QIIE?“£~‘~+-+93T? 2 §»}\-}.C 2 Y p.133 .2 0 1333 OVERDUE FINES: 25¢ per day per iteu RETURNING LIBRARY MATERIALS: Place in book return to remove charge fro: circulation records @1980, Edward D. Seely BEHAVIORS OF PEER LEADERS, ADULT LEADERS, MOTHERS AND FATHERS AS PERCEIVED BY YOUNG PEOPLE FOURTEEN THROUGH EIGHTEEN YEARS OF AGE By Edward DeVoe Seely A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Secondary Education and Curriculum 1980 ‘ABSTRACT BEHAVIORS OF PEER LEADERS, ADULT LEADERS, MOTHERS AND FATHERS AS PERCEIVED BY YOUNG PEOPLE FOURTEEN THROUGH EIGHTEEN YEARS OF AGE By Edward D. Seely The study examined ll behaviors which high school youth perceive as desirable and 11 as undesirable in their leaders (based on Gamelin, 1970). The sample, drawn widely from church, school and other groups, including a national program for delinquent youth, consisted of 1536 young people in the continental United States. The subjects indicated on a Likert scale the degree of importance (of the positive be- haviors) and the degree of seriousness (of the nega- tive) each had when performed by Egg; leaders and adult leaders. The reSpondents were also asked to indicate on the same scale the degree to which each of these behaviors was true of the mothers and fig- thggg. Using the mean scores obtained, rankings were also indicated for the four sets of responses (pggg, adult, mother, father). Subgroups from which data were received were urban, suburban and rural youth: "delinquent" and "average” young people: male and female respondents: church-related and nonchurch related subjects; each year of age: and voluntary group members as well as members of groups on a non- voluntary basis (such as math class). Regional data were also analyzed from within the boundaries of the contintntal United States. A multivariate analysis of scale scores was con- ducted. A scale score is the average of the ll mean scores for a category, for example, Egg; positive. General descriptive statistics for item frequencies are also provided. Pearson Product Moment correla- tions were also made on both scale and item analyses. Several important findings were identified. The top-ranked and highest rated desirable behavior for both Egg; leader and adult leader is "listening." The second and third most important behaviors in ggglt leaders were identified by the youth as "understanding the concerns of young people" and "communicating.” The same two were identified for Egg; leaders in the second and third positions only in reverse order. The most serious undesirable behavior of both Egg; leaders and ggult leaders is "hypocrisy.” A bimodal distribution of the negative scales occurred between peg; and adult leaders and mother and father. The behaviors ranked most serious (negative) for peer and adult leaders (very high mean scores on the Likert scale) received very low mean scores, indicating that the subjects view these behaviors as not very true. for their mother and father. An example is with regard to the most serious behavior of pug; and adult leaders, ”hypocrisy," which the youth re- ported as least true of both mother and father. MQEE’ SEE and fathers received high mean scores (all above the median) for each of the desirable behaviors. For pug; leaders all four of the most desired behaviors were types of consideration. For udult leaders the t0p three were related to consideration and the fourth was a type of initiation of structure. The first and third for mothgr was a type of initia- tion of structure, the second and fourth a type of consideration. The third for fathgr was a type of consideration while the others were types of initia- tion of structure. The top-ranked item for uothgr and fgthg; was the structural ”using of firmness when necessary." I Few differences cocurred among the subgroups. Only a few minor differences were noted across the regions. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS When it became apparent that this dissertation project would deve10p into a national study, I was only slightly aware of the enormity and complexity of the new dimensions it would take. In one sense, it became a test of faith; as such the ultimate ac- knowledgment and praise belongs to the Lord who kept me from being overwhelmed. Happily, most of those who contributed to the magnitude of the task also helped greatly in its ac- complishment and truly deserve recognition. I am grateful to the more than 1500 young pe0ple who gave their time, to the church and school administrators and teachers who made provision for the survey, to the management and staff of Youth for Christ Inter- national who Sponsored the study and provided the resources for gathering the data, and to the members and fellow staff associates of Christ Church of Oak Brook, in Oak Brook, Illinois for their continual expressions of assurance and cheer. Special acknowledgment is due to certain peeple in particular whose contribution to this project ii exceeds anything I could have expected or desired. The first to be mentioned must be Ted Ward, the chairman of my committee and project director, whose guidance and counsel has been a constant source of professional and personal enrichment. An educator 'par excellence, he, and his family as well, have con- tributed immeasurably to my development. The members of my faculty committee have also helped me greatly. Norman Bell has helped me attain many understandings and technical skills for the con- ducting of educational research and insights as to how these skills may be profitably used in the field of church education. Ben Bohnhorst has introduced me to much of the great literature in my field of curric- ulum and has enabled me to develOp confidence and com- petence in my scholarship. Fred Graham's generous gift of time and abiding care as well as his percep- tive exploration of theological and ecclesiastical implications of the study have been a source of blessing to me. As with all such projects, many others have shared in significant ways in the shaping of its construction. Frank Jenkins of the MSU Office of Research Consultation went far beyond what could reasonably be expected of him in his role as iii statistical consultant and computer programmer. Roger Harris, a computer programmer and member of Christ Church, gave much personal time in helping with the statistical analysis of the computer out- put. Vic Glavach, Assistant to the President of Youth for Christ International, went well above the call of duty in managing the many details involved in facilitating the organizational mechanics re- quired in order to conduct the study. Arthur DeKruyter, Senior Pastor of Christ Church, is a colleague and friend whose encouragement and reas- surance have been a constant inspiration. In any endeavor involving the rigors of a dis- sertation, the effects on one's family are signi- ficant. I concur with the youth who were surveyed in the study that parents should be rated very favor- ably. I am appreciative of the fact that not only during the first thirty-nine years of my life but even throughout the struggles associated with this project these first leaders continued to help, to listen and to support me. My wife, Carol, and daugh- ter, Janet, have been enormously patient and under- standing in spite of the considerable absence of their husband and father. In addition, Carol has helped me with editing and doing most of the typing. iv Moreover, her perspectives as a good educator were especially helpful in identifying the meaning of the findings reported in Chapter V. In all these and many other ways as well, she has had an important part in the completion of this dissertation and the contribution it holds for the future. I Edward D. Seely March 20, 1980 TABLE OF CONTENTS _ Page .LIST OF TABLES . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . ix LIST or FIGURES . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . xii A Chapter I.THEPROBLEM.............. 1 Need for the Present Study Purpose of the Study Importance of the Study Assumptions of the Study Definition of Terms Research Questions The First Pilot Study Factors Affecting Generalizability II. THE PRECEDENT RESEARCH . . . . . . . . . 26 Context of the Study Content Base of the Study Method Base of the Study III. THE DESIGN . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 53 The Plan of the Study Internal Validity External Validity Instrument for the Study Reliability Validity Iv. THE FINDINGS e 0 O O C I O I C O I O O I 75 Research Questions and Pertinent Data Research Question #1 Research Question #2 Research Question #3 vi Chapter vii Research Question #h Research Question #5 Tapical Generalizations Generalizations about Peer Leaders Generalizations about Adult Leaders Generalizations about the Relation- ship of Adult Leader Behaviors to the Mother Behaviors Generalizations about the Relation- ship of Adult Leader Behaviors to the Father Behaviors Other Generalizations Page v. THE CONCLUSIOIVS e e e o o e e o e e e e o e 1‘43 APPENDIX A APPENDIX B APPENDIX C APPENDIX D APPENDIX E What the Data Mean Conclusions Regarding Peer Leaders Conclusions Regarding Adult Leaders Conclusions Regarding Comparisons and Contrasts between Pear and Adult Leaders Conclusions Concerning the Relation- ship between Adult Leader Behaviors and Mother and Father Behaviors Meanings for Leader Selection Conclusions for Selecting Peer Leaders Conclusions for Selecting Adult Leaders Meanings for Leader Training Other Conclusions Research Questions for Further Study Additional Suggestions for Further Research The First Pilot Study . . . . . . . . . The Second Pilot Study . . . . . . . . The Instrument . . . . . . . . . . . . Instructions for the Trainers of Data Gatherers . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 177 200 208 212 213 viii Chapter Page Instructions for Data-Gatherers Public Relations Principles for Conducting the Survey Group Description Sheet APPENDIX F Pearson Product Moment Correlation of Mother Negative and Father Negative Scales by Item . . . . . . 216 BIBLIOGRAPHY . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 217 Table 1.0 2.1 2.2 2.3 ”.1 #.2 “.3 “.4 . 4.5 “.6 4.? LIST OF TABLES Subgroup Distributions . . . . . . . . Per Cent of Common Variance Accounted‘ for by Four Factors . . . . . . . . . Program Item Scores as Related to Rating of Leaders . . . . . . . . . . Traits Which Church Youth Like and Dislike in Their Leaders . . . . . . National Mean Scores and Ranking by Item in All Four Categories . . . . . Variable Comparison Rankings . . . . . Youth Perceptions of Peer and Adult Leadership Behavior--Mean Scores and Ranking by Item of Male and Female Subjects . . . . . . . . . . . Youth Perceptions of Peer and Adult Leadership Behavior-~Mean Scores and Ranking by Item of Urban and Voluntary Youth Guidance and "Other” 0 O O C O D O O O O O O O O 0 Youth Perceptions of Pear and Adult Leadership Behavior--Mean Scores and Ranking by Item of Voluntary Subur- ban and Rural Subjects . . . . . . . Youth Perceptions of Leadership Behav- ior--Distribution of All Subjects' Responses . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Youth Perceptions of Leadership Behavior--Distribution of Male and Female Responses . . . . . . . . . . ix Page 20 34 #1 77 79 33 85 87 91 9h Table Page “.8 - Youth Perceptions of Leadership Behav- ior--Distribution of Voluntary Suburban and Rural Responses . . . . . 96 “.9 - Youth Perceptions of Mother and Father. Leadership Behavior--Mean Scores and Ranking by Item of Male and Female 31113386133 e e e e e e s e e e e e e e e 109 ' “.10 - Youth Perceptions of Mother and Father Leadership Behavior--Mean Scores and Ranking by Item of Urban and Volun- tary Youth Guidance and ”Other” . . . . 112 “.11 - Youth Perceptions of Mother and Father Leadership Behavior—~Mean Scores and Ranking by Item of Voluntary Subur- ban and Rural Subjects . . . . . . . . 117 “.12 - Youth Perceptions of Leadership Behav- ior-~Distribution of Urban and Voluntary Youth Guidance and "Other” Responscs O O O O O O I O O O O O O O O 123 “.13 - Groups Which Tended to Rate Behaviors Higher O O C O O O O O O O O O O O D O 126 Al - Traits Which Church Youth Like and Dislike in their Leaders . . . . . . . 180 A2 - Frequency (Weighted) of Selection of the Eight PEER Leadership Competency Factors . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 182 A3 - Frequency (Weighted) of Selection of the Eight PEER Leadership Incompe- tency Factors . . . . . . . . . . . . . 183 A“ - Frequency (Weighted) of Selection of the Eight ADUDT Leadership Compe- tency Factors . . . . . . . . . . . . . 185 A5 - Frequency (Weighted) of Selection of the Eight ADUDT Leadership Incompe- tency Factors . . . . . . . . . . . . . 186 Table A6 - Frequency (Weighted) of Selection of the Sixteen Important Characteris- tics of Mother Factors A7 - Frequency (Weighted) of Selection of the Sixteen Important Characteris- tics of Father Factors A8 - Summary of Rank-Order of Characteris- tics xi Page 188 190 192 Figure “.1 - “.“ - “.6 - A1 - LIST OF FIGURES Youth Perceptions of Peer and Adult Leadership Behavior--Mean Scores and Ranking by Item of Male and Female ReSponses . . . . . . . . Youth Perceptions of Peer and Adult Leadership Behavior--Mean Scores and Ranking by Item of Urban and Voluntary Youth Guidance and "Other" Responses . . . . . . . Youth Perceptions of Peer and Adult Leadership Behavior--Mean Scores and Ranking by Item of Voluntary Suburban and Rural Subjects . . National Mean Scores and Rank by Item for Peer, Adult, Mother and Father Behaviors . . . . . . . . Youth Perceptions of Mother and Father Leadership Behavior--Mean Scores and Ranking by Item of Male and Female Responses . . . Youth Perceptions of Mother and Father Leadership Behavior--Mean Scores and Ranking by Item of Urban and Voluntary Youth Guid- ance and "Other" Responses . . . Youth Perceptions of Mother and Father Leadership Behavior-~Mean Scores and Ranking by Item of Voluntary Suburban and Rural SUbJeCtSeeeeeeeeeeee Scoring Sheet for the Ranking of Traits O O O O O O I O O O O O O xii Page 82 8“ 89 92 108 11“ 116 178 xiii Figure Page A2 - Weighted Scores for Peer, Adult, Father and Mother Characteristics Cited by Combined Male and Female Responses 0 O I O O O O O O O O O C O O O 19“ Bl - Cover Letter for Second Pilot Study .'. 201 B2 - Leadership Behaviors Youth Perceive as Important - INTERVIEW INSTRUMENT . . 202 CHAPTER I THE PROBLEM The education of today's young people continues to challenge theoreticians and practitioners in the ' schools and in the churches. Ideal situations have eluded us thus far. Conversations with colleagues in many parts of the world confirm widespread simi- larities of experience in the difficulties attending youth education and a concensus as to the integral role of leadership in improving the present condi- tions. Empirical investigation, while raising fur- ther questions, has provided some concrete bases for solving many of the biggest problems. Need for the Present Study In his landmark research on the processes of moral judgment, Kohlberg identified three levels through which human beings progress in their values development. He found that in normal development persons who have attained the capacity for reasoning at the second, conventional level view the source of truth as outside themselves. They impute a morality to that external source and respond accordingly. 1 2 The motivation to make these value judgments, and all others, is seen to be internal. Kohlberg also found the first part of this level (Level II) to be that form of thinking which is typically characteristic of adolescents (Kohlberg in Beck, dt_al., 1971, pp. 36- 378 Stewart, 197“, p. 3“). The findings of Kohlberg have fundamental and far-reaching significance for peOple who have the responsibility for planning and implementing programs which involve youth. Program planners must assume that youth participation in that which is designed for them will be essentially voluntary. Indeed, even in those situations where one still finds a certain amount of forced attendance, as in some Sunday Schools, it is not uncommon to also find a considerable amount of absenteeism on the part of those whose participa- tion is "required" by parents or others. To these conditions are added an integral phenom- enon concerning leadership. Leaders are the key fac- tors in the dynamics and outcomes affecting groups (Downton, 1973, p. 12: Strommen, 1971, p. 719: Perrow, 1972, p. 197: Good, Biddle and Brophy, 1975, pp. 7, 5“ ff.: Stogdill, 197“, pp. 7 ff.). As important as are such variables as room setting, curriculum mate- rials and socio-economic status, the leader is even 3 more critical. A configuration of variables designed to produce the most ideal environmental setting in which the brightest students were studying the "best” curriculum would produce ineffective results if the ,leader were incompetent. Conversely, a good leader can be effective in the midst of such adverse con- . straints as a substandard environment with poor curriculum materials and students who possess inade- quate understanding of the subject matter. Pur ose of the Stud Viewing the problem more specifically, at least four purposes were served by the present investiga- tion. First, since leaders are such key parts of groups, and since an adolescent's initial desire to join and continuing desire to remain in a group de- pends upon his or her own internal motivation, the study sought to identify what leaders do which attract and repel youth. Some organizations have youth groups with a very large membership while similar organiza- tions struggle to attract and hold merely a handful of young people. To know what the key peOple, the lea- ders, are doing in the former groups can be useful to those in the latter. a Second, the study tried to determine whether there is a relationship between the actions performed by 222: and adult leaders as to which attracts and repels youth. Because of their Level II orientation, young people are concerned about which other people their age are going to be part of a given group. As all task-oriented groups have leaders, whether emer- gent, appointed or elected (Fiedler, 1967, p. 8), the adolescent constituency of a group has its lea- dership also in addition to_any adult leader function- ing at the same time. To the extent that the tag; leaders are appointed,as occurs in some organizations, it is useful to know which actions of their 2222 and adult leaders youth value most and how the behaviors of both leaders compare. Such information is espe- cially valued, for example, by the Youth for Christ International (YFC) organization, which funded and staffed this study.* YFC is in the process of estab- lishing a new approach to reaching out to young peo- ple in which 2222 leaders will have the key role. The present study will help YFC and other organiza- tions who employ youth (222:) leaders in their *Support from YFC consisted of (l) participation of their staff as data-gatherers, (2) cost of duplica- tion and mailing, and (3) cost of statistical services. 5 recruitment procedures. Third, the study sought to determine whether a connection exists between what youth perceive as being true concerning their parents' actions. Psychological and sociological research has well established a re- lationship between the behaviors of parents and chil- dren (e.g., Horrocks, 1969, p. 599). but this study took this understanding a step farther and asked whether there was a relationship between the same ape- cific behaviors (which created positive and negative affect) of 2222 and adult leaders compared with their perception of their first leaders, mother and fatheg. A fourth purpose was to demonstrate again the usefulness of empirical research in the field of church education, which in the writer's own denomina— tion has been generally viewed as of questionable value as a valid and reliable means of inquiry, pre- ferring rather to depend on revelation and logic. While churches in such denominations have not always valued education as a high priority, they have valued it. Moreover, they value even higher the obtaining of the required number of teachers and other leaders, and the most effective ones, with the least difficulty. The results of this study will help them in these 6 endeavors and, at the same time, indicate the utility of empirical investigation in the field of church education and provide one more reason to place a higher value on the field itself. Importance of the Study At least four important anticipated outcomes can be identified in addition to the above. First, administrators in church education are becoming aware that it is no longer possible to select leaders and make other educational decisions apart from consider- ation of the felt needs and opinions of the youth for whom the programs are designed. To ignore this awareness will probably result in the development of programs attended by fewer and fewer young people. The study provides data that will help administrators base leader selection and evaluation on important matters of leadership performance. Second, administrators of education programs in the local church are concerned with the characteristics of 222! and adult leadership of youth. These adminis- trators as well as those of other organizations seeking to relate to young people will find this information useful in order to better reach their goals. Such or- ganizations include both religious and nonreligious , 7 groups, for example, YFC, Young Life, Inter-Varsity Christian Fellowship, Campus Crusade for Christ International, the Boy Scouts and Girl Scouts of America and Campfire Girls. Third, parents will be benefitted by knowing how children view them with respect to the positively and negatively perceived behaviors included in the study. They can use this information as a self- evaluation to determine whether this perception is parallel with the way they_are and want to be. Fourth, the results of this project will contri- bute to knowledge needed in extending the theory of leadership behavior. One specific area in which it is extended is the relationship between tag; and adult leaders with respect to the 22 behaviors (11 positive and 11 negative). Another contribution is the identification of parallels drawn between the pre- ceding and youth perceptions of what is true about their mothegs and fathers. Assumptions of tha_Study Five assumptions were made in the development of this research. First, though the scope of the study in its entirety is intended primarily for the religious education context, due to the nature of the research 8 design, the value of the study is similarly apparent for secular organizations. Phenomenological reality applies to both religious and nonreligious organiza- tions. Yet due to the fact that religious organiza- tions operate within constraints that sometimes differ from other organizations, such as the need to plan on V the premise that youth involvement will be largely voluntary, religious education is the focal point of the research. While this limitation will primarily only affect Chapter V, it is an organizing principle that shapes the text as a whole. Second, the philosophical orientation of the writer may be expressed as a commitment to the or- ganismic-structural-develOpmental approach to educa- tion.* Other approaches such as behaviorism and *The organismic-structural-developmental (OSD) orientation to education refers to an approach to human learning distinct from the other major ap- proaches, viz, behavioristic and psychoanalytical. The "organismic" aSpect refers to the perception of the learner as essentially healthy, intrinsically motivated, and actively functioning in an holistic, fully integrated, manner in which he relates trans- actionally with his environment. The "structural" sepect of this orientation refers to the underlying, organized, dynamic and universal patterns which typify human behavior, particularly thought. The ”developmental” dimension of the OSD approach refers to the observable (content) and mental (structural) change within the organism as he constructs new structures as a result of transacting with the en- vironment. In the process of this construction the organism pragresses through a series of qualitatively 9 psychoanalysis have important contributions to make with reapect to such leadership matters as helping people who are struggling with pathological dis- orders of various kinds, but for the overall founda- tion on which to build a view of leadership, the organismic orientation will be employed. Third, organismic theory has given rise to gen- eral systems theory and the attendant structural- functional theory of leadership behavior (Downton, 1973, p. 5). It is structural-functional theory which has supplied the framework of the current study's observation of leaders' behaviors. Fourth, a theological commitment affects not only the philosophical orientation described above but also the writer's conceptualization of the nature of leadership bepavio: which will be reflected in the discussion below with respect to the definition of this term. Holding the Bible to be the unique reve- lation of God and Jesus Christ his only Son, the author maintains the necessity of shaping the concept of leadership in accord with Jesus' teachings. Thus the primary concern of leadership is the function of different, structurally whole, and hierarchically in- tegrated stages which proceed according to an invar- iant sequence. (Stewart, 19?“, pp. “2o“7). 10 service, often lacking in secular definitions, and even where included usually applies to the group it- self rather than to include pe0ple outside the group as its objects. Fifth, it is assumed that in reporting on their preferences for leader behaviors, young people are describing what effectiue leader behavior is. The discussion below in Chapter II (particularly that which examines the precedent literature that serves as the content base for the present study) supplies empirical support for this assumption. Definition of Terms Throughout this dissertation a number of words and terms will be used consistently with their technical meanings given in the leadership literature. Several will be identified here and, where necessary, others will be defined where they occur in the text below. LEADERSHIP BEHAVIOR The Specific acts a leader employs in helping his followers meet their collective and individual needs. Studies on leadership during the last fifty years have produced many definitions of leadership and related constructs. However, working within the structural-functional theoretical framework narrows the number of acceptable definitions ll considerably, though, in the Opinion of this writer, none is perfect. Nevertheless, Fiedler has develOped a definition which is the basis of the one employed in this study. In this conceptualization leadership behavior refers to ”the particular acts in which a leader engages in 'the course of directing and coordinating the work of his group members. This may involve such acts as structuring the work relations, praising or criti- cizing group members, and showing consideration for their welfare and feelings“ (Fiedler, 1962, p. 36). Fiedler's definition is as complete as any but it lacks a specific reference to service. The concept of service is found in the leadership literature, though it is uncommon (Downton, 1973. p. 7). and the articu- lation which comes closest to the conceptualization herein is that of Downton who states that leadership can be broadly defined as the coordinating structure of social systems. Through goal-setting and attainment, leader- ship coordinates the activites of other structures in order to increase the capa- bilities of the system. By increasing capabilities, leadership contributes in a positive way to the service capacity of the system, which enhances its ability to per- sist. (p. 1“) Yet it is to be noted that the service is oriented to the group of which the leader is a part. 12 Leadership behavior should be viewed more inclu- sively. As used in this study, leadership behavior refers to service outside as well as within the group, with personal concerns last of all. Theological and pragmatic reasons apply to this viewpoint, and the latter are occasioned by the former. With respect to the former, Jesus said, You know that the rulers of the Gentiles lord it over them, and their great men ex- ercise authority over them. It shall not be so among you: but whoever would be great among you must be your servant, and whoever would be first among you must be your slave; even as the Son of man came not to serve, and to give his life as a ransom for many" (Matthew 20:25-28 RSV). The pragmatic aspect follows in the expectations of the religious organizations described above. In both church and parachurch groups, a high expectation exists that leadership behaviors will be designed to accomplish goals which pertain to both the group of which a person is a part and to peOple outside the group who are potential members. The accomplishment of goals is an integral aspect of leadership behavior and a determinant of its effectiveness: one of the main goals of church and parachurch organizations is reaching outside their own groups to obtain new mem- bers (Matthew 28:19-20). 13 GROUP An interdependent set of two or more indi- viduals interacting c00perative1y to achieve a commonly valued goal. As a number of writers have noted, lea- dership presupposes the existence of a group (e.g., Krech and Crutchfield, 1959. p. 683), even though that group, sometimes called a "social system“ is as small as two members (Downton, 1973, p. 5). While there are almost as many definitions of ”group" as of "leader- ship,” the structural-functional theory will provide a means of selection. The conceptualization of Fiedler (1967, p. 6) has been adapted here because of its consistency with the theory and limitation to the key aspects of the issue affecting this research. That aspect of group life which is most important with respect to the current study focuses on the concept of goal. This awareness is one of the reasons Mitchell prefers the term "social system:' He insists that one of the main properties of a social system is that "the interaction is relatively persistent: a chance meeting of strangers on a street cannot be considered as a social system" (p. “), but such a configuation of per- sons could be considered a group. This latter under- standing is not accepted within the meaning of "group” for the purposes of this research. l“ CHURCH EDUCATION The formal and nonformal pro- grams in which groups within a specific local church meet to accomplish particular learning objectives. Not only the interrelationships within the group affect the members but also the group's holistic . association within the larger organizational struc- ture. However, the primary setting is within the confines of the local institutional body, i.e.. the specific church. RELIGIOUS EDUCATION The formal and nonformal programs of learning conducted by church and para- church organizations. It is a term which is broader than ”church education” as it includes such organi- zations as YFC as well. DBLINQUENT A person who has been judged by the due process of the community in which he or she is a resident to be in violation of one or more of that community's laws. The word usually appears here in' its adjectival form. Contacts with these young people were made through the social service branch of YFC. The word appears in quotes because it is ne- cessary to recognize that while these young people do share a common characteristic (criminal behavior), some of the other youth also share this same charac- teristic, along with the similar mentality. It is 15 thus possible that the only noteworthy difference between the "delinquent” youth and the other youth who have committed a crime is that the latter were not caught (and perhaps that evasion was not even due -to any skill, care, or effort of their own making)! Yet this situation also occurs in society as a whole, ‘and the inclusion of the ”delinquents" was for the purpose of learning whether they view behaviors in their pup; and adult leaders, and in their parents, differently from the other youth (who are referred to as "typical,” "average,” or ”other"). as h stio There are five main research questions. First, do young people view any of the 11 positive and 11 negative behaviors as more important on any kind of consistent basis with respect to their paap leaders? Second, do young people view any of the 11 positive and 11 negative behaviors as more important on any kind of consistent basis with respect to their adult leaders? Third, what is the similarity or dissimi- larity in responses to paap and adult leaders? Fourth, in what ways is the ranking of the behaviors for adult leader similar and dissimilar with the be- haviors seen by young peOple in their own uptpapa? 16 Fifth, in what ways is the ranking of the behaviors for adult leader similar and dissimilar with the be- haviors seen by young peOple in their own fat 8? Tha First Pilpt Study In the light of these questions, a pilot study ' (hereafter referred to as the first pilot study) of the main study herein described was done in Muskegon, Michigan (n1 November 12, 1977. During a conference of the Muskegon chapter of YFC, data were collected from each of the 70 participating teen-agers. About one-third of the young people were active and about two-thirds were currently inactive in the organiza- tion. The ages of the participants varied from l“-18. There were 11 fourteen year-olds (10 female and 1 male), 12 fifteen year-olds (11 female and 1 male), 20 sixteen year-olds (18 female and 2 male) 2“ sev- enteen year olds (12 female and 12 male) and 3 eighteen year-olds (1 female and 2 male). The mean age was 15.5. The median age was 16.5. The modal age was 17. At least four purposes were served by the first pilot study. First, it was determined that the general scheme of the research had potentiality for 17 a wider and more far-reaching investigation (Ap- pendix A). Second, the pilot study identified what needed to be done differently in a major study of the same subject, which differences were incorporated into the design of the main study and are discussed . in Chapter III. Third, as with descriptive research in general, it was intended that the findings, in whole or in part, would lend themselves to the generation of new quggtiong, This subject will be discussed in Chapter V. Fourth, the study indicated that there is rea- son to doubt the recent thinking of some leaders of youth organizations who maintain that young people are currently looking for more authority figures who are very autocratic as leaders. The findings of the first pilot study suggested that leadership of this sort does not appear to be what youth desire. Such a finding is of considerable importance to churches and youth organizations as guidance for staff selec- tion and training and in designing programs. Faatopa Affepting Gapepallzability The scope of this study was limited to high school young people, specifically those in the age range of 1“-18. The sample of 1536 was drawn from 18 public and private schools, YFC groups, ad hoc youth gatherings and church youth groups for those young people termed "typical.” For comparison, surveys were also administered to Youth Guidance groups which work especially with young peeple who have been arrested and referred to Youth Guidance (YG) for rehabilita- tion. (Youth Guidance is a social service branch of Youth for Christ International.) YFC's work in the continental United States is divided into nine regions. 'Groups in each region pro- ' vided respondents who completed the questionnaires. The selection of groups was made on the basis of availability rather than randomization, so generaliza- tion is limited. The subject of generalizability will be discussed in further depth in Chapter III, however, in general it can be said here that since the "typical" youth involved were from within normal settings, it can be concluded that the findings will hold for young peo- ple with similar backgrounds. What, precisely, are those backgrounds? More males (808) than females (689) participated in the sur- vey. It should be noted here and in the figures below which also appear on Table 1.0, that the numbers given will not always add up to 1536, the total sample size, for here with regard to the sex variable, and elsewhere 19 as well, a number (in this case 39 or 2.5%) did not complete all the information requested on the ques- tionnaire, e.g., sex. Hence the percentages given in the far right column in Table 1.0 are relative, rather than adjusted,frequencies. Fourteen year-olds numbered 208: fifteen year- } olds 392: sixteen year-olds “O3: seventeen year-olds 3h9: and eighteen year-olds 159. The last number is likely due to the fact that most of the surveys were completed in school and church groups which use grade- . level contexts for their instructional settings, and many seniors do not become eighteen years old until after graduation. The survey was conducted in April and May of 1979. Groups with voluntary membership, such as church youth groups and school music and sports teams, as well as groups with a nonvoluntary membership, such as math, science and history classes, were surveyed to see whether any differences exist in the perception of leader behaviors with respect to this aspect of group membership. Groups with voluntary membership con- tained 801 of the sample subjects, while groups with a nonvoluntary membership (such as math classes) had 735 of the subjects. Determining a group's membership presents 20 Ei‘ [ _ TABLE 1 0 - S b rou Distributions Clas- Raw Relative ehlca- Subgroup Number Frequenc .ILQDI (Pe e g gale 808 52.6 V’ 68 ““. Female 2 f 2 , l“ Year-Olds 208 l3.5 . o 15 Year-Olds 392 25.5 :0 ,_1t6 Yeap-Olds “03 26.2 12 Year-Olds 43“9 22.7 . 18 Year-Olds I 152 10.“ g a Voluntag 801 52.1 9“ Non-Volgtary ‘ 225 I “2.2 _ s ”3 Urban “96 32.3 § Suburban 56lg 36.5 A “28 21.1 . Church Related 179 11.7 1m 811.8. , ,3... Youth Guidance 230 15.0 55 Other 1 o 8 .o _ Region 1 - Pacific Northwest 12“ 8.1 Region 2 - Pacific Southwest 2“0 15.6 Region 3.- Mid-West States 1“6 9.5 g . Region “ - Northepn States 118 1,7 '3}, Region 5 - Western Guest Lakes 225 l“.6 a? Region 6 - Southepn States 2“6 16.0 , Region 2 - Bastepp_Great Lakes _9“ 6.1 Regiop 8 - Elaips Stataa 25“ 16,5 KW :38 $3? 21 considerable difficulty. Some schools consider a given subject as a core, and therefore required, sub- ject while for others it is an Option the students can choose or reject. In church youth groups, which are normally considered voluntary as far as membership is 'concerned, some members are present due to varying de- grees of parental pressure, the young people having no choice but to attend. The lines of differentiation were drawn as follows: due to compulsory attendance laws. all school groups were considered nonvoluntary except for music and sports groups which are not re- quired. Although, some church groups contain young people who are members due to parental coercion, it was considered that they would be termed voluntary on the basis that most of the membership is usually of that nature. Moreover, as those who have experience in working with church youth groups well know, when a teennager does not want to attend he can find inge- nious ways to absent himself. The sample was also composed of youth from urban and suburban as well as rural settings. Urban youth numbered “96: suburban 561: and rural “78. These demo- graphic designations were made by the YFC regional offices according to how the community in which the survey was taken views itself, rather than according to 22 distinctions in the sociological literature pertain- ing to how large a community is before it is considered urban rather than rural. In fact, sociologists are aware that the mentality of a person in a city like .Des Moines, Iowa (with a population of over 100,000 people) may be more "rural" than a person living in a [city like Rye, New York, which has only about 10,000 people. (See Bierstedt, 1957, p. 381; Broom and Selznick. 1963, p. 601.) Church relationship was also investigated. This variable, too, must be carefully considered, due to the fact that school groups contain many peOple who also have membership in churches. Any usefulness this measure may have, though, will be in its indication of the mentality existing when the survey was taken. The people in the church groups surveyed were meeting for a specific purpose different from that operating in the school settings. That purpose, or those purposes where there were more than one, was considered to be possibly related to the perception of leader behaviors held by the members. It was thus entered into the study as a variable to be observed. Church related groups contained 179 subjects, while non-church related subjects numbered 1357. Since YFC has a division which serves young 23 people who have been judged "delinquent" by societal authorities and who have been referred to this Youth Guidance division by the courts, an opportunity existed for observance of data which would indicate whether there may be a difference in the perception of leader behaviors by youth convicted of some kind of ideviancy, as contrasted with "average" youth, or the rest of the youth papulation, containing, it must be acknowledged, youth similar to those in Youth Guidance, who have performed similarly, yet without being ‘caught. The Youth Guidance subjects numbered 230; the contrasting part of the sample, the ”average," totalled 1305. The nine YFC regions include territory outside the continental United States, but the sample was drawn only from within the continental borders. The regions, referred to by name at YFC, were also assigned numbers for the purpose of this study. Region one, Pacific Northwest, includes Alaska. Idaho,0regon and Washington. Sample subjects numbered 12“ or 8.1% of the total sample. Region two, Pacific Southwest, includes California and Hawaii. Sample subjects numbered 2“0 or 15.6% of the total sample. Region three, Midwest States, includes Texas, 2“ Colorado, Mexico and Wyoming. The number of subjects from this region was l“6, which constituted 9.5% of the sample. Region four is the Northern States region which includes Moline, Illinois: Minnesota: Wisconsin and Iowa. The number of subjects from region four is 118 or 7.79%. Region five, Western Great Lakes, encompasses Indiana and all of Illinois except Molina. Region five subjects numbered 225 or l“.6% of the sample. Region six, Southern States, contains Florida, Georgia, Tennessee, Mississippi and North Carolina. This region supplied 2“6 or 16% of the sample. Region seven, Eastern Great Lakes, covers Michigan and Ohio. The number of subjects from this region was 9“ or 6.1% of the total sample. Region eight, Plains States, comprises Kansas. Nebraska and Missouri. Subjects from this region numbered 25“ or 16.5% of the sample. Region nine, Eastern States, includes Pennsylvania, New York, New Jersey, Connecticut, Massachusetts, Dela- ware, Maryland and Washington, D.C. The subjects from this region (88 or 3.7%) were drawn only from New York and New Jersey. Seven of the 88 were urban YG subjects, and rest of the sample from region nine were non-YG 25 youth from suburban Rochester, New York. With the above identifications made, it was next necessary to determine what is already known from previous studies in order to begin answering the re- search questions. This investigation is the subject of the next chapter. CHAPTER I I THE PRECEDENT RESEARCH Leadership is a complex and sophisticated concept. The literature on leadership is vast, particularly ex- panding since World War II. The first section of this chapter will be limited to a brief overview to estab- lish a frame of reference. Following will be those precedent studies that provide the bases for the con- ' tent and method used in the study under investigation. Context of the Study In many cultures words meaning "chief” or "king" are the only verbal symbols designating the differ- entiation of the ruler from the other members of so- ciety. It is in the countries with an Anglo-Saxon background that one finds a concentrated study of leadership. The word ”leader" appeared in the English language as early as 1300, yet it isn't until 1800 that the variation "leadership” occurs in this language (Stogdill, 197“, p. 7). While it is true that the sub- ject of leadership is treated in such ancient lore as Confucius' Apalegt and Plato's Republig, the systematic 26 2? phenomenon that caused the displacement of trait theory from its position of prominence. As Jennings concludes, "Fifty years of study . . . failed to pro- duce one personality trait or set of qualities that ~can be used to discriminate leaders and nonleaders" (1961, p. 2). Furthermore, scholars began to recog- inize as Leavitt explains that "the search for leader characteristics was bound to be of limited value, be- cause leadership is so clearly an interactive, rela- tional activity" (1973, p. 21?). Later studies included situational variables which were seen to significantly affect leadership behavior, followed by the current inquiry into how people become effective leaders. Having the capacity for rationality and logic, human beings, particularly the scientific sort, like to see categorization and order. The above must not be so construed at least in the absolute sense, for while there exists a general progression of develOp- ments in the field as indicated, it is not as neat as may be inferred. For example, traits are still being investigated, and situational variables were studied prior to the Second World War. In fact, it was Lewin and Lippitt's landmark study of authoritarian and demo- cratic group environments in 1938 which was the first 28 that any major impetus developed in the field.’ One of the main concerns at that time was the employment of intelligence testing in a concerted effort to ob- tain information helpful to overcome problems with respect to officer selection and placement. Between the world wars, research in the field focussed mainly on personal traits and on the ways in which people obtain leadership positions. This orienta- tion the so-called trait theory, resulted in the iden- tification of numerous personality characteristics that were supposedly associated with successful leadership. It was assumed that if one could isolate the key char- acteristics that were possessed by all successful lea- ders, it would then be possible to predict which people would do the job required in a given situation. Some of the traits claimed to be associated with leadership, out of the many suggested, are as follows: age, size, physical appearance (including dress), self-confidence, sociability, energy, intelligence, education, assertive- ness, and motivation (Berelson and Steiner, 196“, pp. 3“l-3“2: Stout and Briner, 1969, p. 700). Gradually, however, the trait theory gave way to other orientations which could be described as behav- ioral theories, some of which will be discussed in fur- ther detail below. It was not simply a bandwagon 29 study of the concept is very recent, comparatively speaking (Fiedler and Chemers, 197“, p. 1). Why, then. is the systematic study of leadership predominately conducted in Western, and particularly English-speaking, countries? Perhaps it is because much of the money to fund such investigations has largely come from organizations within the United States. Fiedler and Chemers (197“, pp. 2, 3), however, indicate that a more basic reason most likely lies in the political and social traditions of the countries ' of the Western hermisphere, especially those function- ing as a democracy. The authors point out that where only aristocrats can obtain leadership, it is useless to study selection and recruitment. It was only with rapid industrialization and the growth of large bureaucratic organi- zations in business and government that the need for new leadership was really felt. The aristocracies of Europe were reluctant to become involved with the un- gentlemanly calling of trade, and the larger and increasingly more complex business organizations required substantial numbers of managers to staff their depart- ments, plants, and offices. By the end of the nineteenth century it had become ob- vious that the selection of managers and military leaders could no longer be left to chance or to the accident of birth (Fiedler and Chemers, 197“, pp. 2. 3). Thus the first empirical research on leadership was published in 190“, but it wasn't until World War I 30 major research to study leadership as a two-way transaction between leader and followers (Stogdill, p. 365). Furthermore, Lewin, Lippitt and White's work and categories are still being used (e.g., Angell and DeSau, 197“, pp. ““-“5: Pandey, 1976, pp. “75-“89). Lewin, Lippitt and White described the behaviors of authoritapian leaders as determining all group pol- icies, dictating techniques and activity steps seriatum, dictating the particular work tasks and work companions of each group member, rendering ”personal" praise and criticism of the performance of each member and remain- ing aloof from active group participation with the ex- ception of giving demonstrations. Qappgzatlp leaders made policies through assisting the members in group discussion. General goals were sketched for the group, but alternatives were also given, the selection of which was made by the group. The choices of division of tasks and work partners were left to the group. The leader, who tried to be a regular member of the group, rendered praise and criticism in as "objective” and "fact-oriented” a manner as possible. Laissez-faipe leaders granted complete freedom to the group to deter- mine its policies. They only supplied resource materials for the group's activities, they did not direct them, and . 31 indicated they would supply information only when asked. He did not participate in the group's work and only made very infrequent evaluative comments on the perfor- mance of group members, unless they were questioned. ‘(Lewin, Lippitt and White in Puch, 197“, p. 232). This signal research has influenced studies to the present, but it contains flaws that should be noted. First, considerable confusion has diffused through the literature through admirers' of Lewin, Lippitt and White equating in their own studies the concepts of democratic and laissez-faire leadership behavior, thus claiming for the latter the results and benefits of the former (Stogdill, 197“, pp. 366-367). Later scholars have used the terms democratic and laissez-faire interchangeably. There has also been some inconsistency in the use of the authoritarian and democratic constructs. Second, as Perrow points out in his scathing critique of this aspect of leadership re- search in what he terms the human relations school of organizational research, the categories are too simple a dichotomization of those who practice good human re- lations and those who do not. A considerable number of other variables also effect leadership, for example ”the Ohio State studies indicated that 'structural' or 'task-oriented' expertise-~planning work, eliciting 32 ideas, scheduling, etc.--was as important as good in- terpersonal relations“ (Perrow. p. 110). Third, the earlier studies of Lewin, Lippitt and White, while cognizant of cultural and other influences (Lewin, Lippitt and White in Puch, p. 256), did not account _for them. For example, when groups have an expectancy and/br desire for authoritarian leadership, they will perform more effectively and be more satisfied than under another leadership style of behavior (Stogdill, 197“, p. 367). Another significant finding is that authoritarian leadership is related to the satisfac- tion of members when their group is large and/or has a major goal of task performance (Stogdill, 197“, pp. 369-370). Other dimensions have been described by Leavitt (pp. 218-220). The Lewin, Lippitt and White research has just been described for two reasons: its focus on the behavioral dimension of leadership set a trend followed by subsequent research: its particular findings are easily recognized as having far-reaching implications for religious education. The Ohio State project, however, did more to shape the content and method of the leadership research in recent times, and this outcome was not accidental: the framers of the ten-year project that began in l9“5 had that result in mind while designing their studies 33 (Stogdill and Coons, 1957. p. 7). The project sought to identify and catalogue all of a leader’s behavior which are related to his effectiveness and which were most important. One of the principal foci was the testing of hypotheses with reapect to the situational aspects affecting leader behavior (Shartle in Stogdill and Coons, 1957, p. l). The studies were structured to answer two basic questions: "(1) fipat does an indi- vidual do while he operates as a leader, and (2) up! does he go about what he does?” (Hemphill and Coons in Stogdill and Coons, 1957, p. 6). An instrument was developed as a preliminary questionnaire that contained 150 items (pared down from an initial collection of 1,790 items obtained from the researchers' personal experience, the leadership lit- erature and the work of two advanced university classes studying the subject). The instrument, called the Lea- der Behavior Description Questionnaire (or LBDQ), was subsequently revised by reducing the number of items from 150 to 130. After several years of study utilizing increasingly sophisticated techniques of data analysis, four cate- gories emerged to account for most of the variation in respondents' descriptions of leaders' behaviors. Table 2.1 identifies the four categories, or factors, as presented by Halpin and Winer (in Stogdill and Coons, 1957. p. “1). Factors III and IV were discarded, being consid- ered not important enough to significantly describe eleader behavior. Actually, not only the percent of common variance figures support the decision to drop 1 these two factors, but also a careful comparison of the descriptions of and items in each of the two eliminated categories reflects, at least in this re- viewers' opinion (with the advantage of the more ob- jective standpoint afforded by time), that the two could be incorporated into the ”consideration" and "initiating structure" categories (IV with I and III with 11). TABLE2.1 - Per Cent of Common Variance Accountefi for b Fo . tors to dill d oons Per Cent of Factor Factor Common Number. Deaignation Vap’anga I Consideration “9.6 II Initiating Structure 33.6 III Production Emphasis 9.8 IV e si vit So a1 wa eness) .210 "Consideration” refers to behavior indicative of "friendship, mutual trust, respect, and warmth in the relationship...it does not imply laxity in the . 35 performance of his duties." "Initiating structure" was described as indicating the degree to which the leader ”organizes and defines the relationship between himself and the members . . . 3' It is the definition ,of the role which he expects each member . . . to assume, and the @egree to whicEI endeavors to establish well-defined patterns of organization, channels of communication, and ways of getting jobs done. This factor probably represents a basic and unique func- tion of leadership. It is possible that other factors (including Consideration) may represent only facilitating means for accom- pl shing this end. (Ipid., pp. “2-“3) Consideration and initiating structure were con- sidered as discrete categories and not polar Opposites on a continuum. Thus, a leader would (and, from a personnel manager's perspective, hOpefully would) score high on both of these factors. Furthermore, this con- ceptualization represents an advance beyond the simple dichotomies of the earlier leadership research which limited the focus to studies of those leaders who prac- tice good human relations and those whose behaviors are inferior. An enormous number of leadership studies followed the Ohio State project, a large percentage of which used the same or similar constructs as well as the LBDQ or an equivalent. Aligned with the "consideration" .36 rubric are such concepts as harmony, equalitarian, group-oriented, person-oriented, Theory Y, expressive mode, influence, permissive, and even democratic and laissez-faire. Following the "initiation of structure" focus are guidance, authoritarian, task-oriented, pro- duction-oriented, Theory X, instrumental mode, control, restrictive. and autocratic (Berelson and Steiner, pp. 3““, 3“6, Stogdill, 197“, pp. 22, 27, 37“: Downton, 1973. p. 21: Larkin, 1976, p. 815: Coughlin, 1971, p. 15: Fiedler, 1967, p. 12). In Perrow's view, all of these related factors are "more or less compatible with the initiating structure and consideration dimen- sions discovered in the Ohio State studies, but most offered elaborations and recombinations of elabora- tions" (1972, p. 111). Unfortunately, reviewing the above research re- veals a tragic inability of the studies to hold up to the rigors of replication and methodological inspection. Many of the correlations reported were in- significant: the correlation between consi- deration and performance was better than that between initiating structure and per- formance (the latter was sometimes nega- tive): the research did not take into account the situations of the groups or the possibility of relations for consider- ation were the o posits of those pre- dicted . . . . Perrow, p. 112) Thus the past decade has seen leadership research 37 take another step, a move beyond the "two-variable” approach which characterized the field previously. However, the more recent investigations are still using consideration and initiation of structure as a base but eXploring variables that can intervene with them. One of the most promising of these research ef- forts in the view of Perrow and many others who have followed him is that of Fiedler. Building upon the work of Lewin and Lippitt and the Ohio State studies, ‘ he developed what he calls the "contingency theory” or ”contingency model," which ”postulates that the ef- fectiveness of a group is contingent upon the relation- ship between leadership style and the degree to which the group situation enables the leader to exert in- fluence" (Fiedler, 1967, p. 15). Fiedler discovered that group climates which are either highly favorable or highly unfavorable for the leader are situations in which a leader oriented to- ward the initiation of structure will be more succes- ful. However. in climates which are in between with respect to favorableness, a leader oriented toward a considerate style will function more effectively. Sit- uation favorableness refers to the extent to which the relationships between the leader and his followers are .33 positive. In such contexts the programming of tasks and the leader's position is well-established. Hence, the most effective leader is the one who provides straightforward task direction and facilitates the group's wbrk. Member relationships are stable and functional and thus take care of themselves. On the other hand, when relationships between the leader and group members are negative, tasks are not clear and the leader's position is questioned, giving him what Fiedler calls "weak position power.” In such a situation strong direction is required in order to be effective. A focus on interpersonal re- lationships would prove fruitless. If, though, situational favorableness is in be- tween, neither good nor bad, then considerate behavior is required. Strong direction in the sense of the in- itiation of structure, is neither necessary nor effec- tive. Thus, the current state of the leadership liter- ature is characterized by a focus upon the situational variables which intervene and with which the leader must cope. Other recent findings, for example, indi- cate that the size of a group, urban versus semirural environments, expectations, self-esteem and wishes of subordinates all influence how a leader behaves (Perrow, j 39 1972, p. 112). Stanley found that a factor called "incremental influence" is a possible moderator of a leader's behavior within his group. Incremental in- fluence refers to the leader's behavior that is over pand above what is required by his official position, or, more specifically, the combined amounts of expert and referent power the leader possesses. Expert power refers to the influence a person has due to his abil- ity to perform his function competently. Referent power indicates the ability of a leader to influence others on the basis of his relationships with them (1975, p. 3639-B). In another study Pandey reported that a leader's style and traits as well as the method of leader selection used in his recruitment all pro- duce joint effects on his behavior (1978, pp. 592- 593). The above overview of the most important develOp- ments in the field of leadership with respect to the theoretical position of this study, its subject matter, and its procedures provides a framework against which the more specific discussion of content and method can be most fully understood. The former will be considered first. . “O Qontept Baselpf the Study Spaulding and Haley in a 1955 study found a sig- nificantly high correlation between program success and leadership. The study, conducted in 188 Protes- tant churches, showed that the higher youth programs were rated on “3 criteria, the more likely youth were to rate their adult leaders as "effective,” as indi- cated in Table 2.2 (1955, p. 31). Respondents were interviewed with a questionnaire comprising both closed and open-ended items. The first part of the instrument contained “3 questions concerning the effectiveness of six different aspects of the church's program pertaining to youth. Re- spondents were to answer each question by checking one of five boxes on a Likert scale ranging from "very ef- fective“ to ”very ineffective.” Through assigning pos- itive and negative numerical values to each of the five response categories, a "program item score" was obtained for each subject. This score refers to the total score of each person's questionnaire as a measure of his opinion regarding the effectiveness of the en- tire church program for youth. The scores are grouped into six categories in the far left column of Table 2.2. Among other open-ended questions, the respon- dents were asked to indicate whether they felt adult “1 TABLE 2. - Program Item Scores as Related to Rating of Leaders (Spaulding and Haley; 1955, p. 31). Program Number Youth's Rat ng of Adult Leaders __§:;;e I ogeggIgg Effective Fair Ineffective (94$) (6%) (of) 120-159 1“1 11“ 18 9 (81¢) (13$) (6’) (67$) (2“5) (9;) “0’79 157 79 41 37 (50$) (26$) (205) °‘39 99 34 39 26 (3“!) (395) (27$) Less than 0 79 18 26 35 123$) (33$) (““1) 398 163 (598$)_ 12“1) _tl8$) leadership of youth in their church had been ef- fective, and, if so, why. When these responses were compared with those of the “3 items, a statistically significant (.05) correlation was discovered. As seen in Table 2.2, the higher youth programs were rated on the “3 criteria, the more the subjects gave their adult leaders an "effective” rating. Noting the Spaulding and Haley findings, Gamelin (1970) designed a research project ”with the express purpose of discovering what personality'traits, “2 competencies, motives, and other characteristics typ- ify effegtive adult leaders of church youth" (p. l). The project, sponsored by The Lilly Foundation, con- sisted of several descriptive studies of youth and denominational and other organizational adult leaders of youth. The first of these studies involved 1090 youth from five denominations, representing a broad theolo- gical spectrum (ranging from those considered "con- servative" to those considered more ”liberal," but without much representation in the "middle" of the theological continuum). Two hundred thirty-four United Methodist youth elected to the denomination's 100 youth work councils, 250 Mennonite youth consti- tuting 855 of a random national sample, 2“? Evangelical Covenant youth constituting 83% of a random national sample, 75 EpisOOpal youth from Ohio attending a sum- mer leadership camp and 28“ Lutheran Church-Missouri Synod youth attending volunteer camps throughout the U.S. formed the sample of 1090 young people whose opinions were investigated in late 1968 and 1969. The youth were asked to write endings for two incomplete sentences. The first sentence was "I especially like an adult youth leader who . . . ." The second sentence was phrased, "I especially dislike “3 an adult youth leader who . . . ." The youth cited an average of two characteristics they liked in their adult leaders and one or two they disliked. Gamelin discovered that their responses were able to be clas- sified into eight positive and eight negative cate- gories. The categories were comprehensive enough to include 98$ of the responses and discrete enough to allow reliable sorting. The categories were given trait names (Table 2.3) by the researchers. The rank order in which they are .given in Table 2.3 indicates the frequency with which the young peOple mentioned the characteristics. It should be noted that the column listing the disliked characteristics is not meant to be the Opposite of the traits indicated in the ”Likes" column. The categories are discrete, not the polar Opposites on a continuum. The Gamelin study of traits that youth like and dislike in their leaders initiated a good start in the scientific investigation of such characteristics in youth leadership. However, serious weaknesses exist in this study which limit the degree to which generaliza- tion is possible. The study made an important contri- bution, though, in identifying basic traits upon which another (including the present) research project, could build. an TABLE 2.3 - Traits Which Church Youth Like and Dislike in Their Leaders (Gameli . l970. p! 2) Rank 1 (D\l0\\l\¢'UN Likes Concerned and Encouraging Receptive and Communicative Understanding Lively Competent Helpful and Involved Mature and Secure Open-minded and Flexible Dislikes Domineering Patronizing Unrelating Stodgy Immature Disinterested Phony Distrustful The limitations of the Gamelin study are several, not the least of which is the orientation toward trait theory. However, as also noted above, the trait school, while having hit upon hard times, is still receiving some attention, and, moreover. the Gamelin traits raised the question in the present study as to whether some of these traits are connected with attendant be- haviors, e.g.. "Receptive and Communicative” which could be expressed "Communicates.” The fact that this question was answered in the affirmative in the second pilot study, described in Chapter III, indicated the value of the Gamelin study as a base. The Gamelin research was also limited by sampling bias due to differences in the groups studied (which, “5 to his credit, he noted in his report, p. “), sampling bias in the selection of the groups to be studied, and sampling bias within the instrument. With respect to the groups themselves, Gamelin notes that two of the groups were representative of the total pOpulation of youth in their respective denominations (due to random selection), but the other three groups constituted leadership types. Noting these differences, Gamelin reported that the representative youth indicated greater preference for adult leaders who are concerned and encouraging, understanding and lively, while dis- liking mostly those who are disinterested and dis- trustful. Leadership types of youth on the other hand expressed greater preference for adult leaders who are receptive and communicative, competent, mature and se- cure while expressing greater dislike for those who are domineering, patronizing and stodgy. Sampling bias stemming from the selection of the groups which were studied centers in the fact that there is no way to be certain that those groups are representative of youth in the United States generally or of any other sector outside of themselves. While it has been recognized that they are from a broad theological spectrum. this observation permits no con- fident generalization to any other groups in similar . “6 theological orientations. It has also been noted that there is little or no representation in what may be described as the "middle” Of this theological spectrum. Moreover, the scientific value of this construct (a ’theological spectrum) may be questioned due to the fact that virtually every denomination (including those in the Gamelin study) has its own internal theological spectrum. For example, while in some respects the United Methodist denomination may be placed on the "more liberal" and of the spectrum. a sizable number of Methodists would not classify themselves as "lib- eral" to any degree. Sampling bias within the instrument is present due to the type of design employed. Sentence comple- tion items have a number of strengths (one of which, in a study such as Gamelin's, especially for the pur- poses of this research, is to generate information that can be explored in greater depth with a more rigorous design), but they also have important limi- tations. One of the most critical limitations is the inability to ascertain to what degree a response to an item applies to any other persons in the sample other than the one responding with that particular statement and to others with similar (yary similar) responses. In such cases one cannot be sure that if another respondent had thought of any given completion state- ment he or she would have used that one rather than the one he or she gave. Thus the Gamelin study has provided a foundation upon which to build. It has identified eight posi- tive and eight negative traits which church youth like and dislike in their adult leaders. The limitations noted were used as guidelines in designing the present study, as well as the pilot studies, the details of which are presented in Chapter III. About the same time the Gamelin study was being undertaken, Ward and Harmon of Michigan State univer- sity were investigating the values of youth in a study sponsored by YFC. Their purpose was to obtain 1) a clear picture of the outlook and values of today's youth in terms of certain criti- cal aspects Of life values to which YFC's program particularly attempts to relate, 2) an investigation of the probable conse- quences of using a teen-to-teen approach to expansion, and 3) a comparison between the youth now related to YFC and those with whom YFC has not yet made contact (1970, p. l). The outlook of the adolescents studied was spec- ified in terms of five major aspects of life values, namely, education, religion, patriotism, morality and purpose. Each of the aspects was scored on the basis of responses on a Likert scale of items relating to “8 that aspect. The instrument, a twenty-five item question- naire, was designed "to identify the persons whose influence constitutes the source or major support for each element in the person's outlook" (p. 1). Each youth queried was asked to indicate the degree to which he agreed with a stated position on a given issue. He or she was then asked to select from five given peOple (mother, father, best friend, favorite teacher, religious leader) the one who would most likely agree with him on each of the twenty-five subjects. Ward and Harmon discovered that these five per- sons constitute sources of a high degree of influence upon youth, even though each is a source in a dif- ferent area. For example, while identification with the Opinions of peers (best friend) is predominant in most categories, religious leaders stand out as having a high degree of influence in matters pertaining to religion (1970, pp. 2-3). Since these five persons are sources of a high degree of influence upon youth, it was decided to in- corporate this aspect of the Ward and Harmon research into the present study. However, in the new investi- gation, "favorite teacher” and "religious leader" “9 were combined in the category of "adult leader” and "best friend“ was broadened into the redesignated category of ”a person about my age (peer)," in the attempt to retain the adult leader and peer emphases 'but, at the same time, to focus more specifically , upon the aspects of leadership. Method Base of the Study Because of the content decisions made as indi- cated in the preceding section of this chapter, particularly regarding the limitations of the Gamelin study, it was considered necessary that this study be descriptive in nature. In order to comprehend the factors youth most desire and least want in their leaders, it is necessary to ask them, but in such a way as to be able to generalize as widely as possible. A first step toward this generalization was taken by building upon the start Gamelin made in his identification of the eight positive and eight nega- tive factors. Those factors, their behavioral mani- festations, and other behaviors exhibited by leaders, were the subject of focus in this study. It was determined at the outset, however, that the Gamelin factors would be subjected to examination to see whether they are still important to youth today. .50 This examination took the form of the second pilot study which will be described in Chapter III. It was felt necessary to determine whether any changes in the thinking of youth regarding leadership terminology have occured since the Gamelin study. No subsequent re- search has explored this possibility. A The area of research concerned with the research questions in this study is that of attitude measure- ment. According to the taxonomy develOped by Mayhew (in Payne, 197“, pp. 230, 232), the aspect of attitude measurement here being focused on is youth Opinion. The opinion being asked for is what leader behaviors are most desirable and least desirable, and how they are ranked in the minds of the young people. A standard practice within behavioral science today with regard to attitude measurement is the utilization of the method of self-report as a means of obtaining data. Since self-report offers the advan- tages of ease of administration and scoring as well as low cost (Borg and Call, 1971, p. 178) and, when used with a form of the closed question, provides the ad- vantage of obtaining responses on all items germane to the research questions, it was decided to employ self- report as the framework for obtaining the needed in- formation (see also Scott in Lindzey and Aronson, 1968, 51 p. 211). There are dangers in using the self-report method, and these dangers have been identified by Borg and Call as three different types of response sets. A discussion of these sets and how they were controlled will be undertaken in the next chapter. Following Scott, it was determined to use a form of the multiple choice kind of forced choice in the closed question format (Scott in Lindzey and Aronson, p. 213). This form of instrumentation, as any form, has a number of disadvantages, which will be discussed in Chapter III, but it is considered here that the dis- advantages are outweighed by the advantages. More- over, the design to be discussed in the third chapter has incorporated procedures for mitigating these dis- advantages. Mayhew (in Payne, pp. 232-233) has further noted the importance of unambiguous phraseology in the in- strument items and that a carefully worded instrument can help achieve a high degree of reliability. In compliance with this caution, the positive and negative factors were worded in terms used in the common lan- guage of contemporary youth. Of special help in this regard were the second and third pilot studies as well as conversations with teenagers in the l“ to 18 year- old age range. Further discussion of the wording of 52 instrument terms and other matters pertaining to re- liability and validity will be undertaken in the next chapter. With this help from the literature, the next step was to develop the plan of approach in Order to obtain the data needed. It is to the matter of re- search design that Chapter III addresses itself. CHAPTER III THE DESIGN The task of this research project was to an- _swer questions concerning whether young people perceive leadership behaviors differently on any kind of con- sistent basis with reapect to their paar and adult leaders. and in what ways the youth's ranking of these behaviors for adult leader compares and con- trasts with behaviors of their parents. The sub- jects were questioned as to what degree the behaviors were perceived as being true of their parents in con- trast to the questions concerning paar and adult leaders in which cases the youth responded as to the degree the behaviors are considered important (in the positive instances) and serious (in the negative instances). The Plan gt_the Study Professional YFC staff peOple in the nine re- gions covering the continental United States surveyed groups of high school youth fourteen through eighteen years of age. The groups were obtained from public 53 5“ and private schools, churches and YFC youth groups. Since random selection was not possible, the data-gatherers tried to Obtain as many youth as they could from each region. The goal was to have at least 50 subjects in each cell. The total number in the sample was 1536. The survey consisted of the subjects' complet- ing a four-page questionnaire (APPENDIX C) which took an average of 11 minutes. For each of 22 leader be- haviors (11 positive and 11 negative) the subjects were to circle a number on a scale of O - “ which would indicate how important they perceived the be- haviors to be for their leaders and how true the acts were seen to be of their parents. In this study validity and reliability were considered crucial, the former with regard to both the design and instrument, and the latter pertaining especially to the instrument. With reSpect to valid- ity, both internal and external validity were con- sidered in the planning of the study. Internal Validity Concerning the internal validity of the design, rival hypotheses pertaining to history, maturation, testing. instrumentation, and statistical regression 55 were not operative due to the nature of the plan. Those factors. which function either independently or in a combination to produce varying degrees of internal invalidity from these sources, are absent in the design of the study. Mortality Mortality, however, was a concern, though to a limited degree in a special way. The data were obtained from intact groups and were accumulated on a single occasion. This prOcedure resulted in a low loss of respondents. While law requires provid- ing students in public schools the Option of electing not to participate in any activity being done under the auspices of a religious organization (such as YFC), most members of the groups surveyed completed the questionnaire. Those who did not fill in the questionnaire were few in number. The larger and more applicable problem of sample selection will be further discussed below. Since sample selection was done on a conve- nience basis according to the location of YFC per- sonnel throughout the continental U.S.A.. and since, therefore, it was necessary to obtain as many re- pondents as possible from each of the subdivisions 56 of the population (e.g., urban, voluntary, Youth Guidance), carefully constructed procedures were followed in order to obtain the most subjects (see APPENDIX E). To illustrate, data-gatherers were ‘careful to administer the questionnaire on an ordi- nary day (rather than, for example, on a day prior to a three-day weekend) where absenteeism was at a minumum, and at an apprOpriate time during the day (which was established on an appointment basis with the school principal ahead of time), also described in the instructions to data-gatherers in APPENDIX E. A basic principle underlying the issue of mor- tality gave rise to a special way in which this source of invalidity was potentially Operative. Some mortality did occur through certain respondents' failing to complete certain items on the instrument. For example, it occurred, as reported on the com- puter printout where some respondents omitted certain items in the category of father. Provision for anonymity, indication of the importance of the study, explanation that there were no right or wrong answers and the request to complete all items, were proce- dures used to obtain as little item mortality as possible (see APPENDIX E). This subject will be discussed further in the sections below which deal 57 with response sets and external validity and in Chapter IV. Selection Bias More,serious was the possibility of selection bias. This study was designed as a nationwide pro- 'ject. While it would have been ideal to be able to use random sampling in the study in order to assure complete representativeness and the limitation of bias, time and money were constraints that prohib- ited the random selection of subjects. However, since scientifically valid and reliable results were a high priority, the design had to be rigorous and sound. Following Jones (1973, pp. 73'7“), it was determined that accurate results would still be achieved through obtaining as many young peOple as possible in the sample from as many different parts of the nation as could be reached. YFC, learning of the purpose and intent of this study, indicated that it paralleled needs they had and offered to staff and fund the project on a national ‘basis. YFC staffing involved the use of regional and local personnel in the data gathering process. The or- ganization Operates throughout the United States in the nine regions described in Chapter I. Each of the nine . 5 8 has a regional director. YFC does not, though, have staff pOOple in every part of the country, nor does it have access to every area and every type of sub- group. _ Thus, the sampling problems raised the rival hy- pothesis of selection bias. The question appeared: "would different groups give different answers?” However, at second look, selection bias does not ap- pear to be as formidable a threat to validity as at first thought, for at least three reasons. These three reasons, while not eliminating selection bias, serve to control its effects. (The term, "control its effects," is here used to refer to the indication of the degree to which the factor is functioning. It is not used with the unrealistic thought that there would be some possible way to eliminate the effects of the factor (selection bias) under the given conditions such as are applying in this study.) First, the study was conducted with a large number of respondents (see Jones, pp. 73-7“: Borg and Call, p. 123). Second, the study contained a wide variety of groupings (see Jones, pp. 73-7“). Most different types of youth were repre- sented, for example, urban, suburban, rural, and ”de- linquent" and "typicalf The results obtained enabled the viewing of a wide variety of contrasts. Third, 59 Douvan and Adelson found that "clear regional dif- ferences exist only in highly specific activities which depend on climate or special geographic fea- tures. Adolescents in the South do not know winter ,sports: those in the urbanized East report active lei- sure centered on camping or field interests less often” than youth in the West (1966, 310-312). Thus, these reasons provided a high degree of confidence that existing Opinions would be obtained. This con- clusion was supported statistically in several ways, . for example, in that little difference across regions was found in a multivariate analysis of regional difference scores. Observer Bias Another factor which threatened the internal va- lidity of the study was observer bias. Mitigation of this factor was an important goal of the training sessions for the data-gatherers and their trainers. The trainers of the data-gatherers were them- selves trained at a meeting during a national con- ference held in Miami on February 25, 1979. These pOOple constituted the members of the Research and De- velopment Committee who were the heads of the nine regional YFC units. The training session was 60 conducted on the basis of the forms which appear in APPENDIX D and APPENDIX E. The presentation began with a statement review- ing the purpose of the research (as indicated in item #1 of the INSTRUCTIONS FOR DATA-GATHERERS sheet which appears in APPENDIX E) which had been previously ex- ‘plained to this committee in a meeting the preceding fall in Elburn, Illinois on September 27, 1978. Care was taken to avoid indicating specific expectations which could result in the committee members developing a bias that might influence their direction-giving to the data-gatherers they would train and to the youth to whom they would give the instrument (the members were to both train others and collect data themselves). It was indicated that this is descriptive research: the only interest is in what la, not in what should be or .nhy. The explanation indicated what would be learned, specifically what the instrument was capable of showing. COpies of the instrument were distributed. Opportunity was provided for any changes the committee wanted to suggest in any of the forms or methods. Some suggestions were made but only one was needed for implementation (the addition of a space for YFC region on the GROUP DESCRIPTION SHEET in APPENDIX E). The writer explained how the other suggestions 61 would conflict with established procedures of scien- tific research. The committee appeared to appre- ciate this involvement with the development of the study. This procedure provided practical help, and it was a useful public relations procedure as well, motivating interest and participation in the study.* 1 Motivation was also stimulated by the writer's identification of the study with needs expressed by the committee members. YFC is engaged in a shift to- ward more effective selection and use of paar leaders among the youth. Thus, the probable benefits of the study were apparent. Other possible sources of observer bias were also worked on, including the importance of indicating to the respondents that their honest responses were what was desired, that there were no "right" (or even pre- ferred) or ”wrong" (or even undesired) answers. Other ways of controlling observer bias were incorporated through the development of the instrument, namely, not structuring it so it would contain leading questions *The writer learned how important motivation on the part of the data-gatherers and their trainers is in a wide-spread study, for even though they may be doing such research in the employ of a sponsoring organiza- tion, it was soon seen that money and pressure from superiors are not adequate motivators, especially when the workers are constrained by a heavy work load in other areas of their job. 62 which would give the subjects clues as to a preferred response, not permitting observers to record behaviors they would like to see or not to see, not permitting observers the Opportunity to draw inferences from subjectsNresponses, and not including items that would threaten, embarrass, or annoy reapondents (Borg and Call, 1971, p. 105). Also discussed at the Miami meeting were what groups to survey (e.g., voluntary, urban, church- related) and how many people were needed in each of these cells (they were asked to obtain at least 50 subjects per cell). A realistic time frame was dis- cussed in which the writer and committee members com- mitted themselves to certain responsibilities. The committee members agreed to select and train data- gatherers, obtain their own data and the others' data, and bring the data back to the YFC home office in Illinois by April 2“.* The specific procedures that would be used in data gathering were discussed: the importance of each observer's functioning according to 4*Due to many factors, such as work load, difficulty obtaining access to some schools, cancellation of the April 2“ meeting and forgetfulness, the last of the data were not returned until early June, in spite of a system of communication through biweekly contacts made by phone and letter from the home office. At the end, the writer had to make a number of phone calls to one particular region. 63 the planned methods was emphasized. Response Sets Before proceeding to the subject of external va- tlidity, one more statement should be made with regard to internal validity. As indicated in Chapter II, 'Borg and Gall (p. 178) have shown that there are dan- gers in using the self-report method of data-gathering, and these dangers have been identified as three dif- ferent types of response sets in the minds of the re- spondents. These sets threaten to give support to the rival hypothesis which may be stated in the form of the question, ”How do you know that the tendency of subjects to make their choices on the basis of three major types of response sets has not accounted for the data you have received instead of accurate indications of their actual opinions?" The set for social desirability was dealt with through anonymity. Sex and age were the only identi- fying factors asked of the respondents in the study. The other categories (for example, urban. suburban rural, and ”average" or Youth Guidance) were indicated by the data-gatherer on the GROUP DESCRIPTION SHEET. The limitation of choices (the selection of a number on a Likert scale for each item on the instrument) was . 6“ also a help in mitigating the concern to present one- self in a favorable light. As no other gain existed to encourage faking (for example, the possibility of a higher grade), it was considered that the set for isocial desirability was contained within allowable limits. A The set for aoquiecence was reduced by the in- structions that there are no true or hOped for re- sponses. Thus, this set to respond "true," regardless of an inventory item's content, is not a significant factor in this study. ’ The set for deviance also did not produce any significantly negative effect in this study. There was no reason to believe that there was any general hostility in the respondents as a whole which inclined them to want to give answers in this mentality. Borg and Call have advised that if a researcher "has good reason to believe that his research sample will fake or give atypical answers, then a self-report inventory should not be selected” (1971, p. 178). In the absence of such belief, and as no apparent gain was considered likely to be present to reward such deviance, it was concluded that the self-report method was suitable for this study. This conclusion was supported by the data which showed a wide diversity of responses. 65 External Validity With regard to external validity of the design, a reaction to measurement procedures may seriously affect a study (Campbell and Stanely, 1963, p. 6). A test situation can stir negative affect toward test- ing and thus cause within the respondent his or her negative motivations toward behavior different from that of youth in general. To mitigate such reactive effects, the questionnaire was designed in the form of Likert-scaled items in which opinions (not information recall to be graded right or wrong) were given. In addition, the observers gave precise verbal instruc- tions explaining that there were no "right" or "wrong" answers being called for on this questionnaire. Since many people like to be asked for their Opinions in non- threatening subject areas, particularly where anonymity is provided, the data-gathering experience was consid- ered a positive one for the respondents, and that awareness was a guiding principle in the design. A greater threat to external validity was the selection bias resulting from the fact that random sampling was not possible in this research project. However. for the reasons indicated in the discussion of the effect of selection bias on the internal validity of the design, the effect of selection on external 66 validity was not considered to be so adverse as to impugn the study. Caution is in order regarding gen- eralizations of the conclusions. Claims are not being made herein pertaining to all youth in the United States, even within the continental borders. It is possible to draw conclusions with a reasonable degree or confidence in regard to groups with similar char- acteristics. This subject will be further discussed in Chapter V. A ’s nt fo h St d Concerning the validity and reliability of the instrument, these matters were attended to in several ways. Since the latter is a prerequisite of the for- mer. care was taken, therefore, to provide for maxi- mum reliability. Reliability Instrument reliability was strengthened by using a closed question form for the instrument (Scott in Lindzey and Aronson, pp. 210-212): establishing rapport with the respondents in such a way that it was clear to them that their frankness was truly desired (Cronbach in Payne, 197“, p. 120): conducting the data collec- tion at a time when contextual fluctuations were at a minumum (Adkins in Payne, 197“, p. 19“): and keeping 67 items relatively homogenous and in the middle range of difficulty (Ebel, 1972, pp. “27, 567: Ebel in Payne, 197“, pp. 262 ff.). The reliability of the instrument was also strengthened by dealing with the three major causes of unreliability in the following manner (Ebel, 1972, A p. “09). One major factor which negatively affects reliability is the inappropriateness of the task. It was, therefore, a major concern in designing the in- strument to attend to making sure that each term, - especially with respect to the questionnaire items, was clearly understood by the respondents. In order to provide this clarity and to update the Gamelin re- search, a second pilot study was undertaken. The Saapnd Pilot Study The second pilot was conducted to find out what words youth across the country use in describing lea- dership. YFC regional directors interviewed two dif- ferent types of young peOple. They were to interview the youth in each of three major sections of the con- tinental United States, West, Midwest and East. In each section one group of young peOple was to be inter- viewed who could be described as "average." The other was to be a Youth Guidance group. The age range of . 68 the youth interviewed was 1“ through 18, the same as in the main study. The youth were to be interviewed in groups, as would be done in the main project, and the size of the groups would range from three to ten in number; The interviews were audio recorded, transcribed, and assessed. The question was whether or not the Gamelin terminology could still be used or whether certain terms would have to be changed. The interview instrument, instructions and cover letter for the data-gatherers (interviewers) can be seen in APPENDIX B. The second pilot was helpful in the develOpment of an instrument that would be as reliable and valid as possible. Certain changes in wording were made to- gether with the addition of six new items which can be seen by comparing the instrument (APPENDIX C) with the instrument used in the first pilot study (APPENDIX A). With the instrument develOped through the re- sources provided in the review of the precedent research (Chapter II), the sources identified in this chapter, and the confirmational insights from the se- cond pilot study, it remained necessary to determine whether the questionnaire was workable in a setting similar to which it would be used in the main study. For that determination a third pilot was utilized. . 69 The Third Pilot Study The third pilot study took place at Christ Church of Oak Brook, in Oak Brook, Illinois On Feb- ruary 20, 1979. five days prior to the Miami meeting. The writer trained the data-gatherers in the same man- ner he planned to have the data-gatherers trained for the main study. He first trained the Youth Director of the church who in turn trained the person who would be administering the questionnaire to the youth in their regular Tuesday evening group meeting. The writer observed the Youth Director's training of the data-gatherer and the data-gatherer's work with the youth group. The time taken in training and in ad- ministration of the questionnaire was noted. The third pilot study confirmed that the questionnaire was usable. Thus, the first major factor negatively affecting reliability, inapprOpriateness of the task, was con- trolled. The second and third pilot studies verified the list of behaviors which would be used on the in- strument and which would serve to answer the research questions. These two pilot studies also assured that the items were worded in language the target pOpula- tion uses and understands. Second, reliability is also affected by human 7O factors such as fatigue. All data were collected, therefore, during one fifteen minute period, usually in the morning when the subjects could be assumed to be alert. Mornings were preferred because the youth »were awake long enough to be mentally alert and yet not having experienced a long period of sitting and I listening or a period of time in the day following a meal, for example, when they would have been less alert. The third factor which causes unreliability, in adition to inapprOpriateness of task and fatigue, may be described as inconsistency and nonobjectivity of the observer. This factor was taken into account by making the instrument highly structured (Mayhew, in Payne, 197“, p. 233). The closed question form re- stricts variable and subjective elements entering the study via the observer. A reliability analysis of the eight scales (e.g., the 11 positive behaviors for padr constituted scale 1: the 11 positive behaviors for adult constituted scale 3) was done using the Cronbach alpha test. Con- sidering .80 or above as good (i.e., that 805 or more of the variation in the scales was due to the items themselves rather than to error), all eight of the scales were seen to be reliable. The lowest 71 coefficient was .83306 for the positive scale of padr. The highest was .92982 for the negative scale of adult. . Validity The validity of the instrument was provided for -by utilizing the preceding procedures for producing the highest degree of reliability possible and by keeping the instrument items relevant to the research questions (Ebel, 1972, p. ““8). Exercising much cau- ‘tion in phrasing the items in clear language which was understandable to the respondents was an important help in assuring validity. One further matter, which also affects content validity, needs to be discussed concerning the instru- ment. The closed question format has both advantages and disadvantages. While the former outweigh the lat- ter because of the reasons cited throughout the dis- cussion of the design of the study in this chapter, the rationale for not shifting the weight toward the disadvantages should also be indicated. Scott (in Lindzey and Aronson, 1968, pp. 210-211) claims that the disadvantages are as follows: the closed question form (1) suggests particular answers which might be uncritically accepted by acquiescent respondents, (2) 72 doesn't permit the researcher to find out whether the respondents have no attitude toward the subject in question. (3) doesn't facilitate the discovery of at- titudes not anticipated a priori, and (“) may inhibit observer-respondent rapport by inserting unnatural and disinteresting factors into their relationship. Numbers (1) and (“) have been dealt with in the above discussion in this chapter. Items (1) and (3) have also been taken into consideration through the use of the Gamelin research as a base. While the Gamelin study does not permit generalization on any of its 16 items, it does, through the method used to ob- tain the data it procured, permit a rather high degree of confidence to prevail concerning the attainment of the thinking of youth regarding these subjects. More- over, the results of the second pilot study supplied leadership behaviors that youth from the Eastern, Mid- western and Western continental United States perceive to be both attractive and repulsive. It is noteworthy that all 16 of Gamelin's aspects of leadership (albeit their behavioral manifestations) were sustained and only three positive and three negative behaviors were added that did not appear in Gamelin's original study. While the Gamelin research suggests that the 73 issue raised in (2) is unlikely, that study does not preclude its possibility, nor do any of the pilot studies undertaken with this research project in con- junction with the main study. However. it was assumed for the present study that the respondents do have an Opinion with regard to this subject matter, even though that opinion may not be conscious and articu- lated in a cogent manner. This assumption was cor- roborated by the completion of most of the items by every respondent in the first pilot, by the enthusiasm and extent of the discussions on the tapes of the se- cond pilot, and by the completion of most of the items on the questionnaires in the third pilot and main studies. These reasons, then, combined with the aforementioned advantages of the closed question form for the instrument, led to the selection of this meth- odology for the study. Instrument validity is supported by the results of a multivariate analysis of the eight scales. The correlation of tatuar positive and negative scales, for example, is -.“18“l7. The tendency to score high on the positive scale and low on the negative scale strengthens confidence in the validity of the posi- tive and negative scale of the instrument. The foregoing has described the design the study 7“ employed. The data procured by following this plan will be discussed in the next chapter. CHAPTER IV THE FINDINGS The study sought the answers to five questions. This chapter reports what was discovered. The ques- tions asked whether young people view any of the 11 positive and 11 negative behaviors as more important on any kind of consistent basis with respect to their paar and adult leaders. Also asked was an indication of what ways the ranking of the behaviors for adult leader were similar and dissimilar with the behaviors seen by young peOple in their mothers and fathers. Following the presenta- tion of the data, a summary of topical generalizations concludes the chapter. Research Questions and Pertinent Data The questions stated in Chapter I guided the analy- sis of the data. For convenience they will be restated at the beginning of each section. Research Question #1 The first question asked, "Do young people view any of the 11 positive and 11 negative behaviors as more 75 76 important on any consistent basis with reSpect to their .pggx leaders?" The findings show that some behaviors are, indeed, identified as having more value on certain consistent bases. The differences noted in that which follows are , with respect to behaviors ranked plus or minus four or more places. The selection of the number four is arbi- trary, though not without logic. The rationale for less than four places as the cut-off is that consider- able interchangability is seen to occur among items within one, two or three positions of each other. Four or more indicates more contrast and it appears less fre- quently. Further, four or more places constitutes a spread of more than 1/3 of the 11 scale positions. The cut-off at less than four was used consistently in all cases. Moreover. since the sample is large almost all of the scores were statistically significant to at least the .01 level. Thus the following discussion will focus on those relationships which appear to yield the great- est practical significance. The subject of statisti- cal significance will be considered further in connection with Research Question #3. Table #.1 shows that while some behaviors are more important than others, all are considered important 77 TABLE 9.1 - National Mean Scores and Ranking by _I 7 ' Categories Egg; Adult Moths; Fatbg; 0 3.993 c D 3.565 c I 3.189 i I 3.21? i A 3.350 c J 3.991 c P 3.139 c 8 3.115 1 J 3.325 c A 3.968 c B 3.019 1 C 2.888 c P 3.296 c 3 3.379 1 C 3.017 c 8 2.883 1 8 3.207 r 3.399 D 3.011 a 2.853 3 3.183 8 3.293 A 2.998 P 2.688 I 3.178 I 3.279 8 2.996 D 2.635 I 3.089 C 3.226 H 2.911 A 2.619 I 3.063 K 3.175 J 2.887 J 2.530 C 2.897 I 3.192 I 2.750 I 2.982 0 2.665 O 2.762 0 2.692 G 2.078 U 3.338 U 3.318 R 2.119 R 2.092 I 3.181 I 3.297 V 1.71? V 1.832 I 3.168 I 3.285 I 1.663 I 1.676 I 3.161 0 3.289 0 1.985 0 1.379 0 3.197 L 3.267 0 1.383 T 1.373 P 3.112 8 3.198 L 1.292 O 1.327 I 3.106 P 3.198 T 1.230 I 1.179 8 3.100 T 3.099 P 1.228 L 1.109 0 2.961 0 3.068 I 1.137 8 1.109 R 2,759 R 2.897 S 1.129 P 1.096 V 2.626 V 2.792 U .975 U 1.018 c - consideration 1 . initiation of Structure Item Sym- l 991 .Behgyior 1 A Communicates 2 I Doesn't relate to young 2 3 Displays adequate know- people ledge and ability 3 N Doesn't show concern for 3 C Lets young peoole (me) young people (me) take responsibility 9 0 Doesn't trust young people 9 D Listens (me) 5 E Organizes well 5 P Favors some over others 6 P Seeks to help when 6 Q Forces ideas on young needed peeple (me) 7 0 Shares own shortcomings 7 R Gets upset when things and problems don't go right 8 I Shows sense of humor 8 S Looks down on young 9 I Tries new ideas--open people (me) 10 J Understands (my) con- .9 T Puts own interests ahead cerns of young people or group 11 K Uses firmness when 10 U Says one thing, but does necessary another-~dishonest 11 V Won't change--old 1 Doesn't follow through-- fashioned H dishonest 78 (with respect to the positive acts) and serious (with respect to the negative). For example, the ninth-ranked behavior for 222; leader is “uses firm- ness when necessary” (K). Though ranked ninth, the ‘national mean for that item is 3.063, which, on a _scale of 0 - 9, is high. The four top-ranked positive behaviors are "listens” (D), "communicates” (A), "understands concerns of young people” (J) and ”seeks to help when needed” (F); these items are all concerned with consideration. The last-ranked positive behavior is "shares own shortcomings and problems” (C). With respect to the 11 negative behaviors, hypocrisy, "says one thing, but does another--dis- honest” (U), is ranked first (most serious) for Egg; leaders. The two last-ranked negative behav- iors, mentioned here, due to their consistent reap- pearance in these positions, are "gets upset when things don't go right” (R) and ”won't change--old- fashioned" (V). Essentially the same results obtain when the demographic variables are isolated, as can be ob- served from the data displayed on Table 9.2, graph- ically portraying the comparisons the research question eXplored. Care must be taken in the contrasts made 79 Father Zun.mcn.<0p. 2ma.mca.HH 2mm. wcn. pp 2mn.xcn.ww zmn.wswnww oasem.cwc. a} r.» I _ 0: L M oedema u m “MUD a 3:3 I I... .o w m .. «m J m. oofioy u oz m oosood u no soapsnaapmdoIIsOa>msom naswaoomoq go msoapnoopom zusow I m.d mqm21: UTJ O F' wragx: gDsz 2 vs r*Io on A) C "U C: - 1.0.. .8. .6. .m,’ .2. on I: vat zr' crogprsc O 9’ (D an .b———————-——-———-—-—>——-—— FOB- Neg. P08. Neg. P08. . eg. 08. eg. Male Female Male Female MOTHER FATHER 109 TABLE 4.9 - Youth Parceptions of Mother and Fatger Leadership Behavior-- : ': :1 'z b _; : _':n:l; .° ; ; Mother Father ugle Zumale Mulu Eem§1§__ P 3.161 c K 3.31? i K 3.11? i K 3.344 1 K 3.085 i P 3.128 c B 3.059 i B 3.193 1 D 3.042 c B 3.042 1 C 2.867 c H 2.953 c A 3.027 c C 3.037 c E 2.848 1 E 2.951 1 B 3.010 D 2.981- H 2.733 C 2.918 C 3.001 A 2.978 F 2.7 5 P 2.638 E 2.953 H 2.969 A 2.686 D 2.604 J 2.872 E 2.951 D 2.669 A 2.545 H 2.871 J 2.915 I 2.553 J 2.319 I 2.771 I 2.739 J 2.547 I 2. 07 O 2.637 0 2.655 0 2.183 G 1.978 R 2.145 R 2.087 R 2.141 R 2.029 V 1.746 V 1.691 V 1.803 V 1.87? M 1.71? M 1.387 M 1.69? M 1.657 0 1. 41 0 l. 26 T 1.48? 0 1.310 Q 1. 33 Q 1.319 0 1.449 T 1.241 T 1.332 P 1.139 Q 1.444 Q 1.183 L 1.3 a L 1.124 N 1.263 N 1.068 P 1.29 T 1.081 L 1.241 P .981 N 1.221 N 1.018 S 1.211 S .970 S 1.20? S 1.015 P 1.192 L .956 U 1.046 U . .873 U 1.135 U .878 c = consideration 1 = initiation of structure Item Sym- g bol Behavior 1 A Communicates 2 M Doesn't relate to young 2 B Displays adequate know- people ledge and ability 3 N Doesn't show concern for 3 _ C Lets young people (me) young people (me) ‘ take reaponsibility 4 0 Doesn't trust young people 4 D Listens (me) 5 E Organizes well 5 P Favors some over others 6 F Seeks to help when 6 Q Forces ideas on young needed people (me) - 7 G Shares own shortcomings 7 R Gets upset when things and problems don't go right 8 H Shows sense of humor 8 S Looks down on young 9 I Tries new ideas-~0pen people (me) 10 J Understands (my) con- 9 T Puts own interests ahead cerns of young people or group 11 X Uses firmness when 10 U Says one thing, but does necessary another-~dishonest 11 V Won't change--old l L Doesn't follow through-- fashioned dishonest 110 did score the tOp positive behavior, "uses firmness when necessary” (K) notably higher than did males. The two genders differed most in the ranking of the top four behaviors. The males' selection included three types of'consideration while the females' contained two types of initiation of structure, one of which, "uses firmness when necessary” (K), was top-ranked as in the total sample. Youth Guidupue und "0thep" Responses As reflected in Table 4.2, these two subgroups differed more than any of the other demographic vari- ables. Youth Guidance respondents ranked "shows sense of humor” (H) third in contrast with ”other” subjects who ranked this behavior seventh and with the national sample where it was ranked eighth for ugpuup. The scores of all three, however, are within one standard deviation from the mean of the scale. By comparison, Youth Guidance ranked this act ninth, while ”other” subjects and the national sample ranked it tenth for 52311 leaders. The scores in this latter category are not notably differentiated. Youth Guidance respondents ranked ”displays ade- quate knowledge and ability" (B) seventh for pupugp in contrast to third for "other" young people and third lll nationally. The rankings were similar for uuulp leader. ”Other” youth ranked "lets me take responsibility for important tasks” (C) first for gupuup with a mean of 3.057.‘ (See Table 4.10.) The national sample con- trasts with this value by ranking the behavior fourth with a mean of 3.017 and with Youth Guidance subjects who ranked it eighth with a mean of 3.030. Here is another example of the importance of checking the mean along with the ranking. A difference of seven places in rank appears to be quite large, and is not unimpor- tant, but a mean difference of .027 between the first and eighth ranked behaviors mitigates that difference. With respect to uuulp leaders the ranking and rating is similar for the national sample, Youth Guidance and "other” young peOple. Youth Guidance subjects split their top four choices with regard to considerate and structural be- haviors of uguugp. The first and fourth ranked be- haviors are types of initiation of structure and the second and third are types of consideration. The "other” subjects' tOp four are also split evenly, though with the top two being types of consideration. For uuulp leaders both Youth Guidance and ”other” respondents' top three were considerate with the 1L12 TABLE 4.10- Youth Perceptions of Mother and Father Leadership Behavior-Mean R tem of U ban and Volun o Guid d ”Other” Mother Father Youth Youth "Otpep' Quiduugg ”Otpup' K 3.313 1 C 3.057 c K 3.050 i K 3.125 1 P 3.181 c P 3.035 c H 2.906 c 8 2.899 1 H 3.139 c B 3.035 1 P 2.829 c C 2.872 c a 3.122 1 x 3.034 1 B 2.829 i a 2.862 1 A 3.120 D 2.988 D 2.77? H 2.813 D 3.080 8 2.920 C 2.755 D 2.696 B 3.075 H 2.860 A 2.739 P 2.587 C 3.030 A 2.837 ' J 2.734 A 2.392 J 2.988 J 2.655 E 2.724 I 2.392 I 2.916 I 2. 32 I 2.671 J 2.387 6 2.820 G 2. 1 G 2.281 C 1.938 a 2.264 a 2.126 R 2.123 , a 2.075 I 2.025 V 1.770 I 2.080 V 1.962 0 1.982 H 1.721 V 2.014 H 1.825 V 1.939 0 1.517 N 1.914 T 1.662 N 1.866 Q 1.333 0 1.899 0 1.658 L 1.788 N 1.291 T 1.814 N 1.312 P 1.783 T 1.26? L 1.714 Q 1. 38 T 1.782 P 1.256 U 1.65? L 1.412 S 1.728 L 1.161 P 1.616 P 1.215 Q 1.646 8 1.034 Q 1.468 8 1.162 U 1.582 U .966 S 1.420 U.1.125 c = consideration i = initiation of structure Item Sym- # bol Behavior 1 A Communicates 2 M Doesn't relate to young 2 B Displays adequate know- people ledge and ability 3 N Doesn't show concern for 3 C Lets young people (me) young people (me) take responsibility 4 0 Doesn't trust young people 4 D Listens (me) 5 E Organizes well 5 P Favors some over others 6 F Seeks to help when 6 Q Forces ideas on young needed people (me) 7 0 Shares own shortcomings 7 R Gets upset when things and problems don't go right 8 H Shows sense of humor 8 8 Looks down on young 9 I Tries new ideas--open people (me) 10 J Understands (my) con- 9 T Puts own interests ahead cerns of young people or group 11 K Uses firmness when 10 U Says one thing. but does necessary another--dishonest 11 V Won't change--old Doesn't follow through-- fashioned H t" dishonest 113 fourth structural. The fourth-ranked behavior for Youth Guidance subjects was "organizes well" (E). and for "other” respondents it was "displays adequate knowledge and_ability” (B) as did the total sample. 0n the negative scale Youth Guidance subjects ranked "doesn't show concern for me” (N) fifth with a mean of 1.866 for mgthggg "other" respondents ranked it sixth with a mean of 1.291 and the national sample ninth with a mean of 1.137. By contrast Youth Guid- ance ranked this behavior third (2.988) for adult leaders while "other” ranked it second (3.278) and the national sample third (3.285). The reader should again notice the difference in the mean scores which are much higher in agglt leader where the reference is to the degree of seriousness this behavior has for this leader. The lower scores for mother refer to how true the behavior is perceived to be of her. Youth Guidance ranked "forces ideas on me" (Q) tenth in contrast to fifth for ”other” and the national sample with respect to mgthgr. The rankings and rat- ings were similar with regard to ggglt leaders. As seen in Figure 4.6. Youth Guidance subjects tended to rank behaviors higher than did the "other" respondents for ggth_§. This finding contrasts with the prior observation for adult leader where 114 FIGURE 4.6 - Youth Perceptions of Mother and Father Leadership Behavior--Mean Scores and Ranking by Item of Urban and Voluntary Youth Guidance and "Other" Responses “.00 1 08'“ .61) .4.» I I I I I l I .21» F I I BQOlP---—-.— n--- ...... J o 1’ I .60 I I gun. ' .21D : 200'”. I I r33< CCD’UDZ v . 100‘ O L f .-—-——-——--------—“--“----_—-w-- .b-——-—-—-—’-—— L's—"w 4 TM 08. eg. Pos. Neg. 703.. eg. 08. eg. Youth Youth Guidance 892922" Guidance 192523" MOTHER FATHER . 115 (Figure 4.2) "other” youth tended to rank the be- haviors higher than Youth Guidance young peOple. Suburban and Rural Responses suburban and rural subjects were similar in their responses pertaining to EQEQQI- The latter. however. tended to both rank and rate the desirable behaviors higher than did the former. as can be seen in Figure 4.7 and Table 4.11. By contrast suburban youth scored both positive and negative behaviors higher than did rural young people for ddulu leaders. (See also Table 4.5 and Figure 4.3.) Suburban subjects ranked "lets me take responsi- bility for important tasks” (0) third for EQEEEI- The national sample ranked this behavior fourth and rural respondents seventh. By contrast, suburban young people ranked this behavior ninth for 22212 leader while the national sample ranked it eighth and the rural youth fifth. Rural respondents ranked two behaviors which are types of initiation of structure first and fourth with the second and third being types of consideration for udthdn. Suburban subjects also split their four tap- ranked behaviors in the same manner, the first and fourth being types of initiation of structure, the 116 FIGURE 4.7 - Youth Perceptions of Mother and Father Leadership Behaviors--Mean Scores and Ranking by Item of Voluntary Suburban and Rural Subjects “.00 1......__..”_.._.-- £9 HI I I I I I I I C) c.. H'IJ (3‘ 3,5 :1) :13 e #3 83 < (DO rap 3 Z N O 5’ m “U m - - ' 1.00 Cth-B cm *9 1" <3 *s 60 t“ c: ’13 e60 .p-_------------—------------- IF——----—a- 0, Pos. Nag. Pos. Neg. Fos.l Neg. TostEg. Suburbag Rural Suburban Rural MOTHER FATHER 117 THEE 4.11 - Youth Perceptions of Mother and Father Leadership Behavior-- Mean Scoresfiund Ranki b Ite of 01 ' Mother - Father Marina. _nur.s1_ Suburban. ML K 3.191 1 K 3.222 1 K 3.291 1 B .l i P 3.078 c P 3.200 c B 3.200 1 K 3.1;; 1 c 2.952 e D 3.109 c C 2.978 c C 2.976 c 8 2.928 1 B 3.075 1 B 2.972 1 H 2.806 c D 2.883 3 3.040 H 2.900 3 2.734 B 2.8Z2 A 3.029 P 2.678 A 2.706 J 2.8 9 C 3.028 D 2.669 D 2.698 A 2.847 H 2.955 A 2.622 F 2.692 H 2.810 J 2.881 J 2.603 J 2.641 I 2.685 I 2.841 I 2.502 I 2.45 G 2.631 G 2.619 G 2.135 G 2.09 R 2.043 R 2.114 R 2.023 R 1.976 V 1.691 M 1.760 V 1.756 V 1.688 M 1.368 V 1.608 M 1.56? N 1.420 Q l. 51 Q 1.486 Q 1.356 0 1.268 0 1.426 0 1.434 T 1.284 T 1.218 L 1.175 T 1.161 S 1.099 S 1.04? P 1.163 P 1.149 N 1.042 P .9 6 8 1.145 N 1.109 L .965 L .9 7 N 1.024 S .977 U .962 N .888 U .963 U .841 P .923 U ..865 c = consideration 1 = initiation of structure Item Sym- # bol Behavior 1 A Communicates 2 M Doesn't relate to young 2 B Displays adequate know‘ people ledge and ability N Doesn't show concern for 3 O Lets young people (me) young people (me) take responsibility 4 0 Doesn't trust young people 4 D Listens (me) 5 E Organizes well 5 P Favors some over others 6 F Seeks to help when 6 Q Forces ideas on young needed people (me) 7 G Shares own shortcomings 7 R Gets upset when things and problems don't go right 8 H Shows sense of humor 8 S Looks down on young 9 I Tries new ideas-~open people (me) 10 J Understands (my) con- 9 T Puts own interests ahead cerns of young people or group 11 K Uses firmness when 10 U Says one thing. but does necessary another-~dishonest 11 V Won't change--old Doesn't follow through-- fashioned p t" dishonest 118 second and third consideration. On the other hand with regard to ddult leaders, all of the top four behaviors ranked by suburban youth were considerate. Rural young people ranked considerate behaviors for the top three, but ranked a structural behavior fourth. Research Question #5 The fifth question asked, "In what ways is the ranking of the behaviors for ddult leader similar and dissimilar with the behaviors seen by young peeple in their own fathdpd?" Again, the reader should keep in mind that the ranking of the behaviors was ordered according to which is perceived as most IMPORTANT with respect to the 29213 leader, and to which is most TRUE of 32532;. The top-ranked behavior on the positive scale for fatheg is ”uses firmness when necessary" (K). This behavior was ranked only ninth for adult leader, but, as can be seen in Table 4.1, the mean scores are close. The top-ranked behavior, seen as most important. in 22211 leaders is "listens” (D), a type of consider- ation. The tep-ranked "uses firmness when necessary" (K). seen as most true of fathers, is a type of initia- tion of structure. Of the top four behaviors indi- cated as being most important in 29211 leaders, the . 119 first three are considerate. Of the top four be- haviors indicated as most true of fathdzs, three are structural: only the third is a type of consideration. ”Understands my concerns" (J) was ranked second in importance for ddul; leaders, yet this behavior was ranked ninth by respondents as being true of their -£££h£2§. Again, the mean scores are important. Even though "understands my concerns" (J) is ranked ninth, it has a mean of 2.530. In fact, all 11 of the be- haviors in the positive scale for gdludg have mean scores over 2.0. I The last-ranked behavior seen as true of gggug; is the same as that ranked least important in udulg leader, "shares own shortcomings and problems” (G). The mean scores, however, are divergent, being sep- arated by more than two standard deviations. Both £2213 leader and gulhdz_have much lower mean scores for this behavior than do 92322; and pug; leader, which can be seen in Table 4.6. The shaded areas of the ddul; leader and father distributions illustrate the degree of separation of "shares own shortcomings and prob- lems” (G) from the other behaviors. With respect to the 11 negative behaviors, "says one thing, but does another--dishonest" (U) is ranked first as the most serious for 22312 leaders, ‘ 120 having a mean score of 3.318. By contrast the young people in the survey indicated a perfect negative correlation (of rank) on this behavior, ranking it last as being least true of their fathegs with a na- tional mean of 1.018. Figure 4.4 shows another contrast between the I positive and negative scales in the 59212 and 222222 categories. It shows nine of the 11 negative be- haviors for ddul; leaders as being above 3.0, thereby indicating that the subjects consider these acts as serious. At the same time they rated all 11 of the negative behaviors as relatively untrue of their $512221. only one being above 2.0. This bimodal dis- tribution is also illustrated in Table 4.6. Two negative behaviors, ”gets upset when things don't go right" (R) and "won't change--old-fashioned” (V), were ranked least serious for udult leaders. Yet these same two were ranked first and second as being most true of lulud; (as of udthdg). However, again the mean scores must be noted. While these two be- haviors are ranked most serious, it is important that the highest mean is only 2.092, and the other behavior, ”won't change--old-fashioned" (V), is well under 2.0 as are the other nine behaviors. 121 Male and Female Responses Male and female subjects were quite similar in their responses with respect to lduudu. On most of the positive and all but one of the negative behaViors, however, the males rated the items higher than did the -females, the former thus indicating that they see these behaviors as more true of their fdthezs than do the latter. The clearest example of this tendency of the females to rate £2353; lower is shown on Table 4.? re- garding the last-ranked behavior, ”shares own short- comings and problems” (G), which is ranked more than two standard deviations below the mean for males but is more than three standard deviations below the mean, and off the chart, for females. The shaded areas, again, refer in each case to the degree of difference between the mean scores of the last-ranked behavior and the next highest in the distribution. By contrast, females scored both positive and negative behaviors higher for ddult leaders. Both males and females included only one of their four top-ranked behaviors as a type of consideration, and both ranked it third for £21h2I- They differed only in their choices. Males selected, as did the total sample ”lets me take responsibility for important 122 tasks" (C), while for females it was “shows sense of humor” (H). For ddult leader males ranked the struc- tural "displays adequate knowledge and ability“ (B) third; the other three of the top four behaviors are considerate. Females, on the other hand, chose all considerate behaviors as their four tOp-ranked leader acts. Yough duidduge dud 'dthez” Respdnseg Youth Guidance and "other" subjects showed the most but not great differences with regard to £53221- Youth Guidance respondents ranked "seeks to help when needed” (F) third in contrast to the national sample who ranked it sixth and the ”other” youth who ranked it seventh. At the same time the ”other" subjects ranked "organizes well” (E) fourth, as did the national sample, yet in contrast with the Youth Guidance re- spondents who ranked it ninth. Regarding the last-ranked item, "shares own short- comings and problems" (G), more of a discrepancy can be observed as is illustrated in Table 4.12. Consistent with their trend of scoring both parents higher than did the ”other" young peOple. Youth Guidance respon- dents rated this behavior with a mean of 2.281 as dis- tinct from the ”other” subjects' 1.938 for fdludn. 123 am m+ am ~+ om T. N am H. 8 N- am? lawn.sv mme.n mmo.~ oa~.~ Hom.a Nam.o mma.o «3. .u am. 4 _ .3 u mum“. _ _ dance _ J . sense: I o: I I sesame n as . F 0 EL 3:2 u 2 Eu EU am m+ om ~+ am u+ N cm H- mm N- mm m- sam.m cam.m mom.n wme.m mmm.~ «mm.~ mso.~ 3mm... am 1 fl - 6H - .mmmm - _ 1 - .uesuo. u o f . eosmcaso sumo» u “my :msofipoaa cam m adsoouaosm: 23o moamcm: an copoem Ium swamp .8 £525 wu§§§§§§§&&&&& BJOIABQOH HAIIVOSN “£0149q98 HAILISOd noncommom cheapo: can eonmddsc 39:0» unmansao> and . sent: 90 sodusnaspmanIaoa>mn m adamaocmonmo msoauneoaehrmpsow I ‘ 124 The former score is more than two standard deviations below the mean but the latter more than three. Youth Guidance and ”other” rankings of this and the rest of the behaviors with regard to ddulu leader were essen- tially similar. Both groups rated the behaviors higher for ddult leader (consistent with the gener- ally higher ratings for £32.11). Youth Guidance rated ”shares own shortcomings and problems” (G) almost two standard deviations higher than they did for m, and "other” rated this behavior almost three standard deviations higher than for fathgr. Responses of Youth Guidance young people were divided evenly among the four top-ranked behaviors for fdther, two being types of consideration and two types of initiation of structure. The "other" youth, how- ever, had three of the tOp four as types of structure. For udult leader the three top-ranked behaviors se- lected by both Youth Guidance and "other" respondents are all types of consideration, and the fourth choice of both subgroups is a type of structure. subuzbdu and Rugal Resuouses Suburban and rural young people were very similar in the IQEBEE category. Suburban youth did tend to rate the behaviors higher than did rural young people . 125 with only three exceptions in the positive scale and one in the negative. Again, the clearest example, as illustrated on Table 4.8, is seen with the last- ranked behavior on the positive scale, ”shares own ashortcomings and problems” (G), which suburban subjects ranked almost three standard deviations below the mean, (but which rural respondents ranked more than three standard deviations below the mean and even off the chart. Paralleling the generally higher scores for ddulg given by the other subgroups, both suburban and rural respondents rated the 52213 behaviors higher than for Ilihlto For example, as illustrated in the shaded part of the distributions on Table 4.8, both suburban and rural ratings of "shares own shortcomings and problems" (G) for ddult are over two standard devia- tions above those given for 221222- Suburban youth ranked only one behavior as a type of consideration in their top four in contrast with rural youth whose top four behaviors included two types of initiation of structure and two types of considera- tion for lutudx. By contrast, suburban subjects' tap four behaviors for ddult leaders were all types of con- sideration as were the top three of rural respondents. The latter ranked fourth a type of initiation of struc- ture for adult leaders. 126 A trend can be observed in the subgroups' rating of behaviors in the four leader categories. In most cases the subgroups who rated behaviors high for the 2222 and adult leaders tended to rate them low for moths: and tutudg. _ exception of the desirable behaviors for mothet and As seen in Table 4.13, with the both scales for fathe , the subgroup which had the highest mean scores for the most behaviors in the scales for 2222 and adult had the lowest for mother and fatngt. TABLE 4.13 - Groups Which Tended to Rate Behaviozs Higher PEER ADULT MOTHER FlTflgfi SUBGROUP Female Female Male Male SCALE Both Both Negative Both "Other” "Other" Youth Youth SUBGROUP Guidance Guidance SCALE Both Both Both Both SUBGROUP sub- Sub- Rural Sub- urban urban urban SCALE Both Both Both Both It will be noted that analysis of the voluntary and nonvoluntary as well as church-related and 127 nonchurch-related groups has not been reported. These subpopulations are not included because statistical analysis of these and the age variables with respect to their scale scores showed no significant differences. Furthermore, due to the confounding elements identified ‘in Chapter I, and the limited usefulness of the infor- mation for religious education in the church, the de- cision was made to limit the inspection of the findings for these variables to an examination of the scales. The data, however, are being kept on file should any need arise in the future for the information they could provide. Tepidal Genetdlizations The foregoing has been an identification of the most pertinent data that were discovered in the effort to answer the research questions. The concluding sec- tion of this chapter will summarize the most important findings and describe some other generalizations which may also be drawn from the data. The generalization of the findings from this sam- ple to other 14 - 18 year-olds is limited. However, a relatively high degree of confidence in the findings derives from the rigors of the design, the size of the sample and the situational and geographic breadth of 128 the study. But since randomization was not possible, generalization should be tentative and to populations with backgrounds similar to the sample. More wide- spread generalization awaits another research project, building upon the present base but with a design that will permit more confident generalization. Generalizations About Egg; Leaders The data reveal seven particularly important findings. Again, "ranking" refers to the order of the behaviors based on their mean scores. ”Rating” refers to the mean scores, per se. 1. Ligtguiug is rugged as the most impoztgut pad: ldddet bdugvioz. This leader act was ranked first by the national sample and by all sub- groups. Its rating was well above 3.0 on the Likert scale by all groups. 2. donsidetate behgviors gre tanked as more impor- tgut thau behaviots whidh initiate stgugture. All four of the top-ranked behaviors in the total sample are types of consideration. The same is true for the subgroups with only two exceptions. ”Other” young people ranked a structural behavior fourth, and rural youth ranked a structural act third. 129 3. All of the desipable and undesirable behaviors gpe pgtdd as impOptant. The behaviors re- ceived different rankings in each of the sub- groups, though most of the behaviors varied within three positions of each other unless noted otherwise above. However, the mean scores were all rated above the halfway point on the Likert scale which indicates a high degree of importance for each of these be- haviors as perceived by the respondents. 4. Hypodrisy is ranked as the most serious un- desirable behaviop. Hypocrisy, or ”saying one thing, but doing another-~dishonesty" (U), traditionally a serious tgug pug of leaders in the eyes of youth, was the top-ranked negative act. It also received the highest rating of the negative scale. 5. degles pated thp desipable and undesipable be- puvlops up mope ippoptdpt than did pglds. The former consistently rated the 22 behaviors higher in terms of mean score than did the lat- ter. Otherwise there was no notable dif- ference between the genders. 6. ”Delinguent' youth rated the desirable and un- desirable behaviops as less important than did 130 the ”average” youth. The "delinquent" youth, though, rated none of the 22 behaviors as un- important or even of just ”little" impor- tance. By rating the behaviors with lower mean scores than those given by the other youth, they indicated that they are not as con- cerned with most of the behaviors as the others are. 7. dubuptgu ydutu pgted the positive and negg- tlyg bdpguidpp up pope impoptgpt than did zu:gl_yduth. The mean scores of the suburban subjects were higher than those of rural re- spondents. The higher means occurred on both the desirable and undesirable scales for pug; leader. Generalizations about Adult Leaders Eight particularly important findings with respect to pdult leaders emerge from the data. These obtained from analysis of the data from the total sample are identified first, followed by observations from the subgroups. l. Llsteulug is pankdd as thg most impoptant adult ldgdd: pungviop. This behavior was ranked first with a mean score of over 3.5 on the 4.0 3. 4. . 131 scale. All subgroups rated it over 3.3, and all ranked it first except for "other" youth who ranked it second (yet with a mean of over 3.5). . Qonsldgrgtd bphaviOIs up: punked up pope id- 0 an han st tu al behavio s. The three top-ranked behaviors in the total sample and in all but one of the subgroups are considerate. Only males ranked a structural behavior third. gharipg one's own shortcomings and problems is thd least importdpt gdult leadep behgvlop. This behavior was ranked least important (elev- enth, of all 11 on the positive scale in the total sample and for all subgroups. Neverthe- less, it was rated above midpoint on the Likert scale by the total sample and all subgroups thus indicating its importance in the respon- dents' perception. fiypogrisy is pdpked as the most sepious undu- sizable behavior. The negative behavior, "says one thing, but does another--dishonest" (U), was ranked first on the undesirable scale by the total sample and half of the subgroups. The behavior was rated above 3.0 by the whole sample and by all subgroups. 5. 132 Thu positivd dud neggtivd behaviops are rated pore important and more serious when seen in udult lgaddys thgp when seep ip peep leadeps. Only two behaviors, both on the negative scale, received lower mean scores for adult leader than for pddp leader. The same emphasis is generally true for the subgroups. Fe a as d e ositive d n tive be- v i o d s ious did udldd. Female subjects rated all 22 behaviors higher than did male respondents. The ranking of the behaviors, however, was quite similar for the two genders. ”Del uent" 0 ea 1 I ated t e ositive dud pgggtlyd bppaviOIs us less impoptgpt tugp did tug "gvepgge” youth. Youth Guidance respon- dents generally rated the behaviors lower in terms of mean score than did the ”other” sub- jects with respect to udult leaders only two exceptions occured in the positive scale and two in the negative. The two groups were simi- lar in their rankings for both scales. ban 0 a d he osi ive and no a iv bungviops gs pope impoptant than did rupal ydutn. Each of the 22 behaviors received a 133 higher mean score from the suburban subjects. All 11 behaviors in the positive scale and all but two in the negative were rated over 3.0 by suburban youth. Generalizations About the Relationship of Adult Leader Behaviors to Moths; Behaviors Nine especially important findings can be listed with respect to the relationship between ddult leader and pptgpp behaviors. It will be noted that the rela- tionships are mostly in the form of dissimilarity rather than of alikeness. l. Llstdnipg is tanked most important in adult leadeps but using firmness when negessapy is yanked most tpue of mothep. Both behaviors were rated over 3.0 for ddult leader and pdtpdp, even though the top-ranked in each category was ranked lower in the other. ”Uses firmness when necessary” (K) was ranked ninth for ddult leader, and "listens" (D) was ranked fifth for pdthdz. 2. 1112 turde top-[guns bguavidrs fop gdult leaddp ups all donsidepate, while two of the to thre o 0 he a e s u a1. ”Lis- tens" (D), ranked first for adult leader, is a type of consideration. ”Uses firmness when 134 necessary" (K), ranked first for uptudy, is a type of initiation of structure. The other behaviors for ddult leader were "understands concerns of young people (J) and ”communi- cates" (A). The others for uptpdp were the considerate "seeks to help when needed" (F) and the structural “displays adequate know- ledge and ability" (B). 3. dugping ong's own shortpomings and problems is {dated ad leudt important lp adult leadep dud s of o . This behavior was ranked eleventh, least, on the positive scale for both udult leader and uptudp. The mean scores for both are similar and both above 2.5. 4. flypoppipy id yanked as most seyious for adult leader but least true of mothep. ”Says one thing, but does another-~dishonest' (U) is ranked first as the most serious of the nega- tive behaviors for gdult leader but last, or least true, for pdtndp. A notable difference in mean scores is also evident. The mean for the top-ranked "says one thing but does an- other--dishonest" (U) for ddult leader is 3.318, while the mean score for this same be- havior, ranked last for uothdp, is .975. 7. fey gdult leadep but not vepy tpug fop mothez. The mean scores on all of the 11 negative be- haviors for ddult leader were above 2.7, thus ih the "very serious" range on the Likert scale. The mean scores for each of these behaviors with respect to hdthdp, however, were all be- low 2.0 except for the top-ranked, ”gets upset when things don't go right" (R), which was 2.119. The distributions of these means were completely bimodal.- The two hehaviops panked least sepious for d 1t d ked mos t u fo ot . ”Gets upset when things don't go right" (R) and ”won't change--old-fashioned” (V), ranked tenth and eleventh for ddult leader, were ranked first and second respectively for hp- thdz. The mean scores are notably higher for these two behaviors in their ranking for gdult leader than they are for pdthdp. nggled gated bdth sgdles highs: than pglds f9; Wigs sgulg highdp fdp uothdz. Females rated every behavior on the desirable and undesirable scales higher than males for adult leader. By 136 contrast. for mother males rated all 11 be- haviors of the negative scale higher than did the females. The behaviors were rated quite similarly in the mother positive scale by both genders. ”Average" youth rated both scdles highdr thgh ”delinquent” youth for adult leaderll but "de- linguent“ youth rdted both sgales higher for mother. "Other" subjects' mean scores for the 22 behaviors were higher than those given by Youth Guidance respondents with the exception of two in the positive scale and two in the negative for adult leader. 0n the other hand, Youth Guidance rated all but one of the 22 be- haviors higher than the "other" for mother. §uhurban youhg pQOple rated both spales higher thah rural youth for adult leader, but rurgl youhg people rgtpd both spales higher for me- th r. Suburban subjects rated all 22 behaviors with higher mean scores for gdult leader than did rural respondents. However, for hdthdr rural sample members' mean scores were higher on all but one of the behaviors in the positive scale and on all but five of the behaviors in the negative scale. 13? Generalizations About the Relationship of Adult Leader Behaviors to Father Behaviors Nine findings which are particularly important can be listed with regard to the relationship between adult leader and father behaviors. As with the rela- 'tionship between adult and mother behaviors, the com- parisons are more in the form of dissimilarity than of similarity. However, it will also be noted that seven of the nine observations which follow correspond to the nine in the preceding section. 1. Listening is ranked most important in adult ldgder but uding rirmness when nedessary is rankgd mogt true of father. Both behaviors were rated over 2.6 for gdult leader and t_- thdr, even though the behavior ranked first in each category was ranked lower in the other. ”Uses firmness when necessary" (K) was ranked ninth for adult leader, and "lie- tens" (D) was ranked seventh for father. 2. All thred tap-ranked behaviors for adult led- der ard gonsiderate, while two of the tap three for father are strudtural. "Listens" (D), ranked first for adult leader, is a type of consideration. "Uses firmness when necessary" (K), ranked first for father, is a type of ‘ 138 initiation of structure. The other behaviors for ddult leader were ”understands concerns of young people” (J) and "communicates” (A). The others for father were the structural ”displays adequate knowledge and ability” (B) and the considerate ”lets me take responsi- bility for important tasks" (C). 3. Sharihg one's own shortcomihgs and problems is ranked gs least important in adult leader ghd least trud of father. This behavior was ranked eleventh, last, on the scale of desir- able behaviors for both ddult leader and rd- th_r. The mean scores for both, however, are separated by more than half a point on the Likert scale, yet are both above 2.0. 4. Hypogrisy is ranked as most serious for adult leader but least trudyof father. ”Says one thing, but does another-~dishonest” (U) is ranked first as the most serious of the unde- sirable behaviors for gdult leader but last, or least true, for tdth_r. A notable difference in mean scores is also evident. The mean for this behavior is 3.318 for pdult leader, while the mean score for tdthdr is 1.018. 5. The negative behaviors are all rated as serioup 139 £9! adult leader hut not very true fur father. The mean scores on all of the 11 negative be- haviors for adult leader were above 2.7, thus in the "very serious" range on the Likert seale. The mean scores for each of these be- haviors with respect to father, however. were all below 2.0 except for the top-ranked "gets upset when things don't go right" (R), which was 2.092. The distributions of these scales were completely bimodal. 6. The two bdhaviors ranked least serious for adult leader dre ranked most true for fgther. ”Gets upset when things don't go right" (R) and ”won't change-~old-fashioned" (V), ranked tenth and eleventh for gdult leader, were ranked first and second respectively for lg- th_r. The mean scores are notably higher for these two behaviors in their ranking for adult leader than they are for father. 7. Females rated both sgales higher than males for adult leader, but males rated both sgales higher for father. Females rated every be- havior on the positive and negative scales higher than did males for adult leader. By contrast, males rated six of the 11 behaviors 140 on the positive scale and all but one on the negative scale higher than did the females. 8. "Average" youth rdted bothfigpules higher thuh "delinguent" youth for adult_legderr put "de- llhduent" yguth rgtdd both scules higher for father. ”Other" subjects' mean scores for the 22 behaviors were higher than those given by Youth Guidance respondents with the exception of two in the desirable scale and two in the undesirable for ddult leader. On the other hand, Youth Guidance subjects rated all but four of the behaviors on the desirable scale and all of the behaviors on the undesirable scale higher than did the "other" sample mem- bers. 9. Suburban young people rdted poth suales higher than rural youth tor adult leader and for fa- thdr. Suburban subjects rated all 22 behaviors with higher mean scores for gdult leader than did rural respondents. In a reverse of the trend noted above where one set of subjects scored the behaviors higher in one category and lower in another, the suburban subjects also scored the behaviors on both scales higher than did the rural reSpondents for father. 141 Suburban youth rated seven of the 11 desirable behaviors with higher mean scores and nine of the 11 undesirable behaviors with higher means than did rural young people with regard to father. Other Generalizations While examining the data in order to answer the research questions, a number of findings were discov- ered that do not apply to the questions but which are important with regard to related issues. Three such observations have been identified, and their presenta- tion below concludes this chapter. 1. More data were available for:peer and adult leaders than for udther dud father. For analy- sis of findings the computer was programmed to reject all cases in which a reapondent failed to provide a reaponse to more than five items. For pddr and gdult leaders the average number of invalid cases reported by the computer was 28. However, for mother the number rose sharply to 52, and for fathgr it more than doubled to 123 out of 1536 (85). 2. helpihg when needed was seen as more true of pother than father. ”Seeks to help when needed" 142 (F) was ranked second for mother in contrast to sixth for father in the frequency counts for the total sample. Furthermore, the mean for this behavior was more than a standard deviation higher for mother. These observa- tions generally held as well for each of the subgroups. 3. Regional subgroups' ranking of the 22 behav- iors paralleled the ranking of the other duh; groups. Confirming the conclusions of Douvan and Adelson as well as others, young people in the regional groups reported similarly to the total sample. Where statistical signifi- cance emerged it was observed to agree with the other findings. The rare exceptions have been noted in the text above. The foregoing has been an identification of the most important data obtained by the design described in Chapter III. What do the data mean? What conclusions can be drawn from the findings? These questions to- gether with some implications for further research are the focus of Chapter V. CHAPTER V THE CONCLUSIONS This study sought answers to five research ques- tions. The first and second questions asked whether young people view some of the 11 positive and 11 nega- tive behaviors as more important on any kind of consis- tent basis with respect to their pddr and gdult leaders. The third question asked what similarity or dissimi- larity existed in responses concerning pddr and udult leaders. The fourth and fifth questions asked what ways the ranking of the behaviors for adult leader was similar and dissimilar with the behaviors seen by young people in their own mothers and fathers. The preceding chapter has indicated the findings of the study with respect to these questions. The following will indi- cate practical implications of the findings for each of the areas explored in the research questions and for the selection and training of leadersu New questions and suggestions for further research will conclude the chapter. 143 144 What the Data Mean While not presuming to be an exhaustive list, the following practical applications can be seen as impli- cations of the findings discovered in the attempt to answer the research questions. This first section fo- 4 cusses Specifically on those implications which pertain to peer leaders. Conclusions Regarding Egg; Leaders The meanings identified here pertain to what young people perceive as a good pddr leader. At least three implications can be listed. 1. Peer ladders should be good listendrs. It is very important to young people that peers who are exercising leadership over them provide them with Opportunities to express their needs and concerns. The close proximity of three other types of consideration, all indicated as most important in the study, suggests that the pddr leader listen empathically and act in ac- cord with the message received. A pddr leader should spend more time in drawing out others as to how they are doing rather than in talking about himself/herself. The consistently last- place ranking of "shares own shortcomings and 2. 3. 145 problems" (G) is an additional indication that youth want the subject of conversation with their pddr leader to be more on themselves than on the leader or on something else. Peer leaders should emphasize behaviors that urdoriented to pupplelrdther than to tddh. Behaviors that are types of consideration are perceived as more important in pddr leaders. Therefore, if a pddr leader wishes to be ef- fective, he or she should act accordingly, whereby the majority of his or her behaviors ought to involve relating to the followers in such ways as listening, communicating, and seeking to help when needed as a result of un- derstanding the concerns of young people. He or she should be seen doing these behaviors at each meeting of the youth group rather than being observed spending time arranging the chairs, lining up the peeple to bring next week's refreshments or setting up the pro- jector and screen. Thd peer leader's words and deeds should matgh. Hypocrisy is considered more serious than most of the positive behaviors-pug; leaders perform. To be effective the pddr leader should make 146 sure that his or her actions are consistent with the verbal message he or she is communi- cating. ”Practice what you preach” is essen- tial for the pddr leader. The Biblical teaching of the Apostle James is still true in the application of this conclusion to church education, "faith without works is dead" (James 2:26). In the light of Jesus' state- ment that ”you will know them by their fruits" (Matt. 7:20), a.2§££ leader in a church youth group should modela lifestyle and values con- sistent with those commanded in the Bible and held to be important in the church. Conclusions Regarding ldult Leaders The meanings identified in this section pertain to what young people perceive as a good adult leader. At least four implications can be listed. 1. Adult leaders should bg good listehers. It is very important to young peeple that their adult leaders take time to allow them to express their needs and concerns. While they are mak- ing this expression the leader should listen attentively and not be engaged in other activ- ities, checking his watch to make sure the 147 meeting starts on time or looking around at other people or objects. The leader should listen empathically and try to draw out his/ her followers as to how they are doing rather than in talking about himself and what he thinks concerning certain subject matters. The consistent ranking of ”shares own short- comings and problems” (G) last of the 11 posi— tive behaviors is an added indicator that youth want to talk about themselves rather than something else when conversing with their adult leader. Adult legderp should emphasize behaviors thgt gr; orightdd to people rather than to tash. More behaviors that are types of consideration were ranked among the highest than were types of initiation of structure. Thus, the udult leader who wishes to be effective should con- sciously make the majority of his behaviors relational in nature, such as listening, under- standing and communicating. He should plan to spend most of the time at each meeting of the youth group in relating to his followers. While it is easier for the ddult leader to do such structural acts as room arrangement, 148 setting up displays and planning future events with those to whom he has given certain respon- sibilities, and to take meeting time to do such things, he will be more successful in the eyes of the youth if he delegates as much work as he can to others and takes time to do most of his structural work on other occasions. These procedures are likely to also be perceived as demonstrating the leader's adequate knowledge and ability which was the structural behavior youth in the sample indicated as most important in their ddult leader. 3. Thu adult lgadpr ghould shard hip own short- gopings dud problems disgriminately. While, as noted above, young people want the leader to listen to them more than to talk about himself, the high rating of the behavior ”shares own shortcomings and problems" (G) shows that youth consider it important to know how the leader copes with difficulties. This observation both supports and illustrates the youth counselling literature (e.g., Richards, 1972) which indi- cates the desirability of youth leaders sharing how they have caped with a problem similar to that a client has been talking about. As 149 Richards states, however, this sharing by the udult leader should take place after an appro- priate amount of listening, understanding and communicating so the young person can be sure the adult knows what he is thinking and feeling (p. 142). The lower mean score for this item indicates the degree of emphasis this behavior should receive in the leader's relationship with his followers. It should come after, not before. a lot of other communicating. u. The adult ladder's words and deeds should matgh. Hypocrisy is considered more important than most of the positive behaviors. and more than all of the negative behaviors, that udult lead- ers perform. If he/she is to be effective with his/her followers, the gdult leader should avoid a conflict between what he/she says and what he/she does. With regard to the latter deeds of emission and commission need to be remembered. If, for example, a leader tells the youth that it is important to be loving and caring toward everyone and then proceeds to ig- nore some of the young people himself, he will be violating this principle. The leader can be seen as a hypocrite in the eyes of his/her 150 followers on the basis of what he/she doesn't do as well as by what he/she does if these behaviors are in conflict with what he/she says. ”Do as I say but not as I do” repre- sents a serious shortcoming in the eyes of many young people. Conclusions Regarding Comparisons and Contrasts Between Peer and Adult Leaders The implications of the findings identified in the following section refer to similarities and dissimi- larities youth make in what they view as important in their peer and adult leaders. At least six implica- tions can be listed. 1. Peer and adult ladders should perform all the poditive behaviors in order to be most effeg- tlyd. All 11 of the positive behaviors are considered by young people to be important. and they want to see their pddr and ddult lead- ers function accordingly. While the behaviors themselves vary in degree of importance among young people, the youth still view them all with high regard. 222! ghd adult ldaders should make listening to their followers thelr top priority. Young peo- ple want their leaders to listen to their 151 concerns. Leaders who want to be successful will therefore avoid allowing program con- straints to take time away from talking with their followers. The leaders should encourage their group members to share their concerns and demonstrate a desire to hear and to help. 3. Peer and adult leaders should spend more time oh behaviors oriented to people than to tasks. Young peeple prefer leaders to show more con- sideration than concern for structure in their behavior. The top three leader acts are per- ceived to be listening, understanding and com- municating, and these apply to both pddr and udult leaders. It should be readily evident to the youth that their leaders are spending more time with them than with program details. 4. Adult ldaders should be hor§_doncerned with task oriented behaviors than should peer legd- 3rd. Young people expect the ddult leader to demonstrate adequate knowledge and ability more than pddr leaders. This behavior was the only structural behavior indicated as important in the four top-ranked behaviors. Youth recognize that structure is important, and they want it, but they want it more in their adult leader 152 than in leaders of their own age. 5. Peer and adult leaders should avoid all the negative behaviors in order to be post eff - tlyd. All 11 of the negative behaviors are considered by youth to be seriously undesir- able. They therefore want to see little or no evidence of these actions in their leaders. 2222 and ddult leaders who don't want to "turn their kids off" will avoid functioning in these undesirable manners. 6. Peer and adult leaders should be especially pgrgful to matdh their words ghd ddeds. Of all the negative leader behaviors, hypocrisy is considered by youth to be the most serious in any leader. All leaders should demonstrate in action the prOpositions taught in the Bible if they are claiming to teach and value these concepts and principles. Conclusions Concerning the Relationship Between ldult Leader Behaviors and Muthdr and thher Behaviors In certain ways it is possible to see to what de- gree preferences for adult leaders are viewed by youth as true of their mothdrs and fgthers. Both mothdr and fathdr will be discussed together in this section since the important conclusions for each are the same. 153 An ideal situation would obtain if it were pos- sible to observe certain rankings for ddult leader as present in mother and father. It would then facili- tate, for example. the selection of ddult leaders. As can be seen in the first pilot study, the same three traits ranked highest for ddult leader are the three ranked highest, and in the same order, for mother. Thus, one could conclude that to recruit an ddult leader who will be effective in working with high school young people, he should look first to mothers. Such is not the case with the rankings in the data produced by this study. As seen in the generali- zations about the relationship of desired udult leader behaviors to perceived mother and father behaviors in Chapter IV, the relationships with regard to ranking are all dissimilar with the exception of the last-place "shares own shortcomings and problems” (G). Neverthe- less, the rankings as they are and the ratings (mean scores) of the behaviors do lead to at least two impli- cations. 1. Both mothers and fathers are appropriate as adult leaders. All of the 11 desirable behav- iors young people view as important in their udult leaders are seen by them as true in their pothdrs and fathers. Furthermore. all 154 ll of the undesirable behaviors, which youth see as serious in their ddult leaders are not generally true of their parents. Corre- Spondingly, the two undesirable behaviors youth see as most true of mothers and fathers are those which are the least serious of the undesirable behaviors performed by gdult leaders. 2. Mothers and fathers should share thdlrgown_pr& perienges about as mugh as is eupdcted of adult ldadgrs. Young peOple want their ddult leaders to share their own problems and concerns but only to a limited extent, indicating that the other 10 behaviors are more desired. Corre- spondingly, they see their parents as doing such sharing less than the other 10 and about as frequently (indicated by similar mean scores) as desired in their ddult leaders, with the possible exception of father who shares his concerns considerably less than indicated for the others. Mappings for Leader §elegtion The preceding has been an identification of prac- tical implications of the data discovered in the . 155 attempt to answer the research questions. The focus of the foregoing was on those areas of youth leadership relative to the questions. In addition, the data yield implications for leader selection as well. Conclusions for Selecting Eddr Leaders The following implications are of importance to these churches and parachurch organizations, such as YFC, which are placing an emphasis on providing cppor- tunities for helping young peOple grow and develop through interaction with leaders their own age. At least four implications can be listed. 1. Look for youth who listen more to their peers thuh talk about themselves and whut they thihk. Since young people want their pddr leaders to listen to them, the organization which wants to use youth in leadership positions will want to look for those who demonstrate such ability. This approach does not mean that listening em- pathically cannot be taught as a skill, but it is reasonable to assume that this leadership behavior will be more often utilized more ef- fectively if a recruit can be obtained who already has been performing in this manner. 2. look for youth who are people-oriented more 156 thah task-orientdd. Young people who are ob- served to be more interested in talking with their peers about how they are doing than about which school is going to beat which school for the football championship, about whether the principal will resign before the end of the year, or about their extracurricu- lar activities, are prime considerations for pddr leadership. Recruiters of pddr leaders should expect a large amount of talk such as that just listed and a similar amount of talk about oneself, for, as Kohlberg discovered (Chapter I), adolescents are still in a rela- tively egocentric stage of development. Nevertheless, some young peeple do stand apart from others their age in the amount of time they spend talking about such matters and in the degree of interest they display toward others' well-being. This study suggests they are most likely to provide effective pddr leadership. 3. Look for youth who perform thd desirable ghd gvoid thg uhdesirdble behdvlors. Since all the behaviors on each scale are rated as very im- portant by young peeple, it is essential the 157 preposed 2§££ leader act accordingly. The high mean scores indicate that youth take each of the 22 behaviors seriously, and unless one who aspires to lead his peers performs the positive acts and avoids the negative ones he will most likely be ineffective. Therefore, the recruiter of pddr leaders can use the 22 behaviors as a checklist in observing young peeple in action while seeking to determine who demonstrates these abilities. The highest ranked behaviors, of course, should be given tap priority: all else being equal, the selec- tion should be made on the basis of which youth performs the highest ranked behaviors most fre- quently. Ask the girls who would be a good peer leader. The tendency of girls to rate the behaviors higher than the boys do indicates that the former have stronger feelings about the behav- iors of their pddr leaders. Their pddr leader should have a high degree of homophily with what the girls value if he is going to be successful in relating to the female gender. Thus, an informal sociometric inquiry among 158 the girls is likely to be very helpful in identifying an effective peer leader. Conclusions for Selecting Adult Leaders The findings also yield implications for the re- cruitment of gdult leaders, the following of which suggestions will likely result in more effective lead- ership. At least five implications can be identified. 1. Look for adults who listen to others more than tglk about thdmselves and thdlr Opiniong. Ob- serve the preportion of time potential ddult leaders spend in listening to those with whom they are in conversation. A recruiter should also try to participate in such conversations unobtrusively, that is, without any indication of his objective, and notice if the prospective youth leader is listening in an empathically caring manner or in a more disinterested mind- wandering way. Observation should also be made as to the amount of time a potential udult leader spends in talking with young people. Does he search them out or at least talk with them when in proximity to them? Is he inter- ested in what they might have to say? These questions provide answers that serve as ' 159 indicators of the degree to which he is likely to listen in the manner the young people want. and need. 2. Look for adults who dre more peeple-oriented 3. than task-oriehtgd. The subjects of conver- sation which potential gdult leaders engage in are indicators that are likely to be useful in determining whether a prospective leader will be peOple or task-oriented. Is an gdult who is being considered as a youth leader spending more time trying tounderstand what a person is saying and how he feels about what he is saying? Or, does such a person spend more time talking about meeting production quotas, explaining how to balance the budget and com- plaining about the disagreeable weather? The importance of this implication is difficult to overestimate. Young people are less interested in the program an ddult leader has planned for a given occasion than with how he treats them when they come to the meeting. 00 fo ad ts w o fo m the abl d gydld thg uhddgirdbld behavlors. As each of the 11 behaviors on the desirable and each on the undesirable scale are rated as very ‘ 160 important by young people, the proposed ddult leader should act accordingly. Since the youth take the 22 behaviors seriously, an ddult leader will be effective to the degree he per- forms the desirable and avoids the undesirable behaviors. Thus, a recruiter can use the 22 behaviors as a checklist for determining which prospective adults function as desired and which do not. Furthermore, the potential ddult leader should be rated higher on this checklist than the potential pddr leader. Since youth see the desirable and undesirable behaviors as more important and more serious in their ddult leaders than in their ppdr leaders, they are expecting more from the former. Parents are a good starting point, for youth see both pdthdrd and tgthdrd as performing desirable and avoid- ing the undesirable behaviors. Since both parents are seen as having an emphasis on structural behaviors, care must be taken, as the primary emphasis in ddult leadership should be on acts of consideration. However, parents vary and what is true of them in the home can- not be assumed to be the same in a youth group. 4. Ask the girls who would be a good adult leader. 161 The higher rating of the desirable and unde- sirable behaviors for ddult leader by girls is indicative of their stronger feelings with reapect to how the leader should perform. The ddult leader's behaviors should parallel the expectations of the girls if he is going to be effective in working with them. An informal sociometric inquiry among the girls as to which adults they would like for an ddult leader often yields successful results in the experi- ence of this writer. 5. Leah for a mother or g father if an gdult leader is needed for disgipline probleus. Young peOple's viewing their parents as having a high degree of structure in their behaviors indicates that such leaders would likely per- form well in groups where youth sometimes be- come unruly. While it is not at all certain (and cannot be concluded from the data) that all parents will perform in a youth group as they are perceived in a parental context at home, this writer has found that parents fre- quently function well in such environments. The findings in this study thus appear to have uncovered one reason why that happens. 162 Meanings for Leader Traihing The preceding identified practical implications of the data for the selection of leaders. In addition, the data obtained in the study yield implications for helping leaders to develop their skills in relating to ' young people. At least three implications can be listed. 1. The basig purridulum should inplude pomponents for devlepihg pommuniggtidn skilld. In order to accomplish the mission which is part of each Christian's life purpose (Matt. 5:13-16; 20:19-20), leadership is performed. When young people are being taught in the context of church education, they are being equipped for such leadership. Therefore, at regular inter- vals on a formal basis, and through nonformal programs, the curriculum for children and youth should provide opportunities for helping the learners develop the ability to listen empath- ically and employ the other positive behaviors skillfully. 2. T basi ur i l s cvld 1 de a at o duphasis on the nedessdry link between vdlugs judgmdht dud vdlues agtioh. One of the key 163 messages of the Bible is the necessity of link- ing word and deed (Matt. 7:20: James 2:26). The Hebrew word, ddhdr, and the Greek word, ldgdd, both mean word and deed as an insepar- able unity. The believer is to thus make his actions parallel to his judgments, which is a principle esteemed by youth as discovered in this study. At specific points in the curric- ulum the importance of this conceptualization and areas of practical application should be emphasized. 3. huggership should be taught as involvihg pddpld-orientdd agtivities as well as task- oriented aptivities. Leadership in church education is frequently conceived as produc- tion oriented and highly structural. Such a concept is especially true with regard to church teaching. Many teachers feel very un- comfortable if they are not talking or struc- turing learning activities. The findings of this study, however, suggest that teachers and other youth leaders should spend most of their time in behaviors that are types of considera- tion. Therefore, the leadership training pro- gram ought to include an emphasis on the 164 importance of considerate leader acts and help in developing those abilities. Othdr Qohglusiohs The foregoing has been an identification of impli- cations of the data obtained in this study for the 'purposes of answering the research questions. The first section listed the implications for the research questions themselves, following which implications were presented for the related matters of leader recruitment . and education. The following section contains a list of implications for other subjects which can be seen in the findings discussed in the fourth chapter. At least three other conclusions can be drawn from the data. 1. Egrdhts ghould hp dhgourggdd in thdir relutloh- phips with thelr tdep-agers. Young people view their parents in a favorable light, seeing the desirable leader behaviors as true of their parents and the undesirable behaviors as not very true of them. This message is not often heard or seen in accounts of parent-teen re- lations communicated today in the medias in fact, it is quite the Opposite. Parents are portrayed as existing in perpetual conflict with their adolescent offspring. Yet this 165 conclusion from this study was corroborated by Gallup who stated that his polls show the existence of similar Opinions by today's young people toward their parents.* However, this conclusion is perplexing to many parents when they hear it, for they respond that they only receive negative criticism from their youngsters. The explanation Of this phenom- enon may lie in Erikson's research (Erikson, 1963, pp. 216 ff.). The Harvard psychologist has develOped a theory of human growth which conceptualizes man as going through eight basic stages. The stage in which 14 to 18 year-olds function is referred to as identity formation, so-called because the human or- ganism at this stage is forging a self-concept in which he is trying to become a psychologi- cal adult and to dissociate himself from child- hood in every way possible. He, therefore, is less likely to want to communicate to his parents in any way that he is dependent upon them or to suggest that the filial relationship which existed in childhood is still intact. *Conversation with Dr. George Gallup, Jr.. Oak Brook, Illinois, November 15, 1979. 166 Thus, the adolescent avoids communicating that he perceives his parents positively, which would affirm them and his relationship with them. Independence is more readily perceived through taking an opposite position. However, when anonymity is preserved, as in the present study, the youth can feel safe to express his views as they are. Hence, he can say that hother and fdther are good and that he is pleased with them and their behavior without the risk they will then assume everything is the same as it always was and try to keep treating him as a child. 2. Thu legddr shduld egphadize ggrtgin behdviors i i o d to a i e ula objddtives more guigkly and effedtively. This implication extends not only to individual teachers but also to educational administrators who must choose what kind Of teacher tO put with what kind Of youth to obtain which objec- tives. For example, recognizing Maslow's findings concerning the necessity for a secure environment in order for social and personal- ity develOpment, a class with a high percentage of ”energetic” young peOple will require 167 leader behaviors with a considerable amount of structure in order for these social and per- sonality needs to be met. The leader himself should thus employ these behaviors. However, even prior to the develOpment of this situa- tion, the educational administrator, e.g.. the Director of Christian Education in a church setting, should use what Fiedler calls ”organizational engineering" (1967, p. 255) to match the leader who is strong in structural behaviors with such a class. "It is essen- tial," states Fiedler, ”that we realize that poor performance in a leadership position is likely to be as much the function of the lead- ership situation which the organization pro- vides as it is the function of the individual's personality structure” (p. 260). In curric- ulum construction this is all a part Of what should constitute the needs assessment upon which objectives are established (Tabs, 1962, p. 12). In this manner the leadership behav- iors, which are such key elements in the suc- cess of the educational institution's objectives (of. Chapter I), will facilitate the attainment of those purposes instead of 3. 168 mitigating them. Adult ldgders should not use an authoritarian styld as their gdneral orientation. As men- tioned in Chapter I, the Evghgeligal News- letter (1977) reported that young people may be ”looking more for authority figures who forthrightly state, 'This is the way it is.'” The article cited a return to the use of large youth rallies similar to those used in the middle Of this century. It indicated that one reason for the return to this form of youth ministry might be a desire on the part Of young peeple for leaders who use an authori- tarian approach. If this were the case one would expect the ranking and rating of "forces ideas on young people" (Q) to indicate ”not strongly undesirable.” However, this behavior was ranked ninth, and the mean score was 3.068, thus being in the "very serious” range. Fur- thermore, the top-ranked behaviors are types of consideration rather than initiation of structure. For these and other reasons iden- tified in APPENDIX A, this study does not sup- port widespread use Of an authoritarian ap- proach to youth ministry. 169 Resegrch Questions for Further Study The present study has been descriptive in design. As such it is unable to draw conclusions as to cause and effect. Since one of the purposes of descriptive ’research is to generate areas for further investiga- ‘tion, the following suggestions have grown out of the present study. These questions are stated as they are to indicate their measurability, but the technical precision has been omitted, for that must await the specific problem statement and design required by the study which will treat them. 1. Will the adult ledder whosd initiation of gtrudture behaviors outwdigh higlher consid- grate hdhaviord dggomplish more objeptives under pertain renditions thdhpthe one who funptions mostly in a ponsiderate manner uhder the same renditions? Generally speaking, as indicated in this study, the emphasis on con- sideration will likely yield the more effective accomplishment of Objectives in relating to high school youth. However, while the con- siderate approach is useful as a general framework, specific instances (such as a class with youth who are Often unruly) seem to 170 mitigate that orientation, and empirical sup- port would be most desirable. gravided the Opportunity to remain anonyuous, will young peeple give family life a positive I rutlhg? The high rating of desirable behaviors for both uother and tdthdr as well as the low rating of undesirable behaviors for each parent indicates a generally favorable view of these important relationships within the fam- ily. This finding suggests that family life as a whole in America may not be as negative an experience in the lives of most people as is often presented to be the case by much of the publicity currently being produced in the media. Will youth rate mother and father equal in authority with respedt to depisiohs ponderning dhildren? The high ranking and rating of be- haviors which are types Of initiation of structure for hdthdr indicate that young peo- ple see her as having considerable involvement in decision making with respect to the chil- dren's activities. This awareness combined with the substantial amount of the time tgthdr is away from the family leads to the conclusion that mothdr makes many of the decisions 171 concerning the children. While this con- clusion is not new, one implication which should be explored is the effect this develop- ment has on parental leadership functions and relationships, specifically the strong indica- tion that ppthdr does not relate to fathdr as second in a chain Of command with respect to these decisions in the view of youth. Hence, pother may be seen as not accountable to futhdr in this area, and a democratic rather than autocratic relationship with regard to parental leadership may be characterizing the American family. Additional Suggdstions for Eurther Researdh Due to the nature Of empirical research which re- quires a specific focus on a particular problem, thorough treatment of important related issues is not possible. These areas must thus be undertaken by sub- sequent investigations. The present study has brought to light at least five such areas in addition to the foregoing research questions which should be explored by continuing research. 1. Do youth see the behaviors ranked and rated as most importdht and serious for 29;; and adult _ 1'72 leaders as true for thosd leadgrs? Investi- gation of this question could involve a design similar to the one in the presentation with only a change of the rubrics on the instrument. In effect the subjects would be asked to do for their pddr and udult leaders what those in this study were asked to do with respect to their parents. 2. Tu whgt ddgrde do the behavior; of their pur- dhts ddppgrd to whdt yduth vidw as most impor- bdhavior? As with the first suggestion, a possible design for such a study would involve a change in rubric on the present instrument. Thus, youth would be asked to indicate for each parent which of the behaviors is most important and serious instead of which is most true. 3. To what degree is therd a disprgpancy bdtween how youth vldw thdir pgrentd dud how purgnts vidw thepdelydg? The findings and conclusions reported in this study must be considered as indications of how youth view their parents and not necessarily as evidence of what is in fact true concerning their mothers and fdthers. A possible design for Obtaining information he 173 leading to the answer of this question would be to survey the parents of the youth whose Opinion would be investigated. What gre the rdasons as to why youth IQEEOQQ A ldss td gugstiohs pertainihg to their fathers? As reported above, the average number of cases where respondents failed to answer at least six items on a given scale for pddrd and udultd was 28. For.pdthdr the number increased sharply to 52: for fdthdr it more than doubled to 123. This Observation seems important in itself, for the reasons could have considerable implications for American family life and fam- ily education in the church. A possible way to Obtain this information would be to identify the most commonly reported reasons in several pilot tests with an Open-ended question per- taining to the rationale for not responding to items for tgthdr, Those reasons could then be included at the end Of such an instrument as the one used herein in a closed item format where respondents would indicate which of them is most characteristic of their decision. Would any diffdrendes be Observed by stratify- ing the populdtion on the basis Of the 174 following variables? At least five other demographics chould have been used as orga- nizers for data analysis. Each of the fol- lowing is a potential influence on the perception of leader and parent behavior. a. A subjegt's experidnde of strohg negativg affept on the day Of the survey. A ques- tion should be asked on the instrument relative to whether the respondent had any experience on the day of the survey which bothered him/her to any considerable ex- tent. Such an experience could be an argument with his/her parent(s), a break-up of a romance, or a failure Of a test. b. A suhjegt'p pergeption of the datg-gatherer. An Opportunity should be provided for the respondent to indicate how he/she perceives the data-gatherer. A positive affect toward the one who is distributing and collecting the instrument, giving the instructions, and observing the responses could produce different results from a respondent who has developed a negative affect toward the one conducting the survey (who is performing certain leadership functions in his/her data 175 collecting). c. Nuuber Of subjedt's siblings. Building upon the common observation that children in a given family view their parents' treatment ' Of them differently, it would be instructive to see what differences exist, if any, be- tween the Oldest and the youngest (and any others). Therefore, a question should be added to the instrument which asks the re- spondent to record how many brothers and/or sisters he/she has and his/her position ‘among them. d. De e of latio s i . It would still be useful for church educators to know whether (and. if so, to what degree) any differences exist between church related and nonchurch related young peeple with respect to their perception of leader be- haviors. One way to Obtain this information without the confounding of data due to church subjects in the schools would be to ask the respondents to check on the instru- ment whether they attend any church youth group. In conjunction with this question could be a scale on which the subjects ‘ 176 would also check their degree of involve- ment with such a group. e. Subjdgt's age. The views of younger high school youth (14 - 15 years Of age) may ‘ differ from the perceptions Of their older peers (l6 - 18). The data for this anal- ysis already exists from the present study and should be used. Of course it is to be hOped that the present study will be replicated. In the interests of the scientific pursuit of what is, man must continually use all avail- able tools in the objective attempt to Obtain under- standing Of the realities which constitute his envi- ronment. As crucial as is the leadership of those who comprise the future, such investigation has special urgency. APPENDIX A The First Pilot Study Muskegon, Michigan November 12, 1977 The respondents in this study were presented with two slips Of paper, One green-~on which in eight dif- ferent sections were written the eight positive traits, and the other pink--on which in eight different sec- tions were written the eight negative traits. The group was told to rip the eight sections of each slip into separate cards, so each slip would represent one (trait. They were next each given the sheet (Figure Al) upon which they were asked to record their rankings Of the eight positive and eight negative traits for each of the categories on the sheet. In sections five and seven they were to only identify the tOp four of all sixteen traits. In sections six and eight they were asked to write yes or no as to whether they were living with the parent whose traits they had ranked in the preceding section. In these spaces they were also asked to indicate their age and their sex. With only a couple Of exceptions everyone provided all the data requested. A major concern in designing the instrument was to make sure that each term, especially withregard to the leadership trait cards, was clearly understood by the age group for which the instrument was designed (l4 - 18 years Old). The eight positive and eight 177 178 FIGURE A1 - Scoring Sheet for the Ranking of Traits I. IIIIAT Is 2. NHAT CAN MAKE A 3. IIIIAT IS IIPORTANT I. WHAT CAN NANE AN INPORTANT IIIEN PERSON (ABOUT NI WHEN AN ADULT ADULT A- POOR A PERSON ABOUT AGE) A POOR LEADS LEADER OP GE 5 LEADER TEENAGERS TEENAGERS NOST NOST MOST MOST IMPORTANT SERIOUS _ IMPORTANT SERIOUS _ LEAST _ LEAST — LEAST _ LEAST ‘— INPORTANT SERIOUS IMPORTANT SERIOUS 5. WHAT IS MOST IMPORTANT ABOUT HY "OTHER 7. HHAT IS MOST IMPORTANT ABOUT MY FATHER VALUES DEVELOPMENT EDUC ATION PROGRAM -- Michigan State University 179 negative traits of the Gamelin study were reworded (except for ”understands" and "phony") in terminology geared to the level of high school youth. The way the new terms were decided upon was to present a sheet containing the eight positive and eight negative terms from the Gamelin study to two different groups 'of Grand Rapids, Michigan teenagers. The sheet had the definitive phraseology beside each of the eight positive and eight negative terms which Gamelin used in his study to indicate the meaning Of the 16 factors. Some additional words and phrases which are part of the contemporary language of high school youth in the United States were also included in the definitive section opposite each Of the 16 traits. The youth were then asked to select one word or phrase for each of the 16 traits, from either the original 16 terms or from the definitive statements concerning the traits, which best (most accurately and meaningfully) communi- cated that concept in their understanding. The orig- inal terms and their counterparts for this study are listed in Table A1. The precautions concerning the control of poten- tial causes of invalidity and unreliability identified above with respect to the national study were also followed in the Muskegon pilot but for two exceptions. 180 Rank 1 TABLE Al - Traits which Church Youth Like and Gamelih Concerned, Encouraging Receptive, Communica- tive Understand- ing Lively Competent Helpful, Involved Mature, Secure Open-minded, Flexible Dislike in their Leaders Gamelin Reworded Rewarded Respects my Domineering Forces ideas ideas Easy to talk to Understand- ing Sense of humor Able to lead Willing to become involved Uses good judgment Open to new Distrustful ideas Patronizing Unrelating Immature Disinter- on 118 Looks down on us Doesn't com- municate Dull (not fun to be with) Selfish Doesn't seem con- cerned Phony Doesn't trust us First, some observer bias is present due to explanations that had to be made in the conference setting during which the data were collected. External validity is thus affected in that generalizability is limited to the ex- tent that other youth may not have the awareness created by these explanations. limited due to the membership of the sample. Second, generalizability is also The most 181 that can be said with much confidence is that the findings hold for all those present at the meeting dur- ing which the data was collected. The Researph Findings For analysis the raw data were statistically ’weighted in order to give each item a common basis for comparison. The weighting procedure used for each analysis is indicated in each accompanying table. Peer Leadership Ability to lead is clearly the most important con- cern youth have for their peers. ABLE TO LEAD was ranked "most important" with WILLING TO BECOME IN- VOLvn, UNDERSTANDING and RESPECTS MY IDEAS being placed in the second, third and fourth ranking levels respect- ively. There does not appear to be any significant difference between males and females concerning peer leadership competency items with regard to the number of responses to a trait (in contrast, for example, to the ”trust” item in Table A3). There is, however, a notice- able difference between females and males concerning some traits (i.e., with regard to the ranking level selected). Table A2 shows that girls consider it much more important that a peer leader is WILLING TO BECOME INVOLVED (ranking this item second) than do boys who see 182 TABLE A2 - Frequency IWeighted) of Selection of the El ht PEER Leadershi Com etency Faptors Fifi“? . 53:13 £2131 3* Respects My Ideas 255 7 81‘757‘ 336 (4) L Easy to Talk to 216 (6) 38 (4) ‘ 304 (6) M Understanding 264 (3) 90 (3) 354 (3) C Sense of Humor 98 (8) 34 (8) 132 (8) T Able to Lead 311 (1) 106 (l) 417 (l) J Willing to Become 295 (2)4___p75 (7) 370 (2) Involved 295 (2) 75 (7) 370 (2) S Uses Good Judgment 223 (5)e———e94 (2) 317 (5) H Open to New Ideas 210 (7) 80 (6) 290 (7) Weighting procedure: Most important choice 8 8 Least important choice 8 1 Others sealed 7 - 2 Numbers in parentheses - rank e——sunusually large discrepancies across sex Ratio Panel; to hdle - Blil this item as next to last (Of those given) in importance (ranking it seventh). With regard to the item USES GOOD JUDGMENT, the converse is true. Males rank this item second in importance, while females place it considerably lower (fifth). On the negative side, considering these items *Upper case letters appeared on each trait card to facilitate ranking the traits on the response sheet (Figure l). 183 TABLE A3 - Frequency (Weighted)of Selection Of the Eight PEER Leadership Indohpetengy Faptors Page Page 5321' G Forces Ideas on Us 299 (l) 91 3) 390 1 D Looks DoWn on Us 288 (2) 92 (2) 380 (2) R Doesn't Communicate 238 (6) 79 (5) 317 (5) K Dull (Not Fun to Be With) 73 (8) 24 (8) 97 (8) s Selfish 4 243 (5) 66 (7) 309 (6) N Doesn't Seem Concerned 273 (4) 90 (4) 363 (3) U Phony 278 (3)¢—-—e?8 (6) 356 (4) W Doesn't Trust us ' 180 (7)e——e98 (l) 278 (7) Weighting procedure: Most important choice . 8 Least important choice 8 1 Others sealed 7 - 2 Numbers in parentheses = rank e——sunusually large discrepancies across BOX. Ratio Femgle to Mule = 3:1 relating to peer leadership incompetency, there is a no- table difference between males and females with regard to two of the traits. DOESN'T TRUST US is ranked first by boys and only seventh by girls. Viewed another way, 180 isn't even twice 98 in spite Of the fact that the female to male ratio is 3:1. This finding leads us to suspect that not being trusted by peer leaders is much more important to males than to females. It is also noted that male and female responses are 184 considerably different with regard tO a peer leader's being PHONY. Males ranked this trait in sixth place, thus considering it not as serious as the females who placed it third. The other traits are relatively close together in rank. These findings are visualized in Table 3. In spite of the wide differences Of opinion be- tween boys and girls with regard to peer leader's trust, they quite clearly ranked FORCES IDEAS ON US as the most serious trait which would result in peer leadership incompeteney. The second, third and fourth most seri- ous traits are LOOKS DOWN ON US, DOESN'T SEEM CONCERNED and PHONY. One of the most striking points of compar- ison is the considerable agreement that of all the traits, being DULL (NOT FUN TO BE WITH) is the least serious. Adult Leadership Male and female responses are remarkably similar with regard to most adult leadership competency items. Ranked most important was UNDERSTANDING. Almost tied for first was EASY TO TALK TO, which missed being con- sidered most important by only one point. The third and fourth traits were RESPECTS MY IDEAS and WILLING TO BECOME INVOLVED. There was also a high degree Of 185 agreement that of all the traits, SENSE OF HUMOR is least important. These relationships can be seen in Table A4. TABLE A4 - Frequency (Weighted) of Selection of the Eight ADULT Legdership Competengy Fadtors 2.... F22? Iii; £231 SB Respects My Ideas 292 73) 91 737 383 (3) L Easy to Talk To 306 (l) 111 (2) 417 (2) M Understanding 305 (2) 113 (1) 418 (1) C Sense of Humor 126 (8) 44 (8) 170 (8) T Able to Lead 167 (7) 74 (5) 241 (7) J Willingdto Become 256 (4) 8O (4) 336 (4) S Uses Good Judgment 184 (6) 64 (7) 248 (6) H Open to New Ideas 236 (5) 71 (6) 307 (5) Weighting procedure: Most important choice = 8 Least important choice 8 1 Others sealed 7 - 2 Numbers in parentheses = rank Rgtio deale to Mule = 3:l It is noteworthy that three Of the top four traits ranked most important in peer leader competencies ap- pear also in the tOp four for adults (see Table A2, A4, and A8). While occurring in different orders, youth view UNDERSTANDING, RESPECTS MY IDEAS and WILLING TO BECOME INVOLVED as being considerably important in both their peer and adult leaders. Similarly, of all the 186 traits, they consider having a SENSE 0F HUMOR as being least important by a substantial margin. On the negative side, as visualized in Table A5, there is also considerable agreement of both sexes con- cerning these traits which lead to adult leadership incompetency. Heading the list as being viewed as 'most serious is DOESN'T TRUST US. It is of interest to note (while probably not statistically significant) that an adult leader's distrust is more important to girls than to boys, whereas it was seen above (Table A3) that boys were more concerned than girls with distrust TABLE A5 - Frequency (Weighted) of Selection Of the FadtOEs L - :Ffiégéé '“" :giEFa ' iggtal G Forces Ideas on Us 300—72) 9772)w 397 D Looks Down on Us 287 (3) 104 (l) 391 (3) R Doesn't Communicate 255 (4) 74 (6) 329 (4) K Bull)(Not Fun to Be 97 (8) 31 (8) 128 (8) N Doesn't Seem Concerned 229 (5) 85 (4) 314 (5) U Phony 185 (7) 76 (5) 216 (6) W Doesn't Trust Us 333 (l) 90 (3) 423 (l) Weighting procedure: Most serious choice = 8 Least serious choice = 1 Others sealed 7 - 2 Numbers in parentheses = rank Ratio Female to Male = 3:1 187 in peer leaders. .In second, third and fourth place, respectively, are FORCES IDEAS ON US, LOOKS DOWN ON US, and DOESN'T COMMUNICATE. Two of these tOp four, FORCES IDEAS ON US and LOOKS DOWN ON US are also included in the tOp four traits Of incompetency Of peer leaders (Table A3), and they are listed in the same order for peer leaders, with the exception that in the peer listing they are ranked first and second instead Of second and third as in the adult. Characteristics of Mother There are very slight differences between male and female responses with regard to mother (Table A6). The only noticeable differences are with regard to ABLE TO LEAD and LOOKS DOWN ON US. If these differences say anything, it appears that boys are a little more con- cerned with seeing leadership in mother and about being looked down on by her than are girls. What is most striking about the findings concern- ing mother is the parallel between the first three traits ranked for mother with the first three ranked for adult leader competency. The first three traits in both cat- egories are exactly the same, namely, UNDERSTANDING, EASY TO TALK TO, AND RESPECTS MY IDEAS, and they appear TABLE A6 - Frequency (Weighted) of Selection of the Slrteen Important dharagteristids of Mother Fagtors Parse 332:: I221 R Respects my Ideas 89 (3) 37 (3) 126 (3) L Easy to Talk to 107 (2) 39 (2) 146 (2) M Understanding 110 (l) 48 (l) 158 (l) C Sense Of Humor 68 (4) l7 (4) 85 (4) T Able to Lead 2 (12)e——-e4 (2) 6 (10) J Willing to Become 31 (6) 8 (7) 39 (6) Involved 8 Uses Good Jugment .49 (5) 14 (5) 63 (5) H Open to New Ideas 24 (7) 13 (6) 37 (7) G Forces Ideas on Us 3 (ll) 1 (ll) 4 (11) D Looks Down on Us 0 (14)e———e4 (9) 4 (11) R Doesn't Communicate 6 (9) 3 (10) 9 (9) K Dull (Not Fun to Be 0 (14) O (12) O (14) With) E Selfish l (13) O (12) 1 (13) N Doesn't Seem Concerned 2 (12) O (12) 2 (12) U Phony 4 (10) O (12) 2 (12) W Doesn't Trust Us 14 (8) 5 (8) l9 (8) Weighting procedure: First citation = 4 Last citation Others sealed 3 - 2 Numbers in parentheses = rank e——eunusually large discrepancies across sex Ratio Female to Male = 33l 189 in the same order. This relationship should be watched in the next study and even examined in more depth in future research. Characteristics Of Father More variability between male and female responses is seen with regard to important characteristics of father (Table A7). By noting the ranking it can be seen that there are considerable differences between boys and girls with respect to UNDERSTANDING, ABLE TO LEAD, WILL- ING TO BECOME INVOLVED, DULL (NOT FUN TO BE WITH), SELF- ISH, and PHONY. Examining the point spread of the weighted frequencies of selection, unusually large dis- crepancies between girls and boys are also seen with regard to SENSE OF HUMOR, FORCES IDEAS 0N US, DOESN‘T COMMUNICATE and DOESN'T SEEM CONCERNED. For example, while FORCES IDEAS ON US is ranked similarly (10th for girls, 9th for boys), the ratio of reSponses is almost 1:1 instead of the 3:1 which would be expected due to the sexual distribution of the respondents. Conversely, SENSE OF HUMOR, while ranked about the same by both girls and boys (first by girls and third by boys), has a score of 93, considerably more than the 3:1 ratio would lead one to expect to see. Boys tend to see father as being more UNDERSTANDING than do girls. 190 TABLE A7 - Frequency (Weighted) of Selection of the Sigtedh Important thrapteristigs of Father Faptors Weighting procedure: First citation = 4 Ratio Female to Male = 3:1 Last citation = 1 Others scaled 3 - 2 Numbers in parentheses = e——vunusually large discrepancies BCI'OBS 88X Female Male Total B Resp::::0;: Ideas 482(5) lgi(%) 262(57 L Easy to Talk To 46 (6) 14 (5). 6o (6) M Understanding 51 (4)e———e21 (l) 72 (3) AC Sense of Humor 93 (l)e,-——el9 (3) 112 (l) T‘Able to Lead 58 (3)e———+10 (8) 68 (4) J irinlrfisgdto Become 32 (NH 7 (10) 39 (7) S Uses Good Judgment _73 (2) 20 (2) 93 (2) H Open to New Ideas 12 (10) 3 (12) 15 (12) G Forces Ideas on Us 12 (lO)o—-———e9 (9) 21 (10) D Looks Down on Us 9 (12) 4 (ll) l3 (13) R Doesn't Communicate 22 (8) em-e 12 (6) 34 (8) K Dull (Not Fun to be 2 (l4)e———elO (8) 12 (14) With) E Selfish 5 (UM—+11 (7) 16 (11) N Doesn't Seem Concerned l6 (9)«————ell (7) 27 (9) U Phony l (15)‘“--*3 (12) 4 (15) W Doesn't Trust Us 10 (ll) 3 (12) l3 (l3) 191 Table A7 also shows that more reapondents used the neg- ative in describing the fathers than the mothers. Respondents tend to see leadership in father but not in mothers. ABLE TO LEAD was ranked fourth for fathers and tenth for mothers. The first four traits seen as most important about father were as follows: SENSE OF HUMOR, USES GOOD JUDG- MENT, UNDERSTANDING and ABLE TO LEAD. However, it is notable that boys placed UNDERSTANDING first in the father category and in the mother and adult leader cate- « gories as well. SENSE OF HUMOR, seen as least important in peer and adult leaders, is viewed by boys and girls as one Of the four most important traits of mother and father, ranked fourth for mother and first (except by boys who placed it third) for father. Comparing the Categories Table A8 Ofers a view of how the four main cate- gories compare with respect to the leadership traits. It enables the reader to Observe the major similarities and differences on a single page. The comparisons be- tween important characteristics Of mother and adult leader competencies appear to be cOnsiderably useful. The relationship exists not only with regard to the com- peteney factors but also the incompetencies. DOESN'T 1192 TABLE A8 - Summary of Rank-Order of Charactersltics Desirable With Desirable with Perceived Perceived Reapect to has set to Characteristics Characteristics PEER as deder ADU T as leader ,.21.MQIH§BS of FATHER Rank Female MalengtalLFemale_Malg~Total,Femalngale Total Female Male Total 1 T T T L M M M M M G M C 2 J S J M L L L L L S S S 3 M M M B B B B B B T C M 4 B L B J J J C C C m B T 5 S B S H T H S S S B L B 6 L H L S H S J H J L R L 7 H J H T S T H J H J E,N J 8 C C C C C C. W W W R T,K R R T,D R N G N U R T H,G J G G G G,D W D 8 KEY T,N §,E N,U D H,U H ,U W B Respects My Ideas L Easy to Talk TO E E E D,W M Understanding C Sense Of Humor D,K K K K T Able to Lead J Willing to Become Involved U U S Uses Good Judgment H Open to New Ideas G Forces Ideas on Us , v on Us 1- 232.33.3322223 R Doesn't Communicate for this anal sis K Dull (Not Fun to Be With) Y - E Selfish , Note 2. The 8 desirable and 3 2:32“ t Seem Concerned the 8 undesirable W Doesg't Trust Us traits were ranked (in free mixture) for this analysis. 193 TRUST US is ranked first (at least by girls, third by boys) for adult incompetence and first among the negative factors by both girls and boys with respect to mother. Figure A2 graphs the relationships even more visually. The numerical values represented on the graph are those identified in the "Total" column to Tables A2 - A7 in the above discussion in this appendix. The dotted lines on Figure A2 point out some of the most notable differences discussed above. For example, while UNDERSTANDING (M) is within the top three traits considered by young people as most important for both peer and adult leaders, the figure shows that it is not only ranked first for adults (as Opposed to third for peers) but also that UNDERSTANDING scored about 420 for adults compared with slightly less than 360 for peers. In the figure these numbers provide more information which is not as readily evident from the tables. For example, as consistently sure as youth are concerning what they like (cf. the parallels between adult leader and mothers, they are even more sure of what they don't like (of. DOESN'T TRUST US (W), which is ranked first in important characteristics of adult). 194 FIGURE A2 - Weighted Scores for Peer, Adult, Father and Mother Characteristics Cited by Combined Male and Female Responses 460 440 420 3 ML .4 . I+00 T. / f \ G l/ I I CD 380 . D’ / B I J N ,’ I 3601 M.’ U./ I; 3&0 B /AAV ‘ 320 ,’ ‘ 'l J R 300? L‘ E H N I H /I ‘\ 280; w“ \ I‘ \ 260; \ g 2405 ’TS 220 200 180 c 160 M (UNDERSTANDING) L‘ EASY TO TALK TO) 140 C \x K ‘ , . 120 B‘TRESPECTS NY IDEAS) _ ‘\\ \ ‘ \- C 100 K C ‘ \\ S x; “ 80 s '\ .%T 60 ‘L 40 JH JR N 20 w G E R DHK W o _ .,....‘--_,.......-1.--....PQ_T kENU . PEER ADULT MOTHER FATHER LEADER LEADER 195 The Research Conclusions What do these findings mean? The research ques- tions asked, (1) What are the factors which young peOple perceive as desirable and undesirable in their leaders and. (2) of those factors are there some that are con- sidered more important than others? Conclusions Regarding Peer Leaders It can be concluded, at least for the Muskegon group, that youth expegt their peer leaders to possess definite skills which make them ABLE TO LEAD. The having and using of such skills is considerably more important than the other seven traits as far as peer perception is concerned. There is a concern that peer leaders demon- strate skills which support their holding the position of leadership they do. Thus, when adults select certain young people for leadership positions in youth groups, they should be careful to select youth with these skills even though they may not be high on other nonessential criteria in the opinion of the adult leader (e.g., avoid selection on the basis of favoritism). o so 1 0 de 1 is e1 tiv l 1 as im 0 - tant for their peer leader to have a SENSE 0F HUMOR and t i is ti el less as ious if he is DULL NOT FUN TO BE WITH). These qualities were ranked last by a 196 large majority of the group in this study. This state- ment does not mean that these qualities are unimportant, it only means that vis-a-vis the other seven traits, they are less important. Conclusions Regarding Adult Leaders ‘ Both peer and adult leadeps, but especially the lattep, must bg UNDERSTANDING. UNDERSTANDING, ranked in the upper half of all four categories, placed third and first, respectively, in the peer and adult categories. It is relgtively less important for an adult leader to hgve a SENSE OE HUMOR, and it is relatively less serioug if he is DULL jNOT FUN TO BE WITH). These qual- ities, ranked last by a majority of the group in this study, are not, thereby, to be considered unimportant to youth, but are to be seen as less important over against the other seven examined. It is essential that pp adult leadep tpust youth. DOESN'T TRUST US was seen as the most serious trait caus- ing adults to be poor leaders. Yoppg people pay not be "looking pope f9: authority figppes who forthrightly state, 'Thig is the way it is,'” The readerwill note that the October 21, 1977 Evangeli- gal Newslgppgp stated that there appears to be a return to the use of large youth rallies used in the 50's and 197 60's. The article suggested that one reason might be an underlying desire on the part of youth for leaders who speak with a voice of authority. If this were the case, one might suspect to find in the Gamelin traits such characteristics as SPEAKS WITH AUTHORITY in the listing of desirable qualities. However, such is absent. Moreover, one might also expect to find a rela- tively low rankingof FORCES IDEAS ON US. It may be of considerable importance with regard to this issue that the Opposite occurred in each category. FORCES IDEAS ON US was ranked first (most Serious) for ppgp_leaders, second for pgplp leaders, third for ppppgp and second for 2.21.9.1:- Ten years ago YFC was moving toward a "Teen-to- Teen" strategy for which the Ward and Harmon study pro- vided information. The present study does not disconfirm that prior approach. Neither, however, does it indicate that large ral- lies are wrong. The present.findings also do not indicate that leaders should refrain from giving their Opinion of "This is the way it is." This study does suggest, rather strongly, however, that if youth leaders do say "This is the way it is," they should allow youth to come to their own conclusions with regard to "it" and not force their position on others. 198 The thzeg tpgits post valhed by youth in theip adult leadeps and motheps ape UNDERSTANDING, being EASY TO TALK TO and having RESPECT fop their ideas, in that order. This selection seems to indicate that youth prefer adults in leadership capacities who can relate to others over those who may have intellectual and administrative skills but function less effectively in the interpersonal dimen- sion. Relational rather than technical characteristics are valued by youth in their choices concerning adult leadership. The high ranking of USES GOOD JUDGMENT and ABLE TO LEAD in the father category, in place of the more har- mony-building traits attributed to ppthpp (such as EASY TO TALK T0 or RESPECTS MY IDEAS), may reflect what Berelson and Steiner (196%, p. 34#) refer to as the dual demands of leadership (the guidance function of the intellectual leader and the harmony function of the social leader) which are “rarely combined in the same person" (p. 3##). The findings in the present study would seem to indicate that the majority of the youth see the father as the in- tellectual leader and the mother the social. In fact, Berelson and Steiner note that they have found it to be the case that these two reaponsibilities are divided be- tween the ;gthgp and the mothep in families (p. 3u6). The authors also give a clue as to one possible reason 199 for the mother categOry being more closely associated with favorable ggptt leader traits than the £3thgp. "When put to the choice, most group leaders give up the instrumental (guidance) role in favor of popularity" p. 3&6. \ The findings have generated at least two hypotheses, the confirmation of which in a study of a different de- sign would yield useful information. The first hypoth- esis is as follows: ability to lead is considered by youth to be the most important single trait for a peer leader to possess. ABLE TO LEAD was selected by a sub- stantial majority in the present study as the most impor- tant peer trait. The second hypothesis is that there is a significant correlation between ppthgp and ggptt leader with respect to UNDERSTANDING, EASY TO TALK TO and RESPECTS MY IDEAS. This relationship was obtained in the data in this study. Confirmation of this hypothesis would lend strong support to the findings presented in this report which suggest that youth most desire relational and understanding- oriented traits in their adult leaders. APPENDIX B The Second Pilot Study 200 The design of the second pilot study was such that two taped interviews were to be made in each of three major sections of the continental United States. Two were to come from the West, two from the Midwest and two from the East. Of the two in each sector, one was to be from a group of "average" youth and two from a group who had had trouble with the law through con- 'viction as a result of some kind of delinquent be- havior. The purpose of the study was to validate the instrument for the main study which would include both types of young peOple. As reported in the mainitext of the study, two of the six tapes were never made. Since both of those tapes were from Youth Guidance groups, who work with de- linquent young people, there is reason to believe that the validity is lower for these groups than for the average youth. Comments in this regard from Youth Guidance leaders confirm this suspicion. On the next page is a cOpy of the cover letter which accompanied the interview instrument that was used in the study. It was sent out over the signature of the YFC Director of Research and DevelOpment whose (R & D) committee members, who were heads of the regions to which the tapes were sent, conducted the interviews. The two pages which follow the cover letter comprise 201 Figure Bl - Cover Letter for Second Pilot Study May 1978 Dear Thank you very much for your willingness to participate in the study of leadership behaviors which youth perceive as important. With your help we'll be able to find out whether some potentially valuable insights obtained from a small- scale pilot study will hold up on a national level. If 80. the implications for all of us in youth work will be far— reaching. In order to develop an accurate questionnaire for the main study, we're first going to have to find out what words youth use in describing leadership. To obtain this informa- tion two different types of youth will be interviewed in three major sections of the United States (west, midwest and east . One type of youth will be those typical of young people usually involved in Campus Life clubs. Yet we are also in- terested in contrasting these youth with those farther out of the social norm, for example, those who are worked with in Youth Guidance or similar types. We need information pertaining to "deviant" as well as to "average" young peO« ple. We would like to ask you to interview a group or groups as indicated: (one group who fit the basic norm/one group who are different from the basic social norm). Please select a groug of young people of this type who are in the age range of l - 18. The size of the group should be roughly in the range of from 3 - 10 in number. We would like you to tape the interview. Taping will free you to follow the flow of the conversation without having to write down all that is said, which we can do back at the office. The introduction to the enclosed instrument provides further details pertaining to the interviews. Please h? sure to mark on your tape the following informa- tion: ( your name, (2) the type of young people, (3) the date, (U) the city and state where the interview was held. It will be very helpful if you can return the tape to me within the next three weeks. Thanks very much again for all your help. I'm looking forward to meeting you in person in a few months and to working with you on this very promising endeavor. Sincerely, 202 FIGURE BZ - Leadership Behaviors Youth Perceive as Important INTERVIEW.INSTRUMENT Introdugtion l. 2. Before meeting the reapondents prepare the tape re- corder. a. It's best to use a plug-in (110 volt) cassette recorder. b. If you are using tape which has a leader (the clear colored piece of tape at the beginning which doesn't record) be sure to cue up the tape so you are past the leader before you turn on the re- corder. Otherwise some of your interview will be lost. c. If possible, use a mike which contains a remote control on-off switch. Have the buttons on the machine pre-set to record so that all you have to do when you begin the interview is to move the remote control from ”off" to ”on." Minimize the mechanics involved with the taping as these can be distracting to some reapondents and make them nervous, thus reducing their productivity in the interview. Arrange to conduct your interview at a tips and plape convenient for your respondents. a. Make it at a time when other reSponsibilities are minimal, so they aren't preoccupied with other matters or too tired. b. Make it at a place where interruptions are least likely to occur and where the respondents will feel most comfortable. In conducting the interview, the following will be helpful. a. Explain to each group exactly what is being done and for what purpose. 1) In order to obtain leaders who will do the best job, we need to know what good leaders are like, so we know how to choose. 2) We also need to have a better understanding of what makes a person a poor leader, so we 203 can avoid selecting peOple who do these things, and so the leaders we already have can avoid doing these things. b. Talk with the group for a few minutes about general matters of interest to them (for exam- ple, where they go to school, what they have the most fun doing, vocational interests). c. When the group appears to be at ease, turn on the recorder as you ask the first question. d. Avoid rushing the interview. If a pause de- velops after a question, let them think. On the other hand, watch your time. By the end of the first side, when it comes time to flip the tape over, the group should at least be throu h question #2. (We are assuming a 0-30 or a C- O cassette, as you choose.) e. Probe for further meaning if necessary. If some- one says something that is unclear to you, it will quite likely be unclear to the rest of us as well. Ask them to explain what they mean if there is any doubt. 929m 1. When a person pbOpt yopp own pg; is your leader, what do you li e? a. What is good about such a leader? b. What do you want this leader to do? c. What does this leader ppt do? d. How would you complete this sentence? ”What I especially like in a leader my own age is . . . .” 2. When a person bo t o own is your leader, what do you dtslIEE? a. What makes such a person a poor leader? b. What does this person do that he or she shouldn't do? c. What does this person ppt do that he or she should do? d. How would you complete this sentence? "What I especially dislike in a leader my own age 1. O O O O" 3. When an pgult is your leader, what do you 113;? 20# 3. When an.§pp;t is your leader, what do you like? a. b. c. d. What is good about such a leader? What do you want this leader to do? What does this leader ppt do? How would you complete this sentence? ”What I especially like in an adult leader is . . . ." vh. When antpgplt is your leader, what do you gtptihp? a. b. c. d. What makes such a person a poor leader? :hat does this person do that he or she shouldn't o? What does this person ppt do that he or she should do? How would you complete this sentence? What I {specially dislike in an adult leader . O O O 0' 205 the interview instrument. As also indicated above in the text, three new behaviors were obtained for the positive scale and three for the negative scale of the instrument for the main studyzwhich were not part of Gamelin's original work. The second pilot also provided payoff in indi- cating which items from the instrument used in the first (Muskegon) pilot should be reworded. Three three new positive items obtained from this second pilot were ”communicates," “organizes well," and ”shares own shortcomings and problems." Confirm- ing the benefit of the second pilot is the awareness that all three of these behaviors figured prominently in the findings of the main study. The three new negative behaviors obtained from the second pilot were ”favors some over others (picks fa- vorites),“ "puts own interests ahead of group (others)," and ”gets upset when things don't go right.” Here, again, one of the new items, the last, figured signifi- cantly in the findings of the main study. The second pilot was also very helpful in phrasing the wording of the items in such a way that they ex- pressed the thoughts in the language the young peOple in the target pOpulation regularly use. For example, the author questioned whether to use the word ”relate," 206 raising the issue of whether the content of the word was precisely enough understood by youth from the various sectors of the country as to be useable. In listening carefully to the tapes, it was readily ap- parent that the young people from each region Surveyed used the word consistently in the same manner and was, indeed, therefore useful. Another way in which the second pilot fulfilled an important function with regard to the main study was in facilitating decision-making as to which words indi- cated behaviors even though they were not expressed in verbal form. For example, after analyzing the tapes it was decided to leave the word "understand" in the item expressing that concept. The decision was based on the belief that this word not only reflects a mental behavior, but, moreover, is an activity that young people recog- nize and have a concensus concerning the meaning of which. This concern for meaning was one of the main principles that governed the decision to use observable behaviors for the expression of the items, the other principles being measurability and reduction of overlap, i.e., behavioral expression would facilitate the develOp- ment of items that would be more mutually exclusive. With the arrival of the Miami conference came a 207 rare opportunity to meet with all the regional R & D leaders who would be performing a crucial role in the data gathering and in the training of the data gatherers. It was thus necessary to terminate the second pilot without the two tapes from Youth Guidance and train the trainers of the data gatherers at this meeting. The main study was about to begin, but not before these important and formative developments the second pilot had already made possible. APPENDIX C The Instrument 208 Your Age: Male/Female THINGS I LIKE AND DISLIKE IN PEOPLE MY AGE WHO ARE LEADERS Please circle the number which shows how important each thing is to you. SAMPLE How Important Is It? ’ Little Very Smiles when he or she gives commands 0 1 © 3 4 In the sample the person circling number 2 sees the leader's smiling as having m but not great importance. THINGS I LIKE How @13le Is It? (LEADERS MY AGE) Little ery Communicates (l l .3 3 4 Displays adequate knowledge and ability 0 l .2 3 4 Lets young people take responsibility {or important tasks 0 l .3 3 4 Listens 0 l 2 3 4 r Organizes well 0 l .2 3 4 Seeks to help when needed 0 l .2 3 4 Shares own shortcomings and problems 0 l 2 3 4 Shows sense of humor 0 1 2 3 4 Tries new ideas--open 0 l 2 3 4 Understands concerns of young people 0 l 2 3 4 Uses firmness when necessary 0 l 2 3 4 THINGS I DISLIKF. How S3193 Is It? (LEADERS MY AGE) Little Very Doesn't follow through--irresponsible O l 2 3 4 Doesn't relate to young pcople--doesn't see their point of view 0 l 2 3 4 Doesn't show concern {or young people 0 l 2 3 4 Doesn‘t trust young people 0 l 2 3 4 Favors some over others (picks favorites) 0 l 2 3 4 Forces ideas on young people 0 1 2 3 4 Gets upset when things don't go right 0 1 2 3 4 Looks down on young people 0 l 2 3 4 Puts own interests ahead of group 0 l 2 3 4 Says one thing. but does another--dishonest O 1 2 3 4 Won't changc--old-fashioned 0 l 2 3 4 209 THINGS I LIKE AND DISLIKE ABOUT ADULT LEADERS Please circle the number which shows how important each thing is to you. J— Communicates , . 0 ‘ l 2 3 4 Displays adequate knowledge and ability 0 1 2 3 4 Lets young people take responsibility for important tasks 0 l 2 3 4 Listens 0 l 2 3 4 Organizes well 0 I Z 3 4 Seeks to help when needed 0 l 2 3 4 Shares own shortcomings and problems 0 l 2 3 4 Shows sense of humor 0 I 2 3 4 Tries new ideas--open 0 l 2 3 4 Understands concerns of young people ‘ 0 l 2 3 4 Uses firmness when necessary 0 1 2 3 4 THINGS I DISLIKE How Serious Is It? (IN ADULT LEADERS) Little Very Doesn't follow through--irresponsible ' 0 l 2 3 4 Doesn't relate to young people--doesn't see their point of view 0 l 2 3 4 Doesn't show concern for young people 0 1 Z 3 4 Doesn't trust young people 0 l 2 3 4 Favors some over others (picks favorites) 0 l 2 3 4 Forces ideas on young people 0 l 2 3 4 Gets upset when things don't go right 0 l 2 3 4 Looks down on young people 0 l 2 3 4 Puts own interests ahead of group 0 l 2 3 4 Says one thing, but does another--dishonest 0 l 2 3 4 Won't change-~old-fashioned 0 1 2 3 4 210 Your Age: Male/Female IMPORTANT THINGS ABOUT MY MOTHER Please circle the number which shows how much each thing is true of your mother. Never Sometimes Always Communicates . 0 1 ' 2 3 4 Displays adequate knowledge and ability 0 l 2 3 4 Lets me take responsibility for important tasks 0 1 Z 3 4 Listens 0 1 2 3 4 Organizes well 0 l 2 3 4 Seeks to help when needed 0 l 2 3 4 Shares own shortcomings and problems 0 l 2 3 4 Shows sense of humor 0 l 2 3 4 Tries new ideas--open 0 1 Z 3 4 Understands my concerns 0 1 2 3 4 Uses firmness when necessary 0 1 2 3 4 Never Sometimes Always Doesn't follow through--irresponsible . 0 l 2 3 4 Doesn't relate to me--doesn't see my point of view 0 1 Z 3 4 Doesn't show concern for me 0 1 Z 3 4 Doesn't trust me 0 1 2 3 4 Favors some over others (picks favorites) 0 l 2 3 4 Forces ideas on me 0 l 2 3 4 Gets upset when things don't go right 0 1 2 3 4 Looks down on me 0 1 2 3 4 Puts own interests ahead of others 0 l 2 3 4 Says one thing. but does another--dishonest 0 1 2 3 4 Won't change--old-fashioned 0 1 2 3 4 211 IMPORTANT THINGS ABOUT MY FATHER Please circle the number which shows how much each thing is true of your father. Never Sometimes Always Communicates 0 1 . 2 3 4 Displays adequate knowledge and ability 0 l 2 3 4 Lets me take responsibility for important tasks 0 l 2 3 4 'Listens 0 l 2 3 4 Organizes well 0 l 2 3 4 Seeks to help when needed 0 l 2 3 4 Shares own shortcomings and problems 0 l 2 3 4 Shows sense of humor 0 l 2 3 4 Tries new ideas--open 0 l 2 3 4 Understands my concerns 0 l 2 3 4 Uses firmness when necessary 0 l 2 3 4 Never Sometimes Always Doesn't follow through--irresponsible 0 l 2 3 4 Doesn't relate to me--doesn't see my point of view 0 l 2 3 4 Doesn't show concern for me 0 l 2 3 4 Doesn't trust me 0 1 Z 3 4 Favors some over others (picks favorites) 0 1 2 3 4 Forces ideas on me 0 l 2 3 4 Gets upset when things don't go right 0 1 2 3 4 Looks down on me 0 1 2 3 4 Puts own interests ahead of others ' 0 1 Z 3 4 Says one thing. but does another-dishonest 0 1 2 3 4 Won't change--old-fashioned 0 l 2 3 4 APPENDIX D Instructions for the Trainers of Data Gatherers i. 3. 4. 6. 7. 9. 10. ll. 212 INSTRUCTIONS FOR THE TRAINERS OF DATA GATHERERS At a group session meet with the data gatherers to go through each of the following points on this sheet. Meet personally with any who couldn't attend the group session. Distribute the “INSTRUCTIONS FOR DATA GATHERERS" sheet. Read through item I]. Distribute the questionnaire. a. Let them complete the questionnaire. b. After they've completed the questionnaire. direct their attention to items 2-4 on the "INSTRUCTIONS FOR DATA GATHERERS" sheet. c. Read through the rest of the items on the "INSTRUCTIONS FOR DATA GATHERERS" sheet. Ask if there are any questions. Demonstrate the procedures called for in items 2-4 of the "INSTRUCTIONS FOR DATA GATHERERS" sheet. Ask each data gathercr to do a demonstration as you just did. If the group in your training session is too large to permit each to do a demonstration in the time allotted. divide into groups of two so each person can have a turn. a. Then ask one to volunteer to do a demonstration for the large group. b. Discuss any questions. Establish what groups of young people will be selected. See and explain the 'GROUP DESCRIPTION SHEET." We need to survey groups where the member- ship is nonvoluntary (e.g. . biology class) and voluntary (e.g. . band. baseball): urban. suburban and rural; and Youth Guidance groups. Try for 50 in each. Distribute and discuss the "PUBLIC RELATIONS PRINCIPLES FOR CONDUCT- ING THE SURVEY” sheets. Establish a date in accord with the Miami guideline as to when each data gatherer will have completed his or her investigations. Follow-up with a phone call within a week after this training meeting to check whether the following have occurred: a. ~Ths selection of youth groups have been made. I). The dates for data collection have been set. Within a week after the date established in Miami. call each data gatherer to check whether the following have been done: a. The data has been obtained. b. The questionnaires and "GROUP DESCRIPTION SHEETS" have been mailed back to the UTE-Eton office. c. If the data has not been gathered. do the following: 1) Call once a week to check on progress. 2) Recognize that there may be legitimate reasons for delay. 3) Review the importance of obtaining the data soon. a) The validity and reliability of the study will be impaired by delay. b) Staff time and machine time at headquarters is geared to receive the data at a specific time and delay will be costly. Thank the data gatherers by phone or letter for their time and effort. APPENDIX E Instructions for Data Gatherers Public Relations Principles for Conducting the Survey Group Description Sheet 3. 4. 5. 213 INSTRUCTIONS FOR DATA GATHERERS Explain the following to the young people: b. We are studying the behaviors of leaders as viewed by high school young people. 1) We want to know what leaders do that attract youth and what they do that turn them off. 2) This understanding will help us select leaders who will be most effective in their work with young people. We are working with Michigan State University in this study which will involve several thousand young people from all across the United States. Distribute the questionnaire to each person. Study the following so you can explain this information to the young people without reading it to them. BE SURE, however. to cover each subject below. a. b. Co Ask the respondents to mark their age and the Male/Female section at the top of the first and third pages. Read the instruction and SAMPLE item at the top of the first page aloud. On both sides of the FIRST page: note that responses (circled numbers) refer to how important and how serious each statement is with respect to leaders their own age (on the front side) and to adult leaders (on the back side). On both sides of the LASTpaggi responses refer to how much each state- ment is true of their mother and father (or stepmother or stepfather). 1) If a respondent's mother or father is dead, ask him to complete the statements on the basis of what he remembers of the parent(s) . 2) If parents are divorced or separated. complete the statements per- taining to the parent the respondent is not residing with on the basis of memory. If the respondent can't remember a parent. leave the page uncompleted. 3) If a respondent has both a natural and a stepmother or stepfather, let him choose which one to report on (either the natural or the step). Explain to the group that each page contains 11 statements of what leaders do that are pleasing and 11 that are displeasing. Ask them to BE SURE to keep this difference in mind when completing the questionnaire. Ask if there are any questions. Ask them to please make a response to EACH statement. Ask them to Work independently. (If . however. you notice talking during the completion of the questionnaire. don't reprimand those who are talking.) Complete a "GROUP DESCRIPTION SHEET" for this group while they are completing the questionnaire. When the young people are finished, ask them to go back over their questionnaires to be sure each statement has been answered. l. 2. 214 PUBLIC RELATIONS PRINCIPLES FOR CONDUCTING THE SURVEY To conduct the survey in a school: Ce Meet first with the principal. 1) Explain the purpose of the study. See item #1 on "INSTRUCTIONS FOR DATA GATHERERS" sheet. 2) Mention the length of time for the survey: 15 minutes (including introductory explanations). 3) Indicate that only one session is needed for the survey. 4) Try to obtain time in the mornin . preferably not too close to lunch. for the survey. Also avoid the day before a holiday or a three-day weekend. 5) Ask to meet with the teacher and that the teacher be present during the survey. ' 6) Offer to share the results of the study with the principal if he or she desires. Meet with the teacher. 1) Cover items 1-3 as with the principal. 2) Establish a date and time with the teacher. See above #4. Be prompt in keeping that time. 3) Ask the teacher to be present during the survey. Explain the following to the young people: 1) Their frankness is truly desired: there are no right or wrong responses. We are not looking for any "hoped for" answers. 2) See the "INSTRUCTIONS FOR DATA GATHERERS" sheet. To conduct the survey in a church: a. b. C. d. Meet first with the pastor. or director of education if there is one. Follow the procedures indicated above for working with the principal of a school. Avoid unusual weekends. Meet with the teacher or group leader. Follow the procedures listed above for working with a school teacher. Include the above explanations for school young people in your explanations to a church youth group. Try to meet with groups on Sunday. preferably in the morning. If you are surveying a Sunday School class. try to conduct the survey at the beginning of the class. Try to avoid weekday afternoons and evenings after the young people have been in school. Number in the Group: Date of Survey: YFC Region: 215 One form needed for each group surveyed Fasten this cover sheet to the set of responses. GROUP DESCRIPTION SHEET Data gatherers: Please answer all questions. Feel free to use the space pro- vided (#7) for any additional information which would identify thoughts and feelings held by your respondents that are germane to this study. Thank you. 1. Z. 3. City and state in which survey was conducted Organization or school name Please check one of the following. (If you surveyed more than one group. please fill out a GROUP DESCRIPTION SHEET for each group and attach the appropriate sheet to the survey forms you return to the Wheaton YFC office. Please take care to avoid mix-ups.) choral group band baseball team. or other team: classroom (Please specify the course. e.g. . biology. history. math) club (Please specify. e.g.. French club. Scouts. ski club) church group (Please specify. e.g. . Sunday School class. cate- chism. youth fellowship) other (Please specify) Age range: Circle one: Urban . Suburban. Rural Circle one: Youth Guidance. other Any additional information about the group. (We need everything you can tell us!) Your name APPENDIX F Pearson Product Moment Correlation of Mother Negative and Father Negative Scales by Item 2115 PEARSON PRODUCT MOMENT CORRELATION OF MOTHER NEGATIVE AND FATHER NEGATIVE SCALES BY ITEM seas. wo=~. Noam. moam. a-~. aaa~. amsw. boom. waoa. Wham. «saw. amsm. mane. aSa . omua. amam. sooa. ween . moon. mmwa. menu. SR. mamm. aamn. sown. “can. aea~.. aoaa. aaaa . mamw. ~am~. ~ao~. aaea . da : m oa : h o t m anon. mnmn. oaon. mend. $2. . aaaa. 8R . owaa. 33a . awmw. San . m o m Nmom. Nova. omma. mama. NmbN. owma. opaN. «naa. aoaa. :ama. mafia. m I m mama a sense: canssm aoo. no unseauaBMao one musmaanuooo aa< aonu. eemm. aaoa. swam. aasm. mama. Naoa. mew“. oacu. anew. ouau. w u a aaou. Rana. aoaa. coma. mNaN. eaaa. woes. omoa. waaa. acm~. flea . a - a H‘ Boua .hosumm I a i m ax sous .uunpo: : a s z aomm. aaaa. Neon. Nosa. amam. maoa. oaaa. maoa. aaaa. mean. mama. a i m msaw. «maa. aooa. mama. emow. aamm. mama. aoca. anus. sea~. soma. a-» baom. ommm. anon. «new. cwom. moom. memw. aamN. aawm. mama. ONon. N i m Ran. Rena. amen. aaaa. Raou. cmwu. iaa . saaa. ooma . anew. ages. a i m CH Hr'l HNM3“\O¢\-QO\ 3:.EESZESSI: BIBLIOGRAPHY BIBLIOGRAPHY Adkins, Dorothy C. "Principles Underlying Performance Testd" in David A. Payne (ed). Curriculum Evalu- gtion. Lexington, Massachusetts: D. C. Heath and Company, 1974. Angell. Donald L. and DeSau, George T. "Rare Discord- ant Verbal Roles and the Development of Group Problem-Solving Conditions.” Spell Group Behavior, February, 197“, Vol. 5, No. l. Berelson, Bernard and Steiner, Gary A. Human Behavior: An Igventory of Scientific Findiggs. New York: Harcourt. Brace and World, Inc.. 196u. Borg, Walter R. and Call. Meredith D. Edugational Bgsegrgh: An Introdggtion. New York: David McKay Company. 1971. Campbell, Donald T. and Stanley, Julian C. Experiggntgl ggd Qggsi-Expezimental Designs for Research. Chéeago: Rand MeNally College Publish ng Company, 19 3- Coughlin. Joseph W. "The Study of a Program for Train- ing Leaders of Youth in the Formulation of Objec- tives." Unpublished Ph. D. dissertation, Department of Secondary Education and Curriculum, College of Education, Michigan State University, 1971. Douvan, Elizabeth and Adelson, Joseph. The Adolgscent Eggezienge. New York: John Wiley and Sons. Inc.. 19 . Downton, James V., Jr. Rgbel Leadership: Commitment ggd gaggisma in the Revolutionggy Proggss. New York: The Free Press, 1973. 217 218 Ebel, Robert A. ”A Consultant Talks about Evaluating a Social Studies Curriculum Improvement Project" in David A. Payne (ed). Qprrigplpp Evpluatiop. Lexington, Massachusetts: D. C. Heath and Company, 1974. . Essentials of Edu ation l Mea urement. Englewood Cliffs, New Jersey: Prentice-Hall, Inc., 1972. Erikson, Erik H. Childhood and Sogiety. (2nd ed.) New York: W. W. Norton and Company, Inc., 1963. Evppggiipgl Newslepter. October 21, 1977, Vol. a, No. 21. Fiedler, Fred Edward. A Theory of Leadership Effeg- rivgpgss. New York: McGraw-Hill, 19 7. and Chemers, Martin M. Leads shi nd Effe - . Glenview, Illinois: Scott, Foresman and Company, 1979. Gallup, George Jr. in a private conversation with the author in Oak Brook, Illinois on November 15, 1979. Gamelin, Francis C. "Descriptive Studies of Church Youth Workers.” Unpublished research report, Search Institute, Minneapolis, Minnesota, 1970. Good, Thomas L., Biddle, Bruce J., and Brophy, Jere E. T h e a ffe e . New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston, 1975. Halpin, Andrew W. and Winer, B. James. "A Factorial Study of the Leader Behavior Descriptions” in Ralph M. Stogdill and Alvin E. Coons (eds). Leader Behgvior: Its Dgsgription and Measurement. Colum- bus, Ohio: The Bureau of Business Research, College of Commerce and Administration, The Ohio State University, 1957. Hayes, William L. §§atistips for Ppyghologists. New York: Holt, Rinehart and W nston, 19 3. 219 Hemphill, John K. and Coons, Alvin E. "Development of the Leader Behavior Description Questionnaire" in Ralph M. Stogdill and Alvin E. Coons (eds). ngder Behavior: Its Description and Measurement. Columbus. Ohio: The Bureau of Business Research, College of Commerce and Administration, The Ohio State University, 1957. Holy Bible, The. Revised Standard Version. New York: Thomas Nelson and Sons, 1959. Horrocks, John E. The Ps cholo of Adoles en e: Behavior and Development. :3rd ed.) Boston: Houghton Mifflin Company, 1962. Jennings, Eugene E. "The Anatomy of Leadership," Management of Personnel Quarterly, VoI. I, No. 1 (Autumn, 1961), p. 2. Jones, Ralph H. Methods and Techniques of Edpcational Researph. Danville, Illinois: The Interstate Printers and Publishers, Inc., 1973. Kohlberg, Lawrence. "Stages of Moral Development as a Basis for Moral Education," in Beck, C. M., Crittenden, B. S., and Sullivan, E. V. Moral Education: Interdisgiplinary Approaches. New York: Newman Press, 1971. Krech, David and Crutchfield, Richard S. Elements of Psychology. New York:|Alfred A. KnOpf, 1959. Larkin, Ralph W. "Social Exchange in the Elementary School Classroom: The Problem of Teacher Legiti- mation of Social Power." Sogiology of Edupation, Fall 1975, Vol. #8, No. A. Leavitt, Harold J. Managerial Psychology. (3rd ed.) Chicago: The University of Chicago Press, 1973. Lewin, K., Lippitt, R. and White, R. K. "Patterns of Aggressive Behavior in Experimentally Created '80- cial Climates'” in D. S. Pugh (ed). Organizatiop Theory. London: Cox and Wyman Ltd., 197 . 220 Mayhew, Lewis B. "Measurement of Noncognitive Objec- tives" in David A. Payne (ed). Curriculum Eval- ugpion. Lexington, Massachusetts: D. C. Heath and Company, 1979. Pandey, Janek. "Effects of Leadership Style, Person- ality Characteristics and Method of Leader Selec- tion on Members' and Leaders' Behavior." EurOpean No. . Journal of Serial Psychology, 1976, Vol. 6, Perrow, Charles. Complex Organizations: A Critigal Es- s y. Glenview, Illinois: Scott, Foresman and Company, 1972. Richards, Lawrence 0. Youth Ministry: Its Renewal in the Logal Church. Grand Rapids, Michigan: Zondervan Publishing House, 1972, Scott, William A. "Attitude Measurement" in Gardner Lindzey and Elliot Aronson (eds). The Handbook of Sogial Psychology. (2nd ed.) Vol. II. Reading, Maggachusetts: Addison-Wesley Publishing Company, 19 . Shartle, Carroll L. "Introduction" in Ralph M. Stogdill and Alvin E. Coons (eds). Lgader Behavior: Its Despription and Measurement. Columbus, Ohio: The Bureau of Business Research, College of Commerce and Administration, The Ohio State University, 1957. Spaulding, H. and Haley, O. "A Study of Youth Work in Protestant Churches."l An unpublished report. Chicago: The National Council of Churches of Christ, 1955. Stanley, Donald Malcolm. "The Effects of Consideration and Initiating Structure as Moderated by Incremental Influence." Dissertation Abptrapts International B: The Sgienpes and Engineering. Vol. 35, No. 7, January 1975. Stewart, John S. "Toward a Theory for Values Development Education." Unpublished Ph. D. dissertation, De- partment of Secondary Education and Curriculum, College of Education, Michigan State University, 1974. N. Stogdill, Ralph M. Handbook_ofg;gadership: A Survey of Theory pnd Research. New York: The Free Press, 197U. 221 Stout, Robert and Briner, Conrad. "Leadership." Robert L. Ebel (ed). Encye10pedia of:§ducational Research. Toronto, Canada: Collier-Macmillan Canada Lts.. 1969. Strommen, Merton P. (ed). Research on Religious De- velopment: A Comprehensive Hgndbook. New York: Hawthorn Books, Inc., 1971. Ward, Ted and Harmon, Robert. "The Facts Unfold." Unpublished research report, Institute for Research in Human Learning, Michigan State University, 1970. I...rl..lllf I II.II}| .llll: Ill.