32mm” '11:} “4?; “a {"""I '3‘} 3 3 E."$3'3;"~3!'}"'. -3\'L"’ 3- y'.9‘"31 3 '3' _- 3333333331,, 33'“ 3333,3333: 33333:?3 33"; 333333333 33" 3 3333 333 » -7 9 ~m ‘T'W “u— __, fl-:.'.l_‘ . _. ff“: .3 :.;..: ~-— I. r '- -v's' 4-239» , ‘ .-. I "‘ 4.- __ 4—.34 --—v.- ,- .o‘ 4 :«Jr' . 5 .v' ' 4 . u . ' '—'. I .n-s. vi '_ u . '. ~371'f‘ .. 0.. poison ¢~.. ‘Ibv r n . . .._ _' 1. . '3 . —4- -3 .A. — c- 3 _33 —' n ‘ 1.4. .1 -- n -. v 30‘ >3 of“: . -_ q.-. w -‘.T'.‘ ' o r - . -o " -. v v—V‘.‘ -- y. :T'v't-z-L. _ 3 .3 .‘.-_ 3 3 _ «‘1‘: .43, « . 3. 3 3,. 3“ 4-41313 .. _m531333 i” A u" . 4‘ - . . ‘. . . ‘ _ . “ 4 .:-.;‘:. érh" '5'. Y} . h ' ' ‘ ‘ 3. 0 - ' . .. 3'" 2' ,3 3... 333 3 3. .fu 3 3 . ‘ 3 'b'. I ‘3: .3 ' 23:: . 333 3 3 0 J}.$3:' 4 V ‘ '.' £3.5- ' ' ‘ .‘J‘ T ‘ ‘ .7; h . “I. .L 3 3 .M 33 3333333 33 333333333 3 7333. 3:33 3 33 :33'33'3 3.333333 . 3 .33.3.333 lg; “3' W" " '3“. I" 3;; 1":3::' I33 3' [3.3. :' ,: 33,!z':"3|3“3 l!!i:“| . ' 370,333 .3 33333333333333 333333 33333333: 3 ""3333" " 33' 3 ' 33'3' 3"‘33‘ .‘llll:"n\ttl'.' 312" . :333'anu... "15913“ 433631 33 33 -3 333,33 :33'333} $3113"! 9333.33 v _y_ A .. 'b‘ ‘i':: " I» _ 3 ‘ A - A -‘ A. V ' " - h. . L 3.3 . .. . , 3 :23? - _ 3. - V _ _3 3 33 u ‘3 r.—-‘ ‘ ‘ . .. ' --.... ‘ .3 fK- _ A ‘ _ ‘ ‘A ‘ ~ 1.. ~ 3 A .3—- ' ‘ —3 "‘."' —. ‘3‘ ; . -.. - _ 3 3 _ 3 3 LIBR/ " Y Michigan to UlliVC 1'51 .. Y 333‘} ‘1 1‘» INN“ “m! This is to certify that the thesis entitled STYLES OF PARENTj-CHILD INTERACTION AS A MEDIATING FACTOR IN CHILDREN'S LEARNING FROM ANTISOCIAL TELEVISION PORTRAYALS presented by Felipe Korzenny has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for Ph 0 D. degree in Communication th Ma professor U DateSeptember 16, 1977 0-7639 STYLES OF PARENT-CHILD INTERACTION AS A MEDIATING FACTOR IN CHILDREN'S LEARNING FROM ANTISOCIAL TELEVISION PORTRAYALS BY Felipe Korzenny A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Communication 1977 Accepted by the faculty of the Department of Communication, College of Communication Arts, Michigan State University, in partial fulfillment of the require- ments for the Doctor of PhiloSOphy degree. M . irec of Dis er a ion Guidance Committee: Chairman ABSTRACT STYLES OF PARENT-CHILD INTERACTION AS A MEDIATING FACTOR IN CHILDREN'S LEARNING FROM ANTISOCIAL TELEVISION PORTRAYALS BY Felipe Korzenny The present study dealt with the role of different modes of parent—child interaction as mediators of children's model- ing of antisocial television portrayals, i.e., physical and verbal aggression, theft, and deceit. More specifically, two main orientations of parent-child interactions with regard to social situations have been iden- tified, namely, an internal and an external orientation. The main difference between the two types of orientations is that internally oriented parents are said to provide children with the necessary cognitive structure for evaluating their social behaviors. Externally oriented parents do not provide cogni- tive resources upon which the child can rely when social decisions have to be made. Six hypotheses were tested in this study. Three hypoth- eses dealt with the main effects expected from the internal and external orientations of parents and children and exposure to antisocial television portrayals. The fourth hypothesis predicted the relative magnitude of antisocial predispositions in children for different subsamples defined by the internal and external orientations and antisocial television exposure. The last two hypotheses were concerned with the contingent relationships between exposure to antisocial television exposure Felipe Korzenny and the child's antisocial predispositions at different inter- sections of the internal and external orientations of parents and children. Two hundred and twenty-seven mothers and their children in the fifth, seventh and ninth grades were tested. The results of this study were as follows: Exposure to antisocial television portrayals had a signif- icant direct independent effect on the children's anti- social predispositions, as reported by both mothers and children. The external orientation of parents had a significant direct independent effect on physical and verbal aggres- sion and deceit, based on the mothers' report of the child's behavior. However, with the children's data the external orientation of children had a negligible effect on the children's antisocial predispositions. The children's internal orientation was found to be the most powerful negative predictor of antisocial predispo- sitions. The mothers' data, on the other hand, did not show an appreciable relationship between the parental in- ternal orientation and the children's negative predisposi- tions. With both sets of data, from parents and children, it was consistently found that highly internally oriented chil- dren, who watch small amounts of antisocial television, and whose parents and themselves are low in their external orientation displayed the least amount of antisocial Felipe Korzenny predispositions. Also, the children highest in antisocial predispositions were those lowest in the internal orienta- tion and high in antisocial television exposure and the external orientation. 5. Those highly internally oriented parents who were low in their external orientation had children for whom the cor— relation between exposure to antisocial behaviors on tele- vision and their display of antisocial predispositions was the lowest when compared with all the children and with other subsamples. The children who showed the highest cor- relation between exposure and predispositions were those whose parents were highly externally oriented and low in their internal orientation. With the children's orienta- tions the pattern of findings was the opposite to the hy- pothesized expectations. Few of these contingent correla- tion comparisons were found to be statistically significant. One of the main conclusions of this study was that tele- vision remains a contributing source of socialization independ- ently of parental practices and the children's internalization of moral values. The results were discussed taking in consider- ation the research literature dealing with the internalization of moral standards in children. To Sandy, for the third time. iii ACKNOWLEDGMENTS I know I won't be able to thank all the peOple that have contributed to my graduate education and to this dissertation. However, I will try to do justice to the most notable individ- uals and organizations. I would like to thank Dr. Bradley S. Greenberg for his continued guidance and help. He has been a model for me and has taught me many direct and indirect lessons. From a schol- arly point of view, I was lucky to have him as my advisor and as the chairman of my committee. Thank you, Dr. Greenberg. Dr. R. Vincent Farace has been very helpful in guiding me in the direction of the study of communication and change. His participation in my committee was distinct, from my point of view, in teaching me about the relativity of our discipline. Dr. Stephen L. Yelon introduced me to the area of moral development and took me by the hand in understanding critical issues regarding the role of parental practices on children's behavior. Dr. Charles K. Atkin was always helpful in encouraging me to be flexible regarding analytic methods and in the con- sideration of alternatives. All four, Drs. Greenberg, Farace, Yelon and Atkin, be- sides being members of my doctoral committee are good friends that I hope to preserve. iv Several other members of the faculty in the Department of Communication gave me assistance in understanding issues related to the present dissertation. Dr. Edward L. Fink was one of the most notorious. My family played a crucial role throughout my career: My wife, my parents, my grandparents, and my parents-in-law gave me always unusual support that I will never forget. The data for this dissertation were collected as part of a larger project (CASTLE) sponsored by the Office of Child Development of the United States Department of Health, Educa- tion and Welfare. A grant from the National Association of Broadcasters facilitated the dissertation work. The National Council of Science and Technology of Mexico (CONACYT) partially sponsored me during my graduate education. Ruth Langenbacher efficiently typed the manuscript. Thank you all Chapter I II TABLE OF CONTENTS STYLES OF PARENT-CHILD.INTERACTION AS A MEDIATING FACTOR IN CHILDREN'S LEARNING FROM ANTISOCIAL TELEVISION PORTRAYALS . . . . . INTRODUCTION . . . . . Parental Controls over Viewing Parental Interpretation of Tele- vision Content . . Parent-Child Interaction Modes with Regard to Social Situations . Family Interaction Perspective Hypotheses . . . . . Summary . . . . . . METHODS . . . . . . . . PETEST O O O O 0 Preliminary Methodology . . Instrument Design . . . Questionnaire Administration . FINAL STUDY . . . . . Sample Description . . . Measures and Descriptive StatiStics O O O O 0 Internal and External Orientations Mothers . . . . . . Children . . Television Exposure to Antisocial Behaviors . . Children's Measures of Antisocial Predispositions . . . Validity and Reliability . . Parent and Child Internal and External Orientations . . Exposure to Antisocial Tele- vision Portrayals . . . vi Page A1 to +4 H 18 24 29 31 31 31 36 44 44 45 45 46 47 54 56 62 64 73 Chapter Chapter II (cont'd.) III RESULTS Child's Antisocial Predispositions Data Replicating the Emergence of the Internal and External Orien- tations . Analytical Methods . Summary . Analysis of Variance and Regression: and HC a. Physical Aggression b. Verbal Aggression Ha, Hb, c. Theft . d. Deceit . T-tests: H a. Physical Aggression b. Verbal Aggression C. Deceit . Contingent Correlations a. Physical Aggression b. Verbal Aggression c. Deceit . Summary . IV SUMMARY AND DISCUSSION REFERENCES Summary . Discussion . Dimensions of Parent-Child action Modes Independent and Mediational Effects . and e Inter- Suggestions for Future Research Limitations Conclusion . vii Page 74 78 86 87 89 90 91 95 96 103 107 109 110 111 112 113 116 118 121 123 123 130 130 133 140 142 144 146 LI ST OF TABLES Table Page 1. Means, Standard Deviations, and Number of Cases for Each Index of Parental or Child Responses to Social Situations . . . . 39 2. Principal Factor Matrix with Varimax Rotation, Ordered According to Hypothesized Indexes Loadings . . . . . . . 41 3. Means, Standard Deviations, and Number of Cases for Each Index of Parental Responses to Social Situations (Index range 0-8) . . 51 4. Means, Standard Deviations, and Number of Cases for Each Index of Child Responses to Social Situations (Index range 0-8) . . 52 5. Means, Standard Deviations, Number of Cases and Index Range of Mother and Child Indexes of Reports of the Child's Anti- social Behaviors in Hypothetical Situations . 63 6. Pearson Correlation Coefficients Between Mother and Child Items Concerning the Parent's Reactions to a Positive and a Negative Situatian . . . . . . . 66 7. Pearson Correlation Coefficients Between Mother and Child Items Concerning the Child's Reactions to a Positive and a Negative Situation . . . . . . . . . 68 8. Internal Consistency a Coefficients for the Indexes of Parental Responses Reflecting the InternaIIandvfixternaIIOrientations for Positive and Negative Social Situations . . 69 9. Internal Consistency a Coefficients for the Indexes of Children's Responses Reflecting the Internal and External Orientations for Positive and Negative Social Situations . . 71 viii Table Page 10. a. Test-retest Reliability for Children's Reactions . . . . . . . . . 72 b. Test-retest Reliability for Parent's Reactions . . . . . . . . . 72 ll. Correlations Between the Indexes of Exposure to Antisocial Behaviors in the First and Second Years . . . . . . . . . 74 12. Correlations Between Mother and Child Indexes of Reports of the Child's Anti- social Behaviors in Hypothetical Situations . 76 13. Test-retest Reliability for the Indexes of the Children's Antisocial Predispositions in Hypothetical Situations. Correlations Between One Year Lag Indexes . . . . . 77 14. Internal Consistency a Coefficients for the Indexes of the Mother's Report and the Child's Own Report of Antisocial Behaviors in Hypothetical Situations. Year 2 . . . 77 15. Principal Factor Matrix with Varimax Rotation, Ordered According to Hypothe-- sized Indexes Loadings. Parental Reactions . 79 16. Principal Factor Matrix with varimax Rotation, Ordered According to Hypothe- sized Indexes Loadings. Children's Reactions . . . . . . . . . . 81 17. Means, Medians, Standard Deviations, Number of Cases, Index Range and Internal Con- sistency a Coefficients for the Internal and External Indexes of Parent-Child Modes of Interaction . . . . . . . . 83 18. Parental Orientations, Child's Exposure on the Mothers1 Report of the Child's Physically A ressive Predispositions: Regression and AnaIysIs of Variance Results . . . . . 92 19. Children's Orientations, Exposure on the Self- report of—Physically Aggressive Predisposi- tions: Regression and Analys1s of Variance . 94 ix Table Page 20. Parental Orientations, Child's Exposure on the Mothers' Report of the Child's Verbally Ag- gressive Predispositions: Regression and Analysis of Variance Results . . . . . 96 21. Children's Orientations, Exposure on the Self- report of—Verbally Aggressive Predispositions: Regression and Analysis of’Variance . . . 98 22. Parental Orientations, Child's Exposure on the Motherrs Report of the Child's Predispositions Towards Theft: Regression and Analysis of variance Results . . . . . . . . 99 23. Children's Orientations, Exposure on the Self- report of Predispositions Towards Theft: Regression and Analysis of Variance Results . 101 24. Parental Orientations, Child's Exposure on the Mother's Report of the Child's Predisposi- tions Towards Deceit: Regression and Analysis of Variance Results . . . . . 104 25. Children's Orientations, Exposure 0n the Self- report of’Predispositions Towards Deceit: Regression and Analysis of variance Results . 105 26. Contingent Correlations Between Exposure and the Child's Ph sical A ression as Reported by Mother and Child at Different Intersections of the Internal and External Orientations of Parents and Their Children . 114 27. Contingent Correlations Between Exposure and the Child's verbal A ression as Reported by Mother and Child, at Different Intersections of the Internal and External Orientations of Parents and Their Children. . . . . . 117 28. Contingent Correlations Between Exposure and the Child's Dispositions Towards Deceit as Reported by Mother and Child, at Different Intersections of the Internal and External Orientations of Parents and Their Children. . 120 CHAPTER I STYLES OF PARENT-CHILD INTERACTION AS A MEDIATING FACTOR IN CHILDREN'S LEARNING FROM ANTISOCIAL TELEVISION PORTRAYALS INTRODUCTION During the past decade, a great deal of concern has been expressed by public and private sectors of society with regard to the effects that negative television content may have on children. A large amount of research has been gen- erated by this concern and the results point to a positive association between the child's exposure to undesirable tele- vision content and antisocial attitudes and behavior on the part of the child (The Surgeon General's Advisory Committee on Television and Social Behavior, 1972). However, few re- searchers have focused their attention on the role of other socialization agencies in mediating television's effects (Chaffee, 1976). Parents constitute the first source of socialization to which children are exposed. This primacy factor makes parental practices a central point of inquiry when one raises questions about the social behavior of children. The main point of the present investigation is to see if contrasting parental styles of discipline and interaction substanially mediate the relationship between watching antisocial portray- als on television and the child's own negative social.behaviors. l 2 Some recent research has been concerned with differ- ent activities parents carry out with regard to television exposure by their children. This research can be categor- ized into two tendencies: parental control of television exposure, and parental interpretation of television content. The innovative contribution of this dissertation is the ex- ploration of the role of parent-child interaction modes on the relationship between exposure to antisocial portrayals on television and the children's antisocial predispositions. In this first chapter, the research on parental con- trol and interpretation of television will be summarized as the most immediate antecedent for this line of inquiry. Next, the role of parent-child interactions with regard to social situations in general,* as mediators of television modeling of antisocial behaviors will be explored. Here, also, the relevant literature on family interaction will be reviewed. Lastly, the derived empirical hypotheses that constitute the nucleus of this dissertation will be presented. Parental Controls over Viewing Hanneman g£_al. (1976) indicate that "research on con- trol of children's television behavior by parents indicates that parents generally do not control their children's TV behavior: (p. 13). Atkin (1972) reported data that suggest "that the parents are not always aware of the television fare * Without reference to the TV viewing situation. 3 that their children are exposing themselves to" (p. 1). Atkin also mentions that parental censorship, when it occurs, tends to be with regard to sex content rather than violence. Sears 33 21. (1957) found that some mothers use tele- vision as a soporific to keep their children "quiet and out of mischief" (p. 289). They found that 58% of the mothers of kindergartners interviewed reported from moderate to severe restrictions on watching: "Children may look only during specific times, or at specific programs" (p. 291). Barcus (1969) reported that formal rules were stated about what the child should not watch, with or without power exertion, once the infant had started exposure to undesirable shows.‘ Formal rules use was the most widespread type of control. Greenberg, Ericson and Vlahos (1972) interviewed 100 fourth and fifth graders and their mothers. Four items concerned parental regulation of the child's viewing habits. One item asked if there were any rules about how late the child could watch television. Another asked if there were some shows the child was not allowed to watch. Two items dealt with the loss of viewing privileges as punishment and extended viewing privileges as reward for something special the child did (p. 398). They found that the agreement between the mother and her child was low (r=.19) with respect to the perceived nature of television rules in the household. However they found that "all of the parents and 90 percent of the youngsters said there were rules about how late television could be watched; two-thirds of each group said there were some 4 forbidden shows; one-third said there was punishment in the form of not being allowed to watch television; one-third of the children and one-fifth of the parents said more tele- vision watching was used as a reward for good behavior" (p. 403). Lyle and Hoffman (1972) in a study with first, sixth and tenth graders found that "although the majority of the first grade mothers interviewed stated that they did try to guide program selection for their young children, few indi- cated that they attempted to restrict amount of viewing. About one-third of the students themselves said that their parents tried to control their viewing, either 'now' or 'when they were younger'" (p. 134). In the studies reviewed here one finds that parental control of their children's television exposure is not very widespread. This may be due to the pervasiveness of the medium. Television has taken roots in the family setting. Parental Interpretation of Television Content In a large summarizing effort with regard to instruc- tional television, Chu and Schramm (1967) documented that discussion of television content promotes learning. Ball and Bogatz (1972), in their evaluation of the effects of Sesame Street, found that children who rehearsed the concepts they watched on Sesame Street with their mothers, learned more than children who watched by themselves. 5 Atkin and Gantz (1974) discovered that parental expla- nation and amplification of news program content tends to in- crease the child's acquisition of current events knowledge. McLeod, Atkin and Chaffee (1972) found that parents can miti- gate some of the harmful influences of television violence by interpreting the violent acts as they are portrayed. Tolley (1973) found some evidence that parental communication, re- garding TV news reports, leads to a greater adoption of paren- tal attitudes about issues such as war. Hicks (1968) in an experiment with seven year-old children found that positive and negative sanctions by an adult co-viewer produced corresponding increases and decreases in imdtative aggressive behavior when the adult co-viewer re- mained with the children who had viewed a film of aggressive behavior. These effects disappeared when the co-viewer did not remain with the children in the observation period. Feshbach (1972) found that adult labeling of a violent act as fantasy or reality tended to decrease or increase, respec- tively, the level of aggression displayed by his young subjects. These adults were not the children's parents. Walling (1976) conducted an experiment with first graders. He assigned them to three groups at random: Inter- action with parents, no interaction, and a control group. After one week, the children were interviewed in order to assess their learning of problem resolution techniques from three television shows. He found that "children who 6 interacted with their parents were significantly better able to complete problems which paralled those" (pp. 21-22) they had viewed on television than children who did not interact with parents during viewing and children who did not watch television at all. More recently, Atkin and Greenberg (1977) have re- ported that "the most important factor is joint viewing: the relationship between exposure and aggression is half as strong for children who frequently view police-detective shows with their parents than for children who seldom view with parents" (p. 4). In general, it seems to be that those parents who take the time and exert the effort to explain television content, "and teaching children discernment and discrimination in regard to television” (Leifer 22,31. 1974) may effectively innoculate them against possible negative influences. This may generalize to those occasions in which the child watches alone or with peers. When one considers parental interpretation, it seems that parent-child interaction with regard to television offerings has the potential for modifying the medium's impact. In a related vein, it is to be expected that parent- child interactions in everyday situations, without regard to television, but with regard to the social behaviors of the child, may constitute important determinants of what the child acquires from television portrayals, given the cogni- tive structure provided by parental tuition. 7 Parent-Child Interaction Modes with Regard to Social Situations Now the research on parent—child interaction modes will be considered. In the past this has not been related to what the child acquires from television, with the excep- tion of the family interaction paradigm, that will also be reviewed below. Here the evidence available with regard to parent- child interactions will be reviewed, and testable hypotheses that predict the influence of those interaction modes on chil- dren's social learning from television will be derived. Generally, the writer interprets parent-child interaction during TV time as a special case of the more general phenom- enon consisting of enduring and ongoing interaction and re- sponse modes across the wide variety of social situations in which parents have the opportunity for shaping their children's response patterns. The ways in which parents handle the development of moral behavior in their children should be especially rele- vant to the examination of the child's acceptance or rejec- tion of television portrayals of antisocial behavior. Children may acquire what they watch without necessarily translating their acquisitions into practice. ”Social learn- ing theory distinguishes between acquisition and performance because pe0ple do not enact everything they learn" (Bandura, 1977, p. 28). 8 Children whose parents have enabled them to differ- entiate between "good" and "evil" should be able themselves to approve or disapprove of television behaviors to which they are exposed. Bandura explains: Parents cannot always be present to guide their children's behavior. Successful socialization requires gradual substitution of symbolic and internal controls for ex- ternal sanctions and demands. After moral standards of conduct are established by tuition and modeling, self—evaluative con- sequences serve as deterrents to trans- gressive acts (Ibid, p. 43). Moral training has been a major concern with child psychologists. Internalization of moral values, identified as conscience or superego, should be the result of certain parental practices. Internalization has been defined in a variety of ways, for example: By 'internalization' I mean that they have a set of cognitions that they employ under ap- propriate conditions and thus guide their own behavior, and that they experience self-rein- forcement and self-punishment as a function of their behavior (Staub, 1975, p. 118). Generally to those qualities of children's behavior represented by the control of impulse and the reactions to loss of such control . . . resistance to temptation and feelings of fear, shame, or guilt concerning deviation (or antici- pated deviation) from right and proper conduct . . . it is a part of the mind that controls other parts, directing behavior in ways that are mainly inhibiting and self-punishing (Sears EE.El-r 1965, p. 199). Is the predisposition to subordinate one's hedo- nistic needs in favor of the social and moral requirements of a situation, without regard for external sanctions . . . it is reasonable to A11 9 assume that the most important socialization experiences for the development of such moti- vation are those in which the child faces the conflict between hedonistic needs and moral demands, as communicated by the parent early in life (Hoffman, 1975, p. 234). The tendency of society to satisfy itself as cheaply as possible results in appeals to 'good conscience,‘ through which the individual pays to himself the wages for his righteousness, which otherwise would probably have to be as- sured to him in some way through law or custom (according to Simmel, Hoffman, 1970, p. 262). The concept of internalization is often used to refer to the child's adOption of social norms or roles as its own, and to the resulting con- trol of its behavior by the most complex media- tional functions of cognitive and verbal processes (Aronfreed, 1969, pp. 264-265). these definitions of internalization have at least three elements in common. First, internalization requires the adopti on of social norms; second, these social norms give internal direction to the behavior of the child; and thirdly, such internal direction is achieved by self-generated conse- quences* for the social behavior of the child. The most highlighted point of the concept of inter- nalization is precisely the idea of inner-directedness. The child who has been brought up to be independent of external stimulation for the control of his/her behavior should also be more re counter to one aspect resistance counter to sistant to the modeling of behaviors that run the child's values. Hoffman (1970) mentions that of internalization is precisely "the amount of offered to pressures and temptations to behave the standard" (pp. 286-287). * The child's internal punitive or rewarding reactions. 10 The child who is inner directed should then be able to behave according to what he/she considers apprOpriate, and then, when exposed to direct or vicarious models that do not fit his/her norms, discount and criticize their be- haviors. Conscience, also called internalization or supergo, has been Operationalized in a variety of ways. Some commonly accepted indicators of conscience are: (l) "maintenance of control, in the face of temptation, when there is no one present to insist, and when there is little danger of being caught" (Sears gplal., 1957, p. 366); (2) occurrence of guilt feelings when temptation is not overcome (Ibid.); (3) admitting transgression when asked (Grinder, 1962); (4) emotional upset and confession (Sears pp 31., 1965, p. 240); (5) moral judgments about others which are based on internal rather than external considerations, e.g., confession, accept responsibility, consideration for others (Hoffman and Saltzstein, 1967, p. 45); (6) "moral stage advance" (Lickona, 1976, p. 25); (7) self-condemnation (Hoffman, 1970, p. 264); etc. A low level of moral internalization has been identi- fied in the literature as "behaving prOperly through fear of external sanctions and evaluating moral action on the basis of the likelihood of external punishment" (Hoffman, 1975, p. 232), or as external resolution (or externally oriented initiation and performance) indicated in story completion 11 tasks (Aronfreed, 1961, p. 227). How is moral internalization achieved? The most likely source of influence to greatly affect moral develop- ment are parental practices and interactions with regard to social behavior. The importance of parental practices is emphasized because parents are the initial source of influ- ence on their children (Hoffman, 1975, p. 232). Numerous studies have shown that there is a relation- ship between parental disciplinary practices and the intern- alized control of their children's social behavior. Among them one can identify the following: Allinsmith (1960); Aronfreed (1961); Bandura and Walters (1959); Burton, Maccoby and Allinsmith (1961); Hoffman and Saltzstein (1967); and Sears gp‘gl. (1957). Of these studies, perhaps the most salient are the ones by Aronfreed (1961), and by Hoffman and Saltzstein (1967). Aronfreed utilized a projective story completion technique to elicit response tendencies in 122 white children from the sixth grade classes of two public schools. In the stories, the central child committed an act of aggression directed against parents, a friendly neighbor, or a close companion. The mothers of the children were interviewed at home to find out "about the different things which mothers do to help their children grow up" (p. 227). The mother was asked 12 questions, each describing a particular form of ag- gressive behavior and then asking the mother to report how 12 she usually responded when her child behaved in that way. In general, the findings indicated that middle-class children were more likely to give internalized moral responses, while lower socioeconomic class children tended to be more con- cerned with the external aspects of the transgressions. In a congruent fashion, middle-class mothers were more likely to report the use of love oriented practices, such as reasoning and explanation. Lower-class mothers showed a preference for externally oriented, or power assertive modes of disci- pline. Also, there was a clear tendency for love oriented. inductive, mothers to have children who focus their attention on internal aspects of the wrongdoing, while power assertive, or externally oriented mothers were found to be more likely to have children who focus their attention on the external aspects of transgressions. Hoffman and Saltzstein assessed the degree of internal- ization of morality of a sample of 444, seventh grade children by means of projective story completion and hypothetical trans- gression items. Parents, teachers and peers were also asked to rate the children on their level of moral internalization. Children reported their parents' disciplinary practices, and the parents were interviewed to obtain a report of their own disciplinary techniques. Each respondent (the child or parent) was asked to imagine four concrete situations. . . . Following each situation was a list of from 10 to 14 practices. The respondent was asked to look over the list, then rate the absolute frequency of each (p. 42). 13 The authors found that parental power assertion tech- niques were consistently associated with weak moral intern- alization in the children. . . . in sum it is a pattern of infrequent use of power assertion and frequent use of induction by middle-claSs mothers which generally appears to facilitate the facets of morality included in this study (p. 49). No parental data was obtained from lower-class parents, and the results for this subsample were mixed. Two main global categories of disciplinary types of interactions among parents and their children have been identified by Aronfreed (1976) and Hoffman (1975). One of these disciplinary types is called induction, and the other sensitization. Induction types of discipline have in common . . . that they tend to make the child's con- trol of its behavior independent of external contingencies. In contrast, disciplinary habits of direct physical and verbal attack may be characterized as sensitization because they tend merely to sensitize Ehe child to the anticipation of punishment (Aronfreed, 1969, PP. 309-310). In general, induction techniques include the use of reasoning and explanation, e.g., to point out the require- ments of a situation, or the consequences of the behavior for the child or others. Also, appeals to the child's pride and achievement are considered to be inductive techniques (Hoffman, 1970, p. 286). Sensitization, sometimes called power assertion, "includes physical punishment, deprivation of material objects 14 or privileges, the direct applications of force, or the threat of any of these" (Ibid., p. 285). Although less documented in the literature, the use of external rewards is also expected to belong to the class of parental practices that have been labeled sensitizing techniques. External rewards "reduce intrinsic motivation by creating the impression that one's be- havior is externally prompted and by weakening feelings of competence and self-determination" (Bandura, 1977, p. 107). The main difference between these two modes of parental discipline is that induction is communication oriented and sensitization is based on the exercise of actual and implied power. Induction can be said to provide the cognitive struc- ture with which the child will be able to categorize his social experiences: "Cognitive structure facilitates intern- alized suppression by serving as an intrinsic mediator of anxiety which can intercede before the commission of a punished act" (Aronfreed, 1969, p. 276). Sensitization, by requiring continuous survellance, and by being limited to the disciplinary event, reduces the length of time during which the child experiences anxiety for transgressions (Ibid., p. 313). According to Hoffman (1970), dissonance theory would also account for the effectiveness of induction in forming an independent moral orientation. Induction techniques exert little external power over the child and if she/he refrains from a negative behavior, she/he will be more likely to 15 reduce dissonance by thinking that she/he refrained volun— tarily. However, sensitizing techniques are too obvious and the child may just think that she/he refrained due to extern- al demands (p. 284). Furthermore, because inductive tech- niques point out the consequences of the child's behavior for others, they may develop the child's capacity for empathy. So if transgression occurs, the child who learned by induc- tion strategies will be more likely to experience emotional discomfort or guilt (p. 328). Actually, inductive techniques may be more severe than sensitizing techniques, due to their property of conservation of anxiety, e.g., guilt feelings may be more long lasting than the physical pain derived from a spanking. Induction and sensitization are not mutually exclusive. The parent may balance both techniques in a variety of ways. It has been suggested that a maximizing situation for promot- ing internalization is a minimal amount of sensitization and some larger proportion of inductive parental acts (Hoffman and Saltzstein, 1967). "Apparently, a spoonful of medicine helps the reasoning go down" (Lickona, 1976, p. 25). An affective and love oriented family seems to be a pre- requisite for inductive techniques to be effective. "Being well loved provides the emotional security and feeling of confidence in the essential good of the world . . . neces- sary for considering the needs of others" (Hoffman, 1970, p. 286). Sears g£_al. (1957) in their extensive study 16 of children's acquisition of social behaviors, found that accepting mothers who love their children and use inductive more than sensitizing techniques produce children with more moral internalization than other mothers. Henry (1971), found that in the homes of mentally disordered children "a person is viewed as an adversary and treated like one. 'The child as enemy' is a fairly constant theme in these families." This stands as evidence of the possible negative effects of lack of love. In general, a fairly constant positive cor- relation has been found between nurturance of parents and internalization (Aronfreed, 1969, p. 305). Love withdrawal in the form of ignoring, refusing to speak, explicitly stating dislike, and isolating the child, are considered as inductive techniques by Aronfreed (1968, p. 314). However, this has not been found to be as con- sistently related to the internalization of morality as the other inductive techniques detailed above (Saltzstein, 1976, p. 254; Hoffman, 1975, p. 233). The reasons cited for this lack of consistent relationship is that other-oriented in- duction capitalizes on the child's capacity for empathy, while love withdrawal only emphasizes egoistic concerns, more in accord with sensitizing child rearing practices. To summarize, one can say that a love oriented family will tend to have children who are less dependent upon ex- ternal stimulation for proper behavior to take place. A love oriented family is that in which parents use power as- sertion (sensitization) only when absolutely necessary, but l7 tend to guide their children's social behavior on the basis of considering the consequences of the child's behavior for others, and use reasoning and explanation as the mode of problem resolution. Loss of love as a disciplinary measure is not necessarily effective in promoting moral internaliza- tion. It is illuminating to call attention to a parallel that Saltzstein (1976) has drawn between the set of techniques described above and Kelman's types of attitude change in re- sponse to social influence: "Power assertion goes with com- pliance; love withdrawal and sometimes parent-oriented, and even peer-oriented, induction with identification; and the reasoning component of induction with internalization (pp. 261-262). A child who has internalized moral standards can be expected to be in a better position to reject portrayals of negative behaviors offered on the television screen. The core of the matter resides in the differentiation between "good" and "evil" based on internal conviction and not on external considerations. A child who does not have an in- ternal pilot for social behavior will model those negative portrayals which he/she has witnessed when the likelihood of discovery and external punishment is minimal. A strong moral conscience can be thought of as a servant who follows the child everywhere and prevents him/her from undesirable behavior. This servant is inside the child and its 18 punishments and rewards may be more powerful than all the spankings or prizes of a parent. Children may learn, from many televised instances, that by resorting to force they can obtain certain satisfactions. However, only those with in- ternalized moral values will say "no, it's wrong." Some also will say "no," but because "I may be punished." Family Interaction Perspective Another trend of related research has evolved from the work of a team of social scientists at the University of Wis- consin (McLeod, Chaffee, and Eswara, 1966). This trend of in- quiry emerged from an interest in studying "the possibilities of changing personality through the change in the person's interaction patterns" (p. 4) mainly within the family. These researchers "felt that families were likely to differ in the degree to which they emphasize harmony in interpersonal rela- tions and impose restrictions on the expression of anger" (p. S). MCLeod and Chaffee (1972, p. 83) report that they have consistently found two uncorrelated dimensions of communica- tion structure in families. The first of these dimensions is called concept-orientation, and the second one socio- orientation. In concgpt-oriented families, "the child is stimulated to express his ideas and to challenge others' beliefs. He MM is frequehfiyfiexpased to both sides of an issue, and takes part in controversial discussions" (McLeod, Chaffee and Wackman, 1967, p. 2). 19 In families which are socio-oriented, "the child is encouraged to maintain harmonious personal relations with his parents and others. He may be advised to give in on arguments, avoid controversy, repress anger, and generally keep away from trouble" (Ibid., p. 1). These two dimensions interact to render a fourfold family style typology: (l) ppotective families emphasize harmonious and hierarchical socio relations but do not stress exposure to controversy or expression of ideas; (2) consen- sual types emphasize both harmonious, hierarchical relations and controversy or expression of ideas; (3) laissez faire families, where neither socio, nor concept orientation is emphasized; and (4) pluralistic families which stress con- cept orientation and not harmonious, hierarchical social relations. This typology was first used to predict public affairs knowledge and participation. It was expected that children from pluralistic families would be the most likely to acquire and rehearse political knowledge. The pluralistically raised child should feel free of constraints which might inhibit expression of his/her own opinions, and the exposure to dif- ferent points of view should foster diverse interests. The results of at least two studies supported the prediction: "Pluralistic parents and children appear to be more politic- ally informed and active, and more often use the media for information" (Ibid., p. 6), while the children from the other 20 three types of family differed little among themselves. The offsprings of pluralistic families have been shown to be more independent in the judgment of social situ- ations. They should also be expected to be more resistant to persuasive messages. In two experiments with 9th grade children Eswara (1968) and McLeod, Chaffee and Wackman (1967) found that students from concept-oriented homes were less persuasible and more sensitive to the amount of infor- mation substantiating arguments in a persuasive message. Persons from a socio-oriented background indicated they would be more likely to abandon a plan if a neighbor friend said he thought it was a poor idea. In an experiment with adolescents, Stone and Chaffee (1970) replicated those findings. Youngsters from highly socio-oriented homes were more persuaded than others to change their opinion with respect to a social truism (p. 243-244), when attacked by a highly credible source. Pluralistic families may in fact immunize their chil- dren against many sources of influence in the child's en- vironment, as bases for decision making, and provide him/her with a tool for judging evidence in order to arrive at sat- isfactory solutions. "The socio-oriented person . . . asks: Who is involved? Do I like them? Do they like me? The con- cept-oriented person asks: What is involved? Is it a good idea? How does it compare to what I know?" (McLeod and Chaffee, 1972, p. 87). 21 Further studies using the family typology approach have found suggestive evidence on the role of family inter- action patterns in mediating the relationship between TV violence viewing and aggressive behavior. Protective fam- ilies have children who are the heaviest television viewers, and are quite high in violence viewing. Children from plu- ralistic families have been found to be very low consumers of television content, and specifically violent television fare. Protective families tend to have highly aggressive children, while pluralistic families raise quite pacific children. The consensual and laissez faire types fall some- where in between. This set of findings by McLeod, Atkin and Chaffee (1972) and others, have rendered suggestive evidence of the possible indirect role of parental practices in re- ducing the relationship between television violence viewing and the child's aggressive behavior. For example, Atkin (1972) reported that: The relationship between violence viewing and aggressive behavior in homes where the parent tried to teach the child not to act aggressively was compared to homes where a more laissez-faire attitude was implemented. The relationship between violence viewing and aggressive behavior was much stronger in the half of the . . . samples where no emphasis was placed on nonviolent behavior-- while only a slight positive relationship was found where the parents did emphasize non- violence (p. 2). Reiss (1971) reports that families of delinquents have been found to experience the acceptance of "suggestions, ob- servations, or ideas of others as a sign of his own weakness" 22 (p. 23) rather than in a rational frame of mind. One would expect that a child who is positively oriented towards the expression of ideas and their consideration and who is less likely to be manipulated by external influences, would more likely discount television violence as an appropriate means of problem resolution. This type of child may consider many options, including reasoning, before resorting to alternatives that may go counter to his/her empathy concerns. This child can be expected to be more likely to try to understand the ideas and needs of others, without necessarily adopting them. Two main theoretical and research tendencies have been outlined with regard to parent-child interaction modes and their effects on social behavior. Some implications of both perspectives have been indicated for the relationship between anti-social television viewing and negative behaviors on the part of the child. A synthesis of these two perspectives is now required. From our review, one can observe the similarities between the moral internalization perspective and the family interaction orientation. Children from pluralistic families can be expected to be children of inductive parents, and furthermore, to be those children who are relatively inde- pendent of external pressures in order to decide about appro- priate social behavior. These children may watch less anti- social television content, but even if they watch the same amount as their counterparts, they will be less influenced by it. 23 The parental styles of both research trends are con- gruent. Concept orientation goes with reasoning, explanation and an informational orientation in general. Power assertion or sensitization fits the social power orientation where ex- ternal pressures towards conformity are the rule. At this point two general labels are proposed in order to characterize the two general categories that have been outlined. We can think of parental practices and child char- acteristics as belonging to either external or internal types of orientation (Aronfreed, 1968b, p. 34). The externally oriented child is one who looks for the possible external consequences of his/her behavior in a sen- sitizing or socio-oriented parent. The internally oriented child will focus on his internalized resources for judging the behavior that might be appropriate under certain circumr stances, and will use the information supplied by the family in an atmosphere of love, for furthering his/her understand- ing of the world. An externally oriented child is expected to be morally heteronomous or dependent, and an internally oriented child is expected to be morally autonomous. The first will repro- duce undesirable behaviors if they are expected to go un- punished or undetected; the latter will refrain from such behaviors when it is known that the welfare of others is in jeopardy. 24 Hypotheses The combination of a high regimen of internal guide- lines and scarce externally oriented behavioral tendencies should be the optimum mix to permit the child to evaluate social situations and self-responses in terms of internal considerations. Internally oriented children will be in a better position to reSpond to the behaviors they have intern- alized as prOper or inappropriate. On the other hand, when parents generally resort to power assertion and less to reasoning and explanation, their children should be external- ly oriented and be less inhibited from performing the antisocial behaviors witnessed on television. This is specifically so if they feel that they can get away with it in order to re- solve conflict or attain satisfaction of their needs. The basic model underlying the present research is: EXPOSURE TO ANTISOCIAL TV BEHAVIORS PARENT-CHILD INTERACTION A ORIENTATIONS i7 CHILDREN'S MODELING OF TV ANTISOCIAL BEHAVIORS Parent-child orientations should mediate the relation- ship between TV exposure to antisocial portrayals and the child's performance of such behaviors. 25 Before examining the complex contingent relationships that properly test the mediation of parent-child interaction orientations on the relationship between exposure and anti- social predispositions, attention should first be focused on the main effects and specific cell contrasts derived from the theoretical statements presented above. It is clearly the case that an internalized approach or orientation to social situations should inhibit antisocial attitudes and behaviors on the part of the child. Externally oriented parents and children should, on the other hand, facilitate antisocial attitudes and behaviors. Exposure to antisocial portrayals on television has been repeatedly shown to be associated with antisocial predispositions in children. Consequently the independent effect of the internal and ex- ternal orientations and exposure should behave in the follow- ing way: H : The more exposure to antisocial portrayals on television, the more antisocial behavioral predisposi- tions will be displayed by children. H : The more externally oriented the parents and the children, the more antisocial behavioral predisposi- tions will be displayed by children. H : The more internally oriented the parents and the children, the less antisocial behavioral predisposi- tions will be displayed by children. The cells resulting from the intersections of the three factors, internal and external orientations and ex- posure, should result in specific contrasts that can be 26 logically derived. Let §l€§8 be the means in each of the following cells INTERNAL ORIENTATION LO HI EXTERNAL ORIENTATION EXTERNAL ORIENTATION LO HI LO HI E ’1: L0 3E1 322 is x6 0 S HI x3 x4 x7 x8 U R E and given Ha' Hb, and Hc, we expect that i4, which represents the mean of the children's antisocial predispositions at a high level of the external orientation and exposure and a low level of the internal orientation, to be the highest mean in the table. On the other hand is, which is the mean of the chil- dren's antisocial predispositions at a high level of the internal orientation and a low level of exposure and the ex- ternal orientation, should be the lowest mean in the table, since the only factor impinging upon those children in any substantial manner is the internal orientation. According to Hypotheses a, b and c, it is expected that the low internal orientation cells should be higher than the high internal orientation cells: that the high exposure cells should be higher than the low exposure cells; and that the 27 high external orientation cells should be higher than.the low external orientation cells. Following these expectations, one can posit the rank order of the cells in Hypothesis d. Hd: 3E4>§3 = §2>§E1>328>3€7 = §6>§ES Since there is no theoretical reason to expect that exposure to antisocial TV portrayals would be more powerful in affecting antisocial predispositions than the external orientation, or vice versa, it is indicated that i3 should be equal to i2 and that I7 should be equal to E6. Finally, the relationship between exposure to tele- vision antisocial portrayals and the children's antisocial predispositions should be mediated by the external and in- ternal orientations. Highly internally oriented and low ex- ternally oriented parents and children should result in the lowest correlation between exposure and antisocial predis- positions in the children. Highly externally oriented and low internally oriented parents and children should result in the highest correlation between exposure and antisocial predispositions. Also, the overall correlation between ex- posure and antisocial predispositions, and the correlation at the high-high and low-low intersections of the internal and external orientations should all be localized in between the two extremes specified above. To illustrate, let's visualize the combinations that are expected from both the external and the internal parent- child orientations: 28 INTERNAL ORIENTATION LOW HIGH .33 E g: .4 r1 r2 9 2% E3 xva H r3 r4 nag :n Let rA be the overall correlation between exposure to anti- social portrayals on television and antisocial predisposi- tions in the child. r1 = correlation between exposure to antisocial portrayals r2 r3 r4 on television and the antisocial predispositions of the child, at a low level of the internal orientation and a low level of the external orientation. correlation between the same two variables among chil- dren with a low external orientation and a high internal orientation. correlation between the same two variables among chil- dren with a high external orientation and a low internal orientation. correlation between the same two variables among chil- dren with a high internal orientation and also a high external orientation. Considering the theoretical framework presented above one would expect cell r2 to yield the lowest correlation in the table, cell r3 to be the highest, and rA and cells r1 and r4 to have a coefficient lying in between cells r2 and r3. In other words, 29 r2 1 ) .‘ _' .l J . I. .. .J 1. u . . . . . ‘ .‘ i . .. . \ 3 : -. f . , . l ' '\ a I ‘ A h‘ .4 '.‘ a I . 3' s I ' '4 I ( . ' ’ - 'f “I ‘ , . t 1 4 x,_ -~ . ‘. .J .‘ , . q a . . . . V s f I \ '. . I r‘ (a . I 2 g }\ ‘1 \ _} i ’1‘ I ~. ( I . ‘ I 9 . i 1 l- w - ‘ t I n I ' ,4 L - o } .' ‘ A : I‘L -‘ I ., lb . DO _. ' “. ‘5. , i o ‘ .' ’Q’ ~ ' I' h. '5 ,. ' , I ‘ u ) F .4. 4' ‘ \ I ' .- ‘ . ~5f _ J _. ‘ - "cult , s . a <~:.H'~ 1. r}' u . 0 1 J ~ .4 D“ 1'. to}. I- o v ' r ’ I. ‘ -' \I l."" -v, ’ 'n l< . . a l' r s , - ' T '4 {I J.. a ‘ _. ~) v ' I'. ‘ 6" 1..) ~ 0 ‘. _ b :11—"36‘": -?l‘{.f€ E; I (9;.531‘ci ,' r nl‘l ‘V . t) :Lk. rarer. ~. -- . r. . a . . 1 ~. ,*_,':€.' 5 If. '4’ ." e- s :- " ..' /. ‘ -' T. .)1\ "~ .4” ~v.:: ;" r I ‘ L . ‘ -I.L K -4 r...‘ J' i _. ~1'.‘ . \' . x L. ,t it ‘ Q I .b ‘ x \ g. In H r'. J "N ‘4 38 In Table 1 all indexes have some degree of vari- ability. Even in the case with the smallest standard devia- tion ("WOuld you feel happy with yourself?"), the average variation was 1.8 units in a scale of 0 to 8. In the case of parental responses to negative situa- tions, parents were mostly expected to yell and express dis- appointment in the average given the means of 5.5 and 5.4, respectively. Love withdrawal ("Don't talk to you for a while"), on the other hand, resulted in a mean score of 1.9 in a scale of 0 - 10, being this the least likely parental disciplinary technique as perceived by the children. The parental responses to positive situations indicate that in the average children expect their parents to express pride more often than anything else. However, the item ”pay extra attention to you" showed the lowest mean. In hypothetical negative situations, children were most likely in the average to indicate that they would try to repair their undesirable actions. They didn't show, how- ever, that much concern for being caught or punished. In positive situations, the highest mean score was for "feel happy with yourself," and the lowest for feeling that they "deserve a prize." One way of deciding which items should be reused would be to select items frequently endorsed in the average, and with a minimally acceptable variation. This procedure, however, may not be acceptable in the present case. The 39 Table 1. Means, Standard Deviations, and Number of Cases for Each Index of Parental or Child Responses to Social Situations. Y s N Parental Responses Negative Situations (Scale 0 - 10) 1. Keep you from watching TV . . . . . . 2.8 2.7 195 2. Don't let you go out for a while . . 4.1 3.0 194 3. Hit or slap you . . . . . . . . . . . 2.7 2.8 194 4. Yell at you . . . . . . . . . . . . . 5.5 2.9 190 5. Don't talk to you for a while . . . . 1.9 2.4 190 6. Say that they are disappointed in you 5.4 3.0 190 7. Tell you to make up for what you did 3.8 3.1 194 Positive Situations (Scale 0 - 8) 8. Let you watch extra TV . . . . . . . 2.6 2.3 195 9. Give you something special . . . . . 2.7 2.0 193 10. Pay extra attention to you . . . . 2.1 2.0 189 11. Kiss or hug or pat you on the back . 4.0 2.6 194 12. Tell you they are proud of you . . . 5.1 2.2 193 13. Tell you to feel good about what you did 0 O O O O O O O O O O C O O O O 4.4 2.6 197 Child Responses Negative Situations (Scale 0 - 10) 14. Would you feel guilty? . . . . . 5.6 2.9 193 15. WOuld you try to make up for what you did? . . . . . . . . . . . 6.0 3.0 194 16. Would you be afraid your parents(or teacher or other people) may find out? . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 3.8 3.1 193 17. Would you be afraid you may be punished? . . . . . . . . . . . . . 3.8 3.2 191 Positive Situations (Scale 0 - 8) 18. Would you feel happy with yourself? . 6.3 1.8 198 19. Would you feel that you deserve a prize? . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1.6 2.2 195 20. WOuld you want people to know about it? . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2.7 2.1 196 40 theory presented in Chapter I predicted the emergence of two dimensions, internal and external. Those indexes of response clearly associated with each dimension should be the ones to be retained. Furthermore, the indexes should be closely associated with the dimension of which they were predicted to be a part. It was considered that the most appropriate technique to use would be confirmatory factor analysis (Rummel, 1970, p. 22). A certain type of response may not be very widely utilized and still constitute an important mediator of what children model from antisocial television portrayals. Two questions are to be answered in this section: 1. Do an internal and an external orientation emerge as hypothesized?; and 2. Which are the items that better dif- ferentiate one dimension from the other? To answer the above questions, the indexes for paren- tal and child responses were submitted to a principal axis factor analysis routine with varimax rotation. Since two dimensions were hypothesized, the solution was limited to the extraction of two factors. According to the numbering of indexes in Table l, the items predicted to belong in the internal orientation dimension were: 6, 7, ll, 12, 13, 14, 15, and 18. The items hypothesized to load on the external orientation dimension were: 1, 2, 3, 4, 5, 8, 9, 10, 16, 17, 19, and 20. 41 Table 2 presents the results of the factor analysis procedure. A loading of .35 is considered to be the cutoff point for including an index in a certain factor, since ”loadings exceeding .35 have been found to be stable and replicable" (Overall and Klett, 1972, p. 109). Table 2. Principal Factor Matrix with Varimax Rotation, Ordered According to Hypothesized Indexes Loadings. Item Factor 1 Factor 2 Internal Orientation 6. Say that they are disappointed in you .13 .58 7. Tell you to make up for what you did .41 .47 11. Kiss or hug or pat you on the back .11 .53 12. Tell you they are proud of you .12 .62 13. Tell you to feel good about what you did .20 .64 14. WOuld you feel guilty? .08 .60 15. WOuld you try to make up for what you did? .02 .68 18. Would you feel happy with yourself? .02 .50 External Orientation 1. Keep you from watching TV .81 .08 2. Don't let you go out for a while .57 .25 3. Hit or slap you .46 -.05 4. Yell at you .36 .09 5. Don't talk to you for a while .53 .07 8. Let you watch extra TV .62 .14 9. Give you something special .48 .22 10. Pay extra attention to you .47 .26 16. Would you be afraid your parents (or teacher or other people) might find out? .36 .36 17. WOuld you be afraid you may be punished? .43 .34 19. WOuld you feel that you deserve a prize? .42 -.06 20. WOuld you want people to know about it? .44 .16 42 Factor loadings can be interpreted to represent "correlations between original measurements and the factors" (Ibid., p. 91). Looking at Table 2 one can say that in general, the two hypothesized dimensions contain the pre- dicted indexes. Factor 2 clearly represents the internal orientation dimension. All hypothesized internal indexes load highly and consistently on Factor 2, and in general show low or almost zero loadings on Factor 1. The one ex- ception is response index number 7, which does not seem to differentiate between the two factors. Factor 1 consistently represents the external orien- tation dimension. Two response indexes, numbers 16 and 17 do not discriminate between the two factors. The results support the expectation that postulated the existence of two parental and child orientations, one external and one internal. Second, although the majority of the indexes pretested to tap both dimensions in fact seem to underly them, three different items correlate about equally with both factors. This last finding deserves further consideration. Index number 7 “Tell you to make up for what you did" was originally hypothesized to be part of the internal orien- tation dimension. This index does show a higher loading on the predicted dimension, however, its loading on the external factor is above the .35 criterion, and the difference between both loadings is very small, .06. One may conclude that 43 parental requests for reparation accompany both a spanking in one case, and an expression of disappointment in another, or that at least children expect their parents to act in this manner. One course of action would be to simply delete such response item from a future instrument. However, it is not known whether the actual parental response would behave in the same fashion. This writer considered collecting the parents own responses in the final study, and did. Two more indexes did not discriminate between the two types of orientation: 16. "WOuld you be afraid your parents (or your teacher or other peOple) may find out?" and 17. "WOuld you be afraid you may be punished?" These two indexes were originally hypothesized to belong in the external orien- tation. Index 16 loads equally on both factors, and index 17 loads higher on the external dimension but the difference between the two loadings is small (.09). Besides, index 17 is close to reaching the .35 criterion. Both indexes reflect the children's own responses as reported by themselves. The first explanation that comes to mind is that perhaps these responses, as they are formulated, are too vague. The chil- dren who report other reactions also report fear of punish- ment and fear of detection. Some children might have internalized moral values, but if they do transgress then they are afraid of the consequences. In the case of detec- tion or finding out, it seems to be best to delete 44 the response item from future consideration. In the case of being afraid of punishment, the item could be rewritten in two separate ones, for example: "WOuld you be afraid your parents may spank you?" and "Would you worry about disappointing your parents?" With these two new items, one could identify children who are afraid of the painful physical consequences for themselves, and children who wouldn't want to hurt their parents. In the first case we would have an external orientation, and an internal in the second. F INAL STUDY Sample Description Three hundred mothers and their children who were part of a panel sample contacted a year before (Atkin and Greenberg, 1977) provided the data base for this study. The mothers were interviewed at home by trained interviewers, and the children were administered questionnaires in their school classrooms. No more than 5% of the children were administered the questionnaires in their homes, since they could not be reached in the schools. The data were collected in two comparable cities of the United States, Haslett, Michigan, and Verona, Wisconsin, from middle class respon- dents. The children were from the fifth, seventh and ninth grades. 45 The actual data return was from 227 respondent pairs from the original sample of 300. This was due mainly to geographical mobility and refusals. The composition of the final sample was as follows: Location: Haslett, Michigan: 130 Verona, Wisconsin: 97 Grade in school: Fifth: 74 Seventh: 81 Ninth: 72 Sex: Males: 112 Females: 115 The average interviewing time for the mothers was approximately 45 minutes. The children took about one hour in the average to complete their questionnaires. Measures and Descriptive Statistics Several sets of data will be presented in this sec- tion. In the first place the reader will find the measures utilized in order to tap the internal and external dimensions of parent-child interactions. Next, the measures of tele- vision exposure used in.this project will be presented. Finally, the childs antisocial measures will be introduced. Internal and External Orientations The measures used in order to tap the internal and external orientations of parent-child interactions do not 46 differ substantially from the measures utilized in the pre- test, and the results of the preliminary study were taken into consideration in order to improve the instrument. Four sets of items were assembled to tap the two dimensions. The mothers responded to the parental practices items, and the children responded to the child response items. Each, mother and child, was presented with eight hypothet- ical situations, four positive and four negative, as follows: Mothers: Positive situations Suppose (NAME OF CHILD) does something really nice for you (and your husband) to show that (HE/SHE) loves you. What would you (and your husband) do? Suppose (NAME OF CHILD) helps a friend in the neighborhood with some hard work, and you (and your husband) hear about it. What would you (and your husband) do? Suppose (NAME OF CHILD) does something really nice for someone in your family. What would you (and your husband) do? Suppose (NAME OF CHILD) apologizes and tells you (and your husband) (HE/SHE) is really sorry for some- thing bad (HE/SHE) did to you. What would you (and your husband) do? W Suppose (NAME OF CHILD) hits a kid in the neighbor- hood after an argument, and you (and your husband) find out. What would you (and your husband) do? Suppose you (or your husband) asked (NAME OF CHILD) to do something for you, and (HE/SHE) doesn't do it. What would you (and your husband) do? Suppose (NAME OF CHILD) lied to you (and your husband) and you find out. What would you (and your husband) do? 47 Suppose (NAME OF CHILD) gets mad and yells at you (or your husband). What would you (and your husband) do? Children: Positive situations Suppose you do something really nice for your parents to show that you love them. Suppose you help a friend in the neighborhood with some hard work. Suppose you do something really nice for some- one in your family. Suppose you apologize and tell your parents you are really sorry for something bad you did to them. Negative situations Suppose you hit a kid in the neighborhood after an argument. Suppose you lie to your parents. Suppose your parents ask you to do something for them, and you don't do it. Suppose you get mad and yell at your parents. The results of the pretest data presented above were taken into consideration for the generation of the response items used in the final study. The rationale for including the different response items was presented in the preceding section dealing with the pretest, and it will not be repeated here. However, two major types of items were added. For both parental and children orientations response items were included to tap parental emphasis about the consequences of the children's behaviors on others, and the children's 48 consideration for the welfare of others including the parents. These types of responses have been found to be important components of the internal orientation of both parents and children (Aronfreed, 1968b and 1976; and Staub and Feinberg, in press). The final set of items also included reasoning and explanation as a parental technique since it has been found to promote the internalization of moral values in children (Sears EE.2l-r 1957; Aronfreed, 1969; Aronfreed, 1976; and Saltzstein, 1976). The number of items used to tap the internal and ex- ternal orientations in children and parents was determined by the antecedents present in the literature reviewed, the pretest results, and by a set of ten parental and children interviews conducted in order to check for those response modes that take place in the home setting.* Consequently, the reader will find that the number of items for the intern- al and external orientations and for both parents and chil- dren is not necessarily the same. i The following parental responses were generated for the positive situations: Internal: Say you are proud of (HIM/HER). * These were informal interviews in the home setting, where parents and children were asked for criticisms and elaboration. Also, a team of researchers highly familiar with this type of research provided inputs for the question- naire formation. tions tions 49 Tell (HIM/HER) to feel good about what (HE/SHE) did. Kiss or hug or pat (HIM/HER) on the back. Explain why it was a good thing to do. Say that you appreciate the good things (HE/SHE) does. Tell (HIM/HER) reasons why (SHE/HE) should keep doing these things. External: Let (HIM/HER) do something (HE/SHE) wanted to do very badly. Let HIM/HER) watch extra TV. Give (HIM/HER) something special. The parental response items for the negative situa- were: Internal: Say you are disappointed in (HIM/HER). Explain why (HE/SHE) shouldn't behave that way. Say (HIS/HER) behavior makes you feel bad. Tell (HIM/HER) another way to solve (HIS/HER) problem. External: Don't talk to (HIM/HER) for a while. Yell at (HIM/HER). Hit, spank or shake (HIM/HER). Keep (HIM/HER) from watching TV. Don't let (HIM/HER) go out for a while. The response items for the children's positive situa- were: Internal: Would you feel good about it? Would you be proud of yourself? Would you feel happy for them? were: a yes, 50 Would you feel happy with yourself? External: Would you feel you deserve a treat? Would you want people to know about it? Would you think you should get something special? The items for the children's negative situations Internal: Would you feel guilty? WOuld you worry about how they feel? Would you worry about disappointing your parents? Would you try to make up for what you did? External: Would you worry that your parents may hit, spank or shake you? Would you worry that your parents may yell at you? Wbuld you worry that your parents may take something away from you? Would you worry that your parents may not let you watch your favorite TV show? The response categories for all the above items was maybe or a no, coded as 2, l, and 0, respectively. All responses to identical items were summated to create indexes of response, across varied situations for both parents and children, and for positive and negative situa- tions. The descriptive statistics of these indexes of re- sponse are presented in Tables 3 and 4. 51 mNN MNN mNN «NN mmm mmm mmm mmm own wNN mNN QNN mNN wNN mmm mmm ANN mam NON HON o o o o o o o m.~ e.e . . . . . . . m.~ e.m . . . . manta e.~ m.e motes» mama» ~.H e.e . . . . . HON mtw O O O O O O O e.~ H.e . . . . . . . HON mow O O O 0 O I O ”OH ”ON. 0 O O o 0 0 0 amoeba) O ONNNN MV'NV'M OMANV‘ o o o o mmha O u>h\0h- O O O O m .m . . . . . . . . . . Hmwoomm maesuofiou Ammm\zH=V w>ww . . . . . . . . . . . . >9 tuuxo nouns lemmszms hog sum> on on cause: 1m=m\mms scenuosom on lemmxzem. awn AGZmWme mcaoo mmox pasonm Amm\mmmv was accuses Ammm\szv Haws moon Amm\mmmv moses» ooom one macaooummm no» nos» mum . . . . . on on mean» ooom n was us has camamxm . . . . . . xomn on» so .mmm\szv use no man so when . can .mmm\mms .umee uaonm poem Heme op lemmsze. Heme . . . .mmm\szv mo ozone mum sch hem AdzmmBZH .mH .hH .mH .mH .vH .MH .NH .HH .OH acoHumsuwm m>wuwmom . . . . . . . . Tawn3 o now use omammm\szv you u.:oa . . . . . . . . . . . >9 ma~noum3.&ouu Ammm\szv moon 0 o c o o o o o o o o .- AmmmgHmv mxm-m HO xcmmm ~U...Hm~ o o o o o o o o o o o o o o o o o AmmmEHmv U.“ HHwM . o o o o o . . wflflnz M HON AmmmgHmv CU. u—HMU U. C8 Adzmmfixm amenoum Ammm\mHss meson 0» so; hmeuoem Ammmxzems Heme . . . own Home 50% nexus H0a>mnon Ammm\mmmv mum . . . >m3 page m>m£0n u. coaaonm Ammm\mmv has sandman . . . . . . . . Ammm\=Hmv a“ omucwommmmwo one so» mom AdzmmBZH maceumsuwm m>wummmz .Amlo mucus xmoch ncoHumsuem Heeoom on noncommom Hmucmumm no xwocH even you mommo mo umnfisz one .m:0eum«>oa tumocmum .mcmoz .m manna 52 vNN mNN mNN wNN mNN bNN bNN oNN MNN emm omm mNN MNN mNN «NN m.H m.H h.H m.m m.H m.H m.H m.m m.H o.m o.N o.w v.H 0.5 m.N m.H ».N m.~ m.N H.v ¢.N ¢.N H.N m.m m.N v.m H.N w.m N.N m.m m _m H50» noum3 5o» umH uoc ~50» EOHH »m3m mcHnuosom 0xmu O O O O O O O O ~50“ pm Hflmh ~50» mecca H0 Hemem .uHs NHMHomem mcHnumeom umm ©H5ocm 50» stnu muH u50nm zocx ou 0He00e ucm3 . mumeu m 0>H0mmo 50» Hmmm . «HH0EH50» suH3 »eemn Homm »ME »ME »08 »m& . «Beau How »eemn Home . mmHmmH5o» Ho o50He on . wuH u50n0 poom Hmwm so» eHnoz .mH 50» oH503 .vH so» eHsos .mH Aézmmaxm so» pesos .NH 50» UH503 .HH no» ounce .oH 50» 0H503 .m AdzmmBZH mGOHu05uHm 0>HuHmoe musmHme muamee mucmHme mucmHme H50» umnu »HH03 H50» umnu »HH03 H50» umuu »HH03 H50» umnu »HH03 mpHp 50» uses How e5 0x08 ou »Hu mmusmHme H50» msHucHoeemmHt u50nm »HH03 «Homm »mnu 30: u5onm »HH03 m»uHH5m mem N30£m >9 OuflHO>MH 50» ©H503 . 50» 0H503 50» UH503 . 50» UH503 IDKDI‘Q {Adzmmfixm 50» 6H503 50» oHsoz 5o» 6H503 . 50» UH503 O HNMQ‘ AdszBZH mcoHum5uHm 0>Hummoz OHHfiU HO mecH comm .Amlo TOGMH xmocHV NGOHum5uHm HMHoom 0u momsoemmm How mommo m0 HonE5z can .mcoHuMH>oe onocmum .mcmoz .v OHQMB 53 The indexes in all cases could range from 0 to 8. In Table 3 one can observe that, in general, the means for the response items belonging to the positive situations are high- er than for the negative ones, but more interesting is to notice that across both positive and negative situations, the internal items have higher means than the external ones. Seemingly, parents tend to be more likely to explain and show affection than to withdraw it or physically punish their chil- ren, or at least these are the things that they are most willing to report. Congruent with the pretest results, item 5 shows the lowest mean in the table and the lowest variability as indi- cated by the standard deviation. Item 5 is at this point a good candidate for exclusion from the analysis. All other items in Table 3, at this point seem to be acceptable, since the discriminatory value for the internal and external orien- tations of an item with a low mean can be of importance in selecting those cases that are extremes in either orientation. In Table 4 one finds a similar pattern of means for children reactions to that of the parental practices in Table 3. The means for the internal are consistently higher than the means for the external orientation items. At this point, all of the items seem to be appr0priate for further analysis, since all the means evidence the existence of the behaviors of interest to an acceptable degree. 54 Television Exposure to Antisocial Behaviors The children in this study were asked to rate a list of 29 shows as to whether they watched each of the shows "every week," "most weeks," "some weeks," or "never." The scale points were coded as 5, 4, 2, or 1 respectively. The exclusion of the value 3 was done because it was considered that the interval between watching "most weeks” and ”some weeks" was about twice as large the distance between "every week" and "most weeks," or the distance between "some weeks" and "never." The 29 shows were selected on the bases of high view— ership, distribution across the days of the week and the be- haviors characteristically portrayed in those shows. The selection of high viewership levels of each show and the variability of portrayal of antisocial behaviors in each show was carried out according to the results of a recent viewership and content analysis set of studies (Greenberg, Atkin, Edison and Korzenny, 1977 and forthcoming). The shows included in the questionnaire were: The Six Million Dollar Man, Rhoda, The Jeffersons, Little House on the Brairie, Happy Days, Laverne and Shirley, Baa Baa Black Sheep, Police Woman, Kojak, Good Times, the Bionic Woman, Charlie's Angels, The Waltons, Welcome Back Kotter, Hawaii Five-O, Sanford and Son, Chico and the Man, The Rock- ford Files, Emergencyl, The Mary Tyler Moore show, Bob Newhart, 55 Starsky and Hutch, All in the Family, Jabber Jaw, The Rink Panther, The Bugs Bunny/Roadrunner hour, Shazam, Isis, and Fat Albert and the Cosby Kids. The viewership rating of each show was weighted by the frequency of occurrence of antisocial behaviors in each show, as determined by the content analytic studies cited above. In those studies it was found that the most prevalent antisocial behaviors available on the current television fare are, in order of prevalence: verbal aggression, physi- cal aggression, deceit and theft. Based on those results it was decided to limit the study of antisocial behaviors to those more available for modeling on television programming. Four indexes of exposure to antisocial behaviors were then created, one for each type of antisocial behavior. In each index, each amount of exposure to a show in the ques- tionnaire list was multiplied by the frequency of each of the four types of antisocial behaviors in that show. So, if a show did not contain any physical aggression, the product of exposure by frequency of portrayal would be zero. The advantage of such indexes of exposure is that they are intended to reflect more accurately than other methods used in the past the degree to which individuals are actually exposed to specific television portrayals. The stability of television portrayals by the methods of content analysis used by Greenberg pp 31. (Ibid) has been demonstrated in ongoing research across three episodes of a subsample of programs. 56 Below are the means, Standard deviations and number of cases for each index of exposure to antisocial behaviors on TV. Index of Exposure to: Y S N TV Physical Aggression (range 365-1502) 848.63 249.84 213 TV Verbal Aggression (range 534-1857) 1020.31 248.33 212 TV Theft (range 32-116) 62.92 17.77 215 TV Deceit (range 136-518) 287.60 73.93 213 Children's Measures of Antisocial Predispositions In order to tap the children's proclivity to utilize antisocial modes of conflict resolution, the mothers and their children were asked to report the likelihood that the child may respond antisocially across varied social situa- tions. This method has been widely utilized in the research concerning children's modeling of antisocial behaviors, especially physical aggression (e.g. Leifer and Roberts, 1972). The mothers interviewed were presented with the fol- lowing social situations and response items: 57 For instance, what if someone out in front of (HIM/ HER) in a long line. What would (HE/SHE) do? . . . Would (HE/SHE) push them out . . . yes, maybe, or no?1 Would éHE/SHE) yell at them . . . yes, maybe, or no? Would (HE/SHE) tell them politely to leave? The cashier at a store gives (NAME OF CHILD) one dollar more in change than (HE/SHE) is supposed to get. What would (HE/SHE) do? Give the extra money back Keep the money3 Suppose (HIS/HER) friends are all going to the movies, but you order (HIM/HER) to stay home and finish (HIS/ HER) homework for school. Would (HE/SHE) argue with you?2 Would (HE/SHE) finish (HIS/HER) homework like you tell (HIM/HER)? Would (HE/SHE) lie and say (HE/SHE) already finished it?4 What if a kid is using (NAME OF CHILD)'s bike without asking (HIM/HER). (HE/SHE) asks to ride it, but the kid won't give it back. What would (HE/SHE) do? Scream at the kid?2 Wait awhile until the kid gives it back? Hit the kid?1 1These items constituted the index of physical aggression. 2These items constituted the index of verbal aggression. 3These items constituted the measure of theft. 4These items constituted the index of deceit. 58 Suppose your child is playing a game. No one is watching when (HE/SHE) takes (HIS/HER) turn, and (HE/SHE) doesn't do well. What would (NAME OF CHILD) do? Say (HE/SHE) did well4 Say (HE/SHE) hasn't had (HIS/HER) turn yet, and takes another turn Tell the truth All response items could be answered with a yes, may- be or no, coded respectively as 2, l, and 0. Items indicat- ing the same type of response, e.g. physical aggression, verbal aggression, theft or deceit were summed to form in- dexes for that type of response. Table 5a contains the means, standard deviations, the number of cases and the range for each of the indexes of the mothers' report regarding their children's antisocial pre- dispositions. The children's questionnaires contained the following social situations and response items. What if someone cut in front of you in a long line. What would you do? Would you push them outl. . . Would you yell at themz. . . 1These items constituted the index of physical aggression. These items constituted the index of verbal aggression. 3 4 These items constituted the measure of theft. These items constituted the index of deceit. 59 Suppose you are playing a ball game and one kid throws the ball at your head while you aren't looking, and it hits you. What would you do? Hit the kid with the balll. . . Kick the kidl. . . Shout bad names at the kidz. . . The cashier at a store gives you one dollar more in change than you are supposed to get. What would you do? Give the extra money back . . . Keep the money3. . . A kid is using your bike without asking you. You ask to ride it, but the kid won't give it back. What would you do? Scream at the kidz. . . Hit the kidl. . . Knock the kid off your bikel. . . What if a friend is feeling bad because they keep making stupid mistakes while trying to play a new game. What would you do? Help them to learn the rules . . . Say a mean thing to themz. . . Say you still like them anyway . . . 1These items constituted the index of physical aggression. These items constituted the index of verbal aggression. 3These items constituted the index of theft. 4These items constituted the index of deceit. 60 Suppose you are playing a game. No one is watching when you take your turn, and you don't do well. What would you do? Say you did well4. . . Say you haven't had your turn yet, and take another turn4. . . Swear at the kid who tells on youz. . . Your friends are all going to the movies, but your mother orders you to stay home and finish your home- work. What would you do? Argue with your motherz. . . Lie and say you already finished it4. You are playing a game with another kid and you are winning. The other kid decides to quit in the middle. You ask them to stay, but the kid says no. What would you do? Say a mean thing to the kidz. . . Grab the kidl. . . What if you find somebody's pen on the floor at school. What would you do? Try to find the owner . . . Keep the pen3. . . You work really hard to finish your homework for school. Some kid takes your papers and rips them up. What would you do? Shout at the kidz. . . 1These items constituted the index of physical aggression. These items constituted the index of verbal ag- gression. 3 4 These items constituted the index of theft. These items constituted the index of deceit. 61 Beat up the kidl. . . C0py someone else's homework4. . . You are walking down the street and dr0p a dollar bill. Another kid grabs the money and won't give it back. What would you do? Yell at the kidz. . . Start a fight with the kidl. . . Throw something at the kidl. . . What if your friends are playing a game and really need one more person to play, but you don't like that game. What would you do? Join in so they can play it . . . Help them find another player . . . Lie and say you have to go home4. . . All response items could be answered with a yes, may- be or no, coded as 2, 1, and 0 respectively. Items indicat- ing a similar type of antisocial behavior were added to form and index for that type of behavior. Table 5b contains the means, standard deviations, the number of cases and the range for each of the indexes of the child's own report of antisocial predispositions. 1These items constituted the index of physical aggression. 2These items constituted the index of verbal aggression. 3These items constituted the index of theft. 4These items constituted the index of deceit. 62 Within the range for the mother and child indexes, one finds that the means indicate that all behaviors were reported to exist to some extent, and that none of them is not within the potential behavioral repertoire of the child. The standard deviations indicate that, within the limits of the index, all the behaviors are reported to varying extents by different mothers and children. Validity and Reliability Validity addresses the question of whether we are measuring the phenomenon that we intended to measure or not. Reliability addresses the question of whether measurements render consistent results over time or internally, within a measurement instrument. This last reliability estimate is better known as internal consistency. In the instruments there were three main sets of variable measurements: 1. Parent and child responses to social situations in terms of internal and external orientations; 2. Exposure to anti- social television portrayals, in the areas of physical ag- gression, verbal aggression, theft and deceit; and 3. Antisocial predispositions on the part of the child for re- solving social conflict situations. The questions of valid- ity and reliability will be addressed for each set of measurements separately. 63 Table 5. Means, Standard Deviations, Number of Cases and Index Range of Mother and Child Indexes of Reports of the Child's Antisocial Be- haviors in Hypothetical-Situations. Indexes of the Mother's Report Y S N Index Range Physical aggression 1.2 1.1 224 0-4 Verbal aggression 3.6 1.7 225 0-6 Theft 0.4 0.7 223 0-2 Deceit 1.2 1.5 223 0-6 Indexes of the Child's Report Physical aggression 7.4 5.1 220 0-18 verbal aggression 8.3 3.9 222 0-18 Theft 1.9 1.4 225 0-4 Deceit 2.4 2.2 221 0-10 64 Parent and Child Internal and External Orientations a. Validity: As documented in the first chapter, several studies in the past have found both an internal and an external orientation. Through a factor analytic confirmr atory solution, with the pretest data presented above, it was found that the two hypothesized factors did appear with- in the same type of instrument described for use in the present study. The confirmatory factor analysis solution is an indication of "factor validity" which can be inter- preted as evidence of construct validity. Those items de- signed to measure the constructs clustered together, accord— ing to the indications of past research, some of which is based on direct observations of behavior. Nunally (1967) explains: "With content validity, factor analysis mainly is important in suggesting ways to revise instruments for the better. With construct validity, factor analysis provides some of the tools that are most useful for determining internal structures and cross-structures for sets of vari- ables" (p. 101). The ideal and most definitive validation of these instruments would consist of correlating actual observations of behavior with responses to the questionnaire items. However, 65 it was not possible to conduct such concurrent test valida- tion given the resources available. Convergent validity estimates were derived from items administered independently to the mother and the child. The mothers were asked to respond to one positive and one neg- ative child situations, and the child was asked to respond to one positive and one negative parental situations. The mothers were asked to report how the child "would feel or react if (HE/SHE) did any of these things," and the child was asked to report "what your parents would do to you if you did any of these things." The two situations with the appr0priate wording for mothers and children were the ones in which the child helps "a friend in the neighborhood with some hard work," and the one where the child gets mad and yells at his/her parents. Tables 6 and 7 contain the situations and the response items used for validating parental and child responses respectively. These tables also contain the zero order correlation coef- ficient between the mother and the child responses, the number of cases, and the significance of the coefficient. In Table 6 one can observe that when children are asked to report about parental practices the covariation be- tween the mother's responses and the child reports is gen- erally very low. Only one statistically significant correlation is found in the case of the positive situation, and two in the case of the negative situation. The average 66 Table 6. Pearson Correlation Coefficients*Between Mother and Child Items Concerning the Parent's Reactions to a.Positive and a Negative Situation. Suppose (NAME OF CHILD) helps a friend in the neighborhood with some hard work, and you (and your husband) hear about it. What would you (and your husband) do? Say you are proud of (HIM/HER) . Kiss or hug or pat (HIM/HER) on the back . . . . . . . . . . . Explain why it was a good thing to do . . . . . . . . . . . . Let (HIM/HER) watch extra TV . . Give (HIM/HER) something special. Suppose (NAME OF CHILD) gets mad and yells at you (or your husband). What would you (and your husband) do? Say you are disappointed in (HIM/ HER) o e o o o o e e o e e e Yell at (HIM/HER) . . . . . . Explain why (HE/SHE) shouldn't behave that way . . . . . . . . Hit, spank or shake (HIM/HER) . Keep (HIM/HER) from watching TV . ._£_ .;E_ .04 227 .18 227 -.02 226 .01 226 .08 227 Average r .01 226 .02 226 -.04 226 .27 226 .29 225 Average r Signif- icance * Zero-order 67 correlation for the positive situation was .07, and .12 for the negative one. In Table 7 the reader finds that the mothers seem to perceive their children's reactions quite differently from themselves. The average correlation for the positive situa- tion is .05, and .15 for the negative situation. The correlations, in general, are very low, despite the statistically significant coefficients, and at this point the only validation available is that forthcoming from the factor validation replicating the existence of the in- ternal and external dimensions. A separate section, below, will deal with factor analytic results replicating the emergence of the two dimensions. b. Reliability: A measure of internal consistency for the newly created indexes was obtained for all response items corresponding to parental and child responses, and to the positive and negative situations. Cronbach alpha as an ex- tension of the Richardson Kuder formula 20 was utilized for this purpose. Cronbach alpha is a summary measure of the average correlation between all possible random halves of the components of the scale. Table 8 contains the alpha coefficients for the in- dexes of parental responses to social situations. The reader should recall that these items were only presented to the mothers. With one exception, all the indexes seem to be internally consistent to an acceptable degree. The only 68 HOUHOIOHON .4. mH. Hoo. .mofi woo. hoo. .m.G mo. mo. Hoo. mo. .m.c .m.z .m.: .m.: .m.c .m.c mo. v TOGMOHHHcmHm H 0m0H0>< wmm oNN mmm mmm MNN omm mmm oNN mm. . . . . . . . . . . . . . m30nm >9 ouHHo>mm H50» noumz 50» umH uo: »mE mucmHme H50» umnu »HH03 50» UH503 no. . . . . . . . moHo 5o» umnz How e5 0x08 ou »Hu 50» 6H503 NH. . o o o o o o o o o o o o o o o o o o o «0.50% EOHW xmzm mcHnuTEom mxmu »mE mucone H50» umnu »HH03 5o» oH5oz 0H. . . . mmucmHme H50» msHucHoeemmHo u5onm »HH03 5o» oH503 mo. . . . ~50» um HHo» »ME mucmHme H50» umsu »HH03 5o» oH503 NH. . . . . . . . . . . «Homm »0su 30: u50um »HH03 5o» oH503 HH. . o o o o o o o o o o o o o o o o ngflfigm Hmmm 50W Uflsoz mN. ..oooooooocooococa-oooNSOHAmvfimgm H 0m0H0>< hNN mNN »NN ham QNN NNN hNN 2 mo. . . . oo. . . . vo.u . . . Ho. . . . mo. . . . mo. . . . MH. . . . IMI H0 xcmem .an »mE mucone H50» umsu »HH03 50» oH503 .mucmHme H50» um HHw» can one umm 5o» mwoee5m . . . . . . . NHHmmH5o» nuHB »eemn Hmmm NHMHomem moHanEOm umm UH50nm 50» xCHnu . . . . . . . . . . NETS“ HON %QQM£ Hmmm . . . . . . . . . mHHmmH5o» H0 @50He on . . . . . muH u5onm Socx ou oHeome ucm3 . . . . . . . . . . muH u5onm ooom Hmom . . . . . . . mumoHu m 0>Homoo 50» Hoom 5o» ©H503 5o» oH503 5o» oH503 5o» UH503 50» 0H503 5o» oH5oz 50» oH503 .xHo3 onn 080m zqu coonHonanoc ecu :H ocmHHm m eHmn 5o» omoee5m coHum5uHm 0>Hummmz m can 0>HuHmoe m ou mcoHuommm m.oHHnU ecu moHcHoocoo mEmuH oHHno one Honuoz c003u0mtmuc0H0Hmmoou coHuMHoHHou comHmme .5 0H959 69 Table 8. Internal Consistency a Coefficients for the Indexes of Parental Responses Reflecting the Internal and External Orientations for Positive and Negative Social Situations. Item Index a Positive Situations Say you are proud of (HIM/HER) . . . . . . . . . .70 Tell (HIM/HER) to feel good about what (HE/SHE) did 0 O O O O O O I O O O O O O O O O O O O O O .76 Kiss or hug or pat (HIM/HER) on the back . . . . .86 Explain why it was a good thing to do . . . . . . .78 Let (HIM/HER) do something (HE/SHE) wanted to do very badly . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .78 Let (HIM/HER) watch extra TV . . . . . . . . . . .86 Say that people appreciate the good things (HE/SHE) does . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .80 Give (HIM/HER) something special . . . . . . . . .84 Tell (HIM/HER) reasons why (HE/SHE) should keep doing these things . . . . . . . . . . . . .85 Negative Situations Don't talk to (HIM/HER) for a while . . . . . . . .00 Say you are disappointed in (HIM/HER) . . . . . . .66 Yell at (HIM/HER) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .81 Explain why (HE/SHE) shouldn't behave that way . .67 Hit, spank, or shake (HIM/HER) . . . . . . . . . .84 Say (HIS/HER) behavior makes you feel bad . . . . .81 Keep (HIM/HER) from watching TV . . . . . . . . . .90 Don't let (HIM/HER) go out for a while . . . . . .87 Tell (HIM/HER) another way to solve (HIS/HER) prOblem O O C O O C O I O O O O O O O O 71 70 index for which there is no internal consistency is the first one for the negative situations (don't talk to him/her for a while). That one item has been pointed out as a candidate for exclusion from the final analysis, as indicated by its descriptive statistics. Table 9 presents the alpha coefficients for the in- dexes of child responses to social situations for the intern- al and external orientation dimensions. All indexes seem to be internally consistent to varying degrees. The lowest co- efficient in the table is .61 for "would you feel good about it," and the highest is .88 for "would you worry that your parents may not let you watch your favorite TV show?." Measures of test-retest reliability were also obtain— ed for both parental and child responses. All items dealing with the internal and external orientations were included at the beginning of the questionnaire or interview, and one of the situations containing a subset of the items was repeated at the end of the questionnaire or interview in order to obtain an estimate of test-retest reliability. Table 10, parts a and b contain the correlation co- efficients between duplicate response items in the mother's interviews and in the children's questionnaires. The social situation in both cases is that in which the parents ask the child to do something for them, and the child doesn't do it. All the test-retest correlation coefficients are above .40, and all are statistically significant. The average Table 9. 71 Internal Consistency o Coefficients for the Indexes of Children's Responses Reflecting the Internal and External Orientations for Positive and Negative Social Situations. Item Indexes Positive Situations Would you feel you deserve a treat? . . . WOuld you feel good about it? . . . . . . W0uld you want pe0ple to know about it? . W0uld you be proud of yourself? . . . . . Would you feel happy for them? . . . . . . Would you think you should get something speCial? I O O O O O O O O O O O O O I 0 Would you feel happy with yourself? . . . Negative Situations Would you worry that your parents may hit, spank or shake you? . . . . . . . . . . W0uld you feel guilty? . . . . . . . . . . Would you worry about how they feel? . . . Would you worry that your parents may yell at you? O O I O O O O O O O O O 0 O O I Would you worry about disappointing your parentS? O O O O O O O O O O O O O I O Q Would you worry that your parents may take something away from you? . . . . . . . . Would you try to make up for what you did? W0uld you worry that your parents may not let you watch your favorite TV show? . . .76 .61 .64 .78 .67 .76 .83 .81 .73 .76 .79 .82 .86 .82 .88 72 Table 10. a. Test-retest Reliability for Children's Reactions. Suppose your parents asked you to Signif- do something for them, and you icance don't do it. r* N p < Would you worry that your parents may hit, spank or shake you? . . . .65 226 .001 Would you feel guilty? . . . . . . . .57 225 .001 Would you worry about how they feel? .60 225 .001 Would you worry that your parents may yell at you? . . . . . . . . . .49 225 .001 Would you try to make up for what you did? . . . . . . . . . . . . . .66 224 .001 Average r = .60 b. Test-retest Reliability for Parent's Reactions. Suppose you (or your husband) asked (NAME OF CHILD) to do something for you, and (HE/SHE) doesn't do it. What would you (and your husband) do? Don't talk to (HIM/HER) for a while . .41 225 .001 Yell at (HIM/HER) . . . . . . . . . . .60 223 .001 Explain why (HE/SHE) shouldn't behave that way . . . . . . . . . . .52 222 .001 Hit, spank or shake (HIM/HER) . . . . .69 225 .001 Say (HIS/HER) behavior makes you feel bad . . . . . . . . . . . . . .68 224 .001 Average r = .58 * O O O Zero-order Pearson correlat1on coeff1c1ents. 73 test-retest correlation coefficient is .60 for both parental and child responses. These measures were considered to be reliable enough for further analysis. Exposure to Antisocial Television Portrayals a. Validity: Direct validation of the exposure measure that was used has not been conducted. Bechtel, Achelpohl, and Akers (1972) attempted such a task with direct observa- tion of viewing behaviors of 20 families and a diary record kept by the participants. Respondents in that study were found to overestimate their exposure time. No attempt was made to obtain exposure measures from both mothers and children since children are the best source of information with regard to the television programs they watch. Besides, the mothers' interviewing schedule was al- ready lengthy. b. Reliability: A measure of test-retest reliability was obtained for the indexes of exposure for each of the four areas of concern in this research, namely, physical aggres- sion, verbal aggression, theft and deceit. Identical indexes to the ones created in this study were available from the survey and content analysis conducted one year earlier. Table 11 contains the correlation between the first and the second year indexes of exposure to antisocial be- haviors. All the correlations are statistically significant (p j .001) and all of them are of the magnitude of about .60. 74 Table 11. Correlations*Between the Indexes of Exposure to Antisocial Behaviors in the First and Second Years. Significance Indexes of Exposure r N p 1 Physical Aggression .65 188 .001 Verbal Aggression .62 186 .001 Theft .56 199 .001 Deceit .62 190 .001 .i Zero-order Given the time lag of one year between one index and the other, and given the fact that there are measures for only two points in time, it is impossible to ascertain what part of the variance left unexplained is due to unreliability and what part is due to true change. However, the coeffi- cients obtained are conservative in any case. Given the composition of the indexes, the reliabil- ity coefficients reported do not only refer to viewing re- liability, but to show content stability as well. Concept- ually, one can interpret the coefficients to mean the amount of stability of exposure to the four types of behaviors of interest. Child's Antisocial Predispositions a. Validity: Leifer and Roberts (l972)obtained a correla- tion between similar items to the ones used here and a teacher's rating of r=.33 and r=.49. A similar coefficient was obtained by this writer with Latin American children. In this study, the mothers and the children were asked to 75 report the antisocial behavioral tendencies of the child, and similar measures obtained from the mother and the child were correlated in order to obtain an estimate of convergent validity. The indexes detailed in the section above were correlated for each one of the subcategories of antisocial behaviors that we are concerned with. The results of that analysis are reported in Table 12. The correlations for physical aggression and for theft were found to be statist- ically significant and not very high but in line with the coefficients typically encountered with this type of data. The correlations for~verbal aggression and deceit were found to be unexpectedly low. Given the low degree of agreement between mother and child about the child's pre- dispositions, it was decided that the data for mother and child should be separately analyzed with regard to these measures. b. Reliability: Leifer and Roberts (1972 ) found a test- retest reliability coefficient of r=.72 with measures similar to this writerTL In order to obtain a test-retest reliabil- ity coefficient for the child's antisocial predispositions as reported by the mother and the child, indexes similar to those created for the second year were created for the first year data. Theft was absent from the first year data, consequently no coefficients could be calculated for that type of behavior. 76 Table 12. Correlations*Between Mother and Child Indexes of Reports of the Child's Antisocial Behaviors in Hypothetical Situations. Significance Index r N p 3 Physical Aggression .30 217 .001 Verbal Aggression .10 220 .067 Theft .23 221 .001 Deceit .10 217 .074 *Zero-order Table 13, parts a and b, presents the correlations be- tween the indexes of antisocial behaviors for the first year with the ones for the second year. The reliability coefficients for the children's data are generally higher than for the mothers' report, with the exception of deceit. For both mothers' and children's reports the correlations are statistically significant, although not as substantial as one might have expected. Here again, one does not know how much of the unexplained variance is due to true change, and what part is due to unreliability. Table 14 presents the internal consistency coefficients for the mothers' and children's reports of antisocial pre- dispositions of the child. Since a minimum of three items are necessary in order to compute any measure of internal consistency, physical aggression and theft for the mothers' report are excluded from the table. It can be seen that the internal consistency for the children's indexes is generally Table 13. 77 Test-retest Reliability for the Indexes of the Children's Antisocial Predispositions in Hy- Correlations*Between pothetical Situations. One Year Lag Indexes. Significance r N p i a. Mothers' Report Physical Aggression .52 223 .001 Verbal Aggression .51 221 .001 Deceit .43 221 .001 b. Children's Self-report Physical Aggression .68 214 .001 Verbal Aggression .57 217 .001 Deceit .37 217 .001 1r Zero—order Table 14. Internal Consistency a Coefficients for the Indexes of the Mother's Report and the Child's Own Report of Antisocial Behaviors in Hypo- thetical Situations. Year 2. Index a Mother Verbal Aggression .50 Deceit .58 Child Physical Aggression . .88 Verbal Aggression .81 Theft .61 Deceit .72 Note: Physical aggression and theft coefficients for the mother's report were noncomputable due to the small number of variables. 78 higher than for the mothers'. This difference, however, may be due to the larger number of items available for the children's indexes. All coefficients in Table 14 are of the magnitude of .50 and over. Qata Replicating the Emergence of the Internal and External Orientations As indicated earlier, mothers were asked to report parental practices, and the children responded to the child reaction items dealing with the internal and external dimen- sions. Both parental and child items were submitted to a principal axis factor analysis routine with varimax rotation, limiting the extraction of factors to the number of two. This was done in cider to try to replicate the findings of the pretest and to further validate the existence of the internal and external dimensions. The results of this fac- tor analysis were discouraging. Two clear dimensions emerged, but contrary to expectations, the children's responses clustered on one factor and the mother's responses on the other. Given this initial finding, the two sets of data for mothers and children were factor-analyzed separately. Since these were confirmatory factor analytic solutions, the ex- traction of factors was limited to two with the two sets of data. Table 15 contains the results of the ppincipal axis fac- tor-analytic solution for parental practices. The indexes were ordered according to their position in the internal or in the 79 em. mH. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . HmHowem OGH£HQEOm Ammm\ZH=v 0>H0 v9 0 HH 0 o 0 o e e 0 e o e 0 0 0 o e e o 0 0 0 a ”Huxm sou“; Ammm\2Hmv “.3 Ho. mm. . . . HHooo Hum> on on counts Ammm\mmv mcHeuoaom oo 1mmm\zems nod mm 0 mH o o o e 0 o 0 0 e 0 0 0 o 0 o mHflFfi; M “OH “:0 cm Amm=\szv umfl U.- Goo ”m o N N o o o o 0 0 0 o 0 0 0 o I 0 O O 0 >8 @Gflnoums duo-Hm Ammm\zH=v “”0” mm 0 0o 0 I. o 0 0 o o o 0 0 0 0 O I o 0 O O O I mmm=\=Hmv wxmgm Ho §xcmmm § “H” mm 0 mo 0 II o o o o o e o 0 o o o o 0 o o o o o e o 0 sammm\szv “m HHmM 5H 0 v0 0 0 I o 0 0 o O O 0 O O O 0 O o mHnoflgz a HON Ammm\2Hmv O“ xnfim“ “a “on :oHumucoHHo HmcHouxm «H. om. modes» moon» mduoo dome oHsono 1mmm\mms Hes woodman 1mmm\zumc HHoe HH. mm. . . . . . noon mumm\mmv mmoHnu ooom Tau oumHooHeem 5o» umnu »mm no. me. . . . . . . . . . . on ou mcHnu 000m 0 nmz uH »33 :HmHexm «H. mm. . . . . . . . . . . . . . xomn onu so Ammm\szv ume Ho 55: Ho mme mo. em. . . . . . . . oHo Ammm\mmv umsz u5oum oooo Home ou Ammm\szv HHoa $0. Nmo o o o o o o o o o o o o o o o o Ammm\szv Ho QQOHQ mHm 50% “mm Ho.l me. . . . . . . .EanOHe Ammm\mHmv 0>H0m ou »03 Hosuocm Ammm\szv HHoa hN. we. . . . . . . . . . own Home 50» nexus H0H>m£0n .mmm\mHmv »mm no. he. . . . . . . . . . . »03 umnu 0>mn0n u. :oH5ozm Ammm\mmv »n3 :HmHexm wm. we. . . . . . . . . . . . . o . .Ammm\SHmv :fl Uflflflflommmmflfl OHM 90% %mm coHumuomHHo HmcHouoH N Houoce H Houome souH .mcoHuommm Hmucone .mocHomoq moxmocH oTNHm Ionuoe»m ou mnHoH000¢ oonoHo .coHumuom xMEHHm> guHS NHHumz Houome HmeHocHHe .mH oHnma 80 external orientation. As can be seen the two expected di- mensions appeared. The reader should be reminded that a loading of .35 was considered to be the cut-off point. Factor 1 underlies the internal orientation, and factor 2 comprises the external orientation of parental practices. Only one item loaded about equally and very low on both fac- tors, namely, "don't talk to him/her for a while." It al- ready.has been noted that the mean and standard deviation for that index was the lowest, and also that this index had the lowest internal consistency coefficient. Apparently, the withdrawal of attention is not a pervasive parental technique neither does it accompany any consistent parental strategy. This one index also had a very low mean in the pretest. However it clearly loaded in the external dimen- sion as hypothesized. In the pretest, the children provided the information regarding both parental and child reactions, and perhaps for children this technique of withdrawal of attention or love, is perceived to be symptomatic of ex- ternally oriented practices by their parents. However, when the parents themselves are questioned, the technique is not a part of the externally oriented syndrome. In the factor-analytic solution, the internal dimen- sion accounted for 28% of the total variance, and the ex- ternal orientation accounted for 12%. Table 16 presents the results of the factor analysis of the child responses indexes. Here, once again, the 81 he. Hm.u . . . . . . . . mHmHoQO mcHnumfiom pom cHnonm so» xcHsu Dom oHsoz MH. mo. . . . . . . . . . . . . . muH usonm zocx cu meomQ pan: :0» cHsoz om. m~.n . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . mason» m m>ummmo so» Hmom so» oHsos H“. OH. 0 o o O O o o o o o o o o o o O I O o O 0 O O “zonm >8 mufiuo>mw use» nouns so» uwH no: hue mucmumm Mao» umsu muuoz so» cHsos up. NH. msom Eoum muss manuoEom mxwu was mucwumm mac» uwnu >HH03 so» oHsoz om. mm. . . . . . . . . «so» no HHmm hue mucmumm use» was» huuo3 so» cHsos Hp. vH. . . mac» mxmnm Ho xcmmm .HHn hue mucmumm use» was» muuoz no» 6H503 COHumucwHuo Hmcumuxm no. om. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . NMHmmuaoa suHs mamas Hmmm no» oHsoz mo. 00. . . . . o . . . . . . . . . . . . N505“ HON kmmfln wam 90% ©H903 0H. mm. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . NMHGmHSO% HO USOHQ 0Q 90% OHSOS No.l HF. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Nfifl “Sonm @000 H60“ 50% DHDOS MH. on. . . . . . . . . . . . . mcHn so» ums3 HON mu mme cu »Hu :0» UHsoz so. om. . . . . . . . . mmucwnmm know mcHusHomech usonm muuo3 90> cHsoz mo. Nm. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . NHGOH %0£# 303 HDO£M hHHO3 50% OHDO3 No. cm. 0 o o o o o o o o o o o o o o o o o o o o o Nhuflflgm Hmmm 5.0% Mudflaoz coHumucwHuo HmcuwucH N Houomm H Houomm ETUH .mcoHuommm m.cmH©HH£U Imnuommm ou mcHUHooo¢ commune .coHumuom meHum> nuHs .mmchmoq mmxoccH cmNHm xHuumz Houomm HmmHocHum .mH mHnma 82 clear emergence of the two dimensions is found. Factor 1 underlies the internal orientation and factor 2 reflects the external one. One item loads about equally low on both dimensions, "would you want peeple to know about it." This finding is surprising since the same index did discriminate between factors in the pretest, and in both the pretest and in this study the children were the respondents for this item. The children in this study were older than the chil- dren in the pre-test. However, if age was the explanatory factor for the discrepancy, other indexes should reflect the discrepancies as well. At this point this inconsistency escapes explanation. The children's internal factor accounted for 33% of the total variance, and the external dimension accounted for 19%. At this point the existence of the two internal and external dimensions for both parental practices and children's responses to social situations have been successfully repli- cated. However, the replication was done for parental and child indexes separately, since the solution including both sets of data renders a parental set of practices and a child's cluster of responses without discrimination between the two orientations. The low convergent validity coefficients re- ported below may explain to some extent the overriding co- variation within the two separate sets of data. It is ap- parent that mothers and children see things quite differently. 83 To represent the internal and external orientations for parental practices and children's responses, four in- dexes were created by summating those indexes that loaded together in different factors. The two indexes that did not clearly load in any one dimension were excluded. Parental internal and external indexes were created as well as children's internal and external indexes. Table 17 contains the means, medians, standard devia- tions, number of cases, index ranges and the internal con- sistency alpha coefficients for the four indexes. From examining this table it is clear that the external orienta- tion is consistently lower than the internal. The variation within indexes is proportionally higher, however, for the external dimensions. Table 17. Means, Medians, Standard Deviations, Number of Cases, Index Range and Internal Consistency a Coefficients for the Internal and External Indexes of Parent-Child Modes of Interaction. Index i' Median S N Index Range a Child Internal 47.6 50.5 11.6 216 0-64 .89 External 14.1 12.17 9.7 218 0-48 .79 Parents Internal 68.6 72.92 11.8 217 0-80 .85 External 18.3 17.45 10.2 219 0-56 .72 84 The alpha coefficients of internal consistency indi- cate that the indexes are sufficiently internally reliable given that the lowest of them all was .72. The four indexes were intercorrelated among them- selves and the results are as follows: Child Internal x Child External r=.l3 p<.05 Child Internal x Parent Internal r=.ll p<.06 Child External x Parent Internal r=.08 n.s. Child External x Parent External r=.27 p<.001 Child Internal x Parent External r=.03 n.s. Parent Internal x Parent External r=.36 p<.001 According to the literature reviewed in the first chap- ter, besides expecting to find one factor for both parental practices and child reactions for each of the internal and ex- ternal orientations, it was to be expected that the internal parental orientation should be associated with the internal child orientation, and that the parental external orientation should be associated with the external child orientation. The correlation obtained between the two internal indexes was low (r=.11, p=.053) and fell short of significance. However, the correlation found between two external orientations was 85 .27 and statistically significant. These results seem to indicate that either the perceptions of parents and children differ substantially, or that internally oriented parents do not necessarily contribute to the internalization of moral values in their children. However, the more obvious types of discipline encompassed by the external orientation do seem to contribute to the child's focus on external factors of social situations. The correlations obtained between the internal and external orientations for both the parental practices and the children's reactions were .36 and .13 respectively, both of them statistically significant. These findings seem to indicate that to the extent that parents and children are more internally oriented, they tend to also be more con- cerned with external considerations. In the children's case, the correlation, although statistically significant, is not substantial in magnitude. In the parents case the coeffi- cient is more substantial and it may indicate that parents that are more concerned about their.chi1dren tend to do more of everything, although it is noted that generally there is stronger tendency towards being internally oriented. Given the discrepancies obtained from the mothers' and the children's data, it was decided that the analysis to test the hypotheses presented in Chapter I should be done separately for the mothers' and the children's data. 86 Analytical Methods There were six main hypotheses to be tested, Ha - Hf, and different analytical tools seemed most apprOpriate in each case. Hypotheses Ha - HC were tested by means of multiple regression analysis. Multiple regression analysis renders regression coefficients that, when standardized, indicate the magnitude of the unique contribution of one standard deviation of one of the independent variables on the depend- ent variable when holding all other factors constant. This form of analysis will also supply us with a multiple correla- tion coefficient that, when squared, indicates the amount of variability in the dependent variable that is accounted for by the set of independent variables. The interactions among variables can also be ascertained by this method when enter- ing the interaction terms as independent variables in the regression equation. To complement the regression analysis, a multiple analysis of variance was conducted to obtain contingency tables where the magnitude of individual cell means could be visually inspected. The results from both forms of analysis were expected to be very similar since both forms are de- rived from one general analytic model (Kerlinger and Pedhazur, 1973, pp. 6-9). Hd, was tested by means of Student t-tests. 87 Finally, Hypotheses He and Hf were tested by means of contingent correlations. A contingent correlation is a conditional statement that separates those cases that fall in certain categories for which a Pearson Product Moment correlation is computed. All analyses were conducted in a CDC 6500 computer system, with programs of the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences, at the computer facilities of Michigan State University. Summary First, the results of a preliminary study pretesting a set of instruments intended to tap two dimensions of parent and child modes of response were introduced. The sample utilized in this study was described and the descriptive statistics of the measures used in this re- search project were presented. Three main sets of variables were considered: a. Internal and external orientations of parents and children; b. Exposure to antisocial television portrayals; and c. Antisocial behavioral predispositions of children. Validity and reliability estimates for the meas- ures utilized were obtained and discussed. The existence of an internal and an external orienta- tion of parental and children's reactions to social situa- tions was replicated, and it was found that althought the two dimensions emerged for parents and children separately, they did not appear when the data for parents and children 88 were jointly analyzed. It was decided that the hypotheses of this study would be tested for the parental and the children's data separately. Finally, the analytical tools utilized for testing the hypotheses of this research were presented and described. CHAPTER III RESULTS The results of this study will be presented in three general sections, according to the order in which the hy- potheses were originally introduced. The first section pre- sents the results dealing with the main effects of the internal and external orientations, and exposure to anti- social television portrayals on the child's antisocial pre- dispositions. Here, the interactions are explored. Secondly, the hypothesized contrasts among different cells resulting from the intersections of exposure to television antisocial portrayals and the internal and external orien- tations on the child's antisocial predispositions, are pre- sented. The last section comprises the results dealing with contingent correlations between exposure to antisocial por- trayals on television and the child's antisocial predispo- sitions at different intersections of the internal and external dimensions of parent-child orientations. Within each section the internal and external parental and child modes of interaction are separately considered. Also, the four different categories of antisocial portrayals and children's predispositions are separately analyzed, namely, physical aggression, verbal aggression, theft, and deceit. 89 90 Analysis of Variance and Regression: Ha, Hb, and Hc In this section, Hypotheses a, b, and c are tested by means of regression analysis and analysis of variance. The hypotheses were worded as follows: H : The more exposure to antisocial portrayals on television, the more antisocial behavioral predisposi- tions will be displayed by children. The more externally oriented the parents and the children, the more antisocial behavioral predisposi— tions will be displayed by children. H : The more internally oriented the parents and the children, the less antisocial behavioral predisposi- tions will be displayed by children. All three hypotheses will be simultaneously tested by both analytic methods, regression and analysis of variance, for each of the sets of data we have, the mothers and chil- dren, and for each category of antisocial behaviors of interest in this study: physical aggression, verbal aggres- sion, theft and deceit. Since there are three main effects that are being hy- pothesized, there are three possible two—way interactions, and one possible three-way interaction. In the analyses of variance the interactions are dealt with by the traditionally accepted methods (Overall and Klett, 1972, pp. 448-449). In the regression analyses, however, the interaction terms are constructed as multiplicative terms of the different combina- tions of the independent variables. These multiplicative 91 terms, by including the independent variables in themselves, are highly correlated with the independent variables. This collinearity presents a problem in determining the magnitude of regression coefficients.* In order to avoid collinearity the interaction or multiplicative terms are standardized, and through such standardization procedure the correlations be- tween the independent variables and the interaction terms are attenuated (Pink and Noell, 1972). The resulting regression coefficients for the interactions are directly interpretable. Now, the test of Hypotheses a, b, and c will be presented. a. Physical Aggression As the reader will recall, the parental internal and ex- ternal orientations were reported by the mother, and the child's internal and external orientations were reported by the child. Table 18a indicates that exposure to physical aggres- sion on television and the external orientation of the parents have a relatively strong effect on the children's aggressive predispositions, when the interaction terms and the internal orientation are held constant, as evidenced by the partial standardized regression coefficients (B). The same trend is observed when one looks at the zero order correlation coefficients. The interaction term of ex- posure by the external dimension also shows a statistically * If extreme collinearity exists, it may be impossible to invert the correlation matrix of the independent vari- ables, and if regression coefficients are obtainable they tend to be unstable from sample to sample. See Kerlinger and Pedhazur (1973). Table 18. 92 Parental Orientations and Child's Exposure on the Mothers' Report of the Child's Physically Aggressive Predispositions: Regression and Analysis of Variance Results- a. Regression B r Exposure .29** .31** External .20* .28** Exposure x Internal x External -.15 .01 Exposure x External .14 .16* Exposure x Internal -.13 .03 External x Internal .07 .09 Internal -.02 .01 R = .41** R2 = .17 b. Analysis of Variance INTERNAL LO HI External External LO HI LO HI LO §1=.83 §2=1.os §5=.54 ‘ §6=l.23 Exposure N=35 N= 17 N=24 N=22 HI §3=1.50 '§4=2.00 §7=.77 §8=l.70 N=26 N=21 N=l7 N=34 OVERALL MEANS ‘Tfiange0;1) EXPOSURE** EXTERNAL** INTERNAL HI LO HI LO HI LO 1.55 0.89 1.54 0.92 1.14 1.29 * p 5 .05 **< P _ .001 Note: No interactions were significant 93 significant correlation coefficient which is not relevant since that correlation is still the result of the attenuated original correlation between the external orientation and exposure. Table 18b which presents cell means for the dichot- omized independent variables and significance of the analy- sis of variance test, provides identical results. The multiple correlation in Table 18a when all the variables are correlated with the dependent variable is statistically significant, and the coefficient of determina- tion (R2) indicates that all the regressed variables jointly contribute 17% to the variation of the child's physically aggressive predispositions. Hypotheses a and b are supported and Hypothesis c is rejected in favor of the null, since the internal parent- al orientation does not appear to diminish the children's favorable predispositions towards physical aggression. None of the interaction terms exhibit a significant regression co- efficient, nor were the interactions found to be significant in the analysis of variance. When we turn to the children's data in Table 19a and b we find that both forms of analyses render identical results. The internal dimension is overwhelmingly the most critical factor in predicting the children's aggressive predisposi- tions. Exposure and the external child orientation are also found to be significant contributors, and none of the 94 Table 19. Children's Orientations and Exposure on the Self- report of Physically Aggressive Predispositions: Regression and Analysis of Variance. a. Regression B r Internal -.49** -.45** Exposure .19* .23** External .15* .14* External x Internal -.10 .05 External x Internal x Exposure -.03 -.13 Exposure x External .02 .09 Exposure x Internal .01 .09 R=.53** R = .29 b. Analysis of Variance INTERNAL LO HI External External LO HI LO HI LO §1=7.55 §2=10.73 §s=3.79 §6=5.22 Exposure N=30 N=11 N=28 N=27 HI ‘§3=9.06 '§9=11.63 '§7=5.7o ‘§8=6.17 N=l8 N=32 N=23 N=24 OVERALL MEANS (Range 0-18) EXPOSURE* EXTERNAL* INTERNAL** HI LO HI LO HI LO 8.39 6.16 8.29 6.32 5.16 9.66 * p': .05 ** p i .001 Note: No interactions were significant 95 interactions are statistically significant. The contribu- tions of all three variables are in the expected direction, and it was decided to reject the null hypotheses in all three cases, for Ha, Hb, and Hc‘ ‘It is observed that all three independent variables and interaction terms con- tribute 29% of the variation in the dependent variable, the principal contributors being the main effects. It is interesting to note that while the external dimension was significant and the largest predictor with the mother's data, the internal orientation was a substantial and significant predictor in the child's case. With the mother's data the internal orientation was unimportant, and with the Child's data the external orientation contributed a smaller amount to the child's physically aggressive pre- dispositions. Exposure to television physical aggression was about equally important in both sets of data. b. verbal Aggression Table 20a and b present the results regarding the parental orientations and the mother's report of the child's verbally aggressive predispositions. Again, one finds that both forms of analyses give the same results, and that these results are very similar to those for physical aggression. The external orientation and exposure to TV verbal aggression are the most important predictors of the child's verbally aggressive predispositions. Neither the interaction terms nor the internal orientation were found to be significant or 96 Table 20. Parental Orientations and Child's Exposure on the Mothers' Report of the Child's Verbally Ag- ressive Predispositions: Regression and AnaIysis of Variance Results. a. Regression B r External .20* .24** Exposure .17* .23** Exposure x Internal -.06 -.07 Internal -.08 .06 Internal x External -.07 -.08 Exposure x External .04 .05 Exposure x External x Internal -.01 .10 R = .31* R2 = .09 b. Analysxs of Variance INTERNAL LO HI External External LO HI LO HI LO '§1=3.17 ‘§2=3.81 ‘§5=3.12 §6=3.58 Exposure N=35 N=16 N=26 N=24 HI x3=3.81 x4=4.26 x7=3.13 x8=4.29 N=26 N=23 N=15 N=31 OVERALL MEANS (Range’046) EXPOSURE* EXTERNAL* INTERNAL HI L0 HI LO HI LO 3.97 3.36 4.02 3.31 3.62 3.69 * p 5 .05 ** p 5 .001 Note: No interactions were significant 97 substantial. Nine percent of the variance in the dependent variable was accounted for by total set of predictors. Hypotheses a and b were supported by these data, and Hypothesis c was rejected in favor of the null. The results of the analyses that tested the three hypotheses with the children's data are presented in Table 21a and b. The children's internal orientation shows again to be the most potent negative predictor of the child's pre- dispositions, verbal aggression in this case. Exposure is again found to be important, and the external orientation has a contribution of zero. None of the interactions turned out to be significant or important in any sense. With the children's data it was decided to reject the null hypotheses in the cases of Ha and Hc, but it wasn't possible to reject the null in the case of Hb' All the pre- dictors in the system accounted for 12% of the variance in the children's verbally aggressive predispositions. c. Theft Table 22a and b presents the results for the mothers' data regarding theft, for Hypotheses a, b, and c. The analysis of variance results indicate that none of the main effects or interactions were statistically sig- nificant. The regression analysis results are almost iden- tical except for the regression coefficient for the interaction of exposure with the external orientation. This discrepancy between both forms of analyses can be readily explained due 98 Table 21. Children's Orientations and EXPOSUFe on the Self- repOrt'of—Verbally Aggressive Predisp051tions: Regression and Analysis of Variance. a. Regression B r Internal —.26** -.29** Exposure .19* .18* Exposure x Internal .05 .14* Exposure x Internal x External -.06 -.l4* Internal x External -.05 .06 Exposure x External .04 .07 External -.01 .01 a = .35** R2 = .12 b. AnalySis of Variance INTERNAL LO HI External External LO HI LO HI LO §l=8.70 §2=8.77 §5=6.77 §6=6.96 Exposure N=27 N=13 N=35 N=23 HI §3=9.33 SE4=9.9 SE7=9.41 328:7.68 N=21 N=30 N=l7 N=28 OVERALL MEANS (Range 0-18) EXPOSURE* EXTERNAL INTERNAL“ HI LO HI LO HI LO 9.05 7.61 8.37 8.28 7.50 9.26 t p S. .05 ** Note: No interactions were significant 99 Table 22. Parental Orientations and Child's Exposure on the HathngE Report of the Child's Predispositions Towards Theft: Regression and Analysis of Variance Results. a. Regression B r Exposure x External .17* .17* External .14 .14* Internal -.13 -.10 Exposure x Internal -.14 -.01 Exposure .07 .09 Internal x External .05 .10 Exposure x External x Internal -.05 -.01 2 R = .27 R = .08 b. Analysis of Variance INTERNAL LO HI External External LO HI LO HI L0 351=.51 352:.47 355:.28 §6=.44 Exposure N=39 N=15 N=25 N=18 HI §3=.44 §4=.54 §7=.20 §8=.43 N=23 N=24 N=15 N=37 OVERALL MEANS (Range 0-2) EXPOSURE EXTERNAL INTERNAL HI LO HI LO HI LO 0.42 0.43 0.47 0.39 0.36 0.50 p‘: .05 'k p i .001 ote: No interactions were significant with ANOVA * N 100 to the elimination of variation in the independent variables resulting from their dichotomization into high and low. The significant interaction means that although none of the com- ponents, exposure or the external orientation, contribute to favorable predispositions towards theft in children, the joint action of both variables does seem to have an impact. Originally the writer did not hypothesize this two way interaction effect, and will not elaborate further on it, except to recognize its interest as a post hoc finding. In this instance, Hypotheses a, b, and c are rejected in favor of the null. The inconsistency of this set of find- ings with regard to the pattern that has been identified with physical and verbal aggression can be explained in terms of the restricted variability of the dependent variable. The mothers were only presented with one item regarding their children's predispositions towards theft, and the range of the scale was extremely limited as can be seen in Table 22b. The children's data in Table 23a and b are more con- sistent with the patterns of results identified above. The internal orientation on the part of the child is the most powerful negative predictor of the child's predispositions towards theft. Neither the external orientation or the measure of exposure were found to predict the dependent vari- able to any extent. In the regression analysis, it was found that there was a small significant effect of the 101 Table 23. Children's Orientations and Exposure on the Self- report of Predispositions Towards Theft: Regression and Analysis of Variance Results. a. Regression B r Internal -.33** -.33** Internal x External -.l7* -.07 Exposure x External x Internal -.09 -.l6* External -.08 -.13* Exposure .03 -.01 External x Exposure .01 .01 Internal x Exposure .01 .03 R = .38** 32 = .15 b. Analysis of Variance INTERNAL LO HI External External LO HI LO HI LO §1=2.07 §2=2.39 §5=1.59 ‘§6=1.23 Exposure N=29 N=13 N=32 N=22 HI '§3=2.oo ‘§4=2.16 '§7=1.81 ‘§8=1.3§ I N=20 N=32 . N=21 N=30 OVERALL MEANS (Range 0—4) EXPOSURE EXTERNAL INTERNAL** HI LO HI LO HI LO 1.82 1.76 1.72 1.85 1.44 2.13 .3 -<_ .001 : No interactions were significant with ANOVA 102 interaction of the internal and the external orientations, and this constitutes another post hoc finding that is pre- sented at face value. This interaction term indicates that the unique effect due to the joint action of both orienta- tions depresses the likelihood of the child's favorable attitudes towards theft. The larger range of this scale makes the results to be trusted a little more than the ones for the mother's data, however they must be treated with caution. Hypothesis c is supported and Hypotheses a and b are rejected in favor of the null. Fifteen percent of the variance in the dependent vari- able is accounted for by all the independent variables, being the internal dimension the most important of all of them. At this point it is important to emphasize that no correlation between exposure to television theft and the children's predispositions towards theft was found either with the mothers' or the children's data. Further exams ination of the data dealing with theft would be useless since the original focus of this study is the analysis of parental mediation of children's learning from antisocial television portrayals. Since no original correlation was found no mediation can be investigated. Consequently the rest of the hypotheses to be tested will not include theft. 103 d. Deceit The results regarding deceit for both the mothers' and children's data are in Tables 24 and 25. Both regression and analysis of variance have render- ed identical results in this case, with one exception. The mothers' data in Table 24a and b show that exposure and the external orientation are significant predictors of the child's favorable predispositions towards deceit. The in- ternal dimension was found to be a significant predictor with the regression analysis, but not so with the analysis of variance. The reason for the discrepancy can one more time be explained by the restriction of variability due to the dichotomization of the independent variables in the analysis of variance approach. Given this restriction of variability we can confidently Opt for placing more weight on the regression analysis results and decide to reject the null hypotheses in favor of the research Hypotheses a, b, and c with the mothers' data. The complete set of independ- ent variables accounted for 18 percent of the variance in the dependent variable, and no interactions were found to be significant. When one considers the results for the children's data, we find that one more time the internal dimension is a very powerful negative predictor by itself, and that no other main effects or interactions are statistically signif- icant. These results are in Table 25a and b. Taking into Table 24. 104 Parental Orientations and Child's Exposure on the Mother's Report of the Child's Predispositions : Regression and Analysis of Variance Results. Towards Deceit a. Regression B r External .30** .28** Internal -.29** -.08 Exposure .21* .28** External x Internal -.12 -.02 Exposure x External .07 .09 Exposure x Internal -.06 -.04 Exposure x Internal x External .01 .08 R = .42** r2 =.18 b. Analysis of Variance INTERNAL LO HI External External LO HI LO HI LO §1=.81 §2=1.31 §5=.64 §6=1.09 Exposure N=37 N=16 N=25 N=22 HI x3=l.39 x4=l.65 x781.00 x8=l.61 N=23 N=23 N=16 N=33 OVERALL MEANS (Range 0-6) EXPOSURE* EXTERNAL* INTERNAL HI 1 L0 HI LO HI LO 1.46 0.91 1.45 0.93 1.14 1.22 * p i .05 **p < .001 Note? No interactions were significant with ANOVA Table 25. 105 Children's Orientations and Exposure on the Self- report of Predispositions Towards Deceit: Regression and Analysis of Variance Results. a. Regression B r Internal -.40** -.36** Exposure .12 .15? Internal x External -.12 .00 External .09 .06 Exposure x External x Internal -.02 —.11 Exposure x Internal .02 .08 Exposure x External -.00 .05 R = .41** R2 = .17 b. Analysis of Variance INTERNAL LO HI External External L0 HI LO HI LO §1=2.41 §2=2.54 §s=1.53 §6=1.91 Exposure N=29 N=13 N=32 N=23 HI x3=3.00 x4=3.97 x7=l.90 x8=1.79 N=l9 N=29 N=;9 N=29 OVERALL MEANS (Range 0-10) EXPOSURE EXTERNAL INTERNAL** HI LO HI LO HI LO 2.71 2.02 2.60 2.14 1.76 3.06 * p §_.05 ** < p‘_ .001 Note: No interactions were significant 106 consideration all the independent variables, the amount of variance explained in the child's favorable predispositions towards deceit is 17%. Hypothesis c is confirmed and Hy- potheses a and b are rejected by the results obtained. To summarize, two main patterns of results have been found, one with the mothers' and another with the children's data. In the mothers' case it was consistently found that television exposure to antisocial behaviors and the parent's external orientation towards the child's behavior are the most consistent predictors of the child's antisocial pre- dispositions. In the children's case, it was found that exposure to antisocial behaviors in general is also a consistent pre- dictor of the child's antisocial predispositions, but not as powerful as the internal orientation of the child with regard to social situations. The internal orientation on the parents part was not found to always significantly predict the children's anti- social predispositions, in the sense of reducing them. With the children's data the external orientation of the child was not found to always enhance the antisocial predisposi- tions of the child. The analyses of variance have given us the opportun- ity to inspect the means of the dependent variable at differ- ent intersections of the dichotomized independent variables. 107 In Hypothesis d specific predictions were made regard- ing the differences or similarities among the cells in those tables, and will now proceed to present the results of those hypothesized comparisons. T-tests: Hd In order to check for the specific effects of differ- ent values of the independent variables on the dependent variable,a set of one-tailed t-tests was conducted. The reference for the specific means being tested are the tables in the above section that deal with the analyses of variance results. A special notation will be utilized for indicating the statistical significance of the comparisons hypothesized and this notation will be exemplified with the original hypothesis. It was stated and expected that: §4>SE3 = §2>§1>§8>§7 = SE6>SES If the hypothesis were completely supported with a set of data it would look exactly as the original statement of the hypothesis except that it would have the actual means between the symbols larger than (>) or equal (=). Whenever a symbol ">" is placed between two means the difference between them is at a probability level of less than .05. In the cases of hypothesized equalities, in order to avoid a type I error, the probability level is established at .5 two-tailed, and only in those occasions in which the 108 statistical significance level of "t" is larger than .5 it is said that the equality is confirmed. Both symbols "> " or "==" will only be used for hypothesized differences or equalities. At this point the reader should be reminded that according to the theory in_the first chapter, there are reasonstxnexpect that i4 should be the highest mean and E5 should be the lowest. Clearly, those children who themselves or their parents are highly externally oriented and who are highly exposed to television antisocial portrayals, and who themselves or their parents are low in their internal orien- tation should be the children most likely to be antisocially predisposed. However, those children who are not highly exposed to television antisocial behaviors, who themselves or their parents are highly internally oriented, and low in their external orientation, should be the children least likely to be antisocially predisposed. The above relation is expressed in Hypothesis d, and the rest of the comparisons are derived from that expectation giving equal weights to exposure and the external and in- ternal orientations, the external orientation being con- sidered a facilitator and the internal an inhibitor of anti- social predispositions. With these considerations in mind we proceed to present the results of comparisons between specific means. 109 a. Physical Aggression Below are the results for Hypothesis d regarding physical aggression for the mothers' and the children's data. Hd: §4>§3=§2>§1>§8>§7=§6>§5 Mothers: 2.0 1.5 1.6 0.83 1.7 > 0.77 1.23> 0.54 Children: 11.63> 9.06 10.73> 7.53 6.17 5.7 = 5.22 3.79 The pattern of the means for the mothers' data con- firms the expectations for the comparisons comprising §4-§1, but not so for the comparisons for the second half of the hypothesized order of means. There are no statistically significant differences between the pairs from E4 to El, and there are two significant comparisons, as postulated, in the second part of the expression. The comparison between £4 and 55 was found to be sta- tistically significant (t=4.55, df=30.25, p < .001 with a separate variance estimate) in the expected direction, and in general we can conclude that Hd is partially supported with the mothers' data. The general pattern of the means is consistent with the results presented in Table 18a and b. In that table it was found that exposure and the external orientation had significant main effects but not so the internal orientation, that is why we find that i8 is larger than £1 (t=-3.38, p < .001) in the direction Opposite to that hypothesized. 110 The means corresponding to the children's data are all in the expected direction. Three of the comparisons were statistically significant. The comparison between §4 and i5 is also significant (t=8.08, df=58, p < .001) in the predicted direction. With the children's data Hypothesis d can be said to be partially supported as was the case with the mothers' data. b. Verbal Aggression The results pertaining to the mothers' and the chil- dren's data with regard to verbal aggression are presented below: H §4>§3=§2>§1>§3>§7=§6>§5 d: Mothers: 4.26 3.81 = 3.81 3.17 4.29> 3.13 3.58 3.12 Children: 9.9 9.33 = 8.77 8.70 7.68>’9.91 6.96 6.77 With the mothers' data the means are generally in the direction predicted. The equality between i3 and i2 is sig- nificant below the probability level established of .5, and the other hypothesized equality was not statistically signif- icant. Only one of the hypothesized comparisons was signifi- cant, and one post hoc comparison was found to be significant in the opposite direction to that hypothesized (t= -2.86, df=64, p i .006, two-tailed test) for 3'61 and £8. The differ- ence between E4 and x5 is one more time found to be signifi- cant (t=2.43, df=47, p:<.02). Hypothesis d regarding verbal aggression, with the mothers' data can be said to be partially supported. 111 The means for the children's data are all in the pre- dicted direction with one exception, that is, there was a post hoc significant difference between E7 and i6, where an equality was initially expected (t=2.13, df=38, p < .05, two-tailed test). The equality between i3 and i2 was found to be statistically significant, and the hypothesized dif- ference between R8 and i7 was significant in the predicted direction. The difference between i4 and is, the two ex- tremes of Hypothesis d was significant (t=3.77, df=63, p ‘<.001), and Hypothesis d is supported in part one more time. c. Deceit The results dealing with pred18positions towards deceit are: Hd: §4>§3=§2>§1>§8>§7=§6>§5 Mothers: 1.65 1.39 = 1.31 0.81 1.61 1.0 = 1.09 0.64 Children: 3.97 3.0 = 2.54 2.41 1.79 1.9 8 1.91 1.53 The mothers' data follow the same pattern found for physical and verbal aggression. One more time i8 was found to be larger than 321 (t= -2.55, df=68, p < .02, two-tailed test) in a post hoc fashion. The two hypothesized equalities were corroborated, and the difference between i4 and i5 was found to be significant (t=2.59, df=36.05, p < .02, with a separate variance estimate). These results one more time follow the main effects encountered in Table 24a and b, that is, no main effect was found for the parental internal orien- tation. We can say that Hypothesis d received partial support 112 with the mothers' data in the case of deceit. The children's data exhibit the hypothesized pattern of means, but only the equalities found statistical support. The difference between x4 and §5 is statistically significant in the hypothesized direction (t=4.9l, df=46.21, p <.001 , with a separate variance estimate). In this case we can say that Hypothesis d follows the pattern identified for physical aggression and verbal aggression, and is partially supported. Contingent Correlations: He and Hf The internal and external dimensions of parent-child orientations were divided at the median* in order to obtain the following four cells, each of which contains a zero order correlation coefficient. INTERNAL ORIENTATION 6 LOW HIGH g r1 r2 0 r3 r4 HIGH LOW The correlation coefficient in each of the cells is be- tween one of the types of exposure to antisocial behaviors on television, and the same type of antisocial predisposition in the child. * Three different breakdowns also were considered: the dimensions were subdivided into three equal segments and then two extreme combinations were produced. Whatever the break- downs the results rendered were similar. Consequently the partition at the median was chosen as the least arbitrary for presentation. ‘ 113 As indicated in the first chapter, if rA is the over- all correlation between a type of specific television expo- sure and the same type of antisocial predisposition in the child, it was then hypothesized: He: r2 £3 = $2 > i1 > £8 > £7 = R6 > is Where i1 - E8 were the means in each Of the following cells: INTERNAL ORIENTATION LO HI E EXTERNAL ORIENTATION EXTERNAL ORIENTATION § LO f HI - LO p HI OLO 21 l 352 L 325 J Re ] s U HI ‘E3 1 ‘i4 1 ‘i? J ‘iaj R , , , E The cell means were the average amount Of each Of the three antisocial predispositions--physical aggression, verbal aggression and deceit. With both sets Of data, parents and children, it was consistently found that highly internally oriented children, who watch small amounts Of antisocial television, and whose parents and themselves are low in their external orientation display the least amount Of antisocial predispositions when 129 compared to other children. Also the children highest in antisocial predispositions were those low in the internal orientation and high in antisocial television exposure and the external orientation. Other subsample comparisons were generally found to follow the predicted patterns but just a few were statistically significant. Hypothesis d was said to be partially supported by the data. He: r2 < rA < r3 and Hf: r2 3 r1 fi’r3 r2 5’r4 §_r3 where rA was the overall correlation between exposure to antisocial portrayals on television and antisocial predis- positions in the child. r1 - r4 represented contingent cor- relations at the following intersections Of the internal and the external orientations of both parents and children: INTERNAL ORIENTATION LOW HIGH EXTERNAL LOW ‘1 f ‘2 l ORIENTATION HIGH r3 1 r4 ] Those highly internally oriented parents who were low in their external orientation had children for whom the cor- relation between exposure tO antisocial behaviors on tele- vision and their display Of antisocial predispositions was the lowest when compared with all the children and with other subsamples. The children who showed the highest correlation 130 between exposure and predispositions were those whose parents were highly externally oriented and low in their internal ori- entation. With the children's orientations, the pattern Of findings was Opposite tO the hypothesized expectations. Almost none Of these contingent correlation comparisons were found to be statistically significant. This finding was replicated when no three-way interactions were found to be statistically sig- nificant with regression analyses or analyses of variance. Hy- potheses e and f were inferentially rejected in favor Of the null. Discussion The discussion of the results Of this study will first focus on the dimensionality Of parent-child interaction modes and then on the independent and mediational effects Of parent- al and child orientations. To finalize, some suggestions for future research, limitations Of this study and some concluding remarks will be presented. Dimensions Of Parent-Child Interaction Modes The pretest data Obtained from children regarding their parents' and their own orientations showed that when data are collected from the same respondents the expectations regarding the emergence of two overall dimensions of internal and ex- ternal parent-child orientations were fully corroborated. This finding encourages the most recent theoretical formula- tions in the literature dealing with the internalization Of :moral principles (Lickona, 1976; and DePalma and Foley, 1975). 131 On the other hand, when data were collected from parents and children separately parent-child dimensions failed to appear with both sets Of data; however, the intern- al and external dimensions did emerge for parents and child- ren separately. These results suggest that either the mothers and children perceive things differently or that for other reasons they prefer to report dissimilar perspectives about a common phenomenon. That different peOple perceive Objects or events in dissimilar forms is not a new Observation. "Lewin argued that the phenomena to which the psychologist should direct his attention are what the individual subjectively perceives, not what the Observer perceives as the 'Objective reality'" (Shepherd, 1964, p. 24). This notion is similar to Weick's (1969) enactment process: "The human creates the environment to which the system adapts. The human actor does not react to an environment, he enacts it" (p. 64). If human subjec- tivity is crucial for the understanding of psychological phenomena, it is not surprising to find that parents and children differ in their reports about the same phenomenon, or about phenomena that are supposed to be related. Future research on parent-child interactions may well benefit from collecting all relevant information from the subjects of direct interest, be they parents or children. Children who perceive their parents to be oriented in a certain direction do perceive themselves to be oriented in 132 a similar direction. However, the association between parental perceptions Of their orientation and children's perception of their reactions is not as consistent. The research literature does not specify the degree of association between the two internal and external dimen- sions. This research has shown that the external and intern- al orientations do not lie on a continuum, they tend to be somewhat positively correlated with parental orientation reports (r=.36, pp: .001), and mildly associated with the children's report Of their orientations (r=.13, p i .05). What these associations suggest is that parents that are con- cerned aboutwhat their children do, do more Of everything in order to guide their social behaviors, and that children who are socially active tend to be oriented in both directions to some extent. Another contribution of this study to the parent- child interaction literature regarding the internalization of moral guidelines is that parental and children's reactions to positive social situations are consistent with the orien- tations derived from responses to negative situations. Past research has been mainly concerned with reactions to trans- gressions. In this study, based on scarce literature, posi- tive social situations were presented to mothers and children and their reactions were assessed. Parents who internally respond to their children's transgression do also internally respond to their children's prosocial or positive behaviors. 133 Externally oriented parents with regard to transgressions were found to at the same time be externally oriented re- garding positive acts. The children's data showed the same consistency regarding their internal and external orienta- tions across positive and negative situations. Future re- search may well benefit from incorporating positive situations in order to more fully account for the internalization Of standards in children. Independent and Mediational Effects The role of parent-child interaction modes in mediat- ing the children's modeling of antisocial television por- trayals was found to be negligible in this study. However, some indications of a possible inhibiting effect due to the internal orientation and some enhancing effect on the part Of the external orientation Of parents was suggested by the data. Future research utilizing larger samples may more definitely unveil these patterns. It was of utmost importance to Observe that television antisocial exposure has a moderate and very consistent effect on the children's antisocial predispositions regardless of parental or children orientations. Television as a pervasive agent of socialization showed to be as important as extern- ally oriented parental practices in directly contributing to antisocial predispositions in children. Given the demonstrated independent effect Of tele- vision exposure it can be said that among all the sources Of 134 socialization of children in modern society television is one more "significant other" (WOelfel and Hernandez, 1972) for the child. Television along with other media, parents, siblings, teachers and peers may account for the complete socialization of children. Different interactions of the child with significant forces in his/her environment may do little to alleviate the pervasive influence of the tele- vision medium. The mediation impact Of other socialization forces besides parents still has to be demonstrated. Parents were considered here due to their constant social proximity to the child and due to the potential that parents represent for modifying the social environment of the child. Parental and children orientations were demonstrated to contribute to different extents to the children's anti- social predispositions. However, regardless of whether or not parents use inductive or power assertive socialization techniques, or whether children have internalized moral guide- lines Or not, television and its fare of socially undesirable portrayals teach the seemingly unintended lesson that the way to solve problems is through aggression and deceitful behavior. Future research should clearly look for other modifi- able aspects of the children's environment for attenuating negative television exposure effects. It might be that the only answer to the problem of antisocial television modeling consists of constant surveilance and company at the time 135 that antisocial television programs are aired (Atkin and Greenberg, 1977). It might also be necessary for broadcasters to reconsider their position in view of these findings. If there is little that parents can do to alleviate negative television effects, broadcasters and producers may find themselves undertaking a major responsibility regarding the examples they provide to the youth. Parents, on the other hand are not exempt of responsi- bility since this study showed that parents do contribute to their children antisocial predispositions if they are extern- ally oriented in their child's rearing practices. It is not television alone that is the cause of social illnesses. If parents provide an example of power assertion when dealing with their children they will learn the lesson independently of what television does. Children who by some means have internalized moral values are less likely to behave antisocially but the effect of such internalization was found to be independent Of tele- vision exposure. SO, when controlling for the moral develop- ment of the child, television exposure to antisocial portrayals enhances the children's antisocial predispositions. If there had been a significant interaction between the children's internal orientation and exposure, one might have concluded that the more the internalization of moral values by children, the less the contributing effect of television exposure. No such interaction was encountered in our analyses. Children benefit from internalized guidelines but if they watch negative 136 television examples, they seem to be willing to cOpy them. The question is now raised with regard to the differ- ential effects found for parental and children orientations. Why should the external orientation in parents predict the antisocial predispositions in children and not so the intern- al orientation? In the first place, the external orientation in parents consists Of behaviors similar to those expressed in the children's antisocial attitudes. Parents who hit and yell provide a direct example of antisocial behavior (Bandura, 1973). On the other hand, inductive or internally oriented parents provide an example Of reasoning and explanation that might be reflected in the prosocial attitudes of children, but prosocial predispositions were not studied here. The parental internal orientation was expected to provide a cog- nitive structure for the children's evaluation of their social behaviors, but it was Observed that the relationship between the internal orientation reactions of parents and their children was low and fell short of statistical significance (r=.ll, p=.053). Future research that includes prosocial attitudes or behaviors as the dependent variable may reveal that internally oriented parents also serve as models for their children's positive be- haviors. Second, it may be that verbal exhortations by parents are modeled as verbal endorsements by children, and that motor behaviors by parents are modeled in the same behavioral cate- gory. Children who hear some preaching from their parents may 137 model their verbal behavior and motorically imitate what the parents do. Since here it was found that the internal and external orientations of parents are moderately related (r=.36, p i .001), it wouldn't be farfetched to say that since parents that do more of one thing also do more Of the other, their children match their behaviors both ways. Overtly, the chil- dren express antisocial predispositions for problem resolution and they may verbally reason and explain as their parents do. Bryan (1975) reports that "children who had witnessed a non- giving model would preach charity but, in fact, practice self- ishness. In effect, then, hypocritical children had been produced" (p. 104). In this research it was not investigated whether children model their parents' expressions of concern or reasoning and explanation. At this point the evidence pro- vided by this study suggests that it may be the case that children say what they hear and do what they observe. Why should children be influenced by their own internal orientation and not by their externalized expectations? The components of the children's internal orientation are behav- iors that indicate guilt reactions, consideration for others, reparation and self evaluations. A child who is likely to experience anxiety after transgressions was expected to be less antisocial than a child who does not experience inner conflict. This expectation was confirmed and it can be argued that according to the theory (Aronfreed, 1968b) internally produced anxiety is more long lasting and potent in inducing 138 the child to refrain from antisocial behavior than external considerations. Not only should the children's external orientation be less effective than internalized values in reducing antisocial predispositions. An externally oriented child should have been found to be more willing to behave antisocially since worries regarding external consequences should be less en- during (Aronfreed, 1969, p. 313) than the anxiety produced by internalized principles. It was found that among the anti- social behaviors investigated here, only physical aggression 1 was at all predicted by the child's focus on external consider- ations. Children may in general worry about external conse- quences regardless of their internalization of moral values. The correlation found between the external and internal orientation of the children was indeed low (r=.13, 9.1 .05). When transgressions do occur, or in its case, when positive behaviors take place the fear of discovery might be a function of the specific contingencies of the situation, e.g., the actual possibility of discovery in a certain circumstance. At this point the theoretical perspective that guided this investigation should be reconsidered to incorporate the results of this study and to guide future investigations. Although parental practices are related with the children's orientations, they do not underlie common factors. .Parental practices contribute to the children's socialization, but other influences in the social environment of the child should 139 be considered. Television exposure might not only be an in- fluence on the children's antisocial predispositions, but it may in turn serve as a source for the children's acquisition of internal controls. In this sense, multiple influences might be considered as determinants Of the children's acqui- sitions Of internal controls. The present research suggested that parental extern- alized practices and television exposure are two contributors to negative attitudes. On the other hand, the children's degree of internalization of moral values was shown to inde- pendently reduce those antisocial predispositions. With external parental practices and television ex- posure a modeling effect has been supported. With the chil- dren's internalization of moral standards a more cognitively mediated type of effect has been shown. What needs to be in- vestigated now is the nature of the antecedents of internal- ization in children. Clearly, the relationship between parental practices and the children's internalization of moral standards has not been found to be substantial enough in this study or in the past (Hoffman, 1970, p. 291). This investigation was initiated under the basic prem- ise that children who have internalized moral standards should be less likely to model antisocial television examples. Child psychologists may want to consider the findings of this re- search in order to reevaluate the theory behind the socializa- tion of moral values in children. 140 Research regarding television effects may benefit from having encountered that television has an independent effect on the children's display of negative behavioral predispo- sitions. Parents will be well advised to consider that tele- vision is not the only source of negative behaviors in their children. Broadcasters may want to take into consideration that parental socialization practices do not seem to change the likelihood of television effects on children. Suggestions for Future Research Future research should consider the possibility that parental or child orientations may enhance television model- ing of positive or desirable television examples. It might well be that parent-child interaction modes effectively inter- act with television exposure in such a way that low doses of external considerations and high internally oriented parents and children enhance modeling of socially acceptable tele- vision portrayals. Parents who guide their children to be internally oriented may cue their children to selectively focus on the virtues of prosocial television content. In certain television contexts, children may find models who are suggestive of opportunities for giving form to their intern- alized moral values. Prosocial television examples may channel the internalized orientations of children. The child may find the examples of prosocial television to be a model for expressing their internalized convictions. 141 For television research purposes it might be profit- able to continue exploring the conditions under which parent child orientations underlie common factors. If data are col- lected from parents regarding their practices and their children's reactions, and if data are collected from children regarding their parents' practices and their own reactions it may be found that when all data are collected from the per- spective of one of the participants in parent-child inter- actions two clear dimensions appear. If this is found to be the case in future replications, the results of this research may be altered and more definitive evidence for the role of parent-child interaction modes in mediating children's learn- ing from antisocial television portrayals may be obtained. Further attempts at validating parent-child interaction data should be carried out. Siblings might be asked to re- port on parental practices regarding the child of interest in the family. Also siblings can be asked about the response orientations and the social behaviors of the child of interest. Perhaps of more importance would be the collection of data from mothers and fathers independently. In this study mothers were asked to report for themselves and their husbands and were asked to generalize. This type of measurement might not have reflected what we call parental practices. Obtaining both sets of data can provide for different analyses which might untangle the effect of both parental sources of influence. 142 On the other hand, children might be asked to report on their mothers' and fathers' practices separately. In this study the child was asked to report about what both his/her parents in combination might do. If all the data suggested above are collected, the analyses would become more complex but may render clearer insights into the process under study by separating sources of influence and at the same time providing more valid obser- vations. Limitations The data for this study were collected from interviews and questionnaires. verbal reports of behavior may not come pletely reflect the interactions that take place in the home or family environment. The reader should be cautioned that since no Observational data were collected and since the con- vergent validation of a key portion of the instrument was generally low, the research results may deviate from the actual phenomena that the research attempted to analyze. On the other hand, the reader should also be aware that Observa- tional data are not free of validity problems. Most methods for collecting observational data are Obstrusive to some ex- tent and the behavior of parents and children can also be influenced by the presence of an observer or a recording instrument. The results of this study are also limited by its asso- ciational nature. We Obtained indications of the degree of 143 predictability and relationship between and among measures but in no sense can it be said that the evidence supports a causal relationship. The theoretical statements presented do give indications about causality. However, the analysis was done with data collected at one point in time. In order to establish causality the temporal order of the variables under consideration has to be demonstrated and this was not done here. Longitudinal data collected from parents and children may help in assessing the causal flow indicated by the theory. The socioeconomic status of parents and children in this study was not utilized. Past research indicates that parental orientations or disciplinary practices are related to their socioeconomic status (Aronfreed, 1961). The relative contri- bution of the socioeconomic class of the respondents should be considered in order to assess the degree of independence between parental practices and class. If their contributions are relatively independent both sets of antecedents may be considered as separate predictors. If their contribution is shared, the social class conditions that promote different disciplinary practices should be studied. The main focus of this study was to investigate the potential role of parental practices and children's responses to social situations in mediating negative television effects; the roles of peers, siblings, teachers and other socializa- tion agents were ignored. The deletion of alternate sociali- zation agents limits the conclusions that can be derived from 144 this study. As children grow older different socialization agents increase in impact on their social behavior and moral considerations. The distributions of reports of disciplinary practices by parents and the children's reactions to social behaviors were skewed. Although the analytic methods used in this re- search are robust, alternate methods for diminishing the skewness of the distributions should be considered. One way of achieving this is by expanding the range of the scales utilized, e.g., "Out of ten times that John lied to you, how Often would you consider spanking him?" Less skewed distri- butions should allow the researcher to place more confidence on parametric statistical analyses like those conducted here. Another limitation of this study is the implicit bias in calling a set of behaviors "antisocial." Although he- haviors such as yelling, hitting, stealing and cheating are considered antisocial by a large segment of society, some of those behaviors are encouraged and found positive in some situations, e.g., "if somebody hits you, you should hit back." The value judgment implied in the selection of "antisocial" behaviors may affect the nature of the analysis and conclu- sions that can be drawn from this research. Conclusions Although some indications were found that parent-child orientations may partially account for what the children model from television portrayals, the overall results of this study 145 indicated that television exposure is in itself a consistent and independent contributor to the children's favorable atti- tudes towards the negative behaviors they watch on television. The task of further clarifying the determinants and the nature of children's internalization of moral standards be- longs to child psychologists. However, this task cannot be disassociated from research dealing with television effects if scientists, policy makers, broadcasters and the public are to understand the broader context in which television effects take place. Studies of this type are unlikely to give definitive answers to complex questions in an initial attempt. The bases upon which to build have been explored here. REFERENCES REFERENCES Adkins, Dorothy C., Frank C. Payne and J. Michael O'Malley 1974 Moral DevelOpment. In F.M. Kerlinger and J.B. Carroll, editors, Review of Research in Educa- tion, Vol. 2. Itasca, 111.: F.E. Peacock' Publishing, Inc. r Allinsmith, W. 1960 Aronfreed, 1961 1968 1968b 1969 1976 The Learning of Moral Standards. In D.R. Miller and G.E. Swanson, editors, Inner Conflict and Defense. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston. J. The Nature, Variety, and Social Patterning of Moral Responses to Transgression. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, Vol. 63, No. 2, pp. 223-240. Aversive Control of Socialization. In William J. Arnold, editor, Nebraska Sym osium on Mgtivation. Lincoln, Neb.: University 0 Nebraska Press. Conduct and Cpnscience: The Socialization of Internaliged Control over Behavior. New York: Academic Press. The Concept of Internalization. In D.A. Goslin, editor, Handbook Of§gcializationrgheory and Research. Chicago: Rand McNally College Publishing Company. Moral Development from the Standpoint of a General Psychological Theory. In T. Lickona, editor, Moral Development and Behavior Theory; Research and Social Issues. New York: Holt, Rinehart and WInston. Atkin, Charles K. 1972 Statement at Hearings before the Subcommittee on Communications of The United States Senate, March 24. Atkin, C.K. and W. Gantz 1974 Children's Response to Broadcast News: Exposure Evaluation and Learning. Paper presented at the meeting of the Association for Education in Journalism, San Diego, Ca., August. 146 147 Atkin, C.K. and B.S. Greenberg 1977 Parental Mediation of Children's Social Behavior Learning from Television. Paper presented at the annual convention of the Association for Educa- tion in Journalism, Madison, Wisconsin, August. Ball, S. and G.A. Bogatz 1972 Summative Research of Sesame Street: Implications for the Study of Preschool Children. In A.D. Pick, editor, Minnesota Symposia on Child Psychology, Vol. 6. Minneapolis, Mihn.: University of Minnesota Press. Bandura, Albert 1973 Aggression: A Social Learning Analysis. Engle- wood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice Hall. 1977 Social Learninngheory. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice Hall. Bandura, A. and R.H. Walters 1959 Adolescent Aggression. New York: Ronald Press. Barcus, F.E. 1969 Parental Influence on Children's T.V. Viewing. Television Quarterly, 8: 63-73. Bechtel, R.B., C. Achelpohl and R. Akers. 1972 Correlates Between Observed Behavior and Question- naire Responses on Television Viewing. In E.A. Rubinstein, G. A. Comstock, and J. P. Murray, editors, Television and Social Behavior, VOl. Iv: Television in Day-to-DayL Life: Patterns of Use. Washington, D. C.: U. S. Government Pr1nting Office, pp. 274-344. Bruning, J. L. and B. L. Kintz 1968 Computational Handbook of Statistics. Glenview, 111.: Scott, Foresman and CO. Bryan, James H. 1975 You Will be Well Advised to Watch What We Do Instead of What We Say. In D. J. DePalma and J. M. Foley, editors, Moral Development: Current Theory and Research. Hillsdale, N. J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Burton, R.V., Maccoby, E.E. and W. Allinsmith 1961 Antecedents of Resistance to Temptation in Four- Year-Old Children. Child Development, 32: 689-710. 148 Chaffee, Steven H. 1976 Comparing Television to other Agencies of Social- ization. Memorandum to the Ford Foundation, September. Chu, G.C. and W. Schramm 1967 Learning fromTelevision: What the Research Says. Washington, D.C.: National Association of Educa- tional Broadcasters. DePalma, D.J. and J.M. Foley (Eds.) 1975 Moral Development: Current Theory and Research. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Eswara, Harogadde S. 1968 An Interpersonal Approach to the Study of Social Influence: Family Communication Patterns and Attitude Change. Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, University of Wisconsin. Feshbach, S. 1972 Reality and Fantasy in Filmed Violence. In J.P. Murray, E.A. Rubinstein and G.A. Comstock, editors, Television and Social Behavior, Vol. 2. TeleviSion and Social Learning. Washington, D.C.: Government Printing Office. Fink, Edward L. and James J. Noell 1972 On the Representation of Interaction Effects in Multiple Regression: A Note on Portes' 'On Class Consciousness.‘ Unpublished manuscript, Department of Communication, Michigan State University, East Lansing, Michigan. Greenberg, B.S., C.K. Atkin, N.G. Edison and F. Korzenny 1977 Pro-Social and Anti-Social Behaviors on Commercial TElevisiorin 1975976. Department of Communication, Michigan State University. Report to the Office of Child Development, HEW, Washington, D.C., February. Ero-Social and Anti-Social Behaviors on Commercial TelevisiOn in 1976-77. Department ofLCOmmunication, Michigan State University. Report to the Office of Child Development, HEW, Washington, D.C., forthcoming. Greenberg, B.S., P.M. Ericson, and M. Vlahos 1972 Children's Television Behavior as Perceived by Mother and Child. In E.A. Rubinstein, C.A. Com- stock and J.P. Murray, editors, Television and Social Behavior, Vol 4. TelevisiOn in Dayeto-Day Life: Patterns of Use. Washington, D.C.: Govern- ment Printing Office, pp. 359-409. 149 Grinder, R.E. 1962 Parental Childrearing Practices, Conscience and Resistance to Temptation of Sixth-Grade Children. Child Development, 33: 803-820. Hannenman, Gerhard J. and Herbert S. Dordick 22' El 1975 The Family Hour: A Solution in Search of a Problem. A Research Study of Los Angeles TV Viewers' Response to the Family Hour. Summary. Center for Communications Policy Research of the Annenberg School of Communications. Los Angeles: University of Southern California. Henry, Jules 1971 Pathways to Madness. New York: Random House. Hicks, D.J. 1968 Effects of Co-Observer's Sanctions and Adult Presence on Imitative Aggression. Child Develop- ment, 38: 303-309. Hoffman, Martin L. 1970 Moral Development. In Paul H. Mussen, editor, Carmichael's Manual of Child Psychology. New YorK: John Wiley and Sons, Inc. 1975 Moral Internalization, Parental Power, and the Nature of Parent-Child Interaction. Developmental Psychology: 11(2): 228-239. Hoffman, M.S. and H.D. Saltzstein 1967 Parent Discipline and the Child's Moral Develop- ment. Journal of Personality_and Social Psy- chology, V01. 5, No. 1, pp. 45-57. Kerlinger, Fred N. and Elazar J. Pedhazur 1973 Multiple Regressign in Behavioral Research. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston. Leifer, A.D. and D.F. Roberts 1972 Children's Responses to Television Violence. In J.P. Murray, E.A. Rubenstein, and G.A. Comstock, editors, Televigion and Social Behavior, Vol. 2: Television and Social Learning. Washington, D.C.: Government Printing Office, pp. 43-180. Leifer, A.D., N.J. Gordon, and S.B. Graves 1974 Children's Television: More Than Mere Entertain- ment. Harvard Educational Review, 44: 213-245. 150 Lickona, Thomas 1976 Critical Issues in the Study of Moral Develop- ment and Behavior. In Thomas Lickona, editor, Moral Develgpment and Behavior Theory, Research, aid Social Issues. New York: Hoit, Rinehart aHHLWinston. Lyle, J. and H.R. Hoffman 1972 Children's Use of Television and Other Media. In E.A. Rubinstein, G.A. Comstock, and J.P. Murray, editors, Television and Social Behavior, Vol 4. Television in Day-to-Day LifezIratterns of Use. Washington, D.C.: Government Printing Office, pp. 129-256. McLeod, J.M., C.K. Atkin, and S.H. Chaffee 1972 Adolescents, Parents and TV Use: Adolescent Self-Support Measures from Maryland and Wisconsin Samples. In G.A. Comstock and E.A. Rubinstein, editors, Television and Social Behavior. Vol. 3, Television ass Adolescegt Aggressiveness. Washing- ton, D.C.: Government Printing Office, pp. 173-238. McLeod, J.M. and S.R. Chaffee 1972 The Construction of Social Reality. In James T. Tedeschi, editor, The Social Influence Processes. Chicago: Aldine-Atherton. McLeod, J.M., S.H. Chaffee and H.S. Eswara 1966 Family Communication Patterns and Communication Research. Paper presented to the Theory and Methodology Division, Association for Education in Journalism Convention. Iowa City, Iowa, August. McLeod, J.M., S.H. Chaffee, and Daniel Wackman 1967 Family Communication: An Updated Report. Paper presented to the Theory and Methodology Division, Association for Education in Journalism. Boulder, Colorado. Nunnally, Jum C. 1967 Psychometric Theory. New York: McGraw Hill. Overall, J.B. and C.J. Klett 1972 Applied Multivariate Analysis. New York: McGraw Hill. Reiss, David 1971 Varieties of Consensual Experience. 1. A Theory for Relating Family Interaction to Individual Thinking. Family Process, March, Vol. 10, No. 1: 1-27. 151 Rummel, R.J. 1970 Applied Factor Analysis. Evanston, 111.: Northwestern University Press. Saltzstein, Herbert D. 1976 Social Influence and Moral Development: A Perspective on the Role of Parents and Peers. In Thomas Lickona, editor, Moral Development and Behavior Theory, Research and Social Issues. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston. Sears, R.R., E.E. Maccoby, and H. Levin 1957 Patterns of Child Rearing. Evanston, 111.: Peterson and Co. Sears, R.R., L. Rau and R. Alpert 1965 Identification and Childrearing. Stanford: Stanford University Press. Shepherd, C.R. 1964 §mallGroups= Some Sociological Perspectives. San Francisco, CalifOrnia: Chandler Publishing Company. Staub, Ervin 1975 To Rear a Prosocial Child: Reasoning, Learning by Doing and Learning by Teaching Others. In David J. DePalms and J.M. Foley, editors, Moral DevelOpment: Current_Theory and Research. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Eribaum Associates, Publishers. Staub, E. and H.R. Feinberg Personality, Socialization, and the Development of Prosocial Behavior in Children. In D.R. Smith and J. Macauley, editors, Voluntary Action Research, Lexington Press, in press. Stone, vernon A. and Steven H. Chaffee 1970 Family Communication Patterns and Source - Message Orientation. Journalism:guarterly, 47: 239-246. The Surgeon General's Scientific Advisory Committee on Television and Social Behavior 1972 Television and Growin Up: The Impacrof Tele- Vised'Violence. Mary and: NatiOnal Institute of Mental Health. Tolley, H. 1973 ghildren and War: Political Socialization to International ggnflict. New York: Teachers COllege Press, COlumbia University. 152 Walling, James I. 1976 The Effect of Parental Interaction on Learning from T.V. Communication Education, V01. 25. Weick, R.E. 1969 The Soggal Psychology of Organizing. Menlo Park, CalifOrnia: Addison-Wesley. Woelfel, J. and D. Hernandez 1972 Media and Interpersonal Effects on Attitude Formation and Behavior. Unpublished manuscript, University of Illinois at Urbana. PF A--Burks 9 character. In a stable environment, under what circumstances will an evolu- tionary process approach a limit? With respect to what properties and to what extent does :volution maximize? What kind and degree of randomness is needed to make evol~ ion operate? What is the r 1e of levels of organization (hierarchies) in evol ion? It has been argue that there has not been time for evolution to produce its results by chanc , and countered that evolution proceeds hierarchically, p-oducing small co ounds from.atoms, large compounds from small compounds, and so on up to org and organisms. Such questions should be investigated by sim lation and so formulated quantitatively, at least in ideal form. These are all questions tv be 3 died by computer scientists and bi- ologists, rather than philosophers. B the answers should give rise to con- ceptual and foundational problems in t e philosophy of science. Some tele- ological or maximizing theories are eo‘ivalent to mechanical explanations, for example, Snell's law and the law of .east time. Does this equivalence hold in biology, for example, for th: evol.tion of natural species? What is the relation of maximization in evol tion to »aximization in utility theory, game theory, and economics? What k ad or kind- of relevant quantitative measures Of complexity can be assigted to organi~us and computers and their abilities, so that the information processing powe of different organisms and computers can be meaningfully Compared and so thz development in evolu- tion, learning, and computational a-ility can be measu ed quantitatively? I'll close my remarks by con acting this last su;gestion to the ra- tionalist-empiricist controversy. S ppose we have an autou:ton-1ike account of evolution beginning with physics, yroceeding through chemistry, biochem- istry, the origin of life, and biology and ending with mode men and their genetic programs of, say, 3000 years ag-. Suffix to this thz subsequent his- tory of science up to today. The whole p ocess has produced modern science. How much of science is attributable to evol~tion up to 1000 :.C., and how much to subsequent history? How much of man's\present complexity is due to evolution, and how much to learning from the environmenrg/// The rationalist would hold that in fact innate ideas and principles were produced by evolution and existed in man's genetic program 3000 years ago. Mere strongly, he might maintain that innate ideas are necessary for science in the sense that there has not been time for modern science to de- velOp in 3000 years without such a head start. In contrast, the empiricist would hold that evolution only produced a very general learning program, and emit“.- d!” :‘l‘ "- , "I" D ' “ 11! ’.~..\‘ V7“ '1 - um - , - n ..r.o aw: ILNEC’EQ "c. or” hit. ‘68 ‘ .0.) mt «1 I mean. "l{'dl w u E-Iu .\.a L”‘: lit-ml, ..; .713a1 . 1; Juana. , . n. no no: 2" than! .- dial 7: m l :4. .nnlaolavi n ."I.K.-;" sd'. . >21me ',.A-to oval .III _,-..,..-. . “oo- “1“” "Q Vh' 2'; \ wit!“ W. M w ’1‘ nlqwaw‘. "WITHIN)11113