,——-—w_'——-V—' .. ' -I ...I I, AN INVESTIGATION OF THE RELATIONSHIPS BETWEEN TWO PRESCHOOL PROGRAMS ON THE ADJUSTMENT AND READINESS OF DISADVANTAGED PUPILS Thesis for the Degree of Ph. D. MICHIGAN STATE UNIVERSITY VERA L. mm 1.957 - IIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIII II I I II ‘ ,3; 33;: g; 1410 . . ’ Umvcrsrty This is to certify that the thesis entitled AN INVESTIGATION OF THE RELATIONSHIPS BETWEEN TWO PRE-SCHOOL PROGRAMS ON THE ADJUSTMENT AND READINESS OF DISADVANTAGED PUPILS presented by ‘ Vera L9. Pitts has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for ___P_h__-_D__degree in_E_I..e..meOtarY Education Major professor t' Date 1‘ - ——v 0-169 ABSTRACT AN INVESTIGATION OF THE RELATIONSHIPS BETWEEN TWO PRE -SCHOOL PROGRAMS ON THE ADJUSTMENT AND READINESS OF DISADVANTAGED PUPILS by Vera L. Pitts Statement of the Problem The purpose of this study was to compare the social growth, emotional f development, academic readiness, and general readiness of kindergarten pupils with varying amounts of pre -kindergarten formal school experience. One group had participated in a nine month pre-school program, the second in an eight week summer project, while the third had no formal antecedent experience. Eight hypotheses related to the aforementioned variables were tested. The Population This study included eighty-seven kindergarten pupils who were matched according to birthdate (born in same month), birthplace (born in Flint), sex, and race (Negro). They were divided into three equal groups (twenty-nine each), according to the duration of there pre kindergarten experience. Experimental Group I - pupils who participated in a nine month pre -school program. Experimental Group II - pupils who participated in a summer pre -school Vera L. Pitts program . Control Group - pupils with no pre -school experience. Methodology Instruments used to gather basic data were: The Brenner Gestalt Developmental Test of School Readiness, The Metropolitan Readiness Tests Form A, and The Operation Head Start Behavior Inventory. The data were statistically described by a Kruskal Wallis One Way Analysis of Variance designed by the CDC 3600 Computer at Michigan State University Computer Laboratory. Findings Social Growth (hypotheses I and 11) Significant differences were found among the three groups favoring the group with the longest period of pre-school attendance on several dimensions of social growth: dependability, cooperation and independence. Emotional Development (Hypotheses III and IV). Findings regarding emotional development were inconclusive. On the instrument M, used in October and May, no significant differences were perceived. The 9__H___BI, used only in May, reported significant differences on dimensions of emotionality, self-confidence, jealousy, persistence, and enthusiasm. Without the additional data afforded by the OHBI, the latter hypotheses would have been accepted. Vera L. Pitts Results were inconclusive and contradictory; thus, no defensible conclusion was possible. On rank sum scores on both instruments differences favored the longer pre -school experience. Academic Readiness (Hypotheses V and VI). No significant differences existed among the three groups in reading nor arithmetic readiness. Total Readiness (Hypotheses VII and VIII). No significant differences were found among the three groups related to total readiness (adjustment). Conclusions Implications The length of pre-school attendance was positively related to facilitating some dimensions of social growth (cooperation, independence, and dependability) among disadvantaged children. The length of pre-school attendance was not related to the academic readiness nor total readiness of the groups studied. A consistent pattern emerged from rank sum scores indicating that the duration of pre -kindergarten attendance may be related to the readiness variables though not at a statistically significant level. Differences were in favor of the longest period of attendance . Advocates of pre -school education programs for the disadvantaged Vera L. Pitts cannot continue indefinitely to claim successes in view of research evidence available. Generally programs seem inadequate and inappropriate for tasks they purport to accomplish. Personnel (teachers and administrators), need background in early childhood education. Programs implemented must be articulated with the total educational enterprise. Research findings regarding the disadvantaged must be translated into meaninng curricula and parents must be involved in a significant way. It is important that instruments be developed which would be suitable to measure social growth and emotional development for use with young children. A non-verbal instrument would seem appropriate. Pre-school education sounds so logical and promising that few have paused to question its validity. This program is not a panacea to the problem and etiology of disadvantagement, and hopefully it would not be oversold. The danger of turning this new trend into a fad must be recognized and averted. Recommendations 1. A meaningful and appropriate curriculum which reflects goals of early childhood education pertinent to educating disadvantaged children should be developed. 2. Parent involvement must be solicited. Vera L. Pitts The strengthening of the community school program at primary and pre -school levels. In-service education for teachers, aides, and administrators. Longitudinal studies should be made to determine the optimal duration of pre -kindergarten attendance. The potency of pre-school programs needs to be increased. AN INVESTIGATION OF THE RELATIONSHIPS BETWEEN TWO PRESCHOOL PROGRAMS ON THE ADJUSTMENT AND READINESS OF DISADVANTAGED PUPILS By Vera LI. Pitts A THESIS Submitted to Michigan State University In Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree of Doctor of Philosophy Department of Elementary Education 1967 ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS The culmination of this project leaves the writer indebted to many people who deserve to be acknowledged. To attempt such a list, however, would be impos- sible for the number is legion. Consequently many people must be nameless, but it is not the intent of the writer that they should go unrecognized, nor that their importance should be diminished. The writer would be remiss, however, if she failed to acknowledge and express gratitude to the committee who guided this endeavor. Dr. W. Robert Houston, chairman, worked patiently with the writer throughout the project. His counsel and encouragement far exceeded duty. Dr. Ann G. Olmsted's skill was invaluable in assisting the writer to critically evaluate the project. The concern of William V. Hicks provided encouragement for completing this study. Dr. Clyde Campbell offered personal and professional guidance. His interest was that of a close friend. In retrospect it would be difficult to determine when the idea for this undertaking was conceived. Dr. Robert Whitt, however, was responsible for the writer's move to Michigan where study began with this goal in mind. A Mott Fellowship provided financial assistance during the first year. The Flint Community Schools and Mrs. Harriet Latimer, Research Director, graciously extended their resources. Sincere thanks to the principals and teachers who facilitated the operational aspects of the study. Mrs. Marion Stebbins assisted with the editing of the manuscript. Mrs. Judy Holtz, the typist, exercised inexorable patience in deciphering the hieroglyphics. Without the support of my husband, Leonard, this project would have never materialized. His confidence and encouragement were a constant source of inspiration. It is the hope of the writer that she will justify the trust and confidence of the many people who gave unstintingly of their time and energy to make this dream a reality. Vera L. Pitts ii TABLE OF CONTENTS Chapte r I. INTRODUCTION ............................ Significance of the Study The Problem Hypotheses Assumptions Definitions of Terms The Research Design Plan of Presentation 11. REVIEW OF RESEARCH AND RELATED LITERATURE ....... Pre -School Education and The Disadvantaged Pre -School Compensatory Education Programs Effects of Pre -School Attendance on Social Growth Effects of Pre -School Attendance on Emotional Development Readiness and Pre -School Attendance Summary IH. PROCEDURES OF THE STUDY .................... Methods of Investigation Collection of The Data Methods of Research IV. ANALYSIS OF THE DATA ....................... Hypothesis I Hypothesis II Hypothesis III Hypothesis IV Hypothesis V Hypothesis VI Hypothesis VII Hypothesis VIII V. SUMMARY, CONCLUSIONS AND RECOMMENDATIONS ....... Synopsis of the Problem Page 22 50 67 85 Chapter Page Objectives of the Study Limits of the Investigation Statistical Analysis Summary of Findings Social Growth (Hypotheses I and H) Emotional Development (Hypotheses III and IV) Academic Readiness (Hypotheses V and VI) Total Readiness (Hypotheses VII and VIII) Conclusions Implications Recommendations BIBLIOGRAPHY ................................ 96 APPE NDDC .................................. lO 3 Appendix A - Better Tomorrow for the Urban Child (BTU) Appendix B - Location of Inner-City Schools Appendix C - The Anton Brenner Developmental Gestalt Test of School Readiness Appendix D - Metropolitan Readiness Tests Form A Appendix E - Operation Head Start Behavior Inventory LIST OF TABLES Table Page 1. Diagrammatic Representation of the Development of Emotion . . . . l3 2. Pre -Kindergarten Programs in Flint Inner-City Elementary Schools ................................ 52 3. School Buildings By Year of Construction, Age and Portable Units ................................. 53 4. Student Enrollment and Racial Composition of Schools in the Study ................................. S4 5. Age and Sex of Ninety Negro Kindergarten Children By Experimental Groups (October, 1966) ................ 56 6. Letter Ratings and Readiness Status Corresponding to Various Ranges of Total Scores ....................... 61 7. Age and Sex of Sixty Negro Kindergarten Children By Experimental Groups (May, 1967) .................. 64 8. BGT Rank Sum Scores on Social Growth Dimensions For Three Experimental Groups; October, 1966 ................ 67 9. Kruskal Wallis One Way Analysis of Variance H Ratio on BGT Social Growth Dimensions Among Three Experimental Groups; October, 1966 ............................ 68 10. BGT Rank Sum Scores on Social Growth Dimensions For Three Experimental Groups; May, 1967 .................. 70 ll. Kruskal Wallis One Way Analysis of Variance H Ratio on BGT Social Growth Dimensions Among Three Experimental Groups; May, 1967 .............................. 7O 12. OHBI Rank Sum Scores on Social Growth Dimensions For Three Experimental Groups; May, 1967 .................. 71 Table l3. 14. 15. 16. 17. 18. 19. 20. 21. 22. 23. Kruskal Wallis One Way Analysis of Variance H Ratio on OHBI Social Growth Dimensions Among Three Experimental Groups ............................... BGT Rank Sum Scores on Emotional Development Dimensions For Three Experimental Groups; October, 1966 ........... Kruskal Wallis One Way Analysis of Variance H Ratio on BGT Emotional Development Scale Among Three Experimental Groups ............................... BGT Rank Sum Scores on Emotional Development Dimensions For Three Experimental Groups; May, 1967 ........... Kruskal Wallis One Way Analysis of Variance H Ratio on BGT Emotional Development Scale Among Three Experimental Groups ............................... OHBI Rank Sum Scores on Emotional Development For Three Experimental Groups ....................... Kruskal Wallis One Way Analysis of Variance H Ratio on OHBI Emotional Development Dimensions Among Three Experimental Groups ............................... BGT Rank Sum Scores on Academic Readiness Among Three Experimental Groups; October, 1966 ............... Rank Sum of Scores on Academic Readiness For Three Experimental Groups on the BGT and Metropolitan Readiness Tests ............................... BGT Rank Sum Scores on Total Readiness Among Three Experimental Groups; October, 1966 ............... Kruskal Wallis One Way Analysis of Variance H Ratio on BGT Achievement -Ability Scale Among Three Experimental Groups; October, 1966 ........................... vi Page 72 73 74 75 75 76 77 78 79 80 81 Table Page 24. BGT Rank Sum Scores on Total Readiness For Three Experimental Groups; May, 1967 ............... '. . 83 25. Kruskal Wallis One Way Analysis of Variance H Ratio; May, 1967 ............................. 84 Reclaim a child and you reclaim a whole world --- everyone he will meet, everything he will touch Dr. Albert Schweitzer CHAPTER I IN TRODUC TION With the advent of compensatory education the pre-sChool movement has experienced a renaissance and been given a new impetus. Major goals of such programs include providing equality of opportunity for all children and attention has focused on the needs of a group heretofore largely ignored. These are children whom we have variously labelled as "disadvantaged, " "disaffected, " and "culturally deprived. " It is alleged that for them the typical school expe- rience offers minimal satisfactions due to physical, psychological, and/or cultural handicaps outstanding at the time they enter school. Multiple factors seem to conspire to deny them many of the rewarding experiences that their middle ~class counterparts enjoy both within the school environment and in the broader society. Programs initiated to ameliorate handicaps and deficiencies of disaffected children have used varying approaches. It seems natural that one strategy is aimed at preparation for entrance into school and the beginning of a satisfying educational career. Pre-school programs offer one such attempt. Research shows clearly that the first four or five years of a child's life are the period of most rapid growth in physical and mental characteristics and of greatest susceptibility to environmental influ- ences. Consequently, it is in the early years that deprivations are most disastrous in their effects. They can be compensated for only EINII with great difficulty in later years. and then probably not in full. 1 Contemporary aims and objectives of pre-school instruction as expressed in child development literature are often classified into four cate - gories: intellectual, social, emotional and intellectual. Social needs are funda- mental and predominant, but intellectual needs are vital too. Although it is theorized that early school attendance can provide a foundation for significant changes in the disadvantaged child's readiness for beginning school tasks and influence his social growth and emotional development, research has not substantiated these allegations. The need to improve the prognosis for pupils with limited backgrounds is urgent. A study made by the superintendents of fourteen large urban areas revealed that whereas one in ten children in large cities was classified as being disadvantaged in 1950, the ratio in 1960 was one in three. They projected that unless major steps are taken, the figure might well be one in every two pupils in 1970. 3 This group could offer society considerable intellectual 1Educational Policies Commission, "Universal Opportunity for Early Childhood Education, " National Education Association Journal, LV, No. 8 (November, 1966), 8. 2Pauline Sears and Edith Dowley, "Research on Teaching in the Nursery School, " Handbook 9_f Research 9_n_ Teaching (Chicago: Rand McNally and Company, 1963), p. 818. 3Frank Reissman, The Culturally Deprived Child (New York: Harper and Brothers, Inc. , 1962), p. l. potential and leadership. . the greatest reservoir of undiscovered and undeveloped intellectual talent in America is not in the upper or middle class neighborhoods . . . the actual numbers of intellectually bright children in poor homes are in excess of those to be found in relatively few homes of business and professional leaders. 4 Recently the Educational Policies Commission of the National Educa- tion Association passed a resolution recommending that formal schooling begin at age four "for those children who, through economic or social deprivation, may be seriously impeded in their progress through public schools, and conse- quently in their participation in a democratic society. " Federally supported programs inaugurated during the past few years have attempted to accelerate the intellectual and social development of disadvantaged children through pre-school projects. These are usually referred to as "Head Start" programs. Some of these encompass the nine month school year, while others include an eight week summer experience. The extensiveness of these programs is determined by the local community and is usually dependent upon financial resources available. Significance of the Study Much speculation exists regarding the effects of pre -school attendance. 41bid. , p. 6. This study was concerned with evaluating the consequences of varying time periods of pre -kindergarten attendance on the social growth, emotional devel- opment, and school readiness of socially disadvantaged children. Generally researchers have devoted attention to school readiness, and have neglected studies involving motor skills, social growth and emotional development. The paucity of recent research in these areas is gima facie evidence of the need to investigate these dimensions of early childhood education. Far too often comparisons are made in terms of effectiveness of formal school work, to which the contribution of nursery school and kindergarten is minor. Much too seldom are comparisons made in terms of general adjustment, social relations, personal development, and zest and enthusiasm for living --- the areas emphasized in early education. 5 During the past two years approximately 1. 3 million disadvantaged children have been enrolled in pre -school programs in 2, 400 communities. In 1965 the Federal Government spent more than $90 million on this enterprise. This expenditure was repeated last year, and indications are that projects of this nature will be continued and even expanded in the future. 5John B. Anderson, "The Theory of Early Childhood Education, " Early Childhood Education, National Society for the Study of Education Yearbook, Part 1 (Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1947), pp. 96 -97. 6William Brazziel, "Two Years of Head Start," Phi Delta Kappan, XLVIII (March, 1967), 344. It seems safe to predict more changes in the approaches to early education in this country, however, and this includes the primary years of the elementary schools in depressed areas. Even such conservative (but public -spirited) men as John McCone are urging federal outlays of $4 -5 billion per 7year . . . to provide underprivileged children with a good education. Such wholesale adoption of pre-school intervention as a technique to resolve the dilemma of disadvantagement seems premature in view of findings now available. Prominent educators are sounding serious warnings and are concerned that it may become nothing more than a fad. Dr. Martin Deutsch, a pioneer and leading authority in this movement, commented: There is more interest in pre-school programs than is supported by the knowledge we now have. There seems to be a feeling that putting a good deal of money into special education programs will stop the violence in the streets . . . . I just don't think this is the case. It seems appropriate, almost imperative, to review and reevaluate earlier research, and to initiate new studies which will provide additional insights and dimensions. The profusion of literature available on cultural disadvantagement and pre -school programs attest to the relevance and time - liness of this study. The vast sums being spent to enhance educational oppor- tunities for the disadvantaged and to facilitate social, emotional, and academic 7Ibid. , p. 348. 8F red M. Hechinger (ed. ), "Passport to Equality" Pre -School Education Today (New York: Doubleday and Company, Inc. , 1966), pp. 9-10. adjustment through pre -kindergarten programs of varying designs, emphasized the need for this study. The Problem Statement of the Problem. The purpose of this study was to compare the social growth, emotional development, academic readiness, and general readiness of kindergarten pupils with varying amounts of pre -kinde rgarten formal school experience. One group had participated in a nine month pre- school program, the second an eight week summer project, while the third group had no formal school experience prior to kindergarten. Hypothe se 3 Specifically, this study was designed to test eight hypotheses, each of which is stated in the null form: Hypothesis I - There are no differences in social growth at the beginning of kindergarten attendance among pupils who participated in a full year pre -kindergarten program, pupils who participated in a summer pre-kindergarten program, and pupils with no pre -kindergarten participation. Hypothesis H - There are no differences in social growth at the end of kindergarten attendance among pupils who participated in a full year pre- kindergarten program, pupils who participated in a summer pre -kinde rgarten program, and pupils with no pre -kindergarten participation. Hypothesis III - There are no differences in emotional development at the beginning of kindergarten attendance among pupils who participated in a full year pre -kindergarten program, pupils who participated in a summer pre ~kindergarten program, and pupils with no pre ~kindergarten participation. Hypothesis IV - There are no differences in emotional development at the end of kindergarten attendance among pupils who participated in a full year pre -kindergarten program, pupils who participated in a summer pre- kindergarten program, and pupils with no pre -kindergarten participation. Hypothesis V - There are no differences in academic readiness at the beginning of kindergarten attendance among pupils who participated in a full year pre -kindergarten program, pupils who participated in a summer pre- kindergarten program, and pupils with no pre -kindergarten participation. Hmothesis VI - There are no differences in academic readiness at the end of kindergarten attendance among pupils who participated in a full year pre -kindergarten program, pupils who participated in a summer pre -kindergarten program, and pupils with no pre -kindergarten participation. Hypothesis VII - There are no differences in total readiness at the beginning of kindergarten attendance among pupils who participated in a full year pre -kindergarten program, pupils who participated in a summer pre -kindergarten program, and pupils with no pre -kinde rgarten participation. Hypothesis VIII - There are no differences in total readiness at the end of kindergarten attendance among pupils who participated in a full year pre -kindergarten program, pupils who participated in a summer pre -kindergarten program, and pupils with no pre -kindergarten participation. Assumptions The following assumptions underlie this study: 1. That it is of value to know how varying periods of pre-school attendance is related to social growth, emotional development, and readiness of disadvantaged youth. 2. That the quality of the pre-school expe iiences was similar and comparable. 3. That the instruments used were reliable and valid. 4. That the population sample of the study was representative of disadvantaged children in urban areas. Definitions of Terms Culturally Disadvantaged. For purposes of this study a disadvantaged child is defined as one who lacks many of the opportunities and advantages available to most American children, and is, therefore, unable to make satisfactory prOgress in a typical school. 9 These youngsters have a cultural background, but it is one which places them at a disadvantage in the school environment. The culturally deprived or disadvantaged have been described as those living in deprived neighborhoods, generally of average intelligence, . who have only a 50-50 chance to graduate from high school. More than thirty per cent of these children will end up on relief, and over a five or six year period in school their I. Q. scores will steadily decline. 10 The deprivations of this group include a lack of or need for necessities which may be economic, educational, social -psychological, and/ or cultural. In any case there is a common denominator: not enough. Not enough income, information, skills to get along successfully; no precedent for success; insufficiencies of every sort. 11 CompensatorXEducation. This term, coined by President John Fischer of Columbia University, describes efforts to improve educational opportunities and eliminate deficiencies that some pupils bring to the classroom. These 9Reissman, 9p. 31L , p. 36 10Robert J. Havighurst and Thomas E. Moorefield, The Disadvantaifid Industrial Cities, National Society for the Study of Education Yearbook (Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1967), p. 46. 11Association for Supervision and Curriculum Development, "Dis- affected Children and Youth," Educational Leadership, XX, No. 5 (February, 1963), 291. 10 programs are as varied as the needs of the children they serve. Some consist of a single activity. Others encompass a planned sequence of experiences and constitute an entire program. Head Start is one such program. Compensatory education implies that schools shall provide necessary stimuli to make up for differences in pupils' experiences and opportunities. Head Start. An antipoverty pre -school program designed to prepare disadvantaged children for successful entrance into school. It has six major classifications: the education program, health services, social Services, psychological services, nutrition, and a parent participation program. The educational aspect of the program helps children to develop vocabulary and verbal skills to become familiar with school routines, and to want to learn. The program stresses cultural enrichment. Social Growth. Learning to live happily and cooperatively with others is a determinant of social growth. At birth the child has no social concern and is not basically a social being. Social development reflects the child's expanding world: from ties to his mother and family, he reaches out for peer relationships and approval from other significant adults. Cronbach12 posits that children who feel accepted and wanted during pre -school years usually remain trusting as they work out later social relationships. 12Lee J. Cronbach, Educational Psychology (2nd ed.) (New York: Harcourt, Brace and World, 1963), p. 114. ll Piaget distinguished three stages of social development: 1. egocentrism - individual impervious to social stimulation from without. 2. child makes effort to enter into real social communication. 3. reciprocity and mutual respect between persons. The age sequence of these stages probably varies with different socio -economic groups and cultures. Each social class appears to have distinct values, characteristic patterns of behavioral expectation, and differences in child- rearing methods. Evidence indicates that upper-class children pass through the earlier stages more rapidly than disadvantaged children. 13 Socialization is one of the most significant tasks a child must learn. During the pre-school years the child is expected to become aware of others' feelings and to act so that others will also be happy. 14 Social dealings with others, whether they be friendly or combative, involve many techniques and skills much as do the performances of riding a bicycle or tying one's shoes. The technique which a child uses to hold his own involves skills which must be learned. 15 13Ruth Strang, All Introduction £9 Child Study (New York: The Macmillan Company, 1959), p. 293. 14Don C. Dinkmeyer, Child Development: The Emerging Self (New Jersey: Prentice-Hall, Inc. , 1965), p. 152. 15Arthur T. Jersild, Child Development and the Curriculum (New York: Teachers College, Columbia University, 1946), p. 68. 12 Emotional Development. Emotional growth is that personality dimension describing the ability to live comfortably with one's self and his feelings. 16 Actually emotions are easier to describe than to define, and are often used synonymously with such concepts as needs, feelings, and prejudices. Emotions play a significant role in the development of the child. Several contemporary psychologists believe emotion to be antecedent to all experiences and fundamental to them to the extent that all learning is acquired in emotional terms. 17 Emotions are highly individualized. Similar situations may elicit different responses from different children. Emotional development begins with relatively undifferentiated responses at birth which develop into a pattern as the child matures. This pattern is represented in Table 1. As his interests and abilities expand, the child is able to cope with many situations which aroused him at an earlier time. Simulta- neously he becomes vulnerable on new fronts. During pre-school years the child's expression of emotion tends to become less violent and explosive and more refined, subdued, or furtive. 18 Emotional maturity is always relative. 19 16Willard C. Olsen, Child Development (Boston: D. C. Heath and Company, 1959), p. 4. 17DiIrlkmeyer, 9p. 9;}, p. 254. Islersild, 92; _c_i_t_., p. 94. 19Dinkmeyer, 9p. 39, p. 264. 13 TABLE 1 DIAGRAMMATIC REPRESENTATION OF THE DEVELOPMENT OF EMOTIONZO ’ ' Birth Key: I I Excitement . An - Attention for adults Ac - Affection for children [—_' 3 months An - Anger _ ——-1 Amt - Anxiety _ . De - Delight Distress Exo Delight Dg - Disgust Di - Distress 6 months Dpt - Disappointment . El - Elation Pear Disgust Anger Di Exo Delight En - Envy Exo - Excitement ' Fe . Fear - 12 months Ho - Hope Jo - Jealousy Fe Dg An Di Exc De Elation Affection Jo - Joy I - . Sh Shame 18 months Fe Dg An Di Exo De El An Ao ' 24 months . r—1 Fe D3 An 6Di JeExo De Jo El Aa Ac [—- 5 years ' ShAane DgDptAn En Di JeExc Hone Jo El A3 A0 Some researchers posit that basic emotional behavior patterns are established by the age of three. Subsequent to this time changes are the result of specific environmental changes. 20K. M. B. Bridges, Journal o_fGenetic Psychology, xxxvn (1930), 524 and Child Development, 111 (1932), 340. From H. B. English, Dynamics pf Child Development (New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston, Inc. , 1961), p. 114. From Don C. Dinkmeyer, Child Development: The Emerging Self (New Jersey: Prentice -Hall, Inc. , 1965), p. 113. l4 Emotions affect performance of all kinds. It is closely related to the quality of personal development, as well as to language development and to the continuity of the academic learning curve. Its effect on mental growth and on motor coordination has likewise been demonstrated. Adjustment. Characteristics of social growth and emotional devel- opment often overlap and complement each other so as to legislate against delineation. Nevertheless for purposes of this study it was necessary to make such a division. The writer attempted to synthesize findings in the field of child development and educational psychology, and on the basis of what appeared to be a consensus of leading authorities made the following classifications which is intended to include only diminsions which were pertinent to this study. Characteristics of Social Growth Con side ration Sociability Dependability Independence - Goal Direction Dependence Cooperation Achievement Politeness Leadership Characteristics of Emotional Development Self -Confide nce E xploration Em otionality Creativity Interest in Learning Persistence Motivation Jealousy Curiosity Attention - Enthusiasm Seeking 2 1Cecil V. Millard, Growth prpd Development in the Elementary School Years (Boston: D. C. Heath and Company, 1951), p. 208. 15 Thus adjustment is used generically to include components of social growth and emotional development. It describes the person's ability to cope with his environment. Readiness. The concept of readiness represents an educational approach as well as a principle of development. It considers the timeliness of what is to be taught in relation to the child's ability. Some criteria utilized in estimating readiness include age, I. Q. scores, and reading ability. Read- iness, however, is not readiness for a subject or topic; it is readiness for a certain learning experience. 22 Social development and emotional development represent learned behavior for which readiness experience must be planned. Readiness for learning of any type is a complex product of many factors which include psychological and physical maturity. past expe- rience, home background, previous mastery or experiences with success. 23 This is a highly individualized matter and children differ in rate, level of growth. and pattern. For this study academic readiness was limited to reading and arith- metic. Total readiness referred to the dependent variables included in this study: social growth, emotional development, and academic readiness. This was based 22Cronbach, pp. _c__i_t., p. 250. 23Vernon E. Anderson, Principles :‘fll Procedures pf Curriculum Improvement (New York: The Ronald Press, 1965), p. 124. 16 on the organismic view of child development which posits that all changes in humans involve the total organism because the organism is an integrated whole. 24 The Research Desigp Selection of Schools. The schools selected for this research are three of the thirteen elementary schools involved in compensatory education programs in Flint, Michigan. The thirteen inner-city schools are found in an old part of the city and enroll the majority of Negro pupils who attend elementary schools. These schools serve the center of the city and represent the hard core of academic under-achievement and the myriad of other problems which char- acterize inner-city environments. Selection and Testing of Pupils. Ninety pupils were selected for this research from five kindergarten classrooms in the three schools. They were matched on the basis of age, sex, race, and birthplace, and divided into three groups using length of the pre -school experience as the independent variable. Experimental Group I - pupils who participated in a nine month pre -school program. 24Olsen, pp. 933., pp. 209-211. 17 Experimental Group II - pupils who participated in a summer pre-school program. (Eight weeks) Control Group - pupils with no pre-school experience. All pupils were tested on the Brenner Gestalt Test of School Readiness in October of their kindergarten year. A selected random sample of this population (60 pupils) was tested on the same instrument in May of the ensuing year (1967). The Metropolitan Readiness Test was administered to all pupils in May. Simul- taneously teachers completed the Operation Head Start Behavior Inventory on each pupil. Collection of Data. To test the hypotheses posed by the study the following instrumentation was used: To test Hypothesis 1, October test scores on the Brenner Gestalt Test, Social -Emotional Behavior Scale, were analyzed separately. Then the sum of the scores was analyzed. Components of this scale were: consideration, dependability, goal direction, and cooperation. T 0 test Hypothesis II, May test scores on the Brenner Gestalt Test, Social -Emotional Behavior Scale, were analyzed separately on the components enumerated in the previous hypothesis; then the sum of the scores was analyzed. Separate scores were analyzed on dimensions of the Operation Head Start Behavior Inventory which included: cooperation, independence, achievement, and leadership. Then the sum of the scores was analyzed. 18 To test Hypothesis III, October test scores on selected components of the Brenner Gestalt Test, Social -Emotional Behavior Scale, were analyzed sep- arately. Then the sum of the scores was analyzed. Components were: emotion- ality, self-confidence, interest in learning, and motivation. To test Hypothesis IV, May test scores on the Brenner Gestalt Test. Social -Emotional Behavior Scale. were analyzed separately on the components enumerated in the previous hypothesis; then the sum of the scores was analyzed. Separate scores were analyzed on dimensions of the Operation Head Start Behavior Inventory which included: enthusiasm, persistence. emotionality. self-confidence, and jealousy. To test Hypothesis V, October total test scores on the School Read- iness Evaluation scale of the Brenner Gestalt Test were analyzed. To test Hypothesis VI, May test scores on the School Readiness Evaluation scale of the Brenner Gestalt Test were analyzed. Total scores on the Metropolitian Readiness Test were also assessed. To test Hypothesis VII, October test scores on the Brenner Gestalt ISLE}: Achievement -Ability scale were analyzed separately; then the sum of the scores was analyzed. Components of this scale were: persistence, dependence, thinking ability, reading and number achievement, hand -eye coordination. adjustment to environment and methodical ability. To test Hypothesis VIII, May test scores on the sum of the scales in 19 the Brenner Gestalt Test (Social -Emotional Behavior Scale, School Readiness Evaluation Scale and Achievement -Ability Scale) were analyzed to determine general readiness. The Instruments Used. The Anton Brenner Developmental Gestalt Test of School Readiness, a test based on principles of developmental and Gestalt psychology, was used to assess readiness and also as a predictive measure of social growth and emotional development. It consists of five tasks which require one or more perceptual responses. This test has been standardized and correlated with teachers' judgments of children's achievements, abilities, and maturities. Also, it has been correlated with many standardized tests including the Gates Reading Test. Stanford -Binet Intellegence Scale, and Metropolitan Readiness Tests, It appears that this is a valid and reliable measure of school readiness and can be used in place of any of the above tests for making this kind of determination. The Metropolitan Readiness Tests measure aspects of readiness including perceptual ability, attention span, linguistic maturity, and ability to follow directions. There are six subscales: word meaning, listening, matching skills, alphabet recognition, number concepts and copying. The score on each subtest is the number of items correct. Readiness scores can be translated into letter ratings and percentile ranks. The Operation Head Start Behavior Inventory is a teacher rating 20 instrument which estimates pupils adjustment to school environment. The total rating is a composite of teacher's judgements on sixteen characteristics which are divided into nine behavior categories: I II Sociability Independence Cooperation Dependence Polite ne 8 5 IV V Persistence Emotionality VII VIII Jealousy Achievement Attention -See king III Curiosity Enthusiasm Exploration Creativity VI Self ~Confidence IX Le ade rship This instrument was developed by the Research Division of the Office of Economic Opportunity. No norms are yet available. Methods of Research. The Kruskal Wallis One Way Analysis of Variance was used to analyze data from the Brenner Gestalt Test, the Metropolitan Readiness Tests, and the Operation Head Start Behavior Inventory. This nonparametric procedure was used to test the null hypothesis that k (number) independent samples come from identical populations against the alternative that the means of these populations are not always equal. Scores are converted into ranks rather than simply dichotomized as above and below 21 the median. Plan of Presentation The review of research and related literature pertinent to the problem is considered in Chapter 11. Chapter III outlines the research design and in- cludes information regarding the sample with whom the investigator worked, data collection procedures. and methods of research. The instrumentation in the study is described. Data analysis are included in the subsequent chapter; and the summary of findings, conclusions, and recommendations are in the final chapter. CHAPTER II REVIEW OF RESEARCH AND RELATED LITERATURE Although pre -school education is not a new phenonema in American education, it was ignored for several decades. In 1965, the Head Start Project focused national attention on early school intervention as a possible way to reduce the effects of cultural deprivation on learning and socialization. For many years research findings in child development have pointed out the impor- tance of the pre-school years. Since school failure and personality maladjust- ment probably begin at this time, the pre-school level would seem a most appropriate time to intervene. If we in education begin our work with three and four year olds, with nursery school classes, we have a powerful chance to be influential in canceling out deprivations that will otherwise affect every aspect of their lives. If we wait until they reach third grade, our prognosis is drastically diminished. 1 The period of greatest research concentration in pre-school education was 1925-1940. During this time the primary concern was intellectual devel- opment and the research attempted to determine whether pre -school attendance could accelerate mental growth. Studies to determine the relationship between 1Francis Keppel, "In The Battle For Desegregation: What are The Flanking Skirmishes? What Is The Fundamental Struggle7", Ph_i Delta Kappan, XLVI (September, 1964), 5. 23 pre-school attendance and other aspects of child growth and development are conspiciously absent in the literature between 1940 and 1960. The few studies that were made in this interim concentrated on subjects from the middle socio- economic strata. Only recently have researchers focused on the sub-culture of the disadvantaged. The paucity of current research is prima facie evidence of the need to investigate the several dimensions of pre-school education and the impact on the problems of disadvantagement. Studies made prior to 1940 are listed, but not reported in detail. Pre -School Education and The Disadvantaged The idea of pre -school education for children from lower socio- economic groups appears to have its genesis in Rome with Maria Montessori. Educators are rediscovering Montessori and investigating parallels between the pedagogical problems she faced in the slums of Rome and the problems of the culturally dispossessed. Many salient features of the model which Montessori provided for early childhood education are now included in Head Start and other pre-school programs. Studies of reports, proposals and curriculum guides reveal that objectives for pre-school programs for disadvantaged children include the 24 development of . a positive self-image . . an interest in his environment . a wide background of information and first hand experience . the ability to work and play individually and with other children . a sense of respect for others . the ability to be independent and self -sufficient . an increasing attention span . the ability to give and receive affection . . adequate habits of conduct . wholesome attitudes toward schools 2 These goals are compatible with child development literature, and are also related to the investigation. Presently there is a plethora of information about disadvantaged children. Most of the studies have delineated characteristics thought to be unique to this group and have focused on weaknesses, limitations and deficits. Reissman's3 work is an exception; it concentrated on strengths. Because the child's early environment and family background affect his future adjustment and success, it seemed appropriate to ferret out findings on the aspects of this environment which have implications for the questions posed in this investigation. The Frame of Reference. Studies of home and family life of the disadvantaged suggest low formal educational levels and limited vocational and 2Language Prpgrams f9}: t_l_1_e_: Disadvantaged, "Report of the National Council of English Teachers, 1965, pp. 45 -46. 3Reissman, pp. pi_t_. , 25 economic competence. The home provides few playthings and reading materials, thus a meager experiental background results. Although the family may be prematurely broken by divorce, dese rtion, and/or death, several parent substitutes share the household. Overcrowded and inadequate housing are not uncommon. Bloom estimated that the "long term over-all effect of living in a culturally deprived as against a culturally abundant environment" can be twenty I. Q. points. 4 He spaced the differences as follows: from birth to four years of age, ten 1. Q. points; from four to eight years of age, six I. Q. units; from eight to seventeen, four I. Q. units. The implications are obvious. In terms of real or potential readiness and retardation. the most serious harm is already done by the time the child reaches the age of four. Thus environment. not heredity, puts a ceiling on his potential. Bloom favors a good antecedent experience to compensate for the inadequacies of this background. Goldfarb stated that the absence of early stimulation . . . appears to result in a paucity of feelings and emotional responses . . . and may produce, firstly, a complex emotional existence revolving about the core feelings of not being wanted and, secondly, social responses in which the child directs toward others the hostility that he has himself experienced. 5 4Benjamin Bloom, Allison Dance and Robert Hess, Compensatcp'y Education f_o_r Cultural Deprivation (New York: Holt Reinhart and Winston, Inc. , 1965), 84. 5William Goldfarb, 'Infant Rearing and Problem Behavior. " American Journal pf Orthopsychiatgy, XIII (January, 1943), (AMS Reprint), 249-265. 26 From his study of early social environment on school adaptation, Deutsch concluded that lower class pupils are not likely to be indoctrinated at an early age regarding the importance of school achievement and adjustment. He identified several differences between the lower and middle class environments which tend to handicap the former in his effort to adjust and succeed in school. Deutsch recommends pre-school experience to provide the "socio -cognitive preparation" necessary for the school experience. He posits that it offers a transition between the home and the school. . . . there are certain possibilities for social intervention on the child focused level that may open individual escape hatches and that might require minimal structural and process change in the current school operation. The most important of these areas of social intervention, and one that comes least into conflict with existing institutionalized barricades to change, would be that of an intensive, highly focused, pre-school training program. Voluminous literature in the field of child development confirms the fact that pre-school years constitute a critical and important period of devel- opment. Researchers now theorize that good pre -school programs can achieve the following: . . . antecedent preparation for school; enrich developmental areas which are functional and operative in school learning; foster acquisition of basic skills; interpret appropriate behavior for coping with school and 61pm. , p. 10. 27 society; help develop a positive self-concept. 7 Pre -School Compensatory Education Programs Much descriptive information is available regarding many pre-school compensatory education programs, but most projects are in the "progress report" stage and results to date have been more suggestive than conclusive. Pre-school programs for the disadvantaged are shaped largely by assumptions about characteristics of this group which underlie their inferior academic performance. Cognitive and affective deficiencies are assumed to exist which include: "language facility, constriction in dealing with symbolic and abstract ideas, narrowness of outlook because of the narrowness of the familiar environment, passivity and lack of curiosity, low self-esteem, and lack of motivation for achievement. "8 Reports about these programs revealed that other than differences of quality, principal differences among them have to do with duration, personnel, finance and cost, academic emphasis and parent participation. Getzels9 has described program differences in terms of the different underlying assumptions 7L. F. Ezekiel, 'Changes in Egocentricity of Nursery School Children, " Child Development, 11 (1939), 74-75. 8Clay V. Brittain, "Pre -School Programs for Culturally Deprived Children," Children, XIII, No. 4 (July-August, 1966), 130. ‘9 . Ibld. , p. 133. 28 which the programs reflect about the deficiencies of culturally disadvantaged children. He suggested three broad categories: 1. Programs based on the assumption that differences between the culturally deprived child and the middle class child are matters of degree rather than kind. These programs would aim to provide supplementary experience. 2. Programs based on the assumption that what culturally deprived children mainly lack is familiarity with school related objects and activities. These programs would aim to provide academic - preparatory pre-school experience. 3. Programs based on the assumption that the culturally deprived child differs fundamentally from the middle class child in self- concept, language, values, and perceptual processes. The aim here would be to provide compensatory experience sufficient to modify environmental effects. Programs vary in length from a few weeks or months to several years. No data was available regarding the number of full year programs now operative. Summer programs of the kind generated by Project Head Start are now found in approximately 2, 400 communities. 10 The amount of time the child spends in the program varies from sessions of two and one -half hours a week to sessions 10Brazziel, pp. gt: , p. 344. 29 of five hours a day for four or five days a week. Pre-school programs use more volunteers than other compensatory education programs. 11 Many are staffed almost entirely by volunteers. The National Council of Jewish Women operate programs in about forty communities on a volunteer basis. 12 Government, foundation supported projects, and school related programs use a concentration of paid professional personnel. Programs of this kind are very expensive. Comparative data reported in the Philadelphia project are significant. In one program, all teachers had at least one year of experience in teaching deprived children; in two others the staff included inexperienced teachers. Differences favoring the experimental group were clearly evident in the first program. In the Peabody Early Training Project staff assignment considered sex and ethic group hoping to provide children with appropriate role models. 13 Sources of finance include volunteer contributions, foundation grants, local school district funds and government financing. It is predicted that pre- school program 3 for the disadvantaged will become more numerous in the next 11Edmund Gordon and Daxey Wilkerson, Compensatory Education 3): Disadvantpged (New York: College Entrance Examination Board. 1966). p. 49. 1211nd. , p. 271. 13Susan W. Gray and R. A. Klaus, "An Experimental Pre -School Program for the Culturally Deprived Children, " Child Develppment. XXXVI, No. 4 (December, 1965), 887-898. 30 few years and that Federal money will provide a powerful stimulus to their development. 14 The academic emphasis, if any exists, varies widely. Deutsch's project in New York 15 probably the best known of the pre-school programs, utilized a basic nursery school curriculum and added a variety of enrichment techniques. These focused on the areas of cognitive functioning, memory training, language development and motivation. Olson and Larson16 focused on providing an abundance of field trips and language experiences for children. They assumed that increasing the breadth of experience would help the child accumulate concepts necessary for successful school achievement. The Baltimore Early Admissions Projectl7 appears to be based upon individual as well as cultural differences. Particular attention is devoted to language development and the development of self -concept. l4Frost, pp. pip. , p. 187. 15Martin Deutsch, "Nursery Education: The Influence of Social Programming on Early Development, " Journal p_f Nursery Education, XIX, No. 3 (April, 1963) 16James L. Olson and Richard Larson, "A Pilot Study Evaluating One Method of Teaching Culturally Deprived Kindergarten Children, " (Racine, Wisconsin, August, 1962) (Mimeographed.) 17Baltimore Public Schools, An Early Admissions Project: Progress Report, 1963-1964 (Baltimore, Maryland, 1964). 31 Many pre -school compensatory education programs involve the home and parent. This emphasis is not incidental. The possibility of altering the environment is assumed to be greater if parents participate. The Ypsilanti Project18 uses a unique method of extending the school program into the home. The teacher goes into the home every week for two hours and works with parent and child. Many programs provide for parent discussion groups, parent education on matters of child guidance and individual parent meetings with professional psychiatric personnel. Programs differ in (1) the degree of parental commitment prerequisite to the chfld's enrollment in the program, (2) the level of parental involvement in the educational processes of the program, and (3) the means of enlisting and encouraging parent involvement. 19 This probably makes a difference in types of children served and attrition rates. The Flint Community School Prpgram. Compensatory education summer pre-school programs were initiated in two Flint elementary schools in 1964. Full year programs, however, have been operative in five elementary schools since 1960. Currently eight schools have full year programs and five schools have summer projects. These programs were originally funded by the 18D. P. Weikart and N. Raden, "Perry Pre -School Progress Report," Ypsilanti Public Schools (Ypsilanti, Michigan, 1964). 19Brittain, pp. 911., p. 134. 32 Flint Board of Education; now they are Federally supported. Thirty children are served each day in each of the pre -kindergarten classes: fifteen in the morning and fifteen in the afternoon. The sessions are two and one half hours in length. During the 1966 -67 school year approximately 550 children were recipients of a pre-school experience. Although the schedule varied among classrooms, a similar pattern of activities was noted in all schools. The program included: 1. Relatively unstructured activities - indoor and outdoor play activities, working with puzzles, flannelboards, and other games. Group activities - "show and tell" time, discussing interests. Work time - developing ideas with crayons, scissors, paste and other mate rials. Snack time - enjoying snacks and developing social courtesies. Rest time - relaxing, sometimes to music. Music - singing and participation in rhythmic activities. Language arts - listening to stories, watching films and other activities which are intended to develop language. Neither formal goals and objectives, nor a course of study have been developed for pre -kindergarten nor kindergarten programs. The kindergarten year seemed to duplicate many of the experiences of the previous year. In 33 both programs the emphasis is purported to focus on socialization and adjustment to school. Most teachers who work in the pre -school program are former kindergarten or primary grade teachers. Their training was in the field of elementary education and neither child development courses nor the philosophy of the nursery school were emphasized. Curriculum offerings of this kind are usually found in home economics departments rather than education departments. Generally the teachers who worked in the pre-school program were enthusiastic and felt the prOgram had merit. Although parents were always welcome to visit the school, no efforts were made to solicit their participation nor to involve them in the pre -school program. Theoretically the community school program, however, does offer activities which would attract the parent to school. Effects of Pre -School Attendance on Social Growth Pre-school attendance usually affords the child his first significant peer group contact. Some of the responses learned at home may be reinforced while others are likely to be punished by peers and/ or teachers. It might be anticipated that the child's social behavior may be changed as a consequence of this experience, and this is one of the goals of pre-school training programs. 34 Early studies in the area of social growth were made by Ezekiel20 (1930), Walsh21 (1931), Hattwickzz (1936), Andrus and Horwitz23 (1938), and Joel24 (1939). Allen and Masling25 (1957), reported better social adjustment following pre -kindergarten attendance. They gave a battery of five sociometric questions to 116 children: thirty-four with pre-school experience, and eighty- two without this background. The two groups were equated in terms of scores on the Vineland Social Maturity Scale, extent of parents' education, age and sex. Although the occupations of the fathers of nursery school participants had more prestige than parents of the other subjects, the data revealed no relation- 20Ezekiel, g): pip. , pp. 74-75. 21Mary E. Walsh, "The Relationship of Nursery School Training to the Development of Certain Personality Traits, "Child Development, 11 (March- December, 1931), 72-73. 22Betta W. Hattwick, "The Influence of Nursery School Attendance Upon the Behavior and Personality of the Pre -School Child, "Journal _o_f Experi- mental Education, V, No. 4 (May, 1937), 260-264. 23Ruth Andrus and E. L. Horwitz, "The Effect of Nursery School Training: Insecurity Feelings, "Child Development, Di, No. 2 (September, 1938), 169-174. 24Walther Joel, "Behavior Maturity of Children of Nursery School Age," Child Development, VII-VIII, No. 3 (September, 1936), 189-200. 25Gregory B. Allen and J. M. Masling, "An Evaluation of the Effects of Nursery School Training on Children in the Kindergarten, First and Second Grades, "Journal pf_ Educational Research, LI, No. 4 (December, 195 7), 285 -296. 35 ships between fathers' occupation and near-sociometric choices received. Computations on the five questions in each of three grades showed that the group with pre-school attendance received the higher mean score in fourteen out of fifteen comparisons. Differences between the two groups were not statistically significant either in kindergarten or first grade. Significant differences, however, were recorded in grade two. Nursery school participants were seen by classmates as having more prestige, being more spontaneous and more intelligent. Vitz26 (1961) based his conclusions on observations of the same children over a period of time. He examined "grown-up" behavior. This was defined as taking an adult role in any way; e. g. , playing mother or father. giving advice in an adult manner, comforting another child. Results indicated that anti-social aggression steadily decreased from the second to the eighth and final week. During the second week of school anti-social aggression was observed 8. 9 per cent of the time. The final week this figure was 5. 6 per cent. "Grown- up" behavior steadily increased from 13. 5 per cent to 18. 5 per cent. 26F. Vitz, "Some Changes in Behavior of Nursery School Children Over a Period of Seven Weeks, " Journal p_f Nursery School Education. XVI. No. 1 (January, 1961), 62-65. 36 Studies by Bonney and Nicholson27 (195 8), and Brown and Hunt28 (1961), did not find that pre -school attendance made any difference in social growth. The former study replicated an earlier study of the effects of pre-school expe- rience on children in later life. Former nursery school pupils who were in kindergarten, first, second or third grade were studied. Findings concluded that sociometric scores of former nursery school pupils were significant at the . 02 level. The second study used 402 children in grades one - six in a lower middle class neighborhood who had attended a nursery school and/ or a kindergarten. None of the differences between children who had and had not attended nursery school were found to be significant either on the basis of the sociometric technique or on the basis of teacher evaluation. The third study in this series used four classes of sixth graders and reported the same results as the previous study. The authors suggested that the quality of the pre—school program may be an important factor in producing differences between children who had attended pre -kindergarten and those who had not. 27Merle E. Bonney and Ertie L. Nicholson, "Comparative School Adjustments of Elementary School Pupils With and Without Pre -School Training," Child Development, XXIX, No. 1 (March, 1958), 125-133. 28A. Brown and R. Hunt, "Relations Between Nursery School Attendance and Teachers' Ratings," Child Development, IX, No. 3 (September, 1938), 293-302. ‘TTT TI‘. 37 Kirk's study29 (1958) focused on mentally retarded children. Using the California Test of Personality, his findings were that children with pre- school experiences were superior in social skills to those without. They had better peer relations during play, were rated by teachers as more socially mature, and were given more positive social choices more often on sociometric tests by peers. Several factors may be responsible for producing differences in the studies cited: differences in training and experience of teachers doing the rating, differences in the later school environment in which the child was observed, and the wide variety of behaviors used by different investigators as measures of social adjustment. Other variables include the home atmosphere and the environment of the nursery school. Schwe rtfeger and Weikert30 attribute the paucity of studies on the social growth of pre -school children to: (I) the availability of suitable instru- ments (usually these are more suited to older children or adults), (2) the disagreement among experts as to what constitutes ideal social behavior, and (3) the difficulty involved in isolating critical experiences within the school's 29$. A. Kirk, Early Education p_f the: Mentally Retarded (U rbana: University of Illinois Press, 1958). 30Jane Schwertfeger and D. Weikert, "The Nature of Pre -School Benefits," Michigan Education Association Journal, XLIV, No. 25 (March, 1967), 18 ~34. 38 jurisdiction. The development of a non-verbal instrument would be of great value. "-Child development researchers allege that social interaction among children increases rapidly after the age of three, that prior to this time children lack both the readiness and social experience. Social development during the pre-school years reflects the child's expanding world. From a close tie to his mother, he expands his relationship to include other members of the family, his peer group, and finally other adults. Many factors determine the child's social behavior and development. ")Although it seems reasonable to conclude that pre -kindergarten attendance strengthens important social skills and behaviors, on the basis of information now available, it is not possible to make unequivocal assertions. Effects of Pre -School Attendance on Emotional Development Studies of the emotional development of pre-school children have used both positive and negative approaches. Some researchers have attempted to determine the positive benefits derived from pre-kindergarten experiences, while others have tried to determine possible deleterious effects. Much of the latter research has come from studies of institutionalized children who have been separated from their parents. Some practitioners have expressed the fear that negative effects of this kind of experience might also result when the child is 39 separated from his mother to attend nursery school. Heinicke31 (1956) compared the behavior of two groups of two -year olds during the period of their first separation from parents. One group of children was placed in a residential nursery situation, the other group in a day nursery. Initially both groups were disturbed over the separation, particularly during the first two days. After this the children in the day nursery showed less concern, while the other group continued to show significantly greater disturbance in a number of areas. When returned to parents after three weeks the latter group displayed severe hostility, demanded a disparate amount of adult attention, increased autoerotic behavior, and some refused to recognize parents. These observations of children in residential programs are consistent with other reports of children who have been institutionalized. Glass32 (1949) studied children two through five years old who attended pre-school while their mothers worked. They were compared with a control group cared for by mothers at home with respect to eating, sleeping, elimination habits and the incidence of problem children. Findings indicated 31C. M. Heinicke, "Some Effects of Separating Two Year Olds from Their Parents, A Comparative Study," Human Relations, IX (1959) 105-176. 32Netta Glass, "Eating, Sleeping and Elimination Habits in Children Attending Day Nurseries and Children Cared For At Home by Mothers, " American Journal p_f Orthpsychiatry, XIX, No. 4 (October, 1949) 697-711. 40 only slight differences between the two groups and the author concluded that there was no evidence to suggest that children in nursery school are more likely to develop emotional problems than children cared for by their mothers. The likelihood that children at the pre-school level can learn to deal with their emotional problems has been indicated by studies conducted in clinical and educational settings. Chittenden33 (1942) found that a selected group of nursery school children showed a decrease in dominative behavior without becoming more inactive or subdued after a training series in which they had a chance to study what happened when dolls used quarrelsome and cooperative methods of handling social problems such as those that arise in the young child's play. Appel34 (1942) found that a successful technique in dealing with fights and quarrels of nursery school children was to try to interpret the wishes and feel- ings exhibited. Since 1956, the Virginia Frank Child Development Center in Chicago has worked with emotionally disturbed pre-school children. Using nursery school activities, trained teachers and social workers attempt to help children cultivate inner resources for responding to frustrations. Work at the 33G. E. Chittenden, "An Experimental Study in Measuring and Modifying Assertive Behavior in Young Children, " Child Development, VII. (Monograph. ) 34M. H. Appel, "Aggressive Behavior of Nursery School Children and Adult Procedures in Dealing With Such Behavior," journal o_f Experimental Educa- tion, IX, No. 2 (September, 1942), 185-199. 41 Center is described in an article entitled, "Problem Pre -Schoolers. "35 Such research indicates that the pre-school child probably has a greater capacity for understanding and using psychology than customarily is assumed in connection with training at school. No indication is given, however, as to how early and how well the child might learn to bring reason to bear in understanding his own emotional behavior. With the expansion of the child's mental operations, motor abilities and social activities, circumstances which arouse his emotion become more and more complex. It becomes more difficult to understand his feelings, and when he shows emotion, the cause may not be understood. Emotional development is always an individual matter. No two children ever react in exactly the same manner or make the same response to the same environmental situation. Factors which influence responses include their temperament, past experiences, and primary reactivity pattern. 36 One of the most difficult areas for research and accurate measurement is the study of emotional development. Perhaps the most considerable amount of re search conducted has used the Children's Manifest Anxiety Scale. It can be used upward from the second grade. Generally studies show that anxiety 35Edith G. Neisser, "Problem Pre -Schoolers, " Parent's Magazine, XLI, No. 1 (March, 1966), 43-45. 36Dinkmeyer, pp. _e_i_t_., p. 265. 42 interferes with performance, 37 although some studies indicated that under certain conditions anxiety facilitated early identification of emotionally disturbed children. In a research project in California, Bowers38 found ten per cent of a group of fourth, fifth, and sixth grade children to be moderately to seriously emotionally handicapped. He used a method based on teacher ratings, peer perceptions and self-perception for in-school screening by teachers. Good mental health is dependent on wholesome emotional development. The goals of early childhood education include promotion of the child's sense of security, self -respect, and other concomitants included in this dimension of personality. The school environment must provide a supportive climate which nurtures individual differences and enhances self-concept. The child must feel respected and valued. . it is not the overt behavior we seek to change but the child's goals and purposes. This necessitates providing a consistent relation- ship of mutual trust and mutual respect to enhance development. 39 Readiness and Pre -School Attendance. The term "readiness" is not found in educational literature prior to 37Ibid. , p. 271. 38E. M. Bowers, Early Identification of Emotionally Disturbed Children in School (Springfield, Illinois: C. C. Thomas, 1960). 3913351. , p. 275. 43 1935. However, some contemporary educational psychologists allege that Pestalozzi had this concept in mind when he wrote: All instruction of man is then only the Art of helping nature to develop her own way; and this Art rests essentially on the relation and harmony between the impressions received by the child and the exact degree of his developed powers. It is also necessary in the impressions that are brought to the child by instruction that there should be a sequence, so that beginning and progress should keep pace with the beginning and progress of the powers to be developed in the child. 40 The term is used by educators to indicate the time when a child is sufficiently advanced mentally, socially, emotionally, and physically, to learn. Much research within the last decade on the effects of pre -kinder- garten attendance on readiness was undertaken by Brenner, the author of the instrument used in this study. Brenner41 (195 7), Brenner and Samuelson42 (1959), and Hoffman43 (1957, 1958), have isolated characteristics of kinder- garten pupils which purport to indicate readiness for first grade. The Brenner- Samuelson study examined early kindergarten behavior in relation to first grade 40Lewis F. Anderson, "How Gertrude Teaches Her Children," Pestalozzi (New York: McGraw Hill Book Company, Inc. , 1931), pp. 57-58. 41Anton Brenner, "Nature and Meaning of Readiness for School, " Merrill-Palmer Quarterl , III, No. 3 (Spring, 1957). 42Anton Brenner and Nancy Samuelson, "Kindergarten Behavior and First Grade Achievement: A Case Study Exploration," Merrill -Palmer Quarterl , V, No. 3 (Spring, 1957), 140. 43Helmut Hoffman, 'Behavior Patterns in Kindergarten and First Grade, " Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, III, No. 3 (Spring, 1957), 136. 44 achievement. Sixteen children in'a private school were ranked by their first grade teacher in terms of functioning in first grade and preparedness for second grade. The five most successful and five least successful children were chosen for case study analysis. The ten children were described in their transactions with objects, verbal symbols, peers, teachers, and adults. Following a six week observation period, the researchers generalized about different styles of child-school relationships that are characteristic of children who are likely to be successful in first grade and of children who are likely to be less successful. A study by Wolff44 in 1965 compared children in four elementary schools who had participated in a Head Start program with classmates who had not. Using the Caldwell Pre ~School Inventory, four measures of social and educational readiness were compared: initial adjustment to class and school routines, behavior toward peers and teachers, speech, work habits and listening habits, and educational attainment. Head Start participants tended to rank high in greater proportions than did the children without this exper- ience. They appeared less frequently at the bottom three deciles than did children who had participated in Head Start. Teachers who had fewer than twenty- five per cent Head Start children in their classes thought the program made no difference. In classes where Head Start participants constituted 44Max Wolff and Annie Stein, "Head Start Six Months Later, " P_h_i Delta Kappan, XLVIII, No. 7 (March, 1967), 349-350. 45 fifty per cent or more of the class, teachers thought the pre-school attendance helped the child's initial adjustment. Henderson45 (1965), Kirk46 (1958), and Weikert47 (1964), found no statistically significant differences between control and experimental groups on reading readiness at the end of kindergarten. Weikart does suggest, however, that after pre -school exposure the disadvantaged child is' "More ready to learn and participate in an educational expe rience."48 Vandeberg‘j‘9 posited that all aspects of language arts, not only reading, need attention in readiness programs. Activities described as essential for language arts readiness were: developing favorable attitude toward the language activities; providing Wide experiences which are fundamental to understanding primary grade learnings; extending concepts through teaching children to classify familiar objects; improving oral 45A. S. Henderson, "1964-1965 Annual Report To the Ford Foundation On the Pre -School and Primary Education Project, " Council f_op Human Services (Harrisburg, Pennsylvania, 1965). (Mimeographed.) 46Kirk, pp. C_it_. 47Weikert, pp. 219 48 Gerald D. Alpern, "The Failure of a Nursery School Enrichment Program for Culturally Disadvantaged Children, "American Journal pf Orthopsy- chiatry, XXXVI, No. 2 (March, 1966), 244-245. 49Ethy1Vandeberg, "Readiness for Language Arts Begins In Kinder- garten, " Elementary School Journal, LIII, No. 8 (April, 1953), 447 -453. 46 language facility, auditory and visual discrimination. Artley50 cited twenty- five studies that support using language arts programs to foster readiness. Koenker's5 1 study demonstrated the effectiveness of a kindergarten arithmetic readiness program. Two elementary schools with morning and afternoon kindergarten sections were used. Experimental and control groups were set up. In the fall an intellegence test and an individual arithmetic read- iness test were administered to all pupils. During the year the experimental group studied the regular program plus an arithmetic readiness program which included number experiences in life -like classroom activities, (examples: measuring heights, room dimensions, with ruler and yardstick; using money). Retesting done in the spring showed that the experimental group gained signif- icantly more in arithmetic than those in the control group. Alpern'ss'2 research reports the failure of a nursery school enrichment program for culturally disadvantaged children. Forty -two four-year old children typical of this population were individually tested by eight psychologists for intelligence (Stanford-Binet Intelligence Scale), and three aspects of readiness 50A. Sterl Artley, "Oral Language Growth and Reading Ability, " Elementary School Journal, LIII, No. 6 (February, 1963), 321-358. 5 1Robert H. Koenker, "Arithmetic Readiness at The Kindergarten Level," Journal p_f Educational Research, XLII, No. 3 (November. 1948), 218-223. 52Alpern, g): 313. 47 (first three scales of Metropolitan Readiness Tests,Form K). On the basis of the initial testing two groups were matched on the basis of sex, intelligence and readiness. One group was assigned at random as the experimental group (E), and the other as the control group (C). The E group attendeda nursery school three times a week over a seven month period. The curriculum was purported to be designed specifically to: (1) increase language skills; (2) develop positive attitudes toward concepts of teacher, learning and school, and (3) increase knowledge of middle class values and experience. Upon termination of the program, all children originally tested who were available were retested by seven psychologists. Statistical analysis of data indicated: (1) no difference in intelligence between groups at the time of the initial or second testing; (2) both groups made significant gains in all three-readiness measures from the initial to the second testing, and (3) there were no significant differences between groups in any of the readiness tests. Further analysis of group differences is scheduled to take place after all pupils have completed at least one year of elementary schooling. Summary Literature in the field of child development generally concludes that the pre-school years are the most opportune time for intervention if behavioral and attitudinal changes are to be made. Abundant evidence makes it clear that a 48 child's early social growth and emotional development influence his later life. Almost daily new research and papers on the disadvantaged child appear. In several instances the writer found that although she was able to ascertain information about studies, actual research was not available. Based on the review of literature it must be concluded that the effects of pre-school attendance on social growth, emotional development and readiness remain moot issues. Limited evidence suggested that certain social skills may be enhanced by a good pre -kindergarten experience. Some studies asserted that emotional development and readiness were facilitated by pre-school attendance; others made no such claims. I The research makes it clear that the effectiveness of pre-school efforts depend on whether or not the program is appropriate to the child's readiness level. Crucial to the whole program is the effectiveness of the teacher. A wide range of behaviors is reported, and teachers vary both in methods used and degree of support given to the child. Behavior learned in the pre -school years tends to persist throughout childhood. A longitudinal study made by Kogan and M03553 verifies this fact. Thus the important role that pre-school programs can play is under scored during this period of growth and development. It is not yet possible to document 53Jerome Kogan and Howard Moss, Birth 3p Maturitpy: A Study 1p Psychological Development (New York: Wiley and Son, 1962) 49 conclusively the impact of pre ~kindergarten education. However, research would lead one to conclude that under certain circumstances, positive effects can result. The time for pre-school experiment is doubly right. That it is needed for the sake of the excluded and deprived is self-evident; but the movement is gathering momentum at offering persuasive evidence that all children can learn - much more, much sooner. What has been introduced as a lifesaving device for those at the bottom of society's ladder may, in time, help to loosen the rigidity of the educa- tional structure as a whole. 54‘Hechinger, pp. _p_i_t_. , p. 12 CHAPTER III PROCEDURES OF THE STUDY Methods of Investigation Selection of the Study Site. In considering possible sites for the study, the writer investigated school districts in and around Flint, Michigan. The use of several school districts in the area was studied. Because of differences in the size and complexion of communities, curriculum construction and school organization it seemed unwise to elect this alternative. It was deemed more appropriate to use one school district. This decision eliminated differences attributable to varying practices among school districts. The following criteria were used in selecting the school system in this study: 1. Pre-school programs of varying lengths in operation in areas labelled "disadvantaged. " 2. Sufficient population in each program to draw matched groups of children from disadvantaged homes. 3. Cooperation of school administrators, teachers, and pupils. 4. Accessibility of study site to the investigator. The Flint Community School District was selected as the site for the 51 study. The community and schools met all necessary criteria. Since pre-school education composes one of the six major segments of compensatory education programs in the Flint Schools, the Research Depart- ment felt that this study would evaluate some aspects of this program and help to answer questions concerning it. The compensatory program of the Flint Public Schools entitled, "Better Tomorrow for the Urban Child" (BTU), is described in Appendix A. The Community. Flint, the second largest city in Michigan, is located in the southeastern part of the state, approximately sixty miles northwest of Detroit, and fifty miles northeast of Lansing. It is the home of Buick Motor Car Company and AC Spark Plug, as well as the major assembly plants of Chevrolet and Fisher Body companies. Many other factories that serve heavy industry are also located here. Originally incorporated as a city in 1855, Flint has a population of about 200, 000. Approximately 82, 000 of these residents are employed by General Motors. The city occupies an area of thirty-three square miles. The area which includes the inner-city schools from the middle to the north end of the community covers some seventy-five blocks. See Appendix B for the location of these schools in the city. Selection of Schools. Thirteen elementary schools are located in this area. Some of them offered eight week summer pre -kindergarten programs; others had full year programs. The type of programs offered in each of these 52 schools is illustrated graphically in Table 2. TABLE 2 PRE -KINDERGARTEN PROGRAMS IN FLINT INNER-CITY ELEMENTARY SCHOOLS Head Start School Full Year Program (Summer Program) 1 X 2 3 X X 4 5 X 6 X X 7 8 9 X X 10 X X 11 12 X 13 X X TOTALS 13 8 5 Five inner-city elementary schools operated summer Head Start pre-school projects. Eight schools provided full year (nine month) pre -kinder- garten programs. Since none of the schools enrolled a sufficient number of children in both programs from which an adequate sample could be drawn, three schools were selected at random. These three schools are in the old part of the city; they represent the 53 hard core of scholastic underachievement, and typify the myriad of problems characteristic of urban depressed areas. 1 Building Characteristics. Sexton reports that school building in underprivileged neighborhoods are poorer; that they have less equipment and poorer standards of maintenance. Observations by the investigator would confirm this statement. There are, however, many reasons. The buildings were old. The mean age of the schools in this study was forty-four years. The build- ings we re well worn and maintenance becomes difficult as indicated by chipped and cracked plaster and peeling paint. The schools were crowded. All of the schools included in this study had at least three portable classrooms. TABLE 3 SCHOOL BUILDINGS BY YEAR OF CONSTRUCTION, AGE, AND PORTABLE UNITS Year of Original Portable School Construction Age Classroom s 1 192 l 46 7 2 19 18 49 3 3 1926 4 1 4 lPatricia Cayo Sexton, Education and Income (New York: The Viking Press, 1961). 54 The Students. Enrollment in these buildings approximates 2150 pupils; almost all pupils are Negro. Although no census of racial composition is recorded in the Flint Schools, the following table represents an estimate arrived at through consensus by several observers in these schools. TABLE 4 STUDENT ENROLLMENT AND RACIAL COMPOSITION OF SCHOOLS IN THE STUDY School Enrollment Percent of Students by Race Negro Caucasian 1 1100 98 02 2 500 100 0 3 550 98 02 TOTAL 2150 (more than) 98 (less than) 02 The Staff. Each school was staffed with a complement of professional educators, including the principal, reading specialists, arithmetic specialists, a social worker, a community school director, home counselor, and an instruc - tional materials specialist. In addition, all consultant and diagnostic services of the Flint Board of Education were available. Selection of Pupils. Pupils in the kindergartens of the three selected schools were classified according to the extent of their pre -kindergarten expe- riences. Those who had attended the full year program were placed in Group I; 55 those who had participated in the eight week Head Start Project were in Group II; and those who had not participated in any pre -kindergarten experience were in Group IH. The Research Office of the Flint Community Schools, kindergarten teachers and office records were used to classify pupils during September, 1966. From these lists, ninety pupils were selected for the study, thirty from each of the three groups. Pupils were matched using the following criteria: (1) birthdate (born in same month and year), (2) birthplace (all were born in Flint), (3) sex, and (4) race (Negro). Birthdate and sex were chosen in an attempt to reduce variability within the populations. Children of the same age tend to be ready for similar experiences at the same time. 2 However. research indicates that girls mature more rapidly than boys. 3 Lee's study4 suggested the variable of birthplace. He found that intelligence test scores improved significantly and steadily with length of residence in a northern city (Philadelphia). Thus, to eliminate the possibility of regional handicaps, this was included as a control variable. Racial matching was coincidental. Due to the segregated housing pattern that existed in Flint, elementary schools in the inner-city enrolled an overwhelming majority of Negro students. In Table 5, the population of the study is described 2Olson, pp. pip. 3Cronbach, pp. g. 4E. S. Lee, "Negro Intelligence and Selective Migration," American Sociology Review, XVI, No. 2 (March, 1951) 227-233. 56 by the control variables of the study. TABLE 5 AGE AND SEX OF NINETY NEGRO KINDERGARTEN CHILDREN BY EXPERIMENTAL GROUPS Experimental Group Sex Age in Years and Months Total 5 5-1 5-2 5-3 5-4 5-5 5-6 5-8 5-9 5-10 male 1 2 1 2 3 1 1 1 12 Group I female 3 2 3 2 1 2 3 2 18 total 1 5 2 4 2 3 5 4 1 3 30 male 1 3 1 1 2 2 1 1 12 Group 11 female 2 3 4 2 1 l 18 total 2 4 6 1 1 6 4 2 30 male 1 1 1 2 2 3 1 l 12 Group 111 female 1 2 2 1 3 2 3 2 2 18 total 1 l 3 3 3 5 5 4 3 2 30 male 2 3 4 3 3 4 8 4 3 2 36 TOTAL fem ale 2 7 7 5 5 4 8 8 3 5 54 total 4 10 1 l 8 8 8 l6 l2 6 7 9O Included in the study population were thirty-six boys and fifty-four girls ranging in age from five years to five years ten months old. Each exper- imental group had twelve males and eighteen females. Three boys were eliminated from the study. One child was hurt in an accident and one moved during the year. 57 The third member of the group was dropped as counterparts were no longer available. Complete data was gathered from eighty-seven pupils. Collection of the Data Instruments were selected that would measure the specific variables of the study. Test instruments used in the study were: The Anton Brenner Developmental Gestalt Test of School Readiness, the Metropolitan Readiness Tests Form A, and The Operation Head Start Behavior Inventgy. The Anton Brenner Developmental Gestalt Test of School Readiness. This instrument is an elaboration of principles of Gestalt and development psy- chology. The basic principles posit that perceptual and conceptual differentiating ability are the most important factors in personality development and learning from the time of birth. Five sub-tests are included which require one or more perceptual responses. Sub -test I: Number producing Sub-test 11: Number recognition Sub -test HI: Ten dot Gestalt Sub-test IV: Sentence Gestalt Sub-test V: Draw-a-man The test is primarily non -verbal and purports to "tap the potentials of culturally deprived children with language difficulties. " Administration takes approximately 4-8 minutes for each child. A simple numerical scoring system is used. Test scores have various meanings depending on when the test was administered. Brenner Gestalt Test scores are interpreted differently if testing occurs during the first part of the 58 kindergarten experience, or toward the end of that period. Low scores (0 -24 in October, 0-54 in May) suggest that the child is maladjusted to school and is unready. Probably they are also unhappy. High scores (55-80 in October, 70- 80 in May) may indicate a rapidly and early maturing, well -adjusted child. The Achievement -Ability sub-scale was completed by the teacher. Eight items were assessed: persistence, independence, thinking ability, reading and number achievement, motor coordination, method of working, and environ- mental adjustment. This rating evaluated functional ability at the time and was predictive of potential performance. The Social -Emotiona1 Behavior Scale was also completed by the teacher. Categories included were: considerateness, dependability, self -confidence, emotionality, interest in learning, motivation, and cooperation. This scale provided insights into several aspects of the child's personality composition. Perceptual and conceptual differentiation have been found to correlate highly with social and emotional behavior in school situations. Consequently the Brenner Gestalt Test in addition to being predictive of reading and number read- iness may be considered as a measure of general social readiness, and predictive, to some extent, of future success or failure in situations involving social and emotional behavior. Studies of reliability and validity have included all socio -economic strata. Studies indicate that the Brenner Gestalt and its sub-tests have signif- icantly high reliabilities. Accuracy of discrimination and prediction of the test 59 was examined through the use of two different criteria measures. The range of accurate predictions on the Achievement -Ability Scale and the Social —Emotional Behavior Scale were from sixty -one to ninety-three per cent. Norms are based on Michigan kindergarten pupils with an age range from four years nine months through six years, ten months. The rationale for this instrument as stated by the author posits: Life, growth, development, maturation, personality formation and readiness for tasks are characterized by continuous formation and change of forms and structures (Gestalten) in the self and of the external milieu. These processes occur during the entire life cycle of an individual. There is constant interaction between an organism and its environment. Through such processes sensory organs develop and increase their functionality; so do thinking, emotionality, social behavior, needs, interests, experiences, values, and abilities to cope with reality and the tasks of living. These processes and experiences lead to increasing differentiations within the organism. The increase of organismic structure and differentiation modifies and improves organismic functions; it heightens the capacity for input and output --- learning and performing. This occurs in the child's physical, mental, emotional and social development as well as in specific skills and achievements. A copy of the Brenner Gestalt Test is found in Appendix C. The Metropolitan Readiness Tests. Designed for use at the end of kindergarten or the beginning of first grade, these tests measured the extent to 5Anton Brenner, The Anton Brenner Developmental Gestalt Test of School Readiness Manual, Department of Education and Psychology (Detroit Michigan: The Merrill-Palmer Institute, 1964), p. 6. 60 which school beginners have developed in several skills and abilities that con- tribute to readiness for first grade instruction. Six tests are included which sample traits, knowledge and skills that are important to success in handling the formal instruction of the typical first grade program. The sub-tests are as follows: Test I : Word Meaning - range of vocabulary and verbal concepts. Test 11 : Listening - comprehension of phrases and sentences, sustained attention, capacity for inference. Test 111 : Matching - visual perception of similarities and differences in word forms and figures. Test IV : Numbers - number maturity - vocabulary, counting, recognition of written numerals, interpreting number symbols, simple numerical problems. Test VI : Copying - a combination of visual perception and motor 0 control that is important in learning to write. Scoring required comparison of the pupil's responses with the correct responses marked on a key. Total scores may be converted to a percentile rank or to a letter rating from A to E. This is the simplest mode of interpretation of scores, and the one most widely used. Table 6 describes the scale. 61 TABLE 6 LETTER RATINGS AND READINESS STATUS CORRESPONDING TO VARIOUS RANGES OF TOTAL SCORES Score Letter Readiness Range Rating Status Significance Above 76 A Superior Apparently well equipped for first grade work. Enriched opportunities should be offered in line with abilities indicate. 64-76 B High Normal Good prospects for success in first grade work provided other indications, such as health, emotional factors etc. , are consistent. 45 -63 C Average Likely to succeed in first grade work. Careful study should be made of specific strengths and weaknesses and instruction planned accordingly. 24-44 D Low Normal Likely to have difficulty should be assigned to slow section and given more individualized help. Below 24 E Low Chances of difficulty high under ordinary instructional circum- stances. Further readiness work, assignment to slow sections or individualized work is essential. The manual pointed out that efforts to attach significance to sub-test scores were not encouraged. Consequently, only the total test scores were used 62 in this study. The Metropplitan Readiness Test was standardized on a socio -economic group which may have been slightly superior to the national average with respect to median income and average schooling of adults in the communities. However, the effect of such selection was not believed to be of sufficient magnitude so as to impair the norms' usefulness. The test is believed to be both valid and reliable. A copy of this instrument may be found in Appendix D. Operation Head Start Behavior Inventory. This inventory consisted of fifty items divided into nine categories. Within each category the items were evenly divided between positive and negative attributes. Each item was rated on a four point scale from "very much like the child" to "not at all like the child. " Two sets of scores can be derived from this instrument: (1) an overall adjust- ment score, (2) a separate adjustment score for each behavioral category. The nine behavior categories included in this instrument were: sociability, independence, curiosity, persistence, emotionability, self -confidence, jealousy, achievements, and leadership. This instrument was developed by the Research Division of the Office of Economic Opporutnity. The Operation Head Start Behavior Inventory was widely used throughout the United States to evaluate children in Head Start programs. At the time this report was being written, the Michigan State University Head Start Research and Evaluation Center had in its possession about 150 of these inventories to be scored. The Center expeced to have data completed by the end 63 of the summer, 1967. The comments from the personnel there who had worked with this instrument were generally unfavorable. The Assistant Director of the Center remarked that several statements seemed ambigious and some of the terms used could evoke wide interpretation. The writer concurs. Information regarding norms and standardization we re not available. A copy of the instrument is found in Appendix E. Administration of Test Instruments. The Brenner Gestalt Test was administered during the first two weeks of October, 1966, to all pupils included in the study population in individual testing situations at the pupils' respective schools. At each school a quiet, non -distracting setting was provided. All of the testing was done by the investigator. At this time the investigator asked kindergarten teachers to complete the Achievement -Ability Scale and the Social -Emotional Behavior Scale. They were completed by teachers during the last two weeks in October. All data required by and related to the Brenner Gestalt Test were completed by October 30. Between May 17-29, of the ensuing year, the Brenner Gestalt Test was administered individually to sixty pupils who we re selected at random from the total study population. Table 7 describes this group. Again teachers were asked to assess pupils on the Achievement -Ability Scale and the Social -Emotiona1 Behavior Scale. Data were returned to the investigator by June 2. 64 TABLE 7 AGE AND SEX OF SIXTY NEGRO KINDERGARTEN CHILDREN BY EXPERIMENTAL GROUPS (POST-TEST) Experimental Group Sex Age in Years and Months Total 5-0 5-1 5-2 5-3 5-4 5-6 5-8 5-9 5-10 male 2 1 l 2 l 1 8 Group I female I l 2 1 l 2 2 12 total 1 3 3 2 2 3 2 2 2 20 male 1 1 2 l 1 1 1 8 Group 11 female 2 l 1 2 1 2 12 total 3 l 2 2 3 2 3 2 2 20 male 1 l 1 l 1 1 1 1 8 Group 1H female 1 l 1 l 3 12 total 2 2 2 1 4 1 3 4 l 20 male 2 3 3 3 4 3 3 2 1 24 TOTAL fem ale 4 3 4 2 5 3 5 6 4 36 total 6 6 7 5 9 6 8 8 5 60 Each year in the Flint Public Schools kindergarten teachers in each school administer the Metropolitan Readiness Tests to all pupils. The instrument was administered during the second and third weeks of May at the schools included in this study. Results were made available to the investigator. Teachers re- ported that pupils were tested in groups of ten as per instructions from the Flint Re search Office. 65 The Operation Head Start Behavior Inventopy was completed by kinder- garten teachers during May, 1967. Statistical Analysis The Kruskal Wallis One Way Analysis of Variance. 6 This statistical method was used for analyzing data from the Brenner Gestalt Test, Metropolitian Readiness Test, and the Operation Head Start Behavior Inventory. The Kruskal Wallis is a nonparametric statistic. It tests the null hypothesis that k samples (number of) come from the same or identical pop- ulation with respect to averages. It assumes that the variables under considera- tion have an underlying and continuous distribution. In the computation of the Kruskal Wallis Test, each of the N (number of cases in all samples combined) observations are replaced by ranks. That is, all the scores from all the k samples (number of samples) combined are ranked in a single series. The smallest score is replaced by rank 1, the next by rank 2, and the largest by rank N. N = the total number of independent Observations in the k samples. When this has been done, the sum of the ranks in each sample (column) is found. The Kruskal Wallis test determines whether these sums of ranks are 6Sidney Siegel, Nonparametric Statistics hp: Ell? Behavioral Sciences (New York: McGraw Hill Book Company, Inc. , 1956), pp. 184-193. 66 so disparate that they are not likely to have come from samples which were all drawn from the sam population. If H is true, then the H (statistic used in the Kruskal Wallis Test) is distributed as chi square with df=k-1, if size of samples exceeds 5. When compared with the powerful parametric F test, under conditions where assumptions associated with the statisitcal model of the F test are met, the Kruskal Wallis has asymptatic efficiency ofg; 95. 5 per cent. The program for this statistical analysis was designed by the Michigan State University Computer Laboratory for the CDC 3600 computer; program 40. 02. This program was utilized in this study. Significance Level Chosen. The five per cent level for acceptance or rejection Of the null hypotheses being investigated was selected as being suffi- ciently rigorous for the conditions of the study. Thus, if the probability was at or less than five times in one hundred that the observed difference or one greater could arise by chance, the hypothesis was rejected; but if the observed difference was of such a magnitude that it or one greater might arise more than five times in one hundred through the operation of chance factors, the null hypothesis of no difference was accepted. CHAPTER IV ANALYSIS OF THE DATA The problem posed in this study was stated in eight hypotheses. Each was analyzed separately and the data presented. Hypothesis I There were no significant differences in social growth in October among pupils who participated in a full year pre -kindergarten program, pupils who participated in a summer pre -kindergarten program, and pupils with no pre -kindergarten participation. On the Brenner Gestalt Test (BGT) the following categories were interpreted as characteristic of social growth: consideration, dependability, goal direction, and cooperation. Rank sum scores for these dimensions are reported in Table 8. TABLE 8 BGT RANK SUM SCORES ON SOCIAL GROWTH DIMENSIONS FOR THREE EXPERIMENTAL GROUPS; OCTOBER, 1966 Sum of Ranks Group I Group II Group III Consideration 1493. 5 1 104 1230. 5 Dependability 1523 1227. 5 1077. 5 Goal -Direction 1328 1277.5 1222- 5 Cooperation 15 18. 5 1272 1037.5 68 Rank sums for Group I were higher on all dimensions than were rank sums for the other two groups. Group II's sum Of ranks was higher than those of Group III in three categories: dependability, goal -direction, and cooperation. In the category labelled consideration, Group H's sum of ranks was lower than Group III's. Raw scores were lowest for Group III on all dimensions with the exception noted. Dimensions of social growth were analyzed by the H Ratio in Table 9. TABLE 9 KRUSKAL WALLIS ONE WAY ANALYSIS OF VARIANCE H RATIO ON BGT SOCIAL GROWTH DIMENSION (OCTOBER, 1966) AMONG THREE EXPERIMENTAL GROUPS Kruskal Wallis Level of Social Growth Scale H Ratio Significance Consideration 4. 5 l Dependability 6. 22 < . 05 Goal -Direction . 4 1 Cooperation 6. 67 <. 05 Significant differences among the three groups were found at greater than the . 05 level on two dimensions of social growth: dependability and cooperation. The null hypothesis was rejected because significant differences were 69 found among the three groups on two of the four scales. These differences favored Experimental Group I (full year pre -kindergarten experience) over both Experimental Group 11 (summer pre -kindergarten program) and Control Group III (no pre-kindergarten education). Further, Experimental Group H scored higher on these two dimensions than Control Group III. Hypothesis II There were no significant differences in social growth in May among pupils who participated in a full year pre -kindergarten program, pupils who participated in a summer pre -kindergarten program, and pupils with no pre -kindergarten participation. Data gathered in May used two instruments: the BGT, and the Operation Head Start Behavior Inventory (OHBI). The same social growth categories of the Bil" tested in Hypothesis I were used. Dimensions included on the ELI-IE were: independence, achievement, leadership and cooperation. Data regarding social growth dimensions are reported in Tables 10, 11, and 12. In Table 10, the raw score data showed a consistent pattern. Group I had the highest sum of ranks for all dimensions. Rank sums for Group II were second highest, and Group III scored lowest on all dimensions related to social growth. A Kruskal Wallis H computed for each social growth category to determine if significant differences existed among the groups was reported in the subsequent Table. 70 TABLE 10 BGT RANK SUM SCORES ON SOCIAL GROWTH DIMENSIONS FOR THREE EXPERIMENTAL GROUPS; MAY, 1967 Sum of Ranks Group I Group II Group IH Consideration 1444. 5 12 12. 5 1 l7 1 Dependability 1434. 5 1260 1132. 5 Goal -Direction 1339 1269. 5 1219. 5 Cooperation 1434. 5 126 l 1 132. 5 TABLE 1 1 KRUSKAL WALLIS ONE WAY ANALYSIS OF VARIANCE H RATIO ON BGT SOCIAL GROWTH DIMENSION (MAY, 1967) AMONG THREE EXPERIMENTAL GROUPS Kruskal Wallis Level of Social Growth Scale H Ratio Significance Consideration 2. 46 Dependability 2. 62 Goal -Direction . 4 l Cooperation 6. 61 <. 05 Cooperation, a dimension on social growth was found to differ 71 significantly at the . 05 level. No significant differences were found for other components of social growth. Data from the Operation Head Start Behavior Inventory related to social growth are described in Tables 12 and 13. TABLE 12 OHBI RANK SUM SCORES ON SOCIAL GROWTH DIMENSIONS FOR THREE EXPERIMENTAL GROUPS; MAY, 1967 Sum of Ranks Group I Group II Group III Independence 1582. 5 129 I. 5 954 Achievement 1524 l 175. 5 l 128. 5 Leadership 1456 1 14 1 1231 Cooperation 1584. 5 1236. 5 1007 Group I had the highest rank sum scores on all dimensions of social growth considered. Rank sum scores for Group II were higher than those for Group IH on all items except leadership. Cooperation, one components of social growth, was measured by both instruments. Comparisons showed rank sum scores were higher for Group I than for the other two groups. 72 TABLE 13 KRUSKAL WALLIS ONE WAY ANALYSIS OF VARIANCE H RATIO ON OHBI SOCIAL GROWTH DIMENSIONS AMONG THREE EXPERIMENTAL GROUPS Kruskal Wallis Level of Social Growth Dimensions H Ratio Significance Cooperation 9. 18 <. 05 Independence 10.81 $7 woon .,, l ’ S a? §\ Wash: gton 2 LONGWAY '— Longellow Stevenson fligninistmtion ‘ Flt/5 l «x W 112. entral . we ‘<\ 4 er Flznt Community Whigrjunzor College t3 ‘ 6;, Pie'rce 7 7a) .r Scott 4’ LAPEER Cglidge Hazowon 7 0e» 1e Oak c 6, COURT .gMz'ller Road \ War, 9 CORUNNA Zimmerman a will ‘32» Southwestern 18'; 9* Cody lLincoln Cummings «093‘ g 0 ‘5 ATHERTON Freeman Neit ercut M‘K‘”! ' APPENDIX C THE ANTON BRENNER DEVELOPMENTAL GESTALT TEST OF SCHOOL READINESS THE ANTON BRENNER DEVELOPMENTAL GESTALT TEST OF SCHOOL READINESS PROTOCOL BOOKLET by Anton Brenner, Ph. D. Published By WES WESTERN PSYCHOLOGICAL SERVICES PUBLISHERS 0 DISTRIBUTORS W 1940 BOX 775. BEVERLY HILLS, CALIFORNIA me . . . g last first initial Test Date: year month hool Teacher Birth Date: year month Grade: K - l - 2 - 3 Chronological strict (circle) Date: year month [dress Tel. No. Man’s Name Father’s Name Father’s Occupation _ I. Nunbor III. Ton Dot Gestalt IV. Sentence Gestalt V. Drow-A-Mon . + — — + — 1. H. Top 1. Head 2. H. Center 2. Eyes-noee-mouth 3. H. Bottom 3. Neck 4. V. Left 4. Trunk 5. V. Center 5. Two arms 6. V. Right 6. Hands and fingers: 7. D. LB 7. Two legs 8. D. R-L 8. Clothes (2 min.) 9. Form 9. Hair and ears Total 10. Proportion lo 2. 4. Total Minus — + I + II + III + IV + V (Zero Scores Are Not Used) Use Table 1 in Manual to convert BGT Total Score to Readmess’ Evaluation. SCHOOL READINESS EVALUATION (Circle) LOW AVERAGE HIGH BGT TOTAL SCORES A -H ACHIEVEMENT-ABILITY SCALE: SCORE VALUE (Letter) I - P SOCIAL-EMOTIONAL BEHAVIOR SCALE: SCORE VALUE (Letter) commendations: Examiner Copyright © I964 by WESTERN PSYCHOLOGICAL SERVICES Not to b- moduend in what. or and without writhn nominIon of mow-lam own". Date READINESS RATING SCALES: ACHIEVEMENT — ABILITY: SOCIAL-EMOTIONAL BEHAVIOR DIRECTIONS: Circle the number most descriptive of the child for each aspect of behavior or trait. A circled “l” is the lowest ratin;I a circled “5” is the highest rating. SCORING: The sum of the circled ratings A-H is the Achievement-Ability Rating. The sum of the circled ratings LP is the Social-Em..- I I I tional Behavior Rating. If circles are connected by lines, a profile is constructed, showing relative values. Ratings are converted in:: values by use of the table below. Scores: 8-13 14-19 20-26 27-33 34-40 Values: Inadequate Fair Average Good Excellent i Letter Values: E D C B A IA-H ACHIEVEMENT-ABILITY SCALE Score Value (Letter) {1 I- P SOCIAL-EMOTIONAL BEHAVIOR SCALE Score Value (Letter) A. Gives up Makes reasonable Persistent and easily efforts absorbed 2 3 5 B. Depends on Needs some help Very independent adults 1 2 3 5 C. Poor thmk' ing 2 Average 3thin' king Superiorsthmkmg' ' l D. Never methodical 2 Usually rgethodical Always n51ethodieal l E. Low reading Average reading High reading achiever 2 achiever achi5ever F. Low number work Average number High number work achiever 2 work aghiever achiever G. Lacks hand-eye Fair hand-eye Good hand-eye coordination coordination coordination 1 2 3 5 H. Poor environmental Aware of environmental Responds and alert to adjustments 2 prolglems environmental problems 1 5 I. Inconsiderate, . Sympathetic Thoughtful, unconcerned considerate l 2 3 5 J. Often irresponsible, Usually responsible, Alwa s responsible. undependable dependable ependable 1 2 , 3 5 __. K. Lacks goal Usually is goal Always highly direction directed goal directed l 2 3 L. L. Lacks self- Is confident High degree of confidence selfleonfidence l 2 3 5 M. Easily upset Fairly stable Copes well with most situations 1 2 3 5 N. Little interest Interest in some High interest in in learning learning areas learning 1 2 3 5 0. Little motivation 2 Fair mgtivation High mgtivation 1 P. Lacks cooperation 2 Average cgoperation High cogperation l ___d’" cl .v L-) s g. / INA». / 1...»... / A? ...... .1 / / s; ‘ 1‘3. / ..r.. 6 / .,... y... // Fred is here APPENDIX D METROPOLITAN READINESS TESTS FORM A Metropolitan Readiness Tests by GERTRUDE H. HILDRETH, Ph.D., NELLIE L. GRIFFITHS, M.A., MARY E. McGAUVRAN, Ed.D. NAME BOY GIRL _DATE OF TESTING Year Month Day TEACHER ___— SCHOOL DATE OF BIRTH Year Month Day CITY COUNTY STATE PUPIL’S AGE Yrs Mos. GRADE—___— NUMBER OF MONTHS KINDERGARTEN TRAINING TEST RAW SCORE I. WORD MEANING 2. LISTENING 3. MATCHING 4_ ALPHABET 6. COPYING Total Score, Tests 1-6 Percentile Rank Letter Rating Readiness status, recommendation, other scores, remarks: ”I all? [f ‘ \ 1 12 13 14 15 1e (A. MIX. A. ./I\ J GO GO 06 ON . WALK WALK WAKL TALK STOP TOPS STOP BTOP .\ £114 %% LE; SS“ SS 55i Rs drab bard darb drab I STREET STREET TREATS STREAM 1 ' -- -~:. “ T... ~‘Tw t“...- ‘v I, _‘ fr“ I'f .:m-.:- i A u‘fik-T'T-irm ”ET-fl - d _A _A park prak park karp 9 winter writer winter winret 1O TELEPHONE TELEPHONE TELEGRAM ELEPHANT * I grad grab yard grad ‘2 5b % * bk ‘3 €45 ® ® 4 Pb PH FL 9“: ys ni rmsc o (D < LC) ---———-—--——--4 umec vphx ta ah i s O u y pn dwrC 8 9 ril— 5 6 6 2 . 4,13 ‘ . . Millie-1 {“9 ,. ST— A». . . ‘_ _ 6 v w A 18 81 4 6 ‘3///////// -. flflfiflflfifififlfl 15 “- c’ .. c U-SPOSTAOE USPOSIAOE c USPOSTAGE ’r.';”..~--;J"MKT"_*“-“ . M“ 2" H.“ .. s". " 17 ”; 432 199 327 540 10 ,ij 10 25 25 C’ 15 55 1 2 24 51 37 7 4 47 25,9:7/ RC? “:34 HA 2 H . 5 r?! Test 6 .. 2k 10 11 12 13 14 This space is to be used for drawing a man. arc-7.. . x - “VF: air-1'» . I’ --—~. ..-_ z . ._. .g‘. \. -_-. _. , . . . . - ' - ‘4‘. I ' '..’~-' in...“ .‘Am. ' ' T ' ’ DRAW A MAN TEST APPENDIX E OPERATION HEAD START BEHAVIOR INVENTORY Emir-if” 3’ OFFICE OF ECONOMIC OPPORTUNITY PROJECT HEAD START BEHAVIOR INVENTORY SUMMER , Child's name Sclrrvgi Tl r Grant No. I Center NO. Class No. Chilri No. Examiner's identification Date I T ""i ' I I l I l I I I I I 1 i 1 1 Present week of INSTRUCTIONS center's operation reSponses to each question. Base your response to every item on your personal observation and experience with the child. | Please indicate as accurately as possible how this child behaves by marking one of the four 1 | I Very Some- Very Not much what little at all like like like like #1 LuLLLLLg I. Is usually carefree; rarely becomes frightened or apprehensrve . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2. Is sympathetic. considerate, and thoughtful toward others . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Is easily distracted by things going on around_i_ri_m . . ....... . . . . . . . . . . . . . Is very suggestible; lets other children boss him around ...... . . . . . . . . . . . 3 4 5. Talks eagerly to adults about his own experiences and what he thinks . . . . . . . . 6 . Is unduly upset or discouraged if he makes a mistake or does not perform well . . . . _ ._.- --.._.._~.._.. . ._ 4 7. Often keeps aloof from Others because he Is uninterested.Suspicious, or bashful. . 8. Defends or praises his own efforts . . . ........ . ..... . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 9. Is confident that he can do what is expected of hrm . . .......... . . . . . . .. TO. ls Iealous; quick to notice and react negatively to kindness and attention bestowed upon other children . . . . . . . . . . . . ........... . . . . . ...... . IT. IS methodical and careful in the tasks that he undertakes ....... . ....... . l2. Is rarely able to influence other ChiLdffflcllflhlfiflqlFEES. or inter_e_s_ts ........ l3. Tries to figure out things for himself before asking adults or other children forhelp................ .................... r 14. Greatly prefers the habitual and familiar to the novel and the unfamiliar . . ..... _ _______ l5. Appears to trust in his own abilities . . ....... ' ...... . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . I6. Has little respect for the rights of other children; refuses to wait his turn, usurps toys other chIIdren are playing with, etc. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . l7. Seems disinterested in the general quality Of his performance . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 18. Responds to frustration or disappointment by becoming aggressive or enraged . . . I9. is excessive in seeking the attention Of adults . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 20.5tickswithaiobuntilitisfinished 2I. Goes about his activities with a minimum of assistance from others . . . . . . . . . . 22. Is constricted. inhibited. or timid; needs to be urged before engaging in activities 23. Is even-tempered. imperturbable; is rarely annoyed or cross . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 24. Is reluctant to talk to adults; responds verbally only when urged . . . . . . . . . . . . 25. Works earnestly at his classwork or play; does not take it lightly . . . . . . . . . . . 26. Is often quarrelsome with classmates for minor reasons . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Please continue on reverse side 28. 29. When faced with a difficult task. he either does not attempt it or gives up veryquickly.................. Does not like to be interrupted when engaged in demanding activities. e.g.. puzzles. painting. constructing things ........... . . . . . . . . . . . . . Does not need attention or approval from adults to Sustain him in his work or play 0 l I I I I I I I I I I I I I I Very much like Some- whet like Very Not little at all like like 1). 3i. 32. Welcomes changes and new situations: is venturesome. explores. and generallyenioysnovelty...... ................. Calmly settles difficulties that arise without appeal to adults or others . . . . . . is reluctant to use imagination; tends not to enjoy ”make-believe" games . . . 33. 34. 35. Likes to talk with or socialize with the teacher . . ..... Often will not engage in activities unless strongly encouraged . . . . . . . . . . . ls eager to inform other Children of the experiences he has had . . . . . . . . . . . 36. 37. 38. Emotional response is customarily very strong; over-responds to usual classroom problems. frustrations. and difficulties . . . . . . ls uncooperative in group activities Is usually polite to adults; says "Please.” ”Thank yOU.' 39. fir..- -..._._._~_. ._.- . -._ etc. I O I I I O I I I I Asks many questions for information about things. persons. etc. (Emphasis here should be on questions prompted by genuine curiosity rather than bids for attention.) . . ..................... . . . . . Usually does what adults ask him to do Requires the company of other children; finds it difficult to work or play by himself 42. 43. 44. Responds to frustration or disappointment by becoming sullen.withdrawn. or sulky Demonstrates imaginativeness and creativity In his use of toys and play materials Insists on maintaining his rights. e.g.. will not yield his place at painting. or at the carpentry bench. etc.; insists on getting his turn on the slide or in groupgames,etc...-...o.o.c.... 45. 46. 47. ls wanted as a playmate by other children ..... . ........ . . . . . . . . . . ls lethargic or apathetic; has little energy or drive Has a tendency to discontinue activities after exerting a minimum of effort . . . 48. 49. 50. What he does is often imitated by Other children . . lsgenerallyahappychild I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I Approaches new tasks timidly and without assurance; shrinks from trying new th'ngs I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I DO NOT MARK IN THIS SPACE CAP-HS FORM 87 CI-‘O-II, Page 2 GSA DC 66- 20720 Operation Headstart Behavior Inventory Scoring Key The OHBI has 50 items divided into 9 categories. Within each category the items are evenly divided between positive aflributes and negative attributes. Each item is rated on a h-point scale from "very much like the child" to "not at all like the child." Two sets of scores can be derived from the OHBI: l - an overall adjustment score 2 - a separate adjustment score for each behavioral category The items comprising each category are given below, and are followed by a (+) or a (-) to indicate wnether the item refers to a positive attribute or a negative attribute. (+) items are scored as follows: Very Much Like = b Sanewhat Like - 3 Very Little Like = 2 Not At All Like (-) items are scored as follows: it 5.; Very Much Like = 1 Somewhat Like = 2 Very Little Like = 3 Not At All Like = b POSITIVE NEGATIVE DIMENSION ITEMS (+) ITEMS (~) 1. Sociability, 33 7 Cooperation, 35 16 Politeness. l38 . l2h hS 37I 2. Independence, 13 b Dependence. 21 b0 bh bl 3. Curiosity. . 5 1b Enthusiasm, ‘ 30 22 Exploration, 39 32 Creativity;' h3 b6 POSITION NEGATIVE mansion ' I'I‘Fl‘li'i (+) IT EMS H b. Persistence ll 3 2O 28 S. Emotionality l 26 23 ' 36 31 b2 b8 b9 6. Self—Confidence 9 6 ' IS 18 7. Jealousy, 2 10 Attention Seeking 27 19 8. Achievement ' 8 17 25 3h 29 b7 9. Leadership SO 12 NOTE: In addition to the scores outlined above,two other sets of scores can be derived: 1. A score for each of the 50 items 2. A "Lie" score which is an index of the consistency of Of the §s responses. In order to derive this score five pairs of polar items were selected from the inventory. For each pair agreement or disagreement with both items would indicate an inconsistent or non-discrimdnatory response. In each case where this Occurs a score of (I) will be given. The range of this score wilL.therefore, be from (O) to (S). Utilizing this scoring system an inconsistent or non-discriminating set of responses is related to the magnitude Of this score, i.e., the higher the score the greater the inconsistency of the responses. For each of the five pairs of items the scoring is as follows: , (a) IE the (+) item receives a score of 3 or b, and the (~) item receives a score of l or 2, the response to the pair of items is inconsistent and the score of (1) is given. (b) (C) IF the (+) item receives a score of l or 2, and the (—) item receives a score of 3 or b, the response to the pair of items is inconsistent and a score of (l) is given. ALL OTHER CGiBINATllNS indicate that the response to the pair is consistent, and a score of (O) is given. The 5 pairs of items are as follows: M 35. Lb. 3o. 23. (-) vs 7 vs. h.' vs. 22. vs. 26. vs. 1?. HICHIGRN STRTE UNIV. L B 1|11111111111111111IIWIIIWINIIWI 31293101 3814 IR 111“