\ . ’1} (_ -. ’wa I“ \ D \3 ABSTRACT SIX, EIGHT, AND TEN YEAR OLD CHILDREN'S VIEWS OF OCCUPATIONAL ROLES BY Ruth F. Scheresky The subject of this research was children's percep- tions of occupational roles that are traditionally stereo- typed by society. More specifically, the differences among the views of children as they varied with age, gender, and location regarding their acceptance of occupational roles for men and for women were explored. The sampled population for the study included three school districts, each representative of the location fac- tor: rural, suburban, and central city. A stratified ran- dom sample was taken to generate groups of fifteen boys and fifteen girls at the ages of six, eight, and ten years, at each of the locations: rural, suburban, and central city. The total number in the sample was 270 children. The individual interview, using an instrument in which photographs played the significant part, was the method selected to measure the occupational role percep- tions of the children. The measure, including fifteen occupations which are stereotyped by society in general, Ruth F. Scheresky compared each subject to the traditional stereotypic views. For each occupation, the response could be one of three choices: male, female, or both. A stereotypic response received a score of "l"; a response of either the Opposite sex choice of the stereotypic answer or the response "both" received a scores of "0." Interpretation of the score resulted in degree of stereotyping; the higher the score the more stereotyped the subject. It was hypothesized that there would be no signifi- cant differences among the factors of age, gender, and loca- tion in the responses of the children regarding their accep- tance of occupational roles for men and for women. The three-way analysis of variance used for statis- tical analysis of the data revealed no significant differ- ences, resulting in the following conclusions: 1. The degree of stereotyping was high among all subjects. Children perceived occupations as the role of one sex or the other, according to traditional stereotypic views. 2. Age had little or no effect on the child's acceptance of occupational roles for men and for women. 3. Location, whether rural, suburban, or central city, had no significant effect upon the child's acceptance of occupational roles for men and for women. 4. The number of years children had spent in school did not appear to lessen the degree of stereotyping that was done. SIX, EIGHT, AND TEN YEAR OLD CHILDREN'S VIEWS OF OCCUPATIONAL ROLES Byzfi Ruth F3}Scheresky A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Elementary Education 1975 Dedicated to My Parents ii ACKNOWLEDGMENTS Grateful acknowledgment is expressed to Dr. Don Nickerson, my guidance committee chairman, and to the other committee members: Dr. Cal Collier, Dr. Glen Cooper, and Dr. Louise Sause. Each has given me valuable guidance, encouragement, and support throughout my doctoral program. I have sincerely appreciated it. To Dr. John Schweitzer and his staff; and to Eric Gordon for his special assistance and encouragement through- out the research project, my appreciation. To the school districts, specific school staffs, and most of all to the children without whom this research would not have been possible, my sincere thanks. i I am further indebted to White's Photography and John Haan for their photographic skills; and to Elaine White and Jack Hoke, the silhouette models. Finally, an expression of gratitude is extended to friends who have been a continual source of encouragement. iii TABLE OF CONTENTS Page LIST OF TABLES . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Vi LIST OF FIGURES . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Vii PROLOGUE . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . viii Chapter l-‘ I 0 INTRODUCTION 0 O O O O O O C O O I O O O 0 Background . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2 Purpose of the Study . . . . . . . . . . 6 Importance of the Study . . . . . . . . . 6 Statement of the Problem . . . . . . . . 8 The Population and Sample . . . . . . . . 8 Definition of Terms . . . . . . . . . . . 9 Hypotheses . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 10 Limitations of th Study . . . . . . . . ll smary I O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O 0 11 II. SOCIALIZATION AND SEX-ROLE STEREOTYPING: A REVIEW OF RELATED LITERATURE AND RESEARCH . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 13 The Socialization Process . . . . . . . . 14 Culture and Socialization . . . . . . . 16 Beginning of Socialization . . . . . . 18 Socialization and Identification . . . l9 Socialization and Maturation . . . . . 21 Socialization and Play . . . . . . . . 22 Age and Identification . . . . . . . . 23 Sex-Role Socialization and Sex-Role Stereotyping . . . . . . . . . . . . . 25 Stereotype Defined . . . . . . . . . . 26 Effects of Sex-Role Stereotyping . . . 27 The Family as a Sex-Socializing Agency . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 32 School as a Sex-Socializing Agency . . 38 Mass Communication as a Sex- Socializing Agency . . . . . . . . . 49 Occupational Stereotyping . . . . . . . 52 Summary . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 57 iv Chapter Page III. DESIGN OF THE STUDY . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 59 Sample . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 59 Methodology . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 62 DeveIOpment of the Instrument . . . . . . . . 62 Design . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 67 Testable Hypotheses . . . . . . . . . . . . 67 Pilot Study . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 69 Summary . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 69 IV. ANALYSIS AND DISCUSSION OF RESEARCH DATA . . . 72 Hypothesis 1 . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 72 Hypothesis 2 . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 78 Hypothesis 3 . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 82 Interaction of Variables . . . . . . . . . 83 Summary . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 84 V. SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS . . . . . . . . . . . . 86 Summary . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 86 Conclusions . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 88 Implications of the Study to Education . . . 93 Suggestions for Further Resaerch . . . . . 96 APPENDICES . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 98 A. PERMISSION LETTER . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 99 B. SCORE SHEET . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 101 C. THE INSTRUMENT . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 103 BIBLIOGRAPHY O O O O O O O O O O O O O O C O O O O O O 134 LIST OF TABLES Table Page 3.1 Occupations, Roles, and Validation Results of Role Stereotyping by Seventy-Eight Graduate Students . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 64 3.2 Design of the Study . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 68 3.3 Analysis of Pilot Data . . . . . . . . . . . . 70 4.1 Analysis of Research Data . . . . . . . . . . . 73 4.2 A Comparison of the Observed Combined Means Scores for the Age Factor . . . . . . . . . . 74 4.3 - Responses to Occupational Roles .‘. . . . . . . 75 4.4 Least Stereotyped Occupations . . . . . . . . . 74 4.5 Most Stereotyped Occupations . . . . . . . . . 76 4.6 Number of Subjects According to Ages Viewing the Kindergarten Teacher Role . . . . . . . . 77 4.7 A Comparison of the Observed Combined Means Scores for the Gender Factor . . . . . . . . 79 4.8 A Comparison of the Mean Scores for Boys . . . 80 4.9 A Comparison of the Mean Scores for Girls . . . 80 4.10 Occupations With the Greatest Difference Between Boys' and Girls' Stereotypic Responses 0 I O O O I O O O O O O O O O O O O 81 4.11 Occupations With the Least Difference Between Boys' and Girls' Stereotypic Responses . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 82 4.12 A Comparison of the Observed Combined Means Scores for the Location Factor . . . . . . . 83 vi LIST OF FIGURES Figure Page 1. Interaction of Variables Age by Gender . . . . . 83 2. Interaction of Variables Location by Gender . . 84 3. Interaction of Variables Location by Age . . . . 84 4. Interaction of Variables Location by Age by Gender 0 O O O I O O O O O O O O O O O O O O 84 vii PROLOGUE The little girl put the stethosc0pe around her neck and said, "We can't play hospital unless we have a doctor." "Carol," I asked her, "why can't you be the doctor?" "The doctor has to be a boy!" All the girls agreed. "Carol," I persisted, "your mother is a doctor." "But she really is a mother. A daddy has to be the doctor." David ran up. "I'll be the doctor." He grabbed the stethoscope. David's mother is also a doctor. Everyone in the doll corner was satisfied. The dra- matic play they call "Michael Reese" (named after a local hospital) could continue. Vivian Paley "Is the Doll Corner a Sexist Institution?" School Review 81 (1973): 569. viii CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION The subject of this dissertation is children's perceptions of occupational roles that are traditionally stereotyped by society. Specifically, the interest is in exploring the differences among the views of children as they vary with age, gender, and location regarding their acceptance of occupational roles for men and for women. In this "age of antidiscrimination agencies and affirmative action programs,"1 with interest in and pres- sures from the Women's Liberation Movement running higher than ever before, a look at elementary school age chil- dren's views regarding sex roles in occupations is in order. Because the school is an important socializing agent of the child, the study can help the educator to be aware of the degree of stereotyping that the elementary school child is doing and, if necessary, changes can be made in the learning environment to avoid the continuance of sex-role stereotyping. lR. Healy and D. Lund, "Chapter 622: One State's Mandate," Ineguality in Education 18 (1974): 36. 1 Background American children are growing up within the most rapidly changing culture of which we have any record in the world, within a culture where for several gen- erations each generation's experience has differed sharply from the last, and in which the experience of the youngest child in a large family will be extraordinarily different from that of the first born. . . .2 Margaret Mead's statement, made almost a quarter of a century ago, is still applicable today. One of the areas affected significantly by change in a culture is the area of sex roles. Psychologists Stone and Church wrote, "We have already, many times over, changed our conceptions of sex roles, . . . , and will almost cer- 3 Brown indicated sig- tainly have to change them again." nificant changes have taken place within a single genera- tion concerning traditional conceptions of what is masculine and what is feminine.4 Another author, Rogers, said the current status of sex roles in our culture is in a state of flux; it is not clear.5 Evidence indicates the gap between the sexes is narrowing. 2M. Mead, "The Impact of Culture on Personality DevelOpment in the United States Today," Understanding the Child 20 (1951): 17; cited by I. J. Gordon, Human Develope ment (New York: Harper & Row, 1969), p. 131. 3L. J. Stone and J. Church, Childhood and Adoles- cence, 3rd ed. (New York: Random House, 1973), p. 148. 4D. G. Brown, "Sex-Role DevelOpment in a Changing Culture," Psychological Bulletin 55 (1958): 232. 5D. Rogers, Readings in Child Psychology (Belmont, Ca.: Brooks/Cole, 1969), p. 221. The most basic category in which human beings, regardless of race, creed, or culture, are placed is the 6 The psychological and category of sex: male or female. behavioral differences associated with the obvious bio- logical differences between boys and girls and men and women are perhaps even more important for individual adjustment than are the biological differences. To be genuinely happy, in almost all cultures it is essential that biological maleness or femaleness be accompanied by psychological and social maleness or female- ness.7 However, psychological and social maleness or femaleness does not allow for the traditional stereotypic views of maleness and femaleness. The Women's Liberation Movement has applied con- siderable pressure regarding sex discrimination in all aspects of our society. Evidence of the high interest in the Movement is indicated by the enormous volume of writing and speaking it has generated. Feminist articles on sex roles have received extensive print and broadcast pub- licity. This is the age of antidiscrimination agencies and affirmative action programs.8 Schools are under attack 6B. R. McCandless, Children Behavior and Develop- ment (New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston, 1967), p. 448. 71bid. 8Healy and Lund, loc. cit. for promoting sexism. Chapter 622,9 a measure outlawing discrimination based on sex in the public schools of the state of Massachusetts, was the first anti-sex discrimina- tion legislation of its kind:h1the country. This legis- lation was passed in August 1971; it came into being at a time when interest in and concern about sex-role stereotyping and the public schools was just beginning to be expressed. The first broadly circulated report on sexism and public school policies and practices appears to have been Dick and Jane as Victims, a study of children's readers done by women in Princeton, New Jersey (who later organized themselves as Women on Words and Images), first published in 1972. Studies of school systems were done that same year in Ann Arbor ("An Action Proposal to Eliminate Sex Discrimination in .the Ann Arbor Public Schools," March, 1972), and New York City ("Report on Sex Bias in the Public Schools"). Following these, a number of women's groups, teacher groups and individuals came out with collections of materials, studies and action plans for eliminating sex-role stereotyping in the schools. . 10 Children begin to learn about sex roles very early in their lives, probably before they are eighteen months 11 old, and certainly long before they enter school. These roles are learned through relatively simple patterns that most of us take for granted. Some of these patterns are: throwing boy babies up in the air and roughhousing with them, as Opposed to cooing over girl babies and handling 91bid.. pp. 36-46. loIbid., p. 45. JJTH Howe, "Sexual Stereotypes Start Early," Saturday Review 54 (1971): 76. them delicately; choosing sex-related colors and toys for children from their earliest days: and encouraging boys to be physically active while expecting girls to be quieter and more docile. Public education plays a substantial role in sexual stereotyping. Many writers believe the school has become one of the major elements in the socialization of Ameri- can children.12 One of the functions of the schools is socialization into age-sex roles.13 It is true schools reflect the society they serve. Schools function to reinforce the sexual stereotypes that children have been taught by their parents, friends, and the mass culture we live in.14 Levy and Stacey stated that the elementary school promotes sex stereotyping through various aspects of the curriculum, the organization of the classroom, the struc- ture of the school, teacher behavior with children, and the extracurriculuar milieu. The authors further claimed: Schools exist in part to perpetuate various roles which maintain our sexist society . . . schools do "make a difference" in that they remain effective 12D. Gottlieb, "Children in America: A Demographic Profile and Commentary," in Children's Liberation, ed. D. Gottlieb (Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall, 1973), p. 17. 13N. K. Denzin, "Children and Their Caretakers," in Children's Liberation, ed. D. Gottlieb (Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall, 1973), p. 134. 14Howe, op. cit., p. 77. agents of social control, perpetuating the exist- ing classs racial, and sexual divisions in our society. Thus, in fact, it can be assumed that through development and education a child learns broadening con- cepts of what one can accept in regard to the role of men and women. Purpose of the Study The present research was undertaken to explore the differences among the stereotypic views of children: among ages, between boys and girls, and among children in rural, suburban, and central city schools. Specific aspects of the research are discussed in the remainder of this chapter. Importance of the Study The educational system remains a primary agent for socialization, reflecting the society it serves.16 Burton stated, "A basic tenet of our society is that all Ameri- cans should have equal opportunity to pursue interests, develOp talents and grow to full potential."17 In our society a conflict occurs between the ideal the society 15B. Levy and J. Stacey, "Sexism in the Elementary School: A Backward and Forward Look," Phi Delta Kappan, October 1973, p. 105. 16Howe, loc. cit. l7G. Burton, "Sex-Role Stereotyping in Elementary School Primers," A position paper prepared for the Mont- gomery County Chapter of the National Organization for Women (1974), p. l. (Mimeographed.) endorses and the actual practice of the society regarding this equality. Burton continued, "It becomes the duty of the school to hold true to the basic ideal, to cease reflecting deviations, and to take positive steps to correct and to reshape the 'reality.'"18 A study of stereotypic views of children may pro- vide a basis for evaluating various aspects of the educa- tional system so that the Opportunity to become a fully developed human being can be a reality to all. Children become more aware of occupations as they grow Older. This study may reveal whether older children are more predisposed to sex-role stereotyping of occupa- tions than are younger children. An evaluation of the educational process could determine the factors that may contribute to sex-role stereotyping at the various age levels or a lack of sex—role stereotyping. The study may also help determine whether loca- tion is a major factor in the degree of sex-role stereo- typing Of occupations by elementary school children. Results may indicate whether the influence of rural, suburban, and central city environments is a factor in the child's sex-role socialization process. The gender of the child is another factor to con- sider when studying the perceptions of sex roles in lBIbid. occupations. Both boys and girls must be aware Of the opportunities available to them as human beings not to be limited by sex. The study can have implications regarding the planning of career education in school curriculum. Statement of the Problem The research described here addressed the follow- ing questions: Does age level determine to some degree the acceptance of certain occupational roles for both sexes? Are boys or girls more predisposed to stereotype occupational roles according to sex? Do the rural, sub- urban, and central city factors make for differences in viewing the sexes in certain roles? To answer these questions, hypotheses were formulated, a population for the research identified, and a methodology selected, using an instrument developed by the researcher. The Population and Sample The sampled pOpulation for the study included three school districts, each representative of the loca- tion factor in the research: rural, suburban, and central city. A stratified random sample was taken to generate groups of fifteen boys and fifteen girls at the ages of six, eight, and ten years, at each of the location factors: rural, suburban, and central city. The total number in the sample was 270 children. Definition of Terms Stereotype: ”An exaggerated belief associated with a category. Its function is to justify (rationalize) our conduct in relation to that category."19 Ages of children: Six year Olds are those chil- dren whose sixth birthday fell between December 1, 1973, and November 30, 1974, inclusive; eight year Olds are those children whose eighth birthday fell between December 1, 1973, and November 30, 1974, inclusive: ten year Olds are those children whose tenth birthday fell between December 1, 1973, and November 30, 1974, inclusive. Urbanized area: The area consisting of a central city, or cities, and the surrounding closely settled ter- ritory.20 Central city: A city of 50,000 inhabitants or more,21con5isting of the pOpulation of the city named in the title of the urbanized area:22 central city is not synonymous with the "inner city" concept. Suburban: The surrounding closely settled terri- tory of a central city in an urbanized area. 19G. W. Allport, The Nature of Prejudice (New York: Doubleday, 1958), p. 187. 20Dictionary of Economic and Statistical Terms (Washington, D.C. : Government Printing Office, Nov. , 1972) , p. 69. ZlIbid. 22Ibid., p. 62. 10 Rural: That area not included in an urbanized area; can consist Of "a farm and/or non-farm pOpulation."23 Central city school: A school in a school dis- trict located in a central city. Suburban school: A school in a school district located in a suburban area. Rural school: A school in a school district located in a nonurban area. Sampled population: The six, eight, and ten year Olds in the selected schools: "it is the population for strict statistical inference."24 Target population: The group to which a general- ization is made, or "the primary population Of interest";25 the six, eight, and ten year Olds in the Midwest region. Stratified random sample: Sample randomly drawn 26 from each stratum or suprpulation. Hypotheses The following hypotheses were tested in this study: 23Ibid.. pp. 64, 69. 24M. J. Slakter, Statistical Inference for Eduog- tional Researchers (Reading, Mass.: Addison-Wesley, 1972), p. 50. ZSIbid. 26G. Sax, Empirical Foundations Of Educational Research (Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall, 1968), p. 135. 11 Hypothesis 1: There is a significant difference among the responses of the six year Old, eight year Old, and ten year old children in their acceptance of occupa- tional roles for men and for women. Hypothesis 2: There is a significant difference between the responses of boys and the reSponses of girls in relating their acceptance of occupational roles for men and for women. Hypothesis 3: There is a significant difference among children from rural, suburban, and central city schools in their acceptance Of occupational roles for men and for women. Limitations of the Study The following limitations are noted in regard to the study. The study is limited by the influence the interviewer may have had on the subjects' responses, the interviewer being a female. The study was conducted by the researcher alone. The use Of only one interviewer precludes statistical verification of interviewer relia- bility. The absence of such verification constitutes a limitation of the study. The study is further limited to those children willing to participate in the study. Summary In this chapter, a brief introduction to the study was made; the background, purpose, and importance of the 12 study were discussed; and the specific problem to be addressed by the study was defined: Are there differ- ences among the stereotypic views of children among ages, between boys and girls, and among children in rural, sub- urban, and central city schools? The sample and definition of terms were presented. Hypotheses of interest were then specified, and the limitations of the research noted. In the following chapter, literature and research relevant to the present study are examined. The design of the study, including the methodology and the develop- ment of the instrument, is presented in Chapter III, as well as the analysis of the pilot data. In Chapter IV the analysis and the discussion of the research data are presented. The final chapter incorporates a summary, con- clusions, implications of the study, and suggestions for further research. CHAPTER II SOCIALIZATION AND SEX-ROLE STEREOTYPING: A REVIEW OF RELATED LITERATURE AND RESEARCH "As the twig is bent . . ." has long been part of Western folklore. Scientific data now support this View. The origin of self lies in the early years.1 Individuals learn the skills that enable them to function in particu- lar types of roles; they are not born with these skills.2 Gesell stated: Psychically, the child inherits nothing fully formed. Each and every part of his nature has to grow--his sense of self: his fears, his affections and his curiosities; his feelings toward mother, father, playmates, and sex; his judgments of good and bad, of ugly and beautiful; his respect for truth and property; his sense of humor: his ideas about life and death, crime, war, nature, and deity. All his sentiments, concepts, and attitudes are products of growth and experience. . . . 11. J. Gordon, "The Beginnings of Self: The Problem of the Nurturing Environment," in Exploring Human Develop- ment, ed. H. W. Bernard & W. C. Huckins (Boston: Allyn & Bacon, 1972). p. 261. 2A. E. Guskin and S. L. Guskin, A Social Psychology of Education (Reading, Mass.: Addison-Wesley, 1970), P. 98. 3A. Gesell, Infant Development (New York: Harper & Brothers, 1952): P. 83. l3 14 The importance of the early years of a child's life cannot be emphasized enough. The concern in sex— role identity is the child's attitude toward himself, not sex-role stereotyping. In this review of literature and research, two aspects are dealt with: (l) the socialization process of the child and (2) sex-role socialization and stereotyping. The Socialization Process Each individual is born into a society. The mem- bers of the society practice a way of 1ife--a culture.4 Elkin stated, "A child is born into a world that already 5 exists." "A child is shaped by the nuclear group into "6 which he is born. The culture of childhood, like all cultures, is learned, shared, and transmitted. "It is learned, but not necessarily taught."7 Socialization is defined as the process by which an individual learns the ways of a given society--the behaviors, values, and expectations of others—-so that he can take on particular roles in society and function within 4 Teachers College Press, 1970), P. 7. 5F. Elkin, The Child and Society (New York: Random House, 1961), p. 7. 6E. W. King and A. Kerber, The Sociology of Early Childhood Education (New York: American Book, 1968), p. 44. M. E. Goodman, The Culture of Childhood (New York: ’ 7Goodman, loc. cit. 15 it.8 Elkin felt socialization includes both learning and internalizing apprOpriate patterns, values, and feelings. The child not only knows what is expected of him and behaves accordingly, he also feels that this is the prOper way for. him to think and behave. Through the process of socialization, the phenom- enon of the self-concept evolves. Just as individuals are not born with skills that enable them to function in particular types of roles, neither is the self-concept a biological phenomenon with which infants are born. It evolves from the social and environmental aspects of living. An awareness of an individual's own attributes through his experiences with people who surround him is acquired as early as the first weeks of life. The child reflects these experiences in his personality and behavior as he deveIOps.lo Socialization is a function of social interaction.11 The child's interaction with his family, beginning from birth, determines his idea of himself, an essential aspect 8Guskin and Guskin, loc. cit. 9Elkin, Op. cit., p. 4. 10King and Kerber, Op. cit., p. 46. 11Elkin, Op. cit., p. 5. 16 Of his personality. One sees self only as reflected by the attitudes of the people around him.12 Elkin discussed three preconditions that are neces- sary for a child to become socialized: (1) There must be an ongoing society, the world into which he is to be socialized; (2) The child must have the requisite biologi— cal inheritance (e.g., if a child is feeble-minded or suffers from a serious mental disorder, adequate social- ization becomes extremely difficult, if not impossible); and (3) A child requires "human nature," defined as "the ability to establish emotional relationships with others and to experience such sentiments as love, sympathy, shame, envy, pity, and awe."13 Culture and Socialization From the beginning, boys and girls grow up in dif- ferent, culturally determined emotional atmospheres. The uniqueness rooted in the biochemical composition of the individual is fostered by the nature of his cultural experi- ence. The ongoing social environment into which a child is born allows for the first lessons in being a boy or a 14,15 girl. Stressing the importance of the awareness of 12King and Kerber, Op. cit., p. 45. 13Elkin, Op. cit., p. 7. 14L. J. Stone and J. Church, Childhood and Ado— lescence, 3rd ed. (New York: Random House, 1973), p. 151. 151. J. Gordon, Human Development, 2nd ed. (New York: Harper & Row, 1969), p. 15. 17 what goes on around them, Gesell stated: . . . But we may grant, on the basis of objective signs, that even the young infant has a considerable degree of mental life. By the age of one month his behavior is too coherent, too integrated, and too personal in its individuality to be regarded as merely "subcortical." He manifests pleasure, pain, desire. He evidently senses comfort, discomfort, satiety, warmth. He responds to the touch of ministering hands; he heeds the human voice and face. . . . He is . . . an adaptive, changing, growing being--assimilative, explor- ative, and conscious on his plane of immaturity. He may well be capable of awarenesses which arise from processes of synthesis and integration. His "mind" is not larval. It is already in the making. Culture prescribes masculine and feminine beha- viors.17 These behaviors are not learned at any given age. The Swiss psychologist, Piaget, defined learning as "a prOcess provoked by external situations (a psychological experiment, a teacher, some exigency of the environment) 18 The child learns "a single and limited in SOOpe." structure," or solves a single problem. Such learning is subordinate to development; "DevelOpment is the essential process and each element of learning occurs as a function 16Gesell, op. cit., p. 57. 17P. H. Mussen, J. J. Conger, and J. Kagan, Child DevelOpment and Personality (New York: Harper & Row, 1974), p. 398. 18J. Piaget, "Cognitive DevelOpment in Children: The Piaget Papers," in Piaget Rediscovered: A Roport of the Conference on Cognitive Studies and Curriculum Develop- ment, ed. R. E. Ripple and V. N. Rockcastle (Ithaca: Cornell University, School of Education, March 1964): PP. 6-48; cited by M. Almy, E. Chittenden, and P. Miller, Young Children's Thinking (New York: Teachers College, 1967), p. 20. 18 of total develOpment, rather than being an element which explains develOpment."19 The crucial fact for Piaget seems to be that this "set“ or attitude must come from within. What is learned at any given point is, at least in part, determined by what has gone on before--not merely by what the child has experienced, but more by the elements to which he has paid attention. "Every instruction from without presupposes a construction from within."20 Beginning of Socialization Socialization is a process that begins immediately upOn the child's entrance into the world. There are many statuses and roles, some general and some specific, in which a person is involved. A status may be defined as a position in a social structure. A person has many statuses, some of which include age status, sex status, religious status, and nationality status. To each status is attached a role or a pattern of expected behavior. "Role implies not only a knowledge of the expected behavior but also culturally appropriate values and feelings."21 19Ibid. 20Ibid., p. 21. 21Elkin, Op. cit., p. 19. 19 Thus, a child is born with a status determined by his sex. The role of that status, then, is the part that is learned through social interaction within a given society, or through the process of socialization. Socialization and Identification Psychologists such as Gordon,22 Mussen, Conger, and Kagan,23 and Gewirtz and Stingle24 referred to identifica- tion as a process by which a person views himself as being like another and then behaves accordingly; the process by which roles and attitudes are learned. These roles and attitudes become a basic part of the core of the child's self and are fairly stable elements in the way he will feel and behave throughout life, since they are learned early in life in a close interpersonal setting.25 The early childhood period is the period of the greatest intensity of identification, of the greatest need to identify. Regarding sex identification, Ira Gordon wrote, "A major task of early childhood consists of identification with one's own sex--learning the apprOpriate male or female 22Gordon, Human DevelOpment, Op. cit., p. 84. 23Mussen, Conger, and Kagan, Op. cit., pp. 395-396. 24J. L. Gewirtz and K. C. Stingle, "Learning of Generalized Imitation as the Basis for Identification," Psychological Review 75 (1968): 374-375. 25Gordon, Human Development, Op. cit., p. 116. 20 26 Identification is not ordinarily a con- adult role." scious imitation process. The many highly significant, complex patterns of personality characteristics, reactions, motives, attitudes, personal idiosyncracies, and moral standards appear to be acquired by the child spontaneously, without direct training or reward--without anyone's "teach- ing" and without the child's intending to learn. Gordon stated that "parent behavior in the first six years of life influences the child's identity and the standards he will set for typical sex-related behavior."27 The stability of an individual's behavior over time is determined by the major factor of the individual's own desire to make his behavior agree with the culture's defi- nition of sex. Two kinds of processes by which children acquire attitudes, values, and patterns of social behavior were discussed by Bandura.28 First, learning can occur on the basis of direct tuition or instrumental training. In this form of learning, parents and other socializing agents are explicit about what the child is expected to learn. The attempt to shape behavior is through reward and punishing 26Ibid., pp. 114-115. 27Gordon, "The Beginnings of Self," op. cit., p. 260. 28A. Bandura, "The Role of Modeling Processes in Per- sonality Development," in Readings in Child Behavior and Development, ed. C. S. Lavatelli and F. Stendler (New York: Harcourt, Brace, Jovanovich, 1972), p. 334. 21 consequences. Although Bandura felt a certain amount of a child's socialization takes place through such direct training, most of the personality patterns are acquired primarily through the second process--the child's active imitation of parental attitudes and behavior. Most of these attitudes and behavior patterns the parents have never directly attempted to teach. At this time there is no firm evidence that males and females are destined by nature for the arbitrary roles and characteristics assigned to the two sexes in this or any other culture.29 Baller and Charles believed that only in late childhood is there any physiological reason for different behavior between males and females.30 However, our culture insists that boys and girls behave differ- ently, learn different sex roles. The differences in the sex roles are discussed in more detail in the second part of this chapter. Socialization and Maturation Maturation is closely related to socialization. Biological maturation underlies the social develOpment of the child. In a suitable environment the biological organ- ism follows a systematic and orderly pattern in the 29Mussen, Conger, and Kagan, Op. cit., p. 398. 30W. R. Baller and D. C. Charles, The Psychology o§_Human Growth and Development (New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston, 1968), pp. 352-353. 22 develOpment of neural, muscular, and glandular tissue. A person, regardless of the amount of training given, must be biologically ready before he can function in given ways.31 Elkin interpreted Jean Piaget's work as showing the relevance of maturation to socialization.32 There are distinct lines of development, and quite early in a child's life social factors combine with physiological development to permeate the child's ways Of thought and perception of the world. The assumption that the child's mind is not an immature form of the adult mind underlay Piaget's research. Rather, "the child thinks in a world of his own, a world inherently and distinctly different from that of the adult." Socialization and Play Through mimicry, a universal type of play, and par- ticularly the mimicking of adult activities, through observation and imitation of that world, the culture of childhood comes to include versions of adult values as 33 well as of adult roles. As a part of identifying, chil- dren devote many hours to role-playing. In this activity, a child "acts out" the behavior he perceives to belong to certain roles. This is essential to his develOpment.34 31E1kin, op. cit., pp. 21-22. 32Ibid., p. 22. 33Goodman, Op. cit., p. 133. 34Gordon, Human Development, p. 117. 23 Age and Identification During the preschool years, most parents pay atten- tion to the sex appropriateness Of their children's beha- vior.35 Even though the identification process is subtle, the very young child's perceptual abilities are quick to grasp the obvious physical characteristic of sex differ- ences. Children learn about sex roles very early in their lives, probably before they are eighteen months old, cer- tainly long before they enter school.36 By the age of four years, a child has already divided the world into male and female people. The question of boy-girl differences has already been raised.37’38 Goodman stated that the three year old in any society knows his identity as a boy or girl, and is rapidly learning what is considered appropriate behavior for boys and girls, for men and women.39 However, games are played or chores performed quite willingly for another two years or so, which in his culture are generally the domain of the opposite sex. At the age of three or earlier, the child 35Mussen, Conger, and Kagan, loc. cit. 36F. Howe, "Sexual Stereotypes Start Early," Saturday Review 54 (1971): 76. 37King and Kerber, Op. cit., p. 66. 38D. G. Brown, "Sex-Role Development in a Changing Culture," Psyohological Bulletin 55 (1958): 233. 39Goodman, Op. cit., p. 27. 24 is likely to be curious about sex organs. He learns to sex-classify the peOple around him on the basis of body structures. But, according to psychologists Stone and Church, "at age four, even those children who know about genital differences between boys and girls regard them as secondary to styles of coiffure or dress in determining sex and sex differences." To illustrate their point, they added this story: One four-year-old, visiting a family new to the neighborhood, observed their small baby creeping about the sunny lawn in the nude. Reporting on the new family to her mother, she was asked whether the baby was a boy or a girl and replied, "I don't know. It's so hard to tell with their clothes off."40 The processes of identifying and role-taking, begun during earlier stages of self-development, continue through middle childhood, the period of growth from about six to about twelve years. The child is extending his concept of self as "male" or "female," primarily through his relation- ships with his peers. "The peer group is the locus of much of the role-taking that occurs during these years." The role of the "other" now involves the child's peers, in addition to his parents.41 Havighurst said middle childhood is characterized by the child being thrust "out of the home and into the peer group, the physical thrust into the world of games and 40Stone and Church, Op. cit., p. 165. 41Gordon, Op. cit., pp. 200-201. 25 work requiring neuromuscular skills, and the mental thrust into the world of adult concepts, logic, symbolism, and 42 The individual has worked out his partic- communication." ular style and his level in all three areas by the end of this period--middle childhood. Sex-Role Socialization and Sex-Role Stereotyping Socialization occurs in many settings, and in interaction with many people. Organized groups, such as family, church, school, and peer group, and settings, such as the mass communication media, are spoken of as agencies 43'44 Each agency socializes the child of-socialization. into its own patterns and its own values. Each agency also helps to socialize the child into the larger world, for each is a surrogate of wider social and culture-orders, and its impact extends beyond its own organizational limits.45 Cultural trends affect all agencies. Currently, as mentioned in the previous chapter, the Women's Liberation 42R. J. Havighurst, "The Developmental Tasks of Middle Childhood," in The Psychology of the Elementary School Child, ed. A. R. Binter and S. H. Frey (Chicago: Rand McNally, 1972), PP. 12-13. 43Elkin, op. cit., p. 45. 44T. N. Saario, C. N. Jacklin, and C. K. Tittle, "Sex Role Stereotyping in the Public Schools," Harvard Educational Review 43 (1973): 387. 45Elkin, loc. cit. 26 Movement is proclaiming its message about the prOpagation of sexism in schools. Not only is the subject of sexism a concern of the Women's Liberation Movement, it is also becoming a major concern of peOple interested in child develOpment. There is growing awareness of the damage done to individual growth by channeling peOple into narrow roles according to sex.46 Stereotype Defined Allport defined stereotype as "an exaggerated belief associated with a category. Its function is to justify (rationalize) our conduct in relation to that cate- gory."47 In the article, "Decoding the Messages," sex-role stereotypes were defined as "the beliefs we hold about the 'right' way for girls to act or the 'right' way for boys to act." Further, it was stated that these beliefs are "part of our basic value system."48 The Michigan Women's Commission stated the follow- ing concerning sex stereotyping: Sex stereotypes arise from the belief that cer- tain abilities, traits, and interests usually occur only in men and boys and other abilities, traits, 46C. Jacobs and C. Eaton, "Sexism in the Elementary School," Today's Education, December 1972, p. 20. 47G. W. Allport, The Nature of Prejudice (New York: Doubleday, 1958), p. 187. 48 (1973): 8. "Decoding the Messages," The Awareness Game 6 27 and interests occur only in women and girls. These stereotypes are grounded in ignorance and fear and unreasonably restrict the rights of individuals to make their own life choices. They are particularly destructive to children, because they profoundly influence their level of self-esteem, choice of occu- pation, and means of self-expression. Howe defined sexual stereotypes as assumed differences, social conventions or norms, learned behavior, attitudes, and expectations.50 Effects of Sex-Role Stereotyping t As a result of sexism, both boys and girls are limited in develOping to their full human potential.. Men are victimized, as are women, by sex-role stereotypes.51 Sexism places "a higher value on male characteristics and. values," placing men in a "superior" position. This dichotomy, however, prevents all concerned from attaining. full humanity, for sexism predetermines peOple's choices in life on the basis of sex, without regard to individual differences.52 49Sex Discrimination in an Elementary_Reading Pro- gram (Lansing: Michigan Women's Commission), p. 1. 50Howe, Op. cit., p. 77. 518. Tobias, "The Case for Women's Studies," p11 Minority Connecticut Conference on the Status of Women (Washington, D.C.: National Education Association, August 1972), p. 21. 52"From a Parent's Perspective," Sexism in Education, 3rd ed. (Minneapolis: The Emma Willard Task Force on Edu- cation, 1973), p. 20. 28 Personality traits are stereotyped according to sex. Not only at home but at school, girls tend to get the message to be dependent, passive, quiet, and "good." They are rewarded for conforming behavior. This type of behavior promotes the inhibition of negative feelings, viewed as a concomitant Of the charge to women to be accommodating. Females are expected to yield to others' requirements or demands. The male has greater freedom in this area: negative feelings can emerge.53 In contrast, home expectations of boys differ from those of school, which creates different pressures. Traits of aggression, independence, and achievement are encouraged in the home; Obedience and quietness are expected at school. Boys who can reconcile and live up to the dual standards tend to become high achievers and "successful chauvinists." Those who are unable will often "rebel, act out, or become low achievers and unsuccessful chauvin- ists."54 In a study entitled "Sex-Role Pressures and the Socialization of the Male Child," Hartley interviewed eight to eleven year old boys; her results supported the 53J. Mosher et a1., "Masculinity and Femininity," 51% Minority Connecticut Conference on the Status of Women (Washington, D.C.: National Education Association, August 1972), p. 31. 54B. Levy and J. Stacey, "Sexism in the Elementary School: A Backward and Forward Look," Phi Delta Kappan October 1973, p. 107. 29 hypothesis that boys in our culture are under greater pressure to conform to stringent sex role norms than are girls. A boy who is labeled a "sissy" is more ostracized than a girl who is considered a "tomboy." The boy who aspires to female pursuits is thought to be engaged in a kind of a "status slumming." It is the smart girl, how- ever, who wants to do boy things. Furthermore, the boys in the study indicated they had to be able "to fight in case a bully comes along, run fast, play rough games, take care of themselves, and know what girls don't know." They said grown-ups expect boys to be noisy, get dirty, get into more trouble than girls do, and not be cry-babies. They saw men as "strong, able to protect women and children in emergencies, able to sup- port their families, and be in charge of things." The boys' concept of the female role was that girls "stay close to the house, play quietly, keep clean, and be gentler than boys, and they cry when they are scared or hurt." Adult women were seen as "indecisive, afraid of many things, tired a lot, staying home most of the time, squeamish, and 55 Thus, with such ludicrous exag- not very intelligent." geration of the woman's role, the young boy pressures him- self to become an athletic he-man and to suppress his tender feelings. 55R. Hartley, "Sex-Role Pressures and the Social- ization of the Male Child," Psychological Reports 5 (1959): 457-468. 30 Sexism has its effects beyond those on the child in the classroom. A look at the population and the world of work reveals that 51 percent of the population is female, and women make up 40 percent of the labor force. Women work because of economic need: they are single, divorced, deserted, widowed, or married to men earning less than $7,000 a year.56 Women are concentrated in low-paying, dead-end jobs, resulting in the average woman worker earn- ing less than three-fifths of what a man does, when both work full time the year round.57 Bem and Bem cited three basic reasons why women work at jobs rather than pursuing careers. These were discrimination, sex-role conditioning, and the presumed incompatibility of family and career.58 Women do not pur- sue challenging or even well-paying careers because of the "homogenization" of America's women, a consequence of the sex—role ideology. The majority of America's women become full-time homemakers; Of those who work, 78 percent end up in dead-end jobs as clerical workers, service workers, factory workers, or sales clerks. The authors pointed out 56E. D. Koontz, "Education for Survival," Non-Sexist Education for Survival (Washington, D.C.: National Educa- tion Association, 1973), p. 7. 57"Have You Heard???" Sexism in Education (Min- neapolis: The Emma Willard Task Force, 1973), p. 6. 588. L. Bem and D. J. Bem, "Training the Woman to Know Her Place," an article prepared for the Pennsylvania Department of Education (Pennsylvania Dept. of Education, Division of Pupil Personnel Services, 1973), pp. 3-7. 31 .that the role of homemaker is not necessarily inferior, but that in spite of their unique identities, the majority of America's women end up in virtually the same role. It is important that a child develOp a healthy "self." Sex identity and role behavior are inseparable 59 from the develOpment of the self. Binter and Frey emphasized the importance of recognizing and acquiring 60 But if, for a girl, behaviors apprOpriate to one's sex. learning the "prOper“ sex role restricts the right of making her own life choices, this certainly is not devel- Oping the best self-concept, or allowing for a means of self-expression. In a fluid world, with a constantly shifting diver- sity of activities that can be pursued by both men and women, outmoded sex-based stereotypes have no place. Such stereotypes "check both boys and girls from giving free 61 rein to their hOpes and dreams." Olds further stated sex-biased stereotypes hold boys and girls "from following their own talents and inclinations, and keep them from realizing their true potential as human beings."62 59Elkin, Op. cit., p. 55. 60A. R. Binter and S. H. Frey, The Psychology of the Elementary School Child (Chicago: Rand McNally, 1972), p. 294. 615. W. Olds, "Giving Children the Freedom to Be Themselves," The Awareness Game 6 (1973): 9. 62Ibid. 32 The Family as a Sex- Socializing Agency As brought out previously, the socialization of a child begins immediately upon entrance into the world. Thus, the first and most important socializing agency is the family. Even though many of the functions that were once considered routine for the family have been taken over by other agencies, the family remains the major agency of socialization.63 The process of sex stereotyping begins early. Lewis discussed the preoccupation parents have with the sex characteristic of the unborn child--from discussing their preference for the sex of the unborn child and pro- viding names as a function of the sex of the child, to the responsiveness of mothers to the activity of the fetus in 64 . . For instance, an active, a sex-apprOpriate fashion. kicking fetus that moves a great deal is interpreted as a sign that the child is more likely to be male than female. Although parents have other concerns, such as physical and mental health, their attitude toward the fetus usually centers on specific characteristics of sex. Lewis felt this concern may be reflective "of a cultural need, con- cern, and/or value system." 63Elkin, op. cit., p. 47. 64M. Lewis, "Parents and Children: Sex-Role DevelOp- ment," School Review 80 (1972): 230-232. 33 The channeling process of sex stereotyping begins at birth, when the parents are told the sex of their child. If it's a boy, certain expectations are held. The parents might speculate about what their sxul will become-~a chemist, a doctor, a newspaper editor, an artist. The choice of career is his, meeting his individual needs and abilities. If the child is a girl, however, the parents don't bother to do much guessing. Individuality becomes irrelevant if the child is a female, for there seems to be one activity to which most females are channeled--that of housewife.65 Parents' treatment of their children is an important factor in the creation of sex stereotyping. Comparing the life of a young boy to that of a young girl, a critical difference emerges: a girl is treated more protectively. She is subjected to more restrictions and controls. A boy receives greater achievement demands and higher expecta- tions.66 Colors serve as announcements to the world of how the new infant is to be treated. A blue world surrounds the infant boy; around the baby girl everything is pink-- 65N. Frazier and M. Sadker, Sexism in School and Society (New York: Harper & Row, 1973), p. 82; citing S. Bem and D. Bem, "We're All Nonconscious Sexists," in Beliefs, Attitudes and Human Affairs, ed. D. J. Bem (Monterey, Ca.: Brooks/Cole, 1970). 661bid., p. 84. 34 from ribbons to stuffed animals, to the clothes she wears.67'68 A great deal of "appropriate" sex-role behavior is learned as the child rummages through the toy box. Toys are differentiated between the sexes as children begin to grow. Looking at a trade catalogue of a toy manufacturer, one Observes substantial differences between the toys aimed at boys and girls.69’70 Roles are learned through relatively simple pat- terns that most of us take for granted. Energetic and physical activity for boys is encouraged, just as it is expected that girls are to be quieter and more docile. Parents evaluate a child's behavior by the criterion of the sex of the child: thus, boys are allowed, even expected, to be naughtier than girls. Aggression defines masculinity. Girls are expected to be soft and emotional.72 67Ibid., p. 83. 68H. H. Franzwa, "Working Women in Fact and Fiction, Journal of Communication 24 (1974): 104. 69C. Cannan, "Female From Birth?" The Times Educa- tion Supplement 2956 (January 14, 1972): 20. 7OHowe, Op. cit., p. 76. 7llbid. 72Cannan, loc. cit. 35 McNeil discussed aggressive responses by school— age children, categorizing them into physical or cognitive manners of expression.73 Physical expression included fighting, kicking, pushing, throwing stones, bullying, and teasing. Name calling, ridicule, sarcasm, swearing, threats, commands, and criticism were listed as cognitive manners of expression. Developmental studies indicate that as mental age and language competencies increase, overt physical actions as a primary mode of expressing hostility and anger decrease. Girls learn to disguise their anger feelings more rapidly than do boys, using more subtle modes such as gesture and language. Probably the most important variables in effecting this difference between boys and girls are the sex-role expectations as designated by the culture. For example, the admonition given to girls is, "good little girls don't fight." Feshbach and Feshbach pointed out that the general- ization that males are the more aggressive of the two sexes does not hold for all forms of aggressive behavior.74 Con- siderable evidence shows that boys are more physically aggressive than girls. The findings are less consistent 73E. B. McNeil, "Patterns of Aggression," in The Psychology of the Elementary School Child, ed. A. R. Binter and S. H. Frey (Chicago: Rand McNally, 1972), p. 275. 74H. Feshbach and S. Feshbach, "Children's Aggres- sion," in The Young Child, ed. W. W. Hartup (Washington, D.C." National Association for the Education of Young Children, 1972), p. 293. 36 when other forms of aggression are measured. Whereas boys are more likely to hit than are girls, girls are more likely to scold or insult another child. Differences in play between boys and girls are not merely expected, but actively encouraged. Only very young boys may be allowed to play with their sisters' dolls without ridicule or remonstrance. Rarely are boys given dolls of their own, although stuffed animal toys may be allowed. Girls enjoy playing with toy trucks, cars, and train sets but are rarely given these as gifts.75 Older girls particularly are discouraged from par- ticipating in physically aggressive activities. The label "tom-boy" is given to those girls who do not conform to the quieter, gentler, less aggressive activities expected of them. Boys are labeled "sissy" if they avoid rough games, or prefer reading or playing the piano.76 The common assumption is that children's games are one of the ways in which boys and girls learn appropriate sex-role behavior in their own society. Their perceptions of behavior appropriate to their own sex are reflected in the differences between the games chosen by boys and girls.77 75$. Millar, The Psychology of Play (Baltimore: Penguin Books, 1968), p. 194. 76Ibid., p. 195. 77B. G. Rosenberg and B. Sutton-Smith, "Revised Conception of Masculine-Feminine Differences in Play Activi- ties," The Journal of Genetic Psychology 96 (1960): 165. 37 Along with imitation of a model, through which a child identifies appropriate sex behaviors, is the direct communication that an activity is specifically for a girl 78 Liebert, McCall and Hanratty reported a study or a boy. on first-grade children investigating toy preferences, whereby they experimentally manipulated the sex-typed 79 After being told information for two groups of toys. that a group of toys was preferred by their own sex and that a second group was preferred by the Opposite sex, the children were asked to choose the toys they preferred. The results of the study showed that when children were told which toys their own sex preferred, they matched prefer- ences. Knowledge of what the Opposite sex preferred did not influence their preferences. The conclusion drawn from the study indicated that same-sex labels are more effective in influencing toy preference than are opposite-sex labels. Montemayor attempted to extend the positive rela- tionship found between sex label and choice of toy or task 80 A game that to a behavioral index of performance level. was labeled either sex-apprOpriate, sex-neutral, or sex- inapprOpriate was played by children six to eight years Old. 78R. Montemayor, "Children's Performance in a Game and Their Attraction to It as a Function of Sex-Typed Labels," Child DevelOpment 45 (1974): 152-156. 79R. Liebert, R. McCall, and M. Hanratty, "Effects of Sex-Typed Information on Children's Toy Preferences," Journal of Genetic Psychology 119 (1971): 133-136. 80 Montemayor, loc. cit. 38 Measures of performance and attractiveness of the game were obtained. The results of the study indicated perfor- mance was highest when the game was labeled sex-apprOpriate, intermediate when no sex label was given for the game, and lowest when the game was labeled sex-inapprOpriate for both boys and girls. The apprOpriate and neutral label condi- tions were similar for attractiveness, and both were higher than the inappropriate condition. School as a Sex-V Socializing Agency The social world Of the child expands rapidly upon school entrance.81 The school has as a principal function the socialization of youth.82 This function is to "educate the young," transmitting certain basic knowledge and skills of the culture.83 Schools function to reinforce the sexual stereotypes that children have been taught by their par- ents, friends, and the mass culture in which we live.84'85 Denzin wrote: "Another function of the schools is 81R. E. Hartley and F. P. Hardestly, "Children's Perception of Sex Roles in Childhood," The Psyohology of the Elementary School Child, ed. A. R. Binter and S. H. Frey (Chicago: Rand McNally, 1972), pp. 347-348. 2Saario, Jacklin, and Tittle, loc. cit. 83Elkin, op. cit., p. 57. 84Howe, op. cit., p. 77. 85Saario, Jacklin, and Tittle, loc. cit. 39 socialization into ageesex roles. Girls must be taught how to be girls and boys must learn what a boy is."86 Gottlieb believed "the major element in the social— ization of American children has become the school."87 According to statistical reports, more and more children are attending school for longer and longer periods of time. Schools do "make a difference," in that they "remain effective agents of social control, perpetuating the exist- ing class, racial, and sexual divisions in our society."88 Sexism permeates aspects of the curriculum, classroom organ- ization, the structure of the school, teacher behavior with children, and the extracurricular milieu. Boys and girls are taught lessons in sexual equality throughout their schooling experience. These lessons are overt as well as being more subtle.89 Although school is one facet of a child's world that contributes to stereotyping, the messages transmitted to the child in school carry much weight. A large portion 86N. K. Denzin, "Children and Their Caretakers," in Children's Liberation, ed. D. Gottlieb (Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall, 1973), p. 134. 87D. Gottlieb, "Children in America: A Demographic Profile and Commentary," in Children's Liberation, ed. D. Gottlieb (Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall, 1973), p. 17. 88Levy and Stacey, Op. cit., p. 105. 89M. Sadker and D. Sadker, "Sexism in Schools: An Issue for the 70's," The Journal of the National Association for Women Deans, Administrators, and Counselors 37 (1974): 69. 40 of a child's life is spent in the classroom, approximately 90 l 10 , 000 hours by the time he graduates from high school . ' 9 Phil Jackson, in Life in Classrooms, underscored how great an influence, in terms of time, the elementary school actually has: Aside from sleeping, and perhaps playing, there is no other activity that occupies as much of the child's time as that involved in attending school. Apart from the bedroom (where he has his eyes closed most of the time) there is no single enclosure in which he spends a longer time than he does in the classroom. From the age of six onward, he is a more familiar sight to his teacher than to his father, and possibly even to his mother.92 Frazier and Sadker continued on the elementary school: Another remarkable thing about elementary school is that children are forced to be there. Only two other institutions--prisons and mental hospitals-- are so thoroughly compulsory in nature. Of course, this is not to suggest that conditions are similar to those in prisons and mental hospitals. The authors, however, do mean to point out the one fac- tor the three have in common: in Sach, human beings are forcefully institutionalized.9 It is readily apparent that the elementary school is a place where many child-hours are spent. Here children are exposed to a variety of stimuli; they cannot escape from these stimuli, regardless of how much they want to. 90Jacobs and Eaton, loc. cit. 91Frazier and Sadker, Op. cit., p. 80. 92Ibid.; citing Phil Jackson, Life in the Classrooms. 93Frazier and Sadker, loc. cit. 41 Awareness of the symbolism and hidden messages inherent in the structure of the school has been heightened in the last decade by educational reformers and critics. Students learn much more than reading, writing, and arith- metic within the confines of the classroom. Schools func- tion as sorting and classifying mechanisms. Schools foster and amplify such questionable personality traits as pas- sivity, conformity, and dependency. Factors such as cur- riculum materials, testing materials, programmatically prescribed curricular patterns, teacher behavior, counsel- ing practices, peer group influences, and many other instructional factors convey multiple messages to children.94 The standard elementary curriculum contains the element of reading. The child's first contact with school is likely to leave a lasting impression. Since learning to read is the principal task of the early years at school, the contents of the books with which children spend so much time needs attention in regard to sex stereotyping in the schools. Reading texts convey many subtle messages to the young school child regarding role assignment. Through textbooks children learn about role models and expectations. This is influential in the life planning of pupils and is a major supplement to the children's home experiences. 94Saario, Jacklin, and Tittle, loc. cit. 42 Most children are exposed to a reading textbook every day. Even a child who is unable to read will experience in the classroom group discussion of the stories and other mate- rial in the texts. Whatever overt and covert messages the book contains penetrate children's minds early and Often, whether by passive reception or active involvement.95 A study of 144 elementary school textbooks made by the New Jersey Chapter of the National Organiza- tion for Women (NOW) showed that boys were the focus of 881 stories, whereas only 344 of the stories cen- tered around girls. Similarly, adult males were featured in 282 stories, women in only 127. There were 131 biographies of famous men, 23 of women. Time after time, the report declared, girls were por- trayed as passive, dependent, and incompetent; boys as active, self-reliant, and successful.96 Similar studies analyzing reading series in terms of sex stereotyping and career roles have confirmed the findings of the New Jersey Chapter of the National Organi- zation for Women. The studies referred to include those of Britton,97 the Committee to Study Sex Discrimination in the Public Schools,98 and one by Women on Words and Images.99 95Sex Discrimination in an Elementary Reading Pro- ram (Lansing: Michigan Women's Commission), p. ix. 96J. Hoyt, "Target: Sex Bias in Education," American Education 10 (1974): 7. 97G. E. Britton, "Sex Stereotyping and Career Roles," Journal of Reading 17 (1973): 140-148. 98Sex Discrimination in an Elementary Reading Program, op. cit. 99Dick and Jane as Victims (Princeton, N.J.: Women on Words and Images, 1972). 43 Implications from studies analyzing reading series in terms of sex stereotyping and career roles are broad. Role models are used in all societies to encourage self- develOpment of their children as individuals and function- ing members of the group. When the role models remain static, however, while society changes, their value as educational devices is lost. Instead they become psycho- logical straightjackets that constrict individual develop- ment and preparation for life. Models deliberately build- ing up the self-images of one group at the expense Of another become viciously repressive. "If socialization has twin goals, to rear fit individuals as well as individ- uals who fit, the models presented in school readers can "100 be faulted on both counts. Women psychologists are drawing attention to the great damage done to both men and women by our narrowly defined role models. In a statement to the American Psy- chological Association in September 1970, the Association of Women Psychologists said: Psychological oppression in the form of sex role socialization clearly conveys to girls from the ear- liest ages that their nature is to be submissive, servile, and repressed, and their role is to be ser- vant, admirer, sex object and martyr . . . the psycho- logical consequence of goal depression in young women-- the negative self-image, emotional dependence, drugged or alcoholic escape-—are all too common. In addition, both men and women have come to realize the effects on men of this type of sex role stereotyping, the crippling looIbid., p. 34. 44 pressure to compete, to achieve, to produce, to stifle emotion, sensitivity and gentleness, all taking their toll in psychic and physical traumas. The groundwork for a healthy adjustment to social change has to be laid in childhood. Readers are an important part of the groundwork. Changes in role models and behavior patterns they depict can have a great impact on the changing images children have of themselves. Few but the strong can function under a constant barrage of self-doubt and social disapproval. If we wish our children to avoid the destructive con- flicts in social relationships which can be traced to the effects of sex role conditioning with its dehuman- izing consequences for men and women alike, then we must begin now to reform the images with which they will form themselves.102 An example of overt sex typing is "Alpha One," a new phonics program for kindergarteners and first graders being used in an estimated 8,000 classrooms around the nation.103 The program is used as a multi-media approach to reading instruction. It seems to teach more about sex roles, however, than it does about phonics. In this pro- gram, each letter of the alphabet is given a personality and a gender. The twenty-one consonants are male, known as "the letter boys"; the five vowels are female, each having something wrong with her. The sex-role message lOlIbid., citing from printed "Statement, Resolutions, and Motions," presented by the Association of Women Psycholo- gists to the American Psychological Association at its con- vention in Miami, Florida, September 1970. 102Dick and Jane as Victims, op. cit., p. 36. 103A. Kerins, "Vowels Are Seeking Lbrtn Frm th Cnsnnts," Report on Sex Bias in the Public Schools (New York Chapter of the National Organization of Women, 1972), p. 21: cited by B. Levy and J. Stacey in "Sexism in the Elementary School: A Backward and Forward Look," Phi Delta Kappan, October 1973, p. 105. 45 portrayed through this language arts program could hardly be more harmful. Aside from readers, math books, science books, and social studies texts continue the sex-stereotyping tradition. Especially in social studies texts, opportuni— ties for sexism are enormous with such themes as the family, community helpers, and work.104 Not only do textbooks make for the perpetuation of sex-role stereotyping, but teachers also play an important part. Teachers tend to rate girls as more responsible than boys, but see the boys as possessing more leadership qualities.105 A study in which junior high school teachers were asked to select adjectives they felt described male and female students indicated the expectations teachers hold 106 The following lists form a partial for boys and girls. catalogue of sex stereotypes. Through a multitude Of nonverbal ways, teachers transmit these sex-typed expecta— tions to their pupils. 104 Levy and Stacey, Op. cit., p. 106. 105U. Bronfenbrenner, "The Changing American Child-- A Speculative Analysis," in Exploring Human DevelOpment, ed. H. W. Bernard & W. C. Huckins (Boston: Allyn & Bacon, 1972), p. 328. 106B. J. Kremer, "The Adjective Check List as an Indicator of Teachers' Stereotypes of Students" (Ph.D. dissertation, Michigan State University, 1965), p. 64. 46 Adjectives Describing Adjectives Describing Female Students Male Students appreciative emotional active energetic excitable sensitive adventurous enterprising OOOperative dependable aggressive loud changeable efficient curious independent reliable thorough determined inventive healthy awkward The phenomenon demonstrated by Rosenthal and Jacobson that one person's expectations can influence another's behavior is often referred to as the self- 107 It has a substantial effect on 108 fulfilling prophesy. the develOpment of children. Other messages are conveyed in a less subtle fashion. Studies by educational researchers examining the language Of the classroom to determine the nature of verbal interac- tion have revealed that teachers speak to boys and girls in different ways. Boys receive more disapproval than girls. Iflmnrreceive eight to ten times more control messages--comments like "stOp talking" or "listen to the instructions." Teachers are more likely to use harsh or angry tones when criticizing boys than when criticizing girls.109'llo 107R. Rosenthal and L. Jacobson, Pygmalion in the Classroom (Chicago: Holt, Rinehart & Winston, 1968). 108L. R. White, "Sex Role Stereotypes," Nonsexist Education for Survival (Washington, D.C.: National Educa- tion Association, 1973), P. 5. 109 Sadker and Sadker, op. cit., p. 70. 110Levy and Stacey, op. cit., p. 107. 47 Another classroom interaction pattern is that teachers talk to boys more about the subject matter and listen more to what they say. In short, boys receive more of the teacher's attention than do girls.111 Aside from the formal elementary curriculum and teacher behavior, which sexism permeates, other mechanisms of sex-role reinforcement include segregated classes and activities. Physical education and playground activities are frequently segregated. Certain activities, such as cooking and sewing, are encouraged primarily for girls: other activities, such as wood work and mechanical work, are encouraged for boys. In instrumental music, girls are encouraged to play the violin and flute, while percussion and brass are perceived as masculine. Assigned duties or jobs also teach sex roles: "Girls water the plants; boys move the chairs."112 Another way messages Of sexual inequity or lessons about the inferior status of women in this society are transmitted to students is through the staffing patterns prevalent in the schools. Only five or six school districts have female school superintendents, despite the fact that the vast majority of teachers are women. Alimited number of lllP. Sears and D. Feldman, "Teacher Interaction With Boys and Girls," The National Elementarprrincipal, November 1966, pp. 30-37. 112Levy and Stacey, loc. cit. 48 senior and junior high schools are headed by women prin- cipals.ll3’114 Statistics regarding women in the educational pro- fession reveal that "three-fifths of the women professional workers are teachers." Their participation in administra- tive and supervisory positions is not representative of the numbers of women in the profession.115 A national survey taken in 1970-71 revealed that although 67 percent of all public school teachers were women, only 15 percent of principals and .6 percent of superintendents were female. The women administrators were concentrated mostly in the elementary schools: 19 percent elementary principals and 34 percent elementary assistant principals. Only 3.5 percent of the junior high and 3 per- cent of the senior high principals were women. At the district level, the national picture is even more dismal. There were only a few women superintendents, and just 7 per- cent Of the deputy, associate, or assistant superintendents 116 were women. 113S. Martinez, "Sexism in Public Education: Litiga- tion Issues," Inequality in Education 18 (1974): 10. 114S. S. Taylor, "Women in Education," 51% Minority Connecticut Conference on the Status of Women (Washington, D.C.: National Education Association, August 1972), P. 66: citing National Education Association, "Estimates of School Statistics, 1971-72," Research Report l97l-R13. 115"Fact..Bombardment," The Awareness Game 6 (1973): 30. 116C. D. Lyon and T. N. Saario, "Women in Public Education: Sexual Discrimination in Promotion," Phi Delta Kappan, October 1973, p. 120. 49 Because education has traditionally been character- ized as a "woman's field," public awareness of diScrimina- tion against women within education has been slow to emerge.117 Thus, through staffing practices as well as interaction patterns and teacher eXpectations, schools con- tinually send messages to children that help prepare them to assume sex stereotypes and perpetuate a society based on sexual inequality.118 Mass Communication as a Sex-Socializing Agency A relatively recent development and a significant socialization agency, although interpersonal interaction is not directly involved, is the mass communication media. These media include the press, radio, magazines, comic booklets, movies, television, and other means of communi- cation. The mass media function to give the child a wider' range of role-taking models than he ordinarily finds in his family, neighborhood, and school.119 Movie and tele- vision stars can become strong objects of identification. The socialization derived from the mass media generally is incidental, since the child employs it for immediate 117Ibid. 118Sadker and Sadker, Op. cit., p. 71. 119Elkin, Op. cit., p. 73. 50 gratification and not to learn the patterns of society. Of particular significance in socialization are only those situations and models that are used imaginatively or directly in interaction situations.120 Feminists do not accept Elkin's idea that social- ization through the mass media is incidental. Courtney and Lockeretz analyzed women's depictions in advertisements in eight national U.S. magazines. The conclusions indi- cated that magazine advertisements portray women in very limited roles. Implications made by print advertisements included: (1) woman's place is in the home, (2) women do not make important decisions or do important things, and (3) women are dependent upon men and are regarded primarily as sexual Objects.121 These findings were corroborated by the results of four studies conducted in three U.S. cities to analyze the roles of women as portrayed in television 122 The data provided evidence that women are commercials. not portrayed as autonomous, independent human beings. Comparative analysis verified that men and women are 1201bid., pp. 73-74. 121A. E. Courtney and S. W. Lockeretz, "A Woman's Place: An Analysis of the Roles Portrayed by Women in Magazine Advertisements," Journal of Marketing Research 8 (1971): 92—105. 122A. E. Courtney and T. W. Whipple, "Women in TV Commercials," Journal of Communication 24 (1974): 110—118. 51 presented differently in advertising and that each sex is still shown in traditional roles. Sexism characterizes regular television program- ming for children as well as for adults. This is of great concern, since children often spend as much time watching television as they do in school.123 Tedesco reported on a subset of data collected from 1969 through 1972 as part of an ongoing project called Cultural Indicators, which studied the message system of prime-time network dramatic television programming and what these messages cultivate in viewers. The most notice- able finding regarding the major characters who populated the world of noncartoon, prime-time network dramatic tele- . . . . 124 Vision was the continued underrepresentation Of women. This analysis of major non-cartoon television characters has revealed that males are generally active and independent. They are more mature, more serious, and more likely to be employed than females. Males have adventures and get into violent situations. They are powerful and smart, and their independence requires that they be relatively unattached (not mar- ried) and thus able to take risks. Females, on the other hand, are presented as‘lacking independence. They are not usually found in adventure situations: they are younger, more likely to be married, and less likely to be employed.125 123Levy and Stacey, loc. cit. 124N. S. Tedesco, "Patterns in Prime Time," Journal of Communication 24 (1974): 124. 125Ibid., p. 122. 52 In contrast to the major noncartoon characters populating the world of television, Streicher said of stereotyping displayed in cartoons: In general, cartoon females were less numerous than males, made fewer appearances, had fewer lines, played fewer "lead roles," were less active, occupied many fewer positions of responsibility, were less noisy, and were more preponderantly juvenile than males. In many activities in which girls showed some form of skill (e.g., cheerleading), their performance was duplicated by a dog or other pet. . . 25 The perpetuation of stereotypy itself by the producers of cartoons and commercials seems to be most important at this point.127 Occupational Stereotyping A 1973 study by Beuf on sex-role perception of children born since the Women's Liberation Movement showed there has been little change.128 The sample included thirty- seven boys and-twenty-six girls between the ages Of three and six. A twenty to twenty-five minute individual inter- view was held, during which the interviewer asked specific questions regarding parents' occupations and about tele- vision viewing habits while engaged in conversation about the child's interests, school activities, and family. Each child was asked, "What do you want to be when you grow up?" 126H. W. Streicher, "The Girls in the Cartoons," Journal of Communication 24 (1974): 127. 127Ibid., p. 129. 128A. Beuf, "Doctor, Lawyer, Household Drudge," Journal of Communication 24 (1974): 142-145. 53 Then each child was asked, "If you were a boy (girl), what would you be when you grow up?" The children also played a game called "the O.K. picture game." The object of the game was to see whether a picture was "O.K." or not. Among several "dummy" pic- tures were three pictures that reversed traditional sex roles: a father feeding a baby, a man pouring coffee for a woman, a female telephone line repairperson. A strong relationship was noted between sex and career aspiration both for own-sex and projected "other- sex" conditions. This indicated the extent to which the children luni observed and properly grasped the role structure of our society. Boys seized upon rather adven- turous types of careers; policeman, sports superstar, and cowboy were the most pOpular. Girls showed a preference for quieter pastimes. Nursing rated high as a choice occupation here, and was the occupation most selected by boys as what they would do if they were females. The girls' favorite OOposite-sex ambition was to be a doctor. Both sexes had a tendency to stereotype aspira- tions, even though the actual jobs differed by sex. Stereo- typical careers were chosen by over 70 percent of the boys and 73 percent of the girls. Responding to the "what if you were a boy (girl)" questions, 65 percent of the boys and 73 percent of the girls selected careers that are societally labeled appropriate for their sex. 54 An interesting trend in the children's behavior with regard to the same question is that girls had an answer to the question, whereas boys did not. This beha- vior tends to indicate girls think about what it would be like to be a boy, what they would do as adults if they were boys. Boys, on the other hand, often regarded the interviewer suspiciously out of the corners of their eyes, or with open, outright astonishment. This behavior in boys might indicate they do not think about what it would be like to be a girl, or what they would do as adults if they were girls. Even when presented with the question some did not want to think about it. A regular response was, "That's a weird question, you know." This study further revealed that television view- ing is related to career choice. Moderate viewers appeared to exert a wider range of choice in career selection than did the heavy viewers. Seventy-six percent of the viewers selected stereotyped careers for themselves, as compared with 50 percent of the moderate viewers. Sex stereotyping increased on the responses to the O.K. picture game as the move from child-care to husband- wife to occupation roles was made. Fourteen percent of the children said it was "not O.K." for the father to feed the baby; 20 percent believed that it was "not O.K." for the man to be preparing the meal for the seated woman, and 55 49 percent thought the female telephone repairperson was "not O.K." These data indicate that although the Women's Lib- eration Movement has attracted much attention, children are still learning about a role structure that is sex typed, especially in the occupational sphere. The find- ings also reveal that young children, four and five years Of age, hold the same kinds of stereotypes about careers that American society as a whole has, as noted by sociol- ogists and Movement women. Preschool children, in addi— tion to knowing their sex, seemingly know the social implications and limitations of that sexual definition. Another study, an exploratory investigation of the develOpment of children's concepts Of adults' sex-based social roles, with the major emphasis on women's roles, was conducted by Hartley.129 One part of the study involved a measure, the Role Distribution Technique, in which children were to tell who usually performed each of the 150 different activities, covering aspects of social and family living. Results indicated that children clearly differentiated one group of items, which they assigned to men; another group, which they assigned to women; and a third group, which they perceived as belonging equally to 129R. E. Hartley, "Current Patterns in Sex Roles: Children's Perspectives," National Association of Women Deans and Counselors 25 (1961): 3-13. 56, both sexes. Although the children did not agree equally on all items, no single individual had difficulty in mak- ing these judgments. It was apparent these boys and girls knew pretty well what sorts of tasks they could look for- ward to as men and women. They also had a set of clear- cut expectations regarding the Opposite sex. Children learn quite early what roles society expects of them. Indications of this have resulted from studies of children's vocational aspirations. Looft, in 1971, inquired of first and second graders, "What would you like to be when you grow up?" The girls mentioned a total of eight different occupations, all sex typed, the most common being nurse and teacher. The boys' list totaled eighteen different occupations, from doctor, to astronaut, to football player. The children were then questioned, "What do you think you really will do when you grow up?" with the interesting result of Often-changed answers. Many of the girls changed occupations to housewife and mother, or else downshifted their choices; for example, one girl who had said whe wanted to be a doctor changed to "store lady."130 In another study, Iglitzin found that fifth-grade children saw most jobs and their own aspirations as being defined by sex. A series of questions was designed to show 130W. R. Looft, "Sex Differences in the Expression of Vocational Aspirations by Elementary School Children," Developmental Psychology, September 1971, pp. 366+. 57 sex stereotyping based on views of career and employment patterns, social roles in home and family, and the child's view of his future life. Both boys and girls revealed sex stereotyping as measured by the response "men" or "women" rather than "either" or "both" to the questions. It was found that the proportion of girls who had non- stereotyped responses in all categories was significantly higher than the proportion of boys making' such responses . 131 Summary A review of literature and research related to sex-role stereotyping of occupations was presented in this chapter. The review emphasized two major areas of rele- vance to the present research. First, the socialization process of an individual was examined, with specific ref- erences to culture and socialization, the beginning Of socialization, identification and maturation as they relate to socialization, and play and age in relation to socialization. In the second part of the chapter, sex-role social- ization and sex-role stereotyping were examined. Stereo- type was defined; the effects of sex-role stereotyping, the three agencies contributing to the sex-role socialization 131L. B. Iglitzin, "A Child's-Eye View of Sex Roles," Today's Education 9 (1972): 23-25. 58 of the individual, and occupational stereotyping were pre- sented and discussed. In the following chapter, the description of the sample, the methodology, the develOpment of the instru— ment, the design of the research, testable hypotheses, and the pilot study are discussed. CHAPTER III DESIGN OF THE STUDY In this chapter, the description of the sample, the methodology, the develOpment of the instrument, the design of the research, testable hypotheses, and the pilot study are discussed. Sample Three school districts were selected for the study, each representative Of the location factor in the research: rural, suburban, and central city. The settings are in south-central Michigan. The school district representing the rural location factor encompasses approximately 123 square miles.1 The school system includes one senior high school (9—12), a middle school (6-8), two elementary schools (K-5), and one K-3 elementary school. The total school population, K-12, is 2,102.2 All levels of socioeconomic status are repre- sented in the school pOpulation. Minority students comprise less than 1 percent of the school pOpulation. 1Interview with Assistant Superintendent of school district, February 20, 1975. 2Michigan Education Directory and Buyer's Guide (Lansing: Michigan Education Directory, 1974-1975), p. 180. 59 60 The school district representative of the suburban 3 The district location factor is eighteen square miles. is considered a residential suburban area within short commuting time of business, industrial, and cultural cen- ters of a central city. The school system includes one senior high school (10-12), two junior high schools (8-9, 6-7), and five elementary schools (K-5). The total school pOpulation is 4,489.4 .All socio-economic levels are rep- resented. The school population includes 5.85 percent minorities.5 The school district representing the central city location factor encompasses an area of fifty-five square miles.6 The central city is composed of a multi-ethnic pOpulation with industrial, business, and cultural centers. The school system includes four senior high schools (10-12), five junior high schools (7-9), and forty-seven elementary schools (K-6). The total school population is 29,702.7 All. socioeconomic levels are represented. Twenty-six percent of 3Descriptive Statistics, Board of Administration of school district. 4Michigan Education Directory and Buyer's Guide, Op. Cit.’ .p. 1730 5School District Student Services Office, informa- tion obtained through the Elementary and Secondary School Civil Rights Survey, Fall 1974. 6Descriptive Statistics, Board of Administration of school district. 7Michigan Education Directory and Buyer's Guide, Op. cit., p. 172. 61 the students are classified as minority students. Integra- tion is accomplished through the cluster program in the ele- mentary schools. A cluster, consisting of four schools, combines two schools with a majority of students being Of minority representation with two nearly all-white schools. In each of the three locations, a sufficient number of schools was randomly selected so that each of the sample populations would include a similar number of students. Two schools were used in the rural school district, three schools in the suburban district, and three in the central city district. The total population for this study included between eight and nine hundred students (K-6) at each of the location factors. A stratified random sample was used to generate groups containing an equal number of boys and girls at the ages of six, eight, and ten years. The total number in the sample was 270 children. Fifteen boys and fifteen girls were included at each of the age levels: six, eight, and ten years; and at each of the location factors: rural, suburban, and central city. Permission was granted by the schools and parents of sample children to participate in the study. In a small number of cases in which letters were not returned or per- mission was not granted, replacements for the children were randomly selected. 62 Methodology Personal interviews were used as the data-gathering method for the study. An instrument was developed that used photographs of occupational roles to minimize the amount of verbal description by the interviewer. The instrument measured the occupational role perceptions of elementary school children. The researcher conducted all interviews in January and February, 1975. The time involved in each individual interview was three to four minutes. One day was desig- nated for each school so that interviews could be completed within the day, thus avoiding any home communication among children from the same family. Interviews with all children from the same classroom were conducted within one period Of time, to avoid any communication among the children regarding the nature of the interview. All interviews took place on Tuesdays, Wednesdays, and Thursdays. Development Of the Instrument The instrument measuring the occupational role per— ceptions of elementary school children using pictures was based on Taylor's rationale: The use of pictures . . . has at least three distinct advantages: (a) all school children are accustomed to hearing stories based on pictures, (b) pictures 63 help to hold the attention of children, and (c) . . pictures provide an objective method of scoring.é In addition, the pictures as used in the instrument elimi- nated the need for a child to read. In developing the instrument, fifteen occupations were selected on the general preconceived idea that each was either a male or a female occupational role. As a validation procedure, seventy-eight adult graduate stu- dents were used as a try—out group to determine if they agreed about how each of the occupations is stereotyped in society. The results, shown in Table 3.1, indicated strong agreement on how the roles are normally stereotyped. Therefore, all fifteen occupations were included in the instrument measure. A black and white photograph was made portraying the setting Of each occupation. The photographs did not include any person(s) in the settings with the exception of the kindergarten classroom. The kindergarten scene included children involved in various activities; however, the teacher figure was not present. Thus, the child was not influenced by the presence of an adult in the photo- graphic setting. 8R. G. Taylor, Jr., "Racial Stereotypes in Young Children," in The Psychology of the Elementary School Child, ed. R. Binter and S. H. Frey (Chicago: Rand McNally, 1972), p. 257. 64 Table 3.1.--Occupations, roles, and validation results of role stereotyping by seventy-eight graduate students. Occupation Role Results Gas station attendant male 100% Nurse female 100% Farmer male 100% Kindergarten teacher female 99% Executive male 99% Secretary female 99% Waitress female 97% Truck driver male 97% - Fireman male 96% Telephone operator female 96% Garbage collector male 95% Homemaker female 94% Minister male 94% Dentist male 92% Doctor male 85% To avoid any ambiguity about the identification of the photographic settings, thirty children, ten children at each age level of six, eight, and ten years, were asked to identify the photograph fur answering the question, "What is this a picture of?" Verbal clues and comments were noted for use in writing the verbal description of the occupational role to be used in the interview. 65 For each occupational setting, an accompanying photographic silhouette pose of a male and a female model was used. The silhouettes were partially lighted, above- the-waist photographs. The same female and male models in different poses were used for the fifteen occupations, with apprOpriate attire for each occupation represented. The same thirty children used in identifying the photographic occupational settings were asked to identify the male and the female in each silhouette pair. This was done to insure that the photographs of men and women were recognized as such by the children. Each 8" x 10" photograph of the occupational set- ting was mounted On an 11" x 13" heavy-weight poster board. The individual cards were placed in a spiral-type book, allowing the card pages to be turned easily. The 3" x 5" silhouette photographs were mounted on poster board of the same size. Paired silhouette photographs were labeled to correspond to the appropriate occupational setting. These were placed in a file box. The introduction and the verbal descriptive state- ments for each occupational setting were included on the individual score sheets. The interview began with the interviewer reading the introduction to the child. Following the introduction, each child was shown the occupational settings and asked to 66 point to the person or persons who would work at that occu- pation. The instrument was designed to compare the per- ceptions of each individual to the traditional stereotypic views. The measure had three choices--male, female, or both--for each occupation. Responses were scored according to the traditional views of the occupational roles as Obtained by the validation procedure. Responses were given a score of "1" and "0." A stereotypic response received a score of "1"; a response of either the Opposite sex choice of the stereotypic answer or the answer "both" received a score of "0." The score can be interpreted as degree of stereotyping--the higher the score, the more stereotyped the subject. To determine the internal consistency reliability in the instrument measure, Cronbach's Coefficient Alpha Formula9 was employed. This formula is as follows: _ K 2 o 2 rxx _ K-1 (1 - 0x2 ) where r = reliability xx K = number of items 012 = item variances 2 = sum of CW = total test variance 9 L. J. Cronbach, "Coefficient Alpha and the Internal Structure of Tests," Psychometrika 16 (1951): 297-334. 67 The Coefficient Alpha formula is a generalization of the Kuder-Richardson formula, KR-20. Using this formula the instrument generated a reliability of .70. A reliability of approximately .75 is considered sufficient in a measure classified as an attitude scale.lo Design The three-way analysis of variance was used for the statistical analysis of the research data. Table 3.2 illustrates the design of the study. Testable Hypotheses The purpose of this study was to analyze elementary children's perceptions of sex-role stereotyping of occupa- tions as they vary with location, age, and gender. The following null hypotheses were tested: Hypothesis 1: There is no significant difference among the responses of the six year old, eight year Old, and ten year old children in their acceptance of occupa- tional roles for men and for women. Hypothesis 2: There is no significant difference between the responses of boys and the responses of girls in relating their acceptance Of occupational roles for men and for women. 10W. A. Mehrens and I. J. Lehmann, Measurement and Evaluation in Education and Psychology (Chicago: Holt, Rinehart & Winston, Inc., 1973), p. 570. 68 Table 3.2.--Design of the study. Location Age Gender 9: boys sh 6 ‘; girls Sh boys Rural 8 girls boys 10 girls boys 6 girls boys .Suburban 8 girls boys 10 girls boys 6 girls boys Central City 8 girls boys 10 girls Sh 69 Hypothesis 3: There is no significant difference among children from rural, suburban, and central city schools in their acceptance of occupational roles for men and for women. Pilot Study A pilot study was conducted to eliminate ambiguity in the language and to standardize procedures for admin- istering the test. An initial indication of reliability of the instrument was also sought. An Opportunity to familiarize the researcher with the actual administration procedure of the instrument was provided. 4 The pilot study was completed in two locations, rural and suburban. Thirty children in an elementary school at each location were randomly selected--five boys and five girls at each of the age levels: six, eight, and ten years. In total, sixty children were interviewed. The pilot study established the reliability of the instrument, thus eliminating any need for alteration. A summary of the results of the pilot study is found in Table 3.3. Summary In this chapter, the design Of the study was pre— sented and discussed. The sampled population included three school districts, each representative of the location fac- tor: rural, suburban, and central city. A stratified 70 random sample was taken to generate groups of fifteen boys and fifteen girls at the ages of six, eight, and ten years, at each location. The total number in the sample was 270 children. Table 3.3.--Ana1ysis of pilot data. Source of Mean Degrees of Variation Square Freedom F P less than Location .600 l .0796 .78 Age 61.1167 2 8.1039 .001 Gender 4.2667 1 .5657 .46 L x A 13.8500 2 1.8365 .17 L x G .0667 1 .0088 .93 A x G 21.0167 2 2.7867 .07 L x A x G 1.2167 2 .1613 .85 R: L A G 7.5417 48 The interview method, using an instrument in which photographs played the significant part, was selected to measure children's occupational role perceptions. The measure, including fifteen occupations that are stereotyped by society in general, compared the individual to the tra— ditional views Of stereotypeness. The three-way analysis of variance was used for the statistical analysis of the research data. The testable 71 hypotheses were presented. Results of the pilot study indicated the reliability of the instrument was acceptable, eliminating any need for alteration of the instrument. In Chapter IV, an analysis and discussion of the research data is presented. CHAPTER IV ANALYSIS AND DISCUSSION OF RESEARCH DATA The purpose of this chapter is to present, analyze, and discuss the data relevant to each hypothesis. The three-way analysis of variance was the method employed to analyze the data. A sample of 270 children was used. Three factors relevant to the study included location, age, and gender. Fifteen boys and fifteen girls were randomly selected at each of the age levels: six, eight, and ten years; at each location: rural, suburban, and central city. All findings are based on interviews employing the measurement instrument, which used photographs. The results Of the analysis of the research data are presented in Table 4.1. The following hypotheses, stated in null terms, were tested: Hypothesis 1 There is no significant difference among the responses of the six year old, eight year Old, and ten year Old children in their acceptance of occupational roles for men and for women. 72 73 Table 4.l.--Analysis of research data. Sifiiiiififi 52322. ”332233..“ F P less than Location 23.1593 2 2.5325 .08 Age 12.5815 2 1.3758 .25 Gender 8.8926 1 .9724 .33 L x A 3.0426 4 .3327 .86 L x G 10.6037 2 1.1595 .32 A x G 4.6259 2 .5058 .60 L x A x G 8.6537 4 .9463 .44 R: L A G 9.144974 252 In Table 4.1, the analysis of the data reveals an F score Of 1.3758 was Obtained for the age factor. This score was not significant; therefore, the null hypothesis was not rejected. The degree of stereotyping was high among all the subjects. Children at these ages were seeing most occupa- tions as either a masculine role or a feminine role, according to traditional views. In Table 4.2, a comparison of the observed combined means scores for the age factor is shown. The six year Olds had a combined means score of 12.03; the eight year Olds, a score of 11.60; and the ten year Olds had a score of 11.29. 74 Table 4.2.--A comparison of the Observed combined means scores for the age factor. Age Combined Means Score 6 year Olds 12.03 8 year Olds 11.60 10 year Olds 11.29 As shown in Table 4.3, most children appeared to be conservative in their thinking, following closely society's expectations of occupational roles for men and women. Except in a few cases, boys and girls tended to make similar kinds of role judgments. Looking at specific occupations, the least stereo- typed occupation was that of waitress, with a total of seventy-four boys and seventy-three girls responding to it as a female role. The occupation of dentist followed closely; it was perceived as a male role by eighty-two boys and eighty girls (refer to Table 4.4). Table 4.4.--Least stereotyped occupations. . NO. Boys Selecting NO. Girls Selecting Occupation Stereotypic Role Stereotypic Role Waitress 74 73 Dentist 82 80 75 Table 4.3.--Responses to occupational roles. No. Boys NO. Boys NO. Girls No. Girls Occupation Role Selecting Selecting Selecting Selecting Stereotypic Nonstereo- Stereotypic Nonstereo- Role typic Role Role typic Role Gas Station Attendant M 105 30 97 38 Nurse F 119 16 118 17 Farmer M 102 33 102 33 Kindergarten F 102 33 97 38 Teacher Executive M 108 27 88 47 Secretary F 100 35 95 40 Waitress F 74 61 73 62 TrFCk M 125 10 127 8 Driver Fire Fighter M 128 7 129 6 Teleph°ne F 86 49 102 33 Operator Garbage Collector M 121 14 115 20 Homemaker F 119 16 124 ll Minister M 120 15 115 20 Dentist M 82 53 8O 55 Doctor M 107 28 93 42 76 The most stereotyped occupations were fire fighter, truck driver, nurse, garbage collector, minister, and home- maker, as shown in Table 4.5. For the fire fighter, 128 boys and 129 girls responded to it as a male role. The truck driver received responses as a male role from 125 boys and 127 girls; the nurse as a female role by 119 boys and 118 girls; 121 boys and 115 girls said a garbage collector was a male; 120 boys and 115 girls viewed a minister as a male; and the homemaker was seen as a female role by 119 boys and 124 girls. Table 4.5.-—Most stereotyped occupations. N:..E:z:.:::e::izg Nariztipislzziiv Fire Fighter 128 129 Truck Driver 125 127 Nurse 119 118 Garbage Collector 121 115 Minister 120 115 Homemaker 119 124 The role Of the kindergarten teacher as a female was stereotyped more by the six year Olds; the eight and ten year Olds were more Open in their views, accepting both male and female for the kindergarten teacher, as seen in Table 4.6. 77 Most of the children viewing the kindergarten teacher as either male or female were in schools that had some male upper elementary teachers. Table 4.6.--Number of subjects according to ages viewing the kindergarten teacher role. Age Male Role Female Role Both 6 year Olds 4 78 8 8 year Olds 0 63 27 10 year Olds 1 57 32 Several isolated voluntary remarks contrary to the general conservative thinking were noted during the inter- views. An eight year Old boy said, "A woman can do it but I heard that sometimes a man can be a nurse." A ten year old girl made the choice of "both" for all the occupations, remarking at the end, "I see a lot of TV and there's a lot about Women's Lib on it. My sister's for that. My sister's about twenty years old." A ten year old boy, when asked to choose the gas station attendant, said, "They both can work in a gas station because I heard about Women's Lib." Another ten year old girl was extremely hesitant before making her choices. She made several remarks that indicated possibly a thought process was involved to a greater degree than by most of the children. For instance, 78 for the farmer she said, "Well, I think both could do it if the lady was strong enough." For the executive, the answer was, "I think the lady could do it if she was trained for it." "Anybody could do it who can drive" was the remark made for the truck driver. In conclusion, she said, "I think a woman could do anything a man can do." It is interesting that in the pilot a significant difference was observed for age (refer to Table 3.3). Reasons for the inconsistency Of the findings in the pilot and the study may be one or a combination of the following: (a) The greatest number of the ten year old children came from one classroom. The teacher's influence in the classroom regarding sex-role awareness and transmission of this to the children either consciously or unconsciously may be much greater than the average teacher's. (b) Because the P was at the .05 level of significance, the results fell to chance. One considera- tion in dealing with inferential statistics is that the null hypothesis can never be rejected with 100 percent certainty. There is always some error, however small. The probability level (G alpha) selected for rejecting the null hypothesis indicates how much risk of being wrong the researcher is willing to take. Hypothesis 2 There is no significant difference between the responses of boys and the responses of girls in relating their acceptance of occupatiOnal roles for men and for women. 79 Referring to Table 4.1, an F score of .9724 was observed for the gender factor. This score is not signifi- cant. Both boys and girls perceived occupational roles for both men and women closely to the traditional stereotypic views. In analyzing the combined means scores (Table 4.7), the boys combined means score of 11.82 is slightly higher than the girls' score of 11.46. The difference, however, is not significant; therefore, the null hypothesis was not rejected. This is in agreement with the pilot data results. Table 4.7.--A comparison of the observed combined means scores for the gender factor. Gender Combined Means Score Boys 11.82 Girls 11.46 Further examination of the data indicated a greater variation in the individual group means scores of the boys, shown in Table 4.8, than of the girls, Table 4.9. The range of variation is 3.40 in the boys' mean scores; ranging from a mean score of 9.87 for the suburban ten year Olds, to a score of 13.27, the mean of the rural six year old boys. In com- prison, the girls' mean scores ranged from 10.73 to 11.93, a variation of 1.20. 80 Table 4.8.--A comparison of the mean scores for boys. Location 6 Year Olds 8 Year Olds 10 Year Olds Rural 13.27 11.53 11.80 Suburban 10.73 12.00 9.87 Central City 12.80 12.40 12.00 Table 4.9.--A comparison of the mean scores for girls. Location 6 Year Olds 8 Year Olds 10 Year Olds Rural 11.93 11.80 11.40 Suburban 11.93 10.73 11.07 Central City 11.53 11.13 11.60 The six year old boys varied the most within age groups with mean scores of 10.73 (suburban), 12.80 (central city), and 13.27 (rural); the eight year Old boys varied the least with mean scores of 11.53 (rural), 12.00 (suburban), and 12.40 (central city). Of the girls' mean scores, the suburban eight year old girls were the most Open in acceptance of occupational roles for both men and women. The mean score was 10.73. The rural and suburban six year old girls stereotyped to the greatest degree, each having a mean score of 11.93, followed closely by the eight year Old suburban girls with a mean score of 11.80. 81 The occupations that had the greatest difference between the number of boys and the number of girls viewing them as the traditional role were executive, telephone Oper- ator, and doctor. As shown in Table 4.10, the number of boys who viewed the executive as a male was 108 as compared with 88 girls; 86 boys chose the telephone Operator to be female, whereas 102 girls chose the female role for the telephone Operator; and 107 boys chose a male doctor as compared to 93 girls. Table 4.10.v-Occupations with the greatest difference between boys' and girls' stereotypic responses. Occupation Boys Girls Executive 108 88 Telephone Operator 86 102 Doctor 107 93 Since the occupations of executive and doctor were stereotyped less by the girls, it may indicate the girls desired such professional roles for themselves, professions that require more education and training. In Table 4.11 are shown six of the fifteen occupa- tions that had the least difference, varying from zero to two, between the number of boys and of girls making similar kinds of role judgments. These included the occupations of 82 nurse, farmer, waitress, truck driver, fire fighter, and dentist. Table 4.1l.—-Occupations with the least difference between boys' and girls' stereotypic responses. Occupation Boys Girls Nurse 119 118 Farmer 102 102 Waitress 74 73 Truck Driver 125 127 Fire Fighter 128 129 Dentist 82 80 Hypothesis 3 There is no significant difference among chil- dren from rural, suburban, and central city schools in their acceptance of occupational roles for men and for women. In Table 4.1, the results for the location factor showed an F score of 2.5325. This score is not significant. Children living in rural, suburban, and central city loca- tions followed the traditional stereotypic views of accept- ing occupational roles for both men and women. The rural group had the highest combined means score, 11.96; the central city combined means score was 11.91, followed by the suburban group having the lowest score, 11.06, as seen in Table 4.12. 83 The difference is not significant; therefore, the null hypothesis was not rejected. In the pilot data results, the findings were similar. Table 4.12.-—A comparison of the observed combined means scores for the location factor. Location Combined Means Score Rural 11.96 Suburban 11.06 Central City 11.91 Interaction of Variables The data were analyzed for the interaction of the variables. The variables tested for interactions included age by gender, location by gender, location by age, and location by age by gender (Figures 1 through 4). The analy- sis indicated some differences in each interaction; however, no specific kinds of differences were revealed. The differ— ences were not significant in any case. 15.00 14.00 13.00 12.00 _ 11.00 H 10.00 9.00 Boys Girls 0000000. 6 yrs. yrs. 1 yrs. Figure l.--Interaction of variables age by gender. 84 15.00 14.00 13.00 12.00 11.00 " Boys 10.00 Glrls 0 O 0 o o . 9.00 CC Figure 2.--Interaction of variables location by gender. 15.00 14.00 13.00 12.00 . . 6 yrS. . . o o o 0 11.00 "’7 ' ’"‘ "‘ 7 8 yrs. ----- 10.00 9.00 10 yrs. R CC Figure 3.--Interaction of variables location by age. 15.00 6 yr. boys -——————— 14.00 8 yr, boys . . . . . . 13.00 10 yr. boys -- —-- 12.00 , ' 11.00 ’7 ' . 6yr. girls at x a: K x 10.00 Byr.girls _. —. -—. 9.00 lOyr.girls *°*- t- S CC Figure 4.--Interaction of variables location by age by gender. Summary In this chapter the analysis of data was presented. Obtained results for each of the three hypotheses of interest were presented. No significant differences were found in the responses of the children regarding their acceptance of 85 occupational roles for men and women. The three factors included location, age, and gender. In the final chapter, Chapter V, the summary and conclusions are presented. CHAPTER V SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS A brief summary of the study is presented at the beginning of this chapter, followed by the conclusions, implications of the study, and suggestions for further research. Summary The subject of this research was children's percep- tions of occupational roles that are traditionally stereotyped by society. Specifically, the differences among the views of children as they varied with age, gender, and location regard- ing their acceptance of occupational roles for men and for women were explored. The sampled pOpulation for the study included three school districts, each representative of the location factor: rural, suburban, and central city. A stratified random sample was taken to generate groups of fifteen boys and fif- teen girls at the ages of six, eight, and ten years, at each of the locations: rural, suburban, and central city. The total number in the sample was 270 children. The individual interview, using an instrument in which photographs played a significant part, was the method 86 87 selected to measure the occupational role perceptions of the children. The measure, including fifteen occupations that are stereotyped by society in general, compared each subject to the traditional views of stereotypeness. For each occupation, the response could be one of three choices: male, female, or both. A stereotypic response received a score of "1"; a response of either the Opposite sex choice of the stereotypic answer or the response "both" received a score of "0." Interpretation of the score resulted in degree of stereotyping; the higher the score the more stereo- typed the subject. The purpose of the study was to analyze elementary children's perceptions of sex—role stereotyping of occupa- tions as they varied with age, gender, and location. The testable hypotheses were: Hypothesis 1: There is no significant difference among the responses of the six year old, eight year Old, and ten year old children in their acceptance of occupational roles for men and for women. Hypothesis 2: There is no significant difference between the responses of boys and the responses of girls in relating their acceptance of occupational roles for men and for women. Hypothesis 3: There is no significant difference among children from rural, suburban, and central city schools 88 in their acceptance of occupational roles for men and for women. The three-way analysis of variance used for the statistical analysis of the data revealed no significant differences among the factors of age, gender, and location in the responses of the children regarding their acceptance of occupational roles for men and for women. Conclusions The conclusions drawn from the analysis of the data are: l. The degree of stereotyping was high among all subjects. Children perceived occupations as the role Of one sex or the other according to traditional stereotypic views. 2. Age had little or no effect on the child's accep- tance of occupational roles for men and for women. NO sig- nificant difference was found from the six year Olds to the eight year Olds to the ten year Olds. These findings indicate children learn quite early what occupational roles society expects of men and women, substantiating the findings of Beuf in 1973, Looft in 1971, and Iglitzen in 1971 and 1972.1 lA. Beuf, "Doctor, Lawyer, Household Drudge," Journal of Communication 24 (1974): 142-145; W. R. Looft, "Sex Dif- ferences in the Expression of Vocational Aspirations by Ele- mentary School Children," Developmental Psychology, September 89 The data in Beuf's study on sex-role perception of three to six year Old children indicated that although the Women's Liberation Movement has attracted much attention, children are still learning a sex—typed role structure, expecially in the occupational sphere. Also it revealed that young children, four and five years of age, hold the same kinds of stereotypes about careers that American society as a whole has, as noted by sociologists and Movement women. Preschool children, in addition to knowing their sex, seem— ingly know the social implications and limitations of that sexual definition. Looft's investigation of first and second graders, and the research conducted by Iglitzen with fifth-grade children, revealed that boys and girls learn early to aspire to sex-typed occupations. The sex—typed aspirations for girls are dreams that fall aside when they are asked to "be realistic," for then they see themselves doing what society has sanctioned women to do: clean house and raise children. Evidence of the socialization process has been revealed by the findings of such studies. Socialization, defined as the process by which an individual learns the given ways of a society, begins at the time of his birth.2 The 1971, pp. 366+; L. B. Iglitzin, "A Child's—Eye View of Sex Roles," Today's Education 9 (1972): 23-25. 2L. J. Stone and J. Church, Childhood and Adolescence, 3rd ed. (New York: Random House, 1973), p. 151; I. J. Gordon, Human DevelOpment, 2nd ed. (New York: Harper & Row, 1969), p. 15. 90 learning and internalization of "appropriate" patterns, values, and feelings is accomplished early in a child's life. Goodman stated that the three year old in any society knows his identity as a boy or girl, and is rapidly learning what is considered appropriate behavior for boys and for girls, for men and for women.3 The socialization of the child continues in school settings. Minuchin said of the school: Any school is a small society. It offers most chil- dren their first paradigm of a larger society beyond the family and subtly teaches something about the parameters of possible development. It offers models of adult roles and possible life-styles. . . .4 3. Location, whether rural, suburban, or central city, had no significant effect upon the child's acceptance of occupational roles for men and for women. It is generally assumed that the suburban child is in an environment that allows for a broader experience; in contrast, the rural child supposedly has a more limited experiential environment. If, in fact, this were true, the suburban child would be expected to be more Open to the acceptance of occupational roles for men and for women. The data revealed no such difference. Children in all locations were sex typing occupational roles according to traditional views. 3M. E. Goodman, The Culture of Childhood (New York: Teachers College Press, 1970), P. 27. 4P. Minuchin, "The Schooling of Tomorrow's Women," School Review 80 (1972): 200-201. 91 Several factors may contribute to the fact that loca- tions had no significant difference in the child's acceptance of occupational roles for men and for women. First, nearly all American children have easy access to television; it has become an important factor in their lives. The children respect television and "regard it almost as a 'third parent' in terms of affection and trust."5 LeBaron claimed tele- vision is one of the most powerful environmental influences on children. The typical child is "fully familiar with a wide variety of television programming before he is even "6 More than half of the pre- aware of school's existence. schooler's waking day is spent before a television set. After a child begins to attend school, the television still claims more of his time than the classroom. Sexism characterizes regular television programming for children as well as adults.7 The television, providing a wider range of role-taking models for the child than he 5W. Schramm, J. Lyle, and E. B. Parker, Television in the Lives of Our Children (Stanford, Ca.: Stanford Uni- versity Press, 1961), p. 30; cited by E. Christensen, "Tele- vision: Tiger by the Tail," Children and TV (Washington, D.C.: Association for Childhood Education International, 1967), p. 4. 6J. LeBaron, "School, Community and Television," Childhood Education, February 1975, p. 190. 7H. W.-Streicher, "The Girls in the Cartoons," Journal of Communication 24 (1974): 125—129; N. S. Tedesco, "Patterns in Prime Time," Journal of Communication 24 (1974): 92 ordinarily finds in his family, neighborhood, and school, extends the sex-role stereotyping of the culture.8 Mobility of families may account for the location factor's having had no significant effect upon the child's viewing sex roles with no difference. The rural population today includes many families who have moved from the central city and/or the suburbs to live in the rural area. Many children, therefore, may have experienced more than one loca- tion environment. 4. The number of years children had spent in school did not appear to lessen the degree of stereotyping. No significant difference was found from the first grade to the third grade to the fifth grade. These results suggest the question: "What is being done in schools concerning sex stereotyping?" What is being done in career education at the ele- mentary levels to eradicate stereotyping of sex roles? Are teachers making a conscious effort to alleviate sex stereo- typing in the activities of the school day? Are there inservice programs designed for and available to school per- sonnel regarding sex stereotyping? Is there a follow-up of the inservice training to measure its effectiveness? What is being done in teacher education programs to help stem the sex stereotyping of roles? 8F. Elkin, The Child and Society (New York: Random House, 1961), p. 73. 93 Implications of the Study_to Education Considering the current burst Of consciousness and universalization of sex roles in society, what are their effects on education today? In presenting the study to the three school systems, responses of individuals indirectly involved in the study were noted. These were the adminis- trators, teachers, and parents. The administrators' interest in the study was posi- tive. Without exception the principals and central office personnel indicated the study could be of special interest and importance in developing the career education aspect of that school system's curriculum. One assistant superinten- dent related that the previous year a workshop regarding sex-role stereotyping and sexism in the schools had been conducted for the teachers in the school district. An ele- mentary teacher, an intern teacher in the system, and one of the county's social workers had presented the workshOp. Attendance was voluntary, and no follow-up sessions were held. Some teachers in the participating schools expressed a sincere interest in the study. However, the teachers made no comments about teaching specific units on occupations or conscious efforts on the part of teachers to eliminate stereo- typing in the children's learning environment. Some teachers, questioned about whether they were doing anything specific regarding sex stereotyping, responded, "No, do you have a unit I could use?" 94 A limited number of parents wrote comments on the permission letters to the effect that the study was inter- esting and they would like to know the results. The study indicated interest alone is not enough to bring about a change in the existing condition. It is recognized that differential treatment of men and women exists in almost every segment and aspect Of our society. However, when such treatment appears in and is transmitted by the edu- cational institutions, which are supposed to provide all citizens with the tools to live in a democracy, it perhaps is the most damaging.9 Educators must be aware of how sexism is reinforced in the schools. An educator must be able to recognize "the insidious way abilities, values, roles, and rewards are assumed or assigned on the basis of a person's sex."10 Inservice training programs should be provided for school personnel, which would assist them in identification and correction of sex bias. Inservice training related to areas in which sex stereotyping prevails might focus on: (a) instructional materials, e.g., basic textbooks and library books; 9M. C. Dunkle and B. Sandler, "Sex Discrimination Against Students: Implications of Title IX of the Education Amendments of 1972," Inequality in Education 18 (October 1974): 12. lo"Recognizing Sexism in Your School" (Washington, D.C.: National Foundation for Improvement of Education), poster. 95 (b) classroom organization and management, e.g., grouping according to sex--"boys line up," "girls line up"; (c) teacher interaction, such as boys receiving more attention than girls, even though it may be negative, because of aggressive behavior; and teacher expectations, such as girls being quieter than boys; (d) curriculum, e.g., courses designed specifically for one sex or the other; (e) staffing and administrative organization--sex-role stereotyping is purveyed through the staffing patterns in the school settings, especially at the primary levels, with the majority being male principals and female classroom teachers; and (f) teacher beliefs, atti- tudes, and values whereby an Opportunity for educators to self-evaluate might check personal prejudices and biases regarding sex. Career education provides an Opportunity for schools to teach not only the world of work but attitudes toward occupational roles and the peOple in the roles. Beginning in the kindergarten year and continuing through secondary school, a bias-free career education curriculum should be designed so that develOpment of an individual's self-concept and ability is not limited. Teacher education needs to go beyond the conscious- ness level of sex stereotyping. When teachers know themselves, the attitudes they possess and why, they can take action toward making changes in the existing curriculum. 96 The elementary school is the first setting beyond the family that includes the years when basic attitudes, perceptions, and skills are being developed. "All human beings (children) should enjoy the rights that are theirs by virtue of their being human, and not one iota of their rights should ever be abridged on the ground Of sex: . . ."11 Schools can and should be the most powerful influence in readjusting the sexes to each other. Harmony and OOOper— ativeness is the essential human state; culture has managed to produce a complex separateness and lack of understanding between the sexes. To break down this separateness, the sexes must be educated for each other, not in opposition to each other.12 Educators must realize how great the responsibility of the school is: whether it remains a force in perpetuat- ing the stereotyping of sex roles or becomes a force in the elimination of stereotyping in our culture. Suggestions for Further Research Through the process of the current study, several questions arose. These questions, reformulated, are included as suggestions for further study. llA. Montague, The Natural Superiority of Women (New York: Collier Books, 1968), p. 209. lzIbid., p. 199. 97 1. Replication studies using larger samples are seen as useful to yield additional data for support or rejec- tion of the current study results. 2. Using the current design but expanding the instrument to include a greater range of occupations would be of interest to see if the narrow variation of answers would still prevail. 3. Replication studies using different age groups are seen as useful to determine to what degree the preschool child stereotypes occupational roles and/or the junior high and high school individuals accept the occupational roles for men and for women. 4. Research should be undertaken using schools in which career education has been implemented into the curric- ulum, as compared to schools that have not had career educa- tion. Such research could provide information about the amount of stereotyping that might be eliminated through the career education program of instruction. APPENDICES 98 APPENDIX A PERMISSION LETTER 99 APPENDIX A PERMISSION LETTER January 13, 1975 To the Parent(s) of : I am doing a study approved by the School District of which School was randomly selected. School District is one of three school systems participating in the study. It is a study on children's views of occupational roles. Your child has been randomly selected to participate. The only personal information required concerning your child is the birthdate. I will have a 3—4 minute interview with your child, using photos of occupational settings and silhouette photos. I will read a brief introduction to your child indicating this is not a test. There are no right or wrong answers. For instance, as your child sees the photograph of a garage, I will say, "Here is a garage. Choose the mechanic." I will mark your child's response on a score sheet. Your child will remain anonymous in the study of 270 children. A report of the study will be available at the school. If you have any questions, please call me. I will be most appreciative for your child's participation. Sincerely yours, Ruth Scheresky MSU Doctoral Candidate Phone: 332-0902 Please indicate whether you wish your child to participate and return before or on . My child may participate in the study. My child may not participate in the study. Signature of Parent 100 APPENDIX B SCORE SHEET 101 School APPENDIX B SCORE SHEET Date Name Code Birthdate Age INTRODUCTION: Today, I want you to help me by making some choices. There are no right or wrong answers. tures of places where people work or jobs people do. choose whether a man or a woman or both could do the job. A. Here is a gas station. B. Here is a hospital room. C. Here is a farm. D. Here is a kindergarten classroom. Choose the gas station attendant ...... Choose the nurse....... ..... ......... Choose the farmer............................. Choose the kindergarten teacher......................................... E. Here is an executive's office. Choose the executive........... F. Here is a secretary's desk. Choose the secretary.............. G. Here is a restaurant. Choose the person who waits on people... H. Here is a truck. Choose the truck driver.. ........ ............ I. Here is a fire truck. Choose the fire fighter................. J. Here is a telephone switchboard in an office. Choose the telephone Operator.............................. ....... .. K. Here is a garbage truck. Choose the garbage collector......... L. Here is a laundry room in a home. Choose the homemaker, or the one who does the ironing.............................. M. Here is a church. Choose the minister......................... N. Here is a dentist's chair. Choose the dentist................. 0. Here is an emergency room in a hospital. Choose the doctor.... 102 This is not a test. We are going to look at some pic- They you will M APPENDIX C THE INSTRUMENT 103 . '.. 4.17 104 105 106 107 108 109 110 111 ”‘4 ; >3 112 113 114 115 - 116 117 118 119 120 121 Rut-4.. 1.1”)... (.1 . . -uwi 122 123 5‘ IV “I.-. O 124 125 126 127 128 129 J..— 130 131 km... 1.. ..a...... a 132 133 BIBLIOGRAPHY 1 3,4 BIBLIOGRAPHY Allport, G. W. The Nature of Prejudice. New York: Doubleday, 1958. Almy, M.; Chittenden, E.; and Miller, P. YoungoChildren's Thinking. New York: Teachers College, 1967. Baller, W. R., and Charles, D. C. The Psychology of Human Growth and Development.- New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston, 1968. ‘ Bandura, A. Readings in Child Behavior and Development. Edited by C. S. Lavatelli and F. Stendler. New York: Harcourt, Brace, Jovanovich, 1972. Bem, D. J. Beliefs, Attitudes, and Human Affairs. Monterey, Ca.: Brooks/Cole, 1970. Bem, S. L., and Bem, D. J. "Training the Woman to Know Her Place." Pennsylvania Department of Education: Division of Pupil Personnel Services, 1973. Bernard, H. W., and Huckins, W. C., eds. Exploring Human Development. Boston: Allyn and Bacon, 1972. Beuf, A. "Doctor, Lawyer, Household Drudge." Journal of Communication 24 (1974): 142-145. Binter, A. R., and Frey, S. H., eds. The Psychology of the Elementary School Child. Chicago: Rand McNally, 1972. Britton, G. E. "Sex Stereotyping and Career Roles." Journal Of Readipgyl7 (1973): 140-148. Bronfenbrenner, U. Exploring Human Development. Edited by H. W. Bernard and W. C. Huckins. Boston: Allyn and Bacon, 1972. Brown, D. G. "Sex-Role Development in a Changing Culture." Psychological Bulletin 55 (1958): 232-242. Burton, G. "Sex-Role Stereotyping in Elementary School ' Primers." Pittsburgh: KNOW, 1974. (Mimeographed.) 135 136 Cannan, C. "Female From Birth?" The Times Education Sup- plement 2956 (January 14, 1972): 20+. Courtney, A.E., and Lockeretz, S. W. "A Woman's Place: An Analysis of the Roles Portrayed by Women in Magazine Advertisements." Journal of Marketing Research 8 (1971): 92-105. Courtney, A. E., and Whipple, T. W. "Women in TV Commercials." Journal of Communication 24 (1974): 110-118. Cronbach, L. J. "Coefficient Alpha and the Internal Struc- ture of Tests." Psychometrika 16 (1951): 297-334. de Beauvoir, Simone. The Second Sex. New York: Vintage, 1974. - "Decoding the Messages." The Awareness Game 6 (1973): 8. Denzin, N. K. Children's Liberation. Edited by D. Gottlieb. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall, 1973. Dick and Jane as Victims. Princeton, N.J.: Women on Words and Images, 1972. Dunkle, M. C., and Sandler, B. "Sex Discrimination Against Students: Implications Of Title IX of the Education Amendments of 1972." Inequality in Education 18 (October 1974): 12-35. Elkin, F. The Child and Society. New York: Random House, 1961. "Fact Bombardment." The Awareness Game 6 (1973): 30-31. Feshbach, N., and Feshbach, S. The Young Child. Edited by W. W. Hartup. Washington, D.C.: National Associa- tion for the Education Of Young Children, 1972. 51% Minority Connecticut Conference on the Status of Women. Washington, D.C.: National Education Association, 1972. Franzwa, H. H. "Working Women in Fact and Fiction." Journal of Communication 24 (1974): 104-109. Frazier, N., and Sadker, M. Sexism in School and Society. New York: Harper & Row, 1973. Gesell, A. Infant Development. New York: Harper, 1952. 137 Gewirtz, J. L., and Stingle, K. C. "Learning of Generalized Imitation as the Basis for Identification." Psycho- logical Review 75 (1968): 374-375. Goodman, M. E. The Culture of Childhood. New-York: Teachers College Press, 1970. Gordon, I. J. Exploring Human DevelOpment. Edited by H. W. Bernard and W. C. Huckins. Boston: Allyn & Bacon, 1972. . Human Development. 2nd ed. New York: Harper, 1969. Gottlieb, D. Children's Liberation. Edited by D. Gottlieb. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall, 1973. , ed. Children's Liberation. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall, 1973. Guskin, A. E., and Guskin, S. L. A Social Psychology of_ Education. Reading, Mass.: Addison-Wesley, 1970. Harrison, B. G. Unlearning the Lie Sexism in School. New York: William Morrow, 1974. Hartley, R. E. "Current Patterns in Sex Roles: Children's Perspectives." National Association of Women Deans and Counselors 25 (1961): 3-13. . "Sex-Role Pressures and the Socialization of the Male Child." Poychological Roports 5 (1959): 457-468. , and Hardestly, F. P. The Psychology Of the Ele- mentary School Child. Edited by A. R. Binter and S. H. Frey. Chicago: Rand McNally, 1972. Hartup, W. W., ed. The Young Child. Washington, D.C.: National Association for the Education of Young Children, 1972. ' "Have You Heard???" Sexism in Education. Minneapolis: The Emma Willard Task Force, 1973. Havighurst, R. J. The Psychology of the Elementary School Child. Edited by A. R. Binter and S. H. Frey. Chicago: Rand McNally, 1972. Healy, R., and Lund, D. "Chapter 622: One State's Mandate." Inegualipy in Education 18 (October 1974): 36—46. 138 Howe, F. "Sexual Stereotypes Start Early." Saturdopreview 54 (October 16, 1971): 76-77+. Hoyt, J. "Target: Sex Bias in Education." American Educa- tion 10 (1974): 6-9. Iglitzin, L. B. "A Child's-Eye View of Sex Roles." Today's Education 9 (1972): 23-25. Jacobs, C., and Eaton, C. "Sexism in the Elementary School." Today's Education, December 1972, pp..20-22. Kerins, A., in Levy, B., and Stacey, J. "Sexism in the Elementary School: A Backward and Forward Look." Phi Delta Kappan, 1973, pp. 105-109+. King, E. W., and Kerber, A. The Sociology of Early Childhood Education. New York: American, 1968. Koontz, E. D. "Education for Survival." Non-Sexist Educa- tion for Survival. Washington, D.C.: National Education Association, 1973. Kremer, B. J. "The Adjective Check List as an Indicator of Teachers' Stereotypes of Students." Ph.D. disserta- tion, Michigan State University, 1965. Lavatelli, C. S., and Stendler, F., eds. Readings in Child Behavior and Development. New York: Harcourt, Brace, Jovanovich, 1972. LeBaron, J. "School, Community and Television." Childhood Education, February 1975, pp. 190-194. Levy, B., and Stacey, J. "Sexism in the Elementary School: A Backward and Forward Look." Phi Delta Kappan, October 1973, pp. 105-109+. Lewis, M. "Parents and Children: Sex-Role Development." School Review 80 (1972): 229-240. Liebert, R.; McCall, R.; and Hanratty, M. "Effects of Sex— Typed Information on Children's Toy Preferences." Journal of Genetic Ppychology 119 (1971): 133-136. Looft, W. R. "Sex Differences in the EXpression of Voca- tional Aspirations by Elementary School Children." Developmental Psychology, September 1971,;mu 366+. 139 Lyon, C. D., and Saario, T. N. "Women in Public Education: Sexual Discrimination in Promotion." Phi Delta Kappan, October 1973, pp. 120-123. Martinez, S. "Sexism in Public Education: Litigation Issues." Inequality in Education 18 (1974): 5-11. McCandless, B. R. Children Behavior and Development. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston, 1967. McNeil, E. B. The Psychology of the Elementary School Child. Edited by A. R. Binter and S. H. Frey. Chicago: Rand McNally, 1972. Minuchin, P. "The Schooling of Tomorrow's Women." School Review 80 (1972): 200-201. Mead, M., in Gordon, I. J. Human Development. New York: Harper & Row, 1969. . Male and Female: A Study of the Sexes in a Changing World. New York: Dell, 1973. Mehrens, W. A., and Lehmann, I. J. Measurement and Evaluation in Education and Psychology. Chicago: Holt, Rinehart and Winston, 1973. Michigan Education Directory and Buyer's Guide. Lansing: Michigan Education Directory, 1974-1975. Millar, S. The Psychology of Play. Baltimore: Penguin, 1968. Montagu, A. The Natural Superiority of Women. New York: Collier Books, 1968. Montemayor, R. "Children's Performance in a Game and Their Attraction to It as a Function of Sex-Typed Labels." Child Development 45 (1974): 152-156. Mosher, J., et a1. 51% Minority Connecticut Conference on the Status of Women. Washington, D.C.: National Education Association, 1972. Mussen, P. H.; Conger, J. J.; and Kagan, J. Child Develop- ment and Personality. New York: Harper & Row, 1974. Olds, S. W. "Giving Children the Freedom to Be Themselves." The Awareness Game 6 (1973). 140 Piaget, J., in Almy, M.; Chittenden, E.; and Miller, P. Young Children's Thinking. New York: Teachers College, 1967. "Recognizing Sexism in Your School." Washington, D.C.: National Foundation for Improvement of Education. (Poster.) Rogers, D. Readings in Child Psychology. Belmont, Ca.: Brooks/Cole, 1969. Rosenberg, B. G., and Sutton-Smith, B. "Revised Conception of Masculine-Feminine Differences in Play Activi- ties." The Journal of Genetic Psychology 96 (1960): 165-170. Rosenthal, R., and Jacobson, L. Pygmalion in the Classroom. Chicago: Holt, Rinehart & Winston, 1968. Saario, T. N.; Jacklin, C. N.; and Tittle, C. K. "Sex Role Stereotyping in the Public Schools." Harvard Edu- cational Review 43 (1973): 386-416. Sadker, M., and Sadker, D. "Sexism in Schools: An Issue for the 70's." The Journal of the National Association for Women Deans, Administrators, and Counselors 37 (1974): 69-74. Sax, G. Empirical Foundations of Educational Research. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall, 1968. Schramm, W.; Lyle, J.; and Parker, E. B., in Christensen, E. "Television: Tiger by the Tail." Children and TV. Washington, D.C.: Association for Childhood Education International, 1967, pp. 3-6. Sears, P., and Feldman, D. "Teacher Interaction With Boys and Girls." The National Elementary Principal, November 1966, pp. 30-37. Sex Discrimination in an Elementary Reading Program. Lansing: Michigan Women's Commission. Sexism in Education. 3rd ed. Minneapolis: The Emma Willard Task Force on Education, 1973. Slakter, M. J. Statistical Inference for Educational Researchers. Reading, Mass.: Addison-Wesley, 1972. 141 Stacey, J.; Bereaud, S.; and Daniels, J. And Jill Came Tumbling_After: Sexism in American Education. New York: Dell, 1974. Stone, L. J., and Church, J. Childhood and Adolescence. 3rd ed. New York: Random House, 1973. Streicher, H. W. "The Girls in the Cartoons." Journal of Communication 24 (1974): 125-129. Taylor, R. G., Jr. The Psychologyoof the Elementary School Child. Edited by A. R. Binter and S. H. Frey. Chicago: Rand McNally, 1972. Taylor, S. S. 51% Minority Connecticut Conference on the Status of Women. Washington, D.C.: National Edu- cation Association, 1972. Tedesco, N. S. "Patterns in Prime Time." Journal of Com- munication 24 (1974): 119-124. Tobias, S. 51% Minority Connecticut Conference on the Status of Women. Washington, D.C.: National Education Association, 1972. White, L. R. Nonsexist Education for Survival. Washington, D.C.: National Education Association, 1973. MICHIGAN STATE UNIV. LIBRnRIES (I))lWlWIIHIIW/I”IIIWIIHIHIIIUIV)lllllHllllHl 31293100276751