.1 1.', "‘JJ“ JJ‘J‘ 1111I11JJJ-. .111111JIIII """1'J'1"JJ‘ 1‘111I WJJJ'J. 'I'I'JJJ’J "'JJJ 11111|J ”11' 1 JJ 1 I.1111I1' 11111.11 “"1 11. .1 ‘JJJ” JI'JJJJ' 'JfiJJJJ'JJJJ“ "'J‘ 1'1 IJJJJJ ‘JJJJJJ"" I 'JJJ" '1'". 11 . .1111‘J.‘11111-11111""1"11111I.III'I“ 11 ' I'J1'1'11"“""J' 11"1“' "JJIJJJJJJJJ""‘ 11"r II ["1 11'.’J 1 '1'1‘ 1 ‘JJIJJ J111IIII'J1'I1I JJJ JJ . , 1 I. 11'1' ' J' II 1I'1I I'I1 11 1 'JJ 1' ‘I1 "1'I""I1"I"'1"'I"II'"" 11'1' '1 'J IJI1I..IJI "J'I'JJ ' 'J'1":1JIJI "'JJ IIII 1'1J'I: ‘ JIJI"J'IJ IIII-I'1I1'I‘I JJJJ‘I‘JJ JJJ'J1‘I' "J‘ II'IJ 1'1.‘ II'I1IJI 1 1JII1'I1' ‘1 IJI III '1.11"' JJ‘J'HJ.‘ ' ‘ J'IJ‘I'I' "'J1"" 1 IW'JJ“ "'11 ' :' """'“"'J"J'"' ' ""11" 1"‘ 1'" J‘J1J1““1 3:; I13, III” I: '.' 1"“1'1'I'J .1 1. '.11'11 1'11J1JI'I'J‘1 "'1;'1‘1‘J1'J1",,"" 1:1111f'.JI'11-'.'1"J.1‘ ‘1"; iii-III ‘ .¢~JIJ‘1'1LJJ‘ J‘ *1 VHJ‘ ., J '1 '1 ‘1 1 1:11 1" .11:111.1111111111111“11-1.1 '1“ "11" Jr" 1 1;." 11 i' :‘1 1'1'1'J1F'1 ‘1‘1'11 ’111‘1'1' 'r1:1“1i'1111‘,"1"1‘"JE ' ‘ ' .";. "11 . . 1‘1 'J'J I J :I 111. '1'JJJ'J J..""11'1"J1,.'J1"" 1JI1JI1‘1‘JI'I1 J‘1J'1J:£s "13." III .‘I I 1 {va1 VJ J' ' ' IJJJJJJ‘JJJIJ '1'"' IJJJJ' 'JJJIJ JJJI' 'IJJ'JJ' JJHJ JJJJ Jag \Jfi” II] ‘ \J‘k “I?!” " "' "" "‘""' ""1'1' ‘JJJ J‘JJ "' """"J“J“" """" 1"“ J'w'flw' $1”ng 1,11‘JJJ" 1.'-11'1JJI'J1'J1“ i'1'1“1 '1 1I1'I1II1I1IIIJJJ1II'1JJ1IIJ JJJ JJJJJJIIJ JJ‘IJIJ‘J‘J JJJJJJ “WM“ J‘JJ,‘ III ‘JIJJJJL'J'JJJJIW I 'JJJ‘JI" JJJJ ;J'( ‘ J1' 1 ,.1. 1‘11 1.1 111'..1 J 1‘1 1111111 " J 1JI'JIJ1'I‘ ‘J“"‘J‘1 1" ‘JJ‘J‘J J11111‘JIJJI‘ """ " '1‘“ “J "J' .11) ‘ J J 1' 1111‘1 IIJIIJIII J‘11'1J 'Jl J"" ' J" 'IJ‘I’JJJ . 1J J J'J'J 1 1'“ J1 :JJ'JIIJJ {1"{1‘ II1‘III1IIII. 1|J‘J‘JJ'JJIJ1J JIIL5J ‘JJJ“..J "a“ 111V '{Ifw :‘II'JIIII t‘ y& I '9 JJJJme'I'I‘I'I'fiJ)"J"—"‘ J 'IJI'I.«1:I‘.IJJ11'I‘I:IIIJ“"'I:{ JJJ“! JJ1J‘JJ11'J1E'1I L ' 1 1| "'1J‘JJJ"'1 J .‘JII I ' H‘J" II‘IJ’M JJ‘ JJJJ11111111'111111 'IJJ'J JJJ ""'}'J111JJJ11‘ "'IJ "1'1 1."1'11‘ I1III'IJ11J 'JJJ "'J " 'J'.,'1'111 J 11 111111 II Luann; lllzllflifllllil Lu! J1”! "MI wing; llljllzl || ~ OVERDUE FINES ARE 25¢ PER DAY PER ITEM Return to book drop to remove this checkout from your record. @ Capyright by FRANCES WIDEHA STOTT 1979 CORRESPONDENCE BETWEEN PERSONALITY DIMENSIONS AND EXPRESSIONS OF FACIAL AFFECT BY Frances Wideha Stott A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Counseling, Personnel Services and Educational Psychology 1979 ABSTRACT CORRESPONDENCE BETWEEN PERSONALITY DIMENSIONS AND EXPRESSIONS OF FACIAL AFFECT BY Frances W. Stott The purpose of the study was to investigate whether there are significant differences in expressions of facial affect as recorded on videotape and measured by trained judges between persons with specific personality character- istics as measured by pencil/paper personality instruments. The assumption that personality characteristics related to emotional style are discernable in the face by specific ex- pressions of facial affect was the basis for this research. The potential use of such information for subsequent re- search efforts and therapeutic intervention provided impe- tus for the study. One hundred sixty-eight undergraduate students at the University of Texas-Austin were administered a battery of pencil/paper personality instruments: the Plutchik Emotions Profile Index, the gysenck Personality Inventory, and the Birkman Method. Sixty-two subjects were subse- quently selected to participate in the interview phase, on Frances W. Stott the basis of their high or low scores across the nine per- sonality dimensions that were used as the independent variables for the study. Subjects were divided into two groups (high and low) for each of the personality dimensions used, based on their scores on one of the personality dimen- sions at a time. The interviewer was the same for all subjects, and was instructed to engage in a responsive but non-confrontive manner. Subjects were asked to select an item of personal interest from the Moonenyroblem Check List's "Personal Psychological Relations Scale," and then to talk with the interviewer about that topic for 15 minutes. The interviews were videotaped, and a systematic sampling procedure was used to prepare the data for rating. There were 30 samples of facial affect per subject, or a total of 1,680 samples for the study. Three females were trained as raters, to recognize the categories of Enjoy, Anger, Surprise, Interest, Disgust, and Distress. These six categories, plus calculations regarding frequency of change from one affect category to another, and number of affect categories used, provided the total of 8 dependent measures for the study. The fifteen hypotheses used in the study were stated so as to test the relationship between performance on speci- fic personality dimensions and the type, range, or frequency Frances W. Stott of change of facial affect expressed by the subject. Hypo- theses were tested by a comparison of groups on the affective measures, using t-tests, with an alpha level set at .05. Supplementary analyses from the multivariate, univariate, and correlational aspects were also used for further explor-_ ation of differences and relationships. Two of the 15 hypotheses were significant. A sig— nificant difference was found between high and low Dis- trustful groups on the affective measure of Disgust (p=.008) indicating that persons who score high on Distrustful tend to display more disgust than do persons who score low on this scale. A significant difference was also found be- tween high and low groups of Getting Along With Others on the affective measure Range of Affect (p=.028), indicating that persons who score high on Getting Along With Others tend to display more types of affect than do persons scoring low on this scale. Supplementary analyses did indicate further differ- ences between groups: the independent variables Aggressive, Gregarious, Distrustful, and Getting Along With Others all showed evidence of (non-hypothesized) significant dfiienames between their respective high and low groups on at least one affective measure at p<.05. The discussion section noted implications for this study and for future research, as well as methodological Frances W. Stott observations and recommendations for studies which use moving facial affect in the sampling procedures. To my parents, Wideha and Russell Stott who long ago provided me with the affective basis for my interest in this re- search area, who instilled values of intellectual curiosity and academic achievement that have served me in undertaking this degree program, and most importantly, who now celebrate with me in the pride and satisfaction of its completion. ii ACKNOWLEDGMENTS As this dissertation is the culmination of a degree program in Counseling Psychology, so it began not with a research proposal but at the Listening Ear Crisis Inter- vention Center of East Lansing, Michigan. Consequently, I wish first to acknowledge June Jacobson and Dozier Thornton, founders of the Center and initiators of my enthusiasm and understanding for applied psychology. I would like to ack- nowledge the depth and richness of their personal friend- ships for me, and to thank them for the values of personal integrity, professional excellence, and caring that they imparted to us all. I wish also to express many thanks to Bill Hinds, who was my first formal instructor in graduate psychology, who supported my application to this degree program, who served as academic advisor and dissertation chairperson throughout, and whose friendship I value highly. I would like to acknowledge and thank each of my committee members, each of whom has had personal impact on my graduate career, and all of whom have offered thoughtful criticisms as well as interest and support toward this research project: Bill Hinds, who introduced me to the area of facial iii affect, graciously endured the agonies of long distance consultation, and responded both expediently and thoroughly to letters, calls, forms, and drafts: Judith Taylor, who was singularly indispensable in her ability to make this research area a manageable one, to make research in general an intriguing and rewarding pro- cess, and in her generosity of time, expertise, personal interest, and an invaluable sense of humor; Dozier Thornton, who consistently offered studied and helpful suggestions as well as a personal interest in my research efforts; Joanne Hamachek, who conscientiously critiqued and consistently shared her enthusiasm; Bill Farquahr, who thought through with me in the important, initial stages and has maintained a supportive stance throughout. In addition, I would like to acknowledge Tim Sadler of Birkman and Associates, who provided computer re- sources and helpful suggestions along the way. As this research was conducted on the University of Texas-Austin campus, there were numerous persons from that University involved with its progress. I would especially like to acknowledge Ira Iscoe of the Psychology Department who mobilized local resources, acted as a local sponsor, iv and cared fervently that I was able to finish. I would like to acknowledge the Departments of Communication and Psychology, which allowed me to solicit subjects from among their students; the College of Educa- tion which permitted me the use of its videotape equipment, and particularly Jim Spear, Royal Embree, and Coystal Stone who facilitated that process. The University of Texas Counseling-Psychological Services Center allowed me the use of its facilities for the interviewing and videotaping of subjects, as well as for the drafting of the dissertation. I wish particularly to thank David Drum as its Director, Gus Baron who oversaw the project, and Rich Klein, who helped in setting up the equipment and literally abandoned his office space to two weeks of videotapes and clutter. Frank Karass patiently interviewed all 62 subjects, 'and offered support and friendship, as always. Dusty Humes. Karen Hill, and Elizabeth Sackton worked conscientiously as raters, shared my enthusiasm for the research, and at times restored it. Patsy Stice waded through the data with me, helping to tabulate, compile, ponder, and yet maintain a perspective sense of humor. My parents, Wideha and Russell Stott, contributed vacation days in Austin to re- cording data, proofing tables, and generally cheering me on. Susie Stiegelbauer contributed her artistry by drawing graphs and illustrations. To each, as well as to the 168 students who completed the instruments and the 62 who came for interviews, I express thanks and appreciation. To the many friends in East Lansing who were pleased to note my progress, I express thanks as well as delight in our continued friendships; and particularly to Becky Hollingsworth and June Jacobson, who provided un- wavering long distance encouragement, loved me throughout the project (and sometimes despite it) I am most grateful. Likewise, I express thanks to that small group of special Austin friends who insisted that I take time out to enjoy our friendships, and who generally kept my sense of warmth and playfulness alive. Finally, I would like to mention Roger Hall, who had no direct contact with this research project, but who has gently shaped my attitudes toward professionalism and psychotherapy, and who has had profound impact on me and on my life throughout this period of time. I wish to ack- nowledge him for his clinical skills as well as his artistry in his work as a psychotherapist, and to thank him for valuing me as I have come to know and value myself. vi Chapter I. II. III. IV. TABLE OF CONTENTS INTRODUCTION . . . . . . . . . . . . REVIEW OF LITERATURE . . . . . . . . Emotion . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Facial Expression of Emotion . . . . The Scoring of Facial Affect . . . . Affect Categories to be Used in This Summary . . . . . . . . . . . . . . RESEARCH DESIGN AND METHODOLOGY . . Sample . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Instrumentation . . . . . . . . . . Selection of Interview Sample . . . Interview and De-briefing Procedures The Interviewer . . . . . . . . . . Videotape Apparatus Used During Interviews Videotape Editing . . . . . . . . . Rater Sample . . . . . . . . . . . . Rater Training . . . . . . . . . . . Rater Reliability . . . . . . . . . Rater Agreement by Affect Category . Research Design . . . . . . . . . . Measurement of Facial Affect . . . . Research Hypotheses . . . . . . . . Statistical Analyses and Procedure . Summary . . . . . . . . . . . . . . ANALYSIS OF DATA . . . . . . . . . . Results of Hypothesis Tests . . . . vii Page 13 13 24 40 44 55 58 58 60 68 69 72 73 74 75 76 80 82 91 92 94 95 96 100 101 Chapter Further Analyses of Group Differences . . . Multivariate Analysis of Variance . . Further Analyses of Interest . . . . . Correlational Analyses . . . . . . . . . . Within Instrument Correlations . . . . Between Instrument Correlations . . . Correlations Within the Affective Measures . . . . . . . . . . . . . Correlations Between Test Instruments and Affective Measures . . . . . . Correlations Between Methodological Measures and Affective Measures . Summary . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . V. SUMMARY, CONCLUSIONS, AND DISCUSSION . . . Summary . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Results . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Discussion . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Findings . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Methodology . . . . . . . . . . . . . Implications . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Suggestions for Further Research . . . Implications for Therapy . . . . . . . APPENDICES C O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O A: Subject Consent Forms for Research Participation . . . . . . . . . . . B: Interview Materials . . . . . . . . . . C: Rater Training Material . . . . . . . D: Rater and Scoring Forms Used in Measurement of Facial Affect . . . . E: Supplementary Univariate Analyses . . . F: 24 X 24 Inter-Correlational Matrix . . BIBLIOGRAPHY O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O 0 viii Page 112 112 113 119 120 128 131 133 135 136 141 141 146 148 148 153 158 158 160 162 162 165 172 185 189 198 201 Table 3.1 4.1 4.8 4.9 4.10 4.11 LIST OF TABLES Rater Agreement by Affect Category . . . . . . Affective Scores for Subjects Selected for Analysis on Plutchik's Personality Dimensions. T-test Results for Between-Group Differences on Affective Measures . . . . . . . . . . . . Affective Scores for Subjects Selected for Analysis on Birkman's Personality Dimensions . T-test Results for Between-Group Differences on Affective Measures . . . . . . . . . . . . Affective Scores for Subjects Selected for Analysis on Eysenck's Personality Dimensions . T-test Results for Between-Group Differences on Affective Measures . . . . . . . . . . . . Multivariate Analysis of Variance for the Independent Variables on the Set of Dependent Measures 0 I O O O O O O O O O O O O O O I O I T-test Results of Gregarious Group Differences on Affective Measures . . . . . . . . . . . . Affect Scores for High and Low Gregarious Groups . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . T-test Results of Distrustful Group Differences on the Affective Measures . . . . . . . . . . Affect Scores for High and Low Distrustful Groups . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . T-test Results of Aggressive Group Differences on the Affective Measures . . . . . . . . . . Affect Scores for High and Low Aggressive Groups . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Inter-Scale Correlations for PEPI . . . . . . ix Page 83 103 104 107 108 110 111 112 115 116 116 117 118 119 123 Inter-Scale Correlations for EPI . . . . . . Inter-Scale Correlations for BM . . . . . . Correlations of Scales from Plutchik with Scales from Eysenck and Birkman . . . . . . Correlations Between Scales of the EPI and the BM 0 O O O O O O O O O C O O O O O O 0 0 Correlations Within the Set of Affective Measures 0 O O O O I O O I O O I I O O O O 0 Correlations Between Instrument Scales and Affective Measures . . . . . . . . . . . . . Correlations Between Methodological Measures and Affective Measures . . . . . . . . . . . Page 126 127 128 130 131 134 135 Figure 3.1 3.6 3.7 LIST OF FIGURES Percentage of rater agreement across 62 subjects 0 I O O I O O I O O O O O O O O O 82 Third rater's category where two raters were in agreement on Interest . . . . . . . . . . 86 Third rater's category where two raters were in agreement on Distress . . . . . . . . . . 86 Third rater's category where two raters were in agreement on Surprise . . . . . . . . . . 86 Third rater's category where two raters were in agreement on Anger . . . . . . . . . . . . 87 Third rater's category where two raters were in agreement on Enjoy . . . . . . . . . . . . 87 Third rater's category where two raters were in agreement on Disgust . . . . . . . . . . . 87 Between-group differences from independent variables on affective measures . . . . . . . 92 Plutchik's Emotion Circle . . . . . . . . . . 122 Inter-scale relationships for PEPI . . . . . 125 xi CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION While philosophers, artists, scientists, and poets have speculated for centuries about the phenomenon called emotion, it has only been since the late 1800's that the study has been approached from a systematic view- point. Scientists of the evolutionary, physiological, and cognitive traditions have each begun to crystalize theo- retical frameworks and compile empirical data in the area. Others have approached emotion from a psychometric tradition, and have worked to refine instruments that will yield data regarding the individual's affective or emotional orienta- tion. As new data continue to emerge, there has been recent speculation about the use of facial emotion as a psychodiagnostic tool. In‘a similar manner, the face itself received popular attention long before it became a topic of scien- tific inquiry. The seventeenth century salons of France for example, a popular gathering place of the literary set, were noted for such leisure time activities as speculation on facial type and corresponding personality characteris— tics (Adam, 1972). In recent popular literature, books such as Face Reading (Mar, 1975) undertake to predict per- sonality characteristics by a complex system of facial features, musculature, and bone structure. It is apparent that the face is a rich source of information; yet it re- mains unclear how we might systematically study and use this information for research purposes or therapeutic in- tervention. Only recently has empirical data begun to emerge that provides direction as to how we might use ob- servations of the face for these purposes. The facial expression of emotion was introduced by Darwin and has continued to attract researchers and theorists toward its exploration. Only in the last twenty years has the research and theoretical activity become suf- ficient to produce what is now an emerging body of liter- ature. Based largely on the work of Sylvan Tomkins (l962, 1963 , 1965, 1970) who has worked to establish a theoretical basis for emotional experience and psychological functioning with some speculation on facial relatedness, Paul Ekman and Carroll Izard are currently in the forefront of research in facial affect. Each has worked independently to establish systems for identifying emotions through facial expressions, to consolidate and establish major categories of facial af- fect, to train others in the recognition and identification of these categories, and Izard, to use these categories as a basis for discerning psychological information about the individual. Theory The three theoretical assumptions upon which this study is based are as follows: 1) that the face provides immediate and specific information regarding human emotions: 2) that each fundamental emotion has a characteristic fa- cial expression recognizable as distinct from the others; and 3) that the fundamental human emotions have a psycho- logical relationship to the individual's personality. That the face provides immediate and specific in- formation regarding human emotions is in part based on evi- dence that facial expressions are instinctive and occur re- flexively or automatically as a part of the experiential emotion process (Izard, 1977). While the adult may not be as spontaneous nor as automatic in expression as is the infant, this later inhibition is the result of learning and experience, and does not contradict the assumption that fa- cial expressions are instinctive. Evidence that emotions are hereditary and bear a close resemblance to the expres- sions of nonhuman animals (Darwin, 1910; Izard, 1971) would seem in further support of these instinctual and re- flexive qualities. Studies of infants and mothers indicate that the face is an extraordinarily important social stimulus, and plays a vital role in the early bonding that takes place between mother and child (Tomkins, 1962; Izard, 1971, 1977). For human beings, evidence indicates that facial patterns as communication cues have gained independence and in many cases priority over posture, locomotion, and envi- ronment (Ekman, Friesen and Ellsworth, 1971; Izard, 1971). The assumption that each fundamental emotion has a characteristic facial expression recognizable as distinct from the others has led researchers to investigate in one of two ways: to assume the existence of the discrete ex- pressions and seek to determine the cause and role of the neuromuscular activity involved (Ekman, 1965); or to dis- cover the degree of consensus which could be obtained in judging discrete facial expressions (Izard, 1971). Subse- quently, Ekman has mapped the face, and Izard has estab- lished consensus. In an effort to establish the cross-cultural valid- ity of these discrete expressions, the work of Ekman and Izard has shown that certain emotions have the same expressions and experiential qualities in widely different cultures and throughout the world. While it is generally agreed that each culture has its own idiosyncratic rules for when or under what circumstances an emotion can be appropriate- ly displayed, and rules for how to inhibit or mask an emo- tion, it is nevertheless apparent that when the emotion is expressed, its corresponding facial expression is sim— ilar across peoples. Percentages of agreement upon dis- crete categories of facial affect range from 47 percent to 95 percent (Ekman, 1973a; Izard, 1977). That fundamental human emotions have a psycho- logical relationship to the individual's personality is based largely in the work of Tomkins and Izard. Tomkins (1962, 1963) maintains a distinction between emotions and drives, and the phenomenon he describes as the ideo-af- fective organization. This ideo-affective organization consists of a dynamic and relatively stable relationship between an affect (emotion and drive) on the one hand, and certain cognitive processes such as ideas and beliefs on the other. Tomkins assumes that emotion is not a global, undifferentiated experience but rather that there are dis- crete emotions which interrelate in an overall emotions system, and that this emotions system regulates the drives as well as other personality subsystems. In this way, the emotions are both independent of cognition and inter- act with it. Izard also speaks to the interaction between emotion and cognition, and conceptualizes it as an af- fective-cognitive structure. This psychological organi- zation of affect and cognition he believes is a trait-like phenomenon that results from repeated interactions between a particular affect or pattern of affects and a particular set or configuration of cognitions (Izard, 1977). A com- plex affective-cognitive structure, or an interrelated set of structures, he posits, may constitute an affective- cognitive orientation, or a more global personality trait, trait complex, or disposition such as introversion. In combination, these theoretical assumptions suggest that the face is a reliable source of information about human emotion, that it is possible to distinguish which emotion is being expressed on the basis of facial display, that these emotions have a psychological relation- ship to the individual's personality, and that, therefore, relationships between one's emotional experience and one's personality traits may be studied through observation of the facial expression of emotion. Need While there is growing evidence that an individ- ual's emotional experience is accurately (and perhaps most fully) expressed in his face, and while there are years of recorded speculations about the relationship be- tween emotional orientation and personality characteristics there is yet very little that addresses the question of re- latedness of facial activity to personality. To establish this relatedness would mean that a whole range of psycho- therapeutic possibilities and research questions currently unavailable might become promising to pursue. If there is evidence that the type of facial affect one displays is related to personality dimensions, for ex- ample, then it becomes crucial to note the client who nearly always looks disgusted, or angry, or is smiling, and to begin exploring this predominant affect as a diag- nostic tool. Similarly, for the client who has a limited range of affect display, and whose face never expresses more than two or three of eight basic affects, it may be possible that the observation can be used psychodiagnostically. While research in the area of facial affect has demonstrated relationships between personality type and ability to identify facial affect in others, there is only minimal research to indicate a relationship between per- sonality dimensions for corresponding expressions of facial affect. No study thus far has attempted to explore a vari- ety of personality dimensions for corresponding expressions of facial affect. Thus itwould seem that an initial study is necessary to determine whether these questions of relat- edness are even promising to pursue. In the event that meaningful relatedness can be established, then it would also seem promising to later explore the possibility of identifying and defining basic affective personality types from the facial affect perspective, as well as their corresponding treatment modalities. Purpose The primary purpose of this study is to explore the assumption that information about personality charac- teristics related to emotional style is expressed in the face. The secondary purpose of the study is to investi- gate whether methods used in this study appear adequate for use in subsequent research efforts in the field of facial affect. The study will investigate whether there are significant differences in expressions of facial affect as recorded on videotape and measured by trained judges between persons with specific personality characteris- tics as measured by pencil/peper personality instru- ments. The specific questions that will be addressed are: lx>persons on specified personality dimensions vary in the types of facial affect they express; Do persons on specified personality dimensions vary in their frequency of change from one affect to another; and Do persons on specified personality dimensions vary in the range (or number of types of affects) they express. The methodological purpose of the study will respond to the problems of measuring, quantifying, and of recording facial affect. The study is intended to contribute to that literature by describing any further refinements in techniques and procedures, and will con- tinue to address the issue of whether current technology permits meaningful data in this area. Hypotheses The 15 hypotheses used in this study are designed to test the differences in expressions of facial affect between persons of specific personality types. This will be done by comparing groups of persons with these specific personality traits on affective measures designed to indicate type, range, and amount of affec- tive change displayed. These hypotheses are theoretically based in the literature pertaining to facial affect, particularly in the work of Tomkins (1962, 1963, 1965 and Izard, 1971, 1972, 1977), and exploratory in nature. 10 The following are the research hypotheses, which are stated generally again in Chapter III, and for which the results are reported in Chapter IV. H1: The group mean of subjects who score high on the Gregarious scale will be significantly higher on the affective measure Enjoy than will be the group mean of subjects scoring low on the Gregarious scale, corresponding to the predominance of gregariousness they experience. H2: The group mean of subjects scoring high on the Distrustful scale will be significantly higher on the affective measure Disgust than will be the group mean of subjects scoring low on the Distrustful scale, corresponding to the predominance of distrustfulness they experience. H3: The group mean of subjects scoring high on the Depressed scale will be significantly higher on the affective measure Distress than will be the group mean of subjects scoring low on the Depressed scale, corresponding to the predominance of distress they experience. H4: The group mean of subjects scoring high on the Dyscontrolled scale will be significantly higher on the affective measure Surprise than will be the group mean of subjects scoring low on the Dyscontrolled scale, corresponding to the predom- inance of dyscontrol they experience. H5: The group mean of subjects scoring high on the Aggressive scale will be significantly higher on the affective measure Anger than will be the group mean of subjects scoring low on this scale, corres— ponding to the predominance of agression they experience. H6: The group mean of subjects who score high on the Getting Along With Others scale will be signif- icantly higher on the affective measure Range of Affect than will be the group mean of subjects scor- ing low on this scale, corresponding to emotionality and a need for contact with others. 11 H7: The group mean of subjects who score high on the Getting Alone With Others scale will be significantly lower on the affective measure Disgust than will be the group mean of subjects scoring low on this scale, corresponding to the minimization of rejection toward others. H3: The group mean of subjects who score high on the Getting Along With Others scale will be significantly higher on the affective measure Enjoy than will be the group mean of subjects scoring low on this scale, corresponding to a need for incorpora- tion and social bonding with others. H9: The group mean of subjects who score high on the Getting Along With Others scale will be significantly higher on the affective measure Change of Affect than will be the group mean of subjects who score low on this scale, corresponding to expressed emotionality and a sensitivity toward others. H10: The group mean of subjects who score high on the Dominance scale will be significantly lower on the affective measure Distress than will be the group mean of subjects who score low on this scale, corresponding to need for authority and control, and a minimization of sensitivity to distress. H11: The group mean of subjects who score high on the Dominance scale will be significantly lower on the affective measure of Change of Affect than will be the group mean of subjects who score low on this scale, corresponding to a need for con— trol in emotional expressiveness. H12: The group mean of subjects who score high on the Extraversion scale will be significantly higher on the affective measure Enjoy than will be the group mean of subjects who score low on this scale, corresponding to their desire for inclusion and social bonding. H13: The group mean of subjects who score high on the Extraversion scale will be significantly lower on the affective measure Disgust than will be the group mean of subjects who score low on this scale, corresponding to the need to include rather than to reject or push away from. 12 H14: The group mean of subjects who score high on the Neuroticism scale will be significantly higher on the affective measure Change of Affect than will be the group mean of subjects who score low on this scale, corresponding to emotional liability as measured by this scale. H15: The group mean of subjects who score high on the Extraversion and Neuroticism scales (EN) will be significantly higher on the affective measure Range of Affect than will be the group mean of sub- jects who do not score high on both of these scales (ES, IS, IN), corresponding to emotional liability and freedom of experiences. Overview In Chapter II, the literature pertinent to the theory and research of facial affect is presented, with a focus on the areas of emotion, the facial expression of emotion, the scoring of facial affect, and the specific affect categories to be used in this study. Chapter III describes the subject sample, the instrumentation for personality dimensions, the research procedures, the mea- surement of facial affect, the hypotheses, the analyses, and the experimental design. The analysis of data for each hypothesis is presented in Chapter IV, along with the results of supplementary analyses used for explora- tory purposes in this study. Chapter V includes a sum- mary of this investigation, a discussion of the findings and the limitations, and implications for psychotherapy and for further research in the area of facial affect and personality dimensions. CHAPTER II REVIEW OF LITERATURE This study focuses on the relationships between personality dimensions and the expression of facial af- fect. In order to provide the theory and supporting re- search upon which this study is based, literature will be reviewed in the areas of emotion, the facial expres- sion of emotion, the scoring of facial affect, and for the specific affect categories to be used in this study. Emotion Definitions and Major Theorists The phenomenon we describe as human emotion has for centuries been described, identified, researched, and debated and has, as yet, eluded any definitive or conclusive definition. Even the distinction between an emotion and a feeling is a clouded one, with its own historical and evolutional characteristics (Candland, 1977, p. 4). Despite this, however, emotion continues to be a concept central to psychological theorists and consistently utilized by the practicing psychotherapist. The DSM II (as an indicator of current trends in 13 14 psychiatry) devotes an entire section to "major affective disorders" and lists eight diagnostic categories within. Freud, Jung, Adler, Horney, Ellis, Rogers, and Perls each in some way incorporates the emotional experience into his or her theoretical approach and psychotherapeu— tic goal. And while strict behaviorists such as Wolpe focus specifically on the cognitive aspect for treatment, they are nevertheless striving to modify the affective experience of fear or anxiety. Thus usage of the term and utilization of the concept remain consistently central to many areas of modern psychology, despite the lack of clarity or agreement about the phenomenon to which we refer as "emotion." Several theorists have suggested that the term "emotion" refers, in all probability, to a variety of experiences and processes, yet undifferentiated from one another (Candland, 1977; Izard, 1971; Kaywin, 1966; Arnold, 1960) which serves to perpetuate the divergence of thinking in the area. Additionally, however, lack of agreement does not imply fault, but rather reflects the complexity of the area and the fact that much re- mains to be learned. Candland (1977) asserts that there are yet two major disagreements: how emotion is to be defined; and (given our models of mind and 15 behavior) what is the nature of its logical structure (pg. 12). With regard to these questions, three major schools of thought have emerged over the last century as theorists have addressed the problem from the evolu- tional, the biological, or the cognitive perspective. Each will be briefly summarized with regard to the ma- jor theorists and the primary contributions to the field of emotion. The evolutional theorists. Of primary signifi- cance in this area is the work of Charles Darwin, and particularly his book entitled The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals (1910). The work was intended to show that the ontogeny and phylogeny of emotion were susceptible to the principle of natural selection, though with the obvious shortcoming that there is no historical data from which to trace an evolutionary de- velopment. While Darwin was not the first to propose that characteristics may change slowly over time by a process of natural selection, he was clearly the most influential in his observations and writings of this phenomenon. The impact of the evolutional approach has been to assume a survival value for finely differentiated 16 facial expressions and bodily postures, and attempt to determine the reason for their survival value (Candland, 1977). The work of Chevalier-Skolnikoff (1973) and of Izard (1971, 1972, 1977) is clearly influenced by this approach as they have sought to establish the functions and adaptive features of the expression of emotion in both primates and humans. The biological theorists. While precursors of William James (including Aristotle and Descartes) looked toward the body and used physiological functioning as a primary source for understanding and explaining emotion, it was James (circa 1890) who brought physiological theories to the forefront within both the lay and scien- tific communities (Candland, 1977). The predominant characteristics of James' approach were the emphasis on the peripheral component of the emotional experience, and the emphasis on Darwin‘s work which incorporated the principle of natural selection. In the peripheral vs. central argument, James assumed that it was the peri- pheral body states which were initially altered, and then searched for accompanying changes in reported emo- tional state (James, 1890). In addition to pOpulariz- ing and returning interest to the physiological ap— proach in the study of emotion, James leaves us with two 17 major questidns: are there physiological correlates of emotion; and if so, do we experience them before, after, or during the perception. There has been considerable research interest in the area of physiological correlates of emotion (Black, 1970; Brady, 1970; Arnold, 1960a, 1960b, 1970; Ax, 1953), further stimulated by the fact that investigators have consistently found some relationship between the emotional state and the physiological state. This rela- tionship, however, remains unspecific. It has not yet been possible to correlate a precise emotional state, or change from one specific emotion to another, with a mea- surable physiological change. The cognitive theorists. Proponents of this ap- proach hold that psychological understanding comes from examining consciousness, or objects as they are con- sciously perceived. In an attempt to meld psychologi- cal principles with newly discovered evolutional princi- ples, or "to seek for principles and causes of mental evolution in man" (Romanes, 1884), George Romanes de- fined the mature mind as comprising emotion, will, and intellect. It was Freud (1938), however, who spoke to the interrelatedness of these processes, and presented the 18 mind as a coherent whole, a body perceiving, apprehend- ing, appraising, and acting as a cognitive agent. While Freud is not best known as a theorist or researcher in the area of emotion, his influence on the cognitive the- orists has nevertheless had longlasting effects. Schachter and Singer (1962) represent a contem- porary version of the cognitive school of emotions by defining emotion as undifferentiated arousal plus cogni- tion. In their view the underlying physiological state is the same for all emotions, and qualitative distinc- tions result from cognitive appraisal or evaluation of the situation that elicited the arousal. More recently, however, Plutchik has developed an argument for interaction of cognition and emotion, and in his work "Cognitions in the Service of the Emo- tions" (1977), he carefully develops his thesis that cognitive functions have evolved "in the service of needs and emotions" (pg. 190). The argument is based on the premise that sensory stimulus is the primary emo- tional experience, but that evaluation of the stimulus event has led to an intricate and sophisticated system of cognition-affect relations. Congruent with princi- ples in his basic theory of emotions (Plutchik, 1962), he develops this argument from an evolutionary 19 standpoint and in a context that includes all phylogen- ic levels of animal life. Thus Plutchik presents both emotional and cognitive processes as evolntknmrihradap- tive as well as vital to the organism, but in a manner which emphasizes their reciprocal nature. The Measurement of Affective Orientations Still another approach to the study of emotion has been to assume a theoretical position and then at- tempt to test it within the framework of personality measurement. Allen and Hamsher (1974) for example dis- tinguished three aspects of emotionality: responsive- ness (intensity of affect), expressiveness (interper- sonal communication of affect), and orientation (atti- tudes toward emotion) and constructed a Test of Emotion- al Styles on this basis. Other instruments such as the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory have assumed a theoretical stance pertaining not so much to what emo- tion is, but rather how to distinguish normal from ab- normal populations on the basis of emotion related be- havior, and then pursued the usage of those distinctions as a psychodiagnostic tool. Of particular interest to this study are three instruments which utilize very different constructs but all of which attempt to measure affective orientation in 20 some manner: the Eysenck Personality InventQEY: the Birkman Method, and Plutchik's Emotions Profile Index. The Eysenck Personality Inventory (based on the earlier Maudsley Personality Inventory) comes directly from the work of Heyman and Wiersma, who were among the first to use the modern approaches of measurement and calculation (Eysenck, 1969, p. 25). Similarly, Eysenck comes from a tradition of personality measurement rather than emotion theory; his primary focus was to describe behavior and classify personalities by certain hypothet- ical traits, while working to refine the empirical ap- proach. At an earlier point, Heyman and Wiersma had col- 1ected data on more than 2,000 individuals using a three- dimensional rating system composed of: emotionality, activity, and extraversion-introversion. In a later statistical analysis of the data, Eysenck determined that emotionality was relatively orthogonal but that ac- tivity and extraversion were highly correlated. Subse- quently he eliminated activity and maintained the dimen- sions of emotionality or neuroticism, and extraversion- introversion, thereby incorporating elements of affective styles into his attempts to describe personality types. As Eysenck points out, both his method and his 21 constructs are closely related to predecessors of the Greek and Renaissance periods, who attempted to classify man on the basis of temperament (sanguine, melancholic, choleric, and phlegmatic), and to Wandt who took the fourfold division and proposed two major divisions: strength of emotion, and speed of change of emotion. Wandt's dimensions became emotional-non-emotional, and changeable-unchangeable, and were based on an assumption similar to Eysenck's: that it is desirable to reduce the observed correlations between large numbers of traits to a smaller number of more fundamental dimen— sions or all-embracing types (Eysenck, 1969, p. 14). Also from a measurement background, Birkman (1961) undertook to develop a personality inventory that would 1) survey personality differences using new- ly constructed social and self-perception questionnaires, and 2) provide data for analyzing these differences on the basis of positive or negative social perceptions. Working from a theoretical point of View that assumes that behavior is a function of the meaning the individual assigns to stimuli, and that there are two pivotal points of positive and negative perception around which personality organizes (Birkman, 1961, p. 5), Birkman has constructed an instrument which 22 classifies the individual on the basis of twelve perfor- mance traits. The data from the social vs. self-percep- tion dichotomy and the positive vs. negative social per- ceptions generated by the instrument are used to be able to predict interpersonal needs both in normal situations and in situations when the individual is under stress. As with Eysenck, Birkman enters the literature on emotion from the standpoint of a psychometrist rath- er than as an emotions theorist. He incorporates emo- tional dimensions into the instrument by blending them with behavioral and attitudinal characteristics in order to define his basic personality traits. The self-con- sciousness dimension, for example, is a blend of emo- tionality, sensitivity to others, achievement orienta- tion, and nervousness around superiors. Birkman's in— strument, then, uses personality dimensions that are in- terpersonal in nature and provide a collection of state- ments about the individual's affective orientation in combination with a variety of other statements about his interpersonal style. In contrast to Eysenck and Birkman, Plutchik is an emotions theorist and offers an instrument entitled Emotions Profile Index, which is based on his own pre- viously noted theory of emotion. It is designed to 23 measure the relative importance to each individual of what he calls prototypic patterns of emotions. These eight prototypic patterns, so named because the behav- iors are identifiable at all phylogenetic levels of ani- mal life, are identified by terms such as destruction, protection, rejection, reproduction, exploration and orientation. These terms are intended to identify be- havior patterns that are basic to all species in re- sponding to stimuli, and suggest a clear evolutional function: "prototypic patterns of behavior are 2922' tiye and help the organism in its struggle for surviv- al" (1965, pg. 106). Plutchik further proposes that "all other emo- tions are combinations of these few primary ones, just as all colors are mixtures of a few primary colors" 1965, p. 296). In addition, the prototypic patterns have been labeled in such a way as to emphasize the existence of bipolarities: destruction vs. protection, or incor- poration vs. rejection. In situations where basic reac- tion patterns are aroused at the same time, for example destruction (or anger) and protection (or fear), a con- flict develops. In general, says Plutchik, "situations which arouse Opposite action tendencies generate con- flict, and tend to produce inhibition or immobilization" 24 (p. 106, 1966). The therapeutic implications suggested here are to respond to the emotional conflict, either by discerning it from the situation presented, or by iden- tifying it on the basis of the client's profile from Plutchik's Emotions Profile Index. These instruments, the Test of Emotional Styles, the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory, the E17 senck Personality Profile, the Birkman Method, the Plutchik's Emotions Profile Index, are representative of current efforts to establish personality characteris— tics or affectively based interpersonal styles on the basis of the individual's emotional experience or affec- tive orientation. While some address the issue of "what is emotion" more directly than others, all attempt to differentiate types of people into a manageable number of categories which describe traits or behavior related to the emotional experience. Facial Expression of Emotion As with the phenomenon of emotion itself, the concept that facial expressions are related to internal emotional experience has been assumed for centuries but elusive of systematic scientific investigation. The arts have made use of the concept despite its lack of 25 scientific validation. Delsarte's system for training actors relied heavily upon observations about individual parts of the body and the moods, attitudes, and emotions expressed by the specific position of the minutest de- tail. To express fury or madness, for example, the ac- tor was trained to lower the brow portion nearest the nose, and to raise the brow near the outer edge (Steb- bins, 1886, pg. 145). To express a neutral feeling, the eyeball must be "calm and midway between the two corners" (Stebbins, 1886, pg. 138). Chekhov used his observations not to train oth- ers but to describe them. In his short story "The Kiss," Chekhov describes personalities at a dinner party by giv- ing them types of faces: a flat face, a clever face, a well-fed face, and a face with forced smiles. Even the central character is revealed to us in this manner, as "one whose face seemed to say...'I am the shyest, most modest, and most undistinguished officer in the whole brigadel'" (Chekhov, 1965, pg. 1052). A contemporary of Delsarte and Chekhov, it was Charles Darwin who first proposed from the scientist's standpoint that facial expressions were related to in- ternal emotional states, and set about describing and recording his observations in a manner that has stimulated 26 scientific investigation up to the present day. Darwin and The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals Published subsequent to his famous Origin of the Species, Darwin's The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals applied the principles of evolution and natural selection to his notions about facial expressions of emo- tion, suggesting that they had a purposeful function and survival value for the organism. In this later work, Darwin presents three principles of expression: 1) that serviceable actions become habitual in association with certain states of the mind (and the subsequent passage of habits into reflex actions); 2) the principle of anti- thesis (or development of some behaviors simply because they are opposite to others rather than because they have functional value of their own); and 3) the principle of direct action of the excited nervous system on the body (or involuntary expression and behavior) (Darwin, 1910). Using examples from observations of children, animals, mentally ill, blind, and members of different cultures, Darwin identifies more than thirty emotions and expres- sions thereof, describing in detail the elements of the expression and theorizing about the function and surviv- al purpose of the emotion. 27 While it was nearly a century before the tenets expressed by Darwin became active in empirical research (Ekman, 1973b), it is nevertheless clear that Darwin's observations and theoretical principles have served the function of pioneer work in the field, and have had pro- found impact on those who followed. Current Theory and Research in Facial Expression of Affect Proceeding from Darwin's assumptions that emotion- al experience and facial expressions are related, Sylvan Tomkins undertook a two-volume work entitled Affect, Im- agery and Consciousness wherein he identifies and de- scribes all major affect categories and hypothesizes in detail regarding the development of the specific affects as well as their psychodynamic functions. These psycho- dynamic functions will be addressed later under'Affect Categories to be Used in This Study" and are to date the primary theoretical basis for assuming a relationship between personality characteristics and expressions of facial affect. It is Tomkins who introduces the term affect in- to the literature, and uses the term as synonomous with emotion. Tomkins conceives of affect as primarily 28 facial behavior, and secondarily as bodily (outer skele- tal and inner visceral) behavior. When we become aware of our facial and/or visceral responses, he hypothesizes, we are aware of our affects (Tomkins, vol. 1, 1962). Thus Tomkins attempts the logical extension of Darwin's work, proceeding from biological on to psychological survival. Both an active theorist and researcher, Izard has recently proposed that the facial expression of an emotion can actually determine the quality of the emo- tional experience. How specific, mixed, conflicted, or vague the emotional experience is, says Izard, is relat- ed to and/or determined by not only the visceral experi- ence, but to what extent and in what manner the emotion is expressed in the face (1972). Thus Izard's position, along with that of Tomkins, emerges as compatible with Darwin's as each explores and argues the adaptive and functional purposes of emotion and the facial expression of emotion. Izard goes one step beyond by arguing that the expression of the emotion interacts with and shapes the emotional experience itself, and thatit is the facial expression which we must study and regard as the criti- cal feature in the process. In this light, Izard has conducted exploratory 29 studies into the functional purposes of facial affect by surgically sectioning the facial nerve of rhesus monkeys (rendering them incapable of facial display or emotional expression in the face) and observing the subsequent ef- fects on social status, mating, and child rearing (1971). While the findings from these studies are reported as preliminary, the subsequent increase in unusual behav- iors, the loss of status among ruling females, and the social changes made within the observed monkey community point to this direction in research as promising. Beyond the theories and research of Tomkins and Izard, which seek to substantiate the relationship of internal emotional experience and facial expression of affect as well as to address the psychological and so- cial functions of those facial expressions, much of the research to date has centered around the identification and validation of facial affect categories (presented later under "The Scoring of Facial Affect"). An out- growth of this research in affect categories has been to study the characteristics of how people apply the af- fect categories or are able to use them in identifying facial affect in others. Zlatchin, for example, compared a group of male medical students to male and female Haight-Ashbury 30 residents and found that those persons who were more in- volved, steadfast, and better adjusted in terms of group norms had significantly higher overall accuracy in rec- ognizing specific categories of emotion than were those who were alienated, depressed, or withdrawn (Zlatchin, 1974). Both Schiffenbauer and Zuckerman have contrast- ed groups of males and females in their ability to label facial affect in same sex and opposite sex faces, and have found significant differences 1) in the females' ability to label more accurately (Schiffenbauer, 1976), and 2) in the attributions that males and females assign to faces expressing unexpectedly hnfimse (non-normative) emotion (Zuckerman et 31., 1976). Dougherty, Bartlett, and Izard investigated the pattern of perceptual dysfunction in schizophrenics by comparing their judgments of facial affect to judgments by normals. This study produced significant findings in l) the differential number of times specific categories were used by the two groups, and 2) the overall accuracy of the two groups in recognizing specific affect cate- gories (Dougherty et. 21., 1974). Sackheim, Gur and Saucy have approached facial affect from the standpoint of facial symmetry and found 31 that left side composites of the face were judged to ex- press emotions more intensely than right side composites. Findings of this study have stimulated speculations about using the face as a mode for examining the nature of functional brain symmetry as well as the neurologi- cally based organization of emotions and emotional com- munication (Sackheim, Gur, Saucy, 1978). Current theoretical and research efforts in this area, then, continue to seek refinement in describing the relationship between internal emotion states and facial expressions, and have begun to yield empirical data about ability to use facial affect categories as well. Facial vs. Other Nonverbal and Verbal Expressions To what extent the face is a reliable and accur- ate source of information has been investigated from a variety of approaches. The question of context, or whether knowledge of the situation precipitating the emo- tion is necessary in order to make a correct interpreta- tion or judgment of the facial expression, is one ad— dressed by both Munn (1940) and again by Turhan (1950), Munn developed two sets of slides, one with a context background and one with the face alone, and found no difference in the percentage of agreement 32 among his raters for the two sets. Turhan found that judgments of emotion based on the isolated face were con- siderably different from judgments of the same face when viewed in a context, and concluded that judgments of emotions depend heavily on the perception or interpreta- tion of the total situation in which the stimulus face is involved. Izard (1971) regards the argument in an evolution- ary-phylogenetic perspective, and posits that "the impor- tance of facial communication as compared with postural activity in emotion, and particularly in emotion differ- entiation and emotion communication, increases with phy- logenetic and ontogenetic development" (1971, pg. 189). He illustrated this by citing the 1967 work of VanHooff, suggesting that facial displays become somewhat more in- dependent of posture and locomotion in the anthropoid apes compared to the rhesus monkeys. Izard summarizes that in humans, there is mounting evidence to indicate that facial patterns as communicative cues have gained considerable independence from posture, locomotion, and context (1971, pg. 192). Ekman (1965, 1967) found that in humans, posture indicates something of the intensity and of the global nature of the emotion, while the facial pattern is 33 characteristic of a particular discrete emotion and may convey additional information with regard to an emotion mixture or combination. In later, related experiments he has tested the prominence of facial vs. body cues by asking subjects to try to deceive judges (Ekman and Friesen, 1969a, 1974a). The results from these studies indicate that if a person is trying to disguise or hide his feelings, he will attend most carefully to his face as the area to be most controlled. In this situation, observers are reported to judge emotions more accurately from the body than from the face. In addition, Ekman has investigated the congruence between facial expres- sion and verbal statements in an interview setting, and found a significant and reliable relationship between the two. Thus Ekman's work suggests the face as the more specific conveyor of emotion, but notes that it is also the more readily disguised communication source, sug- gesting not so much that it must be observed in context, but that it is useful to interpret in combination with other body cues. Of particular research interest has been the question of whether facial affect is a pan-cultural or culture specific phenomenon. From Darwin's position of discrete emotions, each having a specific, innately 34 programmed facial pattern, the argument is pro cross- cultural or universal expressions. The research evidence is mixed at this point, but there is strong evidence to suggest that basic emotions such as anger, disgust, fear, surprise, and enjoyment are experienced and labeled sim- ilarly across cultures, while the more subtle emotions such as bliss, worry, and upset are more culture specif- ic. As Harrison (1975) points out, there is not this same kind of agreement across cultures for "emblems" or non-facial gestures, indicating that nonverbal gestures with arms, hands, body posture, and physical proximity have been clearly substantiated as culture specific. The implicit relevance of this research is that work with nonverbally expressed emotions in culturally mixed popu- lations must rely heavily in facial expressions as op- posed to body cues, and will probably have greatest ef- ficacy if restricted to the emotions previously re- searched and established as more basic to all cultures. The face as an indicator of emotion, then, was introduced by Darwin and substantiated by Tomkins, Munn, Turhan, Izard, and Ekman, both in terms of the functional purposes of the emotion expressed, and the importance of the emotion expressed by the face. A proliferation of research headed by Iaard and Ekman suggests that the 35 face, in or out of context, in combination with other body cues, in congruence with verbal behavior, and as cross-culturally similar, is in fact not only a reli- able and appropriate place to study affect, but perhaps one of the most important vantage points from which to study it. Affective Facial Display as a Psychodiagnostic Tool While viewing hours of client videotape for another purpose, Haggard and Isaacs stumbled across a phenomenon they later called micro-momentary expression: a facial expression so short-lived that it seems to be quicker than the eye (1966, pg. 154). As they further explored the phenomenon, establishing methods for re- cording and measuring it and searching for meaningful correlates, some interesting speculations regarding ap- plications to psychotherapy began to emerge. The rapid changes of MME's seemed to occur most often, for example, when normal defenses were function- ing effectively (periods of general expressiveness) or in a conflict context (when denial statements or verbal blocking were evident as identified by incongruence be- tween the verbal content and the facial expression). Based on these observations, Haggard and Isaacs 36 speculated that micromomentary changes appeared to be re- lated to intrapsychic dynamics. This speculation was further supported by correlating the number of MME's to the manifest content with which they were associated during a therapy session, and finding a positive rela- tionship between the frequency of the two (1966, pg. 162). Haggard and Isaacs propose that micro-momentary expressions may be indicators of ego mechanisms, and ob- servable in psychotherapy. They speculate that MME's might, for example, be particularly frequent during ther- apy when newly released but not yet integrated impulses and affects are sporadically monitored by previously es- tablished controls. During this phase of "transitional instability," the individual might allow split second expressions of new affect, resulting in an MME rather than a fully expanded expression of emotion. If that were, in fact, the case then an increased frequency of MME's could be an indicator of therapeutic change in process. While many of these speculations yet lack the empirical evidence to substantiate them, there is never- theless sufficient evidence to suggest that they are promising directions in which to move. Building from this work of MME's and ego 37 mechanisms, Wilson (1976a) investigated the relationship between repression and facial affect, hypothesizing that there would be a positive relationship between frequency of micro-momentary expressions and other indicators of psychological defense Operations. "To the extent" says Wilson "that micro-momentary expression originates as a manifestation of self-deception, to that extent is it the result of repression-defense Operations and fits Freud's view of repressive Operations” (1976b). While the study did not yield as clear cut a re- lationship as had been hoped for, there was nevertheless sufficient evidence to indicate that "in clinical settings, a high rate of emission of micro-momentary expressions suggests an hypothesis of difficulty with the expression of anger coupled with fear or anxiety over interpersonal safe- ty" (1976b, pg. 7). In summary, Wilson concludes that micro-momentary expressions appear, at this point, to be a psychological trait related to trait defensiveness, and that the frequency of emission seems to be related to the expression of angry feelings. Based on Kell and Mueller' 3 observations of compacted affective experiences (1966) , Hinds explores this phenomenon by positing the relationship between facial expressions and intrapsychic conflict. "Compacted affect" says Hinds "is the inhibition Of one affect by the expression of another, 38 resulting in the compression of the affective experience and of its display on the face (1976, pg. 1). Hinds' theoretical position is that not only thoughts but af- fects serve as an internal stimulus fixsanxiety, which consequently serve to maintain the individual's neurotic behavior through negative reinforcement. The self-de- feating behavior may be sustained, for example, in order to avoid the more painful affect such as fear or anxiety. Hinds' theory is that these painful affects are experi- enced, but the experience is compressed so as to mini- mize the discomfort; this compression of affect leaves the individual without a full range of affective experi- ences, thus limiting his behaviors, alternatives, and problem solving capacity. Hinds notes the use of facial affect as an important diagnostic tool for the therapist in this area and points to the potential for using facial affective patterning "as a means for therapists to dis- cover how affects influence and maintain neurotic behav— ior" (1976, pg. 3). Finally, Izard's work on emotion, and particu- larly on emotion as it is expressed in the face, has pointed again and again to the face as a potential psy- chodiagnostic cue. With regard to the symmetry of facial motion, he reports that subjects under LSD treatment show 39 a considerable decrease of symmetry quotients, and that movements become less extensive and synchronous for per- sons under mental stress (1971, pg. 354). Additionally Izard reports work using the assessment of expressions and gestures as a basis for a prognosis in behavior ther- apy with autistic children (1971, pg. 371); work with us- ing the identification of facial affect in others as a psychodiagnostic cue (1971, pg. 372); and his own work with an electromyograph (EMG) on facial muscles to de- lineate different types of emotional tensions as well as in training subjects to become more sensitive to their own internal cues (1971, pg. 391). While there are yet no readily available or wide- ly substantiated techniques or procedures for using fa- cial expressions diagnostically, there are nevertheless individual and isolated evidences that this is a promis- ing area to explore. Haggard and Isaacs, Wilson, Hinds, and Izard all point to psychodiagnostic information po- tentially available through the face could be useful and is to some extent unavailable through other diagnostic measures. Technological issues (to be discussed in "The Scoring of Facial Affect") are troublesome. To date, 40 the only instruments for recording facial data are video (still photos, movies, and videotapes) and physiological (as in the electromyograph); the equipment is cumber- some, expensive, and yields data that is time consuming to judge. In addition, the theoretical arguments and experimental results are clearly at an exploratory level in this area. Nevertheless the theoretical framework and technological data are already providing information useful to the trained eye of the psychotherapist, at least for his or her own speculative purposes. The Scoring of Facial Affect General Background As the problems of measuring, judging, or scoring facial affect are reviewed, it becomes pertinent to re- turn once again to the consideration of what an emotion is. Since this study is focused specifically on the fa- cial expression of emotion, considerations and defin- itions at this point will be based on the literature in that area rather than the literature on emotion in gen- eral. 41 Izard offers a definition, however, that attempts a com- parative perspective as well as a specifically useful guideline: Emotion is a complex concept with neurophysiolog- ical, neuromuscular, and phenomenological aspects. At the neurophysiological level emotion is de- fined primarily in terms of electrochemical ac- tivity in the nervous system, particularly in the hypothalamus, the limbic system, and the fa- cial trigeminal nerves....At the neuromuscular level emotion is primarily facial activity and facial patterning, and secondarily it is bodily response. At the phenomenological level emotion is essentially motivating experience and/or ex- perience which has immediate meaning and signifi- cance for the person (1972, pg. 185). Emotion in the face, then, particularly as it can be de- scribed and identified on a neuromuscular basis, will be the context in which facial judgments are presented. The early literature in this area includes the names of Langfeld (1918), Rumrick (1922), Frois-Wittmann (1930), Woodworth (1938), Schlosberg (1941), Munn (1940), Hanawalt (1944) and Coleman (1949), and argues issues such as whether or not emotions are discrete and unique expressions, and if so, how they are to be labeled or classified. Frois-Wittman worked to identify the role of facial muscles in emotion identifi- cation; WOodworth's contribution was to simplify the categorization process by reducing the number of cate- gories from 110 to six. Munn, Hanawalt and Coleman 42 focused on the reliability of judgments by researching judgments made with and without contextual cues, and mouth region vs. eye region judgments. Building on the work Of these earlier research efforts, the two most widely regarded writers at this time are Carroll Izard and Paul Ekman. Izard (1965, 1971, 1972) has worked extensively to provide theory in the field, but additionally has developed a categoriza- tion system Of facial affect as well as having contrib- uted greatly in the areas of applied research. Ekman (in Ekman and Friesen, 1965, 1971, 1975) has been less concerned with theory, but has made significant contri- butions through procedures for the observing and judging of facial behavior. Carroll Izard Izard's primary contribution to the scoring of facial affect has been a set of research labels or affect categories, established to a large extent on the basis of his cross-cultural research and additionally "by the- oretical Observations, common-sense Observations, pre- vious descriptions from the literature, and from collabor- ative work with Tomkins" (1971, pg. 248). The eight categories he uses are: interest/excitement; enjoyment/ 43 joy; surprise/startle; distress/anguish/ disgust/con- tempt; anger/rage; shame/humiliation; and fear/terror. Izard describes his initial efforts in establish- ing these categories as attempts to establish the exis- tence Of discrete fundamental emotions common to all man- kind. He relied heavily on the use of emotion labeling, showing photographs to members of four different cul- tures and collecting data on the basis of the free-re- sponse labels generated by the photos. The initial study provided 244 different words or free-response 1a- bels, which were sorted and reduces into eight categor- ies, compatible with the same categories mentioned above. He has subsequently used procedures of emotion recognition, emotion labeling, and attitudes toward emo- tions in establishing agreement within these categories across cultures. Enjoyment/joy typically has the high- est percentage of agreement, ranging from 71-90%; shame/ humiliation has the lowest percentage at 7.2%. The other six categories usually have from 30 to 90% agreement, with several marked male/female and cross cultural dif- ferences (Izard, 1971). The development of these cate- gories, as well as the high degree of agreement elicited by them, provides an empirical argument for the existence of fundamental or basic emotions as well as a firm basis 44 for continued research. Paul Ekman Ekman describes his early work in the area of encoding and decoding of affect as having revolved around the construction of "an Atlas of the face" to de- pict photographically each of the universal facial ex— pressions of emotion. He photographed models who were instructed to move particular facial muscles listed in the Atlas table, and separately photographed the three areas Of the face capable of independent movement (brow/ forehead, eyes/lid and root of nose, and lower face). Subsequently he made comparative studies with these pho- tographs to study whether or not the Atlas was accurate, and in addition studied videotapes, isolating and mea- suring all muscular movements, and making independent measurements for each of the three facial areas. This work has provided the basis for all subsequent study of categories of facial emotion. A second contribution of Ekman's was the identi- fication and description of management techniques for the control of facial behavior. Ekman says there are four management techniques: intensifying an emotional expression; deintensifying it; neutralizing it; or 45 masking it by displaying another emotion instead. These management techniques are used in combination with dis- play rules, which are norms that dictate with whom and in what circumstances these management techniques are to be used. Display rules are based on static characteris- tics of the person (age, sex, physical size); static characteristics of the setting (ecological factors and social definitions of the situation); transient charac- teristics of the person (role, attitude); and transient regularities during the course of the interaction (en- trances, exits, periods of conversation and of listen- ing). Display rules are established primarily on cul- tural and personal (idiosyncratic and familial) norms. The ability to manage the face, and the variety of pro- cedures a person might use clearly presents a problem to the researcher trying to judge emotion in the face. Ek- man suggests that individual differences in knowledge Of display rules could be explored through self-report by subjects as a way to determine to some extent what man- agement techniques the subject may be using (1975, 138- 39). Ekman has, at this time (1975) established six categories of facial affect for use in research studies: happiness, sadness, surprise, fear, anger, and disgust. 46 These categories, says Ekman, have been found "by every investigator in the last 30 years who sought to deter- mine the vocabulary of emotion terms associated with fa- cial expressions" (1975, pg. 22). Using these six labels as the primary basis for research, much of Ekman's earlier research focused on judgment studies, where facial behavior was treated as the stimulus and questions were hypothesized with respect to the observers. From these as well as other research experiences, he speaks to the problems of sampling and establishing generality while recording and measuring the face. With regard to sampling the behavior of the face, it is crucial that the sampling procedures be reported says Ekman, or the related question of how 93223 the face provides accurate information (and for what kinds of observers, emotions, and circumstances) cannot be an- swered (1972, pg. 41). With regard to the sampling across persons, Ekman emphasizes the need for represen- tative sampling in order to avoid error due to either morphological characteristics or differing ability to show certain emotions. Establishing generality, for either posed or spontaneous circumstances, must consider issues across 47 circumstances, persons, time, decoding skills. Issues that Ekman presents, for example, are whether the find- ings in one eliciting circumstance or setting would be valid for another such circumstance or setting. If the spontaneous behavior elicited is in a laboratory setting, then the researcher must provide a discussion of the real life events to which this lab eliciting circumstance are relevant. Issues of generality across persons include considerations of whether the findings are general to most people, or just to trained persons such as actors. He reports that generality has been severely limited when the posers have been actors, but not when untrained persons pose emotions (1972, pg. 21). With regard to generality across time, the re- searcher must consider whether his investigation has chosen a sample from an infrequent moment when the face showed something, or whether the facial behavior shown in the situation provides precise information for many points in time. Decoding considerations refer to how readily other observers could make the same judgments. Did the study use specially trained or gifted persons in this area, for example, or were enlargements or slowed motion required? If so, these elements will lim- it the generality of who could make these observations. 48 Beyond generality, Ekman introduces another as- pect which adds to the difficulty of making accurate judgments: the phenomenon of blended emotions. Affect blends are thought to occur when l) the emotion-elicit- ing circumstance by its very nature elicits more than one feeling, or 2) habits (common to a group, or idio- syncratic) link the elicited emotion to another, as for example when a second emotion is generated in response to the initially inspired one (1972, pg. 25). The fre- quent finding that observers disagree about which of two affects is present "can no longer be interpreted as evi- dence of low information in the face, but alternatively as the consequence of presenting a multiple message stimulus and allowing the Observer only a single mes- sage judgment" (Ekman, 1972, pg. 25). This issue of blended affects represents a yet unresolved problem in the area of facial affect measurement, but is necessar- ily one that must continue to be addressed. As indicated in this section, the field of fa- cial affect measurement is relatively young and faces many unresolved problems and issues. There remains a lot of work to be done. Nevertheless there is a history of early research that explored the problem of where and how to begin, and has provided a basis for later 49 refinements and continually developing new methodolo- gies. Izard and Ekman have contributed enormously by establishing categories and collecting empirical data within that framework. Despite the fact that their numbers of categories differ, and the category labels are slightly varied, these differences do not make the two systems incompatible; data generated by one set of categories is Often applicable to the other. Ekman's continued research and writing with re- gard to methodological procedures has been of signifi- cant importance to the field, and has been particularly useful in considering research procedures for this study. Affect Categpries to be Used in This Study In considering what affect categories to use in this study, literature from Tomkins, Ekman, and Izard was reviewed as well as recent dissertation literature from Michigan State University. From Wilson's study (1976b) for example, it is clear that not all affects are likely to occur during an interview setting, so that fear, despite its high percentage of rater agreement, is for this study not a useful category for hypothesis 50 building because it is not likely to occur in the experi- mental setting. Inman (1976) provides additional infor- mation on the training of judges and percentage of agree- ment on categories used. He found (as has Izard) that shame has a very low agreement rate, thus while shame would be an appropriate and desirable category for tes- ting against personality dimensions in this study, it will not be used because of the apparent difficulty in judging the emotion. The six affect categories to be used are: Interest; Anger; Disgust; Surprise; Enjoyment and Distress. Interest The literature on interest (Izard, 1971; Tom- kins, 1962; Plutchik, 1962) suggests that this affect is a central one, yet it is also one Of the most commonly used for masking other emotions as a facial management technique (Ekman, 1975). Tomkins states that the func- tion "of this very general positive affect is to 'inter- est' the human being in what is necessary and in what is possible for him to be interested in" and continues by pointing out that "to the extent to which interest is attenuated later in life the individual thereby ceases to develop perceptually" (1962, pg. 342). Because it is 51 a more general affect, however, the category interest will not be treated as an affective category to be test- ed, and consequently will not appear in the formal statements of hypotheses. For the purposes of this study, interest will be considered a neutral category and will be labeled only in the absence of an expression from one of the five other categories. This should provide some indica- tion of the subject's usage and/or experience of inter- est without penalty to ratings in the other five areas. Anger Tomkins speaks of anger only as it can be a learned substitute for distress. The type of innate stimulation which activates distress, he proposes, is the same type Of stimulation that activates anger. If the experienced distress is unrelieved for a period of time, "it can produce sufficient increment of stimula- tion to innately activate anger, then this sequence may be telescoped so that the beginning of the distress cry becomes the learned activator Of anger" (1963, pg. 64). Whether or not the individual then experiences bggh emo- tions, in a chaining sequence similar to Hinds' proposal (1976), or whether the substitution takes place so rapidly 52 that only the anger is experienced, is not clear. Darwin refers to anger as one of the more excit- ing emotions, particularly with regard to intensity,and proposes that it (like other emotions) is so closely connected to its expression that the emotion can hard- ly exist if the body remains passive (1910, pg. 237). The function of anger, Darwin postulates, is to prepare oneself against perceived or anticipated danger. He notes examples of animals bearing the canine tooth, and humans compressing the mouth while frowning the brow, as if in warning to the perceived enemy. While anger may not be expressed as typically in an interview set- ting as in other environments, it is nevertheless thought that the subject will display it as frequently in this setting as he would be likely to express it in everyday interaction. Disgust Disgust, says Tomkins, is designed to prevent ingestion Of noxious material or to achieve its total rejection and regurgitation if it has been ingested. This is as true for psychic or psychological material as it is for the more physiologically based process. Its function is to guard against any type of 53 incorporation or increase in intimacy with the person/ object found to be noxious. Clearly this is a distancing technique, although Tomkins points out that the experience of disgust can become central to the person, as opposed to an indepen- dently stimulated reaction: When the experience of dngust is recurrent and becomes central, there develops a cognitive elaboration which organizes these experiences in- tO a relatively unified theory. The theory sen- sitizes the individual to contempt-relevant in- formation and provides ready-made strategies for coping with these paradigms (1973, pg. 250). Disgust is usually a more passive emotional experience than is anger, thus it is more possible for a person to sustain the expression facially for long periods of time. It is anticipated that disgust will be expressed fre- quently by some subjects during the interview sessions, and that it will be comparatively easy to measure. Surprise Surprise is neither a positive nor a negative affect by its own virtue, though a value is usually associated with it according to the stimulus event. Surprise functions as an interrupter to ongoing activity, so that the person may attend immediately to new information. It functions neurologically as a circuit breaker, and 54 according to Darwin is similar to anger in that it al- lows for preparation. It is believed that there will be a relationship between persons who frequently experi- ence and/or allow themselves to express surprise and persons who find Plutchik's concept Of dyscontrol impor- tant or central to their lives. Enjoyment Enjoyment is a social bond, says Tomkins, learned in early infancy through interactions with the mother. The smiling of the infant is quickly reinforced, and the socialization process begins. Children who are reared with a minimum of social interaction and Of smiling will ordinarily become less social adults (Tomkins, 1972, pg. 404). An additional feature of the smile (a primary expression of enjoyment) is that it frees the individual to give positive rewards or feedback without the neces- sity of body contact; thus it is a method of social bond- ing that has versatility. Smiling is also a frequently used technique for facial management (Ekman, 1975), though the choice of this expression as a mask can of itself be a statement about the individual. 55 Distress Distress, says Tomkins, is a natural part of the human condition; the differences are not whether but in how the individual experiences and expresses distress (1973, pg. 48). He outlines four modes of distress de- velopment, ranging from the healthy, flexible stance where it can be expressed when experienced, to the "ice- burg mode" characterized by denialthat becomes disrup- ‘ tive in crisis situations when the denial no longer suf- fices. Apart from the psychodynamic implications, dis- tress in this study will be defined and measured by ex- pressions of sadness, worry, or the more general dis- tress musculature. It is usually a more difficult emo- tion to purposefully mask, and is anticipated that it will be relatively easy to measure. Summary Though not conclusively defined, human emotion has been studied from the evolutional, biological, and cog- nitive perspecitives, as well as from the psychometric standpoint, with each of these traditions offering its own contribution to an overall understanding of this phenom- enon . 56 Charles Darwin was the first widely recognized scientist who prOposed a relationship between facial ex- pressions and internal emotional states. From the stand- point Of natural selection, Darwin began to identify categories Of emotion on the basis of facial expressions, and to speculate on the functional or survival value of each. Subsequent to Darwin, Sylvan Tomkins deveIOped a theoretical framework for the emotions system as it interplays with other physiological and psychological mechanisms, which includes the psychodynamic functions Of the different affects and the relatedness of facial ac- tivity to the experience of these affects. Based largely on the work of Darwin and Tomkins, other researchers such as Ekman and Izard have worked to more systematically classify categories of affect, develOp labeling and recognition techniques for these categories, and generate empirical data about the labeling process as well as about the cross-cultural evidence for these funda— mental categories. A recent development in this research area was stimulated by Haggard and Isaacs (1966), with the discovery that the expressions of facial affect may have a demonstrable relationship to certain aspects Of psycho- logical functioning, such as the presence of ego neonmfisms. 57 The scoring Of facial affect continues to be problematic, partly due to the subjective nature of the task, and partly due to the technological procedures, which for the most part are cumbersome and costly. Systematic procedures have been developed, nevertheless, to facilitate this process. Carroll Izard has worked extensively to continually refine fundamental categories of facial af- fect and to validate as well as establish reliability for the use of these categories. Paul Ekman has focused more on a study of the face itself, identifying specific muscle groups involved in the expression of different affects, and proposing guidelines for the sampling, generalizing, and accurate defining of expressions Of facial affect. In deciding upon which facial affect categories to use in this study, considerations Of reported rater reliability and liklihood of occurance for each affect category were made. The categories of interest, anger, disgust, surprise, enjoyment, and distress were selected to be used as the dependent measures for the study. Ad- ditionally, however, it should be noted that interest is considered a neutral category and does not appear in the formal statements of hypotheses. CHAPTER III RESEARCH DESIGN AND METHODOLOGY This chapter is divided into 15 sections which cover the following areas: sample, instrumentation, se- lection Of interview sample, interview and de-briefing procedures, the interviewer, videotape apparatus used during interviews, videotape editing, rater sample, rat- er training, rater reliability, rater agreement by affect category, research design, measurement of facial affect, research hypotheses, statistical analyses and procedure, and summary. Sample The sample for this study consisted of one hun- dred sixty-eight undergraduate students enrolled at the University of Texas-Austin during fall term, 1978. Ap- proximately twenty percent Of these students were solic- ited from Communications classes, and participated on a strictly volunteer basis. The remaining 80 percent were solicited from the Department of Psychology undergradu- ate subject pool. Psychology students who signed up for this particular research study received three hours of experimental research participation credit toward the six 58 59 hours required by their course. The only screening cri- terion used within this population was visibility of face; persons who wore glasses or who had beards were not selected for participation in the study. Of the 168 subjects who completed the pencil/ paper instruments, 97 percent of them were between the ages of 18 and 21. The remaining 3 percent were between the ages of 22 and 30. Fifty-five of these subjects were males; 107 were females. Subjects were told that they would be partici— pating in a study designed to investigate the relation- ships between personality characteristics and problem solving styles. The study was presented as a two phase process: during phase I subjects would be asked to come plete three pencil/paper personality inventories; phase II would consist of a 15 minute interview which would be videotaped, wherein the subject would be asked to talk briefly about something that was of personal concern. It was explained that not everyone taking the pencil/paper instruments would be asked to participate in the interview phase, and that subjects selected for par- ticipation in the interview would be on the basis of the experimenter's need for a balanced number of different types of subjects. Subjects were also informed that 60 expressed benefits of participation in the study were not contingent on their participation in the interview phase; namely that all subjects were eligible for one test interpretation of their choice as well as a writ- ten description Of the results of the study upon its com- pletion. All subjects were asked to fill out a Subject Consent Form (Appendix A), as well as a form for Birkman & Associates (Appendix A) allowing the firm to release their test scores Unthe experimenter. Subjects were in- structed that they could discontinue participation at any time during the course Of the study. After testing, and based on test scores of bal- anced groupings, the total number of subjects selected to participate in the interview phase of the study was 62. This group was composed of 22 males and 40 females. Approximately 22 percent of this sub-sample was from the Communications pool; the remaining 78 percent from the Psychology Department pool. The racial and ethnic break- down for this group was 57 Anglos, 1 Black, and 3 Hispa- nics. The breakdown of academic status was 35 freshmen, 16 sophomores, 7 juniors, and 4 seniors. Instrumentation Three pencil/paper personality instruments were 61 used in the study: the Plutchik Emotions Profile Index, the Birkman Method, and the Eysenck Personality Inven- tory . The Plutchik Emotions Profile Index (EPI) was developed by Robert Plutchik and Henry Kellerman (1974) and is based directly upon the general theory of emotion developed by Plutchik (1962). The theory postulates eight basic emotion dimensions: Protection (or Timid), Destruction (or Aggressive), Reproduction (or Gregari- ous), Reintegration (or Depressed), Incorporation (or Trustful), Rejection (or Distrustful), Exploration (or Controlled), and Orientation (or Dyscontrolled). The EPI is designed to assess the relative importance of each of these eight emotions in a person's life. The EPI consists of 62 items, in a forced- choice format. The test is based on 12 trait terms which are paired in all possible combinations, and which yield, in the final results, an "emotion circle" with a separ- ate score for each of the eight basic emotions. The nor- mative data presented by the manual is based upon 500 men and 500 women. The data include college students, housewives, nurses, motor vehicle inspectors, and public school teachers, and represent "a broad range of individ- uals characterized by a lack of overt pathology or 62 hospitalization" (Test Manual, pg. 2). Test-restest re- liabilities for each scale are reported at over +.90. Split half reliabilities for individual scales range from +.6l to +.90. Validity has been established through correlat- ing individual scales to scales on the MMPI, the Edwards Personal Preference Schedule, the Gough Adjective Check List, the Barrett Impulsivity Scale, and the Clyde Mood Spglg. In addition, distinctions between specific pop- ulations (hospitalized and normal women; matched groups of normal, neurotic, and psychotic subjects) have been illustrated by the EPI as further data for validity as- sessment. Due to the specific traits Plutchik uses to de- fine the eight basic emotion categories, only five of the eight categories or scales are facially measurable by current technological methods and thus usable for this study. The scales used are: Aggressive (primarily expressed by anger) Gregarious (primarily expressed by joy) Depressed (primarily expressed by sadness) Distrustful (primarily expressed by disgust) Dyscontrolled (primarily expressed by surprise) Since only five of the eight scales are used, and because of the ipsative nature of the instrument, the composite profile cannot be compared across subjects nor used for 63 questions regarding the range of affects displayed or the frequency of change in affect. It is possible, however, to use the individual scales to determine whether or not an individual's facial display matches his reported fre- quency of experience of that emotion. The Birkman Method (Roger W. Birkman, 1974) is an instrument based largely on the dissertation work and subsequent research Of its author. Its theoretical and philosophical orientation is in the Maslovian concept of self-actualization. The focus of the instrument is to provide individuals with sufficient information about themselves from as many aspects as possible (both person- al-social and vocational) so that they have an increased awareness of self as well as pertinent information upon which to base decisions about both setting and reaching personal-social and vocational goals. The format of the test is divided into three parts: perceptions of self (forced choice, 125 items); perceptions of others (forced choice, 125 items); and preferred interests (48 sets of quadruple groupings of first and second choice rankings). The instrument is computer scored. The client is provided a computer print-out which focuses on six major areas of social needs: ability to get along with others; the way one 64 gives and accepts directions; how one handles conflict and competition; how one maintains emotional and physi- cal stamina; how one organizes and plans activities; and the manner in which one makes effective decisions. In addition, a rank ordering of one's occupational interest areas is provided. Reliability measures regarding internal consis- tency have been tested through odd-even item correlations (using the 120 items from both Self and Most People por- tions of the test), and have produced correlations from .78 to .95. Test-retest procedures have also been used. Immediate (same day) test-retest correlations are report- ed at .69 and above. Additionally, test-retest correla- tions of forms completed two weeks apart show more than half of the itemrby-item correlations above £’= .60, and for almost all of the individual items, reliabilities were significantly different from zero. Efforts have been made to establish criterion- related and content validity through subject self-report and by consultation with industrial clientele. The pri- mary research regarding construct validity has utilized the intercorrelations between components of the Birkman and of specific scales from a variety of other well known standardized instruments. Personality and interest 65 instruments used in these studies include Cattell's Six- teen Personality Factorguestionnaire (Form A), the Strong Vocational Interest Blank for Males and Females, the Edwards Personal Preference Schedule, the Eysenck Personality Inventory (Form A), the Minnesota Multiphas- is Personality Inventory, and the California Psychologi- cal Inventory. The test is normed on an industrial pop- ulation ranging from senior management to hourly workers. For the purposes of this study, two components of the Birkman Method will be used: 1) "Getting Along With Others" (a combination of the Self-Consciousness and Feelings Scales), and 2) "Need for Authority" (the Domi- nance Scale). Persons who score high on the "Getting Along With Others" dimension typically exhibit a high aware- ness of others, a need for positive attention and em- pathy from others, difficulty in self-assertion, and a tendency toward shame over errors. Persons who score low on this component are more generally insensitive and aloof toward others, less reflective, and prefer more de- tached, logical relationships. Persons who score high on the "Dominance" dimension tend to seek and exercise firm authority or control. They are assertive, directive, 66 demanding, and argumentative. Those who score low pre- fer autonomy, independence, and pleasant relationships, are uncomfortable exerting authority, and generally non- assertive and unable to discipline subordinates. The Eysenck Personality Inventory (EPI) was developed by H. J. and Sybil B. G. Eysenck (1963) . It is a revi- sion of the earlier Maudsley Personality Ingentgry, and has two primary scales: E (extraversion-introversion) and N (neuroticismrstability). In addition, the EPI con- tains a Lie Scale which was developed and adapted from MMPI, but will be used in this study only for the purpos- es of descriptive statistics. The test consists of 57 items in a forced choice (yes-no) format. The test is published in both a British and an American edition. The manual for the American edition provides percentile norms, using American college students as the standardi- zation group. Test-retest reliabilities range between .80 and .97, and correlations between forms A and B range from .75 to .91. The test reflects Eysenck's previous research and theory, establishing extraversion-introversion and neuroticismrstability as two pervasive, independent di- mensions of personality. Factorial and construct valid- ity are spoken to in part by correlations between the 67 EPI and the MMPI, and the EPI and the California Person- ality Inventory. The E factor is regarded as extraver- sion at the high end and applies to individuals tending to be outgoing, impulsive, and uninhibited, having many social contacts and frequently taking part in group ac- tivities. The low score, reflecting introversion, re- fers to the more quiet, retiring sort of person who tends to be introspective, fond of books rather than of people, one who has reserve and who is distant except to inti- mate friends. This person tends to plan ahead, and to distrust the impulse of the moment. He does not like excitement, takes matters of everyday living with seriousness and likes a well-ordered mode of life (Buros, 1972). The high end of the neuroticism dimension is in- dicative of emotional instability and over-reactivity. Persons scoring high on this factor tend to be emotional- ly over-responsive and have difficulties in returning to a normal state after emotional experiences. Such indi- viduals frequently complain of vague somatic upsets of minor kinds such as headaches, digestive troubles, in- somnia, and backaches. Such individuals are also pre- disposed to neurotic disorders under stress. Those per- sons with 1ow scores on the neuroticism dimension tend 68 in general to be better adjusted and more emotionally stable. The N and E dimensions are reported as essen- tially uncorrelated. Selection of Interview Sample The three pencil/paper personality instruments used in the study were scored before the interview phase, in order to determine which of the subjects from the to- tal sample would be selected to participate in this sec- ond part. Subjects for the interview phase were select- ed on scores from the nine specific dimensions that were to be the formal measures used in hypothesis testing. Those scales are as follows: Emotions Profile Index Aggressive Gregarious Depressed Distrustful Dyscontrolled Birkman Method Getting Along With Others Dominance Eysenck Personality E (Extraversion/Intro- Inventory, version) N (Neuroticism/Stabil- ity) In order to determine who would participate in the interviews, groups of high and low scores were es- tablished for each Of the Birkman and Eysenck scales by dividing the scores at the mean and considering all 69 scores that were 1/2 standard deviation above and below the mean on each scale. In order to establish high and low groups on the Plutchik scale, scores above the 70th percentile and below the 30th percentile were consid- ered. The next step was to consider all subjects who fell into either a high or low group on any of the scales, and to identify subjects who fell into the great- est number of high/low groups, so as to minimize the to- tal number of subjects needed for interviews. An effort was made to include all subjects who fell at the extreme end of any one scale so as to maximize the differentia- tion between high and low groupings. In addition, an effort was made to proportionally balance the number of subjects used for any one scale, so that the two groups (high/low) did not exceed a 1.5 to 1 ratio. Using these criteria, a total Of 62 subjects were selected to participate in the interview phase of the study. This group of 62 was composed of 22 males and 40 females, as previously described on page 60. Interview and De—briefing Procedures The sixty-two subjects selected for interview participation were asked to come, by scheduled 70 appointment, to the Counseling Center where the inter- views took place. All interviews were scheduled within a twelve-day period, approximately 10-30 days after the initial testing took place. The interview setting was a room that approxi- mates the typical office Of a counselor at the Univer- sity of Texas Counseling-Psychological Services Center, including several chairs, a small table, and a carpeted floor. The control Of the video-tape equipment was man- aged from a room adjacent to the interview room, so that the interview could be taped without interrupting the interview itself. At the beginning Of the interview, the subject was presented with a stack of index cards containing in- dividual items from the "Personal Psychological Rela- tions" scale of the Mooney Problem Check List (see Appen- dix B for a complete listing of these items). The sub- ject was asked to look at each card until he or she found an item of current interest or concern, and then to talk with the interviewer about that item for 15 min- utes (see Appendix B for standardized script used by interviewer). In the event that the subject exhausted the topic before the 15 minutes had ended, the inter- viewer was instructed to suggest that the subject choose 71 another topic in the same manner described above. Immediately following the interview, the sub- ject was met by the experimenter, who guided him to a de- briefing room. At that time, the subject was given a self-report form to fill out (see Appendix B), inquir- ing about the nature of the feelings the subject has ex- perienced during the day as well as during the interview. After this form.was completed, the subject was given a written description of the specific nature of the study (Appendix B). The experimenter de-briefed the subjects by answering any further questions, and by inquiring about whether they had now any reservations or concerns about having participated in the study. At the end of the de-briefing session, the experimenter re-stated the nature of the study, the specific use of the video-tapes, and requested that the subject sign the video-tape re- lease form if he or she was willing to do so. (See Ap- pendix B for this form.) This was also the point at which subjects who were interested could sign up to par- ticipate in a test interpretation session and/or to have the results Of the study mailed to them. (Subjects who did not participate in the interview were contacted by mail after all interviews were completed for these sign- up procedures.) 72 The Interviewer The interviewer was the same individual for all subjects. He is a 40 year old male, employed at the time of the study as an intern at the University of Texas Counseling-Psychological Services Center. He was also at that time a graduate student in the Counseling Psy- chology doctoral program at the University of Texas. In order to maintain a consistency of response across subjects the interviewer was instructed to engage with subjects in a responsive but non-confrontive manner. He was instructed to engage with subjects by reflecting feelings, paraphrasing content, or indicating understand- ing in a non-verbal manner as a way to encourage the sub- ject to continue talking. He was not, however, to make any attempts to direct the interview or to challenge the subject. (See Appendix B for complete instructions to interviewer.) The spontaneity of the interview procedure and possibility of contamination through the interviewer's interaction with the subject was presented as a limita- tion of the study. In order to minimize this limitation, the interviewer was requested to participate in prelimi- nary practice sessions, whereby he was videotaped while performing in the interviewer role. Subsequently he 73 reviewed the tapes with the Experimenter, and received feedback on the extent to which he has appropriately maintained his role proscribed by the aforementioned guidelines. Additionally, the interviewer was asked to record brief anecdotal information about the nature of the interview after each subject was interviewed. Videotape Apparatus Used During Interviews Two Panasonic cameras were used for taping dur- ing the interview phase of the study. The two cameras provided for a split screen image that later facilitated the editing of the videotapes. One camera, a Panasonic 16-54 mm zoom lens was situated in the corner of the interview room and focused for close-up shots of the subject's head and shoulders. This camera was stationery, and adjusted only at the be- ginning Of each interview. Subjects were seated in a high backed chaise lounge so as to minimize head and shoulder movement. The second camera, a Panasonic 14-70mm zoom lens, was Operated from the adjacent equipment room and was focused on a digital clock with minutes and tenths of seconds. This image appeared in the upper right hand corner of the screen, and allowed for precise editing 74 of the tape at a later time. The two Panasonic video cameras fed into a Pana- sonic Special Effects Generator (SEG-WS-545P) which re- sulted in the split-screen image. The interview was re- corded in black and white on 3/4 inch cassetts video- tapes, using a Panasonic NV-9200 3/4 inch cassetts vid- eotape recorder. A11 videotaping procedures were per- formed and monitored from the equipment room adjacent to the interview room. Videotape Editing Each subject was videotaped throughout the full 15 minutes of the interview. The videotaped interviews were logged by subject code number. Master tapes were then prepared for rater viewing in the following manner. Systematic sampling of the subjects' interview was achieved by beginning from the point at which the subject laid the deck of problem topic cards on the ta- ble and looked up to begin talking with the interviewer. Beginning at this point, a 3 second sample was taken every 20 seconds, for a total Of 30 samples per subject, or a total of 1,860 samples for the study. This proced- ure allowed for discrete samples of facial affect, as well as for 10 seconds of blank tape between each sample. 75 In order to minimize rater response set, the or- der of subjects was randomly chosen, and subject samples were edited onto the master tape in sets of five. Be- cause of the large number of samples collected, a total randomization was not attempted, but rather 3-5 subjects were randomly chosen at a time, and alternatively sam- pled in sets of five until the total of 30 samples from each subject had been edited onto the master tape. Rat- ers were told not to assume that there was a chronologi- cal order either within the five samples per set, or in the order of the sets that they viewed for each subject. The master tapes onto which the videotaped same ples were edited were 3/4 inch black and white cassettes. The equipment used for this procedure was: two Sony tel- evision monitors; two Panasonic videotape recorders (Mod- el #NV 9200); a video editor controlled (Model #NV-A950); a Panasonic editor recorder (Model #NV 9500); and a Mi- crotime time base corrector (Model #1020). Rater Sample Three raters were used in this study to judge the videOtaped facial expressions sampled from subject interviews. The raters were not paid for time spent dur- ing the training program, but were paid for time spent 76 rating the tapes. All three of the raters were female, and all between the ages of 25 and 30. At the time Of the study, two of the raters were employed by the Uni- versity of Texas Telephone Counseling & Referral Service, had undergone training for that position, and were exper- ienced crisis and telephone counselors. One of these persons was also a graduate student in the M.S.W. School Of Social Work program at the University of Texas, as was the third (non-TCRS) person. Rater Training The three raters underwent a rigorous training program which was based on programs developed by previ- ous researchers in this area at Michigan State University (Inman, 1976; Wilson, 1976; Bowles, 1978). The training program focused on the six categories used in this study (Interest, Anger, Disgust, Surprise, Enjoy, Distress) and included diagrams, photos, slides, and videotapes using both posed and unposed samples of facial affect. Since there is no standardized package program for train- ing in the recognition of moving facial affect, and since replicability Of the study depends largely upon the ability to reproduce rater training and measurement procedures, the rater training program is described in 77 detail. The program began with a brief discussion of fa- cial affect and an overview of research on the area, par- ticularly the research regarding the development of fa- cial affect categories. Izard's research (1971) report- ing the categories most commonly overlooked by males and those most commonly overlooked by females was noted and discussed. The six affective categories were defined, and synonyms common to each of these affective labels were presented and discussed (see Appendix C). A description of each category as well as the dynamic function of the category was also presented and discussed (Appendix C). A diagram of facial musculature (Appendix C) was pre- sented and the muscle groups involved with each affect category were illustrated and discusSed. At this time, slides illustrating posed faces in each of these affect categories were also presented (Appendix C). Once the raters became familiar with the six af- fective categories, twenty-five slides of posed facial affect developed by Ekman (1976) were shown in groupings of five. Each slide was projected onto the screen for six seconds, after which raters were given a lO-second interval during which to write their judgments of the 78 category on an Affective Data Rating Form (Appendix D). The six-second stimulus with a 10-second interval for rating was used to approximate the timing sequence of the master tapes to be used in the formal rating proced- ure. After each set of five slides was shown, random- ly illustrating the affect categories used in the study, the correct answers were given. The slides were then shown for unlimited time periods, so that questions could be raised and discussed. After all 25 slides had been shown and discussed, a set of 15 more slides were shown for three seconds each at lO-second intervals. The Af- fective Data Rating Forms were used by raters to write their judgment for each of the 15 slides, and then col- 1ected in order to calculate rater reliability. Raters identified the same affective label for each of the 15 slides of posed facial affect with 95.5 percent agree- ment. The final phase of the training program involved showing raters segments of unposed facial expressions of affect from a pilot videotape, edited to be identical in format to the master tapes which raters would later be judging for the study. The procedure for practice with rating moving facial affect was parallel to the procedure 79 used with the slides. The raters were first shown thir- ty samples of unposed facial expressions, in sets of five, and requested to write their judgments on an Af- fective Data Rating Form. After each set of five seg- ments were viewed, correct answers were given, and rat- ers were encouraged to compare answers as well as to raise questions. After all thirty segments had been viewed, rated, and discussed, raters were then shown thirty more segments and requested to write their judg- ments on an Affective Data Rating Form. These forms were then collected in order to calculate rater relia- bility. The raters identified the same affective labels for these thirty segments of unposed facial affect with 94.4 percent agreement. Because Of the problems unique to judging mov- ing facial affect, the raters devised a set of rating rules based on Bowles' training program (Bowles, 1978) and on issues that arose while rating the pilot training tapes. Those rules are as follows: 1) If several categories of affect are Observ- able, record the dominant or more intense affect category. 2) If unable to determine which affect cate- gory was dominant, record the first dominant 80 category of affect Observed. 3) Only use the category interest if no other category of affect is clearly observable. These rules were briefly written and taped to the bottom of the video monitor throughout the rating of the master tapes. In addition, each rater had a card on a table be- fore her listing the six affect categories throughout the rating procedure. Because the raters were not able to complete the rating task in a consecutive two day period, as had been previously anticipated, the first of the two train- ing videotapes (which was accompanied by an answer key) was used to establish rater reliability at the beginning Of each rating session. Raters were asked to come in be- fore the beginning of the session and practice with the tape until they had established 90 percent agreement with the answer key. Despite the fact that practice effects began to occur by re-using the same videotape, this pro- cedure nevertheless did serve to standardize the rater judgment process. The rating task was completed in four sessions, or a total of 20 hours. Rater Reliability Raters were able to choose only one affect 81 category for each sample they viewed. One thousand, eight hundred sixty samples were viewed and judged in this manner, resulting in a total of 5,580 affective la- bels assigned by the three raters. Samples which had no agreement across raters were counted as discards, thus every affect rating used had agreement by at least two Of the three raters. A total of 130 samples were dis- carded, which came to an average of 2.09 discards per subject, or a total of 2 percent discard for the total sample. For the purposes of this study, inter-rater re- liability was established by percentage of agreement. This was calculated per subject by dividing the total number Of ratings per subject (both discards and agree- ments) into the number of agreed upon ratings for that subject. The mean interrater reliability across sub- jects was 77.3 percent, which is generally high for the task of judging moving facial affect. It is of note that these scores varied from 60 to 90 percent, however, indicating that reliabilities for some subjects were much higher than for others. Figure 3.1 presents a graph based on percentage of rater agreement per subject. 82 100 90 T t 1 80 O a Number 70 of 60 Subjects 50 40 30 20 w 10 l ‘ l 0 60- 65- 70- 75- 80- 85- 90- 64 69 74 79 84 89 94 Percentage of Agreement llllllllll Figure 3.1. Percentage of rater agreement across 62 subjects. Rater Agreement by Affect Categpry In addition to varying by subject, rater agree- ment also varied by category Of affect. Table 3.1 pre- sents, by affect category, the percentage of times a rat- ing was used when all three raters were in agreement, the percentage of times a rating was used when two Of the raters were in agreement, the percentage of single rat- ings which became unscoreable data, and the total per- centage of ratings that could be used as a judgment in the category. As indicated by Table 3.1, the category Enjoy had the highest total number Of ratings (1,956) followed by Interest, Disgust, Distress, Anger, and fi- nally Surprise. In the case of the first four 83 Table 3.1 Rater Agreement by Affect Category Percentages of Ratings Total 1 out percentage 3 out 2 out of 3 of usable Category Ratings of 3 of 3 (discard) ratings Interest 1,352 45% 33% 22% 78% Distress 966 25 42 33 67 Surprise 56 22 14 64 36 Anger 121 15 33 52 48 Enjoy 1,956 71 20 9 91 Disgust 1,129 42 30 28 72 categories, the order of unanimous agreement Was the same: Enjoy (71 percent); Interest (45 percent); Dis- gust (42 percent); and Distress (25 percent). Anger and Surprise reversed order, with Anger having only 15 per- cent unanimous agreement and Surprise 22 percent. When looking at the percentage of usable ratings, however, the order is once again parallel to that in the total number of ratings column: Enjoy (91 percent); Interest (78 percent); Disgust (72 percent); Distress (67 percent); Anger (48 percent); and Surprise (36 percent). Thus the categories with the highest number of ratings also had 84 the highest overall percentage of agreement by raters. Differences between columns two and three indi- cate that while percentages of unanimous agreement (3 out of 3) were generally higher than percentages of two- rater agreement, this is not the case with Distress and Anger. Each of these categories has a higher percentage of agreement in the third column, suggesting it was less likely that all three raters would agree on the expres- sion of this category, and more likely that only two out of the three would agree. In contrast, the large gap be- tween columns two and three for Enjoy (51 percentage points) is also Of note, indicating that raters were far more likely to agree on this category unanimously than on a two—rater basis. The column of single ratings (1 out of 3) indi- cates high percentages for Surprise and Anger. Sixty- four percent of the ratings for Surprise were single rat- ings and subsequently not counted as a judgment; fifty- two percent of the ratings for Anger were single ones and likewise were lost as data toward the subject's affect score in this category. Surprise, with the lowest number Of total ratings for the study, had a relatively high percentage Of unan- imous agreement when compared to Disgress and Anger. Yet 85 it also had the highest percentage of single ratings for all categories indicating that it had the highest amount of disagreement among raters. Finally, rater agreement was studied from the perspective of disagreement. Figures 3.2 through 3.7 are presented by affect category, and designed to illus- trate what affect category the third rater chose when the other two raters were in agreement on one category. As indicated by Figure 3.2, when two raters were in agreement on the category Interest, the third rater was most frequently in conflict by choosing the category Distress (91 ratings), next most frequently in conflict with the category Disgust (69 ratings) and least fre- quently in conflict with the categories Anger (12 rat- ings) and Surprise (6 ratings), suggesting in this case that it was most difficult for raters to distinguish be- tween Interest and Distress. As indicated by Figure 3.3, when two raters were in agreement on the category Distress, the third rater was most frequently in conflict by choosing the categor- ies of Interest (83 ratings) or Disgust (80 ratings), and least frequently in conflict with the category Sur- prise (3 ratings), suggesting in this case that it was difficult to distinguish between Distress and either 86 100 90 80 70 60 50 40 30 20 10 ITIIIHITI oe Surp Anger Dsgst Enjoy Dstrs Figure 3.2. Third rater's category where two raters were in agreement on Interest. (23216) 100 90 80 70 60 50 40 30 20 10 '1'?- 0 . 55;: ,,,,,,,,,, . __ .................... 315-2’ W Surp nnnnnnnnnnnnnnn ................... lleTll1|| Intrst Anger Dsgst Enjoy Figure 3.3. Third rater's category where two raters were in agreement on Distress. (2&202) 100 90 80 70 60 50 40 30 20 10 IITIIIIIII qu ui‘v-vmw.~- A AAJAJ AA.- - a. A: e e I Intrst Anger Dsgst Enjoy Dstrs Figure 3.4. Third rater's category where two raters were in agreement on Surprise. (ps4) 100 90 80 70 60 50 4O 30 20 10 87 flllllllll Intrst Dsgst Enjoy Dstrs Surp Figure 3.5. Third rater's category where two raters were in agreement on Anger. (2321) 100 C Dstrs Surp Figure 3.6. Third rater's category where two raters were in agreement on Enjoy. (33195) 100 90 80 70 60 50 40 30 20 10 p..- — .— h— .— _ n.— — —- r- . .:.'.- : 5.... ....... a . .;. Enjoy Surp Figure 3.7. Third rater's category where two raters were in agreement on Disgust. (33171) 88 Interest or Disgust. Figure 3.4 indicates that when two raters were in agreement on Surprise, the third rater was in conflict by choosing either Interest (2 ratings), Anger (l rat- ing), or Enjoy (1 rating). Because of the small number of 2-rater judgments in this category, it is difficult to determine whether this pattern would be representa- tive of a rating norm in a larger sample. The Figure does indicate, however, that the categories Interest, Disgust, and Enjoy were the most frequently disagreed up- on in relationship to the category Surprise. As indicated by Figure 3.5, when two raters were in agreement on Anger, the third rater was in conflict by choosing Interest (10 ratings), Distress (8 ratings), or Disgust (3 ratings). As with the previous figure il- lustrating Surprise, the number of 2-rater judgments in this category is so small that it is difficult to deter- mine whether this would actually become a trend in a larger sample. It is indicated, however, that Interest, Distress, and Disgust were more difficult to distin- guish from Anger than were Enjoy or Surprise. Figure 3.6 indicates that when two raters were in agreement on Enjoy, the third rater was in conflict by choosing either Disgust (59 ratings) or Interest (64 89 ratings), and least frequently in conflict by choosing Surprise (10 ratings) or Anger (5 ratings), suggesting that as for the category Distress, it is most difficult for raters to distinguish between Enjoy and either Interest or Disgust. Finally, Figure 3.7 indicates that when two rat- ers were in agreement on Disgust, the other rater was most frequently in conflict by choosing the category Dis- tress (78 ratings), and least frequently in conflict by choosing the categories Anger (7 ratings) and Surprise (4 ratings). This is in contrast to Figure 3.3 for Dis- tress, where both Disgust and Interest had a high number of ratings. Distress was the single category most fre- quently in conflict when two of the raters were in agree- ment on Disgust. In general, Interest, Disgust, and Distress were the three categories most frequently conflicting in rat- er judgments, with Enjoy the more clearly defined both by a higher percentage of unanimous agreement (Table 3.1) and by fewer ratings in conflict with the other categor- ies. The only exception to this was with respect to agreement in the category Disgust, where Enjoy was the second most frequently chosen of the conflicting cate- gories. 90 Anger and Surprise, the two categories with the lowest overall frequency throughout the study, occurred in small numbers of single ratings throughout the other categories. Anger, which had a higher total frequency than Surprise, also generally had more single ratings in conflict with judgments in other categories. Each Of these categories had more single ratings than ratings which could be used as a judgment for a sample. As indicated in Figures 3.4 and 3.5, the only two categories with entirely discrete ratings were Anger and Surprise. Neither of these categories were chosen by the third rater where the other two raters were in agreement on either Anger or Surprise. While inter-rater reliability was high during the training sessions and with the training tape used at the beginning of each rating session (90 percent or above), the inter-rater reliability throughout the rat- ing task varied considerably (from 60 percent agreement to 90 percent agreement across subjects). It should be noted, however, that the judgment of moving facial affect is considerably more complex and difficult than is the judgment of either posed or unposed facial affect in still form. The trends which have emerged regarding the variance of rater agreement across subjects as well as 91 across affect categories will be reviewed in the Discus- sion section in an attempt to provide still more specif- ic guidelines for this task in the future. Research Design The research design of the study was developed to address a comparison of groups on the dependent mea- sures. The purpose of this design was to determine whether subjects in the high group on any one of the in- dependent measures differ in expression of facial affect from subjects in the low group on that same independent measure. The independent measures used in the study were the Eysenck Personality Inventory, the Plutchik Emotions Profile Index, and the Birkman Method, all described pre- viously in this chapter. In addition to using a total of nine independent scales from these three instruments, a combination of the Eysenck Extraversion and Neuroticism scales were used as a tenth independent variable. The dependent measures used in the study were eight categories of facial affect: Interest, Disgust, Anger, Surprise, Distress, Enjoy, Range of Affect, and Change of Affect. The design used for hypothesis test- ing of group differences on these affective measures is 92 graphically displayed in Figure 3.8. 81* mean score High 2 on dependent Group é variable 20 821 mean score Low 2 on dependent Group é variable 40 Figure 3.8. Between group differences from independent variables on affective measures. *These numbers are for illustration purposes only. While an attempt was made to have 20 subjects in each group, priority was given to maintaining balanced groups (see pg. 68). Measurement of Facial Affect The measurement of facial affect was designed to take into account the types of affect categories dis- played, the range of affect, and the frequency of change of affect. The six types Of affect categories used in the study are previously researched categories of emo- tional experience, defined by specific facial expressions (Ekman, 1975; Izard, 1971). Scores in these categories, Interest, Disgust, Anger, Surprise, Distress, and Enjoy, were determined by rater judgment. An agreement by two of the three raters constitutes a judgment in any one particular category. A disagreement across all three 93 raters on any one sample of facial affect constitutes a discard of that sample and becomes non-scoreable data. A subject's score for each affect category was based on the number of rater judgments in that category. Since the number of samples per subject would vary by the num- ber of discarded samples, it was determined that the fi- nal subject's score for each category would be calculat- ed on a percentage basis: total number of samples in the category divided by total number of samples used for that subject. (See Appendix D for subject scoring sheet.) The category Range of Affect was determined by the number of affect categories for which the subject re- ceived at least one judgment. The highest possible score was six. (See Appendix D for an illustration Of this calculation.) The category Change of Affect was determined by the number of times a subject changed from one judgment of affect to another. Discards were not: counted as a change in affect. In the case where a subject received a judgment in the category Surprise, followed by no judgment because of a discard, followed by a judgment in the category Interest, the change score for the sequence would be one. (See Appendix D for an illustration Of 94 this calculation.) In addition to these affective measures to be used in the formal hypothesis testing, two more measures, Percentage of Rater Agreement and Number of Discards Per Subject, were also used to provide further descrip- tive data. Research Hypotheses Fifteen hypotheses were generated to empirically test the relationship between performance on specific scales of the three pencil/paper personality instruments and the type, range, or frequency of change of facial affect displayed. 4 The null hypotheses may be generally stated as follows: Null Hypothesis: NO difference will be found on the affective measure between the mean of the group scoring high and the mean of the group scoring low on the independent variable. HO: "(1 =’“2 alpha level of .05 The alternative hypotheses may be grouped into the following two categories: Alternative Hypothesisl: The high group's mean score will exceed that of the low group's mean score on the dependent affective measure. 95 Hla: .q 1”“ 2 or Alternative Hypothesis : The high group's mean score will be less thafi the low group's mean score on the dependent affective measure. "2.21““( 1‘“ 2 Statistical Analyses and Procedure Since all hypotheses were addressed as a compar- ison of groups on the dependent variables, and since equality of population variance, normality of distribu- tion, and independence between each set of high/low grups was assumed, all hypotheses were tested by means of a t-test. The alpha level for each hypothesis was set at .05, which is of concern to the overall alpha level of the study and possibility for Type I error. Due to the exploratory nature of the study, however, it was considered more important to use a .05 alpha lev- el for the individual hypotheses. Also due to the exploratory nature of the study, analyses that would provide descriptive data beyond the formal hypothesis testing were also used. T-tests were used to explore the nature of group differences between all of the dependent and independent variables in the study. The high and low groups on each of the indepen- dent variables were subsequently tested for differences 96 on each of the dependent variables. For exploration in the multivariate sense, a multivariate analysis of variance was used to examine the set of eight dependent measures as they relate to each of the independent measures. Finally, an inter-correlational matrix made up of all independent and dependent variables as well as additional scales from Plutchik and Eysenck and the two additional rater affect measures was explored. Areas of focus for correlational relationships were: relation- ships within the scales Of each personality instrument, relationships across the set of personality instruments, relationships across the set of affective measures, and relationships between the personality instruments and the affective measures. Pearson Product Moment statis- tics were used to estimate the correlations. Summagy One hundred sixty-eight undergraduate students enrolled fall term at the University of Texas-Austin vol- unteered to participate in this study. These subjects were administered three pencil/paper personality instru- ments in order to determine which of them would partici- pate in the interview phase. Cut-off points for high 97 and low groups on each of the research scales were es- tablished. Sixty-two subjects were then selected (22 males, 40 females) on this basis to participate in in- terviews. Each of the 62 subjects participated in a 15 min- ute interview by choosing items from the Personal-Social Relations Scale of the Mooney Problem Check List and discussing that item with the interviewer. Data was collected by videotaping the interviews and then syste- matically sampling facial expressions by editing the tapes to 30 3-second segments per subject. A total of 1,680 samples were used for the study, and organized in semi-random order onto master tapes. Subject samples were grouped in sets Of five and subjects were alternat- ed by order Of appearance on tapes. Three female raters were trained to recognize and accurately label six affective categories Of facial expression: Interest, Anger, Disgust, Surprise, Dis- tress, and Enjoy. Affective data was then calculated by subject for: percentage of each type of facial affect category used; percentage of change from one category to another, and total number of categories used. Percen- tage of rater agreement per subject was also calculated as well as total number of discards per subject, in order 98 to provide further methodologically related data for the study. The study was designed to address a comparison of groups on the affective or dependent measures. Hy- potheses were developed to test the relationship between performance on specific scales of the three pencil/paper personality instruments and the type, range, or frequency of change of facial affect displayed. T-tests were used to empirically test these relationships, with the proba- bility of significance set at the p==.05 level. Due to the exploratory nature of the study, ad- ditional tests were used to further examine the relation- ships between the independent and dependent measures which had not been addressed in the hypothesis testing. Additional t-tests between all independent and dependent variables were run, as well as a MANOVA which examined the set of dependent measures as it relates to each of the independent measures. Finally, an inter-correlational matrix was used to examine the relationships of all scores from person- ality instruments and affective measures, with a focus on relationships within the scales of each personality instrument, relationships across the set of personality instruments, relationships across the set of affective 99 measures, and relationships between the personality in- struments and the affective measures. The analysis of this data is presented in Chapter IV. CHAPTER IV ANALYSIS OF DATA This chapter is divided into three major sections for the purpose of reporting the results of the investiga- tion. The first section presents the results of the hypothesis testing, the second section presents further analyses of group differences, and the third section reports correlational analyses. Results of Hypothesis Tests The fifteen hypotheses used in this study can be divided into three groups, corresponding to the three personality inventories used as independent measures. The first group of hypotheses (1-5) is related to the Plutchik Emotions Profile Index; the second group (6-11) is related to the Birkman Method; and the third group (12-15) is related to the Eysenck Personality Inventopy. Following each group of hypotheses is the empirical data germane to the retention or rejection of the null hypothesis. Hypotheses related to Plutchik's Emotions Profile Index (PEPI) propose a relationship between the 100 101 frequency of a specific affect expressed facially and the percentile score of that affective area on the subject's PEPI test profile. Hypothesis 1 Null Hypothesis: There will be no significant difference on the affective measure of Enjoy between subjects who score above the 70th percentile on the Gregarious scale and the sub- jects who score below the 30th percentile on the Gregarious scale. Alternative Hypothesis: The group mean of sub- jects scoring above the 70th percentile on the Gregarious scale will be significantly higher on the measure Enjoy than will be the group mean of subjects scoring below the 30th percentile on the Gregarious scale. Hypothesis 2 Null Hypothesis: There will be no significant difference on the affective measure of Disgust between subjects who score above the 70th percentile on the Distrustful scale and subjects, who score below the 30th percentile on the Distrustful scale. Alternative Hypothesis: The group mean of sub- jects scoring above the 70th percentile on the Distrustful scale will be significantly higher on the measure Disgust than will be the group mean of subjects scoring below the 30th percen- tile on the Distrustful scale. Hypothesis 3 Null Hypothesis: There will be no significant difference on the measure Distress between sub- jects who score above the 70th percentile on the Depressed scale and subjects who score below the 30th percentile on the Depressed scale. 102 Alternative Hypothesis: The group mean of sub- jects scoring above the 70th percentile on the Depressed scale will be significantly higher on the measure Distress than will be the group mean of subjects scoring below the 30th percentile on the Depressed scale. Hypothesis 4 Null HypOthesis: There will be no significant difference on the measure Surprise between subjects who score above the 70th percentile on the Dyscontrolled scale and subjects who score below the 30th percentile on the Dyscontrolled scale. Alternative Hypothesis: The group mean of subjects scoring above the 70th percentile on the Dyscontrolled scale will be significantly higher on the measure Surprise than will be the group mean of subjects scoring below the 30th percentile on the Dyscontrolled scale. Hypothesis 5 Null Hypothesis: There will be no significant difference on the measure Anger between sub- jects who score above the 70th percentile on the Agressive scale and subjects who score below the 30th percentile on the Aggressive scale. Alternative Hypothesis: The group mean of subjects scoring above the 70th percentile on the Aggressive scale will be significantly higher on the measure Anger than will be the group mean of subjects scoring below the 30th percentile on the Aggressive scale. 103 Table 4.1 Affective Scores for Subjects Selected for Analysis on Plutchik's Personality Dimensions Affective Group Measure Q E SD Hl High Gregarious Percentage 25 37.88 23.30 of ENJOY Low Gregarious 17 27.88 17.27 H2 High Distrustful Percentage 22 23.63 17.15 of DISGUST Low Distrustful 23 12.52 12.27 H3 High Depressed Percentage 19 17.92 17.66 of DISTRESS Low Depressed 28 16.19 15.44 H4 High Dyscontrol Percentage 22 .48 1.25 of SURPRISE Low Dyscontrol 17 .62 1.38 H5 High Aggressive Percentage 17 3.51 9.08 of ANGER Low Aggressive 1.08 2.47 27 104 Table 4.2 T-test Results for Between-Group Differences on Affective Measures Affective Signif- Group Measure icant Id ['0 H High Gregarious l contrasted with Percentage Low Gregarious of ENJOY 1.51 .065 No H2 High Distrustful contrasted with Percentage Low Distrustful of DISGUST 2.51 .008 Yes H3 High Depressed contrasted with Percentage Low Depressed of DISTRESS 0.36 .361 NO H4 High Dyscontrol contrasted with Percentage Low Dyscontrol of SURPRISE -0.32 .374 No H High Aggressive contrasted with Percentage Low Aggressive of ANGER 1.32 .097 NO Statistical significance at the .05 level was not ob- tained for any of the Hypotheses l, 3, 4, or 5, and there- fore each null was not rejected. The test of Hypothesis 2, however, indicated a significant difference (with a p Of 2.51 and a one-tailed probability of .008) between high distrustful and low distrustful groups on the affec- tive measure Disgust. The mean for the high distrustful group was 23.63, and for the low distrustful group was 12.52, for a difference of 11.11 in the hypothesized 105 direction. Hypotheses related to the Birkman Method pro- pose a relationship between two specific personality di- mensions (Dominance and Getting Along With Others) and affective facial display (including specific categories used, number of categories used, and frequency of facial changes). Hypothesis 6 Null Hypothesis: There will be no significant difference on the measure Range of Affect between subjects who score high on the Getting Along With Others scale and subjects who score low on the Getting Along With Others scale. Alternative Hypothesis: The group mean of sub- jects who score high on the Getting Along With Others scale will be significantly higher on the measure Range of Affect than will be the group mean of subjects scoring low on this scale. Hypothesis 7 Null Hyppthesis: There will be no significant difference on the affective measure of Disgust between subjects who score high on the Getting Along With Others scale and subjects who score low on the Getting Along With Others Scale. Alternative Hypothesis: The group mean Of sub- jects who score high on the Getting Along With Others scale will be significantly lower on the measure Disgust than will be the group mean of subjects scoring low on this scale. Hypothesis 8 Null Hypothesis: There will be no significant difference on the affective measure of Enjoy be- tween subjects who score high on the Getting Along With Others scale and subjects who score 106 low on the Getting Along With Others scale. Alternative Hypothesis: The group mean of sub- jects who score high on the Getting Along With Others scale will be significantly higher on the measure Enjoy than will be the group mean of sub- jects scoring low on this scale. Hypothesis 9 Null Hypothesis: There will be no significant difference on the measure Change of Affect be- tween subjects who score high on the Getting Along With Others scale and subjects who score low on the Getting Along With Others scale. Alternative Hypothesis: The group mean of sub- jects who score high on the Getting Along With Others scale will be significantly higher on the measure Change of Affect than will be the group mean of subjects who score low on this scale. Hypothesis 10 Null Hypothesis: There will be no significant difference on the affective measure of Distress between subjects who score high on the Dominance scale and subjects who score low on the Dominance scale. Alternative Hypothesis: The group mean of sub- jects who score high on the Dominance scale will be significantly lower on the measure Distress than will be the group mean Of subjects who score low on this scale. Hypothesis ll Null Hypothesis: There will be no significant difference on the measure Change of Affect be- tween subjects who score high on the Dominance scale and subjects who score low on the Dominance scale. Alternative Hypothesis: The group mean of sub- jects who scoreihigh on the Dominance scale will be significantly lower on the measure Change of Affect than will be the group mean of subjects who score low on this scale. 107 Table 4.3 on Birkman's Personality Dimensions Affective Scores for Subjects Selected for Analysis Affective n i SD Group Measure — H6 High G.A.W.O. Number of Af- 13 4.23 0.43 fects Displayed Low G.A.W.O. (RANGE) 15 3.73 0.79 H7 High G.A.W.O. Percentage 13 21.52 12.96 of DISGUST Low G.A.W.O. 15 16.17 16.80 H8 High G.A.W.O. Percentage 13 35.03 19.92 of ENJOY Low G.A.W.O. 15 42.70 24.06 H9 High G.A.W.O. Percentage Of 13 63.53 13.11 CHANGE OF Af- Low G.A.W.O. fect 15 55.86 16.51 Hlo High Dominance Percentage 16 13.00 9.86 of DISTRESS Low Dominance 14 10.75 9.34 Hll High Dominance Percentage of 16 61.00 18.69 CHANGE of Af- Low Dominance fect 14 56.42 15.81 Statistical significance at the .05 level was not ob- tained for Hypotheses 7-11, and therefore each null was not rejected. The test of Hypothesis 6, however, indi- cated a significant difference (with a ptof 2.00 and a one-tailed probability of .028) in the number of affect categories used when the high Getting Along With Others and the low Getting Along With Others groups were 108 Table 4.4 T-test Results for Between-Group Differences on Affective Measures Affective Signif- Group Measure p_ p icant Number of H6 High G.A.W.O. Affects contrasted with Displayed Low G.A.W.O. (RANGE) 2.00 .028 Yes H7 High G.A.W.O. _ contrasted with Percentage Low G.A.W.O. of DISGUST 0.93 .175 NO H8 High G.A.W.O. contrasted with Percentage Low G.A.W.O. of ENJOY -0.91 .185 NO H9 High G.A.W.O. Percentage contrasted with Of CHANGE Low G.A.W.O. of AFFECT 1.35 .095 No Hlo High Dominance contrasted with Percentage Low Dominance of DISTRESS 0.64 .264 No H11 High Dominance Percentage contrasted with of CHANGE Low Dominance of AFFECT 0.72 .239 No compared. The mean of the high Getting Along With Others group was 4.23, and the mean of the low group was 3.73, for a difference Of .50 in the hypothesized direction. Hypotheses related to the Eysenck Personality In- ventory propose a relationship between the personality characteristics Extraversion/Introversion and Neuroticism/ 109 Stability, and facial affect display as measured by spe- cific categories used, number of categories used, and fre- quency Of facial changes. Hypothesis 12 Null Hypothesis: There will be no significant difference on the affective measure of Enjoy be- tween subjects who score high on the Extraver- sion scale and subjects who score low on the Ex- traversion scale. Alternative Hyppthesis: The group mean of sub- jects who score high on the Extraversion scale will be significantly higher on the measure En- joy than will be the group mean of subjects who score low on this scale. Hypothesis l3 Null Hypothesis: There will be no significant difference on the affective measure of Disgust between subjects who score high on the Extraver- sion scale and subjects who score low on the Ex- traversion scale. Alternative Hypothesis: The group mean of sub- jects who score high on the Extraversion scale will be significantly lower on the measure Dis- gust than will be the group mean of subjects who score low on this scale. Hypothesis 14 Null Hypothesis: There will be no significant difference on the measure Change of Affect be- tween subjects who score high on the Neuroticism scale and subjects who score low on the Neuroti- cism scale. Alternative Hypothesis: The group mean of sub- jects who score high on the Neuroticism scale will be significantly higher on the measure Change of Affect than will be the group mean of subjects who score low on this scale. 110 Hypothesis 15 Null Hypothesis: There will be no significant difference on the measure Range of Affect between subjects who score high on the Extraversion and Neuroticism scales (EN) and those who do not score high on both of these scales (ES, IS, IN). Alternative Hyppthesis: The group mean of sub- jects who score high on the Extraversion and Neu- roticism scales (EN) will be significantly high- er on the measure Range of Affect than will be the group mean of subjects who do not score high on both of these scales (ES, Table 4.5 IS, IN). Affective Scores for Subjects Selcted for Analysis on Eysenck's Personality Dimensions Affective Group Measure pl 'i SD H12 High Extraversion Percentage 25 40.29 24.04 Of ENJOY Low Extraversion 26 34.88 16.04 H13 High Extraversion Percentage 25 16.56 12.96 of DISGUST Low Extraversion 26 20.08 17.14 Hl4 High Neuroticism Percentage 27 58.22 1.87 of CHANGE Low Neuroticism of Affect 23 60.86 .99 H15 EN Number of 16 3.75 .57 Affects IN, IS, ES Displayed 15 4.14 .70 (RANGE) Statistical significance at the .05 level was not Obtained for Hypotheses 12-15, and therefore each null was not re- jected. Hypothesis 15 was a contradictory finding, with 111 Table 4.6 T-test Results for Between-Group Differences on Affective Measures Affective Signif- Group Measure 'p p_ icant H12 High Extraversion Percentage contrasted with of ENJOY Low Extraversion 0.95 .174 No H13 High Extraversion contrasted with Percentage Low Extraversion of DISGUST'-0.82 .207 No Hl4 High Neuroticism Percentage contrasted with of CHANGE Low Neuroticism Of Affect -0.61 .273 No H15 EN contrasted Number of with IN, IS, Affects ES Displayed -2.00 .027 No (RANGE) a'p of -2.00 and a one-tailed probability of p = .025. It should be noted that since the t value is negative (in- dicating a direction other than that which was hypothe- sized), the degree of probability used by this study is consequently in the wrong tail of the test, and the prob- ability level of p .025 has no meaning for the tail in which it suggests significance. 112 Further Analyses of Group Differences Multivariate Analyses of Variance In order to explore how the two groups of sub- jects on each independent variable relate to the set of eight dependent measures, ten multivariate analyses of variance were performed. In Table 4.7 each independent variable is presented along with the empirical data ger- mane tO its significance test for group differences on the set of dependent measures. Table 4.7 Multivariate Analyses of Variance for the Independent Variables on the Set of Dependent Measures Independent Variable F p Significant Gregarious 1.91 .091 NO Distrustful 1.96 .079 NO Depressed .77 .629 No Dyscontrolled .53 .828 No Aggressive 1.40 .227 No Getting Along With Others .75 .648 No Dominance 1.12 .385 No Extraversion .98 .464 No Neuroticism .61 .764 NO Extraversion/Neuroticism .75 .647 No the: Significance is considered at an alpha level of .05. 113 Statistical significance at the .05 level was not Obtained for any of the independent variables when tested by a mul- tivariate analysis of variance. Further Analyses of Interest While the multivariate analyses of variance were not significant when used to relate the independent vari- ables to the set of dependent measures, the univariate analyses Of variance did indicate the presence of several significant relationships beyond those mentioned in the hypothesis tests. Univariate analyses were used to test group differences on each of the independent variables as they relate individually to each of the eight dependent measures. In addition, univariate analyses were used to provide further methodological information by testing group differences on rater agreement and number Of dis— cards per subject. It should be noted here that the term significant is used with caution since it was defined at an alpha level Of .05 and does not speak to the problems of the overall Type I error rate. In this section those independent variables show- ing significant differences on the affective measures are presented, along with the empirical data germane to the tests for significance and the tables that display the 114 nature of the significant differences. The independent variables presented are: Gregarious, Distrustful, and Aggression. Tables for univariate analyses of the remain- ing independent variables, Depressed, Dyscontrolled, Get- ting Along With Others, Dominance, Extraversion, and Neu- roticism, can be found in Appendix E and will be referred to in the Discussion section, Chapter V. NO significant differences were found among the univariate analyses testing for group differences on the measures of rater agreement and of discards per subject. Tables presenting the empirical data for these tests can be found in Appendix E and will also be referred to in the Discussion section, Chapter V. Table 4.8 presents the independent variable Gre- garious along with the results of significance tests for between-group differences on each of the affective mea- sures. As indicated in Table 4.8, six of the eight af- fective measures show no significant difference at the .05 level between high and low groups on Plutchik's Gre- garious scale. Tests on two affective measures, however, Disgust and Change, indicate that there may be signifi- cant differences between high and low Gregarious groups in the percentage of disgust displayed, and in the per- centage of affect changes the subjects displayed. 115 Table 4.8 T-test Results of Gregarious Group Differences on the Affective Measures Dependent Variable p p Significant Percentage Of Interest 0.07 .490 No Percentage Of Disgust -2.30 .027 Yes Percentage of Anger -1.61 .116 No Percentage of Distress 0.41 .682 No Percentage of Surprise 1.24 .221 No Percentage Of Enjoy* 1.51 .065 No Percentage Of Change -2.20 .034 Yes # of Affects Used (Range) -1.36 .180 No *Hypothesis 1 (reported here as a one-tailed test). Note: Significance is considered at an alpha level of .05. The mean for the high Gregarious group was 15.16, and for the low Gregarious group 27.41 on the affective measure Disgust, for a difference of 12.25 between groups, at a probability level of p_= .027. The mean for the high Gregarious group on the affective measure Change was 56.68, and for the low Gregarious group 66.82, for a dif- ference of 10.14 between groups at a probability level of p = .034. (See Table 4.9 for a display of these differ- ences.) 116 Table 4.9 Affect Scores for High and Low Gregarious Groups Dependent Variable Group H i’ SD Percentage of Disgust High Greg 25 15.16 15.85 Low Greg 17 27.41 18.41 Percentage of Change High Greg 25 56.68 15.23 Low Greg 17 66.82 13.83 Table 4.10 presents the independent variable Dis- trustful along with the results of significance tests for between-group differences on each of the affective mea- sures. Table 4.10 T-test Results of Distrustful Group Differences on the Affective Measures Dependent Variable p p Significant Percentage of Interest -2.05 .047 Yes Percentage of Disgust* 2.51 .008 Yes Percentage of Anger 1.28 .207 No Percentage of Distress -0.50 .621 No Percentage Of Surprise -1.72 .093 No Percentage of Enjoy -0.21 .832 No Percentage of Change -0.77 .448 No # Of Affects Used (Range) -0.21 .836 NO *Hypothesis 2 (reported as a one-tailed test). Note: Significance is considered at an alpha level of .05 . 117 As indicated in Table 4.10, six of the affective measures show no significant difference at the .05 level between high and low groups on Plutchik's Distrustful scale. A significant difference is indicated between groups on the affective measure Disgust, as presented earlier in this chapter. In addition, the test between groups on the af- fective measure Interest indicates that there may be a difference in the percentage Of interest displayed by those who score high on the Distrustful scale and those who score low on this scale. Those who score high on Dis- trustful have a mean interest score of 17.23, those who score low a mean Interest score of 26.54, for a differ- ence Of 9.31 at a probability level of p = .047. (See Table 4.11.) Table 4.11 Affect Scores for High and Low Distrustful Groups Dependent Variable Group 3 2 SD Percentage of Inter- High Dstrfl 22 17.23 12.21 est Low Dstrfl 23 26.54 17.67 Table 4.12 presents the independent variable Ag— gression along with the results of significance tests for betweenégroup differences on each of the affective mea- sures. 118 Table 4.12 T-test Results of Aggressive Group Differences on the Affective Measures Dependent Variable p p Significant Percentage Of Interest -l.99 .053 No Percentage Of Disgust 2.37 .022 Yes Percentage of Anger* 1.32 .097 No Percentage of Distress -O.10 .919 NO Percentage of Surprise -0.51 .612 No Percentage of Enjoy -0.81 .423 NO Percentage of Change 0.90 .375 No # of Affects Used (Range) 1.03 .311 No *Hypothesis 5 (reported as a one-tailed test). H933: Significance is considered at an alpha of .05. As indicated in Table 4.12, seven of the eight affective measures show no significant differences between high and low groups on the Aggression scale. The test between groups on the affective measure Disgust, however, indi- cates that there may be a difference in the percentage of disgust displayed between those who score high and low on Plutchik's Aggression scale. Those who score high on Ag- gression have a mean Disgust score of 25.75, those who score low a mean Disgust score of 13.92, for a difference 119 of 11.83 at a probability level Of p = .022. (See Table 4.13.) Table 4.13 Affect Scores for High and Low Aggression Groups Dependent Variable Group n i SD Percentage of Disgust High Aggr 17 25.75 17.57 Low Aggr 27 13.92 15.17 It should be noted that the group differences in this section have been explored to provide further infor- mation about the relationships between the independent and dependent variables in this study. While tests for group differences did provide further evidence of signif- icant relationships, these results should be looked at with caution since significance was defined at an alpha level of .05, and consequently information about the ov- erall Type I error for these tests is inadequate. Correlational Analyses A 24 x 24 intercorrelational matrix was used to further explore relationships and provide descriptive in— formation about the variables used in this study (see Appendix F' for complete matrix). Following are data 120 which address relationships within and between the set of pencil/paper instruments, within the set of affective mea- sures, and between the test instruments and the affective measures. In addition, two methodological measures, per- centage Of agreement among raters and percentage of dis- carded samples per subject, were included in the analyses and are presented at the end of this section. All data in this section are based on scores from the 168 subjects in the total sample, with the exception of data pertaining to the affective measures, for which there were only 62 subjects. Within Instrument Correlations The three pencil/paper instruments used in the study, the Plutchik Emotions Profile Index (PEPI), the Eysenck Personality Inventopy (EPI), and the Birkman Meth- pd (BM) are all standardized instruments designed to mea- sure affective orientation. The following data include all scales from each instrument used in this study, and in the cases of the PEPI and the EPI, the data includes all remaining scales of each instrument at well. Plutchik Emotions Profile Index. The PEPI has a total of nine scales, five of which were used as inde- pendent variables in this study. The Bias scale is used as an indicator of social desirability, though it may 121 also be an accurate description of the person. A high score indicates a tendency to choose the more socially desirable descriptor (shy rather than resentful, adven- turous rather than gloomy); a low score indicates the op- posite tendency (brooding rather than obedient, quarrel- some rather than sociable). The remaining eight scales are constructed so as to be four sets of bi-polar opposites. Figure 4.1 presents these eight scales graphically on Plutchik's "Emotion Circle," and includes the list of item descrip- tors from the test that make up each scale. (Note that the weights of the items per each scale are not included.) Correlations between the scales of this instru- ment are presented in Table 4.14. It should be noted, however, that this is an ipsative instrument and that consequently the reported correlations between scales will be confounded by this factor. As indicated by Table 4.14, the range of correlations for the instrument is -.80 to +.79. Aggressive is the only scale which consistently shows relationships with all other scales, indicating that it might not be as independent a measure as the other scales. The bi-polar pairs Of scales have negative cor- relations ranging from -.19 to -.60: Dyscontrol/Control 122 S‘ TR :9 < 69 affectionate affectionate OJ- Q; sociable sociable \S' é obedient 00 J,” 1. la 91°03? ; ‘q Umm3ve immune E; O self-conscious obedient 0 THE.” “3;“ O( resentful g (6‘ quarrelsase gloomy O self-conscious brooding (y ' SZRU (Leg? STFUL ° DE?“ Figure 4.1. Plutchik's Emotion Circle *Scales used in this study as independent variables. with a negative relationship at -.19; Depressead/Gregar- ious with a negative relationship at -.33; Distrust/Trust with a negative relationship at -.58; and Aggressive/Timid with a negative relationship at -.60; suggesting that Distrust/Trust, and Aggressive/Timid may have stronger bi-polar relationships than do Dyscontrol/Control and Depress e d/Gregarious. 123 mmflm Huuco eases amass oo.H mo.o oo.H mono Nv.o mh.o Hm.o mm.o vm.ot me.ot oo.H mm.o ha.o1 oo.H mh.o oo.H Hmmd om.ol vm.ol om.OI vm.ol ov.OI oo.H Human Ho.OI no.0 hm.OI mm.oa Hm.ou me.o oo.H mummo comma mm.ol NH.ot mo.ol hm.on mm.OI vv.o no.OI oo.H mo.o ma.on om.ol $0.0: mH.o Hm.o va.ol mm.ol oo.H mafia omHHOHucOU cases Hsmumsua msOwummmuu o>flmmmumm¢ Hsmumsuumao ommmmumma omaaouucoomho Hmmm How mcoflumHmquU mamomuumucH vH.v mance 124 The three categories most generally perceived as negative in social contexts (Depressed, Distrustful, and Aggressive) correlate negatively to the Bias scale, indi- cating that persons who choose the more socially desirable descriptor items would not have high scores on these three scales. Figure 4.2 summarizes the relationships Of the nine scales to one another by grouping them into categor- ies of strong negative relationships (-.80 to -.35), strong positive relationships (.35 to .77) and minimal relationships (00 to 1.34). Eysenck Personality Inventory. The EPI has a total of three scales: Extraversion and Neuroticism were used as independent variables in this study; the Lie scale was used only for the purpose of eliminating sub- jects at the high end from the interview phase of the data collection. The extraversion scale is constructed so that persons scoring at the high end are characterized as ex- traverted, and at the low end, as introverted. Similar- ly, the Neuroticism scale is constructed so that persons scoring at the high end are characterized as neurotic, at the low end, stable. In general, average scores on both scales fall between the 3lst and 70th percentiles; per- sons scoring above or below these cutting points would JJZS .Hamm MOM madamCOMucHOm manomuumucu .~.v musmwm oases o>wmmoum pmaaouu .Hsuumsuu nos .Hsmumsuu Icoo .pmaaouusoomap .msOwummeum Imwp .pommoummp mean moan .w>wmmmumme stumsuu .Hsu lumsuumwo .pmmmmum loo .poaaouusooeap pwafiu anewummmum Houucoo o>wmmwum stuwsuu .mso mean Imp .stumsuumwp nflummmum .ommmwuamp .OOHHOuucoo .OOHHouucOOmmp passe mean .m>wmmoum poaaouusoo .pw loo .Hsuumsuu law» .owaaouucoomap msofiummoum leap .pmmmmuawo asuumsua commonest .pfi mafia Houucoo .m>«mmeum nawu .OOHHOuucoomap .stumsuu tum .Hsmumsuumwp msOwummmuo mean meadow» Hawumsuu peed» .Hsuumsuu Icoo .pmaaouucoomxp swap .pwmmmummp .msOwucmwum m>wmmeuom< mews Omaaouucoo .pOmmoua pwswu .Hsuumsuu Imp .pmaaouusoomxp O>flmmmummm .msOwummmum Houumsuumwn pmaaouucoo .pwaau .nsofiumm mean .Hsuumsuu Imam .Hsuumsuumwp m>wmmmummc .pwaaouusoomwp ommmmummo mean Houusoo .stumsuu .ms0«ummmum .m>wm twosome .stumsuumwp pwEwu .pmmmmumoo pmHHOMuOOOmao 3m.“ 3 8.. E. 3 5.... mos Enron 8.1 omz maaom Hashes: mcouum mcouum 126 be characterized as above or below average on the trait under consideration. Table 4.15 presents the inter-scale correlations for this instrument. Table 4.15 Inter-scale Correlations for EPI Extraversion 1.00 Neuroticism -0.05 1.00 Lie Scale -0.24 -0.23 1.00 Extrv Neurot Lie As indicated in Table 4.15, the relationship of the Ex— traversion and Neuroticism scale shows a slight negative correlation (-.05), which is congruent with Eysenck's findings (Eysenck, 1968). This data seem congruent with Eysenck's hypothesis that the two scales are essentially uncorrelated and are orthogonal in nature. The relationship of each of these scales to the Lie Scale shows a slightly stronger negative correlation (Extraversion/Lie Scale at -.24 Neuroticism/Lie Scale at -.23), suggesting that in this sample subjects who score high on Extraversion and Neuroticism are less prone to "fake good" on the test. Eysenck does not provide inter- correlational data for this scale, and suggests only that 127 a score at 10 or above shows that "faking good" is likely to have occurred. The Birkman Method. The BM has a total of elev- en personality scales; two scales (Self-Consciousness and Depressed) were combined for the purposes of this study to create a research scale labeled Getting Along With Others. The other BM scale used in the study was Dominance. Ta- ble 4.16 presents the inter-scale correlations for these two scales. Table 4.16 Inter-scale Correlations for BM Dominance _ 1.00 G.A.W.O. .58 1.00 Dom GAWO As indicated in Table 4.16, the relationship between Get- ting Along With Others and Dominance shows a positive correlation at .58 suggesting an overlap of approximate- ly 31 percent between these scales for this sample. This corresponds to information reported by Birkman (1976) indicating that Dominance and the two scales combined to create Getting Along With Others (Self-Consciousness and Empathy) all have positive correlations to one another. 128 Between Instrument Correlations Correlations of scales from Plutchik's Emotions Profile Index with scales from the Eysenck Personality Profile and the Birkman Method are presented in Table 4.17. Table 4.17 Correlations of Scales from Plutchik with Scales from Eysenck and Birkman Plutchik Dyscontrolled 0.38 -0.29 -0.10 0.09 -0.13 Depressed -0.26 0.27 0.02 -0.04 0.11 Distrustful -0.08 0.39 -0.24 0.17 0.18 Aggressive -0.01 0.25 -0.16 0.13 0.09 Gregarious 0.44 -0.33 0.10 -0.23 -0.31 Trustful 0.32 -0.30 0.20 -0.14 -0.24 Timid -0.40 -0.11 0.23 -0.06 0.14 Controlled -0.42 0.03 0.03 0.02 0.20 Bias 0.14 -0.43 0.18 -0.17 -0.24 Extra- Neuro- Lie Domin- G.A.W. versn ticsm ance O. \ y:/ \; 11 Eysenck Birkman As indicated by Table 4.17, the range of correlations be- tween the Plutchik scalesarfl.those of Eysenck and Birkman is -.43 to +.43. Again it should be noted that the 129 Plutchik instrument is an ipsative one, so that correla- tions between its scales and the scales of the other two instruments may be confounded by this factor. Of these between-instrument correlations, only four correlations indicate relationships at i.40 or more. Gregarious and Extraversion show a positive correlation at .44; both Timid and Controlled show negative correla- tions to Extraversion at -.40 and -.42, respectively. The Plutchik Bias scale shows a negative correlation to the Eysenck Neuroticism scale (-.43) indicating an inverse correlation between those who score high on Neuroticism and those who choose the more socially acceptable descrip- tor items in describing themselves. Neither Of the Birkman scales shows evidence Of strong relationships with any of the Plutchik scales. It is of interest to this study, however, that the Birk- man scale Dominance indicates a positive relationship with Distrustful and Aggressive (.17 and .13, respective- ly) and a negative relationship with Trustful and Gregar- ious (-.14 and -.23, respectively). It is also of inter- est that the Birkman Getting Along With Others Scale shows a negative relationship with Gregarious and Trust- ful (-.31 and -.24, respectively). Correlations between the scales of the Eysenck 130 Personality Inventory and the Birkman Method are present- ed in Table 4.18. Table 4.18 Correlations Between Scales of the Eysenck Personality Inventory and the Birkman Method Eysenck Extraversion 0.05 -0.19 Neuroticism 0.10 0.36 Lie -0.17 -0.20 Domie G.A.W.O. nance Birkman As indicated by Table 4.18, the range of correlations be- tween the scales of Eysenck and Birkman is -.20 to +.36. All relationships between the scales of these two instru- ments are slight. Both Birkman scales have mild negative correlations to the Eysenck Lie scale. The two scales differentiate slightly, however, in their relationships to the Extraversion and Neuroticism scales. While Domi- nance shows virtually no relationship to Extraversion or Neuroticism, suggesting that it measures characteristics quite independent of these two scales, Getting Along With Others does suggest a slight overlap, with a mild posi- tive relationship with Neuroticism (.36) and a mild nega- tive relationship with Extraversion (-.19). 131 Correlations Within the Affective Measures As previously noted, the data for these correla- tions is based only on the 62 subjects participating in the interview phase of the study. Table 4.19 presents the correlations within the eight affective measures used in this study. Table 4.19 Correlations Within the Set of Affective Measures INTEREST 1.00 DISGUST 0.35 1.00 ANGER 0.09 0.17 1.00 DISTRESS 0.47 0.33 0.13 1.00 SURPRISE 0.30 0.19 -0.04 0.17 1.00 ENJOY 0.41 0.41 0.13 0.34 0.16 1.00 CHANGE 0.75 0.67 0.28 0.68 0.26 0.69 1.00 RANGE 0.73 0.67 0.31 0.69 0.35 0.75 0.96 1.00 INTER-DIS- ANGER DIS- SUR- ENJOY CHANGE RANGE EST GUST TRESS PRISE As indicated by Table 4.19, all relationships within the set of affective measures are positive ones with the ex- ception of Surprise and Anger, which show a negative cor- relation at -.04. As might be expected, due to their low frequency of occurrence during the interviews, Surprise and Anger in general show the lowest correlations to one 132 another and to all the other scales within the set of affective measures. The four strongest relationships are within the categories of Change, Range, Interest, and Enjoy. The high positive correlation between Change and Range (.96) suggests that those who change from one category of af- fect to another most frequently are also most likely to display a wider range of affect categories. The high positive relationships between both Enjoy and Interest with Range (.75 and .73, respectively) suggest that those who most frequently use Enjoy and/or Interest are likely to display a wider range of affect categories. Finally, the high positive relationship between Interest and Change (.75) suggests that those who use Interest most frequently are also most likely to change from one affect category to another the most frequently. With regard to potential patterns of usage among affect categories, correlations within the four most fre- quently used affects (Distress, Interest, Enjoy, and Dis- gust) do show some differential combinations. The high- est correlates Of Distress and Interest are one another, at a .47 correlation. The highest correlate for Disgust is Enjoy, at a .41 correlation. The highest correlates for Enjoy are Disgust and Interest, each at a .41 133 correlation. Thus Distress/Interest and Disgust/Enjoy seem to be the two most consistent sets of combinations. Correlations Between Test Instruments and Affective Measures ‘ Correlations between scales of the pencil/paper instruments and the eight affective measures are present- ed in Table 4.20. As Table 4.20 indicates, the range of correla- tions between the affective measures and scales of the pencil/paper instruments is -.29 to +.27, suggesting only mild relationships between any of these dependent and in- dependent variables. The affect measure Disgust shows the largest number of relationships at 1.20 or above, and also has the highest correlational values, with a nega- tive relationship with Trustful (-.29), a positive rela- tionship with Distrustful (.27), and a negative relation- ship with Gregarious (-.25). As previously noted, the data for correlations between the independent variables and the affective mea- sures are based on scores from 168 subjects on the pen- cil/paper instruments and scores from 62 subjects on the affective measures. Since some of the earlier tests (based only on subjects who score high or low on the in- dependent measures) do show significant relationships, the 134 mmHmm mmmme 9mm mozHmmmmOU¢ $H.0 va.o 00.0 00.0! 0H.o ma.o hN.0 no.0 ADmBmDMBmHQ $0.0 $0.0 no.0 $0.0 00.0 mH.o No.0 no.0! Qmmmmmmma 00.0 Ho.0! 00.0 no.0! no.0 00.0! 00.0! no.0! Omnqomszoomwo 50.0! 00.0! No.0! Ho.0 $0.0! $0.0! $0.0! H0.o quum mHA v0.0 mo.o $0.0 $0.0! 00.0 00.0 $0.0 $0.0! ZmHUHBOMsz $H.o! hH.0! $0.0! va.0! $0.0! 0H.0I NH.0! 00.0! ZOHmmm>HHUQHW< U36 WQHMUW HCQEDHUWCH G003H0m MCOHUMHGHHOU o~.¢ OHQOB 135 generally weak relationships indicated by Table 4.20 sug- gest that there may be a curvilinear relationship between the independent and dependent variables. That is, rela- tionships between test scores and affective measures ap- pear more clearly differentiated when only high and low groups are compared on the affective measures, and not when the full range of test scores is compared on these measures . Correlations Between Methodological Measures and Affective Measures Rater scores on two measures, percentage of agreement per subject and number Of discards per subject, were averaged and included in the correlational matrix. Table 4.21 presents correlations between these scores and subject scores in each of the affect categories. Table 4.21 Correlations Between Methodological Measures and Affective Measures Percentage of Agree- ment 0.76 0.65 0.22 0.63 0.29 0.82 0.93 0.96 # Of Dis- cards 0.45 0.62 0.15 0.56 0.25 0.58 0.75 0.75 INT- DIS- ANGER DIS- SUR- ENJOY CHANGERANGE EREST GUST TRESS PRISE 136 As indicated by Table 4.21, correlations between the per- centages of rater agreement per subject and subjects' scores on affective measures all show positive relation- ships. Subjects who displayed high percentages Of Enjoy show the strongest relationship to high percentages of rater agreement (.82) followed by subjects who displayed high percentages Of Interest (.76), of Disgust (.65), of Distress (.63), of Surprise (.29) and of Anger (.22). These relationships are similar to those presented in Table 3.1 (Rater Agreement by Affect Category) where the category with the highest rater agreement was Enjoy, fol- lowed by Interest, Disgust, and Distress. Surprise and Anger switch order here, as the category Anger had a higher percentage of rater agreement than did Surprise, while subjects displaying a high percentage Of the af- fect Surprise had a higher correlational relationship to rater agreement than did those displaying Anger. Summary The fifteen hypotheses used in the study were tested to determine group differences on the affective measures. The nine independent variables on which sub- jects were divided into high and low groups were Gregar- ious, Distrustful, Depressed, Dyscontrolled, Aggressive, 137 Getting Along With Others, Dominance, Extraversion, and Neuroticism. The affective measures used were Enjoy, Disgust, Distress, Surprise, Anger, Interest, Range of Affect, and Change Of Affect. T-tests were used to test group differences, with the alpha level set at p‘(.05. Significant differences were found between high and low Distrustful groups on the affective measure of Disgust (p=.008); and between high and low groups Of Getting Along With Others on the affective measure of Range of Affect (p=.028). A multivariate analysis Of variance was per- formed in order to examine how the two groups of subjects on each independent variable relate to the set of eight dependent measures. Significance was considered at an alpha level Of .05 for this test. No significant dif- ferences were found for any of the independent variables on the set of eight affective measures. Additional t-tests were used to further explore the relationships between independent and dependent vari- ables not addressed in the hypotheses. In addition, t-tests were used to test group differences on two meth- odological measures, rater agreement and number of dis- cards per subject. NO significant differences were found between groups on the methodological measures. 138 Additional differences were found, however, between high and low Gregarious groups on the affective measure DiSe gust and on the affective measure Change of Affect; between high and low Distrustful groups on the affective measure Interest; and between high and low Aggressive groups on the affective measure Disgust. Significance was considered at an alpha level of .05 for these tests; evidence of significant differences, however, was re- garded cautiously since there is inadequate information about the overall Type I error rate for this group of univariate analyses. Correlational analyses were used to provide descriptive information about relationships within and between the set Of pencil/paper instruments used as the independent variables, within the set of affective meas- ures used as the dependent variables, and between the test instruments and the affective measures. Scales from Plutchik's Emotions Profile Index were found to range in correlational values from -.80 to +.79. Scales comprising bi-polar pairs showed differ- ential strengths of negative relationships, ranging from -.19 to -.60. It was noted that correlations within the scales of this test may be confounded by the ipsative nature of the instrument. 139 Scales from the Hysenck Personality Inventory were found to range in correlational value from -.24 to -.05, with the two major scales (Extraversion and Neuro- ticism) at -.05. This relationship seems congruent with Eysenck's hypothesis that the two scales are orthogonal in nature. The two scales from the Hirkman Method used in the correlational analyses show a positive relationship at .58, suggesting a moderate overlap between these two dimensions. Correlations between the instruments range from -.43 to +.43 for Plutchik and Eysenck, from -.31 to —.20 for Plutchik and Birkman, and from -.20 to +.36 for Eysenck and Birkman. Relationships that were of interest to the study were noted and will be discussed further in Chapter V. Correlations within the set of eight affective measures ranged from -.04 to +.96. The strongest cor- relational relationships were found between the measures of Change and Range (.96), Enjoy and Range (.75), Enjoy and Interest (.73), and Interest and Change (.75). The possible implications of these relationships will, again, be discussed in Chapter V. 140 Correlations between test instruments and af- fective measures ranged from -.29 to +.29, suggesting only mild relationships between any Of the independent and dependent variables. Since the earlier t-tests (using only high and low scores from the independent vari- ables) indicated stronger relationships than did the cor— relations (which included all scores from the independent variables) it was noted that there may be a curvilinear relationship between the sets of independent and depen- dent variables. CHAPTER V SUMMARY, CONCLUSIONS, AND DISCUSSION In this chapter, the study is summarized, con- clusions based on data analysis and methodology are ex- plored, and a discussion of suggestions for future re- search and implications for therapy is presented. Summary The primary purpose of this study was to inves- tigate whether there are significant differences in ex- pressions Of facial affect as recorded on videotape and measured by trained judges between persons with specific personality characteristics as measured by pencil/paper personality instruments. The specific questions addres- sed were: DO persons on specific personality dimensions vary in the types of facial affect they express; Do per- sons on specific personality dimensions vary in their frequency of change from one affect to another; and DO persons on specific personality dimensions vary in the range (or number of types of affects) they express. The secondary purpose of the study was a metho- dological one, and responded to the problems of measuring, 141 142 quantifying, and recording facial affect. The study was intended to contribute to the methodological literature by describing any further refinements in techniques and pro- cedures, and by continuing to address whether current technology permits meaningful data in this area. The assumption that personality characteristics related to emotional style are discernable in the face by specific expressions Of facial affect was the basis for this research. The potential use Of such information for subsequent research efforts and therapeutic inter- vention provided impetus for the study. A review of the literature was conducted in four related areas: Emotion, Facial Expression of Emotion, The Scoring of Facial Affect, and Specific Affects to be Used in This Study. The summary of Emotion noted that while this phenomenon has been studied from the evolu— tional, biological, and cognitive perspectives, as well as from the psychometric standpoint, it has not yet been successfully delimited nor conclusively defined. Darwin, as the first widely recognized scientist to prOpose a re- lationship between facial expressions and internal emo- tional states, began to identify categories of emotion on the basis of facial eXpressions, and to speculate on the functional or survival value Of each. 143 Subsequent to Darwin, Sylvan Tomkins develOped a theoretical framework for the emotions system as it interplays with the other physiological and psychological mechanisms, and includes the psychodynamic functions of the different affects and the relatedness of facial ac- tivity to the experience Of these affects. Based largely on the work Of Darwin and Tomkins, other researchers such as Ekman and Izard have worked to more systematically classify categories Of affect, develop labeling and recognition techniques for these categories, and generate empirical data about the labeling process as well as about the cross—cultural evidence for these funda- mental categories. A recent develOpment in this research area was stimulated by Haggard and Isaacs, with the dis- covery that the expressions of facial affect may have a demonstrable relationship to certain aspects of psycho- logical functioning, such as the presence Of ego mechanismh The scoring of facial affect continues to be problematic, partly due to the subjective nature of the task, and partly due to the technological procedures, which are for the most part cumbersome and costly. Systematic procedures have been developed, nevertheless, to facilitate this process, and were used where applicable as guidelines for research procedures in this study. .144 In deciding upon which facial affect categories to use in this study, considerations of reported rater reliability and likeLflmod Of occurance for each affect category was made. The categories Of interest, anger, disgust, surprise, enjoyment, and distress were selected to be used as the dependent measures for the study. It was noted, however, that interest was used as a neutral category and did not appear in the formal statements of hypotheses. Subjects were solicited from the undergraduate population Of the University of Texas-Austin. One hun- dred sixty-eight subjects were administered the battery Of pencil/paper research instruments used in the study. Sixty-two were subsequently selected to participate in the interview, on the basis of their high or low scores across the nine personality dimensions used. The three pencil/paper instruments used to assess :personality characteristics were the Plutchik Emotions PrOw _£ile Index, the Eysenck Personality Inventory, and the Birkman Method. Subjects were divided into two groups (high.and low) based on their scores on one of the per- sonality dimensions at a time. The interviewer was the same individual for all 145 subjects, and was instructed to engage in a responsive but non-confrontive manner. Subjects were asked to select an item of personal interest from the Mooney Problem Check Hisp's "Personal Psychological Relations" scale, and then to talk with the interviewer about that topic for 15 minutes. The interviews were videotaped, and a systematic sampling procedure was used to prepare the data for rating. The last three seconds of each 20 second interval of the interview was recorded as a discrete segment onto a master tape, resulting in a total Of 1,860 samples for the study. Samples from individual subjects were grouped in sets of five, and arranged in semi-random order on the master tapes so as to minimize response set on the part of the raters. Raters subsequently scored 30 individual samples of facial affect for each subject. Three females were trained as raters for the study. The training was an intensive five hour program based on techniques developed by other researchers in the area at Michigan State University (Inman, 1976; Wilson, 1976; Bowles, 1978). The training included the use Of both slides and videotapes, with both posed and unposed samples of facial affect. The six categories used in the study (Interest, Anger, Disgust, Surprise, Enjoyment, and 146 Distress) were clearly defined, and criterion characteris- tics labeled. Two additional measures, Change of Affect and Range of Affect, were compiled on the basis of cal- culating number of changes from one category to another and number of categories used/from the rater scoring sheets. A judgement for any one of the six categories was defined as agreement between at least two of the three raters. In the case where there was no agreement for a specific sample, the sample was considered unscoreable data. Judges did not begin the rating task until a .8 percentage of agreement had been established. All hypotheses were stated so as to test the relationship between performance on Specific personality dimensions and the type, range, or frequency of change of facial affect. Hypotheses were tested by a comparison of groups on the affective measures, using t—tests, with an alpha level set at .05. Supplementary analyses from the multivariate, univariate, and correlational aspects were also used for further exploration of differences and re- lationships. Results The fifteen hypotheses used in the study were designed to determine group differences on the affective 147 measures. The nine independent variables on which sub- jects were divided into high and low groups were Gregar- ious, Distrustful, Depressed, Dyscontrolled, Aggressive, Getting Along With Others, Dominance, Extraversion, and Neuroticism. The affective measures used were Enjoy, Dis- tress, Disgust, Surprise, Anger, Interest, Range of Affect and Change of Affect. All hypotheses were tested at an alpha level of .05. The null hypothesis was rejected for two of the 15 hypotheses. A significant difference was found between high and low Distrustful groups on the affective measure of Disgust (p;.008) indicating that persons who score high on Distrustful tend to display more disgust than do per- sons who score low on this scale. A significant difference was also found between high and low groups of Getting Along With Others on the affective measure Range of Af- fect (ps.028), indicating that persons who score high on Getting Along With Others tend to display more types of affect than do persons scoring low on this scale. While the hypothesized differences for Distrust- ful and Getting Along With Others were significant and congruent with Tomkins (1962, 1963), the lack of support for the remaining hypotheses may, tentatively, indicate a lack of empirical support for Tomkins' suggested rela- tionships between facial affect and personality character- istics. This lack of support might have been more a 148 measurement than a theoretical problem, since the variance within groups indicated that means between high and low were not as independent as would have been desired; never- theless, the lack of significance for predicted relation- ships and the evidence of significance for non-predicted relationships (discussed under Findings) remains trouble- some . Discussion The discussion section will be presented in two parts: Findings and Methodology. The Findings section will present conclusions and limitations related to the hypothesized relationships between facial affect and per- sonality dimensions, and to the supplementary analyses per- tinent to this issue. The Methodology section will pre- sent conclusions and limitations related to the use of raters and affect categories in the study. Findings Significant findings of hypothesized differences were limited, but additional t-tests did show evidence of further differences between high and low groups on the independent variables. The independent variable Dyscon- trol was the single personality dimension which failed to show any evidence of group differences on any one of the ‘ ML 149 affective measures. The independent variables Gregarious, Distrust- ful, Aggressive, and Getting Along With Others all showed evidence of significant differences between their respec- tive high and low groups on at least one affective measure at p < .05. The high Gregarious group showed less Disgust (p=0.27) and less Change of Affect (p=.034) than did the low Gregarious group. The high Distrustful group showed less Interest (p=.047) than did the low Distrustful group. The high Aggressive group showed more Disgust (p=.022) than did the low Aggressive group. The high Getting Along With Others group showed greater Range of Affect (p=.028 as reported under Hypothesis 6) than did the low group. In addition, Gregarious and Aggressive did show group differences between means in hypothesized direc- tions, but the high within-group variances may have obscured significance. The high Gregarious group had a higher mean than the low Gregarious group on the measure Enjoy, at p=.065; the high Aggressive group had a higher mean than the low Aggressive group on the measure Anger, at p=.097 (see Appendix E for these tables). The four remaining independent variables, Depres- sed,Dominance, Extraversion, and Neuroticism, all showed evidence of group differences, though not always in the 150 hypothesized direction, nor on the affective measure ad- dressed in the hypothesis testing. Depressed showed no evidence of high/low group differences on the hypothesized measure of Distress, but did show group differences on the measures of Interest, Anger, Range of Affect, and Per- centage of Rater Agreement, at probability levels of .164 or less. Dominance showed evidence of high/low group differ- ences on the hypothesized measures of Distress and Change of Affect, but in the direction opposite of that hypothe- sized, at probability levels of .264 and .239, respectively Extraversion showed evidence of high/low group dif- ferences on the measures of Enjoy and Disgust, at proba- bility levels of .174 and .175, respectively, and in the hypothesized directions. In addition, there was evidence for group differences on the measures Range of Affect, Change of Affect, and Anger at probability levels ranging from .063 to .118. Finally, Neuroticism did not show evidence of group differences on the hypothesized measure Change of Affect, but did show evidence of group differences on the measure Interest at p=.080. The probability level of these later tests is such that they must all be viewed with caution; nevertheless 151 there appears to be evidence beyond the two hypotheses showing significant test results that high and low groups on these personality dimensions do differ in their expres- sions of facial affect when measured by the categories in this study. While this evidence for further differences is encouraging, it is nonetheless difficult to interpret. Evidence of further differences between these groups on the affective measures mentioned in some cases appears to contradict theoretical relationships prOposed by Tomkins (1962, 1963), and in many cases appears to be without theoretical underpinnings and is consequently left unex- plained. Aside from the basic question of whether there is sufficient theory upon which to base predictable differ- ences, a second question is whether a broader population containing more extreme personality scores might be needed in order to find the differences postulated by this study. Noting that there was evidence of a curvilinear relationship between the independent and dependent variables, it is pos- sible that while the population in this study did range from high to low on the independent measures, it was too homogeneous or too limited in its extremes to allow evi- dence for clearer differences between groups. In addition, the subjects used for high and low 152 groups on the independent measures overlapped, so that one subject was used in from two to five of these high/ low subgroups, thereby decreasing the independence of affective measures between these subgroups. The limita- tions of the subject pool,then, both in terms of extreme scores and in terms of overlap, may have contributed to the lack of significant group differences, despite the fact that group differences were found on some of the measures. A third question is whether the differences pre- dictedcxlthe affective measures between these personality types are observable in the interview setting that was used. While this setting was purposefully designed to be a neutral stimulus so that natural interaction patterns might emerge, it was limited to a 15 minute time frame, and might have elicited only those patterns which are pre- sent in the initial phase of an interaction between two strangers. It is possible that samples of facial affect over a much longer period of time would reflect an increase of self-disclosure such as that described by Jourard (1964), and would be different from the introductory behaviors that were measured by this study. Finally, there is the question of whether the af- fective measures used as dependent variables were the ap- propriate ones for which to test these differences. Since 153 these behavioral measures are considerably more specific than the more global trait measures to which they were com- pared, it may be that the information they yield is too limited to establish significance on these grosser person- ality measures. A more appropriate procedure might have been to have established a single, primary mode of affect for each subject which could then have been tested across personality dimensions. A second possibility would have been to collapse the specific affect categories into grosser measures such as happy and unhappy, or into the more general depressed/non-depressed categories used by Ekman and Friesen (1974b) in their study of depression in a clinical pOpulation. In conclusion, while it was possible to establish empirical support for two of the hypotheses, and to estab- lish evidence of differences or of significant differences between personality traits on the affective measures, it remains unclear whether the lack of support for the re- maining hypothesized differences is a result of inadequate theory or of the limitations of the study. The following section addresses further limitations in considering the methodological aspects of the study. Methodology While rater agreement was generally high for the 154 task of rating moving facial affect, it was less than op- timal for considerations of accuracy and replicability. In comparison to Izard's study reporting percentage of agreement among American females for labeling affect cate- gories, the inter—rater reliability per affect category for the raters in this study was generally five to fifteen percentage points higher for each category (Izard, 1971, pg. 272). For the categories of Anger and Surprise, how- ever, agreement was considerably lower than than reported by Izard (Anger, 48 vs. 70 percent; Surprise, 36 vs. 89 percent). Low percentages of rater agreement for Surprise and Anger in this study were confounded by the fact that the two affects appeared so infrequently they had few to- tal ratings on which to establish a percentage of agree- ment. A second consideration, however, is that of the six types of affects used, Anger and Surprise are typically the most intense but also the most fleeting of these. Thus their transient nature not only makes them more difficult to sample, but also more difficult to judge. It is also of note that Izard's percentages of agreement are based on judgments of photos or slides, thus while an adequately high percentage of agreement has been established for these categories on the basis of still sam- ples, agreement based on this study suggests that these 155 categories might be much less reliable measures in the judgment of moving facial affect. Of the four categories used most frequently by subjects (Enjoy, Interest, Disgust, and Distress), Dis- gust and Distress had the lowest agreements and seemed the most difficult to differentiate from one another (indi- cated by Figures 3.2 through 3.7 showing rater disagree- ment). It is difficult to determine whether the problem was confusion about the distinctions between the two categories, whether both appeared in the same 3-second sample and there was disagreement about which to label as predominant, or whether they were experienced simultaneously by the subject and thus expressed asaablended affect. Since Enjoy is also a frequent component of blended affect, however (Ekman and Friesen, 1975),and since the distinctions between Enjoy and either Distress or Dis- gust seemed much clearer (Figurei3.6h it.seems more likely that the problem was in adequately distinguishing them from one another, rather than that they occurred as blends or consistently together within the same samples. The subjectivity of the rating task remains a per- plexing issue. Informal observations suggested that it was difficult for raters to maintainaaconsistent standard for whether an expression of affect was sufficiently strong 156 to be labeled as such. While raters maintained agreement with one another, they were more demanding of a strong, clear affect display at the beginning of a rating session in order to consider it different from neutral; toward the end of a rating session they were more likely to accept subtle expressions as sufficiently different from neutral to be judged as one of the other categories. Some subjects were more difficult to rate than others, as evidenced by Figure 3.1, which indicates a range of 30 percentage points of agreement across subjects. There is also evidence to suggest that percentage of rater agreement may vary with personality type in some cases. The supplementary univariate analyses, for example, show rater agreement as higher for those scoring low on Getting Along With Others and on Dominance (ps.078; ps.077, respec- tively) than for those who scored high on these measures. (See Appendix E for Group Differences on Percentage of Rater Agreement.) A final problem was with the affect measures them- selves, in that raters commented on differences between some subjects which were not captured by the affect cate- gories used in this study. On an informal basis, for ex- ample, raters agreed that some faces were distinctly more labile than others, and that this dimension was not always 157 reflected in the Change of Affect measure. Additionally, they agreed that types of smiles between subjects were qualitatively different (anxious smiles; rigid, mask—like smiles; relaxed, casual smiles) and were not discriminated for by the one measure of Enjoy. In conclusion, for the task of rating moving facial affect it was possible to establish normatively high rater agreement for categories of Enjoy, Interest, Distress, and Disgust, but not for Anger and Surprise. While this might be reflected as a limitation of the study, in that the neutral setting did not elicit these affects as fre- quently, or in that the methodology did not provide for prOportional sampling of these categories, it may also im- ply that the reliability for these affects is not suffic- iently high to use with moving facial affect. In addition, it was difficult to distinguish be- tween Distress and Disgust, suggesting that these two categories may yield less reliable data and might profit from the development of more clearly differential criteria regarding their respective qualities. While subjectivity was controlled for at the be- ginning of each rating session by re-viewing the training tape, there were differences in rater standards betweentme beginning and end of a session, suggesting that this must 158 still be considered a limitation of the methodology as well as of the study. Finally, there were observable differences between subjects that were not scoreable by existing measures, which did not appear to influence the reliability of the measures used, but did result in..a loss of data that might have been pertinent to this particular study. A notable deficit in the scoring of moving facial affect is the lack of a standardized, packaged training program such as Ekman's "Facial Affect Scoring Technique" (Ekman, Friesen and Tomkins, 1971), which was developed for use with still samples. While standards of reliability established by percentage of rater agreement were viewed as adequate for the purposes of this study, the replica- bility is severely limited by the subjective and idiosyn- cratic nature of the training program, and of the process itself. Repeated use of this methodology with different samples, however, might allow for the develOpment of a standardized rater training program for use with videotape or moving affect. Implications Suggestions for Further Research The field of facial affect is a relatively new 159 one, and the possibilities for relating expressions of facial affect to personality dimensions or using them as psychodiagnostic information is even more recent (Haggard. and Isaacs, 1966; Wilson, 1976a; Izard, 1971; Ekman and Friesen, 1974b). While this study shows only limited em- pirical support for these efforts, it nevertheless does not contradict these possibilities. Of primary importance is a tighter, more comprehensive theory to provide direc- tion in this area. Continued research efforts can provide more stimulus for theoretical models, as well as empirical support or lack thereof for existing theory. In order to work within the current state-of-the- art perspective, more descriptive studies should be done in order to factor out which variables can be most profitably considered. Measurement, research conditions, and popu- lation parameters must continue to be investigated in or- der to determine which procedures may be most useful. In order to overcome the possible effects of homo- geneity within the sample, the current study should be replicated with a larger sample, and with a sample substan- tially more diverse on the abnormality/normality continuum with respect to each of the independent measures used. Finally, considerations of facial affect measure- ment based on this study suggest that at this stage, it may 160 be more profitable to establish a primary mode of affect for each subject, which can be compared across independent measures, rather than to consider a multiple set of affects in comparison to a diverse set of personality dimensions, with an insufficient amount of theory to direct hypothesized differences. Secondarily, it may be more appropriate to use grosser measures such as micro- momentary expressions or happy/unhappy dimensions than it is to use the more discriminate categories of facial affect to relate to the more global personality measures such as those used by this study. It is of utmost importance that methodological considerations continue to form part of the basis for future research in this area. Implications for Therapy Due to the exploratory nature of this study and the aforementioned limitations, results must be regarded with caution, and particularly those which were not formally hypothesized. There is evidence to suggest, nonetheless, that the clinician who observes facial affect might begin to speculate on the amount and type of information available through these facial expressions. Based on this study, the clinician might begin to observe whether, indeed, a high frequency of the affect Disgust 161 signals personality characteristics of high Distrustful- ness or Aggression, or of low Gregariousness; or whether a wide range of affect signals a personality character- istic of internal sensitivity, such as that described by the Getting Along With Others scale. Hopefully, for those who use nonverbal informa- tion in the clinical setting, this research has pin- pointed one particular area Of nonverbal behavior in a manner that will stimulate further thought and observa- tion, as well as having explicated it in such a way that the limited amount of information we do now have in this area can become useful. - 4.. su.1...!...c.4..u.. .l.l.. . . f . . 4.... .. .. . -1... . A . .. . 1:! f1 71%).}; l. . . 4 . .l‘ APPENDIX A SUBJECT CONSENT FORMS FOR RESEARCH PARTICIPATION 162 1153 SHORT CONSENT FORM FOR RESEARCH STUDY 1, , hereby give my consent to participate in research study on , the general plan of which has been explained to me including anticipated bene- fits, risks, and potential complications. I fully understand as it has been explained to me that by notice given to the undersigned principal investigator that I may withdraw from this re- search project anytime that I may elect to do so. Parent's/Guardian's Signature in the Participant's Signature case of a minor (under 18 yrs of age) ********** I hereby certify that I have given to the above individua1(s) an explana- tion of the contemplated study and its risks and potential complications. Date Signature of Principal Investigator 164 H 0.53253 .nomomusm noumomou you manomem smexu.m on» one mauow omega cm: 0» co.umwwsmouo no :Omuom damn n.5u ouwuosuse .>m00u we >n pouoaeeco on maqu convex scexuwm one mo nwm>qmsm uwonu uo madame» onu Ou uuomou ou ..0:H .moum«00mm¢ a scaxuwm caduceusm >nmuon H fizmzmmmud ZOH9 c2632. .82an”. 8a»... .25 80.62.. .0083.an he. so 5332 so. 08: 2. =0: guesses. 9.0.6.3.. 2.... "50...? 3.. £9. .— 02 D 3> D ”80.2. 8.2.5.330 00502 550.0 05 082an". :2. 26.. see 3...? 80.8.. ”on. 00.3.? seeds—ob £50092 no. .— .850 D 35800 D 05.8560 D .5003... no— D A28 b.0260 8:35.330 55...: mean—assoc 3.. 083% 8...... 00.. 388.. ”3.3.0:... 388.. .5 23m .96 .925 uoasis—5. 0.00.: .3... .3 02.82 2. :8 =9. ”oEez 050:3 boar—.3: GEOF—m 6:5 hmmzm 20....(5mcuz. .2... corpus. zSszm ure. Obi .>wt APPENDIX B INTERVIEW MATERIALS 165 16. 17. 18. 19. 20. 71. 72. 73. 74. 75. 126. 127. 128. 129. 130. 181. 182. 183. 184. 185. 236. 237. 238. 239. 240. 291. 292. 293. 294. 295. 166 Items of the Personal Psychological Relations Scale of the Mooney Problem Check List Being timid or shy Being too easily embarrassed Being ill at east with other people Having no close friends in college Missing someone back home Wanting a more pleasing personality Losing friends Wanting to be more popular Being left out of things Having feelings of extreme loneliness Feelings too easily hurt Being talked about Being watched by other people Worrying how I impress people Feeling inferior Being too envious or jealous Being stubborn or obstinate Getting into arguments Speaking or acting without thinking Sometimes acting childish or immature Disliking someone Being disliked by someone Feeling that no one understands me Having no one to tell my troubles to Finding it hard to talk about my troubles Too self-centered Hurting other people's feelings Avoiding someone I don't like Too easily led by other people Lacking leadership ability 167 VERBAL INSTRUCTIONS TO SUBJECTS DURING INTERVIEW (As subject enters the interview room) Hi, my name is . (Wait for subject to respond with his/her name; if s/he doesn't, ask "What's your name?") (Motion for subject to sit in appropriate chair while ex- plaining.) This is the interview part of the research study. We'll be talking for about 15 minutes about something that is of concern to you. I'd like you to take this stack Of cards (hand subject the index cards) and just turn them.over, one by one, until you see a top- ic that is of concern or interest to you. Stop with the first topic you find that you can talk about; then just let me know what the topic is as you begin to talk about it. Interviewer instructions, as previously stated in "Pro- cedures" are to indicate understanding by nonverbal ac- knowledgement, or to verbally reflect or briefly para- phrase the content that the subject is presenting. The interviewer will not challenge, nor lead the subject in a specific direction, but rather engage in a more passive style that simply encourages the subject to continue talking. If the subject exhausts the topic before the 15 minutes are us, s/he may look at the cards again, select another topic, and begin again. In the event that a subject ap- pears particularly anxious and unable to explore any cho- sen topic at length, the interviewer may respond to the anxiety in a reflective manner, and encourage the sub- ject to stay with a topic a little longer by considering different aspects of the issue. 1158 Code # Ageyyv Male Female Fresh Soph Jr Sr SELF-REPORT FORM 1) Check any of the following adjectives that apply to how you have been feeling in general today: ANGRY CONTENT DISAPPOINTED upszr suapaxszn HAPPY IRRITATED ANXIOUS SAD INTEREst WORRIED DISGUSTED SCARED FRUSTRATED CYNICAL EMBARRASSED 2) Here any of these feelings particularly strong for you during the )5 minute interview? If so, which one(s)? 3) How are you feeling now? 4) Please check the level of comfort you felt in talking with the interview person: L l J l 1 a I j I . T V V very mostly slightly slightly mostly very comfortable comfortable comfortable uncomfortable uncomfortable uncomfortable 5) Here you aware of any emotions that you experienced during the interview, but tried not to let the interview person know about? If so, please list those emotions here: Thanks again for your cooperation. Your participation has been greatly appreciated. Fran Stott,Staff Member Augustine Baron, Jr.. Psy.D. Counseling Center, Chairman, Research Committee and Principal Investigator Counseling Center 169 AUDIO/VISUAL RELEASE FORM I understand that my signature below gives the experimenter permission to keep and use the audio/visual tape recording made during my interview, and to take selected samples from it in the preparation of a master tape that will be used to rate facial affect. I understand that the primary purpose of these tapes will be for the collection of dissertation data, and that their primary use will be for observation and scoring by trained judges par- ticipating in the study. I also understand that segments of the tapes may on occasion be used for teaching purposes or presentations to professional groups. I understand that the confidentiality of the material will be protected, and that in no way will my name or other personal- ly identifying information be linked with the audio or video taped material. I also understand, however, that inasmuch as my facial features will be a part of the recording, there is a possibility that I may be recognized by those to whom the re— corded material is played, and for this I do not hold the experimenter or her associates reSponsible. SIGNATURE DATE 170 EXPLANATION OF THE STUDY There is a good deal of recent literature to suggest that one's emotions or emotional experiences can be studied on the basis of facial expressions. The type of emotions that we express on our faces, the emotions that we try to hide or cover up, and even the emotions that never appear can all be clues to the emotions we are experiencing, or to our emotional experience. This project has undertaken to study the facial expression of emotion in three ways: by the types of emotions that are expressed facially, by the range of types that are expressed, and by the frequency of change from one emotion to another. This information about the facial expressions will be correlated to information about particular personality characteristics as measured by the three instruments completed earlier in the study, in order to examine possible relationships between personality characteristics and facial affect. The interview setting was designed so that participants would be talking about a problem or personal concern, and so that the facial expressions recorded would to some extent provide information about the emo- tions the individual normally experiences in a problem solving setting. This information, in combination with the other test instruments, should provide new and useful 171 information regarding the manner in which students cope with problems. It is hoped that this information can be useful in designing programs and interventions that will be helpful to students in the future. We would prefer that you not discuss the focus on facial affect with other participants until after all interviews have been completed. Our experience has been that people become very self-conscious when aware that their faces are being studied, thus this information could distort or alter the facial expressions we are videotaping. Our preference is that participants be aware of the more general nature of the study--the issue of problem solving styles--until they are de-briefed following this interview. Again we thank you for your cooperation and participation in this study. Should you wish to receive a report on the outcome of the research, please write your name and address on the attached sign-up sheet. Fran Stott, Principal Investigator Counseling Center University of Texas, Austin APPENDIX C RATER TRAINING MATERIALS 172 I73 CROSS-CULTURAL SIMILARITIES IN FREE-RESPONSE LABELING 0F EXPRESSIONS (Izard) N= 268: 89 Americans, 62 British, 67 French, 50 Greeks A_Priori definitions of emotion categories are centered, followed by correct transcultural free-response labels in columns. INTEREST-EXCITEMENT concentrating, attending, attracted, curious attentive fervor (b) questioning concentration inquisitive reflection (a) concern (a) interest religious fervor (b) contemplation (a) ovservation (b) seriousness curiosity pensive somber reflection deliberating pondering thoughtfulness excitement (b) puzzlement wonder expectation (a) ENJOYMENT-JOY glad, merry, delighted, joyful amusement gratitude (b) playful bliss (a) happiness pleasantness clowning humor (a) pleasure contentment jovial rapture delight (a) joy satisfaction ecstasy laugh sees something pleasant elation merry self-satisfaction (a) enjoyment (b) mystical ecstasy serenity (a) gaiety optimism smile glee (a) SURPRISE-STARTLE sudden reaction to something unexpected, astonished amazed pleasant astonishment surprise amused surprise (b) pleasant surprise (b) joyful surprise astonishment shock startle (a fearful astonishment a; most commonly used by females b most commonly used by males 174 DISGUST-CONTEMPT sneering, scornful, disdainful, revulsion aversion (b) dislike scorn contempt distaste skepticism cynical insolence (b) smirk (a) derision mockery (a) smug (b) disapproval repugnance (a) sneer disdain repulsion superiority disgust sarcasm ANGER-RAGE angry, hostile, furious, enraged aggressive furious revenge (a) anger fury spite bitterness (b) mad vengeful (a) enmity (a) rage vexation (b) ferocity (a) DISTRESS-ANGUISH sad, unhappy, feels like crying about to cry (a) grief sad anguish hurt sorrow bad news loneliness suffering (a) crying melancholy (b) troubled (a) dejected misery (a) uneasiness dejection not going well unhappy depression pain (a) unloved (a) despair pathetic upset (a) disappointment pity (b) worry distress From Izard, Carroll, 1971. A. B. C. 175 DESCRIPTION OF FACIAL AFFECT CATEGORIES INTEREST (l) (5) Description: Interest is usually aroused when one spontaneously attends to the environment. Duration: Most people can maintain this affect over long periods of time. Function: The function of Interest is taking in information about the environment. Synonyms: Curious, concentrating, attending, absorbed, involved, attracted, intense, fascinated. Facial Components: Eyebrows level but may be slightly raised or lowered. Eyes open and usually fixated. Lips may be parted and jaw dropped slightly. ENJOYMENT (l) (2) (3) (5) (1) Description: Enjoyment is most people's favorite affect-- stimulated either by the onset of pleasant stimuli or the cessation of unpleasant/painful stimuli. Duration: The duration of this emotion varies with intensity. Mild enjoyment can be maintained for extended periods of time while ecstasy is maintained for shorter periods. Function: The function of Enjoyment includes both an internal component (self reinforcement) and an external component (social bonding). Synonyms: Happy, glad, merry, joyful, cheerful, blissful, jubilant, gay, elated, ecstatic, gleeful, jovial. Facial components: Eyebrows level or slightly lowered. Eyes bright or may be partially closed; eyes often have wrinkles ("crows' feet") in the outer corners. Corners of the mouth lifted back and up (exaggerated with laughing). Teeth often partially exposed, upper lip tensed. Nasolabial folds (run- ning from the nose to the outer edge of the mouth) are evident. SURPRISE Description: Surprise is triggered by the unexpected. If one anticipates an event, then it is impossible to be startled or D. E. 176 surprised. Dramatic examples of stimuli that trigger the surprise/startle response include an unexpected gunshot, or a flashbulb going off close at hand. (2) Duration: Surprise is distinguished by its very brief dura— tion. It is sudden in its onset and fades quickly. (3) Function: The function of Surprise is the clearing of the sensory systems for assimilation of new information. (4) Synonyms: Astonished, amazed, shocked, flabbergasted. (5) Facial Components: The brows are raised so that they are curved and high, the forehead wrinkles horizontally. The eyes are widened so that the white is visible above and some— times below the pupil. The jaw drops so that the lips are loosely parted. DISGUST (1) Description: Disgust is aroused by a disagreeable sensory experience: a "yeechhy" taste, a bad smell, an unpleasant sight, or a repulsive feeling object. More complex stimuli, such as ideas, things, or people can arouse disgust as well. There is an aloofness and a distancing or "put-down" com- ponent to the emotion Disgust; a sense of getting away from or getting rid of. (2) Duration: In mild to moderate forms, Disgust may be experienced for extended periods of time. However as disgust increases in intensity, so does the probability of nausea and vomiting, thus people often try to "leave the scene" before the feeling be- comes this intense. (3) Function: The function of Disgust is expulsion, a getting rid of or getting away from the disagreeable object or experience. (4) Synonyms: Scornful, disdainful, skeptical, condemning, critical, arrogant, sarcastic, spiteful, revolted, indignant. (5) Facial Components: The brow is lowered; one or both cheeks are raised; the nose is sometimes wrinkled. One or both sides of the upper lip are raised and protrude slightly; teeth are sometimes exposed. At times the tongue is slightly extended. ANGER (l) Description: Anger is likely to be triggered by frustration, physical threat, psychological hurt, violation of values, and F. 177 -3- failure to fulfill expectations. (2) Duration: Anger can be of short or long duration depending on circumstances and the reaction received from the environment. Duration does not vary with intensity; one may be mildly angry for a short or a long period of time and one may also be in- tensely angry for a short or a long period of time. (3) Function: The function of Anger is to mobilize the individual's resources for confrontation with a disturbing element in the environment. Further, Anger serves to arouse fear in others-- fear of loss of control on the part of the angry individual-- creating distance. (4) Synonyms: Irritated, hostile, mad, hateful, aggressive, annoyed, cranky, cross, disagreeable, furious, resentful. (5) Facial Components: Brows are lowered and drawn together, creating vertical lines in the forehead. Eyes have a hard stare and a bulging appearance. Lips are either firmly pressed together with corners down or drawn back in a squarish shape, baring the teeth. DISTRESS (1) Description: Distress is stimulated by loss: loss of a loved one, loss of an opportunity, loss of self-esteem, loss of health, loss of security are all experiences which stimulate feelings of distress. One can also experience distress in anticipation of a loss. (2) Duration: Distress is often a prolonged feeling, lasting more than a few minutes, often hours, sometimes days, and occasionally a year or more. (3) Function: Distress communicates deficiency to others in the environ- ment and attracts help-givers. (4) Synonyms: Sad, unhappy, miserable, hurt, dejected, depressed, despondent, dismal, low, grieved, suffering, worried. (5) Facial Components: Inner corners of the eyebrows are drawn up, vertical wrinkles in the forehead often appear. The skin below ' the forehead is triangulated with the inner corner up. The corners of the mouth are down and the lower lip may be trembling. From Inman, David, 1976. 179 Surprise* *Taken from Ekman and Friesen slide series (1976). 180 Distress* *Taken from Ekman and Friesen slide series (1976). 181 Disgust* *Taken from Ekman and Friesen slide series (1976). 182 .4w’.~..w.....;‘ , .a- "“ Anger* *Taken from Ekman and Friesen slide series (1976). 183 Enjoyment* *Taken from Ekman and Friesen slide series (1976). 184 «1.4., Tm.“ 49" 'md“"‘ a» ,1: w 34 3, ,‘x > Interest* *Taken from Ekman and Friesen slide series (1976). APPENDIX D RATER AND SCORING FORMS USED IN MEASUREMENT OF FACIAL AFFECT 185 186 all Atlanta 6011‘ 1.2.3.41... Coda” Lasts. Codi“ M1 Cola." 0041.11 Com! 1.11%.»). Mt 1.1.14.5 Cecilat L2.3.d..$. Coda.” 1.134s Codi?! 1.2.3.4.: 187 Femiooaw $5.230 nrmdhns 63.2». Lnoona .33 mt: #7. 7. “7. Z. i. ”7. J. # Dam a< ' a. W' 11 4451A— 45:“ 404.344! 44 dgoLu-qu #chfio'muA-IJ.____ 188 Ill/TE Les‘r D15 6 031' SULPBI$€ a W 4 DI. 1‘ 4A5.“— 14 Dim... 41W “W 11% olefin fiafigom‘awwl. § t%6h~tv‘é(“ can; i 905‘]; 201.14 I‘lfi‘.uo".‘v~? 9] 6/6 APPENDIX E SUPPLEMENTARY UNIVARIATE ANALYSES 189 190 T-test Results of Depressed Group Differences on the Affective Measures Dependent Variable E 2 Significant Percentage of Interest -1.45 .154 No Percentage of Disgust 0.12 .907 No Percentage of Anger 1.42 .164 No Percentage of Distress* 0.36 .361 No Percentage of Surprise 0.86 .395 No Percentage of Enjoy 0.20 .840 No Percentage of Change 0.74 .466 No # of Affects Used (Range) 1.53 .133 No *Hypothesis 3 (reported as a one tailed test). 191 T-test Results of Dyscontrol Group Differences on the Affective Measures Dependent Variable E 2 Significant Percentage of Interest -0.25 .805 No Percentage of Disgust 0.12 .902 No Percentage of Anger -0.86 .394 No Percentage of Distress 0.49 .627 No Percentage of Surprise* -0.32 .374 No Percentage of Enjoy 0.13 .900 No Percentage of Change 0.63 .533 No # of Affects Used (Range) 1.18 .246 No *Hypothesis 4 (reported as a one-tailed test). 192 T-test Results of Getting Along With Others Group Differences on the Affective Measures Dependent Variable ‘E p_ Significant Percentage of Interest -0.19 .853 No Percentage of Disgust* 0.93 .175 No Percentage of Anger -0.10 .921 No Percentage of Distress 0.52 .607 No Percentage of Surprise 1.08 .291 No Percentage of Enjoy* -0.91 .185 No Percentage of Change* 1.35 .095 No # of Affects Used (Range)* 2.00 .028 Yes *Hypotheses 6-9 (reported as one tailed tests). 193 T-test Results of Dominance Group Differences on the Affective Measures Dependent Variable ‘t 2, Significant Percentage of Interest 0.55 .589 No Percentage of Disgust 1.09 .284 No Percentage of Anger 0.98 .335 No Percentage of Distress* 0.64 .264 No Percentage of Surprise -1.61 .118 No Percentage of Enjoy -1.91 .066 No Percentage of Change* 0.72 .239 No # of Affects Used (Range) 0.03 .974 No *Hypotheses 10-11 (reported as one tailed tests). 194 T-test Results of Extraversion Group Differences on the Affective Measures Dependent Variable E 2 Significant Percentage of Interest -0.11 .911 No Percentage of Disgust* -0.82 .207 No Percentage of Anger -1.70 .096 No Percentage of Distress 0.51 .615 No Percentage of Surprise -1.31 .196 No Percentage of Enjoy* 0.95 .174 No Percentage of Change -1.59 .118 No # of Affects Used (Range) -1.90 .063 No *Hypotheses 12-13 (reported as one tailed tests). 1.95 T-test Results of Neuroticism Group Differences on the Affective Measures Dependent Variable ‘E 2 Significant Percentage of Interest -l.79 .080 No Percentage of Disgust 1.06 .296 No Percentage of Anger -0.07 .943 No Percentage of Distress 0.31 .760 No Percentage of Surprise -0.88 .386 No Percentage of Enjoy 0.49 .627 No Percentage of Change* -0.61 .273 No # of Affects Used (Range) -0.84 .403 No *Hypothesis 14 (reported as a one tailed test). 196 T-test Results of Group Differences on Percentage of Rater Agreement _ Signif- Group ‘3 x SD ‘3 p, icant 22222;. 223“ 22 32:22 3:22 0-44 -663 No Ziggoti- High 27 76.81 7.58 _.40 .688 No Low 23 77.69 7.78 Siigari' 222“ 23 32:22 2:22 -68 ~499 No 3:3: :3 3:32 3:3: N. 2:31: 2: 33:22 3:32 N. 2:25:25 High 22 76.36 8.81 _.24 .812 No Low 17 77.00 7.42 222263‘ 222h 23 32:22 2:22 --83 .412 No GA”° 222“ 22 32:22 2:32 -1-84 .073 No D°minan°e 222“ 22 22:22 2:22 -l-84 ~077 No T-test Results of Group Differences on Number of Rater Discards 197 Signif- Group E x SD ‘3 E icant 22222Sn 222h 22 2:22 2222 -03 ~974 No 2222°ti' 222h 22 2222 2232 ~21 -836 No Egigar— High 25 2.64 1.72 -1.40 .171 No Low 17 3.41 1.80 2222““St' 222“ 3; 3:23 1:32 -o.51 .609 No ......... 222“ 2: 2:23 2:3: ... .... N. 222222; 222“ 22 2:22 2:22 -06 -949 No 2:258- 2:3: :3 3:2: 2:32 N. :23“ 22 2:2: 1:3: N. 2222; 222“ 22 2:22 2:22 1-04 .307 No APPENDIX F 24 X 24 INTERCORRELATIONAL MATRIX 198 1199 mmwm .coAAouucou .cAEAe .Aswumoua .msoAummouu .o>Aomoucm¢ .Aomumzugon .oomnoummc .UOAAouucoomhc "Homo xAnuusAm coma Mom omcm mczu mam muom Dunc hand ammo HucH 0349 son oc.A cc. m5. mh. oc.A on. no. oc.A on. oc.A om. No. mp. co. oc.A mm. mm. mm. on. cA. oc.A omcmm .omcmnu .honcm .umAumuom .mmouuon .uom:< .umamnAo .umououau "mundane: o>Auoowu< om. nm. no. no. on. NA. oc.A mA. nN. An. on. nA. vo.1 nA. oc.A No. co. no. no. Av. cA. nn. hA. oc.A me. oh. nb. m5. Av. on. hv. mo. mm. oc.A pumomwo .ucoaomumc "moucmooz noummo p mumcuo 29A: mcoA¢ chuuoo .oocchEoo "vogue: cmEqumo Ho.- oo.u mH.- so.- po.s SH.- no.1 mo.. AH.- co. mc.- cA.- ho.- cA.- so.u .mo. ~H.- No. me. on.. «H.- Na. NH. H~.- Ho.- No. n~.- vo.- Ho.n co. oc.A mm. v~.- oc.A AH.- oc.A n oAmom 0AA .EMAOAu0uooz .coAmuo>muuxm "Ham xocomxmm «can ucoo oefia so. no.1 ma. co.u SH. oc.i AM. No.1 mo. cc. ho. Ho. mo. HH.- mo. cA.- Ha. ma.u ma. mo. cm. «A. «o. oo.1 me. «e. oc.A mm. oc.A umHB hA.1 oA.1 nN.1 nN.1 cA.1 no.1 AA.1 NN.I o~.1 no.1 vN.1 VA.1 on. vn.1 nm. oc.A mono hood uon ammo coho 0AA cA.1 oA.1 NN.1 nN.1 AA.1 mo. no.1 NN.1 m~.1 no.1 An.1 nN.1 Ac. mv.1 5A.! or. oc.A cA. AA. AA. cA. mA. cA. mA. VA. mo. mo. Ac. no.1 AA. cA. cA. oA. AN. hm. no.1 no. cc. cA. nA. FA. oo.1 Ao.1 on.1 no. cc.1 hn.1 vo.1 cm.1 ov.1 Am.1 oc.A av. oc.A ho. Ac.1 no.1 Ao. No.1 no.1 mo. co. no.1 mo. Ac.1 vo.1 no. cc. no.1 mo. no.1 Ac. vo. no. no.1 mA. no.1 oo.1 no. no.1 no.1 no.1 no.1 Ao. AA. nA.1 cN.1 vc.1 co. hA.1 mm.1 co. cA. NA.1 mA.1 no. no.1 om.1 nN. hn.1 oc.1 on. nn.1 mA. cA. vv. AN. cA.1 no.1 «A.1 vm.1 oc.A mn.1 No. oc.A cA.1 oc.A usoz uuxm no. No. vc. no. mo. no.1 no. vo. cc. no.1 on. cA. no.1 no. AA.1 cn.1 nn.1 mm. on. mm. mm.1 n~.1 oc.A ma.u coma ~H.i oum< oa.u omam FH.- menu oo.u flaw cA.- dusm oo.u name «A.1 umc< ~H.- ammo ma.u ouocH mH.- 0340 mo. oeoo «a. mafia «v.1 ucoo 94.- cans Na. anus «e. mono Ho.- name no.1 yawn o~.u ammo an house ¢~.- and mo.. usmz oc.A auoxm BIBLIOGRAPHY 200 BIBLIOGRAPHY Abbott, R. "Improving the validity of affective self- report measures through constructing personality scales unconfounded with social desirability: A study of the personality research form." Educa- tional and Psychological Measurement. 1975, 35, 361-370. Adam, A. Grandeur and Illusion; French Literature and Society 1600-1715. London: Weidenfeld and Nicolson, 1972. Allen, J. "Correlates of emotional styles." Journal of Consulting and Clincial Psychology. 1976, 44(4), 679 . _ Allen, J. & Hamsher, J.H. "The development and validation of a test of emotional styles." Journal of Con- sulting and Clinical Psychology. 1974, 42(S), 663-668. Argyle, Michael. "The laws of looking." Human Nature. January, 1978, 32-40. Arnold, M. Emotion and Personality: Neurological and Physiological Aspects. New York: Columbia Univer- sity Press, 1960a. Arnold, M. Emotion and Personality: Psychological Aspects. New York: Columbia University Press, 1960b. Arnold, M. (Ed.) Feelings and Emotions - The Loyola Sym- posium. New York: Academic Press, 1970. Ax, A.F. "The physiological differentiation between fear and anger in humans." Psychosomatic Medicine. 1953, 15(5), 433-442. Birkman & Associates, Inc. The Birkman Method: An Inter- pretative Handbook for Participants. Unpublished manual, 1974* *Materials available from Birkman & Associates, Inc., 3637 West Alabama, Houston, Texas 77024. 201 202 Birkman & Associates, Inc. The Birkman Method: Reliabili- ties and Validities for Business and Industry. Un- published report, January 15{T1975.* Birkman, R.W. Development of a Personality Test Using Social and Self-PerceptiOn Inventories. Unpub- Iisheleh.D. Dissertation, University of Texas- Austin, 1961.* Black, P. (Ed.) Physiological Correlates of Emotion. New York: Academic Press, 1970. Bowles, J.M. Learnedgelplessness: Personality Correlates and Affective Re5ponses. UnpublishediP .D. Disser- tation, Michigan State University, 1978. Brady, J.V. "Endoctrine and autonomic correlates of emotions." in Black, P. (Ed.), Physiological Correlates of Emotion. New York: Academic Press, 1970. Buros, O.K. (Ed.) The Seventh Mental Measurements Year- book. Highland Park, New Jersey: The Gryphon Press, 1972.2 Candland, D.K. "The persistent problems of emotion." in Candland, D.K. gt 31., Emotion. Monterey, Cali- fornia: Brooks/Cole Publishing Co., 1977. Chekhov, A. "The Kiss." in Mack, M. (Ed.), World Master- pieces. New York: W.W. Norton and Company, 1965. Chevalier-Skolnikoff, S. "Facial expressions of emotion in nonhuman primates." in Ekman, P. (Ed.), Darwin and Facial Expression: A Century of Re- search in Review. New York? Academic Press, 1973. Clynes, M. Sentics: The Touch of Emotions. Garden City, New York: Anchor Press7Doubleday, 1977. Coleman, J.C. "Facial expressions of emotion." Psycho— logical Monographs. 1949, 63 (1, whole No. 296). Darwin, C. The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals. New York: D. Appleton and Co., 1910. (originally published in 1872.) 203 Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. Washington, D.C.: American Psychiatric Assoc., 1968. Dougherty, F., Bartlett, E., & Izard, C. "Responses of schizophrenics to expressions of fundamental emo- tions." Journal of Clinical Psychology. 1974, '5g(3), 243-246. Ekman, P. "Body position, facial expression, and verbal behavior during interviews." Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology. 1964, 55, 295-301. Ekman, P. "Differential communication of affect by head and body cues." Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1965, 2f5), 725-735, Ekman, P. "Universals and cultural differences in facial expressions of emotion." in Cole, J.D. (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, 1971. Lincoln: Universify of Nebraska Press, 1972. Ekman, P. "Universal facial expressions in emotion." Studia Psychologica, 1973a, 55(2), 140-147. Ekman, P. Darwin and Facial Expression. New York: Academic Press, Inc., 1973b. Ekman, P. & Friesen, W. "Head and body cues in the judg- ment of emotion: a reformulation." Perceptual and Motor Skills. 1967, 24, 711-724. Ekman, P. & Friesen, W. "Nonberbal leakage and clues to deception." Psychiatry. 1969a, 55(1), 88-106. Ekman, P. & Friesen, W. "The repetoire of nonverbal be- havior: Categories, origins, usage, and coding." Semiotica. 1969b, 5, 49-98. Ekman, P. & Friesen, W. "Constants across cultures in the face and emotion." Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1971, 11(2), 124-129. Ekman, P. & Friesen, W. "Detecting deception from the body or face." Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1974a, 25(3), 288-298. 204 Ekman, P. & Friesen, W. "Nonverbal behavior and psycho- pathology." in Friedman & Katz (Eds.), The Psy- chology oleepression. Washington, D.C.: V.H. Winston & Sons, 1974b. Ekman, P. & Friesen, W. Unmasking_the Page: A Guide to Recognizing Emotions from Facial Expressions. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall, Inc., 1975. Ekman, P. & Friesen, W. "Pictures of Facial Affect" (slide series). Palo Alto, California: Consulting Psychologists Press, Inc., 1976. Ekman, P., Friesen, W. & Tomkins, S. "Facial affect scoring technique: A first validity study." Semiotica. 1971, 5! 37-58. Ekman, P., Friesen, W. & Ellsworth, P. Emotion in the Human Face. New York: Pergamon Press, 1972. Ekman, P. & Oster, H. "Facial Expressions of Emotion." Annual Review of Psychology. 1979, 55, 527-554. Ekman, P., Sorenson, E.R. & Friesen, W. "Pan-cultural elements in facial displays of emotion." Science. 1969, 164, 86-88. Eysenck, H.J. & Eysenck, 8.8. Manual for the Eysenck Personality_Inventory. San Diego: Educational and Industrial Testing Service, 1968. Eysenck, H.J. & Eysenck, S.B. Personality Structure and Measurement. San Diego: Robert Knapp, Publisher, 1969. Finalyson, G. "Effects of exposure to and re-appraisal of facial expressions depicting affect.” Dissertation Abstracts International. 1974, 55(10-B), 5188. Forrest, D.W. & Lee, S.G. "Mechanisms of defense and readiness in perception and recall." Psychological Monographs. 1962, 15(4), (No. 523). Freud, Sigmund. The Basic Writings of Sigmund Freud. New York: Random House, 1938. 205 Frois-Wittmann, J. 'The judgment of facial expression." Journal of Experimental Psychology. 1930, 55, I13-151. Gordon, Jesse, "Interpersonal predictions of repressors and sensitizers." Journal of Personality. 1957, 35, 686-698. Haggard, E. & Isaacs, K. "Micromomentary facial expres- sions as indicators of ego mechanisms in psycho- therapy.” in Gottschalk, L. & Auerbach, A. (Eds.) Methods of Research in Psychotherapy. New York: Appleton Century Croft, 1966, 154-165. Hanawalt, N.G. "The role of the upper and lower parts of the face as a basis for judging expressions." Journal of General Psychology. 1944, 55, 23-36. Harrison, R. & Crouch, W. "Nonverbal communication: theory and research." in Hanneman, G. & McEwen, W. (Eds.), Communication and Behavior. New York: Addison-Wesley, 1975. Harrison, R. Controversy On the Face. Kenilworth, N.J.: Schering Corporation, 1977. Harvey, N. The Feelings of Man: Their Nature, Function, and Interpretation. Baltimore: WarwiCk & York, Inc., 1914. Hill, D.J. The Elements of Psychology: A Textbook. New York: Sheldon, 1888. Hinds, W.C. "Compacted affect: a therapeutic concept." Paper presented at 84th Annual Conference of the American Psychological Association, Washington, D.C., September 7, 1976. Inman, D.J. "Investigation of affective facial expressions through slow motion and normal speed videotape techniques." Unpublished Ph.D. Dissertation, Michigan State University, 1976. Izard, C. The Face of Emotion. New York: Appleton- Century-Crofts, Meredith Corporation, 1971. 206 Izard, C. Patterns of Emotions: A New Analysis of Anxiety and Depres§ion. New York: Academic Press, 1972. Izard, C. Human Emotions. New York: Plenum Press, 1977. James. W. The Principles of Psychology. New York: Holt, 1890. Jourard, S. The Transparent Self. New York: Van Nostrand Reinhoid Company, 1964. Jung, C.W. Analytical Psychology: Its Theory and Practice. New York: Random House, 1968. Kaywin, L. "Notes on the psychoanalytic theory of affect." Psychoanalytic Review. 1966, 55, 112-118. Keen, Ernest. "Emotion in personality theory." in Candland, D.K. 55 al., Emotion. Monterey, Califor- nia: Brooks/Cole PEEIisEing'Company, 1977. Kell, B.L. & Mueller, W.J. Impact and Change: A Study of Counseling Relationships. New York: Appleton- Century Crofts, I966. Laird, J. "Emotion and touch: interesting observations and a discordant theory." Contemporary Psychology; 1977, 55, 570-571. Langfeld, H.S. "The judgment of emotions from facial ex- pressions." Journal of Abnormal and Social Psy- chology. 1918, 55, 172-184. Lazarus, R.S. "Emotions and adaptation: conceptual and empirical relations." in Arnold, W. (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation. Lincoln: Uni- versity of Nebraska Press, 1968. Lazarus, R., Averill, J. & Opton, E. "Psychological ap- proaches to the study of emotion." in Arnold, M (Ed.), Feelings and Emotions: The Loyola Sympos- igm, New York: Academic Press, 1970. Leathers, D.G. Nonverbal Commupication Systems. Boston: Allyn & Bacon, Inc., 1976. 207 Mar, T. Face Reading. New York: The New American Library 1975. Mooney, R. Mooney Problem Check List. New York: The Psy- chological Corporation, 1950. ' Munn, N.L. "The effect of knowledge of the situation upon judgment of emotion from facial expression." Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology. 1940, 55, 324-338. Plutchik, R. "The Multifactor-analytic theory of emotion." Journal of Psychology. 1960, 22x 153-171. Plutchik, R. The Emotions: Facts, Theories and A New Model. New York: Random House, 1962. Plutchik, R. "What is an emotion?" Journal of Psychology. 1965, 55, 295-303. Plutchik, R. "Emotions as adaptive reactions: Implications for therapy." Psychoanalytic Review. 1966, 55, 106-110. Plutchik, R. "Emotions, evolution and adaptive processes." in Arnold, M. (Ed.), Feelings and Emotions: The Loyola Symposium. New York: Academic Press, 1970. Plutchik, R. "Individual and breed differences in approach and withdrawal in dogs." Behavior. 1971, 55, 302-311. Plutchik, R. Foundations of Experimental Research. New York: Harper & Row, 1974. Plutchik, R. "Cognitions in the service of emotions." in Candland, D. 55 55., Emotion. Monterey, Califor- nia: Brooks/Cole Publishing Company, 1977. Reymert, M. (Ed.) Feelings and Emotions: The Wittenbepg Symposium. Worcester, Mass: Clark University Press, 1928. Rockberger, H. "The role of an eclectic affect theory in multiple therapy." Psychoanalytic Review. 1966, 55, 120-128. 208 Romanes, G. Mental Evolution in Animals. New York: Appleton, 1884. Sackeim, H., Gur, R. & Saucy, M. "Emotions are eXpressed more intensely on the left side of the face." Science. 1978, 202, 434-436. Schachter, S. & Singer, J. "Cognitive, social, and phy- siological determinants of emotional state." Psychological Review. 1962, 55, 379-399. Schiffenbauer, A. "Sex role stereotypes and the spon- taneous attribution of emotion." Journal of Research in Personality. 1976, 55(2), I37-145. Schlosberg, H. "A scale for the judgment of facial ex- pressions." Journal of Experimental Psychology. 1941, 55, 497-510. Schlosberg, H. "Three dimensions of emotion." The Psy- chological Review. 1954, 55(2), 81-88. Sloman, C. "Micromomentary facial expressions and the actor: an investigation.” Empirical Research in the Theatre. 1972, 5(1), 52-60. Stebbins, G. Delsarte System of Dramatic Exppession. New Yotk: Edgar S. Werner, 1886. Strongman, K. The Psychology of Emotion. London: John Wiley & Sons, 1973. Tomkins, S. Affect, Imagery, and Consciousness: V01 I, The Positive Affects. New York: Springer Pub- lishing Company, 1962. Tomkins, S. Affect, Imagegy, and Consciousness:Vol II, The Negative Affects. New York: Springer Pub- Iishing Company, 1963. Tomkins, S. "Affect as the primary motivational system." in Arnold, M. (Ed.), Feelings and Emotions: The Loycla Symposium. New York: Academic Press, 1970. Tomkins, S & Izard, C. (Eds.) Affect, Cognition, and Personality: Empirical Studies. New York: Sprin- ger Publishing Sompnay, 1965. 209 Turhan, M. 'Uber die deutung des gesichtsausdrucks." Psychologic Beitrage. 1960, 5. Wilmot, T. "The effects of sensitizing clients in therapy to the analysis of facial expressions." Disser- tation Abstracts International. 1973, 55(2-A), 574 Wilson, F.R. Micro-Momentary Facial Expressions and Psychological Defenses. Unpublished Ph.D. Disser- tation, Mighigan State University, 1976a. Wilson, F.R. "Repression and affect: a facial cue to de- fense operations." Paper presented at the 84th Annual Meeting of the American Psychological Assoc- iation, Washington, D.C., September 7, 1976b. Winer, B.J. Statistical Principles in Experimental Design. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1962. Woodworth, R.S. Experimental Psychology. New York: Holt, 1938. Young, P. Emotion in Man and Animal. New York: John Wiley & Sons, Inc., 1943. Zerba, M. Labeling and Recognitign of Izard's Facial Expressions by Three Age Groups. Unpublished Ph.D. Dissertation, Michigan State University, 1977. Zlatchin, C. "Accuracy and inaccuracy in the recognition of facial affects and the relationship to perso- nality." Dissertation Abstracts International. 1974, 55(12-8, Part I), 6228. Zuckerman, M., DeFrank, R., Hall, J & Rosenthal, R. "Encoding and decoding of spontaneous and posed facial expressions." Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1976, 5545), 966-977. "‘2222222222‘2272