis of relational control. One pattern, symmetry, refers to the interchange of similar control messages, and the other, complementarity, refers to the interchange of maximal- ly dissimilar control messages. The terminology available for describing relational communication is increasing, but operational definitions of relational concepts, are particularly lacking. At present, only Sluzki and Beavin (1965) and Mark (1971) have dealt with the measurement of symmetrical and complementary trans- action patterns. These patterns will be investigated as part of the further development of a methodology for analy- zing relational communication. The conceptual and opera- tional aspects of this effort are discussed at length in Chapter III. The present study represents a flow of concerns that are necessarily involved in increasing the understanding of interpersonal dyadic communication. One of the first concerns is to present a theoreti- cally derived classification framework for categorizing dyads in a conceptually meaningful way. The value of a classification scheme becomes apparent as one reviews the over-abundant literature on small groups and the often non- accumulative nature of the research results. Using a typo- logical frame for classifying dyads along basic, generic dimensions, research findings can more readily be accumula- ted concerning the similarities and differences of communi- cation patterns in different kinds of dyads. The study will investigate the communication patterns of one example of one category of dyads. A notable feature of the dyad under study (husband-wife pairs) is that it is a system with a history. Long-standing, intimate dyadic re- lationships have less frequently been the subject of small group research, than ahistorical, non-future oriented, ad hog groups. An assumption of most ad Egg group research is that the same dynamics apply to historical groups; the pre- sent research avoids this doubtful generalization. The investigation of communication patterns will in- volve two levels of analysis. Descriptions of communication behavior at the monadic level are important and will be made, but this study has particular significance for the analysis of communication behavior at the relational level. As pre— viously mentioned, this involves the development of a trans- actional level coding scheme. This scheme focuses on mes- sage sequences rather than individual messages; on relation- al control rather than content of message exchange and on mapping transactional patterns over time. This coding system and its empirical application is an important contribution to the study of interpersonal communication. Interactional research concerned with the relational level of communication has largely been based on family sys- tems which contain a schizophrenic child. The present re- search is based on "normal" family systems.1 This study will provide insight as to whether relationships found in studies of "sick" families have relevance for normal families. The present study has importance in a number of ways, but particularly in its methodological contributions and re- lational level analysis of interpersonal interaction systems. It is seen as a contributing effort toward future higher level, process-oriented analyses of transactional communica- tion. A General Statement of the Research Problem The purpose of the present study is to investigate dyadic communication behavior in a family context. This study is part of a larger research project which is concern- ed with family communication patterns and Optimal strategies 1Another aspect of this study which has merit at least in the area of the sociological study of the family, is that it includes the husband's views and descriptions of family system behaviors. See Salfilios—Rothchild's article "Family Sociology or Wives' Family Sociology? A Cross- Cultural Examination of Decision-Making," 1969. for introducing information on emergency preparedness into the family system.2 The present research is designed to describe the relationship between selected dyadic system dimensions and dyadic communication patterns. In this endeavor a general systems approach, as applied to the family research area is taken as the main theoretical guide. A basic assumption of this approach is that dyads Operate on the basis of relation- al rules that define how the members are to interact with one another and that these rules, or the attempt to esta- blish these rules, will be reflected in the communication behaviors of the system's members. In line with the systems point of view, the dyad is seen as an open, rule-governed system of interdependent members linked together by communi— cative acts oriented toward an adaptive steady state.3 To further our understanding of the application of the systems approach to the study of communication behavior, the present research problem will involve a movement of con- cern from theoretically describing system characteristics of 21n this research the system under study was limited to the husband-wife relationship. This decision was based on (1) reducing the complexities of studying family commun- ication at this exploratory state and (2) the assumption that the husband-wife components form the most basic sub- system around which other family relationships are formed (Satir, 1967). 3Haberstroh (1965) defines a steady state in terms of a "consistent shared set of perceptions (among system ele- ments) of what the task is and how it should be accomplished (p. 1173)." 10 dyads to indexing system states concerning role relation- ships, to observing system patterns and processes of com- munication. Thus, the major tasks of the study are: (l) the identification of basic or "generic" characteristics of a dyadic communication system, (2) obtaining and com- paring member evaluations of the system, and (3) sampling and operationalizing dyadic communication. The final task will consist of describing and interrelating selected as- pects of these system variables. Dyadic Dimensions The focus of the research is on interpersonal com- munication within the dyad. There are, however, many kinds of dyads. The accumulation of research findings in this area has been hindered by the lack of a conceptual a framework within which to describe the behavioral variabil- ity of different dyadic systems. In previous research these differences have too often been ignored, with the assumption that all dyads behave in a similar way. What is needed is a classification scheme by which to delineate ciifferent types of dyads. A search of the theoretical literature concerning <211aracteristics of different types of social groupings and illle more empirical oriented of group "properties" resulted 'jlrl the formulation of five dimensions by which to describe . €11??ads. These dimensions were selected on the basis of the 1?‘:>3Llowing concerns: that they have reference to the 11 relationship, not to individual members alone; that they have a potential variation effect on communication patterns; that they are operational; that they have as low a degree of subjectivity as possible; and that they are general enough in level to be descriptive of most, if not all, dyads. The dimensions are as follows: 1. History: The length of existence of duration of the relationship. 2. Time allocation: The frequency of interaction be- tween the two participants and the amount of time expended in the dyad. 3. Continuance: The probability of recurring inter- action between the two participants. 4.’ Function: The extent to which the purpose of the interaction is based on production, maintenance, and innovation (Berlo, 1970). Interaction may focus on the task to be accomplished (the produCr tive function); on routine events and problematic concerns of the participants (the maintenance func- tion); or on non-routine concerns, the generating of new ideas, techniques and goals, the expansion of alternatives of the participants (the innova- tive function); or on any mixture of the three functions. 5. Regulation: The extent to which interaction is guided by role position rules or by personal qualities of the participants. 12 Within this framework, the present research will look at one example -— husband-wife pairs -- of one type of dyad. These dyads are described as having a history, varying time allocations, high probability of continuance, being multi- functional with an emphasis on maintenance and being role diffuse. The formulation and discussion of these dimensions are described in detail in Chapter II. Dyadic Evaluation Dimension Considering the dyad under study as a micro-social sys- tem, defined by basic relational rules, we now move "into" the system itself to measure the members' evaluation of the system in terms of discrepancy from each members' definition of distributive justice, i.e., equity. There is both theo- retical (Homans, 1958, Thibaut and Kelley, 1959) and empiri- cal (Adams, 1965, Blumstein and Weinstein, 1969) evidence to suggest that exceeding the limits of the rule of distributive justice is a basic rule violation of fair exchange which underlies the negotiation and maintenance of open relation- ships.4 The amount of inequity that is perceived by the members to exist in the dyadic relation is the major index of the "state of the system" to be investigated. The in- equity dimension is referred to as role discrepancy and is 4See "Social Exchange: Basic Issues" (1971), an un- published paper by the author for a discussion of this issue. l3 operationalized by the difference between the normative and behavioral aspects of the dyad. This involves a comparison of relationship expectations with perceived behavior per- formance.5 Expectation can have two meanings, one of prediction that does not involve evaluation and one of prescription that does. The prescriptive aspect (referring to ranges of preferred, or acceptable behaviors) is the concern here. These expectations serve as a criteria for evaluating the actual behavior of a dyad. The assumption is that dyadic relations of the type being investigated are oriented toward equity, fluctuating within a range of expectational tolerance limits. Inequity exists when the actual performance (outcome) is discrepant from the expectation criteria of the person making the so- cial comparisons. When system members perceive that the rule of distributive justice is not being met, they will activate control communication in an attempt to redress the inequity. It is hypothesized that the communication patterns 5To obtain data on role performance and expectations each system member was presented (in an interview situation) a list of selected family tasks, e.g., child care, house care, social-emotional, etc., and they were asked to respond as to who actually does the task, and who should do the task. A comparison of each members' description of task performance and his preferences or expectations about this performance provide the discrepancy scores. A complete discussion of these procedures is given in Chapter III. 14 of dyads with lower perceived inequity will be differentiated from the communication patterns of dyads with higher perceived inequity. Dyadic Communication Two different sets of communication measures were gathered: First, self-report data from the respondents and secondly, observational data of dyadic discussions. The self-report data secured by questionnaire6 provides the following descriptions of dyadic interaction: a. Talk-time and time allocations: The amount of time spent talking with spouse and the amount of time spent with spouse, with family, with others. b. Talk-time desired: An index of the time desired to talk with spouse on both a weekday and a Sun- day. This provides a relative deprivation or saturation measure of dyadic communication. c. Topic distribution: Frequency of dyadic commun- - ication on various topics. d. Control of communication: Measures of who talks the most, who initiates and ends conversations and who dominates the subjects discussed. 6Background information about the dyads, as well as the previously mentioned evaluation measures, were also gathered by questionnaire. See Chapter III. 15 e. Communication satisfaction: Measures of general dyadic communication satisfaction and a composite measure of satisfaction on a number of communica- tion items concerning the dyad.7' The discussion data were obtained by tape recording conversations of husband and wife dyads as they discussed four tOpics, two dealing with predicting family behavior under threat of emergency and two with family oriented con- cerns. These topics discussions provide the data base for the relational communication analysis. Relational communication refers to the transactional characteristics of message exchange by which dyadic members reciprocally control and define the ongoing nature of their relationship. The major relational patterns investigated are complementarity and symmetry as mentioned before, and transitory, a control neutralizing category developed in the operationalization of a procedure for indexing relation- al patterns. The present research involves both the develop- ment of a relational coding scheme and its application. See Chapter III for a more complete discussion of relational communication concepts and the present analysis system. 7Satisfaction with the dyadic relationship was also measured and will be related to dyadic role discrepancy. 16 Summary The major variables of the overall research design are given below: Dyadic Dimensions Dyadic Communication A. Descriptive dimensions8 I. Self-report data Measured dimensions a. Talk-time 1. History b. Talk-time desired 2. Time allocations c. Topic distribution Typological assiments d. Control of communi- cation 3. Continuance e. Communication sat- 4. Function isfaction 5. Regulation B. Evaluation dimensions II. Discussion data 1. Role discrepancy a. Relational patterns Within the five-dimension descriptive framework for classifying different types of dyads, the communication pat- terns of husband-wife pairs, representing one example of one type of dyad, will be investigated. The major dyadic variable under study is the role discrepancy evaluation measure. The 8Two of the theoretically derived dyadic dimensions were measured. The history of the dyad, i.e., length of marriage, and estimates of the time spent in the dyad were obtained from the dyad members. The other three dimensions were assumed to be generally descriptive of the dyads studied, e.g., that they have high probability of continuance, are multi-functional and role diffuse. 17 history, or length of the marriage dyad, however, will also be investigated. These two dyadic variables will be related to both the self-report and the discussion communication data. The central aims of this study are: (l) to describe dyadic communication patterns of husband-wife pairs; (2) to determine what and to what degree differences exist be- tween the communication patterns of husband-wife dyads who have different levels of discrepancy in evaluations of role behavior and expectations; and (3) to further explore pos- sible dyad-communication relationships, particularly at the relational level. This study has importance not only in increasing the descriptive knowledge of the dyadic system, but more importantly, in increasing the understanding of processes and patterns of communication which develOp over time in long-term, intimate relationships. CHAPTER II THE RESEARCH PROBLEM IN PERSPECTIVE: A REVIEW OF LITERATURE The concern of this study is the dyad and the commun- ication patterns of this two-person interaction system. In placing this problem in perspective, three major areas of study will be reviewed. First, the literature dealing with the categorization of dyads along theoretical dimensions will be reviewed. The concern will be the delineation of basic dimensions by which behavioral variability of dyadic system can be described. The second area of concern is the litera- ture dealing with the description of interpersonal communica- tion activity. A review of the observational approaches used in conceptualizing ongoing communication will be pre- sented. Third, a review will be given of the research on family communication that has relevance for the present study. Dyadic Dimensions Uniqueness of the Dyad as a Small Group A dyad, the smallest of the small groups, has certain distinctive characteristics. Simmel (1950) was one of the first social scientists to be particularly sensitive to the inherent frailty of the two-person interaction system. "A 18 19 dyad depends . . . on each of its two elements . . . for its life, it needs both, but for its death, only one" (p.124). The negation of a dyad comes not so much from the physical departure, but from the removal of relational reciprocity. In larger groups, the effect of one member upon the group's existence and continuance is considerably reduced. Simmel points out that the . . . . . . dependence of the dyad upon its two individual members causes the thought of its existence to be accompanied by the thought of its termination much more closely and im- pressively than in any other group . . . This fact is bound to influence the inner attitude of the individual toward the dyad . . . it makes the dyad into a group that feels itself both endangered and irreplaceable . . . (pp. 123-124). The uniqueness, the potential intimacy, and particular— ly the directness of reciprocity are distinct features of a dyad. Neither of the two members can hide what he has done behind the group, nor hold the group responsible for what he has failed to do . . . The significance of this characteristic, how- ever, is by no means only negative. On the contrary, it also makes for a close and highly specific coloration of the dyadic relationship (Simmel, 1950, pp.134-135). The research literature, based mainly on laboratory discussion groups of varying size, indicates that groups of two show more signs of tension, have higher rates of asking for opinions, of giving information and agreement while a- voiding disagreement than larger groups (Bales and Borgatta, 1955; Borgatta and Guerin, 1960; Thomas and Fink, 1963). 20 Other distinctions of a dyadic system are the limited dynamics of power. In a dyad, maneuvers are of a less com- plex nature than in a triad or larger social system. In a two person group each participant relates directly to the other; sub-systems and coalitions are not possible. There is no involved audience or mediator present.1 Simmel ob- serves, "The essential point is that within a dyad, there can be no majority which could outvote the individual"2 (p. 137). The addition of one member to a dyad allows the addition of several group dimensions.3 Becker (1932) sug- gests that the addition of a third person is more significant in its consequences for interaction in the group than the ad- dition of any 4th, 5th, or 6th person. Bossard's (1945) for- mula for the number of possible relations between members of various sized groups, NZ-N/Z, gives a sharply accelerating curve of potential relationships. Group size is found to vary with increased differentiation of communication parti- cipation, decreased satisfaction and a decreased feeling of unity. The importance of the dyad seems evident. What is needed, however, if we are to compare dyads, is a system for classifying them. 1One must always be aware, however, of the openness and connectedness of a dyadic system to the potential effects of the larger systems in which it operates. 2For sure, there may be power differences in a dyad but numerically there is no majority. 3See Simmel (1950) pp. 135-169 and Weick (1969) pp.23-25. 21 Need for a Classification Scheme If one were to enumerate dyads on the basis of noun- pairs, e.g., nurse-doctor, mother-son, son-father, client- counselor, teacher-student, friend-friend, pilot-passenger, seller-buyer, etc., the list would be extensive. A compre- hensive classification of dyads in this fashion, at the paired individual or positional level, would also be end- less. There needs to be a more abstract system of classifi- cation that is built upon a relatively small number of basic dimensions on which large numbers of dyads can be placed. At the theoretical level there have been a variety of efforts to categorize dyads but usually only on one dimen- sion; e.g., voluntary - nonvoluntary, formal-informal, primary-secondary, fleeting-repetitive, ritualistic-idiosyn- cratic, instrumental-expressive, etc. At the research level, distinctions concerning the kinds of dyads being investigated are too often ignored. The implied but unstated assumption is that "a dyad is a dyad, and what is true for one will be true of another."4 -4This assumption underlies research in groups larger than the dyad as well. Argyle's comment concerning the "artificiality" of much small group research is appropriate here. "The implicit strategy of research has been to assume that all kinds of small groups follow the same empirical laws, and that these can best be studied under controlled labora- tory conditions, where confusing environmental variables are absent. The result is that there is now an extensive litera- ture about small social groups of students engaged mainly in problem-solving or allied tasks, and we really do not know very much about real groups in the outside world" (1969, p.216). 22 Small group hypothesis, such as the one that increas- ed interaction increases liking, would benefit from taking into account the kind of dyad for which the prediction is being made. The process might be quite different for non- voluntary versus voluntary interactors. Thibaut and Kelley (1959) point out that many times individuals stay in dyadic relationships in which reward outcomes are below their com- parative level because of their dependency on the relation- ship. This is antagonistic to the general unqualified social exchange proposition that interaction ceases when outcomes are not profitable ones. When the dependency of a relation- ship is altered by a modification of the alternatives, so is the dyadic behavior. The need for a unifying descriptive frame is strongly suggested as one reviews the massive literature dealing with small groups and the confusion that results from it? Re- search findings are nonadditive if there is no accounting for the type of small group being studied. A conceptual framework for classification of dyads is not only necessary for an inventory of research but for the further develop- ment and accumulation of research propositions. The Search for Group Properties A classic problem in the social sciences has been the search for determining the basis (or bases) of unity of 5 See Golembiewski (1962), especially Chapter II. 23 group members. From the field of sociology has come a series of polar typologies which have been developed to describe dif- ferent dimensions of social organization. Prominent examples are T3nnies (1957) distinction between Gemeinschaft and Gesellschaft, Durkheim's (1947) contrast between mechanical and organic solidarity, Redfield's (1941) folk and urban con- structs, and Becker's (1932) sacred and secular distinctions. These ideal types are based on the kind of bonds that bring and hold men together in social groups and associations. One basis of unity (Gemeinschaft, folk,mechanica1, sacred) is conceptualized as resting on the intergrative "forces" of mutual sentiment between group or society mem— bers, while the other (Gesellschaft, urban, organic, secu- lar) rests on the contractual obligations that exist between people. The former is characterized by normative homeo- geneity and psychological dependency; the latter by differen- tiation and functional dependency. With few exceptions, these early efforts were in- terested in characterizing total societies, not small groups or dyads. The predominance of the macroscopic study of social organization continued until the World War II re- search era and the discovering of the influence of the "small group." The emergency of small group research rested on the premise that we could explain behavior in ways that were not possible by utilizing individual psychologi- cal concepts on the one hand, nor the global society level 24 concepts at the other extreme. Since that time there has been a flood of small group studies, but the concern over the dimensions problem has been small. In reviewing this situation, Golembiewski states: Whatever the reason, the state of research may be typified fairly in this way: most students have been content to handle a few variables, to make some assumptions concerning the homogeneity of the population considered along other dimen- sions, and to determine statistically significant relations between the few variables. The implied conclusions hardly needs detailing: the general approach is less demanding than the search for group dimensions; unfortunately, the general ap— proach is also less promising (1962, p. 73). Homans' work, beginning with The Human Group (1950), is one attempt to integrate a number of empirical studies on the basis of four basic variables -- activity, inter- action, sentiments and norms. Emphasis is placed on the quantity (frequency) of activity and interaction, and the value (sentiment based on normative evaluation) of the activity. Parsons' (1951) patterned variable analysis is another prominent attempt to classify social systems along several dimensions. These dimensions or modes of social integra- tion are based on the choices any actor mustmake between incompatible alternative-pairs when acting in relation to any other actor. These alternatives or "dilemmas of action" are: Affectivity vs. affective neutrality, diffuseness vs. specificity, particularism vs. universalism, ascription 25 (quality) vs. achievement (performance), self-orientation vs. collectivity orientation.6 Other efforts to outline dimensions by which groups may be classified, which are more specific in nature, are offered by several students of small groups.7 Hemphill (1949, 1956) and Cattell (1948, 1951) have developed lists of variables by which to describe groups characteristics and have used factor analysis to reduce this larger number to the major factors. Borgatta and Cottrell (1955) have used a similar procedure and in an article with Meyers (1956) have discussed some of the problems involved in this procedure. One particularly perplexing problem noted by Golembiewski is that: a factor-analytical approach to the dimensions of the small group requires the existence of a set of dimensions which can determine whether or not a batch of collectivities is homogeneous ' as-a species.‘ And this set of dimensions, of course, is precisely what does not exist (1962, p. 81). Hemphill's scheme consists of 14 group dimensions by which a group may be described (on a five point scale for each dimension). These are as follows: 6These are the original five variables which were later reduced to four. 7Some classification efforts which by title seem ap- propriate to the problem at hand, but by content are not, were not included for review. For example, DeGre (1949) in an article entitled "Outline for a Systematic Classification of Social Groups" proposes a classification of roups on the bases of three major considerations: (1) terri oriality or nonterritoriality bases, (2) degree of organization and (3) degree of complexity of organization. Ei ht sub-categories result by which he classifies nations, vi lages, neighborhoods, families, friends, structured organizations, collectivities, and crowds. 26 l. Autonomy, the degree to which a group functions independently of other groups. 2. Control, the degree to which a group regulates the behavior of individuals while they are functioning as group members. 3. Flexibility, the degree to which a group's activities are marked by informal procedures rather than by ad- herence to established procedures. 4. Hedonic tone, the degree to which group membership is accompanied by a general feeling of pleasantness or unpleasantness. 5. Homogeneity, the degree to which group members are similar with respect to socially relevant charac- teristics. 6. Intimacy, the degree to which members of a group are mutually acquainted and are familiar with the personal details of one another's lives. 7. Participation, the degree to which members of a group apply time and effort to 'group' activities. 8. Permeability, the degree to which a group permits ready access to membership. 9. Polarization, the degree to which a group is oriented and works toward a single goal. 10. Potency, the degree to which a group has primary significance for its members. 11. Stability, the degree to which a group persists over a period of time with essentially the same characteristics. 12. Stratification, the degree to which a group orders its members into status hierarchies. 13. Viscidity, the degree to which group members func- tion as a unit. 14. Size. These efforts to categorize groups are laudable; how- ever, they have had limited impact on the development of a 27 typology of groups. Clarification of group dimensions has suffered from the lack of consistency in the use of terms by different investigators. Extensive lists of descriptive characteristics fail to identify the key properties of groups while one-dimensional classifications (dichotomies)combine and conceal independent distinctions. A comprehensive, yet useful set of group properties that have theoretical linkage, are operational and limited in number, is still to be for- mulated. Review of Attempts to Define and Classify Dyads Other than the many one-dimensional classifications of dyads, there have been few attempts to delineate dyads in a generic way. One exception is the scheme formulated by Becker and Useem (1943). However, their definition of a dyad is limited to the following: Two persons may be classified as a dyad when intimate, face-to-face relations between them have persisted over a length of time sufficient for the establishment of a discernible pattern of interacting personalities (p.13). Thus, from a larger set of two-person interactions they have selected only one subset with which to deal. In line with their definition of a dyad they set up a classification scheme based on the extent to which the personality is involved in the dyad. Their classification permits two major breakdowns: (1) comprehensive dyads -- "those pairs in which a relatively large portion of the 28 personalities of both are included in the relationship." (p. 21) and (2) segmentalized dyads -- "those groups of two which involve relatively less of 'segments' of the personali- ties of both." Under the first category are three subsets: (1) friendship pairs, (2) sexual pairs, (3) generation pairs. Under the latter category are five subsets: (l) aider-aided pairs; (2) teacher-pupil pairs; (3) ordinated pairs [an supersociate-subsociate, (b) equisociates, (c) superordinate- subordinate -- the distinction between a and g rests on a subsociate having some power over a supersociate while a sub- ordinate has little or no power over a superordinate]; (4) common interest pairs; and (5) patterned-contact pairs. On the basis of Simmel's work, Becker and Useem dis- cuss many important characteristics which they feel must be present to meet their definition of a dyad. Their discussion of these characteristics seems more descriptively insightful than the devised classification scheme. Marwell and Hage (1969) offer an interesting analysis of 100 dyadic role relationships on the basis of a matrix of 16 general variables. These variables are generated from a combination of four "fundamental elements" of all relation- ships -- occupants (who), activities (what), location (where), occurrences (when) -- with four "basic characteristics" of all relationships -- the sc0pe, intensity, integration and independence of the relationship. Raters scored each role- relationship on each of these variables. A factor analysis 29 of the ratings reflected three major dimensions: (1) in- timacy (expressive vs. instrumental), (2) visibility (pri- vate vs. public nature of role relationship), and (3) re- gulation (high vs. low behavior specification of relation- ship). A dichotomized cross-classification of these three dimensions resulted in eight types of relationships. The similarity of these dimensions to those described by Tonnies, prompted Marwell and Hage to discuss their eight-fold classi- fication as sub-categories of the Gemeinschaft and Gesell- schaft typology. In an insightful essay, McCall (1970)8 describes five types of social bonds: (1) ascription: a linking of two people based on social position, not on individual characteristics; (2) commitment: the extent to which a person "has privately and publically committed himself, or been committed, to honoring a restrictive covenant, a trade agreement, with another party" (p. 7) -- this refers to an actor's self involvement in giving energy and loyalty to the fulfillment of a promise; (3) attachment: the "building of specific others into the very contents of (self) role-iden- tities" (p.8); (4) investment: the consideration of self and alter's amount of scarce resources (time, energy, money, 8This builds heavily on the previous work of McCall and Simmons (1966) concerning the nature of interpersonal ties (PP. 170-175) 0 30 etc.) expanded in establishing and maintaining a relation- ship; (5) reward dependability: the degree of dependability of others as a source of social rewards. McCall feels these are the most important elements for promoting continued relationships between two people. These bounds are often found in combination, yet McCall also feels "they are distinct factors; they are present in dif- ferent proportions in different relationships, and they often vary independently of one another" (p. 9). In a paper on deviance, Denzin (1970) discussed the nature of social relationships9 and suggests three very basic analytical dimensions: (1) duration, (2) location, and (3) degree of mutual involvement. Relationships that are time-specific, place-specific and involvement-specific (e.g., buyer-seller, civil authorities, employer-employee, acquaintances) are quite distinct from diffuse relation- ships of long duration and many levels of involvement (e.g., friends, lovers, relatives). While Denzin feels these are clearly important analy- tical dimensions, he states that the distinction must be more refined. This task yet remains. Denzin comments that, 9A relationship, as defined by Denzin, exists between two or more people when those people engage in recurrent forms of either symbolic or co-present interaction. 31 The range and types of social relationships that characterize human interaction have remained largely unconceptualized by the sociologist. So- ciologists have yet to describe adequately the ef- fects of such relationships and to sufficiently come to grips with how these ranges of experience are created, stabilized, and dismissed by persons themselves (p. 67). Basic Dyadic Dimensions In seeking to analyze the range of dyadic relation- ships, it was suggested that it is useful to think in terms of a number of analytical dimensions upon which any relation- ship can be located for descriptive and comparative purposes. Producing the argument for the necessity of being concerned with general properties of groups may be easier than genera- ting the dimensions themselves. The present offering of dimensions is not an exhaustive list. The dimensions are suggestive, not definitive. The previous review of group properties and the commonalities that exist among them have been heavily taken into consideration. The guiding question, however, is: Given a dyad, what are the general kinds of descriptive statements that can be made about the relationship -- not the individuals, but the relationship?10 A relationship exists between two people 10As we have noted, relatively few studies have been truly based on a dyadic conceptualization, that is, the linking of the components of a system rather than based on the components themselves. Cha p e,(l940),and Homans (1961), for example, have given us gui e Wlth their concern With fre- quency and duration of interaction. These are variables that age measurable for all relationships, and only for relation- s ips. 32 engaging in recurrent forms of symbolic interaction. Thus, the focus is on the occurrence and organization of reciprocal behaviors. How long has the relationship existed? How fre- quent and for how long does it get "put into action?" How likely is it to occur again? For what reasons do the parti- cipants "come together?" What are the constraints that guide, limit, and pattern otherwise random activity? Dimensions of interest are those that (1) refer to the relationship, not to individual members alone, (2) have re- levance for probable explanation of communication variance, (3) are operational, (4) have as low a degree of subjectivity as possible, and (5) are general enough in level to describe most dyadic occurrences by use of relatively few dimensions. With this in mind, as well as the review of the theoretical identifications of characteristics of groups of different types and the more empirical oriented listings of group "properties," the following dimensions were formulated by which to distinguish most, if not all, dyadsJJ- Size in this case is, of course, a given. 1. History: The length of existence or duration of the relationship, varying from newly formed to long-time established dyads. lLThe discussion of groups and group properties has been predominantly concerned with voluntary groups rather than in- voluntary ones. While the totally compliant relationship can be described b the dimensions given, these dimensions were formulated wit the more .freguent occurring type of dyads in mind, the ones with varying egrees of voluntariness. 33 Time allocation: The frequency of interaction be- tween the two participants and the amount of time expended in the dyad. This may vary from infre- quent to regularly recurring interactions with little time involved to high time involvement. Continuance: The probability of recurring inter- action between the two participants. This rests on the mutual involvement or investment in the preservation of the dyad. It may vary from high to low obligation and/or commitment, and thus reflects the substitutibility of participants. Some relationships may be highly transferable while others are much less so. This dimension forces us more into the domain of the actors' feelings than the others. Thus, there is a hesitancy to include it; yet it touches such a fundamental aspect of social intergration, it is difficult not to include it. Function: Dyadic interaction may be based on variations of productive, maintenance and innova- tive functions. The interaction may focus on the task to be accomplished (the productive function), or on the preservation of the interaction of the participants (the maintenance function) or on the generation of new ideas, techniques and goals, the expansion of alternatives of the participants 34 (the innovative function), or on any mixture of the three functions. 5. Regulation: Dyadic interaction varies to the extent that it is guided by role position rules or by personal qualities of the participants. All interaction operates within some minimal de- gree of normative or consensual framework; how- ever, it may be based on variations of knowledge of the role relations or on knowledge of persons. This distinction has been similarly referred to as formality-informality, ascription-attachment. Communication Variables Review of Selected Observational Systems An understanding of man and his relationships can be abstracted from the character of talk. Verbal behavior is a rich source of data, but analytically difficult to handle. The analysis of talking behavior seems to necessitate "out- side" observation. The objective awareness and descriptive ability of one's own behavior or that of others when involved in a conversation is difficult, if not impossible. Kenkel (1963), for example, found that perceptions of a variable as simple as the amount of time that respondents and their mari- tal partner talked during a discussion period were quite different from the actual times. 35 A variety of observational methods have been devised since the beginning work in the 1930's for describing the communication processes of group interaction.12 These methods vary from simple two category systems to highly complex, multi- categorical ones, from minimally inferential to highly inferen- tial descriptions, from interpretation of individual intention to group function of messages, from broad to specific commun— ication situation application. The observational systems that are reviewed here were selected to be representative of a larger set of methods that have been developed over the past 30 years, that have appli- cation to many interaction situations, are not specific in nature, are not necessarily intended for a therapeutic popu- lation, and that focus on verbal behavior. Thus, eliminated from consideration were systems that apply to particular types of groups, e.g., teacher-students, (Flanders, 1960; Amidon and Flanders, 1963); that are highly specific in analy- sis, e.g., tabulating nouns, pronouns, etc., (Goldman-Eisler, 1954); that deal with nonverbal behavior (Rosenfeld, 1966); and that are mainly used in therapeutic setting with a heavy emphasis on inferring the psychological function of the message and/or the psychological state of the actor (Leary, 1957; Adler and Enelow, 1965). 12Carr (1929) and Thomas, et al., (1933) pioneered in this area. 36 The subset of methods to be reviewed was purposely chosen across time to sample the kinds of recurring commun- ication variables that have been described, defined, and measured by various observational techniques. The classification system of Chapple (1940) treats only the time element of interaction. His scheme deals with the observation of action (linguistic) and inaction (silence) between two interactors. Chapple describes the number and length of units of actions of each individual. This fairly uncomplicated procedure gives a variety of structural mea- sures of interaction -- the length of the total interaction, the length and ratio of each participant's action, the fre- quency and ratio of each participant's action, etc. Chapple (1949) has conceptualized the characteristics of tempo, acti- vity, initiation, etc., on the basis of the time element of interaction. 7 The Scheme has the advantage of being relatively sim— ple and highly reliable. It produces more indicates of in- teraction than one might think, based only on a time dimen- sions; however, the shortcoming, of course, is the limited amount of information that it gives about the total commun-' ication process. Taking into account interaction content, Steinzor (1949) developed a fairly complicated classification system to analyze the intent of verbal behavior in face-to—face groups. His scheme includes 18 categories describing the 37 communication intent of the actor, with three sub-classifica- tions according to the direction of the intent; (1) toward self, (2) toward group, and (3) toward issue. The 18 cate- gories are: 15. 16. 17. 18. .Activate and originate, Structure and delimit, Diagnose by labelling, Evaluate, Analyze and explore, Express and give information, Seek information, Clarify confusion, Defend, Offer solution, Conciliate, Understand and reflect, Give support, Seek support, oppose and attack, show deference, Conform, Entertain. A nineteenth category labeled miscellaneous included all un- classifiable statements. Steinzor's basic assumption was that behavior is mo- tivated, that an understanding of motivation is necessary to an understanding of behavior, and that a significant aspect of motivation is the intent or purpose of the individual's verbal behavior. It was assumed that the greatest understanding of group relationships would be gained if inferences as to the individual's intent or purpose most nearly approximated the way the individual would define his purpose (pp. 104-105). The complexity and nonordered pot pourii aspects of these categories cause serious problems in coding. He also 38 lacks a unifying notion of what communication variables are being measured. Disagreement with his basic assumption can be defended, and the methodological approach is highly in- ferential with low potential for reliability. Steinzor does not specifically take into account the sequence of sequen- tial effects of the verbal behavior being analyzed. Carter, et_§l. (1951), in their studies of leadership, devised a 53 category system by which to classify verbal acts according to their function in the group process as seen by an outsider. There are seven main category groupings with from three to fifteen subcategories. The main category headings are: . Shows personal feeling, Proposes and initiates action, Disagrees and argues, Leader role in carrying out action, Follower and worker role in carrying out action, Abortive and nonproductive behavior,. . Miscellaneous. \lO‘U‘IubWNl-J I Except for the first section dealing with personal feelings, the categories require minimal coding inference. This array of categories allows a large amount of specific information to be taken into account, but this also produces a very com- plex and cumbersome system. Bales (1950) produced a category scheme concerned with interaction content, but one with higher classification ob- jectivity than Steinzor's and more conceptual structure than Carter's. Perhaps the longevity of Bale's scheme, Interac- tion Process Analysis, is indicative of this. 39 The emphasis of Bale's observational scheme is more on the group and less on the individual. The conceptual framework is based on four main problems confronted by a social system: Adaptation to outside influences, instru- mental control over task, expression of feeling and mainten- ance of integration. Adaptation and instrumental control are considered to be basically task oriented and are handled primarily by expression of question and answers, that is, the giving and the asking for information (orientation), opinion (evaluation) and suggestions (control). Expression of feel- ings and maintenance of integration are considered to be basically socio-emotional oriented and are dealt with pri- marily by the expression of positive and negative reactions. The observation list for coding behavior is as follows: . Shows solidarity, Shows tension release, Agrees, Gives suggestion, Gives opinion, Gives orientation, . Asks for orientation, . Asks for opinion, . Asks for suggestion, 10. Disagrees, 11. Show tension, 12. Shows antagonism. \OQQO‘UIQWNH I The major communication variables imposed upon these categories are: Orientation (6,7), Evaluation (5,8), Con- trol (4,9), Positive reaction (1,2,3), Negative reaction (10,11,12). The main measures of message frequency and flow available from the coding scheme are: Distribution 40 of acts by category and total, who-to-whom matrix by category and total, and ratio of instrumental acts to socio-emotion- a1 acts. An important measure of communication process that is possible by using the Bales' system is a phase analysis of the distribution of acts over time. This type of analy- sis, utilized by Bales and others, has significant implica- tions for the study of the process aspects of communication. Borgatta's (1961) revision of the IPA was designed to sharpen the discrimination of several of Bale's categories by further specification. For example, category four, "gives suggestion" becomes (4a) "gives procedural suggestion" and (4b) "gives solution suggestions." Borgatta's subdivisions and reorganization were made within the "parent" framework, thus maintaining the continuity and comparability of commun- ication variables and research results. The revised categories referred to as the Interaction Process Scores, are listed below with the numbers in paren- theses indicating the connection to the original IPA cate- gories. 1. Common social acknowledgments (1a) 2. Shows solidarity through raising the status of others (1b) 3. Shows tension release, laughs (2) 4. Acknowledges, understands, recognizes (3a) 5. Shows agreement, concurrence, compliance (3b) 6. Gives a procedural suggestion (4a) 7. Suggests solution (4b) 8. Gives opinion, evaluation, analysis, expresses feeling or wish (5a) 9. Self-analysis and self-questioning behavior (5b) 10. Reference to the external situation as redirected aggression (5c) 41 11. Gives orientation, information, passes communication (6a) 12. Draws attention, repeats, clarifies (6b) 13. Asks for opinion, evaluation, analysis, expression of feeling (8) 14. Disagrees, maintains a contrary position (10) 15. Shows tension, asks for help by virtue of personal inadequacy (11a) 16. Shows tension increase (11b) 17. Shows antagonism, hostility, is demanding (12a) 18. Ego defensiveness (12b) 13 categorizes Borke's (1967) classification system each communication according to the probable intent of the person initiating it. It is embedded in Horney's (1945) broad theoretical scheme of classifying interpersonal be- havior as "going toward," "going against," or "going away from" others. These three interpersonal styles of relating to others are broken into primary and secondary mode cate- gories.14 Both the initiator and the recipient of the inter- personal message is recorded in each exchange. The cate- gories are given below: I I. Goes toward 1. Contributes a. Offers information b. Seeks information c. Entertains d. Miscellaneous 13This scheme has been revised by Borke (1969) for use with video tape. 14Borke (1967) notes that in analyzing data on family interaction that the secondary mode inferences were too sparsely distributed for overall systematic comparisons (p. 19). 42 2. Supports a. Actively promotes cause b. Shows concern 3. Petitions a. Seeks support b. Seeks attention c. Seeks direct gratification 4. Directs a. Organizes b. Behaves strategically c. Instructs 5. Accepts from others a. Accepts support b. Accepts other's point of View II. Goes against other 1. Resists a. Ignores b. Opposes 2. Attacks a. Behaves provocatively b. Attacks directly III. Goes way from other 1. Retreats a. Evades b. Withdraws physically Borke's system provides both profile data and sequen- tial data on communication acts. From this, a number of com- parisons are possible by total unit and by each participant. The most general variable derivable is an overall "going toward, against or away" style of relating to other members. 43 At the primary and secondary mode level, the proportion of communication in each category can be determined, the pro- portion of communications initiated and received by each member is available by total or by category; and the inter- action sequence can be traced by speaker and/or category. Soskin and John's (1963) analysis of talk behavior included four kinds of approaches to the data: (1) ecologi- cal, a description of the behavior setting, (2) structural, a description of the amount, frequency and duration of talk- ing behavior, (3) functional, a description of the major classes of verbal acts in terms of their function, and (4) dynamic, a description of the emotional state of the speaker.15 Of particular concern to our interest here are the structural and functional categories for characterizing verbal behavior. The three main variables under the structural analy- sis are: (l) the subjects' absolute and relative amount of talking, (2) the subjects' number and prOportion of utter- ances exchanged, and (3) the distribution of subjects' speech duration. The basis of the functional analysis rests on a dis- tinction made by Soskin and John between informational and 15Soskin and John note that, from the coder's point of view, the structural analysis is practically inference free, the functional analysis involves more inference, and the dy- namic analysis is quite inferential. 44 relational functions of talk. The first type consists of objective statements about one's self and one's world. "In- formational messages are those which develop or report what are thought to be facts; they identify, classify, analyze, organize, etc., and are primarily information transfer state- ments" (p. 253). Relational talk includes statements "by which a speaker manages his interpersonal relations“ (p. 253) by direct specification of preference or by indirectly pro- viding information about his present state which can be taken account of and adjusted to by the listener. Six types of statements are given below which have been distinguished by Soskin and John. I. Informational messages 1. Structiones: Objective informational statements II. Relational message 2. Signones: Subjective messages of physical or psychological state of speaker 3. Metrones: Evaluate, interpretative statements 4. Regones: Regulative statements, both giving restrictions and opportunities III. Quasi-relational messages 5. Expressive: Utterances to discharge im- mediately experienced tension 6. Excogitative: "Thinking out loud" statements Using these categories the usual method of comparison can be made of (l) speaker's distribution of messages by cate- gory, (2) shifts of distribution of messages at different time points, (3) sequential distribution of messages. 45 Mark's (1970) interpretation and extension of Sluzki and Beavin's (1965) transactional coding system is the final interaction analysis to be reviewed. It is based on the theoretical orientation of Bateson (1936), and co-workers at the Mental Research Institute at Palo Alto. It is an attempt to emphasize the transactional aspect of the commun- ication process and operationalize the symmetrical and com- plementary dimensions of interpersonal interaction. Rather than a sender-receiver model, this approach is built upon a relational model. The smallest unit of analy- sis is a paired exchange of two messages rather than a sin- gle message. As Watzlawick and Beavin (1967) point out, if a sequence of messages between A and B is broken down into statements about A separately from B, an intrapersonal or in— dividual explanation of communication behavior results. A quasi-interactional explanation results from a comparison of A with B. But a truly interactional explanation results when the measurement is based on the sequentially ordered interpretation of a series of two message units. Symmetrical interaction is characterized by "equality and the minimization of difference, while complementary in- teraction is based on the maximization of difference" (Watzlawick, et_§l., 1967, p. 69). Complementarity is fur- ther broken down into "one-up" and "one-down" positions. Neither communication symmetry nor complementarity are to be considered as "good" or "bad." The basic assumption is 46 that complementary and symmetrical relationship patterns func- tion homeostatically to stabilize the communication system. Pathological forms of interaction are suggested to be the nonmixing of different patterns, thus resulting in an over use of style of relating.16 The predominant potentially pathological forms outlined by Watzlawick, et_§l., are sym- metrical escalation (p. 107) and rigid complementarity (p. 108). Examples of symmetrical and complementary transactions given by Sluzki and Beavin are: giving/taking instruction=complementary (giving= one-up, taking=one-down) asking/answering=complementary (asking=one—down, answering=one-up) asserting/agreeing=complementary (asserting=one- up, agreeing=one-down) referential statement/referential statement: symmetrical agreeing/agreeing=symmetrica1 giving instructions/countering with instructions: symmetrical (1965, p. 326). In this system, the designation of symmetry and com- plementary one-up and one-down is based on both the gramma- tical form of the statement (e.g., question, assertion) and 16It is the suggestion of the present author that this proposition should be limited by the definition of the role relationship of the interactors, e.g., in a socially defined complementary role relation setting, heavy use of a particu- lar form of complementary communication pattern, would not necessarily be pathological or dysfunctional. 47 the response style ( e.g., agreeing, disagreeing) . Each speech is coded via a three-digit designation. The first digit designates the speaker. The second digit refers to the grammatical form of the speech and the third digit in- dicates the meta-communication aspect of the speech relative to the statement that came before it. The code categories are given below. lst digit code: 1. 1st speaker 2. 2nd speaker question assertion instruction orders talking over assertion and question question and assertion other laughter 2nd digit code: 3rd digit code: agreement disagreement extension answer disconfirmation topic change agreement and extension disagreement and extension other laughter \DmflmUI-bDJNH \DmflmU'lubWNH [—1 0 Code all silence over two seconds as 000; add another 000 for every additional five seconds of silence. Specific coding rules are outlined by Mark (pp.47-48) for the symmetry and complementarity relational designations of paired messages. Each pair is coded as representing one of the nine relational categories possible from the three by 48 three matrix, based on complementary one-up and one-down symmetry.l7 By tape recording the dialogues, the usual time mea- surements of amount of talking, frequency and distribution are available for each participant and for the total discus- sion. This coding system allows for frequency counts by in- dividual categories, for a sequential analysis of code cate- gories, and for a relational analysis based on successively paired message exchanges.18 Overview of Observational Approach As we have seen, the coding systems vary in a number of ways, not only in the more specific categories, but in their basic approach to the observation and codification of ongoing communicative acts. With the exception of the sys- tem developed by Carter, et;§lfl,which detailed acts large- ly by their manifest content, most of the systems tend to de-emphasize communication content. Chapple's procedure, however, was the one example that was totally content free, dealing only with the activity dimension of verbal behavior. The coding perspective of the systems differed as to the level of analysis. The methods of Steinzor and Borke 17The procedure is similar to the matrix analysis set forth in Amidon and Hough (1967). 18Inone sense each form of analysis has a relational aspect since the third coding digit refers to the metacom- munication aspect of the interaction. 49 are examples of classifying communication acts on the basis of the speaker's intent, while the methods of Bales and of Soskin and John, offer examples of classifying messages on the basis of the function of the act for the group. Most of the systems have not attempted a sequential or a relational analysis of communication acts. This aspect of message ex- change is the prime consideration given by Sluzki and Beavin in their analysis. The coding system developed and utilized in the pre- sent study is based on the work of Sluzki and Beavin, and Mark. The conceptual development and the coding procedures of this system are described in detail in Chapter III. The present research is one of the few empirical studies so far, to carry out a relational communication analysis. Family Communication Research This section will review two areas of studies deal- ing with the family. First, a selected review of family description variables and their relation to communication that have particular relevance for the present study will be presented. Secondly, an overview of selected research findings in the area of family interaction will be given. Family Studies Length of Marriage Marital communication was a major part of an inves- tigation by Komarovsky (1962) of blue collar (working class) 50 marriages. Based on a case study of 58 married pairs, Komarovsky found that the longer the marriage relation had existed, the less the communication between the married pair. Smardan (1957) found that the shorter the period of marriage, the more the couples had to talk about. Deitcher (1959) studied 120 families in different stages of the family life cycle and discovered that the estimated time spent talking together daily declined over the stages. Using Duvall's (1957) definitions of family stages, Deitcher reports that the time estimates for Stage I couples in the "recently married no children" stage was 2-1/4 hours and for Stage VI couples in the ”launching of children" stage was an hour. He also found a pattern of change for topics discussed over stages. In Stage I the topics tended to be predominately subjective-oriented, dealing with inner feelings, sex and plans for the future. In the childbearing and preschool stages, Stages II and III, the topics were more family-task oriented, with children, finances and care of the home being predominant topics. Es- timated time spent talking together decreased the most during these two periods. In Stages IV and V, the school year stages, the tOpics most discussed shifted toward com- munity participation, friends and acquaintances and local current events. In Stage VI, launching stage, the topic of community participation is still frequently discussed with subjective concerns least discussed, but with a renewed 51 interest in cultural subjects and relatives. These findings suggest that length of marriage is a factor in determining how much a couple talk and what they talk about. The relationship between length of marriage and sat- isfaction with marital communication, however, was not a linear one. Smardan (1957) found that older married couples (defined as married ten years or more) tended to be more satisfied with marital discussion than the younger married. The couples in Deitcher's study indicated that the peak level of communication satisfaction was at Stage I (childless, recently married), the second higher level at Stage VI (the launching of the children) and the lowest level of satis- faction at Stage V (teenage children). Kormarovsky found an interesting set of relationships when she compared length of marriage and educational level to measures of interspousal communication. She found that high school graduates communicated more fully with their mates about their experiences than did the less educated,19 but the longer the high school educated couples were married the less content they became with their marital communication, while the less educated couples became slightly more content. In Deitcher's study, marital couples of lower management 19A number of studies substantiate the inverse rela- tionship between social class and amount of interspousal communication (Rainwater, et al., 1962; Hill, 1955; Smardan, 1957; Straus, 1958). 52 (white-collar) occupations expressed higher communication satisfaction than did couples of the professional group. Komarovsky concludes from her data, that although the higher educated couples "enjoy fuller communication, their discontent may be attributed to their higher expecta- tions . . . high aspirations carry with them the risk of more discontent . . . with the duration of marriage the gap between expectations and reality widens for the educated" (p. 204). Sex Differences More wives than husbands in the Komarovsky study felt dissatisfied with their marital communication. Once again the higher educated wives felt more discontent than did the less educated group. This difference by educational break- down was true for the husbands, but at a lesser level than for the wives. Smardan (1957) also found in his study that wives expressed lower satisfaction than husbands with their communication relationship. This difference seems most likely to be due to the role structure of the family. The typical husband and wife situation is one in which more of the wife's total rewards are tied to one social system -- the family -- with the husband being the primary adult granter of approval and emotional gratification. The husband usually interacts in other external social systems (the work group being a 53 dominant one) and his psychosocial needs can be supplied by others besides the wife. Thus, it is speculated that the state of the marital relationship is more crucial for the wife than for the husband20 and needs for communication with spouse are higher for wives. Role Perceptions Two main aspects of role systems are (l) the way in which system members perceive the performance of system tasks being carried out behaviorally and (2) their expecta- tions as to how the performance of these tasks should be done. Role discrepancy as defined in the present research, refers to the degree to which these perceptions differ. Literature on the family contains many studies rela- ting shared role expectations or self-other-images of spouses and marital satisfaction (Dymond, 1954, Corsini, 1956, Luckey, 1960, Mangus, 1957, Stuckert, 1963, Hurvitz, 1960, Kolar, 1965). There are few studies on the perceived per- formance-expectation agreement dimension. Several of the fore-mentioned studies have used the MMPI or Leary's Inter- personal Check List (ICL) to measure the amount of cognitive congruency. In general, these studies have found that hus- bands and wives with more favorable self-ratings and more 20For a discussion of various aspects of marital sat- isfaction over the family life cycle for husbands and wives, see Rollins and Feldman (1970). 54 congruent self-images had greater marital happiness than married pairs with less favorable and less congruent images. Other studies have measured role congruency by dif- ferent combinations of spousal role expectations and per- ceptions of other's expectations. As Stuckert (1963) points out the relationship between the agreement of percep- tions and satisfaction is not a simple one and the findings of various studies have not been consistent. Of various comparisons, Stuckert found the congruence between wives' perceptions of their husband's expectations and the husband's actual expectations concerning marital roles to be associated with satisfaction, but for husbands, the actual agreement between their expectations and their wives' expectations was the dominant factor of association. A study by Luckey (1961) supports these findings. As mentioned, few studies in the family have focuses on perceived behavioral and role expectation agreement, even though the notion that conformity to role expectations is associated with positive feelings, and vice versa, is a common social-psychological generation (Gross, et al., 1958, Homans, 1961, Thibaut and Kelley, 1959). An early study by Ort (1950) compared the marital role expectations and perceived role performances of 100 married respondents and found a high negative correlation between deviations from expectations, and marital satisfac- tion. Hawkins and Johnsen (1969) replicated Ort's study 55 with a sample of thirty married, psychiatric clinic out- patients and found strong supporting evidence for Ort's findings. Perceived role discrepancy, defined as the degree to which a subject sees marital role performance deviating from his role expectations, was again highly correlated with marital satisfaction in a negative direction. A similar study by Burr (1971) with 116 couples, also found a nega- tive relationship between the two variables. Hawkins and Johnsen concluded that "the empirical generalization the conformity to role expectations produce positive emotional responses seems to hold for the marriage relationship as well as in numerous other types of relationships thus far studied" (p. 510).21 The performance versus expectations type of con- gruence, as used in this thesis, appears to have potential as an important system variable for analyzing interaction, particularly when it is viewed from the perspective of the social exchange/interpersonal bargaining theories of be- havior and the general systems approach to the study of communication. Normative definitions set up boundaries or permis- sible ranges of variation that may occur among the variables constituting a system, so that the system still retains its 21Boyd C. Rollins, Department of Child Development and Family Relations, Brigham Young University is in the process of developing a Marital Roles Inventory designed to measure congruency of performance and expectations. 56 unity. These definitions concerning role behavior are usually quite situational, with fairly high degress of freedom. Nevertheless, these role expectations are seen as a generalized constraining frameworks within which par- ticipants improvise as they act out their roles. These normative expectancies function as comparison levels in the social cost-reward process, and as such, set limits for the behavioral system. These subjective "rules of the game" promote social homeostasis, or what Ackerman (1958) so aptly calls "controlled instability," a process by which system members maneuver and adapt to internal and external inputs as they attempt to preserve the identity of the system. The term homeostasis as utilized here involved the principle of preserving the intactness and continuity of a system, but not at a static, equilibrium level. Homeo- statis is to be interpreted as an adaptive capacity to sta- bilize, which is equivalent to Buckley's (1967), "morphogenic" concept. Social homeostasis refers to a system's capacity to resist and/or modify internal and external forces ef— fecting the system so that a necessarily level of balance and integration is maintained for sufficient preservation of the system and for adaptive change and development of the system. Applying the concept of homeostasis to the family, Jackson (1965) views the family as a rule-governed system with built-in self-corrective processes that attempt to 57 maintain a relatively stable definition of the system. In a similar manner Haley (1962) states that within the family there are two sorts of governing processes, family members not only attempt to set limits, but that they behave in error- activated ways if member exceeds established limits.22 Family Interaction Studies The family as a small group has been involved in remark- ably' few studies. The small group area of research has its beginnings in the early 1900's, but not until the 1950's did the family come under study from this research perspective.23 22 Festinger's (1950) work on internal group processes and communication frequency and direction is applicable to this point. Festinger cites three forces that will determine when the group will communicate about a given topic: (1) When the perceived discrepancy in group opinion regarding the topic is great, (2) when the topic is highly relevant to the functioning of the group, and (3) when the attraction (cohesion) of the group members is high. The three forces that are seen as deter- mining who will receive the communication are: (l) The per- ceived discrepancy of a member's opinion, (2) the likelihood of successfully influencing the discrepant member, and (3) the degree to which the person is desired as a group member. In general, these hypotheses have been supported. Of particular note, the evidence strongly supports the notion that communi- cation is directed to desired deviant group members (Festinger and Thibaut, 1951; Schachter, 1951), and that this communica- tion is increased when group members are more highly interde- pendent for the accomplishment of goals (Berkowitz and Howard, 1959). 23 The family as a unit of study differs significantly from most of the "groups" studied with in the small group research tradition. The family is a social group with a history (Frambo, 1965) and as importantly with a future, while most small group studies have been done with ad hog groups. Small group research is correlated to family studies but the difference of history must be kept in mind. See Waxler and Mishler (1970) for a re- cent review drawing together findings from both small group and family research. 58 Bishop (1951) pioneered in the observational study of the mother-child dyad and Strodtbeck (1951) in the study of the husband-wife dyad. Strodtbeck used a "revealed difference" technique to engender discussion between family members. This technique involves having members respond to situational items individ- ually, then in joint sessions, coming to an agreement on items in which there had been an initial disagreement. This procedure has been used extensively in later family inter- action studies. Strodtbeck (1954) found in experimental decision mak- ing situations involving three family members, husband, wife and adolescent son, that the greater the amount of talking done by a member, the greater the influence and dominance of that family member on family decisions. In a study of only husband-wife interaction, Strodtbeck (1951) found that there was less differentiation in speaking time in the two- member group, but nevertheless, "talking the most" and "de- cision winning" went together. Most-talking spouses tended more frequently to ask questions, carry out opinions and analysis and make more rewarding comments. Least-talking spouses tended to give simple agreements and disagreements, and aggressive reactions. Kenkle (1963) also found that the extent of talking and influence in decision making were re- lated in a positive direction, but more strongly related for the husbands than for the wives. 59 Both Strodtbeck's and Kenkel's studies were done with "normal" families, i.e., they did not have a known record of emotional disturbance in the family. The bulk of the family interaction research, however, has been done in the family therapy area. This research has mainly concentrated on searching out differences between normal and abnormal fam- ilies, particularly in their styles of interacting with one another. Some of the more relevant findings from this area will not be presented. Farina (1960), using a procedure similar to Strodtbeck's "revealed difference," compared indices of role dominance (total speaking time, yielding to one's spouse, who spoke first and last) and of conflict (interruptions, frequency of simultaneous speeches, disagreements and agressions) of three groups of parents; those of good premorbid patients, of poor premorbid patients, and of normal children. Maternal domi- nance and conflict characterized the interaction of parents of poor premorbid patients the most, and of normal parents the least. Caputo (1963) in a similar study found that there was more sharing of power in normal families than in abnormal families. An interaction study of family discussions of parents of neurotics and parents of schizophrenics, by Fisher, et al., (1959) found that parents of neurotics disagreed less, talked more and with more clarity than parents of schizophrenics. 60 In a study comparing family triads with a schizo- phrenic child and normal family triads, Cheek (1964) found, using a modified Bales interaction system, that schizophrenic triads had less verbal activity than normal triads. Data gathered on interaction variability in therapy sessions by Lennard and Bernstein (1969) tend to show schizo- phrenic patients having less variability in interaction rate from session to session than did neurotic patients. Further, variability in terms of direction (who talks to whom) with- in a mother-father-child triad was found to be much lower in the "schizophrenic" families. Lennard, et_al., (1965) recorded the rate and success of interruptions of ongoing interaction and found that schizo- phrenic sons and their mothers made fewer interruptions than the fathers of schizophrenics or of normal mothers and their sons.‘ And the interruptions made by the schizophrenic fam- ily members were less successful in effect that in the nor- mal families. Ferreira (1963) and Ferreira and Winter (1965) inves- tigated decision-making in normal and abnormal families. They found that normal families had a higher degree of spon- taneous agreement (agreeing on an initial individual choice), had a lower rate of "inappropriate" decisions (choosing an alternative that had not been selected by any of the family members) and took less time to reach a decision. Bodin (1966) confirmed the finding that higher spontaneous 61 agreement was more characteristic of normal than abnormal families. In a later study by Ferreira et_al., (1966) several other interaction variables were investigated. The normal and abnormal families did not differ significantly in terms of who talked the most, number of simultaneous speeches or equality in decision making, but did differ on amount of silence and time needed to complete decisions. Abnormal families had a higher percentage of silences and took long- er to complete the task. In studying the frequency and order with which family members communicate to one another Haley (1964) found when observing normal and abnormal three-member family groups, that the mother-child interchange had the highest frequency in the normal family with the mother-father interchange most frequent in the abnormal families. When analyzing the order of speech, Haley found that the speech sequences differed from a random distribution. This indicated that communica- tion organization existed in both types of families, but the normal families tended to use more of the six possible order sequences, than did the disturbed families. In a later study, however, Haley (1967) did not find the speech sequence de- viations from randomness to hold up between normal and ab- normal families with two children present. Mishler and Waxler (1969) studied speech sequence but were more concerned with the degree to which the response 62 to a speech acknowledged the immediately preceeding speaker, which they refer to as the predictability of responsive- ness. They found predictability of responsiveness was higher in abnormal families than normal families but not at a significant level. Subjective interpretations of therapy sessions have been the stimulus of most of the family interaction re- search. VA hoSt of "theories" and concepts have been generated, but much less hard data. The findings of the studies cited are somewhat inconsistent. They do not add up to a large number of generalizations. Keeping in mind, however, the methodological difficulties of this type of research and the recentness of the interest in this area, these studies begin to build an empirical base for the in- terpretations of family dynamics. In general, these studies support one of the most pervasive notions in the psychiatric literature: That abnormal families are more rigid in their interaction than normal families (Lidz, 1957, 1958; Wynne, et_al., 1958; Rychoff, 1959; Paul and Grosser, 1964; Meissher, 1964). In discussing the self- governing process of the family, Haley (1967) states "the tightness and rigidity of family networks becomes particu- larly evident to the therapist attempting to bring about a change in whole families. As with individuals, family studies indicate flexibility is synonymous with normality and rigidity with pathology." 63 While the present study is not an investigation of the differences between abnormal and normal families, the basic transactional hypotheses of this study will be based on the psychiatric oriented family interaction literature. CHAPTER III RESEARCH METHODOLOGY In the previous chapters a general view of the re- search project has been described and put into perspective with other relevant work. The task of this chapter is to examine in detail the sampling and data collection proce- dures, the resulting sample and the measures of the princi- pal variables. SamplingiProcedures The present study is part of a larger communication study sponsored by the Office of Civil Defense. The total research project, titled, "Communication Processes in Civil Defense Program" (OCD Contract No. DAHC-20-7l-C-0297), con- sists of a Phase I in which the impact and effectiveness of a Civil Defense Community Shelter Plan information campaign are evaluated and a Phase II in which more extensive infor- mation on family communication patterns is investigated. Phase I is based on individual responses and Phase II on paired husband and wife responses. The respondents for Phase I were randomly selected from telephone lists of the greater Dayton, Ohio area. This was the distribution area of the OCD Community Shelter Plan information campaign 64 65 carried out in the summer of 1971. The data for Phase I were gathered by telephone interviews in the fall of 1971. Half of the interviews were carried out with the male head of household and half with the female head of household. The resulting 399 completed interviews of Phase I were the data base for the Phase II sub-sample. For respondents of Phase I to be potential subjects for Phase II, two criteria were established: (1) That the respondents be presently married and (2) that he/she have children under 12 years of age. One hundred and forty one respondents met these criteria. These respondents and/or their spouse were contacted, or an attempt was made to con- tact them, during the spring of 1972 for the purpose of setting up a personal interview with each couple. Of the 141 husband-wife pairs, 64 were interviewed, 33 were not at the residence where they were contacted in the fall for Phase I or were not available during the three week field interview period, and 44 refused to be in the study. One additional interview was completed with a couple who was living at the address of a Phase I respondent, but who did not participate in Phase 1. Thus the total number of completed husband and wife interviews meeting the se- lection criteria for Phase II is 65. 66 Table 1. Number and percent of dyads by interview category. Interview Category Number Percent Completed 65 46% Not available 33 23% Refused 44 31% Total 142 100% Data Collection Procedures The data for this study were collected by personal interviews. Arrangements for these interviews were made by contacting the subjects at their home. After the interview- er expressed appreciation for their (or their spouse's) pre- vious participation in the communication project, they were given an introductory letter explaining the purpose of the second phase of the research. They were also told that they would receive ten dollars for taking part in the study (See Appendix I). Appointments were set up with the participat- ing couples (at their convenience) for a personal interview in their home. Each couple's interview took approximately two hours- Two types of information were obtained in the interview; (1) self-report data from each husband and wife and (2) actual discussion data for each husband and wife pair. The self-report 67 data were gathered by questionnaire (See Appendix II) and the discussion data were secured by tape recording the couples interaction as they discussed several family involved topics (See Appendix III). An interview began by asking the couple to fill out the questionnaire separately without talking to one another. When both husband and wife had completed the questionnaire and returned them to the interviewer, they were presented with four topics, one at a time, and requested to talk to one another about the issues and in three cases to reach a joint decision. (See Appendix IV for details on interviewer instructions.) Two of the topics dealt with family behavior in an emergency situation and two topics dealt with family behavior in a nonemergency situation. The first topic was concerned with how the couple met and decided to marry. This topic was purposefully utilized as an "ice breaker" and initiator of reciprocal interaction between the husband and wife. The second topic concerned a nuclear emergency and the sharing of home basements for protection, the third topic dealt with career interests versus family interests particularly for the wife, and the final topic concerned a natural emergency, a tornado, and family plans for protection. As each topic was presented, the interviewer gave a standardized set of noncontent stimuli that might be included in their discussion; first impressions, expectations, feelings, 68 problems, and solutions. The couples were asked to discuss each topic as fully as they could within a time limit of ten minutes. The interviewer attempted to stay out of the conver- sation as much as possible by (l) stressing that this was a discussion between the couple themselves, by (2) not having eye contact with the couple and by (3) not talking to the couple unless asked a question or if a silence of over fif- teen seconds occurred in the first five minutes of the inter- action. In the event of being asked for direCtion or of a fifteen second silence, the interviewer utilized standard- ized probes to promote at least five minutes of discussion per topic. In the majority of the cases, the couples averaged over five minutes per topic. See Appendix V for a listing of the minimum, maximum and average discussion times across dyads by topic. I Data collection is rarely free of all problems, but the procedures utilized in this study were enhanced by a pretest of the instruments and research procedures on a Lansing, Michigan sample. Fifty Lansing area residents were selected at random from census tract data to provide a sampling base for the pretest. Ten interviews were com- pleted with husband-wife dyads that met the criterion of having children under twelve years of age. Five other couples were also contacted to fill out only the self-report questions. The pretest resulted in several modifications 69 of the questionnaire, the topics discussed and data gather- ing procedures. Type of Data As stated before, two types of data were gathered during the interviews. The self-report questionnaire data provides informa- tion and indices of various aspects of husband and wife be- havior. More specifically data wereobtained on: (1) The time allocations of husbands and wives with one another, with the family, with extra-family relationships (See Appen- dix II, questions 1 and 4a); (2) the amount of time husbands and wives talk to one another and their satisfactions with that time (See Appendix II, questions 2, 3, 4b, 5, 6); (3) the frequency of husband and wife communication on various topics (See Appendix II, question 7); (4) control of dyadic communication between husband and wife in terms of who talks the most, who initiates and ends conversations and who do- minates the subjects discussed (See Appendix II, question 8); (5) husband and wife evaluation of their communication re- lationship (See Appendix II, questions 9 and 10); (6) husband and wife's processing of information (See Appendix II, questions 11, 12 and 13; however, this data will not be analy— zed in the present study.); (7) husband and wife perceptions of who does certain family tasks, i.e., role performance, and who should do these tasks, i.e., role expectations, which 70 form the basis of the role discrepancy measure (See Appendix II, questions 14 and 15); (8) husband and wife scores on a dominance scale (See Appendix II, question 16. This varia- ble is analyzed in a separate study by Ericson, 1972); (9) husband and wife demographic background information and their satisfaction with their marriage (See Appendix II, questions 17 through 24). The tape recorded discussion data of verbal inter- action between spouses provides the basis for a relational analysis of husband-wife communication patterns. This is a relatively content-free analysis of the control or defining aspects of dyadic interaction. A detailed explanation of the relational level of analysis is given in a following section, "Operationalization of Variables." The discussion data also provides substantive information of husbands' and wives' viewpoints and projected family behaviors on four topics. A content analysis, however, of these discussions will not be undertaken in this study. Description of the Sample As described in the previous section, the subjects for this study are sixty-five husband and wife dyads living in the greater Dayton, Ohio area who had children under twelve years of age. The average age of the wives was 32.6 years with a range from twenty-one to forty-five years. The average age 71 of the husbands was 35.9 with a range from twenty-one to fifty-four years. Three of the couples were Black and the other 62 couples were White. The educational level of the wives ranged from eighth grade to graduation from college with the average level being 12.6 years of education. The educational level ranged from eighth grade to six years of graduate school for the husbands with the average years of education being 14.0. Annual family income ranged from less than $3,000 to more than $15,000. The average yearly income was ap- proximately $11,000. The majority of the wives, 57 out of 65, classified themselves as full-time homemakers. Only eight of the wives gave occupations other than housewife. The length of years married ranged from one year to twenty-six years, with the average years married being 11.6. For almost all couples their present marriage is their first marriage. Operationalization of Variables The task of this section is to describe the methodo— logical procedures for measuring two major research variables under study; role discrepancy, analyzed as an independent or factor variable, and relational communication patterns, considered as dependent variables. 72 Role Discrepancy The role discrepancy measure is based on a comparison of role performance and role expectation descriptions. Each dyad member was presented (on the questionnaire) a list of fifteen items concerning different family task areas: Child- care; household jobs; family decisions, both financial and social; and social-emotional. The subjects were asked to describe their perception as to who performs these respon- sibilities in their family by responding to the following description alternatives: "husband almost always," "husband more than wife," "both about the same," "wife more than husband," "wife almost always," or "neither one.“ After completing the description of who gees the various tasks, they were asked to respond to the same items, with the same response alternatives, with their preference descriptions of who should do the tasks. The response alternatives, given above were coded from one to five. The "neither one" response was coded as a three to remove its effect on the discrepancy score. The largest number of "neither one" responses were given by subjects without school age children to item.deealing with helping children with school work. (See questions 14 and 15, item ja through item 0 in Appendix II.) The role discrepancy score for each dyad was the summation of each member's difference scores between item 73 responses to who should do the task and who does do the task, across the fifteen items. A larger score represents a wider level of discrepancy. On the basis of these scores,1 rang- ing from three to twenty-seven and approximating a normal curve, dyads were grouped by thirds into low, medium, and high role discrepancy categories.2 Breaking the distribution curve in this manner, 22 dyads fell into the low category, twenty in the medium cell and 23 into the high discrepancy category. These role discrepancy levels will be used as the major factor variable in the analysis of the data. Since the discrepancy scores are based on items as- sumed to represent five different task areas of a husband- wife interpersonal system, there was no reason to believe the fifteen items would necessarily be internally consistent. The design was that the items would sample enough different tasks to allow detection of areas within the system where expectation and performance consensus was lacking. Role discrepancy is a composite measure of the degree to which members perceive nonconsensus existing on the various system dimensions sampled. It is assumed that the more dimensions on which there is normative-behavioral discrepancy, the higher the level of inequity existing in the system. 1The discrepancy scores had a mean of 13.12 and a standard deviation of 4.9 2Acomparison was made between the above categorization method and using individual husband and wife scores broken at a median point to divide the dyads into discrepancy cate- ories of hi h-high, a mixed group of high-low, low-high, and ow-low. On y eight dyads out of the 65 did not fall into the same category levels. Thus, both methods gave approximately equivalent breakdowns. 74 A factor analysis, carried out on the discrepancy scores, indicates that the items tend to load along the task dimensions suggested. The major factor consisted of the items sampling social and interpersonal concerns, except for item 0, dealing with taking the 'other's point of view. This latter item loaded with two of the household task items for a second factor. Factor three, consisted of two child- care items plus preparing meals and deciding the family bud- get. A fourth factor consisted of two economic oriented items and helping children with homework. These four factors account for half of the variance. The tendency for the items to cluster along the specified family task areas, give some degree of evidence of reliability. Validity claims for the discrepancy measure are based on construct validation (Cronbach and Meehl, 1955), i.e., the ability to order and predict social events. As Peak states, "the ultimate test of validity of a construct and of the measures which enter into its definition is found in the utility of the construct in the process of reducing the matrix of events to some meaningful order" (1953, p. 273). Jahoda, Deutsch and Cook echo this argument with their statement that, "the ultimate test of validity of any mea- sure is the correctness of the predictions based on it" (1955, p. 93). This approach to validation is criterion-oriented, but not of the traditional single correlation check variety; 75 rather it is a continuous process of checking predictions against results, a process in which both theory and method must be simultaneously validated. We find in the examina- tion of the research results of this study adequate support for construct validity of the discrepancy measure. History, or length of marriage, will be utilized as a factor control variable. In the research design, an attempt was made to control somewhat on the stage of marriage by limiting the study to couples who had children under twelve years old. This means that the couples are in the childbear- ing or childrearing stages. The range of years in these stages can be quite extended. Therefore, length of marriage is considered to be a further controlling factor in this study. The median point of years married was used to break the dyads into two groups. Thirty three couples married from one to eleven years constitute the shorter history group and 32 couples make up the longer history group.3 Relational Communication Patterns In this section the relational communication concepts will first be explored and then a system for indexing 3 A t-test between the two groups on the number of years married was computed. The t-value was 12.18, significant at the .001 level, indicating that the groups do indeed differ. 76 relational patterns of communication will be presented. The study of the relational level of communicative behavior, as opposed to the content level dealing with the referents of symbols, is an emerging concern in the commun- ication area. Relational communication refers to the as- pects of message exchange by which individuals control and thus define their relationships with other.4 All interaction involves some ongoing reciprocal de- finition of the interactor's relationship. Awareness of the relationship-defining aspects of communication has prompted considerable conceptual progress in this area, but the operational level is still relatively underdeveloped. The present analysis will focus on message sequences, rather than on individual message units; on indexing the relational control aspects of communication rather than the substantive content; and on indexing transactions over time. More specifically, transactional message units are concep- tualized in terms of their (1) control-defining nature, (2) the type of control similarity or dissimilarity between con- tiguous messages and (3) their sequential configurations. The present coding scheme builds on the theoretical orientation of Bateson (1958) and his co-workers, particularly 4An illustration of the relation-defining aspects of communication is when one interactor (A) issues an imperative and the other grants control by complying. Compliance con- firms the asymmetrical definition designated by A. If B's response is noncompliant, B is denying A's definition of be- ing in eontrol. If B responds with a counter imperative, B has defined the relationship at that moment in that given exchange as symmetrical. 77 Watzlawick et_§1., (1967) Jackson (1965) and Haley (1962), but more directly on Sluzki and Beavin's (1965) symmetrical- complementary typology of dyadic interaction patterns and Mark's (1970) interpretation and operational application of their work. All of these efforts emphasize the transactional aspects of the communication process and incorporate the con- cepts of symmetry and complementary as two basic patterns of interaction. Symmetrical interaction has been characterized by "equality and the minimization of difference, while comple- mentary interaction is based on the maximization of difference" (Watzlawick, et_al., 1967, p. 69). Complementarity has been further categorized as "one-up," being in control and "one- down" accepting control. In a symmetrical transaction or re- lationship one interactor behaves toward the other, as the other behaves toward him. There is a similarity of conduct between the two individuals; there is a symmetry of relation- al control. In a complementary transaction the interactor's behaviors are fully differential. The control definition of the relationship offered by one interactor is accepted by the other.5 Sluzki and Beavin operationally define communication symmetry and complementarity in terms of "the structural re- semblance or lack of resemblance (respectively) of the 5See Erving Goffman, pp. 63-64 of "The Nature of De- ference and Demeanor" in Interaction Ritual (1967). 78 reciprocal communication behaviors of the members of a dyadic system" (1955: P-323)- Note that whether the transaction is symmetrical or complementary, both interactors participate in the definition of the relationship. Thus, the smallest unit of a relational analysis is a paired exchange of two messages.6 Examples of symmetrical and complementary transactions given by Sluzki and Beavin were: giving/taking instruction=complementary (giving= one-up, taking=one-down) asking/answering=complementary (asking=one-down, answering=one-up) asserting/agreeing=complementary (asserting=one- up, agreeing=one-down) referential statement/referential statement= symmetrical agreeing/agreeing=symmetrical giving instructions/countering with instructions= symmetrical (1965, p. 326) Note that the transaction type is based on the con- trol nature of both the grammatical form (e.g., question, assertion) and the response style (e.g., agreeing, disagree- ing) of each message. This approach is basic to the present 6Watzlawick and Beavin (1967) point out that if a se- quence of messages between A and B is broken down into state- ments about A separately from B, an interpersonal or individ- ual explanation of communication behavior results. A quasi- interactional explanation results from a comparison of A with B. But a truly interactional explanation results when the measurement is based on the sequentially ordered interpreta- tion of a series of at least two message units. 79 coding scheme, but it allows only for the acceptance or re- jection of control. In the exchange of messages, however, relational definitions offered by one participant may not only be accepted or rejected by the other, they may also be modified. The concepts of symmetry and complementarity catch the maximal differences of relational control, with acceptance confirming the definition offered and rejection refuting the definition. These polarities, as significant as they are, do not provide a way for taking into account lees extreme modifying responses to the role definitions offered. In the development of the present scheme a neutralizing direction of relational control has been incorporated to refer to commun- icative behaviors that redefine the relationship, but in more moderate terms than total acceptance or rejection. With the addition of a third control direction, a third general trans- actional type results by which to describe communication systems. The three major transactional types refer to mes- sage exchanges consisting of control behaviors that are (1) similar in nature (symmetrical); (2) are of a mixed nature with one message being neutralizing (transitional); or (3) are maximally dissimilar (complementary). The coding system to be presented has many similarities to the work of Sluzki and Beavin (1965) and Mark (1970), but has some differences as well. The main differences refer to the additional neutralizing control direction and the trans- itory transactionary category. Incorporation of these 80 modifications in the present scheme is prompted by a con- tinuing effort for methodological refinement. The Coding System Using Sluzki and Beavin's definition, a message is defined as each verbal intervention of each member in a dialogue. A message may be a single utterance or a flow of continuing utterances. Each message is viewed as being a response to the preceding message and in this aspect is the definer of that transaction, as well as a stimulus for the following message. Thus in a series of two-message ex- changes, it is the second of a pair of messages that defines the nature of the transaction. See Figure 1, representing an exchange of messages between persons A and B. \ ——-\ ’-~ \ L \xp—q-—-—-- --\F---- K—A‘l V ‘ Message AI L Message B1 Mes_s_age A Message B2 Transaction 1 Transaction 2 Transaction 3 Figure 1. Message transactions. In transaction 1, message Bl defines the relation- ship while in transaction 2 it is the stimulus for which A2 provides the transactional definition. For example, if message A1 is an instruction, e.g., "Come on, let's go," and message B1 is an acceptance of this instruction, e.g., "O.K., I'm coming," the transactional exchange is complementary 81 with A in the one-up or controlling position. If message B1 is an instruction in return, e.g., "No, we're going to stay here," the transaction is symmetrical, with B matching and thus rejecting A's attempt to be one-up.- Taking Sluzki and Beavin's lead that the classification of symmetry and complementarity is based both on the gramma- tical form of the message and the response style or the meta- communication of the message, a coding scheme was set up to code each of these aspects for each message. Following Mark's method, a three digit designation is used to code each utter- ance. The first digit designates the speaker. The second digit refers to the grammatical form of the message and the third digit indicates the meta-communication response of the message relative to the statement that came before it. Both the grammatical and the more evaluative response form reflect Bateson's distinction of the “command" or relational aspect of a message rather than the "report" or content level. The coding categories are presented below: lst Digit 2nd Digit 3rd Digit 1. Speaker A l. Assertion 1. Support 2. Speaker B 2. Question 2. Nonsupport 3. Talk-over 3. Extension 4. Noncomplete 4. Answer 5. Other 5. Instruction 6. Order 7. Disconfirmation 8. Topic change 9. Initiation- termination .0. Other 82 A period of silence that occurs for three seconds is coded as 000, as are each additional five second periods of silence. Periods of laughter that occur for three second durations are coded by speaker (using the first digit) and 99 in the second and third digit positions. If the laughter is simultaneously produced by both interactors it is coded as 999 for each three second period. Code Categories The code categories under the first digit refer to the two speakers and allow the flow of messages to be accounted for by speaker. The code categories under the second digit refer to the format of the message. These category decisions involve very little inference on the part of the coder. An assertion is any completed referen- tial statement. This category includes the declarative and imperative forms. A question is any speech which takes an interrogative grammatical form. The remaining categories under the second digit are not grammatical forms of speech per se., but are descrip- tive of the format of a message. A talk-over refers to the way in which a speaker comes into the dialogue, what Mark calls a "mobilization" style.7 The normative communicative procedure is to alternate speeches at the end of a completed speech or at a pause in the exchange. This category refers to an interruptive manner of coming into a conversation. Any 7See Mark (1970) p. 41. 83 distinguishable interruption (verbal intervention made while other is talking) is defining as a talk-over. If the third digit has a code number from one to nine, it is defined as a successful talk-over since the speaker gained control enough to express an intention, if the third digit has a code of 0, it is considered an unsuccessful attempt to gain speaking control. The noncomplete category refers to any utterance that is initiated but not expressed in a completed format. Some examples of noncompletes would be, "well, ah . . .," " now then, what I thought was . . .," etc. The category ether refers to verbal utterances that are unclassifiable as to their form. The code categories under the third digit refer to the response mode of the speech. These classifications in- volve more inference than the previous categories. However, careful delineation of the meaning of each of these cate- gories will lower the subjectiveness of the coding. I The support category refers to both the giving and seeking of agreement, assistance, acceptance and approval. The nonsupport code is used to denote disagreement, rejec- tion, demands and challenges. The extension code is used to classify a message that continues the flow or theme of the preceding message. Included under this category is a noncommittal response to a question. 84 The answer code is reserved for a response to a question which has substance and/or commitment. A noncommittal re- sponse to a question such as "I don't know," is coded as an extension, while a definitive response such as "It was July 6th," is coded as an answer since the two types of re- sponses have different control defining natures. The categories instruction and order both denote a regulative response but of different intensities. An £27 struction is a suggestive and evaluative statement which is often accompanied with qualification and clarification, while an ggder is an unqualified command with little or no explanation. For example, "I think it's time you went to bed now because you have school tomorrow," is an instruction. "Go to bed," is an order. The distinction between disconfirmation and topic change made by Mark is used in the present analysis. Both categories refer to a response switch or noncontinuance but a ". . . disconfirmation occurs after a statement has been made which demands a response to it by the other individual and he does not respond to the demand . . ." while a topic change occurs with the ". . . introduction of a new idea after discussion of (another topic) . . ." (Mark, 1970, p. 44). Thus, disconfirmation refers to a message exchange in which one interactor requests a response and the other interactor ignores the request. Topic change refers to a message exchange in which the second message has no topic 85 or theme commonality with the first message but no response commonality was requested.8 The initiation-termination code is used to denote a message that begins or attempts to end an interaction. The category ether is used if the response mode is unclear or unclassifiable. A list of examples for all second and third digit codes are given in Appendix VI. For code decisions that are difficult, i.e., do not readily fall into one category, use the following set of priority considerations to facilitate coding. These priorities are based on the predominant func- tion of the message. Thus for second digit codes, consider first if the message is a talk-over (#3). A talk-over may take the form of other second digit categories, but indepen- dent of form, an interruptive speech is coded as a talk- over. If the speech is not ea talk-over, then consider in order, if it is a question (#2), an assertion (#1) or a non- complete (#4). If it is none of these, code it as "other" (#5) . For third digit codes, make code decisions in a simi- lar manner using the following order: Consider first if the message is an initiation or termination (#9). An initiation 8In this scheme the categories of support, nonsupport and disconfirmation or topic change are similar to Horney's (1945) broad interpersonal classification patterns of "Going toward," "Going against," and "Going away from." 86 or termination may take the form of other third digit cate- gories, but our first interest is if it initiates or terminates. Secondly, consider if a message is an answer (#4). This is the first priority on which to judge noninitiating and non- terminating responses. Again regardless of form, a speech is coded as an answer if that is its main response function. For messages that are response switches, consider first if it is a disconfirming switch (#7), if not, then if it is a topic change (#8). For messages that have a regulative function decide first if it is an order (#6). If not, it is coded as an instruction (#5). If the message is none of the above, then consider if it is a support (#1), nonsupport (2), or extension (#3) and if it is none of these, code it as an "other" (#0). To summarize, each utterance is assigned a three digit code. The first digit denotes the speaker. The second digit describes the form of the speech. The third digit describes the response mode of the speech. Second digit category de- cisions are based only on the message being coded. Third digit category decisions are based on consideration of the preceding message, as well as the message being coded. In this manner any two-person communication exchange can be re- presented by a series of sequentially ordered three digit codes. On the basis of these codes, descriptions of the frequency and relative frequency of message form and response mode can be made. 87 Control Direction Based on the combined defining nature of the grammati- cal form and response mode of a message, it is given one of three control direction possibilities. These assignments are made in terms of whether a message is (1) a movement toward control of the exchange, which is designated asi-one up; (2) a movement toward being controlled by seeking or accepting control of other, which is designated al'one-down; or (3) a movement toward neutralizing control, which has a leveling effect and is designated as+-one-across. Code categories representing message forms and response modes that are viewed as control maneuvers toward one-up are: Nonsupport responses including questions demanding an answer, answers with substance, instructions, orders, disconfirmations, topic changes, complete statements of initiation and termina- tion and all successful talk-overs except supportive talk- overs. The one-down code categories are: All support responses including questions that seek supportive responses, noncom- plete phrases that seek others to take control, supportive talk-overs, questions that continue the dialogue (extension) or have uncodable responses (other), and unsuccessful talk- overs. Neutralizing, or control leveling categories are viewed as carrying an interaction along with a minimized effort at controlling the relationship. Code categories that 88 are seen as one-across maneuevers are assertions Of exten- sion and those with uncodable response modes, noncomplete phrases and "other" (unclassifiable message forms) that are extensions. This includes statements of continuance, "filler" phrases and noncommittal responses to questions (i.e., the "empty" answer response). Also included in the one-across category are noncompletes that initiate or terminate and that have unclear response modes (i.e., have third digit codes or "other"), and finally, utterances with both uncod- able form and response modes (i.e., "other-other"). The control direction of message types can perhaps be seen more clearly and more completely in the following ma- trix of combined form and response modes. c o '3 a) ' c m ow 8- JJ 0-: "4 U '4" U 4.; .,4 H (L m :4 s c m o :3 s m L: :4 o o W,E p cm : +J m m 13 m o. "4“ a :3 o >< c s :4 -H o u a: 2 an s +4 0 :3 e #3 o 1. 2 3 4 5 6 '7 8 9 0 Assertion 1 + -+ -+ + + + 4+ + + + Question 2 + + + + i + + + + + Talk-over 3 + ‘f f f + i ‘+ + i + Noncomplete 4 + ‘f + ¢ ¢ 4. f 4 .1 + Other 5 4. + + + 1~ + J} + 1. + Figure 2. Message type and control direction. 89 The following set of rules give the direction of con- trol for a second and third digit message code combination. There are fifty combinations possible. The digit combina- tions are coded as follows: 1. one—up + = 12, 14, 15, l6, 17, 18, 19 one-down + = 11 one-across + = 13, 10 2. one-up + = 22, 24, 25, 26, 27, 28, 29 one-down + = 21, 23, 20 3. one-up + = 32, 33, 34, 35, 36, 37, 38, 39 one-down + = 31, 30 4. one-up + = 42, 44, 45, 46, 47, 48 one-down + = 41 one-across + = 43, 49, 40 5. one-up + = 52, 54, 55, 56, 57, 58, 59 one-down + = 51 one—across + 53, 50 To briefly illustrate the relational coding rules, note the following description of message form and response modes. If the message is an assertion which is an extension of the dialogue, it is coded as a thirteen and represents a one-across-rmovement. If the message is an assertion ex- pressing support, it is coded as an eleven which is a one- downwvmovement. A nonsupportive assertion is coded as a twelve, and represents a one-up‘rmovement. 90 Earlier in the paper, it was stressed that a message is both a response to what precedes it and a stimulus for the message that follows. For most messages these two as- pects will be the same and will not require separate codes. Thus, most messages will have only one set of code numbers, and one control direction code. However, with this coding scheme each message has the possibility of having two sets of code numbers to identify these two aspects and therefore, the possibility of being identified by two control directions. If the response interpretation is categorically different from the stimulus interpretation, two sets of second and third digit codes will be used to describe the message, and two control directions will be identified in accordance with the coding rules. The following examples are given to illustrate situa- tions in which a message has two sets of second and third digit codes but the control directions are the same in Example A and different in Example B. Example A: Speaker 1 "I think we should take this now." 115 + Speaker 2 "OK, I guess so, but what about the other things?" 211-221 +-+ Speaker 1 "Well, we'll have to get them later." 114 + Example B: Speaker 1 "I think we should take this now." 115 + 91 Speaker 2 "OK, I guess so, but I don't like you al- ways deciding things." 211-212 +-+ Speaker 1 "Well, that's tough!" 112 + Transactional Codes The preceding explanation of message codes allows us to now move to the transactional coding procedure necessary for a relational analysis. By combining the control direc- tion of individual messages into pairs of sequential exchanges it is possible to operationalize transactions by the symmetry or nonsymmetry of control for each exchange as it "unfolds" in a two person interaction. Each transaction code (like the individual message code) contains three digits. The first digit refers to the first speaker in the exchange, the second digit refers to the first speaker's message control direction and the third digit designates the second speaker's message control direction. For illustration the transaction values in Example A are a ++, following by a ++, in Example B, a ++is followed by a ++. When a single message, as with Speaker two in Examples A and B has two control codes note that both code directions are utilized in the producing the transactional measure. The transactional types that result from the combina- tions of paired messages with three directional possibilities are given in the following matrix: 92 one-up one-down one-across one-up 1 ++ 4 ++ 7 ++ one—down 2 ++ 5 ++ 8 ++ one—across 3 ++ 6 ++ 9 ++ Figure 3. Matrix of transactional types. The symmetrical transactions are represented.in cells 1, 5 and 9. An advantage of the present coding system is the ability to identify by direction the different kinds of sym- metry, rather than leave the type of symmetry unspecified as in previous systems. We refer to ++(ce11 1) symmetry as competitive, ++(ce11 5) symmetry as submissive and ++(ce11 9) symmetry as neutralized symmetry. Cells 2 and 4 represent the two forms of complementarity transactions, one-up and one-down. Previous investigators have conceptualized the control defining nature of a message as having only two directions, either one-up or one-down, and transactional units as being either symmetrical or complementary. The addition of a third direction, one-across, which is an attempt to sensitize the control measure, produces an additional type of symmetry and a third type of transactional exchange -- the transitory category. Cells 3, 6, 7 and 8 represent the four forms of trans- actional types we call transitory. Cells 3 and 6 respective- ly refer to movements from neutralized control toward one-up 93 and neutralized control toward one-down. Cells 7 and 8 respectively refer to movements from one-up to neutralized control from one-down to neutralized control. The three types of symmetry, the four types of transi— tion and the two types of complementarity offer a more com- plete set of relational patterns by which to describe on- going interaction than have previously been available. Analysis Levels Several levels of analysis are possible based on the coding procedure outlined. At the monadic level, it is pos- sible to obtain -- by actor and overall -- absolute and re- lative measures of length of talk, frequency of talk, type of grammatical form, and type of message response. From the marginal totals of the transactional code matrix given in Figure three, tendencies toward one-up, one-down and one-across message control can be accessed again by actor and overall. A combination of Cells 1, 2, 3 and 1, 4, 7 will give the frequency of each actor's use of one-up control. Summing across Cells 4, 5, 6 and 2, 5, 8 gives the frequency of each actor's use of one-down control. And Cells 3, 6, 9 and 7, 8, 9 give measures of each actor's movement towards neutralization of control. The overriding goal of this coding scheme, however, is to obtain a methodology for describing interactor patterns of communication exchange. This is accomplished by using a 94 two-message unit of analysis, combining the message control directions for the transactional code, and describing the transactional types that occur in an interaction. Moving to the dyadic level, a relatively simple inter- actor measure is available in the nonsequential tabulation of symmetry, complementarity, transitory transactional types and their subcategories, by actor and overall, for phases of the interaction or the total interaction. This gives an impor- tant description of the relational communication patterns of a two-person system, but a fairly static one. A more complex measure of the relational aspects of interaction is one that accounts for sequential patterning of transactional types over time. This method of describing interactor patterns moves the analysis further toward a pro- cess level, with an attempt to describe a series of trans- actions in terms of a configuration patterns. Series of interactions cumulate into ongoing relationships and to the extent that the data sample interaction typical of a dyad, generalizations about the total interpersonal relationship, can be suggested. The shift from individual message units to paired- message units is a significant change in analysis level. But the movement from paired messages to transaction series is even more of an analytical jump. It is a movement from indexing patterned regularities of the system, which is no small task, to indexing changing patterns of a system over time. 95 The conceptual tool bag of the social scientist is notably lacking in process terminology and methodology. For the most part, it has not gotten past the concepts of conflict, competition and cooperation. Symmetry and complementarity refine and expand the concepts available for describing and predicting interactional processes. The development of these concepts comes predominantly from the work of the family- oriented psycho-socioanalysts. But, because they deal most- ly with "sick" interaction, their point of view is somewhat tinted by stressing potential or actual communication patho- logies. For example, symmetry, as defined by this group, almost always refers to competitive (one-up, one-up) symme- try, which in our view is only one kind of symmetry.9 Other types of symmetrical interaction are rarely mentioned. Sym: metrical escalation referred to as "one-upmanship" which is a series of competitive symmetrical transactions, and rigid complementarity in which one interactor typically takes a "one-up" position and the other a "one-down" position, have been pointed out as the main pathological forms of inter- action. A mix of these patterns, however, is hypothesized to be a "healthy" interaction style.lo 9Watzlawick, et al., (1967), mention in one interpre- tation of an interaction sequence "that symmetry can be based on one-downness as well as other forms of competition" (p. 116), but these dimensions are not developed. lOWatzlawick, et al., (1967) p. 115. 96 The transactional types that can be formulated on the basis of the present relational coding scheme, continue to expand the range of concepts for describing interaction. These concepts can be used to describe transactional con- figurations, referring to a series of homogeneous transactions, or the overall interactional patterns based on the total sequential set of transactional configurations. For illustra- tion, a hypothetical interaction consisting of a series of ten transactional configurations is given in Figure 4. Most likely an interaction would consist of a mixture of several different types of configurations, but it might consist of a single configuration. The pure typological graphings of the three kinds of transactional configurations and their sub-types are given below in Figure 5. A total of nine transactional types are possible. These nine ideal transactional types include the two relational patterns (symmetry and complementarity) discussed by Watzlawick, et_§1., and although the terminology is dif- ferent, the six patterns outlined by Sluzki and Beavin (stable symmetry, stable complementarity, symmetrical com- petition towards one-up, symmetrical competition towards one- down, assymmetrical competition towards one-up and symmetry, assymmetrical competition towards one-down and symmetry) which are based on dyadic agreement-disagreement as to the control value of a message. Watzlawick, et_al., and Sluzki and Beavin suggest interactions be classified in terms of 0H m 97 h .mGOAumusmfiwcoo HMGOApUMmcmuu sou mo mamfimxm as .v musmflm m w A. 3 News. Em. .flmwn we......m.-flflm :-'i' -l . 1:11:31 + not». nséwmvfia sm 3 bmw. re mrm as $1.3 .m .4 mm ”AW... mm Nam Hm Hfl 98 Symmetry: + -++= 1+ :+—>_ + r -++= Complementarity: + -- / 1+: “* VVVVVV + ++= Transitory: +-+-+ _). + II +—+-+ .(_ 4' ll competitive symmetry — neutralized symmetry submissive symmetry complementarity one-up complementarity one-down one-up leveling leveling one-up one-down leveling leveling one-down Figure 5. Transactional configuration typology. 99 the predominance of one type of pattern. They also suggest a "fluid" category referring to the mixing of different types of pattern within the interaction. Certainly the identification of the predominant transaction form is a major way of labeling interations, but interactions can be described in a number of ways, de- pending on the guide of the research question. Using the transactional configuration typology, presented in Figure 5, interactions can be identified as suggested above, by the prevailing transactional type(s) utilized, either in terms of the longest occurring sequence(s) or the most often oc- curring type(s); by their rigidity or flexibility in terms of the number of configuration patterns used and the fluc- tuations or turn-over rate of these patterns; by the sequen- tial orderings of configuration patterns in terms of which patterns tend to follow or precede which other patterns. For illustration, the interaction given in Figure 4 is graphically presented below: A v 1111‘ *1/11---” 71/ 11 W This sample interaction utilized two forms of sym— metry, both forms of complementarity and two types of trans- itory transactions. There was a fairly equal mixing of com- petitive symmetry and complementarity followed by a run of 100 neutralized symmetry prefaced and closed with short transi- tory insertions. The symmetrical forms of interaction are separated by complementary forms. The interaction began with a run of competitive symmetry while it concluded in a rapid succession of short exchanges representing each of the transitory transactional types and terminated with a com- plementary exchange. In more general terms this interaction is characterized by a flexible mix of complementarity and symmetry oriented towards one-up. The analysis procedures are still quite unrefined, yet with the approach outlined an interaction can now be described in several important ways: (1) The number of con- figuration types observed in an interaction; (2) the rate of change or fluctuation from one type to another, during an interaction; the most persistant configuration pattern in terms of, (3) the longest consecutive run and (4) the type occurring most frequently; and (5) how likely it is that one pattern will occur in sequence with other patterns. These transactional descriptions allow a variety of interactive questions to be answered. Is there a "phrase" effect on transactional patterns? Do different configuration patterns emerge at different stages of the interaction? Do different patterns tend to occur with low or frequent fluc- tuation of transactional type, or with rapid versus lower rates of interchange? What sub-patterns are associated with different predominant configuration types? And importantly 101 these descriptions permit comparative analyses of interaction patterns across theoretically differentiated categories of dyads. In assessing the coding system, its limitations must also be noted. It utilizes a two-message unit of analysis which is limited to the verbal band of two-person interaction situations. It allows a sequential interpretation of mes- sages, that is, person A's first message with person B's first message, B's first message to A's second message, and A’s second message to 3'5 second message and so on, but it does not allow out-of—sequence interpretations of messages, for example A's third message with B's first message. In ad- dition, the present analysis rests on the assumptions that interpersonal messages do have a relational-defining char- acter, that this character can be conceptualized along a control dimension and that this control dimension can be operationalized. Further, it is assumed that relational patterns can be inferred from sampled, and in a sense, public interchanges between interactors. Summary The development of a transactional coding scheme com- bines several current, mutually reinforcing, lines of theo- retical concern. First, it focuses on the ongoing aspects of interpersonal interaction more than consequences, that is, it permits a fuller explication of the communication 102 aspects of relationships which are associated with various consequences. Second, it is concerned with the form, or structure, of interaction as distinct from a concern for referent. And third, it stresses the inter-relational as- pects of communication rather than individual behavior. A relational analysis of communication patterns involves at least a dyadic level of analysis and necessitates an emphas- is on process. The relational analysis presented in this paper is a continuation of Sluzki and Beavin's and Mark's efforts to develop a set of transactional concepts for describing basic communication patterns. The methodology set forth not only explicates, but expands the control-defining conception of message-exchange, and provides procedure for operationalizing this promising approach to communication. Coding Procedures and Reliability for the Discussion Data The first step in processing the tape recorded dis- cussion data was to transcribe it on to special coding forms. For all discussions, each message, which was defined as everything a speaker says until the next person speaks, was designated by speaker and typed out sequentially. This was done separately for each of the four topics. The typists were instructed to pay particular atten- tion to inflection patterns and to indicate interrogative 103 message forms. Also "talk-over" messages were to be indicated by the typists and silence that intially lasted for three seconds and any additional five second periods of silence. See Appendix VI for transcribing procedures. The principal investigators of the study checked all transcriptions with the recorded discussion for accuracy of content, successful and nonsuccessful talk-overs and silence. The interaction time for each discussion was also determined. Using the transcribed coding forms, three raters who were independent of the research project, coded each message according to the three digit coding system described in the previous section. Lengthy training sessions were held with the coders to explain the general outline of the research process, but the emphasis was upon the specific coding rules to be applied for the message codes. See Appendix VII for coding instructions. I After the first training session, the raters were given two topic discussions of one of the dyads interviewed for the study but not included because they did not have children under twelve years of age. The next training ses- sion consisted of comparing the message codes of the raters with the principal investigators and clarifying question and points of confusion. The raters were given another two discussion topics to code. At the next training session, the agreement resulting from code comparisons merited the beginning of the actual research coding. Three transcripts 104 were given to each coder which were the same across coders. These were used for a first stage coding evaluation. After coding was completed on the first set of trans- cripts, a check across coders was made.' Seven errors were detected on the coding of speaker (first digit code) out of the 1980 messages across all twelve topics. Some errors in coding talk-overs, which had previously been identified on the transcripts by parentheses or brackets, were also found and code disagreement between assertion and noncomplete mes- sages was evident. A final training session was held where these inconsistencies were discussed and recommendations were made for further minimizing error. The large number of hours involved in coding the thousands of messages of the dyad dis- cussions merited the extensive training procedures. The three transcripts involved in the first checking step were randomly reassigned to one of the three coders for recoding. The remaining transcripts were divided among the coders, including six which were randomly selected were common to each coder. These six transcripts were the base for the final coder reliability check. Using percentage of agreement, reliability figures were determined for three digit codes for 24 discussion topics across the six dyads. These figures are presented in Table 2. The pair comparison percentages range from .68 to 1.00. The average reliability across all 72 pair com- parisons is .86. 105 Comparisons were also made for the second and third digit codes, to determine the possible source of disagree- ment between coders. For the second digit codes, the average reliability across the 72 comparisons was .93; for the third digit codes, the average reliability .91. Most of the dis- agreement on the second digit code appeared to be whether a message was an assertion or a noncomplete. On the third digit code, the major disagreement was on whether a message was an extension or not. As pointed out previously, the message codes will be transformed into control codes. On second digit codes the control code for assertions and noncompletes where the coder disagreement was most frequent, only differ in one instance, on the initiate-terminate code. Thus when the third digit codes are the same, the reliability at the control level is higher than at the message level. Considering, however, just the three digit message level code, the inter-rater agreement is satisfactorily reliable. The message codes will form the base for the message control codes which will be sequentially combined to form the transactional codes. 106 . sea on noafl . mom om uvoH . «ma an umma . can «a 1mmH uxm mumoou . wed ms 1moa ms FF.EMm . am am 1mm mm ONIIIOAIN o\m mHmQOU . mes as "was . mom as noma . ema ms nova . mam ms uoom U\¢ mumpoo mma mayo . mva mo 1ooa ma mm.uwm omH Nfio'mm mm om.umm U\¢ muopou mum aaua mm. . mes mm noma . was Hm noes . «ma m» "ems . mom as 1mma m\< muopou . as mm 1mm ms mm.umm mm. HNH 100H . mm mm 1cm m\a mumvoo OHH vmo'Nm . mm mm “Nm mNH INHH oma Immd mm. vm. U\m muovou m w H Ot\ N v-w b m H m C1m P~ N mm. H H m h 0 H 1 mm.l N V H U\« muopou Ohm echo NHH mm le . mm 0m "Vm omH I¢HH mmH Iowa mm. mm. U\¢ muwooo 000 deo mv 8,104.1 MN FmoHON wh hm.flvb .1bhH om 1voa m\¢ mumwou . OHH vm "MNI HO mm.flwm om.INWH IvHH hm. mmH I¢OH m\< WHQGOU mamswm mommoa vm mmouom >usaaQMMHom momuo>< . mad em "mod . pom mm noes . mos mm "was . mma mm 1¢oa U\m muowou m. mm m 1cm me oo.Humw om mm.1ms mm. mud 1qHH U\m muwvoo oo.HuM1 o m m 0 H m H 0L” N Hrfi mm. H m «in Q 0‘ m H F4 F N am. w m H H U\¢ mumvou mvo DH3 Had .mo mamovm meEMm UMMC onu msnu can osmnmsn on» .sowpmmsv mflsu ou psommmH-www1mww1mwsmnmss osen .manmu o>onm may mo esopxmmun sEsHoo wmw3 on» on umafleflm on Haas m.s Hamo on» can mm ma c Hmuou on“ soaaom pan» mmanmu osu mo muwuonms on» cH .mHQMB was» as so>wm on Mano Haas Hamo comm How musopsommmn mo Hones: ones No.m vm.m mo.m om.m mm.m NN.© mm.m om.m on.m mo.m mm.m mm.w Hmuoe mmuz mmnz monz mmnz mmuz mmnz manz manz omnz mmnz mmnz mmuz mm.m «m.m mb.m mm.m 5H.o om.m vu.m mn.m om.m mo.m om.m om.m msoq Hmuz Hmnz mmuz manz manz mauz on an oauz oauz oanz oan HH.m «m.m mm.o mm.m ww.m 000.5 mm.m mm.m om.m oo.m mw.m na.m . unocm Nmuz Nmuz mmuz HHuz Hauz Hanz muz Qmuz oauz Nauz NHuz omauz coho .msm muag coma .msm wwwz mama .msm OMHS puma .msm OMHB Hmuoa swam EsHUmz 30A muoumflm mosmmmnomflm maom .wmpxmmz o co omsomm spas oEHu Hmuou mo moumfiflumo msoom coo: .m magma 120 mm.mve mo Hmuoe mo.s mmdmmw mm gasps: .m.a em.s mm.ms mm.m~ m cosnomumucH .m.c em. om.a om.a H snoumflm .m.c ms.a H~.m me.oa m scammmuomfla maom ammo ms.msa mm Hmuoe mm.a mm.voa Mm canvas .m.: ms. mm.H mo.~ N nosuomnmucH .m.: oo. oo. oo. a whoumwm .m.s ~m.m m~.v om.m m hosmmmnomfla oaom pcmnmsm ma.mms so sauce mo.m mmqmmm mm gasps: .m.c sm.s ma.¢ o~.m m cofluomnmch .m.c em.s mm.q ma.v H snoumflm .m.c mm. mm.a ms.m m socmamuomfia msom muss mmwmq mgsm> m .MMMMm mwmmmwM an mo.....> so mouse. .mwwmwmm 121 Table 4. Mean score estimates of total time with spouse on a Sunday. Role Discrepancy History Low Medium High Total Wife Hus. Dyad Wife Hus. Dyad Wife Hus. Dyad Wife Hus. Dyad Short 7.67 7.92 7.79 7.20 7.30 7.25 7.45 7.45 7.46 7.45 7.58 7.52 Long 7.70 7.30 7.50 6.90 6.90 6.90 6.33 5.58 5.96 6.94 6.53 6.74 Total 7.68 7.64 7.66 7.05 7.10 7.08 6.87 6.48 6.68 7.20 7.06 7.13 Note: Higher score indicates higher time allocations. Response Source of Variance DF Sum Of Mean F Ratio 819' Unit Squares Square Level Wife Role Discrepancy 2 8.07 4.04 2.23 n.s. History 1 4.35 4.35 2.40 n.s. Interaction 2 3.31 1.66 .92 n.s. Within 52_ 106.67 1.81 Total 64 122.40 Husband Role Discrepancy 2 15.12 7.56 3.50 .05 History 1 17.72 17.72 8.20 .01 Interaction 2 5.24 2.62 1.21 n.s. Within 52_ 127.67 2.16 Total 64 165.75 Dyad Role Discrepancy 2 44.01 22.01 3.41 .05 History 1 39.62 39.62 6.41 .05 Interaction 2 16.08 8.04 1.25 n.s. Within 52_ 380.84 6.45 Total 64 480.55 122 more time together than long history dyads (married from twelve to 26 years) and second, there is a tendency for §h2£E_history—high discrepancy dyads to give the highest time-together estimates while 123g_history—high discrepant dyads spend notably less time together. These tendencies will be discussed in the next section when Table 5 is presented. Table 4 presents the mean estimates of total time spent with spouse on a Sunday. As expected, the time allocations for Sunday, a nonwork day, are higher than those given for a weekday. Overall, couples report spend- ing about six hours together on a Sunday-~two hours more than on a weekday. In Table 4 the history differences mentioned above become pronounced. Short history dyads report spending significantly more time together on a Sunday than long history dyads. The overall difference is about an hour and a half. Also, there is a distinct pattern of time estimates by discrepancy categories. The low discrepant (low system strain) dyads report spending the most time together and the high discrepant (high strain) dayds the least time together. This relationship is more prominent for the dyads with a longer length of marriage (long history). The history and discrepancy effects approach significance for wife responses and are significant at the 123 .05 level (or above) for husband and dyad responses. The short history—low discrepancy dyads estimate the highest amount of time together on Sunday, a day when time alloca- tions are less affected by work demands and assumed to be more a personal choice, and the long history-high discrep- ant dyads estimate the least amount of time together. It appears that on a weekday, with heavier demands on available time, role discrepancy levels within dyads do not affect time allocations, but on a Sunday, those dyads who perceive the system operating more in accord with their expectations (low discrepant dyads) spend more time in the dyad than those who perceive inequities in the system (high discrepant dyads). Time Spent with Spouse Alone Estimates of time spent alone with spouse are pre- sented in Table 5. The overall mean scores of husbands and wives are very similar, both indicating an average of one hour spent alone with spouse on a weekday. For wife, husband and dyad responses the history effect is statistically significant. (This tendency was observed in Table 3, but not at a significant level.) In each case, the dyad members with a shorter history (length of marriage) spend more time alone with their spouse than dyad members with a longer history. This is most pronounced for high discrepant dyads. On the average (across all 124 Table 5. Mean estimates of time spent with spouse alone on a weekday. Role Discrepancy History Low Medium High Total Wife Hus. Dyad Wife Hus. Dyad Wife Hus. Dyad Wife Hus. Dyad Short 4.55 4.25 4.41 4.30 4.33 4.39 5.13 5.09 5.13 4.85 4.56 4.65 Long 3.50 3.50 3.50 3.00 3.89 3.56 3.17 3.08 3.12 3.22 3.45 3.37 Total 4.09 3.91 4.00 3.90 4.11 3.97 4.13 4.04 4.08 4.05 4.02 4.03 Note: Higher score indicates more time. Regfiizse Source of Variance DF :gzagzs 8:3:26 F Ratio ingi Wife Role Discrepancy 2 ".63 .32 .11 n.s. History 1 43.15 43.15 14.63 .001 Interaction 2 2.76 1.38 .47 n.s. Within 52_ 174.32 2.95 Total 64 220.86 Husband Role Discrepancy 2 .43 .22 .09 n.s. History 1 19.43 19.43 8.30 .01 Interaction 2 7.66 3.83 1.64 n.s. Within §1_ 133.46 2.34 Total 62 160.98 Dyad Role Diecrepancy 2 .61 .30 .04 n.s. Hietory 1 104.05 104.05 13.71 .001 Interaction 2 19.65 9.83 1.30 n.e. Within §Z_ 432.55 7.59 Total 62 556.86 125 categories), couples married a shorter period of time spend around an hour and a half alone, while couples married longer spend less than an hour (about 40 minutes) alone with one another. There are no significant role discrepancy differ— ences or interaction effects. However, one of the more intriguing aspects of the interspousal time estimates is observed when length of marriage is taken into account. This tendency noted in Table 3, is more clearly observed here in Table 5. The short history-high discrepant dyads gave the highest estimate of time spent alone together, while the long history-high discrepant dyads gave the lowest estimates of time alone. Husband and wife pairs who have high role discrep- ancy but a shorter length of marriage report that they spend approximately two hours in the dyadic relationship, while couples at the same discrepancy level with a longer history report they spend about 30 minutes together by themselves. One of the major reasons for controlling on history in this study, was the notion that the effects of inequity, as measured by perceived role discrepancy, might operate differently depending on the length of history of the. dyad. It was felt that when higher levels of inequity were present in more recently formed dyads (in relative terms), the members would be more likely to "confront" one 126 another in attempts to modify the system and redress the perceived inequities, while in dyads with high inequity that had existed for a much longer period of time, the members would be more likely to "withdraw" and avoid con— tinued failure attempts to modify the system. The above data do not give support of this explanation, but they do give suggestive evidence for this interpretation. Time Spent with Children Alone The respondents were asked to estimate the amount of time they spent just with one or more of their children on a weekday. These data, presented in Table 6, were obtained to further our descriptive knowledge of family time allocations. As expected, wives uniformly report that they spend large amounts of time alone with their children during a weekday. The typical estimate is about five hours. Note that total time with children (with others present) would be more. Wives who have been married fewer years (short history) report spending slightly more time alone with children than wives who have been married longer. All the dyads in the study are in the childbear— ing and childrearing family life stage, but the couples married for a shorter period, having younger children, are probably in the higher time-demand period of child care, as suggested in the data. 127 Table 6. Mean estimates of time spent with children alone on a weekday. Role Discrepancy History Low Medium 7 High Total Wife Hus. Wife Hus. Wife Hus. Wife Hus. Short 6.58 3.73 7.10 2.89 6.36 3.91 6.67 3.55 Long 6.20 3.80 5.50 5.33 6.50 3.42 6.09 4.10 Total 6.41 3.76 6.30 4.11 6.43 3.65 6.38 3.82 Note: Higher score indicates more time. Regfiitse Source of Variance DF :Sfla::s szizges F Ratio ::351 Wife Role Discrepancy 2 .21 .ll .02 n.s. History 1 5.33 5.33 1.12 n.s. Interaction 2 8.38 4.19 .88 I n.s. Within 52_ 279.46 Total 64 293.38 Husband Role Discrepancy 2 2.24 1.12 .40 n.s. History 1 4.66 4.66 1.67 n.s. Interaction 2 23.65 11.83 4.24 .05 Within 56. 156.50 2.79 Total 61 187.05 128 The husbands report considerably less time spent alone with their children than wives. The average esti- mate was about an hour. Once again, recall that this is time spent with children only, so that the total time with children is higher. An interesting pattern is observed in the medium discrepancy-—short and long history cells of Table 6. In the medium discrepancy-short history cell, wife responses are the highest estimates given of time with children (over six hours) and the husband responses are the lowest (about 30 minutes). In the medium discrepancy-lgnq_his- tory category, the highest husband response (slightly less than three hours) and the lowest wife response (three hours) are given. It appears that there is a fairly inflexible amount of time that is needed to be spent with children1 and when one partner spends less time, the other spends more time. The similarity of times spent alone with children reported in the long history-medium discrepancy by wives and husbands is probably affected by the fact that four of the eight working wives in the sample fell in that cell. The other four working wives are distributed among three other categories. 1The combined responses of husband and wife time spent with children alone is practically the same amount of time (six hours) across all cells. 129 For additional descriptive purposes, a "family" time allocation estimate is available by combining the overall average of total time spent with spouse, given in Table 3, with the overall average time spent with at least one of one's children, given in Table 6. The mean estimated time that wives spend in this combination is approximately nine hours per weekday and the average for husbands is about five hours per weekday. Time Spent Outside the Family The reported times that husbands and wives spend with people other than spouse and children are given in Table 7. The most obvious and nonsurprising result is that husbands spend more time with nonfamily members than do wives. Wives reported spending on the average about an hour per weekday with people other than the immediate family; husbands spend approximately three hours with other people. Length of marriage (history) was found to be a significant factor affecting the amount of time wives spent with others but not for husbands. Wives of short history dyads spent less time with nonfamily members than wives of long history dyads. This again is probably due to the higher time demand of having younger children. Wives of short history-medium discrepant dyads were found to report the lowest amount of time with people outside 130 Table 7. Mean estimates of time spent outside the family on a weekday. Role Discrepancy History Low Medium High Total Wife Hus. Wife Hus. Wife Hus. Wife Hus. Short 3.58 5.25 2.40 6.44 4.30 5.82 3.44 5.78 Long 5.30 4.90 4.90 5.11 4.08 6.25 4.72 5.48 Total 4.36 5.09 3.65 5.781 4.18 6.04 4.08 5.63 Note: Higher score indicates more time. Response Source of Variance DF Sum Of Mean F Ratio 819' Unit Squares Square Level Wife Role Discrepancy 2 5.70 2.85 .68 n.s. History 1 26.27 26.27 6.25 .05 Interaction 2 21.31 10.66 2.54 n.s. Within 58_ 243.33 4.20 Total 63 296.61 Husband Role Discrepancy 2 10.72 5.36 .78 n.s. History 1 1.39 1.39 .20 n.s. Interaction 2 8.35 4.18 .60 n.s. Within 51_ 394.14 6.91 Total 62 414.60 131 the family. They estimate less than 30 minutes. This corresponds to their responses being the highest (over six hours) for time spent just with children, given in Table 6. It is interesting to note the trend in the husbands' estimates of time spent outside the family. There are no significant difference by discrepancy levels, but husbands of low discrepancy dyads report the least amount of time spent outside the family unit while husbands of high dis- crepancy report the largest amount of time spent outside the family. This is particularly true of long history dyads. This lends slight, but further evidence to the suggested avoidance technique of dealing with higher inequity, in longer established dayds. This trend is not observed in the wife responses, since spending time out- side the family unit is a more probable alternative for husbands than for wives. Talk-Time with Spouse Along with time allocations, dyadic talkftime allocations were also obtained. Via the self-report instrument, dyad members estimated how much of the total time they were together with their spouse, was spent in conversation. Estimates were given for both a weekday and a Sunday (see Question 2a and 4b of the questionnaire in Appendix II). The mean values of these time estimates are reported in Table 8 and Table 9, respectively. 132 Table 8. Mean estimates of talkwtime with spouse on a weekday. Role Discrepancy History Low Medium High Total Y'v Wife Hus. Dyad Wife Hus. Dyad Wife Hus. Dyad Wife Hus. Dyad Short 4.58 4.33 4.46 4.44 4.10 4.28 4.00 4.00 4.00 4.34 4.15 4.25 Long 3.70 3.60 3.65 4.10 4.10 4.10 3.17 3.42 3.29 3.63 3.69 3.65 Total 4.18 4.00 4.09 4.26 4.10 4.18 3.57 3.70 3.43 3.98 3.92 3.95 Note: Higher score indicates more time. Regiizse Source of Variance DF 2333::8 823226 F Ratio iigél Wife Role Discrepancy 2 6.38 3.19 2.38 n.s. History 1 8.27 8.27 6.17 .05 Interaction 2 .53 .27 .20 n.s. Within §§_ 77.80 1.34 Total 63 92.98 Husband Role Discrepancy 2 1.95 .97 .50 n.s. History 1 3.50 3.50 1.81 n.s. Interaction 2 1.38 .69 .36 n.s. Within 52_ 113.79 1.93 Total 64 120.62 Dyad Role Discrepancy 2 15.31 7.65 1.67 n.s. History 1 22.56 22.56 4.93 .05 Interaction 2 3.81 1.91 .42 n.s. Within §§_ 265.76 Total 63 133 Table 9. Mean estimates of talk-time with spouse on a Sunday. Role Discrepancy History Low Medium High Total Wife Hus. Dyad Wife Hus. Dyad Wife Hus. Dyad Wife Hus. Dyad Short 5.58 5.92 5.75 5.50 5.30 5.40 4.91 5.55 5.22 5.33 5.61 5.47 Long 5.30 5.00 5.15 5.70 5.10 5.40 4.00 4.75 4.37 4.94 4.94 4.94 Total 5.45 5.50 5.47 5.60 5.20 5.40 4.43 5.13 4.78 5.14 5.28 5.21 Note: Higher score indicates more time. Response Source of Variance DF Sum Of Mean F Ratio 319' Unit Squares Square Level Wife Role Discrepancy 2 17.85 8.92 4.17 .05 History 1 2.55 2.55 1.19 n.s. Interaction 2 2.83 1.42 .66 n.s. Within 52_ 126.52 2.14 Total 64 149.75 Husband Role Discrepancy 2 1.71 .85 .28 n.s. History 1 7.26 7.26 2.42 n.s. Interaction 2 1.15 .58 .19 n.s. Within 52_ 176.90 3.00 Total 64 187.02 Dyad Role Discrepancy 2 25 . 98 12 . 99 1 . 77 n . s . History 1 18.41 18.41 2.51 n.s. Interaction 2 6.12 3.06 .42 n.s Within 52_ 433.27 7.34 Total 64 483.78 134 Although the time estimate categories are fairly general, it is again reassuring, as it was with the inter- dyadic time allocations, to find the average talk-time estimates given by husbands and wives to be quite similar. The overall mean for talk—time on a weekday indicates approximately one hour of the average four hours (see Table 3) of total time couples spend together, is spent talking to one another. For a Sunday, the average esti— mated talk-time is approximately two hours out of the total average of six hours (see Table 4). In accordance with previous time allocation esti- mates, Table 8 and 9 both show that short history dyads spend more time in conversation than long history dyads. The history effect is statistically significant for wife and dyad responses for a weekday, and show strong, but not statistically significant tendencies for husband responses and for estimates of talk-time on a Sunday. Analyzing talk—time in terms of discrepancy levels, it is seen that the data in both Table 8 and 9 show a tendency for low discrepant dyads to spend more time talk- ing than high discrepant dyads. This is particularly true for wife responses, which are significant at the .05 level for Sunday and approach significance for weekday responses. Analysis of variance of the talk-time data show no significant interaction effects, but once again, the previously mentioned pattern concerning the mix of history 135 and discrepancy levels is readily observable. The short history-low discrepancy dyad members with one exception (wife responses for Sunday give the second highest (instead of the highest talk-time mean) report the highest amount of talk—time of all cell combinations and the long history-high discrepant dyad members report the lowest amount of talk-time. An emerging, although not signifi— cant, tendency is for this polar history and discrepancy combination to effect both the time dyad members spent together and the time spent talking together. Topic Discussion Frequency Husband and wife respondents were asked to check how frequently they talked about ten different topics. Almost all respondents said they talked about each of the topics, which varied from interpersonal, to financial, to national events, but with different levels of frequency. Mean scores representing the frequency of discussion across the ten topics are presented in Table 10. On each topic a socre from 1 to 4 was possible, with 1 represent— ing a frequency of once a day or more and 4 indicating a frequency of less than once a month (see Question 7 of the questionnaire in Appendix II for more detail). The total score had a possible range from 10 to 40. A lower score indicates more frequent discussions. 136 .o0flmwsomwo usoswoum ones moumoflosw onoom uozoq "ouoz mm.mH vm.mH mm.ma mm.om mm.ma mo.am hv.ma mm.ma sh.ma mh.oH om.oH mm.oH Hmuoe oa.ma mo.ma ~¢.ma om.om mo.am mv.am mm.ma mv.om oo.om oo.na ov.ha oo.ma mcoq mo.>a 0H.SH hm.mH mo.oa om.ma om.om mm.ha on.na mm.ha om.oa m>.ma mm.ba uHOSm coho .msm ova: coma .msm. -VMAS. coma. .msm muss moan .msm mmaS Hmuoa swam Eswooz 30a Snowman . mosmmouomflo oaom .mofimou so» mmonom wososvoum scammsomflo sow: .oa magma 137 mm.mHv~ mm Hmuoa oo.mm ms.~msH .mm aHauHs .m.c mo. Hm.H H6.~ m aoHuomuoucH .m.a .ms.m sm.mMH so.mmH H snoumHm Ho. Ho.s so.omm mH.oom m scammoHomHo mHom ammo >F.Hmm om Hmuoa Hm . NH % Mm :2qu .m.: 00. oo. co. m coHuomumuaH Ho. em.a oo.mm oo.mm H snoumHm mo. om.v om.mo mm.omH m scammmuomHo 0H0m ocmnmsm hm.mmHH om H0909 mm.sH 06.nom .mm :HauHs .m.c om. am.m ms.mH N :oHuomumuaH .m.q mH.H m~.o~ e~.o~ H snoumHm Ho. Ho.m vo.mm mo.osH m scammmuomHo 0Hom 0HH3 «MMMH ofiumm m ommwmm mMMMHWM mo oosmHHm> mo oousom omwwmmom 138 An examination of Table 10 shows that the frequency of discussion is significantly different by role discrep- ancy categories. For both husband and wife, and dyad responses there is a continuous decrease in the frequency of discussion from low to medium to high role discrepancy dyads. Role discrepancy is seen to have a negative effect on these indicants of dyadic communication frequency. History is a significant factor variable only for husband responses although the direction and differences of wife and dyad scores are consistent with the husband response pattern. Short history dyads are shown to talk more frequently about the ten topics, than long history dyads. Again, it is interesting to observe that it is the long history—high discrepancy dyads that report the least frequent discussions. Mean frequency score breakdowns were also made on wife, husband and dyad responses by role discrepancy and history for each of the ten topics. A summary of the results of analysis of variance tests on this data is presented in Table 11. In all cases of significant role discrepancy dif- ferences in Table 11, the mean frequency scores indicate that the low discrepant dyads had the most frequent dis- cussions and the high discrepant dyads had the least frequent discussions. With all significant differences 139 .uGMOHMHsmHm ouo3 mosHm>1m coauomnousfl onu mo osozm 111 mo. H0. 111 mo. 111 111 mo. 111 H0. 111 Ho. 11. mo. mo. 111 mo. 111 111 mo. Ho. Ho. Ho. mo. muco>o Hmsofiumsnousfilamcoflumz .OH muso>m Hmooq .m mo>wumamm .m mosoflnm .n moosmsfim .m meow .m 08H» onsmfloq .v soHoHHnO .m Honuosm oco oumzou msflaoom .m non .H huoumwm >osmmouomwo muoumwm hosomouomflo known“: monomouomwo omso census: L Omaz canoe M .oflmou ma mouoom hososwonm GOHmmsomwp some mo mHo>oH oosmoflmasmflm .HH magma 140 by history, the short history dyads reported more frequent discussions than long history dyads. The topics that particularly distinguish the low discrepant from the high discrepant dyads are talk about job and talk about feelings toward one another. With both topics, low discrepant dyads report high frequency of dis- cussions, while high discrepant dyads report much less frequent discussions of these topics. The discrepancy differences on these two topics, particularly the less frequent talk of high discrepant couples about their feel— ings, is an important finding. It underscores a predominant theme of family therapy that those couples most in need of talking about their interpersonal relationship seem to be least able, or least likely, to talk about their feelings toward one another. The frequency of discussing financial matters was the most notable difference between short and long history dyads. Short history dyads report a high frequency of taling about finances while the long history dyads report significantly less talk about this topic. Of the ten topics, the subject of children was discussed with the highest frequency across all respondents. National and international events were discussed with the least frequency across all categories. 141 Communication Control The respondents were asked four questions concern- ing control aspects of conversation; who initiates and ends conversations between spouses, whose interests are talked about and who does more of the talking (see Question 8, item a through item d, in Appendix II). In all four cases the overriding response was one indicating an approximately equal sharing of "control." See Table 12 for the item response frequencies and total frequency percentages. The responses were tabulated by role discrepancy categories to see if there were any differences between the low, medium and high discrepancy groups. A chi square value was calculated for each item. These figures are also given in Table 12. None of the chi square values are above the .05 level of significance. Only in item c, concerning whose interests are talked about, is there a trend for the dis- crepancy categories to differ from the expected frequencies. The chi square value, 8.38, is significant at the .10 level. The respondents of high role discrepant dyads said that they talked about what their spouse wanted to talk about, or what they themselves wanted to talk about more frequently than the medium or low discrepant dyad members. The respondents of low discrepant dyads reported most fre- quently that they talked about both of their interests equally. 142 Table 12. Response frequencies and total frequency percentages for self-report communication control by item. Role Discrepancy Frzofizit Item Total Pergent- Low Medium High ages 3;. Who starts conversation? 1. My spouse more 6 3 10 19 15 2. I do more 7 8 12 27 22 3. Each of us about same 39_ 22_ 39_ 79 63 43 40 42 125 100 x2 = 6.46 n.s. b. How much do you talk in comparison to spouse? 1. Much more 2 l 5 8 O6 2. Somewhat more 9 9 7 25 19 3. About the same 25 25 26 76 58 4. Somewhat less _8_ _§_ _§_ 21 16 44 4O 46 130 100 x2 = 4.10 n.s. c. Whose interests talk about? 1. Mainly my spouse's 2 4 7 13 10 2. Mainly mine 1 2 6 9 07 3. Each about evenly 41_ 34. 33. 107 83 44 4O 45 129 100 x2 = 8.38 n.s. d. Who ends conversation? 1. My spouse 1 2 l 4 03 2. I do 3 2 5 10 07 3. Each about same 28 26 30 84 65 4. Outside interruptions 12_ 19_ 19_ 32 25 44 4O 46 130 100 x = 2.12 n.s. 143 Desired Talk-Time After dyad members estimated how much time they spent talking to one another they were asked to indicate on a five-point scale how much time they would have liked to have spent talking. A score of 1 indicates "much more" and a score of 5 indicates "much less" (see Questions 2b and 6 of the questionnaire in Appendix II for more detail). Tables 13 and 14 present the mean scores of desired talk- time on a weekday and a Sunday, respectively. A lower score represents a higher level of desire for talk-time. A general prediction made earlier2 was that both dyad members would prefer more rather than less talk-time. Looking at Tables 13 and 14 we see that all mean values are above a score of three, which is the neutral point between wanting more and wanting less talk—time.. Also, as suggested before, the mean values do indicate a higher desire for talk—time on a weekday, with assumed higher external time demands on the dyad members, than on a Sun- day. A t-test between the mean weekday and Sunday scores, however, is not significant (t-value = 1.09 with 129 degrees of freedom). The first formally stated hypothesis of this study, Hypothesis 1, predicted that wives would have significantly 2See Chapter IV, p.110, for hypotheses concerning desired talk-time and their rationale. 144 Table 13. Mean scores of desired talkvtime on a weekday. Role Discrepancy History Low Medium High Total Wife Hus. Dyad Wife Hus. Dyad Wife Hus. Dyad Wife Hus. Dyad Short 1.58 2.00 1.79 1.90 1.90 1.90 1.64 2.18 1.91 1.70 2.03 1.86 Long 2.10 2.20 2.15 2.40 2.00 2.20 1.75 1.67 1.71 2.06 1.94 2.00 Total 1.82 2.09 1.95 2.15 1.95 2.05 1.70 1.91 1.81 1.88 1.98 1.93 Note: Lower score indicates more time desired. Response . Sum of Mean . Sig. Unit Source of Variance DF Squares Square F Ratio Level Wife Role Discrepancy 2 2.32 1.16 1.81 n.s. History 1 2.17 2.17 3.39 n.s. Interaction 2 .61 .30 .47 n.s. Within 52_ 37.92 .64 Total 64 43.02 Husband Role Discrepancy 2 .39 .20 .36 n.s. History 1 .14 .14 .25 n.s. Interaction 2 1.65 .83 1.48 n.s. Within 52_ 32.80 Total 64 34.98 Dyad Role Discrepancy 2 2.66 1.33 .92 n.s. History 1 1.21 1.21 .83 n.s. Interaction 2 4.31 2.16 1.49 n.s. Within 52_ 85.57 1.45 Total 64 93.75 145 Table 14. Mean scores of desired talk-time on a Sunday. Role Discrepancy History Low Medium High Total Wife Hus. Dyad Wife Hus. Dyad Wife Hus. Dyad Wife Hus. Dyad Short 2.50 2.75 2.62 1.90 2.30 2.10 2.18 2.36 2.27 2.21 2.48 2.35 Long 2.60 2.70 2.65 2.70 2.60 2.65 1.83 2.25 2.04 2.34 2.50 2.42 Total 2.55 2.73 2.64 2.30 2.45 2.38 2.00 2.30 2.15 2.28 2.49 2.38 Note: Lower score indicates more time desired. Response Source of Variance DF Sum Of Mean F Ratio 319' Unit Squares Square Level Wife Role Discrepancy 2 3.36 1.68 2.63 n.s. History 1 .28 .28 .44 n.s. Interaction 2 3.67 1.84 2.88 n.s. Within 52_ 37.71 .64 Total 64 45.02 Husband Role Discrepancy 2 2.06 1.03 1.81 n.s. History 1 .00 .00 .00 n.s. Interaction 2 .54 .27 .47 n.s. Within 52_ 33.65 .57 Total 64 36.25 Dyad Role Discrepancy 2 10.56 5.28 3.14 .05 History 1 .35 .35 .20 n.s. Interaction 2 6.93 3.47 2.05 n.s. Within 52_ 99.70 1.68 Total 64 117.54 146 higher talk-time desired scores than husbands because of higher communication deprivation. The mean scores are in the predicted direction for both weekday and Sunday responses, but the differences are not significant (see Table 15). Hypothesis 2 stated that short history dyads would desire more talk-time than long history dyads. Once again, the mean differences are in the predicted direction, but are not statistically significant (see Tables 13 and 14). The third hypothesis concerning desired talk-time stated that higher role discrepant dyad members would desire more talk-time than lower role discrepant dyads. This prediction does not hold for weekday responses, but does receive statistically significant support on dyad responses for Sunday, a nonwork day. Again, see Tables 13 and 14. For Sunday, low discrepant dyads expressed the lowest need to talk-more (mean score = 2.64), the medium discrepant group express the next higher level of need to talk (mean = 2.38) and the high discrepant dyads indicated the highest desire to talk more (mean = 2.15). This cor- responds with the previously noted differences (see Tables 8 and 9) that lower discrepant dyads report the higher levels of interdyadic talking and high discrepant dyads the least amount of actual talk—time. It appears that the strains of higher role discrepancy operate to increase the desired talk-time, but to inhibit the actual talk-time. 147 Table 15. Mean desired talk—time for a weekday and a Sunday by wife-husband. Weekday Sunday gig: 1.88 2.28 Egzgand E;3§_ 3;:3 32:33 1.93 2.38 t = .79 n.s. t = 1.54 n.s. Communication Satisfaction Two measures of dyadic communication satisfaction scores were obtained; one is a single item global evalua- tion, the other is a composite index of fifteen fairly specific interpersonal communication items. In both cases a lower score is indicative of higher levels of communica- tion satisfaction (see Questions 9 and 10, respectively, of the questionnaire in Appendix II). Table 16 presents the more general mean evaluation scores. An examination of this table reveals that there is little expression of dissatisfaction. How much of this is a "halo" effect is not known, but it is assumed some is present. In-depth family studies frequently conclude that there is a tendency to present to others a "public" front of system unity and satisfaction. Even when assuming that 148 Table 16. Mean scores on general communication satisfaction measure. Role Discrepancy History Low Medium High Total Wife Hus. Dyad Wife Hus. Dyad Wife Hus. Dyad Wife Hus. Dyad Short 1.42 1.58 1.50 1.90 1.40 1.65 2.45 1.64 2.04 1.91 1.55 1.72 Long 1.80 1.60 1.70 1.50 2.60 2.05 2.42 1.75 2.08 1.94 1.97 1.95 Total 1.59 1.59 1.59 1.70 2.00 1.85 2.43 1.70 2.06 1.92 1.75 1.84 Note: Lower score indicates higher satisfaction. Response Source of Variance DF Sum Of Mean F Ratio 819' Unit Squares Square Level Wife Role Discrepancy 2 9.45 4.72 4.25 .05 History 1 .01 .01 .01 n.s. Interaction 2 1.59 .80 .72 n.s. Within 52_ 65.57 1.11 Total 64 76.62 Husband Role Discrepancy 2 1.87 .94 1.49 n.s. History 1 2.91 2.91 4.62 .05 Interaction 2 4.37 2.19 3.48 .05 Within 52_ 36.91 .63 Total 64 46.06 Dyad Role Discrepancy 2 10.13 5.07 2.17 n.s. History 1 3.31 3.31 1.41 n.s. Interaction 2 .80 .40 .17 n.s. Within 52_ 137.98 2.34 Total 64 152.22 149 such an effect is present, the communication satisfaction Amean scores, nevertheless, do differ across discrepancy categories. Hypothesis 4 states that lower discrepant dyad members will express more satisfaction with their inter- communication than higher discrepant dyad members. This is supported at the .05 level of significance for the wife responses (see Table 16). For husband responses, we dis— cover that there is a significant (.05 level) history and interaction effect. Husbands married a shorter time express more satisfaction than husbands married a longer period of time. The combination of medium discrepancy with short history results in the highest satisfaction score across all categories and medium discrepancy with long history results in the lowest satisfaction score. The eXplanation of this is not clear, however, remembering that four of the ten wives of long history-medium discrep— ancy category work outside the home may have some bearing on those husbands having lower satisfaction scores. The same category of husbands express the lowest mean satisfac- tion score on the composite scale also. Turning to the composite communication satisfaction mean scores given in Table 17, we find that Hypothesis 4 is strongly supported. The mean differences for wife, husband and dyad responses across discrepancy categories are all significant at the .05 level or above, and all but the .sofluommmaumm Hosmws moumowosw whoom Hw3oq “ouoz 150 no.mm mm.mm ov.m~ om.Hm om.om mm.mm on.om oo.mm ov.m~ mo.>m mm.nm om.mm Hmuoa no.0m .oo.Hm cm.om Hh.mm Nv.am oo.vm om.Hm ov.mm om.mm om.nm 0H.mm 0H.hm mcoq mw.mm mh.mN H®.mN 5h.mm oo.mm mm.om OH.0m om.om oo.mN vm.mm mo.hm oo.©N uuonm pawn .msm muss ooze .msm oma3 coho .msm emaz coma .msm oma3 Hmuoa swam 1 abacus 30H muoumflm homomonomflo mHom 11 1 1 .oamom cowuommmHUMm coaumowssssoo ouwmogsooHsvmoHoom new: .nH mHQme 151 mm.~m~m we Hmuos Hm.6m an.Hmom .mm :Haqu .m.c _ oo. oo. co. m :oHuomumuaH .m.c em.m o¢.mm~ ov.mmm H snoumHm Ho. om.m mo.msv «n.svm N scammmuomHa mHom ammo mo.H~mH 66 Hence mm.om mm.m-H .mm aHaqu .m.a oo. oo. co. m coHuomumuaH .m.c mm.m om.ma om.ma H suoumHm Ho. NH.m om.60H MH.mH~ m socmmmuumHo mHom wamnmsm ma.momm co Hmuos mm.sv Ne.aomm .mm cHaqu .m.c mm. ~¢.MH mm.om m aoHuomumuaH .m.: mo.H oo.me oo.me H anoumHm mo. «0.4 mm.~mH 46.4mm m scammmuomHo 0Hom 00H; MMMMA OHUMM rm OHM—Mm wwwwmwm .mQ OUGMHHM> m0 WOHHHOW wmwwmmwm 152 medium discrepancy husband scores are in the predicted direction. Low discrepant dyad members are found to be significantly more satisfied with their communication relationship than high discrepant dyad members.3 In Table 17, we again observe the tendency for the polar combinations of short history—low discrepancy to have the higher communication satisfaction level across all cells and the long history-high discrepancy combina- tion to have the lower level of communication satisfaction, with the one exception mentioned above, the long history- medium discrepancy husband score. A factor analysis was carried out on the fifteen item communication scale. The major factor resulting from this analysis includes items b, g, h, m, n, o of Question 10. These items deal in general, with the ability to express feelings to one another and account for 27 per- cent of the variance. An analysis of variance on this sub-set of scores by discrepancy and history levels, pro- duce results similar to that of the total set of communica- tion satisfaction items, but at a lower level of significance. 3 . . . A comparison of Wife and husband mean communica- tion satisfaction scores on both the general and composite index were not found to be significantly different. T- values were .98 and .40, respectively, with 129 degrees of freedom. 153 Marital Satisfaction It was predicted in Hypothesis 5 that lower dis- crepant dyads would be more satisfied with their marriage relationship than higher discrepant dyadS. On a six— point scale, dyad members reported their marriage satis— faction level with a score of 1 indicating high satisfaction, while a score of 6 indicated high dissatisfaction. Thus, a lower score indicates greater satisfaction (see Question 23 of the questionnaire in Appendix II). Once again, the "halo" effect mentioned in the previous section, is no doubt present in this type of evaluation. But assuming this effect is operating for all respondents, the important question is whether there are significant predicted differences between the discrepancy groups. The answer is in the affirmative. Looking at the mean marital satisfaction scores given in Table 18, we find that mean wife and dyad response differences support Hypothesis 5 at the .01 and the .05 level of significance, and husband responses approach the .05 level. As with communication (Tables 16 and 17), we find for husband responses that the low discrepancy husbands express the highest satisfaction, but the medium discrep- ancy husbands indicate lower levels of satisfaction than the high discrepancy husbands. Nevertheless, a general relationship between lower levels of discrepancy and 154 higher levels of satisfaction is clearly present. This relation seems particularly important in that it offers strong evidence of construct validity that the discrepancy measure is in fact an index of system strain. The other self-report findings, being generally supportive of the hypotheses put forward so far, are additional supports of construct validity, but the relationship of role discrep- ancy with marital satisfaction seems to be a key indicant. We note in Table 18 that the history factor is not significant. Mean satisfaction scores for short history dyads indicate only slightly more satisfaction with their marriages than long history dyads. A t-test of the mean satisfaction scores of husbands and wives indicate no significant role difference (t-value = .41 with 129 degrees of freedom). An outline of the major self-report findings con— cerning the dyadic system4 is given in Table 19. The direction and significance of differences by history and discrepancy levels are based on dyad responses. No inter- action effects between history and discrepancy were significant. For more detail concerning the differences summarized in Table 19, the reader is referred back to the previous text discussion. 4Separate time allocations of dyad members with children and nonfamily members are not included in Table 17, since they are not concerned with the dyadic system itself. 155 Table 18. Mean marital satisfaction scores. Role Discrepancy History Low Medium High Total Wife Hus. Dyad Wife Hus. Dyad Wife Hus. Dyad Wife Hus. Dyad Short 1.17 1.25 1.21 1.20 1.60 1.40 1.82 1.36 1.59 1.39 1.39 1.39 Long 1.10 1.20 1.15 1.20 1.70 1.45 1.83 1.45 1.71 1.41 1.50 1.45 Total 1.14 1.23 1.18 1.20 1.65 1.42 1.83 1.48 1.65 1.40 1.45 1.42 Note: Lower score indicates higher satisfaction. Regiigse Source of Variance DF Sgfla::s Sgizie F Ratio ingi Wife Role Discrepance 2 6.50 3.25 7.74 .01 History 1 .00 .00 .00 n.s. Interaction 2 .03 .01 .02 n.s. Within 52_ EELQZ. .42 Total 64 31.60 Husband Role Discrepancy 2 1.91 .96 2.91 n.s. History 1 .18 .18 .55 n.s. Interaction 2 .16 .08 .24 n.s. Within 52. 12;§1_ .33 Total 64 22.06 Dyad Role Discrepancy 2 7.47 3.74 3.14 .05 History 1 .01 .01 .01 n.s. Interaction 2 .12 .06 .05 n.s. Within 52_ 12;§g 1.19 Total 64 78.40 156 Total 19. Summary of major self-report dyadic findings.a Direction Significance History Discrepancy +=short > long +=lo > med > hi History Discrepancy Descriptions 1. Total—time, spouse weekday —n- -__ --_ --- Sunday + + .05 .05 N e Time-alone, spouse + -—— .001 ___ w e Talk-time, spouse weekday + --- .05 -—- Sunday + + -—- --- A Topic discussion frequency + + --- .01 U! Communication control ——— —-- --_ ___ Direction Significance Hypotheses O) Desired talk-time: H1 wife > husband + --- H2 short history > Long history H high discrepancy > low discrepancy \) e Communication satisfaction: H low discrepancy > 4 . . high discrepancy (I) Marital satisfaction: H5 low discrepancy > high discrepancy aDirection and significance of differences based on dyad responses. "---" indicates no significant pattern of differences. 157 With this descriptive information and confirming evidence of the main factor variable's measurement power, we now turn to the discussion data and the relational communication analysis of dyadic systems. Discussion Data Discussion data were gathered on all dyads. How- ever, to increase the potential of more accurately sampling interaction style, a criterion established for inclusion of a dyad in the relational analysis was that the discus- sions across the four topics contain at least fifty trans— actions. Eleven of the total 65 dyads did not meet this requirement. A relational analysis of dyadic communication behavior is the main focus of this part of the study. However, the coding system used allows for message descrip- tions and comparisons to be made at several levels. The data in the first three sections are offered as general descriptions of message-types, control directions and transactional types utilized in the dyadic discussions. After looking at this descriptive information, the results of the transactional analyses will be presented within the framework of the hypotheses stated in Chapter IV. All analyses, descriptive and hypothesis-testing, are based on all four topic discussions. 158 Message and Transactional Descriptions Me ssage Types The description of message types is based on the total messages given by the members of dyads meeting the relational analysis criterion stated above. Total messages include both alternate messages of speakers, and as well, messages that follow one another by the same speaker. Transactional messages“ which are utilized in all but this first section, refer _o_n_l_y to alternate speaker messages. Transactional messages refer to those messages which serve as a stimulus or response to the other speaker. They do not include any individual series of messages by one Slpeaker. Total messages are individual in nature; trans- actional messages are dyadic in nature. The total message base is 13,788 and the transactional message baSe is some- what smaller, 11,489. Messages were coded according to speaker, format and response mode. Each of these three aspects of a mes- 3age were respectively represented in a three digit code. Using this coding system, message types may be described bY a combination of digital codes, or by each code Separately. Messages will be described by all three methods with the combined description given first. Table 20 presents the frequencies and percentages of message types based on both second and third digital 159 0.0 v 0.0 m 0.0 H mm Hawmuooosm 0.0 0 0.0 0 0.0 0 mm Hawmmoooomss :oHuosHumcHl 0.0 mm m.0 0H m.0 0H vm HammmoUOSm H.0 h H.0 m 0.0 m vm Hammmooosmss H03msol m n.m NVmH m.0 vmo 0.0H 000 mm Hammmooosm 0 h.v 0mm m.v «Hm 0.v mmm mm Hammmooosmss sonsouxol 0.0 mm v.0 0m m.0 mm mm HammmoUOSm «.0 mm m.0 0H «.0 vH mm Hammmooosmss uuommsmlsosl 0H 0.N 00m >.H 0NH m.m 00H Hm Hsmmmooosm n 0.0 vHo 0.v 0H0 m.w 00m Hm HammmoOUSmss uuommsm1uo>01meu . . . soHumcHsumu 0 0 H0 0 0 m0 0 0 mm mm coHuMHuHcHu 0.0 H 0.0 H 0.0 0 mm mmcmno onouu m.v 0.5 HmoH 0.0 vmv m.0 hmo mm soncouxwl 0.0 H 0.0 o 0.0 H mm unadmsm1aoa1 H.0 0H H.0 v H.0 0 Hm uuommsmscoHumosw . . . GOHumsHEHou m m m 00v 0 0 wow m m 0mm 0H sOHuMHuHsHI H.0 HH H.0 0 H.0 m 0H omsmno 0Hmou1 H.0 m H.0 0 0.0 m 5H coHumsunsoomel H.0 NH H.0 0 H.0 0 0H H00H01 N.0 0N m.0 mm 0.0 m mH :oHuosuumsHl m m.m on v.m 00H ~.m 00H vH Ho3ms01 H m.sm thm 0.0m bHom H.5m mmmm mH consouxol HH o.H mHm o.H mHH 0.H 00H, NH uuommsmucoc1 N m.mH 0HOH m.vH 000 «.mH VHO HH uuommsm1s0Huuommm omens 11 usoouom aososwonm unmoumm mucosvonm usooumm accoswonm wH o>on¢ 0000 soHumHuomoo 000mmoz 000mmoz ommmmoz ommmmoz ommmmoz 000mmoz mommucoouom case no soono xcmm moan csmnmsm oMH3 momma ommmmoz .ucooumm H o>onm omens ommusooumm mo Hopuo Hams new comp use .Usonmss .oMH3 an momwu 000mmoe mo mommusoonom 0cm moHososwoum .om oHQma .Homv nonuo 0cm Hmmv sOHumsHsuoulsoHumHUHsH .Hmmv son Isouxo so no mHso poms mos poshom omommos oHQMHMHmmmHoss as .Hmvv moosono 0Hmou no .Hhvv msoHumEhHmsoomHo .Hmev mHmUHo .Hmvv mGOHuosuumsH mm now: no: muo3 .omHzoxHH .mouonEoolsoz .Hva msoHumsuHmsoomHo Ho .HmNV muoouo .HmNV mGOHuosuumsH .HwNv muozmsm mm wows no: ohms msoHumosv .Hmsuusm .H0N .OHV 0000 uncommon uHme UHHnu uHosu on no oHanMHmmMHoss ouo3 000mmoa a mo poshom soHumosv no: soHuHommm map HonuHoz .0m .0m .mm .mm .em .Nm .Hm .06 .ee .00 .me .om .sm .mm .mm .vm .OH ”mcoHumaHnsoo c000c0>0m mcHsoHHom 0:» mmosHocH mHae .msonmsomHU 0H0m>0 on» sH UQNHHHus no: mno3 oHnmu 0:» CH umommm nos 00 was» mommulomMmmozm 160 0.00H wmhmH 0.00H 0000 0.00H N000 Hmuoe b.0 mm 0.0 Nm 0.0 vm 0m Hosuou . . . GOHumsHEMou 0 0 0 H 0 v 0 0 N mm soHHMHuHsHI 0.0 o H.0 m 0.0 H mm consouxol Hosuo N.0 0N N.0 NH N.0 mH 00 MonuOI soHumsHEHou m 0 H0 0 0 vN m 0 5H 0v soHumHuHsHl N 0 HN N 0 mH H 0 0 cw Hozmsos m b hmOH H w mmm b h Nmm mv consouon H 0 m 0 0 m H 0 0 N0 uuommsmlsosl N.o em N.o NH N.o 0H He uuommsm1 ouonsoolsos 0.0 H 0.0 0 0.0 H on Hammmooosm 0.0 N0 0.0 mm 0.0 00 0m stmmooosmss HoAHOI 0.0 0 0.0 m 0.0 0 mm Hsmmmmoosm 0.0 0 0.0 N H.0 0 mm Hammmooosmss soHumsHEHou GOHuMHuHsHI 0.0 m 0.0 0 H.0 m mm Hammmooosm 0.0 0 0.0 0 0.0 0 mm Howmmmoosmss omsmso UHQOHI 0.0 H 0.0 H 0.0 0 hm Hammmooosm 0.0 0 0.0 0 0.0 0 mm Hommwooosmss coHumsHHmsoochu 0.0 m H.0 v 0.0 H mm Hsmmmooosm 0.0 v 0.0 H 0.0 m 0m Hsmmmooosmss Hausa: 161 code descriptions. Of the total individual message base acorss all four topics (13,788), 6,902 of these messages were made by wives and 6,886 were made by husbands. We note a fairly similar style of message type utilization by the two sexes. The only noticeable difference is in the 9.2 percent usage of questions as extension by wives versus 6.6 percent usage of this message type by husbands. Message types that had more than a 1 percent usage by the dyad were rank ordered. We see that assertions as extensions were by far the most frequent (37.5 percent) type of message used in the discussions, assertions as supports were the next most frequent occurring message type (13.9 percent) followed by successful talk—overs as extensions (9.7 percent), noncompletes as extensions (7.9 percent) and questions as extensions (7.9 percent). (See Table 20 for further descending rank order of message type usage.) Table 21 shows the usage of message types based on the second digit code. Assertions were the predominant format used (59.3 percent) followed by talk-overs (22.5 percent). Consistent with Table 20, we find slightly more frequent use of questions by wives and that they had slightly more successful talk-overs than husbands. Message types based on the third digit code are summarized in Table 22. Extension is by far the most frequent response mode (67.8 percent) followed by support 162 0.00H 000.MH 0.00H 000.0 0.00H H0009 m 0.0 00 0.0 H0 0.0 00:00 .m m 0.0 mH~.H H.0 sum 0.0 mumesoo 0oz .0 1 H0.NH mm0.H 0.HH 0mm H.mH H9000000900 1 HH.0H Hmm.H 0.0H 000 H.0H H500000050000 m m.- 00H.m 0.0m mHm.H m.mm mum>01mee .m 0 0.0 00H.H m.s mom 0.0 maoHummso .N H H.0m 00H.0 0.H0 00H.0 0.50 maoHuHmmm0 .H 0MMMMGMMH00 0000000000 00:0500H0 000us0on00 >000s00u0 0000000000 000050000 0000910000002 00000 H000 0000 0cmnmsm uHmHo 000000 .00000000000 0000 no H00H0 xs0u 000 0000 000 000000: .0MH3 an 0000» 0000005 0H0H0 000000 00 00000000000 000 00Hos0s0000 .HN 0Hn0a 163 0.00H 00>.mH 0.00H 000.0 0.00H «00.0 H0000 0 0.H 00H ~.H m0 0.H 0HH 00000 .0 00Hu00HEH0B m 0.0 000 0.0 000 «.0 000 10000000000 .0 0.0 H.0 0H H.0 e «.0 0H 000000 00000 .0 0.0 H.0 0 H.0 a 0.0 m 0000000H0000000 .0 0.0 «.0 H0 «.0 HH 0.0 0H 00000 .0 0.0 0.0 00 0.0 00 H.0 0 00H0000000H .m 0 0.0 00m 0.m 000 0.0 00H 003000 .0 H 0.00 Hmm.0 0.00 000.0 0.00 000.0 000000000 .m m 0.0 0H0 0.0 00H 0.0 00H 0000000002 .0 m 0.00 H00.~ H.H~ 000.H 0.0m 00m.H 0000000 .H 00WMMM0MH00 0000000000 000000000 0000000000 000000000 0000000000 000000000 0000910000002 00000 0000 0000 0000000 00000 00000 .00000000000 0000 Mo 00000 #000 000 0000 000 0000000 .0MH3 00 00000 0000000 0H0H0 00Hnu mo 00000000000 000 00H00000000 .NN 0HO0B 164 (20.6 percent). Support messages heavily outweigh the use of nonsupport (2.3 percent) messages. This is much higher than Bales' (1955) two to one ratio of positive to negative reactions, although Bales notes that dyadic systems avoid disagreement more than larger sized groups (Bales, 1955; Bales and Borgatta, 1955). Message Control As discussed earlier, the data for all following descriptions and comparisons are based on the transactional messages manifested by the 54 dyads who meet the relational analysis criterion of having at least 50 transactions in their topic discussions. A summary of the control directions used in those discussions is given in Table 23. Remember that each three digit descriptive code was defined as a movement toward control (one-up), a movement toward accepting or seeking other to control (one—down), or a movement toward neutralizing control (one-across). Almost half of the messages (48.2 percent) are one—across movements indicat- ing a leveling or deemphasis of control. The remaining messages are split about evenly between being one-up and one-down control statements. Husbands manifested slightly more one-up movements than wives and slightly less one- down movements. 165 Table 23. Percentages of control direction by wife, hus- band and dyad. Control Direction Wife Husband Dyad + 23.9 24.6 24.2 + 27.8 25.9 26.9 + 48.2 49.4 48.8 Total 99.9 99.9 99.9 Transactional Types A combination of the control directions of sequen— cially occurring two—message units results in nine possible transactional types. Table 24 shows the percentage of occurrence of these nine types in the dyadic discussions. (The marginal percentages of Table 24 are, of course, the same percentages as presented in Table 23 for husband and wife.) Neutralized symmetry and the transitory transac- tional types make-up the larger proportions of transaction type utilization. Neutralized symmetry accounts for the largest proportion, while submissive symmetry accounts for the smallest proportion of transactions and competitive symmetry second to the smallest. Transitory transactions with a one-down movement are more frequent occurring than those with a one-up movement. 166 Table 24. Percentages of transaction types. Husband Total + + + + 5.5 5.7 12.7 23.9 Wife + 6.5 4.3 17.0 27.8 + 12.6 15.9 19.7 48.2 Total 24.6 25.9 49.4 99.9 Combining the sub—categories of the three major types of transactions together gives the percentage break- downs shown in Table 25. The transitory type is most frequently utilized, followed by symmetry, with complimen- tarity exhibited least frequently. (This is so even when one takes into account that the general classifications are based on different numbers of cell combinations.) Individual husband-wife pairs differ on the use of these types, but as a group, they produce seemingly low levels of complementarity, the maximally contrasting control directions. It would be interesting to have a data base for comparison of transactional patterns of generically different dyads. The level of complementarity exhibited in the dyad under study would be more meaningful compared to the amount exhibited, for example, in a func- tionally specific, high time allocation, low history dyad. 167 Table 25. Percentages of the three general transaction types. Transaction Type Percentage Symmetry 29.5 Complimentarity 12.2 Transitory 58.2 One-up (25.3) One-down (32.9) Total 99.9 Comparison data of this type hopefully will be accumulated in future studies. §ummary of Message and Transactional Descriptions It was found that assertions and extensions were by far the most frequent format and response message types exchanged during the dyadic discussions. Talk-overs and support statements were the next most frequently used message types. It was interesting to note the relatively high proportion of support statements, 20.6 percent, com— pared to the 2.3 percent of nonsupport messages. Agreeing and approving seems to be a much more likely verbal response than challenging and disagreeing. Disconfirmation and 168 topic change, instructions and orders, all one-up responses, were found to be rare occurrences. It follows from the above that the most frequent occurring control direction is one—across. One—across control accounts for about half of all messages. Slightly more than one fourth of the messages were one-down, and slightly less than a fourth were one-sup. Across dyads, message type usage and control direction are of a very similar pattern for husbands and wives. Of the three major transactional types, transitory transactions accounted for almost 60 percent of the exchanges. One-down transitory transactions occurred more frequently than one—up transitory types. Symmetrical exchanges made up about 30 percent of all transactions, with neutralized symmetry accounting for the largest pro— portion (19.7 percent) and submissive symmetry accounting for the least (4.3 percent). Complementary transactions made up the smallest proportion, a little more than 10 percent, of the messages exchanged by husband and wife dyads. Message and Transactional Hypotheses Based on previously discussed theoretical reasons and research results, several interaction hypotheses were put forward at three different levels. The activity level hypotheses dealing with discussion time, silences and 169 number of transactions will be presented first, message level hypotheses concerning support statements and talk— overs second, and the transactional level hypotheses third. In the presentation of the hypotheses—testing data the direction of predicted differences, as well as notice- able significance levels, even though below the traditional .05 level, will be pointed out. Several aspects of this study make it important to look for trends in the data, as well as clear statistically evidenced differences. First, research that involves groups, rather than individuals, as the basic unit of data, increases the difficulty of obtain- ing large samples where statistical differences are detected more readily. Secondly, the research guiding this study has been based on extreme group comparison of families with a severely disturbed member and those with no notice- able emotional problems. The present research probes more in the middle ranges of family imbalance. It asks if behavior pattern predictions based on general systems theory can be detected in normal families of different, but still moderate levels of role discrepancy strain. Thirdly, the exploratory nature of the present research and the operational initiation of the major transactional measuring instrument are factors that also foster a close look at the data for suggestive trends. 170 Activity Level Hypotheses It was theorized that the members of dyads with lower levels of role discrepancy strain, would have longer discussion periods, would talk more freely with fewer hesitations and more frequent changes of speaker than those with higher discrepancy strain. Hypothesis 6: Dyads with lower role discrepancy will talk longer than higher role discrepant dyads. The length of discussion time ranged from seven to 46 minutes, even though the discussions were monitored by the interviewer (see Interviewer Instructions, Appendix IV) and somewhat affected by the transaction criterion necessary for inclusion in the discussion data analysis. However, as seen in Table 26, the different discrepancy level means differ very slightly from the overall mean of 28.3 minutes. Hypothesis 6 is not supported. Further, the dyads that were dropped from the relational analysis were from each of the three levels, and a comparison of the number of interviewer probes made during the discus- sions, showed no noticeable difference by discrepancy level. A positive note can be argued from the rejection of Hypothesis 6. In terms of testing the following hypothe- ses, it is methodologically more shound to have the discrep— ancy categories with such similar lengths of discussion time. 171 Table 26. Mean discussion time. Role Discrepancy History Total Low Medium High N=10a N=10 =9 N=29 Sh°rt 28.40b 29.40 26.67 28.21 N=6 N=9 N=10 N=25 L°ng 27.67 28.22 29.00 28.40 N=l6 N=l9 N=19 N=54 Total 28.13 28.84 27.89 28.30 aThe number of dyads in each category are given in this table and are the same for all the following analyses. b o o o 0 Time is expressed in minutes. . Sum of Mean Sig. Source of Variance DF Squares Squares F Value Level Role Discrepancy 2 9.95 4.98 .08 n.s. History 1 .26 .26 .00 n.s. Interaction 2 33.82 16.91 .28 n.s. Within 48 2927.23 60.98 Total 53 2971.26 172 Hypothesis 7: Dyads with lower role discrepancy will have less silence during the discussion period than higher role discrepancy dyads. Table 27 presents the data concerning Hypothesis 7. The number of silences were tabulated for each initial three seconds of nontalking and each additional five second period. This number was then divided by the number of minutes talked to give an index of silence per minute for each dyad. The data do not support Hypothesis 7, but there is a small mean difference in the predicted direction and this is particularly so for the short history dyads. Hypothesis 8: Dyads with lower role discrepancy will have more total number of transactions than higher role dis- crepant dyads. r The number of transactions that take place in a discussion is a direct measure of the turn-over rate of "floor" control. The number of times speakers change during a discussion was postualted to be one indicant of the flexibility, or openness of a dyad's interaction style. The range of transactions that occurred in the dyadic dis- cussions was from 55 to 528. It was predicted that those dyads with less discrepancy strain would exhibit more flexibility. The data concerning this hypothesis is given in Table 28. The discrepancy factor approaches Table 27. Mean silences per minute. 173 Role Discrepancy History Total Low Medium (High Short .15 .39 .54 .36 Long .54 .64 .37 .51 Total .30 .51 .45 .43 . Sum of Mean Sig. Source of Variance DF Squares Square F Value Level Role Discrepancy 2 .24 .12 .32 n.s. History 1 .30 .30 .81 n.s. Interaction 2 .76 .38 1.03 n.s. Within 48 18.02 .37 Total 53 19.32 significance (.20), with the mean differences in the pre— dicted direction the short history dyads. . Again, this is especially evident for This offers limited support for the notion that lower discrepant dyads have a more flexible interaction style than high discrepant dyads. Summary Dyads of different discrepancy levels did not differ significantly on discussion length, silences, or 174 Table 28. Mean number of transactions. \ M Role Discrepancy History Total Low Medium High Short 248.70 210.00 153.33 205.76 Long 266.83 184.78 225.80 220.88 Total 255.50 198.05 191.47 212.76 Source of Variance DF Sum Of Mean F Value 819' Squares Square Level Role Discrepancy 2 44,994.65 22,497.33 1.61 (.20) History 1 6,204.56 6,204.56 .44 n.s. Interaction 2 22,683.06 11,341.53 .81 n.s. Within 48 669,929.60 13,956.87 Total 53 743,811.87 number of transactions. However, mean discrepancy differ- ences showed a particular tendency in the predicted direction for number of transactions. It is interesting to note that the short history dyads manifested clear and linear mean differences as predicted on each of the activity dimensions, but long history dyads were not con- sistent. 175 Message Level Hypotheses The following analyses are based on the transac- tional messages exchanged between dyad members. The results show whether different discrepanCy level dyads differ as to the number of support statements they inter- change or by number and type of talk—overs manifested in their discussions. In order to have comparable measures across dyads, the message characteristics under study are expressed as proportions of the total number of transactional messages for each dyad. This is true for all following message and transaction type analyses. For the analysis of variance computations used to test the hypotheses, these proportions were transformed to arcsins. This procedure is recommended for more appropriate application of the F-distribution (see Winer, 1962, p. 221). The mean values that are given in the tables, however, are expressed in proportions. Hypothesis 9: Dyads with lower role discrepancy will express more support statements than higher role discrepant dyads. It was theorized that support statements, being expressions of acceptance or giving of control, would be more characteristic of dyads with less role discrepancy strain. As shown in Table 29, there is strong evidence for this hypothesis. The discrepancy level does effect in an inverse manner the proportions of transactional 176 Table 29. Mean proportions of support messages. Role Discrepancy History Total Low Medium High Short .23 .22 .17 .21 Long .21 .19 .19 .19 Total .22 .20 .18 .20 . Sum of Mean Sig. Source of Variance DF Squares Square F Value Level Role Discrepancy 2 .102 .051 2.83 .06 History 1 .008 .008 .44 n.s. Interaction 2 .042 .021 1.17 n.s. Within 48 .874 .018 Total 53 1.026 statements that are support messages. nounced for the short history dyads. Hypothesis 10: Hypothesis 11: This is more pro- Dyads with lower role discrepancy will have more talk-overs than higher role discrepant dyads. Dyads with lower role discrepancy will have more successful talk- overs than higher role discrepant dyads. 177 It is suggested that more frequent talk—overs—- coming into the discussion without waiting for the other to finish talking——is another indication of flexibility of floor control, and this will be more characteristic of the lower discrepant dyads. It was further felt that when talk—overs occurred, the lower discrepant dyads would yield the floor (giving a successful talk—over) more often than high discrepant dyads. Neither of these hypotheses are supported (see Tables 30 and 31). But notice again that the predicted difference on talk-overs is displayed by short history dyads, but not for long history dyads. The same type of analysis for successful talk— overs was carried out on unsuccessful talk-overs. The results of this analysis are a significant (.05) difference by discrepancy level. As seen in Table 32, high discrep- ancy dyads have a lower mean proportion of unsuccessful talk—overs than low discrepant dyads. It appears that instead of the speaker of a low discrepant dyad yielding the floor when a talk—over occurs, as suggested in Hypothe- sis 11, it is more likely for the interruptor to yield and not press for floor control. In reverse, when a talk-over occurs in a high discrepant dyad, it is more likely that the interruptor will persist in talking until he/she has 178 Table 30. Mean proportions of talk—over messages. Role Discrepancy History Total Low Medium _ High Short .23 .22 .18 .21 Long .21 .20 .23 .21 Total .23 .21 .21 .21 . Sum of Mean Sig. Source of Variance DF Squares Square P Value Level Role Discrepancy 2 .027 .014 .40 n.s. History 1 .000 .000 .00 n.s. Interaction 2 .131 .066 1.89 (.15) Within 48 1.678 .035 Total 53 1.84 gained floor control.5 This again is more pronounced for short history dyads. Summary Low discrepant dyads interchange significantly higher proportions of support statements and manifest 5Referring back to Table 20 we find that almost all talk-overs were extensions or support responses. It is suggested that for future research, a time measure of the verbal over-lap of speakers (length of talk-over) would give interesting information on the length of struggle for gain- ing or conceeding floor control by response mode and dis- crepancy level. 179 Table 31. Mean proportions of successful talk—over messages. Role Discrepancy History Total Low Medium . High Short .12 .11 .11 .12 Long .10 .12 .14 .12 Total .11 .12 .13 .12 . Sum of Mean Sig. Source of Variance DF Squares Square P Value Level Role Discrepancy 2 .015 .008 .32 n.s. History 1 .007 .007 .28 n.s. Interaction 2 .052 .026 1.04 n.s. Within 48 1.211 Total 53 1.285 higher proportions of unsuccessful talk-overs than higher discrepant dyadic systems. Dyads differentiated by dis- crepancy do not show difference in the proportion of total talk-overs or successful talk-overs. On the dimensions of support, total talk-overs and unsuccessful talk-overs, short history dyads exhibit more pronounced, predicted message exchange behavior than do long history dyads. The accumulating number of variables for which this history difference occurs, leads one to 5 Table 32. Mean proportions of unsuccessful talk-over messages. 180 Role Discrepancy History Total Low Medium High Short .12 .ll .06 .10 Long .10 .08 .08 .09 Total .11 .10 .07 .09 . Sum of Mean Sig. Source of Variance DF Squares Square F Value Level Role Discrepancy 2 .152 .076 3.45 .04 History 1 .000 .000 .00 n.s. Interaction 2 .117 .059 2.68 .08 Within 48 1.072 .022 Total. 53 1.341 suggest that system strain may operate differently for systems of different lengths of history. It appears that the distributive justice postulates examined in this section have more predictive power for shorter history dyads than those with longer history. Transactional Level Hypotheses The first two transactional level hypotheses deal with the flexibility of dyadic transaction patterns, the 181 second two with complementarity and symmetrical transac- tion usage, and the final two hypotheses deal with transi- tory transaction patterns. Hypothesis 12: Dyads with lower role discrepancy will exhibit more random usage of transaction types than higher role discrepancy dyads. As previously stated, it was hypothesized that system discrepancy would be related inversely to system flexibility. The measure utilized to test this notion, as expressed in Hypothesis 12, is a difference score from totally random usage of the transactional types. Since there are nine possible types, an eleven percent usage of each type would represent maximum randomness (equality) of transactional usage. For each transactional type, the proportion usage was subtracted from eleven percent for each dyad and these values were summed across the nine categories to produce an absolute value representing a dyad's deviation from total randomness. These difference scores range from 24 to 87. The theoretical range is from one to 177. A lower score indicates more random usage of the nine transactional types. Table 33 shows that there is no consistent pattern of difference scores from randomness among the discrepancy and history level categories. Hypothesis 12, relating higher flexibility with low discrepancy, is rejected. 182 Table 33. Mean difference scores from random usage of each transactional type. Role Discrepancy History _ Total Low Medium High Short 51.00 56.10 51.44 52.90 Long 59.67 52.67 52.60 54.32 Total 54.25 54.47 52.05 53.91 Note: A lower score indicates more random usage. . Sum of Mean Sig. Source of Variance DF Squares Square F Value Level Role Discrepancy 2 101.59 50.79 .22 n.s. History 1 59.25 59.25 .26 n.s. Interaction 2 308.18 154.09 .68 n.s. Within 48 10,848.31 226.01 Total 53 11,317.33 Hypothesis 13: Dyads with lower role discrepancy will exhibit fewer long-run homoger neous transactional sequences (con- figuration patterns) than higher role discrepancy dyads. This hypothesis is also based on the lower discrepancy-higher flexibility notion. Here flexibility 183 is measured in terms of long-run sequences of the same transaction type. These homogeneous sequences are referred to as configurations. Empirically a configura- tion was defined as a series of five transactions or more involving identical transactional types of exchange. For each dyad, the number of transactions that constitute the configurations produced in the discussions were tabulated, divided by the total number of transactions occurring in the discussions and expressed as proportions. Table 34 expresses the mean proportion of configura- tion transactions. The range of proportions was from .00 to .22, but as the overall mean indicates, only six percent of all transactions occurred in homogeneous series. Across all dyads there were similar low proportions of configuration transactions. A simple counting of con- figuration occurance gives a 1.5 mean across dyads. Thirteen dyads had no configuration patterns in their interactions. Only six dyads had more than five configura- tions. A 2X3 analysis of variance by discrepancy and history of the frequency of configurations shows no discernable patterns (F-value for discrepancy = .61, for history .00, for interaction 1.57). Hypothesis 13, as Hypothesis 12, is rejected. Both types of transaction flexibility measures used, show little difference in transaction patterns across all dyads. Table 34. 184 Mean proportions of configuration transactions. M Role Discrepancy History Total Low Medium High Short .06 .07 .04 .06 Long .05 .07 .06 .06 Total .05 .07 .05 .06 . Sum of Mean Sig. Source of Variance DF Squares Squares F Value Level Role Discrepancy 2 .110 .055 1.04 n.s. History 1 .033 .033 .62 n.s. Interaction 2 .067 .033 .62 n.s. Within 48 2.527 .053 Total 53 2.737 Hypothesis l4: Hypothesis 15: Dyads with lower role discrepancy will have more complementary trans- actions than higher role discrep- ancy dyads. Dyads with lower role discrepancy will have less competitive symmetry than higher role discrepant dyads. 185 A basic notion underlying this research was that dyads who expressed the least role discrepancy strain would not struggle to modify or negate interaction control definitions as much as dyads who expressed high discrep- ancy strain. It was felt that low discrepant couples would not necessarily display high proportions of comple- mentarity, but that they would manifest more of this type of transaction (+~+ and 4'+) than high discrepant couples. Using the above rationale, it is suggested in Hypothesis 15 that competitive symmetry (++) would be a more characteristic type of transaction for high discrepant dyads than low discrepant dyads. Again, it is not suggested that this transaction type, involving nonacceptance would occur frequently, but that it would occur more frequently for high, than for low discrepant dyads. The data presented in Tables 35 and 36 concerning complementary transactions offer no support of Hypothesis 14. Table 35 gives the mean proportions of complementary transactions where the husband is one-up and the wife is one-down; Table 36 gives the reverse, where the husband is one-down and the wife is one-up.6 6These transaction types do not give sequential information, i.e., information concerning the order of the control messages. This is a suggestion to be made for later research. 186 Table 35. Mean proportions of H+ W+ Complementary trans- actions. Role Discrepancy History . Total Low Medium High Short .08 .07 .06 .07 Long .05 .06 .07 .06 Total .06 .06 .06 .06 Note: Husband control direction is always given first. . Sum of Mean Sig. Source of Variance DF Squares Square F Value Level Role Discrepancy 2 .004 .002 .08 n.s. History 1 .004 .004 .15 n.s. Interaction 2 .027 .014 .54 n.s. Within 48 1.230 .026 Total 53 1.265 Looking at the two tables, we see that one-up messages are matched with one—down messages, and one-down messages with one-up messages, as often in high discrepant dyads as in low discrepant dyads. There is little differ- ence in the mean proportions of the two tables.7 7An analysis of variance of the combined complement- ary transaction cells by discrepancy and history levels, is given in a later section (see Table 49), with similar non- significant results. 187 Table 36. Mean proportions of H4 W+ complementary trans- actions. Role Discrepancy History , Total Low Medium High Short .06 .05 .07 .06 Long .05 .06 .05 .06 Total .06 .05 .06 .06 . Sum of Mean Sig. Source of Variance DF Squares Square F Value Level Role Discrepancy 2 .022 .011 .58 n.s. History 1 .008 .008 .42 n.s. Interaction 2 .023 .011 .58 n.s. Within 48 .910 .019 Total 53 .963 Table 37 gives the mean proportions of competitive symmetry by discrepancy and history levels. Hypothesis 15 is also rejected, but the discrepancy differences are in the predicted direction. Competitive symmetry (one—up, one-up transactions) occur slightly more often, as suggested, for higher discrepant dyads than lower discrepant pairs. Inconsistent with previous results, long history dyads displayed this pattern more than short history dyads. Table 37 188 . Mean proportions of competitive symmetry (++) transactions. Role Discrepancy History Total Low Medium High Short .05 .05 .06 .06 Long .03 .05 .07 .05 Total .04 .05 .06 .05 . Sum of Mean Sig. Source of Variance DF Squares Square F Value Level Role Discrepancy 2 .051 .026 1.24 n.s. History 1 .014 .014 .67 n.s. Interaction 2 .045 .023 1.10 n.s. Within 48 1.026 .021 Total 53 1.136 Hypothesis 16: Hypothesis 17: Dyads with lower role discrepancy will exhibit less neutralization toward one-up and one—up toward neutralization transitory trans- actions than higher role discrep- ancy dyads. Dyads with lower role discrepancy will exhibit more neutralization toward one-down and one-down 189 toward neutralization transitory transactions than higher role dis— crepancy dyads. In accordance with earlier predictions and rationale, it was hypothesized that transitory transactions with move- ments toward control (one—up) would occur more frequently in higher, rather than lower, discrepancy dyads. Likewise, it was predicted that transitory transactions with one- down movements, accepting or seeking other to control, would be more characteristic of lower, rather than higher, discrepancy dyads. Each of these hypotheses were tested under the two possible situations: (1) where the husbands' messages were one-across and the wives' messages were one-up as in Table 39, or the wives' messages were one-down as in Table 41; and (2) where the wives‘messages were one-across and the husbands' messages were one-up in the case of Table 38, or the husbands' messages were one-down in the case of Table 40. The mean proportions of the two possible combina- tions for transitory transactions involving a one-up move— ment are given in Tables 38 and 39. Comparing the two tables, we notice quite similar mean proportions (around .12 or .13) across categories whether the husband is one- across or the wife is one—across. In Table 38, where the wife is one-up, there is more variance in the mean 190 Table 38. Mean proportions of H7 W+ transitory trans- actions. Role Discrepancy History . Total Low Medium High Short .13 .12 .13 .13 Long .13 .12 .12 .12 Total .13 .12 .12 .12 . Sum of Mean Sig. Source of Variance DF Squares Square F Value Level Role Discrepancy 2 .004 .002 .10 n.s. History 1 .003 .003 .15 n.s. Interaction 2 .003 .001 .05 n.s. Within 48 .953 .020 Total 53 .963 proportions. There are, however, no significant discrep- ancy level differences, thus Hypothesis 16 is not supported, but the history level approaches significance (.20) with long history dyads having a higher proportion of transitory transactions with wife one—up. Tables 40 and 41 present the transaction data involving neutralization (one—across) and a one-down direc- tion. In Table 40 both a significant discrepancy (.03) 191 Table 39. Mean proportions of H+ W+ transitory trans— actions. Role Discrepancy History , Total Low Medium High Short .11 .11 .13 .12 Long .12 .14 .15 .14 Total .11 .12 .14 .13 . Sum of Mean Sig. Source of Variance DF Squares Square F Value Level Role Discrepancy 2 .053 .026 .84 n.s. History 1 .048 .048 1.55 (.20) Interaction 2 .025 .012 .39 n.s. Within 48 1.480 .031 Total 53 1.606 and history (.05) difference are found in the use of transitory transactions where the husband is in a one- down position. This is not the case where the wife is one-down, as shown in Table 41; however, the interaction F-value is significant at the .10 level. The discrepancy differences indicate that transi- tory transactions with the husband in the one-down posi- tion are significantly more characteristic of lower level 192 Table 40. Mean proportions of H+ W+ transitory trans- actions. Role Discrepancy History ~ . Total Low Medium High Short .21 .18 .14 .18 Long .17 .14 .13 .14 Total .19 .16 .13 .16 . Sum of Mean Sig. Source of Variance DF Squares Square F Value Level Role Discrepancy 2 .199 .100 3.70 .03 History 1 .106 .106 3.93 .05 Interaction 2 .021 .010 .37 n.s. Within 48 1.288 .027 Total 53 1.614 discrepancy dyads. This transitory pattern is more descrip- tive of shorter history dyads. When the wife is one-down, there is a tendency for the predicted differences to hold for short history pairs, but for high history dyads, the mean differences are of a curvilinear nature. Thus, Hypothesis 17 is supported by the husband one-down, wife one-across transitory transaction data, but not by the husband one-across, wife-one—down transitory data. 193 Table 41. Mean proportions of H+ W+ transitory trans- actions. Role Discrepancy History Total Low Medium High Short .17 .19 .14 .17 Long .18 .15 .19 .17 Total .17 .18 .16 .17 . Sum of Mean Sig. Source of Variance DF Squares Square F Value Level Role Discrepancy 2 .016 .008 .24 n.s. History 1 .009 .009 .27 n.s. Interaction 2 .160 .080 2.43 .10 Within 48 1.575 .033 -Tota1k 53 1.760 To restate the main findings concerning Hypothesis 17, it appears that with low discrepant couples, a one- across message given by a wife is more likely, than with high discrepant couples, to be followed by a one—down message by a husband, or if a husband is in a one-down position for a wife to go one-across. As mentioned before, the transactional categories do not contain sequential information. From this data, it is not known which of the above pattern was the more 194 frequent occurrence. The classification of transaction types by ggdgr of speaker, rather than by individual speaker (husband, wife) is a major suggestion of this writer for expanding the amount and value of information that can be acquired from the transactional coding scheme presented in this study. Summary The analyses of transactional flexibility found no differences by discrepancy level (or history) in the randomness of either transactional type usage or configura- tion occurrence. Dyads of different discrepancy levels do not differ in their use of complementary transactions, but they do differ somewhat in the expression of competitive symmetry. There was a tendency for higher discrepant couples to manifest slightly more symmetry of a one-up, one- up type than lower discrepant couples, but not signifi- cantly. No relationship was found between discrepancy level and one-up transitory transactions. However, a significant relationship was found between discrepancy and husband one-down type of transitory transactions, but not with wife one-down transitory type. 195 Summary of Message and Transactional Hypotheses A summary of the three levels of hypotheses test- ing results is presented in outline form in Table 42. A plus sign in the first column indicates that the mean differences by discrepancy level were in the predicted direction. The significance level of the F—values is given in the second column. Exploratory Questions To expand the information that may be gained from this study, a number of exploratory questions were posed. No a priori predictions are involved. The three groups of dyads, categorized by discrepancy level, will be com- pared further on the dimensions of message control, transactional and configuration patterns. Question 1: What type of message control is more frequently used by lower versus higher role discrepant dyad members? In a previous description of the message charac- teristics, based on the total sample, we found that almost half of all messages exchanged were one-across, and that the rest were fairly equally divided among one— up and one-down messages. In testing the discussion hypotheses, we found low discrepant dyads differing from high discrepant pairs on the use of support messages, 196 Table 42. Summary of discussion data hypothesis testing. Predicted Significance Direction Level 1. Activity level H6 low discrep=>discuss time —-— —-— H7 low discreptransactions + (.20) 2. Message level: H9 low discrep=>support + .06 Hlo low discrepi>talk-overs -—- --- H11 low discrep=>suc. talk-overs --- --- (found low discrep:>unsuc. talk-overs sig. at . level)a 3. Transactional level: H12 low discrep>>random use of ___ ___ ' transaction types H13 low discrep<>complementarity —-- --- H15 low discrepone—down transitory transactions (a) husband one-down + (b) wife one—down ___ aThis relationship was not predicted, but be significant. found to 197 unsuccessful talk-overs and transitory transactions. All of these differences were ones in which the low discrepant dyads had more one-down movements. Thus, we have some clues about the use of message control, but we did not, as a separate concern test control directions by discrep- ancy level. Recall that message control may be tabulated separately by dyad member, or for the dyadic system as a whole. Both individual and dyad classifications of message control were obtained and compared for discrepancy level differences. The mean proportions of one—up, one- down and one-across control direction of transactional messages are given in Tables 43, 44, and 45, respectively, with the accompanying F-ratio results. We find that there are differences in the use of control directions. In Table 43 it is found that high discrepant wives send the most one-up messages and it is clear they send more one-up messages than low discrepant wives (F-value = 2.45, significant at the .10 level). Husbands of different discrepancy levels do not differ significantly in their use of one-up control. Complementing this finding, we see in Table 44 that husbands of low discrepant dyads give proportionately more one-down messages than high discrepant husbands. The F-value, 1.44, however, is not statistically significant. By comparing Tables 43 and 44 at the dyad level, we see that low discrepant dyads have a higher proportion of 198 Table 43. Mean proportions of one—up messages. Role Discrepancy History Low Medium High. Total Wife Hus. Dyad Wife Hus. Dyad Wife Hus. Dyad Wife Hus. Dyad Short .23 .26 .25 .21 .25 .26 .25 .25 .23 .25 .24 Long .20 .22 21 .25 24 27 .26 .27 2.5 .24 .24 Total .22 .25 .23 .23 .24 .27 .25 .26 .24 .25 .24 Response Source of Variance DF Sum Of Mean F Ratio 519' Unit Squares Square Level Wife Role Discrepancy 2 .142 .071 2.45 .10 History 1 .004 .004 .14 n.s. Interaction 2 .041 .020 .69 n.s. Within 48_ 1.412 .029 Total 53 1.599 Husband Role Discrepancy 2 .066 .003 '.O9 n.s. History 1 .016 .016 .47 n.s. Interaction 2 .037 .019 .56 n.s. Within 4§_ 1.634 034 Total 53 1.693 Dyad Role Discrepancy 2 .047 .023 1.35 n.s. History 1 .001 .001 .06 n.s. Interaction 2 .034 .017 1.00 n.s. Within 4§_ .805 .017 Total 53 .887 199 Table 44. Mean proportions of one—down messages. Role Discrepancy History Low Medium High‘ Total Wife Hus. Dyad Wife Hus. Dyad Wife Hus. Dyad Wife Hus. Dyad Short .29 .31 .30 .30 .26 .24 .25 .24 .28 .27 .28 Long .29 .27 .28 .27 .24 .30 .22 .26 .29 .24 .26 Total .29 .29 .29 .28 .25 .27 .24 .25 .28 .26 .27 Response Source of Variance DF Sum Of Mean F Ratio 519' Unit Squares Square Level Wife Role Discrepancy 2 .021 .010 .21 n.s. History 1 .011 .011 .23 n.s. Interaction 2 .124 .061 1.27 n.s. Within 48 2.312 .048 Total 53 2.468 Husband Role Discrepancy 2 .105 .052 1.44 n.s. History 1 .080 .080 2.22 (.14) Interaction 2 .007 .003 .08 n.s. Within 48 1.754 .036 Total 53 1.946 Dyad Role Discrepancy 2 .052 .026 1.37 n.s. History 1 .008 .008 .42 n.s. Interaction 2 .035 .017 .89 n.s. Within 48 .912 .019 Total 53 1.007 200 Table 45. Mean proportions of one—across messages. Role Discrepancy History Low Medium High. Total Wife Hus. Dyad Wife Hus. Dyad Wife Hus Dyad Wife Hus. Dyad Short .48 .43 .46 .49 .49 .50 .51 .50 .49 .47 .48 Long .52 .52 .52 .49 .53 .43 .52 .47 .47 .52 .50 Total .50 .46 .48 .49 .51 .46 .51 .49 .48 .49 .49 Response Source of Variance DF Sum Of Mean F Ratio 819' Unit Squares Square Level Wife Role Discrepancy 2 .044 .022 .65 n.s. History 1 .014 .014 .41 n.s. Interaction 2 .112 .056 1.65 (.20) Within 48 1.615 .034 Total 53 1.785 Husband Role Discrepancy 2 .056 .028 .68 n.s. History 1 .117 .117 2.85 .10 Interaction 2 .054 .027 .66 n.s. Within 48 1.974 .041 Total 53 2.201 Dyad Role Discrepancy 2 .006 .003 .14 n.s. History 1 .014 .014 .64 n.s. Interaction 2 .075 .037 1.68 (.20) Within 48 1.036 .022 Total 53 1.131 201 one-down messages than one-up, .29 versus .23, for medium level discrepancy dyads the ratio is .27 to .24, and for high discrepant dyads the relation has changed to a slightly higher ratio of one—up to one—down messages, .26 to .25. In Table 45 we find the variations in the propor- ”fl tions of one-across usage are related to the husband-wife E 1 discrepancy one-up anui one-down control patterns. Husbands of higher discrepant dyadic systems express more one-across message control than those of lower discrepancy, while the reverse is true for wives. Higher discrepant wives express less one-across control than lower discrepant wives. But none of these variations are significant. To summarize, we find that strain of inequity in the dyadic systems is particularly associated with differ- ent proportions of one-up to one-down messages. More specifically, we find that role discrepancy is directly related to wife one—up control communication, and at a lesser level, inversely related to husband one—down con- trol. Question 2: What transactional types are more frequently expressed by lower versus higher role discrepant dyads? In the previous hypothesis-testing section several transactional types were compared by discrepancy level. It was found that dyads differentiated by discrepancy: 202 (1) did not differ in the proportion of complementary transactions, (2) differed some in the use of competitive symmetry with high discrepant dyads manifesting slightly higher proportions of oneuup, one-up transactions than low discrepant dyads, and (3) did not differ on one-up transi- tory types but did differ on one—down type of transitory exchange, with lower discrepancy associated with a higher proportion of husband one—down transitory transactions. The two remaining transaction types, submissive and neutralized symmetry were compared by discrepancy level (controlling on history). The results are given in Tables 46 and 47. Submissive symmetry was the least displayed type of transaction by all dyads, with one exception, high history- 1ow discrepancy dyads had a one percent lower usage of competitive symmetry. In Table 46 we find very similar proportions of submissive symmetry across categories. With reference to Table 47, we find neutralized symmetry varying by discrepancy level, but this variation is due principally to the short history differences. An interaction effect (significant at the .03 level) between history and discrepancy shows the mean proportions of neutralized symmetry being directly associated with dis- crepancy for EEQEE history dyads, while the association is the reverse, though less pronounced for long history dyads. Thus, in addition to discrepancy differences on 203 Table 46. Mean proportions of submissive symmetry (++) transactions. Role Discrepancy History . Total Low Medium High Short .05 .03 .04 .04 IP45 Long .05 .04 .04 .05 : g Total .05 .04 .04 .04 . Sum of Mean Sig. Source of Variance DF Squares Square F Value Level 5 - Role Discrepancy 2 .007 .004 .17 n.s. History 1 .013 .013 .57 n.s. Interaction 2 .006 .003 .13 n.s. Within 48 1.098 .023 Total 53 1.124 husband one-down transitory and slight competitive symmetry transaction differences found previously, we find differ- ences on use of neutralized symmetry for short history dyads. A final set of analyses of variance were run on the three general transaction types; symmetry, complemen- ‘tarity, and transitory. The following three tables ;present these data. First, in Table 48, the mean propor- 'tions of the combined symmetrical transactions are given. 204 Table 47. Mean proportions of neutralized symmetry (++) transactions. Role Discrepancy History . Total Low Medium High Short .15 .19 .24 .19 5“”- Long .22 .22 .18 .21 i I Total .17 .20 .21 .20 . Sum of Mean Sig. Source of Variance DF Squares Square F Value Level . , Role Discrepancy 2 .041 .020 .65 n.s. History 1 .023 .023 .74 n.s. Interaction 2 .234 .117 3.77 .03 Within 48 1.482 .031 Total 53 1.780 The mean proportions show differences by discrepancy level, but this association (significant at the .10 level) is mainly the result of EEEEE history dyad differences, the interaction effect is significant at the .03 level. For short history dyads, symmetrical exchanges are proportion- ately more frequent occurring transactions for high, rather 205 Table 48. Mean proportions of symmetrical transactions. Role Discrepancy History r— Total Low Medium High Short .25 .27 .34 .29 Long .30 .32 .30 .30 F" 1 Total .27 .29 .32 .29 ‘ . Sum of Mean Sig. , Source of Variance DF Squares Square F Value Level 5 Role Discrepancy 2 .073 .037 2.31 .10 History 1 .019 .019 1.19 n.s. Interaction 2 .119 .059 3.69 .03 ‘Within 48 .754 .016 Total 53 .965 'than low, discrepancy pairs. From the above comparisons, 1N6 know that this is particularly true on the sub-type, neutralized symmetry. Table 49 gives the mean proportions of complemen- ‘tary transactions. We find no differences, as noted before, lxy discrepancy level on the combined types of complemen- -tarity. 206 Table 49. Mean proportions of complementary transactions. Role Discrepancy History Total Low Medium . High Short .14 .12 .13 .13 Long .11 .12 .12 .12 H Total .13 .12 .12 .12 . Sum of Mean Sig. Source of Variance DF Squares Square F Value Level .LW Role Discrepancy 2 .005 .002 .09 n.s. History 1 .022 .022 .96 n.s. Interaction 2 .023 .011 .48 n.s. Within 48 1.126 .023 Total 53 1.176 The mean proportions of combined transitory trans— action types are given in Table 50. There is a tendency for discrepancy to be inversely related to transitory type transactions. This again is predominately due to short history differences; the interaction effect is sig- nificant at the .10 level. 207 Table 50. Mean proportions of transitory transactions. Role Discrepancy History Total Low Medium High Short .61 .61 .53 .59 Long .59 .56 .58 .58 f“! i'. Total .61 .59 .56 .58 E l . Sum of Mean Sig. Source of Variance DF Squares Square F Value Level r Role Discrepancy 2 .075 .038 2.00 (.15) History 1 .003 .003 .16 n.s. Interaction 2 .091 .045 2.37 .10 Within 48 .931 .019 Total 53 1.100 To explore Question 2 in a less formalized style than analysis of variance, information about transactional usage will also be described by most frequent occurring types. A comparison of the most frequently expressed transaction type (in terms of proportions) was tabulated for each dyad by discrepancy level. frequencies and percentages. Table 51 gives these Comparing discrepancy levels, 208 Table 51. Most frequently expressed transactional pattern by discrepancy level. Transaction Low Medium High Type Frequency % . Frequency ' % . Frequency % ++, ++ 10 63 7 37 7 37 r} ++ 4 25 10 53 6 32 ‘ ++. ++ 2 12 2 10 5 26 ' ++, H 0 00 0 00 1 05 . , Total 16 100 19 100 19 100 Note: A chi-square test on the first three transaction types given agove did not show any significant difference (X = 5.54, d.f. = 4). ‘we see that low discrepant dyads were most characterized Iby one-down transitory transactions and medium discrepancy (dyads by neutralized symmetry, while high discrepancy dyads were characterized by a fairly equal first order usage of 'three types of transactions: one-down transitory, neutral— sized symmetry and one-up transitory. These differences, however, were not significant. A combination of the second most frequently expressed izransaction type with the first, give similar discrepancy patterns as above, but when the third most frequently used 209 transaction types were combined with the first and second, most of the differences across categories disappear because of the high usage of neutralized symmetry and transitory types by all dyads. Question 3: What types of configuration patterns are more characteristic of lower versus higher discrepant “ dyads? A1 In the previous hypothesis—testing section, we found 1 no differences between discrepancy levels and frequency of configurations, that is, of long-run homogeneous transac- la- G‘"_d 37.04.".i- 3~ #1 l tions, and further, that configurational message exchange was a fairly infrequent occurrence. There was an average of 1.5 configurations per dyad across all topics. There is no data base by which to compare frequency of configura- tion of different social systems, but it appears that long- run sequences of the same transactional type is not typical dyadic interaction behavior. This verbal behavior is some- what analogous to Arglye's (1967) studies concerning eye-contact. He found various eye movements related to verbal behavior, but simultaneous eye contact of two inter- actors, was limited. Likewise, with the flow of messages, there are a variety of control movements utilized, but sequentially identical control responses are limited. The previous analyses concerning configurations dealt only with the frequency of occurrence regardless of 'type. Table 52 gives the frequency of type of configuration 210 r M19111: ifil’qu-fli ill-Ill.-lu.l i la L .coHpooHHo Houucoo panama: moumcmHmoo mmmsz 3ouum umHHm "ouoz ow . mm nH mH HH NH 5 m w mH m NH v Hmuoa ooH om am 3 v o m m NH m H m Hm m m o m ,1on 00H mm mm on n v m o no N H H vm h o o H ESHUTE 00H mm om on o m o m mo m H H NH m o N H 30H ucmo mocoaw ucoo Hmuoe ammo Hmuoe ucoo Hmuoa Iuom loam luwm loam ++ ++ ++ ++ snow loom ++ ++ Iuom loom .?+ 4+. ++ , wocmmmuomHQ Hmuoa mHOUHmsmuB auHumucmEonEoo muuoEEwm .Ho>oH xoqmmmuomHo wo mcumuumm coHumusmHmcoo mo mommucwonom HmuothSm can moHocmswmnm .Nm oHnme 211 occurrence for the three discrepancy levels. We see that the frequency of configuration types is very similar to transaction type occurrence. TransitOry configurations are the most common type of long-run exChange sequences, and submissive symmetry the least common configuration pattern. Looking at Table 52 for configuration variations by discrepancy, we note two differences: (1) the lower usage of neutralized symmetry by the low discrepant pairs and (2) the less frequent use of transitory configurations by the high discrepant couples. In order to have large enough cell n's to test any difference in type of con- figuration pattern by discrepancy level it was necessary to combine the cell frequencies of each general type of transaction. A chi-square test was run on the symmetrical, complementary and transitory sub-totals. No significant differences were found (x2 = 3.26, d.f. = 4). Summary of Exploratopy_ Questions Dyads with different levels of discrepancy were found to differ significantly in the prOportion of one-up messages given by wife; the higher the discrepancy, the higher the prOportion of wife one-up movements. There was a tendency for an inverse relation between discrepancy and husband one-down messages. No relationship was found between discrepancy and expressions of one-across control. _ ' . It": ..’:.,_ . " our: 212 In addition to the previous discrepancy and trans- actional findings, several associations were found to exist for §h2£2_history dyads. First, neutralized sym- metry was found to be associated significantly with higher discrepancy. Secondly, when the sub—types of symmetry, complementarity and transitory transactions were combined into these three general categories, symmetry was found to be associated with higher discrep- g ancy, while transitory transactions tended to be related 3- to lower discrepancy. No discrepancy relationship existed for complementary transactions. Describing dis- crepancy levels by the transactional type most frequently expressed, different patterns were found, but they were not statistically significant. First order use of one- down transitory transactions characterized the low dis- crepancy pairs, while high discrepancy couples had no single first order pattern. Concerning configuration patterns, about an equal number of long-run configuration sequences occurred by discrepancy level. For higher discrepancy dyads, symmetri- cal configurations were a bit more prevalent and transitory configurations a bit less prevalent, but so significant difference by discrepancy were found. Table 53 gives an outline of these exploratory question findings. 213 Table 53. Summary of exploratory question findings. Difference Significance l. Message-Control (a) one-up for wife: (b) one-down (c) one-across 2. Transaction Types (a) submissive symmetry (b) neutralized sym- .metry for short history: 3. General Transaction Types (a) complementarity (b) Symmetry (c) transitory 4. Configuration Patterns (a) type high discrep. > low discrep. high discrep. > _._&¢u%discrep. high discrep. > low discrep. particularly for short history high discrep. < low discrep. particularly for short history .10 ___ 0' ‘ 1 . I interaction=.03 .10 interaction=.03 (.15) interaction=.10 214 Transactional Examples To illustrate the relational communication pat- terns manifested by dyadic systems of different discrep— ancy levels, several discussion examples expressed by transactional control sequences are presented below. All the examples deal with the discussion of emergency I preparedness. ‘1 The first set of graphs given in Figure 6, show the transactional patterns of a high, medium, and low discrepant level dyad. Looking first at the overall 1 sequencing pattern, we see that the general flow of con- trol in the high discrepant dyad's interaction (#356) is one-across towards one—up, in the medium discrepant dyad (#152) it is one-across towards one-down, and in the low discrepant dyad (#132) there is more fluid mix of control movements with considerably less one-across symmetry (26 percent neutralized symmetry compared to 52 percent for the high dyad and 41 percent for the medium dyad). In the high discrepancy dyad, both members "move" upward, but there is no escalation; in the medium discrep- ancy dyad it is the wife that moves downward. Typical of the patterns found for the low discrepant dyads, it is the husband of dyad #132 that moves one-down, particularly in transitory transactions. A comparison of transitory types shows one-up transitory transactions outnumbering one-down transitory exchanges in the high discrepant dyad .momao wocommnomHo 30H can EdHomE .omHn mo monouumm HmcoHuommcmuB .m musmHm AMHOpmHm unosmv mocmmouomHo 30H NMH¢ coho ‘4‘44 ‘ _. AhuoumHm mcoqv mocmmouomHo ESHUmS mmH# town . C“. . 215 panama: mMHz n O HmuoumHm uncomv wocmmmuomHo anm mmmw pawn 216 (eight to three), while one—down transitory movements cunnmber one—up movements for both the medium (nine to six) and low (fifteen to ten) discrepant dyads. The second set of graphs presented in Figure 7, show several patterns that were found to be characteristic differences between short and long history high discrepant dyads and high versus low discrepant systems. The high discrepant dyadic transactional examples in Figure 7 (dyads #336 and #172), show more variation in control movements than the previous high discrepant dyad example in Figure 6, but there are still noticeable series of symmetrical transactions, much more than in the low discrepant dyad (#282). Further comparisons show not only sequences of symmetrical transactions being more characteristic of the two high discrepant dyads, but also symmetry of response in general. Symmetrical transac- tions, occurring singularly or in a series, account for 29 percent of all transactions in dyad #336, 35 percent in dyad.#172 versus 21 percent in dyad #283, the low 218 significantly more one-across, one-across symmetrical transactions than other dyads, while there was a tendency shown for long history-high discrepant dyads to manifest one-up symmetry. The short history-high more one-up, discrepant dyad, #336, has 14 percent more neutralized (++) symmetry than competitive (M) symmetry, while the long history-high discrepant dyad, #172, has a reverse ratio of 16 percent more competitive symmetry than neu- tralized symmetry. Additional patterns found to be characteristic of high versus low discrepancy dyads are also manifested in the examples given. Both the high discrepant dyads have more wife one-up movements than in the low discrep- ant dyad. Further, in the two high dyads the one-up messages of the husband are less than the wife's, while in the low discrepant dyad this ratio is reversed with the husband giving more one-up messages than the wife. Also, we find one-up transitory transactions occurring more frequently than one-down transitory exchanges in the two high discrepant dyads, and again, this pattern is reversed for the low discrepant dyad with one-down transitory exchanges occurring more frequently than one-up transitory transactions. 7One of the configuration patterns shown in Figure 7 is of a competitive symmetrical nature for dyad #172. 219 The above examples were presented as illustrations of the major differences found in the relational communi- cation patterns of intimate dyadic systems of different discrepancy and history levels. U.- I 'i-?¢I‘s.s-‘. ’-..I CHAPTER VI SUMMARY AND IMPLICATIONS This study was designed to investigate the rela- tionship between system inequity and communication behavior in a dyadic setting. Discrepancy between role expectations and perceived role behavior was the major independent variable which was related to self—report descriptions of communication behavior and a relational communication analysis of dyadic discussions. Under these two types of communication measures the major research results of this study are given. This general summary is followed by a discussion of the implications of the study and suggestions for future research. Self-Report Description Summary Time and Talk Allocations .An overall estimate of the time that husbands and wives spent together on a typical Sunday was six hours, for a typical weekday it was four hours. Of the time couples are together on a weeday, they report an average of one hour spent alone with one another. 220 221 Husband and wife pairs married a shorter length cm'time (one to eleven years) spend more time together both alone and with others present than couples married longer (twelve to 26 years). Dyads with low role discrep- ancy were found to spend significantly larger portions of time together on a Sunday (a nonwork day) than higher discrepant dyads. For both weekday and Sunday time allocations, an emergent pattern was for members of long history-high discrepant dayds to report less time spent together than other dyad types. In relationships outside the husband-wife system, wives report spending approximately five hours a day with at least one, or more of their children; husbands report spending about an hour. Wives spent on the average about one hour with people other than family mem- bers; husbands spend about three hours outside the family system. Of the estimated six hours that couples were together on a Sunday, they reported on the average, that two hours were spent in conversation with one another. Of the four hours together on a weekday they reported (Hue hour was spent talking and listening to one another. Usixn; these estimates, the "average" couple spends a tuytal of eight to nine hours a week talking to one another. .‘I'. a’vTir-Ll— $7157” JF immfZaFfrai 1 .._J 222 Dyadic history and discrepancy analyses of talk- time allocations found (consistent with time allocations) that short history dyads spend more time talking to one another than long history dyads and low discrepancy dyads spend more time in conversation than do high dis- crepancy dyads. Members of long history-high discrepancy dyads reported the least amount of talk—time; the polar opposite, short history—low discrepancy dyad members reported the higher amounts of talk-time. Discussion Frequency and Communication Control Lower discrepant dyads were found to talk signi- ficantly more often about a variety of topics than higher discrepant dyads. The frequency of discussing work activities and feelings toward one another were particu- larly distinctive differences between the lower and higher discrepant couples. Short history dyads were also found to talk more often about various topics than long history dyads, particularly about financial matters. .Again, it was observed that members of dyads characterized by short history and low discrepancy had the more frequent topic discussions and those characterized by long history and high discrepancy had the least frequent topic discus- sions. 223 An analysis of communication control by discrepancy lewfls did not reveal differences among dyads, except for ‘Uuatendency of lower discrepant dyads to report more equalldiscussion of poth_members' interests, while high discrepant dyads reported more discussion of either their own or their spouse's interests rather than both of their interests. Desired Talk-Time, Communication and Marital Satisfaction All dyad members desired more talk—time than less talk-time. As predicted, wives desired more talk-time than husbands and members of short history, more than long history dyads, reported a desire for more talk-time, but in both cases the differences were not significant. It was hypothesized and found that higher discrepancy dyads desired significantly more talk-time (on a Sunday) than lower discrepancy dyads. Further predictions found to be significant were that lower discrepant dyads were more satisfied with 'their communication and marital relationship, than higher .role discrepancy dyads. 224 Relational Communication Analysis Summary Message Type and Control Direction Assertions and extensions, singularly or together were clearly the most frequently used type of message format and response. The second most frequently used message type was assertion—support statements, while successful talk-overs as extensions were the third most frequent message types utilized. Other forms of exten- sion made up the next three most frequent types of messages. It was predicted that support messages and talk- overs would differ by discrepancy level. Lower discrep- ant dyads were found to exchange significantly higher proportions of support messages than higher discrepant dyads. With talk—overs, it was discovered that dis- crepancy was related to unsuccessful talk-overs. Lower discrepant dyads had significantly more unsuccessful talk-overs than higher discrepant pairs. For total talk-overs and unsuccessful talk-overs an interaction effect between dyadic history and dis- crepancy differences were more pronounced for ghogt history than long history dyads. Remembering the control directions of different message types, it is consistent with the above statements to find that the largest proportion (about half) of the 225 total messages were of a one—across neutralizing control nature. For the remaining messages a little more than one-fourth were one—down messages and slightly less than one-fourth were one-up control messages. Across dyads, husbands expressed slightly more one-up messages than wives, and wives gave slightly higher proportions of one- down messages than husbands. However, when we look at message control by discrepancy level, we find different patterns of one-up and one—down directional differences. There was a tendency for discrepancy to vary directly with one-up control and inversely with one-down message control. Thus, while higher versus lower dis- crepant dyadic systems tended to have higher proportions of one-up messages, it was found that this was particu- larly so for wives. Higher discrepant wives sent a higher proportion of one-up messages than any other cate- gory, and significantly higher proportions than lower discrepant wives. Further, there was a tendency for lower discrepant husbands to express higher proportions of one-down messages than higher discrepant husbands. One-across messages were not found to be related to dis- crepancy. Transactional Activity and Flexibility Lower levels of discrepancy were hypothesized to be related to higher levels of interaction activity, as 226 measured by length of discussion period, amount of silence during the discussions and the total number of transac- tions. Only limited evidence of support were found. A fairly strong tendency existed for lower discrepant couples to have more transactions, i.e., more speaker turn over, than higher discrepant couples. This was particularly so for short history dyads. Also, a tendency was found for discrepancy levels of short history dyads to vary directly with amount of silence was found, but no differences existed between discrepancy levels and length of discus- sions. It was further predicted that lower discrepancy would be related to more random usage of transactional types. No significant variations from randomness were found by discrepancy levels. Transaction Types Transitory transactions accounted for almost sixty percent of all transactions. There were more one— down transitory exchanges than one-up. Approximately thirty percent of the transactions were symmetrical with neutralized symmetry accounting for the largest propor- tion. Submissive symmetry, followed by competitive symmetry, were the least frequent occurring types of transactions (both about five percent). A little more than ten percent of the transactions were complementary. 227 When transaction types were compared by discrep- ancy levels, no variations in complementary exchanges were found. However} symmetrical and transitory trans— action patterns did vary with discrepancy level. First, at the broad level of analysis, referring to all combined sub—types of symmetry and transitory transactions, higher levels of discrepancy were related to higher proportions of symmetry and lgwgg proportions of transitory exchange. Both relationships were particu- larly evident for the short history dyads. At the more specific level, analyzing each sub- type separately, there was a tendency for higher propor- tions of competitive symmetry, to be expressed in higher discrepant dyads. With neutralized symmetry, there was a significant discrepancy difference for ghorg history dyads with higher proportions of neutralized symmetry found to be expressed in higher discrepancy systems. There were no differences in the use of submissive symmetry. The analysis of transitory types of discrepancy found that dyads did not differ significantly on one-up transitory exchanges but that they did vary on one-down transitory type transactions, particularly when the hus- band was one-down. These differences again were more pronounced in short history dyads. 228 Configuration Patterns There was a low rate of occurrence of configura— tion patterns (long—run sequences of identical transac- tions) in the dyadic discussions. There were no significant differences between discrepancy levels and configuration sequences. The tendencies displayed for lower discrepancy dyads to have less symmetrical and more transitory configurations and the reverse for higher dis- crepancy dyads, parallel the transactional differences by discrepancy level discussed earlier. Implications Major Conclusions The present research was an application of the general system cybernetic principles to the study of dyadic interaction processes. Inequity (role discrepancy) was the main system characteristic under study. It was defined as the perceived variance from distributive justice and was hypothesized to be related to the self- governing homeostatic dynamics of ongoing systems. The basic research question was: Do systems of different levels of inequity strain manifest behavior, particularly communication behavior, that is predictable from a homeo- static model based on the principle of an adaptive capacity to stabalize. Taking into account the exploratory nature 229 of this area of research and the recent development of the research instruments, it appears that the directional and statistical support found for many of the predictions, and the way in which those findings “fit“ together, give ample evidence that the systems approach has sound poten- tial as a theoretical base for the study of interaction systems. One important consideration emerging from the research needs to be made. It was noted both in the analysis of the self- report data and in the relational analysis that many of the research hypotheses had stronger prediction power for short history dyads than long history dyads. It was suggested in the text that the homeostatic principles may operate differently depending on the length of the system's history. In particular, history differences were most noted in the high_discrepancy categories. The following comments are highly intuitive, but an overriding impression of the author was that Horney's (1945) classic delineation of interaction styles is descriptive of the history-discrepancy differences found. The interaction style of the low discrepant couples (both short and long history, but more evident for those with short history) seemed to "go towards" one another (e.g., spent larger amounts of time together, talked to one another more—-about personal matters, gave more support, had fewer symmetrical exchanges). The short 230 history-high discrepant couples appeared to "go against" one another (e.g., high allocations of time spent together and of talk—time, but lower satisfaction with communica- tion relation, least support, more symmetry), while the 122g history-high discrepancy dyads seemed to "go away" from one another (e.g., spent less time together, less time spent talking, lower communication satisfaction, least satisfaction with marriage relationship). The suggestion arising from the various analyses is that when system inequity is present, short history dyads may exemplify patterns of confrontation and long history dyads may exemplify patterns of avoidance. Homeostatic-based hypotheses appear to be more predictive of the former than the latter style of accommodation. A continued effort toward development of improved indicators of system strain will offer increased oppor- tunity to assess the value of the homeostatic model. One suggestion for improvement is to utilize a behavioral measure to tap this dimension more effectively than the self-report type of measurement used in this study. On the basis of the present research findings, an example of such a measure might be the ratio of support to non- support messages manifested in the transactions. This type of measurement has the advantage of being based on actual behavior output of the system, rather than individual reporting of the system state. Thus, it is 231 a measure that is potentially less affected by a public or halo effect and also, and very importantly, it is more of a systems level measurement. One aspect of this study that is of considered importance is the attempt to move from the more frequent monadic level of analysis to a dyadic level. It is felt that with the transactional communication analysis this attempt was successful, but hopefully a first step toward higher level analyses, both in terms of moving toward triadic and larger system studies and in moving toward higher levels of abstractions of interaction patterns. As indicated earlier, this shift is conceptu- ally and methodologically difficult, but necessary for studying system processes. The relational analysis technique, developed and first utilized in the family communication project of which this research is a part, does appear to have value at the general descriptive level of message type and control, but as indicated above, to have particular value in describing communication behavior and processes at the transactional level. Further, the emergent com- munication pattern profiles of high versus low discrep- ant dyads--with higher discrepancy related to fewer support statements, more wife one-up control movements, fewer husband one—down transitory transactions, fewer unsuccessful talk-overs and more symmetry give evidence 232 for the diagnostic potential of the relational communica- tion analysis. The present research is a beginning effort to relate system states and communication patterns. Whether the behavior styles found are interpreted as arising from perceived system inequity, or as modes of behavior which promote the perception of system inequity, patterned variations of interaction were observed among "normal" dyads of different system states. If, in future research the types of dyads and the dimensions of sys- tem states analyzed are expanded, the potential compari- sons of relational communication patterns of different systems, at different states, will increase the diagnos- tic possibilities. Improved diagnostic skill is an integral aspect of the present social concern with the enrichment of interpersonal relations. Research Suggestions A variety of additional suggestions for future research are possible, e.g., analyze the data by topics, instead of across topics, include more nonverbal dimen- sions in the analysis, expand the research beyond the dyadic system, etc. These all have merit; however, only the more specific suggestions that arose in the design- ing and carrying out of this research will be mentioned 233 here. These will deal with the relational communication analysis. First, and considered the most important sugges— tion, is the consideration of analyzing transaction pat— terns not only by speaker order, but by control order of the messages making up the transaction. In this manner, more specific sequential information can be obtained. For symmetrical transactions order of control, of course, I 1 is no problem, but in the analysis of complementarity and ! transitory types of exchange, essential process informa- g tion is missed by a noncontrol ordered transaction matrix. L Thus, instead of having one axis represent one speaker and the other axis represent the other speaker, have one axis indicate the first control direction of a trans- action and the other the second control direction of the tranSaction. This type of summarization gives the flow of direction for all nine transactional types. Also, this method of analysis would allow utilization of the relational control analysis with groupings larger than the dyad. Two further suggestions for modification and expansion of the relational analysis scheme are: (1) an intensity measure and (2) a time dimension of message control of transactional exchanges. As experience with the relational analysis technique is developed and know- ledge of its utilization expanded, further and more 234 complicating additions may be added to increase the infor- mation obtained from its application. A measure of control intensity is possible by viewing messages as having more than equal units of one-up and one-down movements. For example, an instruc- tion might be considered one unit of onewupness and an .051“; order two units of one—upness. For one—down, giving '2 anu‘ifi - 7" approval might be an example on one unit of movement downward, while asking for approval or assistance, two units of downward movement. P 9.1 BA\ mal-o. finnv 1 The addition of a time dimension to control movements is another suggestion for increasing information concerning transactional processes. This point can be made clearer by referring to the graphic presentations that were made in the last chapter. The examples given considered all messages as equivalent units, so that the graphing of the transactional control movements is much like a musical score of identical notes. Graphs of timed control movements would be analagous to a musical score ‘with a variety of whole notes, mixed with quarter and half time notes, etc. This would require large expendi- ‘tures of energy, but the potential transactional discov- eries would seem to merit this experimentation. A concluding thought--or warning--is that any :study of transactional communication processes will 235 require heavy investments of time and involvement. Yet with research efforts oriented toward an understanding of social interaction, it is essential to focus on the relational aspects of communication. As Duncan so aptly stated, "We do not relate and then talk, but we relate in talk. . ." (1967, p. 249). r‘ APPENDICES APPENDIX I INTRODUCTORY LETTER )4" fl MICHIGAN STATE UNIVERSITY EAST LANSING . MICHIGAN 48823 COLLEGE OF COMMUNICATION ARTS . DEPARTMENT OF COMMUNICATION 0 CABLE: COWDBPT March 6, 1972 A study is now underway concerning family communication. We are interested in learning how husbands and wives talk about differ- ent issues that come up in their daily lives. In order to gain a better understanding of family Opinions, we are asking many families to participate in this study. We would like your per- mission to talk with both you and your spouse, if you have children under twelve years of age. Couples who are willing to participate in this study will receive ten dollars ($10) for their time. This study is being done by the Department of Communication at M50. The findings will be placed in a report so that no indi- vidual's or family's views can be identified. We want to assure you that your Opinions will remain totally confidential. In order for the findings to show a true picture of different families' vieWpoints, your participation is very important to us. You are part of a random sample and your cooperation is essential to the value of this study. If you have any questions about the study, feel free to ask the person bearing this letter, or contact me directly at my office phone, 517-355-3478. I think you will find this study interesting and you will be making an important contribution to the understanding of families. Sincerely, Richard V. Farace Director of the Family Communication Project 524 South Kedzie Hall RFV:jo 236 APPENDIX II SELF-REPORT QUESTIONNAIRE INSTRUCTIONS This study consists of two parts. First, we would like you to fill our a questionnaire on family communication patterns. In the second part, we would like you and your spouse to discuss a few topics. PART 1: The questionnaire is to be filled out by each of you. Please do not consult your spouse about any of the questions in the questionnaire until you have completed it. After you have completed your questionnaire, please place it into the en- velope, seal it, and give it to the interviewer. PART 2: When both of you have completed the questionnaire, you will be given three topics to discuss. Two deal with emergency situations and the other deals with a family topic. We would like you to take about ten (10) minutes to talk about the possible alternatives for each topic, and decide what you and your family would do in each situation. In case you have any questions about any part of the questionnaire, please feel free to ask the interviewer about them. WE APPRECIATE YOUR COOPERATION IN THIS STUDY AND HOPE THAT YOU WILL FIND IT INTERESTING! 237 FAMILY QUESTIONNAIRE 239 The following questions concern the time you spend with other people during the course of a day. We would like you to give a rough estimate of the time you spent, yesterday, with the pe0ple listed below. For each question, please check the response that best estimates this amount of time. 1. How much of your time yesterday during waking hours was spent . . . a. with your spouse? Count the time you were with one another even though others might have been present. ___we didn't spend any time together 1 to 30 minutes 30 minutes to 1 hour 1 to 2 hours 2 to 4 hours 4 to 6 hours 6 to 8 hours __pmore than 8 hours b. with just your spouse? we didn't spend any time '_—' alone 1 to 30 minutes 30 minutes to'l hour 1 to 2 hours 2 to 4 hours 4 to 6 hours 6 to 8 hours more than 8 hours lllll c. with just one or more of your children? __pwe didn't spend any time alone 1 to 30 minutes 30 minutes to 1 hour 1 to 2 hours 2 to 4 hours 4 to 6 hours 6 to 8 hours more than 8 hours 240 d. with people other than your spouse and children? I wasn't with anyone else 1 to 30 minutes 30 minutes to 1 hour 1 to 2 hours 2 to 4 hours 4 to 6 hours 6 to 8 hours more than 8 hours 2. a. When you and your spouse were together yesterday, how much time did you spend in conversation? Count the time actually spent talking and listen- ing to your spouse. we didn't spend any time 1 to 30 minutes 30 minutes to 1 hour 1 to 2 hours 2 to 4 hours 4 to 6 hours 6 to 8 hours ___more than 8 hours b. How much time yesterday would you have liked to have talked with your spouse? much more somewhat more‘ about the way it was somewhat less much less I 3. Is the amount of time you spent talking with your spouse yesterday typical of most week days? yes--If yes, go to Question 4. no-- If no, please estimate the amount of time you spend talking with your spouse on a typical weekday. we don't spend any time 1 to 30 minutes 30 minutes to 1 hour 1 to 2 hours 2 to 4 hours 4 to 6 hours 6 to 8 hours more than 8 hours 241 4. Since weekends may be different from weekdays, please estimate the amount of time you spent last Sunday . . . a. with your spouse? we didn't spend any time ___ together 1 to 30 minutes 30 minutes to 1 hour 1 to 2 to 4 to 6 to ___more 2 hours 4 hours 6 hours 8 hours than 8 hours b. in conversation, talking and listening, to your spouse? we didn't spend any time 1 to 30 minutes 30 minutes to 1 hour l to 2 to 4 to 6 to ___more 5. How much time last Sunday, would 2 hours 4 hours 6 hours 8 hours than 8 hours you have liked to have talked with your spouse ? much more somewhat more about the same somewhat less much less I 6. Was the amount of time you spent talking with your spouse last Sunday typical of most Sundays? yes--If yes, go to Question 7. no-- If no, please estimate the amount of time you spend talking with your spouse on a typical Sunday. we don't spend any time 1 to 30 minutes 30 minutes to 1 hour 1 to 2 hours 2 to 4 hours 4 to 6 hours 6 to 8 hours more than 8 hours ‘4“; ’"1". 242 7. The previous questions dealt with time estimates. Now we are interested in the general kinds of topics you and your spouse talk about. Please indicate how often you and your spouse talk about each of the following topics by checking the appropriate columns. Once Once or Once or Lessthan Never a day twice twice once a talk or more a week a month month w; a. your job(s) {2 b. your feelings to- i ward each other g c. your children ? d. family leisure : time activities u e. care of the home f. financial matters 9. friends h.’ relatives 1. local community events e.g., church, P.T.A., elections, committees, etc. j. national and inter- national events K. PLEASE SPECIFY OTHER TOPICS BELOW 243 What just you and your spouse talk, who starts most of the conversations between you? My spouse does more than I I do more than my spouse Each of us about the same Don't know, can't say When just you and your spouse talk, how much of the time do you talk in comparison to him/her? Much more 3 Somewhat more 9 About the same E Somewhat less ‘ Much less When just you and your spouse talk, whose interests or concerns do you talk about? g __;Mainly talk about what my spouse wants to discuss __JMainly talk about what I want to discuss '___Split about evenly between his/her interests and mine When just you and your spouse talk, who usually ends or stops the conversation? ___My spouse usually ends them ___I usually end them ___Split about evenly between us, depends on topic __;Usually ended by outside interference or interruptions 9. In general, how satisified are you with the communication between you and your spouse? Very satisfied Somewhat satisfied About as satisfied as dissatisfied Very dissatisfied Somewhat dissatisfied 244 10. When husbands and wives talk to one another different kinds of feelings may result. Listed below are some of the ways that you may have felt when talking with your husband. Please indicate with the appropriate number (l-S), how often you have had the feeling mentioned in each of the statements. 1 2 3 i 4 5 Always Often Now and then Seldom Never a. In a conversation with my husband, I am uncomfor- table during a period of silence. b. I am satisfied with our ability to talk things out together. c. My husband does not listen to me when I'm talking. d. I find it difficult to express my true feelings to my husband. e. I know my husband's feelings and emotions from his gestures and facial expressions. f. I avoid talking about certain subjects with my hus- band because it may be unpleasant to us. 9. When we're talking, my husband understands me and how I feel. h. My husband encourages me to express my concerns. i. I can anticipate what my husband is going to say before he says it. j. My husband's manner of speaking is irritating. k. My husband lets me know how he feels about what I'm saying. 1. In our conversations, I don't understand how my husband feels. m. I find other people more interesting to talk to than my husband. n. It's easy to talk to my husband about any problem or complaint. 0. I feel dissatisfied with my husband's ability to express his feelings and emotions in words. *For husband's form of questionnaire, the word "husband" was replaced with "wife." 245 11. Each of us receives information from many sources. We are interested in how you learn about different types of events. Listed below are several ways you may have learned about them. Please check all your sources of information for the following types of events (e.g., school events, community events, etc.) You may have more than one answer for each event. SCHOOL COMMUNITY NATIONAL & NATURAL & i EVENTS EVENTS' INTERNATIONAL CIVIL DEFENSE SOURCES OF . EVENTS ‘ PREPAREDNESS INFORMATION I n i P1 Co-workers ‘ Neighbors . i j 5 My Spouse ' ‘ My Children Newspapers/ . , Magazines ' ' L”, TV or radio Delivered in Mail Never received 12. Now, considering just your own immediate family, who tends to bring the most information about these same events to the attention of the other family members. FAMILY MEMBER SCHOOL COMMUNITY NATIONAL & NATURAL & WHO BRINGS MOST EVENTS EVENTS INTERNATIONAL CIVIL DEFENSE INFORMATION EVENTS PREPAREDNESS My Spouse Myself My children All of us about the same None of us 13. 246 Earlier, we mentioned "delivered in mail" as one way of learning about events. We are now interested in finding out who looks at, and what your family does with the third class mail (Like advertisements, public information announcements) that is delivered to your house. a. In your family, who is usually the first person to look at this type of mail? Advertisements Public Information (store ads, magazine (School events, community notes, ads, special offers) civil defense literature, etc.) My spouse My spouse Myself Myself My children My children No one in particular No one in particular b. Who else in the family looks at it? Advertisements Public Information My spouse My spouse I do I do My children My children No one in particular; No one in particular; depends on material depends on material c. What is usually done with this type of mail? Advertisements Public Information Thrown away immediately Thrown away immediately Kept for a short time Kept for a short time Kept for future reference Kept for future reference d. How often is this material discussed with other members of the family? Advertisements Public Information Often Often Seldom Seldom Never Never 14. 247 Each family works out its own way of doing things. We would like to ask you about how certain things are done in your family. Please indicate, by using the number of the appropriate response below (l-6), how you and your spouse divide up some of the family jobs. 1 2 3 ‘ 4 5 6 Husband Husband Both Wife Wife Neither Almost More than About the More than Almost One Always Wife Same Husband Always a. Who does the grocery shopping? b. Who prepares the meals? c. Who repairs things (appliances, furniture, toys) around the house? d. ___Who disciplines the children? e. ___Who gets up at night, if necessary, with the children? f. ___Who helps the children with their homework? g. ___Who decides on the family budget? h. ___Who makes complaints, if necessary, to salesmen, service repairmen or landlord? i. ___Who selects the family car(s)? j. ___Who plans what to do on a Saturday night? k. ___Who decides what people you will invite to the house? 1. __4Who keeps in touch with relatives? m. ___Who shows affection for the other spouse? n. Who takes the initiative to make up when there's been a disagreement? 0. Who tries to see the other's point of View when there is a difference of opinion? 15. 248 We would now like to ask you how you feel these family jobs should be done. In your family, who do you think should be responsible for doing the following things, regardless of whether that person actually does them or not. Please indicate below, by using the number of the appropriate response (1-6), your own preference as to how these jobs should be divided up between yourself and your spouse. 1 2 3 4 _ 5 6 Husband Husband Both Wife Wife Neither Almost More than About the More than Almost One Always Wife Same Husband Always a. Who do you think should do the grocery shopping? b. Who do you think should prepare the meals? c. Who do you think should repair things (appliances, furniture, toys) around the house? d. Who do you think should discipline the children? e. Who do you think should get up at night, if necessary, with the children? f. Who do you think should help the children with their homework? g. Who do you think should decide the family budget? h. Who do you think should make complaints, if necessary, to salesmen, service repairmen or landlord? i. Who do you think should select the family car(s)? j. Who do you think should plan what to do on a Saturday night? k. Who do you think should decide what people you will invite to the house? 1. Who do you think should keep in touch with re- latives? m. Who do you think should show affection for the other spouse? n. Who do you think should take the initiative to make up when there's been a disagreement? 0. Who do you think should try to see the other's point of View when there is a difference of Opinion? 16. 249 Now, we would like to ask you to react to a number of pairs of statements about things that you may or may not like; about ways in which you may or may not feel. This is not a test. It is an attempt to find out how people feel about themselves. Therefore, there are no right or wrong answers. For each pair of statements, choose the statement you think best describes the way you feel. CIRCLE the letter of that statement. Some choices may be difficult; nevertheless, choose the one that best describes how you feel. (If neither state- ment accurately describes how you feel, choose the one which you consider to be less inaccurate). For example, in the sample item below you might choose item (a) if you feel that statement describes you best. Example: (1) @ I like to tell amusing stories and jokes at parties. b I would like to write a great novel or play. Please select one statement in each pair by circling the letter of the one you choose. (1) a I feel timid in the presence of other people I regard as my superiors. b I like to supervise and to direct the actions of other people whenever I can. (2) a I like to be called upon to settle arguments and disputes between others.\ b I like to avoid responsibilities and obligations. (3) a I like to tell other people how to do their jobs. b I feel like getting revenge when someone has insulted me. (4) a I like to show a great deal of affection to- ward my friends. b I like to be regarded by others as a leader. (5) a I like to sympathize with my friends when they are hurt or sick. b I like to be one of the leaders in the organ- izations and groups to which I belong. (6) a When with a group of peOple, I like to make the decisions about what we are going to do. b I like to predict how my friends will act in various situations. (7) (8) (9) (10) (ll) (12) (l3) (14) (15) (16) a 250 I like to put in long hours of work without being distracted. I like to be regarded by others as a leader. I like to tell other people how to do their jobs. I like to ask questions which I know no one will be able to answer. I get so angry that I feel like throwing and breaking things. I like to tell other people how to do their jobs. When serving on a committee, I like to be appointed or elected chairman. I would like to write a great novel or play. I like to be one of the leaders in the organ- izations and groups to which I belong. I like to be able to do things better than other people can. I like to do things in my own way without re- gard to what others may think. I like to supervise and to direct the actions of other people whenever I can. I like to be called upon to settle arguments and disputes between others. . I like to be regarded as physically attractive by those of the Opposite sex. When serving on a committee, I like to be ap- pointed or elected chairman. When I am in a group I like to accept the leadership of someone else in deciding what the group is going to do. I like to be able to persuade and influence others to do what I want to do. I like to think about the personalities of my friends and to try to figure out what makes them as they are. I like to keep my letters, bills, and other papers neatly arranged and filed according to some system. I like to be one of the leaders in the organ- izations and groups to which I belong. (17) (18) (19) (20) (21) (22) (23) (24) (25) (26) U'fl’ 251 I like to write letters to my friends. I like to argue for my point of view when it is attacked. I like to be called upon to settle arguments and disputes between others. I like my friends to do many small favors for me cheerfully. When with a group of people, I like to make the decisions about what we are going to do. I like my friends to sympathize with me and to cheer me up when I am depressed. I like to be regarded by others as a leader. I like to keep my letters, bills, and other papers neatly arranged and filed according to some system. I like to do things with my friends rather than by myself. I like to argue for my point of view when it is attacked by others. When I am in a group, I like to accept the leadership of someone else in deciding what the group is going to do. I like to supervise and to direct the actions of other people whenever I can. I like to argue for my point Of view when it is attacked by others. I like to experience novelty and change in my daily routine. I like to tell other people how to do their jobs. I like to be the center of attention in a group. I feel depressed by my own inability to handle various situations I like to be able to persuade and influence others to do what I want. I like to engage in social activities with persons of the Opposite sex. When with a group of peOple, I like to make the decisions about what we are going to do. 252 (27) a I like to be able to persuade and influence others to do what I want. b I like to finish any job or task that I begin. (28) a When serving on a committee, I like to be ap- pointed or elected chairman. b I like to try new and different jobs--rather than to continue doing the same Old things. Finally, we would like to ask you just a few more questions about yourself. 17. What is your age? years 18. Please circle the last grade completed in schools. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 ll 12 l 2 3 4 l 2 3 4 5 6 Grade School High School College Graduate School 19. What is your present occupation? 20. Approximately, what is your family's yearly income? less than $3,000 $3,000 to $5,999 $6,000 to $7,999 $8,000 to $14,999 over $15,000 21. How long have you been married? . years 22. Is this your first marriage? yes no 23. How would you describe your satisfaction with your marriage? 1 2 3 4 5 Very Somewhat Somewhat Satisfied Satisfied Satisfied Dissatisfied Dissatisfied 6 Very Dissatisfied 24. How satisfied do you think your spouse is with your marriage? ‘ l 2 ‘ 3 4 5 Very Somewhat Somewhat Satisfied Satisfied Safigsfied Dissatisfied Dissatisfied Very Disatisfied 253 25. What about your marriage do you like the least? 26. What about your marriage do you like the most? THANK YOU AGAIN FOR YOUR COOPERATION APPENDIX III DISCUSSION TOPICS 254 Topic 1 To begin with, talk with each other about how you first met, dated and decided to marry. Topic 2 Imagine it is a typical weekday and at least one of you is at home. A Civil Defense alert--a loud wailing siren-— is sounded. After turning on the radio, you learn that there has been a nuclear attack. You are warned to prepare for the radiation fallout which will reach your area in about two hours. Your neighbors have also heard the warning and need to find shelter. (A) If your house has a cellar or basement, would you be willing to share it with your neighbors during the disaster? (B) If you don't have a basement, would you be willing to go to someone else's house for protection? Please discuss the possible alternatives and decide on a plan Of action. Topic 3 Some married couples feel that both the husband and the wife should be able to have independent careers, jobs and interests outside the family. Other couples feel that both should be devoted to the interest of the family, and that the wife in particular should be in the home as a full time homemaker and mother. What are your feelings on this matter and what is your joint conclusion? 255 Topic 4 Imagine a tornado watch has been in effect for several hours in your area. You, your spouse, and your children are at home one evening watching television, when a tornado warning is broadcasted indicating the sighting of a tornado. You are told that you have about 15 minutes to prepare and take shelter. What would you and your family do in this situation? Please discuss the possible alternatives and decide on a plan of action. APPENDIX IV INTERVIEWER INSTRUCTIONS INTERVIEWER'S INITIAL INTRODUCTION If you recall, you or your husband/wife were inter- viewed by phone about six weeks ago from Michigan State University concerning your Opinions about a number of topics. We sincerely appreciated your cooperation in that phase of the study. From the large group of persons we spoke to by phone, we have selected a smaller group to represent them in a more meaningful study of family communication patterns. We hope that you and your wife/husband will cooperate further by participating in this part of the study. This letter will explain more about the study . FIRST-- Greet the person who answers the door. Ask for the male or female head of the household. Introduce yourself by name and say you are working for Mich- igan State University (East Lansing, Michigan -- Department of Communication). 256 257 RESPONDENT DIRECTIONS QUESTIONNAIRE (To be given orally by interviewer) First, we'd like you to fill out this questionnaire. There's a separate one for each of you. Please wait until after you've finished the questions to talk about them. Perhaps you'd be more comfortable for writing if you sat at the table (if you see an "eating table" readily available). I'll be glad to answer questions for you, if there is any- thing puzzling about the questionnaire. If they complain that it will take too long or looks too thick: Most of these pages only require you to check Off choices. There's almost no writing involved, so it doesn't take a long time to complete. If they complain that some Of the choices are difficult to £2321, I know it's difficult to make some Of those choices, but don't spend too long on any of them; just choose the one that describes your feelings pegp, or the one that seems least wrong. 258 INSTRUCTIONS TO RESPONDENTS Discussion Topics (to be given orally by the interviewer) Thank you for your help with the questionnaires. Would you like to take a short break or should we go on to the discussion? We'll handle each discussion topic separately. I'll introduce the tOpic and we'll briefly discuss it to make sure you have no questions. If there are no questions, I'll start the tape and you may begin talking to each other. While the tape is playing, my attention will be centered on the recording equipment to make sure it's working properly, so I probably won't be looking at either of you. Please discuss each topic in some depth--as fully as possible--but we'll have to stop you at ten minutes. (START TOPIC I) 259 I To begin with, talk with each other about how you first met, dated and decided to marry. Some things you might include in the discussion are: 1. Your first impressions or reactions. 2. Your expectations. 3. Your later feelings. 4. Any problems you encountered. 5. Any solutions you worked out. PROBES--to be used only when essential: a. when asked for direction, b. when there has been 15 seconds of silence, less than five minutes discussion AND one of the five discussion areas has been ne- glected. 1. Tell each other about the problems you may have had. 2. Talk about how you felt during this time. 3. Consider how you handled any problems you had. 4. Try to talk about your first impressions. 5. Discuss any expectations about marriage. "it” NA 1! a .-r_' ' .‘u Iva-1"). ". am 260 II Imagine it is a typical weekday and one of you is at home. A Civil Defense alert--a loud wailing siren--is sounded. After turning on the radio, you learn that there has been a nuclear attack. You are warned to prepare for the radiation fallout which will reach your area in about two hours. Your neighbors have also heard the warning and need to find shelter. (A) If your house has a cellar or basement, would you be willing to share it with your neigh— bors during the disaster? (B) If you don't have a basement, would you be willing to go to someone else's house for pro- tection? Please discuss the possible alternatives and de- cide on a plan of action. Some things you might include in the discussion are: 1. Your first impressions or reactiOns in this situation. 2. Your expectations. 3. Your later feelings. 4. Any problems you encountered. 5. Any solutions you worked out. PROBES--to be used only when essential: a. when asked for direction b. when there has been 15 second of silence, less than five minutes discussion AND one of the five discussion areas has been ne- glected. 261 Talk about any other problems you might en- counter. Tell each other how you might feel in this situation. Consider how you could try to handle any pro- blems you might encounter. Discuss what your first reaction to this situation would be. Try to consider what you might expect to happen under these conditions. III Some married couples feel that both the husband and the wife should be able to have independent careers, jobs and interests outside the family. Other couples feel that both should be devoted to the interest of the family, and that the wife in particular should be in the home as a full time homemaker and mother. What are your feelings on this matter and what is your joint conclusion ? Some things 1. you might include in the discussion are: Your first impressions or reactions to this topic. Your expectations. Your later feelings. Any problems you encountered. Any solutions you worked out. PROBLES--tO be used only when essential: a. when asked for direction, 262 b. when there has been 15 seconds of silence, less than five minutes discussion AND one Of the five discussion areas has been ne- glected. 1. Tell each other your expectafions regarding this topic. 2. Talk about how you feel about this topic. 3. Consider any problems that might develop in such a situation. 4. Discuss any solutions to such problems. 5. Try to talk about your first reaction to or impression of this topic. IV Imagine a tornado watch has been in effect for several hours in your area. You, your spouse, and your children are at home one evening watching television, when a tornado warn- ing is broadcasted indicating the sighting of a tornado. You are told that you have about 15 minutes to prepare and take shelter. What would you and your family d9 in this situation? Please discuss the possible alternatives and decide on a plan of action. Some things you might include in the discussion are: 1. Your first impressions or reactions in this situation. 2. Your expectations. 3. Your later feelings. 4. Any problems you encountered. 5. Any solutions you worked out. 263 PROBLES--to be used only when essential: a. when asked for direction, b. when there has been 15 seconds of silence, less than five minutes discussion AND one of the five discussion areas has been ne- glected. Talk about any other problems you might en- counter. Tell each other how you might feel in this situation. Consider how you could try to handle any pro- blems you might encounter. Discuss what your first reaction to this situation would be. Try to consider what you might expect to happen under these conditions. 264 Interviewer Instructions Discussion Topics Try to have respondents comfortably seated, at corner- wise chairs in a livingroom, or at a table in an eat- ing area. If it is possible to have a clock visible to the respondents, it would be ideal. Deal with each topic separately. Hand only one dis- cussion topic to the respondent at a time. Spend no more than ten minutes per topic. Stop the topic discussion at the end of that time. Say something like; "I'm sorry folks, the time is up; and we don't want you to have to spend extra time on this, so we'll go on to the next topic now." Do not turn on the recorder until both respondents have had a chance to: a. Read the tOpic from their own copy; b. Listen to you emphasize the underlined phrases; c. Ask any questions they may have about the topic; d. Listen to you direct them regarding some of the general things to consider in their discussion Of the tOpic. Attempt to stay out of their discussion, unless you are asked a question by one of the, or there is a silence of over 15 seconds. Use eye contact only when giving a standard probe for the particular discussion topic. Use the standard probes only when asked, or after about 15 seconds silence, when one of the five general areas Of consideration has not yet been mentioned by respon- dents; and the discussion has not lasted at least five minutes. If the respondents are engaged in a discussion, let them continue. Make no attempt to pull them back to the five areas of consideration, unless they are silent and have not used all the areas. Some of what they say may sound irrelevant. Do not attempt to stop this sort of conver- sation. Following each topic's discussion, give some sort Of ver- bal reward to the respondents, such as,"Thank you, that was fine;" or "You've both done a good job." APPENDIX V DISCUSSION TIME BY TOPIC 265 mm H aa m wm OH mo H OHQOB ow N mm A no «a HH m unmoe mm N mm A em NH ma H canoe mm m mm m ma ma «m a canoe mOnOOOm mmpdcaz .Omm .cflz .Omm .OHE .Omm .sflz coflumw>molcnmocmum mfifielsmwz OEwBIEdEflxmz OEHBIEOEHCHS Demos ha mafia coflmmsomfln com: UGO wmcmm APPENDIX VI SECOND AND THIRD DIGIT CODE CATEGORY EXAMPLES 266 Second digit code category examples: 1. Assertion: Any complete referential statement. "I'm 10 years old. It's cold. Yes. I'm going. I don't know. I'll get it. You're the one who has to decide. It's going to be one of those nights. We couldn't find it. He was here yes- terday." 2. Question: Any comment punctuated with a "?." "How old are you? OK? Right? What do you think? Why don't you got? If you're so smart, why don't you do it? Well, what will we talk about? Do you listen?" 3. Talk-over: Any codable interruption, i.e., a verbal intervention made while other is talking. A talk- Over can take any grammatical form. 4. Noncomplete: A phrase or incomplete sentence. "Well, ah . . . But, what I . . . Hmmmm . . . You are . . . Now then . . . But what I really thought was . . .It's like this, we're going to . . . That was in . . . ah. . .Let's see if . ." 5. Other: All messages that do not fit in the previous four categories. ~ Third digit code category examples: 1. Support: Any message that facilitates the other interactor via assistance, agreement, encouragement, or seeks Opinion, approval, etc. Nonquestion form: "Yes. I agree. OK I'll help? That's a great idea you had." Question form: "What do you think? Could you help me with that? Can I come too? Are you OK?" 2. Nonsupport: Any message that Opposes the other via resistance, rejection, disagreement, demand, chal- lenging, etc. Nonquestion form: "NO, I don't like it. I won't do it. You did not. I didn't say that. I bet I can do it better than you can." 267 Question form: "Where the hell have you been? You disagree so--what's you View? Will you stand still? Are you crazy?" Extension: A message that continues the flow or theme (not always the precise topic) of preceding message; or a noncommittal response, like "I don't know," or "I can't remember." Answer: A response with knowledge, firmness, sub- stance that follows a question. F“ "Yes. NO. He's ten. It's two miles. I'm going home. I want to go to the movies. You have to add water, then Oil." Instruction: A statement that is a qualified sug- gestion involving clarification, justification and/ or explanation. "I think we should go. It would be best if you did it now, because it's got to be done sometime. I really ought to. You shouldn't do that, you'll get hurt." - Note: If an order is qualified by a question or "other," it is coded as an instruction, e.g., "You go get it, OK?" Order: A statement of command, involving no or little explanation. "Get the paper. Go on to the next one. Don't do that. Come on--right now. You do it." Disconfirmation: A response that disregards the demands of the previous message. (Are you going?) "The cat's eyes are green." (What should we do about Johnny?) "It's a little cloudy, might even rain." Topic change: A message that switches the flow or theme of the preceding message. (The baby's learning to walk now.) "Where tonight's paper?" (I bought a new dress.) "The neighbors sure don't take very good care Of their lawn." 268 Initiation: The first message to start a dialogue, or if dialogue has been brought to closure, the message that starts it up again. "Hey, that's a good one. Well, what do you think? To start off with, I think we need to consider what the inputs are." Termination: A message that brings a dialogue to a close. "That's about it. That does it. I gotta go now. There's no more to say." Other: Any message with an unclassifiable response mode. APPENDIX VII TRANSCRIBING PROCEDURES INFORMATION FOR TYPISTS/TRANSCRIBERS General Procedures Type dyad number from tape and topic number (1, 2, 3, 4) in appropriate places on coding form. Listen to Topic One (1) on tape in order to distinguish speakers and their style of interaction. After listening to T0pic One, rewind tape and, using the coding form, begin typing the interaction when the first member Of the dyad speaks. Identify speaker as Husband (H) or Wife (W) by placing the appropriate code letter in the SPEAKER column of the coding form. NOTE: On some of the tapes, the inter- viewer gives a probe or message designed to extend the discussion. You will be given a sheet which indicates those discussions that were probed. Probes will be in the form of suggestions ("You might discuss. . .") or requests ("Could you discuss. . .") and will include one or more of the following points: a. Your first impressions or reactions b. Your expectations c. Your feelings d. Any problems e. Any solutions Identify the interviewer giving one Of these probes with an (I) by placing this code in the SPEAKER column. When Topic One is transcribed, rewind tape and compare transcription with the actual interaction; correct any errors in the transcribed copy. When Topic One transcriptions is complete and accurate, proceed to other topics on tape utilizing the same general procedures. 269 270 Rules for Specifying Messages For each person, a message represents everything he or she says until the other person speaks. Include all interviewer probes as messages, but please exclude mes- sages spoken by children. Type messages accurately as heard, including grammatical errors. For those words that involve mispronunciations, determine from context what word was intended and give its correct spelling. Indicate questions by a question mark; pay attention to inflection patterns of a speaker's voice to determine the interrogative form. "Talk-Overs" are messages initiated by the second speaker while the first is already speaking. A "successful" talk-over is one where the first person stops talking and allows the interruptor to speak. For this situation, the last words spoken by the first speaker prior to be- ing interrupted should be followed by three periods (. . .). In addition, the interruptor's message should be placed within brackets [ ] to indicate that it was a successful talk-over. If, after the interruptor com- pletes his message, the first speaker continues his previous thought, it should be introduced by three periods. A "nonsuccessful" talk-over is a message ini- tiated by the second speaker while the first speaker is talking and does not stop talking or relinquish the floor. In other words, when the second person talks over, both speakers continue to talk at the same time. If distinguishable, the interruptor's message should be placed within parentheses ( ) to indicate that it was an unsuccessful or nonsuccessful talk-over. If the in- terruptor's message is not distinguishable, NONDISTINGUISH- ABLE should be typed within the parentheses. The following examples illustrate the typing format for these two types of talk-overs. Successful Talk-Over H: We would probably leave the radio on and--uh--tune in W: [We'd probably use our transistor radio.] H: . . .uh, the local warnings and weather forecasts. 271 Nonsuccessful Talk-Over H: I think we'd go to the closest public shelter and W: (We don't even know where it is.) H: Stay there until the all clear sounded. Pay attention to silence. For any period of silence that is at least three (3) seconds, type the word SILENCE in the middle of the line following the preceding mes- sage. If the silence runs an additional five (5) seconds, type another SILENCE below the first. For every addition- al five second periods of silence, follow the same pro- cedure. For any message that is not distinguishable, identify the speaker and type NONDISTINGUISHABLE in place in place of the message. For a part of a message that is not dis- tinguishable, type as much of the message that is recog- nizable and where it becomes unclear or garbled, type NONDISTINGUISHABLE. For example, the former situation would be typed as follows: H: I suppose I would come from work as soon as I could get out. W: NONDISTINGUISHABLE H: Yeah, I guess that's what I'd do. The latter situation would be typed as follows: W: One of us would have to go to the school to check with the teacher and the principal. H: It depends on who it was who accused him of doing NONDISTINGUISHABLE W: We'd sure check up on that teacher. APPENDIX VIII INSTRUCTIONS FOR CODERS RELATIONAL COMMUNICATION CODING SCHEME DIGIT ONE DIGIT TWO DIGIT THREE (Speaker) (Format) (Response) 1 = Wife 1 = Assertion l = Support 2 = Husband 2 = Question 2 = Nonsupport 3 = Talk-Over 3 = Extension 4 = Noncomplete 4 = Answer 5 = Other 5 = Instruction 6 = Order 7 = Disconfirmation 8 = Topic change 9 = Initiation- Termination 0 = Other Silence is coded by a 000 code for every period of silence longer than three seconds. Every additional five second period Of silence is also coded as 000. DO NOT CODE LAUGHTER ************************ ****** To illustrate how this scheme may be used to categorize messages, a sample from an interaction is shown below: Husband: But then I don't remember what'd I say to him? 223 Wife: You explained the wrong ways and the right ways and childbirth and other things. ll4 Husband: Oh, yah, I remember that . . . let's see . . . 211 Wife: Over in E. Lansing they already have a program in reproduction so he really already knew the right way. 113 272 273 The husband's first message is coded 223 tO indicate a ques- tion of extension that seeks information about the tOpic be- ing discussed. The wife's first message is coded as 114 to show that it is an assertion that serves as an answer. The husband's second message is a supportive assertion and the wife's second message is an assertion Of extension. DIGIT TWO "Assertion" "Question" "Talk-Over" Definitions of Code Categories Any completed referential statement that may be in either the declarative or imperative form; i.e., a message that has a subject and a verb. Also, words such as "yes," "no," "sure," "Hmmm—mmm," "Uh-uh," and "right" which clearly indicate a control function. Any statement that takes an interrogative form. Any interruption or verbal intervention made while the other person is speaking. "Noncomplete" Any utterance, other than those coded as "talk- "other" DIGIT THREE "Support" overs," that are initiated but are not completed. Any utterance that is indistinguishable or is not classifiable as to form. Any statement giving or seeking acceptance, agreement and/or approval. "Nonsupport" Any statement or utterance that is a disagreement, "Extension" "Answer" rejection, demand, and/or challenge. Any statement that continues the flow or theme of the preceding message, or any statement that is a noncommittal response to a question. Any statement that is a definitive response to a question or at least has substance and/or commitment. 274 "Instruction" Any statement that is a regulative response to a in the form of a suggestion; it is Often qualified or contains an explanation. "Order" Any statement that is a regulative response but is in the form of an unqualified command with little or no explanation.. "Disconfirmation" Any statement that ignores or by-passes the request of the other individual. "Topic Change" Any response that has little continuity with previous messages and no response continuity was requested. "Initiation- Termination" Any statement that either begins or ends an interaction; its use signifies starting and ending points for particular discussions or conversations. "Other" Any response that is unclear or unclassifiable. Priority Considerations for Using Categories DIGIT TWO 1. First, determine if the message should be considered as a talk-over (#3). A talk-over may be a question, as- sertion, or noncomplete, but an interruptive speech is coded as a talk-over, independent of form. 2. If not a talk-over, the message should be considered as a question (#2). 3. If not a question, it should be considered as an asser- tion (#1). 4. If not a question, it should be considered as a noncom- plete (#4). 5. If it cannot be categorized as any of these, it is coded as "other" (#5). For instance, any message or partial message that is, or follows, an "INDISTINGUISHABLE" or "NONDISTINGUISHABLE." 275 DIGIT THREE 1. The first consideration should be whether the message initiates or terminates. (Although messages coded in this category can also be coded in other categories-- e.g., as an answer, a nonsupport, or a tOpic change--, the major consideration should be whether the message is an initiation or a termination.) 2. If the message is not an initiation-termination, it should be considered as an answer (#4). The main re- sponse function Of an answer is to provide information. However, an answer which gives support or nonsupport should be coded according to its control function. 3. If the message is a response switch, it should first be considered a disconfirmation(#7). 4. If the message is not a disconfirmation, it should be considered a topic change (#8). 5. If the message has a regulative function, it should first be considered as an order (#6). 6. If the message is not an order, it should be coded as an instruction (#5). 7. If the message is not codable in any of these categories, it should be considered an extension (#3), a message of support (#1), or a message Of nonsupport (#2). NOTE: Coding Of any of these three categories should follow this priority scheme; i.e., any indecision be- tween the three should result in a code of (#3), ex- tension. 8. If a message cannot be categorized as any of these, it is coded as an other (#0). Any message or partial message that is, or follows, an "INDISTINGUISHABLE" or "NONDISTINGUISHABLE " should be coded as an other. Coding of Dual Response Messages As shown on page one, each message by a speaker is coded with a three digit number. In most cases, these messages will have only a three digit code to indicate who was speaking, what form the message was, and what the message was in res- ponse to the other speaker's previous message. In some cases, however, a speaker's message may serve more than just one response mode; it may represent support as well as being 276 of a regulative nature. In these situations, such a message will receive at least two three digit numbers to categorize its different response functions. For example, in the follow- ing sample interaction, the second speaker's message illustrates a situation in which his message is an agreement or support and also a question seeking information. Speaker 1 (Wife): I think we should leave now. 115 Speaker 2 (Husband): Okay, I guess so, but what about bringing the food? 211-223 Speaker 1 (Wife): Well, we'll have to come back for it. 114 In the next sample interaction, the second speaker's message shows both support and nonsupport Of the first speaker's previous message. Speaker 1 (Husband): I think we should leave now. 215 Speaker 2 (Wife): Okay, I guess so, but I don't like you always deciding what time we leave. 111-112 Speaker 1 (Husband): That's too bad! 212 Sample Interaction The following example represents the form of interaction that you will possibly code and illustrates both single and dual response messages. Wife: That's a very interesting situation, but I really don't know how I'd handle it, what about you, honey? ll9-l23 Husband: Well, I think that in the case of an emergency we would probably first tune in the radio and listen for directions and at the same time begin to gather up some food . . . 214 Wife: [. . . and clothing . . . ] 133 Husband: . . . to the basement . . . 243 Wife: [We' d have to take blankets . . .] 133 Husband: . . . where we'd . . . 243 Wife: [. . .because we'd need the warmth in the basement and besides the children would need it.] 133 Husband: Right, that sounds about right. 211 In this example, the wife's first message receives two codes-- a 119 to indicate an assertion that, arbitrarily, is defined as starting the interaction, and a 133 to show a question that 277 seeks to extend the discussion Of the topic. The husband's first message is an assertion that serves as an answer. The next message from the wife is enclosed in brackets to indicate- a successful interruption and is coded 133 to indicate a talk-over in extension. The husband's continuation Of his original thought is coded 243 to indicate a noncomplete that is an extension. The wife again comes in with a talk-over in extension and the husband tries to Complete his original statement. The wife again successfully talks over and com— pletes her idea. Finally, the husband makes an assertion that supports his wife's previous message. In some situations, messages which begin as assertions (or talk-overs) may end as questions, or vice versa. For example, note the following interactions: Wife: After we met, you asked me to come visit you, right? 113-123 Husband: Yeah, then, uh, we went home to visit my folks. 211-213 The wife's message is double coded to indicate that she kept the discussion going (113) but then sought information agree- ment for what she had said (123). The husband provided agreement (211) and then continued the flow of the conversa- tion(213). Words that might frequently turn assertions into questions are "Okay?", "right?", and "huh?". Husband: And what would you do? I think you should get some blankets, flashlight, and water together, open all the windows and get the kids to the base- ment. 223-215 Wife: Shouldn't I also bring some food? Well, oh wait, I could bring some graham crackers for the kids. 123-113 Here, the husband asked a question (223) but didn't allow his wife to answer. Instead, he followed his question with an instruction (215). The wife responded with a question (123) and then followed that with an assertion in extension (113), rather than waiting for her husband to answer her question. In some cases, one person may have an unsuccessful talk- over followed by another message. For example: Wife: I could carry the metal buckets from the garage down to the basement and fill them with water from the water heater. 113 Husband: 278 (They'd be too heavy for you. You'd need . . .). One of the boys would have to help you carry them or you'd trip all over yourself. 232-212 Here, the wife had a message that continued the previous discussion (113), the husband had an unsuccessful talk-over in disagreement or nonsupport (232), and then followed with a statement that was also a disagreement with what the wife had said. DIGIT TWO Assertion Question Talk-over Code Category Examples - "I think, uh, we should talk about the public shelters." - "It's going to depend on several things." - "You're the one who has to decide." - "I'll get it." - "How Old are you?" - "Right?" - "Did you listen?" - "What do you think?" - Any message or partial message enclosed within brackets [ ] or parentheses ( ) that is a verbal interruption. These messages may take any grammatical form but will be iden- tified by the brackets or parentheses. NOTE: In some cases, a series of three messages may be coded as follows: H [It would probably be at least two hours] W (What about . . .) H [before we could come out of the basement.] In this sequence, the husband had a success- ful talk-over, indicated by the [ ], the wife had an unsuccessful talk-over while the hus- band was talking, indicated by the ( ), and the husband's message was continued, as in- dicated by the [ ]. Any message within a [ ], like any message enclosed within regular brackets [ ], is considered a successful talk- Over. Any message contained within a [ ] is coded as an Assertion or Noncomplete in Ex- tension and is 22E coded as a talk-over in Extension. Noncomplete Other DIGIT THREE Support Nonsupport Extension Answer Instruction 279 "Well, ah . . ." Mmm . . ." "But what I really thought . . ." "Umm. ." ". . . and besides, uh, we, uh . . .""Ah. ." All messages that do not fit in the previous four categories. QUESTION FORM - "Are you Okay?" "What do you think?" - "Could you help me with that?" - "Can I come too?" ASSERTION FORM "Yes, I agree." "Yeah." "Hmm-mm." - "Okay, I'll help." "Right." - "That's a great idea." "Sure." QUESTION FORM "Why would you want to do something stupid like that?" - "Are you crazy?" - "I suppose you're smarter?" ASSERTION FORM "I don't like it." "uh-uh." - "We won't do it." - "That's ridiculous." "Furthermore, its the best way to do it." "I don't know." . (NOTE: In order to be coded as an extension, a message has to continue the flow or theme of a preceding message, or be a noncommittal response to a question.) "It's forty miles to the nearest public shelter." "You have to add water, then oil." (NOTE: An answer is a definitive response to a question.) "I think we should go." "You have school tomorrow and its time you went to bed." "You shouldn't do that because you'll get hurt." "You go get it, okay?" Order Disconfirmation Topic change Initiation Termination Other 280 "Close the door." "Go on to the next one." "Don't do that." "You do it." - (Are you going?) "It's fourteen above in Kalamazoo." . - (What should we do tomorrow night?) "It's raining outside." - (NOTE: Disconfirmations represent messages that disregard requests or demands Of pre- vious messages.) (The baby's learning to walk now.) "Where's tonight's paper?" (I bought a new dress.) "Hey, guess who I saw today at work?" (We're eating late tonight.) "Let's gO to the hockey game on Saturday." "Well, to start off with, I think we need to consider what the conditions are." "We would probably go to the basement right away." (NOTE: An initiation is any message that starts a dialogue or discussion.) "That's about it." "There's no more to say." (NOTE: A termination is any message that brings a dialogue to a close.) Any message with an unclassifiable response mode, i.e., messages or parts of messages labeled as "INDISTINGUISHABLE" or "NONDISTIN- GUISABLE." BIBLIOGRAPHY BIBLIOGRAPHY Ackerman, N. W. The ngchodynamics of Family Life. New York: Basic Books, 1958. Adams, J. S. "Inequality in Social Exchange." In Berkowitz, L. (ed.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology. Vol. 2. New York: Academic . Press, 1965, pp. 267-299. Adler, L. E. and Enelow, A. J. "A Scale to Measure Psychotherapy Interaction." Unpublished Manuscript, University of Southern California, 1964. Amidon, E. J. and Flanders, N. The Role of the Teacher in the Classroom. Minneapolis: Amidon and AssoEiates, I963. Amidon, E. J. and Hough, J. B. (eds.). Interaction Analysis: Theory, Research and Application. Reading, Mass.: Addison-Wesley, 1967. Argyle, M. The Psychology oflInterpersonal Behaviour. Middlesex, England: Penguin Books, Ltd., 1967. . Social Interaction. New York: Atherton Press, 1969. Bales, R. F. Interaction Process Analysis: A Method for the Study of Small Groups. Reading, Mass.: Addison—Wesley, 1950. . "How People Interact in Conferences." Scientific American, 192, 1955, 31-35. and Borgatta, E. F. "Size of Group as a Factor in the Interaction Profile." In Small Groups: Studies in Social Interaction. A. P. Hare, E. F. Borgatta, and R. F. Bales (eds.). New York: Knopf, 1955, 395-413. Bateson, G. Naven. 2nd ed. (first published in 1936). Stanford: Stanford University Press, 1958. 281 282 Becker, J. Systematic Sociology on the Basis of the Beziéhungslehre and Gebildelehre of Leopold von Wiese. New Yofk: WIley, I932. and Useem, R. H. "Sociological Analysis of the Dyad." American Sociological Review, 7, 1942, 13-26.' Berkowitz, L., and Howard, R. C. "Reactions to Opinion Deviates as Affected by Affiliation Need and Group Member Interdependence." Sociometry, 22, 1959, 81-91. Berlo, D. K. "Essays on Communication.“ Unpublished Manuscript, Department of Communications, Michigan State University, 1970. Bertalanffy, L. von. "General System Theory--A Critical Review" General Systems, VII, 1962, 1-20. Bishop, B. M. "Mother-Child Interaction and the Social Behavior of Children." Psychol. Monogr., 11:65, 1951, 1-43. Blumstein, P. W. and Weinstein, E. A. "The Redress of Distributive Injustice." The American Journal of Sociology, 74, 1969, 408-418. Bodin, A. M. Family Interaction, Coalition DiSagreement and Compromise in Problem, Normal and Synthetic Family Triads. Unpublished Ph.D. Dissertation. State University of New York at Buffalo, 1966. Borgatta, E. F. "A Systematic Study of Interaction Process Scores, Peer and Self-Assessments, Personality and Other Variables." Genetic Psychology Monographs, 63, 1961, 3-65. and Cottrell, L. S. "On the Classification of Groups." Sociometry, 18, 1955, 665-678. ; Cottrell, L. S.; and Meyer, H. J. "On the Domensions of Group Behavior." Sociometry, 19, 1956, 223-240. and Guerrin, R. F. "The Two Person Group: Some Notes on Theory and Research." Sociology and Social Research, 45, 1960, 3-13. 283 Borke, H. "The Communication of Intent: A Systematic Approach to the Observation of Family Interaction." Human Relations, 20, 1967, 13-28. . "The Communication of Intent: A Revised Procedure for Analyzing Family Interaction from Video Tapes." Journal of Marriage and the Family, 31, 1969, 541—544. Bossard, J. H. "Low of Family Interaction.“ American Journal of Sociology, 50, 1956, 292-294. Buckley, W. Sociology and Modern Systems Theory. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice-Hall, 1967. Burgess, E. W. "The Family as a Unity of Interacting Personalities." Family, 7, 1926, 5. Burr, W. R. "An Expansion and Test of a Role Theory of Marital Satisfaction." Journal of Marriage and the Family, 33, 1971, 368;372. Caputo, D. V. "Parents of the Schizophrenic." Family Process, 2, 1963, 339-356. Carr, L. J. "Experimental Sociology: A Preliminary Note on Theory and Method." Social Forces, 8, 1929, 63—74. Carter, L. F.; Haythorn, W.; Meirowitz, B.; and Lanzetta, J. "The Relation of Categorizations and Ratings in the Observation of Group Behavior." Human Relations, 4, 1951, 239-254. Cattell, R. B. "Concepts and Method in the Measurement of Group Syntality." Psychological Review, LV, 1948, 43—63. . "New Concepts for Measuring Leadership in Terms of Group Syntality." Human Relations, 4, 1951, 161-184. Chapple, E. D. "Measuring Human Relations: An Introduc- tion to the Study of Interaction of Individuals," Genetic Psychology Monography, 22, 1940, 3-147. . "The Interaction Chronograph: Its Evolution and Present Application." Personnel, 25, 1949, 295-307. 284 Cheek, F. E. "The Schizophrenogenic Mother in Word and Deed." Family Process, 3, 1964, 155-177. Corsini, R. "Multiple Predictors of Marital Happiness." Marriage and Family Living, 18, 1956, 240—242. Cronbach, L. J. and Meehl, P. E. "Construct Validity in Psychological Tests." Psychological Bulletin, 52, 1955, 281-302. I' DeGre, G. "Outline for a Systematic Classification of Social Groups." American Sociological Review, 14, 1949), 145—148. Deitcher, S. "Development of Interspousal Verbal Com- munication of College-Educated Couples." Unpub- lished Ph.D. Dissertation, Cornell University, 1959. Denzin, N. K. "Rules of Conduct and the Study of Deviant Behavior: Some Notes on the Social Relationship." In G. T. McCall (ed.), Social Relationships. Chicago: Aldine, 1970, 62-94. Duncan, H. D. "The Search for a Social Theory of Com— munication in American Sociology." In F. Dance (ed.), Human Communication Theory. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston, 1967, 236-263. Durkheim, E. The Division of LaDor in Sociepy. Trans. by G. Simpson, Glencoe, 111.: Free Press, 1893. Duvall, E. M. Family Development. Chicago: Lippincott, 1957. Dymond, R. "Interpersonal Perception and Marital Happi- ness." Canadian Journal of Psychology, 8, 1954, 164-171. Ericson, P. M. "Relational Communication: Complementarity and Symmetry and Their Relation to Dominance- Submission." Unpublished Ph.D. Dissertation, Michigan State University, 1972. Farina, A. "Patterns of Role Dominance and Conflict in Parents of Schizophrenic Patients." Journal of Abnormal and Social Psyghology, 51, 1960, 31-38. 285 Ferreira, A. J. "Decision—Making in Normal and Patho— logical Families." Archives of General Psychiatry, 8, 1963, 68—73. and Winter, W. D. "Family Interaction and Decision Making.“ Archives Of General Psychiatry, 13, 1965, 214—223. and Pointdexter, E. J. "Some Interactional Variables in Normal and Abnormal Families." Family Process, 5, 1966, 60—75. Festinger, L. "Informal Social Communication." Psycho- logical Review, 57, 1950, 271—282. and Thibaut, J. "Interpersonal Communication in Small Groups." Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 46, 1951, 92—99. Fisher, 8.; Boyd, I.; Walker, D.; and Sheer, D. "Parents of Schizophrenics, Neurotics, and Normals." Archives of General Psychiatry, 1, 1959, 149-166. Flanders, N. "Interaction Analysis in the Classroom: A Manual for Observers." Unpublished Manuscript, 1960. Framo, J. L. "Systematic Research on Family Dynamics." In I. Boszormenyl-Nagy and J. Framo (ed.), Intensive Family Therapy, Chapter 11.. New York: HOEBer Medical DivisiOn, Harper and Row, 1965. Goffman, E. "The Nature of Deference and Demeanor." Interaction Ritual. Garden City: Doubleday and Company, Inc., 1967. Goldman-Eisler, F. "A Study of Individual Differences and of Interaction in the Behavior of Some Aspects of Language in Interviews." Journal of Mental Science, 100, 1954, 177—197. Golembiewski, R. T. ThetSmall Group. Chicago: Univer- sity Of Chicago Press, 1962. Gross, N.; Mason, W. S.; and McEachern, A. W. Ex lora- tions in Role Analysis. New York: John Wiley and Sons, 1958. Haberstron, C. J. "Organization Design and Systems Analysis." In J. March (ed.) Handbook of Orgagiza- tions. Chicago: Rand McNally, 1965, p. 1173. 286 Haley, J. "Family Experiments: A View Type of Experi- mentation." Family Process, 1, 1962, 265-293. . "Marriage Therapy." Archives of General Psy- chiatry, 8, 1963, 213—224} . "Research on Family Patterns: An Instrument Measurement." Family Process, 3, 1964, 41-65. . "Speech Sequences of Normal and Abnormal Families with Two Children Present." Family Process, 6, 1967, 81-97. Hall, A. D. and Fagen, R. E. "Definition of System." General Systems, I, 1956, 18—28. Hawkins, J. L. and Johnsen, K. "Perception of Behavioral Conformity, Imputation of Consensus, and Marital Satisfaction." Journal of Marriage and the Family, 31, 1969, 507-511. Hemphill, J. K. Situational Factors in Leadership. Ohio State University: EducatiOnal Research Monograph NO. 32, 1949. . Group Dimepsions: A Manual for their Measure- ment. Ohio State University: Bureau of BuSiness Research Monograph, 1956. Hess, R. and Handel, G. Family Worlds. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1959. Hill, R.; Back, K.; and Stycos, J. M. "Intra-Family Communication and Fertility in Puerto Rico." Rural Sociology, 20, 1955, 258-271. Homans, G. C. The Human Group. New York: Harcourt, Brace and World, 1950. . "Social Behavior as Exchange." The American Journal of Sociology, LXII, 1958, 597-606. . Social Behavior: Its Elementary Forms. New York: Harcourt, Brace and World, 1961. Horney, K. Our Inner Conflicts. New York: Norton, 1945. Hurvitz, N. "The Marital Roles Inventory and the Measure- ment of Marital Adjustment." Journal of Clinical Psychology, 16, 1960, 377-380. 287 Jackson, D. D. "The Study of Family." Family Process, 4, 1965, l-20. . "Family Interaction, Family Homeostasis and Some Implications for Conjoint Family Psycho- therapy." In J. H. Masserman (ed.), Individual and Famil Dynamics. New York: Grune and Statton, Inc., 1959, 122-141. Jahoda, M.; Deutsch, M.; and Cook, S. W. Research Methods in Social Relations, Vol. I. New Yofk: Dryden Press, I955. Kenkel, W. F. "Observational Studies of Husband-Wife Interaction in Family Decision-Making." In M. B. Sussman (ed.) Source Book in Marriaga and the Famil . Boston: Houghton Mifflin, 1963, 144-156. Kolar, S. :"Middle—Class Marital Role Perceptions and Marital Adjustment." Sociology and Social Research, 49, 1965, 283-293. Komarovsky, M. Blue-Collar Marriage. New York: Random House, 1962. Leary, T. Interpersonal Diagnosis of Personality. New York: Ronald,i1957. Lennard, H. L.; Beaulieu, M. R.; and Embrey, N. G. "Interaction in Families with a Schizophrenic Child." Archives of General Psychiatry, 12, 1965, 166-183. Lidz, T.; Cornelison, A.; Fleck, S.; and Terry, D. "The Intrafamilial Environment of Schizophrenic Patients: II. Marital Schism and Marital Skew. American Journal of Psychiatry, 114, 1957, 241-248. . "The Intrafamiliar Environment of Schizophrenic Patients: VI. Transmission of Irrationality. Archieves of Neurological Psychiatry, 79, 1958, 305-316. Luckey, E. B. "Marital Satisfaction and Congruent Self- Spouse Concepts." Social Forces, 34, 1960, 153-157. . "Perceptional Congruence of Self and Family Concepts as Related to Marital Interaction." Sociometry, 24, 1961, 234-250. 288 Mangus, A. "Family Impacts on Mental Health." Marriage and Family Living, 19, 1957, 256—262. Mark, R. A. "Parameters of Normal Family Communication in the Dyad." Unpublished Ph.D. Dissertation, Michigan State University, 1970. Markwell, G., and Hage, J. "The Organization of Role- Relationships: A Systematic Description." American Sociological Review, 35, 1970, 884—900. McCall, G. J. "The Social Organization of Relationships." In G. J. McCall (ed.) Social Relationships. Chicago: Aldine, 1970, 3-34. , and Simons, J. L. Identities and Interactions. New York: Free Press, 1966. Mishler, E. and Waxler, N. "Interaction Sequences in Normal and Schizophrenic Family Triads: Appli- cations of Multivariate Informational Analysis." Unpublished Manuscript. Boston: Massachusetts Mental Health Center, 1969. Ort, R. S. "A Study of Role—Conflicts as Related to Happiness in Marriage." Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, XLV, 1950, 691-699. Parsons, T. The Social System. New York: Free Press, 1951. - Peak, H. "Problems of Objective Observation." In L. Festinger and D. Katz (eds.) Research Methods in the Behavioral Science. New York} Dryden Press, 1953, 243—300. Rainwater, L.; Coleman, R.; and Handel, G. Workingman's Wife. New York: MacFadden Books, 1962. Redfield, R. The Folklgulture of Yucatan. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1941. Rogers, L. E. "Social Exchange: Basic Issues." Unpub- lished Manuscript. Department of Communication, Michigan State University, 1971. Rollins, B. C. and Feldman, H. "Marital Satisfaction Over the Family Life Cycle." Journal Of Marriage and the Family, 32, 1970, 20-28. 289 Rosenfeld, H. "Instrumental Affiliative Function of Facial and Gestural Expressions." Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 4, 1966, 65'72 o Ruesch, J. and Bateson, G. CommunicatioanThe Social Matrix of Psychiatry. New York: Norton, 1951. Rychoff, J.; Day, J.; and Wynne, L. "Maintenance of Stereotyped Roles in the Families of Schizophre- nics." Archives Of General Psychiatry, 1, 1959, 93'980 Safilios-Rothchild, C. "Family Sociology or Wives' Family Sociology? A Cross—Cultural Examination of Decision—Making." Journal Of Marriage and the Family, 31, 1969, 291-292. Satir, V. Conjoint Family Therapy. Palo Alto: Science and Behavior BOOks, 1967. Schachter, 8. "Deviation, Rejection and Communication." Journal of Abnormal Social Psychology, 46, 1951, 190-207. Simmel, G. The Sociology of Georg Simmel. Glencoe: Free Press. Translated by K. Wolff, 1950. Smardan, L. "An Exploratory Study of Communication." Unpublished Ph.D. Dissertation, Cornell University, 1957. Sluzki, G. E. and Beavin, J. "Simetriay Complementari- did: Una Definicion Operacional y una Tipologia de Parejas." Acta Psiquidtrica y Psicologica de America Latina, 11, 1965, 321-330. Soskin, W. F. and John, U. P. "The Study of Spontaneous Talk." In R. G. Barker (ed.), The Stream of Behavior. New York: Appleton-Century Crofts, 1963, 228-281. Steinzor, B. "The Development and Evaluation of a Measure of Social Interaction." Human Relations, 2, 1949, 103-122. Straus, M. A. "Communication, Creativity and Problem- Solving of Middle and Working Class Families in Three Societies." In M. Sussman (ed.), Source- book in Marriage and the Family, 3rd edition. Boston: Houghton Mifflin, 1968, pp. 15-27. 290 Strodtbeck, F. L. "Husband-Wife Interaction over Revealed Differences." American Sociological Review, 16, 1951, 468-473. . "The Family as a Three Person Group." American Sociological Review, 19, 1954, 23—29. Stuckert, R. "Role Perception and Marital Satisfaction-- A Configurational Approach." Marriage and Family Living, 23, 1963, 415—419. Sullivan, H. The Interpersonal Theory of Psychiatry. New York: Norton, 1953. Thibaut, J. W. and Kelley, H. H. The Social Psychology of Groups. New York: Wiley, 1959. Thomas, D.; Loomis, A.; and Arrington, R. Observational Studies Of Social Behavior. Yale University, Institute of Human Relations, Vol. I, 1933. Thomas, E. J. and Fink, C. F. "Effects of Group Size." Psychological Bulletin, 60, 1963, 371—384. TOnnies, F. Egmeinschaft angyGesellschaft. (Translated by C. P. Loomis, 1957), East Lansing: Michigan State University Press, 1887. Watzlawick, P. and Beavin, J. "Some Formal Aspects of Communication." The American Behavioral Scientist, 10, 1967, 4-8. , and Jackson, D. D. Pragmaticslof Human Communi- cation. New York: Norton, 1967. Waxler, N. and Mishler, E. "Experimental Studies in Families." In L. Berkovitz (ed.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, Vol. 5, 1970, Weick, K. E. The Social Psychology of Organizing. Reading, Mass.: Addison-Wesley, 1969. Winer, B. J. Statistical Principles in Experimental Design. New York? McGraw-Hill, 1962. ll)llllflllHHIIHIIIWIWIIHIIIHIIIIWIIHII)WWHI