THESIS “‘5 LIBRARY 293 Michigan State University This is to certify that the dissertation entitled THE BIOBEHAVIORAL DYNAMICS OF THE INFANT FEEDING PROCESS 4 a. ,7 presented by Sara Ann Quandt has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for o , Ph.D. degree in Anthropology a jor professor mafiafl Date _N03Le.mhm:_.l_1,_l_9_8l MSU is an Affirmative Action/Equal Opportunity Imtitution 0-12771 W- a_ | Li ”(LN ll Nil Hill in (ill 11mg! iii Iii IL H! ”ii i L r . “is OVERDUE FINES . 25¢ per 60 per 1m RETURNING LIBRARY MATERIALS: Place in book return to runove charge from circulation records I 35"=\\\\ k \ . ‘4 \ THE BIOBEHAVIORAL DYNAMICS OF THE INFANT FEEDING PROCESS BY Sara Ann Quandt A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Anthropology 1981 ABSTRACT THE BIOBEHAVIORAL DYNAMICS OF THE INFANT FEEDING PROCESS BY Sara Ann Quandt This research examines longitudinally the interaction of infant growth, maternal postpartum nutritional status, and infant feeding strategies involving breast-feeding. Subjects were 80 normal, first-born neonates and their mothers, drawn from a low-risk, Caucasian obstetrical population in Michigan. Infant weight, length, and fat- folds were measured monthly from birth to six months. Maternal weight, upper arm circumference, and triceps fat- fold were measured monthly during the postpartum. Infant dietary data were reported by mothers in diary form every eight days throughout their six months of participation. Additional social data were obtained through interviews and observations at monthly home visits. The number of nursing bouts per day during the early weeks of infancy appears to be a key variable in predicting long term infant diets and to be part of a complex inter- action with infant growth. Those infants nursing seven or more times per day were far more likely to be exclusively breast-fed for at least three months and to be weaned after six months than were infants initially fed less frequently. Infants larger at birth and one month followed this pattern of frequent nursing more than did smaller infants. These findings are used to demonstrate that the seemingly contradictory results of previous growth and diet research can be explained by focusing on how infants become members of particular feeding groups. Maternal postpartum weight loss was not associated with breast-feeding strategies. Two patterns of change in maternal midarm fat area were found among women ex- clusively breast-feeding at least three months and not weaned until after six months postpartum. Frequent nursing was strongly correlated with progressive reduction in fatness, while infrequent nursing was associated with increases in fatness. It is hypothesized that the differ- ences observed in maternal fat change patterns result from variations in lipoprotein lipase and hormone activity, induced by variable levels of serum prolactin resulting from different breast-feeding styles. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS I gratefully acknowledge the financial support I have received for the research reported here. The Medical Anthropology Training Program at Michigan State University provided predoctoral training funds from National Institute of Mental Health grant SSS T32 RM 243 MH15132. Substantial additional funding was contributed by Gerber Products Company, Fremont, Michigan. Both the College of Social Science and the College of Human Medicine, Michigan State University, provided computer funding. My thanks go to the members of my doctoral guidance committee: Drs. Cheryl Ritenbaugh, Norman Sauer, Kathryn Kolasa and Ann Millard. I appreciate the help they have given me throughout my graduate training and their mainte- nance of high standards for my work in both anthropology and nutrition. I am especially grateful for their prompt and critical reading of the earlier drafts of this disser- tation. Special thanks go to Dr. Cheryl Ritenbaugh who urged me to undertake this research. Her interest in the study and faith in the significance of its results helped to maintain my enthusiasm during three rather long years of data collection. ii iii Perhaps my greatest debt is owed to the mothers and infants who participated in this research. Their willing- ness to share their personal experiences with me and to cooperate in all phases of the study is sincerely appreci- ated. I thank my friends in the Department of Anthropology at Michigan State for tolerating endless discussions of breast-feeding and baby fat and for providing the diversions necessary for the successful completion of graduate school. And finally, I thank my parents who encouraged me to return to school. During the past six years they have supported me in many ways--and almost never lost hope that I would someday graduate! TABLE OF CONTENTS Chapter I Introduction . . . . . . . . . . II Review of the Literature . . . . Infant Diet U.S. Breast-Feeding and Artificial Practices since 1900 . . . . Current Research Directions Infant Nutrition . . . . . . . Milk Consumption and Growth Growth and Consumption of Non-Milk Underlying Assumptions . . . Maternal Nutritional Status . Breast-Feeding and Weight Loss Breast-Feeding and Fatness . Summary . . . . . . . . . . . III Methods . . . . . . . . . . . . Research Design . . . . . . . The Sample Description of the Sample . Sample Recruitment . . . . . Data Collection . . . . . . . Maternal Data Collection . . Infant Data Collection - Anthropometric 13 17 18 21 23 29 30 32 37 39 39 41 44 46 46 49 Infant Data Collection - Anthropometric . Statistical Considerations and Reporting Reliability Levels of Statistical Significance Indicators of Accuracy: Infant Diet Diaries Combining Subjects from Study I and Study II IV Results . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Infant Diet General Description of the Infant Diets . Association of Infant Diet with Education and Household Income Length and Frequency of Nursing Transitions to a Mixed Diet . . Major Dietary Patterns . . . . Infant Growth . . . . . . . . . . Description of the Sample . . . Preliminary Data Transformations weaning 0 O O o o o o o o o o o Maternal Bouts Introduction of First Non-Breast Milk Common Infant Dietary Patterns Details of Breast Feeding . . . Interview Data - - - . . . . . . Prenatal Plans for Infant Feeding . Infant Feeding in the Parental Generation Spatial Arrangements of Families Decision Making Data . . . . . Maternal Nutritional Status . . . Descriptive Statistics for the Total Sample 49 54 55 56 S9 59 64 73 84 91 97 98 101 102 107 108 124 127 130 130 131 134 138 140 vi Maternal Measures by Common Infant Dietary Patterns . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 146 Maternal Measures by Months of Breast- . Feeding O O O O O O O C I O O O O 0 O O O O O O 149 Maternal Measures and Details of Breast- Feeding . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 152 V Discussion Emergence of Infant Feeding Strategies . . . . . 164 Interaction of Infant Diet and Infant Growth . . 173 Influence of Growth on the Dietary Pattern of Infants . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 174 Influence of Dietary Patterns on the Growth of Infants . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 180 Supporting Evidence from the Infant Growth and Diet Literature . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 186 Conflicting Evidence for the Impact of Diet on Growth . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 188 Interaction of Infant Diet and Maternal Nutritional Status . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 190 Breast-Feeding and Weight Loss . . . . . . . . 190 Breast-Feeding and Changes in Maternal Body Fat . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 194 VI Synthesis and Conclusions - . . . . . . . . . . . . 199 A General Model of Infant Feeding Systems 199 ‘/Lactation Amenorrhea and Maternal Body Fat . . . 205 The Adequacy of Breast Milk . . . . . . . . . . . 209 Conclusions . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 213 References Cited . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 216 Appendix A: Letters of Introduction and Consent Forms 0 o o o o c o o o o o o o . . . 227 Appendix B: Distribution of Mothers' Returning Infant Diet Diaries . . . . . . 233 Appendix C: Appendix D: vii Infant Outlines Used in Data Collection . . . . . . . . . . . . Reasons for Introducing New Food to a Breast Milk Diet . . . . . . . . Table Table Table Table Table Table Table Table Table Table Table Table Table III.1 III.2 III.3 III.4 IV.l IV.2 IV.3 IV.4 IV.5 IV.6 IV.7 IV.8 IV.9 LIST OF TABLES Number of Infants Measured at Birth and at each Month . . . . . . . . . . . . . Gross Household Income by Study and for Total Sample . . . . . . . . . . . . . Highest Level of Maternal Formal Education by Study and for Total Sample Comparison of Biological Variables, Analysis of Variance: Study I versus study II C C O C O O O O O C O I C O 0 Number and Percentage of Infants Consuming Major Non-Breast Milk Food Types during each Month of the Study First Solid Consumed; Number of Infants by Month of Introduction . . . . . . . Percentage and Number of Infants Consuming Combined Dietary Components during each Month of the Study . . . . Highest Level of Maternal Formal Education and Month of Introduction of First Non-Breast Milk Food . . . . . Highest Level of Maternal Formal Education and Month of Weaning . . . Month of Introduction of First Non- Breast Milk Food by Household Income Category . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Month of Weaning by Household Income Category . . . Percentage and Number of Infants Consuming Formula by Sex and Month . . Percentage and Number of Infants Consuming Solid Food by Sex and Month . viii 42 43 43 58 62 62 63 65 66 67 68 69 71 Table Table Table Table Table Table Table Table Table Table Table Table Table IV.10 IV.11 IV.12 IV.13 IV.14 IV.15 IV.16 IV.17 IV.18 IV.19 IV.20 IV.21 IV.22 Number and Percentage of Infants Consuming First Solid Food by Month, Sex, and Study Number of Breast-Feedings per Day in Month 1 by Length of Exclusive Breast- Feeding . Number of Breast-Feedings per Day in Month 2 by Length of Exclusive Breast- Feeding . Change in Number of Breast-Feedings per Day between Month 1 and Month 2, by Months of Exclusive Breast-Feeding Change in Minutes per Breast-Feeding between Month 1 and Month 2, by Months ix of Exclusive Breast-Feeding . Role of Newly Introduced Foods in Diet Formerly Consisting Exclusively of Breast Milk Test for the Presence of a Population- Wide Decline in Number of Breast- Feeding Bouts per Day during Months 2 and Introduction of Formula and Solids Relative to Median Date for Introduction of First Non-Breast Milk Food Role of New Food in Infant Diet, by Sex and Month of Introduction Month of Weaning for Infants Given Formula or Solids in Month 1, by 3 Addition and Replacement Categories . Nutrient Values for Average Daily Intake of Non-Breast Milk Foods at their First Introduction; Mean : Standard Deviation . Nutrient Values for Average Daily Intake of Non-Breast Milk Foods at their First Introduction for Feeding Group 3; Mean ‘1 Standard Deviation Descriptive Statistics for Infants at Birth and at End of each Month 71 78 79 80 81 86 88 89 90 91 96 97 99 Table Table Table Table Table Table Table Table Table Table Table IV.23 IV.24 IV.25 IV.26 IV.27 IV.28 IV.29 IV.30 IV.31 IV.32 IV.33 x Mean NCHS Percentile : Standard Deviation for Infants at Birth and at End of each Month . . . . . . . . . . . . Means : Standard Deviations of Four Infant Growth Measures at Birth and End of Months 1 to 6, by Month of Weaning . . Means : Standard Deviations of Four Infant Growth Measures at Birth and End of Months 1 to 6, by Month of First Introduction of Non-Breast Milk Foods . . Means : Standard Deviations of Four Infant Growth Measures at Birth and End of Months 1 to 6, by Infant Feeding Groups . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Regression Equations: Dependent Variable is Weight for Length and Independent Variable is Month of Age (1 - 6 months). . Comparisons of Number of Infants in Lowest and Highest Quartiles of Weight for Age, at Birth and End of each Month, by Feeding Group . . . . . . . . . . . . . Comparisons of Number of Infants in Lowest and Highest Quartiles of Weight for Length, at Birth and End of each Month, by Feeding Group . . . . . . . . . Fisher's Exact Test of Difference in Number of Infants in Highest and Lowest Weight for Length Quartiles between Groups "Replacing" Breast Milk with Formula (lb and 2a) and those NOT "Replacing" with Formula (la and 2b) . . . Relationship between Growth and Details of Breast-Feeding among ; Infants of Mothers in Groups A and B . . . . . . . . Study II Responses to the Question: When will you first give your baby anything but breast milk (or an occasional "convenience bottle") to eat? . Parental Generation Feeding as Infants by Feeding Group of Infants in Present Study (Study II only). . . . . . . . . . . 100 103 109 114 122 123 '125 126 128 131 132 Table Table Table Table Table Table Table Table Table Table Table Table Table IV.34 IV.35 IV.36 IV.37 IV.38 IV.39 IV.4O IV.41 IV.42 IV.43 IV.44 IV.45 IV.46 xi Distribution of Infant Feeding Groups by Spatial Arrangement of Family Dwelling O O C O O O C O O O I O C O O O 0 Distribution of Spatial Arrangement of Dwellings by Maternal Formal Education . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Categories of Maternal Decisions to Introduce First Non-Breast Milk Food . . . Addition or Replacement of First Non- Breast Milk Food by Decision Categories . Cumulative Measures of Maternal Weight Loss during the Postpartum . . . . . . . . Cumulative Changes in Midarm Muscle and Fat Areas during the Postpartum . . . Cumulative Maternal Weight Loss from Last Prepartum Weighing by Postpartum Month and Infant Feeding Group . . . . . . Cumulative Change in Midarm Fat Area from First Postpartum Measurement by Postpartum Month and Infant Feeding Group . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Cumulative Weight Loss from Last Prepartum Weighing by Postpartum Month and Months of Breast-Feeding . . . . . . . Cumulative Change in Midarm Fat Area from First Postpartum Measurement by Postpartum Month and Months of Breast- Feeding. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Number of Mothers with Increasing and Decreasing Fat Measures in the Month Following Weaning, by Month of Weaning . . Numbers of Mothers with Increasing and Decreasing Fat Measures in the Month Following First Introduction of Non- Breast Milk Food, by Month of Intrdduction. Spearman Rank-Order Correlation Coefficients: Change in Maternal Weight and Fat with Length and Number of Breast— Feedings . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 133 134 135 139 141 144 147 148 150 153 154 154 Table Table Table Table Table Table Table IV.47 IV.48 IV.49 IV.50 IV.51 IV.52 VI.1 xii Maternal Anthropometric and Birth Outcome Variables for Groups A and B, Mean 1 Standard Deviation . . . . . . Number of Breast-Feedings per Day at Four Weeks for Infants of Mothers in Groups A and B . . . . . . . . . . . . . Number of Breast-Feedings per Day at Eight Weeks for Infants of Mothers in Groups A and B . . . . . . . . . . . . . Number of Breast-Feedings per Day at Twelve Weeks for Infants of Mothers in Groups A and B . . . . . . . . . . . . . Minutes per Feeding for Groups A and B. Mean 1 Standard Deviation . . . . . . . Spearman Rank—Order Correlation Coefficient: Minutes per Breast-Feeding with Numbers of Feedings per Day . . . . Hypothesized Maternal Effects of Decreased Breast-Feeding Frequency with Varying Levels of Caloric Intake . . . . 160 161 161 162 163 163 208 Figure Figure Figure Figure Figure Figure Figure Figure Figure Figure Figure Figure IV.1 IV.2 IV.3 IV.4 IV.5 IV.6 IV.7 IV.8 IV.9 IV.10 IV.11 IV.12 LIST OF FIGURES Number and Percent of Infants Breast- Fed (Consuming any Breast Milk) and Weaned (Consuming no Breast Milk) during First through Sixth Months of Life 0 O O O O O I O O O O O O O O O I O I 60 Sex Differences in Breast-Feeding Status by Month of Study . . . . . . . . . . . . 70 Percent of Infants First Consuming Solids for Male and Female Infants by Month of Age 0 O O C O O C O C C O O O O C O I O O 72 Average Number of Breast—Feedings per Day for Infants Exclusively Breast-Fed during Months 1 and 2 . . . . . . . . . . 75 Number of Breast-Feedings per Day in Month 1 by Length of Exclusive Breast- Feeding . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 76 Number of Breast-Feedings per Day in Month 2 by Length of Exclusive Breast- Feeding . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 77 Mean Number of Minutes per Feeding during Month 1 . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 82 Mean Number of Minutes per Feeding during Month 2 . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 83 Length of Exclusive Breast-Feeding and Infant Feeding Groups . . . . . . . . . . 93 Mean Weight for Age (Percent of NCHS Median) by Month of Introduction of First Non-Breast Milk Food . . . . . . . . . . . Mean Weight for Length (Percent of NCHS Median) by Month of Introduction of First Non-Breast Milk Food . . . . . . . . . . . 113 Mean Weight for Age (Percent of NCHS Median) for Infant Feeding Groups la through 3 . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 118 xiii Figure Figure Figure Figure Figure Figure Figure Figure Figure Figure Figure Figure IV.13 IV.14 IV.15 IV.16 IV.17 IV.18 V.1 VI.1 V1.2 V1.3 3.1 C.1 xiv Mean Weight for Length (Percent of NCHS Median) for Infant Feeding Groups la and lb 0 I O O O O O O O O O O I O O D 0 Mean Weight for Length (Percent of NCHS Median) for Infant Feeding Groups 2a, 2b, and 3 . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Maternal Postpartum Weight Status; Mean i Standard Deviation . . . . . . . Cumulative Postpartum Change in Maternal Midarm Muscle and Fat Areas; Mean : Standard Deviation . . . . . . . . . . . Cumulative Postpartum Weight Loss by Month of Weaning . . . . . . . . . . . . Distribution of Significance Levels for Regression of Postpartum Maternal Fat Area on Days since Parturition . . . . . Hypothesized Pathways for the Inter- action of Nursing Frequency and Maternal BOdy Fat 0 C O C C C O O O O O O O O O 0 Model of the Interaction of Infant Feeding Behavior, Infant Growth, and Parameters of Maternal Physiology . . . Components of the "Social Environment“ . Components of "Parameters of Maternal Physiology" . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Distribution of Mothers Returning Diet Diaries 0 O O O O O O C O O O O O O O 0 Infant Outlines . . . . . . . . . . . . 121 121 142 143 151 158 196 201 202 203 233 235 CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION In order to survive, an animal must eat; if it is a young and helpless mammal, it must be fed. All successful mammalian species have evolved ways of nourishing their young and, from an intra-specific perspective, these behaviors are remarkably uniform. However, one such species does not conform to this generalization. When one compares human populations, a high degree of variability emerges in what is fed to infants, when it is fed, and by what modality. Even within specific populations, infant feeding behaviors vary. It is a basic premise of this study that such behaviors are not random, but are patterned by the interactions of both cultural and biological characteristics of the group which the infant belongs. It is a second premise that the detailed examination of the feeding behaviors for individual children can help to map these biocultural interactions. In EurOpe and the United States, infant feeding behaviors are increasingly cited as determinants of lifelong growth and health. As a result, the medical professions prescribe what they consider to be desirable patterns for feeding young children. Yet there is an enormous amount of variability in 1 2 the diets actually fed to infants--and the observable results are not easily classified as harmful. Obviously, factors other than medical advice are affecting infant diet. While research has been done on the factors in- fluencing certain isolated choices which mothers make concerning infant diet, little study has been done of the manner in which these choices interact to result in observed, long term infant dietary patterns. Further, although the effect of specific foods on growth has been studied, little is known about the way in which feeding . strategies as a whole affect both infant and--in the case of breast-feeding--materna1 nutritional status. This research follows a trend toward the increased use of interaction and feedback models to describe and analyze human diets. On a general, theoretical level Katz (1980) and Jerome et a1. (1980) have published models of the adaptability of foodways for human populations. Also at the theoretical level, Bass et a1. (1979) have presented a feedback model for understanding individual food habits. All of these models consider the interaction of a variety of aspects of the physical and social environments as well as aspects of human biology. Related more directly to infant feeding are two models which deal with the biosocial components of lactation. Jelliffe and Jelliffe (1979b) outline the basic reflexes necessary for successful breast- feeding in individual women. Gussler and Briesemeister (1980) have expanded on this to explore specific attributes 3 of social interactions which can lead to lactation failure both in individual cases and as a population-wide pheno- menon. However, their model remains untested on a single body of data. This dissertation presents a prospective study in which infant growth, maternal postpartum nutritional status, and infant feeding strategies involving breast-feeding were documented in a free-living population from central Michigan. The dissertation has three primary goals. The first is to identify the principle infant feeding strategies which exist in this population. A processual approach is taken in the analysis of these feeding strategies. This approach-~in contrast to that of many previous studies-- serves to emphasize that an infant feeding strategy is not a set of independent behaviors. Rather, it is the product of a series of decisions, each informed and limited by previous ones and, in turn, influencing those subsequent to it. In addition to examining the interaction of dietary components with each other, the study examines the influence on the decision points of factors external to the infant feeding process. These include socioeconomic variables and food beliefs as they are shaped by parental interaction with the medical community, family, and friends. Having identified the major infant feeding strategies and associated influences, the second goal of the dissertation will be to examine the interaction of these strategies with infant growth. Specifically, it will determine whether 4 different patterns of growth as well as differences in attained size can be linked to feeding strategies. Weight, length and measures of body fat will be examined. The final goal is to study the interaction of those feeding strategies involving breast-feeding with maternal nutritional status assessed anthropometrically. Varia- bility in times of weaning and supplementation, as well as differences in lactation "styles" (e.g., length and frequency of nursing bouts) will be examined in attempts to explain postpartum changes in weight and body composition. The results of this investigation may be valuable in a number of ways. First, they will provide a comparison of the explanatory power of processual descriptions of diet for infant growth with more traditional infant diet cate- gories. The results should provide mother/infant "profiles" of the most common growth and diet patterns. Furthermore, the results will identify the decision points in the infant feeding process for particular subsets of the mother/infant population, a finding of potential significance for those interested in diet and growth intervention. And finally, this investigation will provide basic data on the impact of different kinds of breast-feeding practices on maternal nutritional status. Currently, such data are virtually nonexistent in both the medical and nutritional literature. They are potentially relevant to policy decisions regarding promotion of various infant feeding practices among women of varying levels of nutritional status, and are of interest S in the formulation of statements on the biologically adaptive value of breast-feeding as a human food habit. The chapters of the dissertation are arranged in the following sequence: literature review, methods, results, discussion, and a final synthesis with conclusions. Chapter II reviews previous studies of infant diet, infant growth, and maternal nutritional status. It presents findings relevant to the present study and assesses the limitations of current knowledge in the area of infant feeding. Chapter III describes and justifies the research design. It also discusses the sample and the data collection techniques. The rationale for choosing a particular infant dietary data collection strategy is pre- sented and its effectiveness is assessed. Chapter IV contains the results of the data analysis. In the first section, the most common infant dietary patterns are defined in terms of sample demographics, timing of major feeding events, and details of the breast-feeding process. A second section summarizes data obtained through interviews with mothers. These focus on the process by which infant feeding decisions are made. Analyses of infant growth are presented in the third section, and the fourth describes analyses designed to identify patterns of change in maternal nutritional status. These somewhat discrete categories of results are each summarized and discussed in Chapter V. They are then merged 6 to form the basis for a synthetic biobehavioral model of the infant feeding process in Chapter VI. The potential application of this model to two current scientific contro- versies--the critical body composition hypothesis and the nutritional adequacy of breast milk-~conclude the chapter. CHAPTER II REVIEW OF THE LITERATURE This review of the literature will discuss infant feeding studies from the last several decades in terms of three general and not necessarily discrete categories. The first is infant diet, the behavioral study of what is fed to infants and the rationale for such behavior. The second is infant nutrition, the interaction of infant diet and physiology. Infant growth studies fall into this category. The third involves the relationship of lactation to the nutritional status of nursing mothers. Investigations will be reviewed with respect to their findings and their assumptions in order to discover why the current picture of infant feeding is generally fragmentary and inconsistent. It will be shown that these inconsistencies can often be understood through considering the nature of the researcher and of the research design. Finally this chapter will attempt to demonstrate that by changing or at least making more explicit the assumptions about infant feeding inherent in these studies, it may be possible to design new and more productive research which can present a more complete and coherent picture of the infant feeding process. 7 8 Infant Diet U.S. Breast-Feeding and Artificial Feeding Practices since 1900 An historical overview of infant feeding practices and recommendations provides two types of information: what has been fed to infants and the sources of influence on physicians and others which have caused the observable trends in feeding patterns. Although a survey of medical texts throughout the 19003 shows a consistent recommendation of breast milk as the best source of nutrients for infants, physicians have apparently been less than consistent in their enthusiasm for or effectiveness in promoting the practice. Reported figures vary, but in general, the incidence of breast- feeding in the United States and Western Europe decreased from high levels at the beginning of the 20th century to a very low percentage in the 19503 and 19603 (Fomon 1974). Meyer collected data on infant feeding modality at hospital discharge at ten year intervals in a representative group of U.S. hospitals and found a steady decline from 38% to 21% to 18% in 1946, 1956, and 1966, respectively (1958, 1968). The decrease was not uniform across all social strata. Breast-feeding remained the most common form of infant feeding in the lower social classes until the 19603, while the upper classes adopted the more "modern" practice of artificial feeding. Since the 19603 there has been a resurgence of interest and incidence of breast-feeding in 9 the U.S. and Europe. This has been particularly apparent in the upper social classes, with the lower classes lagging behind due to what one author has called the "osmotic trickling down" action of social attitudes and practices (Meyer 1968:713). Medical recommendations of appropriate ages for the feeding of solid foods have shown marked changes through time. (Most U.S. authors include cereal, juice and all non-milk foods in the "solids" category.) In the early 19003 solids in general were forbidden until after one year of age, with vegetables, for example, considered inappro- priate until three years of age (Fomon 1974). By 1937, pediatricians were suggesting such foods by four to six months of age (A.M.A. Council on Foods 1937). The extreme was reached by the 19503 when Sackett published his "new concept in baby feeding" (1953), which prescribed solids on the second day of life. Since that time, surveys of medical textbooks and practicing physicians show a return to the 19303 recommendation of four to six months as the appropriate age for solids (Jelliffe and Jelliffe 1979b). This is also the most recent recommendation of the American Academy of Pediatrics (1980). In reviewing changing patterns of infant feeding, it is evident that trends in the relative proportions of infants consuming breast milk or formula and in ages for introduction of solids have tended to coincide (American Academy of Pediatrics 1978). This suggests that infant 10 feeding has been influenced by changes in medical wisdom through time. It also suggests that the entire process may be responsive to general changes in lifestyles external to the health professions. The amount of information available to physicians relevant to infant feeding increased rapidly at the beginning of this century. New (although not always accurate) data on thecomposition of human milk led to what has been called the "chemical period" (Meyer 1952). Rotch, for example, advocated."percentage feeding," in which minute proportions of different nutrients in infant milks were calculated (1907). Milk prescriptions were in vogue, and the intricacies of these--some were over a page in length--were responsible for the term "formula" which is still in use today. Knowledge about rates of gastric emptying and curd tension of different kinds of milk contributed to the scientification of infant feeding. While the official medical policy on issues such as breast- feeding versus artificial milks remained unchanged, the increasing research emphasis on artificial feeding and its inclusion in medical texts probably helped make physicians more amenable to the use of such methods when advocated by the public. Research on lactation or any aspect of infant feeding dealing with the mother was conspicuously absent in the first half of the century. When writers did start to mention mothers, it was usually in the context of easing their ll workload through reducing the number of infant feedings per day, often by lowering the age at which solid foods were introduced (cf. Clein 1943, Sackett 1953). However, this one-sided view of infant feeding was replaced to some extent in the 19703 by an interest in the interactions inherent in the total infant feeding process (e.g., maternal-infant interactions, food interactions). Paralleling the changes in infant feeding through the 19003 have been broader trends in life styles and popular conceptions about infants. The increased use of infant formulas, first mixed at home and later commercially pre— pared, was part of a general increased reliance on things scientific and technologically modern. Time and labor saving devices were sought in all areas of women's work in the home, and the use of formula and canned baby foods fed on a rigid schedule was consistent with this. At the same time, the conceptualization of the infant changed. Writers of the early 19003 (cf. Rotch 1907) referred to infants as totally helpless and vulnerable creatures. By the 19403 and 19503 this view had changed. Child rearing practices were seen as the time in which lifelong habits were taught. The emphasis was on conformity, on orderly patterns of behavior, and on molding the willful infant into his role as a member first of a family and later of society (Aldrich 1942; Clein 1943; Sackett 1953). However, since the 19603, reaction against this trend has become widespread, as an emphasis on naturalism has 12 become a pervasive force in certain segments of Western society. For diet in general, this has included advocating a reduced consumption of processed and fortified foods and for infants and mothers in particular, a return to breast- feeding. Role models for this naturalism in food consump- tion come from a variety of sources, including pOpular, romanticized notions about the ways in which technologically simple societies function. Part of these ideas focus on the child as a free, self-determining spirit. This has resulted in a widespread belief that breast-feeding should be carried out on demand from immediately after birth for as long as several years. Other foods should be restricted until at least the second six months of life, a time when the infant has enough muscular control to regulate its own intake. Infant diet research has tended to follow these popular influences. Articles on the long term adequacy of breast milk are now increasingly common (Ahn and MacLean 1980; Saarinen and Siimes 1979). Yet despite the current medical recommendations for extended breast-feeding and delayed non-breast milk foods and despite the vocal nature of the support this receives from many mothers, data from a number of sources (Fomon et a1. 1979; Kemberling 1979; American Academy of Pediatrics 1978) indicate that 40 to 50% of all newborns are formula- fed rather than breast-fed at hospital discharge and that most are fed some non-milk solid foods by two months of age. This contradiction has led to a new dimension of research-- 13 the study of why infants are fed in different ways. Current Research Directions Recently there have begun to appear reports of research designed to discover the rationale for different patterns of infant feeding from the side of those directly re- sponsible for implementing such action: parents, particu- 1arly mothers. These include studies of the following decisions: to breast- or bottle-feed from birth, when to cease breast-feeding, and when to introduce solid foods into the infant diet. The first decision--to breast-feed or use formula-- appears to be made well before delivery. Guthrie and Kan (1977) report that among unmarried and non-pregnant females up to 21 years of age in Pennsylvania, most had first thought about their future choice in infant feeding methods between the ages of 12 and 17. Furthermore, all of the oldest women could articulate reasons for their choice beyond simple previous exposure to the infant feeding method. This indication that the bottle/breast-feeding decision is made long prior to delivery is supported by Brimblecombe and Cullen (1977). .They report that a concerted effort by midwives, public health visitors, and physicians to increase the rate of mothers choosing to breast-feed in 1975 was unsuccessful. Rates of breast-feeding were not significantly different in before and after surveys. The most significant factors in these mothers' feeding decisions were the methods by which they had fed previous infants and the way they l4 themselves had been fed as children. Postpartum decisions on the length of breast-feeding have been investigated in New Zealand by Hood et a1. (1978) and in Sweden by Sjolin et a1. (1977, 1979). In a retro- spective survey of 574 New Zealand mothers, Hood et a1. (1978) found that 26% of the weanings (defined by the authors as the complete cessation of breast-feeding) were said by the mother to be voluntary. Either she had breast- fed as long as she wanted to, or the infant had "weaned itself." The latter usually occurred after six months of age. Thirty-seven percent reported weaning because of "concerns with the milk supply," and nine percent because of "maternal stress." The categories "maternal illness," "nipple problems," and "other breast problems" accounted for 10%, and several other miscellaneous reasons accounted for the remaining mothers. In another retrospective survey of 298 Swedish mothers, Sjolin et a1. (1977) found that the following accounted for over 40% of the decisions to wean: 1) concern about the child's getting sufficient milk, 2) anxiety and insecurity, 3) lack of motivation, and 4) stress. There is no indication whether these are actual reasons given by the mothers, categories presented to them from which to choose, or whether the authors grouped the mothers' responses as they saw fit. A prospective study of individual breast-feeding ex- periences among 75 Swedish mothers was designed to explore in detail the factors leading to weaning (Sjolin et al.1979). 15 These factors were categorized as emotional, rational, and medical reasons connected with the mother; environmental reasons; and the interplay of maternal and environmental reasons. However, the distinctions among these categories are not clear, nor are sufficient statistical computations done to show whether or not each of these categories is specific to any group of children or age of weaning. There have been very few studies of the decisiOn to add solids (and other non-milk foods) to the infant diet. Auerbach (1978), in a study of 102 American mothers, notes that infants on mixed breast milk and formula diets tend to be fed solids much earlier than those on a diet con- sisting exclusively of breast milk. The results of Wilkinson and Davies' (1978) study of British mothers support those findings and indicate that maternal perceived hunger is the most common reason for starting solids, regardless of composition of the milk portion of the diet. While the emphasis on maternal decision making and behavior in infant feeding is recent and thus the studies cited above do much to open up this area of research, several important shortcomings must be mentioned. First, the research generally focuses on a single food or food category. For example, Brimblecombe and Cullen concentrate only on the effect of nutrition education on the breast/bottle decision. Yet, delayed feeding of solids is normally part of the breast-feeding education message and may in fact have been effectively transmitted. Similarly, Sjolin et a1. (1979) 16 purport to examine causes of lactation crises and weaning, without ascertaining the composition of the infant diet beyond breast milk. A second and related problem with many of the studies is that foods are viewed in presence/absence terms. Women in the Hood et a1. (1978) study are listed as breast-feeding at up to three years postpartum. Obviously other foods are being consumed by the child, and having more information about them would make it possible to interpret reasons for weaning in the appropriate context, as would such details as daily frequency of breast-feeding sessions. A final problem is that while factors such as "maternal stress," "insufficient milk supply," and "poor infant growth" constitute a major proportion of the reasons given for wean- ing or adding solids to the diet, these are rarely explored in any detail. Even though some studies did collect infant anthropometric data (Wilkinson and Davies 1978; Brimblecombe and Cullen 1977), no attempts were made to associate specific decision making factors with growth parameters. One reason for the failure to examine these categories in any depth may be the orientation of those conducting the research. The majority are either physicians or public health workers coming out of a medical training background. It may be that while such training sensitizes them to the subtle distinctions in some categories (e.g., Hood et a1. [1978] have three separate categories related to maternal health), possibly meaningful distinctions in other categories are ignored. 17 Although the studies cited above fail to integrate the factors cited by mothers as reasons for modifying the course of breast-feeding, the interaction of these and their connection via the physiology of lactation have been discussed by others. Jelliffe and Jelliffe (1978) have called attention to the importance of alleviating maternal anxiety in order to insure an adequate oxytocin induced let-down reflex. This, in turn, promotes suckling, which stimulates the prolactin secretion necessary for continued milk production. Gussler and Briesemeister (1980) have developed this in greater detail, concentrating on the specific components of the social and physical environments which, when combined, result in either success- ful lactation or the "insufficient milk syndrome." This Gussler and Briesemeister see as a phenomenon specific to Western society or to those non—Western societies undergoing a transition from traditional to more modern lifestyles. The model they propose for this "syndrome" contains many of the factors cited by mothers in studies such as Hood et al. (1978) and Sjolin et a1. (1977, 1979), but it remains to be verified with a single complete body of behavioral and biological data. Infant Nutrition Research interest in the interaction of infant diet and growth has centered on two main questions. First, are there growth differences between infants breast-fed and those fed artificial formulas? Second, does the age at which infants 18 start to consume solid foods influence their later growth? The way in which these questions have been phrased has dictated the design of the research based on them and has probably contributed to the confusion and controversy surrounding diet and growth of young infants. Milk Consumption and Growth A recent review of this research area is provided by Himes (1979), who notes the conflicting results obtained by investigators studying infants in industrialized countries. Some of the most recent or most frequently cited studies are discussed below. The strongest support for the notion that growth differences exist between breast milk- and formula-fed infants comes from Fomon et a1. (1970, 1971). They studied the growth from 8 to 112 days of 149 infants breast-fed and 154 formula-fed from birth. Breast-fed infants were per- mitted one supplementary formula feeding daily. After 28 days, both groups of infants were permitted strained cereal and fruit on a prescribed schedule of introduction. However, feeding these foods was left up to parental discretion. Although there were no significant differences between groups as a whole in terms of mean weight and length parameters, -differences did appear between groups by 112 days in the distribution of body weight. While the distributions had been the same for breast- and bottle-fed infants at 8 days, at 112 days the 90th percentile for bottleefed infants was "substantially" higher. The 10th percentiles remained equal 19 for the two groups. These results have been supported by results obtained in England by Taitz (1971). In this study, 21 breast-fed and 240 bottle-fed infants were com- pared, and significantly greater weight gain velocity was noted from birth to six weeks among those infants bottle- fed. In contrast to data in the Fomon et a1. studies, Taitz's came entirely from clinic records. There were no exclusions for feeding solid foods in any form or amount or at any age. A number of other studies, however, have found no growth differences between breast milk and formula-fed infants. Thomson (1955) found no significant differences in weight gain to 16 weeks between wholly bottle-fed and wholly breast-fed members of 40 pairs of female infants in Scotland matched for birthweight. Evans (1978) studied 94 normal British infants from birth to six months of age. Each infant consumed either breast milk or one of three commercial formulas. No significant differences in weight, length, and skinfold velocities were found. It was pointed out that the breast-fed infants made the most rapid gains during the first month. No data were given on samp1e~ attrition. In another frequently cited study, deSwiet et a1. (1977) found no effect on body weight of the type of milk fed to 758 infants in Great Britain. Their comparisons were made at six weeks and six months, among groups identi- fied at six weeks as breast-feeding, bottle-feeding, or mixed breast- and bottle-feeding. Some infants in each of 20 these three groups consumed solid food as well. No- significant differences were found at six weeks or at six months. The same three feeding types were studied to nine weeks of age in another sample of 457 British infants (Holly and Cullen 1977). Again, no significant differences in weight could be found between feeding groups. Despite the lack of significant results, an examination of the data shows differences in the patterns of growth. The mixed feeding group is most notable. Although it has a lower growth rate than either of the other two feeding groups in the first month, these infants recover and by nine weeks are over-represented in the highest decile of weight for age. Saarinen and Siimes (1979) followed the growth of 238 Finnish infants during the first 12 months of life. One group of these infants was breast-fed for at least six months, with no solids until three and a half months. These infants were compared with others who were weaned from breast milk to formula by one month of age. These infants also consumed solids after three and a half months. No signifi- cant differences in weight or length were found between these two groups at any point during the study. However, an examination of the data Saarinen and Siimes present shows- differences in the average patterns of weight gain. The extended breast-feeding group gained weight more rapidly for the first two months. After that, its growth velocity slowed relative to the early weaning group, which was on the 21 average heavier from six to twelve months of age. One recent study of 92 female infants has concentrated on the relationship of infant fatfold thickness to feeding modality (Ferris et a1. 1979). Four fatfolds were measured monthly from one to six menths. There were no significant differences among four groups of infants identified at two months as breast-fed or formula-fed, exclusively or with solids. Thus, a review of the literature presents no definitive answers to questions concerning the differential impacts of breast milk and formula on growth. After examination of studies involving growth and solid food, reasons for the inconclusive nature of the breast milk/formula investigations will be discussed. Growth and Consumption of Non-Milk Foods Results of studies designed to answer the question of i the impact of solids on growth are as conflicting as those described above from milk types. Weil (1977) provides a review and critique of some of the older research in this category. Other investigations will be reviewed below. One study which is repeatedly cited as evidence for an impact of solid food on infant weight is that of Shukla et a1. (1972). In it, weight, length, fatfolds, and types of foods consumed were monitored periodically from birth to one year of age in 300 British infants. The investigators found that at all ages, these infants consistently exceeded accepted weight, weight for length, and fatfold standards. Furthermore, 22 they found that 40% had been given solids by four weeks and 93% by 13 weeks. From this, a casual link between the con— sumption of solids and overweight was inferred. Almost all these infants also consumed formula, but the investigators failed to determine whether or not the solid food was replacing or truly supplementing this intake. Of the more recent studies of growth and solids, only a few confirm the 1972 findings of Shukla et al. One of these is Hodgson (1978). In this, 301 British infants were weighed at birth, six weeks, six months, and one year of age. Dietary histories were collected at unspecified times. Hodgson divided the infants into the following groups by date of first solid food: earlier than six weeks, six weeks to three months, and later than three months- At six months, infant weight of the first group was found to be significantly greater than that of the last. Hodgson thus concluded that early introduction of solid food leads to increased weight gain in infancy. Davies et al. (1977) studied 821 British infants and used the same age divisions for introduction of solids as those used by Hodgson (1978). In this study, however, no significant differences in weight or length were found. In a later study of only 50 infants, Wilkinson and Davies (1978) again found no statistically Significant relation- ship of growth parameters to date of introduction of solids. Thus, studies exist to support either side of the question of the impact of solids on infant growth.' 23 Underlying Assumptions The confusing and conflicting results of the studies discussed above appear to be derived, at least in part, from the nature of the assumption on which the research questions are based. There are at least five such assump- tions, not all of equal impact and significance, and each will be discussed in turn. The first assumption implicit in this research is that infants in dietary studies can be treated as though they are randomly assigned to feeding groups. If this assumption were valid, then there should be no differences, for example, in demographic variables between infants breast- or bottle-fed. However, Sauls (1979), in a review of the infant feeding and morbidity literature, has shown that consistent differences do exist. At present in the U.S., compared with bottle-feeders, breast-feeding mothers are likely to be older, married mothers, who are healthy, non- smokers, not employed outside the home, and not receiving any type of governmental assistance. Furthermore, the educational level of breast-feeding mothers is generally higher than that of bottleefeeders (Cunningham 1977). .Such differences make it likely that basic differences in pre- natal influences, postnatal health care, and other environ- mental factors exist. These may, in turn, be confounding factors in studies which do not control for them. A corollary of the first assumption of random assign- ment of infants to feeding groups is the assumption that 24 "breastéfed" and "bottle-fed" are the same sort of categories. Since, as Himes (1979) has stated, there is a one-way passage of infants from the breast-fed to the bottle-fed category, the former is a highly selected group. This is particularly obvious in Ahn and MacLean's (1980) study of growth in exclusively breast-fed infants. Their sample size decreases rapidly after five months of age; at eight months only 28 of 96 infants remain. There is no way of knowing whether or not actual or perceived growth crises were responsible for the attrition. Thirty percent of the breast-fed infants studied by Fomon et al. (1970) dropped out of the project by day 112, compared to an attrition rate of only 8% among formula-fed infants (1971). A comparison of those leaving the breast-feeding study with those continuing to participate showed the former to be on the average lower in weight in the early weeks of the study than the latter. Fomon et a1. (1970) report that at least some attrition was due to failure to thrive on breast milk. Bottle-feeding, in contrast with breast-feeding, is more of a cumulative group than a selected one. In studies such as Saarinen and Siimes (1979), many of the infants in the bottle- fed category had originally been breast-fed, but for one month or less. The proportion of these weaned due to growth in- adequacies is unstated. However, based on data of Hood et a1. (1978) and Sjolin et a1. (1979), it may have been quite high. This may, in turn, have confounded the results of growth data analyses. 25 The second implicit assumption in these studies is that a onefway, casual relationship exists between infant diet and growth. That is, dietary manipulations lead to growth differences. This orientation is contrary to what most clinicians would acknowledge to really be the case: that dietary changes are made when growth has deviated from an expected norm. While a growth correction following the diet change is expected, it was a growth, rather than a dietary, action which originally set the chain of events in motion. Only attending to the one-way effect of diet on growth may obscure the interrelationships of infant diet and growth. The issue can be further confused by the way in which infant dietary intake data are collected. Twenty-four hour diet recalls and diaries such as those used by Shukla et a1. (1972) and Evans (1978) provide information for only that short period of time. There is no way of knowing whether the diets reported represent long term patterns of feeding-- and thus "causes" of the ob3erved growth--or recent feeding changes which might be "effects" of previous growth. This problem can be overcome to some extent by increasing the frequency of the diet recalls or, as Ferris et a1. (1979) have done, by taking a diet history at the same time. However, the investigator must still decide which information is to be given priority in assigning infants to feeding groups. And his or her assumptions about the interrelationships of infant diet and growth will help to inform that decision. 26 This can be a fairly serious confounding factor if feeding changes tend to coincide with times chosen for dietary data collection. This problem has been described by Ritenbaugh and Harrison (1980) for a study of Arizona infants in which dietary and growth data were obtained at six month intervals, often at approximately the same time as a regular medical examination at which a diet change was prescribed. In these cases several days difference in the researchers' appointment scheduling made little difference in anthropo- metric data, but resulted in major shifts in dietary intake. Thus, the fact that most of the British infant studies are conducted by physicians collecting data at regular well- baby checkups, while most of the American studies are not, may explain some of the conflicting results obtained. At least it indicates another set of problems to be considered in interpreting such results. A third assumption about diet and growth made by several researchers is that excessive weight indicates fat- ness. Although it is widely recognized that the contribution of fat to body weight is less than that of the same volume of lean body tissue, the confusion of body size with body composition persists. Taitz (1971), for example, uses no measure of fat, yet talks about the prevalence of over- nutrition and obesity in the study population. It is, in fact, quite possible for individuals of the same body weight to have significantly different percentages of body fat (Brozek 1963). 27 A fourth assumption is that when solids are introduced into the diet, they are nutritional additions to the exist- ing dietary base. It is this assumption which has been behind research designed to identify the suspected con- nection between early solids and obesity. However, Marlin et al. (1980) have shown that, at least until the age of 12 weeks, solids actually replace either breast milk or formula in caloric terms. They conducted a longitudinal study of 39 infants fed a diet selected by the mother. Mothers kept three-day diet diaries for their infants at two, four, eight and twelve weeks.’ Breast milk volume was obtained by pre- and post-feeding infant weighings, and its nutrient composition was estimated using values published by Fomon (1974). When caloric intake of the entire diet was calculated, there were no significant differences at any age betWeen those consuming solids and those consuming only breast milk and/or formula. The research practice of attending only to single foods-or food categories has led to this ignorance of the impact of dietary change on the existing diet. It is not known whether formula also acts as a nutritional addition or replacement in a breast milk diet and whether solids continue to be replacements at ages of greater than three months. A final assumption inherent in much of the research on infant nutrition conditions the tone in which results are discussed and evaluated. It is the-assumption that "normal growth" exists as a constant standard against which growth 28 or research subjects and samples can be evaluated. Shukla et a1. (1972) and others discovering high rates of "over- nutrition" in Great Britain in the late 19603 and early 19703 assumed this, and never investigated what their subjects' idea of normal growth was or whether their own medical notion of normal growth had changed through time. Normal growth does, of course, follow certain curves at different ages or stages of maturation. However, both the magnitude and amplitude of the curves can vary. The boundaries between "normal" and "abnormal" variability are culturally defined. In the past in almoSt all cultures and continuing to the present in many, "bigness" in infants had been considered healthy and desirable. Bryant (1978) found Anglo, Puerto Rican, and Cuban mothers in Dade County, Florida still using evaporated milk formula because it "fattens" babies better. Weil (1977) describes a group of mothers he labels "more nutritionally;sophisticated" who fed diets designed to produce fatter infants due to their concerns about the possibility of brain damage resulting from underfeeding. In contrast, among the medical community and other segments of the public, such healthy looking (by one standard) babies have recently come to be labeled obese and considered at risk for adult obesity, heart disease, and reduced lifespan (of. Charney et a1. 1976; Kannel and Dawber 1972). Adair (1980) found white, middle- class Philadelphia mothers manipulating their infants' growth patterns through sometimes extreme dietary means to 29 produce the thin infants now considered "normal" by some physicians. Thus the findings of Fomon et a1. (1971), - Hodgson (1978), and others cited above that different patterns of infant feeding produce statistically significant differences in infant growth should not necessarily lead to the conclusions that these differences: 1) are culturally meaningful, or 2) are indicative of abnormalities in the infants' growth. These five assumptions, then, have shaped the research conducted on infant growth and diet interactions in recent years. Like any research base, they have in some ways limited and selected for the research results which have been derived, and they have contributed to the ambiguity of those results. Maternal Nutritional Status Several long term studies exist of lactation and maternal nutritional status of women living under con- ditions of nutritional stress. A British study in the Gambia, West Africa, for example, has identified annual patterns of maternal weight change, birthweight, and lac- tation performance and has correlated these with seasonal fluctuations in disease, rainfall, and food supply para- meters (Rowland 1978; Rowland and Paul 1981; Whitehead et a1. 1978). In an experimental study in Guatemala, the effects of different nutritional supplementation plans on reproductive performance have been studied (Lechtig et a1. 1975; Newman et al. 1980). In addition to these, a large 30 body of knowledge exists specifically on the composition of human milk under conditions of nutritional stress. Jelliffe and Jelliffe (1978) have published a review of this literature. Together, the studies cited above and others like them have provided a general picture of the average pat- terns of maternal nutritional status and milk production in populations existing under sub-optimal conditions. What are missing, however, are comparable baseline studies of lactation and the postpartum period in well-nourished women in which the dynamics of maternal nutritional status, including milk production, are documented. This section will review the few studies of well-nourished mothers which can be found in the literature. It will then con- sider evidence from a variety of other types of studies which point to possible patterns of change during the postpartum period among lactating women and the ways in which these may interact with milk production and, hence, infant growth. Breast-Feeding and Postpartum Weight Loss It is widely assumed that women store nutrients during pregnancy which are later used to meet the demands of lac- tation and that, as these stores are mobilized, weight loss occurs. Twenty years ago Hytten and Thomson commented that there was "very little evidence in the literature on this rather elementary point" (1961:27). The evidence that did exist at the time of their review was inconclusive, showing 31 that women both gained and lost weight during lactation. Hytten and Thomson also presented their own data on mean weight change of 37 primiparae who lactated more than 10 weeks and 35 who lactated less than three weeks (1966). Between the second and thirteenth weeks postpartum most women lost weight. Those breast-feeding longer tended to lose more weight than those breast-feeding for a limited time. However, the distribution of weight change is wide in both groups ranging from losses of over twelve pounds to gains of more than eight. Furthermore, no data on pregnancy weight gain or supplementation of breast milk are given to help interpret these results. Research since 1961 to substantiate the alleged association of rapid weight loss and breast-feeding is equally meager. Dennis and Bytheway (1965) combined post- partum weight loss data gathered for other reasons in a number of different studies. They show that between ten days and ten weeks postpartum lactating women lose on the average 13 pounds more than non-lactating women. While this study is widely cited (e.g., Jelliffe and Jelliffe 1979), it is based on a highly selected sample of 17 subjects. All women with any pregnancy edema, heavy blood loss at parturition, or cesarean delivery were excluded. While such exclusions are reasonable in a research context, they may invalidate or greatly limit any promises of weight loss to individual women based on this research. 32 Naismith and Ritchie (1976) report postpartum weight changes in 22 lactating and 20 non-lactating women. Group weight losses from hospital discharge to three months postpartum average 2.6 kg and 2.9 kg, respectively. By six months, the nonflactating women had a mean cumulative weight loss of 4.4 kg, compared to only 2.7 kg for those lactating. These findings run counter to the assumed relationship of postpartum weight change and breast-feeding. However, the investigators note that dieting was reported more frequently among non-lactating than lactating women. This points to one of the major factors confounding such studies. In non-laboratory samples, dietary intake cannot be standardized. Blackburn and Galloway (1976) studied energy intake and expenditures in 12 mothers at various stages of lactation and five controls who were not lac- tating. They found that breast-feeding women consumed an average of 30 kcal/kg and expended the same amount on non- lactation activities. In contrast, the non-lactating women consumed an average of only 19 kcal/kg, while expending 34 kcal/kg. If these energy consumption patterns are typical of postpartum women in general, they may explain the failure of postpartum weight 1033 studies to provide conclusive support for the assumed relationship of breastf feeding to maternal body weight. Breast-Feeding and Changes in Maternal Body Fat The primary component of weight loss expected during lactation is fat. Taggart et a1. (1967), in a longitudinal 33 study of primagravidae in Scotland, documented the regular growth of adipose tissue during pregnancy. It has been reasoned that this is an adaptive mechanism to support breast feeding, and the automatic assumption of fat loss during lactation follows. Popular publications (e.g., LaLeche International 1963) promise women that the fat will be "burned" during lactation and health pro- fessionals predict a "slimming effect" (Jelliffe and Jelliffe l979b:1l3). However, studies of infant feeding and postpartum changes in maternal fatness are virtually non-existent. A review of the literature yielded a single research report. In the study by Naismith and Ritchie (1976) described above, biceps, triceps, subscapular, and superiliac fatfolds were measured and used to compute total body fat. No distinct patterns of change in total body fat were found which could be correlated with lac— tation status. Despite the lack of supporting evidence, this assumed loss of fat during lactation has been used uncritically as the basis for other proposed interactions, most notably the critical body composition hypothesis of Frisch and colleagues (cf. Frisch et al. 1973; Frisch and McArthur 1974). According to this hypothesis, a certain minimum level of body fat is necessary for onset and maintenance of ovulatory cycles. This is alleged to be a determining factor in menarche as well as in the length of amenorrhea during lactation. 34 Although what is commonly believed about the relation- ship of infant feeding to maternal fatness is based more on logic than on solid evidence, there are a series of interactions documented in the literature which suggest relationships between maternal body fat and a number of components of the infant feeding process. The following paragraphs review this evidence. One breast-feeding variable--the daily frequency of nursing-fhas been linked to levels of prolactin and gonadal hormones in mothers. These in turn have been correlated with maternal fatness and with the quality and quantity of milk produced. In both animal and human studies, the frequency of nursing periods has been found to be directly related to the level of serum prolactin. Grosvenor et a1. (1967) found that frequent suckling was necessary to maintain pituitary prolactin release in laboratory rats. The length of the suckling stimulus had no effect on prolactin level. Delvoye et a1. (1977) studied a group of 97 nursing mothers in Zaire and obtained similar findings. Serum prolactin levels rose with frequency of daily nursing. Moreover, nursing frequency was also related to the length of time mean prolactin levels remained elevated after parturition. Women nursing seven or more times per day maintained high prolactin levels for a year. Those nursing four to six times per day returned to non-lactating levels by about ten months, and those nursing less than four times per 35 day, by six months. The relationship of nursing bout frequency to serum concentrations of the gonadal hormones estrogen and pro- gesterone has also been documented. Among lactating lKung mothers, Konner and Worthman (1980) found that high nursing frequency was associated with low levels of estrogen and progesterone. Less frequent nursing correlated with higher hormone levels. They hypothesize that this is a result of the suppression by prolactin of gonadal hormone production. (Tyson [1977] reviews the details of this interaction.) Among the EKung, total nursing time and length of feedings were uncorrelated with hormone levels. The time interval between the initiation of nursing bouts was the crucial variable. Variations both in the gonadal hormone progesterone and in prolactin have been linked to body fat. An inverse relationship exists between serum prolactin and progesterone: the higher the prolactin, the lower the progesterone pro- duction. Studies in both humans (Hytten and Leitch 1971) and rats (Galletti and Klopper 1964) show that increasing the level of progesterone induces fat deposition. Thus, the variations in progesterone levels mediated during lactation by prolactin would be expected to have an impact on fat levels. Those women nursing frequently should be less disposed to increase fat stores than those women nursing infrequently. 36 Prolactin is associated with body fat through the activity of lipoprotein lipase, the enzyme responsible for the uptake of dietary lipids into adipocytes. In studies of laboratory animals, prolactin has been found to induce and maintain shifts in lipoprotein lipase activity level in different body sites (Zinder et a1. 1974; Scow et a1. 1973). When prolactin is at normal low levels, lipOprotein lipase activity is high in the peripheral layers of adipose tissue. However, when prolactin levels are elevated, there is a suppression of lipoprotein lipase activity in adipose tissue and a concomitant increase in activity in the mammary glands. Thus, when nursing frequency is high, serum prolactin concentration should be high also, causing dietary lipids to be diverted from body stores to the mammary glands. When nursing frequency is low, uptake of lipids by peripheral adipose tissue should be closer to normal levels. It is well known that the frequency of nursing bouts has an impact on the quantity of milk produced. Egli et a1. (1961) in a frequently cited study varied the feedings per day of a woman who was successfully lactating. They noted reduced milk output following a reduction in feedings and an increase to original levels of milk production three days after the number of feedings returned to normal. The cause of such variations in milk production is assumed to be the corresponding variation in prolactin, which causes milk secretion in the alveoli. 37 In addition to affecting breast milk quantity, the frequency of feedings affects milk quality. Tyson et a1. (1972) found that artificially stimulated prolactin release in lactating women was followed by increased fat content of their breast milk. The mechanism for this may be the prolactin-induced increases in mammary lipoprotein lipase activity. Therefore, it appears that infant growth should also be affected by the ongoing interactions of feeding frequency and maternal physiological parameters during the postpartum period. Thus, the literature suggests that the relationship between breast-feeding and maternal nutritional status is considerably more complex than the simple loss of weight and fat often assumed to occur during lactation. It appears that attention to specific behavioral aspects of breast-feeding may help to reveal patterns of maternal biological response. These responses, in turn, may affect the way in which infants grow. Summary This review of the literature has indicated that a complex interaction exists among the components of the infant feeding process: infant diet, infant nutrition, and maternal nutritional status. It has further shown that different aspects of this interaction are better understood than others. This chapter has attempted to indicate what those aspects are and why the remaining ones have either not been studied or, having been studied, have produced 38 contradictory results. Studies of maternal decision making in infant diet indicate that perceptions of infant growth patterns are a. major factor in mothers' choice of feeding strategies. However, infant growth data are generally analyzed as though only a one-way relationship between diet and growth exists. Because of this and other assumptions about the relationship of infant diet to growth, few conclusions can be drawn at present about this interaction. The aspect of the relationship of infant diet to maternal nutritional status about which most is known is the average composition and volume of milk produced by malnourished mothers. However, the dynamics of milk production in individual women-~particularly in well- nourished women--are relatively unknown. The effect of infant feeding on maternal nutritional status is widely assumed to be a gradual draining of body stores, but research on the hormones involved in lactation indicates that this may be overly simplistic. This research hints that different patterns of change in maternal nutritional status may accompany different styles of breast-feeding. To investigate these requires attention to behavioral as well as biological data, and to the ways in which these two types of phenomenon interact in individual women. CHAPTER I I I METHODS This chapter describes the research undertaken to meet the objectives identified in Chapter I. After a discussion of the overall research plan, it details the sample and the methods by which it was obtained. Data collection procedures are described, and the rationales for their use discussed. The final section of the chapter presents the justification for combining various types of data. Research Design Because the objectives of this research are to examine the interaction of infant diet and growth and maternal postpartum nutritional status through time, a longitudinal design was selected. Such a research strategy has a dis- tinct advantage over the less time-consuming cross-sectional design in that it enables one to document individual courses of growth or behavior, thus clarifying the nature of the relationship between events at one time and those at another. The research reported here actually comes from longi- tudinal studies of two samples from a single population. The first, an exploratory study referred to hereafter as 39 40 Study I, was conducted in central Michigan from January 1978 through January 1979. Its purpose was to examine general relationships between infant diet and growth. The data collected in Study I were subjected to only minimal analysis (Quandt and Ritenbaugh 1979). Further analysis was postponed because, in the course of the study, it became apparent that a number of potentially interesting problems in the data could be explored only with the collection of additional behavioral data. Thus, the second study (Study II) was conducted from August 1979 through February 1981. It continued the same data collection strategy used in Study I, but added other items to the research protocol which were designed to ex- plore the problem areas which had emerged during Study I.- It should be stressed that although it was hoped the two samples could be combined into one larger one, Study II was not in any way intended to increase sample size to improve levels of statistical significance of some already recognized associations. It was planned that, if no differences were found between the two studies in basic data (e.g., pregnancy characteristics, parental size), it could be assumed that they were samples from the same population. Thus, while different frequencies of behaviors might exist at the times of the two studies--and indeed would be expected given the rapid changes which take place in infant feeding norms-- this would not preclude their being combined into a single 41 sample. Biologically similar individuals should react in similar ways to the same diet or behavior at different times (Bass et a1. 1979). On the other hand, it was assumed that for any analysis examining cultural factors and their changes through time, the two studies could legitimately be separated into two independent groups. The Sample Description of the Sample The total sample consisted of 79 mothers and 80 neonates. All infants weighed at least 2,500 grams at birth and were products of at least 36 week pregnancies. Forty-five of these mothers and 46 infants (44 singleton infants and one set of fraternal twins) participated in Study I. The remaining 34 mothers and 34 infants were in Study II. Table III.1 shows the number of infants by sex, Study, and month of participation. The diminishing sample size is due to mothers moving from the area. This group of subjects was taken from a larger sample, drawn to study other pregnancy and postpartum issues, which included infants never breast-fed, infants born prematurely, and infants whose mothers left the study area before two months postpartum. 42 TABLE III.l—-Number of Infants Measured at Birth and at each Month. Month‘ Sample Birth 1 2 3 4 5 6 TOTAL SAMPLE Total N 80 80 80 79 76 75 72 Females 43 43 43 42 41 40 40 Males 37 37 37 37 35 35 32 STUDY I Total N 46 46 46 45 44 43 41 Females 27 27 27 26 25 24 24 Males 19 l9 l9 l9 l9 19 17 STUDY II Total N 34 34 34 34 32 32 31 Females 16 16 16 16 16 16 16 Males 18 18 18 l8 l6 16 15 The sample used in this study represents a predominantly middle class population. The annual household incomes ranged from $5,000 to over $40,000; the median falls in the $20,000 to $24,999 category (Table III.2). The median income was slightly higher in Study II than in Study I. However, con- sidering the inflation rate during the two years between studies, the effective income can be considered equal. Maternal education ranged from high school graduate to Ph.D. The modal category was composed of women who had completed a bachelor's degree program (Table III.3). 43 TABLE III.2--Gross Household Income by Study and for Total Sample. ~ - ---- Study-II-- Study II Total Income Category N N N (1) $5,000 - 9,999 3 2 5 (2) $10,000 - 14,999 9 4 l3 (3) $15,000 — 19,999 11 7 .18 (4) $20,000 — 24,999 10 12 22 (5) $25,000 - 29,999 7 5 12 (6) $30,000 - 39,999 6 2 8 (7) $40,000 0 2 2 TABLE III.3--Highest Level of Maternal Formal Education by Study and for Total Sample. Study I Study II Total Education Category N N N (1) High School or 10 9 . 19 Vocational School (2) Some College (Less 10 ' 8 18 than Bachelor's Degree) (3) Bachelor's Degree 16 ll 27 (4) Any Graduate School 10 6 l6 44 Sample Recruitment Throughout the sample recruitment, care was taken not to influence potential participants' choices of feeding modality or to differentially attract women with different feeding plans. Women asked to participate in the study were told that the researcher had a number of interests that involved new mothers and their infants. They were told that she was aware that there were many different ways in which mothers fed their infants and that she was trying to find out what these feeding methods were. She was also interested in the way infants grew and how that might be related to the way they were fed. Finally, they were told that some mothers might be breast-feeding their infants and, in those cases, maternal postpartum anthropo- metric data might help to explain infant growth. Since comparisons between those breast-feeding and those not breast-feeding were useful, they were told, the researcher would like to monitor postpartum changes in all mothers. Slightly different procedures for enrolling mothers were followed in Studies I and II. Women participating in Study II were identified during pregnancy with the assistance of obstetricians in private practice in the Lansing area. The physicians agreed to give letters explaining the study (Appendix A) to patients who met the criteria for the study. These criteria are discussed below. These women were then contacted by telephone by the investigator, who further explained the research and asked their participation. The 45 rate of refusal was approximately 20%. In Study I, women already enrolled in a study of pregnancy weight gain were given a letter by the 36th week of pregnancy requesting their participation in a study of infant growth and dietary practices (Appendix A). These women had originally been recruited through most of the same physicians and in the same way as described for Study II. The rate of refusal to participate in the preg— nancy weight gain study was approximately 10%; that for the follow-up infant study was 8%. In order to be included in either study, women had to meet certain criteria. First, only Caucasian women were contacted in order to minimize possible genetic differences in growth patterns. Second, each woman had to be either a primipara or, if a multipara, expecting her first liveborn child. This requirement was an attempt to, minimally at least, standardize experience in child- rearing. Finally, the women were expected by their physicians to have no pregnancy complications which might adversely affect the health or development of the infant. There were no criteria related to mode of infant feeding, and the fact that breast-feeding was of primary interest was not mentioned during any of the study recruitment or during the course of the research. Mothers read and signed statements of informed consent (Appendix A) at their first meeting with the investigator. 46 Data Collection All data were collected in the subjects' homes at their convenience. S. Quandt collected all Study II data and about 80% of that in Study I. C. Ritenbaugh collected the remaining 20%. Maternal Data Collection The nutritional status of mothers was assessed anthro- pometrically eight times: once during the third trimester of pregnancy (a maximum of two weeks before delivery), early in the postpartum, and at monthly intervals from one to six months postpartum. The early postpartum data were collected at one week after delivery in Study I and at two weeks, in Study II. This change was made to facilitate the collection of infant data described below. Measurements on each of the occasions included weight, mid-arm circumference, and triceps fatfold. Weight was measured to the nearest tenth of a kilogram on an Accu- Weigh portable beam balance. Women were weighed without shoes; they were requested to wear approximately the same weight indoor clothing for each weighing. A steel tape and Lange skinfold calipers were used for the remaining anthro- pometrics. Standard methods for mid-arm circumferences and triceps fatfold described by Weiner and Lourie (1969) were used. Stature was measured using a free-standing anthropometer once during the study. Prepregnant weight was obtained from the mother when the prenatal measurements were made. She 47 was asked to give her best recollection of her weight at the time of conception, rather than her "normal" weight, which might have been different. Although it is possible to measure fatfolds at a variety of sites on the body, triceps was chosen for two reasons. First, unlike sites such as superiliac or midabdomen, triceps is unlikely to be distorted shortly before or after parturition by gross anatomical adjustments related to delivery. Second, triceps, along with mid-arm circumfer- ence enables one to compute upper arm fat area (Gurney and Jelliffe 1973). This is an expression of adiposity which can be used to assess changes in adiposity independent of changes in underlying lean body tissue. It may be parti- cularly important in monitoring changes following pregnancy, which is--to a variable degree in all women--a super- hydrated physiological state. No biochemical or dietary intake data were collected as additional assessments of maternal nutritional status. Although such information would possibly have been valuable, it was decided that the high degree of long term cooperation required for the study as a whole might be jeopardized with the inclusion of these types of data collection. The collection of dietary data is not usually considered invasive. However, it was observed that many of these women were ex: tremely sensitive about their food consumption during the postpartum. Although most had expected to weigh more after pregnancy than before, many were quite unprepared for the 48 proportion of weight left to lose. Furthermore, they experienced conflicting pressures regarding diet. On the one hand, they were told to eat more than usual for breast- feeding. On the other, they were told to eat less than usual to return to their pre-pregnant size and shape. Thus, because of the feelings attached to food intake, it was decided that the collection of such data might endanger the rapport between mothers and researcher necessary for other portions of the study. Mothers were asked their expected delivery dates at the prenatal measuring session, as well as the method by which the dates were calculated. Parental ages at the child's birth as well as socio- economic information were solicited during an interview at the six month measuring session. Total gross household income for the previous year was obtained, as well as highest level of maternal education. The decision to use maternal education was based on research by Green that indicates that in American society, most health related decisions are made by mothers; and thus their education rather than that of fathers correlates most closely with health behaviors (1970). Observations were also made of the style of dwelling in which the mother and infant lived. Since it has been suggested by others that differences in physical environment affect maternal child interaction including breast-feeding in the neonatal period (Klaus and Kennell 1970), the possibility that gross differences in 49 environment might continue to be influential was considered. Infant Data Collection-—Anthrgpometric Infant growth was assessed seven times during the study: in the hospital within 24 hours of birth and at home, monthly from ages one to six months. Weight, length, and eight fatfolds were measured on each occasion. Birth weight was obtained from the hospital chart. Subsequent. weights were calculated to the nearest 0.1 kg by subtracting the weight of the mother from that of the mother holding the child on the beam balance. Infants were weighed naked except for a diaper. Length was measured to the nearest 0.1 cm as the distance on a flat surface from the crown of the head to the soles of the feet. The researcher positioned the child and applied pressure to the infant's knees while another person (usually the mother) held the head still and in the sagittal plane. The triceps fatfold was measured on the posterior of the arm midway between olecranon and acromion. Biceps fatfold was taken at the same level on the anterior surface of the arm over the biceps muscle. The midabdominal fatfold was measured in a vertical line at a point 2 cm lateral to the umbilicus, and the subscapular, below the angle of the scapula on the back. All of these fatfolds were taken bilaterally with the child in a relaxed, recumbant position. The calipers were applied to the fold and read as soon as the needle had stabilized, after the method of Brans et a1. (1974). A total fatfolds variable was calculated by summing each set of fatfolds. This was 50 done to control for assymetry and trunk-extremity variability in fat distribution. Infant Data Collection--Dieta£y Data pertaining to infant dietary intake were obtained through written records kept by the mother and through information elicited during interviews at monthly measure- ment sessions. The emphasis on mothers was not meant to deny the role others--particu1arly fathers--play in infant feeding. Whenever other family members were present during a measuring session, their views were solicited as well as those of the mother. However, since among families such as those in the pOpulation represented by this sample, mothers have the major responsibility for care of newborn infants, most of the data collection strategy concentrated on mothers. Mothers completed 24-hour diet diaries for the infants approximately every eight days from the second week post- partum to the end of the study. These forms were mailed to the mother at the appropriate interval with a stamped, addressed envelOpe in which to return the completed diary. Mothers were asked to complete the form as soon as possible, recording everything the child consumed during a 24-hour period, the times of consumption, and the amount consumed. For breast-feeding, they were asked to note the minutes of active suckling for each nursing bout. Time and length of sleep episodes were also noted to provide information on feeding frequency. 51 Of the numerous methods used by researchers to collect dietary intake data, the diet diary system was chosen because it seemed best able to provide the data on infant feeding behaviors desired for this investigation while minimizing problems inherent in many recall reporting techniques. Data were reported frequently enough to allow reasonable verification of dates of dietary changes such as first introduction of solids, breast milk to formula transitions, and elimination of night feedings. Had information on specific nutrient intake been desired, more or less frequent reporting might have been required. With the eight day interval, food behaviors specific to a parti- cular day of the week were not overrepresented. Because the data were recorded by the mother or caretaker as the day progressed, the problems of accurately remembering numerous similar events were avoided. The level of cooperation of mothers in completing and returning the diet diaries was generally high (Appendix B). The median number of diaries returned was 15. Mothers tended to be more conscientious about the forms during the first months of their participation. It became apparent within the first few weeks postpartum that certain mothers-- often despite their professed good intentions--were never going to return many of the diet diaries. In these cases other means of obtaining the data were intensified. These methods are described below. 52 The second source of infant diet data is information obtained during both unstructured and formal interviews and through observations conducted whenever the investi- gator met with subjects. In Study I, observations and unstructured interviews took place at all postpartum measuring sessions. Questions were asked to verify changes in infant feeding which had appeared on the diet diaries since the previous meeting. In cases where no diet diaries had been completed, specific questions about the infant's usual diet-were asked. At the sixth month measuring session a more formal interview took place during which mothers were asked to give as detailed a diet history from birth to six months for the infant as they could remember. They were also questioned about their own re- covery from pregnancy and childbirth (e.g., when did they resume a normal schedule of activities; when did they feel like they were really "back on their feet?) Responses to questions at six months were written during the inter- view. On all occasions prior to that, notes were written after leaving the subject's home. In Study II the content of the postpartum interviews was expanded. In addition to using measuring sessions to verify content of the diet diaries, information was elicited on the decision making process related to each dietary change. While the sequence and wording of questions varied, in general the investigator tried to establish why the change had been made, whether or not there had been input 53 from family members or others, and whether or not any behavioral or physical change observable in the child preceded or followed the dietary change. Much of this discussion took place while the child was being measured 30, as in Study I, responses were recorded after leaving the home. At six months a formal final interview was conducted with Study II mothers. They were asked to give a diet history for their infants and describe the decision making process involved at the various transition points. The same questions on maternal recovery were asked as had been asked in Study I. Each mother was also asked to describe her infant's body shape by choosing from a set of six infant outlines varying from thin to fat (Appendix C) the one she thought looked most like her own child. In any case where a mother did not complete a diet diary for a particular time period for which data is analyzed in later chapters, one of two possible actions was taken during the analysis. If data were available from interviews during that time, such data were used and the mother or infant included in the analysis. If none were available, the subjects were excluded from that parti- cular analysis. Data from interviews on types of foods consumed and dates of introducing new foods or weaning were considered of acceptable accuracy. Data on numbers of feedings per day and length of feedings were not. Hence, the numbers of cases varies somewhat in those data analyses 54 involving any of the infant feeding data. Statistical Considerations and Rgporting Reliability Levels of Statistical Significance The statistics used throughout this dissertation vary with data being analyzed. Anthropometric data meet the conditions of statistics which assume a normal distribution and an interval or ratio level of measurement. However, many of the other data--e.g., socioeconomic measures, numbers of breast-feedings--do not meet such assumptions. Therefore, nonparametric tests are used in their analysis. At various points throughout the description of the results (Chapter IV), specific statistical treatments will be done, based on subgroupings of data derived in the course of the data analysis. These statistical treatments will be de- scribed in the apprOpriate section of Chapter IV. When the results of statistical tests are reported in this dissertation, the actual significance level associated with each test will be given. Any results at or below the 0.05 level of probability that the relationship is due to chance will be considered significant. This level was chosen because many of the tests used involve nonparametric statistics. The weakness of such tests is their relatively low power. That is, they fail to show statistical signifi- cance even when samples being compared are different. Hence, a 0.05 level of probability is normally used in such tests. In some of the description of the data analysis, results significant at the level of up to 0.10 will be 55 discussed, though not described as statistically signifi- cant. This will be done where groups with small sample size are being compared because lower sample sizes, in general, reduce the power of a test. That is, they increase the likelihood of failing to show statistically significant associations where such associations do exist (Siegel 1956). Indicators of Accuracy: Infant Diet Diaries Because the 24-hour diet diary has not been widely reported as a means of collecting infant dietary data and because verification could only be made by further question- ing of the same person who had filled out the diary, several factors were examined which were assumed to indicate accuracy in completing the forms. One indicator was the presence or absence of night feedings. In all cases when a child was reported to be regularly sleeping through the night no night feedings were noted in the diary. More importantly, when many infants were reported to be awakening for night feedings again at about five months of age, the night feedings reappeared on the diet diary forms. A second indicator of reporting accuracy was evidence that the diet diaries were completed as ongoing records rather than as a mother's recollections at the end of a day. In many cases, differences in writing implements and in hand writing styles indicated that feeding events were jotted down as they occurred throughout the day, instead of all at one time. Many mothers reported carrying the form around 56 in a pocket during the day they chose to record data, and the crumpled condition of the forms supported this. Also, in cases where a mother had returned to work, notes by the babysitter were evident because of differences in handwriting and because of the types of foods fed the child—-e.g., "frozen mother's milk." A third possible indicator was variation in reported length of breast-feeding bouts. It might be expected that feedings reported in five minute increments were estimates or evidence of rounding off the actual length of a feeding. While such times appeared fairly commonly, closer checking revealed that for many infants, this was accurate reporting. Mothers had read or been told that they should limit nursing to a certain number of minutes to avoid nipple irritation, and they tended to stay with such a pattern for a long time. The final indicator was the correlation of infant growth extremes with diets reported. As will be described below, such correlations exist. Moreover, significant relation- ships exist between details of reported breast-feeding be- havior and maternal body composition. Thus, despite the lack of an independent means of verifying the accuracy of the diet diaries, the indicators discussed above suggest that the majority of mothers were honest and accurate in their reporting. Combining Subjects from Study I and Study II In order to combine data from Studies I and II for some of the analytical procedures which follow, it was necessary 57 to establish whether or not the two samples differed signi- ficantly. Several categories of variables were examined: parental biological characteristics, pregnancy, birth outcome, and socioeconomic status. Analysis of variance showed no statistically signifi- cant differences between the studies in parental ages, heights, or weights (p <.05; Table III.4). There also were no differences in length of pregnancy, maternal weight gain during pregnancy, and infant birth weight. Chi square tests of independence were used to compare income and edu- cation categories for the two studies.8 Again, no statisti- cally significant differences were found. Thus, for all variables it was impossible to reject the null hypothesis that the two samples do not differ biologically and demographically. For this reason, the samples are combined for analysis where appropriate in Chapter IV. 58 N8. 03.3 0.0 H 0.0 8.0 H 8.0 1033 32038333333 00. 30.3 8.33 H 0.003 3.03 H 3.003 383883 230003 300800833 00. 30.3 0.0 H 0.03 0.0 H 0.83 1033 e388 3:0383 300800833 80. 30.3 0.0 H 3.83 0.0 H 8.03 1838031 804 38:38383 88. 83.3 0.0 H 0.83 «.0 H 0.33 1838030 803 38038382 88. 30.3 0.8 H 8.383 3.3 H 0.033 1301 300303 38038383 03. 08.3 8.0 H 8.03 0.33 H 8.00 1033 330383 38038383 80. 30.3 0.3 H 0.883 0.0 3 0.083 1301 300383 38:38383 88. 33.3 8.3 3 8.38 8.0 3 0.08 303v 830383 3:800833833 33 30038 3 30030 038 3 8338338> >00 838 H 0802 .HH acsum msmuo> H xcsum "0038338> mo mHmaamsm .moanmwum> HmofimoHoflm mo conflnomfioollv.HHH m3m30 mC3EDmcoov comlummoum muccmcH mo ucoouom can HonEDZII3.>H mmDon 60 £8.00 03 I L 28.08 80 8 338.08 03 .. (M 308.08 38 8 303.08 33 l L 80.08 88 0 383.03 83 I p. 28.8 88 8 300.08 0 ! L 30.08 80 3 300.00 0 m C 100.33 00 3 c: z 2: z mmmmmm 803-388838 aucoz 61 12% of the infants were weaned. Sixty-three percent of the infants were still being breastefed at six months of age. By the end of the first month, 20% of the infants received commercial formula of some kind (Table IV.1). This prOportion increased to a peak of 41% in the fifth month. The largest incremental change after the first month was a change of 11% in the second month. Cows milk replaced or supplemented formula or breast milk in only a small percentage of cases; in no case was cows milk con- sumption reported until the fourth month. There were no reports of home prepared evaporated milk formula being consumed by any infant. The term "solids" is used throughout this study to refer to all foods except breast milk, formula, or cows milk. It includes fruit juices. The addition of solids to the infant diet in general started later than that of formula, but the rate of increase over the six month period was greater, with the cumulative total rising smoothly from 9% in the first month to 93% in the sixth. An increase in 29% in the fifth month was the greatest monthly change. Cereal was the first solid food given to most of these infants (Table IV.2), regardless of timing of introduction of solids. Seventy percent of those ever consuming non-milk foods were fed cereal first. An additional 14% consumed it as a first food in combination with fruit, vegetables, or meat. 62 TABLE IV.1--Number and Percentage of Infants Consuming Major Non-Breast Milk Food Types during each Month of the Study. Month' Type of Food 1 2 3 4 5 6 Formula N 16 25 28 28 31 26 (%) (.20) (.31) (.35) (.37) (.41) (.36) Solids N 7 18 29 42 63 67 (%) (.09) (.23) (.37) (.55) (.84) (.93) Cows Milk N - - - 2 9 l4 (%) - - - (.03) (.12) (.19) TABLE IV.2--First Solid Consumed; Number of Infants by Month of Introduction. Month Food 1 2 3 4 5 6 Total Cereal 3 12 8 10 16 3 52 Fruit/Juice 2 l l 2 4 l 11 Meat - - - - - 1 1 Cereal + (other) 1 - 3 3 2 l 10 Totals 6 13 12 15 22 6 741 1Six infants consumed no solids. 63 Table IV.3 shows the frequency of combined consumption of major dietary components. Concurrent consumption of breast milk and formula was most frequent during the first month and disappeared entirely by six months. In contrast, frequency of breast milk and solids combinations increased from lows of 3% and 4% during the first two months to a high of 74% by six months. This increase was greatest (26%) during the fifth month. Consumption of both solids and formula with breast milk ranged from 5% to 17% and followed no distinct pattern during the six month period. TABLE IV.3--Percentage and Number of Infants Consuming Combined Dietary Components during each Month of the Study. Month Diets l 2 3 4 5 6 Breast Milk N 12 9 4 2 l - & Formula (%) (.15) (.12) (.06) (.04) (.02) - Breast Milk N 2 5 7 17 29 34 & Solids (%) (.03) (.04) (.11) (.31) (.57) (.74) Breast Milk, N 4 10 11 “7 10 8 Formula (%) (.05) (.14) (.17) (.13) (.20) (.17) & Solids 64 Association of Infant Diet with Maternal Education and HousehoId Income Highest level of maternal formal education and first introduction of non-breast milk food are not independent (Table IV.4). Mothers with less than a four year college education tended to supplement at or before the median month of supplementation (the fourth month), while those with more education supplemented later (x2 = 5.55, p < .02). Mater- nal education is also related to weaning (Table IV.5). Women with fewer years of education weaned before six months more frequently and, within that time, earlier than women with more education. The relationship between the occurrence of weaning before or after six months and maternal education is significant (x2 = 10.90; p < .001). However, total household income is not significantly related to infant diet. Timing of first supplementation and incidence and date of weaning are distributed evenly across the range of incomes found in the sample (Tables IV.6, IV.7). A review of sex differences in consumption of single food items or dietary combinations reveals a weak associ- ation of longer breast-feeding with female infants (Figure IV.2). Although a higher percentage of females were weaned initially, by four months of age males had a higher rate of weaning. At the end of six months, the sex differences in numbers weaned and still breast-fed are not statistically significant. The lower incidence of breast—feeding among males is reflected in their higher rate of formula consump- tion (Table IV.8). 65 .Ammma 30m03mv 303008 0:3 A m0sa0> mo 03300 00333800 053 £333 E023 m030QEoo cc0 c03c0E 0s» m. 00330> mo 03300 m0cwheoo 3003 03:9 .0003 30 m 03 >030sv03m c0300mx0 0:3 50353 30% 303x0 03300 0030003 0oz0cc0m0c33 303 3003 0303001350 023 30:3 303303 030: U003 03 3003 :03c0E 0c9m .0m0 mo mnucos 0 030303 mcsum 033 3303 0330303 0031300033 >H0>3msaox0 039 ~0.v_ d .88.8 n ~x .0 03002 n 003002. "33003 003002 300 03 03 m3 0 33 03 030303 8.2.... .88. {2.88 88 m 8 8 0 8 33 003000 0.30300000 :mmm ...... w ....... ....3 ................ Human... 03003 I: .coom £332 30003mlcoz 30333 no 303305c03333 30 £330: cc0 30330oscm 308303 30230302 mo H0>0A um0£m3milv.>H mqmfla 66 Hoo.v m «om.oa u mx 3000 30 033308 c 30330 30 030303 @232003 30 002003033 cc0 303300200 30 0030cc0m0003 30 3003 030sv013no .0233003 30 38 0 m m 0 8 0 030303 --m-----m-----... ..... .... 8.... 9.9.8.. 83 0 m N 8 8 0 003000 0.30300000 ....... ............... 33.33.33.300 03002 00002 000 003300003 300303 30030302 30 30>03 300003mun8.>3 33000 67 .03300 >3m80 030238330 03 .3002032 033 023 030 00 .00233800 030 0033000300 300303 033 0:93 8 00 03 03 83 8 33 03 030300 8 u u 3 n 3 . 000.000.3 0 3 3 3 m 3 8 000.08 n 000.080 03 3 0 0 3 3 3 000.08 1 000.880 88 8 8 8 3 8 8 000.00 s 000.000 03 0 0 8 3 3 8 000.03 I 000.830 83 3 8 n 8 8 8 000.03 s 000.030 8 I 8 3 n i . 000.0 u 000.8 0 030303 8 8 0 8 8 3 000003 03002 .23000300 080023 030200202 >3 0002 x332 30003mi2oz 30333 30 203302003323 30 232021Im.>3 33223 68 0033000300 00030800 0333 00300800 000 00030800 000.0mw.w 0033000300 080003 .ooo.m~w A 30003330030 300 N00.3 0x N000.300.000.002 n 003002 000 30 003002 X30 30330 000 030300 0030003 30 000003003 000 080003 030000000 30 3009 3003002 00 38 u 8 8 0 0 0 030303 0 0 u u 1 n u 1 000.000 M 0 8 u - 0 3 u n 000.08 I 000.080 03 03 u 3 - u I 3 000.00 s 000.800 00 03 u u u 8 8 0 000.00 n 000.000 03 03 u 0 u 3 0 3 000.03 . 000.830 83 0 u n 3 8 3 3 000.03 . 000.030 8 8 n u n u u u 000.0 I 000.8 0 30303 0 0 8 0 8 0 3 000003 0300: .33000300 080003 030000000 00 0030003 30 030021I>.>3 03009 69 TABLE IV.8--Percentage and Number of Infants Consuming Formula by Sex and Month. Month 1 2 3 4 5 6 Males N 7 14 15 15 18 14 % (.19) (.38) (.41) (.43) (.42) (.44) Females N 9 ll 13 '13 13 12 % (.21) (.26) (.31) (.32) (.33) (.30) Total N 16 25 28 28 31 26 % (.20) (.31) (.35) (.37) (.41) (.36) A higher proportion of males were fed solids than fe- males in any month (Table IV.9). By six months all males were consuming solid food. Sex differences in introduction of first solids are more apparent in Study I than in Study II (Table IV.10). The distribution of Study I males is bimodal, with the highest percentage of male infants re- ceiving first solids at three months of age or less or at five months or more. Study I females were introduced to solids at a less variable rate, with a peak in the fourth month. In contrast, Study II males and females had almost identical distributions, with median age of introduction of solids at four months (Figure IV.3). 70 000.00 000.ov Ahm.ov 008.00 000.00 338.00 303.00 303.00 303.00 300.00 Aoo.ov 000.00 300 .30030 30 03002 30 003030 003000m|30003m 03 00000303330 x0mllm.>3 000030 33 m3 33 m3 83_L_. 20 000003 380.00 Amm.ov 0m0.ov 0mm.ov 300.00 Amm.ov Avm.ov 300.00 300.00 0m0.ov 000.30 Aoo.3v 300 0m 03 mm mm 3m 0N mm 3m mm mm mv 0m 2 0001300030 0030800 00302 0030800 00302 0030800 00302 0030800 00302 0030800 00302 0030800 00302 71 TABLE IV.9--Percentage and Number of Infants Consuming Solid "Food by Sex and Month. Month Males N 4 10 17 22 32 6 % (.ll) (.27) (.46) (.63) (.91) (.100) Females N 3 8 12 20 31 3S % (.07) (.19) (.29) (.49) (.78) (.88) Total N 7 18 19 22 63 67 % (.09) (.23) (.24) (.29) (.84) (.93) .-~-~_--‘*n-‘-‘----“*--‘---_-‘------n~--_-~*~—---*-‘~“~---~ TABLE IV.10—-Number and Percentage of Infants Consuming First Solid Food by Month, Sex, and Study. Month 1 2 3 4 5 6 Total STUDY I Males N 3 5 4 l 3 3 19 % (.16) (.26) (.21) (.05) (.16) (.16) (1.00) Females N 2 5 3 6 5 2 23 % STUDY II Males N O 2 3 5 6 l 17 % Females N 1 l 2 3 8 0 15 % PERCENT OF EACH SEX INFANTS 72 .50 .40 STUDY I .30 .20 ‘ .10 .50 1 STUDY II .40 .30 .20 . .10 MONTHS FIGURE IV.3--Percent of Infants First Consuming Solids for Male (O—-——O) and Female (l——-l) Infants by Month of Age. 73 Length and Frequency of Nursing Bouts The only infants included in analyses in this section are those for whom at least one initial diet diary of exclusive breastffeeding is available. This excludes those 18 infants whose diets were supplemented at or before 30 days. Also, as was discussed in Chapter III, infants whose diet diaries are incomplete or missing for a particular period are excluded from that section of the analysis. Mean number of feedings per day in the first month ranged from 5 to 13, with a median of 7 feedings per day (Figure IV.4). In the second month, the median of mean feedings per day of those still exclusively breast-fed remains at 7, although the range shifts slightly lower. Infants receiving first non-breast milk foods during Months 1 and 2 tended to have fewer feedings per day in the first month than did those infants exclusively breast- fed for longer periods of time (Figure IV.5). The median number of feedings per day ranged from six among infants supplemented between one and two months to eight among those supplemented during the third month or later. A median test produces a x2 of 13.64, showing a significant (p '<.01) relationship between month of supplementation and proportion of infants less than and equal to ( y: ) or exceeding ( > ) the overall sample median of seven feedings (Table IV.11). Similarly, infants exclusively breast-fed longer had more feedings per day in Month 2 74 (Figure IV.6). A median test demonstrates the statistical significance of the association between length of exclu- sive breastefeeding and the number of feedings per day in Month 2 (Table IV.12); p < .05). The duration of exclusive breast feeding also varied with the change in the number of feedings per day for individual infants between the first and second months (Table IV.13). Although the overall tendency was for the number of feedings to decrease with time, infants who consumed their first non-breast milk foods later appeared to be more likely to have the same or greater number of feedings in the second as in the first month. Conversely, the fewer the months of exclusive breast-feeding, the more likely was the infant to have had a stable or de- creasing number of feedings from one to two months of age. Mean minutes per feeding ranged from 7 to 53 in the first month and from 7 to 42 in the second. Both had medians of 18 minutes. There was no correlation of minutes per feeding with length of exclusive breast- feeding similar to that observed in number of feedings per day (Table IV.14). In both months, the distribution of minutes per feeding of those feeding non-breast milk foods early is not significantly different from that of those doing So later (Figure IV.7 and IV.8). 75 N 29202 .N 000 3 003002 003300 0001300030 030>3003oxm 0300003 3om 000 300 0m030000l30003m 00 300802 0m030>0llv.>3 000030 mBZ¢0ZH 00 mmmZDZ HEM. “.2 U 3 5.202 u U N3 m3 L— NdQ 000 mUZHDmmm 00 KWMZDZ mw¢mm>¢ 76 1 F‘— 11 1 ¢ * g 2 H a 1 gr: 1:1 9' 1 U) n: 1 E 1 m 3 E 1— § ‘ {—1__ I747 NF] 2 m 1 U) * a 4 z 4 1 O ' , , [_1 ( >4 1 * UHHH n 5 6 7 8 91011 12 13 FEEDINGS PER DAY (*Monthly Medianl) FIGURE IV.5--Number of Breast-Feedings per Day in Month 1 ' by Length of Exclusive Breast-Feeding. 1Since there are an even number of observations, median for Month 3 is the mean of the two observations in the central position (Dixon and Massey 1969). 77 T 'k 2 4 0 fl 4 2 H o 0 It} ‘7 ‘ "—1: a 3 " U) fi . E H [1 mm 0 > a 4 3 4 Fh'kr (>3 41 [:J E? 1 H a {—1 e g * 2 PW >4 : UHF—l 4567891011 FEEDINGS PER DAY (*Monthly Median) FIGURE IV.6--Number of Breast-Feedings per Day in Month 2 by Length of Exclusive Breast-Feeding. 78 H O v CL V \O 0; H II N :4 >1 (0 Q \ U) 01 C "-1 'O (D (D In l‘ H C. 10 "-1 ‘U (D 0 4..) U) Q) E4 0 10 -H 'U Q) 2 mm wm mm 030309 0 m v 003002 3000 A m3 m3 0 . 003800 3:00 0 m 0 003002 00309 m3 3 «3 003:0: 033 M 030309 000\00000 0 000\00000 0 . 00300001300030 0300003 30 300802 0300303.30 300802 .0>3003000 003002 .00300001300030 0>3003000 30 030003 00 3 03002 03 000 300 000300001300030 30 3008021133.>3 00009 79 «w 03 0 0 03 03 03 m 030309 000\00000 0 0300303 00 300802 0>3003000 30 030000 00 N 03002 03 >00 N mm 030309 m 003002 3000 A 0 003002 3000 m3 083:0: 00303 w 000\00000 9 0030000 300030 0300303,30 300802 0>3003000 003002 .00300001300030 300 000300001300030 30 30080211N3.>3 03049 80 m m N 003002 3000 A m 0 v 003002 3000 m 3 3 003002 00309 N m c 003002 039 00003000 000000 02 00003003 00300001300030 0>3003000 003002 .00300001300030 0>3003000 30 003002 00 .N 03002 000 3 03002 0003300 000 300 000300001300030 30 300802 03 00000011m3.>3 03009 81 m 3 m 003002 3000 A 03 m 0 003002 3000 m o 0 003002 00309 m N 0 003002 039 00003000 000000 02 00003003 00300001300030 0>3003ox0 003002 'll'll"ll'lllII'|'||l|'|""'ll"I'I"l'lll||| ll'llllllIII-I'llIllIll!IIll'lllIIIlllllll'lllll'l'lll'lll'l .00300001300030 0>30030x0 30 003002 00 .N 03002 000 3 03002 0003300 00300001300030 300 0030032 03 00000011V3.>3 03049 82 .3 03002 003300 0030000 300 0030032 30 300802 0002110.>3 000030 om m0 00 mm om mN cm 03 03 m o 1! n1: Si vA DNIGHBJ-LSVEHH HAISHTDXE JO SHLNOW 83 .N 03002 003300 0030000 300 0030032 00 300802 000211m.>3 000030 mv 00 mm om mN 0N m3 03 m 0 ii I‘I II vA i I1 I. J DNIQHHJ-LSVEHH EAISH'IDXE JO SHCLNOW 84 Transitions to a Mixed Diet The feeding of formula, juice, or solid food in an infant diet previously consisting solely of breast milk is usually termed "supplementation" (e.g., Fomon 1970). The use of this term, however, can create confusion. Although it implies the addition of more food to an existing level of intake, this is not necessarily the case. While--in terms of absolute numbers of foods consumed--the supplement is always a dietary addition, it is not necessarily a nutritional addition. The new food can replace part or all of the previous diet; or it can, in fact, be a true addition, increasing overall nutrient intake. To avoid confusion, the terms "addition" and "replace- ment" are used in the following analysis rather than "supple- ment." If the number of breast feedings stays the same or increases when a new food enters the diet, the food is considered an addition. If the number of breast-feedings decreases, that food is considered a replacement. Since prior feeding data do not exist for Month 1, criteria for differentiating additions from replacements have been modified slightly. The consumption of formula as one isolated feeding is labeled replacement. "Addition" is considered to be the use of small amounts of formula immediately after breastefeeding bouts. Of the two breastefeeding attributes readily observable in a study such as this one-~number of feedings per day and length of feedings--the former is used here as the criterion 85 for assessing the impact of dietary change. It is used with the recognition that it is hardly an exact estimate of breast milk intake. The other variable, length of feeding, varies with the enthusiasm of the infant. It has been shown that an infant can empty a breast in 10 minutes and that the length of feeding is more an indi- cation of the vigor and attentiveness of the infant than the amount of milk consumed. Number of feedings per day, on the other hand, can indicate actual differences in amount consumed (Egli et al. 1961). Only that amount of milk which can be stored in the mammary gland ducts will be available, regardless of how long the infant sucks or whether or not more time is allowed to elapse between feedings. In fact, a reduction in milk production may occur with decreased sucking frequency. Thus, the "replace- ment" of a breastefeed should reflect lower total milk intake from both loss of one or more feedings per day and from lower milk output at the remaining feedings. In this study, the effect of the introduction of the first non-breast milk food on the diet appears to depend on the infant's age at introduction. Although there is no real distinction between the frequency of addition and that of replacement in Month 1, replacement clearly pre- dominates in Months 2 and 3. After that time, the majority of new foods are additions (Table IV.15). 86 TABLE IV.15--Role of Newly Introduced Foods in Diet Formerly Consisting Exclusively of Breast Milk. —_——--—===—_-Month _ Role of Food 1 2 3 4 5 6 Totals Addition 7 l 2 8 15 5 38 Replacement ll 10 8 2 4 l 36 Totals 18 ll 10 10 19 6 74 A x2 test of additions and replacements at or before the median of three months with those after is highly signifi- cant (x2 = 21.70; p < .001). The predominance of replacements in Months 2 and 3 is not simply a function of some overall decrease in (feedings per day. If such a decrease were the rule, any new food item would automatically appear to be replacing the missing breast-feed(s). To test for the existence of a general decrease in feedings, the number of feedings per day in Months 1 and 2 were examined in infants who did not experience the first introduction of non-breast milk foods until after Month 3. Among those infants, 23 (51%) had a decrease in number of feeds per day. Thus, about half would have been classified as "replacing" breast milk if a dietary change had been made. Almost the same number--22 (49%)--either increased their rate of feedings (N=9) or 87 maintained the same level (N=l3), either of which would have made the introduction of a new food appear to be an “addition." The difference between this distribution and that observed among those actually experiencing introduction of a new food in Months 2 or 3 is signifi- cant (Table IV.16, p < .01). Thus, the predominance of "replacement" in Months 2 and 3 is not simply an artifact of a general reduction in feedings per day during those months. Whether the non-breast milk item consumed was formu- la or solid food appeared to be a function of the infant's age at introduction of the food rather than its action as an addition to or replacement of existing intake. Formula consumption predominated before four months and solids, after (Table IV.17, p < .001). Sex of the infant was unrelated to whether a food was an addition or a replacement. Table IV.18 shows that the percent of male and female infants was almost equal for the two categories at every month. As mentioned above, there was little difference in the frequencies of addition and replacement of non- breast milk food in Month 1. These categories, however, are predictive of future time of weaning for the 18 infants involved (Table IV.19). In those 11 cases where the new food was a replacement, lO weaned by the end of the second month. In contrast, those 7 for whom the new food was an addition continued breast-feeding longer. 88 .N 000 3 003002 03 000 300 00030003 30 300800 30 00300000033 0>330300800 003 00 00000 03 0303 .3003 003 03 000303000 00 .0000003303 0000 000 000 .000 0>00 03003 0003 0 0000 0303 0303030 003 30 3008000000 00 300003003 030003 00003 .30303 30 v 03002 03 0338 3000301000 30333 003 00300003303 00003 300 3 30. v 0 .30.3 u «x “3003 030000 300 00 03 33 30303 33 mm 33 30000003008 3 3 000 N 003000 30 0030 03 0003 3332 3000351202 NO 0300 30030000 mm mm 3 300333000 030303 30303 30 3 0300: m 0:0 N 003000 0000 0332 3000301002 30330 30 003300003303 l'll'l'-|||l|llll'l'"l"lI'|l|'l'IIIIIIIII'II'll|'l'll'lll'|llll'l"llll i]I'lllll'iillllllIlllllll'llllllllllll'l'lll'lliillllll"lll I'll-Ill .m 000 N 003002 003300 000 300 03000 00300001300030 30 300802 03 0033000 003310033030000 0 30 00000030 003 303 30091103.>3 03009 89 300 0300 300. v 0 “3.33 n «x 03 03002 n 003002 "3003 003002 33 03 030303 N mN MHDEHOM mm 33 003300 003002 mA 003002 mm... 0000 .0000 x332 30003mL0oz 30330 30 003300003303 00300: 03 0>33030m 003300 000 0308300 30 0033000033031103.>3 00009 90 Adv fay AHW Amv Amy Any 2 mm. HH. om mm. oo.H on. w mmamemm on Amv Adv Amy Amv Avv z oo. cm. «a. mu. mm. om. w mmamz ucmsmomammm Amy Amv Avv AHV on Amv 2 no. mm. om. 5H. 00. om. w mmameom Amy Amy Amy AHV AHV Avv z oo.a om. gm. mm. 5H. cm. W mmamz coflufinc< m m e m m H nucoz .COwuosoouucH mo nucoz can xmm ha .umfln unamcH :a moon 3mz mo mHOMLImH.>H mqmda 91 TABLE IV.19--Month of Weaning for Infants Given Formula "or Solids in Month 1, by Addition and Replacement Categories. ' MOnth of Weaning‘ l 2 3 4 5 6 Totals Addition - l 4 l - l 7 Replacement 7 3 - - - 1 ll Totals 7 4 4 l 0 2 18 Median Test: Median = Month 3; Fisher's Exact Probability = .002' None weaned until the second month; the majority weaned inf Month 3. Using Fisher's Exact Test to compare frequency of weaning at or before the median of 2 months with that after 2 months, the difference is highly significant (p = .002). Major Dietary Patterns The interaction of dietary components, maternal infant feeding behaviors, and certain demographics as discussed above facilitates the identification of major dietary patterns for these infants. These patterns permit the breakdown of this heterogeneous sample into relatively homogeneous groups to be used in a number of analyses pree sented later in this chapter. This section will present the rationale for group differentiation and describe the groups so formed. 92 The primary basis for defining the most common dietary patterns is length of exclusive breastffeeding. Whether or not causal relationships exist (and regardless of the direction of any causality), length of exclusive breast-feeding has been found in this study to be con- cordant with variables from a wide variety of domains. These include parental socio-economic status, details of breast-feeding behaviors, and the impact of introduction of non-breast milk foods on the overall diet. Using length of exclusive breast-feeding as the criterion, there are three main groups of infants (Figure IV.9): Group 1: 4 weeks or less exclusive breast- feeding. N=18 Group 2: more than 35 days but less than 94 days exclusive breast-feeding. N=21 Group 3: more than 101 days exclusive breast- feeding. N=4l Group l--Most of the infants in Group 1 were exclu- sively breast-fed for a very limited time--shorter than the four weeks used to define the group. However, because of differences in data collection between Studies I and II and because of the necessity of drawing on various sources of information to reconstruct the perinatal period, four weeks is used here as a conservative estimate of the end of exclusive breast-feeding. Mothers and infants in Study I were first seen at home only one week after parturition. At that time most were newly discharged 93 Group 1 Group 2 Group 3 N=19* l O 20 40 60 80 100 120 140 160 180 >180 Days Postpartum FIGURE IV.9--Length of Exclusive Breast-Feeding and Infant Feeding Groups. *Because the length of exclusive breast-feeding in this group was determined by a combination of methods, its duration as reported on diet diaries is only approximate and reflects the timing of the early diet diary rather than actual feeding behavior. 94 from the hospital and had not had time to settle into any routine. Some cases of mixed formula and breast milk feeding were identified at that point, but most were still determined to establish exclusive breast-feeding. Although diet diaries were sent to these women at approximately ten days postpartum, some were not completed and returned until two weeks later. When mixed feeding was reported at that point, additional details on the events of that first month were obtained retrospectively at the one month measuring session and even later. The latest date for modification of the breast milk diet for any case is in the fourth week. Had data collection been more precise, the four week cut off could probably have been made earlier. Thus the fact that these infants had been fed breast milk exclusively for a very short time came from the first diet diary. The date of this diary was used unless interview data could pinpoint the date more accur- ately. Because of the difficulties in obtaining early feed- ing data in Study I, the first at-home contact in Study II was at two weeks postpartum. In all cases but one, the infants in Study II in "Group 1" were receiving mixed diets or had already been weaned by the second week visit. In the remaining case, a diet diary at 20 days listed formula and included a note explaining that exclusive breast-feeding had ended two days earlier. In addition to limited exclusive breast-feeding, Group 1 is 95 characterized by a high frequency (89%) of the use of formula rather than solids within the first month. As discussed above, there is no clear preference for addition over replacement or vice versa. However, since each of these categories correlates with later differences in time of weaning, two subgroups will be identified for some of the later growth analyses. These subgroups are: la less than four weeks exclusive breast- §:$ding. Introduced food is an "addition." lb less than four weeks exclusive breast- feeding. Introduced food is a "replace- ment." N=ll In Group la the average amount of formula consumed per feeding in Month 1 was 2.2 i 1.1 ounces. The total daily consumption ranged from 2 to 20 ounces, with a mean of 10.8 i 7.6 ounces. In comparison, the average formula consumed per feeding by infants in Group lb was 4.0 i 1.4 ounces. The daily total ranged from 13.5 to 39 ounces, with a mean of 24.5 i 8.0 ounces. Group 2--This group consists of infants who had at least 35 days of exclusive breast-feeding. This is in contrast to Group 1 in which exclusive breast-feeding never became established as a feeding pattern. The upper limit of 94 days reflects both the end of this relatively long period in which exclusive breast-feeding was termif nated at scattered ages and the upper limit of the clustering of "replacements" for breast milk by formula or solid food. Within this group two subgroups will be 96 distinguished in later analyses, based on the first food introduced. These subgroups are: 2a exclusive breast-feeding of more than 35 and less than 94 days with formula the first nonfbreast milk food introduced. N=9 2b exclusive breast-feeding of more than 35 and less than 94 days with solids the first non-breast milk food introduced. N=12 In Group 2a the quantity of formula first introduced per day varied from 6 to 15 ounces, with a mean of 10.9 i 3.2 ounces. In Group 2b, the most commonly consumed first solid was cereal mixed with formula, cows milk, apple juice, or water. When the nutrient content of each mix- ture is computed and compared with that of the average intake of formula in Group 2a, the nutritional contribution of solids or formula to the diet is markedly different (Table IV.20). TABLE IV.20--Nutrient Values for Average Daily Intake of ’Non-Breast Milk Foods at their First Introduction; Mean 1 Standard Deviation. Infant Feeding Group (N) Nutrient 2a (9) 2b (12) Energy (kcal) 213.9 : 63.5 66.9 i 51.8 Protein (gm) 4.8 i 1.4 2.0 i 2.3 Fat (gm) 11.5 i 3.4 1.5 i 1.7 Carbohydrate (gm) 22.8 + 6.8 11.2 i 7.8 Source: Nutrient values used in Tables IV.21 and IV.22 for commercial formulas come from Michigan Dept. of Public Health (1980). Values for other foods come from Gerber Products Company (1977). 97 Group 3f-These infants were exclusively breastefed for at least 102 days. After that time the first none breast milk food was introduced to 35 infants. One infant first consumed formula; 34 first consumed solids or fruit juice. In 28 cases, the new food was a dietary addition. In only 7 cases was it a replacement. Estimated nutrient values per day are shown in Table IV.21. TABLE IV.21--Nutrient Values for Average Daily Intake of Non-Breast Milk Foods at their First Introduction for Feeding Group 3; Mean :_Standard Deviation. Role of Food in Diet Replacement (N=7) Addition (N=28) Energy (kcal) 101.6 : 77.0 47.8 i 71.1 Protein (gm) 3.3 i 1.8 1.2 i 1.6 Fat (gm) 3.8 i 4.1 1.9 i 4.2 Carbohydrate (gm) 13.7 i 8.3 6.7 i 8.4 Four infants did not consume any non-breast milk food before the age of six months. Two others who had consumed only breast milk left the study during the fourth month. They are also considered part of Group 3, bringing the total number of infants in the group to 41. Infant Growth This section presents the results of analysis of infant growth data. After describing the growth of the sample as a whole and the preliminary transformations of 98 the data, infant growth will be analyzed in terms of four growth-related dietary parameters: length of breast- feeding, age at first introduction of nonfbreast milk food, the common dietary patterns identified above, and details of the breast-feeding process. Description of the Sample At birth the 80 infants studied all weighed more than 2500 grams, the traditional anthropometric boundary between high risk and normality. From birth to six months of age, males were heavier and longer than females (Table IV.22). Females had higher summed fatfold measures at birth and one, five, and six months. Males, in contrast, were leaner at birth, had a somewhat faster increase in adiposity during the first months of life, and then actually decreased in fatness during the later months of the study (Table IV.22). When compared with the National Center for Health Statistics percentiles for infant growth (National Center for Health Statistics 1976), the sample was consistently heavier and longer for age than the NCHS norms (Table IV.23). Males had higher mean percentile rankings than did females, though these differences were not statisti- cally significant. In weight for height, the sample more closely approximated the NCHS standards, though infants were, on the average, considerably thinner than the norm at one and six months. m .HHHO 23 m .NHHo .Hw bJHHm .mm 99 o.m H>.mm o.m Hm.Hm k.> Hh.~v H.m Ha.m~ mmamsmm m.mHHm.mm c.4HHa.mm m.mHHo.om H.HHHm.am a.oaun.mm 5.5 Hm.av 0.0 Hm.m~ mmamz Assv wagon neat qaaoa a.~ Hm.am S.~ Hm.mm o.~ Hm.mm v.~ HH.HG e.~ HH.mm m.~ Hm.vm o.~ Hm.om mwamsmm m.~ H~.os G.~ Hm.mm m.~ Ho.mo ~.~ H~.mm o.m Hm.wm o.~ Ha.om v.~ HH.Nm mmamz have meozmq 92mmzsomm e.o Hq.k e.o Ho.a e.o Hm.o o.o Hm.m m.o Hm.m m.o Hm.q v.0 Hv.m mmamsmm o.a Hm.m m.o Hm.e m.o Hm.a m.o Ho.m A.o Hm.m A.o “v.4 m.o Hm.m mmamz xmxv emonz o m a m N H auufim wanmfium> sumo: .coflumwbmo onmpcmum H com: .cucoz comm mo can an tam nuuflm um mucmmcH now mowumwumum o>flumfluommollmm.>H Manda 100 .HsmmCAcmmE on on A.o.c .mnmpaouv Emumoum mmuz an pmumasoamo on panoo mmHHucmouom camcmq MOM unmwmz 30m 008« e.m~HH.mv m.a~Hm.m¢ ¢.omHH.ma m.m~Hm.mm m.m~H«.mm .nun .uuu mmamemm H.4NHv.km m.m~Ho.qa m.m~HH.mv ”.mwfim.~m a.a~HH.mm k.m~H~.mq .-u- mmamz :aozmq mom amonz m.a~H~.HG o.mmuo.mm ¢.mmuo.mo a.m~Hm.~o m.mmum.¢o «.mmfia.~m m.¢~Hv.HG mmamamm m.omum.ma o.m~Hv.ok m.~mum.oe «.mmflm.mm m.-Hm.wo o.m~Hm.vm o.o~Hm.mo mmamz mwfi mom meozmq m.¢~Hm.vm ~.4~Hm.om A.H~Hm.mo a.HNHH.mo m.-Hm.mm m.qNHe.mo m.m~Ha.km mmamemm m.m~H>.om m.¢mum.mm «.mmua.mm a.vmum.mm A.H~HH.oa m.mmHm.Hm k.amuk.om mmamz moa mom amonz G m a m N H zuuflm manmflum> sumo: III|I|I|"III|I||Il I‘IIIIIIIIIIII I.au:oz comm mo cam am can cuuflm um mucmmcH How coflumw>mo oumocmum + maflucooumm mmoz cmmzllmm.>H mam¢9 lOl Preliminary Data Transformations In order to combine males and females into a single sample, several data adjustments were made preliminary to actual data analysis. Weights and lengths for each infant were transformed into three new variables based on their percentage of the NCHS sex-specific medians for weight for age, length for age, and weight for length. These transformations were performed by the computer center of Fels Research Institute under the supervision of Dr. Alex Roche. The growth data analysis program of Goldsby (n.d.) was used. Since the NCHS growth curves for infants from birth to 36 months are themselves com- puted from the Fels data base, this analysis provided exact and accurate transformations of the data. More readily available representations of the NCHS norms-- such as those published by Ross Laboratories (1976) and widely used in clinical settings--were not used. For these, the extreme percentiles have been collapsed, giving the impression of a narrower distribution of in- fant size than is actually the case. All results presented below are based on these transformations. "Percent of the median" is used rather than percentiles because the latter cannot be calculated for many of the low infant weights and lengths common at birth and one month of age. National standards were not available for infant fatfolds. Therefore, within-sample adjustments were made 102 to take account of sex differences. First, all eight fatfolds for each infant were totaled for each measure- ment session. Then male and female sex-specific least squares regression lines were computed for the entire sample, using total fatfolds in milimeters as the depen- dent variable. Residuals were then computed for each infant measurement entered in the equation. These re- siduals represent the difference between the actual infant measurement and that predicted by the regression equation. They are used as the new data points in all analyses in this chapter. Total fatfolds were used rather than any single fatfold for two reasons. First, since infants are frequently somewhat assymetrical at birth and for variable periods of time thereafter, using both left and right side measures prevents the confounding of results by biases resulting from the arbitrary choice of either left or right side. Second, the combination of trunk and extremity measures controls for difference in fat patterning. Weaning Length of breastefeeding was most significantly related to infant growth in the first several months of life. Table IV.24 presents means and standard deviations for the percent of median for all growth variables. There were no significant differences between groups assigned by month of weaning in length for age or weight for length at any time from birth to six months of age. 103 m.m Hm.m0H m.aHHo.mOH m.aHH~.mm Aka.v om. o.OHH¢.HOH $.4HHm.GOH o 1mm.v mm.H H.OHHm.o0H m.mHHm.mOH H.m Hm.mm m.m Hm.NCH m.aHHm.AOH m xa~.v os.H H.HHHo.mOH o.mHHm.moH m.m “n.00H m.m Hm.MOH m.~HHm.OHH a Hmo.v mH.~ A.OHHq.OHH m.mHHH.OHH G.OHHm.mm a.m Hm.mOH m.~HHm.mOH m xao.o km.~ m.HHHq.HHH q.MHHv.~HH o.m Ho.mm m.a Hq.aOH a.mHHN.HHH m Hao.v ms.~ m.MHHq.mOH H.HHH~.mOH v.HHHm.mm ~.oHHH.MOH «.mHHa.moH H AHG.V mm. 8.4HHB.GOH q.HHHm.VOH N.¢HHm.oOH H.~HHo.mm ~.~Hwo.mOH nuuHm 32...... ........... m----mmwma AwmmQ oumpcmum + mammzulvm.>H mqmda h,” 104 Go.H e.m+m.NOH m.q+m.mOH H.m+m.HoH m.~+o.NOH H.m+m.NOH m am.H m.v+m.NOH a.m+m.m0H o.m+m.OOH m.~+m.ooH m.q+¢.HOH m mm. m.a+o.MOH m.m+o.vOH H.m+q.HOH m.H+m.HOH m.v+m.NOH 4 mm. ~.¢+m.~0H a.m+m.mOH h.~+m.oOH H.~+¢.~¢H m.a+m.HoH m hk.H m.v+m.NOH m.v+m.vOH v.m+o.mm m.m+m.NOH m.¢+v.NOH m kH.H a.a+m.NOH m.v+q.~0H 4.4+H.ma m.~+a.HOH m.¢+m.HOH H mm. 8.4+S.NOH m.m+q.aOH m.m+H.ocH o.m+~.HoH ~.~+~.~0H suuHm .......... ..Z..~.Z.a 09m Acchmz mmoz we we mm< How a umcmHJnmcwcmmz mo cucoz cmscHuaoou+«~.>H mHmaa 105 Am¢.v mm. q.a Hu.>m e.m Hm.vm m.vHo.mm H.mHa.mm ~.m HA.OOH w Hmm.v no. ~.m Hm.mm m.m HH.mm «.mum.mm ~.¢Ho.mm m.v HA.NOH m Aom.c am. «.mHHm.o0H a.OHHo.ma e.mHm.km m.mHm.mm m.m Hm.NOH v Am~.1 Gm.H e.m H~.NOH H.owm.mm m.mHm.mm m.mHv.mm H.m Hm.vOH m Aom.v mm. b.4HHm.mOH m.mHm.mm o.mH~.mOH m.mHH.mm m.¢HH~.voH N Ane.v m4. o.HHHH.HOH v.an.mm «.mum.mm «.mHm.mm o.o Ho.OOH H Aom.v m~.H ~.m Hm.mm m.>HH.mm «.mflo.hm H.5Hm.Hm m.OHH~.va nuHHm 4mm ...... .....m... ........... m----mmmma mad .Acmwcmz mmoz mo wv sumcmq How unmwmz1tmcficmm3 mo sucoz.. omscHucoouu¢~.>H mHmae 106 av. m.mHHH.vu o.m~Hu.m- o.OHHG.0Hu m.mHHm.mn m.vHHm.ku m mm. m.hHHm.H 0.5Huo.au m.a Hm.~- G.HmHm. G.AHHG.- m Hm. ~.mHHa.a m.GHHH.m H.mHG.H a.o~Hm.m e.mHHm.m a on. a.OHHm.a v.4HHH.m o.m HH.m H.~HHm.m >.mHH¢.HH m on. m.m Hm.m m.GHHH.m ~.m Hv.H m.m HH.m m.HHHH.m m ea.~ m.a Hm. H.m H~.vn a.“ He.mu m.v H~.H s.m Ho.o H mo.H m.m Hm.au «.8 Hm.HH- e.v Hm.nu H.m Hm.mu ~.m H~.mu nuuHm ........... ..:.HNH2§5 ). mod Ass memschmmv moHomunm Hmuoasnmchmmz mo chaos noscflucOOIlvm.>H mumma 107 Significant differences relating to future weaning did appear in weight for age at one and two months (p < .05). Reduced weight for age at one and two months was found among infants who would be weaned in the third and, to a lesser degree, the second months. In Month 1, the highest weight for age was of infants who would not be weaned until after six months of age, a continuation of the trend apparent at birth. By two months of age this group had been equalled in size by other groups of infants. Analysis of total fatfolds for age suggests that the differences between groups in weight do not necessarily reflect similar differences in fatness. At one month of age infants who would be weaned in the third month were the leanest infants (p < .05) and, of all weaning groups, had experienced the least fat increase from birth. Although those who would be weaned after six months did show a marked initial increase in fatness, the fattest infants were those who had been weaned during the first month. Furthermore, this group of infants had experienced the largest increment in total fatfolds from birth. Introduction of First NoneBreast Milk Food Age at introduction of first non-breast milk food was more closely associated with weight for age than with any other growth variable (Table IV.25). Figure IV.10 shows that those infants exclusively breast-fed for four or more months were on the average heavier at birth than those exclusively breast-fed for shorter periods of time. 108 Furthermore, this higher mean weight continued through five months of age. Those infants eXclusively breast- fed the shortest period of time were lightest at birth and, in fact, experienced an initial decrease in percent of the median weight for age. Differences between groups in weight for age approach statistical significance at one month (p < .06) and two months of age (p < .08) (Table IV.25). Infants exclusively breast-fed longest had a greater mean weight for length from birth to four months (Figure IV.11) and were slightly fatter than other infants in the early months of life, but these relationships are not statistically significant. Common Infant Dietary Patterns Infant growth variables were also analyzed by the major dietary patterns identified earlier in this chapter during analysis of the infant feeding data. Table IV.26 shows the average monthly percent of median for each growth measure by dietary pattern group. Although these distri- butions overlap, trends in weight for age (see Figure IV.12) are consistent with those observed among groups defined by timing of weaning and introduction of first non-breast milk food. Infants in dietary Group 3e-those with longest exclusive breast-feeding-—were heavier at birth, and maintained this size superiority until five months. Group la-fexperiencing the "addition" of formula by one month of age-~had a marked decrease in weight for 109 1N4.c NN. m.m N.NOH a.oH H N.OOH m.mH H o.mOH o ANN.V mN.H N.m H N.NoH H.HH H N.NOH N.NH H N.GOH m ANN.V em.H m.OH H o.mpH N.oH H H.40H o.NH H m.NOH a smo.c om.N H.¢H H N.HHH m.NH H N.mOH N.HH H m.GOH m ANH.V NN.H N.HH H o.NHH N.4H H N.NOH H.mH H m.ooH N soo.v Nm.N N.NH H N.NOH o.qH H m.moH a.mH H N.¢¢H H Hoo.v Nm. v.4H H v.ooH o.mH H o.VOH m.mH H o.NOH suuHm ..:--.....1mmw ...... Mi---..---w-m..-..--::.::..-:m ............ .H.-------------mmw..mm..s mod HmmHoo: mmoz mo NC was How usoHoZnooom sHH: ummoumnmoz umuHm_uo sumo: .mooom :HH: umomum Isoz mo sofluoscouucH umuwm mo nusoz an .m on a mcucoz mo cam can suuflm um mousmmmz suaouw acumcH Hsom no mcofluMH>oo oumocmum + msmozllmm.>H mqmH mquwB 111 ANH.V HN.H o.m H o.Nm N.N H m.mm m.o H m.mm o soH.V om.H N.N H N.Nm N.m H «.mm o.o H m.OOH m smo.o mm. q.NH H m.OOH N.m H N.Nm o.m H H.ooH a ANN.C mm.H o.OH H H.m0H o.N H m.mm N.N H m.OOH N :HN.V mm. a.mH H o.moH N.OH H H.N¢H o.HH H m.HoH N sam.v om. o.oH H ¢.HOH o.OH H H.¢OH 4.N H o.mm H AoN.V om. m.m H o.mm N.m H N.¢m o.N H N.mm suuHm sonc m a.“ N H Amsumo:c mod ooosflucoounm~.>H mamde 112 AHN.V HN. m.mH H N.vu o.NH H o.Nu N.vH H H.ou o Hom.v oH. m.NH H o.H H.HH H N.H- m.oH H o. m HNM.V Ho.H m.mH H m.¢ o.OH H m. N.NH H m.N a .mH.v ON.H N.m H_o.m m.OH H N.H m.mH H o.m m 10H.c NN.N m.N H m.o m.m H m.H m.HH H H.o N sHm.v NH.H H.N H H.H a.» H N.Nu N.m H m. H HNG.V Hm. o.m H m.Nn N.m H o.N- N.v H ¢.ou sumHm -------.1mu.m ...... m...:~mfimss mad oommHumoouumN.>H mHmme 113 110 1 c; 108 -4 ma: H m B4106 , 21H «.8 O 104 4 JJ 4): 5.9102 J o m 3'43 o m 100 ‘ Birth 1 2 3 4 5 6 months FIGURE IV.10-~Mean Weight for Age (Percent of NCHS Median) by Month of Introduction of First Non-Breast Milk Food. Month 1 (0—0) , Months 2 and 3 (l———-—l), Month 4 or Later.(A—————A). Percent of Median Weight for Length H +4 \D \D \O O O A ox (I) O N \O N I T v I U Birth 1. 2 3 4 5 6 months FIGURE IV.ll--Mean Weight for Length (Percent of NCHS Median) by Month of Introduction of First Non-Breast Milk Food. Month 1 (o—————O), Months 2 and 3 (Ik————I), Month 4 or Later (Ar————A). 114 cm. m5. mm. Hm.a VH.H mh.H Hv. m.m Hm.NOH m.m HN.N0H m.OHHo.m0H H.OHHN.HHH N.HHHo.NHH m.NHHN.m0H H.HHHH.GOH H.NHHN.OOH o.NHHH.HOH m.m HN.NOH H.HHHo.mOH N.mHHm.NOH H.HHHm.o0H m.HHHh.moa h.m H«.Hoa m.oaflw.moa o.NHHm.mOH v.maHo.moa m.wHHm.boa H.mHHo.voH h.vHHm.HOH o.maflw.moa m.NHHm.¢oa m.HHHm.boa m.oaflm.hoa h.NHHm.moa m.NHHm.MOH m.HHHm.HOH o.mHHH.hoa m.maflfi.moa m.maHo.moH h.mafim.voa h.maHH.moa m.mHHm.mm ¢.oHH¢.HOH mad . ACMHomS mmuz mo wv mad HON unmamzllmmsonw.mcHomoh ucmmcH. .mmsouo msHoomh “camcH an hm OH H mSUCOE MO UCW UCM Cub—”Hm Hm mmHflmmmz SH3OHU HEMHCH .HSOh HO .Q.m + WCMQZIImN.>H ”NJ—”g9 115 1H4.c Ho.H m.mHv.NoH «.mHm.mOH H.NHm.HOH o.¢Hm.HOH o.¢HH.HOH o 14H.V om.H N.qHH.NOH o.¢Hm.HOH a.mHN.o0H m.mHN.OOH o.vHN.NOH m ism-V mo.H m.aHH.NOH N.NHH.MOH N.NH¢.OOH m.mHo.NOH m.NHm.HOH . Ham-o HN. N.vHN.NOH m.mHH.mOH N.mHo.HOH N.NHmHHOH o.mHN.NOH m sea-v Ne. N.4Ho.moH o.qu.NoH N.¢HN.OOH m.¢HH.NoH m.mHm.HoH N mks-c mm. H.HHN.NOH N.mHm.NOH N.HH4.HOH m.mHN.HoH «.mHo.HOH H ANN.V m.- ¢.va.NOH o.mHH.MOH H.mHN.OOH o.mHo.HOH m.mHm.NOH sumHm 3.2 :.-------. ............ m ........... m---:------m ----------- m----mm.-.....a mod concHunoollmm.>H mamH mnmflfi 117 smm.v am. N.MHHN.HI N.NNHo.qu N.NHHN.OH- H.oHHo.Nn H.mHHm.m- o mom-V SH. m.NHHH.H N.NHHH.N- o.OHHo.H- o.oNHm.H- H.NHHo.m m ANN-V Nm. m.mHHm.v o.mHHN. m.N HN. m.NHHo.N N.mHH¢.m a svm.c «H.H N.m Ho.m m.HHHv.H- ¢.oHHN.m o.mHHN.m o.HHHH.o m ANN.V NH.H m.N Hm.o H.HHHm.u N.N HN.m m.m HN.N H.HHHH.4 N Ame-V HN.N H.N HH.H m.N HN.N- H.m Hm.- m.N HN.N N.N HN.v- H AHN.V mm. N.m Hm.Nu N.o HN.N: N.v Ho.N- o.m Ho.m- v.m HN.N- suuHm ...m----.m-::-:-m ............ mm ........... m .......... m ------------ m---m.mmm..s- omd oommHumoonnmN.>H mHmae 112 110 o 108 (31 d H O (4.4 4, 106 I: U‘ H 0.) 3 5 104 H "U (D S 5H O u 102 C: (D U M 0) °‘ 100 98 96 118 Group 1a ~ Group 3 Group 1b Group 2a ‘I Group 2b r t f r Y j ‘ Birth 1 2 3 4 5 6 months FIGURE IV.12-—Mean Weight for Age (Percent of NCHS Median) for Infant Feeding Groups la through 3. 119 age at one month, but recovered steadily and, in fact, was the heaviest group of infants at Months 5 and 6. Group 2b, which had breast-feeding partially replaced by solids during Months 2 and 3, is notable for its rapid decrease in weight for age after two months. Although these infants were, on the average, heavier at birth than all but Group 3, after three months they were the lightest. A comparison of mean total fatfolds in the different dietary groups reveals a difference approaching signifi- cance at one month (p < .08). This reflects the extreme leanness of Group la as opposed to the relatively extreme adiposity of Group 1b. At birth there were no significant differences among the dietary pattern groups in percent of the median weight for length (Table IV.26). Changes in later months indicate possible dietary effects. Group la was virtually unchanged until two months, while Group lb increased substantially in weight for length during the first month (Figure IV.13). Groups 2a, 2b, and 3 were similar to each other in weight for length growth pattern from birth to two months of age (Figure IV.14). After that time all three groups decreased in weight for length. H0weVer, the decrease in Group 2b was particularly rapid, resulting in a significant difference among feeding groups at Month 5 (Table IV.26; p ‘<.01). 120 Since Groups 2a and 2b are in fact parts of a larger dietary group subdivided on the basis of first nonébreast milk food introduced, the marked difference in pattern of change was further explored. A least squares re- ‘gression line was calculated for each dietary group, using weight for length measures at Months 1 through 6 as the dependent, and age in months as the independent variable (Table IV.27). The slope--rate of decrease in percent of median weight for length--was over twice as great among those for whom breast milk was replaced by solids (Group 2b) as among those for whom breast milk was replaced by formula (Group 2a). Shifting the analysis to concentrate on the growth extremes, both weight for age and weight for length were in some way significantly related to the dietary groups. For these analyses, infants were ranked from largest to smallest for each growth measure at each month. Rankings were then divided into quartiles. Although the heaviest and lightest infants were randomly distributed among the feeding groups in the first few months of life, a significant pattern emerges by Month 3 (Table IV.28). The frequency of lowest weight for age quartile infants in Group 3 declined and that in the upper quartile increased somewhat. At the same time the weight for age of infants in the other groups was skewed toward lighter infants in the later months. These differences are significant (p < .05) at three, four, and 121 104 ‘ /.~ “‘ I '.‘ 102 ~ / “mu-«4. 100 - 98 5 96 ‘ 94 1 Birth 1. 2 3 4 5 6 months FIGURE IV.13--Mean Weight for Length (Percent of NCHS Median) for Infant Feeding Groups la (0—0) and lb (0 ----- O). 106 4 1.04 1 102 5 100 ‘ 96 < 94 4 92 1 9O ‘ W i l ' V T r Birth 1 2 3 4 5 6 months FIGURE IV.14--Mean Weight for Length (Percent of NCHS Median) for Infant Feeding Groups 2a (l-——-|), 2b (I ----- I), and 3 (L———A). 122 NG Nv.u mmHoo: uo N u Hsumo:c Hmm.Nc - No.mOH sN omouo om NN.- mmHoo: uo u u :sumo:c Ham-o1 - oq.mOH mN omomo mandamusmmmz H coHumsvm conmonom macho mchmom mo Honssz .H.OE m I HV 004 no nusoz mH poMHHm> ucmocomoocH new numeog How uanmz mH mHQMHHm> ucoocommn umcoHumsvm :onmmHmmmlnn~.>H mqmde 123 .nm :msounu MH mmsouw posHano Scum m macho mo mcoHuanHuch CH oozmquMHo How umou mumswm H£U«« .mHHuHmsq nomad .uanH «oHHuHmsv umo3oH CH Hogan: moHMOHocH puma so msam>m mo. mo. mo. DOCMOHMHcmHm HH.H. oq.m Hm.¢ «.ouomom Hso HH m NH m HH 4 OH H m m NH N NH N m N a N m H m m o m m m N N N sN H m N N H a N H m H H N N N mN a m H a N m N N N m N v N m sH v N m N N N H N H m H m N N oH G m H m N H sumHm omomo mcHommm .mazo: .msouw omHooom :s .sumo: somo uo omm omm sumHm um .omm uou usoHoz Ho moHHuHmso ummsmHm new umm3oq CH mucmmsH mo HmnESZ mo mCOmHHMQEOUIImm.>H mqm¢9 124 five months of age. In weight for length (Table IV.29), it is also <3bvious that distribution of infants into quartiles was non-random with respect to diet groups. However, the jpattern of variation between groups was different from that for weight for age discussed above. Groups lb and 2a, both based on dietary patterns "replacing" breast Inilk with formula, tended to have a high proportion of infants in the highest weight for length quartile, while Groups la and 2b, which did not replace breast milk with formula, had a high proportion of infants in the lowest quartile. These differences were significant in Months 3 through 5 (Table IV.30, p < .05). .Qetails of Breast Feeding Because early growth appeared to be predictive of lboth subsequent growth and diet patterns, two aspects <3f the association of early growth and the breast-feeding 1?rocess were explored in greater detail. These were llength of individual breast-feedings and number of feed- iJags per day. To minimize the confounding effects of crther food in the diet, only those infants who were ex- c=llusively breast-fed for at least two months were included (bh=47). Diet diaries at four weeks were examined and the rnxmber and mean length of feedings on this data noted for each infant. Infants were then assigned to two groups according to whether they had long or short and numerous or few feedings on this date. The medians were 12S .mHHuHmsw Moan: .quHH «mHHuHMCU umo3oH CH HonECC moumoHUCH uon Co mCHm>u HH m HH N NH 0H mH m CH m NH 0H NH m m H m o m o v o m m v N N m v QN N N H H m o N H m o N m N N mN m H m o m H m H v N N N N m QH H H N o o N H m H m o N H H MH 0 m H m N H suuHm omouo mCHpomm HSUCOZ .msouw omHooom Ns .sumo: sumo uo mmm omm suuHm um .summoH uoH usoHoz Ho mmHHuumso umoanm oCm umoSOH CH mqumCH mo HmQECz mo mComHHmmEoollmN.>H mHmme 126 hvo. voo- mNo. muHHHnmnoum uomxm m.HmCmHm H H G H m N mN omm sH monouo N G o G H N sN mmm oH momoHo ummCmHC umm3oH HmoanC ummon umoCmHC HmoBOH masono mCHoomm mHHuHqu mHHuHMCU oHHuHqu m CHCoz v CHCoS m CuCoz .AQN UCm MHV MHCEHom CuH3 :mCHUMHmmmm Boz mmosu UCo AmN UCm nHv.oHCEHom Cqu xHHz ummmum mmCHomHmmms mmCOHO Cmm3umn mmHHuHMCO CumCmH How quHoB umGBOH on umosmHm CH mqumCH mo HonECz CH mOCmHomeo mo umos uomxm m.HmCmHmllom.>H mHmda 127 used as the upper limits of the "few" feedings and "short" feedings categories. Table IV.31 shows the results of t-tests for differ— ences between the groups based on breast-feeding details in size at birth and one month of age. An inverse re- lationship existed between size at birth and length of nursing bouts. Infants who had short breast-feeds had greater weight, length, and total fatfolds for age than did infants with lengthy breast-feeds. At one month of age, infant size was more strongly associated with the number of feedings per day, than their length. This relationship of size and number of feedings was direct. Infants who had a high number of feedings per day had greater weight for length and were significantly fatter (p .01) than infants nursing a relatively few times per day. Growth velocity in the first month also appeared to be associated with number of feedings per day. Those infants with a high number of feedings had substantially greater increases in weight for age, weight for length, and total fatfolds than did those infants nursing relatively fewer times per day. Interview Data A variety of data obtained during pre- and postnatal interviews is compiled here. Portions of it come only from Study II in which the interview portion of the study was expanded and intensified. 128 smm.i HG. HGH.G Gm.H ANN.G mN. HGH.G Gm.H soommoHHHmon. u G.G H N.Nu o.G H m.HoH m.m H m.Gm N.mH H H.NGH momHmoom mmouosmz H.m H m-G- o.m H G.moH m.N H H.mm G.HH H o.moH mmmHooomuummoum zom AGO-G Go.N- HNo.G GG.H- ism-G NG.- sGo.G oo.N- roommoHHHmmHmG u G.H H N.oH- G.m H G.HoH H.m H H.NN N.mH H G.HoH momHooomuumoomm omoH G.G H H.G- N.G H H.HoH H.m H G.mm a.mH H H.NHH momHooomnumoon uuosm moHouuom summoH summoH\usmHoz usoHoz suuHm um oNHm o 'l I’lll'll"l'I'llI!I'lllllll'l'lllllllll"|'l'l|lllllll'I'I'I'I'l'll'l'l'l'lll"'lll|| I'l'lll'lllllI'lIll'lllll'lllll'll'lllll'II"IIIIIIII'|l'l'Ill'Illlll'lllll'lll'll .m CCm 4 mmsouw CH mHoCuoz Ho mUCmmCH mCOEm mCHCommnummon mo mHHmuma oCm Cu3ouw Cmm3umn mHCmCoHuonMIIHm.>H mqmma 129 AHo.v mm.NI Hov.v mm. Aho.v hm.HI mm.l +1 H.HHH +| h.m0H HwOCmonHCmHmV u mmCHUomm mCouoECz mmCHpomm ammoum 30m AmOCoonHCmHmV u mmCHomomnummmHm mCOH mmCHUmmqummon unonm o.N H H.H m.N H H.NoH m.G H o.GOH o.G H G.H- G.m H N.moH o.HH H m.mm sGG.G HG.- HHN.G NN.H- ANN.V HN. N.m H H.H- G.m H G.HoH m.oH H G.ooH G.N H v.5 m.m H G.NOH H.N H m.mm mpH0mumm CymCoH CumCmH\quH03 CHCoz 0:0 as oNHm. cmDCHHCOOIIHm.>H MHmda 130 Prenatal Plans for Infant Feeding All mothers in both Studies I and II planned to breast- feed their infants from birth. The earliest age planned for weaning was one month in the case of a mother return- ing to work at that time. No other mother had a specific length of time for which she intended to breast-feed. Most replied in general terms, with six to nine months being a common answer. Plans for first feeding of non-breast milk foods were elicited in Study II. Many mothers reported they had not given it much thought. Those mothers were in the highest frequency in feeding Group 1, those infants exclusively breast-fed the shortest time. In comparison, mothers of Group 3, those infants exclusively breast-fed the longest time, were most likely to give a specific age range (Table IV.32). Infant Feeding in the Parental Generation Thirty-two women knew whether or not they had been breast-fed as infants. Of these, only nine had been breast-fed. Three knew that, although they had been bottle- fed, their husbands had been breast-fed. Six reported that either their mother or mother-in-law had tried to breast-feed, but had given up after a maximum of two weeks. They reported "insufficient milk" as the main cause of weaning. The remaining fourteen had been bottle-fed, as had their husbands. 131 TABLE IV.32—-Study II Responses to the Question: When will you first give your baby anything but breast milk (or an occasional "convenience bottle")to eat? Mother's Responses 1 2 3 Total Have not thought about this 4 l l 6 When doctor advises 2 l - 3 Have thought about; not yet decided - - 4 4 When returning to work - 1 l 2 Between 3 months and 6 months - 2 - 2 After 3 months - 3 13 16 Missing data - l - 1 Totals 6 9 19 34 Table IV.33 breaks these categories down by feeding Groups 1 to 3. There is a distinct trend toward feeding Group 3 in cases where one or both parents had a history of being breast-fed. Bottle-feeding predominated in parents of Group 1 and 2 infants. Spatial Arrangement of Families The possibility that spatial arrangements of the family might be linked to infant feeding was assessed. It was assumed that intensity of mother-infant interaction might vary with spatial distributions. In order to ex- amine this, two categories of family dwelling types were devised. The first consisted of those dwellings judged 132 TABLE IV.33-~Parental Generation Feeding as Infants by Feeding Group of Infants in Present Study (Study II only). Infant Feeding Group Neither Parent Breast-Fed 5 5 4 14 One Parent Unsuccessfully Breast-Fed; Other not Breast-Fed 1 l 4 6 Father Breast-Fed; Mother not Breast-Fed - - 3 3 Mother or both Parents 1 Breast-Fed l 2 7 9 Feeding Modalities Unknown - l l 2 Totals 6 9 19 34 1 Both of these mothers cut short their infants' exclusive breast-feeding to return to work. to be compact: one story apartments and small houses with a maximum of four rooms, all on one floor and in a 2 x 2 arrangement. The second category consisted of dwelling arrangements judged to be dispersed: two story townhouses and houses, and one story or split level ranch style homes. Table IV.34 shows the distribution of dwelling types by infant feeding group. Infants raised in dispersed type dwellings were far more likely to be exclusively breast- fed for a short period of time (Groups 1 and 2) than were those raised in compact dwellings (x2 = 10.30; p < .01). 133 This does not appear to be a spurious association result- ing from a more direct association of socio-economic status with spatial arrangement. Table IV.35 shows that maternal formal education, the best socio-economic pre- dictor of breastffeeding behavior,is randomly distributed with respect to dwelling type. TABLE IV.34--Distribution of Infant Feeding Groups by ’Spatial Arrangement of Family Dwelling. Spatial Arrangement Infant Feeding Group Compact Dispersed Totals Group 1 5 13 18 Group 2 5 16 21 Group 3 25 16 41 Totals 35 45 80 134 TABLE IV.35--Distribution of Spatial Arrangement of Dwellings by Maternal Formal Education. Spatial Arrangement Education Level Compact Dispersed Totals High School or Vocational School 9 9 18 Some College (Less 6 14 20 than Bachelor's Degree) Bachelor's Degree 12 14 26 Any Graduate School 8 8 16 Totals 35 45 80 Chi Square Test: x2 — 2.16; Not Significant Decision Makinngata Reasons given by mothers of Study II infants for adding the first non-breast milk foods to their infants' diets can be grouped into four general categories. De- tailed data are listed in Appendix D. The categories are: 1. Infant behavior 2. External suggestion or prescription 3. Situational 4. Maternal health Table IV.36 shows the distribution of these categories by month of infant age at first introduction of non-breast milk food. Each is described in more detail in the following paragraphs. 135 TABLE IV.36--Categories of Maternal Decisions to Introduce First Non-Breast Milk Food Maternal Decision 1 2 3 4 5 6 Totals Infant Behavior 2 2 1 4 4 0 13 External Suggestion l 0 0 l 5 l 8 Situational 0 2 l 0 2 l 6 Maternal Health 3 o o o o o ' 3 Mothers in the modal category-~infant behavior-— emphasized the failure of their infants to behave in ways they considered appropriate for infants of that age. This included instances in which infants had not yet begun sleeping through the night, as well as cases in which infants who had previously slept through the night began to awaken again for a night feeding. This category also included infants who cried or exhibited other he- haviors interpreted as indicative of a desire to nurse at what the mother considered overly frequent intervals during the day. Although it is highly likely that many of these mothers had received advice from other persons or from the media which contributed to their introduction of formula or solids, each claimed that she had arrived at 136 the decision herself. While a few mothers explicitly stated that their infants' behavior indicated a lack of satiety with breast milk, most treated the infants' behavioral state as something unrelated to current diet, but something which the foods introduced had the power to ameliorate. For example, small amounts of cereal were thought to cause infants to sleep through the night, though their precise action was unknown. Thirteen mothers were placed in this category. Examination of the temporal distribution shows that these mothers augmented the breast milk diet either early-- before the end of the second month--or 1ater--at four and five months--after a long period of exclusive breast- feeding. Mothers in the second most frequent category of reasons attributed their behavior to a suggestion from someone external to the nursing couple that the infant had a biological need not being met by the breast milk diet. These included one instance in which a pediatrician told a mother that her infant's weight gain was inadequate due to insufficient calories in the breast milk. It also included a number of cases in which physicians claimed that breast milk could not provide a specific nutrient-- iron--which the infants needed at a particular age. Also included in this category are mothers who responded to pressure from relatives charging that infants of a given age or size could not be adequately nourished on 137 breast milk alone. Seven infants are in this category. All feeding changes but one took place in or after the fourth month. Most of these cases share common features. In all but the instance of inadequate infant weight gain, the existence of an unmet need in the infant was diagnosed without a specific means of assessment. Physicians did not test for iron levels and mothers-in-law did not weigh or measure infants. The criteria for recommending a new food was a combination of age and "bigness." Although the mother herself may have been inclined to make the dietary change at the same time, she attributed her be- havior to an external source. The third most frequent category of reasons for introducing new foods was that based on situations ex- ternal to the nursing couple. They included mothers going back to work or to school and choosing to either replace breast-feedings or wean completely. There were five infants in this category, and the feeding changes occurred randomly from the second through the fifth month. Three mother-infant pairs fall into the fourth category--maternal health. In these cases some health problem specific to the mother prevented nursing. In two instances mothers were hospitalized again within the first month postpartum. In the other, the mother reported she was unable to nurse due to sore nipples. All these feeding changes occurred by the end of the first month. 138 The four reasons discussed above account for 28 mother-infant pairs. Of the remaining five mothers, two could not articulate reasons for feeding new foods, one cited a reason which did not match any others given by other mothers (wanting to take advantage of her infant's interest in solid foods so as not to have a "picky eater"), and two exclusively breast-fed throughout their participa- tion in the study. From inspection of the data it appeared that regard- less of reason for feeding the non-breast milk item, the foods themselves followed the patterns of the entire sample. That is, formula tended to be given in the earliest months and solids, in the latest. Furthermore, there appeared to be no effect of decision making category on whether the new food was a dietary addition or replace- ment (Table IV.37). Maternal Nutritional Status Analyses reported here were designed to examine the relationship of breast-feeding practices to changes in maternal nutritional status in the postpartum. These changes include loss of body weight and changes in fat stores. Characteristics of the sample as a whole will be described first. Maternal changes will then be analyzed in terms of the common feeding patterns identified earlier in this chapter, months of breast-feeding, and other details of the breast—feeding process. 139 mmH\mNH mH\- osom umonH mHmuoa G m w m N H ConHooo HMCHoumz sumo: .moHHomwumo ConHooo Ns ooom sHH: ummommnmoz ummHm Ho umosoomHoom Ho moHuHooH-usm.>H HHmme 140 Descriptive Statistics for the Total Sample The amount of weight women lost between the last pregnancy weighing and the first postpartum weighing averaged 8.7 i 1.6 kg (Table IV.38). After a continued steady loss to three months postpartum averaging 2.7 kg, the weight loss plateaued, with only a slight additional loss (0.6 kg) to six months (Figure IV.15). This loss represents 1.9 i 3.6 kg above reported prepregnant weight at six months. The degree of variability in weight loss-- indicated by the standard deviation--is lowest for that period spanning the birth. Afterwards, it increases rapidly to three months, and then, like the average weight change itself, it stabilizes. Midarm muscle area was calculated from arm circumfer- ence (c) and triceps fatfold (8) using the formula (C ;"552 (Gurney and Jelliffe 1973). After a slight initial increase during the first month postpartum, average muscle area declines steadily to five months, followed by a slight increase in the sixth month (Table IV.38). The mean overall loss is 1.0 cm2 (Figure IV.16). The standard deviation at each point ranges from i 2.5 to i 6.6 cm2. In contrast, the long range trend in midarm fat area %? - %?2 ) is an increase. By the end of the third month the average mother has had an increase of 2.4 cm2 (Table IV.39). There is some fluctuation in the mean fat area increments after that time, but the cumulative effect at six months is an increase of 1.9 cm2 (Figure IV.16). 141 sz CoHHMH>oC oumoCmum mev Coos omoH ou uHoH usoHoz sz CoHumH>ma CHMCCmum mev Coo: mmOH quHmz Enuummumom Hmmv HHNV Amps Hmhv Anny Anny Hmnv N.N N.N H.N H.N N.N N.N G.N N.H N.N N.N N.N G.N N.a N.m Hmov HHNV Ammo Achy Anny Anny Ammo G.N N.N N.N H.N N.N H.N G.H o.NH H.HH N.HH G.HH N.oH G.N H.N G N G N N H m sumo: .Eduummumom oCu mCHHCU mmOH pCmHmz HMCHoumz HO mmHCmmoz 0>HHMHCECUIImm.>H MHmHB 17 16 15 14 13 11 10 POSTPARTUM WEIGHT LOSS (kg) FIGURE IV.lS--Maternal Postpartum Weight Status; Mean : 142 T- /- N/ / 1 1 L 1 1 J -5. / r r v v v T v I 3 1 2 3 4 5 6 MONTHS POSTPARTUM Standard Deviation. ' 10 ’ 11 12 13 WEIGHT LEFT TO LOSE ABOVE PREPREGNANT WEIGHT (kg) 144 Hmmv AHNV Hmhv HGNV Hubs HNNV sz hm.N 0H.m mv.N mm.N om.N mN.m COHumH>mQ UHCGCmym vm.H Nm.N HN.N v¢.N mm.H mo. ANEOV Cow: omCMCU omud Ham Ammv HHNV Hmnv HGNV Anny Anny sz h¢.N mm.N «v.0 Hm.m hm.N mm.N COHumH>mQ cumccmum oo.HI MH.HI Nb.0l HN.OI HN.OI 0N. HNEOV Com: mmCmCU moud OHUmCz .ECuHmmumom «Cu mCHHCC momma Ham UCm mHomCz EHCGHZ CH mmmCmCO m>HuwHCECUIImm.>H mHmde 145 The variability in fat area changes--represented by the standard deviation-sis greater in the first and fifth months, and lowest in the fourth and sixth. The change observed in midarm muscle area constie tutes an average decline of three percent from the first postpartum measurement to the last. The increase in fat area for the same period represents an average change of nine percent. Although the postpartum change in mean muscle area was fairly smooth, there were enormous month to month fluctuations in the measurements of individual women, resulting in extremely large standard deviations such as those in Months 4 and 5. Such fluctuations were too great to have resulted from gain or loss of muscle mass due to mobilization of pregnancy protein stores. They could have been a reflection of variations in hydration associated with the physiological changes of the post- partum period and lactation. They also could have been influenced by changes in muscles brought about by intensive lifting and carrying of infants. Given these possible confounding variables, the value of muscle area as a measure of nutritional status in such women is question- able. Since adipose tissue is affected by hydration to a far lesser extent, fat area, but not muscle area, will be used throughout these analyses as an assessment of maternal body composition. 146 Maternal Measures by Common Infant Diet Patterns Table IV.40 shows cumulative weight loss at each postpartum measurement by infant diet patterns. Using analysis of variance, no statistically significant differences among groups were found at any time. Despite the lack of statistical significance, trends in group weight loss patterns are notable. Group lb--those mothers who began replacing breast-feedings with formula in the first month-~had the lowest rate of loss throughout the study, with a total loss of 10.06 kg. This compared with a mean loss of 12.29 kg in the remainder of the sample.' Although Group 2a--those mothers who replaced breast- feedings with formula during months two and three-- had an initial weight loss comparable with most other groups, by six months its total loss was only 10.75 kg. Thus, in terms of overall rate of loss, Groups 1b and 2a appeared to lose weight most slowly and Groups la, 2b, and 3, most rapidly. When cumulative changes in midarm fat area were analyzed by infant dietary pattern by month, there were no statistically significant differences between groups (Table IV.41). However, trends similar to those in weight loss discussed above were present. Groups lb and 2a--those mothers replacing breast-feeds with formula-- had the greatest overall increase in fatness. All groups increased in fat area during the first three months. After that time the direction of change was variable. 147 ANN-G HN.H GN.NHNN.NH HN.HHNo.NH NN.NHNN.GH NG.NHGo.oH GG.NHNN.NH G ANN-v No.H HN.NHNH.NH NN.HHNN.NH HN.HHGG.oH NG.NHNN.N GG.NHNG.NH N ANN.V NH.H NN.NHNN.HH NG.NHNo.NH NN.HHHo.HH GG.NHNH.N HN.NHoa.HH a AHH.G oo.H GH.GHNN.NH NH.NHGN.HH GN.GHHN.GH HN.NHNN.N NN.NHHN.HH N ANN-V Na. HN.NHNG.oH HG.NHHN.GH oo.aHaH.oH GH.NHoN.N NN.NHNG.GH N ANN-G HN. HN.HHGN.N NN.NHNN.N HN.NHNG.N NN.HHNN.N Go.NHHo.OH H AHN.G GN. NG.HHGG.N NN.HHoH.N NN.HHHN.N HN. HNG.N mo.NHNN.N N\H ioHGG m .HHG N ANHG mm Has mN HHHG sH 1N1 mH sumo: <>oz¢ Esuummpmom I .COHHMH>oQ oumoCmum + mev Cum: .mCouw mCHpoom quwCH UCm CuCoz Esuuwmumom wn mCHCmHoB ECuHmmon ummq EOHH mmOH uanoz HmCHouwz o>HumHCECOIIov.>H mqmfie 148 me. mb.H um. VN. mm. wv. HN.NHGN.H GN.NHNG.H NN.NHGG.N HN.HHNG.N HG.~HNG. G HN.NHNN.N oa.HHHN.N HN.HHNG.N NN.NHGN.N Na.NHNN.H G GN.NHNG.H NN.HHNH.N NN.NHNH.H HN.NHNN.N NG.NHNo.N G GG.NHoo.N GN.NHHN.N oa.GHaG.N HN.HHNG.H NN.NHNN.N N HN.NHNo.H NN.NHGH.H NN.NHNN.N HN.NHNN.N No.NHGN.H N HN.NHNG. GN.NHoG. HN.NHGG. NN.HHNN.H HN.HHNH. H AHHG N ANHG sN sac mN AHHG sH ANG mH sumo: Enuummumom .sz mCouw mCHomom qumCH .COHHMH>OQ ouprmum H HNEov Com: .msouu mCHUmwm uCMMCH UCC CuCoz Esuuomumom >9 quEmHCmmmz ECuHmmumom umHHm Scum mmum pom EHMCHE CH omCMCU o>HumHCECUIIHv.>H MHmmB 149 Maternal Measures by Months of Breast Feeding Table IV.42 shows cumulative monthly weight loss analyzed in terms of breastffeeding. Although no statisti- cally significant differences exist, mothers who weaned before the third month had a substantially lower mean loss (10.6 kg) than those who weaned later (12.3 kg). The possible connection between overall weight loss and the timing of weaning appears to be the result of differences in weight loss velocity before and after weaning. In all groups breast-feeding less than five months, weight loss velocity before weaning is greater than after (Figure IV.17). In all four of these groups there was either no further weight loss or an actual gain in weight in the month following weaning. Furthermore, the preweaning velocity tended to be greater in those breast-feeding longer. Thus, the net result is the relationship between timing of weaning and weight loss. The sample sizes in those groups being compared in Figure IV.17 are small and the variation in weight change at any point is great. Thus, while the trends in mean weight change are suggestive of an association with weaning, they are not predictive of the postpartum weight loss for any individual woman and analysis of them does not produce statistically signifi- cant results. There were no significant differences in midarm fat area between groups based on months of breastffeeding. Moreover, there was no discernable pattern of change when 150 AHN.G NG- No.mHNH.NH HG.HHoo.HH NN.NHNN.NH NG.¢HNN.NH No.NHNN.oH HN.HHNN.GH G ANN-G HN. NG.NHGN.HH .NG.NHNH.OH NH.NHNN.NH HN.HHHN.NH NH.NHNN.N NN.HHNN.GH N AHa.G No.H NG.NHHG.HH HH.NHNN.N .NG.NHoo.NH NG.NHGN.NH oo.NHNG.N HN.NHNN.N a ANN.V NN. NN.NHNN.HH GN.HHNN.N HG.NHNN.NH «NH-«HHN.NH HN.HHON.N NH.NHGH.N N AHN.V HN.H NG.NHNG.GH NN.HHNN.N NH.NHNH.NH HN.NHHG.HH .HN.HHNN.N NN.HHHN.N N ioa.o NG.H NN.NHNG.N GN.HHNH.N GN.NHGN.HH NN.NHGH.oH NG- HGN.N «Na. HNN.N H HNN.G GN. NN.HHWN.N NG..HGN.N NN.NHNN.N HH.HHHH.N HN..HHH.N oak Hon-N N\H :onv m AHNG G 1N1 N AN1 4 ANG N 1G1 N ANG H .sumo: Esuummumom <>OZ< sz mCHomom unmonm mo mCuCoz l'lllll'l'l'l'I'Il'lll'llll"IIIII'IllllIIIlllllll|'|"l||'lll'|'llll'll'l'lll||"lll'll lllllllll'III'IlllIII|Illllll'lllll'IIllllll"|llll|lll.lllllllll'l||I|I'|"I|'llllllllll .CoHumH>oo UHCUCwum H mev Coo: .mCHomom ummmum mo mCuCoz UCM CuCoz Eduuomumom an mCHCmHmS Esuummmnm ummq Eoum mmOH uanoz o>HumHCECUIINv.>H mHmde 151 12 1 10 ‘ ‘ .56 .57 12 T 1.25 .24 12 1 10 ‘ .77 .54 12 5 10 .50 .38 POSTPARTUM MONTH Pre- Post- Weaning Weaning Weight Change Velocities FIGURE IV.17-~Cumulative Postpartum Weight Loss by Month of Weaning. Pre- and Post-Weaning Weight Change Velocities (kg/month) Are Indicated. (0 =5- Month of Weaning). 152 the overall rates of growth in fat were compared (Table IV.43). It appears that early weaning was followed by an immediate increase in fatness whereas later weaning (after two months) was followed by a much lower increase or a decrease. When numbers of women increasing and decreasing in fat area in the month following weaning are compared (Table IV.44), the same result appears. A similar, but stronger, relationship exists between fat change and the timing of the introduction of the first non—breast milk food. Table IV.45 shows that introduction of such food in the early months was followed by an increase in maternal fat, whereas later introduction was followed by a decrease (p < .01). Maternal Measures and Details of Breast Feeding Practices In order to study the association of maternal post- partum weight and fat changes with the number and length of nursing bouts, a series of Spearman correlation co- efficients was computed for the entire sample. The values for the breast-feeding variables were calculated by averaging the number of all feedings and length of all feedings reported within each month. Any woman who reported any breastffeeding during a particular month is included in the computation of the correlation for that month. Thus, the sample size decreases from the first to the sixth month as mothers wean their infants. 153 mCdSHEHmoshF82« ANG.V NG. NN.NHNN.N NN.NHGH.N HN. HHH.H HN.NHHH. NN.NHNN.H- NN.NHGN.N G ANG.G GN. NH.NHNN.N .GN.oHNN.N NN.NHGG.H HN.NHoo.N HN.NHNN. NN.NHNG.N N AON.V NN.H GN.NHGN.N GN.HHNN.N .NN.HHNG.N GN.NHNN.H .NN.¢HNN. NN.HHNN.N a ANH.G NN.H NN.NHNN.N HN.NHHN.N NN.NHNH. n._.NN.HH.._.o.H HN.NHoN.H HN.NHNN.N N sic 3. NHHQH fioflq NfimflN SNWWT fiifiSH Qéflfia N ANN.G GN.H No.NHoN. NH.NHHN.N HN. HHo. NG.NHNN.HI HN.HHoa.- «GN.HHNH.N H AoHGG m IHNV G :NG N ANG a 1N1 N 1G1 N INC H suso: Esunmmumom ¢>OZ< Izv mmHooom-ummoum Ho msumo: I .CoHu0H>mo UHMUCmum + HNEOV Cmoz .mCHUommlummoum mo mCuCoz oCm CuCoz Enuummumom ha uCoEoqummz Ezuummumom umHHm Eoum moufl pom EHMCHE CH memnu o>HHMHCECUIImv.>H mqmfia 154 TABLE IV.44--Number of Mothers with Increasing and 'Decreasing Fat Measures in the Month Following Weaning, by Month of Weaning. 'MOnth of Weaning“ f Change in - Fat Area 1 2 3 4 5 6 Totals Increase 7 3 5 l 1 - 17 Decrease - l 3 2 1 - 7 Totals 7 4 8 3 2 - 24 TABLE IV.45--Numbers of Mothers with Increasing and Decreasing Fat Measures in the Month Following First Introduction of Non- Breast Milk Food, by Month of Introduction. MOnth of Introduction Change in Fat Area 1 2 3 4 5 6 Totals Increase 16 7 5 7 7 - 42 Decrease l 2 2 3 10_ -_ 3 _ _-_21 ------- Totals 17 9 7 10 17 3 63 .__...___..___._.________.._.....g _________________________ 155 The correlations between monthly maternal weight loss and both breast feeding variables were not statisti- cally significant (Table IV.46). A weak positive associa- tion existed for Month 1 between weight loss and length of feedings (p < .09), but it did not continue into the later postpartum months. Table IV.46 also shows the correlations of changes in maternal fat area and the breast-feeding variables. The length of feedings was not statistically correlated with change in fat area until the‘sixth month. At that time long feedings were associated with the greater loss of fat; and short feedings, with the lower loss or gain of fat (p < .02). The pattern of interaction of fat area with number of feedings per day is more distinct. Higher numbers of feedings per day are associated with greater loss of fat; lower numbers of feedings, with less loss of fat. The correlation is fairly strong in the second month (p < .066) and highly significant in the fourth month (p < .005). To further explore the relationship between number of feedings per day and fat area change, a series of analyses were performed in which several possible con- founding variables were controlled for. Mothers were included in these analyses only if they exclusively breastefed for at least two months and did not wean before the fifth month, and if clear patterns of change in their postpartum fatness could be identified (N=46). 156 TABLE IV.46--Spearman Rank-Order Correlation Coefficients: Change in Maternal Weight and Fat with Length and Number of Breast-Feedings. Monthly weight.Loss with: bkmtmm'of Feedings/Day Correlation Coefficient .019 (N) (68) Significance .434 Nfinutes/Feeding Correlation Coefficient .170 (N) (66) Significance .086 anthly Fat Loss With: NmflmmcfifFaafingsflhw Correlation Coefficient -.004 (N) (66) Significance .487 Nfinutes/Feeding Correlation Coefficient -.099 (N) (64) Significance .219 .111 -.051 (68) (55) .135 .355 -.029 .045 (68) (55) .409 .374 .196 -.067 (61) (49) .066 .325 -.125 -.113 (61) (49) .169 .220 .142 (50) .164 .068 (49) .321 .394 (44) .005 -0 037 (43) .408 —. 033 (45) .416 -.135 (44) .193 .194 (37) .126 -o 130 (36) .225 -.O65 (41) .343 .148 (40) .182 -.194 (35) .133 .363 (34) .018 157 The means by which such patterns were identified will be described below. The feeding criteria were aimed at limiting unmeasurable differences in the intensity of breast-feeding due to consumption of other foods by 'the infants. Data on breast-feeding details for these analyses were taken from diet diaries at specific dates of four, eight, twelve, sixteen, and twenty weeks. This was to control for differences between infants in the dates of diet diaries within the same month. Finally, in these analyses changes in maternal fat area were classified according to the overall pattern of such changes. This was designed to control for minor monthly fluctuations in fat area which might have been obscuring overall trends in maternal nutritional status. To define patterns of change in fatness, a least squares linear regression line was first computed for each subject, using days since parturition as the independent, and fat area as the dependent variable. An "r" value was calculated for each of the regression lines and a test of significance was performed. Figure IV.18 shows the distribution of significance levels for those 50 mothers who met the infant feeding criteria described above. There is a clear clustering of subjects at and below the .10 significance level with several outliers at and above the .30 level. Because of this distribution, .10 was used as the cut off for inclusion in these analyses rather than the .05 level which is otherwise used Flllllllllllllllllj O H FIGURE 158 .1 __ :5: .:3___BE]_._EJ .05 .10 .15 .20 .25 .30 .35 .40 .45 .50 LEVELS OF SIGNIFICANCE IV.18--Distribution of Significance Levels for Regression of Postpartum Maternal Fat Area on Days since Parturition. 159 throughout this dissertation as the upper limit of statistical significance. After narrowing the sample down to those for whom a consistent pattern of change in fat area could be docu- mented, the group was further subdivided according to the direction of this change. All those women for whom the slope of the regression line was negative formed one group (Group A; N=l7). This was the expected pattern, but numerically it was less common. Those with a positive slope formed the second group (Group B; N=29). The following analyses compare these two groups. A comparison of maternal variables shows no signifi- cant differences between these groups in prepregnant weight, height, birthweight of the child, or length of pregnancy (Table IV.47). Group A did gain significantly more weight during pregnancy than did Group B, but after the first month postpartum there was no significant difference between the groups in the amount of weight left to lose. Also, there were no significant differences between the groups in the timing of introduction of non- breast milk foods after two months. Two major between group differences in breastefeeding behavior were observed. The first confirms the relations ship noted in Table IV.46: number of feeds per day is linked to postpartum changes in maternal fatness. Group A reported a significantly higher median number of feeds per day at 4, 8, and 12 weeks than did Group B (Tables IV.48-50). 160 NN. NH. GG. Na. 6N. 6N. NN. w NH. GN. NG. NN. NH.H No. HN.H Go. NN.H Ho. NN.N HN. GH. No. NH.N unuunuun-uuleme ..... H---- o.NH N.HNN N.GH N.HNN smNmmc Nommmouma uo summoH N. .H N.N a. H H.N loss usmHozsuuHm N.N.H N.GGH N.N.H N.NGH Amos usoHom Hmmuoum: G.N H G.N N.H H N.N smuuooumoa G sumo: uo omm N.N H H.N H.H H N.N smummoumoa N sumo: Ho omm N.N H H.N G.N H N.N smuumoumoa a sumo: Ho omm N.G H N.H N.G H H.H smuuooumoa N sumo: uo omm N.N H G.N H.N H N.N smuuooumom N sumo: mo omm N.N H N.N N.N H N.G smuuomumoa H sumo: Ho omm "uanmz HCMCmonmHm o>onm mm N.N H N.NN N.G H N.NN loss usmHoz ummmmouomum N.N H H.NH H.N H N.GH lost mHoo usmHoz somomooum 55m-mmmmm ------- m-mmmmm ------------------------ mmmmH-mmm .CoHumH>mo CHMGCmum H Cmmz .m UCm H mmsouw HON mOHQMH-Hmkr QEOOHDO SUM-mm @CM OflHUmEOQOHSUCAN HMQHGUN—lebwokfin HQQB 161 TABLE IV.48--Number of Breast-Feedings per Day at Four Weeks for Infants of Mothers in Groups A and B. Feedings per Day' Group A l 0 2 4 4 2 2 l 0 16 Group B 2 2 12 5 2 3 l 0 l 28 Totals 3 2 l4 9 6 5 3 l l 44 Median Test: Median = 7 Feedings/Day; x2 = 4.35; p < .05 TABLE IV.49--Number of Breast-Feedings per Day at Eight Weeks for Infants of Mothers in Groups A and B. Feedings per Day 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 Totals 162 TABLE IV.SO--Number of Breast-Feedings per Day at Twelve Weeks for Infants of Mothers in Groups A and B. ..... Feedings per Day Group A l 3 3 3 1 3 0 2 0 16 Group B 2 10 9 2 2 O 0 0 1 26 Totals 3 13 12 5 3 3 0 2 l 42 The second difference between Groups A and B is in mean length of individual feedings. Group A has shorter feedings than does Group B at each point in time. These differences approach statistical significance at four weeks and are highly significant from 8 to 20 weeks (Table IV.51). Thus, mothers with decreasing fat area during the postpartum have, in general, more and shorter feedings than do mothers whose fat area increases during the same time. In the combined group of 46, minutes per feeding is significantly and inversely correlated with number of feedings per day at 4, 8, 16, and 20 weeks (Table IV.52). 163 TABLE IV.51--Minutes per Feeding for Groups A and B. Mean : Standard Deviation. Week Group A Group B t Sig. 4 19.1 i 7.4 24.4 i 11.0 1.89 .07 8 16.4 i 5.5 24.0 i 10 8 2.65 .01 12 14.8 i 4.3 21.2 i 8.3 2.89 .01 16 13.6 i 5.4 19.7 I 9.1 2.42 .02 20 12.2 i 3.6 17.1 i 7.0 2.60 .01 TABLE IV.52--Spearman Rank-Order Correlation Coefficient: Minutes per Breast-Feeding with Numbers of Feedings per Day. Age of Infant (Weeks) Correlation Coefficient -.514 -.468 -.221 -.424 -.269 (N) (44) (44) (44) (44) (42) Level of Significance .001 .001 .075 .003 .043 CHAPTER V DISCUSSION Emergence of Infant Feeding Strategies In order to identify major infant dietary patterns, a series of analyses were done which assessed the patterns of interaction of the various food behaviors which com- prise infant diets. The following variables were con- sidered: length of exclusive breast-feeding, age at weaning, introduction of first non-breast milk food, and mean duration of nursing bouts and number of feedings per day in early infancy. From these it was apparent that one variable, in a sense, formed the core around which all the other dietary components were arrayed. This variable was the length of exclusive breast-feeding. On the basis of this three major dietary patterns are distinguishable. Although they are defined primarily on the basis of this one variable, the concordance of other variables supports this division. The first feeding group is composed of 18 infants exclusively breastefed less than four weeks. Among these, breast-feeding was never well established. In some cases, 164 165 maternal health problems precluded the establishment of lactation. In the remainder, a variety of factors working together led to the early introduction of non- breast milk foods. The detailed decision making data from Study II indicate that failure of infants to behave as expected by the mother as well as external suggestion of nutritional insufficiency led to early changes in the infant diet. Data from Study I support this: of twelve infants in Study I with this feeding pattern, four were known to have been diagnosed by physicians as losing weight beyond the expected initial perinatal weight loss. Most of the remaining eight infants were supplemented for other reasons. A large prOportion of mothers, for example, reported that their infants cried and fussed too frequently, as if unsatisfied and as a result, the mothers changed the infants' diets. In both studies, the new mothers were in many cases without "doulas."1 Their own mothers were frequently there to help in the early postpartum, but, never having breast- fed an infant themselves, either could not give advice appropriate to breastefeeding or suggested using non-breast 1Raphael (1966) devised the term doula as a label for the person who "mothers the mother." She has noted that such a role is culturally defined in most traditional societies. Furthermore, she attributes the decline of breast feeding in the 19505 and 19605 and increasing incidence of lactation failure in the industrialized countries to the isolation of new mothers from other sup- portive, experienced older breast—feeders. 166 milk foods. In Study II where maternal and paternal grandmothers' infant feeding modalities were known, all women in Group 1 were, in a sense, first generation breast- feeders. Five of six had been bottleéfed, and the other had been breastefed for two weeks until her mother "quit because it was so frustrating." Thus any advice these grandmothers could give from personal experience would have been from their 19503 experience with early solids, rigid schedules, and few feedings per day. It is possible that these mothers had a set of ex- pectations for infant behavior which differed from those of mothers in the other feeding groups. This seems un- likely, however, since all were raising their first child and most had read the same books and been exposed to the same information in prenatal classes. On the other hand, it is quite possible that due to the inhibition of the let-down reflex by factors such as stress, anxiety, and infrequent nursing as described by Jelliffe and Jelliffe (l979b), these infants really were unsatisfied and there- fore behaved in ways indicating it. Mothers of infants in Group 1 followed two different strategies for coping with the alleged insufficiencies of breast milk. The first was to add food to the existing breast milk diet (N=7). In most cases this was formula, and it was consumed by the infant in small amounts immediately after each nursing session. This feeding pattern was one which physicians usually prescribed to mothers if there was 167 any question about milk sufficiency. The second strategy was one of replacing entire breast-feedings with formula (N=ll). In no case where information was available was this pattern known to have been suggested by a physician. This distinction between the use of formula and, in two of eighteen cases, solids as additions or replacements roughly predicts the time of weaning. "Replacements" correlate with early weaning-~in all but one case, weaning occurred by the end of the second month. In contrast, with "addition" there was only one case in which the infant was weaned by the end of the second month. One possible reason for this difference in length of breast-feeding is physiological. Since milk production is maintained by the hormone prolactin, its production being stimulated by the beginning of each nursing session, the decrease in number of feedings per day in the "replacement" group should be expected to decrease milk supply, thus hastening the wean- ing process. Since in the "addition" category the number of breast-feedings per day remains unchanged after intro- duction of formula or other foods, no decrease in breast milk supply would be expected. One striking feature of the women in Study II who follow the Group 1 feeding pattern is their almost total lack of prenatal planning of their overall infant feeding strategy. All had made the choice of breastffeeding over bottle-feeding, but none had thought any further about the use of other foods. Some seemed rather startled to be 168 asked about this and reported that family and acquaint- ances often wanted to know whether or not they intended to breast-feed, but none ever showed any interest in other aspects of infant feeding. Thus, while they had every intention of nursing their infants, they were quite susceptible to suggestions from others about feeding their infants in ways which, at least in the early weeks of life when lactation was being established, could undermine their chances for being successful breast-feeders. In the remainder of the sample, all infants were exclusively breast-fed over five weeks, and thus lactation might be considered well established. They are divided into moderate length exclusive breast-feeders (N=21) and extended exclusive breast-feeders (N=4l), with the division at the end of the third month. This separation is neither arbitrary nor artificial. Instead, it reflects the contri- bution of the new non-breast milk food to the total diet. In those with the moderate length exclusive breast-feeding referred to as Group 2, the formula or solids introduced were almost always replacements for breast milk feedings, while in the extended exclusive breast-feeding group (Group 3), these foods were additions. The reason for this differential impact of intro- ducing either solids or formula into the diet may lie in the fact that, althOugh both groups were consuming solely breast milk at the time of the introduction, there were important internal differences in the configuration of 169 the diets. Group 2 infants had, from birth, had fewer breastffeeding bouts per day than Group 3. In Month 1, for example, the median number of feedings was six for Group 2 infants and eight for those in Group 3. Moreover, the average number of feedings per day tended to decline in Group 2 between the first and second months of life, while at the same time many Group 3 infants actually in- creased their daily feeding frequency. In some cases this increase may have been unnoticed. However, in at least one case it was deliberate. A mother in Study I, whose pediatrician prescribed formula supplementation because the infant failed to gain weight, instead chose to increase the number of feedings per day to increase her milk supply. (She also chose to change pediatricians to one more sup— portive of and knowledgeable about breast-feeding!) Thus when new foods were added to the diets of Group 2 infants, they may have reduced the children's appetites to some extent, thereby reducing suckling. Then through lowered levels of prolactin, the milk supply might well have decreased, hastening a transition from breast milk to a non-breast milk diet. The frequently earlier weaning of Group 2 infants may be an end product of this process. Of twenty-one infants in Group 2, eleven had been weaned by the end of the study, compared with only one infant in Group 3. Although the new foods fed to Group 3 infants may have had a satiating effect similar to that in Group 2, the 170 frequency of nursing bouts was already at such a high level that any reduction associated with the addition of new foods may not have appreciably affected prolactin levels. Thus, there would have been no progressive de- cline in milk production such as that hypothesized for Group 2. This is supported by Delvoye et a1.'s observation that a minimum of six feedings per day maintains high prolactin levels (1977). The actual reasons for the variability in the early months in number of feedings per day are unknown. Although it is tempting to describe Group 2 as implementing a pattern of "scheduled" feedings and Group 3 as feeding "on demand," this distinction cannot be made. A study of individual infants' diet diaries shows that while some nursed at the same time each day, these infants were a definite minority. For most Group 2 and 3 infants, the times of feedings and the intervals between them fluctuated from week to week. Relatively little concern was expressed by mothers over feeding schedules--beyond hoping for an uninterrupted night's sleep. Many claimed to be feeding their infants on demand and among these, the full range of feeding frequencies was represented. Thus, the term "on demand" may be more useful in describing maternal intentions than in describing actual maternal behavior. It is, in fact, the perception of the infant's demand which initiates a feeding, rather than the demand itself. Many factors can shape this perception. 171 Vis and Hennart (1978) have attributed the pattern of extremely frequent feedings and apparent contentedness of infants among the Kivu of Zaire to the continuous body contact of mother and child. A mother soon learns to read her infant's smallest behaviors and to react to them. In contrast, in the developed countries such as the United States, numerous factors now serve to distance a mother from her child and thus reduce her ability to perceive hunger or other needs. For example, cribs, strollers, and infant seats now alleviate the need to hold infants. In Lansing, Michigan it was observed that most pediatricians, if asked, strongly discouraged an infant's sleeping in the parents' bed or even in the parents' bedroom. One mother in Study I took this advice so seriously that the infant was given the home's only bedroom and the parent slept on a rather broken down sleep-sofa in the living room. Although it was not possible in this study to take objective measures of the amount of time mothers spent interacting with their infants, it was possible to separate the families into two categories, based on the degree to which their living situations forced close contact or permitted distancing. Infants in Group 2 were far more likely to live in two story dwellings (76%) than in single story, compact ones (24%). Conversely, a majority of Group 3 infants spent their early months of life in compact dwellings (61%) and a minority, in dispersed ones (39%). Thus, mothers living in situations which forced 172 them to be in close proximity to their infants fed them more frequently in the first few months than did women in larger dwellings. Among the latter, infants often napped in a second story bedroom while the mother was occupied on the first floor or even in the basement. An infant's quiet fussing which might be audible to a mother in a small apartment and perceived as hunger could go unnoticed in a larger home. The time necessary for an infant to gradually rouse itself to give a loud cry could be sufficient to create a significantly longer interbout interval, resulting in fewer nursing bouts per day. Another factor which might have affected the fre- quency of early feeding is breast-feeding advice received during the early postpartum. Like Group 1, most Group 2 mothers from Study II were really first generation breast- feeders. Some recognized this and, while accepting their own mothers' help after the delivery, restricted it to housekeeping activities, choosing to manage all baby care themselves. In Group 3, many of the mothers and fathers had been breastéfed themselves. Others who had not been breast-fed did have sisters or friends who were experienced breast-feeders, and the new mothers often chose these women over their ineXperienced mothers for help after coming home from the hospital. Therefore, Group 3 mothers probably received infant feeding advice more appropriate to breastffeeding than did Group 2. This may have contributed to the observed differences in patterns of 173 breast-feeding. Thus, the study of infant dietary data clearly shows that breastffeeding is not a unidimensional behavioral category. Within it are several distinctly different infant dietary patterns. These patterns appear to be relatively unplanned, instead evolving through the early months of the infant's life as one action taken by the mother selects certain later possible options for her and eliminates others. Because of this, it is possible to make predictions of later dietary patterns of infants based on early feeding habits. Of the variables examined in this study, number of nursing bouts per day appears to be the best predictor. Igteraction of Infant Diet and Infant Growth The analysis of infant growth data shows that its relationship to infant diet is a complex and dynamic one. Two types of interaction appear to occur. As has been assumed by much previous research, the foods and feeding habits of infants certainly seem to influence their growth. On the other hand, the way in which infants grow, particu- larly in the early months of life, appears to affect the manner in which they are fed. Together these form a reinforcing feedback relationship. This section will summarize the results of the data analysis which led to the inference that these two aspects of the infant feeding process exist. It will then show that supporting evidence can be found in some of the most widely cited studies of 174 infant growth and feeding. Finally, it will attempt to demonstrate that by changing one's assumption from that of a unidirectional causal link between feeding and growth to one of a more dynamic relationship such as that found in these data, some of the inconsistencies which have been the focus of so much attention and debate can be resolved. Influence of Growth on the Dietary Pattern of Infants This study demonstrates that, starting at birth, the feeding regime to which any infant is subjected is not a random event with respect to the size and growth of the child. In the most general terms, it appears that the feeding patterns of infants who are large at birth and display rapid growth velocity in the early weeks of life can be contrasted with those of infants which at birth are relatively small and initially grow rather slowly. Infants in Group 3--that is, those exclusively breast- fed until at least the fourth month of life and not weaned until the sixth--have at birth the greatest mean weight for age (106.4% of NCHS median) and weight for length (95.6% of median) of any of the three feeding groups. Those in Group l--with the shortest exclusive breast- feeding--have the lowest mean weight for age (102.6% of median) and weight for length (93.7% of median). The size superiority of the Group 3 infants is maintained through five months of age in weight and four months of age in weight for length, though the differences between groups narrows over time. One detail of breast-feeding practice 175 is also linked to size at birth. Those infants whose mothers settle into a pattern of shorter nursing bouts during the first month tend to be heavier and fatter at birth than those whose mothers report longer feedings. Not only are Group 3 infants bigger at birth, but their initial growth velocity in weight for age and total fatfolds exceeds that of either of the other two groups. Mean changes in percent of median weight for age in Month 1 were -l.8%, 1.5%, and +3.3% for Groups 1 through 3, respectively. In total fatfolds, the average changes were +5.9mm, +5.8mm, and +6.4mm for the groups. Although Group 1 experienced a major dietary change during that time, Group 2 along with Group 3 was exclusively breast- fed. There are several possible reasons why the feeding patterns of these infants appear to be a function of their early size and growth. First of all, there may be behavioral differences between large and small infants. If the former were more aggressive nursers or more vocal in communicating their demands to be fed, one would expect them to more rapidly establish the successful lactation which is characteristic of their feeding group. Data to document this are not available here, though when talking about infants in general, many mothers tended to attribute these behavioral traits to certain size infants. A second possible explanation is that the relations ship between infant size and feeding may exist as a part 176 of a larger complex of variables concerning maternal health and health consciousness. Favorable life-long living conditions and nutritional status (Weinstein 1981) as well as conditions during pregnancy (Philipps and Johnson 1977) have been linked to the birth of larger infants. These factors, in turn, characterize the health- awareness of women most likely to be successful, long term breast-feeders (Sauls 1980). The direct associations in this study of mothers' educational level and length of lactation are consistent with such a link. Thus, the correlation of birth size and feeding modality may in fact simply be incidental to this more direct relationship. The third possible--and perhaps most readily docu- mented--explanation for the association of growth and diet in this study is that mothers have concerns about and perceptions of infant needs which are specific to particular sizes and shapes of infants. Concern was expressed by a number of mothers in this study, for example, whose infants were relatively low in birth weight. This was particularly true of mothers who had been falsely led to believe they would give birth to larger than average children. The birth of a small infant in such cases seemed to cause considerable anxiety over the child's health. Mothers appeared to handle the infants more tentatively, often having a great deal of trouble dressing and undressing them for the first month or so. One of these mothers (with a husband 6'2" tall and herself 5'7" with a 49 pound 177 pregnancy weight gain) gave birth to a six pound son. The oversized clothes she had purchased in anticipation of a nine pound infant further accentuated what to her was his abnormally small size. Like other mothers in similar situations, she initially worried a great deal about his health and treated him--and made sure others treated him--as though he were very delicate. On the other hand, mothers of large infants tended to be more at ease with them, treating them as more rugged and less fragile individuals. The mother of the one set of twins studied gave only conditional consent before the birth to allow them to be measured. When each weighed over seven and a half pounds, she readily agreed to participate. She later attributed her earlier hesitation to the fear that they might be small babies. This variability in concern over the well-being of infants based on size appeared to manifest itself in attitudes toward infant feeding. Despite the growing public awareness that "bigger" infants are not always "better," such attitudes are not universal among new mothers. And even among those who were seriously con- cerned about obesity, there was a strong feeling that, while fat babies are not necessarily the healthiest babies, smaller ones are even more at risk. In discussing the infant silhouettes used to stimulate discussion of body shape, most mothers chose the fattest and thinnest infants as less healthy and of those, finally chose the m thinnest as the least healthy, saying that it looked malnourished. Some reasoned that a fat infant would probably lose its "baby fat," but an overly thin one might well be seriously ill. Such concern over thinness was not limited to hypo- thetical infants. Several mothers referred to their infants' thin limbs and visible ribs as "bird-like" when seen in the hospital after the birth or at home in the early postpartum. Others commented that they would "have to fatten up" their infants. Mothers such as this could easily doubt the efficacy of a feeding method such as breast-feeding with no immediate, tangible evidence (such as an emptied formula bottle) of success or benefit. An infant's cries which might, to a self-assured mother, have a different meaning, might be more likely to be interpreted as indicative of insufficient milk by a mother already anxious about her infant's health. Furthermore, the tension surrounding the feeding event in those cases might well result in inhibition of the let-down reflex. If a non-supportive atmosphere were added to this, the chances of successfully establishing lactation would probably diminish even further. The later growth of infants whose mothers were unable to successfully establish lactation indicates possible overcompensation. By Month 3, Group 1 infants were fatter than either Groups 2 or 3. In several cases, infants who had had initial weight losses became the 179 fattest infants in the study. One such mother was told by her pediatrician to overdilute her son's formula at four months. When interviewed after that, she expressed her frustration: "You know, at first I was so worried because he wasn't gaining weight. Now we have to worry because he's gaining too fast. You can't win--you never know what is right." While the small size of an infant sometimes worked against mothers' efforts to breast-feed, large size was used to advantage by others. Mothers with large infants who wanted to exclusively breast-feed used the issue of their infants' size to silence grandparents or pedia- tricians who considered breast milk an inadequate source of nutrients or thought supplements of solid food should be added at an early age. When asked if anyone had suggested a dietary change in the preceding month, these mothers (such as one whose child weighed over twelve pounds by one month of age) would laugh and poke at their infants' robust bodies, replying, "Yes, but does he 123k underfed?" Thus, infants who were initially small and grew slowly appear more likely to be exclusively breast-fed for short periods of time. This may be due to a combina- tion of factors, including maternal anxiety, inhibition of the let-down reflex, and succumbing to external pressures to supplement the breast milk diet. Larger infants may be less likely to be the objects of anxiety 180 and their mothers, better able to maintain breast-feeding in a nonsupportive social environment. Since the early feeding of an infant has been shown in this study to be a prime determinant of the future course of feeding, the impact of infant size on early feeding behavior may be an important factor in attempts to understand the dynamics of the infant feeding process. Influence of Dietarnyatterns on the Growth of Infants While the previous paragraphs have discussed the possible impact of size at birth and early growth patterns on early and important feeding behaviors, the following will examine what is, to some extent, the reciprocal relationship. It will concentrate on the way in which diet seems to affect growth. In a study such as this of a free-living sample of 80 infants consuming almost 80 different diets, it is impossible to try to answer some of the questions which have traditionally been asked in infant nutrition research. The relative impact of formula or breast milk, for example, becomes meaningless when infants are consuming varying amounts, perhaps eating solid foods as well, and, as has been argued above, are of different sizes. However, there is a type of issue which can be explored here better than in more controlled studies. That is the impact on growth of minor variations in dietary patterns in infants where mothers have decided these are the optimal feeding strategies for their children. Two of these variations were identified in the analysis of 181 the dietary intake data: 1) the use of the same food in diets for the same age children as replacements or additions to the existing diet, and 2) the use of different foods to fulfill the same dietary role in children of similar ages. Those infants in Group 1, all exclusively breast-fed for less than four weeks, provide the basis for comparing the effects of the use of formula as a dietary addition (Group la) and as a replacement (Group 1b). As stated earlier, those infants for whom it served as an addition consumed approximately two ounces after one or more nursing bouts per day. Those for whom it was a replace- ment consumed larger quantities of formula in place of entire nursing bouts. Generally among the former, weaning occurred after the second month and among the latter, before the end of the second month. Growth differences between these two groups were marked during the first month. Although the "addition" of formula suggests Group 1a infants might display accelerated growth while "replacement" suggests Group 1b infants would maintain steady growth, practically the opposite was observed. Group la fell from an average of 104.4% of median birth weight to 96.6% of the median weight for age at one month, while during the same period Group 1b increased from 101.5% to 103.5%. The decreasing percent of the median for la represents an actual loss of weight in at least two cases. Group la remained at a 182 constant 93.6% of weight for length during the first month, while Group lb rose from 93.8% to 100.1%. While both increased in total fatfolds, that for la was slight (+2.9mm), while the average increase for 1b was relatively large (+9.4mm). By one month Group la was the thinnest group of infants, compared to Group lb which was the fattest. Those Group lb infants which were weaned during Month 1 had the most marked increase in fatfolds during that month. Undoubtedly these early growth differences were due to differences in caloric intake. The larger amounts of formula in Group lb provided sufficient calories to maintain growth, but decreasing breast-feeding frequency in almost all cases eliminated any chances of breast- feeding success. The smaller amounts of formula in Group la were probably unable to compensate for the apparent insufficiency of breast milk, but, by not inter- fering with feeding frequency, allowed lactation to become established. In fact, some mothers in Group la were later able to reduce the formula supplementation and maintain infant growth and satiety on a diet consisting almost solely of breast milk. Thus, the introduction of the same food into the diets of similar aged infants produces different patterns of early growth. A closer examination of the role the new food has in the diet and the circumstances surrounding its introduction provide plausible explanations. 183 The second diet variation noted in this study which appears to have a significant impact on growth is the use of different foods in similar dietary capacities for the same age infants. The distinguishing characteristic of Group 2 was the overwhelming use of newly introduced foods as replacements for breast milk in the diet, rather than as additions. This is consistent with the research of Marlin et al. (1980) who found that before three months of age, most dietary changes are caloric replacements. In the present study, Group 2 has been further subdivided into those mothers who replace breast milk with formula (2a) and those who do so with solids (2b). The patterns of growth for Groups 2a and 2b began to show differences between one and two months, when the first introductions of non-breast milk foods occurred. From that time on, Group 2b displayed a slower overall growth rate than Group 2a. In weight for age, Group 2a went from 104.0% of the median at one month to 103.6% at five months. At the same time, Group 2b dropped from 106.5% to 101.1%. The difference between the groups was even more striking in weight for length. Between one and five months, Group 2a increased from an average 99.7% of the median to 102.0%, while Group 2b fell from 100.3% to 90.0% in the same time period. The difference between the groups was highly significant at five months. After one month of age, Group 2b infants were frequently smaller than infants in any other feeding group. 184 When the extreme growth quartiles were compared, both Groups 2a and 2b tended to be overrepresented in the lowest quartile of weight for age by three months. But of the two, only the infants in Group 2b were overrepre- sented in the lowest quartile of weight for length. Thus while all the infants in combined Group 2 tended to be small, those in 2b tended also to be thinner, and the difference between the groups increased over time. This plateauing of growth rates among infants consuming breast milk and solids after two months is consistent with that observed by Ferris et a1. (1979) for fatfolds. These differences between the groups in growth seem to reflect the caloric differences in the replacement food. The mean caloric value of the initial replacement formula was estimated to be 218 kcal, while that of the solids was 48 kcal. Thus, the total daily energy intake of the infants consuming solids probably declined immedi- ately. Since breast milk and formula are more nearly equivalent in calories, the energy intake of Group 2a infants probably began to decline only after the decrease in numbers of nursing bouts per day had an impact on milk production. Two reasons for introducing non-breast milk foods predominate in Study II, where decision making factors were specifically elicited, and in cases in Study I where such information appears in fieldnotes. It was during this period between one and three months that most mothers 185 who had planned to return to work did so. Also during this time, the remaining mothers began to feel that their infants were demanding to nurse too frequently, waking too frequently at night, or exhibiting other behaviors they deemed inappropriate. Mothers returning to work almost always introduced formula into the diet. Those seeking to change their infants' behavior used both formula and cereal, but the latter was more common. When the two reasons for the feeding change are compared, the introduction of a new food because of the physical separation of the nursing mothers and their infants clearly implies replacement. When these mothers discussed their plans, they did so in terms of which feedings (usually morning and late evening) they would retain as nursing bouts and which ones would be changed to formula feedings by the caretakers. The feeding of solids was not discussed in the same way. Although the mothers of infants in Group 2b were never asked, their attitudes and actions seem in retro- spect to indicate that they were not consciously trying to decrease their infants' breast milk intakes. They often reported, for example, that they had begun to feed their infants a certain amount of cereal at a specific time of day. In doing so, the event was often described in relation to either parental activities or non-feeding activities of the infant (e.g., before bedtime, after the parents' meal, or before the father came home from work). 186 Although the feeding categories defined here give the impression that the introduction of solids or formula are comparable options, little was observed to indicate that mothers viewed them as interchangeable. Many mothers in both studies attributed a pacifying effect to solids, particularly cereal, and it appears that it was this quality which those mothers in Group 2b sought. Formula was instead classified as a milk. Thus, those returning to work substituted a similar substance for the unavailable breast milk. Thus, the use of two different kinds of infant foods with similar impacts on the existing diet produces different patterns of growth. This can be explained by comparing their caloric values. An examination of the situations in which they tend to be used reveals the rationales for their introduction. SupportingiEvidence from the Infant Growth and Diet Literature Although other authors (Sauls 1980; Himes 1979) have stated that samples of breast-fed infants are highly selected and that those infants not thriving on breast milk automatically drop into other feeding categories, data to support this have never been explicitly presented. However, in reviewing several of the growth studies cited in Chapter II, similarities of data gathered in those studies to those data presented here are apparent. 187 In three investigations, infants who were either weaned or changed from breast milk to a mixed diet within the first six weeks of life had slower rates of growth during that period than did those infants who continued to be exclusively breast-fed. Fomon et al. (1970) compare the 45 infants who dropped out of their study with those 149 who adhered to the prescribed breast milk diet. "Insufficient milk" was reported to be the reason behind much of the attrition. Female infants who did not complete the study had a weight gain velocity only slightly lower than that of those in the study. However, the difference for males was more pronounced, significantly so in the second month. Holly and Cullen (1977) compare breast-fed infants and those receiving both breast milk and formula by six weeks. While the former had a per- centage weight gain of 33% in the first month, the latter increased by only 24%. Saarinen and Siimes (1979) show that infants weaned to either of two types of formula before one month of age had lower weights and lower weight gain velocities from two weeks to two months of age than did infants who continued to be exclusively breast-fed. Thus infants making more significant early weight gains do appear to be more likely to continue to consume a diet composed solely of breast milk than those with slower rates of growth. The possible overcompensation in feeding by mothers whose infants were either weaned or changed to a mixed 188 diet before one month of age is supported by evidence from two studies. By six months of age the mixed-feeding infants in Saarinen and Siimes study (1979) had gone from their earlier weight deficit (in comparison to those breast-fed) to a weight advantage. And in Huenemann's study of six month old infants, she finds that those breast-fed less than one month are overrepresented in the upper 10% and underrepresented in the lower 10% of her sample when ranked on an obesity index derived from weight, fatfolds, and girth measures (1974). Therefore, examples of the interactions of growth and feeding observed in the present study can be found in the infant feeding literature. However, they have normally been presented without comment or statistical analysis, perhaps because of the assumptions about the interactions of infant growth and diet held by the researchers. Conflicting Evidence for the Impact of Diet on Growth As shown in Chapter II, the current literature on in- fant diet and growth is plagued by conflicting results. Evidence can be cited both for and against the argument that formula leads to heavier and fatter infants than does breast milk. The same is true for early or late introduc- tion of solid foods. This section suggests that an approach to the study of_growth and diet which does not assume a unidirectional relationship between growth and diet may be useful in trying to resolve these inconsistencies. The 189 disputes over the effects of formula are perhaps most easily resolved. When several studies purporting to compare infants "breast-fed" and "bottle-fed" are examined, their results appear to depend on the way in which infants are assigned to feeding groups. Saarinen and Siimes (1979) classify as bottle-fed all infants who were initially breast-fed, but were weaned by one month of age. In contrast, the University of Iowa studies (Fomon et al. 1970; Fomon et al. 1971) and the early study by Thomson (1955) consider as bottle-fed only those infants EEZEE breast-fed. The patterns of growth for breast-fed and bottle-fed infants do not differ in the Fomon et al. (1970; 1971) and Thomson (1955) studies. However, there are differ- ences between the two groups in the Saarinen and Siimes study (1979). Breast-fed infants have a high initial growth rate. Bottle-fed infants recover and surpass the breast-fed group in later infancy. These results look very much like those obtained in the present study: early growth superiority of those infants successfully breast- fed and later overcompensation in cases where lactation was unsuccessful. Thus, differences in growth patterns among the three studies cited above may reflect the feedback relationship of growth on diet and not simple differences between the nutritional impacts of breast milk and formula. 190 Interaction of Infant Diet and Maternal Nutritional Status Two assumptions about the relationship of infant feeding and maternal nutritional status were examined: 1) that breastffeeding is associated with postpartum weight loss, and 2) that breast-feeding leads to decreases in maternal fat stores. This section will summarize the results of those analyses. It will then propose possible explanations for the patterns of anthropometric changes observed. Breast-Feeding and Weight Loss No clear relationship was found between weight loss and breast-feeding. When the data were analyzed by months of breast-feeding, those weaning within the first two months appeared to have the lowest overall loss during the study. This may be due to the fact that the greatest weight losses for the sample as a whole occurred within the first three months. Those weaning before that showed very little loss during that time. Weaning appears to be accompanied by a temporary stabilization in maternal weight. When data were analyzed by infant feeding group, the greatest initial loss was among mothers of Group la infants and the lowest, among those of Group lb. Overall, the lowest mean weight losses were in Groups lb and 2a. However, none of these differences were statistically significant. These results indicate that there may be 191 some effect of the proportion of the infant diet cone sisting of breast—feeding on weight loss. Both Groups lb and 2a replaced nursing bouts with formula feedings, while other groups followed feeding practices which maintained the child's demand on the mother for nutrients at a higher level for a longer time. The failure of these analyses to clearly confirm the assumed relationship between weight loss and breast- feeding may be due to three factors. First, mothers may have compensated for the nutritional costs of lactation by increasing their own dietary intake. When questioned at six months, virtually none of the women reported dieting or greatly increased activity level. On the other hand, most reported little change in appetite or intake from "normal" levels. However, no dietary intake data are available to substantiate these state- ments. The second factor which may complicate attempts to isolate a significant relationship between weight loss and breast—feeding is the absence of a control group of women never breast-feeding. Most of the calculations of nutritional needs during lactation treat it as a physioé logical state identical to that of the "reference female," with the demands of milk production simply added to an otherwise normal body. Lactation may, in fact, be accompanied by basic alterations in metabolic status. The wide variety of physiological responses of women in 192 this study to breastffeeding suggests this. Thus women breast-feeding even for a limited period of time may be affected to the extent that they are an inappropriate comparison group in analyses trying to isolate the effects of lactation on weight. In fact, if a change in metabolism accompanies breast-feeding--and the association of altered weight loss velocity with weaning indicates it might-- the assumed relationship may not exist. Viewed from an evolutionary perspective, there certainly is no apparent selective advantage for a biological phenomenon which constitutes a progressive and unchecked drain on the mother. There would, however, be a very definite selec- tive advantage in being able to support lactation without physical depletion of the mother, especially in environ- ments with an uncertain food supply, high risk of infectious disease, or high work demands on females. Given this, the lack of a strong relationship between weight loss and breast-feeding makes sense. Although lactation may have no significant effect on weight loss, the prevalence of the assumption that it does can nonetheless be explained. One explanation is related to the simple input/output model of nutrition which persons in this culture (and many others) have. Mothers in this study frequently mentioned that they should be drinking more milk than usual. While this is a suggestion fre- quently made by dietitians and physicians, supposedly because certain amounts of whole milk are convenient 193 measures of the daily nutritional supplement which has been calculated as needed to support lactation (Food and Nutrition Board 1980), this was often interpreted by women to mean that milk input was necessary for milk output. In fact, women with milk allergies or aversions questioned their ability to successfully breast-feed. Women also reported that they felt they should drink a glass of water while nursing their infants, apparently to provide the necessary liquid. An extension of this input/output model is the belief that if a mother is producing breast milk and not significantly increasing food intake, the milk putput must come from her existing body stores, and this will be reflected in weight loss. Other ways by which it may be possible to accomodate the energy drain of lactation are not always considered. Most women, for example, have no notion of the possibility of metabolic alterations accompanying lactation. Nor do they consider the fact that motherhood may bring about re- ductions in exercise and energy expenditure sufficient to offset the cost of breast-feeding. For a woman used to being active, just the act of breastffeeding may impose restrictions on physical activity. One woman in the present study, for example, reported daily totals of 365 to 423 minutes nurSing during the first two months postf partum. There may also be a poor understanding of the relative weights of various body components. While fat may be mobilized to support lactation, it will result 194 in weight loss only if there is no corresponding increase in the relatively heavier lean body mass. Once a mother begins to habitually carry a growing infant, such an increase in lean body tissue is very likely to occur. One of the most impressive features of the postpartum weight change data in this study is their high degree of variability. Some women lost weight rapidly. In one case, a mother with a pregnancy gain of 30 pounds was several pounds below her prepregnant weight by two months post- partum. One of the factors influencing her decision to wean her son was her seemingly uncontrollable loss of weight. In other cases, women actually gained weight after the initial perinatal loss. Thus, at a population level, some weight loss probably does accompany breast- feeding. However, such a promise to women is meaningless until the regulating factors of weight change and dietary intake within individuals are better understood. Breast-Feeding and Changes in Maternal Body Fat The simple assumption that all women lose body fat while breast-feeding was not confirmed by analysis of data on midarm fat area. On the average, women increased, rather than decreased, in upper arm fatness during the postpartum period. There were variations in the amount of increase, and these coincided with the degree to which breast milk was the sole source of nutrients for the infant. Mothers of infants in Groups lb and 2a-—all of whom replaced breast-feeding bouts with formula--had the 195 greatest initial as well as cumulative increases in midarm fat area. Variations in postpartum fatness also appeared to be dependent on time since parturition. Weaning or introduction of nonebreast milk food in the early months was followed by an increase in fatness. When these same feeding changes occurred late in the period of time studied, they were followed by a decrease in fat. When the relationship of breast-feeding to maternal body fat was studied among long term breast-feeders who were losing weight, the mean number of nursing bouts per day in the first three months of lactation was found to be closely linked to fat area change. Two patterns emerged. Those with an overall decrease in midarm fat area (Group A) nursed their infants significantly more times per day than did those whose fat stores increased during the post- partum (Group B). There existed a strong inverse corre- lation of length of breast-feeding bouts with number of bouts per day. Thus, two different nursing styles-~short, frequent feedings and long, infrequent feedings--were strongly associated with two patterns of change in maternal fat stores. Since one of the most direct effects of nursing frequency is on serum prolactin levels, the results of this study suggest that the concordant variation of maternal fat may be'a function of the same hormonal system. There are two physiological pathways through which this might occur (Figure\L-l). The first is the mediation of 196 .umm hpom accumumz can mocmsvmum mcflmnsz mo :oHuomumucH man MOM m>m3£uwm omNflmmnuommmIIH.> mmDon mum>mq mzozmom umomzow» a: Son» 5850mm scammeAull oszmaz azmnommm mammHe mmomHo< zH ~9H>Heo<fi mmmmHu szeommoqu 197 gonadal hormone production by prolactin. Serum prolactin levels in nursing women are inversely proportional to that of estrogen and progesterone (Donner and Wortman 1980). The level of progesterone, in turn, has been found to be related to fat deposition (Hytten and Leitch 1971). Thus, women who nurse frequently (such as Group A mothers in the present study) should be subject to greater suppression of ovarian hormone production and therefore less disposed to deposit fat in body stores than women who nurse infrequently (Group B). The second pathway linking nursing frequency and fatness is mediated by the activity of the enzyme lipo- protein lipase, which is responsible for the uptake of lipids into adipose tissue. Since prolactin has been found to induce a decrease in lipoprotein lipase activity in peripheral adipose tissue and a corresponding increase in that in the breast (Zinder et a1. 1974), women who nurse frequently and have higher serum prolactin should be less likely to increase in peripheral fatness than women who nurse less frequently. Thus, possible explanations can be formulated for the observed patterns of fat change. These explanations relate nursing behavior to variations in maternal physiof logical parameters. Causes of these behavioral variations and the interaction of the hypothesized hormonal patterns with fecundity will be discussed in Chapter VI. Whether the fat changes indicate overall changes in body composition 198 or fat patterning still needs to be investigated. Although the interactions hypothesized in Figure V.l seem to account for the overall trends seen in maternal postpartum fat changes, these trends obscure a high degree of variability. The correlation between fat change and breast-feeding behavior was by no means perfect. Cases were noted in which women nursed their infants only four times per day. Yet their levels of midarm fat plummeted without apparently extreme weight loss. As with the in- vestigation of weight loss and breast-feeding, it is this intra-population variability which is in many ways surpri- sing. The participants in this study were purposely selected by criteria designed to produce as homogeneous a sample as possible. The fact that such variability exists again points to the need to understand the inter- actions of breast-feeding and maternal body changes in individual women before expecting to understand the impact of these interactions at the pOpulation level. CHAPTER VI SYNTHESIS AND CONCLUSIONS Previous chapters have documented and discussed the dynamics within each of three major components of infant feeding systems involving breast feeding: infant diet, infant growth, and maternal nutritional status. The interactions among the three areas have been discussed in only a limited way. This chapter will formulate a more general biobehavioral model for infant feeding and discuss the implications this model has for formulating testable hypotheses for two areas of research. The first is lactation amenorrhea and the critical body composition hypothesis. The second is the adequacy of breast milk as a sole, long term nutrient source for infants. A General Model of Infant Feeding Systems The central component in the model presented here is the infant feeding behaviors which make up infant diet (Figure VI.1). These include what is fed to an infant, at what age it is fed, and perhaps most importantly, how it is fed (e.g., the scheduling, frequency, and mode of feeding). Some aspects of the physical environment can 199 200 have a direct effect on feeding behaviors. These include factors which impose a physical separation on the mother- infant pair. However, according to the results of the present study, the most profound influence comes from the social environment in which the nursing pair exists. Figure V1.2 amplifies the components of this environment. They include knowledge and beliefs about infant feeding, growth, and behavior, as well as the entire range of interpersonal relationships of which a mother is a part. Together these form the basis for a mother's perception of her infant's needs and of her options in meeting those needs. It is this perception, then, which determines what infant feeding behaviors will be implemented by the mother, within the limits imposed by the physical environ- ment. It is important to note that infant growth and behavior affect feeding behaviors, but this relationship is for the most part mediated by the social environment. In an infant feeding situation where breast-feeding is the sole source of infant nourishment, variations in feeding behaviors affect infant growth indirectly, through day-to-day changes in the parameters of maternal physiology involved in milk production. The factors involved in these changes are presented in Figure V1.3. Levels of serum prolactin affect the quantity of milk produced and, by regulating the level of lipoprotein lipase activity in the breast, the composition of the milk. Prolactin induced changes in peripheral lipoprotein lipase activity 201 .ucmfim>ow£om m>wuonp0ummu no: can .Hmflucmuom m>wuosooummu some on mum: com: me uH .mwm3 ucmummmwp :H auwpcaomm Emma man mm: mmcflamwomwp ucmumwmfina .mmoHmemsm Andaman: mo mumumEmumm paw .cusouw ucmucH .uofl>mzwm maommm unnucH mo :OwuomumucH can no Hmoozlna.H> mmDon NOOAOHmwmm Adzmmadz \ mo 25.1555 ZOHBHmOMSOU Mnom Adzmmadz BzmzzomH>Zm AdUHmMmm / \ m0H>dmmm UZHQmmm BdeZH 6 \ mow>§Mm SEE / ”W 5305 9235 iv razmzzomgzm .EHoom _ 202 .AH.H> madman mmmv .=ucmficouw>cm Hmwoom= mmOH> Aqdzmomzoz 024 A mmame .3 .H> 95mg 008 .wmmoaowmmnm Hog—m: mo muwumfinmm: mo mudmconwfioulm .3 $505 \ EB; 2033850 A ..| \ Ema 330:3 2038 SEQ mama / ZHEmOmHH gammAlg Eam gmé g mmmmfl \ 559.6de g _ . 5.550 Va: 283 Va: \ ZOHHUHHHHE MHHZ III 983% GEE E g Elam: NV— MN m\ SEEM? Va: 204 also affect milk composition, by varying the uptake of dietary lipids into non-breast tissue and thus effectively varying the lipids available to mammary lipoprotein lipase. Maternal fatness and fecundity are hypothesized to be byproducts of variations in the serum prolactin levels, which vary themselves with different infant feeding behaviors. According to the proposed model, infant feeding behaviors also affect infant growth directly in cases where non—breast milk foods are fed. And, in addition to feeding behaviors and maternal physi- ology of lactation, infant growth is influenced by infant behaviors, especially through levels of energy expenditure. Besides influencing feeding behaviors, the social environment affects maternal physiological parameters directly by either promoting or inhibiting the let-down reflex. In breast-feeding situations which are supportive and free of tension, this conditioned response to the infant readily occurs, causing the release of oxytocin into the blood.stream and the subsequent ejection of milk from the alveoli. However, when the breast-feeding context is one which is non-supportive or stressful, the - let-down reflex is inhibited. Milk may be produced, but without an effective letedown reflex it is unavailable to the infant. Three properties of this model make it a particularly appropriate one for studying the infant feeding complex. First, it can accommodate the full range of feeding 205 behaviors to which any infant may be subjected, not just those associated with the lactation process. Second, the model stresses the feedback cycle between infant growth and feeding and the fact that this relationship is always mediated by the social environment in which infant feeding decisions are made. Third, it allows for the impact that the whole range of infant feeding be- haviors can have on maternal physiological parameters, suggesting that observed differences in maternal post- partum physiology may be explained, in part, by attention to variability in infant feeding behaviors. Thus, as social environments vary both inter- and intraculturally, feeding behaviors, maternal physiology, and infant growth patterns will all vary. While the different types of data gathered in this research support this model, other variables were not examined which may invalidate the interactions proposed. One of these is maternal diet. It is not known whether variations in nutrient intake would affect the factors entailed in milk production and in the regulation of maternal body composition and fecundity. A second variable not studied is the quality of the motherfinfant interaction immediately following birth. Lactation Amenorrhea and Maternal Bodnyat The results of the present study have suggested underlying patterns of change in the parameters of maternal postpartum physiology (Figure VI.3). Further research 206 to confirm these patterns may help to resolve one current controversy: that of the relationship of lactation amenorrhea to maternal body fat. This controversy origi— nated with the publication of the "critical body composition hypothesis" by Frisch and colleagues (Frisch et a1. 1973; Frisch and McArthur 1974). Frisch cited evidence which she said demonstrated that a minimum percentage of body fat is necessary for ovulation to occur. Although her original argument dealt with menarche, it has been extended to include amenorrhea experienced by victims of starvation, female athletes and dancers, and lactating mothers. Although the etiology of the failure to ovulate is vague in Frisch's work, the hypothesis at first was widely entertained. It appeared quite logical from the perspective of adaptation that an organism--particularly a member of a species which invests high levels of energy in raising only a few offspring—-should be fecund only if able to support a pregnancy and the offspring produced. Nonetheless, Frisch's hypothesis has been severely criticized on a number of grounds. First, it is charged that the measures of body composition on which she bases her conclusions are not adequately sensitive to pick up the small variations in nutritional status she claims are responsible for the presence or absence of ovulation (Johnston et a1. 1975; Trussell 1978). Second, it is charged that the work has used inappropriate statistics (Trussell 1980). Finally, she has been criticized because 207 research by others fails to replicate her findings (Huffman et al. 1978). The results reported here suggest that female body fat levels are a side effect of a more direct relationship between infant feeding practices and fecundity, mediated by the prolactin induced gonadal hormones. To substantiate this, the following hypothesis and corollary should be tested: Hypothesis: Lengthening interbout intervals will lead to increased production of gonadal hormones and the re- sumption of ovulation. Corollary: If a mother's energy intake exceeds that expended for non-lactation activity and maintenance, ovulation will be accompanied by increase in her levels of body fat. Therefore, as a side effect of hormonal change and subject to other unrelated influences, body fat may or may not vary with ovulation. If a mother changes breast- feeding frequency, thereby allowing levels of serum pro- lactin to drop and levels of the hormones necessary for ovulation to rise, ovulation should occur. But body fat levels will probably increase only if there is adequate caloric intake to support this rise (see Table VI.1). Therefore, changes in fatness with ovulation cannot be expected to occur in all populations or even among all women from the same population. Thus, changes in infant feeding behavior rather than in maternal body fat should show the better correlation with the termination of lactation amenorrhea. While some 8 0 2 mmcmso o: mmmmuocfl umm atom Hangman: mm» mmmmuocfl mmmmuomp mucmamuwswmu Ummoxw uo: moon mm» mmmmuOCa wmmmuomp mucwfimuwsvmu mpmmoxm COfifiMHDer mQCOEHOE HMUMCOU CflUOMHOHm wv—MHCH UHHOHMU HMGHGUMZ coflumuucwocoo Esumw .mxmucH OHHOHMU mo mam>mq mcw>um> cufl3 hocmsqwum mcflomomnummmum pommmuomo mo muommmm Andaman: owuflmmnuommmnlaquw mqmde 209 studies exist which document ovulation and feeding behavior (Knauer 1981; Chen et a1. 1974) and others, ovulation and maternal nutritional status (Delgado et a1. 1978; Chowdhury 1978), none has collected all three types of data. Doing so may help to provide a satisfactory resolution of the current debate over the critical body composition hypothesis. The Adequacy of Breast Milk The second controversy to which the biobehavioral model proposed here is relevant concerns the nutritional adequacy of human milk to provide for maintenance and growth of infants. In 1979, Waterlow and Thomson published a paper entitled, "Observations on the Adequacy of Breast Feeding." In it they cited reports from developing countries which indicated that infant growth frequently faltered at about three months of age. They attempted to show that energy requirements after three months are such that breast milk may be insufficient to meet them. Waterlow and Thomson's paper was not well received, since it appeared at a time when the adequacy of breast milk was a crucial issue in attempts to limit marketing of infant formulas in developing countries as well as in the pro- motion of late introduction of solids in Europe and the United States. Jelliffe and Jelliffe (l979a), for example, replied by citing other studies in which no growth impairment had been reported and by claiming the nutrient composition of human milk was indeed sufficient 210 for infant needs to six months of age. The results of the present study suggest that breast milk may well be nutritionally inadequate (that is, fail to support steady weight gain) at three months in some populations, but that this has less to do with the requirements of infants at this specific age than with changes in feeding habits likely to occur at about this time. To confirm this, the following hypothesis and corollaries can be tested: Hypothesis: Lengthening the interbout interval will lead to decreased quantity and decreased fat content of breast milk produced. Corollary I: Infants for whom this breast milk is a sole source of nutrients will manifest indications of reduced growth rates after the interbout interval is lengthened. Corollary II: In societies where changes in maternal behaviors which affect feeding frequency commonly occur at a particular time in an infant's life, population-wide alterations in infant growth rates will occur at that age. Declining frequency of breast-feeding has been shown in the second and third months of life. According to the proposed model of infant feeding, two factors may be responsible for this. One is a decrease in total milk output and the other is a decrease in breast milk fat content due to a reduction in mammary lipoprotein lipase activity. Thus, in other populations where similar patterns of decreasing feeding frequency occur, the same infant growth trends might be expected. If these changes 211 in feeding behavior are imposed by institutionalized patterns of maternal behavior, the trends may be even more pronounced. Existing studies support portions of the proposed hypothesis and corollaries. Data from the Gambian studies of maternal and child health and nutrition (Rowland 1978; Rowland and Paul 1981; Whitehead et al. 1978), for example, show that the growth of breast-fed infants begins to falter in the third month, regardless of the time of year at which the child reaches this age. The investigators hypothesize that breast milk is inadequate to provide sufficient calories at that age. But when the ethno- graphic data which Rowland (1978) gives are examined, it is evident that it may not be breast milk per se which is inadequate, but the quantity and composition of that produced by these particular mothers. Immediately after birth, these infants are fed very frequently-~in a true demand pattern. By three months however, their mothers' social roles have changed. They are expected to return to work in the fields, and they leave the infants behind in the care of siblings (Rowland 1978). An infant is nursed thereafter on the mother's schedule of availability. Thus, with a longer interbout interval, milk production may decrease at this time. Rowland and Paul's (1981) data on concurrent changes in maternal body fat further support this line of reasoning. They find that by 13 weeks, mothers' fatfold measures 212 begin to increase, indicating increases in body fat. If this is indicative of increases in peripheral lipof protein lipase activity accompanying changes in infant feeding frequency, it also suggests the occurrence of the hypothesized decrease in mammary lipoprotein lipase activity and the consequent decrease in breast milk fat content proposed in the model above. Thus, infant growth retardation at three months of age may be the result of concomitant changes in maternal availability for breast- feeding which in turn causes a change in breast milk quantity and composition. There are indications from the literature that such an association of maternal breast-feeding behavior and particular ages of infants is not unusual. Other popu- lations have similar demands placed on mothers when infants are about three months old. Chen et a1. (1974) note that among mothers in Bangladesh and other South and Southeast Asian areas, conceptions are highly seasonal, occurring shortly after the harvest. Thus, these infants are approximately three months old at the subsequent harvest. It is at this time that mothers devote in- creasing amounts of time to agriculture, at the expense of infant feeding. Although the effect on the infants is not documented, Chen et a1. note that the change in feeding is often sufficient to allow resumption of ovulation (1974). Therefore, apparent inadequacies of breast milk may appear at three months of age in these pOpulations as well. 213 Collection of the sort of data necessary to test the proposed hypothesis may help to resolve the question of breast milk adequacy by directing attention to sources of variability in the milk produced by individual mothers over the course of lactation. Consideration of pressures exerted on such mothers by their physical and social environments may then help to explain observed differences in the apparent inadequacy of breast milk to provide for satisfactory infant growth. Conclusions The original objectives of this research were to identify major infant dietary strategies and to document their interaction with infant growth and patterns of change in maternal postpartum nutritional status. From the re- sults of the analysis, several summary statements can be made: 1. The patterns of infant feeding in the first month of infancy are predictive of later dietary components. The frequency of nursing bouts in the first months of life is one of the best predictors of dietary practices in later infancy. 2. Infant size at birth and early growth are associated with both early and late infant feeding patterns. The less the deviance of an infant from culturally approved patterns of growth, the lower the likelihood that the 214 infant will experience a change in feeding regime. 3. A non-breast milk food can serve as either a replacement for breast milk or an addition to it. Which of these two dietary roles it takes determines the impact of such a food on infant growth, on the course 0f breast- feeding, and on maternal nutritional status. 4. The common assumptions of loss of weight and fat during lactation belie the complexity of the associations. Both the frequency and duration of breast-feeding correlate with observed patterns of maternal body change. Hormonal and enzymatic action induced by difference types of breast-feeding practices may be the underlying modifiers of weight and body composition in the postpartum period. This study was designed to limit known sources of genetic and behavioral variability. Only low-risk, Caucasian primiparae with healthy, normal, full term infants were included. Nonetheless, throughout the study the high degree of variability in both behavioral and biological factors was obvious. That distinct relation- ships between infant feeding strategies and infant growth on one hand and infant feeding strategies and maternal 215 nutritional status on the other have emerged from this indicates that in studies of other populations it will be important to document ranges of variability as well as the averages in order to produce a coherent picture of the infant feeding process. REFERENCES CITED Adair, L.J. 1980 Biocultural determinants of infant feeding practices and physical growth. Doctoral dissertation, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia. Ahn, C.H. and W.C. MacLean, Jr. 1980 Growth of the exclusively breast-fed infant. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition 33:183-192. Aldrich, C. Anderson 1942 Ancient processes in a scientific age: Feeding aspects. American Journal of Diseases of Children 64:714-722. American Academy of Pediatrics 1978 Breast-feeding. Pediatrics 62:591-601. 1980 On the feeding of supplemental foods to infants. Pediatrics 65:1178-1181. American Medical Association Council on Foods 1937 Strained fruits and vegetables in the feeding of infants. Journal of the American Medical Association 108:1259—1261. Auerbach, K.G. 1978 A study of timing of first solid foods: Comparison of breast- and bottlefeeding mothers. Birth and the Family 5:27—31. Bass, M.A., L. Wakefield, and K. Kolasa 1979 Community nutrition and individual food behavior. Minneapolis: Burgess. Blackburn, M.W. and D.H. Calloway 1976 Energy expenditure and consumption of mature, pregnant and lactating women. Journal of the American Dietetic Associ- ation 69:29-37. 216 C. 217 Brans, Y.W., J.E. Sumners, H.S. Dweck, and G. Cassady 1974 A noninvasive approach to body composition in the neonate: Dynamic skinfold measure- ments. Pediatric Research 8:215-222. ' Brozek, J. 1963 Quantitative description of body composition: Physical anthropology's "fourth" dimension. Current Anthropology 4(1):3-39. Bryant, C.A. 1978 The impact of kin, friend and neighbor networks on infantile obesity. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Kentucky, Lexington. Charney, E., H. Goodman, M. McBride, B. Lyon, and R. Pratt 1976 Childhood antecedents of adult obesity. New England Journal of Medicine 295:6-9. Chen, L.C., S. Ahmed, M. Gesche, and W.H. Mosley 1974 A prospective study of birth interval dynamics in rural Bangladesh. Population Studies 28(2):277-297. Chowdhury, A.K.M.A. 1978 Effect of maternal nutrition on fertility in rural Bangladesh. In W.H. Mosley, ed., Nutrition and Human Reproduction. New York: Plenum. Clein, N.W. 1943 Streamlined infant feeding. A feeding routine utilizing earlier addition of solid foods and fewer feedings. Journal of Pediatrics 23:224-228. Cunningham, A.S. 1977 Morbidity in breast-fed and artificially fed infants. Journal of Pediatrics 90:726-729. Davies, D.P., O.P. Gray, P.C. Elwood, C. Hopkinson, and S. Smith 1977 Effects of solid foods on growth of bottle- fed infants in first three months of life. British Medical Journal ii;7-8. Delgado, H., A. Lechtig, E. Brineman, R. Martorell, C. Yarbrough, and R.E. Klein 1978 Nutrition and birth interval components: The Guatemalan experiences. In W.H. Mosley, ed., Nutrition and Human Reproduction. New York: Plenum. 218 Delvoye, P., M. Demaegd, J. Delogne-Desnoeck, and C. Robyn 1977 The influence of the frequency of nursing and of previous lactation experience on serum prolactin in lactating mothers. Journal of Biosocial Science 9:447-451. Dennis, K.J. and W.R. Blytheway 1965 Changes in body weight after delivery. Journal of Obstetrics and Gynaecology 72:94-102. deSwiet, M., P. Fayers, and L. Cooper 1977 Effect of feeding habit on weight in infancy: Lancet i:892-894. Dixon, W.J. and F.J. Massey, Jr. 1969 Introduction to statistical analysis. New York: McGraw-Hill. Egli, G.E., N.S. Egli, and M. Newton 1961 The influence of the numbers of breast feedings on milk production. Pediatrics 27:314-317. Evans, T.J. 1978 Growth and milk intake of normal infants. Archives of Disease in Childhood 53:749-760. Ferris, A.G., V.A. Beal, M.J. Laus, and D.W. Hosmer 1979 The effect of feeding on fat deposition in early infancy. Pediatrics 69(4)397-401. Fomon, S.J. 1974 Infant nutrition. Philadelphia: Saunders. Fomon, S.J., L.J. Filer, T.A. Anderson, and E.E. Ziegler 1979 Recommendations for feeding normal infants. Pediatrics 63:52-59. Fomon, S.J., L.J. Filer, Jr., L.N. Thomas, and R.R. Rogers 1970 Growth and serum chemical values of normal breastfed infants. Acta Paediatrica Scandinavia Supp. 200. Fomon, S.J., L.N. Thomas, L.J. Filer, E.E. Ziegler, and M.T. Leonard 1971 Food consumption and growth of normal infants fed milk-based formulas. Acta Paediatrica Scandinavia Supp. 223. Food and Nutrition Board - 1980 The recommended dietary allowances, ed. 9. Washington, D.C.: National Academy of Sciences. 219 Frisch, R.E. and J.W. McArthur 1974 Menstrual cycles. Fatness as a determinant of minimum weight for height necessary for their maintenance or onset. Science 185: 949-951. Frisch, R.E., R. Revelle, and S. Cook' 1973 Components of weight at menarche and the initiation of the adolescent growth spurt in girls: Estimated total water, lean body weight and fat. Human Biology 45:469- 483. Galletti, F. and A. Klopper 1964 The effect of progesterone on the quality and distribution of body fat in the female rat. Acta Endocrinologia 46:379-383. Gerber Products Company 1977 Nutrient values. Gerber Baby Foods, Professional Communications Department, Gerber Products Company. Fremont, Michigan. Goldsby, J.M. ' n.d. PCTL9D Anthropometry Subroutine Set. Atlanta, Georgia: Center for Disease Control. Green, L.W. ' 1970 Manual for scoring socioeconomic status for research on health behavior. Public Health Reports: 85:815-827. Grosvenor, C.E., F. Mena, and D.A. Schaefgen 1967 Effect of nonsuckling interval and duration of suckling on the suckling-induced fall in pituitary prolactin concentration in the rat. Endocrinology 81:449-453. Gussler, J.D. and L.H. Briesemeister 1980 The insufficient milk syndrome: A biocultural explanation. Medical Anthropology 4(2): 147-174. Gurney, J.M. and D.B. Jelliffe 1973 Arm anthropometry in nutritional assessment: Nomogram for rapid calculation of muscle cirCumference and cross-sectional muscle and fat areas. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition 26:912-915. 220 Guthrie, H., and E. Kan 1977 Infant feeding decisions--timing and rationale. Environmental Child Health 264-266. Himes, J.H. 1979 Infant feeding practices and obesity. Journal of the American Dietetic Association 75:122-125. Hodgson, S.V. 1978 Early infant feeding and weight gain. Journal of the Royal College of General Practitioners 28:280-281. Holly, D., and D. Cullen 1977 A comparison of weight gain in breastfed and bottlefed babies. Public Health (London) 91:113-116. Hood, L.J., J.A. Faed, P.A. Silva, and P.M. Buckfield 1978 Breastfeeding and some reasons for electing to wean the infant: A report from the Dunedin Multidisciplinary Child Development Study. New Zealand Medical Journal 88: 273-276. Huenemann, R.L. 1974 Environmental factors associated with preschool obesity. 1. Obesity in six-month- old children. Journal of the American Dietetic Association 69(5):480-487. Huffman, S.L., A.K.M.A. Chowdhury, and W.H. Mosley 1978 Postpartum amenorrhea: How is it affected by maternal nutritional status? Science 200:1155-1157. Hytten, F.E., and I. Leitch 1971 The physiology of human pregnancy. Oxford: Blackwell Scientific Publications. Hytten, F.E., and A.M. Thomson 1961 Nutrition of the lactating woman. In S.K. Kon and A.T. Cowie, eds., Milk: The Mammary Gland and Its Secretion, Vol. II. New York: Academic Press. Jelliffe, D.B., and E.F.P. Jelliffe‘ 1978 The volume and composition of human milk in poorly nourished communities. A Review. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition. 31:492-515. 221 1979a Adequacy of breast-feeding. Lancet ii: 691-692. ' 1979b Human milk in the modern world. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Jerome, N.W., G.H. Pelto, and R.F. Kandel 1980 An ecological approach to nutritional anthropology. In N.W. Jerome, R.F. Kandel, and G.H. Pelto, eds., Nutritional Anthro- pology. Pleasantville, NY: Redgrave 'Publishing Company. Johnston, F.E., A.F. Roche, L.M. Schell, and H.N.B. Wettenhall 1975 Critical weight at menarche: Critique of a hypothesis. American Journal of Diseases of Children 129:19-23. Kannel, W.B., and T.R. Dawber 1972 Atherosclerosis as a pediatric problem. Journal of Pediatrics 80:544-554. Katz, S.H. 1977 Toward a new concept of nutrition. In L.S. Greene, ed., Malnutrition, Behavior, and Social Organization. New York: Academic Press. Kemberling, S.R. 1979 Supporting breast feeding. Pediatrics 63:60-63. Klaus, M., and J.H. Kennell 1970 Mothers separated from their newborn infants. Pediatric Clinics of North America 17: 1015-1038. Knauer, M. - 1981 Breastfeeding and the return of postpartum ovulation in urban women. Paper presented at the 50th Annual Meeting, American Association of Physical Anthropologists, Detroit. Konner, M., and C. Worthman 1980 Nursing frequency, gonadal function, and birth spacing among £Kung hunter-gatherers. Science 207:788-791. LaLeche International 1963 The womanly art of breastfeeding. London: Tavistock. 222 Marlin, D.W., M.F. Picciano, and E.C. Livant 1980 Infant feeding practices. Journal of the American Dietetics Association 77:668-676. Meyer, H.F. 1952 Essentials of infant feeding for physicians. Springfield: Charles C. Thomas. 1958 Breast feeding in the United States: Extent - and possible trend. Pediatrics 22:116-121. 1968 Breast feeding in the United States. Report of a 1966 national survey with comparable 1946 and 1956 data. Clinical Pediatrics 7:708-715. Michigan Department of Public Health 1980 Recommendations for feeding healthy infants: Reference for health professionals. Public Health Paper No. 2, Lansing. Naismith, D.J. and C. Ritchie 1976 The effect of breast feeding and artificial feeding on bodyweight, skin fold measure- ments and food intakes of forty-two primiparous women. Proceedings of the Nutrition Society 34:116A. Newman, B.C., H. Delgado, R. Martorell, and R.E. Klein 1980 Maternal anthropometric changes during pregnancy and lactation in rural Guatemala. Presented at the 49th Annual Meeting, American Association of Physical Anthro- pologists. Niagara Falls, New York. Philipps, C., and N.E. Johnson 1977 The impact of quality of diet and other factors on birth weight of infants. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition 30:215-225. Quandt, S., and C. Ritenbaugh 1979 Variations in infant growth associated with feeding patterns. Paper presented at the 48th Annual Meeting, American Association of Physical Anthropologists. San Francisco. Raphael, D. 1966 The lactation-suckling process in the matrix of supportive care. Ph.D. thesis, Columbia University. Microfilm #69-15, 580. 22 3 — r" . x . . I - l - ‘ .-v / [A . «’1 V 1 _,, ‘1 , ' . .- I -\ ‘ _ tn , ._A' , - / Ritenbaugh, C., and G. Harrison 1980 Mexican-American and Anglo infant feeding patterns: A comparative study. Presented at the Annual Meeting of the American Anthropological Association. Washington, D.C. Ross Laboratories 1976 Boys and girls: Birth to 36 months. Physical Growth NCHS Percentiles. Columbus, Ohio. Rotch, T.M. 1907 An historical sketch of the develOpment of percentage feeding. New York Medical Journal 85:532-537. Rowland, M.G.M. 1978 Factors affecting lactation capacity. Presented at Infant and Child Feeding Symposium, Michigan State University, East Lansing, October 16. Rowland, M.G.M., and A.A. Paul 1981 Factors affecting lactation capacity: Implications for developing coutries. In J. Bond, W. Weil and G. Leveille, eds., Infant and Child Feeding. Academic Press. Saarinen, U.M., and M.A. Siimes 1979 Role of prolonged breast feeding in infant growth. Acta Paediatrica Scandinavia 68:245-250. Sackett, W.W., Jr. 1953 Results of three years experience with a new concept of baby feeding. Southern Medical Journal 46(4):358-363. Sauls, H.S. 1979 Potential effects of demographic and other variables in studies comparing morbidity of breast-fed and bottle-fed infants. Pediatrics 64(4):523-527. Schneider, M.H., and B.S. Worthington-Roberts 1981 A new look at breast-feeding. In B.S. Worthington-Roberts, eds., Contemporary DevelopmentsiJINutrition. St. Louis: Mosby. 224 Scow, R.O., C.R. Mendelson, O. Zinder, M. Hamosh, and E.J. Blanchette-Mackie 1973 Role of lipoprotein lipase in the delivery of dietary fatty acids to lactating mammary tissue. In C. Galli, G. Jacini, A. Pecile, eds., Dietary Lipids and Postnatal Develop- ment. New York: Raven Press. Shukla, A., H.A. Forsyth, C.H. Anderson, and S.M. Marwak 1972 Infantile overnutrition in the first year of life. British Medical Journal 4:507-515. Siegel, S. 1956 Nonparametric statistics for the social sciences. New York: McGraw-Hill. Sjolin, S., Y. Hofvander, and C. Hillervik 1977 Factors related to early termination of breast feeding. A retrospective study in Sweden. Acta Pediatrica Scandinavia 66:505-511. 1979 A prospective study of individual courses of breast feeding. Acta Pediatrica Scandinavia 68:521-529. Stunkard, A., E. d'Aguili, S. Fox, and R.D.L. Filion 1972 Influence of social class on obesity and thinness in children. Journal of the American Medical Association 221(6): 579-584. Taggart, W.R., R.M. Holliday, W.Z. Billewicz, F.E. Hutten, and A.M. Thomson 1967 Changes in skinfolds during pregnancy. British Journal of Nutrition 21:439-450. Taitz, L.S. 1971 Infantile overnutrition among artificially fed infants in the Sheffield region. British Medical Journal i:315-3l6. Thomson, J. 1955 Observations on weight gain in infants. Archives of Disease in Childhood 322-327. Trussell, J. 1978 Menarche and fatness: Reexamination of the critical body composition hypothesis. Science‘200:1506-1509. 225 1980 Statistical flaws in evidence for the Frisch hypothesis that fatness triggers menarche. Human Biology 52(4):711-720. Tyson, J.E. 1977 Nursing and prolactin secretion: Principal determinants in the mediation of puerperal infertility. In P.G. Crosignani and C. Robyn, eds., Prolactin and Human Reproduction. London: Academic Press. Tyson, J.E., H.G. Friesen, and M.S. Anderson 1972 Human lactational and ovarian response to endogenous prolactin release. Science 177: 897-900. Vis, H.S., and P.H. Hennart 1978 Decline in breast feeding: About some of its causes. Acta Pediatrica Belgium 31: 195-206. Waterlow, J.C., and A.M. Thomson 1979 Observations on the adequacy of breast feeding. Lancet ii:238-242. Weil, W.B., Jr. 1977 Current controversies in childhood obesity. Pediatrics 91(2):175-187. Weiner, J.S., and L.A. Lourie (eds.) 1969 Human biology: A guide to field methods. IBP Handbook #9. Philadelphia: F. A. Davies Co. Weinstein, R.S. 1981 Nutritional status and biological varia- bility in an urban Mexican population. Presented at the 50th Annual Meeting, American Association of Physical Anthro- pologists, Detroit. Whitehead, R.G., M.G.M. Rowland, M. Hatton, A.M. Prentice, E. Mueller, and A. Paul. 1978 Factors influencing lactation performance in rural Gambian mothers. Lancet ii:l78- 181. ' Wilkinson, P.W., and D.P. Davies 1978 When and why are babies weaned? British Medical Journal 1:1682-1683. 226 Zinder, O., M. Hamosh, T.R.C. Fleck, and R.O. Scow 1974 Effect of prolactin on lipoprotein lipase in mammary gland and adipose tissue of rats. American Journal of Physiology 226(3): 744-748. APPENDICES APPENDIX A LETTERS OF INTRODUCTION AND CONSENT FORMS MICHIGAN STATE UNIVERSITY 227 ”WMWY MW‘W‘W “WWW “WWW mmmnmlcm CONSENT FORM I, authorize the participation of myself and my infant(s) as subjects in the postnatal portion of the project entitled Incremental Weight Gain in hem which is being conducted by Dr. Cheryl Ritenbaugh of Michigan State University and such qualified female assistants as she may need and employ. I understand that these assistants will be under her supervision at all times during the project. The nature and general purpose of this part of the project have been explained to me. The procedures to be performed (Appendix) have been fully described to me by - . ‘ I understand that no risk to my infant's or my health and wellbeing is associated with: our participation in this project. Dr. Ritenbaugh or her assistants have offered to answer any inquiries I might wish to make at any time during the course of the project. I also authorize Dr. Ritenbaugh to obtain from my prenatal and hospital charts and from my infant's hospital chart the information listed in the Appendix. I understand that no other parts .of my medical records will be available to her. I understand that at no time will any information obtained regarding me or my infant(s) be made available to anyone not connected with the study. Any published reports on this study will 'present data in cumulative form, with no references to individual subjects. . I.~.understand that I ’am free to withdraw myself and/or my infantCs) from partici- pation in any part of this project at any time I may wish to do so without penalty or prejudice. I understand that nothing in the above waives any of my legal rights. Date _§ignatme of individual fir consent Si gnatureTaf representative from the study MICHIGAN STATE UNIVERSITY 223 . ”WWWWY mLm-W-m mumm mammalian MENOSMPAMCW Appendix DATA TO BE COLLECTED Mother. Monthly measurements (also at 7 days post-partum)' weight ' minimum ankle and wrist and maximum calf circumferences mid-arm circumference fatfold thicknesses at triceps and subscapular locations maximum abdominal circumference distance pubic tubercle to xyphoid process Bimonthly interview 24-hour dietary recall feed habit questionnaire Infant Brazelton Behavioral Assessment (at 1 and 7 days) Monthly measurements (also at l and 7 days} weight recumbent length head, arm, and chest circumferences fatfolds at triceps, biceps, and midabdomen Materials to be completed by the mother weekly--one 24-hour diet journal for the infant monthly--report on eating habits, health, and activity level of infant ,At completion of the study mother's prenatal and hospital chart urine protein and sugar hematocrit and hemoglobin blood pressure blood sugar, if available placental weight - infant's hospital chart birth and daily weight length APGAR scores at l and 5 minutes feeding modality (breast, bottle, supplemental sugar water, etc. 229 MICHIGAN STATE UNIVERSITY DEPARTMENT OF ANTHROPOLOGY ' EAST LANSING ° MICHIGAN ' 4cm COLLEGE or SOCIAL SCIENCE COLLEGE OF HUMAN MEDICINE CO“! 0! OSTBOPATHIC MEDICINE CONSENT FORM l. I, , authorize the participation of myself and my infant(s§n:2elubjects in the study of third trimester pregnancy growth and infant growth and feeding practices which is being conducted by Sara Quandt of Michigan State University. 2. The nature and general purposes of the project have been explained to me. and the procedures to be performed (Appendix) have been fUIly described. 3. I understand that no risk to my infant's or my health and well being is associated with our participation in this project. 5. I understand that at no time will any information obtained regarding me or my infant(s) be made available to anyone not connected with the study. Any published reports will present data in cumulative form, with no reference to individual persons. 6. I understand that I am free to withdraw myself and my infant(s) from participation in the study at any time I may wish to do so. 7. I understand that nothing in the above waives any of my legal rights. Date Signature of individual giving consent Investigator -- Sara Quandt 230 APPENDIX Data to be Collected Mother 1. Prenatal interview: plans for infant feeding, previous experience with infants, expectations for own infant's behavior 2. Measurements biweekly during pregnancy and post partum at 2 weeks, 3 and 6 months weight height (one time only) midarm and calf circumferences triceps and subscapular fatfold thicknesses Infant 1. Measurements at l day, 2 weeks, and monthly from l to 6 months weight recumbent length midarm, head, chest, and thich circumferences triceps, biceps, subscapula, midabdominal, and thigh fatfold thicknesses 2. One 24-hour diet diary per week 3. Questions on infant growth, behavior, and feeding to be answered by mother at measuring sessions. Sara Quandt Department of Anthropology Michigan State University 353-2950 231 MICHIGAN STATE UNIVERSITY ”AWOFAWMY EASTLANSNG'MICHIGAN'm mmmm mumume MNWAWW M E M 0 R A N D U M T0: Participants in the project, Incremental Weight Gain in Pregggggy_ FROM: Cheryl Ritenbaugh, Ph.D. Assistant Professor Sara Quandt a Debbie Gregg. Research Assistants DATE: November 15. 1977 When we wrote the first memorandum asking your help in the study of weight gain in pregnancy, we indicated that we intended to continue monitoring your’weight and that of your infant for a short time after delivery. Since that time we have re- ceived funding for a longer and more detailed follow up of the infants. In this letter we would like to tell you more about this portion of the project and ask your continued participation. Previous studies of weight gain in pregnancy have ended at delivery. _Those studies which have been conducted on infant development have begun at birth with little information on the preceding pregnancy. Since we have collected such detailed data on the prenatal progress of you and other women, we are in the unique position of being able to study the correlation between this progress and the early growth and development of infants. To do this, we would like to extend the postnatal portion of the project to up to 6 months after delivery. During this period we are inter- ested in gathering data on the normal body adjustments of mothers after birth and on the growth, eating habits, and activity levels of their infants. One part of the postnatal study mentioned in the first memorandum'was an evaluation of your infant's behavior at about a week after delivery. This would involve using a safe, nationally-used rating scale which would be explained to you in detail before asking your permission to do it. Also, we would like to weigh and measure you and your baby once a month. The measurements are similar to those used during your pregnancy, and the new ones for your baby will be explained to you. On severa occassions we would like to do a diet recall on a 24-hour period followed by a shor' questionnaire on your eating habits. Since it will be important to us to know what your baby is eating, we will provide you with a form on which you can record at you‘ convenience what your baby is eating during one 24-hour period each week. If’you are breast feeding, we would want you to record the time and length of each nursing session for that day. we are interested in having you and your infant continue with us in this project. If for some reason you will be unable to do so for the full 6 month period and would still like to help, we would be most grateful fbr any participation possible. Jr 232 MICHIGAN STATE UNIVERSITY DEPARTMENT OF ANTHROPOLOGY EAST LANSING ' MICHIGAN ' 48814 COLLEE Of SOCIAL SCIENCE COLLEGE OF HUMAN MEDICINE COLmE OF OSTEOPATHIC MEDICINE MEMORANDUM To: Potential participants in study of third trimester pregnancy growth and infant feeding and growth From: Sara Quandt, Graduate Research Trainee, Department of Anthropology Date: July 25, l979 I am a biological anthropologist with a strong interest in human growth and nutrition. Currently I am conducting a study which involves pregnant women in the Lansing area. This letter is to explain the project and to ask your help in its completion. During a study ‘I helped to conduct last year on weight gain during pregnancy and its effect on infant growth, I became quite interested in two different topics. The first is the variability of third trimester weight and body changes in pregnant women and their possible relation to factors such as onset of labor and sex of the child. The second is the interaction of nutrition and growth and the way in which mothers decide on a feeding plan for their infants. When I began reading the scientific literature on this subject, I discovered that most previous studies have looked at infants only in terms of whether bottle or breast-fed. They have generally ignored other factors such as the frequency of feeding and the timing of introduction of solids. For this reason, it is hard to apply the results of these studies to other infants. I think there are probably many different patterns of feeding which lead to healthy babies. What I would like to do is to find out what the variability in infant feeding is and how that relates to growth. Additionally, I would like to learn about the ways mothers decide among various ways to feed their infants. You and the other women who are being asked to participate in this study were chosen from healthy women having their first babies under the care of several obstetricians in the Lansing area who are cooperating with me. Your parti- cipation would involve, first,an interview before your baby is born so I could find out your feelings on infant feeding and you could get to know a little more about me and this study. Also, I would want to weigh you and do a few measurements periodically before delivery. After delivery, I would measure your daughter or son at regular intervals--about every four weeks--until age 6 months. The measurements would include weight, length, and several others which would each be explained to you. When measuring, I would also want to ask a few questions about any changes you had made in your infant‘ s feeding, as well as general questions about how things were going. The measuring would take place at your home--unless you preferred that we met elsewhere--and would take 30 minutes or less. Since it would be important to know what your baby was eating, I would provide you with a form on which to record at your convenience what your baby ate during one 24- hour period every week. I will be calling you within the next few days to answer any questions you m-I-‘LL L.... L-..- .411 L- 4-4-ana-I-All «In lent-tune. an {nunIuae undid-In m In +511- APPENDIX B DISTRIBUTION OF MOTHERS RETURNING INFANT DIET DIARIES APPENDIX B DISTRIBUTION OF MOTHERS RETURNING INFANT DIET DIARIES Figure B.l shows the distribution of number of diaries returned by mothers during the study. For those women who dropped out of the study before six months, a projected number of returned diaries is entered. This is based on rate of return before leaving the study. 0 2 4 6 8 10 12 14 16 18 20 22 24 Number of Diaries FIGURE B.l--Distribution of Mothers Returning Diet Diaries. 233 APPENDIX C INFANT OUTLINES USED IN DATA COLLECTION APPENDIX C INFANT OUTLINES USED IN DATA COLLECTION The six infant outlines on the following pages (Figure C.1) were used to elicit maternal ideas of appropriate and inappropriate body shape. Each was traced on a blank card. These were then shuffled and presented as a group to the mothers. 234 235 .mmcflauso ucmwcHIIH.U mmDUHm 236 .Ao.u:ooc H.o mmDon APPENDIX D REASONS FOR INTRODUCING NEW FOOD TO A BREAST MILK DIET APPENDIX D REASONS FOR INTRODUCING NEW FOOD TO A BREAST MILK DIET Infant Behavior ID# 48 B3 had slept through the night at an earlier age; had now stopped sleep- ing through the night. 51 M wants to tide the baby over to four hour feedings. B currently wants to nurse too frequently. 57 B couldn't go long enough without nursing. Kept crying, making sucking motions with mouth, want- ing more food. 58 B not satisfied on breast milk. Kept crying, making sucking motions with mouth, wanting more food. 61 B seemed to want to eat all the time. Because of hot, humid weather this annoyed M. lC=cereal; Fo=formula; Fr=fruit Foodl/=+2/Month Fr + 4 2"+" represents addition; "=" represents replacement 3B=baby; M=mother 237 62 66 68 72 73 74 80 81 238 B was crying "constantly." M said, Fo "Not satisfied on breast milk." M thought it would get infant to F0 sleep through the night. B wanted to nurse frequently--at C one to two hour intervals day and night. B crying a lot, colicky, wanted F0 to nurse "all the time." B fussy in the evening, breast milk C did not pacify. B waking one or more times during C the night. M trying to get baby to sleep C through the night. B not satisfied after nursing. Fr M thought breast milk not enough. B fussed after feedings. External Suggestion 50 53 54 55 Pediatrician told M to start cereal C because infants that age need iron not obtainable from breast milk. Pediatrician noted insufficient Fo infant weight gain; said M's breast milk supply "inadequate" to provide needed calories. Pediatrician told mother infants C that age need additional iron; M could use cereal or iron drops (told her certain ill effects possible from drops). Pediatrician told mother infants C that age need additional iron; gave her choice of cereal or iron drops (told her certain ill effects possible from drops). 67 69 76 77 239 M had been receiving increasing pressure from mother-in-law, who thought the baby was "old enough to need real food." M reports feeling pressure from husband's family, particularly mother-in-law who thought baby "so big he needs food? mother- in-law also doubted whether the daughter-in-law‘s small breasts could supply enough breast milk. Doctor told M he thought the baby was "big enough" to need more iron than breast milk could provide. Doctor told M that the baby needed iron now and that breast milk did not contain any. Situational 52 64 65 70 71 M returning to work (teaching); needed to have something for baby- sitter to feed infant during day. M returning to work (nurse in hospital); wanted to get infant used to food so the babysitter could feed him during the day. M planned to nurse at night. M returning to work (teaching); wanted to be the person to introduce infant to solid foods which she would eat while at the babysitter's. M going back to graduate school classes; needed something for father to feed infant to tide him over until M re- turned home to nurse him. M returning to work (secretarial); was satisfied she had given infant a good nutritional start; now wanted to wean so babysitter could keep infant content with formula during the day. F0 F0 Fr Fo Mater 59 63 79 240 nal Health M hospitalized at bout one week post- partum for hemorrhage, D & C; tried to keep nursing baby, but found milk supply affected by anesthetic; hospital staff impeded attempts to have infant in room with mother. M hospitalized for appendectomy at 2 weeks postpartum; was not allowed to nurse because of possible effects of drugs on infant; milk supply had diminished by the time allowed to nurse, so continued with formula infant had received while she was in hospital. M reported breasts engorged, nipples too sore to nurse. Other 49 75 78 56 60 B was beginning to display an interest in food; picking up parents' food and putting it in mouth; mother wanted to start solids while B was interested to avoid having a picky eater later in life. M could not articulate reason. M could not articulate reason. B exclusively breast fed. B exclusively breast fed. F0 F0