THE EFFECTS OF SELEBTED FACTORS AND INDIVIDUAL INSTRUCTION WITHIN THE MUSIC CLASSHHOM ON THE PEHFIIRMANCE ACHIEVEMENT BF SEVENTH GRADE BEGINNING INSTRUMENTALISTS Thesis for The Degree 0‘ PL. 3. MICHIGAN STH'IEIIN-IVERSIIT. MINES FRANCIS MCCARIHY 19:2 Michigan State ;- . an University This is to certify that the thesis entitled The Effects of Selected Factors And Individual Instruction Within The Music Classroom On The Performance Achievement of Seventh Grade Beginning Instrumentalists presented by James Francis McCarthy has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for Ph. D. degree in Music /&fi/ Robert G. idnell Major professor Date LL‘ IO ”7" ABSTRACT THE EFFECTS OF SELECTED FACTORS AND INDIVIDUAL INSTRUCTION WITHIN THE MUSIC CLASSROOM ON THE PERFORMANCE ACHIEVEMENT OF SEVENTH GRADE BEGINNING INSTRUMENTALISTS BY James F. McCarthy This study investigated the effect of tutorial instruction within the music classroom on the performance achievement of male and female instrumental beginners relative to their varying levels of I.Q., academic achievement, attitude towards music, musical aptitude, and personal adjustment. The main hypothesis was that beginning instru- mentalists of both sexes who are taught individually within the classroom will earn significantly higher scores on performance achievement than their counterparts under ensemble instruction. It was further hypothesized that students of high 1.0., grade point average, attitude towards music, musical aptitude, and personal adjustment James F. McCarthy will earn significantly higher scores on performance achievement than students of low 1.0., GPA, attitudes towards music, musical aptitude, and personal adjustment. Finally, it was hypothesized that a significant effect on performance achievement would occur as a result of the interaction between the conditions of instruction and the five variables listed above. Erggegure. Two groups of subjects comprising the total enrollment of beginning instrumentalists (less three students with prior instrumental training) from two Lansing, Michigan schools participated in the study. Stu- dents in the experimental group (N = 45) were dispersed about the classroom for the purpose of self-drill, and they exclusively received tutorial instruction and evalua- tion only when they requested it, or when they wished to progress to a succeeding unit (page) in their text. Later, when a student had acquired the cognitive and aural skill necessary for competent self-evaluation, he was allowed to proceed from unit to unit at his own discretion. Students in the control group (N = 45) were taught by the "ensemble" method in which the rate of unit pro- gression is governed for all students by the teacher, and which is characterized by simultaneous and repetitious James F. McCarthy drill. Both groups used the same text, and the period encompassing the study was one school year. Data concerning 1.0., grade point average, attitude towards music, musical aptitude profile, and personal adjustment were obtained for both groups from school records or by pre-testing. Scores on the dependent variable, performance achievement, were obtained by recording each student's performance on the Watkins-Farnum Eerformange.§gg;§, and submitting those recordings to an independent judge. gesults. Correlation coefficients were computed between the subject variables of 1.0., GPA, attitude towards music, musical aptitude profile (MAP), and per- sonal adjustment. The similarities of these relation- ships between the experimental and control groups, the failure of analyses of variance to establish any significance for the small differences between paired means, and the lack of any significant differences when those variables were dichotomized by sex, led to the con- clusion that the subject variables, by themselves, would not account for differences in scores on the dependent variable of performance achievement. James F. McCarthy Analysis of the data pertaining to the dependent variable, performance achievement, yielded the following results: 1) 2) 3) 4) Students who received individual instruction were superior, in terms of performance achievement, to students receiving ensemble instruction. The difference was statistically significant at the .025 level of confidence. The performance achievement for all students of higher levels of I.Q., GPA,.MAP, and personal adjustment was superior to students with lower levels of similar variables. The differences were significant at the .001, .001, .01, and .01 levels of confidence, respectively. The interaction of instructional conditions and l) I.Q., 2) attitude towards music, and 3) personal adjustment was found to be significant at the .025, .05, and .01 levels of confidence, respectively. No statistical significance could be attached to the effect on performance achievement of attitude towards music, sex of the students, or the inter- action of GPA levels and instructional conditions. THE EFFECTS OF SELECTED FACTORS AND INDIVIDUAL INSTRUCTION WITHIN THE MUSIC CLASSROOM ON THE PERFORMANCE ACHIEVEMENT OF SEVENTH GRADE BEGINNING INSTRUMENTALISTS BY James Francis McCarthy A THESIS Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfilhment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Music 1972 To Carol and James ii ACKNOWLEDGMENTS The writer expresses sincere appreciation for the assistance which has been given him by each of the members of his doctoral committee: Dr. Robert Sidnell, Chairman, Dr. Merle Sherburn, Professor Robert Unkefer, and Professor Richard Klausli. Special appreciation is due Dr. Sidnell for his generous sharing of time, effort and experience in bringing this study to its final form. The impact of Professor Richard Klausli's intel- lectual and personal influence on the writer can only be acknowledged; expressions of gratitude are inadequate. Finally, appreciation is due my wife, Carol, for her support and forbearance. iii Chapter TABLE OF CONTENTS I O INTRO DUCT I ON 0 O O I O O O O I O O O I O O O The Problem and Purposes of the Study . . . . . Hypotheses . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Need for the Study . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Definition of Terms . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Basic Assumptions . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Delimitations . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Further Organization of the Study . . . . . . . II. REVIEW OF RELATED LITERATURE . . . . . . . . . Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Types of Programs . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Correspondence Courses . . . . . . . . . . Grouping Plans . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Laboratory Methods and Self-Paced Unit Plans. Current Self-Paced Plans . . . . . . . . . . Programmed and Computerized Instruction . . . Individualizing Music InStruction . . . . . . . Programmed Instruction . . . . . . . . . . . Private vs. Class Instruction . . . . . Individualized Music Instruction . . . . . Subject Variables and Music Achievement . . . . Summary . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . III. DESIGN OF THE STUDY . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Procedures . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Sample . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Subjects . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Method . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Control Treatment . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Experimental Treatment . . . . . . . . . . . Description of Instruments and Method of of Gathering Data . . . . . . . . . . . . Method of Analysis . . . . . . . . . . . . . . iv 18 18 21 21 22 29 33 38 41 41 47 50 53 57 59 59 59 6O 61 61 63 65 71 TABLE OF CONTENTS (cont'd.) Chapter Irv. ANALYSISOFTHEDATA Analysis of Subject Variable . . . . . . . . . Analysis of Subject Variable Data: Sex Differences . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Analysis of Data Relative to Performance Achievement and subject Variables . . . . 1.0. and Performance Achievement . . . . . . . Grade Point Average and Performance Achievement . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Attitude Towards Music and Performance Achievement . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Musical Aptitude and Performance Achievement . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Personal Adjustment and Performance Achievement . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Sex and Performance Achievement . . . . . . . . _ V. SUMMARY, FINDINGS, DISCUSSION, CONCLUSIONS, IMPLICATIONS AND RECOMMENDATIONS . . . . . . . Summary . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Findings . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Discussion . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . The Main Effect of Treatments . . . . . . . . 1.0. and Performance Achievement . . . . . . GPA and Performance Achievement . . . . . . . Attitude Towards Music and Performance Achievement . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Musical Aptitude and Performance Achievement. Personal Adjustment and Performance Achievement . . . . . . . . . . . . . . NOiSe O O O I O O O O O O O I 0 O O O O O O O conCluS ions 0 O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O Implicat ions 0 O O O O O O O O O O O O O 0 O 0 Recommendations . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . BIBLIOGMPHY O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O 0 0 O O 0 APPENDIX A O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O 0 O O O O Page 75 75 78 79 81 84 86 89 91 94 96 96 100 102 102 105 107 107 109 110 110 111 112 113 114 124 LIST OF TABLES Page Means and Standard Deviations of subject Variables for the Experimental and contrOJ- Groups 0 o o o o o o o o o o o o o o o 76 Correlations of Subject Variables: Experi- mental Group 0 O O O O O O I O O C I O 0 0 O I 77 Correlations of Subject Variables: Control Group 0 O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O I O O O 7 7 Performance Achievement Means and Standard Deviations for Experimental and Control Group 0 O O I O O O O O O O I O O O O 0 O O O 80 Correlations between Performance Achieve- ment and Subject Variables . . . . . . . . . . 80 Analysis of Variance of Performance Achievement by Method of Instruction and Level of 1.0. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 83 Analysis of Variance of Performance Achievement by Method of Instruction and Le ve 1 Of GPA O O O O 0 O O O O O O O O O O O O 86 Analysis of Variance of Performance Achievement by Method of Instruction and Level of Attitude Towards Music . . . . . . . 88 Analysis of Variance of Performance Achievement by Method of Instruction and Level of Musical Aptitude . . . . . . . . . . 91 vi LIST OF TABLES (cont'd.) Table Page 4:10. Analysis of Variance of Performance Achievement by Method of Instruction and Level of Personal Adjustment . . . . . . 93 4:11. Analysis of variance of Performance Achievement by Method of Instruction and Sex I O O I O I O O O O O O O O 0 O O O O 94 vii LIST OF FIGURES Page Performance Achievement Means of Experimental and Control Groups at Low, Medium, and High Levels of 1.0. . . . . . . . 82 Performance Achievement Means of Experimental and Control Groups at Low, Medium, and High Levels of Grade Point Average . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 85 Performance Achievement Means of Experimental and Control Groups at Low, Medium, and High Levels of Attitude Towards Mus ic O O O O O O I O O O O O O I I O O O O O 87 Performance Achievement Means of Experimental and Control Groups at Low, Medium, and High Levels of Musical Aptitude O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O 90 Performance Achievement Means of Experimental and Control Groups at Low, Medium, and High Levels of Personal Adjustment 0 O O O O O O I O O O O O O O O O O 92 viii c- CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION One of the most striking characteristics of a classroom is the wide variations in ability and achieve- ment demonstrated by its pupils, even within a group possessing a common history of educational experiences. The scenario of individual differences faced by the class- room teacher each morning is further embellished by the frequent shifts in attitudes and motivations within students. Students not only differ from each other butaflso within themselves from day to day and from situation to situation as well. Consequently, the ”lecture-discussion" method of teaching common to most classrooms can be deemed an efficient manner of broadcasting information only if the processes of reception are ignored. .All too frequently, teachers assume that every child receives sensory data in the same manner. Minor,l working with children who have learning difficulties, found a wide range of sensory preferences, even in children whose complete sensory equipment was intact. For some, kinesthetic experiences were utilized to reinforce visual and aural perception, while others were unable to shift easily from one sensory mode to another. Given the broad categories of hereditary, maturational, environmental, and glandular differences to which recognizable trait variabilities can be ascribed, it would be difficult to imagine the lecture-discussion mode of teaching as a viable system for transferring knowledge. Instructional programs in the United States which attempt to shift the burden of adaptation from the student to the teacher began to be developed in the latter decades of the 19th Century.2 Efforts to relax the rigid con- formity of the traditional lock-step system were given support by the appearance, shortly after the turn of the century, of psychometric tools and techniques which 1Frances Minor, "A Child Goes Forth: Ideas Invite Involvement," Igdiyidualizing Instruction (washington, D.C.: Association for Supervision and Curriculum DeveloP- ment, National Education Association, 1964), p. 58. 2Chester W; Harris, ed., Encyclopedia 2; Edgga- tiggal Research (New York: Macmillan, 1960), p. 222. attempted to quantify the sc0pe of trait variability. The increasing sophistication of these tools has recently established new parameters of human competence and per- formance. It is now clear that students differ widely not only in intelligence but in creativity3 and in at least eighty elements of intellect.4 It has also been demon- strated that these inequalities increase rather than decrease as a student moves through the grades. Among ninth graders, reading and mathematics test scores may range from grade three to college junior levels.5 Yet mass teaching continues, seemingly oblivious to its inappropriateness. The Problem and Purposes of the Study This study was concerned with formulating and evaluating a method of individual instruction which will provide learning conditions compatible with the physio- logical and psychological differences that individuals 3M. A.‘Wallach and N. Kogan, nodes 9; Thinking in xggng Children (New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston, Inc., 1965). 4J. P. Guilford, The Natuge.g;_fluman Intelligence (New York: McGraw Hill, 1967). 5R. M. Thomas and S. M. Thomas, Ingiyidugl Di - fierenges in the Classroom (New York: David McKay Company, Inc., 1965), p. 3. bring to beginning instrumental music classes. More specifically, the purpose of this study is to compare the effects, on the performance achievement of beginning junior high school instrumental music students, of both individual instruction within a class and ensemble instruction of a more traditional nature. Further objec- tives were: 1. to determine the effect of students' intelligence, academic achievement, attitude towards music, musical aptitude, and personal adjustment on their scores for the dependent variable, performance achievement. to examine the differing effects on performance achievement which may occur as a result of the interaction between the two instructional treat- ments and the five subject variables of intel- ligence, academic achievement, attitude towards music, musical aptitude, and personal adjustment. Hypotheses The primary hypothesis of this study is that individual instruction in the experimental classrooms will produce significantly higher scores on performance achievement for beginning instrumentalists than a traditional ensemble method of teaching in the control classes, and that this difference will occur regardless of the subject's sex. In addition, it is hypothesized that students with higher levels of intelligence, scholastic achievement, attitude toward music, musical aptitude, and personal adjustment will attain signifi- cantly higher performance achievement scores than students at lower levels of the same variables. It is further hypothesized that significant interaction effects on performance achievement will be found occurring between types of instruction and levels of the aforementioned subject variables. Investigation of these hypotheses necessitates examination of the following null hypotheses: 1. There is no significant difference in performance achievement between the experimental and control groups. 2. There is no significant difference between males and females within the experimental and control groups, respectively, in performance achievement. 3. There is no significant difference in performance achievement between students of low, medium, and high I.Q. 4. There is no significant difference in performance achievement between students of low, medium, and high grade point average (GPA). 5. There is no significant difference in performance achievement between students of low, medium, and high attitude towards music. 6. There is no significant difference in performance achievement between students of low, medium, and high musical aptitude. 7. There is no significant difference in performance achievement between students of low, medium, and high personal adjustment. 8. There is no significant interaction effect on per- formance achievement between the instructional treatment and each of the subject variables of intelligence, academic achievement, attitude towards music, musical aptitude, and personal adjustment. Need for the Study A fundamental premise of educational principle is that each child should be able to develop from his current level of achievement to another level of competency in a manner suitable to his particular needs. Unfortunately, music classes devoted to instrumental instruction typically demonstrate the inequality engendered by the equal treat- ment of students of unequal ability. The instructional procedures found therein are best described by Froseth as "lock-step, where all students in the class play (rehearse) each exercise until the slower members of the class succeed or until the teacher gives up. No doubt, learning by imitation (i.e., once through to see how it goes, or follow the leader) and other forms of musical dependency and musical illiteracy can be attributed to a lock-step approach to teaching beginning instrumental music."6 The inefficiency of simultaneous, repetitious, identical learning tasks for all students is patently obvious, particularly for those of higher ability. The latter, unable to benefit from the “overlearning” inherent in such drill (see studies by Becker7 and 6James 0. Froseth, "Individualizing Instruction in the Beginning Music Class" (unpublished paper presented at the North-Central Division MENC Convention, Research Section, 1971), p. 2. 7W. R. Becker, "The Effect of Overlearning, Initial Learning Ability and Review upon the Musical Memory of Junior High School Cornet and Trumpet Players" (unpub- lished Ph.D. dissertation, State University of Iowa, 1962). Rubin-Rabson,8 cited in Chapter II), must feel an overwhelming sense of frustration resulting from their entrapment by an instructional procedure virtually designed to waste their time and insure their boredom. Likewise, the student of lower intellectual or musical ability, perhaps already imbued with the expectation of failure, finds the pace of instruction too rigid, the evaluatory systems employed by a teacher ineffectual and inappropriate to his needs. He may experience frustration and seek defense against it by turning away from the initial excitement and aspirations characteristic of most beginners, and may ultimately "tune-out" the source of irritation by dropping out at his earliest Opportunity. 9 1 There is some evidence (Kruth, Bergen, O and 8Grace Rubin-Rabson, "Studies in the Psychology of Memorizing Piano Music VI: A Comparison of Two Forms of Mental Rehearsing and Keyboard Overlearning," Journal.gf Educational Psychology, XXXII (1941), 593-602. 9E. C. Kruth, "Student Dr0p-Out in Instrumental Music in the Secondary Schools at Oakland, California" (unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, Stanford University, 1964). 10 u - Hal Bergen, A Study of Dr0p-Outs in Instru- mental Music in Five Selected High Schools in Michigan" (unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, Michigan State University, 1957). Pruitt,ll cited in Chapter II) that students of the lowest and highest ability comprise the major proportion of drop— outs from instrumental music programs. This study will attempt to prescribe an instructional method which is simple in conception, easily administered, and which will perhaps remove some cause for attrition among beginning students by providing them with instruction tailored to their needs. While the literature contains an abundance of articles deploring the undesirable attitudes engendered in students by harmful teaching techniques, one rarely finds any mention of the frustration and desperation experienced by teachers. Instructors often discover that they are unable to fulfill their best intentions of bringing knowledge and intellectual excitement to children, par- ticularly children far removed from the class, culture, or race of that teacher. In a filmed documentary examination of communication in schools produced by the BBC television network, the too-typical, agonizing conflict between teachers who pursue their instructional goals with 11Jack S. Pruitt, "A Study of Withdrawals in the Beginning Instrumental Music Programs of Selected Schools in the School District of Greenville, South Carolina" (unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, New York University, 1966). 10 sincerity and dedication and students who resist the teacher's efforts with considerable determination and occasional cruelty was described as a "war: sometimes cold, sometimes hot, with undeclared periods of truce or rarer periods of peace, but always--a war."12 This con- flict between teachers and students is presumed to be partially caused by patterns of communication which result from the roles adopted by the participants in the tradi- tional lecture-discussion classroom. Such roles involving the formal dissemination and reception of information are often inimicable to the establishment of productive rela- tionships. If communication is primarily between "the teacher" and "the class,' it is likely that such a rela- tionship will be marked by distance, suSpicion, alienation, and occasional hostility. Conversely, if teaching is carried out on a tutorial basis, the prime mode of com- munication is between two individuals, and their personal relationship may become supportive, thus defusing the source of the conflict between the two roles. 12"Communication," BBC Telecast, May 12, 1972: "The School." 11 Definition of Terms Performance Aghievenent is the ability to perform on the appr0priate musical instrument as measured by the Watkinsfignnnnn_Pengonnange.§nnln.13 The test provides for the measurement of errors in pitch, tempo, rhythm, dura- tion, expression, slurs, rests, pauses, and repeats. Intelligence is measured by the gninrnennon Mental agility Tnnn, Intenmediate.nnynl.l4 (According to the authors, the test has "been designed to provide compre- hensive, carefully articulated assessment of the general mental ability, or scholastic aptitude, of pupils in American schools. Emphasis is placed upon measuring the pupil's facility in reasoning and in dealing abstractly with verbal, symbolic, and figural test content sampling a broad range of cognitive abilities."15 tholastig Annieyement is defined as the success attained by students in scholastic endeavors as evidenced 13John‘Watkins and Stephen Farnum, Tne watkins- ‘Ennnnm.2nn§nnmnnnn Scale (Winona, Minnesota: Hal Leonard Music, Inc., 1965). 14Arthur Otis and Roger Lennon, Otis-Lennon.nennal Ability Tesn, Intennegiate Leyel (New York: Harcourt, Brace and World, Inc., 1967). 15Arthur Otis and Roger Lennon, Otis-Lennon Mental agility Test: Mannal :0; Administration (New York: Harcourt, Brace and‘World, Inc., 1967), p. 4. 12 by their respective grade point averages (GPA). These averages were computed for all curricular courses taken by students prior to the inception of this study, but exclude all grades received during the experimental period. Attitnde Towardsiungin is defined as the cognitive component of the students' affective behavior in music. It is measured by the Thurstone—Chavel6 "method of equal- appearing intervals“ scale developed by Carter.17 Musicnl Antitnde is best defined as general musical abilities which are a product of inherited poten- tial and environmental influences. Since all aptitude tests are to some degree a measure of acquired learning, an aptitude test may be distinguished from an achievement test to the extent that the generalized function of aptitude is maximized, and the specifically learned course-content material is minimized. l6Louis L. Thurstone and E. J. Chave, Tne Mensnnement.nf,5ttitudes (Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1929). 17warrick Carter, "Ethnic Music as a Source for the Musical Development and Enrichment of Culturally Dif- ferent students in General Music Classes" (unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, Michigan State University, 1970). 13 The measure of musical aptitude is the composite score attained by all students on the Musical Aptitude Pnofile (MAP).18 According to the author, the test is designed to "minimize musical achievement so that the most basic factors of musical aptitude--aura1 perception, kinesthetic musical feeling, and musical expression--may be adequately assessed."19 Pensnnal Adjustment is defined as feelings of personal security, and refers to the manner and effective- ness with which the whole individual meets his personal needs. The measure of personal adjustment is the composing score attained by students on the personal adjustment sec- tion of the galifonnia.gn§n.n§ Personality, Intermediate Leyel.20 According to the authors, the test is designed to "detect the areas and specific types of tendencies to think, feel, and act which reveal undesirable individual adjustments."21 18Edwin Gordon, Mnsig Antitnge Profile (Boston: HoughtoneMifflin Co., 1965). l?1§§fl-: "The Musical Aptitude Profile," usi .EQEEQEQ£§.£QE£DQL: 53:6 (February, 1967), 52, 20Louis Thorpe, Willis Clark,, and Ernest Tiegs, Qalifornia Test 2; Personality (Monterey: CTB/McGraw-Hill, 1953). 2J'Louis Thorpe, Willis Clark, and Ernest Tiegs, ‘unnnn_: gnlifonnia Iegt n; Personnlity (Monterey: CTB/McGraw-Hill, 1953), p. 2. l4 Ingiyidnal Instnnctinn is defined as an exclusively tutorial, one-to-one basis of instruction involving a single teacher and student within the experimental classes. It should not be confused with "individualized instruc- tion, which connotates the relatiye degree to which instruction, materials, programs, and curricula, are adapted to groups of varying sizes.* Ensemble Instnnction is defined as instruction given to students within an ensemble comprised of dis- similar wind and percussion instruments. It is charac- terized by identical, simultaneous, and repetitious drill instigated and concluded at the discretion of the instructor. Instruction is usually directed to the whole class, occasionally to a sub-section, e.g., similar instruments, but rarely to an individual student.* Basic Assumptions Conclusions which may be drawn from the analysis of data are based on the following suppositions: 1. Since only those students with no previous train- ing in instrumental music were included in the TA complete description of the individual and ensemble instructional technique used in this study can be found in Chapter III. 15 study, it is assumed that a pre-test on the dependent variable of performance achievement is neither necessary nor appr0priate, and that stu- dents in the experimental and control groups could be equally described as naive and unskilled in terms of performance achievement. 2. It is assumed the indices of performance achieve- ment, intelligence, scholastic achievement, attitude towards music, musical aptitude, and personal adjustment measure satisfactorily what they purport to measure. 3. It is assumed that instruction and the factors of intelligence, scholastic achievement, attitude towards music, musical aptitude, and personal adjustment bear a causal relationship to per- formance achievement. Delimitations Texts. The efficacy and appropriateness of the texts to the experimental and control methods of instruc- tion was not examined. Texts for both groups were identical. Leyel 2; Subjects. This study was concerned exclusively with seventh grade, beginning wind and 16 percussion instrumental students, and generalizations drawn from the analysis of the data will be confined to similar constraints. Intonation. In accordance with the instructions given by the authors of the Watkins-Fannum Scale, evalua- tion of performance achievement did not include measurement of intonation. .Tgng Quality. Although a topic involving con- siderable instructional effort on the part of each teacher, judgments of tone quality were not included in the test of performance achievement. Ensemble Skills. Performance achievement was measured solely on an individual basis and no attempt was made to evaluate or compare the ensemble skills of the experimental or control group. Further Organization of the Study The preceding pages of this chapter have presented a statement, definition and discussion of the problem and its related aspects. The study continues in the following order: Chapter II, a Review of the Related Literature; Chapter III, Design of the Study: Chapter IV, Analysis of I) 17 the Data; and Chapter V, Summary, Findings, Discussion, Conclusions, and Recommendations. CHAPTER II REVIEW OF RELATED LITERATURE Introduction ". . . we are not born all exactly alike but dif- ferent in nature, for all sorts of different jobs."1 Therefore Plato, recognizing the extent of human vari- ability, specified its social implications and anticipated to a remarkable degree our century's use of aptitude batteries by pr0posing tests to measure traits important to the military.2 Comenius also treated individual dif- ferences at length, admonishing teachers to consider their pupils' ages, intelligence and knowledge.3 In the eighteenth century, Rousseau believed the child's education should not be governed by adult 1W. H. D. Rouse, (Translator), Great Dialognes.nn Plato (New York: The New American Library, Mentor Books, 1956), p. 166. 21bid. 3J. A. Komensky, The Analytical Qidactic.nf ‘ggngninn (Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1953), pp. 160-164. 18 l9 interests and activities, and that "the needs and interests of the individual are above those of organized society."4 Pestalozzi insisted that the natural curiosity of a child should provide the motives for learning, and he founded, in the early nineteenth century, a boys' boarding school wherein coercion to learn was not used. Believing that free expression would allow the natural powers of the child to develop, he maintained that a teacher's respon- sibility was to adapt instruction to each individual according to his changing nature as required by the vary- ing stages in his development.5 Thus the recognition of individual differences and the provision of appr0priate individualized instruction-- the problem central to this study--is neither unique to this century nor to this country. In pre-l850 America, formal education was generally available only to a relatively small number of people. Most schools were therefore ungraded, and learning in the one-room school was very much an individual 4s. E. Frost, Jr., History 2; Egncation (WOodbury, New York: Barrows Educational Series, Inc., 1947), p. 139. 5Caroll Atkinson and Eugene Maleska, The Stony n: Egncation (New York: Bantam Books, 1962), p. 79. 20 affair.6 But the subsequent pressure of a rapidly expanding population combined with its concentration in the cities lead to the abandonment of low student-teacher ratios and the adoption of more efficient and uniform "production" techniques. Opposition to uniformity of curriculum and stu- dents was expressed by many educators, including Charles W} Elliot, chairman of the Committee of Ten and former Presi- dent of Harvard University, when he said in 1892: Uniformity is the curse of American schools. That any school or college has a uniform product should be regarded as a demonstration of inferiority, of incapacity to meet the legitimate demands of a social order whose fundamental principle is that every career should be open to talent. Selection of studies for the individual, instruction addressed to the individual . . . , and diversity of product as regard to age and acquisitions must come to characterize the American public school, if it is to answer the pur- poses of a democratic society.7 Many programs proposed before and after Elliot's remarks are referred to as "individualized." Others exhibit the characteristics but do not bear the title. 6Robert G. Scanlon and John Bolvin, Indiyigually gnescnined Instznction (Philadelphia: Research for Better Schools, Inc., 1969), p. 2. 7Charles W. Elliot, "Shortening and Enriching the Grammar School Course," in gnarles Elliot nng Ponnla; Egncagion, ed. by Edward Krug (New York: Teachers College Press, 1961), pp. 55-56. 21 All of these programs constitute a diverse family based on differing interpretations of individualizing, justified by varying theories, influenced by increasingly sophisticated technology, and confounded by the ambiguity of their label. "In fact, the term indiyidualiged instrnctional program is used to describe such a varied assortment of curricula that it is no longer a useful, restrictive category of instructional methods. It likely never was."8 A survey of these programs, even excluding pre— nineteenth century forms and those designated for such specific subject areas as reading, reveals that attempts to individualize instruction have been a facet of American education almost from its inception, and that the renewed interest has taken a bewildering array of forms. Types of Programs gnyyesnondenge gonrses. One of the first programs in this country for individualized instruction was insti- tuted in 1873, taking the form of what is contemporarily called correspondence courses. In 1882 a systematic plan was adopted for the study of foreign languages in which weekly 8Maurice Gibbons, Ingiyidnnlized Instrnction (New York: Teachers College Press, 1971), p. 2. 22 assignments of reading and translating sent to the student were corrected by the teacher "with notes and suggestions adapted to his needs."9 A recent Carnegie Corporation study showed that nearly five million students were enrolled in correspondence courses. Qronning Plans. The literature describes a large number of more or less pervasive procedures and devices, such as self-teaching materials, "enrichment" or supple- mentary assignments, differential assignments, workbooks, independent study, supervised study, personal programs, administrative plans and various grouping arrangements. Each of the categories listed above may in practice involve superficial one-shot innovations or serious and radical modifications of school organizations. Each may involve a myriad of forms: Shanell catalogued thirty-five methods of grouping practices alone in the United States, e.g. 9John s. Noffsinger, gnrnesnondence Sgnools ‘Lyggnmg, gnantagnas (New York: Howard MacMillan, 1926), p. 10. 10B. H. Pearse, "The Postman is the Proctor," American Egngntion, III (February, 1967), 10-12. 11H. G. Shane, "The School and Individual Dif- ferences," Ingiyidnalizing Instructio , Sixty-first Year- book of the National Society for the Study of Education, Part I (Chicago: The University of Chicago Press, 1962), pp. 20-22. 23 up-graded groups, primary-intermediate grouping, grade level grouping, homogeneous grouping, x-y-z grouping, intrasubject-field grouping, and departmental grouping. The grouping of students is usually an attempt to provide for individual differences by minimizing them within a classroom. Homogeneous grouping is defined by the pigtionany p; Education12 as "the classification of pupils for the purpose of forming instructional groups having a relatively high degree of similarity in regard to certain factors that effect learning." In a 1932 survey of 432 schools, Billet13 found homogeneous grouping to be the most popular method of improving the teaching and learning environment. Harap reported a few years later that ability grouping was "the most common method of adjusting learning to individual differences."14 A 12Carter B. Good, ed., Dictionary pf Edngation (2nd ed.; New York: McGraw-Hill, 1959), p. 255. 13Roy 0. Billet, The ggninisnnanion and Snpepyision .g§_flonogeneons Gnouping (Columbus, Ohio: Ohio State University Press, 1932). 14H. Harap, ”Differentiation of Curriculum Prac- tices and Instruction in Elementary Schools," TEE Gronping .9; Enpils, Thirty-fifth Yearbodk of the National Society for the Study of Education, Part I (Bloomington, Illinois: Public School Publishing Co., 1936), p. 163. h. i 24 comprehensive review of grouping practices and research can be found in the book edited by Yates.15 Sponsored by the UNESCO Institute for Education, the study reports the results of grouping in various countries and lists seven- teen varieties of grouping in primary and secondary schools. Relative to academic achievement, research dealing with homogeneous groupings has produced qualified and con- tradictory results. In an early critique, Rock reviewed only studies he considered "scientific" and concluded that: The experimental studies of groupings which have been considered fail to show consistent, statistical or educationally significant differences between the achievement of pupils in homogeneous groups.l6 Likewise, Miller and Otto found that "so far as academic achievement is concerned, there is not clear-cut evidence that homogeneous grouping is either advantageous or dis- advantageous."l7 They concluded "that ability grouping is quite ineffective unless accompanied by proper changes in 15Alfred Yates, Q;QEELQQ.1B Egncation (New York: Wiley and Sons, 1966). 16Robert T. Rock, "Critical Study of Current Prac- tices in Ability Grouping," Egngational Resnangh Bnlletin, IV (May-June, 1929), 67. 17W} Miller and M. J. Otto, "Analysis of Experi- mental Studies in Homogeneous Grouping," The gournal.g; Egngational Eeseargn, XXI (February, 1930), 100. 25 method. Unless adaptation of methods and materials is a necessary correlation to ability grouping, one of the pur- poses of the project is defeated."18 Turney's analysis of grouping studies found that: a) Most of the studies purporting to evaluate ability grouping have proved nothing regarding ability grouping but have only added evidence bearing upon the nature and extent of individual differences, b) most experimental attacks upon the value of ability group- ing have failed to evaluate the chief claims for it, i.e., the possibility of adopting content, method, or time, and c) the true evaluation of ability grouping must be deferred until adequate experimental attacks have succeeded in measuring its alleged advantages.19 The failure to obtain conclusive results, the sum- maries couched in tentative terms, and the conflict in research findings caused Cornell to observe that "a review of the objective results of ability grouping leaves one convinced that we have not yet attained any unequivocal experimental results that are capable of wide generaliza- tion.”20 Reviewing published studies, he could lalpig. 19A. H. Turney, "The Status of Ability Grouping," Edngapion;Administ;ation and Su er ision, XVII (January- February, 1931), 126-127. 20E. L. Cornell, "Effects of Ability Grouping Determinable from Published Studies," IDS Gnonping pf Rnpils, Thirty-fifth Yearbook of the National Society for the Study of Education, Part I (Bloomington, Illinois: Public School Publishing Co., 1936), p. 29. 26 only conclude: Experimental studies have in general been too piecemeal to afford a true evaluation of the results, but when attitudes, methods and curricula are well- adapted to further adjustment of the school to the child, results, both objective and subjective, may be favorable to grouping.2 Nearly thirty years later, Goodlad observed that "studies since the 1930's have not added precision to the conclu- sion or clarification of the problems analyzed by Cornell."22 More recent studies have done little to enlighten us. Millman and Johnson analyzed more than 8000 gain scores in English and mathematics for pupils in 327 class sections in twenty-eight schools. The study failed to show that the amount of gain depended on within-class variability, and concluded that "whatever the potentiali- ties may be for increasing achievement through narrowing the ability range of classes, such improvement is apparently not taking place."23 As wyndham observed, "the 211mg” p. 302. 22J. I. Goodlad, "Classroom Organization," Engyclopedia pf Edncational neseannn, ed. by C. W} Harris (New York: MacMillan, 1960), p. 224. 23J. Millman and.M. Johnson, "Relation of Section Variance to Achievement Gains in English and Mathematics in Grades 7 and 8," The American Edncation gesearch, 1964, p. 51. VI. 5 L a: . O .~L \ 2; MN... Pu .Ii C. c. I C I . .l ‘0 hi «Pu C at . C m l n a . S 27 first general impression one gains from these studies is that, granted their unequal experimental significance, they raise more issues than they settle."24 Some of these issues are sociological rather than academic in nature. In England, where most children in "public" schools and many in state-supported grammar schools are placed in "streams" (the English label for ability grouping), it has been suggested that such group- ing is responsible for a self-proving hypothesis. The results of the vocationally important "ll-plus" examina- tions given at the end of the fourth year of junior school may simply reflect the consequences of four years' stream- ing during which "A" classes get "A”-minded teachers and therefore "A" results, while "C” classes are taught by "C"-minded teachers whose expectations influence the academic achievement of their students. Daniels, after a thorough analysis of research from British and.American sources concluded: a) Streaming lowers rather than raises the average level of attainment in junior schools, b) streaming slightly reduces the level of attainment of "bright" junior school children, c) streaming markedly retards the educational process of the "slower" junior children, 24H. S. wyndham, Ability gronping (Melbourne: .Melbourne University Press, 1934), p. 107. 28 d) streaming artificially increases the range of educational attainment of junior school children, and e) widens the gap between the "bright” and "backward."25 Douglas examined the practice of streaming from a socio-economic basis as well as academic, and concluded that: Children who come from well-kept homes and who are themselves cleaned, well-clothed and shod stand a greater chance of being put in the upper streams than their measured ability would seem to justify. Once there, they are likely to stay and improve their per- formance in succeeding years. This is in striking contrast to the deterioration noticed in those children of similar initial measured ability who are placed in lower streams. In this way the validity of the initial selection appears to be confirmed by the subsequent performance of the children and an element of rigidity is introduced early into the primary school system. The sociological questions raised in class- conscious England have had echoes in race-conscious America, most notably in the furor raised by the practice of "tracking" in the Washington, D.C. public schools. As a result, and despite nearly eighty years of practice and forty years of study, the issues concerning the effect of 25J. C. Daniels, "The Effects of Streaming in the Primary Schools,“ The pritish gournal pf Ps cholo , XXXI (1961), 80. 26J. W. B. Douglas, 11.12 Home _ang1;_h_e_ Sgnool: A Study pf Ability nnd Attainment in pan Many Sghools (London: MacGibbon and Kee, 1964), p. 118. 29 homogeneous grouping on academic achievement are largely unresolved and likely to remain so. Laponatony MEtLOQS.éng.§§1£12322QHHB1E.EL§2§- In 1888 Preston Search, superintendent of schools in Pueblo, Colorado, began a program designed to permit a child to pace his own coverage of course material rather than await his turn in daily recitation.27 Search later used similar methods of organization in Los Angeles, but his ideas failed to be accepted and lay dormant for several years.28 In California, Frederick Burk and Mary ward initiated a new movement toward individualized instruction in 1912-13. Burk's Individual System, as it was later called, provided prescribed courses of study for each student and made provisions for the testing of pupils as their units of work were completed. Class lectures and daily assignments were abandoned. In their place, a pro- gram of relatively independent study was substituted, with teachers assisting students in the location of necessary study material. Later, a series of self-instructional 27Preston Search, "The Pueblo Plan of Individual Teaching,".§gngnp;pnnl,Beyiew, VIII (June, 1894), 154-158. 28Guy M. Whipple, ed., Adapting the Scnools.np .Lngiyidnnl Qiffenences, Twenty-fourth Yearbook of the National Society for the Study of Education, Part II (Chicago: The University of Chicago Press, 1925), p. 59. 30 bulletins was published and distributed across the United States until a ruling by the California Attorney General st0pped their publication in that State.29 Several methods or plans for individualizing instruction were reported in the Twenty-fourth Yearbook of the National Society for the Study of Education. One of the most prominent, Carleton Washborne's Winnetka Plan of Winnetka, Illinois, divided the curriculum into two parts. The first, dealing with essential common knowledge and skills, was studied in intensive units each morning. The second, stressing the self-expression of the child's own interests and abilities, involved the free and creative activities chosen by students each afternoon. While allowing for group or class work, emphasis was on indi- vidual study and progress.30 Group promotion was replaced by individual achievement, with students taking self- administered tests to determine their readiness for the teacher tests.31 zglhisl- , pp. 59-60. 3OCarleton‘Washborn and Sidney Marland, Jr., Winnetka: Tne Histony'nng Signifigance.p§,nn Edugational Expergnent (Englewood Cliffs, New Jersey: Prentice-Hall, 1963). 31‘Whipple,__c_>.p. nit., p. 82. 31 At about the same time, Parkhurst's Dalton Laboratory Plan32 was developed in Dalton, Massachusetts, and like the Winnetka Plan, it owed much of its inspiration to Frederick Burk. Properly a sociological rather than a curricular plan, it attempted to modify the conditions within schools by a commitment to three principles: (1) freedom for the individual child to work on his assignments, (2) cooperation in group life, and (3) the budgeting of time in relation to the proportion of effort- to-attainment. A unique feature furnished students with a "contract" specifying the completion of a certain number 33 of assigned units within a fixed period of time. Other programs cited in the Twenty-fourth Yearbodk include the Indiyidualized Instrnction Plan for Anithmetic in Detroit, Michigan, and the Individualination.pf‘Work in the Vocational School at Madison, Wisconsin, all marked by the attempt to center instruction more or less on the learner's h . 4 interests while pacing it according to his abilities.3 32Helen Parkhurst, Education pn tne paltpn Plan (New York: Dutton, 1922). 33Whipple,pp. git., pp. 84-87. 34Ipid., pp. 101-114. 32 Mayer-Cakes,35 adopted the Dalton Plan success- fully and reported a gain of 25% in the pr0portion of students who passed the state-wide examinations, but Thompson,36 evaluating results from a more rigorously controlled experiment, did not find any special advantage for the Dalton Plan. A modified Dalton Plan was success- fully administered by Underhill.37 38 describes a third individualized method Billett of instruction known as the Morrison Plan. The classroom is transformed into a laboratory in which sequenced lesson units and guide sheets for assignments are differentiated for pupils of varying ability. The teacher's role is to give personal guidance to the pupil's work and study activities. The Morrison Plan was most frequently associated with the teaching of science, and 9% of the secondary schools in this country were reported using this plan in 1932. 35G. H. Mayer-Cakes, “The Dalton Plan in a Small High School,"‘1nn Egncation gournal, LVII (December, 1936), 244-248. 36W. H. Thompson, "Experiment with the Dalton Plan," Ine.gpnpnnl,gf Egngationnl Researgn, XXVI (March, 1933), 493-508. 37R. J. Underhill, "Experience of Scarsdale with Individual Instruction," new'xonk State Egncation Jo nal, XVII (March, 1931), 677-678. 38Roy 0. Billet, The Administnation and Supep- yisign pf agnogeneons‘gngnping (Columbus, Ohio: Ohio State University Press, 1932). 33 .QQIIQDE Selffizngpgnunip Elnns. The development of teaching materials, referred to as "learning packages," have become an integral aspect of recent organizational programs which are, in part, indebted to the older Winnetka and Dalton Plans. Each package, whether teacher- initiated and prepared or developed commercially, is a self-contained set of materials designed to teach a single concept, idea, skill, or attitude. Specific objectives to be learned are listed for the student and stated in behaviorial terms. Various media, materials, and methods are used in the accomplishment of these objectives, and evaluation by pre-test, self-test, and post-test is included to allow the student to measure his own progress. Referring to UNIPAC, a set of teacher-prepared materials developed by the IDEA Materials Center and founded by the Charles F. Kettering Foundation, Bishop describes the format as follows: UNIPACs are designed to help students achieve at their own best learning rates. Given UNIPACs, stu- dents will be able to achieve measurable performances under given conditions, at or above specified minimum levels, and at rates which are individually unique to each student. When the student with the assistance of a teacher selects a particular UNIPAC in his sequential learning program, he takes a pre-test based on the behavioral objectives in that UNIPAC. If the pre-test results indicate that he is ready for the concepts or skills of the UNIPAC, he selects from suggested learning 34 materials and activities in the UNIPAC those which fit his own unique learning style. Behavioral objectives, which are contained in his UNIPAC, guide him as he learns. When he feels that he has achieved one behavioral objective, he proceeds to the next one and again selects from suggested learning materials and activities. When the student feels that he has achieved all of the behavioral objectives in his UNIPAC, he takes a self-test. If the self-test results indicate that he is ready for teacher evaluation, the student can request the post-test for his UNIPAC. Upon successful completion of the post-test, the student may proceed to this next UNIPAC or he may participate in quest activities. If the student elects to participate in quest, he defines a problem for in-depth or in-breadth study, and he conducts his research in order to achieve some level of resolution of his problem. During the entire learning sequence the teacher provides as many opportunities as possible for a student-teacher and student-student interaction during conferences and seminars. Small learning teams, made up of from two to six students are formed whenever feasible. The teacher monitors each student's progress, diagnoses learning problems, prescribes possible alternative learning materials and activities, and evaluates each student's progress in achieving the stated behavioral objectives.39 Perhaps the most significant example of the commercially-prepared materials is the Individually Prescribed Instruction (IPI) program administered by the Learning Research and Development Center of the University of Pittsburgh and Research for Better Schools, Inc. based in Philadelphia. 39Lloyd K. Bishop, Individualining Educational Systems (New York: Harper and Row, 1971), p. 36-37. 35 Scanlon and Bolvin define IPI as the . . . plan- ning and conducting with each student a program of his studies that is tailored to his learning needs and to his characteristics as a learner. IPI takes into account such parameters of individual differences as rate of learning, amount of practice and, to some extent, preference for mode of instruction."40 They further describe the func- tion of their project as an ". . . instructional system based on specific objectives, correlated with diagnostic tools, teaching materials and methods. It represents one specific way of providing for wide ranges of differences that exist in classrooms. Certainly it typifies what can be done to help resolve the age-old problem of providing for each student, each day, his own program of studies."41 The IPI program is primarily involved with the subject areas of reading, mathematics, spelling, science, and handwriting, and attempts to plan and implement a pro- gram adapted to the personal and learning needs for every student. Four main considerations are postulated: l. The rate of speed at which each child progresses depends upon his own capacities. He OScanlon and Bolvin, pp. cit., p. 2. 41Ibid., p. 1. 36 places himself upon the continuum by taking both placement tests and pre-tests. 2. The curricular material is arranged in a sequential order called continuum. The assignments are given by a prescription to fit his individual needs. (A prescription is an individual lesson plan for each student each day.) 3. The student's mastery of the curriculum is judged by curriculum-embedded tests and post-tests. He is required to perform at a level of 85%. 4. The child works independently in most cases, thus building up his sense of responsibility and also his confidence in his own knowledge. He begins to realize that learning is a process that is dependent on his own participation and initiative.42 The saluatory effect of individualized instruction on achievement is well-documented in research studies from , 43 44 . many fields by Appleby, Armstrong, Postlethwaith and Novak,45 McCarley,46 and others. In a study of 421pid., p. 2. 43B. C. Appleby, "The Effects of Individualized Reading on Certain Aspects of Literature Study with High School Seniors" (unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, Univer- sity of Iowa, 1967). 44W. H. Armstrong, "An Experimental Investigation of The Instructional Effectiveness of Published Programmed Instructional Materials vs. Individualized Instruction in Area Vocational-Technical Schools"(unpublished Ph.D. dis- sertation, The Florida State University, 1967). 458. N. Postlethwaith and Joseph Novak, "The Use of 8mm Loop Films in Individualized Instruction," Annals.p§ png Ngy xOrk Academy pf Science (New York: The New York Academy of Science, March 31, 1967). 46W} W. McCarley, "An Experimental Study to Evalu- ate the Effectiveness of an Individualized Instructional Method and Lecture-Discussion Method of Teaching Vocational Agriculture Classes" (unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, Michigan State University, 1968). 37 mathematics under the IPI curriculum, Glaser observed twenty-one experimental classes in four schools, once before the introduction of the individualized program and four times after the program began. Control classes were similarly observed during the school year. Besides the superior achievement of students in the experimental groups, Glaser reported that: In the control class, three aspects of the com- munication pattern appeared as follows: 1) over half of the communications in the classroom were non- instructional, 2) about 90 percent of the communica- tions were teacher-oriented; half of these were directed to the single student and half to groups of students, and 3) where the teacher talked to one student, it was most likely that the communication was noninstructional; when the teacher talked to more than one student, it was likely that the communication was instructional. Before the initiation of the individualized pro- gram, the communication pattern in the experimental classes was highly similar to this control-school pattern. After the introduction of the individually prescribed instruction procedure, the following appeared: 1) over three-quarters of the communica- tions were instructional in nature, 2) 20 percent of the communications were teacher-initiated; of these, three-quarters were directed to the single student, 3) about 80 percent of the communications were student initiated; of these, three-quarters were instructional in nature, and 4) there was a trend for the overall number of communications to decrease in the experi- mental-classes.47 47Robert Glaser, fignpping tne Elenenpary.§gnppl Cnrpicnlnn pg Ingiyidual Perfonnance (Pittsburgh: Publi- cation of the University of Pittsburgh Learning Research and Development Center, Reprint 26, 1967), pp. 3-4. 38 Pr ra e and ggmpnterized Inspnnction. Pro- grammed teaching by text or machine is "an attempt to obtain the kind of behavioral control shown possible in the laboratory."48 The concept of dividing the segments of a course into "small, but rigorous steps, each of which is rewarding,"49 has revolutionized some schools and had a profound influence on the role of the teacher. Pro- grammed methods whether "linear" or "branched“ can be adapted to the individual by alterations in the method and sequence of presenting those units, and by virtue of the fact that the student, working at his own pace, either corrects his response to each frame or has it corrected for him. Regardless of the technique, the central element of the system is the positive or negative reinforcement of responses, leading to the extinction of incorrect behavior. The convergence of computer technology and pro- grammed learning will have perhaps the greatest single impact on instruction in the future. Because of the inordinant amount and types of material which can be 48James G. Holland, "Teaching Machines: An Appli- cation of Principles from the Laboratory," Jonpnal,gf tne Enperinental Analysis‘p;.§gnnyipn, III, No. 4 (October, 1960), 275- 49B. F. Skinner, The Technology 9; Teacning (New York: Appleton Century Crofts, 1968), p. 3. 39 stored and immediately recalled for presentation, the high degree of control over the learner, and the ease of vary- ing the sequence of presentation, computer-assisted instruction (CAI) provides unusual opportunities for creating individualized instructional programs. To a great extent, CAI is an extension of the simpler programmed teaching machines. In that format, the student is presented with a series of statements and ques- tions to which he must correctly respond by pushing one of a choice of keys. If his response is correct, the machine informs him of that fact and presents the next question. If he is incorrect, the machine announces the error, presents more information, and requires the student to try again. The computer's contributions to the system are described by Carter: At the System DeveloPment Corporation, we have built an experimental computer-based schoolroom called ”CLASS" which stands for "Computer-Based Laboratory for Automated School System." CLASS is a rather unusual schoolroom in that each student works in a separate small cubicle which has equipment allowing him to receive visual and auditory instruction through film projectors, tape recorders, or television. The student is able to respond to the material being presented through a special switchboard, or set of buttons, which is connected to our Philco 2000, a medium-sized.modern computer. The student's response is instantly transmitted to the computer which has in it a program for recording the response, for analyzing it, and for determining what instructional material should be presented next to the student. On the basis 40 of this program, the computer transmits a signal back to the student which instructs him about the film frame or auditory grouping to which he should next attend. As the student progresses through the instructional material, the computer is able to specify the sequence of materials to meet his par- ticular needs. That is to say, as the student responds to the instructional items, his progress is constantly monitored and wherever items are being missed or trouble appears, the computer analyzes the difficulty and presents to the student items of sequences of material which attempt to remedy it.50 PrOperly programmed, the computer's power lies in its ability to continually monitor and assess the student's progress, to evaluate and modify its instruction presenta- tion relative to that progress, and to provide a detailed record of the student's achievement and manner of achievement. For example, the computer will: 1. Engage in two-way communication with a student by means of natural language messages. 2. Guide the student through a program of tasks, helping him where he has difficulty, and accelerating his progress where he finds little challenge. 3. Observe and record significant details of the student's behavior, including steps undertaken in performing tasks, time taken for particular steps, and values of varying psychological and environmental qualities. 4. Simulate the operation of a physical, mathe- matical, or social process responding to variations in parameters. 50Launor F. Carter, "Computers: Their Impact on Instruction, on Educational Planning, and on the Cur- riculum,” cited in Lloyd K. Bishop, Individualizing Educational Systems (New York: Harper and Row, 1971), p. 48. 41 5. Analyze and summarize performance records and other behavioral records of individual students and also groups of students.51 There are significant limitations to CAI learning systems. The computer's ability to understand partial responses and meanings is limited, and it cannot devise on-the-spot solutions and alternatives. The most serious impediment to the extensive use of CAI is the currently prohibitive cost of the equipment. It is possible that the continuing development of time-sharing facilities will allow schools to have access to this tool, via terminals, without the expense of an "in-house" installation. Individualizing Music Instruction Pnggrammed Instruction. The attempts of music educators to individualize instruction have been largely confined to the development of self-instructional programs and other devices designed to promote more efficient learning of specialized musical skills. In 1958-59, Spohn52 assembled structured drill material for use in the 51E. N. Adams, "Computer-Assisted Instruction," anpnters and.nntgmation, II (March, 1966), 17-18. 52C. L. Spohn, "An Exploration in the Use of Recorded Teaching Material to Develop Aural Comprehension in College Music Classes" (unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, Ohio State University, 1959). 42 teaching of melodic perception at the college level. The results of his studies indicated that structured drill provided to students for use outside of normal class hours was significantly more effective than unstructured drill in the same conditions. Carlsen53 compared the effectiveness of linear and branched programs with teacher-instruction in melodic dictation. Both types of programmed learning were shown to be more beneficial to the college students than a comparable group taught by more conventional methods, but no superiority could be claimed for either the linear or the branching technique. Wardian54 andAshford55 also found programmed materials to be a more efficient method of teaching the fundamentals of music theory to college 53James C. Carlsen, ”An Investigation of Pro- grammed Learning in Melodic Dictation by Means of a Teaching Machine Using a Branching Technique of Programming" (unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, Northwestern University, 1962). 54J.‘Wardian, "An Experiment Concerning the Effectiveness of Programmed Learning for Use in Teaching the Fundamental of Music" (unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, Washington State University, 1968). 55T. Ashford, "The Use of Programmed Instruction to Teach Fundamental Concepts in Music Theory," Journal.gf Research In unsic Educatio , XIV (Fall 1966), 171-177. 43 students. Hargiss56 developed a text, after numerous revisions, to teach the rudiments of music theory to elementary education majors in piano classes. While stu- dents who used the final revision for only one semester attained the same level of proficiency as a group who studied in a traditional class, a third group subjected to both teacher instruction and programmed material achieved more than either of the former groups. Tape recorder models for use in instrumental instruction were first devised by La Bach.57 His device, a two-track tape recorder with microphone, relay switches and controls, was designed to enable students to record their practice on a given exercise, hear it played back, and compare it with the playback of a pre-recorded model. Unfortunately, a statistical evaluation of student pro- gress was not attempted, but La Bach concluded that students enjoyed using the device and that students "may ‘well" have shown significant improvement in performance skills through its use. 56Genevieve Hargiss, "The Acquisition of Sight Singing Ability in Piano Classes for Students Preparing to be Elementary Teachers," gonrnal.pI_Reseanch.IninusIc .EgucagIgn, X (Spring, 1962), 69-75. 57Parker La Bach, "A Device to Facilitate Learning 0f Basic Music Skill," pnlletin p; the gonncil for lfififlgnngn.In_Mnsig Egncation, No. 4 (Winter, 1965), 7-10. 44 In a study using similar tape-recorded models and instructions for practice, but without provision for recording and playback, a group of elementary school violin students who practiced with these materials were judged by a panel of expert violinists to have superior skill in phrasing, bowing, tempo, intonation, rhythm, expression and tonal quality than their counterparts who practiced normally. In a similar study, Pu0polo58 pre- pared practice tapes for use by beginning instrumentalists. Each tape generally consisted of a model performance or an exercise with simple piano accompaniment, and verbal instructions, explanations, and counting of meter before, during and after all model performances. According to Pu0polo, the following format was adhered to: l. A brief reminder of problems to be encountered preceded each tune or exercise to be practiced (new rhythms, new notes, fingerings, chromatics, new note values, phrasings, etc.). 2. Student listened to model performances of tune or exercise while reading along from the score. 3. While reading from the score, student listened to first isolated segment. 4. Student played segment very slowly, then slightly faster, faster, and finally ”a tempo.” 58Vito Puopolo, "The Development and Experimental Application of Self-Instructional Practice Materials for Beginning Instrumentalists" (unpublished Ph.D. disserta- tion, Michigan State University, 1970). 45 (Directed by recorded counting and piano accompani- ment.) 5. Student was asked if he remembered to cope with specific problems, for example, "Did you remember to use the second valve for that F# on the third beat?" 6. Student listened to reinforcement and com- pared. 7. After each segment was drilled, student per- formed entire tune or exercise, then listened to reinforcement. (According to instructions, student either listened to reinforcement or played in unison with it.)59 Practice material, the weekly band assignment, was the same for both experimental and control groups. Students in the former group were issued cassette players and the appropriate tape, and were assigned individual daily prac- tice sessions by the music teacher. Control group members were also required to practice daily, but without the use of the tapes. After a period of ten weeks, the students 0 were tested on the watkins-Ennnnn,Perfonmange Scale6 and students using the self-instructional practice tapes were found to have achieved significantly superior results. These and other studies involving various aspects of performance and using such nonprogrammed technical 5?;p;g., pp. 37-38. 60John‘Watkins and Stephen Farnum, Ins Watkins- .En;nnm,Perfo;nance Scale (Winona, Minnesota: Hal Leonard Music, Inc., 1965). 46 . l . . . devices as the tachistoscope,6 instructional films,62 and oscillosc0pic transparencies63 have shown that effective, although somewhat cumbersome and expensive methods exist for the improvement of music instruction. Summarizing his review of research, Duerkson states that: Results of studies of programmed instruction and the use of various instructional devices have at least two important implications for the music teacher: (a) there are some instructional programs and devices that supplement traditional methods of class or private instruction; (b) there are other instructional programs and devices that are equal in effect to traditional classroom or private instruction. Use of these programs and devices can free instructional time for other pursuits in which the teacher is essential. In both cases, the pr0per use of instructional tech- nology seems to lead to more effective instruction in instrumental music.64 61R. F. Difranzo, "A Comparison of Tachistoscopic and Conventional Methods in Teaching Grade Three Music Sight-Playing on a Melody Wired Instrument" (unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, University of Connecticut, 1966). 62D. M. Beley, ”The Development and Evaluation of the Effectiveness of a Pilot Instructional Sound-film for Teaching Beginning Trumpet Students with a Brief Survey of the History of Trumpet Instruction and the History of Educational Film Research“ (unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, University of Iowa, 1970). 63T. S. Small, "The Evaluation of Clarinet Tone Through the Use of Oscilloscopic Transparencies,” gpunnal .2§;3eseanch‘In_Mnsig Egngation, XV (Spring, 1967), 11-22. 64George Duerkson, Teacning Insgnnnental Mnsig (Washington, D.C.: MENC, 1972), p. 6. 47 P;iyate.y§. glass Instnnctipn. Patterns of instrumental class organization have rarely been questioned by music educators, and the few systematic investigations have mainly dealt with the relative effectiveness of private or class lessons. Waa65 compared the effect of both methods of instruction on the musical achievement and musical aptitude of fifth and sixth grade instrumental beginners. He found that private students attained sig- nificantly higher scores than class students on the Watkins-Earnnn Eerfonnance Scale, but admitted that because the private students' scores appeared to be seriously influenced by an uncontrolled band-no band variable, this difference could have been due to the combination of band experience and private lessons. Unfortunately his groups were quite small, some as small as N=4, and his evaluation of data required the use of non-parametric statistics (Kruskal-Wallis one-way analysis of variance and the Mann- Whitney U Test) which precluded the statistical confirma- tion of that interaction. 65Loren waa, "An Experimental Study of Class and Private Methods of Instruction in Instrumental Music" (unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, University of Illinois, 1965). 48 Shugert,66 in an attempt to more rigorously control the confounding variables which had plagued Waa's study, evaluated the effects of class (homogeneous and hetero- geneous) and private lessons on musical achievement (watkins-Eannum Performance Scale and the Fannnm MusIc Notation Test), musical aptitude (Seasnone Tests pg nusigal Ialent) and attitude towards school music (genners Scnogl Supjecn Rating Scale). He found: 1. no significant differences between class and private students on the tests of aptitude and notation, 2. that private students were significantly superior to class students in performance achievement, 3. no significant differences in performance achievement between homogeneous and heterogeneous classes.67 Shugert used'p-tests exclusively in evaluating his data and could therefore make no comment on the role of aptitude on achievement nor the interaction of lessons and achievement. More seriously, he experienced a massive mortality rate in the thirteen weeks of the study: only 123 students of the original 219 completed the experimental period. One might both justifiably assume that the 66James Shugert, "An Experimental Investigation of Heterogeneous Class and Private Methods of Instruction with Beginning Instrumental Music Students" (unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, University of Illinois, 1969). Ibid. 49 drOp-outs represented the least successful students in the sample, and speculate about the possible effects on per- formance achievement had they finished the experiment and their scores been included. . . 68 . In a rev1ew of Shugert 3 study, Noble questioned the use of the watkins-Farnnn Perfonnange Scale to measure performance achievement, pointing out that the test does not measure such ensemble skills as balance, precision, and vertical intonation. Unfortunately no studies are available which delineate the extent, if any, to which class instruction influences those variables. However, it is apparent that Shugert intended only to measure the effect of instructional conditions on individual achieve- ment, and that Noble's charge of a biased measuring instrument is imprecise: there is no evidence that the watkinsfignnnnn,Perfprmance Scale fails to objectively evaluate performance achievement, and studies cited by 68Robert Noble, "Critique: An Experimental Investigation of Heterogeneous Class and Private Methods of Instruction with Beginning Instrumental Music Stu- dents,” BnlletIn pg pnp.gpnngIIHfipn Research In_nnsig Educatipn, No. 21 (Summer, 1970), 47-51. 50 watkins,69 Farnum7o andMitchum71 indicate that the scale indeed measures what it purports to measure. Noble's statement should be considered a value judgment on Shugert's decision not to include a measure designed to evaluate the relative effect of the teaching methods on ensemble performance. Shugert's decision was clearly a practical one; the complexities involved in the construc- tion and validation of an objective scale for measuring ensemble performance is staggering, and would itself encompass the scope of an important thesis. Subjective analyses by a panel of judges would be limited by its very subjectivity and/or its inability to produce quantifiable data. Indiyidualined_nn§Ig Instruntion. In a project to individualize instruction within classes of beginning instrumentalists by means of scores on an aptitude test, 69 John watkins, ObjectIye Measnne pg Instrumental .Ennnpnmnngn (New York: Teacher's College Bureau of Publi- cations, 1942). 70 Stephen Farnum, "Prediction of Success in Instrumental Music" (unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, Harvard University, 1950). 71John Mitchum, "The Wing 'Standardized Tests of Musical Intelligence'; An Investigation of Predictability with Selected Seventh Grade Beginning Band Students" (unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, Florida State University, 1969). 51 2 Froseth7 constructed texts designed for use in like- instrument (homogeneous) classes with provisions for the simultaneous performance of material written at various levels of difficulty. The texts used by the experimental groups allow students of high, average, and low levels of achievement to perform lesson material simultaneously and without the frustration caused by a pace of drill too slow or quick for their capabilities. According to Froseth, In order to provide more musical instruction, traditional non-musical exercises were excluded in favor of folk-song literature and musical sounds. Teachers were instructed to avoid lock-step instruc- tion and to use the flexibility of the experimental texts to elicit musical responses for the purpose of developing musical independence.7 Froseth also constructed supplementary texts and exercises of folk-song literature so that the singing voice could be used to reinforce concepts of tonality, musical phrasing, and musical styles of articulation. Eurhythmic activities and the use of percussion instru- ments were used to deve10p rhythmic aptitudes. 72James O. Froseth, "Using MAP Scores in the Instruction of Beginning Students in Instrumental Music," Jonrnal g; Research InIMusic Education, XIV (Spring, 1971), 98-101. ”mm. p. 99. 52 Describing instructional procedures, Froseth states that: The provision for self-initiated and self-directed study was an important innovation employed in experi- mental group classes. In order to facilitate varying interests and needs, students in the experimental group were encouraged to "play by ear,” improvise and choose songs to practice and play from-a supplementary book of folk songs. Specific assignments were avoided in order to allow students to determine their own goals and pursue their interests freely and indepen- dently. In addition, many lesson projects made provision for independent and self-directed study. Self-direction was facilitated by sequencing study material in a manner that allowed students to correct their own errors, and, therefore, learn indepen- dently.74 Thirty-four sixth-grade students were given the Musical Aptitude Profile (MAP), subsequently grouped into high, average and low categories according to their MAP composite scores and then randomly placed into either the experimental or control group. No other basis was used to establish the equivalency of the groups. One of the dependent variables, performance achievement, was tested by means of students' recorded performances on self-prepared and sight-read etudes con- structed by the author and rated by two judges. The results demonstrated the superiority of the individualized 74IpId. 53 instruction and the relationship of MAP scores to achieve- ment. No significant interaction was found to exist between instruction and aptitude. Subject Variables and Music Achievement Music educators are continually intrigued by the relationship of certain subject variables to performance achievement. A variety of tests measuring intelligence, aptitude, academic achievement, personality, etc. are available, and are inevitably used to correlate students' scores on those variables with some aspect of musical suc- cess. Knowledge of these relationships are, unfortunately, sometimes used to encourage or discourage an individual planning to participate in an instrumental music program. [Such attempts to use these criteria in selecting students are unfortunate because, philosophical considera— tions aside, research inevitably informs us that the prediction of success or failure cannot be accurately made for individuals. As Duerkson summarizes: Studies at different levels ranging from kinder- garten to college lead to a single conclusion: scores on presently available standardized tests, used alone or in combination, do not correlate well with success or failure in instrumental music and cannot be used as the only predictor of whether or not an individual will succeed. Success in instrumental music has been uncovered in several ways in these studies, but 54 however measured, correlations with standardized test scores seldom reach 0.50. While this degree of cor- relation is of value in research and in making predic- tions for groups, it is not high enough to warrant confidence in predictions for individuals. High scores on the various tests do not assure that the student will succeed, nor do low scores assure that he will not. The data suggest that it is not justifiable to bar students from instrumental music programs solely on the basis of their scores on the aptitude type of test.75 In a study investigating the power of musical aptitude, general intelligence, and academic achievement to predict drop-outs, Young76 found he was unable to predict a student's successful completion of the instru- mental program. Mitchum,77 using a deletion or addition regression program, found that measures of 1.0. and academic achievement showed the most potential for predic- tion of scores on the Watkins-Farnum, ; = .49 and .50 respectively, and Corno78 obtained similar results. 75Duerkson,pp. cit., p. 6. 76William Young, "An Investigation of the Relative and Combined Power of Musical Aptitude, General Intelli- gence, and Academic Achievement Tests to Predict Musical Attainment" (unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, University of Iowa, 1969). 77Mitchum,_<_>_p. cit. 78Guy Corno, "Relationship Between the Degree of Intelligence and Performance on a Test of Musical Apti- tude" (unpublished Master's Thesis, Dusquesne University, 1958). 55 Likewise Rhoades79 found a measure of intelligence to be a more superior predictor of musical success than a test of musical talent. 8O . Gordon, however, compared performance achieve- ment with other variables and found correlation coeffi- cients of .91 for MAP, .62 for I.Q., and .60 for academic achievement. Because administrative testing practices allowed him to use only a small number of subjects, how- ever, he cautioned against the generalization of his results. Schneider and Cady reviewed all research in music education between 1930 and 1962, and concluded: 1. Intelligence appears to be the most important single variable in predicting success in instrumental music. 2. Musicality (as measured by musical aptitude tests), by itself, does not appear to be a satis- factory predictor of achievement in music. 3. A definite relationship seems to exist between academic average, music grade average, musical apti- tude, intelligence, and musical achievement.81 79Fordyce Rhoades, "An Evaluation of Measures for the Prediction of Success in Instrumental Study" (unpub- lished Master's Thesis, University of washington, 1938). 80Edwin Gordon, TIStudy of the Efficacy of General Intelligence and Musical Aptitude Tests in Predicting Achievement in Music," Bnlletin pg Eng Council In; Researcn‘In_Musig Education, No. 13 (Spring, 1968), 40-45. 81Erwin Schneider and Henry Cady, "Evaluation and Synthesis of Research Studies Relating to Music Education: 1930-1962," Bulletin pg tne gouncil for Research In_nnsic Edu ation, No. 9 (Spring, 1967), 8-9. 56 In addition to the variable mentioned by Schneider and Cady, other experimenters have attempted to relate personality factors to achievement in instrumental music. Cooley,82 in a study plagued by faults of design and uncontrolled variables, found very little relationship between intelligence, personality, and various tests of musical abilities. In 1957, Cramer83 attempted to predict achievement of fourth through eighth grade students on the watkins-Farnnn Scales using measures of I.Q., per- sonality, physical growth, and the Seashore tests of Pitch, Rhythm, and Tonal Memory. Although no test could reliably predict success, he found that high performance achievement was accompanied by high scores on the variables listed. Dealing solely with aspects of per- , 84 . . sonality, Kaplan found that instrumental mu51c students 82John Cooley, "A Study of the Relation Between Certain Mental and Personality Traits and Ratings of Musical Abilities" (unpublished Ed.D. dissertation, University of Michigan, 1952). 83William Cramer, "The Relation of Maturation and Other Factors to Achievement in Beginning Instrumental Per- formance at the 4th through 8th Grade Levels" (unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, Florida State University, 1958). 84Lionel Kaplan, "The Relationship Between Certain Personality Characteristics and Achievement in Instru- mental Music" (unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, New York University, 1961). 57 in general and high achievers in particular were charac- terized as more self-confident, self-controlling, and intellectualizing than non-music subjects and low achievers. Using a test of cognitive musical achievement, 85 86 . however, both Thayer and Schleuter could find no systematic relationship between achievement and personality traits. Summary In summary, it can be said that the evidence of research shows that some individualized instrumental tech- niques can be an effective way of coping with and providing for the differences students bring to class. It can also be fairly stated that until recently, many music educators have been reluctant to concern themselves with characteristics of the individual learner and the adoption of teaching methods suitable to him, preferring instead to seek yet new tests which might screen out a potential 85Robert Thayer, "An Investigation of the Inter- relation of Personality Traits, Musical Achievement, and Different Measures of Musical Aptitude" (unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, University of Iowa, 1971). 86Stanley Schleuter, "An Investigation of the Interrelation of Personality Traits, Musical Aptitude and Musical Achievement" (unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, University of Iowa, 1971). 58 non-contributor to the band program. Despite that bias apparent in most studies in music reviewed here, the literature levels lead us to expect that "lock-step” conditions of instruction will yield fairly substantial relationships between performance achievement and the variables of I.Q., academic achievement, and musical aptitude. Low-to-moderate relationships would be expected between performance achievement and personality measures. Because of the paucity of relevant research on attitude towards music and individualized instruction relating to achievement, it is impossible to suggest with any cer- tainty their potential role in this study. CHAPTER III DESIGN OF THE STUDY Procedures Sample. Two Lansing, Michigan schools, Patten- gill Junior High School and Dwight Rich Junior High School, participated in the project during the 1970-71 academic year. Because of the Lansing Public Schools' policy of discouraging doctoral research in their city's schools, permission to carry out the study could only be obtained by assuring the superintendent that the author would be the instructor for the experimental group (Pattengill Junior High School) in which drastic changes of instruc- tion would be made. Thus only the control group (Rich Junior High School) was chosen by random means. However, the city of Lansing employs an extensive system of bussing to achieve a racial, social and economic balance of students within its schools. Not unexpectedly, therefore, the two groups displayed remarkable similarity relative to the subject 59 60 variables. While it must be admitted that the selection procedures will limit the generalizability of the results, there is no firm evidence that either group arises from differing p0pulations. Subjects. The sample was comprised of the seventh grade students enrolled in the beginning instrumental pro- gram from both schools; the data for all students with prior instrumental music instruction (two students at Pattengill Junior High School, one student at Rich Junior High School) were excluded. No special process beyond the students' desire to participate was invoked to select participants for the program, and no criteria other than the students' preference played a role in the selection of their musical instruments. Table 3:1 contains the.u for sex, the n of each group, and the total N for the study. Table 3:1. .3 for Groups, Sex, and Totals. School Male Female Totals Pattengill JHS 26 19 45 Rich JHS 25 20 45 Totals 51 39 9O 61 The mortality rate was extremely low, consisting of one student at Pattengill Junior High School and two students at Rich Junior High School, or a rate of 3.33 per- cent for the whole sample. Method Both groups were sub-divided into three classes of approximately fifteen students per class. Instruction was provided by two certified public school music teachers, and consisted of one fifty-minute, mixed-instrument class lesson every school day for a complete school year of 180 days. The instructional texts for all students were the 1 First Diyision Band Method series, Part I and Part II. Qontrol Treatment. Students in the control group were taught by what may be described as the ensemble method. In these classes, the students are seated in a semi-circle and in groups of similar instruments. Rehearsal procedure typically consists of the instructor's analysis and explanation of the content and novelties in a 'particular line of music, followed by extensive ensemble drill on that line. During or between repetitions, lFred'W'eber, Finst Diyision gand Method (Rockville Center, Long Island: Belwin, 1962). 62 further critical comment or demonstration by singing is made by the instructor, and the class may repeat the drill or progress to the next line, depending upon the instruc- tor's evaluation of class competency. The decision to progress to a new line is made only by the instructor, and is usually based on his opinion that a sufficient number of students have attained a "reasonable" level of com- petency. This judgment may also be influenced by such extra-musical factors as the group's willingness to con- tinue the drill or the need to "cover" certain material within a given class hour. This lock-step procedure seemingly tends to maxi- mize the teacher's ability to control both the direction and rate of class learning. While the specific concepts and skills presented for learning to the students are primarily governed by the design of the method book, the teacher may employ various means to emphasize those areas which he considers most important for the acquisition of performance ability. Although the instructor may drill a small section of similar instruments, individual rehearsal is confined to an occasional solo performance, or the placing of a student in an area outside the main rehearsal room. With 63 the exception of the last procedure, which is usually used only with poor students who are far behind the class, the individual student has virtually no voice in the decision to continue or terminate drill. Experimennal Tneatment. After the first week of the term, in which instruction concerning the assembly and maintenance of the instrument was given, students in the experimental group were not seated in a pattern, but instead were assigned to chairs widely dispersed about the classroom. At the start of the daily period, the students would begin individual practice at the point in their instruction book reached the previous class period. Of course, this point for one student was rarely identical to the material being practiced by any other student. Instruction given by the teacher was exclusively tutorial and provided in only two instances. The first was at the student's request and could occur at any time an individual sought instruction. The second instance occurred when a student, deciding that he was capable of satisfactorily playing the complete assigned page he had been practicing, requested the teacher to evaluate his efforts on that material. What followed could best be described as a "mini-lesson": the instructor would set 64 tempi, listen to the student's recital, comment on the strengths and weaknesses of the performance, and make sug- gestions leading to the improvement of such factors as tonal and rhythmical accuracy. During the initial stages the instructor assumed the responsibility of deciding whether the student had demonstrated sufficient competency to progress to new material on the next page. Later, and at a rate which varied from student to student, the decision to proceed gradually became a matter of discussion between student and instructor, and eventually became the sole prerogative of the student. If the decision, however determined, was to begin a new page, the teacher explained any novel material, heard the student attempt to play it, and corrected errors of a cognitive nature such as failing to observe key signature. To prevent a single student from monopolizing the instructor's time and to promote accurate self- evaluation, the students were limited to one unit (page) evaluative conference per class period. This limit did not apply to requests for explanatory assistance. 65 Description of Instruments and Method of Gathering Data At various times during the experimental period, data were gathered for all students on the subject variables of 1. Sex 2. Intelligence (I.Q.) 3. Grade Point Average (G.P.A.) 4. Attitude Towards Music 5. Musical Aptitude 6. Personal Adjustment and on the dependent variable, performance achievement. Information concerning gpn of the students was obtained by observation or, where necessary, by inSpection of the school records. The measure of I.Q. was the Otis-Lennon Mental Ability Test, Intenmediate Level, by Arthur S. Otis and Roger T. Lennon and was administered by school psycholo- gists to all seventh grade students in the Lansing Public Schools during the third week of the experimental period. According to the authors, the eight various types of verbal and non-verbal items which make up the test empha- size skills of abstract reasoning ability. One score 66 summarizes all eighty items. The Mannal fig; AngnInnnn- .nIpnz gives a split-half reliability coefficient of .95. No data pertaining to validity is presented, but the results of then-current (1967) studies are scheduled to appear in the forthcoming Tegnnical flandbook. .an n gpInp,Ayerages were obtained from school records and were computed exclusive of the year during which the students participated in the study. They there- fore did not include grades received for the instrumental music classes in which they were enrolled for the duration of this study. The grades themselves reflect teachers' assessments of student academic achievement, and are based on a four-point scale, e.g., below 1.0 = failure, 4.0 = excellence. The Attitude Towards anIg test was constructed by Carter3 using the Thurstone and Chave4 "method of equal-appearing intervals.’ The scaling was performed by 2Arthur S. Otis and Roger T. Lennon, Otis-Lennon Mental Apility Test, Intennediate Leyel (New York: Harcourt, Brace and‘World, Inc., 1967). 3warrick Carter, "Ethnic Music as a Source for the Musical Development and Enrichment of Culturally Different Students in General Music Classes" (unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, Michigan State University, 1970). 4Louis Thurstone and E. J. Chave, The Measurement _pI,Attitndes (Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1929). 67 ten music specialists who were asked to judge, on an eleven-point continuum, the degree of favorableness or unfavorableness of the attitude towards music expressed by each statement. The composite results were used to obtain the scale value of each attitude statement, cal- culated by Edwards'5 "calculation of scale" formula. The test was given to all subjects during the first week of classes at the beginning of the school term. The adminis- tration was performed by a school counselor unknown to the students and during the planned absence of the classroom instructors involved in this study. All students were given the MnsIna1,AppInngn Pnofile6 battery of tests during the second week of the initial school term. The MAP is divided into three main sections: Tonal Imagery, Rhythm Imagery, and Musical Sensitivity. These sections are further subdivided into subtests. They are: Melody and Harmony for the Tonal Imagery division: Tempo and Meter for the Rhythm Imagery division: and Phrasing, Balance, and Style for the Musical 5Allen Edwards, Iegnnignes pf Anninnde Sgaleygpn- §trngtion (New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, Inc., 1957). 6E. Gordon, MnsIcaI Aptitnde ProfIle Manual (Boston: Houghton Mifflin Co., 1965). 68 Sensitivity division. The tests of Tonal and Rhythm Imagery are described as "nonpreference" tests because there is an unequivocally correct or best answer for each item contained therein. The subtests of the third divi- sion, described as a preference test, are intended as measures of musical taste and were constructed to conform with the consensus of both music students and profes- sionals. Although it is possible to obtain eleven separate scores from the battery of tests, only the single com- posite score was used in this study. The standard score scale is based on a mean of fifty, and a standard deviation of tan, although special norms are provided for various grades and categories of students. The author lists various reliability coefficients for each test by grade, generally in the .90's for the total battery. The measure of personality was the galifornia Test .pI,2e;sonalIty.7 Although the complete test includes two 'sections, Personal Adjustment and Social Adjustment, only the composite score for the test of Personal Adjustment, 7L. Thorpe, W. Clark, and E. Tiegs, galifonnia Test,p§ Personalit , 1953 Revision (Monterey, California: CTB/McGraw-Hill Book Co., 1953). 69 administered to all subjects during the first week of the experimental period, was used in this study. The items in the Personal Adjustment section measure six components of personal security: Self-Reliance, Sense of Personal WOrth, Sense of Personal Freedom, Feeling of Belonging, Withdraw- ing Tendencies, and Nervous Symptoms. The test is designed to enable teachers and school officials to identify the types of maladjustment from which a student may be suf- fering, and thus is primarily a diagnostic test. The authors quote a reliability coefficient (Kuder-Richardson) of .83 and a standard error measurement of 2.87 for the Personal Adjustment section. No validity coefficient is given: instead, the Mannal cites the results of various studies relating the test to clinical findings and other tests. The dependent variable, performance achievement, was measured by the watkins-Farnnn PerformancegnnI;8 During the final week of the term, all students were required to sight-read Form A of the Eenfonnance.§gnIg, administered in a room equipped with a tape recorder, metrenome, chair, and music stand containing the test 8John watkins and Stephen Farnum, Tne watkins- ‘EnnnnnsPeyfpnnance Sgale (Winona, Minnesota: Hal Leonard Music, Inc., 1954). 70 items. Testing procedure was as follows: 1. Student reported individually to the testing room. 2. Instructor read the directions aloud to the student, and recorded his name. 3. Instructor activated the metrenome at the tempo specified by the test sheet, turned on the tape recorder and left the room. 4. Instructor waited near the closed door until the student's performance of all items at a given metrenome marking was completed. He then entered the room, shut off the metrenome and tape recorder, and repeated step three for the next set of items. These recorded performances were scored by two judges, both senior lecturers in music at a College of Education in London, England. The judges had no knowledge of which students belonged to the experimental or control group, and thus could maintain complete impartiality. To prevent instruction from being influenced by the teacher's knowledge of students' scores on the various tests, all data concerning the subject variables were either a) gathered at the end of the experimental 71 period or b) gathered at the initial stages of instruc- tion but not scored until after the experimental period was completed. Method of Analysis At the conclusion of the experimental period, data for all students were collated. The means and standard deviations of both the experimental and control groups were computed for the subject variables of I.Q., G.P.A., Attitude Towards Music, Musical Aptitude, and Personal Adjustment. To establish the similarity between groups on the subject variables and the role of sex within groups, separate analyses of variance were computed to determine if a significant difference existed: 1. between the experimental and control groups on the subject variables of I.Q., G.P.A., atti- tude towards music, musical aptitude, and personal adjustment, 2. between males and females of each group on the subject variables, 3. for the interaction between groups and sex on all subject variables. 72 In addition, correlation matrices showing the relation- ships between the subject variables were computed for all combinations of sex and groups. The scores for the dependent variable, performance achievement, were analyzed with reference to the students' scores on the subject variables, i.e., as though they arose from a treatments-by-levels design. In other words, the performance achievement scores for both groups were assigned to trichotomized (low, medium, and high) cate- gories and subjected to separate analyses of variance in order to determine the significance of: l. the main effect of the treatments or instruc- tional conditions on performance achievement, 2. the effects of each subject variable on performance achievement, and 3. the interaction between treatments and levels. To investigate the effect of the students' sex on the dependent variable, the scores of both groups were dichotomized by sex. The resultant data of this 2 x 2 design were subjected to analysis of variance to yield F-ratios for: 1. the main effect of treatments, The investigate 1. 73 the effects of sex on the dependent variable, the interaction between treatments and sex. object of the analyses described above was to the following null hypotheses: There is no significant difference in per- formance achievement between the experimental and control groups. There is no significant difference between males and females within the experimental and control groups, respectively, in performance achievement. There is no significant difference in per- formance achievement between students of low, medium, and high I.Q. There is no significant difference in per- formance achievement between students of low, medium, and high grade point average. There is no significant difference in per- formance achievement between students of low, medium, and high attitude towards music. There is no significant difference in per- formance achievement between students of low, medium, and high musical aptitude. 10. 11. 12. 13. 74 There is no significant difference in per- formance achievement between students of low, medium, and high personal adjustment. There is no significant performance achievement treatment and sex. There is no significant performance achievement treatment and I.Q. There is no significant performance achievement treatment and G.P.A. There is no significant performance achievement interaction effect on between instructional interaction effect on between instructional interaction effect on between instructional interaction effect on between instructional treatment and attitude towards music. There is no significant performance achievement interaction effect on between instructional treatment and musical aptitude. There is no significant performance achievement interaction effect on between instructional treatment and personal adjustment. CHAPTER IV ANALYSIS OF THE DATA Analysis of Subject Variable Data The means and standard deviations of both the experimental (Pattengill Junior High School: N = 45) and control (Rich Junior High School: N = 45) groups for the subject variables of I.Q., G.P.A., Attitude (Attitude Towards Music), MAP (Musical Aptitude Profile) and Per- sonal Adjustment are listed in Table 4:1. Analyses of variance failed to locate a signifi- cant difference between the experimental and control groups for any pair of the above means (see Appendix A for ANOVA Tables). While this does not preclude the possi- bility of a true difference, it is apparent through inspection of the means that, although no attempt was made to match the samples, the groups are in fact quite evenly balanced and that the difference between the experimental and control group in terms of subject variable means is non-significant. 75 76 mm.ma mm.ma mm.om mm.mm pomspmonpe Hmoomumm 00.5 mn.m mh.am mm.om ma: Ho.m mm.m ma.ma No.5H opsuwuum Hm. be. mg.m mm.m .¢.m.o gm.va mm.ma mm.mm om.¢m .O.H Honucou HmucoEHummxm Houucoo mcwwmrmfiwuwmxm moaflmaum> pumnnsm .mmoouw Houucou pom HmucwE Ianmmxm mnu How moHQmHHm> pomnnsm mo mc0flume>on pumpcmum pom homo: .Hug magma 77 Tables 4:2 and 4:3 contain the correlation coefficients (Pearson's product-moment) between all sub- ject variables except sex. Table 4:2. Correlations of Subject Variables: Experi- mental Group. . Personal I.Q. GPA Attitude MAP, Adjustment I.Q. -- GPA .815 -- Attitude .337 .426 -- MAP .472 .400 .324 -- Personal ' Adjustment .230 .210 .312 I .217 Table 4:3. Correlations of Subject Variables: Control Group. . Personal I.Q. GPA Attitude MAP Adjustment I.Q. -- GPA .785 -- Attitude .395 .494 -- MAP .431 .391 .359 -- Pers°na1 .261 .267 .367 .266 -- Adjustment k 78 A comparison of the correlation matrices above reveals that the experimental and control groups are probably as well-matched on the subject variables as the means shown in Table 4:1 would indicate. Analysis of Subject Variables: Sex Differences Because of the large number of tables obtained by organizing each subject variable according to sex, and the possible combinations of sex and school, the means, standard deviations and analyses of variance for these data will be found in Appendix A, Tables A:l to A:10. These tables show that sex differences do not seem to play a meaningful role in determining scores on the means of the subject variables. For example, the mean I.Q. for males in the experimental group is nearly identical to: l. the mean I.Q. score for females in the experi- mental group, 2. the mean I.Q. score for males in the control group, and 3. the mean I.Q. score for females in the control group. 79 Analysis of variance revealed no significant differences between any pair of subject variable means, dichotomized by sex, within the experimental or control group. While the disclaimer concerning the danger of attributing significance to the failure in finding sig- nificance bears repeating, it may be stated with reasonable assurity that the experimental and control groups, while not equivalent, are quite evenly matched relative to the subject variables. In the absence of quantitative evidence to the contrary, inferences about the effect of treatment on the dependent variable can be made with the fair sup- position that the subject variables, by themselves, do not play dissimilar roles in the experimental and control groups. Analysis of Data Relative to Performance Achievement and Subject Variables Table 4:4 shows the performance achievement means and standard deviations for the experimental and control groups. Table 4:5 shows the correlations between performance achievement and the subject variables for both groups. 80 Table 4:4. Performance Achievement Means and Standard Deviations for Experimental and Control Groups. Experimental Control (Pattengill JHS) (Rich JHS) Means 23.22 17.11 Standard Deviation 13.68 11.23 Table 4:5. Correlations between Performance Achievement and Subject Variables. Variable Experimental Control I.Q. .673 .700 GPA .766 .634 Attitude Towards _ 054 .669 MuSic MAP .471 .582 Personal Adjustment .076 .623 The students in the experimental group showed a superiority on the dependent variable, performance achievement, of 6.11 points. In addition, the relation- ship of the dependent variable to certain criteria variables, particularly to the tests of attitude towards 81 music and personal adjustment, varied markedly between the groups. The performance achievement scores of both groups were analyzed, as stated in Chapter III, with reference to the students' scores on each subject variable, i.e., as though they arose from a treatments-by-level design. The effects of particular interest tested for significance were: 1. the main effects of Treatments, 2. the effect of each subject variable levels, on the dependent variable, and 3. the interactions between Treatments and Levels. .IIQ..nng Performance Achievemen . Figure 4:1 illustrates the performance achievement scores of the experimental and control groups, each trichotomized as to low, medium, and high I.Q. Figure 4:1 clearly shows: 1. That students in the experimental group were superior on the dependent variable to their counterparts in the control group. 2. That in both groups, students of higher I.Q. were superior on the dependent measure than students of lower I.Q. 3. The interaction between Treatments and Levels. 82 45 - m 40 - m ‘6 o 35 - m 4,3 30- o 5 25 5 A. H g 20" x d I 8 15 - .‘i E 10 - O 8 H 5 - o m 0 Low Medium High (x = experimental: o = control) I.Q. Scores Figure 4:1. Performance Achievement Means of Experimental and Control Groups at Low, Medium, and High Levels of I.Q. 83 Table 4:6 contains the results of the two-way analysis of variance for the data graphed in Fig. 4:1. Table 4:6. Analysis of Variance of Performance Achievement by Method of Instruction and Level of I.Q. Source of Degrees of Sum of Mean F Variance Freedom Squares Square Statistic Treatments 1 840 840 8.92** I.Q. Levels 2 5005 2502.5 26.57*** Interaction 2 962 481 5.11* Within 84 7911 94.18 Total 89 14,718 *Significant beyond the .025 level of confidence. **Significant beyond the .01 level of confidence. ***Significant beyond the .001 level of confidence. The null hypothesis that there is no significant difference in performance achievement between students receiving individual instruction within a class and stu- dents receiving ensemble class instruction is rejected. The F value of 8.92 exceeds the .01 level of confidence. The null hypothesis that there is no significant difference in performance achievement between students of 84 low, medium, and high I.Q. is rejected. The F value of 26.57 exceeds the .001 level of confidence. The null hypothesis that there is no significant interaction effect on performance achievement between method of instruction and level of I.Q. is rejected. The F value of 5.11 exceeds the .01 level of confidence. .andp,EpI p Avera e nng Eerfonnange Achievemenp. Figure 4:2 illustrates the performance achievement scores of the experimental and control group, each trichotomized as to low, medium, and high GPA. Besides the main effect established earlier, the graph shows the proportional relationship of GPA to the scores of all students on the dependent variable, per- formance achievement. The distinct interaction between Treatments and Levels seen in Figure 4:1 is less distinct in this instance. Table 4:7 contains the results of the two-way analysis of variance for the data graphed in Figure 4:2. The null hypothesis that there is no significant difference in performance achievement between students of low, medium, and high GPA is rejected. The F value of 35.47 exceeds the .001 level of confidence. 85 45 - m 40 - o ‘6 u 35 - x m 230- w 5 >25- . w 2 o g 15 - x E10- 8 H 5 - m m 0 Low Medium High (x = experimental; o = control) Grade Point Average Scores Figure 4:2. Performance Achievement Means of Experimental and Control Groups, at Low, Medium, and High Levels of Grade Point Average. 86 Table 4:7. Analysis of Variance of Performance Achievement by Method of Instruction and Level of GPA. Source of Degrees of Sum of Mean F Variance Freedom Squares Square Statistic Treatments 1 840 840 9.56* GPA Levels 2 6233 3116.5 35.47** Interaction 2 264 132 1.5 Within 84 7381 87.87 -- Total 89 14,718 -- -- *Significant beyond the .01 level of confidence. **Significant beyond the .001 level of confidence. The null hypothesis that there is no significant interaction effect on performance achievement between method of instruction and level of GPA is accepted. The F value of 1.5 does not reach the .05 level of con- fidence. Athtnde Towards M2§1£.§DQ Eegfonnance Achieve- .mpnp. Figure 4:3 illustrates the performance achievement scores of the experimental and control groups, each trichotomized as to low, medium, and high levels of attitude. 87 45 - 40 - 35 - 10 - Performance Achievement Scores A) o I X7 Low Medium High (x = experimental; o = control) Attitude Towards Music Scores Figure 4:3. Performance Achievement Means of Experimental and Control Groups at Low, Medium, and High Levels of Attitude Towards Music. 88 While the performance achievement scores of stu- dents in the control group are clearly related to their test of attitude towards music, a nearly random relation- ship (see Table 4:4) is apparent between the two variables for students in the experimental group. Table 4:8 con- tains the results of the two-way analysis of variance for the data graphed in Figure 4:3. Table 4:8. Analysis of Variance of Performance Achieve- ment by Method of Instruction and Level of Attitude Towards Music. Source of Degrees of Sum of Mean F Variance Freedom Squares Square Statistic Treatments 1 840 840 5.87* Attitude Levels 2 773 386.5 2.70 Interaction 2 1092 564 3.82** Within 84 12,013 143.01 -- Total 89 14,718 -- -- *Significant beyond the .025 level of confidence. **Significant beyond the .05 level of confidence. The null hypothesis that there is no significant difference in performance achievement between students of low, medium, and high attitude towards music is 89 accepted. The F value of 2.70 does not reach the .05 level of confidence. The null hypothesis that there is no significant interaction effect on performance achievement between method of instruction and level of attitude towards music is rejected. The F value of 3.82 exceeds the .05 level of confidence. Musical Aptitude nng Performance Achievement. Figure 4:4 illustrates the performance achievement scores of the experimental and control groups, each trichotomized as to low, medium, and high levels of MAP. The relationship of musical aptitude to performance achievement is well-defined for both groups, and it appears to have played a similar role for each condition of instruction. Table 4:9 contains the results of the two-way analysis of variance for the data graphed in Figure 4:4. The null hypothesis that there isrx>significant difference in performance achievement between students of low, medium, and high musical aptitude is rejected. The F value of 10.90 exceeds the .01 level of confidence. The null hypothesis that there is no significant interaction effect on performance achievement between 90 45 - 8 40 - H 8 35 - m E 30 - .__x 95’ x—— $25- .2 o 'c' 20' / 2 . 8 15 - S E 10 - ' o '44 H 5- o m 0 Low Medium High (x = experimental; o = control) Musical Aptitude Scores Figure 4:4. Performance Achievement Means of Experimental and Control Groups at Low, Medium, and High Levels of Musical Aptitude. 91 Table 4:9. Analysis of Variance of Performance Achievement by Method of Instruction and Level of MAP. Source of Degrees of Sum of Mean F Variance Freedom Squares Square Statistic Treatments 1 840 840 6.44* MAP Levels 2 2844 1422 10.90** Interaction 2 78 36 .27 Within 84 10,956 130.41 -- Totals 89 14,718 -- -- *Significant beyond the .025 level of confidence. **Significant beyond the .01 level of confidence. method of instruction and level of musical aptitude is accepted. The F value of .27 does not reach the .05 level of confidence. Eersonal Adjustment nng Pergonnange Aghieve- .EEEE- Figure 4:5 illustrates the performance achievement scores of the experimental and control groups, each trichotomized as to low, medium, and high levels of personal adjustment. The relationship of personal adjustment to per- formance achievement is evident for all students in the control group and for students at medium and high levels 92 45 - 3340- H 8 m 35 - 'a’ g 30 - x o g 25 - x-‘\-----~ o H fizo- x r o 815- C m E10- 0 H m 5 - m 0 Low Medium High (x = experimental; o = control) Personal Adjustment Scores Figure 4:5. Performance Achievement Means of Experimental and Control Groups at Low, Medium, and High Levels of Personal Adjustment. 93 of personal adjustment in the experimental group. The interaction of Treatments and Levels is clearly shown by the disparity of performance achievement scores at the lowest level of personal adjustment. Table 4:10 contains the results of the two-way analysis of variance for the data graphed in Figure 4:5. Table 4:10. Analysis of Variance of Performance Achieve- ment by Method of Instruction and Level of Personal Adjustment. Source of Degrees of Sum of Mean F Variance Freedom Squares Square Statistic Treatments 1 840 840 6.78* Personal Adjustment 2 2,015 1,007.50 8.13** Levels Interaction 2 1,452 726 5.86** Within 84 10,411 123.94 -- Totals 89 14,718 -- -- *Significant beyond the .025 level of confidence. **Significant beyond the .01 level of confidence. The null hypothesis that there is no significant difference in performance achievement between students of :94 low, medium, and high personal adjustment is rejected. The F value of 8.13 exceeds the .01 level of confidence. The null hypothesis that there is no significant interaction effect on performance achievement between method of instruction and level of personal adjustment is rejected. The F value of 5.86 exceeds the .01 level of confidence. jgpxmnng Performance Achieyemen . The sex of the students played a minimal role in performance achievement, and no interaction of Treatment and sex was apparent. Table 4:11 contains the results of the two-way analysis of variance of performance achievement by method of instruc- tion and sex. Table 4:11. Analysis of Variance of Performance Achieve- ment by Method of Instruction and Sex. Source of Degrees of Sum of Mean F Variance Freedom Squares Square Statistic Treatments 1 840 840 5.20* Sex 1 5.00 5.00 .030 Interaction l 3.00 3.00 .018 Within 86 13,870 161.40 -- Totals 89 14,718 -- -- *Significant beyond the .025 level of confidence. 95 The null hypothesis that there is no significant difference in performance achievement between male and female students is accepted. The F value of .030 does not meet the .05 level of confidence. The null hypothesis that there is no significant interaction effect on performance achievement between treatments and sex is accepted. The F value of .018 does not reach the .05 level of confidence. CHAPTER V SUMMARY, FINDINGS, DISCUSSION, CONCLUSIONS AND RECOMMENDATIONS Summary This study investigated the effect of tutorial instruction within the music classroom on the performance achievement of male and female instrumental beginners relative to their varying levels of I.Q., academic achieve- ment, attitude towards music, musical aptitude, and per- sonal adjustment. The main hypothesis was that beginning instrumentalists of both sexes who are taught individually within the classroom will earn significantly higher scores on performance achievement than their counterparts under ensemble instruction. It was further hypothesized that students of high I.Q., grade point average, attitude towards music, musical aptitude, and personal adjustment will earn significantly higher scores on performance achievement than students of low I.Q., GPA, attitudes 96 97 towards music, musical aptitude, and personal adjustment. Finally, it was hypothesized that a significant effect on performance achievement would occur as a result of the interaction between the conditions of instruction and the five subject variables listed above. A review of literature related to individualized instruction cited educators' early attempts to structure programs for individual differences. Music education research in this area has centered almost exclusively on the techniques and devices associated with programmed learning, and only a few studies have investigated the efficacy of individualized instruction within the class- room or on a tutorial basis. Nearly all researchers using programmed materials report the superiority of that system over conventional methods of teaching cognitive, percep- tual, or performance skills. Most studies dealing with individualized learning have investigated the private lesson vs. class mode of instruction, and have been so damaged by problems of interval validity that it was dif- ficult for the authors to draw firm conclusions from the data. The exception, Froseth's1 study, found that 1James O. Froseth, "Using MAP Scores in the Instruction of Beginning Students in Instrumental Music," .JQB£B§1.2£ Aesea;ch.In_Mnsic Ed atio , XIV (Spring, 1971), 98-101. 98 educational and statistically significant advantages were realized by providing beginning instrumental students with texts and instructional procedures designed to take into account individual differences. A number of correlative studies measuring the rela- tionship between successful participation in instrumental programs and intelligence, academic achievement, musical aptitude, and personality variables were reviewed. They indicate that these variables all bear some positive rela- tionship to playing skills, and that intelligence and academic achievement are the most closely related to per- formance achievement. The author of the Musical AptItnde ErofIle and his colleagues from the University of Iowa, however, have presented the results of experiments which show the MAE to be the most effective predictor of instru- mental skill. No trustworthy data was found concerning attitude towards music and its relationship to attainment of proficiency. For the present experiment, two groups of subjects Comprising the total enrollment of beginning instru- mentalists (less three students with prior instrumental training) from two Lansing, Michigan schools participated in the study. Students in the experimental group (N = 45) 99 were dispersed about the classroom for the purpose of self-drill. They exclusively received tutorial instruc- tion and evaluation only upon their request, or when they wished to progress to a succeeding unit (page) in their text. Later, when a student had acquired the cognitive and aural skill necessary for competent self-evaluation, he was allowed to proceed from unit to unit at his own discretion. Students in the control group were taught by the "ensenble" method in which the rate of unit progres- sion is governed for all students by the teacher, and which is characterized by a surfeit of repetition for gifted students and an inadequate amount for those less talented. Both groups used the same text, and the period encompassing the study was one school year. Data concerning I.Q., grade point average, attitude towards music, musical aptitude profile, and personal adjustment were obtained for both groups from school reCords or by pre-testing. Scores on the dependent variable, performance achievement, were obtained by record- ing each student's performance on the W‘m Performance Scale, and submitting these recordings to an independent panel of judges. 100 Findings Correlation coefficients were computed for the subject variables of I.Q., GPA, attitude towards music, musical aptitude (MAP), and personal adjustment. The similarities of these relationships between the experi- mental and control groups, the failure of analyses of variance to establish any significance for the small dif- ferences between paired means, and the lack of any significant differences when those variables were dichotomized by sex, led to the conclusion that the subject variables, by themselves, would not account for differences in scores on the dependent variable of per- formance achievement. The hypotheses tested were divided into three main categories: 1) the effect of individual instruction on performance achievement, 2) the effect of I.Q., GPA, attitude towards music, musical aptitude, and personal adjustment on performance achievement, and 3) the effect on performance achievement of the interaction between instructional treatment and levels of the subject vari- ables. The results of the analyses, stated in terms of null hypotheses, were as follows: 101 There is no significant difference in perfor- mance achievement between the experimental and control groups. Rejected. There is no significant difference between males and females within the experimental and control groups, respectively, in performance achievement. Accepted. There is no significant difference in perfor- mance achievement between students of low, medium, and high I.Q. Rejected. There is no significant difference in perfor- mance achievement between students of low, medium, and high grade point average. Rejected. There is no significant difference in perfor- mance achievement between students of low, medium, and high attitude towards music. Accepted. There is no significant difference in perfor- mance achievement between students of low, medium, and high musical aptitude. Rejected. There is no significant difference in perfor- mance achievement between students of low, medium, and high personal adjustment. Rejected. 102 8. There is no significant interaction effect on performance achievement between instructional treatment and sex. Accepted. 9. There is no significant interaction effect on performance achievement between instructional treatment and I.Q. Rejected. 10. There is no significant interaction effect on performance achievement between instructional treatment and GPA. Accepted. 11. There is no significant interaction effect on performance achievement between instructional treatment and attitude towards music. Rejected. 12. There is no significant interaction effect on performance achievement between instructional treatment and musical aptitude. Accepted. 13. There is no significant interaction effect on performance achievement between instructional treatment and personal adjustment. Rejected. Discussion ‘IhpuMnIn EIfegt.pI Treatments. This study has questioned, as have many music educators, the efficiency of the ensemble method of instruction for Classes of beginning instrumentalists. Such classes involve an 103 educationally intolerable amount of repetitive drill which is superfluous to the learning needs of superior students. Conversely, the same drill may be inappropriate in quality or quantity to the different requirements of slower students. Referring to the latter, it is probable that the redundancy inherent in this method is a vehicle for a permanent dependence upon rote learning, and thus promotes varying degrees of musical illiteracy in certain students. It seems obvious that the members of a class are faced with differing musical and personal obstacles in their quest for performance facility. The teacher's ability to perceive, evaluate, and prescribe a remedy for each student's particular impediment is severely limited by the simultaneous rendition of an identical line of music. Admittedly, the teacher may focus upon a general weakness common to all beginning instrumentalists. He may even occasionally isolate and deal effectively with a single student's particular handicap. The pressure of an idle class, however, insures that the latter event will occur infrequently. It is a plausible hypothesis that individual instruction allows the teacher to remedy the deficiencies 104 discussed above, thus accounting for the effect of the main treatment variable used in this study. The students were presumably able to make efficient use of their class time by limiting or increasing the amount of drill neces- sary for the achievement of a satisfactory level of per- formance. The teacher was presumably able to accurately diagnose and remedy musical defects for each student by requiring a formal, aural evaluation before the student was allowed to proceed to the next unit. By requiring that evaluation, the teacher could directly influence the criteria against which each student measured himself. Of more significance was the fact that each student was forced to evaluate his playing begone asking the teacher to hear the page: the request itself was an indication that the student had already engaged in self-diagnoses and improvements. This process is in marked contrast to the musical dependencies fostered by due exclusively external evaluation inherent in the ensemble method. Finally, it is presumed that the tutorial method allowed the teacher to adjust tuition to meet the personal and differing needs of each student. The role played by 105 some of these factors in performance achievement is dis- cussed below. Inn, And Perfonnance Anhieyemen . Correlative studies by other researchers have consistently shown a high positive relationship between intelligence and musi- cal achievement. The significance found in this experiment between levels of I.Q. and performance achievement is therefore not surprising. However, the interaction* found between levels of intelligence and method of instruction leads to the tentative assumption that bright students receiving ensemble instruction are being prevented from nearing the limits of their potential. It is certainly possible that these students become frustrated when forced to perform seemingly endless repetitions of exercises played for the benefit of more mediocre classmates. It is suggested that frustration leads to boredom, and that boredom reduces motivation which in turn partially accounts for the discrepancy between the two groups' brighter students. * See Figure 4:1, page 82. 106 It is interesting to note that Pu0polo2 found an opposite interaction when using programmed materials: students of below-average I.Q. in his experimental group registered the highest scores on the wntkins-Farnum Eenfonnance,§gnIn, even higher than above-average I.Q. students in the same group, while students in his control group under conditions of instruction similar to this study fall into the same pattern of I.Q. and achievement observed in the control group here. It therefore appears that programmed learning will be of greatest benefit to the less intelligent, while individual instruction aids those who are gifted. One possible reason for this phenomenon may be that programmed materials provide learning strategies to those who are incapable of deve10ping their own effective modes of learning, while individual instruction allows or releases bright students to get on with the business in which they excel, i.e., finding their own efficient methods of learn- ing. Puopolo's study raises another interesting question: 2Vito Pu0polo, "The DevelOpment and Experimental Application of Self-Instructional Practice Materials for Beginning Instrumentalists," (unpublished Ph.D. disserta- tion, Michigan State University, 1970). 107 Does programmed practice deppIye bright students of learn- ing strategies, thereby lowering their scores on tests of performance achievement? .QBA.§DQ Perfonmance Achieyenen . While a stu- dent's grade point average is not, strictly defined, a personal attribute, it may be partially considered an indication of a willingness or reluctance to achieve scholastic success. Viewed in that light, the significant differences relating to performance achievement between levels of GPA were predictable. The somewhat disappoint- ing failure to find a significant interaction between instructional treatments and levels of GPA may indicate a failure on the part of the experimental instructor to properly motivate students with lower scholastic aspira- tions. It may also reveal the degree to which such students' motivations are resistant to change. Attinnde Towards Mnsic.nng Penformance Aghieye- .mpnp. The most striking disparity between the experimental and control groups centered on the relationship of attitude to performance achievement. While levels of attitude did not appear to account for significant differences in per- formance achievement, the relationship between the two variables in the control group (r = .669) was apparently 108 masked by the nearly random (r = .054) relationship found in the experimental group. This was reflected in the sig- nificant interaction found between levels and treatments. It is difficult to accurately account for this phenomenon. The instrument used for measuring attitude towards music may have been too unreliable or crude to consistently and validly measure the variable. Considering its extensive preparation and use by its author in widely differing geographic locations, however, there is little evidence by which its findings can be refuted. Alternative hypotheses must lie within the experi- mental and/or control classrooms. A student's attitude towards music must influence to some extent his attitude toward the music class. The latter will be perceived by the instructor and his attitude towards that student will be influenced accordingly. A student can quite easily perceive a teacher's attitude towards him, and may change his behavior in class with results amenable or detrimental to his musical achievement. In other words, the differing attitudinal effects on performance achievement found in this study may be a result of differences between instructors. Although nearly all teachers strive to accept without bias the 109 personal qualities of their students, they meet with varied success in doing so. The extent to which a teacher can accept a student's attitude plays some role in determining that student's achievement. It is probable that the dif- ferences discussed here between the experimental and con- trol groups reflect the differing ability of each group's teacher to accept or ignore the "negative" attitudes of some students. Musigal AptitndeIAnd Peyfopnange AchIeyemen . The effect of musical aptitude on performance achievement was not unexpected, and it duplicated the results of virtually every research study extant which has compared the two variables. The reasons for the influence of aptitude upon achievement seem obvious and need no explanation here. In agreement with Froseth's3 findings, however, this study found that no particular advantage could be claimed for the individual instruction of students with differing levels of musical aptitude. These results would seem to indicate that musical aptitude seeks its outlet in behavior regardless of instructional conditions. 3Froseth,.pp. git. 110 Bensonal Adjustment and Eerfpzmance Achieyenen . The results show that students at higher levels of per- sonal adjustment attain significantly higher levels of performance achievement. More importantly, the signifi- cant interaction Of treatments and levels at the lowest level of personal adjustment holds serious implications for teachers who continue to use the ensemble method of instruc- tion. Students of low personal adjustment who receive individual instruction are able to attain virtually iden- tical results as students of high personal adjustment, while their counterparts in the control group rank lowest in performance achievement. This finding lends credence to the supposition that individual instruction may allow a teacher to give support and encouragement to students in areas other than musical, and prevent personal handicaps from adversely affecting their attainment. Conversely, it is an indictment of uniform teaching methods which apparently prevent students from reaching potential levels of success. MgIse. It is obvious that the experimental classrooms were noisy. Stated more precisely, those class- rooms were continually cacophonous; measured in decibels and given an equal number of students for both kinds of 111 classes, however, the level of sound in each would be nearly identical. Noise, of course, is an impediment to perception, but only related to the students' ability to screen it out. The seriousness of this issue must be determined by the results of this experiment, and other factors have apparently more than compensated for its assumed disadvantage. Perhaps the best indication of its influence is that no student ever complained of it. Conclusions Based upon the analysis of the data from the investigation, the following conclusions are admissible: 1. When junior high school beginning instru- mentalists are instructed by the individual methods described earlier in this study, a moderate increase in performance achievement is noted. 2. Students of higher I.Q., grade point average, musical aptitude and personal adjustment exhibit significantly greater performance achievement than students of lower levels for the same attributes. 112 Individually-instructed students of the highest level of I.Q. and lowest levels of attitude towards music and personal adjustment receive the greater benefits, in terms of performance achievement, from that mode of instruction. Implications The adOption of an individual instruction method as described in this study would have the following impli- cations for instrumental music education: 1. A more efficient, rapid growth in the perfor- mance achievement of individuals together with an increase of performance capabilities for junior high school instrumental groups may be attained. The development of musical self-reliance and realistic self-evaluation would presumably be encouraged. The drop-out of bright students from instru- mental music programs cited by Kruth4 may be 4E. C. Kruth, "Student DroP-out in Instrumental Music in the Secondary Schools of Oakland, California," (unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, Stanford University, 1964). l. 113 reduced by minimizing the repetition and resultant boredom experienced by those stu- dents. Opportunities for the teacher to provide for individual differences, both musical and non- musical, will be greatly increased. Recommendations A similar investigation, using programmed material, such as those conducted by Puopolo,5 together with the use of varied and appro- priate instructional texts, is recommended. These additions may particularly influence the achievement of students of lower intelligence and ability. A study should be made at other grade levels of instrumental study to determine whether effects noted herein will similarly appear. 5Puopolo,,pp. git. BIBLIOGRAPHY BIBLIOGRAPHY Books Atkinson, Caroll, and Maleska, Eugene. .Ihp Stony p; Edncation. New York: Bantam Books, 1962. Billet, Roy 0. Ihe Administnation and SnpenyisIon.pfi figmpgeneonslgnpanng. Ohio State University Press, 1932. BishOp, Lloyd K. Indiyignalizing Edncational Systems. New York: Harper and Row, 1971. Carter, Launor F. "Computers: Their Impact on Instruc- tion, on Educational Planning, and on the Cur- riculum. " Cited in Lloyd K. Bishop, Indiyidnalinny’ Edncational Systems. New York: Harper and Row, 1971. Cornell, E. L. "Effects of Ability Grouping Determinable from Published Studies," Ihn,Ahility G;onping.gfi ganl . Thirty-fifth Yearbook of the National Society for the Study of Education, Part I. Bloomington, 111.: Public School Publishing Co., 1936. Douglas, J. W. B. fle figmn and the SchooI: A Study o_f Ahility And AttnI‘nment In the Pm ary Sghppl . London: MacGibbon and Kee, 1964. Duerkson, George. ‘IpnghIng Instrumental anIg. ‘Washing- ton, D.C.: MENC, 1972. Edwards, Allen. Teghnignes‘pI,AntItnde Scale gonstrng- tion. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, Inc., 1957. 114 115 Elliot, Charles W; "Shortening and Enriching the Grammar School Course." Charles ElIiot and Popnlar Education. Edited by Edward Krug. New York: Teachers College Press, 1961. Frost, S. E., Jr. History pp Edugation. WOodbury, New York: Barrows Educational Series, Inc., 1947. Gibbons, Maurice. IndiyigualIzeg Instnnction. New York: Teachers College Press, 1971. Glaser, Robert. Adapting the Elementany Sghool QupnIculum .EQ Individual Performance. Pittsburgh: Univer- sity of Pittsburgh Learning Research and Develop- ment Center, Reprint 26, 1967. Good, Carter 3., ed. Dictionary pg Edngation. 2nd ed. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1959. Goodland, J. I. "Classroom Organization." Engyglopedia .pI EdugatIonal Research. Edited by C. W. Harris. New York: MacMillan and Co., 1960. Guilford, J. P. Ihe Nature.pI Human Intelligence. New York: McGraw-Hill, Inc., 1967. Harap, H. "Differentiation of Curriculum Practices and Instruction in Elementary Schools.“- Ihp groupIng ,gI Pupils. Edited by G. M. Whipple. Thirty-fifth Yearbook of the National Society for the Study of Education, Part I. Bloomington, Ill.: Public. School Publishing Co., 1936. Harris, Chester W., ed. EncyclOpedia.pI EducatIonal Aesearch. New York: Macmillan Co., 1960. Komensky, Jon A. The Analytical DidactIc,pI Comenius. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1953. Minor, Frances. "A Child Goes Forth: Ideas Invite Involvement." Individualizing Instruction. Edited by R. C. Dell. washington, D.C.: Asso- ciation for Supervision and Curriculum Development, National Education Association, 1964. 116 Noffsinger, John S. gorrespondence Schools, L ceums, Chantaguas. New York: Macmillan Co., 1926. Parkhurst, Helen. Edngntion.pn the palton Elan. New York: Dutton, 1922. Postlewaith, S. N., and Novak, Joseph D. "The Use of 8mm Loop Films in Individualized Instruction," Annals .9; the Mew Iork Agademy.gI Science. New York: The New York Academy of Science, 1967. Rouse, W. H. D. (trans.). gneat Dialogues pn Plato. Mentor Books. New York: The New American Library, 1956. Scanlon, Robert G., and Belvin, John O. Individnally Erescribed Instruction. Philadelphia: Research for Better Schools, Inc., 1969. Shane, H. G. ”The School and Individual Differences." IndividualizIng Instrugtign. Edited by Nelson B. Henry. Sixty-first Yearbook of the National Society for the Study of Education. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press, 1962. Skinner, B. F. Ihe Teghnology g; IeaghIng. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1968. Thomas, R. M., and Thomas, S. M. IndiyidnaI Eifferences .In the Classroom. New York: McKay Company, Inc., 1965. Thurstone, Louis L., and Chave, E. J. The Measnnnnent.p§ Attitndes. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1929. Wallach, M. A., and Kogan, N. Modes.gI Ihinhing‘In_Xonng .QhIId;n_. New York: Holt and Co., 1965. Washburn, Carleton, and Marland, Sidney P., Jr. EInnetka: .Ihp fiispg;y.nng Signifigance.pgmnn,EdncatIpnal .EKEQELEQDE- Englewood Cliffs, New Jersey: Prentice-Hall, 1963. 117 Watkins, John. Objentive Measure pi Instrumental Perfor- mance. New York: Teacher's College Bureau of Publications, Columbia University, 1942. Whipple, Guy M., ed. Adapting nhp_Schools pp Indiyidual Differences. Twenty-fourth Yearbook of the National Society for the Study of Education, Part II. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press, 1925. Wyndham, H. S. Apility Gronping. Melbourne: Melbourne University Press, 1934. Yates, Alfred, ed. grouping in Edncation. New York: John Wiley and Sons, Inc., 1966. Manuals Gordon, E. Musical Aptitude Erofile Manual. Boston: Houghton Mifflin Co., 1965. Otis, Arthur S., and Lennon, Roger T. Otis-Lennon.Menta1 Ability Test, Innermediape hevel. New York: Harcourt, Brace and World, Inc., 1967. Thorpe, L., Clark, W., and Tiegs, E. galifornia Test,pi Personality, 1953 Aeyision. Monterey, California: California Test Bureau/McGraw Hill Co., 1953. Watkins, John G., and Farnum, Stephen E. The flatkins- Farnnn Eerformance Scale. Winona, Minnesota: Hal Leonard Music, Inc., 1954. Periodicals Adams, E. N. "Computer-Assisted Instruction." ers nnggAutgnation, (March, 1966), 17-18. Ashford, T. H. A. "The Use of Programmed Instruction to Teach Fundamental Concepts in Music Theory." gournal,pi Aeseargh‘in,Mnsic Education, XIII (Winter, 1965), 171-177. 118 Daniels, J. C. "The Effects of Streaming in the Primary Schools." The Aritish Journal‘pi Ps hol , XXXI (July, 1961), 75-81. Froseth, James 0. "Using MAP Scores in the Instruction of Beginning Students in Instrumental Music." Journal pi Aesearch in Music Edncation, XIV (Spring, 1971), 98-101. Gordon, Edwin. "A Study of the Efficacy of General Intelligence and Musical Aptitude Tests in Pre- dicting Achievement in Music." Qouncil pi Beseargh in_Musig Educatio , Bulletin NO. 13 (Spring, 1968), 40-45. . "The Musical Aptitude Profile." Music Educatons gounnal, 53:6 (February, 1967), 52-55. Hargiss, Genevieve F. "The Acquisition of Sight Singing Ability in Piano Classes for Students Preparing to be Elementary Teachers." Journa1.gi Researgh in Music Education, X (Spring, 1962), 69-75. LaBach, Parker. "A Device to Facilitate Learning of Basic Music Skill." gonncil Ior Research in_Music Education, Bulletin No. 4 (Winter, 1965), 7-10. Mayer-Oakes, G. H. ”The Dalton Plan in a Small High School." The Edngation Journal, No. 57 (December, 1936), 244-248. Miller, W. S., and Otto, M. J. "Analysis of Experimental Studies in Homogeneous Grouping." The Journal_pi Educational Aesearch, XXI (Jan.-May, 1930), 76-101. Millman, J., and Johnson, M. "Relation of Section Variance to Achievement Gains in English and Mathematics in Grades Seven and Eight." American Egncation Aesearch Journal, I (1964), 42-53. Noble, Robert F. "Critique." gonncil.gi Aesearch in Mnsig Ed cation, No. 21 (Summer, 1970), 47-51. 119 Pearse, B. H. "The Postman is the Proctor." Ameripan Education, III (February, 1967), 10-12. Rock, Robert T. "Critical Study of Current Practices in Ability Grouping." Educational Aeseangh Aulletin, IV,V,VI (May-June, 1929), 3-17. Rubin-Rabson, Grace. "Studies in the Psychology of Memorizing Piano Music VI: A Comparison of Two Forms of Mental Rehearsing and Keyboard Over- learning." Agurnal pi Edncational Psypholpgy, xxxrr (June, 1941), 593-602. Schneider, Erwin, and Cady, Henry. "Evaluation and Synthesis of Research Studies Relating to Music Education: 1930-1962." gpuncil.pi Aesearch in Mnsic Education, Bulletin NO. 9 (Spring, 1967), 4-9. Search, Preston. "The Pueblo Plan of Individual Teaching." Educational Re iew, VIII (June, 1894), 154-158. Small, T. S. "The Evaluation of Clarinet Tone Through the Use Of Oscillosc0pic Transparencies." Journa1,gi Research inansic Education, XV (Spring, 1967), 11-22. Thompson, W. H. "Experiment with the Dalton Plan." Ihe gonnnal pi Education Aesearch, XXVI (Sept.-May, 1933), 493-500. Turney, A. H. "The Status of Ability Grouping." Edncation Administration and Su ervision, XVII (January- February, 1931), 126-127. Underhill, R. J. "Experience of Scarsdale with Individual Instruction." new Ionh State Edncation JO a1, XVII (March, 1931), 677-678. Texts ‘Weber, Fred. Fingt QiyipionIAnngiMetho . Rockville Center, Long Island: Belwin, 1962. 120 Unpublished.Material Appleby, B. C. "The Effects of Individualized Reading on Certain Aspects of Literature Study with High School Seniors." Unpublished Doctoral dissertation, University of Iowa, 1967. Armstrong, W. H. "An Experimental Investigation of the Instructional Effectiveness of Published Pro- grammed Instructional Materials vs. Individualized Instruction in Area Vocational-Technical Schools." Unpublished Doctoral dissertation, The Florida State University, 1967. Becker, W. R. "The Effect of Overlearning, Initial Learn- ing Ability and Review Upon the Musical Memory of Junior High School Cornet and Trumpet Players." Unpublished Doctoral dissertation, State Univer- sity of Iowa, 1962. Bergen, Hal. "A Study of DrOp-Outs in Instrumental Music in Five Selected High Schools in Michigan." Unpublished Doctoral dissertation, Michigan State University, 1957. Boley, D. M. "The Development and Evaluation of the Effectiveness of a Pilot Instructional Sound-Film for Teaching Beginning Trumpet Students with a Brief Survey of the History of Trumpet Instruc- tion and the History of Educational Film Research." Unpublished Doctoral dissertation, University of Iowa, 1970. Carlsen, James C. "An Investigation of Programmed Learn- ing in Melodic Dictation by Means of a Teaching Machine using a Branching Technique of Program- ming." Unpublished Doctoral dissertation, Northwestern University, 1962. Carter, W. "Ethnic Music as a Source for the Musical Development and Enrichment of Culturally Different Students in General Music Classes." Unpublished Doctoral dissertation, Michigan State University, 1970. 121 Cooley, John C. "A Study of the Relation Between Certain Mental and Personality Traits and Ratings of Musical Abilities." Unpublished Doctoral dis- sertation, University of Michigan, 1952. Corno, Guy. "Relationship Between the Degree of Intel- ligence and Performance on a Test of Musical Aptitude." Unpublished Masters thesis, Dusquesne University, 1958. Cramer, William. ”The Relation of Maturation and Other Factors to Achievement in Beginning Instrumental Music Performance at the 4th through 8th Grade Levels." Unpublished Doctoral dissertation, Florida State University, 1958. Difronzo, R. F. "A Comparison of Tachistosc0pic and Con- ventional Methods in Teaching Grade Three Music Sight-Playing on a Melody Wired Instrument." Unpublished Doctoral dissertation, University of COnnecticut, 1966. Farnum, Stephen. "Prediction of Success in Instrumental Music.” Unpublished Doctoral dissertation, Harvard University, 1950. Froseth, James O. "Individualizing Instruction in the Beginning Music Class." Unpublished paper presented at the North Central Division MENC Convention, Research Section, 1971. Kaplan, Lionel. "The Relationship Between Certain Per- sonality Characteristics and Achievement in Instrumental Music." Unpublished Doctoral disser- tation, New York University, 1961. Kruth, E. C. "Student DrOp-out in Instrumental Music in the Secondary Schools of Oakland, California." Unpublished Doctoral dissertation, Stanford University, 1964. 122 McCarley, W. W. "An Experimental Study to Evaluate the Effectiveness of an Individualized Instructional Method and Lecture-Discussion Method of Teaching Vocational Agriculture Classes." Unpublished Doctoral dissertation, Michigan State University, 1969. Mitchum, John. ”The Wing 'Standardized Tests of Musical Intelligence': An Investigation of Predictability with Selected Seventh Grade Beginning Band Stu- dents." Unpublished Doctoral dissertation, Florida State University, 1969. Pruitt, Jack S. "A Study of Withdrawals in the Beginning Instrumental Music Programs of Selected Schools in the School District of Greenville, South Carolina." Unpublished Doctoral dissertation, New York University, 1966. Puopolo, Vito. "The Development and Experimental Applica- tion of Self-Instructional Practice Materials for Beginning Instrumentalists." Unpublished Doctoral dissertation, Michigan State University, 1970. Rhoades, Fordyce L. "An Evaluation of Measures for the Prediction of Success in Instrumental Study." Unpublished Masters thesis, University of Washington, 1938. Schleuter, Stanley L. "An Investigation of the Inter- relation of Personality Traits, Musical Aptitude and Musical Achievement." Unpublished Doctoral dissertation, University of Iowa, 1971. Shugert, James M. "An Experimental Investigation of Heterogeneous Class and Private Methods of Instruc- tion with Beginning Instrumental Music Students." Unpublished Doctoral dissertation, University of Illinois, 1969. Spohn, C. L. "An Exploration in the Use of Recorded Teaching Material to Develop Aural Comprehension in College Music Classes." Unpublished Doctoral dissertation, Ohio State University, 1959. 123 Thayer, Robert W. "An Investigation of the Interrelation of Personality Traits, Musical Achievement, and Different Measures of Musical Aptitude." Unpub- lished Doctoral dissertation, University of Iowa, 1971. Waa, Loren Roger. "An Experimental Study of Class and Private Methods of Instruction in Instrumental Music." Unpublished Doctoral dissertation, University of Illinois, 1965. Wardian, J. F. "An Experiment Concerning the Effective- ness of Programmed Learning for Use in Teaching the Fundamentals of Music." Unpublished Doctoral dissertation, Washington State University, 1963. Young, William Thomas. "An Investigation of the Relative and Combined Power of Musical Aptitude, General Intelligence, and.Academic Achievement Tests to Predict Musical Attainment." Unpublished Doctoral dissertation, University of Iowa, 1969. APPENDIX A 124 APPENDIX.A ANALYSES OF VARIANCE FOR THE CRITERION VARIABLES OF I.Q., GPA, ATTITUDE TOWARDS MUSIC, MUSICAL APTITUDE, AND PERSONAL ADJUSTMENT, BY SCHOOL AND SEX Table Azl. I.Q. Means and Standard Deviations for Males and Females, Pattengill JHS and Rich JHS. Pattengill gas Rich gas Mean SD Mean SD Males 34.10 18.05 35.50 14.00 Females 35.30 18.49 36.92 15.51 Table A:2. Analysis of Variance of I.Q. Scores by School and Sex. Source df SS MS F p Schools 1 49.88 49.88 .175 N.S. Sex 1 38.02 38.02 .133 N.S. Schools x Sex 1 .26 .26 .001 N.S. Within 86 24,522.17 285.14 Total 89 24,610.33 125 Table A:3. GPA Means and Standard Deviations for Males and Females, Pattengill JHS and Rich JHS. Pattengill gas Rich JHS Mean SD Mean SD Males 2.36 .76 2.51 .81 Females 2.42 .78 2.43 .81 Table A:4. Analysis of Variance of Grade Point Averages By School and Sex. Source df SS MS F p Schools 1 .05 .05 .08 N.S. Sex 1 .11 .ll .17 N.S. Schools x Sex 1 .33 .33 .53 N.S. Within 86 53.46 .622 Total 89 53.95 126 Table A:5. Attitude Towards Music Means and Standard Deviations for Males and Females, Pattengill JHS and Rich JHS. Pattengill JES ic S Mean SD Mean SD Males 10.84 2.35 16.19 3.00 Females 17.28 2.13 16.11 3.11 Table A:6. Analysis of Variance of Attitude Towards Music Scores by School and Sex. Source df SS MS F p Schools 1 17.07 17.07 2.37 N.S. Sex 1 .75 .75 .10 N.S. Schools x Sex 1 1.54 1.54 .21 N.S. Within 86 619.57 7.20 Total 89 638.93 127 Table A:7. Musical Aptitude Profile Means and Standard Deviations for Males and Females, Pattengill JHS and Rich JHS. Pattengill gas Rich gas Mean SD Mean SD Males 51.92 9.39 51.92 8.71 Females 49.80 7.80 51.40 6.40 Table A:8. Analysis of Variance of Musical Aptitude Scores by School and Sex. Source df SS MS F p Schools 1 20.54 20.54 30 N.S Sex 1 46.88 46.88 .68 N.S. Schools x Sex 1 28.89 28.89 .42 N.S. Within 86 5900.59 68.61 Total 89 5996.90 128 Table A:9. Personal Adjustment Means and Standard Deviations for Males and Females, Pattengill JHS and Rich JHS. Pattengill Jfl§ gigh gas Mean SD Mean SD FF Males 54.35 16.13 57.44 14.27 Females 56.79 15.97 55.05 11.12 Table A:10. Analysis of Variance of Personal Adjustment Scores by School and Sex. Source df SS MS F p Schools 1 22.50 22.50 .11 N.S. Sex 1 .01 .01 .00 N.S. Schools x Sex 1 129.00 129.00 .61 N.S. Within 86 18,340.15 213.26 Total 89 18,491.66 HICHIGR STQTE UN 1111” 1111111 312931017556 I RARIES 111111 853