.. 3.: mar! 2.. v. Ech Er s E b... :1 51,... 4%.. J J. awmwmwfl. Em? x. m... I A... .n .45....» $4.. €31.41: : Lawfixhi 75:93! v.33}... 1:53:- 2'1; if.” :1: h. EL»... 3....» ~ .511. it. l.....fl.uu-.«Véi..hnwl 1:412... . .1551: 2.... 167%... .- 51:59.»..ruilxllnfn «lift... :83 a 1. )»y(w...y(1.‘l) 1.: \ A... . ’31.? . '- lI \ .N/ )....I.v.1.¢’.vl../! ...l. 3st,? lllllllll lllll llllllllllllllll - 3 1293 10181 2497 £11594 i1! 7 Michigan State g. University r5 l l BjThis is to certify that the 4 i ‘ thesis entitled The Effects of Intuition, Feeling, and Similarity on Predictive Empathy presented by Martha L. Aldenbrand has been accepted toWards fulfillment of the requirements for Ph.D. mag“... in Psychology 9" lfj/gk 4/ ‘5:sz7—\ Major professor DateLL". '~ y’ 4 E 0-7 639 W278". ,139 [1149 - 0X A350 .. ._.____.—_-—l ABSTRACT THE EFFECTS OF INTUITION, FEELING, AND SIMILARITY 0N PREDICTIVE EMPATHY By Martha Lynn Aldenbrand This investigation concerned relationships between empathic ability and preferences in the use of Intuition and Feeling as modes of understanding others. A secondary feature was the exploration of how similarity of Intuition and Feeling preferences between subject and target persons related to empathy. Empathy was defined in the "person perception” tradition as ”the ability to accurately predict the feelings, thoughts, and behaviors another person would ascribe to himself.” The Myers-Briggs Type Indicator was used to assess Intui- tion versus Sensing and Feeling versus Thinking. Four 30 minute videotapes were made of the interaction of each of two groups of female target persons, each containing a representa— tive of Intuitive Feeler (IF), Intuitive Thinker (IT), Sensing Feeler (SF), and Sensing Thinker (ST) types on the Myers-Briggs. All fell within the middle range on Extraversion-Introversion. Following each videotaped session, targets individually monitored a replay and stopped the tape to identify feelings and thoughts they had had during the original meetings which had been structured to elicit typical examples of personality functioning. From these data four types of Martha Lynn Aldenbrand multiple-choice questions were constructed, those assessing prediction of (l) target feelings, (2) target thoughts, (3) target behaviors, and (4) inter-target sociometric relationships. Female observers were also selected for Myers-Briggs scores to represent 9 categories: IF, IT, SF, ST, plus observers scoring in the middle range on Intuition-Sensing and Feeling-Thinking. Observers were informed which target person was the focus of each question, shown a short videotaped sequence, and when the tape was stopped, requested to answer the relevant multiple-choice question. A second measure of empathic accuracy and similarity was given after the final taped session of each group, when observers completed an abbreviated form of the Myers-Briggs test the way she thought each individual target would respond. Hypotheses that empathic accuracy would be facilitated by Intui- tion and Feeling orientations as well as similarity between subject and target were tested. Findings revealed that empathic accuracy was only slightly above a chance level (approximately 38% versus 25%) across all observers. There were no significant differences related to Intuition, Feeling, or similarity. The present investigation's design fell into the common error in this research area that the products of empathy, i.e., correct answers, were obtained without attention to the underlying process of empathy. Empathy being a discontinuous process of attending and inte— grating, adequate research must allow both sub-processes to emerge. This study was directed toward the products of the attentional aspect, Martha Lynn Aldenbrand where the observer actively selects the cues she deems most relevant to understanding. It ignored, however, periods of integration where the observer was minimally attending to new cues, and, instead, internally deciphering and organizing already received cues. Thus, when the experimenter stopped the videotape during moments of integration, natural minimal attending may have reduced the chances of responding accurately. It was suggested that empathy research allow both natural timing and the personal interpretation of the stimulus event if the integrative aspect of empathy is to be expressed. Finding differential accuracy toward targets who supposedly repre- sented the same Myers-Briggs personality category obfuscated a meaning- ful evaluation of similarity. The expression of desired stimulus qualities was unduly influenced by other confounding personality traits, such as dogmatism, extraversion-introversion, etc. An approach which assures the prominence of appropriate target behaviors was suggested. l THE EFFECTS OF INTUITION, FEELING, AND SIMILARITY ON PREDICTIVE EMPATHY By Martha Lynn Aldenbrand A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Psychology l974 Primary: Secondary: Tertiary: DEDICATION To my parents, Marjorie and Godfrey, who taught me the importance of'empathy and attending to feelings. To my husband, John, whose patience, warmth, and caring made writing easier. 2b Melanie, who sang "Look What They’ve Done to My song, Ma" during those times I failed to see the logic of'my committee members suggestions. ii ACKNOWLEDGMENTS I wish to express my gratitude to my committee members: Dozier Thornton, Elaine Donelson, Joel Aronoff and Dave Wessel. Dozier's empathic understanding has always intrigued me and has made working with him enjoyable. Listening to my ideas as they hatched and knowing exactly when to add his own made the research fun. Later, his creative suggestions concerning the organization and readability of the disser- tation were very helpful. And his thoughts about future research modi- fications made me think of this research as a continuing project. Elaine contributed a substantial knowledge of the research literature which saved me considerable time and broadened my perspec— tives on empathy as a concept. Her valuable and detailed criticism of the design and manuscript helped me over many rough spots. She was also a much used source of warmth and comfort during trying times. I needed and appreciate Joel's dedication to simplifying my original design. His gentle, but persistent,_way of pointing out things I was overlooking helped anticipate and correct many potential problems. I am grateful for the considerable time and effort he spent proofreading as his comments were useful and thought-provoking. Dave Wessel gave useful statistical advice, especially concerning pre-design issues. iii TABLE OF CONTENTS Page LIST OF TABLES . . .. ..................... vi LIST OF FIGURES ........................ viii I. INTRODUCTION ...................... l Choosing a Definition of Empathy .......... 3 II. THE PROBLEM ....................... 8 III. REVIEW OF THE LITERATURE ................ 12 Empathy as a General or Specific Ability ...... l3 Recognition of Emotions .............. l6 Veridicality .................... l7 Implicit Personality Theories ........... l9 IV. DEFINITIONS, HYPOTHESES, AND RATIONALE FOR HYPOTHESES. . 22 V. MEASURES ........................ 27 Personality Assessment Measures .......... 27 The Myers-Briggs Type Indicator ........ 27 Empathy Measures .................. 33 l. The Videotape Empathy Series ........ 33 2. The Personality Empathy Test ........ 50 VI. PROCEDURE ........................ 52 Selection of Subjects and Group Members ...... 52 Procedure for Subjects ............... 54 Statistical Procedures ............... 57 VII. RESULTS ......................... 59 Main Effects .................... 6O Differential Effects ................ 70 iv TABLE OF CONTENTS—«continued Page Measurement Effects ................. 74 Post-experiment Data ................ 85 Summary of the Results ............... 86 Main Effects .................. 86 Differential Effects .............. 87 Measurement Effects .............. 87 VIII. DISCUSSION ........................ 90 The Stimulus: The Videotaped Group Discussion . . . 90 The Response: The Empathy Measures ......... 94 Assumptions Concerning the Videotape Empathy Series ..................... 95 Measurement Error gua_Videotape Empathy Series. 97 Measurement Error Related to the Personality Empathy Test .................. 100 Selective Videotape Empathy Series versus Person— ality Empathy Test Error .............. 102 Implications for Further Research .......... 103 Assumptions About Empathy as a Process ..... 103 The Complexity of the Design .......... 105 Specific Design Problems and Alternative Soluv tions ..................... 106 Summary ....................... 110 APPENDICES A. THE ASSESSMENT MEASURES ................. 112 B. DATA SUPPORTING THE USE OF THE MYERS-BRIGGS SCALES AS CONTINUA ........................ 126 C. THE EMPATHY MEASURES .................. 129 D. THE AWARENESS MATERIALS ................. 142 E. FEELING SUMMARY USED BY TARGET TO IDENTIFY FEELINGS. . . 146 F. POST-EXPERIMENT DATA .................. 148 BIBLIOGRAPHY .......................... 150 LIST OF TABLES TABLE 10. 11. 12. 13. . Outline for constructing the Thinking and Feeling Empathy Tests ........................... . Summary of Analysis of Variance for Videotape Empathy Series .......................... . Summary of Analysis of Variance for Personality Empathy Test ........................... . Analyses of Variance from Table 2 with Special Relevance to Hypothesis I ...................... . Analyses of Variance from Table 3 with Special Relevance to Hypothesis I ...................... . Analyses of Variance from Table 2 with Special Relevance to Hypothesis II ..................... . Analyses of Variance from Table 3 with Special Relevance to Hypothesis II ..................... . Means from Judgers-Measures Interaction (J x M) with Rela- tion to Empathy Measured via a Videotape Source ...... . Means from Judgers-Measures-Perceiver Targets Interaction (J x M x X) with Relation to Empathy Measured via a Video- tape Source ........................ Means from Judgers-Measures-Perceiver Targets-Judger Targets Interaction (J x M x X x Y) with Relation to Empathy Measured via Personality Test Source ....... Analyses of Variance from Table 2 with Special Relevance to Hypothesis III ..................... Analyses of Variance from Table 3 with Special Relevance to Hypothesis III ..................... Analyses of Variance from Table 2 with Special Relevance to Hypothesis IV ..................... vi Page 34 61 62 64 64 66 66 67 67 69 7O 71 71 LIST OF TABLES-«continued TABLE 14. 15. 16. 17. 18. 19. 20. 21. 22. Analyses of Variance from Table 3 with Special Relevance to Hypothesis IV ..................... Analyses of Variance from Table 2 with Special Relevance to Hypothesis V ...................... Summary of Analysis for Order of Presentation Effects. . . Means from Films (F) Analysis of Variance Reported in Table 16 ......................... Means from the Presentation x Films x Measures (M : F x P) Interaction Reported in Table 16 ............. Summary of Analysis of Variance for Congruence of Individu ual Target Representatives on Videotape Empathy ...... Means from the Measures x Perceiver Targets x Judger Targets x Alter Targets Interaction (M x X x Y x A) reported in Table 18 ................... Summary of Analysis of Variance for Personality versus Videotape Empathy Measures ........... . . . . . Means from the Form x Perceiver Targets X Judger Targets Interaction Reported from Table 21 ............ vii Page 72 73 75 77 77 80 81 83 84 LIST OF FIGURES FIGURE Page 1. Time schedule for group meetings and individual recall sessions ......................... 37 2. Eye direction of group members using split-screen cameras ......................... 47 3. Videotape screen configuration using one member as primary focus ...................... 47 4. An example of a Social-interaction rating scale ..... 49 5. Selection procedure according to distribution of scores on experimental variables ................ 53 6. Classification of subjects by scores on independent vari- ables .......................... 53 7. Arrangement of subjects and subject-names on screen . . . 55 8. Order of presentation of empathy tests per group ..... 57 9. Distribution of means from Measures x Perceiver Targets x Judger Targets x Alter Targets Interaction (M x X x Y x A) reported in Table 19 ................... 82 viii I. INTRODUCTION When one person speaks to another, he sends a verbal message, but he also watches to see whether, or how much, the other person understood what he meant. Likewise, the other listens to the verbal content of the message, but also watches for situational and non-verbal cues, like voice quality, posturing and the presence of others, to interpret the meaning of the content. It seems natural that with the number and complexity of cues available to the listener, some listeners understand cues better than others. This ability to understand the thoughts and feelings of another person is called empathy. Sullivan says the rudiments of empathy arise during infancy. The infant apprehends anxiety in the mother and in turn becomes anxious. However, early infant empathy is a phenomenon which the infant cannot control. He feels anxious, but his anxiety does not lead to construc- tive actions. He cannot soothe the mother or alter the environment. The usual result is that the mother soon apprehends the infant's anxiety and thereby increases her own. Since the infant is totally dependent on the mother, this leads to an intolerable situation. The only defense that works, according to Sullivan, is that the child falls asleep. As the infant grows, however, apprehending the emotional state of the mother does lead to increased skill, the child learns specific mechanisms of approach and avoidance. One would wonder, then, why people are not perfectly empathic, since empathy begins so early and people seem to grow more intelligent with age. There is one important altering factor, however, which con- founds the steady growth of empathy, that is language. As the child grows he must encode the meaning of feelings into words. But the source of the words, primarily the mothering-figure, does not always want the child to know her feelings. She may fail to label some feel- ings, leaving them less manageable, especially as the world is becoming more verbal to the child. She may mislabel them, either by intention or through her own distorted learning, calling anger "sadness" or fear ”confusion". The child must then cope with having some "admissible" feelings, some distorted feelings, and some non-verbal feelings. A study by Feigenbaum (1967) showed children to lag two to four years in their understanding of negative versus positive feelings in others. The need to encode basic experience into verbal labels, and the difficulty of attaining a complete translation, may be related to another factor often connected with empathy, namely intuition. Since intuition is commonly defined as the non-verbal apprehension of the primary meaning of a situation, an intuitive ability may be advantageous to understanding those blurred situations in which the child was in- tentionally or unintentionally mistaught the characteristics of certain feelings. An intuitive person seems to have left many basic experiences uncategorized, or freely flexible, so that verbal labels have not com- mitted him to a single rigid definition. This intuitive "regression in the service of the ego," matching present data to non-verbal data seemingly extant in all its original complexity, has long been a factor attributed to empathic skill (Katz, 1963; Rommetveit, 1960; Wescott, 1968). However, because of the elusive nature of non-verbal data and because the logical processes behind intuitively derived labels (if such logical processes exist) are seldom conscious in all their com- plexity, scientists have hesitated to measure intuition and use it as a variable in research. Thus, it has been long asserted that empathy and intuition are highly correlated, but it has rarely been studied. The process of matching the experience expressed by another with one's own experience leads to the consideration of a final factor related to empathy, that of similarity. Similarity between perceiver and perceived facilitates empathy because translation across experi- ences is easier. And in on-going situations, where a continual flow of information must be assimilated, ease of translation may be directly related to the amount of empathy produced. Similarity in terms of attitudes, beliefs, socioeconomic class, and perhaps more basically, cognitive-perceptual style, then, should tend to increase the likeli- hood of empathy. The present research employs the three concepts discussed above (attending to the other's feelings, intuition, and similarity) to assess the qualities of an empathic person. Choosing a Definition of Empathy Relating intuition, feeling-orientation, and similarity to empathy is not as easy as one would initially assume, however. One immediately must face the problem of the definition of empathy. At least five well recognized, but different definitions of empathy exist in the field. Directly related to the discussion of similarity is Smith's (1966) definition that empathy is the correct inference that similarity exists between the self and other. A second broader definition includes dis- similarity in the accurate prediction of the thoughts, feelings or behaviors of another person. A third definition emphasizes increased responsiveness to the feelings of another. A fourth adds the ability to communicate to another that one understands his feelings. And, finally, a fifth defines empathy vaguely as the capacity to adopt a broad moral perspective. The first approach to empathy considers similarity so vital to empathy that it defines empathy in terms of similarity. Thus, Smith (1966) defines empathy as “the tendency of a perceiver to assume that another person's feelings, thoughts, and behavior are similar to his own (p. 93)." According to this view, empathy is derived from identifi- cation with another person. Through the process of generalization one assumes the other is like himself in many ways. To the extent that one is correct in assuming similarity, he is empathic. With respect to dissimilarity, empathy is an irrelevant term. The second definition, that empathy is the ability to accurately predict the feelings, thoughts, or actions of another person is the definition focused upon in this paper and will be discussed in greater detail later. Since empathy is seen as the process of viewing the world as another person views it, similarity as well as dissimilarity are considered important discriminations in assessing the other's personal space, hence the departure from Smith. Empathy viewed as emotional responsiveness paralleling the feel— ings of the other was perhaps the earliest systematic treatment of empathy (Lipps, 1907). It was strongly related to Darwin's position in The Expression of Emotions in Man and Animals (1872) that certain universal expressions, such as sneering, result from common emotions, and that common emotions, e.g., fear, are elicited by the stimulus expressions. Following this tradition, role-playing may be viewed as the empathic awakening of emotions connected with adopting differ- ing role positions which results in deeper understanding of the experi- ences of the other. In fact, Kerr and Speroff (1951) refer to empathy as role—playing. Speroff (1953) further suggests that empathy is facilitated by role-reversal, in which the subject restates the other's point of view until the other verifies it as correct. Another branch of this tradition is the work of Stotland (1969), Stotland, Sherman, and Shaver (1971), and Mehrabian (1972). Stotland measures empathy by recording palmar sweating, vasoconstriction and basal skin conductance of subjects viewing an emotional scene. Mehrabian has devised an empathy test based on emotional lability. Those who report increased emotional responding in diverse social situ- ations are considered empathic. In assessing the Lipps tradition, however, the primary considera- tion will be whether concomitant emotional experiences are necessary for empathy to occur, not simply whether they are advantageous to empathic understanding. It is at this point where this author departs from Lipps' tradition. While emotional responses to the situation of another are assumed to benefit assimilating the other's experience, there appear to be many instances when one understands but does not share the experience of the other. In fact, Rogers (1951) differentiates between shared feeling (sympathy) and understood but non-shared feel- ings (empathy). In therapy this differentiation may be crucial-—one might understand that a client's mother had nagged him once too often and as a result he killed her, but if the therapist felt as angry as the client, we might have two murderers on our hands. A fourth view, usually preferred by counseling psychologists, defines empathy as effectively communicating that one understands the feelings of the other. Thus, inarticulate individuals are rarely empathic. Rogers (1951) was one of the first to add communication as a second step in empathy following understanding the other's frame of reference, although Murray (1938) did describe an interactive process, called ”recipathy”, through which a therapist might understand his client. Recipathy involved the therapist watching the feelings invoked in himself as the client talked. These feelings were then treated as feelings the client needed to invoke in others, not merely as unrelated therapist arousal. Truax and Carkhuff (1967), closely aligned with Rogers, have done extensive investigation into empathy viewed from the communication per- spective. Part of this work has included a scale for the measurement of counseling empathy (Truax, 1961). Empathy according to this approach includes responding with words, gestures, and tone of voice that matches the emotional intensity expressed by the other, responding concretely rather than abstractly to the messages of the other, and responding frequently enough that the other is assured that understanding continues to exist. While this paper agrees that the process of communicating understood messages is important, it takes as its focus the more fundamental process of understanding pg: sg without confounding under- standing with its communication. Finally, empathy has been defined as making moral choices (Hogan, 1969). However, this seems a distant relative from the definitions mentioned above. The scale derived by Hogan seems to be measuring a conglomerate of values, e.g., standing up for ”what is right”, feeling sorry for others, enjoying the company of diverse types of people. Empathy defined this way may be considerably correlationally based, i.e., people who are apt to be courageous, sympathetic, and openminded are also apt to be empathic. Since the present research attempts to measure empathy directly, this definition must be considered irrelevant. This research will use the second definition of empathy, that empathy is the ability to accurately predict the feelings, thoughts, or actions another person describes as his own. It is closely related but more comprehensive than the first definition in which empathy is defined as the correct prediction of similarity between self and other. Dissimilarity, as well as similarity, may be correctly predicted accord- ing to the definition adopted. This definition is also closely related but more comprehensive than the third definition in which empathy is defined as the concomitant arousal of feelings in the subject as in the other. In the present definition, the subject may respond to his own thoughts as well as feelings in attempting to ascertain the state of the other. The present definition arises from its own tradition and is often found in the literature called "person perception” or “interpersonal perception”. These terms will be used interchangeably with "empathy" when citing literature denoted as such. II. THE PROBLEM This research attempts to answer three major questions: (1) Does an individual's perceptual style with respect to whether he handles incoming information by using Intuition as opposed to Sensation, effect his empathic ability? (2) Does an individual's style for making deci- sions with respect to whether he decides primarily by Feeling as opposed to Thinking, effect his empathic ability? And (3) Does simi- larity between the target and the subject on the two orientations effect empathic ability? The hypothesized relationship of intuitive and feeling orienta- tions with empathic skill has lengthy common sense and clinical support, but, as yet, little experimental justification. Lack of research justi- fication is most likely the result of what seems to be the intangible nature of the process of intuition and the extensive amount of non- verbal interpretation necessary in identifying feelings. Only recently have some authors tried to measure the process of intuition (Rommetveit, 1960). These attempts, however, rely on making judgments when the premises behind the judgments remain out of the awareness of the judger. To assume that one cannot be aware of the intuitive process, or that all non-aware judgments are intuitive,seems absurd. Some studies have given minimal cues to subjects and ”forced“ intuitive judgments (Hathaway, 1955; Valentine, 1929). A few studies have defined intuition vaguely and explored it as a trait manifested in some people more than others. Estes (1937) found that the most accurate judges of feelings during motion picture segments were those who worked at intuitive occupations or had intui- tive avocations, such as painting and drama. Allport (1961) reports that people who adopt an analytical, reflective approach to perception are less accurate than those who adopt a more global, intuitive approach. Similarly, very little research has studied whether those who are oriented toward attending to the feelings of others and making judgments according to feelings as opposed to logic are more empathic than those who do not. Most research concerning feelings and empathy has focused on whether and under what conditions it was possible to recognize the feelings of others. They have attended less frequently to perceiver characteristics of good predicters of feelings. Although, Halpern (1954) did find a significant correlation between predictive accuracy on a personality inventory and femininity of attitude--a scale loaded heavily on the experience and expression of intense emotion. In order to study these two orientations, this research has employed a theoretical framework which definitively describes these processes, that is Jung's theory. According to Jung, each human being uses one of two alternative perceptual modes (called “functions”) from which he garners information about the world. He also uses one of two judgmental functions with which he makes decisions about how to use the information perceived. Jung calls the alternative perceiving functions “intuition“ and ”sensing”. Intuition is an unconscious organization of stimuli resulting in the individual only experiencing ”a complete whole, without ... being able to explain or discover in what way this content has been arrived at. (It) ... is a kind of instinctive apprehension, irrespective of the nature of its contents” (Jung, 1923, p. 263). On the other hand, Sensation relies on conscious experience gained by the sense organs, denying unconscious Intuition. Sensing is directed toward the objective, external environment, responding most decisively to the strongest stimuli impinging on the individual. According to Jung, Intuition and Sensation are opposite functions of perception, one uncon- scious the other conscious. The judging functions are ”thinking" and ”feeling”. They relate to how the individual makes decisions or comes to conclusions about the stimulus data he has acquired. The function of Thinking is based on certain ”laws” or logical structures, which may be either conscious or unconscious, and consequently may be either rational or irrational. An example Jung uses of the latter are thoughts arising during dreams, which seem reasonable in the dream, but unreasonable when awake. The function of Feeling relates to forming value judgments with reference to acceptance or rejection. Feeling may refer to specific objects, such as a beautiful rose, or whole situations, such as an ugly mood. Feeling and Thinking may also have contradictory components, an individual may think_a dress is ugly, but notice that her friend is wearing it and feel that the dress on the friend is beautiful. Jung views most people as operating with one dominant perceiving and one dominant judging function. Thus, most people are either Intuitive-Thinkers (NT), Intuitive—Feelers (NF), Sensing-Thinkers (ST), or Sensing-Feelers (SF). Moreover, they will display one of two primary attitudes toward the world, Introversion or Extraversion. These atti- tudes represent the degree to which the individual openly uses his functions. An Introvert keeps to himself, is wary about the environ- ment, and relies on his own resources for fulfillment. An Extravert is out—going, trustful, and dependent on the external world for fulfillment. Finally, Jung says that although most people can be assigned to one or the other of the perceiving or judging functions, those who become most fully functioning move toward greater differentiation and balance, thus tending to blend Introversion-Extraversion and the perceiving and judg- ing functions in their everyday lives. From Jung's perspective, then, the most empathic person should be one who does not exclusively use either judging or perceiving dimensions. To determine whether empathy is higher among Intuiters and Feelers, or middle perceivers and judgers as Jung would hypothesize, a simple design was employed which measured the empathic accuracy of subjects designated Intuiter, Senser, Middle Intuiter—Senser, and Feeler, Thinker, Middle Feeler—Thinker toward targets presented via a videotaped discus- sion. To determine whether similarity influences empathic accuracy tar- gets were chosen who represented each combination of Jung's perceiving and judging orientations. Thus, accuracy scores toward each NF, NT, SF, ST target were analyzed with respect to the subject's own perceiving or judging orientation. In this research, empathic accuracy was measured two ways: (1) by correctly predicting what targets said they were think— tngrorufeeling.during the videotaped discussions, and (2) by correctly predicting how targets would answer questions on a personality test. III. REVIEW OF THE LITERATURE Comprehensive literature reviews were made early by Bruner and Tagiuri (1954) and Taft (1955) and again more recently by Tagiuri (1969). These have been augmented by more circumscribed reviews by Shrauger and Altrocchi (1964) and Kanekar (1972). From these and other sources it can be noted that predictive empathy research has taken two primary directions, one investigating whether empathy was a general or specific ability, the other assessing the extent to which an individual could understand the emotions of a target person. This paper will dis- cuss these divergent paths as well as two additional topics affecting the outcome of empathy research: veridicality and the subjective forma- tion of implicit personality theories. It should be pointed out at the onset that a unified tradition of research has not been established in this area. In fact, experi- mental stimuli and responses have been so varied that few generaliza- tions can be inferred when comparing data. Stimuli have included photo- graphs, handwritihg samples, graphic displays of human faces, written personality descriptions and autobiographical accounts, projective test protocols, tape recordings, motion pictures, videotapes, role plays (usually of emotional situations), and other live interactions. Responses have included recognition of the emotion or personality dis- played by free responding or multiple choice answers, by sociometric 12 13 rank ordering, by predicting future behaviors or present occupations, and by predicting responses on personality or attitude tests. Subjects have been directed to interpret how specific targets would respond and how certain classes of persons, e.g., college students, would respond. In general, much of the literature agrees that stimuli and responses that accurately allow the complexity of the target and the subject to emerge are more likely to show significant differences among the vari- ables studied than are the most simplified measures (Gage and Cronbach, 1955, Jackson and Messick, 1963; Cline, 1964; Shrauger and Altrocchi, 1964, Tagiuri, 1969). Empathy as a General or Specific Ability One of the first problems predictive accuracy research tackled was to determine whether certain individuals had a heightened capacity to be empathic in all situations, i.e., in general, or whether empathy was more a function of the situation or the transparency of the specific target individual involved. Two of the earliest studies reported in this area established the format for later research. Steinmetz (1945) had subjects predict a target individual's answers on a personality test as a measure of "psychological perception'l or awareness of others. Dymond (1949), first using the term "empathy“ for this method, had group members predict how others in the group would rank themselves on certain personality traits, e.g., friendly-unfriendly, sense of humor. Both Steinmetz and Dymond report differences supporting empathy as a general ability. 14 The literature is equivocal on this issue, however. Allport (1937) suggests that judging ability is neither entirely general nor 'Tfimtirely specific. Some individuals are usually more accurate in general, but specific situations play a contributing role. Cline (1955) and later Cline and Richards (1960 and 1961) demonstrated that some generality in judging ability does exist apart from similarity of sub- ject and target. However, Gage and Cronbach (1955), Purcell, Modrick and Yamahiro (1960) and Stone and Leavitt (1954) suggest that little generality exists. Crow and Hammond (1957) and Sechrest and Jackson (1961) attribute whatever generality that exists to response set and other response biases. Reviews of the literature by Bruner and Tagiuri (1954) and Taft (1955) side with the generality view, believing that "good" and "poor" judges do exist. The methodology used with the generality-specificity issue has been criticized from two viewpoints which may partially account for the discrepant results cited above. It has been noted that success in predicting others ratings on personality tests or questionnaires may result from knowledge of a stereotype of the individual, e.g., male factory worker, as well as knowledge of the particular target involved. Most research has not differentiated between these two sources. Another criticism concerns the similarity versus dissimilarity of the targets and judges. Failure to systematically assess the effects of this dimension may unduly bias success toward target and judge pairs which are similar. Cronbach (1955) first pointed to the influence of the sterotyping factor. He demonstrated that accurate knowledge of the stereotype of the target was usually the best predictor of his performance. There- fore, one did not need a stimulus at all, other than a classification label, to "empathically" predict target responses. Thus, Cronbach devised elaborate scoring procedures for factoring out stereotype accuracy, differential accuracy (how the target differs from the stereo- type) and other response set tendencies. Bronfenbrenner, Harding and Gallwey (1958) found that two similar abilities: accuracy toward a generalized other versus sensitivity toward a specific person, appear to be independent. Cline and Richards (1960) found similar results. Another issue was confronted by Bender and Hastorf (1950) and Gage and Cronbach (1955). They distinguished accuracy on the basis of real versus assumed similarity and dissimilarity. Real similarity between subject and target has long been recognized as facilitating empathy. Allport (1937) reports this effect for sex, age, background, complexity, and personality characteristics. Similar results have been found by Notcutt and Silva (1951); Suchman (1956); Gage, Leavitt and Stone (1956); and Bronfenbrenner, Harding and Gallwey (1958). This beneficial effect may be related to the relative ease of assimila— tion and reliance on oneself as referent (Blanchard, 1966 and 1967) versus differentiation from oneself as referent (Lundy, 1956). Assumed similarity and dissimilarity were terms used by Gage and Cronbach (1955) to assess instances where the subject assumes the tar- get is similar or dissimilar to himself when he is not. Bender and Hastorf (1950) found that some subjects consistently tend to be ”empathizers” (assumed similarity) and some ”projectors“ (assumed dis- similarity). Bieri (1955) notes that cognitively simple subjects tend to assume similarity between themselves and targets, whereas cognitively complex subjects tend to assume dissimilarity and differentiation. Recognition of Emotions The literature on the recognition of emotions has shown that ease of recognition is related to at least three factors: the method of stimulus presentation, the degree of discrimination demanded of the subject, and the extent to which labels are self-generated. As described earlier, person perception experiments have used varied presentation modes, including character drawings, photographs, and motion pictures as stimuli. It is obvious that the amount of information presented, as well as its naturalness, may greatly differ among these methods. It is not surprising, then, to find that accurate labeling is more often found in complex presentations, such as motion pictures, than in simple, acontextual examples, such as photographs of real emotions elicited in a laboratory (Tagiuri, 1969). The difficulty of the discrimination demanded of the subject is also important. Since the work of Woodworth (1938) and Schlosberg (1952) who devised, respectively, scalar and orthogonally dimensional models for delineating the similarity of emotions, we have had a method to test the accuracy of discriminating emotional stimuli. Thus, it was shown to be harder to discriminate between love and happiness, than between love and anger (Woodworth, 1938). Research in this area has generally supported the degree of difficulty reported to exist between two given emotions on both Woodworth's and Schlosberg's scales (Davitz and Davitz, 1959b, Abelson and Sermat, 1962; and Engen, Levy and Schlosberg, 1938). Thompson and Meltzer (1964) did find, however, that some emotions: happiness, fear, love and determination, were easier to recognize overall than was disgust, contempt, and suffering. Thus, research which fails to account for the differential discriminability of emotional stimuli may confound the real results concerning accurate perception by making some tasks or items more difficult than others. A final factor affecting the recognition of emotions is the label itself. Subjects have been shown to be more often correct in labeling an emotion when they use their own labels than when they are supplied multiple-choice labels (Munn, 1940). However, free responding also produces methodological problems. Which label, the experimenter's, the target's, or the subject's, is correct? Fernberger (1928) found that ”false“ interpretations, i.e., those not generated by the target, were nevertheless acceptable to the targets unless they grossly dif- fered from the emotion originally named. On the one hand the new label may represent a simple substitution of a synonym, but it may also allow another more diffuse, non-contradictory emotion to pass for correct. Thus, the degree of difficulty of the empathy response desired should be reflected in the latitude allowed in labeling. Veridicality One central problem confronting empathy research is the determina- tion of what the target individual was actually experiencing as the stimulus was presented. Experimenters who use role players as stimuli have predetermined the emotion or behavior intended to be presented. The role player acts angry, fearful or surprised and the subject must guess the feeling imitated. One problem with this method, however, is that the emotion expressed is often exaggerated and/or stereotyped. In real life people may express their emotions subtly, only sometimes in a stereotyped fashion. But the problem is even more difficult for researchers who are trying to assess empathy by observing natural behaviors. Sometimes an individual is vividly aware of his emotions and behaviors, but some- times he may have only unconscious or non-verbalized awareness of his feelings. An individual may not know he is angry for three or four minutes after he has been displaying non-verbal cues signaling his anger. The knotty issue, then, is: who really knows what the target is feeling? The target himself? A seasoned observer? Who is correct? Most predictive accuracy research has avoided the issue by de- fining empathy as the prediction of answers on a personality test or attitude questionnaire. While this does have the appeal of objectivity, i.e., the answers can easily be verified, the flavor of the decision process or the actual experience of the target taking the test is un— known. Predictive accuracy via personality tests, then, is a valid but highly circumscribed method for assessing empathy. When we try to assess those circumstances most common to our usual meaning of empathy: the interpretation of live experiences, we are again faced with the veridicality dilemma. Research in counseling psychology, where empathy consists of recognizing the feelings of a client, has tended to favor the use of ”experts“, i.e., people assumed to be empathic by others, Such as counselors and advanced graduate students in counseling (Buchheimer, l- Goodman and Sircus, 1965; Campbell, 1967; Chapman, 1966; Rank, 1966). Subjects, in these cases, are actually trying to be empathic with the labels of empathic persons. When the expert and the target agree on the label, no grave problems arise. But despite their skills, being removed from the actual experience, often tends to make experts rely on more stereotyped or overly simplified answers than would be true with direct experience as reported by the target (Allport, 1961). At least one group of counseling psychologists (Kagan, Schauble, Resnikoff, Danish, and Krathwohl, 1969) have attempted to breech the gap in interpretation by employing a technique designed to help the target discover and elucidate the feelings he was having during the taped session. This ”process recall” consists of replaying the video— taped session before the target and an interrogator. The interrogator asks open-ended questions designed to elicit clear and vague feelings, thoughts and body cues of the target. While this method does not facilitate recall of truly unconscious material, it does help the tar- get focus on minimally aware cues. Kagan and Krathwohl (1967) and Kagan §t_pl, (1969) have shown substantial results using this method with counselor training. A more detailed description of the process recall method can be found in the Measures section. Implicit Personality Theories Implicit personality theories are inferential linkages which people assume to exist between personality traits. Among the earliest research in this area was the work of Asch (1946), who found the importance of a central trait, "warm-cold“, to significantly effect 20 the formation of impressions made by subjects, and Kelly (1955) whose Role Construct Theory gave central importance to the notion that people form idiosyncratic personality theories. Other psychologists have pointed to the importance of cognitive complexity in the differential assessment of persons (literature reviewed by Crockett, 1965; and Hastorf, Schneider, and Polefka, 1970), and the importance of the strength of a subject's evaluative dimension in judging others (Steiner, 1954; Warr and Simms, 1965). In addition, Zajonc (1960) and Cohen (1961) have shown that pieces of information are related more complexly when subjects expect to receive rather than to communicate the informa- tion. Research in this area has been of two sorts: (1) trying to un- cover commonly held personality theories derived from stereotypes or language based correlations, and (2) trying to link idiographic impli- cit theories to personality traits like cognitive complexity or authori- tarianism. Concerning the latter, Schneider (1973) concludes ”it is relatively easy to show that individuals differ in their implicit per- sonality theories, but there has been limited success relating such dif- ferences to traditional personality variables." The methodology of empathy research has dealt inconsistently with the effects of implicit personality theories, sometimes maximizing their influence, sometimes minimizing it. Methods employing the judg— ment of emotions from photographs, diagrams, or brief vignettes imply that empathy is merely the interpretation of specific stimuli, and thereby minimize the implicit personality effect. Methods utilizing personality tests, sociometric measures, and handwriting analyses imply 21 that from a few cues the subject extrapolates an image of a cohesive personality structure, maximizing the implicit personality influence. The present research recognizes the discrepancy and employs each of the two methods, one designed to enhance the influence of implicit per— sonality theories--a personality test measure, and one designed to minimize the effect--a videotape measure. IV. DEFINITIONS, HYPOTHESES, AND RATIONALE FOR HYPOTHESES Hypothesis I: With regard to Perception, subjects with high Intui- tive orientation will show more empathic accuracy than subjects with middle Intuitive-Sensing or Sensing orientations. Definitions: Perception (P): According to Jung, the method by which sensory stimulus data are translated into information by the sub- ject. The two extreme types of Perception are Intuition and Sensation. Intuition (1): According to Jung indirect awareness by the un- conscious, accompanied by conscious ideas or associations (hunches) which become attached to the unconscious material. Sensation (S): According to Jung direct awareness through the five senses. Middle Intuition-Sensation (N/S): Awareness which alternates between the use of Intuition and Sensation. Hypothesis II: With regard to Judgment, subjects with high Feeling orientation will show more empathic accuracy than subjects with middle Feeling—Thinking or Thinking orientations. Definitions: Judgment (J): According to Jung, the method by which conclusions and/or decisions are reached. The two extreme types of Judgment are Feeling and Thinking. Feeling (F): According to Jung, an emotional procedure culminat- ing in an appraisal on a subjective basis, usually involving some form of a good-bad dimension. Thinking (T): According to Jung,a procedure aimed at making an impersonal finding on the basis of logic or reasoning. Middle Feeling-Thinking (F/T): A decision making process which alternates between feeling and thinking. 22 23 Rationale for Hypothese_>I and II: These hypotheses are based on the conception that empathy is an alogical, intuitive process aimed prim- arily at understanding the feelings of others. Theoretical support for these hypotheses can be linked to Sullivan's developmental approach where empathy is prototaxic and parataxic, i.e., intuitive, originating when anxiety in the mother is apprehended by the child. Hypothesis III: Similarity of Perceiver orientation between subjects and targets will result in more empathic accuracy than between subjects and targets dissimilar in Perceiver orientation. Hypothesis IV: Similarity of Judger orientation between subjects and targets will result in more empathic accuracy than between subjects and targets dissimilar in Judger orientation. Definitions: Similarity of Orientation: The presence of the same Perception or Judgment function in both target and subject, e.g., a target categorized as a Senser and a subject categorized as a Senser. Dissimilarity of Orientation: The presence of opposite or par— tially opposite Perception or Judgment functions in both target and subject, e.g., a target categorized as Senser and a subject categorized as either Intuiter or Middle Intuiter-Senser. Target: A person viewed via videotape with whom subjects are asked to try to be empathic. Rationale for Hypotheses III and IV: These hypotheses are related to the assumption that we understand best those who are similar to us. According to this reasoning, an individual discriminates more stimuli in areas which are familiar to him, and thus can be more accurate, than in areas in which he is unfamiliar. Allport (1937) and others report this effect for age, sex, background, complexity, and personality character— istics. Smith (1966) defines empathy in terms of similarity. 24 Hypothesis V: With regard to empathy viewed via a videotape source, Feeling oriented subjects will be most accurate on a feeling- oriented measure, while Thinking oriented subjects will be most accurate on a thinking-oriented measure. Definitions: Feeling-oriented measure: A multiple—choice measure whereby sub- jects choose which of four alternative feelings a particular target experienced immediately prior to the completion of a video- tape sequence. Thinking-oriented measure: A multiple-choice measure whereby subjects choose which of four alternative thoughts a particular target experienced immediately prior to the completion of a video- tape sequence. Rationale for Hypothesis V: Hypothesis V is a logical extension of the similarity hypotheses related to employing the variables of Judgment: those designated as Thinkers should discriminate thinking in others better than those designated as Feelers, and Xi££.!§£§§: If this hypothesis is not supported, one of two assumptions may be true: (1) although individuals indicate that they use one process of attending more than the other, they do not focus on that process in others, or (2) an error source has interfered with assessing this effect. Hypothesis VI: Neither the order in which the target groups were pre- sented, nor the order in which the films within the target groups were presented, will result in significant increases in empathic ability as the research progresses. Rationale for Hypothesis VI: Increased empathic accuracy over sessions can be a confounding variable denoting practice effects or reduction of anxiety. Since empathy has been considered a relatively permanent, or slowly changeable trait, an increase in empathy might undercut this assumption. However, increased empathy can originate from two sources: 25 (1) learning to be more empathic in general, and (2) learning to be more empathic toward a particular target. If (1), then learning becomes a confounding influence on ability. The effect of unequal learning rates, pre-test abilities, etc., cannot be assessed, and the results must be considered unstable, capable of changing at the next practice. If (2), then the results may represent a natural, less distorted process of empathic understanding related to familiarity with the stimulus. Hypothesis VII: The two target individuals representing each Intui- ting—Sensing, Feeling-Thinking orientation will draw similar empathy scores from the subjects. Rationale for Hypothesis VII: Since two targets have been used to represent each extreme combination of Perceiving and Judging functions such that there are two Intuitive-Feelers, two Intuitive-Thinkers, two Senser-Feelers, and two Senser-Thinkers, the two individuals represent- ing each type should draw similar scores. If this occurs, we can assume that the desired target characteristics were salient factors in the empathic discrimination of the subjects. If this fails to occur, the conclusion that the targets actually presented discriminable data according to their label is questionable. This particularly effects Hypotheses III and IV, since similarity unrecognized in targets can hardly be assumed to influence empathic accuracy. Hypothesis VIII: A different pattern of mean responses will exist on the videotape measures of empathy than on the personality test measure. Rationale for Hypothg§is VIII: While both personality test and tests based on videotape stimuli have been used to assess empathy, it is reasonable to assume that they are not measuring precisely the same 26 qualities. Personality tests measure global abstractions in response to a stimulus presentation, while videotape tests measure the meaning of specific behaviors. The personality test assumes consistency, self- knowledge, and honesty on the part of the target, while specific behaviors on the videotape are more related to the situational context, may be straightforward or camoflaging, and may be ”out of character“ from time to time. In addition, certain empathy processes may be detected by one measure, hidden by others. Strategy behavior may be facilitated by per- sonality tests where diverse information must be related to hitherto unexplored questions. On the other hand, extrapolation may cause con— fusion and error in videotape tests where specific situational and behavioral cues are to be interpreted. V. MEASURES The measures used in this research may be divided into “personal- ity assessment measures" and ”empathy measures”. The personality assessment measures were the Extravert-Introvert, Sensing-Intuition, and Thinking-Feeling scales from the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator. The empathy measures were the Videotape Empathy Series and the Personality Empathy Test both constructed by the experimenter. Personality Assessment Measures The Myers-Briggs Type Indicator The Myers-Briggs Type Indicator is a 166 item forced-choice persi ality test which purports to measure personality type from a Jungian perspective. The MBTI measures four central dimensions: Extraversion» Introversion (E-I), Sensing-Intuition (S-N), Thinking—Feeling (T-F), a1 Judging-Perceiving (J-P). Each person is seen as functioning primaril, within the framework of one side of each dichotomy. While each of the dichotomies seemed relevant to the functioning of empathy, the Judging-Perceiving dimension was eliminated when searc of the literature (Stricker and Ross, 1964; and Sundberg and Mendelsoh in Buros, 1961) indicated that the J-P dimension seemed to be measurin preference for order and planning versus preference for spontaneity an novelty, rather than preference for making decisions versus preference for open-ended experiencing as the manual suggests. Correlations betw 27 28 the J-P and S-N dimensions have also been found to range from 0.26 to 0.47. In this context, these moderately strong correlations provide good reason to discount the use of both scales concurrently. Intercorrelation among the other MBTI scales were cited in the MBTI manual. They include eight studies of high school and college populations. Correlations between the E—I and S—N scales ranged from -.14 (N = 184) to 0.06 (N = 2511). Correlations between the E—I and T-F scales ranged from —.19 (N = 240) to 0.04 (N = 184). Correlations between the S-N and T-F scales ranged from 0.02 (N = 541) to 0.10 (N = 614). In sum, the scales appear to be largely independent of one another. a) Extraversion-Introversion: Myers (1962) describes E-I in the following way: The introvert's main interests are in the inner world of concepts and ideas, while the extravert's main interests are in the outer world of people and things. Therefore, when circumstances permit, the introvert directs both perception and judgment upon ideas, while the extravert likes to direct both upon his outside environ- ment.... Introverts are harder to understand than extraverts for two reasons. They are not merely less communicative; they are also a good deal more complicated. (P. 57) Although Myers and Briggs conceived of all four dimensions to be dichotomous, few investigators agree that statistical analyses of the regression curves of the dichotomies support this contention. More— over, Jung (1928) describes as less differentiated the “normal man, who is normal partly because nothing excessive is allowed." Following this thinking, and the substantial amount of research that has disconfirmed the dichotomous nature of the scales, this study will consider extra— version-introversion, sensing—intuition, and thinking-feeling each to be 29 continua. See Appendix B for confirmation of the non-dichotomous nature of the scores of the initial sample in this study. In this research, the middle range of the E-I scale was used as a control for (l) the effect of extreme extravert-introvert response styles grossly influencing behavior, and (2) the reduction of adjust- ment as a confounding variable. Validational data presented in the manual shows the E-I scale to correlate positively (p< .01 with Deference and Abasement and nega— tively with Exhibition, Affiliation and Dominance on the Edwards Person- ality Preference Schedule (EPPS).1 0n the Allport-Vernon-Lindzey Scale of Values (AVL), E-I correlated positively (p< .01) with Theoretical and Aesthetic scales, while correlating negatively with Economic and Political scales. 0n the Personality Research Inventory (PRI), E-I correlated positively (p< .01) with Free-floating Anxiety, Impulsivee ness, and Gregariousness. Introverts have less people-oriented values (Theoretical and Aesthetic), and seem to react to others with a flight rather than fight reaction (Abasement and Deference). While introverts seem to experience more anxiety, they also seem to be more independent. The extraverts, on the other hand, present themselves more to others (Exhibition, Affiliation), have more people-interactive values (Economics and Politics), and fight rather than flee when confronted (Dominance). These results seemingly correlate accurately with tradi- tional definitions of extraversion-introversion. 1Extraverts score high on the E-I scale. 30 Additional correlational data collected by Stricker and Ross (1964) found the E-I scale to correlate positively with the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory (MMPI) extraversion-introversion scale (Si, p_= 0.63) and the Gray—Wheelwright Psychological Type Questionnaire, an earlier questionnaire measuring Jung's psychological types (p.= 0.79). Stricker and Ross also cite previous research reporting highly signifi— cant correlations with the E-I scale on the Maudsley Personality Inven— tory (Eysenck, 1959 and Howarth, 1962), and an extraversionvintroversion factor identified by Ross (1963). Stricker and Ross conclude that the MBTI E-I scale measures extraversion-introversion as it is commonly defined: "interest and facility in social relations, frequently in- volving talkativeness“ (Stricker and Ross, 1964). Stricker and Ross also note the possibility that the E-I scale may be partially measuring adjustment. The E-I correlated (Ef1.01) with a neurotic scale (0, p_ .39) and three of four psychotic scales (Pt, p.= .30; Ma, y_= -.29; and Sc, p_= .23) on the MMPI. While it did corre- late consistently with relevant Maudsley scales, and correlated nega- tively with the CPI Self-Assurance scale, previous studies cited by Stricker and Ross have found the E-I scale to correlate consistently with measures of adjustment: the Maudsley Neuroticism scale (Howarth, 1962), the PRI Free-Floating Anxiety scale (Myers, 1962b), ratings of Needs Psychologist's Attention and Low Stamina (Ross, 1961). b) Sensingrlntuition: Myers (1962b) describes intuition and sens- ing in the following way: 31 When people prefer sensing, they find too much of interest in the actuality around them to spend much energy listening for ideas out of nowhere. When people prefer intuition, they are too much interested in all the possibilities that occur to them to give a whole lot of notice to the actualities. For instance, the reader who confines his attention strictly to what is said here on the page is following the habit of the people who prefer sens- ing. One who reads between the lines and runs ahead to the pos- sibilities which arise in his own mind is illustrating the way of the people who prefer intuition. (Pp. 51-52) Validational data presented in the manual shows the S—N scale to correlate positively (Ef:.01) with Autonomy and negatively with Defer- ence and Order on the EPPS.1 0n the AVL, S-N correlated positively (ps .01) with the Aesthetic and Theoretical, but negatively with the Economic and Political scales. On the PRI, the S-N scale correlated positively (p< .01) with Liking to Use Mind,* Artistic versus Practical, Tolerance of Complexity, Impulsiveness,* and Progressive versus Conserva- tive. The S-N scale correlated negatively with the Gregariousness, Masculine Vigor and Social Know-How scales on the same test. Inspection of these positive and negative lists shows the Intuitive end of the con— tinuum to be aesthetic, theoretical and autonomous, while the Senser end to be gregarious, ordered and economic. Although “hunch” oriented be— havior, usually closely associated with intuitive behavior was neither pin-pointed by the MBTI questions nor has it been expressly studied with relation to the S-N scale, behavior contingent on making hunches does seem to correlate positively, supporting the conceptual definitions stated by the manual. 1Intuiters score high on the S-N scale. * Denotes more than one study in which the scale has correlated significantly at the .01 level with the attribute indicated. 32 Data by Stricker and Ross (1964) found a significant positive correlation (pf:.01) between the Myers-Briggs S—N scale and a similar S-N scale on the Gray-Wheelwright Psychological Type Questionnaire. However, they also note that non-conformity seems positively correlated to the intuition end of the scale. In this case, the scale may have a heavy conformity-non—conformity loading, or since Stricker and Ross were using the N score on a non-continuous basis with the S, the N scale may be measuring non-conforming intuitives rather than conforming ones. The chief drawback of the S-N scale is its lack of breadth in covering the diverse aspects of intuition, such as valuing hunch be- havior versus valuing empiricism. It is, however, the only scale which measures preference for intuition and gives validational data to sup- port its claim to do so. c) Thinking-Feeling: Myers (1962b) describes the Thinking- Feeling dimension in the following way. There are two distinct and sharply contrasting ways of coming to conclusions. One way is by the use of thinking, which is a logical process, aimed at an impersonal finding. The other way is by the use of feeling, which is a process of appreciation, equally reasonable in its fashion, bestowing on things a personal, subjective value.... If, when one judges these ideas, he concentrates on whether or not they are true, that is thinking-judgment. If one is conscious first of like or dislike, of whether these concepts are sympathetic or antagonistic to the ideas he prizes, that is feeling-judgment. (P. 52) Validational data presented in the manual shows the T—F scale to corre- late positively (p< .01) with the Nurturance, Affiliation, Succorance, and Abasement scales, and negatively with the Endurance, Order, Autonomy, 1 Dominance, and Achievement scales on the EPPS. On the AVL, the T-F 1Feeling scores high on the T-F scale. 33 scale correlated positively (pf:.01) with the Social and Religious scales, and negatively with the Theoretical, Economic, and Political scales. On the PRI, the T-F scale correlated positively (BK .01) with Free-Floating Anxiety, Spiritual vs. Material, and Tolerance for Com- plexity scales, while it correlated negatively with Masculine Vigor, Attitude toward Work, and Self~Sufficiency. Combined, these results seem to indicate that the Feeler end of the continuum is related to the reconciliation of positive and negative feelings, and social and re— ligious life preferences. The Tolerance for Complexity correlation is mystifying. The Thinker end of the continuum can be described as vigorous, achievement oriented, and self—sufficient. With reference to the manual descriptions, the subjective and evaluative aspects seem largely fulfilled by the validational results. The impersonal, objec- tive orientation of the Thinker seems likewise substantiated. Again a highly significant correlation (p< .01) was found between the Myers- Briggs T-F scale and the Gray Wheelwright Psychological Type Question- naire by Stricker and Ross (1964). Empathy Measures 1. The Videotape Empathnyeries (V.E.S.) The Videotape Empathy Series consists of four multiple-choice sub- scales used to measure empathic accuracy of subjects toward group members viewed via videotape. The four measurements include: Thinking Empathy, Feeling Empathy, Behavior Empathy, and Social-interaction Empathy. Defined operationally, Thinking Empathy is the ability to accurately predict, given four alternative choices, what a target individual would 34 say he was thinking at a particular moment during a videotaped group session. Similarly, Feeling Empathy consists of the ability to accur- ately predict what a target individual would say he was feeling at a particular moment during a videotaped group session. Behavior Empathy is defined as the ability to predict which behaviors a target individual will perform immediately following an experimenter stopped segment. Social—interaction Empathy is the ability to choose which of three other group members a target individual will say he places closest or farthest from himself on selected sociometric continua, e.g., trust, liking, similarity to self. Table 1. Outline for constructing the Thinking and Feeling Empathy tests. 1. 1/2 hour group session videotaped. II. Awareness training. III. Recall session, group members interviewed separately. IV. Construction of test items by experimenter. V. Evaluation of face validity of items by group members. VI. Omission or rewording of test items by experimenter with reference to group members input. VII. Pilot study to determine level of difficulty for items and weight of alternative responses within items. VIII. Omission or substitution of items or alternatives according to level of difficulty and/0r unbalanced weight of alterna— tives. 35 a) The Thinking and Feeling Empathy Tests The procedure for constructing the Thinking and Feeling Empathy tests is outlined in Table 1, on the preceding page. Since essentially the Thinking and Feeling Empathy tests ask subjects to predict what group members would say they were experiencing during intermittent seg- ments of their group sessions, it is imperative that the group members relate as accurately as possible what they were experiencing as the group session progressed. In order to facilitate the recognition of subtle as well as obvious experiential data, a special awareness train- ing technique was instituted after the first group session and before the first recall session.1 The awareness training session consisted of arranging feelings presented in the form of a jigsaw puzzle (see Appendix 0) into dichotomies ”experience frequently“ and ”experience rarely” and later “feel comfortable experiencing” and “feel uncomfort- able experiencing”. From each of the dichotomies the group members were asked to select a feeling they would like to explore in greater depth. Using an adaptation of Gendlin's focusing technique (see Appendix 0), each group member was instructed to silently attend to the bodily sensations, emotional responses and mental meanderings accompanying the experience of the original feeling. For approximately 10 minutes, the individual was guided toward discovering the many gradations of experi- ences occurring concurrently. After a short rest, another feeling was 1The awareness training session was given following the first group session in order to reduce contamination or carry over to the videotaped group session. Awareness training was not instituted on re- call days in order to reduce the chance of bringing in extraneous feelings which might interfere with recall. 36 chosen and the procedure was repeated. A total of six feelings were selected for focusing. The awareness training session lasted one hour. A technique closely related to the Interpersonal Process Recall developed by Kagan and Krathwohl (1967) was used to elicit feeling and thinking responses during the replay of the videotaped session. Recall data was collected on the odd days following the group sessions (see Figure 1), each group member being interviewed individually for approximately 1 1/2 hours. During the recall session, the group member was asked to watch the videotape and say ”stop” whenever she recognized feelings she had had about herself or the other group members, thoughts she had had but did not express, bodily sensations she had experienced, or expectations she had had about herself or the situation.1 Statements which the group member made after she stopped the tape were written down by the interviewer, either by capturing the general tone and mean- ing of the answer or by an exact quotation if the response was short. In addition to the verbal explication by the group member, she was given a list of 70 feeling words grouped by similarity, e g., angry, frus- trated, disgusted (see Appendix E), and asked to check off all the rele- vant feelings associated with the interaction just interrupted. She was permitted to check the feelings before or after she commented on the interaction, depending on which was more functional. (She might have 1A remote control switch by which the group member could stop the tape was originally planned for use instead of saying ”stop”. The video- tape technician suggested, however, that stopping the tape for as long as a minute might have a detrimental effect on the quality of the tape, so the ”stop“ method was instituted instead. It is recognized by the experimenter that a remote control switch has advantages over the "stop" method especially with regard to making it easier to stop anxiety toned segments. 37 .mcowmmwm Fqumc Fascw>wvcv use mmcwpmms azocm Low o_:uw;om weak .P mczmwm LmnEmE asogm dz u a mamasme azogm Fm u o memnEmE azogm #2 u m mgmnsme asocm mm u < garage; mmmcmLmZm o copmmmm . . . . a cormmmm . . . . o comemm . . . . o comewm . . . . . a u m < . a o m < . a u m < . a u m < FFmomL a cowmmmm Fpmumg m cowmmmm Ppmumg N cowmmmm F_mumc P :owmmmm u commmmm azocm u cowmmmm azogm u sewmmwm azocm o :owmmmm asocm Ppmomg Ffimomc __mowc Fpmum; m :owmmmm m cowmmmm m cowmmwm m cowmmmm __muwg FFmomL __mumc Freumc < covmmmm < cowmmmm < commmmm < cowmmmm FPmomL Ffimumc __momg Prmumc m sea in see 0 see m sea e sag m sea N sag _ sac 38 wanted to state clear or strong feelings first, or check the list first, if feelings were less clear or hard to verbalize.) Finally, the inter- viewer sometimes stopped the videotape to elicit feeling and thinking responses which she had summised from cues in the interaction, such as abrupt shifts in theme, body posture, and voice level or tone. After the recall session, the experimenter organized the recall data into feeling and thinking responses from which 6 feeling and 6 thinking multiple choice questions were constructed. Each feeling item contained a verbalized and a clarifying statement about the feeling, e.g., "careful--not to say anything that would hurt ______fs feelings.” This is in accord with Chapman's (1966) observation that “adjectives (describing feelings), without clarifying phrases, have different mean- ings for different people.... The use of phrases might be more specific and therefore might give more consistency of interpretation than the adjectives alone“ (p. 56). Thinking items are also stated in sentences, e.g., “I hope they ask me what instrument I play." Three “incorrect“ alternatives were constructed by the experimenter by using either feel- ing words left unchecked by the group member or by thoughts not indi- cated as present during recall. To ascertain the accuracy of the multiple choice items, the video- tape was replayed for the group members after all the group sessions had been completed. The tape was stopped after each interaction for which a multiple choice item had been constructed, and the designated group member was given the item and asked to mark whether the alterna- tive was "true”, "false”, or "somewhat true". Alternatives which the experimenter had intended to be "incorrect", but which the group member 39 marked “true” or “somewhat true” were either omitted or reworded to make them false. In order to test the likelihood to choose each ”incorrect" alternative per item, a pilot study was conducted. Twenty subjects viewed each videotape in series and indicated which feeling or thinking response they thought the targeted individual would say she was experi- encing just before the videotape stopped. Results of an item analysis showed the average percent correct to be 34%, a little above chance prediction. This meager score was thought to result from two sources: (1) the difficulty of some items, and (2) the unusual drawing power of some ”incorrect" alternatives. Items diagnosed to be suffering from either of these two error sources were either omitted, reworded, or new alternatives were substituted. The instructions given to the subjects before taking each empathy test are similar to those used by both Chapman (1966) and Campbell (1967). They are: The following is a multiple choice test used in conjunction with a videotape. Prior to today a series of videotapes were made in which four women participated in group discussions. Sub— sequently each group member individually watched a replay of each videotape and recalled what she was thinking or feeling during certain segments of the tape. One correct and three false altern- atives were constructed for many of the segments recalled. In this part of the experiment, the first videotape of Group A will be replayed and stopped at the segments mentioned above. When the videotape stops you are to turn the page and read a ques— tion about one of the group members. You will be given ten seconds to think about what that individual was thinking or feel— ing just before the videotape stopped. While you are thinking, the alternatives to the questions which begin in the middle of the page, should be kept covered with the sheet of paper provided. After ten seconds, the experimenter will signal you to remove the paper and choose which of the four alternatives you think the indi- cated group member would say she was feeling or thinking just before the videotape stopped. After you have circled the answer 40 to the question, 00 NOT TURN THE PAGE. Wait until the videotape has been started and stopped again at a new segment to turn the page. IMMEDIATELY COVER THE BOTTOM HALF OF THE PAGE WITH THE PAPER PROVIDED WHEN YOU TURN THE PAGE. Remember, you are to choose which alternative the indicated group member WOULD SAY she was thinking or feeling just before the videotape is stopped. Before each videotaped segment was begun, the subject was told the name of the group member who was the target of the next question. This was done to insure that the subject was paying attention to the behavior of the targeted member and not, by chance, to another member. The names of the group members were posted near their image on the screen. The ten-second reflection period was designed to enable the sub- ject to get a clear picture of her own view of the situation before reading the four logical possibilities presented in the multiple choice alternatives. This was performed to avoid the possibility that the cleverness or persuasibility of the "wrong“ alternatives would be a crucial error factor in determining the subject's response. Results of similar tests. Although the procedure for construct- ing the Videotape Empathy Series (VES) is unique, two empathy scales constructed by Chapman (1966) and Campbell (1967) have many similari- ties to the VES. Both Chapman's and Campbell's Affective Sensitivity scales (AS I and AS II, respectively) were designed to measure the empathy of persons viewing certain videotaped counselor interviews. The subject was presented with short counseling segments and asked to choose which of three alternative responses most accurately stated what the counselee was feeling about either himself or about his counselor. 41 Correct and incorrect responses in Chapman's empathy test were determined by two criterion groups, judged high and low in empathic regard who were asked to view short counseling vignettes and "try to :feel as the client felt" when the tape was stopped. From a list of 57 feeling adjectives, each individual selected those feelings which he thought were most clearly revealed by the counselee in the vignette. The item selected most frequently by the high criterion group and not by the low criterion group, and vice versa, were selected as items for the AS I. Four to seven items were chosen for each vignette. The subjects, master's candidates and counseling educators at two universities, were ranked on an empathy continuum by their peers or superiors. The upper and lower thirds of the distribution were shown the counseling vignettes and asked to ”try to feel as the client last felt.“ For each vignette they were given the 4 to 7 feeling words described in the previous paragraph and asked to rate each on a con- tinuum for “I have this kind of feeling strongly,‘l to "I have this kind of feeling not at all.” No significant difference was found between the means of the two groups. Among the explanations for these results were that (l) the feeling adjectives were too imprecise and needed clarifica— tion, and (2) the rankings used to select the criterion groups and the subjects according to their empathy skills, by the admission of some of the rankers, was sketchy at best, faulty at worst. The present research has taken into account Chapman's first ob- servation by adding explanatory statements to the feeling adjectives for clarification. Since no outside judges are used, the second problem is not applicable. 42 Campbell's AS II also followed Chapman's suggestion and added explanatory statements to clarify feelings described. Campbell's method for ascertaining the true feelings presented in the videotaped segments was more complicated, however. He used three sources for identifying feelings: (1) judges who watched the videotapes and described the feelings they observed; (2) judges supplied with back- ground data regarding previous counseling interviews between the counselor and counselee; and (3) actual statements of feelings made by the counselee during an Interpersonal Process Recall of the counseling session. In addition three levels of sophistication of judges were used: practicing counselors and doctoral candidates in counseling; M.A. counseling and guidance candidates, and non-counseling and guid- ance individuals. The latter two groups were used to construct “distractors”, i e., “wrong" answers. Based on item analyses of total scores, peer ratings and staff ratings, Campbell found no significant differences between the three methods of item selection, although he found that some counselees verbalized more about their feelings than others, and therefore pro« duced more data from which to garner type III items. Concerning the AS II as a whole, low positive correlations occurred between scale scores and peer and staff ratings of counselor effectiveness (average correlation = .26), and between scale scores and peer and staff ratings of affective sensitivity (average correlation = .38). Although suggestions for improving the AS II were not offered by Campbell, at least two alterations seem necessary to this experi- menter. First, some method for verification that the target individual 43 was actually feeling the feelings attributed to him by the judges should be implemented. Second, a method for analyzing the quality of the "wrong” answers should be constructed. With relation to the use of outside judges, in the opinion of this experimenter outside judges often confound the results of the experiment. The subject must be empathic with an empathic person, rather than with the target person himself. Often outside judges are employed, however, because they are more willing to identify negative or anxiety producing feelings than are the target individuals. To overcome this predicament in the present research, it was decided that helping the group members recognize and accept their feelings would, if successful, alleviate most of the need for outside judges. Therefore the series of awareness exercises described previously were constructed to facilitate this learning. To insure that the “wrong“ answers on the multiple choice test were not the greatest variant in determing the subject's empathic ability, or lack of it, either because some of the "wrong” answers were “right” or because the level of difficulty of the items was extreme, a method for analyzing the multiple choice items was constructed. As described previously, each group member reviewed the videotape and the multiple choice items related to her, commenting on the right and wrong alternatives and rating each item. ng Videotaped Group Discussion. The videotaped discussions were designed to provide several samples of everyday behavior. While it was thought that examples of everyday conversations could be generated by allowing the group members simply to talk among themselves about 44 whatever seemed relevant at the time, in order to reduce anxiety which might arise from the special circumstance of being videotaped, two discussion topics were supplied to the group members at the beginning of each session. Specifically, the group members were told that their primary job was to get to know each other as well as they could. They could do this by talking about either of the two topics supplied at the beginning of each group session, or by discussing whatever they thought would facilitate their understanding of each other. The choice of topics supplied for the videotaped discussion are based on observations from two theories. The first is Eric Berne's, who observes that individuals communicate on certain levels of inter- action during their everyday lives. They begin at simple levels and move to more intricate ones. His levels: the ritual, pastime, game and intimate interaction, reflect how committed the individual is to the interaction and how safe he feels in the interaction. The second theoretical observation, Maslow's, states that people move through their lives in a hierarchical fashion, first solving basic needs then moving on to more complex interpersonal ones, and finally to self-enhancing ones. His levels: the physiological, safety, belonging- ness and love, esteem, and self-actualization, reflect the needs the individual has satisfied, what problems the individual is focussing on, and which needs are still vaguely defined because he has not entertained them yet. With these two observations in mind, an order of topical discussion was designed that would encounter the safest and least commit- ting discussions first and would move into deeper, more complex, and riskier subjects later. 45 The topics and order of discussion are: (1) What are the advant- ages and disadvantages of dorm life? (pastime); (2) How do you usually spend your time? (pastime--allows predominant hierarchy level to emerge); (3) As a child, how did you get what you wanted from your parents? (game—-safety, belongingness and love); (4) How do you react when you"re angry? (game, intimate-—safety); (5) If someone thought something about you which might hurt you, would you rather have him tell you about it, or keep it to himself? Why? (game, intimate-- safety, esteem); (6) What are some of the things you've noticed about how your group functions? (game, intimate--safety, belongingness and love, esteem); (7) Are you more like a Parent, an Adult or a Child? (game, intimate--esteem); (8) What would you have liked to have said in this group, but you didnt? (intimate--safety, belongingness and love, esteem). In fact, the group did discuss the two topics supplied each session, but often the discussion of these was minimal, lasting between 5 and 10 minutes. The actual group discussions, then, usually reflected topics generated by the group members. The topics characteristically remained at Berne's pastime level and Maslow's belongingness level, although when the supplied topics were discussed, the level of conversa- tion usually attained that previously signified in parentheses. Visual organization of the videotaped group. The visual format tZY'which the videotaped members are arranged on the screen is an lrnportant factor in the degree of empathic accuracy that is technically airtainable. Three methods of recording group sessions via remOte- (XJritrol cameras were considered; two being encorporated, one being dis- car~ded. Factors seeming most essential were: (1) placing four 46 individuals on the screen without appearing so distant as to miss subtle behavioral cues needed for empathic judgment, (2) obtaining a front view, as opposed to a side view, of all the target individuals, and (3) retaining a sense of the group, i.e., knowing who is talking to whom. The first and most natural method was simply to videotape all four group members sitting in a half-circle. This method satisfied condition 3, i.e., it allowed a sense of group functioning, but the image of the target individuals on the screen was too small to allow the easy interpretation of subtle, but important behavioral cues. Moreover, only profiles of the two outside members were usually attain- able. A second split-screen method solved the latter two concerns, but created the first, the absence of a feeling for group cohesiveness, as the two inside members no longer faced each other but the outside of the screen (see Figure 2). A final method, utilizing one camera to focus on one group member in a corner of the screen while shooting the total group as a backdrop (see Figure 3), was discarded because of the unpredictable nature as to who would finally be the targeted individual of a particu- lar series. (Empathy sequences were not determined until after recall sessions were complete.) Also, while this quarter-screen method allowed the total group to be seen, it reduced attentiveness to the other members of the group as they appeared smaller and more recessed. A compromise to maximize satisfaction of the factors initially mentioned was accomplished by taping the group as a whole for the first five minutes, followed by 10 minutes of split-screen close-up focussing, Figure 2. Eye direction of group members using split- screen cameras. O 00 OO Figure 3. Videotape screen configuration using one member as primary focus. five minutes of full group, 10 minutes of split-screen. Seating posi- tions were rotated each session to disallow for side view distortions being distributed unequally. b) The Behavior Empathy Test The Behavior Empathy Test (B.E.) was designed to measure how accurately a subject could predict which behaviors a target individual would engage in following the cessation of a videotaped segment. 48 It answered the question, “how well does an individual know what another individual is going to do?" B.E. was constructed by noting instances in each group session where target individuals accompanied their verbal messages with clearly describable behavior, e.g., asked others for their opinions, laughed at a remark, changed the subject. From these instances, an equal number of behavioral responses were chosen per target person, and "incorrect“ behavioral responses were constructed by the experimenter. The problem of the subjective validity of “incorrect" responses did not arise with this test, since the actual behavior of the subject could be objectively ascertained by watching the tape, i.e., it was obvious from watching the tape which behaviors the individual did or did not perform. The relative power of the “incorrect" alternatives was examined by the same procedure mentioned in the previous section. l'Incorrect" responses with exaggerated drawing power were omitted or reworded. c) The Social-interaction Test The Social-interaction Empathy test (SIE) was designed to measure how accurately a subject could predict which of the group members a targeted individual would feel closest to or farthest from on various sociometric continua: trust, liking, similarity to self. It was de- signed to determine how well an individual understood the social ties between group members. SIE was constructed from double-layered scales (see Figure 4) given to each group member following every videotape recall session. One level of the scale asked each group member to rate the remaining 49 Sue Gail Betty x x x \ V I I , \ I V \ \ \ LIKE \ \\ q_IKE MOST \\ \ \ LEAST \ \ \ \\ \\ \‘ \ \ \ PERSON I HAVE KNOWN PERSON I HAVE KNOWN AND LIKED MOST AND LIKED LEAST Figure 4. An example of a Social-interaction rating scale. group members on a continuum from ”most" to "least” according to a specific sociometric trait. Because this level was an intragroup comparison, both ends of the continuum were instructed to be used. To balance this forced extreme rating, a second level was employed which instructed the individual to draw a line from each person on the first level to a position on the second level which represented where that person belonged with respect to all the people he had known regarding that trait. The top rating was the only rating used in the research. The second level was designed to reduce anxiety concerning extreme judgments and to balance the top rating according to his own perspective. The SIE test was composed of multiple choice questions which re— flected the ratings of the sociometric scales, e.g., ”Who would Mary say she liked best?" Because the items were constructed directly from the scales, rewording was unnecessary. An item analysis was used to determine difficulty level, and items deemed too easy or too difficult were omitted. 50 2. The Personality Empathy Test The Personality Empathy Test (PET) is a variant of a widely used empathy test format in which subjects and target individuals both take a personality, interest, or attitude inventory. The subjects view a limited sample of the target person's behavior, e.g., interview, dis- cussion, photograph, tape-recording, handwriting, and retake all or part of the pre-test inventory as they think the target person would take it. The Personality Empathy Test is a 100 item test composed of questions from the Myers-Briggs E-I, S-N and T-F scales, and the Com- plexity scale of the Omnibus Personality Inventory. The subject is asked to answer each item the way he thinks specified videotaped group members would answer that item. The test was given after the final taped session of each videotaped group. Often multiple-choice empathy tests are constructed by merely ask- ing the subjects to fill out a complete personality test as a target person would. One major drawback of this method is that it includes items which the target person himself may answer with uncertainty. Thus the variability in the test-retest reliability is built into the new method. During the pre-experiment testing in this research, each individual is asked to mark an "X“ by every question which "importantly describes some aspect of her personality." She is instructed to mark between 30 and 40 of 150 responses. The PET was constructed from these designated items of each videotaped group member. Cronbach (1955) cites another drawback of this method of empathy testing. Some questions may have a sterotyped answer, such as "Some- times I just like to relax and listen to music," which reflects more 51 knowledge of the average person, or a stereotype like ”college student", than empathic understanding of the specific target person. To reduce the effect of these non—discriminatory items, only two—alternative items which are answered one way by more than 35% but less than 65% of a sample of college students were used. The PET is scored by summing the correct predictions made by each subject about each group member. A total of 25 points is possible concerning each group member, and a cumulative score of 100 is possible toward the total videotaped group. Thus, both person-specific and total empathy scores are available. IV. PROCEDURE Selection of Subjects apd Group MEEPEEE Eight group members (targets), 20 pilot study subjects, and 36 experimental subjects were selected from similar populations. The targets and pilot study subjects were selected from a group of 383 female students enrolled in introductory and personality classes at Michigan State University during the Spring term, 1972. Following com- pletion of videotaping and test construction, the experimental subjects were selected from another sample of 157 female students enrolled in various level psychology classes during the Summer term, 1972. Female students were asked to volunteer to take a general per- sonality interest test for which they received 2 ”experimental credits" which could be used as extra credit toward their final grade in the course. They were also told that they might be chosen to participate in a second experiment involving empathy toward videotaped group members, for which various amounts of experimental credit and money would be given depending upon their function in the experiment. Video— taped group members received $20 plus 4 extra credit points. Pilot study subjects received $10 plus 4 extra credit points. Experimental subjects received $8 plus 4 extra credit points. The difference in the amount of payment received reflects the proportional amount of hours worked. 52 53 Scores for potential subjects and targets were first screened on the control variable, Extraversion-Introversion. Scorers falling outside the middle fiftieth percentile, i.e., extreme scorers, were dropped from consideration. From the remaining pool, 2 targets were chosen to repre- sent each combination of the four extremes on the Sensing-Intuition and Thinking-Feeling scales, i.e., ST, SF, NT, NF. Extremes were defined as the top and bottom 25 percentiles on each scale (see Figure 5). z 25% : 121/2%: 25% : 121/,%: 25% Extreme Middle ' Extreme Figure 5. Selection procedure according to distribution of scores on experimental variables. Similarly, potential subjects were screened first on the Extra- version-Introversion scale with only the middle fiftieth percentile con- sidered further. Then, four representatives of each possible extreme and middle scoring combination were selected (see Figure 6). Pilot study subjects had been selected at random from the distribution of experi- mental variables. ' Thinking--T Feeling (F) Feeling (F) Thinking (I) Intuition NF NTr NT m) (4) 44f (4) Intuition- NF NT NT Sensing S SF S (151) (4) <4) (4) Sensing SF ST ST (S) (4) (4)F (4) Total = 36 subjects Figure 6. Classification of subjects by scores on independent variables. 54 Procedure for Subjects Subjects were divided into two groups. One group viewed one videotaped group series, Group A, first. The second group viewed the remaining videotaped group series, Group B, first. This was to assure that any improvement in empathic understanding that accrued to practice was equally distributed among the two groups videotaped. To determine whether such a difference did occur, an analysis of variance (Hypothesis VI) was performed on the data with respect to accuracy versus first or last viewing of the series. Between 8 to 10 subjects viewed a group series at a time. As instructions, the subjects were told that they were partici« pating in a study designed to measure empathic ability. Empathic abil— ity was defined as the ability to understand what another person was thinking or feeling the way the other person experienced it. For instance, if a person assumed a happy stance, to cover up sadness he really felt, it would be empathic to say he was sad. On the other hand, if he acted happy and did not experience sadness, even if the judge thought he was really sad underneath, it would be empathic to say he was happy. Empathy, therefore, is the ability to understand what another person is experiencing at a particular moment. The format was explained: the subjects would watch two groups of four people who were strangers to each other discuss various topics. One group would be seen each week. In order that the group members could be viewed more closely on the screen, two methods of presentation would be used: sometimes the group would be shown in a semi—circle on the screen just as they had been sitting originally, and sometimes by 55 using two cameras, one-half of the semi-circle would be shown on the top half of the screen, the other half on the bottom. To make this switch less confusing, the names of the subjects were pasted on the top of the screen in the order they sat in the semi-circle, and each name was pasted on the side of the screen next to where they were sitting when the screen was split (see Figure 7). L M I J A N P O R D A A Y A M N MARY LINDA PAM JOAN Figure 7. Arrangement of subjects and subject-names on screen. The subjects were told that at the beginning of each segment, they would be advised as to which particular group member would be the subject of the next test question. The subjects were instructed to pay close, but not total, attention to this group member, particularly with regard to what the individual was thinking, feeling, or about to do. As the videotaped session progressed, the subject was told that the experimenter would stop the tape, after which she was to turn the page of the test booklet and cover the bottom half of the page with the cover sheet provided, i.e., cover the alternative answers. The subject and the experimenter would then read the question together (the 56 experimenter reading aloud so all subjects would be sure of the ques- tion), and the subject would be given 10 seconds to think about her own answer to the question. Following this 10 second period, the subject would be instructed to remove the cover sheet, look down to the bottom of the page, and answer which of the four alternatives she thought was correct. The importance of the 10 second interval to clearly answer the question in the subject's own mind was stressed. It was explained that all the alternatives could be logical answers for the question, and that sometimes one alternative might fool them if they did not have their own answer firmly in mind. The subjects were admonished to remain silent throughout the session, so as not to influence others, and not to be rushed when answering. It was explained that the research would be completed in three weeks or less and they could compare notes with others, or ask questions in a follow-up session concerning their scores, the content of the sessions, or the research design. Stimuli for measuring feeling, thinking and behaving empathy were presented by means of videotape stops. The social interaction items, concerning the total session, were asked at the end of each session. The Personality Empathy Test was given at the end of the last session for each group (see Figure 8). 57 Session 1 Session 2 Session 3 Session 4 Film 1: Film 2: Film 3: Film 4: Feeling, Feeling, Feeling, Feeling, Thinking, Thinking, Thinking, Thinking, Behavior Behavior Behavior Behavior Empathy Empathy Empathy Empathy Social Social Social Social Interaction Interaction Interaction Interaction Empathy Empathy Empathy Empathy Personality Empathy Figure 8. Order of presentation of empathy tests per group. Statistical Procedures Five repeated measures analyses of variance were performed to determine whether the differences predicted in the hypotheses were sup— ported or rejected by the null hypothesis. To minimize Type I errors resulting from chance rejection, a total confidence level of .10 was used. This seemingly high confidence leVel was divided among the five analyses, however, such that .025 levels were acceptable for the two primary designs and .0167 levels were acceptable for the three minor designs. It was felt that the number and complexity of the variables, in addition to the exploratory nature of the minor designs, warranted a more lenient .10 confidence level. Note that the two designs measuring the main effects have a combined confidence level of .05 as is standard in most research. 58 Design 1 explored the differences between subject variables with respect to videotape empathy measures, while Design 2 determined dif- ferences between subject variables on the personality empathy measure. Design 3 determined the effects of order of presentation on level of empathic accuracy. Design 4 analyzed the effects of utilizing two target individuals per target category on similarity of response toward the two category members. Design 5 compared subject response on videotape versus personality empathy test measures. VII. RESULTS Five analyses of variance using a repeated measures design were employed to test the hypotheses. Two analyses, considered to be the major analyses, were used to test the main effects and differential effects. Three analyses, considered to be minor analyses, were used to test measurement effects. Because of the complex and exploratory nature of some of the hypotheses, a total confidence level of .10 was used over the five analyses. The major two analyses utilized a combined confidence level of .05 to attain significance, or .025 per design. The three minor analyses also utilized a combined confidence level of .05 to attain significance, requiring a .0167 level per design. The procedure for the analysis of results is to consider the two major designs, or main and differential effects, first, then to consider the measurement effects. Overall interpretation of the major results tables will proceed before more detailed inspection of the specific analyses related to each hypothesis will be made. Because some analyses are relevant to more than one hypothesis, a left hand column in each major results table indicates the analyses relevant to each hypothesis. It should be noted that careful attention has been paid to the possibility that one significant analysis, being related to more than one hypothesis, could result in overestimating the strength of that analysis. The chance for this Type I error has been 59 60 decreased by (1) anticipation of its occurrence, (2) careful inspec- tion of the direction of the means differences with respect to each hypothesis, and (3) use of a statistic (wz) which estimates the percent of the total variance attributable to a particular analysis. Thus, only analyses contributing to a large percent of the variance will be considered worthy of supporting more than one hypothesis. Main Effects The main effects are concerned with the total empathy displayed by subjects regarding two personality orientations: Perceiving, measured on a continuum from Intuition to Sensing, and Judging, measured on a continuum from Feeling to Thinking. Hypothesis I states that among all the Perceivers, those with an Intuitive orientation will be most empathic. Hypothesis II states that among all the Judgers, those with a Feeling orientation will be most empathic. Tables 2 and 3 assess these effects, Table 2 concerning empathy via a videotape source, Table 3 concerning empathy measured via a written personality test. While not specifically related to Hypothesis I and II, it may be worthwhile to note the outstanding features of Tables 2 and 3 in order to clarify their overall meaning. In Table 2, 6 of 31 analyses, or 19%, are significant at the prescribed .025 level. Chance predicts that 1% of the analyses should be significant at this level, so it can be assumed that some significant differences are evident beyond chance. Table 2 suggests a strong measurement (M) component as the factor creat- ing most of the significant variance, i.e., all significant analyses contain an “M” variable. This factor might be interpreted as an 61 Table 2. Summary of Analysis of Variance for Videotape Empathy Series Hypotheses Source df MS F p w2 I Perceivers (P) 2 .000 <1 n.s II Judgers (J) 2 .007 <1 n.s I, II P x J 4 .007 <1 n.s Subjects (S) : P x J 27 .026 Measures (M) 3 .316 33.79 .0001 .10 I P x M 6 .006 <1 n.s. II J x M 6 .025 2.67 .021 .01 I, II P x J x M 12 .009 <1 n.s. S x M : P x J 81 .009 Perceiver Targets (X) 1 .034 1.75 n.s I, III P x X 2 .018 <1 n.s II, IV J x 2 .039 1.98 n.s I,II,III,IV P x J x X 4 .007 <1 n.s S x X : P x J 27 .009 M x X 3 .139 11.09 .0001 .04 I, III P x M x X 6 .010 <1 n.s. II, IV J x M x X 6 .035 2.76 .017 .01 I,II,III,IV P x J x M x X 12 .006 <1 n.s. S x M x X : P x J 81 .013 Judger Targets (Y) 1 .000 <1 n.s I, III P x Y 2 .003 <1 n.s II, IV J x Y 2 .003 <1 n.s I,II,III,IV P x J x Y 4 .011 <1 n.s S x Y P x J 27 .015 M x Y 3 .126 10.99 . 001 .04 I, III P x M x Y 6 .007 <1 n.s. II, IV J x M x Y 6 .013 1.17 n.s. I,II,III,IV P x J x M x Y 12 .009 <1 n.s. S x M x Y P x J 81 .011 X x Y 1 .006 <1 n.s I, III P x X x Y 2 .001 <1 n.s II, IV J x X x Y 2 .001 <1 n.s I,II,III,IV P x J x X x Y 4 .015 <1 n.s S x X x Y P x J 27 .015 M x X x Y 3 .062 4.44 .006 .02 I, III P x M x X x Y 6 .004 <1 n.s. 11, IV J x M x X x Y 6 .017 1.26 n.s. I,II,III,IV P x J x M x X x Y 12 .014 1.04 n.s. S x M x X x Y ' P x J 81 .014 Total 575 .016 62 Table 3. Summary of Analysis of Variance for Personality Empathy Test Hypotheses Source df MS F p w2 I Perceivers (P) 2 .006 <1 n.s II Judgers (J) 2 .056 1.27 n.s I, II P x J 4 .044 <1 n.s Subjects (S) : P x J 27 .044 Measures 3 .293 12.41 . 001 .05 I P X M 6 .025 1.05 n.s. II J x M 6 .026 1.09 n.s. I, II P x J x M 12 .030 1.28 n.s. S x M : P x J 81 .024 Perceiver Targets (X) l .804 24.35 . 001 .05 I, III P X X 2 .032 <1 n.s. II, IV J X 2 .053 1.61 n.s. I,II,III,IV P x J X X 4 .018 <1 n.s. S x X ' P x J 27 .033 M x X 3 .082 3.64 .016 .01 I, III P X M x X 6 .019 <1 n.s. II, IV J X M x X 6 .041 1.80 n.s. I,II,III,IV P x J x M x X 12 .020 <1 n.s. S x M x X : P x J 81 .023 Judger Targets (Y) 1 .090 6.09 .020 .01 I, III P X Y 2 .013 vwgzmmmz HcpmJEJ 0p Coprme cpwz A> x x x z x av cowpumcmch mpmeaH LomuzalmpmmLmH Lm>mmongummcsmmmzimcmmvzn EOLJ «com: .o_ mHan 70 Differential Effects Three hypotheses assess the differential effects. Hypothesis III states that similarity of Perceiver orientation between subjects and targets will facilitate empathy. Similarly, Hypothesis IV states that similarity of Judger orientation between subjects and targets will facilitate empathy. Hypothesis V states that among Judgers, subjects with Feeling orientations will score higher on Feeling measures of empathy, while subjects with Thinking orientations will score higher on Thinking measures of empathy. Tables 11 and 12 collapse relevant data concerning Hypothesis III from Tables 2 and 3. And Tables 13 and 14 collapse data concerning Hypothesis IV from Tables 2 and 3. Table 15 relates to Hypothesis V. Table 11. Analyses of Variance from Table 2 with Special Relevance to Hypothesis III Source df MS F p P x X 2 .018 <1 n.s. P x J x X 4 .007 <1 n.s. P x M x X 6 .010 <1 n.s. P x J x M x X 12 .006 <1 n.s. P x Y 2 .003 <1 n.s. P x J x Y 4 .011 <1 n.s. P x M x Y 6 .007 <1 n.s. P x J x M x Y 12 .009 <1 n.s. P x X x Y 2 .001 <1 n.s. P x J x X x Y 4 .015 <1 n.s. P x M x X x Y 6 .004 <1 n.s. P x J x M x X x Y 12 .014 1.04 n.s. 71 Table 12. Analyses of Variance from Table 3 with Special Relevance to Hypothesis III Source df MS F p P x X 2 .032 <1 n.s. P x J x X 4 .018 <1 n.s. P x M x X 6 .019 <1 n.s. P x J x M x X 12 .020 <1 n.s. P x X x Y 2 .023 <1 n.s. P x J x X x Y 4 .007 <1 n.s. P X M x X x Y 6 .020 1.26 n.s. P x J x M x X x Y 12 .025 1.57 n.s. Inspection of Tables 11 and 12 shows no significant differences on any analyses related to Hypothesis III. Thus, subjects did not have greater empathic accuracy toward targets with similar perceiver orienta- tions. Table 13. Analyses of Variance from Table 2 with Special Relevance to Hypothesis IV Source df MS F p J x Y 2 .003 <1 n.s P x J x Y 4 .011 <1 n.s J x M x Y 6 .013 1.17 n.s P x J x M x Y 12 .009 <1 n.s J x X x Y 2 .001 <1 n.s P x J x X x Y 4 .015 <1 n.s J x M x X x Y 6 .018 1.26 n.s P x J x M x X x Y 12 .014 1.04 n.s 72 Table 14. Analyses of Variance from Table 3 with Special Relevance to Hypothesis IV Source df MS F .p . w2 J x Y 2 .049 3.36 .05, n.s P x J x Y 4 .024 1.61 n.s. J x M x Y 6 .008 <1 n.s. P x J x M x Y 12 .006 __. _ooo. km.k Nw_. em a x a ” sz maasmma: H> nu 0N0. eoN g x a u m x a .m.: .oeo. mN.~ mmo. o a n w x a H> _o. FPO. mm.N mac. 0 Adv E_wa H> w_o. em a u a x m .m.: _ _oo. F a x a H> .m.c .m_o. mF.o e_F. _ Aav cowpapcamaaa H> eeo. em w ” Amv mpuaflnsm .m.: _ moo. _ Amy mazoaw H> N3 a m m: we mogzom mummgpoamz mpuwmmm cowpmpcwmwgm we Lmvgo Low mwmz—m:< we zgmaazm .o_ wpnmh 76 Hypothesis VI predicts that empathic accuracy will not increase over testing sessions. As explained in the rationale for the hypothe— ses, consistent differences could arise from at least two primary sources: (1) learning related to experimental practice and (2) perform- ance increments related to increased information and/or strategy testing. If the first were true, accuracy would increase constantly according to presentation (P) and film-order (F). If the second source held, accuracy would increase from Film 1 to 4, then decrease as the new Film 1 is introduced, repeating the pattern of increase from the first film series. Thus F, but not P, would be significant. These trends may be detected from Tables 17 and 18 which show the means of the significant analyses from Table 16. From Table 17 it can be observed that neither trend is clear. Presentation I shows instability over testing, while Presenataion II shows an increase in accuracy. It is unclear whether the inconsistency in Presentation I is related more to lack of accommodation to the experi- mental procedure, to failure in trial and error learning or developing adequate strategies, or to the fact that empathy is primarily situation— al and not substantially increased over short intervals. Due to lack of consistency over Presentation 1, it is difficult to assess whether a meaningful decrement occurred between Film 4 of Presentation I and Film 1 of Presentation II. Therefore, learning related to increased ability or to increased information is impossible to assess from these data. Table 18 provides more information with respect to the measures influence. 0n the Feeling Empathy measure, no increase in accuracy was shown between Presentation I and II, and a slightly decreasing trend was 77 Table 17. Means from Films (F) Analysis of Variance Reported in Table 16 Presentation I Presentation II F1 F2 F3 F4 F1 F2 F3 F4 Subjects .37 .39 .34 .37 .36 .36 .42 .41 Table 18. Means from the Presentation x Films x Measures (M : F x P) Interaction Reported in Table 16 Presentation I Presentation II F.E. T.E. B.E. S.E. F.E° T.E. B.E. S.E. Film 1 .45 .42 - .25 .37 .37 .43 .25 .41 Film 2 .40 .32 .38 .45 .40 .34 .31 .40 Film 3 .34 .29 .29 .45 .34 .45 .36 .51 Film 4 .43 .40 .31 .33 .36 .41 .32 .53 found within each presentation. Thus, it seems unlikely that either increased empathic skill or adequate informational predictors were gained with respect to feeling empathy. It should be remembered that much feeling recognition is situation specific, although, of course, some dispositional behavior does exist. With respect to Thinking Empathy, some increase does exist in Presentation 11 over Presentation 1, although dips in accuracy occur in the middle of both presentations. Thus, some empathic gains due to learning or practice, although not 78 consistent, are evident. Behavior Empathy shows a pattern of initial chance responding with slight increments as the film progresses. Empathic carry over seems to be negligible, probably due to the complex nature of open-ended predictions of behavior and because of the great emphasis on situation versus disposition in answering accurately. Thus, learning Behavior Empathy seems minimal. Finally, empathy related to understanding social interactions increases from presenta- tion to presentation and within presentations to a degree. It is un- clear whether the large decrement in Film 4 of Presentation I should be attributed to faulty strategy changing, or whether the apparent gains evident on Films 3 and 4 are illusory. Overall, then, some measurement-specific effects can be noted across films and presentations. The near significant differences on the presentation dimension (P) coupled with the significant differences in the films within presentations, and measures within films and pre— sentations leads to a possible rejection of Hypothesis VI. However, while films on Presentation II showed fairly consistent increases on Thinking Empathy and Social Interaction Empathy, scores hardly improved on Feeling Empathy and Behavior Empathy. In addition when looking from Film 1 to Film 4, no constant increases are observed, although Social Interaction Empathy in Presentation 11 is almost consistently gaining. With regard to accepting or rejecting Hypothesis VI, then, a condi- tional rejection seems most reasonable. While empathy does not increase over sessions on Feeling and Behavior Empathy, inconsistent empathic increases are observed on Thinking and Social Interaction Empathy. Thus, on two measures either learning or measures-adaptation increases 79 are likely and measurement of a natural responding pattern has not been achieved. Hypothesis VII tests the effects of utilizing two target repre- sentatives for each target dimension. It is hypothesized that no sig- nificant differences will occur between the two representatives (A1 and A2). Table 19 gives a summary of the analysis of variance results relating to this hypothesis. As has been true, measurement variance is again prominent (M, M x X, M x Y, M x X x Y). With regard to the critical A variable, however, a complicated picture is presented. Five of eight analyses relevant to Hypothesis VII are significant at the required .0167 level. However, cumulatively these significant analyses are estimated to account for only 5% of the variance in Table 18. The interaction most sensitive to the complicated effects of the A di- mension is the M x X x Y x A interaction, and the means of this inter- action are listed and graphed in Table 20 and Figure 9 respectively. While similar patterns of mean responding toward the targets can be seen for each measure, gross differences between the means can be seen toward NT targets on Feeling Empathy, ST targets on Behavior Empathy, and SF targets on Social-interaction Empathy. Thus, gross dif- ferences toward alternative targets seem evenly distributed. Further observations of Figure 9 would seem to indicate that while mean differ- ences between A's are significant at the required level, similar pat- terns seem to exist to responding to A's, e.g., on T.E., SF targets draw most empathic accuracy, followed by NF, NT and ST targets in that order. While not totally consistent, these patterns do seem substantial. In addition it may be noted that in 2 target pairs (NF and ST), one 80 Table 19. Summary of Analysis of Variance for Congruence of Individual Target Representatives on Videotape Empathy Hypotheses Source MS df p w2 Subjects (S) .040 35 Measures (M) .682 3 41.15 .0001 .06 S x M .017 105 Perceiver Targets (X) .055 l 1.60 n.s. S x X .035 35 M x X .274 3 10.30 .0001 .02 S x M x X .027 105 Judger Targets (Y) .007 1 <1 n.s S x Y .024 35 M x Y .251 3 11.19 .0001 .02 S x M x Y .022 105 X x Y .019 1 <1 n.s S x X x Y .023 35 M x X x Y .133 3 5.04 .003 .01 S x M x X x Y .026 105 VII Alter Targets (A) .168 1 10.20 .003 .01 S x A .016 35 VII M x A .149 3 5.44 .002 .01 S x M x A .027 105 VII X x A .334 1 10.37 .003 .01 S x X x A .032 35 VII M x X x A .013 3 <1 n.s. S x M x X A .030 105 VII Y x A .079 1 3.35 .076, n.s. S x Y x A .024 35 VII M x Y x A .015 3 <1 n.s S x M x Y A .022 105 VII X x Y x A .293 l 10.81 .002 .01 S x X x Y A .027 35 VII M x X x Y A .090 3 3.64 .015 .01 S x M x X Y x A .025 105 Total .030 1151 81 Table 20. Means from the Measures x Perceiver Targets x Judger Targets x Alter Targets Interaction (M x X x Y x A) reported in Table 18 NF NT SF ST Feeling Empathy .37 .40 .48 .30 .36 .32 .42 .46 Thinking Empathy .36 .41 .36 .37 .39 .45 .32 .39 Behavior Empathy .28 .31 .42 .36 .29 .29 .23 .33 Social-inter- .39 .44 .35 .35 .44 .58 .41 .49 action Empathy representative consistently scored higher than the other. This may indicate greater transparency of feelings and thoughts by some target representatives than others. With regard to the rationale behind assessing Hypothesis VII, then, that differences in A would not create enough variance to confound the determination of main effects, it seems best to conditionally reject Hypothesis VII on the basis of five out of eight relevant analyses attaining significance. However, because tar- get pairs did often seem to attract similar accuracy patterns, consider- able reservation must be attached to the rejection. Hypothesis VIII predicts that differences will occur between videotape and personality test empathy. It is evident from Table 21 that 5 of 31, or 16%, of the analyses are significant at the prescribed .0167 level. Again, this exceeds the 1% chance of significance ex- pected when this many analyses are conducted requiring this level of significance. It will be noted that only analyses devoid of the 60 58 56 54 52 50 48 46 44 42 4o 38 36 34 32 3o 28 26 24 22 82 \ l \ I \“ . \‘II NF NT SF ST NF NT SF ST NF NT SF ST NF NT SF ST Feeling E. Thinking E Behavior E. Social-int. E. Key: —»——~——-= Target Group A - — — - = Target Group B Figure 9. Distribution of Means from Measures X Perceiver Targets x Judger Targets x Alter Targets Interaction (M x X x Y x A) reported in Table 19. 83 Table 21. Summary of Analysis of Variance for Personality versus Video- tape Empathy Measures Hypotheses Source df MS F p w Perceivers (P) 2 .003 <1 n.s. Judgers (J) 2 .007 <1 n.s. P x J 4 .009 1.11 n.s. Subjects (S) : P x J 27 .008 VIII Form of Test (F) 1 3.752 382.61 .0001 .65 VIII P x F 2 .000 <1 n.s. VIII J x F 2 .007 <1 n.s. VIII P x J x F 4 .008 <1 n.s. S x F : P x J 27 .010 Perceiver Targets (X) l .022 2.99 n.s. P x X 2 .009 1.19 n.s. J x X 2 .007 <1 n.s. P x J x X 4 .007 <1 n.s. S x X P x J 27 .007 VIII F x X 1 .124 14.08 .001 .02 VIII P x F x X 2 .012 1.34 n.s. VIII J x F x X 2 .017 1.90 n.s. VIII P x J x F x X 4 .001 -EXTRAVERT Table 24. Distribution of Sample Population on Sensing-Intuition Dimension SCORES _.a 3.0 .—I KO 0 =3 [\3 O—‘Nw-bU'IOWNm 3 6 912 1518 2124 2730 3336 3942 4548 5154 57 SENSING —€>'INTUITION 128 Table 25. Distribution of Sample Population on Thinking-Feeling Dimension ._I_J._a ROD—4N SCORES O—‘NMJ>U‘IO\\IOO 3 6 912 1518 21 24 27303336 394245485154 57 FEELING —>- THINKING APPENDIX C THE EMPATHY MEASURES 129 130 The following is a multiple choice test used in conjunction with a videotape. Prior to today a series of videotapes were made in which four women participated in group discussions. Subsequently each group member individually watched a replay of each videotape and recalled what she was thinking or feeling during certain segments of the tape. One correct and three false alternatives were constructed for many of the segments recalled. In this part of the experiment, the first videotape of Group A will be replayed and stopped at the segments mentioned above. When the videotape stops you are to turn the page and read a question about one of the group members. You will be given ten seconds to think about what that individual was thinking or feeling just before the videotape stopped. While you are thinking, the alternatives to the questions which begin in the middle of the page, should be kept covered with the sheet of paper provided. After ten seconds, the experimenter will signal you to remove the paper and choose which of the four alternatives you think the indicated group member would say she was feeling or thinking just before the videotape stopped. After you have circled the answer to the ques- tion, DO NOT TURN THE PAGE. Wait until the videotape has been started and stopped again at a new segment to turn the page. IMMEDIATELY COVER THE BOTTOM HALF OF THE PAGE WITH THE PAPER PROVIDED WHEN YOU TURN THE PAGE. Remember, you are to choose which alternative the indicated group member WOULD SAY she was thinking or feeling just before the videotape is stopped. 21. 21. 131 GROUP A, SESSION 1 . How did Mary feel as she said she had to be older than her sisters did to get the same privileges? . How did Linda feel when she said most of her friends were going to U of M or State or out of state? What was Pam thinking as Linda described lying to her mother about the time? . How did Mary feel as she said she had to be older than her sisters did to get the same privileges? A. Embarrassed—-the others might think she was not as responsible as her sisters. B. Protected-~her parents seemed to watch over her more, even though they did not give her privileges early. C. Guilty-—for putting her family in a bad light. D. Resentful——she always seemed to get the raw end of the deal. . How did Linda feel when she said that most of her friends were going to U of M or State or out of state? A. Lonely--most of her friends seemed to be elsewhere. B. Satisfied-—to go to State. She did not have to apply anywhere else. C. Regretful--that she did not apply to U of M or out of state. D. Cynical——because her friends used going out of state to gain status. What was Pam thinking as Linda described lying to her mother about the time? . A. I agree, you have to lie to your parents sometimes. B. Lying is stupid, there is no sense to it! C. Her mother must be pretty dumb to believe it. D. I remember the time I lied to my parents and got caught. 22. 22. 132 . What was Pam thinking as Linda talked about Nassau? . What was Linda thinking as she said she paid half her college expenses? What will Mary do next? . What was Pam thinking as Linda talked about Nassau? A. I have never been out of Michigan! B. C. B. C. D Linda is trying to impress us. I would like to go to Nassau too! D. I do not want to go to Nassau. . What was Linda thinking as she said she paid half her college expenses? A. I did not want to pay anything, but my parents made me pay half. I do not want to give the wrong impression, it was my idea to pay half. I do not like situations where you have to prove you are poor. . It is a lie, but maybe they will like me better. What will Mary do next? A. B. CG D. Say, "Yeah, lots of times I didn‘t know.“ Say, “I really knew though.“ Say, “They should be happy I came home.“ Say, “Sometimes I‘d try to make a joke out of it.” 23. 23. 133 . What was Pam feeling when Linda said she got away with more than her sister ever tried to? . How did Pam feel after she told about her roommate asking about her grades? What was Pam thinking as Mary described her dorm? . What was Pam feeling when Linda said she got away with more than her sister ever tried to? A. Amused——she usually got away with more too. B. Disgusted——she saw Linda as proud and headstrong. C. Regretful——that she had teased her older brother about how much she got. D. Impatient—-she wanted to get on to another topic. . How did Pam feel after she told about her roommate asking about her grades? A. Close-—the others seemed to agree that asking what your grades are is wrong. B. Amused——at how scared she had been to tell anyone. C. Depressed—-it seemed like her grades would never get better. D. Guilty——she always seemed to put her roommate down. What was Pam thinking as Mary described her dorm? A. She must be very unhappy up here! B. She seems like she is acting—«her expressions are not real. C. She is making me look bad for liking the dorm. D. I never looked at it that way. But that is what it is like. 24. 24. 134 . How did Mary feel as Pam told how her brother usually drives her places? . How was Ceci feeling after Pam told about her roommate asking about her grades. What was Ceci thinking as Mary described her dorm? . How was Mary feeling as Pam told how her brother usually drives her places? A. Amused--she thought it was funny. B. Impatient--she wanted to go on to deeper things. C. Anxious—-she was afraid she would have to come up with a story too. D. Surprised--that Pam let her brother get away with that. . How was Ceci feeling after Pam told about her roommate asking about her grades? A. B. C. D. Capable——her grades were good, she did not mind telling. Ashamed-~her grades were not too good either. Surprised-—she wondered what was so wrong with asking about grades. Angry--what a dumb thing for the roommate to ask! What was Ceci thinking as Mary described her dorm? A. B. C. D. Why doesn‘t she move out? I like the dorm--but I had to move out because it was too expen- sive. That is exactly what it is like-~very clinical. 1 am glad Mary got more into the discussion. 25. 25. 135 . What will Ceci do next? . What was Mary thinking as Linda said that classes were hard up here? What was Ceci thinking as Pam described finding friends that are like you? . What will Ceci do next? A. Tell what Linda's brother might have been thinking when he would' not let her drive. B. Say that she would not let anyone else drive her car either. C. Acknowledge that similar things had happened to her. D. Ask what reasons Linda‘s brother gave. . What was Mary thinking as Linda said that classes were hard up here? A. I disagree-—classes are easy for me. B. Yep--I have to work hard here for the average grades I get! C. I wonder if I am the dumbest one here? D. Probably I went to a better high school than she did. What was Ceci thinking as Pam described finding friends that are like you? A. It is not so hard for me to find friends. B. Pam likes a lot of friends; I like only a few. C. I agree, most of my friends are pretty much like me. D. I disagree, opposites attract, not those who are just like you. 26. 26. 136 . What was Linda thinking as she told about getting the keys to her boyfriend's car? . What will Pam do next? How did Linda feel as Mary talked about moving winter term? . What was Linda thinking as she told about getting the keys to her boyfriend's car? A. I am used to things like that. I guess they are not. B. They think I am spoiled. C. I love telling others this. They really think I am good. D. I wonder what they think of my boyfriend? . What will Pam do next? A. Say that her parents never helped her with school work before, so they do not supervise now. B. Say that she paid for all of first term, so her parents could not get down on her for her grades. C. Ask how the other‘s parents react to grades. D. Tell about her roommate's parents reaction. How did Linda feel as Mary talked about moving winter term? A. Annoyed--she could not understand why Mary would have moved out in the first place. B. Sorry for Maryembecause she must have been very lonely. C. Surprised--that Mary could not make friends easier. D. Helpful--she usually made it easier for new people to fit in. 27. 27. 137 . What was Ceci thinking as Linda told about getting the keys to her boyfriend's car? . How did Linda feel about Mary's parents rejoicing when her grades were good? How did Pam feel as she commented on the room looking antique. . What was Ceci thinking as Linda told about getting the keys to her boyfriend's car? A. I would like to have a car to borrow! B. I would not want the keys to my boyfriend‘s car. C. I could not even afford the gas. D. I wonder where she found a boyfriend like that. . How did Linda feel about Mary's parents rejoicing when her grades were good? A. Skeptical--that Mary's parents really did that to control her. B. Guilty--her grades are never good enough to be rejoiced about. C. Disappointed--her parents never praise her for her grades. D. Bored-—everybody always talks about grades. How did Pam feel as she commented on the room looking antique? A. 0ptimistic--she liked to talk about antiques and old places. B. Impatient--she wanted to get this group over with. C. Disappointed--she wanted the group to be more interesting than it turned out to be. . D. Comfortable--she could tell she was getting to know and like the others when she started talking about the surroundings. 28. 28. 138 . What will Linda do next? . What was Mary thinking as Ceci told about not drinking water with meals? How did Mary feel after saying that everybody dried out this winter? . What will Linda do next? A. Talk about the responsibility she feels driving her boyfriendfs car. B. Say that his insurance will cover her accidents. C. Tell an incident about almost getting in a crash. D. Change the subject. . What was Mary thinking as Ceci told about not drinking water with meals? A. Ceci's father must be a health nut. B. She is making me thirsty. C. I should pay more attention to my weight. D. That is interesting. I would like to hear more about it. How did Mary feel after saying that everybody dried out this winter? A. Comfortable-—she felt like she was more a part of the group now. B. Awkward-—she felt like she was talking to fill the silences. C. Guilty-—she thought she was not saying enough Of substance. D. Annoyed——she thought others in the group should have said more. 139 9. How did Ceci feel after she mentioned hitch-hiking? 19. How did Ceci feel when Linda said, ”That's great!”? 9. How did Ceci feel after she mentioned hitch-hiking? A. B. C. D. Foolish--she thought the others considered hitch-hiking dangerous. Guilty--for putting Linda down. Superior--she could do things for herself. Discouraged--her idea had not seemed to go over well. 19. How did Ceci feel when Linda said, ”That's great!”? A. B. C. Uneasy-—it really was not that great. Anybody could do it. Bitter——she really would like to live in a plush off—campus apartment. Embarrassed--she thought she was beginning to sound poverty stricken. . Happy--Linda was saying she had good sense. 140 10. What was Linda thinking after Ceci mentioned hitch-hiking? 20. What was Mary thinking as Ceci described how her father brought up the family? l0. What was Linda thinking after Ceci mentioned hitch—hiking? A. Hitch hikers get all wet or splashed. I would not like it. B. I have hitch-hiked, but it takes too long. C. I would not hitch-hike. I am more dependent. D. I do not think Ceci likes me. 20. What was Mary thinking as Ceci described how her father brought up the family? A. It sounds pretty cold to me! 8. That is what I hate about families-—they test you. C. I would rather have a car than know why I could not have one. D. That is a good way to raise kids-—to let them decide for them- selves. 141 NOW THAT YOU HAVE VIEWED THE VIDEOTAPED GROUP SESSION, ANSWER THE FOLLOWING GENERAL QUESTIONS ABOUT THE GROUP MEMBERS. Questions on Linda 29. Who did Linda like best during the group session? B) Mary B) Pam C) Ceci 30. Who did Linda like least during the group session? A) Mary B) Pam C) Ceci 31. Who did Linda think was most similar to her? A) Mary B) Pam C) Ceci Questions on Mary 32. Who did Mary like best during the group session? A) Pam B) Ceci C) Linda 33. Who did Mary like least during the group session? A) Pam B) Ceci C) Linda 34. Who did Mary think was most similar to her? A) Pam B) Ceci C) Linda Questions on Pam 35. Who did Pam like best during the group session? A) Ceci B) Linda C) Mary 36. Who did Pam like least during the group session? A) Ceci B) Linda C) Mary 37. Who did Pam think was most similar to her? A) Ceci B) Linda C) Mary Questions on Ceci 38. Who did Ceci like best during the group session? A) Linda B) Mary C) Pam 39. Who did Ceci like least during the group session? A) Linda 8) Mary C) Pam 40. Who did Ceci think was most similar to her? A) Linda B) Mary C) Pam APPENDIX D THE AWARENESS MATERIALS 142 143 GENDLIN'S FOCUSING TECHNIQUE This is going to be just to yourself. What I will ask you to do will be silent, just to yourself. Take a moment to relax ... (5 seconds). All right now, just to yourself, inside you, I would like you to pay attention to a very special part of you.... Pay attention to that art where you usually feel sad, glad, or scared ... (5 seconds). Pay attention to that area in you and see how you are now. See what comes to you when you ask yourself, ”How am I now?” "How do I feel?" “What is the main thing for me right now?” Let it come in whatever way it comes to you, and see how it is. 30 seconds or less If among the things you have just thought of, there was one major problem which felt important, continue with it. Otherwise, select a meaningful personal problem to think about. Make sure you have chosen some personal problems of real importance in your life. Choose the thing which seems most meaningful to you.. . (19 seconds). Of course, there are many parts to that one thing you are thinking about-eto many to think of each one alone. But, you can feel all of these things together. Pay attention there where you usually feel things, and in there you can get a sense of what all of the problem feels like. Let yourself feel all of that. 30 seconds or less As you pay attention to the whole feeling of it, you may find that one special feeling comes up. Let yourself pay attention to that one feeling. l minute Keep following one feeling. Do not let it be just_words or pictures, wait and let words or pictures come from the feeling. l minute If this one feeling changes, or moves, let it do that. Whatever it does, follow the feeling and pay attention to it. l minute Now, take what is fresh, or new, in the feel of it now ... and go very easy. Just as you feel it, try to find some new words or pictures to capture what your present feeling is all about. There does not have 144 to be anything that you did not know before. New words are best, but old words might fit just as well. As long as you now find words or pictures to say what is fresh to you now. 1 minute If the words or pictures that you now have make some fresh differ- ence, see what that is. Let the words or pictures change until they feel just right in capturing your feeling. 1 minute Now I will give you a little while to use in any way you want to, and then we will stop. ' af'ANxmus 7 APPENDIX E FEELING SUMMARY USED BY TARGET TO IDENTIFY FEELINGS 146 147 FEELING INDICATOR Check all the feelings you were having in the last few seconds before the videotape stopped. in response to. surprised awe talkative amused happy hopeful optimistic enthusiastic courageous--daring sensible--reasonable patient flexible helpful capable determined proud relieved comfortable--at ease satisfied refreshed protected appreciated ambivalent confused-~mixed up baffled erratic--disorganlzed.::::: impatient listless--indifferent:::::: bored daydreaming exhausted reluctant careful cautious stubborn--resistant::: forced controlled trapped frustrated annoyed-~angry disgusted--fed up upset-disturbed anxious embarrassed foolish-ridiculous guilty worried scared disappointed abused bitter regretful discouraged depressed defeated lonely helpless hopeless other Explain, if you can, what these feelings were III» APPENDIX F POST-EXPERIMENT DATA 148 149 SUBJECTS' OBSERVATIONS CONCERNING THE EXPERIMENT Observations I. Problems with quality of videotaped information. A. Some videotaped sessions were more interesting than others. B. Group II was easier to understand than Group I. C. Targets did not say their real feelings. The situ- ation was artificial. . Thought targets were trying to be safe. II. Problems with attending. E. Subject was more awake some days than others. .31 KLHICD . Took awhile to become accoustomed to the testing situation. . Had a hard time following participants. . Had a hard time seeing the videotape. Too complex. Felt pressured during the testing situation. . Felt empathized differently with some than with others. III. Problems with tests. L. Information on videotape not adequate to answer questions. M. Had different alternatives than those given. . Questions too difficult, too specific; better if more general. . Thinking questions were more flexible than the others. . Subject reacted to the same words on the Myers- Briggs differently. No. of Ss Responding —l-—l—J|\)—J BIBLIOGRAPHY BIBLIOGRAPHY Abelson, R. P., and Sermat, V. Multidimensional scaling of facial expressions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1962, 63, 546-54. Allport, G. W. Personality; A Psychological Interpretation. New York: Henry Holt, 1937. Allport, G. W. Pattern and Growth in Personality. New York: Harper and Row, 1961. Asch, J. E. Forming impressions of personalities. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1946, 41, 258—290. Bender, I. E., and Hastorf, A. H. On measuring generalized empathic ability (social sensitivity). Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1953, fig, 503-506. Bender, I. E., and Hastorf, A. H. The perception of persons: Fore- casting another person's responses on three personality scales. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1950, 45, 556-61. Berne, E. Transactional Analysis in Psychotherapy. London: Evergreen Books Ltd., 1961. Bieri, J. Cognitive complexity-simplicity and predictive behavior. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1955, 51, 263-268. Blanchard, W. A. Relevance of information and assimilation-contrast in interpersonal prediction. Perceptgal and Motor Skills, 1967, 24, 515-25. Blanchard, W. A. Assimilation and contrast in interpersonal predic- tion with control for the interaction of real similarity and dif- ferential accuracy. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1966, 3, 567—73. Bronfenbrenner, U., Harding, J., and Gallwey, M. O. The measurement of skill in social perception. In A. L. Baldwin et al. (eds.), Talent and Society. Princeton, N. J.: Van Nostrand, 1958. Bruner, J. 5., and Tagiuri, R. The perception of people, in Lindzey, C. (ed.), Hardbook of Social Psychology. Cambridge, Mass.: AddiSOn-Wesley, 1954, 634—54° 150 151 Buchheimer, A., Goodman, J., and Sircus, G. Videotapes and kinescope recordings as situational tests and laboratory exercises in empathy for the training of counselors. Hunter College of the City University of New York, 1965. A technical report submitted to the U. S. Office of Education, NDEA Grant No. 7-42-0550-1670. Buros, 0. K., Ed. The Sixth Mental Measurements Yearbook. Highland Park, N. J.: The Gryphon Press, 1965, pp. 321-26. Campbell, R. J. The development and validation of a multiple-choice scale to measure affective sensitivity (empathy). Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Michigan State University, 1967. Chapman, J. L. The development and validation of a scale to measure empathy. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Michigan State University, 1966. Cline, V. B. Ability to judge personality assessed with a stress inter- view and sound-film technique. Journal of Abnormal and Social * Psychology, 1955, £9, 183—7. Cline, V. B. Interpersonal Perception. In B. A. Maher (ed ), Progress in Experimental Personality Research, 1964, v01. 1, 221-284 Cline, V. B., and Richards, J. M. Accuracy of interpersonal perception-- a general trait. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1960, 99, 1—7. Cline, V. B. and Richards, J. M. A comparison of individuals versus groups in judging personality. .Journal Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1961, gg, 446—49. Cohen, A. R. Cognitive tuning as a factor affecting impression forma- tion. Journal of Personality, 1961, g2, 235-245. Cook, M. Interpersongl Perception. London: Penguin Books Inc., 1971. Cottrell, L. S., and Dymond, R. F. The empathic responses: A neglected field for research. Psychiatry, 1949, 1g, 355-59. Crockett, W. H. Cognitive complexity and impression formation. In B. A. l Maher (ed.), Progress in Experimggtal Personality Research, Vol. ‘ 2, New York: Academic Press, 19 5 Cronbach, L. J. Processes affecting scores on ”understanding of others” and “assumed similarity". Psychological Bulletin, 1955, gg, 177—193. sets in interpersonal perception. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology 1957, 54_, 384- 390. l Crow, W J , and Hammond, K. R. The generality of accuracy and response i 152 Darwin, C. The Expressions of the Emotions in Man and Animals. London: J. Murray, 1872. Davitz, J., and Davitz, L. Correlates of accuracy in the communication of feelings. Journal of Communication, 2; 5—13. Dymond, R. F. A scale for the measurement of empathic ability. Journal of Copsulting Psychology, 1949, 15, 127—133. Ekman, P., and Friesen, W. V. Constants across cultures in the face and emotion. Journal of Personality 55d Social Psychology, 1]; 124-129. Engen, T., Levy, N., and Schlosberg, H. The dimensional analysis of a new series of facial expressions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1958, 55, 454-458. Estes, S. G. Judging personality from expressive behavior. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1938, 55, 217-36. Estes, S. G. The judgment of personality on the basis of brief periods of behavior. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Harvard Univer- sity, 1937. Eysenck, H. J. Manual of the Maudsley Personality Inventory. London: University of London Press, 1959. Feigenbaum, K. D. Egocentrism in young children. Paper presented to Psychology Department Colloquium, Michigan State University, 1967. Fernberger, S. W. False suggestion and the Piderit model. American Journal of Psychology, 1928, 59, 562-568. Gage, N. L., and Cronbach, L. J. Conceptual and methodological problems in interpersonal perception. Psychological Review, 1955, 523 411-422. Gage, N. L., Leavitt, G. S., and Stone, G. C. The intermediary key in the analysis of interpersonal perception. Psychological Bulletin, 1956, 55, 258—266. Gendlin, E. T. Experiencing and the Cregtion of Meaning. New York: The Free Press of Glencoe, 1962. Halpern, H. M. Some factors involved in empathy. Dissertation Abstract, 1954, 143 l458~9. Hastorf, A. H., Schneider, D. J., and Polefka, J. Person Perception. Reading, Mass.: Addison-Wesley, 1970. in. 153 Hathaway, S. Clinical intuition and inferential accuracy. Journal of Personality, 1955, 24, 223-250. Heist, P., and Yonge, G. Omnibus Persopglity Inveptory, Form F. New York: The Psychological Corporation, 1962. Hogan, R. Development of an empathy scale. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 1969, 55, 307—16. Howarth, E. Extroversion and dream symbolism: an empirical study. Psychological Reports, 1962, 19, 211-214. Jackson, D. N., and Messick, S. J. Individual differences in social perception. British Journal of Social gpd Clinical Psychology, 1963, 2, 1-10. Jung, C. G. Psychological Types. Trans. H. G. Baynes. New York: Harcourt, Brace and Company, 1923. (First German edition, 1921.) Jung, C. G. Contributions to Analytical Psychology. Trans. C. F. Baynes and H. G. Baynes. New York: Harcourt, Brace and Company, 1928. Kagan, N., Schauble, M. S., Resnikoff, A., Danish, S. J., and Krathwohl, D. R. Interpersonal Process Recall, Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 1969, 135, 365-374. Kagan, N., and Krathwohl, D. R. Studies in Human Interaction. (Final report, Grant number 0ET-32-0410-270) East Lansing, Michigan: Michigan State University, Educational Publication Services, 1967. Kanekar, S. Accuracy in person perception. Psychologia: An Inter- national Journal of Psychology in the Orient, 1972, 153 76—88. Katz, R. L. Empathy: Its Nature and Uses. London: The Free Press of Glencoe, 1963. Kelly, G. A. The Psychology of Persopgl Constructs. New York: Norton, 955, 2 Vols. Kerr, W. A., and Speroff, B. T. The Measurement of Empathy. Chicago: Psychometric Affiliates, 1951. Kirk, R. E. Experimental design: procedures for the bepgvioral sciences. Belmont, Ca1if.: Brooks/Cole, 1968. Lipps, T. Das Wissen von fremden Ichen. Psychologische Untersuchungen, 1907, 1, 694-722. Lundy, R. M. Assimilative projection and accuracy of prediction in inter- personal perception. Journal of Abnormgl and Social Psychology, 1956, 52, 33-38. 154 Maslow, A. H. Motivation and Personality, New York: Harper and Row, 1954. Mehrabian, A., and Epstein, N. A measure of emotional empathy. Journal of Personality, 1972, 45, 525-543. Mendelsohn, G. A. Myers-Briggs Type Indicator. In 0. K. Buros, The Sixth Mental Measurements Yearbook. Highland Park, N. J.: Gryphon Press, 1965, 331-332. Munn, N. L. The effect of knowledge of situation upon judgment of emo- tion from facial expressions. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology; 1940,_55, 324-338. Murray, H. A. Explorations in Personality. New York: Oxford University Press, 1938. Myers, 1. B. The Myers-Briggs Type Indicator. Princeton, N. J.:‘ Educational Testing Service, 1962a. Myers, I. B. Manual(196g),the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator. Princeton, N. J.: Educational Testing Service, 1962b. Notcutt, B., and Silva, A. L. M. Knowledge of other people. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1951, 45, 30-37. Purcell, K., Modrick, J. A., and Yamahiro, R. Item vs. trait accuracy in interpersonal perception. Journal of General Psychology, 1960, 55, 285-292. Rank, R. C. Counseling competence and perceptions. The Personnel and Guidance Journal, 1966, 45, 359-365. Rogers, C. R. Client-Centered Therapy. Boston: Houghton Mifflin, 1951. Rommetveit, R. Selectivity, Intuition, and Halo Effects in Social Perception. Oslo: Oslo University Press, 1960. Ross, J. Progress Report on the College Student Characteristics Study: June, 1961. Research Memorandum 61-11. Princeton, N. J.: Educational Testing Service, 1962. Schlosberg, H. The descriptions of facial expressions in terms of two dimensions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1952, 44, 229- 237. Schneider, 0. J. Implicit personality theory: A review. Psychological Bulletin, 1973, Z5, 296-311. 155 Sechrest, L., and Jackson, 0. N. Social intelligence and accuracy of interpersonal predictions, Journal of Personality, 1961, 55, 167-182. Shrauger, S., and Altrocchi, J. The persOnality of the perceiver as a factor in person perception. Psyghological Bulletin, 1964, 55_ 289—308. Smith, H. C. Sensitivity to People.) New York: McGraw-Hill, 1966. Speroff, B. J. Empathy and role reversal as factors in industrial harmony. Journal of Social Psychology, 1953, 55, 117-120. Steiner, I. Interpersonal behavior as influenced by accuracy of social perception. Psychological Review, 1955, 55, 268-274. Steinmetz, H. C. Directive psychotherapy: V. Measuring psychological understanding. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 1945, 4, 331—335. Stone, G. C., and Leavitt, G. S. Generality of accuracy in perceiving standard persons. Chicago: University of Illinois, 1954. (Mimeo.) Stotland, E. Exploratory investigations of empathy. In L. Berkowitz (ed.), Advanced Experimental Social Psychology, 1969, Vol. 4, 271-314. Stotland, E., Sherman, S. E., and Shaver, K. G. Empathy and birth order. Lincoln, Neb.: University of Nebraska Press, 1971. Stricker, L. J., and Ross, J. Intercorrelations and reliability of the Myers—Briggs Type Indicator scales. Psychological Reports, 1963, 15, 287-293. Stricker, L. J., and Ross, J. Some correlates of a Jungian personality inventory. Psychological Reports, 1964, 44, 623-643. Strunk, Orlo. Empathy: A review of theory and research. Psycho- 1ogig§1_News1etter, 1957, 5, 47-57. Suchman, J. R. Social sensitivity in the small task-oriented group. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology. 1956, 55, 75-83. Sullivan, H. S. The Interpepspnal Theory of Psychiatry. New York: Norton, 1953. Sundberg, N. D. Myers-Briggs Type Indicator. In 0. K. Buros, The Sixth Mental Measurements Yearbook. Highland Park, N. J.: Gryphofi‘Tl ‘ Press, 1965, 332-335. 156 Taft, R. The ability to judge people. Psychological Bulletin, l955, 5 l-23. _9 Tagiuri, R. Person perception. In G. Lindzey (ed.), Handbook of Social Psychology. Reading, Mass.: Addison-Wesley, 1968, Vol. 4, 395-449. Thompson, D. F., and Meltzer, L. Communication of emotional intent by facial expression. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, I964, 55, 129—l35. Truax, C. B. A scale for the measurement of accurate empathy. Psychiatric Institute Bulletin, Wisconsin Psychiatric Institute, University of Wisconsin, l96l, l) 12. Truax, C. B., and Carkhuff, R. R. Toward Effective Counseling and Psychotherapy: Training and Practice. Chicago: Aldine Publishing Company, 1967. Valentine, C. W. The relative reliability of men and women in intuitive judgments of character. British Journal of Psychology, l929, 12, 213-238. Warr, P. B., and Sims, A. A. A study of cojudgment processes. Journal of Persona1ity, 1965, 553 598-604. Wescott, M. R. Toward a Contgmporary Psychology of Intuition. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston, 1968. Vernon, P. E. Personality Assessment: A Critical Survey. London: Methune, 1963. Woodworth, R. S. Experimental Psychology. New York: Henry Holt, l938 Zajonc, R, B. The process of cognitive tuning in information. Journgl of Abnormal and Social Psychology, l960, 51, 159-167. , 77,,_ __ .7 _ _ ___.___ 7. _._. i III I _ "I7'11lii’iiiliilliiii1E5