I «H ,5 I III III III III III I III II IIII II III LIBRARY Michigan State University This is to certify that the thesis entitled THE CLINICAL PROBLEM SOLVING BEHAVIOR OF CLASSROOM TEACHERS AS THEY DIAGNOSE CHILDRENS' READING PERFORMANCE IN EXPERIMENTAL AND CLASSROOM SITUATIONS presented by DORON GIL has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for Ph.D. Eduaation degree in Major professor 11.1.79 Date 0-7639 THE CLINICAL PROBLEM SOLVING BEHAVIOR OF CLASSROOM TEACHERS AS THEY DIAGNOSE CHILDREN'S READING PERFORMANCE IN EXPERIMENTAL AND CLASSROOM SITUATIONS BY Doron Gil A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Student Teaching and Professional Development 1979 ABSTRACT THE CLINICAL PROBLEM SOLVING BEHAVIOR OF CLASSROOM TEACHERS AS THEY DIAGNOSE CHILDREN'S READING PERFORMANCE IN EXPERIMENTAL AND CLASSROOM SITUATIONS BY Doron Gil The purpose of this study was to investigate the clinical problem solving behavior of classroom teachers in reading diagnosis. The literature on reading instruction views classroom diagnosis as an essential part of the total reading instruction. Most researchers believe classroom teachers have the responsi- bility to conduct necessary diagnoses and provide the appropriate remediations for students with reading defi- ciencies. No empirical evidence exists, however, to show how capable classroom teachers are of performing these functions. Also lacking from the literature is a description of the cognitive processes teachers employ as they attempt to diagnose students' reading difficulties. There were two objectives to this study: (1) to investigate the clinical problem solving behavior of class- room teachers, in both experimental and classroom situations, as they gather data about children's reading difficulties to arrive at diagnostic judgments about the children's Doron Gil difficulties; and (2) to explore the similarities and differences between teachers' diagnostic practices in experimental and classroom situations. Ten teachers from the Chicago and the Lansing areas participated in the study. Research was carried out in two phases. In the first, the teachers interacted with simulated cases of reading difficulties in a laboratory setting; in the second, they were observed and interviewed in their own classrooms. Each of the ten teachers interacted with two simulated cases (at two different times). The cases were built on materials that would be available to the teacher in a regular classroom setting and made use of children's natural language patterns. After each interaction, the teachers went through a stimulus recall session. A few weeks after the laboratory experiment, each teacher was observed in her own classroom during one reading session and interviewed about her reading instruction and diagnostic practices. Data analysis consisted of product measures, process measures, and analysis of classroom interviews. Product measures dealt with the outcomes of the clinical encounter between a teacher and a case (e.g., what final diagnostic judgments were made about the case; what cues were collected, etc.). Process measures focused on the manner in which the problem was diagnosed (e.g., length of interaction, number Doron Gil of cues collected, number of final stated diagnostic judgments, and the relationships between cues collected and teachers' verbatim comments about these cues). Classroom interviews were analyzed in terms of (l) diagnostic categories mentioned in the classroom, (2) remedial techniques offered, and (3) teachers' responses to various questions in the interview. Comparisons were also made between teachers' classroom diagnoses and their laboratory diagnoses. The major findings of this study were that: (1) teachers showed little agreement on their final stated diagnostic judgments for the cases of reading difficulties, (2) they apparently lack comprehensive or systematic approaches to gathering and evaluating information about cases of reading difficulties, and (3) they use the same global diagnostic categories in laboratory situations and in the natural classroom environment. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS I wish to express my sincere appreciation to those who have helped me throughout this investigation: Dr. John Vinsonhaler, who directed my dissertation, for his on-going help, feedback, and trust in my ability to conclude this complex study. Dr. Donald Freeman, chairman of my committee, for his everlasting encouragement and critical comments on previous drafts of the dissertation. Dr. Lee Shulman, for introducing me, together with John, to this fascinating area of clinical decision making, and for helping me reach decisions regarding the objectives, form and methods of this study. Dr. George Sherman, for teaching me reading diagnosis on an informal basis, and devoting many hours helping me with this study. Dr. Robert Hatfield, who was always helpful with his constructive feedback on previous drafts of the dissertation. Dr. Rebecca Barr, from the University of Chicago, for developing the simulated cases used in this study, consulting with me regarding the study's methods of data collection, and enabling me to use the reading clinic at University of Chicago for part of my observations. Mrs. Annette Weinshank, who volunteered her time to assist me with data collection in Chicago. Mr. Chris Wagner and Ms. Francine Kitchen, who designed the computer programs needed for data analysis, and helped me with those analyses. Mrs. Ruth Polin, Ms. Debie Salters, Mr. Ray Smith and Mrs. Jerri Zemper, for providing me with the technical and administrative assistance so vital for this enterprise. My wife, Rakel, for her continuous spiritual support. ii TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES LIST OF FIGURES Chapter I. II. Introduction . . . . . . . The Problem . . . . . . . Research Tasks . . . Significance of the Study Rationale for the Study. . Overview of the Study . . . . THE PROBLEM O I O O O O C O O C C O O O 0 REVIEW OF THE LITERATURE . . . . . . . . . IntIOduction O O O O O O I O O O O O O O 0 Nature of Reading Diagnosis . . . . . . Place of diagnosis in the teaching- learning situation . . . . . . Importance of reading diagnosis . Efficacy of diagnosis . . . . . . Ways of defining reading diagnosis. Levels of reading diagnosis . . . . Nature of reading diagnosis: Summary Competence of classroom teachers as diagnosticians . . . . . . . . . Research on diagnostic skills of classroom teachers . . . . . . . . . Models of reading diagnosis . . . . . . Barr's Model of diagnosis . . . . . . . Sherman's Model of Reading and Learning to read . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . The literature on reading diagnosis: Conclusions . . . . . . . . . . . Clinical Studies of Reading Diagnosis at the Institute for Research on Teaching Introduction . . . . . . . The Inquiry Theory . . . . The agreement corollary . . Case simulation . . . . . . Observational studies . . . Computer simulation studies iii Page add-bu)!“ l-I £000 13 23 33 35 40 47 47 49 52 54 54 55 57 62 63 65 Chapter Training studies . . . . . . . . . . . Clinical studies of reading diagnosis: Conclusions . . . . . . . . . . . . . Summary III. DESIGN AND METHODOLOGY . . . . . . . . . . Introduction. The Subjects Design . Phase 1: Laboratory conditions . . . . Phase 2: Classroom interviews . . . . . Data Analysis . . . . . . . . . . . . . Comparisons between teachers' diagnostic processes in laboratory and classroom situations . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Summary . IV. RESULTS . Introduction . Presentation of Results . . . . Product measures . Process measures . Part 1. Part 2. Part 3: Summary . Classroom interviews other findings 0 O O O O O O O O O O C O O O O O O O O O O O O O V. SUMMARY, CONCLUSIONS AND RECOMMENDATIONS . Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Procedures . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Data Analysis . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Results . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Conclusions . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Recommendations . . . . . . . . . . . . . . APPENDICES APPENDIX A: CASE INVENTORY . . . . . . . . APPENDIX B: DIRECTIONS FOR OBSERVATIONAL SESSION . . . . . . . APPENDIX C: CLASSROOM INTERVIEW . . . APPENDIX D: DESCRIPTIONS OF CHILDREN WITH READING DIFFICULTY . . . . . APPENDIX : FREQUENCIES OF DESCRIPTIONS OF READING DIFFICULTIES SELECTED BY TEACHERS IN THE CLASSROOM . APPENDIX F: DIAGNOSTIC DOMAIN CASE 7 AND 8 iv Page 67 69 69 72 72 72 74 78 81 83 160 160 167 169 170 180 184 193 194 APPENDIX G: APPENDIX H: APPENDIX I: LIST OF REFERENCES LENGTH OF INTERACTION, NUMBER OF CUES COLLECTED AND NUMBER OF DIAGNOSTIC JUDGMENTS FOR CASES 7 AND 8 AND 10 TEACHERS DIAGNOSTIC DOMAIN BASED ON CLASSROOM INTERVIEWS OBSERVATIONAL STUDY DATA ANALYSIS SYSTEM (OSDAS) STATISTICS Page 199 200 203 208 LIST OF TABLES Table Page 1 A comprehensive model of reading diagnosis . . . 34 2 Teachers, school and grades background . . . . . 75 3 Overall design of the study . . . . . . . . . . 77 4 Assignment of cases and inventor forms. . . . . 82 5 Most frequently mentioned diagnostic categories for Case 7 and Case 8 and 10 teachers . . . . 98 6 Most frequently collected cues for Cases 7 and 8 and 10 teachers . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 101 7 Diagnostic commonality score for Cases 7 and 8 and 10 teachers . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 106 8 Cue commonality scores for Cases 7 and 8 and 10 teachers . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 107 9 Inter-clinician correlations for diagnostic judgments for Cases 7 and 8 and 10 teachers . 109 10 Inter-clinician correlations for cues collected for Cases 7 and 8 and 10 teachers . . . . . . 110 11 Length of interaction for Cases 7 and 8 and 10 teaChers O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O 116 12 Number of cues collected on Cases 7 and 8 and 10 teachers . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 117 13 Number of diagnostic judgments for Cases 7 and 8 made by 10 teachers . . . . . . . . . . . . 119 14 Correlations among length of interaction, number of cues collected and number of diagnostic judgments for Cases 7 and 8 and 10 teachers . 120 vi Table 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 Number of verbatim comments, mean cue percent time and verbatim percent time for Cases 7 and 8 and 5 teachers . . . . . . . . . . . . Twenty-eight most frequently mentioned diagnostic categories in the classroom (Total of 96) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Number of diagnostic categories mentioned by teachers in laboratory and classroom Situations O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O 0 Most frequently mentioned diagnostic categories in laboratory and classroom settings . . . . Most frequently mentioned diagnostic categories for 1977 observational study (8 clinicians, 4 cases)* . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Most frequently mentioned diagnostic categories for Cases 7 and 8 and 10 teachers* . . . . . Diagnostic and commonality scores for the classroom teachers study and the 1977 study. vii Page 125 131 136 139 163 164 165 LIST OF FIGURES Figure Page 1 Percent of diagnostic categories for Cases 7 and 8 and 10 teachers . . . . . . . 100 2 Percent of all cues collected for Cases 7 and 8 by 10 teachers . . . . . . . . . . . 104 3 Cue to statements relationships . . . . . . . 127 4 Percent of the diagnostic categories mentioned by teachers in the classroom . . . . . . . 133 viii CHAPTER I THE PROBLEM Introduction Many of today's students have reading deficiencies, a problem which is of major concern among educators (Satz, 1977). Many children, taught under a skill-model, seem to have failed to acquire some basic reading technique(s) in the early grades: Among those are word identification skills, phonics and structural analysis, comprehension, etc. The sources of the problems, educators claim, are multiple. Because classroom reading instruction is deve10p- mental (Austin, 1968; Carter & McGinnis, 1970, p. 28), reading deficiencies, if uncorrected, may become pro- gressively worse, and might interfere with general learning. Wilson (1977) maintains that "inability to read coupled with the lack of desire to read, leads directly to school failure" (p. 1). Thus, if the school teaches the child anything, it is what failure means (Smith, Carter & Dapper, 1970, p. 4). Furthermore, the fear of failure becomes more intense as the child grows older (Reich, 1962) and this, in turn, might result in a number of emotional problems: tension, anxiety, frustration, short attention span, and inability to do independent work (Smith, Carter & Dapper, 1970). These 1 2 problems may impair personal development and behavior (Whitcombe, 1976; Medrano, 1977). Thus, children with reading difficulties do not perform at their potential level of effectiveness, since every phase of their academic and personal development is directly or indirectly affected by their reading problems (Sherman, 1968). Classroom teachers must be able to identify reading difficulties early and to diagnose and treat them. Early identification and correction may prevent problems from becoming more complex and, hence, more difficult to treat (Bond & Tinker, 1967). Early treatment might also prevent many of the learning and emotional problems that accompany reading deficiencies. Unfortunately, a number of reading consultants believe that, at present, the retarded reader is not identified and treated or referred to help early enough (Carter & McGinnis, 1970). Classroom teachers, some researchers feel, seem to lack the basic clinical skills necessary to engage in these preventive processes (Adams, 1971; Stephens, 1978). This is a serious problem, because, according to some educators, "educational adjustment alone cannot correct a problem which has been ignored for a long time to which there is a severe emotional problem" (Wilson, 1977, p. 65). If diagnosis and remediation of reading difficulties are fundamental aspects of reading instruction, there is a need to improve the diagnostic and remediation skills of classroom teachers. For this to be done, one must first 3 study the diagnostic and remediation skills which teachers currently have. The Problem The purpose of this study was to investigate the clinical problem solving behavior of classroom teachers as they diagnosed children's reading problems, in both experi- mental and classroom situations. In other words, it was intended to examine how teachers gather data about children with reading difficulties and how they diagnose those difficulties. In a recent paper about instructional practices in reading,Pearson and Kamil (1978) argue that the techniques teachers use for instruction are determined largely by the models of reading they follow. That is, a teacher might emphasize print translation or context and memory, or both, depending on the model he/she‘prefers. The model, or theo- retical conceptualizations of the reading process, further influences which sub-reading skills, -- such as decoding, getting meaning from the printed page, etc., -- are stressed. Models also suggest certain diagnostic strategies. For example, those models which "teach to weaknesses" and empha- size sub-skills, sequencing and automaticity suggest the following diagnostic steps: "(1) define a set of objectives, one for each skill or interest, (2) place them in a logical sequence, (3) write a test for each objective, (4) administer the test to a group, (5) examine skill profiles, looking for peaks and valleys, (6) beginning with the earliest skill in the sequence, remediate all the weaknesses" (Pearson & Kamil, 1978, p. 20) 4 Those models which focus on meaning and "teaching to strengths", on the other hand, suggest these diagnostic steps: "(1) determine the components of the reading process in which a student demonstrates proficiency, and (2) capitalizing on those strengths, help the student learn how to generate meaning, hypotheses, gathering whatever data he needs to confirm or dis— confirm those hypotheses: (Ibid. p. 20) An integral part of a diagnosis, regardless of the strategies used, is the teacher's cognitive and diagnostic processes; e.g., how he/she processes the information he/she has collected to reach diagnostic and remedial decisions about the child's problems. As the review of the Literature section will show, these cognitive processes, collectively referred to as "clinical problem solving behavior”, have not yet been researched; hence there is a clear need for studies such as this one. This study builds upon research in progress of the Institute for Research on Teaching (IRT) at Michigan State University. The main thrust of this research, conducted by the Clinical Studies component of the IRT, is the investigation of clinical problem solving behavior in reading diagnosis (Gil gt_al. 1979). Most research efforts to date have concentrated on the clinical problem solving behavior of reading specialists. The present study was intended to broaden the scope of this research by exploring the clinical problem solving behavior of classroom teachers. Research Tasks Investigation of the clinical problem solving behavior followed by teachers in diagnosing reading difficulties have 5 rarely been conducted. For that reason, this study was intended to be, and should be viewed as, exploratory; it did not attempt to pose and test hypotheses, but rather to investigate, probe and describe the diagnostic processes currently employed by teachers. Theypurposes of the study: The first purpose of the study was to investigate the clinical problem solving behavior of classroom teachers as they diagnosed children's reading problems. Toward this end, 10 teachers were asked to interact with two simulated cases of reading difficulties in a laboratory setting. An observational session was held, during which the teachers were instructed to gather information about a case and to verbalize their thoughts as they attempted to reach diag— nostic decisions about the case's problems. During a debriefing session that followed, techniques of stimulus recall were used to stimulate the teachers' memory of how they attempted to diagnose the case. Among the research questions posed were: I 1. What are the diagnostic judgments most frequently mentioned by teachers about a given case? 2. To what degree do teachers arrive at similar diagnostic judgments about a child's reading problems? 3. Do teachers spend similar amounts of time diagnosing a case of reading difficulty? 4. What are the most frequently collected cues about a given case? 6 5. What kinds of relationships exist between cues teachers collect and the teachers' verbatim comments about these cues and the case? These questions (particularly No. 1,2, and 4) are based, in part, on an Agreement Corollary derived from a theory of clinical problem solving behavior named the "Inquiry Theory"(Vinsonhaler, Wagner & Elstein, 1977). The agreement corollary states that, the greater the simi- larity of teachers' "memories", the greater the agreement of their diagnoses. For a complete discussion of the Inquiry Theory and the agreement corollary see Chapter II, pp. 55-62. A second purpose of the study was to explore the similarities between teachers' diagnostic practices in experimental situations and their practices in the natural classroom environments. Interviews were conducted with the 10 teachers in their own classrooms. The interviews were designed to provide information about the teachers' instructional, diagnostic, and remedial practices in the classroom. Using a standardized interview-form, the researcher asked the teachers to match their students with descriptions of reading difficulties (one student to one description), and to describe how they had determined the students' reading problems and what remedial actions they had taken. Specific research questions included: 1. What are the diagnostic categories most frequently mentioned by teachers in the classroom? 2. To what degree do teachers mention similar or different diagnostic categories in laboratory and classroom situations? 7 3. To what degree do teachers formulate similar or different remediation plans across two cases or reading diffi- culties? Significance of the Study Little is known about the effectiveness of class- room teachers in diagnosing and remediating reading difficulties. A review of the literature reveals a lack of empirical research in this area and an absence of descriptions of the teachers' diagnostic processes. The data from this study should add to our under- standing of the clinical problem solving behavior of clinicians. The study should also help researchers plan and execute further research on clinicians' cognitive processes. And, it should help teacher educators seeking methods for training inservice and preservice teachers in diagnostic and remediation skills. Rationale for the Study Many of today's students have reading deficiencies which may lead to severe learning and emotional problems. Early identification is essential to remediate these problems and to prevent them from becoming more serious. Because reading specialists usually treat only the more severe cases of reading difficulties, the task of early identification must be given to classroom teachers, who interact with and observe the students' reading behavior on a continuous basis. This study should serve as a first step toward determining how teachers might be trained for this role. Overview of the Study A review of the literature pertinent to this study is presented in Chapter II. The review focuses on two areas: (1) the nature of reading diagnosis and its implications for classroom teachers, and (2) recent studies of clinical problem solving behavior in reading diagnosis upon which the present study is drawn. Chapter III is devoted to a discussion of the research design and methods of the study. Data, treatment of data, and results are presented in Chapter IV. A summary, conclusions, applications for further research, and implications for teacher education are provided in Chapter V. CHAPTER II REVIEW OF THE LITERATURE Introduction The purpose of this chapter is to review the current literature on reading diagnosis. Two major areas will be discussed: 1) the nature of reading diagnosis and its implications for classroom teaching, and 2) recent studies of clinical problem solving behavior in reading diagnosis conducted by the Clinical Studies Component of the Institute for Research on Teaching at Michigan State University upon which the present study is drawn. Nature of Reading Diagnosis Place of diagnosis in the teaching-learning situation Although there is an abundance of books and papers about reading instruction, not a great deal is yet known about reading problems, their causes and remediation (Smith, Carter & Dapper, 1970, p. l). Nor, it seems, do researchers know enough about the developed skills of the fluent reader (Smith, 1971). The general notion, however, is that reading is a more complex process than was originally believed (Sawyer, 1974). 9 10 Many educators also seem to feel that, since reading is a complex process, the teaching of reading, reading diagnosis and remediation are all parts of one interrelated process. The section that follows con- centrates on the diagnostic aspect of reading; specific attention will be given to the importance of diagnosis, definitions of the process, and its effectiveness, as suggested by the literature. Importance of reading diagnosis Review of the literature reveals that most educators consider the diagnosis of reading performance an essential and integral part of reading instruction, and a basis of all efficient teaching (Otto, McMenemy & Smith, 1973). Moreover, because the reading process appears to be com- plicated and reading failure may be attributed to many compounding factors (Satz, 1977), some educators acknowledge the need for an interdisciplinary approach to diagnosis (and to research on reading in general) (Weintraub & Farr, 1976). This interdisciplinary approach, they argue, should focus attention on the individual child and his or her learning problems, and should be carried out by professionals from various fields (e.g. psychiatrists, psychologists) and educators who occupy various positions within the school system (administrators, principals, reading consultants, and classroom teachers) (Kress, 1965: Smith, Carter & Dapper, 1970; Hollingsworth, 1970; C.W. Peters, 1977). The diagnostic process, some maintain, should start in the 11 classroom, because "diagnosis is an essential aspect of teaching and a preliminary step to sound instruction" (Sheldon 1968). Therefore, it is argued that teachers be "armed with the diagnostic and remedial techniques necessary to instruct students as effectively as possible" (Wilson, 1977, p. 1). Thus, the teacher is viewed not only as reading-instructor, but also as diagnostician, who con- stantly studies the reading strengths and weaknesses of students in order to improve instruction (Bond & Tinker, 1967). Why do so many educators view the diagnostic act in reading instruction as so important? Furthermore, why do they place such heavy emphasis on classroom diagnosis (rather than leave the diagnosis to the reading specialist)? Review of the literature suggests several reasons: First, there is an increasing number of students with reading difficulties, on the one hand, and a small number of reading specialists, on the other, making it impossible to refer all children with difficulties to specialists. Therefore, it is important that classroom teachers develop the necessary diagnostic and remediation skills to detect and help children with reading difficulties, and thus reduce their number (Gallant, 1970); specialists will be able to handle the number of students referred to them only if the classroom teacher assumes major responsibilities in diagnosis (Wilson, 1977). 12 Second, it is essential that reading difficulties be detected and corrected as early as possible (Sheldon, 1965), so that they will not become more complex (Bond & Tinker, 1967: McCarthy, 1971; Rabinovitch, 1965). The basic assumption here is that "the earlier the problems are discovered, the more hope there is for conquering them" (Smith, 1969, p. 15). Third, there is a notion that reading is an important enough skill that every phase of the academic career of the poor reader, and his adjustment to his environment and peer-group is directly or indirectly affected by defective reading ability (Sherman, 1968; Stevens, 1971); if this is so, diagnosis and remediation of reading difficulties can enhance a child's concept of himself. Fourth, reading diagnosis should help teachers plan and modify instruction to meet the needs of individual students (Austin, 1965: Bond & Tinker, 1967: Dauzat, 1977; Dietrich, 1972: Farr, 1971: Karlsen, 1976; Olson & Dillner, 1976; Sawyer, 1968; Swalm, 1973). The assumption underlying the arguments for classroom reading diagnosis (rather than clinical diagnosis) is that since classroom teachers have relatively long and extensive acquaintances with their students, they are in an Optimal position to diagnose the students' reading performance through observations and informal tests (Wilson, 1977). 13 Efficacy of diagnosis Although the prevailing assumption throughout the literature is that reading diagnosis is an essential part of instruction and remediation (e.g., "individualized diagnosis leading to prescriptive treatment is a widely accepted and acclaimed approach to reading disorders" [Bateman, 1971]), the literature largely ignores two important considerations: first, whether early and accurate diagnosis actually promotes effective remediation, and second, whether the remediation does, in fact, help the retarded reader. If it does not, one must ask, of what value is the diagnosis? Spache (1976) maintains that diagnosis is not always relevant to remediation, and attributes this problem to two conditions: (1) "the presence of biased or prejudiced thinking resulting in limited diagnostic efforts or in stereotyped remedial programs," and (2) "the differing emphases in the training of clinicians, who are apt to do the diagnosis, and of remedial or classroom teachers, who are prominent in remedial work" (p. 436). In other words, Space claims that the procedures used in remediation efforts are not directly related to the detailed diagnostic findings. In many cases, he says, it appears that the two processes are carried out by different personnel who do not communicate sufficiently with each other. He recommends that treatment procedures be more closely related to diagnostic findings. 14 Theoretical work dealing with the associations between diagnosis and remediaton is not the only thing missing from the literature; there is also an obvious lack of empirical research in this area. Bateman (1971) states that a detailed diagnosis of a student's reading performance should yield data on what she/he needs to be taught, and how she/he should be taught. "The question of whether diagnosis makes a difference," Bateman adds, "must be rephrased in terms of which diagnostic data make a difference for what purpose." She notes that there are conflicting Opinions about the efficacy of diagnosis and little conclusive research to clarify this issue. Discussing the value of diagnosis to the remedial act, Della-Piana (1968) cites three studies, conducted over a period of 33 years, which provide data about the relationship between diagnosis and remediation. The first study, by Monroe (1932), dealt with procedures for systematic diagnosis leading to specific remedial acts. Thorough diagnoses were conducted with children who exhibited specific reading deficiencies. Taking into con- sideration the students' age and I.Q., Monroe compared their reading achievement with their skills in other areas, such as math and spelling. Educational profiles were created for each child which assisted the researcher “in understanding the child's problem of maladjustment by indicating the magnitude of the discrepancy or lack of harmony between his accomplishments" (p. 33). Results showed that remediation 15 based on such an extensive diagnosis yielded significantly greater gains than ordinary school instruction. The direct associations, however, between the thorough diagnosis and the remedial gains were questioned by Monroe herself. The educational profile, she said, "does not help directly in understanding the causative factors involved in the defect,...it does not assist directly in the selection of remedial methods, although it indicates in some cases discrepancies between the reading skills which offer a point of attack. The reading index sets a goal toward which to work in applying remedial instruction and gives a satisfactory means of measuring the improvement brought about by the corrective work" (p. 33). The second study, by Robinson (1946), demonstrated that thorough diagnosis, followed by remediation, can help many "unteachable" children learn to read. Diagnoses were conducted independently by professionals from various disciplines -- social workers, psychologists, pediatricians, neurologists, etc. These specialists then met to evaluate and plan for remediation. Reading deficiencies had been caused by multiple factors, Robinson found, and the thorough diagnoses helped many of the children learn to read. Although the diagnostic process in this study appeared to be highly valuable, the question remained "whether similar gains could be obtained by small-group teaching, without expensive diagnosis" (Della-Piana, 1968). 16 A third study, conducted by Glad (1965), provided additional data about the value of diagnosis to subsequent remediation. Results indicated that students who went through intensive diagnosis showed an average gain of 1.4 on an oral reading test, compared with a preremedial yearly average gain of 0.5. Taken together, these three studies still "leave open the question as to exactly what role diagnosis plays in obtaining reading gains" (Della-Piana, 1968, p. 2). In each study it was unclear whether the thorough diagnosis was the main determinant which effected the high gains. Many writers believe that diagnosis is necessary to avoid "standardized remediation." The diagnosis, they say, should be oriented toward the student's specific reading problems. However, recent review of the literature on the association between diagnosis and remediation, from 1900 to 1979 (Weinshank, in preparation), reveals that, in general, diagnosis and remediation are far from interrelate and integrate. Some empirical data provided by Weinshank supports this claim. A study examining the relationships between diagnostic decisions made by reading clinicians and their remedial plans concluded that the diagnoses and remediations were only modestly correlated (Weinshank, 1979). In summary, there is an apparent lack of theoretical discussion and empirical research on the question of whether thorough and accurate diagnosis actually determines the 17 effectiveness of the remediation. It seems logical to believe that the stronger the link between diagnosis and effective remediaton, the higher the value that can be attached to the diagnostic act. To enhance the likelihood that diagnosis will promote successful remediation, at least three conditions should be met. First, the diagnosis must be reliable. A reading specialist working with a particular case of reading difficulties should be able to reach similar diagnostic judgments at two different times. One must be aware, how- ever, that even if a reading specialist is consistent in his/her diagnoses, it may be that he/she has consistently reached the same faulty conclusions. For that reason, it is preferable that at least two specialists independently diagnose the same case. If both arrive at similar diagnostic judgments, it is likely that their diagnoses are reliable and accurate. A second condition under which diagnosis is important is that both the diagnosis and the remediation be specific. It is not enough for a diagnosis to be reliable, it should also indicate the specific areas in which the child experiences reading difficulties. Specific treatments apprOpriate for the student and his/her cognitive abilities must then be prescribed, rather than some stereotyped treatments recommended for all children and all types of problems. Finally, the remediation must be relevant and efficacious: that is, it should be directed toward those 18 specific reading problems identified during the diagnostic act as the cause of the child's reading difficulties. Remediation will be effective to the degree that it is relevant. Suppose, for example, that the diagnosis reveals that retentive comprehension, one of the four vital signs, is the problemrarea for a particular child [The four vital signs are: (l) instant word recognition; (2) decoded word recognition; (3) fluent message segmentation, and (4) re- tentive comprehension (Sherman, in preparation)]. The remediation would be relevant only if it treated the child's comprehension. The remediation would be irrelevant, and ineffective (although it might still be specific), if it called only for phonic work to treat the problem. The issue of relevancy incorporates another issue, that of time. How long must the remediation be continued for the reading problem to be cured? If the remediation lasts for an extended period, it might not remain relevant, because the child may begin to fall behind in another area of reading. If, on the other hand, the remediation does not last long enough, the problem may remain uncured, and might eventually become as severe as it originally had been. If the conditions of reliability, specificity, and relevancy are met, we can argue that reading diagnosis of reading difficulties is indeed valuable, since it is likely to lead to treatment and cure. 19 The value of reading diagnosis may also be associated with the success or failure of the remedial act. Even when the conditions of reliability, specificity and relevancy exist, if reading remediation does not improve students' reading performance, reading diagnosis may be of no value. If, on the other hand, remediation does show to have effects on students' reading performance, then reading diagnosis, leading to a differentiated (rather than stereotyped)remediation, is important. The question of whether remedial programs result in long-term or permanent gains for the students is discussed by Spache (1976). He claims that there are several flaws in the traditional ways of measuring students' remedial gains; specifically, he says: (1) different reading tests are not comparable, so dual testing yields incomparable data; (2) different reading tests measure different aspects of the reading performance, so inter-tests comparisons are relatively meaningless: (3) measuring the decrease in gap between achievement level and mental age is also relatively meaningless, because intelligence and reading achievement are not parallel attributes: (4) scores from pre and post tests, used to evaluate short-term remedial reading programs, are faulty because of the phenomenon of regression to the mean. In summary, Spache argues, standardized tests that evaluate remedial outcomes are very narrow and are not reliable. 20 In a subsequent discussion of the factors that affect the outcomes of remediation, Spache comes to the following conclusions: 1. There does not appear to be any specific amount of time needed for remediation. What is most important is the number of contact hours with the student, which may continue even after the treatment has ceased; The manner in which remediation is conducted -- whether on an individual basis or with a small group (5-6 students) -- does not really matter, because gains are related directly to the number of contact hours with the student and progress is more readily stimulated by frequent sessions: The student's initial reading level when remediation begins is positively related to gains from remediation. Those students with the highest pre-remediation test scores tend to achieve the highest post-training scores; Remedial work with elementary school students can result in significant, short-term gains, whereas secondary school students may show greater long-term profit: The personality adjustment of the retarded reader is a significant aspect of his/her problem and may affect his/her remedial gains. Little is known, however, about the relationships between personality characteristics of the retarded reader and permanent reading gain scores. 21 Discussion of the factors affecting remedial outcomes leads to an important question: What are the long-term outcomes of remediation? After reviewing 25 follow-up studies, Spache (1976) concluded that "remedial treatment apparently does not affect school progress appreciably, over time" (p. 336). Of 14 studies in which reading tests were administered as part of a long-term follow-up, 12 indicated no continued growth after treatment, and only two showed that retarded readers were functioning at levels normal for their age—grade status. The latter two studies involved secondary school students and college students, which is consistent with Spache's conclusion that secondary students tend to show greater long-term profit than primary grade students. Overall, Spache concludes that, in general, follow-up studies indicate little or no further development of reading ability as compared to the accelerated rate during the treatment. One of the studies, for example (Buerger, 1968), revealed that "pupils who receive remedial reading instruction demonstrated significant immediate post remedial reading gains", but "did not make greater long-term educational progress than a similar control group". Buerger's conclusion was that "reading disability is, for the most part, a chronic condition needing long-term treatment. What is needed after a rather intensive remedial period is provision for supportive reading assistance during this follow-up period." 22 Both Buerger and Spache maintain that, although remedial reading temporarily relieves reading problems, there is little evidence of long-term gains from single remedial treatments. The "chronic condition" mentioned by Buerger, and resultant need for continuous and supportive reading assistance, warrant further discussion. They depict a situation that may be analogous to that in medicine, where we witness two kinds of problems: chronic and acute. Whereas acute problems, such as appendicitis, can be treated and remediated in a single act, many chronic problems, such as allergy, can not be remediated for years, or even in a life—time. It may well be that in reading, we can establish similar distinctions between chronic and acute problems. Some problems, such as poor use of phonics in contextual reading, might be acute, while others may be chronic, such as poor retentive comprehension resulting from poor cognitive linguistic structures or low interest in reading. The possibility that chronic reading problems exist, coupled with the evidence of minimal long-term effects of single remedial treatments, emphasizes the need for continued "health-care" in the area of reading diagnosis and remediation. This, in turn, suggests several questions about the manner in which reading problems are currently treated and remediated: What should the role of the reading specialist be? Can reading specialists continue to work with children on an individual basis and for short periods of time only? 23 How skilled should classroom teachers be to conduct reading diagnosis and remediation on a continuous basis in their classrooms (rather than referring children for short-term clinical work in a clinic setting)? To what degree should the parents be involved in the remedial act? Further research is needed to determine the proportion of acute and chronic problems in reading. Such research, consisting mainly of follow-up studies, should also investigate some of the questions raised above. In summary, the literature on reading diagnosis emphasizes the importance of the diagnostic act for the retarded reader. It fails, however, to provide theoretical discussion and empirical data which deal with the association between diagnosis and remediation and which demonstrate whether thorough and accurate diagnosis leads to more effective remediation. The literature is also inconclusive about the long-term effects of remediation. Much research is needed to investigate these two issues. Ways of defining reading diagnosis The terms "diagnosis of reading" and "reading diagnosis" are defined in a number of ways in the literature: there does not appear to be a universal definition. The term "diagnosis" is frequently equated with "evaluation" and "assessment" (Bamman, 1970; Harris & Sipay, 1975; Harris & Smith, 1976), where all these terms are taken to mean: identification and measurement of the 24 students' level of performance in reading. Even when the term "diagnosis" itself is used, it is frequently interpreted in different ways. The literature reveals that some educators view diagnosis as a process, while others see it as a product. Furthermore, some are concerned with the student's general reading status, while others focus primarily on the student's weaknesses which need remediation. The definition of diagnosis as a process emphasizes the sequence of tasks the teacher performs to determine a child's reading performance. Wilson (1977), for example, states that "diagnosis implies that the educator will actively search for clues to assist in evaluating the present state of the reader's skill development" (p. 19). Spache (1976) defines diagnosis as a continuous process of prOposing hypotheses about a child's performance and testing those hypotheses (p. 9). Guszak (1972) views the process as a sequence of relatively simple determinations of a pupil's reading achievement level, his/her achievement potential, and his/her prominent skill needs. Peters, N.A. (1977) defines diagnosis as an "interrelated process through which a teacher attempts to ascertain the specific strengths and weaknesses of individual students". Similar definitions are offered by others who view diagnosis as a process, but who emphasize the child's weaknesses. For Harris (1972) diagnosis is "nothing more than the application of a straightforward, common sense, problem-solving approach to the study of children who have 25 difficulties in reading" (p. 403). The purpose of this problem solving approach, says Harris, is "to find out what is wrong, what caused the difficulty, and what can be done for it". Similarly, Strang (1964) states that "diagnosis puts more emphasis on defining the nature of the individual's reading difficulties and the conditions causing them". Strang notes, however, that diagnosis is also concerned with those positive factors of the child's reading on which one can build a program for remediation. When diagnosis is described as a product -- as the explanation or description of the child's reading performance -- the description usually focuses on the child's weaknesses and needs; that is, diagnosis is seen as a systematic and rationale explanation of an individual's inability to read (Carter, 1970; Carter & McGinnis, 1970). Most of these definitions suggest some characteristics of a "good" diagnosis. At the very least, to be effective, diagnosis cannot be a one-time operation. Rather, it should be a continuous process (Otto et 31., 1973) which will be combined with instruction and improve reading through the reinforcement of the child's strengths and remediation of his/her weaknesses (Strang, 1964). This continuous process is interwoven with treatment (Smith, 1969: Smith, Carter & Dapper, 1970) and is designed to evaluate students very carefully before instruction begins, and on a regular basis (Sheldon, 1968; Wilson, 1977). The process of diagnosis and remediation should be recycled, as 26 long as the child fails to respond to the remedial instruction (Bond & Tinker, 1967). What seems to be missing from the various definitions of diagnosis are (1) the specific factors teachers and reading specialists should be looking for when they try to diagnose a child's reading problems, and (2) the mental tasks performed by the teacher or the reading specialist as they determine the child's reading performance. To say that diagnosis is "problem solving" or a "rationale explanation" does not explain how teachers or reading specialists solve the problem -— how they arrive at apprOpriate judgments and explanations of the child's reading difficulties. More attention must be given to the manner in which teachers process information they have obtained through observations and the use of various tests. Levels of reading diagnosis As we have seen, the term "diagnosis" or "reading diagnosis" is not yet defined in a way that enables one to thoroughly understand the skills involved in the process. Furthermore, the definitions of this term do not draw a distinction between two different kinds of diagnosis -- classroom diagnosis and clinic diagnosis. Rather, review of the literature reveals that educators prefer to talk about levels of diagnosis, which correspond to reading difficulties of different severity. Wilson (1977) describes three types of diagnosis: informal on-the-spot: classroom, and clinical. 27 Informal on-the-spot is, as the name suggests, an informal diagnosis of a student's reading status based on teacher's questions or exercises. It is followed immediately by adjustment of instruction; this immediacy may prevent serious reading problems from developing. Classroom diagnosis is a more formal and planned process which is carried on by the classroom teacher if the adjustments made after the informal diagnosis do not solve the child's problem. The teacher gathers more information by observing the student as he/she reads and through the use of a more specific test of skills. After careful evaluation of this information, the teacher applies the findings to the instructional situation. Clinical diagnosis is conducted by a reading specialist outside the classroom. The specialist takes precise measures of the student's reading skills and potential through the use of precise testing and evaluation instruments. Smith 33 a1. (1970) list four levels of diagnosis, identified according to the persons who carry them out: classroom teacher, reading specialist, psychologist and clinician. Classroom teachers usually have the training necessary to observe their students' strengths and weaknesses. They can use a group test to gain information about the students‘ reading performance and, on the basis of that information, make immediate instructional adjustments to 28 build the strengths and overcome the weaknesses. Classroom teachers can also make informal observations of the students' attitudes, willingness to participate in class, articulation and speaking vocabulary. Reading specialists can analyze the reading process more thoroughly. Using individual diagnostic procedures, they can measure the gap between a student's achievement and ability and suggest specific treatments to overcome whatever weaknesses there might be. Psychologists and clinicians are called in when it is necessary to go beyond the student's observable strengths and weaknesses and beyond measures of the child's reading level. In such cases, mental and physiological tests might be necessary. While Wilson and Smith 35 El: differentiate between classroom diagnosis and clinical diagnosis, most educators do not make this kind of distinction. Rather, they prefer to classify diagnosis according to levels of specificity and, in so doing, they seem to suggest that there is no clear-cut distinction between classroom and clinic diagnosis. Bond and Tinker (1967) describe three levels of diagnosis: general, analytical and case-study. While many children's reading problems need only a general diagnosis, others, few in number, may need an analytical or case-study diagnosis. General diagnosis is designed "to identify children who are doing relatively poor work in reading as compared with their other achievements" (p. 155). The purposes of 29 this kind of diagnosis are to gain the information necessary to adjust instruction to the needs of individual students and to locate those students whose problems require more detailed analysis. This information is obtained through the use of group tests and other sources of information available to the teacher. Analytical diagnosis is a systematic exploration of specific reading strengths and weaknesses. It is intended to locate specific areas of reading difficulties. Case-study diagnosis is used to deal with those students whose reading difficulties have not been adequately diagnosed by the general and analytical approaches. Under this approach, the diagnostician examines the student's reading potential as well as other physiological and psychological characteristics. Like Bond and Tinker, Kennedy (1971) has identified three levels of diagnosis: group, individual, and clinical. Group diagnosis is intended to identify students who have reading difficulties and need corrective instruction. In this approach, students are grouped according to age or grade level and given a series of mental, achievement, and diagnostic group tests. This is the most elementary level of diagnosis and, according to Kennedy, is used extensively by classroom teachers. Individual diagnosis is designed for those students "who are very slow or those suspected of having problems too difficult to treat in a regular classroom" (p. 102). Here, students take individual tests administered by reading specialists. 30 Clinical diagnosis is a very thorough and compre- hensive process, conducted by educational diagnosticians with the aid of other specialists, such as physicians, psychologists, eye and hearing specialists, etc. At this level, the diagnosis is intended to provide clues to the causes of the reading problems. Strang (1968; 1969) and Carter and McGinnis (1970) have also categorized diagnostic sequences according to levels of complexity. Strang has identified seven such levels, beginning with description of reading performance by the classroom teacher (Level I) and ending with intro- spective reports (Level VII), in which the reader is asked to describe the processes he uses in reading selected articles. The five intermediate levels include finding clues to different aspects of the student's behavior that influence his/her reading performance (Level II); analysis of the student's reading process (Level III): identification of the student's mental abilities which underlie success in reading (such as visual memory and association) (Level IV): analysis of personality traits and values (Level V), and neurological examinations (Level VI). Carter and McGinnis (1970) list four levels of diagnosis: Level I -- identification of the disability, Level II -- more detailed description of the problem; Level III -- identification of the individual's reading needs, and Level IV -- determination of causal factors: the determination of why the individual does what he does. 31 Otto gt_al. (1973) feel that the first two levels of diagnosis described by Carter and McGinnis amount to a single step, and so they suggest that the diagnostic process consists of three levels: survey, specific and intensive. Survey diagnosis is carried out by the classroom teacher and is designed to uncover the strengths and weaknesses of the class as a whole, and to identify individual students who seem to have special instructional needs. This level of diagnosis involves the use of class-wide testing and evaluation and attempts to evaluate the success of the developmental teaching program. Specific level diagnosis is intended to uncover the reading difficulties of individual students who were identified at the survey level. This kind of diagnosis is carried out by either the classroom teacher or a reading specialist, depending on time, facilities, complexity of the difficulty, and the classroom teacher's proficiency in the use of diagnostic techniques. Intensive level diagnosis is used for those individuals with the most severe reading difficulties; it is designed to discover the basic problems which impede learning. A complete case-study, usually carried on by a reading specialist, either in the school or in a clinical setting, is done to gain a general understanding of the student and his/her needs. Monroe (1968) divides the diagnostic process into two types of analysis: descriptive, in which the examiner tries to describe as completely as possible the nature of the 32 student's reading difficulty, and causative, in which the examiner tries to understand the factors that have created this difficulty. In all of these hierarchical definitions of diagnosis, it is unclear where, exactly, the line falls between class- room and clinical diagnosis and where the clinician starts to diagnose. A careful look reveals that, usually, the lowest levels of diagnosis are carried on by teachers in a classroom setting, while the highest levels call for the services of reading specialists in a clinical setting. What seems to differentiate classroom from clinical diagnosis, then, is the thoroughness of the diagnosis and some of the diagnostic techniques employed. Whereas classroom diagnosis is based primarily on observations and informal testing procedures, clinical diagnosis tends to emphasize more formal and extensive testing (Wilson, 1977). Whereas classroom diagnosis leads to adjustment of instruction, clinical diagnosis results in a detailed description of the child's reading problems and a list of recommendations for remediation; and, finally, whereas classroom diagnosis is a continuous process, clinical diagnosis is a one-time assessment. Another deficiency of the literature is that it does not deal with the question of how different environmental conditions influence reading and reading problems. A child who experiences difficulties in the classroom setting may exhibit totally different behavior when sent to a reading clinic. The problem then becomes one of diagnosing in one 33 context area (clinic) difficulties that are exhibited in and affected by a totally different context area (classroom). Related issue which also lacks sufficient discussion, is that of the remoteness of the remediation from the diagnosis. Not only can the problems observed in a class- room setting be different from those exhibited in a clinical setting: it may well be that the remediation does not correspond to the diagnosis. This is especially likely when a teacher plans a remediation on the basis of a diagnosis made in a clinic (by a reading specialist) or, even more remote, by a psychologist from outside the school. Nature of reading diagnosis: summary The literature pertaining to reading diagnosis deals with three major issues: The purpose of diagnosis (Why), its content (What) and its methods (How). Table 1 describes this domain of diagnosis in greater detail. The purpose of reading diagnosis is threefold; it is (l) necessary for the early identification of reading problems; (2) a prerequisite for remediation, and (3) essential for planning, modification, and individualization of instruction. The content of such a diagnosis focuses on two areas: skill performance and causes of reading difficulties and, accordingly, involves two activities: (1) description of skill performance, and (2) determination of causality. The methods involve a continuous process of diagnosis and the employment of one of three levels of diagnosis corresponding to the severity 34 nomoummm mumcwamwomwcumucH .oa Ammmsmo mo mEHmu cwv Damaommm mpsum wmm01m>flmcmucH Hmscw>wccH scaumucmwuo OHCAHU Acowumfluomoc mo mEHmu may owmwommm HmowuhamCMIHmfiuom HospH>H©cH cofiumacwfluo Hoonom Hmumcoo HmEuomcH macaw COH¥MHCOHHO EOOHm WMHU mmoooum mooscaucoo mwozwdHQ 30m coauosnumcw mo CQADMNHHMSUM>Hpr can coaumowmapoa muflHMmsmo mo coflumcfieumuoa .m .mcwccmam How Hmwucmmmm .m ucoEm>ownom mam Hofiucmuom cowumacwsmu How cmo3umn hocmmmuomwc An ouflmwswmuoum mummmoomz .N mocmEHOMme mEmHnoum mcflpmmu mo Hawxm mo cowumfiuomoa Am .v coaumowmwucmcw hanmm .H mHmozwHmcm£mHmEoo «1|.H mamaa 35 of the problem: (a) classroom, (b) school, or (c) clinic. It may well be that an effective model of reading diagnosis should incorporate all of these elements. Competence of classroom teachers as diagnosticians Something which seems to be missing from the literature is a discussion of how an effective diagnosis and remediation should be conducted. Different educators express different opinions about the purpose, content and methods of diagnosis, but their writings tend to be very general and do not describe how information (Table 1) should be put together to make decisions about individual students. Although there appears to be no comprehensive description of diagnosis -- of how to process information after is is collected -- many educational authorities believe that teachers are qualified to diagnose students' reading difficulties. They consider classroom diagnosis an integrative part of effective teaching: in fact, some go so far as to suggest that all content-area teachers should be involved in reading diagnosis (Dupuis and Askov, 1977: Niles, 1965). It might be remembered too, that some of the diagnostic levels described in the literature are intended to be carried out by the classroom teacher. The underlying assumption of the above is, that classroom teachers are, indeed, capable of diagnosing students' reading difficulties. 36 Smith (1969) and Smith 33 a1. (1970) identify two major objectives of reading diagnosis conducted by the classroom teacher: (1) determination of the appropriate instructional level for a given student, and (2) deter- mination of the specific skills the student lacks. The more capable the classroom teacher is of accomplishing these objectives, the smaller the number of students will be who need remedial reading instruction. However, as Smith (1969) says, "part of the classroom teacher's job is to know when a disabled reader needs help beyond that offered in the classroom" (p. 20). McGinnis (1970) believes that the classroom teacher "can meet the reading needs of 92 percent of the school population. (The teacher) can observe the child in the classroom environment, can identify his problems, and can help him solve them." Harris (1972) states that "children who have slight to moderate reading disabilities are generally able to be helped considerably by the classroom teacher" (p. 400). To be a diagnostic teacher, Harris (1977) claims, the teacher must (1) know the scope and sequence of the total reading program: (2) be familiar with the materials which foster independent learning; (3) have the support of administrators or supervisors, and (4) be skilled in analyzing pupil needs, organizing the class for differentiated instruction, keeping records, and motivating pupils. Basically, Harris (1974) argues, the reading teacher can be a diagnostician "if the 37 term diagnostician is defined apprOpriately, and if the teacher has the knowledge, the facilities and the skills necessary to make diagnostic teaching work." Ekwall (1976, 1977), who also feels teachers are capable of diagnosing reading difficulties in the classroom, offers some diagnostic guidelines for teachers who work with disabled readers (at any given grade level). These guide- lines are: 1. Gather that amount of initial diagnostic information which falls somewhere between the position that it is good to do a great deal of diagnosis before remediation begins, and the contrast position that it is better to do only enough initial diagnosis to initiate flexible remediation; 2. When deciding upon tests to administer to children, consider each student in terms of the type of problem he/she exhibits and select the tests which correspond to this problem; 3. Administer the tests in a situation that is similar to the actual reading situation. 4. Do not attempt to diagnose those factors for which it is not possible to provide remediation or for which remediation has not proven effective in the past, and 5. Diagnosis for a seriously disabled reader should involve more than an appraisal of educational factors. Spache (1960) thinks it is possible to conduct clinical diagnosis in the classroom. He defines the 38 retarded reader as an individual who (1) is behind in a number Of reading skills by one year or more, if in the primary grades, or by two years of more if older, (2) is below the reading level necessary for full participation in the reading tasks of his/her peer-group; (3) has had normal Opportunities for schooling, and (4) has continued to show this degree Of retardation despite corrective efforts over a period of months. Spache argues that the classroom teacher can apply this definition to identify those pupils who have reading problems, and then administer group tests of reading and intelligence to distinguish severely retarded readers from those needing moderate corrective treatments; he also claims that the teacher has the capacity to take further diagnostic steps with those children needing careful diagnosis. These steps include assessment of physical factors, personality, and sociological factors; choice of teaching approach; and administration Of informal tests. In a similar vein, Austin (1965) and Bond (1970) state that classroom teaching should be based on diagnostic teaching. More specifically, they suggest that teaching should be based on an understanding of the reading strengths and needs of each child. This knowledge makes it possible for teachers to adjust their teaching and to ”make modifications to correct any confusion before it becomes seriously limiting to a child's future growth in reading" (Bond, 1970). If such modifications were made, only the more severely disabled readers would need more careful diagnostic treatment, which has usually been 39 provided outsine the classroom setting. Otto, McMenemy and Smith (1973) believe that teachers are competent enough to conduct diagnoses in the day-to-day classroom setting and to take the steps required for effective corrective instruction. Moreover, they argue, the competent teacher usually knows how far he can go in diagnosing students' reading problems and when he/she should refer them to a specialist. Similarly, Kennedy (1971) suggests that group diagnosis (Level I) which is "conducted by classroom teachers, can uncover many of the more common reading deficiencies" (p. 103). He goes so far as to state that "identifying students who need help in reading is the concern of classroom teachers; it is not a function Of the remedial reading staff" (p. 73). Because early identification and treatment can prevent reading problems from becoming worse, Kennedy adds, "it is...necessary that each teacher of reading be prepared to administer a preliminary diagnosis of their children's reading and be able to present simple corrective measures which will eliminate many minor and some major problems" (p. 545). Smith gt El. (1970), maintain that most classroom teachers have the necessary training to Observe the strengths and weaknesses of students in the areas of vocabulary, word recognition and comprehension. Consequently, these educators say, teachers occupy the pivotal position for discovering reading difficulties, treating them, Or referring the students to reading specialists. Wilson also claims that the classroom teacher is in the best 40 position to first notice students' potential problem areas (Wilson, 1977). Hence, as Smith points out, the more able the classroom teacher is in preliminary diagnosis and implementing corrective instruction, the fewer the students who will need remedial instruction. Accordingly, attempts have been made by practitioners to identify teaching skills which seem essential for diagnosing reading difficulties (Blount, 1973) and to disseminate this knowledge to teachers (Florida Department Of Education, 1977). As this discussion suggests, for diagnosis to make a positive contribution to the child, teachers must assume the responsibility for conducting informal diagnoses in the classroom setting (Dauzat, 1977; Jan-Tausch, 1971) and accept the notion that identification and correction of reading problems is an important aspect Of the teaching job (Smith gt gt. 1970). It must be remembered, of course, that a diagnosis made by a classroom teacher is different in its aims and in its depth from one carried on by a reading specialist who interacts with a child on a one-to-one basis (Harris, 1953). For teachers, to carry out their diagnostic responsibilities, they must have access to rapidly implemented diagnostic procedures which yield accurate data (Dauzat, 1977). Research on diagnostic skills of classroom teachers Despite the many statments about the teacher's ability to conduct diagnoses, research on teachers' diag- nostic skills is still lacking. Acknowledging the need for 41 such research, Burnett (1963, 1970) developed a paper-and- pencil problem solving test in reading to measure teachers' skills in using diagnostic procedures. Burnett's principal task was to construct a measure which would "be valid in terms Of discriminating levels of proficiency at problem solving in the teaching of reading, and would as well hold some promise of providing insight into why individuals differ in proficiency" (1963). The first step Burnett took to establish this validity was to develop problems similar to those encountered in the classroom, problems whose solutions called for operations similar to the Operations used by the problem solver in an actual situation. The test consisted of sets of problems designed to measure teachers' proficiency in the use of diagnostic procedures, at five levels of Operation (1963): (1) selection Of critical information from a pool of data; (2) selection of a means for securing additional data; (3) interpretation Of the data; (4) suggestions on how to improve instruction based on the information collected, and (5) re-evaluation of these suggestions in light of new information provided. At each Of these levels, the examinee was asked to rank four responses in terms Of how well they met the specifications called for in the problem. For example, the examinee may have been asked to rank four explanations Of a child's reading problem on the basis of how well the explanations fit the information that had been presented. 42 Three groups were involved in the study: (1) students enrolled in undergraduate elementary education courses; (2) elementary-school teachers, and (3) reading teachers, consultants, and supervisors who met the International Reading Association's specifications for certification as reading specialists. Results indicated that the reading specialists significantly outscored the teachers at all five levels of Operation. However, Burnett (1970) noted, "a shocking finding was that the performance of experienced teachers in terms of mean scores on the problems was only slightly higher than that of students in undergraduate courses". Burnett acknowledged that paper-and-pencil tests cannot really tell us how effectively teachers might perform in an actual teaching situation. Another shortcoming of these tests is that they did not trace the decision making and thought processes of teachers as they attempt to diagnose children's reading problems. A 1971 study (Weule, 1971) of practices used in the diagnosis of reading difficulties at the primary school level indicated that teachers' diagnostic skills were inadequate. In the study, a questionnaire was distributed to about 350 classroom teachers in New South Wales. The questionnaire was designed to provide descriptions Of reading diagnoses made by these teachers; the information asked for dealt with both stated and actual practice. About 199 of the 350 questionnaires were returned. 43 While asked to consider their own role in diagnosis and treatment, Weule noted, almost all of the teachers said they felt it is "the teacher's duty and responsibility to diagnose and treat reading difficulties. Many underlined the importance of this task. Many placed the teacher above everyone else in this job, saying that he is in the best position to make the decision required...Many stressed the teacher's role in prevention and early identification". Moreover, Weule said, "seventy per cent of the sample thought they could cope with pupils of average ability who are retarded one year or less in reading achievement. Forty per cent also felt capable of providing adequate diagnosis for the seriously retarded, average ability child. Fifteen per cent felt capable of providing adequate diagnosis and treatment for disabled readers, and thirty per cent felt capable of OOping adequately with mentally retarded readers". However, many of the teachers talked about "the disparity between what they want to do and what they could do under existing teaching conditions". Many felt there was no time available to provide individual instruction to pupils who needed it. When asked to rate the relative importance placed on diagnosis and treatment of reading difficulties and other aspects of education and teaching, the teachers indicated that much more emphasis is placed on discipline, pupil-teacher relationships, daily preparation, and so forth. 44 Weule's conclusions were that "teachers appear to be rather poorly equipped to carry out a crucial part of their task as educators. Their relatively poor knowledge of tests,...the poor use of diagnostic techniques all reflect seriously upon the teacher's knowledge of the children he is attempting to teach. At the present moment most diagnostic attempts appear to be inadequate and ineffectual in providing for successful treatment of reading difficulties." More recently, Dupuis and Askov (1977) reported on a Content Area Reading Project which examined classroom diagnosis and which yielded more positive results than the Weule study. The hypotheses examined in the Content Area Project were: (1) that informal diagnosis of reading levels is the responsibility of the teacher, and (2) that teachers can use diagnostic information and teach the necessary reading skills without neglecting their content respon- sibilities. Sixty teachers were instructed to use a Decision Model for Diagnostic Teaching by Grouping, developed by Cartwright, Cartwright and Yssledyke (1973), which called for them to employ a step-by-step planning sequence in the teaching-learning process. The steps were: (1) careful assessment of each student to determine his/her reading ability; (2) specification of teaching goals for each student; (3) grouping of students according to interests, needs, and abilities; (4) selection of instructional strategy and management procedure for each group; 45 (5) selection of instructional materials for each group; (6) implementation of strategy and materials with each group; (7) evaluation of each student's performance. After being instructed on how to use the instructional techniques for carrying out these steps, the teachers were asked to indicate whether they felt the techniques were valuable to them or not. Most of their evaluations were very positive. Moreover, classroom Observations indicated that most teachers did, indeed, use the instructional techniques which they were taught. There are at least two apparent deficiencies with the decision model, however. First, even if teachers are taught to implement instructional techniques, it is unclear how they are-to make the various decisions necessary for diagnosis, e.g. how they should go about assessing the reading ability of each student. In other words, the model tells them what to do, but not how to do it. Second, although classroom observations seem to indicate that teachers do, indeed, use these instructional techniques in the classroom, it does not indicate how well they use them in diagnosing students' reading difficulties. In another recent study Stephens (1978) used a simulated case of reading difficulty to investigate teachers' diagnostic skills. The simulated case was based upon the case history of an eight year-old boy and contained various diagnostic tests usually administered to individual children in a clinic setting. The teachers' task was to 46 interact with the simulated case, to gather and examine information about the child, and to arrive at a diagnostic judgment about his reading problems. The study included 30 teachers divided into three groups of 10 as follows: Group (1) classroom teachers with training -- experienced elementary school teachers with graduate level instruction in the diagnosis and correction of reading difficulties; Group (2) classroom teachers with no training -- experienced elementary school teachers without graduate level instruction in the diagnosis and correction of reading difficulties; and Group (3) Pre-Service Elementary Education teachers -- pre-service teachers with no full time teaching experience or graduate level instruction in the diagnosis and correction of reading difficulties. Data analysis included both product and process measures. The product measures were the final diagnosis made by the teachers; the process measures were selected features relating to the manner in which the problems were diagnosed. The conclusions were that neither graduage level instruction in reading diagnosis nor classroom teaching experience significantly affect teachers' knowledge or strategies of diagnosis. A possible limitation of the study, however, was that the teachers were asked to interact with materials originally developed for the use of reading specialists interacting with children on a one-to-one basis in a clinic situation, and not by 47 classroom teachers in an actual classroom setting. This condition may have had a significant impact on the results. Models of reading diagnosis If, as the literature on reading diagnosis suggests, classroom teachers should be diagnosticians, two elements are needed: (1) models which will more precisely and accurately describe the aspects of a child's reading performance and/or the reading process and the factors which affect reading; and (2) research, based on these models, which investigates the effectiveness of reading diagnosis made by classroom teachers. Presently, in two independent projects, researchers are working on two such models. One is a Model of Diagnosis, develOped by Dr. Rebecca Barr at the University of Chicago; the other is a Model of Reading and Learning to Read developed by Drs. George Sherman and John Vinsonhaler at the Institute for Research on Teaching, Michigan State University. Barr's Model of Diagnosis The model developed by Dr. Barr is one which indicates those aspects of a child's functioning level about which the diagnostician must secure information. Teachers trained under this model are called upon to practice diagnoses based on materials which would be available to them in the classroom for reading instruction and for diagnosing reading problems. The model makes use 48 of Children's natural language patterns in teaching teachers to make decisions about reading performance. The Barr model emphasizes the design and modification of instructional objectives apprOpriate for the child (Barr and Sadow, 1978). Training builds upon the teachers' prior knowledge of reading instruction; it is intended to refine the teachers' concepts and to help them become more effective in their instruction. An important assumption of the model is that teachers do, in fact, possess an ability to process children's natural language, but that they do not have a systematic way to process and organize the information collected to render judgments about reading problems. Accordingly, this approach is designed to train teachers to carefully and systematically observe and record children's natural language in the classroom and to use the information collected for diagnosis and remediation. Training emphasizes the assessment of three main areas of the child's reading develOpment: reading comprehension, print translation and language concepts. To make such assessments, the teacher must obtain information that reveals the children's level of functioning in the three areas -- oral reading of text or portions of text in the first case; their responses to comprehension questions based on printed text, in the second; and responses to aurally-presented language, in the third. With this information, the teacher determiens the child's strengths and weaknesses within each area. For example, in evaluating a student's proficiency in print translation, the teacher 49 will use oral reading evidence to draw conclusions about the number of words the child is able to recognize instantaneously; his/her knowledge of word components, including letter sound association; and his/her integration of this knowledge into a systematic strategy for recognizing print. Similarly, in assessing a student's comprehension, the teacher will draw conclusions about the child's ability to respond to questions demanding recall of stated information, his/her inferential reasoning from stated information, and his/her application of concepts to new situations. Comprehension is viewed as being dependent on print translation skill and familiarity with the verbal concepts used by the author of the text. Barr's model defines diagnosis as a process whereby the teacher identifies the conditions that interfere with the child's ability to comprehend assigned or selected materials. Intervention by the teacher may involve: (l) instruction in print translation, language concepts, and/or comprehension, and (2) modification of assigned materials. Sherman's Model of Readingyand Learning to Read The major assumption on which Sherman's Model of Reading and Learning to Read (MORAL) is based is that the process of clinical problem solving is governed by the interaction of two factors (Sherman, in preparation): (1) the clinician's theoretic conceptualization of the 50 cleint's potential problems and strengths and of possible cures, and (2) the clinician's actual experience, which is a major source of the associations between cues and problems, and between problems and remediation plans. If this assumption is correct, then the accuracy and clarity of a clinician's Model of Reading and Learning to Read determines the consistency and effectiveness of the clinician's decisions with regard to diagnosis and remediation. on According to Sherman, reading diagnosis is based the client's performance on four Vital Signs of Reading: Instant Word Recognition: the ability to recognize words without hesitation, measured by relative size of sight vocabulary; Mediated Word Recognition: the ability to recognize unfamiliar words through the use of graphemic redundancies; Phrase Comprehension: the ability to translate groups of words into cognitive structures with semantic meaning and to retain these structures briefly, and, Retentive Comprehension: the ability to retain the results of perceptive comprehension and to organize these results into more complex semantic structures suitable for long-term retention, complex inference, and problem solving. Sherman's MORAL holds that the performance level of a client on most reading activities is a probabilistic function of performance levels on these four fundamental reading tasks. Therefore, it seems that one of the 51 components of reading diagnosis should be the description of a child's performance in these four areas. For many teachers, and some clinicians, establishing the child's levels of performance on the four vital signs is the diagnosis. Sherman argues that diagnosis must proceed beyond mere surface indications of vital signs. The diagnostician, he says, must attempt to associate each vital sign with specific effectors (causes), such as subskill prerequisite knowledge and specific learning paradigms (e.g., assumptions about learning) and with specific causal factors (e.g., observable and sometimes manipulatable characteristics of the client and his/her environment). Sherman's belief is that each Vital Reading Sign performance level can be improved if the diagnostician has an understanding of the different effecting antecedents and conditions of learning. He assumes that the learning necessary to improve each sign is modulated by a different (but not exclusive) set of factors. The different antecedents of learning for Instant Word Recognition, for example, might be cue discrimination and associative strategies. The conditions for learning might be repetition of recognition (practice), motiviation and feedback. The effecting child variables may be inferred from the Learning Paradigm. They are characteristics of the client and his/her environment which influence Antecendent 52 Processes and Learning conditions. There are three groups of such factors: Child (e.g., cognitive and affective factors); School (e.g., academic length of interaction, regular training given to students), and Home (e.g., the student's language environment). In summary, Sherman's Model of Reading and Learning to Read implies that effective clinical diagnosis and remediation is determined by the clinician's theoretic conceptualization of reading as well as his actual experience. Diagnosis, according to Sherman, should proceed beyond mere examination of the four vital signs and consider the various antecedents and conditions for learning associated with the child's performance on each of the vital signs. The literature on reading diagnosis: conclusions q The purpose of this section has been to review the current literature on reading diagnosis with emphasis placed on that information which is pertinent to the objectives of this study. Conclusions suggested by this review are as follows: 1. Most educators seem to agree that reading diagnosis in the classroom is an integral part of the teaching-learning process and is a prerequisite to effective teaching. 2. Educators agree that reading difficulties tend to hinder students' intellectual and emotional development. Thus, early and continued classroom 53 diagnosis designed to prevent reading problems from developing is fundamental. The term "reading diagnosis" has different connotations; e.g., process vs. product; state of a child's reading performance vs. weaknesses. Furthermore, there appears to be several levels of diagnosis (e.g., classroom, school, clinical) used to identify and help groups and individuals with reading difficulties; the level used depends upon the severity of the problem. The line, however, between classroom diagnosis and clinical diagnosis is not well drawn. Moreover, neither the definitions of reading diagnosis nor the description of its levels describe the mental tasks performed by the diagnostician. Overall, what seems to be missing is a comprehensive model of reading diagnosis which would put together all the elements that should be involved in an effective diagnosis. An implicit assumption implied throughout the literature is that classroom teachers are capable of diagnosing students with reading difficulties. (Some educators even argue that teachers should be accountable for such a diagnosis). There is little empirical data, however, to confirm or disconfirm this notion. In general, the literature fails to provide an adequate description of the diagnostic processes employed by classroom teachers, nor does it provide substantive empirical evidence of the competence of classroom teachers in reading diagnosis. 54 The present study attempted to respond to these two deficiencies by providing comprehensive data about teachers' diagnostic skills. The data was obtained through investigation of the problem solving behavior of classroom teachers as they interacted with cases of reading difficulty. By using this approach we also sought to describe the mental tasks involved in the diagnostic process. This study was one of a series of interrelated studies of clinical problem solving behavior in reading diagnosis that are being conducted at the Institute for Research on Teaching (IRT) at Michigan State University. A discussion of the general research project will provide the reader with a better understanding of the present study, and a framework with which to examine it. This discussion may also be viewed as an introduction to the third chapter on design and methodology. Clinical Studies of Reading Diaggosis at the Institute for Research on Teaching Introduction The present study is a part of a series of investigations of the process of effective reading diagnosis and remediation which was begun three years ago by the Clinical Studies Component of the Institute for Research on Teaching (Gil gt gt., 1979). These studies are based on a theory of clinical problem solving known as the "Inquiry Theory" and have two general objectives: 55 1. Better understanding of clinical problem solving behavior of reading clinicians and classroom teachers; and 2. Application of research findings to the training of teachers and reading clinicians in diagnosis and remediation skills. Three different forms of studies are being conducted (Gil, Vinsonhaler and Wagner, 1978): 1. Observational studies, in which reading clinicians or classroom teachers were Observed as they interact with simulated cases of children with reading difficulties; 2. Computer simulation studies, in which the implications of the Inquiry Theory are explored by programming computers to simulate the diagnostic and remediation behavior described by this theory. (These "simulated" clinicians reflect both ideal and typical approaches of reading clinicians to diagnosis and remediation). 3. Training studies, in which instruction in reading diagnosis and remediation in classes is guided by the Inquiry Theory concepts. Students enrolled in the classes interact with simulated cases of children with reading difficulties, and are guided by computer- based decision aids, to improve their diagnostic skills. The Inquiry Theory The Inquiry Theory is a theory of clinical problem solving, which originated in the medical education research 56 "Inquiry Project" conducted at Michigan State University by Arthur Elstein and Lee Shulman (Elstein, Shulman, and Sprafka, 1978). It was refined by the Clinical Studies group at the Institute for Research on Teaching and has come to be known as the "Inquiry Theory" (Vinsonhaler, Wagner, and Elstein, 1977). The Inquiry Theory describes the behavioral domain in which a clinician (e.g., a physician, a reading specialist, a teacher) interacts with a case (e.g., a patient, a student) to reach diagnostic and treatment decisions about the case's problems. By describing this behavioral domain, the Inquiry Theory attempts to predict those characteristics of the clinical interaction which will occur (and be observed) over and over again. The theory holds that clinical diagnosis, or clinical problem solving, is determined by the interaction of clinical memory (consisting of sets of problems, cues, treatments and the relationships among them) and clinical strategy (the sequencing of the mental tasks performed by the clinician), and the case. Six information processing tasks characterize the clinical encounter between a clinician and a case: 1. Cue acquisition -- gathering of data, beginning with initially available cues. 2. Hypothesis generation -- formulation of alternative assumptions about the case's problems based on a limited number of cues. 57 3. Cue interpretation -- interpretation of the data based on the cues collected and the hypotheses under consideration. 4. Hypothesis evaluation and diagnositic judgment -- estimation of the likelihood of each hypothesis in order to reach a diagnostic judgment. 5. Treatment evaluation -- estimation of the expected gain for each of the possible treatments in relation to the diagnosis. 6. Prescription selection —- selection of the specific methods of remediation on the basis of the possible treatments. The agreement corollary As stated, the Inquiry Theory holds that clinical problem solving behavior is a function of the clinician's memory and strategy. This assumption suggests the following agreement corollary: If diagnosis is determined by clinical memory and strategy, then similar memories and strategies will result in similar diagnoses. According to the agreement corollary, memory and strategy are determined, in part, by the training clinicians receive.. The more similar the clinicians' training, the more similar their memories and strategies will be, and the more closely they will agree when diagnosing the same cases of reading difficulties. To the extent that their training varies, the clinicians will 58 employ different "memories" or "models" of the reading process, and will show limited agreement in their diagnostic judgments. Suppose, for example, that for reliability reasons, two teachers, with different training backgrounds, are asked to independently diagnose the same case of reading difficulties. Assume that one teacher operates from a skill-model orientation, the other from a socio-psychological model. Both teachers will use the same initial informal reading assessment procedures, such as analysis of school records and oral reading, to evaluate the case's reading performance. Their different training background, however, will affect their subsequent choice of information about the child's reading performance. The first teacher will tend to collect information relevant to his/her skill-model orientation, such as the child's ability to chunk, analyze, and identify words, while the other will probably seek socio-psychological information, such as psychological reports, the social-worker's comments about the child, and home background. The differences in the information collected will probably lead the teachers to different diagnostic judgments about the child's reading problems. The following is a list of possible diagnoses which the two teachers might entertain. (In other words, it is a list of some of the diagnostic categories "stored" in each teacher's memory, based on, and derived from, the training the teachers have received): 59 Skill orientation Socio-psychological orientation poor phonics* low interest in reading* poor word analysis poor visual acuity poor oral reading* reads below grade level* poor comprehension poor auditory acuity* poor sight words* inadequate verbal ability reads below grade level* no intellectual models of reading at home Suppose the asterisk (*) indicates those diagnostic judgments each teacher made about the child, based directly on the model from which he/she operates. A careful look at these judgments indicates that, although each of the teachers might have mentioned some of the child's problems, their diagnoses cover only a small part of the child's reading performance. The skill-oriented teacher, for example, mentioned "poor phonics" as a problem area, but did not suggest a possible cause for this problem, which may be "poor auditory acuity" (mentioned by the second teacher). She/he also failed to identify the cause of the child's "poor sight words," which may very likely be "low interest in reading" (mentioned by the second teacher). Teachers' diagnostic agreement can be determined by various statistical procedures, such as calculation of the proportional agreement statistic, commonality score, or inter-clinician coefficients (See Appendix I: Observational studies data analysis systems [OSDAS] statistics). In this instance, the inter-clinician phi coefficient yields a score 60 of around 0.0, meaning that the two teachers showed almost no agreement in their diagnoses of the child's problems; of a total of seven diagnoses mentioned by the teachers, only one (”reads below grade level") was made by both. This limited agreement is a result of the different kinds of training the teachers received. The teachers developed different "memories" about reading and reading deficiencies, and they considered only those factors that could be derived from the narrow list of categories stored in those memories. It should also be noted that neither teacher's diagnosis, considered separately from the diagnosis made by the other teacher, was sufficiently reliable to form the basis for remedial plans for the child. A combination Of their diagnoses would probably be more accurate, comprehensive, and reliable, and lead to more effective remediation. The agreement issue is an important one for all professions in which clinicians must make diagnostic decisions about their clients. The importance of the issue stems from the assumption that the higher the clinicians' agreement about a patient's problems, the more likely it is that they have reached accurate and reliable diagnoses (which, in turn, may lead to apprOpriate treatment). The question of agreement is especially important in.medicine, where incorrect diagnosis can result in permanent damage to the patient, and, in some cases, even in death. Because of the seriousness of the issue, many 61 studies have been conducted to investigate inter-physician agreement. Several investigations have revealed that "a surprising and clinically important degree of inaccuracy is to be expected in the interpretation or evaluation of many clinical and laboratory procedures used in every day practice" (Garland, 1959). For example, writes Garland, "experienced interpreters of a series of plain chest roentgenograms are apt to miss about 30 percent of those films positive for roentgenologic evidence of disease, and to over-read about 2 percent of those negative for disease. Further, in evaluating pairs of serial roentgenograms for alternations in the status of disease, one experienced physician is apt to disagree with another in about one-third of the cases, and (on review) to disagree with himself on one-fifth of them. Comparable degrees of error occur in many forms of clinical practice. Indeed, if all branches of medicine could be tested, the phenomenon would probably be found quite universal". Studies by other researchers support Garland's statements. In an examination of the reliability of chest radiography in the diagnosis of pulmonary lesions, Yerushalmy (1955) found that an individual physician agreed with other radiologists on only about 30 percent of the x-rays. Yerushalmy concluded that "observer error in chest radiography is of such magnitude as to create special problems in diagnosis and treatment." In another study, Paton (1957) found that "the accuracy rate in the diagnosis of myocaridal infractional is surprisingly low, 62 only 44 percent." Given the importance placed on diagnostic agreement in medicine, it seems logical that this is an important consideration in reading diagnosis, as well. For this reason, a major part of the data collected in the present study deals with teachers' diagnostic agreement. Case simulation Observational, computer simulation, and training studies of the clinical encounter and the tenets of the Inquiry Theory all involve the use of simulated cases of a client's problems. (A case is a set of information that the clinician can collect). These simulated cases are based on actual clients and represent the relevant characteristics of the client's problems. Two types of simulated cases have been develOped: manual based and computer based. The manual based case consists of various kinds of information about a child's reading performance (e.g., background data, school records, results of different tests that the child took, etc.) which are stored in a box and retrieved manually. The computer based case involves the same information, but it is stored in a computer file and is retrieved via computer terminal. The use of simulated cases in studies of reading diagnosis is desirable for several reasons: (1) they provide a means for presenting the child's behaviors to the clinicians; (2) they allow for replications of the clinical encounter with the same case (and with the same or 63 different clinicians), so that an acceptable level of objectivity can be achieved; (3) they may prove to be a low cost tool for training clinicians in diagnosis and remediation skills; and (4) they may prove to be an effective tool for training clinicians and educators on diagnostic skills under immediate feedback. Following is an examination of the three types of studies designed to investigate the clinical problem solving behavior of reading specialists and classroom teachers in reading diagnosis. All three are guided by the principles of the Inquiry Theory and utilize simulated cases as an experimental tool. The studies which will be reviewed are observational studies, computer simulation and training studies. Observational studies Several observational studies, other than the present one, were conducted during 1977 and 1978. The prime objective of the studies was to investigate the clinical problem solving behavior of reading specialists and learning disabilities personnel as they diagnosed children's learning difficulties. The studies used manually based simulated cases of learning difficulties. The general procedure was as follows (Gil, Vinsonhaler and Wagner, 1978): 1. An Observational session was held in which the clinician (reading specialist or learning disabilities personnel) interacted with a simulated case of learning difficulties 64 to reach diagnostic decisions about the case. The clinician was given an inventory of information about the case, and was instructed to ask for any piece of information that he/she wanted. An experimenter retrieved the items requested and handed them to the clinician. As the clinician examined this information, the experimenter encouraged him/her to talk aloud and verbalize his/her thinking. In the meantime, a clinical observer (a reading specialist or a teacher with a reading background) sat behind a one-way mirror, managing recording equipment and keeping a precise record of all information requested, the time of each request and the clinician's verbatim comments. Immediately following the Observational session, the clinician was instructed to write down his/her diagnostic judgments about the case and to map out a general plan for remediation. A debriefing session was held, during which the clinician was asked to recall his/her problem-solving behavior in requesting and evaluating information about the case. Each clinician interacted with two simulated cases; the second case was either a replication of the first or a different case. Data analysis consisted of both process and product measures. Process measures included such variables as number of hypotheses generated by the clinician during the session, number of cues (items of information) requested, sequencing, and orgnization of the information. 65 Product measures focused on diagnostic consistency (how consistent the clinician was in describing reading per- formances across two cases) and diagnostic commonality (how closely the clinicians agree with themselves and with each other in their diagnoses). Also included in the product measures was a comparison of each clinician's final diagnosis and the criterion diagnosis written by an expert clinician. Preliminary results from the first study (Vinsonhaler, 1979) indicate that agreement is quite low, both between an individual clinician's performances on alternte forms of the same case, and between his/her diagnosis and those of other clinicians. This means, then, that the reliability of a clinician's diagnosis is low. Furthermore, diagnostic consistency does not appear to be a function of the time spent on a case. Rather, consistency seems to vary with the number of replicate cues (items of information) that a clinician examines across cases. This suggests that clinicians may use a heuristic or a systematic data collection plan across cases. Computer simulation studies Computer simulation studies are another means by which knowledge of effective clinical problem solving can be gained. According to the Inquiry Theory, the behavior of a clinician is a function of his/her memory and strategy (Vinsonhaler, Wagner, and Elstein, 1977). Therefore, a 66 simulated clinician can be created by programming into a computer a memory, a strategy, and the interaction between the two, where changes in memory, in strategy, or in both will be reflected in changes in the simulated clinician's performance. In computer simulation studies, a simulated clinician interacts with a simulated case of a child with reading difficulties (Gil, Wagner and Vinsonhaler, 1979). Through- out this interaction, the computer uses its memory and strategy to arrive at a diagnosis for a child. In so doing, it uses a process similar to the one applied by human clinicians. The interaction between the computer clinician and a case starts when the computer receives the "initial contact" about the case, which is some basic information about the case's reading status. With this information, the computer proceeds to collect more information, on the basis of which it generates hypotheses about the child's reading problems. Then the simulated clinician collects still more information to confirm or disconfirm these hypotheses. After collecting a certain amount of information about the case, the simulated clinician then begins its final diagnosis. Different memories and strategies may be used to create different simulated clinicians, which may interact with various cases to reach diagnostic decisions. Analysis of the effects of these diagnostic outcomes would increase our understanding of the clinical diagnostic process, and the information could then be applied to the training of human clinicians. 67 Training studies The third kind of study based on the principles of the Inquiry Theory is the training or application study. The prime objective of such studies is to examine the application of clinical problem solving theory in the training of reading specialists and classroom teachers. (The instructional corollary from which this objective is derived states that if clinical performance is determined by the clinician's memory and strategy, then this performance may be improved by alterations in clinical memory and strategy). One training study (Sherman, 1978), carried on during the summer of 1977, addressed the following questions: (1) can teachers be trained to diagnose reading difficulties in a manner similar to that of effective reading specialists? (2) does the instructional corollary hold for reading diagnosis? To answer these questions, the performance of 36 students enrolled in a graduate diagnosis course was examined. Instruction was explicitly guided by the principles of the Inquiry Theory. Pre and post tests consisting of two instruments were administered: First, a memory battery association test examined the students' memory of problems in reading. It measured how well the students matched information to cue association (e.g., located specific findings about a child's reading per- formance); how well they matched cue to factor association 68 (e.g., identified important strengths and weaknesses given a set of findings), and how well they matched factor to cue association (identified important cues in testing hypothe- sized strengths or weaknesses). The second instrument was a diagnostic performance test, which examined the students' ability to accurately diagnose simulated cases of reading difficulty. This test consisted of a set of materials designed for use with simulated cases; included in the test were standardized instructions, response forms, and a scoring key, each intended to facilitate the student's interaction with the simulated cases. In addition to the traditional presentation of course content by the instructor, the students interacted 'with manual based and computer based simulated cases of reading difficulty, using the materials from the diagnostic performance test. At the end of the course, the students took the memory battery association posttest, and the diagnostic performance test (as they interacted with different simulated cases). Results indicated that the students did learn to diagnose in a manner similar to that of reading specialists; there was a significant increase in their mean diagnostic score. Results also suggested that the learning of clinical memory seems to be related to the learning of clinical diagnostic performance. 69 Clinical studies of reading diagnosis: Conclusions The Inquiry Theory presented in this section and the three kinds of studies derived from it suggest the following: 1. Clinical Problem solving behavior seems to be determined by the clinician's memory, strategy, and the interaction between the two. Therefore, clinical problem solving per- formance can be improved by providing clinicians with external and training aids to memory and to strategy. Clinical problem solving behavior can be simulated by computer; hence, by altering memory and strategy, simulated clinicians can be created which represent both typical and ideal approaches to problem solving (e.g., with or without heuristics). Human clinicians can then be trained to utilize those problem solving approaches which are found to be effective in the computer simulations. Case simulation appears to be a useful, effective, and low cost technique for training reading specialists and classroom teachers in diagnostic skills. Educators can be trained to make more precise diagnostic judgments given proper training involving simulated cases with feedback. Summary An attempt has been made in this chapter to review the literature dealing with two subjects: 1) the nature of reading diagnosis and its implications for classroom teaching, and 2) recent studies of clinical problem solving behavior 70 in reading diagnosis upon which the present study is drawn. Examination of the literature in the first area focused on the importance of reading diagnosis, various ways of defining reading diagnosis, different levels of reading diagnosis, and diagnosis made by classroom teachers, including analysis of some pertinent research studies. It concluded with description of Barr's Model of Diagnosis and Sherman's Model of Reading and Learning to Read, which represent two recent attempts to deal, in a more systematic way, with the content and process of reading diagnosis. The conclusions drawn from this examination of the literature about the nature of reading diagnosis were that: 1. Reading diagnosis is regarded by most educators as an integral part of all teaching; 2. Early and continuous reading diagnosis is essential to prevent reading problems from becoming more severe; 3. Various definitions are attached to the term "reading diagnosis", and the description of levels of reading diagnosis differs from one source to another; 4. The literature fails to describe or to analyze the cog- nitive processes performed by teachers as they diagnose children; 5. Although many educators call upon classroom teachers to assume the responsibilities of diagnosing and remediating children's reading difficulties, there is a lack of empirical research which might determine teachers' effectiveness in diagnosis and remediation skills. 71 The review of recent studies of clinical problem solving behavior in reading diagnosis began with a general description of the "Inquiry Theory" and of the utilization of simulated cases in studies of clinical problem solving behavior. It continued with brief description of three kinds of studies derived from the tenets of the Inquiry Theory -- Observational, Computer simulation and Training -- all of which use simulated cases as a research tool. The conclusions drawn from the review of these studies were that: 1. the Clinical problem solving behavior is a function of the clinician's memory and strategy. Clinical problem solving behavior can be simulated by computer; Simulated cases can be used effectively in studies of clinical problem solving behavior, and Educators can be trained to make more accurate diagnostic judgments through interaction with simulated cases. The implications of this review of the literature for present study are as follows: If classroom teachers are regarded as diagnosticians, it is essential that extensive research be conducted on teachers' diagnostic skills. Such research should focus on the mental tasks performed by the teachers as they diagnose cases, so that we will begin to understand the diagnostic process; and Simulated cases of reading can be used as research tools in studies of teachers' diagnostic skills and processes. CHAPTER III DESIGN AND METHODOLOGY Introduction The purpose of this study was to investigate the Clinical Problem Solving Behavior of classroom teachers. There were two main Objectives: 1. To examine the clinical problem solving behavior of classroom teachers as they diagnosed children's reading problems. 2. To explore the similarities and differences between teachers' diagnostic practices in experimental and classroom situations. The Subjects The subjects of the study were 10 elementary school teachers (all females) who volunteered, for payment, to participate. Because data collection was a lengthy procedure, and because it involved both experimental and classroom situations, the number of the subjects was limited to 10. For this reason, and because of the exploratory nature of the study, a deliberate effort was made to select teachers with different backgrounds to ensure diversity of performance. Five teachers were chosen from a group of teachers trained 72 73 in'a reading diagnosis course taught by Dr. Rebecca Barr at the University of Chicago; the other five were from the Lansing area. Chicago teachers have all taken a reading diagnosis course taught by Dr. Rebecca Barr. This course trains teachers to diagnose students' skill level in reading by observing the children's reading behavior and by listening and questioning them. Teachers are given materials which would be available to them in a regular classroom setting and are asked to interact with cases of reading difficulties. The five teachers who participated in this study were selected from a group of eight teachers. Two criteria were used for selecting them: 1) number of years of classroom teaching, and 2) guaranteed employment for the following fall. The second criterion was necessary because the second phase of the study involved interviews with the teachers in their own classrooms. ' Lansing teachers: Three of the five Lansing teachers were also participants in another study in progress at the IRT -- the Teachers Conceptions of Reading study, coordinated by Dr. Gerald Duffy. These teachers were asked to participate in the present study during a meeting of all the teachers taking part in Dr. Duffy's project. A fourth teacher was recommended, and invited to participate, by a member of the Clinical Studies group. The fifth teacher was recommended by an experienced teacher known to the experimenter. 74 To participate in the study, all teachers had to have completed at least two graduate courses in reading instruction, preferably one in reading diagnosis. The teachers' backgrounds, schools and grade levels taught are summarized in Table 2. Design The design of the study called for two phases of research: Phase 1 was conducted during the summer and early fall of 1978 under laboratory conditions. The teachers were asked to interact with simulated cases of reading difficulties and to arrive at diagnostic judgments about the cases' problems. In Phase 2, carried out during the fall of 1978, the teachers were observed for one reading session in their own classrooms, and then interviewed about reading instruction and diagnostic practices. The overall design of the study is illustrated in Table»3. 75 mm ~\H mmmao manta: .m.z ca me n mm H mmmao manta: .4.2 ha mm o ma m mmmHO manna: .B.m.z H mm m .a.m.z ow m mmmao wanna: .<.z m Hm v manoeuiounosm \Hmsmcflaflm mm m Immoao 30A .B.w.2 ha we m mauwm Ham\oflaonumu NH m Immmao wanna: .B.m.z m mm N Hoonom non mm N Immwao OHOOHZ .m.z ma me a mmmao cw mcanommu waucon mocmwnmmxm mucmpsum luau nown3 ca mcasomou mo .02 Demon» ocmuo Hoonom wo mama Odo: omumoo mumom mo .02 O04 muocomoe pcsoumxomn mopmum Ocm Hoosom .muonommell.m mummy 76 m.mN Humemum sum mlmnmucoEme momma nlhumucmfiwam Hm3oq Huamsmanann\zoa HIMMMHU maeeasusoa Humauam Ham oaaoaumo \mmmHo mavens enema \mmmao maven: mummmHo mavens mumafism no cows HN mmmao mavens mm ca NN mmmao OHUOHS mm NN mmmao maennz\3oq AH Nm mmmHO cw mucmcsum MO .02 pcmsmv OUOHO mcflsowou maucou IHDO cOH£3 cw Hoosom mo mama pawn moumoa ewsaflucoouum mamas OOOOHHOQRO mcwnommu mums» mo .02 mmfl mumcomme 77 mchmou OH mmOHuomnm 3OH>HODOH 0H HOHOOEOH conmom .um H ume m can “mom muouco>cH w m OHumOOOMHO DHEHH m HO < Show once a w m .HchHu GOHmmmm mmmo mEHu huoucm>cH can once m m nonhumcH mcwpmmu nonuocm oz .um m ume pmumHaeHm m m .wnwnowou mco mo cqu H m no u so v o on» usonm chHum> OOHmmmm monmom maloan3 5 .Oz mmmu mchHmnB m 4 m3OH>noucH luomno mo mch OOHDMHmeom ”conmwm Hm N m EooummMHu mchuoz .MOHHmmm uOHHan a mHmocmea IGOHum>Homno .HumcH Hmuocmw H a monB¢> N H m3MH>mmezH ummmmo onmmmm onmmmm mhmH HHOI|.m mHmme 78 Phase 1: Laboratory conditions Each of the teachers visited a reading clinic at two occasions. The Chicago teachers went to a clinic at the University of Chicago, the Lansing teachers reported to a clinic at the College of Education at Michigan State University. At each session, the teacher was asked to interact with a simulated case and to arrive at diagnostic decisions about its reading difficulties. The two cases used were developed by Dr. Rebecca Barr and were based on two Chicago elementary school students. They were chosen by Dr. Barr for this study because both had problemsthat were representative of reading difficulties encountered in the public schools by classroom teachers. One of the students was an eight-year-old girl; the other a 10 year-old girl. Both cases were built around materials available to classroom teachers for reading instruction and the children's natural language patterns. As such, each case included the following: 1) a copy of the passage that the child read orally; 2) a tape-recording of the child reading the passage; 3) an oral transcription of what the child read; 4) background information about the child and the school, and 5) transcripts of the child's performance on various tasks related to the reading passage, such as her answers to comprehension questions; her definition of a word presented to her, and her ability to identify a word printed 79 on the board (See Appendix A for the Case Information Inventory). The sequence of steps followed during this phase of the study was as follows: 1. A training session was held, during which the teacher was given an introduction about the purpose of the study. Instructions about the procedures to be followed were read, and each teacher was asked to practice with a training case and a training case inventory. (This step was essential to ensure that all teachers understood what they were expected to do, what information in the Case Inventory was available to them, and how they were to use the Case Inventory to retrieve and receive the desired information.) (See Appendix B, Directions for Observational Study.) A one-hour observational session was held, during which the teacher was given an inventory of information available for the case and was instructed to ask for any information she wished to have. The use of a Case Inventory was necessary for two reasons. First, it let the teachers know what specific units of information were available. Second, it helped standardize procedures, including the information available, which in turn helped standardize the teachers' memories. This allowed for a more objective analysis of the data than would have otherwise been possible. 80 As each teacher examined the information about a case, she was asked to verbalize her thinking. The experimenter who has instructed the teacher retrieved the information requested and handed it to the teacher. Teachers were allowed to keep all items of information requested throughout the session. The experimenter also encouraged the teacher to talk aloud as she examined the information. In the meantime, a clinical observer (a reading specialist) sat behind a one-way mirror, managing the recording equipment and keeping a precise record of all information requested, including time of each request and the teacher's verbatim statements. 3. Immediately after the observational session, the teacher was instructed to write down (1) her diagnostic judgments about the child's reading problems and (2) a general plan for remediation. A time limit of half an hour was set for this activity. 4. A debriefing session (with no time constraint) followed, during which techniques of stimulus recall were used to help the teacher remember her problem—solving behavior in requesting and evaluating information about the case. The clinical observer helped the experimenter conduct the debriefing session. The teacher was shown each of the items of information she had requested during the Observational session. A standard set of questions about the items was asked to explore and clarify her problem-solving behavior. 81 A second interaction between the teachers and a different case took place several days later. The same procedure was followed as for the first case. In addition, after the debriefing session for the second encounter, a brief demographic questionnaire dealing with the teachers' training experience and teaching experience was administered. All 10 teachers interacted with the same simulated cases, but in different orders. Five interacted with Case No. 7 first and Case No. 8 second; the others interacted with Case NO. 8 first and then with Case No. 7. Two inventory forms were available for each case. The forms were identical, except for the order in which they listed the information about the student. The teachers were given Form A for one case, and Form B for the other. The distribution of cases and inventory forms is summarized in Table 4. Phase 2: Classroom interviews During the fall of 1978, each teacher was visited once in her own classroom. There she was observed for one reading session and interviewed about her reading instruction and diagnostic practices (See Appendix C for Classroom Interview Form). The question on which the interview focused was: How do teachers diagnose children with reading difficulties in the natural classroom situation? Specifically, questions were asked which were designed to explore the teachers' memory 82 TABLE 4.--Assignment of cases and inventory forms* SESSION 1 SESSION 2 TEACHER Case Inventory Case Inventory 1 8 A 7 B 2 7 B 8 A 3 8 B 7 A 4 7 A 8 B 5 8 A 7 B 6 7 B 8 A 7 8 B 7 A 8 7 A 8 B 9 8 A 7 B 10 7 B 8 A *For analysis purposes, the cases were identified as Number 7 and 8, respectively, rather than by the children's names. 83 and strategy with regard to reading diagnosis. The interviews proceeded in the following manner: The experimenter handed the teacher four descriptions of real children with typical reading problems. Two of the descriptions were based on the simulated cases the teacher had diagnosed in the laboratory. The teacher was instructed to read these descriptions and think of children in her own classroom who corresponded to them. After she identified such children, she was asked to indicate which parts of the descriptions did or did not fit the selected children. This explanation provided the experimenter with a more precise idea of the resemblance between the children in the classroom and those in the description. The teacher was then asked a set of questions, which explored how she identified children with reading problems and how she diagnosed the problems. Each teacher was asked to describe diagnostic procedures for two children. (See Appendix D: Descriptions of the four children. Also see Appendix E, which shows the frequency with which teachers indicated similarities between descriptions and children in their classrooms.) Data Analysis Data analysis included two major types of measures: Product and process (Wagner et al., 1979). Product measures dealt with the outcomes of the clinical encounter between 84 teacher and case, and included the final stated diagnostic judgements, what cues were collected, etc. Process measures focused on the manner in which the problem was diagnosed, e.g., on relationships between cues collected and statements made about the cues. Analysis was also conducted based on the classroom interviews, to compare teachers' diagnostic processes in both laboratory and classroom situations. Product measures involve three types of statistics (Vinsonhaler, 1979): 1. The proportional agreement, which is a measure of group agreement. This measure is based on the final stated diagnoses written by all teachers for a given case, or on the list of cues collected by all teachers for a case, and is determined in the following manner: First, a do- main of statements (stated diagnoses or cues collected) is defined. Next, the percent of teachers who arrived at the same diagnoses or collected the same cues about a case is calculated. The purpose of this measure is to indicate which diagnoses or cues are most frequently mentioned or collected by the teachers. 2. The commanality score, a measure of how closely an individual teacher and a defined group of teachers agree in terms of diagnostic statements or cues collected for the same case. 3. The inter-clinician agreement correlation, which is a measure of the agreemnt among teachers on the diagnostic 85 statements or cues collected for a given case. This statistic is calculated as follows: (1) a domain of statements (written diagnoses or cues collected) is defined for a case; (2) those statements appearing in an individual teacher's interaction with the case are identified; and (3) a two by two contingency table is prepared and a Phi coefficient is calculated; the cells in the contingency table include (a) the number of statements mentioned in both encounters; (b) statements present in the first encounter (one teacher) but not in the second; (c) statements present in the second encounter (second teacher) but not in the first; and (d) statements not present in either encounter. (See Appendix I: Observational Studies Data Analysis System [OSDAS] statistics). Process measures of the clinical encounter between a teacher and a case were made which indicated the manner, or diagnostic process, by which the case was diagnosed. Four measures were selected for analysis in this study: 1. Length of interaction: amount of time (in minutes) that the teacher interacted with the case during the observational session. Time was measured from the point at which the teacher received the initial contact in- formation about the case until she indicated she was ready to write the diagnosis. Number of stated diagnoses: When the teacher indicated 86 she was ready to write the diagnosis, she was allowed half an hour to write down her diagnostic statements about the case. Number of cues collected during the observational session. The teacher was allowed to look at the case inventory and collect as many cues as she wished. However, she was required to ask for them one at a time. This condition will allow the experimenter to conduct an exploratory investigation of the order in which the teacher collected information about the case. Relationships between cues and statements: During the observational session, it will be recalled, the teacher was asked to verbalize her thinking as she collected in- formation about the case. This "thinking aloud" continued during the debriefing session. During both sessions a clinical observer kept a precise record of the teacher's verbatim comments about each of the cues she collected. It was thought that the relationships between the teacher's verbatim comments and the cues she collected might shed light on her diagnostic processes. Accordingly, a lO-point scale was devised for characterizing the relationships between the teacher's statements (verbatim comments) and the cues: l) The cue requested confirmed a statement, for example: "It confirmed what I suspected that a general weakness exists." 2) 3) 4) 5) 6) 7) 8) 9) 10) 87 The cue requested disconfirmed a statement, for example: "I ruled out that concepts are a major problem." The cue requested neither confirmed nor disconfirmed a statement. The statement deals specifically with the cue, for example: "She doesn't use whole words." The statement is a generalization of findings about cues, for example: "Comprehension skills are more of a problem for her (than I thought)." The statement is related to cues in a manner other than the above, for example: "I don't know why she didn't understand." The statement indicates the teacher's method of collecting information about a case, for example: "I wanted to probe the issue of word identification." The statement made has no relation at all to the cue, for example: "Would be a help to me if it wasn't quite so challenging to her." The statement is about remediation, for example: "Should go back to easier level." The statement is about children's reading behavior in general, or about the teacher's philosophy of reading, for example: "sometimes re-reading helps kids to get more information." 88 Comparisons between teachers' diagnostic processes in laboratoty and classroom situations. Two means of validating the teachers' performance on a simulated case were used in this study. First, the teachers were asked to interact with a second case, while the investigator looked for consistency in performance across the cases; and second, the investigator studied the diagnostic processes teachers employed in their natural classroom situation and looked for similarities and differences between these processes and the diagnostic processes exhibited in the laboratory setting.. This second step was very important, because it may indicate the degree to which teachers' interactions with simulated cases in the laboratory setting reflect their natural diagnostic processes. It should be emphasized, how- ever, that the laboratory phase is essential if one wants to trace the diagnostic processes employed by teachers. Observations of systematic cue collection and "thinking aloud" are not available in a classroom situation. Comparisons of the diagnostic processes used in laboratory situations and those used in the natural classroom setting centered around the following questions: What are the diagnostic categories most frequently mentioned by teachers in the classroom? Are there similarities between the diagnostic categories mentioned by teachers in the 89 laboratory situation and those mentioned in classroom setting? Questions relating specifically to classroom inter- views were also examined, among these were: What are the remedial approaches offered by teachers in the classroom? How successful do teachers perceive themselves to be in remediating children's reading difficulties in the classroom? Summary This study was conducted to investigate the clinical problem solving behavior of classroom teachers in diagnosing reading problems in laboratory and classroom situations. Research was divided into two parts: (1) observation of teachers' interactions with two simulated cases in a laboratory setting, and (2) interviews with the same teachers in their classrooms. Two major types of measures were used to analyze the data: product measures and process measures. Product measures were concerned with the outcomes of the clinical encounter between a teacher and a case. Process measures were concerned with the manner in which the problem was diagnosed. Comparisons were also made between teachers' diagnostic practices in the laboratory situation and their practices in the natural classroom environment. The data which were collected, treatment of the data, and results are presented in the next chapter. CHAPTER IV RESULTS Introduction This study was designed to investigate the clinical problem-solving behavior of classroom teachers as they diagnose reading performance in laboratory and classroom situations. The study involved 10 teachers from the Chicago and Lansing areas and was conducted in two phases. In the first part, the teachers interacted with two simulated cases of reading difficulty in a laboratory situation. Techniques of stimulus recall were used to obtain recorded verbatim of the teachers' diagnostic processes (written protocols of the teachers' "thinking aloud" were also taken, as were their final written diagnoses about the case). In the second phase of the study, the teachers were observed in their own classrooms and interviewed about their instructional, diagnostic, and remedial practices. The interviews were audiotaped and transcribed. Because few investigations have been done of the clinical problem solving behavior engaged in by classroom teachers as they diagnose reading performance, no hypotheses 90 91 were established for this study. Rather, the study was viewed as an exploratory one, intended to yield a description of the teachers' diagnostic processes. Data analysis focused on the degree of similarity (and difference) among the memories and models the teachers used in diagnosing the cases of reading difficulties. The theoretical framework on which data analysis was based was the Agreement Corollary, derived from the Inquiry Theory of Clinical Problem Solving Behavior (Vinsonhaler, Wagner and Elstein, 1977). The Agreement Corollary, it will be recalled, states that, if diagnosis is determined by clinical memory and clinical strategy, then the use of similar memories and strategies will result in similar diagnoses. Memory and strategy are determined, in part, by the training clinicians receive; the more similar the training they receive, the more similar their memories and strategies will be, and the more closely they will agree with each other when diagnosing the same case of reading difficulties. Conversely, if training varies from one teacher to another, the teachers will operate from different "memories" or "models" of the reading process and will tend to show limited agreement in their diagnoses (for a more complete discussion of the Inquiry Theory and the Agreement Corollary, see Chapter II, pp. 57—62). 92 For purposes of clarification, recall the two teachers, discussed in Chapter II, who employ different memories or models. One, it was said, Operates from a skill-model orientation, the other from a socio- psychological model. Because of their different diagnostic backgrounds, they tend to entertain different diagnoses about a case of reading difficulties. The following is a list depicting some of the diagnoses the two might entertain for the same case (in other words, this is a list of some of the diagnostic categories "stored" in each teacher's memory, based on, and derived from, the training teachers received): Skill orientation Socio-psychological orientation poor phonics* low interest in reading* poor word analysis poor visual acuity poor oral reading* reads below grade level* poor comprehension poor auditory acuity* poor sight words* inadequate verbal ability reads below grade level* no intellectual models of reading at home The * indicates the final stated diagnostic judg- ments reached by each teacher about the case. As seen, they differ significantly. A careful look at the judgments indicates that each teacher mentioned only some of the child's problems. The skill-oriented teacher, for example, cites "poor phonics" as a problem area, but does not identify a possible cause for this problem, which may 93 be "poor auditory acuity" (mentioned by the second teacher). The first teacher also fails to identify the cause for the child's "poor sight words," which may very likely be "low interest in reading" (mentioned by the second teacher). Statistcal procedures -- such as proportional agreement statistic, commonality score and inter-clinician correlations -- can be used to calculate the teachers' diagnostic agreement [See Appendix I: Observational Studies Data Analysis System (OSDAS) Statistics]. An inter- clinician Phi coefficient calculated to find the diagnostic agreement of the two teachers in the example, yields a score of around 0.0. This means that the two teachers show almost no agreement with one another about the child's problems. Out of a total of seven diagnoses mentioned by the two teachers, only one ("reads below grade level") is made by both. This limited agreement can be attributed to the teachers' different training background, which have left them with different "memories" about reading and reading deficiencies. In diagnosing the child's performance, each teacher considered only those factors that could be derived from the narrow list of categories stored in his/her memory. Thus, neither teacher's diagnosis, by itself, is reliable enough to determine the remedial plans to be taken for the child. A combination of their diagnoses would probably yield a more accurate, comprehensive, and reliable diagnosis, which would lead to a more effective remediation. 94 As mentioned, the Agreement Corollary also applies to the strategies teachers use. In the realm of reading diagnosis, there may be two general and distinct strategies, or ways of reasoning: deductive and inductive. For purposes of illustration, recall the two teachers in the example, one of whom operates from a skill-model orientation and one of whom works from a socio-psychological model. Both used the same initial informal reading assessment procedures to evaluate the student's reading performance (e.g., examination of school records, analysis of audiotapes of the child reading a passage, etc.). They differed, however, in their subsequent cue collection. The skill-oriented teacher, being an inductive reasoner, tested the child on various skills associated with reading, such as the child's ability to chunk, analyze, and identify words. After collecting the information about the case, this teacher concluded that the child lacked sufficient reading skills. The teacher Operating from the socio-psychological orientation, on the other hand, was a deductive reasoner; she/he assumed that the child's problems were emotionally-based and thus collected only that information pertaining to the child's personality, such as psychological reports, social-worker comments about the child, and home background. In summary, it may be stated that due to inadequate and/or insufficient training, different teachers may Operate from very distinct, and narrow, models of reading 95 and the factors that affect reading performance. When diagnosing a given case of reading difficulties, they may operate from different memories and strategies, which will result in very limited agreement about the case's reading performance. Presentation of Results The purpose of this chapter is to present the findings of this study, most of which relate to the issue of agreement among teachers. These findings will be presented in three parts; 1. Product measures - which were concerned with the outcomes of the clinical encounter between a teacher and a case, 2. Process measures - which were concerned with the manner in which the problem was diagnosed, and 3. Classroom interviews - which were concerned with teachers' instructional, diagnostic and remedial practices in the natural classroom environment. Part 1: Product measures Product measures, which were concerned with the outcomes of the clinical encounter between a teacher and a case, dealt with the teachers' final stated diagnoses and the cues they collected for a given case. The measures were divided into three parts: 96 l. Proportional agreement - a measure of group agreement on diagnoses and cues collected; 2. Commonality score - a measure of the agreement between individual teacher and a defined group of teachers in terms of diagnostic judgments and cues collected on the same case, and 3. Inter-clinician agreement correlations - a measure of the agreement of one teacher with another on the diagnostic judgments or cues collected for a given case. The proportional agreement statistic.--the pro- portional agreement statistic was calculated by dividing the number of teachers who stated the same final diagnosis (or collected the same cues) for a given case by the total number of teachers who interacted with that case. This process yielded the percentage of teachers who arrived at the same diagnostic judgments or collected the same cues about the case. It also indicated the most frequently stated diagnoses or cues collected. Prgportional agreement for diagnostic judgments.-- before any statistical operations were carried out, a list of total diagnostic judgments was prepared. Included were all diagnostic judgments mentioned by all 10 teachers for each case. Categorizing the judgments turned out to be somewhat of a problem. Many of the judgments were stated differently, although they were essentially the same, 97 because there is no generally agreed upon vocabulary for reading diagnosis. To determine which of the differently— stated judgments were, in fact, identical, a senior reading clinician looked at the diagnostic list and omitted all redundant categories. For purposes of reliability, another reading clinician did the same thing, and his list of diagnostic categories was checked against the first clinician. The result was a list of 54 diagnostic categories for Case 7, and 62 for Case 8 (See Appendix F for the diagnostic domain for Case 7 and 8). Proportional agreement statistics were then calculated for the most frequently-mentioned judgments. Table 5 shows these statistics. The first column in the table lists the diagnostic judgments mentioned by at least 20 percent of the teachers for a given case. The second and third columns indicate the percentage of teachers mentioning each category for Case 7 and Case 8, respectively. For example, the diagnostic category "Endings: Ignores", was mentioned by 50 percent of the teachers for Case 7, but not at all for Case 8. As the table suggests, in general, the teachers showed a very low level of agreement in diagnosing the cases. Only one diagnostic category for each case was mentioned by five teachers (50 percent of total), and only one, for Case 7, was mentioned by four teachers. Seven categories were mentioned by three teachers, and 16 by two. The rest of the judgments were mentioned by only one teacher. 98 TABLE 5.--Most frequently mentioned diagnostic categories for Case 7 and Case 8 and 10 teachers SELECTED DIAGNOSTIC CATEGORIES Comprehension: Poor Comprehension: Adequate Understanding: Good when listening Listening comprehension: Adequate Oral reading comprehension: Good Vocabulary concepts: Adequate Vocabulary: Weak Oral reading: Skips words Oral Reading: Does not pay attention to punctuation Puncuation: Lacks Endings: Ignore Sight words: Weak Sight words: Good Reversals: Problem area Mispronunciation errors: Did not correct Phonic skills: Weak Beginning sounds; Knows Initial consonants: Strong Syllabication skills: Poor Unfamiliar words: No attempt to sound Vowels: Need work Word attack skills: Lacks Context to help with the unknown word: Does not use Word analysis: Weak Language: No language problems Print translation: Problem area CASE 7 (54 total DX) .30 .20 .30 CASE 8 (62 total DX) 20 20 20 30 .30 .20 .30 *—-denotes 0.00 It should also be noted that only one diagnostic category (”Vocabulary concepts: Adequate") was mentioned by as many as three teachers for both cases. All other categories were mentioned either for Case 7 or Case 8, but not for both. 99 Figure 1 shows the percentage of diagnostic categories in the entire diagnostic domain that the different teachers mentioned. The verticle line indicates percentage of total diagnostic judgments made; the horizontal line indicates the number of teachers making these percentages of total judgments. The figure reveals limited agreement among the teachers on the percentage of total diagnostic judgments they made for both Case 7 and Case 8. For example, 80 percent of the total diagnostic judgments made for Case 7 were made by only one teacher, whereas only 2 percent of the total were made by five teachers. For Case 8, 73 percent of the total judgments were made by only one teacher, and only 1.5 percent were made by five teachers. In summary, the data presented in Table 5 and in Figure 1 show little agreement among the teachers on diagnostic judgments for either of the two cases. Proportion agreement for cues.--the proportion agreement for cues was calculated in a manner similar to that used for determining the proportional agreement for judgments. First, a list of all cues collected by all ten teachers for each case was prepared. All redundant cues were eliminated, and a frequency count was made to determine how many teachers collected each cue. Table 6 shows the proportional agreement statistics for the most commonly collected cues for Cases 7 and 8. 100 100. 95‘ _ . , , J . . case 7 (N=34 diagnostic judg- 90 ments) mean = 1.45 85' x ----- x case 8 (N=62 diagnostic judg— 80. ments) mean = 1.34 75. 70- 65- 60- 55- 50‘ 40‘ 35- 30' 25- 20 ‘ 15 ‘ ioI Percent of diagnostic judgments mentioned y t I i 1 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 Number of teachers mentioning category Figure l.--Percent of diagnostic categories for Cases 7 and 8 and 10 teachers 101 TABLE 6.--Most frequently collected cues for Cases 7 and 8 and 10 teachers SELECTED CUES CASE 7 CASE 8 (221 cues (321 cues total) total) Background: Cultural/Home .90 .90 Background: Student's personality -- .90 Background: Comments on student reading the passage .70 .60 Background: Test information .50 .70 Vocabulary concepts: "Fancy" (in isolation).70 -- Background: Reading program .60 .40 Background: Language development -- .60 Word recognition: "Simple" (in isolation) .60 -- Vocabulary concepts: "Detective" (in isolation) -- .60 Background: Oral reading transcription .50 .40 Background: Performance in school .50 -- Background: Picture accompanying the reading passage .50 -- Vocabulary concepts: "Fancy" (in teacher sentence) .50 -- Word recognition: "Sammy" (teacher chunk) .50 -- Comprehension: Oral reading (paragraph 1, list) -- .50 Comprehension: Oral reading (paragraph 1, question 10) -- .50 Word recognition: "Everyone" (in isolation) -- .50 Word recognition: "Detective" (student chunk) -- .50 Background: School history/description .40 .40 Background: Description of picture in the passage .40 -- Comprehension: Oral reading (no paragraph specific, list) .40 -- Comprehension: Oral reading (no paragraph specific, question 3) .40 -- Comprehension: Oral reading (paragraph 4, list) .40 -- Comprehension: Listening (paragraph 4, list) .40 -- Comprehension: Listening (paragraph 4, question 1) .40 -- Vocabulary concepts: "Simple" (in isolation) .40 -- Vocabulary concepts: "Simple" (student chunk) .40 -- Word recognition: "Sammy" (student chunk) .40 -- 102 TABLE 6.--Continued SELECTED CUES CASE 7 CASE 8 (221 cues (321 cues total) total) Comprehension: Silent reading (oral questions & answers, paragraph 1, list) -- .40 Comprehension: Oral reading (paragraph 1 question 3) -- .40 Comprehension: Oral reading (paragraph 1, question 7) -- .40 Vocabulary concepts: "Famous" (in isolation) -- .40 Vocabulary concepts: "Dosens" (in isolation) -- .40 Word recognition: "During" (in isolation) -- .40 Word recognition: "Detective" (in isolation -- .40 Word recognition: "Picnic" (in isolation) -- .40 Word recognition: "Plates" (in isolation) -- .40 Word recognition: "Detective" (teacher chunk) -- .40 The first column in the table lists the cues collected by at least 40 percent of the teachers for one or both cases. The second and third columns show the percentage of teachers who collected these cues. For example, "Cultural-Home Background" was collected by 90 percent of the teachers, for both Cases 7 and 8. "Word recognition: 'Detective'", on the other hand, was collected by only 40 percent of the teachers, for Case 8 (Background information cues were the only ones selected for both cases; that is, all other cues were slightly different from case to case. The teachers could not, evidently, collect the exact same one for both cases.) 103 As Table 6 shows, the proportional agreement for cues collected was much higher than for the diagnostic judgments (Table 5); teachers collected the same cues more often than they arrived at the same diagnostic judgments for a given case. The Case Inventory may have been partially responsible for this result; in providing a specific set of cues it might have increased the likelihood that the same cues would be collected. The kind of organization it offered was not available to the teachers when they made their diagnostic judgments. As the table shows, background data about the child was the most frequently requested information, across both cases, and cultural-home data was the type of background information most commonly asked for. This might indicate that the teachers depended on socio-cultural data to a great degree in attempting to make decisions about the cases' reading problems. It may further suggest that when the teachers dealt with reading problems, they did not necessarily operate from models which were not directly related to the reading process (e.g., socio-cultural models). Figure 2 denotes the percentage of all cues collected by the 10 teachers for Case 7 and 8. The verticle line indicates the percentage of all cues cOllected, and the horizontal line the number of teachers who collected these percentages of cues. The figure shows limited agreement among the teachers in their use of the 221 cues collected for Case 7 and 321 collected for Case 8. For example, 55 percent of the total 100 . 95 - 9o 1 85 . so . 75 7o . 65 - 60 I 55 4 so . 45 « no 35 I 30 . 25 , zo . Percent of cues collected 15‘ 10I 104 . case 7 (N=221 cues) mean = 1.90 x ----- x case 8 (N=321 cues) mean = 1.61 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 Number of teachers collecting cues Figure 2.--Percent of all cues collected for Cases 7 and 8 by 10 teachers 105 cues collected for Case 7 were collected by only one teacher, while only 5 percent of the total were collected by nine teachers. Similarly, nearly two-thirds of all cues collected for Case 8 were collected by one teacher, and only 5 percent of the total were collected by nine. In summary, the data presented in Figure 2 show that there was little agreement in cue collection among the 10 teachers for either case. Commonality score.--commonality scores were cal- culated to determine the degree of agreement between each teacher's diagnoses and the group of diagnoses made by all other teachers for the same case. The score takes into account which diagnostic judgments, and how many, were made by each teacher. The same analyses were performed with cues collected. Table 7 shows the diagnostic commonality scores for Cases 7 and 8. The first column in the table gives the representative values of the Phi correlations; the second column indicates the frequency count for each of the values. For example, two teachers for Case 7, and three for Case 8, correspond to the .4 value. The distributions were quite normal for both cases. The table shows that the mean commonality scores for diagnostic judgments made by all 10 teachers were .45 and .45, for Case 7 and 8, respectively. This means that, on the average, each teacher's diagnosis for a given case 106 TABLE 7.--Diagnostic commonality score for Cases 7 and 8 and 10 teachers HISTOGRAM/FREQUENCES CASES = 1-10 MIDPOINT FOR 6.V6 (Each X=1) -1.0000 -.90000 -.80000 -.70000 -.60000 -.50000 -.40000 -.30000 -.20000 -.10000 0.0 .10000 .20000 .30000 + .40000 +XX000 .50000 +X000 .60000 + .70000 +Xx0 .80000 +x0 {.90000 +X 1.0000 + X=case 7 mean=.45 N=10 standard dev.=.29 0=case 8 mean=.45 N=10 standard dev.=.23 +-;-++-+-+4—+-+4—+-+ O + N N O l . \ I I TOTAL 10 (INTERVAL WIDTH=.10000) included about half of the most frequently-mentioned diagnostic judgments made by the other teachers. Table 8 gives the cue commonality scores for Cases 7 and 8. The first column in the table shows the representative value for the Phi correlations; the second column indicates the frequency count for each of the values. For example, 107 TABLE 8.--Cue commonality scores for Cases 7 and 8 and 10 teachers HISTOGRAM/FREQUENCIES CASES = 1-10 MIDPOINT FOR 6.V6 (EACH X=1) -1,0000 + X=case 7 mean=.56 -.90000 + N=10 standard dev.=.ll -.80000 + -.70000 + 0=case 8 mean=.55 -.60000 + N=10 standard dev.=.l4 -.50000 + -.40000 + -.30000 + -.20000 + -.10000 + 0.0 + .10000 + .20000 +0 .30000 +X .40000 +0 .50000 +XXX00 .60000 +XXXX00 .70000 +XX0000 .80000 + .90000 + 1.0000 + TOTAL (INTERVAL WIDTH=.10000) three teachers, for Case 7, and two, for Case 8, corresponds to the .5 value. As this table reveals, the mean cue commonality scores for the two cases were higher than the diagnostic commonality scores (Table 7). This suggests that, on the average, the teachers agreed more on what cues to collect than on what diagnostic judgments to make. 108 Inter-clinician agreement correlations--the inter- clinician correlations denoted the agreement between one teacher and each of the others on diagnostic judgments and cues collected for five cases (Phi correlations are the Pearson Product Moment correlations with the assumption that the two variables are dichotomos and not necessarily normally distributed). Specifically, these correlations indicated the degree to which particular cues and diagnostic judgments in one teacher's diagnosis were present in or absent from, another teacher's diagnosis. Table 9 shows the inter- clinician correlations for diagnostic judgments for Cases 7 and 8. The first column in the table shows the representative value of the Phi correlations; the second column gives the frequency count for each of the values. For example, the frequency count for the .2 value was 3, for Case 7, and 2, for Case 8. The data in Table 9 indicate that the agreement scores for diagnostic judgments were near zero, for both cases. In other words, on the average, the teachers showed very limited agreement (if any) on their diagnostic judgments. Table 10 shows the inter-clinician correlations for cues collected for the two cases. The first column in the table gives the representative Phi coefficient values, the second column the frequency count for each of the values. For example, the frequency count for the .2 value were 6, for Case 7, and 3, for Case 8. 109 TABLE 9.--Inter-clinician correlations for diagnostic judgments for Cases 7 and 8 and 10 teachers HISTOGRAM/FREQUENCIES CASES = 1-45 MIDPOINT FOR 7.V7 (EACH X=1) -1.0000 -.90000 -.80000 -.70000 -.60000 -.50000 -.40000 -.30000 -.20000 +XXXXXXXXXXXXX00000000 -.10000 +XXXXXXXXXXXXX000000000000000000 0.0 +XXXXX000000 .10000 +XXXXXXXXXX00000000000 .20000 +XXX00 .30000 +X .40000 .50000 .60000 .70000 .80000 .90000 1.0000 X=Case 7 mean= -.04 N=45 standard dev.= .13 0=Case 8 mean= -.03 N=45 standard dev.= .11 +-++-+-++-+-+ +-++-+-++-+ TOTAL 45 (INTERVAL WIDTH= .10000) As Table 10 indicates, the inter-teacher agreement scores for cues collected was around zero, for both cases. This suggests that, on the average, the teachers showed little agreement in collecting cues for a given case. 110 TABLE lO.--Inter-c1inician correlations for cues collected for Cases 7 and 8 and 10 teachers HISTOGRAM/FREQUENCIES CASES = 1-45 MIDPOINT FOR 7.V7 (EACH X=1) -1.0000 -.90000 -.80000 -.70000 -.60000 -.50000 -.40000 -.30000 -.20000 +X -.10000 +XXXXXXXXX00000000 0.0 +XXXXXXXXXXXXXXXX000000000000000000000000 .10000 +XXXXXXXXXXXX0000000000 .20000 +XXXXXX000 .30000 +X .40000 .50000 .60000 .70000 .80000 .90000 1.0000 X=Case 7 mean= .03 N=45 standard dev.= .ll 0=Case 8 mean= .01 N=45 standard dev.=.07 +-++-+-++-+-+ +-+4-+-++-+ TOTAL 45 (INTERVAL WIDTH= .10000) Product measures: Summary The product measures presented in this section suggest the following: 1. Proportional agreement among teachers on both diagnostic judgments made and cues collected was very limited. 2. The most frequently-mentioned diagnostic judgments differed from case to case. This seems reasonable, given that the two cases included different reading 111 problems and different cues. 3. The most frequently-mentioned cues for both cases were those providing background information. This suggests 'that teachers may apply models of students' personal characteristics to explain students' reading achievement. Many of the models teachers use, do not seem to deal directly with reading or reading skills; rather, they center around the socio-psychological aspects of reading behavior. A possible explanation for this observation is that teachers usually take many classes in the socio—psychological areas, without focusing on how they affect reading or reading acquisition. 4. Inter-clinician correlations for both diagnostic judgments and cues collected were near zero, meaning that the teachers showed little agreement on diagnostic judgments or on cues collected for a given case. Results across the two cases were almost the same, suggesting that the teachers employed similar diagnostic processes for both cases. 6. On all three measures, agreement on cues collected was slightly higher than on diagnostic judgments made. That is, the teachers tended to show closer agreement in collecting cues than in making final diagnostic judgments. It must be noted that the results are based on data obtained from a very small number of teachers. But it should 112 also be noted, however, that the findings are highly consistent across the two cases, and that similar studies, involving reading specialists and learning disability personnel, have provided similar results (Van Roekel, 1979; Vinsonhaler, 1979). Even so, replication studies with classroom teachers are needed not only to check the validity of findings from this investigation, but to determine whether simulated cases accurately represent real classroom cases of reading difficulty. Part 2: Process measures A question of interest to the study of clinical problem-solving behavior is how teachers gather information about a case and reach final diagnoses. Specific elements of problem-solving behavior which can be measured and com- pared include length of interaction, number of cues collected on a case, number of final stated diagnoses for a case, and the relationships between cues collected and teachers' verbatim comments about the cues. The following illustration of a process of diagnosis intends to clarify the way teachers' memories and strategies affect these specific elements of the problem solving behavior. As noted at the beginning of this chapter, different teachers may Operate from different memories and strategies. A teacher who operates from a skill-model orientation, for example, will tend to collect and process information in a manner different from that of a teacher 113 Operating from a socio-pyschological orientation. The first teacher may be an inductive reasoner, the second a deductive reasoner. Differences in memories and strategies, the reader will recall, may be attributed, in part, to different kinds of training teachers received. Following is an example of the diagnostic process followed by a teacher who Operates from a socio-psychological orientation. This example is based on the behavior of one of the teachers (no. 7) who participated in this study. After receiving initial contact information about the case, the teacher asked for information pertaining to the child's personality. She explained: "I feel children's personality has a lot to do with how they perceive them- selves. It has an effect on classwork". The teacher listened to a tape of the child reading a passage and observed that "she was a very poor reader and made no effort to sound words". The teacher then asked for information about the child's cultural-home background, and explained: "I wanted to see if she was Black. Accented like a Black. Was surprised she was Spanish. My hunch: she's under stress. Might be emotional/social problem, not an intellectual one". The next cue the teacher requested was an audio tape on which the child read the words "Pittsburgh" and "Pennsylvania" (this tested the child's auditory-sound blending). The teacher commented: "She didn't seem to be able to sound out and blend. (However), 114 sometimes English-as-a-second-language-kids have this problem. (So), it might be a waste of time to do phonics -- they don't have the same sounds. Silly to try and get them to do it." At this point, having collected four cues, the teacher was not sure what else to ask for. She decided to look at Test Results and "was very surprised at how well (the child) did on the tests." The teacher collected two more cues (written questions and answers for silent reading, and the word ”picnic" in student sentence) after which she said: "I'm getting swamped here with all this information. I'm surprised she did as well on tests as she did. Most of her problem might be emotional/social." The teacher asked for one last one -- the eighth (comprehension question following a silent reading) and said: "Some children can read silently and comprehend -- that's O.K." The teacher concluded the session with two stated diagnostic judgments about the case: (1) that emotional or social problems might have been hampering the student's school work, and (2) that she was insecure about herself. This example of a process of diagnosis illustrated some of the elements of teachers' diagnostic processes which can be measured and compared. As mentioned at the beginning of this section, among those elements are length of interaction with a case, number of cues collected, and number of final stated diagnoses made by the teachers. 115 Teachers' performance on the two cases Teachers' performance in terms of length of interaction, number of cues collected, and number of diagnostic judgments: Examining teachers' performances in terms of these variables is one way the researcher can explore some of the characteristics of the teachers' clinical problem-solving behavior. Analyses of process measures for this study were based on data obtained during both the observational and the debriefing sessions. Length of interaction Table 11 shows the length of time the teachers interacted with Cases 7 and 8. The first column in the table represents the length of the interaction, in minutes; the second column shows the frequency count for each of the time variables. The table indicates, for example, that two teachers interacted with Case 7 for 40 minutes, and two interacted with Case 8 for 40 minutes (See Appendix G for raw data on measures of time, cues collected, and diagnostic judgments for the 10 teachers on the two cases.) As the table shows, different teachers interacted with the cases for different amounts of time. Length of interaction ranged from a minimum of about 25 minutes to a maximum of 60 minutes (arbitrary cut-off time), with a mean 116 TABLE ll.--Length of interaction for Cases 7 and 8 and 10 teachers HISTOGRAM/FREQUENCIES CASE = 1-10 MIDPOINT FOR 1. TIME (EACH X= 1) 0. + 5.0000 + X= Case 7 mean= 41.3 10.000 + N=10 15.000 + 20.000 + 0= Case 8 mean= 48.1 25.000 +XXO N=10 30.000 + 35.000 +XX 40.000 +XX00 45.000 +000 50.000 +XX 55.000 +X0 60.000 +X000 TOTAL 10 (INTERVAL WIDTH= 5.0000) time of 41.3 minutes for Case 7, and 48.1 minutes, for Case 8. This fihding suggests that, as with most other psycho- logical variables (which were investigated in various studies), teachers' interactions with simulated cases reveal marked individual differences. Number of cues collected Similar individual differences were found in the number of cues the teachers collected. Table 12 displays this information. The first column in the table gives the number of cues collected; the second the frequency count (i.e., number 117 TABLE 12.--Number of cues collected on Cases 7 and 8 and 10 teachers HISTOGRAM/FREQUENCIES CASES=CASE 1-10 MIDPOINT FOR 1.CUE7 (EACH X= 1) 0. + X= Case 7 mean= 42.3 10.000 +0 N=10 20.000 +X 30.000 +X0 = case 8 mean= 51.4 40.000 +XXXXXO N=10 50.000 +XX00 60.000 +00 70.000 +00 80.000 +0 90.000 +X 100.00 + TOTAL 10 (INTERVAL WIDTH= 10.000) of teachers) for each of these cue values. For example, one teacher collected about 30 cues for Case 7, and one collected 30 for Case 8. As the table shows, different teachers collected different numbers of cues, from a minimum of about 10 to a maximum of about 90. In other words, the teachers tended to collect different amounts of information to reach diagnostic judgments. These differences might have stemmed from the different models the teachers used (if they used any). A teacher operating from a comprehensive model was likely to gather more information about the case than one operating from a non-comprehensive model. Indeed, teacher No. 7, 118 who seemed to operate from a socio-psychological (non- comprehensive) model, collected the fewest cues of all teachers (17 for Case 7; 12 for Case 8), and made the least number of diagnostic judgments (3 for Case 7, 2 for Case 8). Her diagnostic judgments for Case 8 dealt only with emotional problems that might have affected the child's reading performance (It is interesting to note that, during the observational session, this teacher wanted to stOp gathering information about the case after having collected only 10 cues. She explained: "I think I know what the problem is; I already made up my mind"). Number of diagnostic judgments Table 13 shows the number of diagnostic judgments the teachers made for Cases 7 and 8. The first column represents the number of diagnostic judgments made; the second column shows the frequency count for each of these values. For example, two teachers made four judgments for Case 7, and one made four diagnostic judgments for Case 8. As Table 13 indicates, the teachers differed in the number of diagnostic judgments they made for a given case. The number of judgments ranged from 2 (made by one teacher for Case 8) to 14 (made by two teachers for Case 8). As in the case of cues collected, these differences might have been caused by the different models of reading and diagnosis from which the teachers operated. 119 TABLE 13.--Number of diagnostic judgments for Cases 7 and 8 made by 10 teachers HISTOGRAM/FREQUENCES CASES = 1-10 MIDPOINT FOR l.DIAGNOSE (EACH X= l) 0. + X= Case 7 mean= 7.2 2.0000 +0 N=10 4.0000 +XXO 6.0000 +XX 0: Case 8 mean= 9.0 8.0000 +XXXO N=10 10.000 +XX000 12.000 +X00 14.000 +00 16.000 + 18.000 + 20.000 + TOTAL 10 (INTERVAL WIDTH= 8.0000) As with length of interaction and number of cues collected, Table 13 also suggests that similar individual differences existed in teachers in terms of the number of diagnostic judgments they made for a case. Summary The data presented in Table 11 through 13 suggest that the teachers differed significantly from one another in the length of time they interacted with the simulated cases, the number of cues they collected, and the number of diagnostic judgments they made. 120 Since differences among the teachers were observed for each of these variables, it might be worth asking what relationships existed among these variables. Table 14 summarizes the mean correlations among length of interaction, number of cues collected, and number of diagnostic judgments for Cases 7 and 8. TABLE l4.--Correlations among length of interaction, number of cues collected and number of diagnostic judgments for Cases 7 and 8 and 10 teachers TIME 1.0000 (7)CUES .4942 1.0000 DIAGNOSES .4650 .6109 1.0000 TIME .4970 .5120 .4574 1.0000 (8)CUES .5172 .4679 .1450 .3278 1.0000 DIAGNOSES .5381 .5390 .9672 .4614 .2421 1.0000 TIME CUES DIAGNOSES TIME CUES DIAGNOSES CASE 7 CASE 8 Because Of the small number of teachers involved, no generalizations can be drawn from these data. Descriptive statistics, however, suggest that, in this study, the following observed relationships existed: 1. Between length of interaction with a case and number of cues collected (.49 for Case 7, .32 for Case 8). On 121 the average, the more time teachers interacted with a case, the more cues they collected. Between length of interaction and number of diagnostic judgments made (.46 for Case 7 and Case 8, respectively). On the average, the longer teachers interacted with a case, the more diagnostic judgments they made. Between number of cues collected and number of diagnostic judgments made (.61 for Case 7, .24 for Case 8). In general, the more cues teachers collected for a given case, the more diagnostic judgments they made. (The marked differences in mean values between Cases 7 and 8 are probably due to differences in the number and kinds of cues provided. Case 8 included more items of information and more varied types of information than Case 7. Thus, to attempt a diagnosis on Case 8, the teachers had to collect more cues than they did for Case 7.) Between length of interaction with Case 7 and length of interaction with Case 8 (.49). Most teachers interacted with the two cases for about the same length of time. Between total number of cues collected for Case 7 and total collected for Case 8 (.46). Teachers who collected many cues for one case tended to collect many for the other as well; conversely, teachers who collected few cues for one case also tended to collect few for the other. These differences among teachers in the number of 122 cues they collected might be explained by the Agreement Corollary. It may well be that different memories or models from which the teachers operated prompted them to collect different amounts of information. A skill-oriented teacher, for example, might have felt it necessary to collect a large amount of data to check the specific skills the child lacked. A teacher operating from a socio- psychological model, on the other hand, might have based his/her diagnosis on few cues, gathered in the narrow area of psychological and social background. Differences in the teachers' personal characteristics (such as self-confidence, independence/dependence, etc.) might also have accounted for the differences in their diagnostic performances. Some peOple will always collect a large amount of information on which to base a decision, while others will use relatively little data. 6. Between number of diagnostic judgments made for one case and number of diagnostic judgments made for another (.96). Teachers tended to make approximately the same number of judgments for each case, relative to each other. The explanation for this observation might also lie in the Agreement Corollary. The memories and models from which teachers operate affect the number of judgments they make. A teacher operating from a psycho-social model, for example, will tend to entertain a limited number of 123 diagnoses about a case. The same is true of a teacher who operates from a skill-model orientation. Consistent use of one distinct model probably leads the teachers to make similar diagnoses for different cases of reading difficulties. It is logical that, if the teachers make similar cross-case diagnoses, they entertain about the same number of diagnostic judgments. The data presented in this section suggest that, although teachers differed from one another in their diagnostic performances on the three variables, they were fairly consistent in their own interactions with the two cases of reading difficulty. Analysis of the relationships between cues collected and teachersTiverbatim comments about these cues While interacting with the simulated cases, the teachers were asked to verbalize their thoughts about the information they collected. They were asked to do this again during the debriefing session. Their verbatim statements were audiotaped and transcribed by a clinical observer. Data analysis explored the relationships between cues collected and the teachers' comments about the cues. The examination of these relationships was guided by a basic question: What diagnostic strategies did the 10 teachers employ? This question was approached in two ways: (1) through analysis of quantitative features of teachers' diagnostic processes, and (2) through analysis 124 of the types of verbatim comments the teachers made about the cues. The data selected for analysis were those collected during the teachers' initial diagnostic interactions (five with Case 7 and five with Case 8). The researcher felt that the experience of interacting with the first simulated case might have altered the manner in which teachers dealt with the second case (session 1 served as a training session for session 2), and he preferred to focus on the teachers' initial processes of gathering and evaluating information about the reading difficulties. Table 15 lists three of the quantitative features of the teachers' diagnostic processes -- (1) number of verbatim comments made, (2) average cue percent time (i.e., proportion of time elapsed when half of cues were collected), and (3) average verbatim percent time (i.e., proportion of time elapsed when half of verbatim comments were made). As Table 15 suggests, marked individual differences existed among the teachers in relation to number of verbatim comments they made about the cues they gathered and the case. They could have made from zero to l or more comments about each of the cues. The table also indicates that, for both cases, teachers tended to collect cues throughout the interaction. They also tended to talk and express their thoughts about the case in relation to the cues collected throughout the interaction. As the table shows, on the average, about 125 TABLE lS.--Number of verbatim comments, mean cue percent time and verbatim percent time for Cases 7 and 8 and 5 teachers STATISTIC TEACHERS *1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 X Number of verbatim comments 59 82 60 78 85 72.8 (7) Average cue percent time .535 .637 .513 .635 .625 .589 Average verbatim percent time .470 .565 .402 .521 .484 .488 Number of verbatim comments 50 41 64 34 69 50.6 Average cue percent time .513 .655 .465 .630 .493 .511 (8) Average verbatim percent time .518 .578 .471 .565 .509 .528 half of the cues were collected after 60 percent (.589) of the session with Case 7, and after 55 percent (.551) of the session with Case 8. This means that a somewhat larger number of cues was collected during the second half of the observational sessions than during the first half. This may suggest that, as the teachers felt time was running short, they accelerated the rate at which they gathered information about the case. 126 The teachers' cognitive or diagnostic processes were also explored through analysis of the types of verbatim comments they made about the cues they gathered during the interactions and the debriefing sessions that followed. This anlaysis provided data about some of the charac— teristics of the teachers' clinical problem-solving behavior, including: (1) how much they observed specific reading behaviors, and (2) whether they posed and tested hypotheses about the cases' problems. Figure 3 shows the percentage of verbatim comments and their relationships to cues. This figure shows the distribution of teachers' verbatim comments among 10 different types of possible relationships between cues and statements. The verticle line represents the percentage of verbatim comments made by all 10 teachers for Cases 7 and 8. The horizontal line denotes the types of relationships which existed between cues and statements. (See pages 86-87 for description of each of these relations. The data indicates that: l. The teachers did not formulate hypotheses about the reading difficulties of a given case. That is, their data-gathering was not oriented toward confirmation or disconfirmation of hypotheses about the case's problems. Only seven percent of the teachers' total verbatim comments dealt with hypotheses. 2. The teachers tended to observe rather than assess or evaluate students' reading difficuties. Twenty-four Percent of verbatim comments made by 10 teachers for Cases 7 & 8 127 V 100 1 95' 90- 85. 80. N=617 28X 20 1 271 242 . F'—‘—1' 5 11 """"‘"'“ 1% 62 6E 0.0: 4: . 32 V r V I f I W I l 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 confirm. neither. generali- strategy remedia- hunch zation tion disconfirm specific other no relation philosophy hunch. comment to one of reading I 7x I 33: on 10: ‘ ] statements about statements about other hypotheses cues Type of verbatim comments Figure 3.--Cue to statements relationships 128 percent of the statements related specifically to the cue collected (relation type 4). Although the teachers did relate several observations to one another (relation type 5 - twenty-eight percent), they did not go beyond the mere observation level (to a diagnostic level). The teachers tended to talk about what they did as they interacted with a case (twenty-seven percent of their statements self-described the way in which they gathered information). They did not, however, describe how they reached diagnostic judgments. Process measures: Summary The process measures presented in this section revealed the following: 1. The teachers differed significantly in terms of how long they interacted with the simulated cases, the number of cues they collected, and the number of diagnostic judgments they made. There were observed relationships among length of interaction, number of cues collected, and number of diagnostic judgments. The relationships held for interactions with both cases. The teachers were inferential problem solvers, to the degree that they problem solved at all. They did not pose and test hypotheses about the cases, and did not gather information to confirm or disconfirm hunches. 129 Rather, they gathered data and commented about it. They were observers of children's behaviors, but they did not process the observed information in a manner that led to accurate and complete diagnostic judgments. Part 3: Classroom interviews The main purpose of the classroom interviews was to explore teachers' diagnostic skills in the natural classroom environment and to investigate the relationships between these diagnostic skills and those exhibited in the laboratory situations. The interviews were intended to help provide answers to two questions: (1) Is the examination of teachers' interactions with simulated cases in Laboratory situations a valid means of studying teachers' diagnostic skills? and (2) What are some of the diagnostic processes employed by teachers in the natural classroom environment? This part of the investigation was approached in two ways: (1) through analysis of the diagnostic categories most frequently mentioned by teachers in the classroom, and (2) through analysis of the similarities and differences between diagnostic categories mentioned in the Laboratory and the classroom situations. Following a discussion of the results of these analyses, several other findings, based on a few other interview questions, are discussed. 130 What are the diagnostic categories most frequently mentioned by teachers in the classroom? During the classroom interviews, the teachers discussed various elements of reading to which they attend when reaching diagnostic decisions about students' reading performances. A domain of these diagnostic categories was created, and redundancies were eliminated. The final list contained six specific categories, grouped into 6 general classes: Comprehension, Vocabulary, Word recognition, Word attack, Oral reading and Student's characteristics (See Appendix H: Diagnostic domain based on classroom interviews). A question of interest was, what proportion of teachers mentioned each category. This information is important because it might give some idea of which diagnostic categories teachers most frequently deal with, or think of. Table 16 shows the diagnostic categories most frequently mentioned in the classroom interviews. The first column in the table lists the diagnostic categories mentioned by at least 30 percent of the teachers. The second column shows the exact percentage of teachers who mentioned each category. For example, "Sight Vocabulary" was mentioned by 100 percent of the teachers (n=10), whereas "Blends” was mentioned by only 30 percent (n=3). A close look at these data about the diagnostic categories reveals that: 131 TABLE l6.--Twenty—eight most frequently mentioned diagnostic categories in the classroom (Total of 96) PROPORTION OF TEACHERS MEN- TIONING THE DIAGNOSTIC CATEGORIES CATEGORIES Sight vocabulary 1.00 Comprehension .90 Silent reading comprehension .80 Oral reading .80 Oral reading comprehension .70 Word recognition .70 Vocabulary .60 Endings .60 Context .60 Listening comprehension .50 Word analysis .50 Decoding .50 Phonetic analysis .50 Beginning sounds .50 Short vowels .50 Word attack .40 Chunking .40 Sounds .40 Word family approach .40 Able to sound out .40 Phonetic skills .40 Print translation .40 Substitutions .40 Factual and inferential comprehension .30 Vocabulary meaning .30 Guesses .30 Ending Sounds .30 Blends .30 132 1. Five of the six general categories composing the total diagnostic domain (Comprehension, Vocabulary, Word recognition, Word attack, and Oral reading) were mentioned by at least three teachers; however, different teachers discussed different sub-skills under each of these categories. 2. Those categories mentioned by at least 70 percent of the teachers were very general: Sight vocabulary, Comprehension, Silent reading comprehension, Oral reading, Oral reading comprehension and Word recognition, This suggests that the teachers rely on the same general diagnostic categories in reaching decisions. The experimental design might have been partially responsible for this finding. All ten teachers were given the same descriptions of reading difficulties, and these descriptions may have cued them to mention similar general diagnostic categories. The teachers seemed to differ, however, from one another, on the sub-skills within each of the general categories (e.g., miscues, skip words, punctuation, etc.). Figure 4 shows the percentage of total diagnostic categories (n=96) mentioned by the teacehrs in the classroom interviews. The verticle line gives the percentage of diagnostic categories mentioned, and the horizontal line shows the number of teachers who mentioned the categories. For example, 133 N = 96 mean = 2.32 75. 654 501 554 50. 45. 404 35. 30. 25. 20 15 10 Percent of diagnostic categories mentioned \I 1? 5.- v ““‘fi--t$ I T 9 10 I I I r I I l 2 3 4 5 6 ‘34!) 9°40 Number of teachers mentioning category Figure 4.--Percent of the diagnostic categories mentioned by teachers in the classroom 134 52 percent of the categories were mentioned by only one teacher, and 1 percent were mentioned by nine and ten teachers, respectively. It should be noted, however, that teachers' agreement on diagnostic categories mentioned in the classroom interviews is slightly higher than their diagnostic agreement for Cases 7 and 8 (Figure 1). This is so because, during the interviews, the teachers tended to make general statements, whereas during the laboratory interactions with the cases they were more case-specific. It must be pointed out that the classroom interviews were based on four descriptions of deficient readers which were given to the teachers. The teachers were asked to match two of these descriptions with students in their own classrooms. As one might expect, the teachers found differences among the described readers, and made different diagnostic judgments about them. The number of teachers who matched readers in their own classrooms with the descriptions varied from seven (70 percent), for the description of "Michelle", to three, for the description of "Diane" (Appendix E lists the number of teachers who noted similarities between children in the descriptions and children in their own classrooms). 135 Similarities and differences between teachers' diagnostic categories In laBOratory and classroom situations After the domain of diagnostic categories (judgments) mentioned by the teachers in the classroom was compiled, it was compared with the domain of judgments made by the teachers in the laboratory setting. This was done for two reasons: 1. It was hOped the comparison might point to the existence or absence of a model of reading held by the teachers, or at least, to a consistency or inconsistency in their thinking about reading problems. If similar diagnostic categories are mentioned by teachers in both laboratory and classroom situations, it may suggest that they operate from a model, or from a certain"memory" which contains these cateOgires. Checking for similarities and differences between diagnostic categories mentioned in the laboratory situations and those mentioned in the classroom was one way of exploring whether carefully-desi observational studies in laboratory settings are reliable and valid means of investigating the clinical problem-solving behavior of teachers. Similarities among the categories, it was thought, would suggest that teachers perform in both situations in a similar manner, or at least from the same model or with the same memory. 136 A careful look at the data reveals that, on the average, 50 percent of the diagnostic categories mentioned by the teachers in laboratory settings were also mentioned during classroom interviews (See Appendix F). Table 17 shows the numbers of diagnostic categories mentioned by each teacher in laboratory setting (for the two simulated cases) and during classroom interviews. TABLE l7.--Number of diagnostic categories mentioned by teachers in laboratory and classroom situations CONDITION TEACHERS l 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 X Laboratory: Cases 7 & 8 13 15 24 21 24 6 5 l9 17 18 .16.2 Classroom 30 18 22 24 19 26 22 26 17 26 23.0 Number of categories mentioned in both 8 9 13 13 6 5 3 8 7 11 8.5 laboratory and class- room As the table indicates, the number of diagnostic categories mentioned by the teachers in the classroom outnumbered the number of diagnoses made in the laboratory. This may suggest that the domain of diagnostic categories simulated by the two simulated cases was more limited than 137 that which evolved from the natural classroom setting. Looking carefully at the kinds of diagnostic statements mentioned in both the laboratory and the classroom (Appendix F), we can see that the statements tended to fall into two categories: 1. general statements, such as: "Vocabulary concepts: knows major concepts"; "Sight vocabulary: good". In other words, these are general statements about the reading act which do not require careful and detailed diagnosis of a child's reading difficulties; statements which seem to be characteristic of individual teachers rather than individual students, such as: "Insecure about herself"; "the manifestations of her problems are dialectical based". In other words, some statements about the reading act seem to emerge primarily from the teacher's philosophical orientation (or model) about the causes of reading difficulty, rather than from some specific aspect of the child's reading performance. (An extreme example was the set of statements made by teacher no. 7, who, in both laboratory and classroom situations, emphasized, above all, that children's emotional problems and self—concept are the main causes of reading deficiency. At the opposite extreme was teacher no. 3, who, in both settings, conducted thorough and detailed diagnoses and considered many variables as potential causes of the reading difficulty). 138 Diagnostic judgments mentioned in either laboratOEy or classroom situatIOn Tbut not in both) Diagnostic categories mentioned in the laboratory but not during classroom interviews (see Appendix F) tended to be very case specific. They applied directly to the simulated case the teacher was attempting to diagnose, and frequently consisted of some sub-skills of the reading act applicable to this case (e.g., "punctuation: does not pay attention to";"syllabication skills: poor"; "plural endings: poor," etc.). During classroom interviews, when the teachers discussed either the individual students in their classroom or their diagnostic practices in general, specific sub-skills, which were case specific, were not mentioned. Diagnostic statements mentioned in the classroom but not in the laboratory settings (See Appendix H) tended to fall into two categories: 1. Case specific statements, i.e., statements related directly to the individual student whom the teacher was describing during the interview. Especially prevalent were statements which related to the child's factors. 2. General statements, usually mentioned by the teacher as she described her instructional and diagnostic practices in reading (e.g., syllables, compound words, decoding). These were not mentioned in the laboratory setting because the interactions with simulated cases forced the teachers to be as specific as possible in their diagnoses. 139 The data presented in Table 18 show the diagnostic categories most frequently mentioned in the laboratory and classroom situations. TABLE 18.--Most frequently mentioned diagnostic categories in laboratory and classroom settings DIAGNOSTIC CATEGORIES LABORATORY CLASSROOM Case 7 Case 8 Children in specific specific general Sight vocabulary 5 7 (12) 10 Comprehension 2 4 ( 6) 9 Oral reading 4 3 ( 7) 8 Silent reading comprehension 0 0 ( 0) 8 Oral reading comprehension l 2 ( 3) 7 Word recognition 5 l ( 6) 7 Endings 5 0 ( 5) 6 Context 2 l ( 3) 6 VOcabulary 3 3 ( 6) 6 Beginning sounds l 2 ( 3) 5 Short vowels 0 1 ( l) 5 Listening comprehension 0 2 ( 2) 5 Decoding 0 0 ( 0) 5 Print translation 0 3 ( 3) 4 Phonetic skills 2 3 ( 5) 4 Word attack 2 3 ( 5) 4 Chunking 2 l ( 3) 4 Word family approach 0 0 ( 0) 4 Factual & inferential comp. l 2 ( 3) 3 Vocabulary meaning 2 3 ( 5) 3 Guesses 2 2 ( 4) 3 Ending sounds 0 l ( l) 3 Blends 0 2 ( 2) 3 These data indicate that, in general, the diagnostic categories mentioned most often in the laboratory were also mentioned (and usually more frequently) during the classroom 140 interviews. This may suggest that teachers think of these general reading categories in both laboratory situations and the natural classroom environment. The few differences which were noted between the categories the teachers entertained in the laboratory setting and in the classroom (i.e., Silent reading comprehension, Decoding, and Word family approach) might have stemmed from the fact that the Laboratory interactions were case-specific, and thus discouraged the teachers from considering the diagnostic categories they would consider in the classroom. In summary, looking at similarities and differences between diagnostic statements mentioned in the laboratory setting and those mentioned in the classroom, we noted that the diagnostic categories most frequently mentioned in the laboratory were also mentioned in the classroom. Those which were mentioned in only one setting tended to be either (1) case specific to the simulated case, or to the individual student described by the teacher in the classroom, or (2) general comments, usually about the instructional and diagnostic practices the teachers mentioned during classroom interviews. Overall, (when one excludes these two types of diagnostic categories), the data indicate that teachers appear to rely on similar diagnostic categories in both laboratory and classroom settings. This suggests that whatever the diagnostic "memory" teachers have, it is dominant in their interactions with cases of reading dif- ficulties, whether these interactions take place in a 141 laboratory setting or in the classroom. Other findings As stated (p. 129), the interviews with the teachers included several additional questions related to reading diagnosis and remediation;. the teachers' responses to these questions are discussed below. An important question about the processes of reading diagnosis and remediation is whether teachers Offer differentiated remediation plans for different reading problems or different children. The question is important because it deepends the investigation into teachers' memories and their functional models of reading diagnosis. One can expect that, the more complete the memories teachers have, and the more complete and sophisticated their models of reading are, the more differentiated the remediations they offer will be. These different remediations will depend on the specifics of the reading characteristics exhibited by the different students the teachers deal with. If the teachers' memories are not intact, and if they either lack a model of reading or have an incomplete one, they will probably offer similar (or standardized) remediation plans for all cases of reading difficulty they encounter. Following is a list of remedial plans mentioned by each of the 10 teachers during the classroom interviews. It is composed of only those remediations which were mentioned 142 for both of the cases whose reading difficulties the teachers addressed: Teacher Teacher Teacher Teacher Teacher Teacher Teacher 1: 2: 3: 5: 6: Work on phonetic skills. a) b) a) b) C) d) a) b) Take words out of context and see how the child can handle it. Encourage the child to work. Work on beginning, middle and ending sounds. Have the child work with an aid on short- paragraph stories. Have the child go to a reading specialist for half an hour each day. We'll work on short paragraphs with many questions. Work on print translation. Focus on context until the child realizes that things make sense. A lot of practice. a) b) a) b) C) d) e) Work on vocabulary. Spend time with him on an individual basis. Try to get the child to read words he/she cannot sound out. Send words home, ask mother or father to help the child. Ask the child to look at the picture. He/she needs to pay more attention. Have games with words. 143 Teacher 8: a) Give the child additional material to read. b) Have him/her read on a one-to-one basis. c) Have him/her work on worksheets. Teacher 9: a) Send the child to a reading teacher. b) Do more individualized work with the child on a one-to-one basis. Teacher 10:a) Encourage the child to do more by himself. b) Encourage the child to ask for help. c) Send the child to a reading lab. An examination of this list of remediations reveals the following: 1. Most of the recommended remediations are very general, such as: "Work on print translation"; "work on phonetic skills", etc. Many of these remediations call for additional practice rather than dealing with the specifics of the problems, e.g.: "He needs a lot of practice"; "give him additional material to read"; "encourage him to work", etC. Much of the practice prescribed is carried out on an individual basis with the teacher. Here agin, the remediation is expressed in very general terms: "He will work with an aid on short paragraph stories"; "spend time with him on an individual basis" (6); ”do more individualized work with the child" (9); "have him read on a one-to-one basis" (8). 144 The data obtained from the list of all remedial strategies mentioned by the teachers (whether applicable to the two cases they described or to only one of them) not only support the notion of a "general remediation" phenomenon, but reveals three additional points: 1. At least seven (70 percent) of the ten teachers stressed psychological characteristics of the child: "I didn't hassle her" (3); "encourage her to be brave" (4); "make him feel comfortable about himself" (6), etc. Thus, it appears that these teachers tend to emphasize personal factors which might affect the reading act, rather than offering specific remedial strategies to overcome the problems. At least five (50 percent) of the teachers said that they send students like those described to reading specialists for remediation (or that they at least confer with specialists). Again, the teachers do this in lieu of offering some specific remedial techniques. At least four (40 percent) of the teachers, when asked about remediation for a specific area of reading difficulty, such as word recognition or word analysis, said that they do not know what to do ["I don't know where I'm going from here" (1); "I would have known at least what not to do" (2); "the problem was never really remediated" (3); "I don't know" (5)]. 145 Summary: The remediation plans offered by the teachers for the cases of reading difficulty reveal three general trends: 1. The teachers seem to offer remediation at a very global level. Emphasis is placed, specifically, on practice, drill, and individualized help from others. 2. The teachers often refer students to reading specialists. 3. They try to help the students overcome some perceived personal problems. If these are the major remediation plans offered by teachers for cases of reading difficulty, it must be asked how successful are they in remeidating children with reading problems in their classrooms? Following are the teachers' responses, in their own words, to the question: How do you know when and if you have remediated a child's reading problem? (Question No. 12 in the interview) Teacher 1: "I feel that if a child is able to read orally a page, or silently, and be able to recognize or to sound out words and at least answer little questions...the child is on his way to really learning to read." "I never felt that I've (remediated). I don't know. I never reached the point where I felt a child has been remediated. I can see improvement perhaps, or growth, but I don't know of anyone that's ever reached that point." Teacher 2 "When I see a change of attitude, that they are liking reading, beginning to participate in class work, raising their hand, answering questions better, when things like initial sounds (that were difficult for them at the beginning) become easy, and the testing in general." Teacher 3 Teacher 4: Teacher 5 Teacher 6: Teacher 7: Teacher 8: Teacher 9: Teacher 10: 146 "I start to hear it: a) when a kid starts to make sense; b) when (a child) starts to make pretty close graphic or graph0phonemics correspondences: c) when the kid starts to pull all things together." "a) If they can do exercise or practice after a lesson; b) I guess with a number of kids I'm aware of some specific problems that they had and so I try to notice if they still have them." "For some you can never remediate, that's just their ability. For the average child that has difficulty just a lot of repetition and drill." "I have work sheets that I give out in the afternoon and usually they correspond with something the child has read about. If they can do the worksheets then I assume the problem has been remediated." "a) When it comes time to change books or through a test that I may give at the end of the book and see how well they do; b) I try to see if a child can orally read a page without any or few mistakes. I (also) check back in his workbook and see if he made a lot of mistakes." "I don't know if I've ever run into that. I guess any kid that I've had that's really been a low reader has still been a low reader at the end of the year." "In terms of specific skills: the reading lab has individual tests for different objectives at different grade levels. So after the child has special instruction in a reading skill they get this test and if they get them all right then they figure the child knows the skill. If he misses -- they go back and reinstruct and have more work sheets." Several characteristics of these responses stand out: 1. They range from general statements (e.g., "when the kid starts to pull all things together" (4) to more specific ones (e.g., "if they are able to recognize or sound out words" (1) . 147 2. They range from statements about non-measured attitudes (e.g., "if I see a change of attitude, that they are liking reading" (3) to statements about observed and measured performance (e.g., "if they can do the worksheets" (7). 3. They range from statements which express teacher's confidence in remediations (e.g., "I start to hear it" (4), to those which suggest a lack of confidence in remediation ("I never felt that I've remediated" (2). Three of the ten teachers expressed a similar lack of confidence in their ability to remediate reading problems. Teachers' responses to specific interview ggestions: QuestiOn No. 8: What overall methods do teachers use in the classroom to find out chiIdren's reading performance? Teachers' responses to this question indicated that, in general, there are two general approaches to assessing a child's reading performance: (1) to consider his or her grouping from the previous year, with some additional testing (3,9,10); and (2) to listen to the child read a passage (1,4,5,6,7,8,10) and to pay special attention to comprehension and vocabulary. Almost never are specific tests or detailed diagnostic techniques mentioned. Question No. 9: How do teachers teach reading in their classrooms? VOcabulary and comprehension, which were emphasized in answers to the previous question, were also emphasized in 148 the teachers' responses to this question. These two aspects of reading are apparently central to the reading programs of all 10 teachers. Specific strategies for teaching reading were rarely mentioned; most teachers discussed what and how they teach only in global terms (exceptions were teachers 6 and 9 who outlined, more systematically, the different activities they use in a typical reading lesson). This lack of specificity might be attributed to two factors: (1) failure of the interview to probe the more specific steps involved in reading instruction, and (2) absence of a model which enables teachers to more thoroughly formulate for themselves (and, thus, to describe to others) the act of reading and its various components. Question No. 11: Do teachers individualize readingrinstrucEIOn? Most of the teachers said they do not individualize instruction. Teacher No. 3 said she would individualize only for readers in the lowest group, but not for those in the top and middle groups. Teacher No. 8 emphasized that, although she does not individualize instruction, she does individualize the workbooks. Teacher No. 10 was the only one to say that she individualizes reading instruction "almost completely". These responses seem to contradict some of the findings discussed earlier, based on data obtained during the 149 initial part of the interview, when the teachers were asked to talk about their remedial practices. As stated (p. 143), at least three of the teachers (6,8,9) said they work with students on an individual basis. There is a two-part explanation of the teachers' apparent inconsistency: 1. Usually, the teachers did not think of themselves as individualizing reading instruction. However, during the interviews, when they were encouraged to focus on an individual child, they noticed that they sometimes do, in fact, work with him/her on an individual basis. 2. Although the teachers do not individualize reading instruction, they do individualize remedial reading. In other words, there is a distinction between instruction and remediation. Instruction is usually provided to the class as a whole (or to groups). However, those in need of remediation work with the teacher on an individual basis. Question No. 13: How do teachers know when to refer a child to a reading specialiEt? Teachers' responses to this question can be divided into two: Three teachers indicated that they do not refer the poor reader to a reading specialist. They said they feel they are qualified to help the student overcome his/her problems (4,6,11). 150 Of the seven teachers who said they do refer students to specialists, six explained that they make the move when they feel they have done everything they can with the students, but the reading problems persist. The seventh teacher in this group, no. 9, said she does not know much about reading remediation ("even though I teach reading every day"), so she refers children after only a week or so. (The six others indicated that they spend more time collecting informal data about the reader before deciding that she/he must be referred. Teacher no. 3 stood out in this regard. She said she waits until January to see whether a child has learned anything before sending him/ her to the specialist.) Question No. 14: What do teachers think the reading specialist should do? Six of the ten teachers said that the specialist should work with teachers -- as a consultant -- on the diagnosis and remediation of reading problems. One teacher (no. 3) even claimed that, in many cases, this consultation should take the place of work between student and specialist. She explained that there are too many students with reading difficulties for the reading specialist possibility to accommodate. At least four teachers said that the reading specialist should conduct diagnoses. Some explained that they, as teachers, do not have the expertise to do 151 sufficiently thorough diagnoses. One teacher (no. 4) called upon the reading specialist to do a "decent and accurate diagnoses", instead of "employing standardized testing procedures with all children". Furthermore, this teacher said the reading specialist should "address instruction to remediate the deficit", instead of recommending some standardized remediation (e.g., "every- body should do phonics"). Five teachers (5,7,8,9,10) said that the reading specialist should also work on remediation, particularly with students in great need of help. An important comment made by teacher no. 9 was that the specialist should also work with gifted children. Overall, the teachers identified several duties and responsibilitieis of the reading specialist: 1. They agreed that the specialist is an important resource person in the school. (Some teachers said they wished there could be even more specialists in the school.) 2. They said the specialist should divide his/her time between students and teachers; the teachers explained that students are not the only ones who need help in reading related activities. 3. They suggested that, because there are more deficient readers than the specialist can accommodate, he/she should work with only those students who are in great need of help. This suggestion is consistent with two findings reported earlier: 152 a. That even when instruction is not indi- vidualized in the classroom, remediation is (p. 149). b. That most teachers refer students to a specialist only when they feel they have done everything possible for the students, but have not succeeded in remediating the problems (p. 150). Question No. 15: What do teachers feel are their responsibilities as reading teachers? Because teacher's responses to this question were diverse and highly interesting, they are provided verbatim: Teacher 1: "Getting the children who are below grade level as near to grade level as possible. Those who are at grade level -- to enrich what they already have." Teacher 2: "Provide the child with appreciation for reading. Develop in children the sense that reading tasks vary from pleasurable reading to reading for specific purposes. Encourage students to be involved with the reading act. Provide extra assistance to students who are showing some prob- lems." Teacher 3: "Get children to like reading. Get them to become used to the system ("there is a system in reading, once they become comfortable with it, they have learned to like to read")." Teacher 4: "Work on inferential understanding. Build the children's vocabulary. Get the children to understand that they are reading for two reasons: to get information and to enjoy it." Teacher 5: "To be aware of specific problems. To individu- alize instruction. To get through the books by June." Teacher 6: "To do the best in teaching kids how to read." 153 Teacher 7: "To teach dhilren to read at a fundamental level. Teacher 8: "That at the end of the year a majority of the children can go into a third year with no problem." Teacher 9: "Give the children positive attitude toward reading. Introducing them to different kinds of reading materials. Help them feel successful in whatever they are in their reading (even if they are below grade level). Make sure they have the skills taught by the materials or program we use." Teacher 10:"To determine where the kids are in terms of reading skills and level when they come to me. Make sure that they are moving from there at a pace that seems apprOpriate for each of them." Several characteristics of the teachers' answers are apparant: a) b) C) They range from very general statements (e.g. "to do the best in teaching kids how to read" -- teacher (6) to more specific ones (e.g., "build the children's vocabulary" -- teacher (4). They sometimes deal with students in general (e.g., "to teach children to read at a fundamental level" -- teacher (7) and sometimes with individual children (e.g., "to individualize instruction" -- teacher 5). They range from specific statements about reading activities at the cognitive level (e.g., "work on inferential understanding" (4) to more general statements about the affective domain (e.g., "get children to like reading" (3). 154 d) Several make no reference to the final product, or results (e.g., "encourage children to be involved with the reading act" (2), while others do take such things into consideration (e.g., "that at the end of the year a majority of children can go into a third year" (8). It is interesting to note, in this context, that one teacher (no. 5), who had as one objective "to get through the books by June" later said that the interview made her aware that this objective is notso important. It is more important, she said, to help children overcome their reading difficulties. Accordingly, she added, "I begin to feel that diagnosis is much more important than what I have previously thought." Question No. 16: Which of the following do_you feel best characterizes your classroom situatiOn with regard to reading instruction? Four teachers (l,3,4,6) said that they have enough time to diagnose all the students in their classroom (A). Three (5,8,10) said they have enough time to diagnose individual children who have reading difficulties (B). Two teachers (2,9) said that, because of time pressure, it is impossible for them to conduct individual diagnoses (D). One teacher (7) was not sure of how to characterize her diagnostic patterns (E). 155 These findings suggest that teachers do tend to view reading diagnosis as a prime responsibility and that they do take the time to diagnose children with reading difficulties. It appears that only those who feel absolutely unqualified to conduct diagnoses refer students to reading specialists (e.g., teachers 2 and 9, who also mentioned earlier that they do not know how to diagnose) (pp. 145-146). These findings raise two important questions: (1) How good is the diagnosis conducted by teachers who do not refer children to reading specialists? and (2) When teachers take the time to attempt diagnoses with slow readers, how much time is left for them to work on reading instruction with the other students in the classroom? Summary Results of this study were divided into three parts: findings derived from product measures, findings derived from process measures, and those derived from classroom interviews. The most significant finding suggested by the product measures was that there is little agreement among teachers on their final stated diagnostic judgments for given cases of reading difficulties. This finding was supported by an analysis of the number and type of diagnostic judgments each teacher made, as well as by measures of the relationships between the diagnostic judgments of each of the teachers (diagnostic agreement score). 156 The major conclusion suggested by the process measures was that teachers apparently lack comprehensive or systematic approaches to gathering and evaluating information about cases of reading difficulties. This conclusion is supported by (1) differences among the teachers on length of interactions with the cases, number of cues collected and final number of diagnostic judg- ments made, and (2) the type of verbatim comments they made about the cues and the cases. The major finding derived from the classroom interviews was that teachers consider similar global diagnostic categories in both laboratory situations and the natural classroom environemnt. It was also found that diagnostic and remedial categories mentioned by teachers in the classroom tend to be very general. This may indicate an absence of a comprehensive functional model of reading and diagnosis. It may also raise questions about the adequacy of the training teachers receive in diagnosis and remediation skills. CHAPTER V SUMMARY, CONCLUSIONS AND RECOMMENDATIONS Introduction This study was an exploratory one designed to investigate the clinical problem solving behavior of classroom teachers as they diagnosed students' reading difficulties in experimental and classroom situations. The study was part of an extensive research program undertaken by the Clinical Studies component of the Institute for Research on Teaching at Michigan State University. The literature on reading diagnosis indicates that classroom diagnosis is viewed as an essential part of all reading instruction. More specifically, it suggests the following: 1. There is an agreement among educators that reading difficulties hinder students' intellectual and emotional development, and that early and continued classroom diagnosis is needed to prevent and remediate the reading problems. 2. There are varied meanings associated with the term "reading diagnosis." Most definitions suggest that there 157 158 are different levels of diagnosis: classroom, school, or clinical. However, missing from all definitions are descriptions of the cognitive processes teachers employ as they gather information and attempt to reach diagnostic decisions. 3. Although most researchers believe classroom teachers are capable of diagnosing students with reading difficulties, there is not enough empirical data to confirm or disconfirm this belief. In summary, a review of the literature failed to provide a description of the diagnostic processes classroom teachers use. Moreover, the review revealed a lack of empirical evidence about the competence of classroom teachers in diagnosing reading problems. Procedures Ten teachers were involved in the study, five from the Chicago area and five from the Lansing area, all had taken at least two courses in reading instruction at a university level. All 10 teachers went through the same procedures. First, they interacted with two simulated cases of reading difficulties in a laboratory situation. These simulated cases, developed by Dr. Rebecca Barr of the University of Chicago, represented the behaviors of students with reading deficiencies. The cases were based on materials that would 159 be available to a classroom teacher and made use of the students' natural language patterns. In a second phase of the study, the teachers were observed in their own classrooms, and interviewed about their instructional, diagnostic, and remedial practices in reading. Data Analysis Data from laboratory interactions consisted of verbatim comments made by the teachers as they gathered and evaluated information about the two simulated cases, and of their final stated diagnostic judgments. These data were analyzed by means of product and process measures. Product measures consisted of (l) diagnostic agreement scores, (2) diagnostic ommonality scores, (3) cue agreement scores, and (4) cue commonality scores. Process measures included (1) length of interaction, (2) number of final diagnoses, (3) number of cues collected, and (4) relationships between cues and the teachers' verbatim comments. Classroom interviews were analyzed in terms of (l) diagnostic categories mentioned in the classroom, (2) remedial techniques offered, and (3) teachers' responses to various questions in the interview. Comparisons were also made between teachers' classroom diagnoses and their laboratory diagnoses. 160 Results The major finding suggested by the product measures was that the teachers differed from one another on their final stated diagnoses for the simulated cases of reading difficulties. The process measures yielded two major findings: (1) that the teachers apparently lacked comp- rehensive and systematic strategies for gathering and evaluating information about the cases in order to reach diagnostic judgments, and (2) that the teachers differed markedly from each other on length of interaction with a case, number of cues collected, number of final stated diagnoses,and.number of comments made about case. Analysis of classroom interviews revealed that the teachers mentioned similar global diagnostic categories in both the laboratory situations and the natural classroom environments. It was found that these categories tended to be very general. Conclusions Despite the small number of teachers participating, the study provides convincing evidence for the following conclusions: 1. Contrary to what the professional literature on reading diagnosis implies, classroom teachers do not seem qualified to diagnose and remediate cases of reading difficulties. The results of this study suggest 161 that teachers are ill-equipped to carry out these functions. This apparent deficiency may be the result of two interrelated causes: (1) insufficient training of teachers in reading diagnosis and remediation, and (2) absence of a comprehensive model of reading and diagnosis. Several examples support this conclusion. As one teacher (no. 7) interacted with a simulated case, she said: "The problem is that (the child) can't have phonetic analysis of words". Later, when this teacher stated her final diagnoses, she focused entirely on emotional problems (she also cited emotional problems during classroom interviews). It seems that this teacher Operates from a psychological model, which is only remotely related to the reading act, if at all. Another teacher (no. 8), after looking at only a few items of information about a case, said: "From her word meaning knowledge I assumed very fast that comprehension was one of her better areas". In her subsequent work on this case, this teacher looked at no items of information about comprehension (whereas seven other teachers had something to say about the comprehension level of this case). A third teacher (no. 9), interacting with simulated case (Case 7), made two final diagnoses that contradicted each other: (1) "the child reads hesitantly", and (2) "the child reads very well". 162 These examples suggest that these teachers do not operate from comprehensive models of reading and diagnosis. The notion that teachers are ill-dquipped to conduct diagnoses is also consistent with results from studies of reading specialists and learning disabilities personnel conducted by the Clinical Studies group. Tables 19 and 20 show the diagnostic categories most frequently mentioned by eight senior reading clinicians who interacted with simulated cases in a 1977 observational study (Vinsonhaler, 1979), and those categories most frequently mentioned by the teachers in the present study. A comparison of Tables 19 and 20 demonstrates that, in general, results for the proportional agreement in the two studies are quite similar. In both instances, most of the diagnostic categories listed were mentioned for a given case by only about 30 percent of the clinicians. There were, however, more diagnostic categories cited by at least 50 percent of the clinicians in the 1977 study than in the present study, possibly because the clinicians had more similar training backgrounds than did the 10 teachers. This explanation is consistent with the Agreement Corollary described earlier (pp. 57-62). The consistency of results between the two studies is further illustrated by a comparison of the diagnostic agreement and commonality scores for each. Table 21 summarizes these comparisons: 163 TABLE l9.--Most frequently mentioned diagnostic categories for 1977 observational study (8 clinicians, 4 cases)* Diagnostic categories At least average reading potential Adequate verbal skills Poor oral reading Problems with vowels Sight words: Low Phonics: Weak Auditory acuity: Problem Consonant blends: Not a problem Good use of context Writing: Problem Spelling: Problem Normal interest and behavior Attitude toward reading: poor No problem isolated letter sound skills Speech: Problem Problem with Syllables Handwriting: Problem Problem with visual memory Health problems in school Poor word analysis skills Auditory discrimination: problem eases .67 .33 .50 .50 .33 .33 .33 .33 .33 .50 .50 Case B .33 .50 .67 .33 .50 .33 .33 .50 .50 .50 Case C .50 .50 .33 .33 .83 .33 .33 .33 .50 Sea-“£2 .67 .67 .67 .33 .33 .50 .50 .50 *--denotes 0.00 164 TABLE 20.--Most frequently mentioned diagnostic categories for Cases 7 and 8 and 10 teachers* SELECTED DIAGNOSTIC CATEGORIES Case 7 Case 8 (54 total DX) (62 total DX) Endings: Ignores .50 -- Sight words: Weak -- .50 Sight vocabulary: Good .40 -- Vocabulary concepts: Adequate .30 .30 Comprehension: Poor .30 -- Oral reading: Skips words -- .30 Oral reading: Does not pay attention to punctuation -- 30 Phonic skills: Weak -- Word attack skills: Lacks -- Print translation: Problem area -- Oral reading comprehension: Good -- Understanding: Good when listening -- Literal comprehension: Adequate -- Beginning sounds: Knows -- Initial consonants: Strong -- Syllabication skills: Poor -- Unfamiliar words: No attempt to sound -- Context to help with the unknown word: I O O O O O C O I O O N 0 Does not use -- .20 Language: No language problem -- .20 Comprehension: Adequate .20 -- Vocabulary: Weak .20 -- Punctuation: Lacks .20 -- Reversals: Problem area .20 -- Mispronunciation errors: Did not correct .20 -- Vowels: Need work .20 -- Word analysis: Weak .20 -- *--denotes 0.00 165 TABLE 21.--Diagnostic and commonality scores for the classroom teachers study and the 1977 study Classroom teachers study 1977 Study 10 Classroom teachers 8 Reading Clinicians STATISTICS Case—7 Case 8 (24 data points) score SD score SD score SD Diagnostic agreement -.04 .13 -.03 .11 -.07 .17 Diagnostic commonality .45 .29 .45 .23 .55 .18 As Table 21 shows, diagnostic agreement and commonality scores were markedly similar for the classroom teachers and the reading clinicians. This further suggests that the findings of the present study are generalizable. 2. Overall, teachers seem to lack the information processing strategies that are necessary for gathering and evaluating information about students' reading difficulties. Their diagnoses in both the laboratory situations and the natural classroom environment tend to be very global, non-specific and incomplete. They are confined to a set of general observations that do 166 not necessarily form a comprehensive picture of the child's reading performance. The teachers are observers of students' behaviors; at best, they can say what the children cannot do, rather than offer complete and specific diagnoses. That this is so should come as no surprise, for several reasons: For one thing, teacher education programs rarely provide teachers with thorough preparation in decision-making and diagnostic skills. Moreover, the models of reading and learning teachers are given in training programs may be inadequate and incomprehensive. Finally, most programs emphasize method courses and the acquaintance of prospective teachers with a variety of subject areas, rather than more specialization. 3. Investigations conducted in laboratory settings, carefully planned and monitored, seem to be a valid and apprOpriate means of studying the clinical problem solving behavior of classroom teachers. The fact that the performance of the individual teachers in this study were consistent across two simulated cases suggests that the cases elicited at least some of the teachers' habitual problem—solving behavior. That the general diagnostic categories mentioned by teachers in the laboratory settings were also mentioned during classroom interviews suggests that the teachers operated from similar models of reading in both 167 situations. Furthermore, the teachers' own comments about the laboratory interactions support the claim that the laboratory conditions did, indeed, resemble the natural classroom situation with regard to the diagnosis of individual readers. Recommendations There is a clear need for the classroom teacher to be a reading diagnostician. However, training in the diagnosis of reading difficulties has been reserved mainly for graduate level courses; most undergraduate teacher education programs do not provide sufficient instruction in this area. The adequacy of this kind of teacher education curriculum may therefore be questioned. Teachers need to be trained more thoroughly in diagnostic and remedial skills in reading. Such training should be based on a model of reading and diagnosis, and focus on the diagnostic strategies involved in gathering and evaluating data about students with reading difficulties. Training of teachers in diagnosis and remedial skills can be conducted in laboratory situations, using simulated cases of reading difficulties. Teachers should be given much practice interacting with simulated cases, where it is possible for them to receive immediate feedback on their performance. 168 It is also necessary that additional studies be carried out to investigate further the clinical problem-solving behavior of classroom teachers in reading diagnosis. Instruments should be develOped to relate, more precisely, teachers' diagnostic performances in laboratory situations to their performances in the natural classroom environment. Research should also be conducted to investigate the impact of laboratory training with simulated cases on teachers' performances in the natural classroom environment. APPENDICES APPENDIX A CASE INVENTORY APPENDIX A CASE INVENTORY .xmcu coHcmnmumEoo mchcou Hmuo or» CH hHuomuuoo umzmcm no: OHO acmosum mnu noHna mcoHumosU mmocu acmcsum mcu mxmn nonomou on» coca can .mEHu ccoomn on» u0u command on» psoHc mommy ucmozum an» I mmomm onmzmmmmmzoo quadmm Admo .xmmu concmsmumEoo mcHommu Hmuo on» cH aHuomunoo umsmcm uoc ch ucmpsum on» :oHrz mcoHumosq mmocu unmosum on» wxmm umsomou or» cocu .ommmmmm m mchcmu morons» on» o» mcmumHH unocsum on» I onmzmmmmmzou oszmemHH “mmomm ZOszmmmmmSOU .uH usond mcoHummsq uozmcm Ou coxmm mH one OOOHIuso command mchomu m mommu unoccum on» I onmzmmmmmzoo wzHoHucmdfim wmmmwmm m mvmmu ucmvsum On» I mmm3m2¢ flaw mZOHBmmDO ZMFFHm3 nufl3 ZOHmzmmMM&ZOU OZHD¢mm BZMQHm .mcoHummsq OHmwommm admummummIco: no smcummucm OHMHommm m cquH3 mcoHummsq uo umHH ”onmzmmmmmzou .uH mocsocoum o» ucmpsum mcu mxmm can msoum umuumH uo umuumH onch m mucHum morocco mnu I AmwnaHmcm OHumcocmv onecHUCmmm Homzwm ozoom .uH mocsocoum ou ucmcsuw mnu mxmm can» can mOHOMHHmm OucH puo3 coucHud m mmcH>Hc umncmou on» I AmmumoHanHam accommu. quxzumU «aromas .uH mocsocoum 0» coca can mmHanHam Oucw ouoz OOOCHum c mcH>Hc Op coxmm mH ucocsuw on» I AmmumOHOMHHNm ucmpsum. UZHXZDmu Bzmoaem .uH mocoocoua ou campsum or» mxmm can Ono: m mucHuQ umnommu on» I onedqomH AZOHfiflloHBZMQHV ZOHBHzooumm Qm03 commune mchmmu one CH musuoHd on» NO coHumHuOmmn commune OCHpmmu on» mcHhccmEooom musuOHm command mcu mcHocmn MHuoHu co mucoEEoo coHumHuomcnuu OCHpmmu cho mucoEEOO m.noHOmcsoo Hoonom coHucEMONCH umma >uouan Hoozom Emumcrm OCHOmmm Hoocom CH mocmEuOunom m.MHuoHu ccsoumxomn mEonIHmusuHsU DZDOmUxUV UZHdez QmOZ meBzm>zH mm "unequSufiumnsm vac: .N xwua_huo> "mmumoumuum coauuuwmauamvw pups .x .x .x x x ~3an 3335 .N on uswfia u“ wags um nomads» can whoa mo mafiaaawmn man an axooa .N N3M£m mauuwa m "nouufinwouou vuo3 N No "nouumowufiuaopa who: .N wmamm oauuHH mafixma mm? wuwvwmu cog: wawuouuuoo uaosuaa vamp ou «mudguaou .N N mmvuaaoax Aaowumauossmv manna ”soaumSuuasm N usuam>ouqaw ovum: "wdwvwou Homo N Nausuuamon upoum N HHoa.hum> apnea N mwsfiummscpuos.ocwmov ou muoauwafimov mo kuumafi noaaamxm «on: .N wouwawa on ou ammo unummoaoo N .N sums. "Numaaawuo> .N .N N munoocou gonna maogx ”muaooaou NHdHSbwoo> wouafiun mu N umga Na pmaamuumsoo on umsa mafia :Nuoum: may uMSu pnmumuopas uoa has mam N oxma ou some you mmop ”maoauuundoo mosmusomuousH N Nous "dogmausouaaoo u>aumunuouaH N commas "huoum onu cw mucosuom vac uuowmo can mango mo mucosmam .N .N .N noon “cowwamnauaaou .N wawumua nuwa mvmuN N No "Annouumuwao .wcwuufimv maqwuov Honouwa .N .N maoaumwsv gnu mo meow mommaa am53.am>m .oumavava ”nowmaunounaoo CH a w n o n q n N H mmmmudmfi mBZmSUDDb UHBmozwfiHQ h Mmdu ”2H4200 UHBmOzwfiHQ h NHQmemd 194 195 0H «3% Q") ”Q” MN mm FIN H CNN .N .N .N N m m n o n e m N H Ofi' \Dfi' \Dx'f II sooumnmao ca voaouunoa u . Nuouuuonma aw vmsoauaoa I N l [I 1‘ . . . . . usuavuuu uoa www.moaumas.vu03_aoasooa= .3oaon haunwwan “Hm>mH macaw muoauusum onww> o>wu pawumumvns no: moov "muumawsfinm< woman uuuauwv mum mamanoum no: mo maoauoumomasma may msucmoa_nufia_waapmou ma one away uauumamwv mu amanoun no: «H noon “aduuhm nowamam mama ”Aoaumsosm saga umsumuv muouuo Huuaumaawuu amanoua m we upoonuonswwms can «so: scum mosoaamca owmawawq Noamuofimmv Honouaao uwmawsma a was umwmawama meow "mofiwmuuuum nowuuouuou xuoz_um«mhamau pups wsanmamam coca hogan“ mono; um oommoau maum> Naumasowuumm .mpuoa ou moanmaama muuxu mvn< coauwowmausmvw whoa aw was ou uxmusoo mo on: vmuuaaa “uxuuaoo sauna ou so: vaoumumuas you mmov "wawxnano No "mono uxuuaou No “wafixaanu Nuoa.vuoa “maoao> mo mamas mwwa on 0» atoms “mauso> use muamsomsoo uamvaonmwaw on umwmhaaau owuosonm axoma “mHHme ouumaonm ozoax umvnsom wawasfiwum Sosa «0 Name uumuuoo pap :muouum cowumuunaoaoummfiz QQHOGwH :A:m._u.—: :Uflw: .1503: :09: Mu—HHV w—uuo3 HHGn—m pmawwpwaom no: no: “Numaonaoo> unwwm mono afiumaoanoun m "maumu0>mN mpuoa unwwm canon so monomfia "aoaufiamooum voow "humasnmoo> unwwm uooamfip uumamwu mwuo3 mo paw on» ad monumfia «soaumouwausmvfi who: 196 .N N N .N .N .N .N .N .N N N N .N N .N N N .N N h .N .N N N N .N N .N .N CH o w n o m mNWflUdflB <>é>< .N oaoax "occaoo wawasuwmm N oaoo onu uaono o>o£ odowuauwuonoo you moo: mam oouoa "oaoquuwuonoo who: oona ”oxaao NOOH uonu mono: oouo amanoua uoHooHo>ou vuoz .N .N .N Nook. "mono: unwwm No "soauoaooa aw who: unwwm wdouuo “mono: unwfim ooocoooaouoo ouoofipcfi ououuo "coauoowmauaooa who: wouoaoma poo amuufiua coca uoosoouomawou poo xuoa Homo swaounu nouuon uou opwo>o "mono: :Hs: no :3: opwo>o nachos zq: Houuoq N Ou aowusouuo Non uoc omov "nowuoouonam mono: :30 won a: ooxoz onaxo "opuoz N Eouw mucouowooo no“: ocofiuwaamop puoa o>aw ou poo "odowuwafimov vuo3 mo odouuwuamov Nuouoowouuoo oo>ww ”ovuoa NQN Nuoaonooo> ouoop o no: “Nuoaonooo> .N muoovopo "ouaooooo whoa Noo3 "uxouooo Nuouo mo mahou ca oooaouaom Nooa "aoaoaosouuaoo HowuaouomsH poom "oafiouop pom Nuoaoz boom uwswwoou mo uoo wcfisooa wawuuow yam ooawououum ouosvopo "aoaoconoumaoo Honouaa mafiaouofia gog3 boom "wafiwdououovsp poow "soamaosouaaou wawpoou Homo .N .N .N ouosvovo "soaooosounaoo m N H mBZMSQDDb UHBmOZUoH ououw quSu uo SUHnoun owoowaoH on “owoowcoa ou oHnosn "oHomHoao whoa aH ooHHHaom who? ououosou mo: uoc ooov “who; azoaxa: osu nufis AHos ou uxousoo oouo ENHaoum u:o= Hoswm Sufi: oaanoua “wawxaano Noo3 "oo>woooooom xooz_ "owaHvao Houon o>oa uos ooop "ooawououuo sowuoouuoo oNooH ”opuoa uao «Human ou hwououum oNooH ”oHHon Noouu< ouoHumouaao NHHosuxoucoo ouo oaoo “maOHusuHuonso vuoz wawaooa How Aoouo uxousOUV moo: oosfluoaoo "mono unoucoo Ncano ou NuHHHno oaoo “ovHOB vaaonaoo 3on3 no: No8 "soHuoHooH :H oanosowuoHou Hoaaho vasom aHon Saws pooHn pHsou "waHoooHn vaaom no mo: uaouoHoaouaH "ovaon uso canon ou unsouuo o: ”ovuoa uoHHHsowo: saws msoHnoun "oucosooaou poo oHozo> nufla oaanoua uwsfivaon noon uoHHon coHuooHpoHHNm Now: "ouaocooaou waHvao poo oHopfiz Nook “oHoso> wdoH Now? uoHozo> uuonm Nova ”ocoHuoHooooo vcaoo wouuoH Nova "oHHHNo oHconm amHnoun o: “waHpcoHn fiasco Houa< mo: ou NuHHHno oaoo ”waHnsoHo oHaoHHNm wsouuo "oucoaooaoo HoHuHaH osoax ”oaowuoaooooo venom uouuoH 198 N c e H N o N 0H m e u . soouoooHo ow voaowudoa I . S a 2 N n 2 NH 3 m n u x 33333 38385 n N N oHHHoooa “osoHnoun aOHumoouom oH m .w B. o m a mu N H APPENDIX G LENGTH OF INTERACTION, NUMBER OF CUES COLLECTED AND NUMBER OF DIAGNOSTIC JUDGMENTS FOR CASES 7 AND 8 AND 10 TEACHERS APPENDIX G NUMBER OF CUES COLLECTED AND NUMBER LENGTH OF INTERACTION, OF DIAGNOSTIC JUDGMENTS FOR CASES 7 and 8 AND 10 TEACHERS oomocmoHp cow: H.m o.m m.m m.m m.N o.m o.NH m.OH o.NH m.h m.m o.m OH m HH N m MH NH vH m h m omoo N.n m m m m m HH m CH m m h mmou mmmocmoHo mo Hmnaoz m.m¢ o.Nm o.ov m.wv o.mH m.mm o.N> m.om m.Hm m.Nw m.hm mono coo: v.Hm no mo mo NH mm mm on Nm an mm m omoo m.N¢ Hw mm mo NH mm am no Hm we on N mmou mono mo Honfisz h.ov o.om m.hm m.nm m.mN o.Hv m.mm o.H¢ o.N¢ o.>¢ o.Nv mEHa coo: H.mv om Ho om mN no hm wv mv No om m omou m.H¢ ow vm mm mN mm om mm mm Nm «N h omou Nmoa so oEHB N 0H m m b o n v m N H mammomma munamnadam 199 APPENDIX H DIAGNOSTIC DOMAIN BASED ON CLASSROOM INTERVIEWS N .N .N N N N.N .N N N N N N N N N N .N N N N .N N .N N .N N N N N N.N .N N N N .N .N .N .N .N N N .N N N .N N .N .N .N .N .N .N N .N .N .N N .N .N N .N N N .N N N N N .N .N N N N N .N N N .N N N .N .N .N N N .N N N N N . N .N CH m m N o n e m N H mmmmUde ooooosu ooaHo ououon :o: omoun 85 En waHvouoa oHoNHoao vuoz xoouuo vuox Huoa_souuHH3 vao Hoao mono: ounaocoun uonsoo ovuoz_uoHHaHo oouo>oa cowuoHooH dH mono: huoHonooo> waHvooN muoHanooo> uanm coauwawooou pups ouoouaoo mo wauauom waacooa NuoHapouo> axouaoo aH wdaaooa huoHsnooo> NuoHonouo> Honuowou cowuoauomsH waauuom uxouaou mo: uoa moon uxounou huoum nacho GH aOHuoahomsH wsHuuow maoHnoum aoHoconounsoo HoHuGouomaH poo Ho9uoom aoHoconmunaoo waHaouoHH GOHoconouaaoo wchoou usoHHm aOHoconoumsou wfiwwoou Houo :oaoaoaouasou mmHmowmfido UHBmOZUmmBZH SOONmm oo>omH ovaaoo nouuoH oooHu UHuoaonm oHoso> waoH cocoon Hoaob oHoso> uuonm oHoao> monopaoaoouuoo AoHsoaonmonaouwv vanoo Houuoa ousoaooaoo HoHuHaH uao bosom ou oHA< suooummo AHfiaom who: opsooo usosooooo ovaaoo wawuam oucaoo ovafiz ovasoo waHadeom ovasom oHoNHoao oHuoaonm ooNmeom oowaoum ouauoauuo ofismaonmonmuoz oHoHSHm ovuoa.vaoonsoo mqumHHNm waaxaonu oHoNHoao Houauoauum ouuon any coca nonuou who: oHoaa onu ovum 202 HH 0 w m m m NH mH N N I .. knouoHOHoH aw Goaunma I . 0N NH 0N NN eN mH «N NN NH on I N aoonoooHu ca pmfioauaua I N N ononosuH N pononnoo MHom N vonoonuowv NHHoom N oOHoH> nonoa noom n aanona Huamn> N osoHnona Hoauoounom N unopano vnoaon ovoann< N oamHHonm HooHoNsm N xnos_aonuwn3 N xnoz.owoswdoa .N N naooooo HHom .N vo>Hnnov NHHonouHou N ooHnoHawnHH N Nnoaua Hono N aOHuoaHonooo none: N nooomm N oanon wnHNnoa noon x Haswannnm .N .N nooHoHn opasoo Nnouava< aoHnonm Nnosuz sownnonno wdHNoa noz munnnnumnu mananomn Nnoaua Hoaow> Nnosoa.NnonHv:< onouoom o.onno .N Ho>oH ovonw SOHon waaooom .N N aOHuoanunom .N .N ooxonoua no: onoonnoo .N oonoa.omwxm N ooaoofiz N ooooo vnoz N N oaoauauanopam .N wagooon unonHm N N N N woavoon Hono o c m NNNNNN HN N mN mN h~>< >< APPENDIX I OBSERVATIONAL STUDY DATA ANALYSIS SYSTEM (OSDAS) STATISTICS APPENDIX I OBSERVATIONAL STUDY DATA ANALYSIS SYSTEM (OSDAS) STATISTICS ‘ The following statistics are all calculated by (l) a computer statistical analysis system (Observational Study Data Analysis System) developed and maintained by the Institute for Research on Teaching, Michigan State University, and (2) an interactive statistical analysis system (MIDAS) at the University of Michigan. A random sampling of data was computed by hand across numerous observational studies and the system was found to be operating accurately. PROPORTIONAL AGREEMENT Given a domain for diagnoses/remediations/ or cues (DX/Rx/ or CX) for a given case, proportional agreement is the proportion of clinicians who mentioned each Dx/Rx or CX statement. One proportion is computed for each statement in the domain. The statistic is bounded by 0 and l and is calculated by dividing the number of clinicians who mentioned a statement by the total number of clinicians. 203 204 number of Cji where Cji Cj clinicians mentioning the ij category Cj = total number of clinicians for a given case. For example, if 3 clinicians of a total of 6 who diagnosed a given case mentioned the category "Basic sight words, weakness", the proportional agreement would be: No. of clinicians mentioning the category 3 POA. = —— = 050 6 COMMONALITY Given a domain for diagnoses/remediations/ or cues (DX/RX/CX) for a given case, the commonality statistic is a measure of agreement between one clinical session and all other clinical sessions for a given case, e.g., an individual is being compared with a group. The statistic is bounded by 0 and l. A value of x for a given clinician roughly implies that she/he has included in her/his session x percent of those statements 205 mentioned most frequently by the group for that case. For example, a clinician who has a score of .34 has included in her/his session roughly 34 percent of those statements mentioned most frequently by the group. INTER-CLINICIAN CORRELATION Given a domain for diagnoses/remediations/ or cues (DX/Rx or CX) for a given case, the phi correlation is a measure of inter-clinician agreement. computed for each pair of clinicians. One phi correlation is The DX/RX/CX categories mentioned by one clinician are compared with those mentioned by a second clinician for the same case. below. CLINICIAN B, SIMCASE Y 62mmmwo HZMUIUI'JP' This comparison is summarized in the table (+) (-) CLINICIAN A, PRESENT (+) SIMCASE Y ABSENT (-) Frequency count of statements in the domain present in both clinicians DX/RX or CX Frequency count of statements in the domain present in clinician B's session but not in clinician A's DX/RX/ or CX Frequency count of statements in the domain present in clinician A's session but not in B's DX/RX or CX Frequency count of statements in the domain absent in both clinicians' DX/RX or CX 206 The calculation of the Phi correlation is derived from the table as follows: Clinician A, Simcase Y + - + a(++) b(+-) a+b Clinician B, _ Simcase Y - c(-+) d(--) c+d a+c I b+d | N I I Phi = (axd - bxc) (a+c)x(b+d)x(c+d)x(a+b) The statistic is bounded by -l (statements are in cells b and c only) and l (statements are in cells a and d only), only if the distribution in the marginals are equal. In all other cases the maximum and minimum values will be less than 1 and greater than -1. An example of a completed table is as follows: Statements of Statements of Domain of Clinician A, Clinician B, statements Simcase Y Simcase Y $1 $1 81 52 $2 82 S3 S7 S3 S4 SS 36 S7 207 Clinician A + — + 2 l 3 Clinician B - l 3 4 l 3 I 4 7 l u LI ST OF REFERENCES LI ST OF REFERENCES Adams, R.K. Early identification of potentially retarded readers within the school system. In E.O. Calkins (Ed.), Readinngorum (NINDS Monograph no. 11). Bethesda, Md: U.S. Department of Health, Education and Welfare, 1971. Austin, M.C. Diagnostic teaching: The primary grades. In J.H. Root (Ed.), Diagnostic teaching: Methods and materials. Syracuse, N.Y.: Syracuse University, School of Education, 1965. Austin, M.C. Identifying readers who need corrective instruction. In D.G. Schubert & T.L. Targerson (Eds.), Readings in reading: Practice theory research. New York; Thomas Y. Crowell Company, 1968. Bamman, H.A. Assessing progress in reading. In R. Farr (Ed.), Measurement and evaluation of reading. New York: Harcourt, Brace and World, Inc. 1970. Barr, R. & Sadow, R. Classroom reading diagnosis and instructional planning. Unpublished manuscript, University of Chicago, 1978. Bateman, B. The role of individual diagnosis in remedial planning for reading disorders. In E.O. Calkins (Ed.), Readinngorum (NINDS Monograph no. 11). Bethesda, Md: U.S. Department of Health, Education and Welfare, 1971. Blount, D. Competencies essential for diagnosing reading difficulties. Teacher Corps Association: Resources for CBTE, no. 6, 1973. ED 095 151. Bond, G.L. Diagnostic teaching in the classroom. In D.L. De Boer (Ed.), Reading diagnosis and evaluation. Proceedings of the thirteenth annual convention of the International Reading Association, (Vol. 13), Newark, Del: International Reading Association, 1970. Bond, G.L. & Tinker, M.A. Reading difficulties: Their diagnosis and correction. (2nd ed.). New York: Meredith Publishing Company, 1967. 208 209 Buerger, T.A. A follow up of remedial reading instruction. Reading Teacher, 1968, 21, 329-334. Burnett, R.W. The classroom teacher as a diagnostician. In D.L. De Boer (Ed.), Reading diagnosis and evaluation. Proceedings of the thirteenth annual convention of the International Reading Association (Vol. 13). Newark, Del: International Reading Association, 1970. Burnett, R.W. The diagnostic proficiency of teachers of reading. The Reading Teacher, 1963, 16 (4), 229-34. Carter, H.L.J. The teacher's diagnosis of the disabled reader. In W.K. Durr (Ed.), Reading difficulties: Diagnosis, correction and remediation. Newark, Del: International Reading Association, 1970. Carter, H.L.J. & McGinnis, D.J. Diagnosis and treatment of the disabled reader. London: The MacMillan Company, 1970. Cartwright, G.P., Cartwright, C.A., & Yssledyke, J.E. Two decision models: Identification and diagnostic teaching of handicapped children in the regular classroom. Psychology in the Schools, 1973, 19, 4-11. Dauzat, S.W. Informal diagnosis: The nucleus. In R.A. Earle (Ed.), Classroom practice in reading. Newark, Del: International Reading Association, 1977. Della-Piana, G.M. Reading diagnosis and prescriptions: An introduction. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston, Inciil968. Dietrich, D.M. A diagnostic approach to corrective reading in the classroom. (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. 063 576). Paper presented at the annual meeting of the International Reading Association, Detroit, May 1972. Dupuis, M.M. & Ashkov, E.N. Diagnostic teaching in content area reading. In P.D. Pearson (Ed.), Reading: Theory, research and practice. Twenty-sixth yearbook of the National Reading Conference. St. Paul, Minn: Mason Publishing Company, 1977. Ekwall, E.E. Diagnosis and remediation of the disabled reader. Boston, Mass: Allyn and Bacon, Inc.'1976. Ekwall, E.E. Teacher's handbook on diagnosis and remediation in reading. Boston, Mass: Allyn and Bacon, Inc. 1977. 210 Elstein, A.S., Shulman, L.S., & Sprafka, S. Medical problem solving: An analysis of clinical reasoning. Cambridge, Mass: Harvard University Press, 1978. Farr, R. Reading diagnosis: Trends and issues. (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. 050 899). Paper presented at the meeting of the Nassau Reading Council, Uniondale, New York, March 1971. Florida Department of Education, Division of Public Schools, Bureau of Curriculum Services. Diagnostic prescriptive reading instruction. (ERIC Document Reproduction SerVice No. 149 310), 1977. Gallant, R. The development of a diagnostic instrument. In R. Farr (Ed.), Measurement and evaluation of reading. New York: Harcourt, Brace and World, Inc., 1970. Garland, L.H. Studies on the accuracy of diagnostic pro- cedures. American Journal of Roentgenology, 1959, 83, 25-38. Gil, D., Hoffmeyer, E.C.M., Van Roekel, J.L. & Weinshank, A.B. Clinical problem solving in reading: Theory and research. (Res. Ser. No. 45). East LanSing, Mich: Institute for Research on Teaching, Michigan State University, 1979. Gil, D., Vinsonhaler, J.F., & Wagner, C.C. Studies of clinical problem solving behavior in reading diagnosis (Res. Ser. No. 42). East Lansing, Mich: Institute for Research on Teaching, Michigan State University, 1979. Gil, D., Wagner, C.C. & Vinsonhaler, J.F. Simulating the clinical problem solving behavior of reading clinicians (Res. Ser. No. 30). East Lansing, Mich: Institute for Research on Teaching, Michigan State University, 1979. Glad, J.R. Evaluation of the remedial reading program in Utah Public Schools. UnpubliSHed doctoral diSsertation, Salt Lake City, Utah: University of Utah, 1965. Guszak, F.J. Diagnostic reading instruction intthe ele- mentary school. New York: Harper and Row Publishers, I972. 211 Harris, A. J. Can the teacher of reading be a dia nostician? (ERIC Document Reproductionfi Service No. 032 909). Paper presented at the annual meeting of the International Reading Association, New Orleans, May 1974. Harris, A.J. Diagnosis of reading disabilities. In H. M. Robinson (Ed. ), Corrective reading in classroom and clinic. Supplementary Edfibational Monographs No. 70 Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1953. Harris, A. J. The diagnosis of reading disabilities. In A. J. Harris 8 E. R. Sipay (Eds. ), ReadingsJ on reading instruction (2nd ed. ). New York: David McKay Harris, A. J. The reading teacher as diagnostician. In R.A. Earle (Ed. ), Classroom practice in reading. Newark, Del: International Reading Association, 1977. Harris, A.J. & Sipay, E. R. How to increase freading_abiligy. (6th ed.). New York: David McKay Company, Inc., 1975. Harris, A.J. & Smith, C.B. Dingnostic teaching in the classroom. New York: Holt, Reinhart and Winston, 1976. Hollingsworth, P. M. Diagnosis and pro nosis: An interdisciplinary approach. (ERIC Document Repro- duction Service No. 042 582). Paper presented at the annual meeting of the International Reading Association, Anaheim, Calif., May 1970. Jan-Tausch, E. Reading diagnosis: The essential ingredient in teaching every child to read. In R.E. Leibert (Ed.), Diagnostic viewpoints in reading. Newark, Del: International Reading Association, 1971. Karlsen, B. Reading: Assessment and diagnosis of abilities. In P. Lamb & R. Arnold (Eds. ), Reading: Foundations and instrugnional strategies. Belmot, Ca1if.: Wadsworth Publishing Company, Inc., 1976. Kennedy, E.C. Classroom a roaches to remedial reading. Itasca, III.: F.E. Peacock PubliShing, Inc., 1971. Kress, R.A. Diagnosis: An interdisciplinary approach. In J.A. Figurel (Ed.), Reading and inguiry. Conference proceedings (Vol. 10). NewarE, De : International Reading Association, 1965. McCarthy, W.G. Diagnosis, prescription, correction and evaluation in the teaching of reading. Contemporany Education, 1971, 42 (5), 233-235. 212 McGinnis, D.J. The teacher's approach to the stud of the disabled reader, A homeopathic concept. (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. 042 577). Paper presented at the annual meeting of the International Reading Association, Anaheim, Calif., May 1970. Medrano, P.P. A psychatric perspective of reading disability. In W. Otto, N.A. Peters & C.W. Peters (Eds.), Readingpproblems: A multidisciplinary perspective. Cambridge, Mass.: Addison-Wesley Publishing Company, 1977. Monroe, M. Children who cannot read. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1932. Monroe, M. General principles of diagnosis of reading disabilities. In D.G. Schubert & T.L. Targerson (Eds.), Readings in reading: Practice theory research. New York: Thomas Y. Crowell Company, 1968. Niles, O.S. Diagnostic reaching in the junior high school. In J.H. Root (ed.), Diagnostig teaching: Methods and materials. Syracuse, N.Y.: Syracuse University, School of Education, 1965. Olson, J.P. & Dillner, M.H. Learning to teach reading in the elementary school utilizing a competency based instructional nystem. New York: Macmillan Publishing Co., Inc. 1976. Otto, W., McMenemy, R.A. & Smith, R.J. Corrective and remedial teaching (2nd ed.). Boston: Houghton Mifflin Company, 1973. Paton, B.C. The accuracy of diagnosis of myocardial infarction. American Journal of Medicine, 1957, 23, 761-768. Pearson, P.D., & Kamil, M.L. Basicpprocesses and instructional practices in teaching reading (Reading Education Report No. 7YT’ Urbana-Champaign, Ill: Center for the Study of Reading, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, 1978. Peters, C.W. Diagnosis of reading problems. In W. Otto., N.A. Peters, & C.W. Peters (Eds.), Reading problems: A multidisciplinar ers ective. Cambridge, Mass.: Addison-WesIey PuEIishing Company, 1977. 213 Peters, N.A. An interdisciplinary approach to the assessment and management of severe language- learning problems. In W. Otto., N.A. Peters. & C.W. Peters (Eds.), Reading‘pnoblems: A multi- disciplinary pers ective. CamBridge, Mass.: Addison-Wesley PuEIishing Company, 1977. Rabinovitch, R. Differential diagnosis in children with reading retardation. In J.H. Root (Ed.), Diagnositic teaching: Methods and materials. Syracuse, N.Y.: Syracuse University, School of Education, 1965. Reich, R.R. More than remedial reading. In A.J. Harris & E.R. Sipay (Eds.), Readings in readinginstrnction (2nd ed.). New York: David McKay Company, Inc., 1972. Robinson, H.M. Why pnpils failin reading. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1946. Satz, P. Reading problems in perspective. In W. Otto., N.A. Peters, & C.W. Peters (Eds.), Reading problems: A multidisclplinary pergpective. Cambridge, Mass.: Addison-Wesley Publishing Compnay, 1977. Sawyer, D.J. The diagnostic mystique - a point of view. The Reading Teacher, 1974, 27 (6), 555-561. Sawyer, R. Diagnosis for the classroom teacher (ERIC Document Reproductidn Service No. 029 754). Paper presented at the conference of the College Reading Association, Knoxville, April 1968. Sheldon, W.D. Specific principles essential to classroom diagnosis. In D.G. Schubert & T.L. Targerson (EDs.), Readings in rnnding:iPractice theory research. New York: Thomas Y. Crowell Company, 1968. Sheldon, W.D. Teachers in grades K-12 diagnose reading problems. In J.H. Root (Ed.), Diagnostic teaching: Methods and materials. Syracuse, N.Y.: Syracuse University, School of Education, 1965. Sherman, G. A model of rgadingand leanning to read. East Lansing, Mich.: Institute for Research on Teaching, Michigan State University, in preparation. Sherman, G. Training reading specialists in diagnosis (Res. Ser. No.931). East Lansing, Mich.: Institute for Research on Teaching, Michigan State University, 1978. 214 Sherman, M. Psychiatric insights into reading problems. In D.G. Schubert & T.L. Targerson (Eds.), Readings in reading: Practice theory research. New York: Thomas Y. Crowell Company, 1968? Smith, C.B. Correcting readingproblnns in the classroom. Newark, Del.: International Reading Associatién, 1969.’ Smith, C.B., Carter, B., & Dapper, G. Treating reading_ difficulties: The role of the principal, teacher, npecialiét, administrator. Washington, D.C.: U.S. Deaprtment of Health, Education, and Welfare, 1970. Smith, F. Understanding reading: A psycholinguistic analysis of reading and learning to read. New York: Holt. Reinhart and'Winston, Inc., 1971. Spache, G.D. Clinical diagnosis in the classroom. The Reading Teacher, 1960, ll, 14-18. Spache, G.D. Diagnosing and correcting reading disabilities. Boston: Allyn and Bacon, Inc.,—1976. Spache, G.D. Diagnosis of reading problems. The Elementary School Journal, 1955, 56, 18-26. Stephens, E.L.A. A study of the_prob1em solving behavior of teachers as they diagnose a childTS reading performance and’the effects 6f experience and training on that behavior. Unpuhlished doctoral dissertaEion, Michigan State University, 1978. Stevens, D.O. Reading difficulty and classroom acceptance. The Reading Teacher, 1971, g; (1), 52-55. Strang, R. Dia nostic teachin of reading. New York: McGraw-HII1 Company, 1 42 Strang, R. Levels of reading diagnosis. Educational Forum,h Strang, R. Reading diagnosis and remediation. Newark, Del.: International Reading Association, 1968. Swalm. J.E. Diagnostic teaching: A method for assessing reading skills. In M. Kling (Ed.), Language develnp- ment for the classroom and renedial reading. Proceedings of the fall 1972 Rutgers University Reading Confernece, New Brunswick, 1973. (ERIC Document Reproduction Serivce No. 140 260). 215 Van Roekel, J. L. 8 Patriarca, L. Do reading and learning disabilityrclinicians differ in Eheir dIagnosis and remediation? Paper presented’at the annual meeting of the International Reading Association, Atlanta, April 1979. Vinsonhaler, J.P. Clinical problem solving among reading specialists: The problem of agreement on a diagnosis. Paper presented at the annuai meeting of the International Reading Association, Atlanta, April 1979. Vinsonhaler, J.F., Wagner, C.C. & Elstein, A.S. The Inquiry Theory: An information processing approach to clinical problem solving research and application. Medinfo '77. Proceedings of the Second World Conference in Medical Information, 1977. Wagner, C.C., Kitchen, F., Polin, R., Vinsonhaler, J.E., Van Roekel, J.L. & Gil, D. gngervational studies: Data analysis system. (Tech. Rep). ~East Lansing, Mich.: Institute for Research on Teaching, Michigan State University, 1979. ' Weule, G.G. Diagnosis and treatment of reading problems: The teacher's role. The Slow Learning.Child, 1971, .l§ (3), 168-188. Weinshank, A.B. An observational study of the association between diagnosis and remediation in reading. Ph.D. dissertation, Michigan State UniVersiEy, in progress. Weinshank, A.B. The relationships between diagnosis and remediation ih reading: A pilot studyn (Res. Ser. No. 37): East Lansing, Mich.: Institute for Research on Teaching, Michigan State University, 1979. Weintraub, S., & Farr, R. Introduction. In R. Farr, S. Weintraub & B. Tone (Eds.), Inproving reading research. Newark, Del.: International Reading Association, 1976. Whitcombe, L.M. Overcoming reading difficulties and the effect on personal development. In R.T. Williams (Ed. ), Insights into why and how to read. Newark, Del.: International Reading Association, 1976. Wilson, R.M. Diagnostic and remedial reading for classroom and clinic (3rd ed. ). Cqumbus, Ohio: Charles? E. Merrill Publishing Company, 1977. 216 Yerushalmy, J. Reliability of chest radiography in the diagnosis of pulmonary lesions. American Journal of Surgery, 1955, 89, 231-240.