341,, g 1;? ' ‘If‘rl .'n. .,.,‘u;‘.‘- L I B R A R Y Michigan State University ‘ My 1293 1.0179 5973 This is to certify that the thesis entitled A Study of Relationships Between Qelected Factors and the School Achievement of Mexican-American Migrant Children presented bg Mary Alexander Cain has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for Ph.D degree in Educational Psychology Do“ QJLMOLM Major professor Date WM 11; “‘10 0-169 ABSTRACT A STUDY OF RELATIONSHIPS BETWEEN SELECTED.FACTORS AND THE SCHOOL ACHIEVEMENT OF MEXICAN-AMERICAN MIGRANT CHILDREN By Mary Alexander Cain This study investigated whether the factors of age, sex, parent-child relations, or modes of response to problems of frustration and failure, were related to the school achievement of Mexican-American migrant children. Typical patterns of parent-child relationships, as perceived by these children, and typical means employed in their problem—solving were also investigated. ‘Subjects of the study were 58 Mexican-American migrant boys and.girls, ranging in age from seven through 13, who attended a summer.school program in southwestern Michigan. Standard scores in reading.and arithmetic obtained by the school on the Wide-Range Achievement Test were used as measures of achievement. Trend analyses were rnade to determine whether standard achievement scores dropped with age. Analysis of variance was employed to discover possible differences in achievement Mary Alexander Cain between boys and girls. Perceptions of parent-child relationships were measured by the Roe-Siegelman Parent—Child Relations Questionnaire, and various child—rearing practices were compared by rank analysis of variance. A step—wise multiple regression analysis was used to discover possible relationships between patterns of child—rearing and achievement. Problems of frustration and failure related to the Mexican- American migrant child's life were introduced in five incomplete stories. Children's story completions were categorized by three judges, and chi-square analysis was used to determine whether modes of problem-solving were related to school achievement. Arithmetic achievement equalled or excelled reading achievement at each age level. All achieve— ment decreased beyond the nine-year—old level. While a quadratic relationship between reading scores and age proved not significant, a linear relationship between arithmetic scores and age was significant at the .01 level. No difference was discerned between the achievement of boys and of girls. Parent—child relations were perceived as significantly loving and protecting. The child—rearing dimensions of rejection and neglect were significantly less prevalent than other dimensions. Children saw themselves as more rewarded than punished. Mothers were seen as more protecting, more demanding, more rewarding and more Mary Alexander Cain punishing than fathers. Relatively lower reading and arithmetic scores were significantly related to mothers' rejection, neglect, and casualness. Fathers' love was positively related to arithmetic achievement, while their casualness was negatively correlated with reading and performance. Forty—one per cent of children's story completions are goal—oriented, gratification-deferring, "middle— class" solutions. Thirty-four per cent of the solutions employed withdrawal from the problem. Appeals to authority, use of fantasy, and anti-social aggression made up only 25 per cent of the total responses. Despite the notion that typically middle—class modes of problem-solving.tend to produce school success, the Isolutions produced by the subjects of this study bore no relationships to their achievement. The findings of the study suggested desirable adult education practices, and the need for further ~research in the area of problem—solving. The discovery of currently acceptable child—rearing practices and typical "middle-class" problem solving among the Mexican— American migrant families of this sample emphasizes the need for further research to explore correlates of achievement. A STUDY OF RELATIONSHIPS BETWEEN SELECTED FACTORS AND THE SCHOOL ACHIEVEMENT OF MEXICAN—AMERICAN MIGRANT CHILDREN By Mary Alexander Cain A THESIS Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Counseling, Personnel Services and Educational Psychology 1970 ACKNOWLEDGMENTS The author gratefully acknowledges the help of her Guidance Committee in the preparation of this thesis. Special thanks are due to Dr. Maryellen Mc Sweeney for her patient advice concerning analysis of the data, and to Dr. Don Hamachek for his constant encouragement. ii TABLE OF CONTENTS INTRODUCTION Background of the Problem Need for the Study The Problem of Cultural Values Purposes of the Study Procedures: The Sample Instruments and Methodology of the Study Limitations of the Study REVIEW OF RELATED LITERATURE AND HYPOTHESES TO BE TESTED Age as a Factor in Retardation of Achievement Sex as a Factor in School Achievement Parent-Child Relationships and Child—Rearing Practices as Factors in School Achievement School Achievement and Modes of Response to Problems Summary PROCEDURES AND RESULTS OF THE STUDY Age and Relative Achievement Sex and School Achievement Patterns of Parent-Child Relationships 111 \OVNONSN lit 17 17 22 25 32 :42 an an 1:7 us TABLE OF,CONTENTS (con'd) Parent-Child Relationships and Achievemeni 56 Modes of Response to Frustration and 60 Failure Modes of Problem—Solving and Achievement 65 Summary of Research Findings 66 IV. INTERPRETATION OF THE RESULTS 69 Age and Achievement 69 Sex and School Achievement -‘ 72 Patterns of Parent-Child Relationships 72 *Parent-Child Relationships and 78 Achievement . Modes of Response to Frustration 83 and Failure Modes of Problem-Solving and Achievement 8“ Summary of Discussion 85 V. IMPLICATIONS FOR INSTRUCTION AND RESEARCH 89 BIBLIOGRAPHY 95 APPENDICES 103 Appendix A: PCR Questionnaire 103 Appendix B: Unfinished Stories 119 Appendix C: Instructions for Judges 122 Appendix D: The Wide-Range Achievement Test Data 12A Appendix E: Results of the Step—Wise Multiple 126 Regression Analysis iv VI. VII. VIII. IX. t—i XII. XIII. LIST OF TABLES Mean Reading and Arithmetic Achievement Scores, by Age Trend Analysis of Reading Scores, by Age Trend Analysis of Arithmetic Scores, by Age Comparison of Achievement Scores, by Sex Mean Scores of Parent—Child Relationships as Perceived by Mexican—American Migrant Boys and Girls Analysis of Variance of Protection from Mothers and Fathers, as Perceived by Boys and Girls (An Example of Analyses Performed for all Child-Rearing Dimensions) Summary of Perceived Differences in Child~Rearing Patterns, Computed by Analysis of Variance Analysis of Variance in Ranks of Child—Rearing Practices Comparison of Mean Ranks of Mothers' Child—Rearing Practices Comparison of Mean Ranks of Fathers' Child—Rearing Practices Correlation among Mothers' Child—Rearing Practices and their Children's Reading and Arithmetic Achievement Correlation among Fathers' Child—Rearing Practices and their Children's Reading and Arithmetic Achievement Interjudge Reliability for Five Story Completions AA A6 A6 47 50 51 S2 53 5h 57 62 XIV. XV. XVI O XVII. LIST OF.TABLES (con'd) Modes of Story Completion.bf Mexican— American Migrant Children' Chi—SQuares~Indicating'Relationship of Problem—Solving Modes to Reading and Arithmetic Achievement 'Frequency Distribution of Standard Achievement Scores Attained on the WRAT by Mexican-American Migrant Children Multiple Correlation Coefficients and Regression Coefficients for Perceived Parent—Child Relations and Achievement vi 63 65 I25 126 Mean Reading and Arithmetic Scores: by Age vii l45 CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION Background of the Problem A large part of America's food supply is harvested by a population of migrant laborers who are estimated to number between one-half million and one million. Tens of thousands of children accompany their parents through life "on the season", living under conditions of poverty, deprivation, 1 Estimates at the disease, humiliation, and hopelessness. beginning of this decade indicated that the average migrant worker earned approximately $900 a year for his work in the fields.2 The growers who hire migrant workers, beset by problems of unpredictable weather conditions, federal legislation, and social and labor organization, increasingly turn to mechan- ization as a means of cultivating and harvesting their crops.3 It is estimated that within ten years the need for migratory farm labor will disappear. Yet migrant workers are not sufficiently educated to gain other employment. Without effective education, they will remain in poverty, either in the fields, or in rural or urban slums, where they may seek 1Descriptions of the conditions of migrant life may be found in: Koos, E.L., They Follow the Sun, Jacksonville: Florida State Board of Health, 1957, Moore, Truman E., The Slaves We Rent, New York: Random House, 1965; Reul, Myrtle E., Where Hannibal Led Us, New York: Vantage Press, 1966; and Shotwell, Louisa R., The Harvesters: The Story of the Migrant People, Garden City: Doubleday and Co., 1961. 2Moore, pp. cit., 1A0. 3Kelly, Clarence F., "Mechanical Harvesting." Scien- tific American, 213 (August, 1967),50_59, 2 a precarious living on the fringes of an affluent society in which they are not prepared to succeed. Need for the Study most migrant children attend school infrequently. Their education lacks continuity, being constantly interrupted by their travels, by their work in the fields, and by the reluc— tance or inability of many school systems to accommodate migrant children in on—going school programs. A number of states have established temporary experimental plans for the education of migrant children, yet no consistent program for their education has been maintained. Consistent and concerted attempts to educate migrant children, so that they may find a place in society, are long overdue. While there is general agreement that the migrant child is an under-achiever who will profit from welcoming teachers and individualized instruction,l little is known about the possible effect of specific aspects of the migrant child's culture or his family on his ability to achieve in school. Nor is much known about the appropriateness of the school program to which this child is exposed. In order to develop a more adequate educational program for migrant children and their families, specific information regarding many variables is necessary. A study recently conducted by the State of Florida points up the recognized need for knowledge of psychological and cultural variables: lShotwell, 99. cit., 165—6; Sutton, Elizabeth, Knowing and Teaching the Migrant Child. Washington: National Edu— cation Association, 1960. 3 What kind of unique challenges and needs does the migrant child have which result primarily from his mobile pattern of life? School personnel and lay peOple need to deepen their understandings of migratory children and adults. Little is known of the exact thought processes and learnings from their social and economic environment, or the variation in all of these which occurs within and between ethnic groups. 'Stereotyped behaviors and values which are associated with the term "migrant" need to be replaced with factual information about their characteristics and the unique and distinctive needs, which primarily are the results of their life patterns and the subculture from which they come. Florida's attempt to improve migrant education on a state—wide basis is seen in the work of the Chinsegut Hill Workshop of the University of South Florida. Some important recommendations coming from the workshop emphasize more specifically the following needs: The need for studies of the migrant culture. The need to develop teaching strategies utilizing the learning styles of migrant children. The need to involve parents in their children's education and to help them to a better under- standing of school and of the contributions the family can make to children's achievement. The need to help the migrant child develop "posi- tive attitudes toward himself, others, and school learning; behavior patterns which evince: (a) the ability to work for long range goals, (b) respect for hard, productive work, (c) an awareness of his responsibility for his progress or success." 1State of Florida, Planning Florida's Migrant Education Ikrogram. Tallahasseez' Department—of Education, 1966, p. 30. 2Ironically, the educator's bias shows in this recom— mendation. Probably no one knows more about "hard, productive work" than the migrant farm laborer. What the author intends, perhaps, is "hard, productive work" of an academic nature. 3State of Florida, 92: cit. 22—3. A The unique living conditions and subcultures of the migrant stream make it important that the relationship between subcultural variables and the migrant child's school achievement be investigated. One group of migrant workers-—distinct in customs, trad— itions, language, values, and family pattern——is made up of Mexican—Americans who travel north each spring from the southwestern states;' These Spanish—speaking migrants repre— sent the third largest minority group in the United States. Their language and other cultural differences may compound or alleviate learning difficulties encountered by other migrant children. The present isolation from other American citizens produces a unique culture whose general character— istics and their effects on school achievement may be studied. Those who work with migrant children have recognized the need for an examination of some of the factors which make the Mexican—American migrant child culturally different, and the possible effects of these factors on school achievement. The major attention of this study relates to some of these factors. The Problem of Cultural Values Throughout the succeeding discussions of parent—child relationships and modes of response to problems, contrasts will be made between middle— and lower—class practices and values as they relate to school achievement or achievement xnotivation. Research shows that middle—class values and behaviors are more likely to accompany school success. How- ever, we do not intend to suggest that middle—class values A The unique living conditions and subcultures of the migrant stream make it important that the relationship between subcultural variables and the migrant child's school achievement be investigated. One group of migrant workers-~distinct in customs, trad— itions, language, values, and family pattern——is made up of Mexican-Americans who travel north each spring from the southwestern states; These Spanish—speaking migrants repre— sent the third largest minority group in the United States. Their language and other cultural differences may compound or alleviate learning difficulties encountered by other migrant children. The present isolation from other American citizens produces a unique culture whose general character- istics and their effects on school achievement may be studied. Those who work with migrant children have recognized the need for an examination of some of the factors which make the Mexican-American migrant child culturally different, and the possible effects of these factors on school achievement. The major attention of this study relates to some of these factors. The Problem of Cultural Values Throughout the succeeding discussions of parent-child relationships and modes of response to problems, contrasts ‘will be made between middle— and lower—class practices and ‘values as they relate to school achievement or achievement xnotivation. Research shows that middle—class values and behaviors are more likely to accompany school success. How— ever, we do not intend to suggest that middle-class values 1"" ”5F 1 5 are in any sense the "right" values or "better” values, but simply that, given the nature of our present schools and society, they bring greater success to those who hold them. The writer agrees with most current writers who suggest that the way to achieve a meeting-ground between the school and any "disadvantaged" child is to adjust school curriculum and instruction to the culture, the values, and the attitudes of the child, rather than unreasonably to expect the child suddenly to adopt the modes of the school. Attempts to "adjust" the child and thereby produce achievement have con— sistently failed. Knowledge of cultures different from that of the school could be used to help the school adjust to the child. The Mexican-American culture contains many elements which can enhance human development and enrich human exis— tence. Laughter, warmth, humor, color, imagination, gener— osity, and honest relationships are evident in the Mexican— American migrant culture, and in schools which accept migrant children and have concern for their growth. Thes: elements of the migrant culture need not be sacrificed for school achievement. If any personal bias, beyond that of concern for the migrant child, influences this study, it is not that one set of values is superior to another, but that there exists the possibility of helping the migrant child to greater achieve— ment without the cost to the person which the more rigid middle—class values sometimes entail. It is the hope of this writer that schools will come to understand, to accept, and 6 to use the cultural difference of minority—group children in order to further their achievement and success. A fair statement of the writer's preference would also include her hope for a mixing of cultures and values as middle—class schools become more acceptant of lower-class cultures and thereby find ways for lower-class children to attain greater success, without rejection of the beautiful or the valuable in their heritage. Purposes of the Study Three cultural variables offer a beginning to the study of the Mexican-American migrant child and his school achieve— ment. The parent—child relationships typical of this culture may, in other cultures previously studied, affect the motivation, the aspirations, and the achievement of the children of the culture. Typical modes of response to the problems and frustrations of living may also affect the child's approach to problems of school learning and achievement. The purposes of this study stemmed from assertions regarding the cumulative educational retardation of the migrant child as he grows older, and from the three cultural variables of sex-role expectations, parent—child relationships, and modes of response to problems. Specifically, this study investigated: (1) the possible relationship between age and standardized achievement scores, (2) the possible relationship between sex and school achievement, (3) the parent—child relationships of the 7 Mexican—American migrant family and their possible relation— ships with school achievement, and (A) the modes of response to the problems of Mexican—American migrant children and their possible relationship to school achievement. 'The purposes of this study were: 1. to discover whether the cumulative school retardation found in earlier studies of Mexican—American migrant children exists in this sample. 2. to discover whether the sex of the Mexican— American migrant child is related to his school achievement. 3. to discover whether there is a typical pattern of parent—child relationships within the Mexican—American migrant culture. A. to discover whether specific parent—child relationships are related to SChOOl achievement in Mexican- American migrant children. 5. to discover whether there are typical modes of approach to the solution of problems among Mexican— American migrant children. 6. to discover whether these modes of response to problems are related to the Mexican—American migrant Cllild'S school achievement. Procedures The Sample The subjects of this study were 58 Mexican— Anusrican migrant boys and girls, ranging in age from seven j - ,_. ,7 8 through 13,1 who attended at least part of an eight—week school program in southwestern Michigan during the summer of 1967. Children were chosen at random from a daily enrollment which ranged from A5 to 109. Selection was limited by the necessity to choose children who spoke English and who attended the school with sufficient regularity to make a program of testing possible. Although an effort was made to choose at random equal numbers of boys and girls representing various age groups, randomization was limited by the nature of the school enrollment. Migrant parents sent their children to school on a voluntary basis. Many more migrant children stayed in the fields and camps than attended school. Hence, this sample represents only those children whose parents were willing to rise earlier, to undertake extra washing and ironing of children's clothes, and, in the case of older children, to forego the income their children could have earned in the fields. It is possible, therefore, that the subjects of this study constitute a sample of a particular set of Mexican—American'migrants. those vho are motivated to make extra sacrifices for the sake of their children's education. In summary, factors influencing enrollment and factors influencing the feasibility of the testing program produced limitations in the sample used in this study. 1Owing to the irregular school attendance and the mobility of the children, and to the ensuing difficulties in administering tests, not all of the children were included in all phases of the study. 9 Instruments and Methodology of the Study 1. Measurement of Achievement Standard achieve- ment scores in reading and arithmetic were obtained as part of the summer school program. The Wide—Range Achievement Test1 was used for this purpose, and the school's measures were used in this study. The standard score of the WRAT has a mean of 100 and a standard deviation of 15. Because performance on these tests relies upon a language which is essentially foreign to the Mexican—American child, all tests were un—timed and were administered individually, giving subjects an advantage beyond that of the normal administration. '2. Age Differences in Achievement Each age level for which there was a sufficient sample was compared with other age levels by analysis of variance to determine whether standard achievement scores drop significantly with age. 3. Sex Differences in Achievement The mean achievement scores for boys and girls in reading and in arithmetic were compared to determine whether one sex was Significantly superior to the other in reading or arith- metic achievement. lJastak, J. F., and Jastak, s. R.. The Wide—Range Achievement Test. Revised edition. Wilmington, Delaware: Guidance Associates, 1965. Further information concerning the WRATg and scores of the subjects of this study, are contained in Appendix D. 10 N. Parent-Child Relationship; The Roe—Siegelman Parent—Child Relations Questionnairel was chosen as a measure of parent—child relationships in Mexican—American migrant families, because of its simplicity of administra— tion, the feasibility of.its use with children, the high reliability coefficients reported for each category mea~ sured by the instrument, and the possibility of comparison of the Roe—Siegelman factors with the findings of other studies related to child-rearing and parent—child relation— ships. The questionnaire was rewritten in simple language 2 for administration to Mexican-American children. (Appen— dix A contains forms of the Roe-Siegelman instrument for mothers and for fathers, as they were used in this study.) The Roe—Siegelman PCR measures six treatments (e.g., ”Demanding," ”Casual,” "Protecting”) and four reward— punishment systems (e.g., "Direct—Object Reward," "Symbolic— Love Punishment”) giving a total of ten scores which re— flect responding children's perceptions of their relation— ships with their parents. The PCR includes separate forms for mothers and for fathers, since its authors found some items more reliable for one parent than another. Since lRoe, Anne, and Siegelman, Marvin, "A Parent-Child Re— ;atxions Questionnaire." Child Development, 35 (June, 1963), 55‘690 2Dr. Roe's permission to revise forms of the question— na:iJ?e was granted through Barbara P. Brown, Executive Secre— talfiy, Center for Research in Careers, Graduate School in Education, Harvard University. Dr. Roe's kind permission and M1353 Brown's assistance are gratefully acknowledged. 11 current theory holds that mothers may relate differ— ently to their sons than to their daughters, and that fathers, too, have differing relationships with children of different sexes, the population of the present study was treated when possible as four groups: (a) mothers and daugh— ters, (b) mothers and sons, (0) fathers and daughters, and (d) fathers and sons. Analysis of variance was used to dis- cover possible major factors in the parent—child relation— ships of the subjects. Means of child—rearing categories were compared for each sub—group where warranted. Parent— child relationship variables were compared in high- and low-achieving subjects by multiple regression analysis. 5. Modes of Response to Problems of Frustration and Failure It was assumed that Mexican-American migrant children who achieve to a significant degree would respond to frustration and failure in ways which are more typically "middle-class," i.e., with greater goal orientation, more deferment of gratification, more long—range planning, and greater achievement motivation, than their peers whose ‘achievement is lower.‘ Therefore, five unfinished stories concerning migrant children were constructed, after the l 2 manner of Miller and Swanson and of Wallach and Kogan. These stories are similar in purpose to the Rosenzweig lMiller, Daniel R., and Swanson, Guy E., Inner Conflict and Defense. New York: Henry Holt and Co., 19 0. 2Wallach, Michael A., and Kogan, Nathan, Modes of Thinking in Young Children. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston, Inc., 1965. Pp. 211—16. 12 Picture Frustration Studyl but, rather than concerning them— selves with scooters, birthday parties, and the like, con— tain items and events familiar to the migrant child. Separ— ate forms were prepared for boys and for girls. Each set of stories included the following problems: a. school frustration and failure b. frustration regarding a career c. lack of desired material possessions d. failure to live up to parental expectations e. rejection by peers because of inappropriate dress Boys' and girls' forms were constructed to be as much alike as possible and were, in some cases, identical except for the sex of the ”hero." The stories were them submitted to several persons with expert knowledge of the Mexican-American migrant culture. On the basis of their reactions, the stories were slightly modified in vocabulary.2 (Appendix B contains the ten unfinished stories comprising the boys' and girls' lRosenzweig, S., "Rosenzweig Picture—Frustration Study, Children's Form.” In Projective Techniques with Children, edited by A. I. Rabin and Mary Haworth. New York: Grune and Stratton, 1960. 2Experts consulted were Mr. Manuel Alfaro, Migrant Con~ sultant to the Michigan State Department of Education; Messrs. Harold Pena and Joe Sanchez, consultants with the Texas Migrant Education Project; Dr. Joe L. Frost, University of Texas, and Mrs. Frost, a teacher of migrant children; and Dr. Myrtle Reul, University of Georgia, then of the Depart— ment of Social Work, Michigan State University, who has spent many months living and working as a migrant. Messrs. Alfaro, Pena and Sanchez are former migrants. The writer gratefully acknowledges the kind and willing assistance of these people. 13 forms of this instrument.) It was assumed that children, in completing the unfin— ished stories, would identify with the hero of the story, and that the solution they chose for him would reflect their typical modes of solution to similar problems. This assump- tion has limitations and is, as are all projective techniques, open to question.1 Riessman and Miller2 note that many projective tests are inappropriate to working-class subjects. In writing, examining, and revising the stories every attempt was made to present a cultural context familiar to the Mex— ican—American migrant child. Children were tested individually. Each child was told a simple unfinished story as an example before the test was begun. If he finished the story in some way, it was assumed that he understood the process expected of him. Enough time was allowed to examine and to use the tape recorder to insure that the child felt comfortable with it. Then the first story was read to the child, and his completion was recorded on tape. About A5 minutes to one hour were required for the total procedure of completing all five stories. Story completions were subsequently transcribed from the tapes. lcr. Anderson, Harold H., and Anderson, Gladys, L., eds., An Introduction to Projective Techniques. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice—Hall, Inc., 1951; and Murstein, Bernard 1., ed., Handbook of Projective Techniques, New York: Basic Books, Inc., 1965. 2Riessmen, Frank, and Miller, S. M., "Social Class and Projective Techniques.” In Murstein, loc. cit., 95—106. a! , , . _,,1 1A Using stories of children's problems in a study of fantasy and realism in children's story completions, Wallach and Koganl assigned numerical scores to solutions offered by children, scoring '0' for ”fantasy” responses and 'l' for "Realism" responses. Somewhat different responses were expected in the present study. It was assumed that in completing the stories children would produce: (a) goal- oriented, gratification-deferring solutions, (b) reactions of withdrawal or resignation, (c) reactions of hostility or anti-social counter—aggression, (d) solutions relying upon appeal to authority, and (e) fantasy solutions. Three judges read and categorized each story completion.2 Percen— tage of agreement was calculated for each pair of judges. Categories of story completion and intervals of achieve— ment were compared by means of Pearson's Chi-Square Test of Association to determine whether possible relationships existed between modes of response and achievement. Limitations of the Study The present study contains several limitations. Any generalization which can be made from the study is limited to the population from which the sample was taken: Mexican: American migrant children between the ages of seven and 13 whose parents send them to summer school. The value which lWallach and Kogan, 92. c t. 2Dr. Rachel Inselberg, Dr. Elizabeth Johnson, and Mrs. Joan Madden, for whose time and patience the writer is deeply grateful. 15 these parents put on school attendance, although it may be assumed to differ within the sample, is reflected in the special effort and sacrifice which a migrant parent must make in order for children to attend a summer school program. Migrant children from Appalachian white lr southern Negro families represent separate cultures and the findings of the present study cannot justifiably be generalized to these groups. All instruments used in this study were administered in English. The greatest limitation of the study is the achievement measure, which tested the child's ability to perform school—related tasks in a foreign language. In other measures, every attempt was made to simplify vocab— ulary and to use concepts within the realm of these child— ren's understanding. ‘However, some of the error of each analysis must be assumed to be due to the use of a second language.' The use of English and of standard achievement tests is justified, according to the assumptions of this study, if the educator's concern is with helping migrants to develop abilities which will enable them to leave the migrant stream. This accomplishment demands that migrants learn to succeed in the ways and with the language of a culture which is relatively strange to them. The achieve— ment test is one way of gaining some measure of how far migrant children have progressed in the direction of this success. The standard achievement scores obtained in this study must not, however, be construed as an indication 16 of how well these subjects might perform under other cir— cumstances or on other measures, but only as an indication of how well they gig perform on limited achievement measures expressed in the English language. Use of the parent—child relationships questionnaire presents a limited amount of information. A detailed, lengthy interview or series of interviews and observations with parents might, had they been possible, have yeilded more information regarding parental attitudes and childmrearing practices than did the questionnaire. It may be argued that the child's responses to problems of frustration or failure do not necessarily represent what he would do in these situations. An assumption of this study is that since the migrant child is limited by the little he knows of the middle—class, English—speaking culture, in his attempt to produce the "best” solution he can, in his own terms, he will use whatever resources he can summon. It is the assumption of this investigation that subjects' modes of response to problems, therefore, reflect what their families and their culture have taught them about problem— solving. In summary, this study is limited by the nature of the sample studied, by the use of English—language instruments, and by the assumptions underlying a projective story device. CHAPTER II REVIEW OF RELATED LITERATURE AND HYPOTHESES TO BE TESTED Age as a Factor in Retardation or Achievement Little is known about the cognitive abilities or disabilities of migrant children, except that they are retarded in achievement. Many believe that this retard— ation is due solely to infrequent school attendance. "The cause of retardation——sixty—seven percent in our Colorado migrants—~13 centered in lack of school attendance," states Alfred M. Potts of the Colorado State Department of Education. "It definitely is not due to lack of scholastic ability. Studies show these children to have about the same ratio of slow learners, average—intelligence groups, and potentially superior nhildren as other popu— lation groups.”1 We may assume that achievement of Mexican—American children will continue to decrease relative to their age peers as these children grow older. In a study of 1709 children from 665 migrant families, Greene2 showed the increase in school retardation as children progressed in age. In the first year of school, no children were re— garded as retarded. However, the percentage of retardation lsnotwell, EB. cit., 163—4, ‘Greene, Shirley E., The Education of Migrant Child- ren. Washington, D.C.: The Department of Rural Education, 5 EAJ 1954. 17 18 increased progressively from the second year of school (35.5% retarded) to the twelfth (75.0% retarded). The degree of school retardation likewise increased, from one year in the second grade to from one to more than five 1 In 1935, Johnson2 found re— years by the sixth grade. tardation of migrant children in school to accumulate with each added year of age, and the same sort of evi— dence has appeared in subsequent studies conducted be— tween 1939 and 1950.3 Frost found that, among 32 children Venrolled in a summer school, the average degree of retardation was three years.ll Murray, studying Mexican- American children in San Antonio, found that while six per cent had completed high school, thirty per cent of the children had a maximum of three years in one school.5 lGreene, loc. cit., 77. 2Johnson, Elizabeth S., Welfare of Families of Sugar— Beet Workers. Washington, D.C.: Children's Bureau Pub— lication No. 247, U.S. Department of Labor, 1935. 3Greene, 92. git. 83. “Frost, Joe E., "School and the Migrant Child.” Childhood Education, 41 (1964), 129—32. 5Murray, Sister Mary John, "A Socio-Cultural Study of 118 Mexican Families Living in a Low—Rent—Public Hous— ing Project in San Antonio, Texas.” Studies in Sociology, 38, p. 8“. 'Cited in The Disadvantaged: Challenge to Edu- cation, edited berario D. Fantini and Gerald Weinstein. New York: Harper and Row, 1968, p. 28. 19 The factor of mobility is also cited as a cause of school retardation. Continual mobility and instability of school life are a prevailing part of a migrant child's existence. Coles and Huge provide a poignant report of the mi— grant child's enforced mobility: How did school go for him this year: ”Well, I didn't get there much. We moved from place to place, and I helped with the picking a lot, and the schools, when you go to them, they don't seem to want you, and they'll say that you're only going to be there for a few weeks anyway, so what's the use." What does he want to do when he gets elder?: "I don't know. I'd like to stay someplace, I guess, and never have to leave there for the rest of my life, that's what. Manis found that: . . . both the number of moves made each year as well as the total number of years in mi— grancy persistently, though irregu§arly, in— creases the degree of retardation. The mobility of the migrant child, however, may not be the sole or even a determining cause in the retardation of migrant children. Americans live in a mobile society. 3 Approximately one in five Americans moves each year. lColes, Robert, and Huge, Harry, "Peonage in Florida.” The New Republic, 161 (July 26, 1969), 17—21, p. 19. 2Manis, Jerome G., A Study of Migrant Education: Survey of Findings in Van Buren County, Michigan, 1957. Kalamazoo, Michigan: Western Michigan University Press, 1958, p. l1. 3National Education Association, "America on the Move.” NEA Research Bulletin, 36 (December, 1958), 99—102. 20 Downie found‘that children who had'moved‘made scores on the Otis Self—Administering Test of Mental Abilities which were comparable to those made’by'child- ren who did not move.1 Similarly, Bollenbacher found no difference in median reading level between mobile 2 while Evans found‘ and non—mobile school children, that mobile fifth— and sixth—graders were slightly better in reading and in science than their non—mobile peers.3 However, in studying the possible effects of mobility on the achievement of disadvantaged children, Justman found that both IQ and reading scores decreased with the number of moves a child had made from school to school.“ 1Downie, N. M., "A Comparison Between Childreanho Have Moved from School to School with Those Who Have Been in Continuous Residence on Various Factors of Adjustment."‘ Journal of Educational Psychology, nu (Jan— uary, 1953), 50—53. ‘ 2Bollenbacher, Joan, ”A Study of the Effect of Mobility on Reading Achievement.”" The Reading Teacher, 15 (March, 1962), 356—60. 3Evans, John w., Jr., ”The Effect of Pupil Mobility on Reading Achievement." National Elementary Principal, 45 (April, 1966), 18-22. “Justman, Joseph, "Academic Aptitude and Reading Test Scores of Disadvantaged Children Showing Varying Degrees of Mobility." Journal of Educational Measure— ment, 2 (December, 1965), 151—5. 21 Apparently, mobility is a factor whose effects are mediated by social class or by cultural differences. It may be an oversimplification, then, to say that migrant children are retarded because of the mobility of their lives. Other factors in the child's culture or socio— economic situation are probably.operating to make his mobility an additional.disadvantage with respect to his school achievement. In studies made up to the pre— sent time, these factors appear to have cumulative effects with the age of the child. During recent years, greater Federal resources have been devoted to special educational projects for migrant“ children. As this is written, the Michigan State Depart— ment of Education is embarking on a state—wide migrant education project for the third consecutive summer. Other states with heavy migrant populations are also making special efforts to improve the achievement of migrant children. However, no : ULTCl can be exercised over the regular attendance of migrant ;L;llren in sum—' mer schools, where attendance is voluntary, and none of these programs have been in effect for very long. Given the additional fact that the older the Mexican— American migrant child, the less likely he is to be enrolled in school,1 this study would expect to find lGreene, gp. cit., 83. 22 cumulative retardation as the migrant child grows older. The evidence leads to the hypothesis that standarized scores for both reading achievement and for arithmetic achievement will decrease as age increases. As is the case with other groups of children who have recently been labelled "educationally disadvantaged,” however, greater school attendance will not necessarily improve performance if other retarding factor: are oper- ating, and if the school program does not take these factors into account. Sex as a Factor in School Achievement Girls often receive higher school grades than boys.1 Whether this fact reflects actual differences in over— all achievement is doubtful. As Anastasi notes: . girls were found to excel in school grades, when compared with boys receiving the same achievement test scores. Thus the grades showed a far greater female superior- ity than seemed to be warranted by peEfor— mance on objective achievement tests. The better school grades received by girls is more likely lAnastasi, Anne, Differential Psychology. Third edition. New York: The MacMillan Co., l95t. p. 492. Italics are those of the author cited. 2loc. cit., H95. 23 attributable to their greater quietness and tractability and to their relative lack of aggression——characteristics which are often prized by teachers and which might be reflected in the teacher expectancy which Rosenthal and Jacobsonl have discussed. These authors did not find sig- nificant differences in gains due to higher teacher ex— pectancy between the boys and the girls of their study. The exception to this finding occurred when boys' achieve— ment gains were correlated with the degree of "Mexican— ness" of their faces, when the boys who looked most ”Mex— ican” benefited most from their teachers' higher expec— tancies, but girls' achievement was not affected by this factor.2 Such a finding, particularly considering the limitations of the study in question, may or may not indicate original teacher prejudice in favor of girls. In an examination of factors affecting A and B grades assigned by teachers in reading, Wilson found that belonging to the female sex had a positive effect, while being male had a negative effect. Wilson remarks, ”Girls lRosenthal, Robert, and Jacobson, Lenore, Pygmalion in the Classroom. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston, Inc., 1968. p. 176 2loo. cit., 177. 2A also . . . are perceived as better students than their test performances would warrant."1 Although teacher expectations appear, at least in some cases, to differ with the sex of the student, there is less evidence that parent expectations differ, at least among lower—class children. For example, Cloward and Jones2 found no difference in the percentage of poor parents who mentioned education in connection with their expecta— tions for a "good life" for their boys or girls. Nor did 3 Greene find a significant difference in migrant families' educational aspirations for boys or for girls of the same family. (Rather, educational aspirations for children appeared to be a factor of the parents' own educational attainment.) The families of Spanish—speaking migrants are categor— ized as patriarchalL4 and traditional.5 Pressures for adult lWilson, Alan E., ”Social Stratification and Academic Achievement.” In Education in Depressed Areas, edited by A. Harry Passow. New York: Bureau of Publications, Teachers College, 1963. pp. 229, 231. 2Cloward, Richard A., and Jones, James A., "Social Class: Educational Attitudes and Participation." In Education in Depressed Areas, gp. cit., 203-4. 3Greene, 92. cit., 115. uReul, gp. cit. 5Burma, J. H., Spanish—Speaking Groups in the United States. Durham, North Carolina: Duke University Press, 195 o 25 sex-roles come strong and early.1 Boys go to work in fields and orchards as soon as they are physically able. Nine and ten—year—old girls are expected to care fox "the little ones" (often several younger children, including a baby) for the entire day, while their parents and older children work. The present study asks whether there is reason to expect that sex—role pressures and related cultural char- acteristics have a differential effect on school achieve— ment. From what is known about the patriarchal organi- zation of Mexican-American migrant families and their traditional sex roles, we might expect that boys would llave learned stronger achievement motivation than girls. On the other hand, because girls are expected to behave irl more modest and submissive fashion, one might also lxypothesize that girls, given greater rewards and approval by teachers, would have higher achievement than kHDys. Further study beyond the scope of this paper WOLlld be required for knowledge of the mecnanisms producing seJ( differences in achievement. The present study looks fil?st for the existence of such differences. Parent—Child Relationships and Child—Rearing Practices as Factors in School Achievement Child—rearing practices and parent—child relationships haVe been shown to be important factors mediating between lSutton, 2p. cit., 19—20; Moore, 92. cit., 52—65. f 20 's cultural values and the child's achievement wrtivation or achievement in school. When cultural values, or attitudes toward school, are no‘ appropriate to the middle—class school's concept of achievement, the child‘s school performance suffers. Davis,1 Davis and Havighurst,2 and Douvan3 have provided illustrations of the ways in which middle—class schools put lower—class children at a disadvantage, so that, given the child—rearing practices and the circumstances of their culture, it is nearly impossible for children who are culturally different to achieve to a degree commensurate with the achievement of middle-class children. The need to achieve in school appears to be strongly (zonnected with a middle—class socialization process. I?inneyu found that achievement need was the strongest (Harrelate of parents' status and education, but also that true need to achieve was a product of maternal warmth and 1Davis, Allison, Social Class Influences upon Learning. Ceunbridgc: Harvard University Press, 19U6. LUavis, Allison, and Havighurst, Robert J., Father of ttn3_Man. Boston: houghton Mifflin Co., 19A7. 3Douvan, Elizabeth, "Social Status and Success Striving." flfylrnal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 52 (1956), r3 . n91—323. uFinney, Joseph Claude, ”Some Maternal Influences on Children's Personality and Character.” Genetic Psychology Mggggraphs, 63 (1961), 199—278. 27 nurturance. Separate studies by Douvan,l Hoffman,2 and Watson3 arrived at the same conclusions: in special task situations, middle—class children work to achieve, to do a job well, while the performance of lower-class children is improved by the offer of extrinsic rewards. Lower-class children, unable for whatever reasons to perceive the rewards of academic industriousness, are not motivated to achieve in school, according to these studies. Rosen and D'Andradeu found that mothers ant fathers of high—achieving boys set high but reasonable standards for their sons. This conclusion would confirm the findings of Baldwin and others;5 careless, overly—casual, indulgent, or permissive parents do not create in their children a significant desire to achieve. However, fathers of high— achieving boys made significantly fewer decisions for their sons, and gave them fewer directions. In a comparison of parental behaviors, Baldwin found that "democratic” homes lDouvan, op. cit. 2Hoffman, Martin L., et a1, "Achievement Striving, Social Class, and Test Anxiety." Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 56 (1958), UOlu3. 3Watson, Goodwin, "Some Personality Differences in Children Related to Strict or Permissive Parental Discipline." Journal of Psychology, MA (1957), 227—”9. “Rosen, B. C., and D'Andrade, R. G., "The Psychosocial Origins of Achievement Motivation.” American Sociological Review, 22 (1959), 185—218. 5Baldwin, Alfred L., et al, "Patterns of Parent Behavior." Psychological Monographs, 58 (19U5), No. 3. 28 (homes in which authoritarian methods and punitive measures were not evident) produced the greatest intellectual development, and that children from these homes were "significantly high on the variables of originality, plan— fulness, patience, curiosity, and fancifulness."l Mile32 showed that parents of the children who use their capacities as leaders in school do not overprotect their children, nor do they shield them from normal respon— sibilities. Instead, they allow independence and encourage children to experiment and to make decisions. Crandall 3 connected children's achievement behavior with and others their mothers' reward for independence. A synthesis of studies of the child—rearing antecedents of achievement—oriented behavior leads to the conclusion that either highly authoritarian or highly permissive practices discourage children's achievement. Fathers' heavy-handed insistence on excellent performance destroys achievement, but so does a careless lack of standards. McClelland resolves some confusion by exposing child—rearing patterns most likely to produce successful achievement: lBaldwin, Alfred L., et al, 93, cit., 66. 2Miles, K. A., ”Relationship between Certain Factors in the Home Background and the Quality of Leadership Shown by Children." Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Univer— sity of Minnesota, 19A5. 3Crandall, Vaughn J., et al, "Maternal Reactions and the Development of Independence and Achievement Behavior in Young Children." Child Development, 30 (June, 1960), 243-51. 29 What lies in the middle of all these extremes is reasonably high standards of excellence imposed at a time when the son can attain them, a willingness to let him attain them without interference, and real emotional pleasure in his achievements short of overprotection and indulgence. High achievement need may be created by stimulation for mastery, provided this stimulation is neither restricting, nor authoritarian, nor rejecting.2 Child—rearing practices of a number of populations have been studied, often in relationship to social class.3 If any generalizations can be made from a variety of populations and methodologies, it is probably that lower— class parents tend to be authoritarian and to use physical punishment, while more middle—class parents are either permissive or truly democratic, and are more likely to use love-withdrawal techniques in the process of socialization. 1McClelland, David C., The Achieving Society. Princeton: D. Van Nostrand Co., Inc., 1961. p. 356. 2100. cit., 345. 3The following studies are among those which point to the same general conclusions: Anders, Sarah F., "New Dimensions in F‘thnicity and Child Rearing Attitud.s.“ American Journal of Nontal deficiency, 73 (November, 1968). 505—8. Bronfenbrenner, Brie, ”Soeializatien and Social Class through Time and Space." In Readings in Sccial Psychology, edited by Eleanor E. Maccoby, et al. New York: Henry Holt and Co., 1958. pp. MOO—M25. Bronfenbrenner, Urie, "The Changing American Child—-A Speculative Analysis." Journal of Social Issues, 17 (1961), 6—18. Green, Arnold W., "The Middle Class Male Child and Neurosis." American_ Sociological Review, 11 (February, 19A6), 31—” . Hoffman, Martin L., "Power Assertion by the Parent and its Tmpact on the Child." Child Development, 31 (March, 1960), 129~U3. Medinnus, Gene R., ed., Readings in the Psychology of Parent— Cbild Relations. New York: John Wiley and Sons, Inc., 1967. Riessman, Frank, The Culturally Deprived Child. New York: Harper and Row, 1962, pp. 38—48. 30 However, other factors influencing achievement moti- vation have been found within the same social class. Herriot suggests that socioeconomic status is not so rele- vant to the student's educational plans as are the expecta— l tions of the parents as perceived by the students. Joiner 2 demonstrated that the student's perception of and others parent—child interactions had a greater effect on his educational ambitions than did his social status, and concluded, 8. . . these findings imply that more attention might profitably be given to the study of differences in interactions and norms within the social structure."3 Any attempt to predict from previous studies migrant parents' child—rearing practices and their effects on children's achievement results in a number of alternatives. Mexican—American fathers are reputed to be the authori— tarian leaders of patriarchal families—~a fact which may not bode well for their children's achievement. However, Mexican—American parents are also said to be warm, nurturant, and caring. Yet migrant families lack democratic organization. Certainly, the contingencies of their existence bring about f/ 1Herriot, R. E., “Some Special Determinants of Educa— Eional Aspirations." Harvard Educational Review, 33 (1963), 53—77. 2Joiner, Lee M., Erickson, Edsel L., and Brookover, Wilbur, B., "Socioeconomic Status and Perceived Expecta— tions as Measures of Family Influence." Personnel and Guidance Journal, A? (March, 1969), 655—9. 319g. cit., 659. 31 children's independence and responsibility. In some ways, those same contingencies promote indulgence and somewhat I careless lack of supervision of children's activities. It is impossible, then, to predict with any degree of cer— tainty the parent—child relationships and resulting achieve— ment of Mexican-American families and their children. From descriptions and personal observation of Mexicane American migrant families, one might expect these children' to perceive their parents as often loving and casual but seldom rejecting. If, as is supposed, mothers play a more important role in child—rearing than do fathers, then we would expect mothers to receive higher scores on signifiu cant child—rearing dimensions. If, however, sex roles are taught in part through differential treatment of boys and girls on the part of different parents, it is rea— sonable to suppose that mothers may play a more important role to girls, while fathers may play a more important role to their sons. It remains for this study to categorize the child—rearing practices of the Mexican—American family. Research concerning child—rearing practices indicates that those families whose child—rearing practices are perceived as less authoritarian but still reasonably high in expectation will produce children who achieve. In terms of the child—rearing dimensions measured by the Roe—Siegelman PCR, we might expect that Mexican-American migrant children whose parents are perceived as more loving, casual, and rewarding, and less demanding, protective, neglecting, rejecting, indulgent, and punitive would show higher 32 achievement. This study investigates the predictive value of the child—rearing practices for school achievement. School Achievement and Modes of Reponse tc Problems This section of the study is based on several assump- tions which are, in turn, grounded upon research with ”disadvantaged" populations and on clinical observations of society's institutions in interaction with the poor, We assume, first of all, that behaviors and attitudes often associated with the need for achievement are also linked to success in a typical American school. The student who succeeds holds aspirations toward goals approved by the middle—class culture. He is able to defer gratification, and to sacrifice immediate reward in order to reach his goals. He values the possibilities inherent in long—range planning. He possesses some model of successful achieve— ment with whom he can identify and after whom he can pattern his behavior. Such attitudes are also assumed to be at work as he solves the problems with which life confronts him, and t0' influence the choices he'makes as he attempts to reach his personal goals. Straussl found that a deferred—gratification pattern Was independent of social class but associated with the lStrauss, Murray, A., ”Deferred Gratification, Social Class, and the Achievement Syndrome." American Sociological Review, 27 (June, 1962). 326—35. ff 33 need for achievement. Strodtbeckl found that adolescent boys of the middle class believe that planning for the future was a good idea, while lower-class boys (quite possibly because of their concrete experience with the world and its rejection of them) felt that the future was unpredictable and planning, a waste of time. When long—range planning, deferred gratification, and goal orientation are ralued, achievement is supported. But here the marked disparity between the culture of the school and the cultures of the poor function to put poor children at a disadvantage. For these children, goals have rarely been attainable, basic needs are so great that gratification can not be deferred, and long—range planning has no payoff. The child of the fields, like the child of the streets, has little or no opportunity to learn these values. Indeed, given the conditions of his existence, they are not valuable for him. The patterns of physical growth and maturation and the system of formal education in the public schools are roughly the same for middle— and lower— class Children; yet they grow up in different worlds from a psychological point of view. The public school gives little recognition to these "different worlds," unless it is to condemn the difference. The predominant values of family and subculture which each child internalizes may or may not be appropriate lStrodtbeck, F. L., "Family Interaction, Values and Achievement.” In Talent and Society, D. C. McClelland et al. Princeton: D. Van Nostrano Co., Inc., 1958. pp. 135-9”. 2Kluckhohn, Clyde, Culture and Behavior. New York: The Free Press, 1962, p. 325 3H for school success, when success is limited and restricted by teacher values, and reinforced by a predominantly middle— class community. . . . it seems probable from life histories that lower—class children remain "unsocialized" and "unmotivated“ (from the view—point of middleuclass culture) because (ll they are humiliated and pun— ished too severly in the school for having the lower— class culture WhiCH their own mothers, fathers, and siblings approve, and (2) because the most powerful reinforcements i learning, namely those of emotion— al and social reward, are systematically denied to the lower-class child by the systems of privilege 'existing in the school and in the larger society. It is the school's tacit or stated expectation that children will have a desire for academic achievement and values approved by the middle class. Therefore, the migrant child may be expected to play the game without knowing the rules. He is ignorant of methods by which he might improve his station in life. To the degree that the Mexican—American migrant child has opportunity to learn "the rules," or feels support and reward from his family for adopting ways which bring school success, his achievement may excel that of his peers. Such opportunities, however, are limited by the very nature of the migrant workers' existence. ‘ Like any other child the child from a lower—class family views society within the limits of his immediate family and neighborhood. But often behavior which is sanctioned at home is seen as inappropriate by the middle-class school society. ‘ lDavis, Allison, ”American Status Systems and the SOCialization of the Child.” American Sociological Review, 6 (19141), 3145-514. . 35 . . . A family attempting to keep a roof over its head and enough food on the table to sustain life has little time or incentive to encourage and foster inquisitiveness, experimentation, and evaluation. Fewer Job opportunities make it extremely difficult for the migrant child, or even for his parents, to perceive the connection between education and new types of employ- ment. Separation from the mainstream of American culture and the depression, degradation, and exhaustion which accompany the migrant life make such perceptions well—nigh impossible: What does he want to do when he gets older? "I don't know, I'd like to stay someplace, I guess, and never have to leave there for the rest of my life, that's what. I could have a Job—-maybe it would be where they make cars and trucks and planes. I could make plenty of money, and bring it home, and we'd all live on it, my brothers and my sister. But my mother says someone has to pick the crops, and we don't know what else there is to do, and they'll come and beat you and throw you in the canal, the crew leaders, if you cross them; and then you'd be dead in one minute. So, we'd better stay with the Crops; because my mother is probably right. When asked "Do you think going to school does your children any good?" 93% of one group of migrant workers responded, "A great deal." Yet 73% of these same parents thought that their ch.ldren have "just the right amount" of schooling.3 As is the case with children of the urban lPease, Damaris, "Family Forces Influence Child Behavior." in Tne Disadvantaged Child: Issues‘and Innovations, edited by Joe L. Frost and Glenn R. Hawkes. Boston: Houghton Mifflin Co., 1966; 382—9, p. 385. 2Coles and Huge, 92' cit., l9. 3Man1s, 92. cit., 15—16. . .........._,rr.J; 1. a. ,_ , .1... .,. ..., n... a. .4“, ##5‘. al-}flwl, i 114%.. . fl‘jv.fi.vi I. 36 poor, migrant parents may state high expectations for their children's futures, without knowing or being able to communicate to their children how to reach these expectations: The high percentage expected to achieve the high status business, managerial and professional vocations is in sharp contrast to the small num— bers achieved by older children. This high level of expectation exceeds the proportion in the general population and reflects "The American Dream" of education as a means to occupational mobility.l We may conclude that the opportunities which education presents for the solution of life's problems and for the improvement of one's condition is often perceived by migrant workers. But the intervening steps which carry one through successful school experiences to the desired vocation are unknown. In fact, the very process of a continuum through time, during which one moves through a series of steps toward a specific goal, is a process unavailable to most of the poor in America. Certainly the child whose life moves only from central Michigan "in the strawberries," to the west coast of the state "in the cherries," to Indiana, ”to the tomatoes” encounters a deadening repetition which leaves the middle—class stepping—stone process shrouded in mystery. The limitations of a life in the fields make it impossible for most migrant children to fulfill the dreams of their parents. As Taba puts it, "The greater the variety of reality situations lManis, 92. cit., 18. 37 with which the child has COped, the greater his ability to cope with new stimuli."l Migrant life offers the child little variety, and little opportunity to deveIOp skill in COping with a variety of life situations. The ability to deal with life situations successfully ii; also mediated Jy the behavior of adults with their children. Hess and Shipman2 have clearly demonstrated the difference in this mediation between middle—class and lower—class mothers with their children. When young children, accompanied by their mothers, were given cognitive tasks, such as puzzle—solving, to perform, differences in children's performance depended on the mother's verbal and cognitive emphasis. The language of mothers of the lower socioeconomic group was more vague, less discriminating, less relevant to the task, and generally less helpful. Differences in the kinds of language used with children . in the middle and lower classes show that the middle—class child, given an "elaborated" language which provides him with a knowledge of structure of objects, a knowledge of interre— lationships, a means of thinking deductively and inductively, lTaba, Hilda, and Elkins, Deborah, Teaching Strategies for the Culturally Disadvantaged. Chicago: Rand McNally and Co., 1966, p. 7 2Hess, Robert D., and Shipman, Virginia 0., "Early Experience and the Socialization of Cognitive Modes in Children.” Child DeveIOpment, 36 (December, 1965), 869—86. 38 and an ability to perceive things and events in a matrix of time and circumstance, is far better able to cope with life's events than is the lower—class child who learn only a ”restricted" language.1 We must surmise, then, that the conditions of migrant life make it more difficult for migrant parents to understand or to communicate to their children the attitudes which would enable them to use‘ school experiences productively, and that these conditions would also be reflected in the migrant child's approach to real—life problems. Other factors in migrant life are assumed to affect the attitudes and values of migrant children and their resulting approaches to the solutions of the problems of living. Having little or no economic or political power,- and therefore less control over their own destinies, the poor may well see life as happening to them, rather than seeing themselves as having some control over or responSi— Vbility for the course of their existence. Mexican~American migrants are accustomed to having decisions aboutZtheir existence come from an ”authority" such-as the government, the crew leader, the grower, the priest, or "fate." Such lBernstein, Basil "Social Class and Linguistic Development: A Theory of Social Learning." In Education, Economy and Society, edited by A. H. Halsey, J. Gloud, and C. A. Anderson. New York: The Free Press, 1961. See also Gordon, John E., and Haywood, H. Carl, ”Input Deficit in Cultural—Familial Retardates: Effect of Stimulus Enrichment. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 73 (January, l9697——6OU— 10. 39 dependence may have considerable effect on school achieve— ment and success. In a summary of cognitive functions of disadvantaged children, gathered from a review of the literature, Cliftl notes that these children are generally perceived as having little persistence in problem-solving, dependent on external control, and passive in problem situations. Other writers have described this attitude, which would seem to be an inevitable accompaniment to the condition of being poor. As Riessman describes it: Frequently, the deprived individual feels alienated, not fully a part of society, left out, frustrated in what he can do. . . . He holds the world, rather than himself, responsible for his misfortunes. . . . he sees problems as being caused externally rather than internally. 2 Such an attitude may be labeled ”irresponsibility" and condemned by school teachers and other critics of the poor. It should be remembered, however, that the poor really are alienated, left out, and frustrated, and that society and the school really have not given the poor an opportunity to better or to control their own destinies. The result of being poor, which is fostered by the general society, is recourse to authority, to fantasy, or to a kind of sub— missive withdrawal from what the middle class would see as the major issues of their existence. Coles provides a sensitive report of the prevailing attitudes of many poor 1Clift, Virgil A., "Curriculum Strategy Based on the Personality Characteristics of Disadvantaged Youth." Journal 9f Negro Education, 39 (Spring, 1969), 9U—IOU. 2Riessman, Frank 92. cit., 27. 40 people whom he has interviewed: For them it is a matter—~to use the words I hear again and again—~of ”prayer," of "waiting and hoping," of "seeing what will happen," or ”relying on the next day when it will get better" or alter— natively, of "preparing for tomorrow when the worst will come." In psychiatry we use the word "passive" to describe a certain psychological quality-—to describe the kind of person who does not act, but is acted upon, who does not initiate things but rather waits for them to happen, who yields and submits and does the "taking" in the "give—and—take" of life.1 The "Coleman Report" found this attitude to be an outstanding deterrent to school success: Of all the variables measured in the survey, the attitudes of student interest in school, self—concept, and sense of environmental control show the strongest relation to achievement. . . . A pupil attitude factor which appears to have a stronger relationship to achievement than do all "school" factors together is the extent to which an individual feels that he has some control over his destiny. Lacking this control, the poor are often forced to find other ways of relieving frustration and enhancing self. If one cannot in reality produce solutions, one can dream them: Casting out the poor and the Negro from white society has resulted in a social life so saturated by illusion that the fancy soon becomes the only possible achievement. lColes, Robert, and Clayton, Al, Still Hungry in America. New York: World Publishing Co., 19 9, p. 35. 2U. S. Department of Health, Education and Welfare, Equality of Educational Opportunity. Washington, D. C.: Office of Education, 1966, pp. 319—23. Italics supplied. 3Henry, Jules, "White People's Time, Colored People's Time." Trans—Action, 2 (March—April, 1965), 31-“. p. 32. 41 Like the lower-class adolescents of Havighurst'sl study, who chose the most prominent and glamorous persons with whom to identify, and like the Negro youths studied by Boyd2 and by Antonovsky3, migrant children may well develop aspirations which, given the unjust conditions of their existence, are highly unrealistic and probably unattainable—-aspirations which they have no knowledge of how to reach~~in short, dreams. In characterizing the stories of first—grade, urban, disadvantaged children, Taba adds evidence of withdrawal in response to frustration: The stories that the same children dictated about their families included recurring examples of with- drawal, or "running away” in the face of a conflict of difficulty: when father gets mad he "goes away and stays all night"; when brother gets mad "he goes to a baseball game"; .u One might expect, then, that customary responses of poor children to frustration or failure would be resignation, appeal to authority, fantasy, or withdrawal. The values of the child's subculture, mediated by his parents in their relationships with him, are reflected in the way he attacks problems of daily living. The behavior lHavighurst, Robert J., et al, "The Development of the Ideal Self in Childhood and Adolescence.” In The Self in Growth, Teaching, and Learning, edited by Don E. Hamachek. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice-Hall, inc., 1965. 226-39. 2Boyd, George Felix, "The Levels of Aspiration of White and Negro Children in a Non—Segregated Elementary School." Journal of Social Psychology, 36 (1952), 191—6. 3Antonovsky, Aaron, and Learner, Melvin J., "Occupa- tional Aspirations of Lower Class Negro and White Youth." Social Problems, 7 (Fall, 1959), 132—8. qTaba and Elkins, gp. cit., 5. 42 patterns which his family and his culture teach him, we assume, will be reflected in his typical responses to problems of frustration or failure. His success in a middle—class school and a middle-class world will depend, in part, upon whether he advances or retreats in the face of new tasks, whether he foresees and plans for the future or lives from day to day, whether he is resourceful in finding ways out of difficulties or resigns himself to be defeated by them, and whether he relies upon fantasy to escape from frustration or makes an intelligent attack upon his problems. This study assumes that these behavior patterns will be reflected in school achievement, and that children whose modes of response to problems are typically withdrawal, fantasy, or appeal to authority, would be significantly poorer in school achievement than children in whom achieve— ment—oriented, planful and resourceful responses pre- dominate. If this should be the case, some of the mediating factors in school retardation or success, expressed in modes of response to problems, would be indicated. Such findings would provide clues to ways in which parents could be helped to provide a climate at home more conducive to school achievement, as well as ways in which the school could modify curriculum and instruction to promote achieve- ment among children it has formerly "disadvantaged." Summary The present study investigated several traits in Mexican—American migrant children and the possible relationship 1&3 of each of these traits to school achievement. It was suggested that: l. achievement in arithmetic and in reading would decrease with age achievement would not be related to sex. child—rearing practices would be perceived as predominantly loving and casual certain child—rearing practices would produce greater reading and arithmetic achievement than other practices modes of solving problems, as reflected in children's story completions, either would be goal—oriented and planful, or would seek escape from responsibility through withdrawal, fantasy, or appeals tc authority goaleoriented problem—solving would predominate in children of relatively high school achievement. CHAPTER III PROCEDURES AND RESULTS OF THE STUDY Age and Relative Achievement Standard achievement scores in reading and arithmetic, as measured by the Wide-Range AchievementTest,l were ob— tained by the school for 55 Mexican—American migrant child— ren. Administration was more advantageous than normal, since tests were administered individually and were un-timed. Subjects ranged in age from eight to 13 years. Table I shows the mean achievement scores and the standard deviations for each age grOUp in reading and in arithmetic.2 (See Appendix D fer standard score distribution.) TABLE I MEAN READING AND ARITHMETIC ACHIEVEMENT SCORES, BY AGE Age Reading Arithmetic N M s N M s 8—0 to 9—0 ll 79.09 12.21 10 90.00 7.11 9—0 to 10—0 ll 90.09 12.28 ll 91.00 7.11 10—0 to ll—O 17 82.00 9.31 18 8A.78 6.86 ll—O to 12—0 ll 8A.O9 17.08 ll 84.27 5.71 12—0 to 13—0 5 73.00 9.82 5 75.60 10.20 lJastak, J. F., and Jastak, s. 3., 32. cit. 2In some cases, numbers in an age group differ between reading and arithmetic, owing to the fact that some children were not available for the completion of both reading and arithmetic tests. Standard scores on the WRAT have a mean of 100 and standard deviation of 15. MA ‘45 Figure 1., standard achievement scores for both reading and arithmetic reached a peak at the nine—year old level, and tended to decrease in subjects who were ten years old or older. B 90 — E £180- m E 370- I l V 1 I AGE 8—9 9—10 10-11 11—12 12-13 Reading Arithmetic _______ FIGURE I. MEAN READING AND ARITHMETIC SCORES, BY AGE In all age groups, mean arithmetic achievement equalled or excelled mean reading achievement. A trend analysis:L was employed to determine whether the change in mean achievement scores between age groups was significant. Tables 11 and III show this analysis for reading and arithmetic scores, respectively. 1Coefficients for equal sample size were used to compute trend analyses, although there was some differ- ence in sample size across groups. H6 TABLE II TREND ANALYSIS OF READING SCORES BY AGE Source of Sum of Mean Variation Squares df Squares F Between groups Linear 81 1 81 .37 Quadratic 823 1 823 3.775 Remainder 70“ 2 352 Within groups 10919 50 218 Total 12527 5“ TABLE III TREND ANALYSIS OF ARITHMETIC SCORES BY AGE Source of Sum of Mean Variation Squares df Squares F Between groups Linear 888 l 888 16.15 ** Quadratic 106 l 106 1.93 Remainder 153933 2 76967 Within groups 2766 50 55 Total 157693 5“ **Significant at <<.01 level. Rpm—mew. 1% flIIIIIII::7_____________________________________________""W"r ' "“’” ‘ 47 The quadratic component for the relationship between mean reading score and age did not meet the required level of confidence. However, the linear relationship between mean arithmetic score and age was significant at less than the .01 level of confidence. Sex and School Achievement Table IV shows the mean achievement scores of boys and girls in reading and arithmetic. TABLE IV COMPARISON OF ACHIEVEMENT SCORES, BY SEX Variable Boys Girls N M s N M s Reading 30 82.5 12.37 28 85.0 15.52 Arithmetic 29 86.0 8.26 29 86.0 9.81 Since the number of cases in each age group ranged from 0 (for l2—year-old boys) to 10 (for 10-year~ old boys), boys' and girls' achievement was not compared at each age level. As can be seen from Table IV, mean achievement in reading and in arithmetic did not differ significantly between sexes. A8 Patterns of Parent—Child Relationships The Roe-Siegelman Parent—Child Relationships Questionnairel was rewritten in simple language for administration to Mexican-American migrant children. (Appendix A contains samples of the Questionnaire for both mothers and fathers, as it was rewritten.) These instruments were administered to small groups of children. Each statement was read aloud to the children, to eliminate the effect of reading difficulties on children's scoring. Table V summarizes the mean scores for all children on each dimension included in the questionnaire, for each of four parent—child groups. Table V reveals that despite differences across parent—child relationships, the perceptions of Mexican- American migrant boys and girls of the behavior of their mothers and fathers toward them follow a con- sistent pattern. Mexican—American parents from the migrant stream are perceived by both their sons and their daughters as more loving, protecting, demanding, and casual than as rejecting or neglecting. They are also perceived as tore rewa dixr than punishing. Despite this consistent trend, there are inter- esting differences among parents, children,and dimen- sions of parent-child relationships. Analysis of variance was employed to examine differences in —__ 1Roe and Siegelman, op. cit. “9 Ha.om m~.mm mo.- mm.mm ea.mm om.mm semenmacse HH< -.~N =w.mm om.mm ma.mm mm.am oz.mm esteemaczm ouo ao.mm mm.~m ma.wm mm.mm m=.mm om.mm onesemecsm qum pa.=m ms.mm mm.zm pa.mm wm.mm mm.mm epmzmm HH< mm.:m mm.zm o:.:m om.sm oo.mm om.mm ehmzmm one oo.mm mm.mm oe.sm mm.oe HA.H: mm.oe eeezom gum ms.mm :e.:: ms.:m OH.me oo.oz om.:: oeneomawez mo.fie 50.x: oa.mm HH.:: ew.e: mm.o: meepeommm mm.e: em.w: om.m: oa.am mm.mm om.me Haemeo ms.e: m:.am oo.e: ma.mm ow.om mm.mm meeeeeEeo mo.m: mu.me om.om m>.om mm.om u.en uefleoepoem ou.mm om.mm oe.en e..em ma.nu mm.um mee>oe cpom whom wflhwo Spom whom mates mhmflpmm mhmflpOE COHWCOEHQ mquU DZ< mwom BzHmommm m< mmHzmonB mqmo 4 MD. on» cwcp mmoa .xv,.um . 1:) 2:1» mama :*mow. .Uwom mao.u mfia.l .zom wwo.n mma.: moo. ”csm $*mfim.| **Ns:.- one. mmeflo. .wmm **:m:.x **mam.- Foo. soaz. mmmoa. wmz *mam.v *mmm.n *mmwm.u 5mm. **mon. mama. .mwo Nae :Hfi.) 05H. momm. *me. *zflm. mxsex. .Ema wmo.| 0H0. NHH. **:m:. .mozz. How. *zmm. *ammm. .OLm sec. QNH. sawoo. mam.a mm .| mmo.; magm. Haw. mmam. .>oq .3pflh< .Uwom .zom .dzm .mvm .mun .mM) .EQQ .opm azu2m>mHzo< OHBmszHm< Qz< UZHQoH Ho. on» sags mama pm peacheficwfim *. .Hm>mH mo. was ewes mama pm pcmoaefiewfim * tcmmw. .vMom sqfi. mm“. .Smm 0H0. QNH.I 3mm. .de mmd... 3mm... 00).: -mmu .fimm mom.l omm.: mmH.I ethx. *wmmw. .wmz NNN.I *mmm.l Hut. mum . **Hw:. *ON:. .wmo QHH. mmo.! *moo. *Nmm. **mmq. *omm. **moo. .Emo omo.| mmo. **wmw. wmm. mma. mwa. **mm:. **Hm:. .opm *Onm. mom. $$DNN. MAO. MHM.I Hmm.l 3mm. mmmom. **mm:. .>OA .cpflhd .Udo: .ZOm .sz .nom .mwz .mmo .EmQ .ogm Bzm2m>mHmo< OHBMEEBHK< 92¢ UZHQ >H HHH HH H soapoHQEoo Hmpoe Emmaquo BzHx mqm 97 Gordon, John E., and Haywood, H. Carl, ”Input Deficit in Cultural-Familial Retardates: Effect of Stimulus Enrichment." American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 73 (January, 1969), 603—10. Green, Arnold W., ”The Middle Class Male Child and Neurosis.” American Sociological Review, 11 (February, 1936), 3llfi1. Greene, Shirley E., The Education of Migrant Children. Washington, D.C.: National Education Association, 195“. Havighurst, Robert J., et al, "The Development of the Ideal Self in Childhood and Adolescence.” In The Self in Growth,_Teaching, and Learning, edited by Don E. Hamachek. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice-Hall, Inc., 1965, 226~39. ' Henry, Jules, ”White People's Time, Colored People's Time." Trans—Action, 2 (March—April, 1965), 31—3. Herriot, R. E., "Some Special Determinants of Educa— tional Aspirations.” Harvard Educational Review, 33 (1963), 153—770 Hess, Robert D., and Shipman, Virginia C., "Early Vvoerience and the Socialization of Cognitive Modes in Zhildren.” Child DeveIOpment, 36 (December, 1965), Hoffman, Martin L., et al, "Achiev ment Striving, Social Class, and Ta t Anxiety." Journal of abnormal and Social Psy;hology, 56 (1958) “01-3. "Power Assertion by the Parent and its Impact on the Child.” Child DeveloPment, 31 (March, 1960), 129-43. Joiner, Lee M., et al, "Boole—economic Status and Perceived hxpectations an JcaHJPkl of Family influence.” Personnel and Guidance Journal, A? (March, 1969), 655-9. Johnson, Elizabeth 8., Welfare of Families of Sugare Beet Workers. Washington, D.C.: U.S. Department of Labor, Children's Bureau Publication No. 937, 1935. Justman, Joseph, "Academic Aptitude and Reading lest Scores of Disadvantaged Children Showing Varying Degrees of Mobility." Journal of Educational Measurement, 2 (December, 1965), 151—5. ~(t 1.. Kelly, Clarence H., ”Men h.¢1nica] strvesting.” Scientific American, 217 (August, 1967), 50— 59. Kluckhehn, Clyde, Culture and Behavior. New York: The Free Press, 1962. Kohn, Melvin L., and Carroll, Eleanor H., "Social Class and Allocation of Parental Responsibilities." Sociometry, 23 (December, 1960), 372-92. Koos, E. L., They Follow the Sun. Jacksonville: Florida State Board of Heath, 1957. - ‘ Manis, Jerome G., A Study of Minrant Education. Kalamazoo: Western Michigan University Press, 1958. McClelland D. G., The AChiLVng Society. Princeton: Van Nostrand and Co., Inc., 1961. Medinnus, Gene H., ed., Readings in the Psychology of Parent—Child Relations. New York: John Wiley and Sons, Inc., 1967 , Miles, K. A., ”Relationship between Certain Factors in the Home Background and the Quality of Leadership Shown by Chiltren." Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Minnesota, 19MB. Miller, Daniel H., and Swanson, Guy E., Inner Conflict and Defense. New York: Henry Holt amt Co. ,1960. Moore, Truman E., The Slaves We Rent. New York: Random House, 1965. Murstein, Bernard 1., ed., Handbook of Projective Technigues. New York: Basic Hooks, Inc., 1965. National Education Association, ”America on the Move." NEA Research Bulletin, 36 (December, 1958), 99’102. Passow, A. Harry, ed., Education in Depressed Areas. New York: Bureau of Publications, Teachers College, 1963. - Pease, Damaris, "Family Forces Influence Child Behavior." In The Disadvantaged Child: Issues and Innovations, edited by Joe L. Frost and Glenn R. Hawkes. Boston: Houghton Mifflin Co., 1966, 382-9. Reul, Myrtle E., Where Hannibal Led Us. New York: Vantage Press, 1966. fi—_r -1, 99 Riessman, Frank, The Culturally Deprived Child. New. York: Harper and Row, 1962. ___ and Miller, S. M., "Social Class and Projec— tive Techniques." In Handbook of Projective Techniques, edited by Bernard 1. Murstein. New York: Basic Books, Inc., 1965, 95-106. Roe, Anne, and Siegelman, Marvin, "A Parent—Child Relations Questionnaire." Child Development, 3“ (1963), 355—70. Rosen, B. C., and D'Andrade, R. G., ”The Psychosocial Origins of Achievement Motivation." American Sociological Review, 22 (1959 , 185—218. Rosenthal, Robert, and Jacobson, Lenore, Pygmalion in the Classroom. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston, Inc., 1968. Rosenzweig, S., ”Rosenzweig Picture—Frustration Study, Children's Form." In Projective Techniques with Children, edited by A. I. Rabin and Mary Haworth. New York: Grune and Stratton, 1960. Shotwell, Louisa H., The Harvesters The Story Of the Migrant People. Garden City: t.ubieuay and Co., 1961. 6 State of Florida, Planning Florida's Migrant Bducation Program. Tallahassee: Department of Education, 1966. Stolz, Lois Meek, ”Effects of Maternal Employment on Children." Child Development, 31 (1960), 799—82. Strauss, Murray A., "Deferred Gratification, Social Class, and the Achievement Syndrome.” American . r . ’—\ ~7--7--‘ Soc1ological Review, 2] (June, 1963), 32o~33. Stodtbeck, ”Family Interaction, Values and Achievement." In Talent and Society by D. C. McClelland et al. Princeton: Van Nostrand and Co., in.., 1956. Pp. 135—9”. Taba, Hilda, and Elkins, Deborah, Teaching Strategies for the Culturally Disadvantaged. Chicago: Rand McNally and Co., 1966. U.S. Department of Health, Education and Welfare, Equality of Educational Opportunity. Washington, D.C.: Office of Education, 1960. Wallach, Michael A., and Kogan, Nathan, Modes of Thinking in Young Children. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston, Inc., 1965. 100 Watson, Goodwin, "Some Personality Differences in Children Related to Strict or Permissive Parental Discipline." Journal of Psychdlogy, an (1957), GENERAL REFERENCES Ausubel, David P., et al, "Perceived Parent A‘titudes as Determinants of Children's Ego Structure.‘ Child Development, 25 (195”), 173—83. Brookover, Wilber E., et al, SelfuConcept of Ability and School Ability and School Achievement. East Lansing: Michigan State University Press, 1965. Coles, Robert. Children of Crisis. Boston: Atlantic- Little, Brown, 1967. Colorado State Department of Education, Providing Education for Migrant_phildren. Denver: Office of Instructional Services, 1961. Combs, Arthur W., and Snygg, Donald, Individual Behavior. New York: Harper and Bros., 1959. Cronbach, Lee J., Essentials of Psychological Testing. Second edition. New York: Harper and Row, 1960. Fink, Martin E., "Self—concept as it Relates to Academic Underachievement." In The Self in Growth, Teaching, and Learning, edited by Don E. Hamachek. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice—Hall, Inc., 1965. Pp. U86-92. McClelland, D. C., et al, Talent and Society. Princeton: Van Nostrand and Co., Inc., 1958. Mead, George H., Mind, Self, and Society. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1934. Quillen, I. James, et al, ”The Value Orientation of Teacher Education." Ninth Yearbook of the AACTE, 1956, pp. 11—19. ‘ Radke—Yarrow, Marian, et al, ”Social Perceptions and Attitudes of Children." In The Self in Growth, Teaching, and Learning, edited by Don E. Hamachek. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice—Hall, Inc., 1965. 101 102 Sarason, Seymour 8., et a1, Anfij;tywin_:lgmentary School Children. New York: duhn Wiley and Sons, Inc., 19 0. Sears, Pauline S., and Sherman, Vivian 8., In Pursuit of Self-esteem. Belmont, California: Wadsworth Publishing Co., 196”. Sears, Robert R., ct a1, Patterns of Child Rearing. Evanston: Row, Peterson, and Co., 1957. Sutton, Elizabeth, Knowing and Teaching the Migrant Child. Washington, D.C.: National Education Association, 1960. Winterbottom, Marian R., "The Relation of Childhood Training in Independence to Achievement Motivation." Unpublishri doctoral dissertaion, University of Michigan, 1953. APPENDICES APPENDIX A PCR QUESTIONNAIRE - MOTHERS Roe-Siegelman Let's practice marking one of these questions. Put an X on the line that fits you best. If none of the lines seem quite right, you may put the X between the lines. VERY SORT OF SORT 0F VERY TRUE TRUE UNTRUE UNTRUE My Mother 1. Objects when I am late for meals. 2. tries to get me everything I want. 3. complains about me to others when I do not listen to her. 4. pays no attention to how old I am when she asks me to do things. 5. lets me spend my money any way I like. 6. tells me what is good about what I do. 7. punishes me hard enough when I misbehave to make sure I won't do it again. 8. takes away my playthings when I am naughty. 9. is really interested in my life. 10. keeps forgetting things she is supposed to do for me. 11. takes me places (trips, shows, etc.) for being good. 12. spoils me. 103 PCR QUESTIONNAIRE VERY SORT OF SORT OF VERY TRUE TRUE UNTRUE UNTRUE My Mother 13. makes me feel ashamed when I do things that are wrong. 14. lets me know I'm not wanted 15. makes very few rules for me. 16. tells me I_am better than other children when I do well. 17. makes it clear that she is boss. 18. slaps or hits me when I do something wrong. 19. makes me feel wanted and needed. 20. is too busy to answer my questions. 21. doesn't make me follow rules when I am good. 22. is very careful about protect- ing me from accidents. 23. nags or scolds me when I am bad. 24. thinks it is my own fault if I get into trouble. 25. lets me dress any way I please 26. tells me how proud she is of me when I am good. 27. thinks I should always be busy. 28. takes away some money when she wants to punish me. 29. makes me feel what I do is important. 30. doesn't care if I get into trouble.. 10“ PCR QUESTIONNAIRE VERY SORT OF SORT OF VERY TRUE TRUE UNTRUE UNTRUE My Mother 31. gives me new books or records when I am good. 32. can't stand to punish me 33. punishes me by leaving me alone. 34. doesn't spend any more time with me than she has to. 35. lets me off easy when Ihdo something wrong. 36. treats me more like a grownyup when I behave well. 37. pushes me to be very good in everything I do. 38. won't let me play with other children when I am bad. 39. wants me to do things on my own. 40. pays no attention to what I am doing in school. 41. lets me stay up longer when I have been good. 42. keeps protecting me from teasing or bullying by other children. 43. makes me feel she doesn't love me if I misbehave. 44. doesn't want me to bring friends home. ‘ 45. gives me the choice of what to do whenever she can. 46. praises me in front of playmates. 47. tells me how to spend my free time o 48. spanks me as punishment. 105 PCR QUESTIONNAIRE VERY SORT 0F SORT 0F VERY TRUE TRUE UNTRUE UNTRUE My Mother 49. talks to me in a warm and loving way. 50. doesn‘t think about me when making plans. 51. lets me off some regular jobs when I have been good. 52. doesn't want me to play rough» outdoor games for fear I might be hurt. 53. shames me before my playmates when I misbehave. 54. doesn't like my friends. 55. lets me eat what I want to. 56. shows more love for me when I am good. 57. punishes me quickly when I misbehave. 58. gives me extra jobs as a punishment. 59. tries to help me when I am scared or upset. 60. doesn't care whether I get the right kind of food. 61. gives me candy or ice cream or fixes my favorite food for me when I am very good. 62. teaches me not to fight at any time. 63. frightens me when I do wrong. 64. goes out of the way to hurt my feelings. 65. lets me do as I like with my time after school. PCR QUESTIONNAIRE VERY SORT OF SORT OF VERY TRUE TRUE UNTRUE UNTRUE My Mother 66. pays special attention to me when I have been good. 67. makes me respect and mind her without asking questions. 68. punishes me by sending me out. of the room or to bed. 69. doesn't try to tell me every- thing, but wants me to find things out for myself. 70. lets somebody else take care of me. 71. lets me go to parties or play with others more than usual when I have been very good. 72. teaches me to go for help to my parents or teacher rather than to fight. 73. tells me how ashamed she is when I misbehave. 74. makes fun of me. 75. lets me choose my own friends 76. praises me when I deserve it. 77. always tells me exactly how to do my work. 78. takes away my things as a punishment 79. thinks my ideas are important and wants me to talk about them. 80. acts as if I don't exist. 81. gives me more money when I am good. 82. wants to have me play at home rather than to visit other children. PCR QUESTIONNAIRE VERY SORT OF SORT OF VERY TRUE TRUE UNTRUE UNTRUE My Mother 83. says other children are better than I am when I have done something wrong. 84. complains about me. 85. lets me work by myself 86. makes me feel proud when I do well. 87. pushes me to do well in school 88. punishes me by being more strict about rules. 89. lets me do things I think are important, even if it causes her some trouble. 90. pays no attention to me. 91. hugs me, kisses me, pets me when I am good. 92. doesn't let me go places because something might happen to me. 93. explains why I shouldn't do wrong things. 94. says other children are better than I am, no matter what I do- 95. doesn't care if I loaf or day- dream. 96. praises me to others. 97. won't let me argue with her ideas. 98. punishes me by not taking me on trips or visits etc. that I have been promised. 99. tries to help me learn to live happily. 100. ignores me as long as I don't do anything to bother her. 108 PCR QUESTIONNAIRE VERY SORT OF SORT 0F VERY TRUE TRUE UNTRUE UNTRUE ' My Mother 101. gives me new things as a reward, such as toys. 102. hates to refuse me anything. 103. thinks it's bad for a child to be given love and tenderness i 104. doesn't tell me what time to be home when I go out. 105. wants to have complete control over my ac t ions . 106. will talk about rules with me, and listens to my ideas about them. 107. doesn't care who my friends are- 108. worries about me when I am away. 109. doesn't want me around at all when she has company. ' 110. doesn't care when I am late for meals. 111. teaches me that she knows best and that I must agree with her decisions. 112. wants me to bring friends home, and tries to make things nice for them. 113. leaves me alone when I am upset. 114. won't let me try things if there is any chance I will make a mis- take or not do well. 115. expects children to be bad if they are not watched. 116. is easy with me. 117. expects me to mind her quickly without asking any questions. 109 PCR QUESTIONNAIRE VERY SORT 0F SORT OF VERY TRUE TRUE UNTRUE UNTRUE My Mother 118. teaches me what I want to learn. 119. doesn't try to help me learn things. 120. wants to know all about every- thing I do. 121. believes a child should be seen and not heard. 122. doesn't bother much about rules. 123. keeps the house nice by having a lot of rules for me. 124. makes it easy for me to tell her how I feel and what I do. 125. forgets my birthday. 126. doesn't want me to grow up. 127. keeps away from me. 128. doesn't check up on whether I do my homework. 129. lets me decide only about things that aren't important 130. says nice things about me. 131. doesn't care whether I have the same kind of clothes as the other children. PCR QUESTIONNAIRE - FATHERS Roe—Siegelman Let's practice marking one of these questions. Put an X on the line that fits best for you° If none of the lines seem quite right, you may put the X between the lines. VERY SORT OF SORT OF VERY TRUE TRUE UNTRUE UNTRUE My Father 1. never lets me off easy when I do something wrong. 2. tries to get me everything I want. 3. complains about me to others ' when I do not listen to him. ' 4. pays no attention to how old I am when he asks me to do things. 5° lets me spend my money any way I like. 6. tells me what is good about what I do. 7. punishes me hard enough when I misbehave to make sure that I won't do it again. 8. takes away my playthings when I am bad. 9. is really interested in my life. 10. keeps forgetting things he is supposed to do for me. 11. takes me places (trips, shows, etc.) when I have been good. 12. spoils me- 13. makes me feel ashamed when I misbehave. PCR QUESTIONNAIRE VERY SORT OF SORT OF VERY TRUE TRUE UNTRUE UNTRUE My Father l4. lets me know I'm not wanted. 15. sets very few rules for me. 16. says I am better than other children when I do well. 17. makes it clear that he is boss. 18. slaps me when I do wrong things. 19. makes me feel wanted and needed. 20. is too busy to answer my I questions. I 21. doesn't make me follow rules when I am good. 22. is very careful about protect- - ing me from accidents. 23. nags or scolds me when I am bad. 24. thinks it is my own fault if I get into trouble. 25. gives me as much freedom as I want. 26. tells me how proud he is of me when I am good. 27. never lets me get away with breaking a rule. 28. takes away some money as a punishment. 29. makes me feel what I do is important. 30. doesn't care if I get into trouble. PCR QUESTIONNAIRE VERY SORT OF SORT OF VERY TRUE TRUE UNTRUE UNTRUE My Father 31. gives me new books or records when I am good. 32. believes I should have no secrets from my parents. 33. punishes me by leaving me alone. 34. doesn't spend any more time with me than he has to. 35. lets me off easy when I do some- thing wrong. 36. treats me more like a grown-up when I behave well. 37. pushes me to be very good in everything I do. 38. won't let me play with other 1 children when I am bad. ' 39. wants me to do things on my own. 40. pays no attention to what I am doing in school. 41. lets me stay up longer as a reward. 42. protects me from teasing or bullying by other children. 43. makes me feel I am not loved any more if I misbehave. 44. doesn't want me to bring friends home. 45. gives me the choice of what to do whenever he can. 46. praises me in front of my play mates. 47. tells me how to spend my free time. PCR QUESTIONNAIRE VERY SORT OF SORT OF VERY TRUE TRUE UNTRUE UNTRUE My Father 48. spanks me as punishment. 49. talks to me in a warm and loving way. 50. doesn't think about me when he makes plans. 51. lets me out of some jobs when I am very good. 52. doesn't want me to play rough outdoor games for fear I might be hurt. 53. shames me before my playmates when I misbehave. 54. doesn't like my friends. 55. wants me to take everyday disappointments. 56. shows more love for me when I am good. 57. punishes me quickly when I do something wrong. 58. gives me extra jobs as punish~ ment. 59. tries to help me when I am scared or upset. 60. doesn't care whether I get the right kind of food. 61. gives me candy or ice cream or fixes my favorite food for me when I am good. 62. makes others give in to me. 63. frightens me when I do wrong. 64. goes out of the way to hurt my feelings. PCR QUESTIONNAIRE VERY SORT OF SORT OF VERY TRUE TRUE UNTRUE UNTRUE My Father 65. lets me stay up as late as I like. 66. pays special attention to me when I have been good. 67. expects me always to mind him without asking questions. 68. punishes me by sending me out of the room or to bed. 69. doesn't try to tell me every- thing, but wants me to find things out for myself. 70. leaves me for somebody else to take care of. 71. lets me go to parties or play with others more than usual as a reward. 72. teaches me to go for help to my parents or teacher rather than to fight. 73. tells me how ashamed he is when I misbehave. 74. makes fun of me. 75. lets me do pretty much what I want to do. 76. praises me when I deserve it. 77. always tells me exactly how to do my work. 78. takes away my things a punishment. 79. thinks my ideas are important and wants me to talk about them. 80. acts as if I don't exist. 81. rewards me by giving me money. VERY TRUE ‘—~_'— _.—' u——--. ‘_I. wad... .#--——-. SORT OF TRUE ”urn—n, SORT OF UNTRUE .4. M”). oar—q...— VERY UNTRUE PCR QUESTIONNAIRE My Father 82. wants me to have at least as mudh money as my friends. 83. says other children are better than I am when I do wrong things. 84. complains about me. 85. lets me work by myself. 86. makes me feel proud when I do well. 87. pushes me to do well in school- 88. punishes me by being more strict about rules. 89. lets me do things I think are important, even if it causes him some trouble. 90. pays no attention to me. 91. hugs me, kisses me, pets me when I am good. 92. tries to keep me out of things that might be unpleasant and embarrassing or make me feel bad. 93. explains what might happen if I do the wrong things. 94. says other children are better than I am, no matter what I do. 95. doesn't object when I loaf or day- dream. 96. praises me to others. 97. won't let me argue with his ideas. 98. punishes me by not talking me on trips, visits, etc. that I have been promised. VERY TRUE u.-_—. "—n—_.-x.;_ —...-.—a__.. u—JI-m-l-ac SORT OF TRUE SORT OF UNTRUE .- .a—u VERY UNTRUE PCR QUESTIONNAIRE My Father 99. tries to help me learn to live happily. 100. ignores me as long as I do not do anything to bother him, 101. gives me new things as a reward, such as toys. 102. hates to refuse me anything. 103. thinks it is bad for a child to be given love and tenderness. 104. doesn't tell me what time to be home when I go out. 105. wants to have complete control over everything I do. 106. will talk about rules with me, and uses my ideas when he makes rules. 107. doesn't care who my friends are. 108. worries about me when I am away. 109. doesn't want me around at all when he has company. 110. doesn't care when I am late for meals. lll. teaches me that he knows best and that I must follow every- thing he decides. 112. wants me to bring friends home, and tries to make things nice for them. 113. leaves me alone when I am upset. 114. worries a great deal about my health. 115. thinks children will be bad if they are not watched. PCR QUESTIONNAIRE VERY SORT OF SORT OF VERY TRUE TRUE UNTRUE UNTRUE My Father 116. is easy with me. 117. expects me to mind quickly without asking questions. 118. teaches me what I want to learn. 119. doesn't try to help me learn things. 120. wants to know all about every- thing I do. 121. believes a child should be seen and not heard. 122. doesn't bother much about enforc- ing rules. 123. is always giving me advice about everything I do. . 124. makes it easy for me to tell him how I feel and what I think. 125. forgets my birthday 126. doesn't want me to grow up. 127. stays away from me. 128. doesn't check up on whether I do my homework. 129. only lets me decide about things that are unimportant. 130. says nice things about me. 131. doesn't care whether I have the same kind of clothes as other children. APPENDIX B UNFINISHED STORIES Girls' Form Rosa had not been able to go to school very much. She had not been able to learn to read like some other children at school. When the teacher called on her in class, her heart would beat faster, and she couldn't remember the answers that she once knew. Then the other children laughed at her. Rosa knew she could learn to read if she had the books and some help. Her teacher let her take books home and she'practices reading to her family in the evening. Rosa was really beginning to learn how to read well. One day, the teacher brought some new books to the school. She asked who would like to read one of them to the whole class. Rosa was sure she could do it. She raised her hand and the teacher called on her. Rosa took the book and stood in front of the whole class. Suddenly, her mouth became so dry she could hardly open her lips. When she looked at the page, the words began to blur and jiggle before her eyes. She was so scared she could not read. She opened her mouth to say something, but all that came out was a squeak. The children were giggling at Rosa. Then they began to laugh. Rosa put the book down and went back to her seat. Nobody believed she could read. All the children in the room were laughing at her. Josefina is eleven years old. She has traveled around the country nearly all of her life. Sometimes the places she lives in are pretty good, and sometimes they are not so good. For a long time, Josefina had been looking at the houses people live in when they do not travel to work in the fields. She had decided that when she grew up she wanted to stop traveling and live in a nice house. She thought about it and thought about it. She told her friends what she wanted to do. But they all laughed at her. "How will 19g ever get one of those houses?” they asked. ”Where will you get the money to buy a house?“ Josefina wondered what she could do to earn enough money to buy a fine, large house for herself and all of her family. One day a teacher came to the camp. She came to teach the children who had not gone to school very much. She helped the children learn, and she made the mothers and fathers proud. "That's it!" Josefina said to her friends; ”I am going to be a teacher. I will be able to buy a house, and I will be able to help children learn." Josefina's friends laughed so hard they fell down and rolled on the ground. ”You!” they shouted. ”You can never be a teacher. Teachers have to go to school for years and years. Teachers have ' 1 n 2. 120 to know everything." Josefina wanted so badly to become some- thing, and she was very hurt by the other girls' teasing. She was about ready to turn and run away so they wouldn't see her tears. In Rita's life there had never been anything more beautiful than the red sweater she had seen in a store window in Michigan. The sweater was soft and warm, and white flowers were stitched here and there on it to make it even more beautiful. It had taken much courage for Rita to go into the fancy store to look at the red sweater. She told the woman in the store that she wanted to see the sweater that was in the window. The woman let her look at it, but Rita was afraid to ask to try it on, because she knew she had no money to buy it with. But she could tell when she touched it with her fingers how soft and warm it was. Rita thought and thought about the red sweater. She thought about how warm it would be on cold Michigan nights. She thought about how warm it would keep her when riding in the back of the truck. She told her mother how much she wanted the sweater, but her mother scolded her for dreaming about things that the family didn't have enough money to buy. She told her best friend, Alicia, about the sweater. Alicia laughed and laughed. "You're crazy to think about such things," Alicia said. "where would you even get enough money to buy such a sweater?” But Rita kept on thinking about it. She wanted that sweater more than any- thing. Linda could hardly wait to be old enough to work in the fields and help her family. But Linda is what her family calls a clumsy ox. She was always forgetting things, or dropping things, or breaking things, or bumping into things. Everybody said, ”That's always the way with you, Linda; you are the world's greatest clumsy ox.” Linda pretended to laugh as though she didn't care when people said these things. But she really cared very much. She wanted her family to be proud of her. She wanted her mother to say to the other women, "My daughter, Linda, is a good girl.” One day there were many tomatoes to pick. The farmer wanted everyone who could, to pick tomatoes. Linda knew she could do well and that this was her chance to make her father proud of her. She begged and begged to be allowed to pick tomatoes. Finally, her father said that she could, but warned her to be careful, and not to be a clumsy ox. At fll t, everything went well. Linda learned to look under the vines to get the tomatoes that might rot on the ground. She learned to tell when the tomatoes were just red enough to pick. Linda picked two baskets of tomatoes. She was hot, but happy. From the next row, her mother and father smiled at her. Linda began to pick faster and faster. She picked so fast she picked some tomatoes that were too green. "Look out!" her father shouted. Linda jumped, stumbled, and fell-~right into the lugs 121 of tomatoes! Nearly all the tomatoes were squashed, and Linda was covered with red juice and tomatoes and seeds. Linda's father was very angry. Linda's mother looked cross. The farmer was very angry. He said Linda's father would have to pay for the tomatoes Linda had ruined. Linda was very unhappy. Maria and her mother were anxious to get her ready for her first day at a new school. They thought it was important how Maria looked at school. It was important that her family be proud of her. Maria and her mother had saved for a long time. They saved enough money to buy the cloth for a beautiful red dress. Maria and her mother could sew well. They worked late at night to make her dress. All the family was proud of how beautiful Maria looked as she started off on the bus for school. But when Maria got to school, things didn't turn out right at all. None of the other girls was wearing a dress like Maria's. They thought she looked funny. The skirt of her dress was very full, while theirs were more straight. All the girls were laughing at Maria. Her best dress was no good. Maria had to walk all the way down the long hall to class, while all the girls looked at her and giggled and whispered about her. When she got to her room, she was ready to burst into tears. Boys' Form Story is identical to (l) of the Girls' Form, except the hero is called Luis. Identical to (2) of the Girls' Form, except the hero is Juan, and the teacher is male. Story is essentially the same as (3) of the Girls' Form. The hero is called Pablo, and his best friend is Ernesto. Pablo is interested in a beautiful black jacket, which was warm and soft inside and had big shiny zippers on all the pockets. The same as story (4) of the Girls' Form. This story is about Ramon, who wanted his father to say to the other men, "My son, Ramon, is a real boy.” Except that this story is about Miguel, whose mother made him baggy pants and a funny—looking shirt, this story is the same as (5) of the Girls' Form. fit APPENDIX C INSTRUCTIONS FOR JUDGES Attached you will find five stories (forms for boys and girls are given) and the completions provided for them by Mexican-American migrant 1. 2. children. using this system: You are asked to sort the story completions into five categories, Use one of the enclosed index cards for each child. Put the child's name on the card, and mark the first story completion with the symbol which represents the category you think is appropriate. For example, suppose you think a child's completion to story 1 uses more fantasy than anything else. You will mark the card like this: gal/f. figmgl, 7 M2 ‘ .1 r 1 Judge all the completions to Story 1, then all the completions to Story 2, and so on. If you are puzzled about the category in which to place a particular story completion, it may help you to leave that completion for a while and return to it after you have categorized several others. In some cases a particular completion may appear to contain elements of more than one category. To pick the category that is most influential, look for the way in which the child tries to solve the problem or reach the goal. Settle on the behavior that has the most influence in resolving the problem. The Categories 1. ”Withdrawal,” or ”resignation.“ Mark with the symbol W. Use this category when the child's story contains no real solution to the problem. Here, the child gives up, resigns himself to Fate, or replies as though saying, ”That's just the way life is." He may think about his goal, but he never finds a way to attain it. 129 123 2. ”Appeal to Authority.” Mark with the symbol, A. Use this category when the child does not solve the problem himself, but relies upon or asks an authority figure to take care of the problem for him. Examples of authority figures would be teachers, parents, or older brothers or sisters. 3. "Fantasy." Mark with an F. Use this category when the child resolves the problem by inventing a highly improbable or unlikely "solution." For example, he might meet a man on the street who gave him a hundred dollars, or a person of influence might suddenly undergo a change of personality. Fantasy answers have the quality of day-dreams, or of magic. 4. ”Anti-Social Aggression, Hostility." Mark AS° Put stories in this category when the child strikes back, takes revenge, or commits some other anti-social act in order to reach his goal. Examples would be stealing or the use of illegal force to attain what the child wants. 5. "Problem-Solving.” Mark PS. Use this category when the child presents the goal as reached by reasonable means which involve any or all of these behaviors: a. deferring gratification b. aiming toward long-range goals and taking realistic steps to get there. c. assuming responsibility for his own actions. d. effecting change through his own efforts, but not through authorities or anti—social aggression. APPENDIX D THE WIDE-RANGE ACHIEVEMENT TEST DATA The Wide-Range Achievement Test contains two levels, with three subtests (reading, spelling, and arithmetic) in each lev- el. Measures on the reading and arithmetic subtests of Level I were obtained by the schools for the children of the present study. Administration favored these subjects, since tests were administered individually and time limits were not observed. The normative population for Level I of the revised WRAT con- sisted of 5868 children and adults from seven states. Children were selected from schools of known socio-economic levels, and a representative distribution of IQ's was attained in the samplings of eadh normative age group. Split-half reliability for the read— ing sUbtest ranges from .986 to .990. Reliability coefficients for the arithmetic subtest range from .942 to .966. No other re— liability coefficients are given in the 1965 Manual of Instructions, although the authors cite one study1 which showed only small and statistically insignificant variations in scores of the same indi- viduals over five successive administrations of the WRAT. Standard scores (with a mean of 100 and a standard deviation of 15) and percentile scores are given for age and grade. The children of this study attained standard scores on the arithmetic subtest ranging from 65 to 106. The mean of the distribution of arithmetic scores was 86 and the standard deviation, 8.92. Standard scores on the reading sUbtest ranged from 60 to 128, with a mean of 84 and a standard deviation of 13.89. Table XVI shows the distribution of standard scores obtained by the subjects of the present study. lDeLong, A. R., "The Limits of Accuracy of the Test Scores of Edu- cable Mentally Retarded Individuals." Journal of the Association for Research in Growth Relationships, 3 (1962), 26-44. Cited in Jastak and Jastak, 22..gi£., 14-15. 124 125 TABLE XVI FREQUENCY DISTRIBUTION OF STANDARD ACHIEVEMENT SCORES ATTAINED ON THE WRAT BY MEXICAN-AMERICAN MIGRANT CHILDREN Standard Scores Reading Arithmetic 6O - 69 9 3 70 - 79 13 7 80 — 89 23 29 90 - 99 5 14 100 - 109 5 5 110 - 119 1 O 120 - 129 2 O APPENDIX E RESULTS OF THE STEP-WISE MULTIPLE REGRESSION ANALYSIS TABLE XVII MULTIPLE CORRELATION COEFFICIENTS AND REGRESSION COEFFICIENTS FOR PERCEIVED PARENT-CHILD RELATIONS AND ACHIEVEMENT Variables Dimensions Mult Cor Coef Reg Coef MOthers' perceived Neglecting .519 - 546 parent-child rela- Rejecting .542 - 644 tions and reading Protecting .590 .439 achievement Punishing .607 390 Rewarding .612 - 202 Demanding .613 - 080 Loving .613 049 Casual .613 - 036 MOthers' perceived Rejecting .513 - 684 parent-child rela— Punishing .595 464 tions and arithme- Demanding .619 205 tic achievement Neglecting .627 - 111 Loving .633 - 264 Rewarding .642 .246 Protecting .644 075 Casual .644 - 007 Fathers' perceived Casual .333 - 749 parent-child rela- Loving .520 .596 tions and reading Rejecting .524 306 achievement Punishing .530 — 224 Demanding 535 - 147 Protecting .538 222 Rewarding .541 - 218 Neglecting .544 - 132 Fathers' perceived Loving .339 323 parent-Child rela- Casual .506 - 421 tions and arithme- Demanding .537 186 tic achievement Rewarding .555 - 237 Neglecting .572 - 249 Rejecting .596 .165 Punishing .597 074 Protecting .597 - 021 126 '::",'x-:‘:-7:. '- " - _ "'7 1' - V. ' MICHIGON STATE UNIV 1 Ill/l " w l 312l‘l'l’101795