.:_\___:__‘_*_:_‘__:+__\__,_:_=___ mwm This is to certify that the thesis entitled Physical Growth Characteristics of Early, Average, and Late Maturing Females Grouped According to Age at Peak Height Velocity presented by 2 Crystal Diane Fountain has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for Ph.D. Jegreein HPE&R Major profess @//J%fl/ [hue November 9, 1979 0-7 639 gfiygthlJEfiifi,v‘ ‘;<- ._ J A-d .{_‘_ ft .; W ‘ 3 mafia l: , _:&:2v 02%;; OVERDUE FINES ARE 25¢ PER DAY . PER ITEM Return to book drop to remove this checkout from your record. PHYSICAL GROWTH CHARACTERISTICS OF EARLY, AVERAGE, AND LATE MATURINC FEMALES GROUPED ACCORDING TO AGE AT PEAK HEIGHT VELOCITY BY Crystal Diane Fountain A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Health, Physical Education and Recreation 1979 ABSTRACT PHYSICAL GROWTH CHARACTERISTICS OF EARLY, AVERAGE, AND LATE MATURING FEMALES GROUPED ACCORDING TO AGE AT PEAK HEIGHT VELOCITY By Crystal Diane Fountain The specific purposes of this study were to (1) determine if com- binations of physical growth measures taken during childhood could be useful in predicting age at peak height velocity and onset of menstrua- tion; (2) investigate differences in the magnitude and duration of the adolescent peak height spurt for early, average, and late maturing fe- males; and (3) examine the relationship between age at peak height velocity and age of menarche among the three maturity groups. Longitudinal data on five parameters of growth (standing height, sitting height, biacromial diameter, biiliac width, weight) and recall data on menarche were used to evaluate developmental patterns and ma- turation rates of sixty-five female subjects. Level of maturity (early average, late) was chosen as the independent variable in this study and was determined by the age at peak height velocity for each girl. Early maturers (n=21) had peak height velocity before 11.5 years of age, average maturers (n=28) experienced peak velocity between 11.5 and 12.5 years, inclusive, while late developers (n=l6) attained their peak height velocity after 12.5 years. Crystal Diane Fountain The findings indicate that accurate predictions of age at peak height velocity (APHV) and age of menarche (MA) may be difficult to ob- tain from measures of physical growth taken at nine years of age. Low relationships existed among all the physical growth variables and APHV and MA. The highest coefficient obtained was -.37 between biacromial width and MA. Predictive equations for APHV and MA were similar. Of the five growth parameters measured, the biacromial and biiliac widths provided the best predictors of APHV, accounting for 11.2% of the variance. The stepwise regression procedure yielded the following equation: APHV = 18.822 - .389(Biacromial) + .216(Biiliac) i_.969 The prediction of MA from the growth measures showed that a higher per- centage of the variance (15.0%) was explained than in the prediction of APHV, but that a larger standard error of the estimate existed. The regression was: MA = 19.837 - .510(Biacromia1) + .624(Standing Height) : 1.092 A multivariate analysis of variance revealed that the maturity main effect was significant (p‘(.0001). Univariate F procedures, step- down F tests, and discriminant function analyses indicated that spurt height (SH), spurt duration (SD), peak height velocity (PHV), and in- terval had important roles in differentiating the three maturityrgroups. Significant differences occurred between early and late maturers for SD, PHV, and interval. Moreover, the early developing females also differed statistically from the average maturers on PHV and interval, with the early maturing females displaying a longer SD and interval Crystal Diane Fountain and a higher PHV. These data indicated that early maturers entered their adolescent growth cycle at a younger age but grew for a longer time before maturity than average or late developers. It seems that the later developing female is nearer her complete biological matura- tion at the time of PHV than the early maturer and, therefore, has a shorter interval to MA. DEDICATION To all those teachers who have provided me with the background necessary for graduate study, the desire to learn more, and the incen- tive to strive for higher goals. Special dedication to the following physical educators who had a profound influence in directing my life and chosen profession: Floyd Anderson Natalie Barfield Lea Barrett Delene Darst Anne Hadarits Mary Jane Nicholson Jean Osborne ii ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS I wish to express my sincere gratitude to Professor Vern Seefeldt for his guidance, advice, and support during the five years of my grad- uate study. His "open door policy" and accessibility provided numerous teachable moments that accounted for a major part of my graduate educa— tion. Especially for the extra time he spent during the completion of this dissertation, I thank him. My appreciation also is extended to Professor John Haubenstricker who has given freely of his time and provided invaluable advice on many occasions. I also wish to thank Professors William Heusner and William Schmidt for their assistance on the doctoral committee. To Dr. Dan Gould and Dr. Kwok-Wai Ho, I give my thanks. Their stimulating discussions and help in the data interpretation were a tremendous aid in the completion of this study. Special bouquets are given to my friends who, in various ways, helped me through the ups and downs of preparing a dissertation -- Kathy, Mary, Bev, Dale, and Mo. I will be forever grateful to you. TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES . LIST OF FIGURES . . . CHAPTER I THE PROBLEM . Statement of the Problem Significance of the Study . Research Hypotheses . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Research Plan . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Scope of the Investigation Definitions . . . . . II REVIEW OF THE LITERATURE . Pattern of Growth . . . . . . Phases of Distance Graph . . . . . Characteristics of Velocity Graphs . Techniques of Curve-fitting . . . . . . . . . . Variability in Growth . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Parameters of Growth . . . . . . . Magnitude of Peak Height Velocity . . . . . Duration of Peak Height Spurt . . . . . . . . Relationship of Age at Menarche to Age at Peak Height Velocity . . . . . . . . . . . Prediction of Menarche and Age at Peak Height Velocity Predictions Based on Skeletal Age . . . . . . . . Predictions Based upon Secondary Sex Characteristics . Predictions Based upon Height, Weight, and Body Build Recall of Menarche . . . . . . . . . . . . Summary . . . . . III RESEARCH METHODS . . . . . . . . . Background of the Study . Sampling Procedures . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Sample of the Motor Performance Study . . . . . . . Sub-sample of the Motor Performance Study . . . . Experimental Design . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Dependent Variables . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Independent Variable . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . ConductofTreatments Description of Measurements . . . . . . . . . . . iv vi vii 10 l3 14 17 17 18 20 22 23 24 26 3O 31 35 35 37 37 38 38 39 40 40 41 IV Procurement of Menarche Data . . . . . . . . . . . . Processing the Data . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Preparation of Data on Physical Growth . . . . . . . Preparation of Data on Menarche . . . . . . . . Procedures for Statistical Analyses . . . . . . . Summary . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . RESULTS AND DISCUSSION . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Predictability of Age at Peak Height Velocity and Menarche . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Differences in the Adolescent Growth Spurt among Early, Average, and Late Maturing Females . . . . . . . Discussion . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . SUMMARY, CONCLUSIONS, AND RECOMMENDATIONS . . . . . Suma ry O O I O O O O O I C O C O O O O O 0 Conclusions . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Recommendations . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . APPENDIX A . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . APPENDIX B O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O 0 APPENDIX C O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O BIBLIOGRAPHY O 0 O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O 0 I O O O O O 42 43 43 43 44 44 46 46 52 57 61 61 63 64 66 68 69 76 4—1 4-2 4-3 4-4 4-5 4-6 LIST OF TABLES Measures of Physical Growth Obtained Semi-annually . . Time Interval Between Age at Peak Height Velocity and Menarcheal Age Motor Performance Study: Motor Performance . Research Model . Means and Standard Deviations of the Variables Used to Predict Age at Peak Height Velocity and Menarche Intercorrelation Coefficients of Growth Measures and Body Size Indices at Nine Years, Age at Peak Height Velocity, and Menarche Stepwise Multiple Regression of Predictor Variables on Age at Peak Height Velocity and Menarche Age Regression Coefficients of Predichr Variables Selected Measures of Physical Growth and on the Basis of the Adjusted R Statistic . Intercorrelation Matrix for the Five Dependent Variables of the Adolescent Growth Cycle Maturity Main Effect . Vector Means and Standard Deviations of the Dependent Variable by Maturity Level Results of the Scheffé Post Hoc Tests for the Dependent Variables that Were Affected Significantly by Maturity Level vi 21 36 39 47 48 50 51 53 53 55 57 LIST OF FIGURES 2-1 The Four Phases of Growth in Standing Height for Human Beings O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O 0 2-2 Typical Velocity Curve for Gain in Standing Height for Females O O O O I O O O O O O O O O O O O O 0 2-3 Definitions of the Parameters of the Growth Cycle of Human Beings . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2—4 Example of the Graphical Technique Used to Interpolate the Growth Data . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 4-1 Mean Dependent Variable Scores for the Maturity biain Effect 0 O O O O O O O O O O O O I O O O O 0 vii CHAPTER I THE PROBLEM Physical growth of human beings is highly predictable. Measures of length and breadth follow a definitive pattern of developmeng.encom- passing four phases. However, children proceed through their growth cycles at different rates, and individual variations in physical growth and motor abilities occur throughout the childhood and adolescent years. During the circumpuberal period, subjects can readily be divid- ed into early, average, or late maturers according to their age at the onset of adolescent growth. It seemed appropriate, then, to determine whether the attainment of specific physical and biological events asso- ciated with puberty can be predicted from anthropometric data obtained in childhood. Statement of the Problem The purpose of this research effort was three—fold. First, this study was designed to decide if childhood measures of height, weight, body size, and/or body shape could be used to predict age at peak height velocity and the onset of menstruation in females. Second, the study investigated differences in the magnitude and duration of the adolescent peak height spurt for early, average, and late maturing females. A third area of investigation was to determine the relation- ship between age of menarche and age at peak height velocity among the three maturity groups. Significance of the Study It is common knowledge that children often are:compared to a stan- dard in some area of performance or development; for example, the fif- tieth percentile of the fifty yard dash or the average heightznulweight for a specific chronological age. Too often, all children are judged by the mean values obtained from cross sectional data. It would be more appropriate to evaluate individuals according to standards from a longitudinal study of subjects selected from similar environmental cir- cumstances. Therefore, one objective of this study was to develop cri- teria to assess a girl's progress toward physical maturation according to the appropriate growth pattern of an early, average, or late devel- oping female and not necessarily according to the "normal" cycle. Physical growth and body size have an effect on an individual's ability to perform motor skills. Physical educators and athletic coaches are constantly aware of the need to match students in some sports on the basis of size in order to provide healthy and enjoyable activity for them. The results from this investigation could help classify females by maturation criteria. These groupings then could be used to investigate whether classification of females by maturity level accounts for differences in athletic ability or motor perfor- mance. If such differences were reported, then the maturation criteria would be useful in equalizing competition on youth athletic teams or in physical education and recreational activity settings. This study also would provide the format whereby data relating maturity level to motor performance of males could be analyzed. Research Hypotheses It was hypothesized that: l. A selected number of childhood measures including height, weight, and breadths can account for a substantial per- centage of the variance in age at peak height velocity and age at menarche. 2. The magnitude of the spurt will be greater and the dura- tion of adolescent growth will be longer in the early maturing girls compared to the average and late maturers. The same relationship will exist between the average ver- sus the late maturing groups. 3. The time interval between age at peak height velocity and age at menarche will be inversely related to the age at peak height velocity. Research Plan Data on five parameters of growth were collected semi-annually on the subjects (Table 1-1). In most cases the measurements on the Table l-l: Measures of Physical Growth Obtained Semi-Annually Weight Standing Height Sitting Height Biacromial Diameter Biiliac Diameter girls were obtained first during early childhood and continued semi- annually for the next twelve years. All records containing continuous data from nine years of age through the completion of the adolescent growth spurt were included in the analyses. In addition, menarche data were obtained on the subjects by the recall method. A letter explaining the study and requesting the age and/or date of first menstruation was sent to the girls and/or their mothers (Appendix A). Several guidelines were mentioned in the letter to facilitate the accurate recording of a specific date or time of menarche. Scope of the Investigation This investigation was delimited to the growth patterns and mea- surements of females during middle childhood and encompassing the ado- lescent growth spurt, but excluding the attainment of mature stature. Factors that could influence growth, such as diet, disease, or compe- titive activities, were not studied. Subject selection was limited to 65 females enrolled in the longitudinal Motor Performance Study at Michigan State University on whom data were available from middle childhood through adolescence. The following assumptions were made: 1. The data are representative of other females in the Motor Performance Study and throughout the State of Michigan. 2. Measures of physical growth are stable over time. 3. The subjects were honest in their responses concerning age at menarche. Potential weaknesses existed in the endeavor. One limitation of the study was that the anthropometric measurements were taken by four different investigators throughout the length of the study. Three of these, though, were all instructed and tested for reliability and accuracy by the fourth person, the primary investigator of the Motor Performance Study. A second limitation was that recall of menarche is not entirely accurate. Therefore, adjustment was made for the sys- tematic underestimation of menarcheal age by adding six months to the age cited in the imprecise responses (see Chapter 3, page 30 for a de- tailed explanation). Finally, errors of recording or measurement could have been made. An attempt was made to minimize such errors by having the recorder compare each measurement to the one documented at the previous measurement period. Clarification or remeasurement was requested if a discrepancy, such as regression in the length of a long bone, was reported. Definitions The following definitions will aid the understanding of this study: 1. Acrom—radiale--upper arm length 2. Adolescent growth spurt--period of accelerated growth fol- lowing childhood; also called "puberty" and the "circum- puberal period" 3. Biacromial width--shoulder width 4. Biiliac width-~hip width 5. Chronological age--time since birth 10. ll. 12. 13. 14. Development--used interchangeably with growth and matura— tion; usually denotes a combination of the two Girth—-measure of circumference Growth--changes in physical properties, such as gain in height and weight Maturation--changes in functional capacity, such as sexual development Menarche-~first menstruation Peak height velocity--maximum annual increment in height during the growth spurt Radio-stylion—-forearm length Skeletal age--degree of development of the bones of the body Skinfold--measure of subcutaneous fat CHAPTER II REVIEW OF LITERATURE Normal human beings have highly predictable patterns of growth during their lives. From conception until maturity, marked changes in physical growth occur. For example, measures of height, weight, leg length, and arm girth all change with development. It was the purpose of this review to: (l) examine the pattern of growth in stature for females; (2) discuss individual growth parameters, especially those as- sociated with the adolescent spurt; (3) inspect the relationship of menarche to other variables of physical growth; and (4) report the re- liability of menarcheal data obtained via the recall method. Pattern of Growth When absolute measures of length and breadth are plotted, the re- sulting curve is S-shaped (Figure 2-1). This distance curve shows a cumulative gain in stature throughout the growing years, or actual height attained at successive chronological ages. The cumulative curve is a basic description of the additive outcome of stature or a record of the distance traveled toward final adult height. The oldest longi- tudinal record of stature was provided by de Montbeillard who measured and noted his son's height from birth to eighteen years of age during the years of 1759 to 1777 (Scammon, 1927). The normal patterncflfstand- ing height can be divided into four phases as shown in Figure 2-1 (Scammon, 1927; Deming, 1957; Malina, 1975, p. 8). A.mmma .maHHmz Eouw csmuwmn cam cmuamv..05). The findings indicate that good predictions of APHV and MA may be difficult to obtain from measures of physical growth taken at nine years of age. In general, low relationships existed between all the variables and the ages at PHV and menarche (Table 4-2). Of the four indices used, 48 3E fiomoua 233 52: figs 533 “Ewan; ”Ewan; >mm< zoom essay amaaH Hmumwcom ustmB omHHfiHm HmHEouomHm wefiuuwm wcfivcmum mo. mH.I ma. mH.I ma. HN.I HN.I am.I oa.I oa.I <2 oo.I mm. mo. mo.I oH.I oH.I mm.I oH.I -.I >mm< as. as. oo.I mm. as. mm. so. mm. xooaa oaasa soon so. mN.I as. on. H~.I mo. oo. xmoaa snowman ensue sm.I am. oh. Hm. mo.I aH.I canon unmana\uaaaa mm.I oN.I mo.I so. am. swoon anemones ea. mo. so. No. names: mo. mm. am. .auoaa smashes mo. no. snows Haaaononam so. banana waauoam Ame n av wnopmamz mam .muwooam> unwwmm xwmm um mm< .mumow mcfiz um mmowocH exam atom mam mmusmmmz nuaouu mo mucmfiowwmmoo aofiumamuuooumucH "ale manna 49 the highest correlation obtained was .23 between trunk breadth and APHV. The correlations of the five growth measures with APHV and MA were slightly higher than those of the indices, with the highest coefficient being -.37 between biacromial width and MA. Furthermore, all correla- tions among the individual growth measures, as well as some among the indices of body shapes, were moderate to high. The correlation between APHV and MA was .68. This result, coupled with the fact that the coef- ficients of correlations for APHV and MA were similar for all variables, indicate that predictive equations for these two dependent variables probably are similar. Stepwise multiple regression techniques were used with standing height, sitting height, biacromial width, biiliac width, and weight as the predictor variables of APHV and MA (Table 4-3). The stepwise procedures for predicting APHV showed that weight did not contribute significantly to the regression equation. Biacromial width, followed by biiliac diameter, contributed most to the prediction (pI< .01). The r2 statistic reflects that 13.9% of the variance in APHV can be accounted for by these two measures. However, according to the ad- justed r2 (adjusted for changing degrees of freedom with each step), only 11.2% of the variance can be explained by measures of shoulder and hip widths taken at age nine years. Note that even though standing and sitting heights contributed significantly to the regression (p < .05), the multiple r and r2 increased only minutely, and the adjusted r2 ac- tually decreased. In addition, the inclusion of the two height mea- sures slightly increased the standard error of the estimate (SEE). It seems, then, that of the five growth parameters measured at nine years 50 Gmemmpwmu cw amum Hmafim now n xlc Ho . v 9; wmumefiumm muamfiowmmmoo mo “means u x mumsz .A NmIH V A MHW.V Immumm wmumsnwmm mo.v.n« HNH.wH ucmumcou Nmm. «Noo.m oHH.H HHH. Hwa. was. snows umaaaaa ANN. I Isw~.m woa.a was. owH. ems. unmade oam. «seas.s ooH.H wma. aka. mus. oawaua waauoam mam. «eaoo.o Nao.fl oma. ska. HNS. banana waaoaaum «Hm. I Iaomo.oa aHH.H ems. ems. cam. snows Huaaouomam <2 wcfiuowpmpm s~s.ma uamumaoo mos. Imm.m «mm. awe. sea. owm. unmade waauuam omm. I aam.m has. wmo. osa. sum. names; waaoamum mam. Iamo.m mos. NHH. ans. mam. guess omaaaaa man. I Iaom.a was. oac. oaa. Nam. zoos; Hassonouam >mo<.waauoaoonm ucoaoaocooo cause a. mam m emnmsmoa Im. a ofiaaoasz uabmanmw :onmmmmmm mm N n Ame n av <2 mam >mm< :o mmflpmfium> HOuUfivmum mo cofimwmuwmm mHaHuHDZ mmfi3amum ”Mic manna 51 of age, the biacromial and biiliac widths provided the best predictors of APHV, accounting for only 11.2% of the variance. Table 4-4 provides the regression coefficients, the constant, and the standard error of the coefficients (SEC) based on the inclusion of only biacromial and biiliac measures. Table 4-4: Regression Coefficients of Predictor Variables Selected on the Basis of the Adjusted R Statistic Variable Resgression Coefficient SEE Predicting APHV Biacromial width - .389 .127 Biiliac width .216 .149 Constant 18.822 2.78 Predicting MA Biacromial width - .510 .144 Standing height .624 .361 Constant 19.837 3.62 The prediction of MA from the five growth parameters showed that a higher percentage of the variance was explained than in the prediction of APHV, but that a larger SEE existed (Table 4-3). All five growth variables contributed significantly to the stepwise regression on MA. However, analysis based on the adjusted r2 indicated that biacromial width and standing height, with a multiple r of .421, explained the highest amount of variance (15.0%) with the lowest SEE (1.092) (Tables 4-3 and 4-4). These two regression analyses indicated that prediction of APHV and MA from growth variables during childhood were too low and individual variation too high to be practical. Higher multiple r's and percent variances might be found if skinfolds, circumferences, or 52 measures of secondary sex characteristics had been included as predictor variables. Another regression analysis was run forcing each of the five growth parameters into the prediction of APHV and MA first. After the initial variable was forced into the equation, the program was allowed to enter any of the other growth measures. Results from these regression proce- dures are shown in Appendix C. Differences in the Adolescent Growth Spurt among Early, Average, and Late Maturing Females Five aspects of the adolescent growth spurt were compared in early (EM), average (AM), and late (LM) maturing females to determine (1) if differences in adolescent growth existed among the three groups, and (2) which variables contributed most to the variance. These comparisons by level of maturity enabled the last two research hypotheses to be tested. It was hypothesized that the magnitude and duration of the adolescent growth spurt and the interval of time between APHV and MA would be greatest in the EM females, followed by that of the AM and then LM subjects. Early maturers (n=21) had PHV before 11.5 years of age, AM (n=28) reached PHV between 11.5 and 12.5 years inclusive, and LM (n=l6) attained their PHV after age 12.5 years. The intercorrelation matrix of the five dependent variables is shown in Table 4-5. Interval has low correlations with the other four parameters of the growth spurt, while the relationships among the other four parameters range from low to high (.12 - .90). Spurt height (SH), spurt duration (SD), spurt intensity (SI), peak height velocity (PHV), and the time between peak velocity and menarche (interval) were used as the dependent variables in a one-way 53 Table 4-5: Intercorrelation Matrix for the Five Dependent Variables of the Adolescent Growth Cycle (n - 65) Spurt duration .65 Spurt intensity .90 .87 Peak height velocity .68 .12 .43 Interval .06 .05 .07 -.01 Spurt Spurt Spurt Peak height height duration intensity velocity multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA), with the three levels of physical maturity (EM, AM, LM) as the independent variable. The re- sults revealed that the maturity main effect was significant, F (10,116) a 4.90 (p‘(.0001). Univariate and stepdown F tests, as well as discriminant function analyses, were used to determine the relative contributions of the dependent variables to the significant MANOVA main effect (Table 4-6). These three analyses indicated that SH, SD, Table 4-6: Maturity Main Effect Standardized discrimi- Dependent variable Univariate F Stepdown F nant Function coeff. Spurt height 1.55 1.55 1.48 Spurt duration 4.70** 3.93 -l.27 Spurt intensity 2.69 1.34 .03 Peak height velocity 4. 56** 9 . 78*** -1 . 32 Interval 4.21* 3.19* — .45 * p4(.05 ** p< .01 W: p< .001 54 PHV, and Interval had important roles in differentiating the three ma- turity groups. The two F analyses showed significant contributions of SD, PHV, and Interval to the overall effect, while the standardized co- efficients from the discriminant analyses weighted SH (1.48) highest, followed by PHV (-1.32) and SD (-l.27). The discrepancy in weightings of the variables between the step- down and discriminant procedures probably resulted from these tests using slightly different techniques. In the stepdown analysis, each step takes into account the contribution of the preceeding variable before assigning an F value; whereas, the purpose of discriminant analy- sis is to find the best linear combination of the original variables such that maximum differences among groups will emerge. The values in Table 4-6 show that SH was more important when considered in linear combination with the other dependent variables than when used in a pre- determined stepwise procedures. Likewise, interval is a significant variable in the stepdown techniques, but ranks fourth in the best linear combination as determined by discriminant analysis. The fact that 81 contributes little to the significant MANOVA is not surprising. In this study SI was computed by multiplying SH and SD. Therefore, one would expect that after SH and SD have been entered into the analyses, insignificant benefit would be gained by including SI. Nevertheless, four aspects of the adolescent growth spurt (SH, SD, PHV, Interval) remained important in differentiating the three ma- turity groups. SD and PHV were weighted highly on all three analyses, while Interval was significant on the F tests and SH was ranked on the discriminant analyses. Although PHV includes SH, both variables were 55 weighted heavily in the discriminant analysis, indicating that even though girls have various height velocities during childhood and at the onset of the spurt, SH and PHV still contribute significantly to differences among EM, AM, and LM. Examination of the vector means and standard deviations of each dependent variable revealed trends among the levels of maturity on each aspect of the adolescent growth cycle (Table 4-7). In general, the EM females had the highest mean on each variable, followed by AM and then Table 4-7: Vector Means and Standard Deviations of the Dependent Variables by Maturity Level (n = 65) Dependent Variables Spurt Spurt Spurt Peak height Maturity height duration intensity velocity Interval level (cm) (vr) 3cm”) (cm) (yr) 53:11,: ‘i 3.4 2.58 9.51 8.8 1.77 “ e 5 SD 1.4 .79 5.83 1.0 .76 (n = 21) 3:32; i 2.8 2.26 6.90 8.1 1.17 (n 8 28) SD 1.1 .80 4.26 .9 .95 :Efirers I 2.7 1.79 5.79 7.8 1.02 SD 1 8 .75 5 55 1.3 .80 (n . 16) Sample —- 0 2 2 8 Total X 3. . 5 7.47 .2 1.33 SD 1.4 .83 5.26 1.1 .90 (n = 65) LM. That is, EM had a higher SH, SI, and PHV and a longer SD and In- terval than did the AM and LM subjects (Figure 4-1). Scheffé post hoc procedures were conducted on the values for SD, PHV, and Interval to determine if significant differences existed among the maturity groups on these variables (Table 4-8). On all three 56 «I .4 14.? L. F . . . t. o 8 o. s. 3. 2 2 I 20% I 5:950 .53 «1 $1 JLL: A} P h — — l T- s o 8 o. a 1 2 2 so I 22.: :25 Qual— coat->4 28m Gaul— 009.25 atom Maturity Laval Maturity Laval 2.0 - 9.0 7 IO- HM ‘45} _ .- 384203 n. .10 to.» I .0325 Hm ‘31 flsl — F b s o. 8 8 a 7 so u 3.8.; 22.: so... _ p p p 9 a 7 6 «Eu I 2.2.2:. taam .«OI— 009.34 atom O“ O..— .0223 atom 0.0..- 832.4 3.6m Maturity Laval Maturity Laval Maturity Laval Mean Dependent Variable Scores for the Maturity Main Effect. Figure 4-1: 57 Table 4-8: Results of the Scheffé Post Hoc Tests for the Dependent Variables that Were Affected Significantly by Maturity Level Maturity Level Dependent Variable Early Average ‘ngg Spurt duration 2.58 2.26 1.79 Peak height velocity 8.8 8.1 7.8 Interval 1.77 1.17 1.02 parameters, EM differed significantly from LM. The EM displayed a sig- nificantly higher PHV and longer SD and Interval than the LM. More- over, the EM females also differed significantly from the AM subjects on PHV and Interval. No statistically significant differences between AM and LM emerged on any of the three variables, though. the AM fe- males had higher means than the LM individuals throughout the dependent variables studied. Discussion The data analyses on predictability of APHV and MA generally sup- port the findings published by other investigators. The intercorrela- tion coefficients of APHV and MA with the four body build indices were low, ranging from -.15 to .23. These results agree with those of Dem- ing (1957) and Jacobsen (1954), who found no significant correlations among the growth parameters and no significant differences among ma- turity groups in scores on the indices. Moreover, an inverse and weak relationship existed between standing height and APHV (-.22) and MA (-.10), agreeing with the data of Reynolds and Wines (1948) and also Deming (1957). 58 The predictive equations of APHV and MA from this study reveal lower standard errors of the estimate than do those from previous in- vesigations (Marshall and Tanner, 1969; Zacharias, 23 al., 1970). These scientists published regressions for MA with an SEE of approxi- mately 1.9 years. The highest SEE from the present data is 1.09 years (predicting MA), while the lowest is .97 years (predicting APHV). Marshall (1974) presented predictions of MA with an SEE of .66, while Frisancho, 25 a1. (1969) indicated an SEE of .14. These regressions included a skeletal age in the analyses. It seems, then, that good predictions of APHV and MA, without the inclusion of SA, or a close estimate of this variable, are too difficult to obtain from data taken during childhood. The estimates would be too highly variable to aid in individual counseling. It appears necessary to determine growth characteristics that could reasonably replace SA in predictive analy- ses. In the present endeavor, the interval of time between APHV and MA differed significantly between the early maturing females and those in the two other maturity groups (EM - 1.77 yr, AM a 1.17 yr, LM = 1.02 yr” i - 1.33). These results agree closely with those of Lindgren (1978), who found significant differences among the three levels of maturity (EM = 1.6 yr, AM - 1.2 yr, LM = .7 yr, i = 1.2 yr), and sup- port the data of Boas (1932) and Onat and Ertem (1974) who reported differences in the interval from APHV to MA by maturity group. Contra- dictory data have been published earlier by Simmons and Greulich (1943) and Shuttleworth (1937). It would seem that the later developing fe- male is nearer her complete biological maturation at the time of PHV 59 than the early maturer and, therefore, would have a shorter interval until the onset of menstruation. Data from this investigation on the relationship of APHV to MA lend insight to the understanding of the role of spurt duration in the adolescent growth cycle. Israelsohn (1960) and Marshall and Tanner (1969) determined that menarche occurs at the point of maximum decel- eration of growth in height after the PHV. Therefore, if EM have a longer interval to maximum deceleration, then it could be expected that their SD would be longer. The findings of the present study confirm this observation. Spurt duration contributed significantly to the MANOVA main effect by level of maturation and was statistically different between EM (2.58 yr) and LM (1.79 yr) subjects. These data support those of Marubini, gt 31. (1971) and Onat and Ertem (1974) and contradict the findings of Boas (1932) and Frish and Revelle (1971b), who indicated significant differences in the opposite direction. No significant differences in SD by maturity level were reported by others (Marshall and Tanner, 1969, 1970; Reynolds and Wines, 1948). Some controversy has existed concerning magnitude of the growth spurt in relationship to time of maximum rate of growth. The greatest amount of evidence shows that EM females have a significantly more in- tense spurt than the AM or LM, while other studies have depicted either no differences or opposite trends. The present data do not fully clarify these discrepancies concerning spurt height. According to the discriminant analyses, SH is weighted the highest in contributing to the maturity main effect. The univariate and stepdown F's were not significant, though, so post hoc analyses could not be run to determine if the three maturity levels differed significantly. The EM showed the 60 highest mean (3.4 cm), while the AM (2.8 cm) and LM (2.7 cm) had simi- lar means for SH. Apparently, SH contributes to the MANOVA significant effect, but its true contribution might be compounded within that of PHV. The values of PHV were weighted highly on all three analyses (uni- variate F, stepdown F, discriminant analysis) as significant contribu- tors to the maturity main effect. In addition, EM (8.8 cm) had signi- ficantly higher PHV's than either AM (8.1 cm) or LM (7.8 cm). The fact that EM grow faster during childhood than AM andIJIand, therefore, enter the growth spurt at a faster velocity probably helps account for the differences in PHV. It seems, though, that the SH also has an important role in differentiating the three levels of maturity. CHAPTER V SUMMARY, CONCLUSIONS, AND RECOMMENDATIONS Summary The purposes of this study were to (1) determine if childhood measures of height, weight, body size, and/or body shape could be used to predict age at peak height velocity (APHV) and the onset of men- struation (MA) in females; (2) investigate differences in the adoles- cent growth spurt for early, average, and late maturing females; and (3) study the relationship between age of menarche and age at peak height velocity among the three maturity groups. The following statements summarize the findings: 1. Low relationships existed among all the physical growth variables and the ages at PHV and menarche. The highest coefficient obtained was -.37 between biacromial width and MA. All correlations among the individual growth measures ranked moderate to high (.55 - .84). The indices of body size did not vary greatly through- out the three levels of maturation. None of the ratios made a significant contribution to the prediction of APHV and MA and, consequently, no predictive equations were established that included these indices. Of the five growth parameters measured at age nine years, the biacromial and biiliac widths provided the best 61 62 predictors of APHV, accounting for 11.2% of the variance. The regression equation was: APHV = 18.822 - .389 (Biacromial) + .216 (Biiliac) 11.969 The prediction of MA from the growth parameters showed a higher percentage of the variance (15.0%) was explained than in the prediction of APHV, but that a larger standard error of the estimate (SEE) existed. The best regres- sion equation was: MA = 19.837 - .510 (Biacromial) + .624 (Standing height) : 1.092 6. A one-way multivariate analysis of variance, with the three levels of physical maturity (early, average, late) as the independent variable, revealed that the maturity main effect was significant (p C .0001). Univariate F procedures, stepdown F tests, and discrimi- nant function analyses indicated that spurt height, spurt duration, peak height velocity, and interval had important roles in differentiating the three maturity groups. On all five aspects of the adolescent growth spurt, early maturers displayed higher mean values than average devel- opers who showed larger means than late maturers. Significant differences occurred between early and late maturers for spurt duration, peak height velocity, and interval. Moreover, the early developing females also differed statistically from the average maturers on PHV and interval. 63 Conclusions The results suggest the following conclusions: 1. Accurate predictions of APHV and MA, without the inclu- sion of skeletal age, may be too difficult to obtain from growth data taken at nine years of age. The two regres- sion analyses indicated that prediction of APHV and MA from growth variables during childhood were too low and individual variation too high to be of practical value. Predictive equations for APHV and MA were similar since the correlations between each of these variables with the other growth measures were similar. In addition, the correlation between APHV and MA was .68, which indicates that a prediction of MA that would include APHV would probably be more accurate than that relying only on data at age nine years. Although other studies have shown no differences in spurt duration (SD) by maturity level, these results indicated that SD contributed significantly to such differences, at least between early and late maturing females. Early developers had an adolescent growth cycle of approxi- mately 2.58 years, while that of the late maturers was 1.79 years. The interval of time between peak velocity and menarche decreased as level of maturity moved from early to late. These data, together with those of SD, indicated that early maturers entered their adolescent growth cycle at a younger age but grew for a longer time before 64 maturity than average or late developers. It would seem that the later developing female is nearer her complete biological maturation at the time of PHV than the early maturer and, therefore, would have a shorter interval until onset of menstruation. Recommendations Based on this investigation, the following recommendations are tendered: 1. In order to decrease the SEE in the predictive analyses for APHV and MA, it appears necessary to determine growth characteristics that could reasonably replace skeletal age. It could be beneficial to parents, teachers, physicians, and researchers if regression techniques for MA were utilized that included APHV. These data, in conjunction with that of SD, could be used during the adolescent growth spurt to estimate the time of complete maturity for a female or the amount of growth that could be ex— pected after PHV. The study should be duplicated with a larger sample in order to alleviate the possible biasing effect of delet- ing subjects vflx>showed an incomplete growth spurt. Some early maturers already were into their spurt by age 9 years, while some later maturers had not yet reached maturity at the completion of the study. 65 4. A parallel study for males should be conducted to see if the same aspects of the adolescent spurt cause dif- ferences among the maturity levels. 5. The variables of spurt intensity and body shape indices probably could be deleted in future investigations. 6. Future research in this area should incorporate various growth measures not only during childhood, but also at PHV, MA, and adulthood. APPENDICES APPENDIX A Letter of Request for Menarche Data Form for Recording Menarche Data MICHIGAN STATE UNIVERSITY DEPARTMENT OF I'll-.ALTH PHYSICAL EDL CATION AND RECREATION EAST LANSING ' MICHIGAN ' 48824 MOTOR PERFORMANCE STL'DY ' WOMEN‘S INTRAML'RAL BUILDING The Motor Performance Study is currently completing its llth year of operation. We are in the initial processes of the longitudinal data analyses and are review- ing all records of our subjects. Your daughter's record is excellent and will help us in our attempt to answer many questions in the areas of physical growth and motor performance. A xerox copy of her data record is enclosed with this letter. As doctoral students specializing in growth and development we are interested es- pecially in the development of females. Our study deals with the relationship of the onset of menstruation in girls to their physical growth and motor performance. We need your help in this project. Would you please record the age and/or the date of your daughter's first menstrual period on the form enclosed with this letter? Try to be as specific as possible, for example, "12 years 9 months; June, 1972." If actual dates cannot be recalled, please list a close approximation in terms of grade in school such as, "first day of 8th grade; late summer between the 6th and 7th grades; etc." If menstruation has not begun, please indicate this under 92m- ments. A second objective of this study is to determine the predictability of the onset of maturity indicators in children from those of their parents; there- fore, it would be most beneficial to have your age at first menstruation. Again, please attempt to be as specific as possible in providing the information such as, "14 years, 9 months." After completing the enclosed form, please return it promptly in the self—addressed, stamped envelope. If you have any questions, please contact either Crystal Fountain or Molly Sapp at 353-9467. We appreciate yOur time and thoughtfulness for participating in this project and for your years of diligence to the Motor Performance Study. As always, the data will be maintained in strict confidence. Thank you. Sincerely, Crystal Fountain Molly Sapp With the knowledge and approval of the faculty coordinators, Vern Seefeldt John Haubenstricker 66 67 MOTOR PERFORMANCE STUDY Form for Menarche Data Daughter's Name: I.D. Number Birthdate: Date of First Menstruation: Age at First Menstruation: Comments (if any): Mother's Age at First Menstruation: Today's Date Signature of Person Completing Form APPENDIX B Form for Recording Growth Data SEX GRADE CHRONOL. AGE (MO.) SKELETAL AGE (MO.) BIACROMIAL STANDING HEIGHT SITTING HEIGHT DIAMETER BIILIAC DIAMETER WEIGHT DATE 0-21 22-27 28-33 34-39 40-45 46-51 52-57 58-63 70-75 76-81 82-87 88-93 94-99 100-105 106-111 112-117 118-123 124-129 130-135 136-141 142-147 148-153 154-159 160-165 166-171 172-177 178-183 NAME: NUMBER: BIRTHDATE: 68 APPENDIX C DATA FROM REGRESSION PROCEDURES THAT FORCED EACH GROWTH VARIABLE INTO THE PREDICTIVE EQUATION FIRST Ho. a «a no. a 2 69 kaao amum umnu sew ucmfiowwmmoo m£u mo uoupm cumpcmumn zaco noun umzu Mama «mm. oao. AH. unwamz mom. has. «N. “swam; waauufim okH. mew. ks.H guess umaaaam NS . mmm ... .33 .0 new“; Ease—03m owe. mo~.- om. mac. «as. own. “swam; wcaecmum mlmmmw. was. mas. mm. “swam: mcauuam mma. mam. Na.H nus“; umfifiaam was. amm.u «amm.o sues; Hmantumam seq. om~.u mm. kwo. sea. 0mm. “swam; maavamum mxmmww «ma. mmm. HH.N nupfia omfiafiwm Baa. Nnm.I «xmq.o nupw3 Hmfiaouomfim omm. km~.- on. mac. ass. «km. “swam; wcaecmum mummmm. omH. o-.u «Hm.q Lucwa Hmweouomam mNm. sMN. mm. Hmo. oHH. Nmm. “swam; wcacamum mummmm. Avoouomv «8N. kmq.- o~.m mmo. use. oNN. “swam; mcaeamum wimwww mac mfimwwmc< mmmm. uamwofiwmmoo mMMMMIM mm cmumafiv< mm. m mHmHuHsz >mm<.wd«u0fivuum coammmpwmm w .umtsa aofiumsca w>fiuofivmua msu saga mHanum> zusoum comm pauu0m umsu mwuapaooua :oammmuwwu Baum mama no navaona< 70 NNH. mwm.l mqa. can. NHH. mmo.l «mm. coo. owq. m©~.I Qua. com. NmH. mwm.l mqa. soc. cue. omN.I mma. mam. qu. @nm.l cam. mac. «ma. mom. an. Noe.l 5mm. oqa. mma. «Om.l 0mm. «mm. mom. mm©.l ««mm.m 0H.~ No. NH. om. sq.H «aom.o «N. am. N¢.H «amm.o mm. qw.a «koo.w mo. «xqa.o mm. m©.H NHH. oqa. coo.l oHo. Nmo. qu. mwo. cad. woo. qu. mwc. «Ha. HHo. 0N0. mum. OOH. owm. own. «mm. mmm. Ned. nuuas Hmaeouomam sues: umaaasm Avmuuomv cubes omfiafiwm unwams “swam; mcaecmum guess umaHaam sauna Hmaaotumfim “swam; waauuwm newfim; wcficcmum sagas omAHHHm guess Hmfisouomfim “swam; wcfiuuam cucfis umfifiaam guess Hmfieouumfim “swam: wcfiuuam guess amasouomam unwfimn wafiuuam Ammouomv unmam: wafiuuam N mmum H mwum amuse mfimhwma< m moum a mmum m mmum N mmum H mmum 039 mammama< pmscfiucoo .0 xfipcmaa< 71 00H. HmH. mom. 50H. mma. mmq. mNH. wmq. own. «mm. mq¢. owq. NmH. Qua. cam. qua. mqa. mmH. 0mm. nqa. «ma. mow. owm.I «NH. com. nmm.l Nmn. Hmm.l mmm. maq.l omo. seq. mo~.I mwm.l com. mmq. cm~.| mum.l mam. mmn.| Nmm.1 mum. ac.H «som.w o0. mq.H *«qH.w «N. «*No.c mm. mm.a NH. «N. om. aaom.o n¢.a mm. mm. «smm.o N¢.H om. kkmq.o HH.~ «mo. oqa. nmo. qu. omo. wHH. ooo. «NC. ~50. «QH. mmo. «ca. mmo. 06H. mum. «mm. «on. mmH. own. own. cum. suvw3 omwafifim sumaa HmHEonomwm unwamz nupfis omwawwm nuvfiz Hmfifiouomwm unwams suww3 HmHEouomfim usmfims Apmouomv unwfiwz unwamz uswam: waauuam “swam: mcaeamum guess Haasouumfim new“; umaaaam unwam; wcauuam unwamn mcaeamum spews Haasotomam guess omfiaafim uswaw: wcwpamum songs amasouumam nuefis umaafiam s mmum m mmum N mmum H mmum usom mfimhamc< m mmum s mmum n mmum pmscwucoa .o xfipcmmm< 72 80H. NBA. New. omm. ooH. ooH. kmm. NoH. “as. own. «as. Hem. mwN. mam. ow¢. ONH. NmH. «Nm. «mm. can. sam.- aam. meH.- mom. was.. «mm. mNm. on.- Non. cam.» «No. mQN.I new. moN.1 oON. mwm.1 coo. moH.I «H. xxom.w Nm. HHH. HwH. Hw. «H. *«wo.¢ cm. mNH. mmH. 0H. 0tammNH co. an. mNH. «anq.NH om.N omH. 55H. ma. soo.- ado. «N. om. ks.H ascm.c NH. Nae. «as. mm. ucwame maauuam HoumEmHv HmHEOHUmHm mms. “swam: maaeamum ustw3 unwfimn waHuuHm umuoEmHv HmHEouomHm ems. “swam; maaeamum unwam; maauufim umumEme HmHEouomHm Nms. “swam; waaeamum pmuoEme HmHEouumHm Hms. unwam; magnamum Apmouomv Boa. “swam; mafincmum “swam; wcauunm “swam: waaecmum sue“: omaaaam :uvHB HmHEouomHm owm. ustmB “swam: waaecmum m mmum q mmum m mmum N mmum H Noam moo mehHmc< <2 wcHHUvaum m mmum wmscHucoo .0 menmaa< 73 NNH. NMH. NaH. Ham. H«m. NNH. 00H. o«m. 5mm. 00H. 00H. mNm. m«H. NOH. NMH. 00m. own. NaH. Ham. «mm. NNN.I Nmm. NNN.I mHm. «Hm.l 0mm mcH.I NNm. wm«.l wmm. Nom. ch.I mNm. oo«.l MOH. wn«.l Nmm. NNN.I 0N. MHH. H«H. Hw.N NNO. m«o. um. mH. Nw. «aom.w «H. HHH. HwH. Hm. «m. sawe.m «H. mNH. omH. om. ««mm.NH oH. wMH. wNH. ««wm.HH mH.N mMH. ocH. we. moo.n oHc. an. mH. mum. RON. mN«. «N«. NN«. wo«. NOH. SuwHB UMHHHHm Homouomv guess umfiaaam spews umaafifim unmfimz ucmez wachmuw can“; Hassouumam unwfiwn wcfiuuHm uanmz ucmez wchamum sagas Hmfiaouomfim unwam; waauuam unwamn waaecmum cues: HmHEouomam “swam; wafiuuam guess Hmfieopumfim unmsmn wcauuam meopomv “swam; wafiuuam tmumEmfic umafiaam unwfimz N mwum H mmum amuse mHmNmma< m mmum s mmum m mwum N mmum H Noam 039 mHmmqu< wmscHucou .0 vaamaa< 74 00H. wm«.l ««wo.m o«m. moH.I Hm. mNH. mam. mmm. wn.N mmH. mm«.l «««n.o HNm. oHH.| o«. NMH. o«H. mwm.l ame.e ow«. «NN. HN. NHH. mum. H«o.1 Nm.N mNo. 00H. com. «H. Hmm. NNN.1 mH. N«m. mHm. No. NNH. «Hm.l «aom.w NmH. Nmm. Nu. HHH. mum. owH.I mm. mam. «we. oh.N ONH. NHm.I ««No.m omH. cmm. mm. «NH. Hum. HHo. HN.N coH. mmm.1 ««w¢.m mmH. maN. oN. NMH. o«H. oom.l *«wo.n owH. «N«. wNH. NN«. o«H. «mm. ««o. OHN. HwH. mN«. ¢NH. MN«. mmH. HN«. caves HmHEouumHm unwfimz unmam; wafieamum sue“; Hassouumam “swam: Loews amazotumam uzwfimz Ammouomv ucme3 “swam; wcauuam “swam: unwam; waaeamum sue“: Haasotomam cue“; umHHHHm unwfims ustm: waHvamum guess HmHEopumfim spews omaflaam “swam; weavamum snags Hmfisotomfim guess omfiaaam nova: Hmfisotumfim s mmum m mmum m:m0wm H mmum noon mHthmc¢ m mmum s mmum m mmum vmscHuaoo .o vacmma< 75 NmH. ooH. N«m. NNH. Ham. 00H. 5mm. Nmm. mom. mHm. «Hm.l NNN.I mom. NNm. mm. «H. No. ««c¢.w mH. HHH. HmH. «H. «m. mN«. sue“: umaaaam “swam: wafiuuam “swam; maacamum guess Hmfieouumfim “swam; “swam; wcauusm usta: waHvamum m mmum cmsaHucoo .0 wacmaa< BIBLIOGRAPHY BIBLIOGRAPHY Anderson, M., Hwang, S. C., & Green, W. T. Growth of the normal trunk in boys and girls during the second decade of life related to age, maturity and ossification of the iliac epiphyses. Journal of Bone and Joint Surgery, 1965, 14A, 1554-1564. Barker, R. G., & Stone, L. P. Physical development in relation to menarcheal age in university women. Human Biology, 1936, 8, 198- 222. Bayley, N. Size and body build of adolescents in relation to rate of skeletal maturing. Child Development, 1943, 14, 47-89. Bergsten-Brucefors, A. A note on the accuracy of recalled age at menarche. Annals of Human Biolggy, 1976,_§, 71-73. Billewicz, W. Z., Fellowes, H. M., & Hythen, C. A. Comments on the critical metabolic mass and the age of menarche. Annals of Human Biology, 1976, 3, 51-59. Boas, F. Observations on the growth of children. Science, 1930,_12, 44-48. Boas, F. Studies in growth. Human Biology, 1932, 4, 307-350. Bock, R. D., Wainer, H., Petersen, A., Wissen, D., Murray, J. & Roche, A. A parameterization for individual human growth curves. Human Bodzsar, E. B. Recent data to the physical development of adolescent girls. Growth and Development; Physique Symposia Biologica Hun- garica, 1977, 29, 177-189. Bojlén, K., Rasch, G. & Weis-Bertzon, M. The age incidence of the menarche in Copenhagen. Acta Obstet et Gynec. Scandinavia, 1954, .33, 405-433. Borsos, A., Takacs, I., & Smid, I. Endocrine and somatic background of the perimenarche. Growth and Development; Physique Symposia Biologica Hungarica, 1977, 29, 195-201. Cameron, N. Weight and skinfold variation at menarche and the criti- cal body weight hypothesis. Annals of Human Biology, 1976, 2, 76 77 Damon, A., & Bejema, C. J. Age at menarche: accuracy of recall after thirty-nine years. Human Biology, 1974, 44, 381-384. Damon, A., Damon, S. T., Reed, R. B., & Valadian, I. Age at menarche of mothers and daughters, with a note on accuracy of recall. Human Biology, 1969, 41, 161-175. Deming, J. Application of the: Gompertz curve to the observed pattern of growth in length of 48 individual boys and girls during the adolescent cycle of growth. Human Biology, 1957, 12, 83-122. Dreizen, S., Sperakis, C. N., & Stone, R. E. A comparison of skeletal growth and maturation in undernourished and well-nourished girls before and after menarche. The Journal of Pediatrics, 1967, 19, 256-263. Frisancho, A. R., Garn, S. M., & Rohmann, C. G. Age at menarche: a new method of prediction and retrospective assessment based on hand x-rays. Human Biology, 1969, 41, 42-50. Frisch, R. E., & Revelle, R. Height and weight at menarche and a hypothesis of critical body weight and adolescent events. Science, 1970, 169, 397-398. Frisch, R. E., & Revelle, R. Height and weight at menarche and a hypothesis of menarche. Archives of Disease in Childhood, 1971b, 44, 695-701. Frisch, R. E. & Revelle, R. The height and weight of girls and boys at the time of initiation of the adolescent growth spurt in height and weight and the relationship to menarche. Human Biolo- gy, 1971a,.41, 140-159. Hewitt, D. & Acheson, R. M. Some aspects of skeletal development through adolescence. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 1961, 12, 333-344. Hillman, R. W., Slater, P., & Nelson, M. J. Season of birth, parental age, menarcheal age and body form: some interrelationships in young women. Human Biology, 1970, 41, 570-580. Israelsohn, W. J. Description and modes of analysis of human growth. Human growth. Ed., J. M. Tanner. New York: Pergamon Press, 1960, pp. 21-42. Jacobson, L. On the relationship between menarcheal age and adult body structure. Human Biology, 1954, 16, 127-132. Johnston, F. E., Roche, A. F., Schell, L. M., & Wettenhall, M. D. Critical weight at menarche: critique of a hypothesis. American Journal of Diseases in Children, 1975, 129, 19-23. 78 Johnston, F. E., Malina, R. M., & Galbraith, M. A. Height, weight, and age at menarche and the "critical weight" hypothesis. Science, 1971, 174, 1148-1149. Kantero, R., & Widholm, 0. A statistical analysis of the menstrual patterns of 8,000 Finish girls and their mothers. Acta Obstit and Gynec. Scandanavia, Supplement, 1971, 14, 1-36. Kralj-Cercek, L. The influence of food, body build and social origin in the age at menarche. Human Biology, 1956, 18, 393-406. Krogman, W. The physical growth of children: an appraisal of studies, 1950-1955. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Devel- opment, 1955, 19 (26). Largo, R. H., Gasser, T., Prader, A., Stuetzle, W., & Huber, P. J. Analysis of the adolescent growth spurt using smoothing spline functions. Annals of Human Biology, 1978, 2, 421-434. Lindgren, G. Growth of schoolchildren with early, average, and late ages of peak height velocity. Annals of Human Biology, 1978,_§ (3), 253-267. Lindgren, G. Height, weight, and menarche in Swedish urban school— children in relation to socio-economic and regional factors. Annals of Human Biology, 1976,_1, 501-528. Livson, N., & McNeill, D. The accuracy of recalled age of menarche. Human Biology, l962,_14, 218-221. Ljung, B. 0., Bergsten-Brucefors, A., & Lindgren, G. The secular trend in physical growth in Sweden. Annals of Human Biology, 1974, 1, 245-246. Malina, R. M. Growth and development. Minneapolis: Burgess Publish- ing Company, 1975. Marshall, W. A. Interrelationships of skeletal maturation, sexual development and somatic growth in man. Annals of Human Biology, 1974, 1, 29-40. Marshall, W. A., & deLimonge, Y. Skeletal maturity and the prediction of age at menarche. Annals of Human Biology, 1976, 1, 235-243. Marshall, W. A., & Tanner, J. M. Variations in the pattern of puber- tal changes in boys. Archives of Disease in Childhood, 1970, 42, 13-23. Marshall, W. A. & Tanner, J. M. Variations in patterns of pubertal changes in girls. Archives of Disease in Childhood, 1969, 44, 79 Marubini, E.,IResele,l” F3, & Barghini, G. A comparative fitting of the Gompertz and logistic functions to longitudinal height data during adolescence in girls. Human Biology, 1971, 41, 237-252. Marubini, E., Risele, L. F., Tanner, J. M., & Whitehouse, R. H. The fit of Gompertz and logistic curves to longitudinal data during adolescence on height, sitting height, and biacromial diameter in boys and girls of the Harpenden Growth Study. Human Biology, 1972, 44, 511-523. McNeill, D., & Livson, N. Maturation rate and body build in women. Child Development, 1963, 14, 25—32. Meredith, H. V. The rhythm of physical growth. University of Iowa Studies in Child Welfare, 1935, 11 (3). Neyzi, 0., & Alp, H. Relationships between body build and age of menarche in a group of girls of heterogeneous socioeconomic back- ground. Nutrition Reports International, 1975, 11, 27-34. Nicolson, A. B., & 'Hanlex C. Indices of physiological maturity: derivation and interrelationships. Child Development, 1953, 14, 3-38. Onat, T., & Ertem, B. Adolescent female height velocity: relation- ship to body measurements of sexual and skeletal maturity. H2- man Biology, 1974, 44, 199-217. Poppleton, P. K., & Brown, P. E. The secular trend in puberty: has stability been achieved? British Journal of Educational Psychol- ogy, 1966, 14, 95-100. Prader, A., Tanner, J. M., & vonHarnack, G. A. Catch-up growth fol- lowing illness or starvation. The Journal of Pediatrics, 1963, 62, 646-659. Reynolds, E. R., & Wines, J. V. Individual differences in physical changes associated with adolescence in girls. American Journal of Diseases of Children, 1948, 15, 329-350. Richardson, B. D., & Pietus,L. Menarche and growth. The American Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 1977, 19, 2088-2091. Richey, H. G. The relation of accelerated, normal and retarded puber- ty to the height and weight of school children. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 1937, 1_(l), 1-67. Scammon, R. E. The first seriation study of human growth. American Journal of Physical AnthrOpology, 1927,_1Q, 329-336. Shuttleworth, F. K. The adolescent period: a graphic atlas. Mono- ‘graphs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 1949a, 1§'(1). 80 Shuttleworth, F. K. The adolescent period: a pictorial atlas. Mano- graphs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 1949b, 12(2)- Shuttleworth, F. K. The physical and mental growth of girls and boys age six to nineteen in relation to age at maximum growth. Mono- _graphs of the Societygof Research in Child Development, l939,_4 (3). Shuttleworth, F. K. Sexual maturation and the physical growth of girls age six to nineteen. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 1937, 1 (5). Simmons, K., & Greulich, W. W. Menarcheal age and the height, weight and skeletal age of girls age seven to seventeen years. Journal of Pediatrics, 1943, 11, 518-548. Simmons, K., & Todd, T. W. Growth of well children: analysis of stature and weight, three months to thirteen years. Growth, 1938, 1, 93-134. Tanner, J. M. The assessment of growth and development in children. Archives of Disease in Childhood, 1952, 11, 10-33. Tanner, J. M. Growth at adolescence, 2nd edition. Oxford: Blackwell Scientific Publications, 1962. Tanner, J. M. Trend towards earlier menarche in London, Oslo, Copen- hagen, the Netherlands, and Hungary. Nature, 1973, 243, 95-96. Tanner, J. M., & Whitehouse, R. H. Clinical longitudinal standards for height, weight, height velocity, weight velocity, and the stages of puberty. Archives of Diseases in Childhood, 1976, 21, 170-179. Tanner, J. M., Whitehouse, R. H., Marubini, E., & Resele, L. The adolescent growth spurt of boys and girls of the Harpenden Growth Study. Annals of Human Biology, 1976, 1, 109-126. Tanner, J. M., Whitehouse, R. H., & Takaishi, M. Standards from birth to maturity for height, weight, height velocity, and weight velo- city: British children, 1965. Archives of Disease in Childhood, 1966a, 41, 454-471; 1966b, 613-635. Whipple, D. Changing body proportions from conception to adulthood. Dynamics of development: euthenic pediatrics. New York: McGraw Hill, 1966. Zacharias, L., wurtman, R. J., & Schatzoff, M. Sexual maturation in contemporary American girls. American Journal of Obstetrics and Gynecology, 1970, 108, 833-846. A T HICHIG N 5 WW" I 13112 97 W 533 UNIV. LIBRARIES 1 W “W W VI (5554 0632 E WI 169