WE EWEC‘JS {BF SELF-RENFORCEMENT GE” it’EEDECAL REEASEU'E'AEGN FATEM BEééA‘r’EORS Sisseri aiis ------ is; Segme of Ph. D. MECi-i 3:%ié S?" '{E UNIVERSITY ESE. " .e.-%:7<:3 3. Be VREES fl 1979 LIBRARY M‘cfzynm . mra ' L_._... ’ IINDIN. IV 1’ 2‘6””; This is to certify that the thesis entitled THE EFFECTS OF SELF-REINFORCEMENT ON MEDICAL REHABILITATION PATIENT BEHAVIORS presented by Edward J. De Vries has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for .___Eh‘JL_degree in _C_ans_eLi_nq_ hdaj fessor Date April 30, 1976 0-7639 Pu ABSTRACT THE EFFECTS OF SELF-REINFORCEMENT ON MEDICAL REHABILITATION PATIENT BEHAVIORS By Edward J. De Vries This study attempted to assess the effects of self-directed behavior change on medical rehabilitation in-patients' behaviors. The treatments of self-monitoring (SM) and Self-reinforcement (SR) were appled to three unique behaviors of each of three patients residing in a 72 bed medical rehabilitation treatment center in a midwestern city. The four hypotheses tested in this inten- sive case study (N=1) multiple baseline time series design were as follows: 1.) The implementation of a SM technique for se- lected patient behaviors will significantly change the occur- rence pattern of behaviors in the desired direction over a pre-treatment baseline period (B); 2.) The implementation of a SR technique for selected patient behaviors will change the occurrence pattern of behaviors in the desired direction over the B period; 3.) The implementation of a SR technique will significantly change the occurrence pattern of selected behaviors in the desired direction over a SM period; 4.) As a result of the SM and SR interventions the pattern of occurrence of selected behaviors during the post-treatment extinction (E) period will be improved over the B period. The results of the study showed no consistent support for any of the hypotheses. An analysis of the overall process however, indicated that those behaviors which obtained rein- forcement from the behavior itself showed improvement under Edward J. De Vries the SM treatment. Those behaviors which did not obtain re- inforcement from the behavior itself showed improvement in the SR phase. Further analysis showed that the meaningfulness of the behavior, the potency of the self-administered rein- forcers and the patient's attitude toward the research project itself seemed to influence the outcome of the study. Finally, a consideration of the study in terms of how it was presented to the patients, as a research project as opposed to a treat- ment program, seemed to provide some further support for the treatment effect under hypothesis 4. In that hypothesis the post-treatment (E) behavior occurrence pattern was to be sig- nificantly better than the pre-treatment baseline (B) condition. In that the researcher suggested that the patients no longer needed to monitor or reinforce the selected behavior during this E phase, a reversal of sorts was expected. The pattern of behavior changed dramatically after the SR period to a level similar to the B in many cases. This would seem to confirm the benefit of this type of program as much as a positive change at the outset of the program, the SM period, which in some cases did occur. The implications of this study would seem to indicate that the application of self-control strategies in a medical rehabilitation setting may be a valid consideration for the treatment oriented staff members. Care must be exercised in training patients and staff in the understanding and implement- ation of self-control stategies. The staff needs to be aware of the need to develop a supportive environment for patient Edward J. De Vries attempts at self-control, particularly for those patients who perceive they are dependent upon external reinforcement (external locus of control). The benefits seen for initiation of this type of treatment program include increased patient activity time outside of regularly scheduled therapy periods, possible reduction in length of stay, possible reduction in treatment cost. The benefits to the staff are seen as increased time for work with the more severely disabled, increased feed- back from the patient on off-therapy hour activity and an in- creased opportunity to assess progress in individual patients on specific behaviors. THE EFFECTS OF SELF-REINFORCEMENT ON MEDICAL REHABILITATION PATIENT BEHAVIORS By as) . Edward J. De Vries A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements fOr the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Counseling, Personnel Services, and Educational Psychology 1976 ACKNOWLEDGMENTS I wish to acknowledge the assistance and cooperation of many persons without whose help this learning experience would have been impossible. It was only because of their cooperation that a project of this nature could be planned, implemented, and described. I wish to first acknowledge Dr. Richard Johnson whose leadership and guidance in the planning and evaluation of this research was most crucial. Likewise I wish to express my appreciation to the members of my dissertation committee. I wish to acknowledge Dr. James Engelkes for his suggestions and interest in the study itself; Dr. Betty Giuliani for her time spent in developing the design and assistance with the analysis; to Dr. John Schneider for his personal support and assistance over the occasional hurdles and his invaluable aid in instrumentation; and finally to Dr. Bert Stofflemayr for his accurate perceptions of the rehabilitation process and for keeping the perspective of the meaningfulness of this type of study for rehabilitation programs. This study, of course, could never have been completed without the consent of Dr. George Fahlund, Executive Director of Mary Free Bed Hospital and Rehabilitation Center in Grand Rapids. Furthermore, I deeply appreciated the great deal of personal interest in the study shown by each of the department heads at Mary Free Bed who took many hours of their time in planning assistnace. Finally, a word of thanks is ii due to the many physical and occupational therapists, nursing staff personnel, and social service and psychology department persons who took time in their busy schedules to attend meetings, patient conferences, complete additional forms, and on occasion to live with seemingly omnipresent observers looking over their shoulders for the 10 weeks of the study. A word of thanks is most certainly due also the three patients, Sue Bob, and Doug. Without their cooperation and willingness to subject themselves to new experiences and the intense scrutiny of external observers over the 54 day period this study would not have been possible. Their unique person- alities and overall cooperative nature made believers of each of us involved in the study about the value of every individual to every other individual. In terms of the external observations required, I cannot thank Doug Frens and Lois Kuipers enough for their dedication and interest in the study. Their stability of performance and daily attendance to the study details made the entire experience possible over the 10 weeks. Conducting the experiment was one major effort, but describing the study was quite another. To assist in this detail I must thank Nancy Gendell whose sharp but poignant criticism of my writing style makes the transcript intelli- gible to the unknowing reader. I also owe a considerable amount of thanks to Renee Follett who in the cold winter months rose early many mornings to type the dissertation in the accurate style in which it is presented. And, a word iii of appreciation is due Dave Vidio for his persistant efforts on the behavioral graphs. Finally, but most importantly, I wish to express my most heartfelt thanks to my wife, Joan, whose psychological and emotional support of this entire project was essential to the attainment of this personal goal. The countless hours she spent working, and the sage advice she provided as an experienced nurse and a concerned wife, in order to help me reach this goal are never to be forgotten. Finally, a word of thanks is due my children, Amy and Gregory, whose unique personalities exhibited during the study and its analysis provided a welcome diversion from the scientific rigors of such an experience. iv Chapter TABLE OF CONTENTS The Problem . Introduction . . . . . . . . . . Stigma of the Disabled . Role of Rehabilitation . Purpose . Questions to be Addressed . The Need for Rehabilitation Research . Theory . . . Self- Directed Behavior . Self— Control Defined . . Principles of Self— Control . . . Self- Vs. Other- Controlled Behavior . Overview . . . . . . . . A Review of Literature Self- Reinforcement . Self— -Monitoring Behavior Modification and Rehabilitation : Design . Case Study Approach . . . Multiple Baseline Time Series . Analysis Instrumentation . . . Relation of Self— Control and Locus of Control Methods and Procedures Hypotheses Design . Sample . 64 65 68 Chapter III. IV. Methods and Procedures (cont.) Procedures . . . . . . . Pre-Study Sequence Patient Selection . Patient Conference . Elaboration of the Behaviors Chosen and Monitoring Techniques Self-Monitoring Technique . Self-Reinforcement Period . Additional Data Collected Analysis . . . . . . . . Statistical Analysis of the Data General Findings Hypothesis 1 Hypothesis 2 Hypothesis 3 Hypothesis 4 Comparison of Self— Monitoring and External Monitoring . . Summary of Statistical Findings Descriptive Analysis of the Study . Within-Behavior Analysis . Sue: Behavior 1 (Travel Time and Accident ) . . Sue: Behavior 2 (OT Activity) Sue: Behavior 3 (Range of Motion) Sue: Across- Behavior Assessment Bob: Behavior 1 (Resistance Exercises) Bob: Behavior 2 (Range of Motion). Bob: Behavior 3 (OT Inactivity Time) Bob: Across-Behavior Summary . Doug: Behavior 1 (Range of Motion Doug: Behavior 2 and 2a (Travel Time and Pushes) . Doug: Behavior 3 (OT Activity Time) Doug: Across- Behavior Summary Personal Data Analysis vi Page 73 73 74 75 80 86 88 91 92 95 98 98 101 103 107 110 113 113 113 115 117 118 119 120 121 124 124 125 127 129 129 Chapter Page VI. Summary, Discussion, and Implications . . . . 134 Summary of Results . . . . . . . . . . . . 136 Discussion . . . . . . . . . . . 137 Limitations of the Study . . . . . . . . 142 Implications for Future Studies . . . . . 145 In Retrospect . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 148 References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 150 Appendix A. Figures . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 159 B. Forms Used . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 170 C. Instruments . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 173 vii Table 4-1. 4-5. 4-6. 5-1. 5-2. LIST OF TABLES Page Statistical Probability of SM>B for each behavior using all B period data points . . 97 Statistical Probability of SR>B for each behavior using all B period data points . . 100 Statistical probability of SR>SM for each behavior . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 102 Statistical probability of E )8 for each behavior using all the data points for B and B periods . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 104 Summary of mean values obtained across phases 106 Correlation of Self-Observation and External Observation . . . . . . . . . . . 107 Comparative Scores of the 3 patients on each of 3 tests administered . . . . . . . 131 Average Staff Rating of Patient Activity Level . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 131 viii Figures 3-1. 3-2. A-3. A-4. A-S. A-6. A-7. A-9. A-lO. A-ll. LIST OF FIGURES Multiple baseline design applied to 3 behaviors for each patient . . Multiple baseline design as applied to the selected behaviors of the 3 patients Summary of Sue's Goals, Monitored Behaviors and Self-Reward Contract Summary of Bob's Goals, Monitored Behaviors and Self-Reward Contract . . . . . . Summary of Doug's Goals, Monitored Behaviors and Self-Reward Contract Travel time from nursing unit to OT (Sue-- Behavior 1) . . . . . . . . . . . . . Number of objects struck enroute to therapy (Sue--Behavior la) Number of minutes in OT activity in room (Sue-~Behavior 2) Number of range of motion exercises (Sue-- Behavior 3) . . . . . . . . . . . . . Number of resistance exercises (Bob-- Behav- ior 1) Number of range of motion exercises (Bob-- Behavior 2). . Percentage of time active in OT (Bob--Behav- ior 3) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Number of range of motion exercises (Doug-- Behavior 1). . . . . . . Travel time from CT to elevator (Doug-- Behavior 2) . . . . . . . . . . Number of pushes of wheelchair (Doug-- Behavior 2a) . . . . . . . . . Percentage of time active in OT (Doug-- Behavior 3) . . . . . ix Page 66 67 76 77 78 159 160 161 162 163 164 165 166 167 168 169 Chapter I The Problem Introduction Throughout the centuries of recorded human history, mankind has been seen as the dominant species on the earth. From the earliest times humans have sought to control their environment and each other. They have conquered nations, founded religions, discovered and rediscovered basic scientific principles, walked on the moon, and transplanted human organs. As long as they have written or printed, people have extolled themselves and each other for their feats of brilliance in science, religion, art, philosophy, medicine, sports, and politics. Names like Marian Anderson, Galileo, Einstein, Mozart, Babe Ruth, Mead, Edison, Hammarskjold, Indira Gandhi, St. Augustine, abound in history to record the superachievements of human beings. But almost as long as the chronicles of the great persons of history are the lengthy annals depicting the misfortune and inhumane treatment of those, who for a variety of reasons, lack the physical or mental capability for competing in certain aspects of their society. The countless myths and actual events written about the disabled human leave the average reader confused, mystified, and shocked. The stories of emotionally disturbed children locked in windowless rooms, of lepers banned from the city, of the retarded and emotionally disturbed crammed into bedlams, physically disabled children abandoned, of the deaf and blind being labelled retarded, I of epileptics being condemned as "possessed" by evil spirits, make healthy individuals squirm as they realize the extent of people's inhumanity to each other. In nearly all cases, these shunned persons are perceived by their society to be lacking a segment of "human-ness" and, therefore, they may, with impunity, be isolated or ignored. Stigma of the Disabled Although the number of overt acts of rejection or abandonment have been somewhat curtailed in the recent decades because of increased awareness and knowledge and, because of legislation, studies of attitudes about the disabled show that there is still a stigma attached to those who have an observable physical or mental deviation from the norm. Barker (1948) was an early promoter of the idea that the disabled, like the culturally outnumbered, are considered a minority and are, therefore, stereotyped in their role as are the other minorities. Beatrice Wright (1964), a leader in the field of equality for all, identified a variety of specific situations wherein the non-disabled view the disabled as completely lacking the ability to fit into any segment of society. English (1971) defined stigma as an attribute which dehumanizes the individual, main- taining that it is the way a so-called normal person views others who are different than himself. This seems to put the responsibility for stigmatizing on the "normal" population. This stigmatizing process often leads to the segregation of the disabled person. For example, special education classrooms are part of a nationally legislated procedure to provide adequate education to all persons regardless of physical .- 3 or mental ability. Sharland Trotter (1974) summarized a re- port written by Nicholas Hobbs. a psychologist at Vanderbilt University. His lengthy report discredited this process of educational labeling, indicating it often creates a gulf be- tween the able-bodied and disabled by building and perpetuating social barriers, depersonalized customs, and restricted opportunities. Another significant way the stigma is enforced is by a variety of civil laws passed "to protect society" from poten- tial interaction with the disabled. These laws include restrictions on the disabled with respect to driving, voting, marrying, or working. Revisions in these laws, along with major changes in building construction codes, mass transpor- tation policies and techniques, employment compensation incentives, and other socially controlling regulations, will help the disabled, but they may still be seen as "second class" citizens. As English indicated, "Stigma exists in the lives of most disabled persons, and generally it represents the most salient and frustrating problem to be overcome in rehabilitation" (1971, p. 2). Role of Rehabilitation In the midst of this clash between the production-minded, impersonal and health—oriented society and the personal ambi- tion of the physically disabled individual lies the medical rehabilitation program of our country. For those who have the capacity to relearn lost physical functions or who will benefit from learning new physical coping behaviors over an extended period of time, the medical rehabilitation treatment 4 concept is essential. The task of the medical rehabilitation staff is to assist the physically disabled to overcome their psychological and physical limitations by teaching them new coping behaviors as well as reteaching old adaptive behaviors (Fordyce, 1967). The goal of rehabilitation programs is to assist disabled persons to become productive and complete indi- viduals and thereby restore their sense of first class citizenship. From the patient's perspective, the entire rehabilitation process is often a long and arduous one. For most it is a physical struggle with injured nerve pathways, atrophied muscles, lost limbs, or malfunctioning sensory modalities, as they attempt to regain a sense of "self-dignity and worth" (Raninowitz and Mitsos, 1964). It is psychological combat between weakened bodies and a cultural value system that re- wards healthy nondisfigured bodies and unimpaired functioning. It is an intense relationship with trained professionals which attempts at all costs to re-establish the disabled individual into a familiar environment. It is, then, the "interwoven fabric of value judgments, mutual expectations, and effective bondsthat creates the design of the rehabilitation picture" (Rabinowitz and Mitsos, 1964, p. 3). This dissertation proceeds from the personal observations provided by Goldiamond (1973). Based on his determination that a medical rehabilitation treatment program can be best effected through the application of a self-controlled contin— gency management program, the idea for this study was germinated. Because of his observation that those individuals who were able to maintain a relatively normal life style even though 5 confined to a medical rehabilitation center were the patients who made the greatest gains in treatment, the earlier observa— tion regarding the need for most individuals to be productive and self-enhancing seems supported. Therefore, it seemed meaningful to develop a research study to empirically test the effects of a self-controlled behavior modification program on medical rehabilitation patient behaviors. The meaningful- ness of this study is seen primarily for the thousands of patients who daily are confined to sterile, rigid and often impersonal institutions wherein all characteristics of a previous enjoyable life-style are non-existent and where personal controls are unrewarded. Purpose It has been noted by Rabinowitz and Mitsos (1964) and Rothschild (1970) that the process of rehabilitation is often a psychologically demanding one. The rehabilitation patient must of necessity work through the psychological impact created by the limitation of the disability. This readjustment is further complicated by the fact that the patient is in a strange environment. .It has been mentioned that this environ- ment often includes authoritarian roles, physical confinement, and an estrangement from all normal social activities, and often becomes, in effect, a dependency producing system. In a most thought provoking study, Willems (1972), through an extensive review of patient activity levels, found that the patients' independent behavior activity was directly correlated with their freedom of activity. He found that behavior occurring inthe cafeteria and hallways was significantly more to." .O‘tub 0‘ 4 i. \ . ‘1‘ ~. . - J. u . 0“ " Do. ‘ Q. . ‘-i h ":9 ... 6 independent than those which occured within treatment areas of the rehabilitation center. He simply raised the question in his report about the goals of the treatment programming in the rehabilitation center. Ronco (1972), in discussing the theory of architectural planning for hospitals, considered the psychological impact the hospital setting has on patient behaviors, noted the effects of physical and psychological confinement along with the lack of privacy and familiar support systems. He remarked that although the lack of clocks, calenders, familiar furniture may be minor items, their collective absence is a major factor to consider when assessing patient behaviors. In effect, he said, the patient is seen as being under the "complete control of the system" with disruption of familiar behavior patterns and the lack of autonomy being the ultimate result. Rothschild (1970) also discussed the concept of the patient's dependency on medical professionals. She indicated that one of the basic assumptions in the role of the sick person is the willingness to follow essential medical direction. She does indicate, however, that once the patient's physical condition has stabilized, their participation becomes essential in planning their rehabilitation program. According to Rothschild, "It is always assumed that disabled people . are interested and willing to master their environment and go on struggling with life and meeting its requirements" (p. 76). Although this may be the ideal, it seems apparent from the the literature that patient's input is often neglected, personal ideas for independence conveniently displaced by administrative regulations. 7 Schontz (1975) took up this matter of patients' rights in the introduction to his book on the psychological aspects of illness and disability. He perceived the role of the patient to be one of an active participant, staff and patient working cooperatively. It was his belief that in the rehabilitation of an individual, the individual should be considered as an "indivisible whole". He felt that the patient should be the "peer of those who treat him, not their subordinate" (p. VI), and advised that patients not only have the right to make decisions relative to their own programs, but they also have a right to be heard and to defend themselves against decisions which are made by the institution in which they are kept. The intent of the study reported here was to attempt through the technique of self-controlled behavior, to assist patients to assume more responsibility for their behaviors while they are inpatients in a medical rehabilitation center. The need for self-control has been seen as an essential ingredient in the rehabilitation program for most physically disabled. The opportunity to practice the skill of behavioral control while an inpatient is viewed as a primary step to the later mastery of the social reintegration phase where the self-control will be a crucial element in successful readmission to traditional social roles. Teaching the patients that they can and do have control of certain contingencies and behaviors even in a seemingly dependency-producing environment may prove to be an invaluable resource to those who dislike the routines and impersonal nature of their present life style. The capacity 8 to control themselves and the reinforcements in their present environment may be the essential ingredient in their treat— ment program which keeps them from becoming "lackadaisical" as Goldiamond (1973) suggested. The value of this self-control concept is seen as having a practical side as well. Since patients are generally in treatment for 2 to 3 hours per day, much of their time in a treatment center is spent waiting for the next appointment time. If this waiting period could be changed from a time of inactivity to one of self-initiated and self-controlled thera— peutic activity the potential for increasing staff reinforce- ment as well as self-perceived progress seems greater. In addition, the potential for a shorter treatment program seems possible. Questions to be Addressed Among the possible questions which a study of this nature provides, the following are to be considered. They are stated here in a rather general way, but will be restated later in a testable manner. The concerns in this study relate to the patient's ability to take some personal responsibility for increasing their tolerance for physical activity, for managing some aspects of their rehabilitation activity. Since rehabilitation patients are usually seen as being acted upon rather than acting, being directed as opposed to being directive, being told vs. telling, the principles of self-managed behavior and the patient's rights seem to be in direct opposition to what actually is happening. But in the realization that patients engage in certain activities over which they do and should have control, 9 whether implied or expressed, there may be some opportunity to test the theory of self-directive behavior in a medical rehabilitation center. For this reason, then, the study concentrated on two primary questions: Does the active self-monitoring of a selected behavior cause a change in the occurrence pattern of that behavior? And, does the application of self-reinforcement of selected behaviors cause a change in the behaviors more than that noticed during either the self-monitoring or a no- treatment phase of that behavior? These questions were the focus of this study. The Need for Rehabilitation Research J. V. Basmajian (1975), a noted physician researcher at Emory Rehabilitation Research Institute, in a recent address to the graduating class of physical therapists at the University of Alabama, challenged that group of newly graduated professionals to become active researchers in their new appointments. He discussed the need for continued research in the existing therapeutic procedures, new drugs, and novel treatment ap- proaches, bioengineering for the physically disabled and exploratory work on neuromuscular function. His criticism of the present status of the medical rehabilitation field is that it is based on "intensive relationships" and not on the logic and support of a scientific principle. He maintained that "rehabilitation activities often respond slowly and obliquely to scientific progress because they have no base of science that is uniquely their own" (p. 607). 10 James McDaniel (1969) reflected a similar attitude re- garding the lack of scientific unity in the field of rehabili- tation medicine. In his work on the relationship between physical disability and human behavior, McDaniel commented on the "paucity of hard data" which efficiently relates the effects of a particular injury or disability to probable out- come behaviors for the individual. He claimed that although there is information available, it is usually scattered in the widely diverse areas corresponding to the services offered in a comprehensive treatment facility. In other words, re- search findings are available in speech pathology, physical and occupational therapy, medicine, vocational rehabilitation, and social work, but there is little interdisciplinary re- search which will benefit all at the same time. McDaniel believed that the behavioral science field, because of its commonality across these disciplines, should become the "prime interface" between the groups. Fordyce (1971) also noted the relative absence of an effective data base in rehabilitation services, particularly the lack of data concentrating on contingency management in rehabilitation. It was his feeling, as stated earlier, that rehabilitation is a behavioral science. Success or failure of the medical rehabilitation program can best be judged by observing the patient's behavior. Application, then, of proven learning theory principles to individual treatment programs should provide a necessary input into rehabilitation literature. 11 Friedlander (1974) discussed the need for further re- search in learning and conditioning in the field of rehabili- tation. He observed that there is little application of research in learning and conditioning to rehabilitation work with the disabled. It was his feeling that the reason for the lack of such applications is because both fields are fairly new and relatively little work has been done with the disabled. His requirements for research in the area include practicality and applicability for existing personnel as opposed to the need to add newly trained staff in order to provide service. In addition, it must be simple enough for those trained in other areas to use, it must deomonstrate cost effectiveness, and it must have relationship to the entire issue of accountability in the rehabilitation center. He attributed some of the resistance to utilization of research to rehabilitation per- sonnel who are afraid of being displaced by new research findings which perhaps assist the patients more than the in- tensive relationships characteristic of rehabilitation work. For that reason he advocated that research must be implemented through the staff in order to avoid further resistance. Perhaps part of the reason for the apparent lack of re- search in the field of medical rehabilitation is best explained by Reppucci and Saunders (1974). They described a variety of potential problems related to the introduction of behaviorally oriented social innovation programs into the natural environ- ment. Of particular importance to the idea of conducting research of a behavioral nature in a medical rehabilitation ‘A. 12 setting, are the problems associated with institutional con- straints, the limited resources of staff, time, and funds, the inflexibility necessitated by research designs, and the frequent scientifically stifling compromises between research principles and the daily operation of the facility. Considering these potential roadblocks to conducting applied research in the field setting, it becomes understandable how scientific progress might be slowed. Wright (1972), in listing the principles for rehabilitation psychology, offered a variety of ideological alternatives for the practitioner. She noted that basic research must be of a practical nature, that any research to be conducted with a rehabilitation population should be geared primarily to "problem resolutions". This issue of practicality provides a very significant input in a time of economic inflation. With the spiralling cost of living, there is a corresponding rise in health care costs. Unfortunately, private insurance packages, state-supported medicare and medi- caid benefits and personal finances are inadequate to meet these increased costs. A recent executive order by the Governor of the State of Michigan (1975), made in an attempt to control state finances, indicated that a variety of medical services previously paid for under the medical assistance pro- grams will no longer be covered. Such services as occupational therapy and speech therapy, dental services, hearing services, vision services, and restricted psychiatric inpatient services have been arbitrarily removed from the possible payment list. Such massive curtailment of funding for the medical rehabili- tation process not only creates tension in the employment 13 market, but must also force a reevaluation of the methods and types of services the health care professions provide. In this context, Wright's comments are most appropriate. New and innovative research must be conducted in such a manner that it leads to problem resolution both on a personal and programatic level. The need for research of this nature, therefore, seems rather clear. The cost effectiveness of any treatment pro- gram and the direct observable benefit to the individual patient for any specific treatment demands a research attitude. The lack of systematic application of established principles of behavioral management in a setting where specific behaviors are being constantly refined seems almost ludicrous. The need to provide a treatment system which enhances the time effec- tiveness of the professional, which increases the productivity of the patient time, and which promotes increased rehabilita- tion behavior provides the rationale why this study was done. Theory The principles of learning theory are extremely broad and a great deal has been written about them. Of particular importance to this study, however, are selected principles from learning theory in particular operant behavior and con- tingency management. In addition, there is a recent advance in the field of behavior science called self-controlled { behavior modification. This concept is set in opposition to the more traditional behavior modification approaches which depend on "other-controlled" or externally controlled 14 programs of behavior management. The theory of self-directed or self-controlled behavior is the basic concept to be con- sidered as background for the study. It is essential that we include a brief overview of the entire concept of learning theory before proceeding with an elaboration of the above tOpic areas, however. Learning theory is one perception of personality development which con- centrates on specific behavioral developments, as opposed to the nontangible constructs, theoretical constructs, or other nonobservable effects, as being responsible for personality development. Simply described, learning theory claims that a personality is developed because of a series of reinforcing or punishing consequences of behavior. The reinforced be- haviors have an increasing probability of recurrence, while those behaviors which result in aversive consequences of punishment decrease in the probability of recurring. Through several periods of reinforcement or punishment of this type, the person's predictable behavioral pattern is formed. Through this process, the individual develops sequences of behavior which promote individual growth. In addition, antecedent cues or situations occuring just prior to a reinforced or punished behavior become related totfluaprediction of the recurrence of the behavior. The ability to recognize cues and to under- stand consequences of behavior forms a basis upon which be- havioral control is predicted. Within this broad concept of learning theory are countless principles, techniques, and definitions. .d- n. u 1.! 34A 15 One of these important principles is that of Operant behavior. Skinner (1953) defined operant behavior as "behavior that has an effect on the surrounding world" (p. 59). In other words, operant behavior is a physical response to a stimulus or cue in the environment which causes a measurable effect on that environment. Skinner noted that persons are constantly interacting with the environment. Each activity which the person finds rewarded by the environment will increase in the probability of its recurrence; those which meet with some sort of punishment will, through conditioning, decrease in their probability of recurrence. In the field of rehabilitation, operant behaviors are foremost in importance. Learning the use of new prostheses, relearning the use of injured limbs, and reestablishing nerve pathways by repetitive physical activity are all examples of operant behavior. Those activities which are reinforced by accomplishment soon become part of the behavioral repertoire of the patient. Those activities that result in pain, embar- rasment or failure generally are not individually pursued by the patient because they are punishing in their consequences. The goal of rehabilitation therapies might be restated in operant terms as follows: Rehabilitation is the reshaping of patient behaviors which will ultimately lead to a successful effect upon the environment where successful effect means that the patient approaches the premorbid acceptable level of such a behavior. .4. 16 Contingency management is also an important concept in this particular study. Contingency management is related to the concept of operant behaviors. Contingency management is the control of the various stimuli or reinforcements which affects the probability of recurrence of a selected behavior. By controlling the situation or the specific rewards which ususally serve to produce or maintain the behavior, the be- havior itself can be controlled.' In the rehabilitation setting the prospects for contingency management are varied. Specific patient behaviors occur only in selected areas of the hospital. As Willems (1972) pointed out, patients engaged in a certain level of behavior in their rooms but evidenced considerably different levels of that behavior in therapy. Patients generally never sleep outside of their own room and may never exercise themselves when in their own room. In another situa- tion, the patient may try feeding himself in the therapy area, but may demand to be fed when in his own room. Hence we see that the environment may serve as a cue for specific behaviors. We also find reinforcement or punishment for certain behaviors being provided by the staff, family, or other patients. Patients who are talked to by therapists while performing therapy activities may feel reinforced. Staff may criticize patients for other behaviors, the criticism serving as a punishment and deterrent to further behaviors of that nature. Patients may also receive a great deal of intrinsic pleasure by performing certain behaviors, such as l7 learning to feed themselves again or moving previously paralyzed muscles. They may feel considerable embarrasment over reshaping bladder and bowel functions which before were quite standard. The variety of contingencies which can be expected to be brought to bear on a patient in a rehabilitation center are varied. The successful management of these contingencies is, therefore, important to the efficient rehabilitation of the patient. Contingency management is an extremely important variable to the successful implementation of any Operant behavior modification program. Self-Directed Behavior Within the behavior modification field an increasing amount of consideration has recently been given to the pos- sibility of a self-directed program of behavior change. The alternate terms self—directed, self-controlled, self-regulated, self-managed behavior change all refer essentially to the same concept, 'UD the principle that man has the capacity to control his own destiny. The variety of approaches to the principles of self-control will be discussed later. Now, however, the concepts of self-controlled behavior and the essential ingredients contained therein must be identified. Self-Control Defined Skinner (1953), in one of the original writings on the topic of behavioral self-control, envisioned it as "the reinforcement of those behaviors which make punished behavior less probable" (pp. 3-30). Skinner called this process the "self-determination of conduct". When the punishing effects of certain behavior are reduced, the behavior doing the ll: ‘0 ‘M 18 reducing is rewarded. When that behavior is rewarded, its probability increases and the individual then is seen as controlling himself. Skinner listed a variety of techniques wherein self-control is obtained. These include acts like the use of drugs, punishment, operant conditioning, distrac- tion, aversive stimulation. He advocated the use of these techniques by the individual just as they would be applied in any externally controlled behavior modification program. Perhaps the most complete review of self-controlled behavior theory and application to date is that of Mahoney and Thoreson (1974). They agreed with the basic principle that behavior is controlled either by antecedents and/or cues to behavior. If a person wishes to change a given behavior, he must recognize both the typical consequences of the behavior as well as the cues for that behavior. They indicate that research has verified that for self-control to be effective, at least one of three elements must be present--self—observation, environmental planning, or behavioral programming. The concept of self-observation allows a variety of technical alternatives including the use of graphs and charts or any other systematic record of behavior. The use of any form of immediate feedback device by which the individual can judge progress is recommended. Self-observation is simply the routine, accurate reporting of the activity level of a behavior that a person does for himself. Environmental planning, according to Mahoney and Thoreson, involves "changing the environment so that either the cues that precede the be- havior or the consequences that follow it are changed" (p. 23). 19 This may necessitate avoiding situations which elicit the be- havior or avoiding those situations in which a choice is allowable. The final recommended procedure is behavioral programming. This last recommendation involves altering the consequences of a behavior in such a way that the frequency of that behavior is changed. A variety of techniques can be used, including positive self-reinforcement, self-punishment, or any internal or external consequence which will change the frequency of the behavior. Goldfried and Merbaum (1973) perceived self-control to be a personal decision-making process. They saw self-control as a result of conscious deliberation of the options for behavior and the selection of those actions which accommodate the goals the person has chosen for himself. They indicated that the areas of human behavior which can be self-controlled include maladaptive physiological or instrumental behaviors. The physiological behaviors, they explained, are best controlled by self-administered aversive conditioning, auto-suggestion, or relaxation. They recommended that the instrumental or more overt behaviors be controlled by covert conditioning or self- reinforcement techniques. It was their observation that the literature on self-control seemingly concentrates on weight control and smoking habits, although they noted a few reports of marital problem solving and study habit remediation with the self-control concepts. In a somewhat more theoretical evaluation of the topic of self-control, Klausner (1965) provided considerable input. He considered self-control to be the freedom from external 20 social dictates or the internalizing of authoritarian dictates. According to Klausner, self-control is the ability to control the environment and its impact. He indicated, however, that self-control is not so much total control of self but rather the control of selected aspects of the self. In a review of 290 articles on the topic, Klausner catagorized self-control programs into control of overt performance, physical and psychological drive control, intellectual or cognitive thought control, and control of affect. He also devised the recom- mended methods of controlling techniques between direct and indirect control. Indirect control is the control of segments of a larger complex of behavior until the entire complex can be controlled directly at the outset. He conceptualized the controlling methods in four distinct ways. Efforts of synergy are the skills of controlling the environment and hence the individual himself. Efforts of conquest are the skills necessary for the facilitation or inhibition of drives. Efforts of harmony are those skills needed for controlling the be- haviors to correspond with physical constitution and personality. Finally, a fourth level of controlling behavior is that type of self-control skill which is used to overcome threats to one's self while acting in a desired manner to achieve certain goals. This level Klausner labelled the effort to transcend. Cautella (1969) described self-control in a manner simi- lar to that of Skinner. He saw self-control as a response repertiore in which "the individual can make responses to increase or decrease a response probability that is perceived as injurious to the individual himself or to others" (p. 324). 21 He endorsed the concept of self-control because he felt the individual is more apt to maintain a level of new behavior if he is responsible for that change. Cautella described the method of self-control within an operant framework, which utilizes reinforcement and punishment, or reciprocal inhibition which utilizes the processes of relaxation, thought stopping, or covert sensitization. Goldiamond (1965) described self-control as a functional relationship between man and his immediate environment. The environment he generally refers to provides the reinforcement for selective behaviors. These reinforcers he also called "critical consequences" for behavior (1974). By analyzing and controlling the environmental reinforcers for a given behavior, the person is able to control his own behavior. Hence a basic understanding of man's relation to his environment is important. Kanfer and Phillips (1969) considered the self-control process from a therapeutic standpoint and called it instigation therapy. They defined instigation therapy as a technique by which the individual utilizes specific suggestions and assigned tasks in the patient's usual daily environment, formulated on the basis of the patient's verbal report of the symtomatic behaviors, their con- text and their consequences . . .Self—reinforce- ment by the patient and the natural occurence of reinforcements contingent upon adequate patient behaviors in his usual environment are used to achieve self-control. (p. 474) They added that self-control is a most effective method of behavior change since it is accomplished in the stimulus situa- tions in which the behavior change is desired. The ultimate 22 success of the self-control program rests basically on the patient's self-reports and adequate reinforcement. We will discuss the concept of self—reinforcement in more detail later. Martsen and Feldman (1972) saw the principle of self- control both as a thought oriented process wherein the indi- vidual has his mind set on controlling some behavior and as a specific behavior oriented self-control where the individual performs some function to control his behavior. Essential to their concept is a need for clear definition of the behavior to be controlled as well as an understanding of the person's attitude, insight, consciousness level. The personal awareness (consciousness) of the need to control an attitude which per- suades or reinforces the commitment to change and the insight to realize how the change will affect the total person is essential to the self-control strategy. Rimm and Masters (1974) defined the process of self- control as replacement of nonproductive behaviors with pro- ductive behavior. As others have indicated, the importance of the personal awareness of stimulus situations in the environ- ment which affect the probability of the desired behavior and the response consequences to the desired behavior are emphasized. These authors also felt that alternative or competing behaviors are essential as are the use of principles of approximation (or shaping) to the desired behavior, chaining of behaviors, and the control of the early parts of the chain, and the importance of self-monitoring. Finally, Watson and Tharpe (1972) in an instruction book for behavioral control define self-controlled behavior as the 23 process of "directing the relationships between your environ- ment and your behavior" (p. 23). They, like all the others, emphasized the importance of understanding the relationship of behavior to antecedent cues and consequences. They also advocated the use of self-monitoring, self-reward, and punish- ment programs in whatever manner facilitates the occurence of the appropriate behavior. Principles of Self-Control From the theories noted above, and for this study, it appears that self-controlled behavior is, first of all, self; suggested. Although none of the authors cited specify how the behavior to be changed is to be identified, most authors im- plicitly agree with the idea of self-prescription (Bandura, Grusec, and Menlove, 1967; Bandura and Perloff, 1967). Once the behavior has been self-suggested, it must be clearly identified. At this point, a process of self-monitoring must begin with an awareness of situational cues which precipitate the behaviors and reinforcingficonsequences which, in turn, increase the probability of the recurrence of similar behaviors. The primary aspect of the self-controlled behavior is the ability to manipulate the environment to increase the proba- bility of the behavior and/or managgment of the consequences of the behavior to facilitate its continuation. Of those latter concepts, some writers have added further observations which are essential. Self— Vs. Other-Controlled Reinforcement As a strong prOponent of the self-control ideal, Mahoney (1971) has researched the concepts of self-imposed techniques 24 of reinforcement. His work will be considered in more detail later. Essential to his theory, however, is his observation of the value of "self-regulation" versus "other-regulation" of consequences. He noted that self—imposed regulation is more effective than externally imposed regulations. He found that there seems to be little difference between the effectiveness of apprOpriate uses of positive or negative reinforcement, but indicated that there is little supportive research on the application of self-punishment as a means of behavior control. Bandura (1967b), another leader in the field of self- controlled behavion indicated that self-regulation of behavior demands an initial explanation of the acceptable levels of the target behavior. Once these standards have been adopted, the person must then "self-administer rewards and punishments depending upon whether their performance falls short of, matches, or exceeds their self-prescribed demands" (p. 449). Bandura also found that those persons who desire to regulate their own behavior generally tend to maintain relatively high stan- dards for themselves. He noted that the most important attri- bute of such a self-rewarding contract is its capacity "to maintain effortful behavior over time" (1967a, p. 112). Kanfer and Duerfeldt (1967) compared the varying effects of self-control as opposed to other controlled reinforcement. They found, in a three phase experiment, that the process of self-reinforcement has longer lasting effects, that self- reinforced subjects tended to establish performance standards at levels of previously externally reinforced standards, and that they maintained a higher performance level when compared to the behavioral levels of externally reinforced persons. 25 Kanfer (1971) in a later study described the process of self-control and reinforcement. Speaking fauna forecaster's viewpoint, he envisioned that the rapid rate of change in life settings demands that man be aware of the intense personal interactions he has with his environment. With the possi- bility of such change, man must be consistently able to adapt. He felt that this consistency will only be deve10ped through "self-generated motivations and standards and means for maintaining" (p. 404) that consistency which.in turn, means defining contracts in therapy which utilizes the principles of internal reinforcement. To further elaborate on reinforcement, Kanfer and Philips (1969) indicated such reinforcement can consist of self- evaluations of the person, therapist-related reinforcements or peOple, objects or reinforcing events in the daily life of the individual. It is imperative, however, that the rein- forcement be under the direct control of the person himself and that it be administered only "when reinforcement is appro- priate to his own behavior" (Kanfer, Bradley and Martson, 1962). Kazdin (1973) added further thoughts to support the con- cept of self-directed reinforcement system. In his writing he attempted to explain why some externally imposed token economy reinforcement programs may fail. Among five explana- tions for a breakdown in effectiveness, Kazdin indicated that the behavior being rewarded may not be possible for the indi- vidual, that there may be a lack of understanding about the relationship between adequate performance and receipt of the reward, or that there may be a delay in receiving the reward. 26 As a potential solution Kazdin recommended including the patient in the development and administration of the contin— gency which is, in effect, the self-control principle which has been discussed. In summary, the theoretical principle upon which this study was based is personally controlled behavior change. The particular skills of self-delineation of behavior change, self—established performance standards for the behavior, the utilization of self-monitoring techniques, and the application of a self—devised and controlled reinforcement program were emphasized. This study was applied to a small segment of a population which has traditionally not been conceived of as being self-directed, although the professional's expectation is that these people should become self-directed as a result of their treatment experience. The rehabilitation patient may be a prime candidate for the application of the principle of self-directed behavior change. Originally self-directed, this individual became instantaneously other-controlled and powerless as the result of a trauma or illness. Yet, through retraining, he or she may become maximally self-controlling in a dependency reinforcing situation. Overview The following chapter provides a review of the literature with respect to the application of behavior modification prin- ciples in the field of rehabilitation and the utilization of self-control principles with the rehabilitation patient. The hypotheses will be formulated and the specific procedures for 27 conducting the study are presented in chapter three. A des- cription of the study results is presented in chapters four and five. The final chapter draws together the concepts as discussed and observed in the study. Based on the study, recom- mendations for the application of the data to other situations will be presented. Chapter II A Review of Literature Behavior modification is without a doubt one of the most controversial of the theories of human behavior change, and it is probably the most widely applied. The growth of knowl- edge about behavior modification and the research in the field during the last two decades has created as much excitement and ambivalence in the area of psychology as did Freud's first pr0posals about the unconscious. The use of behavior modifi- cation is praised by many when it works, but has also led to governmental controls when it created pain and nearly animal- like experimental conditions (Trotter and Warren, 1974). The utilization of behavior modification techniques in controlling institutional populations has provided a large volume of literature. The application of concepts such as operant conditioning, covert conditioning, token economies, systematic desensitization, aversive conditioning, and punishment have become standard treatment for behavioral disorders and standard procedures for behavior change pro- grams in mental health institutions, prisons, schools, and workshOps. Couch and Allen (1973)supported the idea of applying behavior modification principles in rehabilitation settings. They indicate, "Studies promoting the utilization of behavior modification in rehabilitation settings offered dramatic proof of its efficacy" (p. 88), and they note that many researchers see it as THE system of patient control. As was cited in the first chapter, a new advance in the field of behavior modification is the process of self-controlled 28 s"" .n: .‘ It], 29 behavior. This technique of modifying selected behaviors demands a self-realization of the cues which surround the behavior and the reinforcement contingencies which affect the behavior. In addition, we noted that self-controlled behavior necessitates the self-establishment of an acceptable standard for the behavior, self-monitoring, and a self- controlled reward system. Reviewed here will be that body of literature which evaluates these specific areas of self- controlled behavior in terms of its proven effects on behavior. In addition, the applicability of behavior modification pro- grams and self-controlled programs as they have been applied to rehabilitation patients will be reviewed. Self-Reinforcement A major component within the self-control concept is the management of reinforcers that will promote or change the de- sired behavior. Bandura, one of the leaders in the field, has published, in conjunction with others, several articles on the topic (Bandura, Grusec, and Menlove, 1967; Bandura and Perloff, 1967; Bandura and Whalen, 1966). In these articles, the authors were generally concerned with the combined effects of role modeling on the self-establishment of reward standards and the effects of self-reinforcement on the individual. In a study of 80 children, Bandura and Perloff reported that the reinforced subjectszusacombined group performed better at a given task than did non-reinforced group. They noted no significant difference in the performance of the externally or self-reinforced children. They did note however, that the self-reinforced group seemed to establish higher levels of "'1 0%": - .hu «1. 'v x AA '11. 30 performance than was necessary in terms of minimal standards for reward. Bandura, Grusec, and Menlove discovered that when children (whether these were the same or different children is not clear) were exposed to role models, they tended to establish a standard for their own behavior in terms of the modelled standards even if the schedule was very demanding and lower performances possible to obtain the same reward. The common observation in each research team was the negative personal effect that the potential for failure had on the children as well as the undue expectations they often held for their own performance. Based on this research, it might be worthwhile, then, in any self-reinforcement program to be alert to the tendency of persons to be overly demanding in establishing behavior standards for the amount of reward obtained. Hall (1972), in a doctoral dissertation, followed the lead of Bandura by testing the difference in productivity resulting from different reinforcement alternative? and per- sonality characteristics. In his study of 60 subjects, he attempted to analyze the differences between reinforcment types, whether external or self, and the subject's perceived locus of control. He found, as did the earlier authors, that there were no differences in task production between the self-reinforced and externally reinforced groups. He also found that the reinforced groups both performed at a higher level than a no-reinforcement control group. He further noted that the generalized expectancy of reinforcement (locus of control) had no effect on task performance, thereby 31 indicating that the personality trait of perception of the locus of control of reinforcement produced no difference between groups. Kanfer and Duerfeldt (1967), in a three-phase research design utilizing three groups, also tested the effects of self-controlled versus other directed reinforcement. Three groups of subjects were shown geometric designs and asked to select correct designs based on a pregiven criteria. Two of the three groups were initially reinforced for responses without any relationship to correctness; the third group was not reinforced for any response. The second phase of the study continued the non-contingent external reward system for one group, the second group was asked to reward themselves if they felt they were correct, while the control group con- tinued on a no-reinforcement program. The final phase of this study was an extinction phase for all three groups. The results showed a significant difference in the final phase behavior with the self-reinforcement group correctly choosing more designs than the other groups. The evaluation of this study supports the hypothesis that "training in self- administered reinforcement enhances performance under extinction" (p. 244). In a much different research design, Lovitt and Curtiss (1969) reported the results of a comparative study on rein- forcement techniques. In a single case study design of a twelve-year-old student, the researchers found that self- administered rewards by the student for correct behavior prompted a higher performance for a specific classroom behavior 32 than either a teacher-dominated point system or other teacher~ dictated reward. In the repeated measure experimental study, these findings were reproduced, thereby confirming the hypo- thesis that self—directed reinforcement program does produce more of a desired change in a specific behavior than does an externally devised system. In another single subject study, Nurnberger and Zimmerman (1970) applied the concept of self-controlled behavior to study habits. They found, through the use of self-administered punishment, that a graduate student significantly changed his behavior with respect to work on his dissertation. The stu- dent increased his study behavior when he required the re- searchers to spend the student's money on social programs with which the student was in basic disagreement, if his behavior was below a self-established standard. It is also surmised by this writer, although not described in the article itself, that the reinforcement gained by the student for pro- ductive behavior undoubtedly served to increase his motivation to continue such goal directed behaviors. Bolstad and Johnson (1972) provided yet another observa- tion on the application of self-controlled versus other- controlled reinforcement in school age children's behavior. In a well designed study, the researchers used a four-group design in five classrooms in each of two separate locations and done over five time periods to test their hypotheses. The treatments were controlled across time. In addition, controls were imposed upon the students for reliability of self-monitoring reports. The authors found, generally, that 33 by reinforcing appropriate in-class behaviors they decreased disruptive in-class behaviors. They noted no significant differences between the self-reward and external reward groups in terms of behavior change. They found further that the self-rewarded group on occasion tended to reward themselves more liberally than did the externally reinforced group, an observation also made by Kanfer and Duerfeldt at the con- clusion of their study (1967). It was noted in the previous chapter that the utilization of behavioral self-control is widely used in weight control programs. This area of behavioral control strategy seems to utilize both the principles of stimulus control and self- reinforcement. The overall results of studies in this area seem to correspond to the previously described observations. Mahoney (1973), for example, found that teaching self-control of eating habits in weight reduction diet, rather than just controlling the quantity of intake, provided a more effective way of controlling weight. He also noted that this self- reward program increased the motivation for weight control over a longer period of time than the more traditional weight control programs which emphasize limited intake. In another comparative study, Mahoney, Maura, and Wade (1973) found that there was a significant difference in weight loss between groups using self-reward, self—punishment, or a combination of the two to control eating. They found that the self-reward group lost the most weight over a period of time. The self reward - self-punishment group was less effective in weight loss than the first group but more effective 34 than the self-punishment only group. This would seem to indicate that the utilization of self-reinforcement has more applicability than self-punishment, however, there may be some consideration given to applying both principles in a self-control study. Another study which combined the use of principles of stimulus control and self-directed reinforcement was reported by Penick, Filion, Fox, and Stunkard (1971). Through the use of an elaborate program of stimulus control and prompt rein- forcement, the researchers sought to increase those behaviors that would promote weight loss. As hypothesized, the group using the self-reinforcement principles lost more weight than the control group which was participating in a traditional group therapy and information exchange format. There was a wide variation in the weight loss of the self-controlled group, however, which negated the statistical significance necessary for proving the hypothesis. Despite the lack of significance, the authors reported that the results were con- sidered superior in the self-controlled group. Based on the data presented in the above studies, there are some general conclusions which can be drawn. Without any doubt, there is reason to believe that the use of reinforce- ment, whether externally controlled or self-directed, enhances the likelihood of a target behavior change. Although the literature is not conclusive on the differences between the externally controlled and self-directed reinforcement programs, there are some subtle points to be made. It seems apparent that the use of self-directed reinforcement causes individuals 35 to set higher goals for themselves in terms of an acceptable standard of behavior. Once established, this behavior pattern seems to exist for a longer period of time than does the be- havior pattern of an externally reinforced group. These considerations are of utmost importance when considering the behaviors of the patients in a medical rehabilitation setting. If the patients can be helped to produce a selected behavior more often and over a longer period of time, a substantial gain might be noted in their rehabilitation program. Since a major portion of the patients'time is spent in situations that are not immediately reinforcing, the teaching of the principles of self-reinforcement in those situations would perhaps help increase the likelihood of those behaviors necessary for a successful and efficient rehabilitation. The possibility of strengthening rehabilitation behavior pat- terns in usually non-rewarding settings is a major reason for completing a study of this nature. Self-Monitoring Self-controlled behavior requires the self-management of reinforcers and it also requires self-monitoring. This concept of self-recording and its overall effect on behavior change has been addressed by a few authors. Walls (1969), for example, stated in his discussion of the application of behavior modification to rehabilitation that the most expeditious plan in observing behaviors is "to have the client count and record his own behavior". He went on to note that "in many cases no reinforcement other 36 than auto-recording may be necessary to accelerate desired behavior" (p. 174). Mahoney and Thoreson (1974) supported this same contention when they indicated that self-monitoring may be more than an assessment tool, it could also be a treat— ment strategy. They called this possibility the reactive com- ponent of self—monitoring and suggested that a baseline of self- monitoring is essential to understanding other specific treatment effects. The self-monitoring becomes a potential confounding effect for the experimental treatment. It must, therefore, be dealt with in this study as a secondary hypothesis. Jeffrey (1974), in discussing the concept of self-mon- itoring, observed that the effects of self-monitoring must be ascertained in any research study. He recommended a procedure whereby a multiple time series or reversal de- sign with self-monitoring as part of the treatment strategy should be considered. Kazdin (1974), in an extensive review of the literature wherein self-monitoring was part of an experimental approach, found studies where the self-monitoring had an effect on the behavior and studies where it apparently had no effect. It was his conclusion that self-monitoring has not been consistently influential in behavior change. He also addres- sed the issue of the reactivity of self~observation. It was his belief that whether or not the reactivity issue was or was not a problem depended on how the self-gained information was to be used. He noted that in many cases, the effects of self-monitoring seem to lose their impact on behavior change over time anyway and, therefore, need not be addressed in a study on self-control. 37 In addition to the importance of self-reinforcement and self—monitoring, it was noted in the previous chapter that a program of self-reward necessitates the self-establishment of a particular standard of behavior. This issue of personal involvement seems to relate closely to the concept of patients' rights as it is applied in a medical setting (Quinn and Somers, 1974). Among the many patients' rights listed in their article, these authors included the following: the right to parti- cipate in decisions about treatment, the right to discuss treatment program alternatives, and the right to have staff hear and understand their complaints and ideas about program changes. These principles seem to be intimately related to the entire process of self-standard setting for selected behaviors. The self-establishment mechanism and result of allowing patient input into the system is described by several authors. Saper (1974) described a group therapy program in a psychi— atric setting. In this descriptive study, he told how the patient's long- and short-term goals were developed with the patient. A major component of this program was the regular contact the patient had with all members of the treatment staff. Of particular note in the program was the inclusion of the patient's immediate family in both structured and informal social situations with patients and staffs. Although no conclusions were reached, the idea of patient- staff communication was advised. 38 Skipper, Tagiacozzo, and Mauksch (1964), reporting on observations made of patient and staff communication processes, noted an interesting potential "anti" argument on the issue of patients'rights. In the era of rising medical malpractice suits, they found that the more that the hospital staff wanted to protect themselves from errors and mistakes, the less oriented they became to communicating with patients. At the same time, they noted the "pro" argument which supports the issue of patient involvement: the amount of communication that existed between patient and staff was directly propor- tional to the level of patient cooperation and to the clear perception of patient needs by the staff. In summary, Skipper et a1. maintain that the interpersonal communication process is essential for effective patient care and treatment planning. In a somewhat similar manner, Armacost, Turner, Martin, and Holt (1974), in another descriptive study, described a technique used with so-called "problem patients" in a chronic disease section of a California V.A. hospital. When frustrated by a number of patients who refused to follow treatment pro- grams, hospital regulations, and staff orders, the nursing staff received consultation from a psychiatric nurse. The prominent recommendation was the apparent need to increase the staff-patient interaction through regularly scheduled meetings. This resulted in decreased hostility, less resis- tance, and fewer antisocial behaviors on the part of patients and in an overall "improvement in patient behaviors and nurses' attitudes" (p. 292)- 39 In an attempt to encourage patient involvement in goal setting, Becker, Abrams, and Onder (1974), at the University of Michigan, applied the principle of patient and family par- ticipation in treatment goal setting in a rehabilitation center. She found, again in a descriptive study, that when the patient and family were included in the admission conference, there seemed to be an enhancement of patient-family communica- tion, better patient-staff understanding of treatment goals, and a reduction in the amount of sabotage of treatment plans by the patient. From these reports, it seems clear that including patients in goal setting and treatment planning produces some positive benefits. There appear to be greater amounts of patient cooperation and reduced patient-staff conflict. In addition, if the patient is included as a primary participant in goal setting, there is a greater likelihood of the patient's cooper- ation in reaching the goals. Furthermore, the more the patients are involved in setting the standards for their behaviors, the less likely they will be to complain about the care, and the more motivation they will have to reach the objectives. It has been noted so far that the principle of reinforce- ment of appropriate behaviors does influence the probability of the recurrance of the behavior. It has also been seen that the application of self-reinforcement is at least as effective, if not more so, than externally applied reinforcement. Evidence has been cited demonstrating that including patients in goal setting and treatment planning seems to promote a 40 better patient attitude and behavior patterns and increased patient-staff interaction. Let us now extend the review of the research to determine whether behavior modification techniques when applied to medically disabled patients has any effect on their behavior patterns. Behavior Modification and Rehabilitation It has been mentioned earlier that the field of medical rehabilitation is closely allied to the area of behavioral science. Couch and Allen (1973) indicated support for the growth of the use of behavior modification techniques in re- habilitation facilities. They indicated the "apparent simplicity and built-in accountability" of behavior modi- fication provides a sound rationale for the use of the tech- nique. Staats (1964) maintained that the application of behavior modification principles is badly needed in the field of rehabilitation, especially since new behaviors are being learned in those situations where reinforcement is generally weak. Let us consider then, the variety of behavioral studies employed in the area of medical rehabilitation. There are countless studies of behavior modification with the re- tarded and emotionally disturbed. However, since this study is geared to the applicability of behavior modification in medical rehabilitation settings, the review will concentrate only on that portion of the disabled population. Reinforcement programs have been applied to a variety of behaviors in the medical rehabilitation setting. In the area of improving the physical performance of the patient, several reports are worth noting. Hollis (1974) reported on a series 41 of studies employing the operant conditioning approach in training patients in the use of artificially powered pros- thetic devices. He described particularly a variety of posi- tive reinforcement techniques applied in the retraining of selected hand muscles. These reinforcement techniques included the observation of colored liquid movement in plastic tubes and electronic signal feedback for the use of appropriate muscle movement in arms and hands. As opposed to teaching a patient to "move a finger or muscle", the therapist instead taught the patient to control the movement of the colored liquid and the feedback of the electronic impulse signals. With the immediate observable reinforcement obtained through external sources, the patient almost unknowingly strengthened hand muscles more easily and with considerable less frustration. In attempting to decrease the frequency of skin breakdown in the spinal cord injured,wheelchair-bound patient, a major problem facing such individuals, Malament, Dunn, and Davis (1975) reported an effective behavioral method. Using a time— buzzer-pressure switch combination, the patients were taught through negative reinforcement to raise themselves periodi- cally in their wheelchairs. By repositioning themselves within a selected time span, the pressure sensitive switch was automatically reset and a buzzer system deactivated. In the three-phase experiment, which used a small number of subjects, the authors found that the patients became effective in timing their repositioning so as not to overact in a given period, but to prevent the 15 second buzzer from sounding. 42 Through the pictorial representation of the data of the five patients, this scheme was seen as effective in promoting body repositioning. No statistical analysis was computed, however, to verify the apparent success of the system. Fordyce, Fowler, and DeLateur (1968) attempted to alter the reinforcement received for low back pain patients. They found that demanding more activity of the patient, encouraging the patient to make daily graphs of activity levels, and self- monitoring caused the patient chronically disabled by low back pain to realize a significant improvement in overall activity levels. They also noted a decrease in the subjective evalua- tion of pain. Friedlander (1974) described in some detail the various technological materials produced for the benefit of patient rehabilitation. His technological system utilizes the scien- tific process of "specifying tasks, providing reinforcement, and keeping detailed accounts" (p. 144). Through the use of separately mounted and experimentally controlled T.V. switches on opposite sides of his bed, a severely immobilized and retarded young patient learned to control to a certain extent his small environment and to "avoid a characteristic re- gression . . . as (had) happened before" (p. 145) when the patient had similar surgery. In another experiment, designed to motivate a severely physically disabled child to grasp objects, the author was able to train a relatively absent grasping function to produce 600 responses in 15 minutes. The self-obtained reward earned by the patient for the grasping reflex was the playing of the Happy Birthday song. 43 Another activity Friedlander was able to promote included the movement of the heel and toe of a young adolescent who was wearing a full body cast for a broken leg. By the use of a timer switch activated by his heel and toe, the patient could listen to a favorite radio station. This activity was not only reinforcing to the patient from the standpoint of the music, but was also physically beneficial to the patient in terms of continued movement of the affected leg muscles. Another study by Friedlander reported on assisting severely involved patients in the relearning of activities of daily living such as pulling drawers, opening doors, etc., all of which were rewarded by the lighting of electric lights when done well. Along similar lines in the treatment phase for rehabili- tation patients, others have also succeeded in increasing appr0priate behaviors when applying behavioral principles. Goodkin (1966) used the behavioral analysis idea to define specific rehabilitation related behaviors. The application of rewarding appropriate behavioral management programs, including self-evaluation of speech patterns in two separate aphasic patients, seemed to expedite the total rehabilitation program. In another case with a positive reinforcement of approach behaviors, a cerebral palsied child was helped to overcome the fear of falling; this case is described by Meyerson, Kerr, and Michael (1967). Booraem and Seacat (1972), in a study of three patients, found that the use of a financial incentive for a selected physical therapy activity resulted 44 in an improvement of more than one standard deviation in the behavior over a baseline period. Unfortunately this study did not appear to control for several possible confounding variables. Along with the apparent success of the application of reinforcement for patients in treatment, other work has been done to promote patient attendance in therapy through the use of behavior modification techniques, also with some success. Ince (1969) reported on the application of the reinforcement principle to stimulate patients to attend therapy. By encour- aging patients to make their attendance in the apparently enjoyable speech therapy activities contingent or dependent upon their participation in other important therapeutic activities which they often missed, the staff of a rehabili- tation center was able to increase patients' attendance in the less frequently attended programs. To increase the de- sired behavior in the less desired therapy, the staff was instructed to call the speech therapist when the patient had attended the required physical or occupational therapy hours. Without this verbal report the patient was not admitted to the speech therapy. Along the line of therapy attendance, Zschokke, Freeberg, and Errickson (1975) found that by posting a chart and subtly recognizing patient attendance in occupa- tional therapy they increased the patient attendance in this therapy setting. They found further that by requiring a patient to attend a certain amount of time in order to attend subsequently a special social activity held each week, the patient attendance increased even more. 45 All of these studies seem to support the recommendations of Sieg (1974) and Wanderer (1974) that occupational thera- pists should adopt the behavioral model in their treatment programs. They felt that by recording rates, keeping time- motion studies, and establishing a measurable standard of behavior along with defining specific behaviors which meet the clients' needs, therapists fit precisely into the be— havioral model. Wanderer adds, further, that patients can be taught to bring their responses under control "by training them to control the functional antecedents of those responses" (p. 207). This quotatflnicogently reflects the philosophy of self—directed behavior and is the view taken in this study. Finally, in an extensive review of the literature of the application of behavior modification principles in rehabili- tation settings, Walls (1969) provided considerable support for the application of behavioral principles to rehabilitation. In his all-encompasing survey, Walls found that application to individuals, application to groups, and applications in institutions had positive results. The behaviors which he found could be changed included speech disorders, prevocational behavior, disruptive classroom behaviors, verbal and social skills, and self-destructive behaviors. It should be pointed out that nearly all the studies described fall within the framework of an externally operated and manipulated behavior management style. In most cases, the behaviors were defined by others although the reward was on occassion controlled by the patient. In few cases is there mention made of the patient's involvement in the 46 standard setting or in the determination or administration of the rewards, Meyerson, Michael, Mowrer, Osgood, and Staats (1960) allude to this as a typical problem in rehabilitation. It was their feeling that the intrinsically motivated will show a decided behavior change over the extrinsically reinforced. They stated that "the problem of rehabilitation often consists of a disabled person who . . . should be doing something other or something more than he is doing" and they go on to ask the inevitable question, "How can a person be stimulated and paced so that he does what he 'should' and 'wants' to do it?" (p. 73). This solution of allowing the patient the opportunity to get involved in establishing standards of behavior, establishing rewards for that behavior, and then initiating a program to change the behavior may provide the answer. It, therefore, forms the basis of the hypothesis of this study. Design It has been shown in the last several sections that the theory of self-directed behavior seems to be supported. The theory has been applied in a wide variety of locations with apparent success. It has also been shown that the applica- tion of behavior modification principles has been success- fully employed in rehabilitation settings to promote desired behavior change. One author, Goldiamond (1973) has subjec- tively described his personal attempt to apply self-control behavior principles within a medical rehabilitation setting with some apparent success. There has been little scientific evidence however, to validate his claim or to push the 47 frontier of self-control into the medical rehabilitation facility. Therefore, in an attempt to validate Goldiamond's observation and to also attempt to understand the dynamic effects of self-monitoring and self-reinforcement on behavior change in a medical rehabilitation setting, the intensive case study or an N of 1 research design has been chosen. Case Study Approach The supporting rationale for the case study approach to research is provided by several authors (Chassen, 1967; Dinsmoor, 1973; Gottshalk, 1968; Holzman, 1963; Kiesler, 1971; Lazarus and Davison, 1971; Leitenberg, 1974; May, 1971; Shapiro, 1966; Thoreson, 1972). Each of these authors noted among many other reasons that research of this type emphasizes the careful monitoring of each individual with respect to the target behaviors. Since this is a necessary element in behavioral area research, the case study approach seemed very reasonable. Another rationale for choosing the case study approach is the diversity of the population in a medical rehabilitation center. Because one patient's medical condition is dif- ferent from another's, since each treatment program is varied with the severity of the disability, it is practically im- possible to establish a true comparative group study. Establishing comparative groups on some selected demographic variable would necessitate the use of several medical centers or time lapse study, both of which promote confounding possi- bilities. The use of a single subject study permits the subject to be his/her own control and the various hypotheses 48 can be tested in a within-subject manner (Chassen and Bellack, 1966). If more than one research subject is used, a comparison across the subjects is also a potential evaluation consideration. A final consideration given to the choice of the inten- sive case study approach is based upon its effectiveness in defining treatment effects on the individual. As Chassen, Thoreson, Lazarus and Davison, and Shapiro indicated, the problem with the comparative group design is that a statis- tically significant effect may, in fact, reflect a true effect in very few individuals but the influence in those is strong enough to elevate the entire group norm. These authors noted, further, that the effect of treatment on the individual per- sons involved is 1ost to the researcher. Davison summarized all of the above considerations, giving four additional supporting reasons for such a design: 1. It (Single Case Study Design) reduces . . . the 'error variance' by eliminating the usual con- founding between variations in behavior associated with different values of the independent varia- ble and the variations between individuals. 2. This design may be used with considerable effectiveness . . . in clinical practice . . . in which each individual represents a unique case. 3. The within-subject design makes it possible to study differences between individuals in the characteristic way. 4. The functions which are apparent when the individual design is employed may be lost or distorted when average values are substituted as required by the group design. (p. 508) In assessing the variety of research designs to use for this experience one must consider the hypotheses to be tested. Since the credibility of the research reviewed with respect to self-controlled behavior seems to be influenced somewhat by the research on self-monitoring there seems to be a need first 49 of all to distinguish between self-monitored behavior patterns and behaviors being self-rewarded. Since the intensive case study will be used which negates the potential to place persons into self-monitoring only and self-reinforcement only groups in true experimental fashion, the most logical and most defensible research design to use is the Multiple Baseline Time Series Design as recommended for intensive case studies by Thoreson (1972). Multiple Baseline Time Series Design The benefits of the multiple baseline time series design must now be considered. The typical single baseline time series design employs a periodic measuring process of a behavior with the introduction of an experimental change into the series of measurements at a preselected point. This quasi-experimental approach, however, provides no control over the potentially confounding effect of an unrelated event, which occurred at the same time as the experimental treatment, causing the ob- served behavior change. Alternative actions to control for this possible confounding effect would be the establishment of a certain amount of isolation of the experimental subjects or the use of a reversal design. The reversal design allows the subject to return to a previous type of behavior after a treatment. It is difficult to manage and justify, especially when behavior change is the goal of the treatment. Reversal in this case is highly illogical and anti-human. The best alternative to control for the potential effects of internal confounding factors is the use of a multiple base— line time series design. Thoreson describes the use of such a 50 design as most applicable when an experimenter is "concerned with (l) the effects of a treatment on several behaviors of the person, (2) the effects of multiple treatments administered by different persons simultaneously or (3) the effects of a treatment on the same behavior in different settings" (p. 28). Wolf and Risley (1971) added the observation that this design can also be useful when carried out (1) across two or more different responses under the same environmental condition(s) and on the same subject(s), or (2) across two or more environ- mental conditions with the same response(s) and on the same subject(s), or (3) across two or more subjects with same response(s) and under the same environmental condition(s). Hence, there are a variety of application possibilities for a design of this nature. The value of the multiple baseline is that it also controls for the potential confounding effect of history by introducing the experimental treatment at several selected points in time. If the experimental treatment pro- motes the desired change at each interval, the treatment will be viewed as effective. Gottman, McFall, and Barnett (1972) advocated the utiliza- tion of a multiple baseline study, observing that such a design is most beneficial in those situations where selected variables cannot be controlled and/or where a control group concept is impossible. They indicated the multiple baseline design pro- vides the analytical advantages of a single group pre-post type of design where the researcher can observe the overall treatment effects and permits a post-hoc analysis of results over a time period. Finally, it provides for a control of 51 the possible confounding effects by removing the effects of history with multiple starting points. Finally, Jeffrey (1974) described the essentials of im— plementing the multiple baseline research design. He indicated that to successfully implement a multiple baseline design, three independent behaviors should be observed. Treat~ ments are then applied to each behavior until a change is observed. If the target behaviors change when the treatment is introduced, a cause-effect relationship can be inferred. The design of this experiment, then, is geared to observing the effects of a treatment on specific individuals and specific behaviors. By initiating the experiment with different indi- viduals and at different times while maintaining a concise data base over time, the mandate of Thoreson (1972) can be met. He postulated that research in counseling must get back to "the basics" meaning there is a need for "direct observation, careful description, and systematic planned interventions with individual subjects" (p. 4). The intensive case study applying the multiple baseline research design provides the structure for fulfilling such a statement. Analysis As was previously noted, one of the benefits of conducting an intensive case study is to assess the specific effects of a treatment program on the individual subjects. This analysis can be both statistical and nonstatistical in nature. One needs simply to observe the graphs of the data obtained and draw some general conclusions about the treatment effects. This 52 approach generally coincides with the detailed post-hoc analysis of the data to further understand the treatment effect, however. In a multiple baseline design involving three independent behaviors and repeated over a few subjects, all time-lagged with respect to their initiation into treat- ment, one can analyze the effects of treatment within each subject and comparisons can also be made across subjects. The analysis of the intensive case study has been per- ceived differently by several authors (Chassen, 1967; Holtzman, 1963; Namboodiri, 1972; Shine and Bower, 1971). These authors prefer to treat the data gathered in a static manner using the t or F statistical tests. The difficulty imposed by such an analysis is the potential violation of the assump- tions underlying the statistical tests. Most prominent among these assumptions is the question of whether the daily obser- vations are statistically independent. Varying techniques have been devised to circumvent the problem however, but with questionable validity. Another proposed way of assessing the effects of treat- ment on behavior is a dynamic analysis. Instead of concen- trating on the analysis of the data as it is grouped through summations, and statistically comparing the values of the grouped data, the dynamic analysis of data keeps each of the data points separate. The analysis is done based on the general distribution of the data points. This method, described by White (1971, 1972) is built from a relatively simple com- putation of the median $10pe of the distribution of points for any period of the research with a comparison of the slopes for each phase. 53 This computation of trend lines improves greatly over the least squares regression line in its predictability. White (1971, 1972), in a study of 166 previously completed research studies of classroom behaviors, found that the median slope method proved to be the most effective predictor of future performance over either the more typical regression model, or a "Quickie Methodfl a. shorter method of calculating the median slope or a corrected median slope method. The median slope of the data is that trend line which divides the data points in half. This method is not then greatly influenced by the extreme deviation of one or two grossly deviant data points. What the median 510pe line becomes then,is»that line above or below which 50% of any given individual's data points can be expected to fall. By projecting this slope into the future, the researcher can test whether the number of data points actually falling above or below the projected trend line is significantly different than expected. The regression line, on the other hand, tends to be more affected by major deviations of scores. Because of this influence, the slope of the line may be significantly changed and may not provide accurate prediction for future data. With the application of a dynamic analysis of an N of 1 study the advantages seem important. Although a static analysis of some data may show no significant differences between two or more sets of data, the dynamic analysis may show that the data, in fact, was proceeding in dramatically different directions. It might also show, for example, that the observed 54 differences between two sets of data was predicted, in fact, based on the trend analysis of the data. Hence, for the pur- pose of using all the data collected in this study, the median slope technique will be employed. Instrumentation In an intensive case study on self-control, considerable observational data will naturally be collected. Rice and Glenn (1973) found in assessing a large number of patient personalities in a rehabilitation center that the per- sonalities generally fell into three categories on the 16 Personal Factors Test. Some they considered "Normal Adjusted", who simply needed to develop interpersonal skills. A second group they found to be "Assertive Aggressive" with the recom- mendation that this group needed to control appropriate and inappropriate behaviors. The third group were considered "Passive Aggressive", with a tendency toward higher anxiety, apprehension, and tenseness. This group needed to learn to express themselves more Openly and thereby control undue anxiety. The Edwards Personal Preference Schedule (1953) is a 225-item forced choice questionnaire. It was designed for research in counseling purposes to provide a quick and con- venient measure of 15 personality variables. One population on which this test was standardized was a college sample composed of 749 college women and 760 college males with an age range from 15 to 59. A second norming sample was comprised of male and female adults (8,963 in total) selected from a nationwide consumer purchasing panel. Means and stan- dard deviations were computed for each of the 15 subscales. 55 The internal consistency scores derived by a split-half calcu- tion ranged from .60 (Deference) to .87 (Heterosexuality). The stability scores range from .74 (Achievement and Exhibi- tion) to .87 (Order). Finally, an intercorrelation of the 15 subscales was conducted with the highest correlation at .46 and the second highest at .36 thereby showing that the sub- scales are seemingly quite independent. Validity studies have been conducted showing a relationship between EPPS scales and other personality measures. Self-ratings on the 15 subscales have compared highly with actual EPPS scores. It thus appears that the test itself has sufficient reliability and validity to be useful in understanding personality characteristics in general. For this study, selected subscales were picked which seemed to have the closest relationship to the concepts of rehabilitation. For this reason the subscales of Achievement, Deference, Autonomy, Succorance, Endurance and Aggression, a total of 130 questions will be used. In this study, the indi- vidual scores obtained have little meaning when compared to national norms, but have considerable meaning in terms of internal comparisons and the information they supply with re- spect to the individual patient personality and behavior patterns. The Self Evaluation Questionnaire, a state-trait anxiety inventory (STAI, Spelberger, 1970) is a self-report measure of two separate anxiety constructs: state anxiety (A-State) and trait anxiety (A-Trait). This inventory of 40 brief statements asks the reader to rate on a 4 point scale how they 56 feel "at this moment" (A-State) and on a 4 point scale how "they generally feel" (A-Trait). The value of administering this test will be to assess the personality of the patients involved, not in terms of a national norm but in order to provide data on which speculation can be made relative to the patients behavior patterns in the study. The STAI has been normed on 484 college undergraduates, 982 college freshmen, and 377 high school students. In addition, normative data has been calculated for male neuro- psychiatric patients, general medical and surgical male patients, and young male prispners. The stability of the A-Trait subtest was shown to range from .84 to .73 over 104 days for the college undergraduate males and from .76 to .77 for the corresponding female group. The A-State subscale are substantially lower, as expected, ranging from .33 to .54 and .16 to .31 respectively. Internal consistency reliability measures show a range from .83 to .92 for both subscales thereby showing a good overall internally reliable test. The concurrent validity of the STAI was done by cor- relating the obtained scores with three other personality tests. It showed a range of correlation between .41 and .85 over the three tests and separate comparison groups (college females, college males, and neurOpsychiatric patients). Construct validity was addressed in separate experiments in which the A—State was expected to change, which it did with an alpha reliability coefficient ranging from .83 to .93. From this data, it would appear that the STAI will be an accurate and sensitive instrument to test the anxiety level S7 of the involved patients throughout the study. Once again, the results will be meaningful when compared within the study and will not be meaningful when compared to a larger population. Another personality theory which on the surface seems to bear some relationship to the study derives from Rotter's Social Learning Theory (1954). In this theory, Rotter described the occurance of the behavior as dependent upon the expectancy that the behavior will lead to a particular rein- forcement in that situation and the value of that reinforcement to the person. He arrived at the concepts of internal and external locus of control. These concepts he defined as follows: When a reinforcement is perceived by the subject as following some action of his own, but not being entirely contingent upon his action, then in our culture, it is typically perceived as the result of luck, chance, fate, as under the control of powerful others or as unpredictable because of the great complexity of the forces surrounding him. When the event is interpreted in this way by an individual we have labelled this a belief in external control. If the person perceives that the event is contingent upon his own behavior or his own relatively permanent characteristics, we have termed this a belief in internal control. (Rotter, 1966, p. l) Some authors have attempted to apply this concept of locus of control to the disabled population. McDonald and Hall (1971) tried to correlate the effects a variety of hypothetical disa- bilities would have on those persons previously categorized on a locus of control scale. They asked 479 non disabled (emphasis mine) college students to complete an Attitude Toward Disability survey. They also asked the students to rate their perception of the debilitating effects of the various disa- bilities on a hypothesized male head of family, as well as cul int beh 58 on his surrounding society. The outcomes showed that ex- ternally classified students rated the disabilities as more personally debilitating than did their internally classified peers. The results showed that the externals also rated the effects of the disability on others as greater than did the internals. It must be emphasized that this is a hypothetical situation and that the students with no identifiable limi- tations took this test and projected themselves into the situation. It is uncertain whether disabled students would perceive the effects of their disability in a similar manner because such a study has not been found. Nevertheless, one might wonder whether the locus of control of the disabled does influence their behavior. In a study of behavior patterns of hospitalized tuber- culosis patients, Seeman and Evans (1962) found some additional interesting information regarding the difference in hospital behavior patterns between the internal and external control groups. The internal control group tended to ask more ques- tions and showed an overall better knowledge of their condition than did the externally controlled group of patients. They also were noted to progress faster in their treatment. This result seems to correspond with that found by Ireland (1973) in a doctoral dissertation. He found that 25 male subjects who were given three separate tests of the I-E variety showed a significant correlation between their involvement in treatment and their locus of control scores once again with the internally controlled showing a greater awareness and knowledge about the situation. S9 Wendland (1972) attempted to investigate the correlations between institutionalization and the chronicity of disability with locus of control scores. In a study of 80 white males, 18 to 35 years of age with an I.Q. of more than 90 and all of whom had muscular and/or skeletal impairments, it was hypothesized that those individuals who had spent less than 1/3 of the days since they were disabled in a medical insti- tution would have a higher external control score. The second hypothesis studied was that those individuals who were disabled for less than l-l/Z years would have higher external control scores than those disabled longer than 1-1/2 years. The results of the study show that there was no correlation between the amount of time spent in the hospital and locus of control scores. However, a significant result at the .07 level occured when relating the onset of disability with the locus of control. The overall results from this study would seem to indicate that immediately post-disabled peOple tend to rely on the external means of reinforcement, but that over time the individual tends to become more internally controlled. One of the other observations that was noted in this study was that those individuals who were disabled by injury showed a higher external score than those who were disabled from birth. Results also showed that external locus of control persons tended to be younger, have more schooling, were instittutionalized less, and rated the disability more severe and the future less severe than did the internally controlled. 60 In a survey on the literature of locus of control and reaction to illness, Ripstra (unpublished manuscript, 1974) summarized the observations of several authors. She noted that internal locus of control patients seem to cope better with their illness than the external control group. It was her observation that in instances of long term illness or disability the locus of control model could be used as a rehabilitation variable. Relation of Self-Control and Locus of Control At this time it is important that the differences and relationship between the concepts of self-controlled behavior and locus of control be explained. It has been noted that self-controlled behavior in this study demands a self-awareness of the need for behavior change, a self-monitoring of the behavior, and a self-controlled reinforcement system. It has been noted that the locus of control concept is related to the issue of reinforcement as well. Locus of control refers to the person's perception as to whether they actually can control their own reinforcers. The subsequent life style bears out this perception. Hence, in a study on self-control, persons may be likely to claim they are capable of controlling their behavior, such as controlling angry outbursts, overeating, or exercising, but, in fact, may live a life style which solicits reinforcement from others for the opposite behavior. On the other hand, persons may indicate they are unable to control their behavior while at the same time giving themselves suitable reinforcements for the behavior. Finally, the self-control and locus of control 61 may be in total agreement when the individual admits to not having control of his behavior or of the reinforcers. The theoretical link, then, between self-control and locus of control is an intriguing one. To further assess the recent work in this field, few studies are available. The two most related studies are presented here. Jeffrey (1974) addressed the issue of self-control and locus of control. He noted that the process of self-control demands a change in the individual's perspective with re- gards to the responsibility for behavior. It was his ob- servation, based on the survey work of Lefcourt (1966) and Rotter (1966), that the internally controlled individual will be more responsive to environmental cues for behavior, will be active in improving his own situation, will be more achievement oriented, and will be resistant to external interfering forces. Bellack (1975), in a study of self-evaluation, self- reinforcement, and locus of control found some interesting relationships. He found that the internally controlled indi- viduals as a group had higher self-evaluations and utilized more positive self—reinforcement and less negative reinforce- ment than did the externally controlled individuals. He wondered whether the external control individuals might have a personality characteristic related to self-regulation which the internal group does not have. Since the locus of control concept seemed to have a definite relationship to the concepts of this study, a locus of control assessment of the patients in the study seemed 62 important. The major question was whether the patient's locus of control affects his use of the self-control concept. To assess the patient's locus of control, 3 Rehabilitation Attitude Survey (Appendix C-l) was previously developed for use in this study. This questionnaire was rationally developed using items similar to that which appeared on Rotter's original I-E scales, but modified to reflect a person's attitude in a medical rehabilitation center. In addition, new items were developed which were also directly related to the patient's perspective of their treatment responsibilities. The scale was developed to measure both the trait and state locus of control scores. Several months prior to the onset of this study, the questionnaire was given to 22 adult patients. At the same time they were given the 29-item of the Opinion Survey developed by Rotter (1966). A correlation of the results of the instruments was completed showing an overall correlation of .53 (p< .02). It is felt that the Rehabilitation Attitude Survey reflects somewhat accurately the internal-external locus of control score of the individual particularly as it pertains to the medical rehabilitation center and in that regard provides additional data upon which to analyze the study outcome. Another instrument used in the study was a Behavioral Rating Scale (Appendix C-2) of patient activity. This 35-item instrument was modified from a Job Behavior Scale developed by Fairweather (1964). The wording was changed to reflect the kinds of patient behaviors commonly found in a medical reha- bilitation center. The instrument is a forced choice checklist 63 of typical patient activity levels and offers an "always" or "never" option for the staff members to describe patient activity. The scored responses were coded in the positive direction. One staff member most knowledgeable about the patient from each of the groups, Occupational Therapy, Physical Therapy, Nursing, and Social Service-Psychology, were asked to complete the Behavior Rating for the three participants in the study. To provide a comparative score, sixteen other inpatients at the time of the study were also rated by the staff. The results were then averaged for each patient and a rank order was established for the patients. Chapter III Methods and Procedures The earlier chapters have noted the need for further research in the area of medical rehabilitation. It has also been suggested that the link between the behavioral science field and rehabilitation should be carefully addressed. The theory and practice of self-controlled behavior modification have been reviewed and the overall positive effect on selected behaviors has been noted. It then seems reasonable to study whether the introduction of a self-controlled behavior modi- fication program into a medical rehabilitation setting has a positive effect on selected patient behaviors. In this chapter the design and implementation procedures for such a self-directed reinforcement program in a medical rehabilitation center will be considered. The hypotheses of the study will be considered first. In subsequent sections the sample, the experimental procedures and, finally, the method of analysis used in assessing the effects of treatment will be discussed. 64 65 Hypotheses The purpose of this study was to test whether the estab- lishment of a self-directed reinforcement program had any effect on selected medical rehabilitation patient behaviors. As noted earlier, a self-reinforcement program necessitates the use of some self-monitoring technique, but self-monitoring itself may have a reactive effect on the behavior being observed. Hence, it was essential to separate out of this study the potential confounding effects of self-monitoring from self- reinforcement effects. For this reason, the design of the experiment was such that the following directional hypotheses could be tested: Hypothesis 1: The implementation of a self-monitoring (SM) technique for selected patient behaviors will significantly change the occurrence pattern of the behavior in the desired direction over a pre-treatment baseline period. Hypothesis 2: The implementation of a self-reinforcement (SR) program (Which includes self-monitoring (SM)) will significantly change the occurrence pattern of behaviors in the desired direction over a pre- treatment baseline (B) period. Hypothesis 3: The implementation of a SR program will signi- ficantly change the occurrence pattern of selected behaviors in the desired direction over a SM only period. Hypothesis 4: As a result of the SM and SR intervention, the occurrence pattern of selected behaviors during the post—treatment extinction period (B) will be significantly different in the desired direc- tion over the pre-treatment baseline period. 66 Design The design chosen for this study was a multiple baseline time-series design as applied to a single subject. The reader is referred to Chapter 2 for the rationale for this choice. To review, however, the N=1 approach was chosen in order to more carefully assess the specific effects of the treatment on the individual behaviors. The multiple baseline time series was chosen in that it adequately controls for potential confounding variables. Figure 3-1 shows the format of the experimental study as it was applied with each subject. In this figure, B refers to a no-treatment baseline, SM refers to Self-Monitoring, SR--Self-Reinforcement, and E refers to a post treatment extinction phase. Figure 3-1 Multiple baseline design applied to 3 behaviors for each patient Calendar Days 9 18 27 36 45 54 Behavior 1 SM SR E E E Behavior 2 B SM SR E E Behavior 3 B B SM SR E Figure 3-2 shows the actual research time line as it occurred over the three patients. Patient B is shown to begin the study on the 17th calendar day while Patient C began the study on day 25 of the study. The total time used for the study was 79 calendar days. One should readily see from Figure 3-2 that there is a multiple baseline effect both within one patient's experiment study as well as across 67 Figure 3-2 Multiple Baseline Design as applied to the selected behaviors of 3 patients Patient Behaviors Calendar Days 18 27, 36 45 54 1518 B SM SR E E E Sue 2 B B SM SR E E 3 B B B SM SR E Calendar Days 17 26 35 44 53 62 71 1 ~) B SM SR E E E Bob 2 e B B SM SR E E 3 e B B B SM SR E Calendar Days 25 34 431 52 61 7o 79 1 ) SM SR E E Doug 262a ; B SM SR E 3 ) B B SM SR 68 the three patients, adequately controlling for any potential confounding due to external factors. Sample Since this study intended to test the effect of a self- controlled reinforcement program on medical rehabilitation inpatients, the population from which the sample was taken was in a medical rehabilitation center. Further, because the study concentrated on the individual effects of an experimental treatment on selected behaviors of an N of 1 design, only a small segment of the medical rehabilitation inpatient pepula- tion was used. The population from which three persons were selected for this study was the adult and young adult inpatient pepula- tion at Mary Free Bed Rehabilitation Center in Grand Rapids, Michigan. The facility is a comprehensive medical rehabili- tation center offering total rehabilitation services to the entire Western Michigan area. The types of rehabilitation services offered include medical and nursing care, occupational, physical, and speech therapies, psychological and social services. Mary Free Bed is a fully accredited 78-bed medical center open to all ages and disability types. Children, which comprise about 25-30 percent of the total population, are admitted with developmental disabilities such as cerebral palsy or brain damage, severe asthma, congenital and traumatic amputations. The facility serves young adults with similar diagnoses or other long-term medically complicated cases, and it admits adults who have a variety of disabilities resulting from 69 closed head injuries, spinal cord injuries, strokes, arthritis, amputations arising from injury or illness,among other diagnoses. The overall length of stay in this center averages about 43 days, with adults staying slightly longer than the population in general. For the purpose of this study, three patients were selected from these two older groups. This number was chosen in order to provide ample opportunity to test the effect of the princi- ple of self-reinforcement on inpatient behavior and to formulate the basis for future work in the area. Since this study was designed to test the application of the theory in a different setting than what had been previously used, the number chosen was small. This intense study permitted a greater in-depth analysis of what transpired rather than to prove that self-control makes a significant change in all medical rehabilitation patients. The three persons selected for the study were chosen because they met certain standards. These standards included the need in the patient's treatment program for both occupa- tional and physical therapy and a minimum anticipated stay of 60 calender days. In addition, the selected patients were required to have little or no mental impairment which would affect their comprehension of the anticipated activities in the study. The ultimate acceptance of the patient into the study rested with the researcher and was based on the medical reports, a subjective assessment of the patient's mental status, as well as on the patient's overall physical functioning level. 70 Hence, variables such as age, sex, disability type, length of previous limitations, work status, and educational levels were not considered specifically in the selection of the patients. The three patients selected for the study are described as follows: Patient A: Sue Demographic: Disability 8 Limitations: Time Since Onset of Disability: Nature of Condition: 40-year-old female, married, mother of two teenage sons, registered nurse, socially active Hemiplegia and Heminopsia due to Cardio- vascular accident and/or right temporal lobe hematoma; partial paralysis of left side, vision loss on left side, weakness of right hand, secondary to pre—existing condition Approximately 9 weeks Confined to wheelchair with functional use of right side only. Wore a short leg brace on left leg and used a mobile arm support on wheelchair for mobility of left arm and shoulder and prevention of drooping shoulder. 71 Patient A: Sue (cont.) Patient's Goals for Treatment: Learn to walk, or at least stand with support, to cope with restricted visual field, to strengthen dominant hand and use affected arm for support activity, to function as mother and wife under restricted limitations. Patient B: Bob Demographic: 27-year-old male, divorced, father of two children, used car salesman, previously physically strong and socially active person living with mother. Disability 6 Limitations: Spinal cord injury at CS-6 (Cervical ver- tebrae #5, 6) causing partial paralysis of hands, fingers, and total paralysis from upper chest down, but with some sensory retention. Time Since Onset of Disability Approximately 7 weeks Nature of Condition: Confined to wheelchair, totally dependent for transer, bowel, bladder, hygiene, eating, and mobility at onset of study. 72 Patient B: Bob (cont.) Patient's Goals for Treatment: Patient C: Doug Demographic: Disability 8 Limitation Time Since Onset of Disability: Nature of Condition: Wore a neck movement restricting Halo Brace until day 42 of the study to assist in healing of spinal fusion. To walk, and use of hands, to regain independence. ll-year-old male, foster child living with grandparents in small rural community, previously enjoyed scientific and athletic activity. Extreme total body muscle weakness due to Guillam-Barre Syndrome, a deteriorating, but reversible condition affecting all muscle groups, following a severe appendicitis. Approximately 11 weeks Confined to wheelchair, nearly totally dependent for assistance in feeding, toileting, transfers. Doug was very weak and frail looking, had supposedly lost 73 Patient C: Doug (cont.) about 30-35 pounds since his illness and moved about very slowly in wheelchair. Began to stand in a tilt table 30 minutes/ day just as study was started. Patient's Goals for Treatment: To walk, run, use arms and hands, and go home. Procedures Pre-Study Sequence Prior to the onset of the study, having obtained the con- sent of the Executive Director of the Center, the physical therapy (PT) and occupational therapy (OT) departments, nursing units, psychology and social service departments were told of the study in general terms. Their respective roles in the study were described as they related to the planned patient selection and patient conferences. Subsequently, a one-week pilot study was introduced with one patient to refine specific systems, procedures, and forms. In addition to staff training, paid observers for the study were given an overview of the study in terms of the research design and hypotheses being tested. Specific considerations for observer behaviors were described as outlined in Appendix B-l. Each of these observers parti- cipated in at least one joint observation(with the researcher) of the chosen behaviors of each patient prior to the beginning of any data collection on the patient. 74 The typical admission procedure to the center remained in effect throughout the study. This procedure included admission office assignment to nursing unit and room. Within one or two days, the patient's medical record was reviewed by each department with assignment to appropriate therapists determined either by who the physician was, the nursing unit the patient was on, or the primary limitations of the patient. Subsequent to this determination, the physical therapist and occupational therapist scheduled the patient into a treatment routine usually consisting of two 30-minute sessions per day, or more, depending on the patient's physical tolerance. The other staff members scheduled their less frequent appointments as fit best with the patient's daily schedule. Patient Selection As new patients were admitted to the center, or as existing inpatients made sufficient progress, the head of the physical therapy department informed the researcher of prospec- tive candidates for the study. The researcher then met with the occupational therapist (OTR) and physical therapist (RPT) assigned to the patient to obtain their subjective evaluations of the patient's capacity to participate in the study. At this point the researcher casually observed the patients in their treatment programs and became briefly acquainted with them. If, after this brief exposure to the patients, the researcher considered the patients to be good candidates for the study, the patients were informed of the study. Each was told they were part of a research project but that it would not hamper their treatment. They were told that there would be some 75 recordkeeping and paper work. The patients were also told they would be included in a special team conference to discuss their personal goals for their treatment and objectives for their own discharge. Each was informed that "as a result of that conference, three behaviors would be selected" which would form the basis of the study. If the patient consented to be part of the study, the patient's immediate family was contacted and briefly advised of the study. If they concurred with the patient's consent, a patient conference was arranged. Of the first four patients selected for the study, three were interested and became the participants for the study. Patient Conference Sue's initial conference was held in the patient lounge on the nursing unit. It was attended by the entire team (physical therapy, occupational therapy,social service, psychology, nursing) and included the patient's husband as well. Sue was informed that this was a research project. The conference enabled Sue to verbalize her goals and feelings about her treatment, allowed the staff to give their perceptions of the goals, and concluded with a general dis- cussion about the total program. The specific goals of the patient, and the related behaviors and recommended time for completing behaviors as determined by the researcher and staff (without the patient's knowledge) are shown in Figure 3-3. After one day of additional instruction for selected behaviors, the baseline period was initiated. The conference for Bob was held at his bedside with RPT, CTR and nursing in attendance. Bob's goals and feelings about 6 7 noasfl um “powwow o>mn waaou mam ouommn momsueoxm zomom >uw>fluom so we mopscme om pom mmEmm no mofiunam page wcfioo Qwuu\mu:owwoom nozom so H Eu“: mxmw m 0;» now make \mH m ou asup\m4 m Scum newzmmm smocma cw aces -o>ouaEH Mom Momam wcwucsou mafia xooHo Hanuo< :oumz doom ass: use“ eases w Mouzsoo saw: momflu -uoxo :ofiuoa mo omcmu o>fim -mmm ponwuomoun .s.aH-NH mo conssz m xuw>fluum coflumcfiwhooo cam:-oxo confluom nessfixms -98 .8 ca .3 EB .cfie-omv Ho Em comzuon .E.Qm-~ ucomm mafia N “mug masses aw: xflucocwoum mam mesa“ we won -53: weapsaocfi w ammponu 90 on cums Scum you o>flun .m omwzomso: mo mofiuw>wuom Euompom .N :oochoumm o>mm mucopwoom ucsou .E.a on eBay Ho>muh meH xflmz .H mam pomuucou mcwhouwcoz mafia mp0w>mzom mfimoo m.u:oflumm pcofiumm pua3om-maom mo memo: pocuamomom uompucou psmzom-mHom can .mao«>m:om wououficoz .mHmou m.o:m mo thessm m-m chewed 77 Maw xmmun -xuoz xao>wuom muuoumwwo mcumo :xooHo-moum: .E.e so .E.m cw >uw>wuom mouscfls om uwauuofim omHHH-HH ucomm mafia m mommopoxo :ofiuoe mo sum puma mo owcmp op mouuoummwo momfiuuoxo :ofiuoz wcwonm ofifigz mcfipczou .E.Qm-v mo owned N quQSm on hawks momfiouoxo oocmpmwmou om pap saw «swan :fimmm pom mmoacs homa2m mo momfluuoxo muse: om: .N Houmm mopuoummfiu oz wcfiucsou .E.am-v museumflmox H Mam: .H pom uomupcoo wcfiuouwaoz mafia mpofl>msom mamou m.u:ofiumm ucowuwm wumzmm-mfiom mo memo: wonuumomom pomhucou chmzom-mHom paw .mhow>m:om wououwcoz .meoo m.nom mo Aymaesm s-m seamen 78 ammuocu mo mouscHE mm Houmm >9H>Huum 90 mo ooHogu monmsm can oeHu coanHnmumo pom mvpmweeum mH: onon ooxmum wsoq :xuoHu-aoum: UHuuoon mcHucsou w qu>Huum Ho .s.a eH on m-oo m peoam osHe m HaNV uHmnoHoonz mo xmmuosu monmsq mo * can .E.m uoum>oHo op Ho MH mom mo emu < nouns moum noumm Eouw oEHh meN Hoodoo mo woo pom .m momHouoxo me make mo mcouum mmoHcs znmaosp momHouoxo :oHpoE menu oxmz .N HmcoHumouoou oz mchcsou conv-omum mo omcmm H me3 .H moon uomuuaou mcHHoanoz osHb mon>mzom mHmou m.u:oHumm ucoHumm ppmzom-mHom mo memo: venoumomom Homnpcou-cumzom mHom one .muoH>m:om wouopHcoz .mHmoo m.m:on mo xumsssm m-m ouamHa 79 his program were solicited. The attending staff were encouraged to respond to the patient's concerns. Again, specific important related behaviors were singled out by the staff and the researcher for inclusion in the study. These are shown in Figure 3-4. The subsequent day was used by the staff to give instructions to the patient on the routine therapeutic activities demanded "for his rehabili- tation" and to write ward orders, a description for the nursing units to follow in implementing therapy-related behaviors during off-therapy hours. The patient conference with Doug, because of scheduling problems, was less efficient. It was anticipated that the conference would take place during a routine medical conference at which all involved staff and attending physician were in attendence. Due to an unavoidable problem,the entire con- ference (including RPT, OTR, social service, nursing, physician and medical students) moved to Doug's bedside where his condition was briefly checked by the physician. The group then reconvened for a reporting session without Doug to dis- cuss treatment plans, problems, and progress. The researcher subsequently interviewed the patient alone to obtain his goals for treatment. Once again, a one day lag time was established to allow for appropriate therapy training and encouragement to occur and to establish observer schedules. Doug's specific goals and the behaviors selected for his treatment are shown in Figure 3-5. 80 Once each of the behaviors had been identified and the best time established for observing the behaviors had been cleared with all team members, the baseline period for each person was begun. In each case where observers were to be trained, a one-day practice observation was completed with an observer to make sure he/she understood the specific behavior to be observed, the best way of monitoring the behavior, and the proper method of recording the behavior. Appendix B-2 shows the log sheets used for recording the daily occurrence of the behavior by the observers. Appendix B-3 shows the research journal sheet that was kept on the logs to use in recording unusual observations or comments which were pertinent to the day's observations. Elaboration of the Behaviors Chosen and MonitoringTechniques Based on Sue's overall desire to regain lost skills needed for homemaking and for her potential return to her job as a nurse, several behaviors were noted for inclusion in the study. Of particular importance to these goals was her definite vision loss. Loss of the vision in one eye was uncompensated for at the onset of the study. Typical of most persons with this type of loss, Sue would forget she couldn't see out of one eye and would frequently hit things with her wheelchair such as doors, other persons, protruding objects, or would not be able to locate things even though they were in front of her but off-centered in her vision field. She also missed turns in hallways and became quickly disoriented in location. The first major behavior unit selected was the process of going from her room to the OT area. This behavior to re was t that room butt: [1'68 whil fina hers both 81 was broken into two separate functions-~physical strength as measured by time, and visual accuracy as measured by lack of accidents. Thus, behavior 1 and 1a were related to the length of time it took Sue to get to therapy from her room and the number of objects she hit which impeded her progress to her destination. These skills were related to her desire to regain homemaking skills. Another benefit of behavior 1 was that it put additional pressure on her right hand in that she wheeled her wheelchair with this hand and was there- fore a strengthening exercise. Since the therapy area is on a different floor than the nursing units, Sue was timed with a st0pwatch as she began her trip to therapy from her room. The watch was stopped when Sue pressed the elevator button and started again as she got off the elevator on the treatment floor and proceeded to the OT area. It was stopped while she waited in the OT room to get directions to her final therapy station, and was reactivated until she got herself to the area. All "accidents" were counted during both timed or untimed periods. Behavior 2 was also developed to assist Sue in her ob- jective of homemaking. The OTR agreed to make a list of recommended activities and place some materials in Sue's room to help her to develop her eye-hand coordination. These exercises were necessary both for strengthening the weak but dominant hand and learning to c0pe with her restricted visual field. The activities prescribed included any writing, playing cards, games, doing leather work. Sue did not have 82 therapy between 2 and 3 p.m. so this was considered an ideal time to engage in these activities. Initially,both the num- ber of specific goal directed movements of the right hand and the amount of actual time Sue spent in a continuous 30- minute time period were monitored. Because of the variety of activities and difficulty in assessing goal-directed activity, as the study progressed, only the actual number of minutes spent in an activity as measuraflby a stopwatch was recorded. Once Sue began an activity, a 30-minute continuous time period was observed. That is, if Sue began playing cards at 2:10 the observation period would end at 2:40. Any major interruption in Sue's activity where her attention was diverted from working with her eyes and hands led to a termi- nation of the timing. But as Sue resumed her activity, the timing resumed until the end of the 30-minute observation time or until Sue terminated the OT prescribed activity. None of Sue's goal directed behavior beyond the 30-minute continuous time period was recorded. The third behavior observed included a series of exer- cises Sue was trained to perform on her paralyzed fingers, hand, arm, and shoulder. Called passive range of motion exercises, these required taking the affected part of the body with the functional hand and moving the affected part through its total range of motion. The need for this activity was to prevent tightening of the affected muscles with possible pain and deformity the result. The activity was designed also to help firm up the muscle groups and build whatever residual strength might be left. Each complete passive flexion of the 83 muscle group was counted separately. The muscle groups affected included the thumb, the fingers as a group, the wrist, the elbow, the shoulder muscles up and down and across the body and out to the side, a total of six separate exercises. Bob's three behaviors were derived primarily from his goals of desiring to walk again and to be functionally inde- pendent. Behavior 1, called resistance exercises, were necessary for building strength in his upper extremities. In this activity, Bob had weighted cuffs (beginning with 5 pounds, slowly increasing) placed around the wrists. He was required to flex the muscles of his arm in order to lift the weights and to swing the arm inward and outward. The goal was to develop gross muscle strength in the biceps and triceps. In this behavior, Bob was given RPT instruction to use the cuff weights daily between 4 and S p.m. when on the nursing unit. Ward orders were sent to the nursing unit "encouraging Bob to use the cuff weights and exercise both arms" between 4 and 5 p.m. For the study, the specific number of contraction and relaxation cycles of his right arm were counted. This arm was chosen since his right hand was his dominant hand and this arm needed the most strengthening for subsequent inde- pendent activity. The anticipated length of time for this exercise was about 15 minutes before Bob would be expected to tire. The cuff weights were left in Bob's room during the study so that he could make a request of the orderly or other nursing staff to put them on. Beh extremit ad strt head an stricte gainful Because 110.181 :ourag betwee instru were 5 Inge nabs] abser‘ takes of th L11 (D ;_'_7 ‘ CL) (4 84 Behavior 2, range of motion exercises of left upper extremity, was related to Bob's overall goal of mobility and strength. Through disuse and the presence of the large head and neck controlling Halo Brace, his left arm was re- stricted in its movement at the shoulder. It was increasingly painful to him to have it moved beyond a certain point. Because this restriction could easily lead to reduced func- tional ability in terms of dressing or hygiene, he was en- couraged to conscioqurmove his left arm as much as possible between the hours of 4 and 5 p.m. Bob was given specific instructions on how to do the ranging activities. Ward orders were sent to the nursing unit to "encourage Bob to do his range of motion" at the selected time. A simple count of the number of complete upper left arm range exercises was the observational requirement. Since this activity generally takes no more than 5 minutes, it was felt that the performance of this behavior would not interfere with the performance of Behavior 1. Behavior 3, OT attending behavior, was selected not so much from Bob's goal statement as from the researcher's observations of his activity level in both PT and OT. With the OTR'S agreement, it was decided to attempt to influence Bob's attending behavior in OT. To measure this behavior once Bob reached his assigned station in the OT area, a stop- watch was activated whenever he was not actively doing some goal directed activity with his upper extremities. He was not timed while the upper extremity muscles were actively 85' exercised. When he stopped, the watch was activated after a count of 5. When he began to work again, the watch was deactivated after a count of 5. This count was necessary since Bob made considerable random movements, and a count of 5 seemed to provide the necessary time to determine if the behavior was to continue in goal directed action. In this setting a 30-minute continuous observation time was maximal. Since a number of observation periods fell short of this maximum, the observed data were converted to percentages of time not engaged in activity. For presentation in the graph in the Appendix (A-8), the percentages were reversed to show percentage of activity time. Doug's behaviors were related to the very obvious need of strengthening. Behavior 1, range of motion exercises, was related to Doug's arm strength. He was advised by both his RPT and OTR to continually and actively move his shoulder, upper arm, and wrist muscles to strengthen them and to keep them from becoming restricted in movement. A ward order was sent to the nursing unit to "encourage Doug to do his upper extremity range of motion exercises between 3fiflland 4:00 p.m.". For observation, the number of complete cycles of exercises were counted and recorded as described for both Sue and Bob. Behavior 2 and 2a, travel time and number of pushes, were also related to upper extremity strengthening. Since Doug was very weak, wheeling himself around in a wheelchair was difficult and tiring for him. In an attempt to get him to become more vigorous in wheeling his own wheelchair, he was timed in his trip from a specific area in OT to the elevator. At the of the push 1 single done i :amt to th each SPECi .th ' that l A“. '11 l I; 86 At the same time the number of distinct and separate pushes of the wheels of his wheelchair were counted. Any simultaneous push with both arms was counted as a single push while any single arm push also counted as one push. The timing was done by a stopwatch while the number of pushes was simply counted as Doug wheeled down the hall, an estimated 80 feet to the elevator. Behavior 3, amount of time spent in OT activity, was related to Doug's overall desire to get out of the rehabili- tation center. It was the observation of the researcher that Doug seemed to spend a great deal of time in OT therapy sessions observing others while accomplishing little himself. Hence, as in Bob's situation, the OTR agreed to try to im- prove Doug's OT activity time. Observation for a 30-minute maximum time period in his afternoon OT session was initiated. Because of the observer schedule, the start of the period was somewhat contingent upon when Sue got to the OT area in the afternoon. As soon as the observer and Sue arrived from her “timed trip to OT, timing began on Doug's activity. Any time Doug was active with his arms and/or hands in therapeutic activities the stopwatch was activated. When Doug's attention ‘was diverted and he stopped working, the watch was stopped. Self>Monitoring Technique On the last day of the pre-treatment baseline period for each behavior, each of the patients was informed of the specifics of the behaviors being observed. They were told that on the next day they were to begin keeping a record of ‘that behavior themselves. In some cases this recordkeeping only'required counting. In some cases the patient was given 87 a stOpwatch with which to keep a record. In Doug's case with behavior 2, travel time, since he lacked the strength to quickly start and stop a stopwatch, a wooden holder was made into which the stopwatch fit snugly. He could then start and stop the watch by hitting the control button rather than squeezing it since the watch was firmly placed in his lap or on his wheelchair seat. He could carry the watch with him relatively simply as he "ran his race". In the behaviors where self-monitoring of OT activity levels was required (Bob and Doug), an electric alarm clock was wired to a very wide, easily engaged electrical switch. Since both Doug and Bob lacked the finger strength to manipulate even an ordinary toggle light switch, the larger switch, which was controlled by the entire hand, allowed easy self-timing of their activity in the therapy hours. Throughout the entire study, the external observers kept a record of observations with the use of a stopwatch and hand-operated digital counters like those used to keep track of expenditures in grocery shopping. The researcher was present at the beginning of each self- monitoring phase to assist in counting and recording of the target behaviors. In all behaviors, Sue used the same type of log used by the external observers to daily record her observations. She generally kept the log book at her bedside throughout the SM and SR periods of her study. Similarly, Doug was given the same type of log which was placed in a folder by his bedside. He did not wish to keep the records on the wall near his bed 88 or in any other visible place. Because Doug was unable to do anything requiring fine hand coordination, the researcher wrote Doug's verbal report into the chart for him. The OTR kept a daily log with him for behavior 3, OT activity time. Bob, being nearly completely paralyzed, could not use his hands for any recording. In his case, a log sheet was posted at his bedside and the orderly was taught to fill in the chart when Bob reported on his target behavior performance. Once again, the OTR filled in the daily performance log for Bob on his activity time in OT. She would verbally check the accumulated clock time with Bob prior to recording the accumulated time on her sheet. These sheets were kept in the OTR'S file. Self-Reinforcement Period At the conclusion of each SM period, the researcher had a conference with Sue about her perception of the activity level of the period. The concept of reinforcement for per- forming the target behaviors was discussed. Sue readily grasped the concepts of reinforcement planning. A discussion was held about what kind of things would be rewarding for her when she successfully completed the target behavior. In addition, a standard of behavior was established by Sue based on her performance level during the SM period. Once this standard was set, the reward was related to it in a contract which was written onto her recording log. Since Sue was attempting to gain weight, eating snacks was a high frequency and pleasant activity for her. It should be noted in Figure 3-3 that for behaviors 1, la, and 3 the reinforcer Sue chose to use was related to eating. The mind games and 89 puzzles which Sue chose to reinforce behavior 2 were acti- vities which she indicated she always had enjoyed, but which the researcher noted were not common occurrences. In lieu of a better alternative at the time, however, her choice remained. Per the theory of self-controlled reinforcement, the reinforcers were all under Sue's direct control pending the accomplishment of what Sue felt was acceptable minimal performance. She continued to monitor the daily results of her behavior in her bedside log book. Bob's self-reward contract was developed in the same way as Sue's for behavior 1 (resistance exercises of left arm). The researcher attempted to explain the concept of reinforce- ment to Bob. He realized the basic values of the concept and suggested some potential reinforcers which evidenced his knowledge of reinforcement. The researcher helped Bob chOOSe a high probability and pleasant activity which was under his direct control. Based on his SM performance levels, a standard of behavior was established by Bob as noted in Figure 3-4, and for meeting this standard for behavior 1, Bob chose to allow himself cigarettes after his supper. A slightly modi— fied approach was attempted for behavior 2 in light of the poor results during the SR period of behavior 1. Since Bob did not really wish to give up a reward for lack of performance, for behavior 2, Bob agreed that during the 3-4 minutes he was smoking a cigarette (a high frequency behavior after his afternoon therapy) he would do the range of motion exercises of his left arm. In this case no specified standard was set, simply that he would perform a less pleasant activity while 90 simultaneously performing a pleasant activity, a direct application of the concept called contingency management. Since Bob's 3rd behavior was occuring in the OT area, the researcher was interested in whether the self-control system could be taught to and implemented through a staff person. In this case the therapist was given instruction in the process of allowing the patient to dictate a standard of behavior, a suitable high probability reinforcement and also a method of recordkeeping. The therapist was encouraged to help the patient select a reasonable standard based on the SM period level of activity and assist in selecting a meaningful reinforcer. Figure 3-4 summarizes Bob's Self-Reward Contract as it was developed for this behavior. Doug's SR phase was handled very similarly to the others. For behaviors l, 2, and 2a, at the end of the SM periods, a conference was held with Doug to talk about the concept of giving rewards to himself as a way of increasing or maintaining appropriate behaviors. Doug also readily grasped the basic idea of the meaning of rewarding behavior with nice things or pleasant activities. At the appropriate times, his SM period jperformances were reviewed with the researcher for behavior 1 and 2 and he was asked to pick something enjoyable which he often did at about the same time of the day as the target behavior which could be related to the research behavior. He was then encouraged to choose a goal for his behavior which would be acceptable to him and for which he could have his self-controlled reward. Since Doug wanted to gain weight, and 91 since his friends often had a snack, he often had p0p and candy from the cafeteria after his afternoon therapy. He also thoroughly enjoyed associating with his peers in recrea- tional and social activities late in the day. These were logical choices totally under his control and were very rein- forcing to him, and as shown in Figure 3-5, became the basis for his self-reward contracts for behavior 1 and 2. As with Bob, Doug's third behavior was monitored in the OT area. Once again, the OTR was encouraged to help Doug establish a reasonable standard of behavior based on his SM period per- formance level and to choose a suitable reward for accomplishing his behavior. In this situation, Doug chose to work on a model, or other activities he preferred to do over some OTR prescribed activities. The chance to work on an activity he particularly enjoyed became the reinforcer for appropriate activity levels for Doug in OT. Additional Data ColleCted In addition to the daily behavior data of the individuals, the test data previously described were also collected. At the onset of the study, each participant was given the pre- viously described Rehabilitation Attitude Survey, the locus of control instrument. Throughout the study, the state trait anxiety inventory was repeatedly given to assess potential change in the A-State. These were administered as often as conveniently possible, given the patients' cooperation and schedule. In addition, the STAI was administered to a variety of other patients in the center to establish a generalized 92 norm against which to compare the results of the research patients. At the conclusion of each patient's study sequence, he or she was given the selected subscales of the EPPS as pre- viously outlined, an Incomplete Sentences Test (Appendix C-3) designed to assess the general attitude to the study itself, and a Research Questionnaire (Appendix C-4), a study related true falsetest designed to test the level of knowledge and interest the patient had in the study. Analysis The analysis of this study was done with the application of median slope analysis described by White (1971 and 197bgb). This technique utilizes data collected over time for the pur- pose of predicting future behavior patterns. Although origi- nally intended for use in assessing the effectiveness of edu— cational programs in a child's learning, the technique is useful as a post hoc test for the effects of an experimental procedure on any single subject study. The accuracy of this predictive technique depends on the number of original data points from which the original trend line is derived. More data points improve the accuracy and increase the length of time into the future which the slope can predict. If a slightly greater deviation about the projected slope is allowed, a still higher chance of accuracy of prediction is possible. White notes that a 25 percent increase in allowable deviation increases the accuracy noticeably. White described four types of significance testing which can be done through the process of median slope calculation. The first includes 93 the test of whether the trend of the data is different than a flat or no trend distribution. A second test is whether the lepe for a particular phase of the study is signifi- cantly different than for a slope in the subsequent phases. This latter test is a test of treatment effects. If the slopes vary significantly between phases, the differences in a single subject study must imply some treatment effect. This test of trend line differences can be further broken down into two more analyses. The third analysis, then, is a test of the immediate effect of the treatment by testing the step changes between two phases of a study. The fourth test is of the longer range effect of the intervention which is again done by testing the differences of slopes between phases but in a slightly different manner than the second test. The step change is noted by simply comparing the behavior level at the completion of one phase and the level of the same behavior immediately after the initiation of a different treatment. In this study, if behaviors show a large difference from one phase to the next phase immediately after the change in process, the deduction would be that the treatment itself caused an immediate change in motivation or facility to perform. If, on the other hand, we notice that the immediate" change in levels of performance takes place over the entire period and the distribution of data points shows a different slope than previously, then we can describe the longer term effect of the treatment itself by noting the direction of the SIOpe. In this study, the analysis of the overall significant differences between phases utilizing the binomial distribution as 94 described by Siegel (1956). In this analysis the significance test derives from whether the actual number of data points falling above and below the projected trend line could be due to chance occurrence and the comparison of phases was done in terms of significant step and significant trends. The re- sults of this analysis are described in the following chapter. As was mentioned previously, the value of the intensive case study is that it permits both a statistical and non- statistical analytical approach. The analysis of this study is carried out at both levels. The statistical approach has been described. The non-statistical approach includes considera- tion of the subjective climate during the study, the individual influences affecting patient behaviors, and the changes in daily procedures which might explain the variety of behavioral observations. Chapter 5 addresses this analytical process. Chapter IV Statistical Analysis of the Data The N of 1 Intensive Case Study Approach to research produces a considerable amount of useful data. The statis- tical analysis technique recommended by White (1971, 1972 a,b) provides a statistical assessment of the immediate and longer term effects of an experimental intervention. However, a statistical analysis of this type does not utilize the nominal data and subjective observations gained over the period of observation. Since the researcher was interested in the effects of SM and SR on patient behaviors, a statistical analysis is important. At the same time, a considerable amount of other observational data were collected which cannot be statistically analyzed but which may have meaning for the prac- titioner interested in the problem. For this type of data a separate analysis is very important. Hence, the analysis of this research will be two-fold-—statistical and descriptive. This chapter will deal with the question of what were the effects of SM and SR treatment on the behaviors statistically. The subsequent chapter, which could be considered an assessment of the meaningfulness of the study, will attempt to isolate factors which might be of benefit to the rehabilitation or medical profession. To assist in the statistical analysis of the data, the reader is referred to Figures 1-11 in Appendix A. These figures show the data point distributions for each of the 11 specific behaviors. The figures also Show the median SIOpe trend line 95 96 for each phase of each behavior. Each of the stated hypotheses will be reviewed and summarized in terms of the overall outcomes. Once again, the point is to be made that in White's median slope analysis there is the possibility of four separate analyses: (1) The testing of difference from a no-trend dis- tribution, which will not be addressed; (2) the testing of differences in slopes between phases, which will be tested; (3) the testing of differences due to step changes or the immediate effect of the treatment,which will be addressed; (4) the differences in trends between phases or the longer range effect of the treatment, which will be assessed. In the following description, the test of overall statistical differences is the test of overall differences between phases. The Step Differences addressed immediate change effects and Slope Differences addressed the longer range effects. In each of the tables a range of accurate prediction is also presented. The figures in this range show the highest possible accurate prediction from one phase to another and from one end of the projected phase to the other. The more data points which originally exist upon which to plot a trend line, the higher the predictability. On the other hand, the farther ahead into the future one wishes to predict a trend, the less accurate is the probable prediction. The figures were computed by White from the actual data analysis Of 166 analyzed studies and reflects a deviation error of .25 in.the data distribution. 97 .m.z .m.z .m.z em.-om. saH>Huu< a Ho n woo. .m.z coo. as.-Ha. magmas «N -- -- .m.z so.-Ha. osHe Ho>aus N -- -- .m.z om. :oHuoz mo omcmm H moon .m.z .m.z Nee. oa.-mm. suH>Huu< a so m -- -- .m.z om. :oHuoz Ho «acme N -- -- .m.z ea. .soxm «>HumHmmm H pom .m.z cmo. omo. as. eoHuoz mo emcee m -- -- .m.z as. ye e:ae-osm N mmo. .m.z mmo. ma.-ow. mueoeHuu< «H .m.z .m.z .m.z wa.-om. oeHe Hmseue H mam .wmwmc «mewmc .MmWMH H AaUm~.+ moH>mgon zoom pom thm mo AHHHHnmn—oum HmUHumHumpm H-4 osamHm 98 General Findings Hypothesis 1: The implementation of a self-monitoring (SM) technique for selected patient behaviors will significantly change the occurrence pattern of the behavior in the desired direction over a pre-treatment baseline period. This hypothesis was rejected. In this analysis, the trend line obtained in the total baseline period was extended into the SM period. The binomial test was used to calculate the probability of the differences between the projected trend and the actual trend of the SM data. The same procedure was used to calculate the significant findings of the step and slope changes. It is noted initially that for Sue (Behavior 1) and Doug (Behavior 2), although the actual travel time was not signifi- cantly altered, the corresponding behaviors (accidents and pushes) were both significantly different because of self-monitoring. Both showed that the statistical difference was due mostly to a change across phases of the slape of the data and not a large step difference. This result might be expected in behaviors of this nature where practice is essential for improvement. A slow improvement would undoubtedly occur rather than an immediate change simply because of the learning required to become more effective. We further note that Sue's ROM exercises show a signif- icant change this time due primarily to a step change. One might speculate that self-recording of her behavior in this case changed the fact that the behavior occured at all. However, upon further analysis, we note that Sue's ROM trend line is steeply declining during the SM period but the scatter of data in that period 99 was enough to make the overall decision regarding significance. We also note that the mean of the SM phase is progressively in line with the changes in the period means from B to SR. Further analysis shows that neither Bob's nor Doug's ROM exercises were significantly altered when they were asked to self-monitor their behavior, however. Finally we note that Bob's percentage of OT activity significantly improved as a result of the self-monitoring. In this case, step and slope differences apparently combined to cause the change although neither the step nor slope differences were significant in themselves. One must conclude from the data that although SM seems to have some effect on some behaviors, it does not routinely effect all behaviors. We can further speculate that the effects, if any, of self-monitoring vary from an immediate effect to a longer term effect depending on the type of behavior being measured where practice or motivation is the basis for activity. Hypothesis 2: The implementation of a self-reinforcement (SR) program (Which includes self-monitoring (SM)) will significantly change the occurrence pattern of behaviors in the deSired direction over a pre- treatment baseline (B) period. This hypothesis was rejected. For this analysis, the total number of B data points were used to develop the trend line. This line was projected across the SM period and into the SR period. Of particular note, 4-2 shows that in Sue's case the probability ranges for successful prediction are very low and little reliability can be placed on the statistical outcome for behavior 1 and la. This is due to the fact that very few baseline data points were available upon which to base a prediction from 9 to 18 days into the future. 100 -- -- .m.z -eo. HHH>Huu< H 90 m mHo. .m.z mHo. em.-oa. moemsa «H -- -- .m.z sa.-ea. oeHH Hm>epe H .m.z Hoe. Hoe. Hm.-ea. eoHuoz mo «meme H msoa -- -- .m.z -Ha.. HuHsHHu< H Ho H -- -- .m.z Hm.-Hm. :oHuoz Ho emcee N -- -- .m.z Hm.-oa. .pmxm mueaumHmoe H pom .m.z Hoo. Hoe. so. eOHpoz mo emcee m -- -- .m.z em.-cm. Ho e:m:-on H .m.z woo. soc. no.-me. mueoeHuu< mH -- -- .m.z He.-me. «EHH Hmsmsa H mam H v3 H v& H v& H 2: mm... 32.2% .5352 .onm .onm .zuHm onHuHamma maoem amem .Honn H-e musmHe some .Hom mhmm .Ho moocouommg .Ho .HHHHHnmnouHH HmoHumHumum 101 Figure 4-2 shows that statistical significance again occurred not in actual travel time for Sue and Doug but in the related activity associated with accidents and pushing behavior. It would appear from this data that recording and reinforcing appropriate behavior levels does little to affect the speed of an activity but it does help to influence the energy exerted (pushes) and awareness of body position (accidents). The other significant results were found in Sue's and Doug's range of motion exercise behavior. In this case considerable significance can be attributed to the immediate effect of the SR program as indicated by the fact that signifi- cant differences were due to step changes which implies that this activity may be motivated rather than learned. Bob had no behaviors affected by the SR program, which detracts from the value of the other findings. Based on these outcomes, it would appear that SR has a limited effect on behaviors. There seems to be a tendency to produce changes in those be- haviors which ordinarily do not occur in daily behaviors and for which additional awareness and motivation is required. Hypothesis 3: The implementation of a SR program will signi- ficantly change the occurrence pattern of selected behaviors in the desired direction over a SM only period. This hypothesis was rejected. To study the effects of a SR program wherein the indi- vidual not only kept records but also reinforced the behavior, the SM median slope was extended into the SR period data. 102 -- -- .H.z om.-HH. HuHsHuu< H Ho H -- -- .H.z HH.-om. magnum «H -- -- .H.z HH.-oo. osHH Hosmse H -- Hoe. Hoe. Ho. :oHuoz mo amuse H msoa -- -- .m.z HH.-cm. HuHsHuu< H 90 H -- -- .H.z om. eoHuoz me cause H n- .. .m.z om. .hoxm ounmumwmom H com .H.z Hoe. Hco. om.-HH. :oHuoz Ho «Heme H -- -- .H.z ca.-HH. Ho eem=-on H -- -- .m.z om.-Hm. mpeoeHuu< «H -- -- .H.z HH.-¢H. oaHH Hosase H mam Hv 3 H X: H v& H 2: mogslmm .HzmHSd .zuHH .zuHH .onm HH.+ maogm amem .H:H .HOHH>¢Q .5 ppmzpm .Hmp Hon poHHOQ :oHpm>Homno moo mucomocoou chom mump comm .b0 09 EH2: qummsz 20mm mzHH Hm>Hca mumpm :mmquHz H H H H own :HH pawn» : Hum: H Hmv :Hom pump u wand OIIIO oHum>wOmno HHmn I Hymn nxUn C) [H N C) ‘. N SPUODOS JO iequmN n:N Hum. Hun. ON. Hum mm 2H Hm H HoH>msom Noam 160 .HHHH .HHHa< Amp Hon OOHHOQ :oHum>Homno Ono mucom0HQoH HHoummHumo>cH .HOHH>OQ .6 phmzpm .>mHca opmum :menon M QfiH QGQHU C HUGO: .. 0 H3 :HOHH pump 9 Omnm Til... a HH HowumSuwmno HHOQ I HH H ”N _. 1... m H a. H 3 — 1H H o ”T: H “v . m H r. p H a __ «w — . S H H H H 0 2m m mH HoH>mcom Noam 161 HHHH .HHHa< :oHum>Homno oco mucomoemou «sum .xme Hon 2H2 mo mmmZDZ--.H-< mmonm .zoom zH >HH>HHU< Ho 2H mMHD "Hoummemo>:H .HOHH>OQ .5 we ucHoa Home comm w>Hcs oumum :mMHnqu H 7 L a 330m pump around .1 poHum>womno HHHOD QIIIIIO HmH m m mm Em N HoH>HHmm ”mam 162 ”Houmeumm>:H .mOHH>OQ .5 pumzpm ucHoo Home nomm an .mmmHummxm ZOHHOZ mo mozHOmno Ono HH:OHOHQOH C) (D O Q C) 9'. 8 sesrozexg won 50 iaqmnN <3 1’ C) 52 .Illll ocHH pcowu :MHvoz Cult... «Hogan 33 Emma? Ollll. COHum>uom aHHmo HuHmHo>Hcs Opmum :mmHnon m mm 2m m m m H HoH>mcom ”mam 163 .oumH .HHHQ< HHOHHMHpmo>cH .HOHH>OQ .5 ppmzpm mucomoamou HcHom Hump comm .Vn n¢. 0m HmH UcHH pawn» : Hpoz vcflom mumc u wand .ulll. :oHumSwmmno HHmn I DIEIDlIOlYOLIOIIYOlYOIIIOIIIOIYOII'OLIflILflIII'flIII HuHm. ; Ho>Hca Oumum m. :mHHequ mm 2m H H HoH>agom .Hmp Hon poHHOQ :oHum>Homno Ono .mmmHummxm mozcH .mOHH>On .5 phmzpm ucHom «amp comm .HHaH .HHHa< .Hmp Hog poHHoa :oHum>Homno moo mucomoumoe .mmmmummxm ZOHHOZ mo muzHes Oumum :menon Hm Hm mm 2H NH Hm N HoH>mgmm g 9. sasrozexg won 30 ieqmnN C) N) 8 .00 ”pom 165 .oan .HHHQ< HuoummHumo>cH .mOHH>Oa .5 ppmzpm .Hmp Hon poHHOa :oHum>Homno oco mucOmOHQOH unwom mump :umm .Ho 2H m>HHU< mZHH mo mou0mnH. kHHmoI 0m HuHiuo>Hca eunum :megon fix: H mm 2H Hm NH Hm H HoH>ezom "pom 166 HHHH HHca< ”HoummHumo>:H .HOHH>OQ .5 pumzpm .Hmp Hem poHHOa :oHHm>Homno Oco mucomouaow peHoa «use comm .HmHHummxm oneoz Ho mozHcs pump :HMHHOHZ OR. Hm Hm Hm mm 2H H H HoH>mnom ”moon 167 .oan HHHQ< HHoummHumo>nH .mOHH>Oa .5 pumzpm mucowOHQOH choa pump :umm .m09<>mqm OH HO 20mm mZHH Hm>Homno Oco mcHH peony c How: HmH :HOnH pump 9 one? Gilli. oHumSHmmno HHMQ I HuHmao>Hcs Oompm THMHEOHE Hum. Hun. 0o; Hun. ON; OH. <1 S! C) C) C) C) C) C) «HemmRSS spuooes JO iaqmnN £2 Hm Hm «H 2H NH Hm N HoH>mzom C) ”moon 168 .chH .HHHQ< HhoummHumo>cH .mOHH>On .5 pemzpm .Hmp Hon UOHHOQ :oHHm>Homno Oco manomOHQOH ucHon pump comm .mHummno HHHmo OIIIIO N" n w. I cm... 1H .uomw H d m Gena 9 S \ ‘ ’ 0N” OH HpHmao>Hcs oumum :menon _o Hm Hm mm 2H NH Hm Hm HoH>mnom ”moon 169 .oan .HHHQ< HHOHmmHHmo>cH .moHH>Oa .5 5Hm3pm mucomopmow HcHoa Hump comm .Hmp Hon poHHOm :oHum>Homno 0:0 .90 zH m>Heu< mzHe mo moHcs pneum :mmHnon :HH pawn» : How: 0. HmHueHom memo u owed 911.0 roHupHrwmmno HHHHOQ I H ON H d H N Hymn H 3 H H. a m H 1 H H HH HVNM ” H H x a .H * HH 1, cum. _ 7 .H H w H . H _ z t oon. H A H H a _. H cam. ._ a H Hum H .Om anH m mm 2m mm mm Hm H HoH>anom "moon APPENDIX B FORMS 170 B - 1 SCHE THOUGHTS ON BEHAVIORAL OBSERVATION ATTENDANCE: It is absolutely essential that records be kept DAILY of patient behaviors. It is imperative that someone be able to observe the behaviors on a scheduled basis. If you are unable to meet your scheduled observation period, notify me as soon as possible!! (office: 459-2837: Home: 949-8957) ACCURACY: Accurate reporting is essential for this study. It is very easy to allow your mind to wonder as you observe others, especially after the first few times. Concentrate on the assignment at all costs, since inaccurate observ- ations will make the study useless. INTERACTION: In many cases it may be very tempting to interact with the persons being observed. Interaction is fine AS LONG AS it doesn't detract from the occurrence of the observed behavior or in any way influence it. The change in observations then is not because of the individual, but because of what you did (and that's a "no-no"). UNOBTRUSIVENESS: If at all possible, make yourself a 'wallflower' while you are observing. Make the recording instruments as inconspicuous as possible. Using paper and pencil to record tally marks is as legitimate as counting with a counter, and is even better if the sound of the counter is too noticeable. Mingle with other patients if necessary to get a better position to observe. RECORDING: Always record your observations BEFORE TURNING YOUR COUNTER OR WATCH back to zero. OBSERVE THE TIME: In addition to accuracy, time will usually be a majoy factor to be observed. If the time for observing is from 7:00—7:30, please make sure that you observe that time! RESEARCH JOURNAL: Note any particular things which seemed to affect the observed behavior, such as a specific activity which prevented the occurrence of the behavior, or any other unusual activity going on at the time. TIME KEEPING: Keep a log of the time you spent for reimbursement purposes. QUESTIONS: Don't assume anything in this project. If you have questions about anything, ROLLER RIGHT AWAY! To delay may be costly, either in terms of inaccurate reporting or inaccurate observing. Stay in touch and don't feel embarrased...I'LL be embarrased if the data isn't right when I present it for my doctorate! Thanks for being the help I know you will be to me. This is an important project for me and your help will be most important! 171 B-Z unaH Nua>nuo¢ «use: wane mane muse onuaoomm mo * new canon "8H2: AdmOH>£=Hm OOHmmm ZOHB¢>MNmmO ho HZHB undue»: mouse vaaoam can ooua sooauon >uw>wuom was: .9.0 cu ucomu ffie oe fees as omum Oduw m\oa mmud moud w\OH ooum 03H» no access ad oo>uouno on o» now>u£om um «mamauxm uncu Nua>auo< oauaoomm MJHthm MHmNA¢mmm «use: no ¢ wade Gaga mama new :«mmm “aoemazoo amnzmmluqum “ona¢>mmmmo mo ocean: «QHZHMNO MOH>¢flmm flxdz BZMHB‘A 172 page # B-3 RESEARCH JOURNAL DATE COMMENTS APPENDIX C INSTRUMENTS 173 -1 C REHABILITATION ATTITUDE SURVEY TO THE PATIENT: The following statements are not a test. There are no right or wrong answers. We are asking that you respond to the statements as you feel about them. Your answers will be anonymous but will help the staff at Mary Free Bed to understand hOw patients feel about things. To answer, just put an X through the "T" if you agree with the statement or through the "F" if you disagree. T F 1. American hospitals would be in much worse shape if it not for the trained medical staff they have. T F 2. I am proud when I do things for myself. T F 3. Others usually know what is best for me. T F 4. Knowing what is happening is not important in treatment. T F 5 There is a direct connection between how hard I work and the progress I make in therapy. 6. A good patient is one who does what the staff tells him. 7. I have often found that what is going to happen will. 8. My return to health is entirely in the hands of others. 9. I feel nervous when asked to make decisions about treatment. Knowing what my problem is helps me to manage my program. 11. I am afraid of doing too much in therapy right now. 12. If given the Opportunity to control my life, I would accept. 13. I am totally at ease with my present situation. HHHHHH-lt-l—I "fl'fl'fl'fl'fl'fl'fl'fl'fl H O 14. At this moment I can change the extent to which my disability affects me. 15. I am not sure that the patient care team will do what they are supposed to do. "'3 ’11 T F 16. "Eat, drink, and be merry for tomorrow who knows" reflects how I feel right now. T F 17. I feel that the nurses know what is best for me. T F 18. Right now I feel there is nothing whiCh can help me to do things any better. I F 19. I feel that I am able to control my treatment program. T F 20. I am regretful about what has led me to my present situation. I F 21. Trying to control what happens to me right now doesn't seem to be paying off--it's hardly worth the effort. T F 22. Being able to know ahead of time what will happen in therapy doesn't affect how well I do. T F 23. A good patient is one who makes sure the staff understands his situation even though the staff may find it annoying. I F 24. Many of the unhappy things in peoples lives are partly due due to bad luck. AGE C-1 25. 26. 27. 28. 29. 30. 31. 32. 33. 34. 35. 36. 37. 38. 39. 40. 174 Right nOw I feel that I am in control of my life. It is important for me to know where I am, what is happening and what may happen in any circumstance. I am most pleasant when allowed complete freedom of activity. I feel tense about what is happening in my treatment. Knowing what my problem is cannot help me to manage my program any better. I feel most content when I am permitted to make my own decisions. I feel totally to blame for what has happened to me. No matter how hard I try I cannot improve my condition. Most patients don't realize the extend to which their rehabilitation is influenced by accidental happenings. I prefer to have the staff tell me what to do. Trying to control what happens to me isn't worth the effort, it doesn't pay off. Right now I feel totally helpless. Where I am right now is a function of my own doing--1uck or fate has little to do with it. I get jittery feelings when asked to think about my future. Trying to change any part of my program is useless, patients opinions don't really seem to count. In the long run, patients are in control of their program. SEX DIAGNOSIS C-Z Patient's name Rater's name 175 PATIENT BEHAVIOR SCALE Unit Date Title Indicate by placing an x before the statement which is most indicative of the above named patient for each of the following comments, based on your observations of the patient. 1. The The 2. The The 3. The The 4. The The 5. The The 6. The The 7. The The 8. The The 9. The The 10. The The 11. The The 12. The The 13. The The 14. The The patient patient patient patient patient patient patient patient patient patient patient' patient' patient patient patient patient patient patient patient patient patient patient patient patient patient patient patient or activity patient follows treatment recommendations. seldom follows treatment recommendations. seldom needs prodding to get activities done. usually needs prodding to get activities done. rarely requests help more often than is necessary. often requests help more often than is necessary. follows directions for treatment. does not follow directions for treatment. works for extended periods of time on tasks. fails to work on a task for any reasonable period. 5 attitude is usually positive. 3 attitude is usually poor. abides by hospital rules and regulations. violates hospital rules and regulations. seldom becomes upset by failure or lack of progress. often becomes upset by failure or lack of progress. usually complains about tasks that are given. seldom complains about tasks that are given. works constantly on therapeutic activities. rarely works on therapeutic activities. reports regularly for appointments. often fails to show up for appointments or is late. sometimes says or does things that are self-defeating. rarely says or does things that are self-defeating. is usually friendly and agreeable. is often unfriendly and disagreeable. never comments in an uncomplimentary manner on the work of the staff. frequently comments in an uncomplimentary manner on the work or activity of the staff. 15. The patient often speaks to others. The patient rarely or never speaks to others. The patient takes pride in the progress of therapy; that is, shows progress or talks about progress to others. The patient takes no pride in the progress of therapy; that is, shows nothing to others nor talks about progress to others. Behavior Scale, page 2 176 17. The patient pays attention to the therapy and progress of others. The patient pays no attention to the therapy and progress of others. 18. The patient makes no worthwhile suggestions about treatment. The patient makes some worthwhile suggestions about treatment. 19. The patient seldom accepts constructive suggestions from the staff. The patient usually accepts constructive suggestions from the staff. 20. The patient participates in the activities around him. The patient rarely participates in the activities around him. 21. The patient often volunteers information which is helpful. The patient never volunteers any information which is helpful. 22. The patient is usually interacting with one or more patients. The patient usually spends time alone. 23. The patient rquests to follow through on ward orders. The patient ignores ward order recommendations. 24. The patient often plays cards, games, etc. with other patients. The patient rarely plays cards, games, etc. with other patients. 25. The patient initiates activity for themselves. The patient waits for others to start him on an activity. 26. The patient is usually playful and good humored. The patient is seldom playful and good humored. 27. The patient is usually busy with something. The patient seems to have a lot of inactivity. 28. The patient seldom becomes upset if something doesn't suit him/her. The patient often becomes upset if something doesn't suit him/her. 29. The patient takes pride in personal appearance. The patient takes no pride in personal appearance. 30. The patient asks for things to do when not occupied. The patient never asks for things to do when not occupied. 31. The patient does not have to be pressured to get to appointments. The patient has to be pressured to get to appointments. 32. The patient never wants to lie in bed during the daytime more than is required. The patient usually wants to lie in bed during the daytime more than is required. 33. The patient makes positive suggestions when problems arise. The patient seldom makes positive suggestions when problems arise. 34. The patient seems interested in nothing. The patient seems interested in everything. 35. The patient does not need to be directed to an activity. The patient would sit all day if not directed to an activity. Name 10. 11. 12. 13. 14. 15. 16. 17. 18. 19. 20. 21. 22. 23. 24. 25. 177 INCOMPLETE SENTENCES BLANK Complete these sentences to express your real feelings about the study you have been part of while a patient at Mary Free Bed. sure to make a complete sentence. The study I was part of Rewarding my own behavior During the study The whole idea Charts and graphs Keeping a daily record of behavior I don't think Making deals about my behavior My greatest desire The thing I would like to do Observers Self-given rewards Observing and counting Three goals I have I feel The thing I liked best about the study Sometimes I am very I do best What pains me The only trouble If I had a chance Now that it's over In the future I want to know Be Name True True True True True True True True True True True True True True True c-4 False False False False False False False False False False False False False False False 1. 10. 11. 12. 13. 14. 15. 178 RESEARCH QUESTIONNAIRE Date The following statements are to be answered either True if you agree or False if you disagree. They are related to the study you were part of. Just recording the amount of a certain behavior I do helps to make me do the behavior better or more often. By requiring that I wait to do something I like to do until I do something that is required or suggested will probably help to make the required behavior happen more. Rewards are something which we only get from others, we can't give ourselves rewards. I felt that keeping a record of the number of times or amount of time I spend doing something was not important to me. The idea of rewarding my own behavior meant that I should do a certain thing before I could do something I would rather do. Giving myself a few minutes rest in therapy for every so many minutes I worked in therapy would be an example of rewarding my own behavior. I felt I understood the "Self—Reward Contracts" I made during the study. I felt that the behaviors I watched for the study were related to my goals I had for myself. I don't see how “making a deal with myself" will help me to change how much I do of some behavior. I don't need to "make a deal with myself” to do something I don't particularly want to do, but which is important. Having a snack, smoke, or some other enjoyable thing BEFORE doing some recommended behavior will increase the possibility that I will do that behavior later. I was interested in what the study was all about. I feel the Self-Reward Contract assisted me in doing more of the behaviors than I would have done without any contract. Having observer(s) around was the main influence that made me do the suggested behaviors rather than my own incentive. I might use the idea of a Self-Reward Contract on my own. MICHIGAN STATE UNIV. LIBRARIES WWIIWIN"“WI"WWIWWWHHIWIIHHHI 31293129381009