saga? *.s I! A .t a ( 0.... Au 54. a: . “ rb .. mun nun Tr. Emu nun I . 4 o v ‘ r- !(EN ARE} 2“ c; 1r ’J' Ea -. .J.‘ 1'. 2. U5 ~ ”1 »;.z .. L"??? f“. 32 s .74 ... .L a Mr... 3! Wu 4...: HI. Ts no “i _. 63 A R; J“ .fin .93 “.0. . an . Fr. Cu 08 gm- A d “F. d1 it“ :2 .1 H. 7? Cu u. L . \ \J: L... ”a." “Ru 2 t. .... 3 “iv A? n .. TC. 5.”)... N V ~ . .u Murmur. .L. i a. 2.”: PM: .. x L? "‘ J & in... i ‘t‘é’fi‘x :-\‘{: -. x U ‘J I K (\st .1 V . lL L-L‘a' run )..._:- l . L BEQRE‘E G if E {a p. “ S .1 mum; MIMI“ an w W «m 111“ 11“ SSSSSS ABSTRACT M‘ or ”P338819! LID MWGI or PSYCHOSOHAIIO SIIITDHS by Judith A. Baseh the purpose of the present stndy was to determine the relationship between psychosomatic symptoms and degree of repression nsing pencil and paper quittionnaires. this relationdnip was previously reported by neyher (1961, 1967). Using post-hypnotic stimulation or a.hypnotically-indneed * conflict to prcdnoe symptoms, he ronnd that as repression progressively weakens, somatic symptoms tend to be replaced by psychological or snbjective ones. ‘1n both investigations, the number or symptoms were ronnd to be inversely correlated with the degree of repression. linety-two introductory psychology stndents were asked to complete a onestionnaire which consisted of the Byrne (1961) Repression-Sensitizatien Scale and a 1h-itam.inventory or psychosomatic symptoms. rho stndy was replicated nsing 32 subjects. Only nonsomatic itemu or the 3-3 Scale which relfected three different degrees or drive representation were used to assess degree or repression. The results showed that there is a significant rela- tionship between the degree of repression and nnmber of somatic symptoms and lend support to the theory that the type of psychopathology is a rnnction or the degree or repression (Reyher, 1961: 1967). /§;L3( {K/Z::>Aé: g ,5: C.» I ‘1“ 6 S” / / DBG‘ 01' HIPRESSiOl LID FREQUEICI Oi" PSYCHOSOHLIlC SEPTOHS by Judith APQBasch L THESIS Submitted to Michigan State University in partial rnlrillment or the reqnirements tor the degree or EASTER or LETS Department of Psychology 1968 LGKNOWEEDGEHEITS The present writer would like to express her sincere thanks and appreciation to her chairman, Dr. Joseph Reyher, without whose ideas, encouragement, and understanding the writing of this paper would not have been possible. i'hanxs also to Dr. Bertram Karon and Dr. John Hurley, committee lembers idiose helprnl suggestions were appreciated. Fellow students who helped in the collection or data were Larry Smyth and Donald Beere. Thanks. to them and especially to Robert i. Bush who helped considerably in the scoring and analysis or the data. -11 AQKNOHLEDGEHENTS . iNTBODUOTiON . . m THOD O O O O TABEE OF CONTENTS Subjects and Materials Procedure . RESULTS . . . . DISCUSSION . . . BlBLlOGRAPHX' . . TABEB 1 . . . . APPENDIX . . . 111 Page 11 somO‘O‘OH 11 13 iNTRODUCTiOH in discussing the concept of psychosomatic disorder, it is customary ror psychoanalytically oriented investigators to dirrerentiate between conversion (hysterical) and vegetative symptoms. Both kinds of symptoms are psyohogenic with the former being symbolic or repressed conflicts and the latter being an indicator or unrelieved emotional tension. It is the vegetative type or symptom, with its medically identiriable physiological loci, that is derined as psycho- somatic. .A psychosomatic disorder is a real physical illness, but it has a.psychological etiology. Two major theoretical issues in psychosomatic research are: (l) the reasons tor a certain individual developing a psychosomatic symptom.and (2) the principles that govern the selection or the organ system.that is rinally arrested. Several hypotheses have been set forth to resolve one or both of these issues. Hendelson, Hirsch, and Hobber (1956) have reviewed some major theoretical models which they have classified in four major types. One or the earliest theories, representative or the 'Personality Profiles” type, was proposed by Dunbar (1935) who attempted to refute the conversion theory or psychoso; matic symptoms and to demonstrate the proposition that certain diseases have a high correlation with certain per- sonality types; that is, certain speciric personality types develop certain diseases. Representing the ”Conrlict Situations and Speciric Responses” type, Alexander (1950) theorized that personality type is not critical in the rormation or a given disorder, but a speciric conrlict situation develops in individuals with varying personalities. Known as the ”speciricity theory”, Alexanderes model mmkes use or the idea that dirrer- ent emotional states have dirrerent patterns or discharge; that is, each psychosomatic disorder results rrom.a speciric constellation of omotions and defenses against thom. Representing the 'rrotective Adaptive Response” type, Holt: (1950) postulated that individuals respond somatically to stress and ccnrlicts or many dirrerent kinds in a rashion that is consistent ror them and determined on a.hereditary basis. Serious disturbance occurs rirst in the bodily organ which is most vulnerable innately. Representing 'Physioletical Regression“ type, Hichaels (l9hh) proposed that the somatic expression or psychological disorder is the result or physiological regression or the adult to an inrantile physiological level. One charac- teristic reatnre or this inrantile mode or runctioning is relatively greater reactivity to stimuli. rhere is a quantitatively:more marked disturbance or homeostasis. The increase in physiological variability, due to the regression, is beyond the narrower limits or the adult, producing a breakdown in function. the preceding theories have their own particular weaknesses when they are put to the test or explaining or conceptualizing the vast amount or experimental and empiri- cal data collected in psyohosomatic research. However, a more basic conceptual problem is their inability to explain the entire continuum or psychopathology rrom.the purely physiological symptoms at one end to purely symbolic or psychic symptoms at the other» the major theories or psychosomatic disorders have been oriented toward the phy- siologieal end or the ecntinuum.or psychopathology (ulcers, colitis, hypertension, etc.) without at the same time being able to explain the more psychic or psychological symptoms (compulsions, obsessions, anxidty, etc.) ror instance, none or these theories accounts tor the apparently whimsical change or symptoms in some patients , both.physical and mental, over time. Most certainly, rather than being into- grative, these theories have served to isolate psychosomatics as a branch separate rrom.the rest or psychopathology when this division has no validity. An attempt at such an integrative theory was made by Reyher (l9dh) as a result or two investigations concerning the posthypnotic stimulation or hypnotically-induced conrlict. 'Decply' hypnotized subjects were given a parmmnesia which generated intense reelings or anger and a destructive act toward a given person. Ehey were to tear up some important papers belonging to this individual after they’were awakened in response to prearranged cues. Observation alone revealed that as subjects became increasingly aware or less repressive h of their hostile impulses, their symptoms seemed to change from those of a somatic nature to others of a psychic or psychological nature. Only a few symbolic symptoms (conversion reactions) were noted. in view of their intrinsic interest, the obtained reactions were placed in categories that seemed to be clinically meaningful. Repression was conceptualised as a continuum.and an objective index of repression of the induced conflict was obtained for each subject. in the two investigations reported by Reyher (1967) the degree of repression was round to correlate .7u and .?8 (.05 and .01 levels of significance) with the relative frequency of somatic symptoms. Correlations of -.68 and -.80 between the degree of repression and number of symptoms were also reported for the two investigations (Reyher, 1967). The latter finding was verified by Perkins (1965). The classification of symptoms is given below: 1. Symptoms characterised by dominance of autonomic system.innervation such as feelings of nausea, gastric distress, headache, tiredness, sleepiness, tachycardia, pressure in head, sweating, flushing, skin disturbances, organ dysfunctions, heaviness, temperature alterations, and such feelings as ”queasy” and 9antsy!. 2. Symptoms dominated by innervation of somatic or muscular nervous systom.such as stiffness, aches, pains, tension, tics, tremors, physical discomfort, etc. 3. Disturbances of affect a. flattening: lack of reeling, apathy. b. Superegc reactions: feelings of beans alone, gbandoned, ashamed, depressed, disgusted, guilty, worried. e. inversion: definite feelings of well being upon recognition of a critical word. d. Alienation: feelings that seem.weird, strange, runny, unreal, unnatural, foreign. h. 'Unspecified distress that cannot be clearly cate- 5. 7. 8. 9. 10. 11. 12. 13. 5 gorized as either physical or emotional in nature, in S*s frame of reference, and are expressed in such conventional terms as being upset, fidgety, jittery, nervous, on edge, restless, bothered, e c. States of emotional agitation that reflect the reaction of the ego to the threat of complete breakdown of repression, such.as feelings of anxiety, fear, apprehension, terror, etc. States of confusion, doubt and disorientation that include statements that one*s thoughts are being pushed or pulled and that the content of thought cannot be specified. ‘Dissociative reactions a. Somatic and ideational delusions, such as limbs feeling detached and paranoid ideas. b. Strong compulsive urges not carried out in behavior, such as wanting to move hands around, scratch at something, etc. o. Compulsive destructive urges acted out in behavior without awareness of relevant hostile or destructive impulses. Disturbance or distortion in perception of the tachistoscopic stimulus. Derivatives of the induced conflict. Conscious correlates of the unconscious hostility, such as feelings of irritation, annoyance, frustration, etc. ‘Delayed awareness of one or both aspects of the conflict. immediate awareness of one aspect of the conflict. immediate and complete awareness of both aspects of the conflict. The theoretical basis for the obtained relationship between the degree of repression and type of psychopathology has been presented elsewhere (Reyher, l96h) and denotes shifting patterns of excitation and inhibition in the Central lervous System.as an impulse is progressively represented over higher levels of cortical integration. The purpose of the present investigation was to verify Reyherfis reported relationship between the number of symptoms and the degree of repression using paper and pencil question- naires. METHOD Subjects Egg Materials linety-two psychology students at Michigan State University in an introductory course were given the Byrne (1961) Repressor-Scnsitiser Scale and a 7h-item.symptom questionnaire. Procedure The Byrne (1961) Repression-Sensitization Scale was rejected for three reasons: (1) it contains many smmatic items: consequently, when correlated with the symptom questionnaire the outcome would represent a simple test- retest of S's tendencies to report sometieowymptoms$h¢2) the R-S Scale indicates the probability of whether a person represses, not the degree of repression or certain drives and impulses; and (3) the experimenters preferred a theoretical rather than an empirical approach to the development of such a scale. ror these reasons, only non-somatic items of the 3-8 Scale were used which could be scored for the degree to which drives or impulses were repressed. The senior investigator scored the items in terms of level of awareness of the impulse or feeling. Three degrees of repression were used: those questions which.were weighted 3 are indicators of impulses on the brink of awareness, whereas those questions weighted 2 or I are progressively remote .6 indicators of impulses. With the use of this scoring systom, a numerical score indicating the degree of repression of an affect or impulse was obtained for each S. Table 1 shows the items from.the Byrne Scale and the 7h-itom questionnaire that were used. insert Table 1 about here. The two questionnaires were scored individually for (l) the total number of somatic symptoms (categories 1 and 2) and (2) the degree of repression (the summation of weighted items). Subjects were then grouped according to the total number of somatic symptoms exhibited, beginning with those So with only one symptomt The types of somatic reactions for each group were recorded. Since both Reyher (1967) and Perkins (1965) reported that the number of symptoms is a function of the degree of repression, it is expected that the ratios between categories 1 and 2 for Se with meny symptoms would tend to be smaller than those ratios for So with few symptoms :;:,~ as repression weakens, .mere symptoms representing category 2 should be added. A replication study was conducted using 32 Se. RESULTS The relationship between degree of repression (R) and the total number of somatic symptoms was tested by obtaining the Spearman rank order correlation between R and the number of somatic symptoms for the 92 So. The obtained correlation of .h3 (significant beyond the .01 level) is consistent with the earlier research reported by Reyher (1967) and Perkins (1965). The replication study indicating a correlation of .57 (significant beyond the .01 level) lends further support to the hypothesis mentioned above. when the ratios were examined, there were no significant trends noticeable although it was difficult to analyse since for Se with 5 symptoms and over, there were no ”new" symptoms. Also, the numbers of symptoms in categories 1 and 2 were unequal and would cause category 1, with a greater number of *gzitoms, to be favored. Finally base rates for these symptoms are unknown and arepresumed to be unequal. DISCUSSIOH The significant correlations found in both the original and replication studies were surprising considering the crudity of the instruments. Investigators who expouse an empirical approach.might criticize the judgemental or subjective element in categorizing the items of the Byrne Scale and the 7h-item.questionnaire in terms of degree of repression: however, the replication group justifies, on empirical grounds, the theoretical frame of reference employed. The favorable outcome of the replication sample also obviates the need for a measure of inter-rater relia- bility although such information is always of interest. A low correlation between raters might merely reflect a differ- ence in their sensitivity to assessing data in terms of degree of repression or as derivatives of unconscious drives and impulses. Our experience with training students in 'free imageryI (Reyher, 1963, 1968: Reyher a Smeltser, 1968) indicates that there are gross individual differences in acquiring this sensitivity to unconscious processes. The results of this study are consistent with the findings of laboratory research involving the posthypnotic stimulation of hypnotically-induced conflict and lend further support to the clinical relevance of hypnotically- induced psychopathology (Perkins, 1965: Reyher, 1962, 1963. 1967). The significance of the results for clinical practice 9 10 is apparent when one considers the possibility that shifts in symptoms may indicate either an increase or decrease of repression. If this is the case, then the clinician may be able to determine whether his treatment is reinforcing or weakening repressive forces. 1. BIBLIOGRAPHY Alexander, 1". Psychosomatic medicine. New York: Norton, 1950. Byrne, D. The repression-sensitization scale: rationale, reliability, and validity. g, 2231,, 1961, 29, 33h-3h9. Dunbar, H. r. Emotions and bodily changes. New York: ‘ Columbia Univ. Press, 1935. Mendelson, M., Hirsch, S., & Webber, C. S. A critical examination of some recent theoretical models in psychosomatic medicine. Psychosom. £133., 1956. 13. 363-373. Michaela, J. J. a psychiatric adventure in comparative pathophysiology of the infant and adult. g. 2251' 919331. 11:51., 191411., 100, 19. Perkins, h. Repression, psychopathology, and drive representation: an experimental hypnotic investi- gation of the management of impulse inhibition. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Michigan State Univer., 1965. ‘ Reyher, J. Posthypnotic stimulation of hypnotically induced conflict in relation to psychosomatic reactions and psychopathology. Psychosom. 1121., 1961, 23, 38h-391. Reyher, J. A paradigm for determining the clinical relevance of hypnotically induced psychopathology. 11 9. 10. 11. 12. 13. 114.. 15. 12 Psychol. £911., 1962, 59, 3144-352. Reyher, J. A reply to Levitt' s cements. Psychol. gull” 1963, 60, 330-332. Reyher, J. Free imagery: an uncovering procedure. ,1. 311.5. Psychol., 1963. 19. ash-1m. Reyher, J. Brain mechanisms, intrapsychic processes and behavior: a theory of hypnosis and psycho- pathology. A_me;_£. J. 311.3. HM” 1961;, 7, 107-119. Reyher, J. Hypnosis in research on psychopathology. In J. E. Gordon (Ed.), Handbook 25: clinical :21 experimental hypnosis. New York: Macmillan Co., 1967. Reyher, J. Hypnosis. in introduction to ggneral psycholog: a self. selection text book. Dubuque, lows: Wm. C. Brown Co., 1968. Reyher, J., a Smeltzer, H. Uncovering preperties of visual imagery and verbal association: a comparative study. 1. abnorm. Psychol., 1968, 73, 218-222. Wolff, H. S. Life stress and bodily disease: a formulation. in Proceedings 25 the association for research in nervous and mental disease. Baltimore, Md.: Williams and Wilkins, 1950, 29. Table 1. items used to measure degree of repression and their respective weights. weight or each item 1 2 3 Byrne R-S Scale Number or item. 105 T 156 T 70 k m9 T as T 1&8 T 155 T 114.0 T 129 i" 12 T 71i-item symptom questionnaire Number of item. h5 T 53 T 6 T 17 33 T 18 T 11 r 73 T 66 T 72 T 6h r h3 T 59 r 13 APPEHDix Seventy-four item.Symptom.Questionnaire This questionnaire consists of some numbered statements. Read each.statoment carefully. if it is true as applied to you, mark T on the answer sheet and if false mark r. r13... answer all statcments as accurately as you can. 1. i do not often feel bothered. 2. l have never been paralysed or had any unusual weakness of any muscle . 3. I find that I must urinate frequently. h. I go to sleep without thoughts or ideas that make me feel guilty. 5. i frequently notice my hand shakes when i try to do something. 6. At times 1 amLon the brink of having a feeling or impulse but am at a loss to know what it is. 7. i hardly ever feel pain in the back of my neck. 8. when something goes wrong, 1 generally feel that 1 mm the blame. 9. i never wake up at night frightened. 10. Sometimes i feel as if i must injure either myself or someone else. 11. i am never ashamed of my thoughts and of the things that i do. 12. i am bothered by a persistent cough. 13. Parts of my body often have feelings like burning, tingling, or crawling. 1h. 1 often notice that my body is tense and 1 have difficulty in relaxing. 15. Once a week or oftener i feel suddenly hot all over without apparent cause. 16. There have been times when i felt like jumping 011 when on a.high place. 1h 17. 18. 19. 20. 21. 22. 23. 2h. 25. 26. 27. 28. 29. 30. 31. 32. 33. 3h. 35. 36. lb 1 often feel as if things were not real. At times i feel as if something dreadful is about to happen. At times i have trouble swallowing. I am.not usually afraid of things or people which i know cannot hurt me. I practically never blush. Sometimes i have strange, unnatural feelings which.are hard to describe. At times i have a strong urge to do something harmful or shocking. At times when things are going particularly well for me, i become suddenly depressed. Often, even though everything is going fine for me, i feel that i donit care about anything. There are very few periods when i am on edge. There are persons who envy my thoughts and ideas and would like to call them.their own. i have never had attacks in which i could not control my movements or speech.but in which.i knew what was going on around me. - i hardly ever notice my heart pounding. There are some people who seem.to have it in for me. Sometimes without any reason or even when things are . goigg wrong, i feel excitedly'happy, 'on tap of the wor d. _ . ‘Hven though i know I do not have arthritis or rheumatism, I often have soreness in some of my joints. I deserve severe punishment for my sins. There are never times when i lose my bearings and am at a loss to know where i am. At times my eyelid twitches for no accountable reason. I feel weak all over much or the time. 37. 38o 39. no. kl. ha. 113. 1+5. h6. 1+7. “.5 e h9. 50. 51. 52. 53- 5h- 55. 16 My thoughts have never raced ahead faster than i could speak theme My mind seems to be divided into two parts which appear to be struggling with one another. Sometimes I have a loss or feeling or numbness in a part of my body. Sometimes 1 break out in a sweat even though it is not hot. i have never had a fainting spell. My sleep is sometimes fitful and disturbed. There have been times in my life when i felt panic or terror without any accountable reason. i sometimes develop hives or raah for no apparent reason. i love my parents dearly and wish that i could live up to their expectations. . i am.neldom.short or breath. I am.nlmost never bothered by pains over the heart or in my chest. i seldom or never have dizzy spells. in the presence of friends and familiar surroundings, i sometimes feel as if the people around me were strangers and the setting unfamiliar. My mouth feels dry much.of the time. i have noticed on occasion that parts of my body have felt detached as if they were not a part creme. There are periods during which.i have abdominal cramps for no apparent reason. 1 cften feel irritated or annoyed without any particular reason for it. At times i have problems with either constipation or diarrhea. I never feel that all my friends and loved ones will abandon me. 56. 57. 58. 59. 60. 61. 62. 63. 65. 65. 66. 67. 68. 69. 70. 71. 72. 73. 7h. 17 I have periods or great restlessness. i feel anxious almost all the time. i am bothered by acid stomach several times a week. I have never had strange and peculiar thoughts. At tunes I become depressed and think that 1 am no good at all. i have little or no trouble with my muscles twitching of jumping. I.hardly ever feel like smashing things. I am easily frightened. I hardly ever become upset without knowing why. There are times that I suddenly become aware that I have been gritting my teeth. At times i feel i lose control over my mind. i feel frustrated much of the time. I often notice that i am fidgety. During sad moments, I never find myself laughing out loud or having the urge to do so. I never get the jitters. i sometimes feel that i am hbcut to go to pieces. There are times when i-don't have any emotions or feelings at all, even though i wish I had. I am.aware of the presence of certain thoughts or ideas which i am.nnable to grasp. i am not bothered by people outside, on streetcars, in stores, etc. watching me. "‘injignluppjg1111mm“ 7915