OVERDUE FINES ARE 25¢ PER DAY PER ITEM Return to book drop to remove this checkout from your record. PERFORMANCE OF SEVERELY AND PROFOUNDLY HEARING IMPAIRED CHILDREN ON AURAL/ORAL AND TOTAL COMMUNICATION PRESENTATIONS OF THE BOEHM TEST OF BASIC CONCEPTS By Daun Christine Dickie A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Special Education Administration 1979 ABSTRACT PERFORMANCE OF SEVERELY AND PROFOUNDLY HEARING IMPAIRED CHILDREN ON AURAL/ORAL AND TOTAL COMMUNICATION PRESENTATIONS OF THE BOEHM TEST OF BASIC CONCEPTS By Daun Christine Dickie Numerous educators and researchers have argued as tO the appropriate methods to be utilized in the education Of severely and profoundly hearing impaired Children. The two major methodologies used in educational training pro- grams today are Aural/Oral and Total Communication, which differ primarily in the addition Of a manual form for the latter approach. In the midst Of such controversy, federal and state mandates have been approved which require that the language performance Of hearing impaired children be assessed using appropriate, nondiscriminatory measures. However, to date, nO standardized testing instrument has been shown tO be appropriate for use with this population when viewed in terms Of presentation method (Aural/Oral or Total Communi- cation). In view Of the above, this study investigated the receptive language performance Of elementary-age severely Daun Christine Dickie and profoundly hearing impaired children using the Boehm Test Of Basic Concepts. Fifteen Of the subjects had been taught using an Aural/Oral approach, and 15 utilized a Total Communication approach, resulting in a total of 30 subjects. Each of the 50 test items Of the Boehm Test Of Basic Concepts was presented to each child twice, using the communication method with which each subject was familiar. The subjects' task was tO mark a pictorial rep- resentation Of the stimulus item. The results revealed that the children Of the Total Communication group performed significantly better than did their Aural/Oral counterparts. Age Of subjects was not found tO be related with test performance for either group investigated. The reliability Of results Obtained during test- retest administration was found to be high for both groups of subjects. However, the internal reliability Of this test was higher for subjects Of the Aural/Oral group. This was due to a ceiling effect for scores correct which was evidenced for subjects using a Total Communication approach. These findings suggest that differences do exist in the receptive language skills, as measured by this test, between hearing impaired children using an Aural/Oral or Total Communication approach. The results are related tO Daun Christine Dickie current concerns for educational programming, and impli- cations for future research are discussed. DEDICATION TO my parents, for their faith and support despite undergraduate days and grades. ii ACKNOWLEDGMENTS I wish to express my sincere appreciation tO the members Of my committee, Dr. Herbert J. Oyer, Dr. Charles Henley, Mrs. Vivian Stevenson, and Dr. Richard Featherstone, for their valuable time and thoughtful contributions. I am also indebted to Mr. Rob Young and Dr. Grace Iverson Of the Lansing School District Department Of Evaluation Services, for their assistance in the evaluation and interpretation aspects Of this study. Appreciation is further extended tO Dr. Velma Allen, Mrs. Mildred Richardson, and Mr. Malcolm Delbridge for their support throughout the entire process Of this research project. In addition, I wish to thank Mrs. Brenda Hull for her active participation in this study, as well as the numerous hours Of challenging thought which she pro— vided. For the friendship and support which I have experi- enced throughout this year, I extend my affection and appreciation to Diane, Kathy, Velma, Jody, Phyllis, and Tom. Finally, I want to extend my extreme gratitude to my chairman, Dr. Charles Mange, whose encouragement, expec- tations, and belief in my ability were demonstrated through- out a time when they were needed most. iii TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES LIST OF APPENDICES Chapter I. II. III. IV. INTRODUCTION . Aural/Oral Communication Total Communication . Investigations Related to Testing Procedures for Hearing Impaired Children Statement Of the Problem . Purpose Of the Study EXPERIMENTAL PROCEDURES Subjects Materials Presentation PrOcedures Analysis RESULTS Main Effect of Group Effect Of Age . Test Reliability DISCUSSION . Group Differences in Test Performance Comparison Of Test Performance With That Of Hard- Of- -Hearing Subjects Relationship Of Age and Subject Performance . . . . Types Of Subject Errors Test Reliability Implications Related tO the IntegratiOn .Of Hearing Impaired Children Implications for Future Research iv Page vi vii 13 18 18 20 20 22 24 27 29 32 33 33 36 36 39 41 42 43 44 46 APPENDICES REFERENCES Page 51 89 Table LIST OF TABLES Summary Table Of an Analysis Performed on the Mean Scores Correct Of Both Groups (Aural/ Oral and Total Communication) and Test Administration (First, Second, and Com- bined Test Results) Summary Table Of an Analysis Of Variance Performed on the Number Of Correct Scores Of Both Groups (Aural/Oral and Total Communication) and Both Test Administra- tions (First and Second) Summary Table Of Test-Retest Reliability by Group (Aural/Oral and Total Communication) Summary Data Depicting the Results Of the Kuder—Richardson Test Of Reliability by Group (Aural/Oral and Total Communication) and Test Administration (First and Second) vi Page 30 31 34 34 LIST OF APPENDICES Appendix A. DESCRIPTIVE DATA OF SUBJECTS USING AN AURAL/ORAL APPROACH . . . B. DESCRIPTIVE DATA OF SUBJECTS USING A TOTAL COMMUNICATION APPROACH . C. CLASSIFICATION OF THE CONCEPTS TO BE TESTED USING THE BOEHM TEST OF BASIC CONCEPTS D. DIRECTIONS GIVEN TO SUBJECTS FOR EACH ITEN OF FORM B OF THE BOEHM TEST OF BASIC CONCEPTS . . . . . . . . . . E. SOURCE/METHOD OF MANUAL PRESENTATION FOR THOSE ITEMS NOT FOUND IN SIGNING EXACT ENGLISH F. INTRODUCTORY DIRECTIONS GIVEN TO ALL SUBJECTS . . . . . . . . . G. SUMMARY DATA DEPICTING TYPES OF RESPONSES IN THE GROUP USING AN AURAL/ORAL APPROACH H. SUMMARY DATA DEPICTING TYPES OF RESPONSES IN THE GROUP USING A TOTAL COMMUNICATION APPROACH . . . . . . . . . . . . . I. SUMMARY DATA DEPICTING TYPES OF RESPONSES FOR EACH ITEM (FIRST AND SECOND TESTS) OVER BOTH GROUPS (AURAL/ORAL AND TOTAL COMMUNICATION) COMBINED . . . . J. SUMMARY DATA DEPICTING RAW SCORES AND PERCENTILE EQUIVALENTS FOR STUDENTS USING AN AURAL/ORAL APPROACH K. SUMMARY DATA DEPICTING RAW SCORES AND PERCENTILE EQUIVALENTS FOR STUDENTS USING A TOTAL COMMUNICATION APPROACH vii Page 52 54 56 59 64 66 68 71 74 77 79 Appendix L. NUMBER OF CORRECT RESPONSES FOR EACH SUBJECT BY GROUP (TOTAL COMMUNICATION AND AURAL/ ORAL) FOR BOTH TEST ADMINISTRATIONS M. SUMMARY DATA OF TEST ITEMS WHICH HAD ZERO OR HIGHLY LIMITED VARIANCE FOR SUBJECTS BY GROUP (TOTAL COMMUNICATION AND AURAL/ ORAL) . . . . . . . . . . . . N. SUMMARY DATA, BY GROUP (AURAL/ORAL AND TOTAL COMMUNICATION) AND CATEGORY (SPACE, QUANTITY, TIME, MISCELLANEOUS) OF TEST ITEMS WHICH WERE ANSWERED INCORRECTLY, 15 TIMES OR MORE, OVER BOTH FIRST AND SECOND TEST ADMINISTRATIONS viii Page 81 83 87 CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION As a result of current state and federal legislation, educators and clinicians are presently faced with the task of assessing the language performance of severely and profoundly hearing impaired students. Section 121a.532 of the Rules and Regulations for the Implementation of Part B of HuaEducation of the Handicapped Act mandates state and local education agencies shall ensure, at a minimum, that: f A. Tests and other evaluation materials: 1. Are provided and administered in the Child's native language or other mode of communication, unless it is clearly not feasible to do so; 2. Have been validated for the Specific purpose for which they are used; and 3. Are administered by trained personnel in con— formance with the instructions provided by their producer; B. Tests and other evaluation materials include those tailored to assess specific areas of educational need and not merely those which are designed to provide a single general intelligence quotient; C. Tests are selected and administered so as best to ensure that when a test is administered to a child with impaired sensory, manual, or speaking skills, the test results accurately reflect the child's aptitude or achievement level or whatever other factors the test purports to measure, rather than reflecting the child's impaired sensory, manual, or speaking skills (except where those skills are the factors which the test purports to measure); D. No single procedure is used as the sole criterion for determining an appropriate educational program for a child; and E. The evaluation is made by a multidisciplinary team or group of persons, including at least one teacher or other specialist with knowledge in the area of suspected disability. F. The Child is assessed in all areas related to the suspected disability, including, where appropriate, health, vision, hearing, social and emotional status, general intelligence, academic performance, communicative status, and motor abilities (Federal Register, 1977, pp. 42496-42497). While the vast majority of professionals would view such a charge as highly desirable, a paucity of standard- ized, appropriately normed tests exists which may be reliably used with severely and profoundly hearing impaired students. In addition, professionals are typically unable to iden- tify research efforts which clearly illustrate that exist- ing standardized tests, which measure language skills, may be reliably used with hearing impaired populations (Michi- gan State Department of Education, 1977; Davis, 1974, 1977; Pressnell, 1973; Cooper, 1967). Efforts to comply with such legal mandates fre— quently result in the indiscriminate use of language tests, which have been normed on nonimpaired populations, with hearing impaired persons. Such tests may or may not be modified in an attempt to assess more accurately the per- formance of hearing impaired individuals. For example, the written form may be added to oral presentations of test items in an attempt to minimize the disadvantages encountered as a result of limited auditory input. Modifications made may vary with the individual examiner, thus further confounding the accuracy or reliability of the results of such tests. An additional alternative employed may be the use of clinician-oa wouoom «pm Ho Ho>mq Chapcspm mo mucus ARMS z .AMpasmon pmou cosmoeoo paw .Ocooom .uhhflmv :OflpmupmHGHEUd pMOp paw ACOHPMOHcSEEOU fiance and Hmuo\adn=¢v mosouw 2909 Ho poouaoo homoom cams map so ooEHOHAOQ Mammamcd as mo manna >pm8€smln.a wands 31 .. .. mm.v mm b®.®mH pouno MHOOnoom swapfla mz ms.H so.m H so.m Md30pm x mflaflae mz mmo. hm.o H hm.o madame .. .. .. om oo.mmH whommnsm chap“; .. .. mm.mm mm no.5wnm MOHAO muomnnom cooapmm Hooo. v d m>.mm Sm.sesm H sm.svsm Manchu .. .. .. mm mm.vmmm MpOOnosm coozwom .. .. .. mm mm.mmvm fiance mooMOHchwfim Ho Hm>mq m m: MU mm condom .Aocooom was phyfiwv mcofipmgpmfiofieos amp» cpon cod AooHpMOHCBEEOU Hapoe com ngo\amns mo wfimzamom as mo macaw >hmEEdmll.m OHQMB 32 two-way analysis of variance with repeated measures (Winer, 1962). Appendices J and K list the raw score data for each subject by group and test presentation. Main Effect of Group Table 1 reveals that the difference between groups (Aural/Oral versus Total Communication) is significant at the p < .0001 level. Thus, the overall means of 47.13 and 74.20 for the two groups, Aural/Oral and Total Commu- nication. respectively, when averaged over both first and second test presentations, do differ significantly. The effects of presentation method were extremely powerful. The Eta Squared for the difference between the two groups was 0.5150, indicating that 50 percent of the variation in language performance skills (as measured by this test) could be accounted for by group membership. The results Of a two-way analysis of variance with repeated measures design are presented in Table 2. These findings would suggest that when degree of loss, compound- ing handicapping conditions, and age of onset are con- sidered in subject selection, hearing impaired children from Aural/Oral versus Total Communication programs would not perform equally on this particular test of receptive language ability. Rather, the children of the Aural/Oral group perform markedly and significantly below those of the group utilizing a Total Communication approach. 33 The tabled values of correct scores for each subject by group are listed in Appendix L. Effect of Age A nonsignificant correlation was found between age of the subjects and test performance. This was true of subjects across both groups combined (r = .065), as well as within groups (Aural/Oral: r = .020; Total Com- munication: r = .150). Thus, regardless of the group investigated, test performance on the Boehm Test of Basic Concepts was not significantly correlated with age of subjects. Test Reliability The Kuder-Richardson test for reliability of measurement was performed on the test responses of both groups. This statistic measured the internal consistency of the test items and the extent to which they measured similar patterns. Table 3 depicts the results of this analysis by group (Aural/Oral and Total Communication). As can be seen, a highly significant (p < .0001) correla- tion exists for test-retest (first and second test) reliability over the two groups. The Fisher Z test for determining the differences between two independent correlations was performed. The resulting difference in Z value of .54 between the 34 correlations for the Aural/Oral and Total Communication groups was not found to be significant at a p < .05 level. Table 3.--Summary table of test—retest reliability by group (Aural/Oral and Total Communication). . Level of Group N Correlation (R) Significance Total Communication 15 0.85 p < .0001 Aural/Oral 15 0.93 p < .0001 Combined 30 0.96 p < .0001 Table 4 illustrates that the obtained reliability coefficients were higher for the Aural/Oral group on both first and second test administrations than for the Total Communication group. This may be due to a ceiling effect which occurs for the Total Communication population on both first and second test administrations. Table 4.--Summary data depicting the results of the Kuder- Richardson test of reliability by group (Aural/ Oral and Total Communication) and test adminis- tration (first and second). KR 20 KR 20 First Test Second Test Administration Administration Total Communication 0.68 0.53 Aural/Oral 0.85 0.89 Combined 0.90 0.92 35 Test items were identified for both groups which had zero or highly limited variance. These are listed in Appendix M. As can be seen, a large number of items had zero variance for the group of subjects using Total Com- munication, as they were correctly identified during test- ings by these children. The internal consistency between subjects was higher for the Aural/Oral group due to the variation of subjects' scores evidenced. Thus, the overall test relia- bility of the Boehm Test of Basic Concepts was high. Sev- eral of the test items appear to be too simple for subjects of this age range using the Total Communication approach, and are limited in their discriminating abilities. They may, however, be appropriate items for inclusion for meet- ing a basic purpose of the test, which is to determine if these concepts are understood by individual students. CHAPTER IV DISCUSSION Group Differences in Test Performance As has been demonstrated, a significant difference exists between the mean test scores of the groups of chil- dren using an Aural/Oral versus a Total Communication approach. This difference is apparent for both first and second test administrations. The range of scores is noticeably greater for the Aural/Oral group (16 to 41 for the first test and 13 to 46 for the second test) than for the Total Communication group (27 to 44 for the first test and 31 to 43 for the second test). As can be seen, no subject using a Total Communication approach performed as poorly as did the lowest-scoring Aural/Oral subjects. One difference between subjects in the compo- sition of the two groups was that of degree of hearing loss. Four of the 15 subjects in the Aural/Oral group demonstrated severe as opposed to profound levels of hear- ing impairments. However, only a single subject in the Total Communication group demonstrated such relatively high hearing ability. There are insufficient data in the present investigation to allow for absolute comparative 36 37 statements relative to subject performance as related to degree of hearing loss (severe versus profound). How- ever, it is interesting to note that despite the Aural/ Oral group's possible advantage due to increased hearing acuity, the subjects nevertheless performed signifi- cantly poorer than did those of the Total Communication group. A Total Communication presentation is often described as being highly ideographic in nature, with sign symbols visibly illustrating the intended word (Davis, 1977). Thus, it may be argued that many of the stimulus sentences were so graphically portrayed for the Total Communication subjects that correct responses were arrived at without actual prior knowledge of the word(s) involved. An examination of the actual concepts tested reveals that, in many instances, such a claim may be accu- rate. The sign symbols for concepts such as "over, around, behind, below, zero, and above" are indeed graphic in nature. However, this is not true for the more obscure sign representations for concepts such as "least, always, some, not many, most, beginning" or fingerspelled items such as "forward." Further, it should be stressed that the 50 concepts were embedded in sentence combinations of up to 22 words in length, during test presentations. It is questionable whether subjects of this age could correctly select the single concept sign to successfully respond to 38 an item without some degree of comprehension of the lan- guage material being presented. This may be illustrated through the examination of such test items as: #9. Look at the clothes hanging on the line. Mark the dress that is farthest from the socks. #27. Look at the box of pencils and the groups of pencils. Mark the group that has as many pencils as the box. #42. Look at the groups of circles and dots. Mark the group that has a dot in every circle. Finally, it should be reiterated that the actual purpose of Total Communication is to present all available forms of input to maximize comprehension. An originating premise purports that an Aural/Oral presentation is too Often insufficient in conveying information to the hearing impaired individual. The addition of graphic cues where possible, to enhance communication abilities, is an intended aspect of Total Communication. The results of this investigation would not allow for the conclusive statement that children trained utiliz- ing a Total Communication approach possess significantly better overall receptive language skills than do their Aural/Oral counterparts. Only those comments regarding performance as measured by this single test may be made with assurance. Therefore, it may be concluded that chil- dren trained to use a Total Communication approach, receiv- ing the stimulus items of the Boehm Test of Basic Concepts via that same presentation mode, perform significantly 39 better on this test than do children of a similar age range utilizing an Aural/Oral approach. Documentation of this finding using other groups of children and measures of assessment could allow for a more conclusive statement to be made. Comparison of Test Performance With That of Hard-Of—Hearing Subjects Davis (1974) provides data depicting the performance of 24 hard-of-hearing children, ranging in age from 6 years to 8 years, 11 months, on the Boehm Test of Basic Concepts. Although these subjects were younger as a group than those included in the present investigation, some interesting comparative observations may be made. The subjects of the Davis study demonstrated mild or moderate levels of hearing loss and the majority were integrated into classrooms for the normally hearing for at least one-half of the school day. Comparisons of the per- centile scores students obtained on the Boehm Test of Basic Concepts were compared to those of middle socio- economic level subjects demonstrating normal levels of hearing acuity. Her results pertaining to hearing impaired subjects revealed that: Only 22 percent of the seven and eight year olds scored at or above the 40th and 60th percentile levels on the test. Fifty percent of the six year olds scored at or below the 10th percentile, while 67% of the seven year olds and 83% of the eight year olds scored below that level. Only two hearing impaired children scored above the 80th percentile; 40 each of them exhibited a high frequency hearing loss characterized by normal hearing through 500 Hz. Most discouraging of all, two-thirds of the seven and eight year old hearing impaired children scored at the 1st percentile in knowledge of these concepts (p. 346). The performance of many of the severely and pro- foundly hearing impaired children of the present investi- gation was very similar to the results reported by Davis for younger hard-of-hearing subjects using an Aural/Oral approach. When the scores of the first and second tests were combined, 12 of the 15 subjects using an Aural/Oral approach scored at the first percentile level for normal hearing first graders. Comparison of performance with that of normal hearing second graders shows that 13 of the Aural/ Oral group scored at the first percentile. (See Appendix J.) Using an average of the first and second test scores, only one of the subjects utilizing a Total Communi- cation approach scored at the first percentile level when compared to the normed first grade data. A total of 9 of the 15 subjects scored at the first percentile level when compared to existing norms for normal hearing second graders. (See Appendix K.) It would appear, then, that the severely and pro- foundly hearing impaired subjects of the present investi- gation performed similarly to those younger hard-of—hearing subjects reported on by Davis. This is especially true of those children using an Aural/Oral approach. Further 41 research is needed to investigate more definitively the performance of children who vary in level of hearing loss, age, and communication approach utilized. Relationship onge and Subject Performance An examination of the percentile rankings of raw scores of the hard-of—hearing subjects studied by Davis (1974) revealed that age of subjects was not a significant factor when correlated with test performance. This find- ing was also confirmed in the present investigation. The total age range of these present subjects studied was 6 years, 11 months to 13 years, 7 months. This large span, however, did not result in significant correlations between age and test performance for either the Aural/Oral or Total Communication group. The severely depressed language abilities of hear— ing impaired children have been frequently documented in the literature (McClure, 1966; Simms & Yater, 1974; Wrightsone et a1., 1963). The results of the present inves- tigation would support Davis' premise of a severe lack of progress in the development of these language concepts as hearing impaired children advance in age. Such a find- ing may have significant implications as related to recom- mendations for programming designed to place emphasis upon specific conceptual development for the child demonstrating a need in this area. 42 Types of Subject Errors The results of an item analysis of the errors made by subjects of the Aural/Oral and Total Communication groups are listed in Appendices G, H, and I. Appendix N illustrates those items by group which were in error one- half of the time or more, and identifies the classifica— tion type (Space, Quantity, Time, Miscellaneous) for each of these concepts. A total of 29 of the 50 concepts were in error 15 times or more during both test presentations combined, for the children of the Aural/Oral group. As can be seen, the errors made by children of the Aural/Oral group are numerous, and appear to be somewhat random in nature. No single classification group appears to present a dispro- portionate amount of difficulty for these subjects. Rather, the Aural/Oral children appear to have problems related to all conceptual classifications tested. It is interesting to note that several of these concepts (“few, over, below, left, above, third") are relatively "visible" in terms of speech production. These are also items which are typically included in language curriculum programs used with hearing impaired students. The results of this study would lend support to previously made statements relative to the limitations of Aural/Oral interpretive abilities of children when deciphering con- cepts presented in sentential forms. 43 A total of 8 of the 50 concepts were in error 15 times or more during both test presentations combined, for the children of the Total Communication group. All eight of these concepts ("right, as many, equal, begin- ning, never, alike, match, skip") were also among those most frequently missed by the Aural/Oral subjects. Further, these concepts were either fingerspelled, or were ones for which the signed representation was minimally graphic in nature. Thus, it would appear that these items are ones which either are rarely presented to hearing impaired children, or, if presented, are not reiterated sufficiently to insure mastery of these concepts. These high-frequency errors made by the subjects of both groups were distributed through all four of the conceptual cate— gories. However, the subjects of the Total Communication group appear to perform better than their Aural/Oral coun- terparts in the identification of concepts related to ”space" (1 error versus 13 errors, respectively) and "quantity" (2 errors versus 10 errors, respectively). Test Reliability Figures related to test/retest reliability have been presented in Table 3. A highly significant (p < .0001) correlation was found to exist for this factor. For both the Aural/Oral and Total Communication groups, scores Obtained by each subject on the first and second tests were either identical or very similar. Thus, it would 44 appear that the score obtained from a single administra- tion of the Boehm Test of Basic Concepts would serve as a reliable estimate for additional testing presentations. The reliability of this test for each of the two groups of subjects was investigated. The Boehm Test of Basic Concepts proved to be an internally reliable instru- ment for use with subjects of the Aural/Oral group. This was largely due to the wide variability of subjects' per- formance on test responses. That is, there were suffi- cient differences among subjects' scores hoallow'individual test items to act as discriminating determiners of overall scores. This high degree of internal reliability was not found for the Total Communication presentations. Generally, these subjects tended to achieve similar scores and demon- strated a reduced range of score variability. A ceiling effect was evidenced, and the Boehm Test of Basic Concepts was found to be limited in serving as a sufficiently powerful tool for discerning differences in subject per- formance for this group. The test could, however, be use- ful in the assessment of an individual child's knowledge of particular concepts or conceptual groups. Implications Related to the Integration of Hearing Impaired Children Public Law 94—142 mandates that handicapped chil- dren must be educated in the least restrictive environment 45 which is deemed appropriate to meet individual needs. As a result, numerous hearing impaired children are being considered for placement in general education classrooms for varying portions of the school day. Karchmer and Trybus (1977) report that, at the time of investigation, 19 percent of hearing impaired students were being served in integrated programs consisting of part-time classes, resource rooms, or itinerant services. Such forms of inte- gration may stress social, academic, or a combination of these areas of need for each child. Decisions as to the appropriateness of such place- ments continue to be predicated primarily upon staff and parental input, as well as academic performance in selected subjects. The need exists, both as a result of legal man- dates as well as concern for successful experiences for children, to identify instruments which may be reliably used to measure the language performances of these chil- dren. The documentation obtained could then be utilized, in combination with staff input and academic performance, to make more appropriate recommendations for programming. The results of this study would suggest that ele- mentary level hearing impaired children of both groups, regardless of chronological age, demonstrate severe limi- tations in their receptive language abilities. The Boehm Test of Basic Concepts measures a very limited sampling of the multitude of concepts which the hearing impaired 46 child may be expected to encounter in the very early ele- mentary grades. If, however, it is indeed indicative of the broader range of language concepts necessary for aca- demic and social success, then all of the hearing impaired students tested demonstrate severe limitations and are highly disadvantaged. It would appear that the Boehm Test of Basic Con- cepts is an instrument which could be used with hearing impaired children in an attempt to more objectively assess the appropriateness of placement in a regular education classroom. Examiners should realize that test performance represents behavior which may be expected to occur under more ideal conditions than are encountered in an on-going classroom experience. However, when used as an additive source of input to existing procedures, the Boehm Test of Basic Concepts could serve to enhance the validity of judg- ments made relative to appropriate child placement and pro- gramming. Implications for Future Research The present investigation has demonstrated that the Boehm Test of Basic Concepts is an instrument which may be used with hearing impaired students in an attempt to more objectively assess the appropriateness of recom- mendations for placement into general education classrooms. The overall performance of the subjects investigated was 47 found to be low when compared to normal-hearing children of younger ages. Further research is needed to deter- mine the percentile or criteria of performance on this assessment instrument necessary for successful academic performance in regular educational placements. Research designed to correlate test scores with an "integration adequacy index" could result in a predictive instrument which would strengthen present subjective judgments of recommendations for student placement and programming. An obvious area requiring further research is that with regard to the nature of the test used in this study. A single test, designed to assess receptive language per- formance through a representative sampling of basic con- cepts, was employed. While data-based information was obtained, the need exists to expand such investigations to other standardized instruments for use with hearing impaired children. Further, the need continues to exist for the identification of other instruments which assess additional areas of language performance which have been demonstrated as reliable for use with children who use Total Communication. The present investigation utilized a ”Signing Exact English" manual portrayal of stimulus items pre- sented. Additional research is needed to determine whether the findings of this study, with regard to subject 48 performance, would be similar for students utilizing other sign systems. Another area for future research deals with pos— sible implications of an examiner bias effect. The indi- vidual selected to present the test items to all of the subjects was employed by, and favored, a Total Communica- tion program. Every testing session was observed by the experimenter in an effort to monitor integrity of presen— tation mode. It may be argued that this bias contributed to the depressed scores of the children in the Aural/Oral group. However, it should also be noted that a true form of Total Communication would have allowed for the use of facial and body cues during presentations, which was not allowed in the present investigation. Additional research could be conducted using an examiner who favors an Aural/ Oral approach and could allow for the total spectrum of input forms for children. Although the range of ages for subjects studied was relatively large, limited representation of subjects at any specific age category was present. Future research should increase the number of subjects by age to obtain more conclusive results. Such investigation could also be expanded to include comprehensive information related to varying degrees of hearing loss, ranging from mild to pro- found levels of hearing impairment. 49 Further research is also needed to address the question of how hearing impaired students of Aural/Oral and Total Communication programs perform on measures of receptive language when the variable of presentation fre- quency is controlled. That is, a research study should be designed to determine the relative efficiency and effec- tiveness of learning by students using either communication method, while controlling for exposure to concept presen- tation. In the present investigation, this factor was an unknown variable. It was impossible to determine con- clusively why the Total Communication subjects performed better than their Aural/Oral counterparts. The question must be raised as to the degree of exposure to the various concepts presented children of each program had experienced. A study designed to match the number of exposures for chil— dren of both groups could better ascertain the effective- ness of conceptual learning by children as related to presentation method. Finally, the present study investigated the per- formance of subjects from public-school settings for whom the confounding variable of additional handicapping condi— tion(s) was controlled. Numerous students are presently enrolled in other types of programs for the hearing impaired, and/or exhibit multiple handicaps (Jensema & Trybus, 1975). This latter faCtor increases the diffi- culty of appropriate assessment of performance. However, 50 the need most definitely exists to identify instruments which may be reliably used with this population as well. Jensema and Trybus (1978) state that too often educators favor a "quick jump to the bottom line” approach to obtain information related to highly complex issues. They stress that the factors related to the communication patterns and aChievement levels of hearing impaired stu- dents cannot be properly understood or evaluated, except in their complexity. The critical need for continued experimental examination, to determine appropriate assess- ment procedures, is an absolute necessity for meeting the goal of providing more effective programming for hearing impaired individuals. APPENDICES 51 APPENDIX A DESCRIPTIVE DATA OF SUBJECTS USING AN AURAL/ORAL APPROACH 52 53 .Omooamon o: u mz* moa mm om was: wINH ma mm om ow was: NINH «a moH 00H 05 oHanm Hum ma mm cm on cam: mum NH moa moa OOH mam: mlma Ha mz ooH ow wads mum 0H om on om was: OIHH m mz mz mz was: win w moH om om mamaom NIoH b ow ow mu CHEEom OHIHH m OHH mod no OHdEom HImH m 00H moH mm mamsom HImH v mHH moH mm mad: Halo m moH moH om was: wnu N Aux oomav *mz mod om bassom «Ina H mm ooom N: oooH um oom xom mango: was noosoz “omnoom Amsv msHOEMMEse mcfihamm made» ow mw< mo¢ommm< AHBmHmUmmm < xHszmm¢ APPENDIX B DESCRIPTIVE DATA OF SUBJECTS USING A TOTAL COMMUNICATION APPROACH 54 55 .omcoomom on n mz* mz ooH mm OHM: NIOH mH mz oHH mm OHM: HIoH wH moH mm ow OHME Hum mH mz moH om OHMS mum NH mz ooH mm mHmz HHIm HH ooH mm om OHmEom mum oH ow mm ow OHMEOM OINH m mz moH mm OHMEOM bImH m mz mz oHH OHmEmm mum m oHH oHH om mHmEOm OHINH m mz OHH ooH OHMEOm NINH m mz mz om oHdEom MINH v mz om Ob OHMEOm onoH m oHH moH mm OHMEOm @10H m mcH *mz OHH OHmsom HIHH H um ooom um oooH Mm oom xmm mspaos use “@2932 pommnsm Amuv mcHonmmhse maHHmom mummy :H ow¢ moHBmHmDme m NHDzmmm¢ APPENDIX C CLASSIFICATION OF THE CONCEPTS TO BE TESTED USING THE BOEHM TEST OF BASIC CONCEPTS 56 CLASSIFICATION OF THE CONCEPTS TO BE TESTED USING THE BOEHM TEST OF BASIC CONCEPTS APPENDIX C Concept Concept Category Space Quantity Time Misc. UILBOONH Top Through Away from Next to Inside NXXNX Some, not many Middle Few Farthest Around N Over Widest Most Between Whole Nearest Second Corner Several Behind Row Different After Almost Half Center As many Side Beginning Other *x 57 58 Concept Category Concept Space Quantity Time Misc. 31 Alike x 32 Not first or last * x * 33 Never x 34 Below x 35 Matches X 36 Always x 37 Medium-sized x 38 Right x 39 Forward x 40 Zero x 41 Above x 42 Every x 43 Separated x * 44 Left x 45 Pair x 46 Skip x 47 Equal x 48 In order x 49 Third * x * 50 Least X X's indicate the context category of each concept as it is tested. Asterisks indicate additional contexts in which the concepts may be employed. For example, the concept of beginning (item 29) is used in the context of time on the BTBC, but it may also be used to express relationships involving space. APPENDIX D DIRECTIONS GIVEN TO SUBJECTS FOR EACH ITEM OF FORM B OF THE BOEHM TEST OF BASIC CONCEPTS 59 10. 11. APPENDIX D DIRECTIONS GIVEN TO SUBJECTS FOR EACH ITEM OF FORM B OF THE BOEHM TEST OF BASIC CONCEPTS Look at the flags on the poles. Mark the pole with the flag at the top. . . . Mark the pole with the flag at the top. Look at the dogs and the hoops. Mark the dog that is going through the hoop. . . . Mark the dog that is going through the hoop. Look at the baby and the blocks. Mark the block that is away from the baby. . . . Mark the block that is away from the baby. Look at the animals. Mark the animal that is next to the rabbit. . . . Mark the animal that is next to the rabbit. Look at the boxes and balls. Mark the box with the balls inside it. . . . Mark the box with the balls inside it. Look at the bowls of flowers. Mark the bowl that has some but not magy flowers. . . . Mark the bowl that has some but not many flowers. Look at the children. Mark the child who is in the middle. . . . Mark the child who is in the middle. Look at the pictures of boxes. Mark the picture that has a few boxes. . . . Mark the picture that has a few boxes. Look at the clothes hanging on the line. Mark the dress that is farthest from the socks. . . . Mark the dress that is farthest from the socks. Look at the flowers and strings. Mark the flower that has a string around it. . . . Mark the flower that has a string around it. Look at the children and the rope. Mark the child who is over the rope. . . . Mark the child who is over the rope. 6O 12. 13. 14. 15. 16. 17. 18. 19. 20. 21. 22. 23. 24. 61 Look at the ties. Mark the tie that is widest. Mark the tie that is widest. Look at the boxes of buttons. Mark the box that has the most buttons. . . . Mark the box that has the most buttons. Look at the pictures of toys. Mark the picture that has a bear between two blocks. . . . Mark the picture that has a bear between two blocks. Look at the apples. Mark the apple that is whole. Mark the apple that is whole. Look at the dogs and the bone. Mark the dog that is nearest the bone. . . . Mark the dog that is nearest the bone. Look at the line of trucks and the sign. Mark the second truck from the sign. . . . Mark the second truck from the sign. Look at the buildings. Mark the building that is at a corner of the street. . . . Mark the building that is at a corner of the street. Look at the groups of knives, forks, and spoons. Mark the group that has several spoons. . . . Mark the group that has several spoons. Look at the boys and the wagon. Mark the boy who is behind the wagon. . . . Mark the boy who is behind the wagon. Look at the pictures of bottles. Mark the picture where all the bottles are in a row. . . . Mark the picture where all the bottles are in a row. Look at the piles of books. Mark the pile that is different from the others. . . . Mark the pile that is different from the others. Look at the pictures of a piece of wood. Mark the picture that shows how the wood looked after it was cut. . . . Mark the picture that shows how the wood looked after it was cut. Look at the baskets of fruit. Mark the basket that is almost full. . . . Mark the basket that is almost full. 25. 26. 27. 28. 29. 30. 31. 32. 33. 34. 35. 36. 37. 62 Look at the boxes. Mark the box that is half black. Mark the box that is half black. Look at the ring and the marbles. Mark the marble that is at the center of the ring. . . . Mark the marble that is at the center of the ring. Look at the box of pencils and the groups of pencils. Mark the group that has as many pencils as the box. Mark the group that has as many pencils as the box. Look at the car and the boys. Mark the boy at the side of the car. . . . Mark the boy at the side of the car. Look at the boys on the stairs. Mark the boy who is beginning to climb the stairs. . . . Mark the boy who is beginning to climb the stairs. Look at the toys. One is a doll and one is a truck. Mark the other toy. . . . Mark the other toy. Look at the socks. Mark the socks that are alike. Mark the socks that are alike. Look at the ducks in the water. Mark the duck that is not the first or the last. . . . Mark the duck that is not the first or the last. Look at the lamp, the wristwatch, and the shoe. Mark the thing that a child should never wear. . . . Mark the thing that a child should never wear. Look at the bench and the birds. Mark the bird that is below the bench. . . . Mark the bird that is below the bench. Look at the shirts and pants. Mark the pants that match one of the shirts. . . . Mark the pants that match one of the shirts. Look at the box, the wheel, and the feather. Mark the thing a bicycle always has. . . . Mark the thing a bicycle always has. Look at the butterflies. Mark the butterfly that is medium sized. . . . Mark the butterfly that is medium sized. 38. 39. 40. 41. 42. 43. 44. 45. 46. 47. 48. 49. 50. 63 Look at the apples on the shelf. Mark the apple at the right end of the shelf. . . . Mark the apple at the right end of the shelf. Look at the little chicks. Mark the chick that is bending forward. . . . Mark the chick that is bending forward. Look at the rabbits and carrots. Mark the rabbit that has zero carrots. . . . Mark the rabbit that has zero carrots. Look at the windows of the house. Mark the window that is above the door. . . . Mark the window that is above the door. Look at the groups of circles and dots. Mark the group that has a dot in every circle. . . . Mark the group that has a dot in every circle. Look at the pictures of boxes. Mark the picture where the boxes are separated. . . . Mark the picture where the boxes are separated. Look at the trees. Mark the tree on the left. Mark the tree on the left. Look at the pictures of dolls. Mark the picture that shows a pair of dolls. . . . Mark the picture that shows a pair of dolls. Look at the circles. One circle has an X in it. Skip a circle and make another X. . . . Skip a circle and make another X. Look at the groups of stars. Mark the groups that have equal numbers of stars. . . . Mark the groups that have equal numbers of stars. Look at the pictures of boxes. Mark the picture where the boxes are in order from small to large. . . . Mark the picture where the boxes are in order from small to large. Look at the store and the houses. Mark the third house from the store. . . . Mark the third house from the store. Look at the pictures of ice cream cones. Mark the picture that has the least cones. . . . Mark the pic- ture that has the least cones. APPENDIX E SOURCE/METHOD OF MANUAL PRESENTATION FOR THOSE ITEMS NOT FOUND IN SIGNING EXACT ENGLISH 64 APPENDIX E SOURCE/METHOD OF MANUAL PRESENTATION FOR THOSE ITEMS NOT FOUND IN SIGNING EXACT ENGLISH 1975) Item Hoops (Street) Sign (Circular) Ring Forward Skip (Ice Cream) Cones Always Not Alike Different Around Never Match Few Order Row (Neck) Ties Store Behind Below Least Inside *"Program Signs” were employed when no existing formal sign could be located which conveyed the approp- (Gustason et al., Source/Method of Presentation riate meaning of the intended item. which the children of this particular program for the hearing impaired utilized to illustrate a particular word. 65 Item Item Item Item Item Item (Babbini, (Babbini, (Babbini, (Babbini, (Babbini, (Babbini, (Babbini, (Babbini, (Watson, (Watson, *Program *Program *Program *Program *Program *Program fingerspelled fingerspelled fingerspelled fingerspelled fingerspelled fingerspelled 1976) 1976) 1976) 1976) 1976) 1976) 1976) 1976) 1973) 1973) Sign Sign Sign Sign Sign Sign These were signs APPENDIX F INTRODUCTORY DIRECTIONS GIVEN TO ALL SUBJECTS 66 APPENDIX F INTRODUCTORY DIRECTIONS GIVEN TO ALL SUBJECTS (Modified from those prescribed hatest manual of the Boehm Test of Basic Concepts) I have given you a book with some pictures in it. Watch, and listen, and do what I say. You are going to look at pictures and mark "X's" on them. This is how you make an "X” (experimenter demonstrates). Now look at the shoe, the hat, and the sock. Mark an "X" on the hat. . . . Mark an "X" on the hat. Now look at the things to ride in. Mark an "X” on the boat. . . . Mark an "X" on the boat. Look at the fruit. Mark the banana. . . . Mark the banana. Very good. If you make a mistake or want a different answer, that is OK. Make a circle around the wrong picture like this (experimenter demonstrates). Then make the new "X.” 67 APPENDIX G SUMMARY DATA DEPICTING TYPES OF RESPONSES IN THE GROUP USING AN AURAL/ORAL APPROACH 68 69 m N H NH O u ON O N O OH O O ON O m N OH b O ON H O H OH HH O NN O N O O O O HN N H H NH O O ON O O m b m O OH O m N OH > O OH O H O OH > O NH O H O HH O O OH O N N HH O O OH O N O O O O OH O H O OH > O OH O H m HH v 5 NH O O O NH O O HH m H N NH HH H OH O N H NH O O O O O H HH O O O O O O O O O b v H m HH v b O N H H OH HH N O N N O NH O O O O O O O O O m m N H NH O m N O O v HH OH H H mmmaoammm HOOP Ocoomm pmmg.Ocoomm OOOOOQOON mcoHpmHsOmmhm_ MOOHpapcmmmhm Hmossz 296$:ch no HOMES co 98.283 HERMES ME. 58 H8. 58 .53 mo MOQEDZ IIHmmB pthm Ilpmme pmnHm Ho HOQEOZ so powhhoo so pomhhoocH pmmh Happh so pomahoqu no pompuoo Hdpoe mom¢223m o NHDzmmm¢ 7O OOH OO NOH OOO OON OON HOHDB O O O NH 5 O OO O O N OH O O OO O O N O O O OO o O o OH OH O NO N O N OH O O OO O O o NH H HH OO O v W b N O OO O O O b O N OO O N O OH O O NO v H O HH v O HO N o N OH NH H CO v H O HH 5 O OO O N H NH N OH OO N H H OH O O OO O N O OH O O OO H O H OH O HH OO O O O NH O O HO O N N HH O O OO O O N O O O NO 0 O o OH OH O HO O H O HH O O CO O O H HH O O ON O O N CH O O ON H H O OH O HH 5N v H O HH v 5 ON mmmoommmm MOB OOOOOO HOB Ocoomm mmmsonmwm pampmHmcoocH no #09300 no #098005 HOEHOOOO MOMMWMHMMMOOHAH mcmwwwpmwwmhm Ame—MHZ Ho M352 {Moe HOE I58. ORE H0 8852 no 6850 so 59:85 Mme H.309 no #09385 no ”58.80 H.309 APPENDIX H SUMMARY DATA DEPICTING TYPE OF RESPONSES IN THE GROUP USING A TOTAL COMMUNICATION APPROACH 71 72 o o o OH O O ON m o m H m m mm O N H NH O O NN o o 0 OH OH o H O N N HH OH H ON N H H OH O O OH 0 o o OH OH H OH H O H OH OH O OH m m o m H 0H m H o o O OH OH O OH H o H OH OH O OH o o O OH OH H NH O O o OH OH H HH H H o E H o oH O o O NH O O O H H O OH OH H O H H o OH NH N b N N O OH O O O o o o OH OH o O o o O OH O O O O O O OH OH N O o o o OH OH o H mmmcogwmm HOB HEOOOO HOB OHHOOOO mmmcommmm mqu HHOOOHAH meH HHGOOOHAH .8852 HOOHMHOHHOOHHH so 8.8.800 :0 HOOHHOOOH pampmHmeO “WOO. apom ”ROB apom 83H , Ho .5952 Ilpmme awthH {MOB HOHHEH Ho .8852 no 909300 so HOOHHOOHHH HOB H.309 no HOOHHOOOH no HOOHHOO H.309 OUOOOQQO ZOHBOUHZDESOO HOBOB O OZHOD QDOOO MOB zH OOOZOQOWO m0 mmwe OZHBDHQMQ O HOBOMB QZOUWO QZO BOOHMV 2MBH NOON mom OWOZOQOOO m0 OWQWB OZHBOHQOQ OBOQ >OHaNhNQEoo How cmucmmmya mAN Homemv mpmmozoo OHmNm mo pmmB Eamom may no dawn m>HpNEuoc Eonm mmhstm Ho>mH oHanoomoHoom mHuuH2* Ob.H O0.0 O0.0H O0.0N bN.OO N0.0N OIOH cam: H H O 0.0N ON ON OINH OH H H O o.hH NH NH NINH OH H H O o.OH OH HO HIO OH O OH Ob 0.00 0O HO Ola NH H H O 0.0H OH OH OINH HH H H O O.bH OH NH NIO OH OH Om OO 0.00 OO 0O OIHH m H H m o.hH OH OH Olb O H H O 0.0H OH ON NIOH b H O on O.NO NO Om OHIHH O H H O 0.0H OH OH HIOH O H H OH 0.0N ON ON HIOH O H H O O.NN HN ON HHIO O H H OH o.hm hm hm Olb N H H O 0.0N OH NO OIOH H N NBS H @920 8533.5 888 magxwaam waxmugam pOmDfifiu .hkfiflz mmnoom Name 8388 Name Smog. 88% 5mg. NEE No «3 N8 Bum No m§m2 ”GEE mmHHpcmopmm 982 *OOOOOQQO AHDGM MAHBZMOOWQ 92¢ OmmOOm BOO OZHBOHQWQ HpNANQEoo you cmpcmmmua mud Homemv mwnmocoo onNm mo pmms Eamom map No «pan m>NuNEuon scum wmpstw Hm>mH oHEocoomoHoom mHvoH=* OO.N OO.NH NN.OO NH.bN NO.bN b0.0N OIOH COOS H H ON O.NN HN EN NIOH OH H N ON O.NN NN NN HIOH OH H N ON O.NN ON NN HIN NH N .OH OO O.NN OO NN OIN NH N ON OO O.NN OO NN HHIN HH OH OO ON O.NO NO OO NIN OH N OH OO O.NN HO NN OINH N H O OO 0.0N ON ON blNH N H N ON O.HN NN ON NIN b H OH OO O.NN ON OO OHINH O N OH ON 0.00 NN HO NINH O H OH OO O.NN ON ON NINH O H OH OO O.NN HO ON OIOH N O ON Ob O.HO HO HO NIOH N H OH OO O.NN ON ON HIHH H N 335 H 035 89.8385 m88N 28m Nam 88m Nam 98 Ram “3:2 8.80m ”Ems 85950 pmme Swan. cacomm memos ”ENE nHo mw< Havana No gm: ”GEE mmHHEmogoN 58: *OUHDGm MHHBzmommm Qz< OOOOOO 3